Sie sind auf Seite 1von 16

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Evidence for Gender-Specific Endophenotypes in High-Functioning


Autism Spectrum Disorder During Empathy
Karla Schneider, Christina Regenbogen, Katharina D. Pauly, Anna Gossen, Daniel A. Schneider,
Lea Mevissen, Tanja M. Michel, Ruben C. Gur, Ute Habel, and Frank Schneider

Despite remarkable behavioral gender differences in patients with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and growing evidence
for a diminished male : female ratio for the putative “male disorder” ASD, aspects of gender are not addressed accordingly
in ASD research. Our study aims at filling this gap by exploring empathy abilities in a group of 28 patients with
high-functioning ASD and 28 gender-, age- and education-matched non-autistic subjects, for the first time by means of
functional neuroimaging (fMRI). In an event-related fMRI paradigm, emotional (“E”) and neutral (“N”) video clips
presented actors telling self-related short stories. After each clip, participants were asked to indicate their own emotion
and its intensity as well as the emotion and intensity perceived for the actor. Behaviorally, we found significantly less
empathic responses in the overall ASD group compared with non-autistic subjects, and inadequate emotion recognition
for the neutral clips in the female ASD group compared with healthy women. Neurally, increased activation of the
bilateral medial frontal gyrus was found in male patients compared with female patients, a pattern which was not present
in the non-autistic group. Additionally, autistic women exhibited decreased activation of midbrain and limbic regions
compared with non-autistic women, whereas there was no significant difference within the male group. While we did not
find a fundamental empathic deficit in autistic patients, our data propose different ways of processing empathy in autistic
men and women, suggesting stronger impairments in cognitive aspects of empathy/theory of mind for men, and
alterations of social reciprocity for women. Autism Res 2013, 6: 506–521. © 2013 International Society for Autism
Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Keywords: autism; empathy; gender differences; fMRI; social interactions

Introduction 1976], it is still commonly seen as a “male disorder”


[Baron-Cohen, 2002]. Though indeed all epidemiological
The diagnostic guidelines of the 10th revision of the surveys confirm overrepresentation of men in ASD
International Statistical Classification of Diseases and [Fombonne, 2005], the estimation of the sex ratio shows a
Related Health Problems (ICD-10) and the 4th edition of broader variety [Baird, 2000; Fombonne, 2005] than pre-
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders viously expected, with an estimated mean male : female
(DSM-IV) classify autism spectrum disorder (ASD) as a ratio of 4.3:1 [Fombonne, 2005]. For high-functioning
pervasive developmental disorder with serious impact on ASD, this ratio declines for females to 6–8:1 [Baird, 2000;
nearly all aspects of social functioning causing daily Fombonne, 2005]. However, several studies in line with
obstacles even for (very) high-functioning ASD patients. clinical experience [Attwood, 2008] suggest a more mod-
Supported by the “extreme male brain theory” erate male : female ratio of 4:1 [Ehlers & Gillberg, 1993] or
[Baron-Cohen, 2002], which assigns an extremely system- even 2.6:1 [Bartley, 2006; Tepest et al., 2010].
izing “male” cognitive style to patients with ASD, and A few studies approached potential female-specific ASD
influenced by prior data on the sex ratio in ASD [Baird, traits and, on a behavioral level, suggested females to be
2000; Fombonne, 2005; Wing, Yeates, Brierly, & Gould, more severely affected by ASD-typical cognitive deficits

From the Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Medical School, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen (K.S., C.R., K.D.P., A.G., D.S.,
L.M., T.M.M., U.H., F.S.); JARA-Translational Brain Medicine, Aachen-Jülich (K.S., C.R., K.D.P., A.G., D.S., U.H., F.S.); Department of Psychiatry, Perelman
School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania and the Philadelphia Veterans Administration Medical Center, Philadelphia (R.G., F.S.); Department of
Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Rostock, Rostock (T.M.M.)
Received November 9, 2012; accepted for publication June 13, 2013
Address for correspondence and reprints: Dr. Karla Schneider, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, RWTH Aachen
University, Pauwelsstraße 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany, E-mail: karla.schneider@rwth-aachen.de
Grant sponsors:
1. Grant sponsor: German Research Foundation (DFG, International Research Training Group 1328: “Brain-behavior relationship of emotion and social
cognition in schizophrenia and autism”)
Grant Number: KFO 112 and HA 3202/7-1
2. Grant sponsor: Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical Research of the Medical Faculty of the RWTH Aachen University
Grant number: IZKF, N2-6
Published online 18 July 2013 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com)
DOI: 10.1002/aur.1310
© 2013 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

506 Autism Research 6: 506–521, 2013 INSAR


[Nydén, Hjemlquist, & Gillberg, 2000]. Likewise, Lemon, comprises both, affective components, thus the ability to
Gragaro, Enticott, and Rinehart [2011] found stronger affectively respond to another person, and cognitive ele-
impairments in girls on the level of executive functions ments, hence to cognitively take the perspective of one’s
during a behavioral stop task, in line with Kopp, Beckung, counterpart [Lamm, Batson, & Decety, 2007]. Addition-
and Gillberg [2010], who also reported a lower level of ally, several studies assume a close relation to social judg-
global functioning in girls with ASD compared with autis- ments involving the understanding of other’s minds and
tic boys. the ability to transfer one’s own inner-life [Farley, Lopez,
Nonetheless, there seems to be broad consensus that & Saunders, 2010; Zahavi, 2010].
girls with ASD commonly exhibit a better level of social On a neural level, empathic processing has been found
adjustment [Attwood, 2008; Bartley, 2006; Kopp, Kelly, & to be in close association with the mirror neuron
Gillberg, 2010]. Putatively, those social adaption skills system (MNS) including the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG)
might make girls/women less likely to be correctly iden- [Hooker, Verosky, Germine, Knight, & D’Esposito, 2010;
tified as autistic according to the current diagnostic cri- Shamay-Tsoory, 2011]. It has also been related to the
teria, and hence leaves them “invisible” to prevalence theory of mind (ToM) network [Völlm et al., 2006], includ-
studies despite an equivalently high-level of autistic traits ing the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, amygdala,
as compared with boys/men [Dworzynski, Ronald, and hippocampus [Phillips, Drevets, Rauch, & Lane,
Bolton, & Happé, 2012]. Thus, Kopp and Gillberg [2007] 2003]. Additionally, empathy and deep social emotions
even proposed a female autistic phenotype sui generis. were linked to more basal areas of emotion processing, the
So far, there are only few studies exploring gender- periaqueductal gray (PAG) in particular [Bartels & Zeki,
specific morphological brain characteristics in patients 2004].
with ASD, most of them either focusing on one sex While not necessarily reflected in the effective behav-
[i.e. Bloss & Courchesne, 2007; Calderoni et al., 2012; ioral results, in empathy-associated tasks women impute
Craig et al., 2007] or contrast patients solely with their themselves a higher performance level than men
respective gender-matched non-autistic control group [Buddeberg-Fischer, Klaghofer, Abel, & Buddenberg,
[Schumann et al., 2010; Schumann, Carter Barnes, Lord, & 2003], accompanied by increased brain activation in
Courchesne, 2009]. They, however do not provide analy- emotion-related areas such as the amygdala [Koch et al.,
ses of gender effects within the autistic group. Nonethe- 2007; Kohn, Kellermann, Gur, Schneider, & Habel, 2011;
less, following their results brain morphology of female Schulte-Rüther, Markowitsch, Shah, Fink, & Piefke, 2008],
patients relative to non-autistic women seems to be char- the IFG [Schulte-Rüther, Markowitsch, Fink, & Piefke,
acterized by increases in brain volume particularly in the 2007; Schulte-Rüther et al., 2008], and a stronger interac-
right temporoparietal junction and the bilateral superior tion between insula and posterior cingulate gyrus
frontal gyri [Calderoni et al., 2012] as well as in the amyg- [Harenski, Antonenko, Shane, & Kiehl, 2008]. Addition-
dala [Schumann et al., 2009], altered gray and white ally, women in contrary to men were found to display
matter density especially in limbic regions and frontotem- significantly larger gray matter volume in core areas of the
poral cortices [Craig et al., 2007], or frontal and temporal MNS, i.e. the inferior parietal lobule [Cheng et al., 2008,
cortical gray matter enlargement [Bloss & Courchesne, 2009]. A recent study [Whittle, Yücel, Yap, & Allen, 2011]
2007; Schumann et al., 2010]. proposed gender-specific neural networks involved in
However, Sparks et al. [2002] and Nordahl et al. [2011] emotion processing and linked this finding to different
could not confirm overall brain enlargement in girls with behavioral patterns in daily life.
autism but rather found increased brain size as an indi- Though evidence is inconclusive regarding the extent
cator for the level of regression in autistic boys [Nordahl and specific characteristics of empathy impairments in
et al., 2011]. Beacher et al. [2011] in line with Tepest et al. patients with autism [Bird et al., 2010; Castelli, 2005;
[2010] even suggested a reduced gender dimorphism in Narzisi, Muratori, Calderoni, Fabbro, & Urgesi, in press],
patients with ASD compared with non-autistic control occasionally suggesting their widely assumed deficits to be
subjects. a “function” of (in-)adequate contextual cues [Narzisi
Convergent evidence of behavioral studies and clinical et al., in press], there seems to be substantial convergence
impression led to the question whether—irrespective of that high-functioning patients with ASD exhibit at least
their putative impairments in executive and/or other cog- subtle difficulties in empathy. Hence, empathy deficits and
nitive functions [Nydén et al., 2000]—females with ASD structural and/or functional alterations of emotion- and
might have a higher “socio-emotional intelligence” than empathy-related brain areas encompassing the ACC
men resulting in a more preserved higher level of social [Dichter, Felder, & Bodfish, 2009], the IFG [Schulte-Rüther
functioning [Barkley, 1998]. et al., 2010], and the fusiform gyrus [Schultz, 2005] are
Empathy constitutes a core feature of social interac- commonly reported for ASD patients.
tions. Despite the heterogeneity in definitions of Nonetheless, so far no study has explicitly addressed
empathy, it seems to be broad consensus that empathy gender differences in empathy in patients with ASD. We

INSAR Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism 507


aimed to fill this gap by analyzing the impact of gender because of an accidental finding of neuroanatomical
on behavioral and/or cerebral (dys-)functions in ASD abnormality (n = 1). Thus, the final sample included 28
patients during an ecologically valid empathy task based autistic patients and 28 gender-, age-, and education-
on emotional or neutral video presentations. matched healthy controls (Con; 15 males).
We hypothesized that: (a) females with high- Four ASD patients and two healthy subjects were left-
functioning autism would show higher levels of handed. All of the other participants were right-handed
empathy compared to autistic males, as they tend to according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory
show a better socio-emotional functioning level [Baird, [Oldfield, 1971]. Six patients were medicated with anti-
2000; Nydén et al., 2000]. Irrespective of group, we depressants (tricyclics: n = 1, selective serotonin reuptake
expected females to show increased task-relevant neural inhibitors (SSRI): n = 5).
activation patterns in empathy-related areas, such as the Subjects also performed a task on moral reasoning
amygdala, insula, and areas of the MNS, when compar- described elsewhere [Schneider et al., in press].
ing emotional to neutral task conditions. Furthermore,
across gender we expected ASD patients to show less Neuropsychological and Psychopathological Testing
empathic responses and less intense ratings of the
emotion displayed by their counterpart in the video as Participants completed the Toronto Alexithymia Scale
compared with healthy participants. (b) As men are (TAS20) [Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994] and the Autism
assigned a more cognitive style of processing empathy Quotient (AQ) [Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner,
and emotional tasks reflected by activation of cognition- Martin, & Clubley, 2001]. Additional psychopathological
related areas [Kohn et al., 2011] such as the dorsal ACC testing in patients included the Autism Diagnostic Obser-
[Phillips et al., 2003], while women would rather rely on vation Schedule—Generic (ADOS-G), Module 4 [Lord,
limbic regions [Kohn et al., 2011], we expected patients’ Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 1999] rated by at least two
behavioral alterations to be accompanied by reduced trained clinicians, the Hamilton Depression Scale
activation of more cognition-related brain areas in male [Hamilton, 1960], and the short version of the Inventory
patients, and of more empathy- and emotion-related for Interpersonal Problems (self rating) [Horowitz, Strauß,
regions in female patients, each relative to their gender- & Kordy, 2000]. Fourteen patients (eight males) fulfilled
matched healthy control group. the ADOS criteria for ASD (cutoff >7), 14 did not (seven
males). Diagnosis of the latter was confirmed by addi-
tional information of relatives and therapists in accor-
dance with the diagnostic criteria of the DSM-IV.
Methods
Participants Furthermore, a neuropsychological test battery was
applied in order to control for the sources of
Thirty patients (17 males) with high-functioning ASD interindividual variances unrelated to the main task: a
were recruited at the in- and outpatient facilities of the German vocabulary test for crystallized intelligence esti-
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychoso- mation [“Wortschatztest”—WST; Schmidt & Metzler,
matics, RWTH Aachen University, as well as at local self- 1992], the German version of the digit span of the
help groups and therapy centers. Inpatients (n = 9) were Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults (WIE) [Von Aster,
diagnosed at a special facility for patients with ASD at the Neubauer, & Horn, 2006], a test for verbal fluency
University Hospital Aachen. Patients from self-help (Regensburger Wortflüssigkeitstest, RWT) Aschenbrenner
groups and therapy centers (n = 21) were included, if they et al. 2001], the Trail Making Test [TMT-A/-B, Reitan,
had received a pre-diagnosis of ASD by experienced 1958], and the NEO-Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI)
psychiatrists. [Borkenau & Ostendorf, 1993]. Patients’ ability to recog-
All participants met the following criteria: age between nize static emotional faces was tested using the Penn
18 and 55 years, no neurological disorders or other Emotion Recognition Test (PERT40) [Kohler et al., 2004].
medical conditions affecting cerebral metabolism, and no Additionally, a test for the tendency to exhibit social
MRI contraindications (metal implants, pregnancy, etc.). desirable behavior was applied to all participants [Stocké,
All participants were native German speakers, and 2009].
screened for mental disorders by means of the Structured Detailed information on demographic and neuropsy-
Clinical Interview for DSM-IV [Wittchen, Wunderlich, chological data is shown in Table 1 (for additional infor-
Gruschitz, & Zaudig, 1997]. Patients had no current psy- mation on each group subdivided by gender see Tables S1
chiatric comorbidities, nor a history of psychosis, person- and S2 of the supplementary material).
ality or eating disorders. After complete description of the study protocol,
Four participants had to be excluded from data analysis written informed consent of each participant was
due to movement artifacts (n = 2), a failure to verify the obtained. All participants received monetary compensa-
pre-diagnosis of an Asperger’s syndrome (n = 1), or tion after study completion.

508 Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism INSAR


INSAR
Table 1. Demographic and Neuropsychological Data of Healthy Control Subjects (Con) and Patients With Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

Males Females

Con ASD Con ASD


n Mean ± SD Mean ± SD t df P n Mean ± SD Mean ± SD t df P

Age (in years) 15/15 34.27 ± 9.73 32.73 ± 9.97 −0.67 28 0.670 13/13 27.85 ± 7.02 29.85 ± 8.02 −0.68 24 0.505
Education (in years) 15/15 12.93 ± 0.26 12.73 ± 0.80 0.92 28 0.364 13/13 12.85 ± 0.38 12.62 ± 0.87 0.88 24 0.388
WST 15/15 115.07 ± 11.35 109.87 ± 7.88 1.46 28 0.156 13/12 112.85 ± 7.22 108.08 ± 10.75 1.31 23 0.203
Digit span forward 12/15 8.93 ± 1.34 8.83 ± 2.59 0.13 25 0.898 12/7 9.75 ± 1.49 9.43 ± 0.79 0.53 17 0.605
Digit span backward 12/15 8.40 ± 2.26 8.42 ± 2.81 0.02 25 0.987 12/7 11.83 ± 10.62 8.14 ± 1.68 0.90 17 0.380
RWT lexical 12/15 15.60 ± 4.50 15.25 ± 7.35 0.15 25 0.880 11/7 18.09 ± 6.24 18.43 ± 4.76 0.12 16 0.904
RWT lexical flexibility 12/15 15.33 ± 4.30 16.00 ± 3.95 0.41 25 0.682 11/7 18.36 ± 5.28 18.29 ± 4.46 −0.03 16 0.975
RWT semantic 12/14 18.36 ± 4.70 16.42 ± 4.64 1.06 24 0.302 11/7 16.82 ± 4.56 21.71 ± 5.19 −2.12 16 0.051
RWT semantic flexibility 12/14 16.43 ± 2.41 16.33 ± 4.77 −0.07 24 0.948 11/5 18.36 ± 3.11 20.33 ± 2.16 −1.37 15 0.190
TMT-A (in seconds) 12/14 19.44 ± 6.14 27.99 ± 11.27 −2.45 24 0.022* 12/7 18.42 ± 5.81 24.04 ± 3.98 −2.26 17 0.037*
TMT-B (in seconds) 12/14 35.33 ± 7.78 45.84 ± 18.60 −1.93 24 0.065 12/7 31.71 ± 7.93 42.43 ± 8.74 −2.74 17 0.014*
AQ 15/14 11.93 ± 7.11 34.86 ± 9.19 −7.55 27 <0.001** 12/13 7.92 ± 3.28 41.00 ± 4.24 −21.92 23 <0.001**
TAS 20 15/14 41.47 ± 10.78 60.57 ± 11.60 −4.60 27 <0.001** 13/11 37.00 ± 8.62 59.09 ± 12.04 −5.23 22 <0.001**
NEO-FFI (N) 14/14 15.86 ± 6.69 29.43 ± 6.12 −5.56 26 <0.001** 13/12 15.38 ± 6.56 31.58 ± 9.29 −5.07 23 <0.001**
NEO-FFI (E) 14/14 29.43 ± 5.67 17.00 ± 7.67 4.88 26 <0.001** 13/12 32.08 ± 5.66 14.75 ± 6.20 7.31 23 <0.001**
NEO-FFI (O) 14/14 32.14 ± 5.22 26.07 ± 5.66 2.95 26 0.007* 13/12 36.69 ± 6.71 29.00 ± 5.48 2.81 23 0.781
NEO-FFI (V) 14/14 32.07 ± 5.08 25.50 ± 6.75 2.91 26 0.007* 13/12 36.08 ± 3.25 27.42 ± 6.22 4.42 23 <0.001**
NEO-FFI (G) 14/14 31.50 ± 7.27 20.00 ± 7.13 1.29 26 0.210 13/12 30.46 ± 9.27 29.17 ± 7.71 0.38 23 0.709
NSAd 15/15 4.73 ± 2.76 3.67 ± 1.95 1.22 28 0.232 13/13 4.77 ± 2.28 5.15 ± 1.86 0.47 54 0.642

Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism


SD, standard deviation; WST, German vocabulary test for crystalline intelligence estimation (“Wortschatztest”); RWT, verbal fluency (“Regensburger Wortflüssigkeitstest”); TMT-A/-B, Trail Making Test
Version A/B; AQ, Autism Quotient; TAS20, Toronto Alexithymia Scale; NEO-FFI (N)/(E)/(O)/(V)/(G), NEO-Five Factor Inventory Neuroticism/Extraversion/Openness/Agreeableness/Conscientiousness; NSAd,
scale for socially desirable behavior.
t-test for independent samples, *P < 0.05; Bonferroni-corrected **P < 0.003.

509
The local Institutional Review Board of the RWTH (−3 = very negative, . . . 0 = neutral, . . . +3 = very posi-
Aachen University approved the protocol, and the design tive). If the participants’ SELF- and OTHER-ratings both
fulfilled the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki matched the target valence with regard to its nuance, e.g.
(2008). −3 up to +3 (self-matching-target, SMT; and other-matching-
target, OMT), this was defined as empathic behavior (self-
other-matching-target, SOMT).
Empathy Task
Between ratings and the next video clip, a white
In order to investigate empathic abilities, we used 32 fixation cross was shown on a black screen (variable
short video sequences (MDuration = 11.8 sec) depicting inter-stimulus interval, M = 6.29 s, SD = 11.42 s). Stimuli
ten different lay actors (five males, five females) who told were presented in an event-related design in a pseudo-
a self-related short story. randomized order using Presentation® 14.0 software
Video sequences included 16 emotional stories with (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany, CA, USA).
emotional semantic content, emotional facial expression
and prosody (“E” condition) comprising the emotion cat- Data Analysis: Behavioral Data
egories disgust, happiness, fear, and anger (four video
clips each), as well as 16 neutral control videos (“N”) with Behavioral data were analyzed using IBM® SPSS Statis-
neutral semantic content, facial expression, and prosody tics 20 (IBM Deutschland GmbH, Ehningen, Germany).
(for examples see Fig. 1). All video clips and spoken sen- The sample size of 56 participants allowed for a
tences had been thoroughly validated and previously 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures analysis of variance
applied by our group [Regenbogen et al., 2012a, 2012b; (ANOVA) with group (Con, ASD) and gender (male,
Schneider et al., 2012]. After each clip, participants were female) as between-subject factors, and condition
asked to indicate by button press how they felt (“SELF”), (emotional, neutral) as a within-subject factor, per-
and what they thought the other person would formed separately for the SMT-, OMT- and SOMT-rating,
feel (“OTHER”) on a seven-point visual analogue scale respectively.

Figure 1. Examples of video clip stimuli; above: emotional (disgust), below: neutral.

510 Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism INSAR


For the analysis of the intensity ratings, the responses assumptions were accommodated using nonsphericity
on the 7-point rating scales were transformed to absolute correction (assuming unequal variance components and
values from 0 (neutral) to 3 (intensively good/intensively independence for factors group and subject, and depen-
bad). dence for factor condition).
These transformed data were introduced into a 2 × 2 × 2 Within this model, to investigate the processing of
repeated-measures ANOVA, computed separately for the dynamic emotional video clips (E>N) in patients and
“SELF”- and “OTHER”-rating, with group (Con, ASD) and control subjects separately we applied a threshold of
gender (male, female) as between-subjects factors, and P < 0.05 family wise error corrected for multiple compari-
condition (E/N) as within-subject factor. sons with an extent threshold of 20 voxels.
ANOVAs were followed by post hoc t-tests (P < .05). For group comparisons (representing a less robust dif-
ference of a difference), a more liberal Monte-Carlo cor-
Data Acquisition and Analysis: Functional Data rected threshold was chosen. Monte-Carlo simulations
were computed using AlphaSim by Ward [2000] imple-
Images were acquired on a 3 Tesla Siemens® Trio MR mented in AFNI 2011 [Cox, 2012]. Assuming a per-voxel
scanner (Siemens AG, München, Germany) at the probability threshold of P = 0.001, after 1,000 simula-
RWTH Aachen University Hospital. A 4-min tions a cluster size of 19 contiguous resampled voxels
magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient was indicated to correct for multiple comparisons at
echo image (MP-RAGE) T1-weighted sequence was P < 0.05.
applied to obtain structural images (TR = 1,900 msec, Additionally, as Bird et al. [2010] suggested a confound-
TE = 2.52 msec, TI = 900 msec, matrix = 256 × 256, 176 ing influence of alexithymia in studies with ASD patients,
slices, FoV: 250 × 250 mm2, flip angle = 90°, voxel we introduced the score of the TAS20 as a covariate of no
size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3). interest into the group analysis subdivided into men and
Functional data of the whole brain (420 volumes) were women [in order to avoid gender as a potential confound
acquired using an echo-planar imaging sequence sensitive in alexithymia analysis; Levant, Hall, Williams, & Hasan,
to the blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) 2009].
effect (T2*, TR/TE = 2000/30 msec, FoV = 200 × 200 mm2, Anatomical localization was performed using the WFU
matrix = 64 × 64, 36 slices, flip angle = 76°, voxel size: Pick Atlas (Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC)
3.1 × 3.1 × 3.1 mm3, slice thickness/gap = 15%). This implemented in SPM.
resulted in a total scanning time of 19 min.
Functional data were processed and analyzed using
SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology,
Results
London, UK) running on a Matlab 10 (The Mathworks,
Behavioral Data
Inc., Natick, MA, USA) platform. The first three images
were excluded to allow for scanner equilibrium stabiliza- Empathy and emotion recognition in ASD patients
tion effects. Accordingly, 417 images of each participant and control subjects. Regarding OMT ratings, the
were included in the final analyses. 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA yielded a significant
Images were aligned to the first remaining image of the main effect of group (F(1,52) = 10.28, P < 0.01) with less
run. Realignment parameters were included as covariates target-matching answers in the ASD group (76.70% vs.
of no interest. The session mean was regressed on a con- 87.33% in the control group). Another significant main
stant term. Realigned images were normalized to the effect was found for condition (F(1,52) = 57.22, P < 0.01).
standard anatomical Montreal Neurological Institute Analyses also revealed significant interactions for group
coordinate space resulting in a voxel size of 3 × 3 × 3 mm3 × condition (F(1,52) = 6.86, P = 0.01), group × gender
and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm (isotropic) (F(1,52) = 5.03, P = 0.03), and group × gender × condition
full width at half maximum. (F(1,52) = 4.53, P = 0.04) indicating significantly less
On a single-subject level, three regressors (one for each target-matching answers for ASD women during the
condition and one modeling the rating period) were N condition, as compared with their healthy counterparts.
created by convolving the respective boxcar functions All other comparisons remained nonsignificant
with the canonical hemodynamic response function. A (Table 2a)).
1/128 Hz high-pass filter was applied to remove slow For SMT ratings, the ANOVA revealed a significant
scanner drifts. To handle within-subject autocorrelations, main effect of group (F(1,52) = 32.28, P < 0.01) with ASD
an approximate AR{1} model was estimated. patients showing significantly less target-matching
Contrast estimates of each participant were used in a responses than control participants (57.34% vs. 77.95%
general linear model (GLM) modeling random effects in the control group).
[factors: group (Con Males/Con Females/ASD Males/ASD For empathy (SOMT), again, a significant main effect of
Females) × condition × subject]. Violations of sphericity group was found (F(1,52) = 510.89, P < 0.01) with ASD

INSAR Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism 511


Table 2. Post Hoc t-Tests of the Behavioral Data (Responses to the Video Clips)

(a) OMT-ratings: % of target-matching answers (= correct identification of the presented emotion) for each of the four groups separately

MCON (± SD) MASD (± SD) t df P

N Males 71.07 (± 24.78) 65.93 (± 24.09) 0.58 28 0.57


Females 86.31 (± 11.85) 50.15 (± 36.84) 3.37 24 <0.01*
E Males 96.73 (± 5.22) 95.13 (± 6.24) 0.76 28 0.45
Females 96.31 (± 5.22) 94.38 (± 6.92) 0.80 24 0.43

(b) Intensity ratings during the “OTHER”-rating

MCON (± SD) MASD (± SD) t df P

N Males 0.31 (± 0.30) 0.35 (± 0.28) −0.33 28 0.75


Females 0.14 (± 0.15) 0.57 (± 0.53) −2.82 24 0.01*
E Males 1.38 (± 0.23) 1.25 (± 0.21) 1.60 28 0.12
Females 1.27 (± 0.15) 1.22 (± 0.18) 0.80 24 0.05

(c) Intensity ratings during the “SELF”-rating

MCON (± SD) MASD (± SD) t df P

“N” condition 0.16 (± 0.24) 0.98 (± 0.36) −2.39 54 0.02*


“E” condition 0.98 (± 0.36) 0.76 (± 0.42) 2.09 54 0.04*

t-test for independent samples, *P < 0.05.

patients giving less empathic responses compared with Group comparisons of patients and healthy par-
control participants (49.45% vs. 70.75% in the control ticipants subdivided by gender. No significant acti-
group) (Fig. 2). vation changes were present for either contrasts in the
male group (healthy males > ASD males as well as ASD
Intensity ratings. For the “OTHER”-rating, a signifi-
males > healthy males).
cant main effect of condition (F(1,52) = 382.61, P < 0.01)
In females, we found decreased activation when com-
was found with the intensity ratings reflecting more
paring patients to non-autistic women in the midbrain
neutral answers during the N condition.
encompassing also limbic regions (left amygdala) with an
Significant interactions for group × condition (F(1,52) =
activation peak in the right PAG compared with healthy
11.36, P < 0.01) and group × gender (F(1,52) = 4.25,
females (Table 3 and Fig. 4). The reverse contrast (ASD
P = 0.04) indicated significantly more (inappropriate)
females > healthy females) did not result in any signifi-
emotional answers during the “N” condition in autistic
cant BOLD changes.
women compared with their healthy control group
Introducing the TAS20 score as covariate in the group
(Table 2b).
comparisons separated for gender yielded no significant
For the “SELF”-rating, there was a also a significant
results.
main effect of condition (F(1,52) = 173.82, P < 0.01).
One-sample analyses of patients and control subjects
A significant interaction was found for group × condi-
are provided in the supplementary material (Table S3).
tion (F(1,52) = 17.97, P < 0.01) yielding significantly
more emotional answers in the ASD group than in the
control participants for the N condition, and significantly Discussion
less emotional answers for the E condition (Table 2c).
The aim of the study was to elucidate gender-specific
empathy-related dysfunctions on a behavioral and a cere-
Functional Imaging Data
bral level in a gender-mixed group of high-functioning
Gender effects in ASD patients and healthy partici- ASD patients relative to a gender-matched control group
pants. While no gender effects emerged in the group of by means of functional neuroimaging.
non-autistic participants, in the group of ASD patients we Behaviorally, data were not significantly affected by
found activation increases for male patients compared gender except the OMT-rating (“How did the actor feel”)
with autistic women in the medial frontal gyrus bilater- of the neutral (“N”) condition, which yielded signifi-
ally (Table 3, Fig. 3). cantly less target-matching answers (in terms of more

512 Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism INSAR


Figure 2. Behavioral data—rating correctness for SMT (at the top), OMT (in the middle), SOMT (at the bottom). SMT, self-matching
target; OMT, other-matching target; SOMT, empathy (self- and other-matching target).

INSAR Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism 513


Table 3. Group Comparisons and Gender Differences in Brain Activation in Response to Emotional vs. Neutral Video Clips

Region BA Hemisphere k t x y z

Con Males > Females N.S.


Females > Males N.S.
ASD Males > Females Medial frontal gyrus 9 L 50 3.92 −21 35 28
R 81 4.03 24 35 31
Females > Males N.S.
Males Con > ASD N.S.
ASD > Con N.S.
Females Con > ASD Periaqueductal gray R 176 4.81 3 −28 −5
ASD > Con N.S.

Note. Whole-brain analysis, flexible-factorial ANOVA, P < 0.05 Monte-Carlo corrected.


ASD, patients with autism spectrum disorder; Con, healthy control subjects; BA, Brodmann’s Area; L, left; R, right; N.S., no significant activation
increases.

inadequate emotional ratings) for the group of autistic 2012]. Thus, its atypical activation in male ASD patients
women compared with non-autistic women. might point to alterations in the processing of those
On a neural level, analyzing gender effects revealed functions.
increased medial frontal gyrus activation in autistic men However, fundamental deficits of ASD patients regard-
compared with autistic women, a pattern of brain activa- ing the level of ToM and (affective) empathy have con-
tion, which was not present in the group of non-autistic sistently been questioned [Frith & Happé, 1994; Narzisi
participants. et al., in press]. Particularly, as autistic patients seem to be
Subsequent comparisons of men and women with ASD able to successfully pass tasks involving social perception,
with their respective control subjects yielded patterns of when they include a sufficient amount of information as
neural hypoactivations for autistic women compared i.e. complete social situations, contextual cues were
with their non-autistic counterparts. As hypothesized, assumed to facilitate social capacities in ASD patients
those hypoactivations in female patients compared with [Narzisi et al., in press]. Nonetheless, there seems to be
their control group were distributed over midbrain substantial convergence that patients with ASD show at
regions extending to parts of the limbic system. least subtle difficulties during higher-level ToM tasks
Potentially due to the relatively small sample size of [Senju, Southgate, White, & Frith, 2009]. Particularly, a
each of the four groups, we did not find significant BOLD recent study [Eigsti, Schuh, Mencl, Schultz, & Paul, 2012]
signal changes when comparing male patients to non- applying a video paradigm linked emotional speech
autistic men. However, there was a remarkable tendency detection, and ToM abilities, and in line with the findings
to hyperactivation of the medial frontal gyrus in the male of others [Chevallier, Noveck, Happé, & Wilson, 2011]
patient group compared with non-autistic men, as demonstrated impaired voice tone recognition in a group
depicted by the parameter estimates (Fig. 3). of high-functioning autistic adult patients, when chal-
lenged with high cognitive load. Simultaneously
recorded brain activation yielded hyperactivation of
Gender Effects Within the Groups of ASD Patients and
Non-Autistic Subjects right-sided medial-prefrontal regions, which was inter-
preted in the light of an increased effort of ASD patients
In line with the literature [Farley et al., 2010; Hervé, to correctly identify emotional prosody [Eigsti et al.,
Razafimandimby, Vigneau, Mazoyer, & Tzourio-Mazoyer, 2012]. Moreover, the authors suggested those
2012; Kennedy, Redcay, & Courchesne, 2006; Masten, hyperactivations as compensatory mechanism in the
Morelli, & Eisenberger, 2011] and our hypotheses, our patient group for their putative impairments on the level
data suggest autism-specific gender differences on brain of ToM [Eigsti et al., 2012].
level during processing (social) information. As our video task especially challenged the ability to
In terms of social functioning, the medial frontal gyrus, integrate multimodal social information, which includes
for which we found significantly increased activation in to identify prosody, increased activation of the medial
the group of autistic men relative to autistic women, and frontal gyrus in our autistic men relative to our autistic
in terms of a tendency also when comparing autistic and women might hence be interpreted in the light of male-
non-autistic male subjects (Fig. 3, parameter estimates), specific alterations in the processing of ToM-related tasks.
was linked to aspects of ToM. It particularly seems to be Particularly, men with an ASD might exhibit specific
involved in affective speech comprehension [Hervé et al., weaknesses in identifying and integrating (multimodal)

514 Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism INSAR


Figure 3. Emotional > neutral and parameter estimates—autistic men > autistic women including parameter estimates (axial: medial
frontal gyrus bilaterally); flexible-factorial ANOVA; P < 0.05 Monte-Carlo corrected.

INSAR Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism 515


Figure 4. Emotional > neutral and parameter estimates—healthy women > autistic women including parameter estimates (sagittal:
periaqueductal gray; coronal: amygdala); flexible-factorial ANOVA; P < 0.05 Monte-Carlo corrected.

516 Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism INSAR


information [Lai et al., 2012], emotional speech in par- pared with all of the other groups reflected on a behav-
ticular [Eigsti et al., 2012], as its underlying factor. ioral and/or neuropsychological level, i.e. by lower
Given the above assumption of compensation strate- emotion recognition rates in autistic women.
gies during ToM tasks [Eigsti et al., 2012], increased acti- Second, in view of recent studies [Bejerot et al., 2012;
vation of the medial frontal gyrus might hypothetically De Vries, Noens, Cohen-Kettenis, van Berckelaer-Onnes,
be the neural reflection of the increased effort of autistic & Doreleijers, 2010] amygdala activation in autistic males
men in solving such tasks [Eigsti et al., 2012], while at the might—rather hypothetically—also be interpreted in
same time the respective features of ToM might widely be terms of a neural reflection of a suggested “masculiniza-
preserved in autistic females [Lombardo et al., 2012, tion” of autistic females, and a concurrent “feminization”
Poster Presentation at IMFAR). Clinically, such predomi- of male ASD patients [Beacher et al., 2011; Bejerot et al.,
nantly unaffected ToM abilities might allow autistic 2012]. According to those studies, autistic females tend to
females to better reflect their (social) behavior, and to display various male-like features such as face shape and
camouflage their autistic impairments by copying social voice quality, while at the same time, autistic men seem
behavior [Attwood, 2008; Bartley, 2006; Kopp & Gillberg, to exhibit rather female characteristics [Bejerot et al.,
2007]. 2012]. Consequently, ASD is occasionally proposed a
However, at the same time for the ASD group parameter gender defiant disorder [Bejerot et al., 2012], which
estimates of the E>N contrast for amygdala activation might also be in line with the finding of an increased rate
surprisingly depict a remarkable tendency to decreased of gender identity disorder among autistic patients [De
amygdala activation in the female ASD group relative to Vries et al., 2010].
male patients as well as relative to both groups of non-
autistic participants (Fig. 4, parameter estimates). This Group Comparisons—Subdivided into Gender
finding is rather unexpected, as women are commonly
supposed to exhibit increased activation of limbic regions While we did not find significant differences within the
during empathy and emotional tasks [Baron-Cohen et al., male group, autistic women relative to non-autistic
1999; Buddeberg-Fischer et al., 2003; Harenski et al., women yielded a pattern of hypoactivations of areas of
2008; Koch et al., 2007; Kohn et al., 2011; Whittle the midbrain, namely the PAG, expanding to limbic
et al., 2011], and as clinically, autistic females are widely regions such as the left amygdala.
accredited with a better social conduct compared with Structures of the midbrain, the PAG in particular, have
male patients [Attwood, 2008; Bartley, 2006; Kopp, Kelly been related to deep social emotions such as compassion-
& Gillberg, 2010; Kopp & Gillberg, 2007]. With it, the ate attitude toward others’ suffering [Kim et al., 2009] and
finding seems to contradict the clinical descriptions of maternal love [Bartels & Zeki, 2004). Additionally, these
social capabilities of autistic men and women [Lai et al., structures comprise oxytocin and dopamine receptors,
2011; Nydén et al., 2000]. Moreover, it challenges the and support the “brain-internal” reward system [Fields,
idea of autism as the extreme variant of the male brain Hjemlstad, Margolis, & Nicola, 2007; Lee, Gallagher, &
[Baron-Cohen, 2002]. Holland, 2010]. In case of non-autistic subjects, the reward
So far, there is no fundamental explanation to that system, its midbrain structures in particular, has also been
[Bejerot et al., 2012]. However, we propose two interpre- proposed to be crucially engaged in the evaluation of
tations: incentive salience of social stimuli like (smiling) faces [Lin,
Taking our overall data into account, brain activation Adolphs, & Rangel, 2012], as depicted by our videos.
of male patients with an ASD relative to all other three Remarkably, for autistic patients atypical behavioral and
groups is characterized by pronounced hyperactivation of neural responses to rewarding stimuli have been suggested
“cognitive” areas as the medial frontal gyrus, while autis- [Kohls et al., 2011]. Reduced neural responses to social
tic women reveal specific alterations in more affective cues in brain areas related to reward processing might
parts of the brain, namely midbrain structures as the PAG, hence reflect the idea of the so-called “social motivation
extending to the amygdala. theory of autism” [Chevallier, Kohls, Troinai, Brodkin, &
Thus, first and in line with our hypotheses these Schultz, 2012], which assigns autistic symptoms to a lack
gender-specific alterations of brain activation in men and of motivation in building social relationships [Chevallier
women with an ASD might mirror common gender char- et al., 2012; Kohls et al., 2011; Schultz, 2005]. Significantly
acteristics [Phillips et al., 2003] in the processing of social decreased activation in the PAG in ASD females compared
stimuli that we used for our task. As its consequence, with healthy women might hence point to reduced social
female ASD patients might exhibit increased impair- interests and/or less emotional contagion in autistic
ments in the respective “affective” parts of the brain, females.
while autism in men might affect more “cognitive” Additionally, there was a tendency to hypoactivation of
regions. Nonetheless, we did not find the particularly the left amygdala in autistic women compared with their
decreased amygdala activation in autistic females com- non-autistic counterparts (Fig. 4, parameter estimates). As

INSAR Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism 517


the left amygdala was proposed to be involved in the References
evaluation of complex social input [Bzdok et al., 2012],
its hypoactivation in autistic females might indicate that Aschenbrenner, S., Tucha, O., & Lange, K.W. (2001).
Regensburger Wortflüssigkeitstest (RWT; “Verbal fluency
females with an ASD in contrary to healthy women
test”). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
exhibit specific impairments in emotional reciprocity
Attwood, T. (2008). The complete guide to Asperger’s syndrome.
during complex social situations [Nydén et al., 2000], as
London/Philadelphia, PA: Jessica Kingsley.
depicted by our video clips. Bagby, R.M., Parker, J.D.A., & Taylor, G.J. (1994). The twenty-
Behaviorally, autistic women’s reduced activation in item Toronto Alexithymia Scale-I. Item selection and cross-
empathy-related brain areas relative to non-autistic validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic
females and also to autistic males in combination with Research, 38, 23–32.
putatively spared ToM abilities in autistic women (rela- Baird, G. (2000). A screening instrument for autism at 18
tive to male patients) might result in an “over- months of age: A 6-year follow-up study. Journal of the
compensatory” emotional overrating of neutral stimuli American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39,
(“N” condition, Table 2c and Fig. 2). 694–702.
Barkley, R. (1998). Hyperactive girls and boys: Stimulant drug
effects on mother-child interactions. Journal of Child Psy-
Limitations and Conclusions chology and Psychiatry, 30, 379–390.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of
Our study has some limitations. The video stimuli we autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.
used have the advantage of a realistic combination of Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H.A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E.T.,
three different input channels, the content, the facial Brammer, M.J., et al. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal
expressions, and the tone of voice. However, we could and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neu-
not analyze the different effects of every modality sepa- roscience, 11, 1891–1898.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., &
rately due to a limited amount of stimuli in order to keep
Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evi-
the scanning time acceptable for patients. We also col-
dence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism,
lapsed our analyses across all four emotion categories and
Males and Females, Scientists and Mathematicians. Journal of
were thus not able to focus on emotion-specific aspects. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.
Additionally, we used basic emotions and simple inten- Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal
sity rating scales, which might neither disclose the subtle and romantic love. Neuroimage, 24, 1155–1166.
difficulties of adult high-functioning ASD patients in Bartley, J.J. (2006). An update on autism: Science, gender, and
emotion recognition and empathy completely [Castelli, the law. Gender Medicine, 3, 73–78.
2005], nor potential subtle differences between males and Beacher, F.D., Minati, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M.V., Lai,
females in either groups. M.-C., et al. (2011). Autism attenuates sex differences in brain
However, autistic men and women seem to exhibit structure: A combined voxel-based morphometry and
diffusion tensor imaging study. American Journal of
differential patterns of cerebral dysfunctions. Particu-
Neuroradiology, 33, 83–89.
larly, our study supports the idea of autism as “gender
Bejerot, S., Eriksson, J.M., Bonde, S., Carlström, K., Humble,
defiant disorder” [Bejerot et al., 2012], indicating
M.B., & Eriksson, E. (2012). The extreme male brain revisited:
specific cognitive deficits for autistic men, and impair- Gender coherence in adults with autism spectrum disorder.
ments on the level of emotional empathy in autistic British Journal of Psychiatry, 201, 116–123.
women. Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T.
These findings implicate the necessity for further (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by
research in order to meet the specific needs of autistic levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133, 1515–
men and women in diagnostic and therapeutic settings. 1525.
Bloss, C.S., & Courchesne, E. (2007). MRI neuroanatomy in
young girls with autism: A preliminary study. Journal of the
Acknowledgments
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46,
This study was supported by the German Research Foun- 515–523.
dation (DFG, International Research Training Group Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1993). NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-
Inventar (NEO-FFI) nach Costa und mccrae (“NEO-Five
1328: “Brain-behavior relationship of emotion and social
Factor Inventory”). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
cognition in schizophrenia and autism,” KFO 112 and HA
Buddeberg-Fischer, B., Klaghofer, R., Abel, T., & Buddenberg, C.
3202/7-1), as well as the Interdisciplinary Center for
(2003). The influence of gender and personality traits on the
Clinical Research of the Medical Faculty of the RWTH career planning of Swiss medical students. Swiss Medical
Aachen University (IZKF, N2-6). Weekly, 133, 535–540.
We would also like to thank all our subjects and the Bzdok, D., Langner, R., Hoffstaedter, F., Turetsky, B., Zilles, K., &
participating self-help groups and therapy centers for Eickhoff, S.B. (2012). The modular neuroarchitecture of soical
supporting this study. judgments on faces. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 951–961.

518 Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism INSAR


Calderoni, S., Retico, A., Biagi, L., Tancredi, R., Muratori, F., & Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of
Tosetti, M. (2012). Female children with autism spectrum Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 56–62.
disorder: An insight from mass-univariate and pattern classi- Harenski, C.L., Antonenko, O., Shane, M.S., & Kiehl, K.A. (2008).
fication analyses. Neuroimage, 59, 1013–1022. Gender differences in neural mechanism underlying moral
Castelli, F. (2005). Understanding emotions from standardized sensitivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3,
facial expressions in autism and normal development. 313–321.
Autism, 9, 428–449. Hervé, P.Y., Razafimandimby, A., Vigneau, M., Mazoyer, B., &
Cheng, Y., Chou, K.H., Decety, J., Chen, I.Y., Hung, D., et al. Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2012). Disentangling the brain network
(2009). Sex differences in the neuroanatomy of human supporting affective speech comprehension. Neuroimage, 61,
mirror-neuron system: A voxel-based morphometric investi- 1255–1267.
gation. Neuroscience, 158, 713–720. Hooker, C.I., Verosky, S.C., Germine, L.T., Knight, R.T., &
Cheng, Y., Lee, P.-L., Yang, C.-Y., Lin, C.-P., Hung, D., & Decety, D’Esposito, M. (2010). Neural activity during social signal
J. (2008). Gender differences in the mu rhythm oft he human perception correlates with self-reported empathy. Brain
mirror-neuron system. PLoS ONE, 3, e2113. Research, 1308, 100–113.
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troinai, V., Brodkin, E.S., & Schultz, R.T. Horowitz, L.M., Strauß, B., & Kordy, H. (2000). Inventar zur
(2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Erfassung interpersonaler Probleme—Deutsche Version
Cognitive Sciences, 16, 231–239. (IIP-D) (“Inventory of intrapersonal problems—German
Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F., & Wilson, D. (2011). version”). Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH.
What’s in a voice? Prosody as a test case for the Theory Kennedy, D.P., Redcay, E., & Courchesne, E. (2006). Failing to
of Mind account of autism. Neuropsychologia, 49, 507– deactivate: Resting functional abnormalities in autism. Pro-
517. ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
Cox, R.W. (2012). AFNI: What a long strange trip it’s been. States of America, 103, 8275–8280.
Neuroimage, 62, 743–747. Kim, J.W., Kim, S.E., Kim, J.J., Jeong, B., Park, C.H., et al. (2009).
Craig, M.C., Zaman, S.H., Daly, E.M., Cutter, W.J., Robertson, Compassionate attitude towards others’ suffering activates
D.M., et al. (2007). Women with autistic-spectrum disorder: the mesolimbic neural system. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2073–
Magnetic resonance imaging study of brain anatomy. British 2081.
Journal of Psychiatry, 191, 224–228. Koch, K., Pauly, K., Kellermann, T., Seiferth, N.Y., Reske, M., et al.
De Vries, A.L., Noens, I.L., Cohen-Kettenis, P.T., van (2007). Gender differences in the cognitive control of
Berckelaer-Onnes, I.A., & Doreleijers, T.A. (2010). Autism emotion: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2744–
spectrum disorders in gender dysphoric children and adoles- 2754.
cents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, Kohler, C.G., Turner, T.H., Stolar, N.M., Bilker, W.B., Brensinger,
930–936. C.M., et al. (2004). Differences in facial expressions of four
Dichter, G.S., Felder, J.N., & Bodfish, J.W. (2009). Autism is universal emotions. Psychiatry Research, 128, 235–244.
characterized by dorsal anterior cingulate hyperactivation Kohls, G., Peltzer, J., Schulte-Rüther, M., Kamp-Becker, I.,
during social target detection. Social Cognitive and Affective Remschmidt, H., et al. (2011). Atypical brain response to
Neuroscience, 4, 215–226. reward cues in autism as revealed by event-related potentials.
Dworzynski, K., Ronald, A., Bolton, P., & Happé, F. (2012). How Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 41, 1523–1533.
different are girls and boys above and below the diagnostic Kohn, N., Kellermann, T., Gur, R.C., Schneider, F., & Habel, U.
threshold for autism spectrum disorders? Journal of the (2011). Gender differences in the neural correlates of humor
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, processing: Implications for different processing modes.
788–797. Neuropsychologia, 49, 888–897.
Ehlers, S., & Gillberg, C. (1993). The epidemiology of Asperger Kopp, S., Beckung, E., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Developmental
syndrome. A total population study. Journal of Child Psy- coordination disorder and other motor control problems in
chology and Psychiatry, 34, 1327–1350. girls with autism spectrum disorder and/or attention-deficit/
Eigsti, I.M., Schuh, J., Mencl, E., Schultz, R.T., & Paul, R. (2012). hyperactivity disorder. Research in Developmental Disabili-
The neural underpinnings of prosody in autism. Child Neu- ties, 31, 350–361.
ropsychology, 18, 600–617. Kopp, S., & Gillberg, C. (2007). Girls with social deficits and
Farley, A.J., Lopez, B., & Saunders, G. (2010). Self- learning problems: Autism, atypical Asperger syndrome or a
conceptualization in autism: Knowing oneself versus variant of these conditions. European Child and Adolescent
knowing self-through-other. Autism, 14, 519–530. Psychiatry, 1, 89–99.
Fields, H.L., Hjemlstad, G.O., Margolis, E.B., & Nicola, S.M. Kopp, S., Kelly, K.B., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Girls with social
(2007). Ventral tegmental area neurons in learned appetitive and/or attention deficits: A descriptive study of 100 clinic
behavior and positive reinforcement. Annual Review of Neu- attenders. Journal of Attention Disorders, 14, 167–181.
roscience, 30, 289–316. Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M.V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A.N.V.,
Fombonne, E. (2005). Epidemiology of Autistic Disorder and Wheelwright, S.J., et al. (2011). A Behavioral Comparison of
Other Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Journal of Clinical Male and Female Patients with High Functioning Autism
Psychiatry, 66, 3–8. Spectrum Conditions. PLoS One, 6, e20835.
Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M.V., Ruigrok, A.N.V., Chakrabarti, B.,
Cognition, 50, 115–132. Wheelwright, S.J., et al. (2012). Cognition in males and

INSAR Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism 519


females with autism: Similarities and differences. PLoS One, Reitan, R.M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an
7, e47198. indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor
Lamm, C., Batson, C.D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural basis of Skills, 8, 271–276.
human empathy—Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive Schmidt, K.H., & Metzler, P. (1992). Wortschatztest (WST)
appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1–7. (“German vocabulary test for crystalline intelligence estima-
Lee, H.J., Gallagher, M., & Holland, P.C. (2010). The central tion”). Weinheim: Beltz Test GmbH.
amygdala projection to the substantia nigra reflects predic- Schneider, D., Regenbogen, C., Kellermann, T., Finkelmeyer, A.,
tion error information in appetitive conditioning. Learning Kohn, N., et al. (2012). Empathic behavioral and physiologi-
and Memory, 17, 531–538. cal responses to dynamic stimuli in depression. Psychiatry
Lemon, J.M., Gragaro, B., Enticott, P.G., & Rinehart, N.J. (2011). Research, 200, 294–305.
Brief Report: Executive Functioning in Autism Spectrum Schneider, K., Pauly, K.D., Gossen, A., Mevissen, L., Michel, T.M.,
Disorders: A Gender Comparison of Response Inhibition. et al. (in press). Neural correlates of moral reasoning in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 352– autism spectrum disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective
356. Neuroscience, in press.
Levant, R.F., Hall, R.J., Williams, C.M., & Hasan, N.T. (2009). Schulte-Rüther, M., Greimel, E., Markowitsch, H.J.,
Gender Differences in. Alexithymia. Psychology of Men and Kamp-Becker, I., Remschmidt, H., et al. (2010). Dysfunctions
Masculinity, 10, 190–203. in brain networks supporting empathy: An fMRI study in
Lin, A., Adolphs, R., & Rangel, A. (2012). Social and monetary adults with autism spectrum disorders. Social Neuroscience,
reward learning engage overlapping neural substrates. Social 5, 1–21.
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 274–281. Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H.J., Fink, G.R., & Piefke, M.
Lombardo, M.V., Lai, M.C., Chakrabarti, B., Ruigrok, A.N., (2007). Mirror neuron and theory of mind mechanisms
Bullmore, E.T., et al. (2012). Spared brain function during involved in face-to face interactions: A functional magnetic
mentalizing and self-representation in females with autism resonance approach to empathy. Journal of Cognitive Neu-
spectrum conditions. Poster presentation at the IMFAR 2012, roscience, 19, 1354–1372.
May 17th-19th, Toronto, ON, Canada. Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H.J., Shah, N.J., Fink, G.R., &
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P.C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Piefke, M. (2008). Gender Differences in brain networks sup-
diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western porting empathy. Neuroimage, 42, 393–403.
Psychological Services. Schultz, R.T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception
Masten, C.L., Morelli, S.A., & Eisenberger, N. (2011). An fMRI in autism: The role of the amygdala and the fusiform face
investigation of empathy for “social pain” and subsequent area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience,
prosocial behavior. Neuroimage, 55, 381–388. 23, 125–141.
Narzisi, A., Muratori, F., Calderoni, S., Fabbro, F., & Urgesi, C. (in Schumann, C.M., Bloss, C.S., Carter Barnes, C., Wideman, G.M.,
press). Neuropsychological Profile in High Functioning Carper, R.A., et al. (2010). Longitudinal magnetic resonance
Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Develop- imaging study of cortical development through early child-
mental Disorders, in press. hood in autism. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 4419–4427.
Nordahl, C.W., Lange, N., Li, D.D., Barnett, L.A., Lee, A., et al. Schumann, C.M., Carter Barnes, C., Lord, C., & Courchesne, E.
(2011). Brain enlargement is associated with regression in (2009). Amygdala enlargement in toddlers with autism
preschool-age boys with autism spectrum disorders. Proceed- related to severity of social and communication impairments.
ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States Biological Psychiatry, 66, 942–949.
of America, 108, 20195–20200. Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind
Nydén, A., Hjemlquist, E., & Gillberg, C. (2000). Autism spec- Eyes: An absence of Spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger
trum and attention-deficit disorders in girls. Some neuropsy- syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.
chological aspects. Europan Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Shamay-Tsoory, S.G. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. Neu-
9, 180–185. roscientist, 17, 18–24.
Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handed- Sparks, B.F., Friedman, S.D., Shaw, D.W., Aylward, E.H., Echelard,
ness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, D., et al. (2002). Brain structural abnormalities in young
97–113. children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology, 59, 184–
Phillips, M.L., Drevets, W.C., Rauch, S.L., & Lane, R. (2003). 192.
Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of Stocké, V. (2009). Deutsche Kurzskala zur Erfassung des
normal emotion perception. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 504– Bedürfnisses nach sozialer Anerkennung (“Scale for socially
514. desirable behavior”). In A. Glöckner-Rist (Ed.), Zusammen-
Regenbogen, C., Schneider, D.A., Finkelmeyer, A., Kohn, N., stellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen, ZIS
Derntl, B., et al. (2012a). The differential contribution of Version 13.00. Bonn: GESIS.
facial expressions, prosody, and speech content to empathy. Tepest, R., Jacobi, E., Gawronski, A., Krug, B., Möller-Hartmann,
Cognition and Emotion, 26, 995–1014. W., et al. (2010). Corpus callosum size in adults with high-
Regenbogen, C., Schneider, D.A., Gur, R.E., Schneider, F., Habel, functioning autism and the relevance of gender. Psychiatry
U., & Kellermann, T. (2012b). Multimodal human Research, 183, 38–43.
communication—Targeting facial expressions, speech Völlm, B.A., Taylor, A.N., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J.,
content and prosody. Neuroimage, 60, 2346–2356. et al. (2006). Neural correlates of theory of mind and

520 Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism INSAR


empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in Supporting Information
a nonverbal task. Neuroimage, 29, 90–98.
Von Aster, M., Neubauer, A., & Horn, R. (Eds.) (2006). Wechsler
Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (WIE) (“Wechsler intelligence Additional Supporting Information may be found in the
scale for adults”). Frankfurt/Main: Pearson. online version of this article at the publisher’s web-site:
Ward, B.D. (2000). Simultaneous inference for fMRI data. AFNI
3dDeconvolve documentation. WI: Medical College. http:// Figure S1. Watching Emotional vs. Neutral Video
afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/manual/AlphaSim.pdf (29th Clips: activation in healthy subjects (green) and
June 2013). ASD patients (red); flexible-factorial analysis (P < 0.05
Whittle, S., Yücel, M., Yap, M.B., & Allen, N.B. (2011). Sex dif- Family Wise Error corrected, extent threshold = 20
ferences in the neural correlates of emotion: Evidence from voxels).
neuroimaging. Biological Psychology, 87, 319–333. Table S1. Demographical and neuropsychological data
Wing, L., Yeates, S.R., Brierly, L.M., & Gould, J. (1976). The for autistic patients and healthy control subjects sepa-
prevalence of early childhood autism: Comparison of admin-
rated for gender.
istrative and epidemiological studies. Psychological Medi-
Table S2. Additional neuropsychological data for
cine, 6, 89–100.
Wittchen, H.U., Wunderlich, U., Gruschitz, S., & Zaudig, M.
autistic patients separated for gender.
(1997). Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV, Achse Table S3. Brain activations when watching emotional
I (SKID-I) (“Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV, Axis I”). vs. neutral video clips (E>N contrast) in healthy control
Göttingen: Hogrefe. subjects (Con) and ASD patients (ASD)—One-sample
Zahavi, D. (2010). Complexities of self. Autism, 14, 547–551. analyses.

INSAR Schneider et al./Gender-specific endophenotypes in autism 521

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen