Sie sind auf Seite 1von 15

Diagrammatic INTRODUCTION

information:
techniques for Pictorial displays are widely considered to be a
exploring its solution to communication problems rather
mental repres- than a source of such problems. While this view
entation and processing is probably quite reasonable for what could be
described as 'realistic' depictions of everyday
subject matter, it may not be the case with other
more abstract and specialised types of pictorial
presentation. This paper focuses upon the latter
class of pictures, in particular technical dia-
Richard K. Lowe grams. It describes two qualitatively different
methodologies that have been developed to
investigate the way individuals with different
Diagrams are increasingly levels of expertise in a specific technical domain
used to present complex and mentally represent diagrams which are charac-
abstract information. Their ulti­ teristic of that domain. Weather map diagrams
mate success as tools for
were chosen as illustrative examples of a specific
communication depends large­
ly upon how effectively they genre or class of technical diagram, and the
can be processed in the mind research for which these methodologies were
of the viewer. The application developed involved comparisons of professional
of established principles of meteorologists and non-meteorologists.
graphic design is a vital part of
developing effective diagrams,
Whereas in the past the use of diagrams as a
but tends to focus upon exter­ means of presenting information tended to be
nal aspects of representation confined largely to certain skilled individuals Acknowledgements
that apply at a general level working in a few quite specialised domains, T h e co-operation and support
across a wide range of subject of the director and staff of the
nowadays they are used to inform a wide range Western Australian Regional
domains. However, the inter­
nal (mental) representation of
of people about many different types of subject Office of the Bureau of Meteor-
a specific set of subject matter matter. In particular, diagrams are now also ology is gratefully acknow-
ledged. T h e author also thanks
is also important in influencing used as a way to present directions, instructions
William Chamberlain for his
what sense viewers make of a or explanations to individuals who may have no fine work in assisting with the
diagram. The task of charac­ particular training in how to deal with this form conversion to a HyperCard for-
terising relationships between mat of the manual versions of
of presentation (see Lowe, 1991a) and who are
the way a diagram is repre­ the techniques described here.
sented mentally and the effec­ professionally unskilled in the content area This paper is based upon a lec-
tiveness with which that dia­ addressed by the diagram. This increased ture given at the 5th Informa-
gram is processed poses reliance upon diagrams has occurred largely tion Design Conference,
novel challenges to research­ London, April 1992.
independently of research into how people in
ers. This paper decribes some
general interact with information presented in
of these challenges and dis­ Author's address
cusses methodologies that this format. Rather, it appears to be based upon
Faculty of Education
have been developed to a belief that diagrams have an intrinsic capacity Curtin University
explore the mental representa­ to make information accessible. While a good GPO Box U 1987
tion and processing of explan­ Perth 6 0 0 1 , Western Australia
diagram undoubtedly has enormous potential
atory diagrams.
in this regard, the realisation of this potential

Information Design Journal 7:1 (1993), 3-17. DOI 10.1075/idj.7.1.011ow


ISSN 0142-5471 / E-ISSN 1569-979X © John Benjamins Publishing Company
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

ultimately depends on the capacity of the indi- itself account for how effective it will be as a way
vidual to make effective use of that portrayal. of communicating with naïve users. In addition,
there are many diagrams that are a firmly en-
trenched part of the graphic heritage of a partic-
DIAGRAM EFFECTIVENESS
ular knowledge domain. As a consequence,
those who wish to find out about that domain
are obliged to develop competence in dealing
The effectiveness of a given diagram in fulfilling
with such diagrams without the opportunity of
a particular role depends both on the quality of
obtaining alternative (and possibly more acces-
its design and on the characteristics of those
sible) forms of depiction. T o a greater or lesser
who will use the diagram as a source of informa-
extent, the success of a diagram with a particu-
tion. A wealth of experience and insight con-
lar audience is only partly due to the diagram's
cerning the design of diagrams has accumulated
design because the capacities of the audience
amongst design practitioners over the years
and the approaches they use in trying to under-
(Macdonald-Ross, 1989). This has led both to
stand the diagram also help to determine its
the development of more powerful techniques
effectiveness. Thus, a research agenda for im-
of portrayal (such as the use of explanatory
proving the effectiveness of diagrams as a means
views and transformations discussed by
of communication needs to include a study of
Edgerton, 1980) and to the introduction of a
the potential users, as well as the quality of the
range of specialised diagrammatic conventions
diagrams. The former study is particularly
and symbols that help to clarify the subject mat-
important within an instructional context in
ter (Gilbert & Mulkay, 1984). Improvements in
which the users are, by definition, naive, and
the design of diagrams come not only from the
diagrams are employed to present unfamiliar
efforts of design practitioners themselves but
content with the intention of making the
also from their professional interaction with
subject matter more accessible.
subject matter experts. For example, dialogue
between a subject matter expert and the design-
er during the development of a given diagram
ensures that it correctly shows all necessary INVESTIGATING DIAGRAMS IN USE
aspects of the content to be portrayed. During
this interaction, the way in which the content is Because the effectiveness of a diagram depends
depicted can be refined to increase the clarity on the way the audience uses it as well as on the
and focus of the diagram. characteristics of the diagram itself, it is impor-
However, although a subject matter expert is tant to investigate the way people interact with
an authority with respect to the correctness of diagrams. Some broad indications of how users
the content depicted, s/he is not automatically deal with a particular diagram can of course be
able to tell what non-expert users will make of gained by conducting trials with an appropriate
the diagram. Neither is it reasonable to expect group. Approaches such as collecting the opin-
the designer of a diagram to offer any more than ions of target users, informally observing the
a very general opinion on how to help ensure behaviour of these users when working with a
that the diagram will suit a non-specialist audi- diagram, studying users' errors, and comparing
ence. Indeed, there is a limit in the extent to responses to various versions of a diagram can
which the quality of a diagram's design can of all be used to refine a particular diagram so that

Information design journal 7/1 (1993) 3-17

4
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

it better suits its intended audience. These by examining diagrams themselves, without
macro-level investigations can all be used to necessarily looking at the behaviour of diagram
supplement the designer's professional judge- users.
ment in developing an effective diagram. U n - However, when we consider the way a dia-
fortunately, investigations at this broad level are gram is actually used by an individual seeking
limited in the type of information they can information, another type of representation
reveal. In particular, they provide little indica- needs to be considered. This is the internal
tion of what is going on inside the mind of the mental representation that the viewer builds
viewer as the diagram is being interpreted. T h e during interaction with the diagram. An indi-
need is for finer-grained micro-level informa- vidual's mental representation of a particular
tion about the way diagrams are mentally repre- diagram is the basis for the way that diagram is
sented and processed. This type of information processed and ultimately understood (or not, as
has the potential to help answer some questions the case may be). If an appropriate mental rep-
that may otherwise be difficult to address, par- resentation is constructed from the diagram by
ticularly those which involve tacit knowledge or the viewer, then his or her interpretation of the
knowledge that is intrinsically non-verbal in entities and relations portrayed in the diagram
nature. will bear a workable correspondence to the situ-
ation that the diagram's author intended the
diagram to depict. It will thus provide a reliable
basis for thought or action with respect to that
DIAGRAMS AND REPRESENTATION
situation. Unfortunately, even very fine dia-
grams can be incomprehensible to the uninitiat-
Diagrams are abstract graphic portrayals of the
ed or can lead to misunderstandings which may
subject matter they represent. The actual enti-
result in a person responding inappropriately.
ties and relations that make up the subject mat-
As mentioned above, diagrams use a range of
ter in the real world are represented by various
quite characteristic transformations and con-
visuo-spatial characteristics of the diagram.
ventions that are not found in 'everyday' visual
However, the mapping of the subject matter
representations. Unless a viewer is able to deal
into the diagram is rarely completely straight-
with these appropriately, s/he will not develop
forward and typically involves various types of
the type of meaning for the diagram that was
transformation. Apart from the obvious trans-
intended by its author.
formation from three to two dimensions, there
is a range of other types of transformation that
have been developed, such as those that allow
non-spatial dimensions (time, semantic similar- MENTAL REPRESENTATION
ity, etc.) to be represented in a spatial manner.
A comprehensive study of diagrams as a means Although little empirical work has been done to
of presenting information needs to consider investigate the way diagrams are represented in
these transformational conventions and what people's minds, there has been considerable
they demand of the viewer. Because they are investigation in recent years of the mental rep-
external graphic representations, this aspect of resentation of information presented in other
diagrams is readily accessible to systematic formats, particularly text. Much of this work
study (given appropriate analytical frameworks) has involved comparing the different ways in

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-17

5
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

which people who are experts and novices in a ed to them. For example, expert physicists men-
particular knowledge domain work with infor- tally represent the information presented in
mation which is specific to that domain. An physics word problems according to the under-
important motivation for comparing these two lying physics principles that are involved (Chi,
groups is the search for ways that may help Feltovich & Glaser, 1981; Larkin, 1983). In con-
novices handle the information more effectively trast, novices represent the same information in
(in other words, in a more expert-like fashion). terms of its superficial characteristics, such as
In many cases, expert-novice comparison the types of arrangements or objects that are
investigations have studied how these two dif- described in each problem. They do not see
ferent types of individual deal with problem through the 'cover story' provided by the prob-
solving tasks. Chi, Glaser and Farr (1988) pro- lem description to the deeper structure that
vide a useful overview of some of this research involves fundamental physics concepts. As a
and also summarise a number of characteristics consequence, they are less successful in dealing
that have been found to distinguish the way with the problems because they base their cog-
experts and novices deal with domain-specific nitive processing upon an inappropriate mental
information. Of particular relevance to the pre- representation of the information.
sent article is the role that an individual's exist- Although most of the research comparing the
ing knowledge of a specialist domain plays in mental representations of experts and novices
the processing of a given set of information has involved text, the few investigations that
from that domain. Not surprisingly, research have dealt with 'pictorial' material have pro-
confirms that experts in a domain have a vast duced findings which parallel the ideas men-
knowledge base of information that is specific tioned above. These include studies of the
to their particular field. However, the amount processing of schematic diagrams of basketball
of knowledge experts have is not the only way plays (Allard & Burnett, 1985), electronic cir-
they differ from novices. As well as these quan- cuit diagrams (Egan & Schwartz, 1979), bird
titative differences, experts' knowledge bases identification pictures (Peeck & Zwarts, 1983),
have fundamental qualitative differences from figure skating patterns (Deakin & Allard, 1991)
those possessed by novices and are organised and architectural drawings (Akin, 1986). In
differently. It is not simply a case of novices' none of these cases can the capacity of experts
knowledge bases being 'watered-down' versions be explained solely by their possession of vast
of those possessed by experts. amounts of knowledge. It is the nature and
T h e knowledge bases in the heads of experts organisation of this knowledge that appears to
emphasise types of entities (individual items of be central to their expertise and leads them to
information) which are different from those of mentally represent external information in a
novices, and contain different types of relations distinctive fashion.
between those entities. Experts organise their The research methods reported in this article
knowledge according to conceptual entities, grew out of previous expert-novice research and
and their knowledge bases feature clusters of provide additional, more detailed approaches
information that have domain-specific mean- for exploring the mental representation and
ings, characteristics which novices lack. These processing of diagrams. Their focus was in
differences in knowledge base are associated some ways different from other approaches in
with differences in the way experts and novices that they were designed to collect detailed non-
mentally represent information that is present- verbal data about the course of diagram pro-

information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-17

6
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

cessing rather than being confined to products with (a) whether or not the viewers will find the
of that processing. Of particular interest was the significant parts of the information, and with
basis upon which the mental representation was (b) what the viewers mentally do with the infor-
constructed as individuals with different levels mation they find. The methods described in this
of expertise worked with a diagram. This raises article could be adapted to help information
two important questions. designers at various stages of the design process.
First, were there differences in how the For example, they could give the designer feed-
different types of individual explored the dia- back on how effective a diagram was in helping
gram's contents that may indicate the extent to the audience develop the desired understand-
which their search strategies were likely to ings. This feedback could be gathered as both
locate critical components of the information? product data (what happened overall as a result
This question is important because if novices in of a viewer's interaction with the diagram) and
a particular knowledge domain fail to explore a process data (how a viewer worked through the
diagram's contents in a productive fashion, they diagram and where interpretation may have
would probably be unable to develop an appro- gone astray).
priate mental representation of the information
it depicts. Since the mental representation of
presented information is the basis for the degree METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES
and type of understanding that results, it is very
important that this initial information-gathering
Finding out about the mental representation
stage of processing is effective.
that a viewer constructs from a diagram (inside
Second, was the information depicted in the
the viewer's head) is naturally much more
diagram represented in the viewer's mind sim-
difficult than it is to study the diagram itself.
ply in terms of its visuo-spatial characteristics
This poses a significant research challenge.
(a superficial form of mental representation that
There are a number of important methodologi-
would be expected for novices) or in terms of its
cal issues that need to be considered in studying
domain-specific significance (a deeper, more
the mental representation of this type of materi-
principled mental representation that would be
al. Some of these issues will be considered,
expected for experts)? This question is impor-
before discussing the methods that have been
tant because if novices mentally represent the
developed in the present research.
diagram in a superficial fashion, it calls into
The first issue arises because diagrams are
question the privileged explanatory status that
often a combination of graphic material and
seems to be accorded to diagrammatic informa-
text (captions and labels). While acknowledging
tion. If novice viewers mentally represent the
the importance of this type of combination,
information they pick up from a diagram on a
studying a mixture of graphics and text would
basis that is qualitatively different from that
make it difficult to distinguish between the con-
used by experts, it seems unreasonable to
tribution of each form to the constructed men-
assume that they will develop the diagram's
tal representation. Considering the continuing
intended meaning.
debate over whether pictorial material is dealt
The implications of these two questions for
with in the mind as images or as propositions
information designers who deal with diagrams
(Paivio, 1986), it seems prudent to deal with
can be highlighted by paraphrasing them some-
graphics separately from text in the first
what more informally. They are concerned

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-17

7
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

instance. Therefore, in the research that forms founding through differences in drawing skill,
the basis for this paper, the focus was upon the second method to be described here, that
graphic constituents only. The weather map relied extensively on drawn output, also includ-
diagram genre was chosen for investigation ed a control drawing task.
because it was a type of diagram that could The final issue to be discussed here concerns
stand alone without any textual adjuncts. analysis and interpretation of the results. As the
However, a focus upon the graphic constituents study of mental representation deals with subtle
of diagrams raises the problem of how to tap the matters that are not available to direct inspec-
mental representation of that type of informa- tion, considerable inference is required. There
tion in a manner which, as far as possible, is an absence of direct precedents for this type
acknowledges its visual nature. Much of the of diagram investigation which makes it even
work that has been done to date in exploring more important than usual to be explicit about
mental representation has involved the investi- the assumptions made when the results are
gation of textual materials and has relied on col- analysed and interpreted. In general, it was
lecting subjects' written or verbal output as assumed that the way a subject's output is
evidence for mental representation (e.g. Erics- organised during performance of a diagram pro-
son & Simon, 1984). It would clearly be inap- cessing task reflects the way in which that indi-
propriate, when investigating diagrams, to vidual's mental representation of the task
collect data primarily in textual or verbal form content is structured. By this assumption, sub-
if we wish to avoid the distortions and modifica- jects' performance during the task gives an indi-
tions that inevitably occur when an attempt is cation of the cognitive operations which they
made to transform visual information into the are performing to complete the task. In addi-
written or spoken word. So the methods tion, these operations are in turn constrained by
described below required subjects to produce the type of mental representation they use dur-
more appropriate forms of output, either by ing that processing.
physically manipulating given diagrammatic
material, or by producing their own drawings.
The second issue flows from the first in that
THE METHODS
the methods described below incorporate draw-
ing tasks for the subjects. This raises the ques-
tion of the extent to which the results obtained Two qualitatively different methods developed
by these methods would reflect the processing for investigating the mental representation and
involved in a subject's cognitive operations with processing of diagrams will now be described in
her or his mental representation, rather than general terms. The intention in using methods
simply reflecting the physical processing that were qualitatively different was to deter-
involved in performing a drawing task (motor mine features that characterise underlying men-
skill, for example). Would the results obtained tal representation and processing rather than
be largely an artifact of the methodology or methodological artifacts. Although the focus of
would they indicate something significant about this paper is upon the basic procedure used in
mental representation? Studies by van Sommers each type of investigation, a brief account of
(1984) have shown that both physical and cog- selected findings is also included. Further
nitive factors can influence the outcomes of details of the theoretical framework and find-
drawing tasks. Due to the possibility of con- ings are discussed elsewhere (Lowe, 1989 and

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-17

8
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

Lowe, 1991b). The paper concludes with some meteorological markings already drawn upon it
comments about the challenges presented in and the subject glued this square onto its cor-
analysing and interpreting the data produced rect location on the blank outline map. How-
from these methods. ever, subjects had a strict limit on the amount of
information they were permitted to build up in
Restricted sampling method this way, so that they also eventually had to con-
This method required the subjects to sample tribute some information themselves. Before
segments of a weather map (that was hidden they began to nominate segments, subjects were
from their view) to help them work out the pat- told that: (a) they would be allowed to have only
tern of meteorological markings it contained. one third of the information which populated
When subjects began the task, they were faced the map (i.e. they were to choose 10 out of the
with a blank outline map of Australia. This 30 map segments as the ones they wished to be
map, which is shown in figure 1, was divided given); and that (b) once the quota of 10
into a grid of 30 individual approximately squares had been used up, the subject was to
square-shaped segments. complete the map by drawing in any markings
Subjects were told their ultimate task was to considered necessary to make the weather map
build up a complete weather map diagram on as complete as possible (so that it corresponded
the blank map to reproduce as closely as possi- closely to the whole map that was hidden from
ble the weather map which the experimenter the subject's view). This meant that the whole
had hidden from their view. The way the sub- task was divided into two distinct stages: a 'map
ject did this was by nominating segments one at search' stage, followed by a 'map completion'
a time that s/he wished to receive for the com- stage. Because subjects were allowed to see only
plete (marked) weather map. As each segment 10 of the segments in all, it clearly was impor-
Figure 1 tant for them to make their selection in a way
was nominated, the experimenter gave the sub-
Segmented that used this limited choice as effectively as
blank output ject a small square of paper which was a copy of
that single segment. The square had the missing possible. In essence, they had to carry out a
map.
search process that they thought would give
A1 A2 A3 A5 A6 them their best chance of completing the map
A4
properly. The main types of data used in the
analysis were (a) the order in which segments
B1 B2 B4 B5
were chosen, (b) the distribution of these seg-
B3 B6
ments across the map, and (c) the characteris-
tics of the completed map drawing. Analysis of
these data was used as a basis for making infer-
C1 C2 C3 C4 C5 C6
ences about the nature of the mental represen-
tation of subjects with different levels of
expertise in reading weather maps.
D1 D2 D3 D5 D6
D4 This paper-based methodology has recently
been transformed into a version that runs on the
Macintosh computer (in HyperCard). The
E1 E2 E3 E4 E5 E6 transformation has made the process of collect-
ing data faster, far more independent of the
experimenter, and easy to modify or extend to

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-17

9
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

include new aspects such as a sequence of results of that construction process were in turn
weather maps. Instead of nominating segments being used to develop a new external physical
to the experimenter, the subject now simply representation of the map.
selects by clicking a grid square on the screen As suggested above, a possible confounding
image of the segmented weather map. This influence (upon both copying and recall perfor-
causes the square to turn black, indicating that mance as an indicator of mental representation)
it has been selected. Upon confirming the selec- is the level of drawing competence of the
tion with a second click on the blackened different groups of subjects. Because profes-
square, the hidden portion of the weather map sional meteorologists are daily engaged in draw-
markings is revealed. The program records the ing up weather maps as part of their normal
sequence and location of segment choices, and working routine, it is likely that they will have
prints a copy of the subject's partly completed become skilled in the spatial judgements and
map for her or him to draw in the remaining physical manipulations required to draw the
markings. types of graphic elements found in weather
maps. In contrast, the non-meteorologists
Copy-recall method would probably lack these skills and so be at a
This method relied more heavily on the sub- disadvantage when it came to tasks such as
jects' drawing behaviour than did the previous making accurate copies of these displays. Before
method. The experimental task required sub- accepting data based upon drawn output as a
jects first to copy and later to recall a given source of evidence for the mental representation
weather map. The copying part of the task was of weather maps, this possible source of con-
designed to generate observations of subject founding needs to be addressed. T o this end, a
performance that could be used to infer the way control task was used in which subjects copied a
a mental representation of a specific weather non-weather map display (a stylised lakeside
map was gradually developed. The recall task scene) that was specially developed to contain
was designed to fulfil two main purposes. First, the types of graphic elements found in weather
it simply served the function of indicating the maps. Figures 2 and 3 respectively show the
effectiveness of storage and retrieval of informa- weather map and lakeside scene used as the
tion from the copied weather map. Second, it experimental and control displays.
was intended to provide further information If differences in drawing skill were largely
about the general nature of the mental repre- responsible for any differences between the
sentation that subjects constructed as a result of accuracy of the meteorologists and non-meteo-
the copying task. Comparison of the way map rologists, this would be reflected in a simple way
information is processed when the map is across the control and experimental tasks. As
copied with the way it is processed during recall well as providing for any differences in drawing
has the potential to reveal characteristics of skill, the control drawing was also the basis for
mental representation that are not apparent an intervening activity performed between map
when each process is examined in isolation. For copying and recall. This activity involved the
example, it would be of interest to determine subjects describing their copy of the control dis-
any similarities and differences between the play so as to prevent them recalling map infor-
processing used in a task in which given weather mation from short term memory.
map information was being used to construct a In the original version of this method, sub-
mental representation, and that in which mental jects were seated at a table and provided with an

Information design journal 7/1 (1993) 3-17

10
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

Figure 2 A Figure 3
4 sheet that was blank except for a map outline of the time which was required for data tran-
Original (or a lake outline for the control task). In front scription in the previous version. In addition, it Original lake­
weather map side scene for
of the subject was a back-projection screen with will make it easy to include new types of mea-
for copy task. control task.
a slide projector behind it. The image to be sures such as the direction in which the graphic
copied was projected onto the screen only when elements are drawn.
the subject pressed a 'viewing button' which
opened an electonically controlled shutter on
the slide projector. The procedure used forced
ILLUSTRATIVE FINDINGS
subjects to interrupt their drawing in order to
view the image so that measures of 'viewing'
Although the main focus of this article is upon
and 'drawing' behaviour constituted two dis-
the methods themselves, some brief examples
tinct data sets. Subjects' drawing behaviour
will be used to illustrate the types of informa-
during the procedure was videotaped to obtain
tion they can generate. While these findings
data about relationships involving their viewing
arise from an experimental rather than a design
and drawing times. This procedure was limited
context, they give an indication of the potential
in the type of data that could be collected, and
these methods have to uncover otherwise hid-
transcription of the videotape was extremely
den aspects of diagram processing. The meth-
time consuming. A HyperCard version of this
ods are robust and could be readily adapted to a
technique is currently under development
variety of design investigations. More extensive
which makes use of a graphics tablet so that
descriptions of these investigations and their
drawing behaviour can be recorded directly into
findings are given elsewhere (Lowe, 1989,
data files. It will not only make the task far sim-
1991b).
pler to administer but will also eliminate much

Information design journal 7/1 (1993) 3-17

11
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

The first set of results summarises some of Figure 5 summarises trends in the order in
the data obtained from the restricted sampling which various regions of the weather map dia-
method. Figure 4 is a graph of the inspection gram were searched. It deals with those regions
frequencies for the various rows of the grid used that were selected by 25 per cent or more of the
to partition the diagram. It provides a general subjects only, and is divided arbitrarily into
level of information about where the different 'early,' 'mid', or 'late' stages of the search
types of subjects decided to concentrate their sequence. Comparison of the exploration pat-
attention when exploring the unseen weather terns produced for the different types of subject
map diagram. One suggestion that arises from across these three stages suggests that they may
this graph is that the meteorologists and non- have different priorities in their information-
meteorologists have different ideas about which gathering activities.
regions of this type of diagram are potentially T h e second sample of results comes from the
most informative. copy-recall method. Figure 6 summarises infor-
mation about clustering of meteorological
markings on the weather map diagram that
arose from subjects' viewing patterns as they
performed the copying task.
The shaded and unshaded 'halves' of the dia-
grams show differences in the broad groupings
resulting from the application of a statistical
cluster analysis technique to the meteorologist
and non-meteorologist data. Verbal data
obtained using 'think-aloud' procedures (that
were a further aspect of the investigation using
the restricted sampling method) suggest that
these differences may reflect fundamental quali-
tative differences in the way the two types of
subjects mentally represent weather map dia-
grams. It seems that the non-meteorologists'
Figure 4 ↑ east-west subdivision of the map is more consis-
Total square tent with the visuo-spatial characteristics of the
A1 A2 A3 A4 A5 A6
inspection fre­ markings or with some other domain-general
quency per
row. The black
influence, rather than with domain-specific
B1 B2 B3 B4 B5 B6 meteorological influences. In contrast, the
columns repre­
sent results for meteorologists' north-south subdivision sug-
the meteorolo­ gests two distinct broad-scale meteorological
C1 C2 C3 C4 C5 rC6
gist group; the- influences that apply at lower (northern) and
white columns
higher (southern) latitudes respectively.
show results
D1 D2 D3 D5 D6
for the non- D4
meteorologist
group. (Right, a
reminder of the E1 E2 E3 E4 E5 E6
5 rows, A to E)

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-11

12
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

Figure 5 Non-meteorologists Meteorologists


Square choices in
early, mid, and late
phases of selec­
tion sequence with
choice frequency of
25% or more. Light
shading indicates
segments already
Early
selected.

Mid

Late

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-17

13
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

Figure 6 Non-meteorologists Meteorologists


Broad group­
ings of meteo­
rological
elements
obtained from
cluster analy­
sis of subjects'
viewing pat­
terns for (left)
non-meteorolo­
gists, and
(right) meteo­
rologists.

Figure 7 Non-meteorologists Meteorologists


Average order
in which mark­
ings were
copied by (left)
non-meteorolo­
gists, and
(right) meteo­
rologists.
Darker shading
indicates earli­
er production.

Figure 7 gives a visual summary of the aver- (e.g. the markings' shapes and positions). In
age order in which the 34 markings that made contrast, the meteorologists' pattern is more
up the weather map diagram were copied by the complex. It begins with a first sweep that starts
subjects. Here, the level of grey used to draw at the bottom right and identifies the position of
markings indicates their relative positions in the major meteorological features using a few mark-
drawing sequence (the darker the marking, the ings which act as landmarks (shown as the dark-
earlier it was drawn). For the non-meteorolo- est elements in the pattern). In a second stage
gists, there is a simple and regular progression this 'skeleton' of markings is then fleshed out in
for the marking sequence which starts at the top a different sweep around the map. This pro-
left hand corner and sweeps around the map in gression is consistent with a domain-specific
a clockwise direction. This progression appears characterisation of the graphic elements that
to be correlated with the domain-general visuo- subordinates visuo-spatial properties of the
spatial characteristics of the set of markings markings to their meteorological significance.

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-17

14
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

DISCUSSION the marks drawn by the subjects during copying


or recall with the marks that constituted the
The results obtained by using the manual ver- original diagram. With realistic drawings of
sions of these two methods were complemen- everyday subject matter, it is relatively simple to
tary. T h e findings suggest that there are clear define what constitutes an 'object' or a discrete
qualititative differences between the way mete- part of an object. However, for abstract dia-
orologists and non-meteorologists mentally rep- grams which portray content that may be quite
resent weather map diagrams, and that these unfamiliar to some of the potential users (as is
differences cannot be dismissed as simply often the case in an instructional setting), such
methodological artifacts. One consequence of definitions are far more difficult. There is a
using these methods and a number of others need for the researcher to distinguish between
which have been developed is that subjects' what constitutes a graphic entity and a concep-
drawn output appears to give useful indications tual entity for a particular type of user. Evidence
of mental representation and processing of dia- - from investigations using the methods
grammatic material, provided it is treated with described above - of the way meteorologists and
appropriate caution. This involves the use of non-meteorologists handle weather maps sug-
safeguards in which alternative techniques, that gests that this distinction is fundamental to the
address the same research questions but do not way these two groups mentally represent this
require subjects to produce drawn output, are type of diagram. Analysis procedures need to be
employed in addition to drawing tasks (these sensitive both to these differences in entity type
non-drawing tasks use pre-drawn material and and to the types of relationships that exist
involve activities such as sorting, matching, and between the objects so identified. Obvious
the assembly of diagrams). forms of relations between objects in a diagram
One of the most challenging (and time-con- are those that are responsible for their particular
suming) aspects of this research is analysing and spatial arrangement; but other types of less
interpreting the drawn output that subjects pro- apparent relations (such as semantic associa-
duce. As mentioned above, in the original tions) are also important. For example, in some
paper-based version of the restricted sampling cases, a physical closeness or similarity of a
method, detailed 'think-aloud' verbal protocols number of entities indicates that they are
were collected as subjects performed the task. semantically related. However, this is by no
Examination of this material following estab- means always the case because the referents of
lished procedures for protocol analysis (Erics- the diagrammatic objects are often related in
son & Simon, 1984) provided useful guidelines ways that do not express themselves as physical
for interpreting subjects' sampling behaviour manifestations. Hence, other methods that tap
and their subsequent completion of the weather non-visual semantic aspects of the mental rep-
map's markings. These guidelines can also be resentation of a diagram are needed to supple-
used with the HyperCard version of the task. ment the analysis of the visual aspects of
However, there are no well-established subjects' output.
approaches for analysing subjects' drawn dia- Once the entities and relations of interest
gram output, especially from procedures such have been identified in a way that takes account
as the copy-recall method. This raises both the- of the theoretical concerns described above,
oretical issues and practical problems. practical ways must be found to perform the
T h e theoretical issues concern comparision of analysis of visual material produced by an inves-

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3 - 1 7

15
Richard K. Lowe ■ Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

tigation. This involves characterising the enti- activity) whereas that involved in the copy-
ties and relations consitituting the output by recall method is somewhat more naturalistic.
means of a set of suitable measures. The work However the copy-recall task relies on the
carried out so far in this area indicates that this capacity of the subject to draw the diagram,
characterisation needs to be carried out at a something that may not be feasible with highly
number of different levels, ranging from global complex depictions. In such cases a method
to local. At the global level (in a weather map involving manipulation of existing diagram
context, how well the overall synoptic pattern elements (as happens in the first part of the
has been drawn), it has been found that using restricted sampling technique) may be more
expert judges to rate diagrams on a number of appropriate.
criteria is most effective. However, analysis at a These two 'visual' methods are a subset of a
local level can be carried out more mechanical- larger group of methods that the author has
ly, based upon characteristics such as position, developed which, despite using a diverse range
shape, orientation, dimensions, proportions, of approaches, provide converging evidence for
area, and symmetry. Analytical techniques are fundamental differences between experts' and
currently being developed by adapting comput- novices' mental representation and processing
er programs designed to be used in medical and of diagrams. However, there is still a need for
industrial image analysis for use in analysing the other approaches that collect verbal data, espe-
drawn output produced by the methods cially as a way to clarify the various types of rela-
described above. tions that exist between the graphic elements
The methods described in this paper have which comprise a diagram. Even when graphic
been designed specifically to probe the mental entities are distant from each other and appar-
representation and processing of diagrams ently unrelated in terms of their appearance,
directly without primary reliance on verbal they can still be closely related in conceptual
data. Because of the difficulty of collecting terms. An example of such a relation in the
information about what 'goes on inside people's meteorological domain is present between the
heads', especially in something relatively inac- apparently unrelated sets of isobars at the bot-
cessible like diagram processing, the use of dif- tom left and bottom right corners of the weather
ferent (but complementary) methods of map used in the copy-recall investigation
approaching this task can be considered as pro- (figure 2). Verbal information gathered from
viding some safeguard against making unwar- expert subjects indicated that, despite their sep-
ranted inferences from the results obtained. aration, these two set are closely related. This is
Each method has its particular strengths and because they are fragments of the same large-
limitations. For example, the restricted sam- scale band of isobars that forms a wave-like pat-
pling method can be used to explore people's tern over the south of the Australian continent.
existing mental representations of particular Below the central part of the map's southern
types of (generic) diagram since it presents only border, these separate sets of isobars are actual-
a limited amount of information. However, if ly linked up into a continuous band. Represent-
the diagram of interest is unique, no pre-exist- ing these two distant graphic entities in the
ing mental representation would be expected mind as parts of the same larger conceptual
and so this method would be inappropriate. entity leads to a quite different basis for under-
In addition, the task that it uses is quite artificial standing the diagram than does representing
(compared with normal diagram processing them as distinct entities.

Information design journal 7 / 1 (1993) 3-17

16
Richard K. Lowe • Diagrammatic information: techniques for exploring its mental representation and processing

CONCLUSION REFERENCES

Because the investigation of how people men- Akin, 0. (1986). Psychology of architectural design.
London: Pion.
tally represent and process diagrams has been Allard, F. & Burnett, N. (1985). Skill in sport. Canadian
comparatively neglected, research tools specifi- Journal of Psychology, 39, 294-312.
cally developed to suit this area are lacking. Chi, M.T.H., Feltovich, P.J., & Glaser, R. (1981).
However, with the increasing emphasis on the Categorisation and representation of physics problems by
experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 1 2 1 - 1 5 1 .
cognitive structures and processes involved in
Chi, M.T.H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M.J. (1988). The nature of
dealing with information in general, it is appro- expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
priate that the study of the effectiveness of dia- Deakin, J.M. & Allard, F. (1991). Skilled memory in expert
grams also includes a cognitive perspective. In figure skaters. Memory and Cognition, 19, 709-716.
Edgerton, S.Y. (1980). The renaissance artist as quantifier.
the past twenty years, methodologies for study-
In M.A. Hagen (ed.), The perception of pictures (vol.1, pp.
ing the subtle and complex cognitive aspects of 179-212). New York: Academic Press.
tasks such as problem solving and reading have Egan, D.E. & Schwartz, B.J. (1979). Chunking in recall of
been developed. Many of the techniques symbolic drawings. Memory & Cognition, 7, 149-158.
Ericsson, K.A. & Simon, H.A. (1984). Protocol analysis.
involved have relied heavily upon verbal output
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford.
as a way of building up an understanding of Gilbert, G.N & Mulkay, M. (1984). Opening pandora's box:
what happens inside people's heads as they per- A sociological analysis of scientists' discourse.
form these tasks. While verbal data can certain- Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
ly contribute to our understanding of some Larkin, J.H. (1983). The role of problem representation in
physics. In D. Gentner& A.L. Stevens (eds.), Mental mod­
aspects of how diagrams are mentally represent- els. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
ed and processed, the capacity to collect high Lowe, R.K. (1989). Search strategies and inference in the
quality non-verbal data should be seen as cen- exploration of scientific diagrams. Educational
tral to research in this area. A range of rigorous Psychology, 9 (1) 27-44.
Lowe, R.K. (1991a). Expository illustrations: A new chal­
approaches and techniques needs to be devel- lenge for reading instruction. Australian Journal of
oped which fully acknowledge the intrinsically Reading, 14, 215-226.
visual nature of diagrams as objects of study Lowe, R.K. (1991b, August). Scientists' and non-scientists'
while providing data that are capable of being mental representation of graphic material in technical dia­
grams. Paper presented at the Fourth European
analysed and interpreted without inordinate Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction,
practical difficulties. The HyperCard versions of Turku, Finland.
the research tools described in this paper offer Macdonald-Ross, M. (1989). Towards a graphic ecology. In
some of the characteristics that would be H. Mandl & J.R. Levin (eds.), Knowledge acquisition from
text and pictures {pp. 145-154). North Holland: Elsevier.
required for convenient and appropriate investi- Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representation: A dual coding
gation of this area. approach. New York: Oxford University Press.
Peeck, J. & Zwarts, J. (1985). Recognition memory for pic­
tures of birds in relation to bird-watching skill. American
Journal of Psychology, 96, 533-566.
van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Information design journal 7/1 (1993) 3-17

17

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen