Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Reprinted with pennission from the publisher,

Guilford Press, pages 171-200, chapter 7, 1996,


Cognitive Behavioral Treatment of Social Phobia and
Generalizad Anxiety Disorder, Drs. Otto and Gould.

7
Cognitive-Behavioral
Treatment of Social
Phobia and Generalice d
Anxiety Disorder
ROBERT A. GOULD
MICHAEL W. OTTO

Relative to panic disorder, social phobia and generaliied anxiety disorder


(GAD) have received considerably less empirical attention over the last
decade. Social phobia was identified in the mid-1980s as a "neglected anxi-
ety disorder" that was expected to move into the limelight during the 1990s
(Liebowitz, Gorman, Fyer, & Klein, 1985); and, despite characterizations
of GAD as the "basic" anxiety disorder, it has continued to receive less
attention than other disorders. Nonetheless, speciali:ed forros of cognitive-
behavioral treatment (CBT) for these disorders have been developed and
validated, and these offer promising outcome to patients with these
problems. This chapter reviews the nature and efficacy of CBT for social
phobia and GAD, and discusses strategies for optimizing treatment outcome.

SOCIAL PHOBIA

General Considerations

In many ways, social phobia represents an extreme form of the shyness,


performance anxiety, or stage fnght that most individuáis have experienced
at some point ¡n their Uves. It is characterized by fears of humiliation or

171

43
172 ANXIETV DISORDERS Cognitive-Behavioral Treíilmenl o¡ Social Pliobin and CAD 173

cmbarrassment ¡n social or performance situations—fears that rcsult in the Thc age of onsct for social phobia is gencrally betwcen 15 and 20 years
regular provocation of anxicty and intcrfercncc in such situations. Social (Liebowitz ct al., 1985; Turner, Beidel, & Townsley, 1992). The increas-
phobia may include physical symptoms rcsembling thosc of panic disord- ing social prcssurcs and social scrutiny during adolescence may contribute
er: incrcased heart rate, hot flashes, sweating, shaking, trcmbling, short- to onset during this lifc phase. Although onset of social phobia is early
ncss of breath, and abdominal distress. Although anxicty in social situations rclative to othcr anxicty disordcrs, treatmcnt-sceking behavior is not.
may quickly crescendo into a "panic attack," social phobia and panic dis- In a rcvicw of 17 studics, Heimbcrg (1989) found that the mean age of
order are distinguished by the focus of thc fcars in each disordcr (Ball, subjccts sccking trcatmcnt ranged from 27 to 41 years, and that thc mean
Otto, Pollack, Ucccllo, & Roscnbaum, 1995; Mannuzza, Fycr, Liebowitz, duration of phobia ranged from 8.2 to 22.1 years. Individuáis prcsenting
&. Klein, 1990). The panic attack and its pcrceivcd consequcnces are the for treatment are as likcly to be mcn as womcn (Ost, 1987; Heimberg,
focus of the fear in panic disordcr, whercas conccrns about humiliation 1989).
and cmbarrassment in front of others are dominant ¡n social phobia, and
thcrc is typically no fcar whcn activitics are pcrformcd alone.
Subtypcs and Diagnostic Issucs
Oftcn thcsc physical symptoms are preceded and triggcrcd by maladap-
tive cognitions in which patients may (1) undcrestimate their ability to Clinical rcsearchcrs havc notcd that somc individuáis with social phobia
cope in social situations (e.g., "1 am certain that my mind will go blank tend to havc fcars about social intcraction that cxtcnd across a variety
when I try to givc this spcech"); (2) cxaggeratc thc perceivcd consequcnces of situations (c.g., public speaking, introducing themselvcs to others, eat-
of performing inadcquatcly in social situations (c.g., "If my face turns red ing in front of others) and individuáis (e.g., authority figures, dates, strangers)
whcn I ask Bonnie out for a date, shc will never want to go out with me"); (McNcill &. Lewin, 1986; Spitzcr ók Williams, 1985). Both the revised
and/or (3) rchearsc sclf-dcfeating and global attributions about thcmselvcs third and fourth editions of thc DíagnoJtíc and Síaiislícal Manual of Mental
and thcir futurc social behavior (c.g., "1 am a social failurc and am des- Disorders (DSM-11I-R and DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association,
tined to be a misfit"). Behavioral corrclatcs typically consist of avoidancc 1987,1994) have incorporatcd this "subtypc" into their diagnostic nomen-
bchaviors (e.g., skipping class, avoiding calling somconc for a date, or avoid- claturc and dcsignated ¡t "social phobia, gencralizcd." Patients who do not
ing a party or a convcrsation). have gcneralizcd social phobia tcnd to prescnt on a continuum of severi-
Data from three cpidcmiologic studics indícate that thc prcvalence ty, ranging from those who have problems in onc circumscribcd arca (e.g.,
of social phobia in the general population ¡s 2-3% (Myers ct al., 1984; public speaking) to those with incrcasing numbcrs of problem arcas. Re-
Pollard &. Hendcrson, 1988; Schneier, Johnson, Hornig, Liebowitz, &. search has suggcsted that gencralized social phobics report grcater fear of
Weissman, 1992). As such, it is thc most common anxiety disordcr and ncgative evaluation, grcater social avoidance, and more depression than
onc of thc most common psychiatric disordcrs. An evcn largcr perccn- social phobics with more discrctc problems (Heimbcrg, Hopc, Dodgc, ¿k
tagc of individuáis (20%) rcport problcms with social anxicty, but do not Bcckcr, 1990b). Turner et al. (1992) also found that generalized social
mect thc diagnostic criteriá for social phobia (Pollard &. Hendcrson, 1988). phobics tended to be more severcly impaired and to pcrform more poorly
It is not uncommon for individuáis with social phobia to suffcr from comor- on behavioral avoidancc tests. Collectively, these findings suggest that
bid problems. Liebowitz ct al. (1985) reportcd that roughly 20% of thc gencralizcd social phobics may havc grcater difficulty responding to treat-
social phobics in their study sample mct thc criteriá for alcohol dcpen- ment, and may requirc more specialized or intensive treatment intcr-
dcnce or abuse, and that approximatcly onc-third prcscntcd with major vcntions.
depression. Ross (1994a) paints an cqually clisturbing picturc of the livcs A scconcl diagnostic issuc relates to thc rclationship bctwccn social
of social phobia patients and describes individuáis who avoid Jobs that phobia and avoidant pcrsonality disorder. Avoidant pcrsonality disorder
requirc social contact, fail to dcvclop sustaincd friendships and rclation- ¡s defined as a pcrvasivc pattcrn of social discomfort and fcar of negative
ships, forgo carecr promotions that may potentially require incrcased so- evaluation ¡n which individuáis avoid social and intcrpcrsonal contact and
cial contact, and ¡solatc themsclvcs from others outside of thcir ¡mmcdiatc are easily hurt by criticism and disapproval of others. Studies have sug-
families. Turner, Beidel, Dancu, and Kcys (1986) found that 92% of a sample gcsted that there is a largc degree of ovcrlap bctwccn the two disordcrs.
of social phobia patients fclt that thcir anxicty has interfcrcd with occupa- Thc perccntagc of social phobics who also meet thc criteriá for avoidant
tional performance, and that 69% wcre unable to attend common social pcrsonality disorder has ranged from 20% to 90% (Heimberg, 1989), with
events. Some social phobics describe years of extensivo and unncccssary thc highcst rates cvidcnt for generalized social phobics. This markcd overlap
medical cxaminations to find thc cause of symptoms associatcd with thc blurs thc diagnostic distinctivcncss of thcsc two disorders and raises qucs-
disorder, such as exccssivc sweating or difficultics in urinating (Craskc, tions of whethcr avoidant pcrsonality disordcr simply rcprcscnts a more
Barlow, ók O'Lcary, 1993). extreme versión of generalized social phobia.
¡74 ANXIETY DISORDERS CiignitivC'Uehavimal Trcalmení vf Social Phobia and CAO 175

Cognitivc-Behavioral Models of the Disordcr cause thc individual is not focusing on appropriatc social cues, this may
in turn lead to dysfunctional orsuboptimal social performance. Lastly, thc
Early bchavioral treatmcnts for social phobia wcre groundcd in thc bclief social phobic adopts avoidance as thc primary method of coping with fu-
that the disordcr reflcctcd a bchavioral déficit that could be rcctificd with turc social situations, and maintains his or hcr negativc expectancies with
social skills training. Lacking social skills, paticnts wcre prcsumed to find rcgard to thcsc situations.
intcrpersonal intcractions more avcrsive; this was bclievcd to prompt For thc socially functional person, this pattern is quite diffcrcnt. So-
avoidance, which would furthcr undcrminc thc acquisition of more cffcc- cial performance is considcrably less connccted to dysfunctional expec-
tive social repcrtoires. Training typically involvcd use of instruction, par- tancies and ncgativc affect in such individuáis. Instcad, thcy approach thcsc
ticipant modcling, corrcctivc fcedback, role plays, and thcrapist-assistcd situations with gencrally positivc expectancies, bascd on thcir past social
in vivo practicc. histories. Within thc social situation, thcy focus on appropriatc social cues
Research on thc effcctivcncss oí social skills training alone has sug- and are ablc to compénsate for ¡ncrcascd autonomic arousal with increascd
gcstcd only modcst outcomc. Somc studies havc found no diffcrcnccs bc- attentional focus on such cues. This aids thcm in social performance and
twccn social skills training and attcntion placebo controls (Marzillier, provides thcm with gcnerally positivo fcedback. Thc result is an adcquatc
Lambert, &. Kellett, 1976), and havc found that social skills training and nonavcrsivc social performance, which encourages thcsc individuáis
does not gencralize well to in vivo performance tasks (Falloon, Lloyd, Si to approach futurc social intcractions (Hcimbcrg &. Barlow, 1991).
Harpin, 1981). Othcrs havc had more promising results, in which pa- Thc markcd contrast bctween these two pattcrns of social bchavior
ticnts rccciving social skills training cxhibit modest dccreascs in social cmphasizcs thc levcl of impairmcnt in many social phobics. Thcir pat-
anxicty, avoidance, and distrcss at posttreatmcnt (Lucock &. Salkovskis, tcrns of scvcre sclf-evaluation and overwhclming anxicty créate a sclf-
1988; Stravynski, Marks, 6k Yule, 1982). In general, conceptualizations fulfilling prophecy of poor performance, anxicty, and avoidancc. Givcn
of social phobia as a skills déficit problcm may fail to incorpórate cogni- thcse sorts of ncgativc cognitivc biascs, it is easy to sec why dysthymia
tivc and affective aspccts of thc disordcr that may be important for and dcprcssion may wcll co-occur with social phobia. Comorbid alcohol
treatmcnt. problcms are also not uncommon. Not surprisingly, social phobics may
Within thc past 5-10 ycars, cognitivc-bchavioral modcls of social pho- at times turn to alcohol as an apparcnt attcmpt to sclf-mcdicatc thcir anxicty
bia have bccome incrcasingly multidimcnsional, and thc treatmcnts as- by decrcasing thcir own social awarcncss and self-cvaluation (Kushncr, Shcr,
sociated with thcm havc accordingly bccome more comprchcnsivc. &. Bcitman, 1990; Otto et al., 1992). Unfortunatcly, thc cffects of alco-
Hcimbcrg and Barlow (1991; Barlow, 1988) havc proposcd a modcl of so- hol are short-livcd and, bccausc of state-depcndcnt Icarning, do not gcner-
cial dysfunction in social phobia that focuscs on thc intcraction of ncga- alizc wcll bcyond a nonintoxicatcd statc.
tive affect, expectancies, and cognitions in thc maintcnance of the disorder.
This model purports that an individual with social phobia cntcrs social
Cognitivc-Behavioral Treatmcnt
situations with a real or perccivcd history of ncgativc social interactions,
which can contributc to ncgativc affect and to expectancies of failurc or Thc general goal of CBT for social phobia is to hclp patients brcak out
embarrassmcnt. Ncgativc expectancies may occur in the form of perccivcd of thcir sclf-dcfcating pattcrns and bchavc more adaptivcly in social situa-
lack of control (c.g., "1 am going to be unablc to stop mysclf from sweat- tions. Consistcnt with the modcl dcscribcd abovc, treatmcnt focuscs on
ing and shaking") or in more general forms (c.g., "I am going to cmbarrass hclping patients elimínate thcir anxiogcnic cognitions and rcdircct thcir
myself," "Shc will notice that I am sweating and think that thcrc is somc- attcntion to more rclcvant cues. Avoidancc is also targctcd, bccause of
thing wrong with me"). Thcsc prcdictions of ncgativc outcomc are likcly its role in dcnying paticnts thc opportunity to challenge inaladaptivc anxio-
to rcsult in hcightcncd autonomic arousal in social situations. As thc in- gcnic thoughts and rcccivc corrcctivc fcedback. Group treatmcnt may be
dividual cntcrs thc social situation, his or hcr attentional focus thcn shifts particularly cffcctivc bccausc of thc built-in cxposurc that it providcs and
away from appropriatc social cues and toward the adverse conscqucnces thc opportunitics for obscrvational Icarning and contact with individuáis
of poor social performance or othcr distracting issucs. Rathcr than focus- having similar conccrns and goals. Programmed simulations of thc cvents
ing on thc social cues in thc situation and thc ongoing social intcractions, that provokc social phobia are more acccssiblc within a group format. Whcn
the pcrson focuses on anxicty symptoms (c.g., thc levcl of hcart ratc, thc thcse general stratcgics are used, thc fcar and avoidancc cyclc can be brokcn,
level of sweating, thc trcmbling of thc hands) orón cognitivc cues of per- and thc paticnt can dcvclop more functional cognitivc and bchavioral
ccivcd failure (e.g., "I can fccl my face turning red," "I will ncvcr gct anothcr responsos to social situations.
date in my life"). This shift in turn Icads to grcater mercases in autonomic One of thc more comprchcnsivc and cmpirically bascd forms of CBT
arousal, thcreby raising anxiety and motivating avoidancc or escape. Bc- for social phobia was dcvcloped by Hcimberg and his collcagucs (Hcim-
176 ANX/ETV DÍSORDERS Cngnitive-Deliaviinal Tr«miirni n( Social fhobia and CAO 177

berg, 1989; Hcimbcrg &. Barlow, 1988; Hcimbcrg ct al.. 1990a). This dencc and competcnce in a widcr varicty of social situations and hclping
12-week cognitivc-bcbavioral group trcatmcnt (CBGT) has (bur basic com- them change the cognitive biascs that led to anxiety in the first place.
ponents. First, paticnts are educatcd about social phobia and presentcd Homework assignments rcquirc that patients practicc thcir newly acquired
with a model (dcscribed abovc) of hovv thcir problcm is inaintaincd. The skills betwccn sessions and report back to the group on their succcsses and
interaction among cognitive, physiologic, and behavioral components is difficultics. Group sizc is typically ftvc to scven paticnts, and sessions last
describcd, as well as how these components escálate during a problem- for 2'/2 hours to insure that cach patient will expcrience several exposure
matic social situation. Particular cmphasis is placed on the notion that simulations by the end of trcatment (Heimberg &. Barlow, 1991).
social phobia is a learncd response, and as such can be unlcarned through
behavioral cxposure and cognitive restructtiring. Group mcmbers are asked
Trcatmcnt Outcome Findings
to share examples of how social phobia has influenccd thcir livcs, and to
develop a hierarchy of aversive social situations that will be used for later
Outcomc for CBGT and Ot/ier Forms o/ CBT
exposure simulations. For many paticnts, adjustmcnt to the group sctting
is the first aspcct of exposurc trcatment, and opcn discussion of thcir anxicty Studies asscssing the cffcctivcness of CBGT havc reponed favorable rcsults.
about participating in the group is cncouragccl by the group leader. In a comparison of CBGT to an educational-supportive psychotherapy
In weeks 2 and 3, patients are taught the fundamentáis of cognitive program, improvcmcnts wcre found for both groups, but the paticnts receiv-
rcstructuring that will be used in the remaining treatmcnt scssions. Pa- ing CBGT werc significantly more improved than the trcatment control
tients are instructcd on how to rccognizc distortions in their thinking during at both posttreatment and follow-up (Heimberg ct al., 1990b). Seventy-
social situations and social performance (e.g., fortunc-tclling errors, mind five pcrcent of patients unclcrgoing CBGT mct critcria for clinical im-
rcading, all-or-nothing thinking), and to develop cognitive rcsponscs to provcmcnt bascd on a phobia scverity rating scale; by the education-
thcir thoughts that crcdibly challenge thcsc errors. Hypothcsis-tcsting procc- al-supportive program resultcd in a 40% improvemcnt rate. Clinical benefit
dures are used to encourage paticnts to come up with more accuratc and was inaintaincd and, in fact, incrcascd over the 6-month follow-up pcri-
honcst appraisals of thcir performance and public perccption. Thesc tech- od: 81% of CBGT-treatcd patients met the clinical critcria for improve-
niques are designcd to help paticnts idcntify and changc their cxpecta- ment at the follow-up assessinent. Consistcnt with a cognitive-bchavioral
tions of ncgativc social outcomcs and thcir rclatively scvcrc cvaluations formulation of the disordcr, dccreases in phobic severity for social situa-
of themselves in social situations. Paticnts rehearsc idcntifying thcsc er- tions \vcrc associatcd with the dccreases in ncgativc self-evaluations.
rors and creating countering thoughts in session; thcy are also askcd to A number of other studies have examined the effcctivencss of othcr
monitor their thoughts during social interactions in the coursc of the sub- cognitive and behavioral techniques, used cither alone or ¡n combination.
scquent wcck. Studies cmploying social skills training have found that thcsc proccdures
The remaining ninc scssions of CBGT consist of complcting cxposure are associated with improvemcnt in paticnts who are defined as both so-
simulations of personally rclcvant situations and assignmcnts of ¡n vivo cx- cially phobic and socially ¡nadequatc, but they may be more cfficacious
posure homework. Exposurc simulations rcquire that paticnts rolc-play in- for unskilled than for overanxious paticnts (Heimberg & Barlow, 1991;
crcasingly difficult fcared situations in the group setting, using other group Óst, Jerrcmalm, & Johansson, 1981). Interventions employing relaxation
members to try to make these situations as truc to lifc as possible. Thesc training techniques havc obtaincd mixcd results. Alstrom, Nordlund, Pcrs-
exposures are designcd to provide paticnts with opportunitics to expcriencc son, Harding, and Ljungqvist (1984) comparad relaxation training to ex-
arousal ¡n social situations, to learn to rcspond diffcrcntly to this arousal, posurc, supportive thcrapy, and a control condition. The authors reported
and to leam that their anxicty will decrcasc with practicc. Patients also that relaxation training was incffectivc, doing more poorly than the cx-
have the opportunity to challenge cognitive distortions in these situations posure and supportive therapy conclitions, and no bctter than the control
and to practice adaptive coping statcments. The group sctting provides condition. Studies using applied relaxation have farcd bettcr. Óst ct al.
an audience and role players for these intcrventions, which may include (1981) compared applied rclaxation to standard progrcssivc musclc relax-
simulations of convcrsations, giving a specch, or eating a messy food. With ation and found that subjccts rccciving applicd relaxation were signifi-
succcssful trcatment, paticnts learn of the scverity of thcir cognitivc dis- cantly more ¡mprovcd on most mcasurcs than the group receiving
tortions about fearcd situations, develop skills for identifying and corrcct- progrcssivc musclc rclaxation.
ing thesc distortions, and gain cxpcricnce at pcrfonning in such situations Studies comparing the cffcctivcness of bchaviorally bascd cxposure
(with particular practice in corrccting and identifying distortions bcfore, trcatmcnts to CBT havc failcd to demónstrate consistcnt superiority of
during, and after these programmed exposurc simulations). As the group the latter. Butler, Cullington, Munby, Amies, and Geldcr (1984) com-
continúes, exposure practice is expandcd, hclping paticnts develop confi- pared exposure thcrapy to exposure therapy plus anxiety managemcnt train-
¡78 ANXIETY DISORDERS Cogiiiiivc-Dc/uivicmiI Trcmmcm o/ Social Phabia and GAO 179

ing and to a waiting-list control group. At posttreatmcnt, both treatment yicld standardizad scorcs (cífect sizcs) that can be uscd as a common met-
groups werc significantly differcnt from thc waiting-list control on meas- ric across scvcral treatment outcome studies using differcnt treatment out-
urcs of phobic severity, anticipatory anxicty, social avoidance and distress, come mcasures. To provide a summary of thc litcraturc to date, we reccntly
and dcpression. Emmclkamp, Mcrsch, Vissia, and van dcr Hclm (1985) conductcd a mcta-analysis of 24 studies that uscd psychosocial or phar-
assigncd subjccts randomly to in vivo cxposurc, rational-cmotivc thcrapy, macologic intcrvcntions for social phobia (Gould, Buckminstcr, Pollack,
and self-instruction training in which subjccts practiccd gcncrating adap- Otto, & Yap, 1994). All studies cmployed treatment control conditions,
tive cognitive rcsponscs. Rcsults indicatcd that all thrcc trcatmcnts wcrc and bctwccn-group effcct sizcs wcrc calculated rclativc to thcsc control
cqually cffectivc at rcducing social fcar and distrcss. Mattick, Pctcrs, and conditions. In addition, ¡n ordcr to reduce mcasurcmcnt bias, effect sizes
Clarkc (1989) found that paticnts receiving cxposurc alone pcrformcd bcttcr wcrc only dcrivcd from sclf-rcport qucstionnaircs. Thrcc cffcct siics wcrc
on mcasures of bchavioral approach than did paticnts receiving cognitive calculated for cach social phobia study to capture thrcc symptom dimcn-
rcstructuring alone, but worsc on mcasures of attitudinal changc. Othcr sions of social phobia: social anxicty or avoidance, cognitive changc, and
studies havc also indicatcd that cxposurc trcatmcnts yicld rcductions in dcpression. Results of this mcta-analysis are presentcd in Table 7.1.
social phobics' anxiety and avoidance (Biran, Augusto, Si Wilson, 1981; Ten studies provided data on thc efficacy of pharmacothcrapy rela-
Mattick ¿k Pcters, 1988). tivc to pill placebo (n = 8 studies), pill placebo plus cxposure instructions
(n = 1 study), or a no-treatmcnt control group (n = 1 study). Thc mean
CBT venus Pharmacologic /ntervcntions effect size for social anxiety was 0.62 and for depression was 0.83. Mcas-
ures of cognitive changc wcrc asscsscd in only onc study. Comparisons
Few comparisons betwccn CBT and pharmacologic intcrvcntions are avail- of thc social anxiety and dcpression effect sizes to the nuil hypothesis (cf-
able. Gclernter ct al. (1991) comparcd a group form of CBT (not Heim- fcct sizc = 0.0) werc statistically significant. In addition, eliminating thc
bcrg et al.'s CBGT) to pharmacothcrapy (with alprazolam, phcnclzinc, or two studies that did not use puré pill placebo control groups did littlc to
a pill placebo) plus instructions for cxposurc to phobic stimuli. All of the changc thc ovcrall cffcct sizcs (0.52 for social anxicty; 0.88 for dcprcssion).
trcatments wcrc associatcd with significant improvcmcnt (mcasurcd by Thc mean dropout ratc for thcse studies was 14.9%.
changcs in sclf-rcport instruments and physician rating scalcs), and no one Monoaminc oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs) in this mcta-analysis includ-
treatment was consistcntly superior to thc othcrs. Intcrprctations of these cd phcnclzinc (n •= 4 studies) and moclobcmidc (n = 1 study), and thcir
findings are limitcd, howcvcr, bccausc paticnts in all of thc drug treat- mean social anxicty effect size was 0.64 with a dropout rate of 14%. Two
ment conditions were instructcd to complete sclf-dircctcd cxposurc to fearíul controllcd studies assesscd the efficacy of high potcncy bcniodiazcpines
social situations. This instrucción hclpcd to ¡nsurc that cxposurc intcr- (BZDs; alprazolam and clonazcpam); thcir mean cffcct sizc was 0.72, and
vcntions wcrc common to all treatment conditions, thus raising thc pos- thcir dropout ratc was 12%. Promising results wcrc obtaincd from SSRls
sibility that cxposure was thc active treatment clemcnt for tliis study. In
anothcr study, cxposurc alone, in thc form of 20 scssions of imagina! and
m vivo flooding, was comparcd to thc drug atcnolol and to a drug placebo TADLE 7.1. Mcta-Analysis oí Acutc Trcatmcnt Outcome
condition (Turner, Beidel, & Jacob, 1994). Thcsc researchcrs found that for Social Phobia
flooding was consistently superior to placebo, whcreas atcnolol was not. Mean social anxicty
In addition, flooding was superior to atenolol on bchavioral and compo- Treatment intcrvcntion • cffcct sizc
sitc outcome measurcs. More reccntly, Hcimbcrg ct al. (1994) comparcd
Pharmacothcrapy vs. pill placebo
CBGT to phcnclzinc and to a pill placebo. At posttreatmcnt (12 wceks), MAOIs (5 studies) 0.64
both CBGT and phcnelzinc were superior to placebo, but not differcnt Beiuodiazcpincs (2 studies) 0.72
from each other. Phenelzine had a faster onset of thcrapcutic cffcct than SSIUs (2 studies) 1.89
CBGT by weck 6, but this diffcrcncc was climinatcd by thc cnd of Bcta-adrcncrgic blockcrs (3 studies) -0.09
treatment. Duspironc (I study) -0.50

CBT vs. controls


Meia-Analyííc Fmdings Cognitive rcstructuring alone (3 studies) 0.60
Exposurc tcchniqucs alone (6 studies) 0.89
Mcta-analysis is a statistical tcchniquc that allows comparison of treat- Exposurc plus cognitive rcstructuring (8 studies) 0.80
ment results in tcrms of quantifiablc units (callcd "cffcct sizcs") bascd on Note. Thc numbcr oí studies proviiling outcome comparisons is notcd íor each Ircal-
thc normal curve. Thc advantíigc of mcta-analytic procedures is that thcy mcnt usted, h'torn Gould, Ouo. Yap. and Pollack (1991).
180 ANX1ETV DISORDERS Cognitive-Behavioral Treaimcni of Social Phobia and CAO 181

including fluvoxaminc (n = I study) (effcct size = 2.73; dropout ratc = Although subjects generally maintaincd thcir trcatment gains at follow-
3%) and scrtraline (n - 1 study) (effect size - 1.05; dropout rate = 0%). up, 44% of thcm sought and obtaincd additional trcatment for their so-
The beta-adrenergic blocker atcnolol (n = 3 studics) (effect sizc = 0.09) cial phobia during this time pcriod.
and buspirone (n = I study) (cffcct size = - 0.50) did not appcar to be Wlazlo, Schroedcr-Hartwig, Hand, Kaiser, and Munchau (1990) com-
effcctive for social phobia bccausc thcy did worse than a pill placebo con- pared a social skills training program to individual and group-administered
dition. in vivo exposure trcatmcnts. Results suggcsted no significant differcnces
Studies of cognitive-bchavioral thcrapy utilizcd cognitivc restructur- betwccn thc two conditions and general improvcment for thc majority
ing, situational exposure, social skills training, systematic dcsensitization, of patients. Long-term asscssmcnts were conductcd betwecn 1 and 5.5 ycars
flooding, and anxicty management tcchniqucs. A total of 16 studics uscd (mean = 2.5 ycars), and the authors reported additional improvements
cognitive-behavioral intcrventions without pharmacotherapy, and their on several measures for both groups at follow-up. It should be noted,
mean social anxiety effect size was 0.71, the mean cogntive change effect howcver, that methodologic problcms cxistcd in thisstudy: Paticnts were
size was 0.74, and thc mean dcprcssion effect sizc was 0.67. Thc mean not randomly assigncd to groups, and patients in thc social skills training
dropout ratc for CBT intervcntions ¡n thesc studics was 11.5%. In gener- condition werc also taught exposure tcchniqucs.
al, therc werc no significant diffcrcnccs bctwccn typcs of CBT intcrven- Heimbcrg ct al. (1990a) compared a crediblc placebo (an educa-
tions, and all were superior to the nuil hypothcsis (cffcct size = 0.0). Studies tion-support group) to CBGT and found that 75% of CBGT patients met
using cognitive restructuring alone (effect size = 0.60) did not differ sig- thc criteria for clinical improvemcnt at posttrcatment, comparcd to 40%
nificantly in cfficacy from studics using exposure techniqucs alone (effect of the placebo group. Data from this study represent the longest mean
size - 0.89) or the combination of exposure plus cognitivc restructuring follow-up pcriod (mean = 5.5 ycars; range - 4.5 to 6.25 ycars) and werc
(effect size = 0.80), ñor did thcy differ ¡n terms of dropout ratcs. dcrivcd from 19 of the 40 original subjects (Heimberg, Salzman, Holt, &
Comparisons of studics utilizing cognitive-behavioral trcatmcnts (ef- Blcndell, 1993). The authors indicatcd that their follow-up subsamplc may
fect size = 0.71) to those using pharmacotherapy (cffcct size - 0.62) were havc becn less itnpaired beforc trcatment than nonparticipating subjects,
not statistically significant. In addition, mean attrition ratcs werc not sig- but also notcd that CBGT and thc placebo trcatment were equivalent
nificantly differcnt among subjects receiving a cognitive-behavioral inter- in terms of participant-nonparticipant diffcrcnccs. At follow-up, 89% of
vention (11.5%) and those receiving a medicación intcrvcntion (14.9%). CBGT patients werc judgcd to be clinically improved by independent as-
CBT intcrventions may also cnjoy a small advantage ¡n tcrms of thcse scssors, comparcd to 44% of the cducation-support group mcmbcrs. More
comparisons due to thcir use of wait-list controls, which tend to be wcak- rccently, Heimberg et al. (1994) reported that although CBGT and phencl-
cr control conditions rclativc to pill placebo conditions. zine werc equivalent and superior to a placebo at posttrcatmcnt, at 6-month
Results from our mcta-analysis are similar to findings by Chamblcss follow-up thc CBGT subjects had maintaincd thcir improvcment, whereas
and Gillis (1993). Thesc authors derived cffcct sizes bascd on within-group onc-half of phcnelzinc paticnts had rclapsed. In summary, results from these
change from prc- to posttrcatmcnt in 10 studies of CBT for social phobia. studics suggcst that thc bcncfit from CBT generally endures ovcr time.
Thcy found large effect sizcs for CBT at thc end of trcatment. In addi-
tion, thcir rcview of controlled rcscarch studics indicatcd that CBT-treatcd
Trcatment Rcsistancc/Maximizing Trcatment Outcome
paticnts did significantly bcttcr on most measurcs than did thcir control
cohorts. For measurcs of social phobia, thc prc-to-post cffcct sizc was 0.68; From thc pcrspcctivc of clinical rescarch, rclativcly littlc is known about
for measurcs of fcar of ncgativc cvaluation, thc cffcct sizc was 0.70. what kinds of patients tcnd to be rcsistant to standard forms of CBT for
social phobia. There is somc cvidencc that paticnts with discrcte social
phobia tcnd to bcnefit more from CBGT than do those with gcneralized
Long-Tcrm Residís social phobia (Heimbcrg, 1989). It is also evidcnt that paticnts who havc
Only four studics using cognitivc and bchavioral tcchniques have includ- rclatively more skills déficits will bcncfit more from an approach thac em-
cd data with follow-up periods of 1 ycar or longer. Fava, Grandi, and Canes- phasizcs skills than from an approach that emphasizcs anxiety reducción
trari (1989), in an uncontrolled trial of the effccts of exposure trcatment (for a rcview, sce Heimberg, 1989). Henee, packagc trcatment approaches
alone for 10 social phobia patients, reportccl that 7 of the 10 hacl main- that are able to stress cithcr anxicty rcductíon, skills acquisition, or a com-
taincd thcir posttrcatment gains at a 1-ycar follow-up. Mcrsch, Emmclkamp, bination of thesc intcrvcntions may offer the bcst bcncfit to a hetcrogencous
and Lips (1991) uscd a 14-month follow-up on a study that originally com- sample of patients.
pared social skills training with rational-emotive thcrapy. At posttrcat- The ovcrlap between social phobia and avoidant personality disorder
ment, both groups showed improvcment on a numbcr of outcomc measurcs. suggests that paticnts with thc gcncralized subtype of social phobia may
182 ANXÍETV DJSORDERS Tri'flimcw uf Social i'iwbkt atol GAD 183

have prominent and fixed pattems of avoidance ¡n social situations, and CBT is a powerful intcrvcntion, producing rcsults that rival thc outcomcs
henee that treatment may nccessarily rcquire a longcr course of interven- obtaincd with antideprcssant intervcntions. CBT intcrvcntions havc bor-
ción. In addition, ncarly one-tliird to onc-half of some samplcs of patients rowcd thc same principies as thosc identificd for treating othcr anxicty
with social phobia havc bccn found to havc comorbid panic disordcr disordcrs; thcse includc idcntifying bchavioral cxcesscs and déficits, and
(Barlow, 1988; Roscnbaum &. Pollack, 1994). Often this comorbidity providing corrcctivc skills training and cxposure cxpericnccs to elimínate
manifests itself in the form of situationally bound panic attacks, in which pattems that maihtain the anxiety disordcr. It appears that cognitive rc-
a panic attack bccomcs incrcasingly common under spccific social condi- structuring and cxposure intcrvcntions providc a powerful combination
tions and then generalizes to othcr social situations. As such, patients ex- of interventions for the treatment of social phobia. For a subsamplc of
periencc anticipatory anxicty not only with rcgard to fcars of humiliation, paticnts who are poorly skillcd ¡n social situations, addítional training ¡n
but also with regard to thc expcricnce of uncontrollablc panic symptoms. social skills may providc furthcr bcncfits. As with othcr anxicty disorders,
Because standard CBGT focuscs the most attcntion on cognitivc rcstruc- comorbidity is likcly to rcquire additional and broadcr treatment intcr-
turing, pacicnts with comorbidity may bencfit from thc addition of tcch- vcntions that includc componcnts of treatments found to be effcctive for
niqucs found to be uscful for panic disordcr, particularly intcroccptivc othcr Axis I disordcrs (c.g., panic disordcr or deprcssion).
exposure techniques. Intcroceptive cxposure—that is, cxposing patients
to thc somatic sensations of anxiety (scc Otto &. Gould, Chaptcr 5, this
volumc)—offers thc therapist thc abtlity to includc in cxposure assignments GENERALIZED ANXIETY DISORDER
not just the extcnsive cues of a social situation, but also rclcvant intcrnal
scnsations of arousal. Treatment can thus focus on dccreasing anxiogenic General Considcrations
rcsponses to both internal and cxternal cues. The addition of othcr symp-
tom managcmcnt skills and cognitivc rcstructuring for panic attacks may Although GAD has bccn dcscribcd as thc "basic" anxicty disordcr, it has
also be of use for thcse patients. rcccivcd only minimal rcscarcb attcntion. This phcnomcnon may havc
Comorbid dcpression has bccn identificd as a ncgativc predictor of stemmcd originally from thc lack of wcll-dcfincd diagnostic critcria for
treatment outcomc (Barlow, 1994). Brown and Barlow (1992) rcportcd that GAD and from its origins as a residual diagnostic catcgory. With thc ad-
35% of patients with social phobia had expcricnced at least onc cpisodc vcnt of DSM-1I1 (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), panic disord-
of major dcpression. Stcin, Tanccr, Gclcrtner, Vittonc, and Uhdc (1990) cr was scparatcd from thc anxiety ncuroses, and GAD was dcfincd as a
rcported that 20% of patients with social phobia prescntcd with an addi- - discrctc diagnostic catcgory. Identification of GAD as a sepárate disordcr
tional mood disordcr (eithcr major dcpression or dysthymia). Comorbid has cncouragcd systcmatic collcction of data with rcgard to thc phcnomc-
dysthymic disorder has bccn found in 15% of social phobics (Van Amcrin- nology and treatment of this condition.
gen, Mancini, Styan, &. Donison, 1991). In more than 80% of cases of GAD is conccptualizcd as a disordcr of cxccssive worry that manifests
social phobia with comorbid deprcssion, thc depressivc symptoms dcvclop itself in cognitivc, physiologic, and behavioral componcnts. Thc cogni-
aftcr thc onset of social fcar and avoidance (Brooks, Baltazar, &. Mun- tivc and physiologic componcnts providc thc core diagnostic critcria for
jack, 1989; Turncr, Bcidcl, Borden, Stanley, &. Jacob, 1991). Because thc disordcr. As dcfincd in DSM-1V, the cognitivc component includcs
comorbid deprcssion tcnds to incrcase thc scvcrity of fcars of negativo evalu- unrcalistic or cxccssive worry about scvcral lifc circumstanccs occurring
atton (Ball et al., 1995; Bruch, Mattia, Hcimbcrg, & Holt, 1993), thc ad- more oftcn than not for at least 6 months (American Psychiatric Associa-
dition of supplcmcntal cognitivc rcstructuring and activity assignmcnts tion, 1994). For examplc, a paticnt may worry about moncy whcn his or
may be nccessary for thcse paticnts. In particular, wc havc noticcd in oúr hcr finances are sound; a mothcr may agonize ovcr thc hcalth of a robust
clinical practice that patients with comorbid depression easily becomc child; or a husband may worry about low-probability catastrophic cvcnts
prcoccupicd with global depressivc thoughts about thcmsclvcs and their (c.g., "My wifc's plañe will crash"). Individuáis with GAD often fccl un-
performance. For patients with such comorbidity, wc routincly apply ad- ablc to control this anxious apprchcnsion and it causes significant distrcss
ditional cognitive restructuring skills, as wcll as ¡nformational intcrvcn- or iinpainncnt in social or occupational functioning.
tions to caution thcm about cognitivc biascs that emerge from dcpression In accordancc with thcse apprchensivc thoughts, paticnts with GAD
and to help them devclop ways of rcstructuring thesc biases. Howcvcr, suffcr from physiologic symptoms of anxicty. Thcsc symptoms includc inus-
whenevcr dcpression dominates thc clinical picturc, we rcfer paticnts for clc tensión, ¡ncrcascd irritability, difficulty conccntrating, and incrcascd
CBT for dcpression prior to rcferral for social phobia treatment. vigilancc (c.g., difficulty slceping, fccling rcstlcss or kcycd up). Although
In summary, social phobia treatment is just now rccciving the rescarch some of thcse symptoms ovcrlap with thosc of panic disorder, thcy have
attention that it descrvcs. Treatment studics conductcd to date affirm that a diffcrcnt profilc: Thcy tcnd to be associated with worry thoughts and
184 ANX/ETV DISORDERS Cogiúlive-Bclutvioral Treatmcnt of Social Phobía and GAD 185

are much less likely to occur spontaneously. In addition, GAD patients that they prescnt for treatment (Blazer, Hughes, & George, 1987). The
without panic disorder are less likcly to be fcarful ofthe physical symptoins percentage of patients diagnosed with GAD who report having rcceivcd
themselves, despitc finding thcm uncomfortablc. Patients with GAD tend previous psychologic or psychiatric treatment varíes widcly; it ranges from
to report anxiety during protractcd pcriods cach day, and are also more 29% (Power, Jcrrom, Simpson, &. Swanson, 1989) to 94% (Durham ók
likcly to expcricnce anxiety on more days ofthe week than panic sufferers. Turvey, 1987).
Barlow (1988) found that patients with GAD suffercd from anxicty 56%
oí thc time, which was considcrably more than for patients with panic
disorder (16%). In addition, the majority of individuáis with GAD are symp- Etiology and Naturc of the Disordcr
tomatic from the time that they acquire the disorder, with only 25% report-
ing episodcs of rcmittancc of 3 months or longer (Noyes, Clarkson, Crowe, As in the other anxicty disorders, a combination of genetic, personality
Yates, & McChcsney, 1987). trait, and life event factors secms to be implicated in the génesis of GAD.
Although such behaviors are not part of thc formal cliagnostic critcr- Howcvcr, idcntification ofthe truc ctiologic mcchanisms of this disorder
ia, individuáis with GAD tcncl to cngagc in behaviors that rcinforce and is hampercd by thc abscnce of prospectivo studics cxamining its onset.
perpetúate theiranxious worry (Craskc et al., 1993). Behavioral manifesta- Barlow and his colleagucs havc dcscribed a numbcr of characteristics in
tions of worry often include checking, avoidance, and vigilancc. Specific their GAD paticnts that they believc predisposc paticnts to devcloping
examples ¡ncludc rcpcatcdly calling thc family physician about a child's thc disorder as well as other anxiety disorders (Barlow, 1988). First, thesc
runny nosc; staying at homc when it is raining lightly for fcar of a car paticnts are charactcrizcd by highcr levéis of diffusc arousal and sensitiv-
accidcnt; and instructing children who are outside playing to come in the ity than are nonanxious normáis. Second, they possess a cognitive bias
house cvery 15 minutes in ordcr for the anxious parent to check on their to vicw the world as a dangerous place, and this view is often precipitated
safety. Functionally, such behaviors providc immcdiate rclief of anxicty. by past life expcrienccs. Other researchcrs have confirmcd that a history
In the long term, however, they do not allovv thc normal habituation of of negativo life experiences may predict later dcvelopment of GAD (Nisita
anxiety to occur. In this way, worry bchaviors and worry thoughts rein- ct al., 1990). Torgerson (1986) found that GAD paticnts werc more likely
force cach other and can cvolvc into a chronic pattcrn. than panic disordcr subjects to havc cxpcrienced the death of a parent
GAD is one of the more common anxicty disorders, with prevalcnce prior to the age of 16. Third, they are more likely to havc hcightcned
rates varying from 1.6% to 4.0% (Wittchen, Zhao, Kcssler, & Eaton, 1994; bclicfs about rcsponsibility, control, and pcrfcctionism. However, it is not
Weissman & Mcrikangas, 1986). Estimates of prevalcnce for individuáis clcar whether these personality charactcristics are causes or conscquences
who cxpericnce cxccssivc worry that does not mect formal Axis I criteria of GAD, bccausc of the rctrospective bias of this finding.
may be as high as 10% (Shephcrd, Coopcr, &. Brown, 1966). Findings Other researchcrs have also lookcd at genetic mediators of GAD. They
rcgarding gcndcr differcnces havc becn inconsistcnt, although the most report cvidence that the frcquency of GAD ¡s higher in relatives of GAD
rccent National Comorbidity Survcy found that thc disorder was twicc probands than it is in relativos of control or panic disordcr probands, and
as common in womcn than in mcn (Wittchen ct al., 1994). Age of onsct the frcquency of panic disorder is highcr in relatives of panic disordcr pro-
is typically ¡n thc late tccns or early 20s (Andcrson, Noyes, & Crowe, bands than it is in relativos of control or GAD probands (Crowc, Noyes,
1984; Rickels & Schweizer, 1990; Rapce, 1991); howevcr, paticnts tcnd Pauls, &. Slytnen, 1983¡ Noyes ct al.,.1987). However, relatives of panic
to seek initial treatment later than patients with panic disorder (Nisita disorder, agoraphobia, and control probands wcrc found to be equally at
et al., 1990). This delay may be attributablc to a paucity of adcquate diag- risk for GAD (Noyes ct al., 1986). It may be that individuáis with GAD
nostic and referral proccdurcs at the level of primary care; poor diagnostic and panic disorder inhcrit a diathcsis to develop anxiety disorders in general.
and referral proccdurcs at this levcl; or a bclicf on thc part ofthe patients How GAD is maintaincd after its onsct has bccn thc subject of some
that thcir anxiety problcm ¡s a fixccl trait that is not treatable. thcorctical debate. A numbcr of authors havc lookcd to cognitive factors
Relatively fcw paticnts with GAD are sccn in anxicty disorder clinics as thc core mechanism of action for this disordcr. Borkovec and his col-
(Dubovsky, 1990). The majority are scen by general practitioners, to whom leagucs havc hypothesizcd that worry is primarily characterized by verbal
they often ¡nitially report complaints of physical anxiety symptoms. thoughts rathcr than imagcs and is mcdiatcd by avoidance bf potcntially
Dubovsky (1990) estimatcd that only 25% of paticnts with GAD rcceivc distrcssing emotional imagcry (Borkovec &. Inz, 1990). They cite cvidence
specific treatments tailorcd to the disordcr. This pcrccntagc may be evcn that during rclaxation nonanxious controls showcd a prcdominance of im-
smaller ¡n more rural settings with fcwer mental hcalth profcssionals. Pa- agcry, whcrcas GAD subjects showcd cqual amounts of thought and im-
ticnts with GAD are more likcly to havc cxpcrienccd some rccent stress- agcry. After successful thcrapy, GAD subjects showed thought and image
ful life event (e.g., marriage, Job promotion, loss of parent) at the time frequencies that rcscmbled thosc of normáis. Accordingly, worry ¡s hypothe-
i 86 ANX/ETV DISORDERS Copiitivc'Bclvivioral Trcaimcm u/ Social Phobia and GAO 187

sizcd to allow thc GAD suffcrcr to escape distrcssing imagcry; bccausc of Cognitivc-Dchavioral Trcatmcnt
this lack of cxposure, thc anxicty associatcd with such imagcry ¡s ncvcr
allowcd to habitúate. Othcr authors havc qucstioncd this conceptual- Thcre are thrcc basic stratcgics for CBT of GAD: cognitivc rcstructuring
izacion and point out that othcr kinds of thinking are at Icast as cffcc- of anxiogcnic thoughts, rclaxation training, and worry cxposure assign-
tive as worry thinking in supprcssing cmotional imagcry (East &. Watts, mcnts, Each of these stratcgics attempts to target clcmcnts of thc worry
1994). cyclc that trigger and maintain thc disordcr. Cognitivc rcstructuring of
A numbcr of cognitivc biascs may triggcr tlic worry proccss. Thcsc anxiogcnic thoughts is at thc foundacion of trcatmcnt for GAD. Thc com-
may ¡ncludc patients' tcndcncy to overcstimatc thc likclihood of thc oc- poncnts of cognitivc rcstructuring are as follows: cducation about thc role
currcncc of ncgativc evcnts and thc ncgativc consequcnccs of thesc cvcnts, of cognitions in incrcasing anxicty; thc logical cxamination of thoughts;
and to undercstimacc thcir ability to cope with thcsc cvcnts (Darlow, 1988). probability cstimations; and thc use of bchavioral assignmcnts and monitor-
In addition, information-proccssing rcscarch suggcsts biascs in thc way that ing Co challenge thc vcracity of anxicty-relatcd bclicfs. Approximatcly onc-
GAD pacicncs attcnd to information in chcir environmcnts. Butlcr and third of thc paticnts prcscnting with GAD to our clinic do not under-
Mathcws (1987) found that individuáis with GAD wcrc more likely to stand thc relationship bctwccn cognitivc and cmotional proccsscs. Thcy
make thrcatcning intcrprctations of ambiguous materials than wcrc nor- do not undcrstand that thcir anxious mood statcs are oftcn triggcrcd by
mal controls, and more likely to ratc thc probability that dangcrous cvents cognitions, and that this relationship is rcciprocal (i.e., how thcy fcel can
would occur as high. Mathcws and McLcod (1985, 1986) uscd modificd also affcct what thcy think). Educación about thcsc proccsscs is a first stcp
Stroop and dichotic listcning tasks to study thcsc information-proccssing for such paticnts.
biascs, and confirmcd that paticnts with GAD allocatc proportionally grcat- The core clcmcnts of cognitivc rcstructuring focus on tcaching pa-
cr rcsourccs for dctccting thrcatcning material than normal controls and ticnts a systcmatic inethod for cxamining thc vcracity of thcir thinking
can dctcct this material more rapidly. and cstimating thc probability of ncgacivc cvcnts. Paticnts are first instruct-
Individuáis with GAD may also havc a tcndcncy to rnisintcrprct com- cd lo use thought records to writc down thcir anxiogcnic thoughts, in ordcr
mon ncgativc cvcnts that are part of thc human condition (e.g., occasional to examine thcsc thoughts more objcctivcly. Paticnts are ncxt instructcd
accidcnts, thc dcath of an cldcrly relativo) as evidcncc that thc world is in uncovcring cognitivc distortions in thcir thinking (c.g., catastrophiz-
a dangerous place and that thcir worrying is justificd. This world view is ¡ng, mind rcading, all-or-nothing thinking) and cxamining thc evidcncc
hypothcsizcd to contributc to hcightcncd vigilancc and to thc belicf that supporting and rcfuting anxiogcnic thoughts. Bccausc individuáis with
thcir worrying is adaptivc and prophylactic. In more scvcrc cases, GAD GAD are likcly to undercstimatc thcir ability Co cope in situations, and to
suffcrcrs may dcvelop a supcrstitious cognitivc schcma that thcy actually overcstimatc both thc potcntial for ncgativc cvcnts to occur and thc ncga-
can prcvcnt futurc ncgativc cvcnts by worrying (Dorkovcc, 1985). Thc worry tivc consequcnccs rclatcd to thesc evcnts, probability estimation tcchniqucs
proccss may be furthcr cxaccrbated by thc suffcrcrs' tcndcncy to shift from are taught to allow paticnts to mcasurc thc actual likclihood of thcsc evcnts
one worry thought or belicf to another vcry rapidly, without giving thcm- more accurately. Paticnts are also taught to genérate aitcrnativc cxplana-
sclvcs che chance to test thc vcracity of thesc thoughts. As such, thcy tions and rational rcsponscs that challenge distortcd anxiogcnic thoughts
bccomc so prcoccupicd with worry that thcy cannot focus thcir attcntion (c.g., "My son clid not cali me bccausc he lose crack of time" instcad of
on finding solutions to thcir problcms, or are unablc to try out potencial "My son did not cali me bccausc he muse havc had an accidcnt"). Finally,
solutions to thcir problcms (Craskc ct al., 1993). In othcr cases, paticnts paticnts are taught problcm-solving tcchniqucs so that thcy can be proac-
with GAD recognizc that thcy are worrying cxccssivcly, and this leacls tivc about gcncrating and implcmcnting solutions to thcir problcms.
thcm to rcsist worrying or to distract thcmselvcs from worrying. Paradoxi- A sccond basic CBT stratcgy is rclaxation training. Rcduction in au-
cally, chis straccgy may havc che oppositc cffcct, in thc way that trying tonomic arousal has bccn targctcd with a varicty of relaxation stratcgics,
Co rcsisc thinking about somcthing oftcn makcs it strongcr, ranging from progrcssivc musclc rclaxation to biofecdback-assisced procc-
In summary, although thc cxact mcchanism or mechanisms mediat- clurcs (Barlow ct al., 1984; Cragan Si Dcffcnbachcr, 1984; Townscnd,
ing GAD are still opcn to debate, thc componcnts of GAD that appcar Housc, & Addario, 1975). Progrcssivc muscic rclaxation is probably thc
to concribucc Co che cyclc of worry ¡ncludc (1) cmotional arousal and most commonly cmploycd stratcgy; it consists of having paticnts Icarn to
information-proccssing biascs to thrcatcning s t i m u l i ; (2) belicf that worry systematically rclax progrcssivcly largcr groups of musclcs until thcy can
prcvcncs future ncgativc cvcnts; (3) incffcctivc problcm solving; and (4) ultimatcly relax all thc musclcs at once by using a single cucd word likc
atccmpcs to rcsisc and discracc ncgativc imagcry without rcaching aitcrna- "rclax" or "calm." This type of rclaxation is oftcn initially taught in trcat-
tivc solutions. mcnt sessions and then assigncd as a homcwork accivity with a casscccc
M
188 ANX/ETY D1SOKDERS C»íii¡(iiv-Dc/iíivi«r«/ Trcfllmcní u¡ Sixr/al Plutbía and GAD 189

tape. Mcrcly giving patients a tape without dcmonstrating thesc techniques son, &. Gcldcr, 1991; Lindsay, Gamsu, McLaughlin, Hood, & Espié, 1987;
has been associatcd with less than optimal trcatment outcome (Borkovec Barlow, Rapcc, & Brown, 1992). Some studies havc employcd a technique
&. Sidcs, 1979). Paticnts are encouragcd to apply thcir relaxation skills called "anxiety management training" (Suinn ók Richardson, 1971), in
when they are engaged in worry and cxpcricncing physical symptoms of which anxiety symptoms are clicitcd in the thcrapy office and the paticnt
anxiety. Research has suggestcd that progressivc musclc rclaxation is lielpful etnploys applicd relaxation tcchniqucs to cope wirh thcm. Two studics
to patients with GAD, but it should be implcmented carly in trcatment, have found thcsc tcchniqucs to be superior to a no-treatmcnt control (Jan-
because it often takes 4-8 weeks for its bencfits to be rcalizcd by patients noun, Oppcnhcimcr, &. Geldcr, 1982; Tarricr &. Main, 1986).
(Craskc et al., 1993). Research comparing CBT techniques to medication intcrventions have
Many individuáis with GAD attempt to mentally block negativc or found gcncrally that CBT ¡s cquivalcnt or superior to mcdications. Lind-
catastrophic images and to distract themselves from such images. As a rcsult, say et al. (1987) found greatcr initial gains in patients rccciving the bcn-
thc anxiety associatcd with thcsc images ¡s never allowed to habitúate. zodiazcpinc lorazepam, but no differcnces between thcsc subjects and
Worry cxposure is a CDT procedure dcveloped specifically to countcract subjccts rcceiving CBT at 4 wccks. In addition, because of thc lack of
this problcm; it involvcs having patients focus on frightcning or catas- sustaincd ¡mpmvcmcnt among lorazcpam-trcatcd subjccts, over half of thcm
trophic imagcs (c.g., "I will lose my Job and be living on thc strcct") íor rcfuscd to wuit without trcatment until thc follow-up asscssmcnt. A study
discreto pcriods of time, usually bctwccn 25 and 50 minutes. Patients are comparing the effectivcncss of diazcpam to CBT intcrventions found that
instructed to try to make thc images as vivid and anxicty-provoking as CBT was superior at posttrcatment, and that subjects receiving this treat-
possiblc. Because of their habitual tcndcncy toward distraction and incnt wcrc less likely to havc uscd subscqucnt psychotropic medications
avotdance, patients may at first havc clifficulty staying focuscd on thc trn- or pursuccl othcr psychologic trcatmcnts at a 12-month follow-up (Power
ages; it is thcrcforc oftcn hclpful initially to complete exposurcs in a trcat- et al., 1989). Power and his collcagucs latcr replicatcd this finding and
ment session, and thcn later to assign them as homework. It may be hclpful also found that diazepam was no more cffective than placebo (Power, Simp-
for patients to makc "subjective units of distrcss" ratings of their anxiety son, Swanson, ck Wallacc, 1990).
at 5-minute intcrvals during the exposures, in order to have a more tangi- Studies dcmonstrating thc supcriority of CBT tcchniqucs to nondirec-
ble measure of whethcr their anxiety is dccreasing. Thcsc ratings are madc tivc treatment havc bccn mixcd. Blowers et al. (1987) found that an in-
on a scale of O to 100, with 100 rcflccting extreme anxiety and O rcflcct- tcrvcntion combining cognitivc therapy with relaxation training was not
ing no anxiety at al!. significantly bcttcr than nondircctive counseling. Similarly, Borkovec and
Somc patients may find that sclf-directcd trcatmcnts, such as bibliother- Mathews (1988) found no diffcrcnccs bctwccn thrcc trcatment groups that
apy, are helpful adjuncts to individual thcrapy. Some matcrials are also cmployed progressivc musclc rclaxation training plus cithcr nondircctive
dcsigned to be uscd by motivatcd patients indcpcnclcntly of professionals. thcrapy, coping dcsensitiíation, or cognitivc thcrapy. This finding
Thc advantages to sclf-dircctcd trcatmcnts includc dccrcased cost, incrcascd challengcd thc results of an carlicr study by thcsc authors (Borkovcc ct
disscmination, and decrcascd stigmatization. Sclf-dircctcd trcatmcnts vary al., 1987), in which thcy found that cognitivc thcrapy with rclaxation was
from general inspirational works (c.g., Ross, 1994b) to spccific forms of superior to nondircctive thcrapy with rclaxation. In general, thesc find-
CBT with cxplicit homework cxcrcises (e.g., Craskc ct al., 1993). ings suggcst that at Icast part of thc cffcctivencss of CBT trcatment can
be attributcd to nonspccific trcatment effccts, such as the instillation of
hope, the cxpcctation of ¡mprovcmcnt, and participation in a trcatment
Trcatment Outcome Findings
study.
Therc is somcwhat more cvidcncc for thc bcncfit of CBT whcn a retum
Outcome for Formj of CBT
to normal functioning ¡s thc outcome critcrion. In a rcvicw of the trcat-
How cffective are standard CBT techniques in terms of cmpirical trcat- ment litcraturc using this critcrion, Durham and Alian (1993) found that
ment outcome? In general, results have been promising. Treatmcnt out- roughly half (57%) of patients in CBT achicved a rcturn to normal func-
come studics havc varicd in their dcsign, comparing CBT to no-trcatment tioning, comparcd to only 22% in relaxation treatment alone. Thcse authors
or waiting-list controls, to relaxation alone, to pill placebo conditions, also rcportcd that psychologic trcatments of GAD in general yiclded a
and to standard pharmacothcrapy trcatmcnts. A number of studics have 50% reduction in thc scvcrity of somatic symptoms and a 25% decrease
found that CBT trcatments are superior to no-treatmcnt and waiting-list in mcasurcs of trait anxiety. In addition, thcsc positivo changes were like-
control conditions in the short tcrm (less than 6 months) (Woodward & ly to be maintaincd at 6-month follow-up.
Jones, 1980; Barlow et al., 1984; Blowers, Cobb, & Mathcws, 1987; But- There ¡s a paucity of long-tcrm trcatment outcome results for GAD.
ler, Cullington, Hibbcrt, Klimcs, ók Gcldcr, 1987; Butlcr, Fcnnell, Rob- Four studics using CBT approachcs havc dcmonstratcd maintenance of
¡90 ANX/ETV DISORDERS C<i£iii(¡vi"Dcíuivnir<il Trcalmcm uf Social Pluilria and GAD 191

trcatment gains over a 3- to 6-month íbllow-up (Lindsay ct al., 1987; Power a relapsc to basclinc symptoms in 63-81% of individuáis who discontinué
et al., 1990; Whitc &. Kccnan, 1990; Butlcr ct al., 1991). Intervcntions thcir bcnzodiazcpinc mcdications. Buspironc, an azapironc anxiolytic agcnt,
that include cognitivc rcstructuring appcar to providc more stablc long- has thc advantagcs of fcwcr scdativc side cffects and Icss potencial for re-
term outcome and bcttcr rctcntion in treatment than rclaxation trcatmcnts bound anxiety upon withdrawal. Thc limitcd research on trcatmcnt cf-
alone (Barlow et al., 1992; Butlcr ct al., 1991; Durham ckTurvcy, 1987). fcctivencss with buspironc suggcsts that it may be as uscful as
In a rcview of psychosocial trcatmcnts, Durham and Alian (1993) found bcnzodiazcpincs (Cohn ct al., 1989; Fcighncr &. Cohn, 1989; Sussman,
an avcrage reduction of 54% in Hamilton Rating Scalc for Anxiety (HAM- 1987).
A; Hamilton, 1959) scorcs at thc cnd of treatment and 50% at longcr- Only onc study in thc GAD treatment litcraturc combincd psycho-
term follow-up asscssmcnts. Thc tcndcncy to worry was itsclf rcduccd by logic and phannacologic trcatmcnt stratcgics. Power ct al. (1990) found
25%. ' ' that CBT plus diazcpam was no bcttcr than CBT alone, but that both
of thcsc trcatmcnts wcrc superior to diazcpam alone. Futurc outcome
research should furthcr invcstigate thc cfficacy of combincd trcatmcnts.
Pharmacologic and Combined Teclmiques
The class of drugs long associatcd with anxiety relicf has been thc ben-
Mcla-Analyíic Fmclings
zodiaicpines, and rcsults on thcir cfficacy for trcating GAD havc becn
gcncrally favorable. A numbcrof studics havc found that bcnzodiazcpincs Somc rcscarchcrs havc uscd mcta-analytic tcchniqucs to asscss diffcrcnccs
are superior to pill placebo controls (Ansscau, Doumont, Thiry, von Frcnck- in trcatmcnt outcome more objcctivcly. Chamblcss and Gillis (1993) rcport-
cll, &. Collard, 1985; Cohn, Rickcls, &. Stccgc, 1989; Rickcls ct al., 1982). cd a largc within-group prc-to-post cffcct sizc (1.69) for CBT in scvcn GAD
Roughly 35% of paticnts with GAD trcatcd with bcnzodiazcpincs show studics; thcsc trcatmcnt gains wcrc maintaincd at 6- and 12-month follow-
markcd ¡mprovcmcnt, and 40% demónstrate modérate improvcmcnt but ups. By comparison, thc effcct sizc for control groups that werc uscd in
are still symptomatic (Uhlcnhuth, Dcwitt, Baltcr, Johanson, &. Mcllingcr, some of thcsc studics was considcrably smallcr (0.60). Hunt and Singh (1991)
1988). Tablc 7.2 prcscnts data comparing psychosocial and bcnzodiaze- also uscd mcta-analytic tcchniqucs to evalúate trcatmcnt outcome in 42
pinc trcatmcnt for GAD. In a rcview of ninc studies of bcnzodiazcpinc studics. Rcsults bascd on short-tcrm outcome indicatcd that on mcasurcs
trcatmcnt of GAD, Barlow (1988) found an avcrage reduction in HAM- of anxiety, short- and long-acting bcnzodiazcpincs and buspironc wcrc all
A scorcs of 48%. By way of comparison, placebo trcatment was associatcd roughly equivalcnt. All thrcc drugs wcrc superior to drug placebo condi-
with an avcrage 30% reduction in thcsc scorcs. Henee, likc thc rcsults tions. CBT that includcd cognitivc rcstructuring and rclaxation training
from CBT studics, thcsc rcsults suggcst thc difficultics of obtaining an out- wcrc cquivalcnt to drug intcrvcntions and superior to waiting-list controls.
come through active trcatmcnt that significantly cxcccds nonspccific or In addition, CBT intcrvcntions had bcttcr long-tcrm rcsults than did drug
placebo trcatmcnt cffccts. thcrapics. Finally, rclaxation tcchniqucs alone wcrc not as cffcctivc as drug
Although bcnzodiazcpincs havc dcmonstratcd thcir cffcctivcncss in or CBT intcrvcntions and wcrc not diffcrcnt from waiting-list controls.
providing symptomatic relicf for GAD paticnts, a numbcr of drawbacks Hunt and Singh havc cndorscd CBT as thc trcatmcnt of choicc bccausc
have becn citcd; thcsc include sidc cffccts (c.g., impaircd cognitivc per- of its short- and long-tcrm bcncfits.
formance, lethargy), drug tolcrancc, drug dcpendcncc, and relapsc upon In a more reccnt mcta-analysis Gould ct al. (1995) comparcd CBT
withdrawal (Shadcr &. Grecnblatt, 1993). Dubovsky (1990) has rcportcd to pharmacothcrapy in 27 studics that cmploycd a control comparison con-
dition. Stibjccts ¡n thcsc studics wcre rcquircd to mcct DSM (111, 111-R,
or IV) criteria for GAD, or would havc met thcsc critcria had thcy becn
TABLE 7.2. Mean Pcrccntagc Ucduction in Hamilton Raling Scalc applicd. Effect sizcs for anxiety wcrc calculatcd only from validatcd sclf-
for Anxiety (HAM-A) Scorcs in Studics of I'aticnls with GAO rcport qucstionnaircs and clinician-ratcd mcasurcs, Pharmacothcrapy studics
(n = 15) yicldcd an acutc (Icss than 6 months) mean cffcct sizc of 0.57
Pcrccntage reduction in scorcs
rclativc to placebo and subjccts droppcd out at thc rate of 15.4%. Bcn-
Trcatmcnt Mean Rangc
zodiazcpincs wcrc thc drug most commonly cmploycd for GAD trcatmcnt
Psychosocial trcatmcnf 50% 20-76% (n = 11 studics) and thcir mean cffcct sizc was 0.67; howcvcr, scvcral
Bcruodlaicpinc trcatmcnt'' 48% 22-62% authors rcported difficultics with tapcring thcsc agcnts and relapsc upon
Placebo trcatmcnt'' 30% 18-48% tapcr. Buspironc was cmploycd in 6 studies and its mean effcct sizc was
'Data (rom Durham and Alian (199)).
0.44. Thrcc controllcd studics asscssed thc efficacy of antidcprcssants and
'Dará frnm líarlnw (1988). thcir mean cffcct sizc was 0.57.
192 ANXIETY DISORDERS Cogiúlive-Behaviinal Traumciu iif Suciiil Phubia and CAD 193

Cognitive-behavioral intervcntions ¡ncluded studies that utilizad cog- about the cognitivc biases that accompany dcprcssion; (2) having patients
nitivc restructuring, relaxation training, exposure, anxicty managcment monitor these biases and apply cognitivc restructuring skills that targct
training, or combinations of these. A total of 14 studies used CBT without thcm; and (3) devoting extra time in scssions to encouraging compliance
pharmacotherapy , and their mean anxiety effcct sizc vvas 0.7 1 . The mean with homework assignmcnts. CBT for GAD alone may help to reduce
dropout rate for CBT intcrventions in thcsc studies was 1 1.6%. In gener- deprcssive symptomatology (Chambless ck Gillis, 1993). In patients with
al, therc were no significant differenccs between subtypes of CBT inter- more scverc depression, a standard trial of CBT for dcprcssion or use of
vcntions, and all were superior to the nuil hypothesis (effcct size = 0.0). antidepressant medication may be neccssary prior to GAD treatment.
Interventions that uscd both cognitivc and behavioral techniques yicldcd Patients with GAD may also present with comorbid panic disorder,
strong effect sizes (0.90) as did Anxiety Management Training (0.74). Puré social phobia, and alcohol abuse (Barlow, 1988; Tyrer, 1984). Butler ct
cogntive therapy (0.59) and relaxation training (0.63) appcarcd to be cf- al. (1987) reported that 29% of thcir GAD study samplc suffercd from panic
fective interventions. Studies cmploying behavioral techniques alone ap- disorder. Similarly, Jannoun et al. (1982) reported that one-third of their
pearcd to be least cffcctivc (0.48). samplc suffcred from comorbid agoraphobia. For patients with comorbid
Comparisons of studies utilizing cognitivc-behavioral trcatments (0.7 1) panic disorder whosc main focus of worry is having a panic attack, a stan-
to those using pharmacotherapy (0.57) were not statistically significant. dard course of CBT for panic prior to GAD treatment may be most help-
In addition, mean attrition rates were not significantly diffcrent between ful. For patients having panic disordcr with mild agoraphobia, we find that
the two forms of treatment. Only onc study (Power ct al., 1990) attempt- an carly introduction to brcathing retraining skills, along with interocep-
ed to combine CBT and medication, and results from this study were tivc cxposure cxcrcises, can cnhancc GAD treatment. In many ¡nstances,
promising. sclf-help books about panic disordcr and agoraphobia (e.g, Clum, 1990;
In summary, the availablc data suggcst that CBT and bcnzodiazcpinc Barlow &. Craskc, 1993) may be cmploycd concurrcntly with GAD treat-
treatment for GAD are both cffective in the short tcrrn; howcver, problctns ment. For patients with comorbid social phobia, extra scssions may need
with drug side effects and tolerancc make benzodiazepines a less attrac- to be dcvotcd to cognitivc restructuring training that teaches them how
tive first-line intervcntion. Treatment gains appcar to be better maintaincd to challenge and change thoughts associatcd with fcar of negativc evalua-
over long follow-up periods (1 to 2 years) with CBT. CBT is also associat- tion. In addition, in vivo homework assignmcnts rcquiring systcmatic ex-
ed with decrcased concomitant medication use ovcr follow-up periods posure to fearcd sociíil situations may be beneficial.
(Barlow et al., 1992; Hunt ók Singh, 1991; White, Kccnan, ck Brooks, Whcn standard CBT techniques for GAD are not succcssful, patients
1991). may bcncfit from treatment strategies appropriate for more sevcrely ob-
scssional conditions. Somc patients rcport that thcy try to push these ob-
sessive worries away by using distraction techniques, but find that this
Treatmcnt Resistance/Maximizing Treatment Effccttvcncss
strategy is only helpful in the short tcrni. For patients with obsessive-com-
A number of patients with GAD may respond only modestly to standard pulsivc disorder charactcrized by primary obscssional symptoms, bencfits
CBT strategies (Barlow et al., 1992). Unfortunately, rescarch on GAD have becn reported for cxposure strategies utilizing a loop tape, which al-
is less advanced than that on othcr anxicty disordcrs, and data indicating lows patients to listen to thcir obsessivc thoughts repeatcdly in an exposure
clinical predictors of treatment rcsponsc are not abundant. Some authors format (Salkovskis, 1983;Thyer, 1985), With repcated exposure, the ten-
have reported that comorbid depression in GAD patients is a predictor dcncy to respond to these thoughts with anxicty or rituals is broken.
of suboptimal treatment outcome and is positivcly corrclatcd with the chro- An additional aspcct of treatment that may influcnce longer-term out-
nicity and severity of anxicty symptoms (Breslau & Davis, 1985; Butler come is relapse prevcntion training. CBT intcrvcntions for anxiety gcncrally
et al., 1987). This finding is particularly troubling, givcn the fact that onc- include instruction in the naturc of the disorder and cncouragemcnt of
third to one-half of GAD patients prcsenting for treatment suffcr from paticnts to Icarn ncw rcsponses to anxicty with cognitive restructuring
comorbid depression or dysthymia (de Ruiter, Rijkcn, Garssen, van Schaik, and cxposure excrciscs. Paticnts are instructcd to apply this new Icarning
ck Kraaimaat, 1989; Butler ct al., 1987). For GAD patients rcceivingCBT, to situations ¡n their own lives, and this learning is designcd to continué
comorbid depression can be particularly problcmatic bccausc of the num- to occur aftcr the complction of formal treatment. In all of our anxiety
ber and varicty of homework tasks (c.g., practicing rclaxation techniques, treatment programs, we cncouragc patients to continué to practice somatic
completing thought records) that are esscntial to this intervcntion. Sevcral symptom managcment skills {e.g., rclaxation) and cognitivc restructuring
strategies that wc have suggested in Chapter 5 of this volume for treating and cxposure tcchniques after the short-term CBT intervcntion. Patients
comorbid panic disorder and depression may also be useful for hclping GAD are also instructed in a model of relapsc prcvention that stresses the reap-
patients with comorbid depression. These include (1) educating patients plication of treatment procedures whcn high-risk situations are encoun-
ANXIETV DISORDERS Trcaimem uf Social f'había and GAD 195
tercd and/or if symptoms rcturn. Such proccdurcs havc been shown to be orders: A comparison of non-dircctivc, cognitivc, and coping dcsensitization
hclpful at ¡mproving long-tcrm outcomc (Óst, 1987). thcrapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 877-884.
Borkovcc, T. D., Mathews, A. M., Chambcrs, A., Ebrahimi, S.. Lytlc, R., &.
Nclson, R. (1987). The cffccts of rclaxation training with cognitivc thcrapy
REFERENCES or nondircctivc therapy and thc role of rclaxation-induccd anxicty in thc
treatmcnt of gcncralizcd anxicty. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psycholo-
gy, 55. 883-888.
Alstrom, J. E., Nordlund, C. L, Pcrsson, G., Harding, M., & Ljungqvist, C. (1984). Borkovcc, T. D., 6k Sidcs, J. R. (1979). Critica! proccdural variables rclatcd to
Efíects of four treatmcnt mcthods on social pliobic paticnts not Mutable for thc physiological cffccts ofprogrcssivc rclaxation: A rcvicw. Behaviour Research
insight-oricntcd psychotherapy. Acta Psychialrica Scandinaw'ca, 70, 97-110. and Thcrapy, 17, 119-125.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual o/ mental Brcslau. N.. & Davis, G. C. (1985). DSM-II1 gcncralizcd anxicty disordcr: An
Asorden (3rd cd.). Washington, DC: Author. cmpirical invcstigation of more stringcnt entena. Psychiatry Research, 5,
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental 231-238.
disorderj (3rd cd., rcv.). Washington, DC: Author. Brooks. R. B., Baltazar, P. L., ck Munjack, D. J. (1989). Co-occurrcncc of pcrso-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental nality disorders with panic disordcr, social phobia, and gcncralizcd anxicty
disorders (4th cd.). Washington, DC: Author. disordcr: A rcvicw of thc litcraturc. Journal of Anxiciy Disorders, 3, 259-285.
Andcrson, D. J., Noyes, R., & Crowe, R. R. (1984). A comparison of panic dis- Brown, T. A., &. Barlow, D. N. (1992). Comorbidity among anxicty disorders:
ordcr and gcncralizcd anxiety disordcr. American Journal ofPiydwtlTy, 141, Implications for treatmcnt and DSM-1V. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
572-575. Psychology, 60, 835-844.
Ansseau, M., Doumont, A., Thiry, D., von Frenckcll, R., & Collard, J. (1985). Bruch, M. A., Mattia, J. I., Hcimbcrg, R. G., & Holt, C. S. (1993). Cognitivc
Initial study of mcthylclonazcpam in gencralizcd anxicty disordcr. ÍVyc/iop/iar- spccificity in social anxicty and dcprcssion: Supporting cvidcncc and qualifi-
macology. 87. 130-135. cationsduc to affcctivc confounding. Cognitivc T/icrap? and Research, 17, 1-21.
Ball. S. G., Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., Ucccllo, R., & Roscnbaum. J. F. (1995). Butlcr, G., Cullington, A., Hibbcrt, G., Klimcs, I., & Gcldcr, M. (1987). Anxic-
Diffcrcntiating social phobia and panic disordcr: A test of core bclicfs. Cog- ty managcmcnt for pcrsistcnt gcncraliscd anxicty. líríitsh Journal of Psychiatry,
niiivc Thcrapy and Research, 19, 473-482. 151, 535-542.
Barlow, D. H. (1988). Anxicty and its clisorcícrs: T/ic naturc and ircatmcnt o f anxiety Butlcr, G., Cullington, A., Munby. M., Amics. P., & Gcldcr, M. (1984). Ex-
and panic. New York: Guilford Press. posure and anxicty managcmcnt in thc treatmcnt of social phobia. Journal
Barlow, D. H. (1994). Comorbidity ¡n social phobia: Implications for cognitivc- of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52(4), 642-650.
bchavioral treatmcnt. Bullctin uf thc Mcnningcr Clitiic, 58, A43-A52. Butlcr, G., Fcnncll, M., Robson, P., & Gcldcr. M. (1991). Comparison of be-
Barlow, D. H.. Cohén. A. S., Wadcll, M. T., Vcrmilyca. B. B.. Kosko. J. S., havior thcrapy and cognitivc bchavior thcrapy ¡n thc treatmcnt of gcncral-
Blanchard. E. B., &. DiNardo, P. A. (1984). Panic and gcncralizcd anxicty izcd anxicty disorder. Joumaíc/Comultingano1 Clinical Psychology, 59, 167-175.
disordcr: Naturc and treatmcnt. Bchavior Thcra¡>y, 15, 431-449. Butlcr, G., & Mathcws, A. (1987). Anticipatory anxicty and risk pcrccption. Cog-
Barlow, D. H., & Craskc, M. G. (1993). Mastcry ofyour anxieiy and panic II. Al- nilive T/icrapy and Research, 11, 551-565.
bany. NY: Graywind. Chamblcss, D. L., &. Gillis, M. M. (1993). Cognitivc thcrapy of anxicty disord-
Barlow, D. H., Rapcc, R. M., &. Brown, T. A. (1992). Bchavioral treatmcnt of ers. Jounial o/Consulling and" Clinical Psychology, 61(2), 248-260.
gcncralizcd anxicty disordcr. Be/uivior T/icrapy, 23, 551-570. Clum, G. A. (1990). Coping with panic. Pacific Grave, CA: Brooks/Colc.
Biran, M., Augusto, F., &. Wilson, G. T. (1981). In vivo cxposurc vcrsus cogni- Colín, J. B., Rickcls, K., & Stccgc, J. F. (1989). A poolcd, doublc-blind compari-
tivc rcstructuring in thc scriptophobia. Bclutviour Research and Thcra¡iy, 19, son of thc cffccts of buspirone, dinz'cpam and placebo in womcn with chronic
525-532. anxicty. Currcnt Medical Research Opinión, I I , 304-320.
Blazcr, D., Hughes. D., &. Gcorgc, L. K. (1987). Strcssful life cvcnts and thc on- Cragan, M. K., & Dcffcnbachcr, J. L. (1984). Anxicty managcmcnt training and
sct of gcncralizcd anxicty syndromc. American Jounial of Psychiairy, 144, rclaxation as sclf-control in thc treatmcnt of gcncralizcd anxicty in medical
1178-1183. outpaticnts. Joumal o/Coniuliing and Clinical Psychology, 31, 123-131.
Blowcrs, C.. Cobb, J., & Mathews, A. (1987). Gcneraliscd anxicty: A controllcd Craskc, M. G., Barlow, D. H., & O'Lcary, T. A. (1993). Mastery ofyour aiwicty
treatmcnt study. Bclviviour Research and Thcrapy, 25, 493-502. ana" worry. Albany, NY: Graywind.
Borkovcc, T. D. (1985). Worry: A potcntially valuablc conccpt. Bc/iaviour Resemch Crowc, R. R., Noyes, R., Pauls, D. L, & Slymcn. D. (1983). A family study of
and Thcra¡>y, 23. 481-482. panic disordcr. Archives uf General Psychiatry, 40, 1065-1069.
Borkovcc, T. D., &. Inz, J. (1990). Thc naturc of worry ¡n gcncraliscd anxicty Davidson, J. R. T., & Vcrsiani, M. (1994). International advanccs in thc trcat-
un disordcr: A prcdominancc of thought activity. Bclwviour Research and T/icra- incnt of social phobia. Joumal of Clinical Psychiatry, 55(3), 123-369.
t>y. 28, 153-158. de Ruitcr, C., Rijken, H., Garsscn, B.. van Schaik, A., & Kraaimaat, F. (1989).
Borkovcc, T. D., & Mathcws, A. (1988). Trcatment of non-phobic anxicty dis- Cormorbidity among anxicty disorders. Journal of Anxicty Disorders, 3, 57-68.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen