Sie sind auf Seite 1von 11

Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Clinical Psychology Review

Factors of PTSD: Differential specificity and external correlates


Joshua Gootzeit ⁎, Kristian Markon
Department of Psychology, The University of Iowa, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) has been found to be a multidimensional disorder most likely consisting
Received 27 January 2011 of four distinct symptom dimensions. Many studies have investigated the fit of two competing structural
Received in revised form 3 June 2011 models of PTSD (King et al., 1998; Simms et al., 2002). However, little research has been done on the utility
Accepted 8 June 2011
and differential external correlates of these dimensions. Meta-analysis was used to find the correlations
Available online 22 June 2011
between dimensions of PTSD and five external variables (depression, anxiety, panic, substance use, and
Keywords:
trauma history), and multivariate analysis was used to find the unique contributions of each dimension in
Posttraumatic stress predicting each variable. It was found that the Simms et al. (2002) model better divides PTSD symptoms into
Meta-analysis specific and general factors. The relative specificity of each factor is discussed.
Model © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Validity

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 993
2. Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 995
2.1. Literature search . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 995
2.2. Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 995
3. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 995
3.1. Zero-order correlations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 996
3.2. Multiple correlations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 996
3.3. Path models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 997
4. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 999
4.1. Predictions and findings regarding the general-dysphoria model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 999
4.2. Predictions and findings regarding other models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 999
4.3. Summary of our findings and areas for further research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1000
4.4. Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1001
5. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1001
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1001
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1001

1. Introduction prevalence is considerably higher among vulnerable populations;


for example, 8.7% of Iraq War veterans meet diagnostic criteria for
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is a debilitating anxiety PTSD three years after combat exposure (Smith et al., 2008). Veterans
disorder that develops in response to a severe traumatic event. The who suffer from PTSD are likely to suffer low quality of life and
national comorbidity survey replication found that the disorder's 12- impaired interpersonal and occupational functioning (Schnurr,
month prevalence rate in a sample of community adults was around Lunney, Bovin, & Marx, 2009).
3.5% (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, Merikangas, & Walters, 2005). Its The diagnostic criteria of PTSD in the DSM-III-R and the DSM-IV
include seventeen symptoms divided into the three symptom clusters of
⁎ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, The University of Iowa, 11
intrusive re-experiencing (5 symptoms), effortful avoidance/emotional
Seashore Hall E, Iowa City, IA, 52242-1407, USA. Tel.: + 1 319 335 2406. numbing (7 symptoms), and physiological hyperarousal (5 symptoms).
E-mail address: joshua-gootzeit@uiowa.edu (J. Gootzeit). To receive a diagnosis, individuals must show at least one symptom of

0272-7358/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.06.005
994 J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003

traumatic intrusions, at least three symptoms of avoidance/numbing, Table 1


and at least two symptoms of hyperarousal. These symptom clusters Possible symptom dimensions of PTSD.

were based primarily on clinicians' rational ideas about PTSD symptom DSM-IV PTSD Models
structure rather than on empirical data (Brett, Spitzer, & Williams, symptoms
DSM-IV King et al. Simms et al.
1988), and the disorder has been controversial from its outset (McNally, 3-factor (1998) (2002)
2003). 4-factor 4-factor
In the 1990s, some exploratory factor analyses (EFAs) were B1. Intrusive thoughts Intrusions Intrusions Intrusions
performed in an attempt to validate these symptom dimensions. of trauma
These analyses showed little agreement, positing four-factor (Keane, B2. Recurrent dreams Intrusions Intrusions Intrusions
1993), three-factor (Foa, Riggs, & Gershuny, 1995; Smith, Redd, of trauma
B3. Flashbacks Intrusions Intrusions Intrusions
DuHamel, Vickberg, & Ricketts, 1999), and two-factor models (Taylor, B4. Emotional reactivity Intrusions Intrusions Intrusions
Kuch, Koch, Crockett, & Passey, 1998). These studies tended to use to trauma cues
small samples, and there was little agreement between them. B5. Physiological reactivity Intrusions Intrusions Intrusions
Although EFAs are useful for finding structure in data, they are not to trauma cues
C1. Avoiding thoughts Avoidance/Numbing Avoidance Avoidance
typically used to test competing factor models. A large number of
of trauma
confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) has therefore been run to test the C2. Avoiding reminders Avoidance/Numbing Avoidance Avoidance
goodness of fit for various structural models of PTSD. Although some of trauma
early studies tested the fit indices for Taylor et al.'s (1998) two-factor C3. Inability to recall Avoidance/Numbing Numbing Dysphoria
model (Buckley, Blanchard, & Hickling, 1998) and the DSM-IV's three- aspects of trauma
C4. Loss of interest Avoidance/Numbing Numbing Dysphoria
factor model (Cordova, Studts, Hann, Jacobsen, & Andrykowski, 2000), C5. Detachment Avoidance/Numbing Numbing Dysphoria
these studies did not compare the investigated structures to other C6. Restricted affect Avoidance/Numbing Numbing Dysphoria
possible solutions. C7. Sense of foreshortened Avoidance/Numbing Numbing Dysphoria
Recent CFAs have suggested two competing four-factor models of future
D1. Sleep disturbance Hyperarousal Hyperarousal Dysphoria
PTSD, both of which show better fits than other proposed models. The
D2. Irritability Hyperarousal Hyperarousal Dysphoria
first of these models involves splitting the DSM-IV's avoidance/numbing D3. Difficulty Hyperarousal Hyperarousal Dysphoria
cluster into separate factors, creating intrusions, effortful avoidance, concentrating
emotional numbing, and hyperarousal factors (King, Leskin, King & D4. Hypervigilance Hyperarousal Hyperarousal Hypervigilance
Weathers, 1998). The second proposed four-factor model creates a D5. Exaggerated Hyperarousal Hyperarousal Hypervigilance
startle response
separate avoidance factor, expands emotional numbing into a broader
dysphoria factor, and reduces hyperarousal into a more specific
hypervigilance factor, creating the four factors of intrusions, avoidance,
dysphoria, and hypervigilance (Simms, Watson & Doebbelling, 2002). mood disorders have been found to be very comorbid, and to
These two models are summarized in Table 1. Over the last decade, collectively form an “internalizing” cluster of psychopathology
much of the research on the structure of PTSD has focused on which of (Krueger, 1999). Cox, Clara, and Enns (2002) have found that PTSD
these two models provides a better fit. Some studies suggest that the is particularly highly comorbid with Major Depressive Disorder,
King et al. (1998) model is superior (e.g., McDonald et al., 2008; Naifeh, Dysthymic Disorder, and Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD). These
Elhai, Kashdan & Grubaugh, 2008), whereas others support the Simms high comorbidities have led some researchers to question the validity
et al. (2002) model (e.g., Boelen, van den Hout, & van den Bout, 2008; of the PTSD diagnosis (Rosen & Lilienfeld, 2008).
Elklit & Shevlin, 2007). Still other studies suggest that one or the other of Mineka, Watson, and Clark's (1998) integrative hierarchical model
these models might be superior depending on the assessment method of anxiety and depression suggests a possible reason for this high
(Palmieri, Weathers, et al., 2007) or the population under investigation comorbidity. This model states that each mood or anxiety disorder
(Elhai et al., 2009). However, in their recent meta-analysis, Yufik and consists of a unique component, or specific factor, and a shared
Simms (2010) suggest that the structure is largely invariant, finding that component, or general factor. Based on their review of the literature,
the Simms et al. (2002) model is superior for a variety of populations they suggest that high negative affect (associated with general distress
and measures of PTSD. or neuroticism) is a general factor across the mood and anxiety
Based on their meta-analysis of the overall fit for the two models, disorders. This model also offers a possible means to evaluate the utility
Yufik and Simms (2010) suggest that PTSD's multi-factor structure of the factor models of PTSD. If some factors represent the shared
indicates that the DSM-5 should include the four symptom clusters of variance among disorders, whereas others are more specific, they can be
intrusions, avoidance, hyperarousal, and dysphoria. They acknowledge, interpreted with these considerations in mind. This information might
however, that both four-factor models fit the data well. also be useful for future revisions of the DSM symptom criteria, allowing
Whereas most structural research that has been done in recent clinicians to emphasize symptom dimensions that are more specific to
years focuses on investigating which structural model provides the PTSD (Spitzer, First, & Wakefield, 2007).
best fit, less research has been done on the clinical utility and external Simms et al. (2002) suggest that their model's dysphoria
validity of a multi-factor model of PTSD. Unlike PTSD, the diagnostic dimension represents this general negative affect factor. They also
criteria for most mood and anxiety disorders in the DSM-IV are not suggest that the intrusions/re-experiencing symptom dimension
broken into separate symptom clusters. For example, individuals are might then represent a factor more specific to PTSD, and that
not required to show symptoms of both negative affect and somatic avoidance and hypervigilance might represent mid-level factors
symptoms of depression to be diagnosed with Major Depressive common to PTSD and the anxiety disorders, although the researchers
Disorder (MDD), despite empirical evidence that these areas did not find that these symptoms were unique predictors of
represent distinct factors of depression (Shafer, 2006). Likewise, a generalized anxiety and panic. They found some evidence that
diagnosis of Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) does not require dysphoria has a higher correlation with depression than other
symptoms of symmetry, forbidden thoughts, cleaning, and hoarding, dimensions of PTSD, but they did not find such support for avoidance
despite evidence that these areas represent distinct factors of OCD and hypervigilance.
(Bloch, Landeros-Weisenberger, Rosario, Pittenger & Leckman, 2008). Grant, Beck, Marques, Palyo, & Clapp (2008) tested directly
One area in which multiple factors might be helpful is in whether PTSD is distinguishable from MDD and GAD at a factor
explaining and modeling comorbidity. The anxiety and unipolar level, using confirmatory factor analysis. They found that PTSD could
J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003 995

be distinguished from MDD and GAD, and that the intrusions, 2. Methods
avoidance, and hypervigilance factors are best understood as lower-
order factors specific to PTSD. Conversely, the dysphoria factor was not 2.1. Literature search
specific to PTSD, but was best conceptualized as a higher-order
dimension common to all three disorders. Although these conclusions In addition to external correlates of PTSD factors, the magnitude of
were based on a single sample (N = 228), they provide some evidence the correlations among the factors was investigated for the purpose of
for differential specificity for the factors of PTSD. multivariate analysis. Analysis was limited to studies that reported
Gros, Simms, and Acierno (2010) further investigated specificity of correlations between PTSD factors and external factors, or among PTSD
PTSD and depression at the symptom level. Using exploratory factor factors. Only measures of PTSD that follow the DSM-IV's symptoms were
analysis of PTSD and depression measures, they found that the PTSD included in this analysis. When a study included multiple time points,
symptoms roughly corresponding to the Simms dysphoria factor and time 1 data were used. Factor correlations in CFA studies were included
the King numbing factor were more highly associated with symptoms in estimates of the correlations among PTSD dimensions.
of depression than with other symptoms of PTSD. In contrast, Literature searches were conducted on PsycINFO and Google Scholar.
symptoms of intrusions, avoidance, and hyperarousal were more Studies citing the original King et al. (1998) or Simms et al. (2002)
highly associated with each other than with symptoms of depression. studies were examined for relevant correlations. Other searches (e.g.,
The results again show evidence that symptoms of PTSD show “PTSD factor analysis”) were also conducted. The search was limited to
differential specificity. papers published in English. Unpublished thesis projects indexed on
In another study, Elhai, Grubaugh, Kashdan and Frue (2008) Google Scholar were included in the search, and unpublished data from
addressed the concern that there is too much overlap between PTSD the authors were used in the analyses. No other unpublished studies
and other disorders. While not directly examining the factor structure were included. In all, 41 relevant studies were identified. Studies were
of PTSD, they examined an alternative set of diagnostic criteria included if they reported correlations between the King et al. (1998) or
(Spitzer et al., 2007), which eliminates symptoms that directly the Simms et al. (2002) factors and external variables, or if they included
overlap with other internalizing disorders. These eliminated symp- correlations among the PTSD factors. Five external variables were
toms were comprised of five of the eight symptoms in the dysphoria identified whose correlations were reported for both four-factor
dimension. The revised diagnostic criteria therefore include a much models in at least two samples: depression, anxiety, panic, substance
weaker general distress factor. However, comorbidity between PTSD use, and trauma history. These variables were included in the final
and other disorders remained unchanged regardless of the criteria analysis.
that were used. These results suggest that a multi-dimensional model If a study included multiple measures of the same construct in a
of PTSD might be of limited utility. These results were replicated by single sample (e.g., Palmieri, Weathers, et al., 2007), the correlations
Grubaugh, Long, Elhai, Frueh, and Magruder (2010). were averaged before being entered into the meta-analysis. If a study
Marshall, Schell and Miles (2010) have directly examined the included correlations for multiple samples (e.g., McDonald et al.,
dysphoria dimension, testing whether the dysphoria symptoms of PTSD 2008), each sample was coded as a separate study.
had stronger associations with measures of depression, generalized Studies included in the meta-analysis are shown in Table 2.
anxiety, and general distress. They found that the dysphoria and
nondysphoria dimensions of PTSD are both associated with these 2.2. Procedure
external measures and that there was no evidence of specificity in the
dysphoria factor. They therefore suggested that there is little reason to Correlations were aggregated using the Hunter and Schmidt bare
differentiate specific and general symptoms of PTSD. These results are bones method of meta-analysis, using a random effects model (Hunter &
consistent with those of Elhai et al. (2008) suggesting little differential Schmidt, 2004). Heterogeneity of samples was assumed when calcu-
specificity in a multi-dimensional model of PTSD. lating estimated effect size variance. A Fisher's Z transformation was not
In a review of the literature, Watson (2009) identified several studies used. Each individual correlation was weighted by sample size before
that reported correlations between PTSD symptom dimensions and being entered into the meta-analysis. Because reliability data do not
depression. He found that, for the King et al. (1998) model of PTSD, exist for some included measures, such as trauma history, correlations
depression is more highly associated with numbing and hyperarousal were not corrected for reliability. Correlations corrected for sampling
than with intrusions and avoidance. For the Simms et al. (2002) model, error, observed standard deviations of the correlations and standard
the dysphoria dimension showed a stronger relationship with depres- deviations corrected for sampling error were calculated using formulas
sion compared to the other factors. These data suggest that the Simms given by Hunter and Schmidt (2004). The meta-analyses were
et al. (2002) model's symptom factors may be superior in separating conducted using software by Schmidt and Le (2004).
specific and general symptoms. To investigate the unique contribution of each factor of PTSD to
In this paper, we examine the external validity of multidimen- external variables, multivariate analyses were conducted using SAS
sional diagnostic criteria for PTSD, across a broad range of external and Mplus software. Forward multiple regression was conducted for
criteria. Yufik and Simms (2010) have examined the internal validity each model and for each external variable, and the squared multiple
of PTSD's latent structure using meta-analysis, and found evidence correlation value was calculated for each step to calculate the
that the Simms et al. (2002) dysphoria model best accounts for the percentage of variance accounted for by each model. Path analysis
symptom structure across populations and symptom measures. was conducted to calculate the unique predictive power of each factor
Although their evidence indicates that such a structure provides a of PTSD, using maximum likelihood estimation. For each path analysis,
better fit, meta-analysis has not been used to investigate the extent to in order to calculate confidence intervals, we used the harmonic mean
which these symptom dimensions show useful differential specificity sample size, based in part on structural equation modeling literature
and discriminant validity. Some previous researchers (e.g., Marshall on how to analyze correlation matrices with differing sample sizes
et al., 2010; Elhai et al., 2008; Grubaugh et al., 2010) have suggested (Enders & Peugh, 2004).
that a multi-dimensional model of PTSD separating out dysphoria
symptoms is unnecessary, and that dysphoria symptoms do not show 3. Results
a greater relationship with external correlates or drive comorbidity.
The current study aimed to address this hypothesis by examining the Results of the meta-analyses are shown in Table 3. All correlations
external correlates of PTSD symptom factors using methods of meta- were positive, and, with the exception of the correlation between
analysis. avoidance and trauma history, all were statistically significant at
996 J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003

Table 2 Table 2 (continued)


Studies included in meta-analysis. Study PTSD Other measures N
measure
Study PTSD Other measures N
measure Shelby et al. (2005) PCL – 148
Asmundson et al. PCL CES-D 1822 Shevlin et al. AUDADIS- – 12,467
(2004) sample 1 (2009) IV
Asmundson et al. PSS-SR CES-D 400 Simms et al. (2002) PCL PRIME-MD depression, anxiety and 1896
(2004) sample 2 sample 1 alcohol abuse, trauma history
Asmundson et al. CAPS CES-D 58 Simms et al. (2002) PCL PRIME-MD depression, anxiety and 1799
sample 2 alcohol abuse
(2004) sample 3
Taft et al. (2007) SCID CAGE alcoholism 1168
Asmundson et al. CAPS CES-D 60
(2004) sample 4 Witteveen et al. SRIP SCL-90 depression 833
Baschnagel et al. PDS – 748 (2006) sample 1
(2005) Witteveen et al. SRIP SCL-90 depression 333
Beck et al. (2009) CAPS BDI-II 109 (2006) sample 2
Boelen et al. (2008) PSS-SR SCL-90 depression 347 Wright and CAPS Trauma history questionnaire 227
Carragher et al. CIDI – 2677 Johnson (2009)
Zahradnik et al. PCL – 677
(2010)
(2009)
Cook et al. (2009) PCL PHQ depression 439
Cox et al. (2008) CIDI – 588 Note: AUDIS-IV = Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule;
Declercq et al. DTS Peritraumatic stress questionnaire 125 BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory II; BAI = Beck Anxiety Inventory; CAGE = Ewing
(2010) alcoholism questionnaire; CAPS = Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale; CES-D = Center
DuHamel et al. PCL – 236 for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; CIDI = Composite International Diagnostic
(2004) Interview; DTS = Davidson Trauma Scale; HTQ = Harvard Trauma Questionnaire;
Elhai et al. (2007) PCL – 215 IDAS = Inventory of Depression and Anxiety Symptoms; PAI = Personality Assessment
Elklit and Shevlin HTQ TSC-33 depression and anxiety 1116 Inventory; PCL = PTSD Checklist; PDS = Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale; PHQ =
(2007) Patient Health Questionnaire; PRIME-MD = Primary Care Evaluation of Mental
Elklit et al. (2010) HTQ – 973 Disorders; PSS-I = PTSD Symptom Scale-Interview; PSS-SR = PTSD Symptom Scale-
Feuer et al. (2005) CAPS – 272 Self Report; PTSD-Q = Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Questionnaire; SCID = Structured
Flack et al. (2005) PCL Trauma history questionnaire 869 Clinical Interview for DSM-IV; SCL-90 = Symptom Checklist; SRIP = Self-Rating
sample 1 Inventory for PTSD; TSC-33 = Trauma Symptom Checklist.
Flack et al. (2005) PCL Trauma history questionnaire 423
sample 2
Forbes et al. (2005) SCID – 692 p = 0.05. Number of studies for a correlation ranged from 3 to 42, and
Gootzeit (2010) PCL IDAS dyphoria (depression), trauma 408 overall sample sizes ranged from 3196 to 55,774.
sample 1 history
Gootzeit (2010) PCL IDAS dyphoria (depression), trauma 297
sample 2 history 3.1. Zero-order correlations
Gootzeit (2010) PCL IDAS dyphoria (depression), trauma 240
sample 3 history An examination of the King et al. (1998) zero-order correlations
Hetzel-Riggin PTSD-Q – 2378
(Table 4) shows that all four PTSD symptom dimensions are related to
(2009)
Hoyt and Yeater PCL – 278
all five external variables. Numbing and hyperarousal are both more
(2010) sample 1 strongly related to depression and anxiety than are intrusions and
Hoyt and Yeater PCL – 226 avoidance. There is no evidence that avoidance has a unique relationship
(2010) sample 2 with anxiety or that numbing has a unique relationship with depression.
Johnson et al. PSS-I CES-D 225
Hyperarousal is slightly more associated with anxiety than numbing is,
(2007)
King et al. (1998) CAPS – 524 but shows no specificity to anxiety over depression. Hyperarousal and
Mansfield et al. PCL – 15,593 numbing have slightly higher correlations with panic compared with
(2010) intrusions and numbing. Hyperarousal shows the strongest relationship
Marshall et al. PCL PHQ depression 357 with substance use, and intrusions factor shows the strongest
(2010) Sample 1
Marshall et al. PCL SCL-90 depression 413
relationship with trauma.
(2010) Sample 2 The Simms et al. (2002) zero-order correlations (Table 5) also
McDevitt-Murphy CAPS PAI depression, anxiety, alcohol, and 55 demonstrate that all four dimensions of PTSD are related to all external
et al. (2005) drug use variables. Dysphoria is the strongest correlate for every external
McDonald et al. DTS – 814
variable. Hypervigilance has a stronger relationship with anxiety and
(2008) sample 1
McDonald et al. DTS – 320 substance use than intrusions or avoidance do, but does not show this
(2008) sample 2 pattern for panic. Intrusive re-experiencing shows a stronger
McDonald et al. DTS – 313 relationship with trauma than avoidance or hypervigilance do.
(2008) sample 3
Miller et al. (2008) CAPS – 315
Naifeh et al. (2008) PSS-SR CES-D 395
3.2. Multiple correlations
Newton et al. PDS BDI-II, trauma severity 39
(2005) Squared multiple correlations are given in Table 6 for the King et al.
Palmieri and PCL – 1218 (1998) model of PTSD. Hyperarousal is the best predictor of depression
Fitzgerald (2005)
(R2 = 0.4617), anxiety (R2 = 0.3437), panic (R2 = 0.2052), and sub-
Palmieri, Marshall PCL, CAPS BDI-II 2960
and Schell (2007) stance use (R 2 = 0.0330). Numbing adds some small additional
Palmieri, Weathers, HTQ – 490 predictive power for depression (ΔR 2 = 0.0466), anxiety (ΔR 2 =
et al. (2007) 0.0149), and panic (ΔR2 = 0.0182), but adds almost nothing to the
Pietrzak et al. PCL CES, PHQ depression, CAGE alcoholism 272 prediction of substance use (ΔR 2 = 0.0013). The addition of intrusions
(2010)
Pruneau (2008) PCL BDI-II, BAI, PAI alcohol and drug use 314
and avoidance accounts for more than an additional 1% of the variance
Pruneau (2009) PCL – 200 for anxiety, but not for depression, panic, or substance use. Intrusions
Schell et al. (2004) PCL – 413 are the best predictor of trauma history (R2 = 0.0779), with the other
factors adding almost no predictive power (ΔR 2 = 0.0050).
J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003 997

Table 3 Table 5
Meta-analysis zero-order correlations. Zero-order correlations for Simms et al. (2002) factors.

Variable 1 Variable 2 K N rc SD robs SD rc 1 2 3 4

Intrusions Anxiety 7 6346 0.396 0.071 0.064 1. Intrusions –


Intrusions Avoidance 42 55,774 0.769 0.126 0.125 2. Avoidance 0.769 –
Intrusions Depression 19 12,491 0.495 0.113 0.108 3. Dysphoria 0.730 0.738 –
Intrusions Dysphoria 16 44,486 0.730 0.114 0.114 4. Hypervigilance 0.678 0.625 0.707 –
Intrusions Hyperarousal 36 35,106 0.725 0.120 0.119 5. Depression 0.495 0.442 0.736 0.502
Intrusions Hypervigilance 15 32,019 0.678 0.113 0.113 6. Anxiety 0.396 0.333 0.583 0.494
Intrusions Numbing 35 35,065 0.694 0.134 0.133 7. Panic 0.403 0.321 0.468 0.372
Intrusions Panic 4 4310 0.403 0.056 0.050 8. Substance Use 0.133 0.097 0.184 0.168
Intrusions Substance 6 5664 0.133 0.051 0.035 9. Trauma 0.279 0.186 0.347 0.201
Intrusions Trauma 10 4877 0.279 0.072 0.057
Avoidance Anxiety 7 6346 0.333 0.058 0.049
Avoidance Depression 22 14,792 0.442 0.100 0.094
Avoidance Dysphoria 16 44,486 0.738 0.128 0.128 3.3. Path models
Avoidance Hyperarousal 35 35,066 0.666 0.137 0.136
Avoidance Hypervigilance 15 32,019 0.625 0.134 0.134 Structural equation modeling was used to explore in greater detail
Avoidance Numbing 35 35,064 0.683 0.152 0.151
the unique predictive power of each dimension of PTSD. Figs. 1–5 show
Avoidance Panic 4 4310 0.321 0.064 0.058
Avoidance Substance 5 4336 0.097 0.049 0.027 the path models for depression, anxiety, panic, substance use, and
Avoidance Trauma 9 4837 0.186 0.114 0.105 trauma history. The path model for depression (Fig. 1) demonstrates
Numbing Anxiety 6 5230 0.542 0.051 0.044 that the King model's hyperarousal (β = 0.417) and numbing
Numbing Depression 18 12,726 0.673 0.083 0.079 (β = 0.443) dimensions are about equally good predictors of depression,
Numbing Hyperarousal 35 35,066 0.800 0.121 0.120
whereas the Simms model's dysphoria dimension (β = 0.887) accounts
Numbing Panic 4 4310 0.443 0.059 0.054
Numbing Substance 4 4064 0.167 0.017 0.000 for nearly all of the variance in depression scores. There is some
Numbing Trauma 8 4564 0.173 0.047 0.019 evidence that avoidance has a negative relationship with depression
Hyperarousal Anxiety 6 5230 0.586 0.045 0.038 after taking other factors of PTSD into account.
Hyperarousal Depression 14 9986 0.679 0.074 0.070
The path model for anxiety (Fig. 2) shows that the King model's
Hyperarousal Panic 4 4310 0.453 0.057 0.051
Hyperarousal Substance 5 5392 0.182 0.040 0.019
hyperarousal dimension (β = 0.492) is that model's best predictor of
Hyperarousal Trauma 9 4605 0.219 0.070 0.054 anxiety, whereas numbing (β = 0.281) also shows unique positive
Dysphoria Anxiety 3 4811 0.583 0.046 0.043 predictive power. The Simms model's dysphoria dimension (β = 0.626)
Dysphoria Depression 11 10,188 0.736 0.072 0.070 is that model's best predictor of anxiety, while hypervigilance
Dysphoria Hypervigilance 15 32,019 0.707 0.143 0.143
(β = 0.221) also shows unique predictive power. As with depression,
Dysphoria Panic 4 4310 0.468 0.059 0.053
Dysphoria Substance 3 3967 0.184 0.051 0.044 avoidance showed a negative relationship with anxiety after controlling
Dysphoria Trauma 5 3196 0.347 0.089 0.081 for other dimensions of PTSD.
Hypervigilance Anxiety 3 4811 0.494 0.035 0.029 The path model for panic (Fig. 3) shows that the King model's
Hypervigilance Depression 11 10,188 0.502 0.089 0.084
hyperarousal dimension (β = 0.229), numbing dimension (β = 0.217),
Hypervigilance Panic 4 4310 0.372 0.079 0.075
Hypervigilance Substance 3 3967 0.168 0.032 0.017
and intrusions dimension (β = 0.172) show unique positive predictive
Hypervigilance Trauma 5 3196 0.201 0.075 0.065 power for panic. The Simms model's dysphoria dimension (β = 0.409) is
the strongest predictor of panic, with intrusions (β = 0.192) and
Note: K = Number of studies; N = Total sample size; rc = Correlation corrected for
sampling error; SD robs = Observed standard deviation of correlation; SD rc = Standard hypervigilance (β = 0.053) showing some positive predictive power.
deviation of correlation corrected for sampling error. Once again, avoidance has a negative relationship with the external
variable after controlling for other dimensions of PTSD.
The path model for substance use (Fig. 4) shows that the King
Squared multiple correlations are given in Table 7 for the Simms model's hyperarousal dimension (β = 0.146) is the best predictor,
et al. (2002) model of PTSD. Dysphoria is the best predictor of all
external variables (Depression: R 2 = 0.5417; Anxiety: R 2 = 0.3404;
Panic: R 2 = 0.2190; Substance Use: R 2 = 0.0338; Trauma: R 2 = Table 6
Squared multiple correlations for King et al. (1998) model of PTSD.
0.1204). During the second step, avoidance accounts for an additional
2% of the variance for depression and anxiety, and an additional 1% of Dependent variable Predictors R2
the variance for trauma history. During the third step, hypervigilance Depression Arous 0.4617
accounts for an additional 2% of anxiety, while the intrusions factor Arous, Numb 0.5083
accounts for an additional 1% of trauma history. Arous, Numb, Avoid 0.5166
Arous, Numb, Avoid, Intrus 0.5168
Anxiety Arous 0.3437
Arous, Numb 0.3586
Arous, Numb, Avoid 0.3742
Arous, Numb, Avoid, Intrus 0.3745
Table 4 Panic Arous 0.2052
Zero-order correlations for King et al. (1998) factors. Arous, Numb 0.2234
Arous, Numb, Intrus 0.2288
1 2 3 4
Arous, Numb, Intrus, Avoid 0.2333
1. Intrusions – Substance Use Arous 0.0330
2. Avoidance 0.769 – Arous, Numb 0.0343
3. Numbing 0.694 0.683 – Arous, Numb, Avoid 0.0364
4. Hyperarousal 0.725 0.666 0.800 – Arous, Numb, Avoid, Intrus 0.0367
5. Depression 0.495 0.442 0.673 0.679 Trauma Intrus 0.0779
6. Anxiety 0.396 0.333 0.542 0.586 Intrus, Avoid 0.0798
7. Panic 0.403 0.321 0.443 0.453 Intrus, Avoid, Arous 0.0811
8. Substance Use 0.133 0.097 0.167 0.182 Intrus, Avoid, Arous, Numb 0.0829
9. Trauma 0.279 0.186 0.173 0.219
Note: Intrus = Intrusions; Avoid = Avoidance; Numb = Numbing; Arous = Hyperarousal.
998 J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003

0.726
Table 7
Squared Multiple Correlations for Simms et al. (2002) Model of PTSD. 0.694 0.666

2
Dependent Variable Predictors R 0.769 0.683 0.800
Depression Dys 0.5417
Intrusions Avoidance Numbing Hyperarousal
Dys, Avoid 0.5638
Dys, Avoid, Intrus 0.5640
Dys, Avoid, Intrus, Vig 0.5640
Anxiety Dys 0.3404
Dys, Avoid 0.3610
Dys, Avoid, Vig 0.3837
Dys, Avoid, Vig, Intrus 0.3837 Anxiety
Panic Dys 0.2190
Dys, Intrus 0.2271
Dys, Intrus, Avoid 0.2357
Dys, Intrus, Avoid, Vig 0.2369
Substance Use Dys 0.0338
Dys, Avoid 0.0371 Avoidance Dysphoria
Intrusions Hypervigilance
Dys, Avoid, Vig 0.0416
Dys, Avoid, Vig, Intrus 0.0418 0.769 0.738 0.707
Trauma Dys 0.1204
Dys, Avoid 0.1310 0.730 0.625
Dys, Avoid, Intrus 0.1416
Dys, Avoid, Intrus, Vig 0.1468 0.678

Note: Intrus = Intrusions; Avoid = Avoidance; Dys = Dysphoria; Vig = Hypervigilance. Fig. 2. Path analysis using King et al. (1998) factors (top) and Simms et al. (2002)
factors (bottom) to predict anxiety. 95% confidence intervals shown.

with numbing (β = 0.084) showing some unique positive predictive


power. The Simms model's dysphoria dimension (β = 0.188) is that external variables compared with the combined nondysphoria factors'
model's best predictor of substance use, with hypervigilance correlations. Structural equation modeling was also used to model
(β = 0.093) also showing some unique positive predictive power. dysphoria and nondysphoria's relationships with these external
For the Simms model, the unique relationship between avoidance and variables (Fig. 6). Dysphoria continued to be the best predictor for
the external variable is again negative. every variable even after collapsing nondysphoria dimensions.
The path model for trauma history (Fig. 5) shows that the King Dysphoria had strong predictive power for depression (β = 0.863),
model's intrusions dimension (β = 0.313) is that model's best whereas nondysphoria did not (β = −0.156). Dysphoria was also a
predictor, with hyperarousal (β = 0.096) showing some unique strong predictor of anxiety (β = 0.519) compared with nondysphoria
positive predictive power. The Simms model's dysphoria dimension (β = 0.079). Dysphoria was the better predictor of panic (β = 0.362),
(β = 0.443) is that model's best predictor of substance use, with but nondysphoria showed some unique predictive power (β = 0.130).
intrusions (β = 0.208) showing some unique predictive power. The Neither dysphoria (β = 0.128) nor nondysphoria (β = 0.068) was a
remaining dimensions are negative predictors of trauma history, with strong predictor of substance use. Dysphoria (β = 0.344) was a strong
the Simms model's avoidance dimension having the most strongly predictor of trauma history, whereas nondysphoria (β = 0.004) is not.
negative predictive power. In order to determine the whether the differences between the
The relative utility of the dysphoria dimension and a combined paths for each model are significant, Root Mean Square Error of
nondysphoria dimension was also examined (Table 8). The multiple Approximation (RMSEA) values were calculated for the models in
correlation between dysphoria and the remaining dimensions of PTSD which all weights were fixed at the same value. When all four
was high (R = 0.814). Dysphoria showed a higher correlation with all dimensions of the King model were assumed to have equal

0.726 0.726
0.694 0.666 0.694 0.666

0.769 0.683 0.800 0.769 0.683 0.800

Intrusions Avoidance Numbing Hyperarousal Intrusions Avoidance Numbing Hyperarousal

Depression Panic

Intrusions Avoidance Dysphoria Hypervigilance Intrusions Avoidance Dysphoria Hypervigilance

0.769 0.738 0.707 0.769 0.738 0.707

0.730 0.625 0.730 0.625


0.678 0.678

Fig. 1. Path analysis using King et al. (1998) factors (top) and Simms et al. (2002) Fig. 3. Path analysis using King et al. (1998) factors (top) and Simms et al. (2002)
factors (bottom) to predict depression. 95% confidence intervals shown. factors (bottom) to predict panic. 95% confidence intervals shown.
J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003 999

0.726
Table 8
0.694 0.666 Multiple correlations for Simms et al. (2002) dysphoria and nondysphoria factors.

0.769 0.683 0.800 1 2

1. Dysphoria –
Intrusions Avoidance Numbing Hyperarousal
2. Nondysphoria 0.814 –
3. Depression 0.736 0.547
4. Anxiety 0.583 0.502
5. Panic 0.468 0.425
6. Substance Use 0.184 0.173
7. Trauma 0.347 0.284
Subst.Use

models, in terms of their psychopathology correlates. The current


study aimed to investigate the relative specificity of each PTSD
symptom dimension, and to determine whether the King et al. (1998)
Intrusions Avoidance Dysphoria Hypervigilance
or the Simms et al. (2002) dimensions better account for the
comorbidity between PTSD and other psychological problems.
0.769 0.738 0.707
4.1. Predictions and findings regarding the general-dysphoria model
0.730 0.625

0.678 If the Simms model's dysphoria dimension represents Mineka


Fig. 4. Path analysis using King et al. (1998) factors (top) and Simms et al. (2002)
et al.'s (1998) general factor, it would be expected to have the
factors (bottom) to predict substance use. 95% confidence intervals shown. strongest relationship with depression, anxiety, and panic compared
with other symptom dimensions of PTSD. Simms et al. (2002)
speculate that intrusions are specific to PTSD, whereas avoidance
predicative power, the RMSEA values were 0.166 for depression, 0.147 and hypervigilance are mid-level dimensions common to the anxiety
for anxiety, 0.069 for panic, 0.038 for substance use, and 0.062 for disorders. If this hypothesis is true, intrusions would be expected to
trauma history. When all four dimensions of the Simms model were have comparatively less overlap with depression, anxiety, and panic,
assumed to have equal predictive power, the RMSEA values were and to have greater overlap with trauma history, whereas hypervi-
0.225 for depression, 0.160 for anxiety, 0.086 for panic, 0.045 for gilance and avoidance would be expected to have stronger relation-
substance use, and 0.102 for trauma history. When unconstrained, all ships with anxiety and panic than with depression. Mineka et al.
models had an RMSEA value of 0. Restricting the four dimensions of (1998) have suggested that negative emotionality/dysphoria may also
each model to have equal associations therefore resulted in underlie the comorbidity between the internalizing disorders and
significantly poor fit of the models, especially for the Simms model. externalizing disorders such as substance abuse. For this reason,
The results of these constraint tests suggest that the factors of the dysphoria may be expected to have a stronger relationship with
Simms model are more differentiated in their patterns of associations substance use problems compared with other symptom dimensions.
with external variables than the King model. In path analyses involving the Simms et al. (2002) symptom
dimensions (Figs. 1–5), dysphoria was the strongest predictor of all
4. Discussion external variables, with hypervigilance showing some additional
positive predictive power for anxiety and substance use. Intrusions
Several studies have investigated the relative fit of the King et al. showed some additional positive predictive power for trauma history,
(1998) and the Simms et al. (2002) models of PTSD. Comparatively whereas avoidance was consistently a negative predictor of all
few, however, have examined the external validity of these two external variables after controlling for other factors of PTSD. These
results support the hypothesis that dysphoria is a general factor,
hypervigilance is a mid-level factor, and intrusive re-experiencing is a
0.726
specific factor of PTSD; they do not support the hypothesis that
0.694 0.666
avoidance is a mid-level factor.
0.769 0.683 0.800 In order to determine whether differences in predictive power
between the factors of PTSD were significant, RMSEA values were
Intrusions Avoidance Numbing Hyperarousal
calculated for models in which all factors of PTSD were assumed to
have equal patterns of associations with external variables. With the
exception of substance use (RMSEA = 0.045), none met traditional
criteria for good fit (RMSEA b 0.05) under this constraint. These
findings suggest that the predictive differences between factors are
Trauma important.

4.2. Predictions and findings regarding other models

Because King et al. (1998) do not discuss the relative specificity of


Intrusions Avoidance Dysphoria Hypervigilance their symptom dimensions, a priori hypotheses are difficult to make for
their model. Because intrusions and avoidance are common to both
0.769 0.738 0.707 models, it was hypothesized that intrusions will be specific to PTSD,
showing a strong relationship to trauma history, whereas avoidance will
0.730 0.625
show a stronger relationship with anxiety than with depression.
0.678
Because numbing and hyperarousal both include symptoms of general
Fig. 5. Path analysis using King et al. (1998) factors (top) and Simms et al. (2002) distress, they should both show strong relationships with depression,
factors (bottom) to predict trauma history. 95% confidence intervals shown. anxiety, and panic. It is possible that numbing, including symptoms of
1000 J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003

0.814 0.814 0.814


Dysphoria Nondysphoria Dysphoria Nondysphoria Dysphoria Nondysphoria

Depression Anxiety Panic

0.814 0.814
Dysphoria Nondysphoria Dysphoria Nondysphoria

Subst.Use Trauma

Fig. 6. Path analysis using Simms et al. (2002) dyphoria and nondysphoria factors to predict external variables. 95% confidence intervals shown.

anhedonia, will show a stronger relationship with depression, whereas These findings are consistent with Grant et al.'s (2008) conclusion
hyperarousal, including symptoms of anxious arousal, will show a that the dysphoria dimension is not specific to PTSD. Some
stronger relationship with anxiety and panic, but this is speculation. It is researchers (e.g., Spitzer et al., 2007) have suggested eliminating
also expected that numbing and hyperarousal will show a stronger PTSD symptoms that overlap with other disorders from the next
relationship with substance use than intrusions and avoidance, again edition of the DSM. However, this study has found that the dysphoria
due to their nonspecific symptoms. dimension has the strongest relationship with trauma history of any
Path analysis involving the King et al. (1998) model suggests that of the dimensions that were investigated. These findings suggest that
numbing and hyperarousal both represent general distress, with the dysphoria symptoms are an important part of the overall
hyperarousal showing some additional specificity to anxiety and construct of PTSD, and likely provide important clinical information.
substance use. The intrusions factor was the best predictor of trauma Given the relatively high likelihood that traumatized individuals will
history, with none of the other factors being strong positive predictors develop MDD or GAD (Grant et al., 2008), there is some obvious value
for this variable. Avoidance was consistently a weak negative in measuring this general factor.
predictor for all external variables after taking the other factors Overall, the Simms et al. (2002) model seems better able to
of PTSD into account. These results support the hypothesis that separate specific and general variance; whereas dysphoria clearly
numbing and hyperarousal are general factors, whereas intrusive re- represents the general factor in this model, the King et al. (1998)
experiencing is specific to PTSD; they do not support the hypothesis model separates this general factor into two components: numbing
that avoidance is a mid-level factor. and hyperarousal. Both factors are equally able to predict depression,
As with the Simms model, RMSEA values were calculated for models with hyperarousal showing some specificity to anxiety. The same
in which all factors of PTSD were assumed to have equal weight. Again, pattern is found for substance use; however, the Simms model's
only the prediction of substance use (RMSEA = 0.038) was well hypervigilance factor also shows some predictive power for substance
predicted when these restrictions were put in place. These findings use, suggesting that the comorbidity between PTSD and substance use
suggest that the differences between the factors are important. is not due to a single shared distress factor. The hypervigilance factor
Further analyses were conducted to test Marshall et al.'s (2010) may in this case represents the hyperactivity associated with
conclusion that PTSD's dysphoria and nondysphoria symptoms are impulsivity and externalizing problems. However, the results for
equally associated with general distress. These researchers suggested substance use are based on relatively few studies compared to the
that dysphoria shows stronger associations largely because it includes results for depression and anxiety, and should be interpreted with
more symptoms than some of the other dimensions and that, since caution. In addition, models in which the factors of PTSD were
dysphoria shows no unique relationship with general distress assumed to have equal predictive power were still able to adequately
compared with a combined nondysphoria dimension, there is no predict substance use problems. Given the high levels of comorbidity
reason to separate these symptoms into dimensions. Although the between PTSD and substance use disorders (Brady, Back, & Coffey,
dysphoria and nondysphoria symptoms in this study were highly 2004), this is an area that warrants future study. In general, the King
correlated, the dysphoria dimension showed higher correlations with model's hyperarousal dimension seems to be a better representation
all external variables (Table 8). Moreover, dysphoria showed an of the general factor than numbing is.
ability to predict depression and anxiety above and beyond the The finding that factors of Simms et al. (2002) model are more
nondysphoria symptoms (Figs. 6–10). Interestingly, dysphoria was differentiated than those of the King et al. (1998) model is also
also superior in predicting trauma history compared with the supported by the RMSEA values for the constrained versions of each
theoretically more specific nondysphoria symptoms. Neither dimen- model. Constraining the models such that each factor is assumed to
sion was able to strongly predict substance use problems, and neither have equal predictive power resulted in higher RMSEA values for the
was clearly superior to the other in predicting this variable. Simms model than for the King model. Because the King model is less
differentiated, it is less affected by these constraints.
4.3. Summary of our findings and areas for further research These results are relevant to the upcoming DSM-5 revisions. The
DSM-5's proposed diagnostic criteria for PTSD divides an expanded
This meta-analysis confirms that the dysphoria dimension repre- set of symptoms into four dimensions, following the King et al. (1998)
sents a general factor common to PTSD, anxiety, and depression. four-factor model (Frueh, Elhai, & Acierno, 2010). Although it had
J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003 1001

been suggested that symptoms overlapping with depression be trauma history and trauma severity. The ability to replicate these
eliminated (Spitzer et al., 2007), the proposed criteria actually expand results may therefore be limited due to instrument-specific variations.
PTSD's numbing factor (now called “negative alterations in cognitions The DSM-IV's criterion A for PTSD offers one possible standardized
and mood”), including persistent distorted blame of oneself or others method for assessing trauma history, and examining the relationship
and a pervasive negative emotional state. The DSM-IV's hyperarousal between criterion A and the dimensions of PTSD across a variety of
factor (now called “alterations in arousal and reactivity”) will likewise samples would be an important follow-up.
be expanded, including a symptom of reckless or self-destructive Third, this study includes findings from a variety of different
behavior. Given our findings, it seems unlikely that these dimensions populations, including military and civilian populations. A recent
will parsimoniously represent specific and general components of meta-analysis by Yufik and Simms (2010) suggests that the structure
PTSD. According to our analyses, both the proposed negative of PTSD is relatively invariant across populations. However, other
cognitions/mood dimension and the proposed arousal dimension studies (e.g., Elhai et al., 2009) have found differences. It remains
are likely general factors; as such their inclusion in DSM-5 may possible that systematic differences across the included populations
actually decrease distinctions between PTSD and depression or other may have influenced our results.
internalizing syndromes. Finally, this study did not include a variety of potentially
Another noteworthy finding in this study is that avoidance was a interesting external variables due to limitations in the literature.
consistent negative predictor of all external variables after accounting The relationships between PTSD dimensions and a variety of DSM-IV
for other factors of PTSD, especially when the symptoms are divided diagnoses would be a potentially fruitful line of investigation to
using the Simms model. This finding is inconsistent with the expand this study's conclusions.
hypothesis that avoidance is a mid-level factor that is associated
with anxiety. This factor's lack of positive predictive power may be
5. Conclusions
due to its small size, consisting only of two symptoms. However,
hypervigilance also consists only of two symptoms, and showed
Amidst studies testing the relative fit for competing factor models
unique overlap with anxiety and substance use.
of PTSD, it is important to ask how these factors relate to external
Because zero-order correlations between avoidance and the
constructs. This study supports the Simms, et al. model's interpreta-
external variables of interest are positive, avoidance does not appear
tion of dysphoria in PTSD as a general factor that accounts for much of
to be a unitary construct. Rather, it appears to be divided into a
the overlap between PTSD and other disorders, whereas intrusive re-
general subfactor, which overlaps with the other factors of PTSD and is
experiencing is a dimension that shows specificity to PTSD. While
positively associated with external psychopathology variables, and a
these findings should be supplemented with investigations of other
specific subfactor, which is negatively associated with external
external clinical constructs, they shed important light on the utility of
psychopathology variables. One possibility is that the specific
these dimensional models.
subfactor is associated with high levels of behavioral activation.
Avoidance is thought to play a critical role in the maintenance of PTSD
symptoms, and the reduction of avoidance is a major goal of Acknowledgments
treatment (Foa, Hembree, & Rothbaum, 2007); a better understanding
of this factor's nature would therefore be helpful. We would like to thank David Watson, Lee Anna Clark, Michael
Finally, this study's findings are relevant to the treatment of PTSD. O'Hara, and Daniel Tranel for their helpful comments on earlier drafts
Gros et al. (2010) have suggested that, given the overlap between of this manuscript.
PTSD and depression, therapies designed for depression, such as
behavioral activation, should be included in treatment protocols for References 1
PTSD. Our results show that nonspecific distress is an important
component of PTSD, and is highly associated with trauma history. *Asmundson, G. J. G., Stapleton, J. A., & Taylor, S. (2004). Are avoidance and numbing
distinct PTSD symptom clusters? Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 467–475.
Treatments that target this nonspecific distress may therefore be *Baschnagel, J. S., O'Connor, R. M., Colder, C. R., & Hawk, L. W., Jr. (2005). Factor
helpful to include in future treatment protocols. For example, Gros structure of posttraumatic stress among western New York undergraduates
et al. (2010) argue that behavioral activation, which targets following the September 11th terrorist attack on the world trade center. Journal
of Traumatic Stress, 18, 677–684.
depression and general distress, would be a useful supplement to *Beck, J. G., Grant, D. M., Clapp, J. D., & Palyo, S. A. (2009). Understanding the
therapies that specifically target PTSD symptoms. interpersonal impact of trauma: Contributions of PTSD and depression. Journal of
Anxiety Disorders, 23, 443–450.
*Boelen, P. A., van, d. H., & van, d. B. (2008). The factor structure of posttraumatic stress
4.4. Limitations disorder symptoms among bereaved individuals: A confirmatory factor analysis
study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 1377–1383.
Important limitations of this study should be noted. First, the *Carragher, N., Mills, K., Slade, T., Teesson, M., & Silove, D. (2010). Factor structure of
posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in the Australian general population.
meta-analysis was limited to PTSD measures that closely follow the Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 520–527.
DSM-IV symptoms. Because the DSM was not written with these *Cook, J., Jakupcak, M., Rosenheck, R., Fontana, A., & McFall, M. (2009). Influence of PTSD
structural models in mind, avoidance and hypervigilance are poorly symptom clusters on smoking status among help-seeking Iraq and Afghanistan
veterans. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 11, 1189–1195.
modeled compared to intrusions, numbing, hyperarousal, and
*Cox, B. J., Mota, N., Clara, I., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2008). The symptom structure of
dysphoria. For this reason, this study may understate the predictive posttraumatic stress disorder in the national comorbidity replication survey.
power of these two constructs. Future studies should use instruments Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 1523–1528.
that better model these dimensions. However, despite these concerns, *Declercq, F., Vanheule, S., & Deheegher, J. (2010). Alexithymia and posttraumatic
stress: Subscales and symptom clusters. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 66, 1–14.
there exists a robust literature examining the structure of PTSD, and *DuHamel, K. N., Ostrof, J., Ashman, T., Winkel, G., Mundy, E. A., Keane, T. M., et al.
this literature has informed work on the disorder's DSM-5 criteria. (2004). Construct validity of the posttraumatic stress disorder checklist in
Given that PTSD is operationally defined by these symptoms, and cancer survivors: Analyses based on two samples. Psychological Assessment, 16,
255–266.
given the heavy emphasis on using a multi-factor model for diagnostic *Elhai, J. D., Gray, M. J., Docherty, A. R., Kashdan, T. B., & Kose, S. (2007). Structural
purposes, an understanding of these factors is important. validity of the posttraumatic stress disorder checklist among college students with
Second, the current meta-analysis aggregates different measures a trauma history. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 22, 1471–1478.
that are thought to measure the same construct. This may be
particularly problematic for measures of trauma history. Most of the
studies cited used their own idiosyncratic instruments to measure 1
* Denotes study included in meta-analysis.
1002 J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003

*Elklit, A., & Shevlin, M. (2007). The structure of PTSD symptoms: A test of alternative *Witteveen, A. B., Van,, d. P., Bramsen, I., Huizink, A. C., Slottje, P., Smid, T., et al. (2006).
models using confirmatory factor analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, Dimensionality of the posttraumatic stress response among police officers and
299–313. fire fighters: An evaluation of two self-report scales. Psychiatry Research, 141,
*Elklit, A., Armour, C., & Shevlin, M. (2010). Testing alternative factor models of PTSD 213–228.
and the robustness of the dysphoria factor. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, *Wright, C. V., & Johnson, D. M. (2009). Correlates for legal help-seeking: Contextual
147–154. factors for battered women in shelter. Violence and Victims, 24, 771.
*Feuer, C. A., Nishith, P., & Resick, P. (2005). Prediction of numbing and effortful *Zahradnik, M., Stewart, S. H., Marshall, G. N., Schell, T. L., & Jaycox, L. H. (2009). Anxiety
avoidance in female rape survivors with chronic PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, sensitivity and aspects of alexithymia are independently and uniquely associated
18, 165–170. with posttraumatic distress. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22, 131–138.
*Flack, W. F., Jr., Milanak, M. E., & Kimble, M. O. (2005). Emotional numbing in relation Bloch, M. H., Landeros-Weisenberger, A., Rosario, M. C., Pittenger, C., & Leckman, J. F.
to stressful civilian experiences among college students. Journal of Traumatic Stress, (2008). Meta-analysis of the symptom structure of obsessive-compulsive disorder.
18, 569–573. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1532–1542.
*Forbes, D., Haslam, N., Williams, B. J., & Creamer, M. (2005). Testing the latent structure Brady, K. T., Back, S. E., & Coffey, S. F. (2004). Substance abuse and posttraumatic stress
of posttraumatic stress disorder: A taxometric study of combat veterans. Journal of disorder. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 206–209.
Traumatic Stress, 18, 647–656. Brett, E. A., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (1988). DSM-III-R criteria for posttraumatic
*Gootzeit, J. H. (2010). Measuring Dimensions of PTSD: The Iowa Traumatic Response stress disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 1232–1236.
Inventory. Unpublished Raw Data. Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Hickling, E. J. (1998). A confirmatory factor
*Hetzel-Riggin, M. D. (2009). A test of structural invariance of posttraumatic stress analysis of posttraumatic stress symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36,
symptoms in female survivors of sexual and/or physical abuse or assault. 1091–1099.
Traumatology, 15, 46–59. Cordova, M. J., Studts, J. L., Hann, D. M., Jacobsen, P. B., & Andrykowski, M. A. (2000).
*Hoyt, T., & Yeater, E. A. (2010). Comparison of posttraumatic stress disorder symptom Symptom structure of PTSD following breast cancer. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13,
structure models in Hispanic and white college students. Psychological Trauma: 301–319.
Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 2, 19–30. Cox, B. J., Clara, I. P., & Enns, M. W. (2002). Posttraumatic stress disorder and the
*Johnson, D. M., Palmieri, P. A., Jackson, A. P., & Hobfoll, S. E. (2007). Emotional numbing structure of common mental disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 15, 168–171.
weakens abused inner-city women's resiliency resources. Journal of Traumatic Elhai, J. D., Grubaugh, A. L., Kashdan, T. B., & Frue, B. C. (2008). Empirical examination of
Stress, 20, 197–206. a proposed refinement to DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder symptom criteria
*King, D. W., Leskin, G. A., King, L. A., & Weathers, F. W. (1998). Confirmatory factor using the National Comorbidity Survey Replication data. The Journal of Clinical
analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD scale: Evidence for the dimensionality Psychiatry, 69, 597–602.
of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Assessment, 10, 90–96. Elhai, J. D., Engdahl, R. M., Palmieri, P. A., Naifeh, J. A., Schweinle, A., & Jacobs, G. A.
*Mansfield, A. J., Williams, J., Hourani, L. L., & Babeu, L. A. (2010). Measurement (2009). Assessing posttraumatic stress disorder with or without reference to a
invariance of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms among U.S. military single, worst traumatic event: Examining differences in factor structure. Psycho-
personnel. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 23, 91–99. logical Assessment, 21, 629–634.
*Marshall, G. N., Schell, T. L., & Miles, J. N. V. (2010). All PTSD symptoms are highly Enders, C. K., & Peugh, J. L. (2004). Using an EM covariance matrix to estimate structural
associated with general distress: Ramifications for the dysphoria symptom cluster. equation models with missing data: Choosing an adjusted sample size to improve
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 126–135. the accuracy of inferences. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 1–19.
*McDevitt-Murphy, M., Weathers, F. W., Adkins, J. W., & Daniels, J. B. (2005). Use of the Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., & Gershuny, B. S. (1995). Arousal, numbing, and intrusion:
personality assessment inventory in assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder in Symptom structure of PTSD following assault. The American Journal of Psychiatry,
women. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 57–65. 152, 116–120.
*McDonald, S. D., Beckham, J. C., Morey, R., Marx, C., Tupler, L. A., & Calhoun, P. S. (2008). Foa, E. B., Hembree, E. A., & Rothbaum, B. O. (2007). Prolonged exposure therapy for PTSD:
Factorial invariance of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms across three Emotional processing of traumatic experiences. New York, NY: Oxford University
veteran samples. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 309–317. Press.
*Miller, M. W., Wolf, E. J., Martin, E., Kaloupek, D. G., & Keane, T. M. (2008). Structural Frueh, B. C., Elhai, J. D., & Acierno, R. (2010). The future of posttraumatic stress disorder
equation modeling of associations among combat exposure, PTSD symptom factors, in the DSM. Psychological Injury and Law, 3, 260–270.
and global assessment of functioning. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Grant, D. M., Beck, J. G., Marques, S. A., Palyo, S. A., & Clapp, J. D. (2008). The structure of
Development, 45, 359–370. distress following trauma: Posttraumatic stress disorder, major depressive
*Naifeh, J. A., Elhai, J. D., Kashdan, T. B., & Grubaugh, A. L. (2008). The PTSD symptom disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117,
scale's latent structure: An examination of trauma-exposed medical patients. 662–672.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 1355–1368. Gros, D. F., Simms, L. J., & Acierno, R. (2010). Specificity of posttraumatic stress disorder
*Newton, T. L., Parker, B. C., & Ho, I. K. (2005). Ambulatory cardiovascular functioning in symptoms: An investigation of comorbidity between posttraumatic stress disorder
healthy postmenopausal women with victimization histories. Biological Psychology, symptoms and depression in treatment-seeking veterans. The Journal of Nervous
70, 121–130. and Mental Disease, 198, 885–890.
*Palmieri, P. A., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2005). Confirmatory factor analysis of posttraumatic Grubaugh, A. L., Long, M. E., Elhai, J. D., Frueh, B. C., & Magruder, K. M. (2010). An
stress symptoms in sexually harassed women. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, examination of the construct validity of posttraumatic stress disorder
657–666. with veterans using a revised criterion set. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48,
*Palmieri, P. A., Weathers, F. W., Difede, J., & King, D. W. (2007). Confirmatory factor 909–914.
analysis of the PTSD checklist and the clinician-administered PTSD scale in disaster Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias
workers exposed to the world trade center ground zero. Journal of Abnormal in research findings (2nd Edition). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.
Psychology, 116, 329–341. Keane, T. M. (1993). Symptomatology of Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress
*Palmieri, P. A., Marshall, G. N., & Schell, T. L. (2007). Confirmatory factor analysis of disorder. In J. R. T. Davidson & E. B. Foa (Eds.), Posttraumatic stress disorder: DSM-IV
posttraumatic stress symptoms in Cambodian refugees. Journal of Traumatic Stress, and beyond (pp. 99–111). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press.
20, 207–216. Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005).
*Pietrzak, R. H., Goldstein, M. C., Malley, J. C., Rivers, A. J., & Southwick, S. M. (2010). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the
Structure of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and psychosocial functioning national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62,
in veterans of operations enduring freedom and Iraqi freedom. Psychiatry Research, 617–627.
178, 323–329. Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General
*Pruneau, G. (2008). Distinctiveness of avoidance and numbing in PTSD: Auburn Psychiatry, 56, 921–926.
University. McNally, R. J. (2003). Progress and controversy in the study of posttraumatic stress
*Pruneau, G. (2009). Relationships among adult attachment, social support, and PTSD disorder. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 229–252.
symptoms in trauma-exposed college students. : Auburn University. Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood
*Schell, T. L., Marshall, G. N., & Jaycox, L. H. (2004). All symptoms are not created equal: disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 49 377-339.
The prominent role of hyperarousal in the natural course of posttraumatic Rosen, G. M., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2008). Posttraumatic stress disorder: An empirical
psychological distress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 189–197. evaluation of core assumptions. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 837–868.
*Shelby, R. A., Golden-Kreutz, D., & Andersen, B. L. (2005). Mismatch of posttraumatic Schmidt, F. L., & Le, H. (2004). Software for the Hunter-Schmidt meta-analysis
stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms and DSM-IV symptom clusters in a cancer methods. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa, Department of Management &
sample: Exploratory factor analysis of the PTSD checklist-civilian version. Journal of Organizations.
Traumatic Stress, 18, 347–357. Schnurr, P. P., Lunney, C. A., Bovin, M. J., & Marx, B. P. (2009). Posttraumatic stress
*Shevlin, M., McBride, O., Armour, C., & Adamson, G. (2009). Reconciling the differences disorder and quality of life: Extension of findings to veterans of the wars in Iraq and
between the King et al. (1998) and Simms et al. (2002) factor models of PTSD. Afghanistan. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 727–735.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 995–1001. Shafer, A. B. (2006). Meta-analysis of the factor structures of four depression
*Simms, L. J., Watson, D., & Doebbelling, B. N. (2002). Confirmatory factor analyses of questionnaires: Beck, CES-D, Hamilton, and Zung. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
posttraumatic stress symptoms in deployed and nondeployed veterans of the gulf 62, 123–146.
war. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 637–647. Smith, M. Y., Redd, W., DuHamel, K., Vickberg, S. J., & Ricketts, P. (1999). Validation of
*Taft, C. T., Kaloupek, D. G., Schumm, J. A., Marshall, A. D., Panuzio, J., King, D. W., et al. the PTSD checklist — Civilian version in survivors of bone marrow transplantation.
(2007). Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms, physiological reactivity, alcohol Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12, 485–499.
problems, and aggression among military veterans. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Smith, R. C., Ryan, M. A. K., Wingard, D. L., Slyman, D. J., Sallis, J. F., & Kritz-Silverstein, D.
116, 498–507. (2008). New onset and persistent symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder self
J. Gootzeit, K. Markon / Clinical Psychology Review 31 (2011) 993–1003 1003

reported after deployment and combat exposures: Prospective population based Watson, D. (2009). Differentiating the mood and anxiety disorders: A quadripartite
US military cohort study. British Medical Journal, 336, 366–371. model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 221–247.
Spitzer, R. L., First, M. B., & Wakefield, J. C. (2007). Saving PTSD from itself in DSM-V. Yufik, T., & Simms, L. J. (2010). A meta-analytic investigation of the structure of
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 233–241. posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119,
Taylor, S., Kuch, K., Koch, W. J., Crockett, D. J., & Passey, G. (1998). The structure of 764–776.
posttraumatic stress symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 154–160.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen