Sie sind auf Seite 1von 17

J. Sleep Res.

(1992) 1, 63-19

Concepts and models of sleep regulation: an overview


A L E X A N D E R A . B O R B E L Y and P E T E R A C H E R M A N N
Institute of Pharmacology. University of Zurich, CH-8006 Zurich, Switzerland

Accepted in revised form 25 February 1992; received 20 January 1992

SUMMARY Various mathematical models have been proposed to account for circadian,
ultradian and homeostatic aspects of sleep regulation. Most circadian models assume
that multiple oscillators underlie the differences in period and entrainment pro-
perties of the sleep/wake cycle and other rhythms (e.g. body temperature).
Interactions of the oscillators have been postulated to account for multimodal
sleep/wake patterns. The ultradim models simulate the cyclic alternation of
nonREM sleep and REM sleep by assuming a reciprocal interaction of two cell
groups. The homeostatic models propose that a sleep/wake dependent process
(Process S ) underlies the rise in sleep pressure during waking and its decay during
sleep. The time course of this process has been derived from EEG slow-wave
activity, an indicator of nonREM sleep intensity. The predictions of homeostatic
models have been most extensively tested in experiments. The interaction of Process
S with a single circadian process can account for multimodal sleep/wake patterns,
internal desynchronization and the time course of daytime sleepiness. Close links
have emerged between the processes postulated by the various models and specific
brain mechanisms. Due to its recent quantitative elaboration and experimental
validation, the modelling approach has become one of the potent research strategies
in sleep science.
KEYWORDS circadian, homeostatic, mathematical model, nap, ultradian

rhythms such as changing phase-relations between the


1 OVERVIEW OF T H E M O D E L S
sleep/wake cycle and the temperature rhythm, and the
Three basic processes underlie sleep regulation: (1) A splitting of the rest-activity rhythm into two components;
homeostatic process mediating the rise in ‘sleep pressure’ and (2) to account mainly for the effects of light, the major
during waking and the dissipation of ‘sleep pressure’ during zeitgeber, on the phase, amplitude and period of circadian
sleep; (2) a circadian process, a clock-like mechanism rhythms. The database of the models consists in general of
defining the alternation of periods with high and low sleep records of rest and activity rather than of polygraphically
propensity and being basically independent of prior sleep recorded sleep and waking. Moreover, the models attempt
and waking; and (3) an ultradian process occurring within to describe general features of the circadian system rather
sleep and represented by the alternation of the two basic than focus specifically on the sleep/wake rhythm. Neverthe-
sleep states nonREM sleep and R E M sleep. The three less, a distinction can be made between models that address
processes are illustrated schematically in Fig. 1. more specifically issues that are relevant for sleep
The tables provide a synopsis of models that have been regulation, and which are being continuously elaborated
proposed to account for circadian, ultradian and homeos- (Table l), and older ones which cover more selective or
tatic aspects of sleep regulation. The mathematical marginal aspects of sleep (some entries of Table 4). The
equations of the major models are compiled in the monographs by Enright (1980) and Strogatz (1986) on
appendix. models of the sleep/wake cycle should be particularly
mentioned not only because they cover the area in
considerable detail but also in view of the many innovative
1.1 Models of circadian rhythms (Table 1)
propositions and analyses they contain.
The main objectives of these models are (1) to simulate A common feature of models of circadian rhythms is that
spontaneously occurring phenomena in free-running they consist of more than one oscillator. The influence of
Correspondence: Professor A. Borbkly, Institute of Pharmacology, these multiple oscillators is little apparent under entrained
University of Zurich, Gloriastrasse 32, CH-8006 Zurich, conditions but may become manifest during free-run. They
Switzerland account for splitting and other features of the rest-activity

63
64 A . A . Borbkly and P . Achermann

necessary to account for the splitting phenomenon in


animals.
The circadian models summarized in Table 1 face two
major problems: (1) There is no compelling evidence for the
existence of more than one circadian pacemaker in
mammals*. (2) The homeostatic aspect of sleep regulation is
not addressed by circadian models (Nakao et a / . 1990a and
1991, make some provisions for such a process), thus
Oi00 23:OO 37.00 requiring additional assumptions.

I Circadian
1.2 Models of the nonREM-REM Sleep cycle (Table 2)
A distinctive feature of this class of models is that they
evolved from neurophysiological data obtained in animals
(McCarley and Hobson 1975). Although the original
anatomical and physiological assumptions have been
somewhat modified (McCarley and Massaquoi 1992), the
postulate that the nonREM-REM sleep cycle is generated
07.00 23:OO 07:OO by the reciprocal interaction of two neuronal systems, has
been maintained. The original proposition o f a Lotka-
Volterra type of interaction was later transposed to humans,
Ulrradian and further elaborated into the limit cycle reciprocal
interaction model (McCarley and Massaquoi 1986; Massa-
quoi and McCarley 1990, 1992).
Qualitative simulations of the changes in the duration and
latency of R E M sleep under various experimental conditions
were performed. Moreover, the model was applied to
predict the ultradian phase-response to a cholinergic agent
I I
2300 07.00 and to forced awakenings. In its most recent extension
Time of day (Massaquoi and McCarley 1992), homeostatic processes
Figure 1. Schematic representation of the three major processes have been incorporated similar to those proposed by
underlying sleep regulation. W, waking; S, sleep; N, NonREM Achermann et af. (1990). Moreover, an arousal process has
sleep; R, REM sleep. The progressive decline of nonREM sleep been included which affects both slow-wave activity and the
intensity i s represented both in the top and bottom diagram (decline limit cycle oscillation.
of ultradian amplitude). The increase in the duration of successive
REM sleep episodes i s indicated.
1.3 Models of sleep homeostasis and its interaction with
circadian and ultradian processes (Table 3)
It has been recognized as early as 1937 that sleep intensity is
pattern in animals, and for the phase-dependence of sleep reflected by the predominance of slow waves in the sleep
duration and the biphasic sleep patterns in humans. EEG (Blake and Gerard 1937). The relationship between
However, the assumption of multiple self-sustaining slow-wave sleep and the duration of prior waking has been
oscillators is not mandatory for accounting for various documented by Webb and Agnew (1971) and placed into a
features of free-running rhythms. As pointed out by theoretical framework by Feinberg (1974). The term ‘sleep
Eastman (1984), the pattern of internal desynchronization homeostasis’ (Borbely 1980) was proposed to characterize
between the sleep/wake cycle and the temperature rhythm the sleep/wake dependent aspect of sleep regulation. The
can be simulated by periodic or occasional phase-shifts of two-process model, originally proposed to account for sleep
the former rhythm. Daan, Beersma and associates (Daan regulation in the rat (BorbCly 1980; 1982a), postulates that
and Beersma 1984; Daan et a / . 1984; Beersma et al. 1987) Process S rises during waking and declines during sleep, and
then demonstrated that the interaction of a relaxation interacts with a circadian Process C. The time course of the
oscillator (Process S) with a single circadian process can
account for various features of free-running rhythms such as * This statement requires qualification, since there i s strong
evidence for an independent circadian oscillator in the eye ( R e m i el
the pattern of internal desynchronization, ‘phase-trapping’, al. 1991). However. this is unlikely t o be rclcvant in the prcscnt
and circabidian cycles as well as polyphasic sleep. However, context. For a discussion of the possibility that separatc paccmakcrs
the assumption of subharmonics of components of a single reside in the left and right suprachiasmatic nucleus, see Beersma
circadian oscillator or of multiple oscillators seems to be and D a m (1992).
Concepts and models of sleep regulation 65

Table 1 Two-oscillator and three-oscillator models of circadian rhythms. Asterisk indicates that model equations are provided in the
appendix

Assumption Des cription Cornm en t


~~~~ ~ ~~~ ~ ~ ~~ ~ ~~~~~

E-M model* (Pittendrigh and Daan 1976; Daan and Berde 197X)
Two coupled oscillators constitute the cir- Model of circadian rest-activity rhythm of Neurobiological basis: Existence of two sepa-
cadian pacemaker. Only phase information is rodents. E-oscillator controlling initial ac- rate oscillators unconfirmed, but some evid-
represented in the model equations. tivity bout in nocturnal rodent; M- ence for bilateral oscillators (see Beersma
oscillator controlling terminal activity bout. and D a m 1992). Assumption of different
Oscillators exert mutual phase control. E phase-response curves not upheld by Tupaija
synchronized by dusk, M by dawn. experiments (Meijer et ul. 1990). Application
Differential effect of light. Accounts for of the model to pineal N A T rhythm (Illne-
(I)intcrdepcndcncc of free-running rova et ul. 19x9).
rhythm and activity time with changing
light intensity; (2) after-effects of prior
conditions on period and activity time; ( 3 )
splitting.
D u d circudiun pucemukrr model (Beersma and Daan 1992)
Two coupled oscillators constitute the cir- Two identical oscillators that arc usually in Neurobiological basis: see previous comment.
cadian pacemaker. Elaboration of the E-M phase. Response to light pulses is identical. Adaptation to photoperiod not accounted
model. Splitting due to change in phase relation. for.
Multiplicative interaction t o account for
activity pattern during splitting. The output
o f the oscillators determines the probability
o f activity.
Originul x - y model*. Predominant effect o//ight on v (Kronauer et ul., 19x2).
Two Van der Pol oscillators x and y affecting Phase and amplitude information reprc- Neurobiological basis: Existence of two sepa-
each other by 'velocity' type coupling. Lar- sented in the model equations but only rate oscillators unconfirmed. In original mo-
ger effect of x on y than y on x . phase information interpreted. 'Strong' (x) del, sleep coincides with central two-thirds of
circadian oscillator controlling R E M sleep. y's trough. Later sleep assumed to occur
core body temperature. cortisol; 'weak' (y) symmetrically about mid-low y with a phase-
circadian oscillator controlling the delay o f 30" (Gander ('1 d.1984a).
sleep/wakc cycle. Simulation of data
(Gander et ul. 1984b;Gander et a / . 198s):
(I)Phcnonicna occurring at transition from
internal synchronization to desynchroniza-
tion (e.g. changes in period; 'phase-
trapping'); (2) phase-dependence of sleep
duration; ( 3 ) types of synchronization by
zeitgcbcrs; (4) resynchronization after
jet lag.
Revised x-y mode/*. Predominant effect of light oti x (Kronaucr. 1990)
Van der Pol oscillator for x ; cumulative Simulation of the effect o f light pulses on The model addresses the difficulty of driving
effect of light. Circadian modulation of sciv phase and amplitude of circadian rhythms the human circadian system to the point of
sitivity to light. in humans. singularity (i.e. zero amplitude. undefined
phase).
Three-oscillutor model * (Kronauer , I9X7aj
Subdivision of y oscillator into identical y , Simulation of 'split-desynchrony' patterns Neurobiological basis of y , and y2
and yz oscillators. Derived from x-y model. o f human bimodal sleep pattern (main unspeci lied.
Only phase information represented in thc sleep. napping), and of splitting of rodent
model equation. rest-activity rhythm.

Two-o.scil1utorthermoregulutory model oJ.sleep conrrol* (Nakao et ul. l99Oa; 1991)


Two circadian oscillators, one influencing Simulation o f biphasic sleepiness pattcrn, Existence o f two separate circadian oscillators
temperature, the other sleepiness. Sleep h o - relation of sleep timing t o temperature not confirmed. Mechanism for the accumula-
meostasis assumed to be related to longterm rhythm. tion of ii heat load and its delayed effect on
temperature regulation (heat load during sleep n o t specified. Available data still
waking; heat dissipation during sleep). Adj- ambiguous.
ustable thresholds determining onset and
termination of sleep.
66 A . A . Borbdy and P. Achermann

Table 2 Models of the nonREM-REM sleep cycle. Asterisk indicates that model equations are provided in the appendix.
Assumption Description Comment

Reciprocal interaction model* (McCarley and


Hobson 1975; revised interpretation: Hobson
el al. 1986).
NonREM-REM sleep cycle generated by Simulation o f data: Dischargc ratc of chol- Neurobiological basis: The role o f postulated
two coupled cell populations in the brain- inergic FTG ( o r LDI/PPT)cells in cat. cell populations in the control of R E M sleep.
stem with self-excitatory and self-inhibitory Application o f the model with a modified and their interactions have undergone revi-
connections according to the Lotka-Volterra definition of REM sleep t o simulate sions (Hobson ef al. 1986: McCarley and
model. changes in REM sleep in depressive pati- M.‘lssdquoi
., 1992).
ents (Beersma et 01. 1984)
Limit cycle reciprocal interaction model:
Original iiersion * (McCarley and Massaquoi
1986; Massaquoi and McCarley 1990)
NonREM-REM sleep cycle generated by Main fcaturcs o f previous model main- Neurobiological basis: see previous comment.
the reciprocal interaction of two coupled cell tained. but assumption of a stahlc limit
populations. cycle oscillation that is independent o f
initial conditions. Introduction o f a cir-
cadian term which determines modc of
approach to limit cycle. Simulation of data:
(1) REMS latency and REMS episode
durations in normal humans under various
experimental conditions and in dcpressivcs;
(2) prediction of an ultradian phase-
response curve to a cholinergic agent; (3)
effect of forced waking on the dynamics of
nonREM/REM sleep cyclc hy an cx-
ogenous excitatory input.
Limit cycle reciprocal interaction model:
Extended uersions (Massaquoi and McCarley
1992)
NonREM-REM sleep cycle generated by Assumption of first-order decay dynamics Neurobiological basis: See McCarley and
the reciprocal interaction o f two coupled cell for the i i r ~ u s a lsystem. Oualitativc simula- Massaquoi (1992).
populations. Incorporation of sleep home- tion of ultradian slow-wave activity
ostasis and arousal events. pattern.

homeostatic variable S was derived from E E G slow-wave Enright (1980) showed that a circadian pacemaker with a
activity. Various aspects of human sleep regulation were high precision can be constituted from an ensemble of
addressed in a qualitative version of the two-process model ‘sloppy’ relaxation oscillators. Wever (1984, 1985, 1987)
(Borbdy 1982b). Independently, an elaborated, quantitative assumed the existence of several distinct yet interacting
version of the model was established (Daan and Beersma oscillators. The rest-activity pattern was obtained by
1984; Daan et al. 1984) which predicted the timing of human applying a threshold. Three separate oscillators were
sleep for a variety of different schedules. A further proposed by Kawato et af. (1982), two of which were
elaboration of the model was accomplished by Achermann assumed to determine sleep onset and sleep termination. In
and colleagues (Achermann 1988; Achermann and BorbCly addition, Winfree (1983) simulated the distribution o f sleep
1990; Achermann et ul. 1990) by including the ultradian onset and sleep termination by analysing the interaction of a
variations of slow-wave activity. Recently, Folkard and recovery process and an oscillator, an approach that uses
Akerstedt (1987, 1989, 1992: Akerstedt and Folkard 1990) similar basic assumptions as in the two-process model.
simulated the changes of daytime alertness by the combined Strogatz (1986) accounted for the internal desynchronization
action of a homeostatic process, a circadian process and ;I of the sleep/wake rhythm and body temperature by simple
sleep inertia process. It is interesting that the time constants two-oscillator models. The gated pacemaker model (Car-
of their homeostatic process that have been derived from penter and Grossberg 1984, 1987) assumes an interaction of
sleepiness ratings, and the time constants of Process S two cell populations in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN)
derived from the sleep E E G , are in a similar range. as well as some other specific physiological processes to
simulate various properties of circadian rhythms. The
current views on the physiological processes in the SCN are
1.4 Miscellaneous models (Table 4) not all consistent with the assumptions of this complex
model. Lawder (1984) proposed that the interaction o f a
The models summarized under this heading address either linearly declining process and a periodic REM sleep process
general aspects of rhythms that are not directly related t o could account for the sleep stage distribution as well as for
sleep regulation or very specific points of sleep regulation. other features of sleep. His qualitative model does not
Concepts and models of sleep regulation 67

Table 3 Models of sleep homeostasis and its interaction with circadian and ultradian proccsses. Asterisk indicates that model equations are
provided in the appendix.

Assumption Description Comrneni

Two-process model* (BorbCly, 1982b; Daan


and Beersma, 1984; Daan ei a/. 1984;
BorbCly et al. 1989)
Sleep propensity in humans is determined by Time course of S derived from E E G slow- Ncurobiological basis of ’slecp homeostat’
a homeostatic Process S and circadian Proc- wave activity; phase position and shape unknown. Possible basis of E E G slow-wave
ess C. The interaction of S and C determines (skewed sine wave) of‘ C derived from activity: Burst discharge pattern in the delta
the timing of sleep and waking. sleep duration data obtained at various rangc of hyperpolarized thalamic neurons
times of the 24-h cycle. Simulation of data: (Steriade et ul. 1991).
( I ) Timing o f sleep after slecp deprivation.
during prolongcd bcdrcst. internal dc-
synchronization during frcc run; variations
of sleep onset and sleep termination; (2)
level and time course of slow-wave activity
in sleep after total o r repeated partial sleep
deprivation, after daytime naps; ( 3 ) varia-
tions of slcep latency.
Model of ultradian variaiion of slow-wave
activity* (Achermann, 1988; Achermann and
Borbtly, 1990; Achermann el al. 1990)
Derived from the two-process model. The In contrast to the original two-process Possible ncurobiological basis of intraepisodic
level of S determines the buildup rate and model, the change of S. n o t the IevcI of S, changcs of slow-wave activity: Progressive
the saturation level of slow-wave activity corresponds to slow-wavc activity. A KEM hyperpolarization of thalamic neurons (see
within nonREM sleep episodes. sleep oscillator triggers the decline of slow- previous comment). Evidence f o r a REM
wave activity prior t o REM sleep. Simula- sleep oscillator in the pons (see McCarley and
tion of data: ( 1 ) Time course o f slow-wave Massaquoi 1992).
activity during normal sleep. after slecp
deprivation, after daytime nap. aftcr selcc-
tive slow wave deprivation. during ex-
tended slecp; (2) increasing duration of
R E M sleep episodcs, slcep onset REM
sleep episode. skipped lirst REM slecp
episode; ( 3 ) parameter estimation and si-
mulation of indcpendcnt datasets.
Three-processmodel of the regulaiion of
sleepiness/alertness* (Folkard and Akerstcdt
1987, 1989, 1992; Akerstedt and Folkard
1990)
Sleepiness/alertness are simulated by the Parameters dcrived from rated slcepincss Time constant of the homeostatic process
combined action of a homeostatic process. a during sleep/wake manipuhtions. Simula- similar t o Process S in the two-process model
circadian Process, and sleep inertia (Process tion of data: Changes o f (Daan ei ul. 1984). However. the homeostatic
W). sleepiness/alcrtness during shift-work process and S change in oppositc directions.
schedules.

establish clear relations to physiological processes. Stoch- the simulations, the x and y rhythms are initially
astic models were formulated to account for sleep synchronized. Then as the period of y lengthens an internal
architecture in humans (Kemp and Kamphuisen 19x6) and phase drift sets in. Finally, there is phase trapping that
for circadian rest-activity rhythms (Dirlich 1984). A model signals the transition from synchrony to desynchrony.
of EEG slow wave generation has been recently published Predictions were generated on the effects of zeitgebers of
(Caekebeke et a/. 1991). Finally, Nakao et a / . (1990b) different strengths and periods (Gander ef a/. 1Y84a).
attempted to account for the dynamics of neuronal activity Wever’s data were used to simulate various patterns of
in specific sleep states in the cat by a neural network model. partial and full entrainment which had been observed in
subjects exposed to artificial zeitgebers (Gander el a / .
2 EXPERIMENTAL VALIDATION OF l984b).
M O D E L S IN H U M A N S In a further study, features of resynchronization after time
zone shifts were simulated and compared to the data of four
2.1 r-y model
subjects (Gander et a/. 1985). The rate of adjustment was
In the first reports typical sleep/wake and body temperature shown to depend o n the rhythm being measured. the extent
patterns under conditions of temporal isolation were of the time zone shift, the flight direction, and the strength
simulated (Kronauer et a/. 1982, fig. 1 ; Kronauer 1984). I n of the zcitgebers in the new time zone. Interindividual
68 A . A . Borbkly and P. Achermann

Table 4 Miscellaneous models.

Designation Description /comment


~
~ ~ ~~~ ~~ ~~ ~~~ ~ ~~
~~

Multiple relaxation oscillator model (Enright 1980) Circadian pacemaker assumed to consist o f an ensemble of relaxa-
tion oscillators whose output feeds into an integrating element.
Simulation of effect of light pulses on phase, and light intensity on
period of circadian rest-activity rhythm. After-effects accounted for
by the model, but not splitting.

Multioscillator model of circadian rhythms (Wever 1984. 1985, 1987) Coupled oscillators; dichotomy of rest and activity by applying a
threshold; simulation of an ultradian slow-wave activity pattern by
assuming an ultradian oscillator.

Three-oscillator model (Kawato et al. 1982) Separate oscillators controlling sleep onset, sleep termination and
body temperature; simulation o f internal desynchronization.
Interaction of recovery process with an oscillator (Win free 1983) Analysis of 'forbidden zones' of awakening.

Beats model and phase model of circadian rhythms (Strogatz 1986) Superposition (beats) o r coupling (phase) of two oscillators; simula-
tion of internal desynchronization.
Gated pacemaker model (Carpenter and Grossberg 1984. 1987) Assumption of interaction of on-cell and off-cell population in SCN
whose feedback signals are gated by slowly accumulating transmit-
ters; on-cells supposed to promote motor activity; fatigue factor
acting on off-cells; simulation of phase-response curves, light effects
in diurnal and nocturnal animals, AschoFf's rule. splitting, etc.

Model of sleep patterning (Lawder 1984) Linearly declining variable interacts with periodic REM sleep
process. Simulation of sleep stage distribution in normal nights and
after sleep deprivation; sleep onset R E M sleep episodes.

Stochastic model of sleep architecture (Kemp and Kamphuisen 1986) Model based o n sleep stage transition probabilities.

Stochastic models of circadian rest-activity cycle (Dirlich 1984) Models based on state transition probabilities o r a network of
random processes.

Model of EEG slow wave generation (Caekebeke et a / . 1991; Da Frequency selective feedback circuit with variable coupling strength.
Rosa et a / . 1991) Simulation of phasic (K-complexes) and tonic slow waves, and sigma
activity.

Neural network model of neuronal transition dynamics during d e e p Simulation of neuronal activity of the white noise type in nonREM
(Nakao et al. 1990b) sleep and l / f type in REM sleep.

differences in pacemaker periods were reported to be an depression (bimodal distribution). They showed that sleep
important factor for explaining the observed variability in abnormalities in depressive patients can be accounted for by
the response to time zone shifts. the decrease in the initial value of a single variable which
Originally it had been assumed that bright light acts on can be taken to represent the extent of REM sleep
the circadian system via the y oscillator. Experiments with inhibition. McCarley and Massaquoi (1986) showed with the
scheduled light exposure led to a modified model in which limit cycle reciprocal interaction model that by varying one
light has its main effect directly on x . In a first version, it of the initial values, a bimodal distribution of REM sleep
was assumed that the change of brightness (dB/dt) and not latency could be obtained.
brightness itself affected x (Kronauer 1987b; Kronauer and Forced awakenings in 4 subjects at various phases of their
Frangioni 1987). Experiments showing that it is difficult to nonREM-REM sleep cycle had phase-dependent effects on
completely suppress the amplitude of the core body cycle duration (Foret et al. 1990). These data were used to
temperature rhythm by scheduled light exposures (Jewett et assess the effect of the excitatory input E o n the cycle length
al. 1991), led to a modified version of the model in which a in the model (Massaquoi and McCarley 1990). The results
cumulative effect of light on x and its complementary are interpreted in terms of 'weak' and 'strong' resettings,
variable ( x c . ) is assumed, and a circadian variation of and i t is concluded that a mixed type of resetting mode
sensitivity to light is incorporated (Kronauer 1990). prevails.

2.2 Reciprocal interaction model 2.3 Two-process model


The basic assumptions of the model, and in particular of its
Beersma el al. (1984) used the concepts of the model to homeostatic facet, has been subjected to extensive
account for the altered distribution of R E M sleep latency in experimental verification.
Concepts und models of sleep regulution 69

2.3.1 Experiments testing the original version of the sleep (sleep duration 4 h) and two or three recovery nights,
model respectively (Brunner et al. 1990b, in preparation). The
repeated curtailment o f sleep duration induced only a minor
Independence of S and C. One of the basic assumptions of rise in slow-wave activity whose magnitude did not differ
the model is the independence of the homeostatic process S significantly from the prediction of the model.
and the circadian process C. It is supported by results from
2.3.2 Experiments testing un elaborated version of the
animal experiments showing that the homeostatic response
model allowing f o r disturbed sleep
to sleep deprivation persists even after the circadian rhyth-
micity has been disrupted or abolished by lesioning the These experiments test the modified version of the model
suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) (Mistlberger et ul. 1983; (proposed by Beersma et al. 1987 and Dijk et al. 1987b. and
Tobler et al. 1983). Evidence that one of the two processes formalized by Achermann and BorbCly 1990) in which the
can be independently manipulated, has been obtained in a change of S, and not its level, is proportional to the
study in which the circadian phase of Process C (as indexed momentary slow-wave activity. The latter model addresses
by body temperature and plasma melatonin) was shifted by not only the global changes of slow-wave activity as
bright light in the morning, while the time course o f represented by Process S, but also the changes within
slow-wave activity remained unaffected (Dijk et al. 1987c, nonREM sleep episodes.
1989).
Intranight rebound of slow-wave activity. When slow-wave
Rise of Process S during waking. In the first, qualitative sleep was prevented by acoustic stimulation during the first
version of the model. it was inferred from the initial and 3 h of sleep, the magnitude and time course of the sub-
terminal level of S in normal sleep and in recovery sleep sequent rebound of slow-wave activity was in accordance
after sleep deprivation, that S rises monotonically during with the modified concept of the two-process model (Dijk et
waking according to a saturating exponential function al. 1987b). Recently, a rebound of slow-wave activity was
(BorbCly 1982b). The time constant of this fuiiction was documented after a 5-h interval of selective slow-wave sleep
then estimated to be 18.2 h (Daan et ul. 1984). By determin- deprivation in the same nighttime sleep episode, and after a
ing slow-wave activity in daytime naps which were preceded 3-h interval of selective slow-wave sleep deprivation during
by different durations of waking, direct evidence for a daytime recovery sleep (Dijk and Beersma 1989). In
monotonic rise of S according to a saturating exponential another study daytime sleep episodes with and without
function was obtained (Beersma et al. 1087; Dijk et (11. slow-wave sleep deprivation were compared (Gillberg et al.
1987a). The time constant of the function fitted through the 1991). The experimental suppression of slow-wave sleep
extrapolated initial points of S in the naps was 18.7 h during an interval corresponding to 90% of the undisturbed
(Beersma et ul. 1987). In another study, slow-wave sleep episode resulted in an increased accumulation of slow-wave
and slow-wave activity was shown to be higher in afternoon sleep and an extension of sleep duration.
naps than in morning naps, which is in accordance with the
predictions of the model (Knowles et al. 1990a,b). Further Extended sleep ut different circadian phuses. To test the time
confirmation for the monotonic rise of S during waking was course of Process S during extended sleep, long sleep
obtained in an analysis of sleep studies in which the duration episodes starting at the regular bedtime (00.00 hours; 17 h
of prior waking differed (Dijk et al. 1990b). Again a close prior waking; Dijk et ul. 1991b), at a phase-advanced
correspondence between the data points and a saturating bedtime (19.00 hours, 12 h prior waking: Trachsel et al.
exponential function with a time constant of 18.2 h was 1900; 3 6 h prior waking: Dijk et al. 1990a) and at 21 8-h
obtained. phase-delayed bedtime (07.00 hours, 24 h prior waking:
Dijk et al. 1991a) were studied. O n average, slow-wave
Effect of a nap on subsequent sleep. The two-process model
activity showed a decline over the first 3-4 nonREM-REM
predicts that due to the decline of S during daytime naps,
sleep cycles and then remained on a constant level. Occasi-
the level of slow-wave activity in the subsequent slecp
onal minor late peaks of slow-wave activity were observed
episode will be reduced. This prediction was confirmed
which can be accounted for by the model (Achermann el al.
(Feinberg et al. 1985; Daan et al. 1988; Knowles et ul.
in prep.).
1990b).
2.3.3 Parameter estimation of the model crnd tests on
Effect of shortened nighttime sleep on a subsequent duytitne independent experimentul dutusets
nap. When the duration of nighttime sleep was shortened,
A further elaborated version of thr model was subjected to
slow-wave activity in a subsequent morning nap was en-
an optimization procedure based on the weighted
hanced (Akerstedt and Gillberg 1986; Gillberg and
least-square error method. and using the mean time course
Akerstedt 1991).
of empirical slow-wave activity from a large dataset (16
Effect of repeated partial sleep depriuation. Slow-wave ac- subjects, 26 nights) as a template (Achermann er d . in
tivity was measured during two or four nights o f shortened prep.). A sensitivity analysis showed the system to be
70 A . A . Borbdy and P. Achermann

rather robust to moderate variations ( f 5 % ) of the before a satisfactory interpretation can be offered.
parameter values. To further test the performance of the
Sleep inertia and wake inertia. Sleep inertia following
model, the estimated parameter values were used to
awakening is reflected by the short sleep latency when
simulate data from three different experimental protocols
subjects are allowed to return to sleep. A n attempt was
(prolonged waking followed either by sleep in the evening
made (BorbCly et al. 1989) to incorporate a decaying sleep
or by sleep in the morning; and extended sleep initiated at
inertia process which had been proposed by Folkard and
the habitual time). Empirical R E M sleep data were used to
Akerstedt (1987, 1989).
activate the REM trigger parameter in the model. In
The possibility that a wake-inertia process may influence
general, a close fit was obtained between the simulated and
the initial part of sleep, was suggested by the declining trend
empirical slow-wave activity data and their time course. In
of the tonic EMG level throughout the first nonREM sleep
particular, the occurrence of late slow-wave activity peaks
episode (Brunner er al. 1990a). A gradual dissipation of
during extended sleep could be simulated. Minor dis-
wake-inertia may impede the initial buildup of slow-wave
crepancies in later cycles of sleep initiated in the evening or
activity and thereby shift its maximum from the first t o the
in the morning could be due t o indirect or direct circadian
second cycle. Such a slow-wave activity pattern occurs
influences on slow-wave activity. The simulations dem-
occasionally in normal subjects (Feinberg et al. 1985) and
onstrate that the model can account in quantitative terms for
has been reported for depressed patients (Mendelson et al.
empirical data and predict the changes induced by the
1987).
prolongation of waking or sleep.
In the two-process model, wake-inertia could be
2.3.4 Emerging discrepuncies and shortcomings ; simulated by raising the level of the lower threshold. This
suggestions for eluborution of the model would allow to account for the brief duration of daytime
naps.
Independence of S and C. One of the basic assumptions of
the model is the independence of its two constituent Multiple sleep latency. The multiple sleep latency test is a
processes. However, a possible feedback of S on C has been method for quantifying the propensity of sleep initiation.
noted (Daan et al. 1984) as the scheduling of sleep and Usually, the test is applied to subjects who are fully aware
waking may alter the timing of light exposure and thereby of the time of day and who adhere at least partially to their
affect the phase of C. This effect has been assumed to habitual daytime activities (e.g. the timing of meals). Under
account for the phenomenon of phase-trapping (Beersma et these circumstances, sleep latency is not a monotonic
al. 1987). A further interaction of S and C may arise from function of the duration of prior waking as is the case for S.
the phase-shift of the circadian pacemaker by daytime motor Apart from the frequently occurring midday dip (Carskadon
activity. Such an effect has been demonstrated for animals 1989) the values remain at a fairly uniform level throughout
(Mrosovsky and Salmon 1987) but not yet for humans. the habitual waking period. If waking is prolonged, sleep
The incorporation of disturbing factors has allowed also latency declines rapidly. Qualitative simulations have been
for an indirect effect of C on S. Thus when sleep is initiated performed with the two-process model by assuming that
in the early morning, the circadian R E M sleep propensity is sleep latency is represented by the difference between the
high. This may impede the full manifestation of slow-wave upper threshold modulated by C , and Process S (BorbCly et
activity in the first nonREM-REM sleep cycle and thus alter al. 1989). By an appropriate skewing of the sinusoidal
its time course (Dijk et al. 1991a; Lance1 and Kerkhof 1991). function representing the threshold, the typical features of
sleep latency including the midday dip could be qualitatively
Upper and lower threshold of the ‘somnostat’. In the model simulated. However, these simulations are based on ar-
the level of the upper threshold was assumed to be bitrary adjustments of the model parameters and various
influenced during waking by external stimuli that promote features are rather sensitive to minor changes. Although the
or reduce arousal (Daan et al. 1984). No analogous provi- basic assumption that the propensity of sleep initiation
sions were made for sleep. This is a shortcoming, since depends on homeostatic and circadian processes is reason-
prolonged sleep can be simulated only if sleep itself lowers able, a stringent comparison with a larger database is
the level of the lower threshold. Moreover, the brevity of necessary.
brief waking bouts at night suggests that also the upper Whereas some authors emphasize the importance of the
threshold is lowered during sleep. An experiment was midday dip (e.g. Broughton and Mullington 1992), it should
designed to test the predicted prolongation of sleep duration be noted that the phenomenon has not been invariably
when slow-wave sleep was suppressed in the first part of the recorded, that its timing varies considerably, and that the
night (Dijk and Beersma 1989). The manipulation did not extent of the change is much smaller than the circadian
prolong sleep. However, in another study in which slow- variation. In particular, it has not yet been demonstrated
wave sleep was suppressed selectively during 90% of a unambiguously that the midday dip is due to an endogenous
daytime sleep episode, a prolongation of sleep ensued process (see below). Only if its endogenous nature is
(Gillberg er al. 1991). The divergent results of these studies confirmed will it be necessary to investigate whether it is a
indicate that a more detailed analysis of the interference result of the interaction of S and C as postulated in the
with sleep is necessary (e.g. by recording brief arousals) model, or of a bimodal variation in the upper threshold.
Concepts and models of sleep regulation 71

Change of Process S during REM sleep. The direction of the pressure’ induced by partial sleep deprivation seemed to
change of S during REM sleep episodes is still an open suppress the typical low-delta peak in the nonREM sleep
question. It could be argued that S declines since REM spectrum (Brunner et al. 1990b).
sleep is a part of the sleep process. However, this argument Before R E M sleep homeostasis and its interaction with
is not compatible with the recent version of the model in nonREM sleep homeostasis can be incorporated into a
which the change of S is determined by slow-wave activity. model, it will be important to determine whether the
Since there are practically no slow waves in R E M sleep, S inhibitory influence of R E M sleep on the nonREM sleep
should remain unchanged. However, if the absence of slow spectrum represents merely a suppression of the E E G
waves is considered to be equivalent to waking, an increase manifestation of Process S, or if S itself is affected.
of S would be expected. The latter assumption was in-
corporated in the most recent version of the model in which Generation of the nonREM-REM sleep cycle. In the original
the rising component of S is permanently active and version of the model, the generation of the nonREM-REM
interacts additively with the declining component of S which sleep cycle was not incorporated. In a recent version, an
is a function of slow-wave activity (Achermann er al. 1990 in oscillating variable interacts with a R E M sleep trigger
preparation). Due to the limited duration of R E M sleep threshold (Achermann et al. 1990). R E M sleep occurs
episodes, it is difficult to devise experiments for testing the whenever slow-wave activity falls below a threshold. Quant-
different hypotheses. itative simulations have not yet been performed. There is a
convergence between the elaborated version of the two-
REM sleep homeostasis. In the first version of the model, process model and the limit cycle reciprocal interaction
REM sleep propensity was assumed to be predominantly model: the ultradian oscillating variable of the former model
determined by Process C (Borbely, 1982b). In addition, a can be substituted by the limit cycle variable of the latter
reciprocally inhibitory interaction of the R E M sleep con- model (Achermann and BorbCly 1992), and homeostatic
trolling factor and Process S was postulated. The homeos- processes can be incorporated in the limit cycle interaction
tatic regulation of REM sleep was recognized but not model (Massaquoi and McCarley 1992).
incorporated in the model. In the subsequent quantitative
version, REM sleep was disregarded (Daan et al. 1984). Is Process S affected by the intensity of waking? Whereas
One of the difficulties in modelling REM sleep regulation is nonREM sleep intensity as indexed by slow-wave activity
the absence of an intensity measure in humans. A rise in determines the time course of Process S during sleep, its rise
‘REM sleep pressure’ manifests itself only in the increased during waking is assumed to occur invariably according to a
duration of REM sleep. saturating exponential function. This means that S depends
During recovery from total sleep deprivation, slow-wave exclusively on the duration of waking, and that the intensity
sleep and E E G slow-wave activity exhibit an immediate of waking is irrelevant.
rebound whereas the increase in REM sleep is delayed to There is some evidence to the contrary. Thus an increase
subsequent nights or is not present at all (references in in slow-wave sleep was observed after a hyperthermic
BorbCly 1982b). These results suggested that the intensity of episode several hours prior t o sleep as well as after a day of
nonREM sleep is much more finely regulated than R E M exposure to intense and varied environmental stimuli (see
sleep. Recent findings contradict this notion. A REM sleep Horne 1988, 1991, 1992 for references). Horne (1992)
deprivation in the first 5 h of sleep induced a REM sleep argues that the common effect of these procedures is the
rebound in the subsequent 2.25 h (Beersma et al. 1990). A increase in brain metabolism. The notion that sleep is
curtailment of sleep duration in 2 nights which induced a promoted by a cumulative effect of heat load has been
substantial REM sleep deficit, was followed by a REM sleep incorporated in the model of Nakao et al. (1990a, 1991).
rebound in the two recovery nights (Brunner et al. 1990b). Further experiments are needed to test this proposition.
In both experiments, the REM sleep rebound occurred at a
time when slow-wave pressure was either low at the end of
sleep (Beersma et al. 1990) or was much less increased than 3 C O N T R O V E R S I A L ISSUES A N D O P E N
‘REM sleep pressure’ (Brunner et a / . 1990b). These results PROBLEMS
suggest also that REM sleep is finely regulated but that the
manifestation of REM sleep homeostasis is hampered by an 3.1 Two-per-day rhythm of sleep propensity: endogenous
elevated slow-wave pressure. or exogenous?
In accordance with the notion of a mutual inhibitory
interaction of the factors controlling slow-wave activity and Broughton ( 1975) proposed an endogenous two-per-day
REM sleep (BorbCly 1982b), not only R E M sleep is rhythm to account for the biphasic time course of vigilance,
inhibited by ‘slow-wave pressure’, but also slow-wave showing reductions in the afternoon and evening. He
activity by ‘REM sleep pressure’. Thus there was a proposed that this circasemidian sleep propensity rhythm
significant reduction in the low-frequency activity of the might be an integral harmonic component of the circadian
nonREM sleep E E G during selective REM sleep depriva- rhythm, and may constitute a fundamental rhythm of
tion (Beersma et al. 1990). Also the rise in ‘REM sleep wakefulness rather than of sleep (Broughton 1989). The
72 A . A . Borbkly and P. Achermanri

proposition was related by the author to the two circadian was proposed that the x oscillator must have an important
activity peaks in animals. second harmonic influence on the y oscillators. Recently
Whereas there is ample evidence for a bimodal Kronauer and Jewett (1992) described both a circadian and
distribution of sleep propensity in subjects living in the usual a hemicircadian* body temperature rhythm, and suggested
24-h world, it is less clear that this pattern is generated by an that the latter may underlie the bimodal pattern of sleep
endogenous rhythm. A polymodal distribution of sleep is propensity. Evidence is presented for independent changes
typical in early childhood. As development proceeds and of the two components under various experimental
both waking and sleep episodes increasingly consolidate, the schedules. It is important to realize, however, that the
polymodal pattern becomes bimodal and eventually conclusions are derived from a mathematical analysis in
monomodal. A rise in sleep propensity near the middle of which harmonic sine waves are fitted to the temperature
the waking episode may represent a remnant of the early data. It has yet to be shown that the hemicircadian
polyphasic sleep pattern. Midday sleep propensity may be component identified by this procedure corresponds to a
enhanced by the relaxation following lunch (the post-lunch biological oscillation.
dip), social habits (siesta), and high environmental
temperature. These factors may be responsible for the
3.2 Multiple napping
considerable variability in the occurrence and timing of this
phenomenon. The bimodal sleep propensity observed in Campbell and Zulley (1985, 1989) pointed out that under
free-running schedules (Strogatz 1986, p. 92) could be prolonged bedrest conditions multiple naps per day can be
equally attributed to the subjects’ tendency of structuring observed. A four-per-day rhythm of sleep propensity has
their days and maintaining a 3-meal-per-day pattern even been described (Zulley 1988; Broughton et ul. 1990). A
under these conditions. Recent results cast doubt on the multiphasic sleep pattern is typical for the human infant
generality of the bimodal pattern. Thus the analysis of (Kleitman and Engelmann 1953). A prominent ultradian
alertness and performance under a forced desynchronization sleep/wake pattern has been reported also for the rat during
schedule did not reveal the presence of a midday dip when early development (Alfoldi et ul. 1990). In terms of sleep
the influence of prior wakefulness and sleep was assessed by structure (i.e. slow-wave activity), each ultradian
folding the data at the 2X-h period of the subjects’ sleep/wake episode showed the characteristics of a
sleep/wake schedule (Dijk e t a / . 1992). ‘mini-day’.
The constant routine protocol constitutes a stringent test A straightforward explanation of the multiple napping
for the endogenous origin of the midday rise of sleepiness. pattern is the inability to sustain either sleep or waking for
In this schedule, the body posture is kept constant, food and prolonged time periods under conditions of extreme
liquid intake occur at short regular intervals, lighting is monotony (e.g. prolonged bedrest) or in early development.
uniform and information on the time of day is withheld. An episodic rather than a rhythmic occurrence of sleep
There was no clear evidence for a biphasic pattern of seems to be typical under such conditions. Lowering the
alertness and performance in young male subjects recorded upper threshold in the two-process model could simulate a
under constant routine conditions (Dijk et al. 1992). multimodal sleep/wake pattern (Daan et al. 1984;
Carskadon and Dement (19x5) reported in an abstract a Achermann 1988).
midday reduction in multiple sleep latency from a test which
was performed at 2-h intervals from morning to evening. It
3.3 Wake maintenance zones
is unclear from the report whether factors such as the
knowledge of the time of day or the anticipation of the end The term ‘wake maintenance zones’ was coined by Strogatz
of the experiment in the evening may have influenced the and Kronauer (1985) and designates the zones in the
results. Since the negative findings of the other groups were circadian cycle (defined by body temperature) in which sleep
based on subjective sleepiness measures, it will be important tends not to begin. Strogatz (1986, Fig. 4.11) described the
to use objective measures of sleep propensity to resolve this position of these zones in internally desynchronized subjects
important issue. at about 8 h before, and about 6 h after the temperature
With the two-process model a polymodal sleep/wake minimum. The two zones thus precede the midday nap
pattern can be simulated by narrowing the interval between phase and the major sleep phase. The presence of the two
the two thresholds (Daan et ul. 1984). Since the level of the zones of low sleep propensity or of the evening zone alone
upper threshold is assumed to be influenced by the arousal has been inferred by Strogatz from the reanalysis of
level, its reduction in sleep-promoting situations (e.g. heat, ultradian sleep studies, the occurrence of unintended
monotony under continuous bedrest; in some subjects with microsleep during a constant routine schedule, the record of
free-running rhythms, see Strogatz 1986, p. 82) is to be a subject entrained to a 23.5-h schedule, and of another
expected. Also the bimodal ‘siesta pattern’ can be obtained subject free running in society (Wollmann and Lavie 1985).
without assuming an additional rhythmic process. The Lavie has referred to the wake maintenance zones as
three-oscillator model (phase only) accounted in a different
way for the bimodal sleep-nap pattern (Kronauer 1987a): it * Hemicircadian is equivalent to circasemidian
Concepts and models of sleep regulation 73

'forbidden zones' for sleep, and supported this notion by In humans, there is no clear evidence for a splitting of the
data from ultrashort sleep/wake cycles (for recent reviews sleep/wake cycle. Strogatz (1986, p. 120) presents records of
see Lavie, 1989; 1991). He refers to the phases of high sleep subjects showing an alternation between split and
propensity as primary (i.e. night sleep) and secondary (i.e. consolidated modes of sleep, the former being defined as
midday sleep) sleep gates. In his studies, the evening zone two naps bracketing the interval of the habitual consolidated
preceding night sleep is characterized by the lowest sleep sleep. According to Strogatz, the relative duration of a
propensity. nap-sleep pair is more dissimilar than for split sleep. A
It should be realized that the terms 'wake maintenance biphasic sleep pattern has been recently described in
zone' and 'forbidden zone' suggest an absolute inability to humans who were exposed for several weeks to a short
sleep during specific intervals, which is not born out by the photoperiod (Wehr 1991, 1992). Strogatz (1986, p. 186)
data. There are at best intervals of reduced sleep succeeded with the two-process model (Daan et a/. 1984) to
propensity. However, even this proposition needs to be simulate certain features of the alternations of the split and
further examined for different schedules. In particular, the consolidated mode of sleep. Thus a model with a single
constant routine protocols are needed to demonstrate that circadian oscillator is able to generate a realistic bimodal
factors such as structuring the day by three meals, sleep/wake pattern. Nevertheless, other possibilities must
knowledge of time of day, and sleep itself are not major be considered. One alternative is the dual pacemaker model
determinants of the bimodal patterns observed. described in the preceding paragraph in which the duality of
the oscillator manifests itself only under certain conditions.
Another possibility is the hemicircadian component
3.4 Recurrence of slow-wave sleep
proposed by Kronauer and Jewett (1992) whose influence on
Gagnon arid Dekoninck (1984) reported a recurrence of the body temperature rhythm becomes manifest only under
slow-wave sleep (SWS) during extended sleep, results that particular circumstances whereas its effect on sleep
were interpreted in terms of a sleep-dependent 12.5-h propensity is assumed to be permanently present. The study
rhythm (Broughton et al. 1990). Studies of extended sleep in of Zulley and Carr (1992) indicates that a biphasic
which sleep was initiated at three different circadian phases. distribution of sleep can be generated in free-running
did not confirm a consistent recurrence of SWS (Dijk et al. subjects by restricting the main sleep episode below a
1990a, 1991b). E E G slow-wave activity was determined critical duration. Further experiments, data analyses and
quantitatively in these studies to assess its extent in later simulations are needed to clarify this basic issue.
parts of extended sleep. The occasional peaks in one of the
studies (Dijk et a/. 1991a,b) may represent random
4 CONCLUDING REMARKS
variations or could be attributed to prior intermittent
waking or to the variable episode durations of nonREM The present overview reflects the increasing importance of
sleep and REM sleep (Achermann et u l . , in prep.). the modelling approach in sleep science. Models help
The data d o not appear to contradict the basic delineate the regulating processes underlying such a
assumptions of the two-process model and d o not constitute complex and little understood phenomenon as sleep. and
compelling evidence for the existence of a circasemidian thereby offer a conceptual framework for the analysis of
rhythm. existing and new data. The major models have already
inspired a considerable number of experiments. This
approach has become particularly attractive by the
3.5 Multiple circadian oscillators? possibility of using quantitative physiological measures in
Presently there is no strong evidence for the existence of humans for testing the predictions of a model. Thus E E G
multiple circadian pacemakers in the mammalian central slow-wave activity represents the key parameter in the
nervous system (see 1.1). Daan and Beersma (1992) investigation of nonREM sleep homeostasis, while the
demonstrated that certain phenomena that have been 'unmasked' core body temperature is being used as the
attributed to multiple osci?lators, may be explained by a indicator of the circadian process. The interactions between
single oscillator. Nevertheless, there are observations that these two processes can be studied by 'conflict experiments'
seem to defy an explanation on the basis of a single in which their usual phase relationship is experimentally
pacemaker. Thus under certain experimental conditions the altered. This has been achieved in animal studies (recovery
circadian rest-activity cycle of rodents may split into two from sleep deprivation during the activity period: Borbely
separate components that drift apart and may assume a new and Neuhaus 1979; Trachsel et ul. 1986) as well as in hurnan
stable phase relation. In their dual pacemaker model, experiments (e.g. forced desynchrony: Dijk et nl. 1992;
Beersma and Daan (1992) propose that two identical sleep curtailment: Zulley and Carr 1992).
oscillators that are usually in phase, exhibit a change in their Another positive feature of the modelling approach is the
phase-relationship which gives rise to splitting. They suggest fact that the regulatory processes postulated are not
that the output of these oscillators interacts in a restricted to a single species but probably represent basic
multiplicative way to determine the probability of activity. mechanisms in mammalian sleep. The basic features of sleep
74 A . A . Borbdy and P . Achermann

homeostasis and its interaction with the circadian process Akerstedt and Folkard 1990) and to predict optimal
has been shown to be similar in many mammalian species schedules. Models describing the effect of light on circadian
including humans, and quantitative simulations in an animal parameters (e.g. Kronauer 1990) need to be combined with
species have been successfully performed (for a review see models accounting for sleep homeostasis. There are
Tobler et a/. 1992). Human circadian research and the preliminary attempts at such combinations (Achermann and
corresponding models have been continuously related t o BorbCly 1992; Massaquoi and McCarley 1992). These
animal experiments, and in one case the same model was considerations are also relevant for the treatment of sleep
used for simulating human and rodent data (Kronauer disturbances in older people in whom both the homeostatic
1987a). Again there is little doubt that a considerable part of and circadian facet of sleep regulation may be impaired. The
circadian physiology is invariant across mammalian species. relevance of the modelling approach to sleep in depression
Finally, the first model of the ultradian process underlying has been recognized early. Thus the circadian coincidence
the nonREM-REM sleep cycle has been derived from data hypothesis (Wehr and Wirz-Justice 1980) and the Process
obtained in the cat. Subsequently, the basic concepts of the S-deficiency hypothesis (BorbCly and Wirz-Justice 1982;
model were applied to human sleep (see Table 2). The BorbCly 1987) have attempted to account in different ways
pervasiveness of the basic sleep regulating mechanisms in for the altered sleep structure in depression as well as for
mammals raises the problem whether they may be present the antidepressant effect of sleep deprivation. The two
also in other classes of animals. The homeostatic regulation hypotheses gave rise to numerous studies which were
of the sleep or rest state has already been explored in a bird designed to test their basic tenets. Recently, a related
(Tobler and BorbCly 1988), a fish (Tobler and BorbCly hypothesis has been advanced by Wu and Bunney (1990).
1985), and even in invertebrates (Tobler 1983; Tobler and One of the gratifying aspects of modelling sleep regulation
Stalder 1988). is the fact that the postulated basic processes seem to be of a
One of the problems of the modelling approach is the very general nature. This has been clear for a long time for
abstract nature of the postulated processes. It is important the circadian process which, in addition to sleep propensity,
to explore the structures in the brain in which these determines to a large extent such diverse parameters as core
processes are generated as well as the underlying body temperature, the secretion of certain hormones (e.g.
neurobiological mechanisms. There is conclusive evidence cortisol and melatonin), retinal disk shedding, performance
that the suprachiasmatic nuclei represent the site of the and memory. Ultradian changes are not restricted to the
circadian pacemaker. However, it is still unknown how the alternation of the two sleep states, but involve many other
information originating in this structure modulates the functions controlled by the autonomic nervous system (e.g.
sleep/wake and rest-activity cycles. There is also strong thermoregulation, cardiovascular parameters, penile erec-
evidence that nuclei in the pons are centrally involved in the tion). Plasma renin levels have been recently shown to be
periodic generation of R E M sleep, although the precise closely associated with the nonREM-REM sleep cycle
mechanisms remain to be elucidated. T h e most recent (Brandenberger et al. 1988). The homeostatic facet of sleep
evidence in favour of the reciprocal interaction hypothesis regulation does not have many associated parameters as yet.
has been summarized by McCarley and Massaquoi (1992). It The plasma level of T S H is affected by sleep deprivation
is still unclear, however, how the processes in the brain stem (Sack et al. 1988), and GH secretion seems to be related to
interact with the forebrain mechanisms of sleep and sleep onset (Born ef a / . 1988). Nevertheless, the basic
wakefulness. Also the structures involved in R E M sleep concept of interacting circadian and homeostatic processes
homeostasis remain elusive. A final comment relates to the has been adopted to account for the regulation of hormones
mechanisms underlying nonREM sleep homeostasis. It has (Van Cauter 1990), body temperature (Nakao et a/. 1990a,
been one of the shortcomings of the two-process model that 1991), and retinal sensitivity (two-process model of retinal
Process S is based on an E E G parameter whose rhythmicity: RemC et al. 1991). To define the relationship
neurobiological origin is obscure. However, recent experi- between these processes and sleep regulation will be one of
ments revealed that hyperpolarized thalamic neurons exhibit the challenging future tasks.
fluctuations of membrane potential in the slow-wave
frequency range (Steriade et a/. 1991; see also McCarley and
Massaquoi 1992). If, as Steriade and his colleagues propose,
these fluctuations are at the origin of E E G slow waves, the ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
mechanisms underlying sleep homeostasis would be open for
investigations at the cellular level. We thank Drs Domien Beersma, Serge Daan and Irene
This overview focused on the role of modelling for Tobler for their comments on the manuscript. The
investigating basic sleep regulating mechanisms. However, comments of the workshop participants o n a draft version of
this work also has implications for applied and clinical sleep this overview are gratefully appreciated. We thank Ms
research. Models can be used to simulate the duration and Annette Kaltbrunner for her help in preparing the
intensity of sleep as well as the time course of daytime manuscript. The authors’ work was supported by the Swiss
sleepiness during shift work (e.g. Daan et a/. 1984; National Science Foundation, grant 31.25634.88.
Concepts and models of sleep regulation 75

Broughton, R . J . Biorhythmic variations in consciousness and


REFERENCES psychological functions. Can. Psychol. R e v . , 1975, 16: 217-239.
Broughton, R. J. and Mullington, J . Circasemidian sleep propensity
Achermann, P. Schlafregulation des Menschen: Modelle urzd and the phase-amplitude maintenance model of human
Computersimulationen. PhD Thesis, E T H Zurich, 1988. sleep-wake regulation. J . Sleep Res., 1992, 1: 93-98.
Achermann, P. and BorbCly, A . A. Simulation of human sleep: Broughton, R . , J . De Koninck, J . , Gagnon, P., Dunham, W. and
ultradian dynamics of E E G slow-wave activity. J . Biol. Rhythms, Stampi, C. Sleep-wake biorhythms and extended sleep in man.
1990, 5: 141-157. In: J . Montplaisir and R. Godbout (Eds) Sleep and Biological
Achermann, P. and BorbCly, A. A. Combining various models of Rhythms. Basic mechanisms and applications 10 psychiafry.
sleep regulation. J . Sleep Res., 1992, 1: 138-141. Oxford University Press, New York, 1990: 25-41.
Achermann, P., Beersma, D . G. M. and BorbCly, A . A. The Brunner, D. P . , Dijk, D. J . and Borbtly, A . A . A quantitative
two-process model: ultradian dynamics of sleep. In: J . A. Horne analysis of phasic and tonic submental E M G activity in human
(Ed.) Sleep '90.Pontenagel Press, Bochum, 1990: 296-300. sleep. Physiol.Behav., 1990a, 48: 741-748.
Akerstedt, T. and Folkard, S. A model of human sleepiness. In: J . Brunner, D. P., Dijk, D. J . , Tobler, I. and Borbtly, A . A . Effect
A . Horne (Ed.) Sleep '90. Pontenagel Press, Bochum, 1990: of partial sleep deprivation on sleep stages and E E G power
3 10-3 13. spectra: evidence for nonREM and R E M sleep homeostasis.
Akerstedt, T. and Gillberg, M. Sleep duration and the power Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 1990b, 75: 492-499.
spectral density of the EEG. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurvphysiol., Caekebeke, J . F. V., v. Dijk, J . G . , Rosa, A . C . and Kemp, B. A
1986, 64: 119-122. model relating K-complexes to spontaneous slow-wave activity
Alfoldi, P., Tobler, I. and BorbCly, A. A . Sleep regulation in rats during sleep. In: M. G. Terzano, P. L. Halasz, and A. C .
during early development. A m . J . Physiol., 1990, 258: Declerck (Eds) Phasic Events and Dynamic Organization of
R634- R644. Sleep. Raven Press, New York, 1991: 41-51.
Beersma, D. G. M. and Daan, S . Generation of activity-rest Campbell, S. S. and Zulley, J . Ultradian components of human
patterns by dual circadian pacemaker systems: a model. J . Sleep sleep/wake patterns during disentrainment. In: H . Schulz and P.
Res., 1992, 1: 84-87. Lavie (Eds) Ultradian Rhythms in Physiology and Behavior. Exp.
Beersma, D. G. M., Daan, S. and Dijk, D. J . Sleep intensity and Brain Res. 1985, Suppl. 12: 234-255.
timing-a model for their circadian control. Lect. Math. Life Sci., Campbell, S. S. and Zulley, J . Evidence for circadian influence on
1987, 19: 39-62. human slow wave sleep during daytime sleep episodes.
Beersma, D . G. M., Daan, S. and Van den Hoofdakker, R. H . Psychophysiol., 1989, 26: 580-585.
Distribution of REM latencies and other sleep phenomena in Carpenter, G . A . and Grossberg, S. A neural theory of circadian
depression as explained by a single ultradian rhythm disturbance. rhythms: Aschoff's rule in diurnal and nocturnal mammals. A m .
Sleep, 1984, 7: 126-136. J . Physiol., 1984, 247: R1067-RI082.
Beersma, D. G . M . , Dijk. D . J . and Blok, C. G . REM sleep Carpenter, G. A. and Grossberg, S. Mammalian circadian rhythms:
deprivation during 5 hours leads to an immediate REM sleep a neural network model. Lect. Math. Life Sci., 1987, 19:
rebound and to suppression of non-REM sleep intensity. 151-203.
Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol., 1990, 76: 114- 122. Carskadon, M. A . Ontogeny of human sleepiness as measured by
Blake, H. and Gerard, R . W. Brain potentials during sleep. A m . J . sleep latency. In: D.F. Dinges and R.J. Broughton (Eds) Sleep
Physiol., 1937, 119: 692-703. and Alertness: Chronobiological, behavioral, and medical aspects
BorbCly, A. A . Sleep: circadian rhythm versus recovery process. In: of napping. Raven Press, New York, 1989: 53-69.
M. Koukkou, D. Lehmann and J . Angst (Eds) Functional States Carskadon, M. A. and Dement, W . C . Midafternoon decline in
of the Brain: their determinants. Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1980: MSLT scores on a constant routine. Sleep R e s . , 1985, 14: 292.
151-161. Cauter van, E. Diurnal and ultradian rhythms in human endocrine
Borbtly, A . A. Sleep regulation: circadian rhythm and homeostasis. function: a minireview. Hormone R e s . , 1990, 34: 45-53.
In: D . Ganten, and D . Pfaff (Eds) Current Topics in Daan, S. and Beersma, D. G. M. Circadian gating of human
Neuroendocrinology, Vol. 1 : Sleep. Clinical and experimental sleep-wake cycles. In: M. C. Moore-Ede and C. A. Czeisler
aspects. Springer Verlag, Berlin, 1982a: 83- 103. (Eds) Mathematical Models of the Circadian Sleep- Wake Cycle.
Borbtly, A. A . A two-process model of sleep regulation. Hum. Raven Press, New York, 1984: 129-158.
Neurobiol., 1982b, 1: 195-204. Daan, S. and Beersma, D . G. M. A single pacemaker can produce
Borbtly, A. A. The S-deficiency hypothesis of depression and the different rates of reentrainment in different overt rhythms. J .
two-process model of sleep regulation. Pharrnacopsychiatry, Sleep Res., 1992, 1: 80-83.
1987, 20: 23-29. Daan, S. and Berde, C. Two coupled oscillators: Simulations of the
Borbtly. A. A . and Neuhaus, H . U . Sleep-deprivation: effect on circadian pacemaker in mammalian activity rhythms. J . Theor.
sleep and E E G in the rat. J . comp. Physiol., 1979, 133: 71-87. Biol., 1978, 70: 297-313.
BorbCly, A . A . and Wirz-Justice, A . Sleep, sleep deprivation and Daan, S . Beersma, D . G. M. and BorbCly, A . A . The timing of
depression. A hypothesis derived from a model of sleep human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker.
regulation. Hum. Neurobiol., 1982, 1: 205-210. A m . J . Physiol., 1984, 246: R161-R178.
BorbCly, A . A . , Achermann, P., Trachsel. L. and Tobler I. Sleep Daan, S., Beersma, D . G . M . , Dijk, D . J . Akerstedt, T . and
initiation and sleep intensity: interaction of homeostatic and Gillberg, M . Kinetics of an hourglass component involved in the
circadian mechanisms. J . Biol. Rhythms, 1989, 4: 149- 160. regulation of human sleep and wakefulness. In: W. T. J . M.
Born, J . , Muth, S . and Fehm, H . L . The significance of sleep onset Hekkens, G . A . Kerkhof and W. J . Rietveld (Eds) Advances in
and slow wave sleep for nocturnal release of growth hormone the Bio-Sciences, Trends in Chronobiology. Pergamon Press,
(GH) and cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 198X. 13: Oxford, 1988, 73: 183-193.
233-243. Da Rosa, A . C . , Kemp, B., Paiva, T., Lopes da Silva, F. H. and
Brandenberger, G., Follenius, M . , Simon. C . . Ehrhart, J . and Kamphuisen, H . A . C . A model-based detector of vertex waves
Libert, J.P. Nocturnal oscillations in plasma renin activity and and K complexes in sleep electroencephalogram. Electroenceph.
REM-NREM sleep cycles in humans: a common regulatory Clin. Neurophysiol, 1991, 78: 71-79.
mechanism? Sleep, 1988, 11: 242-250. Dijk, D . J . and Beersma, D . G. M. Effects of SWS deprivation on
76 A . A . Borbdy and P. Achermann

subsequent E E G power density and spontaneous sleep duration. 1990, 15: 141-147.
Elecfroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol., 1989, 72: 312-320. Gagnon. P. and De Koninck, J . Reappearence of E E G slow waves
Dijk, D. J . , Beersma, D . G . M. and Daan, S . E E G power density in extended sleep. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophy.siol., 1984, 58:
during nap sleep: Reflection of an hourglass measuring the 155-157.
duration of prior wakefulness. J . B i d . Rhythms, 1987a. 2: Gander, P. H . , Kronauer, R . E., Czeisler, C. A. and Moore-Ede,
207-219. M. C. Simulating the action o f zeitgebers on a coupled
Dijk, D . J . , Beersma, D . G . M. and Daan, S . Bright morning light two-oscillator model of the human circadian system. A m . J .
advances the human circadian system without affecting NREM Physiol., 1984a. 247: R418-R426.
sleep homeostasis. A m . J . Physiol., 1989. 156: R 1 0 6 - R l l l . Gander, P. H . , Kronauer, R . E., Czeisler, C . A . and Moore-Edc.
Dijk, D. J., Brunner, D. P. and BorbCly. A. A. Time course of M. C. Modelling the action of zeitgcbers o n the human circadian
E E G power density during long sleep in humans. A m . J . system: comparisons of simulations and data. A m . J . Physiol.,
P h y ~ i ~ l1990a,
., 258: R650-R661. 19846, 247: R427-R444.
Dijk, D. J . Brunner. D . P. and BorbCly, A. A . E E G power density Gander, P. H., Kronauer, R . E. and Graeber, R . C . Phasc shifting
during recovery sleep in the morning. Electroenceph. Clin. two coupled circadian pacemakers: implications for jet lag. A m .
Neurophysiol., 1991a. 78: 203-214. J . Physiol., 1985, 249: R704-R719.
Dijk, D. J . , Duffy. J . F. and Czeisler, C . A . Circadian and Gillberg, M . and Akerstedt, T. The dynamics of the first slcep
sleep-wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cycle. Sleep, 1991. 14: 147-154.
cognitive performance. J . Sleep Res., 1992, 1: 112-117. Gillberg, M., AnderzCn, I . and Akerstedt, T. Recovery within
Dijk. D. J., Brunner. D. P., Beersma, D. G. M. and BorbCly, A . day-time sleep after slow wave sleep suppression. Elecfroenceph.
A . Slow wave sleep and electroencephalogram power density as a Clin. Neurophysiol., 1991, 78: 267-273.
function of prior waking and circadian phase. Sleep, 1990b, 13: Hobson, J . A,. Lydic, R . and Baghdoyan, H . A. Evolving concepts
430-440. of sleep cycle generation: From brain centers t o neuronal
Dijk, D. J., Cajochen, C., Tobler, I . and BorbCly, A . A . Sleep populations. Behav. Brain. Sci., 1986, 9: 371-448.
extension in humans: sleep stages, E E G power spectra, and body Horne, J. A . Why We Sleep: fhe funcfions of sleep in humans and
temperature. Sleep, 1991b, 14: 294-306. ofher mammals. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1988.
Dijk, D. J . , Beersma, D. G . M., Daan, S . , Bloem, G . M. and van Horne, J . A . Dimensions t o sleepiness. In: T. H. Monk ( E d . )
den Hoofdakker, R . H. Quantitative analysis of the effects of Sleep, Sleepiness and Performance. John Wiley, Chichester. 1991:
slow-wave sleep deprivation during the first 3 h of sleep on 169- 196.
subsequent E E G power density. Eur. Arch. Psychiutr. Neurol. Horne, J . A . Human slow wave sleep and the cerebral cortex. .I.
Sci., 1987b, 236: 323-328. Sleep Res., 1992, 1: 116-118.
Dijk, D. J . , Visscher. C. A , , Bloem, G . M., Beersma, D . G . M . Illnerovri, H . VanCfek, J . and Hoffmann, K. Different mechanisms
and Daan, S . Reduction of human sleep duration after bright of phase delays and phase advances of the circadian rhythm in rat
light exposure in the morning. Neurosci. Lett., 1987c, 73: pineal N-acetyltransferase activity. J . B i d . Rhyfhms. 1989. 4:
181-186. 187-200.
Dirlich, G . Looking at human circadian phenomena from a Jewett, M. E. Kronauer, R. E. and Czeisler. C . A . Light-induced
framework of simple stochastic models. In: M. C. Moore-Ede suppression of endogenous circadian amplitude in humans.
and C. A . Czeisler (Eds) Mathematical Models of the Circadian Nafure, 1991, 350: 59-62.
Sleep-Wake Cycle. Raven Press, New York. 1984: 159-185. Kawato, M . , Fujita, K., Suzuki, R. and Winfree, A . T. A
Eastman, C. Are separate temperature and activity oscillators three-oscillator model of the human circadian system controlling
necessary to explain the phenomena o f human circadian rhythms‘? the core temperature rhythm and the slecp-wake cyclc. J . Theor.
In: M. C. Moore-Ede and C. A. Czeisler (Eds) Mathematical Biol., 1982, 98: 369-392.
Models of the Circadian Sleep-Wake Cycle. Raven Press, New Kemp, B. and Kamphuisen, H. A . C. Simulation 0 1 human
York, 1984: 81-103. hypnograms using a Markov chain model. Sleep. 1986, 9:
Enright, J. T. The Timing of Sleep and Wakefulness. Springer- 405-4 14.
Verlag, Berlin, 1980. Kleitman, N . and Engelmann, T. G . Sleep characteristics of infants.
Feinberg, I . Changes in sleep cycle patterns with age. J . Psychiaf. J . Appl. Physiol., 1953, 6: 269-282.
Res., 1974, 10: 283-306. Knowles, J . B., Coulter, M., Wahnon. S . . Reitz, W. and MacLean,
Feinberg, I . , March, J . D . , Floyd, T. C . , Jimison, R . , A. W. Variation in Process S : effects on sleep continuity and
Bossom-Demitrack, L. and Katz, P. H . Homeostatic changes architecture. Sleep, 1990a, 13: 97-107.
during post-nap sleep maintain baseline levels of delta E E G . Knowles, J. B., MacLean, A . W . , Brunet, D . and Coulter, M.
Elecfroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol., 1985. 61 : 134-137. Nap-induced changes in the time course of Process S . Effects on
Folkard, S . and Akerstedt, T. Towards a model for the prediction nocturnal slow wave activity. In: J . A . Horne (Ed.) Sleep ’90.
of alertness and/or fatigue on different sleep/wake schedules. In: Pontenagel Press, Bochum, 1990: 68-70.
A . Oginski, J . Polorski and J . Rutenfranz (Eds) Contemporary Kronauer, R . E. Modeling principles for human circadian rhythms.
Advances in Shifrwork Research : Theoretical and prucfical aspects In: M. C. Moore-Ede and C. A . Czeisler (Eds) Mafhematical
in the lafe eighties. Medical Academy, Krakow, Poland, 1987: Models of the Circadian Sleep-Wake Cycle. Raven Press. New
231-240. York, 1984: 105-128.
Folkard, S. and Akerstcdt, T. Towards the prediction of alertness Kronauer, R . E. Temporal subdivision o f the circadian cycle. Lect.
on abnormal sleep/wake schedules. In: A . Coblentz (Ed) Mafh. Life Sci.. 1987a, 19: 63-120.
Vigilance and Performance in Automatized Systems. Kluwer, Kronauer, R . E . A model for the effect of light on the human
Dordrecht, 1989: 287-296. “deep” circadian rhythm. Sleep Re.s., 19X7b, 16: 620.
Folkard, S . and Akerstedt, T. A three-process model of the Kronauer, R . E . A quantitative model for the cffects of light o n the
regulation of alertness-sleepiness. In: R . J . Broughton and R . D. amplitude and phase of the deep circadian pacemaker, based on
Ogilvie (Eds) Sleep, Arousal, and Performance. Birkhauser, human data. In: J . A . Horne (Ed) Sleep ’YO. Pontenagel Press,
Boston 1992: 11-26. Bochum, 1990: 306-309.
Foret, J . , Touron. N., ClodorC, M., Benoit, 0. and Bouard, G . Kronauer, R. E. and Frangioni. J. V. Modeling laboratory
Modification of sleep structure by brief forced awakenings at bright-light protocols. Sleep R e s . , 1987, 16: 622.
different times of the night. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol., Kronauer, R. E. and Jewett, M . E . The relation between circadian
Concepts and models of sleep regulation 71

and hemicircadian components of human endogenous tempera- 11: 3200-3217.


ture rhythms. J. Sleep Res., 1992, 1: 88-92. Strogatz, S. H. The Mathematical Structure of the Human
Kronauer, R. E., Czeisler, C. A., Pilato, S. F., Moore-Ede, M. C. Sleep-Wake Cycle. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 1986.
and Weitzmann, E. D. Mathematical model of the human Strogatz, S. H. and Kronauer, R . E. Circadian wake-maintenance
circadian system with two interacting oscillators. A m . J. Physiol., zones and insomnia in man. Sleep Res., 1985, 14: 219.
1982, 242: R3-Rl7. Tobler, 1. Effect of forced locomotion on the rest-activity cycle of
Lancel, M. and Kerkhof, G. A. Sleep structure and E E G power the cockroach. Behav. Brain Res., 1983, 8: 351-360.
density in morning types and evening types during a simulated Tobler, I. and Borbtly, A . A. Effect of rest deprivation on motor
day and night shift. Physiol. Behav., 1991, 49: 1195-1201. activity in fish. J. Comp. Physiol., 1985, 157: 817-822.
Lavie, P. To nap, perchance to s l e e v u l t r a d i a n aspects of napping. Tobler, 1. and Borbtly, A . A . Sleep and E E G spectra in the pigeon
In: D. F. Dinges and R. J . Broughton (Eds) Sleep and Alertness: (Columbia Livia) under baseline conditions and after sleep
Chronobiological, behavioral, and medical aspects of napping. deprivation. J. C o m p . Physiol. A , 1988, 163: 729-738.
Raven Press, New York, 1989: 99-120. Tobler, I. and Stalder, J . Rest in the scorpion-a sleep-like state? J.
Lavie, P. The 24-hour sleep propensity function (SPF): practical Comp. Physiol. A , 1988, 163: 227-235.
and theoretical implications. In: T. H. Monk (Ed) Sleep, Tobler, I. Borbtly, A. A. and Groos, G. The effect of sleep
Sleepiness and Performance. Wiley, Chichester, 1991: 65-93. deprivation on sleep in rats with suprachiasmatic lesions.
Lawder, R. E. A proposed mathematical model for sleep Neurosci. Lett., 1983, 42: 49-54.
patterning. J. Biomed. Eng., 1984, 6: 63-69. Tobler, I . , Franken, P., Trachsel, L. and BorbCly, A. A . Models of
Massaquoi, S. and McCarley, R. W. Resetting the R E M sleep sleep regulation in mammals. J. Sleep Res., 1992, 1: 119-121.
oscillator. In: J . A. Horne (Ed.) Sleep '90.Pontenagel Press, Trachsel, L., Tobler, I. and BorbCly, A. A . Sleep regulation in rats:
Bochum, 1990: 301-305. effects of sleep deprivation, light and circadian phase. A m . J.
Massaquoi, S. and McCarley, R . W . Extension of the limit cycle Physiol., 1986, 251: R1037-Rl044.
reciprocal interaction model of R E M cycle control: an integrated Trachsel, L., Dijk, D . J., Brunner, D. P., Klene, C. and Borbtly,
sleep control model. J. Sleep Res. 1992, I : 32-137. A . A . Effect of zopiclone and midazolam o n sleep and E E G
McCarley, R. W. and Hobson, J . A . Neuronal excitability spectra in a phase-advanced sleep schedule. Neuropsycho-
modulation over the sleep cycle: a structural and mathematical pharmacology, 1990, 3: 11-18,
model. Science, 1975, 189: 58-60. Webb, W. B. and Agnew Jr., H. W. Stage 4 sleep: influence of time
McCarley, R. W. and Massaquoi, S. A limit cycle mathematical course variables. Science, 1971, 174: 1354-1356.
model of the R E M sleep oscillator system. A m . J. Physiol. 1986, Wehr, T. A . The duration of human melatonin secretion and sleep
251: R1011-R1029. response to changes in daylength (photoperiod). J. Clin.
McCarley, R . W. and Massaquoi, S. Neurobiological substrates of Endocrinol. Metab., 1991, 73: 1276-1280.
the revised limit cycle reciprocal interaction model of R E M sleep Wehr, T. A . In short photoperiods, human sleep is biphasic. J.
control. J. Sleep Res., 1992, 1: 126-131. Sleep Res., 1992, 1:
Meijer, J . H., Daan, S., Overkamp, G. J . F. and Hermann, P. The Wehr, T. A. and Wirz-Justice, A. Internal coincidence model for
two-oscillator circadian system of tree shrews (Tupaija belangeri) sleep deprivation and depression. In: W . P. Koella (Ed.) Sleep
and its response to light pulses. 1. Biol. Rhythms, 1990, 5: 1-16. IY80. Karger, Basel, 1981: 26-33.
Mendelson, W. B., James, S. P., Martin, J . V., Wagner, R.. Sack, Wever, R. A. Toward a mathematical model of circadian
D. A , , Garnett, D., Milton, J. and Wehr, T. A . Frequency rhythmicity. In: M. C. Moore-Ede and C . A . Czeisler (Eds)
analysis of the sleep E E G in depression. Psychiat. Res., 1987, 21: Mathematical Models of the Circadian Sleep- Wake Cycle. Raven
89-94. Press, New York, 1984: 17-79.
Mistlberger, R. E . , Bergmann, B. M.. Waldenar, W. and Wever, R. A . Modes of interaction between ultradian and circadian
Rechtschaffen, A . Recovery sleep following sleep deprivation in rhythms: Toward a mathematical model of sleep. In: H. Schulz
intact and suprachiasmatic nuclei lesioned rats. Sleep, 1983, 6: and P. Lavie (Eds) Ultradian Rhythms in Physiology and
217-233. Behavior. Exp. Brain Res. Suppl. 12, 1985: 309-317.
Mrosovsky, N. and Salmon, P. A . A behavioral method for Wever, R . A . Mathematical models of circadian one- and
accelerating re-entrainment of rhythms to new light-dark cycles. multi-oscillator systems. Lect. Math. Life Sci., 1987, 19: 205-265.
Nature, 1987, 330: 372-373. Winfree, A . T . Impact of a circadian clock on the timing of human
Nakao, M., McGinty, D . , Szymusiak, R . and Yamamoto, M. sleep. A m . J . Physiol., 1983, 245: R497-R504.
Human circadian system model based o n thermoregulatory Wollmann, M. and Lavie, P. A hypernycthemeral sleep-wake cycle:
mechanism of slow wave sleep. Proc. .5th Symp. Biol. Physiol. some hidden regularities. Sleep, 1986, 9: 324-334.
Eng. (Japan), 1990a: 217-220. Wu, J . C. and Bunney, W . E . The biological basis of an
Nakao, M., Takahashi, T., Mizutani, Y. and Yamamoto, M. antidepressant response to sleep deprivation and relapse: a
Simulation study on dynamics transition in neuronal activity review and hypothesis. A m . J . Psychiatry, 1Y90, 147: 14-21.
during sleep cycle by using asynchronous and symmetry neural Zulley, J . The four-hour sleep wake cycle. Sleep Res., l985, 17:
network model. Biol. Cybern., 1990b. 63: 243-250. 403.
Nakao, M., McGinty, D., Szymusiak, R . , Ichikawa, T. and Zulley, J . and Carr, D. Forced splitting of human sleep in
Yamamoto, M. A thermoregulatory model of the ultradian free-running rhythms. J. Sleep R e s . , 1992, 1 : 108- I 1 1.
rhythm of human sleep. Sleep Res., 1991. 20A: 554.
Pittendrigh, C. S. and Daan, S. A functional analysis of circadian APPENDIX
pacemakers in nocturnal rodents. I. The stability and lability of
spontaneous frequency. J. comp. Physiol., 1976, 106: 223-252. The mathematical equations of the major models are
RemC, C. E., Wirz-Justice, A. and Terman, M. The visual input compiled in this appendix. For a detailed description of the
stage of the mammalian circadian pacemaking system: I. Is there models the original papers should be consulted.
a clock in the mammalian eye? J. Biol. Rhythms, 1991, 6: 5-29.
Steriade, M., Curro Dossi, R . and Nunez, A . Network modulation E-M model (Daan and Berde 1978)
of a slow intrinsic oscillation of cat thalamocortical neurons
implicated in sleep delta waves: Cortically induced synchroniza-
tion and brainstem cholinergic suppression. J. Neurosci., 1991.
e, = tt - m, + A , sin
1
78 A . A . Borbkly and P . Achermann

m, = r,,,,- el-

e, = E, - M,
, + A,,,,sin [ :
- (e,- I - $,, ,)I Two-oscillator thennoregulatory model of sleep control
(Nakao et al. 1990a [in Japanese, with English abstract];
Nakao et ul. 1991; Nakao, pers. comm.)
m,= M, - El- , TBODY + 2cwnTBOVY + wf,TBOVY = TSEI.+ TM

E , M: time of onset of 5 h of activity triggered by the E - and TsET= x ( t ) - ch heat-load(t)


M-oscillator, respectively; t E , t,,,,:period of the E - and (0 t
M-oscillator; A,, A,,,,: coupling strength; sine function: x ( t ) = a, sin(+ + 0),
phase response curve (PRC); Gt-,$,,,,: phase of the P R C
relative to the E and M events. See original paper for
(A ,+ A,s)(1 e-'h'T) A s (waking)
- -

zeitgeber terms. ( A , + A,s)e-'s'r- A,s (sleep)

Original x-y model. Predominant effect of light on y


(Kronauer et ul. 1982)
k2x+kpL,(-1+x2)X+w~++F,,y=0
heat-load(t) =
I,+ - TyFT(t')}
{ TBODY(t') dt'

@ [ y ( t ) c , heatLload(t); h,]
Sleepiness = 1 + e-[%)l>Y(f)- ~\lT(f)l/cz

x: strong circadian oscillator controlling core body


temperature; y : weak circadian oscillator controlling the
sleep/wake cycle; z : zeitgeber; k : scale factor ( k = 24/2n); TBODY:observable body temperature; TSE.,.:temperature
p , , p,,; stiffness; w,, io,,: intrinsic angular frequency; Fvx: set point; TM:masking effect due to sleep and waking; 5:
strength of velocity coupling of y to x ; F z v : strength of damping factor; w,: intrinsic frequency; x ( t ) : circadian
velocity coupling of x to y; Fzy: Coupling strength of the oscillator with amplitude a,, frequency w, and phase 0,;k :
zeitgeber . scaling factor (k = 2n/24); t: time constant; t,: beginning
of wake; t,y, tso: beginning of sleep; @[. . . ;h,]: linear
increasing function above threshold h,, 0 otherwise; y ( t ) :
Revised x-y model. Predominant effect of light on x
circadian oscillator with amplitude uy, frequency w , and
(Kronauer 1990)
phase 8."; ch, A,, A,,, E , ~ ,c S , c,, c,: positive constants.
X):( = x,. + 0.13(x 43+
-- B
Sleep onset occurs when sleepiness crosses a threshold h,s
upward and wake onset is defined by a downward crossing
of a threshold h , > h,).

B = ( I -mx)C/'/3 Reciprocal interaction model (McCarley and Hobson


1975)
x , x c . : deep circadian pacemaker; I : light intensity; B :
perceived brightness; r , : intrinsic period; m : modulation X = ux - bxy
index; C: scaling factor. I; = -CY + dxy
x : activity of FT'G cells (REM-on); y : activity of LC cells
(REM-off); a , b, c, d: positive constants representing
connection strength. R E M sleep occurs if x is above a
certain threshold.

Limit cycle reciprocal interaction (LCR) model: original


version (Massaquoi and McCarley 1990; McCarley and
Massaquoi 1986; Massaquoi, pers. comm.)

x : strong circadian oscillator with intrinsic frequency 1


controlling body temperature; y , , y2: weak circadian
oscillators with intrinsic frequency 1 + w controlling the
sleep/wake or rest-activity cycles; Or: phase of x ; 0 , : phase
of y , ; HZ: phase of y z ; F,,, F,,, FvA:coupling functions.
Note: Simulations were carried out with a simplified
version of the model.
Concepts and models of sleep regulation 79

Model of ultradian variation of slow-wave activity


(Achermann et al. 1990)

SWA = rcSWA 1 -
( s3
~ - fc(SWA - SWA,)

dci,, = 0.975
c;
+ 0.125 sin -t + p,,
0.39 + 0.3 sin(p,,) - 0.05t,
1
if E > 0
X REMT(t)
S = -~CSWA + rs(1-
u= V
+ n(t)SWA
S)

else
V = aV(6 - U 2 )- w2U

x: REM-on activity, REM sleep occurs if x > 1.4; y : 1, if U > R T


REMT(t) = {0
REM-off activity; E: declining excitatory input depending , else
on circadian phase of sleep onset; po: circadian phase at
sleep onset; a @ ) , 6(x), S,(x), S , ( y ) : constraining functions; REM(t) = { 1,, ifSWA<R
else
d,,,,: circadian variation of parameter d.
Note: Time step is 10.7 min. SWA: slow-wave activity; S: homeostatic Process S; U , V :
ultradian oscillator (Van der Pol oscillator), generates the
REM trigger (REMT) event; rc: rise constant; fc: fall
constant; SWA,: lower asymptote of SWA; n ( t ) : noise
Two-process model (Daan et al. 1984) component, normal distribution; gc: gain constant ('decay
rate'); rs: 'rise rate' of S ; a : stiffness; 6 : 'amplitude'; w :
intrinsic frequency; RT, R : thresholds determining R E M T
and R E M sleep, respectively.
(wake)
A(0.97 sin[w(t - f , , ) ] + 0.22 sin[2w(t - t,,)] Three-process model of the regulation of
+ 0.07 sin[3w(t - t , J ] sleepiness/alertness (Akerstedt and Folkard 1990; Folkard
+ 0.03 sin[4w(t t,J]
- and Akerstedt 1987, 1989, 1992)
+ 0.001 sin[5w(t - t , J ] ) n
H=H+C [
C=2.5cos ( t - 16.8)-]
12
L=L+C
S={
+
- 2, 4)e --o 0353t 2.4 (waking)

14.3 - (14.3 - S,)e-" "Ir (sleep)


S: homeostatic process, increasing during waking, decreas- W = 5.72e-I '"
ing during sleep; C: circadian process, modulates an upper
A=C+S-W
threshold ( H ) and a lower threshold ( L ) ;d : decay factor of
S ; r : rise factor of S; t,,,t,: time constants; At: time step; A: alertness; C: changes in alertness due to circadian
A: circadian amplitude; w : angular frequency of the factors; S: changes in alertness due to homeostatic factors;
circadian oscillator; t,l: circadian phase at the beginning of W: sleep inertia, wake u p process; S; level of S at
the simulation; H : mean value of upper threshold; L: mean awakening; S,; level of S at retiring. Time t has different
value of lower threshold. A gaussian noise component is reference values: wake up for W a n d S during waking; sleep
superimposed on the thresholds. The timing of sleep and onset for S during sleep; midnight for C.
waking is determined by the interaction of S and C; S Note: The time course of the homeostatic process is the
oscillates between H and L. Note: S is described by a inverse of the time course of Process S of the two-process
recursive equation (iteration). model; it builds up during sleep and declines during waking.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen