Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

Pain 99 (2002) 91–99

www.elsevier.com/locate/pain

Blinding effectiveness and association of pretreatment expectations with


pain improvement in a double-blind randomized controlled trial
Judith A. Turner a,b,*, Mark P. Jensen b, Catherine A. Warms b, Diana D. Cardenas b
a
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Box 356560, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle, WA 98195-6560, USA
b
Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Box 356490, University of Washington School of Medicine, Seattle, WA 98195-6490, USA
Received 11 December 2001; accepted 27 February 2002

Abstract
Patient, provider, and clinical investigator expectations concerning treatments are believed to play important roles in patient response. This
study examined the association of patient and research nurse/physician pretreatment expectations of pain relief with actual pain relief, the
accuracy of patient and research nurse guesses about patient medication assignment, and changes in research nurse and patient pain relief
expectations over the course of a randomized double-blind trial of amitriptyline versus an active placebo for patients with chronic pain and
spinal cord injuries (SCI). Patient expectations of pain relief with amitriptyline were associated significantly with actual pain decrease for
patients in the amitriptyline, but not placebo, condition. Research nurse/physician expectations did not predict patient pain relief. Both
patients and the research nurse were able to guess patient medication assignment at a rate significantly greater than chance. The research
nurse’s, but not the patients’, expectations of pain relief with amitriptyline decreased significantly over the course of the study. These findings
have implications for future randomized controlled trials. Fully double-blind conditions are very difficult to achieve, and it is informative to
assess patient and research clinician expectations and guesses regarding medication assignment. q 2002 International Association for the
Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Expectations; Blinding; Randomized controlled trial; Nonspecific effects; Pain

1. Introduction Surprisingly, however, little research has prospectively


examined whether patient and clinician expectations of
The role of nonspecific treatment effects (those attributa- degree of pain relief with the study treatment predict actual
ble to factors other than specific active components of the pain relief. One of the few such studies found that physician,
treatment; also referred to as placebo effects) in the but not patient, expectations of amount of pain amelioration
outcomes of patients receiving interventions for pain has immediately following a pain-relieving procedure were
been the subject of much interest and controversy. Patient associated significantly with patients’ actual pain relief
and health care provider expectations concerning treatments (Galer et al., 1997). A recent study of patients with low
are believed to be particularly important (Turner et al., back pain randomly assigned to receive acupuncture or
1994; Turner, 2001). Studies conducted more than two massage therapy found that patients with higher pretreat-
decades ago found that patients’ expectations concerning ment expectations for the treatment they received were
the effects of a medication were significantly associated more likely to have improved function after treatment
with their responses to that medication (Luparello et al., (Kalauokalani et al., 2001). Further examination of the asso-
1970; Macdonald et al., 1980). More recently, clients’ posi- ciations of patient and provider pretreatment expectations of
tive expectancies of treatment have been shown to be asso- pain relief with patient outcomes in different patient popu-
ciated with improvement in psychotherapy studies (Safren lations and with different treatments is needed to increase
et al., 1997). Provider expectations related to study medica- scientific understanding of the role of such expectations in
tions have also been found to be associated with patients’ pain treatment response. It is also important to study expec-
responses to treatment (Shapiro et al., 1954; Gracely et al., tations of other study personnel, such as research nurses,
1985). who have contact with patients in clinical trials. Concern
has been expressed that research nurses in such trials might
influence patients’ responses by behaviors reflecting expec-
* Corresponding author. Tel.: 11-206-543-3997; fax: 11-206-685-1139.
E-mail address: jturner@u.washington.edu (J.A. Turner). tations based on their experience of other patients’ reactions

0304-3959/02/$20.00 q 2002 International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0304-395 9(02)00060-X
92 J.A. Turner et al. / Pain 99 (2002) 91–99

(McQuay et al., 1995). To our awareness, however, no abnormalities in a screening electrocardiogram (ECG),
studies have reported whether research nurse expectations hyperthyroidism, glaucoma, unwilling to use a contracep-
change significantly over the course of a study. tive during the drug trial (if female with childbearing poten-
Patient and provider guesses concerning patient treatment tial), pregnant, currently on any type of antidepressant
assignment may also potentially influence the results of medication, current consumption of more than two alcoholic
double-blind randomized clinical trials. Such studies have drinks per day, and current major depressive episode.
long been thought to be the only method of establishing the The mean age of the sample was 41.43 years (SD 10.02,
effect of a treatment above and beyond natural history and range 21–64) and 80% were male. Median duration of SCI
nonspecific or placebo effects, with the double-blind was 13.4 years (range 8.5 months to 42.9 years). Thirty
designed to prevent clinical investigator and patient knowl- percent were college graduates. There were no significant
edge of treatment condition from biasing the results. differences between the amitriptyline and placebo groups in
However, even in randomized double-blind controlled age, gender, education, neurological level of injury, etiology
trials, it is possible that the efficacy of the active treatment, of injury, duration of SCI, ethnic group, marital status, or
the lack of efficacy of the placebo, the lack of side effects of employment status.
the placebo, or the side effects of the active treatment may
‘unblind’ the patient and/or the investigators. Indeed, multi- 2.2. Study design and procedures
ple studies have documented that patients (Marini et al.,
1976; Brownell and Stunkard, 1982; Byington et al., This was a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled
1985; Hughes and Krahn, 1985; Rabkin et al., 1986; trial to evaluate the efficacy of amitriptyline in relieving
Moscucci et al., 1987; Margraf et al., 1991), study clinicians chronic pain associated with spinal cord injury. Benztropine
(Brownell and Stunkard, 1982; Byington et al., 1985; mesylate was chosen as an active placebo based on its abil-
Rabkin et al., 1986; Margraf et al., 1991), and study evalua- ity to produce dry mouth, a common side effect of amitripty-
tors (White et al., 1992; Carroll et al., 1994) can correctly line. Study participants were solicited through mailing lists,
guess patients’ treatments at high rates, even under double- notices placed in clinics serving patients with SCI, and arti-
blind conditions. cles in newsletters. Potential participants were screened in a
The present study was conducted to examine the role of telephone interview conducted by the research nurse to
patient and research nurse/physician expectations regarding determine if they met the inclusion/exclusion criteria. The
pain relief in patient treatment response, as well as the University of Washington Human Subjects Review
accuracy of and reasons for patient and research nurse Committee and the Institutional Review Board of Good
guesses as to patient treatment assignment, in a randomized, Samaritan Hospital, Puyallup, WA, USA, approved the
double-blind trial. The study was part of an investigation of study, and each participant gave written informed consent.
the pain-relieving effects of amitriptyline versus an active The consent form included information concerning random
placebo (benztropine mesylate) among individuals with assignment to amitriptyline or benztropine mesylate, and
spinal cord injury (SCI) and chronic pain. The efficacy of the possible side effects associated with these medications
amitriptyline in this study was reported in a separate paper (dry mouth, constipation, sedation, lightheadedness, blurred
(Cardenas et al., 2002). The objectives of the present study vision, palpitations, changes in heart rate).
were to examine: (1) the association of patient and research Forty-four patients were assigned randomly to amitripty-
nurse/physician pretreatment expectations of pain relief line and 40 to placebo. Although individuals who met
with actual pain relief, (2) the accuracy of and reasons for psychiatric diagnostic criteria for a current major depressive
patient and research nurse end-of-study guesses about episode were excluded from the study, we expected that
patients’ actual treatments, and (3) the change in research some subjects would have some depressive symptoms.
nurse and patient expectations about pain relief over the Because it was possible that subjects with some depressive
course of the study. symptoms might respond differently to amitriptyline, we
stratified randomization by low versus high level of depres-
sive symptom severity based on the Center for Epidemiolo-
2. Methods gical Studies-Depression Scale scores (Radloff, 1977). The
cutoff ð,16 versus $16Þ recommended for depression case
2.1. Study participants identification was used (Radloff, 1977).
Random assignment to medication group and provision of
A complete description of the 84 study participants and medication was performed by the University of Washington
the study procedures was provided by Cardenas et al. Medical Center Pharmacy Investigational Drug Services. A
(2002). The study inclusion criteria were SCI more than 6 7-day supply of medication was provided to each patient
months ago, pain for at least 3 months, and average pain each of the 6 study weeks in identical gelatin capsules so
rating in the last month of at least 3 on a scale of 0 to 10. The that study personnel and patients were blind to medication
exclusion criteria were as follows: less than 18 or more than assignment. All patients, regardless of medication assign-
65 years of age, cardiovascular disease, seizure disorder, ment, were instructed to take a single capsule 1–2 h before
J.A. Turner et al. / Pain 99 (2002) 91–99 93

bedtime each evening. This single capsule contained the ing their pain on a scale from 0 ¼ not at all to 10 ¼
fixed dose of benztropine mesylate (held constant at 0.5 complete pain relief.
mg/day throughout the study) or the appropriate dose of
amitriptyline (from 10 to 125 mg/day). The amitriptyline 2.3.1.2. Posttreatment guess. At the end of the medication
dose schedule protocol was as follows: week 1, 10 mg/ trial, before unblinding, patients were asked to guess what
day; week 2, 25 mg/day; then increasing each week there- drug they were on, amitriptyline or placebo, and to rate their
after by 25 mg/day to a possible maximum dose of 125 mg/ certainty of this guess on a scale from 0 ¼ not at all to 10 ¼
day. Amitriptyline dosage was titrated to the level of extremely certain. They were then asked whether this guess
complete pain relief or maximal dose (up to 125 mg/day) was based on pain relief (this could include no pain relief as
tolerated over the 6-week trial. well as some degree of pain relief) and whether the guess
All patients were telephoned by the research nurse on day was based on side effects (this could include no or minimal
4 or 5 of each week to assess pain and medication side side effects as well as greater side effects). Patients who
effects. The nurse then communicated with the pharmacy indicated that they guessed based on pain relief were
as if each patient was on amitriptyline. If the patient asked to rate how much pain relief they received on a
reported bothersome side effects, the nurse asked the phar- scale from 0 ¼ no relief to 100 ¼ complete relief. Patients
macy to hold the medication dosage at the current week’s who indicated that they made their guess based on side
level. If reported side effects were intolerable, the nurse effects were asked whether this was based on severity or
asked the pharmacy to decrease the dosage to the previous type of side effect, or both.
week’s level. If side effects were minimal or tolerable, the
2.3.1.3. Outcome measure. Patients were telephoned by a
pharmacy was instructed to increase the dosage per the
research assistant three times in the week prior to beginning
study protocol. If the medication was benztropine mesylate,
medication and in the last week on medication, and asked to
the pharmacy made no change in dose over the trial. The
rate their average pain in the past 24 h on a scale from 0 ¼
pharmacy did not inform the research nurse of the patient’s
no pain to 10 ¼ pain as bad as could be. The three ratings
medication or changes made. Two patients experienced
each week were averaged to obtain single scores for
symptoms of sufficient severity to warrant both medication
pretreatment and posttreatment average pain intensity. The
discontinuation and unblinding prior to the end of the study;
outcome measure was pretreatment to posttreatment change
patient- and research nurse-completed posttreatment
in average pain intensity.
measures (described below) were not completed for these
individuals. Nine (11%) patients discontinued medication
2.3.2. Investigator-completed measures
(eight due to symptoms and one due to inability to adhere
to the study protocol) prior to the end of the 6-week trial, but
2.3.2.1. Treatment expectations. At the beginning of the
neither the research nurse nor the patients were unblinded
medication trial, the research nurse (or, in the case of six
and both the nurse and the patients completed the posttreat-
subjects recruited from Good Samaritan Hospital in
ment measures.
Puyallup, Washington, the patient’s physician) guessed
All patient-rated measures were completed in telephone
how much pain relief the patient would obtain if he/she
interviews conducted by research assistants blind to patient
received amitriptyline and if he/she received placebo, on a
medication group assignment. Patients completed measures
scale from 0 ¼ 0% to 10 ¼ 100% pain relief. This measure
prior to randomization and again at the end of the trial,
was added to the study protocol after the study began; thus,
before unblinding. Each study participant was paid
it was available only for the last 59 (70%) study patients.
US$100 for completing the study.
2.3.2.2. Posttreatment guess. At the end of the medication
2.3. Measures trial, before unblinding, the research nurse completed a form
on which she indicated what drug she thought the patient
2.3.1. Patient-completed measures had received and her certainty about this guess (on a scale
from 0 ¼ not at all certain to 10 ¼ extremely certain). The
2.3.1.1. Treatment expectations and history of amitriptyline nurse also indicated whether the guess was based on the
use. Before starting medication, all patients guessed what amount of pain relief (this could include no pain relief as
their pain relief at the end of the trial would be if they well as pain relief), the side effects (this could include no or
received amitriptyline and if they received placebo, on minimal side effects as well as greater side effects), and/or
scales from 0 ¼ 0% to 10 ¼ 100% pain relief. In addition, other reasons. If the guess was based on the amount of pain
patients were asked whether they had ever taken amitripty- relief, the nurse rated the amount of pain relief from 0 to
line (both generic and brand names were mentioned in the 100%. If the guess was based on side effects, the nurse
question) before, and if so, the reasons for taking it and indicated whether this was based on side effect severity,
whether they had a pain problem at the time. Patients who type, or both.
said they had a pain problem at the time they were on
amitriptyline were asked to rate how helpful it was in reliev- 2.4. Data analysis. The distributions of all measures were
94 J.A. Turner et al. / Pain 99 (2002) 91–99

inspected to ensure they met the assumptions of the nonsignificant difference (x2 ¼ 2:23, P ¼ 0:14). The most
statistical tests performed. Patient expectations of common side effects or symptoms were identical in the two
posttreatment pain relief with placebo were highly groups (from most to least common: dry mouth; drowsiness,
skewed; therefore, log transformed scores of this rating tiredness, or fatigue; constipation; increased spasticity; urin-
were used for all parametric analyses that involved this ary retention; and sweating). Among patients who remained
variable and the Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test on medication through the end of the trial, there were no
was used to compare patient expectations of pain relief with statistically significant differences between those on amitrip-
amitriptyline versus placebo. Patient and research nurse tyline and those on placebo in frequency or severity of any
ratings of amount of patient pain relief with placebo were side effect except for higher severity of increased spasticity in
also significantly skewed, and square-root transformations the amitriptyline group (t ¼ 3:33, P ¼ 0:005).
of these variables were used in analyses. Regression
analyses were performed to examine whether patient 3.1. Treatment expectations
expectations, research nurse/physician expectations, and
the interaction of expectations with assigned medication Patient and research nurse/physician expectation ratings
predicted posttreatment pain intensity, after adjusting for are shown in Table 1. Not surprisingly, both patients and
pretreatment pain intensity and assigned medication investigators had significantly (P , 0:001) higher expecta-
(forced entry). Chi-square tests were used for comparisons tions of pain relief with amitriptyline than with placebo. The
of categorical variables, and t-tests were used for investigators had higher expectations of both amitriptyline
comparisons involving patient and investigator ratings. To and placebo than did the patients, with these differences
examine whether patient and research nurse expectations reaching statistical significance (P , 0:05) for expectations
changed over the course of the study, we used t-tests to concerning pain relief with placebo.
compare the first versus the last 15 ratings made by the
research nurse, and the ratings made by those first 15 3.2. Do patient pretreatment expectations predict pain
versus last 15 patients. relief?

The placebo and amitriptyline groups did not differ


3. Results significantly in their pretreatment expectations of pain relief
if assigned to amitriptyline. Table 2 shows the results of the
Results of comparisons of the two study medications in regression analysis predicting posttreatment average pain
terms of efficacy for pain relief were reported separately intensity from pretreatment average pain intensity, medica-
(Cardenas et al., 2002). We summarize the results briefly tion group, expectations of pain relief with amitriptyline,
here, to serve as context for the findings relevant to expecta- and the interaction between medication group and expecta-
tions and medication guesses. At baseline, there were no tions. Pretreatment average pain intensity explained a large
significant differences between the active placebo and proportion of the variance in posttreatment average pain
amitriptyline groups on the measures of depressive symptom intensity, and neither medication group nor expectations
severity and average pain intensity. The sample showed of pain relief with amitriptyline made a significant addi-
significantly decreased average pain intensity over the 6- tional contribution. However, there was a significant inter-
week trial (pretreatment mean ðSDÞ ¼ 5:27 (1.79), posttreat- action between medication group and expectations. In the
ment mean ðSDÞ ¼ 4:25 (1.98); t ¼ 6:98, P , 0:001), with group of patients who actually received amitriptyline, their
no significant difference between the amitriptyline and active expectations of pain relief with amitriptyline were asso-
placebo groups. Over the course of the study, one or more ciated significantly and moderately with pre- to posttreat-
side effects, or increased severity of an existing symptom, ment change in pain intensity (r ¼ 0:37, P ¼ 0:02).
were reported by 43 of the 44 (97.7%) patients on amitripty- Expectations of greater pain relief predicted greater
line and 36 of the 40 (90.0%) patients on the placebo, a improvement. In the group of patients who actually received

Table 1 Table 2
Patient and research nurse/physician pretreatment expectations of pain Prediction of posttreatment pain intensity from pretreatment pain intensity,
relief with amitriptyline and placebo drug group, expectation of pain relief with amitriptyline, and interaction of
expectation and medication group
If amitriptyline If placebo
Predictor R2 R 2 change F change P
Rater Mean (SD) Median Mean (SD) Median
Posttreatment pain intensity
Patient 4.61 (2.67) a 5a 1.45 (2.42) b 0b Pretreatment pain intensity 0.55 0.55 87.55 0.000
Nurse/physician 5.76 (1.64) c 6c 2.61 (1.67) c 2c Drug group 0.55 0.00 0.18 0.67
a Expectation of pain relief 0.56 0.01 1.23 0.27
n ¼ 75, excluding nine subjects who gave ‘don’t know’ responses.
b with amitriptyline
n ¼ 82, excluding two subjects who gave ‘don’t know’ responses.
c Drug group £ Expectation 0.60 0.04 5.95 0.02
n ¼ 59.
J.A. Turner et al. / Pain 99 (2002) 91–99 95

placebo, their expectations of pain relief with amitriptyline both groups, and not significantly different between the two
were not significantly associated with change in pain inten- groups (placebo mean 7.58, SD 2.52, median 8, range 0–10;
sity (r ¼ 20:16). amitriptyline mean 6.43, SD 3.03, median 7.50, range 0–
The regression analysis was repeated for patient expecta- 10).
tions of pain relief with placebo. These expectations were Patients’ guesses, guess certainty, and reasons for guesses
not significantly associated with posttreatment pain inten- are summarized in Table 3. Fewer patients in the amitripty-
sity after adjusting for pretreatment pain intensity, and the line (68%) than in the placebo (85%) group said their guess
interaction between expectations and drug group was not was based on the amount of pain relief, and more patients in
significant. However, it should be noted that most patients the amitriptyline (68%) than in the placebo (53%) group
had very low expectations of pain relief with placebo and said their guess was based on side effects. However, neither
thus there was little variance in this measure. difference was statistically significant. Among the patients
who said their guess was based on pain relief, average
3.3. Do research nurse/physician pretreatment expectations reported pain relief was moderate (mean 41.08, SD 35.67,
predict pain relief? median 35, range 0–100% pain relief) in the amitriptyline
group and significantly lower (mean 22.61, SD 32.53,
To examine the extent to which research nurse/physician median 5, range 0–100%) in the placebo group (t ¼ 2:11,
expectations were associated with pre- to posttreatment P ¼ 0:04). Among patients who reported that their guess
changes in patient pain intensity, we used a regression was based on side effects, a higher proportion of the amitrip-
model. The dependent variable was posttreatment average tyline (60%) than the placebo (45%) group indicated that
pain intensity and the independent variables were pretreat- their guess was based on side effect severity. A somewhat
ment average pain intensity, medication group, research higher proportion of amitriptyline (92%) than placebo
nurse/physician pretreatment expectations of pain relief, (85%) group patients indicated that they guessed based on
and the interaction between medication group and expecta- side effect type. Neither difference was statistically signifi-
tions. Pretreatment average pain intensity made a large and cant.
statistically significant contribution to the prediction of
posttreatment average pain. None of the other predictors
was associated with posttreatment pain. 3.5. Research nurse guesses regarding patient medication
assignment
3.4. Patient guesses regarding medication assignment
Table 3 summarizes the research nurse’s guesses of
At the end of the 6-week trial, 62.5% of the 80 patients patient treatment assignment, guess certainty, and reasons
who made guesses as to what medication they received were for the guess. There was a significant association between
correct (x2 ¼ 5:12, P ¼ 0:02). (Guesses were not made by the research nurse’s guess and the actual study medication
the two people who were unblinded before the end of the (x2 ¼ 18:80, P ¼ 0:000). She correctly guessed 73% of
trial, one person who could not be contacted before unblind- medication assignments in the amitriptyline group and
ing, and one person who guessed ‘don’t know’). The accu- 75% in the placebo group. The nurse’s certainty of her
racy rate was higher in the amitriptyline (70%) than in the guess was somewhat higher for subjects in the placebo
placebo group (55%). The certainty of guess, regardless of than in the amitriptyline group (placebo mean 5.38, SD
whether it was correct, was moderately high on average in 2.65, median 5.50, range 0–9; amitriptyline mean 4.49,

Table 3
Patient and research nurse guesses of treatment assignment, certainty of guess, and reasons for guess

Actual treatment Guess n Certainty of guess Guess based on a (%)

Mean (SD) Pain relief Side effects

Patient guesses
Amitriptyline Amitriptyline 28 6.75 (2.73) 61 75
Placebo 12 5.67 (3.65) 83 50
Placebo Amitriptyline 18 7.28 (3.01) 83 61
Placebo 22 7.82 (2.08) 86 46

Research nurse guesses


Amitriptyline Amitriptyline 30 4.37 (2.41) 43 77
Placebo 11 4.82 (1.89) 55 73
Placebo Amitriptyline 10 3.50 (2.42) 70 70
Placebo 30 6.00 (2.45) 73 77
a
Percentage values. Patients and the research nurse were allowed to base their guesses on more than one factor.
96 J.A. Turner et al. / Pain 99 (2002) 91–99

SD 2.27, median 5, range 0–9), but this difference was not (14%) said they took it for depression, 13 (59%) said they
statistically significant. had taken it for pain, five (23%) said they had taken it for
The nurse indicated that her guess was based on the sleep, and three (14%) said they had taken it for miscella-
amount of pain relief for a higher proportion of patients in neous other reasons. These categories were not mutually
the placebo (73%) than in the amitriptyline (46%) group. exclusive. Nineteen (86%) said they had a pain problem
For guesses based on pain relief, the nurse ratings of amount while on amitriptyline. In this group, the average reported
of pain relief were significantly higher in the amitriptyline pain relief from amitriptyline in the past was 2.61 (SD 2.95)
than in the placebo group (amitriptyline mean 45.88, SD on the 0 (not at all) to 10 (complete relief) scale. Patients
34.83, median 50, range 0–90; placebo mean 16.30, median who had taken amitriptyline in the past, as compared with
0, SD 24.20, range 0–80; t ¼ 3:06, P ¼ 0:004). those who had not, had significantly lower pretreatment
The nurse indicated that her guess was based on side expectations of pain relief with amitriptyline (mean ðSDÞ ¼
effects in comparable and high proportions of patients on 3:24 (2.36) versus 5.17 (2.61), t ¼ 22:90, P ¼ 0:005).
amitriptyline (76%) and placebo (75%). Guesses based on Patients who took amitriptyline previously also showed a
side effects were said to reflect side effect severity (which stronger association between pretreatment expectations of
could be either low or high) in more placebo (69%) than pain relief with amitriptyline and pre- to posttreatment
amitriptyline (52%) group patients and side effect type in change in pain intensity (r ¼ 0:28, P ¼ 0:11) than did
more amitriptyline (83%) than placebo (52%) group patients. those who did not or were unsure whether they had taken
Patient and nurse guesses were significantly associated it previously (r ¼ 0:05, P ¼ 0:36), but neither correlation
with one another (x2 ¼ 9:37, P ¼ 0:002) in the whole was statistically significant. Patients who had versus had not
sample. Further analysis revealed that patient and nurse taken amitriptyline previously did not differ significantly in
guesses were associated significantly only for the patients pre- to posttreatment change in pain intensity (t ¼ 21:35,
assigned to placebo (x2 ¼ 6:60, P ¼ 0:01), not for the P ¼ 0:18). Furthermore, patients who had taken amitripty-
amitriptyline group. line in the past were no more likely than those who had not
or were unsure to correctly guess their medication assign-
3.6. Changes in expectations over the course of the study ment at the end of the trial (x2 ¼ 0:02, P ¼ 0:90).
Patient and research nurse ratings of expectations of pain
relief with amitriptyline and with placebo for the first and
last 15 patients are summarized in Table 4. Patient expecta- 4. Discussion
tions concerning amitriptyline and research nurse expecta-
In this double-blind, randomized controlled trial of
tions regarding the placebo did not change significantly over
amitriptyline versus an active placebo, SCI patient pretreat-
time. However, there were significant decreases over the
ment expectations of pain relief from amitriptyline were
course of the study in patient expectations concerning the
significantly associated with pre- to posttreatment change
amount of pain relief with placebo (t ¼ 2:52, P , 0:05) and
in average pain intensity only for those who actually
the research nurse’s expectations concerning the efficacy of
received amitriptyline. This association, which was moder-
amitriptyline (t ¼ 2:90, P , 0:01).
ate in size, may reflect an influence of expectations on pain
3.7. Association of prior amitriptyline use with patient experience during active treatment (i.e. an enhancement of
expectations and guesses the pain-relieving effects of amitriptyline by positive expec-
tations), an influence of expectations on responses to the
At baseline, 22 (26.2%) of the 84 patients said that they measures of pain intensity (i.e. a bias towards reporting a
had been on amitriptyline in the past, 53 (63.1%) said they change consistent with that predicted), the ability of patients
had not, and nine (10.7%) were unsure. Among the 22 to accurately predict their responses to amitriptyline, or
patients who had taken amitriptyline in the past, three some combination of these factors.

Table 4
Changes over time in patient and research nurse expectations of pain relief with amitriptyline and placebo

Patient expectations of relief Nurse expectations of relief

With amitriptyline With placebo a,* With amitriptyline** With placebo

Study period Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

First 15 patients 3.67 2.46 2.29 3.12 6.20 1.74 2.60 1.30
Last 15 patients 4.15 3.72 0.29 1.07 4.60 1.24 2.60 1.64
a
Means are shown in table, but log transformed scores were used in analyses. *Difference between first 15 and last 15 patients statistically significant,
t ¼ 2:52, P , 0:05; **Difference between first 15 and last 15 patients statistically significant, t ¼ 2:90, P , 0:01.
J.A. Turner et al. / Pain 99 (2002) 91–99 97

Pretreatment expectations of the research nurse (or, in six had taken amitriptyline previously would have been statis-
cases, the patient’s physician) did not predict patient tically significant if the number of patients in the group had
response. Accurate prediction of individual patient response been larger. Patients who had taken amitriptyline in the past
is hampered by the lack of scientific data concerning the were no more likely than those who had not to correctly
types of pain most responsive to amitriptyline in this popu- guess their medication assignment at the end of the trial.
lation and the fact that most patients have more than one These findings point to the importance of assessing subjects’
pain problem (e.g. spinal cord injury pain and mechanical prior experiences with medications under study in clinical
spine pain). Although there is evidence that amitriptyline is trials of drugs that subjects may have taken previously, and
efficacious for postherpetic neuralgia and diabetic neuropa- of examining associations of this with subject expectations
thy pain (McQuay et al., 1996), little is known concerning and outcomes.
whether amitriptyline is efficacious for SCI neuropathic The research nurse was more accurate than were the
pain or other SCI pain syndromes (Finnerup et al., 2001). patients in guessing patient drug assignment. This was due
It is of interest that both patients and the research nurse to the nurse’s better ability to correctly guess that a patient
had higher expectations for amitriptyline’s efficacy than was had been on placebo. The nurse correctly guessed the medi-
warranted by the study findings. At the beginning of the cation for 73% of the amitriptyline group and 75% of the
study, the research nurse and the other study investigators placebo group. Previous RCTs involving different medica-
were optimistic about the efficacy of amitriptyline in reliev- tions and patient populations have also found high overall
ing pain, based on findings in past studies. For example, one accuracy (69–88%) of treatment guesses made by physi-
meta-analysis (Onghena and Van Houdenhove, 1992) cians and other clinical evaluators (Byington et al., 1985;
reported an average effect size of 0.73 for amitriptyline Margraf et al., 1991; White et al., 1992; Carroll et al., 1994).
across placebo-controlled studies of patients with a variety That the nurse was able to guess more accurately than were
of pain syndromes. At the time of this study, many health the patients is not surprising given her more extensive
care providers who treated patients with chronic pain knowledge about the side effects associated with amitripty-
believed amitriptyline was effective in relieving pain. We line and, especially, the active placebo, benztropine mesy-
can only speculate about patients’ expectations, but perhaps late. Although some patients may have been familiar with
they thought that if this was a funded study to evaluate the amitriptyline’s side effects due to prior personal experience
efficacy of amitriptyline, then amitriptyline was quite likely or discussion with others who had been on amitriptyline, it
to be efficacious. is unlikely that any study patient had experience with benz-
Overall, 62.5% of patients in this study (70% of those on tropine mesylate.
amitriptyline and 55% of those on placebo) correctly That placebo group patient and research nurse guesses
guessed the medication they had received. This rate was were significantly associated with each other is not surpris-
somewhat lower than that reported in many previous ing given that they had weekly communication. The
double-blind placebo-controlled studies of different medica- research nurse and patients did not discuss their guesses,
tions for different disorders (Brownell and Stunkard, 1982; but the medication assignment may have become apparent
Byington et al., 1985; Rabkin et al., 1986; Margraf et al., to both in some cases due to the degree of pain relief and
1991; Bystritsky and Waikar, 1994). These previous studies side effects. After the end of the study, the nurse reported
compared the active medication to an inert placebo; the use that when patients had no or little pain relief by the end of
of an active placebo in the present study may well have the drug trial (as reported in the weekly telephone calls), she
reduced the accuracy of guesses, particularly those of guessed that they were on placebo unless they had several
patients on placebo. side effects or more severe side effects. In that case, she
Because some patients in the present study had taken guessed that they were on amitriptyline. Most patients in
amitriptyline in the past, we examined whether prior both medication groups (but more in the placebo group)
amitriptyline use was associated with pretreatment expecta- indicated that their guess was based on the amount of pain
tions, pain improvement, and accuracy of guesses concern- relief. Among those patients whose guess was based on the
ing medication assignment. The 22 patients who said that amount of pain relief (but not in the whole sample), pain
they had taken amitriptyline previously had significantly relief was rated as greater in the amitriptyline than in the
lower pretreatment expectations of pain relief with amitrip- placebo group. Thus, patients may have guessed that they
tyline, as compared with patients who had not. However, were on placebo due to minimal or no pain relief.
these two groups did not differ significantly in pain Although the medication groups did not differ signifi-
improvement during the study. Patients who took amitripty- cantly in any side effect type or severity as reported to the
line in the past also showed a stronger association between research nurse during the trial (except for the single finding
their pretreatment expectations of pain relief with amitripty- of greater severity of increased spasticity in the amitripty-
line and their actual change in pain intensity pre- to post- line group), more patients in the amitriptyline than in the
treatment than did patients who had not taken amitriptyline placebo group indicated that their posttreatment guess as to
previously. This association was not statistically significant medication assignment was based on side effects. Of those
in either group; however, the correlation in the group who on amitriptyline, 60% indicated that the guess was based on
98 J.A. Turner et al. / Pain 99 (2002) 91–99

side effect severity and 92% on the type. Of those on on patient and clinician guesses as to patient assigned
placebo, 45% indicated that the guess was based on side condition. The lack of this information in published articles
effect severity (i.e. mild) and 85% on the type. Previous may be due to concern about the risk of invalidating results
studies have also found that side effects play an important (White et al., 1992). We strongly recommend that results
role in patient, clinical investigator, and evaluator guesses of a double-blind trial not be dismissed when complete
as to patient group assignment in double-blind clinical trials double-blindness has not been achieved, as this may be
(Brownell and Stunkard, 1982; White et al., 1992; the situation with many, if not most, trials.
Bystritsky and Waikar, 1994).
Despite the better-than-chance accuracy of patient and
research nurse guesses as to patient medication assignment Acknowledgements
and the positive and moderate association between patient
pretreatment expectations and pain relief in the amitripty- This research was supported by grant no. 1 P01 HD/
line group, amitriptyline was not superior to active placebo NS33988 from the National Institutes of Health, National
in relieving pain in this study. Furthermore, although the Institute of Child Health and Human Development and
research nurse’s expectations of patient pain relief with National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke.
amitriptyline decreased significantly over the course of the The research was conducted through the Clinical Research
study, patients’ expectations of amitriptyline did not. This Center at the University of Washington, supported by the
finding, in combination with the finding that nurse expecta- National Institutes of Health grant no. M01RR0037. The
tions did not predict patient response, suggests that research authors gratefully acknowledge the contributions by Amy
nurse expectations did not have a strong influence on patient Hoffman, Catherine McClellan, and Helen Marshall,
outcomes in this trial. We would recommend that future University of Washington Department of Rehabilitation
studies be designed to enable examination not only of Medicine, in data collection and database management.
whether study physician or nurse (whoever has contact The authors would also like to express appreciation to
with subjects) expectations change over the course of the Marvin M. Brooke, M.D., for assistance in subject recruit-
study, but also of whether such changes, if found, are asso- ment.
ciated with changes in study results.
This study’s findings related to expectations and guesses
References
may not generalize to a study of a new treatment, which may
be associated with greater expectations and less knowledge Brownell KD, Stunkard AJ. The double-blind in danger: untoward conse-
on the part of study clinician, investigators, and patients. quences of informed consent. Am J Psychiatry 1982;139:1487–1489.
Furthermore, findings might differ in studies enrolling Byington RP, Curb JD, Mattson ME. Assessment of double-blindness at the
only patients with more severe levels of pain intensity. conclusion of the beta-blocker heart attack trial. J Am Med Assoc
We enrolled individuals who rated their average pain inten- 1985;253:1733–1736.
Bystritsky A, Waikar SV. Inert placebo versus active medication. Patient
sity as 3 or greater on a 0–10 scale and asked them their blindability in clinical pharmacological trials. J Nerv Ment Dis
expected percent pain relief. An expectation of 50% pain 1994;182:485–487.
relief might well have a different meaning for someone with Cardenas DD, Warms CA, Turner JA, Marshall HM, Brooke MM, Loeser
an average pain intensity level of 3 than it would for some- JD. Efficacy of amitriptyline for relief of pain in spinal cord injury:
one with an average pain intensity level of 8. results of a randomized controlled trial. Pain 2002;96:365–373.
Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ, Nich C. Blind man’s bluff: effectiveness
Study limitations include the lack of research nurse/ and significance of psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy blinding
physician expectation ratings for the first 25 study patients. procedures in a clinical trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 1994;62:276–
In addition, because a single research nurse made 93% of 280.
the pretreatment expectations ratings and all of the post- Finnerup NB, Yezierski RP, Sang CN, Burchiel KJ, Jensen TS. Treatment
treatment guesses, we do not know whether other evalua- of spinal cord injury pain. Pain Clin Updates 2001;9:1–6.
Galer BS, Schwartz L, Turner JA. Do patient and physician expectations
tors would make the same ratings and guesses. predict response to pain-relieving procedures? Clin J Pain
Nonetheless, the results concerning patient and research 1997;13:348–351.
nurse guesses are generally consistent with those reported Gracely RH, Dubner R, Deeter WR, Wolskee PJ. Clinicians’ expectations
in other studies of different patient populations and study influence placebo analgesia. Lancet 1985;1:43.
treatments. Even under double-blind conditions and with Hughes JR, Krahn D. Blindness and the validity of the double-blind proce-
dure. J Clin Psychopharmacol 1985;5:138–142.
an active placebo, patients and the research nurse were able Kalauokalani D, Cherkin DC, Sherman KJ, Koepsell TD, Deyo RA.
to guess at better-than-chance rates the assigned medica- Lessons from a trial of acupuncture and massage for low back pain.
tion. This study provides further evidence that fully Spine 2001;26:1418–1424.
double-blind conditions in randomized trials are difficult Luparello T, Leist N, Lourie CH, Sweet P. The interaction of psychologic
to achieve, and that either the placebo group and/or the stimuli and pharmacologic agents on airway reactivity in asthmatic
subjects. Psychosom Med 1970;32:509–513.
active medication group may well become unblinded. We Macdonald A, Peden NR, Hayton R, Mallinson CN, Roberts D, Wormsley
agree with Moscucci and colleagues (Moscucci et al., KG. Symptom relief and the placebo effect in the trial of an anti-peptic
1987) that all reports of blinded trials should include data drug. Gut 1980;21:323–326.
J.A. Turner et al. / Pain 99 (2002) 91–99 99

Margraf J, Ehlers A, Roth WT, Clark DB, Sheikh J, Agras WS, Taylor CB. cation guesses in a placebo-controlled trial of imipramine and phenel-
How ‘blind’ are double-blind studies? J Consult Clin Psychol zine. Psychiatry Res 1986;19:75–86.
1991;59:184–187. Radloff L. The CES scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the
Marini JL, Sheard MH, Bridges CI, Wagner E. An evaluation of the double- general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977;1:385–401.
blind design in a study comparing lithium carbonate with placebo. Acta Safren SA, Heimberg RG, Juster HR. Clients’ expectancies and their rela-
Psychiatr Scand 1976;33:343–354. tionship to pretreatment symptomatology and outcome of cognitive-
McQuay H, Carroll D, Moore A. Variation in the placebo effect in rando- behavioral group treatment for social phobia. J Consult Clin Psychol
mised controlled trials of analgesics: all is as blind as it seems. Pain 1997;65:694–698.
1995;64:331–335. Shapiro AP, Myers T, Reiser MF, Ferris EB. Comparison of blood pressure
McQuay HJ, Tramer M, Nye BA, Carroll D, Wiffen PJ, Moore RA. A response to Verloid and to the doctor. Psychosom Med 1954;16:478–
systematic review of antidepressants in neuropathic pain. Pain 488.
1996;68:217–227. Turner JA. Nonspecific treatment effects. In: Loeser JD, editor. Bonica’s
Moscucci M, Byrne L, Weintraub M, Cox C. Blinding, unblinding, and the management of pain, Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins,
placebo effect: An analysis of patients’ guesses of treatment assignment 2001. pp. 1649–1656.
in a double-blind clinical trial. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1987;41:259–265. Turner JA, Deyo RA, Loeser JD, Von Korff M, Fordyce WE. The impor-
Onghena P, Van Houdenhove B. Antidepressant-induced analgesia in tance of placebo effects in pain treatment and research. J Am Med
chronic non-malignant pain: a meta-analysis of 39 placebo-controlled Assoc 1994;271:1609–1614.
studies. Pain 1992;49:205–219. White K, Kando J, Park T, Waternaux C, Brown WA. Side effects and the
Rabkin JG, Markowitz JS, Stewart J, McGrath P, Harrison W, Quitkin FM, ‘blindability’ of clinical drug trials. Am J Psychiatry 1992;149:1730–
Klein DF. How blind is blind? Assessment of patient and doctor medi- 1731.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen