Sie sind auf Seite 1von 5

Personality and Individual Differences 88 (2016) 57–61

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Are achievement goals different among morning and


evening-type adolescents?
Cristina Escribano ⁎, Juan Francisco Díaz-Morales
Departamento de Psicología Diferencial y del Trabajo, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Campus de Somosaguas, s/n, 28223 Madrid, Spain

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The interest in the extent to which time-of-day preferences affect school performance is increasing. Apart from
Received 25 June 2015 biological factors, Morning and Evening types differ in their lifestyle and personality traits which may have
Received in revised form 21 August 2015 implications into diverse areas such as their motivation to learn. Taking into account available data, the aim of
Accepted 22 August 2015
the present study was to investigate achievement goals which Morning-, Neither- and Evening-type adolescents
Available online 7 September 2015
accomplish at school. Participants were 342 students aged 12 to 15 (53.5% girls). Morning-types showed higher
Keywords:
Learning and Performance Goals than Evening-types, and these goals were positively related to self-reported
Morningness–eveningness academic performance. Achievement goals were more associated to self-reported grades in Evening-types
Learning goals than in Neither- and Morning-types. It seems that encouraging pupils to achieve good results in order to advance
Performance goals in their studies may improve school performance, especially among Evening-types.
Academic performance © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Morningness/eveningness has also been related to personality traits


(Adan et al., 2012). Previous studies indicated that conscientiousness
In recent years, the interest in the influence of individual time-of- was positively related with morningness followed by agreeableness.
day preferences on academic performance has been increased Openness to experience, extraversion and neuroticism were negatively
(Horzum, Önder, & Beşoluk, 2014; Tonetti, Natale & Randler, 2015). associated (see Tsaousis, 2010). Furthermore, E-types showed a more
Morning-types (M-types) or “larks” prefer to wake up and go to bed pronounced intolerance for repetitive experiences and routine tasks
early and feel at their best moment during the morning (mental, (Muro, Gomà-i-Freixanet, & Adan, 2012), and lower persistence scores
physical, and social activities) whereas Evening-types (E-types) or (Randler & Saliger, 2011), characteristics that, a priori, are relevant to
“owls” have difficulty in waking up early as they prefer later bedtimes an adequate performance at school. In addition, the study of the rela-
and rise times, become progressively more alert throughout the day tionship between learning–thinking styles and chronotype has demon-
and feel at their best moment at the end of the day. Neither-types strated that M- and E-types differ in cerebral hemisphere preference
(N-types) show an intermediate position and represent the majority and E-types were described as right-thinkers (i.e. right-hemisphere
of the population. Morningness/eveningness is usually assessed by preference) who were creative, intuitive, affective, and inclined to cul-
self-reported measures (Adan et al., 2012; Díaz-Morales, 2015; tural individualism, and M-types as left-thinkers (i.e. left-hemisphere
Tonetti, Adan, Di Milia, Randler & Natale, 2015). preference) who preferred verbal and analytic strategies in processing
A change toward eveningness appears during puberty, consequence information, and cultural collectivism (Díaz-Morales, 2007; Fabbri,
of both the maturation processes typical of puberty (Hagenauer, Antonietti, Giorgetti, Tonetti, & Natale, 2007). Hemisphere preference
Perryman, Lee, & Carskadon, 2009), and the many changes in the has been related to academic performance being M-types and left-
adolescent's life such as school demands, new social relationships, and thinkers the group who reported the highest subjective level of achieve-
family atmosphere (Crowley et al., 2014; Díaz-Morales & Escribano ment and E-types and right-thinkers the group who reported the lowest
2014; Díaz-Morales, Escribano, Jankowski, Vollmer, & Randler, 2014). level (Díaz-Morales & Escribano, 2013). Several researchers have
Students are usually taught and tested during the morning school reported that E-types obtain worse school performance (e.g. Beşoluk,
day despite the shift toward eveningness during adolescence. For Önder, & Deveci, 2011; Escribano, Díaz-Morales, Delgado, & Collado,
this reason, morning school schedules seem to be an advantage for 2012; Preckel et al., 2013; Randler & Frech, 2009) even though they
M-types, who tend to obtain higher grades and better attention levels tend to achieve higher scores on intelligence tests (Díaz-Morales &
(Escribano & Díaz-Morales, 2014a; Vollmer, Pötsch, & Randler, 2013). Escribano, 2015; Kanazawa & Perina, 2009; Roberts & Kyllonen, 1999).
Apart from the time at which they attend classes (Beşoluk, 2011;
Beşoluk et al., 2011; Vollmer et al., 2013), thinking styles (Díaz-
⁎ Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: cescribano@psi.ucm.es (C. Escribano), juanfcodiaz@psi.ucm.es Morales & Escribano, 2013) or sleep patterns (Dewald, Meijer, Oort,
(J.F. Díaz-Morales). Kerkhof, & Bögels, 2010; Fallone, Acebo, Seifer, & Carskadon, 2005),

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.032
0191-8869/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
58 C. Escribano, J.F. Díaz-Morales / Personality and Individual Differences 88 (2016) 57–61

another factor that may contribute to explain why E-types report lower Learning Goals scale consists of 8 items related to student interest
academic performance may be the role of motivation and achievement in acquiring new knowledge and increasing their competence (e.g.
goals (Arbabi, Vollmer, Dörfler, & Randler, 2015). Achievement goal the- “I study because I like knowing new things”). Social Reinforcement
ory supports the existence of purposes that guide students to engage Goals scale consists of 6 items related to the interest of the students
with their school work in order to increase their own competence in obtaining approval and avoiding rejection from others (e.g. “I
(Learning Goals) or not appear incompetent in front of others and/or study because I don't want to be disliked by the teacher”). Perfor-
oneself (Performance Goals) (Dweck, 1986; Inglés et al., 2009). Motiva- mance Goals scale consists of 6 items which assess the interest in
tion and achievement goals contribute to explain learning and academic studying in order to achieve good results and advance in their stud-
achievement (Steinmayr & Spinath, 2009) even in early adolescents ies (e.g. “I study because I want to get good grades”). AGTQ was ini-
after controlling for important predictors of achievement (Arbabi tially created by Hayamizu, Ito, and Yoshizaki (1989) and designed
et al., 2015). Morningness is associated to less sleepiness, which in to measure achievement goal tendencies in Japanese high school stu-
turn is associated to higher motivation to learn whereas eveningness dents. Later, a revised American version was developed by Hayamizu
seems to be associated to dysfunctional attitudes toward work. Insuffi- and Weiner (1991). In this study, the Spanish version was used (see
cient motivation may promote learning difficulties and lower school Inglés et al., 2009). Reliability (Cronbach's alpha) for the present
performance (Roeser, Schlarb, & Kübler, 2013). According to Short, sample was: Learning Goals = 0.81, Social Reinforcement Goals =
Gradisar, Lack, and Wright (2013) adolescents who reported poor 0.77, and Performance Goals = 0.77.
sleep quality, reduced alertness and more evening orientation were
more likely to report worse grades through the association with de- 2.2.3. School performance
pressed mood. Moreover, E-types exhibited the lowest mood levels Self-reported grades: students reported last year grades in common
throughout the school day (Díaz-Morales, Escribano, & Jankowski, subjects for all grades of Compulsory Secondary Education (Spanish
2015). language, mathematics, English language and social sciences) and the
Taking into account available data about lifestyle, personality traits, mean of grades was calculated (GPA). Several studies have also used
thinking styles, sleep patterns and school schedule, the aim of the pres- this method (see Wolfson & Carskadon, 2003).The Spanish grading
ent study was to investigate achievement goals which are accomplished system is coded from 0 (the worst) to 10 (the best), for this, the same
in school and the way in which they are associated to self-reported scale (0 to 10) was used to self-reported grades.
grades considering chronotype in order to test the way in which
achievement goals have an influence on school performance in different 2.3. Procedure and data analysis
chronotypes. In this study, the association between achievement goals
and academic performance will be tested per chronotype in order to All participants were tested in groups ranging in size from 20 to 25
avoid masking individual effects related to circadian preferences. As it is students in school schedule and in their own classroom. Assessment
known, it was hypothesised that Learning and Performance Goals took about 40 min.
would be positively related to self-reported grades whereas Social Rein- Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) was run in order to test
forcement Goals would be negatively related. Moreover, M-types would age and sex differences in all variables and Partial correlations (age as
show higher Learning and Performance Goals oriented to learn and ad- covariate) to test the relationship among all variables. MANCOVA
vance in their studies, whereas E-types would show more Social Rein- (controlling for age) was run to test differences in achievement goals
forcement Goals referred to the interest in avoiding rejection from according to chronotype (M-, N- and E-types). Finally, multiple regression
others since they would be less motivated to learn trying to increase analysis was run to detect the contribution of age, sex and achievement
their own competence. goals (Learning, Social Reinforcement and Performance Goals) to self-
reported grades separately for each chronotype. Effect size was calculated
2. Method using the following procedure [ f 2 = R2 / (1 − R2)] (Cohen, 1992).

2.1. Participants 3. Results

Participants in this study were 342 students aged 12–15 (M = 13.23, 3.1. Preliminary analysis: age and sex differences
SD = 0.95) attending three high-schools of Madrid (Spain). 53.5% were
girls. The board of directors authorized the study after obtaining the Regarding age, MANOVA indicated differences in morningness/
parents' consent. Participation was voluntary and anonymous. eveningness, F(3,334) = 4.16, p b .01, η2p = .036; self-reported grades,
F(3,334) = 20.53, p b .001, η2p = 0.156; Learning Goals, F(3,334) =
2.2. Instruments 2.59, p b .05, η2p = 0.23, and Performance Goals, F(3,334) = 5.41,
p b .001, η2p = 0.046. Post-hoc comparisons indicated that pupils aged
2.2.1. Morningness–eveningness 15 showed the lowest Learning and Performance Goals, and reported
The Morningness–Eveningness Scale for Children (MESC, Carskadon, lower morningness than pupils aged 12 or 13; self-reported grades
Vieira, & Acebo, 1993) has 10 items about the preferred timing of certain decreased progressively with age. With respect to sex, girls reported
activities such as free time, tests, sleep timing, and so forth. Items have a higher grades than boys, F(1,334) = 5.47, p b .05, η2p = 0.016, whereas
response scale with four or five options and the total score ranges from boys showed higher Social Reinforcement Goals than girls, F(1,334) =
10 (eveningness) to 43 (morningness). Spanish version was used (Díaz- 4.87, p b .05, η2p = 0.014. No effects of sex ∗ chronotype interaction
Morales, 2015) which showed a satisfactory internal consistency for the were found (see Table 1).
present sample (Cronbach's alpha) was α = 0.73.
3.2. Relationship among variables
2.2.2. Achievement goals
Given that correlations separated by sex were similar, only data for
2.2.2.1. Achievement Goal Tendencies Questionnaire (AGTQ, Hayamizu & total sample are reported. Since age effects were found in preliminary
Weiner, 1991). The AGTQ is a self-report measure which consists of analyses, age was controlled as a covariate. Morningness was positively
20 items to measure three academic goal tendencies: Learning related to self-reported grades (r = .11, p b .05), Learning Goals (r = .36,
Goals, Social Reinforcement Goals, and Performance Goals. Students p b .001), and Performance Goals (r = .23, p b .001); self-reported
rated each item on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never; 5 = always). grades were positively related to Learning Goals (r = .22, p b .001),
C. Escribano, J.F. Díaz-Morales / Personality and Individual Differences 88 (2016) 57–61 59

Table 1 Results indicated that in E-types, the linear combination of age,


Means (M) and Standard Deviations (SD) according to age and sex for morningness/ Social Reinforcement Goals and Performance Goals accounted for
eveningness (M/E), achievement goals and self-reported grades.
43.1% of the variance in self-reported grades indicating a large effect
12 13 14 15 Total size (adjusted R2 = 0.431, f2 = 0.856). Sex and Learning Goals were
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD not significant variables in the model. In N-types, the linear combination
of age, Learning Goals, Social Reinforcement Goals and Performance
M/E
Girls 25.32 3.89 25.61 4.43 25.25 4.64 23.23 4.83 25.23 4.44 Goals accounted for 28.7% of the variance in self-reported grades indi-
Boys 26.88 5.11 25.61 4.05 25.02 4.48 23.80 5.67 25.51 4.85 cating a medium effect size (adjusted R2 = 0.287, f2 = 0.455). Sex
Total 26.21 4.67 25.61 4.27 25.15 4.54 23.00 5.23 25.36 4.63 was not a significant variable in the model. Finally, in M-types, the linear
GPA combination of age, Social Reinforcement Goals and Performance Goals
Girls 7.50 1.21 7.01 1.54 6.76 1.80 5.42 2.15 6.89 1.69
Boys 7.72 1.43 6.23 1.79 5.93 1.70 4.96 1.72 6.46 1.88
accounted for 17.1% of the variance in self-reported grades indicating a
Total 7.62 1.33 6.71 1.68 6.40 1.80 5.17 1.91 6.69 1.79 medium effect size (adjusted R2 = .171, f2 = 0.263). Sex and Learning
LG Goals were not significant variables in the model.
Girls 19.70 6.23 20.04 5.10 20.61 5.42 17.94 4.60 19.93 5.40
Boys 22.46 5.03 19.92 5.63 19.82 6.13 20.48 5.46 20.48 5.69
4. Discussion
Total 21.29 5.70 19.99 5.29 20.26 5.72 20.19 5.01 20.19 5.53
SRG
Girls 11.21 5.13 10.38 5.08 11.06 5.01 11.47 3.99 10.84 4.96 This study demonstrated that morningness/eveningness was related
Boys 14.08 5.45 12.22 5.10 12.02 6.14 11.40 5.48 12.65 5.57 to self-reported grades; M-types reported higher Learning and Perfor-
Total 12.86 5.47 11.09 5.15 11.48 5.53 11.43 4.79 11.68 5.32 mance Goals than E-types, both of which were positively related to
PG
Girls 21.46 3.75 20.94 3.20 21.78 2.38 19.53 4.62 21.15 3.31
self-reported academic performance. Social Reinforcement Goals and
Boys 21.44 2.70 20.39 2.98 20.70 3.24 18.45 5.16 20.55 3.40 Performance Goals were more associated to academic performance
Total 21.45 3.16 20.72 3.12 21.31 2.82 18.95 4.89 20.87 3.36 among E-types, group who reported the lowest Performance Goals
Note: GPA, Grade Point Average (average of self-reported grades for common subjects to scores.
all classes); LG, Learning Goals; SRG, Social Reinforcement Goals; PG, Performance Goals. Inglés et al. (2009) found that general academic success was more
Number of participants (total: girls and boys): 12 (87: 37 and 50), 13 (127: 78 and 49), likely every time that Learning and Performance Goals scores increased,
14 (91: 51 and 40), 15 (37: 17 and 20). whereas was less likely every time that Social Reinforcement Goals
increased. Students with high Learning and Performance Goals are
and Performance Goals (r = .30, p b .01), and negatively related to Social students concerned with increasing their own competence and they
Reinforcement Goals (r = −.16, p b .01). want to learn in order to achieve good results and advance in their
studies. M-types reported higher Learning and Performance Goals
3.3. Differences in achievement goals according to circadian preference than N- and E-types and E-types, respectively. In this line, Önder,
Beşoluk, İskender, Masal, and Demirham (2014) found that academic
MESC values of 22/29, corresponding to 25/75th percentiles, were motivations were better in M-types than N- and E-types without signif-
used to categorize M-, N- and E-types (93, 136 and 113 adolescents, icant differences between N- and E-types. Contrary to what was expect-
respectively). Regarding Learning Goals, M-types showed higher scores ed, in the current study M- and E-types did not differ in Social
than N- and E-types, F(2,335) = 20.01, p b .001, η2p = .107. E-types Reinforcement Goals, in line with Roeser et al. (2013) who found that
showed the lowest scores. chronotypes did not differ in their motivation to appear competent
Regarding Social Reinforcement Goals, no differences among and to conceal incompetence from others, which may be compared
chronotypes were found, F(2,335) = 0.96, p = 0.91. with Social Reinforcement Goals assessed here.
Finally, regarding Performance Goals, M-types showed higher scores As in previous research (Escribano et al., 2012; Tonetti et al., 2015a;
than E-types without significant differences between N-types and M- or Tonetti et al., 2015b), morningness was slightly but positively related
E-types, F(2,335) = 5.23 p b .01, η2p = .030. No sex ∗ chronotype to self-reported grades; furthermore, morningness was positively relat-
interaction effects were found. ed to Learning Goals and Performance Goals, which in turn were related
to self-reported grades. Similar results were obtained by Roeser et al.
3.4. The influence of achievement goals on self-reported grades according to (2013). In their sample, chronotype was not directly related to school
chronotype performance but morningness/eveningness exerted influence on perfor-
mance indirectly by affecting daytime sleepiness and learning motiva-
Previous analyses indicated that self-reported grades were positive- tion. In the same line, Short et al. (2013) found an indirect effect of
ly related to morningness/eveningness and achievement goals, which chronotype in school performance through its relation with depressed
differed among chronotypes. The next step was to test to what extent mood.
achievement goals had an influence on academic performance in In general, being younger, higher scores on Performance Goals and
different chronotypes, also considering age and sex (see Table 2). lower scores on Social Reinforcement Goals were significantly associated

Table 2
Regression analysis on self-reported grades separated by chronotype.

Evening types Neither types Morning types

β t R2 β t R2 β t R2

Age −.521 −6.39⁎⁎⁎ .431 −.291 −3.91⁎⁎⁎ .287 −.262 −3.01⁎⁎ .171
Sex −.098 −1.23 .052 .68 −.176 −1.97
LG .118 1.18 .210 2.28⁎ .152 1.59
SRG −.326 −3.67⁎⁎⁎ −.274 −3.41⁎⁎⁎ −.216 −2.35⁎
PG .257 2.61* .314 3.47⁎⁎⁎ .196 2.02⁎

Note: Sex: 0 = girls; 1 = boys. LG = Learning Goals; SRG = Social Reinforcement Goals; PG = Performance Goals; R2 = Adjusted R2.
⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.
⁎⁎ p b .01.
⁎ p b .05.
60 C. Escribano, J.F. Díaz-Morales / Personality and Individual Differences 88 (2016) 57–61

with higher self-reported grades. It is worth noting the high contribution Acknowledgments
of achievement goals to self-reported grades. Preckel, Holling, and Vock
(2006) found that underachievement correlated with low levels of need This study was supported by both research grants of Ministerio de
for cognition, achievement motivation, facilitating anxiety, and consci- Ciencia e Innovación (Ref: PSI2008-04086/PSIC) and Ministerio de
entiousness; nevertheless, it was best explained by low levels of need Economía y Competitividad (Ref. PSI2011-26967).
for cognition and facilitating anxiety. Academic motivation was a signif-
icant predictor of academic outcomes after controlling for traditional
predictors related to academic performance; specifically, intelligence References
and intrinsic motivation predicted school grades (R2 = .26) (Vecchioni,
Adan, A., Archer, S. N., Hidalgo, M. P., Di Milia, L., Natale, V., & Randler, C. (2012). Circadian
Alessandri, & Marsicano, 2014). Önder et al. (2014) found a medium typology: A comprehensive review. Chronobiology International, 29, 1153–1175.
effect size (predictors explaining 15.1% of the variance of Cumulative Arbabi, T., Vollmer, C., Dörfler, T., & Randler, C. (2015). The influence of chronotype
GPA) considering corrected mid-point of sleep, academic motivation, so- and intelligence on academic achievement in primary school is mediated by
conscientiousness, midpoint of sleep and motivation. Chronobiology International,
cial jetlag, conscientiousness, intrinsic motivation to surpass themselves,
32, 349–357.
intrinsic motivation to experience pleasant sensations, and neuroticism. Beşoluk, Ş. (2011). Morningness–eveningness preferences and university entrance
In the present study, in which regression model was run split into examination scores of high school students. Personality and Individual Differences,
chronotype, achievement goals appear to have a greater role on self- 50, 248–252.
Beşoluk, Ş., Önder, I., & Deveci, I. (2011). Morningness–eveningness preference and
reported grades among E-types, since the regression model accounted academic achievement of university students. Chronobiology International, 28,
for 43% of the variance in self-reported grades compared to 17% of 118–125.
explained variance in M-types. In both extreme chronotypes, Learning Carskadon, M. A., Vieira, C., & Acebo, C. (1993). Association between puberty and delayed
phase preference. Sleep, 16, 258–262.
Goals related to student's concerns about increasing their own compe- Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality predicts academic
tence, were not significant predictors. It could be explained because performance: Evidence from two longitudinal university samples. Journal of
Learning Goals and task value are more distinct across academic Research in Personality, 37, 319–338.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
domains as Inglés et al. (2009) described when considering the Crowley, S. J., Van Reen, E., LeBourgeois, M. K., Acebo, C., Tarokh, L., Seifer, R., ... Carskadon,
predictive power of Learning Goals for general performance (number M. A. (2014). A longitudinal assessment of sleep timing, circadian phase, and phase
of failing grades at the end of the school year) but not for specific subjects angle of entrainment across human adolescence. PloS One, 9, e112199 http://dx.doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112199.
(Spanish language and maths); nevertheless, the reason why in this Dewald, J. F., Meijer, A. M., Oort, F. J., Kerkhof, G. A., & Bögels, S. M. (2010). The influence of
study Learning Goals were more associated to self-reported grades sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and
among N-types needs to be further explored. adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 14, 179–189.
Díaz-Morales, J. F., & Escribano, C. (2013). Circadian preference and thinking styles:
Differences among chronotypes in personality traits might explain
Implications for school achievement. Chronobiology International, 30, 1231–1239.
why E-types tend to get lower school grades. Students who are intellec- Díaz-Morales, J. F. (2007). Morning and evening-types: Exploring their personality styles.
tually curious, disciplined, organized and show low scores on neuroti- Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 769–778.
cism tended to have higher academic motivation (Önder et al., 2014). Díaz-Morales, J. F. (2015). Morningness–Eveningness Scale for Children (MESC): Spanish
normative data and factorial invariance according to sex and age. Personality and
Academic performance has been found to correlate positively with Individual Differences, 87, 116–120.
agreeableness and conscientiousness (Poropat, 2009), more frequent Díaz-Morales, J. F., & Escribano, C. (2014). Consequences of adolescent's evening
among M-types, while neuroticism and psychoticism, more typical preferences on psychological functioning: A review. Anales de Psicología, 30,
1096–1104.
of E-types, may impair academic success (Chamorro-Premuzic & Díaz-Morales, J. F., & Escribano, C. (2015). Social jetlag, academic achievement and
Furnham, 2003). E-type adolescents show a higher preference toward cognitive performance: Understanding sex/gender differences. Chronobiology
individual values and are the ones with the highest risk for being International, 32, 822–831.
Díaz-Morales, J. F., Escribano, C., & Jankowski, K. (2015). Chronotype and time-of-day
engaged in non-normative behaviours (Muro et al., 2012; Vollmer & effects on mood during school day. Chronobiology International, 32, 37–42.
Randler, 2012) what may affect the way in which they cope with school Díaz-Morales, J. F., Escribano, C., Jankowski, K., Vollmer, C., & Randler, C. (2014). Evening
performance. adolescents: The role of family relationships and pubertal development. Journal of
Adolescence, 37, 425–432.
According to Roeser et al. (2013) M-types presented stronger learn- Díaz-Morales, J. F., Ferrari, J. R., & Cohen, J. R. (2008). Indecision and avoidant procrastination:
ing motivation whereas refusal to work, which was associated to poorer The role of morningness–eveningness and time perspective in chronic delay lifestyles.
school performance, was more pronounced in E-types. In the same line, The Journal of General Psychology, 135, 228–240.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41,
avoidant procrastination is related to eveningness, that is to say, E-types
1040–1048.
tend to avoid some duties during the day making them less conscien- Escribano, C., & Díaz-Morales, J. F. (2014a). Daily fluctuations at school considering
tious and less likely to fulfil certain tasks which require immediate starting time and chronotype: An exploratory study. Chronobiology International, 31,
attention (Díaz-Morales, Ferrari, & Cohen, 2008). 761–769.
Escribano, C., & Díaz-Morales, J. F. (2014b). Are self-reported grades a good estimate of
This study has some limitations that require further research. First of academic achievement? Studies in Psychology, 35, 168–182.
all, academic performance was self-reported. Although recent studies Escribano, C., Díaz-Morales, J. F., Delgado, P., & Collado, M. J. (2012). Morningness–
found that self-reported grades are a good estimate of actual grades eveningness and school performance among Spanish adolescents: Further evidence.
Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 409–413.
(Escribano & Díaz-Morales, 2014b), it would be interesting to replicate Fabbri, M., Antonietti, A., Giorgetti, M., Tonetti, L., & Natale, V. (2007). Circadian typology
these findings by using actual grades. Another limitation is the sample and style of thinking differences. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 175–180.
size and its reduced age range (secondary students). Since the associa- Fallone, G., Acebo, C., Seifer, R., & Carskadon, M. (2005). Experimental restriction of sleep
opportunity in children: Effects on teacher ratings. Sleep, 28, 1561–1567.
tion between academic performance and evening preference changes Hagenauer, M. H., Perryman, J. I., Lee, T. M., & Carskadon, M. A. (2009). Adolescent
over time (Tonetti et al., 2015a; Tonetti et al., 2015b), the large effect changes in the homeostatic and circadian regulation of sleep. Developmental
size of the academic goals in school performance among E-types should Neuroscience, 31, 276–284.
Hayamizu, T., & Weiner, B. (1991). A test of Dweck's model of achievement goals a related
be replicated using well-known predictors of academic performance in
to perceptions of ability. The Journal of Experimental Education, 59, 226–234.
a higher sample with a wider age range. The reason why Learning and Hayamizu, T., Ito, A., & Yoshizaki, K. (1989). Cognitive motivational processes mediated by
Performance Goals are lower among E-types needs to be further achievement goal tendencies. Japanese Psychological Research, 31, 179–189.
Horzum, M. B., Önder, I., & Beşoluk, Ş. (2014). Chronotype and academic achievement
explored.
among online learning students. Learning and Individual Differences, 30, 106–111.
Inglés, C. J., García-Fernández, J. M., Castejón, J. L., Valle, A., Delgado, B., & Marzo, J. C.
(2009). Reliability and validity evidence of scores on the achievement goal
Conflict of interest tendencies questionnaire in a sample of Spanish students of compulsory secondary
education. Psychology in the Schools, 46, 1048–1060.
Kanazawa, S., & Perina, K. (2009). Why night owls are more intelligent. Personality and
The authors indicate no conflicts of interest. Individual Differences, 47, 685–690.
C. Escribano, J.F. Díaz-Morales / Personality and Individual Differences 88 (2016) 57–61 61

Muro, A., Gomà-i-Freixanet, M., & Adan, A. (2012). Circadian typology and sensation Short, M. A., Gradisar, M., Lack, L. C., & Wright, H. R. (2013). The impact of sleep on
seeking in adolescents. Chronobiology International, 29, 1376–1382. adolescent depress mood, alertness and academic performance. Journal of Adolescence,
Önder, I., Beşoluk, Ş., İskender, M., Masal, E., & Demirham, E. (2014). Circadian 36, 1025–1033.
preferences, sleep quality and sleep patterns, personality, academic motivation and Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2009). The importance of motivation as predictor of school
academic achievement of university students. Learning and Individual Differences, achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 80–90.
32, 184–192. Tonetti, L., Adan, A., Di Milia, L., Randler, C., & Natale, V. (2015a). Measures of circadian
Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and preference in childhood and adolescence: A review. European Psychiatry, 30,
academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 322–338. 576–582.
Preckel, F., Holling, H., & Vock, M. (2006). Academic underachievement: Relationship Tonetti, L., Natale, V., & Randler, C. (2015b). Association between circadian preference and
with cognitive motivation, achievement motivation, and conscientiousness. academic achievement: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Chronobiology
Psychology in the Schools, 43, 401–411. International, 32, 792–801.
Preckel, F., Lipnevich, A. A., Boehme, K., Brandner, L., Georgi, K., Könen, T., ... Roberts, R. D. Tsaousis, I. (2010). Circadian preferences and personality traits. European Journal of
(2013). Morningness–eveningness and educational outcomes: The lark has an Personality, 24, 356–373.
advantage over the owl at high school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, Vecchioni, M., Alessandri, G., & Marsicano, G. (2014). Academic motivation predicts
114–134. educational attainment: Does gender make a difference? Learning and Individual
Randler, C., & Frech, D. (2009). Young people's time-of-day preferences affect their school Differences, 32, 124–131.
performance. Journal of Youth Studies, 12, 653–667. Vollmer, C., & Randler, C. (2012). Circadian preferences and personality values: Morning
Randler, C., & Saliger, L. (2011). Relationship between morningness–eveningness and types prefer social values, evening types individual values. Personality and
temperament and character dimensions in adolescents. Personality and Individual Individual Differences, 52, 738–743.
Differences, 50, 148–152. Vollmer, C., Pötsch, F., & Randler, C. (2013). Morningness is associated with better
Roberts, R. D., & Kyllonen, P. C. (1999). Morningness–eveningness and intelligence: Early gradings and higher attention in class. Learning and Individual Differences, 27,
to bed, early to rise will likely make you anything but wise! Personality and Individual 167–173.
Differences, 27, 1123–1133. Wolfson, A., & Carskadon, M. (2003). Understanding adolescents' sleep patterns and
Roeser, K., Schlarb, A. A., & Kübler, A. (2013). The Chronotype-Academic Performance school performance: A critical appraisal. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 7, 491–506.
Model (CAM): Daytime sleepiness and learning motivation link chronotype and
school performance in adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 836–840.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen