Sie sind auf Seite 1von 128

California

Infant/Toddler

Learning & Development

Foundations

CALIFORNIA DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION • SACRAMENTO, 2009

California

Infant/Toddler

Learning & Development

Foundations

California Department of Education

Sacramento, 2009

Publishing Information
The California Infant/Toddler Learning and Development Founda­
tions was developed by the Child Development Division, California
Department of Education. It was edited by Faye Ong, working in
cooperation with Tom Cole, Consultant, Quality Improvement
Office. It was prepared for printing by the staff of CDE Press: the
cover and interior design were created and prepared by Juan D.
Sanchez; typesetting was done by Jeannette Reyes. It was
published by the Department, 1430 N Street, Sacramento, CA
95814-5901. It was distributed under the provisions of the Library
Distribution Act and Government Code Section 11096.

© 2009 by the California Department of Education


All rights reserved

ISBN 978-0-8011-1693-3

Notice
The guidance in the California Infant/Toddler Learning and
Development Foundations is not binding on local educational
agencies or other entities. Except for the statutes, regulations,
and court decisions that are referenced herein, the document is
exemplary, and compliance with it is not mandatory. (See Educa­
tion Code Section 33308.5.)
Contents

A Message from the State Superintendent

of Public Instruction

Acknowledgments

vii

Introduction

ix

The Early Months


1

Social-Emotional Development
7

Language Development
43

Cognitive Development
59

Perceptual and Motor Development


89

Appendix: Summary of
Infant/Toddler Foundations
103

iii
A Message from the

State Superintendent of Public

Instruction

I
am delighted to present the care, which is critically important for
California Infant/Toddler Learning children’s social-emotional well-being
and Development Foundations, a and overall learning. With a goal of
publication I believe will contribute to ensuring that all infant/toddler pro­
providing high-quality care and educa­ grams in California offer high-quality
tion for our youngest children. care, the California Department of
The first three years are a crucial Education collaborated with lead­
time of development. Research on ing early childhood educators and
brain development indicates that the researchers to develop these learning
brains of infants and toddlers are and development foundations.
twice as active as those of adults. By The foundations focus on four
the time children reach the age of domains: social-emotional develop­
three, they have become competent in ment, language development, cognitive
at least one language, formed a sense development, and perceptual and
of self, learned about basic concepts motor development. The foundations
such as cause-and-effect and quantity, provide a comprehensive understand­
and developed numerous large- and ing of young children’s learning and
small-muscle skills. development during the first three
More than half of California’s years of life.
infants and toddlers are cared for in It is my hope that these foundations
child care centers, in family child care will help all California infant/toddler
homes, and by relatives or neighbors programs to offer developmentally
outside the home. Research shows that appropriate and supportive care for
good care and education contribute to our youngest children. By fostering the
children’s social-emotional, language, learning and development described
cognitive, and perceptual and motor in this publication, infant/toddler
development. High-quality infant/tod­ care professionals will contribute to
dler programs provide children with children’s well-being and lay the foun­
caring relationships, environments, dation for children’s future success.
and materials that enrich learning
and development. Those programs also
develop partnerships with families to
connect children’s home experiences JACK O’CONNELL
with experiences in the infant/toddler State Superintendent of Public Instruction
setting. Partnerships with families are
the cornerstone of culturally sensitive


Acknowledgments

T
he following people contributed Katie Monahan, Program Assistant
to this publication or helped to Teresa Ragsdale, Program Assistant
develop the ideas:
University of California, Berkeley
Panel of Experts Berkeley Evaluation and
Marc Bornstein, National Institute of Assessment Research Center
Child Health and Human Development Stephen Moore, Center Associate
Linda Brault, Sonoma State University Director
Deborrah Bremond, Alameda County Mark Wilson, Center Director;
Children and Families Commission Professor, UC Berkeley
Vera Guttierez-Clellan, San Diego State
University California Department
Christopher Lonigan, Florida State of Education
University
Tammy Mann, Zero to Three Meredith Cathcart, Consultant,
Lucia Palacios, Los Angeles Universal Special Education Division
Preschool Tom Cole, Consultant,
Jeree Pawl, Clinical Psychologist Child Development Division
Todd Risley, University of Alaska Cecelia Fisher-Dahms, Administrator,
Ross Thompson, University of Califor­ Quality Improvement Office
nia, Davis Michael Jett, Former Director,
Marlene Zepeda, California State Child Development Division
University, Los Angeles Camille Maben, Director,
Child Development Division
Anthony Monreal, Deputy Super­
WestEd, Center for Child
intendent, Curriculum and
and Family Studies
Instruction Branch
Content development: Mary Smithberger, Consultant,
Ron Lally, Program Codirector Child Development Division
Peter Mangione, Program Codirector Gwen Stephens, Former Assistant
Charlotte Tilson, Senior Program Director, Child Development
Associate Division
Cathy Tsao, Senior Program Associate Maria Trejo, Administrator,
Sara Webb-Schmitz, Program Associate Child Development Division
Osnat Zur, Senior Program Associate
Research assistance: Note: The names and affiliations of individuals were
Amy Schustz-Alvarez, Program current at the time of the development of this publi­
Assistant cation.

ii
Introduction

T
he California Infant/Toddler children’s experiences at home with
Learning and Development their experiences in the infant/tod­
Foundations represents part of dler program. These partnerships with
the Califor nia Department of Educa­ families are the cornerstone of cultur­
tion’s (CDE’s) comprehensive effort to ally sensitive care. Connections with
strengthen young children’s learning children’s early cultural and linguistic
and development through high-quality experiences are critically important
early care and education. The founda­ for their social-emotional well-being,
tions describe competencies infants the development of their identity, and
and toddlers typically attain during the learning. In addition, children may
birth-to-three-year period. In order to have a special need that requires par­
make developmental progress, young ticular accommodations and adapta­
children need appropriate nurturing. tions. To serve all children, infant/tod­
Both supportive home environments dler programs must work to provide
and high-quality early care and educa­ appropriate conditions for each child
tion programs can facilitate children’s and individually assist each child’s
attainment of the competencies speci­ movement along a pathway of healthy
fied in the foundations by providing learning and development.
safe environments and an emotionally Over 20 states have either developed
secure base for active, playful explora­ infant/toddler standards documents or
tion and experimentation. are in the process of doing so. Many of
During the infant/toddler years, all them have sought to align infant/tod­
children depend on responsive, secure dler standards with preschool learning
relationships to develop and learn. standards. Because both infant/tod­
As stated in the CDE’s Infant/Toddler dler and preschool foundations in Cali­
Learning and Development Program fornia cover a broad range of learn­
Guidelines (2007), high-quality pro­ ing and development domains, the
grams offer infants and toddlers pri­ term foundations is used rather than
mary relationships in small groups. standards. This term was selected to
Such programs provide personalized convey that learning across all devel­
care that reflects consideration for opmental domains builds young chil­
individual differences among children. dren’s readiness for school. In essence,
Programs also develop partnerships the foundations pertain to young chil­
with children’s families to connect dren’s current and long-term develop­

ix
x

mental progress. This focus is conso­ The foundations developed for


nant with the position of the National each of these domains are based on
Association for the Education of Young research and evidence from practice.
Children (NAEYC) and the National Suggestions of expert practitioners and
Association of Early Childhood Special­ examples illustrate the foundations.
ists in State Departments of Education The purpose is to promote understand­
(NAECS/SDE) on early learning stan­ ing of early learning and development
dards. As the position statement sets and guide efforts to support the devel­
forth, “Early childhood is a distinct opment and well-being of infants and
period of life that has value in itself toddlers.
as well as creating the foundations for
later years” (NAEYC and NAECS/SDE Overview of the Foundations
position statement 2002, 3). The foundations for each of the four
In California, priority has been domains are listed in this section.
placed on aligning the infant/toddler
learning and development foundations Social-Emotional Development
with the preschool learning founda­ Domain. The social-emotional devel­
tions in four major domains: opment domain covers the following
foundations:
• Social-emotional development
• Interactions with Adults: The
• Language development child’s developing ability to
• Cognitive development respond to social cues from
• Perceptual and motor develop­ adults and engage in back-and­
ment forth social exchanges with
adults
The domains represent crucial areas
of early learning and development that • Relationships with Adults: The
contribute to young children’s readi­ child’s development of close rela­
ness for school (National Research tionships with adults who provide
Council and Institute of Medicine consistent nurturance
2000; NAEYC and NAESC/SDE 2002). • Interactions with Peers: The
The foundations present key concepts child’s developing ability to
in each domain and provide an over­ respond to social overtures from
view of development in that domain. peers, engage in back-and-forth
Young children can be considered from interaction with other children,
the perspective of one domain, such as and, ultimately, to engage in
social-emotional development or lan­ cooperative play with other chil­
guage development. Yet, when taking dren.
an in-depth look at a single domain, • Relationships with Peers: The
one needs to keep in mind that learn­ child’s development of relation­
ing, for young children, is usually an ships with certain peers through
integrated experience. For example, an interactions over time
infant may make a cognitive discovery
about cause-and-effect while making • Identity of Self in Relation to Oth­
the connection that a cry leads to a ers: The child’s developing con­
comforting response from an adult. cept of self as an individual who
xi

operates within social relation­ Language Development Domain.


ships The language development foundations
• Recognition of Ability: The child’s cover the following competencies:
developing understanding of the • Receptive Language: The child’s
ability to take action to influence developing ability to understand
the immediate social and physi­ words and increasingly complex
cal environments utterances
• Expression of Emotion: The child’s • Expressive Language: The child’s
developing ability to communi­ developing ability to produce the
cate various emotions through sounds of language, and speak
facial expressions, movements, with an increasingly expansive
gestures, sounds, or words vocabulary and use increasingly
• Empathy: The child’s developing complex utterances
ability to share in the emotional • Communication Skills and Knowl­
experiences of others edge: The child’s developing abil­
• Emotion Regulation: The child’s ity to communicate nonverbally
developing ability to manage or and verbally
regulate emotional responses • Interest in Print: The child’s devel­
with and without assistance from oping interest in engaging with
adults print in books and in the environ­
• Impulse Control: The child’s devel­ ment
oping capacity to wait for needs Many early childhood experts con­
to be met, to inhibit behavior, sider language development to be one
and to act according to social of the greatest accomplishments in
expectations, including safety the first three years of life. There are
rules many specific milestones and dimen­
• Social Understanding: The child’s sions of language development, such
developing understanding of the as phonology and syntax. As to prac­
responses, communication, emo­ tice, the four foundations provide a
tional expressions, and actions of level of detail that is accessible to fami­
other people lies and infant care teachers seeking
to enhance children’s early language
The many competencies covered development and communication.
by the social-emotional development
foundations underscore the promi­ Cognitive Development Domain.
nence of this domain during the first The following foundations make up the
three years of life. The emotional secu­ cognitive development domain:
rity that infants seek to develop with • Cause-and-Effect: The child’s
others and their ability to interact developing understanding that
effectively with both adults and other one event or action brings about
children support their learning and another
development in all domains. • Spatial Relationships: The child’s
developing understanding of how
things move and fit in space
xii

• Problem Solving: The child’s devel­ tencies become differentiated and can
oping ability to engage in a pur­ be aligned with traditional preschool
poseful effort to reach a goal or to content domains such as mathematics
determine how something works and science. In effect, infants’ and tod­
• Imitation: The child’s developing dlers’ playful exploration and experi­
capacity to mirror, repeat, and mentation in the cognitive domain
practice the actions of others, represent an early manifestation of
either immediately or at a later mathematical and scientific reasoning
time and problem solving.

• Memory: The child’s developing Perceptual and Motor Develop­


ability to store and later retrieve ment Domain. Infants’ and toddlers’
information. perceptual and motor competencies
are receiving increasing attention in
• Number Sense: The child’s devel­ research and practice. The perceptual
oping understanding of number and motor development foundations
or quantity are defined as follows:
• Classification: The child’s devel­ • Perceptual Development: The
oping ability to group, sort, cat­ child’s developing ability to
egorize, and form expectations become aware of the immediate
based on the attributes of objects social and physical environments
and people through the senses
• Symbolic Play: The child’s devel­ • Gross Motor: The child’s develop­
oping ability to use actions, ing ability to move and coordinate
objects, or ideas to represent large muscles
other actions, objects, or ideas
• Fine Motor: The child’s developing
• Attention Maintenance: The ability to move and coordinate
child’s developing ability to small muscles
attend to people and things while
interacting with others or explor­ Infant/toddler programs can foster
ing the environment and play children’s perceptual and motor learn­
materials ing and development through environ­
ments that offer safe and appropriate
• Understanding of Personal Care physical challenges.
Routines: The child’s developing
ability to understand personal Organization of the
care routines and participate in
Foundations
them
The publication begins with a
As the above list suggests, the foun­
chapter that focuses on the first four
dations for the cognitive development
months of life. Separate foundations
domain cover a broad range of knowl­
in each domain were not written for
edge and skills. For infants and tod­
the first four months because every
dlers, these various competencies are
aspect of early development relates to
interwoven and develop together. As
all domains simultaneously. Although
children move out of the birth-to-three
development during the first four
period, some of the cognitive compe­
months is undifferentiated, it has a
xiii

profound influence on subsequent DRDP to the infant/toddler founda­


development in every domain. The tions is complete, the DRDP will pro­
chapter on the early months highlights vide additional detail about the devel­
the inborn behaviors that enable chil­ opmental progression of a foundation.
dren to orient toward adults and begin For each foundation at each of the
to communicate needs. At the same three age levels, broad information
time, the chapter describes how, right on infant development summarizes
from the beginning of life, children are children’s competencies. Together, the
“active participants in their own devel­ three descriptions define the devel­
opment, reflecting the intrinsic human opmental progression of a founda­
drive to explore and master one’s envi­ tion. Underneath each description are
ronment” (National Research Council examples of possible ways that chil­
and Institute of Medicine 2000, 1). dren may demonstrate a foundation
For each of the 28 foundations, in a particular age range. The diver­
a description is specified at three sity of examples gives a sense of the
points of development: at around eight variation among infants and toddlers.
months of age, at around 18 months A foundation for a particular child
of age, and at around 36 months of should be considered on the basis of
age. In addition, behaviors are listed how the child functions in different
that lead to the level of competency contexts—at home, in child care, and
described for each of those three age in the community. An individual child
levels. The behaviors leading up to an may not function like any of the exam­
age level reflect the ongoing change ples listed under a foundation, yet she
that occurs during each age period. may already be able to demonstrate
At around eight months of age, 18 the level of competency described by
months of age, and 36 months of age, that foundation. The examples suggest
children move to a different way of the varieties of contexts in which chil­
functioning and have different devel­ dren may show competencies reflected
opmental needs. For most founda­ in the foundations. Infant care teach­
tions, the change from one age level ers often think of alternative examples
(from eight months to 18 months or when they reflect on how a particular
from 18 months old to 36 months) is foundation applies to the young chil­
quite pronounced. The foundations dren in their care.
are designed to give a general sense
of development at these three points Guiding Principles
along the developmental continuum. Several guiding principles influenced
The subtleties of individual children’s the creation of the infant/toddler
developmental progress at any given learning and development foundations.
time are presented in the CDE’s These principles stem from both devel­
Desired Results Developmental Profile opmental theory and research and
(DRDP) (2005). This teacher observa­ from best practice in the infant/toddler
tion tool for infants and toddlers shows care field.
five or six developmental levels span­
1. The family and its culture and
ning the birth-to-three age range for
language play a central role in
outcomes that will be aligned to the
early learning and development.
foundations. When alignment of the
xi

2. Infancy is a unique stage of life (more differentiated behavior).


that is important in its own right. An example of integration is that
Development in infancy can be older children may be able to
described by three age periods— engage in several behaviors such
birth to eight months, eight as talking, walking, and carrying
months to 18 months, and 18 an object simultaneously (more
months to 36 months. Each age integrated behavior), whereas
period is distinct, although there younger children may need to
is often overlap from one to the focus all of their energies on
next. doing one behavior at a time (less
3. Infants and toddlers are compe­ integrated behavior).
tent yet vulnerable at every stage Those principles apply to the foun­
of development. Nurturing rela­ dations, curriculum planning, and
tionships provide the foundation assessment practices aligned to the
for emotional security and opti­ foundations.
mal learning and development.
4. Emotions drive early learning. Universal Design for Learning
Infants and toddlers are active, These foundations support infant/
curious learners who are inter­ toddler programs in the effort to foster
nally driven to interact with the learning and development of all
social and physical environ­ young children in California, including
ments. Infants and toddlers learn children with disabilities or other spe­
in a holistic way rather than one cial needs. In some cases, infants and
domain at a time. toddlers with disabilities or other spe­
5. Early development includes cial needs will reveal their developmen­
both quantitative and qualitative tal progress in alternative ways. It is
change. With quantitative shifts, important to provide opportunities for
the infant extends or adds com­ children to follow different pathways to
petencies to similar existing com­ learning. Therefore, the infant/toddler
petencies. With qualitative shifts, learning and development foundations
the infant combines new knowl­ incorporate a concept known as uni­
edge and abilities with existing versal design for learning.
knowledge and abilities to func­ Developed by the Center for Applied
tion in a different and more com­ Special Technology (CAST), universal
plex way. design for learning is based on the
realization that children learn in dif­
6. Early development reflects an ferent ways. In today’s diverse infant/
interplay of differentiation and toddler programs, making the environ­
integration. For example, young ment, play materials, activities, and
infants typically use their mouths experiences accessible to all children is
to explore all objects to learn critical to successful learning. Univer­
about them (less differentiated sal design is not a single approach that
behavior), whereas older children will accommodate everyone; rather, it
mainly use their mouth to taste refers to providing multiple approaches
or explore different kinds of food to learning in order to meet the needs
x

of diverse learners. Universal design and development system. The founda­


provides for multiple means of repre­ tions describe how children develop
sentation, multiple means of engage­ and what they learn and are designed
ment, and multiple means of expres­ to illuminate the competencies that
sion (CAST 2007). “Multiple means infants and toddlers need for later suc­
of representation” refers to providing cess. Together the components of the
information in a variety of ways so the infant/toddler learning and develop­
learning needs of all children are met. ment system provide information and
“Multiple means of expression” refers resources to help early childhood pro­
to allowing children to use alternative fessionals support infants, toddlers,
ways to communicate or demonstrate and their families.
what they know or what they are feel­ • In the Infant/Toddler Learning
ing. “Multiple means of engagement” and Development Program Guide­
refers to providing choices within the lines there are recommendations
setting or program that facilitate learn­ for setting up environments,
ing by building on children’s interests. providing infants a secure base
The examples in the infant/toddler for learning and exploration,
learning and development foundations selecting appropriate materials,
have been worded to portray multiple and planning and implementing
means of representation, expression, learning opportunities.
and engagement. A variety of examples
are provided for each foundation, and • The Infant/Toddler Desired
inclusive words are used to describe Results Developmental Profile
children’s behavior. For example, (described earlier in this chapter)
rather than stating “The child looks is an observational assessment
at an object” or “The child listens to a instrument that allows teachers
person,” the more inclusive wording of to document individual children’s
“A child attends to an object” or “The developmental progress.
child attends to a person” is used. • The infant/toddler curriculum
When reading each foundation, an framework will provide general
infant care teacher needs to consider guidance on the kinds of environ­
the means by which a child with a dis­ ments and interactions that sup­
ability or other special need might best port learning and development.
acquire information and act compe­ • The Program for Infant/Tod­
tently. To best meet a child’s needs, a dler Care is a comprehensive
parent and an early intervention spe­ approach to professional develop­
cialist or related service provider are ment that provides infant/toddler
vitally important resources. professionals with opportuni­
ties to become informed about
The Foundations and Infant/ the infant/toddler learning and
Toddler Care and Education development foundations and
in California other components of California’s
The CDE’s learning and develop­ infant/toddler system.
ment foundations are at the center of As a unifying element of California’s
California’s infant/toddler learning infant/toddler learning and develop­
xi

ment system, the foundations offer a Program for Infant/Toddler Care (the
common language for infant/toddler comprehensive approach to training
program directors, teachers, and fami­ collaboratively developed by the CDE
lies to reflect on children’s develop­ and WestEd). The foundations can
mental progress and plan experiences become a unifying element for both
that support children’s learning and preservice and in-service professional
development during the first three development efforts. For infant/tod­
years of life. dler programs, directors and teachers
Professional development is another can use the foundations as a basis
key component in fostering infant/tod­ to reflect on children’s developmental
dler learning and development. Pro­ progress and to plan experiences that
fessionals now have opportunities to support children’s learning and devel­
become informed: through the infant/ opment from birth to three years. The
toddler learning and development foundations are designed to provide
foundations, the CDE’s Infant/Toddler infant care teachers with knowledge
Learning and Development Program of the competencies necessary during
Guidelines, the CDE’s Desired Results the first three years of a child’s life and
Developmental Profile (DRDP), and the later on in preschool and school.
xii

References

California Department of Education (CDE). Standards: Creating the Conditions for


2007. Infant/Toddler Learning and Success. Washington, DC: National
Development Program Guidelines. Sacra­ Association for the Education of Young
mento: CDE Press. Children.
Center for Applied Special Technology National Research Council and Institute of
(CAST). 2007. Universal Design for Medicine. 2000. From Neurons to Neigh­
Learning. http://www.cast.org/udl/ borhoods: The Science of Early Childhood
(accessed June 8, 2007). Development. Committee on Integrating
National Association for the Education the Science of Early Childhood Develop­
of Young Children (NAEYC) and Asso­ ment. Edited by J. Shonkoff and D. Phil­
ciation of Early Childhood Specialists lips. Washington, DC: National Academy
in State Departments of Education Press.
(NAECS/SDE). 2002. Early Learning
The Early Months

W ith regard to very young infants,


Magda Gerber commented:
2000, 1). Their active engagement
with the social and physical world
works hand in hand with the care they
Everything they see, they hear, they
feel, they touch is new. . . . They are receive from adults, especially when
adapting to all that newness, adapt­ the adults are responsive to them.
ing to their inner physiological needs,
which are plenty. . . . A very young The Newborn
baby is busy being a very young baby. From birth babies learn to connect
(Respectfully Yours 1988, 5)
internal sensory experiences to move­
During the first four months of life, ments of their bodies. They repeatedly
babies begin to engage the world and attend to sensory experiences and
the people in it (Advances in Applied explore movements they can make. In
Developmental Psychology 1995). doing so, they make discoveries about
Infants’ motivation to explore and their bodies—how to use their head,
communicate drives them to move eyes, mouth, arms, and legs. Young
their bodies, focus their attention, and babies also use their senses to learn
send and receive signals—the basis about people and things.
for development and learning in all Much of the earliest learning of typi­
domains. These early behaviors mark cally developing infants comes through
the start of a child’s developmental their use of vision. Even very young
progress (Emde 1990). babies watch their mothers’ and other
Young babies seek relationships and adults’ faces intently, and what they
build knowledge. They actively explore see influences their behavior (Schore
what they can do with their bodies, 1994). Babies also seek eye-to-eye
people close to them, and the envi­ connection with adults. They use their
ronment. They are not empty vessels eyes to both send messages and to
waiting to be filled with information, gain information. In the first months
but rather “active participants in their of life, babies are aroused by social
own development, reflecting the intrin­ engagement and quieted by mutual
sic human drive to explore and master gazing experiences (Stern 1977).
one’s environment” (National Research Both the arousal and the calming
Council and Institute of Medicine positively affect the development of the




child’s brain and stimulate the onset signs of pleasure and (2) positive social
of self-regulation (Emde 1988). Read­ responses to people, including the
ing and understanding babies’ gazes above-mentioned smiling, increased
and showing interest and warmth by vocalizations, and bodily activity.
gazing in return benefit the children
greatly. The Three- to Four-Month-
THE EARLY MONTHS

Babies develop quickly by extending Old Infant


their abilities in all domains and by By three months of age, pathways
creating more complex ways of relat­ of hearing and sight are actively shap­
ing to people and things. They send ing in the brain. The developing brain
messages to adults in various ways adapts to the messages it receives from
and come to expect responses from the eyes and the ears by either prun­
adults. For example, when looking into ing (weakening) or making robust
the faces of adults, infants may see synaptic connections for future func­
enlarged dilated pupils—a common tioning.
sign of interest and pleasure—and, in Pruning of some synaptic connec­
response, they smile more (Hess 1975). tions and strengthening of others can
Spitz and Wolfe (1946) observed that be seen in early language development.
newborns exhibit three distinct reac­ Newborns are responsive to the sounds
tions to internal and external stimuli: of all human languages. By the age
• Quiescence (a calm state) of three to four months, babies are
• Undifferentiated excitement (a increasingly sensitive and alert to the
general response to pleasurable sounds of the language(s) spoken by
stimuli) adults who care for them and become
less attuned to the sounds of other
• “Unpleasure” languages.
By the end of the first month the As infants approach four months,
“unpleasure” differentiates or branches they become increasingly skilled at
out into signs of displeasure and signs using a variety of ways to understand
of distress.1 One-month-olds show and relate to the world around them.
signs of displeasure when they dislike They create basic categories, such
an experience and signs of distress as things that move and things that
when they experience discomfort or do not (Mandler 2004), and start to
pain. By the second or third month, treat things differently according to
undifferentiated excitement also dif­ attributes such as “hard,” “soft,” or
ferentiates or branches out into two “sticky.” For example, they may change
types of distinct responses: (1) clear how they hold and grasp things based
on the attributes of the objects held.
1
When a reference is made to a specific age, the Young infants also try to prolong inter­
following qualifying statements always apply: The ref­
esting experiences; then, after doing
erence is to developmental age, rather than chrono­
logical age. The phrase “at around” always either the same activity for a while and mas­
explicitly or implicitly precedes the stated age, to tering it, they experiment in search of
recognize variation in individual development. novelty.
A behavior stated for an age is not strictly based Children in the three- to four-month
on maturation, but also stems from experience and range also show highly differentiated
practice.


social-emotional behavior. By three Interpreting and Responding


months, babies have already learned to to Early Development
alter their responses to adults accord­
In the first few months, amazing
ing to how the adults respond to them.
advances occur in infants’ develop­
For example, when adults acknowledge
ment. Starting with basic responses,
babies’ vocalizations with a smile, a

THE EARLY MONTHS


newborns reach out to the world.
vocal response, or light touch, children
Within weeks they come to expect and
increase their vocalizations. The inter­
depend on appropriate responses from
est of others stimulates the interest of
those who care for them.
the baby (Crick 1984).
Noticing and responding to key
The power children have in relation­
aspects of growth during this rapid
ships, by four months of age, is clearly
developmental period can be chal­
evident, as is the power that relation­
lenging. Early behaviors in one devel­
ships have over them. They are becom­
opmental domain are often coupled
ing more skilled at reading others’
with behaviors in other domains. In
behavior and adapting their own
addition, many early behaviors may
behavior. They are also gaining skills
mean several things at the same time.
to make themselves increasingly
For example, a young baby’s cry may
engaging and effective socially. Four­
simultaneously represent the begin­
month-olds will:
ning of communication (language
• Send clear messages. development), a tool for getting needs
• Become quiet in anticipation as met (intellectual development), and
someone comes near to care for a way of relating to others (social­
them. emotional development). Or behaviors
that look almost identical may have a
• Seek adults’ attention with smiles
different meaning at different times.
and laughter.
For example, at one time prolonged
• Participate in extended back and focus on a person may be an emerg­
forth interaction with others. ing strategy to deepen emotional con­
• Engage in simple social imitation. nections. At another time, this same
behavior may be a way to increase
Emotionally, infants grow just as
understanding of how people move
rapidly as they grow socially. Com­
in space. To be in tune with young
pared with younger babies, babies at
babies, adults need to know both when
around four months send clearer emo­
a baby wants a social response and
tional messages through varying cries,
when a baby is making a discovery
movements, and facial expressions.
through individual exploration and
They also show pleasure when master­
observation.
ing simple motor tasks such as when
Because major changes that occur
they successfully position their bod­
during the first few months of life are
ies for exploration. Positive emotional
sometimes difficult to identify, one can
experiences motivate infants to keep
easily miss them. Yet the advances of
practicing new skills, exploring new
the early months are just as important
possibilities, and learning.
to a young child’s healthy development
as are the more obvious advances of


the eight-month-old and older infant. an incredible gift. Adults communicate


When adults understand the sucking, that the path the baby is progressively
clinging, body position, smiling, cry­ moving along is understood and sup­
ing, and gazes of the young baby, they ported. This communication lays the
are better able to respond to the baby’s foundation for the young baby’s emerg­
needs. ing emotional security and attachment
THE EARLY MONTHS

By recognizing and giving appropri­ relationships, which are essential for


ate responses to a baby’s early devel­ all learning and development through­
opmental achievements, adults offer out the early years.


References

Advances in Applied Developmental Psy­ hood Development. Committee on Inte­


chology: Mastery Motivation: Origins, grating the Science of Early Childhood.

THE EARLY MONTHS


Conceptualizations, and Applications Edited by J. Shonkoff and D. Phillips.
(Vol. 12). 1995. Edited by R. H. Mac- Washington, DC: National Academies
Turk and G. A. Morgan. Norwood, NJ: Press.
Greenwood Publishing Group. Respectfully Yours: Magda Gerber’s
Crick, F. 1984. “Function of the Thalamic Approach to Professional Infant/Toddler
Reticular Complex: The Searchlight Care (Video magazine). 1988. Sacra­
Hypothesis,” Proceedings of the National mento: California Department of Edu­
Academy of Sciences of the United States cation in collaboration with WestEd
of America, Vol. 81, 4586–90. Center for Child and Family Studies.
Emde, R. N. 1988. “Development Termi­ Schore, A. N. 1994. The Neurobiolgy of
nable and Interminable. I. Innate and Emotional Development. Hillsdale, NJ:
Motivational Factors from Infancy,” Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
International Journal of Psychoanalysis, Spitz, R. A., and K. M. Wolfe. 1946. “The
Vol. 69, 23–42. Smiling Response: A Contribution to
Hess, E. H. 1975. “The Role of Pupil Size the Ontogenesis of Social Relation,”
in Communication,” Scientific American, Genetic Psychology Monographs, Vol. 34,
Vol. 233,110–19. 57–125.
Mandler, J. M. 2004. The Foundations of Stern, D. N. 1977. The First Relationship:
the Mind: Origins of Conceptual Thought. Infant and Mother. Cambridge, UK: Har­
New York: Oxford University Press. vard University Press.
National Research Council and Institute of
Medicine. 2000. From Neurons to Neigh­
borhoods: The Science of Early Child­
Social-Emotional

Development

S
ocial-emotional development plex social interactions, to participate
includes the child’s experience, effectively in relationships and group
expression, and management of activities, and to reap the benefits
emotions and the ability to establish of social support crucial to healthy
positive and rewarding relationships human development and functioning.
with others (Cohen and others 2005). Healthy social-emotional develop­
It encompasses both intra- and inter­ ment for infants and toddlers unfolds
personal processes. in an interpersonal context, namely
The core features of emotional devel­ that of positive ongoing relationships
opment include the ability to identify with familiar, nurturing adults. Young
and understand one’s own feelings, children are particularly attuned to
to accurately read and comprehend social and emotional stimulation.
emotional states in others, to manage Even newborns appear to attend more
strong emotions and their expression to stimuli that resemble faces (John­
in a constructive manner, to regu­ son and others 1991). They also pre­
late one’s own behavior, to develop fer their mothers’ voices to the voices
empathy for others, and to establish of other women (DeCasper and Fifer
and maintain relationships. (National
1980). Through nurturance, adults
Scientific Council on the Developing
support the infants’ earliest experi­
Child 2004, 2)
ences of emotion regulation (Bronson
Infants experience, express, and per­ 2000a; Thompson and Goodvin 2005).
ceive emotions before they fully under­ Responsive caregiving supports
stand them. In learning to recognize, infants in beginning to regulate their
label, manage, and communicate their emotions and to develop a sense of
emotions and to perceive and attempt predictability, safety, and responsive­
to understand the emotions of others, ness in their social environments.
children build skills that connect them Early relationships are so important to
with family, peers, teachers, and the developing infants that research
community. These growing capacities experts have broadly concluded that,
help young children to become compe­ in the early years, “nurturing, stable
tent in negotiating increasingly com­ and consistent relationships are the




key to healthy growth, development ers 2007). Brain structures involved in


and learning” (National Research the neural circuitry of cognition influ­
Council and Institute of Medicine ence emotion and vice versa (Barrett
2000, 412). In other words, high- and others 2007). Emotions and social
quality relationships increase the behaviors affect the young child’s abil­
likelihood of positive outcomes for ity to persist in goal-oriented activity,
young children (Shonkoff 2004). to seek help when it is needed, and to
Experiences with family members and participate in and benefit from rela­
teachers provide an opportunity for tionships.
young children to learn about social Young children who exhibit healthy
relationships and emotions through social, emotional, and behavioral
exploration and predictable interac­ adjustment are more likely to have
tions. Professionals working in child good academic performance in elemen­
care settings can support the social- tary school (Cohen and others 2005;
emotional development of infants and Zero to Three 2004). The sharp dis­
toddlers in various ways, including tinction between cognition and emo­
interacting directly with young chil­ tion that has historically been made
dren, communicating with families, may be more of an artifact of scholar­
arranging the physical space in the ship than it is representative of the
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

care environment, and planning and way these processes occur in the brain
implementing curriculum. (Barrett and others 2007). This recent
Brain research indicates that emo­ research strengthens the view that
tion and cognition are profoundly early childhood programs support
interrelated processes. Specifically, later positive learning outcomes in all
“recent cognitive neuroscience findings domains by maintaining a focus on
suggest that the neural mechanisms the promotion of healthy social emo­
underlying emotion regulation may be tional development (National Scientific
the same as those underlying cogni­ Council on the Developing Child 2004;
tive processes” (Bell and Wolfe 2004, Raver 2002; Shonkoff 2004).
366). Emotion and cognition work
together, jointly informing the child’s Interactions with Adults
impressions of situations and influ­ Interactions with adults are a fre­
encing behavior. Most learning in the quent and regular part of infants’
early years occurs in the context of daily lives. Infants as young as three
emotional supports (National Research months of age have been shown to be
Council and Institute of Medicine able to discriminate between the faces
2000). “The rich interpenetrations of of unfamiliar adults (Barrera and
emotions and cognitions establish the Maurer 1981). The foundations that
major psychic scripts for each child’s describe Interactions with Adults and
life” (Panksepp 2001). Together, emo­ Relationships with Adults are inter­
tion and cognition contribute to atten­ related. They jointly give a picture of
tional processes, decision making, healthy social-emotional development
and learning (Cacioppo and Berntson that is based in a supportive social
1999). Furthermore, cognitive pro­ environment established by adults.
cesses, such as decision making, are Children develop the ability to both
affected by emotion (Barrett and oth­


respond to adults and engage with adults is related to children’s emo­


them first through predictable interac­ tional security, sense of self, and evolv­
tions in close relationships with par­ ing understanding of the world around
ents or other caring adults at home them. Concepts from the literature on
and outside the home. Children use attachment may be applied to early
and build upon the skills learned childhood settings, in considering the
through close relationships to inter­ infant care teacher’s role in separa­
act with less familiar adults in their tions and reunions during the day in
lives. In interacting with adults, chil­ care, facilitating the child’s explora­
dren engage in a wide variety of social tion, providing comfort, meeting physi­
exchanges such as establishing con­ cal needs, modeling positive relation­
tact with a relative or engaging in sto­ ships, and providing support during
rytelling with an infant care teacher. stressful times (Raikes 1996).
Quality in early childhood programs
is, in large part, a function of the Interactions with Peers
interactions that take place between In early infancy children interact
the adults and children in those with each other using simple behav­
programs. These interactions form iors such as looking at or touching
the basis for the relationships that

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
another child. Infants’ social interac­
are established between teachers and
tions with peers increase in complexity
children in the classroom or home
from engaging in repetitive or routine
and are related to children’s develop­
mental status. How teachers interact back-and-forth interactions with peers
with children is at the very heart of (for example, rolling a ball back and
early childhood education (Kontos forth) to engaging in cooperative activi­
and Wilcox-Herzog 1997, 11). ties such as building a tower of blocks
together or acting out different roles
Relationships with Adults during pretend play. Through inter­
Close relationships with adults actions with peers, infants explore
who provide consistent nurturance their interest in others and learn
strengthen children’s capacity to about social behavior/social interac­
learn and develop. Moreover, relation­ tion. Interactions with peers provide
ships with parents, other family mem­ the context for social learning and
bers, caregivers, and teachers provide problem solving, including the experi­
the key context for infants’ social- ence of social exchanges, cooperation,
emotional development. These special turn-taking, and the demonstration
relationships influence the infant’s of the beginning of empathy. Social
emerging sense of self and under­ interactions with peers also allow older
standing of others. Infants use rela­ infants to experiment with different
tionships with adults in many ways: roles in small groups and in different
for reassurance that they are safe, situations such as relating to familiar
for assistance in alleviating distress, versus unfamiliar children. As noted,
for help with emotion regulation, and the foundations called Interactions
for social approval or encouragement. with Adults, Relationships with Adults,
Establishing close relationships with Interactions with Peers, and Rela­
tionships with Peers are interrelated.
0

Interactions are stepping-stones to an understanding of their roles within


relationships. Burk (1996, 285) writes: their families and communities. They
We, as teachers, need to facilitate the also become aware of their own prefer­
development of a psychologically safe ences and characteristics and those of
environment that promotes posi­ others.
tive social interaction. As children
interact openly with their peers, they Recognition of Ability
learn more about each other as indi­ Infants’ developing sense of self-
viduals, and they begin building a
efficacy includes an emerging under­
history of interactions.
standing that they can make things
happen and that they have particular
Relationships with Peers
abilities. Self-efficacy is related to a
Infants develop close relationships sense of competency, which has been
with children they know over a period identified as a basic human need
of time, such as other children in the (Connell 1990). The development of
family child care setting or neighbor­ children’s sense of self-efficacy may be
hood. Relationships with peers provide seen in play or exploratory behaviors
young children with the opportunity when they act on an object to pro­
to develop strong social connections.
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

duce a result. For example, they pat a


Infants often show a preference for musical toy to make sounds come out.
playing and being with friends, as Older infants may demonstrate recog­
compared with peers with whom they nition of ability through “I” statements,
do not have a relationship. Howes’ such as “I did it” or “I’m good at draw­
(1983) research suggests that there ing.”
are distinctive patterns of friendship
for the infant, toddler, and preschooler Expression of Emotion
age groups. The three groups vary in
Even early in infancy, children
the number of friendships, the stability
express their emotions through facial
of friendships, and the nature of inter­
expressions, vocalizations, and body
action between friends (for example,
language. The later ability to use
the extent to which they involve object
words to express emotions gives young
exchange or verbal communication).
children a valuable tool in gaining the
assistance or social support of oth­
Identity of Self in Relation
ers (Saarni and others 2006). Tem­
to Others
perament may play a role in children’s
Infants’ social-emotional develop­ expression of emotion. Tronick (1989,
ment includes an emerging awareness 112) described how expression of
of self and others. Infants demon­ emotion is related to emotion regula­
strate this foundation in a number of tion and communication between the
ways. For example, they can respond mother and infant: “the emotional
to their names, point to their body expressions of the infant and the care­
parts when asked, or name members taker function to allow them to mutu­
of their families. Through an emerg­ ally regulate their interactions . . . the
ing understanding of other people in infant and the adult are participants
their social environment, children gain in an affective communication system.”


Both the understanding and expres­ emotional factors play a part, seen as a
sion of emotion are influenced by cul­ “reflection and constituent of an inter­
ture. Cultural factors affect children’s active relationship” (Messinger and
growing understanding of the meaning Fogel 2007, 329). It appears likely that
of emotions, the developing knowl­ the experience of positive emotions is
edge of which situations lead to which a particularly important contributor to
emotional outcomes, and their learn­ emotional well-being and psychologi­
ing about which emotions are appro­ cal health (Fredrickson 2000, 2003;
priate to display in which situations Panksepp 2001).
(Thompson and Goodvin 2005). Some
cultural groups appear to express cer­ Empathy
tain emotions more often than other During the first three years of life,
cultural groups (Tsai, Levenson, and children begin to develop the capacity
McCoy 2006). In addition, cultural to experience the emotional or psycho­
groups vary by which particular emo­ logical state of another person (Zahn-
tions or emotional states they value Waxler and Radke-Yarrow 1990).
(Tsai, Knutson, and Fung 2006). One The following definitions of empathy
study suggests that cultural differ­ are found in the research literature:
ences in exposure to particular emo­

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
“knowing what another person is feel­
tions through storybooks may contrib­ ing,” “feeling what another person is
ute to young children’s preferences for feeling,” and “responding compassion­
particular emotional states (for exam­ ately to another’s distress” (Levenson
ple, excited or calm) (Tsai and others and Ruef 1992, 234). The concept of
2007). empathy reflects the social nature of
Young children’s expression of posi­ emotion, as it links the feelings of two
tive and negative emotions may play a or more people (Levenson and Ruef
significant role in their development of 1992). Since human life is relation­
social relationships. Positive emotions ship-based, one vitally important func­
appeal to social partners and seem tion of empathy over the life span is to
to enable relationships to form, while strengthen social bonds (Anderson and
problematic management or expression Keltner 2002). Research has shown a
of negative emotions leads to difficulty correlation between empathy and pro-
in social relationships (Denham and social behavior (Eisenberg 2000). In
Weissberg 2004). The use of emotion- particular, prosocial behaviors, such
related words appears to be associ­ as helping, sharing, and comforting or
ated with how likable preschoolers are showing concern for others, illustrate
considered by their peers. Children the development of empathy (Zahn-
who use emotion-related words were Waxler and others 1992) and how the
found to be better-liked by their class­ experience of empathy is thought to
mates (Fabes and others 2001). Infants be related to the development of moral
respond more positively to adult vocal­ behavior (Eisenberg 2000). Adults
izations that have a positive affective model prosocial/empathic behav­
tone (Fernald 1993). Social smiling is iors for infants in various ways. For
a developmental process in which neu­ example, those behaviors are modeled
rophysiology and cognitive, social, and through caring interactions with oth-


ers or through providing nurturance and Rubin 2003, 3). Adults can pro­
to the infant. Quann and Wien (2006, vide positive role models of emotion
28) suggest that one way to support regulation through their behavior and
the development of empathy in young through the verbal and emotional
children is to create a culture of caring support they offer children in manag­
in the early childhood environment: ing their emotions. Responsiveness
“Helping children understand the feel­ to infants’ signals contributes to the
ings of others is an integral aspect of development of emotion regulation.
the curriculum of living together. The Adults support infants’ development
relationships among teachers, between of emotion regulation by minimizing
children and teachers, and among exposure to excessive stress, chaotic
children are fostered with warm and environments, or over- or understimu­
caring interactions.” lation.
Emotion regulation skills are impor­
Emotion Regulation tant in part because they play a role
The developing ability to regulate in how well children are liked by peers
emotions has received increasing and teachers and how socially compe­
attention in the research literature tent they are perceived to be (National
Scientific Council on the Develop­
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

(Eisenberg, Champion, and Ma 2004).


Researchers have generated various ing Child 2004). Children’s ability to
definitions of emotion regulation, and regulate their emotions appropriately
debate continues as to the most useful can contribute to perceptions of their
and appropriate way to define this con­ overall social skills as well as to the
cept (Eisenberg and Spinrad 2004). As extent to which they are liked by peers
a construct, emotion regulation reflects (Eisenberg and others 1993). Poor
the interrelationship of emotions, emotion regulation can impair chil­
cognitions, and behaviors (Bell and dren’s thinking, thereby compromising
Wolfe 2004). Young children’s increas­ their judgment and decision making
ing understanding and skill in the use (National Scientific Council on the
of language is of vital importance in Developing Child 2004). At kindergar­
their emotional development, opening ten entry, children demonstrate broad
new avenues for communicating about variability in their ability to self-regu­
and regulating emotions (Campos, late (National Research Council and
Frankel, and Camras 2004) and help­ Institute of Medicine 2000).
ing children to negotiate acceptable
outcomes to emotionally charged situa­ Impulse Control
tions in more effective ways. Emotion Children’s developing capacity to
regulation is influenced by culture and control impulses helps them adapt to
the historical era in which a person social situations and follow rules. As
lives: cultural variability in regula­ infants grow, they become increasingly
tion processes is significant (Mesquita able to exercise voluntary control over
and Frijda 1992). “Cultures vary in behavior such as waiting for needs to
terms of what one is expected to feel, be met, inhibiting potentially hurtful
and when, where, and with whom one behavior, and acting according to
may express different feelings” (Cheah social expectations, including safety


rules. Group care settings provide engage in back-and-forth social inter­


many opportunities for children to actions, and which social scripts are
practice their impulse-control skills. to be used for which social situations.
Peer interactions often offer natural “At each age, social cognitive under­
opportunities for young children to standing contributes to social compe­
practice impulse control, as they make tence, interpersonal sensitivity, and
progress in learning about cooperative an awareness of how the self relates to
play and sharing. Young children’s other individuals and groups in a com­
understanding or lack of understand­ plex social world” (Thompson 2006,
ing of requests made of them may be 26). Social understanding is particu­
one factor contributing to their larly important because of the social
responses (Kaler and Kopp 1990). nature of humans and human life,
even in early infancy (Wellman and
Social Understanding Lagattuta 2000). Recent research sug­
During the infant/toddler years, gests that infants’ and toddlers’ social
children begin to develop an under­ understanding is related to how often
standing of the responses, commu­ they experience adult communica­
nication, emotional expression, and tion about the thoughts and emotions
of others (Taumoepeau and Ruffman

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
actions of other people. This develop­
ment includes infants’ understanding 2008).
of what to expect from others, how to


Foundation: Interactions with Adults


The developing ability to respond to and engage with adults

8 months 18 months 36 months


At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
age, children purposefully children may participate in rou­ children interact with adults
engage in reciprocal interac­ tines and games that involve to solve problems or commu­
tions and try to influence the complex back-and-forth inter­ nicate about experiences or
behavior of others. Children action and may follow the gaze ideas. (California Department
may be both interested in and of the infant care teacher to an of Education 2005, 6; Marvin
cautious of unfamiliar adults. object or person. Children may and Britner 1999, 60).
(7 mos.; Lamb, Bornstein, also check with a familiar infant
and Teti 2002, 340) (8 mos.; care teacher when uncertain
Meisels and others 2003, 16) about something or someone.
(18 mos.; Meisels and others
2003, 33)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Attend to an unfamiliar adult • Move close to the infant care • Participate in storytelling with
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

with interest but show wariness teacher and hold his hand when the infant care teacher. (30–36
or become anxious when that a visitor enters the classroom mos.; Parks 2004)
adult comes too close. (5–8 but watch the visitor with inter­ • Tell a teacher from the class­
mos.; Parks 2004; Johnstone est. (18 mos.; Meisels and oth­ room next door about an
and Scherer 2000, 222) ers 2003) upcoming birthday party.
• Take the infant care teacher’s • Bring a familiar object to an (36 mos.; Parks 2004)
hands and rock forward and adult when asked. (15–18 mos.; • Help the infant care teacher
backwards as a way of asking Parks 2004) bring in the wheeled toys from
her to sing a favorite song. • Allow an unfamiliar adult to get the play yard at the end of the
(8 mos.; Gustafson, Green, and close only after the adult uses day.
West 1979; Kaye and Fogel an object to bridge the interac­
1980) • Ask a classroom visitor her
tion, such as showing interest in name.
• Engage in games such as pat­ a toy that is also interesting to
a-cake and peek-a-boo. (7–9 the child. (18 mos.; Meisels and
mos.; Coplan 1993, 3) others 2003)
• Make eye contact with a family • Watch, and then help the infant
member. care teacher as she prepares
snack.
• Vocalize to get an infant care
teacher’s attention. • Seek reassurance from the
infant care teacher when unsure
if something is safe. (10–12
mos.; Fogel 2001, 305; Dickstein
and Parke 1988; Hirshberg and
Svejda 1990)


Interactions with Adults


Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Engage in playful, face-to-face • Engage in back-and-forth inter­ • Practice being a grown-up dur­
interactions with an adult, such action by handing a parent an ing pretend play by dressing up
as taking turns vocalizing and object, then reaching to receive or using a play stove. (18–36
then smiling or laughing. (2–7 the object when it is handed mos.; Lerner and Dombro 2000)
mos.; Lamb, Bornstein, and Teti back. (9–12 mos.; Lerner and • Help the infant care teacher
2002, 375) Ciervo 2003) clean up after snack by putting
• Begin to protest separations
• Show—but not give—a toy to snack dishes in the dish bin.
from significant adults.
the infant care teacher. (9–12
mos.; Parks 2004)

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
6

Foundation: Relationships with Adults


The development of close relationships with certain adults
who provide consistent nurturance
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months At around 18 months of At around 36 months of age, when explor­
of age, children seek a age, children feel secure ing the environment, from time to time
special relationship with exploring the environ­ children reconnect, in a variety of ways,
one (or a few) familiar ment in the presence of with the adult(s) with whom they have de­
adult(s) by initiating important adults with veloped a special relationship: through eye
interactions and seek­ whom they have devel­ contact; facial expressions; shared feelings;
ing proximity, especially oped a relationship over or conversations about feelings, shared
when distressed. (6–9 an extended period of activities, or plans. When distressed, chil­
mos.; Marvin and Britner time. When distressed, dren may still seek to be physically close
1999, 52) children seek to be to these adults. (By 36 mos.; Marvin and
physically close to these Britner 1999, 57)
adults. (6–18 mos.; Mar­
vin and Britner 1999,
52; Bowlby 1983)
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

For example, the child For example, the child For example, the child may:
may: may:
• Seek comfort from the • Run in wide circles • Feel comfortable playing on the other side
infant care teacher by around the outdoor play of the play yard away from the infant care
crying and looking for area, circling back each teacher, but cry to be picked up after falling
him. (7 mos.; Lamb, time and hug the legs of down. (24–36 mos.; Lamb, Bornstein, and Teti
Bornstein, and Teti 2002, the infant care teacher 2002, 376)
372) before running off again. • Call “Mama!” from across the room while
• Cry out or follow after • Snuggle with the special playing with dolls to make sure that the moth­
a parent when dropped infant care teacher when er is paying attention. (24–36 mos.; Schaffer
off at the child care feeling tired or grumpy. and Emerson 1964)
program. (6–9 mos.; • Call for a family member and look out the
• Wave at the special
Ainsworth1967, 4) window for him after being dropped off at
infant care teacher from
• Lift her arms to be picked the top of the slide to school. (24–36 mos.; Marvin and Britner 1999,
up by the special infant make sure he is watch­ 56)
care teacher. (8 mos.; ing. • Communicate, “This is our favorite part” when
Meisels and others 2003, reading a funny story with the infant care
• Follow a parent physi­
17; Ainsworth 1967, 5) teacher.
cally around the room.
• Crawl toward a parent • Bring the grandmother’s favorite book to her
• Play away from the
when startled by a loud and express, “One more?” to see if she will
infant care teacher and
noise. (8.5 mos.; Marvin read one more book, even though she has
then move close to
and Britner 1999, 52) just said, “We’re all done reading. Now it’s
him from time to time
• Turn excitedly and raise to check in. (12 mos.; time for nap.” (Teti 1999; 18–36 mos.; Marvin
his arms to greet a family Davies 2004, 10) and Britner 1999, 59)
member at pick-up time. • Cry and look for the special infant care
(8 mos.; Ainsworth 1967, teacher after falling.
5)
• Seek the attention of the special infant care
teacher and communicate, “Watch me!” be­
fore proudly displaying a new skill.


Relationships with Adults


Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Hold on to a parent’s sweater
• Cry and ask for a parent after • Say, “I go to school. Mama goes
when being held. (5 mos.;
being dropped off in the morn- to work,” after being dropped off
Marvin and Britner 1999, 51;
ing. (9–12 mos.; Lerner and in the morning.
Ainsworth 1967, 1)
Ciervo 2003) • Gesture for one more hug as a
• Babble back and forth with the • Look for a smile from the parent is leaving for work.
infant care teacher. (3–6 mos.; infant care teacher when
Caufield 1995) unsure if something is safe.
(10–12 mos.; Fogel 2001, 305;
• Be more likely to smile when
Dickstein and Parke 1988;
approached by the infant care
Hirshberg and Svejda 1990)
teacher than a stranger. (3–6
mos.; Marvin and Britner 1999, • Cling to a parent when feeling
50) ill. (10–11 mos.; Marvin and
Britner 1999, 52)
• Cry when an unfamiliar adult
gets too close. (7 mos.; Bronson
1972)

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT


Foundation: Interactions with Peers


The developing ability to respond to and engage with other children
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
age, children show interest in children engage in simple children engage in simple
familiar and unfamiliar peers. back-and-forth interactions cooperative play with peers.
Children may stare at another with peers for short periods (36 mos.; Meisels and others
child, explore another child’s of time. (Meisels and others 2003 70)
face and body, and respond 2003, 35)
to siblings and older peers.
(8 mos.; Meisels and others
2003)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Watch other children with inter­ • Hit another child who takes a • Communicate with peers while
est. (8 mos.; Meisels and others toy. (18 mos.; Meisels and oth­ digging in the sandbox together.
2003) ers 2003, 35) (29–36 mos.; Hart and Risley
1999, 124)
• Touch the eyes or hair of a peer. • Offer a book to another child,
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

(8 mos.; Meisels and others perhaps with encouragement • Act out different roles with
2003) from the infant care teacher. peers, sometimes switching in
(18 mos.; Meisels and others and out of her role. (By 36 mos.;
• Attend to a crying peer with a
2003, 35) Segal 2004, 44)
serious expression. (7 mos.;
American Academy of Pediat­ • Tickle another child, get tickled • Build a tall tower with one or
rics 2004, 212) back, and tickle him again. two other children. (36 mos.;
(18 mos.; Meisels and others Meisels and others 2003, 70)
• Laugh when an older sibling
or peer makes a funny face. 2003, 35) • Hand a peer a block or piece of
(8 mos.; Meisels and others • Engage in reciprocal play, such railroad track when building.
2003) as run-and-chase or offer-and­
receive. (12–13 mos.; Howes
1988, v; 10–12 mos.; Ross and
Goldman 1977)
• Play ball with a peer by rolling
the ball back and forth to each
other. (12–15 mos.; Parks 2004;
9–16 mos.; Frankenburg and
others 1990)


Interactions with Peers


Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Notice other infants and chil­ • Engage in solitary play. (toddler; • Use gestures to communicate
dren while sitting on a parent’s Segal 2004, 38) a desire to play with a peer.
or infant care teacher’s lap. (18–24 mos.; Parks 2004, 123)
• Play a reciprocal game, such as
• Cry when hearing another baby pat-a-cake, with the infant care • Refuse to let a peer have a turn
cry. (4 mos.; Meisels and others teacher and a peer. (7–11 mos.; on the swing. (24 mos.; Meisels
2003, 10) Frankenburg and othres 1990) and others 2003, 45)
• Push or bite when another child
takes a toy. (24–30 mos.; Parks
2004)
• Engage in complementary
interactions, such as feeding a
stuffed animal that another child
is holding or pulling a friend
in the wagon. (24–30 mos.;
Meisels and others 2003, 57;
Howes and Matheson 1992,

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
967)
• Join a group of children who
are together in one play space
and follow them as they move
outside. (30 mos.; Meisels and
others 2003, 57)
0

Foundation: Relationships with Peers


The development of relationships with certain peers
through interactions over time
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
age, children show interest in children prefer to interact with children have developed
familiar and unfamiliar children. one or two familiar children in friendships with a small num­
(8 mos.; Meisels and others the group and usually engage ber of children in the group
2003, 17) in the same kind of back-and­ and engage in more complex
forth play when interacting play with those friends than
with those children. (12–18 with other peers.
mos.; Mueller and Lucas 1975)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Watch other children with inter­ • Play the same kind of game, • Choose to play with a sibling
est. (8 mos.; Meisels and others such as run-and-chase, with instead of a less familiar child.
2003) the same peer almost every (24–36 mos.; Dunn 1983, 795)
day. (Howes 1987, 259)
• Touch the eyes or hair of a peer. • Exhibit sadness when the favor­
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

(8 mos.; Meisels and others • Choose to play in the same ite friend is not at school one
2003) area as a friend. (Howes 1987, day. (24–36 mos.; Melson and
259) Cohen 1981)
• Attend to a crying peer with a
serious expression. (7 mos.; • Seek one friend for running
American Academy of Pediat­ games and another for building
rics 2004, 212) with blocks. (Howes 1987)
• Laugh when an older sibling • Play “train” with one or two
or peer makes a funny face. friends for an extended period
(8 mos.; Meisels and others of time by pretending that one
2003) is driving the train and the rest
• Try to get the attention of an­ are riding.
other child by smiling at him or
babbling to him (6–9 mos.; Hay,
Pederson, and Nash 1982)

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Look at another child who is • Watch an older sibling play • Engage in social pretend play
lying on the blanket nearby. nearby. (12 mos.; Meisels and with one or two friends; for
(4 mos.; Meisels and others others 2003, 26) example, pretend to be a dog
2003, 10) while a friend pretends to be
• Bang blocks together next to
the owner. (24–30 mos.; Howes
• Turn toward the voice of a par­ a child who is doing the same
1987, 261)
ent or older sibling. (4 mos.; thing. (12 mos.; Meisels and
Meisels and others 2003, 10) others 2003, 26) • Express an interest in playing
with a particular child. (13–24
• Imitate the simple actions of a
mos.; Howes 1988, 3)
peer. (9–12 mos.; Ryalls, Gul,
and Ryalls 2000)


Foundation: Identity of Self in Relation to Others


The developing concept that the child is an individual
operating within social relationships
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
children show clear awareness children demonstrate aware­ children identify their feel­
of being a separate person and ness of their characteristics ings, needs, and interests, and
of being connected with other and express themselves as identify themselves and others
people. Children identify others distinct persons with thoughts as members of one or more
as both distinct from and con­ and feelings. Children also groups by referring to catego­
nected to themselves. (Fogel demonstrate expectations of ries. (24–36 mos.; Fogel 2001,
2001, 347) others’ behaviors, responses, 415; 18–30 mos.)
and characteristics on the
basis of previous experiences
with them.

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Respond to someone who calls • Point to or indicate parts of the • Use pronouns such as I, me,

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
her name. (5–7 mos.; Parks body when asked. (15–19 mos.; you, we, he, and she. (By
2004, 94; 9 mo.; Coplan 1993, Parks 2004) 36 mo.; American Academy of
2) Pediatrics 2004, p. 307)
• Express thoughts and feel­
• Turn toward a familiar person ings by saying “no!” (18 mos.; • Say own name. (30–33 mos.;
upon hearing his name. (6–8 Meisels and others 2003) Parks 2004, 115)
mos.; Parks 2004, 94; 8 mos.; • Move excitedly when ap­ • Begin to make comparisons
Meisels and others 2003, 18) proached by an infant care between self and others; for
• Look at an unfamiliar adult with teacher who usually engages in example, communicate, “_____
interest but show wariness or active play. is a boy/girl like me.”
become anxious when that • Name people in the family.
adult comes too close. (5–8
mos.; Parks 2004; Johnstone • Point to pictures of friends and
and Scherer 2000, 222) say their names.
• Wave arms and kick legs when • Communicate, “Do it myself!”
a parent enters the room. when the infant care teacher
tries to help.
• Cry when the favorite infant care
teacher leaves the room. (6–10
mos.; Parks 2004)

Chart continues on next page.




Identity of Self in Relation to Others


Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Use hands to explore differ­ • Play games such as peek-a-boo • Recognize his own image in the
ent parts of the body. (4 mos.; or run-and-chase with the infant mirror and understand that it is
Kravitz, Goldenberg, and Ney­ care teacher. (Stern 1985, 102; himself. (Siegel 1999, 35; Lewis
hus 1978) 7–11 mos.; Frankenburg and and Brooks-Gunn 1979, 56)
others 1990)
• Examine her own hands and a • Know the names of familiar
parent’s hands. (Scaled score • Recognize familiar people, such people, such as a neighbor. (by
of 9 for 4:06–4:15 mos.;* Bayley as a neighbor or infant care end of second year; American
2006, 53) teacher from another room, in Academy of Pediatrics 2004,
addition to immediate family 270)
• Watch or listen for the infant
members. (12–18 mo.; Parks
care teacher to come to meet • Show understanding of or use
2004)
the child’s needs. (Birth–8 mos.; words such as you, me, mine,
Lerner and Dombro 2000, 42) • Use names to refer to significant he, she, it, and I. (20–24 mos.;
people; for example, “Mama” to Parks 2004, 96; 20 mos.; Bay-
refer to the mother and “Papa” ley 2006; 18–24 mos.; Lerner
to refer to the father. (11–14 and Ciervo 2003; 19 mos.; Hart
mos.; Parks 2004, 109) and Risley 1999, 61; 24–20
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

mos.; Parks 2004, 113)


• Use name or other family label
(e.g., nickname, birth order,
“little sister”) when referring to
self. (18–24 mo.; Parks 2004;
24 mo.; Lewis and Brooks-
Gunn 1979)
• Claim everything as “mine.” (24
mos.; Levine 1983)
• Point to or indicate self in a
photograph. (24 mos.; Lewis
and Brooks-Gunn 1979)
• Proudly show the infant care
teacher a new possession.
(24–30 mos.; Parks 2004)

*Four months, six days, to four months, 15 days.




Foundation: Recognition of Ability


The developing understanding that the child can take action
to influence the environment
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
age, children understand that children experiment with dif­ children show an understanding
they are able to make things ferent ways of making things of their own abilities and may
happen. happen, persist in trying to do refer to those abilities when
things even when faced with describing themselves.
difficulty, and show a sense
of satisfaction with what they
can do. (McCarty, Clifton, and
Collard 1999)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Pat a musical toy to try to • Roll a toy car back and forth • Communicate, “I take care of the
make the music come on on the ground and then push bunny” after helping to feed the
again. (5–9 mos.; Parks 2004) it really hard and let go to class rabbit. (18–36 mos.; Lally

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
see what happens. (18 mos.; and others 1995, 71)
• Raise arms to be picked up by
McCarty, Clifton, and Collard
the infant care teacher. (6–9 • Finish painting a picture and hold
1999)
mos.; Fogel 2001, 274) it up to show a family member.
• Clap and bounce with joy after
• Initiate a favorite game; for • Complete a difficult puzzle for
making a handprint with paint.
example, hold out a foot to a the first time and clap or express,
(12–18 mos.; Sroufe 1979;
parent to start a game of “This “I’m good at puzzles.”
Lally and others 1995, 71)
Little Piggy.” (8 mos.; Meisels
and others 2003; • Squeeze a toy in different
6–9 mos.; Fogel 2001, 274) ways to hear the sounds it
• Gesture at a book and smile makes. (Scaled score of 10
for 13:16–14:15 mos.;* Bayley
with satisfaction after the infant
care teacher gets it down from 2006)
the shelf. (8 mos.; Meisels and • Smile after walking up a steep
others 2003) incline without falling or carry­
ing a bucket full of sand from
one place to another without
spilling.
• Proudly hold up a book hidden
in a stack after being asked by
the infant care teacher to find
it.

*Denotes thirteen months, 16 days, to fourteen months, 15 days.


Chart continues on next page.


Recognition of Ability
Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Try again and again to roll over, • Drop a blanket over the side of • Insist on zipping up a jacket
even though not yet able to roll the crib and wait for the infant when the infant care teacher
completely over. care teacher to pick it up. (12 tries to help. (20–28 mos.; Hart
mos.; Meisels and others 2003) and Risley 1999, 62; 24 mos.;
• Grasp, suck, or look at a teeth­
Hart and Risley 1999, 122 and
ing ring. (Before 8 mos. of age; • Drop a toy truck in the water
129; 20–36 mos.; Bates 1990;
Fogel 2001, 218) table and blink in anticipation
Bullock and Lutkenhaus 1988,
of the big splash. (12 mos.;
• Shake a toy, hear it make noise, 1990; Stipek, Gralinski, and
Meisels and others 2003)
and shake it again. Kopp 1990)
• Look over a shoulder, smile
• Stop crying upon seeing the
• Point to a stack of blocks he
at the mother, and giggle in
infant care teacher approach
has made and express, “look”
a playful way while crawling
with a bottle.
to the infant care teacher.
past her, to entice her to play a
(28 mos.; Hart and Risley 1999,
game of run-and-chase. (10–14
96)
mos.; Bayley 2006)
• Communicate, “I doing this,” “I
• Turn light switch on and off
don’t do this, “ “I can do this,”
repeatedly.
or “I did this.” (25 mos.; Hart
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

and Risley 1999, 121; Dunn,


1987; Stipek, Gralinski, and
Kopp 1990)
• Say, “I climb high” when telling
the infant care teacher about
what happened during outside
play time, then run outside
to show him how. (30 mos.;
Meisels and others 2003)


Foundation: Expression of Emotion


The developing ability to express a variety of feelings through facial
expressions, movements, gestures, sounds, or words
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
age, children express a variety children express emotions in children express complex, self-
of primary emotions such as a clear and intentional way, conscious emotions such as
contentment, distress, joy, and begin to express some pride, embarrassment, shame,
sadness, interest, surprise, complex emotions, such as and guilt. Children demonstrate
disgust, anger, and fear. pride. awareness of their feelings by
(Lamb, Bornstein, and Teti using words to describe feelings
2002, 341) to others or acting them out in
pretend play. (Lewis and others
1989; Lewis 2000b; Lagattuta
and Thompson 2007)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Exhibit wariness, cry, or turn • Show affection for a family • Hide face with hands when feel­

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
away when a stranger ap­ member by hugging. (8–18 ing embarrassed. (Lagattuta and
proaches. (6 mos.; Lamb, mos.; Lally and others 1995; Thompson 2007)
Bornstein, and Teti 2002, 338; Greenspan and Greenspan • Use words to describe feelings;
Fogel 2001, 297; 7–8 mos.; 1985, 84) for example, “I don’t like that.”
Lewis 2000a, 277) • Express jealousy by trying to (24–36 mos.; Fogel 2001, 414;
• Be more likely to react with an­ crowd onto the infant care 24–36 mos.; Harris and others
ger than just distress when ac­ teacher’s lap when another 1989; Yuill 1984)
cidentally hurt by another child. child is already sitting there.
• Communicate, “I miss Grandma,”
(later in the first year; Lamb, (12–18 mos.; Hart and others after talking on the phone with
Bornstein, and Teti 2002, 341) 1998) her. (24–36 mos.; Harris and oth­
• Express fear of unfamiliar • Express anger at having a ers 1989; Yuill 1984)
people by moving near a famil­ toy taken away by taking it • Act out different emotions dur­
iar infant care teacher. (8 mos.; back out of the other child’s ing pretend play by “crying”
Bronson 1972) hands or hitting her. (18 mos.; when pretending to be sad and
Squires, Bricker, and Twombly
• Stop crying and snuggle after “cooing” when pretending to be
being picked up by a parent. 2002, 115) happy. (Dunn, Bretherton, and
• Smile directly at other children Munn 1987)
• Show surprise when the infant
when interacting with them.
care teacher removes the blan­ • Express guilt after taking a toy
(18 mos.; Squires, Bricker, and
ket covering her face to start a out of another child’s cubby with­
Twombly 2002, 115)
game of peek-a-boo. out permission by trying to put
• Express pride by communi­ it back without anyone seeing.
cating, “I did it!” (15–24 mos.; (Lagattuta and Thompson 2007)
Lewis and others 1989; Lewis
2000b)

Chart continues on next page.


6

Expression of Emotion
Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child


During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
may:
• Become anxious when a parent • Communicate, “Mama mad”
• Get frustrated or angry when leaves the room. (6–9 mos.; after being told by the mother to
unable to reach a toy. (4–6 Parks 2004) stop an action. (28 mos.; Breth­
mos.; Sternberg, Campos, erton and others 1986)
• Knock a shape-sorter toy away
and Emde 1983) when it gets to be too frustrat­ • Use one or a few words to de­
• Express joy by squealing. ing. (10–12 mos.; Sroufe 1979) scribe feelings to the infant care
(5–6 mos.; Parks 2004, 125) teacher. (18–30 mos.; Bretherton
• Show anger, when another child
and others 1986; Dunn 1987)
• Frown and make noises to takes a toy, by taking it back.
indicate frustration. (5–6 mos.; (10–12 mos.; Sroufe 1979) • Express frustration through tan­
Parks 2004, 125) trums. (18–36 mos.; Pruett 1999,
• Express fear by crying upon
148)
• Be surprised when something hearing a dog bark loudly or
unexpected happens. (First 6 seeing someone dressed in a
mos. of life; Lewis 2000a) costume. (10 mos.; Bronson
1972)
• Express sadness by frown­
ing after losing or misplacing a
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

favorite toy. (9–10 mos.; Fogel


2001, 300)
• Smile with affection as a sibling
approaches. (10 mos.; Sroufe
1979; Fox and Davidson 1988)
• Push an unwanted object away.
(12 mos.; Squires, Bricker, and
Twombly 2002, 114)


Foundation: Empathy
The developing ability to share in the emotional experiences of others
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
age, children demonstrate children change their behav­ children understand that other
awareness of others’ feelings ior in response to the feelings people have feelings that are
by reacting to their emotional of others even though their different from their own and
expressions. actions may not always make can sometimes respond to
the other person feel better. another’s distress in a way
Children show an increased that might make that person
understanding of the reason feel better. (24–36 mos.; Hoff­
for another’s distress and man 1982; 18 mos.; Thomp­
may become distressed by son 1987, 135).
the other’s distress. (14 mos.;
Zahn-Waxler, Robinson, and
Emde 1992; Thompson 1987;
24 mos.; Zahn-Waxler and
Radke-Yarrow 1982, 1990)

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Stop playing and look at a child • Offer to help a crying playmate • Do a silly dance in an attempt
who is crying. (7 mos.; Ameri­ by bringing his own mother over. to make a crying peer smile.
can Academy of Pediatrics (13–15 mos.; Wingert and Brant (24–36 mos.; Dunn 1988)
2004, 212) 2005, 35) • Communicate, “Lucas is sad
• Laugh when an older sibling • Try to hug a crying peer. because Isabel took his cup.”
or peer makes a funny face. (8 (18 mos.; Thompson 1987, 135) (36 mos.; Harris and others
mos.; Meisels and others 2003) 1989; Yuill 1984)
• Bring her own special blanket
• Return the smile of the infant to a peer who is crying. (13–15 • Comfort a younger sibling who
care teacher. mos.; Wingert and Brant 2005, is crying by patting his back,
35) expressing “It’s okay” and of­
• Grimace when another child
fering him a snack. (Denham
cries. (Older than 6 mos.; • Become upset when another
1998, 34)
Wingert and Brant 2005, 35) child throws a tantrum.
• Communicate, “Mama sad”
• Gently pat a crying peer on his
back, just like his infant care when the mother cries during
a movie. (24–36 mos.; Dunn
teacher did earlier in the day.
1994; Harris 2000, 282).
(16 mos.; Bergman and Wilson
1984; Zahn-Waxler and others • Communicate, “Olivia’s mama
1992) is happy” and point to or
indicate the illustration in the
• Hit a child who is crying loudly.
picture book. (24 mos.; Harris
• Stop playing and look with con­ 2000, 282).
cerned attention at a child who
• Get an infant care teacher to
is screaming.
help a child who has fallen
• Move quickly away from a child down and is crying.
who is crying loudly.

Chart continues on next page.




Empathy
Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Cry when hearing another baby • Stand nearby and quietly watch • Hug a crying peer. (18–24 mos.;
cry. (Younger than 6 mos; Wing­ a peer who has fallen down Parks 2004, 123)
ert and Brant 2005, 35) and is crying. • Become upset in the presence
• Exhibit social referencing by of those who are upset.
looking for emotional indica­
tors in others’ faces, voices, or
gestures to decide what to do
when uncertain. (10–12 mos.;
Thompson 1987, 129)
• Cry upon hearing another child
cry. (12 mos.; Meisels and oth­
ers 2003, 26)
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT


Foundation: Emotion Regulation


The developing ability to manage emotional responses, with
assistance from others and independently
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
children use simple behaviors to children demonstrate a vari­ children anticipate the need
comfort themselves and begin to ety of responses to comfort for comfort and try to prepare
communicate the need for help to themselves and actively avoid themselves for changes in
alleviate discomfort or distress. or ignore situations that cause routine. Children have many
discomfort. Children can also self-comforting behaviors
communicate needs and wants to choose from, depending
through the use of a few words on the situation, and can
and gestures. (National Re­ communicate specific needs
search Council and Institute and wants. (Kopp 1989; CDE
of Medicine 2000, 112; 15–18 2005)
mos.; American Academy of
Pediatrics 2004, 270; Coplan
1993, 1)

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Turn away from an overstimulating • Use gestures and simple words • Reach for the mother’s hand
activity. (3–12 mos.; Rothbart, Ziaie, to express distress and seek just before she pulls a ban­
and O’Boyle 1992) specific kinds of assistance dage off the child’s knee.
from the infant care teacher in
• Vocalize to get a parent’s attention. • Ask the infant care teacher to
order to calm self. (Brazelton
(6.5–8 mos.; Parks 2004, 126) hold him up to the window to
1992; Kopp 1989, 347) wave good-bye before the par­
• Lift arms to the infant care teacher
• Use comfort objects, such as a ent leaves in the morning.
to communicate a desire to be held.
special blanket or stuffed toy, to
(7–9 mos.; Coplan 1993, 3; 5–9 mos.; • Show the substitute teacher
help calm down. (Kopp 1989,
Parks 2004, 121) that she likes a back rub dur­
348) ing naptime by patting own
• Turn toward the infant care teacher for
• Seek to be close to a parent back while lying on the mat.
assistance when crying. (6–9 mos.;
Fogel 2001, 274) when upset. (Lieberman 1993) • Play quietly in a corner of the
• Cry after her hand was accidentally • Play with a toy as a way to room right after drop-off, until
distract self from discomfort. ready to play with the other
stepped on by a peer and then hold
(12–18 mos.; Kopp 1989, 347) children.
the hand up to the infant care teacher
to look at it. • Communicate, “I’m okay” after • Ask the infant care teacher to
falling down. (National Research explain what’s going to happen
• Reach toward a bottle that is up on
the counter and vocalize when hun­ Council and Institute of Medi­ at the child’s dental appoint­
cine 2000, 112) ment later in the day.
gry.
• Indicate her knee and say “boo • Communicate, “Daddy always
• Make a face of disgust to tell the
boo” after falling down and comes back” after saying
infant care teacher that he does not
want any more food. (6–9 mos.; gesture or ask for a bandage. good-bye to him in the morn­
ing.
Lerner and Ciervo 2003) • Approach the infant care
• Bump head, cry, and look to infant teacher for a hug and express,
“Mommy work,” then point to
care teacher for comfort.
the door to communicate miss­
• Suck on a thumb to make self feel ing the mother.
better.
• Look at the infant care teacher when
an unfamiliar person enters the room.

Chart continues on next page.


0

Emotion Regulation
Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Suck on hands, focus on an in­ • Move away from something • Continue to rely on adults for
teresting toy, or move the body that is bothersome and move reassurance and help in con­
in a rocking motion to calm self. toward the infant care teacher trolling feelings and behavior.
(3–6 mos.; Parks 2004, 10) for comfort. (6–12 mos.; Bron­ (Lally and others 1995)
son 2000b, 64)
• Cry inconsolably less often than • Reenact emotional events in
in the early months. (6 mos.; • Fight back tears when a parent play to try to gain mastery over
Parks 2004, 10) leaves for the day. (12 mos.; these feelings. (Greenspan and
Bridges, Grolnick, and Con­ Greenspan 1985)
• Calm self by sucking on fingers
nell 1997; Parritz 1996; Sroufe
or hands. (4 mos.; Thelen and • Use words to ask for specific
1979)
Fogel 1989; 3–12 mos.; Bron­ help with regulating emotions.
son 2000b, 64) • Look for a cue from the infant (Kopp 1989)
care teacher when unsure if
• Be able to inhibit some negative • Express wants and needs
something is safe. (10–12 mos.;
emotions. (Later in the first year; verbally; for example, say, “hold
Fogel 2001, 305; Dickstein and
Fox and Calkins 2000) me” to the infant care teacher
Parke 1988; Hirshberg and
• Shift attention away from a dis­ when feeling tired or over­
Svejda 1990)
tressing event onto an object, whelmed. (30–31.5 mos.; Parks
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

• Fuss to communicate needs or 2004, 130)


as a way of managing emo­
wants; begin to cry if the infant
tions. (6 mos.; Weinberg and
care teacher does not respond
others 1999)
soon enough. (11–19 mos.;
• Fall asleep when feeling over­ Hart and Risley 1999, 77)
whelmed.
• Repeat sounds to get the infant
care teacher’s attention. (11–19
mos.; Hart and Risley 1999, 79)


Foundation: Impulse Control


The developing capacity to wait for needs to be met,

to inhibit potentially hurtful behavior, and to act according

to social expectations, including safety rules

8 months 18 months 36 months


At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
age, children act on impulses. children respond positively children may sometimes
(Birth–9 mos.; Bronson 2000b, to choices and limits set by exercise voluntary control over
64) an adult to help control their actions and emotional expres­
behavior. (18 mos.; Meisels sions. (Bronson 2000b, 67)
and others 2003, 34; Kaler and
Kopp 1990)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Explore the feel of hair by • Stop drawing on the wall when • Jump up and down on the
pulling it. (4–7 mos.; American a parent asks. (18 mos.; Meisels couch but stop jumping and
Academy of Pediatrics 2004, and others 2003) climb down when a parent
226) enters the room. (36 mos.;
• Choose one toy when the infant

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
Meisels and others 2003)
• Reach for an interesting toy that care teacher asks, “Which one
another child is mouthing. do you want?” even though the • Experience difficulty (e.g., cry,
child really wants both. whine, pout) with transitions.
• Reach for another child’s bottle
that was just set down nearby. (30–36 mos.; Parks 2004, 320)
• Express “no no” while ap­
proaching something the child • Begin to share.
• Turn the head away or push the
knows she should not touch,
bottle away when finished eat­ • Handle transitions better when
ing (8 mos.; Meisels and others because the infant care teacher prepared ahead of time or when
2003, 19). has communicated “no no” in the child has some control over
the past when the child tried to what happens.
do this.
• Touch a pet gently without
• Look to the infant care teacher needing to be reminded.
to see his reaction when the
child reaches toward the light • Wait to start eating until others
switch. at the table are also ready.
• Stop reaching for the eyeglass­
es on the infant care teacher’s
face when she gently says, “no
no.” (Scaled score of 10 for
7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
87; 12 mos.; Meisels and others
2003, 27)

Chart continues on next page.




Impulse Control
Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Cry when hungry or tired. • Crawl too close to a younger • Begin to use words and dra­
infant lying nearby. matic play to describe, under­
• Fall asleep when tired.
stand, and control impulses
• Refrain from exploring another
and feelings. (Lally and others
baby’s hair when reminded to
1995)
be gentle. (8–10 mos.; Brazel­
ton 1992, 256) • Communicate, “Mine!” and
take a doll out of the hands of a
• Look at the infant care teacher’s
peer. (23–24 mos.; Parks 2004,
face to determine whether it is
330)
all right to play with a toy on
the table. (12 mos.; Meisels and • Throw a puzzle piece on the
others 2003, 25) floor after having trouble fitting
it in the opening. (24 mos.;
• Bite another child who takes a
Meisels and others 2003)
toy.
• Open the playground door and
• Reach for food on a plate
run out, even after being asked
before the infant care teacher
by the infant care teacher to
offers it. (12 mos.; Meisels and
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

wait. (24 mos.; Meisels and


others 2003, 25)
others 2003)
• Start to take another child’s toy,
then stop after catching the eye
of the infant care teacher. (24
mos.; Meisels and others 2003)
• Use a quiet voice at naptime.
(30 mos.; Meisels and others
2003)
• Understand and carry out
simple commands or rules.
(Bronson 2000b, 85)
• Have a tantrum rather than
attempt to manage strong feel­
ings. (Brazelton 1992)
• Be able to wait for a turn.


Foundation: Social Understanding


The developing understanding of the responses, communication,
emotional expressions, and actions of other people
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of
age, children have learned children know how to get the age, children can talk about
what to expect from familiar infant care teacher to respond in their own wants and feelings
people, understand what to a specific way through ges­ and those of other people,
do to get another’s attention, tures, vocalizations, and shared describe familiar routines,
engage in back-and-forth attention; use another’s emo­ participate in coordinated
interactions with others, and tional expressions to guide their episodes of pretend play
imitate the simple actions or own responses to unfamiliar with peers, and interact
facial expressions of others. events; and learn more com­ with adults in more complex
plex behavior through imitation. ways.
Children also engage in more
complex social interactions and
have developed expectations
for a greater number of familiar
people.

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Smile when the infant care • Gesture toward a desired toy • Name own feelings or desires,
teacher pauses, to get her to or food while reaching, making explicitly contrast them with
continue playing peek-a-boo or imperative vocal sounds, and another’s, or describe why the
pat-a-cake. looking toward the infant care child feels the way he does.
teacher.
• Squeal in anticipation of the • Describe what happens during
infant care teacher’s uncover­ • Seek reassurance from the infant the bedtime routine or another
ing her eyes during a game of care teacher when unsure about familiar everyday event.
peek-a-boo. something. • Move into and out of pretend
• Learn simple behaviors by imi­ • Vary response to different infant play roles, tell other children
tating a parent’s facial expres­ care teachers depending on their what they should do in their
sions, gestures, or sounds. play styles, even before they have roles, or extend the sequence
started playing; for example, (such as by asking “Wanna
• Try to get a familiar game or
get very excited upon seeing an drink?” after bringing a pre­
routine started by prompting
infant care teacher who regularly tend hamburger to the table
the infant care teacher.
plays in an exciting, vigorous as a waiter).
• Quiet crying upon realizing that manner. • Help the infant care teacher
the infant care teacher is ap­
• Engage in back-and-forth play search for a missing toy.
proaching.
that involves turn-taking, such as • Talk about what happened
rolling a ball back and forth. during a recent past experi­
• Look in the direction of the infant ence, with the assistance of
care teacher’s gesturing or point­ the infant care teacher.
ing. • Help the infant care teacher
• Learn more complex behaviors clean up at the end of the
through imitation, such as day by putting the toys in the
watching an older child put toys usual places.
together and then doing it.

Chart continues on next page.




Social Understanding

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Make imperative vocal sounds • Follow the infant care teacher’s • Vary play with different peers
to attract the infant care gaze to look at a toy. depending on their preferred play
teacher’s attention. activities.
• Hold up or gesture toward
• Participate in playful, face-to­ objects in order to direct the • Imitate the behavior of peers as
face interactions with an adult, infant care teacher’s attention well as adults.
such as taking turns vocaliz­ to them.
ing.
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT


References

Ainsworth, M. D. 1967. Infancy in Uganda: Brazelton, T. B. 1992. Touchpoints: Your


Infant Care and the Growth of Love. Child’s Emotional and Behavioral Devel­
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University opment. New York: Perseus Publishing.
Press. Bretherton, I., and others. 1986. “Learning
American Academy of Pediatrics. 2004. to Talk About Emotions: A Functional­
Caring for Your Baby and Young Child: ist Perspective,” Child Development, Vol.
Birth to Age 5 (Fourth edition). Edited 57, 529–48.
by S. P. Shelov and R. E. Hannemann. Bridges, L. J.; W. S. Grolnick; and J. P
New York: Bantam Books. Connell. 1997. “Infant Emotion Regula­
Anderson, C., and D. Keltner. 2002. “The tion with Mothers and Fathers,” Infant
Role of Empathy in the Formation and Behavior and Development, Vol. 20,
Maintenance of Social Bonds,” Behav­ 47–57.
ioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 25, No. 1, Bronson, G. W. 1972. “Infants’ Reaction to
21–22. Unfamiliar Persons and Novel Objects,”
Barrera, M. E., and D. Maurer. 1981. “Dis­ Monographs of the Society for Research
crimination of Strangers by the Three­ in Child Development, Vol. 37, No. 148,

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
Month-Old,” Child Development, Vol. 52, 1–46.
No. 2, 558–63. Bronson, M. 2000a. “Recognizing and
Barrett, L., and others. 2007. “The Expe­ Supporting the Development of Self-
rience of Emotion,” Annual Review of Regulation in Young Children,” Young
Psychology, Vol. 58, 373–403. Children, Vol. 55, No. 2, 32–37.
Bates, E. 1990. “Language About Me and Bronson, M. 2000b. Self-Regulation in
You: Pronomial Reference and the Early Childhood: Nature and Nurture.
Emerging Concept of Self,” in The Self in New York: Guilford Press.
Transition: Infancy to Childhood. Edited Bullock, M., and P. Lutkenhaus. 1988.
by D. Cicchetti and M. Beeghly. Chi­ “The Development of Volitional Behavior
cago: University of Chicago Press. in the Toddler Years,” Child Develop­
Bayley, N. 2006. Bayley Scales of Infant ment, Vol. 59, 664–74.
and Toddler Development (Third edi­ Bullock, M., and P. Lutkenhaus. 1990.
tion). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt “Who Am I? Self-Understanding in Tod­
Assessment, Inc. dlers,” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. 36,
Bell, M., and C. Wolfe. 2004. “Emotion and 217–38.
Cognition: An Intricately Bound Devel­ Burk, D. I. 1996. “Understanding Friend­
opmental Process,” Child Development, ship and Social Interaction,” Childhood
Vol. 75, No. 2, 366–70. Education, Vol. 72, No. 5, 282–85.
Bergman, A., and A. Wilson. 1984. Cacioppo, J., and G. Berntson. 1999. “The
“Thoughts About Stages on the Way Affect System: Architecture and Oper­
to Empathy and the Capacity for ating Characteristics,” Current Direc­
Concern,” in Empathy II. Edited by tions in Psychological Science, Vol. 8, No.
J. Lichtenberg, M. Bornstein, and D. 5, 133–37.
Silver. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum California Department of Education
Associates. (CDE). 2005. “Desired Results Devel­
Bowlby, J. 1983. Attachment (Second edi­ opmental Profile (DRDP).” Sacramento:
tion), Attachment and Loss series, Vol. California Department of Education.
1. Foreword by Allan N. Schore. New
York: Basic Books.
6

http://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/ci/desire­ Promotion of Prosocial Behavior in Early


dresults.asp (accessed February 7, Childhood. Edited by E. Chesebrough
2007). and others. New York: Kluwer Aca­
Campos, J.; C. Frankel; and L. Camras. demic/Plenum Publishers.
2004. “On the Nature of Emotion Regu­ Dickstein, S., and R. D. Parke. 1988.
lation,” Child Development, Vol. 75, No. “Social Referencing in Infancy: A
2, 377–94. Glance at Fathers and Marriage,” Child
Caufield, R. 1995. “Reciprocity Between Development, Vol. 59, No. 2, 506–11.
Infants and Caregivers During the First Dunn, J. 1983. “Sibling Relationships in
Year of Life,” Early Childhood Education Early Childhood,” Child Development,
Journal, Vol. 23, No. 1, 3–8. Vol. 54, No. 4, 787–811.
Cheah, C., and K. Rubin. 2003. “European Dunn, J. 1987. “The Beginnings of Moral
American and Mainland Chinese Moth­ Understanding: Development in the
ers’ Socialization Beliefs Regarding Second Year,” in The Emergence of
Preschoolers’ Social Skills,” Parenting: Morality in Young Children. Edited by J.
Science and Practice, Vol. 3, No. 1, 1–21 Kagan and S. Lamb. Chicago: Univer­
Cohen, J., and others. 2005. Helping sity of Chicago Press.
Young Children Succeed: Strategies to Dunn, J. 1988. The Beginnings of Social
Promote Early Childhood Social and Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Har­
Emotional Development. Washington, vard University Press.
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

DC: National Conference of State Legis­ Dunn, J. 1994. “Changing Minds and
latures and Zero to Three. http://www. Changing Relationships,” in Children’s
zerotothree.org/policy (accessed on Early Understanding of Mind: Origins
December 7, 2006) and Development. Edited by C. Lewis
Connell, J. P. 1990. “Context, Self, and and P. Mitchell. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Action: A Motivational Analysis of Self- Erlbaum Associates.
System Processes Across the Lifespan,” Dunn, J.; I. Bretherton; and P. Munn.
in The Self in Transition: Infancy to 1987. “Conversations About Feeling
Childhood. Edited by D. Cicchetti and States Between Mothers and Their
M. Beeghly. Chicago: The University of Young Children,” Developmental Psy­
Chicago Press. chology, Vol. 23, No. 1, 132–39.
Coplan, J. 1993. Early Language Milestone Eisenberg, N. 2000. “Emotion, Regula­
Scale: Examiner’s Manual (Second edi­ tion and Moral Development,” Annual
tion). Austin, TX: Pro-ed. Review of Psychology, Vol. 51, 665–97.
Davies, D. 2004. Child Development: A Eisenberg, N.; C. Champion; and Y. Ma.
Practitioner’s Guide (Second edition). 2004. “Emotion-Related Regulation:
New York: Guilford Press. An Emerging Construct,” Merrill-Palmer
DeCasper, A. J., and W. P. Fifer. 1980. Quarterly, Vol. 50, No. 3, 236–59.
“Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Eisenberg, N., and others. 1993. “The
Their Mothers’ Voices,” Science, Vol. Relations of Emotionality and Regula­
208, No. 6, 1174–76. tion to Preschoolers’ Social Skills and
Denham, S. 1998. Emotional Development Sociometric Status,” Child Development,
in Young Children. New York: Guilford Vol. 64, 1418–38.
Press. Eisenberg, N., and T. Spinrad. 2004.
Denham, S., and R. Weissberg. 2003. “Emotion-Related Regulation: Sharpen­
“Social-Emotional Learning in Early ing the Definition,” Child Development,
Childhood: What We Know and Where Vol. 75, No. 2, 334–39.
to Go From Here,” in A Blueprint for the


Fabes, R., and others. 2001. “Preschool­ Harris, P. L., and others. 1989. “Young
ers’ Spontaneous Emotion Vocabulary: Children’s Theory of Mind and Emo­
Relations to Likability,” Early Education tion,” Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 3, No.
& Development, Vol. 12, No. 1, 11–27. 4, 379–400.
Fernald, A. 1993. “Approval and Disap­ Hart, B., and T. R. Risley. 1999. The Social
proval: Infant Responsiveness to Vocal World of Children: Learning to Talk.
Affect in Familiar and Unfamiliar Lan­ Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Pub­
guages,” Child Development, Vol. 64, No. lishing.
3, 657–74. Hart, S., and others. 1998. “Jealousy Pro­
Fogel, A. 2001. Infancy: Infant, Family, and tests in Infants of Depressed Mothers,”
Society (Fourth edition). Belmont, CA: Infant Behavior and Development, Vol.
Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. 21, No. 1, 137–48.
Fox, N. A., and S. D. Calkins. 2000. “Mul­ Hay, D. F.; J. Pederson; and A. Nash.
tiple Measure Approaches to the Study 1982. “Dyadic Interaction in the First
of Infant Emotion,” in Handbook of Year of Life,” in Peer Relationships and
Emotions (Second edition). Edited by M. Social Skills in Childhood. Edited by
Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones. New K. H. Rubin and H. S. Ross. New York:
York: Guilford Press. Springer-Verlag.
Fox, N. A., and R. J. Davidson. 1988. “Pat­ Hirshberg, L. M., and M. Svejda. 1990.
terns of Brain Electrical Activity Dur­ “When Infants Look to Their Parents:

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
ing the Expression of Discrete Emotions Infants’ Social Referencing of Mothers
in Ten-Month-Old Infants,” Developmen­ Compared to Fathers,” Child Develop­
tal Psychology, Vol. 24, 230–36. ment, Vol. 61, No. 4, 1175–86.
Frankenburg, W. K., and others. 1990. Hoffman, M. L. 1982. “Development of Pro-
Denver II Screening Manual. Denver, social Motivation: Empathy and Guilt,”
CO: Denver Developmental Materials. in The Development of Prosocial Behav­
Fredrickson, B. 2000. “Cultivating Posi­ ior. Edited by N. Eisenberg. New York:
tive Emotions to Optimize Health and Academic Press.
Well-Being,” Prevention and Treatment, Howes, C. 1983. “Patterns of Friendship,”
Vol. 3, No. 1. Child Development, Vol. 54, No. 4,
Fredrickson, B. 2003. “The Value of Posi­ 1041–53.
tive Emotions,” American Scientist, Vol. Howes, C. 1987. “Social Competence with
91, 330–35. Peers in Young Children: Developmen­
Greenspan, S., and N. T. Greenspan. 1985. tal Sequences,” Developmental Review,
First Feelings: Milestones in the Emo­ Vol. 7, 252–72.
tional Development of Your Baby and Howes, C. 1988. “Peer Interaction of Young
Child. New York: Penguin Books. Children,” Monographs of the Society for
Gustafson, G. E.; J. A. Green; and M. Research in Child Development, Vol. 53,
J. West. 1979. “The Infant’s Chang­ No. 1.
ing Role in Mother-Infant Games: The Howes, C., and C. C. Matheson. 1992.
Growth of Social Skills,” Infant Behavior “Sequences in the Development of
and Development, Vol. 2, 301–8. Competent Play with Peers: Social and
Harris, P. L. 2000. “Understanding Emo­ Social Pretend Play,” Developmental Psy­
tion,” in Handbook of Emotions (Second chology, Vol. 28, No. 5, 961–74.
edition). Edited by M. Lewis and J. M. Johnson, M., and others. 1991. “Newborns’
Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Preferential Tracking of Face-Like
Press. Stimuli and Its Subsequent Decline,”
Cognition, Vol. 40, Nos. 1-2, 1–19.


Johnstone, T., and K. R., Scherer. 2000. Lerner, C., and A. L. Dombro. 2000. Learn­
“Vocal Communication of Emotion,” in ing and Growing Together: Understand­
Handbook of Emotions (Second edition). ing and Supporting Your Child’s Develop­
Edited by M. Lewis and J. Haviland- ment. Washington, DC: Zero to Three
Jones. New York: Guilford Press. Press.
Kaler, S. R., and C. B. Kopp. 1990. “Com­ Levenson, R., and A. Ruef. 1992. “Empa­
pliance and Comprehension in Very thy: A Physiological Substrate,” Journal
Young Toddlers,” Child Development, of Personality and Social Psychology,
Vol. 61, No. 6, 1997–2003. Vol. 63, No. 2, 234–46.
Kaye, K., and A. Fogel. 1980. “The Tempo­ Levine, L. E. 1983. “Mine: Self-Definition
ral Structure of Face-to-Face Commu­ in 2-Year-Old Boys,” Developmental Psy­
nication Between Mothers and Infants,” chology, Vol. 19, 544–49.
Developmental Psychology, Vol. 16, Lewis, M. 2000a. “The Emergence of
454–64. Human Emotions,” in Handbook of
Kontos, S., and A. Wilcox-Herzog. January, Emotions (Second edition). Edited by M.
1997. “Research in Review: Teacher’s Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones. New
Interactions with Children: Why Are York: Guilford Press.
They So Important?” Young Children, Lewis, M. 2000b. “Self-Conscious Emo­
Vol. 52, No. 2, 4–12. tions: Embarrassment, Pride, Shame,
Kopp, C. 1989. “Regulation of Distress and and Guilt,” in Handbook of Emotions
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

Negative Emotions: A Developmental (Second edition). Edited by M. Lewis


View,” Developmental Psychology, Vol. and J. M. Haviland-Jones. New York:
25, No. 3, 343–54. Guilford Press.
Kravitz, H.; D. Goldenberg; and A. Neyhus. Lewis, M., and J. Brooks-Gunn. 1979.
1978. “Tactual Exploration by Normal Social Cognition and the Acquisition of
Infants,” Developmental Medicine and Self. New York: Plenum Press.
Child-Neurology, Vol. 20, No. 6, 720–26. Lewis, M., and others. 1989. “Self Devel­
Lagattuta, K. H., and R. A. Thompson. opment and Self-Conscious Emotions,”
2007. “The Development of Self-Con­ Child Development, Vol. 60, No. 1,
scious Emotions: Cognitive Processes 146–56.
and Social Influences,” in The Self-Con­ Lieberman, A. F. 1993. The Emotional Life
scious Emotions: Theory and Research. of the Toddler. New York: Free Press.
Edited by J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, and Marvin, R., and P. Britner. 1999. “Nor­
J. P. Tangney. New York: Guilford Press. mative Development: The Ontogeny of
Lally, J. R., and others. 1995. Caring for Attachment,” in Handbook of Attach­
Infants and Toddlers in Groups: Develop­ ment: Theory, Research, and Clinical
mentally Appropriate Practice. Washing­ Applications. Edited by J. Cassidy and
ton, DC: Zero to Three Press. P. Shaver. New York: Guilford Press.
Lamb, M. E.; M. H. Bornstein; and D. M. McCarty, M. E.; R. K. Clifton; and R. R.
Teti. 2002. Development in Infancy: An Collard. 1999. “Problem Solving in
Introduction (Fourth edition). Mahwah, Infancy: The Emergence of an Action
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Plan,” Developmental Psychology, Vol.
Lerner, C., and L. A. Ciervo. 2003. Healthy 35, No. 4, 1091–1101.
Minds: Nurturing Children’s Develop­ Meisels, S. J., and others. 2003. The
ment from 0 to 36 Months. Washington, Ounce Scale: Standards for the Develop­
DC: Zero to Three Press and American mental Profiles (Birth–42 Months). New
Academy of Pediatrics. York: Pearson Early Learning.


Melson, G., and A. Cohen. 1981. “Con­ Parritz, R. H. 1996. “A Descriptive Analy­
textual Influences on Children’s Activ­ sis of Toddler Coping in Challenging
ity: Sex Differences in Effects of Peer Circumstances,” Infant Behavior and
Presence and Interpersonal Attraction,” Development, Vol. 19, 171–80.
Genetic Psychology Monographs, Vol. Pruett, K. D. 1999. Me, Myself and I: How
103, 243–60. Children Build Their Sense of Self (18 to
Messinger, D., and A. Fogel. 2007. “The 36 Months). New York: Goddard Press.
Interactive Development of Social Smil­ Quann, V., and C. Wien. 2006. “The Vis­
ing,” in Advances in Child Development ible Empathy of Infants and Toddlers,”
and Behavior (Vol. 35). Edited by R. V. Young Children, Vol. 61, No. 4, 22–29.
Kail. Burlington, MA: Elsevier. Raikes, H. 1996. “A Secure Base for
Mesquita, B., and N. Frijda. 1992. “Cul­ Babies: Applying Attachment Concepts
tural Variations in Emotions: A to the Infant Care Setting,” Young Chil­
Review,” Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 112, dren, Vol. 51, No. 5, 59–67.
No. 2, 179–204. Raver, C. 2002. “Emotions Matter: Making
Mueller, E., and T. Lucas. 1975. “A Devel­ the Case for the Role of Young Chil­
opmental Analysis of Peer Interaction dren’s Emotional Development for Early
Among Toddlers,” in Friendship and School Readiness,” SRCD Social Policy
Peer Relations. Edited by M. Lewis and Report, Vol. 16, No. 3.
L. Rosenblum. New York: Wiley. Ross, H. S., and B. D. Goldman. 1977.

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
National Research Council and Institute of “Establishing New Social Relations in
Medicine. 2000. From Neurons to Neigh­ Infancy,” in Attachment Behavior. Edited
borhoods: The Science of Early Child­ by T. Alloway, P. Pliner, and L. Krames.
hood Development. Committee on Inte­ New York: Plenum Press.
grating the Science of Early Childhood Rothbart, M. K.; H. Ziaie; and C. G.
Development. Edited by J. P. Shonkoff O’Boyle. 1992. “Self-Regulation and
and D. A. Phillips. Board on Children, Emotion in Infancy,” in Emotion and Its
Youth and Families, Commission on Regulation in Early Development (No.
Behavioral and Social Sciences and 55). Edited by N. Eisenberg and R.
Education. Washington, DC: National Fabes. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass/
Academies Press. Pfeiffer.
National Scientific Council on the Devel­ Ryalls, B.; R. Gul; and K. Ryalls. 2000.
oping Child. Winter, 2004. “Children’s “Infant Imitation of Peer and Adult
Emotional Development Is Built into the Models: Evidence for a Peer Model
Architecture of Their Brains,” Work­ Advantage,” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
ing Paper No. 2. http://www.develop­ Vol. 46, No.1, 188–202.
ingchild.net (accessed on December 5, Saarni, C., and others. 2006. “Emotional
2006) Development: Action, Communication,
Panksepp, J. 2001. The Long-Term Psy­ and Understanding,” in Handbook of
chobiological Consequences of Infant Child Psychology (Sixth edition), Vol. 3,
Emotions: Prescriptions for the Twenty- Social, Emotional, and Personality Devel­
First Century,” Infant Mental Health opment. Edited by N. Eisenberg. Hobo-
Journal, Vol. 22, No. 1–2, 132–73. ken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.
Parks, S. 2004. Inside HELP: Hawaii Early Schaffer, H. R., and P. E. Emerson. 1964.
Learning Profile Administration and “The Development of Social Attach­
Reference Manual. Palo Alto, CA: VORT ments in Infancy,” Monographs of the
Corporation. Society for Research in Child Develop­
ment, Vol. 29, No. 3.
0

Segal, M. 2004. “The Roots and Fruits Teti, D. M. 1999. “Conceptualizations of


of Pretending,” in Children’s Play: The Disorganization in the Preschool Years:
Roots of Reading. Edited by E. F. Zigler, An Integration,” in Attachment Disorga­
D. G. Singer, and S. J. Bishop-Josef. nization. Edited by J. Solomon and C.
Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press. George. New York: Guilford Press.
Shonkoff, J. P. 2004. Science, Policy and Thelen, E., and A. Fogel. 1989. “Toward an
the Developing Child: Closing the Gap Action-Based Theory of Infant Develop­
Between What We Know and What We ment,” in Action in Social Context: Per­
Do. Washington, DC: Ounce of Preven­ spectives on Early Development. Edited
tion Fund. http://www.theounce.org/ by J. Lockman and N. Hazen. New
resources/publications (accessed on York: Plenum Press.
Thompson, R. A. 1987. “Empathy and
December 7, 2006)
Emotional Understanding: The Early
Siegel, D. J. 1999. The Developing Mind: Development of Empathy,” in Empa­
How Relationships and the Brain Inter­ thy and Its Development. Edited by N.
act to Shape Who We Are. New York: Eisenberg and J. Strayer. New York:
Guilford Press. Cambridge University Press.
Squires, J.; D. Bricker; and E. Twombly. Thompson, R. A. 2006. “The Development
2002. The Ages & Stages Question­ of the Person: Social Understanding,
naires: Social-Emotional. A Parent- Relationships, Self, Conscience,” in
SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT

Completed Child-Monitoring System for Handbook of Child Psychology (Sixth


Social-Emotional Behaviors ASQ:SE. edition), Volume 3: Social, Emotional,
User’s Guide. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. and Personality Development. Edited by
Brookes Publishing. N. Eisenberg. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley and
Sroufe, L. A. 1979. “Socioemotional Devel­ Sons.
opment,” in Handbook of Infant Develop­ Thompson, R. A., and R. Goodvin. 2005.
ment. Edited by J. Osofsky. New York: “The Individual Child: Temperament,
Wiley. Emotion, Self and Personality,” in
Stern, D. N. 1985. The Interpersonal World Developmental Science: An Advanced
of the Infant: A View from Psychoanaly­ Textbook (Fifth edition). Edited by M. H.
sis and Developmental Psychology. New Bornstein and M. E. Lamb. Mahwah,
York: Basic Books. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sternberg, C. R.; J. J. Campos; and R. N. Tronick, E. Z. 1989. “Emotions and Emo­
Emde. 1983. “The Facial Expression tional Communication in Infants,”
of Anger in Seven-Month-Old Infants,” American Psychologist, Vol. 44, No. 2,
Child Development, Vol. 54, 178–84. 112–19.
Stipek, D. J.; J. H. Gralinski; and C. B. Tsai, J.; B. Knutson; and H. Fung. 2006.
Kopp. 1990. “Self-Concept Development “Cultural Variation in Affect Valuation,”
in the Toddler Years,” Developmental Journal of Personality and Social Psy­
Psychology, Vol. 26, No. 6, 972–77. chology, Vol. 90, No. 2, 288–307.
Taumoepeau, M., and T. Ruffman. 2008. Tsai, J.; R. Levenson; and K. McCoy. 2006.
“Stepping Stones to Others’ Minds: “Cultural and Temperamental Variation
Maternal Talk Relates to Child Mental in Emotional Response,” Emotion, Vol.
State Language and Emotion Under­ 6, No. 3, 484–97.
standing at 15, 24, and 33 Months,” Tsai, J., and others. 2007. “Learning What
Child Development, Vol. 79, No. 2, Feelings to Desire: Socialization of Ideal
284–302. Affect Through Children’s Storybooks,”
Personality and Social Psychology Bul­
letin, Vol. 33, No. 1, 17–30.


Weinberg, M. K., and others. 1999. “Gen­ The Development of Prosocial Behavior.
der Differences in Emotional Expres­ Edited by N. Eisenberg. New York: Aca­
sivity and Self-Regulation During Early demic Press.
Infancy,” Developmental Psychology, Vol. Zahn-Waxler, C., and M. Radke-Yarrow.
35, No. 1, 175–88. 1990. “The Origins of Empathetic Con­
Wellman, H. M., and K. H. Lagattuta. cern,” Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 14,
2000. “Developing Understandings of 107–30.
Mind,” in Understanding Other Minds: Zahn-Waxler, C., and others. 1992. “Devel­
Perspectives from Developmental Cogni­ opment of Concern for Others,” Devel­
tive Neuroscience. Edited by S. Baron- opmental Psychology, Vol. 28, No. 1,
Cohen, T. Tager-Flusberg, and D. J. 126–36.
Cohen. New York: Oxford University Zahn-Waxler, C.; J. Robinson; and R.
Press. Emde. 1992. “Development of Empathy
Wingert, P., and M. Brant. 2005. “Reading in Twins,” Developmental Psychology,
Your Baby’s Mind,” Newsweek, Aug. 15, Vol. 28, No. 6, 1038–47.
2005, 32–39. Zero to Three. 2004. Infant and Early
Yuill, N. 1984. “Young Children’s Coordi­ Childhood Mental Health: Promoting
nation of Motive and Outcome in Judg­ Healthy Social and Emotional Develop­
ments of Satisfaction and Morality,” ment. Fact Sheet, May 18, 2004. Wash­
British Journal of Developmental Psy­ ington, DC: Zero to Three. http://www.

SOCIAL-EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
chology, Vol. 2, 73–81. zerotothree.org/policy/ (accessed on
Zahn-Waxler, C., and M. Radke-Yarrow. December 7, 2006).
1982. “The Development of Altruism:
Alternative Research Strategies,” in
Language Development

“T
he acquisition of language to as “parentese” or “motherese”) has
and speech seems decep­ qualities, notably its pitch or tone and
tively simple. Young chil­ sing-song-like rhythm, that distin­
dren learn their mother tongue rapidly guish it from adult-directed speech
and effortlessly, from babbling at six (Cooper and others 1997). Preverbal
months of age to full sentences by the infants communicate through eye
end of three years, and follow the same contact, facial expressions, gestures,
developmental path regardless of cul­ and sounds. Understanding language
ture.” (Kuhl 2004, 831) precedes speaking it (Bloom and oth­
As is true of human development in ers 1996). In addition, before being
infancy overall, language development able to use language effectively, infants
occurs in the context of relationships. acquire some understanding of the
Emotion and language development in social processes involved in commu­
the early years are linked, as “much of nication. They learn about the social
the form and content of communica­ aspects of communication through
tion between infants and their care­ engaging in turn-taking behavior in
givers in the first year of life depends proto-conversations with their par­
upon affective expression” (Bloom and ents or infant care teachers. In proto­
Capatides 1987, 1513). The relation­ conversations, the adult usually says
ship basis of early language develop­ something to the preverbal infant,
ment appears right at the beginning and the infant responds by making
of life. Newborns prefer the sounds of eye contact, cooing, smiling, showing
their mothers’ voices (DeCasper and lip and tongue movements, or waving
Fifer 1980). They also prefer the lan­ arms. These “conversation-like” con­
guage spoken by their mother during versations go back and forth between
her pregnancy (Moon, Cooper, and the adult and the infant for several
Fifer 1993). turns.
Adults typically modify their speech There is broad variability in lan­
when communicating with young guage development in its pattern and
infants. Research suggests that pace (Bloom and Capatides 1987).
infant-directed speech (also referred However, the process of early language




development is fundamentally the have been altered by linguistic experi­


same across cultures and languages. ence. Phonetic perception has changed
In describing early language develop­ dramatically to conform to the native-
ment, Kuhl (2002, 115) states: “One language pattern, and language-
of the puzzles in language develop­ specific speech production has
ment is to explain the orderly transi­ emerged.”
tion that all infants go through during
development. Infants the world over Receptive Language
achieve certain milestones in linguistic Infants excel at detecting patterns in
development at roughly the same time, spoken language (Kuhl 2000). The
regardless of the language they are literature indicates that infants’
exposed to.” speech perception abilities are strong.
Perceptual processes play an impor­ Not only do infants understand more
tant role in language development. As vocabulary than they are able to
Gogate, Walker-Andrews, and Bahrick produce, but they also demonstrate
(2001, 13) note: “A diverse set of exper­ awareness of the properties of the
imental findings suggests that early language or languages they are
lexical comprehension owes much to exposed to before they acquire words
infants’ developing ability to perceive (Ingram 1999). During the first six
intersensory relations in auditory- months of life, infants are better than
visual events,” [for example, speech]. adults at perceiving various types of
Experience also affects language contrasts in speech (Plunkett and
development from very early in life. Schafer 1999). Infants improve in their
One of the ways experience influences ability to discriminate the sounds
language development is through its characteristic of their native language
impact on perception early in infancy. while losing their abilities to discrimi­
Prior to infants’ first spoken words, or nate some sounds characteristic of
word comprehension, they have languages other than their native
already “come to recognize the percep­ language (Cheour and others 1998).
tual properties of their native lan­ According to Kuhl (2004), the way in
guage” (Kuhl 2002, 119). Infants are which the infant’s brain processes
learning about the prosodic or sound repeated experiences with speech
characteristics of their native lan­ explains language acquisition in a
guage: by nine months of age, English- social and biological context. Accord­
speaking infants demonstrate a prefer­ ing to this view, from early infancy
ence for the sound stress pattern young children use a mental filter to
characteristic of words in the English orient, with greater efficiency and
language (Jusczyk, Cutler, and Redanz accuracy, to the speech sounds char­
1993). Kuhl (2002, 112) concludes: “At acteristic of their native language. This
age one—prior to the time infants strategy enables infants to identify the
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

begin to master higher levels of lan­ phonemic units most useful to them in
guage, such as sound-meaning corre­ their native language and serves as a
spondences, contrastive phonology, building block to later word acquisition
and grammatical rules—infants’ per­ (Kuhl 2004).
ceptual and perceptual-motor systems


Expressive Language find creative ways to do so before they


have mastered words.
Infants use their expressive lan­
guage skills to make sounds or use
Communication Skills and
gestures or speech to begin to com­
municate. Even preverbal infants
Knowledge
use vocalizing or babbling to express Sensitivity to the timing of conver­
themselves. They also imitate the sational exchanges has been dem­
sounds and rhythm of adult speech. onstrated through research on back­
As they develop, infants generate and-forth communication involving
increasingly understandable sounds or young infants (Rochat, Querido, and
verbal communication. They demon­ Striano 1999). Infants use speech,
strate their expressive language abili­ gestures, and facial expressions as
ties by asking questions and respond­ well as direct their attention to com­
ing to them and repeating of sounds municate to others. As they grow, they
or rhymes. Children typically acquire increasingly understand the rules or
their first 50 words between the ages conventions of social communication.
of one and two (Ingram 1999). Kuczaj Infants also gain an expanded vocabu­
(1999, 145) notes: “The 24-month-old lary that helps them express them­
child with a productive vocabulary selves through words. As they develop,
between 50 and 600 words will easily infants benefit from communicating
quadruple or quintuple her vocabu­ with both peers and adults, very dif­
lary in the next year, and then add ferent conversational partners. Accord­
between 3000 and 4000 words per ing to Pan and Snow (1999, 231),
year to her productive vocabulary until “Interaction with peers, who are less
she graduates from high school.” competent and usually less coopera­
Infants’ use of nonverbal gestures as tive partners than adults, requires use
a form of communication appears to of more sophisticated conversational
be a typical feature of early language skills, such as knowing how and when
development, although there is consid­ to interrupt, how to remedy overlaps
erable variability among children and interruptions by others, and how
(Acredolo and Goodwyn 1988). The to make topic-relevant moves.” One
use of communicative gestures type of environment that typically
appears to generally precede the offers abundant opportunities for com­
child’s first words (Carpenter, Nagell, munication with both adult and child
and Tomasello 1998). Commenting on conversational partners is high-quality
the infant’s motivation to use gestures, child care settings.
Acredolo and Goodwyn (1997, 30) state
that the human infant has a special Interest in Print
capacity to communicate with ges­ Infants show an interest in print
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

tures. Acredolo and Goodwyn (1997) at first through physically exploring,


go on to say that typically developing such as putting books in their mouths,
infants seem so intent on communi­ handling books, or focusing on print
cating once they realize there is some­ in the environment around them.
body out there “listening” that they Turning the pages of books, looking at
6

books or pictures, asking for a favorite be considered one aspect of emergent


book or telling a favorite story with an literacy, the idea that literacy devel-
adult are other indicators of interest ops from early childhood rather than
in print. As infants grow older, mak- something that becomes relevant only
ing intentional marks on paper with a upon school entry (Whitehurst and
crayon or marker, pretending to read Lonigan 1998). Because early experi-
and write, repeating stories, repeating ences with print contribute to later
rhymes, recognizing images in books, literacy, shared book reading is recom-
noticing common symbols and words, mended as a valuable way to promote
and enjoying books are all related to emergent literacy (Whitehurst and
interest in print. Interest in print can Lonigan 1998).
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT


Foundation: Receptive Language


The developing ability to understand words

and increasingly complex utterances

8 months 18 months 36 months


At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, chil­
age, children show under­ children show understand­ dren demonstrate understanding of
standing of a small number ing of one-step requests that the meaning of others’ comments,
of familiar words and react have to do with the current questions, requests, or stories. (By
to the infant care teacher’s situation. 36 mos.; American Academy of
overall tone of voice. Pediatrics 2004, 307)

For example, the child For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
may:
• Smile and look toward the • Go to the cubby when the • Look for a stuffed bear when the
door when the infant care infant care teacher says that infant care teacher asks, “Where’s
teacher says, “Daddy’s it is time to put on coats to your bear?” (24–36 mos.; Coplan
here.” (Scaled score of 10 go outside. (Scaled score of 1993, 2–3; scaled score of 10 for
for 7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley 10 for 17:16 to 18:15 mos.; 34:16–35:15; Bayley 2006)
2006, 87) Bayley 2006, 90; 12–18 mos.; • Get the bin of blocks when the infant
Lerner and Ciervo 2003; 12
• Wave arms and kick legs in care teacher asks what the child
mos.; Coplan 1993, 2; by 24
excitement when the infant wants to play with. (24–36 mos.;
mos.; American Academy
care teacher says, “bottle.” Coplan 1993, 2–3; scaled score of 10
of Pediatrics 2004; 12 mos.;
(8 mos.; Meisels and others for 34:16–35:15; Bayley 2006)
Coplan 1993, 2; 24 mos.;
2003, 18) • Show understanding of words such
Meisels and others 2003, 46)
• Smile when the infant care as no, not, and don’t, and utterances
• Cover up the doll when the
teacher uses baby talk and such as when the infant care teacher
infant care teacher says,
make a worried face when says, “There’s no more milk,” or
“Cover the baby with the
she uses a stern voice. “Those don’t go there.” (24–36 mos.;
blanket.” (Scaled score of 10
(8 mos.; Meisels and others Parks 2004, p. 99)
for 17:16–18:15 mos.; Bayley
2003, 18; by end of 7 mos.; • Know the names of most objects in
2006, 90; 12–18 mos.; Lerner
American Academy of Pedi­ the immediate environment. (By 36
and Ciervo 2003; 12 mos.;
atrics 2004) mos.; American Academy of Pediat­
Coplan 1993, 2; by 24 mos.;
American Academy of Pediat­ rics 2004)
rics 2004) • Understand requests that include
• Go to the sink when the infant simple prepositions, such as, “Please
care teacher says that it is put your cup on the table,” or “Please
time to wash hands. (Scaled get your blanket out of your back­
score of 10 for 17:16–18:15 pack.” (By 36 mos.; Coplan 1993, 2;
mos.; Bayley 2006, 90; 12–18 by 36 mos.; American Academy of
mos.; Lerner and Ciervo 2003; Pediatrics 2004; 24–27 mos.; Parks
12 mos.; Coplan 1993, 2; by 2004, 97)
24 mos.; American Academy • Laugh when an adult tells a silly joke
of Pediatrics 2004; 24 mos.; or makes up rhymes with nonsense
Meisels and others 2003, 46) “words.” (By 36 mos.; American
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

• Get a tissue when the infant Academy of Pediatrics 2004, 307)


care teacher says, “Please go • Show understanding of the meaning
get a tissue. We need to wipe of a story by laughing at the funny
your nose.” (18 mos.; Meisels parts or by asking questions. (By 36
and others 2003, 36) mos.; American Academy of Pediat­
rics 2004, 307)
Chart continues on next page.


Receptive Language
Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child


During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
may:
• Follow one-step simple • Show understanding of pronouns,
• Vocalize in response to the requests if the infant care such as he, she, you, me, I, and
infant care teacher’s speech. teacher also uses a gesture it; for example, by touching own
(3–6 mos.; Parks 2004) to match the verbal request, nose when the infant care teacher
such as pointing to the blanket says, “Where’s your nose?” and
• Quiet down when hearing the
when asking the child to get it. then touching the infant care
infant care teacher’s voice.
(9 mos.; Coplan 1993, 2) teacher’s nose when he says,
(3–6 mos.; Parks 2004)
“And where’s my nose?” (19 mos.;
• Look up and momentarily stop
• Turn toward the window when Hart and Risley 1999, 61; 20–24
reaching into the mother’s
hearing a fire truck drive by. mos.; Parks 2004, 96)
purse when she says “no no.”
(4–6 mos.; Coplan 1993, 2)
(9–12 mos.; Parks 2004, 95) • Follow two-step requests about
• Quiet down and focus on the unrelated events, such as, “Put
• Show understanding of the
infant care teacher as he talks the blocks away and then go pick
names for most familiar
to the child during a diaper out a book.” (24 mos.; Coplan
objects and people. (Scaled
change. (4 mos.; Meisels and 1993, 2; by 24 mos.; American
score of 10 for 16:16–17:15
others 2003, 10) Academy of Pediatrics 2004, 270;
mos.; Bayley 2006, 90; 8–12
• Look at or turn toward the 24–29 mos.; Parks 2004, 104;
mos.; Parks 2004, 94)
infant care teacher who says three-part command by 36 mos.;
the child’s name. (Mean for American Academy of Pediatrics
5 mos.; Bayley 2006, 86; by 7 2004, 307)
mos.; American Academy of • Answer adults’ questions; for
Pediatrics 2004, 209; 9 mos.; example, communicate “apple”
Coplan 1993, 2; 12 mos.; when a parent asks what the child
Meisels and others 2003, 27; had for snack. (28 mos.; Hart and
5–7 mos.; Parks 2004) Risley 1999, 95)
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT


Foundation: Expressive Language


The developing ability to produce the sounds of language
and use vocabulary and increasingly complex utterances
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, children At around 36 months of age,
age, children experiment say a few words and use conven­ children communicate in a way
with sounds, practice making tional gestures to tell others about that is understandable to most
sounds, and use sounds or ges­ their needs, wants, and interests. (By adults who speak the same lan­
tures to communicate needs, 15 to 18 mos.; American Academy of guage they do. Children combine
wants, or interests. Pediatrics 2004 270; Coplan 1993, 1; words into simple sentences and
Hulit and Howard 2006, 142) demonstrate the ability to follow
some grammatical rules of the
home language. (By 36 mos.;
American Academy of Pediatrics
2004, 307; 30–36 mos.; Parks
2004; 24–36 mos.; Lerner and
Ciervo 2003; by 36 mos.; Hart
and Risley 1999, 67)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Vocalize to get the infant care • Look at a plate of crackers, then at • Use the past tense, though not
teacher’s attention. (6.5–8 the infant care teacher, and com­ always correctly; for example,
mos.; Parks 2004) municate “more.” (Scaled score of “Daddy goed to work,” “She
10 for 16:16–17:15; Bayley 2006; falled down.” (27–30 mos.;
• Repeat sounds when bab­
14–20 mos.; Parks 2004) Hulit and Howard 2006, 182;
bling, such as “da da da
30–36 mos.; Parks 2004;
da” or “ba ba ba ba.” (By 7 • Point to an airplane in the sky and
28 mos.; Hart and Risley 1999,
mos.; American Academy look at the infant care teacher.
95 and 129–30)
of Pediatrics 2004, 209; 6–7 (17.5–18.5 mos.; Parks 2004, 123)
mos.; Hulit and Howard 2006, • Use the possessive, though
• Use the same word to refer to
122; scaled score of 10 for not always correctly; for
similar things, such as “milk” while
7:16–8:15 on Bayley 2006, example, “That’s you car” or
indicating the pitcher, even though
106; 4–6.5 mos.; Parks 2004; “Her Megan.” (Scaled score
it is filled with juice. (18 mos.;
6 mos.; Locke 1993) of 10 for 34:16–35:15; Bayley
Meisels and others 2003, p. 37)
2006)
• Wave to the infant care • Use two words together, such as
teacher when he waves and • Use a few prepositions, such
“Daddy give.” (18 mos.; National
says, “bye-bye” as he leaves as “on” the table. (33-35.5
Research Council and Institute of
for his break. (6–9 mos.; Parks mos.; Parks 2004, p. 116)
Medicine 2000, 127)
2004, 121) • Talk about what she will do in
• Shake head “no” when offered
• Lift arms to the infant care the future, such as “I gonna
more food. (18 mos.; Meisels and
teacher to communicate a get a kitty.” (33–36 mos.; Hart
others 2003, 37)
desire to be held. (7–9 mos.; and Risley 1999, 131)
Coplan 1993, 3; 5–9 mos.; • Jabber a string of sounds into the
• Use 300–1000 words. (35+
Parks 2004, 121) toy telephone. (18 mos.; Meisels
mos.; Parks 2004, 116)
and others 2003, 37)
• Use the plural form of nouns,
• Gesture “all gone” by twisting
though not always correctly;
wrists to turn hands up and down
for example, “mans,” and
when finished eating lunch. (12–19
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

“mouses.” (By 36 mos.; Ameri­


mos.; Parks 2004, 122)
can Academy of Pediatrics
• Use made-up “words” to refer to 2004, 307; 28 mos.; Hart and
objects or experiences that only Risley 1999, 95)
familiar adults will know the mean­
• Express, “Uncle is coming to
ing of; for example “wo-wo” when
pick me up.” (36 mos.; Hoff
wanting to go next door to visit
2005)
the puppy. (12–22 mos.; Hulit and
Howard 2006, p. 130)

Chart continues on next page.


0

Expressive Language
Behaviors leading up to Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
the foundation ( to  foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
months)
During this period, the child During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
may: • Babble using the sounds of his • Tend to communicate about
• Squeal when excited. home language. (6–10 mos.; objects, actions, and events that
(5 mos.; Lerner and Ciervo Cheour and others 1998) are in the here and now. (12–22
2003; by 7 mos.; American mos.; Hulit and Howard 2006,
• Consistently use utterances to
Academy of Pediatrics 141)
refer to favorite objects or experi­
2004, 209) ences that only familiar adults • Use some words to refer to more
• Make an angry noise when know the meaning of; for example, than one thing; for example,
another child takes a toy. “ba ba ba ba” for blanket. (9 mos.; “night-night” to refer to bedtime
(5–6 mos.; Parks 2004) Bates, Camaioni, and Volterra or to describe darkness. (12–22
1975; 12 mos.; Coplan 1993, 3; mos.; Hulit and Howard 2006,
• Make a face of disgust to
12 mos.; Davies 2004, 166; 9–10 132)
tell the infant care teacher
mos.; Hulit and Howard 2006, 123)
that she does not want • Use many new words each day.
any more food. (6–9 mos.; • Express “Mama” or “Dada” when (18–20 mos.; Coplan, 1993, 1;
Lerner and Ciervo 2003) the mother or father, respectively, 18–24 mos.; Hulit and Howard
enters the room. (10 mos.; Coplan 2006, 137)
1993, 1) • Begin to combine a few words
• Say a first word clearly enough into mini-sentences to express
that the infant care teacher can wants, needs, or interests; for
understand the word within the example, “more milk,” “big
context; for example, “gih” for doggie,” “no night-night” or “go
give, “see,” “dis” for this, “cookie,” bye-bye.” (18–20 mos.; Coplan
“doggie,” “uh oh” and “no.” (Mean 1993, 1; 24 mos.; Meisels and
age 11 mos.; Hart and Risley others 2003, 47; by 24 mos.;
1999, 56) American Academy of Pediatrics
2004, 270; 18–24 mos.; Hulit and
• Name a few familiar favorite
Howard 2006, 143; scaled score
objects. (Around 12 mos.; Coplan
of 10 for 32:16–33:15; Bayley
1993, 3; mean age 13 mos., range
2006, 114; 20.5–24 mos.; Parks
9–16 mos.; Hulit and Howard
2004, 133)
2006, 132; between 10 and 15
mos.; National Research Council • Have a vocabulary of about 80
and Institute of Medicine 2000, words. (19 mos.; Hart and Risley
127) 1999, 61)
• Change tone when babbling, so • Start adding articles before
that the child’s babbles sound nouns, such as, “a book” or “the
more and more like adult speech. cup.” (20 mos.; Hart and Risley
(By 12 mos.; American Academy 1999, 63)
of Pediatrics 2004; 7.5–12 mos.; • Use own name when referring to
Parks, 2004; 7–8 mos.; Hulit and self. (18-24 mos.; Parks 2004)
Howard 2006, 123)
• Ask questions with raised intona­
• Use expressions; for example, tions at the end, such as “Doggy
“uh oh” when milk spills or when go?” (22–26 mos.; Hulit and
something falls off the table.
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

Howard 2006, 144)


(12.5–14.5 mos.; Parks 2004)
• Communicate using sentences
• Say “up” and lift arms to be picked of three to five words, such as
up by the infant care teacher. “Daddy go store?” or “Want
(Scaled score of 9 for 16:16–17:15 more rice.” (30 mos.; Coplan
mos.; Bayley 2006, 108; 12–14 1993, 1; 25 mos.; Hart and Risley
mos.; Parks 2004, 132) 1999, 63)


Foundation: Communication Skills and Knowledge


The developing ability to communicate nonverbally and verbally
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, chil­
children participate in back-and­ children use conventional ges­ dren engage in back-and-forth con­
forth communication and games. tures and words to communicate versations that contain a number of
meaning in short back-and-forth turns, with each turn building upon
interactions and use the basic what was said in the previous turn.
rules of conversational turn- (Hart and Risley 1999, 122)
taking when communicating.
(Bloom, Rocissano, and Hood
1976)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Put arms up above head when • Respond to the infant care • Persist in trying to get the infant
the infant care teacher says, teacher’s initiation of conver­ care teacher to respond by
“soooo big.” (8 mos.; Meisels sation through vocalizations repeating, speaking more loudly,
and others 2003, 19) or nonverbal communication. expanding on what the child
(12–19 mos.; Hart and Risley said, or touching the infant care
• Try to get the infant care
1999, 37) teacher. (After 30 mos.; Hart and
teacher to play peek-a-boo
Risley 1999, 38)
by hiding her face behind a • Initiate interactions with the in­
blanket, uncovering her face, fant care teacher by touching, • Repeat part of what the adult just
and laughing. (8 mos.; Meisels vocalizing, or offering a toy. said in order to continue the con­
and others 2003, 19) (12–19 mos.; Hart and Risley versation. (31–34 mos.; Hulit and
1999, 37) Howard 2006, 186; by 24 mos.;
• Pull the infant care teacher’s
American Academy of Pediatrics
hands away from his face • Jabber into a toy phone and
2004)
during a game of peek-a­ then pause, as if to listen to
boo. (Scaled score of 11 for someone on the other end. (18 • Make comments in a conversa­
7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, mos.; Meisels and others 2003, tion that the other person has
106) 37) difficulty understanding; for
example, suddenly switch topics
• Try to clap hands to get the • Shake head or express “no”
or use pronouns without making
infant care teacher to continue when the infant care teacher
clear what is being talked about.
playing pat-a-cake. (8 mos.; asks if the child is ready to go
(31–34 mos.; Hulit and Howard
Meisels and others 2003, 19) back inside. (18 mos.; Meisels
2006, 192)
and others 2003, 37)
• Make sounds when the infant
• Answer adults’ questions, such
care teacher is singing a song. • Respond to the infant care
as “What’s that?” and “Where
(8 mos.; Meisels and others teacher’s comment about a toy
did it go?” (31–34 mos.; Hulit and
2003, 19) with an additional, but related,
Howard 2006, 185; 24–36 mos.;
action or comment about the
• Interact with the infant care Parks 2004)
same toy; for example, make a
teacher while singing a song
barking sound when the infant • Begin to create understandable
with actions or while doing
care teacher pats a toy dog topics for a conversation partner.
finger plays. (Scaled score of
and says, “Nice doggie.” (By
11 for 8:16–9:15 mos.; Bayley • Sometimes get frustrated if the
18 mos.; Bloom, Rocissano,
2006) infant care teacher does not un­
and Hood 1976) derstand what the child is trying
to communicate. (28.5–36 mos.;
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

Parks 2004, 129)


• Participate in back-and-forth
interaction with the infant care
teacher by speaking, giving feed­
back, and adding to what was
originally said. (29–36 mos.; Hart
and Risley 1999, 36, 39–40)

Chart continues on next page.




Communication Skills and Knowledge


Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Respond with babbling when • Copy the infant care teacher in • Ask and answer simple ques­
the infant care teacher asks waving “bye-bye” to a parent tions, such as “What’s that?” (19
a question. (Hart and Risley as he leaves the room. (Scaled mos.; Hart and Risley 1999, 61)
1999, 55) score of 9 for 12:16–13:15 • Say, “huh?” when interacting
mos.; Bayley 2006, No. 14,
• Laugh when a parent nuzzles with the infant care teacher to
88; 8 mos.; Meisels and others
her face in the child’s belly, vo­ keep interaction going. (19 mos.;
2003, 19)
calizes expectantly when she Hart and Risley 1999, 62)
pulls back, and laugh when she • Purse lips after hearing and • Repeat or add on to what
nuzzles again. (3–6 mos.; Parks seeing the infant care teacher she just said if the infant care
2004, 11) make a sputtering sound with teacher does not respond right
her lips. (9 mos.; Apfel and
• Move body in a rocking motion away. (20–28 mos.; Hart and
Provence 2001)
to get the infant care teacher Risley 1999, 105)
to continue rocking. (4-5 mos.; • Repeat the last word in an • Engage in short back-and-forth
Parks 2004, 57) adult’s question in order to interactions with a family mem­
continue the conversation; for
• Babble back and forth with ber by responding to comments,
example, saying “dat” after the
the infant care teacher during questions, and prompts. (20–28
infant care teacher asks, “What
diaper change. (5.5–6.5 mos.; mos.; Hart and Risley 1999, 39)
is that?” (11–16 mos.; Hart and
Parks 2004, 125)
Risley 1999, 83) • Respond almost immediately
after a parent finishes talking in
• Respond with “yes” or “no” order to continue the interaction.
when asked a simple question. (20–28 mos.; Hart and Risley
(11–16 mos.; Hart and Risley 1999, 97)
1999, 83)
• Get frustrated if the infant care
• Hold out a toy for the infant teacher does not understand
care teacher to take and then what the child is trying to com­
reach out to accept it when municate. (24–28.5 mos.; Parks
the infant care teacher offers it 2004)
back. (12–15 mos.; Parks 2004,
122) • Attempt to continue conversa­
tion, even when the adult does
• Show an understanding that not understand him right away,
a conversation must build on by trying to use different words
what the other partner says; for to communicate the meaning.
example, expressing, “bear” (27–30 mos.; Hulit and Howard
when the infant care teacher 2006, 182)
points to the stuffed bear and
asks, “What’s that?” (16 mos.; • Sustain conversation about
Hart and Risley 1999, 81) one topic for one or two turns,
usually about something that
• Initiate back-and-forth interac­ is in the here and now. (20–28
tion with the infant care teacher mos.; Hart and Risley 1999, 107;
by babbling and then waiting 27–30 mos.; Hulit and Howard
for the infant care teacher to 2006, 182)
respond before babbling again.
• Respond verbally to adults’
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

(11–19 mos.; Hart and Risley


1999, 77; 12 mos.; Meisels and questions or comments. (27–30
others 2003, 27) mos.; Hulit and Howard 2006,
182)
• Say “mmm” when eating, after
a parent says, “mmm.” (11–19
mos.; Hart and Risley 1999, 78)


Foundation: Interest in Print


The developing interest in engaging with print
in books and in the environment
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
children explore books and children listen to the adult and children show appreciation for
show interest in adult-initi­ participate while being read to by books and initiate literacy activi­
ated literacy activities, such as pointing, turning pages, or mak­ ties: listening, asking questions, or
looking at photos and explor­ ing one- or two-word comments. making comments while being read
ing books together with an Children actively notice print in the to; looking at books on their own;
adult. (Scaled score of 10 for environment. or making scribble marks on paper
7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, and pretending to read what is writ­
57; infants; National Research ten. (Schickedanz and Casbergue
Council 1999, 28) 2004, 11)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Point to or indicate an object • Attempt to turn the pages of a • Enjoy both being read to and
that he would like the infant paper book, sometimes turning looking at books by himself.
care teacher to pay attention more than one page at a time. (30–36 mos.; Parks 2004)
to. (15–18 mos.; Parks 2004) • Pretend to read books to stuffed
• Look intently at photographs • Pretend to read the back of a animals by telling a story that
of classmates when the infant cereal box while sitting at the is related to the pictures and
care teacher talks about the kitchen table in the house area. turning the book around to
pictures. (8–9 mos.; Parks (15–18 mos.; Parks 2004, 27) show the picture to the stuffed
2004, 71) animals, just as the infant care
• Recognize a favorite book by
teacher does when reading to a
• Look at pictures that a par­ its cover. (Toddler; National
small group of children. (Ehri and
ent points to while reading a Research Council 1999, 28)
Sweet 1991, 199; 24–36 mos.;
storybook. (Scaled score of • Pull the infant care teacher Sulzby 1985)
10 for 7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley by the hand to the bookshelf,
2006, 57; infants; National • Talk about the trip to the library
point, and express “book” to
Research Council 1999, 28) and ask about the next trip. (35
get the infant care teacher to
mos.; Hart and Risley 1999, 128)
• Hold a book and try to turn read a story. (12–18 mos.;
the pages. (Scaled score of Lerner and Ciervo 2003) • Recite much of a favorite book
10 for 7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley from memory while “reading”
• Point to or indicate a familiar
2006, 57) it to others or self. (36 mos.;
sign in the neighborhood.
National Research Council 1999,
28)
• Try to be careful with books.
(By 36 mos.; National Research
Council 1999, 3)

Chart continues on next page. LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT




Interest in Print
Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)

During this period, the child During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
may: • Try to turn the pages of a paper • Move behind the infant care
• Chew on a board book. (Inter­ book, turning several pages at teacher in order to look over
national Reading Association one time. (scaled score of 10 for her shoulder at the pictures,
and the National Association 9:16–10:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, when there are several children
for the Education of Young 128) crowded around. (18–24 mos.;
Children 1998, 198; 3–6 mos.; Parks 2004, 68)
• Scribble with a crayon. (Scaled
Parks 2004) score of 10 for 12:16–13:15 • Turn the pages of a book one by
mos.; Bayley 2006, 129) one. (18–24 mos.; Parks 2004)
• Smile and point to or indicate • Listen as a family member
pictures of favorite animals in a reads short picture books aloud.
book. (10–14 mos.; Parks 2004) (Scaled score of 10 for 21:15–
22:16 mos.; Bayley 2006, 67;
• Help the infant care teacher turn
27–30 mos.; Parks 2004)
a page of a book. (14–15 mos.;
Parks 2004) • Ask a question about a story; for
example, “Bear go?” while turn­
• Use an open hand to pat a pic­
ing from one page to the next.
ture while reading with a family
(24 mos.; Meisels and others
member. (14–15 mos.; Parks
2003, 47)
2004)
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT


References

Acredolo, L., and S. Goodwyn. 1988. Cheour, M., and others. 1998. “Develop­
“Symbolic Gesturing in Normal ment of Language-Specific Phoneme
Infants,” Child Development, Vol. 59, Representations in the Infant Brain,”
450–66. Nature Neuroscience, Vol. 1, 351–53.
Acredolo, L., and S. Goodwyn. 1997. “Fur­ Cooper, R. P., and others. 1997. “The
thering our Understanding of What Development of Infants’ Preference for
Humans Understand,” Human Develop­ Motherese,” Infant Behavior and Devel­
ment, Vol. 40, 25–31. opment, Vol. 20, No. 4, 477–88.
American Academy of Pediatrics. 2004. Coplan, J. 1993. Early Language Milestone
Caring for Your Baby and Young Child: Scale: Examiner’s Manual (Second edi­
Birth to Age 5 (Fourth edition). Edited tion). Austin, TX: Pro-ed.
by S. P. Shelov and R. E. Hannemann. Davies, D. 2004. Child Development: A
New York: Bantam Books. Practitioner’s Guide (Second edition).
Apfel, N. H., and S. Provence. 2001. Man­ New York: Guilford Press.
ual for the Infant-Toddler and Family DeCasper, A., and W. Fifer. 1980. “On
Instrument (ITFI). Baltimore, MD: Paul Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer
H. Brookes Publishing. Their Mothers’ Voices,” Science, Vol.
Bates, E.; L. Camaioni; and V. Volterra. 208, 1174–76.
1975. “ The Acquisition of Performatives Ehri, L., and J. Sweet. 1991. “Finger­
Prior to Speech,” Merrill-Palmer Quar­ point-Reading of Memorized Text: What
terly, Vol. 21, 205–26. Enables Beginners to Process the
Bayley, N. 2006. Bayley Scales of Infant Print?” Reading Research Quarterly,
and Toddler Development (Third edi­ Vol. 26, 442–62.
tion). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Gogate, L.; A. Walker-Andrews; and
Assessment Inc. L. Bahrick. 2001. “The Intersensory
Bloom, L., and J. Capatides. 1987. Origins of Word Comprehension: An
“Expression of Affect and the Emer­ Ecological-Dynamic Systems View,”
gence of Language,” Child Development, Developmental Science, Vol. 4, No. 1,
Vol. 58, 1513–22. 1–37.
Bloom, L., and others. 1996. “Early Con­ Hart, B., and T. R. Risley. 1999. The Social
versations and Word Learning: Contri­ World of Children: Learning to Talk.
butions from Child and Adult,” Child Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Pub­
Development, Vol. 67, 3154–75. lishing Co.
Bloom, L.; L. Rocissano; and L. Hood. Hoff, E. 2005. Language Development
1976. “Adult-Child Discourse: Develop­ (Third edition). Belmont, CA: Wad­
mental Interaction Between Information sworth/Thomson Learning.
Processing and Linguistic Knowledge,” Hulit, L. M., and M. R. Howard. 2006.
Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 8, 521–52. Born to Talk: An Introduction to Speech
Carpenter, M.; K. Nagell; and M. Toma­ and Language Development. New York:
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

sello. 1998. “Social Cognition, Joint Pearson Education, Inc.


Attention, and Communicative Com­ Ingram, D. 1999. “Phonological Acquisi­
petence from 9 to 15 Months of Age,” tion” in The Development of Language.
Monographs of the Society for Research Edited by M. Barrett. East Sussex, UK:
in Child Development, Vol. 63, Serial No. Psychology Press.
255, No. 4, 1–33.
6

International Reading Association and the National Research Council. 1999. Starting
National Association for the Education Out Right: A Guide to Promoting Chil­
of Young Children. 1998. “Learning dren’s Reading Success. Washington,
to Read and Write: Developmentally DC: National Academy Press.
Appropriate Practices for Young Chil­ National Research Council and Institute of
dren,” The Reading Teacher, Vol. 52, No. Medicine. 2000. From Neurons to Neigh­
2, 193–216. borhoods: The Science of Early Child­
Jusczyk, J.; A. Cutler; and N. J. Redanz. hood Development. Committee on Inte­
1993. “Infants’ Preference for the Pre­ grating the Science of Early Childhood
dominant Stress Patterns of English Development. Edited by J. P. Shonkoff
Words,” Child Development, Vol. 64, and D. A. Phillips. Washington, DC:
675–87. National Academies Press.
Kuczaj, S. 1999. “The World of Words: Pan, B., and C. Snow. 1999. “The Develop­
Thoughts on the Development of a Lexi­ ment of Conversational and Discourse
con,” in The Development of Language. Skills,” in The Development of Lan­
Edited by M. Barrett. East Sussex, UK: guage. Edited by M. Barrett. East Sus-
Psychology Press. sex, UK: Psychology Press.
Kuhl, P. K. 2000. “A New View of Language Parks, S. 2004. Inside HELP: Hawaii Early
Acquisition,” Proceedings of the National Learning Profile: Administration and
Academy of Sciences of the United Reference Manual. Palo Alto, CA: VORT
States, Vol. 97, No. 22, 11850–857. Corporation.
Kuhl, P. K. 2002. “Speech, Language and Plunkett, K., and G. Schafer. 1999. “Early
Developmental Change,” in Emerg­ Speech Perception and Word Learning,”
ing Cognitive Abilities in Early Infancy. in The Development of Language. Edited
Edited by F. Lacerda, C. von Hofsten, by M. Barrett. East Sussex, UK: Psy­
and M. Heimann. Mahwah, NJ: Law­ chology Press.
rence Erlbaum Associates. Rochat, P.; J. Querido; and T. Striano.
Kuhl, P. K. 2004. “Early Language Acqui­ 1999. “Emerging Sensitivity to the Tim­
sition: Cracking the Speech Code,” ing and Structure of Protoconversation
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Vol. 5, in Early Infancy,” Developmental Psy­
831–43. chology, Vol. 35, No. 4, 950–57.
Lerner, C., and L. A. Ciervo. 2003. Healthy Schickedanz, J. A., and R. M. Casbergue.
Minds: Nurturing Children’s Develop­ 2004. Writing in Preschool: Learning to
ment from 0 to 36 Months. Washington, Orchestrate Meaning and Marks. New­
DC: Zero to Three Press and American ark, DE: International Reading Asso­
Academy of Pediatrics. ciation.
Locke, J. L. 1993. The Child’s Path to Spo­ Sulzby, E. 1985. “Children’s Emergent
ken Language. Cambridge, MA: Har­ Reading of Favorite Storybooks:
vard University Press. A Developmental Study,” Reading
Meisels, S. J., and others. 2003. The Research Quarterly, Vol. 20, 458–81.
Ounce Scale: Standards for the Develop­ Whitehurst, G., and C. Lonigan. 1998.
mental Profiles (Birth–42 Months). New “Child Development and Emergent Lit­
York: Pearson Early Learning. eracy,” Child Development, Vol. 69, No.
LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

Moon, C.; R. Cooper; and W. Fifer. 1993. 3, 848–72.


“Two-Day-Olds Prefer their Native
Language,” Infant Behavior and Develop­
ment, Vol. 16, 495–500.


LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
Cognitive Development

“T
he last two decades of members, friends, teachers, and care­
infancy research have seen givers play a vital role in supporting
dramatic changes in the the cognitive development of infants by
way developmental psychologists char­ providing the healthy interpersonal or
acterize the earliest stages of cognitive social-emotional context in which
development. The infant, once regarded cognitive development unfolds. Caring,
as an organism driven mainly by sim­ responsive adults provide the base
ple sensorimotor schemes, is now seen from which infants can fully engage in
as possessing sophisticated cognitive behaviors and interactions that pro­
skills and even sophisticated concepts mote learning. Such adults also serve
that guide knowledge acquisition” as a prime source of imitation.
(Madole and Oakes 1999, 263). Cultural context is important to
“What we see in the crib is the great­ young children’s cognitive develop­
est mind that has ever existed, the ment. There is substantial variation
most powerful learning machine in in how intelligence is defined within
the universe” (Gopnik, Meltzoff, and different cultures (Sternberg and
Kuhl 1999, 1). Grigorenko 2004). As a result, dif­
The term cognitive development ferent aspects of cognitive function­
refers to the process of growth and ing or cognitive performance may be
change in intellectual/mental abilities more highly valued in some cultural
such as thinking, reasoning and contexts than in others. For example,
understanding. It includes the acquisi­ whereas processing speed is an aspect
tion and consolidation of knowledge. of intelligence that is highly valued
Infants draw on social-emotional, within the predominant Western con­
language, motor, and perceptual ceptualizations of intelligence, “Ugan­
experiences and abilities for cognitive dan villagers associate intelligence
development. They are attuned to with adjectives such as slow, careful,
relationships between features of and active” (Rogoff and Chavajay 1995,
objects, actions, and the physical 865.). Aspects of intelligence that have
environment. But they are particularly to do with social competence appear to
attuned to people. Parents, family be seen as more important than speed


60

in some non-Western cultural contexts Spatial Relationships


(Sternberg and Grigorenko 2004). Cer­ Infants learn about spatial relation­
tainly, it is crucial for early childhood ships in a variety of ways; for example,
professionals to recognize the role that
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

exploring objects with their mouths,


cultural context plays in defining and tracking objects and people visually,
setting the stage for children’s healthy squeezing into tight spaces, fitting
cognitive functioning. objects into openings, and looking
Research has identified a broad at things from different perspectives
range of cognitive competencies and (Mangione, Lally, and Signer 1992).
described the remarkable progres­ They spend much of their time explor­
sion of cognitive development during ing the physical and spatial aspects
the early childhood years. Experts in of the environment, including the
the field describe infants as active, characteristics of, and interrelation­
motivated, and engaged learners who ships between, the people, objects, and
possess an impressive range of cogni­ the physical space around them (Cle­
tive competencies (National Research ments 2004). The development of an
Council and Institute of Medicine understanding of spatial relationships
2000) and learn through explora­ increases infants’ knowledge of how
tion (Whitehurst and Lonigan 1998). things move and fit in space and the
Infants demonstrate natural curiosity. properties of objects (their bodies and
They have a strong drive to learn and the physical environment).
act accordingly. In fact, they have been
described as “born to learn” (National Problem Solving
Research Council and Institute of Infants exhibit a high level of inter­
Medicine 2000, 148). est in solving problems. Even very
young infants will work to solve a
Cause-and-Effect problem, for example, how to find
Everyday experiences—for example, their fingers in order to suck on them
crying and then being picked up or (National Research Council and Insti­
waving a toy and then hearing it rat­ tute of Medicine 2000). Older infants
tle—provide opportunities for infants may solve the problem of how to reach
to learn about cause and effect. “Even an interesting toy that is out of reach
very young infants possess expecta­ by trying to roll toward it or by ges­
tions about physical events” (Baillar­ turing to an adult for help. Infants
geon 2004, 89). This knowledge helps and toddlers solve problems by varied
infants better understand the proper­ means, including physically acting on
ties of objects, the patterns of human objects, using learning schemes they
behavior, and the relationship between have developed, imitating solutions
events and the consequences. Through found by others, using objects or other
developing an understanding of cause people as tools, and using trial and
and effect, infants build their abilities error.
to solve problems, to make predictions,
and to understand the impact of their
behavior on others.
6

Imitation importance of early imitation in the


Imitation is broadly understood to following manner: “Modern research
be a powerful way to learn. It has been has shown imitation to be a natural
mechanism of learning and communi­

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
identified as crucial in the acquisition
of cultural knowledge (Rogoff 1990) cation which deserves to be at centre
and language. Imitation by newborns stage in developmental psychology.”
has been demonstrated for adult facial
expressions (Meltzoff and Moore 1983), Memory
head movements, and tongue protru­ The capacity to remember allows
sions (Meltzoff and Moore 1989). “The infants and toddlers to differentiate
findings of imitation in human new­ between familiar and unfamiliar peo­
borns highlighted predispositions to ple and objects, anticipate and partici­
imitate facial and manual actions, pate in parts of personal care routines,
vocalizations and emotionally laden learn language, and come to know the
facial expressions” (Bard and Russell rules of social interaction. The infant’s
1999, 93). Infant imitation involves memory system is quite remarkable
perception and motor processes (Melt­ and functions at a higher level than
zoff and Moore 1999). The very early was previously believed (Howe and
capacity to imitate makes possible Courage 1993). Although age is not the
imitation games in which the adult only determinant of memory function­
mirrors the child’s behavior, such as ing, as infants get older they are able
sticking out one’s tongue or matching to retain information for longer periods
the pitch of a sound the infant makes, of time (Bauer 2004). Infants exhibit
and then the infant imitates back. long-term recall well before they are
This type of interaction builds over able to articulate their past experi­
time as the infant and the adult add ences verbally (Bauer 2002b).
elements and variations in their imita­ The emergence of memory is related
tion games. to the development of a neural net­
Infants engage in both immediate work with various components (Bauer
imitation and delayed imitation. Imme­ 2002b). Commenting on the dif­
diate imitation occurs when infants ferent forms and functions of early
observe and immediately attempt to memory development, Bauer (2002a,
copy or mimic behavior. For example, 131) states: “It is widely believed that
immediate imitation can be seen memory is not a unitary trait but
when an infant’s parent sticks out his is comprised of different systems or
tongue and the infant sticks out his processes, which serve distinct func­
tongue in response. As infants develop, tions, and are characterized by funda­
they are able to engage in delayed imi­ mentally different rules of operation.”
tation, repeating the behavior of others Bauer (2002a, 145) later adds that
at a later time after having observed recent research counters earlier sug­
it. An example of delayed imitation is gestions that preschool-aged children
a child reenacting part of a parent’s demonstrate little memory capac­
exercise routine, such as lifting a block ity and to speculations that younger
several times as if it were a weight. children and infants demonstrate little
Butterworth (1999, 63) sums up the or no memory capacity. Bauer (2002a,
6

145) concludes: “It is now clear that object. Whether early number sensi­
from early in life, the human organism tivity is solely perceptual in nature
stores information over the long term or also numerical in nature, develop­
and that the effects of prior experience mental theorists agree that it sets the
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

are apparent in behavior. In the first foundation for the later development
months of life, infants exhibit recogni­ of children’s understanding of number
tion memory for all manner of natural and quantity.
and artificial stimuli.” As children’s understanding and use
of language increases, they begin to
Number Sense assimilate language based on number
Number sense refers to children’s knowledge to their nonverbal knowl­
concepts of numbers and the rela­ edge of number and quantity (Baroody
tionships among number concepts. 2004). Between 18 and 24 months of
Research findings indicate that infants age, children use relational words to
as young as five months of age are indicate “more” or “same” as well as
sensitive to number and are able to number words. They begin to count
discriminate among small sets of up aloud, typically starting with “one”
to three objects (Starkey and Cooper and continuing with a stream of num­
1980; Starkey, Spelke, and Gelman ber names (Fuson 1988; Gelman and
1990). Infants demonstrate the abil­ Gallistel 1978), although they may
ity to quickly and accurately recognize omit some numbers and not use the
the quantity in a small set of objects conventional number list (e.g. “one,
without counting. This ability is called two, three, seven, nine, ten”). Around
subitizing. the same age, children also begin to
According to one theoretical per­ count small collections of objects; how­
spective, infants’ abilities to discrimi­ ever, they may point to the same item
nate among numbers, for example, twice or say a number word without
two versus three objects, does not pointing to an object. And they begin
reflect “number knowledge.” Rather, to construct an understanding of
this early skill appears to be based on cardinality (i.e., the last number word
infants’ perceptual abilities to “see” is used when counting represents the
small arrangements of number (Cle­ total number of objects).
ments 2004; Carey 2001), or on their
ability to notice a change in the gen­ Classification
eral amount of objects they are seeing Classification refers to the infant’s
(Mix, Huttenlocher, and Levine 2002). developing ability to group, sort, cat­
The alternative view is that the infant’s egorize, connect, and have expecta­
early sensitivity to number is numeri­ tions of objects and people according
cal in nature. In other words, infants to their attributes. Three-month-olds
have a capacity to distinguish among demonstrate that they expect people to
numbers and to reason about these act differently than objects (Legerstee
numbers in numerically meaningful 1997). They also demonstrate the abil­
ways (Wynn 1998; Gallistel and Gel- ity to discriminate between smiling
man 1992). In some sense, they know and frowning expressions (Barrera
that three objects are more than one and Maurer 1981). Mandler (2000)
6

distinguishes between two types of and make sense of past experiences.


categorization made by infants: per­ Research suggests that engaging in
ceptual and conceptual. Perceptual pretend play appears to be related to
categorization has to do with similari­ young children’s developing under­

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
ties or differences infants sense, such standing of other people’s feelings and
as similarities in visual appearance. beliefs (Youngblade and Dunn 1995).
Conceptual categorization has to do Outdoor environments, such as sand­
with grouping based on what objects boxes (Moser 1995) or play structures,
do or how they act. According to Mare­ offer rich opportunities for symbolic
schal and French (2000, 59), “the play or pretending. Although outdoor
ability to categorize underlies much of play areas are often considered most
cognition.” Classification is a funda­ in terms of motor behavior or physical
mental skill in both problem solving activity, they also offer special oppor­
and symbolic play. tunities for symbolic play (Perry 2003).
For example, children playing outside
Symbolic Play may pretend to garden or may use a
Symbolic play is a common early large wheeled toy to reenact going on a
childhood behavior also called “pre­ shopping trip.
tend play, make-believe play, fantasy
play . . . or imaginative play” (Gowen Attention Maintenance
1995, 75). Representational thinking Attention maintenance has been
is a core component of symbolic play. described as a form of cognitive self-
At around eight months of age, infants regulation. It refers to the infant’s
have learned the functions of common growing ability to exercise control over
objects (for example, holding a play his attention or concentration (Bron­
telephone to “hear” Grandma’s voice). son 2000). Attention maintenance
By the time children are around 18 permits infants to gather information,
months of age, they use one object to to sustain learning experiences, to
stand for, or represent, another. For observe, and to problem-solve. Infants
example, an 18-month-old may pre­ demonstrate attention maintenance
tend a banana is a telephone. At when they attend to people, actions,
around 36 months, children engage in and things they find interesting even
make-believe play in which they repre­ in the presence of distractions. The
sent an object without having that ability to maintain attention/concen­
object, or a concrete substitute, avail­ tration is an important self-regula­
able. For example, they may make a tory skill related to learning. There is
“phone call” by holding their hand up significant variability in attentiveness
to their ear. even among typically developing chil­
As children approach 36 months of dren (Ruff and Rothbart 1996).
age, they increasingly engage in pre­
tend play in which they reenact famil­ Understanding of Personal
iar events. Make-believe play allows Care Routines
older infants to try to better under­ Personal care activities are a rou­
stand social roles, engage in com­ tine part of the young child’s daily life.
munication with others, and revisit They also present significant opportu-
6

nities for learning in both child care personal care routines and anticipat­
settings and at home. Infants’ growing ing next steps are skills related to
abilities to anticipate, understand, and the cognitive foundations of atten­
participate in these routines represent tion maintenance, imitation, memory,
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

a significant aspect of their cognitive cause-and-effect, and problem solv­


functioning, one related to their abili­ ing. The cultural perspectives of the
ties to understand their relationships adults who care for infants are related
with others, their abilities to take care to their expectations for the degree of
of themselves, and their skills in group independence or self-initiation children
participation. At first, young infants demonstrate during personal care
respond to the adult’s actions during routines. Depending on their cultural
these routines. Then they begin to par­ experiences, children may vary greatly
ticipate more actively (O’Brien 1997). in their understanding of personal
Understanding the steps involved in care routines.
6

Foundation: Cause-and-Effect
The developing understanding that one event brings about another

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, chil­ At around 36 months of age,
children perform simple actions to dren combine simple actions to children demonstrate an under­
make things happen, notice the cause things to happen or change standing of cause and effect by
relationships between events, and the way they interact with objects making predictions about what
notice the effects of others on the and people in order to see how it could happen and reflect upon
immediate environment. changes the outcome. what caused something to hap­
pen. (California Department of
Education [CDE] 2005)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Shake a toy, hear the sound it • Try to wind the handle of a pop­ • Communicate, “She misses
makes, and then shake it again. up toy after not being able to her mommy” when a child
(5.5–8 mos.; Parks 2004, 58) open the top. (15 mos.; Brazel­ cries after her mother leaves in
ton 1992, 161) the morning.
• Loudly bang a spoon on the
table, notice the loud sound, • Drop different objects from vari­ • Make a prediction about what
and do it again. (By 7 mos.; ous heights to see how they fall will happen next in the story
American Academy of Pediatrics and to hear the noise they make when the infant care teacher
2004, 210; 8 mos.; Meisels and when they land. (12–18 mos.; asks, “What do you think will
others 2003, 21) Ginsburg and Opper 1988, 56) happen next?”
• Watch the infant care teacher • Build a tower with the big card­ • Answer the infant care teacher
wind up a music box and, when board blocks and kick it over to when she asks, “What do you
the music stops, touch her hand make it fall, then build it again think your mom’s going to
to get her to make it start again. and knock it down with a hand. say when you give her your
(5–9 mos.; Parks 2004, 58) (18 mos.; Meisels and others picture?”
2003, 37)
• Splash hands in water and • See a bandage on a peer’s
notice how his face gets wet. • Use a wooden spoon to bang knee and ask, “What hap­
(4–10 mos.; Ginsburg and Opper on different pots and pans, pened?”
1988, 43) and notice how the infant care • Push the big green button to
teacher responds when the
• Push a button on the push- make the tape recorder play.
child hits the pans harder and
button toy and watch the figure (By 36 mos.; American Acad­
makes a louder noise. (18 mos.;
pop up. (6–9 mos.; Lerner and emy of Pediatrics 2004, 308)
Meisels and others 2003, 37)
Ciervo 2003) • Walk quietly when the baby is
• Put objects into a clear con­ sleeping.
tainer, turn it over and watch the
objects fall out, and then fill it
up again. (8 mos.; Meisels and
others 2003, 21)
• Clap hands and then look at a
parent to get her to play pat­
a-cake. (8 mos.; Meisels and
others 2003, 21)

Chart continues on next page.


66

Cause-and-Effect

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Hear a loud noise and turn head • Hold a block in each hand • Roll cars of different sizes down
in the direction of the noise. and bang the blocks together. the slide. (18–24 mos.; Lerner
(3.5–5 mos.; Parks 2004, 37) (8.5–12 mos.; Parks 2004) and Ciervo 2003)
• Explore toys with hands and • Keep turning an object around
mouth. (3–6 mos.; Parks 2004, to find the side that makes it
10) work, such as the reflective
side of a mirror, or the open
• Move body in a rocking motion
side of a nesting cup. (9–12
to get the infant care teacher
mos.; Parks 2004, 65)
to continue rocking. (4–5 mos.;
Parks 2004, 57; Birth–8 mos.; • Cry and anticipate that the
Lerner and Dombro 2000) infant care teacher will come
to help. (9–12 mos.; Lerner and
• Kick legs in the crib and notice
Ciervo 2003)
that the mobile up above jiggles
with the kicking movements. • Drop an object repeatedly from
(4–5 mos.; American Academy the chair to hear it clang on the
of Pediatrics 2004, 209) floor or to get the infant care
teacher to come pick it up.
• Attend to a toy while exploring
(9–12 mos.; Parks 2004, 65)
it with the hands. (Scaled score
of 9 for 5:16–6:15 mos.; Bayley • Watch the infant care teacher
2006, 55) squeeze the toy in the water
table to make water squirt
out, then try the same action.
(Scaled score of 10 for 13:16–
14:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 61)
• Hand a toy car to a family
member after it stops moving
and the child cannot figure out
how to make it move again.
(12–15 mos.; Parks 2004, 59)
• Close eyes and turn face away
from the water table before
splashing with hands. (12 mos.;
Meisels and others 2003, 28)
• Continue to push the button on
a toy that is broken and appear
confused or frustrated when
nothing happens. (12 mos.;
Meisels and others 2003, 29)
6

Foundation: Spatial Relationships


The developing understanding of how things move and fit in space
8 months 18 months 36 months

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
At around eight months of At around 18 months of age, chil­ At around 36 months of age, children
age, children move their dren use trial and error to discover can predict how things will fit and
bodies, explore the size and how things move and fit in space. move in space without having to try
shape of objects, and ob­ (12–18 mos.; Parks 2004, 81) out every possible solution, and show
serve people and objects as understanding of words used to de­
they move through space. scribe size and locations in space.

For example, the child For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
may:
• Use vision or hearing to • Go around the back of a chair to • Hand the big truck to a peer who
track the path of some­ get the toy car that rolled behind asks for the big one. (Scaled score
one walking by. (5.5–8 it instead of trying to follow the of 10 for 28:16–30:15 mos.; Bayley
mos.; Parks 2004, 64; car’s path by squeezing under­ 2006, 95)
birth–8 mos.; Lally and neath the chair. (12–18 mos.; • Use words such as big and little.
others 1995, 78–79) Parks 2004 67; 8–18 mos.; Lally (25–30 mos.; Parks 2004, 82; 36
and others 1995, 78–79)
• Watch a ball roll away mos.; Meisels and others 2003, 73)
after accidentally knock­ • Use two hands to pick up a big • Put together a puzzle with three to
ing it. (5.5–8 mos., Parks truck, but only one hand to pick four separate pieces. (By 36 mos.;
2004, 64) up a small one. (12–18 mos.; American Academy of Pediatrics
Parks 2004, 81)
• Hold one stacking cup in 2004, 308; 30–36 mos.; Parks 2004,
each hand. (6.5–7.5 mos.; • Put a smaller nesting cup inside a 68)
Parks 2004, 50) larger cup after trying it the other • Get the serving spoon off the tray
way around. (12–18 mos.; Parks
• Put toys into a clear con­ when the infant care teacher asks
2004, 81)
tainer, dump them out, for the big spoon, even though
and then fill the container • Choose a large cookie off the there are small spoons on the tray.
up again. (8 mos.; Meisels plate instead of a smaller one. (30–36 mos.; Parks 2004, 83)
and others 2003, 21) (12–18 mos.; Parks 2004, 81) • Stack rings onto a post with the
• Put the child-sized hat on his biggest ring on the bottom and the
head and the larger hat on the smallest ring on the top, without
infant care teacher’s head. (12–18 much trial and error. (30–36 mos.;
mos.; Parks 2004, 81) Parks 2004, 83; 24–36 mos.; En­
gaging Young Children 2004, 44)
• Stack three nesting cups inside
one another, after trying some • Point to a peer’s stick when the
combinations that do not work. infant care teacher asks which stick
(12–19 mos.; Parks 2004, 82) is longer. (33–36 mos.; Parks 2004,
83; 24–36 mos.; Engaging Young
• Put one or two pegs into the peg­
Children 2004, 53)
board. (14:16–15:15 mos.; Bayley
2006, 62) • Understand requests that include
simple prepositions; for example,
• Roll a ball back and forth with
“Please put your cup on the table”
the infant care teacher. (18 mos.;
or “Please get your blanket out of
Meisels and others 2003, 38)
your back pack.” (By 36 mos.; Co­
• Fit pieces into a puzzle board. (18 plan 1993, 2; by 36 mos.; American
mos.; Meisels and others 2003, Academy of Pediatrics 2004; 24–36
39) mos.; Engaging Young Children
• Try to fit a piece into the shape 2004)
sorter and, when it does not fit, • Move around an obstacle when
turn it until it fits. (12–19 mos.; going from one place to another.
Parks 2004, 82) (24–36 mos.; American Academy of
Pediatrics 2004, 303)
Chart continues on next page.
6

Spatial Relationships

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Look at her own hand. (Scaled • Roll a car back and forth on the • Complete a puzzle of three
score of 9 for 4:06–4:15 mos.; floor. (6–11 mos.; Parks 2004, separate cut-out pieces, such
Bayley 2006, 53) 26) as a circle, square, and triangle.
(Scaled score of 10 for 19:16–
• Reach for a nearby toy and try • Dump toys out of a container.
20:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 66)
to grasp it. (4.5–5.5 mos.; Parks (9–11 mos.; Parks 2004, 64)
2004; scaled score of 10 for • Fit many pegs into a pegboard.
• Turn a toy to explore all sides
4:16–4:25 mos.; Bayley 2006, (Scaled score of 10 for 21:16–
to figure out how it works.
54) 22:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 68)
(9–12 mos.; Parks 2004, 65)
• Explore toys with hands and • Turn a book right-side up after
• Throw or drop a spoon or cup
mouth. (3–6 mos.; Parks 2004, realizing that it is upside down.
from the table and watch as it
10) (18–24 mos.; Parks 2004)
falls. (9–12 mos.; Parks 2004,
65) • Fit four nesting cups in the
correct order, even if it takes
• Take rings off a stacking ring
a couple of tries. (19–24 mos.;
toy. (10–11 mos.; Parks 2004,
Parks 2004, 82)
65)
• Assemble a two-piece puzzle;
• Move over and between cush­
for example, a picture of a flow­
ions and pillows on the floor.
er cut into two pieces. (Scaled
(8–12 mos.; American Acad­
score of 10 for 23:16–24:15
emy of Pediatrics 2004, 234)
mos.; Bayley 2006, 69)
• Crawl down a few carpeted
stairs. (Around 12 mos.; Ameri­
can Academy of Pediatrics
2004, 234)
• See a ball roll under the couch
and then reach under the
couch. (12–13 mos.; Parks
2004, 66)
• Stack one block on top of an­
other one. (12–16 mos.; Parks
2004, 66)
• Put one or two rings back onto
the post of a stacking ring toy.
(13–15 mos.; Parks 2004, 66)
• Put the circle piece of a puzzle
into the round opening, after
trying the triangle opening and
the square opening. (Scaled
score of 10 for 15:16–16:15
mos.; Bayley 2006, 63)
6

Foundation: Problem Solving


The developing ability to engage in a purposeful effort
to reach a goal or figure out how something works

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, chil­
children use simple actions to children use a number of ways dren solve some problems without
try to solve problems involving to solve problems: physically having to physically try out every
objects, their bodies, or other trying out possible solutions possible solution and may ask for
people. before finding one that works; help when needed. (By 36 mos.;
using objects as tools; watching American Academy of Pediatrics
someone else solve the problem 2004, 308)
and then applying the same so­
lution; or gesturing or vocalizing
to someone else for help.

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Shake, bang, and squeeze • Pull the string of a pull toy to • Ignore the stick that is much too
toys repeatedly to make the get it closer even when the short to reach a desired object
sounds happen again and toy gets momentarily stuck on and choose a stick that looks as if
again. (5.5–8 mos.; Parks something. (18 mos.; Meisels it may be long enough.
2004, 58; by 12 mos.; Ameri­ and others 2003, 38) • Stack only the cubes with holes
can Academy of Pediatrics • Use the handle of a toy broom in them on the stacking post,
2004, 243) to dislodge a ball under the ignoring the cube-shaped blocks
• Reach for a ball as it rolls
bookshelf. (8–18 mos.; Lally without holes that got mixed into
away. (5.5–8 mos.; Parks
and others 1995, 78–79) the bin. (18–36 mos.; Lally and
2004, 64)
others 1995, 78–79)
• Bring a small stool over to
• Vocalize to get the infant care reach a toy on top of a shelf, • Place the triangle piece into the
teacher’s attention. (6.5–8 having observed the infant puzzle without first needing to try
mos.; Parks 2004) care teacher do it. (8–18 mos.; it in the round or square hole. (By
Lally and others 1995, 78–79) 36 mos.; American Academy of
• Pull the string on a toy to
Pediatrics 2004, 306)
make it come closer. (8 mos.; • Look at a plate of crackers
Meisels and others 2003, 21) that is out of reach and then • Ask the infant care teacher for
at the infant care teacher, and help with the lid of a jar of paint.
• Focus on a desired toy that is
communicate “more.” (Scaled (36 mos.; Meisels and others
just out of reach while repeat­
score of 10 for 16:16–17:15 2003, 75)
edly reaching for it. (5–9 mos.;
mos.; Bayley 2006; 14–20
Parks 2004, 49) • Ask a peer to help move the train
mos.; Parks 2004) tracks over so that the child can
• Turn the bottle over to get the
• Hand the infant care teacher build a block tower on the floor.
nipple in his mouth.
a puzzle piece that the child is (36 mos.; Meisels and others
• Lift up a scarf to search for a having trouble with. 2003, 75)
toy that is hidden underneath.
• Ask or gesture for the infant care
(By 8 mos.; American Acad­
teacher to help tie the child’s
emy of Pediatrics 2004, 244)
shoelace. (36 mos.; Meisels and
others 2003, 75)

Chart continues on next page.


0

Problem Solving

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

During this period, the child


During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
may:
• Crawl over a pile of soft blocks • Use a stick to dig in the sandbox
• Explore toys with hands and to get to the big red ball. (8–11 when unable to find a shovel.
mouth. (3–6 mos.; Parks 2004, mos.; Parks 2004) (17–24 mos.; Parks 2004)
10) • Figure out how toys work by • Use a tool to solve a problem,
• Reach for a second toy when repeating the same actions such as using the toy broom
already holding on to one toy. over and over again. (9–12 to get a car out from under the
(5–6.5 mos.; Parks 2004, 49) mos.; Lerner and Ciervo 2003) couch, using a wooden puzzle
base as a tray to carry all the
• Hold a toy up to look at it • Pull the blanket in order to
puzzle pieces to another place,
while exploring it with the obtain the toy that is lying out
or using the toy shopping cart to
hands. (Scaled score of 9 for of reach on top of the blanket.
pick up the wooden blocks and
5:16–6:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, (8–10 mos.; Parks 2004)
move them to the shelf to be put
55) • Crawl around the legs of a away. (17–24 mos.; Parks 2004,
chair to get to the ball that 52)
rolled behind it. (9–12 mos.;
• Move to the door and try to turn
Parks 2004, 50; 18 mos.; Lally
the knob after a parent leaves for
and others 1995, 78–79)
work in the morning. (21–23 mos.;
• Keep turning an object around Parks 2004, 53)
to find the side that makes it
• Imitate a problem-solving method
work, such as the reflective
that the child has observed
side of a mirror or the open
someone else do before. (Scaled
side of a nesting cup. (9–12
score of 10 for 20:16–21:15 mos.;
mos.; Parks 2004, 65)
Bayley 2006, 66)
• Try to hold on to two toys with
• Tug on shoelaces in order to untie
one hand while reaching for a
them.
third desired toy, even if not
successful. (Scaled score of 9 • Complete a puzzle with three
for 10:16–11:15 mos.; Bayley separate cut-out pieces, such
2006, 58) as a circle, square, and triangle,
even though the child may try to
• Unscrew the lid of a plastic jar
put the triangle into the square
to get items out of it. (Scaled
hole before fitting it in the triangle
score of 10 for 14:16–15:15
opening. (Scaled score of 10 for
mos.; Bayley 2006, 62)
19:16–20:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
66)


Foundation: Imitation
The developing ability to mirror, repeat, and practice
the actions of others, either immediately or later

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around 8 months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age,
children imitate simple actions children imitate others’ actions children reenact multiple steps of
and expressions of others during that have more than one step and others’ actions that they have ob­
interactions. imitate simple actions that they served at an earlier time. (30–36
have observed others doing at an mos.; Parks 2004, 29)
earlier time. (Parks 2004; 28)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Copy the infant care teacher’s • Imitate simple actions that she • Reenact the steps of a fam­
movements when playing pat-a­ has observed adults doing; ily celebration that the child
cake and peek-a-boo. (Coplan for example, take a toy phone attended last weekend. (29–36
1993, 3) out of a purse and say hello as mos.; Hart and Risley 1999,
a parent does. (12–18 mos.; 118–19)
• Imitate a familiar gesture, such
Lerner and Ciervo 2003)
as clapping hands together or • Pretend to get ready for work
patting a doll’s back, after see­ • Pretend to sweep with a child- or school by making break­
ing the infant care teacher do it. sized broom, just as a family fast, packing lunch, grabbing
(7–8 mos.; Parks 2004) member does at home. (15–18 a purse, and communicating
mos.; Parks 2004, 27) good-bye before heading out
• Notice how the infant care
the door. (30–36 mos.; Parks
teacher makes a toy work and • Rock the baby doll to sleep,
2004, 29)
then push the same button to just as a parent does with the
make it happen again. (6–9 new baby. (15–18 mos.; Parks
mos.; Lerner and Ciervo 2003) 2004, 27)
• Imitate using the toy ham­
mer as a parent did. (18 mos.;
Meisels and others 2003, 38)

Chart continues on next page.




Imitation

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

During this period, the child


During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
may:
• Shrug shoulders after the infant • Repeat the most important word
• Listen to the infant care care teacher does it. (9–11 of a sentence the infant care
teacher talk during a diaper mos.; Parks 2004; by 12 mos.; teacher has just communicated.
change and then babble back American Academy of Pediat­ (17–19 mos.; Parks 2004)
when she pauses. (5.5–6.5 rics 2004, 243) • Imitate the last word or last few
mos.; Parks 2004, 125) • Imitate sounds or words im­ words of what an adult just said;
• Copy the intonation of the mediately after the infant care for example say, cup or a cup
infant care teacher’s speech teacher makes them. (9 mos.; after the infant care teacher says,
when babbling. (7 mos.; Parks Apfel and Provence 2001; “That’s a cup” or say, “Daddy
2004) 12–18 mos.; Hulit and Howard bye-bye” after the mother says,
2006, 122; 17 mos.; Hart and “Daddy went bye-bye.” (22 mos.;
Risley 1999, 84) Hart and Risley 1999, 99; 17–19
mos.; Parks 2004, 128)
• Copy the infant care teacher in
waving “bye-bye” to a parent as • Copy several actions that the
he leaves the room. (12 mos.; child cannot see himself do­
Meisels and others 2003, 26) ing, such as wrinkling the nose.
(17–20 mos.; Parks 2004, 32)
• Copy an adult’s action that is
unfamiliar but that the child can • Say, “beep, beep, beep, beep”
see herself do, such as wiggling after hearing the garbage truck
toes, even though it may take back up outside. (18-21 mos.;
some practice before doing it Parks 2004)
exactly as the adult does. (9–14 • Act out a few steps of a familiar
mos.; Parks 2004, 32) routine, such as pretend to fill the
• Watch the infant care teacher tub, bathe a baby doll, and dry
squeeze the toy in the water the doll. (18–24 mos.; Parks 2004,
table to make water squirt 28)
out, then try the same action. • Imitate words that the adult has
(Scaled score of 10 for 13:16– expressed to the child at an
14:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 61) earlier time, not immediately after
• Imitate the hand motion of the hearing them. (24–27 mos.; Parks
infant care teacher. (Scaled 2004; 19–28 mos.; Hart and Ris­
score of 10 for 14:16–15:15 ley 1999, 61)
mos.; Bayley 2006, 135) • Imitate two new actions of the
• Point to or indicate an object, infant care teacher; for example,
pay attention as the infant care put one hand on head and point
teacher labels the object, and with the other hand. (26:16–27:15
then try to repeat the label. mos.; Bayley 2006, 71)
(11–16 mos.; Hart and Risley • Imitate the way a family member
1999, 82) communicates by using the same
gestures, unique words, and
intonation.


Foundation: Memory
The developing ability to store and later retrieve
information about past experiences

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around 8 months of age, chil­ At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, chil­
dren recognize familiar people, children remember typical actions dren anticipate the series of steps
objects, and routines in the en­ of people, the location of objects, in familiar activities, events, or
vironment and show awareness and steps of routines. routines; remember characteristics
that familiar people still exist of the environment or people in it;
even when they are no longer and may briefly describe recent
physically present. past events or act them out. (24–36
mos.; Seigel 1999, 33)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Turn toward the front door • Get a blanket from the doll cra­ • Communicate, “Big slide” after
when hearing the doorbell ring dle because that is where baby a trip to neighborhood park.
or toward the phone when blankets are usually stored, af­ (24–36 mos.; Seigel 1999, 33)
hearing the phone ring. (8 ter the infant care teacher says, • Tell a parent, “Today we jumped
mos.; Meisels and others 2003, “The baby is tired. Where’s her in the puddles” when picked up
20) blanket?” (15–18 mos.; Parks from school. (Siegel 1999, 34)
2004, 67)
• Look for the father after he • Recall an event in the past, such
briefly steps out of the child • Anticipate and participate in as the time a family member
care room during drop-off in the steps of a nap routine. came to school and made a
the morning. (8 mos.; Meisels (18 mos.; Fogel 2001, 368) snack. (18–36 mos.; Siegel 1999,
and others 2003, 20) • Watch the infant care teacher 46)
placing a toy inside one of • Identify which child is absent
three pots with lids and reach from school that day by looking
for the correct lid when the around the snack table and fig­
teacher asks where the toy uring out who is missing. (18–36
went. (8–18 mos.; Lally and mos.; Lally and others 1995,
others 1995, 78–79) 78–79)
• Continue to search for an • Act out a trip to the grocery store
object even though it is hidden by getting a cart, putting food in
under something distracting, it, and paying for the food. (24
such as a soft blanket or a mos.; Bauer and Mandler 1989)
crinkly piece of paper.
• Get her pillow out of the cubby,
• See a photo of a close family in anticipation of naptime as
member and say his name or soon as lunch is finished.
hug the photo.
• Go to the cubby to get his
blanket that is inside the diaper
bag.

Chart continues on next page.




Memory

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Explore toys with hands and • Ask for a parent after morning • Say “meow” when the infant
mouth. (3–6 mos.; Parks 2004, drop-off. (9–12 mos.; Lerner care teacher points to the picture
10) and Ciervo 2003) of the cat and asks what the cat
says. (12–24 mos.; Siegel 1999,
• Find a rattle hidden under a • Reach in the infant care
32)
blanket when only the handle teacher’s pocket after watch­
is showing. (4–6 mos.; Parks ing him hide a toy there. (11–13 • Give another child an object that
2004, 42) mos.; Parks 2004, 43) belongs to her. (12–24 mos.;
Siegel 1999, 32)
• Look toward the floor when the • Look or reach inside a con­
bottle falls off table. (Scaled tainer of small toys after seeing • Remember where toys should
score of 10 for 5:06–5:15 the infant care teacher take be put away in the classroom.
mos.; Bayley 2006, 55; 8 mos.; the toys off the table and put (21–24 mos.; Parks 2004, 318)
Meisels and others 2003, 20; them in the container. (Scaled • Find a hidden toy, even when
birth–8 mos.; Lally and others score of 10 for 8:16–9:15 mos.; it is hidden under two or three
1995, 72) Bayley 2006, 57; birth–8 mos.; blankets. (By 24 mos.; American
Lally and others 1995, 78–79) Academy of Pediatrics 2004,
• Lift a scarf to search for a toy 273)
after seeing the infant care • Express “mama” when the infant
teacher hide it under the scarf. care teacher asks who packed
(By 8 mos.; American Acad­ the child’s snack.
emy of Pediatrics 2004, 244; 8
mos.; Kail 1990, 112)


Foundation: Number Sense


The developing understanding of number and quantity
8 months 18 months 36 months

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, children
children usually focus on one children demonstrate under­ show some understanding that
object or person at a time, yet standing that there are different numbers represent how many and
they may at times hold two amounts of things. demonstrate understanding of words
objects, one in each hand. that identify how much. (By 36 mos.;
American Academy of Pediatrics
2004, 308)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Hold one block in each hand, • Communicate “more” and • Pick out one object from a box or
then drop one of them when point to a bowl of apple slices. point to the picture with only one
the infant care teacher holds (18 mos.; Meisels and others of something. (Scaled score of
out a third block for the child 2003, 37) 10 for 35:16–36:15 mos.; Bayley
to hold. (6.5–7.5 mos.; Parks 2006, 97; 24–30 mos.; Parks 2004)
• Shake head “no” when offered
2004, 50) more pasta. (18 mos.; Meisels • Reach into bowl and take out two
• Watch a ball as it rolls away and others 2003, 37) pieces of pear when the infant
after hitting it with her hand. care teacher says, “Just take two.”
• Make a big pile of trucks and a
(5.5–8 mos.; Parks 2004, 64) (30–36 mos.; Parks 2004)
little pile of trucks.
• Explore one toy at a time by • Start counting with one, some­
• Use hand motions or words
shaking, banging, or squeez­ times pointing to the same item
to indicate “All gone” when
ing it. (5.5–8 mos.; Parks finished eating. (12–19 mos.; twice when counting, or using
2004, 58; 8 mos.; Meisels and numbers out of order; for example,
Parks 2004, 122)
others 2003, 21; birth–8 mos.; “one, two, three, five, eight.” (36
Lally and others 1995, 78–79) • Put three cars in a row. mos.; Engaging Young Children
2004, 178)
• Notice when someone walks
in the room. • Use fingers to count a small
number of items. (around 36 mos.;
Coplan 1993, 3)
• Look at a plate and quickly
respond “two,” without having
to count, when the infant care
teacher asks how many pieces of
cheese there are. (36 mos.; Engag­
ing Young Children 2004, 178)
• Hold up two fingers when asked,
“Show me two” or “How old are
you?” (36 mos.; Engaging Young
Children 2004, 178; by 36 mos.;
American Academy of Pediatrics
2004, 308)
• Identify “more” with collections of
up to four items, without needing
to count them. (36 mos.; Engaging
Young Children 2004, 31 and 180)
• Use more specific words to com­
municate how many, such as a
little or a lot. (Hulit and Howard
2006, 186)

Chart continues on next page.


6

Number Sense
Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Explore toys with hands and • Try to hold onto two toys with • Get two cups from the cupboard
mouth. (3–6 mos.; Parks 2004, one hand while reaching for a when playing in the housekeep­
10) third desired toy, even if not ing area with a friend. (21 mos.;
successful. (Scaled score of 9 Mix, Huttenlocher, and Levine
• Reach for second toy but may
for 10:16–11:15 mos.; Bayley 2002)
not grasp it when already hold­
2006, 58; 8–10 mos.; Parks
ing one toy in the other hand. • Look at or point to the child
2004, 50)
(5–6.5 mos.; Parks 2004, 49; with one piece of apple left on
scaled score of 10 for 5:16– • Hold a block in each hand and his napkin when the infant care
6:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 55) bang them together. (8.5–12 teacher asks, “Who has just one
mos.; Parks 2004) piece of apple?” (24–30 mos.;
• Transfer a toy from one hand
Parks 2004, 74)
to the other. (5.5–7 mos.; Parks • Put several pegs into a plastic
2004) container and then dump them • Give the infant care teacher one
into a pile. (12–13 mos.; Parks cracker from a pile of many when
• Reach for, grasp, and hold
2004, 65) she asks for “one.” (25–30 mos.;
onto a toy with one hand when
Parks 2004; scaled score of 10
already holding a different
for 28:16–30:15 mos.; Bayley
toy in the other hand. (Scaled
2006, 73)
score of 10 for 6:16–7:15 mos.;
Bayley 2006, 56)
• Track visually the path of a

moving object. (6–8 mos.;

Parks 2004, 64)



Foundation: Classification
The developing ability to group, sort, categorize, connect, and have
expectations of objects and people according to their attributes

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, chil­
children distinguish between children show awareness when dren group objects into multiple
familiar and unfamiliar people, objects are in some way con­ piles based on one attribute at a
places, and objects, and explore nected to each other, match two time, put things that are similar but
the differences between them. objects that are the same, and not identical into one group, and
(Barrera and Mauer 1981) separate a pile of objects into may label each grouping, even
two groups based on one attri­ though sometimes these labels are
bute. (Mandler and McDonough overgeneralized. (36 mos.; Mandler
1998) and McDonough 1993)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Explore how one toy feels and • Look at the crayons before • Identify a few colors when they
then explore how another toy choosing a color. (12–18 mos.; are named; for example, get a red
feels. Parks 2004, 77) ball from the bin of multicolored
balls when the infant care teacher
• Stare at an unfamiliar person • Choose usually to play with
asks for the red one. (Scaled
and move toward a familiar the blue ball even though there
score of 10 for 34:16–36:15 mos.;
person. is a red one just like it. (12–18
Bayley 2006, 97; 33 mos.+; Parks
mos.; Parks 2004, 77)
2004, 79)
• Pick the toy car from the bin
filled with toy dishes. (15–18 • Make three piles of tangrams in
various shapes, such as a circle,
mos.; Parks 2004; 77)
square, and triangle. (30–36 mos.;
• Pack the baby doll’s blanket, Parks 2004, 79)
brush, bottle, and clothes into
• Pick two big bears from a bowl
a backpack. (15–19 mos.;
containing two big bears and two
Parks 2004, 77)
small bears, even if the big bears
• Match two identical toys; for are different colors. (Scaled score
example, find another fire of 10 for 30:16–33:15 mos.; Bay-
truck when the infant care ley 2006, 74)
teacher asks, “Can you find
• Sort primary-colored blocks into
a truck just like that one?”
three piles: a red pile, a yellow
(15–19 mos.; Parks 2004; 77)
pile, and a blue one. (33 mos.+;
• Place all toy cars on one side Parks 2004, 79; 32 mos.; Bayley
of the rug and all blocks on the 2006)
other side. (15–18 mos.; Parks
• Point to different pictures of
2004, 77)
houses in a book even though
all of the houses look different.
(30–36 mos.; Parks 2004, 79)
• Put all the soft stuffed animals in
one pile and all the hard plastic
toy animals in another pile and
label the piles “soft animals” and
“hard animals.” (18–36 mos.; Lally
and others 1995, 78–79)
• Call all four-legged animals at the
farm “cows,” even though some
are actually sheep and others
horses. (18–36 mos.; Lally and
others 1995, 78–79)

Chart continues on next page.




Classification

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

During this period, the child


During this period, the child During this period, the child may:
may:
may: • Point to or indicate the realistic-
• Explore toys with hands and • Roll a car back and forth on looking plastic cow when the
mouth. (3–6 mos.; Parks 2004, the floor, then roll a ball. (6–11 infant care teacher holds up a few
10) mos.; Parks 2004, 26) toy animals and says, “Who says,
‘moo’?” (18–22 mos.; Parks 2004;
• Bang a toy on the table. (5.5–7 • Use two items that go togeth­
85)
mos.; Parks 2004, 25) er; for example, brush a doll’s
hair with a brush, put a spoon • Sort three different kinds of toys;
• Touch different objects (e.g.,
in a bowl, or use a hammer to for example, put the puzzle pieces
hard or soft) differently.
pound an object. (9–15 mos.; in the puzzle box, the blocks in the
Parks 2004, 26–27; by 12 block bin, and the toy animals in
mos.; American Academy of the basket during clean-up time.
Pediatrics 2004, 243) (19–24 mos.; Parks 2004, 77)
• Put the red blocks together • Show understanding of what
when the infant care teacher familiar objects are supposed to
asks, “Which blocks go to­ be used for, such as knowing that
gether?” a hat is for wearing or a tricycle is
for riding. (Scaled score of 10 for
23:16–25:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
93)
• Pick a matching card from a pile
of cards. (Scaled score of 10 for
24:16–25:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
70)
• Point to or indicate all the green
cups at the lunch table. (26 mos.;
Bayley 2006)
• Call the big animals “mama” and
the small animals “baby.” (27 mos.;
Bayley 2006)
• Help the infant care teacher sort
laundry into two piles: whites and
colors. (28 mos.; Hart and Risley
1999, 95)
• Put the red marker back in the red
can, the blue marker back in the
blue can, and the yellow marker
back in the yellow can when fin­
ished coloring. (Scaled score of 10
for 26:16–28:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
71)
• Match one shape to another
shape. (26–29 mos.; Parks 2004,
78; 26–30 mos.; Parks 2004)


Foundation: Symbolic Play


The developing ability to use actions, objects, or ideas
to represent other actions, objects, or ideas

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around 8 months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, children en­
children become familiar children use one object to gage in make-believe play involving several
with objects and actions represent another object and sequenced steps, assigned roles, and an
through active exploration. engage in one or two simple overall plan and sometimes pretend by
Children also build knowl­ actions of pretend play. imagining an object without needing the
edge of people, action, concrete object present. (30–36 mos.; Parks
objects, and ideas through 2004, 29)
observation. (Fenson and
others 1976; Rogoff and
others 2003)

For example, the child For example, the child For example, the child may:
may: may:
• Cause toys to make noise • Pretend to drink from an • Assign roles to self and others when play­
by shaking, banging, and empty cup by making ing in the dramatic play area (for example,
squeezing them. (5.5–8 slurping noises and saying “I’ll be the daddy, you be the baby”), even
mos.; Parks 2004, 58; by “ah” when finished. (Segal though the child may not stay in her role
12 mos.; American Acad­ 2004, 39) throughout the play sequence. (30–36
emy of Pediatrics 2004, mos.; Parks 2004, 29; 24 mos.; Segal
• Begin to engage in pretend
243) 2004, 43)
play by using a play spoon
• Roll car back and forth on to stir in the kitchen area. • Line up a row of chairs and communicate,
floor. (6–11 mos.; Parks (12–18 mos.; Lerner and “All aboard! The train is leaving.” (36 mos.;
2004, 26) Ciervo 2003) Vygotsky 1978, 111)
• Pretend that the banana • Use two markers to represent people in
is a telephone by picking the dollhouse by moving them around as
it up, holding it to the ear, if they were walking. (36 mos.; Vygotsky
and saying, “Hi!” (12–18 1978, 111)
mos.; Lerner and Ciervo • Stir “cake batter” while holding an imagi­
2003) nary spoon or serve an invisible burrito
• Laugh at an older brother on a plate. (30–36 mos.; Parks 2004, 29;
when he puts a bowl on scaled score of 10 for 27:16–29:15 mos.;
his head like a hat. (12–18 Bayley 2006, 69)
mos.; Parks 2004, 317) • Communicate with self during pretend
• Imitate a few steps of adult play to describe actions to self; for ex­
behavior during play; for ample, “Now I stir the soup.” (Hart and
example, pretend to feed Risley 1999, 125)
the baby doll with the toy • Plan with other children what they are
spoon and bowl. (15–18 going to pretend before starting to play;
mos.; Parks 2004, 27) for example, “Let’s play doggies!” (Segal
• Use a rectangular wooden 2004, 39; 36 mos.; Meisels and others
block as a phone. (18–24 2003, 74)
mos.; Parks 2004, 28) • Pretend to be a baby during dramatic play
because there is a new baby at home. (36
mos.; Meisels and others 2003, 73)
• Build a small town with blocks and then
use the toy fire truck to pretend to put out
a fire in the town. (By 36 mos.; American
Academy of Pediatrics 2004, 309)

Chart continues on next page.


0

Symbolic Play

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

During this period, the child


During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
may:
• Use two items that go togeth­ • Use the stuffed animals to play
• Explore toys with hands and er; for example, brush a doll’s “veterinarian” one day and then to
mouth. (3–6 mos.; Parks hair with brush, put a spoon play “farmer” the next day. (18–24
2004, 10) in a bowl, or use a hammer mos.; Lerner and Ciervo 2003)
to pound an object through a • Communicate “Time for night-
hole. (9–15 mos.; Parks 2004, night” to a doll while playing
26–27) house. (22–24 mos.; Parks 2004,
• Use objects in pretend play 133)
the way they were intended to • Complete three or more actions in
be used; for example, pretend a sequence of pretend play so the
to drink coffee or tea from actions have a beginning, middle,
play coffee cup. (Scaled score and end, such as giving the baby
of 10 for 15:16–16:15 mos.; doll a bath, putting his pajamas
Bayley 2006, 62) on, and putting him to sleep.
(24–30 mos.; Parks 2004, 28; by
36 mos.; American Academy of
Pediatrics 2004, 309; scaled score
of 10 for 29:16–30:15 mos.; Bayley
2006, 73)
• Pretend that the doll or stuffed
animal has feelings, such as mak­
ing a whining noise to indicate that
the stuffed puppy is sad. (24–30
mos.; Parks 2004, 28)
• Make the stuffed animals move, as
if they were alive, during pretend
play. (24–30 mos.; Parks 2004, 28)
• Engage in extended pretend play
that has a theme, such as birth­
day party or doctor. (24–30 mos.;
Parks 2004, 29)
• Use abstract things to represent
other things in pretend play; for
example, use dough or sand to
represent a birthday cake and
sticks or straws to represent
candles. (24–30 mos.; Parks
2004, 29; scaled score of 10 for
24:16–25:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
70; Segal 2004, 39)


Foundation: Attention Maintenance


The developing ability to attend to people and things while interacting with
others and exploring the environment and play materials

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, chil­
children pay attention to different children rely on order and pre­ dren sometimes demonstrate the
things and people in the environ­ dictability in the environment to ability to pay attention to more than
ment in specific, distinct ways. help organize their thoughts and one thing at a time.
(Bronson 2000, 64) focus attention. (Bronson 2000,
191)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Play with one toy for a few • Expect favorite songs to be • Realize, during clean-up time,
minutes before focusing on a sung the same way each time that he has put a car in the block
different toy. (6–9 mos.; Parks and protest if the infant care bin and return to put it in the
2004, 12 and 26; 8 mos.; teacher changes the words. proper place.
American Academy of Pediat­ • Insist on following the same • Search for and find a favorite
rics 2004, 241) bedtime routine every night. book and ask the infant care
• Focus on a desired toy that is teacher to read it.
• Nod and take the infant care
just out of reach while repeat­ teacher’s hand when the • Pound the play dough with a
edly reaching for it. (5–9 mos.; teacher says, “I know you are hammer while talking with a peer.
Parks 2004, 49) sad because Shanti is using
• Show momentary attention to the book right now, and you
board books with bright colors would like a turn. Shall we go
and simple shapes. to the book basket and find
another one to read together?”
• Attend to the play of other
children.
• Put toy animals into a clear
container, dump them out, and
then fill the container up again.
(8 mos.; Meisels and others
2003, 21)
• Stop moving, to focus on the
infant care teacher when she
starts to interact with the child.

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Remain calm and focused on • Pay attention to the infant care • Play alone with toys for several
people, interesting toys, or teacher’s voice without being minutes at a time before moving
interesting sounds for a minute distracted by other noises in on to different activity. (18–24
or so. (1–6 mos.; Parks 2004, the room. (9–11 mos.; Parks mos.; Parks 2004, 15)
9) 2004; 12) • Sit in a parent’s lap to read a
• Explore a toy by banging, • Focus on one toy or activity book together. (Scaled score of
mouthing, or looking at it. for a while when really inter­ 10 for 21:16–22:15 mos.; Bayley
(Scaled score of 9 for 3:26– ested. (By 12 mos.; American 2006)
4:05 mos.; Bayley 2006, 52) Academy of Pediatrics 2004,
241)


Foundation: Understanding of Personal Care Routines


The developing ability to understand
and participate in personal care routines
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

8 months 18 months 36 months


At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, chil­ At around 36 months of age,
children are responsive dur­ dren show awareness of familiar children initiate and follow through
ing the steps of personal care personal care routines and partici­ with some personal care routines.
routines. (CDE 2005) pate in the steps of these routines. (CDE 2005)
(CDE 2005)

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Turn head away as the infant • Go to the sink when the infant • Go to the sink and wash hands
care teacher reaches with care teacher says that it is time after seeing snacks being set
a tissue to wipe the child’s to wash hands. (Scaled score of out on the table. (CDE 2005)
nose. (8 mos.; Meisels and 10 for 17:16–18:15 mos.; Bayley • Get a tissue to wipe own nose
others 2003, 20) 2006, 90; 12–18 mos.; Lerner or bring the tissue to the infant
and Ciervo 2003; 12 mos.;
• Kick legs in anticipation of a care teacher for help when the
Coplan 1993, 2; by 24 mos.;
diaper change and then quiet child feels that his nose needs
American Academy of Pediatrics
down as the parent wipes the to be wiped. (CDE 2005)
2004; 24 mos.; Meisels and
child’s bottom. (CDE 2005)
others 2003, 46) • Take a wet shirt off when need­
• Pay attention to her hands as ing to put on a dry one. (36
• Get a tissue when the infant care
the infant care teacher holds mos.; Meisels and others 2003,
teacher says, “Please go get a
them under running water and 76)
tissue. We need to wipe your
helps rub them together with
nose.” (18 mos.; Meisels and • Help set the table for lunchtime.
soap. (CDE 2005)
others 2003, 36) (36 mos.; Meisels and others
2003, 77)
• Move toward the door to the
playground after seeing the
infant care teacher put his coat
on. (18 mos.; Meisels and others
2003, 38)
• Put snack dishes in the sink and
the bib in the hamper after eat­
ing.
• Have trouble settling down for a
nap until the infant care teacher
reads a story, because that is the
naptime routine. (12–18 mos.;
Parks 2004, 317)

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
During this period, the child During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
may: • Cooperate during a diaper • Drink from a cup without spilling
• Anticipate being fed upon change by lifting her bottom. much. (24 mos.; Meisels and
seeing the infant care teacher (10.5–12 mos.; Parks 2004) others 2003, 52)
approach with a bottle. • Grab the spoon as the infant • Try to put on own socks. (24
• Hold onto the bottle while care teacher tries to feed the mos.; Meisels and others 2003,
being fed by the infant care child. (12 mos.; Meisels and oth­ 52)
teacher. (4 mos.; Meisels and ers 2003, 31) • Pull her shoes off at naptime.
others 2003, 14) • Raise arms when the infant care (24 mos.; Meisels and others
teacher tries to put a dry shirt on 2003, 52)
the child. (12 mos.; Meisels and
others 2003)


References

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
American Academy of Pediatrics. 2004. Bauer, P. J., and J. M. Mandler. 1989.
Caring for Your Baby and Young Child: “One Thing Follows Another: Effects
Birth to Age 5 (Fourth edition). Edited of Temporal Structure on 1- to 2-Year
by S. P. Shelov and R. E. Hannemann. Olds’ Recall of Events,” Developmental
New York: Bantam Books. Psychology, Vol. 8, 241–63.
Apfel, N. H., and S. Provence. 2001. Man­ Bayley, N. 2006. Bayley Scales of Infant
ual for the Infant-Toddler and Family and Toddler Development (Third edi­
Instrument (ITFI). Baltimore, MD: Paul tion). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt
H. Brookes Publishing. Assessment, Inc.
Baillargeon, R. 2004. “Infants’ Physical Brazelton, T. B. 1992. Touchpoints: Your
World,” Current Directions in Psychologi­ Child’s Emotional and Behavioral Devel­
cal Science, Vol. 13, No. 3, 89–94. opment. New York: Perseus Books.
Bard, K., and C. Russell. 1999. “Evolution­ Bronson, M. 2000. Self-regulation in Early
ary Foundations of Imitation: Social- Childhood: Nature and Nurture. New
Cognitive and Developmental Aspects York: Guilford Press.
of Imitative Processes in Non-Human Brooks-Gunn, J., and G. Duncan. 1997.
Primates,” in Imitation in Infancy: Cam­ “The Effects of Poverty on Children,”
bridge Studies in Cognitive and Percep­ The Future of Children, Vol. 7, No. 2,
tual Development. Edited by J. Nadel 55–71.
and G. Butterworth. Cambridge, UK: Butterworth, G. 1999. “Neonatal Imitation:
Cambridge University Press. Existence, Mechanisms and Motives,”
Barrera, M. E., and Mauer, D. 1981. “The Imitation in Infancy: Cambridge Studies
Perception of Facial Expressions by the in Cognitive and Perceptual Develop­
Three-month-old.” Child Development, ment. Edited by J. Nadel and C. Butter-
Vol. 52, 203–6. worth. New York: Cambridge University
Bauer, P. 2002a. “Early Memory Develop­ Press.
ment,” in Handbook of Cognitive Devel­ California Department of Education
opment. Edited by U. Goswami. Oxford, (CDE). 2005. “Desired Results Devel­
England: Blackwell. opmental Profile (DRDP),” Sacramento,
Bauer, P. 2002b. “Long-Term Recall Mem­ CA: California Department of Educa­
ory: Behavioral and Neuro-Developmen­ tion. http://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/ci/
tal Changes in the First Two Years of desiredresults.asp (accessed February
Life,” Current Directions in Psychological 7, 2007).
Science, Vol. 11, No. 4, 137–41. Carey, S. 2001. “On the Very Possibility of
Bauer, P. 2004. “Getting Explicit Memory Discontinuities in Conceptual Develop­
off the Ground: Steps Toward Construc­ ment,” in Language, Brain, and Cogni­
tion of a Neuro-Developmental Account tive Development: Essays in Honor of
of Changes in the First Two Years of Jacques Mehler. Edited by E. Dupoux.
Life,” Developmental Review, Vol. 24, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
347–73. Clements, D. H. 2004. “Major Themes and
Bauer, P. 2007. “Recall in Infancy: A Recommendations,” in Engaging Young
Neurodevelopmental Account,” Current Children in Mathematics: Standards for
Directions in Psychological Science, Vol. Early Childhood Educators. Edited by D.
16, No. 3, 142–46. H. Clements and J. Samara. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.


Coplan, J. 1993. Early Language Milestone Hulit, L. M., and M. R. Howard. 2006.
Scale: Examiner’s Manual (Second edi­ Born to Talk: An Introduction to Speech
tion). Austin, TX: Pro-ed. and Language Development (Fourth edi­
Engaging Young Children in Mathematics: tion). New York: Pearson Education.
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

Standards for Early Childhood Mathe­ Kail, R. 1990. The Development of Memory
matics Education. 2004. Edited by D. H. in Children (Third edition). New York:
Clements and J. Sarama. Mahwah, NJ: W. H. Freeman.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lally, J. R., and others. 1995. Caring for
Fenson, L., and others. 1976. “The Devel­ Infants and Toddlers in Groups: Develop­
opmental Progression of Manipulative mentally Appropriate Practice. Washing­
Play in the First Two Years,” Child ton, DC: Zero to Three Press.
Development, Vol. 47, No. 1, 232–36. Legerstee, M. 1997. “Contingency Effects
Fogel, A. 2001. Infancy: Infant, Family, and of People and Objects on Subsequent
Society (Fourth edition). Belmont, CA: Cognitive Functioning in Three-Month-
Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Old Infants,” Social Development, Vol. 6,
Fuson, K. C. 1988. Children’s Counting No. 3, 307–21.
and Concepts of Number. New York: Lerner, C., and A. L. Dombro. 2000.
Springer-Verlag. Learning and Growing Together: Under­
Gallistel, C. R., and R. Gelman. 1992. standing and Supporting Your Child’s
“Preverbal and Verbal Counting and Development. Washington, DC: Zero to
Computation,” Cognition, Vol. 44, No. Three Press.
1–2, 43–74. Lerner, C., and L. A. Ciervo. 2003. Healthy
Gelman, R., and C. R. Gallistel. 1978. Minds: Nurturing Children’s Develop­
The Child’s Understanding of Number. ment from 0 to 36 Months. Washington,
Oxford, England: Harvard University DC: Zero to Three Press and American
Press. Academy of Pediatrics.
Ginsburg, H. P., and S. Opper. 1988. Madole, K., and L. Oakes. 1999. “Making
Piaget’s Theory of Intellectual Develop­ Sense of Infant Categorization: Stable
ment (Third edition). Englewood Cliffs, Processes and Changing Representa­
NJ: Prentice Hall. tions,” Developmental Review, Vol. 19,
Gopnik, A.; A. Meltzoff; and P. K. Kuhl. No. 2, 263–96.
1999. The Scientist in the Crib: Minds, Mandler, J. M. 2000. “Perceptual and
Brains, and How Children Learn. New Conceptual Processes in Infancy,” Jour­
York: William Morrow. nal of Cognition and Development, Vol.
Gowen, J. W. March, 1995. “Research in 1, No. 1, 3–36.
Review: The Early Development of Sym­ Mandler, J., and L. McDonough. 1993.
bolic Play,” Young Children, Vol. 50, No. “Concept Formation in Infancy,” Cogni­
3, 75–84. tive Development, Vol. 8, No. 3, 291–318.
Hart, B., and T. R. Risley. 1999. The Social Mandler, J., and L. McDonough. 1998.
World of Children: Learning to Talk. “On Developing a Knowledge Base in
Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Pub­ Infancy,” Developmental Psychology,
lishing. Vol. 34, No. 6, 1274–88.
Howe, M., and M. Courage. 1993. “On Mangione, P. L.; J. R. Lally; and S. Signer.
Resolving the Enigma of Infantile 1992. Discoveries of Infancy: Cognitive
Amnesia,” Psychological Bulletin, Vol. Development and Learning. Sacra­
113, No. 2, 305–26. mento, CA: Far West Laboratory and
California Department of Education.


Mareschal, D., and R. French. 2000. Perry, J. P. May, 2003. “Making Sense of
“Mechanisms of Categorization in Outdoor Pretend Play,” Young Children,
Infancy,” Infancy, Vol. 1, No. 1, 59–76. Vol. 58, No. 3, 26–30.
Meltzoff, A. N., and M. K. Moore. 1983. Rogoff, B. 1990. Apprenticeship in Think­

COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
“Newborn Infants Imitate Adult Facial ing: Cognitive Development in Social
Gestures,” Child Development, Vol. 54, Context. New York: Oxford University
702–9. Press.
Meltzoff, A. N., and M. K. Moore. 1989. Rogoff, B., and P. Chavajay. 1995. “What’s
“Imitation in Newborn Infants: Explor­ Become of Research on the Cultural
ing the Range of Gestures Imitated and Basis of Cognitive Development?”
the Underlying Mechanisms,” Devel­ American Psychologist, Vol. 50, No. 10,
opmental Psychology, Vol. 25, No. 6, 859–77.
954–62. Rogoff, B., and others. 2003. “Firsthand
Meltzoff, A. N., and M. K. Moore. 1999. Learning Through Intent Participation,”
“Persons and Representation: Why Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 54,
Infant Imitation Is Important for Theo­ 175–203.
ries of Human Development, “ in Imita­ Ruff, H., and M. Rothbart. 1996. Atten­
tion in Infancy: Cambridge Studies in tion in Early Development: Themes and
Cognitive and Perceptual Development. Variations. New York: Oxford University
Edited by J. Nadel and G. Butterworth. Press.
New York: Cambridge University Press. Segal, M. 2004. “The Roots and Fruits
Meisels, S. J., and others. 2003. The of Pretending,” in Children’s Play: The
Ounce Scale: Standards for the Develop­ Roots of Reading. Edited by E. F. Zigler,
mental Profiles (Birth–42 Months). New D. G. Singer, and S. J. Bishop-Josef.

York: Pearson Early Learning. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press.

Mix, K.; J. Huttenlocher; and S. Levine. Siegel, D.J. 1999. The Developing Mind:
2002. Quantitative Development in How Relationships and the Brain Inter­
Infancy and Early Childhood. New York: act to Shape Who We Are. New York:
Oxford University Press. Guilford Press.
Moser, R. F. 1995. “Caregivers’ Corner. Starkey, P., and R. G. Cooper. 1980. “Per­
Fantasy Play in the Sandbox,” Young ception of Numbers by Human Infants,”
Children, Vol. 51, No. 1, 83–84. Science, Vol. 210, No. 4473, 1033–35.
National Research Council and Institute of Starkey, P.; E. S. Spelke; and R. Gel-
Medicine. 2000. From Neurons to Neigh­ man. 1990. “Numerical Abstraction by
borhoods: The Science of Early Child­ Human Infants,” Cognition, Vol. 36, No.
hood Development. Committee on Inte­ 2, 97–128.
grating the Science of Early Childhood Sternberg, R. J., and E. L. Grigorenko.
Development. Edited by J. Shonkoff and 2004. “Why We Need to Explore Devel­
D. Phillips. Washington, DC: National opment in Its Cultural Context,” Mer­
Academies Press. rill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. 50, No. 3,
O’Brien, M. 1997. Meeting Individual and 369–86.
Special Needs: Inclusive Child Care for Vygotsky, L. S. 1978. Mind in Society: The
Infants and Toddlers. Baltimore, MD: Development of Higher Psychological
Paul H. Brookes Publishing. Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Parks, S. 2004. Inside HELP: Hawaii Early University Press.
Learning Profile: Administration and
Reference Manual. Palo Alto, CA: VORT
Corporation.
6

Whitehurst, G., and C. Lonigan. 1998. Youngblade, L. M., and J. Dunn. 1995.
“Child Development and Emergent Lit­ “Individual Differences in Young Chil­
eracy,” Child Development, Vol. 69, No. dren’s Pretend Play with Mother and
3, 848–72. Sibling: Links to Relationships and
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT

Wynn, K. 1998. “Numerical Competence in Understanding of Other People’s Feel­


Infants,” in The Development of Math­ ings and Beliefs,” Child Development,
ematical Skills. Edited by C. Donlad. Vol. 66, 1472–92.
Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology
Press.


COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
Perceptual and

Motor Development

P
erception refers to the process late objects. Motor behaviors such as
of taking in, organizing, and reaching, touching, and grasping are
interpreting sensory information. forms of exploratory activity (Adolph
Perception is multimodal, with mul­ 1997).
tiple sensory inputs contributing to As infants develop increasing motor
motor responses (Bertenthal 1996). An competence, they use perceptual infor­
infant’s turning his head in response mation to inform their choices about
to the visual and auditory cues of the which motor actions to take (Adolph
sight of a face and the sound of a voice and Joh 2007). For example, they may
exemplifies this type of perception. adjust their crawling or walking in
Intersensory redundancy, “the fact response to the rigidity, slipperiness,
that the senses provide overlapping or slant of surfaces (Adolph 1997).
information . . . is a cornerstone of Motor movements, including move­
perceptual development” (Bahrick, ments of the eyes, arms, legs, and
Lickliter, and Flom 2004). hands, provide most of the perceptual
“Motor development refers to information infants receive (Adolph
changes in children’s ability to control and Berger 2006). Young children’s
their body’s movements, from infants’ bodies undergo remarkable changes in
first spontaneous waving and kick­ the early childhood years. In describ­
ing movements to the adaptive control ing this development, Adolph and
of reaching, locomotion, and complex Avolio (2000, 1148) state, “Newborns
sport skills” (Adolph, Weise, and Marin are extremely top-heavy with large
2003, 134). The term motor behavior heads and torsos and short, weak
describes all movements of the body, legs. As infants grow, their body fat
including movements of the eyes (as and muscle mass are redistributed. In
in the gaze), and the infant’s develop­ contrast to newborns, toddlers’ bod­
ing control of the head. Gross motor ies have a more cylindrical shape, and
actions include the movement of large they have a larger ratio of muscle mass
limbs or the whole body, as in walk­ to body fat, especially in the legs.”
ing. Fine motor behaviors include the These changes in weight, size, percent­
use of fingers to grasp and manipu­ age of body fat, and muscle strength


0

provide perceptual/motor challenges Pick 1989). In comparison, current


to infants as they practice a variety of research in motor development often
actions (Adolph and Berger 2006). This emphasizes action in the context of
dramatic physical development occurs behavior and development in the per­
within the broad context of overall ceptual, cognitive, and social domains
development. As infants master each (Pick 1989). In particular, contempo­
challenge, their perceptual and motor rary accounts of infant motor develop­
behavior reflects their ever-present ment address (1) the strong relation­
interpersonal orientation and social ship between perception and action
environment. (Bertenthal 1996; Gibson 1988; Thelen
The extent and variety of infant 1995), (2) the relationship between
perceptual and motor behavior are actions and the environment (Gibson
remarkable. Infants and toddlers 1988; Thelen 1995), and (3) the impor­
spend a significant part of their days tance of motives in motor behavior,
engaged in motor behavior of one type notably social and explorative motives
or another. By three and a half months (von Hofsten 2007). Although histori­
of age, infants have made between cal approaches may encourage profes­
three and six million eye movements sionals to focus on the relationship
PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT

during their waking hours (Haith, between growing perceptual/motor


Hazen, and Goodman 1988). Infants skills and the child’s increasingly
who crawl and walk have been found sophisticated manipulation and under­
to spend roughly half of their wak­ standing of objects, contemporary
ing hours involved in motor behavior, understanding suggests the value of
approximately five to six hours per day observation of this progression. How
(Adolph and Joh 2007, 11). On a daily these developing behaviors and abili­
basis infants who are walking “. . . ties play a role in the social/emotional
take more than 9,000 steps and travel aspects of the child’s life and function­
the distance of more than 29 football ing, such as forming early relation­
fields. They travel over nearly a dozen ships and building an understanding
different indoor and outdoor surfaces of others, may be noteworthy.
varying in friction, rigidity and tex­ The contemporary view suggests
ture. They visit nearly every room in that thinking about perceptual/motor
their homes and they engage in bal­ development can be inclusive of infants
ance and locomotion in the context of and toddlers with disabilities or other
varied activities” (Adolph and Berger special needs. Children whose disabili­
2006, 181). ties affect their perceptual or motor
Early research in motor development development still want to explore and
involved detailed observational stud­ interact with the people and environ­
ies that documented the progression of ment around them. Although the
infant motor skills and presented an perceptual and motor development
understanding of infant motor behav­ of children with disabilities or other
ior as a sequence of universal, biologi­ special needs may follow a pathway
cally programmed steps (Adolph and that differs from typical developmental
Berger 2006; Bertenthal and Boker trajectories, sensitive and responsive
1997; Bushnell and Boudreau 1993; caregivers can provide alternative ways


in which to engage children’s drive to It is important to recognize that,


explore, building on their interests and though developmental charts may
strengths and supporting their overall show motor development unfolding in
physical and psychological health. the form of a smooth upward progres­
Pioneering researchers in infant sion toward mastery, the development
motor development used novel and of individual children often does not
painstaking methods to study the follow a smooth upward trajectory. In
progression of infant skill acquisition fact, “detours” and steps backward
(Adolph and Berger 2005; Adolph are common as development unfolds
2008). Their findings were presented (Adolph and Berger 2006, 173). Infant
for both professionals and the public motor development can be understood
in the form of milestone charts that as a process in which change occurs
depicted motor skill acquisition as a as the infant actively adapts to varying
clear progression through a series of circumstances and new tasks (Thelen
predictable stages related to chrono­ 1995). Thelen (1994) demonstrated
logical age (Adolph 2008; Adolph, this experimentally in her well-known
Weise, and Marin 2003). More recent study in which three-month-old
research in the area of perceptual and babies, still too young to coordinate

PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT


motor development has indicated their movements to be able to sit,
substantial variability between chil­ reach, or crawl, learned to coordinate
dren in the pathways to acquiring their kicks in order to engage in the
major motor milestones such as sitting novel task of making a mobile move.
and walking (Adolph 1997; Adolph Cultural and historical factors, includ­
2008). Each child may take a unique ing caregivers’ behavior, also affect the
developmental pathway toward attain­ ways in which infants engage in motor
ment of major motor milestones behaviors. For example, Adolph and
(Adolph and Joh 2007). Crawling, for Berger (2005) observed that mothers
example, is not a universal stage. in Jamaica and Mali “train” infants
Research clearly shows that not all to sit by propping up three- to four­
children crawl before they walk month-old infants with pillows in a
(Adolph 2008). Although most children special hole in the ground designed to
walk independently around age one, provide back support.
the normal range for acquisition of this For years, researchers, educators,
behavior in western cultures is very and early childhood professionals have
broad, between 9 and 17 months of age emphasized the interrelatedness of
(Adolph 2008). Age has traditionally the developmental domains. The cur­
been treated as the primary predictor rent research supports an even greater
of when landmark motor behaviors appreciation of the profound role of
occur, but studies now indicate that interrelatedness and interdependence
experience may be a stronger predictor of factors, domains, and processes
than age is in the emergence of both in development (Diamond 2007). The
crawling (Adolph and Joh 2007) and developmental domains are linked not
walking (Adolph, Vereijken, and Shrout only with one another, but also with
2003). factors such as culture, social rela­
tionships, experience, physical health,


mental health, and brain functioning situations provides an excellent means


(Diamond 2007). In the case of per­ to study infant social cognition (Tamis-
ceptual and motor behavior, Diamond LeMonda and Adolph 2005).
(2007) has observed that perception,
motor behavior, and cognition occur Perceptual Development
in the context of culture, emotion, Infants’ perceptual skills are at work
social relationships, and experience, during every waking moment. For
which in turn influence physical and example, those skills can be observed
mental health as well as overall brain when an infant gazes into a caregiver’s
functioning. Bertenthal (1996) has eyes or distinguishes between familiar
proposed that perception and motor and unfamiliar people. Infants use
action are interrelated rather than perception to distinguish features
autonomous processes. They may be of the environment, such as height,
best viewed as different components depth, and color. “The human infant
of an action system. Common behav­ is recognized today as ‘perceptually
iors such as reaching and turning competent’; determining just how the
the head for visual tracking illustrate senses function in infancy helps to
the interrelatedness of the motor, specify the perceptual world of babies”
PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT

perceptual, cognitive, and social-emo­ (Bornstein 2005, 284). The ability to


tional domains in infant development. perceive commonalities and differences
Even as very young infants, children between objects is related to the cogni­
are highly motivated to explore, gain tive domain foundation of classifica­
information, attend, and engage their tion. Infants explore objects differently
physical and social environments depending upon object features such
(Gibson 1987). As Gibson (1988, 5) as weight, texture, sound, or rigidity
explains: “We don’t simply see, we (Palmer 1989). Parents and profession­
look.” Research by Berthier (1996, 811) als may have observed young children
indicates that “infant reaching is not exploring a slope, such as a slide, by
simply a neural program that is trig­ touching it with their hands or feet
gered by the presence of a goal object, before they decide whether to slide
but that infants match the kinematics down it or not. Research by Adolph,
of their reaches to the task and their Eppler, and Gibson (1993) suggests
goals.” that learning plays a part in young
Perception and motor action play a children’s decision making in physi­
key role in children’s experiences and cally risky situations, such as navigat­
psychological processes (Thelen 1995). ing slopes, and that exploratory behav­
They also contribute to human psycho­ ior may be a means to this learning.
logical development in general, since Perception is also strongly related to
ultimately “behavior is movement” the social-emotional domain, such as
(Adolph and Berger 2005, 223), and when young children perceive the
psychology can be defined as the study differences between various facial
of human behavior. It has been pro­ expressions and come to understand
posed that infants’ use of social infor­ what they may mean.
mation to guide their motor behavior
in physically challenging or unfamiliar


Gross Motor Development Fine Motor Development


Gross motor development includes Through touching, grasping, and
the attainment of skills such as rolling manual manipulation, infants experi­
over, sitting up, crawling, walking, and ence a sense of agency and learn about
running. Gross motor behavior enables the features of people, objects, and the
infants to move and thereby attain environment. Fine motor development
different and varied perspectives on is related to the ability to draw, write,
the environment. Behaviors such as and participate in routines such as
pulling to stand and climbing present eating and dressing. Common early
children with new learning opportuni­ childhood learning materials, such as
ties. When infants push a toy stroller pegboards, stacking rings, stringing
or shopping cart, they are also engag­ beads, and puzzles, offer opportunities
ing in processes related to cognitive for infants to practice their fine motor
development, such as imitation. The skills. Fine motor movements of the
gross motor behaviors involved in hands are coordinated with perceptual
active outdoor play with other children information provided through move­
are related to children’s development of ments of the eyes, as when seven- to
social skills and an understanding of

PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT


nine-month-old infants use visual
social rules. information to orient their hands as
they reach for an object (McCarty and
others 2001).


Foundation: Perceptual Development


The developing ability to become aware of the social
and physical environment through the senses
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, chil­ At around 36 months of age,
children use the senses to ex­ dren use the information received children can quickly and easily
plore objects and people in the from the senses to change the combine the information received
environment. (6–9 mos.; Ruff and way they interact with the environ­ from the senses to inform the way
Kohler 1978) ment. they interact with the environment.

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Look at an object in her hand, • Adjust the way he is walking de­ • Identify a blanket or other
mouth it, and then take it out to pending on the type of surface; familiar objects just by touching
look at it again. (6–9 mos.; Ruff for example, walking slowly on them. (30–36 mo.; Parks 2004)
and Kohler 1978) rocks and faster on pavement. • Identify a truck when she feels
(12–18 mos.; Fogel 2001, 333)
• Hear the infant care teacher’s it buried underneath the sand.
footsteps in the darkened nap • Choose to sit on her bottom (30–36 mos.; Parks 2004, 17)
room and turn his head to try and slide down a steep hill • Watch the lines that she makes
PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT

to look for her. (6–9 mos.; Ruff rather than walk down it. (12–18 with a marker on the paper.
and Kohler 1978) mos.; Adolph, Eppler, and Gib­ (Freeman 1980)
son 1993)
• Show excitement upon recog­ • Climb more slowly as he
nizing the color of a favorite • Sway back and forth to the beat reaches the top of the ladder.
food that is offered on a spoon. of a song while standing up.
(6–9 mos.; Reardon and Bush­ • Press harder on a clump of clay
• Pull hands away from the sen­ than on play dough.
nell 1988) sory table, which is filled with
an unfamiliar slimy substance. • Watch a family member draw
a circle and then try to do it.
• Spend a lot of time in the sand­ (24–36 mos.; Stiles 1995)
box, burying a hand underneath
a pile of sand. • Walk more slowly and carefully
when carrying an open cup of
• Stop pouring sand into a bucket milk than when carrying a cup
that is already full. with a lid.


Perceptual Development

Behaviors leading up to the


Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the
foundation ( to  months)
foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
During this period, the child may: During this period, the child During this period, the child may:
may:
• Have a range of vision that is sev­ • Enjoy rough-and-tumble play.
eral feet. (By 4 mos.; American • Nuzzle his face into a freshly (18–24 mos.; Parks 2004, 16)
Academy of Pediatrics 2004, 207) washed blanket to smell it. • Handle fragile items carefully.
(6–12 mos.; Parks 2004)
• Experience the sensation of be­ (24–26 mos.; Parks 2004, 16)
ing touched, and then search for • Show recognition of sounds, • Enjoy tactile books, such as
the object or person. (4–6 mos.; such as the mother’s foot­ books with faux fuzzy animal
Parks 2004, 11) steps, water running in the fur. (24–29 mos.; Parks 2004,
bathtub, or the refrigerator
• Listen to the sounds that family 17)
door being opened. (18 mos.;
members use while talking in the
Meisels and others 2003, 38) • Play with sand and water by
home language, and use these filling up buckets, digging, and
same sounds while babbling. (4–6 • Pat, push, mound, and pouring water. (24–36 mos.;
mos.; Parks 2004, 11) squeeze play dough, experi­ Parks 2004, 17)
encing all the ways it can be
• Startle when hearing a loud noise.
used. (18 mos.; Meisels and
(By 4 mos.; American Academy
others 2003, 37)
of Pediatrics 2004, 209)

PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT


• Explore pegboard holes with
• Kick feet while lying in the crib,
a finger, then look around for
feel the crib shake, and then kick
something to fit in the holes.
feet again. (By 4 mos.; American
(18 mos.; Meisels and others
Academy of Pediatrics 2004, 209)
2003, 37)
• Recognize an object as some­
• Enjoy messy activities or
thing she has seen before, even
show a dislike for messy
while looking at it from a different
activities. (12–18 mos.; Parks
perspective. (By 4 mos.; Fogel
2004, 14)
2001, 252)
• React to various sensations,
• Notice the difference between dif­
such as extremes in tempera­
ferent songs that the infant care
ture and taste. (12–18 mos.;
teacher sings. (By 6 mos.; Fogel
Parks 2004, 14–15)
2001, 252)
• Crumple and tear paper. (7–9
• Look confused upon hearing
mos.; Parks, 2004, 26)
sounds that do not fit with the
motions observed (for example, • Stop crawling when he reach­
hearing a squeaking noise while es the edge of the couch.
seeing a rattle move). (By 6 mos.; (By the time most infants are
Fogel 2001, 252) crawling; Walk and Gibson
1961)
• Explore objects with the mouth.
(By 7 mos.; American Academy • Be able to remember where
of Pediatrics 2004, 208) toys are stored in the class­
room because she has
• See different colors. (By 7 mos.;
crawled by them before. (By
American Academy of Pediatrics
the time most infants are
2004, 208)
crawling; Bai and Bertenthal
• See things from a distance. (By 1992; Campos and Berten­
7 mos.; American Academy of thal, 1989)
Pediatrics 2004, 208)
• Track moving objects with both
eyes together. (By 7 mos.; Ameri­
can Academy of Pediatrics 2004,
208)
6

Foundation: Gross Motor


The developing ability to move the large muscles
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, Around 18 months of age, chil­ At around 36 months of age, chil­
children demonstrate the ability to dren move from one place to an­ dren move with ease, coordinat­
maintain their posture in a sitting other by walking and running with ing movements and performing a
position and to shift between sit­ basic control and coordination. variety of movements.
ting and other positions.

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Sit on the floor, legs bent, with • Stand on one foot, alone or • Walk and run with skill, changing
one leg closer to the body than with support. (Scaled score speed and direction. (36 mos.;
the other. (8 mos.; Alexander, of 10 for 18:16–19:15 mos.; Parks 2004; by 36 mos.; Davies
Boehme, and Cupps 1993, 134) Bayley 2006, 163) 2004, 194).
• Use forearms to pull forward on • Walk sideways. (Scaled score • Kick and throw a ball, but with
the floor while on her tummy. of 10 for 18:16–19:15 mos.; little control of direction or
(Scaled score of 9 for 7:16–8:15 Bayley 2006, 163) speed. (36 mos.; Meisels and
mos.; Bayley 2006, 155) others 2003, 76)
• Push a doll stroller or play
PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT

• Move from a sitting position shopping cart. (17–18.5 mos.; • Bend over to pick up a toy and
onto hands and knees. (Scaled Parks 2004) stand up without trouble. (By
score of 10 for 7:16–8:15 mos.; 36 mos.; American Academy of
• Climb onto an adult-sized
Bayley 2006, 156) couch. (By 18 mos.; Apfel and Pediatrics 2004)
Provence 2001, 33) • Pedal a tricycle. (32–36 mos.;
Parks 2004; 36 mos.; Davies
• Run. (Scaled score of 10 for
2004, 194)
16:16–17:15 mos.; Bayley
2006, 162) • Climb up climbers and ladders.
(34–36 mos.; Parks 2004)
• Walk backward a few feet. (28–
29.5 mos.; Parks 2004; scaled
score of 10 for 34:16–35:15
mos.; Bayley 2006, #63, 167)
• Jump up with both feet at the
same time. (30–36 mos.; Parks
2004; by 30 mos.; Apfel and
Provence 2001, 33)
• Catch a medium-size ball.
(35–36+ mos.; Parks 2004)
• Walk up stairs, without holding
on, placing one foot on each
step. (30 mos.; Squires, Pot­
ter, and Bricker 1999; by end of
24–36 mos., 34–36+ mos.; Parks
2004, 304; scaled score of 10
for 35:16–36:15 mos.; Bayley
2006, 64)


Gross Motor

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Hold onto a foot while lying • Creep on hands and knees or • Jump off the bottom step. (24–
on her back. (Scaled score of hands and feet. (By 9 mos.; 26.5 mos.; Parks 2004; scaled
10 for 5:16–6:15 mos.; Bayley Apfel and Provence 2001, 31) score of 10 for 19:16–20:15
2006, 153) mos.; Bayley 2006, 164)
• Pull to a stand, using furniture
• Roll from back to stomach for support. (Scaled score of • Kick a ball. (Scaled score of 10
(4–6 mos.; Lerner and Ciervo 10 for 8:16–9:15 mos.; Bayley for 20:16–21:15 mos.; Bayley
2003) 2006, 157) 2006, 164; by 21 mos.; Apfel
and Provence 2001, 33)
• Roll from stomach to back. • Cruise while holding onto
(4–6 mos.; Lerner and Ciervo furniture. (9.61–13 mos.; Parks • Ride a ride-on toy without
2003; Bayley 2006, 25; 2004; scaled score of 10 for pedals, pushing her feet on the
5.38–7.5 mos.; Parks 2004) 9:16–10:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, ground to move. (18–24 mos.;
158) Parks 2004)
• Bring both hands to the mid­
line while lying on his back. (16 • Sit down from a standing • Walk up or down stairs by
weeks; Squires, Potter, and position. (Scaled score of 9 for stepping with both feet on
Bricker 1999) 10:16–11:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, each step, without holding on.

PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT


158) (Scaled score of 10 for up for
• Sit without support and turn
24:16–25:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
to the left or right to reach an • Walk without support. (Scaled
165)
object. (Scaled score of 7 for score of 9 for 12:16–13:15 mos.;
7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, Bayley 2006, 160; by 15 mos.; • Catch a big ball using two arms.
154; 7 mos.; Mercer 1998, Apfel and Provence 2001, 33) (24–26+ mos.; Parks 2004)
243) • Throw a ball, underhand or • Jump forward a few inches.
• Balance on one side, bearing overhand, to the infant care (Scaled score of 10 for 27:16–
weight on the lower hip, arm, teacher. (Scaled score of 10 for 28:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 166)
and leg, leaving the upper arm 12:16–13:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, • Walk on tiptoes. (Scaled score
and leg free to move and to 160) of 10 for 32:16–33:15 mos.;
manipulate objects. (7 mos.; • Squat to explore a toy on the Bayley 2006, 167; 36 mos.;
Alexander, Boehme, and ground and then stand up. Meisels and others 2003, 75)
Cupps 1993, 131–132). (Scaled score of 10 for 13:16–
• Move from hands and knees 14:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 160)
into a sitting position. (7 mos.; • Walk up or down stairs by step­
Alexander, Boehme, and ping with both feet on each step
Cupps 1993, 135) while holding a parent’s hand
• Rock on hands and knees, or the handrail. (Scaled score of
sometimes losing balance. 10 for up for 14:16–15:15 mos.;
(7 mos.; Alexander, Boehme, Bayley 2006, 161; for down for
and Cupps 1993, 138) 15:16–16:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
162)
• Get into a standing position
without support. (Around 11:15
mos.; Bayley 2006, 159)
• Crawl or creep up or down a
few steps.


Foundation: Fine Motor


The developing ability to move the small muscles
8 months 18 months 36 months
At around eight months of age, At around 18 months of age, At around 36 months of age, chil­
children easily reach for and children are able to hold small dren coordinate the fine movements
grasp things and use eyes and objects in one hand and some­ of the fingers, wrists, and hands to
hands to explore objects active­ times use both hands together skillfully manipulate a wide range
ly. (6 mos.; Alexander, Boehme, to manipulate objects. (18 mos.; of objects and materials in intricate
and Cupps 1993, 112) Meisels and others 2003, 40) ways. Children often use one hand
to stabilize an object while manipu­
lating it.

For example, the child may: For example, the child may: For example, the child may:
• Reach for and grasp an object, • Hold a crayon between fingers • Use child-safe scissors in one
using one hand. (5–8 mos.; and thumb. (13–18 mos.; Slat­ hand to make snips in a piece
Introduction to Infant Develop­ er and Lewis 2002, 62; scaled of paper. (Scaled score of 10 for
ment, 2002, 62; by end of 7 score of 10 for 17:16–18:15 34:16–35:15 mos.; Bayley 2006,
mos.; American Academy of mos.; Bayley 2006, 131) 136; 28–35 mos.; Parks 2004)
Pediatrics 2004, 205; 7–8 1/2 • Scribble with big arm move­ • String large wooden beads onto
PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT

mos.; Parks 2004) ments. (13–18 mos.; Introduc­ a shoelace. (33–36 mos.; Parks
• Use hand in a raking or tion to Infant Development, 2004)
sweeping motion to bring a toy 2002, 62) • Build a tall tower with six or more
closer. (7–8 mos.; Parks 2004; • Place pegs into a pegboard. blocks. (28–31 mos.; Parks 2004;
by end of 7 mos.; American (16–19 mos.; Parks 2004) by the end of 24–36 mos.; Ameri­
Academy of Pediatrics 2004,
• Hold a toy with one hand can Academy of Pediatrics 2004,
205; 7–8 mos.; Frankenburg
305)
and Dodds 1990) and use the fingers of the
other hand to explore it. (By • Turn the pages of a paper book,
• Hold a small block using the 18 mos.; Meisels and others one at a time. (By end of 24–36
thumb and fingertips. (item 2003, 40) mos.; American Academy of Pedi­
right before scaled score of atrics 2004, 305)
10 for 7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley • Point to the pictures of a book.
2006, 127) (By 18 mos.; Meisels and oth­ • Twist toy nuts and bolts on and
ers 2003, 40) off. (By end of 24–36 mos.; Ameri­
• Hold a small block in each can Academy of Pediatrics 2004,
hand and bang the blocks • Place a stacking ring on the
305)
together. (Scaled score of 10 post. (By 18 mos.; Meisels and
7:16–8:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, others 2003, 40) • Open a door by turning the round
127) handle. (By end of 24–36 mos.;
• Use two hands to pick up a
American Academy of Pediatrics
big truck, but only one hand
2004, 305)
to pick up a small one. (12–18
mos.; Parks 2004, 81) • Use one hand to hold and drink
from a cup. (By 36 mos.; Meisels
• Use the wrists to rotate
and others 2003, 77)
objects in order to explore all
sides. (18 mos.; Meisels and • Place a wooden puzzle piece in
others 2003, 40) the correct place in the puzzle.
• Use one hand in opposition • Use thumb, index, and middle
to the other. (18 mos.; Meisels fingers to draw or write with a
and others 2003, 40) crayon, marker, or pencil. (Scaled
score of 10 for 21:15–22:15
and 35:16–36:15 mos.; Bayley
2006, 136; by 36 mos.; Apfel and
Provence 2001, 33)


Fine Motor

Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the Behaviors leading up to the


foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months) foundation ( to  months)
During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may: During this period, the child may:
• Transfer a cloth from one hand • Hold on to two blocks while • Fold a piece of paper. (21–24
to another. (6 mos.; Alexander, reaching for another block. (8–10 mos.; Parks 2004)
Boehme, and Cupps 1993, mos.; Parks 2004, 50) • Dump a container by turning
110; scaled score of 10 for • Use thumb and index finger to it over. (By 24 mos.; American
5:16–6:15 mos.; Bayley 2006) pick up a piece of cereal. (Scaled Academy of Pediatrics 2004)
• Pull the spoon out of her
score of 10 for 9:16–10:15 mos.; • Use a crayon to draw lines and
mouth. (6 mos.; Alexander,
Bayley 2006, 128) circles on a piece of paper.
Boehme, and Cupps 1993,
• Drop a block into the wide open­ (Scaled score of 10 for 27:16–
111)
ing of a large container. (9 mos.; 28:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 134;
• Reach toward a toy and make Alexander, Boehme, and Cupps 30 mos.; Squires, Potter, and
grasping motions with the 1993, 157) Bricker 1999; by 30 mos.; Apfel
hand. (4–6 mos.; Lerner and and Provence 2001, 33)
• Turn the pages of a board book.
Ciervo 2003) (Scaled score of 10 for 9:16–
• Reach for a second toy when 10:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 128)
already holding one in the

PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT


• Use hands to follow along with
other hand. (5–6.5 mos.; Parks some motions of a song, chant,
2004, 49) or finger play. (Scaled score of
• Hold one block in each hand, 10 for 9:16–10:15 mos.; Bayley
then drop one of them when 2006, 87)
the infant care teacher holds • Grasp onto and pull the string
out a third block. (6.5–7.5 of a pull toy. (9–12 mos.; Parks
mos.; Parks 2004, 50) 2004, 51)
• Have the hands in an open po­ • Point with the index finger. (12
sition when relaxed. (4 mos.; mos.; Coplan 1993, 3; scaled
Meisels and others 2003, 14) score of 10 for 11:16–12:15
mos.; Bayley 2006, 129)
• Stack two to three small blocks
into a tower. (Scaled score of 10
for 13:16–15:15 mos.; Bayley
2006, 130)
• Unscrew the lid of a plastic jar.
(Scaled score of 10 for 14:16–
15:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 62)
• Put pieces of cereal inside a
container with a small opening.
(Scaled score of 10 for 16:16–
17:15 mos.; Bayley 2006, 130)
00

References

Adolph, K. E. 1997. “Learning in the Adolph, K. E.; I. Weise; and L. Marin.


Development of Infant Locomotion,” 2003. “Motor Development,” in Ency­
Monographs of the Society for Research clopedia of Cognitive Science. London:
in Child Development, Vol. 62, No. 3, Macmillan.
Serial No. 251. Alexander, R.; R. Boehme; and B. Cupps.
Adolph, K. E. 2008. “Motor/Physical Devel­ 1993. Normal Development of Functional
opment: Locomotion,” in Encyclopedia Motor Skills. San Antonio, TX: Therapy
of Infant and Early Childhood Develop­ Skill Builders.
ment. Edited by M. M. Haith and J. American Academy of Pediatrics. 2004.
B. Benson. San Diego, CA: Academic Caring for Your Baby and Young Child:
Press. Birth to Age 5 (Fourth edition). Edited
Adolph, K. E., and A. M. Avolio. 2000. by S. P. Shelov and R. E. Hannemann.
“Walking Infants Adapt Locomotion to New York: Bantam Books.
Changing Body Dimensions,” Journal Apfel, N. H., and S. Provence. 2001. Man­
of Experimental Psychology: Human ual for the Infant-Toddler and Family
PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT

Perception and Performance, Vol. 26, No. Instrument (ITFI). Baltimore: Brookes
3, 1148–66. Publishing.
Adolph, K. E., and S. E. Berger. 2005. Bahrick, L. E.; R. Lickliter; and R. Flom.
“Physical and Motor Development,” in 2004. “Intersensory Redundancy
Developmental Science: An Advanced Guides the Development of Selective
Textbook (Fifth edition). Edited by M. H. Attention, Perception, and Cognition in
Bornstein and M. E. Lamb. Hillsdale, Infancy,” Current Directions in Psycho­
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. logical Science, Vol. 13, No. 3, 99–102.
Adolph, K. E., and S. E. Berger. 2006. Bai, D. L., and B. I. Bertenthal. 1992.
“Motor Development,” in Handbook of “Locomotor Status and the Develop­
Child Psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, ment of Spatial Search Skills,” Child
Perception, and Language (Sixth edi­ Development, Vol. 63, 215–26.
tion). Series Editors: W. Damon and R. Bayley, N. 2006. Bayley Scales of Infant
Lerner. Volume Editors: D. Kuhn and and Toddler Development (Third edi­
others. New York: John Wiley and Sons. tion). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt
Adolph, K. E.; M. A. Eppler; and E. J. Assessment.
Gibson. 1993. “Crawling Versus Walk­ Berthier, N. E. 1996. “Learning to Reach:
ing Infants’ Perception of Affordances A Mathematical Model,” Developmental
for Locomotion over Sloping Surfaces,” Psychology, Vol. 32, No. 5, 811–23.
Child Development, Vol. 64, No. 4, Bertenthal, B. I. 1996. “Origins and Early
1158–74. Development of Perception, Action and
Adolph, K. E., and A. S. Joh. 2007. “Motor Representation,” Annual Review of Psy­
Development: How Infants Get Into the chology, Vol. 47, 431–59.
Act,” in Introduction to Infant Develop­ Bertenthal, B. I., and S. M. Boker. 1997.
ment (Second edition). Edited by A. “New Paradigms and New Issues: A
Slater and M. Lewis. New York: Oxford Comment on Emerging Themes in the
University Press. Study of Motor Development,” Mono­
Adolph, K. E.; B. Vereijken; and P. E. graphs of the Society for Research in
Shrout. 2003. “What Changes in Infant Child Development, Vol. 62, No. 3,
Walking and Why,” Child Development, 141–51.
Vol. 74, No. 2, 475–97.
0

Bornstein, M. H. 2005. “Perceptual Devel­ Haith, M. M.; C. Hazen; and G. S. Good­


opment,” in Developmental Science: An man. 1988. “Expectation and Anticipa­
Advanced Textbook (Fifth edition). tion of Dynamic Visual Events by 3.5­
Edited by M. H. Bornstein and M. E. Month-Old Babies,” Child Development,
Lamb. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Vol. 59, 467–79.
Associates. Introduction to Infant Development. 2002.
Bushnell, E. W., and J. P. Boudreau. 1993. Edited by A. Slater and M. Lewis. New
“Motor Development and the Mind: the York: Oxford University Press.
Potential Role of Motor Abilities as a Lerner, C., and L. A. Ciervo. 2003. Healthy
Determinant of Aspects of Perceptual Minds: Nurturing Children’s Develop­
Development,” Child Development, Vol. ment from 0 to 36 Months. Washington,
64, 1005–21. DC: Zero to Three Press and American
Campos, J. J., and B. I. Bertenthal. 1989. Academy of Pediatrics.
“Locomotion and Psychological Devel­ McCarty, M. E., and others. 2001. “How
opment in Infancy” in Applied Develop­ Infants Use Vision for Grasping
mental Psychology. Edited by F. Morri­ Objects,” Child Development, Vol. 72,
son, C. Lord, and D. Keating. New York: No. 4, 973–87.
Academic Press. Meisels, S. J., and others. 2003. The
Coplan, J. 1993. Early Language Milestone Ounce Scale: Standards for the Develop­

PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT


Scale (Second edition). Austin, TX: Pro­ mental Profiles (Birth–42 Months). New
ed. York: Pearson Early Learning.
Davies, D. 2004. Child Development: A Mercer, J. 1998. Infant Development: A Mul­
Practitioner’s Guide (Second edition). tidisciplinary Introduction. Pacific Grove,
New York: Guilford Press. CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing.
Diamond, A. 2007. “Interrelated and Inter­ Palmer, C. F. 1989. “The Discriminating
dependent,” Developmental Science, Vol. Nature of Infants’ Exploratory Actions,”
10, No. 1, 152–58. Developmental Psychology, Vol. 25, No.
Fogel, A. 2001. Infancy: Infant, Family, and 6, 885–93.
Society (Fourth edition). Belmont, CA: Parks, S. 2004. Inside HELP: Hawaii Early
Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Learning Profile: Administration and
Frankenburg, W. K., and others. 1990. Reference Manual. Palo Alto, CA: VORT
Denver II Screening Manual. Denver, Corporation.
CO: Denver Developmental Materials. Pick, H. L. 1989. “Motor Development: The
Freeman, N. H. 1980. Strategies of Repre­ Control of Action,” Developmental Psy­
sentation in Young Children: Analysis chology, Vol. 25, No. 6, 867–70.
of Spatial Skill and Drawing Processes. Reardon, P., and E. W. Bushnell. 1988.
London: Academic Press. “Infants’ Sensitivity to Arbitrary Pair­
Gibson, E. J. 1987. “What Does Infant ings of Color and Taste,” Infant Behavior
Perception Tell Us About Theories of and Development, Vol. 11, 245–50.
Perception?” Journal of Experimental Ruff, H. A., and C. J. Kohler. 1978. “Tac­
Psychology: Human Perception and Per­ tual-Visual Transfer in Six-Month-Old
formance, Vol. 13, No. 4, 515–23. Infants,” Infant Behavior and Develop­
Gibson, E. J. 1988. “Exploratory Behavior ment, Vol. 1, 259–64.
in the Development of Perceiving, Act­ Squires, J.; L. Potter; and D. Bricker.
ing and the Acquiring of Knowledge,” 1999. The Ages and Stages Question­
Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 39, naires User’s Guide (Second edition).
No. 1, 1–41. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Pub­
lishing.
0

Stiles, J. 1995. “The Early Use and Devel­ Thelen, E. 1994. “Three-Month-Old Infants
opment of Graphic Formulas: Two Case Can Learn Task-Specific Patterns of
Study Reports of Graphic Formula Pro­ Interlimb Coordination,” Psychological
duction by 2- to 3-Year Old Children,” Science, Vol. 5, No. 5, 280–85.
International Journal of Behavioral Thelen, E. 1995. “Motor Development: A
Development, Vol. 18, No. 1, 127–49. New Synthesis,” American Psychologist,
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and K. E. Adolph. Vol. 50, No. 2, 79–95.
2005. “Social Cognition in Infant Motor Walk, R. D., and E. J. Gibson. 1961. “A
Action,” in The Development of Social Comparative and Analytic Study of
Cognition and Communication. Edited Visual Depth Perception,” Psychological
by B. Homer and C. S. Tamis-LeMonda. Monographs, Vol. 75, No. 15.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso­ von Hofsten, C. 2007. “Action in Develop­
ciates. ment,” Developmental Science, Vol. 10,
No. 1, 54–60.
PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR DEVELOPMENT
Appendix

Summary of Infant/Toddler Foundations

Social-Emotional Development Social Understanding: The developing


Interactions with Adults: The developing understanding of the responses, com­
ability to respond to and engage with munication, emotional expressions, and
adults actions of other people

Relationships with Adults: The develop­ Language Development


ment of close relationships with certain
adults who provide consistent nurtur­ Receptive Language: The developing ability
ance to understand words and increasingly
complex utterances
Interactions with Peers: The developing
ability to respond to and engage with Expressive Language: The developing abil­
other children ity to produce the sounds of language
and use vocabulary and increasingly
Relationships with Peers: The development complex utterances
of relationships with certain peers
through interactions over time Communication Skills and Knowledge:
The developing ability to communicate
Identity of Self in Relation to Others: The nonverbally and verbally
developing concept that the child is an
individual operating within social rela­ Interest in Print: The developing interest in
tionships engaging with print in books and in the
environment
Recognition of Ability: The developing
understanding that the child can take
Cognitive Development
action to influence the environment
Cause-and-Effect: The developing under­
Expression of Emotion: The developing
standing that one event brings about
ability to express a variety of feelings
another
through facial expressions, movements,
gestures, sounds, or words Spatial Relationships: The developing
understanding of how things move and
Empathy: The developing ability to share
fit in space
in the emotional experiences of others
Problem Solving: The developing ability to
Emotion Regulation: The developing ability
engage in a purposeful effort to reach a
to manage emotional responses with
goal or figure out how something works
assistance from others and indepen­
dently Imitation: The developing ability to mirror,
repeat, and practice the actions of oth­
Impulse Control: The developing capacity
ers, either immediately or later
to wait for needs to be met, to inhibit
potentially hurtful behavior, and to Memory: The developing ability to store
act according to social expectations, and later retrieve information about
including safety rules past experiences

0
0

Number Sense: The developing under­ Perceptual and Motor


standing of number and quantity Development
Classification: The developing ability to Perceptual Development: The developing
group, sort, categorize, connect, and ability to become aware of the social
have expectations of objects and people and physical environment through the
according to their attributes senses
Symbolic Play: The developing ability to Gross Motor: The developing ability to
use actions, objects, or ideas to repre­ move the large muscles
sent other actions, objects, or ideas
Fine Motor: The developing ability to move
Attention Maintenance: The developing the small muscles
ability to attend to people and things
while interacting with others and
exploring the environment and play
materials
Understanding of Personal Care Routines:
The developing ability to understand
and participate in personal care rou­
tines
Price List and Order Form

ModulE I ModulE II ModulE III ModulE IV

Social–Emotional Group Care learning and Culture,


Growth and DVDs and Magazines development Family, and
Socialization It’s Not Just Routine: DVDs and Magazines
Providers
Feeding, Diapering,
DVDs and Magazines and Napping Infants and The Ages of Infancy: DVDs and Magazines
Toddlers Caring for Young,
First Moves: Essential Connections:
(Second edition) Mobile, and Older
Welcoming a Child Ten Keys to Culturally
Infants
to a New Caregiving Respectfully Yours: Sensitive Child Care
Setting Magda Gerber’s Discoveries of Infancy:
Protective Urges:
Approach to Cognitive Development
Flexible, Fearful, or Feisty: Working with the
Professional Infant/ and Learning
The Different Feelings of Parents
Temperaments of Infants Toddler Care Early Messages: and Caregivers
and Toddlers Space to Grow: Facilitating Language
Development and Print Materials
Getting in Tune: Creating a Child Care
Environment for Infants Communication Infant/Toddler
Creating Nurturing
Relationships with Infants and Toddlers The Next Step: Caregiving:
and Toddlers (Second edition) Including the Infant A Guide to Creating
in the Curriculum Partnerships with
Together in Care:
Print Materials Families
Meeting the Intimacy
Print Materials
Infant/Toddler Caregiving: Needs of Infants Infant/Toddler
A Guide to Social– and Toddlers in Groups Infant/Toddler Caregiving:
Emotional Growth Caregiving: A Guide to Culturally
and Socialization Print Materials A Guide to Cognitive Sensitive Care
Infant/Toddler Development and
Module I Trainer’s Module IV Trainer’s
Caregiving: A Guide Learning
Manual Manual
to Routines Infant/Toddler
(Second edition) Caregiving:
Infant/Toddler A Guide to Language
Caregiving: A Guide Development and
to Setting Up Communication
Environments Module III Trainer’s
Module II Trainer’s Manual
Manual

Each module includes DVDs, video magazines, curriculum guide(s), and a trainer’s manual.
All modules are available in English and Spanish. Additional PITC materials and new infant/
toddler items from the California Department of Education are listed on the last page of this
order form.
http://www.cde.ca.gov/re/pn/rc/documents/yr17pitcof.pdf
ModulE I
Social–Emotional Growth
and Socialization
Title Item no. Quantity Price Total

Audiovisual Materials
DVD (Each DVD includes a video magazine in the same language
as the DVD.)
First Moves (1988) 1636 $75.00 $
Los primeros pasos (1988) 1637 75.00
Flexible, Fearful, or Feisty (1990) 1638 75.00
Flexible, cauteloso, o inquieto (1990) 1639 75.00
Getting in Tune (1990) 1644 75.00
Llevar el compás (1990) 1645 75.00
Print Materials
Curriculum Guides
A Guide to Social–Emotional Growth and Socialization (1990) 0876 $18.00
Una guía para el crecimiento socioemocional y la socialización (2005) 1606 18.00
Module I Trainer’s Manual (1993) 1084 25.00
Manual pedagógico, Módulo I: El crecimiento socioemocional
y la socialización (2007) 1660 25.00
Video Magazines (In packages of 50)
First Moves - English 9960 $23.00
First Moves - Spanish 9736 23.00
Flexible, Fearful, or Feisty - English 9956 23.00
Flexible, Fearful, or Feisty - Spanish 9737 23.00
Getting in Tune - English 9957 23.00
Getting in Tune - Spanish 9738 23.00

MODULE I: SOCIAL–EMOTIONAL GROWTH AND SOCIALIZATION


PACKAGE—$239
Price includes 3 videos, 3 accompanying video magazines, 1 curriculum guide,
and 1 trainer’s manual.
English DVDs/English guides/English manual 9696 $239.00
Spanish DVDs/Spanish guides/Spanish manual 9695 239.00

SUBTOTAL $

06 PITC Price List and Order Form


ModulE II
Group Care
Title Item no. Quantity Price Total

Audiovisual Materials
DVD (Each DVD includes a video magazine in the same language
as the DVD.)
It’s Not Just Routine (Second edition) (2000) 1648 $75.00 $
No es sólo una rutina (Segunda edición) (2000) 1649 75.00
Respectfully Yours (1988) 1640 75.00
Con respeto (1988) 1641 75.00
Space to Grow (Second edition) (2004) 1646 75.00
Un lugar para crecer (Segunda edición) (2004) 1647 75.00
Together in Care (1992) 1632 75.00
Unidos en el corazón (1992) 1633 75.00

Print Materials
Curriculum Guides
A Guide to Routines (Second edition) (2000) 1510 $18.00
Una guía para las rutinas cotidianas del cuidado infantil
(Segunda edición) (2004) 1602 18.00
A Guide to Setting Up Environments (1990) 0879 18.00
Una guía para crear los ambientes del cuidado infantil (2006) 1614 18.00
Module II Trainer’s Manual (1993) 1076 25.00
Manual pedagógico, Módulo II: El cuidado infantil en grupo (2007) 1661 25.00
Video Magazines (In packages of 50)
It’s Not Just Routine (Second edition) - English 9724 $23.00
It’s Not Just Routine (Second edition) - Spanish 9723 23.00
Respectfully Yours - English 9958 23.00
Respectfully Yours - Spanish 9740 23.00
Space to Grow (Second edition) - English 9709 23.00
Space to Grow (Second edition) - Spanish 9710 23.00
Together in Care - English 9873 23.00
Together in Care - Spanish 9742 23.00

MODULE II: GROUP CARE PACKAGE—$319


Price includes 4 videos, 4 accompanying video magazines, 2 curriculum
guides, and 1 trainer’s manual.
English DVDs/English guides/English manual 9694 $319.00
Spanish DVDs/Spanish guides/Spanish manual 9693 319.00

SUBTOTAL $

PITC Price List and Order Form 0


ModulE III
learning and development
Title Item no. Quantity Price Total

Audiovisual Materials
DVD (Each DVD includes a video magazine in the same language
as the DVD.)
The Ages of Infancy (1990) 1634 $75.00 $
Las edades de la infancia (1990) 1635 75.00
Discoveries of Infancy (1992) 1623 75.00
Descubrimientos de la infancia (1992) 1624 75.00
Early Messages (1998) 1625 75.00
El comenzar de la comunicación (1998) 1626 75.00
The Next Step (2004) 1621 75.00
El siguiente paso (2004) 1622 75.00

Print Materials
Curriculum Guides
A Guide to Cognitive Development and Learning (1995) 1055 $18.00
Una guía para el desarrollo cognitivo y el aprendizaje (2006) 1616 18.00
A Guide to Language Development and Communication (1990) 0880 18.00
Una guía para el desarrollo del lenguaje y la comunicación (2006) 1608 18.00
Module III Trainer’s Manual (1993) 1108 25.00
Manual pedagógico, Módulo III: Aprendizaje y desarrollo (2007) 1662 25.00
Video Magazines (In packages of 50)
The Ages of Infancy - English 9954 $23.00
The Ages of Infancy - Spanish 9732 23.00
Discoveries of Infancy - English 9874 23.00
Discoveries of Infancy - Spanish 9733 23.00
Early Messages - English 9747 23.00
Early Messages - Spanish 9734 23.00
The Next Step - English 9715 23.00
The Next Step - Spanish 9697 23.00

MODULE III: LEARNING AND DEVELOPMENT PACKAGE—$249


Price includes 4 videos, 4 accompanying video magazines, 2 curriculum
guides, and 1 trainer’s manual.
English DVDs/English guides/English manual 9692 $249.00
Spanish DVDs/Spanish guides/English manual 9691 249.00

SUBTOTAL $

0 PITC Price List and Order Form


ModulE IV
Culture, Family, and Providers
Title Item no. Quantity Price Total

Audiovisual Materials
DVD (Each DVD includes a video magazine in the same language
as the DVD.)
Essential Connections (1993) 1627 $75.00 $
Relaciones indispensables (1993) 1628 75.00
Protective Urges (1996) 1630 75.00
El instinto protector (1996) 1631 75.00

Print Materials
Curriculum Guides
A Guide to Creating Partnerships with Families (1990) 0878 $18.00
Una guía para establecer relaciones de colaboración con las familias (2006) 1615 18.00
A Guide to Culturally Sensitive Care (1995) 1057 18.00
Una guía para el cuidado infantil culturalmente sensible (2006) 1617 18.00
Module IV Trainer’s Manual (1995) 1109 25.00
Manual pedagógico, Módulo IV: La cultura, la familia y los proveedores
(2007) 1663 25.00
Video Magazines (In packages of 50)
Essential Connections - English 9869 $23.00
Essential Connections - Spanish 9735 23.00
Protective Urges - English 9778 23.00
Protective Urges - Spanish 9739 23.00

MODULE IV: CULTURE, FAMILY, AND PROVIDERS


PACKAGE—$189
Price includes 2 videos, 2 accompanying video magazines, 2 curriculum
guides, and 1 trainer’s manual.
English DVDs/English guides/English manual 9690 $189.00
Spanish DVDs/Spanish guides/Spanish manual 9689 189.00

Supplemental Materials to Module IV


DVD (Each DVD includes a video magazine.)
Talking Points for Essential Connections - English (1998) 1643 $35.00
Talking Points for Protective Urges - English (1998) 1642 25.00
Video Magazines (In packages of 50)
Talking Points for Essential Connections - English 9744 $23.00
Talking Points for Protective Urges - English 9743 23.00

SUBTOTAL $

PITC Price List and Order Form 0


Additional Materials
Title Item no. Quantity Price Total

Audiovisual Materials
DVD (includes a video magazine)
In Our Hands - English (1997) 1629 $25.00 $

Print Materials
Addendum to Trainer’s Manuals I, II, III, IV: Spanish Handouts/Transparencies
(Second edition) 1679 $25.00
The Family Child Care Supplement to Trainer’s Manuals 7096 25.00
Video Magazine (In packages of 50)
In Our Hands - English (1997) 9749 23.00

Materials from the California Department of Education


Infant/Toddler Learning and Development: Program Guidelines (2006) 1619 $19.95
New Perspectives (DVD) (2007) 1665 29.00

SUBTOTAL $
California residents
add sales tax.
Shipping and handling
charges (See chart.)
TOTAL $

To order call: 1-800-995-4099


BUSINESS HOURS: 8:00 A.M.–4:30 P.M., PST, MONDAY THROUGH FRIDAY

NAME/ATTENTION
PAYMENT METHOD:
■ CHECK (Payable to California
ADDRESS Department of Education)
■ VISA
CITY STATE ZIP CODE

( )
■ MASTERCARD
COUNTY DAYTIME TELEPHONE ■ PURCHASE ORDER

SHIPPING CHARGES CREDIT CARD NUMBER


All orders to be delivered within
Number of Items the continental United States
1–50 $4.95 per order plus are shipped via ground service EXPIRATION DATE
$1.00 per item unless expedited shipping is
requested.
51+ Call 1-800-995-4099 AUTHORIZED SIGNATURE
for discounted rate Additional charges apply on
shipments to Alaska and Hawaii.
Note: Shipping and handling charges Mail completed order form to:
for modules are $5.95 for each module. California Department of Education
Visit our Web site: http://www.cde.ca.gov/re/pn CDE Press Sales Office
■ Please send me a free copy of the current 1430 N Street, Suite 3207
Educational Resources Catalog. Sacramento, CA 95814-5901

Note: Mail orders must be accompanied by a check, a purchase order, or a VISA or MasterCard credit card number, including expiration
date and your signature. Purchase orders without checks are accepted from educational institutions, businesses, and governmental agencies.
Purchase orders may be placed by faxing to (916) 323-0823. Telephone orders will be accepted toll-free (1-800-995-4099) for credit card
purchases. Please do not send cash. Stated prices are subject to change. Please order carefully; include correct item number and quantity
for each publication ordered. All sales are final.

0 PITC Price List and Order Form 08-002 PR070082-00 1-09 77M

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen