Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

Original Paper

Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 Received: January 18, 2011


Accepted after revision: June 17, 2011
DOI: 10.1159/000330258
Published online: March 2, 2012

Identity and Eating Disorders (IDEA):


A Questionnaire Evaluating Identity and
Embodiment in Eating Disorder Patients
Giovanni Stanghellini a Giovanni Castellini b Patrizia Brogna c Carlo Faravelli d
Valdo Ricca b
a
Department of Biomedical Sciences, University ‘G. d’Annunzio’, Chieti, b Psychiatric Unit, Department of
Neuropsychiatric Science, Florence University School of Medicine, Florence, c Institute of Psychology, University
‘Cattolica del Sacro Cuore’, Rome, and d Department of Psychology, University of Florence, Florence, Italy

Key Words through the evaluation of the other’, ‘feeling oneself only
Eating disorders ⴢ Embodiment ⴢ Identity ⴢ through objective measures’, ‘feeling extraneous from one’s
Psychopathology ⴢ Questionnaire own body’, and ‘feeling oneself through starvation’. Conclu-
sions: IDEA represents a multidimensional, brief, versatile,
easy-to-perform instrument for clinical evaluation, assessing
Abstract abnormalities in lived corporeality, and of personal identity,
Background: In this paper we tested the hypothesis that which appeared to be specifically associated with the core
persons with eating disorders (EDs) are affected by distur- features of ED psychopathology. The main limitations of the
bances of the way they experience their own body (embodi- study are the cross-sectional design. Also, it is impossible to
ment) and shape their personal identity, assuming that the ascertain whether the domains we assessed are specific
various kinds of anomalies of eating behavior are conse- traits of patients with EDs, or state-related features. To an-
quences thereof. Sampling and Methods: We developed swer this question, a longitudinal study is needed.
and validated a new self-reported questionnaire named Copyright © 2012 S. Karger AG, Basel
IDEA (IDentity and EAting disorders), which was adminis-
tered to 147 ED patients and 187 healthy controls. Test-retest
reliability, internal consistency, psychopathological corre- Introduction
lates, and concurrent validity were evaluated. A factor analy-
sis was performed to verify the distribution of items into sub- Eating disorders (EDs) are severe psychiatric syn-
scales. Results: The questionnaire showed good test-retest dromes that most likely result from, and are sustained by,
reliability, and internal consistency. IDEA scores were spe- sociocultural, psychological and biological factors [1, 2].
cifically associated with ED psychopathology, and they did DSM IV-TR [3] divides EDs into three diagnostic catego-
not show any correlation with sociodemographic and gen- ries: anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa and eating disor-
eral clinical variables. Four factors were extracted, which ders not otherwise specified, a heterogeneous category
were related to the following phenomena: ‘feeling oneself considering those EDs that fail to meet the diagnostic cri-
only through the gaze of the other and defining oneself only teria for either anorexia nervosa or bulimia nervosa and

© 2012 S. Karger AG, Basel Giovanni Stanghellini MD, DHC


0254–4962/12/0453–0147$38.00/0 Department of Biomedical Sciences, University ‘G. d’Annunzio’
Fax +41 61 306 12 34 Via dei Vestini, 31
E-Mail karger@karger.ch Accessible online at: IT–66013 Chieti (Italy)
www.karger.com www.karger.com/psp Tel. +39 0871 355 4040, E-Mail giostan @ libero.it
that includes binge eating disorder [4]. These disorders ‘facticity’, including oneself as ‘this’ body, its form, height,
are characterized by abnormal patterns of eating behav- weight, color, as well as one’s past and what is actually
ior, and pathological concerns about body shape and happening. First and foremost, we have an implicit ac-
weight. In particular, Fairburn and Harrison [5] hold that quaintance with our own body from the first-person per-
patients with EDs typically overvalue their body shape spective. The lived body turns into a physical, objective
and weight. By that they mean that whereas most people body whenever we become aware of it in a disturbing way.
evaluate and define themselves on the basis of the way Whenever our movement is somehow impeded or dis-
they perceive their performance in various domains (e.g. rupted, then the lived body is thrown back on itself, ma-
relationships, work, parenting, sporting prowess, etc.), terialized or ’corporealized’. It becomes an object for me.
‘patients with anorexia nervosa or bulimia nervosa judge Having been a living bodily being before, I now realize
their self-worth largely, or even exclusively, in terms of that I have a material (impeding, clumsy, vulnerable, fi-
their shape and weight and their ability to control them’ nite, etc.) body [27]. In addition to these two dimensions
(p. 407). They also suggest that most of the other features of corporeality, Sartre emphasized that one can appre-
of these disorders (including behavioral anomalies such hend one’s own body also from another vantage point, as
as starvation) could be secondary epiphenomena to this one’s own body when it is looked at by another person.
more profound pathological core, i.e. an overestimation When I become aware that I, or better that my own body
of the importance of bodily appearance instead of other is looked at by another person, I realize that my body can
aspects of existence and personal identity. According to be an object for that person. Sartre calls this the ‘lived-
this theory, different studies found that severe concerns body-for-others’. ‘With the appearance of the Other’s
about body shape and weight can play a significant role look’, writes Sartre, ‘I experience the revelation of my be-
in the persistence of EDs [6–11]. Furthermore, shape and ing-as-object.’ The upshot of this is a feeling of ‘having
weight concerns have been associated with different re- my being outside (…) [the feeling] of being an object’.
sponses to psychological treatment in several reports [12– Thus, one’s identity becomes reified by the gaze of the
15]. other, and reduced to the external appearance of one’s
In this paper, we build on and extend Fairburn and own body.
Harrison’s idea, as well as other reports [16–22], and es- We propose that the reason why persons with EDs
pecially we develop the hypothesis that persons with EDs overvalue their body shape and weight can be better un-
are affected by a more profound disturbance consisting derstood as a specific disorder of lived corporeality, and
in disorders of the way they experience their own body more specifically as the predominance of one dimension
(embodiment) and shape their personal identity, assum- of embodiment, namely the ‘lived-body-for-others’. The
ing that the various kinds of anomalies of eating behavior idea that anomalies of embodiment can play a key-role in
are a consequence thereof. Patients with EDs are charac- conceptualizing mental disorders is not new. It has been
terized by an excessive concern for their own appearance applied in various domains, including schizophrenia [28,
[3, 23]. In order to better understand their disproportion- 29], manic-depressive disorders [30] and body dysmor-
ate worry for their own exteriority, we draw on philoso- phic disorder [31]. Our hypothesis is that persons with
pher Jean Paul Sartre’s [24] analyses of lived corporeality. EDs experience their own body first and foremost as an
Since the beginning of the 20th Century, phenomenology object being looked at by another, rather than cenesthet-
has developed a distinction between lived body (Leib) and ically or from a first-person perspective. Alienation from
physical body (Koerper), or body-subject and body-ob- one’s own body and from one’s own emotions, disgust for
ject. The first is the body experienced from within, my it, shame, and an exaggerated concern to take responsi-
own direct experience of my body in the first-person per- bility for the way one appears to the others, as well as the
spective, myself as a spatiotemporal embodied agent in possibility to feel oneself only through the gaze of the oth-
the world; the second is the body thematically investi- ers, through objective measures and through self-starva-
gated from without, as for example by natural sciences as tion, i.e. many of the features of persons with EDs, can be
anatomy and physiology, a third-person perspective [25, illuminated by looking at it in the light of the Sartrean
26]. One’s own body can be apprehended by a person in concept of feeling a ‘lived-body-for-others’.
the first-person perspective as the body-I-am. This is the In order to test this hypothesis, we developed and val-
cenesthetic apprehension of one’s own body, the primi- idated a new self-reported questionnaire named IDEA
tive experience of oneself, the basic form of self-aware- (IDentity and EAting disorders), which was administered
ness, or the direct, unmediated experience of one’s own to a large sample of patients with EDs, as well as controls.

148 Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 Stanghellini /Castellini /Brogna /Faravelli /


       

Ricca 
Materials and Methods – This iterative process resulted in a developed set of 61 sentenc-
es, in such a way that they could be applied to a wide popula-
Data were collected at the Outpatient Clinic for Eating Disor- tion of ED patients including all the principal diagnoses (an-
ders of the Psychiatric Unit of the University of Florence, Italy. orexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, and binge eating disorder),
regardless of age, sex, familial status, occupation, or education
Development of the Questionnaire level. For each question, the patient is asked to score on a
The Italian version of the questionnaire was developed with 5-point Likert scale (0: do not agree, 4: strongly agree with the
the contributions of several psychiatrists and psychologists, tak- sentence).
ing in consideration the patients’ attitudes toward their lived cor- – Considering that the areas mentioned above were only based
poreality, and the definition of their own identity. on our theoretical model, we effectively verified the structure
The steps for the development of the questionnaire were as fol- of the questionnaire by a an exploratory factor analysis.
lows: – The first version of the questionnaire was administered to a se-
– The generation of a first list of questions was conducted in dif- ries of 221 ED patients being referred for the first time to the
ferent phases, including domain generation, and item genera- Outpatient Clinic for Eating Disorders. Prior to factorization,
tion. To develop domains, experts in ED research and treat- we explored the frequency distribution of each item, and an
ment, together with researchers with a phenomenological item analysis was performed, including difficulty index, and
background, identified areas that were most important and discrimination index. An exploratory factor analysis (with Pro-
impacted ED patients. The theoretical background of the max rotation) was applied, showing that 4 main factors should
questionnaire was an attempt of integrating the phenomeno- be retained. Items that showed a weak unrotated correlation
logical approach of lived corporeality described in the Intro- (r ! 0.30) with all the four factors were excluded. On the basis
duction with the clinical one, based on the cognitive behav- of these initial analyses, the number of items was reduced, and
ioral model. a shorter version of the questionnaire was developed (23 items).
The experts were specifically asked to think about the concept The complete final version of the questionnaire, together with
of lived corporeality, and more specifically the ‘lived-body-for- the scoring system, is reported in the Appendix. The total IDEA
others’, as it applies across a broad range of ED patients. They were score is obtained as the mean of all item scores.
also asked to make use of their familiarity with ED research as
well as their clinical experience. Subjects
The items were conceptually related to different areas: The final version of the questionnaire was evaluated in a series
(1) feeling oneself through the gaze of the other; of ED patients attending the Outpatient Clinic for Eating Disor-
(2) defining oneself through the evaluation of the other; ders of the Psychiatric Unit of the University of Florence between
(3) feeling oneself through objective measures; October 1, 2009, and June 30, 2010, and in a group of healthy con-
(4) feeling extraneous from one’s own body; trol subjects. Patients involved in the development of the IDEA
(5) feeling oneself through starvation; questionnaire did not participate in the present study. Patients
(6) defining one’s identity through one’s own body; were enrolled according to the following inclusion criteria: female
(7) feeling oneself through physical activity and fatigue. gender, age 18–40 years, current DSM-IV diagnosis of anorexia
– Then, items meant to tap each of these content areas were gen- nervosa, bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder. The diagnoses
erated. The most commonly voiced identity concerns and be- were based on the current symptomatology at referral.
liefs were considered on the basis of the clinical experience, Exclusion criteria were as follows: illiteracy, intellectual dis-
statements identified in the literature as particularly associated ability, comorbid schizophrenia and delusional disorders.
with EDs, and qualitative data on self-beliefs in EDs that had Of the 162 Caucasian cases with an ED consecutively referred,
been routinely and systematically collected in the course of pre- 6 patients refused to participate in the study, and 9 subjects were
vious research in this area. Moreover, this material was inte- excluded from the study because of the following reasons: illit-
grated with transcripts from clinical interviews, and with ED eracy (1), intellectual disability (1), comorbid schizophrenia (1),
patients’ diaries. Diaries are tools commonly used in the context and delusional disorder (6). The final sample was composed by
of the cognitive-behavioral treatment provided at our clinic. Pa- 147 ED subjects.
tients provided their written informed consent to the use of Healthy controls (matched with the clinical sample for age,
their diaries for the present research. All the sentences selected gender and education) were recruited among female students of
by the researchers were reviewed, collected and redistributed to the University of Florence, provided they met the following inclu-
add additional items to each domain of the questionnaire. sion criteria: absence of Axis I mental disorders, evaluated by
– Afterwards, junior psychiatrists read the items and provided means of a structured interview [32], and body mass index (BMI)
feedback regarding which of them were confusing or easily between 18.5 and 25. Of the 190 consecutive healthy controls re-
misinterpreted. Next, patients with a diagnosis of EDs pro- cruited, 3 subjects refused to give their informed consent, so that
vided feedback about the content of the items and directions the final sample group consisted of 187 subjects.
for the instrument. Patients explained, in their own words,
what they believed each item and the directions for the instru- Assessment
ment meant. Items or directions associated with common mis- The IDEA questionnaire was administered together with the
interpretations were revised. After this initial field testing, the clinical assessment at the first day of admission. Sociodemo-
sentences were modified considering the feedback from pa- graphic, psychopathological and clinical data were collected
tients and other physicians. Experienced clinicians examined through a face-to-face interview by two expert psychiatrists (V.R.,
each item for clarity, redundancy, and content validity. G.C.). The diagnosis of Axis I mental disorders was performed by

IDentity and EAting Disorders Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 149


means of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV [32]. In- Convergent validity represents the association between con-
ter-rater reliability (␬ coefficient) for diagnoses was 0.94. During structs that theoretically should be related: to assess the conver-
the visits, anthropometric measurements were made using stan- gent validity of IDEA, we examined the association with EDE-Q
dard calibrated instruments. Height (meters) was measured using scores, and depression measured by BDI. Discriminant validity
a wall-mounted stadiometer, weight (kilograms) using electronic reflects the association between constructs that theoretically
scales. BMI was calculated and the psychopathological evaluation should be unrelated: to assess discriminant validity of IDEA, we
was performed. BMI was calculated as weight in kilograms di- examined its association with age, and general symptomatology
vided by the square of height in meters. measured by SCL-90, and STAI.
Eating attitudes and behaviors were specifically investigated
by means of the Italian version of the Eating Disorder Examina- Incremental Validity
tion Questionnaire (EDE-Q) [33]. It consists of 38 items, assessing According to Haynes and Lench [37], incremental validity can
the core psychopathological features of EDs, and contains 4 sub- be defined as ‘the degree to which a measure explains or predicts
scales: dietary restraint, eating concern, weight concern and some phenomena of interest, relative to other measures’. In this
shape concern. The dietary restraint subscale is an admixture of study, we specifically evaluated the degree to which IDEA accu-
cognitions and behaviors pertaining to dietary restriction. The rately identifies persons placed into groups of EDs according to
three other subscales evaluate the dysfunctional attitudes regard- DSM-IV diagnoses. We performed a stepwise logistic regression
ing eating and overvalued thoughts regarding weight and shape. analysis in order to evaluate to what degree the addition of IDEA
The global score represents the mean of the four subscale scores. to other measure of ED psychopathology (EDE-Q) increased the
Furthermore, patients were evaluated by means of the follow- predictive efficacy of ED assessment [37]. The dependent variable
ing questionnaires: was the ED diagnosis according to DSM-IV (coded as dummy
– Symptom Checklist (SCL 90-R), a psychometric instrument variable: healthy controls = 0; patients with EDs = 1). In the first
for the identification of psychopathological distress [34]; step, age, gender, BMI and EDE-Q total score were entered as pre-
– the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), a widely used and well- dictive variables. In the second step, the IDEA total score was
established measure of current depression level and symptoms added to the model. To test whether the overall fit of the model
[35]; was improved by entering the IDEA scores, we used the ␹2 likeli-
– the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI Form Y-1), to measure hood ratio test.
levels of trait anxiety [36].
All the diagnostic procedures and the psychometric tests pre- Factor Analysis
viously described (with the exception of the IDEA questionnaire) To verify the distribution of items into subscales, factor analy-
are part of the routine clinical assessment for ED patients, per- sis was performed. Factor loading was calculated using the prin-
formed at our clinic. Before the collection of data, during the first cipal component analysis with Promax rotation and Kaiser nor-
routine visit, the procedures of the study were fully explained; af- malization, based on the assumption that the retained factors
ter that, the patients were asked to provide their written informed would be correlated to some degree, rather than orthogonal; the
consent. The study protocol was approved by the ethics commit- cut-off of coefficient was set at 0.4. Only components with an ei-
tee of the institution. genvalue higher than 1 were retained, and the Cattell’s scree test
for the number of factors was applied [38]. Cronbach’s ␣ evalu-
ated the internal consistency reliability of the resulting scales.
After selecting the best factor models, scale scores for each di-
Validation of the Questionnaire mension were obtained, calculated through the average of the cor-
respondent items. Next, two-tailed Student’s unpaired t test, and
Test-Retest Reliability univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) were applied for com-
All patients and controls were asked to complete the question- parison of means between patients with EDs and healthy controls,
naire twice, within 7 days. The time between the first clinical and among ED diagnoses, respectively. Clinical correlates of sub-
visit and the beginning of a clinical intervention generally lasts scales derived from factor analysis were evaluated following the
more than two weeks; therefore no specific treatment for EDs was procedures just described. To test the effect of IDEA domains on
administered between the two tests. ED symptoms, multiple linear regressions were performed,
Intraclass correlation coefficients were then calculated. Four adopting EDE-Q subscales as dependent variables and age and
patients failed to complete the questionnaire for the second time; IDEA factor scores as possible determinants.
therefore, test-retest reliability was evaluated on a sample of 143 All analyses were performed using SPSS for windows 15.0
patients. (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Ill., USA).

Internal Consistency
Internal consistency was evaluated using Cronbach’s ␣
method. Results

Clinical Correlates General characteristics of the sample are reported in


To evaluate the relationship between IDEA scores and clinical
parameters, Pearson’s correlations were calculated; for correla- table  1. As expected, patients with EDs showed higher
tion of IDEA scores with parameters that did not show a normal ED-specific and general symptomatology compared with
distribution, Spearman’s method was used. healthy controls, while no significant difference was ob-

150 Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 Stanghellini /Castellini /Brogna /Faravelli /


       

Ricca  
Table 1. General characteristics of the sample

AN BN BED F All patients Healthy t


(n = 50) (n = 48) (n = 49) with EDs controls
(n = 147) (n = 187)

Age 24.387.7 27.889.0 34.9816.7 13.33** 29.0810.8 27.386.1 1.77


BMI 16.181.3 23.486.0 38.088.4 119.0** 23.9810.1 22.183.0 2.16*
SCL-90 GSI 1.8180.56 1.9080.51 1.8080.61 0.18 1.8380.56 0.6580.44 11.99**
BDI 21.8086.08 23.05811.97 21.3487.68 0.77 21.9488.07 5.3985.52 14.69**
STAI 56.85813.52 67.6087.16 57.82811.05 3.13 59.90811.82 38.6589.04 11.24**
EDE-Q total score 3.3380.79 3.7881.28 3.3681.00 1.35 3.4881.05 1.0280.99 16.86**
EDE-Q restraint 3.0981.50 3.2781.58 2.7681.54 0.67 3.0381.53 1.1881.37 8.99**
EDE-Q eating concern 2.9581.18 3.5881.54 2.7281.38 2.56 3.0781.40 0.4480.69 18.66**
EDE-Q weight concern 3.1880.99 4.0681.45 3.5981.32 2.86 3.6181.30 1.1181.09 14.91**
EDE-Q shape concern 4.1681.19 4.3981.40 4.4181.22 0.27 4.3381.26 1.3481.23 16.95**

Values are reported as mean 8 standard deviation. ANOVA (F) for comparison of means among patients’ diagnoses, and two-
tailed Student’s unpaired t test (t) for comparison of means between patients and healthy controls; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
EDs = Eating disorders; AN = anorexia nervosa; BN = bulimia nervosa; BED = binge eating disorder; BMI = body mass index; SCL-
90 GSI = symptom checklist (SCL 90-R) global severity index; BDI = Beck depression inventory; STAI = state-trait anxiety inventory;
EDE-Q = eating disorder examination questionnaire.

served among ED diagnoses in terms of clinical variables Table 2. Scores of individual items
(with the exception of BMI). BMI, age, and STAI scores
did not show a normal distribution. Scores of individual Item Eating disorder Healthy controls
patients (n = 147) (n = 187)
items are summarized in table  2. Patients with EDs
showed higher scores for all IDEA items compared with 1 2.0981.46** 0.8581.01
healthy controls, as well as for IDEA total score (mean 8 2 1.6881.34** 1.0181.23
standard deviation: 1.59 8 0.95 vs 0.45 8 0.47; p ! 0.001). 3 1.3381.42** 0.8481.02
4 1.7782.20** 0.3280.61
5 1.9181.51** 0.5080.86
Test-Retest Reliability 6 1.4781.43** 0.1180.38
Intraclass correlation coefficients, 95% confidence in- 7 1.8181.39** 0.6280.85
terval (CI) of IDEA total score were 0.94, 0.90–0.96 for 8 1.6681.43** 0.2980.66
patients with EDs, and 0.91, 0.84–0.95 for healthy con- 9 2.0681.47** 0.5080.89
trols. 10 1.8381.53** 0.4680.80
11 1.5981.48** 0.3980.71
12 0.9381.34** 0.0580.28
Internal Consistency 13 1.8681.56** 0.5580.86
Cronbach’s ␣ value was 0.92 for IDEA total score. A 14 1.3181.39** 0.0580.28
value of 0.80 is considered satisfactory for internal con- 15 1.7281.40** 0.8480.89
sistency [39]. 16 1.5381.45** 0.5280.82
17 0.9081.23** 0.0880.31
18 1.3181.40** 0.3980.72
Clinical Correlates 19 1.7881.49** 0.3680.67
Table  3 reports correlations for IDEA subscales ob- 20 1.4181.40** 0.3480.64
tained from the retained factors. Considering convergent 21 1.8281.40** 0.3480.64
validity, IDEA total score was significantly associated 22 1.8481.55** 0.3080.62
with EDE-Q total score, and with all EDE-Q subscales, 23 1.0981.36** 0.3880.70
and with BDI scores (r = 0.30; p ! 0.05). Considering dis- Values are reported as mean 8 standard deviation.
criminant validity, IDEA total score did not show signif- Two-tailed Student’s unpaired t test (t) for comparison of
icant correlation with sociodemographic and general means between patients and healthy controls; ** p < 0.01.
clinical variables.

IDentity and EAting Disorders Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 151


Table 3. Correlations between IDEA scores and other psychometric measures for eating disorder patients (n = 147)

SCL-90 BDI STAI EDE-Q


GSI total score restraint eating concern weight concern shape concern

IDEA total score 0.24 0.30* 0.17 0.40** 0.34** 0.25* 0.30** 0.36**
GEO 0.12 0.16 0.07 0.33** 0.32** 0.15 0.20 0.31**
OM 0.26 0.30* 0.16 0.32** 0.19 0.30* 0.32** 0.28*
EB 0.29* 0.28* 0.26 0.37** 0.20 0.19 0.36** 0.36**
S 0.15 0.22 0.05 0.32** 0.39** 0.19 0.14 0.22*

Correlation coefficients: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.


SCL-90 GSI = Symptom checklist (SCL 90-R) global severity index; BDI = Beck depression inventory; STAI = state-trait anxiety
inventory; EDE-Q = eating disorder examination questionnaire; GEO (factor 1) = ‘feeling oneself only through the gaze of the other
and defining oneself only through the evaluation of the other’; OM (factor 2) = ‘feeling oneself only through objective measures’; EB
(factor 3) = ‘feeling extraneous from one’s own body’; S (factor 4) = ‘feeling oneself only through starvation’.

Table 4. Summary of exploratory factor analysis

Item (n = 23) Rotated factor loadings


factor factor factor factor
1 2 3 4

Knowing what the others think of me calms me down (15) 0.79


I can’t stand not to know what the others think of me (18) 0.89
For me it’s very important to see myself through the eyes of the others (1) 0.80
When I meet someone I can’t stay without knowing what he thinks of me (23) 0.78
I am dependent on the evaluation of the others (13) 0.91
Even if I think that the way the others evaluate me is wrong, I can’t do without it (16) 0.86
The way I feel depends on the way I feel looked at by the others (11) 0.82
Sometimes I focalize myself through the gaze of the others (7) 0.70
Seeing myself from their point of view makes me feel very anxious (8) 0.72
Having control of my weight means having control of the possible changes that happen in my body (21) 0.89
Only if I have my weight under control does being looked at by the others make me feel alright (10) 0.85
Having my weight under control makes me feel in control of my emotional states (22) 0.91
In all this confusion knowing that my weight is under control reassures me a little bit (9) 0.77
If my measures remain the same over time I feel that I am myself, if not I feel I am getting lost (5) 0.77
Sometimes, the emotions I feel are extraneous to me and scare me (4) 0.80
I see myself out of focus, I don’t feel myself (14) 0.86
I see myself fuzzy/hazy, as if I had no boundaries (6) 0.79
The fear of change is an emotion that I can’t tolerate (20) 0.80
The flesh is unimportant; it doesn’t let me feel my bones (12) 0.71
Eating according to my own rules is the only way to feel myself (2) 0.84
If I could not eat the way I want I would not be myself anymore (3) 0.86
If I follow your dietary prescriptions I cannot recognize myself when I look at myself in the mirror; 0.73
this does not happen if I do things in my own way (17)
Changing my own eating habits scares me to death, as does any other change in my life (19) 0.71
Eigenvalues 10.80 2.45 1.51 1.35
% of variance 46.96 10.54 6.56 5.87
␣ 0.93 0.89 0.88 0.82

152 Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 Stanghellini /Castellini /Brogna /Faravelli /


       

Ricca 
Incremental Validity
According to stepwise logistic regression analysis, at 12

step 1, EDE-Q total score (with age, gender, and BMI ad- 10
justment) predicted ED diagnosis with odds ratio (OR),
95% CI of 7.49, 4.87–11.52 (–2 log-likelihood = 226.27); at 8

Eigenvalue
step 2, with IDEA total score added to the model, EDE-Q
6
total score and IDEA total score predicted ED diagnosis,
respectively, with OR 95% CI of 4.91, 3.12–7.73 and 4.99, 4
2.77–9.00. The difference between the –2 log-likelihood
of the first and the second step (–2 log-likelihood = 2

184.29) revealed that adding IDEA scores improved the 0


model with a highly significant effect.
1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23
2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22
Factor Analysis Component No.
A principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted
on the 23 items with oblique rotation (Promax) for pa-
Fig. 1. Cattell’s scree plot for the number of retained factors.
tients with EDs. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure veri-
fied the sampling adequacy for the analysis (KMO = 0.90).
Bartlett’s test of sphericity (␹2 (253) = 1,980.88, p ! 0.001)
indicated that correlations between items were sufficient-
Table 5. Correlations among the 4 factors of IDEA
ly large for PCA. An initial analysis was run to obtain ei-
genvalues for each component in the data. Four factors
OM EB S
had eigenvalues over Kaiser’s criterion of 1, and in combi-
nation explained 69.95% of the variance, according to the GEO 0.54** 0.53** 0.42**
factor analysis performed for the development of the OM 0.51** 0.50**
questionnaire. The scree plot (fig. 1) was slightly ambigu- EB 0.51**
ous and showed inflexions that would justify retaining
** Pearson correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
both factors 2 and 4. Given the convergence of the scree GEO (factor 1) = ‘Feeling oneself only through the gaze of the
plot and Kaiser’s criterion on four factors, and the neces- other and defining oneself only through the evaluation of the oth-
sity of an accurate description of different dimensions er’; OM (factor 2) = ‘feeling oneself only through objective mea-
considered on the basis of the clinical observation, this is sures’; EB (factor 3) = ‘feeling extraneous from one’s own body’; S
(factor 4) = ‘feeling oneself only through starvation’.
the number of factors that was retained in the final analy-
sis. Table 4 shows the factor loadings after rotation. The
items that cluster on the same factor suggest that factor 1
represents ‘feeling oneself only through the gaze of the
other and defining oneself only through the evaluation of served among ED diagnostic groups in terms of all the
the other’ (GEO), factor 2 represents ‘feeling oneself only considered factors.
through objective measures’ (OM), factor 3 represents Stepwise multiple linear regressions (age adjusted)
‘feeling extraneous from one’s own body’ (EB), and factor showed that EDE-Q restraint was associated with factor
4 represents ‘feeling oneself through starvation’ (S). 4 ([S] ␤ = 0.30, p = 0.015), EDE-Q eating concern was as-
sociated with factor 2 ([OM] ␤ = 0.30, p = 0.01), EDE-Q
Retained Factor Characteristics weight concern was associated with factor 3 ([EB] ␤ =
Significant correlations were found among the re- 0.27, p = 0.034), and factor 2 ([OM] ␤ = 0.30, p = 0.01),
tained factors (table 5). The ␣ coefficients obtained for the EDE-Q shape concern was associated with factor 1
retained factors (table 4) showed high internal consisten- ([GEO] ␤ = 0.34, p = 0.004), and factor 3 ([EB] ␤ = 0.36,
cy among the items of each factor. p = 0.002). The performed analyses could be biased by
For each factor, an average score of the related items multi-collinearity, which results in a lack of statistical sig-
was calculated and reported (table 6). A significant dif- nificance of individual independent variables, although
ference was found between patients with EDs and healthy the overall model may be strongly significant. To test for
controls groups, while no significant difference was ob- multi-collinearity, we measured the variance inflation

IDentity and EAting Disorders Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 153


Table 6. IDEA scores

AN BN BED F All patients with Healthy controls t


(n = 50) (n = 48) (n = 49) EDs (n = 147) (n = 187)

IDEA total score 1.5680.88 1.6681.04 1.5680.94 0.18 1.5980.95 0.4580.47 15.19**
GEO 1.7981.15 1.5681.16 1.5481.16 0.66 1.6381.15 0.5380.64 10.90**
OM 1.7181.20 2.0581.28 1.9081.27 0.86 1.8981.25 0.4280.63 13.78**
EB 1.1580.88 1.5481.09 1.4481.12 1.82 1.3881.04 0.1780.29 15.01**
S 1.3580.91 1.5481.22 1.3681.19 0.42 1.4181.11 0.6580.60 7.98**

Values are reported as mean 8 standard deviation. ANOVA (F) for comparison of means among patients’ diagnoses, and two-
tailed Student’s unpaired t test (t) for comparison of means between patients and healthy controls; ** p < 0.01.
EDs = Eating disorders AN = anorexia nervosa; BN = bulimia nervosa; BED = binge eating disorder; GEO (factor 1) = ‘feeling one-
self only through the gaze of the other and defining oneself only through the evaluation of the other’; OM (factor 2) = ‘feeling oneself
only through objective measures’; EB (factor 3) = ‘feeling extraneous from one’s own body’; S (factor 4) = ‘feeling oneself only through
starvation’.

factor (VIF). For our current models, the VIF values were In the present study, we decided to administer and test
all below 2, showing that collinearity should not be cause the questionnaire to subjects suffering from anorexia
for concern [40]. nervosa, bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder. This
choice to include different subtypes of ED is based on the
assumption that patients with EDs share some common
Discussion as well as distinctive psychopathological core dimen-
sions. In particular, it has been pointed out that what is
The IDEA questionnaire, administered to a sample of most striking about the variety of EDs is not what distin-
female patients with EDs referred to an ED clinic and to guishes them, but how much they have in common [42],
a control group, showed a satisfactory test-retest reliabil- so that different clinical configurations may not be de-
ity and internal consistency, and IDEA total score was pendent on differences in core psychopathology.
significantly higher in patients than in controls. As far as Moreover, longitudinal studies indicate that most pa-
convergent validity is concerned, IDEA total scores tients migrate among diagnoses over time [42, 43] with-
showed a significant association with EDE-Q total and out a substantial change in basic psychopathological fea-
subscale scores, suggesting that this new questionnaire is tures [44–46]. Finally, The Eating Disorders Work Group
able to identify some important psychopathological phe- of the American Psychiatric Association has recently pro-
nomena which are closely related to the specific anoma- posed some diagnostic revisions and recommendations
lies of patients with EDs. The BDI scores were found to in order to improve the actual ED classification [47], and
be statistically correlated with IDEA scores, which could binge eating disorder has been formally recognized as a
depend on the frequent mood depression affecting pa- full-blown ED.
tients with EDs [41]. According to our findings, IDEA is a specific tool able
The factor analysis allowed for the identification of to clearly identify some peculiar phenomena in patients
four distinct subscales: ‘feeling oneself through the gaze with EDs. In particular, IDEA total scores are significant-
of the other and defining oneself through the evaluation ly higher in patients with EDs than in controls, whereas
of the other’ (GEO), which contains 9 items, ‘feeling one- no significant difference was observed between the three
self through objective measures’ (OM) with 5 items, ‘feel- diagnostic groups. Furthermore, the incremental validity
ing extraneous from one’s own body’ (EB) with 5 items, analysis, according to Haynes and Lench [37], showed
and ‘feeling oneself through starvation’ (S) with four that adding IDEA questionnaire to the assessment would
items (factors 1, 2, 3, 4, respectively). These subscales sig- improve the identification of patients with EDs. In order
nificantly correlated with each other, as with the items of to improve the clinical utility of this instrument in terms
each factor, suggesting the coherent nature of the ques- of incremental validity, further longitudinal studies
tionnaire and of the different factors that compose it. should provide information about the degree to which

154 Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 Stanghellini /Castellini /Brogna /Faravelli /


       

Ricca 
this new measure leads to the design of more effective tion’) were associated with overvalued thoughts regard-
treatments. ing weight and eating concern, and with dietary restric-
Furthermore, the same profile was obtained when the tion, respectively. According to these patterns of
four different factors were taken into account. It is of note association, some characteristic ED behaviors, such as
that these results were obtained considering the simple starvation and the fixated checking of objective mea-
categorical diagnostic criteria for the three disorders, sures, might be interpreted as an alternative coping strat-
which are based on both behavioral and cognitive crite- egy aimed to feel oneself for those patients who are unable
ria. to feel themselves cenesthetically.
As far as the correlations between IDEA scores and the Finally, some of the constructs which were explored by
specific ED psychopathological dimensions are con- the present questionnaire could conceptually resemble
cerned, we found a significant association between the dimensions assessed by different scales or by previous
IDEA total score and the EDE-Q total and subscale scores. qualitative research. For example, the GEO subscale
Also, the severity of embodiment and identity disorders, (‘feeling oneself through the gaze of the other and defin-
measured by IDEA total score, is positively correlated ing oneself through the evaluation of the other’) shows
with the degree of the ED psychopathology. Moreover, the some similarities with the social-psychological construct
lack of a significant association between IDEA total score ‘public self-consciousness’. Public self-consciousness, as
and the SCL-90 global severity score indicates that the opposed to private self-consciousness, includes all those
IDEA items are able to identify some nuclear feature spe- qualities of the self that are formed in other people’s eyes.
cific for patients with EDs, and are not a generic measure In fact, persons with ED have the tendency to think of
of psychological or psychiatric suffering. These results those aspects of their own self that are matters of public
suggest that the disorders of embodiment and identity as- display rather than to attend to the more covert, hidden
sessed by IDEA could represent a core dimension in the aspects of the self, e.g. one’s privately held beliefs and feel-
onset and maintenance of ED psychopathology. ings [49]. More in general, previous research has shown
According to the cognitive model of the maintenance important relationships between abnormal eating behav-
of EDs [48], the ‘core psychopathology’ of EDs is basi- ior and these persons’ self-construct or identity. Nordbø
cally an overconcern about body shape and weight, such et al. [50] documented that anorectic persons may ex-
that self-worth is judged largely or even exclusively in plain their behavior as a tool for achieving a new identity.
terms of satisfaction with weight and shape. Our results Skarderud [51, 52] showed that to some persons with ED,
showed that this core psychopathology is related to the changing one’s body is a tool to become another. They
dimension of embodiment named ‘lived-body-for-oth- want to change, and changing one’s body serves as both
ers’, confirming that persons with ED experience their a concrete and a symbolic tool for such ambitions. Thus,
own body first and foremost as an object being looked at shaping oneself is a ‘concretized metaphor’, establishing
by another, rather than cenesthetically or from a first- an equivalence between a psychic reality (identity) and a
person perspective. physical one (one’s body shape). As suggested by Surgenor
As far as the subscale scores are concerned, it is to note et al. [53], looking into the different ways persons with ED
that even if the four factors showed a high correlation construe their own self, especially in relation to their dis-
with each other, they had a different pattern of associa- order and therapy, has strategic implications for the ther-
tion with EDE-Q domains. The correlations of each sub- apeutic endeavor. Compared with other measures which
scale’s scores with abnormalities in eating behavior were are referred to constructs related to general beliefs, our
those expected, on the basis of the different dimensions questionnaire was specifically designed for patients with
explored by the questionnaire. In particular the GEO EDs, and our results showed its association with the spe-
subscale (‘feeling oneself through the gaze of the other cific psychological dimensions of these disorders.
and defining oneself through the evaluation of the other’) It is important to note that, given the cross-sectional
was associated with overvalued thoughts regarding body design of our study, we have not been able to establish any
shape. Weight and shape concerns are also associated supposed causal relationships between IDEA scores and
with EB subscale (‘feeling extraneous from one’s own ED behavior. A second limitation of this study is that it is
body’), which represents a measure of the alienation of impossible to ascertain whether the domains we assessed
patients with EDs from their own body and emotions. by means of our questionnaire are specific traits of pa-
OM subscale (‘feeling oneself through objective mea- tients with EDs, or state-related features. To answer these
sures’) and S subscale (‘feeling oneself through starva- questions, a longitudinal study is needed.

IDentity and EAting Disorders Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 155


Conclusion trol one’s shape and weight (rather than through other
kinds of performance), are supposedly core features of
IDEA represents a new, multidimensional, brief, ver- ED psychopathology.
satile, easy-to-perform instrument for the evaluation be- From a clinical point of view, we already demonstrated
fore, during and after a psychological treatment, assess- the importance of concerns related to the body (e.g. shape,
ing these patients’ abnormal experiences and attitudes. weight) in determining a different course of patients with
The anomalous phenomena explored and measured by EDs, in terms of response to treatment and long-term
IDEA are mainly two: abnormal attitudes toward one’s outcome [15, 54]. This study suggests that a specific dis-
own corporeality, and difficulties in the definition of order of lived corporeality might help to explain why per-
one’s own identity. We suggest that specific abnormalities sons with EDs overvalue their body shape and weight. If
in lived corporeality, namely experiencing one’s own further cross-sectional and prospective studies would
body first and foremost as an object being looked at by confirm our preliminary findings, the evaluation of the
another (rather than cenesthetically and from a first-per- ‘lived-body-for-others’ dimension could be considered a
son perspective), and of personal identity, namely defin- relevant feature for clinical assessment and treatment
ing one’s own self largely in terms of the way one feels protocols.
looked at by the others and through one’s ability to con-

Appendix

IDentity and EAting Disorders (IDEA) Questionnaire


Instructions: In the following list you find a series of statements which are often reported by people who have eating behavior prob-
lems. Please indicate how much you agree with the following claims.

Not Just a Enough Much Very


at all little (2) (3) much
(0) (1) (4)

1. For me it’s very important to see myself through the eyes of the others 0 1 2 3 4
2. Eating according to my own rules is the only way to feel myself 0 1 2 3 4
3. If I could not eat the way I want I would not be myself anymore 0 1 2 3 4
4. Sometimes, the emotions I feel are extraneous to me and scare me 0 1 2 3 4
5. If my measures remain the same over time I feel that I am myself, if not I feel I am getting lost 0 1 2 3 4
6. I see myself fuzzy/hazy, as if I had no boundaries 0 1 2 3 4
7. Sometimes I focalize myself through the gaze of the others 0 1 2 3 4
8. Seeing myself from their point of view makes me feel very anxious 0 1 2 3 4
9. In all this confusion, knowing that my weight is under control reassures me a little bit 0 1 2 3 4
10. Only if I have my weight under control does being looked at by the others make me feel alright 0 1 2 3 4
11. The way I feel depends on the way I feel looked at by the others 0 1 2 3 4
12. The flesh is unimportant; it doesn’t let me feel my bones 0 1 2 3 4
13. I am dependent on the evaluation of the others 0 1 2 3 4
14. I see myself out of focus, I don’t feel myself 0 1 2 3 4
15. Knowing what the others think of me calms me down 0 1 2 3 4
16. Even if I think that the way the others evaluate me is wrong, I can’t do without it 0 1 2 3 4
17. If I follow your dietary prescriptions I cannot recognize myself when I look at myself in the 0 1 2 3 4
mirror; this does not happen if I do things in my own way
18. I can’t stand not to know what the others think of me 0 1 2 3 4
19. Changing my own eating habits scares me to death, as does any other change in my life 0 1 2 3 4
20. The fear of change is an emotion that I can’t tolerate 0 1 2 3 4
21. Having control of my weight means having control of the possible changes that happen in 0 1 2 3 4
my body
22. Having my weight under control makes me feel in control of my emotional states 0 1 2 3 4
23. When I meet someone I can’t stay without knowing what he thinks of me 0 1 2 3 4

156 Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 Stanghellini /Castellini /Brogna /Faravelli /


       

Ricca  
References
1 Jacobi C, Hayward C, de Zwaan M, Kraemer 14 Bardone-Cone AM, Joiner TE Jr, Crosby RD, 29 Stanghellini G: Embodiment and schizo-
HC, Agras WS: Coming to terms with risk Crow SJ, Klein MH, le Grange D, Mitchell JE, phrenia. World Psychiatry 2009;8:56–59.
factors for eating disorders: application of Peterson CB, Wonderlich SA: Examining a 30 Fuchs, T. Psychopathologie von Leib und
risk terminology and suggestions for a gen- psychosocial interactive model of binge eat- Raum. Phänomenologisch-empirische Un-
eral taxonomy. Psychol Bull 2004; 130: 19– ing and vomiting in women with bulimia tersuchungen zu depressiven und para-
65. nervosa and subthreshold bulimia nervosa. noiden Erkrankungen. Darmstadt, Stein-
2 Klump KL, Bulik CM, Kaye WH, Treasure Behav Res Ther 2008;46:887–894. kopff, 2000.
J, Tyson E: Academy for eating disorders 15 Ricca V, Castellini G, Lo Sauro C, Mannucci 31 Morris KJ: The phenomenology of body dys-
position paper: eating disorders are serious E, Ravaldi C, Rotella F, Faravelli C: Cogni- morphic disorder: a Sartrean analysis; in
mental illnesses. Int J Eat Disord 2009; 42: tive-behavioral therapy for threshold and Fulford KWM, Morris KJ, Sadler JZ, Stang-
97–103. subthreshold anorexia nervosa: a three-year hellini G (eds): Nature and Narrative. An In-
3 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnos- follow-up study. Psychother Psychosom troduction to the New Philosophy of Psychi-
tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor- 2010;79:238–248. atry. Oxford, Oxford University Press, 2001,
ders, Text Revised. Washington, American 16 Serpell L, Treasure J, Teasdale J, Sullivan V: pp 171–185.
Psychiatric Association, 2000. Anorexia nervosa: friend or foe? Int J Eat 32 First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams
4 Thomas JJ, Vartanian LR, Brownell KD: The Disord 1999;25:177–186. JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for
relationship between eating disorder not 17 Surgenor LJ, Plumridge EW, Horn J: ‘Know- DSMIV Axis I Disorders. Patient Edition
otherwise specified (EDNOS) and officially ing one’s self’ anorexic: implications for (SCIDP, Version 2). New York, New York
recognized eating disorders: meta-analysis therapeutic practice. Int J Eat Disord 2003; State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Re-
and implications for DSM. Psychol Bull 33:22–32. search, 1995.
2009;135:407–433. 18 Stanghellini G: For an anthropology of eat- 33 Fairburn CG, Beglin SJ: Assessment of eating
5 Fairburn CG, Harrison PJ: Eating Disorders. ing disorders. A pornographic vision of the disorders: interview or self-report question-
The Lancet 2003;361:407–416. self. Eat Weight Disord 2005;10:21–27. naire? Int J Eat Disord 1994;16:363–370.
6 Ben-Tovim DI, Walker K, Gilchrist P, Free- 19 Nordbø RH, Espeset EM, Gulliksen KS, 34 Derogatis LR, Lipman RS, Covi L: SCL-90:
man R, Kalucy R, Esterman A: Outcome in Skårderud F, Holte A: The meaning of self- an outpatient psychiatric rating scale-pre-
patients with eating disorders: a 5-year starvation: qualitative study of patients’ per- liminary report. Psychopharmacol Bull
study. Lancet 2001;357:1254–1257. ception of anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1973;9:13–28.
7 Allen KL, Byrne SM, McLean NJ, Davis EA: 2006;39:556–564. 35 Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J,
Overconcern with weight and shape is not 20 Skårderud F: Shame and pride in anorexia Erbaugh J: An inventory for measuring de-
the same as body dissatisfaction: evidence nervosa: a qualitative descriptive study. Eur pression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1961; 4: 561–
from a prospective study of pre-adolescent Eat Disord Rev 2007;15:81–97. 571.
boys and girls. Body Image 2008;5:261–270. 21 Ison J, Kent S: Social identity in eating disor- 36 Spielberg CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE.
8 Dalle Grave R, Calugi,Marchesini G: Under- ders. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2010;18:475–485. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Invento-
weight eating disorder without overevalua- 22 Sternheim L, Konstantellou A, Startup H, ry (self-evaluation questionnaire). Palo Alto,
tion of shape and weight: atypical anorexia Schmidt U: What does uncertainty mean to Consulting Psychologists Press, 1970.
nervosa? Int J Eat Disord 2008;41:705–712. women with anorexia nervosa? An interpre- 37 Haynes SN, Lench HC: Incremental validity
9 Shafran R, Farrell C, Lee M, Fairburn CG: tative phenomenological analysis. Eur Eat of new clinical assessment measures. Psychol
Brief cognitive behavioural therapy for ex- Disord Rev 2011;19:12–24. Assess 2003;15:456–466.
treme shape concern: an evaluation. Br J Clin 23 Blechert J, Ansorge U, Beckmann S, Tuschen- 38 Cattel RB: The scree test for the number of
Psychol 2009;48:79–92. Caffier B: The undue influence of shape and factors. Multivariate Behav Res 1996; 1: 245–
10 Haines J, Kleinman KP, Rifas-Shiman SL, weight on self-evaluation in anorexia nervo- 276.
Field AE, Austin SB: Examination of shared sa, bulimia nervosa and restrained eaters: a 39 Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH: Psychometric
risk and protective factors for overweight combined ERP and behavioral study. Psy- Theory, ed 3. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1994.
and disordered eating among adolescents. chol Med 2010;18:1–10. 40 Fox J: Regression Diagnostics. Thousand
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2010; 164: 336– 24 Sartre JP: L’etre et le neant, 1943. English Oaks, Sage Publications, 1991.
343. translation: Being and Nothingness. Wash- 41 Treasure J, Claudino AM, Zucker N: Eating
11 Watson HJ, Raykos BC, Street H, Fursland A, ington Square Press, New York, 1992. disorders. Lancet 2010;375:583–593.
Nathan PR: Mediators between perfection- 25 Husserl E: Ideen zu einer reinen Phaenome- 42 Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R: Cogni-
ism and eating disorder psychopathology: nologie und phaenomenologische Philoso- tive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a
shape and weight overvaluation and condi- phie. II. Phaenomenologische Untersuch- ‘‘transdiagnostic’’ theory and treatment. Be-
tional goal-setting. Int J Eat Disord 2011;44: ungen zur Konstitution. Den Haag: Nijhoff, hav Res Ther 2003;41:509–528.
142–149. 1912–1915. 43 Milos G, Spindler A, Schnyder U, Fairburn
12 Carter JC, Blackmore E, Sutandar-Pinnock 26 Merleau-Ponty M: Phenomenology of per- CG: Instability of eating disorder diagno-
K, Woodside DB: Relapse in anorexia ner- ception. Engl. Transl. by Colin Smith. Hu- ses: prospective study. Br J Psychiatry 2005;
vosa: a survival analysis. Psychol Med 2004; manities Press, New York, 1996. 187:573–578.
34:671–679. 27 Fuchs T: The phenomenology of shame, guilt 44 Tozzi F, Thornton LM, Klump KL, Fichter
13 Loeb KL, Walsh BT, Lock J, le Grange D, and the body in body dysmorphic disorder MM, Halmi KA, Kaplan AS, Strober M,
Jones J, Marcus S, Weaver J, Dobrow I: Open and depression. J Phenomenol Psychol 2002; Woodside DB, Crow S, Mitchell J, Rotondo A,
trial of family-based treatment for full and 33:223–243. Mauri M, Cassano G, Keel P, Plotnicov KH,
partial anorexia nervosa in adolescence: evi- 28 Stanghellini G: Psicopatologia del senso co- Pollice C, Lilenfeld LR, Berrettini WH, Bulik
dence of successful dissemination. J Am mune, Cortina, Milano. New English trans- CM, Kaye WH: Symptom fluctuation in eat-
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007;46:792– lation forthcoming: Disembodied Spirits ing disorders: correlates of diagnostic cross-
800. and Deanimated Bodies. The Psychopathol- over. Am J Psychiatry 2005;162:732–740.
ogy of Common Sense. Oxford, Oxford Uni-
versity Press, 2008.

IDentity and EAting Disorders Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 157


45 Fairburn CG, Cooper Z: Thinking afresh 49 Scheier MF, Carver CS: The Self-Conscious- 52 Skarderud F: Eating one’s words, Part II. The
about the classification of eating disorders. ness Scale: a revised version for use with gen- embodied mind and reflective function in
Int J Eat Disord 2007;40:107–110. eral populations. J Appl Soc Psychol 1985;15: anorexia nervosa - theory. Eur Eat Disord
46 Eddy KT, Dorer DJ, Franko DL, Tahilani K, 687–699. Rev 2007;15: 243–252.
Thompson-Brenner H, Herzog DB: Diag- 50 Nordbø RH, Espeset EM, Gulliksen KS, 53 Surgenor LJ, Plumridge EW, Horn J: ‘Know-
nostic crossover in anorexia nervosa and bu- Skårderud F, Holte A: The meaning of self- ing One’s Self’ anorexic: implications for
limia nervosa: implications for DSM-V. Am starvation: qualitative study of patients’ per- therapeutic practice. Int J Eat Disord 2003;
J Psychiatry 2008;165:245–250. ception of anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 33:22–32.
47 http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevisions/ 2006;39:556–564. 54 Castellini G, Lo Sauro C, Mannucci E, Ra-
Pages/EatingDisorders.aspx. 51 Skarderud F: Eating one’s words, Part I. valdi C, Rotella CM, Faravelli C, Ricca V: Di-
48 Fairburn CG: Eating disorders: the transdi- ‘Concretised metaphors’ and reflective func- agnostic crossover and outcome predictors
agnostic view and the cognitive behavioral tion in anorexia nervosa – an interview in eating disorders according to DSM-IV
theory; in Cognitive Behavior Therapy and study. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2007;15:163–174. and DSM-V proposed criteria: a 6-year fol-
Eating Disorders. Guilford Press, New York, low-up study. Psychosom Med 2011;73: 270–
2008. 279.

158 Psychopathology 2012;45:147–158 Stanghellini /Castellini /Brogna /Faravelli /


       

Ricca  

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen