Sie sind auf Seite 1von 8

Mood States Associated With Transitory Changes in Asthma Symptoms and Peak

Expiratory Flow
GLENN AFFLECK, PHD, ANDREA APTER, MD, HOWARD TENNEN, PHD, SUSAN REISINE, PHD, ERIK BARROWS, BA,
ALICE WILLARD, RN, BSN, JENNIFER UNGER, BA, AND RICHARD ZUWALLACK, MD

Objective: This study examined the within-person relations between transitory changes in mood, asthma symptoms,
and peak expiratory flow rate (PEFR). Methods: Thrice-daily for 21 consecutive days, 48 adults with moderate to
severe asthma entered information in palm-top computers about their mood and asthma symptoms. A multidimen-
sional model of mood, ie, the mood circumplex, informed the assessment of mood arousal and mood pleasantness.
At each observation, participants also recorded their PEFR with peak flow meters that stored blinded data. Albuterol
doses were also monitored electronically. Before and after the 21-day study, spirometric measures of airways
obstruction were taken under controlled conditions. Results: Random effects regression models revealed a signif-
icant, but weak, within-person relation between symptoms and PEFR. Changes in mood vectors with an arousal
component were significantly related to PEFR changes, whereas changes in mood vectors with a pleasantness
component tracked changes in asthma symptom reports, even after adjustment for contemporaneous PEFR and after
controlling for time of day and albuterol dosing. Comparison of spirometric assessments with unsupervised PEFR
suggested that part of the relation between mood arousal and PEFR may be attributable to the “effort-dependence”
of peak flow self-monitoring. Conclusions: Different dimensions of mood were associated with transitory changes
in asthma symptoms and PEFR. This may be one reason why individuals with asthma misperceive the severity of
their symptoms in relation to underlying airways obstruction. Key words: asthma, mood, peak flow, symptoms, diaries.

and asthma symptom reports (5– 8). Consequently, ad-


vances in the clinical management of asthma may
PEFR ⫽ peak expiratory flow rate, ELI ⫽ electronic
come from identifying processes that regulate both
interviewer
bronchoconstriction and the perception of asthma
symptoms and that could help explain the weak con-
cordance between these phenomena.
INTRODUCTION The present study highlights the role that transi-
tory changes in mood play in the fluctuation of
Despite steady progress in the understanding and
airways obstruction and symptom reports in adults
treatment of asthma, the morbidity and mortality of
with asthma. Many investigations have documented
this disease remain disturbingly high (1). One reason
significant associations of emotional states such as
for the suboptimal control of asthma complications
panic, fear, anxiety, arousal, and fatigue with asthma
may lie in patients’ perception of their airways ob-
severity, whether measured by symptom reports or
struction. Those who underestimate its severity may
peak flow monitors (9). This literature, however, is
delay seeking necessary medical care or comply poorly
limited to inferences that justifiably can be drawn
with therapy (2–3). Conversely, those who overesti-
from “between-person” research designs, namely,
mate airways obstruction may use too much aerosol
that individuals who are, for example, more anxious
bronchodilator therapy, which in turn could increase
tend to experience more severe asthma symptoms.
the underlying severity of the disease (4). The likeli-
Few have examined whether changes in these vari-
hood of these perceptual “errors” is suggested by many
ables covary within individuals over time; in other
studies that have documented only a weak correlation
words, whether changes in anxious mood actually
between objective measures of airways obstruction
track changes in asthma severity in the course of
daily life for any of the individuals under study.
From the Departments of Community Medicine (G.A., H.T., J.U.) Such time-intensive “daily process” designs can
and Behavioral Sciences and Community Health (S.R.) and the Gen- provide fresh insights through their capacity to min-
eral Clinical Research Center (E.B., A.W.), University of Connecticut imize retrospection errors in symptom and mood
School of Medicine, Farmington, Connecticut; Section of Allergy
and Immunology (A.A.), University of Pennsylvania School of Med-
reporting, use subjects as their own controls, and
icine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; and Department of Medicine establish sequential relations from the many com-
(R.Z-W.), St. Francis Hospital and Medical Center, Hartford, Con- parisons available (10). Brown and Moskowitz (11)
necticut. anticipate that this approach will advance behav-
Address reprint requests to: Glenn Affleck, PhD, Department of ioral and psychosomatic medicine through its ability
Community Medicine, University of Connecticut Health Center,
Farmington, CT 06030. Email: affleck@nso1.uchc.edu
to better capture the ebb and flow of the many psy-
Received for publication March 15, 1999; revision received July chological and somatic processes that exhibit rapid
19, 1999. moments of change.

Psychosomatic Medicine 62:61– 68 (2000) 61


0033-3174/00/6201-0061
Copyright © 2000 by the American Psychosomatic Society
G. AFFLECK et al.

WITHIN-PERSON STUDIES OF MOOD AND iate analyses required to test this hypothesis adequately.
ASTHMA SEVERITY Nor did our “fixed effects” statistical analysis allow us to
generalize our findings to the population from whom the
Few published studies have inspected relations be-
sample was drawn (21). Time-varying covariates, such as
tween changes in mood and asthma severity at the
time of day and medication use, were also not considered
within-person level from day to day. Steptoe and
as possible confounds of within-person relations be-
Holmes (12) measured mood and peak expiratory flow
tween mood, symptoms, and peak flow.
rate (PEFR) four times daily over 20 days in 7 men with
The present study extends and refines previous re-
asthma, and 7 nonasthmatic men. Mood ratings in-
search on this topic in several ways. First, we studied
cluded “angry-calm,” “relaxed-tense,” and “elated-de-
a larger sample and conducted multilevel random ef-
pressed.” None of the men without asthma, but three fects analyses that do permit generalization to the sam-
of the asthmatic men, exhibited a significant relation pled population. Second, we determined whether the
between one or more mood states and PEFR. Hyland relations between mood and asthma severity could be
(13) examined mood-PEFR relations in 10 adults with specious because they either share the same, but inde-
asthma by twice-daily observations over 15 days. Par- pendent, circadian pattern or they are both affected by
ticipants rated an array of 14 negative moods and 10 the use of as-needed (PRN) asthma medications.
positive moods. Combining these in a single bipolar Changes in mood (22, 23) and in airway obstruction
mood scale, Hyland identified three subjects whose (13, 24) across the day have been documented. Albu-
increasing positive mood (and decreasing negative terol dosing is associated with symptom aggravation
mood) tracked their changes in PEFR. and PEFR (25) and may precipitate mood changes.
Neither of these small sample studies explored the A third key feature of the study is its reliance on a
possibility that there may be different mood correlates theory-derived multidimensional model of mood as-
of PEFR as opposed to symptom perceptions. There is sessment. Two general models have guided the self-
now substantial literature linking affective states with report measurement of mood: the specific affects ap-
both minor and chronic illness symptoms (14 –16). proach and the dimensional approach (26). The first
Symptom exacerbations may have emotional conse- reflects “the belief that there are many different types
quences, but certain emotions may intensify symp- of mood, each with different. . . characteristics and
toms by increasing attention toward the self. In partic- response patterns.” The second model takes the posi-
ular, variations in sad (or happy) mood may trigger (or tion that “there are a few, usually two, ”core“ dimen-
attenuate) both the process of “turning inward” sions of mood; specific moods are thought to be com-
(17, 18) as well as the amplification of physical symp- binations of [these] dimensions” (26, p. 150). For the
toms (19). For these reasons, Pennebaker (20) con- present study, we adopted the theory-driven dimen-
cluded that symptom perceptions can have stronger sional approach expressed in the circumplex model of
relations with emotional states than with the underly- emotion first proposed by Russell (27) and elaborated
ing physiological processes that create symptoms. by Larsen and Diener (28). This approach places self-
We hypothesize that variations in happy and sad ratings of mood in a two-dimensional circular space,
mood would be most likely to track changes in symp- which distinguishes states according to their degree of
tom reports and would do so even after adjusting this pleasantness (or hedonic tone) and degree of activation
relation for concurrent PEFR. We conducted a prelim- (or arousal tone). Thus, for example, anxious mood
inary study of this possibility with 21 asthmatic adults combines high unpleasantness and high arousal,
who rated their mood, provided PEFR readings, and whereas calm mood combines high pleasantness with
chronicled their symptoms three times a day for 21 low arousal. We adopted this model principally be-
days (7). The data were pooled across persons and cause it distinguishes between arousal states, which
observations and analyzed at the within-person level may be most likely to correlate with peak flow ratings,
by fixing the differences between participants in their and hedonic states, which may figure most strongly in
mean levels of mood, PEFR, and symptoms. Rising symptom reports (7).
PEFR scores were most highly related to declining Finally, a novel feature of this investigation is its
fatigue and increasing liveliness. Improvements in use of electronic methods for field measurements of all
symptoms, in contrast, were most likely to correspond study variables: mood, symptoms, peak expiratory
with increasing happiness and decreasing sadness. flow, and beta-agonist use. These methods assured
These findings stand as preliminary evidence that participants’ compliance with demanding schedules
short-term fluctuations in symptom perceptions and for self-reports of mood and symptoms; blinded par-
bronchoconstriction may correlate with different transi- ticipants to their peak flow records when describing
tory moods. However, we did not conduct the multivar- symptoms; and measured medication use without re-

62 Psychosomatic Medicine 62:61– 68 (2000)


MOOD, ASTHMA SYMPTOMS, PEFR

lying on self-report. Taken together, these procedures sequence of three requests for data produced a missing entry for that
mitigate a formidable threat to the validity of experi- time period.
Interview questions were presented one at a time in a fixed order
ence sampling studies.
on a liquid crystal display (2 lines by 20 characters per line). Par-
ticipants replied to each question by scrolling across fixed-response
METHODS AND PROCEDURES options with backward and forward arrows and then pressing an
“enter” button to save the response and its time stamp on a EEPROM
Characteristics of the Sample data pak (which could not be erased without exposing it deliberately
to 30 minutes of ultraviolet light). The response option appearing
Sixty-three adults with moderate to severe asthma being treated
first on the screen with each new question was randomized to
at the allergy and pulmonary clinics of a university and an affiliated
minimize response set. The project’s research associate trained par-
community hospital were asked to participate in the study, and 50
ticipants on a demonstration version of ELI and gave them a manual
accepted the invitation. These participants were required to have a
which reviewed its functions and features.
prebronchodilator forced expiratory flow rate in 1 second (FEV1) of
The mood interview. The 16 items for the mood interview were
less than 80% of predicted for their gender, weight, and height. In
drawn from the mood circumplex items supplied by Larsen and
addition, all demonstrated a 15% or greater increase in FEV1 after
Diener (28). This model classifies affect as pleasant or unpleasant, as
bronchodilator administration within the past year. All participants
aroused or unaroused, or as a mixture of these dimensions. Figure 1
had at least four of the following pretreatment characteristics for
diagrams the resulting mood octants and the specific mood adjec-
National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute criteria for moderate asth-
tives used to measure them.
ma: exacerbations of cough/wheeze more than once or twice per
To reduce the number of analyses required, four bipolar mood
week; cough and wheeze between acute exacerbations often present;
vector scores comprising four adjectives apiece were derived from
reduced exercise tolerance; PEFR ⬍80% predicted with a reversibil-
the circumplex. The first was the degree of mood pleasantness/
ity of 20% to 30%; nocturnal symptoms at least two to three times
unpleasantness with no arousal distinction (ie, a happy-sad vector)
per week; school or work attendance compromised by asthma; sys-
and was calculated by subtracting the two unpleasant mood adjec-
temic steroids usually necessary to treat exacerbations; and regular
tives in octant 7 from the two pleasant mood adjectives in octant 3.
use of oral or inhaled corticosteroids or cromolyn required daily for
The second was the degree of mood arousal/unarousal with no
more than 2 months of the year before entry into this study. All were
pleasantness distinction (ie, an active-passive vector) and was cal-
currently nonsmokers with a less than a 10 pack-year history of
culated by subtracting the unaroused mood adjectives in octant 5
tobacco use. None had significant pulmonary disease other than
from the aroused mood adjectives in octant 1. The third comprised
asthma. During the initial orientation, each participant was required
the opposite poles of unaroused pleasant mood and aroused un-
to demonstrate reliable use of all electronic devices. Two partici-
pleasant mood (ie, a calm-anxious vector) and was calculated by
pants were excluded from subsequent analyses because of technical
subtracting the adjectives in octant 2 from those in octant 6. The
malfunctions with one of the electronic devices.
fourth combined the opposite poles of aroused unpleasant mood and
The remaining 48 patients (64.6% women; 81.3% white; 54.2%
unaroused pleasant mood (ie, a peppy-drowsy vector) and was
married) had a mean age of 42.1 years (SD ⫽ 14.8) and a mean of 14.4
scored by subtracting the adjectives in octant 4 from those in octant
years of formal education (SD ⫽ 2.1). The average participant had been
8.
diagnosed with asthma 22.8 years earlier (SD ⫽ 15.9). Since then, they
We confirmed the appropriateness of this scoring procedure by
reported having had a mean of 4.6 asthma-related hospitalizations
conducting principal components analyses of the disaggregated data
(SD ⫽ 14.9) and 4.4 asthma-related emergency room visits (SD ⫽ 9.33).
set. The mood vectors in Figure 1, which are orthogonal to each
They recalled having missed a mean of 4.3 days of work during the past
other in theory, should have empirical separation as well. In other
year (SD ⫽ 17.6) because of their asthma symptoms.
words, the four adjectives composing the pleasant/unpleasant vector
should form a component separate from that for the four adjectives
Electronic Interviews of Mood and Asthma
Symptoms
For 21 consecutive days, participants carried a palm-top com-
puter programmed as an electronic interviewer (ELI), which asked
them about their current asthma symptoms and mood three times a
day at randomly selected times— once each during the morning
(between 9:45 AM and 11:15 AM), afternoon (between 2:45 PM and
4:15 PM), and evening (6:45 PM and 9:15 PM). The computer was a
programmable battery-powered Psion Organizer II (Psion, Concord,
MA) with dimensions of 1.4 cm ⫻ 7.8 cm ⫻ 2.9 cm and weight of
250 g. This device has amply demonstrated its feasibility and reli-
ability in prospective daily studies of pain, mood, and fatigue
(29, 30); drinking and smoking (31, 32); asthma symptoms (33); and
sleep quality (30, 34).
Some procedures for the ELI protocol parallel those designed by
Shiffman and colleagues (31) for their electronic diary studies of
cigarette smokers. The data entry procedure for each ELI request
proceeded from the user’s termination with a keystroke of an audible
beep to her choice to answer the interview then 5 minutes later, or
15 minutes later. The auditory signal lasted 60 seconds; if not
answered within this time, it was repeated 5 minutes later, and if not Fig. 1. Mood circumplex octants and adjectives used for measure-
answered again, another 5 minutes later. Failure to answer this ment.

Psychosomatic Medicine 62:61– 68 (2000) 63


G. AFFLECK et al.

composing the aroused/unaroused vector. Similarly, the four adjec- basis. The canister was housed in an MDI Chronolog (Medtrac Tech-
tives composing the aroused unpleasant/unaraoused pleasant vector nologies, Inc., Lakewood, CO) which recorded time and date of each
should form a component separate from that for the adjectives com- actuation. For our data analyses, we calculated the number of doses
posing the aroused pleasant/unaroused unpleasant vector. during each morning, afternoon, and evening period dictated by the
Two principal components analyses with varimax rotation con- electronic interviewer protocol.
firmed these predictions. Each mood score was first centered around
the person’s own mean to examine the within-person covariance
structure free of between-person differences in mean levels. The RESULTS
covariance pattern among the eight adjectives included in the pleas-
ant/unpleasant vector and the aroused/unaroused vector yielded Descriptive Findings
two components with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 and accounting
Of the 3024 symptom/mood interviews requested,
for 67.2% of the variance. Each adjective loaded highly on its pre-
dicted vector (⬎0.70 or ⬍⫺0.80). The same was found for the eight 2947 (97.5%) were completed, and of an equal number
adjectives contained in the other two orthogonal vectors. Two com- of electronic peak flowmeter actuations requested,
ponents with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 accounted for 67.1% of 2834 (93.7%) were completed. The mean PEFR across
the variance, and each adjective loaded highly on its predicted persons and observations was 347.9 (SD ⫽ 95.1). On
vector (⬎0.80 or ⬍⫺0.80).
scales that could range from ⫺12 to 12, with higher
The asthma symptom interview. The ELI asked about four current
asthma symptoms: coughing, wheezing, chest tightness, and short- numbers representing more pleasant or more aroused
ness of breath. Each was rated on a 0 – 6 scale, anchored verbally at mood states, the mean happy-sad score was 5.9 (SD ⫽
0 ⫽ none, 2 ⫽ mild, 4 ⫽ moderate, and 6 ⫽ severe. Asthma symptom 2.2); the mean active-passive score was 1.2 (SD ⫽ 1.7);
severity for that interview was scored as the sum across these rat- the mean calm-anxious score was 5.0 (SD ⫽ 2.2); and
ings. Using the mean-centering procedure described previously,
the mean peppy-drowsy score was 1.8 (SD ⫽ 2.5). The
Cronbach ␣ internal consistency estimates for the asthma symptom
severity composite were 0.94 for 79 for the morning reports, 0.96 for mean asthma symptom score, on the 0 – 6 point scale,
the afternoon reports, and 0.93 for the evening reports. was 3.8 (SD ⫽ 2.9). On average, there were 1.92 albu-
terol doses administered for each 24-hour period
Electronic Assessments of PEFR (SD ⫽ 1.42).

Participants used an electronic peak flowmeter (PeakLog, Medt-


rac Technologies, Inc., Lakewood, CO) to measure their airways Between-Person Relations
obstruction. This instrument, which is 13 cm ⫻ 6 cm x 2.5 cm and
weighs 138 g, measures airflow with a hot-wire anemometer. Accu- Correlations between participants’ average values
racy was tested by the manufacturer using recommendations by the on these variables were calculated to examine be-
National Asthma Education Program (35). This involved injecting tween-person associations. Individuals with higher ag-
nine standardized wave forms into each of 10 devices five times gregate PEFR (transformed as the ratio between mean
using a computer-driven pulmonary wave form generator. All in-
struments were within 10% of the target values and were within 5%
raw scores and those predicted by the subject’s weight,
of each other. A built-in barometer allows for self-correction for height, and age) reported more severe symptoms (r ⫽
fluctuations in barometric pressure. The device received FDA ap- ⫺0.30, p ⬍ .05). As Table 1 indicates, mean PEFR was
proval on November 1, 1994. unrelated to mean mood vector scores. However, those
PEFR was recorded along with the time and date of measurement with more severe asthma symptoms scored lower on
by the electronic peak flowmeter after each electronic interview.
Participants were given standardized instructions and observed in
the happy-sad mood vector, on the calm-anxious mood
the use of the device during prestudy training. After prompting by vector, and on the peppy-drowsy mood vector. After
the electronic diary (with the completion of the symptom interview), controlling for PEFR, mean asthma symptoms corre-
patients were instructed to stand, take a deep breath, fill their lungs lated significantly with all mood vector scores. Albu-
completely, place the mouthpiece in the mouth past the teeth with terol use was higher for those reporting more severe
lips tightly closed around the mouthpiece, and blow as hard and fast
as possible in a single exhalation. Three such expiratory maneuvers
TABLE 1. Between-Person Relations of Mean Mood Vector
were obtained, and the highest PEFR was reserved for data analysis.
Scores with Mean PEFR and Mean Asthma Symptoms
The validity of peak flow monitor actuations in the field can be
strengthened by establishing their correlation with assessments un-
Mean Asthma
der controlled conditions. At the beginning and end of the self-
Mean Mean Asthma Symptoms
monitoring period, spirometry was performed on all participants Mean Mood
PEFR Symptoms Controlling for
according to criteria of the American Thoracic Society (36) and the
Mean PEFR
scores averaged across time. Mean daily peak flow ratings were
correlated with spirometric measures of PEFR (r ⫽ 0.81, p ⬍ .001) Happy—sad .12 ⫺.30* ⫺.29*
and FEV1 (r ⫽ 0.69, p ⬍ .001). Active—passive ⫺.12 ⫺.26 ⫺.32*
Calm—nervous .21 ⫺.36* ⫺.32*
Electronic Recording of Beta-Agonist Use Peppy—drowsy .05 ⫺.42** ⫺.42**

The only short-acting beta-agonist allowed in the study was * p ⬍ .05, 46 df.
albuterol by metered dose inhalation, prescribed on an as-needed ** p ⬍ .01, 46 df.

64 Psychosomatic Medicine 62:61– 68 (2000)


MOOD, ASTHMA SYMPTOMS, PEFR

TABLE 2. Within-person Relations of Mood Vector Scores with Peak Expiratory Flow and Asthma Symptoms

Asthma Symptoms
PEF Asthma Symptoms
(Controlling for PEF)
Mood Vector
ba t p b t p b t p

Happy—sad b
.62 1.87 Ns ⫺.08 ⫺3.33 ⬍.01 ⫺.06 ⫺2.75 ⬍.01
Active—passivec 1.38 3.99 ⬍.001 .00 .20 Ns .02 .84 Ns
Calm—nervousd ⫺.34 ⫺1.11 Ns ⫺.07 ⫺3.54 ⬍.001 ⫺.08 3.50 ⬍.001
Peppy—drowsye 1.68 4.39 ⬍.001 ⫺.05 ⫺2.65 ⬍.01 ⫺.03 ⫺1.95 Ns

a
Unstandardized maximum likelihood estimate.
b
Pleasant, unpleasant mood vector, measured by happy, cheerful, sad, and blue.
c
Aroused, unaroused mood vector, measured by active, lively, passive, and quiet.
d
Unaroused/pleasant, aroused/unpleasant mood vector, measured by calm, relaxed, anxious, and nervous.
e
Aroused/pleasant, unaroused/unpleasant mood vector, measured by peppy, stimulated, drowsy, and tired.

symptoms (r ⫽ 0.40, p ⬍ .001) but was unrelated to the differences in the average person’s symptom re-
average peak flow ratings or to average mood scores. ports from morning to afternoon and afternoon to
evening each day are not because of changes in bron-
choconstriction, at least as they are measured by PEFR
Approach to Within-Person Data Analysis
in the field. This leaves substantial room for associa-
1A valid within-person analysis of the relations tions between mood and asthma symptoms when
among PEFR, asthma symptoms, and mood requires a PEFR is controlled and the opportunity to examine
multilevel modeling strategy that partitions the two symptom perceptions independent of an objective
sources of variation in the person-observation data set: measure of bronchoconstriction.3
differences between persons in the mean levels of the
observations and differences within persons in their
own data over time. To generalize our findings to both Within-Person Relations
the population of individuals from which the sample Table 2 summarizes the within-person relations be-
was drawn and to the population of days from which tween the four mood vector scores and PEFR. Rising
their daily experiences were sampled, we used a ran- scores on the active-passive and the peppy-drowsy
dom effects regression model, which allowed between- mood vectors were associated with increasing PEFR.
person differences in intercepts (means) and slopes When examined jointly as predictors of PEFR, each
(within-person relations) to vary randomly when cal- was also an independent correlate of PEFR changes
culating parameters for mood-PEFR-symptom relations (active-passive, b ⫽ 0.70, p ⬍ .05; peppy-drowsy, b ⫽
(37). This procedure provides average estimates of with- 1.28, p ⬍ .001). Together, these two mood vectors
in-person relations regardless of differences between per- accounted for 4% of the within-person variance in
sons in levels of the variables under study. The PROC PEFR.
MIXED procedure in SAS (38) furnished model parame- Table 2 also lists the parameter estimates for within-
ters in the form of maximum likelihood estimates.1 person relations between mood and symptoms, with
Within-person changes in symptoms significantly and without controlling for that observation’s PEFR.
tracked changes in PEFR (b ⫽ ⫺0.005, p ⬍ .001). Yet, Two mood vectors— happy-sad and calm-nervous—
only 3.7% of the within-person variance in symptoms remained related to fluctuating symptoms regardless
could be explained by peak flow actuations at the time of PEFR changes. When examined together as predic-
symptoms were reported.2 This suggests that much of tors, both vectors remained significantly correlated
with asthma symptoms (happy-sad, b ⫽ ⫺0.07, p ⬍
1
Because equal interval time-dependent data are likely to be
.05; calm-nervous, b ⫽ ⫺ 0.08, p ⬍ .01, respectively).
nonindependent, leading to autocorrelated residuals and the mises- Together, these two mood vectors explained 4.6% of
timation of standard errors, an error structure assuming a higher
correlation between consecutive error terms, ie, an AR (1) structure,
was fit to each of the statistically significant models. None of the
3
significant findings reported here were altered by this procedure. A companion set of analyses examined the ability of mood to
2
The HLM estimates of within-person variance explained in these predict the next within-day observation for symptoms or peak flow,
models should not be confused with those provided in OLS regres- as well as the reverse sequence. Controlling for contemporaneous
sion (39). These estimates refer to the ability of explanatory variables to associations, there were no significant lagged relations for any of
reduce the random variance components of their respective equations. these analyses.

Psychosomatic Medicine 62:61– 68 (2000) 65


G. AFFLECK et al.

the within-person variance in symptoms not already passive, b ⫽ ⫺0.24, p ⬍ .001; peppy-drowsy, b ⫽
accounted for by variance in PEFR. ⫺0.47, p ⬍ .001). However, both mood dimensions
These analyses take account of all observations in remained significantly associated with PEFR when
the data set. But in so doing, they may miss effects that time of day was entered along with them in the model
are revealed only when asthma symptoms or airways predicting PEFR, ruling out confounding by time of day.
obstruction are most severe. To examine this possibil-
ity, we recomputed the analyses of relations of mood
Albuterol Use Effects?
with asthma symptoms and PEFR by comparing mood
reports at extreme observations. The first compared Participants’ self-administration of albuterol doses
mood scores at moments when PEFR was relatively during each observation period was unrelated to that
high for that individual (⬎1 SD above that person’s observation’s PEFR, but more doses were administered
mean) with those when PEFR was relatively low (⬎1 during times when they reported more severe symp-
SD below the mean). Only the peppy-tired mood vec- toms (b ⫽ 0.79, p ⬍ .001). Albuterol dosing explained
tor was significantly different for these two sets of 3.7% of the within-person variance in symptoms.
observations (b ⫽ 0.29, p ⬍ .05). The second analysis Higher use of albuterol was also associated with lower
compared mood scores at moments when asthma scores on the active-passive mood dimension (b ⫽
symptoms were relatively severe and relatively mild. ⫺0.28, p ⬍ .05) and on the peppy-drowsy dimension
Only the happy-sad mood vector differed significantly (b ⫽ ⫺0.43, p ⬍ .001). Inspection of the relations
between these two sets of observations (b ⫽ ⫺0.30, p ⬍ presented in Table 1 disclose only one relation that
.04). These two significant findings echo significant could be confounded by albuterol use, namely the
effects reported in Table 1 for the full spectrum of association between symptoms and peppy-drowsy
observations. However, two other significant effects mood. However, even controlling for albuterol dosing,
appearing in Table 1 were missed by this analysis, this relation remained statistically significant.
suggesting that mood may play a more prominent role
in explaining the total variation of PEFR and asthma
Differential Effort in Peak Flow Monitoring?
symptoms across the day than it does in the ability to
differentiate between daily episodes of comparatively The specific moods associated with PEFR signal
severe vs mild symptomatology. differences in arousal states. Thus, it could be argued
Next, we examine three possible sources of con- that these relations are less a function of intrinsic
founding that might spuriously inflate the associations relations between mood and PEFR than they are of the
of mood with PEFR and symptoms: a shared time relations between mood and the effort required to use
course across reporting occasions within the day; as- the PEFR monitor to measure bronchoconstriction ac-
needed administration of albuterol; and the effort-de- curately. For example, might the relation between
pendence of unsupervised peak flow monitoring. drowsiness and peak flow be due to the effect of
drowsiness on suboptimal effort with the peak flow-
meter?
Time of Day Effects?
This is a challenging problem for field studies. To
The relations summarized to this point could sim- understand this possibility, we computed a crude in-
ply be due to sharing the same time course across daily dex of each participant’s overall effort as the ratio of
observations. To address this possibility, each variable his or her mean PEFRs across the 21 days with the gold
was modeled for within-person changes across the standard of average PEFRs measured before day 1 and
day. Asthma symptoms did not differ by time of day, after day 21 with supervised spirometric assessments.
but PEFR did. As the day progressed, PEFR declined Higher ratios could be construed as consistent with a
(b ⫽ ⫺5.94, p ⬍ .001).4 So, too, did the mood states more accurate or effortful use of PEFR monitors at
that were significantly associated with PEFR (active- home.
This ratio did not correlate with the mean mood
scores for either the active-passive vector (r ⫽ 0.05) or
4
This finding may at odds with the common clinical observation the peppy-drowsy vector (r ⫽ 0.14). However, it was
that asthmatics’ bronchoconstriction is worse after awakening and significantly associated with the magnitude of the
better in the evening. The first PEFR in the present analysis was within-person relations between these moods and
taken in the late morning each day. We had also measured PEFR
PEFR. This was revealed by a random effects regres-
one-half hour after awakening, but omitted these data from this
study because mood was not measured on this occasion. These sion analysis that examined the ratio’s ability to pre-
earlier morning PEF ratings were indeed lower than those in the dict variation in the mood-PEFR slopes. Participants
evening. with higher ratios, who arguably were more effortful in

66 Psychosomatic Medicine 62:61– 68 (2000)


MOOD, ASTHMA SYMPTOMS, PEFR

their use of peak flow monitors, were those who ex- this hypothesis. It was variations in happy-sad and
hibited weaker relations between PEFR and active- calm-anxious moods that were independently related
passive mood (b ⫽ ⫺3.94, p ⬍ .05) and peppy-drowsy to the ebb and flow of symptoms. Most important,
mood (b ⫽ ⫺0.4.63, p ⬍ .05). these findings persisted even after controlling for con-
temporaneous peak flow ratings. Thus, within the lim-
its of the reliability and validity of unsupervised peak
DISCUSSION
flow monitoring as a measure of airways obstruction,
This study is the first to identify mood states that are there seems to be a unique relation between these
differentially related to asthma symptoms and peak moods and the individual’s subjective experience of
expiratory flow. To summarize, two mood vectors that bronchoconstriction.
captured the degree of arousal (active-passive and pep-
py-drowsy) were significantly related to PEFR
Mood and PEFR
changes. Two other mood vectors that reflected degree
of pleasantness (happy-sad and calm-nervous) were Interpretation of the relations between mood states
significantly related to asthma symptom reports, after and PEFR is aided in part by having blinded study
adjusting these relations for contemporaneous PEFR. participants to their peak flow ratings. Although symp-
These findings remained significant even after control- tom reports covaried with mood vectors involving
ling for time of day and albuterol dosing. pleasantness, PEFR covaried with mood vectors in-
Several features of our research may be responsible volving arousal. This included variations in both ac-
for these novel findings. These include a) a theory- tive-passive and peppy-drowsy mood.
driven assessment of mood that separates arousal from Straightforward interpretation of the mood-PEFR re-
hedonic tone; b) a daily process methodology that lations, however, is complicated by the effort-depen-
minimizes retrospection error in symptom and mood dence of peak flow monitoring reliability. If arousal
reporting; c) use of electronic methods that blind sub- affects expiration effort, then our findings may reflect
jects to peak flow monitoring results and encourage nothing more than the differential reliability of peak
and assess compliance with symptom ratings, and d) flow monitoring with and without optimal effort.
emphasis on the within-person patterning of changes Without having frequent supervised actuations as pri-
in mood, symptoms, and peak flow within and across mary data, it is impossible to dismiss this possibility.
days. We tried to develop an understanding of this problem
Although we measured mood, asthma symptoms, by calculating a gold standard for each participant’s
and PEFR three times a day, our analyses failed to PEFR under maximum effort with controlled spirom-
establish temporal priority. Thus, we cannot tell etry readings before and after the 21– day self-monitor-
whether these mood states preceded changes in PEFR ing study. Comparing this against each participant’s
or asthma symptoms or were engendered by them. Our aggregate PEFR across the 21– day period afforded us a
decision to sample these processes three times a day crude indicator of his or her optimal effort with peak
was balanced against the burden of a 21 day self- flow monitoring. Interestingly, this indicator did not
monitoring period. It may be that readings need to be correlate with participants’ aggregate scores on the two
taken more frequently within the day to capture se- mood states that correlated with PEFR changes. How-
quential relations. Alternatively, the relations we un- ever, it did affect the magnitude of the within-person
covered may arise from simultaneous processes or se- relation between these mood states and changes in
quential processes that are so ephemeral that they would PEFR. Those whose peak flow monitoring was judged
be virtually impossible to detect in field studies. by this indicator to be more effortful exhibited weaker
connections between PEFR and these moods. Thus,
our findings concerning the relation between mood
Mood and Asthma Symptoms
and PEFR may be due in part to differential effort in
A substantial quantity of literature has elucidated peak flow monitoring. To settle this question, future
connections between mood and the perception of ill- investigations should plan more frequent in-home su-
ness symptoms. Dysphoric mood may be a conse- pervised monitoring of airways obstruction.
quence of symptom exacerbations, but it could also These caveats notwithstanding, the present study’s
amplify symptoms through increasing self-focused at- findings that different moods figure in symptom per-
tention. Whether they are a consequence or antecedent ceptions as opposed to changes in airways obstruction
of symptom changes, fluctuating levels of pleasant/ establish emotions as one factor that may account for
unpleasant mood should most likely track changes in the inaccuracy of some asthma patients’ estimation of
subjective asthma symptoms. Our findings support the severity of their illness. Between 4% and 5% of the

Psychosomatic Medicine 62:61– 68 (2000) 67


G. AFFLECK et al.

within-person variation in symptoms and PEFR were 15. Larsen R, Kasimatis M. Day-to day physical symptoms: individ-
explained by contemporaneous mood. On balance, ual differences in the occurrence, duration, and emotional con-
comitants of minor daily illnesses. J Pers 1991;59:387– 424.
this effect size was equivalent to that captured by the
16. Persson L, Sjoberg L. Mood and somatic symptoms. J Psychosom
within-person relation between symptoms and PEFR Res 1987;31:499 –511.
and between albuterol use and symptoms. How much 17. Ingram R, Smith T. Depression and internal focus of attention.
of the unexplained fluctuation in symptoms and PEFR Cogn Therapy Res 1984;8:139 –52.
is simply idiosyncratic or random, and how much 18. Sedikides C. Mood as a determinant of attentional focus. Cogn
Emot 1992;6:129 – 48.
might be because of predictable individual differences
19. Stalling R. Mood and pain: the influence of positive and negative
between asthma patients needs to be examined in fu- affect on reported body aches. J Soc Behav Pers 1992;7:323–34.
ture studies using larger samples to permit adequate 20. Pennebaker J. The psychology of physical symptoms. New York:
statistical power to address this question. Springer; 1982.
21. Schwartz J, Stone A. Strategies for analyzing ecological momen-
This study was supported by a grant from the Amer- tary assessment data. Health Psychol 1998;17:6 –16.
ican Lung Association of Connecticut and the Univer- 22. Clark L, Watson D, Leeka J. Diurnal variation in the positive
sity of Connecticut General Clinical Research Center affects. Motiv Emot 1988;13:205–34.
23. Egloff B, Tausch A, Kohlmann C, Krohne H. Relationships be-
(National Institutes of Health Grant M01-RR06192). tween time of day, day of the week, and positive mood: explor-
ing the role of the mood measure. Motiv Emot 1995;19:99 –110.
24. Hetzel M, Clark T. Comparison of normal and asthmatic circadian
REFERENCES rhythms in peak expiratory flow rate. Thorax 1980;35:732– 8.
1. Weiss K, Wagener D. Changing patterns of asthma mortality: 25. Mann M, Eliasson O, Patel K, ZuWallack R. An evaluation of
identifying target populations at high risk. JAMA 1990;264: severity-modulated compliance with q.i.d. dosing of inhaled
1683–7. beclomethasone. Chest 1992;102:1342– 6.
2. Li J, O’Connell E. Clinical evaluation of asthma. Ann Allergy 26. Stone A. Measurement of affective response. In: Cohen S,
1996;76:1– 4. Kessler R, Underwood Gordon L. editors. Measuring stress: a
3. Kikuchi Y, Okabe S, Tamura G, Hida W, Homma W, Shirato R. guide for health and social scientists. New York: Oxford Uni-
Chemosensitivity and perception of dyspnea in patients with a versity Press; 1995. p. 148 –174.
history of near-fatal asthma. N Engl J Med 1994;330:1329 –34. 27. Russell J. A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol
4. Sears M, Taylor D, Print C, Lake D, Li Q, Flannery E, Yates D, 1980;39:1161–78.
Lucas M, Herbison G. Regular inhaled beta-agonist treatment in 28. Larsen R, Diener E. Promises and problems with the circumplex
bronchial asthma. Lancet 1990;336:1391– 6. model of emotion. In: Clarke, MS, editor. Newbury Park, CA:
5. McFadden E, Kiser R, DeGroot W. Acute bronchial asthma: Sage Publications; 1992. p. 25–59.
relationships between clinical and physiologic manifestations. 29. Stone A, Broderick J, Porter L, Krupp L, Gnys M, Paty J, Shiffman
N Engl J Med 1973;288:221–5. S. Fatigue and mood in chronic fatigue syndrome patients: results
6. Kendrick A, Higgs C, Whitfield M, Lazlo G. Accuracy of percep- of a momentary assessment protocol examining fatigue and mood
tion of severity of asthma: patients treated in general practice. levels and diurnal patterns. Ann Behav Med 1994;16:228 –34.
BMJ 1993;307:422– 4. 30. Affleck G, Urrows S, Tennen H, Higgins P, Abeles M. Sequential
7. Apter, A, Affleck G, Reisine S, Tennen H, Barrows E, Willard A, daily relations of sleep, pain intensity, and attention to pain
ZuWallack R. Perception of airways obstruction in asthma: se- among women with fibromyalgia. Pain 1996;68:363– 8.
quential daily analyses of symptoms, PEFR, and mood. J Allergy 31. Shiffman S, Fischer L, Paty J, Gnys M, Hickcox M, Kassel J.
Clin Immunol 1997;99:605–12. Drinking and smoking: a field study of their association. Ann
8. Apter A, ZuWallack R, Clive, J. Common measures of asthma Behav Med 1994;16:203–9.
severity lack association for describing its clinical course. J 32. Swendsen J, Tennen H, Carney A, Affleck G, Willard A, Hromi
Allergy Clin Immunol 1994;94:732–7. A. Mood and alcohol consumption: an experience sampling test
9. Lehrer P, Isenberg S, Hochron S. Asthma and emotion: a review. of the self-medication hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol in press.
J Asthma 1993;30:5–21. 33. Hyland M, Kenyon C, Howarth P. Diary keeping in asthma: com-
10. Tennen H, Affleck G. Daily processes in coping with chronic parison of written and electronic methods. BMJ 1993;306:487–9.
pain: methods and analytic strategies. In: Zeidner M, Endler N, 34. Totterdell P, Reynolds S, Parkinson B, Briner R. Associations of
editors. Handbook of coping. New York: Wiley; 1996. p. sleep with everyday mood, minor symptoms, and social inter-
151–180. action experience. Sleep 1993;17:466 –75.
11. Brown K, Moskowitz D. It’s a function of time: a review of the 35. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute National Asthma Ed-
process approach to behavioral medicine research. Psychosom ucation Program Expert Panel Report. Guidelines for the diag-
Med 1998;20:109 –17. nosis and management of asthma. J Allergy Clin Immunol 1991;
12. Steptoe A, Holmes R. Mood and pulmonary function in adult 88:425–34.
asthmatics: pilot self-monitoring study. Br J Med Psychol 1985; 36. American Thoracic Society. Standardization of spirometry:
58:87–94. 1987 update. Am Rev Resp Dis 1987;130:1296 –2009.
13. Hyland M. The mood-peak flow relationship in adult 37. Bryk A, Raudenbush S. Hierarchical linear models. Newbury
asthmatics: a pilot study of individual differences and direction Park, CA: Sage; 1992;.
of causality. Br J Med Psychol 1990;63:379 – 84. 38. SAS Institute. SAS/STAT Software. Changes and enhancements
14. Affleck G, Tennen H, Urrows S, Higgins P. Neuroticism and the through release 6.11. Cary, NC: SAS Institute; 1996.
pain-mood relation in rheumatoid arthritis: insights from a pro- 39. Kreft I, de Leeuw J. Introducing multilevel modeling. London:
spective daily study. J Consult Clin Psychol 1992;60:119 –26. Sage publications; 1998.

68 Psychosomatic Medicine 62:61– 68 (2000)

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen