Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189

DOI 10.1007/s00213-010-2123-1

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

T-pattern analysis of diazepam-induced modifications


on the temporal organization of rat behavioral
response to anxiety in hole board
Maurizio Casarrubea & Filippina Sorbera &
Magnus S. Magnusson & Giuseppe Crescimanno

Received: 27 July 2010 / Accepted: 29 November 2010 / Published online: 23 December 2010
# Springer-Verlag 2010

Abstract of t-patterns including walking for all administered groups,


Rationale By means of t-pattern analysis, it has been and significant reductions of t-patterns including climbing,
observed that the different events, characterizing rat immobile sniffing, and edge-sniff. Front-paw licking and
behavior in hole board (HB), present close interrelation- face grooming were reduced at the higher DZP dose.
ships which occur sequentially and with significant con- Conclusions Present study demonstrates, for the first time,
straints on the interval lengths separating them. that the temporal structure of Wistar rats’ behavioral response
Objectives The aim of present research was to study, by to anxiety in HB changes following pharmacological manip-
means of descriptive and multivariate t-pattern analyses, the ulation of anxiety condition. Moreover, t-pattern analysis is
effects of the reference anxiolytic drug diazepam (DZP) on suggested to represent a useful tool to evaluate and compare
temporal structure of a rat’s anxiety-related behavior in HB. different classes of anti-anxiety molecules.
Methods Fifty-six male Wistar rats were tested for 10 min
in HB. Video files, collected for each animal, were coded Keywords Anxiety . T-pattern analysis . Diazepam . Hole
by means of a software coder, and event log files, generated board . Multivariate analysis . Rat
for each subject, were analyzed by means of a specific
software for temporal pattern analysis (t-pattern analysis).
Results Significant diazepam-induced modifications were Introduction
observed for durations of walking, climbing, edge-sniff,
and face grooming. Dose-dependent decreases of t-patterns’ Various features of anxiety-related behaviors in rodents
total amount, of their mean occurrences and of their mean have been investigated by means of hole board (HB), an
length for each group were detected. Also, t-patterns’ mean experimental assay widely used in the last decades
occurrences, in terms of different composition, were (Adamec et al. 2006; Casarrubea et al. 2009b, 2010a; File
reduced. Percent distributions showed a significant increase and Wardill 1975; Harada et al. 2006; Kamei et al. 2007;
Saitoh et al. 2006). Similar to open-field test, HB offers a
M. Casarrubea : F. Sorbera : G. Crescimanno (*) simple approach to the study of rodent behavior in aversive
Department of Experimental Biomedicine and Clinical Neurosciences,
situations such as an unknown environment (Choleris et al.
Human Physiology Section, University of Palermo,
Palermo, Italy 2001). Multivariate analysis, a set of techniques able to
e-mail: crescima@unipa.it provide information concerning multiple behavioral varia-
M. Casarrubea bles (Espejo 1997; Casarrubea et al. 2006, 2008, 2009c;
e-mail: m.casarrubea@unipa.it Cruz et al. 1994; Kalueff and Tuohimaa 2005; Lino-de-
F. Sorbera Oliveira et al. 2005; Spruijt and Gispen 1984; van Den
e-mail: sorbera@unipa.it Berg et al. 1999), has described rodent activity in HB in
terms of behavioral structure (Casarrubea et al. 2009a, b,
M. S. Magnusson
2010b) and its temporal organization (t-pattern analysis)
Human Behavior Laboratory, University of Iceland,
Reykjavik, Iceland (Casarrubea et al. 2010a). In particular, by means of
e-mail: msm@hi.is t-pattern analysis, it has been observed that the different
178 Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189

events, characterizing rat behavior in HB, present close anxiolytic effect in rodents (lower dose=0.25 mg/kg)
interrelationships which occur sequentially and with signif- without affecting locomotor activity (higher dose=2 mg/kg)
icant constraints on the interval lengths separating them (Casarrubea et al. 2009b; Harada et al. 2006).
(Casarrubea et al. 2010a). On this point, when behavioral
units are repeatedly performed, as parts of non-randomly Experimental procedure
repeated real-time patterns, variations in the content and
structure of t-patterns, as a function of experimental factors, All experimental observations were carried out from 08:00
can be expected to reflect changes in underlying neuro- to 12:00 a.m. To minimize possible transfer effects and
physiological processes. Accordingly, t-pattern analysis has avoid possible visual or olfactory influences, rats were
already been successfully utilized in previous researches to transferred from housing to testing room inside their own
study behavioral modifications in neuro-psychiatric con- home cages and allowed to acclimate for 30 min far from
ditions (Kemp et al. 2008; Lyon and Kemp 2004), patterns the HB apparatus. Environmental temperature in the testing
of behavioral synchronization in human beings (Grammer room was maintained equal to the housing room. Each subject
et al. 1998), motor skill responses in body movement was placed in HB and allowed to freely explore for 10 min.
(Castañer et al. 2009), hormonal–behavioral relationships Rats, experimentally naïve, were observed only once. After
(Hirschenhauser et al. 2002), interaction among human each observation, HB apparatus was carefully cleaned with
beings and animals (Kerepesi et al. 2005) and route-tracing ethylic alcohol (70%). Rodents’ behavior was recorded
stereotypy in rodents (Bonasera et al. 2008). through a digital camera, and video files were stored in a
The aim of the present research was to study, by means personal computer for following analyses. The camera was
of descriptive and t-pattern analyses, whether and how the placed in front of the transparent Plexiglas wall.
temporal structure of a rat’s behavior in HB is affected by Principles of laboratory animal care were followed, and
pharmacological manipulation of anxiety condition. To this experiments were complied with the current laws of Italy
purpose, different doses of the reference anxiolytic drug on the Protection of Animals. Experimental procedures
diazepam (DZP) were administered to different groups of were approved by a veterinary committee appointed by the
male Wistar rats. University of Palermo and conducted in accordance with the
European Communities Council Directive (86/609/EEC).

Materials and methods Data analysis

Apparatus The ethogram (Table 1), including 11 behavioral elements,


was the same utilized in our recent studies (Casarrubea et
The HB apparatus consisted of a square arena 50×50 cm al. 2009a, b, 2010a). Video files were coded by means of a
made of white opaque Plexiglas with a raised floor, software coder (The Observer, Noldus Information Tech-
positioned 5 cm above a white opaque Plexiglas sub-floor nology BV, The Netherlands) and event log files generated
and containing four equidistant holes, 4 cm in diameter. for each subject. To detect temporal relationships among
Each hole center was 10 cm from the two nearest walls so behavioral elements, event log files were processed with
that holes were equidistant from adjacent corners. The THEME™ (PatternVision Ltd, Iceland and Noldus Infor-
arena was surrounded by three white opaque Plexiglas mation Technology BV, The Netherlands), a software
walls and a front transparent one (Casarrubea et al. 2009a). carrying out an analysis aiming at evaluating possible
significant relationships (i.e., critical intervals) among
Subjects and drugs events in the course of time (t-pattern analysis) (Magnusson
2000). T-pattern detection algorithm searches for relation-
Fifty-six specific pathogen-free male Wistar rats (Morini, ships between events in behavioral data by taking into
Italy), 55–60 days old, weighing 220–250 g were used. account, the order, timing, and frequency of these events.
Animals were individually housed in a room maintained at Figure 1 exemplifies the occurrences of a three-element
23±1°C, under a 12-h light/dark cycle (light on, 7:00 a.m.– t-pattern within a hypothetical data set encompassing 40
light off, 7:00 p.m.). Food (standard laboratory pellets— events. The algorithm compares the distributions of each
Mucedola, Italy) and water were available “ad libitum”. pair of the behavioral elements A and B searching for a
Animals were randomly assigned to four groups of 14 time window (interval) so that, more often than chance
subjects: one group received 1 ml of saline IP as control expectation, A is followed by B within that time window. In
and three groups were IP injected with DZP (Roche; 0.25, this case, A and B are by definition a t-pattern indicated as
0.5 and 2 mg/kg in 1 ml of saline). DZP doses were chosen (A B). In a second step, such first level t-patterns are
since they have been demonstrated to provide a clear marked and considered as potential A or B terms in higher
Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189 179

Table 1 Ethogram of rat behav-


ior in hole board Wa Rat walks around sniffing the environment
Cl Rat maintains an erect posture leaning against the Plexiglas box. Usually associated with Sniffing
IS Rat sniffs the environment standing on the ground
HD Rat puts its head into one of the four holes
ES Rat sniffs the hole border without inserting the head inside
Re Rat maintains an erect posture without leaning against the plexiglas box. Usually associated with sniffing
FPL Rat licks or grooms its forepaws
Wa walking, Cl climbing, IS HPL Rat licks or grooms its hind paws
immobile sniffing, HD head-dip,
ES edge-sniff, Re rearing, FPL FG Rat rubs its face (ears, mouth, vibrissae, and eyes) with rapid circular movements of its forepaws
front-paw licking, HPL hind-paw BG Rat licks its body combing the fur by fast movements of incisors
licking, FG face grooming, BG Im A fixed posture is maintained
body grooming, Im immobility

patterns, for example, ((A B) C). Thus, more complex (that is, for each group, the mean number of detected two-,
patterns may be created following this hierarchical bottom- three-, four-, and five-element t-patterns); (5) mean number of
up detection process up to any level. When no more different t-patterns detected in each group; (6) behavioral
patterns are found, the search stops. A more detailed density stripes; (7) percent modification of t-patterns including
description of concepts and theories behind t-pattern each behavioral element; (8) percent modification of t-patterns
analysis, and related detection procedures, can be found in including specific behavioral sequences.
our previous articles (Casarrubea et al. 2010a; Magnusson
2000, 2004). Graphical representations
T-pattern detection software THEME™ allows the
researcher to set specific search parameters. In the present Due to the large amount of space necessary to illustrate
study, t-pattern analysis has been carried out by using the individual t-patterns by means of classical tree representa-
same search parameters employed in our recent investiga- tions (Fig. 1) (Magnusson 2000), t-patterns behavioral
tion (Casarrubea et al. 2010a): stripes, that is, the graphical representation of t-patterns
occurrences in a compact way along a timeline x-axis, have
– “significance level” (maximum accepted probability of any
recently been introduced (Casarrubea et al. 2010a). Thus, in
critical interval relationship to occur by chance)=0.0001
the present paper, besides histograms showing mean values,
– “lumping factor” (forward and backward transition
behavioral stripes are presented.
probability above which A and B of a t-pattern (A B)
are lumped, that is, A and B are not considered
Statistics
separately but only as the (A B) pattern)=0.90
– “minimum samples” (minimum percent of subjects in
Between groups comparison
which a pattern must occur to be detected)=0.51
– “minimum occurrences” (minimum number of times a
One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), followed by
t-pattern must occur to be detected)=8
Newman–Keuls post hoc test for multiple comparisons,
The following parameters of behavioral response to was carried out to assess possible diazepam-induced
anxiety were studied: (1) mean durations of each behavioral modifications of: (a) each behavioral element’s mean
element and mean latencies of its first appearance; (2) duration; (b) mean latency of each behavioral element’s
overall number of t-patterns detected for each group; (3) first appearance; (c) mean number of detected t-patterns for
mean number of detected t-patterns for each group; (4) each group; (d) mean number of detected t-patterns on the
mean number of detected t-patterns, on the basis of their length basis of their length and (e) mean number of detected

Fig. 1 Exemplifying t-pattern encompassing three hypothetical events. behavioral occurrences (grey letters) are left out of the whole timeline.
Bold arrow, period of observation (T0–Tx) consisting of 40 events “First”, “second”, and “following steps” (ellipses) indicate search runs
(black letters). The ((A B) C) t-pattern becomes evident if all the other carried out by THEME software following a bottom-up detection process
180 Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189

t-patterns on the basis of their different composition. Chi- induced modifications for walking (F3,963 =8.69; p≤0.0001),
square test was carried out to assess possible diazepam- climbing (F3,725 =10.99; p≤0.0001), edge-sniff (F3,496 =4.10;
induced modifications on percent of t-patterns encompass- p=0.007) and face grooming (F3,92 =3.07; p=0.032). New-
ing each behavioral element and on percent of t-patterns man–Keuls post hoc test for multiple comparisons highlight-
encompassing specific sequences. ed significant (p≤0.05) modifications for walking at all DZP
doses, climbing at DZP 0.25 and 2 mg/kg, edge-sniff at DZP
Within-group statistical validation 2 mg/kg and face grooming at DZP 0.5 and 2 mg/kg.
Latencies of each behavioral element’s first appearance
Each t-pattern implies a statistical significance test among showed no significant diazepam-induced changes.
critical intervals separating its structural elements. Howev-
er, the huge number of possible relationships raises the T-patterns number and length
question whether the number of detected t-patterns is
different by chance. THEME™ deals with this critical DZP administration induced a dose-dependent decrease
issue by repeatedly randomizing and analyzing original of total detected t-patterns amount: 2,750 (saline), 2,289
data. Thus, for each experimental group, the mean number (DZP 0.25 mg/kg), 1,937 (DZP 0.5 mg/kg) and 1,762
of t-patterns (±1 SD) detected in randomized data was then (DZP 2 mg/kg).
compared with the one obtained from the real data. Mean number±SE of detected t-patterns in each group is
presented in Fig. 3. ANOVA showed a significant effect of
DZP (F3,55 =3.908; p=0.013). Newman–Keuls post hoc test
Results for multiple comparisons revealed significant differences
(p≤0.05) between saline and the two higher DZP doses (0.5
Durations and latencies and 2 mg/kg).
Mean number±SE of detected t-patterns, on the basis of
Mean duration±SE of each behavioral component is pre- their length, is illustrated in Fig. 4. ANOVA showed a
sented in Fig. 2. ANOVA showed significant diazepam- significant effect of DZP for all t-patterns of different

Fig. 2 Mean durations±SE (in


seconds) of each behavioral
pattern. Data were obtained
from the analysis of four groups
of 14 rats IP injected with saline
or diazepam 0.25, 0.5, and
2 mg/kg dissolved in 1 ml of
saline. * p≤0.05, significant
differences in comparison with
saline (ANOVA and Newman–
Keuls post hoc test). For abbre-
viations, see Table 1
Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189 181

required. A demonstration of this possible drawback is


represented in Fig. 6 showing trees of four different
t-patterns, each encompassing walking and concerning only
one subject. Taking into consideration that: (a) much more
different t-patterns, encompassing the same behavioral
component, are present in the same subject, (b) 11 different
behavioral components are present in the ethogram and (c)
14 subjects have been tested for each of the four different
groups, a comprehensive t-pattern description, represented by
behavioral stripes, has recently been introduced (Casarrubea
et al. 2010a). Figure 7 shows stripes of behavioral density on
the basis of the presence of t-patterns encompassing each
Fig. 3 Mean number±SE of t-patterns for each group. *p≤0.05, element of the ethogram (Table 1). Since lower level
significant differences in comparison with saline (ANOVA and
Newman–Keuls post hoc test). Data were obtained from the analysis
t-patterns may be included in higher level ones, bar
of 56 rats thicknesses express behavioral density in terms of t-pattern
occurrences. The stripes show that the largest amount of
lengths except for the two-element ones (F3,55 =2.55; p= t-patterns includes walking, climbing, immobile sniffing,
0.06; F3,55 =17.38; p≤0.0001; F3,55 =3.14; p=0.033; and head-dip, and edge-sniff. Such t-patterns were found
F2,41 =7.81; p=0.001, respectively for two-, three-, four-, prevalently within the first 5-min observation. Rearing,
and five-element t-patterns). Newman–Keuls post hoc test front-paw licking, face grooming, body grooming, and
revealed significant changes (p≤0.05) for three- and five- immobility were less represented. No t-patterns including
element t-patterns at all doses and for four-element t-patterns rearing were detected at DZP 0.5 and 2 mg/kg whereas those
at 0.5 mg/kg dose. encompassing grooming were not identified with the higher
Figure 5 illustrates the mean number±SE of different DZP dose. Table 2 presents quantitative aspects underlying
t-patterns on the basis of their different composition. ANOVA results illustrated in Fig. 7, that is, DZP effects on percent
showed a highly significant effect of DZP (F3,55 =11.74; distribution of t-patterns encompassing each behavioral
p≤0.0001). Newman–Keuls post hoc test highlighted signif- element. DZP induced a significant (p≤0.05, chi-square test)
icant (p≤0.05) modifications for all doses of DZP. increase of t-patterns including walking at all doses, rearing
at 0.25 mg/kg and immobility at 0.5 and 2 mg/kg.
T-patterns composition Significant reductions were found at all doses for t-patterns
encompassing climbing, immobile sniffing, and edge-sniff;
Individual behavioral components as to front-paw licking and face grooming, a decrease was
observed at the higher DZP dose. Concerning t-patterns
To illustrate the occurrences of the numerous and different including head-dip, significant reductions were detected at
t-patterns during the 10-min period of observation through DZP 0.25 and 0.5 mg/kg and significant increase at the
their tree representation, a large amount of space would be higher dose. Finally, t-patterns including body grooming at

Fig. 4 Mean number±SE of


t-patterns on the basis of their
lengths. *p≤0.05, significant
differences in comparison with
saline (ANOVA and
Newman–Keuls post hoc test).
Data were obtained from the
analysis of 56 rats
182 Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189

ing) and (edge-sniff ➔ head-dip) sequences. Significant


increase of t-patterns including (climbing ➔ walking) and
(head-dip ➔ edge-sniff) were evidenced for all DZP
administered groups. T-patterns containing (edge-sniff ➔
walking) were significantly reduced at DZP 0.25 and
0.5 mg/kg and significantly increased at the higher dose.
T-patterns encompassing (head-dip ➔ walking) were signif-
icantly reduced at the lower DZP dose and significantly
increased at 0.5 and 2 m/kg. Finally, t-patterns containing
(face grooming ➔ body grooming) at the higher DZP dose,
and those encompassing (body grooming ➔ face grooming),
in control group and at all doses, were not detected.
Fig. 5 Mean number±SE of t-patterns on the basis of their different
composition. *p≤0.05, significant differences in comparison with Within groups statistical validation
saline (ANOVA and Newman–Keuls post hoc test). Data were
obtained from the analysis of 56 rats
Within groups comparison is illustrated in Fig. 9. For each
experimental group, t-patterns length distribution in real
the higher dose and hind-paw licking at all doses were not data and in random generated data (±1 SD) is showed.
evidenced. Results indicate that the largest amount of independent
t-patterns detected is present in real, rather than in random
Behavioral sequences generated data.

Density stripes, on the basis of t-patterns containing


specific behavioral sequences are illustrated in Fig. 8. The Discussion
evaluated sequences were chosen taking into consideration
the already demonstrated relationships among their consti- The present study demonstrates, for the first time, that
tutive components (Casarrubea et al. 2009a, b, c). For all pharmacological manipulation of anxiety condition causes
groups, t-patterns were prevalently found within the first clear and significant modifications in the temporal structure
period of observation, except (face grooming ➔ body of a rat’s behavioral response to anxiety in hole board, both
grooming) sequence which presented a more homogeneous in terms of complexity and variability.
distribution during the 10-min temporal window. In
addition, at the higher dose, no t-patterns encompassing Durations and latencies
(face grooming ➔ body grooming) were detected. Table 3
presents quantitative aspects underlying results illustrated in Walking duration has been demonstrated to be significantly
Fig. 8 and shows DZP effects on percent distribution of increased following DZP (Fig. 2). Therefore, it would be
t-patterns encompassing specific behavioral sequences. possible to conclude that anxiolytic treatment, making an
At all doses, DZP induced significant (p≤0.05, chi-square animal less anxious toward an unfamiliar environment,
test) reductions of t-patterns containing (walking ➔ climb- facilitates a rodent’s exploratory propensity. However, if

Fig. 6 Occurrences of four different t-patterns encompassing walking t-pattern: (1), first event; (2), second event; (3), third event; (4), fourth
along a 10-min observational period. Progressive numbers, inside event; (5), fifth event. Data were obtained from the analysis of rat 1
brackets on the left, indicate events order appearance within each from saline group
Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189 183

Fig. 7 Behavioral stripes for each group. Bar thicknesses express groups, t-patterns encompassing HPL have not been detected. Data
behavioral density in terms of t-patterns encompassing each behav- were obtained from the analysis of 56 rats. For abbreviations, see
ioral element, during a 10-min observational window (x-axis). For all Table 1

this propensity is increased by DZP, it is unclear why be appraised by means of “conventional” descriptive
climbing, representing the vertical exploration of walls, approaches.
results significantly reduced (Fig. 2). A similar issue arises
for edge-sniff and head-dip: if the former shows a dose- T-patterns number and length
dependent duration increase, in agreement with a possible
drug-induced facilitation of exploratory propensity, the DZP induces, at all doses, a clear-cut reduction of t-patterns
latter remains unexpectedly unmodified. Thus, injected total number. A decrease is evident between saline and
animals present not homogeneous modifications of explor- 2 mg/kg diazepam-injected groups: a difference of roughly
atory components duration which, instead, should be 1,000 t-patterns (par. 3.2—Table 2). Accordingly, mean
influenced similarly by anti-anxiety treatment. It is possible occurrences for each group demonstrate that diazepam
to conclude that exploratory activities, in their various administration induces a dose-dependent decrease of
expressions, are quite differently affected by DZP. More- t-patterns (Fig. 3). Such modification can be further
over, a discrepancy is also present for grooming compo- appreciated from two different points of view: firstly, by
nents, whose durations demonstrate that face grooming is taking into consideration the mean number of t-patterns on
significantly reduced, whereas front-paw licking and body the basis of their length; secondly, by taking into
grooming remain unmodified (Fig. 2). These findings call consideration the mean number of t-patterns on the basis
for complex underlying behavioral dynamics which cannot of their different composition. The number of t-patterns
184 Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189

Table 2 Number and percent values of t-patterns encompassing each behavioral element in saline and diazepam-injected groups

T-patterns encompassing each Saline Diazepam


behavioral element
0.25 mg/kg 0.5 mg/kg 2 mg/kg

2,750a 2,289a 1,937a 1,762a

Wa 1,199 (43.6%) 1,192* (52.07%) 1,002* (51.72%) 1,163* (66.01%)


Cl 1,567 (56.98%) 1,112* (48.58%) 760* (39.23%) 733* (41.6%)
Re 16 (0.58%) 308* (13.45%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
IS 1,670 (60.72%) 1,292* (56.44%) 1,104* (56.99%) 956* (54.25%)
ES 1,036 (37.67%) 641* (28.01%) 407* (21.01%) 508* (28.83%)
HD 1,199 (43.6%) 623* (27.21%) 777* (40.11%) 879* (49.88%)
FPL 42 (1.52%) 48 (2.09%) 36 (1.85%) 10* (0.56%)
HPL 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
FG 79 (2.87%) 52 (2.27%) 45 (2.32%) 10* (0.56%)
BG 57 (2.07%) 47 (2.05%) 44 (2.27%) 0 (0%)
Im 46 (1.67%) 40 (1.74%) 122* (6.29%) 65* (3.68%)

Data were obtained from the analysis of 56 rats. For abbreviations, see Table 1
*p≤0.05, significant differences in comparison with saline (Chi-square test)
a
Total number of detected t-patterns for each group

encompassing three, four and five elements, and the behavioral complexity and variability. Therefore, it is
number of t-patterns of different composition results were possible to suggest that pharmacological modulation of
largely reduced by DZP administration (Figs. 4 and 5). anxiety level significantly affects the temporal structure of
These pieces of evidence demonstrate that DZP modifies behavioral response to anxiety in HB.

Fig. 8 Behavioral stripes for each group. Bar thicknesses express Data were obtained from the analysis of 56 rats. For abbreviations, see
behavioral density in terms of t-patterns encompassing specific Table 1
behavioral sequences, during a 10-min observational window (x-axis).
Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189 185

Table 3 Number and percent values of t-patterns encompassing specific sequences in saline and diazepam-injected groups

T-patterns encompassing Saline Diazepam


specific sequences
0.25 mg/kg 0.5 mg/kg 2 mg/kg

2,750a 2,289a 1,937a 1,762a

Wa→Cl 481 (17.49%) 267* (11.66%) 212* (10.94%) 240* (13.62%)


Cl→Wa 180 (6.54%) 278* (12.14%) 155* (8.01%) 254* (14.41%)
ES→HD 298 (10.83%) 165* (7.2%) 87* (4.49%) 156* (8.85%)
HD→ES 109 (3.96%) 152* (6.64%) 140* (7.22%) 197* (11.18%)
ES→Wa 309 (11.23%) 127* (5.54%) 115* (5.93%) 341* (19.35%)
HD→Wa 209 (7.6%) 140* (6.11%) 193* (9.96%) 306* (17.36%)
FG→BG 57 (2.07%) 37 (1.61%) 32 (1.65%) 0 (0%)
BG→FG 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)

Data were obtained from the analysis of 56 rats. For abbreviations, see Table 1
*p≤0.05, significant differences in comparison with saline (Chi-square test)
a
Total number of detected t-patterns for each group

Fig. 9 Within groups statistical validation. x-Axis, t-patterns length distribution in real data and in random generated data (±1 SD) assessed for
ten random runs as provided by THEME. y-Axis, number of independent t-patterns. Data were obtained from the analysis of 56 rats
186 Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189

T-patterns composition increase of walking duration (Fig. 2) and percent of t-patterns


encompassing walking (Table 2); secondly, because immo-
Individual behavioral components bility durations are not significantly modified following DZP
(Fig. 2); finally, because the doses of DZP, used in this
DZP induces clear and significant changes of t-patterns experiment, were chosen since they have been demonstrated
occurrences containing single behavioral components and not to provoke locomotor activity impairments (Casarrubea
specific sequences. et al. 2009b; Harada et al. 2006). Since grooming activity
Several studies have demonstrated a significant increase has been reported to be related with anxiety condition
of locomotor activity following administration of non- (Kalueff and Tuohimaa 2004; Spruijt et al. 1992), diazepam-
sedative doses of anti-anxiety molecules in rodents (Harada induced anxiolysis could provoke a consensual decrease
et al. 2006; Kralic et al. 2002; Shimada et al. 1995). Our both of anxiety episodes and of t-patterns containing
results are well in agreement with such findings. Indeed, all grooming behavioral elements.
t-patterns including walking (Table 2) are significantly
increased. On the basis of the relationships between walking Behavioral sequences
and climbing, (Casarrubea et al. 2009a, b, 2010a), DZP
should influence horizontal (walking) and vertical (climbing) In untreated rodents, the behavioral components edge-sniff,
exploratory patterns in a similar way. Instead, we observed head-dip, walking, climbing, front-paw licking, face
that t-patterns including climbing were significantly reduced. grooming, and body grooming are arranged in the follow-
Such opposite effects, evident at higher dose, were also ing sequences: (edge-sniff ➔ head-dip), (head-dip ➔ edge-
observed on t-patterns including edge-sniff (decrease) or sniff ), (walking ➔ climbing), (climbing ➔ walking), and
head-dip (increase), hole exploratory elements related to one (front-paw licking ➔ face grooming ➔ body grooming)
another (Casarrubea et al. 2009a, 2010a). Further on, these (Casarrubea et al. 2010a). Present results demonstrate that
data confirm that DZP heavily influences the temporal t-patterns including the sequences (edge-sniff ➔ head-dip)
structure of all exploratory activities. and (walking ➔ climbing) are significantly reduced by DZP
Animal behavior in HB has been suggested to originate at all doses (Table 3). On the contrary, t-patterns including
from an antagonism between an exploratory and an escape the sequences (head-dip ➔ edge-sniff) and (climbing ➔
propensity (Montgomery 1955; Saitoh et al. 2006). The walking) are significantly increased (Table 3). Since
results of this antagonism are strongly related to anxiety climbing can only occur at walls and the animal, as a
level. Such conflictual condition may explain what has result of the anxiolysis, walks more and remains prevalently
been observed in the present study concerning non- in central areas of the arena (where there are no walls), a
consensual DZP effects on t-patterns including different, reduction of the t-patterns including (walking ➔ climbing)
ethologically characterized, behavioral elements of rat sequences can be observed. Walking activity increase may
exploratory activity. Diazepam-induced anxiety level re- also explain the increase of (climbing ➔ walking) sequen-
duction decreases t-patterns encompassing climbing and ces. Indeed, if walking increases, transition possibilities
edge-sniff: this finding may be explained through a lower from climbing to walking will be consensually increased.
need to look for an escape way (climbing) and a lower need Such simple, phenomenological explanations give account
to explore the hole border (edge-sniff) before the animal only in part for the changes in the temporal sequences
inserts its head inside (head-dip). On the contrary, anxiety observed; moreover, they do not take into any consideration
level reduction determines the increase of t-patterns the functional meaning of the described t-patterns. Indeed,
including walking and head-dip, provoking a greater since surrounding walls and ground holes would trigger a
propensity to explore the novel environment (walking) cognitive process of choosing (i.e., whether to explore one
and the hole inside (head-dip). Further proof of DZP’s of the walls or one of holes for a possible escape way),
heavy influence on behavioral structure arises from the (walking ➔ climbing), and (edge-sniff ➔ head-dip)
significant reduction of t-patterns encompassing immobile sequences would express the animal cautious approach to
sniffing which has been suggested to represent a behavioral unknown environment and, as a consequence, would be
component related to cognitive processes of environmental heavily dependent on emotion and motivation: for instance,
information analysis (Casarrubea et al. 2008, 2009a, the subject first sniffs the outside of hole to be reassured
2010a). The significant percent reduction of t-patterns (do-Rego et al. 2006), then the inside to continue the
encompassing all grooming components at 2 mg/kg dose environmental exploration. On the other hand, (climbing ➔
and the significant increase of immobility at 0.5 and 2 mg/kg walking) and (head-dip ➔ edge-sniff) sequences could be
might arise from a drug-induced sedation which negatively more indicative of exploratory activities running to comple-
influences motor activities. However, a sedative effect can be tion, independently from a preliminary watchful approach: the
excluded for several reasons: firstly, because of the significant rat first puts its head inside hole, after that, when its head is
Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189 187

removed from the hole, it completes exploration by sniffing anxiety and/or depression. As a matter of fact, the largest
hole border. Thus, DZP would provoke an increase of those amount of behavioral studies on psychopharmacology of
exploratory sequences like (head-dip ➔ edge-sniff) and anxiety and depression has employed only a few descriptive
(climbing ➔ walking) detached from emotional/motivational parameters (i.e., latencies, durations, and frequencies) of
control. Diazepam-induced decoupling between exploratory distinct behavioral elements. In the hole board test, one of
activities and underlying emotional control could be consid- the most investigated elements is the head-dip. However,
ered in terms of animal risk assessment. Indeed, rodents are quite diverging findings surround this important component
well known to be very cautious toward unfamiliar environ- of the behavioral response to anxiety since increases
ments and/or objects. Risk assessment is extremely important (Takeda et al. 1998), decreases (Pellow et al. 1985), or no
in such mammals and, accordingly, it has been described as a modifications (Sayin et al. 1992) of head-dip frequencies
resulting equilibrium between propensity to investigate a have been described following anti-anxiety drugs. Descrip-
novel stimulus and the fear it causes (Augustsson et al. 2005). tive/quantitative approaches such as assessments of latencies
This delicate balance might be considered strictly dependent or durations are useful because they provide information
on emotional drives (Cardinal et al. 2002). Evidence that concerning each investigated behavioral element. However,
diazepam-administered rats poke their head into a hole such approaches are not able to detect the most important
significantly more often, without any preliminary sniffing features of behavior: the functional relationships among
activity of its border, would suggest the appearance of a behavioral elements. Therefore, descriptive approaches to
disadvantageous behavior for risk assessment. This fearless psychopharmacological studies should be, where possible,
approach to exploration is consistent with observations also partnered with different suitable techniques of behavioral
carried out in monkeys with surgical lesions of limbic investigation.
system. For instance, Klüver and Bucy have reported the so- A very important support to these researches can be
called ‘psychic blindness’, that is, an evident fearless given by multivariate analyses and, in particular, by the
behavior characterized by the ‘strong tendency to approach t-pattern one through which temporal parameters of a
animate and inanimate objects without hesitation’ (Klüver behavior, not discernable/interpretable by means of quanti-
and Bucy 1997). Moreover, in human beings, low doses of tative assessments, can be detected. Indeed, our results
benzodiazepines induce cognitive impairments, weakening suggest that complexity and/or variability of behavioral
of judgment capacity, and serious disinhibitory behavioral organization and the possible temporal rearrangement of
effects such as self-control loss (Trevor and Way 2006). t-patterns containing specific exploratory sequences such as
DZP effects on t-patterns temporal organization may (edge-sniff ➔ head-dip), (head-dip ➔ edge-sniff), (walking ➔
depend on benzodiazepines affinity for various GABA-A climbing), and (climbing ➔ walking) may represent useful
receptors subtypes (González-Pardo et al. 2006; Möhler et parameters to evaluate and compare different classes of anti-
al. 2002). By means of the radioligands [3H]diazepam and anxiety molecules.
[3H]flunitrazepam, CNS benzodiazepines binding sites
have been demonstrated to be highly localized in rat limbic
system (Blaestrup and Squires 1977). Accordingly, acute IP Conclusions
administrations of various GABA-A modulators, such as
diazepam, alprazolam, or zolpidem produces significant The present study demonstrates, for the first time, that the
variations in the metabolic activity of various rat limbic temporal structure of the Wistar rats behavioral response to
regions (González-Pardo et al. 2006) that have been anxiety in HB, significantly changes following pharmaco-
suggested to be implicated in the evaluation of motivation- logical manipulation of anxiety condition. Moreover, our
ally significant inputs (Cardinal et al. 2002). Therefore, the results suggest that multivariate t-pattern analysis of rat
behavioral changes, detected in present study, may depend behavior in HB could be a further, useful tool to study
on DZP activity in those limbic structures likely linked to possible effects of drugs influencing anxiety condition.
risk assessment, a process critically influenced by environ-
mental cues. Acknowledgments This work was supported by a grant from the
“University of Palermo”, Palermo, Italy.

T-pattern analysis in psychopharmacological studies


References
Behavioral Pharmacology has been conservative in the
renewal of its methods. An elegant review by Kalueff et al.
Adamec R, Head D, Blundell J, Burton P, Berton O (2006) Lasting
(2007) underlines an unproductive and persistent associa- anxiogenic effects of feline predator stress in mice: sex differences
tion: the lack of converging findings and new/alternative in vulnerability to stress and predicting severity of anxiogenic
approaches in the study of important disorders such as response from the stress experience. Physiol Behav 88:12–29
188 Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189

Augustsson H, Dahlborn K, Meyerson BJ (2005) Exploration and Harada K, Aota M, Inoue T, Matsuda R, Mihara T, Yamaji T et al
risk assessment in female wild house mice (Mus musculus (2006) Anxiolytic activity of a novel potent serotonin 5-HT2C
musculus) and two laboratory strains. Physiol Behav 84:265– receptor antagonist FR260010: a comparison with diazepam and
277 buspirone. Eur J Pharmacol 553:171–184
Blaestrup C, Squires RF (1977) Specific benzodiazepine receptors in Hirschenhauser K, Frigerio D, Grammer K, Magnusson MS (2002)
rat brain characterized by high-affinity [3H]diazepam binding. Monthly patterns of testosterone and behavior in prospective
Proc Natl Acad Sci 74:3805–3809 fathers. Horm Behav 42:172–181
Bonasera SJ, Schenk AK, Luxenberg EJ, Tecott LH (2008) A novel Kalueff AV, Tuohimaa P (2004) Grooming analysis algorithm for
method for automatic quantification of psychostimulant-evoked neurobehavioural stress research. Brain Res Prot 13:151–
route-tracing stereotypy: application to Mus musculus. Psycho- 158
pharmacology (Berl) 196:591–602 Kalueff AV, Tuohimaa P (2005) The grooming analysis algorithm
Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, Hall J, Everitt BJ (2002) Emotion and discriminates between different levels of anxiety in rats: potential
motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and utility for neurobehavioural stress research. J Neurosci Methods
prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:321–352 143:169–177
Casarrubea M, Sorbera F, Crescimanno G (2006) Effects of 7-OH- Kalueff AV, Wheaton M, Murphy DL (2007) What’s wrong with my
DPAT and U 99194 on the behavioral response to hot plate test, mouse model? Advances and strategies in animal modeling of
in rats. Physiol Behav 89:552–562 anxiety and depression. Behav Brain Res 179:1–18
Casarrubea M, Sorbera F, Crescimanno G (2008) Multivariate Kamei J, Hirose N, Oka T, Miyata S, Saitoh A, Yamada M (2007)
analysis of the modifications induced by an environmental Effects of metylphenidate on the hyperemotional behavior in
acoustic cue on rat exploratory behavior. Physiol Behav 93: olfactory bulbectomized mice by using the hole-board test. J
687–696 Pharmacol Sci 103:175–180
Casarrubea M, Sorbera F, Crescimanno G (2009a) Structure of rat Kemp AS, Fillmore PT, Lenjavi MR, Lyon M, Chicz-Demet A,
behavior in hole-board: I) multivariate analysis of response to Touchette PE, Sandman CA (2008) Temporal patterns of self-
anxiety. Physiol Behav 96:176–179 injurious behavior correlate with stress hormone levels in the
Casarrubea M, Sorbera F, Crescimanno G (2009b) Structure of rat developmentally disabled. Psychiatry Res 157:181–189
behavior in holeboard: II) multivariate analysis of modifications Kerepesi A, Jonsson GK, Miklósi A, Topál J, Csányi V, Magnusson
induced by diazepam. Physiol Behav 96:683–692 MS (2005) Detection of temporal patterns in dog-human
Casarrubea M, Sorbera F, Crescimanno G (2009c) Multivariate data interaction. Behav Process 70:69–79
handling in the study of rat behavior: an integrated approach. Klüver H, Bucy PC (1997) Preliminary analysis of functions of the
Behav Res Methods 41:772–781 temporal lobes in monkeys. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci
Casarrubea M, Sorbera F, Magnusson M, Crescimanno G (2010a) 9:606–620
Temporal patterns analysis of rat behavior in hole-board. Behav Kralic JE, O’Buckley TK, Khisti RT, Hodge CW, Homanics GE,
Brain Res 208:124–131 Morrow AL (2002) GABA-A receptor alpha-1 subunit deletion
Casarrubea M, Sorbera F, Santangelo A, Crescimanno G (2010b) alters receptor subtype assembly, pharmacological and behavioral
Microstructure of rat behavioral response to anxiety in hole- responses to benzodiazepines and zolpidem. Neuropharmacology
board. Neurosci Lett 481:82–87 43:685–694
Castañer M, Torrents C, Anguera MT, Dinusová M, Jonsson GK Lino-de-Oliveira C, De Lima TCM, Carobrez AP (2005) Structure of
(2009) Identifying and analyzing motor skill responses in body the rat behaviour in the forced swimming test. Behav Brain Res
movement and dance. Behav Res Methods 41:857–867 158:243–250
Choleris E, Thomas AW, Kavaliers M, Prato FS (2001) A detailed Lyon M, Kemp AS (2004) Increased temporal patterns in choice
ethological analysis of the mouse open field test: effects of responding and altered cognitive processes in schizophrenia and
diazepam, chlordiazepoxide and an extremely low frequency mania. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 172:211–219
pulsed magnetic field. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25:235–260 Magnusson MS (2000) Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior:
Cruz AP, Frei F, Graeff FG (1994) Ethopharmacological analysis of T-patterns and their detection. Behav Res Methods Instrum
rat behavior on the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacol Biochem Comput 32:93–110
Behav 49:171–176 Magnusson MS (2004) Repeated patterns in behavior and other
do-Rego JC, Viana AF, Le Maitre E, Deniel A, Rates SMK, Leroux- biological phenomena. In: Oller DK, Griebel U (eds) Evolution
Nicollet I, Costentin J (2006) Comparisons between anxiety tests of communication systems: a comparative approach. The MIT,
for selection of anxious and non anxious mice. Behav Brain Res Cambridge, pp 111–128
169:282–288 Möhler H, Fritschy JM, Rudolph U (2002) A new benzodiazepine
Espejo EF (1997) Structure of the mouse behaviour on the elevated pharmacology. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 300:2–8
plus-maze test of anxiety. Behav Brain Res 86:105–112 Montgomery KC (1955) The relation between fear induced by novel
File SE, Wardill AG (1975) The reliability of the hole-board stimulation and exploratory behavior. J Comp Physiol Psychol
apparatus. Psychopharmacologia 44:47–51 48:254–260
González-Pardo H, Conejo NM, Arias JL (2006) Oxidative metabo- Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M (1985) Validation of open:
lism of limbic structures after acute administration of diazepam, closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of
alprazolam and zolpidem. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol anxiety in the rat. J Neurosci Methods 14:149–167
Psychiatry 30:1020–1026 Saitoh A, Hirose N, Yamada M, Yamada M, Nozaki C, Oka T, Kamei
Grammer K, Kruck KB, Magnusson M (1998) The courtship dance: J (2006) Changes in emotional behavior of mice in the hole-
patterns of nonverbal synchronization in opposite-sex encounters. board test after olfactory bulbectomy. J Pharmacol Sci 102:
J Nonverbal Behav 22:3–29 377–386
Psychopharmacology (2011) 215:177–189 189

Sayin U, Purali N, Ozkan T, Altug T, Buyukdevrim S (1992) Spruijt BM, van Hooff JA, Gispen WH (1992) Ethology and
Vigabatrin has an anxiolytic effect in the elevated plus-maze test neurobiology of grooming behavior. Physiol Rev 72:825–852
of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 43:529–535 Takeda H, Tsuji M, Matsumiya T (1998) Changes in head-dipping
Shimada T, Matsumoto K, Osanai M, Matsuda H, Terasawa K, behavior in the hole-board test reflect the anxiogenic and/or
Watanabe H (1995) The modified light/dark transition test in anxiolytic state in mice. Eur J Pharmacol 350:21–29
mice: evaluation of classic and putative anxiolytic and anxio- Trevor AJ, Way WL (2006) Sedative-hypnotic drugs. In: Katzung B (ed)
genic drugs. Gen Pharmac 26:205–210 Basic & clinical pharmacology, 10th edn. McGraw-Hill, pp 347-362
Spruijt BM, Gispen WH (1984) Behavioral sequences as an easily Van Den Berg CL, Van Ree JM, Spruijt BM (1999) Sequential
quantifiable parameter in experimental studies. Physiol Behav analysis of juvenile isolation-induced decreased social behavior
32:707–710 in the adult rat. Physiol Behav 67:483–488
Copyright of Psychopharmacology is the property of Springer Science & Business Media B.V. and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express
written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen