Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological


Psychiatry
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/pnp

Cognitive improvement by acute growth hormone is mediated by


NMDA and AMPA receptors and MEK pathway
Margarita Ramis a, Fiorella Sarubbo a, Jessica Sola a, Sara Aparicio a, Celia Garau b,
Antonio Miralles a, Susana Esteban a,⁎
a
Laboratorio de Neurofisiología, Departamento de Biología, Universitat de les Illes Balears (UIB), Cra. Valldemossa km 7.5, E-07122 Palma de Mallorca, Spain
b
Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: It has been reported that Growth hormone (GH) has an immediate effect enhancing excitatory postsynaptic
Received 15 February 2013 potentials mediated by AMPA and NMDA receptors in hippocampal area CA1. As GH plays a role in adult
Received in revised form 5 April 2013 memory processing, this work aims to study the acute effects of GH on working memory tasks in rodents
Accepted 9 April 2013
and the possible involvement of NMDA and AMPA receptors and also the MEK/ERK signalling pathway. To
Available online 13 April 2013
evaluate memory processes, two different tests were used, the spatial working memory 8-arm radial maze,
Keywords:
and the novel object recognition as a form of non-spatial working memory test. Acute GH treatment
AMPA (1 mg/kg i.p., 1 h) improved spatial learning in the radial maze respect to the control group either in
ERK young rats (reduction of 46% in the performance trial time and 61% in the number of errors), old rats (reduction
Growth hormone of 38% in trial time and 48% in the number of errors), and adult mice (reduction of 32% in the performance time
NMDA and 34% in the number of errors). GH treatment also increased the time spent exploring the novel object respect
Object recognition to the familiar object compared to the control group in young rats (from 63% to 79%), old rats (from 53% to 70%),
Radial maze and adult mice (from 61 to 68%). The improving effects of GH on working memory tests were blocked by the
NMDA antagonist MK801 dizocilpine (0.025 mg/kg i.p.) injected 10 min before the administration of GH, in
both young and old rats. In addition, the AMPA antagonist DNQX (1 mg/kg i.p.) injected 10 min before the
administration of GH to young rats, blocked the positive effect of GH. Moreover, in mice, the MEK inhibitor SL
327 (20 mg/kg i.p.) injected 30 min before the administration of GH, blocked the positive effect of GH on radial
maze and the novel object recognition. In conclusion, GH improved working memory processes through both
glutamatergic receptors NMDA and AMPA and it required the activation of extracellular MEK/ERK signalling
pathway. These effects could be related to the enhancement of excitatory synaptic transmission in the hippocampus
reported by GH.
© 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction WM declines in late adulthood (Bopp and Verhaeghen, 2005;


Borella et al., 2008; Payer et al., 2006) and is considered to be one
Working memory (WM) is the ability to maintain and manipulate of the main contributing factors of cognitive impairment in older
information over short periods of time, a key function for several adults (Park et al., 2002) and many neuropsychiatric disorders. It is
higher-order cognitive functions from childhood to old age (for review thus not surprising that attempts to improve WM have been far
see Klingberg, 2010). explored and a large number of studies have been investigating the
trainability of WM across the lifespan (Morrison and Chein, 2011;
Shipstead et al., 2010; Takeuchi et al., 2010 among others).
Growth hormone (GH, somatotropin) plays an important role in
Abbreviations: AMPA, α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid; the central nervous system throughout life, being essential for normal
DNQX, 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione; ERK, extracellular signal-regulated kinase;
growth and development of the nervous system (Harvey and Hull,
GABAA, γ-aminobutyric acid type A; GH, growth hormone; MAPK, mitogen activated
protein kinase; MEK, MAPK/ERK kinase; MK801, dizocilpine; NMDA, N-methyl-D-aspartate; 2003; Noguchi, 1996; Scheepens et al., 2000). There is growing
SL 327, α-amino-(4-aminophenyl-thio)-methylene-2-(trifluoromethyl)-benzene- evidence that GH might influence some functions of the central
acetonitrile; WM, working memory. nervous system (CNS) including its effects on cognitive functions,
⁎ Corresponding author. memory, mood, neuroprotection, sleep, and well-being (Nyberg, 2000).
E-mail addresses: margaramis87@hotmail.com (M. Ramis),
fioresarubbo@hotmail.com (F. Sarubbo), jessik_ss@hotmail.com (J. Sola),
Decreased GH secretion in some physiological or pathophysiological
sara.aparicio@alvarez.es (S. Aparicio), cgarauga@uci.edu (C. Garau), amiralles@uib.es conditions has been associated with cognitive impairments (Aleman
(A. Miralles), susana.esteban@uib.es (S. Esteban). et al., 2000; de Boer et al., 1995; Deijen et al., 1996; Hoffman et al.,

0278-5846/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.04.005
12 M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20

1992; Nyberg, 2000), which was often resolved following GH replace- conditions of temperature (22 ± 2 °C), humidity (70%) and light/dark
ment (Arwert et al., 2005a, 2005b, 2006). GH deficiency during adoles- cycle (light period: 8:00 a.m. to 20:00 p.m.). They had free access to a
cence has negative effects on learning and memory that emerge by standard diet (Panlab A04) and tap water. The animals were handled
middle age unless prevented by GH supplementation (Nieves-Martinez daily for several days before the behavioural experiments (see below)
et al., 2010). The age-related decline in GH is one of the most robust to reduce stress and aggressiveness that may otherwise arise during
endocrine markers of biological ageing and has been hypothesized to testing. Every effort was made to minimize the number of animals
contribute to the physiological deficits observed in aged subjects. used and their discomfort. Experimental procedures were conducted
Consequently, GH treatment may attenuate effects of ageing on according to standard ethical guidelines (European Communities
memory (Esteban et al., 2010b; Ramsey et al., 2004; Thornton et al., Council Directive 86/609/EEC) and approved by the Local Bioethical
2000) and improves attention performance in adult patients suffering Committee of the University of the Balearic Islands (UIB).
from hypopituitarism with GH deficiency (Oertel et al., 2004). Although
the primary source of GH is the anterior pituitary, there are other sites
for GH production, including the hippocampus (Gossard et al., 1987; 2.2. GH treatment and effect of AMPA and NMDA receptor blockade
Hojvat et al., 1982). Moreover the hippocampus contains GH receptors
(Burton et al., 1992; Lobie et al., 1993; Mustafa et al., 1994), and hippo- Young and old male Wistar rats (weighing 280 ± 15 and 500 ± 20 g,
campal function is affected by GH administration or deficiency (Le respectively) from Charles River (Barcelona, Spain) were used. GH
Greves et al., 2002; Ramsey et al., 2004; van Dam et al., 2000). Therefore, hormone can cross the blood–brain barrier in specific regions (Nyberg,
the effects of GH on memory process may be mediated by the hippo- 2000). Thus, rats were treated with saline or GH (1 mg/kg dissolved in
campus (Nyberg, 2000). 0.9% saline, i.p.) 1 h before the working memory tests. The dose of GH
The effects of GH on hippocampal function may occur as a result of and route for the administration was selected from previous works
modulating glutamatergic synaptic transmission in as much as facili- (Jaworek et al., 2009). Different groups of rats received the AMPA antag-
tation of glutamatergic transmission causes a general improvement in onist DNQX (1 mg/kg dissolved in dimethyl sulfoxide, i.p.) or the NMDA
memory encoding (Staubli et al., 1994). Glutamate is the primary antagonist MK801 dizocilpine (0.025 mg/kg dissolved in 0.9% saline i.p.)
excitatory neurotransmitter in the hippocampus and glutamatergic 10 min before the administration of GH in order to blockade the respec-
synaptic transmission involves the activation of ionotropic receptors tive receptors. Also, the acute effect of DNQX and MK801 alone were
which include the α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic analysed on memory tests 70 min after the treatment.
acid (AMPA) and the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) types of glutamate
receptors (Hollmann and Heinemann, 1994). NMDA receptor is 2.3. GH treatment and in vivo inhibition of ERK activation
known to be essential for the induction of synaptic plasticity and mem-
ory formation (Cao et al., 2007; Tang et al., 1999; Tsien et al., 1996) and Male Swiss albino CD1 middle-age mice (weighing 30 ± 6 g)
the levels of these hippocampal receptors are related to performance on (Charles River, Barcelona) were used. Groups of mice were treated
the WM task (Adams et al., 2001). In the same way, activation of AMPA with saline and GH (1 mg/kg i.p.) 1 h before the working memory
receptors selectively increases hippocampal cell firing and it improves tests. A different group of mice was injected with the MEK inhibitor
both acquisition performance and memory retention (Granger et al., SL 327 (20 mg/kg dissolved in dimethyl sulfoxide, i.p.) 30 min before
1996; Lynch, 2006). the administration of GH to compare with the effect of GH alone in
Recent studies show that GH improved the excitatory postsynap- mice. The dose of SL327 and route for the administration were select-
tic potentials mediated by both AMPA and NMDA receptors in hippo- ed from previous works performed to assess the optimal dose of SL
campal slices, without alter the γ-aminobutyric acid type A (GABAA) 327 that would block substantially ERK1/2 phosphorylation without
receptor-mediated inhibitory synaptic transmission (Mahmoud and altering other proteins in the rat brain (Moranta et al., 2007). We
Grover, 2006; Molina et al., 2012). As this enhanced signal detection observed previously that SL 327 (10, 20 and 30 mg/kg, i.p., 90 min)
by glutamate receptors should improve learning and memory, the decreased dose-dependently the content of p-ERK1/2 and increased
present study aims to investigate whether the GH inducing effects that of p-MEK1/2 (Ramos-Miguel et al., 2010). In addition, SL 327
on hippocampal synaptic excitability through AMPA and NMDA alone (at the dose of 20 mg/kg) produced no changes in behaviour
receptors positively correlate with functional effects on working mem- or motor activity (data not shown).
ory processes. Accordingly, the present study assessed the acute effects
of GH on working memory tasks in young and old rats and middle-age
mice. The promising involvement of AMPA and NMDA receptors on the 2.4. Radial maze task
acute effects of GH on working memory was investigated in young rats.
Also, the involvement of NMDA receptors on the acute effects of GH was Radial maze test was measured as previously published to test
explored in old rats. In addition, the implication of the ERK (extracellu- spatial working memory in rats (Esteban et al., 2010a, 2010b). The
lar signal-regulated kinase, subfamily of MAPS kinase) signalling path- 8-arm radial maze (Panlab, S.L., Barcelona, Spain) consisted of an
way was evaluated in mice because several behavioural memory octagonal central platform (32 cm diameter) with eight equally
paradigms have implicated ERK as an essential component of the signal spaced radial arms (50 cm long, 12 cm wide). To test memory on
transduction mechanisms involved in memory formation (Sweatt, radial maze, animals were allowed to visit the arms to obtain food
2004). To evaluate memory processes, two different hippocampal- pellets until the eight arms had been visited or 20 min had elapsed.
dependent tests were used, the spatial working memory 8-arm radial Animals were previously submitted to 48 h fasting to achieve a con-
maze, and the novel object recognition test as a form of non-spatial venient motivational level. Thus, trials were judged complete when
working memory. animals had visited all eight arms or spent 20 min in the trial
(maxim time to achieve performance). The maze was set in an exper-
2. Methods and materials imental room with several external visual cues. The experimenter
monitored the movements of the animals via a digital video tracking
2.1. Animals system (LE 8300 with software SEDACOM v 1.3, Panlab, SL, Barcelona
Spain). The sum of non-visited arms and re-entry into arms was
Animals were initially housed in the animal facilities in transparent scored as a working memory error. The apparatus was cleaned with
methacrylate cages (Panlab s.l., Barcelona) with wood shavings for ethanol solution between trials. All procedures were performed dur-
nesting material (Ultrasorb, Panlab) under controlled environmental ing the light period.
M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20 13

2.5. Novel object recognition test First, impairment of working memory was observed in old control
rats compared to younger animals in the radial maze (Fig. 1).
The novel object recognition, based on spontaneous exploratory Two-way ANOVA for time performance on the radial maze detected sig-
activity, is a test to measure a form of non-spatial working memory nificant differences between ages (F(1,63) = 162, p b 0.0001) and treat-
in rats and mice (Clark et al., 2000; Okamura et al., 2011). The novel ments (saline, GH and MK801 + GH) (F(2,63) = 31.04, p b 0.0001), but
object recognition test was performed in the open field device (cylinder not for the interaction between ages and treatments (F(2,63) = 2.57,
diameter: 78 cm, height walls: 60 cm). Animals were habituated to the p = 0.0846). Similarly, two-way ANOVA for errors number on the
open field without objects for 10 min daily for four consecutive days. On radial maze detected significant differences between ages (F(1,63) =
day 5, each animal was placed in the apparatus and allowed to explore 22.78, p b 0.0001) and treatments (saline, GH and MK801 + GH)
for 1 min for re-habituation. Then, in the training phase, animals were (F(2,63) = 17.08, p b 0.0001) but not for the interaction between ages
placed in the centre of the apparatus with two identical objects placed and treatments (F(2,63) = 1.15, p = 0.3224). The analysis indicated
16 cm away from the walls and allowed to explore both objects until that the effect of GH was independent of age. Old control rats need
to accumulate 30 s of exploration time or 10 min had elapsed. Object more time to complete the trial and commit more errors than young
exploration was recorded by an experienced observer only when the control animals in the maze test (p b 0.0001) (Fig. 1). The acute admin-
animal's nose or mouth was in contact with the object. After the delay istration of GH significantly improved the spatial memory in the radial
(10 min, short term memory), in the test phase, the animals were maze respect to the control group in both young and old rats (Fig. 1).
placed back in the open field with one object familiar (the same as the Two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni test showed that GH reduced
previous phase) and one novel object. The time spent exploring the the time required to complete the test respect to the control in both
objects was recorded until each animal accumulated 30 s of object young (46% reduction, p b 0.001, Fig. 1A) and aged rats (38% reduction,
exploration or 10 min had elapsed. The test phase reflects the prefer- p b 0.001, Fig. 1B). Also, GH-treated rats reduced the number of errors
ence for the novel object. Recognition index for the novel object was in the radial maze (no visited plus repeated visited arms) respect to
calculated as the ratio between time spent exploring the novel object the corresponding control group in both young (61% reduction,
with respect to overall exploring time, expressed by TN/(TN + TF) p b 0.001, Fig. 1C), and aged rats (48% error reduction, p b 0.001,
ratio [TF = time spent exploring familiar object; TN = time spent Fig. 1D).
exploring the novel object]. During training session both objects are Furthermore, deterioration in performance on the object recogni-
novel, the time spent on both objects should be similar and the recogni- tion task was observed with age (Fig. 2). When compare the time
tion index should be around 0.5. spent exploring the novel object (novel-familiar), two-way ANOVA
The objects were always placed in the same location, 16 cm from the detected significant differences between ages (F(1,70) = 11.58, p b
wall and 37 cm apart. Velcro into the base of the objects was used to 0.0011) and treatments (saline, GH and MK801 + GH) (F(2,70) =
secure the objects in place. In addition, the objects to be explored 16.04, p b 0.0001), but not for the interaction between ages and
were made of plastic in three different shapes that had no significance treatments (F(2,70) = 1.58, p = 0.2127) (Fig. 2C, D). The analysis in-
for animals and had never been associated with reinforcement. The dicated that the effect of GH was independent of age. Young rats
objects and the apparatus were cleaned with ethanol solution between spent more time exploring the novel object than old rats in the test
trials. All procedures were performed during the light period. session (p = 0.0013) (Fig. 2A, B). As observed in Table 1, during
training session, the time spent on both objects was similar and the
2.6. Statistical analysis recognition index was around 0.5. An increase in the object recogni-
tion index from training phase to test session is observed in all ani-
All series of data were analysed with the program Graph-Pad mals except to the old control group. The novel object recognition
Prism, version 3.0. Results are expressed as mean ± SEM. One-way test was significantly improved by acute administration of GH
and two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA), followed by post hoc (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h) in young and old rats (Table 1, Fig. 2). Thus,
test were used for the statistical evaluations. The level of significance two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni test showed that GH admin-
was p b 0.05. istered sixty minutes prior to the training session, significantly in-
creased the time spent exploring the novel object in both young
(from 63% to 79% of total exploring time, p b 0.001, Fig. 2A,C) and
2.7. Drugs old animals (from 53% to 70% of total exploring time, p b 0.001,
Fig. 2B,D).
AMPA receptor antagonist DNQX and NMDA receptor antagonist
(+)-MK801 maleate dizocilpine (Sigma-Aldrich chemical company, 3.2. The effect of blockade of NMDA receptors on GH-induced working
St. Louis, MO, USA). Growth hormone (recombinant human GH; Saizen, memory improvement
Serono, Spain). The selective MEK (MAPK/ERK kinase) inhibitor SL327 α-
amino-(4-aminophenyl-thio)-methylene-2-(trifluoromethyl)-benzene- The improving effects of GH (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h) on spatial memory
acetonitrile (Tocris Cookson Ltd. Avonmouth, United Kingdom). Other in the radial maze were blocked by the administration of MK801
chemicals were from Sigma. (0.025 mg/kg, i.p.) before GH (1 mg/kg, 10 min apart). Accordingly
the time spent to complete the trial was similar to control rats but
3. Results significantly higher respect to the rats treated with GH alone in
both young (p b 0.001; two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni
3.1. Acute effects of GH on working memory tasks in rodents test, Fig. 1A) and old rats (p b 0.001; two-way ANOVA followed by
Bonferroni test, Fig. 1B). Also, the number of errors during the perfor-
The effect of acute treatment with GH on working memory pro- mance of task was higher in rats treated with MK801 plus GH than in
cesses was investigated through two different tests, the eight-arm rats treated with GH alone in both young (p b 0.001; two-way
radial maze for spatial memory, and the novel object recognition ANOVA followed by Bonferroni test, Fig. 1C) and old rats (p b 0.001;
test as a form of non-spatial working memory task. The radial maze two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni test, Fig. 1D). The adminis-
performance is shown in Figs. 1, 3 and 5, and results from the novel tration of MK801 alone (0.025 mg/kg, 70 min before the test) did
object recognition test in Figs. 2, 4 and 6. A significant improvement not cause any effect on radial maze test compared to the correspond-
in memory processes was observed after the acute administration of ing control young or old group (Table 2). Table 2 shows the effects of
GH (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h) in young and old rats, and mice (Figs. 1 to 6). different doses of MK801 in young and old rats on the radial maze.
14 M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20

A) B)
Young rats Old rats
20 20

Time performance (min)

Time performance (min)


15 15

10 10

5 5

0 0
Control GH MK+GH Control GH MK+GH

C) D)
20 Young rats 20 Old rats

Total errors (number)


15 15
Total errors (number)

10 10

5 5

0 0
Control GH MK+GH Control GH MK+GH

Fig. 1. Acute effects of growth hormone treatment (GH, 1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h before the test) in young and old rats on working memory task by means of the eight-arm radial maze test,
compared with the corresponding young or old control group. The effect of GH was blocked by previous administration of the NMDA receptor antagonist MK801 (MK, 0.025 mg/kg
dissolved in saline). The drugs were injected (i.p.) alone or in combination 10 min apart (MK followed by GH). Control rats received vehicle (1 ml/kg of saline i.p.). MK801 administered
alone did not induce significant changes at the mentioned dose (data not shown, see Table 1 for the effects of MK801 at different doses). More details in the Methods and materials section.
Bars represent means ± SEM derived from different animals of the time spent to complete the test (A, B), and the sum of non-visited arm or re-visited arm which was considered as total
errors made during the task (C, D). Two-way ANOVA detected significant differences between ages and treatments but not detected significant differences for the interaction between
ages and treatments. Two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni test was used for statistical evaluation, ***p b 0.001 when compared with the corresponding control group;
†††p b 0.001 when compared with the GH group.

From the results obtained in this previous study, it was selected the 3.3. The effect of blockade of AMPA receptors on GH-induced working
dose of 0.025 mg/kg for analysing the effect of NMDA receptor block- memory improvement
ade on the improvement of working memory induced by GH.
Moreover, the improving effects of GH on the novel object recognition The effect of blockade of AMPA receptors on working memory was
test were blocked by previous administration of MK801 (0.025 mg/kg). analysed in a group of young rats. The improving effects of GH (1 mg/kg,
Accordingly, the time spent exploring the novel and the familiar objects i.p., 1 h) on spatial memory in the radial maze were blocked by the
was similar in the group of rats treated with MK801 plus GH than in con- administration of DNQX (1 mg/kg, i.p.) 10 min before GH. As previously
trol rats, but significantly lower respect to the rats treated with GH alone observed, the acute administration of GH (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h) significantly
in both young (p b 0.001; two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni improved the spatial memory in the radial maze respect to the control
test, Fig. 2A, C) and old rats (p b 0.001; two-way ANOVA followed by group (Fig. 3). Rats treated with GH showed a reduced performance
Bonferroni test, Fig. 2B, D). Accordingly, as observed in Table 1, no signif- time to complete the trial (40% reduction, p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA
icant differences respect to the corresponding control group were [F(2,14) = 12.3, p = 0.0008] followed by Scheffé's test; Fig. 3A). Also
observed on the object recognition index after MK801 (0.025 mg/kg, rats treated with GH reduced the number of errors in the radial maze
70 min before the training) or MK801 followed by GH. (no visited plus repeated visited arms) respect to the control group
These results suggest an implication of NMDA receptors in medi- (49% error reduction, p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA [F(2,14) = 5.8, p =
ating the effects of GH on working memory. 0.0145] followed by Scheffé's test; Fig. 3B). Noteworthy, the improving
M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20 15

A) B)
Young rats Old rats
25 25

20 20
Time spent exploring (sec)

Time spent exploring (sec)


15 15

#
10 10
#

5 5

0 0
new familiar new familiar new familiar new familiar new familiar new familiar
Control GH MK+GH Control GH MK+GH

C) D)

15 15
Time spent exploring (sec)

Time spent exploring (sec)

10 10

5 5

0 0
New - Familiar New - Familiar
Control GH MK+GH Control GH MK+GH

Fig. 2. Acute effects of GH treatment (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h before the test) in young and old rats on the novel object recognition test, compared with the corresponding young or old
control group. The effect of GH was blocked by previous administration of the NMDA receptor antagonist MK801 (MK, 0.025 mg/kg dissolved in saline). The drugs were injected
(i.p.) alone or in combination 10 min apart (MK followed by GH). Control rats received vehicle (1 ml/kg of saline i.p.). MK801 administered alone did not induce significant changes
at the mentioned dose (data not shown, see Table 1 for the effects of MK801 at different doses). More details in the Methods and materials section. Bars represent means ± SEM
derived from different animals of the time spent exploring the novel and the familiar objects (A, B), and the time spent exploring the novel object respect to the familiar (C, D).
Two-way ANOVA detected significant differences between ages and treatments but not detected significant differences for the interaction between ages and treatments.
Two-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni test was used for statistical evaluation, ***p b 0.001 when compared with the novel object of control group; †††p b 0.001 when compared
with the novel object of GH group. #p b 0.001 when compared new vs familiar.

effects of GH on spatial memory in the radial maze were blocked in rats DNQX on the radial maze in rats. From the results obtained in this
that received DNQX (1 mg/kg, i.p.) before GH (1 mg/kg, 10 min apart). previous study, it was selected the dose of 1 mg/kg for analysing
Accordingly the time spent to complete the trial was similar to control the effect of AMPA receptor blockade on the improvement of working
rats but significantly higher respect to the rats treated with GH alone memory induced by GH.
(p b 0.01; one-way ANOVA [F(2,14) = 12.3, p = 0.0008] followed by As it was expected, the recognition memory in the novel object
Scheffé's test; Fig. 3A). Also, the number of errors during the perfor- recognition test was significantly improved by the acute administra-
mance of trial was higher in rats injected with DNQX before GH than in tion of GH (1 mg/kg, i.p.) sixty minutes before the training in a
rats treated with GH alone (p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA [F(2,14) = 5.8, group of young rats (Fig. 4). Control group spent more time exploring
p = 0.0145] followed by Scheffé's test; Fig. 3B). the novel object than the familiar. Two-way ANOVA followed by
The single administration of DNQX (1 mg/kg, i.p., 70 min before Bonferroni test for time spent exploring the novel and the familiar
the test) did not cause any effect on radial maze compared to control object detected significant differences between treatments (saline,
rats (Table 3). However, Table 3 shows the effects of different doses of GH and DNQX + GH) (F(2,36) = 6.28, p = 0.0046), object preference
16 M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20

A) A)
15 25
Time performance (min)

20

Time spent exploring (sec)


10

15

5 10 #
# #

5
0
Control GH DNQX+GH
0
new familiar new familiar new familiar
B) Control GH DNQX+GH
10
B)
Total errors (number)

7.5
15
Time spent exploring (sec)
5

10

2.5

5
0
Control GH DNQX+GH

Fig. 3. Acute effects of GH treatment (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h before the test) in young rats on
the eight-arm radial maze test, compared with the corresponding control group. The
effect of GH was blocked by previous administration of the AMPA receptor antagonist
0
DNQX (1 mg/kg dissolved in DMSO). The drugs were injected (i.p.) alone or in combi- new familiar
nation 10 min apart (DNQX followed by GH). Control rats received vehicle (1 ml/kg of Control GH DNQX+GH
DMSO i.p.). DNQX administered alone did not induce significant changes at the men-
tioned dose (data not shown, see Table 2 for the effects of DNQX at different doses).
Fig. 4. Acute effects of GH treatment (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h before the test) in young rats on
Other details as for Fig. 1. One-way ANOVA followed by Scheffé or Fisher LSD were
the novel object recognition test, compared with the corresponding control group. The
used for statistical evaluation, *p b 0.05 when compared with the corresponding control
effect of GH was abolished by previous administration of the AMPA receptor antagonist
group; †p b 0.05 and ††p b 0.01 when compared with GH group.
DNQX (1 mg/kg dissolved in DMSO). The drugs were injected (i.p.) alone or in combi-
nation 10 min apart (DNQX followed by GH). Control rats received vehicle (1 ml/kg of
DMSO i.p.). DNQX administered alone did not induce significant changes at the mentioned
dose (data not shown, see Table 2 for the effects of DNQX at different doses). Other details
(F(1,36) = 46.15, p b 0.0001), and for the interaction between treat- as for Fig 2. A) Two way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni test was used for statistical
ments and object preference (F(2,36) = 7.61, p = 0.0018) (Fig. 4A). evaluation between treatment (Control, GH and DNQX + GH) and object preference
(new vs familiar). Panel B) shows the difference of the time spent exploring the new
Fig 4B represents the difference of time spent exploring the new object and the familiar; one-way ANOVA followed by Scheffé test was used for statistical
object and the familiar. It shows that the time spent exploring the evaluation. *p b 0.05, **p b 0.01, ***p b 0.001 when compared with the corresponding
novel object minus the time spent exploring the familiar object was control group; †p b 0.05 when compared with the GH group; #p b 0.001 when compared
increased notably after GH treatment (from 64% to 75% of total the novel object vs the familiar.
exploring time, p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA [F(2,18) = 3.58, p = 0.0488]
followed by Fisher test). Thus GH increased the percent time spent
exploring the novel object respect to the familiar object (2.8 fold) respect 3.4. The effect of in vivo inhibition of ERK activation on GH-induced
to the control group (p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA [F(2,18) = 7.18, p = working memory improvement
0.0051] followed by Scheffé's test). Remarkable, the improving effects
of GH on recognition memory were blocked by DNQX (1 mg/kg). Thus, The effect of in vivo inhibition of ERK activation on the improve-
results observed in the group of rats injected with DNQX (1 mg/kg, ment of working memory induced by GH was analysed in a group
i.p.) before GH (1 mg/kg, 10 min apart) were similar to those of the con- of mice. First, it was observed that the acute administration of GH
trol group (Fig. 4) and the group of rats treated with DNQX alone (see (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h) significantly improved the spatial memory in the
Table 1 for object recognition index). radial maze in mice respect to the control group. Mice treated with
These results suggest an implication of AMPA receptors in the GH reduced the time required to complete the test respect to the con-
effects of GH on working memory. trol group (32% reduction, p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA [F(3,25) = 3.55,
M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20 17

A) A)
Mice Mice
25
10

20

Time spent exploring (sec)


7.5
Time performance (min)

15
# # #
#
5
10

2.5
5

0
0 new fam new fam new fam new fam
Control SL GH SL+GH
Control SL GH SL+GH

B) B)
25
15

20
Time spent exploring (sec)
Total errors (number)

10
15

10

0
Control SL GH SL+GH 0
New - Familiar
Fig. 5. Acute effects of GH treatment (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h before the test) in mice on the Control SL GH SL+GH
eight-arm radial maze test, compared with the corresponding control group. The effect
of GH was abolished by previous administration of the MEK inhibitor SL 327 (20 mg/kg Fig. 6. Acute effects of GH treatment (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h before the test) in mice on the
i.p. dissolved in DMSO) 30 min before the administration of GH (1 mg/kg). The drugs novel object recognition test, compared with the corresponding control group. The
were injected (i.p.) alone or in combination 30 min apart (SL 327 followed by GH). effect of GH was abolished by previous administration of the MEK inhibitor SL 327
Control rats received vehicle (1 ml/kg of DMSO i.p.). Other details as for Fig. 1. (20 mg/kg i.p. dissolved in DMSO) 30 min before the administration of GH (1 mg/kg).
One-way ANOVA followed by Fisher LSD was used for statistical evaluation, *p b 0.05 The drugs were injected (i.p.) alone or in combination 30 min apart (SL 327 followed by
when compared with the control group; †p b 0.05 when compared with the GH group. GH). Control rats received vehicle (1 ml/kg of DMSO i.p.). Other details as for Fig. 2. A)
Two way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni test was used for statistical evaluation between
treatment (Control, SL, GH, and SL + GH) and object preference (new vs familiar). Panel
B) shows the difference of the time spent exploring the new object and the familiar;
one-way ANOVA followed by Scheffé test was used for statistical evaluation. **p b 0.01
p = 0.0286] followed by Fisher's test; Fig. 5A). Mice treated with GH
and ***p b 0.001 when compared with the corresponding control group; †p b 0.05
also reduced the number of errors in the radial maze (no visited arm when compared with the GH group; #p b 0.001 when compared the novel object vs the
or re-visited arm) respect to the control group (34% error reduction, familiar.
p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA [F(3,25) = 3.35, p = 0.035] followed by
Fisher's test; Fig. 5B). However, the injection of MEK inhibitor SL
327 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) 30 min before GH administration, blocked the were observed on working memory in the radial maze after the single
positive effects of GH on radial maze test (Fig. 5A, B), since results administration of SL 327 at the used dose (Fig. 5A, B).
in the radial maze were similar in control group and mice injected Also the recognition memory in the novel object recognition
with SL 327 (20 mg/kg) before GH, but significantly different to test was significantly improved by the acute administration of GH
those observed in mice treated with GH alone for time (p b 0.05; (1 mg/kg, i.p., 1 h) to mice (Fig. 6). Two-way ANOVA followed by
one-way ANOVA [F(3,25) = 3.55, p = 0.0286] followed by Fisher's Bonferroni test for time spent exploring the novel and the familiar
test; Fig. 5A) and errors (p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA [F(3,25) = 3.35, object detected significant differences between treatments (saline, SL,
p = 0.035] followed by Fisher's test; Fig. 5B). No significant changes GH and SL + GH) (F(3,22) = 16.82, p = 0.0001), object preference
18 M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20

Table 1 Table 3
Object recognition index. Comparative effect of GH injected alone or GH injected after Effects of several doses of DNQX on working memory task by means of the eight-arm
MK801, DNQX or SL327 on the novel object recognition test. radial maze test, compared with the corresponding control group. No visited arm or
re-visited arms were considered as an error.
Group and treatment Training session Test session
Group and treatment Performance time (min) Errors (number)
Young-saline, n = 10 0.52 ± 0.09 0.63 ± 0.04
Young-GH, n = 10 0.49 ± 0.05 0.79 ± 0.04⁎ Young control (n = 6) 9.2 ± 0.91 6.17 ± 0.87
Young-MK801 + GH, n = 7 0.50 ± 0.01 0.56 ± 0.03†† DNQX (1 mg/kg, n = 4) 9.4 ± 1.14 5.00 ± 1.08
Young-MK801, n = 5 0.49 ± 0.01 0.64 ± 0.02 DNQX (2 mg/kg, n = 4) 12.7 ± 2.05⁎ 12.25 ± 1.11⁎
Old-saline, n = 13 0.49 ± 0.07 0.53 ± 0.04 DNQX (2.5 mg/kg, n = 5) 20 ± 0.00⁎ 7.50 ± 0.26
Old-GH, n = 13 0.51 ± 0.02 0.70 ± 0.04⁎
⁎ p b 0.05 when compared with control group by one-way ANOVA followed by Scheffé's
Old-MK801 + GH, n = 13 0.52 ± 0.02 0.53 ± 0.05†
test.
Old-MK801, n = 5 0.51 ± 0.01 0.50 ± 0.04
Young-saline, n = 7 0.50 ± 0.04 0.64 ± 0.03
Young-GH, n = 9 0.48 ± 0.05 0.75 ± 0.03 ⁎
Young-DNQX + GH, n = 5 0.51 ± 0.02 0.62 ± 0.04†
Young-DNQX, n = 5 0.50 ± 0.03 0.64 ± 0.021 4. Discussion
Adult-vehicle mice, n = 6 0.49 ± 0.03 0.61 ± 0.017
Adult-GH mice, n = 3 0.50 ± 0.02 0.68 ± 0.003⁎ The two main findings of this study are, first, the single administra-
Adult-SL 327 + GH mice, n = 3 0.51 ± 0.01 0.60 ± 0.007†
tion of growth hormone (GH, 1 h) induces immediate positive effects
Adult-SL 327, n = 3 0.49 ± 0.017 0.60 ± 0.003
on working memory (WM) and secondly, that both glutamate receptor
⁎ p b 0.05 when compared with control group.

activation as MEK/ERK pathway plays an essential role in mediating the
p b 0.05 when compared with GH-treated group by one-way ANOVA followed by
acute effect of GH on WM. In this way, the acute administration of GH
Scheffé's test.
††
p b 0.01 when compared with GH-treated group by one-way ANOVA followed by produced favourable effects on two different hippocampal-dependent
Scheffé's test. tests used for evaluating WM. Thus, GH improved both spatial memory
and recognition memory through both glutamatergic receptors NMDA
and AMPA, probably by enhancing excitatory synaptic transmission in
(F(1,22) = 388.32, p b 0.0001), and for the interaction between treat- the hippocampus.
ments and object preference (F(3,22) = 11.90, p = 0.0001) (Fig. 6A). WM is generally considered to involve the brief online storage and
Fig 6B represents the difference of time spent exploring the new object manipulation of stored information (Baddeley, 1998; Cowan, 2008). A
and the familiar. It shows that the time spent exploring the novel object WM operation involves a short-term memory storage component and
minus the time spent exploring the familiar object was increased nota- executive functions, and is considered one of the main contributing
bly by GH administered sixty minutes prior to the training session com- factors of cognitive impairments in older adults and many neuropsy-
pared to controls (from 61% to 68% of total exploring time, p b 0.05; chiatric disorders (Park et al., 2002). In this work, WM was analysed
one-way ANOVA [F(3,11) = 5.39, p = 0.0158] followed by Scheffé's by two different hippocampal-dependent tests to evaluate spatial
test; Table 1). The acute treatment with GH increased the percent memory and recognition memory. Hippocampus is a brain area
time exploring the novel object respect to the familiar object related known to play an essential role in cognitive processes, especially
to the control group (1.8 fold, p b 0.01; one-way ANOVA [F(3,11) = memory and learning. The radial maze widely used in rodents was
8.67, p = 0.0031] followed by Scheffé's test; Fig. 6B). Striking, the used to test spatial memory which is dependent on hippocampus
MEK inhibitor SL 327 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) administered in mice 30 min be- (for review see Martin and Clark, 2007). On the other hand, recogni-
fore GH, significantly blocked the favourable effects of GH on the novel tion memory was assessed by the novel object recognition test, which
object recognition test (Fig. 6A, B), since results in the test were similar deals with the natural ability of animals to explore novelties (Clark et
in control group and mice that received SL 327 (20 mg/kg) before GH, al., 2000) and is considered a common model for non-aversive declar-
but significantly different to those observed in mice treated alone ative memory (Okamura et al., 2011). The acute administration of GH
with GH (p b 0.05; one-way ANOVA [F(3,11) = 8.67, p = 0.0031] caused positive effects on spatial memory and recognition memory in
followed by Scheffé's test; Fig 6B). It can be seen in Fig. 6 as mice treated young and adult rats, and mice.
alone with SL 327 showed no difference with the control group (Fig. 6 A range of observations support a role for GH in development and
and Table 1). function of the brain, and both the GH receptor and GH itself are
Together these results suggest that the observed effects of GH on expressed widely in the CNS. GH is involved in neurogenesis,
working memory are under the control of the MEK/ERK pathway. neuroprotection, plasticity as well as cognitive function (Aberg,
2010; Aberg et al., 2006; Schneider-Rivas et al., 1995). Memory defi-
ciency including both short- and long-term memories is a condition
in GH-deficient patients (Aleman et al., 2000). Also GH levels are
Table 2
high during development and puberty, and then decline with advanc-
Effects of several doses of MK 801 on working memory task by means of the eight-arm
radial maze test, compared with the corresponding control group. No visited arm or ing age (Sonntag et al., 1980). In correlation with a GH decline is the
re-visited arms were considered as an error. decrease in cognitive function (Aleman et al., 2000; de Boer et al., 1995;
Deijen et al., 1996; Hoffman et al., 1992; Nyberg, 2000). Thereby, the
Group and treatment Performance time Errors
(min) (number)
chronic treatment with GH has been shown to be effective in reversing
deficits in working memory of GH-deficient patients (Arwert et al.,
Young-saline (n = 8) 8.2 ± 0.9 7.4 ± 0.9
2005a, 2005b; Arwert et al., 2006) as well as the age-related working
Young-MK 801 (0.025 mg/kg, n = 12) 7.9 ± 0.4 8.5 ± 0.5
Young-MK 801 (0.05 mg/kg, n = 5) 9.7 ± 0.5 9.8 ± 1.2 memory impairment in rats (Esteban et al., 2010b; Ramsey et al.,
Young-MK 801 (0.1 mg/kg, n = 5) 10.5 ± 0.6 10.4 ± 0.9 2004). Several memory impairments in old rats may be partially due
Old-saline (n = 6) 16.8 ± 0.9 11.8 ± 1.2 to abnormal glutamate function, and it was suggested that GH can mit-
Old-MK 801 (0.025 mg/kg, n = 6) 16.2 ± 0.9 11.5 ± 0.3
igate the age-related changes in hippocampal function that underlie
Old-MK 801 (0.05 mg/kg, n = 5) 19.8 ± 0.2⁎ 10.4 ± 0.2
Old-MK 801 (0.1 mg/kg, n = 5) 20.0 ± 0.0⁎ 16.0 ± 2.2
this cognitive impairment. In this context Molina et al. (2012) reported
that old animals treated for 6–8 months with GH restored NMDA-
⁎ p b 0.05 when compared with control group by one-way ANOVA followed by Scheffé's
test; ANOVA F(3, 26) = 1.78, p = 0.1746 for performance time in young rats; ANOVA
receptor-dependent basal synaptic transmission to young adult levels
F(3, 26) = 2.19, p = 0.113 for errors in young rats; ANOVA F(3, 18) = 7.66, p = 0.0017 and enhanced both AMPA-receptor-dependent basal synaptic trans-
for performance time in old rats; ANOVA F(3, 18) = 1.47, p = 0.256 for errors in old rats. mission and long-term potentiation.
M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20 19

Interestingly, some reports demonstrate that hippocampal function reversed the age-related decline in working memory since the old
is affected by GH over a short timescale — minutes to hours (Mahmoud GH-treated rats showed data similar to those of young control rats.
and Grover, 2006). In good agreement, the results of the current These findings are consistent with previous observations revealing
work suggest an immediate effect of GH modulating hippocampal- that there are fewer NMDA receptors in aged rats (possibly because of
dependent memory function. In this manner, the acute administration fewer total synaptic contacts) but the NMDA receptors that remain
of GH caused positive effects on two different tests used to evaluate appear to function normally under certain conditions (Barnes et al.,
working memory in rats and mice. Thus, the single administration of 1997) and drugs enhancing glutamate receptors selectively increase
GH improved spatial memory on the 8-arm radial maze, the animals hippocampal cell firing and it improves both acquisition performance
treated with GH reduced the time required to the performance on the and memory retention in middle-aged rats to levels equivalent to
radial maze, and also reduced the number of errors. Also, GH induced those found in young adult animals (Granger et al., 1996). Also, GH
short-term effects on recognition memory on the novel object recogni- ameliorated hippocampal-dependent cognitive decline by regulating
tion test as it is increased the percent time spent exploring the novel NMDA and AMPA receptors, and improved age dependent changes in
object respect to the familiar object compared to controls. One expla- spatial learning (Ramsey et al., 2004). In good agreement GH has been
nation for this fast improvement could be attributed to an enhance- found to regulate the NMDA receptor subunit transcript levels in old
ment of excitatory synaptic transmission in hippocampus. Thus, rats (Le Greves et al., 2002, 2006).
GH enhanced both NMDA- and AMPA-receptor-mediated excitatory
synaptic transmission in area CA1 of rat hippocampus (Mahmoud
5. Conclusion
and Grover, 2006). Given the paramount importance of both the
NMDA and AMPA receptors in memory formation and the spatial
Our data showed that GH acutely administered in a single dose
learning, we investigated whether the acute positive effects of GH on
promoted improvement on working memory as a cognitive enhancer.
memory process were mediated by NMDA and/or AMPA receptors,
Moreover, the results of this study indicate that the activation of
the effects of GH were assessed in rats injected with the NMDA antag-
NMDA, AMPA and MEK/ERK pathway have, together, a major role in
onist MK801 (0.025 mg/kg) and also in other group of rats injected
mediating the acute effect of GH on working memory in rodents.
with the AMPA antagonist DNQX (1 mg/kg) 10 min before GH injec-
These effects of GH on memory may be mediated by the hippocampus
tion. The results revealed that both NMDA and AMPA receptors were
which contains GH receptors, suggesting a potential role for GH sig-
involved in the effects of GH on working memory since in rats injected
nalling in the regulation of hippocampal dependent behaviours.
with MK801 or DNQX, GH injection did not modify significantly spatial
memory and recognition memory.
Further, mitogen-activated protein kinases (MAPKs) have been Contributors
implicated as playing crucial role in hippocampal synaptic plasticity
and hippocampus-dependent memory formation (Sweatt, 2004). In A. Miralles, C. Garau and S. Esteban designed the research.
particular, a wide variety of studies during the past years indicate M. Ramis, F. Sarubbo, J. Sola and S. Aparicio performed the exper-
the clear importance of the extracellular signal-regulated kinase iments. The manuscript was written by A. Miralles and S. Esteban. All
(ERK, subfamily of MAPKs) in synaptic plasticity and memory forma- authors discussed results and commented on the manuscript.
tion in general, across many species, brain areas, and types of synap-
ses (for a review see Sweatt, 2004). The above mentioned effects of Conflict of interest
GH increasing working memory in the present work required the
activation of MEK/ERK signalling pathway. In this manner, the MEK The authors declare no conflict of interest.
inhibitor SL 327 (20 mg/kg, i.p.) blocked the positive effect of GH on
radial maze and the novel object recognition in mice indicating that
Acknowledgements
activation of MEK/ERK pathway has an important role in mediating
the acute effects of GH on working memory. It is likely that GH
The authors thank the financial contribution of the UIB. The authors
administration initiates a chain of events resulting in enhancement
also thank Dr. JA García Sevilla and Dr. JA Fernández for kindly supply-
of glutamate signalling which leads to sustained activation of signal-
ing research materials. M.R. was supported by a predoctoral FPU fellow-
ling cascades such as ERK1/2 during memory task. In good agreement,
ship from MICINN (Spain), and F.S. was supported by a predoctoral
the enhancement induced by GH on hippocampal excitatory synaptic
fellowship from Postgraduate Studies Center (University of Balearic
transmission mediated by AMPA and NMDA receptors required MEK
Islands, Spain).
pathway activation (Mahmoud and Grover, 2006).
Moreover, ageing is associated with a decline of subunits that
comprise the NMDA receptors in the hippocampus (Adams et al., References
2008; Foster, 2002; Newton et al., 2008; Shi et al., 2007), and NMDA
Aberg D. Role of the growth hormone/insulin-like growth factor 1 axis in neurogenesis.
receptor-mediated synaptic transmission is reduced as well (Barnes, Endocr Dev 2010;17:63–76.
1990; Barnes et al., 1997). Chronic systemic treatment with GH alters Aberg ND, Brywe KG, Isgaard J. Aspects of growth hormone and insulin-like growth
the pattern of expression of NMDA receptor subunits in the hippo- factor-I related to neuroprotection, regeneration, and functional plasticity in the
adult brain. ScientificWorldJournal 2006;6:53–80.
campus (Le Greves et al., 2002) and this could at least partially Adams MM, Smith TD, Moga D, Gallagher M, Wang Y, Wolfe BB, et al. Hippocampal
explain the effects of GH on memory function. Glutamatergic fast syn- dependent learning ability correlates with N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor
aptic transmission is known to be altered with age in a specific man- levels in CA3 neurons of young and aged rats. J Comp Neurol 2001;432:230–43.
Adams MM, Shi L, Linville MC, Forbes ME, Long AB, Bennett C, et al. Caloric restriction
ner in hippocampus of old rats. In the current study, impairment of
and age affect synaptic proteins in hippocampal CA3 and spatial learning ability.
working memory was observed in old control rats compared to youn- Exp Neurol 2008;211:141–9.
ger animals on the 8-arm radial maze test, as previously reported Aleman A, de Vries WR, de Haan EH, Verhaar HJ, Samson MM, Koppeschaar HP.
Age-sensitive cognitive function, growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor
(Esteban et al., 2010a, 2010b). Old rats need more time to complete
1 plasma levels in healthy older men. Neuropsychobiology 2000;41:73–8.
the trial in the radial maze and also increase the number of errors. Arwert LI, Deijen JB, Müller M, Drent ML. Long-term growth hormone treatment pre-
Impairment of working memory was also observed in the current serves GH-induced memory and mood improvements: a 10-year follow-up study
work in old control rats on the novel object recognition test. Neverthe- in GH deficient adult men. Horm Behav 2005a;47:343–9.
Arwert LI, Veltman DJ, Deijen JB, van Dam PS, de Waal HA Delemarrevan-van, Drent
less, acute GH treatment improved performance on memory tasks in ML. Growth hormone deficiency and memory functioning in adults visualized by
young and aged rats, being remarkable that GH treatment significantly functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroendocrinology 2005b;82:32–40.
20 M. Ramis et al. / Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 45 (2013) 11–20

Arwert LI, Veltman DJ, Deijen JB, van Dam PS, Drent ML. Effects of growth hormone Molina DP, Ariwodola OJ, Linville C, Sonntag WE, Weiner JL, Brunso-Bechtold JK, et al.
substitution therapy on cognitive functioning in growth hormone deficient patients: Growth hormone modulates hippocampal excitatory synaptic transmission and
a functional MRI study. Neuroendocrinology 2006;83:12–9. plasticity in old rats. Neurobiol Aging 2012;33:1938–49.
Baddeley A. Working memory. C R Acad Sci III 1998;321:167–73. Moranta D, Esteban S, Garcia-Sevilla JA. Acute, chronic and withdrawal effects of
Barnes CA. Effects of aging on the dynamics of information processing and synaptic the cannabinoid receptor agonist WIN55212-2 on the sequential activation of
weight changes in the mammalian hippocampus. Prog Brain Res 1990;86:89-104. MAPK/Raf-MEK-ERK signaling in the rat cerebral frontal cortex: short-term regula-
Barnes CA, Rao G, Shen J. Age-related decrease in the N-methyl-D-aspartateR-mediated tion by intrinsic and extrinsic pathways. J Neurosci Res 2007;85:656–67.
excitatory postsynaptic potential in hippocampal region CA1. Neurobiol Aging Morrison AB, Chein JM. Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges
1997;18:445–52. of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychon Bull Rev 2011;18:46–60.
Bopp KL, Verhaeghen P. Aging and verbal memory span: a meta-analysis. J Gerontol B Mustafa A, Adem A, Roos P, Nyberg F. Sex differences in binding of human growth
Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2005;60:P223–33. hormone to rat brain. Neurosci Res 1994;19:93–9.
Borella E, Carretti B, De Beni R. Working memory and inhibition across the adult Newton IG, Forbes ME, Linville MC, Pang H, Tucker EW, Riddle DR, et al. Effects of aging
life-span. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2008;128:33–44. and caloric restriction on dentate gyrus synapses and glutamate receptor subunits.
Burton KA, Kabigting EB, Clifton DK, Steiner RA. Growth hormone receptor messenger Neurobiol Aging 2008;29:1308–18.
ribonucleic acid distribution in the adult male rat brain and its colocalization in Nieves-Martinez E, Sonntag WE, Wilson A, Donahue A, Molina DP, Brunso-Bechtold J,
hypothalamic somatostatin neurons. Endocrinology 1992;131:958–63. et al. Early-onset GH deficiency results in spatial memory impairment in mid-life
Cao X, Cui Z, Feng R, Tang YP, Qin Z, Mei B, et al. Maintenance of superior learning and and is prevented by GH supplementation. J Endocrinol 2010;204:31–6.
memory function in NR2B transgenic mice during ageing. Eur J Neurosci 2007;25: Noguchi T. Effects of growth hormone on cerebral development: morphological studies.
1815–22. Horm Res 1996;45:5-17.
Clark RE, Zola SM, Squire LR. Impaired recognition memory in rats after damage to the Nyberg F. Growth hormone in the brain: characteristics of specific brain targets for the
hippocampus. J Neurosci 2000;20:8853–60. hormone and their functional significance. Front Neuroendocrinol 2000;21:330–48.
Cowan N. What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Oertel H, Schneider HJ, Stalla GK, Holsboer F, Zihl J. The effect of growth hormone
Prog Brain Res 2008;169:323–38. substitution on cognitive performance in adult patients with hypopituitarism.
de Boer H, Blok GJ, van der Veen EA. Clinical aspects of growth hormone deficiency in Psychoneuroendocrino 2004;29:839–50.
adults. Endocr Rev 1995;16:63–86. Okamura N, Garau C, Duangdao DM, Clark SD, Jüngling K, Pape HC, et al. Neuropeptide
Deijen JB, de Boer H, Blok GJ, van der Veen EA. Cognitive impairments and mood distur- S enhances memory during the consolidation phase and interacts with noradren-
bances in growth hormone deficient men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1996;21: ergic systems in the brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011;36:744–52.
313–22. Park DC, Lautenschlager G, Hedden T, Davidson NS, Smith AD, Smith PK. Models of
Esteban S, Garau C, Aparicio S, Moranta D, Barcelo P, Fiol MA, et al. Chronic melatonin visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol Aging 2002;17:
treatment and its precursor L-tryptophan improve the monoaminergic neurotrans- 299–320.
mission and related behavior in the aged rat brain. J Pineal Res 2010a;48:170–7. Payer D, Marshuetz C, Sutton B, Hebrank A, Welsh RC, Park DC. Decreased neural spe-
Esteban S, Garau C, Aparicio S, Moranta D, Barcelo P, Ramis M, et al. Improving effects of cialization in old adults on a working memory task. Neuroreport 2006;17:487–91.
long-term growth hormone treatment on monoaminergic neurotransmission and Ramos-Miguel A, Esteban S, García-Sevilla JA. The time course of unconditioned
related behavioral tests in aged rats. Rejuvenation Res 2010b;13:707–16. morphine-induced psychomotor sensitization mirrors the phosphorylation of
Foster TC. Regulation of synaptic plasticity in memory and memory decline with aging. FADD and MEK/ERK in rat striatum: role of PEA-15 as a FADD-ERK binding partner
Prog Brain Res 2002;138:283–303. in striatal plasticity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2010;20:49–64.
Gossard F, Dihl F, Pelletier G, Dubois PM, Morel G. In situ hybridization to rat brain and Ramsey MM, Weiner JL, Moore TP, Carter CS, Sonntag WE. Growth hormone treatment
pituitary gland of growth hormone cDNA. Neurosci Lett 1987;79:251–6. attenuates age-related changes in hippocampal short-term plasticity and spatial
Granger R, Deadwyler S, Davis M, Moskovitz B, Kessler M, Rogers G, et al. Facilitation of learning. Neuroscience 2004;129:119–27.
glutamate receptors reverses an age-associated memory impairment in rats. Synapse Scheepens A, Williams CE, Breier BH, Guan J, Gluckman PD. A role for the somatotropic
1996;22:332–7. axis in neural development, injury and disease. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab 2000;13:
Harvey S, Hull K. Neural growth hormone: an update. J Mol Neurosci 2003;20:1-14. 1483–91.
Hoffman AR, Leieberman SA, Ceda GP. Growth hormone therapy in the elderly: implications Schneider-Rivas S, Rivas-Arancibia S, Vázquez-Pereyra F, Vázquez-Sandoval R,
for the aging brain. Psychoneuroendocrino 1992;17:327–33. Borgonio-Pérez G. Modulation of long-term memory and extinction responses
Hojvat S, Baker G, Kirsteins L, Lawrence AM. Growth hormone (GH) immunoreactivity induced by growth hormone (GH) and growth hormone releasing hormone
in the rodent and primate CNS: distribution, characterization and presence (GHRH) in rats. Life Sci 1995;56:433–41.
posthypophysectomy. Brain Res 1982;239:543–57. Shi L, Adams MM, Linville MC, Newton IG, Forbes ME, Long AB, et al. Caloric restriction
Hollmann M, Heinemann S. Cloned glutamate receptors. Annu Rev Neurosci 1994;17: eliminates the aging-related decline in NMDA and AMPA receptor subunits in the
31-108. rat hippocampus and induces homeostasis. Exp Neurol 2007;206:70–9.
Jaworek J, Leja-Szpak A, Dembiński A, Tomaszewska R, Szklarczyk J, Kot M, et al. Shipstead Z, Redick TS, Engle RW. Does working memory training generalize? Psychol
Involvement of sensory nerves in the protective effect of growth hormone on Belg 2010;50:245–76.
acute pancreatitis. Growth Horm IGF Res 2009;19:517–22. Sonntag WE, Steger RW, Forman LJ, Meites J. Decreased pulsatile release of growth
Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn Sci 2010;14: hormone in old male rats. Endocrinology 1980;107:1875–9.
317–24. Staubli U, Rogers G, Lynch G. Facilitation of glutamate receptors enhances memory.
Le Greves M, Steensland P, Le Greves P, Nyberg F. Growth hormone induces age- Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1994;91:777–81.
dependent alteration in the expression of hippocampal growth hormone receptor Sweatt JD. Mitogen-activated protein kinases in synaptic plasticity and memory. Curr
and N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunits gene transcripts in male rats. Proc Opin Neurobiol 2004;14:311–7.
Natl Acad Sci U S A 2002;99:7119–23. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Kawashima R. Effects of working memory training on cognitive
Le Greves M, Zhou Q, Berg M, Le Greves P, Fhölenhag K, Meyerson B, et al. Growth functions and neural systems. Rev Neurosci 2010;21:427–49.
hormone replacement in hypophysectomized rats affects spatial performance Tang YP, Shimizu E, Dube GR, Rampon C, Kerchner GA, Zhuo M, et al. Genetic enhance-
and hippocampal levels of NMDA receptor subunit and PSD-95 gene transcript ment of learning and memory in mice. Nature 1999;401:63–9.
levels. Exp Brain Res 2006;173:267–73. Thornton PL, Ingram RL, Sonntag WE. Chronic [D-Ala2]-growth hormone-releasing
Lobie PE, Garcia-Aragon J, Lincoln DT, Barnard R, Wilcox JN, Waters MJ. Localization hormone administration attenuates age-related deficits in spatial memory. J Gerontol
and ontogeny of growth hormone receptor gene expression in the central nervous A Biol Sci Med Sci 2000;55:B106–12.
system. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 1993;74:225–33. Tsien JZ, Huerta PT, Tonegawa S. The essential role of hippocampal CA1 NMDA
Lynch G. Glutamate-based therapeutic approaches: ampakines. Curr Opin Pharmacol receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity in spatial memory. Cell 1996;87:1327–38.
2006;6:82–8. van Dam PS, Aleman A, de Vries WR, Deijen JB, van der Veen EA, de Haan EH, et al.
Mahmoud GS, Grover LM. Growth hormone enhances excitatory synaptic transmission Growth hormone, insulin-like growth factor I and cognitive function in adults.
in area CA1 of rat hippocampus. J Neurophysiol 2006;95:2962–74. Growth Horm IGF Res 2000;10:S69–73.
Martin SJ, Clark RE. The rodent hippocampus and spatial memory: from synapses to
systems. Cell Mol Life Sci 2007;64:401–31.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen