Sie sind auf Seite 1von 5

G Model

SOCSCI-1390; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS


The Social Science Journal xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

The Social Science Journal


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/soscij

The cycle of cyberbullying: Some experience required


John Chapin a,∗ , Grace Coleman b
a
Pennsylvania State University, USA
b
Crisis Center North, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Findings from a survey of 1,602 middle school and high school students suggests despite
Received 25 September 2016 heavy use of social media, adolescents believe they are less likely than peers to be victims of
Received in revised form cyberbullying and less likely to bully others. The results suggest a cycle of perceptions and
29 December 2016
behaviors: victims of cyberbullying bully others. Adolescents who believe they are likely
Accepted 6 March 2017
to be bullied acknowledge they are likely to continue bullying others and are also likely to
Available online xxx
blame victims for “bringing it on themselves.”
© 2017 Western Social Science Association. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
Cyberbullying
Risk-perception
Optimistic bias
Victim blaming

1. Introduction are reported to law enforcement; one in 10 adolescents or


teens have had embarrassing or damaging pictures taken
The U.S. Department of Health and Human Services of themselves without their permission, often using cell
(2016) define bullying as unwanted, aggressive behavior phone cameras; about one in five teens have posted or
among school aged children that involves a real or per- sent sexually suggestive or nude pictures of themselves to
ceived power imbalance. The behavior is repeated, or has others; cyberbullying victims are more likely to have low
the potential to be repeated, over time. Attacking some- self-esteem and to consider suicide (BJS, 2015).
one physically or verbally, making threats, and spreading On the perpetration side, a recent study of parent/child
rumors are all forms of bullying. Cyberbullying is bullying dyads (Barlett & Fennel, 2016) found parents’ perceptions
that takes place using electronic technology (devices and of their child’s Internet activity does not match the actual
equipment such as cell phones, computers, and tablets, amount of time spent online and cyberbullying behav-
as well as social media sites and text messages). Rumors iors. Monitoring does seem to reduce bullying though, as
sent by email or posted on social networking sites, mean ignorance was positively related to cyberbullying behav-
text messages or emails, and embarrassing pictures, videos, ior. Parental involvement also seems to play a role on the
websites, or fake profiles are some examples of cyberbul- victim side. A study of 2,000 high school students found
lying. The U.S. Uniform Crime Report (BJS, 2015) indicates strong parental relationships and friends at school were
around half of teens have been the victims of cyber bullying, more effective at limiting the harmful effects of cyberbully-
but only 10% tell a parent and few cyber bullying incidents ing than restrictions of Internet use (Davis & Koepke, 2016).
A large scale survey of 23,000 Canadian adolescents con-
firmed that all kinds of resources, including financial ones,
predict cyberbullying and can offset the consequences for
∗ Corresponding author at: Penn State University, 100 University Drive,
victims (Napoletano, Elgar, Saul, & Dirks, 2016).
Monaca, PA 15061, USA.
E-mail address: Jrc11@psu.edu (J. Chapin).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2017.03.004
0362-3319/© 2017 Western Social Science Association. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article in press as: Chapin, J., & Coleman, G. The cycle of cyberbullying: Some experience required. The
Social Science Journal (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2017.03.004
G Model
SOCSCI-1390; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 J. Chapin, G. Coleman / The Social Science Journal xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

The purpose of this paper is to explore the cycle of cyber- college students do not attend sexual assault workshops,
bullying from victim to perpetrator, using optimistic bias because they blame women for their own victimization
as a theoretical framework. (Rich et al., 2010). Men convicted of domestic violence often
blame their victims for putting them the situation (Armenti
2. Optimistic bias and violence & Babcock, 2016). The study also concluded that mandated
group therapy can be counter-productive, as other group
Optimistic bias (Weinstein, 1980) is the belief that bad members often reinforce that using violence in relation-
things happen to other people. The concept is grounded ships is acceptable. Even unconscious women are blamed
in health psychology and most frequently documented for their own sexual assaults, whether they are sleeping,
within health contexts, like skin cancer (Bryant, Zucca, intoxicated, or drugged (Heyes, 2016).
Brozek, Rock, & Bonevski, 2015), environmental hazards Based on the preceding review of the literature, the fol-
(Flanagan, Marvinney, & Zheng, 2015), and healthy eat- lowing hypotheses are posited:
ing habits (Sproesser, Klusmann, Schupp, & Renner, 2015).
H1. Adolescents believe they are less likely than others to
Crime and violence contexts have been explored more
become the victim of cyberbullying.
recently. College students believe they are less likely than
peers to be sexually assaulted (Untied & Dulaney, 2015). H2. Adolescents believe they are less likely than others to
This is a classic example of optimistic bias. Similar results cyberbully others.
were found regarding dating violence (Chapin, Strimel, &
H3. As optimistic bias about victimization is related to
Coleman, 2014), burglary (Joshi & Carter, 2013), even office
optimistic bias about perpetration.
data security (Bullee, Montoya, Pieters, Junger, & Hartel,
2015). People act on their perceptions, so understanding H3. As experience with bullying increases, optimistic bias
skewed risk perception is important. This is well docu- decreases.
mented within the optimistic bias literature: People who
RQ1. What is the relationship between optimistic bias and
believe they are less likely to get skin cancer are more likely
victim blaming?
to tan without sun screen; people who believe their data is
secure at work take less precautions; people who believe RQ2. What is the relationship between optimistic bias and
they are unlikely to be sexually assaulted also take fewer social media?
precautions.
3. Method
2.1. Optimistic bias and experience
3.1. Procedures and participants
A number of predictors of optimistic bias have been
explored over the years, including education (Flanagan Participants were recruited in the USA through school-
et al., 2015; Singh & Jha, 2013) and perceived exper- based programs about bullying offered by a Pennsylvania
tise (Chapin et al., 2014; Zeeb & Zeeb, 2013), but the women’s center during the 2015–2016 school year. Grades
best established is experience (Greenberg, Dyen, & Elliott, seven through 12 from multiple school districts were
2013; Raptou, Galanopoulos, Katrakilidis, & Mattas, 2012; included. Survey instruments were completed prior to
Sargeant, Majowicz, Sheth, & Edge, 2010; Trumbo, Lueck, the sessions to avoid skewing the data. Post-tests were
Marlatt, & Peek, 2011; Wolfers, de Zwart, & Kok, 2011). For collected to evaluate the effectiveness of the presen-
example, a college student who believes she will not be ter/presentation, but were not used for this analysis.
robbed is more likely to leave her dorm room unlocked. Students could attend the presentation while opting out of
Once she is robbed, the optimistic bias is reduced, and the study, but none did so. Students could also choose not
she begins locking her door, even for quick trips to the to answer individual items. The most commonly skipped
restroom. The studies above consistently document this items dealt with demographics (169 students did not dis-
pattern in a wide range of contexts, including communi- close their gender, and 318 students did not disclose their
cable diseases, smoking, and natural disasters. Greenberg race). The sample (N = 1,602) was 51% female, with an aver-
et al. (2013) interviewed over 1,000 people living poten- age age of 13 (range = 9 (middle school) to 18 (high school)).
tial disaster areas. Participants who experienced a natural These age ranges were selected, because they represent
disaster first-hand, were taking precautions for future the most common reported cases of bullying (National
disasters. Participants with no experience, exhibited opti- Center for Education Statistics, 2014). Consistent with the
mistic bias and were taking no precautions. demographics of the region, 80% of the participants were
Caucasian, 9% African–American, 2% Hispanic, 2% Asian and
2.2. Victim blaming the remaining identified as mixed-race or “other.”

Optimistic bias is the belief that bad things happen to 3.2. Materials
other people. One way to maintain this belief is blaming
the victim. People who get cancer smoke more than I do; Optimistic bias was measured with a standard instru-
people who get speeding tickets are worse drivers than I ment (Weinstein, 1989): “Cyberbullying is bullying that
am, etc. (Chapin & Chirico, 2003). Victim blaming is well takes place using electronic technology (cell phones, com-
documented in sexual assault studies (Rich, Utely, Janke, puters, social media sites, text messages, and websites).
& Moldoveanu, 2010; Taylor, 2009). For instance, male Examples of cyberbullying include mean text messages or

Please cite this article in press as: Chapin, J., & Coleman, G. The cycle of cyberbullying: Some experience required. The
Social Science Journal (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2017.03.004
G Model
SOCSCI-1390; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Chapin, G. Coleman / The Social Science Journal xxx (2017) xxx–xxx 3

Table 1
Zero-order correlations among variables predicting optimistic bias.

2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Optimistic bias (victim) .31** −.23** .13** .07** .03 −.01


2. Optimistic bias (perpetrator) – .03 .04 .03 .15** −.03
3. Experience (victim) – .06* .01 .26** −.09**
4. Social media – .37** .10** .18**
5. Age – .11** .21**
6. Experience (perpetrator) – .03
7. Experience (bystander) –

Note: Optimistic bias is indicated by a negative mean. For ease on interpretation, the signs in the first two rows have been reversed.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.

Table 2
Summary of linear regression analysis for variables predicting optimistic bias.

Predictor Optimistic bias/victim Optimistic bias/perpetrator


Adj. r2 = .25 Adj. r2 = .22
N = 1,498 N = 1,492

B SE B ˇ B SE B ˇ

Optimistic bias/perp .27 .02 .30*** – – –


Optimistic bias/victim – – – .33 .03 .30***
Experience/victim .30 .03 .21*** – – –
Experience/perp – – – .28 .05 .13***
Social media use .08 .02 .09*** – – –
Age .01 .02 .02 – – –
Victim blaming – – – .06 .02 .09***
***
p < .001.

emails, rumors posted on social media, and embarrassing to confine how the adolescents defined themselves. No dif-
pictures, videos, websites, or fake profiles. “Compared to ferences were predicted by gender, age, or race, and none
other people my age in the U.S., my chances of being cyber- were found.
bullied are:” (−3 = much less than others; 3 = much greater
than others). Optimistic bias is indicated by a negative
4. Results
mean. Participants were also asked about the likelihood
cyberbullying others: “Compared to other people my age
SPSS software was used for analysis. T-tests, cor-
in the U.S., my chances of cyberbullying are:”
relations, and linear regression analysis were used for
Experience was measured by combining responses to a
hypothesis testing. Table 1 shows zero-order correlations;
series of questions asking participants about verbal bully-
Table 2 shows the regression model for variables predicting
ing, physical bullying, and cyberbullying: “Verbal bullying
optimistic bias.
(Teasing, name-calling, inappropriate comments, taunting,
H1 predicted optimistic bias. Around a third (38%)
threats to cause harm); Physical bullying (Hitting, kick-
believed they were less likely than peers to become vic-
ing, pinching, spitting, tripping, pushing, taking or breaking
tims of cyberbullying. Some (17%) believed they were more
someone’s things); Cyberbullying (Mean text messages
likely than others to become victims. A single-sample t-
or emails, rumors posted on social networking sites, and
test was used to demonstrate the mean (−1.5, SD = 1.4)
embarrassing pictures, videos, websites, or fake profiles).”
was significantly less than zero, t (1565) = 96.3, p < .000. The
Participants circled responses to indicate “I have witnessed
negative mean indicates optimistic bias. H1 was supported.
this,” “This has happened to me,” and “I have done this to
H2 predicted optimistic bias regarding bullying oth-
others.” Three measures ranging from zero to three were
ers. The majority (73%) believed they were less likely than
created to indicate experience as a bystander (witness), as
peers to become perpetrators of cyberbullying. Some (8%)
a victim, and as a perpetrator.
acknowledge they are more likely than peers to become
Victim blaming was measured using an item adapted
bullies. A single-sample t-test was used to demonstrate
from Just World Beliefs scale: “I feel that many of the
the mean (−2.3, SD = 1.6) was significantly less than zero,
kids who are picked on bring it on themselves by the way
t (1542) = 57.2, p < .000. The negative mean indicates opti-
they dress or act.” Responses were on a Likert-type scale
mistic bias. H2 was supported.
(1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree).
H3 predicted a relationship between both forms of opti-
Participants were asked to indicate if they ever used the
mistic bias. Table 2 shows the strongest predictor of each
following social media platforms: texting, Facebook, Twit-
form of optimistic bias is the other form. Adolescents who
ter, Instagram, YikYak, Pinterest, SnapChat, or other. They
believe they will be victims, acknowledge they will also be
were also asked to indicate which one was their favorite.
perpetrators. Adolescents who acknowledge they are likely
Participants were also asked to provide their gender,
to cyberbully others in the future, believe they will also be
age, and race. These items were left open-ended, so as not
victims. H3 is supported.

Please cite this article in press as: Chapin, J., & Coleman, G. The cycle of cyberbullying: Some experience required. The
Social Science Journal (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2017.03.004
G Model
SOCSCI-1390; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS
4 J. Chapin, G. Coleman / The Social Science Journal xxx (2017) xxx–xxx

H4 predicted optimistic bias would decrease as expe- that the app has surpassed Facebook in popularity among
rience with bullying increased. Most (65%) say they have adolescent users suggests an urgency to school-based pre-
witnessed some form of bullying. Witnessing was not vention education initiatives.
related to optimistic bias. Participants reported a range A number of limitations should be considered before
of experience as victims of bullying: verbal (46%), physi- interpreting these results. Participants were all Pennsyl-
cal (25%), and cyberbullying (23%). As predicted, Table 1 vania students participating in anti-violence programs
shows a significant inverse relationship between optimistic offered by a women’s center. Although all study mea-
bias regarding victimization and experience with bully- sures were drawn prior to the programs, students were
ing. Participants also reported a range of experience as aware of the topic area, so it is possible response sets
perpetrators of bullying: verbal (18%), physical (9%), and were skewed. Although multiple schools were included,
cyberbullying (7%). Table 2 shows past experience with Pennsylvania adolescents are not representative of adoles-
bullying as a relatively strong predictor of future bullying. cents across the USA. Findings may not be generalizable to
RQ1 explored the relationship between optimistic bias other populations. A number of students opted not to pro-
and victim blaming. More than half (56%) exhibited some vide information about their gender and race. While the
level of victim blaming, with 30% on the high end of the disclosed demographics were consistent with the demo-
scale (Many of the kids who are picked on bring it on them- graphics of the region, there’s no way to determine why
selves by the way they act.). Only 14% were at the low these students chose not to disclose. Some of the mea-
end of the scale, believing bullying is always unwarranted. sures should be revised for future research, including the
Tables 1 and 2 show a significant relationship between one-item measure of victim blaming.
optimistic bias and victim blaming. Adolescents who blame The current study shows relationships between risk
the victim say they are more likely to be bullies in the perception, experience, and cyberbullying. Future research
future. should delve deeper to establish how such mispercep-
RQ2 explored the relationship between optimistic bias tions are formed and why they persist, despite heavy social
and social media. Results suggest a shift in social media use media use. There is much to learn, and the stakes are high.
among adolescents. Texting was the most common, with Optimistic bias dissipates quickly in the face of negative
75% of participants saying they text and 44% saying it was consequences. The challenge is to get children and ado-
their favorite method of social networking. Instagram came lescents to take the precautions before the “bad thing”
in second, with 62% saying they use the site and 23% saying happens to them.
it was their favorite. Snapchat, which was introduced since
the previous studies were published, came in third with
51% saying they use the app and 19% saying it was their
favorite. Usage of the remaining social networking sites and References
applications was Twitter (27%), Facebook (22%), Pinterest
Armenti, N., & Babcock, J. (2016). Joint treatment for intimate partner
(17%), and Yikyak (1%). Social media use in the tables is violence: A systematic review and implications. Couple and Family
a sum of the number of sites/apps used. Findings suggest Psychology: Research and Practice, 5(2), 109–123.
the more social media sites adolescents use, the less they Barlett, C., & Fennel, M. (2016). Examining the relation
between parental ignorance and youths’ cyberbullying
believe they will be victims of cyberbullying.
perpetration. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1–14.
http://dx.doi.org.ezaccess.libraries.psu.edu/10.1037/ppm0000139
5. Discussion Bryant, J., Zucca, A., Brozek, I., Rock, V., & Bonevski, B. (2015). Sun protec-
tion attitudes and behaviors among first generation Australians with
darker skin types: Results from focus groups. Journal of Immigrant and
Despite heavy use of social media, adolescents believe Minority Health, 17, 248–254.
they are less likely than peers to be victims of cyberbully- Bullee, J., Montoya, L., Pieters, W., Junger, M., & Hartel, P. (2015). The
ing and less likely to bully others. While the self-reported persuasion and security awareness experiment: Reducing the stress
of social engineering attacks. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 11,
incidence rates of experience with bullying (bystanding, 97–115.
victimization, and perpetration) are likely under-reported, Bureau of Justice Statistics (BJS). (2015). Uniform crime report: Victim char-
most (65%) say they have witnessed some form of bullying; acteristics. U.S. Department of Justice office of Justice Programs.
Chapin, J., & Chirico, J. (2003). Why it won’t happen to me: How older ado-
nearly half (46%) say they been victimized in some form, lescents process personal risk assessments. Communication Annual,
and some (18%) acknowledge bullying others. The results 107, 44–59.
suggest a cycle of perceptions and behaviors: Victims of Chapin, J., Strimel, L., & Coleman, G. (2014). It won’t happen to me:
Addressing adolescents’ risk perception of dating violence. Interna-
cyberbullying bully others. Adolescents who believe they tional Journal of Violence and Schools, 14, 44–54.
are likely to be bullied acknowledge they are likely to con- Davis, K., & Koepke, L. (2016). Risk and protective factors associated with
tinue bullying others and are also likely to blame victims cyberbullying: Are relationships or rules more protective? Learning,
Media, and Technology, 41(4), 521–545.
for “bringing it on themselves.” More than half of the par-
Flanagan, S., Marvinney, R., & Zheng, Y. (2015). Influences on domestic
ticipants engage in victim blaming. The U.S. Department well water testing behavior in a Central Maine area with frequent
of Health and Human Services (2016) warns there is “no groundwater arsenic occurrence. Science of the Total Environment, 505,
1274–1281.
safe space” to escape cyberbullying, which can occur 24 h,
Greenberg, M., Dyen, S., & Elliott, S. (2013). The public’s preparedness:
seven days a week, 365 days a year. Adolescents need Self-reliance, flashbulb memories, and conservative values. American
to be aware of the dangers of social media use and the Journal of Public Health, 103(6), 85–91.
potential consequences of sharing sexual images of them- Heyes, C. (2016). Dead to the world: Rape, unconsciousness, and social
media. Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 41(2), 361–383.
selves electronically. Even Snapchat images, designed to Joshi, M., & Carter, W. (2013). Unrealistic optimism: East and west? Fron-
be temporary, can be screen-captured and shared. Findings tiers in Psychology, 4, 1–15.

Please cite this article in press as: Chapin, J., & Coleman, G. The cycle of cyberbullying: Some experience required. The
Social Science Journal (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2017.03.004
G Model
SOCSCI-1390; No. of Pages 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Chapin, G. Coleman / The Social Science Journal xxx (2017) xxx–xxx 5

Napoletano, A., Elgar, F., Saul, G., & Dirks, M. (2016). The view from the Trumbo, C., Luek, M., Marlatt, H., & Peek, L. (2011). The effect of proximity
bottom: Relative deprivation and bullying victimization in Canadian to Hurricanes Katrina and Rita on subsequent hurricane outlook and
adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31(2), 3443–3463. optimistic bias. Risk Analysis, 31(12), 1907–1918.
National Center for Education Statistics (2014). Student Untied, A., & Dulaney, C. (2015). College students’ perceived risk of sexual
reports of bullying and cyberbullying. Retrieved from victimization and the role of optimistic bias. Journal of Interpersonal
https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2013/2013329.pdf. Violence, 30(8), 1417–1431.
Raptou, E., Galanopoulos, K., Katrakilidis, C., & Mattas, K. (2012). Public U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2016). What is bully-
support toward tobacco control: Consumer responsiveness and policy ing? U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Retrieved from.
planning. American Journal of Health Behavior, 36(5), 666–680. http://www.stopbullying.gov/what-is-bullying/index.html
Rich, M., Utley, E., Janke, K., & Moldoveanu, M. (2010). “I’d rather be doing Weinstein, N. (1989). Perceptions of personal susceptibility to harm. In
something else:” Male resistance to rape prevention programs. The V. Mays, G. Albee, & F. Schneider (Eds.), Psychological approaches to
Journal of Men’s Studies, 18(3), 268–288. the primary prevention of acquired immune deficiency syndrome (pp.
Sargeant, J., Majowicz, S., Sheth, U., & Edge, V. (2010). Perceptions of risk 142–167). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
and optimistic bias for acute gastrointestinal illness: A population Weinstein, N. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal
survey. Zoonoses and Public Health, 57, 7–8. of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 460–806.
Singh, I., & Jha, A. (2013). Anxiety, optimism ad academic achievement Wolfers, M., de Zwart, O., & Kok, G. (2011). Adolescents in the Netherlands
among students of private medical and engineering colleges: A com- underestimate risk for sexually transmitted infections and deny the
parative study. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, need for sexually transmitted infection testing. AIDS Patient Care and
3(1), 222–233. STDs, 25(5), 311–319.
Sproesser, G., Klusmann, V., Schupp, H., & Renner, B. (2015). Comparative Zeeb, F., & Zeeb, H. (2013). Higher risk perception of HIV than chlamydia
optimism about healthy eating. Appetite, 90, 212–218. and HPV among secondary school students in two German cities. PLOS
Taylor, R. (2009). Slain and slandered: A content analysis of the portrayal One, 8(4), 1–8.
of the portrayal of femicide in crime news. Homicide Studies, 13(1),
21–49.

Please cite this article in press as: Chapin, J., & Coleman, G. The cycle of cyberbullying: Some experience required. The
Social Science Journal (2017), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2017.03.004

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen