Sie sind auf Seite 1von 8

The relationship between post-natal depression and mother-child

interaction.
A Stein, D H Gath, J Bucher, A Bond, A Day and P J Cooper
BJP 1991, 158:46-52.
Access the most recent version at DOI: 10.1192/bjp.158.1.46

References This article cites 0 articles, 0 of which you can access for free at:
http://bjp.rcpsych.org/content/158/1/46#BIBL
Reprints/ To obtain reprints or permission to reproduce material from this paper, please write
permissions to permissions@rcpsych.ac.uk

You can respond http://bjp.rcpsych.org/letters/submit/bjprcpsych;158/1/46


to this article at
Downloaded http://bjp.rcpsych.org/ on June 1, 2014
from Published by The Royal College of Psychiatrists

To subscribe to The British Journal of Psychiatry go to:


http://bjp.rcpsych.org/site/subscriptions/
British Journal of Psychiatry (1991), 158, 46—52

The Relationship between Post-natal Depression and


Mother—Child Interaction
ALAN STEIN, DENNIS H. GATH, JANET BUCHER, ALISON BOND, ANN DAY and PETER J. COOPER

The study was based on an index group of 49 mothers who had had depressive disorders
in the post-natal year, and 49 control mothers who had been free from any psychiatric disorder
since delivery. Nineteen months after childbirth, the interaction between mother and child
was assessedby blind assessorsusing defined observationalmethods. Comparedwith controls,
index mother—childpairs showed a reduced quality of interaction (e.g. mothers showed less
facilitation of their children, children showed less affective sharing and less initial sociability
with a stranger). Similar but reduced effects were seen in a subgroup of index mothers and
children where the mother had recovered from depression by 19 months. Social and marital
difficulties were associated with reduced quality of mother—childinteraction.

There is good evidence for a link between depressive interfering with the functions and responsibilities of a
disorder in mothers and emotional disturbance in care giver and with the development of a good affective
their children (Richman eta!, 1982; Biffings & Moos, relationship with the child―.It is arguable that younger
1983; Pound et a!, 1985). The reasons for this link children (up to the age of 18 months) are likely to be
are not established, but Rutter & Quinton (1984) have particularly vulnerable to effects of this kind, since they
suggested several possibilities, including the influence are entirely dependent on their carers. Hence studies
of the mothers' depression on the way the mother of mothers who were depressed in the post-natal year
and child interact. should throw speciallight on mother-child interactions.
Two studies have thrown light on such interaction. Field (1984) found that the young infants of post
Firstly, in a study of children aged two to three years, natally depressed mothers (compared with those of
Radke-Yarrow et al(1985) used Ainsworth's Strange non-depressed mothers) showed less frequent positive
Situation and found that insecure attachment was and more frequent negative facial expressions. They
more frequent in children whose mothers had a also vocalised more. The depressed mothers showed
history of major depression than in those whose similar differences in facial expressions; they vocalised
mothers had no such history. The evidence for less and spent less time looking at and touching their
insecure attachment was the child's avoidance of and children. In a recent study, Murray (personal
resistance to the mother when reunited with her after communication) found that post-natally depressed
brief separation. The second study also examined mothers were slower to respond to their children; the
children aged two to three years (Cox et a!, 1987). latter looked at their mothers less and showed more
Compared with children of non-depressed mothers, self-directed activity.
those of depressed mothers showed more behaviour These studies have largely focused on mothers with
problems and more difficulties in expressive language. concurrent depression. It is arguable however that
The important findings were that depressed mothers mother—child interactions should also be studied
were less likely to use explanations, suggestions and after the mother has recovered from her depression.
questions when dealing with their children, and more Post-natal depression provides a good opportunity
likely to ignore their children's requests. Depressed for studying this question since it is often of relatively
mothers were also less likely to pick up cues from short duration. Thus, by studying the mothers and
their children; if they did so, their responses were children during the second year of the children's
less likely to be taken up by the children. The lives, a good proportion of mothers will have
complexity of possible causal factors was shown by recovered from their depression while some will have
the finding that depressed mothers were also having remained depressed. We therefore examined mother
more marital and social difficulties. child interactions in three groups 19 months after the
When reporting their study of children aged two child was born. In the first group the mother was
to three years, Radke-Yarrow eta! (1985) commented depressed during the post-natal year and also at 19
that: “¿the
depressed parent is the primary environ months; in the second, the mother was depressed in
ment of the young child. . - the behaviours and the post-natal year but not at 19 months; and in the
mental status of a depressed person are all potentially third the mother had been free from depression

46
POST-NATAL DEPRESSIONAND MOTHER-CHILD INTERACTION 47
throughout. The aim was to discover whether impaired unpublished manual). Chronic social difficulties were scored
mother—childinteraction was more frequent in (a) on a five-point scale from absent to severe. Inter-rater
mothers who had been depressed during the post-natal reliability for ratings of events and difficulties was found
year when compared with those free from depression; to besatisfactory
(weighted
kappa0.76—0.94).
(b) mothers who had recovered from their depression Secondly, within the following week, each mother and
child were assessed by one of two research psychiatrists,
by 19 months when compared with mothers free who wereblind to the mother's index/control status. Five
from depression (since the birth of their children). assessments were made in the following order.

Method Child's sociability to a stranger


In a previous study, Cooper etal(1988) had examined a large This assessment was based on a semi-standardised procedure
seriesof 483mothers four times:two months before child devised by Stevenson & Lamb (1979) to assess the child's
birth, and three, six and 12 months after delivery. At each initial responses to a stranger in the mother's presence. In
assessment the mothers had completed the 60-item General this procedure, which was originally designed for laboratory
Health Questionnaire (GHQ; Goldberg, 1972). Mothers with use, the stranger makes a series of increasinglyintrusive
aGHQ total score of l2or above had been further examined approaches to the child. In the present study, the technique
with a standardised psychiatric interview, the Present State was first practised in the laboratory and later adapted for
Examination (PSE; Wing eta!, 1974);the depression rating the home. To adapt the procedure for use in the home, some
scale (MADRS) devised by Montgomery & Asberg (1979) elements of Thompson & Lamb's method were omitted,
was also completed. Sixty women had been identified as but the essentials were the same as theirs.
PSE cases on at least one of the three post-natal assessments. The test lasted three to five minutes. It began with the
In the present study, 19 months after childbirth 49 of child sitting on the mother's lap. The stranger (the research
these 60 women were assessed with their infants. Eleven of psychiatrist) offered the child a toy, and then tried to initiate
the 60 wereomitted; of these, two had had twins,two were a ‘¿give-and-take'exchange of the toy. Next an assessment
known to the research assessors, and the rest had moved was made of the child's response to being placed on the
away. Also assessed was a control group of 49 mothers who floor. Finally, the researcher moved to the floor, and again
had scored below 12on the GHQ at all three post-partum offered the child a toy, and again tried to start a ‘¿give-and
assessments; they were individually matched by age, social take' exchange. The child's response to each initiative was
class and parity. Their sociodemographic characteristics are rated on a 1-5 scale ranging from withdrawn/distressed to
given in Table 1. Fortuitously equal numbers of mothers utgoing/friendly (see Appendix for details). The interrater
in both groups were cohabiting with male partners.
reliability for this assessment was 0.78 (weighted kappa).
Mother and child in structured play
Table 1
Sociodemographic characteristics of mothers The mother and child were observed at play in three
situations each lasting three minutes. The mother was asked
Index group Control group to behave as she usually would.
(n=49) (n=49) Firstly, the child was given a large plastic nut and bolt;
this was chosen as a simple toy which was likely to interest
(17-37)No.
Medianage(range):
years27 (16—40)27
the child only briefly and would require substantial effort
class2322No.
inmiddle
cohabiting4545No.
married
and/or
stably
by the mother to sustain the child's interest. Secondly, the
primiparous1414 child was given a toy hippopotamus which could be pulled
on a string; also five plastic shapes could be posted through
the animal's back and retrieved from its mouth. This was
Assessments chosen as a more complex toy, which was likely to interest
but also to frustrate the child if the mother did not help.
Index and control mothers were examined on two occasions Thirdly, the child was given a picture book and the mother
when the child was 19 months old. Firstly, an independent was asked to read it with the child.
research assessor examined each mother's mental state with These situations were chosen in order to assess how the
the PSE and the MADRS. The assessor confirmed that none mother and child dealt with them; for example, how the
of the control mothers had had any psychological child approached the toy, and how the mother maintained
difficulties since delivery. She also took the opportunity the child's interest and dealt with any difficulties. For this
to assessrecentlifeevents(Paykel, 1980)and chronicsocial purpose six categories of behaviour were defined:
difficulties. This interview ascertains the occurrence of
potentially stressful events during the previous 12 months (a) child approaches assessor with/without toy
under six headings: work, finance, health, relationships, (b) child plays with toy without involving mother
legal and other. The interview was modified and extended (c) child and mother interact verbally or non-verbally
to make a detailed assessment of any chronic social in playing with toy
difficulties, for example marital and housing problems. (d) mother ignores child
Ratings of ‘¿objectivenegative impact' and ‘¿the
degree (e) mother facilitates child (this fifth category, derived
of independence' for each life event was made by the from the work of Mills eta! (1985), was defined as
interviewers (as recommended by Paykel & Mangen, 1980, any maternal behaviour (verbal or non-verbal) which
48 STEIN ET AL
Table 2
Structured play results for (a) 49 cases v. 49 controls, (b) 25 recovered cases v. 25 controls, and
(c) 14 index subjects with severechronic social difficulties (SCSD)v. 35 index subjects without SCSD
Structured play items(a)(b)(C)ControlsCasesCasesSCSD
Non-SCSD(n=49)(n=49)(n=25)(n=14)
(n=35)Mean
ZChild MeanZMeanZMeanMean
0.47Child
playsalone3.1 3.60.793.40.5473.63.6
turnstoobserver0.3
0.9Child's 0.51.30.20.590.60.5
affective
2.1―Mother sharing3.8 2.42.9―2.52.24―1.13.0
1.5Motherignoreschild0.8
and child interact21.1 17.02.7―17.71.614.118.3
1.74Motherfacilitates20.9 1.51.161.61.342.41.1
15.82.78―17.11.91'10.817.8 2.4―
‘¿P<O.06.
“¿P<O.05,
“¿P<O.O1.

helped to maintain the child's interest and attention Reliability


in playing with the toy; for example, the mother
Inter-rater reliability checks were made regularly during the
might rattle the hippopotamus and open its mouth
to indicate how the shapes could be retrieved.) study with the following results: (a) for time-sampled ratings
(f) child shows affective sharing (this type of behaviour and measuresof child's reactionto mother's departureand
was defined as the child “¿smiling and vocalising return:range0.77-0.92(weightedkappa);and (b) for global
and/or smiling and showing toys during joint play― measures: range 0.62—0.82 (weighted kappa).
(Waters et a!, 1979); for example, the child undoes
the nut and bolt, then looks at the mother, smiles, Results
and shows her the toy.)
As mentioned under Method, 49 index mothers had
Ratings of these six categories were made for six previously been identified as PSE cases in the 12 months
30-second periods during each three-minute period. The after childbirth (Cooper eta!, 1988). On allocation to PSE
ratings were: absent, 0; present once or twice, 1; present CATEGO classes, 39 were classified as having depression,
repeatedly or persistently, 2. and ten as having anxiety. Of the ten anxiety cases,
seven could be deemed to have suffered from significant
Developmental assessmentof child depression because they had positive ratings for ‘¿depressed
mood' on the PSE and/or scores over 18 on the MADRS.
The Denverdevelopmentalscreeningtest (Frankenberg& Of these seven women, three scored over 18 on the MADRS
Dodds, 1967) was used. It is made up of four subscales: and on the PSE symptom ‘¿depressedmood', three scored
(a) gross motor; (b) language; (c) fine motor-adaptive; over 18 on the MADRS only and one scored on the PSE
(d) personal and social. symptom ‘¿depressed mood' only. These seven women were
combined with the 39 PSE depressives into a group of 46
Child's responseto mother's departure and return ‘¿previouslydepressed' index mothers.
In the present study, at the 19-month follow-up, nine of
The mother wasaskedto leavethe room and the researcher
assessed the child's resultant distress on a four-point scale.
these 49 original PSE cases were still found to be PSE cases;
all nine had been previously classified as having depression.
The mother returned a minute later, and an assessment
Among the 40 women who had become non-cases at follow
was made of the child's acknowledgement of her and
approaches to her. up, 15 were at levels 3 or 4 (i.e. still with significant
symptoms) on the PSE Index of Definition, while 25 were
at levels 1 or 2.
Global ratingsof motherand child
At the end of the interview,the assessormadeglobalratings Statistical analyses
of the mother's behaviour, the child's behaviour, and the
interactions between mother and child. The mother was The statisticaltechniquesused includedWilcoxontests for
@ rated for intrusiveness (cuts across or disrupts flow of child's matched pairs, Mann-Whitney U-tests, and Fisher exact
play, takes over and even excludes), warmth (extent to probability tests and Spearman's rank correlations as
which she uses endearment and physical contact) and appropriate.
rapport (ease of communication, reflected in her ability to The 49 index and 49 control mother—child pairs were
talk and listen to the child). The child was rated for first compared. With regard to structured play (Table 2),
concentration (maintaining interest and sustaining attention in the index group the children showed significantly less
to task in hand) and negative responses (any including affective sharing (Prz 0.01); the mothers showed
crying, tantrums, sulks and anger). Each item was significantly less facilitation (P<0.0l); while the mothers
rated on a 1—7
scale (mode 4, l=very low, 7=very and children showed significantly less interaction (P<0.0l).
high). No significant differences were found on the measures of
POST-NATAL DEPRESSION AND MOTHER-CHILD INTERACTION 49
Table 3
Globalratings for (a)49 casesv. 49 controls, (b) 25 recoveredcasesv. 25 controls and (c) 14 index
subjects with SCSDv. 35 index subjects without SCSD
Global ratings(a)(b)(c)ControlsCasesCasesSCSD
Non-SCSD(n=49)(n=49)(n=25)(n=14)
(n=35)Mean
ZMaternal MeanZMeanZMean Mean
1.75Maternal
intrusiveness3.2 3.20.43.40.762.9 3.3
2.29―Maternal
warmth4.8 5.00.485.00.064.2 5.3
1.95―Child
rapport5.2 4.42.4―4.41.9'3.6 4.7
concentration4.7 3.72.8―3.72.1―3.3
1.0Childnegativeresponses3.4 3.8
1.2P<O.06. 4.12.38―3.61.34.6 3.9

P<O.05, “¿â€˜P<O.Ol.

child playing alone, child turning to observer, or mother Table 2, index children still showed less affective sharing
ignoring child. With regard to global ratings (Table 3) index than control children (Z= 2.24; P<0.05). There was also
children showed significantly less concentration (P<0.0l), a trend for indexmothersto showlessfacilitation(Z= 1.91;
and significantly more negative responses (P< 0.05) than P= 0.056). In addition, index children showed significantly
control children. Index mothers showed significantly less less sociability to strangers (Z= 2.2; P'czO.05) (not shown
rapport (P<0.05) than control mothers, but did not differ in the Tables).
significantly in warmth or intrusiveness. With regard to the Severe chronic social difficulties (SCSD) had been
other three assessments, there were also some significant experienced by 14 index mothers as against one control
differences. When their mother left the room, 14 index mother (Fisher exact P< 0.001). These difficulties were
children and 4 control children showed marked distress mainly to do with marriage, housing, or finance. When the
(,@= 5.5; d.f. = 1; P<0.05), although when their mothers 14SCSD index mothers were compared with the 35 non
returned, these two groups of children did not differ SCSD index mothers, certain significant differences were
significantly in their responses. Significantly less sociability found. As shown in Table 2, the SCSD mothers showed
with a stranger was shown by the index children than by significantly less facilitation, while their children showed
the control children (Z= 3.4; P.cz 0.01). Of the numerous significantly less affective sharing. There was also a trend
items on the Denver development assessment, only one for SCSD mothers to ignore their children more (Z = 1.74;
significant difference was found between the groups: 19 P<0.08). As shown in Table 3, on global measures the
index children as against 30 controls were able to combine SCSD mothers showed significantly less warmth and
two words (x2= 4.1; d.f. = 1; P<0.05). significantly less rapport. SCSD children were also more
All the analyses reported above for the 49 index women likely to be significantly distressed on their mother's
were repeated for the subgroup of 46 previously depressed departure (x2 4.4; d.f. 1; P<0.05).
women (excluding anxiety cases). The only notable During the first 19 months of the child's life, severe
difference was that on the language item combining two ‘¿negative
impact' life events were experienced by 22 index
words on the Denver development scale, the index v. control mothers and by 10 control mothers (y@= 6.7; P< 0.01).
difference, which was significant for the 49 index patients, However, no significant differences were found on any
was not significant for the subgroup of 46 when compared variables between ‘¿life-event'
and ‘¿non-life-event'subgroups
with their controls. within index or control groups.
A comparison was also made between two groups of The precise timing of depression was noted at each of
index mothers: (a) nine who were PSE cases in the post the post-natal assessments, thus allowing the total length
partum year, and again at 19-month follow-up; and (b) 25 of depression during the post-natal period to be calculated
who were PSE cases in the post-partum year, but who had for each mother (in the indexgroup). This ranged from ito 19
not recovered by 19months. No significant differences were months (mean 5 months). The length of the mother's
found, possibly because of the small number of PSE cases depression was then examined in relationship to each of
at 19 months. However, there was a trend for the children the mother-child outcome measures at 19months. Duration
of mothers who were still PSE cases at 19 months to show of depression was found to be inverselyrelated to sociability
less affective sharing (Z= 1.2; P<0.2), less sociability (r= 0.32; P<0.05) and positively related to the amount of
(Z = 1.74; P< 0.08), and greater distress when the mother the child's negativeresponses(r= 0.27;P<0.05) and there
left the room (x2= 1.74; d.f. = 1; P<0.2). was a tendency for it to be inversely related to maternal
Next, comparisons were made between: (a) the 25 index facilitation (r=0.2; P<0.l).
mothers who had recovered psychiatrically, being at low The relationship between parity on mother—child
levels (1 or 2) on the PSE Index of Definition at 19-month interaction was examined separately within index and
follow-up; and (b) their individually matched controls control groups. All analyses consisted of primiparous
(Tables 2 and 3). Not surprisingly, there were fewer mothers being compared with multiparous ones. Parity was
significant differences between these groups than between found to have no significant effect within the index group.
all the index and control pairs. However, as shown in However, some small differences were noted within the
50 STEIN ET AL

control group. Primiparous mothers were found to show These findings raise two important questions. Why
less warmth (Z= 1.9; P< 0.05) and a tendency to less is maternal post-natal depression associated with a
facilitation (Z= 1.8; P< 0.07). reduced quality of mother—childinteraction? Why
does this association persist in some mothers who
Logistic-regressionanalysis have recovered from their depression?
Three factors may have an important bearing on
As early maternal post-partum depression (0—3months), the first question: (a) the depressive disorder itself;
maternal concurrent depression (19 months) and severe (b) associated marital and social difficulties; and
chronic social and marital difficulties were highly associated (c) the child's temperament.
with each other, a logistic regression analysis using GLIM
(McCullagh & Nelder, 1984) was conducted. The analysis The depressive disorder may itself affect the
was carried out within the index group in the prediction mother—child relationship because the symptoms
of mother-child interaction. One-tailed tests of significance directly interfere with the mother's capacity to relate
were used because of the prior prediction that all three to the child (Rutter & Quinton, 1984). Examples of
factors were related to mother—childinteraction. such symptoms are: sadness; self-preoccupation and
We found that low levels of maternal facilitation were social withdrawal; loss of interest, loss of energy, and
independently predicted by chronic social and marital loss of confidence; and irritability. This idea is
difficulties (t = 2.6; P< 0.01), to a lesser degree by early consistent with the findings in the present study that
post-partum depression (t= 1.79; P<0.05) and to a depressed mothers showed reduced facilitation and
negligible degree by maternal concurrent depression.
Similarly, low levels of affective sharing were predicted by rapport, while their children showed reduced affective
chronic social and marital difficulties (t= 1.88; P<0.05) sharing. It is also consistent with earlier reports that
and early post-partum depression (t= 1.72; P<0.05). mothers with depressive disorder showed reduced
A low level of the child's sociability with a stranger was affection and increased withdrawal and hostility
independently predicted by maternal concurrent depression towards the child (Weissman & Paykel, 1974), and that
(t=2.23; P<0.Ol) but not by other factors. The child's depressed mothers were involved in fewer positive
distress on the mother's departure was predicted by both interactions with their children (Cox et a!, 1987).
chronic social and marital difficulties (t= 1.86; P<0.05) The extent of the effects of the symptoms may be
and maternal concurrent depression (t= 1.86; P<0.05), but
related to the duration of the disorder as Radke
not by early post-partum depression. Lack of maternal
warmthwasstronglypredictedby chronicsocialand marital Yarrow et a! (1985) found. In the present study,
difficulties
(t= 2.75; P<0.0l), but not by maternal duration of depressive disorder was accurately
depression, whether post-natal or current. No interactions established and was found to be related to some
were found between any of the independent factors. aspects of mother—child interaction at 19 months in
These findings indicate that early post-natal depression, univariate analyses. It was not possible, however,
concurrent depression and chronic social and marital to examine whether duration had an independent
difficulties made independent contributions to the prediction effect for two reasons: the mothers with prolonged
of the mother—childinteraction. depression were also those who were still depressed
at 19 months; and the numbers of concurrently
Discussion depressed women were fairly small.
Marital and social difficulties often accompany
This study yielded two main findings about maternal depressive disorder, as cause or effect or both. A
post-natal depression: striking finding in the present study was that logistic
interactive modelling showed marital and social
(a) that there was a significant association
difficulties to be the most important determinant of
between maternal depression during the post
unsatisfactory mother-child interaction. This fmding
natal year and reduced quality of mother-child
is consistent with earlier reports that marital discord
interaction 19 months after the birth of the
and other social factors have important adverse
child
effects on the child (Rutter & Quinton, 1984;
(b) that this association held (though less strongly)
Ghodsian et a!, 1984) as well as on mother—child
not only for mothers who were still depressed
at 19 months, but also for those who had interaction (Cox et a!, 1987).
The child's temperament could also play a part in
recovered from their depression by then.
inducing or aggravating the mother's depression, and
Furthermore, logistic-regression analysis con in reinforcing her lack of confidence and self-esteem.
firmed that maternal depression in the first three In the present study no attempt was made to assess
months, maternal depression at 19 months and this factor.
chronic marital and social difficulties all had Turning to the second question (the persistence of
independent effects on mother-child interaction. reduced quality of mother—child interaction despite
POST-NATAL DEPRESSION AND MOTHER-CHILD INTERACTION 51
recovery of the mother's depression), two of the three on a 1—5scale as shown and then an overall impression
explanations mentioned above could still operate. is rated.
Thus marital and social difficulties might persist (and, (a) Baby's initial reaction to being offered a toy by
as indicated above, logistic interactive modelling stranger on mother's lap (score first 10 seconds following
showed these to be of prime importance). There is initiation of bid).
some evidence that marital and social symptoms do (1) Fusses, cries or turns towards mother
persist despite recovery in the mother's symptoms (2) Refuses toy by looking away, pushing it away,
(Bothwell & Weissman, 1977). Similarly, the child's turning towards another toy, or using words (e.g. ‘¿no')
temperament could continue to influence mother (3) Looks at toy without reaching for it
child interaction. Whatever the reasons, there is an (4) Tentatively reaches for or touches toy
(5) Accepts toy from stranger with little hesitation
interesting parallel among older children, namely that
there may be no direct temporal relationship between (b) Baby's reaction to the stranger's initiation of a give
maternal depression and the child's psychiatric and-take exchange on mother's lap (score first 30 seconds
disorder. In a study of older children whose parents following initiation of bid, or two attempts to get a game
had a variety of psychiatric disorders, Rutter & started).
Quinton (1984) found no relationship between the (1) Crying, fussing or other indications of distress
“¿ebb
and flow of parental symptoms in the course (2) Refusal to join in by turning towards mother or
of disorder in the children―. another toy, turning away from stranger, just looking
Despite our finding of an association between at stranger, or using words (e.g. ‘¿no')
maternal depression and reduced quality of mother (3) Initially reluctant, then participates
(4) Immediately joins in and participates with stranger
child interaction, the considerable variability in the (5) Actively participates in game with stranger by
group of depressed mothers should be stressed. Some smiling, initiating exchanges, anticipating give-and
of the depressed mothers had good interactions take, changing the game, acting playfully or teasing
with their children, and some were rated highly the stranger, etc.
for warmth. Moreover, reduced warmth was not
(c) Baby's behaviour when given floor freedom.
associated with depression but only with severe and
chronic marital and social difficulties. (1) Touches mother or requests to return to lap
What are the practical implications of the findings? (2) Approaches or turns towards mother
In this study few of the mothers had seen a psychiatrist (3) Stays where she/he is or moves away from mother
or other specialist. Therefore any help is probably to play with toys
(4) Approaches stranger
best given in primary care, for example, by the (5) Touches stranger or requests to be picked up
general practitioner and particularly by the health
visitor who is likely to see the mother and child at (d) Baby's initial reaction to being offered a toy by
the appropriate time. stranger on floor (score first 10seconds following initiation
Suitable help would include encouraging and of bid).
supporting the mother in her handling of the child, (1) Fusses, cries or turns towards mother
and involving the father and other family members (2) Refuses toy by looking away, pushing it away,
in child care (Rutter, 1991). Attention should be paid turning towards a different toy, or using words (e.g.
to marital and social difficulties. Further research ‘¿no')
is needed to discover what other approaches may be (3) Looks at toy without reaching for it
(4) Tentatively reaches for or touches toy
beneficial. (5) Accepts toy from stranger with little hesitation
Acknowledgements (e) Baby's reaction to the stranger's initiation of a give
and-take exchange on the floor (score first 30 seconds
We aregrateful
toallthewomen and children
who participatedfollowing initiation of bid, or two attempts to get a game
inthestudy;
to PavlosAnastasiades,
SandraCooper,Dr Joan started).
Stevenson-Hinde,ProfessorsTonyCoxand KathySylvafor their
assistance, Dr Pat Altham for statistical advice, and to Lisa Bowler (1) Crying, fussing or other indications of distress
for typingthe manuscript.Weare particularlygratefulto Professor (2) Refusal to join in by turning toward mother or
Michael Rutter who gave us very helpful comments on an earlier another toy, turning away from stranger, just looking
draft of this paper. We are indebted to the Wellcome Trust for
providingpersonalsupportto AlanSteinand to the OxfordHealth at stranger, or using words (e.g. ‘¿no')
Authority for funding the study. (3) Initially reluctant, then participates
(4) Immediately joins in and participates with stranger
Appendix (5) Actively participates in game with stranger by
smiling, initiating exchanges, anticipating give-and
The procedure for assessing the child's sociability to a take, changing the game, acting playfully or teasing
stranger is given below. Each response by the child is rated the stranger, etc.
52 STEIN ET AL

(f) Overall impression of sociability. MCCULLAGH, P. & NELDER, J. A. (1984) Generalised Linear
Models (GLIM). London: Chapman & Hall.
(1) Quite unfriendly, fussy, fearful MILLS, M., PJCKERING, C. & POUND, A. (1985) What is it about
(2) Between 1 and 3 depressed mothers that influences their children's functioning?
(3) Generally unfriendly, serious, wary In RecentResearch
in Developmental
Psychopathology
(ed.J.
(4) Between3 and 5 E. Stevenson). Oxford: Pergamon Press.
MONTGOMERY, S. A. & ASBERG, M. (1979) A new depression scale
(5) Neutral, neither friendly nor unfriendly
(6) Between5 and 7 designedto besensitiveto change.BritishJournal of Psychiatry,
134, 382—389.
(7) Friendly, positive reaction PAYKEL, E. S., EMM5, E. M., FLETCHER ,J., ci a! (1980) Life events
(8) Between 7 and 8 and socialsupport in puerpuraldepression.BritishJournal of
(9) Very friendly, outgoing, smiling Psychiatry, 136, 339-346.
POUND, A., Cox, A. D., PUCKERING, C., ci al (1985) The impact
of maternaldepressionon youngchildren. In RecentResearch
References in Developmental Psychopathology (ed. J. E. Stevenson).
Oxford: Pergamon Press.
BILUNGS, A. 0. & Moos, R. H. (1983) Comparisons of children LkDIcE-Y@@aaow,M., CusobliNGs,E. M., KUCZYNSKI,
L., ci a! (1985)
of depressedand non-depressedparents:a social-environmental Patterns of attachmentin two- and three-yearolds in normal
perspective. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2, 463-468. families and families with parental depression. Child Development,
Bomwaii, S. & WEISSMAN, M. M. (1977) Social impairments four 56, 884—893.
years after an acute depressiveepisode.AmericanJournal of Ria,wi, N. (1977)Behaviouralproblemsinpre-schoolchildren:family
Orthopsychiatry, 47, 231-237. and socialfactors.BritishJournal of Psychiatry,131,523—527.
COOPER, P. J., CAMPBELL, E. A., DAY, A., et a! (1988) Non —¿. STEVENSON,J. & GRAHAM,P. J. (1982) Pre-school to
psychotic psychiatric disorder after childbirth: a prospective School. London: Academic Press.
studyof prevalence,incidence,courseand nature.BritishJournal RUT-rER,M. (1990) Psychiatric disorder in parents as a risk factor
of Psychiatry,152, 799-806. for children. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Cox, A. D., PUCKERING, C., POUND,A., ci al (1987)The impact Psychiatry (in press).
of maternal depressionin young children. Journal of Child —¿ & QuIwroN, D. (1984) Parental psychiatric disorder: effects

Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 917-928. on children. PsychologicalMedicine,14, 853-880.


FIELD, T. M. (1984) Early interactions between infants and STEVENSON, M. B. & LAMB, M. E. (1979) Effects of infant
their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behaviour and sociability and the caretaking environment on infant cognitive
Development, 7, 517—522. performance. Child Development, 50, 340-349.
FRANKENBERG,W. K. & DODDS, J. B. (1967) The Denver WATERS, E., Wlppp.MN, J. & SROUra, L. A. (1979) Attachment,
developmental screening test. Journal of Pediatrics, 71, positive affect, and competence in the peer group: two studies
181—191. in construct validation. Child Development, 50. 821—829.
OHODSIAN,M., Z@uIcEK,E., & WOLKIND,S. (1984) A longitudinal WEISSMAN,M. M. & PAYKEL,E. 5. (1974) The Depressed Woman: a
study of maternal depressionand child behaviour problems. Studyof SocialRelationships.Chicago:Universityof ChicagoPress.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 91-109. WING, J. K., COOPER, J. E. & SARTORIUS, N. (1974) The Measure
GOLDBERG, D. P. (1972) The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by ment and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. Cambridge:
Questionnaire. London: Oxford University Press. Cambridge University Press.

•¿Alan
Stein, MB, MRCPsych, Weilcome Trust Lecturer, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford,
Warneford Hospita!, Headington, Oxford; Dennis H. Oath, MD, FRCPsych,FRCP, C!inica! Reader, Depart
ment of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; Alison Bond, BA, Research Assistant, Department of Psychiatry,
University of Oxford; Ann Day, Research Administrator, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford;
Peter J. Cooper, BA, DipPsych, DPhil, Research Fellow, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford;
Janet Bucher, BSc, MB, MRCPsych,Consultant Child Psychiatrist, Paxton House Child and Ado!escent Unit,
Bath Road, Reading

Correspondence

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen