Sie sind auf Seite 1von 11

Copyright 1992 by the American I

Journal of Educational Psychology


1992, Vol. 84, No. 3, 353-363

Predicting Progress in Beginning Reading:


Dynamic Assessment of Phonemic Awareness
Janet E. Spector
College of Education, University of Maine

This study investigates the ability of a dynamic measure of phonemic awareness to predict
progress in beginning reading. Thirty-eight kindergarteners who were nonreaders were assessed
in the fall on receptive vocabulary, letter and word recognition, invented spelling, phoneme
segmentation, phoneme deletion, and dynamic phoneme segmentation. They were retested near
the end of the school year on reading, spelling, and phonemic awareness. The results of the
multiple-regression analyses supported the hypothesis that dynamic assessment enhances the
predictive utility of a phonemic awareness measure. Performance on dynamic phoneme segmen-
tation was the best predictor of end-of-year reading scores and of growth in phonemic awareness.
The study demonstrates the applicability of principles of dynamic assessment to the measurement
of phonemic awareness and provides further evidence regarding the relationship between pho-
nemic awareness and reading acquisition.

This study investigates the ability of a dynamic measure of (e.g., Bruce, 1964; Rosner & Simon, 1971); and producing
phonemic awareness to predict progress in beginning reading. invented spellings (e.g., Mann, Tobin, & Wilson, 1987; Morris
The dynamic approach was compared with a more conven- & Perney, 1984; Read, 1971).
tional static approach to assessing phonemic awareness. I The results of both correlational and experimental studies
hypothesized that the dynamic measure would more accu- generally have indicated that students who enter reading
rately predict progress in beginning reading than would a instruction unable to perform phonemic awareness tasks ex-
static measure. The study was influenced by theory and perience less success in reading than students who score high
research on two questions: (a) the relationship between pho- in phonemic awareness when instruction commences (e.g.,
nemic awareness and reading acquisition and (b) the effec- Bradley & Bryant, 1983; Calfee, Lindamood, & Lindamood,
tiveness of dynamic versus static assessment. 1973; Juel, 1988; Liberman, Shankweiler, Fischer, & Carter,
1974; Perfetti, Beck, Bell, & Hughes, 1987; Share, Jorm,
Maclean, & Matthews, 1984; Stanovich, Cunningham, &
Phonemic Awareness and Reading Acquisition Cramer, 1984; Tunmer & Nesdale, 1985; Vellutino & Scan-
One of the most consistent relationships to emerge from Ion, 1987). From a theoretical perspective, this finding is
the past decade of research on reading is the relationship consistent with models of reading acquisition that emphasize
between phonemic awareness and reading acquisition (see the critical role of insight into the alphabetic principle during
reviews by Adams, 1990; Ehri, 1979; Golinkoff, 1978; Jorm the initial stages of learning how to read (e.g., Elkonin, 1973;
& Share, 1983; Liberman & Shankweiler, 1985; Wagner & Gough & Hillinger, 1980; Liberman, 1973; Perfetti, 1985;
Torgesen, 1987; Williams, 1984). Although there is some Rozin & Gleitman, 1977). That is, children must realize that
variation across studies in the definition of phonemic aware- letters stand for sounds and when combined, yield words.
ness, the term generally is used to denote the ability to perceive Previous research on phonemic awareness has identified
spoken words as a sequence of sounds (Lewkowicz, 1980). tasks that appear to be reliable and valid predictors of reading
Phonemic awareness has been measured by performance on progress. Yopp (1988), for example, identified two tasks from
a wide range of tasks, including rhyming (e.g., Calfee, Chap- a battery of 10 phonemic awareness tests that together ac-
man, & Venezky, 1972); isolating beginning, medial, and counted for 58% of the variance in scores on a learning test
ending sounds (e.g., Williams, 1980); breaking words into designed to simulate the learning-to-read process. The capac-
their component sounds (e.g., Fox & Routh, 1975; Goldstein, ity of phonemic awareness tests to predict aspects of reading
1976; Helfgott, 1976); saying words with target sounds deleted progress suggests that such tests could be used to identify
children who would benefit from instructional intervention
(Share etal., 1984).
This article is based in part on a paper presented in April 1990 at A possible obstacle to practical application, however, is the
the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Associa- unfamiliarity and complexity of many phonemic awareness
tion in Boston. tasks. Whereas poor performance on a phoneme segmentation
I am indebted to Pamela Schutz for enabling data collection; the task might indicate low phonemic awareness, it might also
principal, teachers, and students of Pendleton Street School for their
participation in the study; and Ted Coladarci and the anonymous
reflect the child's lack of understanding of task requirements
reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions. or difficulty in meeting ancillary task demands. For example,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to some researchers have asked children to count the number of
Janet E. Spector, College of Education, Shibles Hall, University of sounds in a spoken word and then to use a pencil or dowel to
Maine, Orono, Maine 04469-0155. make a corresponding number of taps (e.g., Liberman et al.,

353
354 JANET E. SPECTOR

1974). Poor performance might indicate difficulty in seg- Traditional tests assess only two states: unaided success and
menting the spoken word, but it might also reflect difficulty failure. That is, the child either answers a question correctly,
in counting the sounds, attending to task instructions, or without prompts or cues from the examiner, or the child is
maintaining the one-to-one correspondence between sounds considered to fail the item. From a Vygotskian perspective,
and taps. Thus, a limitation of conventional tests of phonemic however, the child may be somewhere in between these two
awareness is that they yield too many false negatives, that is, states: unable to perform the task independently but able to
students who are unable to perform the experimental task but achieve success with minimal assistance. Vygotsky (1935/
who actually possess (or could easily acquire) the ability that 1978) considered this in-between state to be the zone of
the task is designed to measure. Dynamic assessment is an proximal development: "the distance between the actual de-
approach that might be useful in eliminating these false velopmental level as determined by independent problem-
negatives. solving and the level of potential development as determined
through problem solving under adult guidance or in collabo-
Dynamic Assessment ration with more capable peers" (p. 85).
Consider, for example, two children who earn the same low
Dynamic assessment is a general term used to describe a score on a traditional phoneme segmentation test that requires
variety of evaluation approaches that emphasize the processes, students to articulate each of the sounds that they hear in a
in addition to the products, of assessment (see reviews by word. With minimal instruction, the first child experiences
Campione, 1989; Lidz, 1981, 1987). These approaches in- significant growth in performance, whereas the second child
clude learning potential assessment (e.g., Budoff, 1987a, shows little improvement. Although the two children received
1987b; Feuerstein, 1979); testing-the-limits procedures (Carl- the same score initially, different degrees of future success
son & Wiedl, 1978, 1979); mediated assessment (e.g., Brans- might be predicted for the two children in tasks that presume
ford, Delclos, Vye, Burns, & Hasselbring, 1987; Burns, 1985); phonemic awareness.
and assisted learning and transfer (e.g., Campione, Brown, In the present study, I developed a dynamic measure that
Ferrara, Jones, & Steinberg, 1985). assesses the ability of kindergarten children to perform a
To obtain information about responsiveness to instruction, phonemic awareness task when given supportive prompts and
dynamic approaches require the interaction between tester cues. On the basis of previous research on dynamic assessment
and student. When a student has difficulty solving a problem (e.g., Budoff, 1987a, 1987b; Campione et al., 1985; Carlson
or answering a question, the tester attempts to move the & Wiedl, 1979; Embretson, 1987; Ferrara, Brown, & Cam-
student from failure to success by modifying the format, pione, 1986), I hypothesized that the dynamic measure would
providing additional examples or trials, modeling an appro- more accurately predict future progress in reading than would
priate strategy for success, or offering increasingly more direct comparable static measures. Also considered was the ability
cues or prompts. The intensity of the dynamic intervention of the dynamic measure to forecast growth in phonemic
varies across approaches, ranging from brief, standardized awareness. I expected the dynamic measure to be a better
prompts (e.g., testing the limits) to complex, individualized predictor of phonemic awareness at the end of the kindergar-
probes (e.g., learning potential assessment). ten year than the traditional static measures of phonemic
Advocates of dynamic assessment characterize traditional awareness.
tests as static: A student's failure to solve a problem or answer
a question is viewed merely as an indicator of where the Method
student stands on some underlying ability scale. However,
although the student's performance may accurately reflect
previous achievement, it is not necessarily predictive of how Subjects
the student will perform in the future (Campione & Brown, In October, parents of students enrolled in a public school kinder-
1985; Feuerstein, 1979). That is, although a student might garten program received a letter from the school requesting permis-
lack the skill in question at the time of testing, with minimal sion for their child to participate in a study of beginning reading. Of
instruction the same student might realize substantial gains. 74 students, 52 (70%) returned signed consent forms. The school,
In addition, because static approaches typically provide little located in a small New England town, serves a predominantly middle-
feedback or practice prior to testing, failure often reflects class, White population. All students spoke English as afirstlanguage.
children's lack of understanding of the instructions more than Given the reciprocal relationship between phonemic awareness and
it reflects their ability to perform the task. reading acquisition, only those children who were unable to read any
words on the San Diego Quick Assessment List (LaPray & Ross,
Most dynamic assessment approaches are linked theoreti- 1969) in the fall of the kindergarten year were included in the study.
cally to Vygotsky's (1935/1978) views of learning and devel- In addition, students who were absent during any of the fall or spring
opment. In his work, Vygotsky emphasized the critical role testing sessions were excluded from the analysis. This resulted in a
in learning of children's social interaction with adults. The final sample of 38 students. The mean age of children at the beginning
adult acts as an expert model in demonstrating how to solve of the study was 5 years, 11 months.
a problem or perform a particular task. Over time, the child
takes on increasing responsibility for task performance, and Procedure
the adult provides help only when needed. Eventually, the
child internalizes the problem-solving routine and is able to Data for the study were collected in November and May of the
perform the task independently. kindergarten year. Testing was conducted at the school and was
DYNAMIC ASSESSMENT 355

completed in three 15- to 20-min sessions. Children were assessed neme segmentation, therefore, is reported as number of words seg-
individually in the fall on letter and word recognition (San Diego mented correctly.
Quick Assessment List; LaPray & Ross, 1969), invented spelling Dynamic assessment. The dynamic assessment procedure paral-
(Mann, Tobin, & Wilson, 1987), phoneme segmentation (Yopp, leled the Yopp-Singer task but provided corrective feedback and
1988), phoneme deletion (Bruce, 1964), and dynamic phoneme seg- increasingly supportive prompts and cues when children were unable
mentation (with a measure designed for this study). Students were to segment a word correctly. The tester used the following series of
assessed again in the spring on all measures except dynamic phoneme prompts each time a child was unable to segment a word:
segmentation. The fall tests were administered in two sessions ap- Prompt 1: pronouncing the target word slowly;
proximately 1 week apart. In Session 1, students completed phoneme Prompt 2: asking the child to identify the first sound of the
segmentation, phoneme deletion, and invented spelling. Session 2 word;
covered dynamic assessment and word recognition. Spring testing Prompt 3: cuing the child with the first sound;
required only one session. The order in which the tasks were admin- Prompt 4: cuing the child with the number of sounds in the
istered was fixed. In the fall, dynamic assessment was completed last word;
to eliminate the possibility that the instructional prompts and cues Prompt 5: modeling segmentation using pennies placed in
would influence performance on the other measures. This design squares to represent the number of sounds in the
feature, however, precluded investigation of possible order effects, a word;
limitation that must be considered in interpreting the results. Prompt 6: modeling segmentation as above, but working hand
over hand with the child while pronouncing the seg-
ments;
Prompt 7: repeating Prompt 6.
Measures
Each child attempted a maximum of 12 items: four consonant-
I used four phonemic awareness tasks: phoneme segmentation, vowel (CV) words (i.e., say, pie, we, two), four vowel-consonant (VC)
dynamic phoneme segmentation, phoneme deletion, and invented words (i.e., age, eat, egg, if), and four consonant-vowel-consonant
spelling. The Yopp-Singer phoneme segmentation (Yopp, 1988) and (CVC) words (i.e., leg, feet, page, rice). The intent was to select
Bruce Word Analysis Test (1964) were selected on the basis of Yopp's familiar words that included a range of vowel and consonant sounds.
study of the reliability and validity of 10 phonemic awareness meas- One student who, on the first 2 items, was unable to produce a correct
ures. In Yopp's study, the results of a factor analysis indicated that segmentation after all seven prompts (including the last three
phonemic awareness comprises two factors: simple phonemic aware- prompts, which required imitation only) was discontinued on the
ness and complex phonemic awareness. In that study the Yopp- task and received a total score of zero. (The dynamic assessment
Singer phoneme segmentation test provided the most reliable measure script is presented in the Appendix.)
of simple phonemic awareness, and the Bruce Word Analysis Test Scores on dynamic phoneme segmentation indicate the degree of
provided the most reliable measure of complex phonemic awareness independence that the child achieved in performing the segmentation
(rs = .95 and .92, respectively). Together, the two measures accounted task. Each item was scored as follows: 6 = correct response with no
for 58% of the variance in the rate at which students learned to prompts required; 5 = correct response after Prompt 1; 4 = correct
decode new words. Invented spelling was included as an additional response after Prompt 2; 3 = correct response after Prompt 3; 2 =
indicator of phonemic awareness on the basis of previous research correct response after Prompt 4; 1 = correct response after Prompt
linking invented spelling with phonemic awareness (Chomsky, 1971, 5; and 0 = no correct response.
1979; Ehri, 1989; Ehri & Wilce, 1980, 1987a, 1987b; Liberman, Performance on the last two imitation trials (i.e., Prompts 6 and
Rubin, Duques, & Carlisle, 1985; Mann etal., 1987; Morris &Perney, 7) did not contribute to scores. Initially, I created a second dynamic
1984; Read, 1971, 1986). Each of these measures is described below. measure that assigned points for successful second and third trials at
Phoneme segmentation. On the Yopp-Singer phoneme segmen- imitation. This measure was scored on an 8-point scale, with a score
tation test, subjects were asked to pronounce, in order, each of the of 8 indicating correct response without prompting and a score of 1
sounds in a word. Four examples were given at the beginning of the indicating correct response on the third imitation attempt. Because
test to familiarize the student with the task. Feedback was given on the two measures yielded equivalent results on all analyses reported
the four trial items, but no feedback was given for the remaining 22 below, I include here only the results for the 6-point scale. The
words on the test. The present administration procedure differs maximum score possible on the dynamic measure was 72.
somewhat from that of Yopp (1988), who provided feedback regard- The present approach to dynamic assessment has both similarities
ing the correct response after each item. In the present study, feedback to and differences from other dynamic approaches. These similarities
was withheld to distinguish more clearly between the static phoneme and differences can be described in terms of three general dimensions
segmentation test and the dynamic version of the same task. identified by Campione (1989): focus, interaction, and target (see
Using Yopp's (1988) approach, I scored the test for number of Campione, 1989, for examples of studies that differ on these dimen-
words segmented correctly. The maximum score possible was 22. The sions).
majority of students (n = 25) scored zero in the fall on this measure. Focus refers to the way in which the researcher operationalizes the
To alleviate the substantial floor effect and to make the scaling more processes and effects of dynamic assessment. Whereas most dynamic
similar to that used during dynamic assessment, I awarded points for approaches involve a test-train-test paradigm, investigators differ
each sound in a word that was segmented correctly. The maximum with respect to the measures they derive. Three types of measures
score possible for the revised measure was 57, and the correlation predominate: (a) measures of change from pretest to posttest, (b)
between the original and the revised fall measures was .88 (p < .001). measures of posttest performance, and (c) measures of performance
Not surprisingly the two scoring procedures yielded equivalent during the dynamic assessment itself. This study followed the third
conclusions regarding the benefits of dynamic assessment. Therefore, approach. The dynamic phoneme segmentation measure indicated
I chose to report fall phoneme segmentation results only for the the degree of independence the student achieved during dynamic
revised measure. However, because of the manner in which students' assessment. I also created a second measure that reflected change in
responses were recorded in the spring, it was not possible to score performance from the static to the dynamic condition. I obtained
spring data for number of sounds correctly segmented. Spring pho- this measure by regressing the fall dynamic measure on static pho-
356 JANET E. SPECTOR

neme segmentation. The resulting residualized gain reflects variability Because of the focus in this study on phonemic awareness, I
in dynamic scores that is independent of static performance. eliminated the distinction between conventionally correct and pho-
Interaction describes the nature of the interaction between the netically equivalent responses. That is, I reduced the scale from 0-4
examiner and the student during dynamic assessment. The interac- to 0-3 by awarding 3 points to both correct spellings and preconven-
tion is either standardized (i.e., all students receive the same prompts tional responses that accurately represented the phonemic structure
or cues) or individualized. The latter condition is clinical; the exam- of the word. The highest possible score on this measure was 42. The
iner addresses specific obstacles to success that student responses correlation between scores on the 0-3 scale used in this study and
reveal. This study followed the standardized approach. I used a fixed scores on Mann et al.'s (1987) original 0-4 scale was .99. Although
set of prompts that were administered to all students in the same Mann et al. did not report information about the reliability of their
order. As can be seen above, success on initial prompts reflects need measure, I assessed the reliability of the scoring system by asking two
for minimal adult intervention, whereas success on later prompts judges to rate the same 10 protocols. Agreement between the two
indicates need for more extensive adult help in performing the task. scorers was 90%.
In Campione's taxonomy, target refers to the nature of the skills Word recognition. The San Diego Quick Assessment List (LaPray
that are tested. Early work on dynamic assessment (e.g., Budoff, 1967; & Ross, 1969) includes a series of graded word lists beginning at the
Feuerstein, 1979) focused on domain-general skills associated with preprimer level. The words on the lists are typical of words that
cognitive ability. Examples of tasks that have been studied include students encounter in instructional materials at different reading
Raven's (1956) Coloured Progressive Matrices, Kohs's (1923) Block levels. As is the case with most informal reading inventories, the
Design Task, and the Representational Stencil Design Task (Arthur, authors provided no information regarding reliability. Evidence of
1947). In the past few years, however, researchers have advocated use content validity is derived by examining the overlap between words
of academically relevant tasks (Bransford et al., 1987; Campione, on the test and words typically presented in basal reading material at
1989; Campione & Brown, 1987). Consistent with this recommen- different levels.
dation, the present study has as its target a domain-specific skill that Students in the present study were asked to read word lists arranged
influences reading acquisition. in order of increasing difficulty. Testing was discontinued when a
Phoneme deletion. Bruce's (1964) Word Analysis Test assesses student was unable to read any of the words on a particular list. In
the student's ability to delete phonemes from words. The test com- practice, only the preprimer, primer, and first-grade lists were used
prises 33 words that vary in length (30 are one-syllable words and 3 because of the limited reading skills of the kindergarten subjects. The
are multisyllable words). Bruce initially selected the words for their test was scored for number correct. The maximum score possible on
familiarity. The test requires children to say the word that remains each word list was 10, and the highest possible score across all three
after a particular sound is deleted from a stimulus word (e.g., "What lists was 30.
word would be left if / j / were taken away from the beginning of Prior to reading the graded word lists, students also completed a
jamV). The positions in the words of the sound to be deleted are letter recognition task that required them to state the names of 10
equally divided between beginning, middle, and end. In the present letters. Scores on letter recognition reflected the number of letters
study, children completed four sample items prior to the test. I that were correctly named.
provided corrective feedback on sample items only. Given the length Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R). The stu-
of the test and its difficulty for beginning kindergarteners, I discontin- dents' PPVT-R (Dunn & Dunn, 1981) results were available in school
ued testing if students were unsuccessful on the first 15 words and records. The test had been administered by school personnel in the
recorded a score of zero. Performance on the test was scored as month prior to the reading and phonemic awareness testing. The
number of words correct. PPVT-R, a test of receptive vocabulary, has frequently been used in
Invented spelling. Invented spelling requires students to represent research on phonemic awareness as a measure of verbal ability. I
in writing the sound structure of words. To assess this ability, I used included it in the present study to control for individual differences
a measure developed by Mann et al. (1987) in their study of the in verbal ability. At ages 5 years through 6 years, 11 months (the age
relationship between invented spelling and phonemic awareness. The levels at which subjects in the present study were tested), split-half
test requires students to write the following words: red, name, bed, reliability coefficients ranged from .73 to .84. Alternate-forms relia-
lady, fish, men, boat, girl, color, angry, thank you, people, dog, and bility is estimated to range from .78 to .80 and from .60 to .67 on
boy. Mann et al. selected these words for their familiarity to kinder- immediate test-retest and delayed test-retest, respectively (Dunn &
garten students and because each presents the opportunity for stu- Dunn, 1981).
dents to make "errors" that are characteristic of children who have
not yet learned how to read (e.g., "nam" for name, "ppl" for people).
Each item contains one or more of the following elements: (a) a letter Results
name within the word (e.g., "d" in lady) or (b) a short vowel, a nasal,
a liquid, or a consonant represented by a digraph (e.g., "e" in bed, The data were analyzed to investigate the predictive ability
"nk" in thank you, "r" and "1" in girl, "sh" in fish). Following Mann of the dynamic phoneme segmentation measure. I used or-
et al.'s instructions, I encouraged students to invent spellings for dinary least squares regression to identify the fall measure
words that they did not know and to write any of the sounds in a that best predicted spring phonemic awareness and spring
word if they did not know the whole word. reading. This approach required separate analyses, one for
Mann et al. (1987) scored student performance on the spelling task each of the four dependent measures administered in the
for phonetic equivalence. That is, they rated each response on a scale spring: phoneme segmentation, phoneme deletion, invented
to reflect its correspondence to the phonetic structure of the word: 4 spelling, and word recognition. The independent variables in
= correct spelling; 3 = preconventional response that accurately each analysis were the following: fall performance on pho-
represents the phonological structure of the word; 2 = preconven- neme segmentation (static and dynamic measures), phoneme
tional response of two or more letters that reflects at least part of the
deletion, invented spelling, and PPVT-R. I also used stepwise
phonological structure of the word; 1 = single-letter response that
represents the initial sound of the word; 0.5 = single-letter response regression to identify the contribution of the dynamic mea-
that represents a sound in the word other than the initial consonant; sure, beyond that explained by general ability (i.e., PPVT-R)
and 0 = no response or a response that does not represent any sounds and the static measure of phoneme segmentation. Alpha was
in the word. set at .05 throughout.
DYNAMIC ASSESSMENT 357

Descriptive Statistics supports the construct validity of the four phonemic aware-
ness measures (see Table 2).
Means and standard deviations for each measure are pre- I also examined the strength of the correlations among the
sented in Table 1. In the fall, most of the children had four phonemic awareness tasks. The correlations between the
considerable difficulty with the phoneme deletion and static dynamic measure and each of the three static measures were
phonemic segmentation tasks. Mean scores on phoneme dele- descriptively greater than the correlations among the three
tion indicated that the average student responded correctly to static measures themselves. That is, the dynamic measure
only 1 or 2 words; many students did not respond correctly appears to have more in common with each of the three static
to any of the items on the task. Examination of fall scores on measures of phonemic awareness than the three static meas-
the phoneme segmentation task suggests that although the ures have in common with each other.
average student correctly segmented only 1 or 2 words on the Given the focus of the study on prediction, I was particu-
test, the typical student was able to segment correctly at least larly interested in examining the simple correlations between
one sound for half of the words. Similarly, although most the fall and spring measures. There were significant positive
students failed to spell correctly any words on the spelling test correlations between fall and spring phonemic awareness
in the fall, the majority of students were able to produce letters measures and between phonemic awareness scores and spring
to represent at least one of the sounds in the words. reading performance (see Table 2). Among all fall measures-
Examination of fall and spring scores reveals that student including the PPVT-R—dynamic phoneme segmentation was
performance improved on all the phonemic awareness tasks the variable that correlated most highly with spring reading
over the course of the year. The increase in accuracy on scores and with spring performance on the phonemic aware-
phoneme segmentation was considerably greater than that for ness measures.
phoneme deletion. On average, students progressed from 12% A secondary question regarding fall-to-spring correlations
accuracy in the number of words segmented correctly in the concerned the relationship between benefit from the dynamic
fall to 50% accuracy in the spring on phoneme segmentation, assessment condition and growth in word recognition. That
whereas accuracy on phoneme deletion only increased from is, Was improvement in performance from static to dynamic
5% to 12%. assessment associated with gains in reading during the second
Scores on word recognition also improved from fall to half of kindergarten? I addressed this question using a measure
spring. In the fall, none of the students were able to identify of residualized gain that I created by regressing dynamic
any of the words on the preprimer, primer, orfirst-gradelists, phoneme segmentation on static phoneme segmentation
whereas by spring, only one third of the students still scored scores. The correlation between residualized gain and spring
zero on word recognition. The average student was able to word recognition indicated that gain from static to dynamic
identify 30% of the words on the preprimer list, and the assessment was, in fact, positively associated with growth in
highest scoring students were able to read all the words on the word recognition (r = .43, p < .01). In other words, students
preprimer list and 1 or 2 words on the primer list. who showed the most growth in word recognition from fall
to spring tended to be those who were helped most by the
prompts and cues provided during dynamic assessment. This
result is not surprising, given the high correlation between
Correlations Among Measures residualized gain and the original dynamic segmentation mea-
sure ( r = .78, p< .01).
Whereas students differed in their ability to perform each
of the phonemic awareness tasks, there were significant cor- Among the three static phonemic awareness measures (i.e.,
relations among all four of the fall phonemic awareness phoneme segmentation, phoneme deletion, and invented
measures (i.e., phoneme deletion, phoneme segmentation, spelling), spring scores correlated more highly with spring
invented spelling, and dynamic phoneme segmentation). The reading than did fall scores. This result might be explained by
phonemic measures generally correlated more highly with the temporal factor: One generally expects two measures given
each other than they did with the PPVT-R, a pattern that at the same time to correlate more highly with each other

Table 1
Mean Scores on Fall and Spring Measures
Fall Spring
Maximum
Measure score M SD M SD
PPVT-R 100.89 13.81
Phoneme segmentation
No. words correct 22 2.68 4.75 11.39 8.18
No. sounds correct 57 13.71 13.28
Phoneme deletion 33 1.58 2.96 3.55 5.68
Invented spelling 42 10.67 10.92 26.65 11.76
San Diego Quick Assessment List
Letter recognition 10 8.50 2.47 9.53 1.39
Word recognition 30 0.00 0.00 3.00 3.36
Dynamic phoneme segmentation 56 23.24 19.16 — —
Note. N = 38. PPVT-R = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Dashes indicate not available.
358 JANET E. SPECTOR

Table 2
Intercorrelations Among Fall and Spring Measures
Fall Spring
Measure
Fall
l.DYN .62** .43** .60** .11 51** .66** .54** .60**
2. PS — .37* .44** .09 .31* .62** .40*
3. PD .38*
— .16 .34* .34* .45** .28 .23
4. IS — .10 .41** .46** .49**
5. PPVT-R .55**
— .51** -.02 .39* .27*
Spring
6. PS .36* .61** .46**
7. PD — .37* .67**
8. IS
9. WR — .62**

Note. N==3S. DYN =» dynamic phoneme segmentation; PS = phoneme segmentation; PD = phoneme


deletion; IS = invented spelling; PPVT-R = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised- WR = San
Diego Quick Assessment List word recognition.
* p < .05, one-tailed. **p < .01, one-tailed.

than measures given at different times. The increase from fall ing, and PPVT-R. I entered fall measures into the regression
to spring in the magnitude of correlations between phonemic equation simultaneously. The results of the analysis are listed
awareness and reading might also result from the reciprocal in Table 3.
nature of the relationship between phonemic awareness and As can be seen in Table 3, the dynamic measure accounted
reading: Phonemic awareness facilitates reading acquisition, for a significant portion of the variance in two of the spring
and subsequent progress in reading itself promotes increased phonemic awareness tests. Along with the PPVT-R, it pre-
phonemic awareness. dicted spring phoneme segmentation. It also forecasted spring
However, these simple correlations do not take into account phoneme deletion, together with fall static segmentation and
the confounding influences among the phonemic awareness deletion. As reflected by the beta values in Table 3, for each
measures. Multiple regression analyses, described below, per- standard deviation increase in dynamic segmentation per-
mitted an estimate of the independent effects of each fall formance, scores on the spring dependent measures increased
variable, holding constant the effects of all remaining vari- by approximately one third of a standard deviation.
ables.
I also approached the analysis in a stepwise fashion. The
purpose of the second analysis was to examine the increment
Multiple Regression in explained variance associated with the entry of the dynamic
measure when PPVT-R and the static version of phoneme
Spring phonemic awareness. To investigate the ability of segmentation were already in the equation. I found that the
the dynamic measure to predict phonemic awareness in the dynamic measure accounted for an additional 12% to 14%
spring, each of the spring phonemic awareness variables (i.e., of the variance in the spring measures, even after entering
phoneme segmentation, phoneme deletion, and invented PPVT-R and the static measure of phoneme segmentation:
spelling) was separately regressed on fall phoneme segmenta- for spring phoneme segmentation, F(l, 34) = 9.27, p < .01;
tion (static and dynamic), phoneme deletion, invented spell- for spring phoneme deletion, F(l, 34) = 9.23, p < .01; and
Table 3
Multiple Regression With Fall Measures Used to Predict Spring Phonemic
Awareness Scores
Spring phonemic awareness measure
IS PD PS
Fall
predictor B B t B t
DYN .21 .34 1.68 .10 .33 1.84* .17 .40 2.09*
PPVT-R .29 .34 2.45** -.07 -.17 -1.36 .27 .46 3.41**
IS .24 .22 1.33 .06 .11 0.45 .11 .15 0.95
PD -.18 -.05 -0.30 .47 .25 1.75* .03 .01 0.07
PS .07 .08 0.49 .12 .29 1.92* -.03 -.05 -0.29
R1 .45 .56 .49
Note. N = 38. IS = invented spelling; PD = phoneme deletion. PS = phoneme segmentation- DYN =
dynamic phoneme segmentation; PPVT-R = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. For all t tests,
df= 32.
* p < .05, one-tailed. ** p< .01, one-tailed.
DYNAMIC ASSESSMENT 359

for spring invented spelling, F(l, 34) = 6.76, p < .05. In each The results of this analysis were almost indistinguishable
of the analyses, the increment in explained variance corre- from those depicted in Table 4: Gain from static to dynamic
sponds to roughly one third of the total amount of variance assessment was the best predictor of spring word recognition,
accounted for by all three independent variables combined. ((32) = 2.38, p < .01. Even after entering static phoneme
Spring reading performance. Two analyses assessed the segmentation and the PPVT-R in a stepwise fashion, dynamic
ability of the dynamic measure to predict spring reading gain accounted for an additional 21 % of the variance in spring
scores. I used the same approach as was reported above for reading scores, F{\, 34) = 11.84, p < .01, which is approxi-
examining spring phonemic awareness. That is, I first entered mately one half the variance accounted for by all three meas-
fall scores into the equation simultaneously to estimate the ures combined.
contribution of each independent variable, holding constant
the effects of the other predictors. In this analysis, independent Discussion
variables were fall phoneme segmentation (static and dynamic
versions), phoneme deletion, invented spelling, and PPVT-R. The results of the present study support the hypothesis that
I then used stepwise regression to investigate the unique dynamic assessment enhances the predictive utility of a mea-
contribution of the dynamic measure, above and beyond that sure of phonemic awareness. Dynamic phoneme segmenta-
of general verbal ability (i.e., PPVT-R) and static segmenta- tion was a better predictor of kindergarten reading progress
tion performance. In both analyses, the dependent measure than any of the three static measures of phonemic awareness:
was word recognition. phoneme segmentation, phoneme deletion, and invented
As can be seen in Table 4, performance on the dynamic spelling. The dynamic measure was also a better predictor of
version of the phoneme segmentation task was the only word recognition than the PPVT-R, a measure that is often
significant predictor of word recognition performance at the used to estimate verbal ability. Similarly, the dynamic mea-
end of kindergarten. For each standard deviation increase in sure accounted for more variance in spring phonemic aware-
performance on the dynamic measure, end-of-year reading ness than did any of the other fall phonemic awareness
performance increased by approximately one half standard measures. These results demonstrate the applicability of the
deviation (/? = .47). principles of dynamic assessment to measurement of pho-
The second analysis found that performance on the dy- nemic awareness and add to the ever-growing corpus of
namic measure accounted for an additional 21 % of the vari- research on the relationship between phonemic awareness
ance in word recognition scores after entering PPVT-R and and reading acquisition.
the static phoneme segmentation measure, F(l, 34) = 11.84,
p < .01. The increment in explained variance corresponds to Interpretive Considerations
roughly one half of the total variance accounted for by all
three independent variables combined. Two interpretive issues are considered: (a) the influence of
The dynamic measure in the previous analyses indicates the distribution of word recognition scores on the results
degree of independence achieved during dynamic assessment. reported above and (b) possible explanations for the effective-
To further investigate the relationship between responsiveness ness of the dynamic measure.
to dynamic assessment and growth in reading, I repeated the Distribution of scores on word recognition. In the spring,
regression of spring word recognition on fall phoneme aware- 13 students were still unable to read any words on the word
ness variables and the PPVT-R, substituting a measure of recognition test. Conceptually, this result is logical; the stu-
residualized gain (described previously) for the original dy- dents who did not progress in word recognition were also the
namic phoneme segmentation variable. lowest scorers on dynamic segmentation. Their lack of growth
in word recognition thus supports the diagnostic value of the
dynamic measure.
Statistically, however, the distribution of word recognition
Table 4 scores raises the possibility of spuriously high correlations
Multiple Regression With Fall Measures Used to Predict between word recognition and dynamic phoneme segmenta-
End-of-Year Word Recognition Scores tion. To address this concern, I recomputed the correlation
End-of-year word between fall dynamic phoneme segmentation and spring word
recognition recognition, eliminating from analysis the 13 students who
Fall measure B t scored zero on word recognition. Although the correlation
dropped from .60 to .43 in the reduced sample (n = 25), the
Dynamic phoneme segmentation .08 .47 2.37*
PPVT-R .05 .22 1.63 magnitude of the latter coefficient remains consistent with
Invented spelling .08 .27 1.64 my original conclusion regarding the positive relationship
Phoneme deletion -.10 -.09 -0.56 between dynamic phoneme segmentation and word recogni-
Phoneme segmentation -.01 -.02 -0.10 tion.
R2 .46 I also replicated the word-recognition regression analysis
reported above, using a combined measure of letter and word
Note. N = 38. PPVT-R = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Re-
vised. Word recognition was measured with the San Diego Quick recognition. The dependent measure in this analysis reflected
Assessment List word recognition task. For all t tests, df= 32. number correct across both the letter and word recognition
• p< .05, one-tailed. subtests of the San Diego Quick Assessment List (the distri-
360 JANET E. SPECTOR

bution of scores on this variable was approximately normal). segmentation, some students may have had difficulty main-
Because the majority of subjects were at ceiling on letter taining the target word in working memory while relating in
recognition—30 students scored 10 (100%) and 6 subjects order the sounds that they heard. In the dynamic condition,
scored 9 (90%)—the combined measure served only to spread on the other hand, the children were asked to identify the
out the scores of the 13 students who scored zero on word initial sound only and were then asked for each succeeding
recognition. The regression results for the combined letter- sound, prompting each response by repeating sounds that the
word recognition measure were equivalent to those obtained child had already given. The dynamic condition clearly placed
for word recognition: Dynamic phoneme segmentation was fewer demands on working memory than did the static seg-
the single best predictor of letter-word recognition, /(32) = mentation and deletion tests.
2.67, p < .01, 0 = .51, followed by PPVT-R, t(32) = 2.17, p The predictive superiority of the dynamic measure over
< .05, p = .28, and invented spelling, f(32) = 1.78, p < .05, invented spelling might also be explained by demands placed
one-tailed, p = .28. on ancillary skills. Whereas 80% of the students were able in
In short, the results reported above do not appear to be an the fall to identify most of the letters on the San Diego Quick
artifact of the distribution of scores on word recognition. Even Assessment List letter recognition task, they might have been
when problematic cases were removed or when alternate considerably less knowledgeable regarding the relationship
versions of a reading recognition measure were used, the between graphemes and phonemes. Also, they might not have
dynamic measure consistently emerged as the best predictor known how to form letters correctly. In other words, the
of reading progress. dynamic measure, in contrast to the static measures, might
Explanations for dynamic effects. What accounts for the have minimized the contribution to successful performance
predictive superiority of the dynamic phoneme segmentation of abilities other than phonemic awareness. Unfortunately, I
measure? Embretson (1987) described three possible goals of did not assess students' knowledge of individual letter-sound
dynamic assessment: "(a) improving ability estimates; (b) correspondences or ask them to write the alphabet, so addi-
assessing new constructs, such as modifiability of perform- tional research is necessary to test the validity of this expla-
ance; and (c) improving true ability" (p. 167). Embretson's nation.
framework is useful in delineating plausible explanations for The second possible interpretation of the present results is
the effectiveness of the dynamic assessment measure used in that the dynamic measure assessed a new ability: modifiability
this study. The dynamic measure (a) provided a better esti- of performance. As discussed above, the rationale underlying
mate of phonemic segmentation ability than did the static many approaches to dynamic assessment is the Vygotskian
measures of phonemic awareness; (b) assessed a different notion of zone of proximal development. Vygotsky (1935/
ability than did the static measure (i.e., responsiveness to 1978) used this term to refer to that area just beyond the
phonemic awareness training); and (c) increased students' child's present capabilities on tests requiring independent
phonemic awareness. I examine below the tenability of each performance but within the child's capability when given
possibility. adult assistance or support.
The first explanation is that the dynamic measure was According to Vygotsky (1935/1978), dynamic measures
simply a more sensitive indicator of phonemic awareness than allow observation of
any of the other three phonemic awareness measures. Exam-
ination of the distribution of scores on phoneme deletion
provides some support for this interpretation. More specifi- those functions that have not yet matured but are in the process
cally, a significant floor effect compromised the ability of this of maturation, functions that will mature tomorrow but are in
measure to predict spring reading performance. In the fall, the embryonic stage. These functions could be called the "buds"
many students were unable to respond correctly to any of the or "flowers" rather than the fruits of development. The actual
developmental level characterizes mental development retro-
items on the task. The dynamic measure had the effect of spectively, while the zone of proximal development characterizes
spreading out the scores of those who scored zero on phoneme mental development prospectively. (pp. 86-87)
deletion.
At the same time, it is unlikely that the predictive strength
of the dynamic measure was merely an artifact of the ex- In the present study, those students who benefited most
panded scaling technique. Both invented spelling and fall from the instructional cues and prompts during the dynamic
static phoneme segmentation (scored for number of sounds testing session were also the students who experienced the
correctly segmented) were nondichotomous scales that most growth in word recognition during the kindergarten
awarded credit for partially correct responses. Constrained year. From a Vygotskian perspective, the effectiveness of the
variance clearly was not an issue for these two measures, yet dynamic measure can be explained in terms of modifiability
dynamic phoneme segmentation exceeded them both in fore- of performance. That is, the dynamic measure assessed indi-
casting growth in reading. vidual differences in responsiveness to segmentation instruc-
A more plausible version of the sensitivity account is that tion.
the dynamic measure was simply a "cleaner" (Calfee, 1977, The above interpretation, although attractive, requires fur-
p. 297) measure of phonemic awareness. Analysis of the static ther investigation. Although the present results are congruent
conditions suggests the possibility that task demands unrelated with this account, the study was not designed specifically to
to phonemic awareness prevented many students from achiev- investigate why dynamic measures have greater predictive
ing greater success. For example, on both deletion and static validity than comparable static measures. In subsequent stud-
DYNAMIC ASSESSMENT 361

ies researchers might consider use of structural equation mod- account, which is the Vygotskian interpretation, proposes
eling, an approach recommended by Embretson (1987) for that, in assessing responsiveness to instruction, the dynamic
testing hypotheses about the locus of dynamic effects. Em- measure is better able to forecast future growth in segmenta-
bretson's design specifications include both fall and spring tion ability. Thus, a goal for subsequent research on dynamic
testing on (a) a dynamic measure, (b) a transfer task (e.g., assessment is to disentangle what appear to be equally plau-
word recognition), and (c) an irrelevant task (i.e., a task that sible interpretations of its effectiveness.
has elements in common with the dynamic task but that does
not tap the construct that the dynamic measure is hypothe-
sized to influence). This design would facilitate more complete Implications for Research and Practice
evaluation of the modifiability hypothesis.
The third possible explanation for the present results is that Clearly, there are unresolved issues surrounding dynamic
participation in dynamic assessment actually changed the assessment. For example, How does one assess the reliability
child's readiness. That is, in teaching nonsegmenters how to of a dynamic measure? By its nature, dynamic assessment
segment, the study effected long-term growth in phonemic induces change in the individual. Such change, in turn, re-
awareness and reading. I found this explanation attractive duces reliability as conceptualized by classical test theory (i.e.,
initially, given the increase in number of words segmented consistency or stability of measurement over time and items).
correctly from fall to spring. Mean performance on phoneme In her review of the psychometric challenges posed by dy-
segmentation rose from 12% accuracy in the fall to 50% namic assessment, Embretson (1987) provided some sugges-
accuracy in the spring, whereas accuracy on phoneme deletion tions for assessing the reliability and validity of dynamic
only increased from 5% to 12%. On the other hand, the measures. More research is needed to document the effective-
relative improvement in phoneme segmentation over pho- ness of the recommended procedures and to establish stand-
neme deletion is explained equally well by the demands of ards for evaluating the technical adequacy of dynamic meas-
the phoneme deletion and segmentation tasks themselves. As ures.
other investigators have reported, phoneme deletion is a more With respect to the dynamic measure developed for use in
complex skill and one that develops later than phoneme the present study, further investigation is necessary to rule out
segmentation (e.g., Perfetti et al., 1987; Yopp, 1988). In other the possibility that the results were influenced by the order in
words, the greater degree of improvement in segmentation which the tasks were administered. Subsequent research
ability can be easily explained by developmental factors. The should also consider repeating the dynamic procedure in the
results do not necessarily imply that participation in dynamic spring to permit more complete analysis of spring-to-spring
assessment effected long-term improvement in phoneme seg- correlations.
mentation ability. Although the dynamic measure developed for use in this
The training effects hypothesis is also called into question study requires further scrutiny, the study suggests a number
by the results of experimental studies that have attempted to of practical applications. One potential application is devel-
teach segmentation skills. First, previous training studies sug- opment of a screening device to identify children in need of
gest that segmentation training is most effective when it is instructional intervention to facilitate reading acquisition.
accompanied by training in blending or spelling (Bradley & Although no test identified in this or any previous study has
Bryant, 1983; Fox & Routh, 1984; Williams, 1980; also see accounted for enough variance in reading achievement to
discussions by Adams, 1990, pp. 328-332; Ehri, 1989; and stand alone as a screening measure, the dynamic measure of
Wagner & Torgesen, 1987). Therefore, the present condition phonemic awareness developed for purposes of this study has
was not optimal for inducing long-term change. Second, advantages over previous measures. First, it is a better predic-
studies that have yielded successful results for phonemic tor of reading progress. Second, in allowing the examiner to
awareness training have taken place over longer periods of observe the child's responsiveness to instructional prompts
time with more intensive treatment. Bradley and Bryant and cues, the measure might be useful in identifying an
(1983), for example, provided 40 training sessions over a 2- appropriate instructional approach to develop phonemic
year period (once or twice per week). Lundberg, Frost, and awareness in each child. Finally, the present procedure does
Petersen (1988) carried out their phonemic awareness training not require extensive training on the part of the examiner and
program over the entire kindergarten year, whereas Fox and can be administered in one 15- to 20-min session. For similar
Routh (1984) conducted training sessions 4 to 5 days a week reasons, the measure would also be valuable in research on
(15 to 20 min) for 5 weeks. In contrast, a study that failed to reading and phonemic awareness in which predictive strength
yield a main effect of segmentation-and-blending training was and ease of administration are critical.
conducted by Treiman and Baron (1983). The training took The results of this study also suggest the benefits of devel-
place over a 4-day period, an amount of time that is more oping dynamic measures for other phonemic awareness tasks,
similar to (but still four times greater than) the intervention such as invented spelling, or even for the criterion measure,
in this study. word recognition. Although the present findings support the
I conclude, therefore, that the sensitivity and modifiability predictive validity of the dynamic phoneme segmentation
explanations are more tenable than the training effects hy- measure, it is possible that a dynamic measure of one of the
pothesis. The sensitivity account suggests that the dynamic other phonemic awareness measures (e.g., invented spelling)
measure was a better predictor because it is a cleaner (or would prove to be an even better predictor of early reading
truer) measure of phonemic awareness. The modifiability progress.
362 JANET E. SPECTOR

References early reading (Vol 2, pp. 43-65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.


Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-
print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Arthur, G. (1947). A point scale of performance tests. New York: Ehri, L. C. (1979). Linguistic insight: Threshold of reading acquisi-
Psychological Corporation. tion. In T. G. Waller & G. E. Mackinnon (Eds.), Reading research:
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 1, pp. 63-114). San Diego,
to read—A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-421. CA: Academic Press.
Bransford, J. C, Delclos, V. R., Vye, N. J., Burns, M. S., & Hassel- Ehri, L. C. (1989). The development of spelling knowledge and its
bring, T. S. (1987). State of the art and future directions. In C. S. role in reading acquisition and reading disability. Journal ofLearn-
Lidz (Ed.), Dynamic assessment: An interactional approach to ing Disabilities, 22, 356-365.
evaluating learning potential (pp. 479-496). New York: Guilford Ehri, L. C, & Wilce, L. S. (1980). The influence of orthography on
Press. readers' conceptualizations of the phonetic structure of words.
Bruce, D. J. (1964). An analysis of word sounds by young children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 371-385.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 34, 158-170. Ehri, L. C, & Wilce, L. S. (1987a). Cipher versus cue reading: An
Budoff, M. (1967). Learning potential among institutionalized young experiment in decoding acquisition. Journal of Educational Psy-
adult retardates. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 72, 404- chology, 79, 3-13.
411. Ehri, L. C, & Wilce, L. S. (1987b). Does learning to spell help
Budoff, M. (1987a). Measures for assessing learning potential. In C. beginners learn to read words? Reading Research Quarterly, 18,
S. Lidz (Ed.), Dynamic assessment: An interactional approach to 47-65.
evaluation learning potential (pp. 173-195). New York: Guilford Elkonin, D. B. (1973). U.S.S.R. In J. Downing (Ed.), Comparative
Press. reading (pp. 551-579). New York: Macmillan.
Budoff, M. (1987b). The validity of learning potential assessment. In Embretson, S. E. (1987). Toward development of a psychometric
C. S. Lidz (Ed.), Dynamic assessment: An interactional approach approach. In C. S. Lidz (Ed.), Dynamic assessment: An interactional
to evaluating learning potential (pp. 52-81). New York: Guilford approach to evaluating learning potential (pp. 141 -170). New York:
Press. Guilford Press.
Burns, M. S. (1985). Comparison of "graduated prompt" and "media- Ferrara, R. A, Brown, A. L., & Campione, J. C. (1986). Children's
tional" dynamic assessment and static assessment with young chil- learning and transfer of inductive reasoning rules: Studies in prox-
dren (Tech. Rep. No. 2). Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University, imal development. Child Development, 57, 1087-1099.
John F. Kennedy Center for Research on Human Development. Feuerstein, R. (1979). The dynamic assessment of retarded perform-
Calfee, R. C. (1977). Assessment of independent reading skills: Basic ers: The Learning Potential Assessment Device, theory, instruments,
research and practical applications. In A. S. Reber & D. L. Scar- and techniques. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
borough (Eds.), Toward a psychology of reading: Proceedings ofthe Fox, B., & Routh, D. K. (1975). Analyzing spoken language into
City University of New York conferences (pp. 289-323). Hillsdale, words, syllables, and phonemes: A developmental study. Journal
NJ: Erlbaum. of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 331-342.
Calfee, R. C, Chapman, R. S., & Venezky, R. L. (1972). How a child Fox, B., & Routh, D. K. (1984). Phonemic analysis and synthesis as
needs to think to learn how to read. In L. Gregg (Ed.), Cognition word attack skills: Revisited. Journal of Educational Psychology,
in learning and memory (pp. 139-182). New York: Wiley. 76, 1059-1064.
Calfee, R. C, Lindamood, P., & Lindamood, C. (1973). Acoustic- Goldstein, D. M. (1976). Cognitive-linguistic functioning and learn-
phonetic skills and reading—Kindergarten through twelfth grade. ing to read in preschoolers. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 68,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 293-298. 680-688.
Campione, J. C. (1989). Assisted assessment: A taxonomy of ap- Golinkoff, R. M. (1978). Critique: Phonemic awareness skills and
proaches and an outline of strengths and weaknesses. Journal oj reading achievement. In F. B. Murray & J. J. Pikulski (Eds.), The
Learning Disabilities, 22, 151-165. acquisition of reading: Cognitive, linguistic, and perceptual prereq-
Campione, J. C, & Brown, A. L. (1985). Dynamic assessment: One uisites (pp. 23-41). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
approach and some initial data (Tech. Rep. No. 361). Urbana, IL: Gough, P., & Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural
Center for the Study of Reading. (ERIC Document Reproduction act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 179-196.
Service No. ED 269 735) Helfgott, J. A. (1976). Phonemic segmentation and blending skills of
Campione, J. C, & Brown, A. L. (1987). Linking dynamic assessment kindergarten children: Implications for beginning reading acquisi-
with school achievement. In C. S. Lidz (Ed.), Dynamic assessment: tion. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1, 157-169.
An interactional approach to evaluating learning potential (pp. 82- Jorm, A. F., & Share, D. L. (1983). Phonological recodingand reading
115). New York: Guilford Press. acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 103-147.
Campione, J. C, Brown, A. L., Ferrara, R. A., Jones, R., & Steinberg, Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of
E. (1985). Breakdowns in flexible use of information: Intelligence- 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational
related differences in transfer following equivalent learning per- Psychology, 80, 437-447.
formance. Intelligence, 9, 297-315. Kohs, S. C. (1923). Intelligence measurement. New York: Macmillan.
Carlson, J. S., & Wiedl, K. H. (1978). Use of testing-the-limits LaPray, M., & Ross, R. (1969). The graded word list: Quick gauge of
procedures in the assessment of intellectual capabilities in children reading ability. Journal of Reading, 12, 305-307.
with learning difficulties. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, Lewkowicz, N. K. (1980). Phonemic awareness training: What to
82, 529-564. teach and how to teach it. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72,
Carlson, J. S., & Wiedl, K. H. (1979). Toward a differential testing 686-700.
approach: Testing-the-limits employing the Raven matrices. Intel- Liberman, I. Y. (1973). Segmentation of the spoken word and reading
ligence, 3, 323-344. acquisition. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 23, 65-77.
Chomsky, C. (1971). Write first, read later. Childhood Education, 47, Liberman, I. Y., Rubin, H., Duques, S., & Carlisle, J. (1985). Lin-
296-299. guistic abilities and spelling proficiency in kindergarteners and
Chomsky, C. (1979). Approaching reading through invented spelling. adult poor spellers. In D. B. Gray & J. F. Kavanagh (Eds.),
In L. B. Resnick & P. A. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of Biobehavioral measures of dyslexia (pp. 163-176). Parkton, MD:
DYNAMIC ASSESSMENT 363

reading II: The reading process and the acquisition of the alphabetic
York Press.
principle. In A. Reber & D. Scarborough (Eds.), Toward a psy-
Liberman, I. Y., & Shankweiler, D. (1985). Phonology and the
chology of reading (pp. 55-141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
problems of learning to read and write. Remedial and Special
Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984).
Education, 6, 8-17.
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W., & Carter, B. (1974). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of
Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Educational Psychology, 76, 1309-1324.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201-212. Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Cramer, B. B. (1984).
Lidz, C. S. (1981). Improving assessment of schoolchildren. San Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Lidz, C. S. (Ed.). (1987). Dynamic assessment: An interactional 29, 175-190.
approach to evaluating learning potential. New York: Guilford Treiman, R., & Baron, J. (1983). Phonemic-analysis training helps
Press. children benefit from spelling-sound rules. Memory & Cognition,
Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive 11, 382-389.
program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool chil- Tunmer, W. E., & Nesdale, A. R. (1985). Phonemic segmentation
dren. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263-284. skill and beginning reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77,
Mann, V. A., Tobin, P., & Wilson, R. (1987). Measuring phonological Ail-All.
awareness through the invented spellings of kindergarten children. Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding,
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 365-391. phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a lon-
Morris, D., & Perney, J. (1984). Developmental spelling as a predictor gitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33,
of first-grade achievement. The Elementary School Journal, 84, 321-363.
441-457. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Interaction between learning and develop-
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University ment. In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman
Press. (Eds. and Trans.), Mind in society: The development of higher
Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L. C , & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic psychological processes (pp. 79-91). Cambridge, MA: Harvard
knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study University Press. (Original work published 1935).
offirst-gradechildren. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283-319. Wagner, R. K.., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological
Raven, J. C. (1956). Coloured progressive matrices. New York: Psy- processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills.
chological Corporation. Psychological Bulletin, 10, 192-212.
Read, C. (1971). Pre-school children's knowledge of English phonol- Williams, J. P. (1980). Teaching decoding with an emphasis on
ogy. Harvard Educational Review, 41, 1-34. phoneme analysis and phoneme blending. Journal of Educational
Read, C. (1986). Children's creative spellings. New York: Routledge, Psychology, 72, 1-15.
Chapman & Hall. Williams, J. P. (1984). Phonemic analysis and how it relates to
Rosner, J., & Simon, D. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 240-245.
report. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 384-392. Yopp, H. K. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic aware-
Rozin, P., & Gleitman, L. (1977). The structure and acquisition of ness tests. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 159-177.

Appendix
Dynamic Assessment Task
Introductory Instructions to Students If incorrect or no response: "Try to tell me just a little bit
"Do you remember the game we played last week with sounds? I of the word."
said a word and asked you to break the word apart. You had to tell If child still does not isolate first sound, skip Prompts 3
me each sound that you heard in the word. When I said 'old,' you and 4. Go to Prompt 5.
had to say 'o-l-d.' Let's try a few more." Prompt 3: If child correctly identified first sound but not next
sound(s):
General Procedures " is the first sound in "
Begin with Item No. 1 ("say") and continue through Item No. 12. "What sound comes next?"
Whenever a child is unable to segment a word correctly, move the "Now can you tell me each sound?"
child through the prompts (in order) until he or she produces the Prompt 4: "There are 2 [or 3] sounds in
correct segmentation. For example, if on Item No. 4, the child What are they?"
achieves success on Prompt 2, move next to Item No. 5. If the child Prompt 5: "Watch me." Model segmentation of word: Place a token
cannot segment Item No. 5 on the first attempt, move the child in a square as each sound is spoken, then repeat word as
through the prompts again, beginning with Prompt 1. Discontinue a whole. After demo say the following: "Try to do what I
testing after the first 2 items if a child does not achieve success in just did."
segmenting on either Item No. 1 or Item No. 2. Remember, a child Score response as correct if child can imitate correct
is considered successful if he or she responds correctly to any of the segmentation.
prompts (including the 3 prompts that require imitation only). Prompt 6: "Let's try together." Model segmentation of word with
child. Work hand-over-hand with child and ask child to
Prompts
pronounce segments along with you.
Prompt 1: "Listen while I say the word very slowly." Model slow "Now try to do it yourself. Do what we just did together."
pronunciation. Prompt 7: Model again with child. "Now try again to do it yourself."
"Now can you tell me each sound?"
Prompt 2: "What's the first sound you hear in ?" Received March 4, 1991
If first sound is correct: "Now can you tell me each of the Revision received January 8, 1992
sounds?" Accepted January 27, 1992 •

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen