Sie sind auf Seite 1von 21

The High/Scope

Perry Preschool Study


Through Age 40
Summary, Conclusions, and
Frequently Asked Questions

Lawrence J. Schweinhart
Jeanne Montie
Zongping Xiang
W. Steven Barnett
Clive R. Belfield
Milagros Nores

This summary is based on excerpts from Lifetime Effects: The High/Scope Perry Preschool
Study Through Age 40 (pp. 194–215), by Lawrence J. Schweinhart, Jeanne Montie,
Zongping Xiang, W. Steven Barnett, Clive R. Belfield, & Milagros Nores, 2005, Ypsilanti, MI:
High/Scope Press. © 2005 by High/Scope® Educational Research Foundation.
1

T he design and findings of the High/Scope


Perry Preschool study and its conclusions
are summarized here, along with answers to
of education, economic performance, crime
prevention, family relationships, and health.
Key findings for education, economic perfor-
frequently asked questions, thereby taking mance, and crime prevention are summarized
advantage of the rich discussion that has sur- in Figure 1.
rounded the study over 4 decades. Complete
information is available in the study’s latest
report, Lifetime Effects: The High/Scope Perry
Education
Preschool Study Through Age 40.1
The program group significantly outper-
formed the no-program group on highest level
of schooling completed (77% vs. 60% gradu-
Summary ating from high school). Specifically, a much
larger percentage of program than no-program
The High/Scope Perry Preschool study is a
females graduated from high school (88% vs.
scientific experiment that has identified both
46%). This difference was related to earlier
the short- and long-term effects of a high-
differences between program and no-program
quality preschool education program for young
females in the rates of treatment for mental
children living in poverty. From 1962 through
impairment (8% vs. 36%) and grade repeti-
1967, David Weikart and his colleagues in the
tion (21% vs. 41%). The program group also
Ypsilanti, Michigan, school district operated
significantly outperformed the no-program
the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program for
group on various intellectual and language
young children to help them avoid school
tests from their preschool years up to age 7;
failure and related problems. They identified a
on school achievement tests at ages 9, 10, and
sample of 123 low-income African-American
14; and on literacy tests at ages 19 and 27. At
children who were assessed to be at high risk
ages 15 and 19, the program group had sig-
of school failure and randomly assigned 58
nificantly better attitudes toward school than
of them to a program group that received a
the no-program group, and program-group
high-quality preschool program at ages 3 and
parents had better attitudes toward their
4 and 65 to another group that received no
15-year-old children’s schooling than did no-
preschool program. Because of the random
program-group parents.
assignment strategy, children’s preschool ex-
perience remains the best explanation for
subsequent group differences in their perfor- Economic Performance
mance over the years. Project staff collected
data annually on both groups from ages 3 Significantly more of the program group than
through 11 and again at ages 14, 15, 19, 27, the no-program group were employed at age
and 40, with a missing data rate of only 6% 40 (76% vs. 62%), which continues the trend
across all measures. After each period of data from age 27 (69% vs. 56%). At age 40, more
collection, staff analyzed the information and program-group males than no-program group
wrote a comprehensive official report. males were employed (70% vs. 50%), although
The study has produced eight mono- at age 27 more program-group females than
graphs over the years. The findings of pro- no-program-group females were employed
gram effects through age 40 span the domains (80% vs. 55%). The program group also had

1 The eighth monograph of the Perry Preschool study, Lifetime Effects: The High/Scope Perry
Preschool Study Through Age 40 by L. J. Schweinhart et al. (2005), is available from High/Scope
Press, 600 N. River St., Ypsilanti, MI 48198. Contact High/Scope at 1-800-40-PRESS or online at
www.highscope.org. E-mail Larry Schweinhart at lschweinhart@highscope.org.
2

Figure 1
Major Findings: High/Scope Perry Preschool Study at 40

Program group No-program group


0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100%

36%
Arrested 5+ times by 40
55%
60%
Earned $20K+ at 40
40%
77%
Graduated high school
60%
49%
Basic achievement at 14
15%
61%
Homework at 15
38%
67%
IQ 90+ at 5
28%

significantly higher median annual earnings second car (30% vs. 13%), especially males
than the no-program group at ages 27 and 40 (36% vs. 15%). At age 40, significantly more
($12,000 vs. $10,000 at age 27 and $20,800 vs. of the program group than the no-program
$15,300 at age 40) and higher median monthly group had savings accounts (76% vs. 50%),
incomes at both ages ($1,020 vs. $700 at age 27 especially males (73% vs. 36%).
and $1,856 vs. $1,308 at age 40). There was a While the evidence of less use of social
consistent tendency for a smaller percentage services by the program group than by the no-
of the program group than the no-program program group is strikingly consistent across
group to receive regular income from family various indicators of social services usage,
or friends, which was statistically significant the evidence of a significant group difference
at age 27 (2% vs. 16%). in use of social services on individual indica-
Rather than paying rent, receiving a sub- tors is equivocal. By age 40, fewer members
sidy, living with others, or being incarcerated, of the program group than the no-program
the program group had significantly more group reported receiving social services at
stable dwelling arrangements at ages 27 and some time in their lives (71% vs. 86%), but
40—that is, more of them owned their own this difference was not significant. At age 27,
homes (27% vs. 5% at age 27, 37% vs. 28% at significantly fewer of the program group than
age 40). At age 40, program males paid signifi- the no-program group reported receiving so-
cantly more per month for their dwelling than cial services at some time in the previous 10
did no-program males. Significantly more years (59% vs. 80%). Among the individual
of the program group than the no-program categories of social services, the only signifi-
group owned a car at age 40 (82% vs. 60%), cant differences between the program group
especially males (80% vs. 50%), as they had and the no-program group involved family
at age 27 (73% vs. 59%). Indeed, at age 27, counseling at ages 34 to 40 (13% vs. 24%)
a significantly larger proportion of the pro- and General Assistance from ages 23 to 27
gram group than the no-program group had a (10% vs. 23%).
3

Crime Prevention vs. 52% ever sentenced), specifically from


ages 28 to 40 (19% vs. 43%). Also, from ages
The study presents strong evidence that the 28 to 40, the program group was sentenced
Perry Preschool program played a significant to significantly fewer months in prison for
role in reducing overall arrests and arrests for felonies (7% vs. 25%) and had served signifi-
violent crimes as well as property and drug cantly fewer months in prison overall (9% vs.
crimes and subsequent prison or jail sentences 21% ever served).
over study participants’ lifetimes up to age
40. The program group had significantly fewer
lifetime arrests than the no-program group Health, Family, and Children
(36% vs. 55% arrested 5 or more times) and
More program than no-program males raised
significantly fewer arrests for violent crimes
their own children (57% vs. 30%) and had
(32% vs. 48% ever arrested), property crimes
second marriages (29% vs. 8%). The two
(36% vs. 58% ever arrested), and drug crimes
oldest children raised by program-group
(14% vs. 34% ever arrested). Significant group
members did not differ significantly from
differences in various types of crime occurred
the two oldest children raised by no-program
at various times of life—crimes other than
group members in education, employment,
violent, property, or drug crimes in adoles-
arrests, or welfare status. At age 40, more of
cence (3% vs. 11%); total arrests (7% vs. 29%
the program group than the no-program group
with 5 or more arrests) and drug crimes
said they were getting along very well with
(9% vs. 25%) in early adulthood; and violent
their families (75% vs. 64%). Fewer program
crimes (14% vs. 31%) and property crimes
than no-program males reported using seda-
(15% vs. 32%) in midlife. Consider also that
tives, sleeping pills, or tranquilizers (17% vs.
by age 40, compared to the no-program group,
43%), marijuana or hashish (48% vs. 71%), or
the program group had significantly fewer ar-
heroin (0% vs. 9%).
rests for property felonies (19% vs. 32% ever
arrested), drug felonies (7% vs. 28%), violent
misdemeanors (19% vs. 37%), and property Cost-Benefit Analysis
misdemeanors (24% vs. 41%); significantly
fewer arrests for property felonies by age 27 In constant 2000 dollars discounted at 3%,
(14% vs. 26%); and significantly fewer arrests the economic return to society of the Perry
from ages 28 to 40 for violent felonies (2% vs. Preschool program was $244,812 per par-
12%), drug felonies (3% vs. 15%), and prop- ticipant on an investment of $15,166 per
erty misdemeanors (10% vs. 28%). By age 40, participant—$16.14 per dollar invested. Of
compared to the no-program group, the pro- that return, $195,621 went to the general
gram group had participated in significantly public—$12.90 per dollar invested (as com-
fewer of 3 of the 78 types of crimes cited at pared to $7.16 in the age-27 benefit-cost
arrest—dangerous drugs (3% vs. 20%), assault analysis), and $49,190 went to each par-
and/or battery (19% vs. 37%), and larceny un- ticipant—$3.24 per dollar invested. Of the
der $100 (9% vs. 22%). These types of crimes public return (see Figure 2), 88% ($171,473)
had significant group differences by age 27; came from crime savings, 4% ($7,303) came
assault and/or battery also had a significant from education savings, 7% ($14,078) came
group difference at age 28 to 40. Moreover, the from increased taxes due to higher earnings,
program group was sentenced to significantly and 1% ($2,768) came from welfare savings.
fewer months in prison or jail by age 40 (28% Preschool program participants earned 14%
4

more per person than they would have ings, as compared to males for whom program
otherwise—$156,490 more over their lifetimes and no-program high school graduation rates
in undiscounted 2000 dollars. Male program were not significantly different. The return to
participants cost the public 41% less in crime society on program investment due to earn-
costs per person—$732,894 less in undis- ings was $70,615 for females as compared to
counted 2000 dollars over their lifetimes. $58,436 for males, only 21% more. We can
Interestingly, 93% of the public return surmise that program females did not earn
was due to the performance of males and only more because wage growth for low-skilled
7% to females. This difference is due to the jobs has been very low in recent decades; not
fact that compared to females, males commit- all females participate in the labor market;
ted substantially more crimes, but program and we omitted the benefits of education on
males committed substantially fewer crimes household production and family behaviors.
than no-program males. This finding stands in The cost-benefit analysis is reasonably
stark contrast to the earlier finding that 84% conservative in two respects. One is the omis-
of the program females, but only 32% of the sion of benefits that are hard to monetize,
no-program females, graduated from regular such as family, health, and wealth benefits.
high school. Because education is itself an The other is the conservative assumptions
investment, it is not surprising that education about the earnings profiles and the unit costs
cost more for program females, but it is dis- of crimes; where multiple data sources were
concerting that the greater educational attain- available, we typically chose the source that
ment of program than no-program females yielded smaller differences between program
did not have a larger impact on their earn- and no-program groups.

Figure 2
High/Scope Perry Preschool Program Public Costs and Benefits

Education savings Taxes on earnings Welfare savings Crime savings

Benefits

$2,768
$14,078 $171,473 Total Public Benefit
$7,303 $195,621

Costs $15,166 $12.90 return per dollar invested.


$0

$20,000

$40,000

$60,000

$80,000

$100,000

$120,000

$140,000

$160,000

$180,000

$200,000

(Constant 2,000 dollars, 3% discount rate)


5

Path Model hood and their school success, economic per-


formance, and reduced commission of crime
A path model of the study (see Figure 3) in adulthood. This study confirms that these
suggests how preschool experience affects findings extend not only to young adults,
participants’ success at age 40. Beginning but also to adults in midlife. It confirms that
with preschool experience and children’s the long-term effects are lifetime effects. The
preprogram intellectual performance, the Perry Preschool study indicates that the re-
model traces cause-effect paths to children’s turn to the public on its initial investment
postprogram intellectual performance, then in such programs is not only substantial but
to their school achievement and commit- larger than previously estimated.
ment to schooling, then to their educational The study draws these conclusions
attainment, then to their adult earnings and about a 2-year preschool education program
lifetime arrests. for 3- and 4-year-olds living in low-income
families. Teachers had bachelor’s degrees and
certification in education, and each served
Conclusions 5–6 children. They used the High/Scope edu-
cational model in daily 21/2-hour classes and
The major conclusion of this midlife phase visited families weekly. In this model, teach-
of the Perry Preschool research study is that ers arranged the classroom and daily schedule
high-quality preschool programs for young to support children’s self-initiated learning
children living in poverty contribute to their activities, provided both small-group and
intellectual and social development in child- large-group activities, and helped children

Figure 3
A Model of the Paths from Preschool Experience to Success at 40

Preschool Commitment Earnings


Experience at 15 at 40

.241 .190
.477 .491 .356 .436

Educational
Post-
attainment
program IQ
by 40

.418 .288
.400 .450 .330 –.265

Pre- Achievement Arrests


program IQ at 14 by 40

.203 .070

Note. Path coefficients are standardized regression weights, all statistically significant at p < .01; coefficients in
each box are squared multiple correlations.
6

engage in key experiences in child develop- ginning of a program. These are hopeful signs
ment. Teachers studied and received regular and models for the future.
training and support in their use of this The High/Scope Perry Preschool study
educational model. serves as a symbol of what government pro-
The most basic implication of this study grams can achieve. The High/Scope Perry
is that all young children living in low-income Preschool study also offers a challenge, a kind
families should have access to preschool of policy gauntlet, for decision makers at local,
programs that have features that are reason- state, and national levels. It demonstrates what
ably similar to those of the High/Scope Perry can be done, and the challenge is to do it. The
Preschool program. Findings from this long- High/Scope Perry Preschool, the Abecedarian,
term study and others reviewed in this report and the Chicago programs described in the
have motivated policymakers to invest more in latest Perry Preschool report all have signifi-
preschool programs. But because policymakers cant benefits. Though they illuminate differ-
practice the art of political compromise, these ent aspects of the question of lasting effects of
programs have seldom met the standard of rea- preschool education, they all reflect the same
sonable similarity identified here. Recognizing challenge of providing high-quality preschool
this problem, more recent efforts, such as the programs that include low-income children so
Abbott court decision in New Jersey and the that these children get a fair chance to achieve
recent ballot initiative in Florida, have sought their potential and contribute meaningfully to
to require key program standards from the be- their families and to society.
7

Q&A
B ecause the long-term High/Scope Perry
Preschool study is well known and re-
spected and stands at the fulcrum of decisions
group; and another variable (father at home)
that had a nearly statistically significant re-
lationship with monthly earnings at age 40
about public investment in early childhood as well as general policy relevance. Second,
programs, it has attracted many questions over because younger siblings were assigned to
the years that deserve thoughtful answers. the same group as their older sibling, we ana-
Many of the questions and answers that fol- lyzed major outcomes with subsamples that
low involve the study’s internal and external included only one sibling per family. Third,
validity. Its internal validity is the extent to because the sample consisted of five classes
which its two groups are the result of simple of children, we analyzed major outcomes us-
random assignment and thus accurately re- ing classes as covariates. The findings for the
flect the impact of a good preschool education major outcomes were the same regardless of
experience against the impact of no preschool which of these analyses were used.
education experience. Its external validity is
the extent to which its study participants and
treatment resemble the children and programs Isn’t the sample size too small to
to which it is generalized. generate scientific confidence in
the findings?

Don’t the departures from random Statistical significance testing takes sample
assignment challenge the internal size into account. To achieve statistical signif-
validity of the findings? icance, group differences must become larger
in magnitude as sample sizes become smaller.
The internal validity of the High/Scope Perry Indeed, a problem with very large samples is
Preschool study is very strong because its that educationally trivial group differences can
design is based on random assignment of achieve statistical significance. If the High/
children to program and no-program groups. Scope Perry Preschool study sample were truly
For this very reason, its departures from strict too small, none of its findings would have
random assignment have received intense achieved statistical significance, and it would
scrutiny. These departures and their effects on never have become influential.
major outcomes are examined at length in the
age 40 report, Lifetime Effects (Schweinhart
et al., 2005). First, the outcome analyses in How can the study be generalized
this monograph are adjusted for seven back- to other programs?
ground covariates: five that had statistically
significant relationships with preschool ex- Because few programs are evaluated by longi-
perience and one or more of the key outcome tudinal studies involving random assignment
variables; one (mother’s employment) that of study participants, it is desirable to be able
had a statistically significant relationship to generalize the results of such studies as
with preschool experience due to the random- broadly as possible. The external validity
assignment departure of assigning some chil- or generalizability of the study findings ex-
dren of employed mothers to the no-program tends to those programs that are reasonably
8

similar to the High/Scope Perry Preschool had fewer than 3 rooms per person. These
program. A reasonably similar program is a families were of lower socioeconomic sta-
preschool education program run by teach- tus than most U.S. residents at that time.
ers with bachelor’s degrees and certification The study does not suggest a sharp cutoff
in education, each serving up to 8 children point for program eligibility.
living in low-income families. The program
• Offering 2 school years at 3 and 4 years
runs 2 school years for children who are 3
of age—The study presents no evidence
and 4 years of age with daily classes of 21/2
that the program would have had similar
hours or more, uses the High/Scope model or
effects if it had served children at earlier
a similar participatory education approach,
(infancy–3 years) or later ages (elemen-
and has teachers visiting families at least
tary school years). Evidence shows chil-
every two weeks or scheduling regular parent
dren should attend a similar program for
events. Each term in this treatment definition
2 school years (October through May for
is examined further below.
the Perry Preschool group); one year is
enough only if one accepts a generaliza-
• A preschool education program—a care
tion from the 13 program-group members
and education program that contributes
in the initial class, who attended the pro-
to young children’s development.
gram for 1 school year and experienced
• Run by teachers with bachelor’s degrees the same effects as did the 45 program-
and certification in education—The group members in the other classes, who
teachers in the Perry Preschool study attended the program for 2 school years.
were certified to teach in elementary, This study, by itself, offers only weak ev-
early childhood, and special education; idence to support the limitation of many
of all their education, the early child- state preschool programs to only serving
hood training was most relevant to their 4-year-old children. The better argument
classroom practices. for this policy is the inequity inherent in
• Each serving up to 8 children—The Perry serving some children for 2 school years
Preschool program had 4 teachers for 20 when, as a result, other eligible children
to 25 children, typical for special educa- are not served at all, because the 3-year-
tion classes (Kakalik, Furry, Thomas, & olds served have taken the places of
Carney, 1981). The equally successful additional 4-year-olds.
classrooms in the subsequent High/Scope • With daily classes of 21/2 hours or more—
Preschool Curriculum Comparison study The program runs at least 21/2 hours a day
(Schweinhart & Weikart, 1997a, 1997b) 5 days a week. A few minutes less should
had 2 teachers for 16 children, a ratio of not matter, nor should hours more: Even
1 to 8. In general practice, High/Scope a full, 9-hour-a-day program, if it meets
preschool classrooms appear to run suc- all the other standards of quality, should
cessfully with 2 adults and up to 20 chil- produce similar if not greater effects.
dren (Epstein, 1993).
• Using the High/Scope educational model
• Children living in low-income families— or a similar participatory education
Children were selected for the study be- approach—The High/Scope educational
cause their parents had low educational model was developed and used in the
attainment (high school graduation or program (Weikart, Deloria, Lawser, &
less), low occupational status (unem- Wiegerink, 1970; Hohmann, Banet, &
ployed or unskilled), and their homes Weikart, 1979; Hohmann & Weikart,
9

1995, 2002). In this model, the classroom visits every 2 weeks rather than every week
is arranged and the day is scheduled to (or regular parent meetings and events). These
support children’s self-initiated learning characteristics are structural, that is to say,
activities along with small-group and they are relatively easy to name, count, leg-
large-group activities. Teachers help chil- islate, regulate, and monitor. One of them,
dren as they plan, carry out, and review use of the High/Scope educational model, is
their own activities. Teachers plan ways structural in its simplest meaning, but encom-
to engage children in numerous key ex- passes process characteristics as well, that is,
periences in child development covering what actually happens in the classroom, such
the areas of personal initiative, social as the nature of teacher-child interaction.
relations, creative representation, move- Programs with similar features, regardless of
ment and music, logic and mathematics, model used, can expect similar results. In cur-
and language and literacy. Teachers study riculum provision, it’s not what you say you
and receive regular training in the edu- do but what you actually do that counts.
cational model and receive support in
its use from a supervisor who knows the
model and assists in its implementation. Were the findings due to curriculum
or other aspects of the program?
• With teachers visiting families at least
every 2 weeks—The program included The High/Scope Preschool Curriculum
weekly home visits, which might be re- Comparison study (Schweinhart & Weikart,
duced to every 2 weeks, or changed to an 1997a, 1997b), which immediately followed
equivalent form of substantial outreach the High/Scope Perry Preschool study, sug-
to parents, such as parent group meet- gests that the curriculum had a lot to do with
ings in which staff acknowledge and the findings. The comparison study found
support parents as partners in the educa- that young people born in poverty experi-
tion of their children and model active- enced fewer emotional problems and felony
learning principles for them. The key is arrests if they attended a preschool program
not to require meetings, but rather to en- that used the High/Scope model or a tradi-
sure that the basic message and lessons tional Nursery School model rather than a
of a strong partnership with parents are Direct Instruction model.
clearly and repeatedly communicated. Since 1967, the study has followed the
Sometimes, issues including the safety lives of 68 young people born in poverty who
of home visitors in the community call were randomly assigned at ages 3 and 4 to one
for creative solutions to this challenge. of three groups, each experiencing a different
curriculum model:
The study provides scientific evidence
that its findings apply to reasonably similar • In the Direct Instruction model, teachers
programs. Program similarities, however, are followed a script to directly teach children
defined somewhat more liberally than the academic skills, rewarding them for cor-
actual program characteristics to allow for rect answers to the teacher’s questions.2
necessary and reasonable variations—serving
up to 8 children rather than 5 or 6, serving • In the High/Scope model, teachers set
children living in low-income families rather up the classroom and the daily routine
than only families living in poverty, home so children could plan, do, and review

2 This
1960s model has undergone subsequent development and current versions differ from the
one in this study.
10

their own activities and engage in active compared to 38% of the Direct Instruction
learning key experiences in child devel- group.
opment individually, in small groups,
• At age 15, 23% of the High/Scope group
and in whole-class groups.
reported that they had engaged in 10 or
• In the traditional Nursery School model, more acts of misconduct, as compared to
teachers responded to children’s self- 56% of the Direct Instruction group.
initiated play in a loosely structured,
• Fewer of the High/Scope group (36%)
socially supportive setting.
said that various kinds of people gave
them a hard time, as compared to 69%
Program staff implemented the curricu-
of the Direct Instruction group.
lum models independently and to high stan-
dards, in 21/2-hour classes held 5 days a week, • With regard to marriage, 31% of the
and conducted 11/2-hour home visits every 2 High/Scope group had married and were
weeks, when children were 3 and 4 years old. living with their spouses, as compared to
Except for the curriculum model, all aspects none of the Direct Instruction group.
of the programs were nearly identical. The
• Of the High/Scope group, 70% planned
findings presented here are corrected for dif-
to graduate from college, as compared to
ferences in the gender makeup of the groups.
36% of the Direct Instruction group.
By age 23, the High/Scope and Nursery
School groups had 10 significant advantages The Nursery School group had two ad-
over the Direct Instruction group, and the ditional significant advantages over the Direct
High/Scope and Nursery School groups did Instruction group:
not differ significantly from each other on any
outcome variable (Schweinhart & Weikart, • Only 9% of the Nursery School group
1997b). The High/Scope and Nursery School had been arrested for a felony at ages
groups both had two significant advantages 22–23, as compared to 34% of the Direct
over the Direct Instruction group at age 23: Instruction group.

• Only 6% of either group needed treatment • None of the Nursery School group had
for emotional impairment or disturbance ever been suspended from work, as com-
during their schooling, as compared to pared to 27% of the Direct Instruction
47% of the Direct Instruction group. group.

• More of the High/Scope group (43%) and Through age 10, the main finding of the
the Nursery School group (44%) had done Preschool Curriculum Comparison study was
volunteer work, as compared to only 11% that the overall average IQ of the three groups
of the Direct Instruction group. rose 27 points—from a borderline impairment
level of 78 to a normal level of 105 after 1 year
The High/Scope group had six addi- of their preschool program—and subsequently
tional significant advantages over the Direct settled in at an average of 95, still at the
Instruction group: normal level. The only curriculum group dif-
ference through age 10 was measured as the
• Only 10% had ever been arrested for a
preschool programs ended: the average IQ of
felony, as compared to 39% of the Direct
the Direct Instruction group was significantly
Instruction group.
higher than the average IQ of the Nursery
• None of the High/Scope group had ever School group (103 vs. 93). Throughout their
been arrested for a property crime, as school years, curriculum groups did not
11

differ significantly in school achievement, nor • Most Head Start teachers do not have
did their high school graduation rates differ a bachelor’s degree. In 2000, only 28%
significantly. The conclusion at that time was of Head Start teachers had a bachelor’s
that well-implemented preschool curriculum degree, while 19% had an associate’s
models, regardless of their theoretical orienta- degree, 32% had some college experi-
tion, had similar effects on children’s intel- ence but no degree, and 74% had a Child
lectual and academic performance. However, Development Associate credential or
time has proved otherwise. Tightly scripted state-awarded preschool certificate (Zill
teacher-directed instruction, touted by some et al., 2003). Teacher salaries in Head
as the surest path to school readiness, seems Start average $21,000—about half of the
to purchase a temporary improvement in average of $43,000 for public school
academic performance at the cost of a missed teacher salaries (National Institute for
opportunity for long-term improvement in Early Education Research, 2003)—while
social behavior. teacher salaries in the High/Scope Perry
Preschool program were at public school
teacher salary levels at the time of the
Does the High/Scope Perry Preschool study, with a 10% bonus for participa-
study apply to Head Start and state tion in a special program.
preschool programs?
• Head Start serves most but not all par-
Because of the demand for knowledge of ticipating children for 2 or more program
the lasting benefits of preschool education years. In FY 2003, for example, 34% of
programs, there has been a tendency to Head Start children were 3 years old, 53%
generalize the High/Scope Perry Preschool were 4 years old, 5% were 5 or older, and
study’s findings beyond reasonably similar 8% were under 3 (U. S. Administration
programs. Several of these generalizations for Children and Families, 2004). In FY
deserve discussion here. 2002, 36% of Head Start children were 3
The most common generalizations of the years old, and it is reasonable to assume
High/Scope Perry Preschool study findings that these children continued in Head
relate to the national Head Start program. Start as 4-year-olds in FY 2003, so that
Indeed, news reports have often imprecisely most of the 4-year-olds in Head Start in
referred to the Perry Preschool Program study FY 2003 (36% among the 53%) had been
as a Head Start program (see discussion by in the program the previous year. We can
Woodhead, 1988). News reporters would argue therefore surmise that in FY 2003 only
that this conflation of terms is a useful conve- 17% of Head Start 4-year-olds attended
nience to simplify the story in that both the the program for only one year.
Perry Preschool program and Head Start serve • Only 20% of Head Start programs re-
young children living in poverty and began port using the High/Scope education-
in the U.S. in the 1960s. Nonetheless, Head al model, while 39% report using the
Start, as nationally defined by its Program Creative Curriculum model, and 41%
Performance Standards (U. S. Administration report using some other curriculum ap-
for Children and Families, 2001), clearly does proach (Zill et al., 2003). The Creative
not meet the standard of reasonable similarity Curriculum model has goals similar to
with the Perry Preschool program for general- the High/Scope model, but emphasizes
ization purposes: different practices to attain these goals
(Dodge, Colker, & Heroman, 2002).
12

• Head Start Program Performance Standards likes of which will never be seen again. To bor-
require only 2 home visits a year. row a phrase from Lisbeth Schorr, the programs
and findings presented in the Perry Preschool
The Head Start Family and Child study monographs are completely and realisti-
Experiences Survey (FACES) found that chil- cally “within our reach” (Schorr, 1989, p. i.).
dren gained 4 points in standard scores on the
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test during their
Head Start year (Zill et al., 2003). Children in Does the study apply to child care
the High/Scope Perry Preschool study gained programs?
8 points in their first year and a total of 14
points in 2 years. In other words, on average Several studies of U.S. child care centers have
Head Start programs are achieving some suc- concluded that their quality is unacceptably
cess, but could be doing more to help chil- low (Cost, Quality, & Child Outcomes Study
dren reach their potential. Team, 1995; Whitebook, Phillips, & Howes,
Forty states have now invested in state 1993). In terms of the quality criteria listed
preschool programs for young children living here, child care programs have certain seem-
in poverty or otherwise at special risk of school ingly insurmountable financial problems.
failure (Barnett, Robin, Hustedt, & Schulman, Unlike Head Start and state preschool pro-
2003; National Prekindergarten Center, 2003). grams, which are fully paid for by the govern-
As these programs have developed, especially ment, an estimated 60% of child care costs are
in the past 2 decades, policymakers have paid borne by the participating families (Stoney &
attention to program quality, thereby acknowl- Greenberg, 1996). While child care programs
edging the argument from the High/Scope can certainly aspire to be genuine preschool
Perry Preschool study and similar studies that education programs and maintain a ratio of
only high-quality preschool programs for poor no more than 8 children per teacher, the need
children are known to have long-term benefits for child care includes, but also extends well
for participants and a strong return on public beyond, 3- and 4-year-old children. By defini-
investment. However, politics is the art of tion, these programs could serve all children
compromise, and the high quality of the High/ whose parents are employed or in school out-
Scope Perry Preschool program (as defined side the home, a definition that includes but
earlier) is seldom if ever achieved in state pre- is not limited to low-income children.
school programs (Gilliam & Zigler, 2004). For the most part, the average pay for
The simple scientific conclusion is that child care teachers is less than half that of
the findings of the High/Scope Perry Preschool public school teachers ($43,000). The aver-
study do not apply to typical Head Start or age annual wage for child care workers in
state preschool programs, but may apply to 2002 was $23,820 in local-government pro-
exemplary ones and could apply to typical grams, $18,279 in state and federal programs,
ones if policymakers and administrators chose $15,155 in private programs, and $11,507 for
to implement the standards of high quality self-employed child care workers (National
described here. It is important to get this point Child Care Information Center, 2004a). It
just right, neither overstating nor understating should come as no surprise that only one
the Perry Preschool program study’s general- state, Rhode Island, requires child care teach-
izability. While the findings do not apply to ers to have bachelor’s degrees, and only 15
typical Head Start programs as they exist today, states have any educational requirements at
it is not because the program studied was an all for child care teachers (National Child
unattainable ideal run by super-educators, the Care Information Center, 2004b).
13

The High/Scope educational model degree of educational productivity in early


widely influences teaching practices in child childhood settings has a large influence on
care programs; but the meager funds available young children’s subsequent lives. All young
for training in child care programs mean that children spend their time in settings that vary
few providers actually receive much training in their educational productivity, so the find-
in the High/Scope model. Daily classes cer- ings apply in this way to all children. But
tainly do run more than 21/2 hours, and there if it is a setting’s educational productivity
is no reason to think that their additional that matters, early childhood programs are
duration per se prevents program staff from not inherently more educationally produc-
delivering as much or more quality education tive than children’s homes, nor are children’s
as briefer programs. Teachers do not provide homes inherently more productive than early
regular home visits to families, but that would childhood programs. Young children from
not be the correct standard to use in these cases. educationally productive homes who attend
Rather, the child care programs’ challenge less educationally productive early child-
is to develop teacher-parent relationships of hood programs would suffer negative effects
mutual respect and understanding that are on their development. The survey of existing
of the same quality as those that result from preschool settings in the previous paragraphs
biweekly home visits or regularly scheduled gives reason to be seriously concerned about
parent meetings. this reverse application of the findings of the
longitudinal preschool program studies.

Does the study apply to open-


enrollment preschool programs? Does the study apply to early child-
hood programs in other countries?
The relatively new open enrollment preschool
programs have also been linked to the High/ As the characteristics of a country’s children
Scope Perry Preschool study findings. These and programs diverge from the characteristics
programs are sometimes called universal and of the Perry Preschool study’s children and
other than age and residence requirements, programs, applications become less certain.
have no demographic restrictions (such as Generalization of the study to other industri-
poverty) on program enrollment. The findings alized countries, such as Great Britain, seems
of the High/Scope Perry Preschool study and probable, but generalization of the study to
similar studies would apply only to children less industrialized countries requires greater
served by these programs who are reasonably caution. The challenge of such applications
similar to children living in poverty or other- becomes clear as one considers the practi-
wise at risk of school failure. cal ranges of outcome variables in various
It is important to keep in mind, how- countries. Improving the high school gradu-
ever, that poverty is not an inherent trait ation rate, for example, is a reasonable goal
of children but is rather a socioeconomic in industrialized countries, but not in some
extreme of settings in which they live. A less industrialized countries. One might rea-
good preschool program offers a productive sonably argue, however, that a high-quality
early childhood educational environment, preschool program would improve children’s
while early childhood poverty by and large educational performance in less industrial-
offers an unproductive early childhood edu- ized countries, but that this effect would be
cational environment. So the longitudinal expressed in ways other than an improved
preschool studies provide evidence that the high school graduation rate. For example, the
14

Turkish Early Enrichment Project (Kagitcibasi, riences have similar effects on human devel-
Sunar, & Bekman, 2001) found evidence of opment—what might be called the principle
long-term program effects on children’s edu- of external validity or generalizability.
cational success and social adjustment in a
very different culture. Cost-benefit analysis
is particularly sensitive to such differences Does the evidence of the effectiveness
between countries. of the High/Scope educational model
come only from programs run
decades ago?
Did the Perry Preschool program
occur too long ago to apply to current No. The Head Start FACES study (Zill et al.,
programs? 2003) is a nationally representative study of
2,800 children who entered Head Start in fall
The Perry Preschool Project operated from 2000. It found that 4-year-olds in Head Start
1962 through 1967. The rapid pace of techno- classes that used High/Scope improved from
logical change in modern society—including fall to spring in letter and word identification
the advent of widespread use of comput- skills and cooperative classroom behavior
ers, worldwide electronic communication, and decreased their behavior problems:
and increased transportation, among other
advances—is unprecedented in history. But • On a scale of letter and word recogni-
there is no compelling reason to assume that tion, children in High/Scope classes reg-
this rapid pace of technological change would istered a highly significant gain (p < .01)
alter basic principles of human behavior and of 12.6 scale points, significantly more
education. Throughout most of the history (p < .05) than children in classes using
of the world, few would have regarded half Creative Curriculum or other curricula.
a century or even a century as a sufficient
• On teacher ratings of cooperative class-
amount of time to permit profound changes
room behavior, children in High/Scope
in traditions, let alone profound changes in
classes experienced a highly significant
human nature that would affect how children
gain (p < .01) of half a standard deviation,
respond to an educational program. Indeed,
significantly more (p < .05) than children
the education and social sciences in gen-
in classes using Creative Curriculum or
eral are quests for timeless principles, not for
other curricula.
principles that must be rediscovered once or
twice a decade. The argument that the find- • On teacher ratings of total behavior prob-
ing of such studies have limited applicabil- lems, particularly problems involving
ity to the present because of rapid change hyperactive behavior, children in High/
is quite similar to a belief that because each Scope classes dropped significantly
human experience is unique, scientific gen- (p < .05) during the year, significantly
eralization is impossible. A current mani- more (p < .05) than did children in class-
festation of this belief is the postmodernism es using Creative Curriculum or other
movement (Dahlberg, Moss, & Pence, 1999). curricula.
Postmodernism is essentially a nonscientific
movement, even antiscientific. In contrast, Of the 91% of Head Start teachers who
the scientific approach adopted in the High/ used one or more curriculum models, 39%
Scope Perry Preschool study is the logical used Creative Curriculum, 20% used High/
application of the principle that similar expe- Scope, and 41% used some other curriculum,
15

such as High Reach, Scholastic, or Los Cantos rated significantly better than comparison
Los Ni§os. The quality of Creative Curriculum classrooms in terms of classroom environ-
and High/Scope classes was significantly high- ment, daily routine, adult-child interaction,
er than the quality of classes that used other and overall implementation. The children in
curricula, particularly with respect to language. High/Scope programs significantly outper-
On the 7-point Early Childhood Environment formed the children in comparison programs
Rating Scale (Harms, Clifford, & Cryer, 1998), in initiative, social relations, music and move-
with 5 identified as good, High/Scope classes ment, and overall child development.
averaged 5.04, Creative Curriculum classes av-
eraged 5.02, and classes using other curricula
averaged 4.55. On its language items, average Didn’t the High/Scope Perry Preschool
scores were slightly higher, but the differences program achieve a level of quality
were about the same. On a quality composite, that cannot be duplicated in ordinary
the average scores for High/Scope and Creative preschool programs?
Curriculum were nearly half a standard devia-
This criticism is rooted in the fact that the
tion higher than the average scores for other
High/Scope Perry Preschool program paid
curricula—clearly an educationally meaning-
teachers public school salaries and added a
ful difference.
10% bonus because the program was a spe-
The High/Scope Training for Quality
cial one. There is no reason to think that such
study (Epstein, 1993) also offers evidence
pay would have attracted teachers who were
for the effectiveness of the High/Scope pre-
substantially better than other public school
school model as practiced throughout the
teachers, and in fact the teachers who worked
U.S. Half of High/Scope-certified trainers in
in the program were hired locally by ordinary
the study were in Head Start, 27% were in
search and hiring procedures. Nevertheless,
public schools, and 20% were in private child current child care (and Head Start) teacher
care agencies. They had a median 15 years of salaries average only about half as much as av-
early childhood experience, 88% had com- erage public school teacher salaries (National
pleted college, and 85% had teacher-train- Institute for Early Education Research, 2003).
ing responsibility—spending an average of 8 More and more preschool programs, however,
hours a week training teachers. At the time are hiring teachers at public school salaries.
of the study, the High/Scope Registry listed It has also been suggested that the quality of
1,075 early childhood leaders in 34 states the Perry Preschool program was due, in part,
and 10 other countries who had successfully to the charismatic leadership of the program’s
completed High/Scope’s 7-week trainer cer- director, David Weikart (Schorr, 1989). While
tification program in the past decade. The Weikart’s leadership was certainly essential to
average trainer had trained 15 teaching teams, the program’s success, there is every reason to
so an estimated 16,125 teaching teams, includ- believe that any dedicated preschool program
ing 29% of all Head Start staff, had received director could exercise similar leadership
High/Scope model training from these train- with respect to assuring the quality of the
ers. Since trainers regard 45% of these class- programs under his or her supervision. Such
rooms as examples of the High/Scope model, leaders insist on program quality and fidelity
they would nominate an estimated 7,256 early to a validated educational model and strive to
childhood classrooms throughout the U.S. and provide program staff with all the resources
around the world as examples of the High/ and encouragement they need to achieve
Scope model. High/Scope classrooms were them, including adequate salaries.
16

Although the program had a strong stood. Multiage intelligence tests actually
effect on children’s intellectual consist of a series of age-specific test batteries
performance, didn’t it fade out (the Stanford-Binet has 6 items per battery)
over time? designed to function with a specific age level,
such as children 4 years old or children 4
It is true that the High/Scope Perry Preschool years and 6 months of age. The preschool
program had a statistically significant effect on studies found effects at the ages during and
children’s IQs during and up to a year after the a year or two after the program, but not sub-
program, but not after that. This pattern has sequently. Children with preschool program
been found in numerous other studies, such experience got more items right on those age-
as those in the Consortium for Longitudinal specific batteries, but did not get more right
Studies (1983). The pattern raises two ques- on age-specific batteries designed for older
tions: How far does it generalize, and what children. It seems reasonable to conclude
does it mean? that, when used to assess preschool program
For some time, the pattern of children’s outcomes, intelligence tests functioned more
intellectual performance found in this study like achievement tests than intelligence tests,
was taken to represent all outcomes of this and indeed that is precisely the use to which
and similar programs. It was concluded that they were put. Imagine if achievement tests
the program had strong effects that faded out for grades 4–8 were all combined into one
over time. However, all of the subsequent grand multiage test of achievement. It would
findings of program effects in this study not be at all surprising if a really good grade
(effects on school achievement, high school 4 classroom improved children’s achievement
graduation, adult earnings, and crime preven- test scores on this test at grades 4 and 5, but
tion) disprove this conclusion. Indeed, so many not at grades 6, 7, and 8. That is precisely
studies have now found evidence of long-term what happened in the temporary effects of
effects of high-quality preschool programs high-quality preschool programs on children’s
that the opposite conclusion is practically in- intellectual performance.
disputable: High-quality preschool programs To take this thinking to a theoretical level
for young children living in poverty do have regarding children’s intellectual performance,
long-term effects. we might simply say that the preschool stud-
So what is the meaning of the fadeout of ies showed intellectual performance to be
program effect on children’s intellectual per- environmentally sensitive—it went up in
formance? More than anything else, it teaches intellectually stimulating preschool settings
us about the nature of multiage intelligence and down in less intellectually stimulating
tests. Unlike most achievement tests that are elementary school settings. Or, to put it in
age-specific, most intelligence tests, like the terms of program and no-program groups, it
Stanford-Binet (Terman & Merrill, 1960), are went up when the program group’s experi-
designed to be used with individuals of a ence was more intellectually stimulating than
wide range of ages, from early childhood to that of the no-program group and returned to
adulthood. Also unlike achievement tests, the same level as that of the no-program group
intelligence tests were not designed to assess when both found themselves in the same
program effects, and so the way they function elementary school settings.
in this role was not, and is not, well under-
17

Was the preschool program’s effect on Why did the High/Scope Perry
intellectual performance critical to its Preschool Program affect males and
success and can this goal be replaced females differently?
by another goal, such as early literacy
skills and other content? Males and females in this study differed
substantially from each other on educational
The causal model presented in the Lifetime attainment and lifetime arrests.
Effects monograph (Schweinhart et al., 2005) Evidence of stronger program effects on
identifies intellectual performance as the females appears for regular high school gradu-
gateway from the preschool program to all ation rate, repeating a grade, and treatment
subsequent program effects. However, the for mental impairment. Over 21/2 times as
original hypothesis was that a good preschool many program females as no-program females
program would increase children’s intellectu- graduated from regular high school (84% vs.
al performance permanently, not temporarily; 32%), whereas about the same percentages
and typically, after early childhood, intel- of program and no-program males graduated
lectual performance does not change much from regular high school (50% vs. 54%). Half
(Terman & Merrill, 1960). Perhaps rather than as many program females as no-program fe-
identifying the gateway variable as early child- males repeated a grade (21% vs. 41%), while
hood intellectual performance, we should call slightly more program males than no-program
it the preschool intellectual boost. males repeated a grade (47% vs. 39%). Less
The High/Scope educational model was than one-fourth as many program females as
originally called the Cognitively Oriented no-program females (8% vs. 36%) were treat-
Curriculum (Weikart et al., 1970) because it ed for mental impairment, while only two-
focused on cognitive, logical processes identi- thirds as many program males as no-program
fied in Piaget’s theory of education (Piaget & males were treated for mental impairment
Inhelder, 1969)—such as representation, classi- (20% vs. 33%).
fication, and seriation. Tests of early childhood Evidence indicates that the program ef-
intellectual performance demonstrably tap these fect on criminal arrests was stronger for males
processes. So the High/Scope preschool class- than for females, partly because males had
room provides a preschool intellectual boost as more arrests: 69% of no-program males, but
measured by these tests. It also provides other only 34% of no-program females, were arrest-
experiences that facilitate these intellectual pro- ed five or more times. The apparent program
cesses, such as planning and reviewing one’s effect in persons with five or more arrests was
activities, exploring what one is curious about, a reduction of about one third for males (45%
and developing a sense of personal control over vs. 69%) and for females (24% vs. 34%),
the events of one’s life—what might be called but because the percentages were higher for
intellectual performance, broadly defined. males, the reduction in number of arrests was
It makes sense to combine or supple- greater. The starkest gender difference was in
ment this emphasis on intellectual processes arrests for drug crimes, for which less than
with a focus on early literacy or mathematics half as many program males as no-program
skills found to predict later achievement, but males were arrested (18% vs. 49%), while the
it does not make sense to replace the first with percentages were about the same for program
the second. To do so runs the risk of sacrific- and no-program females (8% vs. 11%).
ing the known long-term effects on school A possible explanation for this pattern
achievement, high school graduation rates, is that teachers and school staff responded
lifetime earnings, and crime prevention. differently to girls and boys whose academic
18

performance improved as a result of receiv- pairment at about the same rate, despite better
ing the preschool program. As would be ex- performance on intellectual tests by the group
pected, educators responded to the preschool who had preschool. This may be because
program’s effect on girls’ early academic per- teachers and school staff focused primarily on
formance by keeping them in regular classes classroom misconduct (more common in both
rather than by having them repeat a grade or groups of boys than in the girls) rather than on
by assigning them to special classes for mental objective measures of academic performance
impairment. Girls who were not tracked into such as intellectual tests. For this reason, the
repeated grades or special classes were more intellectual gains made in preschool by the
likely to graduate from regular high school. male program group may not have translated
On the other hand, boys in the program and as expected to gains in high school gradua-
no-program groups were retained in grade tion rate and in other long-term indicators of
and assigned to special classes for mental im- educational success.
19

References
Barnett, W. S., Robin, K. B., Hustedt, J. T., & Schulman, K. L. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child.
(2003). The state of preschool: 2003 state preschool New York: Basic Books.
yearbook. New Brunswick, NJ: National Institute for Schorr, L. B. (1989). Within our reach: Breaking the cycle of
Early Education Research. Retrieved July 9, 2004, from disadvantage. New York: Doubleday.
http://nieer.org/yearbook/.
Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D. P. (1997a). Lasting differences:
Consortium for Longitudinal Studies. (1983). As the twig is The High/Scope Preschool Curriculum Comparison
bent…lasting effects of preschool programs. Hillsdale, study through age 23. (Monographs of the High/Scope
NJ: Erlbaum. Educational Research Foundation, 12). Ypsilanti, MI:
Cost, Quality, & Child Outcomes Study Team. (1995). Cost, High/Scope Press.
quality, and child outcomes in child care centers (2nd Schweinhart, L. J., & Weikart, D. P. (1997b). The High/Scope
ed.) [Public Report]. Denver: Economics Department, Preschool Curriculum Comparison study through age
University of Colorado at Denver. 23. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 117–143.
Dahlberg, G., Moss, P., & Pence, A. (1999). Beyond quality Schweinhart, L. J., Montie, J., Xiang, Z., Barnett, W. S.,
in early childhood education and care: Postmodern per- Belfield, C. R., & Nores, M. (2005). Lifetime effects:
spectives. London: RoutledgeFalmer. The High/Scope Perry Preschool study through age 40.
Dodge, D. T., Colker, L., & Heroman, C. (2002). The Creative (Monographs of the High/Scope Educational Research
Curriculum for Preschool. Washington, DC: Teaching Foundation, 14). Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
Strategies, Inc. Stoney, L., & M. Greenberg. (1996). The financing of child
Epstein, A. S. (1993). Training for quality: Improving early care: Current and emerging trends. [Special issue on
childhood programs through systematic inservice train- financing child care]. The Future of Children, 6(2),
ing. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press. 83–102.
Gilliam, W. S., & Zigler, E. F. (2004). State efforts to evalu- Terman, L. M., & Merrill, M. A. (1960). Stanford-Binet
ate the effects of prekindergarten: 1977 to 2003. New Intelligence Scale Form L-M: Manual for the third revi-
Brunswick, NJ: National Institute for Early Education sion. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.
Research. Retrieved July 9, 2004 from http://nieer.org/ U.S. Administration for Children and Families. (2001).
resources/research/StatePreKMeta.pdf. Program performance standards for operation of Head
Harms, T., Clifford, R. M., & Cryer, D. (1998). Early Childhood Start programs by grantees and delegate agencies.
Environment Rating Scale (Rev. ed.). New York: Teachers Retrieved July 11, 2004, from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/
College Press. programs/hsb/performance/1304A.htm.
Hohmann, M., Banet, B., & Weikart, D. P. (1979). Young U. S. Administration for Children and Families. (2004).
children in action: A manual for preschool educators. Head Start program fact sheet for FY2002 and FY 2003.
Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press. Retrieved July 11, 2004, from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/
programs/hsb/research/2003.htm and http://www.acf.
Hohmann, M., & Weikart, D. P. (1995). Educating young chil-
hhs.gov/programs/hsb/research/2004.htm
dren: Active learning practices for preschool and child
care programs (1st ed.). Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press. Weikart, D. P., Deloria, D., Lawser, S., & Wiegerink, R. (1970).
Longitudinal results of the Ypsilanti Perry Preschool
Hohmann, M., & Weikart, D. P. (2002). Educating young chil-
Project (Monographs of the High/Scope Educational
dren: Active learning practices for preschool and child
Research Foundation, 1). Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
care programs (2nd ed.). Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
Whitebook, M., Phillips, D., & Howes, C. (1993). National
Kagitcibasi, C., Sunar, D., & Bekman, S. (2001). Long-term
Child Care Staffing study revisited: Four years in the
effects of early intervention: Turkish low-income mothers
life of center-based child care. Oakland, CA: Child Care
and children. Applied Developmental Psychology, 22,
Employee Project. Retrieved July 11, 2004, from http://
333–361.
www.ccw.org/pubs/nccssrevisit.pdf.
Kakalik, J. S., Furry, W. S., Thomas, M. A., & Carney, M. F.
Woodhead, M. (1988). When psychology informs public policy:
(1981). The cost of special education. Santa Monica, CA:
The case of early childhood intervention. American
Rand Corporation.
Psychologist, 43, 443–454.
National Child Care Information Center. (2004a). Early
Zill, N., Resnick, G., Kim, K., O’Donnell, K., Sorongon, A.,
childhood workforce salaries. Retrieved July 11, 2004,
McKey, R. H., et al. (May 2003). Head Start FACES
from http://www.nccic.org/poptopics/salaries.html.
(2000): A whole child perspective on program per-
National Child Care Information Center. (2004b). Child care formance—Fourth progress report. Prepared for the
workforce qualifications. Retrieved July 11, 2004, from Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Depart-
http://www.nccic.org/poptopics/qualifications.html. ment of Health and Human Services (DHHS) under
National Institute for Early Education Research. (2003). contract HHS-105-96-1912, Head Start Quality Research
NIEER fact sheet on Head Start teachers. Retrieved July Consortium’s Performance Measures Center. Retrieved
1, 2004, from http://nieer.org/resources/facts/index. July 11, 2004, from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/
php?FastFactID=12. core/ongoing_research/faces/faces00_4thprogress/
faces00_4thprogress.pdf.
National Prekindergarten Center. (2003). Prekindergarten
policy framework. Chapel Hill, NC: FPG Child
Development Institute, University of North Carolina
at Chapel Hill.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen