Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

Downloaded from pn.bmj.

com on 17 January 2007

The Mini Mental State Examination


Basil Ridha and Martin Rossor

Practical Neurology 2005;5;298-303

Updated information and services can be found at:


http://pn.bmj.com/cgi/content/abstract/5/5/298

These include:
Rapid responses You can respond to this article at:
http://pn.bmj.com/cgi/eletter-submit/5/5/298

Email alerting Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at
service the top right corner of the article

Notes

To order reprints of this article go to:


http://www.bmjjournals.com/cgi/reprintform
To subscribe to Practical Neurology go to:
http://www.bmjjournals.com/subscriptions/
Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 17 January 2007
298 PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY

HOW TO DO IT

The Mini Mental S


Basil Ridha* and Martin Rossor†
*Clinical Research fellow, †Professor of Clinical Neurology, Dementia
Research Centre, National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, 8
Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG; E-mail: bridha@dementia.ion.ucl.ac.uk
Practical Neurology, 2005, 5, 298–303

INTRODUCTION REQUIREMENTS FOR


Folstein and colleagues published the Mini PERFORMING THE MMSE
Mental State examination (MMSE) for brief • Privacy in a quiet, well-lit room with no dis-
quantitative assessment of cognitive function tractions.
30 years ago (Folstein et al. 1975). Since then • Pen or pencil, watch, and a blank piece of
it has been translated into many languages and paper.
has become the most widely used brief test of • The subject is conscious and alert.
cognition in clinical and research settings, with • The subject has sufficient knowledge of Eng-
adequate validation. The aim of the examina- lish and literacy skills to be able to cooperate
tion is to: with test items (a translated version of the
• screen for cognitive impairment MMSE administered by a trained translator
• assess the severity of any impairment may be used where available).
• monitor change by serial testing • The subject has adequate hearing, vision,
It relies heavily on verbal cognitive function articulation and dominant limb ability to
at the expense of nondominant hemisphere perform the MMSE tasks other than those
skills and is vulnerable to the vagaries of scor- thought to be related to cognitive impair-
ing by different observers. Nevertheless, it has ment. Hearing and visual aids should be worn
stood the test of time and provides a common if needed.
and widely understood tool to measure global • Consistency in the examiner’s way of admin-
cognitive function. istering and scoring the MMSE to minimize
variability.
STRUCTURE
The MMSE is a collection of questions that test HOW TO CONDUCT THE MMSE
various cognitive domains including orienta- The tester needs to establish rapport with the
tion to time and place, repetition, verbal recall, subject in order to put him or her at ease, and
attention and calculation, language and visual obtain cooperation. A simple explanation of
construction. The test takes 5–10 min to ad- the purpose and structure of the test is provided
minister and the total test score ranges from 0 first. The subject is then asked to answer the
(impaired) to 30 (normal). questions to the best of their ability and to guess
© 2005 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 17 January 2007
OCTOBER 2005 299

State Examination
if unsure. If next of kin are present, it is impor- second after each word. Three unrelated words
tant to ask them not to assist the subject, unless are used, most commonly ‘apple, penny, table’.
for help with translation when required. The test Give one point for each correct answer to a
is not timed, but it is sensible to assume a score maximum score of 3, irrespective of recalled
of zero if the subject does not come up with an word order. Phonemic or paraphasic errors
answer after 10–20 s. Whenever possible, record should be penalized. If the subject is not able
the patient’s exact response, and why he or she to repeat all three words on the first trial, repeat
was not able to perform a task. them again up to five times until he or she is able
to say them all back to you. But the performance
MMSE SUBTESTS on only the first repetition is included in the total
The MMSE test items are classified under the MMSE score.
following headings:
Attention and calculation
Orientation to time Ask the subject to take away 7 from 100. Ask him
Ask the subject in turn, ‘what is the (year) (sea- or her to take away another 7 and continue to a
son) (month) (date) (day)?’ One should allow total of 5 subtractions. An answer is considered
for seasonal transitions in the 2-week period correct if it is exactly 7 less than the previous
spanning any seasonal change. In countries answer.
where other seasonal divisions are used, these Give one point for each correct answer to a
can be accepted as alternatives, e.g. monsoon maximum score of 5.
season. If a subject does not attempt the test or does
Give one point for each correct answer to a not get a full score, then he or she is asked to spell
maximum score of 5. WORLD, first forwards correcting any mistakes
and then backwards. One point is given for each
Orientation to place letter spelled in the correct order backwards to
Ask the subject in turn about the state/country, a maximum score of 5. If more or less then 5
city/county, area of city/town, building/street letters are given, then one point is deducted for
name, floor of building/house number or type each extra or missing letter.
of room if in subject’s house. The set of ques- Only use the best score of either the serial
tions will vary depending on whether the test 7 s or spelling WORLD backwards in the total
is conducted in a large city or a small town, and MMSE score.
whether in a hospital or the subject’s home.
Give one point for each correct answer to a Recall
maximum score of 5. Ask the subject to recall the three words you
earlier asked him or her to remember, without
Repetition/registration prompting or giving any clues.
Ask the subject to listen carefully because you Give one point for each correct answer to a
will ask him or her to repeat after you three maximum score of 3, irrespective of recalled
words. Speak loudly and clearly and pause for 1 word order.
© 2005 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 17 January 2007
300 PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY

Naming sentence says. Make sure that there is sufficient lighting and the
Point to a pen or pencil, and a watch, in turn and ask the subject subject is wearing his or her reading glasses if required. Provide
to name them. Glasses or keys are possible substitutes. the command in the subject’s first language if he or she cannot
Give one point for each correct answer to a maximum score read English.
of 2. Only give one point if the patient closes his or her eyes.

Repetition Writing
Ask the subject to listen carefully and repeat what you are about to Ask the subject to write a sentence on a blank piece of paper. Again
say. Say ‘no ifs ands or buts’ making sure all the ‘s’ letters are clearly make sure there is sufficient lighting, and reading glasses are worn
articulated and pausing for a second after each word. if needed. If the subject does not respond then prompt him or her
Score zero unless all the letters are clearly articulated in the cor- to write something about the weather or a pet.
rect order. A maximum score of one point is given. Only give a score of one point if he or she writes a compre-
hensible sentence containing a subject and verb. Ignore minor
Comprehension spelling, grammatical or punctuation errors. Ignore quality of
Ask the subject to listen carefully and do what you are about to handwriting including tremor, letter size and linearity. Examples
ask. Say clearly and loudly ‘ take this paper with your right hand, of acceptable and unacceptable sentences are shown in Fig. 2.
fold it in half and put it on the floor’ (Fig. 1). Do not remind the
subject what to do next if he or she forgets or does not understand Drawing
what is required. Show the subject two clearly drawn intersecting pentagons, with
Score one point for each step performed correctly. If the right each side about 2 cm long. Ask him or her to copy the diagram
hand is disabled, then ask the subject to take the paper with his on a blank piece of paper.
or her left hand. A score of one point is given only if two five-sided figures are
drawn overlapping to form a four-sided embedded figure. Scal-
Reading ing, side length, rotation, and tremor are ignored. Examples of
Show the subject a blank piece of paper with ‘CLOSE YOUR acceptable and unacceptable drawings are shown in Fig. 3.
EYES’ clearly written. Ask the subject to read and then do what the
ALLOWANCE FOR SCORING
It is important to remember that the purpose of the MMSE is to
test cognitive function, not any noncognitive disability. This has
an impact on whether to modify the test items or allow for any
inability to cooperate despite intact cognitive function. Exam-
ples include visual or hearing impairment, aphonia or dysarthria,
and dominant limb disability. However, if the task cannot be per-
formed due to possible cognitive dysfunction then the subject
must be scored accordingly.
One should also consider the exact domain of cognition that
is being tested. An aphasic subject who fails on tests of repetition,
may still be orientated in time and place and be able to recall
the 3 words if allowed to write his or her responses. In this case
it is more valid to document the written rather than the verbal
response.
Overall, it is important to document why a subject cannot per-
form a task, particularly if the cause is thought not to be due to an
underlying cognitive deficit, and to record any minor modifica-
tions to the test items used. Although cut-off scores for dementia
may not be applicable in such subjects, one could still monitor
change with time on repeat testing – provided consistency in test-
ing and scoring is maintained.

FACTORS THAT INFLUENCE MMSE SCORE

Age
MMSE scores tend to decline with increasing age, particularly
over the age of 65 years – even after controlling for education. In
Figure 1 A patient performing the MMSE. addition, the range of scores tends to get wider with age (Crum
© 2005 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 17 January 2007
OCTOBER 2005 301

(a)

(a)

(b)
Figure 3 (a) Acceptable copying of the intersecting pentagons. (b)
Unacceptable copying of the intersecting pentagons.

(b) change of MMSE scores in either cognitively normal or dementia


Figure 2 (a) Acceptable sentences. (b) Unacceptable sentences.
subjects, and so repeat MMSE testing is not confounded (Helkala
et al. 2002).

Gender
There seems to be no significant difference in MMSE scores be-
tween men and women. However, in one study men tended to
et al. 1993). Although decline in performance on the MMSE may make more mistakes on spelling and other language tasks where-
reflect normal ageing, it is worth noting that increasing age is of as women were more likely to err on serial subtractions (Jones
course associated with increasing risk of dementia. & Gallo 2002). Nonetheless, the consensus is that gender is not
a determinant of MMSE performance (Tombaugh & McIntyre
Education 1992).
With increasing educational level, the MMSE scores tend to in-
crease and the range of scores to narrow (Crum et al. 1993). Thus, Socioeconomic status
education introduces a bias resulting in a false positive diagnosis Subjects from lower socioeconomic classes tend to have a lower
of dementia in less educated subjects. Conversely, high education MMSE score (Brayne & Calloway 1990; O’Connor et al. 1989).
level may mask mild cognitive impairment and result in a false This may be in part be because of and so confounded by poor
negative diagnosis. Education does not seem to affect the rate of education. Other possible explanations could be developmental
© 2005 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 17 January 2007
302 PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY

cognitive limitations predisposing to lower socioeconomic at- at 0.8 in nondemented subjects (Hopp et al. 1997). Repeat MMSE
tainment, or vice versa, lower socioeconomic status associated testing over a 5-year period in normal subjects over the age of
with a higher risk of developing dementia. 65 years shows a slight improvement in the first year, probably
reflecting a practice effect, followed by slight decline over the fol-
Culture, language and ethnicity lowing 4 years (Jacqmin-Gadda et al. 1997).
There is some suggestion that MMSE scores are affected by race
and ethnicity. MMSE scores are reported to be lower in older Inter-rater reliability
black and other ethnic minority groups (Parker & Philp 2004; Folstein et al. found a high inter-rater reliability coefficient of 0.8
Espino et al. 2001). However, most of the difference is thought (Folstein et al. 1975). However, this has subsequently been found
to reflect lower educational level, socioeconomic status, type of to be significantly lower (Bowie et al. 1999), mostly due to vari-
neighbourhood, and reduced English language fluency in immi- ability in the scoring criteria and the version of the attention test
grants compared to the indigenous white population. used, i.e., whether serial 7 s alone, spelling WORLD backwards
alone, or both (Davey & Jamieson 2004).
Test location
Subjects tend to perform better at home than in hospital, prob- Validity
ably due to better orientation and reduced anxiety. Therefore, it is The sensitivity and specificity of the MMSE in differentiating de-
important to maintain consistency in the test location on repeat mentia from normality depends on the cut off score, pattern of
testing in order to reduce variability in MMSE performance. cognitive impairment present, disease severity, and subject charac-
teristics such as age, education, cultural influence, language fluency
Test repetition and test location. Using a cut-off score of below 24, the MMSE
Non-demented subjects typically improve their performance if the is 87% sensitive and 82% specific in detecting dementia and de-
test is re-administered within a two-month interval. However, this lirium among inpatients on a general medical ward (Anthony et al.
effect is less prominent in patients with dementia where learning 1982). A higher cut-off score increases sensitivity of detecting mild
is defective and counterbalanced by disease progression with time dementia, particularly in young educated individuals, but reduces
(Helkala et al. 2002; Galasko et al. 1993). Repeat presentation of the the specificity and increases the false positive diagnosis of dementia
MMSE within a short interval (under two months) improves the in older uneducated individuals and in those belonging to ethnic
accuracy of the MMSE as a screening tool for early dementia (Hel- minority groups. The MMSE is more specific and sensitive in de-
kala et al. 2002). In order to reduce the bias of learning, one can use tecting moderate to severe dementia than mild forms of dementia,
different words for the recall task, and different common objects and in detecting cognitive deficits related to language and memory
to name on each testing occasion. However, there are no parallel rather than frontal executive dysfunction.
forms for the other components of the MMSE. Several approaches have been used to improve the validity of
the MMSE. One is to generate population-based normative data
TEST PERFORMANCE to provide different cut-off scores, stratified according to age
and education level (Crum et al. 1993). However, it is doubtful
Internal consistency between the test domains if such adjustments improve the accuracy of the MMSE (Jones
There seems to be a good degree of homogeneity (internal consist- & Gallo 2002). Another approach is to use modified forms of the
ency) between the various test items of the MMSE which suggests MMSE to minimize educational and cultural bias (Xu et al. 2003).
that the test is reliable. However, as the MMSE is meant to meas- Modifications to the MMSE have also been suggested in order to
ure different cognitive domains, some degree of heterogeneity improve the accuracy of differentiating Alzheimer’s disease from
would be desirable. The degree of internal consistency depends normality, including the use of differential weighting of different
on several factors. It tends to be higher in hospital-based studies test items (Galasko et al. 1990), adding extra test items such as
compared with community-based studies, and among subjects word fluency (Galasko et al. 1990), date and place of birth, simi-
with lower education than those with higher education. The use larities and delayed recall of words (Teng & Chui 1987), and tak-
of serial sevens alone as opposed to the combination with spelling ing into account an informant rating score (Tierney et al. 2003).
WORLD backwards yields a higher level of internal consistency Although these modifications tend to produce subtle improve-
(Espino et al. 2004). ments, they prolong the testing time and are not as popular as the
original MMSE.
Test-retest reliability
For a test-retest interval of 2 months or less, the correlation coef- PATTERNS OF COGNITIVE DECLINE IN
ficient between each subject’s baseline and repeat MMSE scores DEMENTIA SYNDROMES
is about 0.9 (Folstein et al. 1975; Tombaugh & McIntyre 1992) in Patients with Alzheimer’s disease tend to perform worst on re-
both normal and demented subjects, suggesting good test-retest call of three words and orientation to time (Galasko et al. 1990;
reliability. The change in score is partly due to improvement in Jefferson et al. 2002), whereas patients with vascular dementia,
performance as a result of practice. With a test-retest interval of Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies tend to per-
about 2 years, the correlation coefficient is less but still acceptable form worst on attention, obeying a three step command, writing a
© 2005 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Downloaded from pn.bmj.com on 17 January 2007
OCTOBER 2005 303

Davey RJ & Jamieson S (2004) The validity of using the mini mental state
SUMMARY examination in NICE dementia guidelines. Journal of Neurology, Neuro-
surgery and Psychiatry, 75, 343–4
• Despite its limitations, the MMSE is the most widely used Espino DV, Lichtenstein MJ, Palmer RF & Hazuda HP (2001) Ethnic differ-
brief test of cognitive function ences in mini-mental state examination (MMSE) scores: where you live
• Clinically, it provides a baseline framework to structure the makes a difference. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 49, 538–48
bedside cognitive assessment Espino DV, Lichtenstein MJ, Palmer RF & Hazuda HP (2004) Evaluation of
• It provides an objective record to monitor disease progres- the mini-mental state examination’s internal consistency in a commu-
nity-based sample of Mexican-American and European-American elders:
sion and response to treatment results from the San Antonio Longitudinal Study of Aging. Journal of the
• It gives an estimate of disease severity that is meaningful for American Geriatrics Society, 52, 822–7
communication among different health care workers Folstein MF, Folstein SE & McHugh PR (1975) ‘Mini-mental state’. A practi-
• Although modified versions of the MMSE do exist, the cal method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
original MMSE is still the most popular form Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–98
Galasko D, Abramson I, Corey-Bloom J & Thal LJ (1993) Repeated exposure
• The utility of the MMSE in clinical practice can be improved
to the Mini-Mental State Examination and the Information-Memory-
by bearing in mind patient characteristics that may affect Concentration Test results in a practice effect in Alzheimer’s disease. Neu-
performance (age, education, culture, ethnicity, hearing or rology, 43, 1559–63
visual impairment and any other relevant physical disabili- Galasko D, Klauber MR, Hofstetter CR, Salmon DP, Lasker B & Thal LJ (1990)
ties), and by using a standardized scoring scheme within The Mini-Mental State Examination in the early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s
disease. Archives of Neurology, 47, 49–52
and between physicians
Helkala EL, Kivipelto M & Hallikainen M et al. (2002) Usefulness of repeated
presentation of Mini-Mental State Examination as a diagnostic procedure
sentence and copying intersecting pentagons, suggesting impair- – a population-based study. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 106, 341–6
ment of working memory and motor, constructional and visuo- Hopp GA, Dixon RA, Grut M & Backman L (1997) Longitudinal and psy-
perceptual skills (Jefferson et al. 2002; Ala et al. 2002). Patients chometric profiles of two cognitive status tests in very old adults. Journal
with fronto-temporal dementia tend to have higher MMSE scores of Clinical Psychology, 53, 673–86
Jacqmin-Gadda H, Fabrigoule C, Commenges D & Dartigues JF (1997) A 5-
in comparison to Alzheimer patients at initial presentation, and year longitudinal study of the Mini-Mental State Examination in normal
slower annual rates of decline (0.9 points vs. 2.0, respectively). aging. American Journal of Epidemiology, 145, 498–506
This reflects the fact that the MMSE is geared more towards de- Jefferson AL, Cosentino SA & Ball SK et al. (2002) Errors produced on the
tecting impairments of orientation, memory and language than mini-mental state examination and test performance in Alzheimer’s
the executive dysfunction typical of fronto-temporal dementia. disease, ischemic vascular dementia, and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of
Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 14, 311–20
Jones RN & Gallo JJ (2002) Education and sex differences in the mini-mental
MMSE AND THE USE OF state examination: effects of differential item functioning. Journal of Ger-
CHOLINESTERASE INHIBITORS ontological B Psychological Science Society of Science, 57, 548–58
Cholinesterase inhibitors such as donepezil, rivastigmine and Lanctot KL, Herrmann N & Yau KKet al. (2003) Efficacy and safety of
galantamine have emerged as modestly useful agents for the cholinesterase inhibitors in Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Cana-
dian Medical Association Journal, 169, 557–64
symptomatic treatment of mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease
O’Connor DW, Pollitt PA, Treasure FP, Brook CP & Reiss BB (1989) The
(Lanctot et al. 2003; Seltzer et al. 2004). Their cost implications, influence of education, social class and sex on Mini-Mental State scores.
and the high disease prevalence in the elderly, have reinforced Psychological Medicine, 19, 771–6
the need for brief and reliable tools for assessing eligibility for Parker C & Philp I (2004) Screening for cognitive impairment among older
treatment, monitoring drug efficacy, and withdrawing treatment people in black and minority ethnic groups. Age Ageing, 33, 447–52
Seltzer B, Zolnouni P & Nunez Met al. (2004) Efficacy of donepezil in early-
– such as the MMSE.
stage Alzheimer disease: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Archives
of Neurology, 61, 1852–6
REFERENCES Teng EL & Chui HC (1987) The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) exami-
Ala TA, Hughes LF, Kyrouac GA, Ghobrial MW & Elble RJ (2002) The Mini- nation. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48, 314–8
Mental State exam may help in the differentiation of dementia with Lewy Tierney MC, Herrmann N, Geslani DM & Szalai JP (2003) Contribution of
bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychia- informant and patient ratings to the accuracy of the mini-mental state
try, 17, 503–9 examination in predicting probable Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the
Anthony JC, LeResche L, Niaz U, von Korff MR & Folstein MF (1982) Limits American Geriatrics Society, 51, 813–8
of the ‘Mini-Mental State’ as a screening test for dementia and delirium Tombaugh TN & McIntyre NJ (1992) The mini-mental state examination:
among hospital patients. Psychological Medicine, 12, 397–408 a comprehensive review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40,
Bowie P, Branton T & Holmes J (1999) Should the Mini Mental State Exami- 922–35
nation be used to monitor dementia treatments? Lancet, 354, 1527–8 Xu G, Meyer JS, Du Huang YF, Chowdhury M & Quach M (2003) Adapting
Brayne C & Calloway P (1990) The association of education and socioeco- mini-mental state examination for dementia screening among illiterate
nomic status with the Mini Mental State Examination and the clinical or minimally educated elderly Chinese. International Journal of Geriatric
diagnosis of dementia in elderly people. Age Ageing, 19, 91–6 Psychiatry, 18, 609–16
Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS & Folstein MF (1993) Population-based
norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational
level. Journal of the American Medical Association, 269, 2386–91

© 2005 Blackwell Publishing Ltd

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen