Sie sind auf Seite 1von 21

A model of consumer response to advertising

music
Geoffrey P. Lantos
Business Administration Department, Stonehill College, North Easton, Massachusetts, USA, and
Lincoln G. Craton
Department of Psychology, Stonehill College, North Easton, Massachusetts, USA

Abstract
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to provide a model of consumer response to music in broadcast commercials outlining four variables (listening
situation, musical stimulus, listener characteristics, and advertising processing strategy) that affect a consumer’s attitude toward the advertising music
(Aam).
Design/methodology/approach – The paper takes the form of an integrative review of the relevant literatures from the psychology of music,
marketing, and advertising.
Findings – Aam can be positively but also negatively influenced by many factors. Only some of these variables are employed in any typical study on
consumer response to music, which may account for some conflicting findings.
Practical implications – The paper discusses factors for effectively using commercial music to affect Aam, with special focus on advertising processing
strategy. Advertisers are urged to exercise extreme caution in using music and to always pretest its use considering factors identified in this paper. The
paper suggests ways in which the model can guide future research.
Originality/value – The paper integrates diverse literatures and outlines the major variables comprising our model of consumer response to
advertising music. Advertisers can use these variables as a checklist for factors to consider in selecting ad music.

Keywords Advertisements, Music, Attitude toward the ad, Music-message fit, Advertisement processing,
Central versus peripheral route processing, Elaboration likelihood model of persuasion

Paper type Conceptual paper

An executive summary for managers and executive receiving a favorable audience response and helping to achieve
readers can be found at the end of this article. advertising objectives so long as it does not distract listeners
from the commercial’s message – what we call the “music as
Introduction garnish assumption.” A second inadequacy of the literature
follows from this: Research has not yet explored the factors
Many broadcast ads incorporate music (Stewart et al., 1990), potentially causing music to be ineffective or even detrimental
and most discussions of music in advertising assume that to an ad’s success. Third, although music triggers both
including music will add value to the commercial (Stout et al., cognitive and affective listener responses, reactions to music
1990). This paper challenges that assumption, proposing a are usually conceptualized as being solely affective (Meyer,
model of consumer response to music in broadcast 1956, as cited in Kellaris and Kent, 2001; Morris and Boone,
commercials that outlines key influential variables (listening 1998). Yet, music ideally cognitively communicates a brand
situation, musical stimulus, listener characteristics, and message or a brand image (Chebat et al., 2001; Hecker, 1984;
advertising processing strategy) that determine a consumer’s Morris and Boone, 1998).
cognitive and affective musical responses. These responses We began addressing these three issues by introducing a
embody a new variable that we introduced previously (Craton new consumer response variable, Aam, which provides a
and Lantos, 2011), attitude toward the advertising music framework for conceptualizing both positive and negative
(Aam). After presenting the model, we offer recommendations cognitive and affective responses to music (Craton and
for advertisers concerning use of music and for researchers Lantos, 2011). The present article builds on that framework
seeking to learn more about consumer response to music in in order to address a fourth shortcoming of the literature:
advertising. Advertising music has almost always been treated as a one-
Our discussion of consumer response to advertising music
dimensional variable. For instance, studies have investigated
addresses four deficiencies in the literature. First is the
the effect of music’s presence versus absence (Macklin, 1988;
assumption that music will always add value to a commercial,
Middlestadt et al., 1994; Morris and Boone, 1998; Olsen,
1995; Park and Young, 1986; Wheatley and Brooker, 1994);
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at music as a classical conditioning variable (Gorn, 1982;
www.emeraldinsight.com/0736-3761.htm Kellaris and Cox, 1989; Pitt and Abratt, 1988); music as a
means of enhancing information processing (MacInnis and
Park, 1991; Mitchell and Olson, 1981); structural
Journal of Consumer Marketing characteristics such as tempo, pitch, and loudness (Alpert
29/1 (2012) 22– 42
q Emerald Group Publishing Limited [ISSN 0736-3761]
and Alpert, 1990; Bruner, 1990; Kellaris and Rice, 1993);
[DOI 10.1108/07363761211193028] and music-message fit (e.g. Kellaris et al., 1993; Shen and

22
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

Chen, 2006). While progress has been made on these and Cognitive components
other issues, an integrated, comprehensive discussion of .
Level and persistence of attention to music. This component
consumer response to advertising music appears to be lacking. of Aam refers to whether the ad music has contributed to
This paper is a first effort to build a model facilitating such a the long-standing advertising goal of gaining and holding
discussion. the consumer’s attention, in turn affecting whether and
In light of the above insufficiencies, this article has three how much the consumer attends to the ad as a whole.
purposes: .
Depth of processing of music. This construct from cognitive
1 to outline the important factors comprising a broad psychology (Craik and Lockhart, 1972; Craik and
conceptual model of consumer response to music in Tulving, 1975) is related to the enhance memory of ad
broadcast commercials; content advertising objective. Deep- or semantic-level
2 to use this model to point out the many possible hazards processing focuses on the meaning of the music (its
of using music in advertising for which advertisers should aesthetic properties), while shallow-level processing
pretest; and focuses on surface features of the music such as the
3 to show how the model can help guide future research. timbre of one of the instruments or the physical
appearance of a performer. Perhaps counter-intuitively,
We proceed by presenting and discussing the various some shallow processing of lyrics (phrasing, line length)
components of our model of consumer response to carrying the ad message is required in order for the sung
advertising (see Figure 1). Throughout, we point out message to enhance recall relative to a spoken message
possible ways music can go awry for the advertiser. We (Wallace, 1994).
conclude with managerial and academic research suggestions. .
Perceived features of music available for association. Relevant
to the create new music-brand associations objective, this
captures elements of the ad music the consumer has
The model
detected from among the wide range of features present
In order to understand the potential perils of using music in (structural features of the music such as rhythm or
commercials, we propose the model of consumer response to melody, its expressed emotion, the musical style, etc.),
music in advertising shown in Figure 1, which captures the and especially whether these are perceived as positive,
key variables that can work both in favor of and also against negative, or neutral.
music’s communication value for a brand. Audience .
Remembered features of music available for association. This
responses to music in commercials stem from a complex ties in with the tap prior associations with familiar music
interplay of four variables: advertising goal. Similar to the previous cognitive element,
1 the listening situation; this captures what these prior associations are (especially
2 the musical stimulus; how common to most people or idiosyncratic to a
3 listener characteristics; and particular consumer they might be) and whether the
4 the listener’s advertising processing strategy. consumer views these associations as positive, negative, or
neutral.
Together these four variables influence Figure 1’s centerpiece, .
Image suggested by music. This concerns the perceived
Aam, which we propose is a significant constituent of attitude “personality” of the music (e.g. sophisticated, sexy, or
toward the ad (Aad) – whether or not a message recipient likes lighthearted), and it relates to the create a brand image
a particular advertisement during a particular exposure goal. A brand personality can be molded either as music
occasion. An abundance of research – much of which is creates new associations or as it uses existing associations.
cited below – has shown that Aad, in turn, affects the .
Music perceived as distinctive or not. This concerns the
consumer’s attitude toward the brand (Ab ), which differentiate the brand objective. We propose that the
subsequently impacts purchase intention and brand choice. consumer makes a distinctiveness judgment, representing
This model does not attempt to depict the many possible how unique versus prototypical s/he considers the music
interactions between all these variables or their constituent to be and the extent to which s/he views this level of
elements but instead focuses on providing a “checklist” of uniqueness as favorable or unfavorable. Distinctive music
factors that influence Aam and its cognitive and affective that is viewed favorably (unfavorably) will help to
elements. We hope the model will be useful to practitioners positively (negatively) differentiate the brand.
and will provide a framework for research, ultimately leading .
Perceived music-message fit. Linked with the reinforce ad
to a more comprehensive, detailed, process-oriented model message with music-message fit objective, this captures
accounting for such interactions. In a subsequent article, we the consumer’s judgment of how well the music suits the
shall examine the literature on psychological mechanisms in message. High fit leads to a more favorable attitude than
music processing with an eye toward developing such a low fit, which can create possible dissonance and/or
model. confusion (Kellaris et al., 1993; North, Hargreaves,
McKenzie and Law, 2004). For instance, an upbeat
Attitude toward the advertising music (Aam) tune could be used for a brand positioned as “fun.” Given
In our previous article (Craton and Lantos, 2011), we the importance of communicating about the brand as an
introduced and developed our new construct, attitude toward overarching advertising objective, we believe this
the music (Aam). There, we discussed the cognitive and component is crucial.
affective components of Aam and how these influence specific Affective components
advertising objectives (see Table I). We now summarize the .
Emotions (feelings) evoked by music. Related to the evoke
components of Aam and their related advertising objectives emotions (feelings) goal, this concerns whether feeling
(objectives are underlined in the following discussion). states are actually evoked by the music, how intense they

23
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

Figure 1 The model of consumer response to advertising music

24
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

Table I Components of attitude toward the advertising music (Aam) and corresponding advertising objectives
Aam Objectives
Cognitive component Desired cognitive response
Level and persistence of attention to music Attract attention
Depth of processing of music Enhance memory of ad content
Perceived features of music available for association Create new music-brand associations
Remembered features of music available for association Tap prior associations with familiar music
Image suggested by music Create a brand image
Music perceived as distinctive or not Differentiate the brand
Perceived music-message fit Reinforce ad message with music-message fit
Affective component Desired affective response
Emotions (feelings) evoked by music Evoke emotions (feelings)
Mood induced by music Create a mood
Emotional memories activated by music Tap into emotion-laden memories
Emotional arousal response to music Alter emotional arousal level
Hedonic response to music Provide a positive hedonic experience

are, and how favorably or unfavorably the consumer views relative music appeal – that is, liking for one piece of music as
those emotional experiences. compared to another at a given point in time (Hargreaves
.
Mood induced by music. Related to the ad objective to et al., 2006). Finally, we define “musical taste” as an
create a mood, this captures whether the ad music leads to individual’s pattern of musical preferences over time.
mood induction, how favorable/unfavorable the mood is, Aam is a product of many influences, to which we turn next.
and whether the mood is perceived as consonant with the
brand’s message and image. Variables influencing formation of Aam
.
Emotional memories activated by music. Related to the tap As we review each of the four variables influencing formation
into emotion-laden memories ad objective, this reflects of Aam, depicted in Figure 1, we will speculate on which
whether such memories occur and how favorably the elements of Aam are most likely to be impacted by each,
consumer views them. referring to prior research when available. Future empirical
.
Emotional arousal response to music. One advertising goal is research can test each of these presumptions.
to alter emotional arousal level. This element concerns
whether any changes in emotional arousal occur as a result (1) The listening situation
of exposure to the ad music and whether or not these A commercial’s music plays in a context, not a vacuum. In
changes are viewed favorably by the consumer. general, music that is inappropriate for the listening situation
.
Hedonic response to music. A final ad objective is to provide a could negatively influence Aam. Research on the “uses and
positive hedonic experience. This variable captures how gratifications” of music affirms that music’s effects depend on
pleasant/unpleasant the consumer finds the ad music to be. listeners’ circumstances (North, Hargreaves and Hargreaves,
2004; North and Hargreaves, 1996a). People most often hear
Because each of the components of Aam described above can
ad music while engaged in other activities, in a particular
be either positive or negative – that is, they each can
social context, as a break from particular program content,
contribute to a favorable or unfavorable Aam – this new
and either voluntarily or involuntarily. We now briefly
variable allows practitioners and researchers to capture the
examine each of these types of circumstances.
complex and sometimes even contradictory nature of
consumer response toward a given piece of ad music. In this Ongoing activities
respect Aam is quite different from related constructs in the Listeners prefer music deemed appropriate in a particular
marketing and psychology literatures – such as “music setting (North and Hargreaves, 1996a, b, c; North and
appeal,” “liking,” or “music preference” – which are typically Hargreaves, 2000, Experiment 2). For instance, if the
measured with a single Likert-scale item (Allan, 2007; Finnäs, commercial’s music results in an undesirable change in
1989; Hargreaves et al., 2006; Rentfrow and Gosling, 2003; arousal level (e.g. intense music in an already stressful work
Tan et al., 2006). We believe that the present multifaceted environment), Aam could be negatively influenced. Although
formulation is a step forward that may help marketers and we have not found any directly relevant marketing research, it
researchers characterize, study, and weigh the trade-offs seems reasonable to suppose that consumer responses to
involved in selecting particular candidates for advertising identical ad music will vary depending on listeners’ activities
music. (driving, eating, etc.).
We distinguish several terms relating to musical response. We speculate that the components of Aam most likely to be
Following Allan (2007), we define “music appeal” as the directly influenced by current activities may be the
extent of liking/disliking of a musical stimulus at a particular consumer’s level and persistence of attention to the music,
point in time but – as we state above and illustrate in Figure 1 depth of processing of the music, and emotional arousal
– we treat it as synonymous with Aam and thus as multifaceted response to the music, since ongoing activities can enhance or
rather than unidimensional. We define “music preference” as inhibit these.

25
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

Social context message. For instance, a particular celebrity might be


Music often occurs in the context of and influences social incongruent with the music (e.g. Britney Spears and
behavior (Konecni, 1982; Lull, 1985; Shepherd, 2003). To classical music):
predict people’s response to music, it is important to
Role of music: foreground versus background
understand the social context in which music is heard:
Consumer response to commercial music is likely a function
whether listeners are alone or with others, and if with others,
of the role music plays within the ad (Stout and Leckenby,
what the nature of the relationships is (e.g. lovers versus
1988), i.e. whether music serves a lyrical foreground or an
coworkers) and whether they are working, socializing,
instrumental background (nonvocal) role. Foreground music
exercising, etc. (Sloboda et al., 2001). For instance, solemn
is an integral part of the commercial, playing a thematic,
music in a radio commercial heard during a party might evoke
message-enhancing role. Such music tends to be attention-
a negative listener response.
getting, distinctive, likely to be part of an affect-based
North, Hargreaves and Hargreaves (2004) suggest that
commercial, and probably relies on lyrics. Background music
when people listen to music with others, they have less control
is the opposite: Less dominant or noticeable – even incidental
over music choice and so like less what they hear, and
– and less distinct, less likely to be affect-based and more
listening alone likely leads to a higher degree of involvement
likely to be purely instrumental than foreground music
and hence liking. These findings could potentially hold for ad
(Alpert and Alpert, 1991). Yalch (1991) observed that music
music too, affecting the hedonic response element of Aam –
assists recall of advertising slogans when the slogans are
music could be more positively impactful when audience
included in a commercial as foreground music in the form of a
members are isolated and have selected by themselves the
jingle or song and music performs more effectively than the
programming in which the commercial occurs, although this
same verbal material presented with background music.
idea needs to be tested. On the other hand, a solitary
Brooker and Wheatley (1994) found that placement of
consumer who dislikes the ad music is freer to disengage
background music at an ad’s beginning sans voiceover drew
(e.g. mute the ad, grab a snack, etc.).
attention (albeit marginally) to and acted as a lead-in for the
Program content message, thereby enhancing it, while music serving as
A consumer’s reaction to an ad’s music might depend in part background for a voiceover drew some attention away from
on the program content during which the commercial the message. More research directly comparing the effects of
appears. Goldberg and Gorn (1987) discovered that, relative different uses of foreground and background music on
to a sad TV program, a cheery TV program induced a happier elements of Aam would be valuable to both practitioners and
mood in viewers, greater perceived commercial effectiveness, academics.
more affectively positive cognitive responses, and, to some Foreground music can either:
degree, enhanced recall. It appears that program content .
carry the ad’s message and meaning through musical
could affect almost any Aam component. One interesting lyrics, usually as a jingle, used to create a brand image,
question worth investigating is whether perceived music- mood, or feeling;
message fit might extend to program-content fit – for .
incorporate lyrics not directly carrying the ad’s message
instance, would consumers respond negatively to music with but used nonetheless as the primary means of
sexual innuendo that is played in a commercial running communication; or
during a family TV program? .
employ instrumental music sans lyrics in the foreground
with little voiceover or verbal message (Alpert and Alpert,
Voluntary versus involuntary exposure 1991).
North, Hargreaves and Hargreaves (2004) concluded that
under forced exposure, music was generally not enjoyed, a In this musical hierarchy, we propose that music’s impact will
finding that should be of key concern for advertisers since be strongest in the first bulleted point and least potent in the
most research on ad music is conducted under forced third bulleted point , although this remains to be tested. In the
exposure conditions (Allan, 2007; Bruner, 1990) and since case of the first bulleted point, in order for the sung message
listeners select neither commercials nor their music. Few to enhance recall relative to a spoken message some shallow
people in North, Hargreaves and Hargreaves’s (2004) study processing (that is, processing of “superficial” aspects of the
reported that they were enjoying the music they were being music like the length of sung phrases) of the lyrics carrying the
compelled to listen to, and even fewer agreed with the ad message must occur (Authors, disguised, under review;
statement “I wanted to hear the music for longer.” Forced Wallace, 1994) – thus, the music should be either simple
exposure would seem to be most likely to negatively affect enough to learn or familiar to the consumer. In all three cases
attention to, depth of processing of, and hedonic response to above, we suspect that foreground music will usually be a
music. significant positive or negative influence on Aad. Indeed,
because of its salience, the risks for negative Aam are probably
(2) The musical stimulus greater for foreground than for background music. In some
The advertising stimulus consists of the advertising message cases, using foreground music at a low volume might prove to
and interacting executional elements, which include many be a useful compromise – appealing to those people who like
factors as well as the ad’s music, such as the setting, the selection while not turning off listeners who dislike it.
characters, voiceover, and storyline. Although they are The potential risks in using background music, especially if
beyond the scope of this model, it should be noted that it is played at a low volume, are unclear. Although
these non-musical executional components become background music is so ubiquitous that we often do not
meaningful by their relationships to one another (Scott, notice its presence, some evidence suggests that it can
1990). Therefore, such elements must be carefully chosen so produce emotional and physical responses (Grayston, 1974).
that they work with, not against, the music and brand Nonetheless, other research has found that background music

26
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

had no significant effect on brand recall (Macklin, 1988; use cognitive capacity to learn the new music instead of the ad
Sewall and Sarel, 1986) nor on Aad or Ab (Macklin, 1988), message (Craton and Lantos, 2011). Allan (2006) reported
suggesting that its impact on consumer response is relatively that several studies have found original lyrics to be more
insignificant. By definition, background music may not draw effective than altered lyrics for gaining attention and recall,
attention or be deeply processed. However, if it is familiar or perhaps because the changed lyrics can irritate listeners. One
in a familiar genre that taps listeners’ associations, it may of Englis and Pennell’s (1994) studies (not cited by the
suggest an image for the product, much as retail stores use authors) found that a well-liked musical piece, when placed in
background music to provide ambience. Given our current the context of a commercial communication, might negatively
limited knowledge of its effects, background music too should affect brand perceptions because people dislike “their” music
be selected carefully. being appropriated for commercial purposes, thereby
negatively affecting emotions evoked by the music.
Musical source
However, the growing use of popular music in advertising
Ad music has three possible origins:
and increased willingness of musical artists to “sell out”
1 An original composition written, scored, and recorded
suggests that this is now less a problem as people become
specifically for the commercial. Although initially
more accepting of familiar artists allowing commercialization
unfamiliar to the audience and the most expensive
of their music and as commercial artists are more willing to
option, this provides marketers with the most control
“sell in” new tunes to get them the kind of exposure that radio
over variables affecting Aam. In particular, such music can
airplay no longer provides (Hampp, 2010).
be best tailored to the message, thereby enhancing
cognitive elements of Aam. Recognizable jingles create Structural characteristics
brand equity and feel more simple and real (Hampp, Music theorists, psychologists, and marketers interested in
2010). music differ on taxonomies for music’s structural elements.
2 An existing tune can either be copyrighted and available Bruner (1990) and others (cited in Kellaris and Kent, 2001)
via direct licensing; “needledrop” music (Allan, 2006) in suggest the following parsimonious scheme:
the public domain, available without cost (Belch and .
Time. Tempo (speed), rhythm (the pattern of accents
Belch, 2009), or stock music, prerecorded for purchase or given to beats or notes), meter (the division of a
rental (Allan, 2006). Music that is very well learned draws composition into units of equal time value called
attention to linked surface properties of melody and lyrics measures), and duration.
(shallow-level processing), which can enhance recall of an .
Pitch. Tonality (roughly, the “key” a piece is in), major/
ad message if it is contained in lyrics (Rubin, 1977; minor modality (Apel, 1973, cited in Kellaris and Kent,
Wallace, 1994). On the other hand, since existing tunes 1991; Stout and Leckenby, 1988), melody, and harmony
will be familiar to some or most listeners, they might come (chords or the blending of notes played simultaneously).
with undesirable remembered features of music available .
Texture. Timbre or tone color (the distinctiveness in tone
for association and/or have induced “wearout” making instruments sound different from one another),
(Hargreaves et al., 2006; Tan et al., 2006), thereby orchestration (the art of weaving together the unique sonic
negatively affecting the music perceived as distinctive or properties of multiple instruments to produce the complex
not Aam component. textural fabric of a musical piece), and volume.
3 Altered (adapted) music that modifies existing music .
Complexity. Objectively, complexity is the way in which the
somewhat in order to create some distinctiveness (music music is organized along the preceding three
perceived as distinctive or not) and be a better fit with the characteristics. Complex music has more elements and
commercial (perceived music-message fit), as well as to more variation of them (Finnäs, 1989).
avoid royalty payments if copyrighted (e.g. eBay modified
While research results are sometimes contradictory
Frank Sinatra’s hit “My Way” to “eBay”) (Allan, 2006).
concerning listener responses to structural characteristics
Until recently, the trend was away from original jingles in (Stout et al., 1990), some general patterns have emerged.
favor of classic pop songs (e.g. Nike’s pioneering use of the Marketing research and work in experimental aesthetics
Beatles’ “Revolution” in 1987), current hit songs (e.g. “1 2 3 (Hargreaves et al., 2006) both suggest that musical structure
4” by Feist for iPod Nano in 2008), or potential hit songs, all plays a central role in musical preference and musical taste.
in either existing or adapted form (Belch and Belch, 2009; Higher preference is associated with fast tempo, distinct
Howard, 2008; Steinburg and Parekh, 2008). However, in the rhythm, coherent melodies, absence of pronounced
late 2000’s the trend of licensing tunes from the ’60s, ’70s, dissonances in harmonies, a moderate amount of
and ’80s is being replaced by the countertrend of creating or complexity (Finnäs, 1989), and major keys, which lead to
reprising original branded melodies (e.g. State Farm’s “Like a positive thoughts and feelings, while minor keys are
good neighbor State Farm is there” and McDonald’s “I’m associated with negative cognition and affect (Hevner,
lovin’ It” (Hampp, 2010). 1935, cited in Stout and Leckenby, 1988; Kellaris and
Research comparing the effectiveness of these three musical Kent, 1991). However, preferences vary between
sources is scarce and has yielded mixed results. Allan’s (2006) sociodemographic groups (Finnäs, 1989; Hargreaves et al.,
research found that original vocals were more effective for 2006). Elements including tempo, texture, modality, and
gaining attention and recall (depth of processing) when a tonality have also been demonstrated to potentially boost
person found personal significance (emotional meaning) in arousal and hedonic response, resulting in greater purchase
popular music integrated with advertising, while altered intention (Allan, 2007). Stout et al. (1990) and Stout and
vocals were more effective when the opposite is true. Leckenby (1988) found modality was the most influential
However, we have speculated that advertisers using original structural characteristic of those tested on eliciting consumer
music might be defeating their cognitive goals if consumers reactions in TV commercials. Major and mixed modes

27
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

resulted in consumers learning more from, having greater teen-oriented retailer’s ads should feature pop while a high-
behavioral intent toward, and making more personal end fashion retailer’s commercials should include classical
connections with the ads than did minor modes. music).
Commercials featuring minor mode music were rated as However, note that negative attitudes toward disliked
more irritating and were generally scored less favorably. musical styles are as intense as positive attitudes toward
The above general patterns are somewhat misleading, favorite musical styles (Craton et al., 2008). Clearly, musical
however, since the literature is quite mixed. Consider the “pet peeves” are an important advertising consideration since
range of findings on the effect of tempo. Bruner (1990) they can lead to negative hedonic response.
reported that Anand and Holbrook (1985) found that a Musical artist/performance
moderate tempo is preferred. Stout and Leckenby (1988) Research on the impact of a musical singer or band is
reported faster tempo generally elicited positive comments, surprisingly lacking. Hecker (1984) suggested that using a
whereas commercials featuring moderate or slow tempi well-known musical group or singer can add spice, authority,
receive less favorable comments. Purchase intent has been stature, and/or charm to the commercial. The findings on use
impacted by upbeat music both positively (Kellaris and Kent, of celebrity endorsers are probably relevant here, especially
1991) and not at all (Brooker and Wheatley, 1994; Morris that celebrities, and hence musical artists, should “fit” the
and Boone, 1998). Based on their review, Brooker and brand (“celebrity-brand congruency,” similar to music-
Wheatley (1994) concluded that faster music should increase message fit), be attractive, and be credible (Atkin and
attention to the ad and have a positive impact on recall, Block, 1983; Erdogan et al., 2001; Till, 1998).
emotion, attitudes, and purchase intent, while slower music Another understudied factor is the expressive actions of the
should have the opposite effects. However, while uptempo performer (Thompson et al., 1989). Expressive variations in
music was perceived as more cheerful than slower music, timing (Ashley, 2002), loudness (Thompson and Cuddy,
tempo had no significant effects on any of the other 1997), and facial and gestural expressions of emotion
dependent measures. The authors concluded that the (Thompson et al., 2005, 2008) strongly influence listeners’
findings contradicted those of Kellaris and Kent (1991) emotional reactions and even their perception of musical
because the latter’s behavioral intentions concerned listening structure (perceived features), though we are not aware of any
to or buying the music, rather than intentions regarding the research exploring these aspects of performance in
advertised product. advertisements.
Together these findings suggest that negative cognition and
affect arising from any of the Aam components can result (3) Listener characteristics
from an unfavorable combination of a commercial’s musical Many studies demonstrate that consumer taste for art and
structural characteristics. Importantly, Bruner (1990) and entertainment varies predictably with general customer
Kellaris and Kent (1991) found that structural elements can characteristics, i.e. demographics and socioeconomic status,
produce main effects but also statistically significant psychographics, and personality. The literature on people’s
interactions in generating cognitive, affective, and reactions to music in general (Finnäs, 1989; Shepherd, 2003)
behavioral consumer responses. They argue that it is and to music in advertising (references are cited below)
crucial to consider these interactive effects, something that suggests that certain sociodemographic variables – age, sex,
single-variable studies obviously fail to do. culture, subculture, social class, and social group – plus
personality and mood state will influence a listener’s response
Genre and style to music in a commercial. Musical training, knowledge, and
North and Hargreaves (1997a) found that musical style is experience also influence musical responses (Hargreaves et al.,
more important than musical piece in determining listeners’ 2006; Holbrook, 1995; Konecni, 1982), but these listener
responses: Liking ratings for the same musical piece vary characteristics are beyond the scope of this paper.
greatly depending on the style in which it is performed. Thus,
consumer response to musical stimuli, such as whether they Musical taste
continue to listen to a commercial, will often be determined Marketers seek to know their target audience’s long-term
by style identification processes, which are very fast musical tastes, which intuitively have a direct and powerful
(Gjerdingen and Perrott, 2008; Plazak and Huron, 2010; effect on their reaction to ad music and probably influences
Schellenberg et al., 1999); after one second of exposure, style every element of Aam. Below, we describe a number of
identification is nearly perfect. This implies that marketers listener characteristics that appear to affect consumer
have a very short window of opportunity before consumers musical taste and thus, indirectly, Aam. We focus on
might decide to change the channel – first impressions as sociodemographic and psychological variables that have
conveyed by genre/style are, indeed, critical to gaining been found to impact musical tastes and, consequently,
attention to both the commercial and its music. music appeal and/or preference for a piece of music at a
Musical style preferences are firmly rooted in personality particular point in time.
traits (Rentfrow and Gosling, 2003), are used by individuals Age
to communicate information about their personalities and to Two age-related phenomena influence music preferences:
form impressions of others (Rentfrow and Gosling, 2006), 1 Life cycle effects are differences due to age-related (1)
and musical preferences are correlated with numerous lifestyle physical distinctions, such as older people having difficulty
variables (North and Hargreaves, 2007a, b, c). Consequently, hearing, and (b) psychological differences, like younger
different genres (e.g. pop, classical, and jazz) and styles within people craving edgy fun and excitement (and music
each genre (e.g. rap, baroque, and bebop, respectively) are reflecting that) (Hamermesh, 1984). Together, these two
potentially powerful tools for establishing an image for the life cycle effects would seem to especially impact
brand and for creating the right mood in commercials (e.g. a emotional arousal response to music.

28
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

Some work has found that hedonic responses may social class (Alpert and Alpert, 1991; Holbrook and
change over the life cycle. Young adults report Schindler, 1989; Pitt and Abratt, 1988; Scott, 1990).
significantly stronger overall preference ratings for music Abundant research suggests that responses to music are
than older adults (LeBlanc et al., 1996, cited in Rentfrow directly related to their cultural meaningfulness (North,
and Gosling, 2006). Consistent with this, Pincus (2004, Hargreaves, McKenzie and Law, 2004). For instance,
cited in North and Hargreaves, 2008) described research national anthems help people assert their national identity.
demonstrating that 64 per cent of 14-to-25-year-olds Hence, patriotic music could help create an image as an “all-
mentioned music as their greatest influence when American brand.” Consequently, the effects of culture seem
choosing among brands. Intuitively, older adults may be to work more on cognitive elements of Aam such as
less influenced by ad music, although we do not know of remembered features of music available for association and
any research documenting this. Because exposure to image suggested, although culturally-based emotional feelings
various styles of music accumulates over the lifespan, and memories can also be evoked by music.
listeners come to understand these styles better with age.
Subculture
The same music that is regarded as complex and
Each ethnic, religious, geographic, and age cohort subculture
distasteful in youth may be enjoyed more as one ages,
has particular music tastes that can be included in
although to our knowledge this notion remains untested
commercials, such as salsa, polka, gospel, country, and
(Hargreaves et al., 2006).
bluegrass. The prevalence of musical genre radio stations
2 Cohort effects are preferences for elements of popular
appealing to the Hispanic subculture – regional Mexican,
culture, such as music, movies, books, fashion, and sports,
tropical Latin, Latin urban, and Spanish contemporary – is
due to one’s birth year and consequently the pop culture
evidence of such ethnic subcultural influence (http://america
in which s/he grew up. Accordingly, in the 1960s, Benny
smusiccharts.com).
Goodman music was nostalgic for middle-aged adults,
Consumption subcultures based on music are pervasive.
whereas in the ’80s and ’90s, Beatles and Rolling Stones
Many print media articles have discussed the variety of
tunes became sentimental for this same age group, and in
clothing and other lifestyle choices (such as use of specific
the current decade artists such as Blondie, Olivia Newton-
illegal drugs and sexual permissiveness) affiliated with
John, and Prince are nostalgic for mid-lifers.
particular musical subcultures (North and Hargreaves,
Such preferences may arise during the late
2007a). Youth tend to form musical subcultures focused on
adolescence/early adulthood (late teens/early 20s)
pop music, such as the 1950s’ Teddy Boys, the 1960s’ flower
“critical period” of development (Hargreaves et al.,
children, 1970s’ punks, and the Goths, “metalheads,” and rap
2006; North and Hargreaves, 1995, 2002), being
fans of the last few decades (North and Hargreaves, 2008).
formed by the time of young adulthood and persisting
Thus, subculture seemingly taps into a wide variety of
into old age (Craton et al., 2008, Study 2; Holbrook and
cognitive and affective Aam dimensions, with hedonic
Schindler, 1989; for a review, see Hargreaves et al., 2006).
response being particularly influenced.
Hence, TV commercials aimed at Baby Boomers are
replete with oldies and classic rock, while ads targeted Social class
toward Generations X and Y contain alternative rock and While some studies have demonstrated a negligible
rap. Cohort effects seem to most influence affective relationship between socioeconomic status and musical
components of Aam such as emotional memories and preference, others have observed higher socioeconomic
hedonic response. groups favoring more serious music, such as classical, opera,
Sex and ballet (Di Maggio and Useem, 1978; Finnäs, 1989).
Studies on the relationship between sex and musical Research has been conducted on “taste cultures,” which
preference are conflicting (Wheeler, 1985). Nonetheless, exist to “entertain, inform, and beautify life and to express
research reviewed by North and Hargreaves (2008) confirms values and standards of taste and aesthetics” (Fox and Wince,
the everyday observation that women prefer “softer” musical 1975, p. 199). These studies categorize fans of particular
styles such as mainstream pop, while males favor “harder,” musical styles into broader social classes based on lifestyle
more aggressive styles like hard rock. Consistent with preferences. Such groups consist of people subscribing to a
women’s greater emotional expressiveness in non-musical particular taste culture, ranging along a social class
domains, females rated musical excerpts as more emotionally continuum: “high culture,” “upper-middle culture,” “lower-
expressive and more likeable than did males (Kamenetsky middle culture,” “low culture,” and “quasi-folk low culture”
et al., 1997, citing several studies). Finnäs (1989) reports that, (Gans, 1974, cited in Fox and Wince, 1975 and in North and
although in some studies no clear relation between sex and Hargreaves, 2007b). Fox and Wince (1975) observed that five
music preference was found, others have discovered females musical taste cultures – “jazz-blues,” “popular hits,” “folk
preferring serious, traditional, or more “peaceful” music, music,” “rock-protest,” and “country and western” – were
whereas males favor “tougher” music like hard rock as well as strongly correlated with social class. Many studies have
sometimes jazz. Kellaris and Rice (1993) also found that depicted patterned differences in consumer tastes variously
females respond more positively to music at lower volumes. In termed as “highbrow” versus “lowbrow,” “high culture”
short, sex seems to primarily impact the affective elements of versus either “mass culture” or “popular culture,” or the
Aam. “cultural hierarchy” (Holbrook, 1995, p. 57). At one end of
this spectrum, high culture entails elite activities, such as
Culture museum-caliber art, opera, and ballet. Popular culture is one
One perspective on perception of music holds that it is of mass appeal, such as movies, books, fashion, sports, and
primarily a cultural and social phenomenon reflecting the music, all mass produced and standardized. North and
values and attitudes of a particular culture, subculture, and Hargreaves (2007c) observed that liking for “high-art” music

29
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

(e.g. opera, classical music, and jazz) correlated with living music styles and those with an athletic self-view preferred
the lifestyle of the upper-middle and middle classes, while vigorous music. High sensation seeking appears to be
preference for “low-art” music (e.g. hip hop/rap, dance/ positively related to predilections for rock, heavy metal, and
house, indie, and DJ-based music) was indicative of a lower- punk music, and it is negatively associated with preferences
middle and lower-class lifestyle. Hence, it appears that social for sound tracks and religious music (Little and Zuckerman,
class primarily affects the affective aspects of Aam. 1986). Similarly, a penchant for highly arousing music (heavy
metal, rock, alternative, and rap) is positively correlated with
Social groups/social identity
resting arousal, high sensation seeking, and an antisocial
People gain an understanding of their “self” or social identity
personality (McNamera and Ballard, 1999, cited in Rentfrow
– who they are as individuals – in part from the social groups
and Gosling, 2003). North and Hargreaves’ (2008) extensive
to which they belong (Tarrant et al., 2002). North and
literature review reports that numerous studies demonstrate
Hargreaves (1999) maintain that people use music as a
that devotees of certain “problem music” styles, such as dance
“badge” to communicate their values, feelings, and self-views.
music, rap, and heavy rock, are more involved than other fans
Socially consumed products such as music are often
in lifestyles characterized by rebellious and anti-authoritarian
purchased to achieve a sense of belonging or to make a
behavior.
statement about who we are, not just as individuals, but also
Moreover, people use their music preferences to
as members of social groups (North and Hargreaves, 2007a;
communicate information regarding their personalities to
Tarrant et al., 2002). Individuals respond more positively to
others (Rentfrow and Gosling, 2006). Intellectual individuals
objects that are congruent with their self-image (Landon,
favor complex, esoteric music, thinking it projects a
1974), and this includes music (North and Hargreaves,
sophisticated image (Rentfrow and Gosling, 2006).
2007a; Rentfrow and Gosling, 2003).
Recent work on the effect of social influence on musical Research also suggests that preference for liberal versus
preference and taste has been guided by social identity theory conservative musical styles (e.g. rap, dance music, and rock
(SIT; Tajfel and Turner, 1979). This approach emphasizes that versus classical music, musicals, and opera) is associated with
individuals strive to evaluate their own social group, and thus respectively liberal versus conservative personalities and
their social identity, positively. For instance, Tarrant et al. lifestyles (North and Hargreaves, 2007a).
(2002) suggested that a major appeal of music for adolescents All of this suggests that music can have a strong impact on
is its differentiation (“badge”) function that helps them form the image dimension of Aam for so-called “badge” products.
positive social identities (see also North and Hargreaves, 1999; Prior mood state
North et al., 2000). Indeed, music is more important to young Mood is a person’s subjectively perceived affective state that is
people’s social identity than other behaviors such as media- usually not intense; is not tied to a specifiable behavior, event,
related activities or sports (Hargreaves et al., 2006). Musical or object (Bharucha et al., 2006; Gardner, 1985a), and can be
preferences in adolescence are guided by group norms and either positive or negative (Alpert and Alpert, 1991; Gardner,
tend to conform to the perceived preferences of peer group 1985a). Prior mood states perhaps have their greatest effect in
leaders (Finnäs, 1989). According to Hargreaves et al. (2006), low involvement circumstances where brand differences are
expressing liking for a style of music can facilitate group minimal (Clark and Isen, 1982).
distinctiveness but also increases intergroup discrimination. Generally, moods seem to bias evaluations and judgments
Violation of the group norm for musical preference, on the in mood-congruent directions (Gardner, 1985a; Srull, 1983)
other hand, threatens the group’s distinctiveness and often by evoking mood-congruent thoughts (Clark and Isen, 1982;
leads to derogation of the individual expressing the deviant Goldberg and Gorn, 1987; Isen et al., 1978), transferring to
preference. This sensitivity to group norm violation appears to product-related attitudes and behaviors (Goldberg and Gorn,
develop in late childhood and may have a strong influence on 1987). However, there are exceptions; products that alleviate
the development of musical preferences. a negative mood state might be evaluated favorably by
The above work from social psychology leads to the consumers when in such mood states (Gardner, 1985a). So, a
prediction that social group and social identity are important gloomy consumer watching a commercial for an anti-
determinants of the music perceived as distinctive or not and depressant might react negatively to upbeat music in the ad.
image elements of Aam. Also noteworthy is that purchase intention can be affected
Personality/personal identity/lifestyle by interaction of music and mood (Alpert and Alpert, 1990;
As opposed to social identity, which is based on social Alpert et al., 2003; Brooker and Wheatley, 1994; Kellaris and
categorizations, personal identity focuses on personal, Kent, 1991; Morris and Boone, 1998; North, Hargreaves,
idiosyncratic attributes such as personality or intelligence McKenzie and Law, 2004). Mood also directly influences Aad
(North and Hargreaves, 2008). Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) (MacKenzie and Lutz, 1989). Clearly, the mood induced by
note that as early as the 1950s, personality researcher Cattell music Aam component is influenced here.
suggested that musical preferences reveal important
information about unconscious aspects of personality. (4) Advertisement processing strategy
Today, a large number of personality researchers view music We turn now to the consumer’s advertisement processing
preferences as reflecting more “explicit” personality traits strategy, which we define as how the consumer acquires
(North and Hargreaves, 2008; Rentfrow and Gosling, 2003, information or meaning from an advertisement. Ad processing
2006; Wheeler, 1985). strategy is known to affect Aad (Brown and Stayman, 1992;
For instance, easygoing people prefer soothing musical Gardner, 1985b; Homer, 1990; Mitchell, 1986; Park and
styles since such music helps them stay calm (Sloboda and Young, 1986). Here, we suggest that it also feeds into the
O’Neill, 2001). Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found that various elements of Aam. This section on ad processing will
people with a conservative self-view preferred conventional explain different theories of how music contributes cognitively

30
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

and affectively to formation of Aam’s cognitive and affective satisfies: cognitive involvement and affective involvement.
components, and hence development of Aad. Cognitive involvement relates to thoughtful information
Prior research cited below suggests that key elements of acquisition. Here, persuasion begins with a change in salient
advertisement processing strategy are level of advertising beliefs about brand performance on brand-differentiating
processing involvement (high versus low) and the attributes, resulting in attitude change (Fishbein, 1963;
corresponding processing route (central versus peripheral), Mitchell and Olson, 1981). Such belief-based processing can
nature of product involvement (cognitive versus affective), result in attitude change under both high and low cognitive
and type of advertising processing involvement (ad-message involvement circumstances (Fishbein, 1963; Middlestadt
involvement versus ad-execution involvement, including et al., 1994). However, high (low) cognitively involved
involvement with the music). Table II summarizes these consumers decide based on a lot of (little to no) brand-
factors, which we now briefly discuss before we look at each of attribute information. All cognitive Aam elements can
the relevant persuasion theories through which advertising potentially be impacted by the consumer’s level of cognitive
music can affect Aam in light of various combinations of these involvement.
factors. Affective involvement is founded on emotional, symbolic,
Level of advertising processing involvement aesthetic, or social needs, relating to pleasure, enjoyment, and
Involvement is commonly conceptualized as the degree to experiences (Holbrook and Hirschman, 1982). Here,
which a purchase decision has perceived personal importance, persuasion is grounded in nonbelief-based processing and
relevance, and consequences for the buyer (Greenwald and entails molding attitudes emotionally or experientially, based
Leavitt, 1984; Park and Young, 1986; Petty et al., 1983). A on hedonic needs. All affective Aam elements are potentially
consumer highly involved with purchasing and/or using the influenced by the consumer’s level of affective involvement.
advertised product is motivated to extensively search for and Whether the consumer’s involvement is cognitive or
deeply process marketplace information (Krugman, 1965), affective in nature will determine which type of advertising
thereby exhibiting a high level of advertising processing processing involvement (ad-message or ad-execution) and
involvement – the consumer’s degree of interest in paying which persuasion theory (discussed below) will be operative.
attention to advertising (Batra and Ray, 1983; Laczniak et al., This also determines which components of Aam are affected –
1989). A strong argument against using music in advertising i.e. cognitive (affective) involvement will more likely affect
is that it can serve as a distraction – an attention-grabber that cognitive (affective) components.
is irrelevant to the message (Brooker and Wheatley, 1994; Type of advertising processing involvement
Macklin, 1988). Based on the available research, we suggest We distinguish two types of advertising processing
that high-involvement consumers are likely to perceive music- involvement: Advertising-message involvement entails the
message fit and to respond positively (negatively) to good degree of cognitive effort expended by the consumer toward
(poor) fit, creating a favorable (unfavorable) Aam. Negative processing an ad’s message contents, being high (low) under
response may occur even for music which in other contexts a high (low)-cognitive involvement conditions (Park and Young,
consumer would enjoy but which exhibits a poor music- 1986). Under high advertising-message involvement, music
message fit. For instance, Park and Young (1986) found that would primarily impact the cognitive Aam dimensions.
for high-involvement consumers, preselected popular and Advertising-execution involvement concerns the extent of
liked music was a distraction from the message, lowering cognitive effort exerted by the buyer toward processing an
response variable scores because it was unrelated to brand-
ad’s executional elements, such as music (MacKenzie and
attribute message contents. Low-involvement consumers are
Lutz, 1989), and it is elevated (depressed) under high (low)-
unlikely to wish to attend to any aspects of an ad, including
affective involvement circumstances. It would seem that both
the music, although it may be difficult to ignore. Whether
cognitive and affective Aam components could be affected
they distinguish high and low music-message fit is currently
here since cognitive effort is expended and yet affect is
unclear (MacInnis and Park, 1991; North, Hargreaves,
experienced.
McKenzie and Law, 2004).
We suggest that music involvement is a type of advertising
Nature of product involvement processing involvement, that it can occur as either high or low
Product involvement – the extent of a consumer’s enduring, levels of either cognitive involvement or affective involvement,
inherent interest in a particular product or brand – is of two irrespective of the product’s inherent involvement level
types, depending on the nature of the need the product (MacInnis and Park, 1991), and thus is an aspect of

Table II Components of advertising processing strategy


Level of advertising processing Type of Advertising processing
involvement (including music involvement and music
involvement)/processing route Nature of product involvement involvement Persuasion theory
High/central 1. Cognitive Ad-message Cognitive response model: Lyrics and musical fit
2. Affective Ad-execution Emotional appeals
Low/peripheral 3. Cognitive Ad-message or ad-execution Cognitive response model: Ad-execution
thoughts and simple inferences
4. Affective Ad-execution Mere exposure Classical conditioning: Mood
inducement and cue association

31
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

advertising – message involvement or of advertising-execution 1985). If thoughts regarding the brand due to the commercial
involvement. Indeed, Sullivan (1990) found that high (including its music) are favorable (unfavorable), the
musical-involvement consumers showed better recall for consumer’s Ab might also become favorable (unfavorable)
radio commercials (we would suggest due to high attention (Shimp, 1981).
to and depth of processing of music), Aad, and Ab for low Music can alter brand beliefs about brand-attribute
involvement products than did low music-involvement performance through lyrics, usually in a jingle. Catchy
consumers. lyrics can grab attention and if simple enough can encourage
Processing route an optimal depth of processing of music that enhances recall of
Petty and Cacioppo’s (1986) and Petty et al.’s (1983) the ad message. They also create new or tap into existing
elaboration likelihood model (ELM) of ad processing and associations and aid brand distinctiveness. Effective lyrics help
persuasion considers the degree of effort consumers exert in consumers perceive music-message fit by creating an image,
learning about a brand’s attributes and forming an Ab, i.e. how feeling, or mood congruent with the message (Alpert and
deeply they process (elaborate on) information on the brand Alpert, 1991).
(Alpert and Alpert, 1989). This model proposes two distinctly MacInnis and Park (1991) argued that music may positively
different “routes” to persuasion, depending on consumers’ influence high-involvement consumers if there is high musical
motivation (i.e. level of advertising processing involvement), fit since it primes relevant product beliefs (e.g. sophisticated
opportunity, and ability (MOA) to process information and classical music featured in a perfume advertisement). Stout
make decisions. Under central-route processing, MOA is and Leckenby (1990, cited in North and Hargreaves, 1997b
high, and hence active effort is put into information and in North, Hargreaves, McKenzie and Law, 2004)
processing. This can result in either high cognitive, ad- discovered that commercials were perceived more favorably
message involvement, i.e. high engagement with advertising, and as more informative when containing music easily
focusing on product-relevant information, or high affective, associated with the advertised brand, i.e. when there was
ad-execution involvement, as evoked by executional elements musical fit.
such as a picture of a starving child or very solemn music
(Sherman et al., 1999). Music’s influence for high affective (ad-execution) involvement,
central route consumers
Under peripheral-route processing, MOA is low –
In the cases of affective involvement (second and fourth rows
consumers devote little energy to ad processing (Petty et al.,
1983) and to making choices, passively encountering in Table II), the underlying assumption is that pleasant, well-
unsought information (Batra and Ray, 1983), such as liked ads yield a more favorable Aad and Ab than unpleasant
commercial jingles or music that evokes a mood. This can or irritating ads (Mitchell, 1986; Moore and Hutchinson,
result in either low cognitive, ad-message or ad-execution 1985; Shimp, 1981). Under the central route, sometimes
involvement, or low affective, ad-execution involvement audience members experience high affective involvement –
(Bordens and Horowitz, 2001). strong emotional responses to a commercial, leading to
Under the peripheral route consumers use peripheral cues attitudes founded on feelings as an information source (Pham,
– less relevant, easily processed simple positive and negative 1998). Here, emotional appeals through music can yield
executional advertising stimuli (North and Hargreaves, emotional responses changing the consumer’s Aad, leading to
1997b; Petty et al., 1983). Background (nonvocal) music in an alteration of Ab (Holbrook and Batra, 1987).
commercials is an important peripheral cue because it can Music is a powerful way to garner emotional responses by
significantly impact both physiological and psychological creating a general feeling of global affect or liking for a
reactions (Grayston, 1974; Jacob, 1968; Ross, 1966; particular musical piece or emotional suggestiveness of that
Uhrbrock, 1961; Zimny and Weidenfeller, 1963). Indeed, music (Alpert and Alpert, 1990; Galizio and Hendrick, 1972;
some researchers have discovered that background music Stout and Leckenby, 1988; Zimny and Weidenfeller, 1963).
(MacInnis and Park, 1991) as well as foreground music (Park MacInnis and Park (1991) found that music aroused
and Young, 1986) can positively affect message processing emotion-laden memories consistent with the ad’s primary
under low involvement conditions. message and thereby generated positive feelings and Aad In
those studies where music evoked little or no emotional
Persuasion through music based on four consumer advertising response (MacInnis and Park, 1991; Stout and Rust, 1986), it
processing situations
was likely due to a poor fit between the commercial and its
We now briefly examine how elements of Aam are influenced
background music (Alpert and Alpert, 1990; MacInnis and
by each of four advertising processing scenarios, based on
Park, 1991; Morris and Boone, 1998), suggesting that central
whether high/central route or low/peripheral route processing
route processing is operative and highlighting once again the
occurs (column 1 in Table II) and product involvement is
centrality of music-message fit. We suggest that all of the
cognitive or affective (column 2 in Table II). Italicized terms in
affective Aam components can potentially be affected by music
the next four sections are cognitive and affective dimensions
for high affective involvement consumers.
of Aam found in Figure 1. Bold italicized terms are music
persuasion processes as outlined in column 4 of Table II. Music’s influence for low cognitive (ad-message or ad-execution)
Music’s influence for high cognitive (ad-message) involvement, involvement, peripheral route consumers
central route consumers Peripheral route cognitive responses to ad music occur under
High cognitive involvement consumers (first row under low cognitive, low ad-message involvement (third row in
column headings in Table II) do not focus on executional Table II). These include ad execution-related thoughts,
cues, but ad music might nevertheless influence Ab through which can arise among low MOA consumers attending to ad
brand cognitions, enhancing or distracting from message presentation elements such as music (Belch and Belch, 2009),
processing (Petty and Cacioppo, 1986; Swasy and Munch, thereby enhancing Aad and/or Ab. A jingle, although an

32
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

executional element, can contribute only a small amount of music can effectively operate (Edell and Burke, 1984, Lutz,
message information even for low involvement consumers. 1985; Shimp, 1981; Petty et al., 1983). However, research
Another type of low cognitive involvement ad execution- findings on classical conditioning via advertising are
related thought is simple inferences – beliefs based on equivocal. While some researchers report that consumers
simple associations of the brand with peripheral cues, can be classically conditioned to prefer a product by pairing it
including music (Hoyer and McGinnis, 2007). For example, with well-liked music (e.g. Gorn, 1982), others have failed to
jazz music might suggest a “cool,” relaxed brand image. obtain this effect; (Allen and Madden, 1985; Alpert and
Again, music-message fit might be relevant, although probably Alpert, 1989; Alpert and Alpert, 1990; Gresham and Shimp,
not to the degree it is under central route processing. We 1985; Kellaris and Cox, 1989; Kleine et al., 1986; Macklin,
believe that all other cognitive Aam components might also be 1986; Pitt and Abratt, 1988). Some research finds that
potentially affected by the music for low cognitive classical conditioning is successful for novel brands but not
nvolvement, peripheral route consumers, albeit with weaker for familiar brands (Shimp et al., 1991) and for unfamiliar
effects than with high cognitive involvement consumers. conditioned stimuli having few preexisting associations
(Cacioppo et al., 1992).
Music’s influence for low affective (ad-execution) involvement,
It appears that at best classical conditioning occurs
peripheral route consumers
unreliably (Kellaris and Cox, 1989) and only for low
One way music can elicit consumer response for low affective
involvement/passive consumers. Tom’s (1995) research also
involvement consumers is through mere exposure –
suggests that classical conditioning is more likely to arise
repeated exposure to a stimulus that creates liking for it
(Zajonc, 1968), as demonstrated by substantial evidence where there is musical fit, once again highlighting the
(Mitchell and Olson, 1981). Consequently, frequently criticality of music-message congruence.
encountered music might become liked, as consequently do
the ad and brand. This would primarily seem to impact the Attitude toward the ad (Aad)
The previous discussion suggests that Aam can be a significant
emotions affective Aam element. However, overplayed jingles
can become annoying due to wearout, and mere exposure component of Aad – a “predisposition to respond in a
would probably not work for disliked music, even having the favorable or unfavorable manner to a particular advertising
opposite effect, although this merits research. stimulus during a particular exposure occasion” (Lutz, 1985),
Low affectively involved consumers might also be or “an individual’s evaluation of and/or affective feelings
influenced by music via classical conditioning, a passive, about an advertisement” (Park and Young, 1986). This is
low involvement process of learning stimulus-response regardless of level of advertising involvement (high/central
associations. Under this first and most widely discussed versus low/peripheral) and nature of product involvement
mechanism explaining the influence of Aad on Ab (Allen and (cognitive vs. affective) and advertising processing
Janiszewski, 1989; Edell and Burke, 1984), Ab is formed via involvement (ad-message vs. ad-execution).
frequently associating a positively or negatively unconditioned Aad’s influence on Ab is well documented (Burke and Edell,
(natural) stimulus (UCS) such as music with a conditioned 1989; Brown and Stayman, 1992; Gardner, 1985b; Gorn,
(initially neutral) stimulus (CS) such as a brand. Through 1982; Homer, 1990; MacKenzie et al., 1986; Mitchell, 1986;
pairings of the UCS and CS, learners attribute to the CS Muehling and Laczniak, 1988; Stayman and Aaker, 1988). Ab
(brand) whatever thoughts or feelings they have about the is affected by both brand beliefs formed via Aad (including
affiliated UCS (e.g. liked or disliked commercial music) cognitive elements of Aam) as well as by a direct link from Aad
(Gardner, 1985a; Mitchell and Olson, 1981). to Ab (Mitchell and Olson, 1981; Park and Young, 1986)
One way classical conditioning works is through mood (allowing both cognitive and affective Aam dimensions to be
inducement (Alpert and Alpert, 1991; Galizio and influential).
Hendrick, 1972). Here, music creates an appropriate mood Ab, in turn, provokes brand purchase intentions, all else
for the brand, such as upbeat, happy music for an amusement equal (e.g. brand availability), leading to a brand choice, as
park, biasing Ab in a mood-congruent direction (Gardner, shown in the right-hand portion of Figure 1. Research has
1985a; Hoyer and McGinnis, 2007). This later influences demonstrated Aad’s influence on purchase intention, both
brand choice, such as by the presence of point-of-purchase indirectly via Aad’s impact on Ab (Homer, 1990; MacKenzie
cues that reinitiate the original mood (Shimp, 1981). Moods and Lutz, 1989; MacKenzie et al., 1986; Mitchell, 1986;
have been found to affect Aad, Ab, purchase intentions (Edell Mitchell and Olson, 1981; Shimp, 1981) and directly
and Burke, 1984; Holbrook and Batra, 1987; Stayman and (Gresham and Shimp, 1985; MacKenzie et al., 1986). Aad
Aaker, 1988), and behavior (Clark and Isen, cited in Gardner, also affects brand choice directly as well as indirectly through
1985a). Ab (Biehal et al., 1992).
Classical conditioning can also operate via cue Lutz (1985) posited that Aad has antecedents that are
association, which employs unique marketing stimuli such cognitive, central processing (e.g. ad credibility, ad
as music to identify and differentiate brands. For instance, perceptions) and affective, peripheral processing (e.g. mood,
very different associations will be developed by upbeat and attitude toward advertising in general). The cognitive
zippy versus somber and melancholy songs. This suggests that elements consist of people’s judgments regarding the ad’s
classically conditioned cognitions (as well as feelings) can also characteristics (e.g. humorous, informative, contains classy
be created for low affective involvement consumers. music, i.e. ad-execution thoughts and simple inferences),
Classical conditioning has been supported by many studies while the affective components are founded on feelings people
(Bierley et al., 1985; Gibson, 2008; Shimp et al. 1991; Stuart experience during ad exposure (e.g. amused, moved,
et al., 1987; Tom, 1995). It provides a plausible mechanism i.e. emotional reactions to emotional appeals) (Burke and
explaining the peripheral route to persuasion through which Edell, 1989). Thus, while someone might, for example,

33
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

believe that an ad is characterized by humor (cognitive), the (whereas NR lyrics might distract, although “Good Night,
person might not find the ad funny (affective). Sleep Tight” could be a good choice) and would have more
Similarly, Park and Young’s (1986) research suggests that impact as foreground music whose lyrics are attended to.
Aad can contribute to Ab formation in two ways: The tune’s remembered features for association would
1 through the cognitive route by facilitating communication likely be positive (whereas some NRs like “Little Miss
of the ad message, such as via simple information and Muffet” would tap negative associations). Structural
peripheral cues that relate to the ad’s theme (rows 1 and 3 characteristics of BL would likely create positive thoughts
in Table II), or and feelings, whereas some NRs might be found annoying.
2 through the affective route via its effect on positive feeling Pop singer-songwriter Jewel, being a mother with an album
states without a relevant connection to the ad’s theme Lullaby!, could be the musical artist for NR (Braham’s
(rows 2 and 4). Lullaby appears on her album). A relaxed mood could be
induced here, reinforcing the comforting message
According to the dual-mediation hypothesis, consumers can .
Listener characteristics. Demographics would not be a
have a positive Aad either because:
major consideration as either selection is universally
. they have a favorable cognitive response, i.e. the ad
familiar and liked), although they would be relevant if a
provides them with useful information (rows 1 and 3 in
particular performer (Jewel) or an idiosyncratic version of
Table II); or
the song was used. Either NR or BL would appeal to
.
they have a positive affective response, i.e. they gain good
parents who are “into” the parenting lifestyle and view
feelings from or are interested in the ad (Brown and
their parenting role as a primary element of their social
Stayman, 1992; Homer, 1990; MacKenzie et al., 1986)
identity and lifestyle – the music’s image would be on
(rows 2 and 4).
target.
In either case, Aad is enhanced, leading either to make .
Advertisement processing strategy. The products lend
consumers more accepting of brand beliefs or claims, themselves to high cognitive involvement with central
resulting in a more favorable Ab (rows 1 and 3), or to route processing, so no matter the music selection, lyrics
create affect transfer – positive feelings that directly transfer would be important. Since these are products for the
from the ad (Aad) to the brand (Ab) (Gresham and Shimp, parents’ baby, affective involvement would be high too.
1985; MacKenzie et al., 1986; Mitchell and Olson, 1981) BL would be appropriate since its soft tune brings a
(rows 2 and 4). familiar feeling of being protected and warm (emotions
evoked by music).
Implications and directions for managers and for
Research implications
future research Although our primary concern in this paper has been to assist
Managerial implications managers in the selection of ad music, we hope that the
Managers can use Figure 1 as a checklist of considerations to checklist provided by our model will be useful to researchers
evaluate how target audience members might respond to music as well. We conclude by pointing out several ways the model
in their TV and/or radio commercial. They can review Table II can help guide future research and noting limitations in our
to determine the likely nature of advertisement processing their model that might be addressed in future work.
target audience uses and to determine which persuasion First, the model draws attention to variables that have
theories are likely to be operative so that they can tailor their generated tantalizing or contradictory findings that beg for
use of music accordingly (e.g. use lyrics to teach consumers follow-up. For instance, we would like to see further empirical
about brand benefits in the high ad involvement, central route work comparing the effects of foreground versus background
scenario). Through all this we suggest they think long and hard music and the use versus nonuse of lyrics in foreground
about possible ways proposed music can negatively impact a music.
significant number of target market members. Second, by integrating established variables in a broad
For instance, suppose an advertiser of baby furniture (cribs, framework, the model underscores the need for research on
changing tables, rockers, strollers, etc.) is trying to decide the links between them. For instance, little is known about
between using Brahms’ Lullaby (BL) (“Lullaby, and good what factors – musical style/genre, structural characteristics,
night . . . ”) or a children’s nursery rhyme (NR) (Baa, Baa, etc. – lead to high perceived music-message fit both in general
Black Sheep, Hickory Dickory Dock, etc.) to be aired during as well as for various product categories. Because the model
particular TV and radio programming. The message is that offers a comprehensive list of variables, it can also help
the product line is safe, comfortable, comforting, and investigators identify factors that need to be strictly controlled
protective. The following could be considered as key factors: in experiments, which may reduce the mix of findings in
.
Listening situation. Many parents (mostly mothers) might certain portions of the literature. For example, we believe it is
be harried and hassled with attending to the baby’s needs crucial that future research on music-message fit judiciously
and somewhat cranky while at home with their spouse avoid confounding fit with other factors such as structural
during late evening programming following a hard day’s characteristics of musical stimuli (to take a hypothetical
work either in the office or home, suggesting the more example, if fast music that experimenters select as having
relaxing BR. Cognitively this could communicate an “good fit” with a product enhances response to the ad, was it
image of comfortable and comforting, and affectively it fit or tempo that caused the effect?).
could induce emotional memories of the safe, protective Third, the model may be useful because it includes
home environment in which most of them grew up. variables that have not yet been thoroughly investigated. In
.
Musical stimulus. If a rocker is featured as a product in the particular, these include elements of Aam, namely level and
commercial, BL would achieve excellent music-message fit persistence of attention to music, depth of processing of

34
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

music, perceived features of music available for association, .


which processes are automatically triggered by a musical
remembered features of music available for association, image event and which involve conscious or controlled
suggested by music, music perceived as distinctive or not, processing;
mood induced by music, and emotional memories activated .
which reactions are universal or similar among all
by music. Consequently, we hope that researchers will use individuals and which are subjective, relative, and inter-
Figure 1 as a guide in finding new research topics. We believe individually variable; and
that work on consumer response to ad music will advance as .
which processes are innate and which are learned through
researchers systematically test the influence of particular experience and thus possibly dependent on the
elements subsumed under our four variables (listening individual’s “listening biography” or particular history of
situation, musical stimulus, listener characteristics, and exposure to music (Huron, 2006).
advertising processing strategy) on these unexplored aspects Future researchers should be aware of several limitations of the
of musical response. While we have pointed to some of the current model. For instance, we have not endeavored to offer
opportunities for new research throughout this paper, the measures or strict operational definitions for the various
model is rich enough to generate many more than we have variables and their elements. This is an important area for
space to discuss. future work. One challenge that arises here is whether to
Fourth, we hope that the model will provide an impetus for construe certain variables as objective or subjective. We strongly
future theoretical and empirical work that elucidates how the caution against the common practice of using experimenter
many elements of the listening situation, musical stimulus, intuition in selecting “pleasurable” versus “unpleasurable”
listener characteristics, and advertising processing strategy musical stimuli, given the strong evidence for variation and
variables interact with each other. Similarly, the components subjectivity in musical preferences and taste. Even pretested
of Aam probably influence each other. Does low perceived stimuli are unlikely to generate the same response in all
music-message fit reduce attention to ad music? Does it individuals, so it is important at the very least to employ
produce negative feelings or negative hedonic response rigorous manipulation checks in one’s experimental procedures.
toward the music? Many similar questions can be addressed Similar concerns apply to constructs such as complexity and
in future work. even music-message fit. It would be erroneous to assume that
Fifth, we believe that a strength of the present model is the variables such as these are either objectively present in the
multifaceted nature of Aam, which leads to a richer conception stimulus or uniform across individuals. Consequently, achieving
of “music appeal” than is typical in the literature. This universal response to any musical selection in a commercial is
richness provides opportunities for researchers to rigorously most challenging (Craton and Lantos, 2011)!
test the intuition that reactions to music are complex and Several additional limitations concern the scope and
sometimes contradictory – as when a song performed in a generality of the model. Regarding scope, although we
disliked genre is nevertheless regarded as infectiously upbeat consider Aam to be a significant and highly influential
or catchy, or when listeners enjoy listening to music that component of Aad, we believe that empirical research is
expresses a negatively valenced emotion such as sadness necessary before it will be possible to model the relationship
(Schellenberg et al., 2008). In fact, recent work suggests that between these variables. Variables analogous to Aam that also
even some individual elements of Aam can be contradictory. are constituents of Aad, such as attitude toward the celebrity
For instance, Hunter et al. (2008) found that music made and attitude toward the storyline, could also be proposed and
listeners feel simultaneously happy and sad when musical their interactions with music studied. Also, we did not include
the ad message and non-musical executional elements of
structural characteristics for both emotions (slow tempo
commercials as components in the model. This will be
depicting sadness, major mode signaling happiness) were
important to do, since these vary in their relationship to the
present. Results like these support anecdotal accounts of the
ad music (Scott, 1990) in ways that probably influence
richness of musical experience, and our model provides a tool
audience response. Finally, we did not consider ad processing
for exploring this systematically.
scenarios that combine different levels of cognitive and
Sixth, while the current model should be viewed as an
affective involvement (e.g, high cognitive but also high
outline of the structural relations of the various factors, we
affective involvement, suggesting emotional but meaningful
hope that it will provide a steppingstone for researchers who
lyrics).
aspire to develop a detailed model of the underlying Regarding generality, the present model has focused on
psychological mechanisms that can help explain the television and radio commercials using music. It may be
relationships between the variables in our framework. In possible to draw on this work and that of others such as those
doing so, it will be fruitful to pay attention to emerging work noted below to shed light on other uses of music in marketing,
in psychology. For instance, the richness and sometimes such as:
contradictory nature of musical response alluded to above .
Employing music microchips in print ads.
may sometimes occur when different affective mechanisms .
Using in-store background music on the P.A. system
operate simultaneously and generate contrasting emotions (e.g. in-store irritation caused by loud or disliked music)
(Juslin and Västfjäll, 2008). Process-oriented models have to create store “atmospherics” and service establishment
been proposed to account for response to visual art (e.g. Leder “servicescapes” that enhance buyers’ moods and
et al., 2004), and comparable work on modeling aesthetic contribute to a store image. In fact, Allan (2008)
response to music is progressing rapidly (Bharucha et al., suggested that in-store music might be the most
2006; Brattico and Jacobsen, 2009). Among other things, a important retail atmospheric element. However, disliked
process-oriented model of consumer response to advertising in-store music can adversely change the shopping
music should explicitly capture: experience for patrons and the working experience for

35
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

employees, whose music preferences are also important, Alpert, J.I. and Alpert, M.I. (1991), “Contributions from a
given employees’ impact on customer service. musical perspective on advertising and consumer
.
Sponsoring music concerts. For instance, funding a rock behavior”, Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 18,
concert conveys that the brand is cool, while supporting a pp. 232-7.
symphony orchestra connotes traditionalism and Alpert, M.I., Alpert, J.I. and Maltz, E.N. (2003), “Purchase
sophistication. occasion influence on the role of music in advertising”,
.
Using music in online advertising such as pre-roll Journal of Business Research, Vol. 58 No. 3, pp. 369-76.
advertising, webisodes (video webcasting and video ads. Anand, P. and Holbrook, M.B. (1985), “Chasing the wundt
.
Using music in product placements, a marketing curve: An adventure in consumer esthetics”, Advances in
communications strategy reported in many studies cited Consumer Research, Vol. 12, pp. 655-7.
in Gibson, 2008 (p. 186), based on associative learning Ashley, R. (2002), “Do[n’t] change a hair for me: the art of
mechanisms since the brand is affiliated with celebrities in Jazz rubato”, Music Perception, Vol. 19, pp. 311-32.
well-liked entertainment media such as TV shows, movies, Atkin, C. and Block, M. (1983), “Effectiveness of celebrity
and video games. endorsers”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 23 No. 1, pp. 57-61.
.
Using sonic branding – the attempt to use very short Batra, R. and Ray, L.L. (1983), “Advertising situations:
periods of music and other auditory cues to convey core the implications of differential involvement and
brand values and prime brand recognition whenever accompanying affect responses”, in Harris, R.J. (Ed.),
consumers come into contact with a company (e.g. in Information Processing Research in Advertising, Erlbaum,
advertising, on their web site, in their premises, while Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 127-51.
holding on the phone). (North and Hargreaves, 2008). In Belch, G. and Belch, M. (2009), Advertising and Promotion:
effect, sonic branding is a form of music fit wherein the An Integrated Marketing Communication Perspective,
advertising tries to raise the salience of particular brand McGraw-Hill/Irwin, New York, NY.
attributes in listeners’ minds (North and Hargreaves, Bharucha, J.J., Curtis, M. and Paroo, K. (2006), “Varieties of
2008). musical experience”, Cognition, Vol. 100, pp. 131-72.
Bierley, C., McSweeney, F.K. and Vannieuwkerk, R. (1985),
Conclusion “Classical conditioning of preferences for stimuli”, Journal
We believe our model of consumer response to advertising of Consumer Research, Vol. 12, December, pp. 316-23.
music, despite the limitations just noted, is sufficiently robust Biehal, G., Stephens, D. and Curlo, E. (1992), “Attitude
to provide much food for practitioner thought and practice. It toward the ad and brand choice”, Journal of Advertising,
also suggests for advertising researchers in both academia and Vol. 21, pp. 19-36.
practice many opportunities to further empirically investigate Bordens, K.S. and Horowitz, I.A. (2008), Social Psychology,
consumer response to advertising music. 3rd ed, Freeload Press, St Paul, MN.
Brattico, E. and Jacobsen, T. (2009), “Subjective appraisal of
music”, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
References Vol. 1169, pp. 308-17.
Allan, D. (2006), “Effects of popular music on attention and Brooker, G. and Wheatley, J.J. (1994), “Music and radio
memory in advertising”, Journal of Advertising Research, advertising: effects of tempo and placement”, Advances in
Vol. 46 No. 4, pp. 1-11. Consumer Research, Vol. 21, pp. 286-90.
Allan, D. (2007), “Sound advertising: a review of the Brown, S.P. and Stayman, D.M. (1992), “Antecedents and
experimental evidence on the effects of music in consequences of attitude toward the ad: a meta-analysis”,
commercials on attention, memory, attitudes, and Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 19, June, pp. 34-51.
purchase intention”, Journal of Media Psychology, Vol. 12 Bruner, G.C. (1990), “Music, mood, and marketing”, Journal
No. 3, available at: www.caltatela.edu/faculty/sfischo of Marketing, Vol. 54, pp. 94-104.
Allan, D. (2008), “Sound retailing: a review of experimental Burke, M.C. and Edell, J.A. (1989), “The impact of feelings
evidence on the effects of music on shopping behavior”, in on ad-based affect and cognition”, Journal of Marketing
Lowrey, T.M. (Ed.), Brick & Mortar Shopping in the 21st Research, Vol. 26, February, pp. 69-83.
Century, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ. Cacioppo, J.T., Marshall-Goodell, B.S., Tassinary, L.G. and
Allen, C.T. and Janiszewski, C.A. (1989), “Assessing the role Petty, R.E. (1992), “Rudimentary determinants of
of contingency awareness in attitudinal conditioning with attitudes: classical conditioning is more effective when
implications for advertising research”, Journal of Marketing prior knowledge about the attitude stimulus is low
Research, Vol. 26, February, pp. 30-43. formation through classical conditioning”, Psychological
Allen, C.T. and Madden, T.J. (1985), “A closer look at Science, Vol. 12, September, pp. 413-7.
classical conditioning”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 12 Chebat, J., Chebat, C.G. and Vaillant, D. (2001),
No. 3, pp. 301-15. “Environmental background music and in-store selling”,
Alpert, J.I. and Alpert, M.I. (1989), “Background music as an Journal of Business Research, Vol. 54, pp. 115-23.
influence in consumer mood and advertising responses”, Clark, M. and Isen, A. (1982), “Toward understanding the
in Srull, T.K. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 16, relationship between feeling states and social behavior”,
Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, in Hastorf, A. and Isen, A. (Eds), Cognitive Social Psychology,
pp. 485-91. Elsevier/North-Holland, New York, NY, pp. 73-108.
Alpert, J.I. and Alpert, M.I. (1990), “Music influences on Craik, F.I.M. and Lockhart, R.S. (1972), “Levels of
mood and purchase intentions”, Psychology & Marketing, processing: a framework for memory research”, Journal of
Vol. 7 No. 2, pp. 109-33. Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, Vol. 11, pp. 671-84.

36
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

Craik, F.I.M. and Tulving, E. (1975), “Depth of processing Gresham, L.G. and Shimp, T.A. (1985), “Attitude toward the
and the retention of words in episodic memory”, Journal of advertisement and brand attitudes: a classical conditioning
Experimental Psychology: General, Vol. 12, pp. 268-94. perspective”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 14, pp. 10-17.
Craton, L.G. and Lantos, G.P. (2011), “Attitude toward the Hamermesh, D.S. (1984), “Consumption during retirement:
advertising music: an overlooked potential pitfall in the missing link in the life cycle”, The Review of Economics
commercials”, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 5. and Statistics, Vol. 66, pp. 1-7.
Craton, L.G., Poirier, C.P., Hayden, E., Schur, A., Hampp, A. (2010), “A reprise for jingles on Madison
Gunnery, S.D., Zellers, H.J. and Merola, E.E. (2008), Avenue”, Advertising Age, Vol. 6 1, September, p. 22.
“The development of musical taste and distaste”, Poster Hargreaves, D.J., North, A.C. and Tarrant, M. (2006),
presentation at the Annual Meeting of the Eastern “Musical preference and taste in childhood and
Psychological Association, Boston, MA, 13-16 March. adolescence”, in McPherson, G.E. (Ed.), The Child as
Di Maggio, P. and Useem, M. (1978), “Social class and arts Musician: Musical Development from Conception to
consumption: the origins and consequences of class Adolescence, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
differences in exposure to the arts in America”, Theory Hecker, S. (1984), “Music for advertising effect”, Psychology
and Society, Vol. 5, pp. 141-61. & Marketing, Vol. 1 Nos 3/4, pp. 3-8.
Edell, J.A. and Burke, M.C. (1984), “The moderating effect Holbrook, M.B. (1995), “An empirical approach to
of attitude toward an ad on ad effectiveness under different representing patterns of consumer tastes, nostalgia, and
processing conditions”, in Kinnear, T.C. (Ed.), Advances in hierarchy in the market for cultural products”, Empirical
Consumer Research, Vol. 11, Association for Consumer Studies of the Arts, Vol. 12, pp. 55-71.
Research, Provo, UT, pp. 644-9. Holbrook, M.B. and Batra, R. (1987), “Assessing the role of
Englis, B.G. and Pennell, G.E. (1994), “This note’s for emotions as mediators of consumer responses to
you . . .: Negative effects of the commercial use of popular advertising”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 14,
music”, Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 21, p. 97. pp. 404-20.
Erdogan, B.Z., Baker, M.J. and Tagg, S. (2001), “Selecting Holbrook, M.B. and Hirschman, E.C. (1982),
celebrity endorsers: the practitioner’s perspective”, Journal “The experiential aspects of consumption: consumer
of Advertising Research, Vol. 41 No. 3, pp. 39-48. fantasies, feelings, and fun”, Journal of Consumer Research,
Finnäs, L. (1989), “How can musical preferences be
Vol. 9.
modified? A research review”, Bulletin of the Council for
Holbrook, M.B. and Schindler, R.M. (1989), “Some
Research in Music Education, Vol. 102, pp. 1-58.
exploratory findings on the development of musical
Fishbein, M. (1963), “An investigation of the relationship
tastes”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 16, pp. 119-24.
between beliefs about an object and the attitude toward the
Homer, P.M. (1990), “The mediating role of attitude toward
object”, Human Relations, Vol. 16, pp. 233-40.
the ad: some additional evidence”, Journal of Marketing
Fox, W.S. and Wince, M.H. (1975), “Musical taste cultures
Research, Vol. 27, February, pp. 78-86.
and taste publics”, Youth and Society, Vol. 7, pp. 198-224.
Howard, T. (2008), “Out: jingles. in: cool songs”, USA Today,
Galizio, M. and Hendrick, C. (1972), “Effect of musical
accompaniment on attitude: the guitar as a prop for 16 June, p. 1B, 3B.
Hoyer, W.D. and McGinnis, D.J. (2007), Consumer Behavior,
persuasion”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 2, pp. 350-9.
Gardner, M.P. (1985a), “Mood states and consumer 4th ed., Houghton Mifflin Company, Boston, MA.
behavior: a critical review”, Journal of Consumer Research, Hunter, P.G., Schellenberg, E.G. and Schimmack, U. (2008),
Vol. 13, pp. 281-300. “Mixed affective responses to music with conflicting cues”,
Gardner, M.P. (1985b), “Does attitude toward the ad affect Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 22, pp. 327-52.
brand attitude under a brand evaluation set?”, Journal of Huron, D. (2006), Sweet Anticipation: Music and the
Marketing Research, Vol. 22, pp. 192-8. Psychology of Expectation, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Gibson, B. (2008), “Can evaluative conditioning change Isen, A.M., Shalker, T., Clark, M. and Karp, L. (1978),
attitudes toward mature brands? New evidence from the “Affect, accessibility of material in memory and behavior: a
Implicit Association Test”, Journal of Consumer Research, cognitive loop?”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Vol. 35, pp. 178-87. Vol. 36, pp. 1-12.
Gjerdingen, R.O. and Perrott, D. (2008), “Scanning the dial: Jacob, J. (1968), “Work music and morale: a neglected but
the rapid recognition of music genres”, Journal of New important relationship”, Personnel Journal, Vol. 47,
Music Research, Vol. 37 No. 2, pp. 93-100. December, pp. 882-6.
Goldberg, M.E. and Gorn, G.J. (1987), “Happy and sad TV Juslin, P.N. and Västfjäll, D. (2008), “Emotional responses to
programs: how they affect reactions to commercials”, music: the need to consider underlying mechanisms”,
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 14, December, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 31 No. 5, pp. 559-75.
pp. 387-403. Kamenetsky, S.B., Hill, D.S. and Trehub, S.E. (1997),
Gorn, G.J. (1982), “The effects of music in advertising on “Effect of tempo and dynamics on the perception of
choice behavior: a classical conditioning approach”, Journal emotion in music”, Psychology of Music, Vol. 25, pp. 149-60.
of Marketing, Vol. 46, pp. 94-101. Kellaris, J.J. and Cox, A.D. (1989), “The effects of
Grayston, D. (1974), “Music while you work”, Industrial background music in advertising: a reassessment”, Journal
Management, Vol. 4, June, pp. 38-9. of Consumer Research, Vol. 16, pp. 113-8.
Greenwald, A.G. and Leavitt, C. (1984), “Audience Kellaris, J.J., Cox, A.D. and Cox, D. (1993), “The effect of
involvement in advertising: four levels”, Journal of background music on ad processing: a contingency
Consumer Research, Vol. 11, June, pp. 581-92. explanation”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 57, pp. 114-25.

37
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

Kellaris, J.J. and Kent, R.J. (1991), “Exploring tempo and Middlestadt, S.E., Fishbein, M. and Chan, D.K.-S. (1994),
modality effects, on consumer responses to music”, “The effect of music on brand attitudes: affect- or belief-
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 18, pp. 243-8. based change?”, in Clark, E.M., Brock, T.C. and Stewart,
Kellaris, J.J. and Kent, R.J. (2001), “An exploratory D.W. (Eds), Attention, Attitude, and Affect in Response to
investigation of responses elicited by music varying in Advertising, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 149-67.
tempo, tonality, and texture”, Journal of Consumer Mitchell, A.A. (1986), “The effect of verbal and visual
Psychology, Vol. 2 No. 4, pp. 381-401. components of advertisements on brand attitudes and
Kellaris, J.J. and Rice, R.C. (1993), “The influence of tempo, attitude toward the advertisement”, Journal of Consumer
loudness, and gender on responses to music”, Psychology Research, Vol. 13 No. 1, pp. 12-24.
& Marketing, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 15-29. Mitchell, A.A. and Olson, J.C. (1981), “Are product attribute
Kleine, R.E., Macklin, C.M. and Bruvold, N.T. (1986), beliefs the only mediator of advertising effects on brand
“Print ads and Pavlov: are they compatible?”, Proceedings of attitude?”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 18, pp. 318-32.
the 1986 Conference of the American Academy of Advertising, Moore, D.L. and Hutchinson, J.W. (1985), “The influence of
American Academy of Advertising, Provo, UT, pp. 97-102. affective reactions to advertising: direct and indirect
Konecni, V.J. (1982), “Social interaction and musical mechanisms of attitude changes”, in Alwitt, L.F. and
preference”, in Deutsch, D. (Ed.), The Psychology of Mitchell, A.A. (Eds), Psychological Processes and Advertising
Music, Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 497-516. Effects: Theory, Research and Application, Lawrence Erlbaum
Krugman, H.E. (1965), “The impact of television advertising: Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 65-87.
learning without involvement”, Public Opinion Quarterly, Morris, J.D. and Boone, M.A. (1998), “The effects of music
Vol. 29, pp. 349-56. on emotional response, brand attitude, and purchase intent
Laczniak, R.N., Muehling, D.D. and Grossbart, S. (1989), in an emotional advertising condition”, Advances in
“Manipulating message involvement in advertising Consumer Research, Vol. 25, pp. 518-26.
research”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 18 No. 2, pp. 28-38. Muehling, D.D. and Laczniak, R.N. (1988), “Advertising’s
Landon, E.L. Jr (1974), “Self concept, ideal self concept, and immediate and delayed influence on brand attitudes:
consumer purchase intentions”, Journal of Consumer considerations across message involvement levels”, Journal
Research, Vol. 1, pp. 44-51. of Advertising, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 23-34.
Leder, H., Belke, B., Oeberst, A. and Augustin, D. (2004), North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1995), “Eminence in pop
“A model of aesthetic appreciation and aesthetic music”, Popular Music and Society, Vol. 19, pp. 41-66.
North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1996a), “Situational
judgements”, British Journal of Psychology, Vol. 95,
influences on reported musical preferences”,
pp. 489-508.
Psychomusicology, Vol. 15, pp. 30-5.
Little, P. and Zuckerman, M. (1986), “Sensation-seeking and
North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1996b), “The effects of
music preferences”, Personality and Individual Differences,
music on responses to a dining area”, Journal of
Vol. 7, pp. 575-7.
Environmental Psychology, Vol. 16, pp. 55-64.
Lull, J. (1985), “On the communicative properties of music”,
North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1996c), “Responses to
Communication Research, Vol. 12, pp. 363-72.
music in aerobic exercise and yogic relaxations classes”,
Lutz, R.J. (1985), “Affective and cognitive antecedents of
British Journal of Psychology, Vol. 87, pp. 535-47.
attitude toward the ad: a conceptual framework”, in Alwitt,
North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1997a), “Liking for
L.F. and Mitchell, A.A. (Eds), Psychological Processes and musical styles”, Musicae Scientae, Vol. 1, pp. 109-28.
Advertising Effects: Theory, Research and Application, North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1997b), “Music and
Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 45-63. consumer behavior”, in Hargreaves, D.J. and North, A.C.
MacInnis, D.J. and Park, C.W. (1991), “The differential role (Eds), The Social Psychology of Music, Oxford University
of characteristics of music on high- and low-involvement Press, Oxford.
consumers’ processing of ads”, Journal of Consumer North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (1999), “Music and
Research, Vol. 18, pp. 161-73. adolescent identity”, Music Education Research, Vol. 1,
MacKenzie, S.B. and Lutz, R.J. (1989), “An empirical pp. 75-92.
examination of the structural antecedents of attitude toward North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (2000), “Music preferences
the ad in an advertising pretest context”, Journal of during and after relaxation and exercise”, The American
Marketing, Vol. 53, April, pp. 48-65. Journal of Psychology, Vol. 113, pp. 43-67.
MacKenzie, S.B., Lutz, R.J. and Belch, G.B. (1986), North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (2002), “Age variations in
“The role of attitude toward the ad as a mediator of judgments of ‘great’ art works”, British Journal of
advertising effectiveness: a test of competing explanations”, Psychology, Vol. 93, pp. 397-405.
Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 23 No. 2, pp. 130-43. North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (2007a), “Lifestyle
Macklin, C.M. (1986), “Classical conditioning effects in correlates of musical preference: 1. Relationships, living
product/character pairings presented to children”, arrangements, beliefs, and crime”, Psychology of Music,
in Lutz, R.J. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 12, Vol. 35, pp. 58-87.
Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (2007b), “Lifestyle
pp. 198-203. correlates of musical preference: 2. Media, leisure time,
Macklin, M.C. (1988), “The relationship between music in and music”, Psychology of Music, Vol. 35, pp. 179-200.
advertising and children’s responses: an experimental North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (2007c), “Lifestyle
investigation”, in Hecker, S. and Stewart, D.W. (Eds), correlates of musical preference: 3. Travel, money
Nonverbal Communication in Advertising, Lexington Books, education, employment, and health”, Psychology of Music,
Lexington, MA, pp. 225-45. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

38
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (2008), The Social and Sewall, M.A. and Sarel, D. (1986), “Characteristics of radio
Applied Psychology of Music, Oxford University Press, commercials and their recall effectiveness”, Journal of
Oxford. Marketing, Vol. 50, pp. 52-60.
North, A.C., Hargreaves, D.J. and Hargreaves, J.J. (2004), Shen, Y.-C. and Chen, T.-C. (2006), “When east meets west:
“Uses of music in everyday life”, Music Perception, Vol. 22, the effect of cultural tone congruity in ad music and
pp. 41-77. message on consumer ad memory and attitude”,
North, A.C., Hargreaves, D.J., McKenzie, L.C. and Law, R. International Journal of Advertising, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 51-70.
(2004), “The effects of musical and voice ‘fit’ on responses Sherman, S.J., Beike, D.R. and Ryalls, K.R. (1999), “Dual-
to advertisements”, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, process accounts of inconsistencies in responses to general
Vol. 34, pp. 1675-708. and specific cases”, in Chaiken, S. and Trope, Y. (Eds),
North, A.C., Hargreaves, D.J. and O’Neill, S.A. (2000), “The Dual Process Theories in Social Psychology, Guilford Press,
importance of music to adolescents”, British Journal of London.
Educational Psychology, Vol. 70, pp. 255-72. Shepherd, J. (2003), “Music and social categories”,
Olsen, G.D. (1995), “Creating the contrast: the influence of in Clayton, M., Herbert, T. and Middleton, R. (Eds),
silence and background music on recall and attribute The Cultural Study of Music: A Critical Introduction,
importance”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 14 No. 4, Routledge, London, pp. 69-79.
pp. 29-44. Shimp, T.A. (1981), “Attitude toward the ad as a mediator of
Park, C.W. and Young, S.M. (1986), “Consumer response to consumer brand choice”, Journal of Advertising, Vol. 10,
television commercials: the impact of involvement and pp. 9-15.
background music on brand attitude formation”, Journal of Shimp, T.A., Stuart, E.W. and Engle, R.W. (1991),
Marketing Research, Vol. 23, pp. 11-24. “A program of classical conditioning experiments testing
Petty, R.E. and Cacioppo, J.T. (1986), Communication and variations in the conditioned stimulus and context”, Journal
Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitudes Change, of Consumer Research, Vol. 18, June, pp. 1-12.
Springer-Verlag, New York, NY. Sloboda, J.A. and O’Neill, S.A. (2001), “Emotions in
Petty, R.E., Cacioppo, J.T. and Schumann, D. (1983), everyday listening to music”, in Jusling, P. and Sloboda, J.
“Central and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: (Eds), Music and Emotion: Theory and Research, Oxford
University Press, Oxford, pp. 415-29.
the moderating role of involvement”, Journal of Consumer
Sloboda, J.A., O’Neill, S.A. and Ivaldi, A. (2001), “Functions
Research, Vol. 10, pp. 135-46.
of music in everyday life: an exploratory study using the
Pham, M.T. (1998), “Representativeness, relevance, and the
experience sampling method”, Musicae Scientiae, Vol. 5,
use of feelings in decision making”, Journal of Consumer
pp. 9-32.
Research, Vol. 25 No. 2, pp. 144-59.
Srull, T. (1983), “Affect and memory: the impact of affective
Pitt, L.F. and Abratt, R. (1988), “Music in advertisements for
reactions in advertising on the representation of product
unmentionable products – a classical conditioning
information in memory”, in Bagozzi, R.P. and Tybout, A.M.
experiment”, International Journal of Advertising, Vol. 7
(Eds), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 10, pp. 520-5.
No. 2, pp. 130-7. Stayman, D.M. and Aaker, D.A. (1988), “Are all the effects of
Plazak, J. and Huron, D. (2010), “The first three seconds:
ad-induced feelings mediated by [attitude toward the ad]?”,
listener knowledge gained from brief musical excerpts”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 15 No. 2, pp. 368-73.
in Demorest, S.D., Morrison, J.P. and Campbell, S. (Eds), Steinburg, B. and Parekh, R. (2008), “Judges’ panel or Ad
Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Music Age front page?”, Advertising Age, 21 July, p. 10.
Perception and Cognition (ICMPC11), Seattle, WA, pp. 88-91. Stewart, D.W., Farmer, K.M. and Stannard, C.I. (1990),
Rentfrow, P.J. and Gosling, S.D. (2003), “The do-re-mi’s of “Music as a recognition cue in advertising-tracking
everyday life: the structure and personality correlates of studies”, Journal of Advertising Research, Vol. 30 No. 4,
music preferences”, Journal of Personality and Social pp. 39-48.
Psychology, Vol. 84, pp. 1236-56. Stout, P. and Leckenby, J.D. (1988), “Let the music play:
Rentfrow, P.J. and Gosling, S.D. (2006), “Message in a music as a nonverbal element in TV commercials”,
ballad: the role of music preferences in interpersonal in Hecker, S. and Stewart, D.W. (Eds), Nonverbal
perception”, Psychological Science, Vol. 17, pp. 236-42. Communication in Advertising, Lexington Books/D.C.
Ross, S.A. (1966), “Background music systems – do they Heath, Lexington, MA, pp. 207-23.
pay?”, Administrative Management Journal, Vol. 27, August, Stout, P.A., Leckenby, J.D. and Hecker, S. (1990), “Viewer
pp. 34-7. reactions to music in television commercials”, Journalism
Rubin, D.C. (1977), “Very long-term memory for prose and Quarterly, Vol. 67, pp. 887-98.
verse”, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, Stout, P.A. and Rust, R.T. (1986), “The effect of music on
Vol. 16 No. 5, pp. 611-21. emotional response to advertising”, in Larkin, E.F. (Ed.),
Schellenberg, E.G., Iverson, P. and McKinnon, M.C. (1999), Proceedings of the 1986 Conference of the American Academy of
“Name that tune: identifying popular recordings from brief Advertising, School of Journalism, University of Oklahoma,
excerpts”, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, No. 6, pp. 641-6. Norman, OK, pp. R82-5.
Schellenberg, E.G., Peretz, I. and Vieillard, S. (2008), Stuart, E.W., Shimp, T.A. and Engle, R.W. (1987), “Classical
“Liking for happy- and sad-sounding music: effects of conditioning of consumer attitudes: four experiments in an
exposure”, Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 22, pp. 218-37. advertising context”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 14,
Scott, L.M. (1990), “Understanding jingles and needledrop: pp. 334-9.
a rhetorical approach to music in advertising”, Journal of Sullivan, G.L. (1990), “Music format effects in radio
Consumer Research, Vol. 17, pp. 223-36. advertising”, Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 7, pp. 97-108.

39
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

Swasy, J.L. and Munch, J.M. (1985), “Examining the target About the author
of the receiver elaborations: rhetorical question effects on
Geoffrey P. Lantos is Professor of Business Administration
source processing and persuasion”, The Journal of Consumer
and Marketing Major Program Director, Stonehill College,
Research, Vol. 11, pp. 877-86. Easton, Massachusetts, USA. He is the author of Consumer
Tajfel, H. and Turner, J.C. (1979), “An integrative theory of Behavior in Action: Real-life Applications for Marketing
intergroup conflict”, in Austin, W.G. and Worschel, S. Managers (2011, M.E. Sharpe). He has previously published
(Eds), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations, Brooks/ in such journals as Journal of Consumer Marketing, Strategic
Cole, Monterey, CA, pp. 33-47. Direction, Journal of Biblical Integration in Business, Marketing
Tan, S.L., Spackman, P. and Peaslee, C.L. (2006), Education Review, Journal of Product & Brand Management,
“The effects of repeated exposure on liking and Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, Journal of the Academy
judgments of musical unity and patchwork compositions”, of Marketing Science, and Journal of the Market Research
Music Perception, Vol. 23, pp. 407-21. Society. He serves as Book Reviews Editor for the Journal of
Tarrant, M., North, A.C. and Hargreaves, D.J. (2002), Consumer Marketing and Journal of Product & Brand
“Youth identity and music”, in MacDonald, R.A.R., Management and is on several editorial review boards. His
Hargreaves, D.J. and Miell, D. (Eds), Musical Identities, research interests include ethics, corporate social
responsibility, and educational pedagogy. Geoffrey P. Lantos
Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 134-50.
is the corresponding author and can be contacted at:
Thompson, W.F. and Cuddy, L.L. (1997), “Music
glantos@stonehill.edu
performance and the perception of key”, Journal of
Lincoln G. Craton is Associate Professor of Psychology,
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Stonehill College, Easton, Massachusetts, USA. A
Performance, Vol. 23, pp. 116-35. developmental psychologist, he has previously published in
Thompson, W.F., Graham, P. and Russo, F.A. (2005), and served as reviewer for such journals as Child
“Seeing music performance: visual influences on perception Development, Developmental Psychology, Evolutionary
and experience”, Semiotica, Vol. 156, pp. 1203-27. Psychology, and Perception and Psychophysics. His current
Thompson, W.F., Russo, F.A. and Quinto, L. (2008), research interests include own-age bias in face recognition
“Audio-visual integration of emotional cues in song”, and topics in music perception/cognition, particularly the
Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 22, pp. 1457-70. cognitive basis of musical preferences and explicit and
Thompson, W.F., Sundberg, J., Friberg, A. and Fryden, L. implicit knowledge of harmony in listeners without musical
(1989), “The use of rules for expression in the performance training. He enjoys playing jazz guitar with his trio, Linc
of melodies”, Psychology of Music, Vol. 17, pp. 63-82. Cray and the CrayTones.
Till, B.D. (1998), “Using celebrity endorsers effectively”,
Journal of Product & Brand Management, Vol. 7 No. 5, Executive summary and implications for
pp. 400-10. managers and executives
Tom, G. (1995), “Classical conditioning of unattended
stimuli”, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 12, pp. 79-87. This summary has been provided to allow managers and executives
Uhrbrock, R.S. (1961), “Music on the job: Its influence on a rapid appreciation of the content of this article. Those with a
particular interest in the topic covered may then read the article in
worker morale and production”, Personnel Psychology,
toto to take advantage of the more comprehensive description of the
Vol. 14, Spring, pp. 9-38.
research undertaken and its results to get the full benefits of the
Wallace, W.T. (1994), “Memory for music: effect of melody
material present.
on recall of text”, Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 20
No. 6, pp. 1471-85. Music is a core element of most broadcast commercials and
Wheatley, J.J. and Brooker, G. (1994), “Music and the general assumption is that including music enhances the
spokesperson effects on recall and cognitive response to a advertisement. Scholars typically argue that advertising
radio advertisement”, in Clark, E.M., Brock, T.C. and objectives will be achieved providing that listeners focus
Stewart, D.W. (Eds), Attention, Attitude, and Affect in more on the message than the music. According to Lantos
Response to Advertising, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, and Craton, this is a serious misconception which ignores the
Hillside, NJ, pp. 189-204. fact that consumer response to music in advertising is
Wheeler, B.L. (1985), “Relationship of personal significantly more intricate. The authors argue that certain
characteristics to mood and enjoyment after hearing live factors make music’s contribution to an ad “ineffective or
and recorded music and to musical taste”, Psychology of even detrimental”. Another concern is that responses to music
are mainly conceptualized as affective, despite the fact that
Music, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 81-92.
Yalch, R.F. (1991), “Memory in a jingle jungle: music as a cognitive responses occur too.
Attitude to advertising music is seen as a key variable
mnemonic device in communicating advertising slogans”,
incorporating both cognitive and affective dimensions which
Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 76 No. 2, pp. 268-75. impact on specific advertising aims. Cognitive components
Zajonc, R.B. (1968), “Attitudinal effects of mere exposure”, include:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph .
Degree of attention to music.
Supplement, Vol. 9, February, pp. 1-27. .
Surfacing and deeper level processing.
Zimny, G. and Weidenfeller, E. (1963), “Effects of music on .
Features of music available.
GSR and heart rate”, American Journal of Psychology, .
Remembered features of music.
Vol. 76, June, pp. 311-4. .
Images suggested by the music.

40
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

.
Musical distinctiveness. loudness and expressions and gestures can shape the
.
Perceived fit between music and advertising message. emotional reaction of listeners.
These components function to determine attention to and The third variable is listener characteristics, which impact on
recall of the commercial, create and remember associations the musical taste of consumers:
with the brand, reinforce the message and achieve brand .
Age. Life cycle and cohort effects are the two sub-
differentiation. components. Hedonic responses might alter over time due
The affective dimension likewise refers to how music to both physical and psychological factors. Preference is
stimulates and arouses feelings and emotions, mood and also influenced by the musical culture that people grow up
emotional memories. Hedonic response to music and by in.
extension to the advertisement is another aspect. For many of .
Sex. Some studies concluded that women favor softer
both component types, consumer response can be positive, music types, whereas men prefer rock and other harder
negative or neutral. forms.
A core premise of the model proposed in the present study .
Subculture. Particular musical tastes can emerge based on
is that attitude towards advertising music is determined by such as ethnicity, religion, geography or age. Musical
complex interaction between four variables, each of which has subcultures have been a defining feature of various youth
several strands. It is suggested by Lantos and Craton that each movements.
strand potentially impacts on one or more of the attitude .
Social class. Limited evidence suggests possible links such
components. as higher socioeconomic groups favoring “serious” music
The listening situation is the first variable and incorporates:
like classical or opera.
.
Ongoing activities. A commercial’s music plays in a context
and must be appropriate to a particular setting in order to Other possible influences on musical taste are group or
positively impact on attitude. Response to the music is individual identity, motivated by such as a need to belong or
determined by what the listener is doing. personal intelligence. Prior mood is another factor and its
.
Social context. Whether listeners are alone or with others positive or negative nature can impact on response to
and the nature of the relationships. Working, socializing, advertising music and to purchase intention, certain
exercising or other activities might also influence their academics claim.
reaction. What strategies a consumer uses to process advertisements
.
Program content. Fit between ad music and what the is the last variable and consists of:
program is about. .
Level of ad processing involvement. Individuals highly
.
Voluntary or involuntary exposure. Response to advertising involved with the advertised product are likely to
music could be negative when people are forced to listen. consider music-message fit to be important. When fit is
Included in musical stimulus, the second variable are: poor, music becomes a distraction and consumer response
.
Whether music is foreground or background. Music can be is negative.
distinctive or incidental, in part helped by the inclusion or .
Nature of product involvement. Interest in a specific product
omission of words. Slogans or jingles might help message or brand can be cognitive or affective. The first inspires
recall. In the authors’ opinion, selection of foreground information acquisition; the latter reflects a desire to fulfill
music carries the most risks. Although its impact on needs of a more emotional, aesthetic or social nature.
attitude is often inconsequential, background music must .
Type of advertising processing involvement. The two types
also be carefully chosen. respectively relate to processing a commercial’s message
.
Musical sources. Advertisers can use an original content or its ‘executional elements’ like music. Nature of
composition, an existing tune or an adaptation of a consumer’s product involvement determines the type.
existing music. Factors to consider are familiarity or .
Processing route. Consumers engaging in central-route
unfamiliarity to the audience and the music’s level of processing are more motivated and able to process
distinctiveness. How the music fits with the commercial information. Either high cognitive, ad-message
and appropriateness of remembered features are other involvement or high affective, ad-executional
issues to influence the decision. Investigations into the involvement can result. Motivation is lower among those
relative effectiveness of these musical sources have proved using peripheral-route processing, where response is more
inconclusive. passive using less important cues that are easier to process.
.
Structural characteristics. Time, pitch, texture and Background music in a commercial can be a key
complexity are the components here. Listener responses peripheral cue.
to these characteristics are often contradictory but certain
elements can positively affect arousal, hedonic response Lantos and Craton make additional suggestions about how
and purchase intention. However, the wrong combination different advertising processing scenarios influence attitude to
of characteristics can negatively impact on cognitive and advertising music. These include:
affective components. .
Simple, catchy lyrics can inspire deeper processing of ad
.
Genre and style. Style significantly influences response and music and improve recall of ad message.
preference is possibly determined by listener personalities. . Emotional appeals made through music can trigger
Mixing genre and style helps create brand image and emotional responses and influence the listener’s attitude.
mood of the advertisement. .
Repeated exposure to music can create a positive response
. Musical artist/performance. Using well-known performers among low affective involvement consumers. Although
often boosts ad credibility and scholars claim that timing, overkill can damage if a jingle becomes irritating.

41
A model of consumer response to advertising music Journal of Consumer Marketing
Geoffrey P. Lantos and Lincoln G. Craton Volume 29 · Number 1 · 2012 · 22 –42

.
Consumers can be conditioned by music if it is able to might affect music-message fit and the relationships and
generate a mood which is deemed appropriate to the possible contradictory effects between different variables
brand. included in the present study. An investigation of music
The authors alert managers to various considerations that will within other contexts like retail environments, online
help gauge the likely response to their commercial. They also advertising and sponsorship of music concerts is another
emphasize the need to identify the type of advertising approach to consider.
processing in order to focus on aspects able to make the music
more effective. (A précis of the article “A model of consumer response to
Future research could further explore the impact of advertising music”. Supplied by Marketing Consultants for
foreground versus background music, other factors that Emerald.)

To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com


Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints

42

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen