Sie sind auf Seite 1von 15

Journal of Sports Sciences

ISSN: 0264-0414 (Print) 1466-447X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20

The Prosocial and Antisocial Behaviour in Sport


Scale: Further evidence for construct validity and
reliability

Maria Kavussanu , Nicholas Stanger & Ian D. Boardley

To cite this article: Maria Kavussanu , Nicholas Stanger & Ian D. Boardley (2013) The Prosocial
and Antisocial Behaviour in Sport Scale: Further evidence for construct validity and reliability,
Journal of Sports Sciences, 31:11, 1208-1221, DOI: 10.1080/02640414.2013.775473

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2013.775473

Published online: 11 Mar 2013.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 1122

Citing articles: 30 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rjsp20
Journal of Sports Sciences, 2013
Vol. 31, No. 11, 1208–1221, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2013.775473

The Prosocial and Antisocial Behaviour in Sport Scale: Further


evidence for construct validity and reliability

MARIA KAVUSSANU1, NICHOLAS STANGER2, & IAN D. BOARDLEY1


1
University of Birmingham, School of Sport and Exercise Sciences, Edgbaston, Birmingham, B15 2TT United Kingdom, and
2
Canterbury Christ Church University, Canterbury, United Kingdom

(Accepted 6 February 2013)

Abstract
The purpose of this research was to provide further evidence for the construct validity (i.e., convergent, concurrent, and
discriminant validity) of the Prosocial and Antisocial Behaviour in Sport Scale (PABSS), an instrument that has four subscales
measuring prosocial and antisocial behaviour toward teammates and opponents. We also investigated test-retest reliability and
stability of the PABSS. We conducted three studies using athletes from a variety of team sports. In Study 1, participants
(N = 129) completed the PABSS and measures of physical and verbal aggression, hostility, anger, moral identity, and
empathy; a sub-sample (n = 111) also completed the PABSS one week later. In Study 2, in addition to the PABSS,
participants (N = 89) completed measures of competitive aggressiveness and anger, moral attitudes, moral disengagement,
goal orientation, and anxiety. In Study 3, participants (N = 307) completed the PABSS and a measure of social goals. Across
the three studies, the four subscales evidenced the hypothesised relationships with a number of variables. Correlations were
large between the two antisocial behaviours and small between the two prosocial behaviours. Overall, the findings supported
the convergent, concurrent, and discriminant validity of the scale, provided evidence for its test-retest reliability and stability,
and suggest that the instrument is a valid and reliable measure of prosocial and antisocial behaviour in sport.

Keywords: concurrent, convergent, discriminant, item stability

Introduction at others, etc.) to determine whether behaviour is


reprehensible. Finally, Bandura (1999) has distin-
The importance of developing valid and reliable
guished between proactive morality, which is mani-
instruments that assess the morally relevant dimen- fested in the power to behave humanely, and
sions of sport experience was emphasised by inhibitive morality, which is expressed in the power
Bredemeier and Shields (1998) over a decade ago. to refrain from behaving inhumanely. In this view of
These scholars have also pointed to the significance morality “people do good things as well as refrain
of moral behaviour as the “bottom line” of morality. from doing bad things” (Bandura, 1999, p. 194).
Thus, developing instruments that measure morally In the sport context, the terms prosocial and anti-
relevant behaviour in sport is an important prerequi- social behaviour have been used to refer to the two
site to conducting research that furthers our under- dimensions of morality (see Kavussanu, 2008).
standing of this behaviour in the sport context. Prosocial behaviour has been defined as voluntary
One such instrument has been recently developed behaviour intended to help or benefit another indi-
by Kavussanu and Boardley (2009) based on vidual or group of individuals (Eisenberg & Fabes,
Bandura (1991, 1999) and previous work on proso- 1998), and examples in sport are helping an injured
cial and antisocial behaviour (e.g., Eisenberg & opponent and congratulating a teammate. The term
Fabes, 1998; Sage, Kavussanu, & Duda, 2006). antisocial behaviour has been used to refer to volun-
Bandura (1991) has argued that how people act is tary acts intended to harm or disadvantage another
more important than their justifications of their (Sage et al., 2006), and examples in sport are cheat-
behaviour, implying that moral psychologists should ing or trying to injure another player. Prosocial and
focus on behaviour. He also suggested that indivi- antisocial behaviours can have positive and negative
duals use multidimensional rules or standards (e.g., consequences, respectively, for athletes’ physical and
the consequences of the action, whether it is directed psychological well-being. For example, kicking a

Correspondence: Maria Kavussanu, University of Birmingham, School of Sport and Exercise Sciences, Edgbaston, Birmingham, B15 2TT United Kingdom.
E-mail: m.kavussanu@bham.ac.uk

© 2013 Taylor & Francis


Prosocial and antisocial behaviour 1209

player could cause him or her to experience pain, associated with a criterion variable (external stan-
and helping an injured player could alleviate his or dard), when data are collected at the same point in
her distress. However, if the player does not feel any time (Kline, 2005). Evidence for concurrent validity
pain (e.g., because he or she is distracted or has was obtained by examining whether prosocial and
taken analgesic medication), the behaviours will not antisocial behaviours were related to three variables
have the expected consequences (Kavussanu & that have been consistently linked to these beha-
Boardley, 2012). Thus, prosocial and antisocial viours in past research: empathy, task orientation,
behaviours have the potential to affect others. and ego orientation. Although correlations sup-
In order to measure prosocial and antisocial beha- ported the hypothesised relationships, only one sam-
viour in sport, Kavussanu and Boardley (2009) devel- ple of athletes was studied; thus, the evidence is
oped the Prosocial and Antisocial Behaviour in Sport limited. In addition, Kavussanu and Boardley
Scale (PABSS). First, a large number of behaviours (2009) did not examine convergent validity, which
fitting the respective definitions and occurring in five refers to the degree to which a measure is associated
team sports (i.e., basketball, football, field hockey, with theoretically similar constructs (Brewer, 2000)
netball, and rugby) were identified. Then, items and is evidenced when a scale is correlated at least
were developed to measure these behaviours, and moderately with established measures of the same or
their content validity was examined through expert similar constructs (Kline, 2005).
ratings. Next, an abbreviated item list was adminis- Kavussanu and Boardley (2009) also provided
tered to 1,213 team-sport athletes. Factor analyses of evidence for the instrument’s discriminant validity.
these items revealed four factors: two factors repre- This aspect of validity entails the evaluation of mea-
sented prosocial behaviour toward teammates and sures against each other and is evident when vari-
opponents, and two factors represented antisocial ables assumed to measure different constructs are
behaviour towards teammates and opponents. The not too highly correlated (Kline, 2005). Discrimin-
final instrument consists of 20 items and four sub- ant validity was supported by correlations among the
scales (corresponding to the four factors) that have prosocial and antisocial behaviour subscales that ran-
displayed acceptable levels of internal consistency ged between 0.04 and 0.46, that is, correlations that
with alpha coefficients ranging from 0.74 to 0.87 were not too high. However, no other evidence of
(Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009). Similar levels of this type of validity was provided. Additional support
internal consistency (α range = 0.77–0.89) have for discriminant validity would be obtained by dif-
been reported in a recent study of 292 New Zealand ferential relationships between the four PABSS sub-
athletes (Hodge & Lonsdale, 2011). The large range scales and measures of other constructs.
of alpha coefficients is in part due to the different Another psychometric property not examined by
number of items in each subscale. Kavussanu and Boardley (2009) is test-retest reliabil-
To date, the PABSS has been used in studies with ity, which is typically estimated by administering a
athletes from England (Boardley & Kavussanu, 2009, measure to the same people twice and correlating the
2010), North America (Bolter & Weiss, 2013), two sets of scores (Pedhazur & Schmelkin, 1991). We
Australia (Boardley & Jackson, 2012), and New also examined stability (or reproducibility), which
Zealand (Hodge & Lonsdale, 2011), that have revealed refers to the extent to which scores are resistant to
a variety of findings. For example, prosocial behaviour change on repeated administrations when no change
toward teammates has been predicted positively by is expected (Lane, Nevill, Bowes, & Fox, 2005).
mastery motivational climate (Boardley & In sum, more evidence is needed to further estab-
Kavussanu, 2009) and autonomous athlete motivation lish the validity and reliability of the PABSS. In the
(Hodge & Lonsdale, 2011), while prosocial behaviour present research, we aimed to obtain this evidence in
toward opponents has been positively associated with three studies. In Study 1, we examined convergent,
empathy (Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009). The two concurrent, and discriminant validity using non-
antisocial behaviours have also been positively related sport measures, and investigated test-retest reliability
to moral disengagement (Boardley & Kavussanu, and stability. In Studies 2 and 3, we sought to pro-
2009, 2010; Hodge & Lonsdale, 2011). Thus, a body vide further support for construct validity in two
of literature using the PABSS has started to emerge other samples of team-sport athletes using sport-spe-
and reveals potential antecedents of prosocial and anti- cific measures.
social sport behaviours underscoring the importance of
this instrument and the need to provide further evi-
Study 1
dence for its psychometric properties.
In the original study that developed the PABSS, The main purpose of Study 1 was to examine conver-
Kavussanu and Boardley (2009) provided evidence gent, concurrent, and discriminant validity of the
for the concurrent validity of the scale. This aspect of PABSS using non-sport measures; we also examined
validity is concerned with whether a measure is test-retest reliability and stability of the PABSS. Below,
1210 M. Kavussanu et al.

we discuss the variables used to examine each type footballers (Sage et al., 2006); however, it did not
of validity. predict prosocial sport behaviour (Sage et al., 2006).
We anticipated that moral identity would be nega-
tively linked to antisocial behaviour and positively
Convergent validity
related to prosocial behaviour. However, due to the
We investigated convergent validity of the antisocial inconsistent findings of previous research (Aquino &
behaviour subscales by examining their relationship Reed, 2002; Sage et al., 2006), the latter hypothesis
with two dimensions of aggression, which is beha- was tentative.
viour intended to cause harm or injure another Finally, empathy refers to the responses of one
(Berkowitz, 1993). Two components of dispositional individual to the observed experiences of another
aggression described by Buss and Perry (1992) are (Davis, 1983). Two empathy dimensions linked to
physical and verbal aggression, and refer to the instru- moral variables in past research are perspective tak-
mental component of aggression implemented by ing (i.e., the tendency to adopt the psychological
physical or verbal means, respectively. Given that, in point of view of others) and empathic concern (i.e.,
sport, antisocial behaviour has been defined as beha- the tendency to experience feelings of sympathy,
viour intended to harm or disadvantage another (Sage compassion, and concern for unfortunate others).
et al., 2006), it is conceptually similar to the construct Individuals high in empathy are more likely to attend
of aggression, but broader in that it also refers to to others’ needs and feelings and therefore more
behaviour aimed to disadvantage others (see likely to behave prosocially and refrain from behav-
Kavussanu, 2012). Thus, we hypothesised that the ing aggressively toward others. Empathy has been
two antisocial behaviours would be positively asso- positively linked to prosocial behaviour, and nega-
ciated with physical and verbal aggression, and such tively related to aggression and antisocial behaviour
relationships would provide support for the conver- (Eisenberg & Miller, 1987; Miller & Eisenberg,
gent validity of these subscales. However, we 1988). It has also been positively associated with
expected that compared to teammate behaviour, anti- prosocial opponent behaviour and negatively linked
social opponent behaviour would be more strongly to the two antisocial behaviours (Kavussanu &
associated with physical and verbal aggression. Boardley, 2009). We wished to determine whether
these findings would be replicated in a different
sample, which would further support the concurrent
Concurrent validity
validity of the PABSS.
We investigated concurrent validity by examining
whether the four PABSS subscales are associated
Discriminant validity
with anger, hostility, moral identity, and empathy.
Anger represents the emotional component of We investigated discriminant validity by examining
aggression (Buss & Perry, 1992), and involves phy- whether the four PABSS subscales are differentially
siological arousal and preparation for aggression, related to physical aggression, empathy, and moral
while hostility is the cognitive component of aggres- identity. We expected that physical aggression would
sion and consists of feelings of ill will and injustice be associated more strongly with antisocial behaviour
(Buss & Perry, 1992). We expected positive links toward opponents than teammates, because the for-
between anger and hostility and the two antisocial mer – but not the latter – entails physical acts.
behaviours; these would support the concurrent Empathy and moral identity were expected to be
validity of the antisocial behaviour subscales. more strongly correlated with prosocial behaviour
Moral identity has been defined as the degree to toward opponents than teammates because the for-
which being moral is a defining or central character- mer are helping behaviours, which are more likely to
istic of one’s sense of self. It is the cognitive schema occur when someone has empathy for others and
one holds about his or her moral character (Aquino, views being moral as a central part of the self.
Freeman, Reed, Lim, & Felps, 2009) and is orga- Finally, we expected that prosocial behaviours would
nised around a set of moral traits, such as being have negative or no association with the aggression
honest, caring, generous, and compassionate constructs because such associations have been
(Aquino & Reed, 2002). The degree to which these revealed between prosocial and antisocial behaviour –
moral traits are central to one’s self-concept is which is conceptually similar to aggression – in
known as moral identity centrality, but will be previous research (e.g., Sage & Kavussanu, 2008).
referred to here as moral identity. In past research,
moral identity has positively predicted self-reported
Test-retest reliability and stability
volunteering and actual donation behaviour in col-
lege students (Aquino & Reed, 2002) and has been Finally, we examined test-retest reliability and stabi-
negatively associated with antisocial behaviour in lity of the four PABSS subscales. To this end, we
Prosocial and antisocial behaviour 1211

administered the scale to the same sample on two hostility (eight items; e.g., I am suspicious of overly
different occasions separated by a one-week interval. friendly strangers), and anger (seven items; e.g., I
have trouble controlling my temper). In past
research, the four subscales have shown good levels
Method of internal consistency with α = 0.72–0.85 (Buss &
Participants Perry, 1992).
Participants were male (n = 73) and female (n = 56) Moral identity. The 5-item internalisation dimension
students from a British university competing in hockey of the moral identity scale (Aquino & Reed, 2002)
(n = 24), netball (n = 23), football (n = 60), and rugby was used to measure moral identity. Participants were
(n = 22). These sports were selected because they are presented with nine traits (i.e., caring, fair, kind, help-
contact team sports. Thus, they were appropriate for ful, hardworking, compassionate, etc.) validated as
obtaining validity evidence for the PABSS, as the scale necessary characteristics of a moral person, and were
was developed for use with athletes from contact team asked to respond to statements concerning these traits
sports and some items are applicable only to these (e.g., it would make me feel good to be a person who
sports. At the time of data collection, participants has these characteristics). Participants responded on a
ranged in age from 18 to 33 years (M = 18.43, Likert scale anchored by 1 = strongly disagree and
s = 1.42) and had competed in their respective sport 7 = strongly agree. This subscale has shown very
for an average of 7.17 (s = 3.26) years. The highest good internal consistency (α = 0.83) in previous
ever standard at which they had played their sport was research (Aquino & Reed, 2002).
club (46%), county (33%), regional (14%), national
(4%), and international (3%). Empathy. The 7-item perspective taking (e.g., before
criticising somebody, I try to imagine how I would
Measures feel if I were in their place) and 7-item empathic
concern (e.g., I am often quite touched by things
The following measures were administered: that I see happen) subscales of the Interpersonal
Reactivity Index (Davis, 1980) were used to measure
Prosocial and antisocial behaviour. The PABSS empathy. Participants were asked to indicate how
(Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009) was used to measure well a number of statements describe them and
prosocial and antisocial behaviour in sport. responded on a scale with anchors of 1 = does not
Participants were presented with 20 items describing describe me well and 5 = describes me very well. Because
sport behaviours and were asked to report how often perspective taking and empathic concern are theore-
they had engaged in each behaviour in the previous tically related (Davis, 1983), and in this study the
season on a Likert scale anchored by 1 = never and two variables were moderately correlated, r
5 = very often. The PABSS consists of four subscales (128) = 0.35, P < 0.001, responses on the two sub-
that measure antisocial behaviour toward opponents scales were averaged to form an overall empathy
(eight items; e.g., deliberately fouled an opponent), score. This combined scale has been used in past
antisocial behaviour toward teammates (five items; e. research, and has shown very good internal consis-
g., verbally abused a teammate), prosocial behaviour tency (α = 0.82–0.85; Carlo, Raffaelli, Laible, &
toward opponents (three items; e.g., helped an Meyer, 1999).
injured opponent), and prosocial behaviour toward
teammates (four items; e.g., encouraged a team-
mate). The mean of each subscale was calculated Procedure
and used in all analyses. This procedure was fol- Upon approval of the study by the University ethics
lowed for all measures. committee, one of the investigators administered
questionnaires to participants at the end of a sport
Aggression. The Buss-Perry Aggression science undergraduate lecture. Participants were
Questionnaire (Buss & Perry, 1992) was used to informed that the purpose of the study was to exam-
assess physical aggression, verbal aggression, hosti- ine sportsperson-like attitudes and behaviours and
lity, and anger. Participants were asked to indicate the factors that influence them. It was also explained
how well a number of statements describe what they that all responses would be kept anonymous and
are like in general using a Likert scale anchored by confidential and used only for research purposes,
1 = very unlike me and 5 = very like me. The ques- participation was voluntary, and they could with-
tionnaire consists of four subscales that measure draw from the study at any time. All students pre-
physical aggression (nine items; e.g., if somebody sent in the class agreed to take part, gave informed
hits me, I hit back), verbal aggression (five items; consent, and completed the measures described
e.g., I often find myself disagreeing with people), above.
1212 M. Kavussanu et al.

To assess test-retest reliability and stability of the Times 1 and 2 (α range = 0.77–0.89). The prosocial
PABSS, the instrument was administered to partici- teammate behaviour subscale also exhibited good
pants under standardised conditions, at the end of levels of internal consistency at Times 1 and 2, as
the same sport science undergraduate lecture, given did the prosocial opponent behaviour subscale at
at the same time-tabled lecture slot, a week later (see Time 2 (α range = 0.73–0.77). However, alpha for
also Nevill, Lane, Kilgour, Bowes, & Whyte, 2001). the prosocial opponent behaviour subscale at Time 1
When assessing test-retest reliability it is suggested (α = 0.64) was below traditionally acceptable levels
that the interval between the two administrations is of internal consistency (i.e., α = 0.70). Finally, all
relatively short, that is, one to two weeks, to allow other measures used in the study had acceptable
one to tap only random measurement error and not levels of internal consistency (α range = 0.71–0.87).
true changes (Pedhazur & Schmelkin, 1991). Given
that the PABSS measures behaviour, which could
Construct validity
change over time as it is affected by situational fac-
tors, we chose a one-week interval to minimise the Convergent, concurrent, and discriminant validity
possibility that behaviours would change from the of the PABSS were examined by computing the
first to second administration (see Anastasi & correlations of the four behaviours with theoreti-
Urbina, 1997). A total of 63 males and 48 females cally similar or different constructs. To account
(who were present in both lectures) completed the for error of measurement, we also computed disatte-
questionnaire on both occasions and were used in nuated correlations (Spearman, 1904). As can be seen
our assessment of test-retest reliability and stability. in Table I, the two antisocial behaviours were: posi-
Using university students allowed us to administer tively associated with physical and verbal aggression
the test under standardised conditions which is supporting convergent validity; positively linked to
important when assessing test-retest reliability hostility and anger supporting concurrent validity;
(Schutz, 1998). and negatively related to moral identity and empa-
thy, also supporting concurrent validity. Prosocial
Results and discussion behaviour toward opponents was positively linked
to moral identity and empathy supporting concur-
Descriptive statistics and internal consistency rent validity; both prosocial behaviours were unre-
The means and standard deviations of the PABSS lated to aggression, anger, and hostility, indicating
subscales at Time 1 are presented in Table I. discriminant validity. Discriminant validity was also
Athletes reported that, when playing their team supported by the correlations among the four
sport, they behaved antisocially rarely to sometimes PABSS subscales which ranged from –0.02 to
and prosocially sometimes to often. Table I also 0.47 (disattenuated correlations range –0.03–0.60).
includes alpha coefficients for all measures. It can These results support the validity of the PABSS
be seen that the two antisocial behaviour subscales as a measure of antisocial and prosocial sport
exhibited acceptable levels of internal consistency at behaviour.

Table I. Alpha coefficients, zero-order correlations and descriptive statistics: Study 1 (N = 129).

Antisocial Behaviour (AB) Prosocial Behaviour (PB)

Opponents Teammates Opponents Teammates


Variable (0.77) ((0.89)) (0.79) ((0.87)) (0.64) ((0.73)) (0.77) ((0.73))

Physical aggression (0.87) 0.51***/0.62*** 0.29**/0.35*** 0.03/0.04 –0.07/–0.09


Verbal aggression (0.79) 0.47***/0.60*** 0.30 **/0.38*** 0.05/0.07 0.12/0.15
Hostility (0.79) 0.21*/0.27* 0.34***/0.43*** 0.13/0.18 0.11/0.14
Anger (0.84) 0.40***/0.50*** 0.34***/0.42*** 0.09/0.12 –0.11/–0.14
Moral identity (0.71) –0.27**/–0.37*** –0.32***/–0.43*** 0.21*/0.31** 0.12/0.16
Empathy (0.77) –0.38***/–0.49 *** –0.42***/–0.54*** 0.16/0.23* 0.08/0.10
Gender –0.36***/–0.41*** –0.40***/–0.45*** 0.11/0.14 –0.12/–0.14
AB Teammates 0.47***/0.60***
PB Opponents 0.04/0.06 –0.02/–0.03
PB Teammates 0.18*/0.23* 0.02/0.03 0.22*/0.31**
Ma 2.41/2.44 2.16/2.11 3.32/3.26 4.28/4.21
sa 0.67/0.76 0.66/0.65 0.70/0.67 0.53/0.50

Note: Alpha coefficients are in brackets; double brackets in the case of the one-week follow-up. Disattenuated correlations are presented
after the slash.
a
Time 1 means and standard deviations (s) are presented before the slash, Time 2 after the slash. Gender was coded as 0 = male and
1 = female.
*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, *** P < 0.001.
Prosocial and antisocial behaviour 1213

Test-retest reliability and stability for each item, we calculated test-retest differences
and report the minimum and maximum difference
Test-retest reliability was examined by computing
scores (Table II). We also computed the proportion
Intraclass Correlation Coefficients (ICC) using a
of agreement, that is, the percentage of participants
two-factor mixed-effects model (McGraw & Wong,
whose test-retest difference scores were within ± 1,
1996). Acceptable levels of test-retest reliability are
which is the value recommended for a 5-point Likert
indicated by strong ICCs (> 0.70) between the mea-
scale; an item is assumed to have acceptable stability
sures completed at two time points (Terwee et al.,
if 90% of test-retest difference scores lie within this
2007). All four subscales demonstrated good levels
range (Nevill et al., 2001). Finally, we computed the
of test-retest reliability between Times 1 and 2, with
median-sign test (i.e., the number of participants
ICCs of 0.92, 95% Confidence Interval (CI) [0.89,
with differences above and below the median, 0) to
0.95], and 0.88, 95% CI [0.83, 0.92] for antisocial
determine the degree of potential bias of scores
behaviour toward opponents and teammates, respec-
increasing or decreasing from test to retest. The
tively, and 0.88, 95% CI [0.83, 0.92] and 0.83, 95%
results of these analyses (including means and s)
CI [0.75, 0.88] for prosocial behaviour toward oppo-
are presented in Table II. For all items, at least
nents and teammates, respectively.
90% of participants had test-retest difference scores
With respect to the stability of the four subscales,
within ±1 indicating fairly high stability. However,
we investigated both mean and individual-item stabi-
the median-sign test revealed that the scores of two
lity. We examined mean stability using a 2 Gender
prosocial teammate items (“encouraged a team-
(male, female) by 2 Time (test, retest) multivariate
mate” and “congratulated a teammate for good
analysis of variance (MANOVA), with gender as a
play”) and one prosocial opponent item (“helped
between-subjects factor and Time as a within-subjects
an opponent off the floor”) significantly increased
factor. This analysis confirmed that the four mean
over the one-week interval.
subscale scores did not change significantly over
time, F(4, 106) = 1.06, P = 0.38, ηp2 = 0.04, provid-
ing support for the mean stability of the four subscales
Discussion
(Schutz, 1998). Means and standard deviations (s) for
Times 1 and 2 can be seen in Table I. The results showed strong support for the three
Individual-item stability was examined based on aspects of validity of the antisocial behaviour sub-
recommendations by Nevill, Lane and colleagues scales. The evidence for the concurrent and discri-
(Lane et al., 2005; Nevill et al., 2001). Specifically, minant validity of the two prosocial behaviours was

Table II. Results of item stability analyses: Study 1 (n = 111).

Test-retest
Time 1 Time 2 Difference PA Median Sign Test

M s M s Min Max % (±1) ≥1 0 diff ≤ -1

1. Criticised an opponent (AO) 2.95 0.94 2.83 0.91 −1 2 97.3 17 65 29


2. Helped an opponent off the floor (PO) 3.54 0.88 3.39 0.83 −2 2 98.2 10 76 25*
3. Argued with a teammate (AT) 2.25 0.88 2.20 0.80 −3 2 93.7 23 63 25
4. Gave positive feedback to a teammate (PT) 4.39 0.65 4.29 0.61 −2 2 98.2 12 77 22
5. Deliberately fouled an opponent (AO) 2.43 0.99 2.26 1.00 −1 2 96.4 18 60 33
6. Asked to stop play for injured opponent (PO) 3.02 0.99 3.11 0.88 −2 2 93.7 28 60 23
7. Verbally abused a teammate (AT) 1.57 0.81 1.54 0.70 −2 4 96.4 17 75 19
8. Encouraged a teammate (PT) 4.52 0.57 4.39 0.62 −2 1 99.1 8 79 24*
9. Retaliated after a bad foul (AO) 2.60 0.91 2.68 1.01 −3 3 95.5 23 71 17
10. Helped an injured opponent (PO) 3.39 0.88 3.29 0.78 −2 2 92.8 21 63 27
11. Criticised a teammate (AT) 2.23 0.83 2.17 0.81 −2 3 93.7 23 63 25
12. Gave constructive feedback to a teammate (PT) 3.78 0.82 3.81 0.81 −2 2 94.6 28 58 25
13. Tried to wind up an opponent (AO) 2.93 1.12 2.89 1.15 −3 2 91.9 21 58 32
14. Swore at a teammate (AT) 1.89 1.02 1.88 0.92 −2 2 95.5 22 74 15
15. Congratulated a teammate for good play (PT) 4.45 0.66 4.33 0.64 −4 2 96.4 14 68 29*
16. Tried to injure an opponent (AO) 1.61 0.85 1.68 0.82 −1 2 98.2 25 69 17
17. Intentionally distracted an opponent (AO) 2.52 1.12 2.66 1.07 −4 3 91.9 33 59 19
18. Showed frustration at a teammate’s 2.87 0.93 2.77 0.81 −2 2 97.3 18 68 25
poor play (AT)
19. Intentionally broke the rules of the game (AO) 2.12 0.90 2.26 0.88 −3 1 95.5 26 70 15
20. Physically intimidated an opponent (AO) 2.16 1.17 2.24 1.13 −2 2 91.0 28 66 17

Note: PA = Proportion of Agreement; AO = Antisocial opponent; AT = Antisocial teammate; PO = Prosocial opponent; PT = Prosocial
teammate.
*P < 0.05.
1214 M. Kavussanu et al.

modest. This issue was addressed in Study 3, while self-referenced criteria) and ego (i.e., the tendency to
Study 2 sought further support for the validity of the define success using other-referenced criteria) orienta-
PABSS. All subscales demonstrated very good test- tions (Nicholls, 1989). In past research, task and ego
retest reliability and mean stability. Although a few goal orientations have been positively related to pro-
items fluctuated somewhat from test to retest, gen- social and antisocial behaviour, respectively (e.g.,
erally each item of the PABSS showed acceptable Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009; Sage et al., 2006).
stability over a one-week interval. Similar relationships were anticipated in this study.
Finally, we investigated discriminant validity by
examining the subscales’ relationships with trait
Study 2
competitive sport anxiety, which has a cognitive
In Study 2, we further examined convergent, concur- component (i.e., negative expectations and worry
rent, and discriminant validity of the PABSS using over the task at hand) and a somatic component
sport-specific criterion variables. We investigated con- (i.e., one’s perception of physiological elements of
vergent and concurrent validity of the two antisocial anxiety such as feelings of tension and nervousness)
behaviour subscales by examining their relationships (Smith, Smoll, & Schutz, 1990). As anxiety has not
with competitive aggressiveness (for convergent valid- been associated with any moral variables in previous
ity) as well as anger and attitudes towards moral research, we expected no relationship between anxi-
decision-making in sport (for concurrent validity). ety and prosocial or antisocial behaviours. We also
Aggressiveness refers to the disposition to become examined the associations of behaviours with the
aggressive or acceptance and willingness to use other measured constructs. Given that antisocial
aggression (Berkowitz, 1993), while anger is the sub- and prosocial behaviours are distinct constructs
jective evaluation that increased physiological arousal (Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009) we expected no rela-
is a result of threat to one’s physical or psychological tionship between prosocial behaviours and competi-
well-being (Averill, 1983). We expected positive rela- tive anger and aggressiveness or antisocial attitudes;
tionships between antisocial behaviours and competi- similarly, we expected a null relationship between
tive aggressiveness and anger. antisocial behaviour and prosocial attitudes.
To provide further evidence for convergent and
concurrent validity we examined the link of the four
Method
subscales with variables associated with prosocial and
antisocial behaviour in past research. The first set of Participants
variables is attitudes towards moral decision making.
Participants were male (n = 53) and female (n = 36)
Lee, Whitehead, and Ntoumanis (2007) described
students at a British university competing in team
two antisocial attitudes termed “acceptance of cheat-
sports, who indicated football (n = 31), rugby
ing” and “acceptance of gamesmanship” and one
(n = 17), hockey (n = 15), netball (n = 15), basket-
prosocial attitude termed “keeping winning in pro-
ball (n = 6), American football (n = 3), or lacrosse
portion”. Acceptance of cheating refers to infractions
(n = 2) as their main team sport. At the time of data
of the rules in order to gain some unfair advantage, in
collection, they ranged in age from 19 to 26 years
which there is a degree of successful deception,
whereas gamesmanship refers to actions that do not (M = 19.63, s = 1.03) and had participated in their
respective team sport for an average of 8.18
violate the rules of the sport but appear to violate the
(s = 3.67) years. The highest ever standard at
spirit of the contest, perhaps using the laws to gain
which participants had played their team sport at
some unfair advantage (Lee et al., 2007). Keeping
that time was club (37%), county (37%), regional
winning in proportion involves emphasis on winning
(18%), national (5%), and international (3%).
fairly and de-emphasises the will to win at all costs.
Antisocial and prosocial attitudes were expected to
have positive relationships with antisocial and proso- Measures
cial behaviours, respectively.
We also examined whether behaviours were asso- The following measures were administered:
ciated with moral disengagement and goal orientation.
Moral disengagement refers to a set of psychological Prosocial and antisocial behaviour. The PABSS
manoeuvres that enable individuals to minimise the (Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009) was used to measure
negative affect typically associated with transgressive prosocial and antisocial behaviour in sport (see
behaviour (Bandura, 1999) and has been associated Study 1 for further details).
positively with antisocial behaviour and negatively with
prosocial behaviour in sport (Boardley & Kavussanu, Aggressiveness and anger. We measured aggressiveness
2008, 2009). Goal orientation has been distinguished and anger in sport competition with the Competitive
into task (i.e., the tendency to define success via Aggressiveness and Anger scale (Maxwell & Moores,
Prosocial and antisocial behaviour 1215

2007). The stem “When playing your team sport how (e.g., I beat other people) orientation. Participants
often have you behaved, felt or thought that ...” was used responded on a Likert scale anchored by 1 = strongly
followed by two 6-item subscales measuring aggressive- disagree and 5 = strongly agree. Roberts et al. (1998)
ness (e.g., Violent behaviour directed toward an oppo- have provided evidence for the reliability of the task
nent is acceptable) and anger (e.g., I get mad when I (α = 0.88) and ego (α = 0.88) subscales.
lose). Participants responded on a Likert scale anchored
by 1 = never and 5 = very often. Maxwell and Moores Anxiety. The Sport Anxiety Scale-2 (Smith, Smoll,
(2007) have provided evidence for the factorial validity Cumming, & Grossbard, 2006) was used to measure
and reliability of the aggressiveness (α range = 0.83–0.84) anxiety in sport. Participants were asked to read a
and anger (α range = 0.78–0.83) subscales. number of statements describing thoughts and feel-
ings associated with sport competition and indicate
Moral attitudes. We measured moral attitudes with the how they usually feel prior to or during competition
Attitudes to Moral Decision making in Youth Sport using a Likert scale anchored by 1 = not at all and
Questionnaire (Lee et al., 2007). Participants were 4 = very much so. Example items are “I’m concerned
asked to read a number of statements describing about performing poorly” and “My body feels
thoughts and feelings players may have and indicate tense.” Smith et al. (2006) have provided evidence
their level of agreement on a Likert scale anchored by for the factorial validity and reliability of the scale.
1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree. The ques-
tionnaire measures acceptance of cheating (three
items; e.g., It is okay to cheat if nobody knows), Procedure
acceptance of gamesmanship (three items; e.g., I The procedure was identical to Study 1 with two
sometimes try to wind up the opposition), and keep- exceptions: Participants were recruited from a sec-
ing winning in proportion (three items; e.g., Winning ond-year undergraduate sport and exercise science
and losing are a part of life). Lee et al. (2007) have class and completed measures only at one time point.
provided evidence for the factorial validity and relia-
bility of the acceptance of cheating (α = 0.73) and
acceptance of gamesmanship (α = 0.75). The third Results and discussion
subscale (i.e., keeping winning in proportion) had Descriptive statistics and alpha coefficients
unacceptable levels of internal consistency in previous
research (α = 0.60) and in this study (α = 0.54) and Descriptive statistics for the PABSS subscales and
was not used in the main data analysis. Although the alpha coefficients for all variables can be seen in
questionnaire was developed with a youth sample, it Table III. The means of the PABSS subscales were
has been used successfully with adult athletes (e.g., similar to those reported in Study 1; all subscales
Bureau, Vallerand, Ntoumanis, & Lafreniere, 2012; displayed acceptable internal consistency (α
Ntoumanis & Standage, 2009). range = 0.79–0.84). Alpha coefficients for all other
subscales were acceptable with the exception of
Moral disengagement. We measured moral disengage- acceptance of gamesmanship, which was marginally
ment in sport with the 8-item Moral Disengagement acceptable (α = 0.68).
in Sport Scale – Short (Boardley & Kavussanu,
2008). Participants were asked to read a number of Construct validity
statements describing thoughts players may have and
indicate their level of agreement using a Likert scale In order to assess convergent, concurrent, and discri-
anchored by 1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly minant validity of the PABSS, we examined correla-
agree. An example item is “Insults among players tions of the four subscales with the other measured
do not really hurt anyone”. The scale has shown variables and with each other (see Table III). The two
very good levels of internal consistency (α range antisocial behaviours were: positively linked to aggres-
0.80–0.85), and support has been provided for its siveness providing evidence for convergent validity;
factorial, convergent, and concurrent validity positively linked to anger, acceptance of cheating,
(Boardley & Kavussanu, 2008). acceptance of gamesmanship, moral disengagement,
and ego orientation, and negatively related to task
Goal orientation. We measured task and ego goal orientation supporting concurrent validity; and unre-
orientations with the Perception of Success lated to sport anxiety supporting discriminant validity.
Questionnaire (Roberts, Treasure, & Balague, The two prosocial behaviours were positively asso-
1998). The stem “When playing my main team ciated with task orientation supporting concurrent
sport I feel most successful when…” was used validity and unrelated to anger and anxiety supporting
followed by two 6-item subscales measuring task discriminant validity. However, prosocial teammate
(e.g., I show clear personal improvement) and ego behaviour was also positively linked to acceptance of
1216 M. Kavussanu et al.

Table III. Alpha coefficients, zero-order correlations and descriptive statistics: Study 2 (N = 89).

Antisocial Behaviour (AB) Prosocial Behaviour (PB)

Opponents Teammates Opponents Teammates


Variable (0.84) (0.83) (0.79) (0.83)

Aggressiveness (0.85) 0.80***/0.94*** 0.31**/0.37*** 0.00/0.00 0.16/0.19


Anger (0.74) 0.51***/0.65*** 0.54***/0.69*** 0.02/0.03 0.07/0.09
Acc. cheating (0.89) 0.59***/0.68*** 0.23*/0.27** –0.08/–0.10 0.09/0.10
Acc. gamesmanship (0.68) 0.61***/0.81*** 0.33**/0.44*** 0.07/0.10 0.25*/0.33**
MD (0.84) 0.60***/0.71*** 0.24*/0.29** –0.16/–0.20 0.14/0.17
Task orientation (0.76) –0.25*/–0.31** –0.20/–0.25* 0.17/0.22* 0.19/0.24*
Ego orientation (0.80) 0.20/0.24* 0.04/0.05 0.10/0.13 0.14/0.17
Sport anxiety (0.88) –0.09/–0.10 0.02/0.02 0.14/0.17 –0.09/–0.11
Gender –0.36***/–0.39*** –0.31**/–0.34*** 0.05/0.06 –0.01/–0.01
AB teammates 0.41***/0.49***
PB opponents 0.05/0.06 0.14/0.17
PB teammates 0.24*/0.29** –0.02/–0.02 0.32**/0.40***
M 2.39 2.00 3.34 4.33
s 0.65 0.65 0.81 0.58

Note: Alpha coefficients are in brackets; Acc. = acceptance of; MD = moral disengagement; Gender was coded as 0 = male and 1 = female.
Disattenuated correlations are presented after the slash.
*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

gamesmanship. The correlations among the four One study has examined the relationship between
PABSS subscales ranged from –0.02 to 0.41 (disatte- social goal orientations and prosocial and antisocial
nuated range = –0.09 to 0.49). behaviours in football players. Specifically, Sage and
Kavussanu (2007) found that both social affiliation
Discussion and social recognition were positively associated with
prosocial behaviour, whereas social status was posi-
Overall, good support was provided for the conver-
tively linked to antisocial behaviour. Identifying simi-
gent, concurrent, and discriminant validity of the
lar relationships in this study would support the
antisocial behaviour subscales, while modest evi-
concurrent validity of the prosocial and antisocial
dence was obtained for the concurrent and discrimi-
behaviour subscales. Moreover, if social goals evi-
nant validity of the prosocial behaviour subscales.
dence relationships of varying strength with the two
prosocial behaviours, this would support the discrimi-
Study 3
nant validity of the two prosocial behaviour subscales.
The main purpose of Study 3 was to further examine
the concurrent and discriminant validity of the proso- Method
cial behaviour subscales using variables linked to pro-
Participants
social behaviours in previous research, namely social
goal orientations (Allen, 2003); antisocial behaviour Participants were 307 male football players, whose
in relation to these variables was also examined. The age ranged from 16 to 36 years (M = 21.39,
social goal orientations described by Allen (2003) are s = 4.01). At the time of data collection, they had
social affiliation, social recognition, and social status. played football competitively for an average of 13.04
Social-affiliation orientation reflects a focus on posi- (s = 5.14) years, and had played for their current
tive social experiences and developing reciprocal rela- team for an average of 2.58 (s = 1.97) years. The
tionships; individuals high in social affiliation feel highest ever standard at which participants had
things go well for them in their sport when they played their team sport at that time was club
make friends, socialise, and enjoy their time with (73.9%), county (16.3%), regional (8.1%), national
similar others. Social-recognition reflects a focus on (1.3%), and international (0.3%).
validating oneself through approval from others.
Individuals high in this goal orientation feel things Measures
go well for them in sport when their ability and per-
formance are recognised by others. Finally, social- The following measures were administered:
status orientation reflects a focus on validating oneself
through achieving popularity among peers. Prosocial and antisocial behaviour. The PABSS
Individuals pursuing a social-status goal feel that (Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009) was used to measure
things go well for them in sport when they are one prosocial and antisocial behaviour in sport (see
of the popular players in the group. Study 1 for details).
Prosocial and antisocial behaviour 1217

Social goal orientations. The 15-item Social Construct validity


Motivational Orientation scale for Sport (Allen,
In order to assess concurrent and discriminant valid-
2003) was used to assess social affiliation (seven
ity of the four subscales, we examined their correla-
items; e.g., I make some good friends in the team),
tions with social goal orientations. The two prosocial
social recognition (four items; e.g., I receive recogni-
behaviours were positively associated with social
tion from others for my accomplishments), and
affiliation and social recognition supporting concur-
social status (four items; e.g., I belong to the popular
rent validity; the prosocial opponent behaviour sub-
group in the team). The stem for each item was “I
scale was also positively associated with social status
feel things have gone well in football when....” and
orientation. The two prosocial behaviour subscales
responses were made on a scale anchored by
also had varying strength of association with social
1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. The
affiliation, supporting discriminant validity. As can
scale has shown good levels of internal consistency
be seen in Table IV, antisocial opponent behaviour
(α range = 0.77–0.87; Allen, 2003).
was positively linked to social status supporting its
concurrent validity; it was also significantly and posi-
tively associated with social recognition and social
Procedure
affiliation. Although the correlations between antiso-
After receiving approval from the University ethics cial opponent behaviour and social affiliation and
committee, coaches of 25 football teams were con- antisocial teammate behaviour and social status
tacted and all agreed to allow their athletes to par- were statistically significant, in part due to the large
ticipate in the study. The questionnaires were sample size, they did not reach the 0.15 value that
administered by research assistants during a train- represents small effect size (Cohen, 1992). Thus,
ing session. Athletes were told that honesty in although these relationships were not hypothesised,
responses was vital and that all responses would the findings may not have implications for the con-
be used only for research purposes and would be current validity of these subscales. Antisocial team-
kept strictly confidential. Participants signed an mate behaviour was unrelated to social goals with the
informed consent form before completing the exception of social status. Finally, the correlations
questionnaire. among the four PABSS subscales ranged from 0.00
to 0.55 (disattenuated range = 0.00–0.69), support-
ing discriminant validity.
Results and discussion
Descriptive statistics and alpha coefficients Discussion
Descriptive statistics and alpha coefficients for all In sum, Study 3 provided evidence for the concur-
measures are presented in Table IV. On average, rent and discriminant validity of the four subscales.
participants reported that they behaved antisocially In particular, Study 3 broadly demonstrated antici-
rarely to sometimes and prosocially sometimes to pated relationships between the two prosocial beha-
often. All subscales demonstrated acceptable inter- viour subscales and social goals, a finding that is
nal consistency (α range = 0.72–0.82) with the consistent with previous research (Sage &
exception of the prosocial behaviour toward team- Kavussanu, 2007). However, the positive relation-
mates subscale which was marginally acceptable ship between prosocial opponent behaviour and
(α = 0.68). social status orientation was unexpected. Although

Table IV. Alpha coefficients, zero-order correlations, and descriptive statistics: Study 3 (N = 307).

Antisocial Behaviour (AB) Prosocial Behaviour (PB)

Opponents Teammates Opponents Teammates


Variable (0.82) (0.77) (0.72) (0.68)

Social affiliation (0.90) 0.12*/0.14* 0.03/0.04 0.44***/0.55*** 0.29***/0.37***


Social recognition (0.82) 0.29***/0.35*** 0.03/0.04 0.24***/0.31*** 0.28***/0.37***
Social status (0.87) 0.29***/0.34*** 0.11/0.13* 0.26***/0.33*** 0.08/0.10
AB teammates 0.55***/0.69***
PB opponents 0.26***/0.35*** 0.21***/0.28***
PB teammates 0.03/0.04 0.00/0.00 0.34***/0.49***
M 2.75 2.66 3.28 4.02
s 0.69 0.74 0.87 0.68

Note: Alpha coefficients are presented in brackets. Disattenuated correlations are presented after the slash.
*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
1218 M. Kavussanu et al.

this relationship may characterise only this specific reflected in the magnitude of the correlations – var-
population (i.e., adult male footballers), it should be ied: antisocial opponent behaviour had large correla-
further explored in future research. tions with physical and verbal aggression,
aggressiveness, acceptance of cheating and games-
manship and moral disengagement, whereas antiso-
General discussion
cial teammate behaviour was moderately linked to
A prerequisite to advancing knowledge in any these variables. It makes sense that athletes who
research area is the development of valid and reliable cheat, employ gamesmanship, and are physically
measures that assess the constructs of interest. The and verbally aggressive are more likely to be antiso-
PABSS was developed to measure prosocial and cial toward their opponents rather than their team-
antisocial sport behaviour and some evidence for mates, which may explain the differential strength in
construct validity has been provided (Kavussanu & the identified relationships. In addition, antisocial
Boardley, 2009). The purpose of the present opponent but not teammate behaviour was positively
research was to provide further evidence of the con- associated with ego orientation, in line with previous
struct validity of the PABSS and to examine its test- research (Boardley & Kavussanu, 2010; Kavussanu
retest reliability and stability. & Boardley, 2009). Moreover, antisocial opponent
behaviour was positively linked to the three social
goals – correlations were moderate for social status
Construct validity
and social recognition and small for social affiliation –
Antisocial behaviour. Consistent with our hypotheses, while antisocial teammate behaviour evidenced a very
the two antisocial behaviours were positively asso- small positive link to social status orientation.
ciated with physical and verbal aggression with The differential links of the two antisocial beha-
strong correlations for antisocial opponent and mod- viours to a variety of variables as well as their null
erate correlations for antisocial teammate behaviour. relationships with anxiety provide support for their
A very strong correlation was also observed between discriminant validity (see Kline, 2005). The correla-
antisocial opponent behaviour and competitive agg- tions between the two antisocial behaviours across
ressiveness suggesting that these two constructs are the three studies were large (r range = 0.41–0.55)
very similar. The strong-to-very-strong relationships suggesting that a common antisocial trait may
with established measures of similar constructs sup- underlie these behaviours (Kavussanu & Boardley,
port the convergent validity of the antisocial oppo- 2009).
nent behaviour subscale (see Brewer, 2000; Kline,
2005). The moderate relationships between antiso- Prosocial behaviour. Consistent with previous
cial teammate behaviour and aggression suggest that research (Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009; Sage &
this behaviour is not as similar to aggression as is Kavussanu, 2007) the two prosocial behaviours
antisocial opponent behaviour. Thus, the convergent were positively linked to task orientation, social
validity of antisocial teammate behaviour needs to be affiliation, and social recognition. These findings
further examined. suggest that players, who act prosocially by helping
In support of our hypotheses, the two antisocial an opponent off the floor or encouraging and con-
behaviours were positively associated with hostility, gratulating a teammate, also tend to define success
anger, and acceptance of cheating and gamesman- using self-referenced criteria and aim to develop
ship. These findings suggest that team-sport athletes reciprocal relationships with others and validate
who behave antisocially toward their opponents and themselves through others’ approval. Prosocial beha-
teammates are also more likely to view cheating and viours, particularly toward teammates, should
gamesmanship behaviours as acceptable and to be strengthen relationships within the team, thereby
hostile and angry. Consistent with previous research satisfying participants’ goals for social affiliation
(e.g., Boardley & Kavussanu, 2009, 2010; and recognition. These findings, along with the
Kavussanu, Stamp, Slade, & Ring, 2009; Sage non-significant relationships with physical and verbal
et al., 2006), the two antisocial behaviours were aggression, hostility, anger, aggressiveness, accep-
positively associated with moral disengagement and tance of cheating, moral disengagement, ego orien-
negatively linked to moral identity, empathy, and tation, and anxiety support the concurrent validity of
task orientation. Overall, the identified relationships the prosocial behaviour subscales.
with a range of theoretically related constructs offer Prosocial behaviour toward opponents was posi-
support for the concurrent validity of the two anti- tively associated with moral identity and empathy.
social behaviour subscales. Previous research has also found that moral identity
Although both antisocial behaviours were asso- positively predicts self-reported volunteering and
ciated with the variables discussed above in the actual donation behaviour in college students
expected direction, in most cases, the effect sizes – (Aquino & Reed, 2002) and that empathy is
Prosocial and antisocial behaviour 1219

associated with prosocial opponent behaviour provided support for the test-retest reliability of all
(Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009). The relationships subscales (see John & Benit-Martínez, 2000). In
between moral identity and empathy with prosocial addition, mean subscale scores did not change
teammate behaviour were weak and non-significant. over time supporting mean stability (Schutz,
These findings are in line with previous research (e. 1998), and over 90% of participants’ test-retest
g., Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009; Sage et al., 2006) difference scores were within the acceptable range
and suggest that prosocial teammate behaviour is of ± 1, supporting individual-item stability (Nevill
distinct from prosocial opponent behaviour. The et al., 2001). These findings suggest that the
specific prosocial teammate behaviours included in PABSS is relatively stable over the short term, and
our scale were giving positive and constructive feed- that situational factors may not impart much error
back to a teammate as well as encouraging, and into scale responses.
congratulating a teammate. These are supportive
acts to one’s teammates and therefore less likely to
Limitations and future research directions
be affected by one’s empathy or moral identity.
Prosocial opponent behaviour was also positively This research has some limitations that should be
associated with social status goal orientation. considered when interpreting the findings. A first
Perhaps individuals who focus on validating the self limitation concerns the use of relatively homogenous
through achieving popularity among peers think that samples. Specifically, although participants in the
helping other players may contribute to creating a first two studies had diverse sport experience, they
positive image and increase their popularity among were all university students, while all participants
their peers. Prosocial teammate behaviour was not in Study 3 were male football players; thus, the
related to either of these variables. The differential findings can only be generalised to similar popula-
links of the two prosocial behaviour subscales with tions. Second, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of
moral identity, empathy, and social status goal orien- the prosocial opponent behaviour at Time 1 was
tation support their discriminant validity. The corre- 0.64. It is known that this coefficient varies from
lations between the two prosocial behaviours ranged one sample to another and is greatly affected by the
from 0.18 to 0.34 (disattenuated range = 0.23–0.49) number of items in the scale (Cortina, 1993). As the
suggesting that these behaviours are related, yet they prosocial opponent behaviour subscale has only
are distinct from each other. Finally, in line with three items, the relatively low alpha is not overly
previous research (e.g., Hodge & Lonsdale, 2011; surprising. Future research should try to improve
Kavussanu & Boardley, 2009), in most cases, the the internal consistency of this subscale by adding
two prosocial behaviours were unrelated to the two more items that measure other prosocial behaviours
antisocial behaviours, suggesting that players who toward opponents.
behave prosocially toward their teammates or oppo- A third limitation is the small sample size of Study
nents are no more or less likely to behave antiso- 2, which revealed a number of relationships between
cially. Therefore, to better understand moral the two prosocial behaviours and several sport-
behaviour in sport, it is important to investigate related variables. Although many of the correlations
both prosocial and antisocial behaviour. represented small effect size (Cohen, 1992), only
An unexpected positive link was observed between three reached statistical significance. Given that sta-
prosocial behaviour toward teammates and accep- tistical significance is influenced by sample size, and
tance of gamesmanship, which are behaviours used large samples produce more reliable findings, future
to enhance team performance by taking an unfair research should further examine the relationship
advantage over the opponent. Perhaps players who between the two prosocial behaviour subscales and
engage in prosocial teammate behaviours such as the sport-related variables examined in Study 2 to
supporting and encouraging their teammates also better estimate the magnitude of their relationships.
do this to enhance their team’s performance, and Finally, future research could examine the degree to
for this reason they may also be more accepting of which the prosocial and antisocial behaviours mea-
gamesmanship behaviours that may promote team sured by the PABSS correspond to actual behaviours
success. It is also possible that this unexpected find- occurring during a match. This could be achieved by
ing occurred by chance and may not be replicated in recording the behaviours measured by the PABSS
future research. during a match (e.g., from videotaped matches)
administering the PABSS after the match, and deter-
mining the relationship between observed and
Test-retest reliability and stability
reported behaviours. A strong relationship between
Test-retest reliability and stability of the PABSS observed and reported behaviours would provide
were examined in Study 1. Intraclass correlations further evidence for the construct validity of the
for all four subscales across a one-week period, PABSS.
1220 M. Kavussanu et al.

Conclusion exercise psychology measurement (pp. 257–276). Morgantown,


WV: FIT Press.
In conclusion, the present research provided the Brewer, M. B. (2000). Research design and issues of validity. In
first evidence for test-retest reliability and stability of H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods
the PABSS as well as additional evidence for its con- in social psychology (pp. 339–369). New York, NY: Cambridge
current and discriminant validity. All four subscales University Press.
Bureau, J. S., Vallerand, R. J., Ntoumanis, N., & Lafreniere, M.
were associated with a number of theoretically related A. K. (2012). On passion and moral behaviour in achievement
variables in the hypothesised direction. All four sub- settings: The mediating role of pride. Motivation and Emotion.
scales also showed very good levels of discriminant Advance online publication. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9292-7
validity, as reflected in the varying strength of their Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire.
relationships and links with the variables measured in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452–459.
Carlo, G., Raffaelli, M., Laible, D. J., & Meyer, K. A. (1999).
this study. Finally, strong evidence for convergent Why are girls less physically aggressive than boys? Personality
validity was provided for the antisocial opponent and parenting mediators of physical aggression. Gender Roles,
behaviour subscale. Although we had some unex- 40, 711–729.
pected results, overall, the current findings support Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112,
the reliability and construct validity of the PABSS. 155–159.
Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination
of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78,
98–104.
Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual
References
differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in
Allen, J. (2003). Social motivation in youth sport. Journal of Psychology, 10, 85.
Sport & Exercise Psychology, 25, 551–567. Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empa-
Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing. Upper thy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.
Aquino, K., Freeman, D., Reed, A. II, Lim, V. K. G., & Felps, W. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In
(2009). Testing a social cognitive model of moral behaviour: N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol 3: Social,
The interaction of situational factors and moral identity cen- emotional, and personality development (pp. 701–778). New York,
trality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 123–141. NY: Wiley.
Aquino, K., & Reed, A. (2002). The self-importance of moral Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to
identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, prosocial and related behaviours. Psychological Bulletin, 101,
1423–1440. 91–119.
Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Hodge, K., & Lonsdale, C. (2011). Prosocial and antisocial beha-
Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, viour in sport: The role of coaching style, autonomous vs.
38, 1145–1160. controlled motivation, and moral disengagement. Journal of
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of moral thought and Sport and Exercise Psychology, 33, 527–547.
action. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Handbook of John, O. P., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Measurement:
moral behaviour and development: Theory, research, and applica- Reliability, construct validation, and scale construction. In H. T.
tions (Vol. 1, pp. 71–129). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in
Associates. social psychology (pp. 339–369). New York, NY: Cambridge
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of University Press.
inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, Kavussanu, M. (2008). Moral behaviour in sport: A critical review
193–209. of the literature. International Review of Sport and Exercise
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression. Boston, MA: McGraw Hill. Psychology, 1, 124–138.
Boardley, I. D., & Jackson, B. (2012). When teammates are Kavussanu, M. (2012). Moral behaviour in sport. In S. Murphy
viewed as rivals: A cross-national investigation of achievement (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of sport and performance psychology
goals and intrateam moral behavior. Journal of Sport & Exercise (pp. 364–383). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Psychology, 34, 503–524. Kavussanu, M., & Boardley, I. D. (2009). The Prosocial and
Boardley, I. D., & Kavussanu, M. (2008). The Moral Antisocial Behaviour in Sport Scale. Journal of Sport &
Disengagement in Sport Scale – Short. Journal of Sports Exercise Psychology, 31, 97–117.
Sciences, 26, 1507–1517. Kavussanu, M., & Boardley, I. D. (2012). Moral behaviour. In G.
Boardley, I. D., & Kavussanu, M. (2009). The influence of social Tenenbaum, R. J. Eklund, & A. Kamata (Eds.), Handbook of
variables and moral disengagement on prosocial and antisocial measurement in sport and exercise psychology (pp. 443–454).
behaviours in field hockey and netball. Journal of Sports Sciences, Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
27, 843–854. Kavussanu, M., Stamp, R., Slade, G., & Ring, C. (2009).
Boardley, I. D., & Kavussanu, M. (2010). Effects of goal orienta- Observed prosocial and antisocial behaviours in male and
tion and perceived value of toughness on antisocial behaviour: female soccer players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21,
The mediating role of moral disengagement. Journal of Sport 62–76.
and Exercise Psychology, 33, 176–192. Kline, R. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation model-
Bolter, N. D., & Weiss, M. R. (2013). Coaching behaviours ing (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guildford Press.
and adolescent athletes’ sportspersonship outcomes: Further Lane, A. M., Nevill, A. M., Bowes, N., & Fox, K. R. (2005).
validation of the Sportsmanship Coaching Behaviours Investigating indices of stability using the task and ego orienta-
Scale (SCBS). Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 2, tion questionnaire. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76,
32–47. 339–346.
Bredemeier, B. J. L., & Shields, D. L. (1998). Moral assessment Lee, M. J., Whitehead, J., & Ntoumanis, N. (2007). Development
in sport psychology. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and of the Attitudes to Moral Decision-Making in Youth Sport
Prosocial and antisocial behaviour 1221

Questionnaire (AMDYSQ). Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, Sage, L., & Kavussanu, M. (2008). Goal orientations, moti-
369–392. vational climate, and prosocial and antisocial behaviour
Maxwell, J. P., & Moores, E. (2007). The development of a short in youth football: Exploring their temporal stability and
scale measuring aggressiveness and anger in competitive ath- reciprocal relationships. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26,
letes. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 179–193. 717–732.
McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about Sage, L., Kavussanu, M., & Duda, J. L. (2006). Goal orientations
some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods, and moral identity as predictors of prosocial and antisocial
1, 30–46. functioning in male association football players. Journal of
Miller, P. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to Sports Sciences, 24, 455–466.
aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behaviour. Psychological Schutz, R. W. (1998). Assessing the stability of psychological traits
Bulletin, 103, 324–344. and measures. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise
Nevill, A., Lane, A. M., Kilgour, L., Bowes, N., & Whyte, G. psychology measurement (pp. 393–408). Morgantown, WV
(2001). Stability of psychometric questionnaires. Journal of Fitness Information Technology.
Sports Sciences, 19, 273–278. Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., Cumming, S. P., & Grossbard, J.
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic educa- R. (2006). Measurement of multidimensional sport perfor-
tion. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. mance trait anxiety in children and adults: The Sport
Ntoumanis, N., & Standage, M. (2009). Morality in sport: A self- Anxiety Scale-2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
determination theory perspective. Journal of Applied Sport 28, 479–501.
Psychology, 21, 365–380. Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., & Schutz, R. W. (1990). Measurement
Pedhazur, E. J., & Schmelkin, L. (1991). Measurement, design, correlates of sport-specific cognitive and somatic anxiety: The
and analysis: An integrated approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Sport Anxiety Scale. Anxiety Research, 2, 263–280.
Erlbaum Associates. Spearman, C. (1904). The proof and measurement of associa-
Roberts, G. C., Treasure, D. C., & Balague, G. (1998). tion between two things. American Journal of Psychology, 15,
Achievement goals in sport: The development and validation 72–101.
of the Perception of Success Questionnaire. Journal of Sports Terwee, C. B., Bot, S. D. M., de Boer, M. R., van der Windt, D.
Sciences, 16, 337–347. A. W. M., Knol, D. L., Dekker, J., & de Vet, H. C. W. (2007).
Sage, L., & Kavussanu, M. (2007). Multiple goal orientations as Quality criteria were proposed for measurement properties of
predictors of moral behaviour in youth. The Sport Psychologist, health status questionnaire. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 60,
21, 417–437. 34–42.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen