Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Ecosystem Services
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ecoser

Recreational cultural ecosystem services: How do people describe


the value?
Stålhammar Sanna a,⇑, Pedersen Eja b
a
Lund University Centre for Sustainability Studies, Lund University, Sweden
b
Environmental Psychology, Dep. of Architecture and Built Environment, Lund University, Sweden

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Different ways of viewing human-nature interactions affect the ways in which these are conceptualized
Received 15 March 2017 and operationalized with regards to cultural ecosystem services (CES). To clarify if some conceptualiza-
Received in revised form 22 May 2017 tions provide more appropriate descriptions of benefits, these need to be discussed in relation to the lived
Accepted 25 May 2017
experience. This paper addresses some aspects of the controversy around the use of the concept of CES
Available online 7 June 2017
and associated framing of ‘values’. Our aim is to understand potential distinctions between individuals’
expressions of values of their experiences and the language of value of ES. We use Swedish focus group
Keywords:
material formed to understand how individuals perceive and express their values of the experiences of
Cultural ecosystem services
Recreation
spending time in natural environments in their own words. We apply an interpretivist approach inspired
CES valuation by grounded theory and present our findings as the broader interpretative repertoire ‘axiomatic value’.
Intangible values The interpretative repertoire informed three discourses that participants describe as valuable in relation
Ecosystem services framework to experiences in nature: ‘indivisibility’, ‘incommensurability’, and ‘the goodness of perceived natural-
ness’. The latter comprised the underlying themes ‘nature as authentic’, ‘nature as healing’ and ‘nature
as beauty, magic and movement’. We discuss implications for conceptualizations of value and question
the appropriateness of the non-contextual and categorical language of ES.
Ó 2017 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND
license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

1. Introduction Pascual et al., 2010). Conceptualizing and operationalizing benefits


of nature imply several normative choices making the application
There is more than one way to explore, define and understand of ES fundamentally political (Kull et al., 2015). The subcategory
human-nature interaction. Alternative theories and analytical of cultural ecosystem services defined as the nonmaterial benefits
strategies start from different ontologies, adhere to different epis- people obtain from ecosystems through spiritual enrichment, cog-
temologies and draw on different methods thus giving rise to a nitive development, reflection, recreation, and aesthetic experi-
variety of framings of complex human-nature interaction (Moon ences (MEA, 2005) is considered particularly difficult to
and Blackman, 2014). An appreciation of this methodological vari- operationalize because of its intangibility (Milcu et al., 2013). In
ety implies, we argue, that we need to scrutinize available framings applications of the ES concept, socio-cultural values are assumed
and discuss if some allow for more appropriate and useful concep- to be quantifiable and correlational to ecological functions and
tualizations of the benefits that society derives from nature. We structures, which hides unresolved conflicts about the conflation
seek to call attention to some aspects of the controversy around of ‘nonmaterial’ values with calculable benefits of CES (Fraser
the use of the cultural ecosystem services (CES) concept and asso- et al., 2016; Kull et al., 2015; Daniel et al., 2012). Fraser et al.
ciated framing of ‘values’, based on individuals’ own descriptions of (2016) point out that the conception of culture as a separate cate-
the benefits of nature, captured empirically in a focus group study. gory from material values in nature is a Western, post-
Ecosystem services (ES) is a contested framework (Schröter Enlightenment, Cartesian phenomenon that does not translate well
et al., 2014) that is intended to capture the benefits of nature to to some indigenous ontologies. James (2015) argues that ES as
society and human wellbeing through assessing monetary and assumed to be directly derivable from ecosystem properties cannot
non-monetary values of ecosystem functions (MEA, 2005; provide a satisfactory account of the cultural benefits that people
derive from places, processes or events. The benefits of a place,
⇑ Corresponding author at: Lund University Centre for Sustainability Studies,
for example, are often dependent on a particular place rather than
Lund University, Sölvegatan 10, 223 62 Lund, Sweden. a type of place and the service it gives rise to cannot be seen as a
E-mail address: sanna.stalhammar@lucsus.lu.se (S. Stålhammar). separate function.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2017.05.010
2212-0416/Ó 2017 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V.
This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
2 S. Stålhammar, E. Pedersen / Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9

Various value-typologies to help classify social value of ES have tative of individuals ontological accounts of value, we need to
been suggested within the ES literature as for example Chan et al. explore how benefits of natural environments are expressed by
(2012) who outlined eight binary and non-mutually exclusive individuals independent of ready-made frameworks of value. Of
dimensions and Kenter et al. (2015) who distinguished shared/so- special interest here are the benefits lay people allocate to nature
cial values into transcendental, cultural and societal, communal, experiences in their daily life as this is often the focus of recre-
group, deliberated, other-regarding values and value to society. It ational CES assessments. Interpretative methods can help us
is acknowledged that socio-cultural values are ill-suited for com- develop an in-depth understanding of the meanings of values of
modity metaphors and cannot be captured by conventional mone- ecosystems that lay people describe and the reasons they are val-
tary valuation in meaningful ways (Milcu et al., 2013), thus various ued in certain ways, through understanding the individual and
non-monetary valuation methods have been recommended communal narratives of places (Kenter, 2016).
(Kenter et al., 2015; Kelemen et al., 2014; Kenter, 2014). Given that The aim of this study is to understand individuals’ expressions
the application of the ES concept is situated within mainstream of values from an interpretative perspective and how this can
economics and policy, it initially adopted a utilitarian framing of inform the language of value of ecosystem services. We address
value building on preferences (Gómez-Baggethun et al., 2010). the following questions:
Even some non-monetary valuation methods are similar to the
neoclassical monetary valuation approach in their assumptions of 1. How do local inhabitants perceive and describe how they value
quantification and aggregation of individually perceived values their experiences in nearby ecosystem?
(Raymond et al., 2014). However, recent and ongoing develop- 2. What are the implications of these descriptions for valuation of
ments of theory and method in relation to shared, plural and cul- CES?
tural values using deliberate techniques such as demonstrated by
Irvine et al. (2016) and Kenter et al. (2016b) shows the determina- Our analysis uses empirical material from focus groups that
tion to fundamentally challenge the utilitarian framing and the were initially formed and designed to answer the first research
analytical aggregation of individual values based on consequential- question of how individuals perceive and in their own words
ist assumptions. The discussion on deliberated values includes the express how they value their experiences of spending time in nat-
important recognition of value formation, which is the idea that ural environments. In the qualitative analysis, a second research
preferences and values are not pre-formed but influenced by the question emerged to form the basis for developing the main argu-
valuation process and method (Kenter et al., 2016a). Chan et al. ment in this paper. In using an interpretative approach, we follow
(2016) call for a third category of relational ES values alongside the call from IPBES and Chan et al. (2016) to diversify the perspec-
instrumental and intrinsic values. The launch of the new concep- tive of how values of ES can be analyzed, and explore the plurality
tual framework from the Intergovernmental Platform on Biodiver- of values individuals assign to ecosystems in a specific geographi-
sity and Ecosystem Services (IPBES) shifted their discussions from cal context. We evaluate how our findings can inform the current
ES values to nature’s gifts, and opened up for multiple knowledge debates on conceptualizations of value.
systems such as that of western science, indigenous and locals, and
practitioners (Díaz et al., 2015). Adhering to the critique of value
monism (e.g. Norton, 2015), they emphasize the importance of a 2. Method
pluralistic value approach which allows for ‘‘diverse valuation” of
instrumental, intrinsic and relational value. Fish et al. (2016) have 2.1. Mode of inquiry
also developed a framework aligned with the new IPBES approach,
which presents CES as a relational interaction between environ- Following Charmaz (2006) we applied the constructivist version
mental spaces and cultural practices, dependent on the biophysical of grounded theory. This approach was found suitable since cate-
domain and generating benefits in terms of experiences, identities gories and themes are supposed to emerge from data without any
and capabilities. interference from preconceived ideas and interpretations associ-
Regardless of conceptual development of novel frameworks and ated with, in our case, categories of values of cultural ecosystem
valuation methodologies, various ontological and epistemological services. Constructivist grounded theory is rooted in symbolic
questions regarding the foundation of people’s values of nature interactionism according to which individuals construct reality
remain (Kenter et al., 2016c, Table 1). Moreover, it should be recog- and create meaning based on symbols and social interaction
nized that there is no consensus regarding what constitutes ‘value’ (Cutliffe, 2000). We borrowed the concept of interpretative reper-
of nature for individuals. There is thus a potential distinction toires from discursive psychology (Wetherell et al., 2001), also
between how individuals experience and express their values of rooted in symbolic interactionism, to identify which ideas, notions,
nature and the language of value of ecosystem services. In order and metaphors participants drew on to articulate their experiences
to understand to what extent ES valuation can be seen as represen- and express their values of nature. Interpretative repertoires can be
thought of as coherent ways of speaking about something as well as
a framework that is drawn from for conversations and construction
Table 1 of discourse (Edley, 2001). The concept can be employed to reveal
Scheme of focus group interviews. Order in which the interviews were carried out
how attitudes are motivated, justified and resisted by discourses
(Order), number of participants in each group (N), characteristics of the host (Host),
and venue of the focus group interview (Location).
displayed in talk (Potter and Hepburn, 2007). It was here used to
conceptualize the general construction of participants’ ideas of val-
Order N Host Location
ues of nature in conversation. The process of articulation of values
1 7 Small town dweller Home of the host of nature was seen as constructed directly through interactions of
2 6 Member of a dog owner club Clubhouse talk and not through examining mental processes or through
3 6 Environmental NGO-member Home of the host
4 6 Member of an outdoor club Home of the host
expressing ethical standpoints that are hidden in the conversation.
5 6 Secondary school student School The combination of these approaches were presumed to be useful
6 5 Young city dweller, no children Home of the host in this study since they align with what could be considered a rela-
7 7 Culturally active person Home of the host tional approach to value, with interaction being the basis for inter-
8 11 Church member Church
pretation, and where the distinction between subjective and
Total 54
objective is seen as a discursive construction. This combined
S. Stålhammar, E. Pedersen / Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9 3

approach allowed us to gain an understanding of the meaning and 2.3. Sampling procedure
the characteristics of participants’ descriptions of the benefits they
experience from visits in the natural environment. The sampling procedure was purposeful, meaning that we
selected participants who could potentially provide a detailed,
2.2. Focus groups nuanced and rich material (Robson, 2011). Participants were
recruited through a contact person for each group, here called
To answer the first research question, a method that would give the host, who in turn enrolled people from their network. This
rich descriptions of how individuals perceive and in their own combination of convenience sampling, i.e. contacting an organiza-
words express how they value their experiences of spending time tion or person within once network, and snowball sampling, i.e.
in natural environments was desired. Focus groups interviews asking this person to suggest people that could be interesting to
were found suitable as in applying this method, people are encour- include in the focus group, is a common approach (e.g. Asah
aged to reflect on a topic collectively, allowing a broader perspec- et al., 2014; Quyen et al., 2017) thought not always explicitly
tive to emerge (Robson, 2011). It is an established interpretative described. The sampling method decreases the risk for drop-outs
method that can be used to elicit social and cultural values and and increases the likeliness that participants would feel comfort-
meanings of natural environments (O’Brien, 2003). In the case of able in the interview situation (MacDougall and Fudge, 2001). Six
ecosystem services, focus groups can allow for many types of of the hosts were found via local organizations and two from the
(ecosystem services) values to be expressed (Kenter, 2014) as well researcher’s informal network. Each host was informed that the
as shared rather than individual norms and discourses related to interview would refer to ‘everyday experiences of nature’, and that
values of nature (van Scholte et al., 2015). The components emerge the group should include six to ten people, a recommended focus
from the ground, from the experience and language of individuals group size (Morgan, 1998). The sampling started strategically with
and thereby aligns with the bottom-up perspective of this study. the aim to gather as rich material as possible. Theme saturation, in
The focus group procedure was based on the following princi- this case based on the criteria that no new data that further
ples: (i) that data would be analyzed as a total data-set, (ii) that explained the categories emerged (Bowen, 2008) were obtained
participants’ perception of cultural ecosystem services and the val- after four focus group interviews. The sampling then became the-
ues of services should be captured, but supposedly not in those oretically driven (Glaser and Strauss, 1967), with the main purpose
terms, and (iii) that services and values not previously defined to find stories that would contest and possibly invalidate the
could emerge from the discussions and contribute to the result. emerging themes (Strauss and Corbin, 1998). The following focus
To provide a common base across focus groups, the moderator groups therefore included young people, people living in a more
initiated the focus group session with a three-minute slide show urban setting, people with a focus on cultural activities rather than
comprising photos representing peri-urban greenery with bodies nature encounter, and people with a possible different view on
of water and wetlands common in the south of Sweden. She then spiritual experiences. The already defined themes were found to
opened the discussion with the question ‘Is this a type of natural be relevant also among these groups and no new themes emerged,
environment that you are familiar with?’. The interview guide wherefore no more focus group interviews were carried out.
included questions about the following issues: how local natural
environments are used (e.g. ‘What do you do when you visit nearby 2.4. Participants
nature areas?’, ‘Do you prefer to go alone or with someone else?’),
the perceived benefits (e.g. ‘What are natural environments good In total, 54 people (age 19–83, 67% women) participated in the
for?’, ‘What do you get from spending time in nature?’, ‘What does focus groups. Half of the sample, 50% rated themselves as more of
the experience do to your wellbeing?’) and values (e.g. ‘What is the an ‘outdoor person’ than a ‘city person’, a slightly higher frequency
value of natural environments for you?’, ‘Are there other ways to than previously found among Swedish populations comprising
achieve the same experience as from nature?’). We also included only city-dwellers (Gunnarsson et al., 2017). Most of the partici-
questions that allowed for the discussion on valuation of nature pants spent time outdoors at least weekly to take walks, get some
(e.g. ‘How do you think the benefits your experience of nature could fresh air, and exercise, and in a Swedish context therewith encoun-
be valued?’). Follow-up questions and probes were included for ter natural environments in some form. No statistically significant
clarification and elaboration. All issues were covered in the eight relationships were found between the frequencies of outdoor
focus groups interviews, though the questions within each issue activity and the identity as an outdoor person or city person, with
varied depending on what the participants brought up sponta- one exception, i.e. picking mushrooms which was reported more
neously during the conversation. Each discussion lasted one hour. frequent among outdoor people (Mann–Whitney U = 195.5,
Subsequently the participants filled in a short protocol that cap- z = 3.09, p < 0.01, r = 0,42). Hence, considering one-self as a city
tured age, gender, outdoor habits, and outdoor identity. Outdoor person did in most cases not lead to less outdoor activities. It was
habits were described using the four alternatives ‘almost never’, concluded that the sampling procedure had resulted in a total sam-
‘a few times a month’, ‘a few times a week’, and ‘almost every ple that met the criteria of being able to offer detailed, nuanced and
day’, and comprised take walks, walk the dog, get some fresh air, rich descriptions of nature experiences in their daily life, also
play with children, exercise, go for an outing, pick mushrooms assuring a variation in identity.
when it is the season, hunting or working. Outdoor identity was
captured with the question ‘‘How much of a city-person or 2.5. Analysis procedure
outdoor-person are you?” on a 5-point scale from 1 – ‘mainly
city-person’ to 5 – ‘mainly outdoor person’. All focus groups were The starting point for our analysis was to understand how indi-
moderated and audio-taped by the second author. A professional viduals describe values of their experiences without using pre-
was hired for the transcriptions. defined categories of values. The interview protocol built on an
It should be recognized that even though many Swedes would assumed CES perspective, based on what the experience does for
describe themselves as nature-loving people, all Swedish citizens their wellbeing or how it benefits them. The discussions were ana-
of all ages and social strata are not active participants in outdoor lyzed more broadly in terms of how participants perceived the
recreation and as society is becoming increasingly multicultural, value, in a broad sense, of their experiences. This allowed us to
new forms of recreational practices can be expected that will trans- interpret how participants articulated their wellbeing of experi-
form the old ones (Jensen and Ouis, 2014). ences. The analysis included open coding that resulted in the find-
4 S. Stålhammar, E. Pedersen / Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9

ings being organized into three levels including a general interpre- of values which was frequently expressed when participants were
tative repertoire, themes and subthemes. The three levels were asked to articulate and explain how experiences in nature were
outlined through an iterative and constant comparative approach valuable to them. When asked what experiences in nature ‘‘does
between codes, categories and the transcribed material. to you”, a frequent reply was that it ‘‘is difficult to describe”. Some
The analysis technique followed the constructivist grounded participants answered simply that it ‘‘does a lot I think” but did not
theory constant comparative approach of initial, focused and the- elaborate further. When probed about why or what it does, one
matic coding (Charmaz, 2006). Line by line coding of the tran- said that: ‘‘it simply makes me happy but I cannot describe it”, or
scripts was initially carried out to capture fundamental ideas in ‘‘just feels good”. Participants did not elaborate on or show interest
participant’s expressions. Focused coding then formed a more sys- in reflecting on why or how the experiences made them feel good
tematic analysis and refining of codes into categories. During the- or happy, it was seen as self-evident and self-explanatory. When
matic coding, categories were compared through careful reflection asked if they could describe how their experiences contribute to
of the data and formed into what we refer to as themes and sub- their wellbeing, one group laughed and one person replied ‘‘we
themes. It should be noted that while the themes capture how par- would be very poor without nature”. Another participant put it
ticipants perceived value, the sub-themes refer to the specific char- as: ‘‘it is quality of life to go for walks in nature”. The values or ben-
acteristics of nature that participants described as valuable. As an efits were seen as inherent in the experience and there was no per-
example, a common experience among participants was that ceived distance between participants and their experiences. It
spending time in natural environments made them relax; this therefore seemed difficult for participants to abstractedly talk
was coded as if these environments were perceived as ‘cal about it in terms of value. Axiomatic should here not be inter-
ming’/’soothing’. Together with other codes based on stories of preted as meaningless or vacuous, but rather understood in terms
how nature provided opportunities to recovering from stress and of, what is most obvious for the basis of experience is also most dif-
likewise, ‘calming’ formed the category ‘restorative’. In the final ficult to see and articulate. The notion of axiomatic value was pre-
results, this category is part of the sub-theme ‘nature as healing’. sent throughout the discussions and can be seen as a repertoire
However, the way that participants described the benefits of nat- that inform the rest of the themes presented below.
ure and the conceptualization of value of their experiences are
described by all three overarching themes. By using the concept 3.2. Indivisibility
of an interpretative repertoire, we identified a general idea of
how values of participant’s experiences seemed to be constructed Descriptions of benefits of experiences were indivisible in the
and thought of. The repertoire is a central metaphor organized sense that participants could not meaningfully break down the
around a cluster of terms as a result of coding (Edley, 2001); in descriptions into different features of value. Descriptions of beauty
our case, what participants were drawing on when expressing their and descriptions of sensory experiences were used in conjunction,
values of experiences in nature. The interpretative repertoire and often described as values in themselves. Hearing a bird singing
emerged from the themes, and simultaneously characterized and made several participants ‘‘really happy”, one participant said that
informed the themes. Throughout the analysis process, emerging ‘‘water is so relaxing. . .I don’t know why but seeing it is so relax-
categories and themes were tested in an iterative parallel process ing”, and yet another that a scenic view ‘‘creates calm and har-
comparing directly with primary data, i.e. the transcribed inter- mony”. From the participants’ perspective, beauty, serenity or a
views, resulting in renewed understanding, adjustments of cate- scenic view equaled value or wellbeing, and could not be broken
gories and themes, and confirmation of results. The material was down to something else that further described how value is consti-
initially coded and categorized by the two authors independently, tuted. The indivisibility was also illustrated when participants
following the same procedure, but without communicating. The expressed their experiences, and values thereof, in terms of nostal-
emerging themes were then compared and discussed in a series gia and through nostalgic associations linked to specific places.
of meetings. Themes were aligning across the coders and were These stories commonly comprised nostalgic constructions of sen-
backed up by the same citations, pointing at a large inter-coder sory experiences describing how it looked, sounded, smelt and felt;
agreement (Campbell et al., 2013). Transcribed focus-groups, codes for example, the sound of the woodpecker and the smell of the
and emerging categories were analyzed in the software Atlas.ti7. moss after a day of rain. Descriptions that drew on nostalgia were
also often emotional and associated with a specific landscape; the
participants talked about how they were touched, felt sad or were
3. Results
in awe. These experiences were sometimes referred to as from
their childhood, associated with other people or family members
Findings from the qualitative analyses are presented here as one
but also as experienced alone. The importance of knowing that a
repertoire with underlying discourses and themes. The repertoire
native place is still there, even though they might not visit it was
can be seen as a central metaphor of a broader construction that
also emphasized.
informs the underlying themes and sub-themes that participants
Participants frequently referred to ‘‘the forest” and ‘‘the ocean”
associate and describe as valuable in relation to experiences in nat-
as symbolic and self-directed entities without referring to specific
ure. The themes and sub-themes should be seen as overlapping
places. This was sometimes mentioned in associations with folk-
and not mutually exclusive. Themes 3.2 and 3.3 comprise charac-
lore and Swedish folk artists in the descriptions of positive images
teristics of the values that the participants allocated to their nature
of natural surroundings. Especially mentions of the forest were
experiences as they described them. Theme 3.4 and the underlying
often associated with a romanticized idea of folk mythology, i.e.
sub-themes present what participants described as meaningful
‘‘troll forest” trollskog. These perceptions can be seen as an example
and valuable during their experiences with reference to how they
of a cultural heritage and symbolic landscape memory where folk-
view nature.
lore can have played an important role in shaping Swedish percep-
tions of nature and the forest. There was also a strong reference in
3.1. The repertoire of axiomatic value: ‘‘Nature’s value is self-evident” general to the idea of nature as a subject. Nature was often talked
about as a separate entity from civilization that operates through
Overall, the participants had difficulties describing their experi- an independent and directional natural force. Participants talked
ences in detail, in terms of values and in relation to quality of life. about natures agency when describing how ‘‘nature is taking over”
We here refer to this overriding notion as the axiomatic repertoire in terms of spreading vegetation which was seen as positive and
S. Stålhammar, E. Pedersen / Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9 5

when humans are not present ‘‘nature can grow and manage on its of authenticity, which was in itself beneficial or valuable and that
own again”. Seeing nature as a subject relates to indivisibility since participants could identify with. Another frequent explanation or
it seems unfamiliar to divide a subject into parts. characteristic related to the authenticity of nature was that experi-
ences give rise to ‘‘a sense of freedom”. One participant explained
3.3. Incommensurability this as: ‘‘you are more free where it is quiet”. When probed about
why the experience of walking in the city is different from in the
The participants were explicitly asked how the benefits of their forest, one participant said that: ‘‘[In the city] it’s not the same
experiences could be valued, i.e. how it contributed to their quality sense of freedom, by the ocean and the forest I feel a huge freedom
of life and how they thought that this should be measured. This and I feel like I can breathe easier (. . .) but it is not the same feeling
question allowed for deliberation on the appropriateness of valua- when someone else has planned it, that has thought about what
tion and showed how participants thought about valuation of CES, fits best to plant here, some landscaper or something like that.
albeit implicitly. In general, the participants were puzzled when There are no landscapers in the forest”. There was thus a link in
confronted with these questions and responded to seeing the ben- the experience between connectedness or identification with nat-
efits of nature in quantified terms as unthinkable. These findings ure, naturalness and a feeling of freedom.
also show the inherent axiomatic characteristic of how partici-
pants saw the value of their experiences, and indicate that partic- 3.4.2. Nature as healing
ipants did not have a clear formulation of how they saw the The idea of nature having healing properties was a commonly
benefits, which was explained partly because they take the value expressed theme which was sometimes explicitly stated and
for granted. This was explicitly expressed in one focus group; when sometime implicit in the descriptions. These were interlinked with
asked to deliberate on the value said that they think that they take aesthetic descriptions that showed a romantic view of nature.
the value for granted, and therefore have difficulties thinking about When asked what participants get from visiting natural environ-
it in terms of value. The question of to what extent participants ments and what the motivation is to go there a common response
saw their values of their experiences as replaceable by some other was to ‘‘recharge the brain”, i.e. to restore and to contemplate.
experience was also discussed. There was general consensus that When asked why one participant feels better after walking on
the experience they get from nature could not be replaced by the beach they said that ‘‘It cleanses. Many thoughts disappear,
something else, and can therefore be termed incommensurable. or you can actually find new answers”. Another said that: ‘‘We
Participants expressed that experiencing nature was something need the forest and nature. That is where you recharge. . . I think
unique and thought that since they had become used to it, they it is important for a lot of people”. And: ‘‘You get elevated, like with
could not imagine being without it. the blackbird. I know when I was sick, I had pneumonia and I was
on Gotland, but I heard the blackbird. This was actually a support.”.
3.4. The goodness of perceived naturalness In the descriptions about restoration and contemplation, natural
surroundings were often described as having healing powers or
The participants often mentioned naturalness as a quality or properties. This was also taken for granted, or axiomatic, in the
explanation to the perceived benefit of experiences. The experience sense that it did not seem to require explanation of why or how
of naturalness when visiting non-human nature was described as the perceived restoration works. ‘‘It’s free therapy”. One participant
positive in itself, as well as the feeling of being away from the said: ‘‘nature is healing, isn’t it?” The idea of nature being healing
unnatural civilization. A revealed contradiction in viewing benefits was also shown through the many references to how the experi-
in terms of naturalness was the fact that some participants also ence in different ways is ‘‘good for the soul” or ‘‘a rest for the soul”.
described their preferences in terms of wanting a balance of cul-
tural and natural. They wanted natural environments to be ‘‘wild” 3.4.3. Nature as beauty, magic and movement
and feel authentic, but at the same time they wanted them to be The perceived naturalness or absence of humans was expressed
somewhat ordered and made accessible. Some described a semi- as an inherent part of the aesthetic experience and nature was in
controlled nature, or a ‘‘natural nature” but that was not too general described as more beautiful if untouched by humans. The
unruly. As someone put it: ‘‘[the forest] should be a little planned, beauty of nature was frequently explained as the benefit or value
but not artificial, still authentic”. Environments that were per- of the experience, and when probed about how or why this is valu-
ceived as natural were in general preferred, which was implicit able, common responses were that it makes you feel good or calm,
in the expression that something was beautiful, ‘‘even though it or that it gives a sense of peace or wonder. The notion of nature
is artificial”. being magical was reoccurring. One group discussed water as hav-
ing ‘‘some kind of magical attraction” that is ‘‘not really possible to
3.4.1. Nature as authentic define” but ‘‘there is something magical about it” and that ‘‘it’s
Participants described their identification with nature (as a sub- calming” and ‘‘it’s an element”. Fascination, perceived naturalness,
ject) and, in relation to this expressed feelings of authenticity and beauty and ‘‘the sense of nature being something bigger than
freedom. One participant described it as: ‘‘All of this that we see in humans or civilization” were components that were explained as
front of us now are things that we have constructed, I mean the giving rise to the magical feelings. A sense of wonder and awe were
school and cities and those things, but when we get out in nature also described when seeing wild nature, flowers and in experienc-
that’s when we realize that this has been here all along, I mean. . . ing the changing seasons. The fact that the magic character of nat-
it’s not we who have made it. . .that is also part of why you get so ure was understood as undetermined seemed to be of significance,
fascinated and it’s like. . . wow.” When asked why they thought and can also be described through (the repertoire of) axiomatic
that people can have such strong positive experiences from scenic value. Descriptions of beauty were often associated with enjoying
views of nature, one replied that: ‘‘humans were created in nature” the movement, variation and diversity of life in nature, as well as
and ‘‘it’s kind of like coming home”, and ‘‘it is our place of origin”. to a general preference towards the presence of animals. A com-
Most groups also discussed nature experiences as a basic human mon expression was that they liked when ‘‘something is happen-
‘‘need”, essential for their wellbeing. These views demonstrated a ing” in the environment around them in terms of movement,
discourse of identification, a sense of belonging and dependency variation and the presence of animals. The importance of the aes-
on nature. When probed about why or what was good about the thetics and the affiliation with alive or growing life-forms was
feeling of naturalness, it was described as giving rise to a sense sometimes expressed as more important than the perceived natu-
6 S. Stålhammar, E. Pedersen / Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9

ralness, as shown for example through the appreciation of include identity as part of their novel framework for CES, demon-
horticulture. strating that CES contribute to wellbeing through the identities
they help frame. They do not however give specific advice on
how identity should be captured. Some narrative-based
4. Discussion approaches that intend to elicit identity-related values can be
questioned. Posing questions such as: ‘‘Are there places that are
Our most salient finding is that participants had difficulty important to your sense of identity or the identity of the group
describing their experiences in terms of a benefit to their wellbeing to which you see yourself as a member? (Satterfield et al., 2013.
or as a value. Consequently, we want to raise a concern about the p. 15)” are not capable of capturing values that individuals have
general preoccupation with the attempt to fit the ways that the not previously articulated in form of identity. Furthermore, partic-
natural environment matters to people into the concept and notion ipants’ also described an identification with the idea of nature (as a
of values. Our findings contradict the use of a cognitivist/cognitive subject) rather than identification with place. This aspect of iden-
ontology, or interactional worldview (Altman and Rogoff, 1991) as tification has not, to our understanding, been explored in the sense
a starting point for valuation of CES, which involves a separation of place literature (Williams, 2014) and we want to add this as an
between values and the object of value. As our findings demon- example of how identity can be substantialized by natural environ-
strate, the participants’ articulations of benefits can be described ments. Furthermore, the way that participants talked about nature
as axiomatic since they were in large implicit, taken for granted, as a subject could be interpreted in terms of ‘personhood’ or ani-
difficult to articulate, and indivisible. In general, participants’ mism in relational epistemology (Milton, 2002; Hornborg, 2006;
descriptions of visiting nature to ‘‘recharging the brain” and relax Ingold, 2006; Bird-David, 1999).
can be seen as in line with theories on restorative environments In general, a romantic view of nature characterized the group
(Kaplan, 1995; Ulrich et al., 1983). However, descriptions of discussions which is known to have been influential in Sweden
restoration, serenity or beauty could not be meaningfully broken since the late 18th century (Jensen and Ouis, 2014). Some general
down to describe how participants valued the experiences. This findings are in line with theories about restorative environments
has implications for the consequentialist framing of ES valuation such as the way participants described naturalness aligning with
and the idea of preference-based valuation. The direct sensory preferences of perceived naturalness (e.g. Lindal and Hartig,
experience was explained as inherently valuable to the partici- 2015; Ode et al., 2009). Also, the descriptions of feelings of being
pants, and they had difficulties extrapolating and labelling the away from the unnatural civilization is in line with the sense of
experience in terms of value in an instrumental way. These find- being away in attention restoration theory (Kaplan and Kaplan,
ings are similar to those of Selman and Swanwick’s (2010) where 1989). However, using an interpretivist perspective, we construe
a recurrent theme was the intangibility of natural beauty and par- the idea of naturalness and identification with nature in a different
ticipants’ need to ensure that it reflected non-quantifiable and way. Participants described their experiences through identifica-
emotional qualities, which often could not be specified or defined, tion with nature, naturalness, authenticity, and a sense of freedom.
but was related to the capacity of land and water to evoke emo- We interpret the participants’ descriptions in similar ways as
tional responses. Dovey (2000) who, from a phenomenological perspective, explains
It seems reasonable to question how value possibly can be the search for authenticity in the experience of the environment as
derived or captured through preference-based valuation methods a search for a condition of connectedness in the person – environ-
if it is experienced as axiomatic by individuals. The findings also ment relationships, of which authenticity is a property. Our partic-
stand in contrast to the economic logic of intentionality which ipants described how the undetermined character of nature
takes preferences and expectations to be cognates for beliefs and seemed to give rise to feelings of authenticity and connectedness
desires and assumes that preferences are ordered and transitive because there was something about this undetermined character
in individuals minds (Rosenberg, 2001, p 183). The participants that they identified with. They also expressed feeling a sense of
viewed their experiences in nature as something unique and freedom possibly because nature or the experience requires noth-
incommensurable and the idea of quantifying benefits was incom- ing from them. Drenthen (2015) reporting on life story interviews
patible with their views. These views are thus disparate to the idea suggest that nature, as the realm of wildness, allows one to feel a
in preference-based valuation that natural capital is commensu- sense of autonomy and discover one’s self because of the indiffer-
rable, i.e. that values are equivalent to something else in monetary ence of nature and lack of societal demands and norms. The unde-
or other terms, among themselves or as human-made and financial termined character of how individuals see nature can thus be seen
resources (Pascual et al., 2010, p 11). as part of the benefit, and part of the sense of relief that arises from
Our participants drew on nostalgia, memories and experiences the recreational experience. In that light, the pursuit of increas-
from their childhood and descriptions were associated with other ingly attempting to separate and categorize benefits of experiences
people or family members. These descriptions can be seen as piv- in nature in line with CES appears contradictory to how individuals
otal or likened to what James (2015) refers to as constitutive value, perceive benefits of nature as undetermined.
i.e. those values that play integral roles in people’s lives such as The described healing capabilities of nature, and references to
someone’s native place. Within the CES valuation paradigm, such the experiences being ‘‘good for the soul” show a longing for con-
values would possibly be interpreted in terms of a sense of place, nectedness. Nature connectedness has been extensively studied in
or as heritage values defined as ‘memories’ in the landscape from environmental psychology (Restall and Conrad, 2015; Capaldi
past cultural ties (Church et al., 2011, p. 645). However, we argue et al., 2014), but is not commonly discussed with regard to CES.
in accordance with James (2015) that when a place has constitutive An interpretivist approach like ours could be used to help fill in
value it cannot accurately be conceived as sense of place or a her- the gaps of understanding for example why patterns of recalled
itage value because it decouples meaning from context and turns connectedness differ from one environment to another. Further-
into an instrumental representation. Our participants expressed more, the desire for connectedness and emotional relationships
the importance of place as inherently interlinked with meaning, with nature can be interpreted in terms of spirituality (Tacey,
personal memory and belonging which can be interpreted as form- 2000). With this view, fascination, perceived naturalness, beauty
ing part of their identity. If something forms part of an individual’s and the undetermined character of nature that gives rise to magi-
identity, it does not seem accurate to interpret this as an external cal feelings, can all be interpreted as related to spiritual experi-
service to that person. We instead align with Fish et al. (2016), who ences. Gullestad (1992) describes how many Scandinavians
S. Stålhammar, E. Pedersen / Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9 7

perceive experiences in nature as an almost spiritual experience which also is equated with performing one’s distinctive function
(see also Ween and Abram, 2009). of reasoning of a life ‘‘worth living” (Athanassoulis, (n.d.)). In order
An interpretivist approach to understanding value is not to complement the idea of human flourishing with emotional
directly comparable with mainstream classification schemes for aspects, an exploration of (neo)entimentalism, which hold that
CES, such as the European CICES (Haines-Young and Potschin, emotional responses determine what is valuable, could be fruitful
2012). This classification scheme builds on itemized lists, which (McShane, 2011).
do not make categories mutually exclusive, but incapable of It should be emphasized that using an interpretivist method to
accounting for the relationship or composition of them. We oppose understand value that seems to align with a more relational per-
the idea of stacking or bundling different classes to try to account spective should not be equated with eliciting relational values.
for relationships, since such a procedure would not provide the Some of our findings seem to align with the idea of relational value
depth, meaning and interconnectedness of the experience that (Chan et al., 2016; Fish et al., 2016) in the sense that it emphasizes
the participants themselves attribute to it. Using an interpretivist the non-dualist and meaning-making aspects of how value was
approach, benefits seem best conceptualized as an entanglement seen by participants. However, it is unclear if this supposedly
of all subcategories of CES in the CICES scheme. For example, CICES new category of value is being consistently conceptualized, what
has a separate class of ‘‘aesthetic”, where a sense of place is its ontological foundations are and how it relates to existing rela-
included as an example. Seeing sense of place as a subcategory of tional ontologies and epistemologies. Relationally understood
aesthetic values is misrepresentative if place-based values play value is in a psychological sense for example best conceptualized
integral parts in people’s lives. The way that spirituality could be in terms of a transactional worldview (Altman and Rogoff, 1991).
interpreted as the expression of a desire for connectedness (with Some novel contributions based on a ‘transactional’ worldview to
nature) would also not be captured by the CICES classification. Fur- understand human-environmental relations have been suggested
thermore, as some of our findings suggest the possibility of a desire by Cooke et al. (2016) and Raymond et al., 2014, who both use
for connectedness with nature being foundational for benefits of an ‘embodied’ approach. However, these and other relational epis-
(recreational) experiences in nature, then ‘connectedness’ cannot temologies, such as our reference to animism, cannot be expected
be separated and accounted for or stacked next to other CES. Par- to be comparable or assumed to be compatible with the ecosystem
ticipants descriptions cannot be meaningfully interpreted by iso- services framework. While some aspects could be analogous or
lating for example aesthetic benefits on the one hand and overlap, we espouse epistemological pluralism and call for the
spiritual on the other, an thereby support Kenter et al. (2014) need to clarify the ontology and epistemology of relational values
who state that aesthetic and spiritual experiences can be seen as of ES.
co-emergent (see also Kenter et al., 2016b). In general, our findings
support the framework by Fish et al. (2016) who conceptualize ES
in terms of relational phenomena that continually enables and 5. Conclusions
shapes benefits (through identities, experiences and capabilities).
Exploring how these benefits mutually reinforce each other should There are elements of individuals’ direct experience, being and
be encouraged rather than attempting to separate them artificially. knowing human-environmental relations that remain uncaptured
We want to emphasize the importance of emotion for interpret- by ecosystem services valuation methods. Our interpretative
ing the value and meaning of experiences in nature. Our findings approach allowed us to find some aspects of expressed benefits
align with those of Drenthen (2015) who through life history anal- that are currently not taken into account in the conceptualization
ysis report that people see nature not as an object of value, but of values. These expressions are characterized as being axiomatic,
instead as a meaningful context that can be interpreted through indivisible, and incommensurable and include perceiving value
the Aristotelian concept of ‘eudaimonia’ (human flourishing). through emotions, connectedness, authenticity and spirituality.
Cooper et al. (2016) also use a conceptual analysis to show how These findings can be argued to provide leverage for resistance to
a range of different conceptions of aesthetic and spiritual values the entire idea of value elicitation. The basic argument for valua-
are at axiological odds with consequentialism, since aesthetic tion of ecosystem services is that we need to know how to make
and spiritual interests often do not have an instrumental end, trade-offs between different preferences for decision-making. As
can be experienced without preference satisfaction, and stem from our findings suggest however, all values cannot (ontologically) be
different ontological value relationships. interpreted as preferences and be used to assess trade-offs without
The distinction of instrumental and intrinsic value (Jax et al., losing their fundamental meaning because they are entangled and
2013) and preference-based valuation assumes that people’s co-emergent. Using an interpretivist approach to understand those
appreciation for nature translates into reasoning where value can values seems most appropriate. Interpretative methods have been
be allocated in logically coherent ways. The emotional characteris- suggested to be used alongside mainstream methods to provide a
tic of experiences in nature is however something that has been more comprehensive valuation that not only considers how much
largely overlooked in valuation of ES. Rational justification rather ES are worth but also what they mean to people (Kenter, 2016).
than emotional aspects of values of nature are of greatest concern Our findings bring us to question this dichotomy of values versus
in dominant environmental ethics. If we, as Milton (2002) suggests, meanings and we instead suggest that benefits that humans reap
instead see emotions as a significant part of how people value from nature should best be conceptualized in a way where mean-
things, we can interpret value as perceived meaning, which ings can be interpreted as value. Overriding the fundamental
becomes known through emotion and experienced as feelings. meaning of values would imply a risk of scientific imperialism
Our findings suggest that in order for people’s descriptions of value when applying certain framings of value that masks others. More
to bear the meaning that it has for them, value needs to be con- studies are needed that investigate the possibility of defining crit-
nected to the sensory experiences and emotions that experiences ical circumstances where meanings of experiences are lost through
give rise to. An elicitation of CES value that does not take the emo- conceptualization and valuation, as well as what the consequences
tional component into account will be less meaningful. We wish to of this for different stakeholders are.
add this consideration to the current development on ‘‘relational Our findings could also be considered to provide incentive for
value” (e.g. Chan et al., 2016; Díaz et al., 2015), and encourage developing novel ways to think about and conceptualize ontologies
the exploration of how emotional aspects relates to the idea of and worldviews, and about how these appropriately could inform
Eudaimonia. Eudaimonism bases virtues in human flourishing, decision-making. Attempting to more accurately describe and
8 S. Stålhammar, E. Pedersen / Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9

understand experiences of nature through qualitative social Daniel, T.C., Muhar, A., Arnberger, A., Aznar, O., Boyd, J.W., Chan, K.W.A., Costanza,
R., Elmqvist, T., Flint, C.G., Gobster, P.H., Gret-Regamey, A., Lave, R., Muhar, S.,
science and interpretivist approaches will not easily or adequately
Penker, M., Ribe, R.G., Schauppenlehner, T., Sikor, T., Soloviy, I., Spierenburg, M.,
translate to robust indicators to inform policy and decision- Taczanowska, K., Tam, J., A von der, Dunk., 2012. Contributions of cultural
making. Despite the development of novel frameworks for ‘‘di- services to the ecosystem services agenda. PNAS 109 (23), 8812–8819. http://
verse” valuation that are supposed to include multiple and plural dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1114773109.
Díaz, S. et al., 2015. The IPBES conceptual framework—connecting nature and
values, it is uncertain how the three cultures of science, i.e. the nat- people. Curr. Opin. Environ. Sustainability 14, 1–16.
ural science, quantitative social science and interpretivist tradi- Dovey, Kimberly, 2000. The quest for authenticity and the replication of
tions could be accurately reconciled to inform decision-making. environmental meaning. In: Seamon, D., Mugerauer, R. (Eds.), Dwelling, place
& environment: towards a phenomenology of person and world. Krieger
Practitioners need to be critical and reflexive about the ontological publishing Company, Malabar.
and epistemological grounds of what types of values they intend to Drenthen, M., 2015. How Stories Help to Understand How the World Matters to Us.
capture in CES assessments and the difference between interpre- In: Knippenberg L. (Ed.), A Theory of Committed Action for Natur:
interdisciplinary explanations. BIOMOT reporting output June 2015: 53–68.
tivist and positivist approaches should be highlighted. Moreover, Edley, Nigel., 2001. Analysing Masculinity: Interpretative Repertoirs, Ideological
if we want to manage for more qualitative and plural values based Dilemmas and Subject Positions. In: Wetherell, M., Taylor, S., Yates, S.J. (Eds.),
on relational worldviews, institutions need to be receptive and Discourse as Data: A guide for analysis. SAGE Publications Ltd., London.
Fish, R., Church, A., Winter, M., 2016. Conceptualising cultural ecosystem services: a
adaptive to accommodate for these in their management frame- novel framework for research and critical engagement. Ecosyst. Serv. 21, 208–
works. Regardless of these challenges, there is great potential to 217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2016.09.002.
further theoretically and conceptually develop the understanding Fraser, J.A., Diabaté, M., Narmah, W., Beavogui, P., Guilavogui, K., De Foresta, H.,
Junqueira, A.B., 2016. Cultural valuation and biodiversity conservation in the
of benefits of ecosystems to human society in ways that align with
upper Guinea forest, West Africa. Ecol. Soc. 21 (3), 36. http://dx.doi.org/
the lived experience of people. 10.5751/ES-08738-210336.
Glaser, B., Strauss, A., 1967. The discovery of grounded theory: strategies for
qualitative research. Aldine de Gruyter, New York.
Acknowledgements Gómez-Baggethun, E., De Groot, R., Lomas, P.L., Montes, C., 2010. The history of
ecosystem services in economic theory and practice: from early notions to
markets and payment schemes. Ecol. Econ. 69 (6), 1209–1218. http://dx.doi.
This study was funded by the Swedish Environmental Protec- org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2009.11.007.
tion Agency (no. 13/148) and the Swedish Research Council Formas Gullestad, Marianne, 1992. The Art of Social Relations: Essays on Culture Social Action
(Linneus Centre LUCID; grant no. 259-2008-1718). We like to and Everyday Life in Modern Norway. Scandinavian University Press, Oslo.
Gunnarsson, B., Knez, I., Hedblom, M., Ode Sang, Å., 2017. Effects of biodiversity and
thank the participants without whom this study would not have
environment-related attitude on perception of urban green spaces. Urban
been possible. We also like to thank editors and anonymous Ecosyst. 20, 37–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11252-016-0581-x.
reviewers for their insightful and generous comments. Haines-Young, R., Potschin, M., 2012. Common International Classification of
Ecosystem Services (CICES): Consultation on Version 4, August–December
2012. EEA Framework Contract No.
References Hornborg, Alf, 2006. Animism, fetishism, and objectivism as strategies for knowing
(or not knowing) the world. Ethnos 71 (1), 21–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
00141840600603129.
Altman, I., Rogoff, B., 1991. World Views in Psychology: Trait, Interactional,
Ingold, Tim., 2006. Rethinking the animate, re-animating thought. Ethnos: J.
Organismic, and Transactional Perspectives. In: Stokols, D., Altman, I. (Eds.),
Anthropol. 71 (1), 9–20.
Handbook of Environmental Psychology, vol. 1. John Wiley & Sons, Malabar, pp.
Irvine, K.N., O’Brien, L., Ravenscroft, N., Cooper, N., Everard, M., Fazey, I., Reed, M.S.,
7–40.
Kenter, J.O., 2016. Ecosystem services and the idea of shared values. Ecosyst.
Asah, S.T., Guerry, A.D., Blahna, D.J., Lawler, J.J., 2014. Perception, acquisition and
Serv. 21, 184–193.
use of ecosystem services: human behavior, and ecosystem management and
James, S.P., 2015. Cultural Ecosystem Services: A Critical Assessment, Ethics, Policy
policy implications. Ecosyst. Serv. 10, 180–186.
& Environment, 18:3, 338–350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21550085.2015.
Athanassoulis, Nafsika, (n.d.). Virtue Ethics The Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy,
1111616.
ISSN 2161-0002. http://www.iep.utm.edu/ (accessed 17.03.07).
Jax, K., Barton, D.N., Chan, K.M.A., de Groot, R., Doyle, U., Eser, U., Görg, C., Gómez-
Bird-David, Nurit., 1999. ‘‘Animism” Revisited. Current Anthropology, Special Issue
Baggethun, E., Griewald, Y., Haber, W., Haines-Young, R., Heink, U., Jahn, T.,
Culture–A Second Chance? 40 (21), S67–S91.
Joosten, H., Kerschbaumer, L., Korn, H., Luck, G.W., Matzdorf, B., Muraca, B.,
Bowen, G.A., 2008. Naturalistic inquiry and the saturation concept: a research note.
Nebhöver, C., Norton, B., Ott, K., Potschin, M., Rauschmayer, F., von Haaren, C.,
Qual. Res. 8, 137–152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1468794107085301.
Wachmann, S., 2013. Ecosystem services and ethics. Ecol. Econ. 93, 260–268.
Campbell, J.L., Quincy, C., Osserman, J., Pedersen, O.K., 2013. Coding in-depth
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2013.06.008.
semistructured interviews: problem of unitization and intercoder reliability
Jensen, E.L., Ouis, P., 2014. Det gröna finrummet: etnicitet, friluftsliv och
and agreement. Soc. Methods Res. 42, 294–320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
naturumgängets urbanisering. Carlssons bokförlag, Stockholm.
0049124113500475.
Kaplan, Stephen, 1995. The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative
Capaldi, C.A., Dopko, R.L., Zelenski, J.M., 2014. The relationship between nature
framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 15, 169–182.
connectedness and happiness: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 5, 976. http://dx.
Kaplan, R., Kaplan, S., 1989. The experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective.
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00976.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Chan, K.M.A., Guerry, A.D., Balvanera, P., Klain, S., Satterfield, T., Basurto, X.,
Kelemen, E., García-Llorente, M., Pataki, G., Martín-López, B., Gómez-Baggethun E.,
Bostrom, A., Chuenpagdee, R., Gould, R., Halpern, B.S., Hannahs, N., Levine, J.,
2014. Non-monetary techniques for the valuation of ecosystem service. In:
Norton, B., Ruckelshaus, M., Russel, R., Tam, J., Woodside, U., 2012. Where are
Potschin, M., Jax K. (Eds.): OpenNESS Reference Book. EC FP7 Grant Agreement
cultural and social in ecosystem services? A framework for constructive
no. 308428. Available via: www.openness-project.eu/library/reference- book.
engagement. BioScience 62 (8), 744–756. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/
Kenter, J., 2014. Deliberate and non-monetary valuation: A review of methods.
bio.2012.62.8.7.
Laurence Mee Centre for People and the Sea, Working Papers 2014–02 October
Chan, K.M.A., Balvanera, P., Benessaiah, K., Chapman, M., Diaz, S., Gomez-Baggethun,
2014. http://www.sams.ac.uk/lmc/working-papers/kenter-valuation-review.
E., Gould, R., Hannahs, N., Jax, K., Klain, S., Luck, W.G., Martin-Lopez, B., Muraca,
(accessed 16.07.18).
B., Norton, B., Ott, K., Pascaual, U., Satterfield, T., Tadaki, M., Taggart, J., Turner,
Kenter, J.O., 2016. Deliberative and non-monetary valuation methods. In: Potschin,
N., 2016. Why protect nature? Rethinking values and the environment. PNAS
M., Haines-Young, R., Fish, R., Turner, R.K. (Eds.). Handbook of Ecosystem
113 (6), 1462–1465 http://www.pnas.org/content/113/6/1462.full.
Services. Routledge., Oxon.
Charmaz, Kathy., 2006. Constructing Grounded Theory. Sage Publishing Ltd.,
Kenter, J.O., Reed, M.S., Irvine, K.N., O’Brien, E., Brady, E., Bryce, R., Christie, M.,
London.
Church, A., Cooper, N., Davies, A., Hockley, N., Fazey, I., Jobstvogt, N., Molloy, C.,
Church, A. et al., 2011. Cultural Services. In: The UK National Ecosystem Assessment
Orchard-Webb, J., Ravenscroft, N., Ryan, M., Watson, V., 2014. UK National
Technical Report. UK National Ecosystem Assessment, UNEP-WCMC,
Ecosystem Assessment Follow-on. Work Package Report 6: Shared, Plural and
Cambridge. http://uknea.unep-wcmc.org/LinkClick.aspx?fileticket=
Cultural Values of Ecosystems. UNEP-WCMC, LWEC, UK.
QLgsfedO70I%3d&tabid=82 (accessed 16.07.12).
Kenter, J.O., O’Brien, L., Hockley, N., Ravenscroft, N., Fazey, I., Irvine, K.N., Reed, M.S.,
Cooke, B., West, S., Boonstra, W.J., 2016. Dwelling in the biosphere: exploring an
Christie, M., Brady, E., Bryce, R., Church, A., Cooper, N., Davies, A., Evely, A.,
embodied human-environment connection in resilience thinking. Sustainability
Everard, M., Fish, R., Fisher, J.A., Jobstvogt, N., Molloy, C., Orchard-Webb, J.,
Sci. 11, 831–843.
Ranger, S., Ryan, M., Watson, V., Williams, S., 2015. What are shared and social
Cooper, N., Brady, E., Steen, H., Bryze, R., 2016. Aesthetic and spiritual values of
values of ecosystems? Ecol. Econ. 111, 86–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
ecosystems: Recognising the ontological and axiological plurality of cultural
ecolecon.2015.01.006.
ecosystem ‘services’. Ecosyst. Serv. 21 (Part B), 218–229.
Kenter, J.O., Reed, M.S., Fazey, Ioan., 2016a. The deliberative value formation model.
Cutliffe, J.R., 2000. Methodological issues in grounded theory. J. Adv. Nurs. 31 (6),
Ecosyst. Serv. 21, 194–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoser.2016.09.015.
1476–1484.
S. Stålhammar, E. Pedersen / Ecosystem Services 26 (2017) 1–9 9

Kenter, J.O., Jobstvogt, N., Watson, V., Irvine, K., Christie, M., Bryce, R., 2016b. The Quyen, N.T.K., Berg, H., Gallardo, W., Da, C.T., 2017. Stakeholders’ perceptions of
impact of information, value-deliberation and group-based decision-making on ecosystem services and Pangasius catfish farming development along the Hau
values for ecosystem services: integrating deliberative monetary valuation and River in the Mekong Delta, Vietnam. Ecosyst. Serv. 25, 2–14.
storytelling. Ecosyst. Serv. 21, 270–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Raymond, C.M., Kenter, J.O., Plieninger, T., Turner, N.J., Alexander, K.A., 2014.
ecoser.2016.06.006. Comparing instrumental and deliberate paradigms underpinning the
Kenter, J.O., Reed, M.S., Irvine, K.N., O’Brien, E., Bryce, R., Christie, M., Cooper, N., assessment of social values of cultural ecosystem services. Ecol. Econ. 107,
Hockley, N., Fazey, I., Orchard-Webb, J., Ravenscroft, N., Raymond, C.M., Tett, P., 145–156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolecon.2014.07.033.
Watson, V., 2016c. Shared values and deliberative valuation: future directions. Restall, B., Conrad, E., 2015. A literature review of connectedness to nature and its
Ecosyst. Serv. 21, 358–371 http://dx.doi.org/1016/j.ecoser.2016.10.006. potential for environmental management. J. Environ. Manage. 159, 1–15. http://
Kull, C.A., Arnauld de Sartre, X., Castro-Larrañaga, M., 2015. The political ecology of dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvman.2015.05.022.
ecosystem services. Geoforum 61, 122–134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Robson, C., 2011. Real world research: a resource for users of social research
j.geoforum.2015.03.004. methods in applied settings. Wiley Blackwell, Chichester, West Sussex.
Lindal, P.J., Hartig, T., 2015. Effects of urban street vegetation on judgments of Rosenberg, Alex, 2001. The metaphysics of microeconomics. In: Mäki, Uskali (Ed.),
restoration likelihood. Urban For. Urban Green. 14, 200–209. The Economic World View: Studies in the Ontology of Economics. Cambridge
MacDougall, C., Fudge, E., 2001. Planning and recruiting the sample for focus groups University Press, Cambridge.
and in-depth interviews. Qual. Health Res. 11, 117–126. http://dx.doi.org/ Satterfield, T., Gregory, R., Klain, S., Roberts, M., Chan, K.M., 2013. Culture,
10.1177/104973201129118975. intangibles and metrics in environmental management. J. Environ. Manage.
McShane, K., 2011. Neosentimentalism and environmental ethics. Environ. Ethics 117, 103–114.
33 (1), 5–23. Schröter, M., van der Zanden, E.H., van Oudenhoven, A.P.E., Remme, R.P., Sernia-
MEA, Millennium Ecosystem Assessment, 2005. Ecosystems and human well-being Chavez, H.M., de Groot, R.S., Opdam, P., 2014. Ecosystem services as a contested
– Synthesis. Island Press, Washington, DC. concept – A synthesis of critique and counter-arguments. Conserv. Lett. 7 (6),
Milcu, A., Ioana, J., Hanspach, D.Abson., Fischer, J., 2013. Cultural ecosystem 514–523.
services: a literature review and prospects for future research. Ecol. Soc. 18 (3), Selman, Paul., Swanwick, Carys., 2010. On the meaning of natural beauty in
44. http://dx.doi.org/10.5751/ES-05790-180344. landscape legislation. Landscape Res. 35 (1), 3.
Milton Kay, 2002. Loving Nature: Toward an Ecology of Emotion. London, Strauss, A., Corbin, J., 1998. Basics of Qualitative Research; Grounded Theory
Routledge. Procedures and Techniques. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Moon, K., Blackman, D.A., 2014. Guide to Understanding Social Science Research for Tacey, D., 2000. Re-enchantment: The New Australian Spirituality. Harper Collins,
Natural Scientists. Conserv. Biol. 28 (5), 1167–1177. http://dx.doi:10.1111/cobi. Australia.
12326. Ulrich, R.S., Simons, R.F., Losito, B.D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M.A., Zelson, M., 1983. Stress
Morgan, D.L., 1998. Planning focus groups. Sage Publishing, Thousand Oaks, CA. recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ.
Norton, Bryan.G., 2015. Sustainable values, sustainable change: A guide to Psychol. 11, 201–230.
environmental decision making. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago. van Scholte, S., Teeffelen, A., Verburg, P.H., 2015. Integrating socio-cultural
O’Brien, E.A., 2003. Human values and their importance to the development of perspectives into ecosystem service valuation: a review of concepts and
forestry policy in Britain: a literature review. Forestry 76 (1), 3–17. methods. Ecol. Econ. 114, 67–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
Ode, Å., Fry, G., Tveit, M.S., Messager, P., Miller, D., 2009. J. Environ. Manage. 90 (1), ecolecon.2015.03.007.
375–383. Ween, G., Abram, S., 2009. The Norwegian trekking association: trekking as
Pascual, U., Muradian, R., Brander, L., Gomez-Baggethun, E., Martin-Lopez, B., Berma, constituting the nation. Landscape Res. 37 (2), 155–171.
M., Christie, M., 2010. TEEB Chapter 5 The Economics of Valuing Ecosystem Wetherell, M., Taylor, S., Yates, J.S., 2001. Discourse as Data: A Guide for Analysis.
Services and Biodiversity. In: Kumar, P. (Ed.), The Economics of Ecosystems and Sage Publications Ltd.
Biodiversity: Ecological and Economic Foundations. Taylor & Francis, London, Williams, Daniel R., 2014. Making sense of ‘place’: Reflections on pluralism and
pp. 183–256. positionality in place research. Landscape Urban Plan. 131, 74–82.
Potter, J., Hepburn, A., 2007. Discursive psychology: Mind and reality in practice. In:
Weatherall, A., Watson, B., Gallois, C. (Eds.), Language, discourse and social
psychology. Palgrave, London.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen