Sie sind auf Seite 1von 35

2 3

A t t a c h m e n t D i s o r g a n i z a t i o n

Unresolved Loss, Relational Violence,


and Lapses in Behavioral
and Attentional Strategies

•>

KARLEN LYONS-RUTH
DEBORAH JACOBVITZ

The conceptual cornerstone of our understanding cies to both flee from and approach the caregiver,
of disorganized attachment behaviors in infancy resulting in an experience of "fright without so-
was laid when Main and Solomon (1986) chose lution" (Main, 1995). Under this condition, a col-
the term "disorganized/disoriented" to describe lapse of behavioral strategies necessarily occurs
the diverse array of previously unrecognized and the infant is likely to display mistimed, inter-
fearful, odd, disorganized, or overtly conflicted rupted, and/or incomplete movements and ex-
behaviors exhibited during Ainsworth's strange pressions, as well as the behaviors described di-
situation procedure. As described by Main and rectly above.
Solomon (1986, 1990), infants are to be consid- Main and colleagues' descriptions of disorga-
ered "disorganized/disoriented" when, for exam- nized/disoriented behavioral patterns in infancy
ple, they appear apprehensive, cry and fall hud- (e.g., Main, 1973; Main & Solomon, 1986,1990;
dled to the floor, or put their hands to their Main & Weston, 1981) have led to an explosion
mouths with hunched shoulders in response to of more than 80 empirical and theoretical publi-
their parents' return following a brief separation. cations on the developmental origins, correlates,
Other disorganized infants display conflicting and outcomes of attachment disorganization (see
behavioral movements, such as turning in circles van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-
while simultaneously approaching their parents. Kranenburg, in press). This chapter begins with a
Still others appear disoriented, freezing all move- review of the literature on attachment disorgani-
ments while exhibiting a trance-like expression. zation in infancy, including its definition, preva-
Main and Hesse (1990, 1992; see also Hesse & lence, and associated correlates in infant be-
Mam, in press-a, in press-b) have proposed that a havior. The second major section, "Family
parent who enters altered states of consciousness Correlates of Disorganized Attachment Behav-
during discussions of loss and trauma—states ior," summarizes studies of family risk factors,
such as trance-like or dissociative states—is parental states of mind regarding attachment as-
more likely to engage in inexplicably frightening sessed with the Adult Attachment Interview
and/or frightened behavior,with a child. The (AAI; George, Kaplan, & Main, 1984, 1985,
caregiver, who at once becomes the source of 1996; Main & Goldwyn, 1998; Main, Kaplan, &
comfort and the source of alarm, arouses contra- Cassidy, 1985), and frightened or frightening
dictory responses in the infant—that is, the in- parental behavior toward an infant. In the third
fant experiences inherently contradictory tenden- major section of the chapter, data describing the

520
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 521

longitudinal reorganization of disorganized in- to reduce infant distress and arousal and main-
fant behavior into controlling forms of attach- tain a sense of security (Bowlby, 1969). Although
ment behavior are reviewed, along with evidence the infant continually monitors the physical loca-
linking both disorganized and controlling forms tion of attachment figure(s), signs of danger or
of attachment behavior with disruptive behavior potential danger to the infant (such as strange-
disorders and dissociative symptoms. The fourth ness, hunger, fatigue, illness, injury, or cues to
section of the chapter reviews recent data and anything frightening) result in higher levels of
theoretical models linking disorganized/control- activation of the infant's attachment behavioral
ling forms of attachment with adult violence, system. When the attachment system is strongly
trauma, and child maltreatment. The later devel- aroused, physical interaction with the caregiver
opment of aggression and violence is only one of (e.g., touching, clinging, cuddling) may be nec-
a number of developmental pathways related to essary to terminate the infant's attachment be-
early attachment disorganization, but it has re- havior. When the. environment is benign and the
ceived the most consistent research attention and mother is present or her whereabouts are well
is important because of the serious social conse- known, the child typically ceases to exhibit at-
quences of such behavior. It should be empha- tachment behavior and instead explores the envi-
sized, however, that this is a pathway followed by ronment (Bowlby, 1969).
a minority of infants or adults in the disorganized Separation from an attachment figure in unfa-
group. The final major section of the chapter, miliar circumstances, although not necessarily
"An Integrative Conceptual Framework: Loss, truly dangerous, produces physiological arousal
Trauma, and the Relational Diathesis," outlines a in virtually all infants. This arousal is assumed to
theoretical framework capable of encompassing be a preadapted mechanism to protect the infant
the diverse presentations of disorganized/con- from harm, because separation from an adult
trolling attachment behavior across the lifespan. caregiver exposes the infant to an increased risk
It also offers a proposed model to account for of danger (Bowlby, 1969). What appear to vary
some of the variations in behavior and adaptation across infants are their responses to their care-
within the disorganized spectrum at any given givers in the face of the arousal produced by sep-
age. aration in an unfamiliar environment. Ainsworth,
Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978) discovered three
patterns of behavior displayed by infants when
they are distressed following an unwanted sepa-
DISORGANIZED ATTACHMENT ration from their caregivers in the strange situa-
BEHAVIOR IN INFANCY: tion. Some infants, termed "avoidant," show
DEFINITION, PREVALENCE, minimal displays of affect. For example, they di-
AND ASSOCIATED INFANT rect attention toward objects and away from their
CHARACTERISTICS caregivers; they turn away or look away rather
than seek contact and comfort (Ainsworth et al.,
Definition of Disorganized/Disoriented 1978). Other infants, termed "resistant" (and al-
Behavior ternatively termed "ambivalent" by Ainsworth as
Understanding the central role of fear in attach- well as others), mingle proximity and contact-
ment theory, including the infant's response to seeking with angry behavior. For example, a re-
situations that arouse anxiety, is critical to under- sistant infant may raise his or her arms, signaling
standing attachment disorganization (Hesse & to be held, but then arch away and resist contact
Main, in press-a, in press-b; Main, 1995; Main & as the caregiver tries to pick him or her up. Re-
Hesse, 1990). Bowlby's (1969) initial formula- sistant children seem unable to be comforted and
tion of attachment theory emphasized the organi- calmed by their caregivers, and rather than re-
zation of a diverse set of behaviors around the set turning to exploration following reunion, they
goal of maintaining physical proximity or con- often continue to cry and otherwise exhibit dis-
tact as a basis for describing how a child's tie to tress as the reunion episode ends. Finally, "se-
the mother functions at the level of observable cure" infants seek proximity and contact with lit-
behavior. During the first year of life, the infant's tle or no avoidance or angry resistance toward
attachment to the caregiver is expressed through their caregivers. The contact and comfort they re-
an increasingly sophisticated behavioral system ceive are effective in calming them, allowing
that functions at an evolutionary level to protect them to return to play. These three attachment
the infant from harm and at a psychological level categories have been identified in families from
522 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

a range of socioeconomic backgrounds in the abortive approach; moving away from the parent
United States and in various cultures around the to the wall when apparently frightened by the
world. (For comprehensive reviews, see Thomp- stranger; screaming for the parent by the door
son, Chapter 13; van IJzendoorn & Sagi, Chapter upon separation, then moving silently away at re-
31; and Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, union; raising hand to mouth in an apprehensive
Chapter 4, this volume.) gesture immediately upon reunion; and, while in
For many years, researchers noted that some an apparently good mood, slowly swiping at the
infants did not fit the three established attach- parent's face with a trancelike expression. What
ment patterns (Crittenden, 1985; Lyons-Ruth, these infants had in common was the display of
Connell, Zoll, & Stahl, 1987; Main & Weston, one or several disorganized behaviors—that is,
1981; Radke-Yarrow, Cummings, Kuczynski, & contradictory movements and expressions corre-
Chapman, 1985). It was unclear what should be sponding to an inferred contradiction in inten-
done with children who did not fit the standard tions or plans (approaching a parent with head
categories. Researchers simply put these children averted), or behaviors that involved apprehension
aside or forced them into the best-fitting attach- either directly (fearful facial expressions, oblique
ment category. The description of the disorga- approaches) or indirectly (disoriented behaviors,
nized/disoriented attachment category was including dazed and trance-like expressions;
grounded in a recognition that many infants in freezing of all movement at a parent's entrance).
the strange situation were difficult or impossible Main and Solomon (1986, 1990) called this
to classify, as well as in the direct observation of new category "disorganized/disoriented." They
conflict behaviors in infants and toddlers (Main, proposed that infants be considered disorga-
1973; Main & Solomon, 1986, 1990). nized/disoriented when, in the presence of the
Based on ethologists' descriptions of "conflict caregiver in the strange situation, they display
behaviors" observed in nonhuman primates (i.e., behaviors falling into one or more of the follow-
behaviors believed to result from the simultane- ing seven thematic headings:
ous activation of incompatible systems; see
Hinde, 1970), Main (1979; see also Main, 1973) 1. Sequential display of contradictory behav-
developed a scale for assessing "disorganized/ ior patterns, such as very strong attachment
disordered" behaviors in human infants. This behavior suddenly followed by avoidance,
scale was used to code infant behavior in a pro- freezing, or dazed behavior.
cedure in which each of a group of 1-year-old in- 2. Simultaneous display of contradictory be-
fants was exposed to an initially silent, unmov- haviors, such as strong avoidance with
ing, masked "clown" in the parent's presence strong contact seeking, distress, or anger.
(Main & Weston, 1981). One week later, each in- 3. Undirected, misdirected, incomplete, and
fant participated in Ainsworth's strange situation interrupted movements and expressions—
procedure with the same parent. Thirteen percent for example, extensive expressions of dis-
of the infants were judged "unclassifiable" with tress accompanied by movement away
Ainsworth's three-category attachment classifi- from, rather than toward, the mother.
cation system, and disorganized behavior in the 4. Stereotypies, asymmetrical movements,
"clown session" was most pronounced among mistimed movements, and anomalous pos-
these infants (Main & Weston, 1981). tures, such as stumbling for no apparent
To gain a better understanding of why some reason and only when the parent is present.
infants were unclassifiable, Main and Solomon 5. Freezing, stilling, and slowed "underwater"
(1986, 1990) reexamined over 200 anomalous movements and expressions.
strange situation videotapes. They concluded that 6. Direct indices of apprehension regarding
most of the children seemed to lack any coher- the parent, such as hunched shoulders or
ent, organized strategy for dealing with the stress fearful facial expressions.
of separation. Main and Solomon (1986, 1990) 7. Direct indices of disorganization and dis-
provide several striking illustrations throughout orientation, such as disoriented wandering,
their texts. For example, one unclassifiable infant confused or dazed expressions; or multiple
cried loudly while attempting to gain her moth- rapid changes in affect.
er's lap, then suddenly fell silent and stopped
moving for several seconds. Others were ob- Disorganization in infancy was slow to be
served approaching the parent with head averted; identified, because the behaviors are fleeting and
rocking on hands and knees following an often out of context. For example, disorganized
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 523

behavior is seen when a child who appears to be low me, d-don't"). Main and Hesse (1990; see
in a good mood suddenly strikes at the mother, or also Hesse & Main, in press-b) propose that
when a child stops with a dazed expression for frightened parental behavior occurs spontaneous-
20 seconds during an approach to the* mother. ly and is triggered internally, stemming from par-
Such behaviors are clearly unusual from the per- ents' thoughts or from events or objects in the en-
spective of a well-trained observer. Those who vironment associated with their own traumatic
are new to the system, however, may not take no- and/or. frightening -experiences. According to
tice-of such.behaviors, since they happen quickly these authors (Hesse & Main, in press-b; Main &
and' by definition lack coherence; they do not Hesse, 1990), the apparent inexplicability of
make sense to the observer. However, high inter- such frightened parental behavior will inevitably
rater reliability on the classification of infant be- be alarming to an infant.
havioral patterns as disorganized/disoriented has A frightened or frightening attachment figure
been established in a variety of studies (Lyons- presents an inherent conflict for an infant. Fear
Ruth, Connell, Grunebaum, & Botein, 1990; of the parent activates the attachment system,
Main &, Solomon, 1990; Owen & Cox, 1997), and the infant feels compelled to seek proximity;
while across all studies conducted to date relia- however, proximity-seeking increases the infant's
bility (80%) is acceptable (van IJzendoorn, fear, and he or.she then contradicts the approach.
Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, in press). The attachment figure is "at once the source of
Crittenden (1992) offers a somewhat different and the solution to its alarm" (Main & Hesse,
conceptualization of children originally consid- 1990, p. 163). This paradox results in a collapse
ered "unclassifiable." Similar to Main and her of the infant's behavioral and attentional strate-
colleagues, she proposes that some of these chil- gies. Unable to maintain a single coherent strate-
dren show a mixture of avoidant and resistant gy, the infant shows disorganized and/or disori-
strategies. Crittenden (1992), however, assigns ented attachment behavior. Although Hesse and
them to a "defended/coercive" category and sug- Main (in press-a, in press-b) have specified that
gests that many of them should be viewed as dis- disorganized attachment behavior may stem from
playing an organized attachment pattern, because experiences other than frightened or frightening
they are strategically adapting their behavior to caregiving, the central conflict thought to distin-
the constraints present in the caregiving relation- guish disorganized infants from other insecurely
ship. attached infants is that they cannot find a solu-
tion to the paradox of fearing the figures whom
they must approach for comfort in times of
Theoretical Model stress. Infants frequently frightened by the pri-
Main and Hesse (1990; see also Hesse & Main, mary caregiver are believed, then, to be exposed
in press-a, in press-b) have hypothesized that dis- repeatedly to experiences of "fright without solu-
organized infant attachment behavior arises from tion" (Main, 1995).
the infant^ experiencing the attachment figure
herself as frightening. Infants are likely to find
their attachment figures frightening under sever- Prevalence and Stability
al conditions. Some attachment figures engage in of Disorganized Attachment Patterns
directly frightening behavior, ranging from A meta-analysis conducted by van IJzendoorn,
creeping up from behind an infant and sliding Schuengel, and Bakermans-Kranenburg (in
both hands around the infant's neck and throat to press) indicated that the percentage of infants
incidents of physical or sexual abuse. Other at- classified as disorganized was 14% in middle-
tachment figures, however, appear frightened of class, nonclinical groups in North America (n =
their own infants. Hesse and Main (in press-b; 1,882) and 24% in low-socioeconomic-status
Main & Hesse, 1995) describe frightened pa- (low-SES) samples (n = 493). Results of studies
rental behaviors as including entrance into disso- investigating the stability over time of disorga-
ciative or trance-like states (e.g., freezing of all nized attachment behaviors have been mixed.
movement with 'a "dead" stare, unblinking), The stability of the disorganized pattern in the
seeking safety and comfort from an infant (e.g., maltreatment study conducted by Barnett, Gani-
showing deferential behavior), and viewing an ban, and Cicchetti (in press) was 67% from 12 to
infant as a source of alarm (e.g., one parent 18 months and 81% from 18 to 24 months, com-
backed away from her infant while stammering pared to stabilities of secure attachments of 75%
in an unusual and frightened voice, "D-don't fol- and 69%, respectively. Vondra, Hommerding,

1i
524 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

and Shaw (in press) also reported a stability rate Solomon (1990) reported that 31 of 34 infants
of 67% in disorganized classifications from 12 to classified as disorganized with one parent were
18 months of age in a low-income sample. These not classified as disorganized with the other par-
stability rates are much higher than Cicchetti and ent These findings' suggest that attachment disor-
Barnett (1991) reported among 30- to 48-month- ganization emerges within a particular, relation-
old maltreated preschoolers. Barnett et al. (in ship; they do not support the notion of attachment
press) attribute the change in apparent stability disorganization as an individual trait or inborn
across the two studies to Cicchetti and Barnett's characteristic of the infant. Based on a meta-
(1991) use of scoring criteria for the preschool analysis of 12 samples including 1,877
period based on the system of Cassidy, Marvin, participants, van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, and-
and the MacArthur Working Group on Attach- Bakermans-Kranenburg (in press) reported a
ment (1991)—a coding system that has not been nonsignificant association between disorganized
used and refined in relation to high-risk samples attachment behavior in infancy and constitutional'
as extensively as the infant scoring system for and temperamental variables (r = .003). In the
disorganization. * eight studies (n = 1,639) that examined the associ-
Lyons-Ruth, Repacholi, McLeod, and Silva ation between difficult temperament in particular
(1991) reported a much lower stability rate from and disorganized attachment classification, the'
12 to 18 months, primarily because of a substan- effect size (r = .02) was small and not significant.
tial increase in the number of disorganized attach- Other types of evidence also indicate that indi-
ments from 12 to 18 months of age. Vondra et al. vidual differences in emotional reactivity, infant
(in press) and Beckwith and Rodning (1991) also difficultness, or general behavioral disorganiza-
reported increases in disorganized attachment be- tion are unlikely to account for disorganized ate
havior from 12 to 18 months in low-income sam- tachment behavior. Following up V Carlson, Cic-
ples, and Barnett et al. (in press) reported a signif- chetti, Barnett, and Braunwald's (1989) sample
icant increase in vocal distress among of maltreated and nonmaltreated 1-year-olds',-
disorganized infants from 12 to 18 months (but Barnett et al. (in press) explored directly whether
not from 12 to 24 months). Thus some shifts in in- 18- and 24-month-olds classified as disorga-
fant presentation of disorganized behavior are nized at 1 year were more emotionally reactive—
likely to be occurring during this period, and these that is, exhibited higher levels of separation
deserve further description. Vondra et al. (1997) distress—than nondisorganized infants. Emo-*
also found that disruptive family events were re- tionality was assessed by rating distress vocaliza-
lated to change to a disorganized classification tions at 15-second intervals to create a measure
from 12 to 18 months of age. A meta-analysis of combining peak distress level and duration of
nine samples, including a total of 515 participants distress. There were no significant relations be-
from lower- and higher-SES backgrounds, indi- tween disorganized attachment classification and
cated significant stability of disorganized attach- emotionality ratings at 12, 18, or 24 months. In
ments across time periods ranging from 1 to 60 addition, emotionality at 12 or 18 months did not
months (r = .36, van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & predict disorganization at a subsequent assess-
Bakermans-Kranenburg, in press). ment. Since high negative emotional reactivity is
a core component of the construct of difficult
temperarnent, this evidence also weighs against a
Temperament and Disorganized view of disorganized infants as having a general-
Attachment Behavior ly difficult temperament. In another longitudinal
study' following 157 infants from birth to 1-8
Given the behavioral maladaptation evident in months, E. Carlson (1998) found that ratings of
many disorganize&attachment behaviors, one im* attachment disorganization in the strange situa-
portant question is whether a difficult tempera- tion at 12 and 18 months were not related to maJ
ment, or individual differences in emotional reac- ternal history of serious medical or psychologi-
tivity or behavioral disorganization, may underlie cal problems, medical complications during
or precede the disorganized attachment classifi- pregnancy or delivery, infant anomalies, Brazel-
cation. However, recent studies have shown that ton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale
infants are unlikely to be 'classified as dis- (NBAS) scores when the infants were 7 and 10
organized with more than one caregiver (Main days old, or infant temperament ratings at 3
& Solomon, 1990; Steele, Steele, & Fonagy, months. Finally, Vondra et al. (in press)jbund no
1996; van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans- relation between maternal ratings of difficultness*
Kranenburg, in press). For example, Main and
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 525

or observers' ratings of fussy behavior at 12 or and exploration posits that continued activation
18 months and infant disorganized classifica- of the attachment system will inhibit exploration
tions at 12 or 18 months. of the- environment. Therefore, some relations
However, in one study, disorganized attach- between attachment and cognitive •development
ment patterns in infancy were predicted by new- may be expected. Studying low-income mothers
born behavioral organization. 'In a middle- and their controls, Lyons-Ruth, Repacholi, et al.
income sample of 88 mother-infant pairs, Span- (1991) found that irifant disorganized attachment
gler, Fremmer-Bombik, and Grossmann (1996) classification'was related to infant mental devel-
found that infants displaying disorganized at- opment scores, even after maternal verbal JQ es-
tachment patterns exhibited lower orientation on timates, maternal involvement at home, and ma-
the Brazelton NBAS. Newborns' behavioral di- ternal hostile/intrusive behavior at home were
mensions, however, did not predict the three or- controlled for. They also found that infant disor-
ganized attachment patterns. Since findings that ganization was related to a pattern of mild "men-
have related scores on the NBAS to both orga- tal lag," in which mental scores on the Bayley
nized-and disorganized attachment patterns have Scales of Infant Development lagged behind
not replicated, caution is needed in interpreting Bayley motor scores by one half of a standard
this finding- (E. Carlson, 1998; Crockenberg, deviation or more. Although only 33% of disor-
1981; Grossmann, Grossmann, Spangler, Suess, ganized infants and 8% of nondisorganized in-
& Unzner, 1985; Spangler et al., 1996). fants displayed this pattern, this subgroup of dis-
organized infants contributed disproportionately
to the group of children who were highly exter-
Cortisol Secretion and Disorganized nalizing by age 7 (Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, &
Attachment Behaviors Cibelli, 1997). Whether such cognitive effects
Several studies haVe investigated the extent to will also be observed in middle-income settings,
which attachment security helps children cope or whether these effects represent an interaction
with stress. In animal models, Cortisol secretion between infant disorganization and other aspects
is correlated with an animal's inability to mobi- of the low-income environment, still needs to be
lize an effective strategy to cope with a stressor. explored. A later section of this chapter discusses
To date, three studies have examined the associa- cognitive effects in middle childhood.
tion between infants' attachment behavior and
their stress levels as indexed by salivary Cortisol
levels. In two of the three studies, infants with
disorganized attachment classifications showed FAMILY CORRELATES
significantly higher-cortisol levels than those of OF DISORGANIZED ATTACHMENT
secure infants in response to brief separations BEHAVIOR
(Hertsgaard, Gunnar, Erickson, & Nachmias,
1995; Spangler & Grossmann, 1993). Significant Disorganized Attachment Behavior
differences in the level of salivary Cortisol were and Family Risk Factors ^
not evident prior to participation in the strange The incidence of disorganized attachment classi-
situation but did appear 30 minutes after the fications in infancy has ranged from 13% to
strange situation (Spangler 8^ Grossmann, 1993). 82%, depending on the presence and types of
These findings are consistent with Main and family risk factors. Serious family risk factors,
Solomon's (1986) view that disorganized infant including child maltreatment, parental major de-
behaviors reflect the lack of an effective strategy pressive disorder, parental bipolar disorder, and
for coping with stress; this lack results in higher parental alcohol intake have been associated with
and more enduring levels of stress hormones af- significant increases in the incidence of disorga-
ter the strange situation. nized attachment patterns in infancy.
In both middle- and low-income samples, mal-
treatment by parents has been associated with
Cognitive Correlates of Disorganized disorganization in infancy. In a study of physical-
Infant Attachment Behaviors ly abused toddlers from middle-income back-
Few studies have explored the cognitive corre- grounds, George and Main (1979) initially de-
lates of infant disorganized attachment behavior. scribed abused toddlers' behaviors with their
However, Bowlby's (1969) formulation of the peers and caregivers in a day care center. These
complementary relations between attachment behaviors were similar to indices of disorga-
526 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

nized/disoriented infant behavior in the strange families received no intervention services. Mater-
situation; for example, the toddlers showed prox- nal depressive symptoms in this sample were seri-
imity seeking accompanied by head aversion, de- ous enough to warrant referral to a home-visiting
tours, and oblique approaches. V Carlson et al. service and were stable over a 314-year period
(1989) found that 82% of the maltreated infants (Alpern & Lyons-Ruth, 1991).
in their low-income sample were classified as A meta-analysis of 16 studies (« = 1,053) ex-
disorganized, compared to 18% of those in the amining the relationship between maternal de-
low-income control group. Lyons-Ruth et al. pressive symptoms and infant disorganization in
(1990) found that 55% of maltreated infants who a wide variety of samples, however, showed only
had received extensive home-visiting services a marginally significant relationship (r = .06,
were classifiable as disorganized. p < .06; van Hzendoorn, Schuengel, & Baker-
Several studies have examined the relationship mans-Kranenburg, in press). Only 3 of the 16
between maternal depression and attachment dis- studies reported that infant disorganization was
organization, because depression in mothers has more common among depressed mothers than
been shown to be associated with both irritable among nondepressed mothers. In a large Nation-
and inconsistent caregiving. The results of such al Institute of Child Health and Human Develop-
studies have^been mixed. Several studies have ment (NICHD) study on day care, involving
found an association between maternal depres- 1,131 families, no significant relationship was
sion or bipolar disorder and attachment disorga- found between attachment disorganization in in-
nization in infants. Teti, Gelfand, Messinger, and fants and the psychological adjustment of their
Isabella (1995), studying middle-income de- mothers, as assessed by the Center for Epidemio-
pressed mothers in treatment, found that 40% of logic Studies Depression Scale and the Neurotf-
the infants (aged 16—21 months) of depressed cism, Extraversion, and Openness Personality In-
mothers were classified as disorganized, com- ventory (NICHD Early Child Care Research
pared to 10% in the nondepressed group. In addi- Network, 1997). It appears that more chronic and
tion, using Crittenden's Preschool Assessment of severe maternal depression resulting in signifi-
Attachment (PAA), they found that 29% of older cant clinical impairment is necessary before as-
children of depressed mothers (aged 21-26 sociations with infant disorganization become
months) were also classified in the PAA "anx- apparent.
ious depressed," "defended/coercive," and "inse- Only one study has examined the relationship
cure/other" groups compared to 9% of controls. between maternal alcohol intake and attachment
(Crittenden views these groups as comparable to disorganization in infants. O'Connor, Sigman,
the disorganized attachment classification.) It is and Brill,(1987) found that infants of middle-in-
, notable that the depressed mothers in this study come mothers who had consumed moderated
were all receiving psychotherapeutic treatment, heavy amounts of alcohol prior to pregnancy
including medication when indicated, and that were more likely to be classified as disorganized
I two-thirds had also received 1 year of additional than a comparison group of infants whose moth-
parenting services prior to these attachment as- ers were either abstinent or light drinkers. Repli-
sessments. cation of this finding is needed for confirmation.
In another study of middle-income families,
DeMulder and Radke-Yarrow (J 991) reported
that 50% of the infants and preschoolers of moth- Prevalence of Subgroups within the
ers suffering from bipolar disorder were classified Disorganized Attachment Category
as disorganized, compared to 25% of children of by Family Risk Factors
unipolar depressed mothers, and 18% of the chil- The coding directions for assigning a disorga-
dren of controls. In addition, mothers of disorga- nized classification include instructions also to
nized/controlling children were the most negative assign a secondary best-fitting organized attach-
in their affect tone, displaying a high level of ment classification—either secure, avoidant, or
downcast affect, high levels of more than one kind ambivalent—to each infant. In the disorganized/
of negative affect, and low levels of tenderness secure subgroup, the infant seeks contact with the
and affection. Finally, in an intervention study of caregiver without marked avoidance or ambiva-
depressed mothers on welfare and their controls, lence and is soothed by his or her presence but
Lyons-Ruth et al. (1990) reported that 62% of the shows other unusual signs of hesitation, confu-
infants of chronically depressed low-income sion, apprehension, dysphoria, or conflict in rela-
mothers were classified as disorganized if the tion to the caregiver. Disorganized/avoidant and
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 527

disorganized/ambivalent infants often display un- child maltreatment (Lyons-Ruth, Repacholi, et


expected combinations of distress, contact seek- al., 1991).
ing, avoidance, resistance, or other apprehensive In addition, mothers of disorganized/insecure
or conflict behaviors. In 20 studies across 25 sam- infants were shown i n one study to be more in-
ples (n = 1,219), van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, and trusive and negative with their infants at home
Bakermans-Kranenburg (in press) reported that, than mothers of disorganized/secure infants. In
in 34% of the cases, disorganized attachment was contrast, mothers of disorganized/secure infants
accompanied by a secondary classification of displayed more withdrawal from the infapt
avoidance (disorganized/avoidant infants); in (Lyons-Ruth, Repacholi, et al., 1991; see also the
14% of the cases, it was accompanied by a sec- sections below on frightened/frightening caregiv-
ondary secure classification (disorganized/secure ing). Further work is needed to explore potential
infants); and in 46% of the cases, disorganization subgroups in the disorganized category and to
was accompanied by an ambivalent classification assess whether such subgroups constitute distinct
(disorganized/ambivalent infants). precursors to the caregiving and punitive sub-
Although on most measures reviewed in this groups that emerge within the disorganized spec-
chapter disorganized infants have shown similar trum during the preschool period (see the section
profiles regardless of the best-fitting secondary on longitudinal studies below).
classification, a few studies have found differ-
ences based on the secondary classification. For
example, the incidence of disorganized/secure Disorganized Attachment Behavior
infants -was found to be highest in low-risk, mid- and Parental Unresolved States of Mind
dle-SES samples, as shown in Table 23.1 (Lyons- on the AAI
Ruth, Repacholi, et al., 1991). Compared with Exploring the parental mental representations, or
behavior shown by the disorganized/secure internal working models of attachment relation-
group, disorganized forms of avoidant or am- ships, that are associated with infants' displays of
bivalent infant behavior have been associated particular attachment patterns has deepened our
with low SES, with more severe maternal psy- understanding of the intergenerational transmis-
chosocial problems (e.g., chronic maternal de- sion of attachment. According to attachment the-
pression), and with a history of documented ory, as patterns of interaction and affective re-

TABLE 23.1. Subtypes of Disorganized Attachment Behavior Reported in Previous Studies by Risk
Status of Sample
Age of infant
(in months) n Risk status n(D) % D/secure
Low risk
Main & Solomon (1990) 12-18 268 Middle-class 34 61.7
Ainsworth & Eichberg (1991) 12-18 45 Middle-class 15 53.3
Moderate risk
O'Connor, Sigman, & Brill (1987) 12 46 Middle-class 16 S0.0
(12 prenatal
heavy drinkers)
High risk:
Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Grunebaum, & 18 70 Low-income/ 33 30.3
Botein(1990) at-risk
Carlson, Cicchetti, Barnett, & 11-16 43 Low-income/ 22 27.3
Braunwald (1989) maltreated
Spieker & Booth (1988) 13 60 Low-income/ 17 5.9°
at-risk
Note: Comparing distribution of D/secure and D/insecure subclassifications within the disorganized category by low, moderate,
or high risk, x2 (2, « = 120) = 9.77, p < .01 (Spieker & Booth, 1988, omitted due to different reporting procedures). From
Lyons-Ruth, Repacholi, McLeod, and Silva (1991). Copyright 1991 by Cambridge University Press. Reprinted by permission.
"20 uncertain B (secure) eliminatedfrom,sample.
528 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

sponse are repeated in close relationships over of the first child, predicts the security of the in-
time, children build expectations about future in- fant-parent attachment relationship at 1 year
teractions with parents and others that guide their (van IJzendoorn, 1995; see Crowell, Fraley, &
interpretations and behaviors in new situations. Shaver, Chapter 20, and Hesse, Chapter 19, this
As these expectations, which are largely uncon- volume, for discussions of the AAI).
scious, become elaborated and organized they The unresolved classification is assigned to
are termed "internal working models of attach- adults who show signs of disorientation and dis-
ment relationships." These working models of organization during discussions of potentially
self and others tend to perpetuate themselves in traumatic events (i.e., loss by death, physical
the absence of specific influences for change and abuse, or sexual abuse). As detailed by Main and
become incorporated as stable interpersonal ten- Goldwyn (1998), one such index is a lapse in the
dencies which endure over time and guide later monitoring of discourse, whereby the speaker en-
parental behavior (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, ters a state of mind in which he or she no longer
this volume). appears appropriately conscious of the interview
Main, Kaplan, and Cassidy (1985) have dem- situation and has in fact "lost awareness of the
onstrated that when parents' representations of discourse context." Main and Morgan (1996)
their childhood attachment relationships are ex- provide striking examples: "Some adults fall
plored in an open-ended interview format, four silent in the middle of a sentence discussing loss
broad classifications of the adult's state of mind or trauma, and then complete the sentence 20
regarding attachment can be reliably assigned seconds or more later, as if no time had passed.
(George, Kaplan, & Main, 1984, 1985, 1996; Others may never complete the sentence, as-*he
Main & Goldwyn, 1998). These four classifica- died, and his face [52-second pause]. I guess J
tions, labeled "autonomous," "dismissing," "pre- was just finishing high school'" (p. 125). Indica-
occupied," and "unresolved," predict the four in- tions of losing the discourse context include a
fant attachment classifications: "secure," sudden shift to an odd or poetic phrasing in dis-
"avoidant," "resistant" (or "ambivalent") and cussing loss or trauma. For example, "She was
"disorganized." Adults classified as secure "val- young, she was lovely, she was dearly beloved by
ue attachment relationships and regard attach- all who knew her and who witnessed her as she
ment experiences as influential, but they are rela- was torn from the ground at its moment of splen-
tively independent and objective regarding any dor" (Main & Goldwyn, 1998, p. 123).
: particular experience or relationship" (Main & Other indices of unresolved loss or trauma in-
Goldwyn, 1998, p. 159). Secure adults are aware volve lapses in the monitoring of reasoning.
of the nature of their experiences with their par- Lapses in the monitoring of reasoning are usually
ents during childhood and have considered the brief and should not be confused with "irra-
effects of these experiences on them. Adults con- tional" thinking in the transcript as a whole.
sidered secure appear free to explore their These lapses can take several forms, including
thoughts and feelings during the course of the in- indications of a belief that a lost person is simul-
terview. Adults classified as dismissing devalue, taneously dead and alive (in a physical rather
minimize, or dismiss the importance of attach- than a religious sense). For example, one speaker
ment relationships and experiences. They may said, "It's probably better that he is dead, because
dismiss possible imperfections in their parents in now he can get on with being dead and I can get
the face of contradictory evidence or they may on with raising a family" (Main & Goldwyn,
contemptuosly derogate attachment figures or at- 1998, p. 118). Such lapses may also include dis-
tachment-related experiences. Finally, adults belief that the person is dead (e.g., discussing a
classified as preoccupied appear "confused, un- parent in the present tense even though the parent
objective and preoccupied with past relationships died 20 years earlier). Like disorganized infants,
within the family and/or past experiences" (Main parents classified as unresolved are also given a
& Goldwyn, 1998, p. 168). Preoccupied adults best-fitting alternate classification that identifies
may appear "passive and vague, fearful and over- the pattern most closely corresponding to the
h whelmed, and/or angry, conflicted, and uncon- overall organization of the interview (e.g., unre-
vincingly analytical" (Main '& Goldwyn, 1998, solved/autonomous) .
p. 168) during discussions of early family rela-
tionship experiences. Recent studies have Early loss of a parent through death has been
demonstrated that an adult's internal working associated with infant disorganization. Main and
model of attachment, assessed prior to the birth Hesse (1990) reported that 15% of the adults in
their sample had lost a parent through death be-
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 529

fore having completed high school; 56% of these press), and Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, and Parsons
parents had infants classified as disorganized. (in press) tested aspects of Main and Hesse's
Put another way, only 8% of the parents of (1990) hypothesis regarding the role of fright-
avoidant, resistant, or secure babies had experi- ened or frightening parental behavior as the me-
enced such loss, whereas 39% of the parents of diator of the effect of unresolved parental states
disorganized babies had done so. Lyons-Ruth, of mind on infant disorganization. Results of the
Repacholi, et al. (1991) reported a similar associ- three studies converged in somewhat unexpected
ation between parental death and infant disorga- ways.
nization. Early loss of an attachment figure in it- Using Main and Hesse's (1995) coding system
self, however, does not inevitably lead to infant for identifying frightened or frightening behav-
disorganization (Ainsworth & Eichberg, 1991). iors, Jacobvitz et al. (1997) found very strong as-
Rather, the lack of resolution of the loss, as re- sociations between a. prenatal assessment of ma-
vealed in parents' lapses in the monitoring of rea- ternal unresolved loss on the AAI and a mother's
soning and discourse during discussions of loss display of frightened or frightening behaviors to-
on the AAI, is what forecasts attachment disorga- ward her firstborn infant at 8 months in a sample
nization in their infants. A meta-analysis of nine of 113 middle-income families. Frightening ma-
studies (« = 548) revealed an effect size of .65 ternal behaviors included unusual vocal patterns,
(r = .31) for the relation hetween child disorgani- such as simultaneously voicing and devoicing in-
zation and parental unresolved status (van IJzen- tonations; baring teeth; invasions into vulnerable
doorn, 1995). The effect sizes across studies var- areas of the infant's personal space, such as
ied very strongly in relation to the amount of hands suddenly, silently sliding from behind
training coders had received in the system for across the infant's throat; sudden looming into
coding disorganized attachment, with more train- the infant's face; and movements or postures that
ing being associated with stronger effect sizes. seemed to be part of a pursuit/hunt sequence
(Main & Hesse, 1992b, 1995; see also Main &
Hesse, 1990). One mother in the study conducted
Unresolved Parental States of Mind by Jacobvitz and her colleagues (1997; see also
and Frightened/Frightening Caregiving Thalhuber, Jacobvitz, & Hazen, 1998) appeared
One of the critical questions needing further to enter a trance-like state, sitting immobilized in
study is why unresolved loss or trauma on the an uncomfortable position (with her hand in the
part of parents is related to their infants* display air) and blankly staring into space for 50 consec-
of disorganized attachment behavior. To what ex- utive seconds. She entered into what appeared to
tent are lapses in the monitoring of discourse and be an altered state on several occasions for a total
reasoning in discussions of loss and trauma relat- of 5 minutes during a 20-minute feeding session.
ed to the quality of care parents provide their This mother was classified as unresolved on the
children, and how do these caregiving behaviors AAI. Indications that a mother was afraid of her
contribute to disorganized infant attachment be- baby included instances in whicK.the mother
haviors? Lapses in the monitoring of reasoning handled the baby as if the baby were an inani-
and discourse are notable because they involve a mate object and in which the mother moved her
sudden shift or alteration in the quality of dis- hand away suddenly as if fearful of being hurt
course. Hesse (1996) suggests that these lapses (see Hesse & Main, in press-b).
involve "frightening and/or overwhelming expe- As predicted, then, mothers classified prena-
riences that may momentarily be controlling or tally as unresolved differed significantly from
altering discourse" (p. 8). Hesse and Main (in mothers not classified as unresolved on the 9-
press-a, in press-b) suggest that such a parent is point frightened/frightening scale. However,
still overwhelmed either by a past experience of mothers classified as unresolved did not differ
maltreatment, which is inherently frightening, or significantly from other mothers on any other
by "incompletely remembered loss experiences." negative parenting interaction patterns observed
When the still-traumatized parent responds to in the mothers' homes, including maternal insen-
memories or ideas surrounding loss or trauma, sitivity, interference, or rejection (Jacobvitz et
Main and Hesse (1990) propose that the parent al., 1997). This difference between unresolved
will engage in frightened and frightening behav- mothers and other mothers occurred regardless
ior with the infant. More recently, Jacobvitz, of whether the unresolved mothers were given a
Hazen, and Riggs (1997), Schuengel, van IJzen- secondary secure or insecure adult classification
doorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, and Blom (in (Jacobvitz, 1998). Not all mothers classified as
530 PARTY. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

unresolved, however, engaged in frightened or relationship of the person lost, the more difficult
frightening behaviors toward their infants. Com- the resolution process, particularly if the death
pared to unresolved mothers who were also as- occurred during childhood. Ainsworth and Eich-
signed a secondary insecure classification, unre- berg (1991) did not find this to be the case; they
solved mothers who were also assigned a found no significant association between either a
secondary secure (i.e., autonomous) classifica- mother's age when the loss occurred or her rela-
tion showed a marginally significant trend in the tionship to the deceased (e.g., attachment figure
direction of engaging in fewer frightened or vs. someone else) and unresolved states of mind
frightening behaviors toward their infants. on the AAI. Replicating Ainsworth and Eich-
In a sample of 85 mother-child dyads, Schuen- berg's study with a larger sample (n = 113), Ja-
gel et al. (in press) found a relation between ma- cobvitz (1998) also found that the mother's age
ternal unresolved loss on the AAI when an infant when the loss occurred was unrelated to the
was 12 months old and maternal display of fright- mother's resolution of loss but did find that
ened or frightening behaviors toward the infant at mothers who were unresolved were more likely
10 or 11 months using the Main and Hesse to have lost a parent than to have lost a less im-
(1992b) coding system, but this finding occurred portant figure. Moreover, both the mother's age
only among th&subgroup of mothers classified as when the loss occurred and the mother's relation-
unresolved/insecure on the AAI (analogous to the ship to the deceased discriminated between unre-
disorganized/insecure infant subgroup). Like Ja- solved mothers who engaged in frightened or
cobvitz et al. (1997), Schuengel et al. (in press) frightening behavior toward their infants and
found that mothers classified as unresolved/ those who did not (Jacobvitz et al., 1997). Nine-
insecure displayed significantly more frightened ty-one percent of unresolved mothers who either
or frightening behaviors than mothers classified lost an attachment figure or were younger than
as unresolved/secure. However, in a more puz- 17 when the unresolved loss occurred engaged in
zling finding that goes counter to the findings re- frightened or frightening behaviors toward their
ported by Jacobvitz (1998), Schuengel et al. (in infants. In contrast, only 20% of the unresolved
press) found that mothers classified as secure mothers who were 17 or older when the loss oc-
i displayed significantly more frightened or fright- curred and did not lose an attachment figure dis-
ening behaviors than did mothers classified played frightened or frightening behavior (Jacob-
unresolved/secure. This finding, if replicated, vitz et al., 1997). Therefore, degree of kinship
suggests some overall behavioral inhibition on the and timing of the loss may be important in pre-
part of mothers classified as unresolved/secure, dicting whether the mother's unresolved state of
since the secure but not unresolved mothers dis- mind impinges on her caregiving behavior.
played more frightened or frightening behaviors
than did this unresolved subgroup. One explana-
tion for the discrepant findings across the two Frightened/Frightening Caregiving
studies is that Jacobvitz and her colleagues creat- and Disorganized Attachment Behavior
ed a more stressful situation for mother and child in Infancy
than did Schuengel et al. (in press). In the Jacob-
Schuengel, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranen-
vitz et al. (1997) study, mothers were instructed to
burg, and Blom (1997, in press) and Lyons-Ruth,
feed their babies, play with them, and change their
Bronfman, and Parsons (in press) tested Main
babies' clothes. Not only were parents forced to
and Hesse's hypothesis that maternal frightened
interact with their children, but most of the 8-
and/or frightening behavior would be related to
month-olds resisted having their clothes changed.
the infant's attachment disorganization. In the
Schuengel et al. (in press), in contrast, did not
study by Schuengel et al. (in press), maternal
structure the home observations. Stressful condi-
frightened or frightening behavior marginally
tions may trigger thoughts associated with earlier
predicted infant disorganized attachment classi-
losses or trauma, resulting in the display of fright-
fication (p <.05, one-tailed). However, the sub-
ened and/or frightening caregiving behavior.
scale for maternal dissociated behavior more
To understand further why some mothers clas- strongly predicted infant disorganized behavior,
sified unresolved did or did not display fright- as did a broader set of maternal "disorganized"
ened or frightening behaviors toward their in- behaviors that included the frightened or fright-
fants, Jacobvitz and her colleagues explored ening behavior codes (Schuengel et al., 1997).
factors associated with the loss experience. Schuengel et al. (1997, in press) did not examine
Bowlby (1980) has suggested that the closer the maternal frightened or frightening behaviors sep-
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 531

arately for the two subgroups of disorganized in- errors, hostile/intrusive behavior, and role confu-
fants, as they did for the two subgroups of unre- sion, compared to mothers of organized infants.
solved mothers. It is notable that maternal self- However, mothers of disorganized/secure in-
reported scores on the Dissociative Experiences fants were distinguished from mothers of orga-
Scale (DES) did not predict infant disorganiza- nized infants when the patterning of maternal
tion nor did the closeness of kin of a mother's ex- frightened or frightening behavior and disrupted
perienced loss. Lyons-Ruth and Block (1996) communication was taken into account. Mothers
also failed to find a relation between maternal of disorganized/secure infants exhibited a fear-
scores on the DES and infant disorganization. ful, inhibited pattern of behavior. This pattern
Similar to Schuengel et al. (1997), Lyons- was characterized by subtle frightened behavior
Ruth, Bronfman, and Parsons (in press) found in the absence'of high levels of frightening, dis-
that a broader set of maternal behaviors that in- sociated, and-role-reversed maternal behavior. In
cluded the frightened or frightening behavior addition, mothers of disorganized/secure infants
codes (Main & Hesse, 1992b) significantly pre- exhibited a pattern of withdrawn, but not hostile,
dicted an infant's rating for disorganized behav- interaction on the disrupted communication
ior and marginally predicted an infant's disorga- codes (Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, & Atwood, in
nized attachment classification. Lyons-Ruth and press).
colleagues expanded on Main and Hesse's con- A study of 20 village-living mother-infant
struct of frightened or frightening behavior to im dyads in the Dogon ethnic group. of Mali, an
elude two other broad aspects of maternal behav- African country, provides preliminary cross-
ior hypothesized to be theoretically related to cultural support for a relationship between
infant fear and disorganization of attachment frightened or frightening maternal behavior and
strategies. These included extreme parental mis- attachment disorganization (True, Pasani, Ryan,
attunement to the specific content of an infant's & Oumar, 1998). Although none of the mothers
attachment-related communications and the dis- scored above the midpoint on the frightened or
play of competing caregiving strategies that both frightening behavior scale, infants whose moth-
elicited and rejected infant attachment affects ers scored above 1 on the frightened or frighten-
and behaviors. This broader set of behaviors, ing behavior scale were more often classified as
termed "disrupted affective communication be- disorganized in the strange situation than infants
tween mother and infant," included codes for af- whose mothers showed no frightened or frighten-
fective communication errors, role confusion, ing indices.
negative/intrusive behaviors, disorientation, and Taken together, these studies suggest that ma-
withdrawal. Maternal behavior was coded in the ternal frightened or frightening behaviors are re-
strange situation at 18 months among 65 moth- lated both to infant disorganized attachment sta-
er-infant dyads. When the frightened or frighten- tus and to parental unresolved states of mind.
ing behavior codes were analyzed separately, However, the most broadly based applications of
they significantly predicted infant disorganized the coding inventories for fear-related maternal
behavior. However, with all frightened or fright- behaviors were the most successful in predicting
ening behaviors excluded, the level of disrupted disorganized infant behavior. In addition, it ap-
maternal affective communication also signifi- pears that the overall patterning of parental be-
cantly predicted infant disorganization. haviors within the disorganized spectrum may
However, when the two subgroups of disorga- take quite different forms. Because of these po-
nized infants were examined separately on the tential differences in the profiles of maternal be-
frequency of maternal frightened or frightening havior within the disorganized classification, fu-
behavior and on the frequency of disrupted com- ture researchers may need to plan their data
munication more generally, only the mothers of analyses carefully to capture both the fearful,
disorganized/insecure infants differed signifi- inhibited subgroup and the more pervasively
cantly from mothers of infants with organized frightening subgroup.
patterns of attachment. This finding echoed
Schuengel et al.'s (in press) identification of ele-
vated frightened or frightening behavior only Other Studies of the Caregiving
among mothers in the unresolved/insecure sub- Behavior of Disorganized Infants
group. Mothers of infants in the disorganized/ Several prospective longitudinal studies follow-
insecure subgroup showed elevated rates of ing disorganized infants over time have demon-
frightening behavior, affective communication strated that their mothers may show a particularly
532 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

impaired ability to engage in well-attuned affec- ages 5 to 7 displayed the least reciprocal and
tive communications with their young children. least balanced communication, compared to
Harm, Castino, Jarosinski, and Britton (1991) ex- mothers of children in other attachment cate-
amined mother-infant interactions at 20 months gories.
among 67 low-income adolescent mothers and Spieker and Booth (1988), studying low-
their infants (the infant's attachment strategies income mothers and infants, conducted a number
had been assessed at 13 months). In this sample of analyses of measures taken both prenatally
of low-income adolescent mothers, 60% of the and at 6 weeks and 3 months postpartum. Moth-
infants were classified as disorganized with a ers of disorganized infants did not differ from
secondary insecure classification. They found mothers of secure infants on 23 interview mea-
that negotiation of conflict was significantly im- sures of social support, emotional symptoms, OP
paired among mothers of disorganized/insecure satisfaction with available resources. However,
infants. The secure 20-month-olds and their mothers of both disorganized and avoidant in-
mothers in this study were the most likely to fants were rated lower than mothers of secure in-
make social initiations, initiate nonaggressive fants on total teaching skills and on positive par-
conflict behavior, and follow nonaggressive con- ent-infant mutuality.
flict with further nonaggressive conflict. The Ratings of maternal rnsensitivity, as assessed
small number of toddlers with insecure but orga- using Ainsworth's sensitivity-insensitivity scale,
nized attachment strategies demonstrated inter- have not been associated with attachment dis-
mediate levels of initiative and intermediate fre- organization in infancy. In a meta-analysis of
quencies of nonaggressive conflict initiations. In 12 studies, van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, and
contrast, compared to the other toddlers, those Bakermans-Kranenburg (in press) reported no
previously classified as disorganized/insecure significant relationship between disorganized at-
more often initiated aggressive conflict with their tachment in infants and ratings of parental insen-
mothers and refused their mothers' social initia- sitivity. The sensitivity-insensitivity scale, which
tives, and were the least likely to initiate social is a global rating of maternal caregiving, does
exchanges with their mothers. Moreover, aggres- not appear to be differentiated and specific
sive conflicts initiated by the children in the dis- enough regarding the affective communications
organized/insecure group were less likely to be involved in fear-related behavior to predict infant
resolved than those initiated by their mothers. In disorganization.
addition, compared to the other mothers, mothers
of disorganized infants (regardless of their sec-
ondary classification) were less affectionate.
They initiated more interactions but complied LONGITUDINAL
less frequently with their children's initiatives. REORGANIZATION
This study indicates that mother-toddler ex- OF DISORGANIZED FORMS
changes in dyads from the disorganized/insecure OF ATTACHMENT BEHAVIOR
group were more often conflicted and asynchro- INTO CONTROLLING FORMS
nous. OF ATTACHMENT BEHAVIOR

Other longitudinal studies have reported simi- Two prospective longitudinal studies have
lar results. Main et al. (1985), studying a middle- demonstrated a shift from disorganized behavior-
income sample, initially reported that mothers of during infancy to controlling behavior with
disorganized infants and mothers of avoidant in- mothers (of either a punitive or a caregiving
fants exhibited low fluency and balance in dis- type) at 6 years of age (Main & Cassidy, 19,88;
course with these same children at age 6 (see Warmer, Grossmann, Fremmer-Bombik, &
Strage & Main, 1985). .Similarly, Easterbrooks, Suess, 1994). In a study of 33 families in Berke-
Lyons-Ruth, Biesecker, and Carper (1996), in a ley, California, Main and Cassidy (1988) devel-
low-income sample, showed that mothers of 18- oped an assessment of parent-child attachment
month-old infants classified as disorganized at age 6. Children's behavioral and verbal re-
were rated lower in emotionalyavailability when sponse patterns to a reunion with a parent after
they were observed with their children at 7 years an hour-long separation were classified into four
of age. Similar results were reported by Moss, categories that corresponded to the four infant at-
Parent, Gosselin, Rousseau, and St-Laurent tachment classifications, including "secure,"
(1996) in their study of a middle-income sample. "avoidant," "ambivalent," and "controlling."
Mothers of children classified as controlling at Children are classified as "controlling" if they
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 533

"seem to actively attempt to control or direct the of disorganized attachment behavior at later ages
parent's attention and behavior and assume a role is reviewed below and summarized in Table 23.2.
which is usually considered more appropriate for
a parent with reference to a child" (Main & Cas-
sidy, 1988, p. 418 ).'Eighty-four percent of the Controlling Attachment Behavior
infants classified as disorganized at 1 year were and Parental Internal Models
also classified as displaying controlling behavior
toward the parent when they were 6 years old Like disorganized behavior in infancy, control-
(Main & Cassidy, 1988). Combining data from ling/disorganized behavior in preschool children
two studies (Main & Cassidy, 1988; Wartner et has been related to mothers' mental representa-
al., 1994), van IJzendoorn, Schuengel, and Bak- tions of attachment. Several researchers have re-
ermans-Kranenburg (in press) reported a strong ported an association between controlling/disor-
association (r = .55) between attachment disorga- ganized behavior in children and unresolved loss
nization in infancy and later controlling attach- or trauma (as assessed on the AAI) in their moth-
ment behavior. Interestingly, after correcting for ers (George & Solomon, 1996;.'Greenberg,
coding error, van IJzendoorn and his colleagues Speltz, DeKlyen, & Endriga, 1991). George and
found that continuity from attachment disorgani- Solomon (1996) have further extended our un-
zation in infancy to controlling behavior during derstanding of the mental representations of
the preschool years was much stronger than caregiving among mothers whose children were
short-term stability either in disorganized attach- classified as controlling (see also George &
ment behavior in infancy or in controlling behav- Solomon, Chapter 28, this volume). In semi-
ior in preschool children (Main & Cassidy, structured interviews about mothers' relation-
1988). The array of studies exploring correlates ships to their 6-year-olds, George and Solomon

TABLE 23.2. Longitudinal Outcomes of Disorganization in Infancy and Correlates of Controlling


Behavior in Preschool and Elementary School
Outcomes and correlates
Authors Children's ages Assessment instruments of disorganization
Carlson (1998) 18, 24, and 42 Strange situation, Main Less confident with mothers
months; 4'/2-5 and Solomon coding; (24 months); avoidance of
years; grades 1,2, birth complications; mothers (42 months);
3, 6; 1714 years Neonatal Behavioral dissociation and
Assessment Scale; Carey internalizing behavior
Infant Temperament (grades 1, 2, 3, and 6), lower
Questionnaire; emotional health rank
observations of (Grades 1,2, 3, and 6);
mother-child interactions; overall behavior problems,
preschool behavior internalizing problems, &
problem index; teachers' dissociative symptoms (1714
completion of Child years); and higher ratings of
Behavior Checklist and psychopathology in general
emotional health rank; and dissociation specifically
Schedule of Affective on the K-SADS (17& years)
Disorders and
Schizophrenia for School-
Age Children (K-SADS)
Cohn (1990) 6 years Main and Cassidy system; No significant results for
peer behavior nomina- girls or boys, but
tions; teacher and peer disorganized unclassifiable
liking ratings; teacher infants were not included
completion of Classroom
Adjustment Rating Scales
and Health Resources
Inventory
(continued)
534 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

TABLE 23.2. (continued)


Outcomes and correlates
Authors Children's ages Assessment instruments of disorganization
DeMulder & Radke-Yarrow Preschool Main and Cassidy system Mothers of controlling
(1991) preschoolers displayed the
most negative affective tone
Greenberg, Speltz, DeKlyen, Preschool Main and Cassidy system; Diagnosis of oppositional
&Endriga(1991) DSM-III-R criteria defiant disorder
Harm, Castino, Jarosinski, 13 and 20 Strange situation, Main Fewest social initiatives and
&Britton(1991) months and Solomon coding; more refusals of mothers'
observations of mother- initiatives; initiation of
infant interactions conflict by aggressive
behavior
Hubbs-Tait, Osofsky, 13 and 54 Strange situation, Main Externalizing behavior
Hann,&Culp(1994) months and Solomon coding; problems; no relationship
Child Behavior Checklist between attachment and
and Child Behavior maternal reports of
Checklist Social Scale children's social skills and
completed by mothers friendship
Jacobsen et al. (1994) 7,9,12,15, and Piagetian cognitive tasks; Lower self-confidence,
17 years Chandler's perspective- difficulties with deductive
taking task, Kaplan's reasoning, including poor
coding; behavioral ratings performance on syllogistic
of self-confidence by reasoning task
interviewers
Jacobvitz & Hazen (in press) 18,20,24,30, Strange situation, Main Controlling behavior toward
36,42, and 54 and Solomon coding; mothers, either caregiving or
months observations of punitive; fearful/
I- mother-child and disorganized responses;
father-child interactions, emotional disconnection;
of family dinners, of and aggression toward peers
interactions with two
different peers, and of
peer interactions in
preschool classrooms
Kaplan (1987) 1 and 6 years Strange situation, Main Fearful/disorganized (e.g.,
and Solomon coding; catastrophic fantasies) or
Kaplan's Separation silent responses
Anxiety Test
Lyons-Ruth, Alpern, & 18 months; Strange situation, Main Hostile/aggressive behavior
Repacholi (1993) 5 years and Solomon coding; toward peers at age 5
home observations of
mother-infant
interactions; Bayley
Scales of Infant
Development; teachers'
completion of Preschool
Behavior Questionnaire
Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, 18 months; Strange situation, Main Externalizing behavior
&Cibelli(1997) 7 years and Solomon coding; problems at age 7; mild
home observations of mental lag combined with
mother-infant disorganization at 18 months
interactions; Bayley was ariskfactor for
Scales of Infant externalizing problems
l» (
(continued)
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 535

TABLE 23.2. (continued)


Outcomes and correlates
Authors Children's ages Assessment instruments of disorganization
Development; teachers'
completion of Child
Behavior Checklist

Main & Cassidy (1988) 1 and 6 years Strange situation, Main Controlling (role-reversing)
and Solomon coding; behavior toward parent,
Main & Cassidy system either punitive or caregiving
Moss, Parent, Gosselin, 3-5 years; Main and Cassidy system; Lower social competence in
Rousseau, & St-Laurent 1-7 years questionnaires completed preschool; behavior
(1996) by parents and teachers', problems shown over time,
including the Preschool with no clear pattern of
Socio-Affective Profile internalizing or externalizing
and Child Behavior problems
Checklist
Moss, Rousseau, Parent, 5-7 years; Main and Cassidy system; Externalizing and
St-Laurent, & Saintong 9-11 years questionnaires completed internalizing behavior
(1998) by teachers, including the problems in boys and girls;
Preschool Socio-Affective lower mathematics scores
Profile and Child and lower academic self-
Behavior Checklist esteem at age 8 than secure
or resistant children
Ogawa, Sroufe, Weinfeld, 12,18,24,42, Strange situation, Main Dissociation at 16 to 1714
Carlson, & Egeland (1997) 54, and 61 and Solomon coding; years and 19 years; higher
months; grades Child Behavior Checklist dissociation scores at age 19
1,2,3,6; 16, completed by mothers and for young adults classified
1714, and 19 teachers; life stress as disorganized in infancy
years measures; assessments of who also faced trauma (vs.
physical abuse, neglect, 19-year-olds who were not
and witnessing trauma; classified as disorganized or
Putnam's Child were classified as
Dissociative Checklist; disorganized but did not
K-SADS; Dissociative experience trauma)
Experiences Scale
Shaw, Owens, Vondra, 6-9,18 months; Strange situation, Main Disorganization at 1 year
Keenan, & Winslow (1996) 1,2, 3, and 5 and Solomon coding; combined withparent ratings
years mothers' completion of of difficultness at age 2
Infant Characteristics predicted aggression at age 5
Questionnaire, Child
Behavior Checklist,
Personality Research
Form, Marital Adjustment
Test, Child-Rearing
Disagreements Scale, and
Parenting Daily Hassles
Speltz, Greenberg, & 5-7 years Main and Cassidy system; Aggression and
DeKlyen(1990) DSM-in-R criteria externalizing behaviors
toward peers
Solomon, George, & 6 years Main and Cassidy system; Depiction of self and
de Jong (1995) children's response to caregiver as frightening
stories about a hurt knee, (e.g., themes of catastrophe
a monster in the bedroom, and helplessness), or

(continued)
536 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

TABLE 23.2. (continued)


Outcomes and correlates
Authors Children's ages Assessment instruments of disorganization
and parent-child complete inhibition
separation and reunion
Teti, Gelfand, Messinger, 16-21 months; Strange situation, Main Maternal depression was
& Isabella (1995) 21-26 months and Solomon coding; associated with anxious
Crittenden's Preschool depressed, defended/
Assessment of Attachment coercive, and insecure/other
preschool classifications
Warmer, Grossmann, 1, 5, and 6 years Strange situation, Main Infant disorganization was
Fremmer-Bombik, & Suess and Solomon coding; associated with controlling
(1994) Main and Cassidy system; behavior at age 6; social
observations of preschool incompetence in play groups
play groups; social (5 years) was associated with
perceptions task (cartoons controlling behavior at age 6
depicting themes of
aggression)

(1996) found that a helpless parental stance was Kaplan (1987) administered her Separation
related significantly to controlling attachment Anxiety Test to children in the Berkeley sample
behavior on the part of a child. In some cases, to assess children's responses to drawings of
this helpless stance involved failing to provide parent-child separations. The responses of chil-
reassurance and protection to the child, while in dren classified as disorganized at 1 year were
other cases the helpless stance included fear ei- fearful and disorganized. Some children ap-
ther of the child or of the mother's own loss of peared afraid and yet unable to do anything
control in some way. In still other cases, the about it, while others remained silent throughout
mother described feeling that the child was in the task. Still others engaged in catastrophic fan-
control of the relationship, either because of the tasies, indicating that the attachment figure
child's precocious positive capabilities or be- would be hurt or killed.
cause of the child's unmanageability. Moreover, In another study of a middle-income sample of
in George and Solomon's (1996) middle-income 69 kindergartners, children were asked to re-
sample, 62% of parents with unresolved loss or spond to a set of doll play stories about attach-
trauma described their relationships with their ment-related themes (Solomon, George, & de
children in helpless terms. Helpless stances have Jong, 1995). The doll play of controlling children
not yet been related to disorganized attachment was characterized by themes of catastrophe and
behavior in infancy, however. helplessness, including nightmares and violent
fantasies, or by complete inhibition of play.
Compared to the other children in the study,
Disorganized/Controlling Attachment those classified as controlling more often depict-
Patterns and Children's Internal ed themselves as helpless and their caregivers as
Representations of Attachment Figures frightening. Although this helpless stance in
As described earlier, an infant's display of disor- kindergartners has not been linked empirically
ganized attachment behaviors is thought to occur with attachment disorganization in infancy, such
because the infant is faced with an unresolvable helplessness is compatible theoretically with a
paradox. When fear is aroused, the infant experi- collapse of behavioral strategies resulting in an
ences unresolvable conflict with respect to seek- inhibition of attachment behavior. Jacobsen et al.
ing comfort from a frightened or a frightening (1992) also found an association between chil-
caregiver who is also the only haven of safety. dren's controlling behavior with the caregiver
Several studies examining children's depictions and fearful and disorganized responses to a series
of self and caregiver show that many children of cartoon illustrations depicting a parent-child
classified as disorganized in infancy still display separation and coded with Kaplan's system (see
"fright without solution" at age 6 (Main, 1995). Kaplan, 1987).
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 537

Disorganized/Controlling Attachment that children classified as disorganized at age 5-7


Patterns and Cognitive Correlates had lower mathematics scores and lower academ-
ic self-esteem at age 8 than did children classified
Several studies have demonstrated associations as secure or resistant: Moreover, such results were
between disorganized/controlling attachment obtained even after the children's IQs were con-
patterns and the quality of children's cognitive trolled for. As in the findings reported by Jacob-
functioning. In a 10-year longitudinal study as- sen et al. (1994), the relationship between disor-
sessing 85 Icelandic children at ages 7, 9,12,15, ganized/controlling attachment and mathematics
and 17, Jacobsen, Edelstein, and Hofmann (1994) performance disappeared after children's self-
examined the relationship between attachment confidence with schoolwork was controlled for.
security assessed at age 7 and later cognitive
functioning. Both disorganized/controlling chil-
dren and other insecure children differed from Disorganized/Controlling Attachment
secure children on Piagetian tasks assessing con- Patterns and the Development
crete and formal operational reasoning from 7 to of Peer Relationships
15 years of age. These cognitive differences Most studies of the relationship between attach-
by attachment group disappeared when self- ment disorganization and peer interaction have
confidence was controlled. However, on the sub- relied on behavior checklists completed by moth-
set of formal operational measures that assessed ers and/or teachers, as shown in Table 23.2. Only
syllogistic reasoning, disorganized/controlling a few studies have obtained ratings of friendship
children differed significantly from other secure quality from the peers themselves or from
and insecure children from ages 9 to 17, and these trained observers (Cohn, 1990; Jacobvitz &
differences remained when self-confidence, IQ, Hazen, in press; Wartner et al, 1994). In one
and attention problems were included in the cross-sectional study of 40 children, trained ob-
analyses. Disorganized/controlling children servers rated 6-year-olds classified as disorga-
were particularly likely to give contradictory nized/controlling as more incompetent in play
responses on these tasks compared to other at- quality and conflict resolution than secure chil-
tachment groups, and attachment group differ- dren, but the ratings of disorganized/controlling
ences increased with age, with disorganized/ children's friendship quality did not differ from
controlling adolescents as a group never reaching those given to avoidant children (Wartner et al.,
formal operational level. However, these results 1994). Furthermore, compared with secure chil-
need replication due to the small number of dren, disorganized/controlling children were rat-
disorganized/controlling children in the analyses ed as more incompetent overall in three or more
of syllogistic reasoning (« = 6). The syllogistic (but not in two or fewer) of the areas assessed. In
reasoning tasks involved answering hypothetical another cross-sectional study, in which ratings of
questions posed by an experimenter rather than peer liking, sociometric status, and peer nomina-
the direct experimentation involved in the other tions were obtained from the -children them-
Piagetian tasks with objects. Jacobsen et al. selves, disorganized/controlling children did not
(1994) suggest that disorganized children may differ from the other attachment groups (Cohn,
perform poorly because they are particularly vul- 1990). However, contrary to other investigators,
nerable to deregulated thought processes gener- nine children who were disorganized/unclassifi-
ated by their anxieties regarding the reactions of able were eliminated from the study, creating a
others. In another longitudinal study following 35 small and possibly unrepresentative group of
children in Germany over 5 years, Jacobsen, children in the disorganized/controlling category.
Huss, Fendrich, Kruesi, and Ziegenhain (1997) Few studies have examined the. development
examined the relationship between attachment of peer relationships among children classified
and self-regulatory abilities related to cognitive as disorganized in infancy. As part of a larger
functioning. Compared to the other children, longitudinal study conducted by Hazen, Jacob-
those classified as disorganized/controlling had vitz, and Hazen (in press) followed 66 infants
the most difficulty waiting on a standard delay-of- over 3 years who had been classified as secure,
gratification task. avoidant, resistant, or disorganized at 18 months.
Mother-child, father-child, and peer interactions
All of these difficulties with cognitive func- were videotaped when the children were 20, 26,
tioning suggest that disorganized children may be 32, 44, and 56 months old. At each age, mother-
at risk for academic problems. Moss, Rousseau, child and father-child interactions were video-
Parent, St-Laurent, and Saintong (1998) found
538 PARTY. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

taped in play, cleanup, and problem-solving situ- fog, & Jaskir, 1984). In a subsequent study of a
ations. In addition, at all five ages, each child low-income sample that included the disorga-
was videotaped in the day care classroom and in nized classification, Lyons-Ruth, Alpern, and
dyadic play and cleanup with two different peers Repacholi (1993) found that avoidant behavior
identified by the child's teacher as "friends," or was often displayed in combination with disorga-
children with whom the child interacted most. nized behavior in low-income samples and the
Preschoolers classified as secure, avoidant, or disorganized component predicted later aggres-
resistant in infancy behaved in similar ways with sion, whether or not the disorganization included
the two different peers. However, based on case avoidant behaviors. Several more recent longitu-
study data, children classified as disorganized at dinal studies distinguishing between avoidant
18 months were observed to act quite differently and disorganized/controlling attachments have
with one peer as compared to another at both the replicated this association (Goldberg, Gotowiec,
44- and 56-month assessments. For example, one & Simmons, 1995; Hubbs-Tait, Osofsky, Harm,
child spent much of his interaction with one peer & Culp, 1994; Lyons-Ruth et al., 1997; Shaw,
trying to annoy him, shining a flashlight in his Owens, Vondra, Keenan, & Winslow, 1996; see
eyes, using a puppet to try to grab his nose, and also van IJzendoorn et al., in press).
periodically shoving toy animals in his face. With Cross-sectional studies have also found an as-
the other peer, however, the same child did not sociation between children's disorganized/con-
interact at all and progressively withdrew. By the trolling behavior with mother and aggression or
end of the session, he was lying face down and externalizing behaviors. In two studies of clinic-
burying his head under a large pillow. referred preschoolers with oppositional behavior
Jacobvitz and Hazen (in press) have proposed and their normal controls, oppositional pre-
that disorganized children may be more likely schoolers were more often classified as control-
than other children to carry unintegrated models ling toward their mothers (Greenberg et al.,
of the same caregiver into their interactions with 1991; Speltz, Greenberg, & DeKlyen, 1990). In a
peers, such that they draw on different models cross-sectional sample of low-risk preschoolers,
with different peers. Moreover, only after a disor- controlling children were rated higher by parents
ganized child develops a way to interact with the and teachers on both internalizing and external-
caregiver (e.g., using controlling behavior) may izing behavior (Solomon et al., 1995). Finally, in
the child be able to develop a consistent behav- another cross-sectional study of low-risk 5- to 7-
ioral style, even though often not an optimal one, year-olds, children classified as controlling
for interacting with peers. In the Jacobvitz and scored lower on maternal ratings of social com-
Hazen study, children who took longer to estab- petence and higher on a checklist of childhood
lish more consistent ways of interacting with their behavior problems also completed by mothers
caregivers also took longer to sustain peer interac- (Moss et al., 1996). At a 2-year follow-up, teach-
tions, even aggressive or passive interchanges. ers rated 7- to 9-year-olds previously classified
as controlling higher on internalizing and exter-
nalizing behaviors (Moss et al., 1998).
Disorganized/Controlling Attachment
However, in one prospective longitudinal
Patterns and the Development
study, avoidant or resistant attachments at age 1
of Behavior Problems
also predicted aggression in preschool when oth-
Recent studies demonstrate an association be- er risk factors were present at age 2 (Shaw et al.,
tween attachment disorganization and the devel- 1996). In another longitudinal study following
opment of peer aggression or externalizing be- low-income children from birth to adolescence,
haviors. Early studies of low-income samples infants classified as disorganized scored higher
'that used only the three traditional attachment on teacher ratings of dissociative behavior and
classifications reported that avoidant attachment internalizing behavior in first, second, third, and
behavior in infancy predicted aggressive behav- sixth grades, and on both internalizing behavior
ior toward peers in preschool (Troy & Sroufe, and overall psychopathology in adolescence (E.
1987) and elementary school (Renken, Egeland, Carlson, 1998).
Marvinney, Mangelsdorf, & Sroufe, 1989). Stud- Cohn (1990) also assessed teacher-rated be-
ies of middle-income samples did not replicate havior problems in a cross-sectional study using
those findings (Bates, Maslin, & Frankel, 1985; all four attachment categories. She found that
Fagot & Kavanaugh, 1990; Goldberg, Perotta, ambivalent boys were rated as displaying the
Minde, & Corter, 1986; Lewis, Feiring, McGuf- most behavior problems. However, contrary to
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 539

most other investigators, she discarded the data Attachment Disorganization and
from 8 boys judged to be disorganized/unclassi- Altered States of Consciousness
fiable, leaving only 4 boys in the disorganized/ in Young Adulthood
controlling group; Wartner et al. (1994) also cod-
ed all four attachment groupings at age 6 and Main and Hesse (1990, 1992a; see also Hesse &
coded problem behaviors based on half-day ob- Main, in press-a) have proposed that parents who
servations of children's play groups. Problem be- are unresolved with respect to loss or trauma may
haviors included a -low threshold for distress, be vulnerable to entering altered states of con-
tics, social isolation, hostility, and temper out- sciousness, such as trance-like states. Support for
bursts. No differences were found among the at- this hypothesis comes from a study of 140 low-
tachment groupings in such a brief observation. risk subjects who were administered the AAI and
Other recent studies of both low- and middle- the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS), which in-
income samples have reported a relation between cludes items such as "I sometimes 'step outside'
an avoidant attachment pattern and internalizing my usual self and experience an entirely different
rather than externalizing problems (Goldberg et state of mind," and "At times I feel the presence of
al., 1995; Hubbs-Tait et al., 1994; Lyons-Ruth et someone who is not physically there" (Hesse &
al., 1997; Moss et al., 1996). One study that dis- van IJzendoorn, in press-a). Unresolved subjects
tinguished between children cross-classified as showed significantly elevated scores on the TAS
disorganized and avoidant from those classified when compared with subjects classified into the
as avoidant found only elevated internalizing remaining adult attachment categories (au-
problems among avoidant children, and elevated tonomous, dismissing, preoccupied, and cannot
externalizing and internalizing problems among classify). See also MuscettX Dazzi, De Coro,
children classified as both disorganized and Ortu, and Speranza (in press) for a discussion of
avoidant (Lyons-Ruth et al., 1997). the relationship between unresolved attachment
On the basis of these data, we might expect status and dissociative symptoms.
disorganized attachment relationships in infancy Main and Hesse (1990) have proposed that
to predict social-relational disorders of child- parents who temporarily enter altered states of
hood characterized by aggressive control tactics consciousness will be more likely to engage in
and by mixed externalizing and internalizing inexplicably frightened and/or frightening behav-
symptoms. However, Lyons-Ruth et al. (1997) ior with their children. Frightened and/or fright-
found that only 25% of disorganized infants in a ening parental behavior is theorized to arouse
high-risk sample were rated specifically as high- contradictory responses in an infant, who simul-
ly externalizing at age 7, whereas 50% were rat- taneously experiences tendencies to approach
ed over the clinical cutoff score for generally and to flee from the caregiver, resulting in attach-
maladaptive behavior at age 7. Similarly, follow- ment disorganization in the child. Because hyp-
ing children prospectively from 18 to 54 months notic states can be induced by using "confusion
of age, Jacobvitz and Hazen (in press) found that techniques" (e.g., very rapidly urging.a subject to
some preschoolers classified as disorganized/dis- engage in contradictory movements that cannot
oriented in infancy later displayed hostile/ag- be carried out simultaneously), Liotti (1992; see
gressive behaviors toward peers. However, other also Liotti, in press-a, in press-b) has proposed
children appeared disconnected—for example, that disorganization and disorientation in infancy
moving backward when peers approached, cau- will increase a child's vulnerability to altered
tiously sidestepping along the wall when ap- states or dissociative disorders (e.g., fugues,
proaching peers, or burying their heads under trance states, dissociative identity disorder, expe-
pillows in the corner of the room during a peer riences of depersonalization and derealization,
interaction task. These findings suggest that the and ideas of possession).
forms of later maladaptation associated with ear- Empirical support for Liotti's hypothesis
ly disorganization may not be entirely captured comes from a prospective longitudinal study fol-
by conventional checklists for internalizing and lowing 129 children from birth to age 172/2 (E.
externalizing symptoms. Instead, more subtle Carlson, 1998). Infants classified as disorga-
and more organizationally oriented assessments nized at 12 and 18 months of age (vs. the other
may be needed of the odd, intrusive, controlling, infants) more often displayed symptoms of dis-
or incompetent social behaviors that may be cor- sociation in elementary school and high school,
relates of disorganized/controlling attachment as reported by teachers on the Teacher Report
patterns. Form of the Child Behavior Checklist, and by the
540 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

adolescents themselves on the Schedule of Af- age, or at least a month's separation of mother
fective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School- and child before age 54 months; n = 10); (2)
Age Children (K-SADS). Moreover, attachment young adults classified as disorganized during
disorganization in infancy showed a significant infancy who had faced such trauma (•« = 35); and
association with scores on the Dissociative Expe- (3) other young adults not previously classified
rience Scale at age 17V2 (r = .36; Carlson, 1998). as disorganized (n = 83). This group was not di-
Indirect evidence for Liotti *s hypothesis comes vided by trauma history. A significant elevation
from studies examining the relationship between in DES scores was found only among 19-year-
dissociative disorders in adults and their parents* olds classified as disorganized during infancy
experiences of important losses near the time of who had experienced trauma, providing further
their own births. Liotti, Intreccialagli, and Cecere support for Liotti's theory. It is also notable that
(1991) reasoned that if a parent's unresolved re- 78% of those classified as disorganized during
sponse to loss predicts attachment disorganiza- infancy had experienced later trauma. This high
tion in the infant, then loss near the birth of an rate suggests that caregiving environments asso-
infant should increase the likelihood of a disorga- ciated with infant disorganization also place the
nized attachment to the mother. Attachment disor- infant at risk for further exposure to trauma or
ganization, in turn, should increase a person's loss.
propensity to enter an altered state of conscious-
ness. Liotti et al. (1991) asked 46 patients diag-
nosed as suffering from dissociative disorders and DISORGANIZED/CONTROLLING
119 patients with other psychiatric diagnoses ATTACHMENTS AND
whether their mothers had suffered a loss through RELATIONSHIP VIOLENCE,
death of a parent, sibling, spouse or another child TRAUMA, AND MALTREATMENT
during the 2 years before or after their births. Ap-
proximately 62% of the dissociative patients and Prospective Cohorts and Deviant
only 13% of the controls were the offspring of Groups: Complementary Research
mothers who had lost an important person near Strategies in the Study of Attachment
the time of the patient's birth. Given the emerging web of relations linking un-
Hesse and van IJzendoorn (in press-b) report- resolved trauma, dissociation, and maladaptive
ed similar results in a study of two low-risk sam- social behavior to disorganized forms of attach-
ples («'s = 138 and 308). Young adults were ment in infancy and childhood, investigators are
asked whether their own parents had lost another beginning to consider the implications, of these
child or another loved one within 2 years preced- data for delineating the pathways to adult rela-
ing or following their births. Reports of loss of a tional violence and maltreatment. The body of
child or another loved one within two years of work just reviewed, tracing pathways prospec-
their births were related to elevated levels of ab- tively from prebirth parental states of mind to at-
sorption in an altered state of reality, as assessed tachment status in infancy to outcomes in child-
on Tellegen's Absorption Scale. hood and adolescence, exemplifies one critical
Not all infants classified as disorganized show strategy for testing tenets of attachment theory.
dissociative symptoms later in life. Based on the This approach asks how many disorganized in-
prevailing view of dissociation. as a defense fants have .maladaptive outcomes. However, the
against trauma (see Putnam; 1997), Liotti (1992, most seriously traumatized children, such as
in press-a) has further proposed that infants clas- those in violent or criminal families or those sub-
sified as disorganized will be particularly vulner- jected to multiple foster placements, will be un-
able to dissociative disorders if traumatic cir- derrepresented in most longitudinal studies.
cumstances continue or appear in later life. Identifying children who are disorganized in
Empirical support for this idea comes from a lon- infancy and then studying the various outcomes
gitudinal study following 168 children from birth of such early patterns over time needs to be com-
to age 19. Ogawa, Sroufe, Weinfield, Carlson, plemented by a second-research strategy—a fol-
and Egeland (1997) compared scores on the DES low-back strategy. This strategy involves asking
for three groups: (1) young adults classified as how many children or adults with the most de-
disorganized during infancy who had not faced viant relational pathologies have disorganized/
trauma (defined as near-catastrophic events [in- controlling attachment histories. Attachment the-
cluding death of a family member] at any age, ory makes strong predictions that the subset of
life-threatening illnesses before 54 months of children and adults with seriously pathological
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 541

relational behavior should have histories of dis- An Attachment Perspective


organized attachment patterns in infancy or on Relationship Violence
childhood, as well as further exacerbating fac-
tors. Identifying the forms of relational patholo- Attachment theory has a fundamental contribu-
gy consistent with- these predictions, as well as tion to make in placing serious relational disor-
the forms of later pathology not related to attach- ders, such as violent or maltreating behavior,
ment history, requires supplementing normative into a broader conceptual context—a context
and prospective samples with samples already that includes concepts of internal working mod-
displaying serious deviance. The public policy els, mental segregation, and controlling relation-
importance of attachment theory and research al behavior. An attachment perspective on vio-
will derive in part from the degree to which the lence and abuse needs to integrate four basic
most serious later relational disorders are cap- aspects of attachment theory: (1) the presence of
tured within the larger groups who are classified an attachment behavioral control system; (2) the
as disorganized/controlling in early life. There- role of anger in that control system; (3) the op-
fore, relations among attachment, violence, and eration of both intersubjective and intrapsychic
maltreatment will continue to be pivotal in test- mechanisms in channeling the experience and
ing tenets of attachment theory and in stimulat- expression of attachment-related affects, as well
ing new ideas. as the fear, anger, and unregulated physiological
Adequate exploration of the relations between arousal resulting from frustration of attachment
disorganized forms of attachment and disordered goals; and (4) the intergenerational transmission
forms of adult or child relational behavior will of relational patterns. These basic postulates of
necessarily continue to involve a recursive pro- an attachment model are reviewed in detail in
cess of observation and instrument development. other chapters of the present volume.
For example, the identification and description of Of particular relevance to the study of vio-
disorganized forms of infant attachment behavior lence, Bowlby (1973) has pointed out that anger
emerged partially from theoretically incongruous is a natural response to frustration and serves as
findings. Namely, in studies of maltreated infants an important communicative signal to the attach-
using the initial three-category attachment classi- ment partner that something is awry. If the at-
fication system, sizable proportions of maltreated tachment relationship is in jeopardy or the at-
infants were classified as secure (Crittenden, tachment figure is unresponsive, anger is one
1985; Lyons-Ruth et al., 1987). Those incongru- way of increasing the intensity of the communi-
ous findings became part of a recursive process. cation to the attachment figure. Thus anger may
First, methods of assessing attachment in low-risk occur in the service of the attachment relation-
populations were extended to studying high-risk ship. To the extent that the increased emotional
populations. Then assessment instruments were signaling works well enough to elicit the part-
revised to include the atypical adaptations en- ner's ongoing participation in protection and
countered in high-risk environments and reap- arousal regulation, the attachment,relationship
plied in high-risk studies, with a much better fit to will maintain a consistent and predictable, albeit
the data (e.g., Crittenden, 1985; Lyons-Ruth et al., possibly insecure, organization.
1987, 1990; Main & Solomon, 1986, 1990). This Should the child's increased anger and distress
process began with research on attachment in in- fail to elicit an adequate parental response to the
fancy and has not progressed to the same point in child's fearful arousal, however, increasingly fran-
the study of age groups beyond infancy. Over the tic and futile anger and distress may mount, and
next decade, more differentiated subgroupings strategic behavioral organization may break down
and coding procedures are likely to emerge in as- or take increasingly deviant forms as the commu-
sessments of older children, adolescents, and nication fails to achieve its goal. Bowlby (1980)
adults as a critical mass-of research develops on described these processes as part of the "protest"
high-risk populations in these age groups. The and "despair" phases of the child's response to
continual development of well-validated attach- loss of the caregiver in the absence of an adequate
ment assessment instruments for the study of at- substitute. When an infant is confronted not with
tachment organization in groups at very high risk a loss but with a fearful infant-caregiver relation-
for attachment disorganization is one current ship, attempts to organize a consistent strategy ap-
frontier of attachment research (see also Dozier, pear to disintegrate into the conflict;.behaviors
Stovall, & Albus, Chapter 22, and Hesse, Chapter represented in disorganized attachment behavior.
19,.this volume). Such conflict behaviors may appear in the.form of
542 PARTY. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

anger or distress and alternate with fear and symptoms (Kazdin, Moser, Colbus, & Bell;
avoidance, or may appear in disguised forms (e.g., 1985).
inexplicable, aim-inhibited action sequences). The school functioning of maltreated children'
Therefore, in the absence of adequate caregiving is also compromised, compared to that of con-
regulation of fearful arousal, various intersubjec- trols. Sexually abused children are a noted ex-
tive, intrapsychic, and physiological compensato- ception to this pattern, however, as elaborated
ry mechanisms are set in motion, resulting in mal- further below (Eckenrode, Laird, & Doris, 1993).
adaptive, contradictory, or controlling behavioral There are suggestions that representational mod-
and mental organizations. Violence in mtimate re- els of self and other are more negatively toned
lationships can be one outcome of such a devia- and less coherent among maltreated children
tion in the organization of attachment relation- (Lynch & Cicchetti, 1991; Schneider-Rosen &
ships, and one in which great intensity of positive Cicchetti, 1991). In relation to the family envk
longing, anger, and fear may be combined with a ronment, maltreated children experience distor-
lack of felt security, lapses in attention, dysfluent tions in ongoing parent-child interaction, in'ad-
communication, and unregulated arousal. dition to specific incidents of abuse (Crittenden,
The relations between attachment and adult 1981; Lyons-Ruth, Connell, & Zoll, 1989). As
maltreatment^ physical aggression, or violence noted earlier, Carlson et al. (1989) found that
need to be explored from at least two perspec- 82% of maltreated infants displayed disorga-
tives—that of the recipient of aggression or mal- nized patterns of attachment to their caregivers,
treatment, and that of the aggressor or perpetra- compared to only 17% of socioeconomically
tor of maltreatment. We first consider the effects similar controls. Lyons-Ruth et al. (1990) ob-
of maltreatment on children from an attachment tained figures of 55% among maltreated infants
perspective and then consider the still fragmen- and 34% among low-income controls in a con-
tary evidence concerning working models of at- text where all maltreating families had received
tachment among adults who are either recipients clinical home-visiting services. Thus there is am-
or perpetrators of maltreatment, violence, or ple evidence that maltreated children experience
abuse. more negative family relationships and exhibit
poorer developmental outcomes on multiple di-
mensions, as well as having early disorganized
Child Maltreatment and Attachment attachment relationships. As parents themselves,
maltreated children are at elevated risk for mal-
Child Outcomes and Family Correlates treating their own children (Egeland, Jacobvitz,
of Child Maltreatment & Papatola, 1987; Kaufman & Zigler, 1987).
A large literature deals with aspects of the adap-
tation of maltreated children outside the context
of attachment research (see reviews by Cicchetti, Children's Physiological Adaptations
Lynch, Schonk, & Todd-Manly, 1992; Wolfe, to Maltreating Environments
1985). A complete review of this literature goes In addition to the earlier-reviewed elevated corti-'
beyond the scope of the present chapter. The lit- sol responses to stress among disorganized in-
erature is briefly summarized here, however, in fants, evidence is accruing that the most ex-
order to examine how an attachment perspective tremely impaired early attachment relationships
deepens and extends our understanding of the are associated with even more pervasive changes
developmental pathways associated with child in the organization and functioning of the hypoL
and adult aggression. thalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. M. Carl-
Not surprisingly, the literature on child abuse son et al. (1995) found that normal diurnal-varia-
and neglect has established that many negative tion in Cortisol secretion was altered* among
developmental outcomes are associated with Romanian orphans, with peak values occurring
maltreatment. Peer relations are disturbed, with in late morning or early afternoon, rather than* at
maltreated children exhibiting more withdrawal rising. M. Carlson and Earls (1997) also'fourid
from and avoidance of their peers. Physically that degree of Cortisol elevation among Roman-
abused children, in particular^ display more ag- ian orphans was related to decrements in-infant
gression toward other children (George & Main, developmental scores on the Bayley Scales.
1979; Mueller & Silverman, 1989). Maltreated Boyce, Champoux, Suomi, and Gunnar (1996)
children also display unusual or aggressive be- found similar alterations in diurnal cycles of Cor-
havior in response to the distress of peers (Main tisol secretion among rhesus monkeys raised^ in
& George, 1985) and have more depressive social isolation.
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 543

Blunted Cortisol responses to stress may be an- mechanism in the emergence of many of the dis-
other outcome of particularly deviant early at- turbances associated with 'maltreatment. There-
tachment experiences. Kraemer and Clarke fore, we predict that maltreatment should also be
(1996) reported that compared to mother-reared associated with the developmental sequelae of
monkeys, rhesus monkeys randomly assigned to disorganized attachment patterns—sequelae that
a peer-rearing condition showed subsequent also appear in nonmaltreated groups, including
blunting of physiological responses to stress, disorganized/controlling attachment patterns,
while still showing behavioral signs of distur- verbal deficits; aggressive and other maladaptive
bance (see also Clarke, T993). Similarly, Hart, behavior problems, altered physiological re-
Gunnar, and Cicchetti (1995) found that school- sponses to stress, and contradictory and uninte-
age children who were maltreated were less like- grated mental representations of relationships.
ly to show Cortisol elevations after conflicts with The literature on developmental outcomes
peers than were nonmaltreated children. Hart et among maltreated children just reviewed is con-
al. (1995) speculated that when a child is ex- sistent with these predictions, although the pre-
posed to chronic stress in abusive family con- dictions have not yet been thoroughly tested us-
texts, the HPA axis may down-regulate to avoid ing attachment assessments per se.
chronic hyperactivation. The low Cortisol levels Once the mental and behavioral components
observed among Vietnam veterans exposed to of disorganized attachment patterns are consid-
combat provide convergent support among hu- ered, the question emerges of whether children or
man subjects for the hypothesis that unbuffered adults who have been recipients of sexual or
stress leads to down-regulation of the HPA axis physical maltreatment will display patterns of
(Boscarino, 1996; Yehuda, Southwick, Giller, behavior not shared with other children with dis-
Ma, & Mason, 1990). organized attachment histories. There are indica-
These more extreme alterations in functioning tions in the literature that the type of abuse does
of the HPA axis have not yet been studied in rela- affect the way social behavior and symptomatol-
tion to disorganized/controlling attachment pat- ogy are organized later in childhood or adult-
terns per se. More work is now needed exploring hood. For example, in several studies, hospital-
the interrelations among very deviant early at- ized sexually abused girls were more likely to
tachment experiences, disorganized attachment present dissociative and posttraumatic symp-
behaviors, cognitive development, and chronical- toms, whereas physically abused hospitalized
ly elevated or blunted physiological responses to adolescents were more likely to present de-
stress. pressive symptoms and conduct symptoms
(Deblinger, McLeer, Atkins, Ralphe, & Foa,
1989; McLeer, Callaghan, Henry, & Wallen,
Disorganized/Disoriented Attachment 1994; Pelcovitz et al., 1994). Consistent with this
Patterns and the Maltreated Child picture, physically abused school children were
The attachment literature has made several im- more likely to perform poorly academically, to
portant contributions to the conceptualization be sent to the principal more often, and to be sus-
and further development of the literature on mal- pended from school more often, while sexually
treatment. The first contribution has been an at- abused girls exhibited none of these school prob-
tachment organizational perspective on the un- lems (Eckenrode et al., 1993). In adulthood,
derstanding of these multiple negative correlates. mothers who had been physically abused in
The postulate that early infant-caregiver rela- childhood were more likely to be intrusive and
tional patterns are represented and carried for- covertly hostile in interactions with their infants,
ward as prototypes for how to interact with oth- while mothers who had been sexually abused but
ers in close relationships shifts the conceptual not physically abused were more likely to be
framework from a narrower focus on abuse inci- withdrawn and uninvolved with their infants but
dents per se to a more organizational view of the were not more likely to be hostile (Lyons-Ruth &
integrated behavioral, affective, physiological, Block, 1996). Overall, it appears that sexually
and representational patterns that are being trans- abused women are less likely than physically
mitted from parent to child (Crittenden & abused women to act aggressively toward others,
Ainsworth, 1989). more likely to conform to social expectations,
and more likely to use dissociative defenses to
A more recent and specific contribution of at- maintain areas of appropriate'social functioning.
tachment research is the finding that the disorga-
nization of the attachment relationship, rather Lyons-Ruth and Block (1996) have speculated
than simply its insecurity, may be a central that the different stances of physically abused
544 PARTY CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

and sexually abused mothers toward their infants sponses on the AAI to an adult's earlier history of
are related to the abused women's differential trauma. These studies have implicated both unre-
overt identification with the aggressor or the vic- solved (U) states of mind and rarer states of
tim position in the earlier abusive relationship. mind, such as "preoccupied/overwhelmed by
Identification with a sexual aggressor may be trauma" (E3) and "cannot classify" (CC), as se-
psychologically unlikely for women, whereas quelae of maltreatment. These two rarer AAI
identification with a physically abusive parent subgroups are described briefly before we pre-
may be more psychologically and behaviorally sent the empirical findings.
feasible. Consistent with an attachment frame- Both the preoccupied/overwhelmed by trauma
work, however, difficulties in intimate relation- (E3) and cannot classify (CC) AAI subgroups oc-
ships, vinintegrated mental representations, nega- cur rarely in low-risk samples but are seen with
tive self-concepts, and problems with affect increasing frequency among clinical populations
regulation are expected to be core features of a (see Hesse, this volume). Neither AAI subgroup
disorganized attachment relationship. These fea- has a corresponding infant attachment subgroup,
tures should be common to identification with and neither subgroup has been validated against
either the aggressor or the victim position in an infant data. As described by Main and Goldwyn
abusive relationship. The abuse literature con- (1998), both of these AAI subgroups may also be
firms that both groups of maltreated adolescents given a primary classification of unresolved with
display these core features. respect to loss or trauma if they also meet the cod-
In summary, qualitative differences between ing criteria for lack of resolution.
children or adults who have suffered physical or The preoccupied/overwhelmed by trauma (E3)
sexual abuse and children or adults whose disor- subgroup is one of three subtypes of a preoccu-
ganized attachment relationships stem from oth- pied state of mind identified in the Main and
er kinds of traumatic attachment-related experi- Goldwyn (1998) AAI coding manual. This adult
ences have yet to be demonstrated. Although subtype has no corresponding infant resistant
differences between these groups may be antici- subtype as its expected intergenerational coun-
pated, these distinctions have not yet been made terpart, as do the other two preoccupied sub-
and remain at the frontier of current work. types. Because of its rarity, this subtype also re-
ceives a relatively brief description in the coding
manual and has no corresponding detailed cod-
States of Mind Regarding Attachment ing scale for identifying and rating overwhelmed
among Violent, Maltreating, manifestations in the transcript, as do the other
or Maltreated Adults two preoccupied subtypes. Interviews are classi-
fied as preoccupied/overwhelmed by trauma if
Unresolved States of Mind material related to traumatic experiences repeatr
among Maltreating or Maltreated Adults edly intrudes into the interview context. Adults
From an attachment perspective, there is a strong whose interviews are classified as E3 are often
presumption that a maltreating caregiver is a grouped with adults in the other two preoccupied
frightened or frightening caregiver whose current subgroups for purposes of data analysis, and, the-
mental state is characterized by a lack of resolu- oretically, their infants are expected to display re-
tion of loss or trauma, resulting in contradictory sistant attachment patterns rather than disorga-
and unintegrated mental contents. However, em- nized attachment patterns.
pirical data addressing this prediction are rare. Adult interviews are placed in the cannot clas-
Few studies have examined maltreating adults' sify (CC) category when the discourse manifests
own internal models of attachment. Some data a collapse of strategy at a global level rather than
are available relating these adults' own histories only during discussions of loss or trauma (Hesse/
of trauma or abuse to attachment-related' out- 1996; Minde & Hesse, 1996). This global break-
comes in adulthood. Lyons-Ruth and colleagues down in strategy is characterized by the display
have demonstrated a link between the severity of of two contrasting states of mind over the course
trauma or abuse in a mother's own childhood and of the interview that should be incompatible (dis-
her child's display of disorganized rather than or- missing and preoccupied), or by a general inabil-
ganized forms of insecure behavior, as well as ity to implement an organized stance (as seen in
her own display of withdrawn or hostile behavior transcripts with low coherence scores, as well as
toward the child (Lyons-Ruth & Block, 1996; low scores for both dismissing and preoccupied
Lyons-Ruth, Zoll, Connell, & Grunebaum, strategies).
1989). Several other studies have related re- Patrick, Hobson, Castle, Howard, and Maugh-
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 545

an (1994) found that 9 of 12 clinic patients ex- ed states of mind among adults exposed to child-
hibiting borderline personality disorder were hood trauma.
classified as both preoccupied/overwhelmed by
trauma (E3) and unresolved with respect to
childhood trauma.-The remaining three patients Disorganized Attachment Patterns
with borderline personality disorder were classi- and Violence between Intimate Partners
fied preoccupied/overwhelmed by trauma only. We have noted above the overlap in correlates of
In contrast, clinic patients with dysthymic disor- disorganized attachment patterns and correlates
der were significantly less likely to show these of childhood abuse. West and George (1998)
unresolved/overwhelmed characteristics on inter- have pointed to a similar overlap in the correlates
view, despite a similarly high rate of childhood associated with disorganized attachment rela-
trauma. Comparable results, including the high tionships and those associated with marital vio-
rates of childhood trauma among psychiatric pa- lence. They have proposed that violence in inti-
tients compared to controls, were reported by mate relationships is tooted in disorganized
Fonagy et al. (1996). Neither of these studies attachment relationships and have outlined a
coded the cannot classify category. Stalker and model of marital relational violence based on the
Davies (1995), studying 40 women in psychiatric literature on disorganized/controlling attachment
treatment who were sexually abused in child- patterns. West and George (1998) have further
hood, also found that 7 of 8 abused women with pointed out that marital, violence is associated
borderline personality organization (88%) were with a constellation of adult characteristics that
classified as unresolved on the AAI, and that 5 of are also related to disorganized/unresolved at-
these 7 (71%) were also classified in the preoc- tachment relationships. This constellation in-
cupied group (the authors did not provide a cludes a history of trauma or abuse, borderline
breakdown of the frequencies of the three sub- personality organization, intense abandonment
types in the preoccupied group). Among all sex- anxiety, controlling behavior toward the partner,
ually abused women, 60% were classified as un- and unintegrated or dissociated mental contents
resolved, and 88% of those were also classified associated with trauma.
in the preoccupied group. Stalker and Davies
There are still relatively few data directly ex-
also reported that 37.5% of both resolved and
ploring the attachment behaviors or states of
unresolved women also met criteria for the can-
mind of those who display marital violence,
not classify category, although this category was
however. Owen and Cox (1997) found that ele-
not considered in their data analyses.
vated rates of marital conflict observed between
Thus childhood trauma that eventuates in adult marital partners in a laboratory assessment were
borderline personality disorder is clearly associ- associated with elevated rates of disorganized at-
ated with a concomitant unresolved and over- tachment classifications on the part of their in-
whelmed state of mind with respect to attach- fants. However, Shaw et al. (1996) did not repli-
ment. Sexual abuse survivors in psychiatric cate this finding. Steiner, Zeanah, Stuber, Ash,
treatment are also likely to show these character- and Angell (1994) found that mothers who re-
istics, whether or not they also exhibit borderline ported higher levels of partner violence were
personality disorder, but psychiatric patients more likely to have infants with disorganized at-
with dysthymic disorder are not, even in the con- tachments. In the study by Lyons-Ruth and Block
text of a history of trauma. These data support (1996), infants of mothers who were not abused
the prediction from attachment theory that adults but had witnessed violence between others in
with severe problems in maintaining close rela- childhood were as likely to display a disorga-
tionships—a characteristic associated with bor- nized attachment strategy as were infants whose
derline personality disorder—will exhibit unre- mothers had directly experienced physical or
solved states of mind with respect to attachment. sexual abuse. Bearman and Ogawa (1993) re-
However, the data also indicate that indices of ported, in particular, that mothers who displayed
lack of resolution of trauma by themselves are indicators of lack of resolution in relation to vio-
not sufficient to capture these states of mind. lence witnessed in childhood were more likely to
These studies have been conducted among clini- have infants classified as disorganized.
cal samples rather than general-population sam- The few existing studies of marital violence
ples, however, so they represent the more im- and AAI responses suggest that in addition to un-
paired end of the spectrum of trauma-exposed resolved states of mind, rarer adult states of mind
adults. General-population samples are needed to such as preoccupied/overwhelmed by trauma
fully explore the distribution of attachment-relat- (E3) and cannot classify (CC) appear with in-
546 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

creased frequency among both perpetrators and quencies for unresolved and cannot classify sub-
victims of violent behavior. Holtzworth-Monroe, groups separately, however, nor did they report'
Stuart, and Hutchinson (1997) found that male how many unresolved subjects received a subclas-
batterers were significantly more often classified sification of E3 or CC.
cannot classify on the AAI than were nonbatter- The data as a whole suggest that violence-
ers, and Sullivan-Hanson (1990), studying bat- prone and disturbed men may be particularly
tered women, found that women in shelters had a likely to display the more globally contradictory
high prevalence of E3 classifications on the AAI states of mind associated with the cannot classify
(the unresolved classification was not coded in category, while abused and distressed women
that study). may be more likely to display both unresolved
Two other studies have examined states of and overwhelmed (E3) states of mind with re-
mind with respect to attachment among mentally spect to attachment. As discussed in more depth
disturbed criminal offenders. Levinson and Fon- in the next section of this chapter, however, we
agy (1998) collected AAIs from 22 mentally dis- would not expect this division to be a rigid one.
turbed criminal offenders. Both offenders and Rather, we would expect the abused-abuser in-
nonclinical controls were less likely than psychi- ternal working model of attachment to include
atric controls to be classified as unresolved (36% representations of both positions in this dyadic
and 0% vs. 82%). Eighty-two percent of offend- relational pattern, with the potential for either
ers, but only 36% of psychiatric controls and 4% overwhelmed or contradictory states of mind to
of normals, had been abused in childhood, and be expressed, depending on context, tempera-
current anger toward attachment figures was de- ment, gender role, and other factors.
scribed as "intense" among offenders. Both of- Data are particularly needed tying these rarer
fenders and psychiatric controls were more likely groups (including the CC and E3 classifications)
to be classified preoccupied than nonclinical to infant and child attachment data, since the
controls (45% and 64% vs. 14%). Levinson and parental risk factors associated with these groups
Fonagy did not report whether most of the people overlap with the risk factors for infant disorgani-
classified as preoccupied were in the E3 (over- zation. On the basis of their sample of sexually
whelmed by trauma) subgroup. Because the abused women, Stalker and Davies (1995) sug-
group of criminal offenders would be strongly gested that the AAI categories need continued
predicted to show unresolved states of mind with expansion "to avoid placing individuals with rel-
respect to attachment, based on their histories of atively mild information-processing distortions
i-' abuse, current anger, and violent behavior toward in the same category as those with serious distor-
others, these findings run counter to theory. tions. Delineation of the subgroups of the cate-
Although Levinson and Fonagy found that of- gory currently labeled cannot classify (CC) may
fenders were less likely to display unresolved be the means by which such expansion can be
states of mind than psychiatric controls, a study achieved" (p. 238). We would also add the E3
by van IJzendoorn, Feldbrugge, et al. (in press) of subgroup to the group of AAI categories for
40 mentally disturbed criminal offenders did not which more infant data and coding description
replicate that finding—a difference that may be are needed. This emerging evidence of the preva-
accounted for by the latter authors' inclusion of lence of E3 and CC states of mind among violent
cannot classify codes with unresolved codes in re- or abused adults suggests that there may be a
porting the data, van IJzendoorn, Feldbrugge, et wider spectrum of parental states of mind that
al. (in press) found that autonomous AAI classifi- would be expected to predict infant disorganiza-
cations were almost absent (5%) among offend- tion beyond the unresolved group alone. Further
ers, while the combined unresolved/cannot classi- research on the childhood antecedents and inter-
fy groups constituted 53% of the sample. This generational outcomes associated with these
distribution is a significant departure from the adult states of mind will be crucial to under-
distribution of AAI classifications reported in standing the interface between attachment rela-
low-risk middle-income samples, where about tionships and psychopathology. Further explo-
14% are classified as unresolved (van IJzen- ration of the attachment-related correlates both
doorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, in of childhood abuse and of adult violence and
press). However, criminal offenders were not psychiatric disorder constitutes an active area of
more likely than other clinical groups to display current attachment research, and new findings
unresolved or cannot classify states of mind, van are likely to emerge (see Dozier et al., Chapter
IJzendoorn and coworkers did not present the fre- 22, and Hesse, Chapter 19, this volume).
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 547

AN INTEGRATIVE CONCEPTUAL Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, and Atwood (in press)


FRAMEWORK: LOSS, TRAUMA, have pointed out that placing the process of fear
AND THE RELATIONAL DIATHESIS and its modulation into a relational context
points to a way out of these impasses. If attach-
The literature reviewed in this chapter .on disor- ment disorganization or lack of resolution is a
ganized/controlling attachment patterns, unre- function both of the intensity of fear-producing
solved loss or trauma, and frightened or frighten- experience and of the adequacy of the attachment
ing caregiving behavior raises a number of relationship to help resolve the fearful affect,
issues. A recurring theme regarding the disorga- then potential answers emerge. At one end of the
nized attachment category across the lifespan is a spectrum, particularly abrupt or shocking trau-
theme of contradictory and unintegrated behav- ma, or trauma that also impairs the caregiver,
ioral strategies and mental contents. Contradic- may be sufficient in itself to disorganize the at-
tions are observed within such an individual's be- tachment strategy of a child or adult who has had
havioral strategies and mental contents and otherwise adequate attachment relationships. At
contradictions are also observed at the level of the other end of-the spectrum, even the norma-
the composition of the disorganized group, tive distress-producing experiences of infancy
which includes such sharply contrasting sub- may remain unbuffered and lead to disorganized
groups as punitive versus caregiving and fright- attachment responses in the presence of a fright-
ened versus frightening. Here we point to three ened or frightening caregiver who experiences
of the most pressing questions raised by this lit- unresolved fear in relation to the infant's attach-
erature and elaborate a framework to account for ment behaviors. If the early attachment relation-
such apparent discontinuities. ship remains disorganized/controlling through-
One question that merits more attention in the out childhood, further loss or trauma may not be
literature is why some experiences of loss, necessary for intergenerational transmission of
parental death, or trauma become resolved and disorganization to occur.
others do not. A second related question that has In this relational-diathesis model, organized
not been addressed is how processes of intergen- dismissing or preoccupied attachment relation-
erational transmission of disorganized forms of ships should provide sufficient protection and
attachment work. In the model to date, caregivers caretaking in response to uncomplicated experi-
are viewed as developing an unresolved state of ences of loss to allow integrated mental function-
mind in relation to a specific experience of loss ing. Only if the loss or traumatic experience falls
or trauma—an experience usually occurring later well outside the norm should we expect to see
than the first few years of life and often much the kinds of behavioral and mental lapses associ-
later. In contrast, infants are viewed as develop- ated with disorganization. Similarly, only if the
ing a disorganized attachment strategy through degree of communication and protection avail-
exposure to their caregivers' unintegrated fear. able from the attachment figure falls outside the
This leads to the conundrum that if a disorga- spectrum associated with organized attachment
nized infant grows up without further loss or strategies should we expect to see disorganized
trauma, then he or she will not be codable as un- attachment responses even in the absence of loss
resolved on the AAI. Do we assume, then, that or trauma. This implies that there should be an
all disorganized infants grow out of disorganized additive or interactive effect between the pres-
forms of attachment behavior unless further un- ence and characteristics of the trauma and the
resolved loss or trauma occurs? Or is a caregiv- quality of the childhood attachment relation-
er's frightened or frightening behavior sufficient ship in producing disorganized/controlling/unre-
in itself to lead to intergenerational transmission solved states. This stress-relational-diathesis
of disorganized attachment behavior? A third model is consistent with the primate literature
question posed by the current data is how to ex- demonstrating that peer-nurtured monkeys that
plain the apparently very different behavioral and have experienced early maternal deprivation (the
mental organizations that emerge out of a com- relational diathesis) display adequate social be-
mon fear-based dynamic. Why are some parents havior until stressed, at which time the early rela-
frightened and some frightening, some disorga- tional diathesis becomes manifest again in atypi-
nized infants approaching and some avoiding, cal social behaviors. By itself, however, the stress
and some controlling children battling with their is not sufficient to produce atypical behaviors in
parents while others are caregiving toward their peers that have not been maternally deprived
parents? (Suomi, 1991).
548 PART V. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

The concept of a relational diathesis, or vul- ships contribute to the intergenerational trans-
nerability to disorganization in the face of trau- mission of disorganization. We predict, however,
ma, is foreshadowed in Bowlby's (1980) discus- that trauma will both occur more frequently and
sion of the conditions affecting the course of be more difficult to resolve in the context of an
mourning. He states that "adults whose mourn- already disorganized caregiving system, suggest-
ing takes a pathological course are likely before ing both direct and indirect effects of early disor-
their bereavement to have been prone to make af- ganized caregiving on later adult outcomes. The
fectional relationships of certain special, albeit data of Ogawa et al. (1997) provide some support
contrasting, kinds" (p. 202). He lists the three for these predictions in relation to adult dissocia-
classes of affectional relationships as those "suf- tive symptoms, as noted earlier.
fused with overt or covert ambivalence," those in Finally, the sharply contrasting behavioral
which "there is a strong disposition to engage in styles that make up the disorganized category,
compulsive caregiving," and those in which such as punitive or caregiving control in child-
"there are strenuous attempts to claim emotional hood, can be viewed as stemming from the nature
self-sufficiency and independence of all affec- of the relational diathesis itself. If a caregiver has
tional ties" (p. 202). A similar point was made by not experienced comfort and soothing in relation
Freud (1917/1957), who described persons prone to his or her own early fear-evoking experiences,
to pathological "mourning as those who have ex- the infant's pain and fear will evoke unresolved
perienced marked but consciously unacceptable fearful affects in the caregiver, including memo-
ambivalence toward the lost person. In short, the ries of his or her own helplessness as a child in
more insecure the underlying attachment, the obtaining comfort. To prevent reexperiencing his
more difficult the mental resolution of a loss be- or her own unresolved fearful affects, the care-
comes. giver must restrict attention and responsiveness
Ainsworth and Eichberg (1991) provided em- to the infant's fear and distress. As a result, the
pirical grounding for this concept in their finding caregiver's fluid responsiveness to the infant's at-
that mothers who were classified as unresolved tachment-related cues will inevitably be restrict-
in relation to past loss or trauma were more like- ed (Fraiberg, Adelson, & Shapiro, 1975; Main,
ly to receive secondary classifications of preoc- 1995, 1996; Main et al., 1985; Strage & Main,
cupied or dismissing rather than autonomous on 1985). The more pervasive these restrictions, the
the AAI (for meta-analytic data, see van IJzen- more the caregiver's mental states take prece-
doorn, Schuengel, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, in dence over the infant's attachment-related com-
press). Although in all the above-cited literature munications and initiatives. To the extent that the
the relevant processes were placed in the adult caregiver's unresolved fearful experiences are at-
personalities or states of mind of the bereaved, it tended to at the expense of attention to the child's
is in keeping with Bowlby's broader theory to de- current states, the interaction between the two
I rive these states of mind in part from patterns of partners will become less balanced and less mu-
caregiving experienced in childhood. tually regulated to meet the needs of both part-
In conceptualizing the childhood relational ners.
processes most likely to interfere with the resolu- Such unbalanced relational processes provide
tion of loss or trauma, however, we should distin- a conceptually powerful construct that can order
li guish between those caregiving behaviors that al- many additional aspects of the findings regard-
low the maintenance of organized but insecure ing disorganized attachment patterns. Both
attachment patterns, such as the preoccupied or Bowlby (1980) and his forebears in psychoana-
dismissing classifications, and those caregiving lytic scholarship emphasized that mental repre-
behaviors more likely to be associated with dis- sentations of relationships are inherently dyadic
organized or controlling patterns of child attach- (see also Sroufe & Fleeson, 1986). What is repre-
ment. At present, from a parent's AAI data alone, sented is not only the individual's way of partici-
a distinction cannot be made between the par- pating in the relationship—for example, as the
|S ent's carrying forward the experience of a disor- coercively controlled child—but the entire
ganized or controlling attachment relationship dyadic relational pattern of controlled child >and
from childhood and the parent's further experi- controlling parent. The more the parent must re-
ence of a loss or trauma that fails to be resolved. spond to his or her own need to regulate fearful
Longitudinal data from infancy or childhood will arousal rather than to the child's states, the more
be needed to assess separately how the preva- skewed these relational polarities become, -and
lence and severity of childhood trauma and the the more discontinuous and self-contradictory
extent of insecurity in early attachment relation- are the internalized models that accommodate
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 549

both relational possibilities (e.g., "I should ac- in Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, and Atwood (in
cept external control and take no initiative" vs. "I press).
should control the other by overriding the other Lyons-Ruth and Atwood (1998) have devel-
person's initiative"). oped initial conventions for coding hostile/con-
In a relational model, the unintegrated duali- trolling or helpless states of mind on the AAI,
ties that recur in the disorganized group both at and Hesse (1996; Minde & Hesse, 1996) has de-
the individual level (unintegrated mental con- scribed the more interview-wide characteristics
tents) and at the level of the disorganized group that constitute the CC designation. More needs to
as a whole (frightened or frightening behavior, be done to conceptualize and code the extremes
coercive or caregiving behavior, chaotic or inhib- of relational imbalance in childhood, both as re-
ited play) can be understood as partially rooted in vealed by the AAI and as observed in parent-
a disorganized child's history of participation in child transactions, to better differentiate hostile/
such unbalanced relationships. Inherent in a controlling or helpless states of mind from the
model of unbalanced relationships is an asym- less extreme parental rejection or' overinvolve-
metry of power in which one partner's (attach- ment consistent with the child's maintenance of
ment-related) goals or initiatives are elaborated an organized attachment strategy.
at the expense of the other's. By definition, then,
one partner is more helpless in the relationship
and the other more controlling, whether the con- CONCLUSIONS
trol is exerted through active aggression or
through the more covert mechanisms of with- In summary, Main and colleagues' (Main et al.,
drawal, guilt induction, or self-preoccupation 1985; Main & Solomon, 1986, 1990) discovery
(see Fraiberg et al., 1975, and Zahn-Waxler & of a disorganized attachment pattern in infancy
Kochanska, 1990, for examples). Thus, a help- has opened up new ground in understanding the
less versus hostile/controlling dyadic dichotomy interface between development and psycho-
in relationship roles should he especially evident pathology. Theoretically, Main and coworkers
among individuals in disorganized attachment (Hesse & Main, in press; Main & Hesse, 1990;
relationships, and this may partially account for Main & Morgan, 1996) have pointed to "fear
the bifurcation into punitive or caregiving without solution" as the core dilemma facing in-
stances that occurs among disorganized infants fants who display inexplicable and conflicting at-
during the preschool period. Because such inter- tachment behaviors. These authors have pro-
nal models include both contradictory relation- posed that frightened or frightening maternal
ship roles, however, an individual with such a behaviors, such as trance-like or dissociative
model may mentally or behaviorally activate ei- states, sudden unusual vocal patterns, or sudden
ther role sequentially or simultaneously, depend- looming into the infant's face, are related to the
ing on contextual and temperamental factors; mother's earlier unresolved trauma. When cues
this may lead to highly contradictory behavioral in the current environment activate-these uninte-
or mental sequences. grated trauma-related representations, fear-relat-
Lyons7Ruth and Atwood (1998) present pre- ed maternal behaviors occur out of context and
liminary evidence that these helpless or hos- are therefore inexplicable to the infant. Such in-
tile/controlling stances can be identified in the explicable behaviors are likely to alarm the in-
AAI, independently of indices of unresolved fant. Since infants rely on their attachment fig-
states of mind. These authors have speculated ures to protect them from harm should dangers
that these two AAI patterns are associated with arise, fear stemming from their own caregivers
the overwhelmed by trauma (E3) and cannot places infants in an unresolvable paradox. Such
classify (CC) subgroups that often seem to clus- an attachment figure is "at once the source of
ter with the unresolved classification. In this for- and the solution to [an infant's] alarm" (Main &
mulation, the E3 and CC subgroups represent Hesse, 1990, p. 163). At the same time that the
overall states of mind that correspond to an infant experiences a tendency to approach the
unbalanced helpless versus hostile/controlling caregiver for comfort, the infant experiences a
internal working model, which may manifest competing tendency to flee.
itself in either helpless or hostile interpersonal Competing and irreconcilable tendencies to
stances (or both in alternation) and should move toward and away from the caregiver, who is
be tied to childhood experiences of participation both the source of alarm and the haven of safety,
in very unbalanced or controlling relationships. intensifies the infant's fear, resulting in a col-
A fuller exposition of this model is available lapse of behavioral and attentional strategies.
550 PARTY CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

Contradictory behavioral tendencies inhibit the at home. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 371-387.
infant from employing the strategies used by oth- Barnett, D., Ganiban, J., & Cicchetti, D. (in press). Maltreat-
ment, emotional reactivity, and the development of Type
er infants who are alarmed—namely, seeking D attachments from 12 to 24 months of age. In J. Vondra
comfort and support from the caregiver (i.e., evi- & D. Barnett (Eds.), Atypical patterns of infant attach-
dence of security), shifting attention away from ment: Theory, research, and current directions. Mono-
the caregiver (i.e., avoidance), or oscillating be- graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
tween seeking and resisting comfort (i.e., am- Bates, J. E., Maslin, C. A., & Frankel, K. A. (1985). Attach-
ment security, mother-child interaction, and temperament
bivalent or resistant behavior). As elaborated ear- as predictors of behavior problem ratings at age three
lier, attachment disorganization in infancy years. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points
forecasts controlling behavior with caregivers, of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the So-
t ciety for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial
aggressive and fearful peer relationships, and in-
No. 209), 167-193.
ternalizing and externalizing problems in
preschool, kindergarten, and elementary school, Beckwith, L., & Rodning, C. (1991, April). Stability and cor-
relates of attachment classificationsfrom13 to 36 months
as well as dissociative symptoms and elevated in a sample of preterm infants. In R. S. Marvin & J. Cas-
psychopathology during adolescence. sidy (Chairs), Attachment during the preschool years: Ex-
amination of a new measure. Symposium conducted at the
The parent's fearful affect, which is thought to biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child De-
underlie frightened and frightening caregiving velopment, Seattle, WA.
behavior, is related theoretically to the parent's Berman, D., & Ogawa, J. R. (1993, April). Seeing is believ-
lack of resolution of fear-arousing experiences of ing: Observed trauma as an alternate route to unresolve
loss or trauma. "Lack of resolution" refers here status in the Adult Attachment Interview. Poster presented
at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in
to the failure to integrate fully the reality of the Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
loss or traumatic experience and to rework men- Boscarino, J. (1996). Post-traumatic stress disorder, exposure
tal representations of attachment experiences. To to combat, and lower plasma Cortisol among Vietnam vet-
the extent that multiple, conflicting, and uninte- erans: Findings and clinical implications. Journal of Con-
grated models of past traumatic experiences are sulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 191-201.
maintained in segregated areas of consciousness, Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. I. Attachment'.
New York: Basic Books.
a caregiver is likely to display conflicting and un- Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation:
integrated behaviors with his or her own child. Anxiety and anger. New York; Basic Books.
As a result, affects or behaviors related to earlier Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New
trauma may be reactivated unpredictably in York: Basic Books.
parent-child interactions in ways that are not Boyce, T. W., Champoux, M., Suomi, S. I., & Gunnar, M. R.
(1995). Salivary Cortisol in nursery-reared rhesus mon-
monitored by an overarching, integrated, and co- keys: Reactivity to peer interactions and altered Circadian
herent parental state of mind. These basic theo- activity. Developmental Psychobiology, 28, 12.
retical postulates are gaining substantial support Carlson, E. A. (1998). A prospective longitudinal study of
from recent empirical studies. In turn, new ques- disorganized/disoriented attachment. Child Development,
69, 1970-1979.
tions are emerging that have to do with exploring Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K.
the limiting conditions of these propositions, and (1989). Disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships
with determining how processes related to disor- in maltreated infants. Developmental .Psychology, 25,
ganization of attachment strategies interface with 525-531.
the three organized classifications on the one Carlson, M., Dragomir, C, Earls, F., Farrell, M., Macovei, O.,
Nystrom, P., & Sparling, J. (1995). Effects of social depri-
hand and with clearly defined psychopathology vation on Cortisolregulationin institutionalized Romanian
on the other. In this direction lie some of the fu- infants. Society forNeurosciences Abstracts, 218, 12.
ture developments of attachment theory and re- Carlson, M., & Earls, F. (1997). Psychological and neuroen-
search. docrinological sequelae of early social deprivation in in-
*s stitutionalized children in Romania. Annals of the New
York Academy ofSciences, 807, 419-428.
Cassidy, J., Marvin, R. S., & The MacArthur Working Group
REFERENCES on Attachment. (1991). Attachment organization in
preschool children: Coding guidelines (3rd ed.). Unpub-
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. lished manuscript.
(1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cicchetti, D., & Barnett, D. (1991). Attachment organization
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Eichberg, C. G. (1991). Effects on in- in maltreated preschoolers. Development and Psychopath-
fant-mother attachment of mother's unresolved loss of an ology, 5,397-431.
attachmentfigureor other traumatic experience. In C. M. Cicchetti, D., Lynch, M., Schonk, S., & Todd-Manly, J.
Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Marris (Eds.), Attach- (1992). An organizational perspective on peer relations in
ment across the life cycle (pp. 161-183). London: Rout- maltreated children. In R. D. Parke & G. M. Ladd (Eds.),
ledge. Family-peer relationships: Modes of linkage (pp. 345-
Alpern, L., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (1993). Preschool children at 383). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
social risk: Chronicity and timing of maternal depressive Clarke, A. S. (1993). Social rearing effects on HPA axis ac-
symptoms and child behavior problems at school and tivity over early development and in response to stress in
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 551

young rhesus monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology, George, C, & Main, M. (1979). Social interaction of young
26, 433-447. abused children: Approach, avoidance, and aggression.
Cohn, D. (1990). Child-mother attachment in six-year-olds Child Development, 50, 306-318.
and social competence in school. Child Development, 61,George, C, & Solomon, J. (1996). Representational models
152-162. of relationships: Links' between caregiving and attach-
Crittenden, P. M. (1981) Abusing, neglecting, problematic, ment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17. 198-216.
and adequate dyads: Differentiating by patterns of interac- Goldberg, S., Gotowiec, A., & Simmons, R. J. (1995). In-
tion. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 27. 201-208. fant-mother attachment and behavior problems in healthy
Crittenden, P. M. (1985). Maltreated infants: Vulnerability and chronically ill preschoolers. Development and Psy-
andresilience.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-chopathology, 7, 267-282.
try, 26, 85-96. Goldberg, S., Perotta, M., Minde, K., & Corter, C. (1986).
Crittenden, P. M. (1992). Quality of attachment in the Maternal behavior and attachment in low birthweight
preschool years. Development and Psychopathology, 4,twins and singletons. Child Development, 57, 34—46.
209-241. Greenberg, M. T., Speltz, M. L., DeKlyen, M., & Endnga,
Crittenden, P. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Child mal- M. C. (1991). Attachment security in preschoolers with
treatment and attachment theory. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carl- and without externalizing behavior problems: A replica-
son (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on tion. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 413-430.
the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Spangler, G., Suess, G., &
(pp. 432-463). New York: Cambridge University Press. Unzner, L. (1985). Maternal sensitivity and newborn's
Crockenberg, S. (1981). Infant irritability, mother respon- orientation responses as related to quality of attachment in
siveness, and social support influences on the security of northern Germany. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.),
infant attachment. Child Development, 52, 857-865. Growing points of attachment theory andresearch.Mono-
Deblinger, E., McLeer, S., Atkins, M., Ralphe, D., & Foa, E. graphs of the Societyfor Research in Child Development,
(1989). Post-traumatic stress in sexually abused, physical- 50(1-2 Serial No. 209), 233-256.
ly abused, and nonabused children. Child Abuse and Ne-Harm, D. M., Castino, R. J., Jarosinski, J., & Britton, H.
glect, 13, 403-408. (1991, April). Relating mother-toddler negotiation
DeMulder, E. K., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1991). Attachment patterns to infant attachment and maternal depression
with affectively ill and well mothers: Concurrent behav- with an adolescent mother sample. In J. Osofsky & L.
ioral correlates. Development and Psychopathology, 3,Hubbs-Tait (Chairs), Consequences of adolescent parent-
227-242. ing: Predicting behavior problems in toddlers and
Easterbrooks, A. M., Lyons-Ruth, K., Biesecker, G., & Carp- preschoolers. Symposium conducted at the biennial meet-
er, A. (1996). Infancy predictors of emotional availability ing of the Society for Research in Child Development,
in middle childhood: The role of attachment and maternal Seattle, WA.
depression. In J. Robinson (Chair), Emotional availability Hart, J., Gunnar, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1995) Salivary Cortisol
in mother-child dyads: Predictors across development in maltreated children: Evidence of relations between neu-
and risk status. Symposium conducted at the International roendocrine activity and social competence. Development
Conference on Infant Studies, Providence, RI. and Psychopathology, 7, 11-12.
Eckenrode, J., Laird, M., & Doris, J. (1993). School perfor- Hertsgaard, L., Gunnar, M., Erickson, M. F., & Nachmias,
mance and disciplinary problems among abused and M. (1995). Adrenocortical response to the strange situa-
neglected children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 53-62.tion in infants with disorganized/disoriented attachment
Egeland, B., Jacobvitz, D., & Papatola, K. (1987). Intergen- relationships. Child Development. 66, 1100-1106.
erational continuity of parental abuse. In J. Lancaster & R. Hesse, E. (1996). Discourse, memory, and the Adult Attach-
Gelles (Eds.), Biosocial aspects of child abuse (pp. ment Interview: A note with emphasis on the emerging
255-278). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. cannot classify category. Infant Mental Health Journal,
Fagot, B. I., & Kavanagh, K. (1990). The prediction of anti- 17, 4-11.
social behavior from avoidant attachment classifications. Hesse, E., & Main, M. (in press-a). Frightened behavior in
Child Development, 61, 864-873. traumatized but non-maltreating parents: Previously unex-
Fonagy, P., Leigh, T., Steele, M., Steele, H„ Kennedy, G., aminedriskfactor for offspring. Psychoanalytic Inquiry.
Mattoon, M., Target, M., & Gerber, A. (1996). The rela-Hesse, E., & Main, M. (in press-b). Disorganization in infant
tion of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and and adult attachment: Description, correlates, and impli-
response to psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and cations for developmental psychopathology. Journal of
Clinical Psychology. 64. 22-31. the American Psychanalytic Association.
Fraiberg, S., Adelson, E., & Shapiro, V. (1975). Ghosts in theHesse, E., & van IJzendoorn, M. (in press-a). Propensities to-
nursery. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psy- wards absorption are related to lapses in the monitoring of
chiatry, 74,387-421. reasoning or discourse during the Adult Attachment Inter-
Freud, S. (1957). Mourning and melancholia. In J. Strachey view; A preliminary investigation. Attachment and Hu-
(Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the completeman Development.
psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, Hesse, pp. E., & van IJzendoorn, M. (in press-b). Parental loss of
237-260). London: Hogarth Press. (Original work pub- close family members and propensities toward absorption
lished 1917) in offspring. Developmental Science.
George, C, Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1984) Adult Attachment Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal behavior: A synthesis of etholo-
Interview (1st ed.). Unpublished manuscript, University gy and comparative psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Mc-
of California at Berkeley. Graw-Hill.
George, C, Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Adult Attach- Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Stuart, G. L., & Hutchinson, G.
ment Interview (2nd ed.). Unpublished manuscript, Uni- (1997). Violent vs. nonviolent husbands: Differences in
versity of California at Berkeley. attachment patterns, dependency, and jealousy. Journal of
George, C, Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult Attach- Family Psychology, 11, 314-331.
ment Interview (3rd ed.). Unpublished manuscript. Uni- Hubbs-Tait, L., Osofsky, J., Hann, D., & Culp, A. (1994).
versity of California at Berkeley. Predicting behavior problems and social competence in
552 PARTY. CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

children of adolescent mothers. Family Relations, 43, [Unresolved mourning in mothers and development of
439-446. dissociative disorders in children: A case-control study].
Jacobsen, T„ Edelstein, W., & Hofmann, V. (1994). A longi- Rivista di Psichiatria, 26, 283-291.
tudinal study of the relation between representations of at- Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Patterns of relatedness in
tachment in childhood and cognitive functioning in child- maltreated and nonmaltreated children: Connections
hood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30, among multiple representational models. Development
112-124. and Psychopathology, 3. 207-226.
Jacobsen, T., Huss, M., Fendrich, M., Kruesi, M. J. P., & Lyons-Ruth, K., Alpem, L., & Repacholi, B. (1993). Disor-
Ziegenhain, U. (1997). Children's ability to delay gratifi- ganized infant attachment classification and maternal psy-
cation: Longitudinalrelationsto mother-child attachment. chosocial problems as predictors of hostile-aggressive be-
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 158, 411-426. havior in the preschool classroom. Child Development,
Jacobsen, T., Ziegenhain, U., Muller, B., Rottmann, U., Hof- 64, 572-585.
mann, V., & Edelstein, W. (1992, September). PredictingLyons-Ruth, K., & Atwood, G. (1998) Elaborating the con-
stability of mother-child attachment patterns in day-care cept of unresolved mental states: A relational diathesis
childrenfrominfancy to age 6. Poster presented at the model of hostile-helpless states of mind. Unpublished
Fifth World Congress of Infant Psychiatry and Allied Dis- manuscript.
ciplines, Chicago. Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Atwood, G. (in press). A
Jacobvitz, D. (1998, March). Frightening caregiving: Links relational diathesis model of hostile-helpless states of
with mothers' loss and trauma. Paper presented at the bi-mind: Expressions in mother-infant interaction. In J.
ennial meeting of the Southwestern Society for Research Solomon & C. George (Eds.), Attachment disorganisa-
in Human Development, Galveston, TX. tion. New York: Guilford Press.
Jacobvitz, D., & Hazen, N. (in press). Developmental path- Lyons-Ruth, EC, & Block, D. (1996). The disturbed caregiv-
ways from infant disorganization to childhood peer rela- ing system: Relations among childhood trauma, maternal
tionships. In J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.), Attachment caregiving, and infant affect and attachment. Infant Men-
disorganization. New York: Guilford Press. tal Health Journal, 17, 257-275.
Jacobvitz, D., Hazen, N., & Riggs, S. (1997, April). Disorga- Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Parsons, E. (in press). Ma-
nized mental processes in mothers, frightening/frightened ternal disrupted affective communication, maternal fright-
caregiving, and disoriented/disorganized behavior in in- ened or frightening behavior, and disorganized infant at-
fancy. In D. Jacobvitz (Chair), Caregiving correlates and tachment strategies. In J. Vondra & D. Barnett (Eds.),
longitudinal outcomes of disorganized attachments inAtypical in- patterns of infant attachment: Theory, research,
fants. Symposium conducted at the biennial meeting of and current directions. Monographs of the Society for Re-
the Society for Research in Child Development, Washing- search in Child Development.
ton, DC. Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D., & Zoll, D. (1989). Patterns of
Kaplan, N. (1987). Individual differences in 6-year-olds'maternal behavior among infants atriskfor abuse: Rela-
thoughts about separation: Predictedfromattachmenttions to with infant attachment behavior and infant develop-
mother at age 1. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni- ment at 12 months of age. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson
versity of California at Berkeley. (Eds.), Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the
Kaufman, J., & Zigler, E. (1987). Do abused children be- causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp.
come abusive parents? American Journal of Orthopsychi-464-494). New York: Cambridge University Press.
atry, 57, 186-192. Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D., Grunebaum, H., & Botein, S.
Kazdin, A. E., Moser, J., Colbus, D., & Bell, R. (1985). De- (1990). Infants at social risk: Maternal depression and
pressive symptoms among physically abused and psychi- family support services as mediators of infant develop-
atrically disturbed children. Journal of Abnormal Psy- ment and security of attachment. Child Development, 61,
chology, 94, 298-307. 85-98.
Kraemer, G. W., & Clarke, A. S. (1996). Social attachment, Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). In-
brain function, and aggression. Annals of the New York fants at socialrisk:Relations among infant maltreatment,
Academy ofSciences, 794, 121-135. maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. Devel-
Levinson, A., & Fonagy, P. (1998). Criminality and attach- opmental Psychology, 23, 223-232.
ment: The relationship between interpersonal awareness Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, A., & Cibelli, C. (1997). In-
and offending in a prison population. Manuscript submit-fant attachment strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal
ted for publication. depressive symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and ex-
Lewis, M., Feiring, C, McGuffog, C, & Jaskir, J. (1984). ternalizing problems at age 7. Developmental Psychology,
Predicting psychopathology in six-year-oldsfromearly 33, 681-692.
social relations. Child Development, 55, 123-136. Lyons-Ruth, K., Repacholi, B., McLeod, S-, & Silva, E.
Liotti, G. (1992). Disorganized/disoriented attachment in the (1991). Disorganized attachment behavior in infancy:
etiology of the dissociative disorders. Dissociation, 4, Short-term stability, maternal and infant correlates, and
196-204. risk-related subtypes. Development and Psychopathology,
Liotti, G. (in press-a). Understanding the dissociative 3. 377-396.
processes: The contribution of attachment theory. Psycho- Lyons-Ruth, K., Zoll, D., Connell, D., & Grunebaum, H.
analytic Inquiry. (1989). Family deviance and family disruption in child-
Liotti, G. (in press-b). Disorganization of attachment as a hood: Associations with maternal behavior and infant
model for understanding dissociative psychopathology. In maltreatment during thefirsttwo years of life. Develop-
J. Solomon & C. George (Eds.), Attachment disorganiza- ment and Psychopathology, 1, 219-236.
tion. New York: Guilford Press. Main, M. (1973). Exploration, play, and cognitive function-
Liotti, G., Intreccialagli, B., & Cecere, F. (1991). Esperienza ing as related to child-mother attachment. Unpublished
di lutto nella madre e facilitazione dello sviluppo di dis- doctoral dissertation, Johns Hopkins University.
turbi dissociativi nella prole: Unto studio caso-controllo. Main, M. (1979). Scale for disordered/disoriented infant be-
CHAPTER 23. Attachment Disorganization 553

havior in response to the Main and Weston clown ses- Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 121-160). Chica-
sions. Unpublished manuscript, University of California go: University of Chicago Press.
at Berkeley. Main, M., & Weston, D. R. (1981). The quality of the tod-
Main, M. (1995). Recent studies in attachment: Overview, dler's relationship to mother and to father: Related to con-
with selected implications for clinical work. In S. Gold- flict behavior and the readiness to establish new relation-
berg, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (Eds.), Attachment theory: So- ships. Child Development. 52, 932-940.
cial, developmental, and clinical perspectives McLeer, (pp. S., Callaghan, M., Henry, D., & Wallen, J. (1994).
407-474). Analytic Press: Hillsdale, NJ. Psychiatric disorders in sexually abused children. Journal
Main, M. (1996). Introduction to the special section on at- of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi~
tachment and psychopathology: 2. Overview of the field atry, 33. 313-319.
of attachment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- Minde, K., & Hesse, E. (1996). The role of the Adult At-
chology, 64. 237-243. tachment Interview in parent-infant psychotherapy: A
Main, M., & Cassidy, J. (1988). Categories of response to re- case presentation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17, 115—
union with the parent at age 6: Predictedfrominfant at- 126.
tachment classifications and stable over a 1-month peri- Moss, E., Parent, S., Gosselin, C, Rousseau, D., & St-Lau-
od. Developmental Psychology. 24, 415-426. rent, D. (1996). Attachment and teacher-reported behavior
Main, M., & George, C. (1985). Response of abused and dis- problems during the preschool and early school-age peri-
advantaged toddlers to distress in agemates: A study in the od. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 511-525.
day care setting. Developmental Psychology. 21, 407- Moss, E., Rousseau, D., Parent, S., St-Laurent, D., & Sain-
412. tong, J. (1998). Correlates of attachment at school age:
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1998). Adult attachment scoring Maternal reported stress, mother-child interaction, and be-
and classification system. Version 6.3. Unpublished man- havior problems. Child Development, 69, 1390-1405.
uscript, University of California at Berkeley. Mueller, E., & Silverman, N. (1989). Peer relations in mal-
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents* unresolved traumatic treated children. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.),
experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes
status: Isfrightenedand/or frightening parental behavior and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp.
the linking mechanism? In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, 529-578). New York: Cambridge University Press.
& E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool Muscetta, S., Dazzi, N., De Coro, A., Ortu, F., & Speranza,
years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 161-182). A. M. (in press). States of mind[with respect to attachment
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. and change in a psychotherapeutic relationship: A study
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1992a). Disorganized/disoriented of the coherence of transcript in short-term psychotherapy
infant behavior in the strange situation, lapses in the mon- with an adolescent. Psychoanalytic Inquiry.
itoring ofreasoningand discourse during the parent's National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
Adult Attachment Interview, and dissociative states. (NICHD) Early Child Care Research Network. (1997).
In M. Ammaniti & D. Stem (Eds.), Attachment and The effects of infant child care on infant-mother attach-
psycho-analysis (pp. 80-140). Rome: Guis, Laterza & ment security: Results of the NICHD study of early child
Figl. care. Child Development, 68, 860-879.
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1992b). Frightening frightened, dis- O'Connor, M. J ., Sigman, M., & Brill, N. (1987). Disorgani-
sociated, or disorganized behavior on the part of a par- zation of attachment inrelationto maternal alcohol con-
ent: A coding system for parent-infant interactions (4th sumption. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
ed.). Unpublished manuscript, University of California at 55, 831-836.
Berkeley. Ogawa, J. R., Sroufe, L. A., Weinfield, N. S., Carlson, E. A.,
Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1995). Frightening,frightened,dis- & Egeland, B. (1997). Development and the fragmented
sociated, or disorganized behavior on the part of the par- self: Longitudinal study of dissociative symptomatology
ent: A coding system for parent-infant interactions (5th in a nonclinical sample. Development and Psychopatholo-
ed.). Unpublished manuscript. University of California at gy, 9, 855-879.
Berkeley. Owen, M. T., & Cox, M. J. (1997). Marital conflict and the
Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in in- development of infant-parent attachment relationships.
fancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 152-164.
representation. In I. Bremerton & E. Waters (Eds.), Grow- Patrick, M., Hobson, R. P., Castle, P., Howard, R., & Maugh-
ing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs an, B. (1994). Personality disorder and mental representa-
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, tion of early social experience. Development and Psy-
Serial No. 209), 66-104. chopathology, 6, 375-388.
Main, M., & Morgan, H. (1996). Disorganization and disori- Pelcovitz, D., Kaplan, S., Goldenberg, B., Mandel, F.,
entation in infant strange situation behavior: Phenotypic Lehane, J., & Guarrera, J. (1994). Post-traumatic stress
resemblance to dissociative states? In L. K. Michelson & disorder in physically abused adolescents. Journal of the
W. J. Ray (Eds.), Handbook of dissociation: Theoretical, American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
empirical, and clinical perspectives (pp. 107-138). New33, 305-312.
York: Plenum Press. Putnam, F. W. (1997). Dissociation in children and adoles-
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of a new, inse- cents: A developmental perspective. New York: Guilford
cure-disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern. In T. B. Press.
Brazelton & M.W. Yogman (Eds.), Affective development Radke-Yarrow, M., Curnmings, E. M., Kuczynski, L., &
in infancy (pp. 95-124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of attachment in two- and
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying three-year-olds in normal families and families with
infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth parental depression. Child Development, 56, 884-893.
strange situation. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. Renken, B., Egeland, B., Marvinney, D., Mangelsdorf, S., &
M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years:Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Early childhood antecedents of ag-
554 PARTY.' CLINICAL APPLICATIONS

gression and passive-withdrawal in early elementary Sullivan-Hanson, J. M. (1990). The early attachment and
school. Journal of Personality, 5, 257-281. current affectional bonds of battered women: Implication
Schneider-Rosen, K., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Early self- for the impact of spouse abuse on children. Unpublished
knowledge and emotional development: Visual self- doctoral dissertation, University of Virginia.
recognition and affective reactions to mirror self-image inSuomi, S. (1991). Early stress and adult emotional reactivity
maltreated and nonmaltreated toddlers. Developmental in rhesus monkeys. In The childhood environment and
Psychology. 27, 481-488. adult disease. (Ciba Foundation Symposium 156, pp.
Schuengel, C, van IJzendoorn, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, 171-188). Chichester, England: Wiley.
M., & Biom, M. (1997, April). Frightening, frightened Teti, D. M., Gelfand, D. M., Messinger, D. S., & Isabella, R.
and/or dissociated behavior, unresolved loss, and infant (1995). Maternal depression and the quality of early at-
disorganization. In D. Jacobvitz (Chair), Caregiving cor- tachment: An examination of infants, preschoolers, and
relates and longitudinal outcomes of disorganized attach- their mothers. Developmental Psychology, 34, 361-376.
ments in infants. Symposium conducted at the biennial Thalhuber, K., Jacobvitz, D., & Hazen, N. L. (1998, March).
meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop- Effects of mothers' past traumatic experiences on moth-
ment, Washington, DC. er-infant interactions. Paper presented at the biennial
Schuengel, C, van IJzendoorn, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, meeting of the Southwestern Society for Research in Hu-
M., & Blom, M. (in press) Frightening, frightened and/or man Development, Galveston, TX
dissociated behavior, unresolved loss, and infant disorga- Troy, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (1987). Victimization among
nization. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology.preschoolers: Role of attachment relationship history.
Shaw, D. S., Owens, E. B., Vondra, J. I., Keenan, K., & Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
Winslow, E. F".~(l"996). Earlyriskfactors and pathways in cent Psychiatry, 26, 166-172.
the development of early disruptive behavior problems. True, M., Pasani, L., Ryan, R., & Oumar, F. (1998). Mater-
Development and Psychopathology, 8, 679-699. nal behaviors related to disorganized infant attachment in
Solomon, J., George, C, & de Jong, A. (1995). Children clas- West Africa. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
sified as controlling at age six: Evidence of disorganized Western Psychological Association, Albuquerque, NM.
representational strategies and aggression at home and at van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment representa-
school. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 447-463.tions, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A
Spangler, G., Fremmer-Bombik, E., & Grossmann, K. meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult At-
(1996). Social and individual determinants of infant at- tachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117,
tachment security and disorganization. Development and 387-403.
Psychopathology, 17, 127-139. van JJzendoom, M-, Feldbrugge, J., Derks, F., de Ruiter, C,
Spangler, G., & Grossmann, K. E. (1993). Biobehavioral or- Verhagen, M., Philipse, M., van der Staak, C, & Riksen-
ganization in securely and insecurely attached infants. Walraven, J, (in press). Attachmentrepresentationsof
Child Development, 64. 1439-1450. personality disordered criminal offenders. American Jour-
Speltz, M. L., Greenberg, M. T., & DeKlyen, M. (1990). At- nal of Orthopsychiatry.
tachment in preschoolers with disruptive behavior: A van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C, & Bakermans-Kra-
comparison of clinic-referred and nonproblem children. nenburg, M.J. (in press). Disorganized attachment in early
Development and Psychopathology, 2, 31-46. childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants,
Spieker, S. J., & Booth, C. (1988). Maternal antecedents of and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology.
attachment quality. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Vondra, J., Hommerding, K. D., & Shaw, D. S. (in press).
Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 300-323). Hills-Stability and change in infant attachment in a low-income
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. sample. In J. Vondra & D. Barnett (Eds.), Atypical pat-
Sroufe, L. A., & Fleeson, J. (1986). Attachment and the con- terns of infant attachment: Theory, research, and current
struction of relationships. In W. Hartup & Z. Rubin directions. Monographs of the Society for Research in
(Eds.), Relationships and development (pp. 51-72). Hills-Child Development.
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. Warmer, U. G., Grossmann, K., Fremmer-Bombik, E., &
Stalker, C, & Davies, F. (1995). Attachment organization Suess, G. (1994). Attachment patterns at age six in south
and adaptation in sexually abused women. Canadian Germany: Predictability from infancy and implications for
Journal ofPsychiatry, 40, 234-240. preschool behavior. Child Development, 65, 1014-1027.
Steele, H., Steele, M., & Fonagy, P. (1996). Associations West, M.„ & George, C. (1998). Abuse and violence in inti-
among attachment classifications of mothers, fathers, and mate adult relationships: New perspectives from attach-
their infants. Child Development, 67, 541-555. ment theory. Unpublished manuscript.
Steiner, H., Zeanah, C. H., Stuber, M., Ash, P., & Angell, R.Wolfe, D. A. (1985). Child abusive parents: An empirical re-
(1994). The hidden faces of trauma: An update on child view and analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 461-482.
psychiatric traumatology. Scientific Proceedings of theYehuda, R., Southwick, S. M., Nussbaum, G., Wahlby, V.,
Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Child and Giller, E. L., & Mason, J. W. (1990). Low urinary Cortisol
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31. excretion in patients with post-traumtic stress disorder.
Strage, A., & Main, M. (1985, April). Attachment and par- Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease. 178, 366-369.
ent-child discourse patterns. In M. Main (Chair), Attach- Zahn-Waxler, C, & Kochanska, G. (1990). The origins of
ment: A move to the level of representation. Paper pre-guilt. In R. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on MoJ
sented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research tivation: Vol. 36. Socioemotional development (pp.
in Child Development, Toronto. 183-258). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen