Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Endnote

A Developmental Model Applied to Problems of Deafness


Hilde S. Schlesinger

From the Editors: This article represents another in our se- would explain the persistent discrepancy between the
ries of “classics” that helped to shape the field of deaf studies normal potential and the depressed achievement of . . .
and deaf education. The article first appeared as Chapter
deaf [people]. We have therefore sought to understand
Two in Sound and Sign: Childhood Deafness and Mental Health,
H. S. Schlesinger and K. P. Meadow (1972), Berkeley: Uni- the interaction between the environment and the deaf
versity of California Press. The book reported pioneering re- individual by building a developmental framework that
search and clinical mental health services at the Langley Por- might help to explain the gulfs and the discrepancies.
ter Neuropsychiatric Institute, University of California, San A review of the literature dealing with psychiatric
Francisco. For current readers, some of the language may
services to adult deaf populations . . . reveals that the
seem out of date, and the Editors have made several minor
modifications to ensure that readers fully recognize the origi- incidence of schizophrenia is no higher among the deaf
nal intention of the author. (Such modifications are indicated than among the hearing population, but the incidence
by square brackets or ellipses for contemporary purposes, but of increased problems of living . . . appear inordinately
the intentions of the original all have been maintained, and higher (Rainer & Altshuler, 1966). We have drawn on
Editors’ notes are indicated as such to distinguish them from
the Authors’ notes.) Nevertheless, many of the ideas are fresh
this knowledge in our attempt to provide mental health
and important. Indeed, some passages serve as particular re- services to a deaf population and their family members.
minders of significant changes in opportunities for and atti- Certain shared themata began to emerge from our ear-
tudes about Deaf people over the past three decades. Many liest days of clinical work. We met patients whose lives
of these changes resulted directly from the work of Dr. Schle-
were riddled by the consequences of unbridled impul-
singer and her colleagues.
sivity. We encountered innumerable patients whose
controls remained externalized and vaguely perceived,
Our subjects want to be whole and the clinicians
and who lived as if by a rule book of etiquette (Levine,
must have some theories and methods which offer
1956) rather than by internalized controls. We met
the patient a whole world to be whole in.
many parents who complained about the arduous task
Erikson, 1964, p. 136
of communicating with their deaf children. And we
Our [work reflects] an impulse for synthesis, for finding met their counterparts—children grown to adulthood,
unities, . . . [seeking] to bridge the gulf between [deaf bitter about their parents. They derided and despaired
and hearing people], between research and ser- of the parental insistence on “being normal” (as repre-
vices, . . . between professionals for the deaf child and sented by normal speech); the universal pressure to get
the deaf adult. We also seek a unifying theory that them to belong to “the hearing world”; the false hopes
Correspondence should be sent to Kathryn P. Meadow-Orlans, 8202
and the frequent failures, and the pressure to adjust
Kenfield Court, Bethesda, MD 20817-3147 (e-mail: horlans@erols.com). rather than to grow.
䉷2000 Oxford University Press We began to wonder whether we could trace the
350 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 5:4 Fall 2000

impact of auditory deficit back through the develop- not to do to what children at what age, but then we
mental steps, the “critical moments” that Erikson has must still learn what to do spontaneously and joyfully.”
so eloquently defined. It is difficult and may not be pro- We hope that our analyses will help lessen the inimical
ductive to delineate the effects of the deficit itself (as influences on the deaf child’s development and will
separate from its interplay with significant others), but give some clues about what society and parents can do
as researchers and clinicians we have come increasingly spontaneously and joyfully.
to see how human growth—anatomical, neurological,
and psychological—depends on environmental factors.
Basic Trust Versus Mistrust—Infancy
We began to wonder whether the environmental fac-
tors affecting the resolutions of the critical moments The basic task of infancy, which spans the period from
throughout the development of the deaf individual birth to twelve or eighteen months, is to establish a
were significant; and what is more important, whether sense of trust in the world, which will become a feeling
these factors might be alterable through specific inter- of hope about oneself and the world. The successful
ventions. resolution of this critical moment depends on the mu-
In order to make our theoretical position clear to tual regulation between mother and child. This is a pe-
the reader, we would like to trace the development of riod during which the infant (by active participation)
the deaf individual in an Eriksonian epigenetic scheme. conveys the message—exultant or despairing—“I am
Erikson maintains that the whole life cycle, the eight what I am given.” It is a time when ideally the infant
stages of man, can be seen as an integrated psycho- finds that the physical environment and the people that
social development in a sequence of critical phases. A inhabit it are trustworthy, that they respond to his
critical phase can be described as a biologically moti- needs in largely positive and predictable ways.
vated process of maturation which requires psycholog- The present literature on the early stages of child
ical adaptation to achieve a new level of development. development is vast and often confusing, but there does
Each phase is characterized by a phase-specific task seem to be some consensus on the optimal learning en-
which must be solved although the Anlage (the precur- vironment for the young child as one in which the
sor) for the solution is prepared in the previous ones young child is cared for (a) in his own home, (b) in the
and is worked out further in the subsequent ones. context of a warm, nurturant emotional relation, (c)
with his mother (or a reasonable facsimile thereof) (d)
Each successive step then is a potential crisis . . .
under conditions of varied sensory and cognitive input
used in a developmental sense to connote . . . a
(Caldwell, 1967).1 We shall touch upon these assump-
turning point, a crucial period of increased vulner-
tions throughout our discussion.
ability and heightened potential, and therefore, the
What about the context of a warm, nurturant rela-
ontogenetic source of generational strength and
tion with the child’s mother? The ability to provide this
maladjustment. (Erikson, 1968, p. 96)
depends on the mother’s successful resolution of the
Each crisis can be described in terms of extremes of normative crisis of pregnancy and parenthood, which
successful and unsuccessful solutions, although the depends on the maturity of the mother, influenced in
usual outcome is a balance between these two extremes: turn by her self-esteem as a person and a mate, and her
(1) Basic Trust versus Mistrust; (2) Autonomy versus readiness and her willingness to have a child. Further-
Shame and Doubt; (3) Initiative versus Guilt; (4) In- more, it depends on her ability and willingness to have
dustry versus Inferiority; (5) Identity versus Identity a particular child of a particular sex and temperament
Diffusion; (6) Intimacy versus Isolation; (7) Generativ- (Thomas et al., 1968) at a specific moment of her life.
ity versus Stagnation; (8) Integrity versus Despair. The The successful resolution of the crisis of motherhood
successful solution of any crisis depends on its diffi- also depends on the familial and cultural support to the
culty and the resources (individual, parental, and soci- new mother.
etal) immediately available. Erikson has stated in sev- The concern with the varied sensory input is of
eral places that “we are gradually learning what exactly more recent origin than the focus on the nurturant
Endnote 351

quality of the maternal person. Psychoanalysis has fre- evidence that indicates deaf parents of deaf children
quently referred to the incorporative modes of early in- cope with the crisis of diagnosis easily and quickly,
fancy and to the development of an early sense of iden- while their hearing counterparts prolong and intensify
tification as a precursor to the adult identity. Erikson it. Once the initial diagnosis is made, deaf parents are
(1968) does briefly discuss the fact that the “senses take less likely to seek confirmatory diagnosis or a miracu-
in what feels good;” but for our major understanding lous cure.
of the crucial importance of early cognitive experience Turning to the cognitive environment of the deaf
we must turn to Piaget, Hebb, and others. Hunt (1964) infant, our tentative data do suggest that deaf infants
has summarized this body of cognitive theory and re- may be “more quiet” and slightly more passive. If this
search and generally emphasized the importance of the be true, they may not actively seek out the environ-
environment in early perceptual and cognitive experi- ment, and their parents in turn may permit them to
ence. We learn that at gradually increasing ages the rest more quietly without providing them with the va-
perceptual and cognitive input must be varied (the in- riety of stimuli so necessary for ongoing cognitive de-
fant must see and hear a variety of environmental stim- velopment. What steps might be taken to help a deaf
uli); these stimuli must become familiar to the infant; child to approximate the oft-quoted Piagetian apho-
and they must become reproducible by him. rism (Hunt, 1964) “the more a child has seen and
heard, the more he wants to see and hear”? The litera-
ture on audiology more and more demonstrates that
The Impact of Deafness on Basic Trust
sophisticated hearing aids can successfully be worn by
Literature on the birth of . . . children [with special the very young infant so that he may hear more.
needs] (reviewed in Ross, 1964; Wright, 1960) clearly As for seeing, deaf children may need a greater va-
indicates that their . . . mothers go through stages of riety of visual stimuli to compensate for the lack or
guilt, sorrow, mourning, and anger, which may well in- diminution of auditory stimuli. Burlingham (1967) re-
terfere with stable, warm relations with their infants. fers to the overwhelming importance of sound in the
Our clinical work confirms findings (Fellendorf and lives of blind children. A number of studies (Meadow,
Harrow, 1970; Meadow, 1968b) that the new parents of 1968a; Brill, 1960; Stuckless and Birch, 1966) indicate
a deaf child are frequently “let down” by the experts at that the reason deaf children of deaf parents do better
the time of diagnosis. A large majority of these parents cognitively may be that such deaf infants see language
do not receive adequate support from the professionals at an earlier stage of development; this frequent expo-
they have sought out for diagnostic certainty, therapeu- sure to “seen language” may enable the infant to see
tic guidance, and prognostic formulations. The medical more, thus cumulatively fostering the sensorimotor
expert is hampered by ubiquitous ignorance about schemata (Hunt, 1964). There may be “critical peri-
deafness, an equally ubiquitous repugnance to give out ods” at which different kinds of behavior are most
“bad news,” a frequent reluctance to deal with so-called effectively learned and incorporated. If such learning
irreversible [conditions], and a vast array of conflicting does not take place at the appropriate times, it may be
ideologies surrounding deafness in the young child. less completely learned and may break down in times
The often maligned “anxious parents” who go “shop- of stress.
ping for experts” must indeed shop around arduously We have briefly discussed the caretaking and the
before finding experts who can guide them in rearing a external sensory environment, but what about the deaf
deaf child successfully. Finding helpful experts, meet- infant himself? How does the lack of auditory contact
ing other parents, working out varieties of compensa- with the environment affect his cognitive and affective
tory solutions . . . —all these partial resolutions free being in the sensorimotor infancy stage? We do not
the parent to be more effective. know because, unfortunately, few observational studies
Interestingly, deaf parents of deaf children appear of deaf infants have been performed. Downs (1968),
to expect the diagnosis and to accept it at a much ear- Lenneberg (1967), and other workers have noted the
lier age. There is also some experimental and clinical deaf infant goes through the usual developmental steps
352 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 5:4 Fall 2000

in the sequence of vocalizations, and presumably other the child is frequently described as stubborn, as alter-
developmental milestones. We remain to wonder on nately clinging to mother or violently pushing her away,
two counts. For the hearing infant the warmth and nur- alternately accepting and refusing the power struggle
turance is perceived almost immediately when he hears of toilet training. It appears as if the child were saying
the bustle announcing the arrival of the caretaker; this “I want to do with my muscles, my words, and my
immediacy must be delayed for the deaf infant, who sphincters (which all have recently and laboriously
must wait to see or touch the maternal person before come to be under my control) what feels good to me.” If
obtaining gratification. Furthermore, the hearing in- the parents have some sense of dignity as autonomous
fant’s responsiveness to the perceptual input implies human beings, they will be able to help the child form
that audition plays a major role in the perceptual or- a benevolent conscience during this willful period.
ganization. For as Hunt says (1964, p. 88) “something They will gently help him to “stand on his own feet
heard becomes something to look at, something to look while at the same time protecting him from meaning-
at becomes something to grasp, and sometimes some- less and arbitrary experiences of shame and of early
thing to grasp becomes something to suck.” We do not doubt” (Erikson, 1968, p. 110). But if the outer con-
know if the absence of audition changes the deaf in- trols are not firmly reassuring, if the child is denied the
fant’s organization of perceptual inputs or his abilities gradual and guided experience of the autonomy of free
to actively retain or regain certain patterns of stimulus choice, he may develop a precocious conscience. He
change. will turn against himself all his urge to discriminate
Thus during this early stage the parents and ex- and to manipulate. He may become obsessed with his
perts who make up the external environment clearly need to control the environment and will do so stub-
suffer from the auditory deficit of the infant; it is un- bornly and repetitively. Such attempts are the infantile
clear how much the infant suffers, directly and indi- source of later efforts in adult life to govern by the letter
rectly, but the impact of the deficit becomes clearer in rather than by the spirit of the law. Outer controls ap-
the stages that follow. plied with too much shaming, however, may produce
in the child a secret determination to get away with
things, may indeed lead to defiant shamelessness (Erik-
Autonomy Versus Shame and Doubt—
son, 1963).
Early Childhood

The basic task of early childhood, the period from


The Impact of Deafness on Autonomy
eighteen months to three years, is to develop a sense of
autonomy, the sense of being a separate human entity We speculate that words, like people or products, also
who has control over his body and can influence the can be held onto or let go at will. Fully aware that
environment. This autonomy is accompanied by a feel- speech learning is an arduous cognitive task, we are ac-
ing of good will toward self and environment. The suc- cumulating clinical and research evidence that noncog-
cessful resolution of this critical moment depends on nitive, psychological forces also play their role in the
“mutual regulation” between parent and child, so that [“speechlessness”] of many deaf individuals. As the
the child may develop a feeling of “I am what I will.” mother can precipitate battles over toilet training, she
Bitter power struggles often develop between the par- can precipitate similar battles over “word training,”
ents and the child, who is “[m]aking rapid gains in and in both battles the child can self-destructively win.
muscular maturation, verbalization and in discrimina- There is evidence from the clinical literature of non-
tion and the consequent ability—and doubly felt in- deaf but mute children that delayed talking can be a
ability—to coordinate a number of highly conflicting specific syndrome (Filippi and Rousey, 1968), or that
action patterns characterized by the tendencies of elective mutism (Levy, 1955) can have an interpersonal
holding on and letting go” (Erikson, 1968, p. 107). origin. These nondeaf, mute youngsters interestingly
This developing will to retain or release at will enough demonstrate signs of negativism, impulsive ag-
affects the entire being of the young child. Behaviorally gressiveness, fearfulness of adult disapproval, and
Endnote 353

provocation of adult nurturance and overprotection, all child habitually loses in the struggle for autonomy, then
of which can be seen as sequelae of an unresolved crisis these resistant feelings take on a hostile quality.
of autonomy.
Deaf youngsters show delayed resolutions of the
Initiative Versus Guilt—Childhood and the
crisis of autonomy in many areas other than the verbal.
Anticipation of Roles
Accounts of delayed toilet training, feeding problems,
impositions of stringent safety measures abound in our The task of childhood—three to six years—is to de-
clinical and research material. It appears clear that an velop a sense of initiative with a feeling of the purpose-
optimal imposition of restraints on the young child is fulness of life and of one’s own self. With optimal reso-
hampered by the lack of meaningful reciprocal com- lution of the previous stages, the child has reached a
munication at this age. The parents fear to let their more advanced kind of identification; he is by now con-
deaf child stand on his own feet, despite numerous pro- vinced that he is a person on his own and must now
fessional exhortations to do so. This is less true of deaf find out the kind of person he may become (Erikson,
parents of deaf children, who appear to be more com- 1968). The chant of this age could be “I am what I can
fortable with their children, admit to fewer eating and imagine I will be.” Children at this stage frequently at-
toilet training problems, and permit earlier indepen- tach themselves to people with occupations they can
dence and autonomy. grasp: firemen and policemen, gardeners and plumb-
There is some indication that by being gentle and ers. Erikson feels that such superficial identification
warm in areas of behavior that are extremely important with adults who are removed from the child may be
to her, the mother will secure willing acquiescence; be- safer than identification with the unequal, superior
ing punitive and cold, on the other hand, may bring parent. Ideally, however, he feels that children can de-
temporary conformity but ultimately will spur rebel- velop a more realistic identification with parents as
lion and resentment. Speech seems to be the area of people “who feel equal in worth although different in
importance to most [hearing] parents of deaf chil- kind or function or age. [This identification] permits a
dren,2 and therefore research into the effectiveness peaceful cultivation of initiative, a truly free sense of
of parental insistence on the production of the spo- enterprise” (Erikson, 1959, p. 82).
ken word could have theoretical and practical rele- The [usual] child moves intrusively into a larger so-
vance. It may be that many children perceive such ciety: he does so by vigorous locomotion, occasional
insistence as punitive and react by rebellious mute- aggressiveness, expression of so-called infantile sexual-
ness. Our own work so far indicates that a reduction ity, and insatiable curiosity manifested by unceasing
in the anxious demand for oral communications by questions. As in all stages of development, the environ-
parents, teachers, or therapist has enabled many deaf ment must present certain minimum nutriments to fur-
youngsters to delight in their usage of speech for the ther the growth of the child. Earlier, a variety and
first time. . . . abundance of meaningful sensory input were of prime
As a result of the added arenas in which power importance; now the responsiveness of the environ-
struggles can be waged with their parents, many deaf ment to the child’s activities assumes primary impor-
children go through this stage with an intensity of neg- tance. The parents need to be able both to let the child
ativism that interferes with normal maturation. Levy pursue his locomotor and manipulative intentions and
(1955) postulates that oppositional (or negative) behav- to provide answers to his endless questions. Without
ior originally serves many useful purposes, primarily these supports during the second, third, and fourth
that of resistance to external influences and formation years of life, a child cannot continue to profit no matter
of a more powerful self-concept. The capacity to resist how favorable his circumstances during the first year
external influences thus enables the child to use and (Hunt, 1964).
develop inner controls and to develop feelings of sepa- Ideally, the parents at this stage must show the
rateness and independence. But when an obstacle oc- child by example and by simple precept the ethos and
curs in the way of this maturational process, when the morality of the society within which they live. The
354 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 5:4 Fall 2000

child in turn must begin to experiment with his initia- theory and behave as if reciprocal communication by
tive and to test the world about him: “May I do such- gestures or by American Sign Language will in some
and-such, or will those powerful adults stop me, or dis- magical way interfere with speech development. Thus
own me?” As Erikson says (1968, p. 119), “The great the child’s verbal and motor exuberance remains inhib-
governor of initiative is conscience. . . . It is the onto- ited by his own deficit and by the restrictive envi-
genetic cornerstone of morality.” Conscience at this age ronment.
has a tendency to be more intransigent than the parents The environment places an additional deprivation
had desired and requires an obedience more literal than on the development of the deaf youngster. Although
the parents had intended. The indispensable contribu- identifications at this age still remain primarily famil-
tion of this stage to later identity development is that ial, there is increasing evidence from research on ethnic
of freeing the child’s initiative and sense of purpose for minority group children that self-esteem is already de-
adult tasks which promise but cannot guarantee a ful- veloping, that a youngster at this age “knows” if his
fillment of one’s range of capacities. The sense of pur- person is seen as good or bad by the overall society
pose is the courage to envisage and pursue valued goals (Coles, 1964; Goodman, 1964; Pettigrew, 1964). The
uninhibited by guilt and by fear of unreasoning pun- deaf child of hearing parents is characteristically de-
ishment (Erikson, 1964). prived of contact with successful deaf adults, a fact that
may deeply influence his self concept. For if he only
sees deaf children and never meets deaf adults, he may
The Impact of Deafness on Initiative
develop distorted expectations of what happens to deaf
The childhood stage is characterized in [most young- children grown up: Do they become like the hearing?
sters] by marked verbal and motor exuberance. In the Do they go into hiding? Do they disappear? Do they
deaf child, this exuberance is doubly inhibited—the die? The deaf child of deaf parents clearly has higher
verbal exuberance is almost invariably diminished by a self-esteem at the early ages (Meadow, 1968a). Re-
paucity of symbols, but the feelings remain exuberant, search on disadvantaged youngsters has shown that
and it is not surprising that youngsters “deprived of their self-esteem and school achievement rise dramat-
the ability or opportunity to express powerful feelings ically when successful adults of their own color or
in words [or signs] . . . usually erupt in actions” (Ka- linguistic competence are introduced into their social
tan, 1961). The motor exuberance is thus potentially in- and learning environments (Ervin-Tripp, 1966).
creased for the deaf youngster; but in practice this in- Another characteristic of this age is the torrent of
crease is inhibited by the innumerable safety limits questions children release. These questions are not
[hearing] parents place on their deaf youngsters, reduc- forthcoming from the deaf child at this age, and if they
ing their intrusive meanderings into the outside world. were, the parent would have an impossible task to ex-
School settings (most deaf children attend schools at plain complicated events to a linguistically impover-
early ages) frequently place a premium on immobility. ished child. The analogue with the culturally deprived
Very young children are expected to sit still for long child is striking. Researchers on disadvantaged chil-
periods in order to observe the teacher vigilantly. This dren from minority ethnic groups tell us that “the ver-
forced inactivity appears to place a stress on many chil- bal patterns provided by the parents are important not
dren, a stress that may preclude learning, especially for only in terms of information-processing, but also in
the large-muscled young boys. terms of establishing reasons for external events and
Unfortunately, the optimal teaching situation for reasons for behavior” (Kessler, 1970).
motorically exuberant youngsters with [diminished] As Hess and Shipman (1968) noted in contrasting
auditory contact with the environment is hard to define middle-class and lower-class black children, the depri-
and harder to achieve. Furthermore, in the poignant vation among the lower-class children appeared to be
search for . . . speech, parents and educators disregard a deprivation of meaning in the early cognitive relation
research findings from child development and learning between mother and child. This environment produces
Endnote 355

a child who relates to authority rather than to rationale, more than other disabilities appears to frighten the un-
who may often be compliant but is not reflective in his initiated into a “shock-withdrawal paralysis” reaction
behavior, and who thinks of the consequences of an act on their first exposure. To function optimally under
largely in terms of immediate punishment or reward these circumstances can be a Herculean task. Adding
rather than in terms of future effects and long range to the sense of provincialism and despair felt by many
goals. . . . The parallel between children who are lin- teachers for the deaf, almost yearly publications lament
guistically deprived by virtue of deafness and those the “failure of deaf education,” and thus provoke ex-
who are deprived because of race and social class de- pectations of further failures on the part of the
serves to be investigated further (Schlesinger, 1970). teachers.
Teachers who feel estranged from society, or who
feel estranged from the children they teach naturally
Industry Versus Inferiority—School Age
have difficulty convincing their young students that
The basic task of the school years—from ages six to learning has a pay-off. Teachers who have become con-
eleven—is to develop a sense of industry, with an ac- vinced that a particular group of children are non-
companying feeling of competency. Having mastered a achievers or low achievers can approach the learning
basic trust in the environment and the people that in- environment with negative expectations, which in turn
habit it, the child becomes a relatively autonomous hu- can reinforce underachievement (Rosenthal and Jacob-
man being who feels he can be what he imagines he will son, 1968; Gozali, 1969). Teachers who do not have in-
be. Now during the school years, he can proclaim, “I sight into the positive values of culturally different
am what I can learn.” This necessitates a feeling of learners and understanding of their learning style, lan-
competence which is “a free exercise of dexterity and guage and modes of behavior (Daniel, 1967), or teach-
intelligence in the completion of tasks, unimpaired by ers who evoke fear and hostility are ineffective in moti-
infantile inferiority” (Erikson, 1964, p. 124). A success- vating their pupils to learn.
ful resolution of this critical period depends on the Many teachers of the deaf have consciously and
child’s weathering of the previous crises and his inter- conscientiously struggled with these challenges. But
action with a society, a family, and a learning environ- even with the most dedicated teachers, the deaf child
ment which expects success from that particular child, will demonstrate a delayed solution of the crisis of
in that particular period of history in his particular way. competency and achievement as defined by the major-
ity culture. Although Furth (1970) finds less retarda-
tion in symbolic manipulation or in the performance of
The Impact of Deafness on Competency
Piagetian tasks than other authors do, he does not dis-
We have discussed the unsuccessful resolutions of the pute the verbal [lags] of most deaf subjects. It must be
prior crises for the deaf child, and now let us emphasize remembered, however, that no tests of “verbal compe-
the potentially noxious contributors to a false or dis- tency” have generally been given in the language of
torted crisis resolution on the part of the social environ- signs, known by most deaf adolescents and used by 80
ment that surrounds him. percent of deaf adults with their deaf friends (Cram-
Society has not always been kind to its “different,” matte, 1968).
exceptional members. It has condemned them, ex-
cluded them, and refused them succor and education
Identity Versus Identity Diffusion—Adolescence
(Schlesinger, 1970). Some vestiges of these earlier pat-
terns can still be seen in these more humanitarian Adolescence, the period from approximately eleven to
times. There is still a pervasive disapproval of one of eighteen, can be seen as a “way of life” different from
the most potentially useful coping mechanisms of deaf- childhood and adulthood. Problems of emancipation,
ness—the American Sign Language. People are un- independence, and freedom from the family occupy the
comfortable with those who are different, and deafness early stage, while problems of social role and personal
356 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 5:4 Fall 2000

purpose within the wider world occupy the later stage. society which condemns him to inferiority, depriva-
Over the whole span of adolescence, the developmental tion, and humiliation” (Joint Commission, 1970, p.
task is to integrate earlier elements into a true sense of 358).
identity as a separate individual, no longer taking a par-
tial or external view of the self.
The Impact of Deafness on Identity
Although adolescence is traditionally considered a
time of great stress and difficulty for the youth, his par- The typical deaf adolescent approaches the crisis with
ents, and the adults who deal with him, some recent a delayed or incomplete resolution of the previous cri-
studies question this assumption. While the period ses. Thus even if optimally established in the earliest
may be disturbing for all the participants in the identity stage, his trust receives innumerable jolts, as he repeat-
struggle of the adolescent, it need not be disturbed. One edly meets others who do not accept [him], who appear
can at least imagine an optimal solution of the adoles- fearful in joint human endeavors, who idolize a [state]
cent crisis in which the adolescent is ready for the en- he cannot achieve instead of cultivating his unique
counter, parents who for many reasons need not and areas of competency. The next three childhood stages
therefore do not fear the encounter, and a society that of autonomy, initiative, and competency are probably
is flexible enough to absorb the oncoming generation directly affected by the linguistic deprivation of most
with all its differences. deaf individuals, as language assumes paramount im-
The adolescent ready for the encounter, he who may portance for academic learning and for the “learning”
travel through a gloriously vital phase, will be one who of interpersonal relationships.
is able to approach it with those crises of trust, will, Thus the deaf youngster approaches adolescence
imagination, and competency relatively resolved. with varying degrees of unresolved crises. What of the
The parents who do not fear the encounter will parents of the deaf adolescent? All too often they have
generally be parents who have resolved their own adult crises of their own that tend to interfere with the opti-
crises of generativity and care; whose personal, occupa- mal resolution of the adolescent crisis. Frequently the
tional, marital, and parental roles are relatively satisfac- parents are no longer able to deny the encompassing
tory; and who are able to look at the product of their effect of deafness on some of their cherished goals.
care and generativity—their child—in a positive way. Many parents successfully maintain the myth that their
Thus they need not see the adolescent primarily in deaf infant or child will one day appear [hearing], will
terms of an object to be lost, or an object of envy, or an speak . . ., will mate with a hearing spouse, . . . will not
intruder on the adult scene, or an unsatisfactory prod- talk with his hands, will perform academically at the
uct of their labors. level of his hearing peers. The period of latency, with
A society that will promote an optimal resolution of its relative calm, permits the parents to maintain this
the adolescent crisis is one that, despite its rapid shifts ideal untarnished. In adolescence, with the oncoming
and rapid ideological changes, has the latitude and resolutions of vocational and marital choice, the par-
flexibility to absorb individuals who vary in their ents . . . may note that their youngster relates more eas-
strengths and weaknesses, their conformity and rebel- ily with other deaf peers, frequently ignoring his own
liousness, their similarities and differences. Society family. Many adolescents will reverse roles and keep
thus must have prepared the budding citizen for work, from their parents the knowledge of their . . . compe-
and the work market must be adaptive to individual tencies in sign language, in order to protect the alleg-
differences and special needs. For if society gives edly fragile self-esteem of their parents. Many others
certain so-called disadvantaged youths only the pros- will fervently herald their negativism toward the par-
pect of failure, the disadvantaged young person may ents and their identification with the peer group by us-
have only “two choices: to accept social rejection and ing sign language to the utter exclusion of speech.
educational or occupational failure in a pattern of fatal- The parents may realize for the first time that the
ism and passivity, or to lash out violently against the vocational choices of their deaf children are strictly
Endnote 357

limited both by academic underachievement and by the developmental task of this period is to restore not the
linguistic retardation and the nonoccurrence of the old primary tie with the world and its inhabitants, but
miracle of . . . speech.3 Having placed themselves in the a more mature relation with man and nature. This pe-
position of providing prolonged, arduous, intensive riod reflects the oft-quoted, somewhat laconic, state-
help in the tasks of early child rearing, many parents ment by Freud that Lieben und Arbeiten (love and work)
hope that deferred gratification will finally come to are the hallmarks of a healthy individual. The success-
them during their child’s adolescence—a hope often ful attainment of these characteristics entails a success-
bitterly unfulfilled. Their reactions vary. Sometimes ful resolution of the adolescent identity crisis. Those
they will try futilely to bind the adolescent more closely who are not sure of their identity dare not commit
to the family, in the expectation of a delayed miracle; themselves to work for fear of being submerged by the
sometimes their parenting may turn into overt rejec- world and its institutions. Those who are not sure of
tion as if to say, “You have not lived up to my expecta- their identity dare not submit to interpersonal intimacy
tions, and both of us are doomed.” for fear of losing their fragile sense of self. Love is a
The identity road of deaf adolescents takes many deceptively simple, yet difficult word. Its most mature
paths. Some adolescents militantly enter the “deaf form
world” and exclude at least temporarily any contact
is a very specific feeling; and while every human
with the “hearing world.” Others avoid the deaf world;
being has a capacity for love, its realization is one
refusing any association with deafness, they may pur-
of the most difficult achievements. Genuine love is
sue an isolated “as if ” identification with the hearing
rooted in productiveness and may properly be
world. In an analogy with Negro identification pat-
called, therefore, “productive love.” Its essence is
terns, “they pass.” The consequences of “passing” are
the same whether it is the mother’s love for the
often pathological. Thus, Parker and Kleiner (1965)
child, our love for man, or the erotic love between
note a large body of research documenting the un-
two individuals. . . . (Fromm, 1947, p. 98)
healthiness in aspiring to be white: “Almost every clini-
cal study of psychopathology among Negroes indicates It is a love that involves knowledge, responsibility, care,
that the Negro who is not identified with other mem- and a genuine interest in furthering the growth and de-
bers of his group, or who aspires to ‘be white,’ is rela- velopment of the other person.
tively more prone to manifest various forms of mental And yet love so defined presupposes the ability to
ill health” (p. 157). As in the case of blacks, or immi- fuse identity with the mate. This act of fusion is fright-
grants, the most successful identifications may occur in ening to those who fear a loss of identity. The young
those individuals who accept the differences imposed person who is not sure “will shy away from intimacy or
by color, language, or minority group mores. The suc- throw himself into acts of intimacy which are promis-
cessful individual may thus live in many worlds; the cuous without true intimacy.” These youths may “fre-
alienated individual may be a stranger in all. quently settle for highly stereotyped interpersonal re-
The generation gap may become an insurmount- lations and come to regain a deep sense of isolation”
able chasm if the adolescent rejects the hearing world (Erikson, 1968, p. 136).
of his parents, and if the parents reject the deaf world Work is a deceptively simple, yet complex word.
of the child. Although the meaning of work has changed through
the ages, it has always denoted sustained energy ex-
pended for a goal. But the value of the work and its
Intimacy Versus Isolation—Young Adulthood
goals has changed. Through the ages work has vari-
The young adult has become a free, self-governing be- ously been viewed as degrading, as necessary, and at
ing. However, “when one has become an individual, times even as divine. The version most congruent with
one stands alone and faces the world in all its perilous modem psychiatric theories is that any kind of work,
and overpowering aspects” (Fromm, 1941, p. 29). The even sports “gives a man the joy of victorious force, be-
358 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 5:4 Fall 2000

stows on him the harsh pleasure of feeling his person- the logical sequence of a deep sense of inadequacy
ality triumphant” (Tilgher, 1962, p. 23). . . . of one’s general equipment. Such a sense of in-
adequacy . . . may convey the unrealistic demands
made by an ego ideal . . . may express the fact that
The Impact of Deafness on the Ability to Love the immediate social environment does not have a
And Work niche for the individual’s true gifts; or it may reflect
the paradoxical fact that an individual in early
Many of our young adult deaf patients give evidence school life was seduced into a specialized precocity
of identity diffusion. In this period they have become which early outdistanced his identity development.
separated from the rather protective settings of most
school situations, and they find the world quite differ- These three pathways are often seen in the develop-
ent. Their former identity in school proves inadequate ment of deaf patients. Their parents often make unre-
for an identity in the world. Some young adults have alistic demands. And when hearing parents have ob-
previously achieved competency in the school setting, jected vociferously to having deaf teachers for their
measured in terms of grades and achievement test deaf children, their growing deaf youngster may won-
scores and the ability to live by the rules of the school. der if the niches available to him may not be unneces-
But they find that the world outside does not measure sarily restricted. Furthermore, many deaf youngsters
with grades, and its rules are less tangible. Those who and their admiring parents are seduced into a belief in
have not achieved internalized controls for behavior the “specialized precocity” of the deaf child. . . .
nor internalized motivations for exercise of skills tend
to have a traumatic period in young adulthood; this Generativity Versus Stagnation—Parenthood
trauma often surprises their parents and teachers who
judged from their success in the world of school that The task of parenthood is the constructive concern
they were equipped to face the larger world. In this with the guidance of the next generation, either
traumatic relocation, they frequently take refuge in through biological offspring or through other altruistic
previously abandoned stances of dependence. endeavors of care and creation. The mere fact of having
Others among our patients had not achieved com- children, however, does not automatically guarantee
petence or identity even within the school setting. The generativity. Both ethological studies and research on
more stressful requirements of the world intensified child development, as beautifully integrated by Bowlby
their styles of defending against stress: some showed an (1969), show that young animals and children alike re-
intensification of impulsiveness, others an increase of quire early “maternal attachment” for later healthy de-
dependence, and still others showed an automaton like velopment.
conformity in which each became “exactly as all others More specifically, there is a direct relation between
are and as they expect him to be” (Fromm, 1941). receiving early adequate mothering and being able later
For those who are successful, love and work can be- in life to become an adequate, nurturant mother. Sev-
come the satisfying and exhilarating companions of eral animal experiments (Bowlby, 1969) indicate that
young adulthood. For those who have not reached the animal infants deprived of maternal attachment at a
required level of maturity, love and work will retain in- critical age may be unable to mate or to bear young at
fantile characteristics. Intimacy will be replaced by iso- a later period of life. Human mothers (the paternal role
lation, or by frantic, superficial relationships. Work and its development is not as yet so clearly defined)
may take the form of a repetitive frenetic change of jobs who have done well in their psychosocial development
or be replaced by total avoidance of exercise of skills, will be able to bear their children and care for them
all of which will be unaccompanied by any sense of confidently, competently, and selflessly. They will be
gratifying commitment. Work paralysis is seen by Er- able to introduce the young into the continuing cycle
ikson (1959, p. 144) as of generations.
Endnote 359

The Impact of Deafness on Generativity parents told of their own removal from the familial
and Care home with attendance at a residential school. It was as
if these deaf parents had incorporated the belief held
Individuals who still demand coercive gratification of by their own hearing parents: “I am not able or compe-
their own needs will have difficulty in receiving a child tent to rear my child after the age of six, nor do I
in trust and postponing some narcissistic needs in favor want to.”
of the biological, emotional, and cognitive needs of the It has become clear to us that generativity and care
new infant. can be seriously distorted through unresolved early de-
We have met immature deaf parents with their chil- velopmental crises. It has also become clear that a sense
dren who were unable to see their infant as a feeling of generativity and care may be difficult to maintain—
being. Their infants remained nameless for prolonged even when present—in the face of society’s stigma to-
periods of time, they were “mascoted,” and treated like ward . . . deafness.
dolls or objects. Some immature deaf parents have felt
so incompetent in the task of childbearing that their
Integrity Versus Despair—Old Age
infants were cared for by maternal grandmothers.
On the other hand, many competent and mature The aging person, having adapted himself to the tri-
deaf parents have developed a false sense of inade- umphs and disappointments of the stages of man, will
quacy—a feeling possibly promoted and provoked by experience the culminating sense of integrity at the
society.4 Several of our deaf mother-patients developed eighth stage (Erikson, 1968). Integrity entails the abil-
a symbiotic relation with their hearing children, ex- ity to envisage human problems in their entirety, a
pecting the child to be a protector, a guide through the detached concern with life itself in the face of approach-
mazes of the hearing world, a source of unrealistic grat- ing death. The “autonomous” among the elderly con-
ification. tinue to renew themselves, continue to grow. Others are
Another area of interest is the comparison of deaf merely “adjusted” or well preserved in old age. “If re-
mothers with their deaf and hearing children. Rainer sponsibility accompanies maturity for the autonomous
[and his colleagues, 1963] have indicated in their and takes the place of maturity for the adjusted, the
different works that deaf mothers “do better with their anomic find their way to neither” (Riesman, 1954, p.
deaf than with their hearing children.”. . . 383). The ability to remain productive, or to adjust and
Generally speaking, our deaf patients have had less accept decreased productivity, enables the old to con-
difficulty with their deaf than with their hearing chil- tinue seeing themselves as an integral part of society
dren. Many of the deaf parents have complained more and to view their own life cycle benevolently. Those en-
bitterly about the hearing than the deaf child. We occa- meshed in despair have the heavy sense “that time is
sionally have heard the seemingly paradoxical state- short, too short for the attempt to start another life and
ment “I do not know how to communicate with my to try out alternate roads of integrity” (Erikson, 1959,
child because he hears.” Some deaf parents clearly told p. 98).
us they preferred a deaf offspring. It might be that
these deaf parents had incorporated their own hearing
The Impact of Deafness on the Integrity of
parents’ stance toward child rearing. Because the hear-
Old Age
ing parents had made it abundantly clear that being
different from one’s parents was not desirable, it be- The fact that deaf individuals can develop to the level
came difficult in turn for their deaf offspring to accept of integrity and wisdom has been shown to us again
a hearing child. A few of the deaf parents have shown and again, if not by most of our patients, then by our
an interesting phenomenon—a temporary paralysis of deaf colleagues. A beautiful tribute to ego development
parental competency and nurturance for their children in the face of adversity, based on experience of and with
who were reaching school age. Historically these deaf deaf [people], is Joanne Greenberg’s moving novel, In
360 Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 5:4 Fall 2000

This Sign (1970). And although the book remains a trib- 3. Editors’ note: Changes in the vocational area, resulting
from ADA and other major reforms, are among the most striking.
ute, it fails to encompass a description of deaf individu- and important of the past thirty years.
als known to us who have reached yet higher levels of 4. It has been of marked interest (and sadness) to us that
achievement and growth. society repeatedly expects incompetency on the part of deaf
All we can say is that both literature about, and our mothers. The project frequently has received phone calls from
welfare workers anxious to remove a healthy infant from parents
direct experience with, aged deaf [people] is extremely known to us as competent—solely on the grounds of parental
scanty. We simply do not know the effect that deafness deafness. Deaf parental competency was recently questioned,
has on the crisis of old age. It could be that the impact but received judicial approval in a case where a deaf couple had
originally been denied the adoption of a child (251 Cal. App. 2nd,
of deafness on old age is even more devastating or alter-
221). [Editors’ note: Dr. Schlesinger testified on behalf of the
natively deaf [people] might be better equipped . . . to Deaf adoptive parents in this case.]
adjust to [the disabilities associated with aging] . . . It
may be that many . . . become more “autonomous” as
the hearing world no longer pushes for impossible References
achievements; it may be that many remain merely “ad-
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1. New York: Basic
justed” because of the numerous delays in their ego de- Books.
velopment; it may be that many remain “anomic” be- Brill, R. G. (1960). A study in adjustment of three groups of deaf
cause their identity was never clarified. We feel that children, Exceptional Children, 26, 464–466.
these outcomes for the aged deaf pose interesting ques- Burlingham, D. (1967). Developmental considerations in the occu-
pations of the blind. Psychoanalytic study of the child, Vol.
tions for future research. XXII. New York: International Universities Press.
Caldwell, B. M. (1967). What is the optimal learning environ-
ment for the young child? American Journal of Orthopsychia-
try, 37, 8–21.
Afterword
Coles, R. (1964). Children of crisis: A study of courage and fear.
It must be obvious that we have described the impact Boston: Little, Brown.
Crammatte, A. B. (1968). Deaf persons in professional employment.
of deafness on the eight stages of man in its most nox- Springfield, Ill.: Charles C. Thomas.
ious and nefarious form. We are fully aware from our Daniel, W. G. (1967). Some essential ingredients in educational
contact with deaf individuals that the eight stages can programs for the socially disadvantaged. In J. Hellmuth
(Ed.), Disadvantaged child, Vol. 1. New York: Brunner/
be traversed more productively, more joyfully, and with
Mazel.
a more adequate resolution of each developmental cri- Downs, M. P. (1968). Identification and training of the deaf
sis. This more successful passage through the life cycle child–birth to one year. Volta Review, 70, 154–158.
we have found most often (although not exclusively) in Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. New York: Inter-
national Universities Press.
the deaf children of deaf parents. We hope that our ac- Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
count has helped to explain this higher achievement. Erikson, E. H. (1964). Insight and responsibility. New York:
We also hope that it will help hearing parents to pon- Norton.
der, to increase the acceptance of deafness in their chil- Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth and crisis. New York:
Norton.
dren, and with this acceptance help their children to Ervin-Tripp, S. (1966). Language development. In M. L.
meet and master the challenges of each life crisis. Hoffman and L. W. Hoffman (Eds.), Review of child develop-
ment research, Vol. II (pp. 55–105). New York: Russell Sage
Received July 26, 1999; revised December 6, 1999; accepted Jan-
Foundation.
uary 7, 2000
Fellendorf, G. W., & Harrow, I. (1970). Parent counseling 1961–
1968. Volta Review, 72, 51–57.
Filippi, R., & Rousey, C. L. (1968). Delay in onset of talking–A
Notes symptom of interpersonal disturbance. Journal of the Ameri-
can Academy of Child Psychiatry, 7, 316–328.
1. Caldwell questions, however, our “overly vigorous sup- Fromm, E. (1941). Escape from freedom. New York: Rinehart.
port of (a) when (b), (c) or (d) might not obtain.” Fromm, E. (1947). Man for himself. New York: Rinehart.
2. Editors’ note: Remember that in 1970, few hearing par- Furth, H. G. (1970). A review and perspective on the thinking
ents signed: generally their only means of communication with a of deaf people. In J. Hellmuth (Ed.), Cognitive studies. New
deaf child was through speech and speechreading. York: Brunner/Mazel.
Endnote 361

Goodman, M. E. (1964). Race awareness in young children. New Parker, S., & Kleiner, R. J. (1965). Mental illness in the urban Ne-
York: Collier Books. gro community. New York: Free Press.
Gozali, J. (1969). Teacher expectations and the performance of Pettigrew, T. F. (1964). A profile of the Negro American. Princeton:
the mentally retarded. Focus on Exceptional Children, 1. Van Nostrand Company.
Greenberg, J. (1970). In this sign. Canada: Holt, Rinehart and Rainer, J. D., & Altshuler, K. Z. (1966). Comprehensive mental
Winston. health services for the deaf. New York State Psychiatric Insti-
Hess, R. D., & Shipman, V. C. (1968). Maternal influences upon tute, Columbia University.
early learning: The cognitive environment of urban pre- Rainer, J. D., Altshuler, K. Z., & Kallman, F. J., Eds. (1963).
school children. In R. Hess and R. Bear (Eds.), Early educa- Family and mental health problems in a deaf population. New
tion. Chicago: Aldine. York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Columbia Uni-
Hunt, J. M. (1964). How children develop intellectually. Chil- versity Press.
dren. Children’s Bureau, United States Department of Riesman, D. (1954). Some clinical and cultural aspects of aging.
Health, Education and Welfare, 11. American Journal of Sociology, 29, 379–383.
Joint Commission on Mental Health of Children. (1970). Crisis Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom.
in child mental health: Challenge for the 1970’s. New York: New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Harper and Row. Ross, A. O. (1964). The exceptional child in the family. New York:
Katan, A. (1961). Some thoughts about the role of verbalization Grune and Stratton.
in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 16, Schlesinger, H. S. (1970). Headstart in deafness–Early home en-
184–188. vironment. In Report of the Proceedings of the Forty-Fourth
Kessler, J. W. (1970). Contributions of the mentally retarded to- Meeting of the Convention of American Instructors of the Deaf.
ward a theory of cognitive development. In J. Hellmuth U.S. Government Printing Office.
(Ed.), Cognitive studies. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Stuckless, E. R., & Birch, J. W. (1966). The influence of early
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New manual communication on the linguistic development of
York: Wiley. deaf children. American Annals of the Deaf, 111, 452–460,
Levine, E. S. (1956). Youth in a soundless world, a search for person- 499–504.
ality. Washington Square, NY: New York University Press. Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch. H. (1968). Temperament and be-
Levy, D. M. (1955). Oppositional syndrome and oppositional be- havior disorders in children. New York: New York University
havior. In P. Hoch (Ed.), Psychopathology of childhood. New Press.
York: Grune and Stratton. Tilgher, A. (1962). Work through the ages. In S. Nosow and W.
Meadow, K. P. (1968a). Early manual communication in relation Form (Eds.), Man, work and society. New York: Basic Books.
to the deaf child’s intellectual, social, and communicative Wright, B. A. (1960). Physical disability–A psychological approach.
functioning. American Annals of the Deaf, 113, 29–41. New York: Harper and Row.
Meadow, K. P. (1968b). Parental responses to the medical ambi-
guities of deafness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 9,
299–309.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen