Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

Comparative Studies of Psychotherapies

Is It True That "Everyone Has Won and All Must Have Prizes"?
Lester Luborsky, PhD; Barton Singer, PhD; Lise Luborsky, MA

Tallies were made of outcomes of all reasonably controlled com- in the clinical sense where we believe a great deal is
parisons of psychotherapies with each other and with other treat- known, but in the controlled research sense where we be¬
ments. For comparisons of psychotherapy with each other, most lieve we are just beginning. We "know," for example, that
studies found insignificant differences in proportions of patients psychoanalysis works better with patients who have high
who improved (though most patients benefited). This "tie score ef-
fect" did not apply to psychotherapies vs psychopharmacotherapies ego-strength, but we can find only a little research evi¬
dence for this of the kind considered in this review.
compared singly\p=m-\psychopharmacotherapiesdid better. Combined
treatments often did better than single treatments. Among the com- Comparative studies of psychotherapies is not an area
parisons, only two specially beneficial matches between type of pa- where one or two decisive experiments can be telling—one
tient and type of treatment were found. must rely on the verdict of a series of at least passably
Our explanations for the usual tie score effect emphasize the com- controlled studies. Ideally, one would want to have an im¬
mon components among psychotherapies, especially the helping re- peccable definitive study that would settle the question of
lationship with a therapist. However, we believe the research does comparative worth once and for all, but it is not possible,
not justify the conclusion that we should randomly assign patients since every study has some uniqueness of sample charac¬
to treatments\p=m-\researchresults are usually based on amount of im- teristics measuring instruments, and other less easily de¬
provement; "amount" may not disclose differences in quality of im- fined aspects. A consensus of many studies is what we
provement from each treatment. must hope for.

The subtitle
in Wonderland-it
you will
recognize
the dodo bird
was
issince it
from Alice
who handed
The best way to summarize the studies is to consider
them separately for each of the main types of comparisons
down this happyverdict after judging the race. It was also that have been done; eg, group vs individual psychother¬
the subtitle of that classical paper by Saul Rosenzweig,1 apy, time-limited vs unlimited psychotherapy, client cen¬
"Some implicit common factors in diverse methods of psy¬ tered vs other traditional psychotherapies, and behavior
chotherapy." Our title implies what many of us believe- therapy vs psychotherapy. For each type of comparison, a
that all the psychotherapies produce some benefits for convenient "box score" is given with the number of stud¬
some patients. What we do not know is whether or not ies in which the treatments were significantly better or
there are psychotherapies that produce substantially bet¬ worse, or "tie score"—our term for not significantly differ¬
ter results and are especially suited to certain patients. entstatistically.
Here, when we use the word "know," we are not using it Only studies in which some attention was paid to the
main criteria of controlled comparative research were in¬
Accepted for publication Jan 3, 1975. cluded. The research quality of each study was scored ac¬
From the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania (Drs.
Luborsky and Singer); the Eastern Pennsylvania Psychiatric Institute (Dr.
cording to 12 criteria (see Criteria). Each departure from
Luborsky); Philadelphia Veterans Hospital (Dr. Singer); and Villanova each criterion was scored —1, somtimes —V2. Many of these
University (Ms. Luborsky). criteria were derived from those of Fiske et al.2 These 12
Read in part before the third annual meeting of the Society for Psycho- criteria were only to be considered as guidelines, since the
therapy Research, Nashville, Tenn, June 16,1972. A short version was pre- sum of the weights cannot be matched point for point
sented to the American Psychopathological Association meeting, Boston,
March 5,1974. The present version was read as the presidential address to with the validity of the study. In fact, for a particular
the Society for Psychotherapy Research, Denver, June 14, 1974.
Reprint requests to the Department of Psychiatry, University of Penn- study a single criterion may be absolutely crucial in deter¬
sylvania, 207 Piersol Bldg, University Hospital, 3400 Spruce St, Phila- mining its validity; for example, the use of random as¬
delphia, PA 19104 (Dr. Luborsky). signment in a study may have produced significantly dif-
Table 1.—List of Treatments Compared List of Treatments Compared (Continued)
Qua I- Out- Refer¬ Qual- Out- Refer¬
Treatment ity* comet ences Treatment ity* comet ences
Group vs individual psycho¬ D- Time-limited & short-term B— + Muench22
therapy groups improved more 1965
Decreasing order of Baehr' than long-term samples
effectiveness from group 1954 (on Rotter Test & Maslow
& individual therapy, Security-Insecurity
to individual therapy, Inventory)
to group therapy Time-limited client-centered Shlien23
Similarities of results of Barron & treatment compared 1957
group & individual Leary'0 favorably with longer,
generally greater than 1955 unlimited treatment, on
differences most outcome measures
Changes on discomfort & Imber et al" Time-limited treatment (20 Shlien
social ineffectiveness 1957 sessions) vs unlimited et al2"
scales were independent treatment (median: 37 1962
of type of therapy sessions)
Little difference in Haimowitz & 70% of patients treated Frank
effectiveness Haimowitz12 for 6 mo vs 74% who et al"
1952 dropped out in the first 1959
No difference in effectiveness month showed decrease
Thorley & in discomfort
Craske"
1950 'Ideal" long-term treatment, Pascal &
Group therapy, better + O'Brien brief supportive treat¬ Zax2«
adjustment ratings et al" ment, & environmental 1956
Rehospitalization rates did 1972 manipulation produced
not differ 0 high but not different
level of change
Slightly less improvement in Gelder
group than individual etal'5 Time-limited patients Reid &
therapy in rapidity of 1967 (maximum of 8 sessions) Schyne27
change (rating by patients improved more than those 1969
on main phobia, ratings
in long-term treatment
by psychiatrists on Client centered ("Rogerian")
anxiety & depression) vs other traditional psycho¬
No difference in general Peck" therapies
improvement or in separate 1949 Client centered vs psycho¬ Cart-
areas (adjustment & analytic: no difference wright28
in degree of experiencing 1953
symptoms) & level of self-observation
No differences between Slawson17
Client centered vs psycho¬ Heine29
psychodrama added to 1965
individual plus routine analytic vs Adlerian 1953
treatment vs "controls" psychotherapy: patients
receiving individual reported no difference in
amount of change
plus routine treatment
Little difference between Boe et alls Client centered ("Reflec¬ Baker30
regular hospital treat¬ 1966 tive") vs "leading" therapy 1960
ment with individual vs ("Neo-Freudian"): no
difference
regular hospital treatment
with group treatment Client centered ("Reflec¬ Ashby
Patients treated by brief or B+ + Pearl" tive") yielded lower et al3'
Intensive group therapy 1955 improvement ratings than 1957
showed more reduction "leading" therapy
in California ethnocen- Client centered vs Adlerian Shlien
trism scale than patients et al2"
treated by individual 1962
psychotherapy Psychotherapy vs behavior
Group with diazepam, Covi therapy
imipramine hydrochloride, et al»! Results of behavior therapy Cooper
or placebo vs brief 1974 vs matched psychother¬ et al32·33
individual supportive apy controls 1965,
therapy with diazepam, 29 severeagoraphobias 1963
imipramine, or placebo with behavior therapy,
no difference from
Time-limited vs time-unlim¬
ited treatment matched psychotherapy
Compared to patients in controls
Henry & 12 limited "other phobias"
long unlimited treatment, Shlienîi
patients in brief time- 1958 improved more with
limited treatment showed behavior therapy than
severe decline in affect matched sample with
differentiation (on TAT), psychotherapy +
but no difference on At 1-yr follow-up, the 12
"other phobias" no
therapist rating, difference between
behavioral index, and
Q-sort behavior therapy &
psychotherapy
Table 1.—List of Treatments Compared (Continued) List of Treatments Compared (Continued)
Qua I- Out- Refer¬ Qual¬ Out- Refer¬
Treatment ity* comet ences Treatment ity* comet ences
10 behavior therapy vs 10 Gelder & 13 behavior therapy vs 13 Zitrin et al
conventional psycho¬ Marks3" supportive psychotherapy 1974
therapy (all severe 1966 (unpub¬
agoraphobics) lished
At the end of 1 yr, not data)
different (all severe Psychopharmacotherapy alone
agoraphobics) vs psychotherapy alone
17 students (who went to Crighton & Phenothiazine and/or group C + Gorham"2
health services spon¬ Jehu" psychotherapy (schizophrenics) 1964
taneously) desensitization 1969 Stelazine vs psychotherapy, + May &
vs group therapy on length of hospital Turna43""
Both treatments improved stay, release rate, & 1964 &
but no difference in supplemental treatment 1965
improvement feelings
on
(schizophrenics)
about exams, sleep
Trifluoperazine vs group + Evangela-
disturbance, or grades psychotherapy- kis«
16 desensitization, 16 group, B+ Gelder adjunctive therapy 1961
10 individual at end of 6 et al« + Lorr
mo desensitization did best 1967
Chlordiazepoxide vs
psychotherapy et al"
(severe agoraphobics in 1963
sample did poorly) +
Overall &
At end of 2-yr follow-up, Psychopharmacotherapy alone vs +
differences
no 0 psychotherapy alone Tupín"7
Penothiazine & 1969
Behavior therapy ("operant- King antidepressants vs
¡nterpersonal" therapy) + et al36
psychotherapy
did best (hospitalized 1960
schizophrenics) vs Drug groups (meprobamate, 0 Koegler &
prochlorperazine, Brill"8
verbal therapy, recre¬
1967
therapy,
ational & no phénobarbital) vs
therapy psychotherapy
Group desensitization vs Lazarus37 Amitriptyline hydrochloride + Klerman
vs psychotherapy et al"'.5o
group interpretation 1961
(plus relaxation) for 1974,
matched pairs of 1973
agoraphobics &claustro- Chlorpromazine vs + Hogarty &
phibics psychotherapy Goldbergs'
Group desensitization vs 1973
group psychotherapy for Psychotherapy plus
all patients psychopharmacotherapy vs
10 implosive therapy vs 20 Levis & psychopharmacotherapy alone
conventional therapy vs Carrera38 Chlorpromazine, alone & as + Cowden
10 no treatment; implo¬ 1967 adjunct to group et al"
sive therapy showed shift psychotherapy 1956
from pathology, conven¬ Chlorpromazine & group therapy King*3
tional therapy not more (hospitalized chronic 1958
effective than on waiting schizophrenics)
list 3 mo
Phenothiazine and/or group Gorham
7 systematic desensitization, McReyn- psychotherapy et al"2
7 insight-oriented olds3» (schizophrenics) 1964
psychotherapy, & 14 1969
Stelazine & psychotherapy vs May &
relaxation therapy Turna"3 ""
stelazine on length of
20 behavior therapy (4.1 Marks & hospital stay, release rate, & 1964 &
sessions per week) vs Gelder"» supplemental treatment 1965
20 controls in psycho¬ 1965 (schizophrenics)
therapy (2.4 sessions Chlorpromazine & group King«
per week) (all phobies)
psychotherapy 1963
58 behavior therapy patients Patterson (schizophrenics)
treated (in first 5 mo) et al"'
Trifluoperazine hydrochloride Evangela-
improved more than 69 1971 & group psychotherapy- kis"5
others in psychoanalytic adjunctive therapy 1961
psychotherapy
Patients in both samples Psychotherapy plus drug vs Overall &
in second period improved drug alone Tu in47
equally (inexperienced 1969
therapists did better with Chlordiazepoxide used with Lorr
behavior therapy; with psychotherapy et al"6
experience, effectiveness 1963
of both treatments Antidepressants (amitriptyline) 0 Klerman
equal) and psychotherapy (relapse + et al"».5o
31 behavior therapy vs 30 B+ Sloan rate) (social adjustment) 1974,
insight-oriented therapy; et al 1973
at 4 mo, no difference; 1974 -f
Chlorpromazine & sociotherapy Hogarty &
at 1 yr, no difference (unpub¬ Goldberg5'
lished 1973
data)
Table 1.—List of Treatments Compared (Continued) List of Treatments Compared (Continued)
Qual¬ Out- Refer¬ Qual¬ Out- Refer¬
Treatment ity* comet ences Treatment ity* comet ences
Psychotherapy plus Peptic ulcer: 32 medication, Chappell &
pharmacotherapy vs diet, & group psychological Stevenson66
psychotherapy alone training vs 22 medication & diet 1936
Phenothiazine & group Gorham Ulcerative colitis: 34 Grace
psychotherapy vs group et al"2 superficial psychotherapy et al67
therapy (schizophrenics) 1964 & diet & medication vs 34 1954
Stelazine &psychotherapy + May& diet & medication
vs psychotherapy effect Turna"3·"" Duodenal ulcer (augmented B- Glen68
on length of hospital stay, 1964, histamine test): 21 medical 1968
release rate, & supple¬ 1965 therapy vs 24 psychotherapy
mental treatment
Bronchial asthma: 33 group Groen &
(schizophrenics) psychotherapy & medication Pelser6'
Reserpine alone &as + Cowden vs inhalants & medication 1960
adjunct to psychotherapy et al» vs inhalants
(schizophrenics) 1956
Recovery from heart attack: + Gruen
Psychotherapy & B+ Grinspoon psychotherapy plus medical 1974
phenothiazine pharmaco¬ et a|55,5' regimen vs medical regimen (unpub¬
therapy (chronic 1967, lished
schizophrenics) 1968 data)
Shader
et al*7
Asthma: hypnosis & relaxation -I- Maher-
1969 vs drugs Loughman
et al7»
Psychotherapy plus Gibbs 1962
chlorpromazine vs et al*8
Ulceratitie-colitis: 57 + O'Conner
psychotherapy 1957
psychotherapy & drugs et al7'
(schizophrenics) vs 57 drugs alone 1964
Chlordiazepoxide used with + Lorr
Sinclair-
Warts (subjects who had failed
psychotherapy vs et al«
with physical treatment): Gieban
psychotherapy (outpatients) 1963
7 hypnosis therapy & 14 et al72
Meprobamate & Lorr suggestion applied on only 1959
chlorpromazine with et ais' one side of the body
psychotherapy (outpatients) 1961
Hypertension: group D- Titchener
Psychotherapy and drug Rickels psychotherapy & medical —

et al73
(meprobamate) vs psycho¬ et al«8 management vs medical 1959
therapy (neurotic 1966 management§
outpatients) Dermatoses: hypnotherapy + Zhukov7"
Psychotherapy & imipramine + Daneman61 & resort treatment vs 1961
vs psychotherapy (depressive 1961 resort treatment
reactions) (neurotics) Psychotherapy vs control Grace
Diazepam, phénobarbital, & Hesbacher 34 superficial psychotherapy et al67
placebo: combined et al« vs 34 (matched) treated 1954
treatment better than psycho¬ 1970 with diet & medication
therapy & placebo (neurotics) (patients in hospital with
Trifluoperazine-group + Evangela- ulcerative colitis)
psychotherapy-adjunctive kis« 44 individual psychotherapy Morton75
therapy vs group therapy 1961 matched in pairs with 44 1955
(mixed inpatients) (90 days no treatment)
Psychotherapy plus C + Overall & 10 group therapy, 10 no + Tucker76
phenothiazine & Tupin"7 treatment (chronic hospital 1956
antidepressants vs 1969 soiling behavior)
psychotherapy (mixed inpatients) Group therapy vs no Coons77
Amitriptyline & + Klerman treatment, (mainly 1957
psychotherapy vs et al"' hospitalized schizophrenics)
psychotherapy 1974
Group therapy (2 times a Jensen78
Psychotherapy plus B- + Podobnikar63 week, 13 weeks) vs 1 group 1961
chlordiazapoxide 1971 no therapy, but consultation
hydrochloride (Librium) with nurses, vs 1 group no
vs psychotherapy with placebo therapy, no consultation
Psychotherapy plus Karon & (44 closed-ward women)
pharmacotherapy vs Vandenbos6" 1 sample with psychiatrist < Sheldon7'
psychotherapy for 1970 (outpatient department) 1964
inexperienced therapists vs 1 sample with nurse
(schizophrenics) (day care center) vs 1
Psychotherapy plus sample with general
pharmacotherapy vs practitioners (psychiatric
psychotherapy alone for aftercare, schizophrenic women)
experienced therapists 37 group treatment with + Shattan
Psychological therapy (combined psychiatrist & social et al8°
usually with medical regimen) worker vs 23 no systematic 1966
vsmedical regimen alone (for psychotherapy: better
psychosomatic conditions) rehospitalization rates &
Eczema: dermatological & B- + Brown & highly significant difference
psychiatric treatment vs Bettley« in number granted absolute
dermatological treatment 1971 discharge (mostly schizophrenics)
Table 1.—List of Treatments Compared (Continued) List of Treatments Compared (Continued)

Qual¬ Out- Refer¬ Qual- Out- Refer¬


Treatment ity* comet ences Treatment ity* comet ences
Counseling biweekly, Stotsky 31 behavior therapy vs 30 Sloane
emphasizing vocational et al8' psychotherapy vs 33 et al
counseling vs no counseling 1955 waiting list: on 3 target 1974
(chronic schizophrenics), symptoms after 4 mo all (unpub¬
Q-sort, & work adjunct & 3 samples improved, 2 lished
trial visit measures + treated samples more than data)
Psychiatric symptoms, ward waiting list sample.
adjustment & personality No differences between these
measures samples at 4 mo or 1 yr
Group therapy & electric B- Peyman82 Treated samples (client Shlien
convulsive shock vs no 1956 centered and Adlerian) et al2"
treatment (chronic improved more (in self- 1962
schizophrenics) + ideal correlations) than
Group therapy (chronic waiting list controls
schizophrenics) vs no treatment + (nonpyschotic)
Treated samples vs 2 + Shlien Psychoanalytically oriented Brill
untreated samples on et al2" psychotherapy vs waiting et al85
O-sort measure (mostly neurotic) 1962 list controls: more 1964
2 conventional treatment Levis & Improvement on most
samples vs control sample Carrera38 measures (nonpsychotic) -¬
Implosive therapy vs control 1967 No difference follow-up
on
on some measures, eg, Psychoanalytically oriented + Endicott &
drop of Minnesota psychotherapy vs waiting Endicott86
Multiphasic Personality list controls: more 1964
Inventory score into normal improvement on some
range (patients with relative¬ measures (mixed diagnoses)
ly severe signs of pathology) 2 therapy samples (group Barron &
Operant-interpersonal King therapy & individual Leary'8
treatment improved more et al36 therapy) vs 23 waiting 1955
than other samples on 1960 list controls
most measures (locked Psychotherapy samples + Karon & Van-
ward schizophrenics) + improved more than denbos6"
Verbal therapy group vs controls (P <.01) 1970
control (locked ward (schizophrenics)
schizophrenics) Sociotherapy vs control 0 Hogarty &
3 group therapy groups vs 1 MacDonald (schizophrenics) Goldberg5'
control (no treatment) et al83 1973
group: no difference in 1964
Walker &
releases from hospital & Psychotherapy vs control
number transferred to (schizophrenics) Kelley87
locked ward nor in rule 1960
Infractions (schizophrenics) Psychotherapy (client + Rogers &
Counseled students vs controls Volsky centered) vs wait for Dymond88
treatment 1954
(not counseled) etal8"
1965 Psychotherapy (client Rogers
Treated (psychotherapy) vs centered) vs controls et al8'
May & (routine hospital treatment 1967
untreated (basic hospital Tu ma""
on a variety of measures at
care): no difference in 1965
termination)
rehospitalization rate or (schizophrenics)
time in hospital in 3 yr At 1-year follow-up,
after initial admission
No difference in Health- psychotherapy patients had
Sickness Rating Scale spent more time out of
hospital_
* See text 999. Both studies less formally structured at outset as time-limited
t Treatment (underlined) significantly better (P < .05 or better) than true for other studies.
than compared treatment (+); treatments not significantly different § This was only difference: change for two groups for systolic
(0); treatment (underlined) significantly worse (P < .05 or better) blood pressure; but study was borderline in design, especially be¬
(-). cause of uncontrolled assignment of patients.

ferent patient samples to be compared. grading system was not to provide highly reliable subdivi¬
All studies were graded according to how well they fit sions of grading so much as it was to weed out the worst
the criteria of controlled comparative studies on a scale studies. Nevertheless, it was reassuring to find that the in¬
from A to E. An A indicates the main criteria of search dependent grading judgments on the scale by two of us
design were mainly satisfied; B, one or two were partially (L.L. and B.S.) on 16 randomly selected studies yielded a
deficient; C, three or four were partially deficient; D, three correlation of .84.
or four were partially deficient and one was seriously
deficient; and E, the deficiencies were sufficiently serious Criteria
so that the results were not worth considering and the 1. Controlled assignment of patients to each group: Regardless
study, therefore, was not included. (The grades for each of which methods was used, the aim was to achieve comparability
study are noted in Table 1.) The primary purpose of our of the groups on the important dimensions. (For psychotherapy
studies, one crucial dimension is initial severity of the patient's vanee to the problems of practitioners than studies of non-
illness.) patients, this review will consider only research in which
(a) Random assignment: This is a risky way to assign patients, bona fide patients were in bona fide treatment—excluded
despite its use in most studies. Unless the groups are then checked were role-playing studies and those using student volun¬
for comparability (as in b), random assignment gives little assur¬ teers.
ance of comparability.
Within these limits, the present review is more com¬
(b) Matching of total groups: A fairly adequate method. than any; it combines many of the studies of the
(c) Matching in pairs: This is the most powerful way of assign¬ plete
three most complete reviews: Bergin,4 Meltzoff and Korn¬
ing patients.
No difference in composition of the groups by the end of therapy reich,5 and Luborsky et al,6 with additional types of com¬
by virtue of different amount of kind of dropouts. parisons that have not been reviewed before. The diffi¬
2. Real patients were used. This is important enough so that culties encountered in locating and evaluating the rele¬
our present review only includes those with real patients. vant research are impressive. Therefore, it is not
3. Therapists for each group were equally competent. Very few surprising that some previous reviewers have presented
studies give information on which to judge this, although most biased conclusions about the verdict of this research liter¬
studies probably try to take this obvious factor into account. ature on the relative value of certain forms of psychother¬
4. Therapists were not inexperienced. A high percentage of the
studies used inexperienced therapists, since it is easier to get apy (eg, two replies to one of these reviewers, Luborsky7·8).
Since we tried to do a complete review—within the lim¬
inexperienced therapists to agree to carry out one's study. How¬ its noted—we can now complete our introduction with an
ever, the research is to be considered moderately impaired when
only inexperienced therapists were used. historical perspective. From a tabulation of the publica¬
5. Treatments were equally valued. This is a crucial criterion. It tion dates of the studies (Table 1), we learn that the entire
is violated routinely when a treatment was compared with a con¬ field of controlled comparative treatment research got its
trol in which no treatment wasoffered. However, even when two start only in the middle and late 1950s: the bulk of the
treatments were compared in studies, the treatments were
some studies were done in the last two decades. Within this pe¬
often presented in ways that create different impressions of the
extent to which they were valued-either to the therapists or pa¬
riod, each type of comparison had its special era. Group vs
individual treatment comparisons started as far back as
tients in each form of treatment.
1949 and continued to the present, but most of them were
6. The outcome measures took into account the target goals of
the treatment. Few studies did this explicitly. Probably all studies done in the decade of the 1950s. The time-limited vs time-
that use a therapist- or patient-rating of outcome take this into unlimited comparison was done mostly in the late 1950s
account as a matter of course (weight -%). and early 1960s. The client centered vs other psychother¬
7. Treatment outcome was evaluated by independent measures. apy comparisons began in the 1950s and extended to the
Most studies used the therapist as the main source of outcome in¬ first half of the 1960s. The psychotherapy vs behavior
formation. Some also used the patient; only a few used more inde¬ therapy comparisons only began in 1960, with most stud¬
pendent outcome measures. Because of the difficulty of making a ies being done in the late 1960s and some continuing to
judgment about which outcome measures are inherently best, it is the present. The psychotherapy vs pharmacotherapy com¬
difficult to weight this criterion very highly (see Luborsky3 on sug¬
gested independent clinical measures). parisons were represented by three studies done in the
late 1950s, with most of them being done in the 1960s and
8. Information was obtained about other concurrent treat¬
ments, both formal and informal, and these are not unequal in the continuing until the present. The psychotherapy vs medi¬
compared treatments. The most frequent instance in which this is cal regimen for psychosomatic illnesses covers the longest
important is the patient's taking of a variety of prescribed and time span, beginning in 1936, although studies are sparse
unprescribed drugs during comparative treatment studies. When in the entire period. The psychotherapy vs no psychother¬
there is no information on this (as is often the case) and when the apy comparison started in the 1950s and was well repre¬
compared treatments were associated with different amounts of sented then, but the vogue was over by the first half of the
the incidental, concurrent treatments, the study is impaired 1960s.
(weight lk). It would have been of special interest to compare quan¬
9. Samples of each of the compared treatments were indepen¬

titative comparative treatment research as a whole with


dently evaluated in terms of the extent to which they fit the desig¬ other kinds of therapy research. One way of doing this
nated type (weight V¿).
10. Each of the compared treatments was given in equal
— would have been to follow the procedure of Hoon and
amounts (ie, length or frequency). Lindsley9" of counting publications indexed under Psycho¬
11. Each treatment was given in reasonable amount (and in an logical Abstracts Annual Index for psychoanalysis, be¬
amount that is appropriate to the treatment) so that one can pre¬ havior therapy, client centered therapy, and psychology as
sume (or show) that a reasonable amount of benefit might have a whole (total annual abstracts beginning with the ab¬
occurred. stracts of 1927). It is clear that there has been no falling
12. Sample size was adequate. This is moderately important, es¬ off of publication rate in psychoanalysis. In fact, starting
pecially where random assignment had been used. Small sample in the early 1960s there has been a slight upward trend.
sizes can be tolerated when a matching method has been used for
assignment. (Nevertheless, as we will mention later on, there are
13. Other specific defects: A variety of other defects that may hardly any quantitative comparative treatment studies
be critical for within this.) The most dramatic rise is for behavior ther¬
particular studies.
apy beginning in the early 1960s. Client centered therapy
All included studies dealt with young adults or adults, publication rate has remained approximately the same al¬
and the majority of them were nonpsychotic patients. most since its start.
Since studies of patients seem more likely to have relè- Finally, it is satisfying to note that the research quality
of the studies, in terms of our quality ratings, for most ented therapy.
types of comparisons has improved some in the last few The comparisons of client centered with other psy¬
decades. The simplest way to demonstrate this was to di¬ chotherapies disclosed a similar phenomenon—most (four
vided all studies into quality ratings A and vs C and D, out of five) showed "ties," regardless of what other school
and then note the mean pulbication date in each cate¬ it was compared with (ie, psychoanalytic, neo-Freudian, or
gory—the Cs and Ds tend to be somewhat older. Adlerian).
Box Score
Psychotherapy vs Group Psychotherapy
Client centered (ie, "nondirective")
For comparative studies of individual vs group psycho¬ was better 0
therapy, the gains for each treatment were usually re¬ Tie 4
ported to be similar—in nine comparisons. Only two Other traditional psychotherapies
comparisons showed a slight advantage for individual were better 1
treatment, and two an advantage for group treatment
(but one of these only in terms of improvement in ethno- Behavior
Therapy vs Psychotherapy
centrism). The only study with schizophrenic patients There are 19 controlled comparisons in 12 studies deal¬
(O'Brien et al14) showed an advantage for group treat¬ ing with patients, although there are many more with stu¬
ment. A box score summarizes these results and makes dent volunteers. (Also not reviewed is the large literature
plain that most of the 13 comparisons (one study provided on treatment comparisons for people who have specific
two comparisons) showed no significant difference be¬ "habit" disturbances, eg, smoking, bed-wetting, drug-tak¬
tween these treatments. In view of the general opinion ing, and overeating rather than pervasive personality and
that group psychotherapy is less intensive, the results are
a surprise.
adjustment disorders that lead them to seek psychother¬
apy.) Of these, behavior therapy emerged as superior to
Box Score the other psychotherapies in six comparisons, and as no
2 different in 12. Those that showed some form of behavior
Group was better
Tie 9 therapy to be superior include Gelder et al,15 Cooper and
Individual was better 2 others,32 King et al,3B Lazarus,37 Levis and Carrera,38 and
Patterson et al.41 The 13 comparisons where they were not
Time-Limited vs Time-Unlimited Psychotherapy significantly different include Gelder et al15 (in patients
Since Otto Rank, treatments that are structured at the with more complex symptoms), Cooper et al32 (general
outset as time-limited have been thought by some practi¬ change measures as opposed to specific improvement in
tioners to be as good as the more usual time-unlimited phobias), Gelder and Marks,34 Lazarus,37 Marks and
treatment. The eight available controlled comparative Gelder,40 McReynolds,39 and others (R. B. Sloane, MD, J.
studies are mostly (five out of eight) consistent with this Wolpe, MD, A. Cristol, MD, et al and C. M. Zitrin, MD, D.
view in that there is no significant difference between the F. Klein, MD, C. Lindemann, PhD, et al, unpublished
two. Only in Henry and Shlien21 was time-limited psycho¬ data).
therapy shown to be inferior in one criterion; that is, pa¬ Box Score
tients showed a decline in affect differentiation on the Behavior therapy (usually densensitization
Thematic Apperception Test. In two studies, time-limited was better) 6
psychotherapy was shown to be better (Muench22 and Reid Tie 13
and Schyne27)· Our conclusion, therefore, is that usually Psychotherapy was better 0
differences in this treatment dimension seemed to make
no significant difference in treatment results.
Thus, we see similarly that in most of the comparisons
of behavior therapy with other psychotherapies (ie, 13 out
Box Score of 19), the differences in the amount of benefits they pro¬
Time-limited was better 2 vide for patients are not significant.
Tie 5 All six treatment comparisons where a form of behavior
Time-unlimited was better 1 therapy was superior utilized very brief therapies, and
five of the six were comparisons based on relatively poor
Client Centered vs Other Traditional
Psychotherapies research quality; ie, ratings of C and D. There is a trend
Of 11 studies comparing results of different schools of for behavior therapy to achieve benefits earlier while more
treatment (ie, client centered, psychoanalytic, and Ad¬ traditional psychotherapies move at a slower rate. The
lerian), only four of the 11 found a significant difference more rapid initial gains of behavior therapy may appear
between one school's treatment and another. However, ex¬ because it is more directive or because it is more often
cept for five studies of client centered psychotherapy, structured as time-limited treatment, or both—according
there are not enough comparative studies in any one cate¬ to Shlien et al,24 time-limited treatment yielded earlier on¬
gory to draw conclusions about a specific school of treat¬ set of improvement.
ment. Furthermore, some studies were not acceptably con¬ In the two studies with patients with circumscribed and
trolled (and not included among the 11); for example, mild phobias, desensitization did better (Gelder et al15 and
Ellis,91 with only one therapist (himself) practicing two Cooper et al32). More studies are needed in which behavior
different treatments, reported that rational emotive ther¬ therapies are applied to patients who have generalized
apy yielded better results than psychoanalytically ori- maladjustments (as in Sloane et al).
Most of the behavior therapy studies we have listed deal One other conclusion is noteworthy: a few studies re¬
only with one form of behavior therapy, systematic de¬ ported that pharmacotherapy effects occur earlier and
sensitization. More comparative studies within the behav¬ may decline in time, while psychotherapy effects are
ior therapies need to be done with other specific behavioral slower to develop but may increase in time (eg, Shlien
techniques, such as a study by Boulougouris et al92 compar¬ et al23).
ing desensitization and flooding for phobias that showed a
significant advantage for flooding. Similarly, the typical Psychotherapy Plus a Medical Regimen vs
Medical Regimen Alone For Psychosomatic Conditions
result for the comparison of behavior therapy vs other
psychological treatments (other than psychotherapy) is For a variety of psychosomatic symptoms—ulcer, colitis,
probably consistent with Marks et al93 who compared be¬ asthma, and dermatoses—the comparisons are over¬
havior therapy with hypnosis and found no significant dif¬ whelmingly in favor of combined treatment—psychother¬
ference. Morganstern94 notes that in the comparison of apy plus a medical regimen. Of 11 studies where the
systematic desensitization and implosion, of nine studies, target of treatment was change in a psychosomatic symp¬
six were tied and three showed systematic desensitization tom, nine showed a significant advantage for psychother¬
to be better. (These studies were mostly with student vol¬ apy plus a medical regimen, or psychotherapy as opposed
unteers.) The brief review by Peter Nathan, PhD (at the to a medical regimen alone (two of these studies are pri¬
1973 Society for Psychotherapy Research meeting, Phila¬ marily some form of psychotherapeutic treatment alone).
delphia), also suggests that the trend for results of com¬ Box Score
parisons of behavior therapies with each other will be "tie
scores." Another larger review (B. E. Wolfe, PhD, unpub¬ Psychotherapy plus medical regimen
was better 9
lished data) on the behavior therapies in the treatment of Tie 1
the habit disorders comes to a similar conclusion. Medical regimen was better 1
Psychopharmacotherapy vs Psychotherapy Why do the results for comparative studies of psycho¬
somatic symptoms favor psychotherapy so strongly? In
Many of these controlled comparisons have been sur¬ addition to the fact that combined treatment is being
veyed in the reviews by May95 and Uhlenhuth et al96; our
own review includes those that fit our criteria. The studies compared with a single treatment, most likely the reas¬
surance and support provided by psychotherapy are espe¬
are in three main types of comparisons; psychotherapy vs
pharmacotherapy, psychotherapy plus pharmacotherapy cially useful for the patients with psychosomatic symp¬
toms. The results may also derive from the greater ease of
vs psychotherapy alone, and psychotherapy plus pharma¬
cotherapy vs pharmacotherapy alone, with box scores for evaluating the benefits of psychotherapy for patients with
each below: a clear-cut target psychosomatic symptom.
Box Score
Psychotherapy vs "Control" Groups
Psychopharmacological agent was
better 7 A final special comparison is between psychotherapy
Tie 1 and its absence. "Absence of psychotherapy" is typically
Psychotherapy was better 0 measured in these studies by arranging for a more or less
Box Score matched group of patients to be assessed before and after
an interval without formal psychotherapy. These "con¬
Psychotherapy plus pharmacotherapy trols" include "no psychotherapy," "wait for psychother¬
was better 6
Tie 5 apy," "minimal psychotherapy," or hospital care alone.
Pharmacotherapy alone was better 0 Such groups, by virtue of their contacts and relationship
with the researchers, or because they were sometimes
Box Score
maintained by general hospital care, were provided with
Psychotherapy plus pharmacotherapy some of the nonspecific ingredients of treatment. Such
was better 13 studies tend to be shaky in meeting design criteria, par¬
Tie 3 ticularly the inequality in how the patients and staff value
Psychotherapy alone was better 0 what is provided for each group of patients. Of course,
The studies in these three comparisons include many there is also an inequality in the patient's motivation and
more inpatients (of whom the majority are schizophrenic) level of expectation of benefiting—if the "control" pa¬
than is true for our other comparisons. Of course we won¬ tients achieve any benefits, they might well be surprised
dered whether or not a division into inpatient vs out¬ and pleased; if the treated patients do not achieve bene¬
patient or a diagnostic categorization would make a dif¬ fits commensurate with their investment, they might well
ference in these results. The findings shown in Table 2 be surprised and disappointed. Both conditions might well
suggest there is no obvious difference. However, it is likely affect the outcome judgments so as to increase their in-
that for many, if not most, of these studies the selection of comparability.
the patients, even for the outpatient groups, favored those Many of the 33 comparisons in the box score that fol¬
who would benefit from pharmacotherapy; ie, patients lows were among the much larger number surveyed in
who would expect to be given medication rather than psy¬ Meltzoff and Kornreich.3 Many of those listed by them,
chotherapy and psychotherapy that is unreasonably re¬ however, were not used by us because of research design
stricted in length. inadequacies or because they were not usual patient popu-
Table 2.—Box Scores of Comparative Studies*
Combined Therapy Combined Therapy Drug Therapy Alone
vs vs vs
Psychotherapy Alone Drug Therapy Alone Psychotherapy Alone
_ _
Better Same Better Same Better
Refer¬ Refer¬ Refer¬ Refer¬ Refer¬
ences ences ences ences ences
Schizophrenic inpatients 42, 44, 52, 53, 55 2 53, 58 42, 51, 52, 54 2 36, 44 42, 44, 51 0
Mixed inpatients 2 26, 45 26,45 26,45
Subtotal
Mixed outpatients 46
Depressed outpatients 48 1 48 48 1 48
Neurotic (anxious) outpatients 4 27, 61, 62, 88 37
Subtotal
Total 13
*
Subdivided according to diagnosis and inpatient vs outpatient status.

lations (eg, prisoners). elusion about the comparison with other psychotherapies:
Twenty (or about 60%) of the comparisons significantly client-centered psychotherapy and behavior therapies.
favored psychotherapy, but 13 showed a tie, meaning that The preponderance of nonsignificant differences between
the psychotherapy was not significantly better than the treatments should gain in impressiveness when one con¬
nonpsychotherapy in almost a third of the comparisons. siders that researchers as well as editors of journals may
None of the comparisons favored the control group. tend to hesitate about publishing results of studies with
We considered, in searching for explanations, whether nonsignificant differences. Also, many of these compari¬
or not the 13 comparisons showing a tie might have in¬ sons are studied by partisans of one treatment or the
cluded more chronic inpatients. Hardly any trend in this other.
direction was found—of 19 comparisons for schizophrenic It is natural to question whether or not, despite care in the de¬
patients, eight were a "tie"; of 14 comparisons for non- sign, the therapeutic allegience of the experimenters might in
schizophrenic patients, five were a "tie." Two more appli¬ some way influence the results, since the comparisons are often
cable explanations might be that the nonspecific ingredi¬ not double-blind and not impeccable in other ways. We, therefore,
ents are often powerful for both the psychotherapy and examined the list of authors and asked some of their peers about
their therapeutic allegiences.
the "control groups" (cf, Frank,97 and Sloane et al), and
It appears to be a meaningful question only for those forms of
the treatment effects often are not powerful enough to treatment where a strong allegience is present. Only two of these
produce significant advantage over the beneficial forces clearly qualify: that is, behavior therapy vs other psychotherapies
activated by nonspecific factors. and client centered therapy vs other psychotherapies. For the
Schizophrenic Nonschizophrenic rest, affiliations tend to be less strong.
Box Score Patients Patients For the behavior therapy vs psychotherapy comparison, one ob¬
vious conclusion is that it is partisans of a form of treatment who
Psychotherapy do the studies of it. We could identify the affiliation of all but two
better 20 11 9
Tie
was
8
authorships and all of these were partisans of behavior therapy.
13 5 The same kind of observation occurs for the client centered vs
Control group other psychotherapies comparison—almost all of these are affili¬
was better 0 0 0 ated with client centered psychotherapy. This probably should
have been expected. Who else but a partisan would take the time
Conclusions and Implications
and energy to do a comparative treatment study? Since almost all
1. Most comparative studies ofdifferent forms ofpsycho¬ are partisans in various degrees, it is difficult to draw any conclu¬
sion about the role of partisanship in the results.
therapy found insignificant differences in proportions of
patients who improved by the end of psychotherapy. It is 2. The controlled comparative studies indicate that a
both because of this and because all psychotherapies pro¬ high percentage of patients who go through any of these
duce a high percentage of benefit (see conclusion 2) that psychotherapies gain from them. Meltzoff and Korn¬
we can reach a "dodo bird verdict"—it is usually true that reich,5""781 for example, basing their conclusions on the
"everybody has won and all must have prizes." This pre¬ controlled comparative studies, estimate that for both in¬
dominance of tie scores appears when different forms of dividual and group therapy about 80% of the studies show
psychotherapy are compared with each other; that is, it ap¬ mainly positive results. The same can be said for the other
plies to the first four comparisons: group vs individual psy¬ kinds of treatment that were compared. Even a fair per¬
chotherapy, time-limited vs time-unlimited psychother¬ centage of patients who go through minimal treatment
apy, client centered vs other traditional psychotherapies, seem to make some gains (as pointed out by Sloane et al
and behavior therapy vs other psychotherapies. Only the and others). This may have contributed to our surprising
last two comparisons involved "schools" of psychotherapy. finding that approximately a third of the comparisons of
It is noteworthy that in the 25 or 30 years of comparative psychotherapy with control groups do not show significant
treatment studies, only two schools of treatment have a differences. This general benefit effect may contribute to
sufficient number of comparative studies to permit a con- the high frequency of tie scores—if a very high percentage
of all patients receive benefits, it is, therefore, more diffi¬ cial kind of conditioning for delinquency developed by
cult to achieve a significant difference between different Gerald Patterson, PhD.
forms of treatment. Could the conclusions be artifacts of poor research? Defi¬
3. The "dodo bird verdict" does not apply when one ven¬ ciencies in the research designs and other artifactual
tures beyond comparisons of psychotherapies with each problems (Fiske et al2 and Rosenthal and Rosnow98) proba¬
other; ie, to comparisons ofpsychotherapy with other forms bly do not account for our main conclusion concerning sim¬
of treatment. (1) A preponderance of tie scores does not ilar improvement rates for the different forms of psycho¬
apply when psychotherapy vs other types of treatment therapy, because of the following:
such as pharmacotherapy are compared singly—In the (a) The criterion in the majority of these studies is the
available studies, pharmacotherapy produces significantly usual criterion—that is, therapist's judgment of improve¬
higher numbers of patients judged as benefiting. (2) It ment. (Some rely on independent clinical judges and some—
does not apply to combined treatments vs single treat¬ especially those using inpatients—utilize discharge rates
ments. The advantage for combined treatment is striking and readmission rates.) Although this criterion (like any
in that it appears for all three of the box scores deal¬ criterion) has its own vantage point (the therapist's opin¬
ing with combinations: for psychotherapy plus pharmaco¬ ion), nevertheless those studies using other criteria show a
therapy vs psychotherapy alone; for psychotherapy plus similar trend (in terms of comparative percentages of pa¬
pharmacotherapy vs pharmacotherapy alone; and for psy¬ tients benefiting) to those using only the therapist's judg¬
chotherapy plus a medical regimen vs a medical regimen ment as a criterion. One could argue that if we improved
alone (for psychosomatic illnesses). A combination of the quality of our outcome measures, we might find a
treatments may represent more than an additive effect of higher percentage of significant differences among psy¬
two treatments—a "getting more for one's money"—there chotherapies. While this possibility must be admitted, we
may also be some mutually facultative interactive bene¬ have no evidence so far to support it.
fits for the combined treatments. (3) It does not apply (6) Compared to many studies of psychotherapeutic re¬
to comparisons of psychotherapy vs "control groups" (eg, sults, especially those of three or four decades ago, these
absence of or minimal psychotherapy)-more than half of in our review are relatively well controlled—although only
these comparisons favor psychotherapy. a few of them come up to all of the recommendations for
4. There are only a few especially beneficial matches of comparison of treatments listed by Fiske et al.2 Further¬
type of treatment and type of patient-which is to be ex¬ more, despite deficiencies in the quality of the research in
pected since conclusion 1 is the dominant trend: (1) The the studies selected for the box scores the best designed do
most impressive match for the alleviation of a variety of not show a very different trend from those that are less well
psychosomatic symptoms is psychotherapy (and related designed.
psychological treatments) added to appropriate medical One direct way to illustrate this is to dichotomize the
treatment in comparison with a medical regimen alone. studies into two groups; those receiving a quality rating
(2) Behavior therapy may be especially suited for treat¬ of A or vs those receiving C or D. In general, the sub¬
ment of circumscribed phobias. groups show the same main trends. One possible excep¬
But it is, nevertheless, amazing in view of the large tion, however, is that five out of six of the comparisons in
clinical literature on matching patient and treatment that which behavior therapy is shown to be better than psycho¬
in our review we have come upon only two especially bene¬ therapy are in the poor quality category.
ficial matches between type of treatment and type of pa¬ It may also be of interest to note the overall research
tient. There are some other good candidates but these are quality for each type of comparative study. Here the larg¬
supported by only single studies rather than by the mass¬ est number of poor studies are to be found in the compari¬
ing of studies that we require for our present review. son of psychotherapy plus psychopharmacological agents
A symposium at the 1973 Society for Psychotherapy Re¬ vs psychopharmacological agents alone. Also for psycho¬
search meeting was focused on these, evaluating two logical treatment plus a medical regimen vs a medical
matches and attempting to locate others. This symposium, regimen alone, five out of the nine studies have D or D—
titled "Therapeutic technology: Effects of specific tech¬ ratings.
niques on specific disorders," discussing the advantage for What are the main ways ofimproving these comparative
psychosomatic symptoms of psychotherapy plus a medical treatment studies? Through the experience of evaluating
regimen vs a medical regimen alone (senior author); Ar¬ the quality of these studies, we have evolved a system for
nold Goldstein, PhD, presenting research on modifications judging them according to a list of 12 criteria partly based
of psychotherapy for lower class socioeconomic patients on Fiske et al.2 We will highlight here only those four cri¬
with special focus on prescriptive and modeling tech¬ teria on which most of the research is in need of improve¬
niques; Peter E. Nathan, PhD, reviewing behavior therapy ment.
in the treatment of phobias both circumscribed and gener¬ With regard to criterion 1, the patients should be de¬
alized; and Albert Stunkard, MD, discussing his research scribed, especially on certain crucial dimensions. This will
with Sydnor Penick, MD, on group behavior therapy for permit something better than random assignment of the
obesity. Some other candidates for special patient-treat¬ patients to the treatments. Composing groups by match¬
ment matches were considered briefly; one of them was a ing pairs of patients on crucial dimensions, such as sever¬
special form of conditioning for enuresis provided in the ity of illness, is highly desirable but very few of the stud¬
context of complete environmental control (particularly ies did this. Adequate description of the sample will also
the work of John Atthowe, PhD), and another was a spe- permit additional exploration of specific interactions of
type of treatment with type of patient. This last recom¬ various forms of psychotherapy probably should not be
mendation for improving experimental designs could lead taken to imply that the quality of the improvement is nec¬
to the confirmation of special patient-treatment matches essarily similar. The patient who has improved via group
and the discovery of new ones. Also, the lead provided in therapy or individual therapy may have gained something
the O'Brien et al14 study that group therapy may be espe¬ different in his conception of himself or in his capacity for
cially suitable for schizophrenics should be explored in reflecting from one who has improved via behavior ther¬
new studies; similarly more replications of Penick et al99 apy or chlordiazepoxide hydrochloride (Librium). There is
and Stunkard1™ should be done. only a little evidence for this supposition; eg, Heine,29
With regard to criterion 5, in many studies insufficient Klerman et al,50 and Dudek101; much more research needs
effort was made to present the treatments to the patients to be done on this.
as equally valued. Then, in addition, the patients in some Malan102 makes this the centerpiece in the conclusions to
studies may have known which therapies were most valued his review of outcome research problems, ie, "The failure
by the therapists or by the experimenters. to design outcome criteria and do justice to the complex¬
With regard to criterion 7, this criterion emphasizes the ity of the human personality." Malan has in mind develop¬
importance of evaluating the treatment outcome by inde¬ ing better measures that rely on clinical judgment to esti¬
pendent measures. Since treatments have a variety of im¬ mate the quality of the outcome. Comparative studies of
pacts, it is also important to include the main types in the educational treatments (Messick103) are also becoming
outcome criteria. The two main types of outcome that more concerned with learning the possible outcomes, not
must be evaluated are those related to specific symptoms just the intended outcomes, and with the interaction of
and those related to general adjustment. Different ther¬ the treatment conditions and individual differences in the
apies may produce different proportions of these. For ex¬ students.
ample, the behavior therapies and the pharmacotherapies 2. As noted earlier, the studies we reviewed are almost
may have more influence on the symptom-outcome mea¬ entirely limited to relatively short-term treatment; that
sures while the long-term, intensive psychoanalytically is, about 2 to 12 months. This is a glaring omission in the
oriented psychotherapies may have more influence on the research literature. We do not know enough about what
general adjustment measures. conclusions would be reached for long-term intensive
With regard to criterion 9, usually there was no evi¬ treatment.
dence offered that the treatment given actually fits the in¬ 3. Our conclusions apply to the results of comparative
tended form of treatment. The simplest and most direct studies of several forms of treatment. As indicated above,
way of doing this is rarely done: taking samples of the ad¬ usually no step is taken to show how well the designation
ministered treatment and having them judged indepen¬ fits. Even beyond this problem, it is very likely that cer¬
dently. Judging samples in this way will also do much to tain ingredients of the treatment that apply across treat¬
permit comparisons across treatments in different studies, ment labels are the main influencers of outcome. The ther¬
since there are so many varieties of treatment designated apist, for example, can be supportive, warm, and empathie
"psychotherapy"—eg, the "psychotherapy" provided for in a variety of differently designated forms of treatment,
schizophrenia may be quite different from the "psycho¬ and this may be a powerful influence on the outcome of
therapy" provided for neurotic patients. treatment.
Another aspect of criterion 9 is equally important. The 4. As we have noted in conclusion 4, there are a couple
length of the treatment and the length of the follow-up of especially promising matches of a type of patient and a
must be such as to be considered reasonable examples of type of treatment, and others may be soon established.
the designated form of treatment. Some forms of treat¬ In sum, for these reasons (and for other more general
ment exert their effects early (probably behavior ther¬ ones noted in Luborsky104 we should not yet consider our¬
apy, pharmacotherapy, time-limited therapy, and directive selves ready to make assignments on a random basis.
therapies); some may have a slower course and more long- How do we interpret the main finding in conclusion 1?
lasting effects (probably the insight-oriented psycho¬ Essentially, three factors are involved in accounting for
therapies and particularly psychoanalysis). The insight- the main finding that the studies do not produce any clear-
oriented psychotherapies are poorly represented in most cut winners when psychotherapies are compared with each
of these comparative studies—treatment lengths were other. To start with the least of the three first: (1) Since all
rarely more than one year and usually much, much less, forms of psychotherapy tend to achieve a high percentage
and follow-ups were either absent or too brief to catch the of improved patients (our conclusion 2), it is difficult (sta¬
assumed long-term benefits of the insight-oriented psy¬ tistically) for any single form of psychotherapy to show a
chotherapies. significant advantage over any other form—the higher
Is there a practical application of our conclusions in these percentages, the less room at the top for significant
terms of the assignment of patients to different forms of differences between treatments. A survey of the distribu¬
treatment? Taken at face value, our conclusions seem to tion of improvement ratings reported by different studies
dictate that from now on we should stop paying attention supports our assertion (J. Mintz, PhD, Lester Luborsky,
to the form of the treatment in referring patients for psy¬ unpublished data). (2) Although each form of psychother¬
chotherapy. Yet there are several reasons why we should apy differs in some elements of its philosophy, each offers
hesitate to recommend such a drastic departure from all to provide the patient with a plausible system of explana¬
the clinical wisdom: tions for his difficulties and also with principles that may
1. Similarities in numbers of patients benefiting from guide his future behavior. Such an organized explanatory
and guidance system may be one of the common elements not, however, covered the huge literature specifically on habit dis¬
that facilitates the benefits from all forms of psychother¬ orders (eg, addiction and bed wetting)—behavior therapy might be
apy (as was suggested by Rosenzweig1). (3) The most po¬ better for them—and we have not included many studies with stu¬
tent explanatory factor is that the different forms of dent volunteers rather than genuine patients.
Response of some skeptics about the efficacy of any form of psy¬
psychotherapy have major common elements—a helping chotherapy: "See, you can't show that one kind of psychotherapy
relationship with a therapist is present in all of them, is better than another, or, at times, even better than minimal or
along with the other related, nonspecific effects such as nonpsychotherapy groups. This is consistent with the lack of evi¬
suggestion and abreaction. This explanation is stressed by dence that psychotherapy does any good."
Rosenzweig,1 by Frank,97 by Strupp,105 and many others. Our answer: As we mentioned, the nonsignificant differences
This is exactly where more research needs to be done—on between treatments do not relate to the question of their bene¬
the components of a helping relationship (eg, in Strupp's fits—a high percentage of patients appear to benefit by any of the
comparison of trained vs untrained helpers Strupp105. psychotherapies or by the control procedures.
When differences among treatments do appear in some Response of some balanced psychotherapy researchers of any
orientation: "Before I ask my question, I first want to say that I
studies, they might then be explicable in terms of the pro¬ am pleased to see a careful review of comparative psychotherapy
portions of these components. studies with research quality considered. I hadn't realized, even
These common ingredients of psychotherapies may be
though I know the literature very well, that there were so many
so much more potent than the specific ones that it is controlled comparative studies, and that the trends you found
wrong to lump them together in the sense of giving them emerge so clearly. I was especially surprised about group psycho¬
equal weight. It is like making horse and canary pie by the therapy since I thought it was significantly less effective than in¬
Spanish recipe—horse and canary in equal proportions, one dividual psychotherapy, and I was surprised about behavior ther¬
horse and one canary. apy which I thought had more comparative treatment studies
with general patient populations which showed its superiority.
COMMENT And finally, I hadn't realized the advantages for combined treat¬
ments. Now for my question: Would we not learn more in future
It is not entirely fair and (and it may even be unther-
studies if we constructed the studies to investigate specific treat¬
apeutic) to present a report that arouses strong responses ments for specific types of patients?"
in many readers without giving them some chance to be Our answer: We couldn't agree with you more. But we should
heard. We, therefore, give a few of these responses based underline what has been found so far in the review, that the
on a small prepublication pretest sampling of opinion. breakdowns in terms of types of patients and types of treatments
Response of some psychoanalysts: "This doesn't adequately rep¬ have yielded little in terms of specific matches of type of patient
particularly psychoanalytic
resent long-term, intensive treatment, and form of treatment, with the possible exception of limited
treatment." phobias treated by behavior therapy and psychosomatic patients
Our answer: It is completely true, unfortunately. It is time treated by medical regimen plus psychotherapy.
there were some of such studies to include. This investigation was supported in part by Public Health Service Re¬
Response of some behavior therapists: "Behavior therapy is bet¬ search grant MH-15442 and Research Scientist Award MH-40710.
ter. You must not have looked at the right studies or included all Charles O'Brien, MD, PhD (for the group therapy section), Hans Strupp,
of them." MD, John Paul Brady, MD, Karl Rickels, MD (for the psychopharmacology
section), Bruce Sloane, MD, Peter E. Nathan, PhD (for the behavior ther¬
Our answer: For the general run of patient samples who seek apy section), Marjorie Cohen, and Freda Greene assisted in the preparation
psychotherapy, we have included all that could be found. We have of this report.

References

1. Rozenzweig S: Some implicit common factors in diverse methods. J Consult Psychol 18:179-183, 1954.
methods of psychotherapy. Am J Orthopsychiatry 6:412-415,1936. 10. Barron F, Leary TF: Changes in psychoneurotic patients
2. Fiske DW, Hunt H, Luborsky L, et al: The planning of re- with and without psychotherapy. J Consult Psychol 19:239-245,
search on effectiveness of psychotherapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1955.
22:22-32, 1970. 11. Imber SD, Frank JD, Nash EH Jr, et al: Improvement and
3. Luborsky L: Assessment of the outcome of psychotherapy by amount of therapeutic contact: An alternative to the use of no-
independent clinical evaluators: A review of the most highly rec- treatment controls in psychotherapy. J Consult Psychol 21:308-
ommended research measures, in Waskow I, Parloff M (eds): Mea- 315, 1957.
sures for the Outcome of Psychotherapy: Report of the Clinical Re- 12. Haimowitz NR, Haimowitz ML: Personality changes in
search Branch. Bethesda, Md, National Institute of Mental client-centered therapy, in Wolff W, Precher JA (eds): Success in
Health, to be published. Psychotherapy. New York, Grune & Stratton Inc, 1952, pp 63-93.
4. Bergin AE: The evaluation of therapeutic outcomes, in Ber- 13. Thorley AS, Craske N: Comparisons and estimate of group
gin AE, Garfield SL (eds): Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behav- and individual methods of treatment. Br Med J 1:97-100, 1950.
ior Change. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1971, pp 217-270. 14. O'Brien C, Hamm K, Ray B, et al: Group versus individual
5. Meltzoff J, Kornreich M: Research in Psychotherapy. New psychotherapy with schizophrenics: A controlled outcome study.
York, Atherton Press, 1970. Arch Gen Psychiatry 27:474-478, 1972.
6. Luborsky L, Chandler M, Auerbach AH, et al: Factors influ- 15. Gelder MG, Marks IM, Wolff HH: Desensitization and psy-
encing the outcome of psychotherapy: A review of quantitative re- chotherapy in the treatment of phobic states: A controlled inquiry.
search. Psychol Bull 75:145-185, 1971. Br J Psychiatry 113:53-73, 1967.
7. Luborsky L: A note on Eysenck's article: The effects of psy- 16. Peck RE: Comparison of adjunct group therapy with indi-
chotherapy: An evaluation. Br J Psychology 45:129-131, 1954. vidual psychotherapy. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 62:173-177, 1949.
8. Luborsky L: Another reply to Eysenck. Psychol Bull 78:406- 17. Slawson PF: Psychodrama as a treatment for hospitalized
408, 1972. patients: A controlled study. Am J Psychiatry 122:530-533, 1965.
9. Baehr GO: The comparative effectiveness of individual psy- 18. Boe E, Gocka EF, Kogan WS: The effect of group psycho-
chotherapy, group psychotherapy and a combination of these therapy on interpersonal perceptions of psychiatric patients. Mul-
tivar Behav Res 1:177-187, 1966. depression by drugs and psychotherapy. Am J Psychiatry 131:186-
19. Pearl D: Psychotherapy and ethnocentrism. J Abnorm Soc 191, 1974.
Psychol 50:227-229, 1955. 50. Klerman GL, Paykel ES, Prusoff BA: Antidepressant drugs
20. Covi L, Lipman RS, Derogatis LR, et al: Drugs and group and clinical psychopathology, in Cole J, Freeman A, Friedhoff A
psychotherapy in neurotic depression. Am J Psychiatry 131:191- (eds): Psychopathology and Psychopharmacology. Baltimore, Johns
198, 1974. Hopkins Press, 1973, pp 177-189.
21. Henry WE, Shlien JM: Effective complexity and psycho- 51. Hogarty GE, Goldberg SC: Drug and sociotherapy in the af-
therapy: Some comparisons of time-limited and unlimited treat- tercare of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 28:54-64,
ment. J Project Techniques 22:153-162, 1958. 1973.
22. Muench GA: An investigation of the efficacy of time-limited 52. Cowden RC, Zax M, Hague JR, et al: Chlorpromazine, alone
psychotherapy. J Counsel Psychol 12:294-298, 1965. and as an adjunct to group psychotherapy in the treatment of psy-
23. Shlien JM: Time-limited psychotherapy: An experimental chiatric patients. Am J Psychiatry 12:552-572, 1956.
investigation of practical values and theoretical implications. J 53. King PD: Regressive ECT, chlorpromazine and group ther-
Counsel Psychol 4:318-322, 1957. apy in treatment of hospitalized chronic schizophrenics. Am J
24. Shlien JM, Mosak HH, Dreikurs R: Effects of time limits: A Psychiatry 115:354-357, 1958.
comparison of two psychotherapies. J Counsel Psychol 9:31-34, 54. King PD: Controlled study of group psychotherapy in schiz-
1962. ophrenics receiving chlorpromazine. Psychiatr Dig 24:21-26,1963.
25. Frank JD, Gliedman LH, Imber SD, et al: Patients' expec- 55. Grinspoon L, Ewalt JR, Shader R: Long-term treatment of
tancies and relearning as factors determining improvement in chronic schizophrenia: A preliminary report. Int J Psychiatry
psychotherapy. Am J Psychiatry 115:961-968, 1959. 4:116-128, 1967.
26. Pascal GR, Zax M: Psychotherapeutics: Success or failure? J 56. Grinspoon L, Ewalt JR, Shader R: Psychotherapy and
Consult Psychol 20:325-331, 1956. pharmacotherapy in chronic schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry
27. Reid WJ, Schyne AW: Brief and Extended Casework. New 124:1645-1652, 1968.
York, Columbia University Press, 1969. 57. Shader R, Grinspoon L, Ewalt JR, et al: Drug responses in
28. Cartwright RD: A comparison of the response to psychoana- acute schizophrenia, in Sankar DVS (ed): Schizophrenia: Current
lytic and client centered psychotherapy, in Gottschalk L, Auer- Concepts and Research. Hicksville, NY, PJD Publications, 1969, pp
bach A (eds): Methods of Research in Psychotherapy, New York, 161-173.
Appleton-Century-Crofts Inc, 1966, pp 517-529. 58. Gibbs JJ, Wilkins B, Lautergach CG: A controlled clinical
29. Heine RW: A comparison of patients' reports on psycho- psychiatric study of chlorpromazine. J Clin Exp Psychopathol
therapeutic experience with psychoanalytic, nondirective and Ad- 18:269-283, 1957.
lerian therapists. Am J Psychother 7:16-25, 1953. 59. Lorr M, McNair DM, Weinstein GJ, et al: Meprobamate and
30. Baker E: The differential effects of psychotherapeutic ap- chlorpromazine in psychotherapy: Some effects on anxiety and
proaches on client perceptions. J Counsel Psychol 7:46-50, 1960. hostility of outpatients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4:381-389, 1961.
31. Ashby JD, Ford DG, Guerney BG Jr, et al: Effects on clients 60. Rickels K, Cattell RB, Weise C, et al: Controlled psycho-
of a reflective and a leading type of psychotherapy. Psychol Mono pharmacological research in private psychiatric practice. Psycho-
71:1-453, 1957. pharmacologia 9:288-306, 1966.
32. Cooper JE, Gelder MG, Marks IM: Results of behaviour 61. Daneman EA: Imipramine in office management of depres-
therapy in 77 psychiatric patients. Br Med J 1:1222-1225, 1965. sive reactions (a double-blind study). Dis Nerv Syst 22:213-217,
33. Cooper JE: A study of behaviour therapy in 30 psychiatric 1961.
patients. Lancet 1:411-415, 1963. 62. Hesbacher PT, Rickels K, Hutchinson J, et al: Setting, pa-
34. Gelder MG, Marks IM: Severe agoraphobia: A controlled tient, and doctor effects on drug response in neurotic patients: II.
prospective trial of behaviour therapy. Br J Psychiatry 112:309- Differential improvement. Psychopharmacologia 18:209-226, 1970.
319, 1966. 63. Podobnikar IG: Implementation of psychotherapy by Libri-
35. Crighton J, Jehu D: Treatment of examination anxiety by um in a pioneering rural-industrial psychiatric practice. Psychoso-
systematic desensitization or psychotherapy in groups. Behav Res matics 12:205-209, 1971.
Ther 7:245-248, 1969. 64. Karon BP, Vandenbos GR: Experience, medication and the
36. King GF, Armitage SG, Tilton JR: A therapeutic approach effectiveness of psychotherapy with schizophrenics. Br J Psychia-
to schizophrenics of extreme pathology: An operant-interpersonal try 116:427-428, 1970.
method. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 61:276-286, 1960. 65. Brown DG, Bettley FR: Psychiatric treatment of eczema: A
37. Lazarus AA: Group therapy of phobic disorders by system- controlled trial. Br Med J 2:729-734, 1971.
atic desensitization. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 63:504-510, 1961. 66. Chappell MN, Stevenson TI: Group psychological training in
38. Levis DJ, Carrera R: Effects of ten hours of implosive ther- some organic conditions. Ment Hyg 20:588-597, 1936.
apy in the treatment of outpatients: A preliminary report. J Ab- 67. Grace WJ, Pinsky RH, Wolff HG: The treatment of ulcera-
norm Psychol 76:504-508, 1967. tive colitis. Gastroenterology 26:462-468, 1954.
39. McReynolds WT: Systematic Desensitization, Insight-Ori- 68. Glen AIM: Psychotherapy and medical treatment for duode-
ented Psychotherapy and Relaxation Therapy in a Psychiatric nal ulcer compared using the augmented histamine test. J Psycho-
Population, thesis. University of Texas, Austin, 1969. som Res 12:163-169, 1968.
40. Marks IM, Gelder MG: A controlled retrospective study of 69. Groen J, Pelser HE: Experiences with, and results of group
behaviour therapy in phobic patients. Br J Psychiatry 111:561-573, psychotherapy in patients with bronchial asthma. J Psychosom
1965. Res 4:191-205, 1960.
41. Patterson V, Levene H, Berger L: Treatment and training 70. Maher-Loughnan GP, MacDonald N, Mason AA, et al: Con-
outcomes with two time-limited therapies. Arch Gen Psychiatry trolled trial of hypnosis in the symptomatic treatment of asthma.
25:161-167, 1971. Br Med J 2:371-376, 1962.
42. Gorham DR, Pokorny AD, Moseley EC, et al: Effects of a 71. O'Connor JF, Daniels G, Flood C, et al: An evaluation of the
phenothiazine and/or group psychotherapy with schizophrenics. effectiveness of psychotherapy in the treatment of ulcerative co-
Dis Nerv Syst 25:77-86, 1964. litis. Ann Intern Med 60:587-602, 1964.
43. May PRA, Tuma AH: The effect of psychotherapy and stela- 72. Sinclair-Gieban AGC, Chalmers D: Evaluation of treatment
zine on length of hospital stay, release rate and supplemental of warts by hypnosis. Lancet 2:480-482, 1959.
treatment of schizophrenic patients. J Nerv Ment Dis 139:362-369, 73. Titchener JL, Sheldon MB, Rose WD: Changes in blood
1964. pressure of hypertensive patients with and without group ther-
44. May PRA, Tuma AH: Treatment of schizophrenia. Br J apy. J Psychosom Res 4:10-12, 1959.
Psychiatry 3:503-510, 1965. 74. Zhukov IA: Hypnotherapy of dermatoses in resort treat-
45. Evangelakis MG: De-institutionalization of patients (the ment, in Winn RB (ed): Psychotherapy in the Soviet Union. New
triad of trifluoperazine-group-adjunctive therapy). Dis Nerv Syst York, Philosophical Library, 1961, pp 178-181.
22:26-32, 1961. 75. Morton RB: An experiment in brief psychotherapy. Psychol
46. Lorr M, McNair DM, Weinstein GJ: Early effects of Mono 69:1-386, 1955.
chlordiazepoxide (Librium) used with psychotherapy. J Psychiatr 76. Tucker JE: Group psychotherapy with chronic psychotic
Res 1:257-270, 1963. soiling patients. J Consult Psychol 20:430, 1956.
47. Overall JE, Tupin JP, Investigation of clinical outcome in a 77. Coons WH: Interaction and insight in group psychotherapy.
doctor's choice treatment setting. Dis Nerv Syst 30:305-313,1969. Can J Psychol 11:1-8, 1957.
48. Koegler RR, Brill NQ: Treatment of Psychiatric Out- 78. Jensen MB: Consultation vs. therapy in the psychological
patients. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts Inc, 1967. treatment of NP hospital patients. J Clin Psychol 17:265-268, 1961.
49. Klerman GL, DiMascio A, Weissman M, et al: Treatment of 79. Sheldon A: An evaluation of psychiatric after-care. Br J
Psychiatry 110:662-667, 1964. 92. Boulougouris JC, Marks IM, Marset P: Superiority of flood-
80. Shattan SP, Decamp L, Fujii E, et al: Group treatment of ing (implosion) to desensitization for reducing pathological fear.
conditionally discharged patients in a mental health clinic. Am J Behav Res Ther 9:7-16, 1971.
Psychiatry 122:798-805, 1966. 93. Marks IM, Gelder MG, Edwards G: Hypnosis and desensiti-
81. Stotsky BA, Daston PG, Vardack CN: An evaluation of the zation for phobias: A controlled prospective trial. Br J Psychiatry
counseling of chronic schizophrenics. J Counsel Psychol 2:248-255, 114:1263-1274, 1968.
1955. 94. Morganstern K: Implosive therapy and flooding procedures:
82. Peyman DAR: An investigation of the effects of group psy- A critical review. Psychol Bull 79:318-334, 1973.
chotherapy on chronic schizophrenic patients. Group Psychother 95. May PRA: Treatment of Schizophrenia. New York, Science
9:35-39, 1956. House, 1968.
83. MacDonald WS, Blochberger CW, Maynard HM: Group 96. Uhlenhuth EH, Lipman R, Covi L: Combined pharmacother-
therapy: A comparison of patient-led and staff-led groups in an apy and psychotherapy: Controlled studies. J Nerv Ment Dis
open hospital ward. Psychiatr Q Suppl 38:290-303, 1964. 148:52-64, 1969.
84. Volsky T Jr, Magoon TM, Norman WT, et al: The Outcomes 97. Frank JD: Persuasion and Healing: A Comparative Study
of Counseling and Psychotherapy, Theory and Research. Min- of Psychotherapy. Baltimore, Johns Hopkins Press, 1965.
neapolis, University of Minnesota Press, 1965. 98. Rosenthal R, Rosnow R (eds): Artifact in Behavioral Re-
85. Brill NQ, Koegler RR, Epstein LJ, et al: Controlled study of search. New York, Academic Press Inc, 1969.
psychiatric outpatient treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 10:581- 99. Penick SB, Filion R, Fox S, et al: Behavior modification in
595, 1964. the treatment of obesity. Psychosom Med 39:49-55, 1971.
86. Endicott NA, Endicott J: Prediction of improvement in 100. Stunkard A: New therapies for the eating disorders: Be-
treated and untreated patients using the Rorschach Prognostic havior modification of obesity and anorexia nervosa. Arch Gen
Rating Scale. J Consult Psychol 28:342-348, 1964. Psychiatry 26:391-398, 1972.
87. Walker RG, Kelley FE: Short-term psychotherapy with hos¬ 101. Dudek SZ: Effects of different types of therapy on the per-
pitalized schizophrenic patients. Acta Psychiatr Neurol Scand sonality as a whole. J Nerv Ment Dis 150:329-345, 1970.
35:34-55, 1960. 102. Malan D: The outcome problem in psychotherapy research.
88. Rogers DR, Dymond RF (eds): Psychotherapy and Person¬ Arch Gen Psychiatry 29:719-729, 1973.
ality Change. Chicago, University of Chicago Press, 1954. 103. Messick S: The criterion problem in the evaluation of in-
89. Rogers CR, Gendlin E, Kiesler D, et al(eds): The Therapeu¬ struction: Assessing possible not just intended, outcomes, in Witt-
tic Relationship and Its Impact: A Study of Psychotherapy With rock MC, Wiley D (eds): The Evaluation of Instruction: Issues
Schizophrenics. Madison,O:University of Wisconsin Press, 1967. and Problems. New York, Holt Rinehart & Winston Inc, 1970.
90. Hoon T, Lindsley A comparison of behavior and tradi¬ 104. Luborsky L: Research cannot yet influence clinical prac-
tional therapy publication activity. Am Psychol 29:694-697, 1974. tice. Int J Psychiatry 7:135-140, 1969.
91. Ellis ÁÉ: Outcome of employing three techniques of psycho¬ 105. Strupp HH: Toward a reformulation of the psychother-
therapy. J Clin Psychol 13:344-350, 1957. apeutic influence. Int J Psychiatry, to be published.

CORRECTION
Reprints Available; Word Omitted.\p=m-\Twoerrors occurred in
the article "Narcissism and the Readiness for Psychotherapy
Termination," published in the June ARCHIVES (32:695-699,
1975). On page 695, the last footnote (column 1) should read
"Reprintrequeststo30NMichiganAve,Chicago,IL60602(Dr
Goldberg)." And on page 696, in column 1, the second sentence
in the paragraph preceding the centerhead should read "They
are not in analysis. ." As published, the word "not" was
..

omitted.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen