Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

Overlap Between Borderline and Schizotypal Personality

Disorders
Richard J. Kavoussi and Larry J. Siever

Borderline personality was split into two diagnotic criteria sets; (3) a synergistic association of the two
categories in DSM-III: borderline personality disorder personality disorders; and (4) a manifestation of dimen-
(BPD) and schizotypal personality disorder (SPD). There sional psychopathology. Empirical evidence for each
remains a great deal of diagnostic overlap between of the first three hypotheses is weak and contradic-
these two categories despite modifications in DSM- tory. Recent biologic and treatment studies appear to
III-R. This report discusses four possible hypotheses most strongly support the use of dimensional models
for this overlap: (1) an independent, random associa- of “borderline” and “schizotypal” personality traits.
tion; (2) a&factual overlap due to imperfections in the Copyright 0 1992 by W.B. Saunders Company

S
INCE THE TIME of Bleuler, psychiatrists tween these two disorders remains a concern for
have identified patients with attenuated DSM-III-R and the development of DSM-IV.
symptoms of schizophrenia-poverty of affect, This substantial overlap has prompted ques-
impaired interpersonal relatedness, and magi- tions regarding the validity of the current crite-
cal thinking.‘” Danish adoption studies sug- ria sets for these two personality disorders.” It
gested that these “borderline schizophrenics” has also generated concern since accurate diag-
had personality features that might represent a nosis of these disorders is important in determin-
phenotypic expression of a “schizophrenic ing response to treatment.
genotype.“’ Simultaneously, other investigators There are four plausible hypotheses that
identified a group of patients with unstable might account for the overlap between schizo-
affective symptoms and impulsivity who ap- typal and borderline personality disorders. First,
peared to lie “on the border” of classic affective the overlap may simply be an independent,
disorder.‘.” The identification of these two random association between the two personal-
groups of patients led to the development of ity disorders. Second, the overlap between BPD
diagnostic criteria for two separate personality and SPD may be an artifact of the nature of the
disorders: schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) criteria sets used in DSM-III and DSM-III-R.
and borderline personality disorder (BPD).” Third, there may be an inherent association or
Yet, although defined as separate diagnostic “synergism” between these two disorders. Fi-
categories, these personality disorders fre- nally, these disorders may not exist as discrete
quently overlap. The degree of this overlap has categorical entities, but rather as dimensional
ranged from 7% to 58% in samples of border- manifestations of personality psychopathology.
line and schizotypal patients previously re- Various empirical methods can be used to
ported in the literature (Table 1). The extent of test these hypotheses by studying differences
overlap in these studies is, in part, a function of between three groups: “pure” BPD, “pure”
SPD, and “mixed” BPD-SPD. These methods
the particular sample studied. Studies with
include phenomenological studies, family his-
approximately equal numbers of SPD and BPD
tory and genetic studies, biologic marker stud-
in the sample show more overlap, while studies
ies, treatment response studies, and outcome
with unequal samples have reported low over-
studies. In this report, we will present each
lap. Thus, it is imperative to take base rates into
hypothesis and evaluate the empirical evidence
account when evaluating studies of diagnostic
for each.
overlap.”
Some changes were made in DSM-III-R in an
attempt to reduce the degree of overlap. For From the Department of PsychiatT, Medical College of
example, depersonalization and derealization Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA; and the Department of Psychi-
are no longer examples of unusual sensory atty, Mount Sinai Medical Center, New York, NY.
experiences for SPD and a new criterion-odd Address reprint requests to Richard J. Kavoussi, M.D.,
Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Pennsylvania at
behavior-was added to the criteria for SPD. EPPI, 3200 Henry Ave, Philadelphia, PA 19129.
Still, even with these changes, the degree of Copyright 0 I992 by WB. Saunders Company
overlap is still significant’4-‘h and overlap be- OOlO-44OXl9213301-0006$03.0010

Comprehens/vefsychiatry, Vol. 33, No. 1 (January/February), 1992: pp 7-12 7


8 KAVOUSSI AND SIEVER

Table 1. Overlap of BPD and SPD Artifactual overlap also may result from im-
Study S B SB %SB precise definitions of criteria. This may be
Silk, 1990’6 0 31 8 20.5 especially true for BPD and SPD, since the
Vaglum. 1989 (DSM-III-R)15 6 41 7 13.0 criteria for these disorders had their origins in
Morey, 1988 (DSM-III-R)‘4 27 97 9 6.8 overlapping criteria sets.20For example, Gunder-
Widiger, 1987” 19 24 29 40.3
son’s criteria for BPD stress the importance of
McGlashan, 198724 10 81 18 16.5
George, 198625 4 24 18 39.1 psychotic-like symptoms while DSM-III does
Jacobsberg, 198627 19 6 16 39.0 not incorporate criteria for these symptoms.
F’fohl, 198640 6 23 6 17.1 Those who work with clinical samples argue for
Baron, 1985’* 16 17 20 37.7
the importance of including psychotic like expe-
Gunderson, 198321 8 6 13 48.1
Spitzer, 197912 145 170 436 58.0
riences in the diagnostic criteria for SPD since
this corresponds to current clinical diagnoses
Abbreviations: S, DSM-III schizotypal personality disorder; B,
DSM-III borderline personality disorder; SB, both schizotypal
and is thus useful to practicing clinicians in their
and borderline personality disorder. conceptualization of patients. On the other
hand, those who study genetic and biological
markers of SPD argue that schizotypal patients
RANDOM OVERLAP (as in the original conceptions of Rado and
The overlap between BPD and SPD may Meehl) should be defined less by psychotic
simply be an independent, random association experiences and more by social-interpersonal
between the two personality disorders. Then it dysfunction, impoverished affect, asociality, and
would be expected that the prevalence of over- somatization.”
lap would be equal to the product of the Imprecise criteria for axis II disorders may
prevalence of each personality disorder (% hinder the differentiation of psychotic-like per-
overlap = %SPD x %BPD). In most studies, sonality traits in BPD and SPD. Patients meet-
the overlap between SPD and BPD has been ing criteria for both SPD and BPD may repre-
higher than would be expected by chance associ- sent those borderlines with psychotic-like
ation alone (see Table 1). features as originally proposed by Gunderson.
The high degree of overlap in these samples However, psychotic-like symptoms in the border-
also may reflect the possibility that the overlap line patient may reflect different underlying
group is more ill than those with either SPD or psychopathology than psychotic-like symptoms
BPD alone and thus are more likely to seek in schizotypes, just as psychotic symptoms in
treatment in a clinical setting. Yet, even in bipolar patients do not necessarily reflect the
nonclinical samples, the degree of overlap is still same pathologic process as psychosis in schizo-
higher than would be expected from random phrenics.
overlap.‘” Thus, it is unlikely that this hypothesis There are several ways in which psychotic
alone is correct. symptoms in borderline patients can occur.2’,23
First, in some borderline patients, mood congru-
ARTIFACTUAL OVERLAP ent psychotic symptoms may reflect a coexisting
The overlap between BPD and SPD may be axis I affective disorder and not the underlying
an artifact of the nature of the criteria sets used psychopathology of this personality disorder.
in DSM-III and DSM-III-R. First, because Second, borderline patients may produce dra-
DSM-III uses a prototypic system of classifica- matic and bizarre psychotic symptoms to engage
tion, there is much heterogeneity among mem- caregivers or when faced with disruptions in
bers of each diagnostic category. For example, their chaotic interpersonal relations. Third, al-
there are 93 combinations of criteria that will tered cognitive-perceptual symptoms and para-
lead to a diagnosis of BPD. While the sensitivity noia in the borderline may be closely associated
of a particular diagnostic category is enhanced with intolerable, intense, and rapidly changing
when there is a broad range of combinations of affective states (guilt, anger, sadness, etc.). These
criteria sufficient to make a diagnosis, such a types of psychotic symptoms are different from
strategy reduces specificity, leading to increased the persistent psychotic-like symptoms (ideas of
overlap among diagnostic categories.” reference, paranoia, magical thinking) found in
BORDERLINE AND SCHIZOTYPAL OVERLAP 9

schizotypal patients. DSM-IV will need to ad- were no diagnoses of SPD or BPD in the twins
dress these issues by defining more precisely the of mixed borderline-schizotypal probands. One
nature of psychotic like experiences included in family study found an increased risk for SPD in
the criteria sets. the relatives of pure SPD probands and no
There are other ways in which current criteria increased risk of SPD in the relatives of the
for BPD and SPD may not adequately differen- mixed SPD-BPD sample.‘* Thus, it would ap-
tiate affective-related traits from schizophrenia- pear that pure SPD has a clearer genetic compo-
related traits. For example, both borderline and nent than both BPD and the overlap group and
schizotypal patients can exhibit social isolation that the overlap group is a subset of BPD with
and social anxiety, although these are criteria psychotic-like features.
only for SPD. Yet, these symptoms may reflect One study looking at discharge status, treat-
different underlying psychophysiologic pro- ment variables, psychopathology, functional out-
cesses. The impairment in affective regulation come, and global outcome found that the mixed
seen in BPD may lead to intense, stormy rela- group resembled the pure borderline group
tionships resulting in social isolation. Impaired more than the pure schizotypal group on most
cognitive-perceptual ability in the interpersonal indices of outcome.30 The only measures on
sphere in the schizotypal patient would lead to which the overlap group resembled the pure
chronic discomfort around others and a lack of SPD group were on lack of drug abuse and
interpersonal contact. Future criteria for these problems in heterosexual relationships. An-
personality disorders must explicitly define these other study also found that while the mixed
traits to improve their nosologic utility. group was as impaired as schizophrenics at
If the overlap group in a particular study admission (and significantly lower functioning
consists of borderline patients with hysterical or than pure borderlines), at follow-up these pa-
transient psychoses who are diagnosed as hav- tients were functioning as well as the pure
ing both BPD and SPD, then the overlap group borderlines.“’ While these studies would appear
should resemble the pure BPD group on the to support the artifactual hypothesis, another
validating measures noted above. There is some study found that the mixed diagnosis patients
empirical support for this model. Several stud- were more like the pure schizotypes in having a
ies have found that patients in the mixed SPD- poorer outcome than the pure borderlines.”
BPD group have symptoms more like pure Biologic studies in these groups have not
borderline patients than pure schizotypal pa- supported the artifactual hypothesis. Amphet-
tients.‘4.‘5 The overlap group appears to resem- amine challenge tests performed on a sample of
ble the schizotypal group with respect to psy- borderline patients32 demonstrated that those
chotic-like SPD criteria (suspiciousness, magical patients with pure BPD showed global improve-
thinking), but is more like the borderline group ment in mood, while those patients with both
with respect to the absence of “negative” schizo- SPD and BPD had an increase in psychotic
typal symptoms (social isolation, inadequate symptoms and a decreased sense of well-being.
rapport).” These findings suggest that the diag- Smooth pursuit eye movements were abnormal
nosis of SPD in the overlap group is in part more often in patients with pure SPD, and the
based on the presence of psychotic-like symp- overlap group than in a control group of other
toms and may represent an artifact of the personality disorders.33 These findings argue
criteria sets. Unfortunately, not all studies have against the artifactual hypothesis as the sole or
replicated these findings.“,” major cause of the overlap between these per-
Twin and family studies provide some sup- sonality disorders.
port for the artifactual overlap hypothesis. In
the only twin study conducted to date,2y 33% of SYNERGISTIC MODEL
monozygotic schizotypal probands and 4% of A third explanation for the high degree of
dizygotic probands were concordant for SPD. SPD-BPD overlap is that there is an inherent
On the other hand, none of the monozygotic association or “synergism” between these two
probands and 29% of dizygotic borderline disorders. In this model, etiologic factors for
probands were concordant for BPD. There each disorder are likely to act together to
10 KAVOUSSI AND SIEVER

produce distress and dysfunction as in hyperten- diagnoses to biological markers.42 Abnormali-


sion and adult onset diabetes. These disorders ties in central serotonergic functioning as mea-
are associated with each other much more sured by the prolactin response to the serotoner-
commonly than chance alone would dictate, gic agonist fenfluramine have been associated
sharing common etiologic factors (family his- with disordered impulse control, irritability, and
tory, diet, obesity) and outcome (increased risk aggressivity in patients with personality disor-
of arterial disease). ders, regardless of categorical diagnosis.43 Mea-
One method to investigate synergistic overlap sures of plasma and cerebral spinal fluid homo-
of SPD and BPD is to study the correlation vanillic acid (HVA), which reflect central
between the DSM-III item sets for each disor- dopaminergic functioning, have been found to
der independent of the degree of overlap. In the correlate with “positive” schizotypal symptoms
original study by Spitzer et al.” although there (e.g., magical thinking, ideas of reference), but
was 58% overlap between the two diagnoses, not with “negative” symptoms (e.g., social isola-
there was only a .06 correlation between criteria tion, odd speech, inadequate rapport).44,45
sets. More recent studies have found a some- Treatment studies also support a dimensional
what higher correlation between SPD and BPD model. In a double-blind trial of haloperidol,
criteria, ranging from .19 to .62,34-36suggesting amitriptyline, and placebo, haloperidol was sig-
an inherent correlation between the two disor- nificantly better than both amitriptyline and
ders. placebo in reducing phobic anxiety, hostility,
If the overlap group represents a synergistic paranoia, psychoticism, and obsessive symp-
association between SPD and BPD, the mixed toms.46No specific differences between diagnos-
group would have characteristics and symptoms tic groups were reported. In another double-
not observed in either diagnosis alone. Al- blind study comparing thiothixene with placebo,
though, as noted above, several family studies drug effects were present, but were related
favor the artifactual hypothesis, one study re- more to specific symptoms (such as illusions,
ported higher rates of schizophrenia in the ideas of references, phobic anxiety, obsessions,
relatives of patients with mixed BPD-SPD than and somatization) than to measures of global
in pure SPD or BPD probands. Unfortunately, improvement across diagnoses.47 In a double-
there has been little other empirical evidence to blind, cross-over study comparing the effects of
support this hypothesis. various medications on the symptoms of border-
line personality, the monoamine oxidase inhibi-
DIMENSIONAL MODEL tor tranylcypromine was most effective in im-
Since the inception of the DSM-III personal- proving mood and impulse control, while
ity disorders section, many nosologists have carbamzepine was most effective in the treat-
argued that it is better to think of personality ment of behavioral dyscontro1.48 These results
disorders along dimensions of psychopathology suggest that treatment response in a given
than as categorical entities.” In fact, in attempt- patient depends more on specific dimensions of
ing to explain the overlap of BPD and SPD, symptoms, rather than on the categorical person-
Spitzer and Endicott3’ noted that these disor- ality diagnosis.
ders may be better considered as independent
dimensions of personality that may coexist in CONCLUSIONS
the same individual than as mutually exclusive None of the above hypotheses fully account
diagnoses. Phenomenological studies have sug- for the overlap found between SPD and BPD as
gested that multidimensional scalings of person- defined by DSM-III and DSM-III-R. However,
ality traits have more validity than categorical the empirical evidence tends to support the
diagnoses.3y-4’ dimensional model of overlap. This model is
There is increasing evidence that certain supported by biologic marker studies, treatment
dimensions of psychopathology in personality response studies, and conflicting results of em-
disordered patients (e.g., affective instability, pirical studies in categorical models. The associ-
cognitive-perceptual impairment, impulsivity) ation of specific personality traits with specific
may correspond more precisely than categorical biological markers and specific treatment re-
BORDERLINE AND SCHIZOTYPAL OVERLAP 11

sponse suggests that personality pathology may vary across individuals depending on congenital
be best studied using dimensional models. Vari- factors, exercise, and life-style. When coronary
ous traits such as impulsivity, avoidance, affec- artery disease occurs, manifestations of this
tive lability, avoidance, and perceptual deficits disease will depend as much on preexisting
should be studied independently and correlated myocardial functioning as on the degree of
with genetic vulnerability, phenomenologic man- CAD and can range from no symptoms to
ifestations, biological features, and treatment angina to myocardial infarction. Similarly, cate-
response. gorical psychiatric disorder will be influenced in
This hypothesis of overlap does not necessar- their expression by underlying traits that will
ily exclude the previous hypotheses discussed vary across individuals. Until more empirical
since models of dimensional psychopathology data are available, it would be prudent to use
may coexist with categorical models. It is possi- both categorical and dimensional models to
ble for an individual to have traits that occur improve our diagnostic ability with respect to
along a continuum with a syndromal disease BPD and SPD.
state. For example, myocardial oxygen demands

REFERENCES
1. Bleuler E. A Textbook of Psychiatry (1924). Brill A DSM-III and DSM-III-R schizotypal symptoms in border-
(tram). New York, NY: Macmillan, 1944. line personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry 1990:31:103-
2. Zilboorg G. Ambulatory schizophrenia. Psychiatry 110.
1941;4:149-144. 17. Widiger TA, Frances A, Spitzer RL, Williams JB.
3. Deutsh H. Some forms of emotional disturbance and The DSM-III-R personality disorders, an overview. Am J
their relationship to schizophrenia. Psychoanal Q 1942;ll: Psychiatry 1988;145:786-795.
301-321. 18. Baron M, Gruen R, Asnis L, Lord S. Familial
4. Hoch P, Polatin P. Pseudoneurotic forms of schizophre- transmission of schizotypal and borderline personality disor-
nia. Psychiatr Q 1949;23:248-276. ders. Am J Psychiatry 1985;142:927-934.
5. Rado S. Dynamics and classification of disordered 19. Clarkin JF, Widiger TA, Frances A, Hurt SW, Gil-
behavior. Am J Psychiatry 1853;110:406-416. more M. Prototypic typology and the borderline personality
6. Meehl P: Schizotaxia. schizotypy, and schizophrenia. disorder. J Abnorm Psycho1 1983;92:263-275.
Am Psycho1 1962;17:827-837. 20. Perry JC, Klerman GL. The borderline patient: a
7. Kendler KS, Gruenberg AM, Strauss JS: An indepen- comparative analysis of four sets of diagnostic criteria. Arch
dent analysis of the Copenhagen sample of the Danish Gen Psychiatry 1978;35:141-152.
adoption study of schizophrenia: II. the relationship be- 21. Gunderson JG. Siever LJ, Spaulding E. The search
tween schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia. for a schizotype: crossing the border again. Arch Gen
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981;38:982-984. Psychiatry 1983;40:15-22.
8. Kernberg 0. Borderline Conditions and Pathological 22. Pope HG, Jonas JM, Hudson JI, Cohen BM. An
Narcissism. New York, NY: Aronson, 1975. empirical study of psychosis in borderline personality disor-
9. Gunderson JG. Singer MT. Defining borderline pa- der. Am J Psychiatry 1985;142:1285-1290.
tients. An overview. Am J Psychiatry 1975;132:1-10. 23. Links PS, Steiner M, Mitton J. Characteristics of
IO. Stone MH. The Borderline Syndrome: Constitution, psychosis in borderline personality disorder. Psychopathol-
Coping and Character. New York, NY: Aronson. 1978. ogy 1989:22:188-193.
1I. Akiskal HS. Subaffective disorders: dysthymic, cy- 24. McGlashan TH. Testing DSM-III symptom criteria
clothymic and bipolar II disorders in the “borderline” for schizotypal and borderline personality disorders. Arch
realm. Psychiatr Clin North Am 1981;4:25-46. Gen Psychiatry 1987;44:143-148.
12. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Gibbon M. Crossing the 25. George A, Sololf PH. Schizotypal symptoms in pa-
border into borderline personality and borderline schizo- tients with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry
phrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1979;36:17-24. 1986:143:212-215.
13. Widiger TA, Frances A, Warner L, Bluhm C. Diagnos- 26. Siever LJ, Klar HM, Coccaro EF, Silverman J. Sween
tic criteria for the borderline and schizotypal personality L, Davis KL. Schizotypal and borderline personality over-
disorders. J Abnorm Psycho1 1986:95:43-51. lap. Presented at the American Psychiatric Association
14. Morey LC. Personality disorders in DSM-III and Annual Meeting, Chicago, IL, 1987.
DSM-III-R: convergence, coverage, and internal consis- 27. Jacobsberg L, Hymowitz P, Barasch A, Frances AJ.
tency. Am J Psychiatry 1988;145:573-577. Symptoms of schizotypal personality. Am J Psychiary 1986;
15. Vaglum P, Friis S, Vaglum S. Larsen F. Comparison 143:1222-1227.
between personality disorder diagnoses in DSM-III and 28. Widiger TA, Frances A, Trull TJ. A psychometric
DSM-III-R: reliability, diagnostic overlap, predictive valid- analysis of the social-interpersonal and cognitive-percep-
ity. Psychopathology 1989:22:309-314. tual items for the schizotypal personality disorder. Arch
16. Silk KR, Westen D, Lohr NE, Benjamin J, Gold L. Gen Psychiatry 1987;44:741-746.
12 KAVOUSSI AND SIEVER

29. Torgessen S. Genetic and nosological aspects of 41. Zimmerman M, Coryell WI-I. DSM-III personality
schizotypal and borderline personality disorders: a twin disorder dimensions. J Nerv Ment Dis 1990;178:686-692.
study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984;41:555-564. 42. Siever LJ, Klar H, Coccaro E. Psychobiologic sub-
30. McGlashan TH: Schizotypal personality disorder: strates of personality. In: Klar H, Siever LJ (ed): Biologic
Chestnut Lodge follow-up study: Long-term follow-up per- Response Styles: Clinical Implications. Washington, DC:
spectives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986;43:329-334. American Psychiatric Press, 1985, pp 38-66.
31. Plakun EM, Burkhardt PE, Muller JP. 14-year follow
43. Coccaro EF, Siever LJ, Klar HM, Maurer G, Coch-
up of borderline and schizotypal personality disorders.
rane K, Cooper TB, et al. Serotonergic studies in affective
Compr Psychiatry 1985;24:119-212.
and personality disorder patients: correlations with behav-
32. Schulz SC, Cornelius J, Schulz PM, Soloff PM. The
ioral aggression and impulsivity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;
amphetamine challenge test in patients with borderline
46:587-599.
disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1988;145:809-814.
33. Siever LJ, Keefe R, Bernstein DP, Coccaro EF, Klar 44. Siever LJ, Amin F, Coccaro EF, Bernstein D, Ka-
HM, Zemishlary Z, et al. Eye tracking impairment in voussi RJ, Kalus 0, et al. Plasma homovanillic acid in
clinically identified patients with schizotypal personality schizotypal personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1991;148:
disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1990;147:740-745 1246-1248.
34. Kass F, Skodol AE, Charles E, Spitzer R, Williams 45. Amin F, Coccaro EF, Trestman RL, Knott P, Mahon
JB. Scaled ratings of DMS-III personality disorders. Am J T, Davidson M, et al. CSF HVA concentrations in schizo-
Psychiatry 1985;142:627-630. typal and other personality disorders. Presented at the
35. Hyler S, Lyons M. Factor analysis of the DSM-III Society of Biological Psychiatry Annual Meeting, New
personality disorder clusters. Compr Psychiatry 1988;29:304- Orleans, LA, May 1991.
308. 46. Soloff PH, George A, Nathan RS, Schulz PM, Ulrich
36. Livesley WJ, Jackson DN. The internal consistency RF, Perel JM. Progress in pharmacotherapy of borderline
and factorial structure of behaviors judged to be associated disorders: a double blind study of amitriptyline, haloperidol,
with DSM-III personality disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1986; and placebo. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986;43:691-697.
143:1473-1474.
47. Goldberg SC, Schulz SC, Schulz PM, Resnick RJ,
37. Soloff PH, Millward JW. Psychiatric disorders in the
Hamer RM, Friedel RO. Borderline and schizotypal pa-
families of borderline patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983;40:
tients treated with low dose thiothixene vs placebo. Arch
37-44.
Gen Psychiatry 1986;43:680-686.
38. Spitzer RL, Endicott J. Justification for separating
schizotypal and borderline personality disorders. Schizophr 48. Cowdry RW, Gardner DL. Pharmacotherapy of bor-
Bull 1979;5:95-100. derline personality disorder: alprazolam, carbamazepine,
39. Widiger TA, Trull TJ, Hurt SW, Clarkin J, Frances trifluoperazine, and tranylcypromine. Arch Gen Psychiatry
A. A multidimensional scaling of the DSM-III personality 1988;45:111-119.
disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987;44:557-563. 49. Pfohl B, Coryell W, Zimmerman M, Stangl D. DSM-
40. Livesley WJ, Schroeder ML: Dimensions of personal- III personality disorders: diagnostic overlap and internal
ity disorder: the DSM-III-R cluster A diagnoses. J Nerv consistency of individual DSM-III criteria. Compr Psychia-
Ment Dis 1990;178:627-635. try 1986; 27121-34.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen