Sie sind auf Seite 1von 16

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 35, No.

6, December 2005 (Ó 2005)


DOI 10.1007/s10803-005-0022-9

A Training Study of Theory of Mind and Executive Function


in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders

Naomi Fisher1,2 and Francesca Happé1

This study investigated the relationship between theory of mind and executive functioning in
children with autistic spectrum disorders through a training study. Ten children were trained
on theory of mind, whilst ten were trained in executive function. Seven children were assigned
to a control group, receiving no intervention. Training programmes were administered
individually, lasting for 25 minutes per day for 510 days. Children were tested before
training, after training and at a two-month follow-up. Significant improvements were seen in
performance on theory of mind tasks in both trained groups, whilst the control group showed
no improvement. No improvement on the executive function tasks was seen in any of the
groups. The implications of these findings are discussed.

KEY WORDS: Autism; theory of mind; executive function; intervention.

INTRODUCTION higher level action control, in particular those that


are necessary for maintaining a mentally specified
There is extensive evidence indicating impair- goal and for implementing that goal in the face of
ments in a so called ‘‘theory of mind’’ (defined as the distracting alternatives. Processes subsumed under
ability to attribute representational mental states) the term executive function include inhibition, set
impairment in children with autistic spectrum disor- shifting, planning, coordination and control of action
ders (ASD) (see Baron-Cohen, 2000 for a review). sequences. Many EF tasks (and particularly those
There is also evidence for deficits in executive func- which relate to performance on false belief tasks)
tioning in this group (see Sergeant, Geurts, & Oos- require suppression of a prepotent or habitual
terlaan, 2002 for a review). However, there has been response in favour of a new response (e.g. WCST;
some debate over whether either of these is in fact the Heaton, 1981, Go-NoGo; Drewe, 1975), and failure
fundamental deficit in ASD, capable of explaining the of EF in these tasks is therefore manifested as
deficits seen in the other domain (e.g. Baron-Cohen, perseveration of the old response.
1989; Perner & Lang, 2000; Russell, 1997). There are clear correlations between ToM and
Executive function, as defined for example by EF in young normally developing children (see
Perner (2000), is a term for processes responsible for Perner & Lang, 1999 for a review). However, the
direct evidence for an association between the two is
1
Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College, University of London, not as clear in children with ASD. This study aimed
London, UK. to investigate this relationship by training children
2
Correspondence should be addressed to: Department of with ASD in either ToM or EF (specifically set
Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College, University of
London, PO 78, SE5 8AF, London, UK; Tel.: +44-20-7848-
shifting and inhibition), and thus to look for inter-
0873; Fax: +44-20-7848-0866; e-mail: n.fisher@iop.kcl.ac.uk actions between the domains. If children with ASD

757
0162-3257/05/1200-0757/0 Ó 2005 Springer ScienceþBusiness Media, Inc.
758 Fisher and Happé

are failing ToM tasks because of difficulties in was therefore devised from scratch for this study. This
flexibility or set shifting, then training them on EF programme used a card sort task as the training task,
should improve their performance on ToM. If, as a task in which many children with ASD demon-
conversely, they are failing EF tasks because of a strate difficulties (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996) and
lack of mentalising abilities, then training then on taught children a strategy designed to help them shift
ToM should improve their performance on EF. between sets. This therefore included elements of
A number of studies have demonstrated that inhibitory control and flexibility. The structure of the
children with ASD can be taught to pass basic theory training programme was designed to mirror the theory
of mind tasks (Hadwin, Baron-Cohen, Howlin, & of mind programme. Both training programmes were
Hill, 1996, 1997; McGregor, Whiten, & Blackburn, taught on an individual basis to the children in
1998a, b; Ozonoff & Miller, 1995; Swettenham, 1996; 25-minute long sessions for between 5 and 10 days.
Swettenham, Baron-Cohen, Gomez, & Walsh, 1996). Children were not allowed to progress to the next stage
These studies have used a variety of methods, from until they reached criterion for the previous one. The
simple, computer administered feedback (Swetten- children were tested before training, immediately after
ham, 1996), to more elaborate training programmes, training and at a two month follow up.
taught either on a group basis (Ozonoff & Miller, The predictions for this study were as follows.
1995) or one-to-one (Hadwin et al., 1996; McGregor Firstly, children with ASD would be able to learn a
et al., 1998a; Swettenham et al., 1996). Whether this simple strategy to enable them to pass basic theory of
training generalises either to other, non-trained tasks, mind tasks. Secondly, children with ASD would be
or to real life is more debatable; whilst some studies able to learn a simple strategy to improve their
have found limited generalisation to untrained exper- performance on tests of cognitive flexibility such as
imental tasks (e.g. Swettenham et al., 1996), others card sorting tasks. Then there are four possible
have found no improvement on measures of social alternatives: (i) If ToM and EF are functionally
cognition in everyday life (Ozonoff & Miller, 1995). independent, the training programmes will show no
Several studies have demonstrated the maintenance transfer from one area to the other. This would also
of improvement on ToM tasks over time (Hadwin be the case if they share underlying cognitive factors,
et al., 1996; Swettenham, 1996). Some studies do the not addressed in the training programmes. (ii) If ToM
training over several months (Ozonoff & Miller, is necessary for EF (either because of task-specific
1995), whilst others do it intensively over the course factors, an online need for a certain competence in
of one or two weeks (Hadwin et al., 1996; Swetten- one to facilitate the other, or developmentally), then
ham et al., 1996). The most effective training the ToM training will lead to improvements in EF.
programme for the present purpose appeared to be (iii) If EF is necessary for ToM (because of task-
a strategy based approach taught on a one-to-one specific factors, online or developmentally), then the
basis. Our training programme was therefore mod- EF training will lead to improvements in ToM. (iv) If
elled on that used by Swettenham et al. (1996), with ToM is necessary for EF, and EF is necessary for
certain changes (described in the Methods Section), ToM, then the interactions will go both ways.
designed to maximise generalisation to outside the Obviously this type of short duration training will
training situation. not identify genuine developmental effects. However,
Training programmes for executive function are within the 2-month time span of the follow-up, there
far less plentiful than those for theory of mind. Only a may be sufficient time for more indirect (rather than
single study has attempted to train executive function immediate) effects of the training to become evident.
in children with ASD (Shimmon & Lewis, 2001), and
this study was very short-term (the children were
METHOD
trained on one day and tested the next day). Several
training programmes exist for children with attention
Participants
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (e.g. Kerns,
Eso, & Thomson, 1999; Semrud-Clikeman et al., Participants were recruited from eight schools
1999), however these programmes focus on areas for children with special educational needs in the
which children with ADHD find difficult such as greater London area. They were included on the basis
attention, whilst children with ASD appear to have of two criteria. First, they had to have a VMA (as
more difficulty with areas such as flexibility and set assessed by the BPVS II; (Dunn, Dunn, Whetton, &
shifting (Sergeant et al., 2002). A training programme Burley, 1999) of over 4 years 3 months, being a level
Training Theory of Mind and Executive Function 759

of language at which normally developing children Table I. Chronological Age and Ability of the Three Experimental
would be expected to pass FB tasks. Secondly, they Groups
had to fail at least 2/3 FB tasks. An additional
Group ToM (n=10) EF (n=10) Controls (n=7)
unexpected transfer FB task was administered to
those who failed only one of the two FB tasks initially CA (years) 10.50 (3.02) 10.68 (2.68) 9.67(1.73)
administered. The ages of participants ranged from BPVS VMA 7.23 (2.07) 6.57 (1.51) 5.44 (1.14)
6 years 5 months to 15 years 3 months. (years)
There were 27 children in this study, all except TROG VMA 5.00 (0.61) 5.35 (1.41) 4.49 (0.45)
one of which were being educated in specialist schools (years)
or classrooms for children with autism spectrum Raven’s CPM 22.90 (7.23) 24.60 (8.07) 20.57 (5.97)
disorders. The remaining child (who had a diagnosis (raw scores)
of Asperger’s Syndrome) was being educated in a
Mean (s.d).
school for children with moderate learning difficul-
ties. Twenty of the children had been diagnosed by a
psychiatrist or a psychologist as having an autistic having been absent from school over the weeks on
spectrum disorder (ASD) or autism, whilst one had a which the training took place, and therefore the
diagnosis of Asperger’s Syndrome. The remaining 6 groups were not perfectly matched. A one-way
(who were from a single school and who were all ANOVA showed no significant differences between
being educated in classes for children with autistic the 3 experimental groups on any of the ability
spectrum disorders) were described on their records measures.
as having social and communication disorders. These
children were assigned to the ASD group after Measures
completion of a checklist of symptoms based on Due to the large number of measures included in
DSM-IV after discussion with their teachers. This this study, only brief descriptions are given of the
included symptoms such as a stereotyped or repetitive administration procedures. Further details are avail-
use of language or idiosyncratic language, a preoc- able on request from the author.
cupation with stereotyped and restricted patterns of
interest and an inflexible adherence to routines and General ability
rituals, and teachers had to endorse all or almost all
of the symptoms in order for the child to be included Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM)
in the study. Further details of this checklist may be (Raven, Raven, & Court, 1998)
obtained from the authors. Removing these children Non-verbal ability was assessed using the col-
from the analysis made no difference to the pattern of oured set of Raven’s Matrices, which are recom-
results. Unfortunately it was not possible to complete mended for children and those with learning
questionnaires with all the teachers in order to assess difficulties. The test was administered according to
the severity of the children’s autistic symptoms, since the test manual. As standardized scores are not
the teachers were already being asked to complete available for the full age range tested here, raw scores
questionnaires on the children’s everyday behaviour were used. Raw scores are also more comparable to
before and after training, and it was felt that the the verbal mental ages used for the TROG and
provision of more questionnaires would have been BPVS, which are not age-standardised.
overwhelming.
Children were randomly allocated either to one Test for Reception of Grammar (Bishop, 1989)
of two intervention conditions, the ToM training
group or the EF training group, or to a control Receptive grammar was assessed using the
group who received no intervention. Children from TROG (Bishop, 1989). This task involves presenting
the same school were distributed across groups, to the child with four pictures, and asking them to
control for any school effects. The CA, BPVS and indicate which picture goes with a sentence contain-
TROG VMA and Raven’s CPM (raw scores) are ing a grammatical construct. Items are arranged in
given in Table I. Every effort was made to match blocks of four, all of which test the same grammatical
for both school and VMA across these groups. construct, and a child is considered to have failed a
However, in several cases children were allocated to block if they fail a single item within the block. If the
the control group by default, by virtue of their child fails five consecutive blocks the test is discon-
760 Fisher and Happé

tinued. Raw scores (number of blocks) were con- were scored as passing the whole task only if they
verted to a VMA according to the manual. passed all three of these trials.

British Picture Vocabulary Scale (Dunn et al., 1999) Seeing Leads to Knowing
Receptive vocabulary was assessed using the In this task, based on Pratt and Bryant (1990),
BPVS II (Dunn et al., 1999). In this test, children are one of two toy characters see an object hidden in a
shown four pictures and asked to point to the picture box, whilst the other only touches the outside of the
that best tells the meaning of a word. Items become box. The child is asked who knows what is in the box.
progressively harder. The test is discontinued when a Children were credited with a pass if they gave
child makes eight errors in a group of 12. Raw scores the correct response to this question.
were converted to a VMA using the tables in the
manual. Knowing/Guessing (Based on Kazak, 1997)

Screening ToM Tasks This task was introduced by showing the child a
sealed envelope. The child was told that the
Two different FB tasks were used, counterbal- researcher had received this envelope in the post that
anced in order. One was a picture book version of the morning, and hadn’t looked inside it yet. The child
standard unexpected transfer task (Wimmer & Pern- was then shown the contents of the envelope whilst
er, 1983), modelled after Baron-Cohen et al.’s Sally- the researcher looked away, and said that she was not
Anne task (1985). The characters were a girl called looking. The child was then asked the following
Sally and a boy called David. Children were asked a questions, with the order of ‘‘know’’ and ‘‘guess’’
FB question, a justification question and two control counterbalanced:
(memory and reality) questions. They were only Self knowledgeable: Do you know what’s inside
credited with passing the FB question if they also the envelope, or do you have to guess?
passed both controls. Other ignorant: How about me, do I know
The second test was a standard deceptive box what’s inside the envelope, or do I have to guess?
task using a Smarties tube containing a pencil Another envelope was then introduced, and the
(Hogrefe, Wimmer, & Perner, 1986). Children were researcher looked inside without showing the child.
asked about another person’s FB. They were only They were then asked (with ‘‘know’’ and ‘‘guess’’
credited with passing the FB question if they also counterbalanced in order):
passed a reality control question. Self ignorant: Do you know what is inside the
If children failed both of the FB tasks, they were envelope, or do you have to guess?
included in the study, and if they passed both, they Other knowledgeable: How about me, do I know
were excluded. If children failed one out of the two what is inside the envelope, or do I have to guess?
tasks, they were given a further FB task, which was a This task was scored in two sections, ‘‘self’’ and
‘Sally-Anne’ type task, acted out with two toy animals. ‘‘other’’. Children were credited with passing only if
they passed both questions in a section (e.g. self
Non-Screening ToM Tasks ignorant and self knowledgeable).
Penny Hiding Deception Task
Children’s ‘‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’’ Task
In this task, based on Baron-Cohen (1992), a
(Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, Mortimore, & Robertson,
small object (a penny or a marble) was used. The
1997)
child must hide the object in one of his or her hands
behind her back, and then bring both hands forwards Children were given half of the children’s version
for the researcher to guess where the object is hidden. of this task, which involves choosing one of four
The task was introduced by three trials of the words to match the expression seen in a picture of
researcher hiding the small object. The child was someone’s eyes. The items included were selected by
given three trials, and marked as having passed a trial comparing scores on this task from an earlier study of
if they successfully put both hands behind their normally developing children (Fisher, Dunn, &
backs, hid the object in one hand, and brought both Hughes, unpublished manuscript), to create two
closed hands forwards, with the object hidden, and halves on which scores were comparable. Potential
did not tell the researcher where the object was. They scores therefore ranged from 0 to 14.
Training Theory of Mind and Executive Function 761

Non-ToM Control Task Percentage conceptual level responses, a measure


of what percentage of the time the participant was
False Photograph (Zaitchik, 1990)
sorting by the correct rule, was calculated as follows.
This task involved a toy character being in a box, All correct responses that occurred in runs of three or
and a photo being taken with an instant camera. The more were counted, and these were calculated as a
photo was placed face down on the table, and the percentage of total responses. Participants who
character was moved. The child was then asked where scored highly on this measure were therefore achiev-
the character would be when the photo was turned ing each category quickly with minimal trial and
over. They were counted as passing if they passed this error.
question and a memory control, which all children Percentage perseverative errors, a measure of a
passed. child’s tendency to remain ‘‘stuck’’ on an earlier
category of cards. This variable was calculated as the
Executive Function Tasks number of perserverative errors divided by the
number of total responses.
Card Sort
Perseverative responses had to be either unam-
This was a simplified version of the Wisconsin biguous (i.e. they were only matched by the perserv-
Card Sort Task. It was modified to make it shorter erative category), or, if they were ambiguously
and easier for the present sample of children with matched, had to be contained within runs of three
intellectual impairments. A pack of 27 cards was or more all sorted by the same category.
used. The cards varied along three dimensions, In the first category, the first unambiguous error
colour, number and shape. Three cards were placed became the ‘‘perseverated to’’ category. Thereafter,
in front of the child, with a single red triangle, two the ‘‘perserverated to’’ category was the previous
blue circles and three yellow rectangles on them. The category. If the participant made three unambiguous
children were told that they were going to play a matches in succession according to another rule, the
guessing game, and that the researcher was not ‘‘perserverated to’’ category changed to that rule.
allowed to tell them very much about how to play it, Ambiguous responses were allowed between the
they had to work it out for themselves. unambiguous ones, as long as they were sorted
They were asked to match each of the cards in the according to the same rule as the unambiguous
pile to one of the three in front of them, and told there responses.
was a rule that they must work out to get the matching A score was also given for alternative persever-
right. The researcher gave them immediate feedback ative strategies. These included placing all the cards
on their performance. Once they had matched six in a single pile, or dealing the cards onto the three
consecutive cards correctly, the cards were reshuffled, piles in order (e.g. from left to right), regardless of the
handed back to them and they were told that there feedback. A percentage error score was calculated as
would now be a new rule for them to work out. above. Very few children made such errors, and so in
If the child did not work out the rule within 24 order to avoid multiple comparisons, this count was
trials, they were told the rule and given 10 trials to added to the percentage perseverative error score
demonstrate their ability to match by the given calculated above to create a single variable of
dimension. If they sorted more than two cards percentage of perseverative errors.
incorrectly within those 10 trials, the task was The number of trials to complete the first category
discontinued. Otherwise, the cards were reshuffled was counted as a measure of speed of understanding
and they were asked to try and find a new rule. the nature of the task. This had a minimum of six and
If the child failed to work out two consecutive a maximum of 24, since if the child failed to get 6
rules, the task was discontinued. There were four correct within 24 responses they were told the rule.
rules, presented in the following order; Failure to maintain set was calculated as the
Colour—Shape—Number—Colour. number of times a participant made four correct
Scoring was as follows. responses but failed to get six right and therefore
Five variables were calculated from this task. complete the category. This variable was included in
Number of categories achieved, a general measure order to identify those children who appeared to find
of success on the task, was calculated by counting the a rule but then were unable to sort by it consistently.
number of categories the child achieved without help. To simplify comparisons a ‘‘Card Sort’’ aggre-
This could range from 0 to 4. gate was created combining scores from all five
762 Fisher and Happé

variables as follows. Each child was given a score of Pre-Training Testing


02 for each variable. Those who were in the top
Children were given a pre-test to establish
25% relative to the whole group were given 2, those
baseline measures of ToM, EF and general ability.
in the middle 50% were given 1, and those in the
The pre-training testing session, which lasted approx-
bottom 25% were given 0. These scores were then
imately one hour, was administered by a single
summed to make a Card Sort aggregate score. This
researcher in a quiet room in the child’s school. In
ranged from 0 to 10, with a mean of 4.58 (s.d.=2.45).
the case of some of the younger or least able children
(16 children in total) this was divided into two half
Trails Task (Reitan, 1958) hour sessions to maximize their concentration.
This standard task was administered according Theory of Mind and Executive Function Questionnaire
to the s procedure outlined in Reitan (1958), using 15
circles. Trails A uses just numbers which the child Teachers were asked to complete a question-
must connect in order, whilst Trails B uses numbers naire about the children both before training began
and letters which the child must connect alternately, and at follow-up. This questionnaire was designed
keeping both sequences in order. A difference score, using 15 items from previous studies (e.g. Frith,
reflecting the difficulty of switching set, is then Happé, & Siddons, 1994) that had been considered
calculated by subtracting the time for Trails A from to require a ToM. These included telling white lies,
the time for Trails B. taking things literally, recognising surprise or
Pre- and post-training measures were adminis- embarrassment and responding to indirect hints.
tered prior to beginning the intervention and within Fifteen items were included that were designed to be
2 days of completing the intervention. Follow-up indicative of executive functioning (Booth, Happé,
testing was completed between 6 and 12 weeks later Hughes, & Charlton, in prep.) for example, having
(mean time 9 weeks 4 days, s.d. 2 weeks 4 days), an ability to plan ahead, being able to do mental
according to the schools’ schedules and holidays. arithmetic and being able to follow verbally given
There were no differences in the delay between the lists of instructions. An additional five items were
training or control groups within each diagnostic included which were social items that were thought
condition (F(2,24) =0.45, p=.64). Table II gives a not to necessitate a ToM, for example having
summary of the tasks used at each time point, and appropriate table manners, and recognising happi-
what they were designed to test. Not all tasks were ness and sadness. These items went to form the
used at each time point in order to minimise exposure ‘‘general sociability’’ (G-S) scale. A scoring system
and effects of task repetition, following Swettenham similar to that used by Frith et al. (1994) was
(1996) and Bowler and Strom (1998) who found that utilised: 2 points for behaviour ‘‘definitely’’ shown, 1
repeating a false belief task was enough to effect an point if ‘‘sometimes’’ or ‘‘rarely’’ shown (and for
improvement in some children with autism. don’t know answers) and 0 for behaviours shown
Table II. Tasks Used at Each Timepoint

Pre-test and screening Post-test Follow-up


ToM
FB unexpected transfer FB unexpected transfer FB unexpected transfer
FB deceptive box self and other FB deceptive box self and other FB deceptive box self and other
Penny-hiding deception Penny-hiding deception
Seeing leads to Knowing Knowing/ Seeing leads to Knowing Knowing/
guessing self and other guessing self and other
ToM control
False photograph False photograph False photograph
EF
Flexibility and set shifting: Flexibility and set shifting: Flexibility and set shifting:
Modified Wisconsin Card Sort Card Sort Card Sort
Task (‘Card Sort’)
Set shifting: Trails task Set shifting: Trails task
Everyday behaviour
Teacher questionnaire Teacher questionnaire
Training Theory of Mind and Executive Function 763

‘‘not at all’’. These scores were compiled to give 3 Stage 1: Introduction of camera analogy. ‘‘When a
scales, two of which (the ToM and EF scales) person sees something, they have a thought in their
ranged from 030, and one of which (G-S) ranged head and we can say it’s like a picture.’’
from 0 to 10. Results from a larger sample of Stage 2: ‘‘Thought pictures can stay in a person’s
children with ASD using the same questionnaire head even if they go away. They can use the
found alpha values of .81 for the ToM scale and .77 thought picture to look for things.’’
for the EF scale, both of which indicate very good Stage 3: ‘‘Different people can have different
levels of consistency (Fisher, 2002). The alpha-value thought pictures in their heads’’
for the G-S scale was .08. The lower value for the Stage 4: ‘‘Sometimes thought pictures can be out-
G-S scale reflects the diverse nature of the items in of-date.’’
this scale: there was no theoretical reason why these Stage 5: ‘‘A thought is like a thought-picture, only
items should hang together since they were selected we can’t see it.’’
merely to tap non-ToM social behaviour.
Each of the stages consisted of demonstrations
Training Programmes
and questions to check the child understood the
There were two training programmes, consisting principles involved. The child was allowed to move
of 25-minute long individual sessions held on consec- onto the next stage once they had answered three
utive days. The training was to criterion, and so the consecutive trials correctly. At the end of some of the
children could take between 4 and 10 days to complete stages illustrated stories were given about real chil-
the programme. At the end of each day, the child and dren in scenarios that paralleled the concepts used at
the researcher discussed what had been learnt, and that stage. The children did not have to pass these to
wrote a sentence on a ‘‘reminder card’’. Each new move onto the next stage, but their responses were
session began with reading the sentence on the card, recorded. The training was designed to last between 5
and discussing what had been learnt the previous day. and 10 days. The longest duration was 8 days.
Brief outlines of the training programmes are given
below. Fuller descriptions of each stage, including the Materials
criteria that the child has to pass to progress pass each
stage, are given in the appendices. Two dolls with plastic heads were adapted for the
training. Slots were cut in the top of the dolls’ heads so
ToM Training that Polaroid photos could be inserted. Some Polar-
oid photos were prepared beforehand; others were
This training programme was based on that used taken during the course of the training. Objects used
by Swettenham et al. (1996), which taught a strategy during the training were a purple cylinder, an orange
of thinking about beliefs as ‘‘photos in the head’’. basket, two coloured boxes, an Elmo toy puppet, a toy
However, the programme was modified in a number Mr. Happy, a toy crocodile, a toy elephant, a small
of ways, as follows. A doll was used instead of a teddy bear. Stories were illustrated with line drawings.
mannequin’s head. Illustrative stories about children
in similar situations to those used in the training that
Executive Function Training
day were introduced at the end of most stages, and
two perspectives were introduced in stage 3. The Design
training was to criterion, and presentations were
The EF training paralleled the ToM programme
unlimited up to a maximum of ten 2025 minute
as closely as possible, having five stages on which the
sessions. If a child failed to reach criterion on one
child was trained to criterion. Whilst the ToM
day, the stage was repeated with different materials
programme used a ‘‘thoughts as pictures’’ analogy,
the next day until they passed it. The photos were
this programmes used a ‘‘brain as machine’’ analogy,
referred to throughout as ‘‘thought-pictures’’, and
introducing the idea of using different ‘‘brain tools’’
were linked explicitly to mental states throughout the
for different activities. The five rules to be learnt were
training.
as follows:
Design
Stage 1: People can do lots of things. Sometimes
The five rules to be learnt at each stage of the they need to change how they do things. They can
training were as follows: do this by changing their brain tools.
764 Fisher and Happé

Stage 2. Sometimes if we have been doing some- tasks used at each time point is given in Table III. In
thing for a long time, it’s hard to change our brain order not to repeat tasks too many times (and
tool. therefore to minimize practice effects), a reduced
Stage 3. Some brain tools are easier to use than number of tasks were administered at this stage.
others. Stop-Change-Go sequence for changing Different materials were used for each version of the
brain tools. task administered. Tasks were presented in a single
Stage 4. Sometimes we have to change our brain half hour session at each timepoint.
tools before we finish doing something.
Stage 5. Sometimes we have to decide for ourselves
RESULTS
what brain tools to use.
Baseline scores
Each session consisted of a demonstration and
practice, using cards. The child had to perform to Table III gives baseline scores for the three
criterion in order to move on to the next section. experimental groups on the ToM total score (made
These criteria were set using the binomial distribu- up of a sum of all seven ToM tasks), the Eyes task
tion, and were calculated so that there was less than a and EF measures (Card Sort aggregate score, Trails
5% probability of the child achieving that result by Difference score). The Card Sort aggregate score was
chance alone. The concept of changing tools was calculated from five variables, as described in the
introduced using a toy truck, which had a range of Methods Section.
changeable fitments that could be used to pick up One-way ANOVAs were performed to compare
stones, sand etc. There were illustrated stories at the the three experimental groups. No significant differ-
end of some stages, drawing parallels between the ences were found between groups (all p-values>.16).
exercises and real life. A ‘‘reminder card’’ was Comparisons of the separate Card Sort variables
completed at the end of each session, and was read also showed no significant differences (all p-values
at the beginning of each new session to refresh the >.24).
child’s memory of the previous exercises.
Effect of Training on ToM Performance
Materials Figure 1 shows the pre-test, post-test and follow-
A scale model yellow CAT truck with remov- up scores on the ToM aggregate. As slightly different
able tools was used. A toy plastic car, some twigs, tests were given at post-test and follow-up, the
and a collection of small stones, were used with the proportion of tests passed is used rather than raw
truck. A laminated, line drawn cross-section of a scores. Due to the small size of the groups and the
head, showing the brain, was used as the ‘‘brain non-parametric nature of the data, one tailed Wilco-
card’’. ‘‘Brain tools’’ were oval laminated pieces of xon’s Signed Ranks tests were used to compare
card with descriptions and illustrations for those performance at the different time points. The signif-
who couldn’t read (e.g. shapes on the ‘‘Shape’’ tool, icance values on the graph refer to a significant
numbers on the ‘‘Number’’ one). Several sets of difference between pre-test performance and the
laminated cards were used. These varied along relevant time point.
different and differing numbers of dimensions. The
Table III. Baseline Scores on the ToM Total Score, the Eyes Task
simplest ones varied along only two dimensions, e.g.
and EF Measures for the Six Experimental Groups
yellow and red circles and rectangles. The more
complicated ones used complex shapes and more ToM EF Control
dimensions, e.g. card colour, type of animal, number
ToM aggregate 2.40 2.10 2.29
of animals present. (1.51) (1.29) (0.95)
Eyes task 3.90 5.00 3.86
(1.91) (3.27) (0.69)
Post-Training Testing and Follow-up
Card Sort aggregate 4.60 5.80 3.86
The post-training testing session was adminis- (2.22) (3.29) (2.79)
Trails 78.26 45.88 36.65
tered one or two days after completion of the training
(48.98) (34.73) (25.63)
programme, and the follow-up tests were adminis-
tered between 6 and 12 weeks later. An outline of the Mean (s.d.).
Training Theory of Mind and Executive Function 765

100

of ToM tasks correct


80
** ToM trained

Proportion
60 *
EF trained
40 Controls
20

0
Pre-test Post-test Follow-up
Time

Fig. 1. Graph showing proportion of ToM tests passed at pre-test, post-test and
follow-up in the experimental groups **p<.01, *p<.05 as compared to pre-test
scores. The significance value at follow up refers to both the EF and ToM trained
groups.

Generalisation to ToM Tasks Other Than the Trained brackets gives the percentage who improved on a
Task ToM task other than the trained task.
Results on individual ToM tests were compared
using one-tailed Wilcoxon’s Signed Ranks tests in Eyes Task
order to examine whether the improvements seen in
The pre-test and follow-up group means for the
the ToM-trained group were due solely to improve-
Eyes task showed no significant difference when
ment on the trained (unexpected transfer) task.
compared by the Wilcoxon signed rank test (ToM
Between the pre-test and the post-test, the ToM-
groups pre-test mean 3.90, follow-up 5.10, EF group
trained group improved significantly on two out of
pre-test mean 5.00, follow-up 5.30, Control group
the four tasks administered, the trained ‘‘unexpected
pre-test mean 3.86, follow-up 4.29). 70% of the ToM
transfer’’ task (z=)2.65, p<.01) and the penny-
group performed better on the task at follow up than
hiding task (z=)1.89, p<.05). The EF-trained group
at pre-test, as did 50% of the EF group and 14% of
improved significantly on the penny-hiding task only
the Control group. Being rated positively for one or
(z=)2.24, p=.01). The Control group improved on
more additional items was counted as an improve-
the Deceptive box other task (z=)1.73, p<.05).
ment, since there were only 14 items in the task.
Between the pre-test and the follow-up, the
Fisher’s Exact test found a significant difference
ToM-trained group improved significantly only on
between the number of children who improved in the
the trained task (z=)2.12, p<.05). The EF-trained
ToM group and the Controls only (p<.05).
group improved on Deceptive box self (z=)1.63,
p<.05), Deceptive box other (z=)1.73, p<.05) and Non-ToM Control Task
Seeing Leads to Knowing (z=)2.00, p<.05), and
Performance on the false photo task was very
they got worse on the Know/guess self (z=)1.73,
high from the start, with 8/10 of both the ToM-
p<.05). The Controls improved on nothing.
trained and EF-trained groups passing the task at
The percentage of children in each group who
pre-test. The number of children who passed did not
improved on at least one ToM task (i.e. passed more
differ significantly between pre-test and post-test, or
ToM tasks at post-test or follow-up than they did at
pre-test and follow-up, in any of the three groups (all
pre-test) is shown in Table IV. The number in
p-values>.16), remaining high throughout.
Table IV. Percentage of Children in Each Group Who Improved
on the Proportion of ToM Tasks Passed Between Pre-test and Effects of Training on EF Performance
Post-test or Pre-test and Follow-up (Percentage Improving on a
Task other than the Unexpected Transfer Task) Card Sort

ToM EF Control At post-test: Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests found no


improvement on any of the card sort variables in the
Pre-test Þ post-test 80 (60) 50 (60) 43 (29)
Pre-test Þ Follow-up 60 (50) 50 (50) 14 (14)
EF-trained group (all p-values>.20). The ToM-
trained group improved between the pre-test and
766 Fisher and Happé

post-test on percentage perserverative errors of sorting a novel set of cards, to suggest ways that a
(z=)1.96, p=.05), as did the Controls (z=)2.37, boy could get to his house, and to suggest different
p<.05). options that they themselves had for activities after
school. These were all types of tasks that had been
At Follow-up: The ToM-trained group improved on practiced during the training. The number of
percentage conceptual level sorting (z=)2.09, responses to these three questions was summed,
p<.05), as did the controls (z=)2.37, p<.05). There giving a score with a mean of 8.59 (s.d.=4.02), and
were no improvements in the EF-trained group. a range of 116. Therefore, children who got a low
‘‘attempts’’ score could be said to have grasped the
Card Sort Aggregate: There was no improvement in training quickly, and children who got a high
the ToM-trained group between pre-test and post-test ‘‘generativity’’ score could be said to be demonstrat-
(pre-test mean=4.60, s.d.=2.22; post-test mean=6.20, ing flexibility across a range of questions.
sd=3.36; z=)1.92, p=.06), but there was significant The relationship between these variables and
improvement at follow-up (mean=6.30, s.d.=1.95; ability was calculated using Spearman’s correlations
z=)2.15, p<.05). There was no improvement in the (due to the small sample sizes). None of these
EF-trained group and a marginal improvement correlations were significant, which is unsurprisingly
between pre-test and follow up in the Controls (pre- due to the small sample size. However, several of the
test mean=3.86, s.d.=2.80, follow-up mean=5.30, r-values were moderate to large effect sizes, the
s.d.=2.93; z=)1.86, p=.06). largest being the correlation between the ‘‘Attempts’’
Fisher’s exact test found no difference between variable and the TROG (r=).42) and Raven’s
the percentage of each group who improved between Matrices (r=).37) There was no relationship at all
time points. between the ‘‘Generativity’’ variable and Raven’s
To summarise, there was no evidence for an matrices (r=.01).
improvement on the card sort task in the EF-trained In order to look at the relationship between
group. There was some evidence for an improvement success on the training and improvement on the ToM
in the ToM-trained group, but the controls showed a tasks, the group was split into those who improved
similar improvement and so it cannot be concluded on the proportion of ToM tasks passed, and those
that this improvement was due to the ToM training who did not, and compared on the EF training
programme. variables. There was a significant difference between
the groups on the Attempts measure (improvers
Trails mean=5.40, s.d.=0.55; non-improvers mean=7.60,
s.d.=2.61; z=)1.97, p<.05) and no difference on the
Wilcoxon Signed Ranks tests found no evidence
Generativity measure (improvers mean=5.40,
for improvement in the trails task in any of the three
s.d.=3.65; non-improvers mean=9.40, s.d.=3.78;
groups, and Fisher’s exact test found no significant
z=)1.78, p=.08).
differences between the percentage who improved on
There were no other differences when these two
the Trails task in any of the experimental groups.
groups were compared on ability and age (all p-
values>.08).
Executive Function Training and Theory of Mind To summarise, speed of reaching criteria during
the EF training was significantly related to improve-
As the children were trained, their responses
ment in ToM in the EF-trained group.
were recorded. These responses were used to create
two variables that gave a measure of their response to Who Improves in ToM?
training. The first was ‘‘Attempts’’; a sum of the
number of attempts taken by a child to meet the In order to identify factors that might have
criterion to pass each stage. There were five different influenced improvement in ToM, the participants
assessment points, and so the scores could potentially were divided into those who improved in ToM
range from five (passing on the first attempt at each between the pre-test and post-test (16) and those
stage) upwards. In reality the overall mean was 6.9 who did not (11), regardless of training groups. These
(s.d.=2.5), with a range of 513. The second was groups were compared on the ability scores, using
‘‘Generativity’’; a measure taken in the final testing non-parametric tests. The only significant difference
session, when the children were asked to suggest ways was on the TROG (z=)2.60, p<.01), with those who
Training Theory of Mind and Executive Function 767

did not improve on ToM having significantly lower learning over time (Hadwin et al., 1996; Swettenham,
TROG scores than those who did improve. All other 1996). This study differs from earlier work in a
p-values were greater than .24. Improving and non- number of particulars. A wide range of ToM tasks
improving groups did not differ on any of the EF or was included in the study both at pre-test and after
ToM measures at pre-test. There was a significant training, and some generalisation of learning to other
difference between the two groups on the Eyes task at tasks was evident, particularly to the Penny Hiding
follow-up (z=)2.18, p<.05), with those who deception task. Strenuous efforts were made to
improved in ToM doing better. minimise children’s repeated exposure to false belief
tasks, and there were no occasions during the training
Teacher Rating of Real Life Behaviour in which children were given feedback on a standard
unexpected transfer false belief task.
Teachers were asked to complete questionnaires
about the children’s behaviour before and after
training (at the time of the follow-up). Due to a fall Executive Function Training
off in teachers returning questionnaires, not all the Contrary to our predictions, there was no direct
children had questionnaires for both before and after effect of the EF training on the EF transfer tasks.
(22 children in total had both questionnaires, 8 in the This was not due to a failure to learn the strategy in
ToM group, 9 in the EF group and 5 controls). the training programme, as all children passed all
Two-tailed Wilcoxon’s Signed ranks test found the stages of the training. It may be due to an
no difference between the pre-test and post-test in inability to use this strategy in a test situation
any of the three groups. In order to look at the without support, an inability to see how the strategy
numbers who improved in each group, a difference might be useful in the test situation, or an inability
score was calculated by subtracting the score at pre- to initiate strategy use. It is also possible that the
test on each questionnaire from the score at follow- training programme trained the wrong strategy, and
up. There were no differences between the groups on did not target the specific aspects of the EF tasks
these difference scores. with which the children had difficulties. This train-
The participants were then divided into groups ing programme was deliberately strategy-based
according to who improved between the two ques- (rather than simply repetitively training children on
tionnaire time points and those who did not. About the tasks, which has been successfully used in some
50% of both the ToM- and EF-trained groups other studies with children with ADHD e.g. Sem-
improved between pre-test and follow-up on the run-Clikeman et al., 1999) because it was felt that
ToM scale, whilst none of the controls did. Fisher’s this was both more comparable to the ToM training
exact test was done to test for a significant difference programme and more likely to lead to genuine
between the groups, there was no significant differ- improvement in EF. Another factor which may have
ence between the ToM-trained and the Controls led to the lack of significant improvement in the EF-
(p=.10), nor between the EF-trained and the Con- trained group was that this group started off with
trols (p=.06). There were no differences between the slightly (if non-significantly) better EF than the
groups in the percentage who improved on the EF controls or ToM-trained group—3 of the EF-trained
and general sociability scales. group got 9 or 10 on the Card Sort aggregate score
at pre-test, whilst no one from either of the other
DISCUSSION groups scored more than 8. These children therefore
had very little scope for improvement on this task.
Theory of Mind Training The participants were assigned randomly to the
groups, without reference to their Card Sort perfor-
In line with our predictions and with a growing mance, so this possible ceiling effect was unavoid-
number of studies (Hadwin et al., 1996; McGregor able, if regrettable.
et al., 1998a; Swettenham, 1996; Swettenham et al., Despite this lack of improvement on EF tasks, it
1996; Wellman et al., 2001), this study demonstrated would be premature to cast this training programme as
that children with ASD could be taught to pass a failure, since the children did improve on tasks during
standard ToM tasks. This improvement was still training and progressed successfully through the stages
evident 612 weeks later, also in line with other of the programme, and it appeared to have a significant
studies that have assessed the maintenance of such effect on ToM performance, discussed below.
768 Fisher and Happé

Interactions between Theory of Mind and Executive from those who did not on measures of success
Function during the EF training. It seems likely, therefore,
that something specific about the EF training
None of the four projected scenarios described in
programme itself affected the children’s ToM per-
the introduction occurred, because the EF training
formance.
did not improve performance on the EF tasks.
If this is the case, what might it be about the EF
However, there was a significant improvement on
training programme that improved ToM without
the ToM tasks in the EF-trained group. This
improving performance on EF tasks? There are three
improvement was qualitatively different to that seen
basic possibilities as to how the EF training may have
in the ToM-trained group.
improved ToM performance: (i) The EF training may
As discussed above, the ToM training had an
be inadvertently teaching ToM rather than EF; (ii)
immediate effect, which was attenuated at follow-up,
the EF training might be boosting the children’s EF
whilst the EF training showed its main effects at
skills sufficiently to overcome executive difficulties in
follow-up. It may be that the effects of the EF
ToM tasks (which may have been impairing their
training on ToM performance are more indirect, and
performance), but not sufficiently to improve EF
that a ‘‘trickle-down’’ effect is occurring, where the
skills to the extent that we would see an improvement
children are gradually processing the information
in EF tasks; (iii) at a more developmental level, it is
learnt in the EF training programme, and are
possible that an improvement in EF facilitates ToM
applying it in ways that benefit their ToM perfor-
development, and the strategies which the children
mance. The set shifting which the children were
learnt during the training were sufficient to enable
trained on in the EF programme may have enabled
them to make leaps in ToM understanding. It seems
them to begin to make use of their experiences of
unlikely, however, that such short-term training as
seeing different perspectives, and this in turn may
this would have a developmental impact on under-
have lead to improvements in their ToM. It is striking
standing. A related suggestion is that both ToM and
that those who improved in ToM were no different to
EF may be sharing common cognitive structures at
those who failed to improve on general ability or
this point in development, which the EF training
vocabulary, but were significantly better at grammar
teaches as effectively as the ToM training. However,
than those who failed to improve, regardless of
if both EF and ToM shared common structures, we
training group. It seems possible that grammatical
would surely expect to see an improvement on EF
ability may be a mediating factor when training
tasks as well as on the ToM measures.
theory of mind.
It is possible that the EF training programme
There are a number of possibilities as to why
was inadvertently training ToM. In particular, the
the EF-trained group may have improved on ToM
‘‘real life’’ examples of flexibility (which included
tasks. Perhaps the most obvious explanation is non-
scenarios such as a child getting their brain tool
specific training effects on ToM. It is possible that
‘‘stuck’’ and therefore going to the wrong place out of
one-to-one sessions with the experimenter over a
habit) included elements of false belief—the brain
series of days improved performance on ToM tasks
tool was ‘‘stuck’’ because the child had failed to
regardless of the content of the training. An
acquire a new belief in place of the old one. The
unfortunate limitation of this study was that the
difference was that brain tools were couched in terms
control group received no intervention, rather than
of actions rather than mental states. It is possible that
receiving some sort of control training. However, in
this might be a particularly effective way to help
the past other non-ToM training programmes have
children with ASD think about mental states, since
failed to improve performance on false belief tasks
actions are something that they have direct experi-
(e.g. Hadwin et al., 1996; Steerneman et al., 1996)
ence of and can easily understand (e.g. Swettenham,
In addition, there seems no reason why non-specific
1996). It may also be the case that the EF training
training effects should particularly affect ToM tasks
programme drew the children’s attention to the
whilst not affecting EF tasks. In addition the
existence of the brain, and encouraged them to reflect
profile of improvement in the EF-trained group
on the thoughts behind their actions.
was quite different to that seen in the ToM-trained
The second possibility, that executive difficulties
group, indicating that different factors may be at
are masking competence in ToM, seems very unlikely
work. Finally, and perhaps most convincingly,
to be the case in children with ASD, who demonstrate
those who improved on ToM differed significantly
ToM difficulties across a range of tasks and in real
Training Theory of Mind and Executive Function 769

life (see Baron-Cohen, 2000 for a review). In addi- performance between testing sessions was wide in
tion, some of the tasks on which the EF-trained all of the experimental groups, and it is possible that
group improved (such as the Seeing-leads-to-Know- these EF tasks were simply a less stable measure than
ing task) do not appear to have a significant executive the ToM tasks in these groups, making it harder to
component. discern significant improvements.
The third possibility is that executive functioning Indirect effects in the opposite direction were not
and theory of mind are inter-related to the extent that evident. There was a slight tendency on the part of
training children in executive function will necessarily the ToM-trained group towards improvement on the
improve their theory of mind. For this we have to EF tasks, however, this only reached significance in
assume that the training is in some way improving the Card Sort, where the Control group also
executive function, even though this was not evident improved. It therefore seems unlikely that this was
in the EF post-tests. Russell suggests that executive directly to do with the ToM training.
problems in individuals with ASD are a result of a
failure to follow and shift between arbitrary rules or Real Life Measurements
cognitive frameworks, such as sorting cards by colour
The ‘‘before’’ and ‘‘after’’ questionnaires for
rather than shape in the WCST. He suggests that this
teachers (who were blind to group assignment) were
might be linked to mentalising because everyday
completed over a relatively short time scale (in most
thinking involves moving between cognitive frames,
cases 2 months), and it seemed unlikely that any
and an inability to do so would constrain thinking and
changes in real life behaviour would be seen over this
thus make the first person experience of thinking
time. However, there was some tentative indication
highly abnormal in ASD. The EF training in this task
that those who were trained were more likely to
did train children to shift between arbitrary rules, and
improve on the ToM scale, with over half of the
it is theoretically possible that it might be helping
trained children (4/8 of the ToM-trained group, 5/9
them to learn to shift between cognitive frameworks,
of the EF-trained group) showing some improvement
and therefore improve their mentalising abilities.
over time, as compared to none of the control group.
However, it is equally possible, as Perner (1998)
On the EF and general sociability scales there was no
argues, that ToM is an integral part of EF, and that
difference. However, a much larger sample size is
training on EF helped ToM because in fact the
needed over a longer period of time in order to really
elements of EF that the training programme targeted
investigate this issue.
were so close to ToM. It is not possible from this study
This study inevitably suffered from a range of
to distinguish between Russell’s (e.g. Russell, 2002)
limitations—in common which almost every other
and Perner’s (e.g. Perner & Lang, 2000) theories.
training study the sample size is very small. However,
In addition, both of these options leave us with
the sample size was not too small to find statistically
the open question of why the children did not
significant results (as compared to other studies of
improve on the EF tasks if they were learning to
this type), and was comparable with other training
represent causal schemas as representations (Perner,
studies of this type. In addition, it would have been
1998), or shift between cognitive frameworks (Rus-
useful to include a wider range of simpler tests of
sell, 2002), which is presumably what is needed for
executive function, since it seems plausible that the
tasks such as the Card Sort. It may be that the tasks
difficulty level of the EF and ToM tasks were not
are of differing levels of difficulty—certainly the EF
matched, and the improvement seen during the actual
tasks used would be hard for normally developing 4-
training sessions in the EF-trained group did not
year-olds, whilst the ToM tasks are developmentally
transfer to the post-test and follow-up EF tasks.
appropriate for that age group. In addition, the EF
tasks required the child to demonstrate their abilities
repeatedly (e.g. the multiple trials of the Card Sort),
or to maintain them over a period of time, whilst the CONCLUSION
ToM tasks only required a single trial. As others have
argued (e.g. Leslie & Polizzi, 1998) a true control for In conclusion, this study shows that after a
the executive demands of a ToM task would be a relatively brief ToM or EF intervention programme,
single trial of an EF task, not repeated trials. It may children with ASD were able to learn to pass FB
be that limitations of the tasks also account for the tasks and to show some degree of generalisation to
lack of a direct effect of training—variation in other, non-trained tasks. This learning was sustained
770 Fisher and Happé

at follow-up. There was tentative evidence also that from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger
Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and
both the EF and ToM training programmes were Allied Disciplines, 38(7), 813822.
associated with a real life improvement on behaviours Bishop, D. V. (1989). Test for reception of grammar. Cambridge:
thought to require a ToM. The success of this MRC Applied Psychology Unit.
Booth, R., Happé, F., Hughes, C., & Charlton, R. Executive
intervention in stimulating some generalisation of function and theory of mind in everyday life (in preparation).
learning relative to other studies may be due to the Bowler, D., & Strom, E. (1998). Elicitation of first-order ‘theory of
use of an alternative strategy for mentalising (as in mind’ in children with autism. Autism, 2(1), 3344.
Drewe, E. A. (1975). Go-no go learning after frontal lobe lesions in
Swettenham et al., 1996), as opposed to using humans. Cortex, 11(1), 816.
straightforward feedback on FB tasks (Swettenham, Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1999). The
1996) or a rule based learning procedure (Hadwin British Picture Vocabulary Scale. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
Fisher, N. (2002). The relationship of theory of mind to language
et al., 1996). and executive function in children with autism and children
Training in executive functioning improved chil- with moderate learning difficulties. Unpublished PhD thesis.
dren’s performance on a range of ToM tasks, despite University of London, London.
Fisher, N., Dunn, J., & Hughes, C. Advanced tests of theory of
not improving their performance on EF tasks. This mind: Are appearances deceptive? Findings from a study of 8-
may be because the training programme itself inad- year-olds (unpublished manuscript).
vertently trained some aspects of ToM, or because Frith, U., Happé, F. G. E., & Siddons, F. (1994). Autism and
theory of mind in everyday life. Social Development, 3,
executive function is causally related to theory of 108124.
mind, either as a precursor or because theory of mind Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and
is an integral part of executive control of action. rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10(4), 483527.
Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K. (1996). Can
In order to truly test the potential real life we teach children with autism to understand emotions, belief,
benefits of a training programme such as this, longer or pretence?. Development & Psychopathology, 8(2), 345365.
term, more consistent programmes are needed, with Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K (1997). Does
teaching theory of mind have an effect on the ability to develop
real life behaviour assessed through a variety of conversation in children with autism? Journal of Autism &
methods. In order to get a large enough sample size, Developmental Disorders, 27(5), 519537.
and in order to widen access to such programmes in Hautus, M. J., & Lee, A. J. (1998). The dispersions of estimates of
sensitivity obtained from four psychophysical procedures:
the future, these programmes would need to be Implications for experimental design. Perception & Psycho-
carried out by teachers or carers rather than research- physics, 60(4), 638649.
ers. The next step would therefore be to develop Heaton, R. K. (1981). Wisconsin card sorting test manual. Odessa,
FL: Psychological Assessment Resource Inc.
materials that could be used by teachers and carers of Hogrefe, G. J., Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1986). Ignorance versus
individuals with ASD, and to monitor the effective- false belief: A developmental lag in attribution of epistemic
ness of such programmes. states. Child Development, 57(3), 567582.
Kazak, S., Collis, G. M., & Lewis, V. (1997). Can young people
with autism refer to knowledge states? Evidence from their
understanding of ‘know’ and ‘guess’. Journal of Child Psy-
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS chology and Psychiatry, 38, 10011009.
Kerns, K. A., Eso, K., & Thomson, J. (1999). Investigation of a
The work reported here was carried out by direct intervention for improving attention in young children
with ADHD. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16(2), 273295.
Naomi Fisher as partial fulfilment of the requirements Leslie, A. M., & Polizzi, P. (1998). Inhibitory processing in the false
for a doctoral degree, during which she was supported belief task: Two conjectures. Developmental Science, 1(2),
by a Medical Research Council studentship. 247253.
McGregor, E., Whiten, A., & Blackburn, P. (1998a). Teaching
theory of mind by highlighting intention and illustrating
thoughts: A comparison of their effectiveness with 3-year olds
and autistic individuals. British Journal of Developmental
REFERENCES Psychology, 16(3), 367387.
McGregor, E., Whiten, A., & Blackburn, P. (1998b). Transfer of
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case the picture-in-the-head analogy to natural contexts to aid false
of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology belief understanding in autism. Autism, 2(4), 367387.
and Psychiatry, 30, 285298. Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1995). Teaching theory of mind: A new
Baron-Cohen, S. (1992). Out of sight or out of mind: Another look approach to social skills training for individuals with autism.
at deception in autism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psy- Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 25(4), 415433.
chiatry & Allied Disciplines, 33, 11411155. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive Functions and
Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: a fifteen year Developmental Psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology
review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen and Psychiatry, 37(1), 5187.
(Eds.), Understanding other minds. (2nd ed.). New York: Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and
Oxford University Press. executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 337344.
Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. Perner, J., & Lang, B. (2000). Theory of mind and executive
(1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence function: Is there a developmental relationship? In S.
Training Theory of Mind and Executive Function 771

Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Swettenham, J. (1996). Can children be taught to understand false
Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental belief using computers? Journal of Child Psychology & Psy-
cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 150181). Oxford: Oxford chiatry & Allied Disciplines, 37(2), 157165.
University Press. Swettenham, J. G., Baron-Cohen, S., Gomez, J.-C., & Walsh, S.
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Coloured progressive (1996). What’s inside someone’s Head? Conceiving of the
matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press. Mind as a Camera Helps Children with Autism Acquire an
Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an Alternative to a Theory of Mind. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry,
indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor 1(1), 7388.
Skills, 8, 271276. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Language and understanding minds:
Russell, J. (1997). How executive disorders can bring about an Connections in autism. In D. Cohen (Ed.), Understanding
inadequate ‘theory of mind’. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuro-
executive disorder (pp. 256304). New York, USA: Oxford science (2nd ed., pp. 124149). Oxford: Oxford University
University Press. Press.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., Nielsen, K. H., Clinton, A., Sylvester, L., Wellman, H. M., Baron-Cohen, S., Caswell, R., Gomez, J. C.,
Parle, N., & Connor, R. T. (1999). An intervention approach Swettenham, J., Toye, E., & Lagattuta, K. (2001). Using
for children with teacher- and parent-identified attentional thought-bubbles helps children with autism acquire an alter-
difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(6), 581590. native to a theory of mind. Paper presented at the Meetings of
Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., & Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is the Cognitive Development Society, Norfolk VA.
a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyper- Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Represen-
activity disorder? Behavioural Brain Research, 130(12), tation and constraining functions of wrong beliefs in young
447471. children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103128.
Shimmon, K., & Lewis, C. (2001). Can children with autism be Zaitchik, D. (1990). When representations conflict with reality: The
trained on the tower of London task? Paper presented at the preschooler’s problem with false beliefs and ‘‘false’’ photo-
Poster presented at BPS Developmental and Educational graphs. Cognition, 35(1), 4168.
Sections Conference, Worcester, UK.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen