Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/260151361

Early Child-Parent Attachment and Peer Relations: A Meta-Analysis of


Recent Research

Article  in  Journal of Family Psychology · February 2014


DOI: 10.1037/a0035736 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
66 3,793

5 authors, including:

Susanna Pallini Roberto Baiocco


Università Degli Studi Roma Tre Sapienza University of Rome
66 PUBLICATIONS   327 CITATIONS    210 PUBLICATIONS   1,955 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Barry Howard Schneider Sheri Madigan


Boston College, USA The University of Calgary
150 PUBLICATIONS   4,604 CITATIONS    79 PUBLICATIONS   1,589 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Gay, Lesbian and Bisexual People's Attitudes towards Sexual Minority People: The Adherence to Traditional Gender Roles View project

Existential and pastoral research View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Roberto Baiocco on 03 July 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Family Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 28, No. 1, 118 –123 0893-3200/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0035736

BRIEF REPORT

Early Child–Parent Attachment and Peer Relations:


A Meta-Analysis of Recent Research

Susanna Pallini Roberto Baiocco


University of Rome Tre Sapienza University of Rome
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Barry H. Schneider Sheri Madigan


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

University of Ottawa Hospital for Sick Children and the University of Toronto

Leslie Atkinson
Ryerson University

A central tenet of Bowlby’s attachment theory is that early child– caregiver attachment is reflected in the
quality of the child’s interpersonal relationships throughout life. Schneider, Atkinson, and Tardif (2001)
conducted a meta-analysis of studies conducted up to 1998 to corroborate that contention. They found a
significant but small to moderate effect size (r ⫽ .20). Their finding that studies of friendship bonds had
higher effect sizes than studies of other interpersonal relationships has important theoretical ramifica-
tions. The present brief report is a meta-analysis that covers research conducted for the same purpose
since 1998. The sample consists of 44 studies with a total of 8505 participants. The overall effect size
r of .19 (adjusted r ⫽ .12; 95% confidence interval, .08 –.17) in the current study was similar in
magnitude to the effect size reported in the 2001 meta-analysis, documenting consistency in the
predictive power of attachment theory. However, we failed to replicate the moderating effect of
friendship. One possible explanation for these findings is that the friendships of school-age children and
adolescents no longer invoke very high levels of intimacy. Effect sizes are higher in studies conducted
outside North America than in U.S.- and Canada-based studies.

Keywords: attachment, meta-analysis, peer relations

Caregiver– child attachment influences the quality of close in- the many changes inherent in children’s cognitive, social, and
terpersonal relationships later in the child’s life (Bowlby, 1973). emotional development. Undaunted by such obstacles, researchers
However, the complexity of hypothesized intervening mechanisms invested heavily in research testing the varied outcomes linked
and their abstractness make it difficult to translate the complex with early attachment. Arguing for theoretical clarity, Thompson
components of this theory into empirical research (Thompson & and Raikes (2003) dispute the contention that early attachment
Raikes, 2003). Perhaps the most challenging obstacle is the reality should necessarily be linked to all aspects of subsequent adjust-
that internal working models—mental representations of the at- ment. Instead, attachment might be expected to predict intimate
tachment bonds—are not readily amenable to empirical measure- family and peer relationships most specifically. This contention
ment. Further, the processes linking early child– caregiver attach- must be weighed against evidence that early attachment affects
ment to behavior and relationships later in life act in interplay with humans fundamentally and even physiologically (Diamond & Fa-
gundes, 2010). This suggests a wide array of sequelae, attenuating
arguments for a narrower position.
Susanna Pallini, Department of Education Sciences, University of Rome Schneider, Atkinson, and Tardif (2001) presented a meta-
Tre, Rome, Italy; Roberto Baiocco, Department of Developmental and analysis of 63 studies conducted up to 1998 on effect size linking
Social Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy; Barry H. attachment and children’s peer relations. They reported an overall
Schneider, School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, effect size of r ⫽ .20. This effect size was not moderated by
Canada; Sheri Madigan, Hospital for Sick Children and the Department of method used to measure attachment. There were two important
Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Leslie At-
participant effects: mean effect size was significantly larger for
kinson, Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto, Ontario,
Canada.
middle childhood versus preschool aged children. Of considerable
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Susanna theoretical import, effect size was significantly higher for outcome
Pallini, Via Milazzo 11b, 00185 Rome, Italy. E-mail: susanna.pallini@ measures pertaining to close friendship than for other less intimate
uniroma3.it aspects of peer relations. We present herein a meta-analysis of
118
ATTACHMENT AND PEER RELATIONS 119

studies that appeared since publication of the Schneider et al. parent other than self-report, collected before the child reached age
(2001) article. Recent studies reflect several methodological re- 18, include a quantitative measure of children’s peer relations
finements, including growing recognition that parent– child attach- other than self-report, feature assessment of attachment and peer
ment can be disorganized, such that many children who would relations, include data on securely and insecurely attached partic-
have been considered secure in earlier studies are now classified as ipants or a continuous measure of attachment, written in English or
insecure. The methodology of meta-analysis has also advanced another language understood by our research team (French, Italian,
dramatically (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). Spanish). These criteria resulted in inclusion of 44 studies with
Importantly, recent societal changes have sometimes been seen 8505 participants. Each study was coded for the following: a)
as attenuating the intimacy inherent in peer relationships, espe- attachment measure (e.g., Strange situation, Q-sort); b) attachment
cially close friendships. Goodwin (2009) notes that intimacy may figure (e.g., father, mother); c) dimension(s) of peer relations (e.g.,
be incompatible with the individualism that accompanies the peer-directed aggression, friendship, prosociality); d) source of
“modernizing” transformation of many contemporary societies. In information about peer relations (e.g., observation, peer report,
their sociological treatise on trends in the history of friendship since teacher report); e) degree of familiarity (peers or friends); f) gender
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

World War II, Peel, Reed, and Walker (2009) argue that the last half of child; g) mean age of participants when attachment and h) peer
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of the 20th century witnessed the historic peak of the importance of relations were measured; i) time between the measurement of
voluntary close friendships, at the expense of the clan and extended attachment and peer relations; j) specific subject characteristics
family. It is not clear, however, that friendship ever became as (e.g., diagnosed atypical behavior, low SES, parents divorced); k)
important to boys and men as it did to girls and women. One country; l) publication date; and m) dissemination (i.e., journal
reason cited for a possible more recent decline is that social article, thesis, book). Agreement on coding and application of
interaction increasingly occurs in larger, more anonymous and inclusion/exclusion criteria was established by having the second
lonely communities, schools, and institutions than previously. The author corate; agreement between first and second authors was
lack of rich social interaction at the group level fails to spawn 94%.
intimate relationships at the dyadic level. However, the most
salient reason is that, especially for young people, a greater portion Meta-Analytic Procedures
of social interaction is occurring by means of electronic commu-
nication. Terkle (2011) presents an influential treatise on online We calculated effect size as r, the correlation between attach-
relationships entitled Alone Together: Why We Expect More of ment security and peer relations outcome. Where studies involved
Technology and Less of Each Other. She observed that, although multiple outcomes (e.g., popularity, leadership, withdrawal), we
it has become easy to dub an online interactions “friendships,” the calculated a mean effect size, such that each study is represented
relationships bear little resemblance to the traditional notions of by one effect in a given meta-analysis. However, many studies
friendship and are, in particular, devoid of intimacy. If intimacy is included measures of both friendship and peer relations, such that
declining, attachment theory may be less relevant to relationships the exclusion of either would result in substantial data loss. Be-
than when it was first formulated. The distinction discussed earlier cause the distinction between close friendships and relations with
between narrow and broad outcomes of attachment becomes less other peers may be particularly important (Schneider et al., 2001),
compelling. However, Schneider and Amichai-Hamburger (2010) we followed Hallion and Ruscio’s (2011) strategy of conducting
observe that the Internet may in fact facilitate close interpersonal three nonindependent meta-analyses. The first included all peer
relating by individuals who have trouble being intimate in face- relations studies, the second included only close friendships, and
to-face encounters. In any case, Fletcher, Simpson, Campbell, and the third involved only peers who were not close friends.
Overall (2013) hasten to highlight that intimacy is programmed We conducted analyses as outlined by Borenstein et al. (2009)
into the interpersonal relationships of humans as a result of a using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 2.0 software (Boren-
lengthy evolutionary process that is not easily modified. stein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2005). We used random
Based on theory and on the results reported by Schneider et al. effects analyses. All effect sizes were weighted by sample size. We
(2001), we hypothesized that overall effect size would be signifi- adjusted the observed values to account for potential publication
cantly different from zero, with larger effect sizes for older chil- bias using Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim-and-fill procedures.
dren than for preschoolers. We also hypothesized that there would We used Q-statistics to assess heterogeneity of effect sizes across
be a significantly larger effect for studies focusing on friendship studies. We used Q-statistics and method of moments regression
than for large-group peer relationships. analyses to assess for moderating effects in the full sample (Bo-
renstein et al., 2009), when the number of studies warranted it; we
Method did not pursue the search for moderators in the close friendship and
nonclose friendship subsamples because of low power.
Selection of Studies
Results
To generate samples comparable with the 2001 meta-analysis,
we used the same keywords in searching PSYCINFO, PUBMED Effect Sizes Involving Total Sample
and ProQuest Dissertations. We also scanned the references of
articles retrieved and contacted researchers working in this area. Random effects analyses indicated an overall effect size of r ⫽
Our search included studies available between 1999 and 2012. .19, p ⬍ .001; 95% confidence interval (CI) ⫽ .15–.23 (see Figure
Inclusion/exclusion criteria were identical to those used in the 1). Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill procedure resulted in an
2001 meta-analysis: original data, measure of attachment to a estimated unbiased effect size of r ⫽ .12, CI ⫽ .08 –.17. Effect
120 PALLINI, BAIOCCO, SCHNEIDER, MADIGAN, AND ATKINSON

Meta Analysis
Study name Statistics for each study Correlation and 95% CI

Lower Upper
Correlation limit limit Z-Value p-Value

Verschueren & Marcoen, 1999 0.298 0.084 0.486 2.697 0.007


Bohlin et al., 2000 0.252 0.054 0.431 2.484 0.013
Contreras et al., 2000 0.310 0.065 0.520 2.462 0.014
DeMulder et al., 2000 0.195 -0.008 0.383 1.884 0.060
Attili, 2001 0.541 0.408 0.652 6.878 0.000
Denham et al., 2001 0.215 0.054 0.365 2.603 0.009
Granot & Mayseless, 2001 0.195 0.011 0.367 2.072 0.038
Allen et al., 2002 0.210 0.036 0.372 2.355 0.019
Bar-Haim et al., 2002 -0.293 -0.503 -0.051 -2.357 0.018
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Burns, 2002 0.064 -0.237 0.354 0.410 0.682


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Schmidt et al., 2002 -0.204 -0.459 0.082 -1.403 0.161


Carlson et al., 2004 0.202 0.059 0.336 2.763 0.006
McElwain & Volling, 2004 0.190 -0.183 0.515 0.999 0.318
Milligan, 2004 -0.001 -0.255 0.253 -0. 008 0.994
Wood et al., 2004 0.375 0.058 0.623 2.299 0.022
Zimmermann, 2004 0.466 0.193 0.672 3.194 0.001
Allen et al. 2005 0.120 -0.025 0.260 1.627 0.104
Bohlin et al., 2005 0.320 0.121 0.494 3.093 0.002
Booth la Force et al., 2005 0.080 -0.075 0.231 1.011 0.312
Monks et al., 2005 0.211 0.019 0.388 2.153 0.031
Rydell et al., 2005 0.193 0.008 0.365 2.041 0.041
Szewczyk-Sokolowski et al., 2005 0.250 0.054 0.427 2.489 0.013
Bosquet & Egeland, 2006 0.209 0.053 0.355 2.615 0.009
Laible, 2006 0.451 0.200 0.646 3.367 0.001
Allen et al., 2007 0.208 0.058 0.349 2.703 0.007
Balent ine, 2007 0.243 0.094 0.382 3.156 0.002
Simpson et al., 2007 0.207 -0.016 0.411 1.819 0.069
Wood, 2007 0.430 0.089 0.681 2.434 0.015
Dykas et al., 2008 0.194 0.053 0.328 2.680 0.007
Kerns et al., 2008 0.004 -0.187 0.195 0.041 0.968
Agnor, 2009 0.110 -0.106 0.316 1.000 0.317
Bolin & Hagekull, 2009 0.260 0.087 0.418 2.915 0.004
Chango et al., 2009 0.050 -0.095 0.193 0.673 0.501
Lindsey et al., 2009 0.160 -0.062 0.367 1.416 0.157
Shomaker & Furman, 2009 0.209 0.072 0.338 2.977 0.003
Smeekens et al., 2009(1) 0.190 0.004 0.364 1.999 0.046
Smeekens et al., 2009(2) 0.191 0.005 0.364 2.010 0.044
Stacks & Oshio, 2009 0.054 -0.177 0.279 0.455 0.649
Vu, 2009 0.087 -0.282 0.434 0.453 0.650
McElwain et al., 2011 0.052 -0.003 0.107 1.837 0.066
Roskam et al., 2011 0.310 0.136 0.465 3.422 0.001
Vaughn et al., 2011 0.240 -0.082 0.516 1.469 0.142
Verissimo et al., 2011 0.240 -0.101 0.531 1.385 0.166
Barone & Lionetti, 2012 0.152 -0.097 0.383 1.196 0.232
0.164 0.138 0.190 12.199 0.000
-1.00 -0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00

Favours A Favours B

Figure 1. Forest plots showing the association between attachment and peer relations.

sizes across studies were heterogeneous, Q(43) ⫽ 99.92, p ⬍ .001. measures, source of the outcome measure, percent males in the
Moderator analyses revealed a significant difference in effect size, sample) proved significant.
Q(1) ⫽ 8.66, p ⬍ .01, between North American (n ⫽ 29; ES ⫽
.16; CI ⫽ .12–.20) and European (n ⫽ 13; ES ⫽ .28; CI ⫽
Effect Size for Close Friendship Outcomes Only
.21–.35) samples. Although there was a nonsignificant decline in
effect size over time, neither this nor any other moderating vari- We conducted a supplementary analysis using only the data
ables (attachment measure, attachment metric [classificatory vs. from the friendship variables. The mean effect of .17 (SD ⫽ .13)
dimensional], attachment figure, average ages at assessment of was not substantially different from the pattern of results that
attachment and outcome, time between attachment and outcome emerged in the main analyses. Duval and Tweedie’s trim-and-fill
ATTACHMENT AND PEER RELATIONS 121

procedure resulted in slight modification, with an estimated unbi- It is important to bear in mind several limitations of this study.
ased effect size of r ⫽ .14, CI ⫽ .07–.19. Some of the cell sizes were small, making it difficult to detect
possible moderators of effect size. Our decision to separate more
recent studies from those in the original meta-analysis is consistent
Discussion
with our objective of conducting a true replication. We established
The overall average effect size in the new sample is similar to that the important moderator of friendship is not differentially
that reported by Schneider et al. in 2001. This bolsters the con- related to the ESs for attachment in the sample of recent studies.
tention that early child–parent bonds are related to children’s This update is of substantial theoretical importance. However, our
subsequent relationships with peers. There are, however, some decision may have obscured other potential moderators of ES that
important differences in the moderators of effect size in the two might have emerged in a combined sample. Finally, our exclusion
meta-analyses. We failed to replicate the previous finding that criteria led to the omission of a number of problematic studies,
effect sizes are higher for friendship than for other aspects of peer mostly conducted with adolescents, in which both attachment and
relations. Accordingly, the following section pertains specifically peer relations were measured by self-report.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to the two subsamples on friendship although the sample sizes are


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

too small for statistical comparison. The discussion concludes with References
some remarks on the implications of the small-to-moderate overall ⴱ
effect, which emerged in both this and the previous meta-analysis. Studies marked with an asterisk were included in the meta-analysis.
There were comparable numbers of friendship studies in the two ⴱ
Agnot, C. J. (2009). A proposed model of friendship quality and attach-
samples, 12 in the 2001 article, and 11 in the present sample. Thus, ment in preschool children (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved from
although the construct of friendship received more emphasis in ProQuest Dissertations & Theses database (UMI No. 3360141)

1990s than previously, this enthusiasm did not translate into more Allen, J. P., Marsh, P., McFarland, C., McElhaney, K. B., Land, D. J.,
research. The two samples are similar in terms of the relative Jodl, K. M., & Peck, S. (2002). Attachment and autonomy as predictors
proportions of Q-sort and strange situation methods for measuring of the development of social skills and delinquency during midadoles-
cence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 56 – 66.
attachment and the proportions of measurement strategies in as-
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.56
sessing peer relations. There was, however, a substantial age ⴱ
Allen, J. P., Porter, M., McFarland, C., McElhaney, K. B., & Marsh, P.
difference between the friendship subsamples in this (M ⫽ 69 (2007). The relation of attachment security to adolescents’ paternal and
months) and the earlier meta-analysis (M ⫽ 41 months). Age peer relationships, depression, and externalizing behavior. Child Devel-
differences cannot be invoked to explain the failure to find the opment, 78, 1222–1239. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01062.x

expected higher effect for friendship than for other aspects of peer Allen, J. P., Porter, M. R., McFarland, F. C., Marsh, P., & Mc Elhaney,
relations because age difference is in the wrong direction; intimacy K. B. (2005). The two faces of adolescents’ success with peers: Ado-
increases rather than decreases in peer relationships from school lescent popularity, social adaptation, and deviant behavior. Child Devel-
age onward (e.g., Berndt, 2007). Therefore, the most likely expla- opment, 76, 747–760. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00875.x

nation of this failure to replicate is that the friendships of school- Attili, G. (2001). Ansia da separazione e misura dell’attaccamento nor-
male e Patologico [Separation anxiety and normal and psychopatholog-
age children and adolescents are less intimate than they once were,
ical attachment measure]. Bologna, Italy: Unicopli.
possibly because of the societal changes discussed earlier. Because ⴱ
Balentine, A. C. (2007). The relation of early attachment with kindergar-
most contentions that intimacy is declining in relationships pertain ten social preference: An examination of intervening relational and
to the United States, this explanation might account for the sig- behavioral processes (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest
nificant cultural moderation that emerged only in the current Dissertations & Theses database (UMI No. 3259667)

sample. It is important to remember that our non–North American Bar-Haim, Y., Aviezer, O., Berson, Y., & Sagi, A. Attachment in infancy
samples are mostly from European and Mediterranean cultures and personal space regulation in early adolescence. Attachment & Hu-
where family life is particularly intensive and influential (Stanton, man Development, 4, 68 – 83. doi:10.1080/14616730210123111

1995) and not from Japan, for example, where extremely close and Barone, L., & Lionetti, F. (2012). Attachment and social competence: A
interdependent attachment dynamics may lead to insecure attach- study using MCAST in low-risk Italian preschoolers. Attachment &
Human Development, 14, 391– 403. doi:10.1080/14616734.2012
ment in the Western sense (Rothbaum, Rosen, Ujiie & Uchida,
.691653
2002). We present this interpretation of our findings with some Berndt, T. J. (2007). Children’s friendships: Shifts over a half-century in
reticence because of the limited sample size. perspectives on their development and their effects. In G. W. Ladd (Ed.),
Although we focused on attachment, attachment might not be Appraising the human developmental sciences: Essays in honor of
solely responsible for the predicting outcome but might predict Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (pp. 138 –155). Detroit, MI: Wayne State
outcome together with other simultaneous processes (Thompson & University Press.

Raikes, 2003). This is but one possible interpretation, however, of Bohlin, G., & Hagekull, B. (2009). Socio-emotional development: From
a recurrent pattern of small effects. Another possibility is that the infancy to young adulthood. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50,
effect sizes do not fully reflect the impact of early attachment 592– 601. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00787.x

because of the many complications mentioned at the start of the Bohlin, G., Hagekull, B., & Andersson, K. (2005). Behavioral inhibition
as a precursor of peer social competence in early school age: The
introduction in reducing attachment theory to elements that can be
interplay with attachment and nonparental care. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
measured realistically. At this point, we can assume that the link terly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 51, 1–19. doi:10.1353/mpq
between attachment security and peer relations is established. .2005.0001
Intriguing issues remain pertaining to the potentially changing ⴱ
Bohlin, G., Hagekull, B., & Rydell, A. M. (2000). Attachment and social
association between attachment and close relationships and the functioning: A longitudinal study from infancy to middle childhood.
role that cultural factors might play. Social Development, 9, 24 –39. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00109
122 PALLINI, BAIOCCO, SCHNEIDER, MADIGAN, AND ATKINSON


Booth-LaForce, C., & Oxford, M. L. (2008). Trajectories of social with- Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 54, 473– 498. doi:
drawal from grades 1 to 6: Prediction from early parenting, attachment, 10.1353/mpq.0.0010

and temperament. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1298 –1313. doi: Laible, D. (2006). Maternal emotional expressiveness and attachment
10.1037/a0012954 security: Links to representations of relationships and social behavior.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2005). Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 52,
Comprehensive meta-analysis Version 2: A computer program for re- 645– 670. doi:10.1353/mpq.2006.0035

search synthesis [Computer software]. Englewood, NJ: Biostat. Lindsey, E. W., Caldera, Y. M., & Tankersley, L. (2009). Marital conflict
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). and the quality of young children’s peer play behavior: The mediating
Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley. doi:10.1002/ and moderating role of parent-child emotional reciprocity and attach-
9780470743386 ment security. Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 130 –145. doi:10.1037/

Bosquet, M., & Egeland, B. (2006). The development and maintenance of a0014972

anxiety symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal McElwain, N. L., Booth-LaForce, C., & Wu, X. (2011). Infant-mother
sample. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 517–550. doi:10.1017/ attachment and children’s friendship quality: Maternal mental-state talk
S0954579406060275 as an intervening mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1295–
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss. Vol. 2: Separation: Anxiety and 1311. doi:10.1037/a0024094
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.


anger. London, UK: Hogarth Press. McElwain, N. L., & Volling, B. L. (2004). Attachment security and

Burns, S. R. (2002). Attachment security as a predictor of preschoolers’ parental sensitivity during infancy: Associations with friendship quality
prosocial responses to mothers and peers (Doctoral Dissertation). Re- and false-belief understanding at age 4. Journal of Social and Personal
trieved from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses database (UMI No. Relationships, 21, 639 – 667. doi:10.1177/0265407504045892

3076087) Milligan, K. V. (2004). Attachment security and friendship quality in

Carlson, E. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2004). The construction of early childhood: A theoretical and methodological reconceptulization
experience: A longitudinal study of representation and behavior. Child (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations & The-
Development, 75, 66 – 83. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00654.x ses database (UMI No. NQ94358)
ⴱ ⴱ
Chango, J. M., McElhaney, K. B., & Allen, J. P. (2009). Attachment Monks, C. P., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (2005). Psychological
organization and patterns of conflict resolution in friendships predicting correlates of peer victimization in preschool: Social cognitive skills,
adolescents’ depressive symptoms over time. Attachment & Human executive function and attachment profiles. Aggressive Behavior, 31,
Development, 11, 331–346. doi:10.1080/14616730903016961 571–588. doi:10.1002/ab.20099

Contreras, J. M., Kerns, K. A., Weimer, B. L., Gentzler, A. L., & Tomich, Peel, M., Reed, L., & Walker, J. (2009). The importance of friends: The
P. L. (2000). Emotion regulation as a mediator of associations between most recent past. In B. Caine (Ed.), Friendship: A history (pp. 316 –355).
mother-child attachment and peer relationships in middle childhood. London, UK: Equinox.

Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 111–124. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.14 Roskam, I., Meunier, J. C., & Stievenart, M. (2011). Parent attachment,
.1.111 childrearing behavior, and child attachment: Mediated effects predicting

DeMulder, E. K., Denham, S., Schmidt, M., & Mitchell, J. (2000). Q-sort preschoolers’ externalizing behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental
assessment of attachment security during the preschool years: Links Psychology, 32, 170 –179. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2011.03.003
from home to school. Developmental Psychology, 36, 274 –282. doi: Rothbaum, F., Rosen, K., Ujiie, T., & Uchida, N. (2002). Family systems
10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.274 theory, attachment theory, and culture. Family Process, 41, 328 –350.

Denham, S., Mason, T., Caverly, S., Schmidt, M., Hackney, R., Caswell, doi:10.1111/j.1545-5300.2002.41305.x

C., & DeMulder, E. (2001). Preschoolers at play: Co-socialisers of Rydell, A. M., Bohlin, G., & Thorell, L. B. (2005). Representations of
emotional and social competence. International Journal of Behavioral attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children’s relation-
Development, 25, 290 –301. doi:10.1080/016502501143000067 ships with teachers and peer competence in preschool. Attachment &
Diamond, L. M., & Fagundes, C. P. (2010). Psychobiological research on Human Development, 7, 187–204. doi:10.1080/14616730500134282

attachment. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 27, 218 –225. Schmidt, M. E., Demulder, E. K., & Denham, S. (2002). Kindergarten
doi:10.1177/0265407509360906 social-emotional competence: Developmental predictors and psychoso-
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot based cial implications. Early Child Development and Care, 172, 451– 462.
method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. doi:10.1080/03004430214550
Biometrics, 56, 455– 463. doi:10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x Schneider, B. H., & Amichai-Hamburger, Y. (2010). Electronic commu-

Dykas, M. J., Ziv, Y., & Cassidy, J. (2008). Attachment and peer relations nication: Escape mechanism or relationship-building tool for shy, with-
in adolescence. Attachment & Human Development, 10, 123–141. doi: drawn children and adolescents? In K. H. Rubin & R. J. Coplan (Eds.),
10.1080/14616730802113679 The development of shyness and social withdrawal (pp. 236 –261). New
Fletcher, G., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Overall, A. (2013). The York, NY: Guilford Press.
science of intimate relationships. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. Schneider, B. H., Atkinson, L., & Tardif, C. (2001). Child-parent attach-
Goodwin, R. (2009). Changing relations: Achieving intimacy in a time of ment and children’s peer relations: A quantitative view. Developmental
social transition. New York: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/ Psychology, 37, 86 –100. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.1.86

CBO9780511551796 Shomaker, L. B., & Furman, W. (2009). Parent-adolescent relationship

Granot, D., & Mayseless, O. (2001). Attachment security and adjustment qualities, internal working models, and attachment styles as predictors of
to school in middle childhood. International Journal of Behavioral adolescents’ interactions with friends. Journal of Social and Personal
Development, 25, 530 –541. doi:10.1080/01650250042000366 Relationships, 26, 579 – 603. doi:10.1177/0265407509354441

Hallion, L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of Simpson, J. A., Collins, W. A., Tran, S., & Haydon, K. C. (2007).
cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychological Attachment and the experience and expression of emotions in romantic
Bulletin, 137, 940 –958. doi:10.1037/a0024355 relationships: A developmental perspective. Journal of Personality and

Kerns, K. A., Brumariu, L. E., & Abraham, M. M. (2008). Homesickness Social Psychology, 92, 355–367. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.355

at summer camp: Associations with the mother-child relationship, social Smeekens, S., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., & Van-Bakel, H. J. A. (2009). The
self-concept, and peer relationships in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer predictive value of different infant attachment measures for socioemo-
ATTACHMENT AND PEER RELATIONS 123

tional development at age 5 years. Infant Mental Health Journal, 30, number of reciprocal friends. Early Child Development and Care, 181,
366 –383. doi:10.1002/imhj.20219 27–38. doi:10.1080/03004430903211208
ⴱ ⴱ
Stacks, A. M., & Oshio, T. (2009). Disorganized attachment and social Verschueren, K., & Marcoen, A. (1999). Representation of self and
skills as indicators of Head Start children’s school readiness skills. socioemotional competence in kindergartners: Differential and com-
Attachment & Human Development, 11, 143–164. doi:10.1080/ bined effects of attachment to mother and father. Child Development, 70,
14616730802625250 183–201. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00014
Stanton, M. E. (1995). Patterns of kinship and residence. In B. B. In- ⴱ
Vu, J. A. (2009). Representations of relationships children have with
goldsby, & S. Smith (Eds.), Families in multicultural perspective: Per- mothers, teachers, and friends, and their relation to Social Competence
spectives on marriage and the family (pp. 97–116). New York, NY: (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations & The-
Guilford Press. ses database (UMI No. 3374978)

Szewczyk-Sokolowski, M., Bost, K. K., & Wainwright, A. B. (2005). ⴱ
Wood, J. J. (2007). Academic Competence in preschool: Exploring the
Attachment, temperament, and preschool children’s peer acceptance.
role of close relationships and anxiety. Early Education and Develop-
Social Development, 14, 379 –397. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005
ment, 18, 223–242. doi:10.1080/10409280701282868
.00307.x ⴱ
Wood, J. J., Emmerson, N. A., & Cowan, P. A. (2004). Is early attachment
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Terkle, S. (1911). Alone together: Why we expect more of technology and


less of each other. New York, NY: Basic Books. security carried forward into relationships with preschool peers? British
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Thompson, R. A., & Raikes, H. A. (2003). Toward the next quarter Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 245–253. doi:10.1348/
century: Conceptual and methodological changes for attachment the- 026151004323044591

ory. Developmental Psychopathology, 15, 691–718. doi:10.1017 Zimmermann, P. (2004). Attachment representations and characteristics
.S0954579403000348 of friendship relations during adolescence. Journal of Experimental
ⴱ Child Psychology, 88, 83–101. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2004.02.002
Vaughn, B. E., El-Sheikh, M., Shin, N., Elmore-Staton, L., Krzysik, L., &
Monteiro, L. (2011). Attachment representations, sleep quality and adap-
tive functioning in preschool age children. Attachment & Human De-
velopment, 13, 525–540. doi:10.1080/14616734.2011.608984 Received May 23, 2013
ⴱ Revision received November 27, 2013
Veríssimo, M., Santos, A. J., Vaughn, B. E., Torres, N., Monteiro, L., &
Santos, O. (2011). Quality of attachment to father and mother and Accepted December 5, 2013 䡲

View publication stats

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen