Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/24098786

Situation Variables and Consumer Behavior

Article  in  Journal of Consumer Research · February 1975


DOI: 10.1086/208627 · Source: RePEc

CITATIONS READS

790 18,184

1 author:

Russell Belk
York University
146 PUBLICATIONS   20,037 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

luxury consumption View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Russell Belk on 11 October 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Situational Variables
and Consumer Behavior
RUSSELL W. BELK*

This paper suggests that explicit recognition of situational variables can


substantially enhance the ability to explain and understand consumer
behavioral acts. A definition and description of situations is offered, exist-
ing research is summarized, and implications for consumer research are
considered.

Growing recognition of limitations in the ability of son is a behavioral setting because each involves an
individual consumer characteristics to explain variation interval in time and space in which certain behaviors
in buyer behavior has prompted a number of appeals can be expected regardless of the particular persons
to examine situationa,l influences on behavior. Ward present. But such patterns of behavior require stretch-
and Robertson argued that "situational variables may ing the time and place dimensions to broader and more
account for considerably more variance than actor- continuous units than those defining a situation. While
related variables" (1973, p. 26). Lavidge (1966) cau- a behavioral setting might be a store which is open
tioned that many buyer behaviors may be enacted only from 8:00 AM to 6:00 PM (Barker, 1968, p. 19),
under specific conditions and necessitate situational in- the current perspective would recognize a number of
vestigations of intra-individual variability. Engel, Kol- discrete situations which may occur within this setting.
lat, and Blackwell (1969) urged that both individual The concept of an "environment" extends the time,
and situational factors must be considered in order to place, and behavioral dimensions still further. Although
explain consumer choices. Nevertheless, these and there is less agreement as to what bounds and defines
other suggestions to include situational variables in an environment, l it is clear that situations and behav-
researcIyon consumer behavior have gone largely un- ioral settings are subunits within an environment. In
heeded\.The primary obstacle has been the absence of one of his early formulations Lewin pointed out that
an adequate copception of the variables which com- an environment may be thought of as the chief charac-
prise a situation. It is the purpose of the following dis- teristics of a more or less permanent "situation"
cussion to explore such concepts and to suggest direc- (Lewin, 1933). In this sense situations represent mo-
tions for the study of situational influence in consumer mentary encounters with those elements of the total
behavior. environment which are available to the individual at
a particular time. Environment is also broader in terms
CONSUMER SITUATIONS AND of the geographic area over which it applies. For exam-
RELATED CONCEPTS ple, while the "legal environment" may be described to
consist of laws, legal institutions, and interpretive ten-
Situations, Behavioral Settings, and Environments
dencies within a governmental territory, and the behav-
As a starting point for a definition, most theoreti- ioral setting may refer to a certain courtroom, the spe-
cians would agree that a situation comprises a point in cific experience of individual A being cross-examined
time and space (Belk, 1975). For students of human by attorney G during trial M in city R at 4: 00 on day
behavior, a discrete time and place occupied by one or X, can only be described from a narrower situational
more persons identifies a situation of potential interest. perspective. It is this latter view of the conditions for
A somewhat larger alternative unit of analysis would experiences and the effect of these conditions on spe-
be Barker's (1968) "behavioral setting." A behavioral cific behavioral outcomes which the current perspective
setting is not only bounded in time and space, but also seeks to develop.
by a complete sequence of behavior or an "action pat-
1 A great deal of the effort in the emerging discipline of
tern." For example, a basketball game or a piano les-
environmental or ecological psychology has been spent in
* Russell Belk is Assistant Professor of Business Administra- debating boundaries. See for example Barker (1963), Craik
tion at the University of Illinois. (1970), Proshansky, Ittelson, and Rivlin (1970), Ittelson
(1973), and Rivlin (1973).
157 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH. Vol. 2 • December 1975
158 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Situational Versus Non-situational Determinants dividual. Where the feature is more transitory, such as
of Consumer Behavior having a headache, it must be considered to be at least
partly a function of the situation. It is ne~essary to ~m­
A second group of concepts from which situations pose similar constraints on the conceptlOn of object
must be distinguished are the non-situational determi- characteristics in order to categorize a description such
nants of a particular consumer behavior. Figure 1 as a brand of soda being lO¢ less than competing
shows a familiar stimulus-organism-response paradigm brands. In cases where the characteristic tends to be a
which has been modified to divide the stimulus into an lasting and general feature of the brand it may be at-
object and a situation. This split is analogous to the tributed to the object. Where the characteristic is spe-
perceptual distinctions between figure and ground or cific to a time and place (e.g., a special sale) it should
focal and contextual cues (Helson, 1964). That is, be regarded as a characteristic of the situation.
because behavior with respect to a product or service
object is of primary significance in consumer behavior,
the object to which the consumer is directly respond- Characteristics of Consumer Situations
ing will be regarded as a unique source of behavioral Consistent with the previous distinctions, a consumer
influence. In not including personal and object char- situation may be viewed as comprising ". . . all those
acteristics within the concept of the situation there is factors particular to a time and place of observation
also a purposeful departure from Lewin's (1935) for- which do not follow from a knowledge of personal
mulation of the life space. The rationnale for this more (intra-individual) and stimulus ( choice alternative)
limited view of situation is the greater possibility of attributes and which have a demonstrable and system-
operationalizing a construct which has an existence atic effect on current behavior" (Belk, 1974a). The
apart from the individual's total consciousness. For greatest problem in operationalizing this view lies in
there to be a hope of really adding to the ability to defining "all those factors." Several attempts have been
explain consumer behavior, this separate existence is made to develop comprehensive taxonomies of situ-
essential.2 ational characteristics. Using general guidelines sug-
It might seem that a clear distinction may be made gested by Sherif and Sherif (1956), Sells (1963)
between persons, objects, and situations as separate constructed a subjective classification of over 200
sources of influence on behavior, but some potential situational variables including gravity, temperature,
for confusion exists in attempting to separate the char- group structure, role requirements, and novelty of the
acteristics of each determinant. R. L. Thorndike's situation in relation to prior experiences. Unfortu-
(1947) concept of "lasting and general characteristics nately, from the point of view of the current concep-
of the individual" is a useful device in distinguishing tion of situation, Sell's classification also includes some
personal and situational characteristics. These individ- characteristics of the individual (e.g., age, sex, race)
ual features, including personality, intellect, sex, and and environment (e.g., sources of food, erosion, lan-
race, are stable over times and places of observation guage) , and excludes certain descriptors of the physi-
and may therefore be attributed consistently to the in- cal locale (e.g., noises, colors, room or area size).
Classification attempts by Bellows ( 1963) , Wolf
2 This point is elaborated by Belk (1975), Mausner (1963)
captured the argument in stating that "if one specifies the stim-
( 1966), and Moos ( 1973 ), are less complete and
ulus in terms of the nature of the receiver, lawfulness becomes suffer from similar drawbacks . .,.Although also incom-
impossible ... plete and parochial in its focus, a limited taxonomy by

(STIMULUS) (ORGANISM) (RESPONSE)


FIGURE I
A Revised S-O-R Paradigm
SITUATIONAL VARIABLES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 159

Allen (1965) of the situational factors found to affect shopping for a small appliance for personal use.
conformity highlights several important social dimen- 5. Antecedent States make up a final group of fea-
sions (e.g., public/private, interdependence of partici- tures which characterize a situation. These are momen-
pants) and task di~nsions (e.g., difficulty, impor- tary moods (such as acute anxiety, pleasantness, hos-
tance) of the situatio~ These features contrast sharply tility, and excitation) or momentary conditions (such
with the taxonomy of 66 bipolar adjectival scales as cash on hand, fatigue, and illness) rather than
constructed by Kasmar (1970) to measure situations. chronic individual traits. These conditions are further
From an initial list of 300 characteristics generated stipulated to be immediately antecedent to the current
through room description protocols obtained from ar- situation in order to distinguish states which the indi-
chitecture students, scales were developed to describe vidual brings to the situation from states of the individ-
13 aspects of the situation; size, volume, scale, mood, ual which result from the situation. For instance, a
color, texture, function, illumination, esthetic quality, person may select a certain motion picture because he
climate, color, accoustical quality, and miscellaneous. feels depressed (an antecedent state and a part of the
Such characteristics provide a comprehensive descrip- choice situation) ,but the fact that the movie causes
tion of the design features of the situation, but they him to feel happier is a response to the consumption
completely neglect the social and task attributes which situation. 3 This altered state may then become ante-
Allen's review ( 1965) emphasized. Furthermore a cedent for behavior in the next choice situation en-
higher order factor analysis of data from Kasmar's countered, such as passing a street vendor on the way
scales has shown them to be highly redundant in terms out of the theater.
of an underlying affective dimension (Mehrabian and Given a conception of the dimensions characterizing
Russell, 1974). Mehrabian and Russell's own (1974) a situation, the final element of the definition of a situ-
attempt to develop three comprehensive situational de- ation requiring clarification is the requirement that
scriptors (pleasure, arousal, and dominance) is also not these features have a "demonstrable and systematic
very satisfying to depict the array of possible situ- effect on current behavior." To a greater extent than
ational dimensions. However, by selectively combining the problem of situational dimensions, this is an em-
features suggested in the various taxonomies cited, a pirical question which has been the subject of some
skeletal notion of what is meant by "all those factors" research. The following section reviews this research
comprising a situation may be offered. The following and examines the extent to which situational knowl-
five groups of situational characteristics represent the edge can be expected to add to our ability to explain
general features from these taxonomies which are con- consumer behavior.
sistent with the current definition of situation.
1. Physical Surroundings are the most readily ap-
parent features of a situation. These features include ASSESSMENTS OF SITUATIONAL EFFECTS
geographical and institutional location, decor, sounds, ON CONSUMER BEHAVIOR
aromas, lighting, weather, and visible configurations of Inventory Evidence
merchandise or other material surrounding the stimulus
object. In the past six or seven years a small but growing
2. Social Surroundings provide additional depth to number of empirical tests of situational influence in
a description of a situation. Other persons present, consumer behavior have been conducted using inven-
their characteristics, their apparent roles, and interper- tories of situational scenarios and choice alternatives.
sonal interactions occurring are potentially relevant These inventories ask subjects to rate the likelihood
examples. that they would choose each of several alternative
3. Temporal Perspective is a dimension of situ- products or services under each of several sets of situ-
ations which may be specified in units ranging from ational conditions. Summary descriptions of seven of
time of day to season of the year. Time may also be these inventories are provided in the Appendix. It may
measured relative to some past or future event for the well be argued that the situations investigated in these
situational participant. This allows conceptions such as studies do not constitute random samples of possible
time since last purchase, time since or until meals or situations and do not always reflect the full range of
payday, and time constraints imposed by prior or situational dimensions just outlined. Despite the fact
standing commitments. that most of the inventories have relied on pretests to
4. Task Definition features of a situation include generate familiar situations, the argument is undoubt-
an intent or requirement to select, shop for, or obtain edly valid. Even so, any demonstration that behavior
information about a general or specific purchase. In differs widely between the situations specified is evi-
addition, task may reflect different buyer and user roles
anticipated by the individual. For instance, a person 3 Hansen (1972) distinguishes between purchase, consump-
shopping for a small appliance as a wedding gift for a tion, and communication situations. Comments in this paper
friend is in a different situation than he would be in concentrate primarily on consumer purchase choices.
160 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 1
ANALYSES OF CONSUMER BEHAVIORAL VARIANCEa
(percents of total variance)
Response Categoryb
Source Beverage Products Meat Products Snack products A Fast Foods" Leisure Activities d Motion Pictures
Persons (P) 0.5% 4.6% 6.7% 8.1% 4.5% 0.9%
Situations (S) 2.7% 5.2% 0.4% 2.2% 2.0% 0.5%
Products (Responses: R) 14.6% 15.0% 6.7% 13.4% 8.8% 16.6%
RXS 39.8% 26.2% 18.7% 15.3% 13.4% 7.0%
PXS 2.7% 2.9% 6.1% 2.2% 4.0% 1.9%
PXR 11.8% 9.7% 22.4% 20.1% 21.2% 33.7%
PXSXR _e _8
3.4% _e _e _8

Residual 27.8%f 36.4% 35.6% 38.7% 46.1% 39.4%


Total 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%
a Components of variance analyses for mixed effects model with subjects random. For computational details, see GIeser, Chronbach
and Rajaratnam (1965) and Endler and Hunt (1966).
b Inventories are described in Appendix.
c Variance components from this inventory have not previously been presented.
d Means of four samples.
e Not obtained due to single presentations of each situation-response combination.
f Incorrectly reported as entirely P X S X R.

dence that there are important situational determinants appear to be the consistent choice determinant with
for the class of choices considered. the general popularity of the movies following in
Results comparing the relative influence from per- importance.
sons, products (responses), situations, and their inter- A recent challenge to the validity of these inventory-
actions are presented in Table 1 for the six product based findings has been presented by Lutz and Kakkar
categories that have been examined. 4 For all inven- (1975). They argued that the experimental procedure
tories the effects explaining the smallest proportions of of having subjects rate the same choice alternatives
variance are those which reflect superfluous response under all situational conditions may have spuriously
styles. For instance, a sizable interaction term for per- inflated the situations by products interaction term. In
sons by situations would only indicate that some sub- order to test this possibility they replicated Belk's
jects view using any of the products in certain situations (1974a) snack product inventory, but exposed each
as more likely than do other subjects. Similarly, siz- subject to only one level of the situation factor (see
able main effects for persons or situations would have Appendix for a description of their experiment). Their
little meaning since they do not involve preference analysis obtained a contribution of less than 6 percent
differences by product. A more dominant influence is for the situations by products interaction, with a resid-
the responses by situations interaction, especially for ual term which accounted for over 86 percent of the
the meat and beverage inventories. This is the variance variance. Ii Unfortunately, their analysis assumed an in-
component which directly reflects the influence of sys- appropriate completely randomized factorial design
tematic situational differences in product preferences. rather than the split plot factorial design which was
Furthermore, the lower contributions from the re- actually employed (Winer, 1971, pp. 366-371, Kirk,
sponses main effect in all inventories except motion 1968, pp. 245-318). This problem renders their analy-
pictures, suggest that general product popularity is a sis meaningless and leaves the question of artifactual
substantially less important determinant of consumer situational influence open. By altering the numbers of
preferences than are situational conditions. For the meat situations or responses by a factor of one half, Belk
and beverage inventories the small interaction terms (1974b) has shown the variance component estimates
for persons by responses reveal that in these categories of this inventory to be relatively stable across the re-
situational influence also outstrips individual product sulting formats. Comparable results have been found
preferences. For other inventories, again excepting mo- by Endler and Hunt (1969) for a similar anxiety in-
tion pictures, the effects of situational and individual ventory. However, since these examinations do not
influences are jointly dominant. Motion pictures is the reduce the number of situations to one per subject,
only category examined in which situational effects ap- the possibility that situational influence has been over-
pear to be minimal. In this case individual preferences estimated by these inventories still exists.
4 The research reported generally employed a situations by Ii Hay's Omega squared statistic (Hays, 1964) was employed
products by persons repeated measures experimental design. to derive these estimates. Since this statistic assumes a com-
While nearly all main effects and two way interactions yielded pletely fixed effects model, results are not strictly comparable
significant F-ratios, proportions of variance accounted for by to those of the mixed effects components of variance method
each effect are more revealing (Belk, 1974a). employed in the other studies reported.
SITUATIONAL VARIABLES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 161

Other Evidence resolution before this potential can be fully realized.


Evidence of the importance of consumer situations Foremost among the issues which this research must
has been found using other approaches which lend ad- address is the question of the most appropriate means
ditional credence to a conclusion that situational in- of measuring situations. Two alternative perspectives
fluence is a pervasive factor in consumer behavior. proposed have been labeled "psychological" (Lutz and
Using multidimensional scaling Green and Rao (1972) Kakkar. 1975) and "objective" (Belk, 1975) measure-
found that consumer perceptions of and preferences ments. Psychological measurements of situations rely
for va~ou~ bread and pastry items changed markedly on the subjects' perceptions of the situation and are an
over ddfenng meal and menu situations. In a series of extension of sociological inquiry into the "situation as
experimental choice simulations Hansen (1972) found defined" (Thomas, 1927). The premise for such mea-
that sele~ti~n of a hairdryer as a gift depended upon surements is that the wayan individual construes a
charactenstIcs of the supposed recipient, and that in- situation should be more important to behavior than
formation seeking and choices from a fixed menu var- the inherent features of that situation. Objective mea-
ied according to the description of the restaurant. surements of situations restrict themselves to features
~f the situati?n as it ~xists before subjects' interpreta-
Gr!/lnhaug (1972) found that buyers of tableware uti-
~Ions. The pnm.a!)' ratIOnale for this perspective is that
lized different types and sources of information de-
pending upon whether the purchase was for personal It rem~ves ~h~ IdIOsyncr~cies of perception which may
use or for a gift. By having subjects reconstruct word otherwIse hmIt aggregatIOn and manipulation of con-
of mouth incidents, Belk (1971) found that one-third sumer situations. The possibility has also been raised
of the conversations about a new freeze dried coffee that some situational influences, such as subtle cueing
took place where the prior conversation concerned effects, may ~perate without the subject's perceptual
aw~reness. WIthout some sort of hybrid measurement
food, and that another third of the conversations began
w~Ich merges these perspectives, it appears that situ-
while drinking coffee. Sandell (1968b) was able to
condition choice of specific cigarette brands to either atIOnal research must utilize both types of measure-
stressful or boring situations and to consumption of ments.
a specific brand of beer. A related issue concerns the most appropriate means
Anecdotal evidence of situational influence abounds of manipulating situations in experimental research.
as well. F~r example, in retail settings the mere pres- Both psychologically and objectively defined situations
~ay be manipulated by assigning subjects to different
ence of chIldren (Wells and LoSciuto, 1966), friends
tI~es, places, and conditions, although successful ma-
(Bell, 1967), and sales personnel (Albaum, 1967)
h.ave been observed to alter purchase outcomes; as time mpulatIOns of psychologically defined situations may
need to be more clever or elaborate. However, this
SlDce last meal increased so did the total food bill of
sort of research is costly and is best limited to the in-
non-obese supermarket shoppers in a study by Nisbett
and Kanouse (1968); Pennington (1965) found ap- vestigation of one or two situational dimensions at a
t~me. Alterna~ivel!" and more commonly, the projec-
pliance sales were most frequent when customer and
tIve uSe of sItuatIonal scenarios may continue to be
salesman wer~ similar in their propensity to bargain;
used. Typically these scenarios have ranged from a
and greater nsk was found to be perceived in buying
the same good by mail than at a retail store (Spence, o.ne J:?hrase to ~~e paragraph written description of
sItuatI?nal condItIons. Photographs, motion pictures,
Engel and Blackwell, 1970).
and VIdeo tapes are possible refinements of stimulus
From the variety of methods employed in these
input in this procedure, but these methods favor visual
studies it appears that situational effects can be dem-
cues and may unnaturally focus attention and control
onstrated both descriptively and experimentally. Al-
t~e rat~ of experience. Perhaps the best means of ma-
though the amount of research specifically focused on
n~~ulatIO~, short of actually modifying situational con-
~ituational influence is still quite small, a number of
dItIons, IS to couple written descriptions of features
lDstances have been found in which situations can be
such as temporal perspective, task definition, and ante-
shown. to affect consumer behavior systematically.
cedent states, with visual and auditory input of physi-
There IS further encouragement for situational research
c~ and social surroundings. Comparisons of results
from the refreshing fact that analyses of the behavioral
uSlDg each alternative means of manipulation will be
inventories specifying situations have been able to ex-
needed to assess their relative adequacy.
plain the majority of variance encountered.
Whether the existence of a particular situational ef-
fect has been determined under simulated or actual
SITUATIONAL RESEARCH IN
conditions, interpretation of the importance of this ef-
CONSUMER BEHAVIOR
fect requires ~~owledge of the frequency of occurrence
Despite. the substantial promise and appeal of re- of these condItIons. Because consumers can selectively
search WhICh .employs ~ituational variables to explain seek or avoid many of the situations they encounter
consumer chOIce behaVIOr, several basic issues require and because all unanticipated situations are not equally
162 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

common, descriptive evidence of the frequency of situ- surveys. Subjects were male and female students
ational occurrences is needed. A number of tittle bud- at two colleges and totalled 141.
get studies based on consumer diaries (e.g., Szali, 3. Meat Products. Belk (1974a) examined choices
1973) are available which provide general records of of 11 different meat products (e.g., hamburger,
the times and places of consumer activities. But even steak, chicken) in nine different situations (e.g.,
the most detailed consumer accounts (e.g., Muse, party for friends, meal on a weekday evening,
1946) seldom go beyond "shopping" in their descrip- at a nice restaurant with friends) using five point
tions of purchase situations. Nevertheless activity di- scales from "extremely likely" to "not at all
aries appear to be a useful approach to gathering rele- likely", administered to 100 members of a com-
vant data on situational occurrences. In addition to munity. Situations and meat products were
providing data on relevant situational variables, this chosen from protocols and familiarity pretests.
approach may simultaneously measure individual char- 4. Motion Pictures. Belk (1974b) had 100 stu-
acteristics and behavioral outcomes which can be cross dents rate 12 hypothetical motion pictures (e.g.,
tabulated with situations to obtain a picture of individ- The Motorcycle Freaks, Summer of Dreams,
ual differences in situational exposure and susceptibility Only Fools are Sad) described in mock adver-
to situational influence. tisements, in nine situations (e.g., on a week-
The ultimate problem for all future situational re- night with friends of the same sex, just for
search is the lack of a comprehensive taxonomy of something to do, together with spouse or date
situational characteristics and normal combinations of at their request) on a five-point scale. Situations
these characteristics. Hopefully this discussion has were selected via protocols and pretests based
made some headway in establishing a general concep- on familiarity, and motion pictures were struc-
tualization of consumer situations, but obviously tured to parallel currently popular themes.
greater detail is necessary. It is a false hope at this 5. Snack Products A. Belk (1974b) varied ten dif-
point to expect that we can systematically investigate ferent situations (e.g., while watching television
a complete list of situational characteristics, because no with family, going on a long automobile trip, an
such list exists. Only by continuing to conceptualize urge for a between meal snack) and had 100
and research situational characteristics under a guid- student subjects rate the likelihood of choosing
ing understanding of the scope and criteria for situ- each of ten snack products (e.g., potato chips,
ations can such a summary ever be achieved. pastries, ice cream) on two occasions (two
weeks apart) using five point scales. Situations
APPENDIX: SUMMARY DETAILS FOR and products were chosen as in the meat inven-
SEVEN SITUATIONAL INVENTORIES tory.
6. Snack Products B. Lutz and Kakkar (1975)
1. Beverages. Sandell (1968a) presented 31 stu- replicated Belk's snack product inventory except
dent subjects with ten beverages (e.g., coffee, that subjects in each of ten groups of from 24 to
water, beer) which they rated in seven situations 36 students responded within only one of the
(e.g., when alone, feeling sleepy in the after- situations and on only one occasion. Each group
noon, reading the paper in the morning) using rated products in a different situation and a total
a seven-point scale from "extremely unwilling" of 306 subjects were employed.
to "extremely willing" (to try). Situations and 7. Fast Foods. Using data collected by Leo Bur-
beverages tested were apparently chosen subjec- nett U.S.A., Belk (1975) analyzed the effect of
tively, although five subjects who did not appear ten different situations (e.g., too tired to cook
to view the products as alternatives were elim- dinner, unexpected dinner guests, having a few
inated. friends over for a casual get-together) on re-
2. Leisure Activities. Bishop and Witt (1970) in- sponses to a six-point likelihood scale for each
vestigated the effect of ten situations (e.g., re- of ten (confidential) fast food outlets and re-
turning from studying at a noisy library, relaxing lated meal choices. Subjects were 98 housewives
Friday afternoon following a busy week, waking in a single community.
up fresh and rested on a Saturday morning) on
the likelihood of engaging in each of 13 leisure
activities (e.g., go shopping for clothes, watch REFERENCES
television, visit a friend) using a five-point scale Albaum, G. "Exploring Interaction in a Marketing Situa-
from "almost certainly" to "I would not feel tion," Journal of Marketing Research, 4 (May, 1967),
like" (doing this). Situations were selected based 168-72.
on five alternative theories of leisure behavior, Allen, B. L. "Situational Factors in Conformity," in
and leisure activities were based on their fre- Leonard Berkowitz, Advances in Experimental Social
quency of occurrence in previous community Psychology, Vol. 2, New York: Academic Press, 1965.
SITUATIONAL VARIABLES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 163

Barker, R. G. The Stream of Behavior. New York: Apple- Behavior Sciences, Belmont, California: Wadsworth
ton-Century-Crofts, 1963. Publishing, 1968.
- - - - . Ecological Psychology: Concepts and Methods Lavidge, R. J. "The Cotton Candy Concept: Intra-Indi-
for Studying the Environment of Human Behavior. vidual Variability," in Lee Adler and Irving Crespi,
Stanford University Press, 1968. Attitude Research at Sea, Chicago: American Market-
Belk, R. W. "Occurrence of Word of Mouth Buyer Be- ing Association, 1966, 39-50.
havior as a Function of Situation and Advertising Lewin, K. "Environmental Forces in Child Behavior and
Stimuli," Proceedings. American Marketing Associa- Development," in Carl C. Murchison, Handbook of
tion Fall Conference, 1971, 419-22. Child Psychology, second edition, revised. Worcester,
- - - - . "An Exploratory Assessment of Situational Ef- Massachusetts: Clark University Press, 1933, 94-127.
fects in Buyer Behavior," Journal of Marketing Re- Lewin, K. A Dynamic Theory of Personality. New York:
search. 11 (May. 1974a), 156-163. McGraw-Hill, 1935.
- - - - . "Application and Analysis of the Behavior Lutz, R. J. and P. K. Kakkar. "The Psychological Situation
Differential Inventory for Assessing Situational Effects as a Determinant of Consumer Behavior," in Mary,
in Consumer Behavior," in Scott Ward and Peter Jane Schlinger (ed.), Advances in Consumer Re-
Wright (eds.), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. search, Vol. 2. Chicago: Association for Consumer
1. Urbana: Association for Consumer Research, Research, 1975.
1974b. Mausner, B. M. "The Specification of the Stimulus Situa-
- - - - . "The Objective Situation as a Determinant of tion in a Social Interaction," in Stephen B. Sells, Stim-
Consumer Behavior," in Mary Jane Schlinger (ed.) ulus Determinants of Behavior. New York: Ronald,
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 2. Chicago: 1963.
Association for Consumer Research, 1975. Mehrabian, A. and J. A. Russell. An Approach to Envi-
Bell, G. D. "Self-Confidence and Persuasion in Car Buy- ronmental Psychology. Cambridge: M.I.T. Press,
ing," Journal of Marketing Research, 4 (February, 1974.
1967), 46-52. Moos, R. H. "Conceptualizations of Human Environ-
Bellows, R. "Toward a Taxonomy of Social Situations," in ments," American Psychologist, 28 (1973), 652-663.
Stephen B. Sells (ed.), Stimulus Determinants of Be- Muse, M. "Time Expenditures in Homemaking Activities
havior, New York: Ronald, 1963. in 183 Vermont Farm Homes," Vermont Agricultural
Bishop, D. W. and P. A. Witt, "Sources of Behavioral Experimental Station Bulletin, No. 530, 1946.
Variance During Leisure Time," Journal of Personal- Nisbett, R. E. and D. E. Kanouse. "Obesity, Food Depriva-
ity and Social Psychology, 16 (October, 1970), 352- tion and Supermarket Shopping Behavior," Journal of
60. Personality and Social Psychology, 12 (August, 1969),
Craik, K. H. "Environment Psychology," in Kenneth H. 289-94.
Craik, et al. New Directions in Psychology, Vol. 4, Pennington, A. L. "Customer-Salesman Bargaining Behav-
New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1970. ior in Retail Transactions," Journal of Marketing Re-
Endler, N. S. and J. McV. Hunt. "Sources of Behavioral search, 5 (August, 1965), 255-62.
Variance as Measured by the S-R Inventory of Anx- Proshansky, H. M., W. H. Ittelson, and L. G. Rivlin.
iousness," Psychological Bulletin, 65 (1966), 336-46. (eds.) Environmental Psychology, New York: Holt,
Engel, J. F., D. T. Kollat, and R. D. Blackwell. "Per- Rinehart and Winston, 1970.
sonality Measures and Market Segmentation," Busi- Sandell, R. G. "Effects of Attitudinal and Situational Fac-
ness Horizons, 12 (June, 1969),61-70. tors on Reported Choice Behavior," Journal of Mar-
GIeser, G. L., L. J. Cronbach, and N. Rajaratnam. "Gen- keting Research, 4 (August, 1968a), 405-08.
eraIizability of Scores Influenced by Multiple Sources Sandell, R. G. "The Effects of Attitude Influence and
of Variance," Psychometrika, 30 (1965), 395-418. Representational Conditioning on Choice Behavior,"
Green, P. E. and V. R. Rao. "Configural Synthesis in Stockholm: The Economic Research Institute, Stock-
Multidimensional Scaling," Journal of Marketing Re- holm School of Economics, 1968b.
search. 9 (February, 1972). 65-68. Sells, S. B. "Dimensions of Stimulus Situations Which
Grj1jnhaug. K. "Buying Situation and Buyer's Information Accounts for Behavioral Variance," in Stephen B.
Behavior," European Marketing Research Review, 7 Sells (ed.), Stimulus Determinants of Behavior, New
(September, 1972), 33-48. York: Ronald, 1963.
Hansen, F. Consumer Choice Behavior. New York: The Sherif, M. and C. W. Sherif. An Outline of Social Psychol-
Free Press, 1972. ogy, Rev. Ed. New York: Harper and Row, 1956.
Hays, W. L. Statistics for Psychologists. New York: Rine- Spense, H. E., J. R. Engel, and Roger D. Blackwell. "Per-
hart and Winston, 1964. ceived Risk in Mail-Order and Retail Store Buying,"
Helson, H. "Current Trends and Issues in Adaptation-Level Journal of Marketing Research, 7 (August, 1970),
Theory," American Psychologist, 19 (1964), 26-38. 364-69.
Ittelson, W. H. Environment and Cognition. New York: Szali, A. et al. (eds.) The Use of Time. The Hague:
Seminar Press, 1973. Mouten, 1973.
Kasmar, J. V. "The Development of a Usable Lexicon of Thomas, W. I. "The Behavior Pattern and the Situation,"
Environmental Descriptors," Environment and Behav- Proceedings, Twenty-second Annual Meeting, Ameri-
ior,2 (1970), 133-169. can Sociological Society, 22 (1927), 1-13.
Kirk, R. E. Experimental Design: Procedures for the Thorndike, R. L. Research Problems and Techniques.
164 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Washington: U.S. Government Printing Office, Re- Purchasing Behavior," lournal of Marketing Research,
port No.3 AAF Aviation Psychology Program, 1947. (August, 1966), 227-33.
Ward, S. and T. S. Robertson. "Consumer Behavior Re- Winer, B. J. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design,
search: Promise and Prospects," in Scott Ward and Second Edition. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1971.
Thomas S. Robertson, Consumer Behavior: Theoreti- Wolf, R. "The Measurement of Environments," in Anne
cal Sources. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1973, Anastasi (ed.), Testing Problems in Perspective,
3-42. Washington, D.C.: American Council on Education,
Wells, W. D. and A. LoSciuto. "A Direct Observation of 1966, 491-503.

View publication stats

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen