Sie sind auf Seite 1von 11

811153

research-article2018
AUT0010.1177/1362361318811153AutismÅsberg Johnels et al.

Original Article

Autism

Current profiles and early predictors of 1­–11


© The Author(s) 2018
Article reuse guidelines:
reading skills in school-age children with sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/1362361318811153
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361318811153

autism spectrum disorders: A longitudinal, journals.sagepub.com/home/aut

retrospective population study

Jakob Åsberg Johnels1, Emilia Carlsson1, Courtenay Norbury2,


Christopher Gillberg1 and Carmela Miniscalco1

Abstract
This study explores current reading profiles and concurrent and early predictors of reading in children with autism
spectrum disorder. Before the age of 3 years, the study cohort underwent a neurodevelopmental assessment following
identification in a population-based autism screening. At age 8 years, reading, language and cognition were assessed.
Approximately half of the sample (n = 25) were ‘poor readers’ at age 8 years, meaning that they scored below the normal
range on tests of single word reading and reading comprehension. And 18 were ‘skilled readers’ performing above cut-
offs. The final subgroup (n = 10) presented with a ‘hyperlexic/poor comprehenders’ profile of normal word reading, but
poor reading comprehension. The ‘poor readers’ scored low on all assessments, as well as showing more severe autistic
behaviours than ‘skilled readers’. Group differences between ‘skilled readers’ and ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’
were more subtle: these subgroups did not differ on autistic severity, phonological processing or non-verbal intelligence
quotient, but the ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ scored significantly lower on tests of oral language. When data from
age 3 were considered, no differences were seen between the subgroups in social skills, autistic severity or intelligence
quotient. Importantly, however, it was possible to identify oral language weaknesses in those that 5 years later presented
as ‘poor readers’ or ‘hyperlexics’.

Keywords
autism spectrum disorders, communication and language, literacy, longitudinal, reading, school-age children

Introduction (aged 6–15 years, M = 10.33), 21 children scored either


below the normal range (i.e. standard score below 85), at
Literacy skills are important for lifelong learning, employ- floor levels or were non-readers in terms of single word
ment and independence in our society and studies exploring reading/decoding skills. Another subgroup (n = 10) was
the reading profiles and reading difficulties of children with classified as skilled in word reading/decoding and reading
autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) have grown in recent comprehension on the basis of their performance on age-
years. Much of this research has focused on the mismatch referenced test scores. Finally, another 10 children dis-
between stronger single word reading/decoding and weaker played the ‘hyperlexic’ profile described above. Of
reading comprehension observed in some children with ASD particular interest to Nation et al. (2006) were the differ-
(e.g. Huemer and Mann, 2010), with the term ‘hyperlexia’ ences (and similarities) between the two latter subgroups;
sometimes being used to describe an extreme version of this
profile (cf. Aaron, 2012; Grigorenko et al., 2003; Nation,
1Instituteof Neuroscience and Physiology, University of Gothenburg,
1999; Ostrolenk et al., 2017). Other studies have, however,
Sweden
noted a very considerable heterogeneity in reading capacity 2University College London, UK
among children with ASD (Åsberg et al., 2010; Brown et al.,
2013; Nation et al., 2006; Norbury and Nation, 2011; White Corresponding author:
Jakob Åsberg Johnels, Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Sahlgrenska
et al., 2006). Academy, University of Gothenburg, Kungsgatan 12, Box 500, 411 19
Nation et al. (2006) utilized a subgrouping procedure to Gothenburg, Sweden.
characterize this heterogeneity. In a sample of 41 children Email: jakob.asberg@gnc.gu.se
2 Autism 00(0)

results showed that participants with the ‘hyperlexic/poor A third research strand has addressed the association(s)
comprehender’ profile had difficulties not only with read- between autistic symptom severity and different reading
ing comprehension but also with language (listening) com- skills. A few studies have reported an association between
prehension more generally, relative to ‘skilled readers’ with increased autistic symptoms and reduced reading compre-
ASD. In contrast, the ‘skilled readers’ and the ‘hyperlexic’ hension (Åsberg et al., 2010; McIntyre et al., 2017;
subgroups were not differentiated by non-verbal cognitive Ricketts et al., 2012; Westerveld et al., 2017). Researchers
ability. These cross-sectional results fit well with the gener- have suggested that poor reading comprehension in ASD
ally accepted conclusion in non-ASD reading research: might reflect the underlying social–communicative diffi-
reading comprehension builds on a foundation of oral lan- culties that by definition are integral to ASD. For instance,
guage (i.e. listening) comprehension (Hoover and Gough, impaired coherence making and inferencing skills (e.g.
1990; Hulme and Snowling, 2013). In particular, the so- Ricketts et al., 2013) and/or difficulties in identifying
called ‘Simple View of Reading’ proposes that reading author intentions and tracking the mental states of charac-
comprehension is the product of decoding skills and oral ters in the texts (cf. Hulme and Snowling, 2013) could
language/listening comprehension; deficits in oral lan- affect reading comprehension beyond the role of oral lan-
guage therefore place a primary constraint on reading com- guage comprehension. However, another study showed
prehension in fluent word readers (Hoover and Gough, that this influence of autistic severity on reading compre-
1990). hension attenuated when considered in addition to the con-
Previous research has also explored the extent to which tributions of word reading/decoding and language capacity
word reading development in ASD is coupled with phono- in a multifactorial analysis (Lucas and Norbury, 2014). Yet
logical processing capacity, a lower level linguistic skill another study only found an association between increased
often impaired in poor word readers without ASD (i.e. dys- social impairment and alphabetic knowledge (but not with
lexics; Hulme and Snowling, 2013; White et al., 2006). reading comprehension; Davidson and Weismer, 2014).
Conversely, phonological processing capacity is well Hence, existing results are mixed to date and may reflect
developed in skilled word readers, with a presumed causal differences in samples in terms of age, sample size and
influence on word reading development (Hulme et al., population representativeness. Elucidating the extent to
2012), although the influence between phonology and which reading comprehension difficulties in ASD can be
word reading is likely bidirectional (Nation and Hulme, explained by word reading/decoding and/or language (lis-
2011; Peterson et al., 2018). Nation et al. (2006) did not tening) comprehension difficulties, as stipulated by the
explicitly assess phonological processing in their partici- Simple View (Hoover and Gough, 1990), is of both practi-
pants with ASD, but suggested that the ‘hyperlexic’ group cal and theoretical importance.
may not present with phonological difficulties despite Drawing any causal conclusions based on the cross-
being weak in non-phonological language domains (e.g. sectional results should be approached with caution.
semantic processing). A dissociation between phonologi- Longitudinal studies provide evidence of developmental
cal and non-phonological language skills may therefore primacy that can inform causal theories. Davidson and
explain why these children were able to develop skilled Weismer (2014) assessed various cognitive, linguistic and
word reading while failing to develop age-appropriate autism severity measures at mean age 2½ years in 101 chil-
reading comprehension. This hypothesis is also in line dren with ASD, all of which were longitudinally related to
with the two-dimensional (2D) model of language and early literacy skills when the children entered kindergar-
reading difficulties proposed by Bishop and Snowling ten. In keeping with previous research, the sample tended
(2004). This model extends the Simple View of Reading in to perform better on tasks tapping alphabetic and early
the sense that both phonological and broader language (lis- decoding skills than on comprehension and meaning.
tening) skills are put forward as differentially important Moreover, the results showed that the best longitudinal
factors in children’s literacy development. Children with prediction model of reading comprehension (the ‘Meaning
impaired phonological and listening comprehension subtest’) at age 5.5 years was multifactorial. Of these 2.5-
capacity will accordingly be challenged in both single year variables, non-verbal cognition and expressive lan-
word reading/decoding and reading comprehension, guage stood out as the most significant contributors to later
whereas some children with language comprehension dif- reading comprehension. Similarly, Miller et al. (2017)
ficulties without phonological difficulties will have rela- demonstrated that language ability at age 2 years predicted
tively more selective deficits in reading comprehension. school-age reading comprehension in 26 children with
Studies have empirically confirmed that phonology under- ASD. Both of these studies concluded that oral language
pins word reading/decoding capacity in ASD as it does in skills provide a foundation for later reading comprehen-
non-ASD individuals (Åsberg and Sandberg, 2012; sion, although there appeared to be additional influences
Newman et al., 2007; White et al., 2006), but it is less clear of other early predictors, including non-verbal cognitive
if this is the case when comprehension difficulties are also ability and autistic severity. Neither of these two longitudi-
evident (Bishop and Snowling, 2004). nal studies applied a subgrouping procedure; instead, they
Åsberg Johnels et al. 3

predicted the full variation of reading skills in collapsed Child Neuropsychiatry Clinic (CNC) around 3 years of
convenience samples. ages (Kantzer et al., 2013, 2018) focusing on autism
Although dimensional approaches are typically preferred diagnostics/assessment, language ability, cognitive level
because they increase statistical power, it is more difficult to and adaptive functioning. All the involved professionals
identify cases in which reading comprehension and word then met and made a consensus diagnosis based on all
reading/decoding capacity are decoupled. Consequently, available information, that is, test results, observation
this article considers early precursors to qualitatively differ- data, parental questionnaires and interviews.
ent reading profiles, including the hyperlexic/poor compre- The current follow-up assessment took place when the
hender profile (e.g. Nation et al., 2006). children were between 5.9 and 9.8 years old (n = 85; 15
Furthermore, with the exception of the population- girls and 70 boys) and included measures of oral language,
based sample from the Special Needs and Autism Project non-verbal functioning and literacy. Assessments were
(SNAP) study (Jones et al., 2009; Ricketts et al., 2013), made by two speech and language pathologists (SLPs)
previous research on reading and ASD has utilized con- during one or two sessions at the CNC. Inclusion criteria
venience and/or clinical samples, making it hard to inter- stipulated that children were in the first or second grade of
pret how common different reading profiles are within the school (7–8 years of age); 27 children of the 85 were
ASD population. Therefore, this study employed a popula- excluded since they had either not started first grade (n =
tion-based sample and a longitudinal, retrospective study 24) or entered third grade at the time of assessment (see
design to examine concurrent reading profiles and their Supplementary online material for further information).
early predictors based on assessment at the age of 3 years. Another five participants were excluded because reading
Children had been identified via population-based screen- assessments were not completed due to time constraints
ing (Kantzer et al., 2013, 2018). At age 8 years, we used a and/or errors during administration. Thus, the total number
subgrouping procedure based on individual children’s of participating children was 53 with a mean age of 8.0
scores on standardized tests of word reading and reading (6.6–9.8) years (8 girls and 45 boys).
comprehension to determine the prevalence of different Of these 53 children, there were 5 who did not meet full
reading profiles. The identified subgroups were then com- Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
pared both concurrently and retrospectively on a range of (4th ed.; DSM-IV) criteria for an ASD diagnosis at their
linguistic, cognitive and social skills/autistic severity data. latest full autism assessment, but all were identified with
Three research questions were posed: autistic traits. We chose to include all participants in this
study, due to (1) the unique recruitment procedure and (2)
1. Which reading profiles can be identified within the that the eventual influence of ‘autistic severity’ (measured
sample? both dimensionally and categorically) on reading was one
2. To what extent are the reading profiles associated of the main issues we wished to explore.
with concurrent measures of language, phonologi-
cal processing, non-verbal cognitive ability and Tests and material
autistic severity?
Data from the assessment at school-age follow-up
3. To what extent are the reading profiles associated
with language, cognition, communication and Oral language skills (comprehension and production). Lan-
social functioning, and autistic severity measures guage comprehension was assessed using the Test for
taken at age 3 years? Reception of Grammar-2 (TROG-2; Bishop, 2003 (Swedish
version, 2009)) and receptive vocabulary using the Peabody
Picture Vocabulary Test – Third Edition (PPVT-III; Dunn
Methods and Dunn, 1997). TROG-2 involves matching orally pre-
sented sentences to the correct picture of a choice of four.
Participants
The results are presented as both raw scores (number of cor-
More than 100 children in Gothenburg, Sweden, were rectly solved blocks out of a maximum of 20) and standard
identified as having a suspected ASD by a population scores (M = 100, standard deviation (SD) = 15) based on
screening at 2.5 years of age (Nygren et al., 2012), at Swedish norms. The Cronbach’s alpha is reported to be 0.89.
child health care centres. According to Magnusson On the PPVT, the child listens to a word uttered by the asses-
(1997), 97.5% of all Swedish children participate in sor and then selects one of four pictures that best describes
screening at these centres. The project was called AUtism the word’s meaning. The test is not standardized for Swed-
Detection and Intervention in Early life (AUDIE) and ran ish children, and therefore the original American norms were
from 2009 to 2011. Of the children who screened positive used. The Cronbach’s alpha is reported to be 0.95 in the
for ASD, parents of 107 children gave their consent to manual.
participate in the current research project. These children We indexed language production (Klem et al., 2015)
were assessed in depth by a multidisciplinary team at the using the ‘Recalling Sentences’ subtest from the Clinical
4 Autism 00(0)

Evaluation of Language Fundamentals − 4 (CELF 4; Semel ity (Wechsler, 1999). Results are expressed in raw and
et al., 2003; Swedish version, 2013). ‘Recalling Sentences’ T-scores (M = 50, SD = 10) based on American norms.
consists of 24 sentences which are repeated verbatim by the No Swedish norms are available.
child and scored on a four-point scale depending on the
number of errors present in the child’s repetition. We pre- Autism symptomatology.  The Autism Spectrum Screen-
sent the results as both raw scores and scaled scores (around ing Questionnaire (ASSQ; Ehlers et al., 1999) uses parent
a normative M = 10, SD = 3 based on Swedish norms). ratings on 27 items with a three-point Likert-type scale to
The Cronbach’s alpha is reported to be 0.89. measure autistic symptomatology. Test–retest reliability
is reported to be high (r = 0.96), and validity was estab-
Phonological processing/non-word repetition. Non-word lished by Ehlers et al. (1999) and by Posserud et al. (2006),
repetition was assessed with 30, one- to five-syllable non- showing a clear correspondence between total score on
words that conform to Swedish phonotactics (Radeborg the ASSQ and a clinical consensus diagnosis of ASD. A
et al., 2006). The children repeated the non-words after the screening cut-off for ASD of >18 has been suggested
SLP’s oral presentation. Norms are available for children (Ehlers et al., 1999).
aged 4–6 years. The SLP marked the responses online using
broad phonetic transcription according to the International Data from the first assessment at age 3 years. Not every
Phonetic Alphabet (IPA; Decker, 1999). Each repeated child could participate in all tasks described below, and
non-word was immediately scored as correct or incorrect. therefore the n value on each task is presented in Table 2.
Radeborg et al. report a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.74.
Assessment of cognitive/developmental level.  A psycholo-
Letter knowledge.  The child was asked to name the let- gist assessed children’s general cognitive and developmen-
ters of the Swedish alphabet (which includes three let- tal level using the Griffiths’ developmental scales (GDS;
ters besides those represented in English), written on two Alin-Åkerman and Norberg, 1991). The test includes six
sheets of paper in both uppercase and lowercase forms. subscales; the total score (M = 100, SD = 15) from the
The maximum score for each form is 24. subscales provides a developmental quotient (DQ) which
is used here. McLean et al. (1991) report adequate psycho-
Single word reading/decoding. The LÄST test (Elwér metric properties in terms of both internal consistency reli-
et al., 2009) was used to examine participants’ single ability (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.96) and construct validity
word reading/decoding ability. The task is to read as many according to correlation patterns with other tests of cogni-
words as possible in 45 s, from two lists of words. A total tive functioning.
score is calculated by summarizing the number of cor-
rectly read words. Such efficiency measures – rather than Autism symptomatology.  The Autism Diagnostic Obser-
separate accuracy and fluency measures – are typically vation Schedule – Generic (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000) is
used in Sweden and other semi-consistent orthographies. a standardized, semi-structured play-based assessment of
Swedish norms are available in the manual based on the communication, reciprocal social interaction, play and
stanine scale (i.e. around a mean of M = 5, SD = 2) for behaviour. Either module 1 or module 2 was adminis-
each grade year. The test–retest reliability is reported to be tered, based on the expressive language level of the child.
r = 0.93 for both lists in the Swedish manual. From these data, calibrated severity scores were calculated
(scores from 1 to 10; Hus et al., 2014). Higher scores indi-
Reading comprehension. The DLS Bas test (Järpsten, cate increased autistic symptom severity.
2004) was used in order to assess the child’s reading com-
prehension. The test comprises 20 sentences intertwined Adaptive, communicative and social functioning. The
into a small story. For each sentence, there are five pic- Communication and Socialization domains of the Vine-
tures and the child should mark the picture that best can be land Adaptive Behaviour Scales (VABS; Sparrow et al.,
linked with the written content. The child is asked to read 2005) were administered by a child neuropsychologist in
as many items as possible in 7 min (for 7-year-old children, a face-to-face interview with one or both parents. Results
i.e., school year one in Sweden) or in 5 min (for 8-year-old are expressed in standard scores around a normative M =
children, i.e., school year two in Sweden), with a maxi- 100 and SD = 15.
mum possible score of 20. Swedish norms are available
in the manual based on stanine scores for each grade year. Oral comprehension and production language skills. Lan-
The test–retest reliability is reported to be r = 0.78. guage comprehension was assessed with the Reynell
Developmental Language Scales III (RDLS; Edwards
Non-verbal cognitive ability. The matrix reasoning sub- et al., 1997) which has Swedish norms (Eriksson and
test of Wechsler abbreviated scales of intelligence (WASI) Grundström, 2000; Lindström and Åström, 2000). The
was used as a measure of non-verbal cognitive abil- Kuder–Richardson reliability coefficient is reported to
Åsberg Johnels et al. 5

be 0.97 in the manual from Great Britain. In addition, the scored below cut-off on both single word reading/decoding
expressive language level of each child was rated by the (Mstanine = 1.0; SD = 0.2) and reading comprehension
SLP on a scale from 1 to 5 using the PARIS scale (Philippe (Mstanine = 1.0, SD = 0.2). Another 10 participants were
et al., 1999): 1 = no words at all; 2 = a few single words; 3 assigned in the ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ group,
= a few communicative sentences; 4 = talks a great deal, meaning that they scored above cut-off in word reading/
mostly echolalia; or 5 = talks a great deal, mostly in a decoding (Mstanine = 5.9, SD = 1.0) but below cut-off in
communicative fashion. reading comprehension (Mstanine 1.7, SD = 0.5). For all
‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’, there was a substantial
Statistical analyses discrepancy in the word reading versus reading compre-
hension performance of at least three stanine scores. Finally,
The variables were subject to normality checks. First, the 18/53 was assigned as belonging to a ‘skilled readers’ sub-
reading scores (single word reading/decoding and reading group since they performed above cut-off on both word
comprehension) analysed dimensionally in the full group reading/decoding (Mstanine = 7.1, SD = 1.3) and reading
were found to diverge from the normal distribution (with comprehension (Mstanine = 5.8, SD = 1.6). Note that none
many scores at floor); for this reason, we cannot complement showed the profile of poor word decoding and relatively
the following results based on subgrouping with dimensional better reading comprehension (that sometimes is seen in
analyses across the full range of abilities. The other variables higher-functioning samples of dyslexic readers); hence,
that were compared across subgroups displayed kurtosis and only three subgroups of interest were identified here.
skewness statistics indicative of approximately normally dis- The letter knowledge task was not used for subgroup
tribution (values < 1.2), with the possible exception of the assignment, but demonstrated that the majority of children
PARIS scale. A histogram inspection revealed that this vari- in the ‘poor readers’ groups were preliterate. As a group,
able seemed to be bimodal, with few scores of 3. Therefore, they were only able to identify half of the letters in the
we chose to transform the PARIS scale into a dichotomous alphabet, with 11 children not being able to recognize any
variable, with the scores 1 and 2 representing less/minimally letters at all (see Table 1 for descriptive data).
verbal and 3, 4 and 5 representing relatively more verbal.
RQ2: Are the reading profiles associated with
Results concurrent measures of language, phonological
Attrition from intake at age 3 years to the processing, non-verbal cognitive ability and
current school-age follow-up autistic severity?
Table 1 presents the descriptive data for each of the three
In order to evaluate the population representativeness of
reading groups on all measures of interest from the school-
the sample, we compared those who participated in the
age follow-up assessment. Since the groups differed on
reading assessments at school-age follow-up with the rest
age (F[2, 50] = 8.36, p = 0.001, η p2 = 0.251 ) with post
of the screen-positive cohort on the assessments of interest
hoc test showing that the ‘skilled readers’ subgroup was
from intake. Those who did not participate were on aver-
somewhat older (p < 0.01) than the other two subgroups
age 3 months younger when they were assessed for the first
(who in turn did not differ from one another, p = 0.616),
time (p = 0.023). On all other assessments – for the GDS
we use analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with control for
(DQ total score), the RDLS test of language comprehen-
chronological age in all subsequent group comparisons.
sion, the ADOS (severity total score), the Vineland sociali-
Raw scores rather than age-standardized scores were used
zation and the Vineland communication scores – no
in these analyses in order not to control for age at multiple
significant differences were observed (all p > 0.20; see
times; however, for descriptive purposes age-standardized
Supplementary online material for details).
score values are also reported in Table 1.
In terms of oral language, there were significant group
RQ1: Which reading profiles can be identified differences on TROG-2 (F[2, 49] = 17.95, p < 0.001,
η p2 = 0.423 ), PPVT-III (F[2, 49] = 9.24, p < 0.001,
within the sample? η p2 = 0.274 ) and Recalling Sentences (repetition) results
Subgrouping was performed based on a cut-off of a stanine (F[2, 49] = 13.87, p < 0.001, η p2 = 0.361 ). Post hoc com-
score of ⩽2 on standardized assessments on single word parisons on TROG-2 showed that the ‘skilled readers’ per-
reading/decoding and reading comprehension (which cor- formed significantly better than both the ‘hyperlexics/poor
responds to the ~10th percentile); this is a common cut-off comprehenders’ (p < 0.01) and the ‘poor readers’ (p <
both in research and in Swedish schools to identify children 0.001). In turn, ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ per-
with reading difficulties. Results showed that almost half of formed better than the ‘poor readers’ (p = 0.032). A
the sample (25/53) was classified into a subgroup that will slightly different pattern was obtained on PPVT-III and
henceforward be called ‘poor readers’, meaning that they Recalling Sentences tasks, in which ‘skilled readers’
6 Autism 00(0)

Table 1.  Outcome measures for the three groups: ‘poor readers’, ‘hyperlexic/poor comprehenders’ and ‘skilled readers’ at the
school-year assessment.

Results at the school-year Mean (SD) Group differences using ANCOVA with
follow-up correction for age
Poor readers Hyperlexic/poor Skilled readers
(n = 25) comprehenders (n = 18)
(n = 10)
Age (years) 7.8 (0.7) 7.6 (0.4) 8.4 (0.5) Poor readers = hyperlexic < skilled readers
Language ability
  PPVT-III (raw score) 51.9 (38.9) 70.1 (12.7) 103.7 (39.5) Poor readers = hyperlexic < skilled readers
  PPVT-III (ss)a 63.5 (26.8) 74.1 (9.0) 94.4 (26.8)
  TROG-2 (block score) 3.9 (4.7) 7.4 (3.8) 13.5 (4.4) Poor readers < hyperlexic < skilled readers
  TROG-2 (ss)a 61.6 (14.1) 68.3 (15.1) 89.7 (21.4)
 Recalling Sentences 10.1 (13.3) 19.6 (11.0) 32.8(14.8) Poor readers = hyperlexic < skilled readers
CELF-4 (raw score)
 Recalling Sentences 3.5 (4.3) 6.1 (4.3) 10.1 (5.0)
CELF-4 (scs)b
 Non-word repetition 7.9 (8.8) 21.0 (3.6) 21.1 (8.2) Poor readers < hyperlexic = skilled readers
(max 30) (raw score)
Autism symptomatology
 ASSQc (raw score) 25.1 (9.4) 14.6 (9.1) 16.9 (7.2) Poor readers < hyperlexic = skilled readers
Non-verbal ability
 Matrix reasoning 5.0 (5.6) 12.4(6.3) 14.9 (6.6) Poor readers < hyperlexic = skilled
(raw score)
 Matrix reasoning 35.0 (10.1) 49.0 (9.2) 48.7(10.5)
(T-score)d

SD: standard deviation; ANCOVA: analysis of covariance; PPVT-III: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Third Edition; TROG-2: Test for Reception
of Grammar-2; CELF-4: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals − 4; ASSQ: Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire.
aStandard scores (M = 100, SD = 15).
bScaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3).
cn = 17 of the ‘skilled readers’ parents completed the ASSQ.
dT-scores (M = 50, SD = 10).

performed significantly better than both the ‘hyperlexics/ Comparing the groups on the matrix reasoning subtest of
poor comprehenders’ (both p ⩽ 0.01) and the ‘poor read- non-verbal cognitive ability revealed a significant group differ-
ers’ (both p < 0.001), who in turn did not differ from one ence (F[2, 48] = 12.00, p < 0.001, η p2 = 0.333 ), with post hoc
another on either measure (p = 0.201 and p = 0.786, analyses showing no significant difference between the ‘skilled
respectively). Mean scores in the different groups revealed readers’ and the ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ (p = 0.604),
that the ‘skilled readers’ scored within the normal range on while the ‘poor readers’ scored lower than both the ‘skilled
the standardized measures, whereas the ‘poor readers’ and readers’ (p < 0.001) and the ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’
the ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ scored substantially (p < 0.01). Mean scores in the different groups revealed that
below the normal range. ‘skilled readers’ and ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ scored
Turning to phonological processing, a significant group within the normal range, whereas the ‘poor readers’ scored
difference was found on the non-word repetition task (F[2, approximately 1.5 SDs below the normative mean.
48] = 18.85, p < 0.001, η p2 = 0.440 ). Post hoc tests showed With regard to autistic severity, the groups differed on
ASSQ total scores (F[2, 47] = 7.49, p < 0.01, η p = 0.242
2
that the ‘skilled readers’ and the ‘hyperlexics/poor compre-
henders’ did not differ from one another (p = 0.786), while ), with post hoc comparisons revealing that ‘poor readers’
both these groups outperformed the ‘poor readers’ (both p < had higher symptom severity scores than either of the
0.001). There are no norms available for the current age other groups (both p < 0.01), who did not differ from one
group, but a comparison with norms for 4- to 6-year-olds another (p = 0.807). Although the mean scores on the
shows that the ‘poor readers’ perform below the 25th percen- ASSQ fell below the suggested screening cut-off for ASD
tile for the 4-year-olds. By contrast, the ‘hyperlexic/poor in the ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ ( x̅ = 14.6) and
comprehenders’ and the ‘skilled readers’ scored equivalent to the ‘skilled readers’ ( x̅ = 16.9), it should be stressed that
the 95th percentile for 6-year-olds. these scores were well over 2 SDs above the population
Åsberg Johnels et al. 7

Table 2.  Results from the age 3-year assessment in the three subgroups: ‘poor readers’, ‘hyperlexic/poor comprehenders’ and
‘skilled readers’.

Results from the 3-year assessment Mean (SD) Group differences using
ANOVA
Poor Readers Hyperlexic/poor Skilled readers
(n = 25) comprehenders (n = 10) (n = 18)
Age (months) 35.4 (5.9) 36.1 (6.2) 38.8 (4.8) Poor readers = hyperlexic =
skilled readers
Language ability
  RDLS (raw score) 8.0 (11.3) 14.0 (11.2) 32.6a (20.0) Poor readers = hyperlexic <
skilled readers
  PARIS level (no. verbal 3–5) 10 (40%) 5 (50%) 17 (94%) Poor readers < skilled
readers
Autism symptomatology
  ADOS severity total (raw score) 6.1 (2.9) 4.7 (2.4) 4.4a (2.7) Poor readers = hyperlexic =
skilled readers
Developmental quotient
  Griffiths’ developmental scalesb 79.7c (18.5) 80.1c (11.2) 82.5c (21.8) Poor readers = hyperlexic =
skilled readers
Adaptive functioning
  VABS socializationb 73.2d (7.9) 74.9 (14.3) 78.5d (12.4) Poor readers = hyperlexic =
skilled readers
  VABS communicationb 70.9d (13.4) 71.0 (12.7) 81.8d (15.2) Poor readers < skilled
readers

SD: standard deviation; ANOVA: analysis of variance; ADOS: Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule – Generic; VABS: Vineland Adaptive
Behaviour Scales.
an = 1 of ‘skilled readers’ did not provide a score.
bStandard scores (M = 100; SD = 15). Not all children had been assessed with the instrument.
cn = 3 of ‘poor readers’, n = 2 of hyperlexic/poor comprehenders and n = 4 of ‘skilled readers’ did not provide a score.
dn = 1 of ‘poor readers’ and n = 2 of ‘skilled readers’ did not provide a score.

mean for 7- to 9-year-olds as described by Posserud et al. As predicted, however, when comparing language abil-
(2006). We also analysed whether the prevalence of a ity at intake, significant differences were evident on RDLS
clinical autism spectrum diagnosis differed between the language comprehension (F[2, 49] = 14.47, p < 0.001,
subgroups, with the results revealing a possible trend (χ2 η p2 = 0.371 ), the Vineland communication scale (F[2, 47]
= 3.36, p < 0.05, η p = 0.125 ) and the SLP rating of
2
= 5.01, p = 0.082). The five participants who did not
receive an ASD diagnosis were all classified as ‘skilled expressive language using the PARIS scale (χ2 = 13.52, p
readers’ (n = 3) or as ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ = 0.001). Post hoc (Tukey) tests showed that the ‘poor
(n = 2). readers’ and the ‘hyperlexics/poor comprehenders’ did not
differ on the RDLS or Vineland communication (p = 0.520
RQ3: Are the reading profiles associated and p = 1.00, respectively). By contrast, the ‘skilled read-
retrospectively with language, cognition, ers’ outperformed the ‘poor readers’ on both measures (all
communication and social functioning, and p < 0.05). In terms of PARIS ratings, inspection of adjusted
standardized residuals showed that ‘skilled readers’ were
autistic severity at age 3 years?
more likely to be verbal as toddlers relative to children with
Descriptive data for reading subgroups based on means poor reading skills who were less/minimally verbal. The
and SDs taken when the children were aged 3 are reported skilled readers also outperformed the ‘hyperlexics/poor
in Table 2. Chronological age at the 3-year assessment did comprehenders’ on the RDLS comprehension test (p <
not differ across the subgroups (F[2, 50] = 2.03, p = 0.01), whereas scores from the Vineland communication
0.143, η p2 = 0.075 ). subscale were not significantly different (p = 0.139).
Groups did not differ on cognitive ability assessed with
GDS (F[2, 43] = 0.097, p = 0.908, η p2 = 0.005 ), degree of
Discussion
autistic severity (as assessed with the ADOS-2 severity
scores (F[2, 49] = 2.22, p = 0.119, η p2 = 0.083 ) or on the This study aimed to address the heterogeneity present
Vineland socialization scores (F[2, 47] = 1.15, p = 0.324, among children with ASD in their early reading skills.
η p2 = 0.047 )). Mean scores indicated equal levels of Specifically, we investigated individual differences in
impairment in all groups. language comprehension and production, phonological
8 Autism 00(0)

deficits, cognitive level and autistic symptom severity in approaches targeting poor reading comprehenders without
subgroups identified by different profiles of word read- ASD (e.g. Clarke et al., 2010). Interestingly, preliminary
ing/decoding skills and reading comprehension. The intervention studies including children with ASD lend ini-
study allowed us to identify factors related to reading tial support for this idea (Bailey et al., 2017).
ability profiles both concurrently (mean age 8 years) and Consistent with Nation et al. (2006), our results demon-
retrospectively (around the age of 3). In addition, our par- strate that children with the ‘hyperlexic/poor compre-
ticipants were identified by screening and are thus popu- hender’ profile had age-appropriate phonological
lation representative to a greater extent than is the case in processing (as measured by the non-word repetition task),
previous research, reducing ascertainment bias in estab- even though they performed poorly on tests of oral lan-
lishing reading profiles. guage comprehension. Our results are thus in line with the
The most common reading profile in our sample was suggestion by Bishop and Snowling (2004) in their 2D
that of generally poor reading skills. By contrast, the model of language and reading difficulties, stating that the
‘hyperlexic/poor comprehenders’ profile was relatively less dissociation between (intact) phonological and (poor) non-
common, whereas a sizable minority (approximately 20%) phonological (semantic and syntactic) language skills
performed age-adequate on both single word reading/ might explain why some children are able to develop
decoding and reading comprehension. Hence, our results skilled word reading/decoding but poor reading compre-
replicate in a Swedish population sample the great hetero- hension. However, we need to highlight that in this study
geneity of reading skills and profiles within ASDs (e.g. we were only able to measure this association cross-sec-
Åsberg et al., 2010; Jones et al., 2009; Nation et al., 2006). tional. Indeed, whereas phonology has traditionally been
Our study also confirms the close relationship between interpreted as a ‘predictor’ of word reading development,
reading comprehension and oral language comprehension recent work (e.g. Nation and Hulme, 2011; Peterson et al.,
skills. The ‘poor readers’ subgroup was found to score low 2018) has shown that phonology, including non-word rep-
on most assessment, including non-verbal cognitive abil- etition and related skills, may be causally dependent on
ity, as well as showing a more severe autistic presentation. word reading level, not only vice versa. It is fully possible
Hence, the poor reading and language abilities of this that this is the case also in readers with ASD, and hence the
group occur in the context of more general delay. This causal mechanism in the phonology–word reading link
means that their reading comprehension and oral language observed here needs to be specified in future research.
weaknesses were accompanied by greater impairments Nevertheless, disentangling the complex relation between
across several developmental domains, making it difficult letter knowledge, phonological skills and single word
to propose a distinct relation between reading comprehen- reading might not be critical for being able to provide
sion and language comprehension deficits in ASD if only effective interventions for poor word readers with ASD, at
this subgroup is considered. By contrast, in the ‘hyperlex- least if intervention approaches used in dyslexia can be
ics/poor comprehenders’, the difficulty in oral language transferred to poor word readers with ASD. Indeed, phon-
and reading comprehension was much more selective ics-based training – where the reading intervention
when compared to the ‘skilled readers’, pointing more includes both letter-sound and phonological knowledge
clearly to a distinct association (cf. Nation et al., 2006). training – has been shown to be effective, whereas a pure
This finding is in keeping with the ‘Simple View of phonological approach has gained less clear effects
Reading’ (Hoover and Gough, 1990). (Statens beredning för medicinsk och social utvärdering
Another important feature of this study was the longitu- (SBU), 2014). Hence, phonology and word reading seems
dinal (retrospective) study design. Only two previous stud- developmentally intertwined, and this fact also affects effi-
ies (Davidson and Weismer, 2014; Miller et al., 2017) have cient teaching/intervention practices.
included analyses of developmental precursors to reading The association between ASD severity and reading pro-
ability in ASD. However, neither of these distinguished file varied depending on the time point considered. In early
between different reading profiles. Hence, it has not been school age, we found evidence of more severe autistic
fully clear to what extent the identified predictors are symptoms in the ‘poor readers’ subgroup, which we inter-
important for reading comprehension per se, as opposed to pret as an additional sign that this group has more perva-
aspects of reading comprehension difficulties accompanied sive developmental challenges. However, at age 3 years,
by single word reading/decoding difficulties. Nonetheless, these differences in autistic severity were obvious neither
our results are broadly in line with these previous studies, in the ADOS, nor in the parent report of social skills. These
demonstrating that before the age of 3 it may be possible to results suggest that it is probably hard or impossible to pre-
identify oral language weaknesses in both of the subgroups dict early reading skills from autistic behaviours/social
that presented 5 years later with poor reading comprehen- difficulties observed in toddlers alone.
sion. This result suggests that supporting oral language A related issue is to what extent autistic severity impacts
skills in ASD could be one way to support reading compre- reduced reading comprehension. Several authors (e.g.
hension in children with ASD, similar to intervention Åsberg Johnels et al., 2017; Jones et al., 2009; Ricketts
Åsberg Johnels et al. 9

et al., 2013; Westerveld et al., 2017) have suggested that to be predictable from and align well with established find-
impairments in social–communicative functioning and ings in general reading research: that word reading is
flexibility, on the one hand, and reading comprehension, on strongly associated with phonological skills, whereas read-
the other, may be coupled, possibly independently of basic ing comprehension builds on a foundation of oral language
language and cognitive skills. For instance, failing to skills. By age 3 years, it appears to be possible to identify
understand social and communicative norms may hamper a oral language weaknesses in children with ASD that 5 years
reader’s ability to make inferences and, consequently, con- later present as ‘poor readers’ or ‘hyperlexics/poor
strain the processing of the text content (e.g. Ricketts et al., comprehenders’.
2013). The results presented here do not easily support this
idea, since we could not find any difference in autistic Acknowledgements
severity between the skilled and the ‘hyperlexic/poor com- We thank SLP Anna-Clara Reinholdsson for assistance during
prehenders’ subgroups. This might mean that we need to data collection.
reconsider the hypothesis that ASD characteristics ‘per se’
influence text comprehension negatively. In terms of theo- Declaration of conflicting interests
retical implications, the results thus suggest that the Simple The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
View of Reading can be applied to explain the reading respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this
comprehension difficulties in this cohort of readers with article.
ASD. Still, we believe that the age of the study sample
needs to be considered more in future research. Our partici- Funding
pants attended first or second grade; during these first years
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
in school, the texts and tasks used in reading comprehen- for the research, authorship and/or publication of this article:
sion assessment are typically quite basic, and this was argu- This study was performed with support from the Swedish
ably the case in this study. One possibility is that with Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare (project
increasing age the child meets more complex texts and no. 2013-00092) and Queen Silvia Jubilee Foundation.
assessment procedures, which place greater demands on
the flexible usage of both language and social knowledge References
and strategies, such as making coherence inferences and Aaron PG (2012) Dyslexia and Hyperlexia: Diagnosis and
interpretations of author intentions (cf. Norbury and Nation, Management of Developmental Reading Disabilities, vol. 1.
2011). Hence, we propose that future research should con- New York: Springer.
sider whether the association between reduced reading Alin-Åkerman B and Norberg L (1991) Griffiths’ Development
comprehension and autistic severity is modulated by age, Scales I and II. Stockholm: Hogrefe Psykologiförlaget AB
as well as the text type/context that is used in assessment. (Swedish version).
Åsberg J and Sandberg AD (2012) Dyslexic, delayed, precocious
or just normal? Word reading skills of children with autism
Limitations and conclusions of this study spectrum disorders. Journal of Research in Reading 35(1):
There are a number of weaknesses of this study. First, 20–31.
Åsberg J, Kopp S, Berg-Kelly K, et al. (2010) Reading compre-
although the sample size is fairly large for a population-
hension, word decoding and spelling in girls with autism
based study of autism, it is nonetheless clearly the case that spectrum disorders (ASD) or attention-deficit/hyperac-
the subgroups are small. Hence, the results should be fur- tivity disorder (AD/HD): performance and predictors.
ther corroborated in larger samples. Another weakness is International Journal of Language & Communication
that no assessment of phonology was done at age 3 years. Disorders 45(1): 61–71.
That would have allowed us to assess the predictive value Åsberg Johnels J, Gillberg C and Kopp S (2017) A hyperlexic-like
of early phonological skills on single word reading/decod- reading style is associated with increased autistic features
ing. Finally, this study only focused on reading, whereas in girls with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. Epub
writing skills were not assessed. It has been argued that ahead of print 1 January. DOI: 10.1177/1087054716685838.
spelling performance and analyses of error patterns pro- Bailey B, Arciuli J and Stancliffe RJ (2017) Effects of
vide an important insight into the mechanisms of literacy ABRACADABRA literacy instruction on children
with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Educational
acquisition (Perfetti and Hart, 2002).
Psychology 109(2): 257.
Despite these caveats, the results of this unique popula- Bishop DV and Snowling MJ (2004) Developmental dyslexia
tion-based study of children with ASD confirm a high and specific language impairment: same or different?
degree of heterogeneity in reading skills in ASD. Given the Psychological Bulletin 130(6): 858.
importance of literacy for lifelong learning, employment Bishop DVM (2003) Test of Reception of Grammar-2 (T.R.O.G,
and independence, it is important that practitioners in the Swedish). Stockholm: Pearson.
field of ASD are aware of these various profiles of strengths Brown HM, Oram-Cardy J and Johnson A (2013) A meta-anal-
and difficulties. Results also show that the profiles appeared ysis of the reading comprehension skills of individuals on
10 Autism 00(0)

the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Klem M, Melby-Lervåg M, Hagtvet B, et al. (2015) Sentence rep-
Disorders 43(4): 932–955. etition is a measure of children’s language skills rather than
Clarke PJ, Snowling MJ, Truelove E, et al. (2010) Ameliorating working memory limitations. Developmental Science 18(1):
children’s reading-comprehension difficulties: a randomized 146–154.
controlled trial. Psychological Science 21(8): 1106–1116. Lindström M and Åström A-L (2000) Reynell Developmental
Davidson MM and Weismer SE (2014) Characterization and pre- Language Scales III. Normering Och Studie Over Samband
diction of early reading abilities in children on the autism Mellan Barns Testresultat Och Kön, Respektive Typ Av
spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Barnomsorg. Examensarbete i Logopedi, Umeå Universitet,
44(4): 828–845. Umeå.
Decker DM (1999) Handbook of the International Phonetic Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, et al. (2000) The Autism Diagnostic
Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Observation Schedule – Generic: a standard measure of
Alphabet. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. social and communication deficits associated with the
Dunn LM and Dunn LM (1997) PPVT-III: Peabody Picture spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Disorders 30(3): 205–223.
Service. Lucas R and Norbury CF (2014) Levels of text comprehension
Edwards S, Fletcher P, Garman M, et al. (1997) Reynell in children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD): the
Developmental Language Scales: Manual. Berkshire: The influence of language phenotype. Journal of Autism and
NFER-NELSON Publishing Company Limited. Developmental Disorders 44(11): 2756–2768.
Ehlers S, Gillberg C and Wing L (1999) A screening question- McIntyre NS, Solari EJ, Gonzales JE, et al. (2017) The scope and
naire for Asperger syndrome and other high-functioning nature of reading comprehension impairments in school-
autism spectrum disorders in school age children. Journal aged children with higher-functioning autism spectrum dis-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29(2): 129–141. order. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 47:
Elwér Å, Fridolfsson I, Samuelsson S, et al. (2009) Läst. 2838–2860.
Stockholm: Hogrefe Psykologiförlaget AB. McLean ME, McCormick K and Baird SM (1991) Concurrent
Eriksson L and Grundström P (2000) Reynell Developmental validity of the Griffiths’ Mental Development Scales with
Language Scales III, Språkförståelsedelen. Översättning Och a population of children under 24 months. Journal of Early
Normering, Samt Studie Över Samband Mellantestresultat Intervention 15(4): 338–344.
Och Föräldrars Utbildningsnivå. Examensarbete i Logopedi, Magnusson M (1997) Rationality of routine health examinations
Umeå Universitet, Umeå. by physicians of 18-month-old children: experiences based on
Grigorenko EL, Klin A and Volkmar F (2003) Annotation: data from a Swedish county. Acta Paediatrica 86: 881–887.
hyperlexia: disability or superability? Journal of Child Miller LE, Burke JD, Troyb E, et al. (2017) Preschool predictors
Psychology and Psychiatry 44(8): 1079–1091. of school-age academic achievement in autism spectrum
Hoover WA and Gough PB (1990) The Simple View of Reading. disorder. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 31(2): 382–403.
Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 127–160. Nation K (1999) Reading skills in hyperlexia: A developmental
Huemer SV and Mann V (2010) A comprehensive profile of decod- perspective. Psychological Bulletin 125(3): 33855.
ing and comprehension in autism spectrum disorders. Journal Nation K and Hulme C (2011) Learning to read changes chil-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40(4): 485–493. dren’s phonological skills: evidence from a latent vari-
Hulme C and Snowling MJ (2013) Developmental Disorders able longitudinal study of reading and nonword repetition.
of Language Learning and Cognition. Hoboken, NJ: John Developmental Science 14(4): 649–659.
Wiley & Sons. Nation K, Clarke P, Wright B, et al. (2006) Patterns of reading
Hulme C, Bowyer-Crane C, Carroll JM, et al. (2012) The causal ability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
role of phoneme awareness and letter-sound knowledge in Autism and Developmental Disorders 36(7): 911.
learning to read: combining intervention studies with medi- Newman TM, Macomber D, Naples AJ, et al. (2007) Hyperlexia
ation analyses. Psychological Science 23(6): 572–577. in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
Hus V, Gotham K and Lord C (2014) Standardizing ADOS Autism and Developmental Disorders 37(4): 760–774.
domain scores: separating severity of social affect and Norbury C and Nation K (2011) Understanding variability in
restricted and repetitive behaviours. Journal of Autism and reading comprehension in adolescents with autism spectrum
Developmental Disorders 44(10): 2400–2412. disorders: interactions with language status and decoding
Järpsten B (2004) DLS Bas För Skolår 1 Och 2 [DLS Bas for skill. Scientific Studies of Reading 15(3): 191–210.
Grades 1 and 2]. Stockholm: Hogrefe Psykologiförlaget AB. Nygren G, Sandberg E, Gillstedt F, et al. (2012) A new screening
Jones CR, Happé F, Golden H, et al. (2009) Reading and arith- programme for autism in a general population of Swedish
metic in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: peaks toddlers. Research in Developmental Disabilities 33(4):
and dips in attainment. Neuropsychology 23(6): 718. 1200–1210.
Kantzer AK, Fernell E, Gillberg C, et al. (2013) Autism in com- Ostrolenk A, d’Arc BF, Jelenic P, et al. (2017) Hyperlexia: sys-
munity pre-schoolers: developmental profiles. Research in tematic review, neurocognitive modelling, and outcome.
Developmental Disabilities 34(9): 2900–2908. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 79: 134–149.
Kantzer AK, Fernell E, Westerlund J, et al. (2018) Young chil- Perfetti CA and Hart L (2002) The lexical quality hypothesis.
dren who screen positive for autism: stability, change and In: Vehoeven L, Elbro C and Reitsma P (eds) Precursors
‘comorbidity’ over two years. Research in Developmental of Functional Literacy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins,
Disabilities 72: 297–307. pp.189–213.
Åsberg Johnels et al. 11

Peterson RL, Arnett AB, Pennington BF, et al. (2018) Literacy Insatser. En Systematisk Litteraturöversikt. Stockholm: SBU-
acquisition influences children’s rapid automatized nam- rapport nr 225.
ing. Developmental Science 21: e12589. Semel E, Wiig E and Secord W (2003) Clinical Evaluation
Philippe A, Martinez M, Guilloud-Bataille M, et al. (1999) of Language Fundamentals-IV. Marrickville: Harcourt
Genome-wide scan for autism susceptibility genes. Assessment (Swedish version, 2013).
Human Molecular Genetics 8(5): 805–812. Sparrow SS, Balla DA and Cicchetti DV (2005) Vineland II:
Posserud MB, Lundervold AJ and Gillberg C (2006) Autistic fea- Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN:
tures in a total population of 7–9-year-old children assessed American Guidance Service.
by the ASSQ (Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire). Wechsler D (1999) Manual for the Wechsler Abbreviated
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47(2): 167–175. Intelligence Scale (WASI). San Antonio, TX: The
Radeborg K, Barthelom E, Sjöberg M, et al. (2006) A Psychological Corporation.
Swedish non-word repetition test for preschool children. Westerveld MF, Paynter J, Trembath D, et al. (2017) The
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 47(3): 187–192. emergent literacy skills of preschool children with autism
Ricketts J, Jones CR, Happé F, et al. (2013) Reading com- spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
prehension in autism spectrum disorders: the role of oral Disorders 47(2): 424–438.
language and social functioning. Journal of Autism and White S, Frith U, Milne E, et al. (2006) A double dissociation
Developmental Disorders 43(4): 807–816. between sensorimotor impairments and reading disability:
Statens beredning för medicinsk och social utvärdering (SBU) a comparison of autistic and dyslexic children. Cognitive
(2014) Dyslexi Hos Barn Och Ungdomar – Tester Och Neuropsychology 23(5): 748–761.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen