Sie sind auf Seite 1von 484

580 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

second half of life in those domains where biological been much discussion in life span work about metathe-
factors are important. ory of development (e.g., P. B. Baltes, 1987; P. B. Baltes
To prevent this possible misunderstanding, we next et al., 1980; Labouvie-Vief, 1980, 1982; R. M. Lerner,
describe why this is not necessarily so, why deficits 1991, 2002; J. R. Nesselroade & Reese, 1973; Overton &
in biological status also can be the foundation for prog- Reese, 1973; Reese, 1994; Riegel, 1976). Included in
ress, that is, antecedents for positive changes in adaptive this discussion was a continuing dialogue about the
capacity. At least since the publication of “Limits of shortcomings of extant conceptions of development as
Growth” by the Club of Rome, there has been increasing advanced primarily by child developmentalists (e.g.,
public awareness that more is not always better and that Collins, 1982; Harris, 1957). A family of metatheoretical
progress is possible even in the context of limitations and propositions intended to characterize the nature of life
constraints. Biocultural co-construction was already in- span development was one outcome of this extensive dis-
troduced as a metascript. Similar perspectives derive cussion (P. B. Baltes, 1979a, 1987; R. M. Lerner, 1983).
from considerations of the adaptive processes in evolu- In the following discussion, we attempt to update
tion as well as from consideration of the function of com- this effort at a metatheory of life span development
pensation during ontogenesis (see also P. B. Baltes, (Table 11.3). In doing so, we also point out that similar
1991, 1997; Brandtstädter, Chapter 10, this Handbook, metatheoretical work exists in other quarters of devel-
this volume; Dixon & Bäckman, 1995; Durham, 1991). opmental theory, particularly in conceptual work
The most radical view of the notion that deficits can associated with cultural psychology, evolutionary psy-
spell progress is contained in the notion of culture as chology, and systems theory (see also Fischer & Bidell,
compensation. That is, the condition of a limitation or a Chapter 7; Gottlieb et al., Chapter 5; Thelen & Smith,
loss generates new forms of mastery and cultural inno- Chapter 6, this Handbook, this volume). In the present
vation. As researchers study what is not yet known, cul- context, however, we emphasize the uniqueness of the
tural attention shifts to those areas where there is an positions advanced by life span scholars.
objective or subjectively perceived lack or a deficit. In
this line of thinking, the human organism is by nature a Reformulating the Concept of Development from a
“ being of deficits” (Mängelwesen; Gehlen, 1956) and Functionalist Perspective: Development as Change
social culture has developed or emerged in part to deal in Adaptive Capacity
specifically with biological deficits. From a life span theory point of view, it was important
Memorization strategies, for instance, were devel- to articulate concepts of development that go beyond
oped in part because human memory is not optimal. To unidimensional and unidirectional models that had
give another example: The fact that humans are biologi- flourished in conjunction with the traditional biological
cally vulnerable regarding outside temperatures ( lack of conceptions of growth or physical maturation. In these
perfect thermo-regulation) is among the reasons for a traditional conceptions (Harris, 1957; Sowarka &
highly developed body of knowledge, values, and tech- Baltes, 1986), attributes such as qualitative change, or-
nology about textiles and clothing. This applies both to dered sequentiality, irreversibility, and the definition of
cultural evolution on the societal level and to individual an end state played a critical role. Primarily by consid-
ontogenesis. Research on psychological compensation is ering ontogenetic development from a functionalist per-
a powerful illustration of the idea that deficits can be spective (Dixon & Baltes, 1986), the traditional
catalysts for positive changes in adaptive capacity conception of development was challenged.
(Bäckman & Dixon, 1992; M. M. Baltes & Carstensen,
1996; P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990b; Dixon & Bäckman, Development as Selection and Selective Adapta-
1995; Marsiske et al., 1995; Rowe & Kahn, 1987). tion (Optimization). The traditional concept of
development emphasizes a general and universal devel-
opment of an entity geared toward a higher level of func-
A Family of Metatheoretical Propositions about
tioning which, in addition, continuously incorporates
Life Span Developmental Theory (Level 3)
most if not all previously developed capacities (Harris,
Because of the complexities associated with life span 1957; R. M. Lerner, 1983, 2002; H. Werner, 1948). His-
ontogenetic processes and the challenge involved in the torically, this view of ontogenetic development has been
articulation of adequate theoretical concepts, there has pictured as the unfolding and emergence of an entity,
Toward Psychological Theories of Life Span Development: Five Levels of Analysis 581

TABLE 11.3 Family of Theoretical Propositions Characteristic of Life Span Developmental Psychology
Life span development: Ontogenetic development is a lifelong process that is co-constructed by biology and culture. No age period holds
supremacy in regulating the nature of development.
Life span changes in the dynamic between biology and culture: With age and certainly after adulthood, there is a growing gap between
biological potential and individual-cultural goals. This gap is fundamental to ontogenesis as the biological architecture of life is incomplete
and inevitably results in loss of adaptive functioning and eventually death.
Life span changes in allocation of resources to distinct functions of development:growth versus maintenance versus regulation of loss:
Ontogenetic development on a systemic level involves the coordinated and competitive allocation of resources in three distinct functions:
(1) growth, (2) maintenance including recovery (resilience), and (3) regulation of loss. Life span developmental changes in the profile of
functional allocation involve a shift from the allocation of resources for growth (more typical of childhood) toward an increasingly larger and
larger share allocated to maintenance and management of loss.
Development as selection (specialization) and selective optimization in adaptive capacity: Development is inherently a process of selection
and selective adaptation. Selection is due to biological, psychological, cultural, and environmental factors. Developmental advances are due
to processes of optimization. Because development is selective and age-associated changes in potential, compensation is also part of the
developmental agenda.
Development as gain/loss dynamic: In ontogenetic development, there is no gain without loss, and no loss without gain. Selection and selective
adaptation are space-, context-, and time-bound. Thus, selection and selective adaptation imply not only advances in adaptive capacity but also
losses in adaptivity for alternative pathways and adaptive challenges. A multidimensional, multidirectional, and multifunctional conception of
development results from such a perspective.
Plasticity: Much intraindividual plasticity (within-person variability) is found in psychological development. The key developmental agenda is
the search for the range of plasticity and its age-associated changes and constraints.
Ontogenetic and historical contextualism as paradigm: In principle, the biological and cultural architecture of human development is
incomplete and subject to continuous change with biological and cultural factors, conditions, and co-constructing and modifying each other.
Thus, ontogenetic development varies markedly by historical-cultural conditions. The mechanisms involved can be characterized as principles
associated with biocultural contextualism. As an illustration, development can be understood as the outcome of the interactions (dialectics)
between three systems of biological and environmental inf luences: (1) normative age-graded,
(2) normative history-graded, and (3) nonnormative (idiosyncratic). Each of these sources evinces individual differences and, in addition, is
subject to continuous change.
Toward a general and functionalist theory of development: The ef fective coordination of selection, optimization, and compensation: On a
general and functionalist level of analysis, successful development, defined as the (subjective and objective) maximization of gains and
minimization of losses, can be conceived of as resulting from collaborative interplay among three components: (1) selection, (2) optimization,
and (3) compensation. The ontogenetic pressure for this dynamic increases with age, as the relative incompleteness of the biology- and culture-
based architecture of human development becomes more pronounced.
Updated from “Erfolgreiches Altern als Ausdruck von Verhaltenskompetenz und Umweltqualität ” (pp. 353–377), by M. M. Baltes, in Der Men-
sch im Zusammenspiel von Anlage und Umwelt, C. Niemitz (Ed.), 1987, Frankfurt-am-Main, Germany: Suhrkamp; see also P. B. Baltes, 1987,
1997, and P. B. Baltes et al., 2006.

primarily formed from sources within that entity and by a different strategy (P. B. Baltes, 1987). They attempted
mechanisms of transformation or stage-like progression. to either modulate the traditional definitional approach
Such a unidirectional, growth-like view of human de- to development or to offer conceptions that highlighted
velopment appeared contradictory to many findings in the view that ontogenetic development was not identical
life span psychology, which included negative transfer with the notion of holistic and unidirectional growth.
from earlier development to later developmental out- In these efforts, life span scholars shared the goal of re-
comes, differences in rates, age-onsets, and age-offsets formulating the concept of development, although they
of developmental trajectories, multidirectional patterns differed in the degree of radicality and in specifics.
of age-related change, as well as discontinuities in pre- Labouvie-Vief (1980, 1982; see also Pascual-Leone,
diction. Figure 11.3 represents an early representation of 1983; Riegel, 1976), for instance, introduced new forms
this differentiated view of development elicited by life (stages) of systemic functioning for the period of adult-
span thinking and findings, which posed a challenge to hood, based on conceptions of development as adaptive
traditional conceptions of development as unilinear and transformation and structural reorganization, thereby
holistic growth (see also Labouvie-Vief, 1980, 1982). opening a new vista on Neo-Piagetian constructivism.
Historically, one approach to this gap between theory In our work (e.g., P. B. Baltes, 1983, 1987; P. B. Baltes
and findings was to explore the usefulness of the distinc- et al., 1980), but also that of others such as Brandt-
tion between development and aging (Birren, 1964). städter, Featherman, and Lerner (Brandtstädter, 1984;
Life span theorists, at least within psychology, opted for Featherman & Lerner, 1985; Featherman, Smith, &
582 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Behavior-Change Process lection and selective adaptation displays many attri-


butes. For instance, it can be active or passive, conscious
or subconscious, internal or external, and continuous or
discontinuous. Moreover, in the long run or in different
circumstances, it can be functional or dysfunctional.
This intellectual movement toward a broadly based
functionalist conception of ontogenesis entailed a num-
ber of features. For instance, to reflect more accurately
their understanding of the empirical evidence about life
span changes, and also drawing from alternative concep-
tions of ontogenesis such as canalization and selective
neuronal growth (Edelman, 1987; Waddington, 1975),
Ontogenetic Time self-organization (Barton, 1994; Maturana & Varela,
Figure 11.3 Hypothetical examples of life span developmen- 1980; Prigogine & Stengers, 1984), as well as expert sys-
tal processes. Developmental functions ( behavior-change tems (Chi, Glaser, & Rees, 1982; Ericsson & Smith,
processes) differ in onset, duration, termination, and direc- 1991; Weinert & Perner, 1996), life span researchers
tionality when charted in the framework of the life course. began to emphasize that any process of development is
Moreover, developmental change is both quantitative and qual- not foremost the unfolding of an entity. Rather, they fo-
itative: Not all developmental change is related to chronologi-
cused on development as ontogenetic selection from a
cal age, and the initial direction is not always incremental.
Source: From “Plasticity and Variability in Psychological pool of more or less constrained potentialities and the
Aging: Methodological and Theoretical Issues” (pp. 41–66), subsequent selective optimization of the entered path-
by P. B. Baltes and M. M. Baltes, in Determining the Ef fects of ways including the construction of novel pathways that
Aging on the Central Nervous System, G. E. Gurski (Ed.), 1980, were not part of the original system (P. B. Baltes, 1987;
Berlin, Germany: Schering.
Labouvie-Vief, 1982; Marsiske et al., 1995; Siegler,
1989). As a given pathway of ontogenetic development is
Peterson, 1990; R. M. Lerner, 1983), we were perhaps chosen and optimized, others are ignored or suppressed.
more radical in our departure from extant theoretical In short, some life span theorists ventured a new start
models of development. We attempted to approach the and suggested treating ontogenetic development as a pro-
conceptualization of development by a theoretical cess of dynamic and selective adaptation reflecting the
framework of neofunctionalism (Dixon & Baltes, 1986) interaction of biological, cultural, and contextual factors
and contextualism (R. M. Lerner, 1991; Magnusson, as well as the proactive role of individuals in shaping
1996). Within that approach, the nature of adaptive their course of development (P. B. Baltes, Reuter-
change with life span development was driven by consid- Lorenz, et al., 2006; Brandtstädter & Lerner, 1999).
eration of a larger set of influences and the kind of chal- Thus, with the focus on selection and selective adapta-
lenges that people face as their lives unfold. In our view, tion, life span researchers were able to be more open
such a neofunctionalist approach was the most open to a about the pathways of lifelong ontogenesis.
full consideration of the new facets of ontogenetic
change (such as multidirectionality, multifunctionality, Development as a Gain-Loss Dynamic. Not sur-
adaptive specificities, and predictive discontinuity) that prisingly, a related change in emphasis advanced in life
life span researchers were confronted with. At the same span theory and research was on viewing development as
time, this broadened conception of development permit- always being constituted by gains and losses (P. B.
ted maintaining traditional growth-like conceptions of Baltes, 1979a, 1987; P. B. Baltes et al., 1980; Brandt-
development as a special class of developmental phe- städter, 1984; Brim, 1992; Labouvie-Vief, 1980, 1982;
nomena. J. Smith, 2003). Aside from functionalist arguments,
The result was to go beyond the traditional conception there were several empirical findings that gave rise to
of development as growth and open the concept of devel- this focus.
opment to a larger framework of changes. In our own One example important to life span researchers was
work, we opted for defining development as selective age- the differing life span trajectories proposed and ob-
related change in adaptive capacity. Development as se- tained for the fluid mechanics and crystallized prag-
Toward Psychological Theories of Life Span Development: Five Levels of Analysis 583

matics of intelligence (P. B. Baltes, 1993; Cattell, Vief, 1982). Consider as an example the ontogenesis of
1971; Horn, 1970; Horn & Hofer, 1992; S.-C. Li, Lin- language recognition and language acquisition in child-
denberger, et al., 2004; McArdle, Ferrer-Caja, Ham- hood. When one language is acquired as mother tongue,
agami, & Woodcock, 2002; Schaie, 1996, 2005). Very sound recognition and sound production capacity for
much in line with the life span dynamic between biol- other languages decreases, especially if such second and
ogy and culture expressed in Figure 11.1 (pp. 575), in- third languages are acquired after early childhood (Lev-
tellectual abilities that are thought to reflect the elt, 1989).
neurobiologically based mechanics of intelligence— The study of tasks requiring probability-based imper-
like working memory and fluid intelligence—typically fect rather than logic-based perfect solutions is another
showed normative (universal) declines in functioning example (P. B. Baltes, 1987). The more advanced the
beginning in middle adulthood. Conversely, intellec- cognitive status of children (in the sense of capacity for
tual abilities that primarily reflect the culture-based formal-logical reasoning), the less children are able to re-
pragmatics of intelligence—such as professional spond to cognitive problems that are essentially not per-
knowledge, language competence, and wisdom—may fectly solvable and therefore require the use of
show stability or even increase into late adulthood. As maximization rather than optimization strategies. Weir
to the ontogenesis of intelligence, then, gains and (1964) conducted an early critical experiment on this
losses do co-exist. question in the domain of probability-based learning. In
Thus, as some life span theorists considered substi- probability learning tasks without perfect solutions, there
tuting the concept of an age-related selection-based is the seemingly paradoxical finding that very young
change in adaptive capacity for the concept of develop- children outperformed older children and college stu-
ment, one of the topics that motivated their agenda was dents. Considering adaptive trade-offs between levels
the importance of viewing as fundamental to any ontoge- (stages) of cognitive functioning, this finding becomes
netic change the notion of simultaneous gains and losses meaningful. It is likely that the older children and young
associated with these changes. From a functionalist adults achieved lower performance outcomes because
point of view (Dixon & Baltes, 1986), it is more or less they understood the experimental task as a logical prob-
understood that changes in adaptive capacity can be pos- lem-solving task and, therefore, continued to employ
itive or negative, that a given change in developmental task-inappropriate but developmentally more “advanced”
capacity may imply different consequences depending cognitive strategies aimed at “perfect ” optimization.
on the outcome criteria and the adaptive contexts in- In retrospect, it is perhaps not surprising that the
volved. Thus, the radical view was advanced that, con- gain-loss dynamic was identified primarily by life span
trary to traditional conceptions of development, there researchers as a central topic of ontogenetic analysis. On
was no gain in development without loss, and no loss the one hand, life span researchers, because of their
without gain (P. B. Baltes, 1987). Life span researchers, concern for long-term processes, were pushed toward
then, conceive of ontogenetic development not as a recognizing the varied forms of developmental change
monolithic process of progression and growth, but as an associated with cultural evolution. On the other hand,
ongoing, changing, and interacting system of gains and on a subjective-phenomenological level, the issue of
losses in adaptive capacity. Throughout life, develop- gains and losses becomes more conspicuous as one con-
ment always consists of the joint occurrence of gains and siders adult development and aging. In this phase of life,
losses, both within and across domains of functioning. declines and losses, especially those due to biological
Such an approach does not preclude that on some level of aging, are difficult to ignore.
systemic analysis (i.e., considering the entirety of adap- Recently, one additional concept has been advanced
tive capacity in a fixed cultural context), ontogenetic to characterize the nature of life span changes in adap-
development evinces an overall increase or decrease in tive capacity. This concept is equifinality. Equifinality
adaptive capacity. highlights the fact that the same developmental outcome
To strengthen the general case for reformulating the can be reached by different means and combination of
concept of development, life span researchers also sug- means (Kruglanski, 1996). The role of equifinality (a
gested applying this multifunctional, multidimensional, related notion is the concept of overdetermination) is
and multidirectional view of development to the field of perhaps most evident when considering the many ways
child development (P. B. Baltes, 1976, 1987; Labouvie- by which individuals reach identical level of subjective
584 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

well-being (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990b; Brandtstädter to counteract the prevailing negative stereotype of aging
& Greve, 1994; Staudinger et al., 1995). Other examples as a period of universal decline with no opportunity
come from research on goal attainment conducted in the for positive change (P. B. Baltes, 1987; P. B. Baltes &
framework of action psychology (Brandtstädter, Chapter Labouvie, 1973; P. B. Baltes & Willis, 1977; Labouvie-
10, this Handbook, this volume; Gollwitzer & Bargh, Vief, 1977; S.-C. Li, 2003; Perlmutter, 1988). Thus,
1996). In this approach, researchers have distinguished when aging researchers demonstrated in intervention-
between two general categories of equifinality: equifi- oriented research the enhancement possibility of the
nality associated with contextual (contingency) match aging mind, even in domains such as fluid intelligence
and equifinality based on substitutability (Kruglanski, and memory in which decline was the norm, this was
1996). In life span research, notions of equifinality are counterintuitive evidence. Such evidence made clear
important, for instance, when attempting to speak of that aging, as we observe it today, is but one expression
general-purpose mechanisms and ways to compensate, of what is possible in principle. It makes conspicuous
both in the domains of intelligence and personality. The why the intellectual and societal project of constructing
potential for developmental impact is larger if the re- aging is still in the making (P. B. Baltes, 1987, 1997;
sources acquired during ontogenesis in the sense of Rowe, 1997).
equifinality carry a broad scope of generalization and Second, the concept of plasticity accentuated that life
use in rather different contexts. span development does not follow a highly constrained
(fixed) course, especially when culture- and knowledge-
A Focus on Plasticity and Age-Associated based phenotypic expressions are concerned. Thus, the
Changes in Plasticity focus on plasticity brought into the foreground that “ hu-
Arguably, plasticity is the concept most emphasized by mans have a capacity for change across the life span
life span researchers (P. B. Baltes & Schaie, 1976; P. B. from birth to death . . . [and that] the consequences of
Baltes & Willis, 1982). Note that plasticity does not the events of early childhood are continually trans-
refer to complete or arbitrary malleability of behavior. formed by later experiences, making the course of
Rather, it denotes that behavior is always open and con- human development more open than many have be-
strained at the same time. Hence, the focus on plasticity lieved” (Brim & Kagan, 1980, p. 1). Such views of life-
highlights the search for the potentialities of develop- long plasticity have become prominent in biological
ment including its boundary conditions. This notion of quarters as well (e.g., Cotman, 1985; Finch & Zelinski,
plasticity also implies that any given developmental out- 2005; Kempermann, in press).
come is but one of numerous possible outcomes, and that Third, the concept of plasticity opens new vistas on
the search for the conditions and ranges of ontogenetic interdisciplinary perspectives. A view, more recently
plasticity, including its age-associated changes, is funda- developed (P. B. Baltes et al., 2006) is that the basic
mental to the study of development. Taken to the ex- questions of plasticity can be linked to similar con-
treme, the notion of plasticity can be taken to challenge cepts in the social sciences. Thus, the insistence on
the conceptual foundation of any genetically based fixity lifelong plasticity in human development is also consis-
in ontogenesis including the notion of an immutable norm tent with the argument advanced most prominently by
of reaction (see also Gottlieb, 1998). While such vistas social scientists that much of what happens in the life
are intellectually stimulating, they are likely overextend- course is a direct reflection of the goals, resources, and
ing the scope of the empirical evidence as well as the norms of a given society and that societal contexts dif-
constraints of evolutionary theory (Hagen & Hammer- fer in the structure, emphases, and sequential ordering
stein, 2005). The very concept of plasticity of biological of such factors (Brim & Wheeler, 1966; Mayer, 1990;
plasticity depends on genetically based prerequisites and Riley, 1987; Settersten, 2005). For this purpose, Figure
related constraints for life and its developmental course. 11.4 specifies three types of plasticity: neuronal / bod-
For several reasons, life span researchers increas- ily, behavioral, and societal (see also P. B. Baltes &
ingly moved in the direction of making the study of plas- Singer, 2001; Baltes, Reuter-Lorenz, & Rösler, 2006; S.-
ticity a cornerstone of their metatheoretical posture and C. Li, 2003; S.-C. Li & Linderberger, 2002).
empirical work. In retrospect, we emphasize three such Neuronal/ bodily, behavioral, and societal plasticity,
reasons. First, as many life span researchers did work in as defined in Figure 11.4, form a frame within which the
the field of aging, plasticity-related ideas were invoked contributions to questions of potential and its realiza-
Toward Psychological Theories of Life Span Development: Five Levels of Analysis 585

open and pluralistic than traditional views of behavioral


Biocultural Co-Constructivism:
Toward Completing the Unfinished Biocultural Architecture of Aging
development during childhood and beyond seemed to
suggest. Thus, the concept of plasticity highlighted the
Bodily/Neuronal Behavioral metatheoretical posture that any course of development
Plasticity Plasticity
is but one of a pool of potentialities; that the “nature” of
Ontogeny/Development human development is not fixed; and that (aside from
the fact of finitude) there is no single end state to human
Societal development.
Plasticity The perhaps most important line of inquiry was the
quest for understanding interindividual differences and
Figure 11.4 Each of the major scientific disciplines con- age-related developmental changes in plasticity. While
cerned with human development have developed a focus on plasticity as a phenomenon was extended beyond child-
plasticity to understand mechanisms and variations in out- hood, there were theoretical and empirical reasons why
comes: Genetic/neuronal / bodily, behavioral, and societal plas-
plasticity should not be age-less but changing with age.
ticity are important examples. Research Report of the
Max Planck Institute for Human Development, 2003–2004. Thus, the search for the range of plasticity resulted not
See P. B. Baltes, P. Reuter-Lorenz, & F. Rösler, 2006, for fur- only in evidence for malleability and plasticity; it also
ther elaboration. produced new evidence on individual and age-based con-
straints in the range (norm of reaction) of possible devel-
tion offered by the various life, behavioral, and social opment (P. B. Baltes & Lindenberger, 1988; Kliegl et al.,
sciences engaged in the study of human development can 1990; Plomin & Thompson, 1988). In work on cognitive
be understood and interrelated. Each of the components aging, for instance, the goal was to learn about maxi-
depicted does not operate in isolation. Rather, in the mum potential in different age groups.
sense of biocultural constructivism they interact and This line of inquiry suggested different facets of
modify each other. behavioral /developmental plasticity. One was the dif-
Research on neuronal / bodily plasticity uses out- ferentiation between baseline reserve capacity and de-
comes such as neurogenesis, synaptic powering, and velopmental reserve capacity. Baseline reserve capacity
other indicators of brain differentiation to represent in- identifies the current level of plasticity available to in-
dividual brain development and interindividual differ- dividuals. Developmental reserve capacity is aimed
ences therein. Work on behavioral plasticity highlights at specifying what is possible in principle if optimizing
outcomes on the level of mind and behavior associated interventions are employed to test future ontogenetic
with differing conditions of life experiences, including potential. Furthermore, major efforts were made to
cognitive practice. Societal plasticity illustrates varia- specify the kind of methodologies, such as developmen-
tions at the macrolevel (e.g., resources and norms asso- tal simulation, testing-the-limits, and cognitive engi-
ciated with gender, social class, ethnicity, etc.) and the neering, that lend themselves to a full exploration of
role of social constraints and opportunities. Relevant ontogenetic plasticity and its limits (P. B. Baltes, 1987;
evidence is typically collected by comparative social- P. B. Baltes & Willis, 1982; Kliegl & Baltes, 1987;
science work on groups or nations rather than individu- Kliegl, Mayr, & Krampe, 1994; Lindenberger & Baltes,
als and on theories of societal influences (e.g., norms, 1995b).
socialization) that shape developmental trajectories and Within the frame of sizeable plasticity, then, the ex-
their social differentiation. A societal plasticity per- pression of human development is a matter of collabo-
spective presumes that individuals belonging to differ- ration and co-construction between different factors
ent groups have similar potentialities which, however, and mechanisms. Indeed, an increasingly more
are realized to different degrees and qualities (see also full-blown constructivist perspective on human poten-
Settersten, 2005). tialities has become a modern theme of developmental
Returning to developmental psychology: As work on research (Aspinwall & Staudinger, 2003; P. B. Baltes
individual behavioral plasticity progressed and showed et al., 2006; P. B. Baltes & Smith, 2004; Brandtstädter
large variation in developmental manifestations, the & Lerner, 1999; S.-C. Li, 2003; S.-C. Li & Linden-
concept of plasticity became a mental script that sup- berger, 2002). With a constructivist perspective
ported the general idea of development as being more one highlights the notion that human development is
586 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

constructed by the interplay of biological, psychologi- life course (e.g., Elder, 1994; Elder & Shanahan, Chap-
cal, and social forces. Part of this construction relies ter 12, this Handbook, this volume; Heckhausen, 2000;
on agentic behavior of individuals. Individuals are Kohli & Meyer, 1986; Mayer, 2003; Riley, 1987; Setter-
contributors to their own development. The result- sten, 2005). In fact, this was a time when sociologists
ing concept is that of developmental biocultural co- and developmental psychologists attempted to interre-
constructivism (P. B. Baltes, Freund, & Li, 2005; late their various endeavors (e.g., Sorensen, Weinert, &
P. B. Baltes & Smith, 2004; S.-C. Li, 2003). With the Sherrod, 1986). For life span psychologists, and perhaps
advent of biocultural co-constructivism, the quest for also for child developmentalists (P. B. Baltes, 1979b),
interdisciplinary collaboration has attained a new this dialogue opened their vista on the scope, temporal
state of urgency. In our view, the life span approach patterning, and differentiation of biological and social
with its emphasis on viewing the conditions of human forces (incidentally much instigated by various commit-
development as historically incomplete and more open tees on human development arranged by the U.S. Social
than traditionally assumed has been a major partner in Science Research Council).
advancing this intellectual position.
A Macro-Model of Developmental Influences
Ontogenetic and Historical Contextualism
During this time of intensive collaboration between
as Paradigm
life course sociologists (e.g., Riley et al., 1972) and
Highlighting the notion of plasticity as a cornerstone life span psychologists, the first author and his
of life span research on human development alludes to colleagues (P. B. Baltes, Cornelius, & Nesselroade,
another key feature of life span metatheory, the para- 1979; P. B. Baltes et al., 1980) proposed a heuristic
digm of contextualism. In evolutionary selection theory model that attempted to integrate biological, sociolog-
and the evolutionary basis of adaptive fitness, the role ical, and psychological considerations in one frame-
of context is paramount. Recently, P. B. Baltes and work in order to understand the entire fabric of
Smith (2004) have shown how modern versions of con- development-producing contexts: Three biocultural
textualism include the perspective of biocultural co- components were considered at the foundation of
constructivism to avoid the idea that context is strictly human ontogeny: Normative age-graded inf luences,
environmental in origin. normative history-graded inf luences, and nonnorma-
Therefore, as developmental psychologists attempted tive (idiosyncratic) influences. Normative in this
to move beyond microgenetic representations of the context refers to a high degree of generality. Nonnor-
learning process as a marker of experience to capture mative factors highlight the more individualized con-
context as a system of influence, they engaged them- ditions such as winning in a lottery.
selves into metatheoretical perspectives on contextual- To understand a given life course, and interindividual
ism. Such a contextualist view, rather than a focus on differences in life course trajectories, this model sug-
“mechanist ” or “organismic” models of development gests that it is necessary to consider the operation and
(Overton & Reese, 1973; Reese & Overton, 1970), interaction among these three classes of influences
evolved with force in the 1970s (Datan & Reese, 1977; (Figure 11.5). Note that these sources contribute to sim-
Riegel, 1976), and as already described in the preceding ilarities in development, but also, because they exist in
section, it continues into the present. This approach was systematic group variations, for instance by social
similar to the evolution of ecological-contextualist per- class, genetic dispositions, and ethnicity, they also
spectives offered by cultural psychology (Bronfenbren- contribute to systematic interindividual variations and
ner, 1977; Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994; Cole, 1996). subgroup-specific patterns of life span development
According to contextualism and also action theory (P. B. Baltes & Nesselroade, 1984; Dannefer, 1989; Riley
(see Brandtstädter, Chapter 10, this Handbook, this vol- et al., 1972).
ume), individuals exist in contexts that create both spe- Age-graded inf luences are those biological and envi-
cial opportunities for, and limitations to, individual ronmental aspects that, because of their dominant
developmental pathways. Delineation of these contexts age correlation, shape individuals in relatively norma-
in terms of macrostructural features, like social class, tive ways for all individuals. Consider the temporal
ethnicity, roles, age-based passages and historical peri- and domain structure of life span developmental tasks
ods, is a major goal for the sociological analysis of the (Havighurst, 1948), the age-based process of physi-
Toward Psychological Theories of Life Span Development: Five Levels of Analysis 587

dura, 1982; Brim & Ryff, 1980). The influence of these


Basic Determinants Influences on Development
nonnormative events (such as winning a lottery, losing a
Normative leg in an accident) is especially powerful because they
Age-graded
Biological generate conditions that are less predictable, less

Interaction
Normative amenable to social control and support, and therefore
Interaction History-graded
may represent extreme situations of challenge (ap-
Environmental proaching testing-of-limits), not unlike the concept of
Non-Normative
Grenzsituation introduced by the philosopher Karl
Jaspers (Kruse, 1992; Maercker, 1995).
Ontogenetic Time
In life span theory, these three sources of influence
Figure 11.5 Representation of the operation of three major create the contexts within which individuals act, react,
biocultural influence systems on life span development: (1) organize their own development, and contribute to the
normative age-graded, (2) normative history-graded, and (3) development of others. None of these patterns of biolog-
nonnormative life events. These influence systems vary in ically and environmentally based influences is likely to
their level and interactions for different individuals and for operate independently from the other. They are part of
different behaviors. As a whole, the operation of these sys-
biocultural co-construction with reciprocal and modify-
tems produces commonalities and individual differences in
ontogenesis. Source: From “Plasticity and Variability in Psy- ing influences. Such a focus on the dynamics of biocul-
chological Aging: Methodological and Theoretical Issues” tural co-construction also makes explicit the lack of full
(pp. 41–66), by P. B. Baltes and M. M. Baltes, in Determining predictability of human development as well as the
the Ef fects of Aging on the Central Nervous System, G. E. boundedness that individuals experience as they engage
Gurski (Ed.), 1980, Berlin, Germany: Schering.
in the effort to compose and manage their lives (Brandt-
städter, 1984; Brandtstädter & Lerner, 1999; R. M.
cal maturation, or the sequential arrangement of Lerner, 1984, 1991). And finally, such a focus on con-
developmental contexts (family, school, work, etc.) as textualism places individual development in the context
examples. of the development of others. It is not surprising, there-
History-graded inf luences are those biological and fore, that life span researchers have easily embraced
environmental aspects that may make ontogenetic concepts such as collaborative development, collabora-
development different across historical cohort and pe- tive cognition, or interactive minds (P. B. Baltes &
riods. Consider the historical evolution of the educa- Staudinger, 1996a; Resnick, Levine, & Teasley, 1991).
tional and professional system as an example, or, for a However, what remains underdeveloped in life span psy-
more punctuated period-specific example, the advent chology is the empirical counterpart to this theoretical
of a war. Thus, a given ontogeny proceeds at the same position. Only more recently have we witnessed re-
time in the contexts of age-based ontogenetic time as search efforts to include these contextual- and social-in-
well as historical cohort time. This position has been teractive approaches in the study of interactive networks
argued most fervently by Matilda Riley (1987). In the such as communities of learning (Mandl, Gruber, &
early phases of life span psychology, research on birth- Renkl, 1996), life course convoys (Kahn & Antonucci,
cohort effects has made the strongest case for consider- 1980), mentors (Bloom, 1985), cohort formations
ation of historical contextualism (Elder, 1974, 1990; (Riley, 1987), kinship relationships (Hammerstein,
J. R. Nesselroade & Baltes, 1974; Schaie, 1965, 1996). 1996), cohort-related changes in education and health
The topic of historical embeddedness, and the extricat- (Schaie, 1996, 2005), the role of neighborhoods, or
ing of age-based versus cohort-based differences in on- changing policies in retirement and elderly care.
togenetic development, was also the foundation for the
formulation of new developmental methodologies such Methodological Developments
as cross-sectional and longitudinal sequences (see the Life span research opened new territories and because of
following discussion). the temporal, contextual, and historical complexities in-
Nonnormative inf luences on development, finally, re- volved required much attention to developmental
flect the individual-idiosyncratic biological and envi- methodology (P. B. Baltes, Reese, & Nesselroade, 1988;
ronmental events that, while not frequent, can have Cohen & Reese, 1994; Hertzog, 1985; Magnusson,
powerful influences on ontogenetic development (Ban- Bergman, Rudinger, & Törestad, 1991; J. R. Nesselroade
588 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

& Reese, 1973). In our view, this concern about adequate


methodology was so important to life span researchers
because their orientation toward long-term ontogenetic
processes and linkages and the decomposition of the bio-
cultural dynamic represented an extreme challenge to the
goals and methods of developmental analysis.

From Cross-Sectional to Longitudinal to Sequen-


tial Methodology. A first example is the development
of methods appropriate to the study of age-related
change, interindividual differences in age-related change,
and the role of historical changes in the contexts of de-
velopment. Traditionally, the main designs used in de-
velopmental psychology were the cross-sectional and the
longitudinal method (P. B. Baltes & Nesselroade, 1978,
for historical review). The focus on the interplay between
age-graded, history-graded, and nonnormative factors
suggested, however, that such methods were insufficient
(P. B. Baltes, 1968; N. B. Ryder, 1965; Schaie, 1965).
This challenge to track both historical and individual-
Figure 11.6 Illustration of cross-sectional and longitudi-
ontogenetic change resulted in the formulation of so- nal sequences ( bottom). Source: From “Longitudinal and
called sequential methods (P. B. Baltes, 1968; Schaie, Cross-Sectional Sequences in the Study of Age and Genera-
1965, 1996, 2005). tion Effects” by P. B. Baltes, 1968, Human Development, 11,
Figure 11.6 depicts the basic arrangement of what pp. 145–171; From “A General Model for the Study of Devel-
opmental Problems” by K. W. Schaie, 1965, Psychological
Schaie and Baltes (1975) have come to label as cross-
Bulletin, 64, pp. 92–107.
sectional and longitudinal sequences. Cross-sectional se-
quences consist of successions of cross-sectional
studies; longitudinal sequences of successions of longi- large as age effects. Schaie’s work has also shown that
tudinal studies. When applied in combination, the two the directionality of age and cohort gradients can differ.
types of sequential designs produce, on a descriptive Similarly, J. R. Nesselroade and Baltes (1974), in an
level, exhaustive information about age- and cohort- early application of longitudinal sequences to the study
related change as well as about interindividual differ- of adolescence, presented evidence that personality de-
ences in change trajectories. The sequential design also velopment during adolescence in such measures as
permits the identification of punctuated historical ef- achievement and independence evinced major cohort
fects, so-called period effects. In contrast to cohort differences over time intervals as short as 2 years. Their
effects, which extend over longer time spans of histori- interpretation focused on the role of the Vietnam War as
cal change (such as effects associated with mass educa- the critical modulator variable and its impact on U.S.
tion or the introduction of computer technology), the youth culture, including how adolescents changed in
concept of period effects is typically applied to more their developmental personality gradients.
transient historical events and their consequences, such Meanwhile, through application of sequential meth-
as a natural catastrophe or a war. ods, there is a large body of evidence on cohort effects
There is much research in human development that available in developmental psychology, but especially in
has demonstrated the important role of historical cohort comparative sociology; evidence that makes explicit one
effects. Schaie (1996, 2005), for instance, has compared of the important ingredients to life span theory, namely,
both in cross-sectional and longitudinal sequences the the interplay between individual development and a
adult-age development of several birth cohorts from changing society (cf. Elder & Shanahan, Chapter 12, this
1956 to the present and presented impressive evidence Handbook, this volume; Settersten, 2005). Also important
that, during middle adulthood, cohort effects can be as in this work is the growing recognition of when cohort ef-
Toward Psychological Theories of Life Span Development: Five Levels of Analysis 589

fects are likely to be relevant and when not. For instance, yet been fully explored and understood (Hertzog, Lin-
life span researchers now distinguish between at least denberger, Ghisletta, & Oertzen, 2004).
three types of cohort effects requiring different kinds of .
interpretative efforts (J. R. Nesselroade & Baltes, 1979):
(1) cohort as a theoretical process denoting historical The Experimental Simulation of Development.
change that alters fundamental aspects of human on- A further strategy developed primarily by life span re-
togeny (e.g., changing gender roles); (2) cohort as a di- searchers is the explicit use of simulation paradigms in
mension of quantitative generalization (e.g., higher levels the study of human development. Again, use of such an
of cognitive skills due to an increase in education); and approach was enhanced by the fact that life span ontoge-
(3) cohort as a transitory disturbance (e.g., fluctuant netic processes are time-extensive and, therefore, diffi-
changes in attitudes due to singular events as often re- cult to study without simulation (P. B. Baltes & Goulet,
ported in opinion survey research). 1971; Lindenberger & Baltes, 1995b).
In part as a response to the growing availability of Table 11.4 summarizes the approach of developmen-
longitudinal and sequential data sets, methodologists tal simulation. In a general sense, the experimental sim-
from different research traditions including life-span ulation approach is a theory-testing device that
psychology have refined and expanded statistical meth- arranges for conditions thought to be relevant for the
ods for the study of interindividual differences in devel- phenomenon of interest. Thus, experimental develop-
opmental trajectories (Baltes, Reese, & Nesselroade, mental simulations simulate or mimic variations that
1977; for a recent overview, see Hertzog & Nessel- are thought to exist in real-time and real-world ontogen-
roade, 2003). Various longitudinal modeling techniques esis. As a research strategy, the design of developmen-
such as multilevel modeling, latent growth curve mod- tal simulation consists of a coordinated sequence of
eling, and latent difference score modeling allow re- seven steps that, however, do not need to be performed
searchers to examine the structure of interindividual in the sequence specified. A developmental phenome-
differences in change (e.g., Ghisletta & Lindenberger, non is considered to be well understood if knowledge
2004). These methods attenuate complications com- based on all steps is available.
monly associated with change scores such as lack of re- In life span research, such simulations have been used,
liability (e.g., Cronbach & Furby, 1970), and some of for instance, to examine the effects of aging-associated
them, such as multivariate extensions of the dual changes in sensory input. For this purpose, auditory and
change score model, permit testing of dynamic hy- visual acuity of adults was reduced to the level of older
potheses linking one aspect of behavior to changes in
another aspect (e.g., McArdle, Hamagami, Meredith, &
TABLE 11.4 The Logic of Experimental Simulation in the
Bradway, 2000; for applications to changes in intellec-
Study of Development: A Coordinated Sequence of Steps
tual and sensory functioning, see Ghisletta & Linden-
1 Definition and description of target developmental phenomenon
berger, 2005). A related methodological development to be studied
spurred on by life-course sociologists, in particular, 2 Postulation of a causal hypothesis or causal structure about
concerns methods to organize and study the temporal underlying mechanisms and contextual conditions
flow, correlates, and consequences of life events. Mod- 3 Experimental manipulation of relevant variables in the laboratory
els of event-history analysis and associated methods 4 Test of experimental data against target phenomenon: isomorphism
check
such as hazard rate analysis are especially important
5 Reexamination of causal hypothesis or causal structure
(Blossfeld, Hamerle, & Mayer, 1991; Blossfeld & Roh- (confirmation /rejection /modification) and search for
wer, 2001; Featherman & Lerner, 1985; Greve, Tuma, alternative explanations
& Strang, 2001; Magnusson et al., 1991; Schaie, 1988; 6 Evaluation of external validity: Descriptive evidence
Willett & Singer, 1991). Note, however, that advanced 7 Evaluation of external validity: Interventive evidence
statistical methods for analyzing multivariate longitu- Source: Modified from “ Testing-the-Limits and Experimental Simula-
dinal change often are based on strong assumptions tion: Two Methods to Explicate the Role of Learning in Development,”
such as sample homogeneity, in general, and cross- by U. Lindenberger and P. B. Baltes, 1995b, Human Development, 38,
pp. 349–360; and Life-Span Developmental Psychology: Introduction to
sectional / longitudinal convergence, in particular. Also, Research Methods, by P. B. Baltes, H. W. Reese, and J. R. Nesselroade,
the psychometric properties of these methods have not 1988, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Reprint of the 1977 edition.
590 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

persons and then tested for cognitive performance velopment (Siegler, Chapter 11, this Handbook, Volume
(Dickinson & Rabbitt, 1991; Lindenberger, Scherer, & 2). However, we claim that life span researchers are par-
Baltes, 2001). Another example is a research program by ticularly dependent on the creative use of such arrange-
Margret Baltes on the many faces of dependency and ments; and, moreover, that life span researchers are
autonomy in old age (1988, 1996; M. M. Baltes & Wahl, especially aware of the many methodological limitations
1992). In this research program, the key questions were (such as lack of measurement equivalence, isomorphy,
concerned with the conditions and range of autonomy and external validity) associated with such and with
and dependence including their multifunctional charac- other age-comparative research. The explicit use of the
teristics and plasticity. term of simulation to denote these limitations under-
The opening steps (1 to 3 in Table 11.4) of this re- scores this awareness.
search on autonomy and dependency in old age
conducted by Margret Baltes and her colleagues Testing-the-Limits. An additional example of meth-
were observations in the living environments of elders odological innovations involves a strategy that life span
concerning their transactions with others. Negative researchers have developed to examine the scope and
aging stereotypes were assumed to play a major role limits of behavioral plasticity (P. B. Baltes, 1987; Kliegl
in the observed age-associated emergence of depend- & Baltes, 1987), another key aspect of the family of
ent rather than independent behavior. To examine propositions advanced in life span theory. This method
this hypothesis, a series of experimental laboratory is similar to efforts in child development to study the
studies were conducted to explore the effects of learn- zone of proximal development, for instance, through
ing conditions (stimulus control, practice, reinforce- methods of microgenetic analysis or cognitive engineer-
ment schedules) on self-care behavior in older adults. ing (Brown, 1982; Kliegl & Baltes, 1987; Kuhn, 1995;
This work demonstrated that many aspects of older Siegler & Crowley, 1991).
adults’ dependent behaviors were found to be re- Again, because of the long timeframe of life
versible, supporting the notion that environmental fac- span ontogenesis, it is very difficult in life span re-
tors (e.g., behavioral contingencies) exert some search to identify the sources and scope of intraindi-
influence on the aging-associated emergence of depen- vidual plasticity (malleability) and its age-related
dency or loss of autonomy. In subsequent work, re- changes. At the same time, one key question for life
flective of steps 4 to 6 in Table 11.4, Margret Baltes span researchers is: What is possible in principle in
and her colleagues observed the social conditions sur- human development across the life span? One of the
rounding the occurrence of self-care in the elderly in perennial questions of cognitive aging researchers,
the natural environment. Supportive of their position, therefore, was whether aging losses in functions reflect
a dependency-support script and an independence- experiential practice deficits with cognitive activities
ignore script were identified. In other words, social rather than effects of biological aging (P. B. Baltes &
partners of older persons in the context of self-care ex- Labouvie, 1973; Denney, 1984; Salthouse, 1991; Willis
hibited a high frequency of behaviors indicative of & Baltes, 1980).
support of dependence. Finally, research was con- The resulting method has been labeled the testing-the-
ducted to manipulate the relevant causal variables in limits paradigm (Kliegl & Baltes, 1987; Lindenberger &
the natural environment of older persons. For this pur- Baltes, 1995b; Schmidt, 1971). In testing-the-limits re-
pose, the researchers (see M. M. Baltes, 1996; M. M. search, the goal is to compress time by providing for high
Baltes, Neumann, & Zank, 1994) intervened in the so- density developmental experiences; and by doing so
cial environment of older persons in nursing homes. to arrange for the best conditions possible and to identify
This was done by training nursing home staff to down- asymptotes of performance potential (plasticity). These
play the dependence-support script, and to move to- asymptotes, obtained under putatively optimal condi-
ward an independence-support script. By and large, tions of support, are expected to estimate the upper
these changes in the natural environment resulted in range of the age-specific developmental potentiality
the expected outcome. Older persons displayed a comparable to the traditional notion of the upper limit of
higher level of independence in self-care. the “norm of reaction.” The use of testing-the-limits pro-
Researchers interested in more narrow age spectrums cedures has generated new insights into what is and what
use similar strategies of experimental simulation of de- is not possible in development.
Toward Psychological Theories of Life Span Development: Five Levels of Analysis 591

Testing-the-limits research, however, is not only rel- much resemblance to the notion of compensation in SOC
evant for the study of long-term ontogenetic processes. theory.
It is equally relevant for other important aspects of The SOC model in its generality is still located at a
developmental research and theory. Two examples il- level of analysis that is distant from specific theory.
lustrate this. The first is the question of sex or gender Thus, as the model is applied to specific domains of
differences in cognitive functioning. What would psychological functioning (such as autonomy or profes-
be most necessary is to depart from simple, noninter- sional expertise), it requires further specification to be
ventive comparative research and to invest scientific derived from the knowledge base of the domain of func-
resources into testing-the-limits work. A testing-the- tioning selected for application (e.g., Abraham & Hans-
limits approach would be based on the premise that the son, 1995; B. B. Baltes & Heydens-Gahir, 2003; M. M.
relevant information is knowledge about differences in Baltes & Lang, 1997; Featherman et al., 1990; Freund
asymptotic (peak) levels of functioning. Small, care- & Baltes, 1998, 2002b; S.-C. Li & Freund, 2005; Mar-
fully selected samples could be used for this purpose siske et al., 1995). At the same time, however, because
(e.g., P. B. Baltes & Kliegl, 1992; Kliegl & Baltes, of this generality in formulation, the model of SOC is
1987; Lindenberger, Kliegl, & Baltes, 1992). The same rather open as to its deployability and domain-specific
perspective would hold true for another hotly debated refinement.
topic; that is, research into genetic differences. Rather In principle, the theory of SOC is considered a
than investing most of the available resources into general theory of proactive and adaptive development
largely descriptive behavior-genetics studies, an alter- (P. B. Baltes, 1997; Li & Freund, 2005). As a general
native would be to expose smaller samples of partici- theory of development, it pursues two objectives: First,
pants to time-compressed experiential interventions an account of how developmental resources are gener-
and to search for interindividual differences at the ated, and second, how resources once they are devel-
upper or lower levels of functioning (e.g., S.-C. Li, oped are allocated to master the tasks of life including in
Huxhold, & Schmiedek, 2004; Lindenberger & situations where resources are insufficient.
Oertzen, in press).
Definition of Selection, Optimization,
and Compensation
An Example of a Systemic and Overall Theory
As mentioned earlier, we proceed from the assumption
of Life Span Development: Selective
that any process of development involves selection and
Optimization with Compensation (Level 4)
selective changes in adaptive capacity (P. B. Baltes,
Next, we take one further step toward a more psycholog- 1987; Featherman et al., 1990; Freund & Baltes, 2002b;
ical level of analysis of the nature of life span de- Krampe & Baltes, 2003; Marsiske et al., 1995). Selection
velopment. For this purpose, we describe a model of from a potential pool of developmental trajectories makes
development, selective optimization with compensa- directionality in development and higher levels of func-
tion (SOC), which Margret Baltes, Paul Baltes, and tioning possible. We further assume that for selection to
their colleagues have developed over the past decade result in successful development (maximization of gains
(M. M. Baltes, 1987; M. M. Baltes & Carstensen, 1996; while minimizing losses), it needs to work in conjunction
P. B. Baltes, 1987; P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1980, 1990b; with processes of optimization and compensation.
P. B. Baltes, Dittmann-Kohli, & Dixon, 1984; Freund & If approached within an action-theoretical frame-
Baltes, 2002b; S.-C. Li & Freund, 2005; Marsiske et al., work, which is only one of the many possible theoreti-
1995; Riediger, S.-C. Li, & Lindenberger, in press; see cal frames, the following characterizations of the three
also Featherman et al., 1990). This model offers a sys- components hold: Selection involves goals or outcomes;
temic view of human development across the life span Optimization involves goal-related means to achieve
involving many of the features of life span development success (desired outcomes); and Compensation involves
presented in the previous sections. Heckhausen and a response to loss in goal-relevant means in order to
Schulz (1995; Schulz & Heckhausen, 1996) developed a maintain success or desired levels of functioning (out-
similar model. Finally, the notion of vicariance, promi- comes). Table 11.5 summarizes this approach and offers
nent in francophone differential and developmental psy- as illustrations items from a study on proverbs and
chology (e.g., Lautrey, 2003; cf. Reuchlin, 1978), bears items from a self-report measure developed to assess
592 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

TABLE 11.5 Selection, Optimization, and Compensation: Brief Definitional Frames and Examples from Proverbs and
Questionnaire Items

Role in Sample Proverb Sample Questionnaire Item


Strategy Development (Freund & Baltes, 2002a) (Freund & Baltes, 2002b)
Selection1a Concerns directionality and focus of Jack-of-all-trades, master of none. I always focus on the most important
developmental outcomes such as goals. goal at a given time.
Those who follow every path, never When I think about what I want in life, I
reach any destination. commit myself to one or two important
goals.
Between two stools you fall to the To achieve a particular goal, I am willing
ground. to abandon other goals.
Optimization Concerns the acquisition and Practice makes perfect. I keep working on what I have planned
refinement of means and their until I succeed.
coordinaton to achieve goals/outcomes. If at first you don’t succeed, try, I keep trying until I succeed at a goal.
try, and try again.
Strike the iron when it’s hot. When I want to achieve something, I can
wait for the right moment.
Compensation Concerns maintenance of functioning Those without a horse walk. When things don’t work the way they
by substitution of means in situation used to, I look for other ways to achieve
of losses of means. them.
There are many hands; what one When things aren’t going so well, I accept
cannot do, the other will. help from others.
When there’s no wind, grab the oars. When things don’t go as well as they
used to, I keep trying other ways until I
can achieve the same result I used to.

a
Two facets of selection are distinguished in SOC theory: (1) elective selection and (2) loss-based selection, which encompasses restructur-
ing of goal hierarchy, reducing the number of goals or various processes such as adjusting the level of aspiration, or developing new possible
goals to match available resources.

the degree to which individuals report to use SOC- tional utility of SOC is not given but remains a question
related behaviors. The resulting definitions of selection, of empirical validity. There are contexts where SOC
optimization, and compensation may suggest that the may not be adaptive. Sixth, the function of the SOC
relevant processes are often conscious and intentional. components such as compensation in a given behavioral
This is not necessarily so. Each of these elements or unit are not fixed. Their logical status can change, for
components can be active or passive, internal or exter- instance, from active to passive. Similarly, a behavior
nal, conscious or unconscious. that originally evolved in the context of a compensation
Six additional characterizations help to place SOC for a loss may later be activated in a process where it
into a larger perspective. First, we postulate that SOC is serves as an optimizing means.
akin to a general-purpose mechanism of development. An everyday example may help to clarify the dis-
If available and well practiced, it will produce higher tinctions, drawn from the context of aging research
functioning in all domains of functioning. Second, we that we used in our early efforts at developing the SOC
assume that SOC behaviors are universal processes model (P. B. Baltes, 1984). Into his late 70s and early
generative of development. Third, we assume that SOC 80s, the concert pianist Arthur Rubinstein continued
are inherently relativistic in that their phenotypic to perform with great success. When asked how he
expressions depend on person- and context-specific managed to maintain such a high level of expert piano
features. Fourth, SOC in itself is a developmental con- playing, he hinted in several interviews at the coordi-
struct. We assume that its peak expression is in adult- nation of three strategies. First, he mentioned that he
hood. In childhood and adolescence, the system is played fewer pieces (selection); second, he indicated
acquired and honed, in old age, individuals work on that he now practiced these pieces more often (opti-
maintenance (see Freund & Baltes, 2002b, for data on mization); and third, he counteracted his loss in me-
age trajectories). Fifth, we acknowledge that the func- chanical speed of playing by producing larger contrasts
Toward Psychological Theories of Life Span Development: Five Levels of Analysis 593

in speed so to make the faster pieces appear faster ity to persist or delay gratification. In general, the com-
(compensation). plexity of the system of optimization depends on the
goal or outcome pursuit. If these are complex, optimiza-
Selection: Elective and Loss-Based. As noted al- tion is not the refinement of a single means. Rather, in
ready, selection involves directionality, goals, and speci- more complex situations, optimization requires a mutu-
fication of outcomes. There are two kinds of selection: ally enhancing coalition of factors, including health, en-
elective selection and loss-based selection. Elective se- vironmental, and psychological conditions.
lection involves directionality that is self-initiated and As was true for selection, optimization can be active
considered desirable. Its motivational force is agent- and passive, conscious and subconscious, internal or ex-
driven. Loss-based selection is the consequence of a loss ternal. Moreover, optimization can be domain- and
in functioning and typically involves making adjust- goal-specific as well as domain- and goal-general. The
ments such as changes in level of aspiration or a change most domain-general notion of optimization is the gen-
in goal structures or goal priorities. eration of what in our work we have called developmen-
Strictly speaking, selection already begins in embry- tal reserve capacity (P. B. Baltes, 1987; Kliegl & Baltes,
onic development with features of the sensory system, 1987), or what developmental life scientists might call
such as differential sensitivity to light and pattern con- general plasticity at the neuronal, behavioral, and social
figurations. Neurophysiological processing of informa- level. Because of its investability into many activities,
tion represents another fundamental example of generating a high level of general plasticity is the per-
selection and selection-based specialization. Selective haps most significant target for successful development.
pruning of cells in early biological development is an-
other example. Another concrete illustration of selec- Compensation. The component process called
tion in development can be associated with a concept compensation involves a functional response to the loss of
from developmental biology: Selection as the “canal- goal-relevant means (see also Brandtstädter & Wentura,
ized” (Waddington, 1975) realization of a set of 1995; Dixon & Bäckman, 1995). This definition of com-
outcomes from the “potentialities of ontogenesis” (plas- pensation is more specific or restricted than the one
ticity). Another example of selection is the goal system proposed by Bäckman and Dixon (1992)—that is, it re-
(ranging from skills to attitudes and values) that defines stricts compensation to responses to losses of means (re-
the social and personal frames of desirable development. sources) once available for goal attainment.
Selection can also involve the avoidance of specific out- Two main causes give rise to a compensatory situa-
comes of development such as the undesired self. In fact, tion (Freund & Baltes, 2002b; Marsiske et al., 1995).
life span development can be seen as involving a system- Compensation can be the consequence of the very fact
atic age-related shift in the relative weight and fre- of selection and optimization. For reasons of limited ca-
quency of approach versus avoidance goals (Freund & pacity of time and effort, selection of and optimization
Ebner, 2005). toward a given goal implies the loss of time and means-
related resources, relevant for the pursuit of other goals.
Optimization. The focus of optimization is on Development is always a gain-loss dynamic. When an
goal- or outcome-relevant means or resources. Thus, athlete aims for a high level performance in the shot put,
while selection is a necessary condition for achieving it is unlikely that comparable high levels of performance
development (defined as the maximization of gains and can be achieved in other types of sports such as gymnas-
minimization of losses), selection is not a sufficient tics. Another example is negative transfer. The acquisi-
condition for development to become manifest. tion of a targeted expert skill system A can result in
In addition, conditions and procedural mechanisms of negative transfer to another skill system B (Ericsson &
goal-attainment are required, that is, methods or means Smith, 1991).
of optimization. Optimization, then, involves processes A second category of causes of compensation stems
aimed at the generation and refinement of means-ends from negative changes in biological, social, and environ-
resources and motivational-goal explication to achieve mental resources in the conditions that represent
development-oriented positive outcomes (goals). For a the foundation of resources and their use for develop-
psychologist, means include such processes as the learn- ment (see also Hobfoll, 2001, on resource theory).
ing of a skill or the acquisition of the motivational abil- Changing from one environment to another may involve a
594 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

loss in environment-based resources (means) or may across levels of analysis and can be usefully linked to
make some acquired personal means dysfunctional. other current theoretical streams in developmental psy-
Losses due to the biology of aging are perhaps the best chology, such as to dynamic systems theory. Krampe
known age-associated negative changes in resources. and Baltes (2003) have illustrated in another area, the
With aging, there is a reduction in the rate and scope of field of intelligence, how application of SOC theory
plasticity (Cotman, 1985; Finch & Zelinski, 2005; S.-C. leads to a different conceptualization of the structure
Li & Freund, 2005; Nelson, 2006; Reuter-Lorenz, 2002). and function of intelligence.
As a result, the evolution of compensatory responses, in
addition to loss-based selection, is a continuously chang- Empirical Evidence on SOC Theory
ing dynamic of development in the second half of life. The articulation and testing of SOC theory is proceed-
Understanding this changing developmental dynamic ing in a variety of domains. In general, the evidence has
is particularly important regarding the conceptual dis- been supportive of the theoretical approach. People who
tinctiveness of optimization and compensation (Mar- report the use of SOC-related behaviors show higher
siske et al., 1995). At the point of origin, for instance, levels of functioning. Moreover, on the behavioral level,
some behavior may have been compensatory (such research has shown that individuals manifest behaviors
as acquiring nonverbal techniques of communication that are consistent with SOC theory. These outcomes
due to a loss of foreign language proficiency), at later carry a promissory note.
points in ontogeny or in different contexts these same
compensation-based behavioral means (nonverbal tech- Age Gradients. Figure 11.7 summarizes evidence on
niques of communication) can be used as a technique of cross-sectional age gradients. Young, middle-aged, and
optimization, such as when improving one’s perfor- older adults answered a self-report instrument to assess
mance as an actor. It is important, therefore, to specify preferred use of SOC strategies. As expected, the peak of
the context and the developmental space in which a using all SOC components was obtained for adults. In ear-
given behavioral event is considered when deciding lier and later phases of life, the SOC system seems less
about its category allocation to either selection, opti- fully acquired, activated, or coordinated. In young adult-
mization, or compensation. hood, the task of life planning in a focused and concerted
Because the model of SOC does not designate the spe- manner needs practice and refinement (e.g., J. Smith,
cific content and form of desirable developmental out- 1999). Desires and volitions are less orchestrated. Simi-
comes, it is applicable to a large range of variations in larly, in aging individuals, they need to master situations
goals and means of development. In this sense, then, SOC
is at the same time universal and relativistic. Its universal- 53
Elective
ism rests in the argument that any process of development Selection
52
is expected to involve components of selection, optimiza-
51
tion, and compensation (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990b; Loss-Based
SOC (T-Scores)

Marsiske et al., 1995). Its relativity lies in the variations 50 Selection


of motivational, social, and intellectual resources, as well 49 Optimization
as in the criteria used to define successful development, 48
which can be multivariate and involve both objective and
47 Compensation
subjective indicators (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990a).
In the following two sections, which deal with life 46

span developmental theory and research in two domains 45


Young Adults Middle-Age Older Adults
of functioning, we occasionally return to SOC-related
interpretations. However, our intent is not to elevate that Figure 11.7 Age-group mean differences in four components
model or theory to the one overarching model of life of SOC (elective selection, loss-based selection, optimization,
span development. This would be inappropriate. In our and compensation): Middle-aged adults report the highest and
perhaps most integrated endorsement of SOC. Source: Modi-
view, the model of selective optimization with compen-
fied from “Life-Management Strategies of Selection, Opti-
sation is but one of the theoretical efforts that life span mization, and Compensation: Measurement by Self-Report and
research and theory have spawned. However, we believe Construct Validity” by A. M. Freund and P. B. Baltes, 2002b,
SOC to be a theory that displays much consistency Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, pp. 642–662.
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 595

in which they have fewer resources. SOC behaviors them- Moreover, with age children become better in handling
selves are effortful and require resources. Therefore, it is multiple tasks simultaneously and minimize what is usu-
not surprising that older individuals show lesser frequency ally called dual-task costs. With aging, the reverse is true.
of use of optimization and compensation. As shown in Dual-task research is a prime model to study develop-
Figure 11.7, the primary focus in older ages is on elective ment as a system of co-changing and collaborative
selection and loss-based selection. processes, and of the process of differential allocation
Processes of selection, optimization, and compensa- of resources. Several studies have tested SOC theory
tion also are present in mental representations associ- within this model or examined whether the findings are
ated with the management of everyday lives. Freund and consistent with predictions from the theory. In a later
Baltes (2002a) have used proverbs to examine this ques- section, we will describe these studies in more detail.
tion. They presented life problems to people and asked Here, suffice it to briefly mention one series of studies.
which proverb fits this situation best. Adults preferred In our laboratories, we focused on the joined perfor-
proverbs that indicated SOC behaviors. Moreover, the mance of motor behavior (such as walking and keeping
choice reaction times of the oldest adults, when select- one’s motor balance) and various processes of memory
ing the fitting proverb, was as fast as those of younger and solving cognitive tasks. Although older adults showed
adults. Because reaction speed typically decreases with greater dual-tasks costs, they also exhibited clear prefer-
age during the age span studied, the finding suggests that ence in their task allocation. For instance, they invested a
SOC-based mental representations are well exercised. larger share of their resources into motor behavior ( likely
because falling is a high-risk in aging) and were more
Management and Mastery of Life Tasks. An- ready to de-invest from the cognitive task. Moreover, on
other area of research is the management of the family the behavioral level, older adults were effective in using
career interface (B. B. Baltes & Heydens-Gahir, 2003; compensatory skills to maintain a higher level of perfor-
Wiese, Freund, & Baltes, 2002). Partners who reported mance (K. Z. H. Li, Lindenberger, Freund, & Baltes,
higher use of SOC-related behaviors obtained higher 2001; Lindenberger, Marsiske, & Baltes, 2000).
scores on perceived developmental status in the two do- These initial self-report and observational as well as
mains and higher levels of well-being; cross-sectionally experimental studies lend support to the perspective of
and longitudinally. Similar findings were obtained with the SOC theory of adaptive development. The pattern of
the task of college study behavior (Wiese & Schmitz, findings suggests that individuals who select, optimize,
2002). Regarding tasks of old age, work by Margret and compensate are better able to generate new develop-
Baltes, Frieder Lang, and their colleagues is relevant mental resources and through effective allocation more
(e.g., Lang, Rieckmann, & Baltes, 2002). They demon- effective available resources to manage the tasks of life.
strated that older individuals, especially when in situa- Thus, SOC functions like a development-enhancing and
tions of high difficulty, benefited from showing loss-preventing general-purpose mechanism. As a general
behaviors that were consistent with SOC theory. An- theory of adaptive development, it characterizes a system
other topic of life span research concerns the manage- of strategies that permits individuals to master the gen-
ment of critical life events including illness. In this line eral tasks of life, including those that result from the
of inquiry, Gignac, Cott, and Badley (2002) have shown overall life span script outlined earlier when we outlined
that older people suffering from osteoarthritis managed a systematic change toward a greater proportion of deal-
their illness by use of behaviors that are consistent with ing with losses rather than gains.
selection, optimization, and compensation.

Dual-Task Research and Behavioral Indicators. FIRST LEVEL 5 EXAMPLE:


An additional area where SOC theory turned out to be INTELLECTUAL FUNCTIONING ACROSS
promising is dual-task research. Dual- or multiple-task re- THE LIFE SPAN
search explores the degree to which individuals can per-
form several tasks concurrently and whether concurrent In the following two sections, we focus on two broad
performance of several tasks (such as walking and memo- areas of human development—intellectual functioning
rizing) facilitates or interferes. Such multitask situations and personality—to present more specific life span
are prototypical of the ecology of everyday behavior. research and theory. In general, our approach is to
596 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

present this work such that the general theoretical per- itance in different domains, and at different levels of
spectives outlined provide an umbrella under which analysis. Specifically, the model should be consistent
this research can be positioned and interpreted. with the three-fold characterization of the life span dy-
Throughout, we attempt to highlight also the pervasive- namics between biology and culture summarized in Fig-
ness of the concept of developmental biocultural co- ure 11.1, and with the family of theoretical propositions
constructivism (P. B. Baltes et al., 2006). summarized in Table 11.3.
The productivity of a life span orientation to develop-
mental change depends critically on articulating the the- The Two-Component Model of Life Span
oretical propositions regarding the macroscopic overall Cognition: Mechanics versus Pragmatics
landscape of the entire course of ontogeny with more In the past, initiated by one of us (P. B. Baltes, 1987,
microscopic research on specific developmental func- 1993, 1997) but soon co-developed with others (e.g., P. B.
tions, processes, and age periods. Specifically, the Baltes et al., 1984; P. B. Baltes, Staudinger, & Linden-
knowledge bases generated by researchers interested in berger, 1999; S.-C. Li, 2002; Lindenberger, 2001), have
different aspects of infancy, childhood, adolescence, proposed a theoretical framework for the study of intel-
adulthood, and late life need to be combined and com- lectual development in which two main categories or com-
pared with each other, and organized by the themes and ponents of intellectual functioning are set apart: The
propositions that guide the life span approach. The re- mechanics and the pragmatics of cognition. Juxtaposing
sulting life span integration of perspectives and find- the two does not imply that they are independent or exclu-
ings, in turn, is hoped to feed back into the more age- sive; rather, they interact across ontogenetic and microge-
and process-specific developmental specialties, provid- netic time in the production of intelligent behavior. As a
ing for larger interpretative frameworks and provoking general principle, the cognitive mechanics, because of
the investigation of new or formerly neglected research their evolutionary base, evolve earlier in human ontogeny,
questions (Lindenberger, 2001). and are being “invested” into the acquisition of higher and
The field of intellectual development, that captured knowledge-based cognitive functions (for similar assump-
early (Hollingworth, 1927; Sanford, 1902) and continuing tions in the context of Gf/Gc theory, see Cattell, 1971).
attention in life span psychology (e.g., Craik & Bialystok, Historically, our views on the overall landscape
in press) is ideally suited to demonstrate the potential of human development were developed in close connec-
of this dynamic. Central themes of intellectual develop- tion with the broadening and systematization of the
ment such as relative stability (i.e., covariance change mechanic-pragmatic distinction (P. B. Baltes, 1987,
over time), directionality (i.e., mean change over time), 1997; P. B. Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 1998;
plasticity (i.e., the malleability of mean and covariance S.-C. Li, 2003). Specifically, we construe the mechanics
changes), and the role of knowledge-based processes in of cognition as an expression of the neurophysiological
cognitive development also have played a prominent role architecture of the mind as they evolved during biologi-
in life span theorizing, and are well suited to exemplify cal evolution (cf. Gigerenzer & Todd, 1999) and unfold
the dynamics between specialized research contexts and during ontogeny (McClelland, 1996; W. Singer, 1995).
overarching conceptions of life span development. In contrast, the pragmatics of cognition are associated
with the bodies of knowledge available from and medi-
The Biology and Culture of Life Span ated through culture (see upper portion of Figure 11.8).
Intellectual Development
The Cognitive Mechanics. The mechanics of cog-
Our proposed view of the overall landscape of ontogene- nition are closely linked to biological including neuro-
sis as summarized in Figure 11.1 puts constraints on the physiological brain conditions, and the predominant
possible form and content of theories about life span in- age-graded ontogenetic pattern is one of maturation,
tellectual development. Foremost, any model or theory stability, and aging-induced decline. Especially early
on life span intellectual development needs to recognize and late in ontogeny, age-based changes in this compo-
that ontogenesis is a co-construction of two intertwined nent are assumed to primarily reflect factors closely re-
streams of inheritance, the biological and the cultural lated to biological brain status, albeit in fundamentally
(Durham, 1991; see also P. B. Baltes et al., 2006; S.-C. different ways (P. B. Baltes, 1997; S.-C. Li, Linden-
Li, 2003), and needs to provide a framework for the de- berger, et al., 2004; Lindenberger, 2001). Early in on-
velopmental investigation of these two streams of inher- togeny (i.e., during embryogenesis, infancy, and early
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 597

processing operations as they can be assessed in tasks


Basic Information Processing measuring the quality of information input, sensory and
Mechanics Content-poor motor memory, discrimination, categorization, and se-
Universal, Biological
Genetically Predisposed lective attention, as well as reasoning ability in highly
Ackquired Knowledge overlearned or novel domains (Craik, 1986; Craik & Bi-
Content-rich
Pragmatics Culture-dependent alystok, in press; Craik & Salthouse, 2000; Hommel, Li,
Experience-based & Li, 2004; Salthouse & Kail, 1983). At the neuronal
level, age-graded anatomical, chemical, and functional
changes in the brain and their complex relations to the
cognitive mechanics are being uncovered with increas-
ing precision and scope (P. B. Baltes et al., in press;
Pragmatics (Crystallized) Intelligence Cabeza, Nyberg, & Park, 2004; Craik & Bialystok, in
as Cultural
Me Knowledge press; Lindenberger, Li, & Bäckman, in press).
cha
nic
Performance

s (F
lui The Cognitive Pragmatics. In contrast to the me-
d)
Intelligence chanics, the cognitive pragmatics of the mind reveal the
as Basic
Information power of human agency and culture (Boesch, 1997; Cole,
Processing 1996; Valsiner & Lawrence, 1997; S.-C. Li, 2003;
Shweder, 1991). The cognitive pragmatics also are at the
ca. 25 Life Course ca. 75 ca. 100
center of socialization events that follow the principles
of co-construction (P. B. Baltes et al., in press; S.-C. Li,
Figure 11.8 Life span research on two components of cog-
nition: (1) fluid mechanics and (2) crystallized pragmatics. 2003). Some of these events are normative but specific to
The top section defines the categories; the bottom section certain cultures (e.g., formal schooling), others are more
illustrates postulated lifespan trajectories. Source: Modified universal (e.g., mentoring), and still others are idiosyn-
based on “Psychological Aspects of Aging: Facts and cratic or person-specific (e.g., specialized ecological
Frontiers” (pp. 427–459), by P. B. Baltes and P. Graf, in The and professional knowledge). In any case, the correspon-
Life-Span Development of Individuals: Behavioural, Neurobio-
ding bodies of knowledge are represented both internally
logical and Psychosocial Perspectives, D. Magnusson (Ed.),
1996, Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; (e.g., semantic networks) and externally (e.g., books).
From “Major Abilities and Development in the Adult Period” The pragmatics of cognition direct the attention of life
(pp. 44–99), by J. L. Horn and S. M. Hofer, in Intellectual De- span developmentalists toward the increasing importance
velopment, R. J. Sternberg & C. A. Berg (Eds.), 1992, New of knowledge-based forms of intelligence during on-
York: Cambridge University Press.
togeny (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990a; Ericsson & Smith,
childhood), age-based changes in the mechanics are as- 1991; Hambrick & Engle, 2002; Krampe & Baltes, 2003;
sumed to consist, for the most part, in the unfolding and Labouvie-Vief, 1982; Rybash, Hoyer, & Roodin, 1986).
active construction of more or less domain-specific and Typical examples include reading and writing skills, edu-
genetically predisposed processing capabilities (Elman cational qualifications, professional skills, and varieties
et al., 1996; Wellman, 2003). In contrast, negative of everyday problem-solving, but also knowledge about
changes in the mechanics of cognition late in life pre- the self and the meaning and conduct of life (P. B. Baltes
sumably result from brain-related consequences of less & Staudinger, 2000; Blanchard-Fields, 1996; Bosman &
effective phylogenetic selection pressures operating Charness, 1996; Marsiske et al., 1995; Staudinger et al.,
during this period (Kirkwood, 2003; Thaler, 2002; see 1995; see “Face and Facets of the Study of Personality
“ The Mechanics and Pragmatics in Very Old Age”). In Development across the Life Span”). Such bodies of prag-
that sense, the life span trajectory of level changes in the matic knowledge are acquired during ontogeny but may
mechanics of cognition can be derived from the life span build on evolutionarily prestructured, domain-specific
changes shown in the left panel of Figure 11.1. knowledge (Charness, 2005; Elman et al., 1996;
The cognitive mechanics, then, reflect fundamental Tomasello, 1999).
organizational properties of the central nervous system
(W. Singer, 1995). In terms of psychological operations, Divergence in Life Span Trajectories between Me-
we assume that the cognitive mechanics are indexed by chanics and Pragmatics. The preceding considera-
the speed, accuracy, and coordination of elementary tions imply specific predictions regarding the shape of
598 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

ontogenetic trajectories for mechanic and pragmatic as-


pects of intellectual functioning (see lower portion
of Figure 11.8). Specifically, two different sources of in-
fluence are assumed to govern the level of performance
within these two categories: biological-genetic for the
mechanics, and environmental-cultural for the pragmat-
ics. The expected divergence in age trajectories is seen as
a consequence of this difference in composition.
Empirical evidence in support of a two-component
conceptualization of life span cognition comes from a
great variety of different research traditions (see dis-
cussion that follows). Probably the most longstanding
supportive evidence is the difference between main-
tained and vulnerable intellectual abilities (Salthouse,
1991; cf. Jones & Conrad, 1933). Abilities that critically
involve the mechanics, such as reasoning, memory, spa-
tial orientation, and perceptual speed, generally show a
pattern of monotonic and roughly linear decline during Figure 11.9 Cross-sectional age gradients in six primary
adulthood, with some further acceleration of decline in mental abilities (N = 1628). Abilities were assessed with 3 to 4
very old age. In contrast, more pragmatic abilities, such different tests and are scaled in a T-score metric (i.e., mean =
50, SD = 10). Verbal ability and number ability peak during
as verbal knowledge and certain facets of numerical
middle adulthood and show little or no age decrements before
ability, remain stable or increase up to the 6th or 7th the age of 74. In contrast, perceptual speed, inductive reason-
decade of life, and only start to evince some decline in ing, spatial orientation, and verbal memory show steady mo-
very old age. notonic decline. This differential pattern of prevailing growth,
Figure 11.9, based on the fifth data collection of the maintenance, and subsequent loss supports two-component
theories of life span intellectual development such as the dis-
Seattle Longitudinal Study (Schaie, 1996; see also
tinction between fluid and crystallized intelligence made by
Schaie et al., 2005), may serve as an illustration. It dis- Cattell (1971) and J. L. Horn (1982) or the juxtaposition of the
plays cross-sectional adult age gradients based on mul- mechanics and the pragmatics of cognition proposed by P. B.
tiple indicators for six intellectual abilities (Schaie & Baltes (1987, 1993). Source: From “Age Difference Patterns
Willis, 1993). Verbal ability and number ability peak of Psychometric Intelligence in Adulthood: Generalizability
during middle adulthood and show little or no age within and across Ability Domains,” by K. W. Schaie and S. L.
Willis, 1993, Psychology and Aging, 8, pp. 44–55.
decrements before the age of 74, whereas perceptual
speed, inductive reasoning, spatial orientation, and ver-
bal memory show steady monotonic decline. Recent marker abilities of the crystallized pragmatics (verbal
analyses based on longitudinal as well as longitudinal / knowledge and fluency) to individuals aged 6 to 89 years.
cross-sectional convergence data provide additional and Participants were classified into six age groups, child-
more direct support for a basic divergence between me- hood (6 to 11 years), adolescence (12 to 17 years), early
chanic and pragmatic age gradients in adulthood and adulthood (18 to 35 years), middle adulthood (26 to 54
old age (Salthouse, 1991; Schaie, 1996; Schaie, Mait- years), late adulthood (55 to 69 years), and old age (70 to
land, Willis, & Intieri, 1998; T. Singer, Verhaeghen, 89 years). In addition, S.-C. Li et al. (2004) also adminis-
Ghisletta, Lindenberger, & Baltes, 2003). tered basic reaction time tasks to index processing speed
In a recent cross-sectional study, Shu-Chen Li and col- (i.e., a person’s average speed of responding across the
leagues (2004) investigated whether dissociations in age five tasks) and processing robustness (i.e., the inverse of
trajectories between mechanic and pragmatic intellectual a person’s average within-task reaction-time fluctuation).
abilities across can be observed across the entire life As expected, the life span trajectories of the two infor-
span, as life span psychology would predict. The authors mation processing and the fluid-mechanic composite
administered a psychometric battery comprising fifteen stood in contrast to the trajectory of the crystallized-
tests assessing three marker abilities of the fluid mechan- mechanic composite (see Figure 11.10). Moreover,
ics (perceptual speed, reasoning, and fluency) and two within the mechanic domain, the trajectories for the two
(B) 7

(A) 70 6
65
5 6–11 Years
60 56–69 Years

Eigen Value
55 4 70–89 Years
T-Score

50
3
45
Crystallized Intelligence
40 (Culture/knowledge-based facet) 2
Processing Robustness
35
Processing Speed 1
30 Fluid Intelligence
(Biology/process-based facet)
25 0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Age Dimension

(C) 7 (D) 0,8

6 12–17 Years 0,7


Correlation between gf and gc

18–35 Years
0,6
5 36–55 Years
0,5
Eigen Value

4
0,4
3
0,3
2
0,2

1 0,1

0 0
1–

12

18

36

56

70
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
11

–1

–3

–5

–6

–8
7

9
Dimension
Age Group

Figure 11.10 Intellectual abilities across the life span. (A) Cross-sectional age trajectories for crystallized intelligence, pro-
cessing robustness, processing speed, and fluid intelligence. Crystallized intelligence represents the cognitive pragmatics,
whereas processing robustness, processing speed, and fluid intelligence represent the cognitive mechanics. The divergence in
age gradients between pragmatics and mechanics lends support to two-component theories of cognitive development. (B, C) Re-
sults from principal component analyses of 15 intellectual ability tests for each of six age groups. The arrows indicate the esti-
mated number of principal components with eigenvalues greater than unity. (D) Correlations between broad fluid and
crystallized intelligence for the same six age groups. Panels B-D support the hypothesis that the structure of intellectual abili-
ties is less differentiated in childhood and old age than during adolescence and adulthood. Source: From “ Transformations in
the Couplings among Intellectual Abilities and Constituent Cognitive Processes across the Life Span” by S.-C. Li, U. Linden-
berger, B. Hommel, G. Aschersleben, W. Prinz, and P. B. Baltes, 2004, Psychological Science, 15, pp. 155–163.

599
600 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

information-processing composites showed an earlier The Search for Determinants of


cross-sectional life span peak than the trajectory for the Mechanic Development
fluid-mechanic component, supporting the contention Despite a large overlap in approaches to the study of in-
that the admixture of pragmatic variance contaminates tellectual development, there are surprisingly few at-
standard assessments of broad fluid intelligence. tempts to pursue the themes of infant and child
development into adulthood and old age, or to identify
The Two-Component Model: Relations to
thematic and predictive antecedents of adulthood and old
Other Multiple-Component Theories
age in childhood (see S.-C. Li, Lindenberger, et al.,
Arguably, Tetens (1777) provided the earliest compre- 2004). An important exception in this regard concerns
hensive formulation of a two-component model of life work on age changes in general information-processing
span cognition (Lindenberger & Baltes, 1999); his defi- constraints on intellectual functioning across the life
nition of absolute and relative capacities closely approx- span, or what we would call research on the determinants
imated the definition of the mechanics and pragmatics of age-based changes in the mechanics of cognition. Re-
of cognition, respectively. The closest relative, both searchers both in the fields of child development (Bjork-
conceptually and historically, to the two-component lund, 1997; Case, 1992; McCall, 1994; Pascual-Leone,
model of life span intellectual development is the theory 1983) and cognitive aging (Birren, 1964; Cerella, 1990;
of fluid (Gf ) and crystallized (Gc) abilities by Cattell Craik & Byrd, 1982; Hasher & Zacks, 1988; S.-C. Li,
(1971) and Horn (1982; for comparative discussion, see Lindenberger, & Sikström, 2001; Salthouse, 1996) have
P. B. Baltes et al., 1998; Lindenberger, 2001). Other ap- been trying to identify developmental determinants or
proaches related to the two-component model include “developables” (Flavell, 1992) that regulate the rate of
Ackerman’s (e.g., 1996) process, personality, interests, age-based changes in cognitive and intellectual function-
and knowledge (PPIK) model, Hebb’s (1949) distinction ing. Some scholars have begun to link these two lines of
between intelligence A (i.e., intellectual power) and in- inquiry by attempting to provide unified accounts of age-
telligence B (i.e., intellectual products), the encapsula- based changes in the structure and/or efficiency of infor-
tion model of adult intelligence proposed by Rybash mation processing (e.g., Craik & Bialystok, 2006;
et al. (1986; Hoyer, 1987), and Sternberg’s (1985) tri- Hommel, Li, & Li, 2004; S.-C. Li, Lindenberger, et al.,
archic theory of intelligence, especially its developmen- 2004; Salthouse & Kail, 1983; Wellman, 2003).
tal interpretation by Berg and Sternberg (1985a). In many cases, the central goal of these endeavors is
Here, the two-component model will be further elabo- to identify the number (dimensionality), nature, and
rated in three separate sections: Mechanics, pragmatics, causal dynamics of age-graded changes in the mechanics
and their interrelations. The aim of these three sections of cognition. Though this task seems conceptually
is not to be comprehensive, but to further specify the two straightforward, it is methodologically quite intricate
components of cognition as well as their interaction. (P. B. Baltes & Labouvie, 1973; Hertzog, 1985; Hertzog
& Nesselroade, 2003; Lindenberger & Pötter, 1998;
Reinert, Baltes, & Schmidt, 1966). Chronological age
The Fluid Mechanics of Cognition
carries a multitude of causal agents with different and
We start this section with a life span summary of re- intertwined temporal dynamics and timescales such as
search on constructs that have been proposed to cause or distance from birth, distance from death, distance from
mediate age-based changes in the mechanics of cogni- disease inception, but also number of hours of practice
tion. We then argue that much of the available evidence or formal training. For instance, when two variables as-
about age-based changes in the mechanics derives from sumed to index two causal agents follow a similar path
measures that are contaminated by pragmatic influence, over ontogenetic time, this does not imply that the two
and we underscore the need to arrive at more valid causes are functionally related. Therefore, evidence
estimates of individual differences in upper limits of about determinants of mechanic development needs to
mechanic functioning. In line with the two-component be evaluated with caution, especially if based on age-
model, we predict that age differences in the mechanics heterogeneous cross-sectional data sets (Lindenberger
are magnified under purified measurement conditions & Pötter, 1998).
and provide an empirical example from adulthood in In the following section, we selectively review re-
support of this prediction. search on possible determinants of life span changes in
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 601

the mechanics of cognition. We start with three con- more short-term stores while simultaneously transform-
structs located at the information-processing level of ing the same or some other information (Baddeley,
analysis, and end with a consideration of select age- 2000; Just et al., 1996). Age differences in working
graded changes at the neuronal level. Progress in under- memory have been invoked as a possible cause for intel-
standing determinants of life span changes in the lectual growth during childhood (Case, 1985; Chapman
mechanics of cognition field will depend on integrating & Lindenberger, 1992; Halford, 1993; Pascual-Leone,
these two levels of analysis both empirically and con- 1970), and for age-based decrements during adulthood
ceptually (Buckner, 2004; Craik & Bialystok, in press; and old age (Craik, 1983; Oberauer & Kliegl, 2001).
S.-C. Li, in press; S.-C. Li & Lindenberger, 1999; Lin- With respect to childhood, Neo-Piagetian theorists have
denberger, Li, & Bäckman, in press). argued that changes in working memory are among the
At the information-processing level, processing rate primary pacemakers of intellectual child development
(Cerella, 1990; Salthouse, 1996), working memory (e.g., Pascual-Leone, 1970).
(Baddeley, 2000; Just, Carpenter, & Keller, 1996), and Positive age differences during childhood and nega-
inhibition (Hasher & Zacks, 1988) have been studied tive age differences during adulthood are more pro-
most extensively. Apparently, functional levels of these nounced when demands on processing are increased
three mechanisms follow the inverse U-shape pattern (Mayr, Kliegl, & Krampe, 1996). Despite this support-
predicted by the two-component model for the mechan- ive evidence, the explanatory power of the working-
ics of cognition. In principle, then, any combination of memory construct is difficult to judge. For instance,
these mechanisms could act as a pacemaker of life span age-based changes in working memory are often ex-
development in the mechanics of cognition. plained by alluding to changes in processing efficiency
or processing speed (Case, 1985; Salthouse, 1996). An-
Processing Speed. Across a wide variety of cogni- other problem concerns our limited knowledge about a
tive and perceptual tasks, speed of responding increases central function of working memory—the (conscious)
dramatically from childhood to early adulthood, and control of action and thought. In the most influential
continuously decreases thereafter. This observation has working-memory model (Baddeley, 2000), this task is
led to the processing rate hypothesis of life span cogni- assigned to the central executive. Evidence from devel-
tive development. Probably, this hypothesis holds a more opmental psychology (Houdé, 1995; McCall, 1994),
central place in cognitive aging research (e.g., Birren, cognitive-experimental and differential psychology
1964; Cerella, 1990; Salthouse, 1996; Welford, 1984) (Engle, Kane, & Tuholski, 1999), and the cognitive neu-
than in research on child development (e.g., Hale, 1990; rosciences (Miller & Cohen, 2001) suggests that the
Kail, 1996). In the case of cognitive aging, the general abilities to inhibit actions and thoughts and avoid inter-
slowing-down of cognitive behavior with advancing age ference from competing processing streams are crucial
is portrayed as the consequence of a general decrement for the efficient functioning of this component, rather
in information processing rate. In cross-sectional stud- than working-memory capacity per se.
ies, psychometrically assessed perceptual speed ac-
counts for most or all negative adult age differences in Inhibition and Interference. During the past
other intellectual abilities, even if these other abilities decades, developmentalists from different traditions and
are assessed under time-relaxed or untimed testing con- fields of research have intensified their interest in mech-
ditions (for a summary, see Verhaeghen & Salthouse, anisms of inhibition and interference (Bjorklund, 1997;
1997). However, psychometrically assessed perceptual Engle, Conway, Tuholski, & Shishler, 1995; Hasher &
speed is not a unitary construct or processing primitive Zacks, 1988; Houdé, 1995; McCall, 1994). Curvilinear
but a factorially complex entity whose composition may life span age gradients that resemble those found for
change as a function of age. Also, attempts at identify- measures of perceptual speed have been obtained with
ing neuronal correlates of age-based differences in pro- typical tests of interference proneness such as the
cessing speed have yielded mixed results (e.g., Bashore, Stroop color-word test, suggesting that children and es-
Ridderinkhof, & van der Molen, 1997). pecially older adults have greater difficulties in sup-
pressing currently irrelevant action tendencies than
Working Memory. Generally, working memory young adults (Dempster, 1992; Hommel et al., 2004;
denotes the ability to preserve information in one or Mayr, 2001). However, it has proven difficult to separate
602 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

inhibition-based explanations of this phenomenon from age-associated changes in the functional organization of
activation-based explanations of selective attention and prefrontal cortex such as a reduction in the asymmetry of
working memory capacity (Engle et al., 1995; Hommel hemispheric activation (e.g., Cabeza, 2002).
et al., 2004). The links between behavioral development and re-
gional brain differentiation are only beginning to emerge
Cognitive Neuroscience Approaches to Mechanic (e.g., Lindenberger et al., in press), and the precise rela-
Development: The Sample Case of Prefrontal Cir- tions between life span changes in prefrontal circuitry
cuitry. The advent of brain imaging methods has al- and behavioral changes remain to be uncovered. Func-
lowed researchers to intensify empirical links between tions similar to working memory and typically subsumed
behavioral and neuronal levels of analysis. The concep- under the heading of “executive functions” or “cognitive
tual and empirical implications of this trend for devel- control” appear to be involved (Engle et al., 1999; Kliegl,
opmental psychology are discussed more fully elsewhere Krampe, & Mayr, 2003). Situations deemed to be partic-
(e.g., P. B. Baltes et al., in press; Cabeza et al., 2004; ularly dependent on prefrontal circuitry require the co-
Craik & Bialystok, in press; S.-C. Li, 2002; Linden- ordination of multiple tasks or task components. Typical
berger et al., in press). In the following discussion, we examples include the suppression of stimulus-driven ac-
restrict our presentation to maturational and senescent tion tendencies (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999; Salthouse &
changes in prefrontal circuitry. Available evidence sug- Meinz, 1995), multitasking (Mayr et al., 1996; Salt-
gests that these changes may contribute in important house, Hambrick, Lukas, & Dell, 1996) and response se-
ways to changes in the cognitive mechanics during child- lection under high stimulus ambiguity (Kramer, Hahn,
hood and old age. & Gopher, 1999; Kray & Lindenberger, 2000). Differen-
We begin with some evidence on regional brain devel- tial susceptibility to coordinative demands may help to
opment. In early ontogeny, prefrontal cortex and associ- explain why life span age differences in marker tests of
ated neural networks undergo profound anatomical, fluid intelligence such as Raven’s matrices tend to per-
chemical, and functional changes that extend well into sist when participants are given unlimited amounts of
adolescence. Neural plasticity during corticogenesis time to solve the items (cf. the simultaneity mechanism
entails the production and experience-dependent elimi- in Salthouse, 1996).
nation of neuronal connections (Huttenlocher & Dab- Future research needs to explicate the link between
holkar, 1997). During brain development, the zone of life span changes in prefrontal circuitry and the mechan-
maximum plasticity moves from primary sensory and ics of cognition with greater precision. Given the funda-
motor over secondary association to prefrontal areas mentally different etiology of changes in prefrontal
(Chugani, Phelps, & Mazziotta, 1987). Computational circuitry early and late in ontogeny, and given that late-
models suggest that later maturing areas require input life changes are taking place in a cognitive system with a
from earlier maturing areas to represent higher-order rich and idiosyncratic learning history, any expectation
concepts (Shrager & Johnson, 1996). Arguably, the of a close resemblance between brain-behavior mappings
gradual and orderly progression of the corticotrophic early and late in life seems unwarranted.
wave provides a chronotopic constraint for cerebral cor-
Age-Based Differences in the Mechanics
tex organization.
of Cognition: The Need for Purification
In later adulthood, prefrontal cortex and the function-
of Measurement
ally connected basal ganglia also show greater and earlier
signs of decline than most other areas of the brain. In Observed age differences or age changes on intellectual
a comprehensive review of the neuroanatomical litera- tasks and tests, as obtained in standard cross-sectional
ture, Raz (2000) reported average linear reductions and real-time longitudinal research, cannot be regarded as
in brain weight and volume of about 2% per decade direct and pure reflections of age-based changes in the
during adulthood, which were more pronounced for ante- mechanics of cognition. Rather, in addition to the me-
rior parts of the brain (for longitudinal evidence, see Raz, chanics, such differences or changes are influenced by a
Lindenberger, et al., 2005). At the neurochemical level, wealth of additional factors, ranging from pragmatic com-
changes in the catecholaminergic system, most notably ponents of cognition (e.g., task-relevant preexperimental
dopamine, play a prominent role (Bäckman & Farde, knowledge) to other person characteristics (e.g., test anx-
2004). Finally, neurofunctional studies point to profound iety or achievement motivation; cf. Fisk & Warr, 1996). A
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 603

likely indication for this admixture of pragmatic variance difficulty. Furthermore, and in line with the microge-
to supposedly mechanic measures is the secular rise in netic approach to the study of change (Siegler & Crow-
performance on typical psychometric marker tests of ley, 1991; Siegler, Chapter 11, this Handbook, this
fluid intelligence (cf. Flynn, 1987; Schaie et al., 2005). In volume), the testing-the-limits paradigm is based on the
our view, it seems an open question whether the preferred assumption that the study of microgenetic change and
interpretation by Flynn that these historical changes re- variability may help to identify mechanisms underlying
flect changes in fluid intelligence per se is correct. Unless ontogenetic change (see H. Werner, 1948). Thus, in
more pure measures of basic fluid intelligence were in- addition to the more general goal of measurement pu-
cluded, we hold it more likely that these historical rification, the detailed analysis of time-compressed de-
changes are changes in the pragmatics rather than the me- velopmental change functions is assumed to enhance our
chanics (see also Schaie et al., 2005). understanding of the mechanisms and the range of
The need for better estimates of individuals’ perfor- medium- and long-term developmental changes (Hultsch
mance potential in the mechanics of cognition is further & MacDonald, 2004; S.-C. Li, Huxhold, et al., 2004;
nurtured by the life span proposition that epigenesis is Lindenberger & von Oertzen, in press).
probabilistic but not random; hence, plasticity is more
or less constrained (P. B. Baltes, 1987; Gottlieb, 1998; A Prototypical Example: Adult Age Differences in
Hagen & Hammerstein, 2005; R. M. Lerner, 1984; see Upper Limits of Short-Term Memory (Serial
Table 11.3). If the goal is to separate the possible from Word Recall)
the impossible over age, and to solidify the evidence on Figure 11.11 shows the result of a study involving a total
age differences in the mechanics of cognition, the con- of 38 sessions of training and practice in the Method of
text of measurement needs to be moved toward upper Loci, a mnemonic technique for the serial recall of word
limits of performance potential. This line of reasoning lists. Two findings from this study are noteworthy. First,
resembles claims made by other research traditions, adults in both age groups greatly improved their mem-
such as clinical and developmental diagnostics (Carlson, ory performance. This finding confirms earlier work on
1994; Guthke & Wiedl, 1996), the differentiation be- the continued existence of cognitive plasticity in cogni-
tween performance and competence, gestalt and cultural- tively healthy (i.e., nondemented) older adults (P. B.
historical theoretical orientations (Vygotsky, 1962;
H. Werner, 1948), and early work on life span differ-
Correct Responses (Maximum = 30)

ences in learning (B. Levinson & Reese, 1967). Discrep- 30


ancies in epistemology and purpose notwithstanding, all
25
these traditions are inspired by an interest in exploring
20
individuals’ upper limits of intellectual performance.
15

Testing the Limits of Age Differences in the 10


Mechanics of Cognition. Within life span develop- 5
mental psychology and as alluded to earlier, the testing- 0
the-limits paradigm has been introduced as a research 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 Final Distribution
of Subjects
strategy to uncover age differences in the upper limits of Session:
mechanic functioning across the life span (P. B. Baltes, Instruction
Practice
1987; Kliegl & Baltes, 1987; Lindenberger & Baltes,
1995b). The main focus of this paradigm is to arrange Figure 11.11 Testing-the-limits research, which is aimed
for experimental conditions that produce maximum at the identification of asymptotes of performance
(i.e., asymptotic) levels of performance. Thus, similar to potential, suggests the existence of robust age-related losses in
stress tests in biology and medicine (M. M. Baltes, the mechanics of cognition. The example given involves a
Kühl, Gutzmann, & Sowarka, 1995; Fries & Crapo, memory technique, the Method of Loci. After 38 sessions
of training, most older adults did not reach the level of
1981), testing-the-limits aims at the assessment of age
performance reached by younger adults after only a few ses-
differences in maximum levels of cognitive perfor- sions. In the final distribution, no older person was performing
mance by providing large amounts of practice and/or above the mean of the young adults. Adapted from P. B. Baltes
training combined with systematic variations in task & Kliegl, 1992.
604 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Baltes & Lindenberger, 1988; P. B. Baltes & Willis, & Lindenberger, 1988). For instance, it is not easy to
1982; Verhaeghen, Marcoen, & Goossens, 1992). Sec- argue against alternative interpretations, such as that the
ond, practice and training resulted in a close-to-perfect improvement in the cognitive system is due to adding
separation of the two adult age groups, thereby demon- pragmatic rather than mechanical components to the so-
strating the existence of sizeable negative age differ- lution process. We would need more purist measures of
ences at limits of functioning. Even after 38 sessions of the cognitive mechanics than are currently available.
training, the majority of older adults did not reach the Moreover, issues of transfer and maintenance are at
level of performance that young adults had reached after stake. If the results were an improvement in the mechan-
only a few sessions. Moreover, at the end of the study, ics themselves, the evidence should include the demon-
not a single older person functioned above the mean of stration of improvement in learning gains across a wide
the young-adult group. A more recent investigation range of new tasks, or at least within the “modularity”
has shown that upper limits of performance are further class within which the training tasks are putatively lo-
reduced in very old age (T. Singer, Lindenberger, & cated. The absence of such evidence my be due to the
Baltes, 2003). fact that many training programs are behavioral in ori-
The findings obtained with the testing-the-limits para- gin. It will be interesting to see whether biochemical in-
digm are consistent with our general notion that the me- terventions, such as memory pharmaceuticals to
chanics of cognition decrease during adulthood and old improve the transmission from primary to secondary
age. Given our assumptions regarding life span changes in memory might be more powerful in improving the cogni-
adaptive capacity of the mechanics of the mind and the tive mechanics in a more direct manner. Here, the newly
knowledge-contaminated nature of standard assessments, evolving cooperation between biochemically oriented
we predict that life span peaks in performance levels are neuroscientists and behaviorally-oriented learning psy-
shifted toward younger ages when individuals are given chologists may offer a new window on the plasticity of
the opportunity to come close to their upper limits of me- the cognitive mechanics during adulthood as well (see
chanic potential. Results from a recent experimental also Goldberg & Weinberger, 2004; Kempermann,
study have confirmed these expectations (Brehmer, Li, 2006).
Müller, Oertzen, & Lindenberger, 2005).
In addition to encompassing wide age ranges, future The Crystallized Pragmatics of Cognition
explorations of life span differences in behavioral plas-
ticity may also include functional and anatomical neural We now direct our attention to the ontogeny of the cog-
measures to identify life span differences in the relation nitive pragmatics, or the cultural and knowledge-rich
between behavioral and neuronal plasticity (for exem- dimension of intellectual life span development. First,
plary work, see Kramer et al., in press; Nyberg et al., we discuss the relation between mechanics and prag-
2003). Also, the focus on age differences in maximum matics from an evolutionary perspective. Then, we
level should be complemented by an emphasis on age- introduce the distinction between normative and person-
differential changes in variances and covariances with specific forms of pragmatic knowledge, and discuss
practice (Hertzog, Cooper, & Fisk, 1996; Labouvie, stage- and knowledge-oriented approaches. We end this
Frohring, Baltes, & Goulet, 1973). Specifically, to bet- section with examples from our own research on expert
ter understand neuronal correlates of age differences in knowledge about the fundamental pragmatics of life
the acquisition of expertise, it seems productive to study (wisdom).
life span differences in the brain’s adaptation in re-
Mechanics and Pragmatics in
sponse to intensive training programs.
Evolutionary Perspective
At present the major conclusion regarding the plastic-
ity of the mechanics across the lifespan is that plasticity In recent decades, nativist approaches to infant cogni-
may be substantial in childhood, that it decreases tive development have revealed the evolutionary in-
markedly with age, and that it’s demonstrated plasticity formed nature of the human processing system (e.g.,
after middle adulthood is modest at best. This conclu- Spelke, Vishton, & von Hofsten, 1995). Through innova-
sion holds especially, if one defines a high threshold for tive advances in experimental methodology, it has be-
assessing whether a given training program resulted in a come increasingly clear that infants and young children
true improvement of the mechanics themselves (Baltes cannot be considered a cognitive tabula rasa, as extreme
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 605

interpretations of constructivist (e.g., Piaget, quencies; rather, such formalisms are cultural products
1967/1971, but see Piaget, 1980, pp. 11–12) or behavior- whose acquisition requires specialized instruction. An-
ist (e.g., B. F. Skinner, 1966) theorizing may suggest. other example from a completely different field is the
Rather, not unlike members of other species, humans need to culturally countershape the manifestation of
begin their extra-uterine lives with a well- evolution-based aggressive and interpersonal power ten-
orchestrated set of domain-specific constraints and ex- dencies.
pectations that guide behavior and form the basis for
later acquisitions (Elman et al., 1996; Saffran, Aslin, & Normative versus Person-Specific
Newport, 1996). Pragmatic Knowledge
We assume that the pragmatics of cognition, or the An important, albeit necessarily imperfect, distinction
bodies of knowledge provided by culture build on, ex- within the pragmatics of cognition concerns normative
tend, and reorganize these prestructured core domains, versus person-specific knowledge. Normative bodies of
both during evolution and during ontogeny (Gigerenzer, knowledge are of general value to a given culture. Typi-
2003; Wellman, 2003). These processes of extension and cal examples include verbal ability, number proficiency,
transformation eventually give rise to forms of knowl- and basic general knowledge about the world (e.g.,
edge and behavior that are, in part by virtue of necessity, Ackerman, Beier, & Bowen, 2000). Individual differ-
compatible with the biological architecture of the mind, ences in these domains are closely linked to years of ed-
but cannot be characterized as the direct consequence of ucation and other aspects of social stratification, and
evolutionary selection pressures. are amenable to psychometric testing (Cattell, 1971). In
The resulting potential of human ontogeny to create contrast, person-specific bodies of knowledge that
and adapt to the new (Gottlieb, 1998), or the productive branch off from the normative knowledge-acquisition
tension between current functions and evolutionary his- path are less closely tied to mandatory socialization
tory, has been referred to as exaptative generalization or events, and result from specific combinations of experi-
exaptation (Gould & Vrba, 1982). As a mechanism of ential settings, personality characteristics, motivational
biocultural co-construction, exaptation helps to explain constellations, and cognitive abilities or talent (Mar-
why members of the human species are good at doing siske et al., 1995). As a consequence, these bodies of
things that were certainly not directly at the focus of knowledge often escape psychometric operationaliza-
natural selection, such as reading a book or driving a car tion, and are more amenable to study within the expert-
(Sherry & Schacter, 1987). Put more generally, exapta- ise paradigm (Ericsson & Smith, 1991; Gobet et al.,
tion reminds us that the evolution of culture must reflect 2001; Krampe & Baltes, 2003). Therefore, psychometric
some degree of match with, and reciprocal influence on, research on crystallized abilities needs to be supple-
evolution-based genetic disposition (Durham, 1991; mented by approaches with a more explicit focus on
Gottlieb et al., Chapter 5, this Handbook, this volume). knowledge acquisition and utilization to more fully cap-
For instance, pragmatic knowledge may evolve from ture the diversity and specificity of pragmatic knowl-
and/or mimic predisposed knowledge in evolutionarily edge.
privileged domains but come with the advantage of being For the most part ( but see Brown, 1982; Chi &
tuned to the idiosyncratic demands of specific cultures, Koeske, 1983; Schneider & Bjorklund, 2003; Weinert &
biographies, and contexts (Siegler & Crowley, 1994). Perner, 1996; Wilkening & Anderson, 1990), develop-
Note, however, that culture sometimes appears to mental research on person-specific bodies of knowledge
have produced bodies of knowledge that are antithetical, has been undertaken with adults. A typical approach has
disconnected, or at least not easily articulated to biolog- been to identify the effects of domain-specific knowl-
ical predispositions. For instance, Gigerenzer and Todd edge by comparing the performance of experts and
(1999) have argued that formal-logical expressions such novices both inside and outside their domain of expert-
as Bayes’ theorem do not take advantage of humans’ pre- ise. Examples include the classical domains of expertise
disposition to base judgments about feature conjunction research such as chess (Charness, 1981) and card games
probabilities on frequency counts. Put differently, math- (Bosman & Charness, 1996), but also domains such
ematical formalisms about conditional probabilities do as baseball knowledge (Hambrick & Engle, 2002) or
not build on mechanisms of perception and action that professional expertise (e.g., Salthouse, 2003; for an
directly support the detection of conjunctive feature fre- overview, see Charness, 2005).
606 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Two main conclusions can be drawn from this re- interactive-minds cognition (P. B. Baltes & Staudinger,
search. First, expertise effects, or the consequences of 1996b; Dixon & Gould, 1996; T. Singer et al., 2004;
specific bodies of declarative and procedural knowl- Staudinger, 1996; Staudinger & Baltes, 1996).
edge, rarely transcend the boundaries of the target do-
Intellectual Growth during Adulthood: Stage
main. Specifically, there is little evidence to suggest
Conceptions versus Functionalist Approaches
that the mechanics of cognition are transformed by
domain-specific knowledge (Salthouse, 2003). When- Historically, much of the search for more advanced
ever there is evidence for effects of a more general kind, forms of reasoning and thought in adulthood originated
at least after the age periods of childhood and adoles- from Piaget’s theory of cognitive development (Chap-
cence, transfer of pragmatic knowledge (positive or man, 1988b; Pascual-Leone, 1983; Piaget, 1970; Riegel,
negative) appears to be a more plausible explanation 1976), positing the emergence of one or more postfor-
than a basic change in the mechanics. One example mal or dialectical stages of cognitive development after
comes from longitudinal work by Kohn and Schooler the advent of formal operations. The conceptual de-
(1983; Schooler, Mulatu, & Oates, 1999) on the rela- scription of these stages often connects personality de-
tionship between the substantive complexity of work velopment (e.g., generativity in the Eriksonian sense)
and ideational flexibility. Kohn and Schooler found that with logical considerations (e.g., awareness and accep-
work complexity predicts increments in ideational flexi- tance of contradiction). As a consequence of this partic-
bility over a period of 10 years, even after controlling ular linkage, the emergence of such stages is assumed to
for initial differences in ideational flexibility. A related bring about increments in reflexivity and general aware-
finding is the recent observation that social participa- ness for the human condition (see the next section). Evi-
tion attenuates decline in the cognitive mechanics in old dence in support of such stages is scarce, which is not
and very old age (Lövdén, Ghisletta, & Lindenberger, surprising given the difficulties in obtaining reliable in-
2005). Note, however, that the interpretation of findings dicators of stage-like cognitive change (e.g., Molenaar,
of this type in terms of experiential factors is compli- 1986; L. B. Smith & Thelen, 2003).
cated through nonrandom placement of individuals into Despite his constructivist and dialectical epistemol-
experiential settings and the fact that the measures of ogy (e.g., Chapman, 1988; Lourenço & Machado, 1996;
the cognitive mechanics used include crystallized prag- Piaget, 1980), Piaget himself was reluctant to posit any
matic components (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). stages beyond formal operations. Instead, he argued on
The second major conclusion concerns the power of one occasion (Piaget, 1972) that the notion of horizon-
pragmatic knowledge to make up for losses in the mechan- tal décalage gives sufficient room to adult intellectual
ics within the domain of expertise (Charness, 2005; growth and variability within his theory. Specifically,
Krampe & Baltes, 2003). Here, the results from several he expected that late adolescents and adults would ex-
studies suggest that acquired knowledge endows aging in- hibit formal-operational reasoning within their areas of
dividuals with a form of natural and local (e.g., domain- expertise but not necessarily across all possible domains
bound) ability to withstand or at least attenuate the of knowledge. This view seems consistent with the two-
consequences of aging-induced losses in the mechanics. component model of fluid-crystallized or mechanic-
This finding is of central importance for the issue of suc- pragmatic intelligence in that the potential for adult
cessful intellectual aging, and supports the general life intellectual growth is linked to factors operating within
span theory of selective optimization with compensation rather than across domains (Flavell, 1970; Krampe &
(P. B. Baltes, 1993; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Staudinger Baltes, 2003).
et al., 1995). The postulate of a compensatory relation be- Nevertheless, the quest for identifying structural
tween pragmatic knowledge acquisition and mechanic de- transformations in the organization of thought and ac-
cline receives additional support by attenuated adult age tion in the course of life span development continues to
differences in knowledge-rich domains of everyday rele- be of great theoretical appeal (L. B. Smith & Thelen,
vance. For instance, compared to standard psychometric 2003). To ease the detection of such transformations, if
or cognitive-experimental assessments, negative adult age they exist, it seems advisable to increase the density of
differences tend to be less pronounced or absent in practi- observations within persons, and to use data-analytic
cal problem solving (Sternberg, Wagner, Williams, & tools as well theoretical approaches that highlight rather
Hovath, 1995), social intelligence (Blanchard-Fields, than cover the structural dynamics one seeks to identify
1996), memory in context (Hess & Pullen, 1996), and (e.g., Lindenberger & von Oertzen, in press; Molenaar,
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 607

Huizenga, & Nesselroade, 2003; C. S. Nesselroade & In the Berlin work on wisdom (e.g., P. B. Baltes &
Schmidt McCollam, 2000). Empirically, the emergence Kunzmann, 2004; P. B. Baltes & Smith, 1990; P. B.
of automaticity during skill acquisition provides per- Baltes & Staudinger, 2000), we treat wisdom as the high-
haps the best evidence for structural change (e.g., Ack- est form of knowledge and judgment about human excel-
erman & Cianciolo, 2000), albeit of a different kind lence involving the meaning and conduct of life.
than envisioned by structuralist life span theoreticians. Specifically, we define wisdom as “an expertise in the
fundamental pragmatics of life permitting exceptional in-
Expanding the Concept of Cognitive Pragmatics: sight and judgment involving complex and uncertain mat-
Wisdom as Expertise in the Fundamental ters of the human condition including its developmental
Pragmatics of Life and contextual variability, plasticity, and limitations.”
Individual differences in intellectual functioning also Operationally, this definition corresponds to a family of
reflect and influence individual differences in person- five criteria, factual knowledge, procedural knowledge,
ality and motivation. In the child development litera- contextualism, value relativism, and uncertainty. Clearly,
ture, a good example is school achievement, which is advances along these dimensions require the joint opera-
studied in relation to ability, effort, and other person- tion of cognitive, motivational, and emotional factors.
ality characteristics. In life span psychology, such a Thus far, our main methodological strategy in inves-
view becomes conspicuous when attempting to under- tigating wisdom as an expertise in the fundamental prag-
stand expert levels of intellectual performance, for matics of life has been to ask persons to think aloud
instance, by means of models of expertise (Ericsson & about difficult life problems such as, “Imagine a 14-
Smith, 1991). Similarly, investment theories of intelli- year-old girl who wants to leave home and get married,
gence emphasize that cognition pervades cognitive, what should one think about this?” The think-aloud re-
motivational, and emotional aspects of behavior sponses to such or similar life problems are then evalu-
(Krampe & Baltes, 2003). ated on the five wisdom-related criteria by a trained
To illustrate the point of viewing intelligence in a rater panel. Figure 11.12 displays the results of one of
larger context of human functioning, we use research these studies (P. B. Baltes, Staudinger, Maercker, &
on wisdom (see also P. B. Baltes & Kunzmann, 2004; Smith, 1995). In the figure, an overall wisdom score
Kunzmann & Baltes, 2003a). Wisdom is close to con-
ceptions of intelligence broadly conceived, as it de-
notes a high level of performance in the domain of
Wisdom-Related Score (Maximum = 7)

5
practical and social intelligence. At the same time, wis-
dom also is a personality characteristic since its acqui-
4
sition and expression depends on values and Top 20%
motivation. For instance, it is part of wisdom-related 3
knowledge to understand that wisdom is oriented si- Wisdom
multaneously toward the well-being of oneself and that 2 Nominees

of others. This commitment to the common good high- Old Clinical


1 Psychologists
lights the constituent role of personality and motiva-
Control
tion in wisdom-related thought and behavior. Hence, Group
0
we see wisdom as an ideal combination of mind and 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90
virtue (P. B. Baltes & Kunzmann, 2004; P. B. Baltes & Participant Age
Smith, 1990; P. B. Baltes & Staudinger, 2000). Cogni-
Figure 11.12 Wisdom-related performance of four different
tive, motivational, and emotional attributes need to groups of individuals averaged across two wisdom-related tasks
converge to produce wisdom as the highest form of and five evaluative criteria (factual knowledge, procedural
human excellence in mind and character. Thus, strictly knowledge, contextualism, relativism, and uncertainty). There
speaking, intelligence is only a part of wisdom, unless were no age differences in the age range from 25 to 80 years. In
one was to expand the concept of intelligence produc- addition, wisdom nominees and clinical psychologists provided
significantly more high-level (top 20%) performances than the
tion to cover personality as well (for a further discus-
old control group. Max. = maximum. Source: From “People
sion of these issues, see Ardelt, 2004; Aspinwall & Nominated as Wise: A Comparative Study of Wisdom-Related
Staudinger, 2003; P. B. Baltes & Kunzmann, 2004; Knowledge,” by P. B. Baltes, U. M. Staudinger, A. Maercker,
Krampe & Baltes, 2003; Sternberg, 2004). and J. Smith, 1995, Psychology and Aging, 10, pp. 155–166.
608 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

based on all five criteria is plotted against age for four General Person Factors
different groups: Wisdom nominees (i.e., distinguished e.g., Cognitive Mechanics
individuals nominated as being wise in a two-step Del- Mental Health
Cognitive Style Creativity
phi technique), experienced clinical psychologists, and Openness to Experience Organizing Processes
two control groups involving adults with comparable ad- Life Planning
vanced levels of education (young and old). Life Management
Expertise-specific Factors Life Review
Two findings are noteworthy. First, there was no in- e.g., Experience in Life Matters
dication of a negative age trend in wisdom-related per- Practice in Dealing with
Life Problems
formance when comparing adults of about 25 to 75 Organized Tutelage Wisdom-related
years of age. This finding has been replicated in five Mentorship Performance
Motivational Disposition
other studies (Staudinger, 1999a). Second, older per- (e.g., Strive for Basic Criteria
excellence, Factual Knowledge
sons with wisdom-facilitative experiences (e.g., older generativity) Procedural Knowledge
clinical psychologists and wisdom nominees) con-
tributed a disproportionately large share to the top re- Meta Criteria
Facilitative Experiental Contexts Lifespan Contextualism
sponses (see also J. Smith, Staudinger, & Baltes, 1994; Value Relativism
e.g., Age Uncertainty
Staudinger, Smith, & Baltes, 1992). Both findings stand Education
in clear contrast to the negative age gradients observed Providing Mentorship
Profession
for the cognitive mechanics (see Figure 11.10, both Historical Period
panels), thereby providing further support for the two-
component model. Figure 11.13 A research framework describing antecedent
The findings also underscore that living long (age) in factors and mediating processes for the acquisition and main-
tenance of wisdom-related knowledge and skills across the
itself is not a sufficient condition for the development of
life span. The likelihood of attaining expert levels of perfor-
wisdom (or for any other form of expertise). Rather, as mance in this prototypical domain of the cognitive pragmatics
suggested by our working model of wisdom ontogeny is assumed to depend on an effective coalition of experiential,
(see Figure 11.13), it appears that favorable macro- expertise-specific, and general person-related factors (modi-
structural contexts (e.g., historical period), expertise- fied after Baltes & Smith, 1990; Baltes & Staudinger, 2000).
Adapted from “ The Psychology of Wisdom and Its Ontogene-
specific factors (e.g., experience and training in the
sis” (pp. 87–120), by P. B. Baltes and J. Smith, 1990, in Wis-
fundamental pragmatics of life, strive for excellence, dom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development, R. J. Sternberg
mentorship), and general person factors (e.g., fluid me- (Ed.), New York: Cambridge University Press; and “Wisdom:
chanics, cognitive style, openness to experience) need to A Metaheuristic to Orchestrate Mind and Virtue Towards Ex-
work in coalition to move people toward wisdom (e.g., cellence,” by P. B. Baltes and U. M. Staudinger, 2000, Ameri-
Staudinger, 1999b). Some of these wisdom-facilitative can Psychologist, 55, pp. 122–136.
factors, such as generativity, are age-associated; how-
ever, there are also wisdom-debilitating influences, such Maciel, Smith, & Baltes, 1998). In contrast, intelligence
as rigidity and decrease in the cognitive mechanics, that is a more salient predictor in adolescence, when intellec-
might come with age. On average, the net result of age- tual prerequisites for wisdom-related characteristics
related facilitators and debilitators seems to equal out. such as the abilities to self-reflect and decenter are un-
Only under favorable conditions, facilitators outweigh dergoing rapid developmental advances (Pasupathi,
debilitators and permit increase of wisdom-related per- Staudinger, & Baltes, 2001). To embed wisdom into a
formance with age. more holistic context involving personality and the self,
The theoretical framework of our work on wisdom, we also examined the correlation between wisdom-
and its close connection with dimensions of personality related knowledge and virtue-related outcomes such as
and emotionality, has been supported by a variety of prosocial values and interpersonal conflict-resolution
findings (P. B. Baltes & Kunzmann, 2004; P. B. Baltes styles (Kunzmann & Baltes, 2003b; see also Sternberg,
& Staudinger, 2000; Kunzmann & Baltes, 2003b). For 1998). People high on wisdom-related knowledge exhib-
instance, in adulthood, personality and cognitive style ited a more complex and modulated structure of emo-
measures are more important predictors of wisdom- tions and preferred conflict resolution strategies that are
related performance than traditional measures of intel- based on dialogue rather than power. Of special interest
ligence (Staudinger, Lopez, & Baltes, 1997; Staudinger, is that high wisdom-related knowledge correlates nega-
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 609

tively with the search for personal enjoyment and mate- in line with the view of biocultural co-construction
rial happiness. (P. B. Baltes et al., 2006; S.-C. Li, 2003) mentioned ear-
In addition to illustrating how the pragmatics of cogni- lier. We then argue, with respect to the overall land-
tion are intertwined with other sectors of human develop- scape of life span development, or the ontogenetic
ment, our research on wisdom also illustrates how culture dynamics of gains and losses, that the mechanic-prag-
and culture-based activities shape development during matic interdependence converges on the notion of a com-
adulthood. During normal adulthood, the biology of the pensatory relation between mechanic ef ficiency and
body and brain is sufficiently developed and ready for in- pragmatic knowledge. As SOC theory suggests, this
vestment. It is culture-based learning and development compensatory relation is reciprocal and part of the en-
that defines the agenda (see also P. B. Baltes, Freund, & tire life course. However, we submit that the role of
Li, in press; Lachman, 2001). In this sense, work on compensation increases in importance and culminates
wisdom serves to highlight the relative independence of in old age.
the pragmatics of cognition vis à vis the biology-based
mechanics. Within the normal range of adult mechanic The Mechanic-Pragmatic Interdependence:
functioning, the mechanics’ contribution to individual Evidence at the Cortical Level
differences on wisdom-related tasks is small, both in An early neurocognitive demonstration for the interde-
absolute terms and relative to other factors such as per- pendence between mechanic and pragmatic develop-
sonality and task-relevant life experience. The most im- ment concerns the increased cortical representation of
portant contributors to wisdom-related performance the left hand in players of string instruments (Elbert,
during the adult life span tend to be personality character- Pantev, Wienbruch, Rockstroh, & Taub, 1995; for other
istics as measured by the Neuroticism Extraversion Open- examples, see Draganski et al., 2004; Petersson & Reis,
ness Questionnaire (NEO) as well as wisdom-relevant in press). Compared to normal individuals, areas of the
professional training and the nature of lifetime experi- somatosensory cortex representing the fingers of the
ence, rather than psychometrically assessed intelligence left hand occupy more space in string players. Most
or chronological age. In very old age, however, the me- likely, this increase in cortical representation has been
chanics of cognition again appear to delimit wisdom- induced by large amounts of goal-directed and deliber-
related performance if they fall below a critical threshold ate practice (cf. Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Römer,
of functional integrity (P. B. Baltes et al., 1995). 1993). In line with the notion of a bio-culturally co-
constructed brain (P. B. Baltes et al., in press; S.-C. Li
& Lindenberger, 2002), this research finding illustrates
Varieties of Mechanic/ Pragmatic
the potential of individuals to acquire and represent
Interdependence
pragmatic knowledge.
As has become clear by now, the mechanics and prag- Elbert et al. (1995) also provided evidence in support
matics of life span intellectual development are inter- of age-graded differences in cortical plasticity. Specifi-
twined in many ways and at various levels of analysis cally, the brain’s physiological aptness to provide more
(cf. Charness, in press; Salthouse, 2003), both among cortical space for the fingers of the left hand was found
each other and with other aspects of behavior. Phyloge- to depend on the chronological age at inception of musi-
netically, they are connected in the sense that members cal practice. As this example illustrates, the ability to
of the human species are biologically predisposed to ac- acquire pragmatic knowledge (e.g., the potential for de-
quire cultural knowledge (e.g., Plessner, 1965; Wellman, velopmental change in the pragmatic component) is con-
2003). Ontogenetically, the interdependence also runs ditioned by the age-graded status of the mechanics
both ways. For instance, the potential to acquire and use (Güntürkün, in press; Kempermann, in press).
pragmatic knowledge is conditioned by the development
The Age of Peak Performance in Complex Skills
of the mechanics. At the same time, mechanics alone are
of little use for problem solving in highly specialized do- The mechanics of cognition not only condition the ac-
mains of knowledge; in many cases, domain-specific quisition but also the expression of pragmatic knowl-
knowledge is critical (Gobet et al., 2001). edge, especially at high levels of performance (Bosman
In the following discussion, we further elucidate dif- & Charness, 1996; Hambrick & Engle, 2002; Molander
ferent facets of this interdependence. This approach is & Bäckman, 1993). A good example is the difference in
610 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

peak age for tournament versus correspondence chess position approach (Salthouse, 1984). In this paradigm,
(Charness, in press). The mean age at which a world adults of different ages are equated in general (e.g.,
championship is first won is about 46 years of age for molar) task proficiency to investigate whether equal lev-
correspondence chess, but about 30 years of age for els of criterion performance are attained through age-
tournament chess. In correspondence chess, players are differential profiles of “molecular ” component processes
permitted 3 days to deliberate a move; in tournament (Charness, 1989). Thus, age differences at the molecular
chess, deliberation averages three minutes per move. level of analysis are seen as a reflection of age-based
Thus, the difference in peak age between the two activi- changes in the relative contribution of knowledge and
ties seems to reflect differences in the relative impor- basic processing efficiency to criterion performance.
tance of cognitive/perceptual speed and knowledge Salthouse (1984) studied a total of 74 transcription
(e.g., Burns, 2004). typists ranging from 19 to 72 years of age. Figure 11.14
This example points to a general dilemma governing displays an interpretation of the main findings of this
the relation between the mechanics, the pragmatics, and study in terms of the two-component model. In this
age/time. The acquisition of expertise takes time. For sample, age and level of typing skill (i.e., net words per
instance, Simon and Chase (1973) argued that 10 years minute) were uncorrelated (e.g., molar equivalence). Age
of deliberate practice are needed to reach excellence in a was negatively related to measures of perceptual /motor
particular domain of functioning. For this reason alone, speed (e.g., tapping speed), but positively related to eye-
experts tend to be older than novices (cf. Lehman, hand span. In other words, older typists were slower in
1953). On the other hand, decrements in certain aspects tapping speed but looked further ahead in the text to be
of the mechanics, such as perceptual speed, can be reli- typed. These findings are consistent with the interpreta-
ably identified by age 30 (S.-C. Li, Lindenberger, et al., tion that aging typists extend their eye-hand span to
2004; Salthouse, 1991). Therefore, differences in peak counteract the consequences of aging losses in perceptual /
age across domains can be seen as ontogenetic compro- motor speed, and illustrate the compensatory relation-
mises between biology and culture, and are probably ship between knowledge and speed.
good indicators of the relative importance of pragmatic To the extent that selective attrition does not play a
knowledge and mechanic processing efficiency. prominent role, the performance pattern of older typ-
An exclusive focus on ages of peak productivity or ists may, in part, reflect loss-induced development, or
peak achievement would hide essential and unique compensation in the strict sense of the term (P. B.
features of late-life intellectual growth. For instance, Baltes & Baltes, 1990b; Dixon & Bäckman, 1995; Salt-
some exceptional individuals seem to escape mechanic house, 1995). With respect to methods, this example
decline well into the 9th decade of their lives. If these
individuals also happen to be experts in a particular
domain, they can produce outstanding works through-
out their life. One example would be Sophocles
(497–406B.C.), who won his first prize for the best Slower Reaction Time
Mechanics Biological When Seeing and Typing
Aging
drama of the year at age 28, wrote over 120 dramas, Individual Letters
and developed a new dramatic style in his 80s. Com-
menting on his own late-life artistic development,
Pragmatics Knowledge-based Anticipatory Reading
Sophocles said that he finally had liberated himself Compensation of the Amount of Text
from the artificiality of his earlier style, and had found
a language that was the best and the most ethical
(Schadewaldt, 1975, p. 75; for related evidence on clas-
Figure 11.14 Older typists can maintain a high level of
sical composers, see Simonton, 1988, 1989). functioning by reading farther ahead in the text to be typed,
despite a loss in reaction time when typing individual letters.
A Third Prototypical Example: Speed and The example illustrates the compensatory relationship be-
Knowledge in Aging Typists tween the pragmatics and the mechanics of cognition and sug-
gests that selective optimization with compensation may play
A good empirical demonstration of the gain / loss
an important role in successfully adapting to aging-induced
dynamic between the cognitive mechanics and the cogni- losses in the mechanics of cognition. Source: From “Effects
tive pragmatics comes from a study on aging typists of Age and Skill in Typing,” by T. A. Salthouse, 1984, Jour-
using the so-called molar equivalence/molecular decom- nal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, pp. 345–371.
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 611

demonstrates how the combination of expertise and ber ability; cf. Schaie, 1989; Schaie et al., 2005), the
information-processing approaches may lead to a better general picture resulting from such comparisons is that
understanding of the compensatory relation between higher test scores are obtained at more recent times
acquired bodies of cultural knowledge and basic as- (Flynn, 1987; Schaie, 1996). Probably, this historical
pects of information processing efficiency (cf. Abra- increase in test scores across historical time is not due
ham & Hansson, 1995; Lang et al., 2002). to changes in the genetic composition of the population
or differential sampling bias, but reflects some general
Malleability (Plasticity) in Intellectual change (i.e., improvement) in health- and education-
Functioning across Historical and related conditions. The magnitude of these effects can
Ontogenetic Time be quite large. For the U.S. population during the twen-
tieth century, for instance, they sometimes exceeded a
As is true for development in general, life span changes standard deviation within a 30-year range of historical
in intellectual functioning represent the overdetermined change (Schaie, 1996). It should be cautioned, however,
antecedents, correlates, and outcomes of a large variety that we do not know whether environmental-change
of different sources of influence (e.g., mechanics, prag- effects of the same order of magnitude would be ob-
matics, motivation, personality, societal opportunity served with pure indicators of the mechanics of intelli-
structures). Therefore, differences in level of intellec- gence. For instance, much of the measures used in the
tual performance reflect, within the age-graded bound- battery of the Seattle Longitudinal Study (Schaie,
aries provided by the mechanics, variations in physical 1996) have a strong cultural-knowledge component,
and sociocultural aspects of environmental conditions and are more likely to be affected by historical change
(P. B. Baltes et al., in press; Klix, 1993). In the follow- and dissipation than other, less knowledge-loaded mea-
ing, we report two interrelated lines of research in sup- sures of brain efficiency. With respect to the Seattle
port of this contention. The first line of research Longitudinal study (Schaie, 1996), the convergence
addresses environmental change at a large (i.e., histori- between cross-sectional and independent-sample same-
cal) time scale. The second example refers to the mal- cohort comparisons suggests that the more positive age
leability of adult-intellectual functioning in the context gradients found with longitudinal samples may be
of cognitive intervention studies. partly due to practice effects and selective attrition
(see also Salthouse, 1991). Analyses of longitudinal
Cohort Effects, Period Effects, and
data from the Berlin Aging Study (BASE) are fully
Environmental Change
consistent with both predictions (Lindenberger, Singer,
As expected on the basis of life span contextualism, on- & Baltes, 2002; Lövdén, Ghisletta, & Lindenberger,
togenetic processes unfold in a constantly changing so- 2004; T. Singer, Verhaeghen, et al., 2003).
cial and cultural environment. As a consequence, age Theoretically, the direction and precise magnitude of
gradients in intellectual abilities are not fixed but re- historical-change effects is generally of little impor-
flect history-graded systems of influence, such as en- tance. From a history-of-science point of view, however,
during differences between individuals born at different such effects, and especially their interpretation as
points in historical time (cohort effects), specific in- culture-based cohort effects, were instrumental in point-
fluences of historical events across chronological age ing to the substantial malleability (plasticity) of intel-
(period effects), or generalized and enduring shifts in lectual performance during all periods of the adult life
the environment affecting individuals of all ages and span (P. B. Baltes, 1973). The resulting growth in
subsequent cohorts (general environmental change). For awareness for the existence of life span plasticity even-
methodological reasons, discriminating among these va- tually led to advances in life span theorizing, and to
rieties of environmental change is not easy (P. B. Baltes, more controlled investigations into the range of intellec-
1968; P. B. Baltes et al., 1979; Lövdén, Ghisletta, & tual plasticity and its age-based limits (P. B. Baltes &
Lindenberger, 2004; Magnusson et al., 1991; Schaie, Kliegl, 1992; P. B. Baltes & Lindenberger, 1988; P. B.
1965, 1994, 2005). Baltes & Willis, 1982; T. Singer, Lindenberger, & Baltes,
A first step to discern effects of large-scale environ- 2003; Willis, 1990). Specifically, multidirectional co-
mental change is to compare the performance of hort differences in intellectual trajectories may entice
same-aged individuals across historical time (i.e., time- interdisciplinary collaboration with medicine and nutri-
lagged comparisons). With some exceptions (e.g., num- tional sciences, educational neuroscience, and sociology
612 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

to understand their proximal antecedents and conse- search presented above clearly indicate that the amount
quences (Schaie et al., 2005). (scope) of plasticity decreases with advancing age, at
least during adulthood. At limits of mechanic function-
Cognitive Intervention Work: Activation of ing, older adults definitely display less potential. To
Learning Potential among Older Adults what degree such cognitive training in older adults
Intervention work (P. B. Baltes & Willis, 1982; Kramer changes the mechanics themselves is unclear but possi-
& Willis, 2002; Willis, 2001) is a more direct (i.e., ex- ble (Kempermann, 2006).
perimentally controlled) way to explore the degree A related line of intervention research has found that
of plasticity in intellectual functioning than cohort- aerobic fitness attenuates age-related decrements in cog-
comparative research. In the field of adult development nitive control (e.g., multitasking) in later adulthood
and aging, intervention studies have been undertaken to (Kramer et al., 1999). This finding can be explained in
examine whether age-based decrements in standard at least two ways. First, from the perspective of SOC
psychometric tests of intellectual functioning are re- theory, increasing bodily fitness may reduce older adults’
versible, in full or in part, through training and practice needs to continuously invest portions of their cognitive
(Willis & Nesselroade, 1990). For the most part, inter- resources into the coordination of their increasingly fal-
ventions involved older adults only, and focused on tests lible sensory and motor functions (e.g., Lindenberger
from the broad fluid domain. et al., 2000). In other words, training the sensamotor
The major results of this cognitive intervention work function required for bodily functioning “ frees” up re-
can be summarized in five points (e.g., P. B. Baltes & sources for other cognitive tasks. Second, recent brain-
Lindenberger, 1988; Kramer & Willis, 2002): (1) Train- imaging evidence suggests that aerobic fitness has direct
ing gains in the practiced tests among healthy older beneficial effects on prefrontal cortex functioning (Col-
adults are substantial (i.e., they roughly correspond to combe et al., 2003), which may enhance performance on
the amount of naturally occurring longitudinal decline cognitive tasks that put high demands on cognitive con-
between 60 and 80 years of age); (2) transfer, however, trol. Clearly, the two explanations are not mutually ex-
is limited to similar tests of the same ability; (3) train- clusive.
ing gains are maintained over lengthy periods of time up
to several years (Neely & Bäckman, 1993; Willis & Relative Stability in Intellectual Functioning
Nesselroade, 1990); (4) the factor structure of the abil- across the Life Span
ity space is not altered substantially through training
(Schaie, Willis, Hertzog, & Schulenberg, 1987); and The issue of continuity and discontinuity, or stability
(5) in persons at risk for Alzheimer’s disease or af- and change, has a long tradition within developmental
flicted by other forms of brain pathology, training gains psychology at large (Kagan, 1980), and life span intel-
have been found to be restricted to experimental condi- lectual development, in particular (P. B. Baltes &
tions of high external support (Bäckman, Josephsson, Smith, 2003; Hertzog, 1985; Lövdén & Lindenberger,
Herlitz, Stigsdotter, & Viitanen, 1991) or to be nonexis- 2004; McArdle & Epstein, 1987; J. R. Nesselroade,
tent (M. M. Baltes et al., 1995; M. M. Baltes, Kühl, & 1991; Schaie, 1965). Different forms of stability, such
Sowarka, 1992). as stability in level, rank order, and profiles, have been
These results indicate that the majority of healthy set apart (Caspi & Bem, 1990). The main emphasis of
older adults, including those who display the typical pat- the following life span synopsis of intellectual develop-
tern of age-related losses in the mechanics of cognition ment is on interindividual rank order, or on what Kagan
(e.g., fluid abilities) under untrained conditions, are able (1980) has called relative stability, which denotes the
to greatly improve their performance after a few ses- extent to which individual differences during later peri-
sions of task-related training or practice. Thus, among ods of ontogeny can be predicted on the basis of individ-
healthy older adults, the mechanics of cognition are suf- ual differences observed during earlier periods.
ficiently preserved to permit the acquisition of task- In most cases, evidence on the relative stability after
relevant declarative and procedural knowledge. How- infancy is based on undifferentiated measures of general
ever, there is little evidence to suggest that training intelligence, or IQ tests. We agree with others that an
gains generalize to related abilities or to everyday func- exclusive focus on these omnibus measures hides essen-
tioning. Moreover, the results of testing-the-limits re- tial features of life span intellectual development and
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 613

the structure of intelligence (Cattell, 1971; Horn, 1989). ning. According to one interpretation (e.g., Bornstein,
Specifically, such measures can be seen as mixtures of 1989), infants who habituate more efficiently, and
mechanic and normative-pragmatic components of intel- who tend to look at the novel object, rather than the
lectual functioning that approximate, to varying de- old, are better able to inhibit action tendencies associ-
grees, the centroid of the intellectual ability factor ated with already existing representations (e.g., Dia-
space (i.e., Spearman’s g). With this qualification in mond, 2002; McCall, 1994). The hypothesis that
mind, we restrict the following discussion, with one ex- inhibition may mediate the predictive link is consis-
ception (i.e., infant development), to undifferentiated or tent with neuropsychological investigations of infants’
IQ-like measures of intellectual functioning. recognition memory (e.g., Diamond, 2002; Johnson,
Posner, & Rothbart, 1991). It also supports the more
Predicting Childhood Intelligence on the Basis of general claim that inhibition ability and novelty pref-
Infant Behavior erence are central features of intelligence (Berg &
Until the 1950s, it was generally believed that intelli- Sternberg, 1985a).
gence was an immutable characteristic of the individual,
Relative Interindividual Stability after Infancy
which led to the unchallenged assumption that individu-
als maintain their rank order on measures of intellectual For reasons that are not yet well understood (Cardon &
functioning throughout life. Starting in the 1960s, how- Fulker, 1991; McCall & Carriger, 1993), the magni-
ever, it was found that stability in early mental test per- tude of the correlation between infant measures of ha-
formance was low (McCall, 1979). On the basis of this bituation (i.e., 2 to 8 months) and childhood measures
evidence, it was concluded that standardized tests of in- of intelligence (i.e., 1 to 12 years) is temporally stable
fant development do not predict later intelligence at use- or even increasing (Cardon & Fulker, 1991), rather
ful levels of prediction until after 18 to 24 months of than decreasing over time. In contrast, relative stability
age. This majority view of ontogenetic instability of in- after infancy is rather well described on the basis of
terindividual differences during infancy was again chal- quasi-simplex assumptions (Humphreys & Davey,
lenged and ultimately replaced by more recent research 1988; Molenaar, Boomsma, & Dolan, 1991). Thus, ad-
using habituation and recognition-memory paradigms. jacent time points in ontogeny tend to be more highly
In contrast to standardized infant tests of sensorimotor correlated than more distant time points In addition,
capacities, these two paradigms were originally based stability coefficients computed over identical lapses
on operant-conditioning and/or information-processing of time show a considerable increase in magnitude
perspectives, and refer to infants’ tendency to change from childhood to adolescence into middle adulthood
their behaviors as a function of prior exposure to a stim- and early old age (Hertzog & Schaie, 1986, 1988;
ulus (e.g., decrements in attention in the case of habitu- Humphreys & Davey, 1988; for review, see Lövdén &
ation, or novelty preference in the case of recognition Lindenberger, 2004).
memory). On average, individual differences in habitua- In agreement with others (e.g., Humphreys & Davey,
tion and recognition memory performance between 2 1988; Molenaar, Boomsma, & Dolan, 1993), we propose
and 8 months were found to be moderately correlated that these age-based changes in relative interindividual
with standard tests of intelligence such as the Wechsler, stability should be interpreted in connection with age-
Bayley, or Binet administered between 1 and 8 years based changes in level (e.g., Lövdén & Lindenberger,
(median correlation, r = .45; after attenuation for unreli- 2004). According to this line of reasoning, interindivid-
ability, r = .70; Bornstein, 1989; McCall & Carriger, ual differences change more rapidly early in develop-
1993; for recent evidence, see F. Smith, Fagan, & Ul- ment because the intellectual repertoire is smaller but
vund, 2002). A more recent meta-analysis has con- growing faster than at later points during ontogeny,
firmed these results (Kavsek, 2004). Behavior-genetic thereby giving room for larger amounts of new variance
research suggests that individual differences in at least per unit time ( both environmental and genetic). By the
some of the measures used for prediction have a genetic same token, aging-induced losses and age-associated
component (Benson, Cherny, Haith, & Fulker, 1993; pathologies (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease) may not only
Cardon & Fulker, 1991). lead to decrements in level but also to a reshuffling of
Both relative change and relative stability shape individual differences in very old age (Mitrushina &
life span intellectual development from its very begin- Satz, 1991; cf. P. B. Baltes & Smith, 2004).
614 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Changes in Heritability across the Life Span Third, heritability estimates are fixed-level statistics
(P. B. Baltes et al., 1988; Plomin & Thompson, 1988), in-
We now turn to the study of age-based changes in the dicating what consequences (phenotypic expressions) are
contribution of genetic and environmental sources of in- produced under a given and specific set of interindividual
terindividual variability to individual differences in in- differences in genetic and environmental conditions.
telligence. We start with a consideration of general and Strong evidence demonstrating the environmental mal-
ability-specific effects, and then turn our attention to leability of heritability estimates comes from data on
life span changes in heritability estimates for general 7-year-old twins participating in the National Collabora-
(i.e., undifferentiated) measures of intelligence across tive Perinatal Project (Turkheimer, Haley, Waldron,
the life span. D’Onofrio, & Gottesman, 2003). A substantial propor-
tion of the twins in this sample were raised in families
A Note on the Nature of Behavior-Genetic Evidence living near or below the poverty level. The authors found
Before we summarize the relevant evidence, we will that the proportions of IQ variance attributable to genes
sketch out our views on the meaning, strength, and limi- and environment varied nonlinearly with socioeconomic
tations of the behavior-genetics approach (P. B. Baltes status. In impoverished families, the shared environment
et al., 1988). Given the critical debates surrounding the accounted for 60% of the variance in IQ, and the contri-
interpretation of behavior-genetic data (e.g., Bronfen- bution of genes was close to zero. In affluent families, the
brenner & Ceci, 1994; Gottlieb et al., Chapter 5, this result was almost exactly the reverse. Apparently, factors
Handbook, this volume; R. M. Lerner, 1995; Molenaar associated with low socioeconomic status such as depri-
et al., 2003; Scarr, 1993), such a note may help to avoid vation from developmental opportunity structures hin-
possible misunderstandings. We restrict our comment to dered the behavioral expression of genetically based
three points that are relevant both for the following sec- interindividual differences in intellectual functioning.
tion on intellectual functioning as well as on personality Despite these qualifications, behavior-genetic evi-
and the self. More detailed treatment is provided in P. B. dence provides important information about sources of
Baltes et al. (1998). interindividual differences in life span development, es-
First, heritability coefficients in human research pecially if linked to molecular research on specific ge-
(where selective inbreeding and exposure to extreme en- netic polymorphisms (Goldberg & Weinberger, 2004), to
vironments is limited) are statements about the scope of intermediate phenotypes at the level of brain organiza-
interindividual differences more so than statements tion (e.g., Anokhin et al., in press), or both. Such find-
about the processes and mechanisms of genetic expres- ings, especially if based on longitudinal (e.g., Finkel,
sion at the individual and intraindividual level of analy- Pedersen, McClearn, Plomin, & Berg, 1996), experi-
sis. In other words, population-based behavior genetics mental (e.g., Fox et al., 1996), and cross-cultural (e.g.,
provides clues about the existence of genetically based Turkheimer et al., 2003) data, provide estimates of the
variation in a given population but does not provide di- degree to which, on a population level of analysis,
rect evidence about gene locations or epigenetic events interindividual dif ferences in developmental outcomes
producing this variation (for emerging links between are co-determined by interindividual differences in
behavior and molecular genetics, for example, Dick & genetic predispositions and extant environmental varia-
Rose, 2002; de Geus & Boomsma, 2002). tions. Thus, everything else being equal, high heritabil-
Second, standard behavior-genetic models do not ity estimates of a given behavioral outcome suggest that
provide the best test of the overall role of environmental interindividual dif ferences in this behavioral outcome
forces. The power of such forces is better tested by and in this “life space” are strongly genetically deter-
studies that examine the role of the impact of environ- mined than interindividual dif ferences in behavioral out-
mental factors across the population and across comes with low heritability estimates.
interindividual differences in genetic make-up. Specif-
ically, high heritability estimates do not preclude the Genetic and Environmental Influence over
existence of environmental factors that alter perfor- Ontogenetic Time: Specific and General Effects
mance levels in all individuals of a given sample (for an Numerous studies have shown that genetic and environ-
experimental demonstration, see Fox, Hershberger, & mental influences can be operative in the regulation of
Bouchard, 1996). individual differences at both ability-specific and more
First Level 5 Example: Intellectual Functioning across the Life Span 615

general levels (e.g., Cardon & Fulker, 1994). In longitu- because the genetic variance component has stabilized
dinal analyses of hierarchically organized intellectual at a high level (e.g., not much new genetic variance is
abilities obtained from genetically informative data added over time), and because environments (which, in
sets, it is possible to determine the genetic and environ- part, have been selected on the basis of genetic endow-
mental contributions to stability and change in rank ment) also tend to be stable during this period of the
order and mean level both at the level of specific abili- life span. Similarly, the breakdown of well-orches-
ties and at the level of a general factor (e.g., Cardon & trated genome expression in very old age may cause
Fulker, 1994). An interesting example for the class of late-life decrements in level, relative stability, and her-
findings that can be obtained with this method comes itability. Note, however, that selective mortality may
from child cognitive development. Specifically, data counteract the identification of these trends at the pop-
from the Colorado Adoption Project indicate that strong ulation level in very old age (T. Singer, Verhaeghen,
novel contributions of genetic variance at the level of et al., 2003).
general ability emerge at the ages of three and seven but
seem to be absent during the transition from childhood The Mechanics and Pragmatics in Very Old Age
to adolescence, when genetic variance contributes exclu-
sively to continuity of individual differences. So far, our discussion of life span intellectual develop-
ment was organized around topics, rather than age peri-
Estimates of Heritability of Interindividual ods. In this last section, we deviate from this practice by
Differences across the Life Span giving special attention to the life period of very old
Similar to life span changes in stability, heritability in age. In our view, this last phase of life merits such atten-
intellectual functioning (e.g., the amount of interindi- tion because it represents a natural boundary condition
vidual variance attributable to genetic differences) in- for the validity of the two-component model of intelli-
creases from about 20% to 50% during childhood and gence and cognition. Specifically, we expect that an in-
adolescence to about 80% in early and middle adult- creasing portion of the very old population eventually
hood (e.g., McGue, Bouchard, Iacono, & Lykken, attains levels of mechanic functioning that are suffi-
1993). Interestingly, in old age (e.g., beyond age 75), ciently low to impair intellectual functioning in a rela-
heritability tends to decrease to values around 60% tively global manner. A number of recent empirical
(e.g., McClearn et al., 1997). In contrast, shared envi- cross-sectional and longitudinal observations from the
ronmental influences on interindividual differences BASE (P. B. Baltes & Mayer, 1999; P. B. Baltes, Mayer,
generally do not persist beyond the period of common Helmchen, & Steinhagen-Thiessen, 1993) support and
rearing (McGue, Bouchard, et al., 1993). As stated be- qualify this prediction (for a detailed summary, see
fore, these findings are based on samples representing Lövdén et al., 2004). Three results from this very old
the normal range of environments and genes, and can- sample are most pertinent to the two-component model
not be generalized beyond this normal range (e.g., to (P. B. Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997; Lindenberger &
extremes of environmental deprivation or reshuffled Baltes, 1995a).
environments). Within this normal range, however, the
life span increase in heritability of interindividual dif- Covariance Dedifferentiation
ferences is consistent with the notion that adolescents First, ability intercorrelations both between and within
and adults have more of a chance to actively select en- fluid-mechanic and normative-pragmatic domains were
vironments that match their genes than infants and of much higher magnitude in old age than corresponding
children (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). ability intercorrelations during middle and early adult-
Based on the preceding summaries, it appears that hood. Based on these data, the amount of covariation
relative stability and heritability exhibit similar life among interindividual differences in intellectual abili-
span age gradients (see Plomin & Thompson, 1988). ties, or the prominence of g, seems to increase in very
More multivariate and longitudinal behavior-genetic old age (P. B. Baltes & Lindenberger, 1997). The idea
evidence is needed to fully understand the covariance that g may vary as a function of age and/or ability level
dynamics of this life span parallelism. One possibility dates back to Spearman (Deary & Pagliari, 1991), and
would be that individual differences in intellectual has led to the differentiation /dedifferentiation hypothe-
functioning around middle adulthood are highly stable sis of life span intelligence (Garrett, 1946; Lienert &
616 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Crott, 1964; Reinert, 1970). Despite methodological gence age gradients for the T4 longitudinal sample
difficulties in testing this hypothesis (J. R. Nesselroade (n = 132) using all available data points (i.e., T1, T3,
& Thompson, 1995), the evidence obtained so far seems and T4 data); these gradients combine cross-sectional
generally supportive (for a summary, see Lövdén & Lin- and longitudinal information over chronological
denberger, 2004). For instance, Li, Lindenberger, et al. age ( hence convergence); (2) the cross-sectional
(2004) performed life span-comparative exploratory T1 gradient of the T4 longitudinal sample (i.e., the
principal component analyses of fifteen intellectual same sample as before; n = 132); here, the T1 cross-
ability tests. The results of these analyses are shown sectional age gradient was examined for individuals
in the lower panel of Figure 11.10. In childhood, late who survived and participated up to T4; and (3)
adulthood, and old age, only two components with the cross-sectional T1 gradient of the original T1 sam-
eigenvalues greater one were extracted, but in adoles- ple (n = 516). The three age gradients are shown in
cence, young, and middle adulthood, five components Figure 11.15.
displayed eigenvalues greater than unity. Also, fluid and With respect to both fluid mechanics and crystal-
crystallized intelligence were more highly correlated in lized pragmatics, age-associated decrements in cogni-
childhood, late adulthood, and old age than in adoles- tion were less pronounced for the longitudinal sample
cence, young, and middle adulthood. at T1 than for the full cross-sectional sample at T1.
From the perspective of the two-component model of Specifically, negative gradients prevailed for all four
cognitive development, the decrease of ability intercor- abilities in the full T1 sample but verbal knowledge did
relations during childhood and the increase of intercor- not decline significantly in the longitudinal sample.
relations in very old age point to age-based changes (i.e., This pattern of age gradients suggests that decline in
decrements and increments) in the importance of the fluid mechanics is normative and age-based,
domain-general processing constraints. Cross-sectional whereas decline in verbal knowledge appears to be par-
data from the BASE (P. B. Baltes & Lindenberger, tially or primarily associated with closeness to death.
1997; Lindenberger & Baltes, 1994) suggest that old-age The third class of age gradients, the longitudinal con-
dedifferentiation transcends the cognitive domain, vergence gradients for the T4 sample, reinforces this
and also affects sensory functioning (e.g., Ghisletta & impression.
Lindenberger, 2005) and sensorimotor functioning (e.g.,
Maintenance of Divergence in Explanatory
balance/gait). In line with these correlational findings,
Correlational Patterns
recent neurocognitive evidence demonstrates that pro-
cessing pathways and brain activation patterns are less Given the two preceding findings, one may begin
differentiated in older adults than in young adults to wonder whether the distinction between the me-
(Cabeza et al., 2004; Park et al., 2004). chanics and the pragmatics of cognition loses all of its
empirical foundation in very old age. Figure 11.16 that
Directionality Dedifferentiation compares the correlational patterns of perceptual
The second finding from the BASE concerns the direc- speed, a fluid-mechanic ability, and verbal knowledge,
tionality of the age gradients (Lindenberger & Baltes, a normative-pragmatic marker, with variables re-
1997). In very old age, differences in the directionality lated to individual differences in sociostructural-bio-
of cross-sectional age gradients between mechanic and graphical or biological status suggests that this is not
normative-pragmatic abilities are on the wane. Instead, the case.
gradations of negativity have been observed, with per- Without exception, correlations to indicators of
ceptual speed showing the strongest and verbal knowl- biological functioning were more pronounced for per-
edge the weakest negative age relations. ceptual speed (e.g., the mechanics) than for verbal
These cross-sectional observations have been knowledge (e.g., the pragmatics). The reverse was
corroborated and qualified by longitudinal evidence also true: Correlations to sociostructural-biographical
(T. Singer, Verhaeghen, et al., 2003). Using latent markers were more pronounced for verbal knowledge
growth curve modeling (see McArdle, Hamagami, El- than for perceptual speed. Apparently, then, the me-
lias, & Robbins, 1991), T. Singer, Verhaeghen, et al. chanic-pragmatic distinction does not dissolve com-
(2003) compared cross-sectional and longitudinal age pletely in very old age, but is maintained in the guise of
gradients under three different data selection condi- divergent relations to biological and cultural systems of
tions: (1) the cross-sectional / longitudinal conver- influence.
Life Span Intellectual Development: Conclusions 617

Speed Predictor Intelligence Measure


70
60 Visual Acuity
T-Score

50
Auditory Acuity
40
30 Balance/Gait
20
70 80 90 100 110
Age
Years of
Education
Knowledge Occupational
70 Prestige
60
Social Class
T-Score

50
Perceptual Speed (Mechanics)
40 Household Verbal Knowledge (Pragmatics)
Income
30
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 55 60
20
70 80 90 100 110 Variance Explained by Predictors (%)
Age
Figure 11.16 The divergent validity of the two-component
model of life span intellectual development continues into very
Memory old age. The figure displays differential correlational links of
70
perceptual speed, a marker of the fluid mechanics, and verbal
60
knowledge, a marker of the crystallized pragmatics, to indicators
T-Score

50 of sociostructural-biographical and biological (e.g., sensory)


40 status. Perceptual speed was more highly correlated with biolog-
30 ical indicators than verbal knowledge, and verbal knowledge was
more highly correlated with sociostructural-biographical indica-
20
70 80 90 100 110 tors than perceptual speed. Thus, despite a general tendency to-
Age ward dedifferentiation due to age-based losses in the mechanics,
the two components of life span cognition continue to show
signs of divergent external validity. Data are taken from the
Fluency
70 Berlin Aging Study (N = 516, age range = 70–103 years).
Source: From “ The Fate of Congition in Very Old Age: Six-
60
Year Longitudinal Findings in the Berlin Aging Study,” by
T-Score

50 T. Singer, P. Verhaeghen, P. Ghisletta, U. Lindenberger, and


40 P. B. Baltes, 2003, Psychology and Aging, 18, pp. 318–331.
30
20
70 80 90 100 110
Age LIFE SPAN INTELLECTUAL
DEVELOPMENT: CONCLUSIONS
Figure 11.15 Intellectual ability age gradients observed in
the Berlin Aging Study as a function of sample and measure-
ment occasion. Thick solid lines represent cross-sectional / Based on the foregoing (admittedly selective) review of
longitudinal convergence gradients of the longitudinal sample research and theory, we would like to propose the fol-
(n = 132), and encompass measurements from T1, T3, and T4, lowing synopsis of the overall ontogenetic landscape of
which encompass an average longitudinal observation period of life span intellectual development.
6 years. Thin solid lines represent cross-sectional gradients of
the same longitudinal sample (n = 132), and are based on meas-
urements taken at T1. Finally, dashed lines represent cross- 1. To capture the life span dynamics and biocultural
sectional gradients for the total T1 sample (n = 516). Source: co-construction between biology and culture in the
From “ The Fate of Cognition in Very Old Age: Six-Year Longi- domains of intelligence and cognition (P. B. Baltes,
tudinal Findings in the Berlin Aging Study,” by T. Singer, 1987, 1997; P. B. Baltes et al., 1998), we contrasted the
P. Verhaeghen, P. Ghisletta, U. Lindenberger, and P. B. Baltes,
2003, Psychology and Aging, 18, pp. 318–331. Copyright ©
mechanics with the pragmatics, and propose a two-
2003 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with component model of intellectual development. This
permission. model is inspired by the psychometric theory of fluid and
618 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

crystallized intelligence (Cattell, 1971; Horn, 1970, is based on SOC theory, that highest levels of prag-
1989; cf. Tetens, 1777) but calls for a wider range of con- matic skills in the last third of life carry a strong indi-
ceptualization, including evolutionary-psychological, vidualized component.
cognitive-experimental, expertise, and neuroscience ap- 5. The acquisition of expert levels of knowledge dur-
proaches, to arrive at more valid and comprehensive rep- ing adulthood may lead to an increasing fragmentation
resentations of life span intellectual development. The of the intellectual system, but it also may offer the oppor-
two-component model accurately predicts a relatively tunity for acquiring bodies of knowledge with a wide
late life span peak followed by maintenance for the range of applicability, generality, and integration. Wisdom-
knowledge-saturated cognitive pragmatics, and a much related knowledge, or knowledge about the meaning and
earlier life span peak followed by monotonic decline for conduct of life, is a prototype. The likelihood of acquir-
the cognitive mechanics. It also accurately predicts dif- ing such domain-general bodies of person-specific
ferential ontogenetic sources of explanation. knowledge depends on a special coalition of experiential,
2. In terms of mechanisms, age-related changes in expertise-specific, and person-related factors (Krampe
information processing rate, working memory capacity, & Baltes, 2003; Sternberg, 1985).
and the inhibition of irrelevant information are among 6. Throughout ontogeny, the pragmatics and me-
the most prominent candidates for the explanation of life chanics of cognition are intertwined. In everyday life,
span changes in the mechanics of cognition. At present, intellectual functioning and intellectual products repre-
these constructs tend to suffer from a lack of formaliza- sent joint effects of both. For instance, the emergence
tion, a lack of direct evidence at the physiological level, of domains of pragmatic knowledge builds on, and pre-
and from difficulties in making differential predictions. sumably extends and modifies, evolutionarily predis-
Closer links to the cognitive neurosciences, in particular posed core domains. The mechanisms of this pruning of
to chemical, anatomical, and functional life span cultural knowledge onto species-specific architecture
changes in prefrontal functions, are expected to foster await further study. Another example of pragmatic/me-
further progress in this area of research. chanic interdependence concerns the acquisition and
3. Extant measures of mechanic functioning tend to use of pragmatic knowledge to compensate for me-
be contaminated by pragmatic influences. To arrive at chanic decline. In close agreement with our general
more accurate descriptions of life span gradients in the conception of the overall landscape of life span devel-
mechanics of cognition, and to move toward explanation opment, this compensatory function of the pragmatics
in terms of critical components and mechanisms, mea- increases in importance but loses in efficiency with ad-
surement needs to be purified through the utilization of vancing age.
methods that are better able to assess individuals’ upper 7. The study of plasticity (malleability) of intellec-
limits of functioning. As predicted by theory, the use of tual functioning has been a cornerstone of life span re-
such methods (e.g., testing-the-limits) results in cleaner search (P. B. Baltes, 1987). Within the limits provided
separations of individuals from different ages than the by the mechanics, which remain to be fully explored, in-
use of standard measures (see Figure 11.9). tellectual performance is malleable throughout life. Evi-
4. In contrast to the mechanics, the knowledge- and dence in support of this contention comes both from the
culture-based pragmatics of cognition offer the poten- study of long-term environmental change and from cog-
tial for positive change during adulthood and old age. nitive intervention studies. With some exceptions (e.g.,
Within the pragmatic domain, we proposed the distinc- dementia of the Alzheimer type), there is room for size-
tion between normative and person-specific bodies able plasticity at all ages and for all individuals. How-
of knowledge. Normative bodies of knowledge are ac- ever, plasticity decreases with advancing age, reflecting
quired in the context of general socialization events, losses in the mechanics of cognition. The resulting
such as basic cultural skills and educational curricula, bounded openness of life span intellectual development
and in general are well amenable to psychometric test- is consistent with the biocultural contextualist frame-
ing (e.g., vocabulary tests, aptitude tests). Person- work of life span psychology.
specific knowledge refers to specialized knowledge 8. The joint consideration of different strands of
systems that branch off from the normative (average) research reveals a striking congruence between three
path, with professional expertise being the most promi- different life span trajectories: Heritability of interindi-
nent example studied so far. Our suggestion is, and this vidual differences, relative stability, and level changes
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 619

in the normative pragmatics (e.g., crystallized intelli- allocation of resources. Furthermore, three of the life
gence). In all three cases, there is an increase from span propositions introduced earlier that bear special
childhood to middle and late adulthood, coupled with relevance for personality development across the life
indications of decline in very old age. This life span span are discussed in more detail. These three issues are
parallelism between the genetic component of interindi- stability and change in personality development across
vidual differences, continuity of interindividual differ- the life span, opportunities and constraints of personal-
ences, and general knowledge is consistent with the ity development, and the adaptive potential or reserve
notion of gene-environment correlations in behavioral capacity of personality.
genetics (Scarr & McCartney, 1983), and the notion of
niche picking in ethology (Dawkins, 1982). Whether one Three Approaches to the Study of
likes it or not, this parallelism testifies to the existence Personality Development
of a powerful life span synergism between sociostruc-
tural and genetic interindividual differentiation, at least Research and theory building in the study of personality
within the range of developmental conditions offered by have been quite diverse (e.g., Pervin & John, 1999). Yet,
Western industrialized societies. three longstanding and overarching concerns can be iden-
tified, that is, structure/content, dynamics, and develop-
ment of personality (Funder, 2001). Historically, these
SECOND LEVEL 5 EXAMPLE: THE STUDY
three concerns have been linked with the three approaches
OF PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT
to the study of personality mentioned earlier (Staudinger,
ACROSS THE LIFE SPAN
in press).
Under the trait approach to the study of personality,
In the following, we illustrate what life span theory we subsume efforts to characterize individuals in terms
has to offer in organizing and stimulating the study of of fundamental attributes and behavioral dispositions, a
personality development. To do so, we first introduce line of research that originated primarily in the psycho-
three approaches that in our view need to be taken into ac- metric tradition. Research in this area focuses on the
count when studying personality development: (1) a trait identification of the structure of personality, on in-
approach, (2) a self-system approach, and (3) a self- terindividual differences, and the extent of longitudinal
regulation approach.1 These three approaches are usually stability (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1994; Goldberg, 1993).
treated in different literatures, and cross-links are still The emergence, maintenance, and transformation of
rare, especially with regard to life span development. In personality structure, and the conditions of constancy
the following, we consider all three approaches whenever
and change in interindividual differences clearly are of
using the term personality or personality system.
importance for a life span perspective on personality
The levels-of-analysis approach introduced in the be-
(Brim & Kagan, 1980). In addition, however, a life span
ginning of the chapter is used as an integrating frame-
perspective is aimed at discerning the degree to which
work for presenting research from the three approaches. these personality attributes and behavioral dispositions
Thus, theory and evidence available on personality de- evince intraindividual change trajectories and intraindi-
velopment across the life span are used to illustrate the vidual plasticity (malleability). Such questions are pur-
biology-culture interface and the notion of differential sued in the exemplary research programs involving
scholars such as Block (e.g., 1995), Helson (e.g., Helson
1
Note that selecting “personality” as the overarching term & Kwan, 2000), or John Nesselroade (e.g., 2002).
does not entail that we attribute greater importance to the trait Content and structure have also been of great interest
approach. In the 1998 edition of the chapter, we had chosen
in a self-system approach to the study of personality. But
“self and personality” as a label. This, however, seems im-
the self-system approach has also been very much inter-
practical and is diverting from the goal to integrate the three
approaches. Therefore, we would like to suggest using “per-
ested in understanding the dynamics of personality
sonality” or “personality system” as the overarching term to (Markus & Wurf, 1987). Under the heading of the
denote the field of study comprising all three approaches. This self-system approach, we subsume lines of work that
is also in line with early personality theorists such as Allport characterize individuals as multifaceted dynamic struc-
or Murray who certainly did not link their usage of the term tures of a relatively stable array of self-conceptions
personality exclusively to the trait approach. (e.g., Baumeister, 1992; Greenwald & Pratkanis, 1984;
620 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Markus & Wurf, 1987). Self-conceptions are not meant and self-efficacy, or emotion-regulation. The focus of
to encompass any self-referent attitude but rather are such research is on investigating the self-related adaptive
confined to those beliefs or cognitions that constitute potential and the reserve capacities as well as their limits
important (fundamental) self-components. Whenever in the course of life span development. Because this field
the social meaning of such self-referent attitudes is in encompasses many different constructs, the group of
the foreground, the notion of “identity” rather than self- scholars engaging in this type of endeavor is quite large
concept is used (e.g., Waterman & Archer, 1990). Dif- and still growing. Thus, we can only mention a few labo-
ferent situations or contexts activate different subsets of ratories in order to illustrate the type of work we include
this composite structure of self-conceptions or self- under the heading of self-regulatory processes from a life
schemata. Markus and Wurf (1987) have called this the span perspective, such as the ones instigated by Brandt-
working self-concept. This view of the self-system as städter (e.g., 1998; Brandtstädter & Rothermund, 2003;
both stable and dynamic fits life span conceptions that Greve & Wentura, 2003), Cantor (e.g., Cantor & Fleeson,
emphasize the potential for continuity as well as change 1994), Carstensen (e.g., Carstensen et al., 1999), Filipp
as a characteristic feature of transactional adaptation (e.g., 1996), Labouvie-Vief, (e.g., Labouvie-Vief et al.,
during development. 2003), Lachman (e.g., Lachman & Weaver, 1998), and
In contrast to the trait approach to personality that Blanchard-Fields (e.g., 1996). Other examples are the
aims at inferring behavioral dispositions “ from the out- theory of selective optimization with compensation (e.g.,
side,” research on self-conceptions is often ( but not P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990a; Freund & Baltes, 2002b)
necessarily) related to what J. L. Singer (1984) has and the related endeavor by Heckhausen and Schulz
called the study of private experience or private per- (1995) to construct a life span theory of self-based de-
sonality, and Ryff (1984) has labeled as the study of velopmental control.
personality “ from the inside.” Operationally, however, A number of recent efforts have been made to inte-
at least most of the adult research of both traditions, grate these rather disconnected fields of research (e.g.,
that is, the trait and the self-system approach, rely on Cloninger, 2003; Hooker, 2002; McAdams, 1996; Mc-
self-report. Besides the classics such as Erikson (e.g., Crae et al., 2000). The life span focus in these integra-
1959) or Bühler (e.g., 1933), research programs around tive efforts clearly is on relating structure, content and
scholars such as Loevinger (e.g., 1976), D. J. Levinson process-related dynamics such that both stability and
(e.g., 1986), Ryff (e.g., 1991), Whitbourne (e.g., change characterize personality development during
1987), Dittmann-Kohli (e.g., Dittmann-Kohli, Bode, & adulthood (e.g., Roberts & Caspi, 2003; Staudinger &
Westerhof, 2001), Diehl (e.g., Diehl, Hastings, & Stan- Pasupathi, 2000). In the following section, we not only
ton, 2001), and Herzog and Markus (1999) focus on the present relevant information, but also attempt to inte-
life span development of the self-concept and of its grate the three approaches to the study of personality
adaptive qualities. within a life span perspective. As we attempt this inte-
Focusing on personality dynamics or the processes gration, a necessary by-product is that we may occasion-
underlying microgenetic personality change is yet a ally transform the foci that were at the core of the work
third approach, the study of self-regulatory processes of the original proponents.
(Carver & Scheier, 1998). Under the heading of self-
regulatory processes, we subsume all efforts that are
Key Features of a Life Span Approach to the
aimed at characterizing the organized abilities and
Study of Personality Development
skills a person brings to bear on monitoring behavior
and experience. With regard to life span development, it We define personality to denote the ways in which
is the regulatory behaviors of promoting growth as well human beings behave/act, experience, believe, and feel
as those of reaching, maintaining, and regaining psycho- with regard to themselves, others, and the material
logical equilibrium including in a context of age-related world. With regard to the sources and outcomes of
loss—in particular one’s sense of coherence, continuity, human development, personality has multiple causes
and purpose under conditions of microgenetic and onto- and functions (cf. principles of multicausality and mul-
genetic change—that are of particular interest. tifunctionality). First, personality develops, that is, it is
A host of constructs discussed in the literature can be the outcome of developmental processes. Different path-
subsumed under this heading, such as self-evaluative ways can lead to similar if not the same outcome. Sec-
processes, goal-related processes, coping, control beliefs ond, personality also operates as an antecedent for
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 621

developmental processes and co-regulates outcomes. Nesselroade, and others, we suggest that life span work
And finally, personality is the correlate of other devel- on personality development profits from integrating a
opmental processes. componential and a holistic approach.
Taking a life span view implies (a) that we are con-
The Search for General-Purpose Mechanisms in
cerned with the commonalities across individuals in how
the Area of Personality Development
personality develops. This is reflected in developmental
models like the one presented by Erikson (e.g., 1959) Throughout this chapter, we have emphasized the signif-
but also in theories about the driving forces and the icance in life span work of the search for the conditions
mechanisms of personality development (e.g., Bandura, of successful (adaptive) development. From the field of
1984; R. W. White, 1959). At the same time, as life span cognitive psychology, we have taken the idea of general-
developmentalists we are interested in ( b) the interindi- purpose mechanisms. Thus, we ask whether in the field
vidual dif ferences in personality development. For in- of personality as well it is possible to articulate general-
stance, do developmental trajectories become less and purpose mechanisms. With general-purpose mecha-
less similar as transactions between a given personality nisms, we mean (internal and external) resources and
make-up and particular environmental conditions accu- capacities that individuals employ to master develop-
mulate across the life span? Finally, we want to learn mental challenges in a variety of contexts and at differ-
about (c) the intraindividual variability or plasticity in ent stages of developmental time. General-purpose
the ways an individual behaves/acts, experiences, be- mechanisms in the field of personality might help the
lieves, and feels about him / herself, others, and the individual to organize and coordinate the ways in which
material world. Is it possible, for example, that an ex- he or she behaves, experiences, believes, and feels with
traverted person under certain circumstances behaves regard to him / herself, others, and the material world
more like an introvert? In extreme cases this plasticity such that the goal of maximizing gains and of minimiz-
can also result in system lability and lack of coherence. ing losses is approached. In our own work, the theory of
Do these plasticity-related phenomena increase or de- selective optimization with compensation (e.g., P. B.
crease with age, or do they remain unchanged? Baltes, 1997) is one such general-purpose mechanism;
These three questions of commonalities, interindividual located at a high level of aggregation.
differences, and of intraindividual variability (plasticity) There seems to be a foundation of research on which
can be pursued within a structural and a process-oriented to build when exploring the notion of general-purpose
approach to the study of personality and its development. mechanisms. Empirical evidence, especially when focus-
Under the heading of form or structure, it is primarily the ing on longitudinal studies that search for predictors of
classic personality dispositions and the self-conceptions, adaptive developmental outcomes, has identified a num-
schemata, or images that are considered. Under the head- ber of candidate concepts. Impulse control or ego control,
ing of process research, self-regulatory mechanisms are perceived control, delay of gratification and ego re-
most prominent. At least five categories can be distin- silience/flexibility, for instance, suggest to us that they
guished within that category: emotion regulation, control might possess the characteristics of such general-purpose
beliefs, coping, self-evaluation, and the goal system (goal mechanisms in the personality domain (e.g., Bandura,
seeking, goal pursuit, goal restructuring). 1993; Block, 1993; Caspi, 1998; Masten, 2001; Mischel,
Finally, all three facets of structure, process, and Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989; Rutter & Rutter, 1993; E. E.
function can be approached from a componential (mul- Werner, 1995). As is true in cognitive psychology, such
tivariate) and a holistic (ipsative) view. The componen- general-purpose mechanisms will not function by them-
tial approach is illustrated by evidence on the Big Five selves. Rather, they are part of a system of personality
personality factors (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1994; Gold- characteristics and self-regulatory mechanisms that are
berg, 1993). Block’s ipsative approach to personality functional or adaptive only under specific circumstances.
assessment (e.g., Block, 1995) is an inspiring example One reason why we chose to explore the power
of the holistic approach as is the conceptual and empir- of the notion of general-purpose mechanisms is the
ical work by Magnusson (e.g., Magnusson & Mahoney, relative openness of human development (Maciel,
2003). Taking a typological approach to the study of Heckhausen, & Baltes, 1994). From a life span point
personality development, for instance, by using cluster of view, there is no single endpoint of development of
analysis can also be subsumed under that rubric (e.g., any facet of self and personality. The challenge is to
P. B. Baltes & Smith, 2003). Like Magnusson, John coordinate resources under varying conditions. Thus,
622 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

there seems to be theoretical and empirical grounds There is first evidence accruing that the developmental
for the assumption that personality functioning is effi- model of selective optimization with compensation, in-
cacious if (a) many different ways of being are avail- troduced previously, also holds promise in this respect
able (e.g., being internally as well as externally (e.g., Freund & Baltes, 2002b).
controlled, being optimistic as well as pessimistic, Third, and finally, personality develops to serve a
being introverted as well as extraverted) and if ( b) self-reflective function. Personality reflects and evalu-
adaptive algorithms that monitor the dialectic between ates developmental changes in the other subsystems and
such supposedly antagonistic states are accessible tries to integrate them. This integrative and adaptive
(e.g., Blanchard-Fields & Norris, 1994; Colvin & function of personality is also mirrored in the fact that
Block, 1994; Staudinger, 1999b). By means of such al- subjective measures of adaptation such as subjective
gorithms the most functional personality characteris- well-being or happiness are even used as measures of
tic for a given time, place, and circumstance is self (e.g., Bengtson, Reedy, & Gordon, 1985). Self-
exhibited. This restates in different terms what has reflection is pivotal with regard to evaluating one’s
been characterized as a wise person (e.g., Staudinger, standing in the developmental matrix of possible life
1999a; Sternberg, 1998). The approach is also similar goals and outcomes (e.g., Staudinger, 2001).
to the notion of fluid intelligence in the domain of in-
tellectual functioning (see previous discussion). It is a Providing Links from Infancy to Old Age
special characteristic of that category of the intellect A further issue with regard to a life span perspective on
that in the sense of a general-purpose mechanism it the study of personality development concerns encom-
can be applied to (invested into) a large variety of cog- passing the ontogenetic course from infancy to old age.
nition problems (Cattell, 1971). Empirically and theoretically, this distance may often
still appear as a gap. Infancy and childhood research on
The Executive Function of Personality
the one hand and adulthood and old-age research on the
Life span theory further suggests a systemic view on psy- other typically proceed independently from each other
chological phenomena. This systemic view has at least with little overlap in concepts, methodology, and conse-
two consequences for the way we think about personality quently empirical data basis. Although much progress
(Staudinger, 1999b). First, we believe that the different has been made, especially by longitudinal researchers
components of personality, introduced earlier, together whose participants have grown into adulthood (Pulkki-
form the personality system. Dynamic systems theory nen & Caspi, 2002), the caveats expressed in the past
postulates that, by means of recursive interactions, such continue to exist: For example: Which measures are age-
components form the basis of self-organization as well as invariant? How to capture explanatory continuity when
the emergence and stabilization of new forms (e.g., Ford combined with descriptive discontinuity?
& Lerner, 1992; Thelen & Smith, 1998). Second, the sys- Bridging this gap and establishing connections indeed
temic view directs our attention also to the cross-link- is not easy (e.g., Brim & Kagan, 1980). It seems necessary
ages between personality and other subsystems of the to identify constructs that have been used to describe per-
developing individual, such as physiological and cogni- sonality development across the life span or have at least
tive functioning (e.g., Mischel, 2004; Pervin, 2001). As shown predictive relationships. This concerns the ques-
mentioned already, it seems that personality has some- tion of homotypic and heterotypic continuity, a terminol-
thing like an orchestrating or executive function with re- ogy introduced by Kagan and Moss (1962), or of Block’s
gard to these other systems and the developmental notion of temporal coherence (e.g., Block, 1993). The no-
changes occurring in these systems (e.g., Erikson, 1959; tion of heterotypic continuity implies that the phenotypic
Waterman & Archer, 1990). Besides the (dynamics) sys- behavior might change between childhood and adulthood,
tems approach to the study of personality, the psychology but that specific behaviors in childhood might still be con-
of action has been suggested as a unifying framework for ceptually consistent with adult behaviors. Phenotypically
the microgenetic study of the interplay of cognition, different but conceptually related responses might be de-
emotion, and motivation (Gollwitzer & Bargh, 1996). rivatives of earlier behavior (e.g., Moss & Sussman,
The extension of action psychology to issues of life span 1980). R. G. Ryder (1967), for example, found that child-
development has begun to be explored (e.g., Brandt- hood task persistence was related to adult achievement
städter, 1998; Brandtstädter & Rothermund, 2003). orientation.
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 623

There are a number of interesting candidates that serve ways that it becomes an outcome of a multicausal and
as examples of constructs that span a lifetime, such as at- self-organizing process.
tachment style, control beliefs, the self-concept, or tem- In sum, in this first section we have attempted to en-
perament. For temperamental features, for instance, gage the reader in an integrative view on research on the
extensive longitudinal evidence is available that has development of personality, self-concepts, and self-
demonstrated quite strong predictive links between tem- regulatory processes. In order to gain an understanding
perament in early infancy and personality in adulthood of the life span development of self and personality, it
and even old age. In this work, five temperamental fea- seems useful to consider structure and processes, as
tures are distinguished, activity level, positive affect, well as functions (Mischel & Shoda, 1999). A dynamic
negative affect, avoidance/approach, persistence. Longi- systems approach to development provides a useful the-
tudinal relations show that, for instance, activity level is oretical framework for the integration of the different
related to extraversion and lower levels of conscientious- components of personality discussed in the literature.
ness or negative affectivity predicts neuroticism and In addition, interest of a life span perspective centers
lower levels of agreeableness (e.g., Caspi, 1998; Friedman on features—such as temperament—that allow to study
et al., 1995; Kagan & Snidman, 1991; J. V. Lerner & continuity and discontinuity across the whole life span
Lerner, 1983). These kinds of predictive relationships, and also demonstrate predictive power from childhood
however, should not be interpreted as a deterministic ar- into adulthood. Based on this conceptualization, we will
gument. Rather, work by Chess and Thomas (1984) and now apply the level of analysis approach, introduced in
J. V. Lerner and Lerner (1983) demonstrated that depend- the beginning of this chapter, to the field of self and
ing on the “goodness-of-fit” between the child’s tempera- personality.
ment and expectations of the environment, temperamental
constellations can be aggravated or alleviated.
Illustrations of the Role of Biology and Culture
Recently, there has been notable progress in identi-
in Personality Development
fying some of the mechanisms that might link infant
temperament and adult personality and that might en- How does Figure 11.1 apply to the field of personality?
able a joint organization of concepts of temperament Working from the assumption that the genome and its
and personality (e.g., Strelau & Angleitner, 1991; expressions, by means of evolution, are not optimized
Zuckerman, 1995). Zuckerman (1995), for example, with regard to old age, and that genetic programs poten-
has proposed what he calls the turtle model of person- tially are less orderly and integrated for late than for
ality. In this model, personality traits at the top are young adulthood (e.g., Kirkwood, 2003), what are the
linked to genetics at the bottom through (from top implications for personality functioning across the life
down) social behavior, conditioning, physiology, bio- span? Recently, there has been growing interest in expli-
chemistry, and finally neurology. This model is not cating the evolutionary base of personality, both in
meant to be reductionistic; it is necessary to study each terms of biological and cultural-social forces in the
level of analysis with its own intellectual agenda to sense of co-evolution (e.g., Barkow, Cosmides, & Tooby,
gain a complete understanding. As Zuckerman (1995, 1992; Klix, 1993). This trend, however, is only begin-
p. 331) puts it, “We do not inherit temperamental pat- ning to reach the second half of life (e.g., Plomin &
terns as such. What is inherited are chemical templates Caspi, 1999).
that produce and regulate proteins involved in building As selection primarily operates through fertility and
the structure of nervous systems and the neurotrans- parenting behavior, most of the evolutionary work in the
mitters, enzymes, and hormones that regulate them. We field of personality (in the widest sense) has focused on
are born with differences in reactivities of brain struc- gender differences in altruism, cooperative behaviors,
ture and differences in regulators.” Very much in line sexual competition, or jealousy (e.g., DeKay & Buss,
with our systemic emphasis, it is included in this multi- 1992; Hammerstein, 1996). In addition, however, argu-
ple levels of analysis model of Zuckerman that the type ments have been raised that the ability for self-
of temperamental pattern, which initially may have a deception might have been favored by evolution because
strong genetic component, attains features of contextu- it seems to increase the ability to deceive others and
ally based self-organization. Thus, the temperamental thereby gain a survival advantage (e.g., Gigerenzer,
pattern is transformed by context and experience in 1996; Trivers, 1985). The evolutionary importance of
624 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

the ability to deceive others in turn is related to the cru- multiple assessment techniques. Highly complex statisti-
cial role of reciprocal relationships for reproductive fit- cal methods that allow modeling of the genetic architec-
ness (e.g., Axelrod, 1984). We would like to suggest that ture of development (Pedersen, 1991) by simultaneously
this ability “ to deceive oneself,” or one could also say taking into account mean levels and growth curves (e.g.,
“redefine reality,” indeed serves an important adaptive McArdle & Bell, 1999; J. R. Nesselroade & Ghisletta,
function across the life span and probably increasingly 2003) have become available. However, due to the lack
so in old age. of appropriate data sets and to the recency of their avail-
Thus, the fact that the evolutionary base has ability, they have not been widely applied yet. Therefore,
been less “optimized” for the postreproductive phases authors in the field of behavioral genetics consider the
of life than for younger ages may not be as detrimental available evidence as preliminary (e.g., Pedersen &
for the ontogenesis of self and personality as for bio- Reynolds, 2002).
logical and cognitive functioning. Perhaps what is rele- Taking such limitations into account, the following
vant here is that the “mechanics of the mind” which preliminary insights into the developmental behavioral
evince definite aging losses (see earlier discussion), genetics of personality seem to find consensus among
either carry little implication for personality function- behavioral geneticists (e.g., Pedersen & Reynolds,
ing, or that evolutionary selection in humans provided 2002). First, results of behavior-genetic analyses of per-
a better basis for personality than for intellectual sonality assessments are difficult to compare with the
functioning. equivalent analyses of intelligence assessments because
This interpretation of findings from evolutionary the latter are based on behavioral performance mea-
psychology, that personality is less at a disadvantage sures, whereas personality measures typically refer to
than cognition and biological functioning, is supported self-reports. Thus, strictly speaking, personality-related
by findings on the genetic component of interindividual analyses refer to the heritability and its life span
differences in personality functioning as advanced by changes in how people report about themselves. Second,
behavior-genetic research. Evidence from the cross- the extent to which genetic influences account for phe-
sectional as well longitudinal analyses of the genotype notypic variability in personality measures is smaller
and the phenotype of personality characteristics indi- than for measures of intelligence, with heritability coef-
cate that none of the personality traits is without a 40% ficients between .4 and .6 depending on the personality
to 50% genetic variance component. There are still too trait and the age of assessment. Third, the importance of
few data sets, however, that would allow disentangling genetic influences on interindividual differences in
methods effects. Mostly twin and adoption studies personality seems to decrease slightly with increasing
have used self-report data to assess personality. When age (e.g., McGue, Bacon, & Lykken, 1993; Pedersen &
comparing genetic variance components as derived Reynolds, 2002). And fourth, there is initial evidence for
from self-report with those based on peer ratings clear a quite high overlap in the genetic effects (i.e., stability)
differences emerge that suggest that observational as- operating on personality expression at different ages, al-
sessment and/or test-based assessment of personality though at each point in time they account for not more
may still yield other results. Based on the few data sets than half of the variance (e.g., McGue, Bacon, et al.,
that allow for multimethod testing it seems that genetic 1993; Pedersen & Reynolds, 2002).
factors largely account for what is in common across One of the more recent and exciting directions
assessment methods (Plomin & Caspi, 1999). Across for genetic research on personality involves the use of
the life span, genetic variance components seem to fol- molecular genetic techniques to identify some of
low a different pattern than the one just reported for the specific genes responsible for genetic influences
the domain of intellectual functioning. During the life on personality (Hamer & Copeland, 1998). It is too
course, stability or even slight decreases in heritability early to be certain, but it is possible that ultimately this
coefficients have been found (e.g., Pedersen & molecular genetic analysis will become more and more
Reynolds, 2002). prominent. That it will shift our attention away from
This very general summary statement needs qualifi- focusing on quantifying genetic influences to a focus
cation and differentiation. So far, only few behavior- on the causal mechanisms from cells to social systems
genetic studies of personality based on longitudinal data that will elucidate how genes affect and are affected
with extensive age intervals are available, let alone using by personality development. Currently, progress is
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 625

being made with regard to the molecular analysis of sonality characteristics) by referring to others and the
temperamental features such as approach/avoidance or physical and institutional context as resources (for
positive/negative emotional tone (see the following overview see Staudinger et al., 1995). Others can help to
discussion). In the long run, such molecular genetic do things that one’s own health or time or ability does
analysis may help to answer questions of heterotypic not allow. External memory aids can help to compensate
continuity much more clearly than currently is the case for the loss in memory performance. Given this line of
when referring to behavioral measures (Plomin & thought, one can also conceive of a situation that allows
Caspi, 1999). personality to optimize the use of external resources
such that enough internal resources are left for further
The Allocation of Resources in personality development, for example, toward wisdom.
Personality Functioning An extreme case for such personality-based orchestrat-
ing of resources aimed at selective optimization is the
In an earlier section, we emphasized the life span devel- loss of independent functioning. There the task is to ac-
opmental script of a reallocation of resources, from a cept dependency in such domains as household manage-
predominant allocation into growth to an increase in ment in order to free up resources for other purposes
relative allocation into maintenance, repair, and man- (M. M. Baltes, 1996).
agement of losses (see also Staudinger et al., 1995). In The notion that personality performs an orchestrat-
contrast to the domain of cognitive functioning where ing or executive function with regard to the management
resources in old age are depleted to maintain a certain and identification of resources raises the following ques-
level of functioning, the resource situation for life span tion: Is it possible to distinguish the mechanisms and
growth in self and personality might present itself more characteristics that support the overarching orchestrat-
favorably. Taking a system’s view on psychological ing or executive functions from those that constitute one
functioning, we can assume therefore that personality of the three domains of psychological functioning, that
by virtue of being the self-reflective head of the living is intelligence and cognition, self and personality, and
system “ human being” (a quality emerging during social relations, or are both inextricably intertwined?
childhood) might be able to continue to deliver its or- This question is discussed in research on resilience
chestrating or executive function with regard to manag- (Staudinger et al., 1995). By taking such a research per-
ing the gains and losses across various domains of spective on the origins, maintenance, and consequences
functioning until at least the third age (cf. Staudinger of personality—ideally in a longitudinal manner—it be-
et al., 1995). comes possible to identify, for instance, whether, or
It is less and less certain, however, whether, on aver- how, personality manages itself while at the same time
age, personality-related resources are available in old having to manage extraordinary challenges such as
age to promote further development of the personality- losses in cognitive functioning or losses of significant
system itself. In other words, as life reaches old age, others due to death, or to challenges of one’s own fini-
it becomes more and more necessary for available tude. Each process and each characteristic constituting
personality-related resources to be invested in managing personality can thus be identified as a phenomenon in it-
cognitive, physical, and social declines and losses. Pos- self but also in its executive and orchestrating function.
sibly only under very favorable developmental condi- We next discuss in more detail three issues of person-
tions would personality-related resources be sufficient ality development across the life span. The first is the
to invest in further development of personality itself. question of stability and change across the life span.
Although, in principle, life span changes in personality Aside from questions of stability in individual rank
could include advances, we do not expect them to occur order, this issue can also be phrased as investigating the
in everyone. Under very favorable conditions, however, gains, maintenance, and losses in mean levels in person-
personality growth might even involve such high goals ality functioning. The second issue relates to the oppor-
as wisdom (cf. Erikson, 1959; see also P. B. Baltes, tunities and constraints of personality development.
Smith, & Staudinger, 1992). And finally, the third issue will illustrate the adaptive
Further, we assume that the personality-system also personality-related potential across the life span, which
manages and organizes the extension of internal re- arguably might present the most comprehensive general-
sources (e.g., cognitive capacity, physical strength, per- purpose mechanism involved in life span development.
626 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Personality Development as Lifelong and Baltes found that people have quite clear conceptions
Transactional Adaptation: Continuity and Change about what they consider to be a desirable and what an
undesirable developmental outcome and also when it is
Before we explore further the question of continuity and supposed to occur. For example, only two desirable per-
change (gains, losses) in personality development, it sonality characteristics were reported to continue to grow
seems useful to consider the question what it means to in old age, that is wisdom and dignity, whereas many
speak about gains and losses in the context of personality other positive characteristics were mentioned as emerg-
functioning. Using the notions of growth and decline or ing for the periods of young and middle adulthood (Heck-
gain and loss with regard to personality characteristics hausen, Dixon, & Baltes, 1989). It was also found in these
makes the criterion problem of what is a gain and what a studies that people of different ages and socioeconomic
loss even more obvious and pressing than it is with re- backgrounds agree about how personality develops and
gard to intellectual functioning. In cognitive research, it about what is a desirable and what an undesirable person-
seems obvious that the more words one can remember, ality development, that is, what is a gain or a loss.
the better; the faster we can complete a problem-solving In the following, as we characterize gains and losses
task, the higher the level of performance. But even with in life span development of self and personality, we use
regard to intellectual functioning such criteria of adap- two approaches (see also Staudinger & Kunzmann,
tive fitness, of what is a gain and what a loss, are subject 2005). A first is based on evaluating developmental
to contextual conditions. When it comes to personality, changes with regard to the adaptivity and functionality
we are presented with the problem of determining a of the outcome for the individual, be it from a subjective
“ best ” direction of personality development. What is the (e.g., subjective well-being) or an objective perspective
desirable end state of personality development? Is there (e.g., longevity). The second refers to theoretical growth
one, or are there many potentially incompatible ends de- models of personality (e.g., ego maturity, integrity, gen-
pending on the outcome criteria we examine? To what de- erativity) when making a judgment about gains and
gree do subjective and objective criteria converge? losses. We alert the reader to the problem that these cat-
For example, let us take extraversion and assume that egorizations are preliminary and by no means absolute.
being extraverted is set as an aspired goal of personality Be it gains or losses, it is the core assumption of a life
development. We can think of occasions, however, span perspective that personality does not simply
when, on the contrary, introversion turns out to be the passively unfold as a consequence of the prewired
more adaptive personality feature. Similarly, it is very maturational programs or the mechanistic reaction to
important to strike a balance between affiliation and environmental stimuli. Personality develops out of a
solitude or between autonomy and dependence. Such constant and active process of the individual’s transac-
considerations remind us of the argument presented tions with changing internal and external influences, in-
previously about general-purpose mechanisms. We ar- cluding biological changes and changes in historical
gued there that it is the flexibility and the availability conditions of society. “ Transactional adaptation” (e.g.,
of a monitoring algorithm that is “ best ” with regard to R. M. Lerner, 1984, 2002) or person-environment inter-
self and personality functioning, rather than one or the action (e.g., Magnusson, 1990; Magnusson & Stattin,
other personality characteristic (e.g., Aspinwall & Chapter 8, this Handbook, this volume) are considered
Staudinger, 2003; Staudinger, 1999b). Similar views the central developmental processes. In this process of
can be applied to coping research. There, high domain- transactional adaptation of personality, systemic princi-
specificity with regard to the functionality of coping ples of self-organization are key ingredients. This basic
behaviors has been identified. Furthermore, coping be- life span premise about personality development is fur-
haviors that are adaptive as immediate responses need ther elaborated by the differentiation between the me-
not be adaptive in the long run. Thus, even with regard chanics and pragmatics of life and how it pertains to our
to coping, implications for everyday functioning are not understanding of personality development.
fully known (Filipp & Klauer, 1991).
One approach to deal with the question of functional- The Mechanics and Pragmatics of Life as
ity is to invoke subjective assessments, for instance, Relevant to Personality Functioning
about the perceived desirability or undesirability of a As described earlier the contribution of the mechanics
given self-related attribute. In a series of studies on be- and pragmatics as well as their dynamic interaction
liefs and expectations about development, Heckhausen is quite well understood with regard to cognitive develop-
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 627

ment (see also P. B. Baltes et al., 2006; Cabeza, 2002). (e.g., Rothbart, 2001). On the level of physiological indi-
With regard to personality, however, we are still at the cators, it is impossible to clearly separate from each other
beginning of understanding the interaction between me- aspects of the mechanics that underlie either cognition,
chanic and pragmatic elements in producing developmen- or emotion, or motivation. For instance, changes in heart
tal trajectories. The developmental trajectories of the rate such as acceleration can be observed during negative
cognitive mechanics and pragmatics are well established affective episodes, but also during mental arithmetic (cf.
at least in terms of the behavioral level. We also know Baltissen, 2005; Levenson, 2000). With regard to neu-
that with increasing age the cognitive pragmatics help to roanatomy, there is evidence for specialized brain areas
compensate functional deficits in the mechanics (e.g., (e.g., the amygdala, prefrontal cortex) that contribute to
Salthouse, 1984; see also P. B. Baltes et al., 1998; the formation of both basic emotions and basic motiva-
Staudinger et al., 1995). Do we expect to see similar de- tional tendencies but not to higher cognitive functioning
velopmental trends when investigating the mechanics and (e.g., Davidson, Jackson, & Kalin, 2000). A further dif-
pragmatics as relevant to personality functioning? ferentiation between emotion and motivation, however,
It may be useful to extend the notion of cognitive me- so far seems not possible. The emotion of fear, for in-
chanics and pragmatics to encompass personality func- stance, is inextricably linked to avoidance motivation.
tioning. Thus, instead of speaking of cognitive mechanics Temperamental dimensions also tend to show substantial
and pragmatics one may consider to use the more general interrelations, reflecting an underlying affective-motiva-
notion of mechanics and pragmatics of life (see also tional system rather than separate qualities (cf. Rothbart
Staudinger & Pasupathi, 2000). Clearly, this model is of a & Bates, 1998).
heuristic nature; that is, we do not assume that any phe-
nomenon in the area of personality can be assigned to The Pragmatics of Life as Relevant to Personality
only representing the mechanics or the pragmatics. Functioning. The pragmatics of life as they are rele-
Rather, we assume that it may be useful to categorize as- vant to personality functioning represent the power of
pects of personality functioning according to their rela- experiences and contextual influences. They encompass
tive position on the continuum between the mechanics self-related knowledge as well as self-regulatory compe-
and pragmatics of life. In the following, these more gen- tencies (Staudinger & Pasupathi, 2000). Knowledge
eral notions of mechanics and pragmatics are explicated about the self pertains to trait conceptions of personal-
with regard to their relevance for personality functioning ity as well as to the self-concept. It includes all that we
and development. know about our behavior, past experiences, anticipated
and idealized futures, needs and wishes, abilities, or
The Mechanics of Life as Relevant to Personality weaknesses that characterize our selves. The concept of
Functioning. The life mechanics relevant to who we are and what we are like is closely related to
personality functioning refer to a unique configuration how we pursue goals, evaluate our selves or adjust our
of elements that contribute to interindividual differences self-views or goals under threat. Thus, self-regulation
in self-concept, self-regulation, or trait personality such constitutes the procedural part of our self-knowledge.
as basic emotional and motivational tendencies as they
are studied in temperament research (positive/negative The Dynamic Interaction of the Mechanics and
emotional tone, approach/avoidance, novelty seeking; Pragmatics of Life as Relevant to Personality Func-
Schindler & Staudinger, 2005a; Staudinger & Pasupathi, tioning. The mechanics and pragmatics of life mutu-
2000). This configuration entails basic emotional and ally influence each other. As mentioned previously, we
motivational tendencies, as well as cognitive processes follow Cattell’s (e.g., 1971) investment theory and con-
that can be observed on the behavioral as well as on the sider the life mechanics as the building blocks promot-
neurophysiological level. ing developmental progress in the life pragmatics
The mechanics of life encompass complex interactions (Staudinger & Pasupathi, 2000). At first sight, it seems
between the cellular, neural, endocrine, and immunologi- that the mechanics constrain the pragmatics, and to a
cal system, which in turn provide the basis for basic be- certain degree that is true. But most genetic as well as
havioral indicators of cognition, emotion, motivation, recent brain research has demonstrated that, for in-
and behavior/action. On the behavioral level basic tem- stance, the richness or poverty of the (factual and proce-
peramental features such as emotional, motor and atten- dural) knowledge we accumulate feeds back into the life
tional reactivity as well as their regulation are considered mechanics and indeed may even change them (genetic
628 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

expression, brain structures; Kirkwood, 2003; W. ity trait measures when adults are assessed. In our ter-
Singer, 2003; see also S.-C. Li, 2003). Extremely inhib- minology, however, trait measures of personality are the
ited children, for instance, are able to gain control of result of many cumulative interactions between the bio-
their fearful behavior, changing not only their psycho- logical basis of personality, context and individual
logical state but also the underlying reactive sympa- choices. Thus, they are much closer to the pragmatic
thetic nervous system (Kagan, 1998). This reciprocal than the mechanic end of the continuum.
interaction of mechanics and pragmatics highlights the Behavioral operationalizations of basic temperament
limits of the hardware-software metaphor introduced dimensions clearly are closer to the mechanic end of the
earlier (at least as our current understanding of hard- continuum even though we need to be aware that the
ware is concerned). pragmatic component gains importance whenever self-
The life span conception of ontogenesis (e.g., P. B. report is involved (cf. P. B. Baltes et al., 1998; Kagan,
Baltes et al., 1998; Brandtstädter, 1998) as a product of 1998). Nevertheless, with regard to affective tone it is
the interaction between biology (i.e., life mechanics), possible to consult basic behavioral findings from life
culture, and automated as well as purposeful attempts at span emotion research. Also, there is some scarce be-
regulating one’s development (i.e., life pragmatics) im- havioral evidence on the approach/avoidance system
plies that it is impossible to clearly separate the mechan- stemming from research on the goal system.
ics from the pragmatics of life. Starting at conception, In addition, one can have a look at neurophysiological
biology, culture, and the developing “person” interact. indicators of personality functioning in order to learn
We just showed that, for instance, basic temperamental about the development of the mechanics of life. Two neu-
dimensions (i.e., mechanics) and personality character- rophysiological indicators have been—reliably and across
istics (i.e., pragmatics) show predictive relations across different laboratories—identified as biological indicators
the life span (e.g., Caspi & Silva, 1995). But we do not of basic dimensions of affectivity and motivation: (1) au-
know yet how exactly the underlying mechanics play out tonomic reactivity and (2) cerebral asymmetry (see also
in the development of traits. For instance, do changes in Schindler & Staudinger, 2005a). Those two indicators
the life mechanics result in the age-related decrease in seem to be rather “pure” reflections of the life mechanics
openness to experience (e.g., decay in the physiological given presently available measurement paradigms and
basis of the approach system, or reduced biological re- they have received most of the empirical attention.2 In the
sources “demand” parsimonious; i.e., habituated, rather following, we will first present developmental evidence
than novel functioning) or is it rather the result of years for the two neurophysiological indicators. Subsequently,
of experience (e.g., losing interest because I have seen it progressing from the mechanics a little further toward the
all before), that is, the life pragmatics? Or is it both? De- pragmatics end of the dimension, we present developmen-
spite the seemingly inextricable interaction, it may nev- tal evidence on the behavioral data about emotional tone
ertheless be useful for heuristic purposes to distinguish and about the approach/avoidance tendency.
between the life mechanics and pragmatics of personal- Autonomic reactivity ( heart rate, heart rate variabil-
ity and use this distinction to better understand person- ity). The parasympathetic and sympathetic branches of
ality development across the life span. We start by the autonomic nervous system influence the activity of
reviewing some first and still scarce evidence on the life the heart. Both higher sympathetic reactivity (e.g., Kagan,
span development of the mechanics followed by results 1998) and a weaker influence of the parasympathetic
with regard to the life pragmatics of personality. nervous system (e.g., Porges & Doussard-Roosevelt,
1997) have been linked to behavioral inhibition (i.e.,
The Development of Neurophysiological Indicators withdrawal /avoidance). Our focus is on the relationship
of Life Mechanics of Personality
When we consider the mechanics of life it is not trivial 2
These two indicators only present a subset of the physiologi-
to identify indicators that have a clear emphasis on the cal indicators of temperament and additional indicators that
mechanic side and that at the same time can be used to may be discussed in this context are excitability levels of the
assess participants across the whole life span. Espe- amygdala, asymmetric activation of the amygdala, norepi-
cially on the behavioral level, such as basic behavioral nephrine, cortisol, or dopamine levels (e.g., Davidson et al.,
indicators of temperament are usually not applied 2000; Depue & Collins, 1999; Kagan, 1998; Rothbart &
throughout the life span but are substituted by personal- Baltes, 1998).
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 629

between heart rate and heart rate variability on the one between autonomic reactivity and behavioral indicators
and basic dispositions such as approach/avoidance or of approach/avoidance or other personality indicators
positive/negative emotional tone on the other hand. across the life span.
On the one hand, a low and variable resting heart rate
is generally related to approach behavior and positive af- Cerebral Asymmetry. Over the last 15 years dif-
fect, but also to regulatory disorders and angry irritable ferent laboratories have developed theories and accumu-
affect (e.g., Porges & Doussard-Roosevelt, 1997; Roth- lated empirical evidence concerning the idea that
bart & Bates, 1998). These two findings seem contra- approach (Gray, 1981), activation (Cloninger, 1987), and
dictory, but match the special nature of anger. Albeit engagement (Depue, Krauss, & Spoont, 1987) motiva-
considered a negative affective state, anger is associated tion on the one hand, and avoidance, withdrawal (David-
with a motivation to approach rather than to avoid or son, 1984), and inhibition (Cloninger, 1987; Gray, 1981)
withdraw (e.g., Davidson et al., 2000; Harmon-Jones & motivation on the other hand are related to different
Allen, 1998). On the other hand, higher resting heart neural substrates, different basic emotions, and have dis-
rate combined with lower variability and high reactivity tinct influences on action. The approach/engagement
shows relations to inhibitory or avoidance behaviors system facilitates appetitive behavior, generates particu-
(e.g., Kagan, 1998). These findings highlight the neces- lar types of approach-related positive affect, and is re-
sity to consider the regulation of the heart rate in re- lated to relative increases in left-sided prefrontal
sponse to stimulation in addition to the resting heart activation. There is some evidence that dopaminergic
rate. Indeed, it is the physiological regulation of cardiac pathways play a central role in that system (e.g., Depue &
activity that has been suggested as “an antecedent sub- Collins, 1999). The withdrawal /inhibition system, in
strate for emotional, cognitive, and behavioral regula- contrast, responds to threats or signals of punishment. Its
tion” (Doussard-Roosevelt, McClenny, & Porges, 2001, engagement inhibits ongoing behavior (Gray, 1981) or
p. 58). Infants who have a low and variable resting heart supports withdrawal behavior (Davidson, 1984) and in-
rate and demonstrate appropriate heart rate modulation volves negative affective states such as anxiety, disgust,
tend to show more optimal developmental outcomes (at and heightened vigilance. The activation of the inhibi-
age 3) such as fewer depressive and aggressive behaviors tion /withdrawal system is associated with relatively
and more socially competent behaviors (e.g., Porges & stronger activation of the right prefrontal cortex.
Doussard-Roosevelt, 1997). Interindividual differences in baseline prefrontal ac-
During childhood stability of resting and stress- tivation asymmetry are related to differences in dispo-
tested heart rate as well as its variability reach close to sitional affect, inhibition, and differential reactivity to
perfect levels (Porges, Doussard-Roosevelt, Portales, & negative stimulation (Davidson et al., 2000). For in-
Suess, 1994). Mean-level decreases in resting heart rate stance, infants with higher relative right anterior corti-
and increased levels of variability have been observed cal activation at baseline are more likely to cry in
between 9 months and 3 years of age (Porges et al., response to being separated from their mothers com-
1994). Unfortunately, no studies investigating mean- pared to infants that do not show that asymmetrical ac-
level stability of heart rate and its variability from tivation pattern. Children with asymmetric right-sided
childhood into adulthood and old age are yet available. anterior activation show a tendency toward inhibited
But there is evidence that the resting heart rates of older behavior. During adulthood, greater relative activation
adults hardly differ from those of young adults, while of the right anterior cortex at rest has been related to
maximum heart rate is considerably reduced with in- higher levels of general negative affect, higher self-
creasing age (Folkow & Svanborg, 1993). Heart rate re- reported behavioral inhibition, stronger negative affect
activity is also attenuated in older adults (e.g., in response to unpleasant film clips, and slower recov-
Levenson, Carstensen, Friesen, & Ekman, 1991; see ery following a negative affective stimulus (for an
Baltissen, 2005, for an overview). In addition, once the overview see Davidson et al., 2000).
ANS is activated, there is at least some first indication Although there is evidence of internal consistency and
from emotion research that the activation tends to per- test-retest reliability of measures of prefrontal asymme-
sist for longer time periods in old compared with try (Tomarken, Davidson, Wheeler, & Kinney, 1992),
younger adults (Levenson et al., 1991). To date, how- our knowledge about the intraindividual development of
ever, no data exist about possible changes of the relation cerebral asymmetry especially over longer time spans is
630 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

still very limited. Rank-order stability of baseline pre- try in prefrontal brain activation. Please, note that
frontal asymmetries seems to be very low over an 8-year these changes in the mechanics by no means have to
period during childhood (Davidson & Rickman, 1999), play out as losses on the behavioral level but rather—as
but stability is assumed to increase after puberty when discussed next—the opposite may be the case.
the prefrontal cortex has stopped to undergo develop- Overall, our understanding of life span changes in the
mental change (Davidson et al., 2000). life mechanics of personality as well as their relation
Again, there is no study yet that compares the relative with behavioral indicators is still limited. We need stud-
magnitude of prefrontal asymmetry between infants, chil- ies linking differential age-related change in physiologi-
dren, and adults. Cerebral asymmetry has been demon- cal indicators to differential intraindividual change in
strated during the 1st year of life (cf. Davidson et al., personality. For instance, what happens to inhibited in-
2000), but it is unclear whether these interindividual dividuals who have a tendency to show strong heart rate
differences in brain activity stay stable during further acceleration in response to challenge when they reach
development. It has been speculated that the later devel- old age? We know that heart rate acceleration probably
opment of left-brain abilities might be accompanied by a declines with increasing age due to reduced reactivity of
maturational shift toward better emotion-regulation the ANS. But is there any age-related change in behav-
(see Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Similar to research on ioral inhibition as a result of declined heart rate reactiv-
heart rate and its variability, there seems to be hardly ity? Or what happens to emotion-regulation in old age
any evidence on the mean-level stability of prefrontal given the changes in brain activity discussed previously?
asymmetry in old age. We only found one study of odor Is there a mechanic basis to the age-related changes
perception in older adults that showed that left frontal found in emotion-regulation (e.g., Labouvie-Vief, Lum-
brain activation in response to pleasant stimuli was un- ley, Jain, & Heinze, 2003)? These and related questions
compromised. However, brain activity in response to un- are especially relevant when we describe and explain the
pleasant stimuli did not differ from that to neutral findings about the development of behavioral indicators
stimuli (Kline, Blackhart, Woodward, Williams, & of emotional tone and approach/avoidance that are much
Schwartz, 2000). This may imply that the right pre- closer to the pragmatics of personality next.
frontal cortex area undergoes stronger age-related losses
in functioning than the left prefrontal area. We will see The Development of Behavioral Indicators of Life
in the next section that on a behavioral level of assess- Mechanics of Personality
ment there is evidence accruing for differentially re- Emotional Tone and Reactivity. The physiologi-
duced frequency of negative emotions as well as reduced cal patterns are maintained, however the magnitude of
reactivity to negative stimuli. physiological responses seems to be reduced (e.g., Lev-
enson et al., 1991). Studies assessing emotion via self-
Some Conclusions about the Development of reported positive and negative affect, that is indicators
Neurophysiological Indicators of the Mechanics of much closer to the pragmatic side of emotion-regulation,
Personality Functioning. To date, there are few supported the view of a gain in affective functioning in
studies on either mean-level or rank-order stability of old age. Overall the subjective salience of emotion
the selected physiological indicators of the life me- seems to increase with age (e.g., Carstensen et al.,
chanics of personality and the majority of extant find- 1999). Negative affect has been shown to either stay sta-
ings come from studies on infancy and childhood. ble or decline in old age. Positive affect was found to be
Thus, we can only draw some very preliminary conclu- rather stable or even increasing with age depending on
sions about life span development. Substantial stability the study and the age range under investigation (e.g.,
coefficients are attained past adolescence. Thus, these Charles, Reynolds, & Gatz, 2001; Diener & Suh, 1998;
physiological indicators may possibly contribute to Kunzmann, Little, & Smith, 2000). Increased affective
continuity on the behavioral level. The mechanics un- complexity (i.e., more factors underlying emotional ex-
derlying basic motivational and emotional tendencies perience, plus higher potential for the co-occurrence of
show smaller mean-level changes than the cognitive positive and negative affect) and improved reported
mechanics across the life span. But there are some de- emotional control were also linked to increasing age
creases such as the declining physiological reactivity of (e.g., Gross et al., 1997). A common interpretation of
the ANS in old age and the possibly reduced asymme- those findings is an age-related emotional maturation as
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 631

a result of accumulated experience and knowledge about focus is expected to shift with increasing age from
emotions as well as a changing time horizon (e.g., growth, that is, trying to reach higher levels of function-
Carstensen et al., 1999), that is, changes in the life prag- ing, toward maintenance, that is, preserving levels of
matics of personality. Based on the findings about neu- functioning in the face of challenge, and regulation of
rophysiological indicators of the mechanics of loss, that is, organizing functioning at lower levels
personality presented earlier, we would like to offer (Staudinger et al., 1995). Inspecting findings from re-
some speculations about how those changes may con- search on the goal system (e.g., Emmons, 1996) that dis-
tribute to these changes in emotional tone and emotion- tinguishes goals focusing on gains, that is, approach
regulation observed on the behavioral level. goals, and goals focusing on the avoidance of loss, that
First, as noted, there is indication that ANS activity is, avoidance goals, can be used to test this hypothesized
(particularly the cardiovascular system) in reaction to developmental trend. And indeed in line with the as-
elicited emotions is smaller and refraction times are sumption, it has been demonstrated that growth (ap-
longer in older adults in comparison with younger adults. proach) goals were more frequent in adolescence, while
Further, an age-related reduction of the asymmetry of maintenance (avoidance of loss) goals increased in fre-
prefrontal brain activity seems to occur. These changes quency during middle adulthood and into old age (Ebner
(and we have only looked at a small selection of possible & Freund, 2003; Freund, 2002; Heckhausen, 1997;
mechanic indicators) may contribute to the changes ob- Ogilvie, Rose, & Heppen, 2001). Further, maintenance
served in emotional tone and emotion-regulation. For goals still increase in frequency during very old age (J.
instance, lower ANS reactivity may make it easier to Smith & Freund, 2002). Nevertheless, in spite of mid-
deal with upsetting emotional experiences. Decreased dle-aged and old people becoming more invested in
asymmetry in prefrontal brain activation during emo- maintenance and loss management, approach goals per-
tional experiences may contribute to differences in the sist throughout life (Ogilvie et al., 2001). When asked
relative frequency of specific emotional experiences about their future selves, even a majority among the
and may predispose toward the processing of a particu- very old consistently reported to pursue improvement
lar kind of emotional quality. goals across two measurement points (J. Smith & Fre-
Second, with regard to emotional control there is indi- und, 2002). And elective selection is the component of
cation that in contrast to self-report assessment, behav- the SOC theory that is maintained into old age (Freund
ioral measures of emotional control actually find no & Baltes, 2002b). This pattern of findings is replicated
age-related increases but rather that older adults seem to across different methodological approaches, such as
overestimate their own ability to regulate their emotions coding of goals by raters, self-rating of goals, and goal-
as measured by physiological indicators (Kunzmann, Kup- selection behavior. Further, with regard to a systems
perbusch, & Levenson, 2005). And there is first indication view on goals it was demonstrated that one goal could
that when using highly age-relevant emotional stimuli receive ratings on the approach as well the avoidance di-
(loss of a loved one) older people actually report to expe- mensions (Ebner & Freund, 2003). Relating these be-
rience stronger negative reactions (sadness) than younger havioral and self-report findings on approach and
ones (Kunzmann & Grühn, in press). Thus, it may be the avoidance to the evidence based on neurophysiological
case that certain constellations of emotional reaction are indicators it is striking that it is the right anterior cor-
practiced more often than others and thus by pragmatics tex, which is related to avoidance behavior that seems to
means (i.e., exercise) developmental trends based on the undergo stronger age-related declines in functioning
life mechanics of personality are “compensated.” In sum, suggesting that the goal-related approach/avoidance
we suggest that the interplay between mechanic and prag- findings are more closely linked with the life pragmatics
matics components of personality development is far from rather than the life mechanics of personality.
being understood but that the heuristic distinction be-
tween the mechanics and pragmatics of life may be quite Evidence on the Development of the Life
helpful in order to increase our understanding of emo- Pragmatics of Personality
tional functioning across the life span. In this last part of reviewing evidence on personality de-
velopment, we turn to constructs that on our mechanic-
Approach and Avoidance Goals. Corresponding pragmatic continuum are much closer to the pragmatic
to the age-related declines in the mechanics of life, goal- end such as personality traits, the self-concept but also
632 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

self-regulatory processes. A frequent distinction drawn meaningful comparison between different age groups is
in reviews of personality and aging (Kogan, 1990) is the solely possible if personality dimensions measure the
one between trait and growth models of personality same characteristics at different ages. For the Big Five
development. factorial model of personality cross-sectional as well
Radical trait theorists equate personality with per- longitudinal information on structural invariance is
sonality traits, that is, dispositional behaviors and attri- available (Costa & McCrae, 1994; Small, Hertzog,
butes. Some even argue that personality is “set like Hultsch, & Dixon, 2003). Such studies have demon-
plaster ” after age 30 (Costa & McCrae, 1994). Trait strated high structural invariance during adulthood
models of personality approach the question of continu- and into old age.
ity and discontinuity and stability and instability from According to a recent meta-analysis (Roberts &
the continuity side. Trait-oriented researchers are inter- DelVecchio, 2000), the rank-order stability of the Big
ested in exploring and possibly arriving at a structure of Five increases almost linearly across the life span with
personality characteristics that captures an individual’s stability levels in the .40 to .50 range during adoles-
experiences and behaviors in a way that is as compre- cence and a peak in stability at over .75 in middle
hensive and continuous as possible. adulthood (50 to 59 years). This peak of personality
Among a large number of trait personality re- consistency at age 50 to 60 contradicts earlier argu-
searchers there is consensus that personality can be rea- ments that the consistency of personality traits should
sonably well described by the so-called “Big Five.” The plateau after age 30 (Costa & McCrae, 1994). Compar-
Big Five have been identified by means of factor analy- ison among the five dimensions showed that extraver-
sis across different instruments and different samples, sion and agreeableness had slightly higher stabilities
though labels vary somewhat among authors. We have than the other three dimensions (see also Vaidya Gray,
chosen Costa and McCrae’s factor names to convey Haig, & Watson, 2002). In this meta-analysis (Roberts
the information: Extraversion, agreeableness, conscien- & DelVecchio, 2000), stability estimates did neither
tiousness, neuroticism, and openness to experience. vary by assessment method (i.e., self-report, other-
Growth models of personality, in contrast, like the report, projective test) nor by gender. Finally, control-
most influential one by Erik Erikson (e.g., 1959), con- ling for sample attrition did not alter the results. The
tend that we continuously adapt to changing internal and findings from this meta-analysis are confirmed with
external requirements and thereby grow. If all require- regard to old and very old age by recent publications
ments are successfully met, at the end of an ideal trajec- from two longitudinal aging studies (Mroczek & Spiro,
tory, Erikson envisions a person full of hope, will power, 2003; Small et al., 2003). In both studies, consistencies
purpose in life, competence, fidelity, love, care, and across 12 and 6 years, respectively, were around .7. No
wisdom. Clearly, this outcome is not the rule but rather evidence has been found for cohort differences in con-
the exception. sistency. Please note that even though a consistency of
More and more empirical evidence based on either of .75 at age 50 and 60 is high, on the assumption of
the two models, however, has demonstrated that both higher reliability than stability, it leaves room for indi-
stability and change (i.e., gains and losses) characterize vidual change. In this vein, recent studies using latent
personality development in adulthood and old age. growth modeling found that with increasing age indi-
Thus, the focus of this overview will be on presenting vidual differences in personality change also increase
this evidence and suggesting ways to better understand (Pedersen & Reynolds, 2002; Small et al., 2003). As
the dialectic between stability and change in personal- no age-graded increase but rather a decrease in the
ity development. relative importance of genetic effects on personality
development has been found, this increasing interindi-
Development of Trait Personality. When asking vidual variability of change most likely is related to in-
about stability or change of personality traits, this im- terindividual differences in life circumstances
plies three questions rather than one, that is, stability or (Pedersen & Reynolds, 2002). Alternatively, it may
change of (1) mean levels within a group of individuals, also be the case that overall system robustness is re-
(2) inter-individual differences between individuals duced with age and thus greater intraindividual vari-
(variance), and (3) of the structural interrelationships ability emerges (S.-C. Li, Aggen, Nesselroade, &
of dimensions of personality (covariances). To start, a Baltes, 2001; J. R. Nesselroade, 2002).
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 633

Finally, what happens with the mean levels of person- shared, life events. For instance, a recent cross-sequential
ality traits? Do we become less extravert and less open, study found that two cohorts differed in their develop-
but also less neurotic, as we move through adulthood ment of neuroticism between the ages of 70 and 75
and old age? Taking into account cross-sectional and (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003). The older cohort (1897 to
longitudinal evidence, it seems that neuroticism de- 1919) showed stability whereas the younger cohort
creases across adulthood (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003) and (1920 to 1929) showed decline in neuroticism pointing
may show some increase again very late in life (Small to the fact that aging might express itself differently in
et al., 2003). Some decrease is also found for openness different cohorts.
to experience and extraversion (e.g., Costa, Herbst, Mc- In sum, when we return to our heuristic dual-compo-
Crae, & Siegler, 2000). In contrast, agreeableness and nent model of the mechanics and pragmatics of life, we
conscientiousness increase to some degree (Helson & question the degree to which the Big Five are prototypi-
Kwan, 2000). cal elements of the life mechanics, as McCrae and others
This mean-level decrease in neuroticism, increase in (2000) suggest. In our view, the Big Five are prototypical
agreeableness and conscientiousness across adulthood elements of the life pragmatics. They are the result of an
and into old age can be described as an increase in social endless number of interactions between biology, cul-
adaptation, in the sense of becoming emotionally less ture, and the person. Thus, aside from stable genetic
volatile and more attuned to social demands (Helson & components, their relative high degree of stability can
Wink, 1987; Staudinger, 2005; Whitbourne & Water- also be located in the physical and social environments
man, 1979). The decrease in openness to experience, in in which individuals live and which, as sociological the-
contrast fits a different developmental pattern that is ories of human development and social differentiation
discussed next. suggest (Settersten, 2005), evince systematic and stable
McCrae and others (e.g., 2000) lately offered an in- interindividual differences. Therefore, in our view per-
teresting proxy of a longitudinal study. Comparing sam- sonality traits likely are much less of an exclusively bio-
ples between age 14 and 83 years from Korea, Portugal, logical nature than is assumed to be the case for the
Italy, Germany, Czech Republic, Turkey, they found more fundamental elements of the personality system
cross-country consistency with regard to the pattern of such as basic emotional tone and basic motivational
mean-level changes just described. The authors argue tendency.
that the observed similarity across cultures makes it un- Using a psychometric approach to the study of growth-
likely—given the very different historical and cultural aspects of personality replicates and extends the finding
circumstances in these different countries—that such of increased social adaptation just described for the Big
age differences are indeed cohort differences. Instead Five. Measuring dimensions such as “environmental
the authors suggest that this developmental pattern may mastery,” “autonomy,” or “personal growth” and “pur-
reflect changes in genetic expression selected for by pose in life,” Ryff and Keyes (1995) find that the first
evolution (McCrae et al., 2000). Thus, using the termi- two dimensions increase with age during adulthood and
nology introduced in this chapter, McCrae and others old age and the latter two level off after midlife. The in-
view those results as reflecting changes in the life me- creases in environmental mastery and autonomy can be
chanics underlying personality functioning. described as being highly functional and adaptive for
Knowing the many different ways that genes and en- mastering adult life.
vironment interact in order to produce stability (cf. Personal growth and purpose in life, however, level
Plomin, DeFries, & Loehlin, 1977; Roberts & Caspi, off in midlife. This developmental trend fits well with
2003) and also how important specific life experiences the decline observed in openness to new experiences in
are in personality development (e.g., Magnus, Diener, old age. It has been argued (Staudinger, 2005), that these
Fujita, & Pavot, 1993), we are somewhat reluctant in ac- observed declines in self-reported openness, personal
cepting this explanation as the only valid alternative. growth and purpose in life, may indicate that in contrast
For example, the transition to partnership during early to social adaptation, personal maturity is less likely to
adulthood is accompanied by decreases in neuroticism come with age for most people. And indeed studies of
and shyness and increases in conscientiousness (Neyer wisdom (e.g., Staudinger, 1999b) and of ego develop-
& Asendorpf, 2001). Thus, personality changes can also ment (e.g., Labouvie-Vief et al., 1987) find no norma-
be attributed to normative, in the sense of culturally tive increase with age during adulthood.
634 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Self-Concept Development. When studying the during adolescence, where no further decline in average
self-concept both content and structure need to be con- correlations between preadolescence and late adoles-
sidered (e.g., Filipp & Mayer, 2005; Staudinger & Pasu- cence has been demonstrated (cf. Marsh et al., 2002). In
pathi, 2000). Therefore, developmental trajectories of contrast, there is evidence that the self-structure be-
both will be covered next. comes more integrated again and that self-complexity
The content of the self-concept refers to the beliefs increases between 18 and 22 years of age (Elbogen
we hold about ourselves and to the domains we employ et al., 2001). For the age range from 20 to 88 years,
when describing ourselves. Answers to questions like cross-sectional findings indicate an U-shaped relation-
“Who am I?” (e.g., Freund & Smith, 1999) are indica- ship between self-concept integration and age (Diehl
tors of the content of the self-concept. The content of et al., 2001). Currently, there is not much evidence on
spontaneous self-descriptions shows change, but also the rank-order stability of self-complexity or differenti-
stability (Filipp & Klauer, 1986). There is, for in- ation. 1-week test-retest correlations for measures of
stance, substantial stability in the content of self- self-complexity and integration are in the .60 range
definitions across different age groups when it comes (Campbell et al., 2003). No knowledge about long-term
to central domains of life (e.g., health, social relations; stability is available.
Filipp & Klauer, 1986). But there is also change that In sum: It seems that mean levels of integration of the
can be attributed to the influence of a succession of de- self-concept increase until adolescence, decrease up to
velopmental tasks, critical life events, and also changes midlife and increase again thereafter. In contrast, the
in the life mechanics. During childhood the academic trajectory of self-complexity (in the sense of number of
self-concept and school achievement assume a central relevant self domains mentioned) follows an inverted U-
role, in adolescence we are very much concerned about shape function during adulthood. Thus, in contrast to
our physical appearance (for an overview cf. Filipp & the orthogenetic principle proposed by Heinz Werner
Mayer, 2005). With increasing age, however, people de- (1926), one of the pioneers of developmental psychol-
fine themselves more and more in terms of health and ogy, normatively there seems to be no coexistence of
physical functioning, life experiences, and hobbies complexity and integration. In this vein, recently an ef-
(Dittmann-Kohli, 1991; Freund & Smith, 1999). An- fort has been made to develop a measure of self-concept
other aspect of this adaptation to aging seems to be that growth, which incorporates this combination of differ-
activity and agency become important parts of the self- entiation and integration, and no significant age differ-
definition in old age, as they are no longer self-evident ences have been found during adulthood (Staudinger,
(Freund & Smith, 1999; Herzog, Franks, Markus, & Dörner, & Mickler, 2005).
Holmberg, 1998).
The structure of the self-concept refers to the organi- Development of Self-Regulatory Processes. Hav-
zation of the self-concept. This organization is often in- ing discussed some of the major evidence on the content
vestigated in terms of its complexity and integration (cf. and structure of self and personality, we turn next to re-
Campbell, Assanand, & Di Paula, 2003). One classic sults about the self-related processes. Self-regulation in-
definition of complexity (or differentiation) taps the cludes both the agentic (assimilative, primary control,
number of nonredundant self-aspects or content cate- problem-focused coping) and yielding qualities (accom-
gories of the self-definition (Linville, 1987). Integra- modative, secondary control, emotion-focused coping;
tion often is assessed as the correlation of trait ratings P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990b; Brandtstädter & Greve,
across different domains of the self-concept (Donahue, 1994; Brandtstädter & Rothermund, 2003; Heckhausen
Robins, Roberts, & John, 1993). & Schulz, 1995). Accommodative and yielding qualities
Cognitive representations of the self emerge during have consistently been found to increase with age (e.g.,
the 2nd year of life (cf. Harter, 1998) and can be charac- Brandtstädter & Renner, 1990). Most likely this is a
terized with regard to the degree of differentiation pragmatic response to the declines in the mechanics of
(measured in terms of correlations among different self- life. In contrast, theoretical predictions and findings
concept domains) as early as 4 to 5 years of age (Marsh with respect to the development of agentic and assimila-
et al., 2002). During childhood, the average correlation tive qualities are inconsistent. Different authors speak
among self-concept domains has been shown to become of declining (Brandtstädter & Renner, 1990), stable
smaller. This trend of decreasing integration levels off (Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995), and even increasing
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 635

(Heckhausen, 2000) primary control across adulthood. (powerful others) decline (cf. E. A. Skinner & Connell,
In line with findings of increase and stability of primary 1986). Further, stability and even increases in internal
control is evidence that demonstrates that older adults control beliefs with increasing age are observed. In
report elective, self-governed selection (within the adult samples, no clear-cut changes in perceived inter-
model of selective optimization with compensation) as nal control and a tendency for older adults to report
their primary action strategy (Freund & Baltes, 2002b). more external control are found (Brandtstädter &
Human beings are producers of their development Rothermund, 1994). A global measure of perceived
(R. M. Lerner & Busch-Rossnagel, 1981), but there are internal control demonstrates increases between young
also times when to accept the limits of our agency and to adulthood and old age (up to age 75; Lachman &
cope with challenges, threats and losses (see P. B. Baltes Weaver, 1998).
et al., 1998; P. B. Baltes & Smith, 2004; Brandtstädter, Recent findings from the Berlin Aging Study (a sam-
1998; Staudinger & Lindenberger, 2003). And indeed, it ple of old and very old adults) indicate that a high sense
may be that with age self-regulatory behaviors differ of internal control can be maintained into very old age,
markedly in their prioritization. In order for people to that is, in spite of losses and functional decline the old
maintain active involvement as composers of their lives, adults did not report diminished internal control over de-
it is crucial to believe in one’s ability to control and se- sirable outcomes. Simultaneously, however, mean levels
lect environments, optimize outcomes, and to have com- of perceived others’ control increased (Kunzmann, Lit-
pensatory strategies dealing with emotions in situations tle, & Smith, 2002). Thus, there seems to be a consistent
of failure and stress (e.g., Freund & Baltes, 2002b). In pattern of increasing externality in control beliefs with
the following, we are interested in the developmental increasing age. Results for internal control beliefs are
trajectories of various facets of self-regulation, such as less clear. We can conclude that beliefs in one’s ability
(a) internal and external control beliefs, ( b) emotion- to control desirable outcomes are relatively stable during
regulation, as well (c) goal setting and goal pursuit. adulthood and might even show some increases with
age. In other words, the belief in our agency “survives”
Control and Self Ef ficacy Beliefs. When consider- actual losses in resources. Age differences in domain-
ing the development of control beliefs and related be- specific control beliefs show that control over children
liefs such as self efficacy (Bandura, 1997), one variable and sex life decreases, while control over work, fi-
in the equation has to be the actual potential for control nances, and marriage increases between young adult-
afforded by current psychological and nonpsychological hood and old age (Lachman & Weaver, 1998). Control
resources. Based on the overall life span architecture over mental functioning and health declines in very old
presented earlier (P. B. Baltes, 1997), there is reason to age (Lachman, 1991).
assume that our capacity to influence outcomes in the Selected evidence on the rank-order stability of
external world follows an inverted U-shaped trajectory control beliefs points to moderate stability estimates
across the life span (primary control potential: Heck- ranging between r = .45 and r = .57 (Brandtstädter &
hausen & Schulz, 1995). Thus, the rise in objective con- Rothermund, 1994; Kunzmann et al., 2002; Lachman,
trol potential during childhood and adolescence may be 1986b). In sum, findings on the stability of control be-
reflected in increasing internal control beliefs, while liefs suggest increases (external control) or stability (in-
the declining control potential in old age may be accom- ternal control) and moderate stability in interindividual
panied by declining internal and increasing external differences across time.
control beliefs. However, the ontogenesis of control be-
liefs is a little more complicated than this assumption Coping. When we turn to age-comparative re-
would suggest. search on coping, Folkman, Lazarus, Pimley, and
If the two dimensions are considered separately, Novacek (1987), for example, found that older respon-
which is the current state of the art in research on con- dents were less likely to seek social support or use
trol beliefs (E. A. Skinner, 1996), interesting differ- confrontive coping and were more likely to use dis-
ences between the development of internal and external tancing and positive reappraisal. In fact, more and
control beliefs have been found. For instance, children more of the recent evidence supports this “growth”
from age 8 to 14 show no systematic mean-level change view of coping in adulthood and old age (e.g., Aldwin,
in internal control beliefs, but external control beliefs Sutton, & Lachman, 1996; Costa & McCrae, 1993;
636 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Labouvie-Vief, Hakim-Larson, & Hobart, 1987; Rott sidered another kind of relevant age-related change in
& Thomae, 1991) or at least speaks for stability in the goal system.
coping behavior. With respect to the developmental Thus, whether the goal system shows more change or
stability of coping behavior during adult life, it has stability seems to be an issue of perspective. Although
also been observed that individual differences in the change in the goal system is evident, continuity has also
endorsement of coping mechanisms are more a func- been demonstrated (Frazier et al., 2000; Smith & Fre-
tion of the type of stressful event than of age (Mc- und, 2002). For instance, across a 5-year period, people
Crae, 1989). in late adulthood and old age tended to consistently
Furthermore, older adults seem to be more flexible mention the same domains of possible (future) selves at
in adapting their coping response to the characteristics two measurement occasions (Frazier et al., 2000). And
of the situation (e.g., controllability) than younger even for the old and oldest old more stability than
adults (e.g., Aldwin et al., 1996). Such evidence is con- change was reported with regard to the domains of pos-
gruent with findings that, in comparison to younger sible selves across a 4-year period (Smith & Freund,
adults, older adults have been found to demonstrate an 2002). Obviously, as was true for our discussion of the
accommodative coping style in the face of adversity or trait literature, evidence suggests that continuity as well
failure; that is, older adults were more flexible and bet- as discontinuity play a role in age-related changes of the
ter able to adjust their strivings to changed circum- goal system.
stances than were young adults (Brandtstädter & Personal life investment (PLI ), one aspect of the
Renner, 1990). Conversely, younger adults were more goal system, is defined as the amount of energy and ef-
likely to adhere to their once established goals (i.e., as- fort (in terms of both acting and thinking) that people
similative coping), even if they were no longer realiz- invest in central life domains (e.g., Staudinger et al.,
able. With age, Brandtstädter and Renner (1990) have 1999). This broad conception of investment in terms of
demonstrated that adults favor accommodative (goal thought and action allows to capture all aspects of the
flexible) over assimilative (goal persistent) coping. In a motivational process, that is, goal selection, planning,
similar vein, Heckhausen and Schulz (1993) have pro- and goal pursuit, but also disengagement from blocked
posed and presented empirical evidence (Heckhausen, goals and restructuring of goal hierarchies. The devel-
2000) that an age-related shift from primary to second- opment of PLI in ten life domains ( health, cognitive
ary control strategies takes place in order to master the fitness, independence, well-being of one’s family, rela-
tasks of aging. tionships with friends, sexuality, hobbies, one’s occu-
pation or similar activities, thinking about one’s life,
Personal Life Investment (PLI) and the Goal System. and one’s death and dying) has been studied across the
Self-regulation is also reflected in goal setting and goal entire adult life span.
pursuit (cf. Cantor & Blanton, 1996). The goals people Cross-sectional evidence from a sample ranging in
hold at different points in their life and how they try to age from 14 to 103 years demonstrated that the invest-
achieve these goals in part are reflections of develop- ment patterns across these ten life domains indeed re-
mental contexts and developmental tasks. For instance, flect the developmental tasks of the respective life
although family related goals are prominent throughout periods (Staudinger & Schindler, 2005). Certainly, age
adulthood, young adults frequently mention marriage and its related social and biological demands is only
and family goals, whereas the goals of middle-aged one developmental context influencing investment pat-
adults relate to the lives of their children (Nurmi, 1992). terns. In addition, socioeconomic characteristics and
Another well-established finding is that occupational personality play an important role in moderating age-
goals lose priority with increasing age, while health- related differences in investment patterns. Finally, a
related pursuits do gain (e.g., Frazier, Hooker, Johnson, distinction between investment related to age-graded
& Klaus, 2000). Ryff (1989b) demonstrated that younger societal and biological demands (obligatory) and self-
people strive for accomplishment and career, whereas selected investment (optional) turned out to be useful.
older people are more likely to aim at good health and These two types of investments follow stable and de-
the ability to accept change. In another vein, Riediger, clining trajectories, respectively, as analyses with the
Freund, and Baltes (2005) have suggested that the de- longitudinal data from the BASE showed (Schindler &
gree of goal convergence and interference may be con- Staudinger, 2005b).
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 637

Summary on Stability and Change in Personality is because presenting research on aging permits us to
across the Life Span elaborate more clearly the theme of gains and losses and
Considering the evidence on the development of person- the dynamic of differential allocation of resources into
ality structure presents only half of the picture. Without growth, maintenance, and management of losses.
the investigation of personality processes, we may be We discussed that there is little or no correlation be-
lead to think that personality and aging is about some in- tween age and trait-based personality structure. In a sim-
creases in social adaptation and decreases in personal ilar vein, there is little correlation between age and
growth. Taking self-regulation and developmental regu- various self-related indicators of adaptation (e.g., P. B.
lation into account, however, “complicates” the story. Baltes, 1993; Brandtstädter, Wentura, & Greve, 1993),
Yes, there is stability that can be associated with endur- including self-esteem (e.g., Bengtson et al., 1985), sense
ing genetic and contextual influences. And there is a of personal control (e.g., Lachman, 1986b), or happiness
psychological need for coherence and stability that al- and subjective well-being (e.g., Costa et al., 1987; Ryff,
lows us to have a sense of enduring self. Stability, how- 1989a). This also includes 70- to 80-year-olds (J. Smith
ever, should not be completely equated with standstill. & Baltes, 1993). Only in advanced old age, do we seem to
Rather, stability is also reflective of resilience. Consid- observe more salient changes toward a lower level of de-
erable regulatory processes “produce” that stability. In sirable functioning in trait-like dispositions (P. B. Baltes
addition to biological changes, change on the structural & Smith, 2003; J. Smith & Baltes, 1999). Thus, on the
level of personality occurs when self-regulation cannot group level, for the larger part of the adult age spectrum,
withstand the pressure for modification any longer. In age does not seem to be a “risk ” factor for these aspects
the future, we need to better understand the biological of the personality system (note, however, that age chal-
underpinnings of personality functioning and how they lenges become larger when comparison with younger ages
change with age and interact with contextual features are requested).
and individual choices. The absence of strong relationships between age and
self-related indicators of well-being, despite what we
Reserve Capacities of Self and have characterized above as an increase in risks and po-
Personality Development tential losses with advancing age, but also for certain es-
pecially disadvantaged groups, is theoretically and
As a further life span problematic, we have selected the methodologically important. Indeed, the discrepancy
notion of reserve capacity and resilience for further between an increasing number of risks on the one hand,
elaboration. This notion gains special importance for a and maintenance of adaptive functioning in the self on
functional perspective on personality development, that the other, is perhaps one of the most persuasive indica-
is, for perspectives that aim at an analysis of the orches- tors of the power of the personality system in dealing
tration of self and personality as a system of adaptive with reality (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990b; Greve &
functioning. The topic of reserve capacity and resilience Staudinger, in press; Staudinger et al., 1995). It is sug-
in the domain of self and personality relates to the allo- gested that the personality system exhibits resilience, or
cation of resources introduced earlier but discusses this reserve capacity, in the face of age-related risks and pri-
theme at a more microanalytic level of analysis. What marily health-related losses. In a similar way, it has
are the self-related mechanisms and characteristics that been argued in childhood research conducted in the
either show or contribute to reserve capacity? field of developmental psychopathology that certain self
Traditionally, the central role of reserve capacity, or and environment constellations allow maintenance of
related concepts such as resilience, is articulated within adaptive development even in the face of adversity (e.g.,
the province of child development (e.g., Cicchetti & Garmezy, 1991).
Cohen, 1995). More recently, this view was extended to Multiple arguments can be presented to understand
include adulthood and old age (e.g., Brandtstädter, Wen- this discrepancy between an age-related increase in
tura, & Rothermund, 1999; Staudinger et al., 1995; Vail- risks and stability in self-related indicators of well-
lant, 1993). For the present purpose we have chosen being (Staudinger, 2000). First and foremost, the self
aging as a forum of illustration. We make this selection applies various protective mechanisms to reinterpret
for several reasons. One is the relative novelty of this or transform reality in the interest of maintaining or
age period for researchers in child development. Another regaining levels of well-being (e.g., M. M. Baltes &
638 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Baltes, 1986; Filipp, 1996; Greve & Wentura, 2003). 1987). One possible explanation for the contradictory
Second, age is only a rough proxy of increasing risks; not finding with regard to cheerfulness and optimism might
everyone of a given age cohort needs to be concerned by be that cheerfulness in adolescence is linked to certain
them. Therefore, the negative effects need not necessar- risk behaviors whereas optimism in adulthood is related
ily show on a group level. Third, as just argued, the self to higher levels of positive emotions, which have protec-
has a strong interest in continuity and growth. Over a tive effects in the face of stress (e.g., Fredrickson &
given period of time, the self adapts to even adverse cir- Levenson, 1998). As very few life time studies are avail-
cumstances as if nothing or not much has happened. able, our knowledge about the long-term protective per-
Thus, for researchers interested in the “self at work,” it sonality profile to date is still limited. Nevertheless, it
seems crucial at which point in this adaptive process the seems safe to say that across a number of studies, posi-
assessment takes place. Fourth, the changes due to in- tive expressions of the following personality character-
creasing risks may be chronic rather than acute and istics have been demonstrated to mostly contribute to
therefore might not affect the self suddenly, but gradu- positive outcomes, thus, can be called general-purpose
ally. It may be difficult, therefore, for the self to recog- mechanisms (cf. Friedman et al., 1995; Helson & Wink,
nize them and reflect them in self-report measures. 1987; Manners & Durkin, 2000; Peterson, Seligman,
In the following, we will illustrate the reserve capac- Yurko, Martin, & Friedman, 1998): Conscientiousness,
ity of the personality system by citing select findings or- extraversion, openness to experience, behavioral f lexibil-
dered according to the distinction introduced above ity, ego resilience, ego level, internal control or agency
between (a) form and structure of the personality sys- (ef ficacy) beliefs, and cognitive investment.
tem and ( b) self-regulatory and self-transformational Another strand of research focuses less on trait-based
processes. Except for few studies with objective indica- personality characteristics than on the structure and
tors of adaptivity, such as longevity or professional suc- content of self-conceptions. Evidence is accruing that a
cess, in most of the studies adaptivity is measured by positive, multifocal and diversified, yet integrated
self-report indicators of well-being. First, with regard to structure of priorities and self-conceptions, or identity
form or structure we will highlight information on dif- projects, makes transactive adaptation to developmental
ferences in the adaptive fitness of personality and self changes easier (e.g., Diehl et al., 2001; Freund, 1995;
profiles. Second, we will select evidence on the adaptive Riediger et al., 2005).
value of self-regulatory processes in three domains for It is recent work that has shown that understanding
further illustration of life span developmental changes: the adaptivity of self-definitions is more complex than
(1) goal seeking and reorganization; (2) self-evaluative first thought. Combining dimensions like quantity, rich-
comparison processes; and (3) coping. Finally, note that ness, positivity, integration, and importance of self-
the protective mechanisms described in the following definitions, as well as differentiating real and potential
are an inherent part of theories of successful develop- selves in one theoretical framework will be an important
ment such as the theory of selective optimization with step in capturing the adaptive life span dynamics of
compensation (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990b). self-knowledge. Very important also seems a more ex-
plicit recognition of contextual factors including issues
Evidence for Reserve Capacity in Indicators of
of collective self-concepts such as among spouses
Personality Structure
(M. M. Baltes & Carstensen, 1999; Hermans, 1996).
There is longitudinal and cross-sectional evidence that
individual differences in largely stable personality char- Evidence for Reserve Capacity in Self-Regulatory
acteristics contribute to level of adaptation and some- Processes: The Goal System and Self Evaluation
times even advances. From a life span perspective, it is Selection of Goals and Life Priorities. Life span
important to note that the adaptive patterns differ some- theory (P. B. Baltes, 1997) emphasizes the critical im-
what depending on the first time of assessment (i.e., portance of selection of domains and life priorities for
adolescence or adulthood) and thus whether short- or effective regulation of developmental processes such as
long-term predictions are involved. This applies espe- advancement, maintenance, recovery and regulation of
cially to two traits widely assumed to constitute a risk or loss. In this sense, personality traits as well as possible
a protective factor, that is, neuroticism and optimism or selves act as motivational sources, and are linked to
cheerfulness (Friedman et al., 1995; Scheier & Carver, goals that are either strived for or avoided.
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 639

The evidence on life span development of life priori- less well-being (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 2003; Elliot,
ties and personal life investment patterns reported Sheldon, & Church, 1997). Most of this research is done
above indeed points to selection into individual life con- with young adults and much less is known about
texts and the importance of internal and external con- changes over the life span. Due to reduced resources
texts in defining salient features of the self across the and increased risks, avoidance goals seem to be more
life span (see also Brandtstädter & Rothermund, 1994; prevalent in later life and lose the dysfunctional effect
Cantor & Fleeson, 1994; Carstensen, 1995; Staudinger which they evince in young adulthood (Freund & Ebner,
& Schindler, 2005). Socioemotional selectivity theory, in press). In old age it is the maintenance goals that
for instance, argues for systematic and adaptive life demonstrate the highest adaptive value.
span changes in social goals over the life span (e.g., The adaptiveness of goal investment is also altered
Carstensen et al., 1999). So, temporal constraints like by life circumstances. Given highly restrictive life cir-
impending end of life may shift the criteria used for se- cumstances, such as major health constraints, concen-
lecting social relationships, requiring a corresponding tration on a few selected goals rather than many helps
change in the criteria for judging a particular relation- to sustain levels of subjective well-being (Staudinger &
ship as adaptive (e.g., Carstensen et al., 1999). Fleeson, 1996). This finding has been replicated
Beyond the social realm, the adaptive value of life with longitudinal data from the Berlin Aging Study
priorities in general seems to change. For example, older (Schindler & Staudinger, 2005b). When considering a
adults find meaning in life predominantly by searching life span sample, it was found that it is during middle
for “contentment,” whereas younger adults report adulthood and into old age that the exact composition
searching for “ happiness” (Dittmann-Kohli, 1991). of the investment pattern (i.e., the selection pattern)
Younger people tend to assess their subjective well- contributes significantly to subjective well-being (Staud-
being in terms of accomplishments and careers, whereas inger & Schindler, 2005).
older people associate well-being with good health and
the ability to accept change (Ryff, 1989a). These The Adaptive Value of Self-Evaluations. In ad-
changes are highly adaptive and illustrate the impor- dition to changes in content, ranking, and valence of
tance of flexibility—giving up or reducing investment in self-concepts and goals, self-evaluative processes
those roles and commitments that are no longer avail- can be considered protective or risk factors. Three
able, and investing in commitments which fit current motives of self-evaluation (see Fiske & Taylor, 1991)
conditions of living (e.g., Brim, 1992; Dittmann-Kohli, can serve protective functions—self-verification, self-
1991; Freund & Baltes, 2002b). Flexibility in goals and enhancement (e.g., Taylor & Brown, 1988), and self-
investments, or priorities, is of course facilitated by a improvement (e.g., Taylor, Neter, & Wayment, 1995).
rich variety of self-defining concepts to select from and General statements such as “positive illusions are
prioritize. In this sense, a rich variety of interrelated but adaptive” are simplistic (cf. Taylor & Brown, 1988;
well-articulated life goals is part of a person’s develop- Colvin & Block, 1994; Baumeister, 1989). It’s important
mental reserve capacity (cf. Staudinger et al., 1995; to know when positive illusions are adaptive (e.g., at
Riediger et al., 2005). which point in the action sequence; see e.g., Schwarz &
In addition to the repertoire and selection of goals, Bohner, 1996). It may, for instance, be adaptive to have
other facets of goal pursuit also relate to adaptation. positive illusions before the action is completed to main-
Achieving a goal is usually adaptive, but the meaning- tain motivation. But it may be dysfunctional to maintain
fulness of the goal and the degree of commitment to it positive illusions during implementation and when inter-
may enhance or limit that adaptivity (see also Brun- preting outcomes, because this reduces the likelihood of
stein, 1993; Emmons, 1996). Further, one must act; one an adequate response (Oettingen, 1997). Consistent with
study demonstrated that the relationship between peo- this reasoning, people think more realistically when set-
ple’s goals and well-being was primarily mediated ting goals than when implementing them (Taylor & Goll-
through doing more in the selected domain (Holahan, witzer, 1995). The particular content of the illusions is
1988; see also Harlow & Cantor, 1996). Recent evi- also important. For example, positive expectations about
dence suggests that the pursuit of approach goals (or behavior outcomes contrasted with negative fantasies
hoped-for selves) is related to greater well-being, while about the same outcome can result in the best behavioral
that of avoidance goals (or feared-for selves) relates to outcome (Oettingen, 1996). Such unresolved recent issues
640 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

qualify the existing literature on the adaptivity of self- ferent times in the life span constitute an important
evaluations, which we address next. resource for the self. As noted earlier, evidence on life-
time trajectories of social and lifetime comparisons
Self-Evaluation: Social Comparisons. Goals shift in remains scarce. In one study, higher frequency of
any activity during the life span, and those shifts lead to future-oriented comparisons by older participants was
shifts in the selection and weighting of comparative in- related to lower well-being (Fillip & Buch-Bartos,
formation (Bandura & Cervone, 1983; Frey & Ruble, 1994). In contrast, drawing on past successes in diffi-
1990). Individuals also modify their self-evaluative cult situations can produce adaptive outcomes (Aldwin,
standards within a given domain in order to adapt to de- Sutton, Chiara, & Spiro, 1996; see also Staudinger &
creases in their behavioral competence or negative Fleeson, 1996). Obviously, it is critical to distinguish
changes in their health condition, thus maintaining sta- between upward and downward temporal comparisons,
bility in their self-views (Buunk & Gibbons, 1997; Frey as past and future comparisons can involve standards
& Ruble, 1990). of better or worse functioning. It is not the temporal
Social comparison and other forms of interactive comparison per se that is protective or damaging;
minds (P. B. Baltes & Staudinger, 1996a) are one im- rather, depending on the characteristic or domain, and
portant mechanism of self-regulation (e.g., Wood, on the point in the self-regulatory process, lifetime
1996). New reference groups are selected or sometimes comparisons can result in an enhanced self-evaluation
even constructed in order to permit a reorganization of or a sense of loss and decline. Selectively attending to
personal standards of evaluation (e.g., Buunk, 1994). positive aspects of the self at different points in the
Downward comparisons, in which individuals compare lifetime can support a positive sense of self at the pres-
themselves to people who are worse off in a relevant do- ent. The endorsement of selective lifetime comparisons
main of functioning, may become more important with may contribute to the lack of age differences in concur-
age, increasing levels of risk, or losses that cannot be rent self-evaluations.
remedied through instrumental action (e.g., Filipp & Longitudinal work has demonstrated that self-
Mayer, 2005; Heckhausen & Krueger, 1993; Heidrich & perceived personality change may be biased by what
Ryff, 1993). Of course, little is known about the level of one might call “ time enhancement ” (Woodruff &
consciousness at which people make such comparisons Birren, 1972). Participants perceive improvement as
in everyday life. compared to their own past, whereas the actual ratings
The downward comparison story is not as simple as collected at the two occasions (25 years apart) did
it seems, however (see also Wood, 1996). The opera- not reveal significant change (see also Ross, 1997).
tionalization of downward social comparisons varies Unfortunately, no interactions with age in this ten-
markedly between studies. Some studies evaluate spon- dency to upgrade the past were tested. Other work on
taneous reasons for self-evaluations provided “on- self-perceived personality change shows that discrep-
line,” which are later coded for comparison standards. ancies between ratings of past, present, future, and
Other studies ask in retrospect for the frequency with ideal personality are especially pronounced for young
which social upward, downward, and lateral compar- adults and hardly existent for older adults (Ryff, 1991;
isons are made and relate this to measures of well- see also Ross & Bühler, 2001). It seems adaptive—and
being (e.g., Filipp & Buch-Bartos, 1994). Still other in line with respective developmental tasks—for young
studies have participants rate themselves and a gener- adults to strive for improvement and thus perceiving
alized other on certain personality dimensions, and the past and the future to be of a different category
then indirectly infer upward or downward comparisons (contrast effect; Schwarz & Strack, 1999). While for
(e.g., Heckhausen & Krueger, 1993). As suggested older adults, with decreasing resources, it seems adap-
above, the most critical issue for adaptivity may be the tive to conceive of the future as being close to the pres-
use of the most functional comparison at the appropri- ent and the past and perceiving them as belonging to
ate time during the person-situation transaction, one category (assimilation effect; Schwarz & Strack,
something seldom addressed in these studies. 1999). In this vein, a recent study found that in old age
perceptions of ourselves in the past and the future have
Self-Evaluation: Lifetime Comparisons. Besides a stronger predictive value for our well-being in the
social comparisons, comparisons with oneself at dif- present than in young and middle adulthood, which
Second Level 5 Example: The Study of Personality Development across the Life Span 641

may in fact contribute to perceiving fewer changes endorsing some coping styles very strongly and others
(Staudinger, Bluck, & Herzberg, 2003). not at all, also demonstrate high levels of well-being
Persons’ current view of themselves may not be mean- (Staudinger & Fleeson, 1996). Similar findings are re-
ingful unless we also understand how they believe they ported for coping with depression in old age. Rather
once were and will become. Being moderately ex- than any particular form of coping, better mental health
traverted in the present when having been very ex- means being able to choose from several different
traverted in the past has different implications for responses (Forster & Gallagher, 1986). In a similar
well-being than being moderately extraverted in the pres- vein, the integrated multiplicity and selectivity (with
ent and introverted in the past. Fleeson and Baltes (1998) regard to importance) of self-definitions has protective
showed that past and future ratings of personality pre- value, and social relations with multiple functions are a
dicted well-being above and beyond present ratings. richer resource than other types of relationships (for
When only current personality is assessed, the absence of an overview, see Staudinger et al., 1995). This evidence
information about change may mask relationships be- suggests that access to, and flexible selection from,
tween personality perceptions and well-being (see also a repertoire of regulating functions or characteristics
Fleeson & Heckhausen, 1997). Similar cautions apply to (e.g., coping, self-definitions, functions of a relation-
goal assessments. Regrets about past, unattained goals ship, life investments) may be a key resource used by in-
predict lower subjective well-being over and above cur- dividuals in proactive adaptation.
rent goal ratings and general tendencies toward negative People obviously show highly adaptive coping behavior
affect (Lecci, Okun, & Karoly, 1994). well into old age. In contrast to stereotypical views of the
elderly as rigid, the evidence based on social-cognitive
Coping and Control Strategies: Further processes of self-representation, self-regulation, and self-
Evidence for Reserve Capacity Related to enhancement, points to a substantial capacity for adjust-
Self-Regulatory Processes ment to and mastery of life’s demands. Of course, this
In contemporary contextual models of coping (e.g., capacity for adaptation may find its limits in extreme
Brandtstädter & Greve, 1994; Filipp, 1999; Heckhausen situations, such as the challenges of advanced old age
& Schulz, 1995; Lazarus, 1996; Staudinger et al., 1995) (P. B. Baltes & Smith, 2003; J. Smith & Baltes, 1999).
questions of adaptivity are conditioned on the particu- Many facets of coping and control processes remain
lar situation, including all its inherent constraints and uninvestigated or poorly understood, however, includ-
demands. Whether a coping behavior is adaptive de- ing the microgenesis of coping processes (e.g.,
pends entirely on who does it, in response to which Lazarus, 1996). To outline one example—findings sug-
stressor, and in which situation the behavior occurs. In gest that the critical factors in success at quitting ciga-
this vein, increasing evidence highlights the importance rette smoking involve being reflective and thoughtful
of context. For example, depending on a person’s level (e.g., emotion /self-focused) at a planning stage and
of physical impairment, different coping styles are re- then problem-focused and behavioral during the actual
lated to subjective well-being (Aldwin & Revenson, quitting process (Perz, DiClemente, & Carbonari,
1987; Staudinger & Fleeson, 1996). What has been la- 1996). As in the case of self-evaluative cognitions, it is
beled “regressive” and thus dysfunctional coping under also crucial to focus on the timing of coping. We need
“normal” living circumstances (e.g., “I like someone to to consider which coping behavior is exhibited at which
take over,” “denial,” “I give up”) is functional under point in the coping process (e.g., Suls, David, & Har-
conditions of physical impairment. Thus, older adults’ vey, 1996). For instance, longitudinal studies in the do-
“regression” in coping styles may be adaptive, given main of coping with cancer (e.g., Filipp, 1999) and
higher incidences of physical constraints (Staudinger with death of a close person (e.g., Wortman & Silver,
et al., 1999). Of course, even “normally” adaptive be- 2001) demonstrated that coping strategies such as
haviors are noneffective if not well executed (e.g., Suls “minimizing the threat ” and “ wish for someone to take
& David, 1996). over ” maintained their adaptive power across a period
Beyond the adaptivity of specific coping behaviors, it of 9 months after the critical event. In contrast, the
is important to have multiple coping options to choose strategy “rumination” only showed positive relations
from, while retaining some selectivity. Those old indi- during the first 3 months and turned into being mal-
viduals who report selective flexibility in coping, that is adaptive thereafter.
642 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Summarizing Foci and Facets of Personality to represent the mechanics of life, that is, heart rate
Development across the Life Span activity and cerebral asymmetry, seem to evince less
steep increases in the beginning and lesser decreases
In this section, we brought together theory and research at the end of life; as well as lower longitudinal stabil-
from three different areas of research, that is, trait per- ity of interindividual differences when compared with
sonality, the self-system, and self-regulatory processes. the developmental evidence that is available about the
Each of these areas is characterized by their own foci life mechanics of cognition. One source of this differ-
and methodological approaches. We have argued that ence in trajectories may be that for evolutionary
a life span perspective on personality development, reasons children are already further developed
rather than viewing these three approaches as relatively with regard to basic emotional and motivational ten-
independent from each other or even mutually exclusive, dencies than in terms of higher cognitive functions.
tries to incorporate and integrate theoretical and empir- Consequently, we may speculate that following the
ical evidence from these fields. Dynamic systems theory law of “ first in, last out ” these basic indicators of
and similar theoretical perspectives such as models of emotional and motivational functioning maintain
successful development provide useful theoretical guid- higher levels of functioning until later in life than indi-
ance for this endeavor. cators of the cognitive mechanics. Such an interpreta-
tion would make it understandable why many
1. A central feature of personality development is personality mechanisms are well-preserved into older
the emergence of structure and of an associated system ages and only demonstrate decline when the oldest-old
of self-regulatory mechanisms that mediate successful are considered.
transactional adaptation. Beginning in childhood, we 4. There has been no systematic work as of yet on the
obtain solid evidence for structure, a sense of coher- interaction between the life mechanics and pragmatics
ence, and some stable modes of adaptive behavior (e.g., of personality. Also our knowledge about the links be-
Caspi & Bem, 1990). Such a view is represented in tween the neurophysiological and the behavioral level
dynamic-system models of development (Magnusson, are still at the beginning. Thus, our insight into the func-
1996), where principles of structural emergence and tional relations between the life mechanics and pragmat-
self-organization are critical for successful ontogenesis. ics in that regard is rather limited. Nevertheless, the
Structural organization and coherence of personality, limited evidence available foreshadows the rich and
self, and self-regulatory mechanisms are a necessary complex interactions taking place along the continuum
precondition (constraint) for adaptive fitness and fur- between the life mechanics and pragmatics of personal-
ther growth. In this sense, the ensemble of features de- ity. The picture is further complicated by the special
scribed function as general-purpose mechanisms. role of the self in the development of the mechanics and
2. Theory and research have advanced beyond the pragmatics of life. The self is not merely a developing
traditional trait versus change contrast. Traits them- element in this system, but also has an orchestrating
selves are part of the dynamic personality system. Thus, function, coordinating cognitive, emotional, and moti-
even stability is developmental in the sense that it is the vational development. When it comes to life as a whole,
result of surviving continuous challenge. Indeed, we do the self can be postulated to play a central role in orga-
change during adulthood and old age but to a degree that nizing our actions and thoughts, but we need to recog-
does not jeopardize our sense of continuity. Personality nize that at least the consciously agentic self is but part
development is characterized by a dialectic between of organizing behaviors.
trait expressions and self-regulation at work. 5. Against this background, we suggest that in the
3. We have started to explore the fertility of ex- domain of the personality system, developmental in-
tending the distinction between the biologically driven creases and stability dynamics extend over longer time
cognitive mechanics and the culturally driven cogni- spans than is true for the domain of cognition qua cog-
tive pragmatics to also cover the field of personality nition. In fact, we have argued that the losses and
functioning. The result of this endeavor is a heuristic stressors of adult life may even result in advances re-
model that distinguishes between the biologically garding the acquisition and refinement of self-
driven mechanics and the culturally driven pragmatics regulatory skills. Still, when studying the resilience of
of life. The two neurophysiological indicators selected the aging self, we also need to take into consideration
Concluding Commentary 643

whether the underlying physiological and neurological functional losses may for more and more persons reach
systems have a debilitating and/or facilitating influ- an overwhelming degree or at earlier ages when other
ence on self and personality functioning. The less pos- extreme life circumstances result in an imbalance of
itive evidence on personality functioning in the oldest gains and losses.
old supports this conjecture. 7. The systemic and overall developmental theory of
6. Personality as a dynamic system composed of selective optimization with compensation introduced in
various components with different properties holds a the first part of this chapter serves as a useful theoreti-
domain-general potential for the transactional adapta- cal tool when analyzing the adaptive potential of the
tion of the developing organism. We argued and pre- self and personality (P. B. Baltes & Baltes, 1990b).
sented evidence that personality serves an executive or When orchestrating the optimization of development by
orchestrating function with regard to the management processes such as selection and compensation, the ap-
of gains and losses during ontogeny. Personality pos- praisal of resources is of central importance. Questions
sesses a great ability to negotiate the opportunities and such as how to evolve a goal structure and the associated
constraints of development that come with age, histori- goal-relevant means and motivational investment strate-
cal, and idiosyncratic conditions. What we have called gies, how to deal with selection-related disengagements
general-purpose mechanisms play a central role in this from other possible goals, when to accept a loss and re-
adaptational process. Besides protective personality orient one’s life, and when to still strive harder because
structure and content, it is primarily the availability of current behavior is not yet employed to its fullest capa-
a rich variety of self-regulatory mechanisms, and of an bility become crucial in composing life development.
adaptive algorithm which monitors their application, Brim has argued, for example, that one criterion for
that contribute to the adaptational power of personality. making this decision could be to consider something like
Table 11.6 summarizes these protective features in a “performance/capacity ratio” (Brim, 1992). Accord-
more detail. This adaptive potential reaches its limits ing to this ratio, acceptance of a certain loss becomes
in very old age when, due to biological processes, the necessary when the display of the behavior requires a
“dysfunctionally” high amount of reserve capacity.
TABLE 11.6 Summary of Protective and Optimizing
Characteristics of Self and Personality
Self and Protective and Optimizing CONCLUDING COMMENTARY
Personality Components Characteristics (Examples)
The purpose of this chapter was to present life span de-
Personality Conscientiousness, extraversion,
openness to experience, behavioral velopmental psychology as a theoretical orientation to
flexibility, ego resilience, the study of human development. Because the dominant
advanced ego level, and cognitive theoretical approaches in developmental psychology
investment
have been formed primarily by research on infants,
Self-concept Interrelated, well-articulated
variety of self-conceptions and children, and adolescents, we made special efforts to
life priorities highlight the uniqueness in developmental theory that
Positive agency (efficacy) beliefs emanates from a life span developmental framework. An
Self-regulatory and life- unfortunate by-product of this strategy of presentation
management mechanisms may have been the relative inattention paid to important
–Self-evaluation Application of a functional type of commonalities between age-specialized developmental
comparison (up, down, lateral,
temporal) at the appropriate time theories and theoretical efforts in life span work.
in the adaptational process There is a larger (and growing) commonality in theo-
–Goal setting and restructuring Selection and reorganization of retical approach between more age-specialized develop-
life priorities mental theories and life span developmental theory than
–Coping styles Intraindividual variability and
might appear to be true based on the arguments pre-
f lexibility in coping styles and
compensatory strategies sented in this chapter. In part, this is true because there
Flexibility in adapting goals to are several new sources (only alluded to in this chapter)
circumstances from which innovative theoretical efforts in various
–Systemic processes Selective optimization with quarters of developmental psychology have emerged
compensation (SOC) and which contain a structure of arguments similar to
644 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

those put forward in the short history of life span devel- study that extends over close to 50 years (Schaie, 2005)
opmental theory. Work in cultural psychology, dynamic and demonstrates the varied conditions and outcomes
systems theory, and on other forms of self-organization which we can observe when placing adult development
in ontogenesis, are examples of this new theoretical into the context of historical change and, in addition,
treatment of ontogenesis that is beginning to pervade consider processes of individual differentiation. There
the developmental field as a whole. also have been advances in demonstrating the useful-
As was true for life span psychology and the benefits ness of the life span approach for other specialties such
it derived from its contact with the biology of aging, as clinical (Staudinger et al., 1995; Vaillant, 1990) and
these new kinds of theoretical treatments have benefited applied psychology (Abraham & Hansson, 1995; B. B.
from transdisciplinary dialogue, especially with modern Baltes & Dickson, 2001; Sterns & Dorsett, 1994). In
developmental biologists but also anthropologists. Biol- fact, these intersects of the life span approach to the
ogists have perhaps led the way in moving research away study of human development with other psychological
from unilinear, organismic, and deterministic models of specialties need to be identified and nurtured.
ontogenesis to a theoretical framework that highlights Close to 25 years ago, one of us wrote: “ There can be
the contextual, adaptive, probabilistic, and self- no strong field of life span developmental psychology
organizational dynamic aspects of ontogenesis (P. B. without a solid foundation in and connection to child-
Baltes & Graf, 1996; Magnusson, 1996). Similarly, cul- hood. By the same token, the study of child development
tural psychologists and anthropologists (e.g., Cole, does not exist in a vacuum, but is vitally enriched by
1996; Durham, 1991; Valsiner & Lawrence, 1997) have considering the aftermath of childhood” (P. B. Baltes,
succeeded equally in convincingly demonstrating that 1979b, p. 1). Since then, there has been much progress
human ontogenesis is not only strongly conditioned by in elaborating this reciprocal connection between age-
culture, but that the architecture of human development focused developmental specialities and their integration
is essentially incomplete as to the culturally engineered into a life span view of human development, but at the
pathways and possible endpoints (P. B. Baltes, 1997). same time, this challenge continues to be with us.
Not the least because of this transdisciplinary dia-
logue, a new conception has emerged regarding the “na- REFERENCES
ture” (Kagan, 1984) of human development. In the
modern context, the nature of human development no Abraham, J. D., & Hansson, R. O. (1995). Successful aging at work:
longer refers to the fixed-biological (P. B. Baltes, 1991; An applied study of selection, optimization, and compensation
through impression management. Journal of Gerontology: Psycho-
R. M. Lerner, 1984; Magnusson, 1996). Rather, in modern logical Sciences, 50B, 94–103.
versions of ontogenesis, its nature is both biological and Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual develop-
cultural, and both of these categories are subject to dy- ment: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelli-
namic and interactive changes as well as systemic trans- gence, 22, 227–257.
formations. Of all developmental specialties, life span Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Bowen, K. R. (2000). Explorations
of crystallized intelligence. Learning and Individual Dif ferences,
development, because of its intimate connection with 12(1), 105.
long-term processes of individual development, cultural Ackerman, P. L., & Cianciolo, A. T. (2000). Cognitive, perceptual-
evolution, and generational transmission is perhaps the speed, and psychomotor determinants of individual differences
during skill acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Ap-
field most dependent on, and committed to, such views
plied, 6, 259–290.
(e.g., P. B. Baltes, Reuter-Lorenz, & Rösler, 2006; P. B. Aldwin, C. M., & Revenson, T. A. (1987). Does coping help? A reex-
Baltes & Smith, 2004; S.-C. Li, 2003). Most recently, the amination of the relation between coping and mental health. Jour-
emergence of the concept of biocultural co-construction- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 337–348.
sim is another justification of this orientation. Aldwin, C. M., Sutton, K. J., Chiara, G., & Spiro, A., III. (1996). Age
differences in stress, coping, and appraisal: Findings from the
The future of life span developmental theory will de- Normative Aging Study. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological
pend significantly on the extent to which the metatheo- Sciences, 51B, 179–188.
retical perspectives advanced turn out to be useful in Aldwin, C. M., Sutton, K. J., & Lachman, M. (1996). The develop-
the conduct of empirical inquiry. On this score, the ment of coping resources in adulthood. Journal of Personality,
64, 837–871.
1980s have witnessed impressive growth. In the area of
Anokhin, A. P., Müller, V., Lindenberger, U., & Heath, A. C. (in
intellectual development (see following), for instance, press). Genetic inf luences on dynamic complexity of brain oscil-
we now have available a cohort- and age-sequential lations. Neuroscience Letters.
References 645

Ardelt, M. (2004). Wisdom as expert knowledge system: A critical Baltes, M. M., Neumann, E.-M., & Zank, S. (1994). Maintenance and
review of a contemporary operationalization of an ancient con- rehabilitation of independence in old age: An intervention pro-
cept. Human Development, 47, 257–285. gram for staff. Psychology and Aging, 9, 179–188.
Aspinwall, L. G., & Staudinger, U. M. (2003). A psychology of human Baltes, M. M., & Silverberg, S. B. (1994). The dynamics between de-
strengths: Some central issues of an emerging field. In L. G. As- pendency and autonomy: Illustrations across the life span. In
pinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of human D. L. Featherman, R. M. Lerner, & M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Life
strengths (pp. 9–22). Washington, DC: American Psychological span development and behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 41–90). Hillsdale,
Association. NJ: Erlbaum.
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Baltes, M. M., & Wahl, H.-W. (1992). The dependency-support script
Books. in institutions: Generalization to community settings. Psychology
Bäckman, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Psychological compensation: A and Aging, 7, 409–418.
theoretical framework. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 1–25. Baltes, P. B. (1968). Longitudinal and cross-sectional sequences in
Bäckman, L., & Farde, L. (2004). The role of dopamine systems in the study of age and generation effects. Human Development,
cognitive aging. In R. Cabeza, L. Nyberg, & D. Park (Eds.), Cog- 11, 145–171.
nitive neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral aging. Baltes, P. B. (1973). Life span models of psychological aging: A
New York: Oxford University Press. white elephant? Gerontologist, 13, 457–512.
Bäckman, L., Josephsson, S., Herlitz, A., Stigsdotter, A., & Viitanen, Baltes, P. B. (Ed.). (1976). Symposium on implications of life span
M. (1991). The generalizability of training gains in dementia: Ef- developmental psychology for child development. Advances in
fects of an imagery-based mnemonic on face-name retention du- Child Development and Behavior, 11, 167–265.
ration. Psychology and Aging, 6, 489–492. Baltes, P. B. (1979a). Life span developmental psychology: Some con-
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of verging observations on history and theory. In P. B. Baltes &
working memory? Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417–423. O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 2,
Baltes, B. B., & Dickson, M. W. (2001). Using life-span models in pp. 255–279). New York: Academic Press.
industrial-organizational psychology: The Theory of Selective Baltes, P. B. (1979b, Summer). On the potential and limits of child
Optimization with Compensation. Applied Developmental Sci- development: Life span developmental perspectives. Newsletter of
ence, 5, 51–61. Society for Research in Child Development, 1–4.
Baltes, B. B., & Heydens-Gahir, H. A. (2003). Reduction of work- Baltes, P. B. (1983). Life span developmental psychology: Observa-
family conf lict through the use of selection, optimization, and tions on history and theory revisited. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), De-
compensation behaviors. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, velopmental psychology: Historical and philosophical perspectives
1005–1018. (pp. 79–111). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baltes, M. M. (1987). Erfolgreiches altern als ausdruck von verhal- Baltes, P. B. (1984). Intelligenz im alter [Intelligence in old age].
tenskompetenz und umweltqualität. In C. Niemitz (Ed.), Der men- Spektrum der Wissenschaft, 5, 46–60.
sch im zusammenspiel von anlage und umwelt (pp. 353–377). Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life span develop-
Frankfurt-am-Main, Germany: Suhrkamp. mental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline.
Baltes, M. M. (1988). The etiology and maintenance of dependency Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–626.
in the elderly: Three phases of operant research. Behavior Ther- Baltes, P. B. (1991). The many faces of human aging: Toward a psycho-
apy, 19, 301–319. logical culture of old age. Psychological Medicine, 21, 837–854.
Baltes, M. M. (1995). Dependency in old age: Gains and losses. Cur- Baltes, P. B. (1993). The aging mind: Potential and limits. Gerontolo-
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 14–19. gist, 33, 580–594.
Baltes, M. M. (1996). The many faces of dependency in old age. New Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human on-
York: Cambridge University Press. togeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as founda-
Baltes, M. M., & Baltes, P. B. (Eds.). (1986). The psychology of con- tion of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52,
trol and aging. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 366–380.
Baltes, M. M., & Carstensen, L. L. (1996). The process of successful Baltes, P. B. (Guest Ed.). (2005). Theoretical approaches to life span
ageing. Aging and Society, 16, 397–422. development: Interdisciplinary perspectives [Special issue]. Re-
search in Human Development, 2(1/2).
Baltes, M. M., & Carstensen, L. L. (1999). Social psychological the-
ories and their applications to aging: From individual to collec- Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1980). Plasticity and variability in
tive social psychology. In V. L. Bengtson & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), psychological aging: Methodological and theoretical issues. In
Handbook of theories of aging (pp. 209–226). New York: G. E. Gurski (Ed.), Determining the ef fects of aging on the cen-
Springer. tral nervous system (pp. 41–66). Berlin, Germany: Schering.
Baltes, M. M., Kühl, K.-P., Gutzmann, H., & Sowarka, D. (1995). Po- Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990a). Psychological perspectives on
tential of cognitive plasticity as a diagnostic instrument: A cross- successful aging: The model of selective optimization with com-
validation and extension. Psychology and Aging, 10, 167–172. pensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful
aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). New
Baltes, M. M., Kühl, K.-P., & Sowarka, D. (1992). Testing the limits
York: Cambridge University Press.
of cognitive reserve capacity: A promising strategy for early diag-
nosis of dementia? Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sci- Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (Eds.). (1990b). Successful aging: Per-
ences, 47, 165–167. spectives from the behavioral sciences. New York: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
Baltes, M. M., & Lang, F. R. (1997). Everyday functioning and suc-
cessful aging: The impact of resources. Psychology and Aging, Baltes, P. B., Cornelius, S. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1979). Cohort
12, 433–443. effects in developmental psychology. In J. R. Nesselroade & P. B.
646 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Baltes (Eds.), Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1980). Life span devel-
development (pp. 61–87). New York: Academic Press. opmental psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 65–110.
Baltes, P. B., Dittmann-Kohli, F., & Dixon, R. A. (1984). New per- Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1977). Life span de-
spectives on the development of intelligence in adulthood: Toward velopmental psychology: Introduction to research methods. Mon-
a dual-process conception and a model of selective optimization terey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1988). Life span de-
span development and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 33–76). New York: velopmental psychology: Introduction to research methods. Hills-
Academic Press. dale, NJ: Erlbaum. (Original work published 1977)
Baltes, P. B., Freund, A. M., & Li, S.-C. (2005). The psychological
Baltes, P. B., Rösler, F., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2006). Prologue:
science of human aging. In M. Johnson, V. L. Bengston, P. G.
The perspective of biocultural co-constructivism. In P. B. Baltes,
Coleman, & T. Kirkwood (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of age and
P. A. Reuter-Lorenz, & F. Rösler (Eds.), Lifespan development
aging. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
and the brain: The perspective of biocultural co-constructivism
Baltes, P. B., & Goulet, L. R. (1970). Status and issues of a life span (pp. 3–39). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
developmental psychology. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.),
Baltes, P. B., Reuter-Lorenz, P., & Rosler, F. (Eds.). (2006). Lifespan
Life span developmental psychology: Research and theory
development and the brain: The perspective of biocultural co-
(pp. 4–21). New York: Academic Press.
constructivism. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Baltes, P. B., & Goulet, L. R. (1971). Exploration of developmental
Baltes, P. B., & Schaie, K. W. (1976). On the plasticity of intelligence
variables by manipulation and simulation of age differences in
in adulthood and old age: Where Horn and Donaldson fail. Ameri-
behavior. Human Development, 14, 149–170.
can Psychologist, 31, 720–725.
Baltes, P. B., & Graf, P. (1996). Psychological aspects of aging: Facts
Baltes, P. B., & Singer, T. (2001). Plasticity and the ageing mind: An
and frontiers. In D. Magnusson (Ed.), The life span development
exemplar of the biocultural orchestration of brain and behaviour.
of individuals: Behavioural, neurobiological and psychosocial per-
European Review: Interdisciplinary Journal of the Academia Eu-
spectives (pp. 427–459). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer-
ropaea, 9, 59–76.
sity Press.
Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (1990). The psychology of wisdom and its on-
Baltes, P. B., & Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cogni-
togenesis. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and
tive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are
development (pp. 87–120). New York: Cambridge University Press.
robust. Developmental Psychology, 28, 121–125.
Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of aging:
Baltes, P. B., & Kunzmann, U. (2003). Wisdom. Psychologist, 16, 131–133.
From successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the
Baltes, P. B., & Kunzmann, U. (2004). The two faces of wisdom: Wis- fourth age. Gerontology, 49, 123–135.
dom as a general theory of knowledge and judgment about excel- Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2004). Life span psychology: From de-
lence in mind and virtue versus wisdom as everyday realization in velopmental contextualism to developmental biocultural co-
people and products. Human Development, 47, 290–299. constructivism. Research in Human Development, 1(3), 123–143.
Baltes, P. B., & Labouvie, G. V. (1973). Adult development of intel- Baltes, P. B., Smith, J., & Staudinger, U. M. (1992). Wisdom and suc-
lectual performance: Description, explanation, modification. In cessful aging. In T. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on
C. Eisdorfer & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology of adult de- Motivation (Vol. 39, pp. 123–167). Lincoln: University of Ne-
velopment and aging (pp. 157–219). Washington, DC: American braska Press.
Psychological Association.
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (Eds.). (1996a). Interactive minds:
Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1988). On the range of cognitive Life span perspectives on the social foundation of cognition. New
plasticity in old age as a function of experience: 15 years of inter- York: Cambridge University Press.
vention research. Behavior Therapy, 19, 283–300.
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (1996b). Interactive minds in a life
Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful span perspective: Prologue. In P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger
connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the (Eds.), Interactive minds: Life span perspectives on the social
adult life span: A new window at the study of cognitive aging? foundation of cognition (pp. 1–32). New York: Cambridge Univer-
Psychology and Aging, 12, 12–21. sity Press.
Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life span Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic
theory in developmental psychology. In W. Damon (Editor-in- to orchestrate mind and virtue towards excellence. American Psy-
Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: chologist, 55, 122–136.
Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Life span
pp. 1029–1143). New York: Wiley. psychology: Theory and application to intellectual functioning
Baltes, P. B., & Mayer, K. U. (Eds.). (1999). The Berlin Aging Study: [Review]. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 471–507.
Aging from 70 to 100. New York: Cambridge University Press. Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., Maercker, A., & Smith, J. (1995).
Baltes, P. B., Mayer, K. U., Helmchen, H., & Steinhagen-Thiessen, E. People nominated as wise: A comparative study of wisdom-
(1993). The Berlin Aging Study (BASE): Overview and design. related knowledge. Psychology and Aging, 10, 155–166.
Ageing and Society, 13, 483–515. Baltes, P. B., & Willis, S. L. (1982). Plasticity and enhancement of
Baltes, P. B., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1978). Multivariate antecedents intellectual functioning in old age: Penn State’s Adult Develop-
of structural change in adulthood. Multivariate Behavioral Re- ment and Enrichment Project ADEPT. In F. I. M. Craik & S. E.
search, 13, 127–152. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 353–389). New
Baltes, P. B., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1984). Paradigm lost and paradigm York: Plenum Press.
regained: Critique of Dannefer’s portrayal of life span develop- Baltes, P. B., & Willis, S. W. (1977). Toward psychological theories
mental psychology. American Sociological Review, 49, 841–846. of aging and development. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.),
References 647

Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 128–154). New York: Birren, J. E., & Fisher, L. M. (1995). Aging and speed of behavior:
Van Nostrand Reinhold. Possible consequences for psychological functioning. Annual Re-
Baltissen, R. (2005). Psychophysiologische aspekte des mittleren view of Psychology, 46, 329–353.
und höheren erwachsenenalters [Psychophysiological aspects Birren, J. E., & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.). (1996). Handbook of the psy-
of middle and older age]. In S. H. Filipp & U. M. Staudinger chology of aging (3rd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
(Eds.), Entwicklungspsychologie des mittleren und höheren Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human develop-
erwachsenalters: Enzyklopädie der psychologie [Developmental ment. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153–169.
psychology of the middle and older age] (pp. 123–173). Göttin- Blanchard-Fields, F. (1996). Social cognitive development in adult-
gen, Germany: Hogrefe. hood and aging. In F. Blanchard-Fields & T. M. Hess (Eds.),
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Perspectives on cognitive change in adulthood and aging
American Psychologist, 37, 122–147. (pp. 454–487). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bandura, A. (1984). Representing personal determinants in causal Blanchard-Fields, F., & Norris, L. (1994). Causal attributions from
structures. Psychological Review, 91, 508–511. adolescence through adulthood: Age differences, ego level, and
Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development generalized response style. Aging and Cognition, 1, 67–86.
and functioning. Educational Psychologist, 28, 117–148. Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley, CA: Bancroft Books.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-ef ficacy: The exercise of control. New Block, J. (1993). Studying personality the long way. In D. C. Funder,
York: Freeman. R. D. Parke, C. Tomlinson-Keasey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Study-
Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-evaluative and self-efficacy ing lives through time: Personality and development—APA science
mechanisms governing the motivational effects of goal systems. volumes (pp. 9–41). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1017–1028. Association.
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and personality Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to per-
development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. sonality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187–215.
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The adapted mind: Bloom, B. (1985). Developing talent in young people. New York: Bal-
Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: lantine Books.
Oxford University Press. Boesch, E. E. (1997). Boesch and cultural psychology [Special issue].
Barton, S. (1994). Chaos, self-organization, and psychology. Ameri- Culture and Psychology, 3(3).
can Psychologist, 49, 5–14. Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Stability in early mental development: From
Bashore, T. R., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & van der Molen, M. W. (1997). attention and information processing in infancy to language and
The decline of cognitive processing speed in old age. Current Di- cognition in childhood. In M. H. Bornstein & N. A. Krasnegor
rections in Psychological Science, 6, 163–169. (Eds.), Stability and continuity in mental development
(pp. 147–170). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Basseches, M. (1984). Dialectical thinking and adult development.
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Bosman, E. A., & Charness, N. (1996). Age-related differences in
skilled performance and skill acquisition. In F. Blanchard-Fields
Baumeister, R. F. (1989). The optimal margin of illusion. Journal of
& T. M. Hess (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive change in adult-
Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 176–189.
hood and aging (pp. 428–453). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Baumeister, R. F. (1992). Neglected aspects of self theory: Motiva-
Brandtstädter, J. (1984). Personal and social control over develop-
tion, interpersonal aspects, culture, escape, and existential value.
ment: Some implications of an action perspective in life span de-
Psychological Inquiry, 3, 21–25.
velopmental psychology. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.),
Bengtson, V. L., Reedy, M. N., & Gordon, C. (1985). Aging and self- Life span development and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 1–32). New York:
conceptions: Personality processes and social contexts. In J. E. Academic Press.
Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of
Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development.
aging (2nd ed., pp. 544–593). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Hand-
Benson, F. B., Cherny, S. S., Haith, M. M., & Fulker, D. W. (1993). book of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human de-
Rapid assessment of infant predictors of adult IQ: Midtwin- velopment (5th ed., pp. 807–866). New York: Wiley.
midparent analyses. Developmental Psychology, 29, 434–447.
Brandtstädter, J., & Baltes-Götz, B. (1990). Personal control over de-
Berg, C. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1985a). A triarchic theory of in- velopment and quality of life perspectives in adulthood. In P. B.
tellectual development during adulthood. Developmental Review, Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from
5, 334–370. the behavioral sciences (pp. 197–224). New York: Cambridge Uni-
Berg, C. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (1985b). Response to novelty: Conti- versity Press.
nuity versus discontinuity in the developmental course of intelli- Brandtstädter, J., & Greve, W. (1994). The aging self: Stabilizing and
gence. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and protective processes. Developmental Review, 14, 52–80.
behavior (pp. 2–47). New York: Academic Press. Brandtstädter, J., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1999). Action and self de-
Birren, J. E. (1959). Principles of research on aging. In J. E. Birren velopment: Theory and research through the life span. Thousand
(Ed.), Handbook of aging and the individual: Psychological and bi- Oaks, CA: Sage.
ological aspects (pp. 3–42). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Brandtstädter, J., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and
Birren, J. E. (1964). The psychology of aging. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: f lexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related analysis of
Prentice-Hall. assimilative and accommodative models of coping. Psychology
Birren, J. E. (1988). A contribution to the theory of the psychology of and Aging, 5, 58–67.
aging: As a counterpart of development. In J. E. Birren & V. L. Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (1994). Self-perceptions of con-
Bengtson (Eds.), Emergent theories of aging (pp. 153–176). New trol in middle and later adulthood: Buffering losses by rescaling
York: Springer. goals. Psychology and Aging, 9, 265–273.
648 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2003). Intentionality and time in (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and
human development and aging: Compensation and goal adjustment cerebral aging. New York: Oxford University Press.
in changing developmental contexts. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lin- Bühler, C. (1933). Der menschliche lebenslauf als psychologisches
denberger (Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues problem [The human life course as a psychological problem]. Göt-
with life span psychology (pp. 105–124). Boston: Kluwer Academic. tingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
Brandtstädter, J., & Wentura, D. (1995). Adjustment to shifting pos- Burgess, R. L., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (1995). Commentary. Human
sibility frontiers in later life: Complementary adaptive modes. In Development, 38, 159–164.
R. A. Dixon & L. Bäckman (Eds.), Psychological compensation:
Burns, B. D. (2004). The effects of speed on skilled chess perfor-
Managing losses and promoting gains (pp. 83–106). Hillsdale,
mance. Psychological Science, 7, 52–55.
NJ: Erlbaum.
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). A temperament theory of personal-
Brandtstädter, J., Wentura, D., & Greve, W. (1993). Adaptive re-
ity development. New York: Wiley.
sources of the aging self: Outlines of an emergent perspective. In-
ternational Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 323–349. Buunk, B. P. (1994). Social comparison processes under stress: To-
wards an integration of classic and recent perspectives. European
Brandtstädter, J., Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (1999). Intentional
Review of Social Psychology, 5, 211–241.
self-development through adulthood and later life: Tenacious pur-
suit and f lexible adjustment of goals. In J. Brandtstädter & R. M. Buunk, B. P., & Gibbons, F. X. (1997). Health, coping, and well-being:
Lerner (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research Perspectives from social comparison theory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
through the life span (pp. 373–400). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults:
Brehmer, Y., Li, S.-C., Müller, V., von Oertzen, T., & Lindenberger, The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100.
U. (2005). Memory plasticity across the lifespan: Uncovering chil- Cabeza, R., Daselaar, S. M., Dolcos, F., Prince, S. E., Budde, M., &
dren’s latent potential. Unpublished manuscript, Max Planck In- Nyberg, L. (2004). Task-independent and task-specific age ef-
stitute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany. fects on brain activity during working memory, visual attention
Brent, S. B. (1978a). Individual specialization, collective adaptation and episodic retrieval. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 364–375.
and rate of environmental change. Human Development, 21, 21–23. Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D. (Eds.). (2004). Cognitive neuro-
Brent, S. B. (1978b). Prigogine’s model for self-organization in non- science of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral aging. New York:
equilibrium systems: Its relevance for developmental psychology. Oxford University Press.
Human Development, 21, 374–387. Campbell, J. D., Assanand, S., & Di Paula, A. (2003). The structure
Brim, O. G., Jr. (1976). Life span development of the theory of one- of the self-concept and its relation to psychological adjustment.
self: Implications for child development. In P. B. Baltes (Ed.), Ad- Journal of Personality, 71, 115–140.
vances in Child Development and Behavior, 11, 242–251. Cantor, N., & Blanton, H. (1996). Effortful pursuit of personal goals
in daily life. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psy-
Brim, O. G., Jr. (1988). Losing and winning: The nature of ambition
chology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior
in everyday life. Psychology Today, 9, 48–52.
(pp. 338–359). New York: Guilford Press.
Brim, O. G., Jr. (1992). Ambition: How we manage success and failure
Cantor, N., & Fleeson, W. (1994). Social intelligence and intelligent
throughout our lives. New York: Basic Books.
goal pursuit: A cognitive slice of motivation. Nebraska Sympo-
Brim, O. G., Jr., & Kagan, J. (1980). Constancy and change: A view sium on Motivation (Vol. 41, pp. 125–179). Lincoln: University of
of the issues. In O. G. Brim Jr. & J. Kagan (Eds.), Constancy and Nebraska Press.
change in human development (pp. 1–25). Cambridge, MA: Har-
Cardon, L. R., & Fulker, D. W. (1991). Sources of continuity in infant
vard University Press.
predictors of later IQ. Intelligence, 15, 279–293.
Brim, O. G., Jr., & Ryff, C. D. (1980). On the properties of life events.
Cardon, L. R., & Fulker, D. W. (1994). A model of developmental
In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and
change in hierarchical phenotypes with application to specific
behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 367–388). New York: Academic Press.
cognitive abilities. Behavior Genetics, 24, 1–16.
Brim, O. G., Jr., Ryff, C. D., & Kessler, R. C. (Eds.). (2004). How
Carlson, J. S. (1994). Dynamic assessment of mental abilities. In R. J.
healthy are we? A national study of well-being at midlife. Chicago:
Sternberg (Ed.), Encyclopedia of intelligence (pp. 368–372). New
Chicago University Press.
York: Macmillan.
Brim, O. G., Jr., & Wheeler, S. (1966). Socialization after childhood:
Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life span theory of socioe-
Two essays. New York: Wiley.
motional selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of 4, 151–156.
human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–532.
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nuture reconceptu- time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American
alized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psy- Psychologist, 54, 165–181.
chological Review, 101, 568–586.
Carus, F. A. (1808). Psychologie: Zweiter Teil: Specialpsychologie
Brown, A. L. (1982). Learning and development: The problem of com- [Psychology: Pt. 2. Special psychology]. Leipzig, Germany:
patibility, access, and induction. Human Development, 25, 89–115. Barth & Kummer.
Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of be-
longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, havior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
65, 1061–1070. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2003). Three human strengths. In
Buckner, R. I. (2004). Three principles for cognitive aging research: U. M. Staudinger & L. G. Aspinwall (Eds.), A psychology of
Multiple causes and sequelae, variance in expression, and the human strengths (pp. 87–102). Washington, DC: American Psy-
need for integrative theory. In R. Cabeza, L. Nyberg, & D. Park chological Association.
References 649

Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: From birth to adulthood. Cicchetti, D. (1993). Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, re-
New York: Academic Press. f lections, projections—Setting a path for the coming decade:
Case, R. (1992). The role of the frontal lobes in the regulation of Some goals and challenges [Special issue]. Developmental Review,
human development. Brain and Cognition, 20, 51–73. 13, 471–502.
Caspi, A. (1987). Personality in the life course. Journal of Personal- Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.). (1995). Developmental psycho-
ity and Social Psychology, 53, 1203–1213. pathology (Vols. 1–2). New York: Wiley.
Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In Cicero, M. T. (44B.C./1744). Cato major: De senectute [Cato major or
W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Hand- His discourse of old age] (J. Logan, Trans.). Philadelphia: Ben-
book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social emotional, and personality jamin Franklin.
development (5th ed., pp. 311–387). New York: Wiley. Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method of clinical description
Caspi, A., & Bem, D. J. (1990). Personality continuity and change and classification of personality variants: A proposal. Archives of
across the life course. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personal- General Psychiatry, 44, 573–588.
ity: Theory and research (pp. 549–575). New York: Guilford Press. Cloninger, C. R. (2003). Completing the psychobiological architec-
ture of human personality development: Temperament, character,
Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age 3
and coherence. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.),
predict personality traits in young adulthood: Longitudinal evi-
Understanding human development: Dialogues with life span psy-
dence from a birth cohort. Child Development, 66, 486–498.
chology (pp. 159–181). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action.
Cohen, S. H., & Reese, H. W. (Eds.). (1994). Life span developmen-
Boston: Houghton Miff lin.
tal psychology: Methodological contributions. Hillsdale, NJ:
Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information processing rate. In J. E. Erlbaum.
Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of
Colcombe, S. J., Erickson, K. I., Raz, N., Webb, A. G., Cohen, N. J.,
aging (pp. 201–221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
McAuley, E., et al. (2003). Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue
Chapman, M. (1988a). Constructive evolution: Origins and develop- loss in aging humans. Journals of Gerontology: Biological Sciences
ment of Piaget’s thought. New York: Cambridge University Press. and Medical Sciences, 58(2, Series A), 176–180.
Chapman, M. (1988b). Contextuality and directionality of cognitive Cole, M. (1996). Interacting minds in a life span perspective: A cul-
development. Human Development, 31, 92–106. tural / historical approach to culture and cognitive development.
Chapman, M., & Lindenberger, U. (1992). Transitivity judgments, In P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), Interactive minds:
memory for premises, and models of children’s reasoning. Devel- Life span perspectives on the social foundation of cognition
opmental Review, 12, 124–163. (pp. 59–87). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Charles, S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz, M. (2001). Age-related dif- Collins, W. A. (Ed.). (1982). Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychol-
ferences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. ogy: Vol. 15. The concept of development (pp. 55–81). Hillsdale,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 136–151. NJ: Erlbaum.
Charlesworth, B. (1994). Evolution in age-structured populations Colvin, C. R., & Block, J. (1994). Do positive illusions foster mental
(2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. health? An examination of the Taylor and Brown formulation.
Charness, N. (1981). Search in chess: Age and skill differences. Jour- Psychological Bulletin, 116, 3–20.
nal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfor- Costa, P.-T., Jr., Herbst, J. H., McCrae, R. R., & Siegler, I. C. (2000).
mance, 7, 467–476. Personality at midlife: Stability, intrinsic maturation, and re-
Charness, N. (1989). Age and expertise: Responding to Talland’s sponse to life events. Assessment, 7, 365–378.
challenge. In L. W. Poon (Ed.), Everyday cognition in adulthood Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1993). Psychological stress and
and late life (pp. 437–456). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni- coping in old age. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Hand-
versity Press. book of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (2nd ed.,
Charness, N. (2005). Work /occupation: Macro-structural dimensions pp. 403–412). New York: Free Press.
of experience and skills training—A developmental perspective. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for
In P. B. Baltes, P. Reuter-Lorenz, & F. Rösler (Eds.), Life span the stability of adult personality. In T. F. Heatherton & J. L.
development and the brain: The perspective of biocultural co- Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 21–40). Wash-
constructivism. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Charness, N. (2006). The inf luence of work and occupation on brain Costa, P. T., Zondermann, A. B., McCrae, R. R., Cornoni-Huntley, J.,
development. In P. B. Baltes, P. A. Reuter-Lorenz, & F. Rösler Locke, B. Z., & Barbano, H. E. (1987). Longitudinal analyses of
(Eds.), Lifespan development and the brain: The perspective psychological well-being in a national sample: Stability of mean
of biocultural co-constructivism (pp. 306–325). Cambridge, levels. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 50–55.
England: Cambridge University Press. Cotman, C. W. (Ed.). (1985). Synaptic plasticity. New York: Guil-
Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1984). Origins and evolution of behaviour ford Press.
disorders. New York: Brunner/ Mazel. Cowdry, E. V. (1939). Problems of ageing: Biological and medical as-
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem pects. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
solving. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of Craik, F. I. M. (1983). On the transfer of information from temporary
human intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 7–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. to permanent memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal So-
Chi, M. T., & Koeske, R. D. (1983). Network representation of a ciety of London B, 302, 341–359.
child’s dinosaur knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 19, 29–39. Craik, F. I. M. (1986). A functional account of age differences in mem-
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., & Mazziotta, J. C. (1987). Positron ory. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Human memory and cogni-
emission tomography study of human brain development. Annals tive capabilities, mechanisms, and performance (pp. 409–422).
of Neurology, 22, 487–497. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
650 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E. (Eds.). (2006). Life span cognition: chemistry. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Kinght (Eds.), Principles of frontal
Mechanisms of change. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. lobe function (pp. 466–503). London: Oxford University Press.
Craik, F. I. M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive deficits: The Dick, D. M., & Rose, R. J. (2002). Behavior genetics: What’s new?
role of attentional resources. In F. I. M. Craik & S. Trehub What’s next? Current Direction in Psychological Science, 11, 70–74.
(Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 191–211). New York: Dickinson, C. M., & Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1991). Simulated visual im-
Plenum Press.
pairment: Effects on text comprehension and reading speed. Clin-
Craik, F. I. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (Eds.). (2000). The handbook of ical Vision Sciences, 6, 301–308.
aging and cognition (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Diehl, M., Hastings, C. T., & Stanton, J. M. (2001). Self-concept
Damon, W. (1996). The lifelong transformation of moral goals differentiation across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging,
through social inf luence. In P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger 16, 643–654.
(Eds.), Interactive minds: Life span perspectives on the social
foundation of cognition (pp. 198–220). New York: Cambridge Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1998). Subjective well-being and age: An in-
University Press. ternational analysis. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geri-
atrics, 17, 304–324.
D’Andrade, R. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Dittmann-Kohli, F. (1991). Meaning and personality change from
early to late adulthood. European Journal of Personality, 1, 98–103.
Dannefer, D. (1989). Human action and its place in theories of aging.
Journal of Aging Studies, 3, 1–20. Dittmann-Kohli, F., Bode, C., & Westerhof, G. J. (2001). Selbst- und
Datan, N., & Reese, H. W. (Eds.). (1977). Life span developmental lebensvorstellungen in der zweiten lebenshälfte: Ergebnisse aus
psychology: Dialectical perspectives on experimental research. dem alters-survey. In F. U. J. Bundesministerium für Familie, Se-
New York: Academic Press. nioren (Ed.), Die zweite lebenshälfte: Psychologische perspek-
tiven—Ergebnisse des alters-survey (pp. 549–584). Stuttgart,
Davidson, R. J. (1984). Affect, cognition, and hemispheric special-
ization. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotion, Germany: Kohlhammer.
cognition, and behavior (pp. 320–365). New York: Cambridge Dixon, R. A., & Bäckman, L. (Eds.). (1995). Compensating for psy-
University Press. chological deficits and declines: Managing losses and promoting
Davidson, R. J., Jackson, D. C., & Kalin, N. H. (2000). Emotion, plas- gains. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ticity, context, and regulation: Perspectives from affective neuro- Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1986). Toward life span research on the
science. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 890–909. functions and pragmatics of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg &
Davidson, R. J., & Rickman, M. (1999). Behavioral inhibition and the R. K. Wagner (Eds.), Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of
emotional circuitry of the brain: Stability and plasticity during competence in the everyday world (pp. 203–234). New York: Cam-
the early childhood years. In L. A. Schmidt & J. Schulkin (Eds.), bridge University Press.
Extreme fear and shyness: Origins and outcomes (pp. 67–87). New Dixon, R. A., & Gould, O. N. (1996). Adults telling and retelling
York: Oxford University Press. stories collaboratively. In P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger
Dawkins, R. (1982). The extended phenotype: The gene as the unit of (Eds.), Interactive minds: Life span perspective on the social
selection. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. foundation of cognition (pp. 221–241). New York: Cambridge
Deary, I. J., & Pagliari, C. (1991). The strength of g at different University Press.
levels of ability: Have Detterman and Daniel rediscovered Dixon, R. A., & Lerner, R. M. (1988). A history of systems in devel-
Spearmans law of diminishing returns? Intelligence, 15, opmental psychology. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.),
251–255. Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (2nd ed.,
de Geus, E. J. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2002). A genetic neuroscience pp. 3–50). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
approach to human cognition. European Psychologist, 6, 241–253. Donahue, E. M., Robins, R. W., Roberts, B. W., & John, O. P. (1993).
DeKay, W. T., & Buss, D. M. (1992). Human nature, individual dif- The divided self: Concurrent and longitudinal effects of psycho-
ferences, and the importance of context: Perspectives from evolu- logical adjustment and social roles on self-concept differentia-
tionary psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 834–846.
1, 184–189.
Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., McClenny, B. D., & Porges, S. W.
Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mecha-
(2001). Neonatal cardiac vagal tone and school-age development
nism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and
outcome in very low birth weight infants. Developmental Psy-
aging. Developmental Review, 12, 45–75.
chology, 38, 56–66.
Denney, N. W. (1984). A model of cognitive development across the
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., &
life span. Developmental Review, 4, 171–191.
May, A. (2004). Changes in gray matter induced by training. Na-
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure
ture, 427, 311–312.
of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation,
and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569. Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, culture and human diver-
sity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Depue, R. A., Krauss, S. P., & Spoont, M. R. (1987). A two-dimensional
threshold model of seasonal bipolar affective disorder. In D. Mag- Ebner, N. C., & Freund, A. M. (2003, August). Win or don’t lose:
nusson & A. Oehman (Eds.), Psychopathology: An interactional Age dif ferences in personal goal focus. Poster presented at the
perspective—Personality, psychopathology, and psychotherapy 111th Meeting of the American Psychological Association,
(pp. 95–123). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Toronto, Canada.
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal
birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and bio- group selection. New York: Basic Books.
References 651

Edelstein, W., & Noam, G. (1982). Regulatory structures of the (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences
self and “postformal” stages in adulthood. Human Development, (pp. 50–93). New York: Cambridge University Press.
6, 407–422. Filipp, S.-H. (1996). Motivation and emotion. In J. E. Birren &
Eichorn, D. H., Clausen, J. A., Haan, N., Honzik, M. P., & Mussen, K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging
P. H. (Eds.). (1981). Present and past in middle life. New York: (pp. 218–235). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Academic Press. Filipp, S.-H. (1999). A three-stage model of coping with loss and
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. trauma: Lessons from patients suffering from severe and chronic
(1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left disease. In A. Maercker, M. Schützwohl, & Z. Solomon (Eds.),
hand in string players. Science, 270, 305–307. Posttraumatic stress disorder: A life span developmental perspec-
Elbogen, E. B., Carlo, G., & Spaulding, W. (2001). Hierarchical clas- tive (pp. 43–80). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
sification and the integration of self-structure in late adoles- Filipp, S.-H., & Buch-Bartos, K. (1994). Vergleichsprozesse und leben-
cence. Journal of Adolescence, 24, 657–670. szufriedenheit im alter: Ergebnisse einer pilotstudie [Comparison
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1974). Children of the Great Depression. Chicago: processes and life satisfaction in old age]. Zeitschrift für Entwick-
University of Chicago Press. lungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 26, 22–34.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (Ed.). (1985). Life course dynamics: Trajectories and Filipp, S.-H., & Klauer, T. (1986). Conceptions of self over the life
transitions 1968–1980. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. span: Ref lections on the dialectics of change. In M. M. Baltes &
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1986). Military times and turning points in men’s P. B. Baltes (Eds.), The psychology of aging and control
lives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 233–245. (pp. 167–204). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1990). Studying lives in a changing society: Socio- Filipp, S.-H., & Klauer, T. (1991). Subjective well-being in the face
logical and personological explorations. In A. Rabin, R. Zucker, of critical life events: The case of successful coping. In
R. Emmons, & S. Frank (Eds.), Studying persons and lives F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), The social psychology
(pp. 201–247). New York: Springer. of subjective well-being (Vol. 21, pp. 213–234). Oxford, England:
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1994). Time, human agency, and social change: Per- Pergamon Press.
spectives on the life course. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, 4–15. Filipp, S.-H., & Mayer, A. K. (2005). Selbstkonzepentwicklung [Self-
Elliot, A. J., Sheldon, K. M., & Church, M. A. (1997). Avoidance per- concept development]. In J. B. Asendorpf & H. Rauh (Eds.),
sonal goals and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psy- Soziale, emotionale und Persönlichkeitsentwicklung: Enzyklopädie
chology Bulletin, 23(9), 915–927. der Psychologie [Social, emotional, and personality development:
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Encyclopedia of psychology]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connec- Filipp, S.-H., & Staudinger, U. M. (Hrsg.). (2005). Entwicklungspsy-
tionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. chologie des mittleren und hoheren Erwachsenenalters (Enzy-
Emmons, R. A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Personal goals and sub- klopädie für Psychologie: Entwicklungspsychologie, Vol. 6).
jective well-being. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The Göttingen: Hogrefe.
psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behav- Finch, C. E. (1990). Longevity, senescence, and the genome. Chicago:
ior (pp. 313–337). New York: Guilford Press. University of Chicago Press.
Engle, R. W., Conway, A. R. A., Tuholski, S. W., & Shishler, R. J. Finch, C. E., & Zelinski, E. M. (2005). Normal aging of brain struc-
(1995). A resource account of inhibition. Psychological Science, ture and cognition: Evolutionary perspectives. Research in Human
6, 122–125. Development, 2(1/2), 69–82.
Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S. W. (1999). Individual dif- Finkel, D., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., Plomin, R., & Berg, S.
ferences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about (1996). Cross-sequential analysis of genetic inf luences on cogni-
controlled attention, general f luid intelligence, and functions of tive ability in the Swedish Adoption / Twin Study of Aging. Aging,
the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake & S. Priti (Eds.), Models of Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 3, 84–99.
working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive Fisk, J. E., & Warr, P. (1996). Age-related impairment in associative
control (pp. 102–134). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer- learning: The role of anxiety, arousal, and learning self-efficacy.
sity Press. Personality and Individual Dif ferences, 21, 675–686.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New
of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. York: McGraw-Hill.
Psychological Review, 100, 363–406. Flavell, J. H. (1970). Cognitive changes in adulthood. In L. R. Goulet
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (Eds.). (1991). Towards a general theory & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life span developmental psychology: Re-
of expertise: Prospects and limits. New York: Cambridge Univer- search and theory (pp. 247–253). New York: Academic Press.
sity Press. Flavell, J. H. (1992). Cognitive development: Past, present, and fu-
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Is- ture. Developmental Psychology, 28, 998–1005.
sues Monograph 1. New York: International University Press. Fleeson, W., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Beyond present-day personality
Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (1985). Ontogenesis and socio- assessment: An encouraging exploration of the measurement prop-
genesis: Problematics for theory and research about development erties and predictive power of subjective lifetime personality.
and socialization across the life span. American Sociological Re- Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 411–430.
view, 50, 659–676. Fleeson, W., & Heckhausen, J. (1997). More or less “me” in past,
Featherman, D. L., Smith, J., & Peterson, J. G. (1990). Successful present, and future: Perceived lifetime personality during adult-
aging in a “post-retired” society. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes hood. Psychology and Aging, 12, 125–136.
652 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests re- havioral predictors of longevity: The aging and death of the “ Ter-
ally measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171–191. mites.” American Psychologist, 50, 69–78.
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Pimley, S., & Novacek, J. (1987). Age Friedman, H. S., Tucker, J. S., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Schwartz, J. E.,
differences in stress and coping processes. Psychology and Aging, Wingard, D. L., & Criqui, M. H. (1993). Does childhood person-
2, 171–184. ality predict longevity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Folkow, B., & Svanborg, A. (1993). Physiology of cardiovascular ogy, 65, 176–185.
aging. Psysiological Reviews, 73, 725–764. Fries, J. F., & Crapo, L. M. (1981). Vitality and aging. San Fran-
Ford, D. H., & Lerner, R. M. (1992). Developmental systems theory: cisco: Freeman.
An integrative approach. London: Sage. Funder, D. C. (2001). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology,
Forster, J. M., & Gallagher, D. (1986). An exploratory study compar- 52, 197–221.
ing depressed and nondepressed elders coping strategies. Journals Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience in children’s adaptation to negative
of Gerontology, 41, 91–93.
life events and stressed environments. Pediatric Annals, 20,
Fox, P. W., Hershberger, S. L., & Bouchard, T. J. (1996). Genetic and 459–466.
environmental contributions to the acquisition of a motor skill.
Garrett, H. E. (1946). A developmental theory of intelligence. Amer-
Nature, 384, 356–358.
ican Psychologist, 1, 372–378.
Frazier, L. D., Hooker, K., Johnson, P. M., & Klaus, C. R. (2000). Con-
Gehlen, A. (1956). Urmensch und Spätkultur. Bonn, Germany:
tinuity and change in possible selves in later life: A 5-year longitu-
dinal study. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 237–243. Athenäum.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions George, L. K., & Okun, M. A. (1985). Self-concept content. In E. Pal-
speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emo- more, E. W. Busse, G. L.
tions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191–220. Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2005). Exploring structural dy-
Freund, A. (1995). Wer bin ich? Die sebstdefinition alter menschen namics within and between sensory and intellectual functioning
[Who am I? The self-definition of older people]. Berlin, Ger- in old and very old age: Longitudinal evidence from the Berlin
many: Sigma. Aging Study. Intelligence, 33, 555–587.
Freund, A. M. (2002). Selection, optimization, and compensation. In Gigerenzer, G. (1996). Rationality: Why social context matters. In
D. J. Ekerdt, R. A. Applebaum, K. C. Holden, S. G. Post, K. Rock- P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), Interactive minds:
wood, R. Schulz, et al. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of aging (Vol. 4, Life span perspectives on the social foundation of cognition
pp. 1257–1260). New York: Macmillan. (pp. 317–346). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Freund, A. M. (in press). Differential motivational consequences of Gigerenzer, G. (2003). The adaptive toolbox and life span develop-
goal focus in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging. ment: Common questions. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lindenberger
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization, and (Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues with life
compensation as strategies of life-management: Correlations with span psychology (pp. 423–435). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
subjective indicators of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, Gigerenzer, G., & Todd, P. M. (Eds.). (1999). Simple heuristics that
13, 531–543. make us smart. Oxford University Press.
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration of selection, Gignac, M. A. M., Cott, C., & Badley, E. M. (2002). Adaptation to
optimization, and compensation: An action-theoretical conceptu- disability: Applying selective optimization with compensation to
alization of a theory of developmental regulation. In W. J. Perrig the behaviors of older adults with osteoarthritis. Psychology and
& A. Grob (Eds.), Control of human behavior, mental processes, Aging, 17, 520–524.
and consciousness (pp. 35–58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gobet, F., Lane, P. C. R., Croker, S., Cheng, P. C.-H., Jones, G.,
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002a). The adaptiveness of selec- Oliver, I., et al. (2001). Chunking mechanisms in human learning.
tion, optimization, and compensation as strategies of life man- Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(6), 236–243.
agement: Evidence from a preference study on proverbs. Journal
Goldberg, L. R. (1993). “ The structure of phenotypic personality
of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, 426–434.
traits”: Author’s reactions to the six comments. American Psy-
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002b). Life-management strategies chologist, 48, 1303–1304.
of selection, optimization, and compensation: Measurement by
Goldberg, T. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (2004). Genes and the parsing
self-report and construct validity. Journal of Personality and So-
of cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 325–335.
cial Psychology, 82, 642–662.
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.). (1996). The psychology of
Freund, A. M., & Ebner, N. C. (2005). The aging self: Shifting from
action: Linking cognition and motivation to action. New York:
promoting gains to balancing losses. In W. Greve, K. Rothermund,
& D. Wentura (Eds.), The adaptive self: Personal continuity and Guilford Press.
intentional self-development (pp. 185–202). New York: Hogrefe. Gottlieb, G. (1998). Normally occurring environmental and behav-
Freund, A. M., & Smith, J. (1999). Content and function of the self- ioral inf luences on gene activity: From central dogma to probalis-
definition in old and very old age. Journal of Gerontology: Psy- tic epigenesis. Psychological Review, 105, 792–802.
chological Sciences, 55–67. Gould, S. J. (1984). Relationship of individual and group change:
Frey, K. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1990). Strategies for comparative evalua- Ontogeny and phylogeny in biology. Human Development, 27,
tion: Maintaining a sense of competence across the life span. In 233–239.
R. J. Sternberg & J. John Kolligian (Eds.), Competence considered Gould, S. J., & Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation—A missing term in the
(pp. 167–189). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. science of form. Paleobiology, 8, 4–15.
Friedman, H. S., Tucker, J. S., Schwartz, J. E., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Goulet, L. R., & Baltes, P. B. (Eds.). (1970). Life span developmental
Martin, L. R., Wingard, D. L., et al. (1995). Psychosocial and be- psychology: Research and theory. New York: Academic Press.
References 653

Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality
H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model of personality (pp. 246–276). Berlin, development (5th ed., pp. 553–617). New York: Wiley.
Germany: Springer-Verlag. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension,
Greenwald, A. G., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1984). The self. In R. S. Wyer and aging: A review and a new view. Psychology of Learning and
& T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, Motivation, 22, 193–225.
pp. 129–178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Havighurst, R. J. (1948). Developmental tasks and education. New
Greve, W., & Staudinger, U. M. (in press). Resilience in later adult- York: Davis McKay.
hood and old age: Resources and potentials for successful aging. To Havighurst, R. J. (1973). History of developmental psychology: So-
appear in D. Cicchetti & A. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental Psycho- cialization and personality development through the life span. In
pathology (2nd ed.). P. B. Baltes & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Life span developmental psy-
Greve, W., & Wentura, D. (2003). Immunizing the self: Self-concept chology (pp. 3–24). New York: Academic Press.
stabilization through reality-adaptive self-definitions. Personal- Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 39–50.
Heckhausen, J. (1997). Developmental regulation across adulthood:
Grimm, J. (1860). Rede über das alter. In Kleinere Schriften von Primary and secondary control of age-related challenges. Devel-
Jacob Grimm (Vol. 1, pp. 188–210). Berlin, Germany: Harrwitz opmental Psychology, 33, 176–187.
und Grossmann.
Heckhausen, J. (Ed.). (2000). Motivational psychology of human de-
Groffmann, K. I. (1970). Life span developmental psychology in Eu- velopment: Developing motivation and motivating development.
rope. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life span developmen-
Oxford, England: Elsevier.
tal psychology: Research and theory (pp. 54–68). New York:
Academic Press. Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Gains and
losses in development throughout adulthood as perceived by dif-
Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., Skorpen, C. G.,
ferent adult age groups. Developmental Psychology, 25, 109–121.
& Hsu, A. Y. C. (1997). Emotion and aging: Experience, expres-
sion, and control. Psychology and Aging, 12, 590–599. Heckhausen, J., & Krueger, J. (1993). Developmental expectations for
the self and most other people: Age-grading in three functions of
Güntürkün, O. (2006). Epilogue: Letters on nature and nurture. In
social comparison. Developmental Psychology, 29, 539–548.
P. B. Baltes, P. Reuter-Lorenz, & F. Rösler (Eds.), Lifespan de-
velopment and the brain: The perspective of biocultural co- Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1993). Optimization by selection and
constructivism (pp. 379–397). Cambridge, England: Cambridge compensation: Balancing primary and secondary control in life
University Press. span development. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
ment, 16, 287–303.
Guthke, J., & Wiedl, K. H. (Eds.). (1996). Dynamisches Testen. Göt-
tingen, Germany: Hogrefe. Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life span theory of control.
Psychological Review, 102, 284–304.
Hagen, E. H., & Hammerstein, P. (2005). Evolutionary biology and
the strategic view of ontogeny: Genetic strategies provide robust- Heidrich, S. M., & Ryff, C. D. (1993). The role of social comparison
ness and f lexibility in the life course. Research in Human Devel- processes in the psychological adaptation of the elderly. Journals
opment, 2(1/2), 83–97. of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, 127–136.
Hale, S. (1990). A global developmental trend in cognitive processing Helmchen, H., Baltes, M. M., Geiselmann, B., Kanowski, S., Linden,
speed. Child Development, 61, 653–663. M., Reischies, F. M., et al. (1999). Psychiatric illnesses in old age.
Halford, G. S. (1993). Children’s understanding: The development of In P. B. Baltes & K. U. Mayer (Eds.), Berlin Aging Study: Aging
mental models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. from 70 to 100 (pp. 167–196). New York: Cambridge University
Hall, G. S. (1922). Senescence: The last half of life. New York: Appleton. Press.
Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2002). Effects of domain knowl- Helson, R., & Kwan, V. S. Y. (2000). Personality development in
edge, working memory capacity, and age on cognitive perfor- adulthood: The broad picture and processes in one longitudinal
mance: An investigation of the knowledge-is-power hypothesis. sample. In S. Hampson (Ed.), Advances in personality psychology
Cognitive Psychology, 44, 339–387. (Vol. 1, pp. 77–106). London: Routledge.
Hamer, D., & Copeland, P. (1998). Living with our genes. New Helson, R., & Wink, P. (1987). Two conceptions of maturity exam-
York: Doubleday. ined in the findings of a longitudinal study. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 53, 531–541.
Hammerstein, P. (1996). The evolution of cooperation within and be-
tween generations. In P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), In- Hermans, H. J. M. (1996). Voicing the self: From information process-
teractive minds: Life span perspectives on the social foundation of ing to dialogical interchange. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 31–50.
cognition (pp. 35–58). New York: Cambridge University Press. Hertzog, C. (1985). An individual differences perspective: Impli-
Harlow, R. E., & Cantor, N. (1996). Still participating after all these cations for cognitive research in gerontology. Research on
years: A study of life task participation in later life. Journal of Aging, 7, 7–45.
Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1235–1249. Hertzog, C., Cooper, B. P., & Fisk, A. D. (1996). Aging and individ-
Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1998). Anger and frontal brain ual differences in the development of skilled memory search per-
activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation de- formance. Psychology and Aging, 11, 497–520.
spite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Hertzog, C., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2003). Assessing psychological
Psychology, 74, 1310–1316. change in adulthood: An overview of methodological issues. Psy-
Harris, D. B. (Ed.). (1957). The concept of development. Minneapolis: chology and Aging, 18, 639–657.
University of Minnesota Press. Hertzog, C., & Schaie, K. W. (1986). Stability and change in adult in-
Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In telligence: Pt. 1. Analysis of longitudinal covariance structures.
W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Hand- Psychology and Aging, 1, 159–171.
654 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Hertzog, C., & Schaie, K. W. (1988). Stability and change in adult in- Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences
telligence: Pt. 2. Simultaneous analysis of longitudinal means and in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of Compara-
covariance structures. Psychology and Aging, 3, 122–130. tive Neurology, 387, 167–178.
Herzog, A. R., Franks, M. M., Markus, H. R., & Holmberg, D. (1998). Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Components
Activities and well-being in older age: Effects of self-concept and of visual orienting in early infancy: Contingency learning, antici-
educational attainment. Psychology and Aging, 13, 179–185. patory looking, and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
Herzog, A. R., & Markus, H. R. (1999). The self-concept in life span science, 3, 335–344.
and aging research. In V. L. Bengtson & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Jones, H. E., & Conrad, H. (1933). The growth and decline of intelli-
Handbook of theories of aging (pp. 227–252). New York: Springer. gence: A study of a homogeneous group between the ages of 10
Hess, T. M., & Pullen, S. M. (1996). Memory in context. In F. and 60. Genetic Psychological Monographs, 13, 223–298.
Blanchard-Fields & T. M. Hess (Eds.), Perspectives on cognitive Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Keller, T. A. (1996). The capacity
change in adulthood and aging (pp. 387–427). New York: theory of comprehension: New frontiers of evidence and argu-
McGraw-Hill. ments. Psychological Review, 103(4), 773–780.
Hetherington, E. M., Lerner, R. M., & Perlmutter, M. (Eds.). (1988). Kagan, J. (1964). American longitudinal research on psychological
Child development in life span perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. development. Child Development, 35, 1–32.
Hobfoll, S. E. (2001). The inf luence of culture, community, and the Kagan, J. (1980). Perspectives on continuity. In O. G. Brim Jr. & J.
nested-self in the stress process: Advancing conservation of re- Kagan (Eds.), Constancy and change in human development
sources theory. Applied Psychology: An International Review, (pp. 26–74). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
50, 337–421. Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New York: Basic Books.
Holahan, C. K. (1988). Relation of life goals at age 70 to activity par- Kagan, J. (1998). Three seductive ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
ticipation and health and psychological well-being among Ter- University Press.
man’s gifted men and women. Psychology and Aging, 3, 286–291. Kagan, J., & Moss, H. (1962). Birth to maturity. New York: Wiley.
Holahan, C. K., Sears, R. R., & Cronbach, L. J. (1995). The gifted Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1991). Temperamental factors in human
group in later maturity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. development. American Psychologist, 46, 856–862.
Hollingworth, H. L. (1927). Mental growth and decline: A survey of Kahn, R. L., & Antonucci, T. C. (1980). Convoys over the life course:
developmental psychology. New York: Appleton. Attachment, roles, and social support. In P. B. Baltes & O. G.
Hommel, B., Li, K. Z. H., & Li, S.-C. (2004). Visual search across the Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 3,
life span. Developmental Psychology, 40, 545–558. pp. 253–286). New York: Academic Press.
Hooker, K. (2002). New directions for research in personality and Kail, R. (1996). Nature and consequences of developmental change in
aging: A comprehensive model for linking level, structure, and speed of processing. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 55, 133–138.
processes. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 318–334. Kavsek, M. (2004). Predicting later IQ from infant visual habituation
Horn, J. L. (1970). Organization of data on life span development of and dishabituation: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Develop-
human abilities. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Life span mental Psychology, 25, 369–393.
developmental psychology: Research and theory (pp. 423–466). Kempermann, G. (2006). Adult neurogenesis. In P. B. Baltes,
New York: Academic Press. P. Reuter-Lorenz, & F. Rösler (Eds.), Lifespan development and
Horn, J. L. (1982). The theory of f luid and crystallized intelligence in the brain: The perspective of biocultural co-constructivism
relation to concepts of cognitive psychology and aging in adult- (pp. 82–107). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
hood. In F. I. M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive Kirkwood, T. B. L. (2003). Age differences in evolutionary selection
processes (pp. 237–278). New York: Plenum Press. benefits. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Under-
Horn, J. L. (1989). Model of intelligence. In R. L. Linn (Ed.), Intelli- standing human development: Dialogues with life span psychology
gence: Measurement, theory, and public policy (pp. 29–73). Ur- (pp. 45–57). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
bana: University of Illinois Press. Kliegl, R., & Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theory-guided analysis of mecha-
nisms of development and aging mechanisms through testing-the-
Horn, J. L., & Hofer, S. M. (1992). Major abilities and development
limits and research on expertise. In C. Schooler & K. W. Schaie
in the adult period. In R. J. Sternberg & C. A. Berg (Eds.), Intel-
(Eds.), Cognitive functioning and social structure over the life
lectual development (pp. 44–99). New York: Cambridge Univer-
course (pp. 95–119). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
sity Press.
Kliegl, R., Krampe, R., & Mayr, U. (2003). Formal models of age dif-
Houdé, O. (1995). Rationalité, développement, et inhibition. Paris:
ferences in task-complexity effects. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lin-
Presses Universitaires de France.
denberger (Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues
Hoyer, W. J. (1987). Acquisition of knowledge and the decentraliza- with life span psychology (pp. 289–313). Boston: Kluwer Academic.
tion of g in adult intellectual development. In C. Schooler & K. W. Kliegl, R., Mayr, U., & Krampe, R. T. (1994). Time-accuracy func-
Schaie (Eds.), Cognitive functioning and social structure over the tions for determining process and person differences: An applica-
life course (pp. 120–141). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. tion to cognitive aging. Cognitive Psychology, 26, 134–164.
Hultsch, D. F., & MacDonald, S. W. S. (2004). Intraindividual vari- Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1990). On the locus and process
ability in performance as a theoretical window onto cognitive of magnification of age differences during mnemonic training.
aging. In R. A. Dixon, L. Bäckman, & L.-G. Nilsson (Eds.), New Developmental Psychology, 26, 894–904.
frontiers in cognitive aging (pp. 65–88). New York: Oxford Uni- Kline, J. P., Blackhart, G. C., Woodward, K. M., Williams, S. R., &
versity Press. Schwartz, G. E. R. (2000). Anterior electroencephalographic
Humphreys, L. G., & Davey, T. C. (1988). Continuity in intellectual asymmetry changes in elderly women in response to a pleasant
growth from 12 months to 9 years. Intelligence, 12, 183–197. and an unpleasant odor. Biological Psychology, 52, 241–250.
References 655

Klix, F. (1993). Erwachendes Denken: Geistige Leistungen aus evolu- Kunzmann, U., Little, T., & Smith, J. (2000). Is age-related stability
tionspsychologischer Sicht. Heidelberg, Germany: Spektrum of subjective well-being a paradox? Cross-sectional and longitu-
Akademischer Verlag. dinal evidence from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and
Kogan, N. (1990). Personality and aging. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Aging, 15(511–526).
Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging Kunzmann, U., Little, T., & Smith, J. (2002). Perceiving control: A dou-
(pp. 330–346). New York: Academic Press. ble-edged sword in old age. Journal of Gerontology, 57B, 484–491.
Kohli, M., & Meyer, J. W. (1986). Social structure and social con- Labouvie, G. V., Frohring, W., Baltes, P. B., & Goulet, L. R. (1973).
struction of life stages. Human Development, 29, 145–180. Changing relationship between recall performance and abilities
Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1983). Work and personality. Nor- as a function of stage of learning and timing of recall. Journal of
wood, NJ: Ablex. Educational Psychology, 64, 191–198.
Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., McAuley, E., Labouvie-Vief, G. (1977). Adult cognitive development. In search of
Harrison, C. R., et al. (1999). Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive alternative interpretations. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 23, 277–263.
function. Nature, 400, 418–419. Labouvie-Vief, G. (1980). Beyond formal operations: Uses and lim-
Kramer, A. F., Hahn, H., & Gopher, D. (2000). Task coordination and its of pure logic in life span development. Human Development,
aging: Explorations of executive control processes in the task 23, 141–161.
switching paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 101, 339–378. Labouvie-Vief, G. (1982). Dynamic development and mature auton-
Kramer, A. F., Louis, B., Coats, E. J., Colcombe, S., Dong, W., & omy: A theoretical prologue. Human Development, 25, 161–191.
Greenough, W. T. (in press). Environmental inf luences on cogni- Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakim-Larson, J., & Hobart, C. J. (1987). Age,
tive and brain plasticity during aging. Journal of Gerontology. ego level, and the life span development of coping and defense
Kramer, A. F., & Willis, S. L. (2002). Enhancing the cognitive vital- processes. Psychology and Aging, 2, 286–293.
ity of older adults. Current Directions in Psychological Science, Labouvie-Vief, G., Lumley, M. A., Jain, E., & Heinze, H. (2003). Age
11, 173–177. and gender differences in cardiac reactivity and subjective emo-
Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Intelligence as adaptive re- tion responses to emotional autobiographical memories. Emotion,
source development and resource allocation: A new look 3, 115–126.
through the lenses of SOC and expertise. In R. J. Sternberg & Lachman, M. E. (1986a). Locus of control in aging research: A case
E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Perspectives on the psychology of abili- for mulitdimensional and domain-specific assessment. Psychol-
ties, competencies, and expertise (pp. 31– 69). New York: Cam- ogy and Aging, 1, 34–40.
bridge University Press. Lachman, M. E. (1986b). Personal control in later life: Stability,
Kray, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task change, and cognitive correlates. In M. M. Baltes & P. B. Baltes
switching. Psychology and Aging, 15, 126–147. (Eds.), The psychology of control and aging (pp. 207–236). Hills-
Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Goals as knowledge structures. In P. M. dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Link- Lachman, M. E. (1991). Perceived control over memory aging: Devel-
ing cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 599–618). New opmental and intervention perspectives. Journal of Social Issues,
York: Guilford Press. 47, 159–175.
Kruse, A. (1992). Alter im Lebenslauf. In P. B. Baltes & J. Mittel- Lachman, M. E. (2001). Handbook of midlife development. New
straß (Eds.), Zukunft des Alterns und gesellschaftliche Entwick- York: Wiley.
lung (pp. 331–355). Berlin, Germany: W. de Gruyter. Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, L. (1998). Sociodemographic variations
in the sense of control by domain: Findings from the MacArthur
Kuhlen, R. G. (1963). Age and intelligence: The significance of cul-
Studies of Midlife. Psychology and Aging, 13, 553–562.
tural change in longitudinal versus cross-sectional findings. Vita
Humana, 6, 113–124. Lang, F. R., Rieckmann, N., & Baltes, M. M. (2002). Adapting to
aging losses: Do resources facilitate strategies of selection, com-
Kuhn, D. (1995). Microgenetic study of change: What has it told us?
pensation, and optimization in everyday functioning? Journals of
Psychological Science, 6, 133–139.
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, P501–P509.
Kunzmann, U. (2004). The emotional-motivational side to wisdom.
Lautrey, J. (2003). A pluralistic approach to cognitive differentiation
In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice.
and development. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Lautrey (Eds.), Models of
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. intelligence: International perspectives (pp. 117–131). Washing-
Kunzmann, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003a). Beyond the traditional scope ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
of intelligence: Wisdom in action. In R. J. Sternberg, J. Lautrey, Lazarus, R. S. (1996). The role of coping in the emotions and how
& T. I. Lubart (Eds.), Models of intelligence: International per- coping changes over the life course. In C. Magai & S. H. McFad-
spectives (pp. 329–343). Washington, DC: American Psychologi- den (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging
cal Association. (pp. 284–306). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kunzmann, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003b). Wisdom-related knowledge: Lazarus, R. S., & Golden, G. Y. (1981). The function of denial in
Affective, motivational, and interpersonal correlates. Personality stress, coping, and aging. In J. L. Lecci, L., Okun, M. A., &
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1104–1119. Karoly, P. (1994). Life regrets and current goals as predictors of
Kunzmann, U., & Grühn, D. (2005). Age differences in emotional re- psychological adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
activity: The sample case of sadness. Psychology and Aging, 20, chology, 66(4), 731–741.
47–59. Lehman, H. C. (1953). Age and achievement. Princeton, NJ: Prince-
Kunzmann, U., Kupperbusch, C. S., & Levenson, R. W. (2005). Emo- ton University Press.
tion regulation in adulthood: A comparison of two age groups. Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. (1983). Temperament and adaptation
Psychology and Aging, 20, 144–159. across life: Theoretical and empirical issues. In P. B. Baltes &
656 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 5, Cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp. 103–146). Seattle, WA:
pp. 198–231). New York: Academic Press. Hogrefe & Huber.
Lerner, R. M. (Ed.). (1983). Developmental psychology: Historical Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (2002). Coconstructed functionality in-
and philosophical perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. stead of functional normality: Commentary. Behavioral and Brain
Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York: Sciences, 25(6), 761–762.
Cambridge University Press. Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., Hommel, B., Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W.,
Lerner, R. M. (1991). Changing organism-context relations as the & Baltes, P. B. (2004). Transformations in the couplings among
basic process of development: A developmental contextual per- intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across
spective. Developmental Psychology, 27, 27–32. the life span. Psychological Science, 15, 155–163.
Lerner, R. M. (1995). The limits of biological inf luence: Behav- Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., & Sikström, S. (2001). Aging cognition:
ioral genetics as the emperor’s new clothes. Psychological In- From neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Sci-
quiry, 6, 145–156. ence, 5(11), 479–486.
Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development Lienert, G. A., & Crott, H. W. (1964). Studies on the factor structure
(3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. of intelligence in children, adolescents, and adults. Vita Humana,
Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, N. (Eds.). (1981). Individuals as 7, 147–163.
producers of their development: A life span perspective. New York: Lindenberger, U. (2001). Life span theories of cognitive develop-
Academic Press. ment. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International ency-
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. clopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 8848–8854).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Oxford, England: Elsevier Science.
Levenson, R. W. (2000). Expressive, physiological, and subjective Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory functioning and in-
changes in emotion across adulthood. In S. H. Qualls & N. Abeles
telligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychology and Aging,
(Eds.), Dialogues about aging: Psychology responds to the aging
9, 339–355.
revolution (pp. 123–140). Washington, DC: American Psychologi-
cal Association. Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1995a). Kognitive Leistungs-
fähigkeit im hohen Alter: Erste Ergebnisse aus der Berliner Al-
Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., Friesen, W. V., & Ekman, P.
(1991). Emotion, physiology, and expression in old age. Psychol- tersstudie. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 203, 283–317.
ogy and Aging, 6, 28–35. Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1995b). Testing-the-limits and ex-
Levinson, B., & Reese, H. W. (1967). Patterns of discrimination perimental simulation: Two methods to explicate the role of learn-
learning set in preschool children, fifth-graders, college fresh- ing in development. Human Development, 38, 349–360.
men, and the aged. Monographs of the Society for Research in Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). Die Entwicklungspsycholo-
Child Development, 32. gie der lebensspanne: Johann Nicolaus Tetens zu Ehren
Levinson, D. J. (1986). A conception of adult development. American (1736–1807) [Life span psychology: In honor of Johann Nicolaus
Psychologist, 41, 3–13. Tetens (1736–1807)]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 207, 299–323.
Li, K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Lindenberger, U., Kliegl, R., & Baltes, P. B. (1992). Professional ex-
Walking while memorizing: Age-related differences in compensa- pertise does not eliminate negative age differences in imagery-
tory behavior. Psychological Science, 12, 230–237. based memory performance during adulthood. Psychology and
Li, S.-C. (2002). Connecting the many levels and facets of cognitive Aging, 7, 585–593.
aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 38–43. Lindenberger, U., Li, S.-C., & Bäckman, L. (Eds.). (in press).
Li, S.-C. (2003). Biocultural orchestration of developmental plastic- Methodological and conceptual advances in the study of brain-
ity across levels: The interplay of biology and culture in shaping behavior dynamics: A multivariate life span perspective [Special
the mind and behavior across the life span. Psychological Bulletin, issue]. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews.
129(2), 171–194. Lindenberger, U., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Memorizing
Li, S.-C. (in press). Neurocomputational perspectives linking neuro- while walking: Increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood
modulation, processing noise, representational distinctiveness, to old age. Psychology and Aging, 15, 417–436.
and cognitive aging. In R. Cabeza, L. Nyberg, & D. Park (Eds.),
Lindenberger, U., & Pötter, U. (1998). The complex nature of
Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral
unique and shared effects in hierarchical linear regression: Im-
aging. New York: Oxford University Press.
plications for developmental psychology. Psychological Meth-
Li, S.-C., Aggen, S. H., Nesselroade, J. R., & Baltes, P. B. (2001).
ods, 3, 218–230.
Short-term f luctuations in elderly people’s sensorimotor function-
ing predict text and spatial memory performance: The MacArthur Lindenberger, U., Scherer, H., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). The strong con-
Successful Aging Studies. Gerontology, 47(2), 100–116. nection between sensory and cognitive performance in old age:
Li, S.-C., & Freund, A. M. (2005). Advances in life span psychology: Not due to sensory acuity reductions operating during cognitive
A focus on biocultural and personal inf luences. Research in assessment. Psychology and Aging, 2, 196–205.
Human Development, 2(1&2), 1–23. Lindenberger, U., Singer, T., & Baltes, P. B. (2002). Longitudinal selec-
Li, S.-C., Huxhold, O., & Schmiedek, F. (2004). Aging and attentu- tivity in aging populations: Separating mortality-associated versus
ated processing robustness: Evidence from cognitive and sensori- experimental components in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Jour-
motor functioning. Gerontology, 50, 28–34. nal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B(6), 474–482.
Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification: A Lindenberger, U., & Oertzen, T. von (in press). Variability in cogni-
computational exploration of the link between deterioration of tive aging: From taxonomy to theory. In F. I. M. Craik & E. Bia-
neurotransmitter systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abil- lystok (Eds.), Life span cognition: Mechanisms of change. Oxford,
ities in old age. In L.-G. Nilsson & H. J. Markowitsch (Eds.), England: Oxford University Press.
References 657

Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against Marsh, H. W., Ellis, L. A., & Craven, R. G. (2002). How do preschool
stress-related depression and illness. Journal of Personality and children feel about themselves? Unraveling measurement and
Social Psychology, 52, 663–676. multidimensional self-concept structure. Developmental Psychol-
Loevinger, J. (1976). Ego development: Conception and theory. San ogy, 38, 376–393.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Marsiske, M., Lang, F. R., Baltes, M. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995).
Lourenço, O., & Machado, A. (1996). In defense of Piaget’s the- Selective optimization with compensation: Life span perspec-
ory: A reply to 10 common criticisms. Psychological Review, tives on successful human development. In R. A. Dixon &
103, 143–164. L. Bäckman (Eds.), Compensation for psychological defects and
Lövdén, M., Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2004). Cognition in declines: Managing losses and promoting gains (pp. 35–79).
the Berlin Aging Study (BASE): The first 10 years. Aging, Neu- Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ropsychology, and Cognition, 11, 104–133. Martin, G. M., Austad, S. N., & Johnson, T. E. (1996). Genetic analy-
Lövdén, M., Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2005). Social partici- sis of ageing: Role of oxidative damage and environmental
pation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old stresses. Nature Genetics, 13, 25–34.
age. Psychology and Aging 20, 423–434. Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in devel-
Lövdén, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2005). Development of intellectual opment. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238.
abilities in old age: From age gradients to individuals. In Maturana, H., & Varela, F. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition: The re-
O. Wilhelm & R. W. Engle (Eds.), Handbook of understanding and alization of the living. Boston: D. Riedel.
measuring intelligence (pp. 203–221). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Max Planck Institute for Human Development. (2005). Research Re-
Maciel, A. G., Heckhausen, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). A life span per- port 2003–2004. Berlin, Germany: Author.
spective on the interface between personality and intelligence. In Mayer, K. U. (1990). Lebensverläufe und sozialer Wandel: An-
R. J. Sternberg & P. Ruzgis (Eds.), Intelligence and personality merkungen zu einem forschungsprogramm. Kölner Zeitschrift für
(pp. 61–103). New York: Cambridge University Press. Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie, 42(Sonderheft 31), 7–21.
Maercker, A. (1995). Existentielle konfrontation: Eine untersuchung Mayer, K. U. (2003). The sociology of the life course and life span
im rahmen eines psychologischen weisheitsparadigmas [Existential psychology: Diverging or converging pathways. In U. M.
confrontation: A study in the framework of a psychological wisdom
Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Understanding human de-
paradigm] (Studien und Berichte Nr. 62). Berlin, Germany: Max
velopment: Dialogues with life span psychology (pp. 463–481).
Planck Institute for Human Development.
Boston: Kluwer Academic.
Magnus, K., Diener, E., Fujita, F., & Pavot, W. (1993). Extraversion
Mayr, U. (2001). Age differences in the selection of mental sets: The
and neuroticism as predictors for objective life events: A longi-
role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap.
tudinal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology and Aging, 16, 96–109.
65, 1046–1053.
Mayr, U., Kliegl, R., & Krampe, R. T. (1996). Sequential and coordi-
Magnusson, D. (1990). Personality development from an interactional
native processing dynamics in figural transformations across the
perspective. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: The-
life span. Cognition, 59, 61–90.
ory and research (pp. 193–222). New York: Guilford Press.
McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self:
Magnusson, D. (Ed.). (1996). The life span development of individu-
A contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological
als: Behavioural, neurobiological and psychosocial perspectives.
Inquiry, 7, 295–321.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
McArdle, J. J., & Bell, R. Q. (1999). An introduction to latent growth
Magnusson, D. (2001). The holistic-interactionistic paradigm: Some
models for developmental data analysis. In T. D. Little, K. U.
directions for empirical developmental research. European Psy-
Schnabel, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Modeling longitudinal and multi-
chologist, 6, 153–162.
level data: Practical issues, applied approaches, and specific ex-
Magnusson, D., Bergman, L. R., Rudinger, G., & Törestad, B. (Eds.).
amples (pp. 69–107). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
(1991). Problems and methods in longitudinal research: Stability
and change. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. McArdle, J. J., & Epstein, D. (1987). Latent growth curves within
developmental structural equation models. Child Development,
Magnusson, D., & Mahoney, J. (2003). A holistic person approach
58, 110–133.
for research on positive development. In L. G. Aspinwall &
U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of human strengths: Fun- McArdle, J. J., Ferrer-Caja, E., Hamagami, F., & Woodcock, R. W.
damental questions and future directions for a positive psychol- (2002). Comparative longitudinal structural analyses of the
ogy (pp. 227–243). Washington, DC: American Psychological growth and decline of multiple intellectual abilities over the life
Association. span. Developmental Psychology, 38, 115–142.
Mandl, H., Gruber, H., & Renkl, A. (1996). Communities of practice McArdle, J. J., Hamagami, F., Ellias, M. F., & Robbins, M. A. (1991).
toward expertise: Social foundation of university instruction. In Structural modeling of mixed longitudinal and cross-sectional
P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), Interactive minds: Life data. Experimental Aging Research, 17, 29–51.
span perspectives on the social foundation of cognition McCall, R. M. (1979). The development of intellectual functioning
(pp. 394–411). New York: Cambridge University Press. in infancy and the prediction of later, I. Q. In J. D. Osofsky
Manners, J., & Durkin, K. (2000). Processes involved in adult ego (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 707–740). New York:
development: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review, Wiley.
20, 475–513. McCall, R. M. (1994). What process mediates predictions of child-
Markus, H. R., & Wurf, E. (1987). The dynamic self-concept: A hood IQ from infant habituation and recognition memory? Specu-
social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, lations on the roles of inhibition and rate of information
38, 299–337. processing. Intelligence, 18, 107–125.
658 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

McCall, R. M., & Carriger, M. S. (1993). A meta-analysis of infant Molenaar, P. C. M., Boomsma, D. I., & Dolan, C. V. (1993). Genetic
habituation and recognition memory performance as predictors of and environmental factors in a developmental perspective. In
later IQ. Child Development, 64, 57–79. D. Magnusson, L. R. Bergman, G. Rudinger, & B. Törestad (Eds.),
McClearn, G. E., Johansson, B., Berg, S., Pedersen, N. L., Ahern, F., Problems and methods in longitudinal research: Stability and
Petrill, S. A., et al. (1997). Substantial genetic influence on cogni- change (pp. 250–273). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer-
tive abilities in twins 80 or more years old. Science, 276, 1560–1563. sity Press.
McClelland, J. L. (1996). Integration of information: Ref lections on Molenaar, P. C. M., Huizenga, H. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2003).
the theme of Attention and Performance XVI. In I. Toshui & The relationship between the structure of interindividual and in-
J. L. McClelland (Eds.), Attention and Performance XVI traindividual variability: A theoretical and empirical vindication
(pp. 633–656). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. of development systems theory. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lin-
denberger (Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues
McCrae, R. R. (1989). Age differences and changes in the use of cop-
with life span psychology (pp. 339–360). Boston: Kluwer Aca-
ing mechanisms. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,
demic.
44, 919–928.
Moss, H. A., & Susman, E. J. (1980). Longitudinal study of personal-
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Hrebick- ity development. In O. G. Brim Jr. & J. Kagan (Eds.), Constancy
ova, M., Avia, M. D., et al. (2000). Nature over nurture: Tempera- and change in human development (pp. 530–595). Cambridge,
ment, personality, and life span development. Journal of MA: Harvard University Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 173–186.
Mroczek, D. K., & Spiro, R. A., III. (2003). Modeling intraindividual
McGue, M., Bacon, S., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). Personality stability change in personality traits: Findings from the Normative Aging
and change in early adulthood: A behavioral genetic analysis. De- Study. Journals of Gerontology, 58B, P153–P165.
velopmental Psychology, 29, 96–109.
Müller-Brettel, M., & Dixon, R. A. (1990). Johann Nicolas Tetens: A
McGue, M., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. forgotten father of developmental psychology? International Jour-
(1993). Behavioral genetics of cognitive ability: A life span per- nal of Behavioral Development, 13, 215–230.
spective. In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture, Neely, A. S., & Bäckman, L. (1993). Long-term maintenance of gains
and psychology (pp. 59–76). Washington, DC: American Psycho- from memory training in older adults: Two 31⁄ 2 -year follow-up
logical Association. studies. Journal of Gerontology, 48, 233–237.
Medawar, P. B. (1946). Old age and natural death. Modern Quar-
Nelson, C. A. (2006). Neurobehavioral development in the context of
terly, 1, 30–56.
biocultural co-constructivism. In P. B. Baltes, P. Reuter-Lorenz,
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot /cool-system analysis of
& F. Rösler (Eds.), Lifespan development and the brain: The per-
delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Re-
spective of biocultural co-constructivism (pp. 61–81). Cambridge,
view, 106, 3–19. England: Cambridge University Press.
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal Nesselroade, C. S., & Schmidt McCollam, K. M. (2000). Putting the
cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202. process in development processes. International Journal of Behav-
Mischel, W. (1996). Principles of self-regulation: The nature of ioral Development, 24(3), 295–300.
willpower and self-control. In E. T.
Nesselroade, J. R. (1991). Interindividual differences in intraindivid-
Mischel, W. (2004). Toward an integrative science of the person. An- ual change. In L. M. Collins & J. L. Horn (Eds.), Best methods for
nual Review of Psychology, 55, 1–22. the analysis of change: Recent advances, unanswered questions, fu-
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1999). Integrating dispositions and pro- ture directions (pp. 92–105). Washington, DC: American Psycho-
cessing dynamics within a unified theory of personality: The logical Association.
cognitive-affective personality system. In L. Pervin & O. John Nesselroade, J. R. (2002). Elaborating the differential in differential
( Eds.), Handbook of personalit y: Theory and research psychology. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 37, 543–561.
(pp. 197–218). New York: Guilford Press. Nesselroade, J. R. (2002). Elaborating the differential in differential
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of gratifi- psychology. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 37, 543–561.
cation in children. Science, 244, 933–938. Nesselroade, J. R., & Baltes, P. B. (1974). Adolescent personality de-
Mitrushina, M., & Satz, P. (1991). Stability of cognitive functions in velopment and historical change: 1970–1972. Monographs of the
young-old versus old-old individuals. Brain Dysfunction, 4, 174–181. Society for Research in Child Development, 39.
Molander, B., & Bäckman, L. (1993). Performance of a com- Nesselroade, J. R., & Baltes, P. B. (Eds.). (1979). Longitudinal re-
plex motor skill across the life span: General trends and qualifi- search in the study of behavior and development. New York: Aca-
cations. In J. Cerella, J. Rybash, W. Hoyer, & M. L. Commons demic Press.
(Eds.), Adult information processing: Limits on loss Nesselroade, J. R., & Ghisletta, P. (2003). Structuring and measur-
(pp. 231–257). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. ing change over the life span. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Linden-
Molenaar, P. C. M. (1986). On the impossibility of acquiring more berger (Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues
powerful structures: A neglected alternative. Human Develop- with life span psychology (pp. 317–337). Boston: Kluwer
ment, 29, 245–251. Academic.
Molenaar, P. C. M., Boomsma, D. I., & Dolan, C. V. (1991). Genetic Nesselroade, J. R., & Reese, H. W. (Eds.). (1973). Life span develop-
and environmental factors in a developmental perspective. In D. mental psychology: Methological issues. New York: Academic Press.
Magnusson, L. R. Bergman, G. Rudinger, & B. Törestad (Eds.), Nesselroade, J. R., & Thompson, W. W. (1995). Selection and related
Problems and methods in longitudinal research: Stability and threats to group comparisons: An example comparing factorial
change (pp. 250–273). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer- structures of higher and lower ability groups of adult twins. Psy-
sity Press. chological Bulletin, 117, 271–284.
References 659

Neugarten, B. L. (1969). Continuities and discontinuities of psy- Pervin, L. (2001). A dynamic systems approach to personality. Euro-
chological issues into adult life. Human Development, 12, pean Psychologist, 6(3), 172–176.
121–130. Pervin, L., & John, O. (1999). Handbook of personality: Theory and
Neyer, F. J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2001). Personality-relationship research (2nd ed.). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
transaction in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Perz, C. A., DiClemente, C. C., & Carbonari, J. P. (1996). Doing the
Psychology, 81, 1190–1204. right thing at the right time? The interaction of stages and
Nurmi, J.-E. (1992). Age differences in adult life goals, concerns, processes of change in successful smoking cessation. Health Psy-
and their temporal extension: A life course approach to future- chology 15(6), 462–468.
oriented motivation. International Journal of Behavioral Develop- Peterson, C., Seligman, M. E. P., Yurko, K. H., Martin, L. R., &
ment, 15, 487–508. Friedman, H. S. (1998). Catastrophizing and untimely death. Psy-
Nyberg, L., Sandblom, J., Jones, S., Neely, A. S., Petersson, K. M., chological Science, 9, 127–130.
Ingvar, M. I., et al. (2003). Neural correlates of training-related Petersson, K. M., & Reis, A. (2006). Characteristics of illiterate and
memory improvement in adulthood and aging. Proceedings of the literate cognitive processing: Implications of brain–behavior co-
National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100(23), 13728–13733. constructivism. In P. B. Baltes, P. Reuter-Lorenz, & F. Rösler
Oberauer, K., & Kliegl, R. (2001). Beyond resources: Formal models (Eds.), Lifespan development and the brain: The perspective of bio-
for complexity effects and age differences in working memory. cultural co-constructivism (pp. 279–305). New York: Cambridge
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 187–215. University Press.
Oettingen, G. (1996). Positive fantasy and motivation. In P. M. Goll- Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichaels
witzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking manual of child psychology (pp. 703–732). New York: Wiley.
cognition and motivation to action (pp. 236–259). New York: Piaget, J. (1971). Biologie et connaissance: Essai sur les relations
Guilford Press. entre les régulations organiques et les processes cognitifs [Biology
Oettingen, G. (1997). Die psychologie des zukunftsdenkens [Psy- and knowledge: An essay on the relations between organic regula-
chology of thinking about the future]. Göttingen, Germany: tions and cognitive processes]. Chicago: University of Chicago
Hogrefe. Press. (Original work published in French, 1967)
Ogilvie, D. M., Rose, K. M., & Heppen, J. B. (2001). A comparison of Piaget, J. (1972). Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adult-
personal project motives in three age groups. Basic and Applied hood. Human Development, 15, 1–12.
Social Psychology, 23, 207–215. Piaget, J. (1980). Les formes elementaires de la dialectique [Elemen-
Osiewacz, H. D. (1995). Molekulare mechanismen biologischen al- tary forms of dialectic]. Paris: Gallimard.
terns [Molecular models of biological aging]. Biologie in unserer Plessner, H. (1965). Die Stufen des Organischen und der Mensch: Ein-
Zeit, 25, 336–344. leitung in die philosophische Anthropologie. Berlin, Germany: W.
Overton, W. F., & Reese, H. W. (1973). Models of development: de Gruyter.
Methodological implications. In J. R. Nesselroade & H. W. Reese Plomin, R. (1994). Genetics and experience: The interplay between
(Eds.), Life span developmental psychology: Methodological issues nature and nurture. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
(pp. 65–86). New York: Academic Press. Plomin, R., & Caspi, A. (1999). Behavioral genetics and personality.
Park, D. C., Polk, T. A., Park, R., Minear, M., Savage, A., & Smith, In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality the-
M. R. (2004). Aging reduces neural specialization in ventral vi- ory and research (Vol. 2, pp. 251–276). New York: Guilford Press.
sual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Loehlin, J. C. (1977 ). Genotype-
USA, 101, 13091–13095.
environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of
Parke, R. D., Ornstein, P. A., Rieser, J. J., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1991). human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 309–322.
Editors’ introduction to the APA Centennial Series. Developmen-
Plomin, R., & Thompson, L. (1988). Life span developmental behav-
tal Psychology, 28, 3–4.
ioral genetics. In P. B. Baltes, D. L. Featherman, & R. M. Lerner
Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 8, pp. 1–31).
in Piaget’s developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 32, 301–342. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pascual-Leone, J. (1983). Growing into human maturity: Toward a Porges, S. W., & Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A. (1997). The psychophysi-
metasubjective theory of adulthood stages. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. ology of temperament. In J. D. Noshpitz (Ed.), The handbook of
Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 5, child and adolescent psychiatry (pp. 250–268). New York: Wiley.
pp. 117–156). New York: Academic Press.
Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., Portales, A. L., & Suess, P. E.
Pasupathi, M., Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Seeds of (1994). Cardiac vagal tone: Stability and relation to difficulties in
wisdom: Adolescents’ knowledge and judgment about difficult infants and 3-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 27, 289–300.
life problems. Developmental Psychology, 37, 351–361.
Pressey, S. L., Janney, J. E., & Kuhlen, R. G. (1939). Life: A psycho-
Pedersen, N. L. (1991). Behavioral genetic concepts in longitudinal
logical survey. New York: Harper.
analyses. In D. Magnusson, L. R. Bergman, G. Rudinger, & B.
Törestad (Eds.), Problems and methods in longitudinal research: Prigogine, I., & Stengers, I. (1984). Order out of chaos. New
Stability and change (Vol. 5, pp. 236–249). Cambridge, England: York: Bantam.
Cambridge University Press. Pulkkinen, L., & Caspi, A. (Eds.). (2002). Paths to successful devel-
Pedersen, N. L., & Reynolds, C. A. (2002). Stability and change in opment: Personality in the life course. New York: Cambridge Uni-
adult personality: Genetic and environmental components. Euro- versity Press.
pean Journal of Personality, 16, 77–78. Quetelet, A. (1835). Sur l’homme et de développememt de ses facultés.
Perlmutter, M. (1988). Cognitive potential throughout life. In J. E. Paris: Bachelier.
Birren & V. L. Bengtson (Eds.), Emergent theories of aging Quetelet, A. (1842). A treatise on man and the development of his fac-
(pp. 247–268). New York: Springer. ulties. Edinburgh, Scotland: Chambers.
660 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive perfor- Riley, M. W., Johnson, M., & Foner, A. (Eds.). (1972). Aging and so-
mance: Integration of structural and functional findings. In ciety: A sociology of age stratification (Vol. 3). New York: Sage.
F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging Roberts, B., & Caspi, A. (2003). The cumulative continuity model of
and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–90). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. personality development: Striking a balance between continuity
Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrique, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, and change in personality traits across the life course. In U. M.
D., Williamson, A., et al. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Understanding human de-
healthy adults: General trends, individual differences, and modi- velopment: Dialogues with life span psychology (pp. 183–214).
fiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1676–1689. Boston: Kluwer Academic.
Reese, H. W. (1994). The data /theory dialectic: The nature of scien- Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consis-
tific progress. In S. H. Cohen & H. W. Reese (Eds.), Life span de- tency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantita-
velopmental psychology: Methodological contributions (pp. 1–27). tive review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126,
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 3–25.
Reese, H. W., & Lipsitt, L. P. (1970). Experimental child psychology. Ross, M. (1997). Validating memories. In N. L. Stein, P. A. Ornstein,
New York: Academic Press. B. Tversky, & C. Brainerd (Eds.), Memory for everyday and emo-
Reese, H. W., & Overton, W. F. (1970). Models of development and tional events (pp. 49–82). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
theories of development. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Ross, M., & Bühler, R. (2001). Identity through time: Constructing
Life span developmental psychology: Research and theory personal pasts and futures. In A. Tesser & N. Schwarz (Eds.),
(pp. 115–145). New York: Academic Press. Blackwell handbook in social psychology: Intrapersonal processes
Reinert, G. (1970). Comparative factor analytic studies of intelli- (pp. 518–544). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
gence throughout the life span. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Baltes Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Temperament and human development. In
(Eds.), Life span developmental psychology: Research and theory N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia
(pp. 476–484). New York: Academic Press. of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 15586–15591). New
Reinert, G. (1979). Prolegomena to a history of life span developmen- York: Elsevier.
tal psychology. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life span Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon
development and behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 205–254). New York: Aca- (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child
demic Press. psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development
Reinert, G., Baltes, P. B., & Schmidt, G. (1966). Kritik einer Kritik (5th ed., pp. 619–700). New York: Wiley.
der Differenzierungshypothese der Intelligenz. Zeitschrift für Ex- Rott, C., & Thomae, H. (1991). Coping in longitudinal perspective:
perimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 13, 602–610. Findings from the Bonn Longitudinal Study on Aging. Journal of
Resnick, L. B., Levine, J. M., & Teasley, S. D. (Eds.). (1991). Per- Cross-Cultural Gerontology, 6, 23–40.
spectives on socially shared cognition. Washington, DC: American Rowe, J. W. (1997). Editorial: The new gerontology. Science, 278, 367.
Psychological Association. Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1987). Human aging: Usual and success-
Reuchlin, M. (1978). Processus vicariants et différences individu- ful. Science, 237, 143–149.
elles [Vicariant processes and individual differences]. Journal de Rutter, M. (1987). Resilience in the face of adversity: Protective fac-
Psychologie, 2, 133–145. tors and resistance to psychiatric disorder. British Journal of Psy-
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2002). New visions of the aging mind and brain. chiatry, 147, 598–611.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 394–400. Rutter, M., & Rutter, M. (1993). Developing minds: Challenge and
Riediger, M., & Freund, A. M. (in press). Focusing and restricting: continuity across the life span. New York: Basic Books.
Two aspects of motivational selectivity in adulthood. Psychol- Rybash, J. M., Hoyer, W. J., & Roodin, P. A. (1986). Adult cognition
ogy and Aging. and aging: Developmental changes in processing, knowing, and
Riediger, M., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, M. M. (2005). Managing life thinking. New York: Pergamon Press.
through personal goals: Intergoal facilitation and intensity of goal Ryder, N. B. (1965). The cohort as a concept in the study of social
pursuit in younger and older adulthood. Journal of Gerontology: change. American Sociological Review, 30, 843–861.
Psychological Sciences, 60B, 84–91. Ryder, R. G. (1967). Birth to maturity revisited: A canonical reanaly-
Riediger, M., Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (in press). Selection, op- sis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 168–172.
timization, and compensation (SOC) as developmental mecha- Ryff, C. D. (1984). Personality development from the inside: The sub-
nisms of adaptive resource allocation: Review and preview. In J. jective experience of change in adulthood and aging. In P. B.
E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and behav-
aging (6th ed.). Amsterdam: Elsevier. ior (Vol. 6, pp. 249–279). New York: Academic Press.
Riegel, K. F. (1958). Ergebnisse und probleme der psychologischen Ryff, C. D. (1989a). Beyond Ponce de Leon and life satisfaction: New
alternsforschung [Conclusions and problems of psychological directions in quest of successful aging. International Journal of
aging research.]. Vita Humana, 1, 52–64 and 111–127. Behavioral Development, 12, 35–55.
Riegel, K. F. (1973). Developmental psychology and society: Some Ryff, C. D. (1989b). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on
historical and ethical considerations. In J. R. Nesselroade & the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality
H. W. Reese (Eds.), Life span developmental psychology: Method- and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081.
ological issues (pp. 1–23). New York: Academic Press. Ryff, C. D. (1991). Possible selves in adulthood and old age: A tale of
Riegel, K. F. (1976). The dialectics of human development. American shifting horizons. Psychology and Aging, 6, 286–295.
Psychologist, 31, 689–700. Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological
Riley, M. W. (1987). On the significance of age in sociology. Ameri- well-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
can Sociological Review, 52, 1–14. 69, 719–727.
References 661

Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical Schaie, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (1993). Age difference patterns of psy-
learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928. chometric intelligence in adulthood: Generalizability within and
Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of across ability domains. Psychology and Aging, 8, 44–55.
Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 345–371. Schaie, K. W., Willis, S. L., Hertzog, C., & Schulenberg, J. E. (1987).
Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Effects of cognitive training on primary mental ability structure.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychology and Aging, 2, 233–242.
Salthouse, T. A. (1995). Refining the concept of psychological com- Schaie, K. W., Willis, S. L., & Pennak, S. (2005). An historical
pensation. In R. A. Dixon & L. Bäckman (Eds.), Compensating framework for cohort differences in intelligence. Research in
for psychological deficits and declines (pp. 21–34). Mahwah, NJ: Human Development, 2(1&2), 43–67.
Erlbaum. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1987). Dispositional optimism and
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age physical well-being: The inf luence of generalized outcome ex-
differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–437. pectancies on health. Journal of Personality, 55, 169–210.
Salthouse, T. A. (2003). Interrelations of aging, knowledge, and cog- Schindler, I., & Staudinger, U. M. (2005a). Life span perspectives on
nitive performance. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lindenberger self and personality: The dynamics between the mechanics and
(Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues with life pragmatics. In W. Greve, K. Rothermund, & D. Wentura (Eds.),
span psychology (pp. 265–287). Boston: Kluwer Academic. The adaptive self: Personal continuity and intentional self-
Salthouse, T. A., Hambrick, D. Z., Lukas, K. E., & Dell, T. C. (1996). development (pp. 3–30). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.
Determinants of adult age differences on synthetic work perfor- Schindler, I., & Staudinger, U. M. (2005b). Obligatory and optional
mance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2, 305–329. personal life investments in old age. Manuscript submitted for
Salthouse, T. A., & Kail, R. (1983). Memory development throughout publication.
the life span: The role of processing rate. In P. B. Baltes (Ed.), Schmidt, L. R. (1971). Testing the limits im Leistungsverhalten:
Life span development and behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 89–116). New Möglichkeiten und Grenzen. In E. Duhm (Ed.), Praxis der klinis-
York: Academic Press. chen Psychologie (Vol. 2, pp. 9–29). Göttingen, Germany:
Salthouse, T. A., & Meinz, E. J. (1995). Aging, inhibition, working Hogrefe & Huber.
memory, and speed. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sci- Schneider, W., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Memory and knowledge
ences, 50B, 297–306. development. In D. Kuhn & Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child
psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language
Sanford, E. C. (1902). Mental growth and decay. American Journal of
(pp. 467–521). New York: Wiley.
Psychology, 13, 426–449.
Schneirla, T. C. (1957). The concept of development in comparative
Scarr, S. (1993a). Biological and cultural diversity: The legacy of psychology. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development
Darwin for development. Child Development, 64, 1333–1353. (pp. 78–108). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Scarr, S. (1993b). Genes, experience, and development. In D. Mag- Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S., & Oates, G. (1999). The continuing ef-
nusson & P. Casaer (Eds.), Longitudinal research on individual de- fects of substantively complex work on the intellectual function-
velopment: Present status and future perspectives (pp. 26–50). ing of older workers. Psychology and Aging, 14, 483–406.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (1996). A life span model of successful
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own envi- aging. American Psychologist, 51, 702–714.
ronments: A theory of genotype environment effects. Child Devel- Schwarz, N., & Bohner, G. (1996). Feelings and their motivational
opment, 54, 424–435. implications: Moods and the action sequence. In P. M. Gollwitzer
Schadewaldt, W. (1975). Nachwort zu Elektra [Epilogue to Electra]. & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action (pp. 119–145).
Stuttgart, German: Reclam. New York: Guilford Press.
Schaie, K. W. (1965). A general model for the study of developmental Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1999). Reports of subjective well-being:
problems. Psychological Bulletin, 64, 92–107. Judgmental processes and their methodological implications. In
Schaie, K. W. (1970). A reinterpretation of age-related changes in D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: The
cognitive structure and functioning. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. foundation of hedonic psychology (pp. 61–84). New York: Russell
Baltes (Eds.), Life span developmental psychology: Research and Sage Foundation.
theory (pp. 485–507). New York: Academic Press. Sears, P. S., & Barbee, A. H. (1977). Career and life satisfactions
Schaie, K. W. (1989). The hazards of cognitive aging. Gerontologist, among Terman’s gifted women. In J. Stanley, W. George, & C.
29, 484–493. Solano (Eds.), The gifted and the creative: A fifty-year perspective
(pp. 28–65). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Schaie, K. W. (1994). The course of adult intellectual development.
Settersten, R. A., Jr. (2005). Toward a stronger partnership between
American Psychologist, 49, 304–313.
life-course sociology and life span psychology. Research in
Schaie, K. W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood: The Seat- Human Development, 2(1).
tle Longitudinal Study. New York: Cambridge University Press. Sherry, D. F., & Schacter, D. L. (1987). The evolution of multiple
Schaie, K. W. (2005). Developmental inf luences on intelligence: The memory systems. Psychological Review, 94, 439–454.
Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: Oxford University Press. Shrager, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1996). Dynamic plasticity inf luences
Schaie, K. W., & Baltes, P. B. (1975). On sequential strategies in de- the emergence of function in a simple cortical array. Neural Net-
velopmental research: Description or explanation? Human Devel- works, 9, 111–129.
opment, 18, 384–390. Shweder, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures. Cambridge, MA:
Schaie, K. W., Maitland, S. B., Willis, S. L., & Intieri, R. C. (1998). Harvard University Press.
Longitudinal invariance of adult psychometric ability factor Siegler, R. S. (1989). Mechanisms of cognitive development. Annual
structures across 7 years. Psychology and Aging, 13, 8–20. Review of Psychology, 40, 353–379.
662 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic method: A di- Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). Trends and profiles of psychological
rect means for studying cognitive development. American Psy- functioning in very old age. In P. B. Baltes & K. U. Mayer (Eds.),
chologist, 46, 606–620. The Berlin Aging Study: Aging from 70 to 100 (pp. 197–226). New
Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1994). Constraints on learning in non- York: Cambridge University Press.
privileged domains. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 194–226. Smith, J., & Freund, A. M. (2002). The dynamics of possible
Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in chess. American Scien- selves in old age. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sci-
tist, 61, 394–403. ences, 57B, P492–P500.
Simonton, D. K. (1988). Age and outstanding achievement: What do Smith, J., Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). Occupational
we know after a century of research? Psychological Bulletin, settings facilitative of wisdom-related knowledge: The sample
104, 251–267. case of clinical psychologists. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Simonton, D. K. (1989). The swan-song phenomenon: Last-works ef- Psychology, 62, 989–1000.
fects for 172 classical composers. Psychology and Aging, 4, 42–47. Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a dynamic system.
Singer, J. L. (1984). The private personality. Personality and Social Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(8), 343–348.
Psychology Bulletin, 10, 7–30. Sorensen, A. B., Weinert, F. E., & Sherrod, L. (Eds.). (1986). Human
Singer, T., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Plasticity of development and the life course: Multidisciplinary perspectives.
memory for new learning in very old age: A story of major loss? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology and Aging, 18, 306–317. Sowarka, D., & Baltes, P. B. (1986). Intelligenzentwicklung. In
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & W. Sarges & R. Fricke (Eds.), Psychologie für die Erwachsenen-
Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but bildung-Weiterbildung (pp. 262–272). Göttingen, Germany:
not the sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162. Hogrefe & Huber.
Singer, T., Verhaeghen, P., Ghisletta, P., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, Spelke, E., Vishton, P., & von Hofsten, C. (1995). Object percep-
P. B. (2003). The fate of cognition in very old age: Six-year longi- tion, object-directed action, and physical knowledge in infancy.
tudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and Aging, In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences
18, 318–331. (pp. 165–179). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Singer, W. (1995). Development and plasticity of cortical processing Staudinger, U. M. (1996). Wisdom and the social-interactive founda-
architectures. Science, 270, 758–764. tion of the mind. In P. B. Baltes & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), Inter-
active minds: Life span perspectives on the social foundation of
Singer, W. (2003). The nature–nurture problem revisited. In U. M.
cognition (pp. 276–315). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Understanding human de-
velopment: Dialogues with life span psychology (pp. 437–447). Staudinger, U. M. (1999a). Older and wiser? Integrating results on
Boston: Kluwer Academic. the relationship between age and wisdom-related performance.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23(3),
Skinner, B. F. (1966). The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Sci-
641–664.
ence, 153, 1205–1213.
Staudinger, U. M. (1999b). Social cognition and a psychological ap-
Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of
proach to an art of life. In F. Blanchard-Fields & T. Hess (Eds.),
Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–570.
Social cognition, adult development and aging (pp. 343–375). New
Skinner, E. A., & Connell, J. P. (1986). Control understanding: York: Academic Press.
Suggestions form a developmental framework. In M. M. Baltes &
Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Viele Gründe sprechen dagegen und trotz-
P. B. Baltes (Eds.), The psychology of control and aging
dem fühlen viele Mnschen sich wohl: Das Paradox des subjektiven
(pp. 35–69). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Eohlbefindens [Many reasons speak against it, nevertheless many
Small, B. J., Hertzog, C., Hultsch, D. F., & Dixon, R. A. (2003). Sta- people are happy: The paradox of subjective well-being]. Psychol-
bility and change in adult personality over 6 years: Findings ogische Rundschau, 51, 185–197.
from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Journals of Gerontology,
Staudinger, U. M. (2001). Life ref lection: A social-cognitive analysis
58B, 166–176.
of life review. Review of General Psychology, 5, 148–160.
Smith, F., Fagan, J. F., & Ulvund, S. E. (2002). The relation of recog- Staudinger, U. M. (2005). Personality and aging. In M. Johnson, V. L.
nition memory in infancy and parental socioeconomic status to Bengtson, P. G. Coleman, & T. Kirkwood (Eds.), Cambridge
later intellectual performance. Intelligence, 30, 247–259. handbook of age and ageing. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Smith, J. (1999). Life planning: Anticipating future life goals and University Press.
managing personal development. In J. Brandtstädter & R. M. Staudinger, U. M. (in press). Personality and aging. In M. Johnson,
Lerner (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research V. L. Bengtson, P. G. Coleman, & T. Kirkwood (Eds.), Cambridge
through the life span (pp. 223–255). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. handbook of age and ageing. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Smith, J. (2003). The gain-loss dynamic in life span development: Im- University Press.
plications for change in self and personality during old and very Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1996). Interactive minds: A facil-
old age. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Under- itative setting for wisdom-related performance? Journal of Per-
standing human development: Dialogues with life span psychology sonality and Social Psychology, 71, 746–762.
(pp. 215-–241). Staudinger, U. M., Bluck, S., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2003). Looking back
Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1993). Differential psychological aging: and looking ahead: Adult age differences in consistency of di-
Profiles of the old and very old. Ageing and Society, 13, 551–587. achronous ratings of subjective well-being. Psychology and
Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). Profiles of psychological function- Aging, 18, 13–24.
ing in the old and oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 12, Staudinger, U. M., Dörner, J., & Mickler, C. (2005). Self-insight: Mea-
458–472. surement, validation and plasticity. Manuscript in preparation.
References 663

Staudinger, U. M., & Fleeson, W. (1996). Self and personality in old Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social
and very old age: A sample case of resilience? Development and psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bul-
Psychopathology, 8, 867–885. letin, 103, 193–210.
Staudinger, U. M., Freund, A. M., Linden, M., & Maas, I. (1999). Taylor, S. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1995). Effects of mindset on
Self, personality, and life management: Psychological resilience positive illusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
and vulnerability. In P. B. Baltes & K. U. Mayer (Eds.), The 69, 213–226.
Berlin Aging Study: Aging from 70 to 100 (pp. 302–328). New Taylor, S. E., Neter, E., & Wayment, H. A. (1995). Self-
York: Cambridge University Press. evaluation processes. Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
Staudinger, U. M., & Kunzmann, U. (2005). Positive adult personal- letin, 21(12), 1278–1287.
ity development: Adjustment and/or growth? European Psycholo- Tetens, J. N. (1777). Philosophische Versuche über die menschliche
gist, 10, 320–329. Natur und ihre Entwicklung. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmanns
Staudinger, U. M., & Lindenberger, U. (Eds.). (2003). Understanding Erben und Reich.
human development: Life span psychology in exchange with other Thaler, D. S. (2002). Design for an aging brain. Neurobiology of
disciplines. Boston: Kluwer Academic. Aging, 23, 13–15.
Staudinger, U. M., Lopez, D., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). The psychome- Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In W.
tric location of wisdom-related performance: Intelligence, per- Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
sonality, and more? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development
23, 1200–1214. (pp. 563–635). New York: Wiley.
Staudinger, U. M., Maciel, A. G., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Thomae, H. (1959). Entwicklungspsychologie [Developmental psy-
What predicts wisdom-related performance? A first look at per- chology]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.
sonality, intelligence, and facilitative experiential contexts. Euro-
pean Journal of Personality, 12, 1–17. Thomae, H. (Ed.). (1979). The concept of development and life span
developmental psychology. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.),
Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1993). Resilience Life span development and behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 282–312). New
and levels of reserve capacity in later adulthood: Perspectives from York: Academic Press.
life span theory. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 541–566.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cam-
Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resilience bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
and reserve capacity in later adulthood: Potentials and limits of
Tomarken, A. J., Davidson, R. J., Wheeler, R. E., & Kinney, L.
development across the life span. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen
(1992). Psychometric properties of resting anterior EEG asymme-
(Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, and
try: Temporal stability and internal consistency. Psychophysiol-
adaptation (pp. 801–847). New York: Wiley.
ogy, 29, 576–592.
Staudinger, U. M., & Pasupathi, M. (2000). Life span perspectives on
Trivers, R. L. (1985). Social evolution. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin /
self, personality, and social cognition. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A.
Cummings.
Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed.,
pp. 633–688). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D’Onofrio, B., & Gottes-
man, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of
Staudinger, U. M., & Schindler, I. (2005). Personal life investment: A
IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14(6), 623–628.
window on adaptation across the life span. Manuscript submitted
for publication. Vaidya, J. G., Gray, E. K., Haig, J., & Watson, D. (2002). On the tem-
poral stability of personality: Evidence for differential stability
Staudinger, U. M., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1992). Wisdom-related
and the role of life experience. Journal of Personality and Social
knowledge in a life review task: Age differences and the role of
Psychology, 83, 1469–1484.
professional specialization. Psychology and Aging, 7, 271–281.
Vaillant, G. E. (1990). Avoiding negative life outcomes: Evidence
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intel-
from a forty-five year study. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes
ligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
(Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). A balance theory of wisdom. Review of Gen- (pp. 332–355). New York: Cambridge University Press.
eral Psychology, 2, 347–365. Vaillant, G. E. (1993). The wisdom of the ego. Cambridge, MA: Har-
Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Words to the wise about wisdom? Human De- vard University Press.
velopment, 47, 286–289.
Valsiner, J., & Lawrence, J. A. (1997). Human development in culture
Sternberg, R. J., & Jordan, J. (Eds.). (2005). A handbook of wisdom: across the life span. In J. W. Berry, P. R. Dasen, & T. S.
Psychological perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press. Saraswathi (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2,
Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., Williams, W. M., & Horvath, J. A. pp. 69–106). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
(1995). Testing common sense. American Psychologist, 50, 912–927. Verhaeghen, P., Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L. (1992). Improving
Sterns, H. L., & Dorsett, J. G. (1994). Career development: A life memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: A
span issue. Experimental Aging Research, 20, 257–264. meta-analytic study. Psychology and Aging, 7, 242–251.
Strelau, J., & Angleitner, A. (Eds.). (1991). Explorations in tempera- Verhaeghen, P., & Salthouse, T. A. (1997). Meta-analyses of age-
ment: International perspectives on theory and measurement. New cognition relations in adulthood: Estimates of linear and non-
York: Plenum Press. linear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin,
Suls, J., & David, J. P. (1996). Coping and personality: Third time’s 122, 231–249.
the charm? Journal of Personality, 64, 993–1005. Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Suls, J., David, J. P., & Harvey, J. H. (1996). Personality and coping: Waddington, C. H. (1975). The evolution of an evolutionist. Edin-
Three generations of research. Journal of Personality, 64, 711–735. burgh, Scotland: Edinburgh University Press.
664 Life Span Theory in Developmental Psychology

Waterman, A. S., & Archer, S. L. (1990). A life span perspective on Wiese, B. S., & Schmitz, B. (2002). Studienbezogenes Handeln im
identity formation: Developments in form, function, and process. Kontext eines entwicklungspsychologischen Meta-Modells.
In P. B. Baltes, D. L. Featherman, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Life Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psy-
span development and behavior (Vol. 10, pp. 29–57). Hillsdale, chologie, 34, 80–94.
NJ: Erlbaum. Wilkening, F., & Anderson, N. H. (1990). Representation and diagno-
Weinert, F. E., & Perner, J. (1996). Cognitive development. In sis of knowledge structures in developmental psychology. In N. H.
D. Magnusson (Ed.), The life span development of individuals: Anderson (Ed.), Contributions to information integration theory
Behavioural, neurobiological and psychosocial perspectives (pp. 45–80). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(pp. 207–222). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Willis, S. L. (1990). Contributions of cognitive training research to
Weir, M. W. (1964). Developmental changes in problem-solving understanding late life potential. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Late-
strategies. Psychological Review, 71, 473–490. life potential (pp. 25–42). Washington, DC: Gerontological Soci-
Welford, A. T. (1984). Between bodily changes and performance: ety of America.
Some possible reasons for slowing with age. Experimental Aging Willis, S. L. (2001). Methodological issues in behavioral intervention
Research, 10, 73–88. research with the elderly. In J. E. Birren (Ed.), Handbook of the
Wellman, H. M. (2003). Enablement and constraint. In U. M. psychology of aging (5th ed., pp. 78–108). San Diego, CA: Aca-
Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Understanding human de- demic Press.
velopment: Dialogues with life span psychology (pp. 245–263). Willis, S. L., & Baltes, P. B. (1980). Intelligence in adulthood and
Boston: Kluwer Academic. aging: Contemporary issues. In L. W. Poon (Ed.), Aging in the
Werner, E. E. (1995). Resilience in development. Current Directions 1980’s: Psychological issues (pp. 260–272). Washington, DC:
in Psychological Science, 4, 81–85. American Psychological Association.
Werner, H. (1926). Einführung in die Entwicklungspsychologie. Willis, S. L., & Nesselroade, C. S. (1990). Long-term effects of
München, Germany: Barth. f luid ability training in old-old age. Developmental Psychology,
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. 26, 905–910.
New York: International Universities Press. Wood, J. V. (1996). What is social comparison and how should we
Whitbourne, S. K. (1987). Personality development in adulthood and study it? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 520–537.
old age: Relationships among identity style, health, and well- Woodruff, D. S., & Birren, J. E. (1972). Age changes and cohort
being. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 7, 189–216. differences in personality. Developmental Psychology, 6,
Whitbourne, S. K., & Waterman, A. S. (1979). Psychosocial develop- 252–259.
ment during the adult years: Age and cohort comparisons. Devel- Wortman, C. B., & Silver, R. C. (2001). The myths of coping with loss
opmental Psychology, 15, 373–378. revisited. In M. S. Stroebe & R. O. Hansson (Eds.), Handbook of be-
White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of com- reavement research: Consequences, coping, and care (pp. 405–429).
petence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
White, S. H. (1992). G. Stanley Hall: From philosophy to develop- Yates, E., & Benton, L. A. (1995). Biological senescence: Loss of
mental psychology. Developmental Psychology, 28, 25–34. integration and resilience. Canadian Journal on Aging, 14,
106–120.
Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002). Subjective career
success and emotional well-being: Longitudinal predictive power Zuckerman, M. (1995). Good and bad humors: Biochemical bases of
of selection, optimization and compensation. Journal of Voca- personality and its disorders. Psychological Science, 6, 325–332.
tional Behavior, 60, 321–335.
CHAPTER 12

The Life Course and Human Development


GLEN H. ELDER JR. and MICHAEL J. SHANAHAN

THE EMERGENCE OF LIFE-COURSE THEORY: A Linking Mechanisms 689


HISTORICAL ACCOUNT 668 Paradigmatic Principles of Life-Course Theory 691
Concepts of Human Development across the Life Span 670 Contributions of Life-Course Theory to the Study of
How Lives Are Socially Organized: Roles, Cycles, Human Development 700
and Age 672 INTEGRATING BIOLOGICAL MODELS WITH THE
Age and Life Course 673 LIFE COURSE: A PROMISING FRONTIER 701
BASIC CONCEPTS AND PERSPECTIVES OF THE Mechanisms of Gene-Environment Interaction 702
INDIVIDUAL LIFE COURSE 679 Gene-Environment Interactions in the Life Course 703
Social Pathways, Cumulative Processes, and the Future Directions for the Life Course and
Individual Life Course 680 Behavioral Genetics 705
Trajectories, Transitions, and Turning Points 684 CONCLUSIONS 706
Selection, Endogeniety, and Contextual Effects 687 REFERENCES 707
LIFE TRANSITIONS AND HISTORICAL CHANGE 689

The study of the life course and human development knowledge than ever before about behavioral adapta-
flourished during the closing decades of the twentieth tions in real-world settings. We are also increasingly
century, extending across substantive and theoretical aware of individuals as agents of their own lives. New
boundaries (Mortimer & Shanahan, 2003), and now ap- avenues of research have opened, and the future offers
pears in many subfields of the behavioral sciences. With exciting promise for understanding how dynamic views
this change has come an increasing appreciation for link- of context and the person—including biological dimen-
ages between changing contexts and human development. sions—interact to influence achievements, physical and
Context refers to the social embedding of individuals and psychological well-being, and social involvements.
often calls for the study of longitudinal, historical, and To grasp the magnitude of this change, consider stud-
spatial variations. Human development entails patterns of ies of person and society in the 1960s. In his widely read
growth and adaptation that extend from birth to death. The Sociological Imagination, C. Wright Mills (1959)
Conceptual breakthroughs associated with the life- encouraged “ the study of biography, of history, and of
course framework, coupled with the dramatic growth of the problems of their intersection within social struc-
longitudinal studies, have generated more research and ture” (p. 149). Mills started with the individual and
asked what features of society produce such a person.
He argued that the seemingly “personal problems” of
We thank Ross Parke, Avshalom Caspi, and Richard Lerner,
one’s biography are better understood as repercussions
who provided thoughtful reviews to the first edition of this
chapter (Elder, 1998a). The senior author is indebted to the
of broad social tensions. He had few empirical examples,
Spencer Foundation for a Senior Scholar Fellowship. The however, and was not concerned with dynamic views of
staff of the Carolina Population Center provided valuable as- person and context. Rather, he focused on types of soci-
sistance in preparing the first and second editions of the ety and adult behavioral patterns, with little recognition
chapter. We are particularly indebted to Lilly Shanahan for of social change, development and aging, or even human
her thorough review of the second edition. diversity. Indeed, longitudinal studies of human lives

665
666 The Life Course and Human Development

were an uncommon subject of study, particularly in their roles, health, and personality, with a distinctive empha-
social and historical contexts. The concept of the life sis on life patterns across the middle years (Eichorn,
course had not yet appeared in the scholarly literature Clausen, Haan, Honzik, & Mussen, 1981). Both histori-
and was not addressed in the seminars of leading gradu- cal cohort comparisons and intergenerational connec-
ate programs. tions were part of this project.
The unfolding story of life-course theory up to the At Stanford University, a research team headed by
present owes much to a set of pathbreaking studies that Robert Sears actively followed members of the Lewis
were launched more than 60 years ago at the Institute of Terman sample of talented children into their later
Child Welfare (now Human Development) at the Univer- years. This was the oldest, active longitudinal study at
sity of California in Berkeley: the Oakland Growth the time, with birth years extending from 1903 to the
Study ( birth years 1920 to 1921) and the Berkeley 1920s. By the 1990s, the project had assembled 13
Growth and Guidance Studies ( birth years 1928 to waves of data spanning 70 years (Holahan & Sears,
1929). When these studies began, no one could have 1995), and research had begun to reveal the historical
imagined what they eventually would mean for the field imprint of the times on the study members’ lives (Elder,
of human development. The investigators did not envi- Pavalko, & Hastings, 1991), from the 1920s to the post
sion research that would extend into the adult years of World War II years and into later adulthood (Crosnoe &
study members, let alone into the later years of old age. Elder, 2004; Shanahan & Elder, 2002).
There were many reasons for this limited perspective. This extension of the early child samples to the adult
Except for support from the Laura Spelman Rockefeller years provided initial momentum to the scientific study
Foundation, substantial funds for longitudinal studies of adult development and sharpened awareness of the
were virtually nonexistent. In addition, the idea of adult need for a different research paradigm that would give
development had not yet captured the attention of behav- attention to human development beyond childhood and
ioral scientists. A mature field of adult development and to contexts beyond the family. What social routes to
aging was still decades away from becoming a reality. adulthood promoted behavioral continuity or change
Nonetheless, these considerations did not restrict the from the early years of life? Which ones enabled prob-
studies from continuing into the adult years and middle lem children to turn their lives around and become ef-
age. The Institute of Human Development contacted the fective adults? Child-based models of development had
members of the Oakland Growth Study for interviews little to offer because they did not address development
in the early and late 1950s, and another follow-up, and aging in the adult life course and were not con-
scheduled in 1972 to 1973, joined the lives of all study cerned with changing social contexts. For the most part,
members, some parents, and offspring, in an intergener- studies of continuity and change from childhood to the
ational framework. adult years were limited to evidence of correlational
By the 1970s, Jack Block (1971), with the assistance patterns between measures at time 1 and time 2 (Jones,
of Norma Haan, had completed a pioneering longitudi- Bayley, Macfarlane, & Honzik, 1971). The intervening
nal study focused on continuity and change in personal- years and their mechanisms remained a “ black box.”
ity from early adolescence to the middle years in the Little, if anything, could be learned about linking events
lives of the Oakland and Berkeley Study members. Also and processes from such analysis.
during the 1970s, George Vaillant (1977) followed a Kagan and Moss (1962), for example, studied the Fels
panel of Harvard men (students recruited between 1939 children from “ birth to maturity” by using correlation
and 1942) into the middle years of adulthood, assessing coefficients to depict behavioral stability across the
mechanisms of defense and coping. Another study at years, but this approach ignored the diverse paths that
the Institute of Human Development (Elder, 1974/1999) youth take into adult life. By age 23, some of the respon-
placed the lives of the Oakland Growth Study members dents followed a path to college, full-time employment,
in the Great Depression and traced the influence and marriage, and others entered military service or
of hardship on family life, careers, and health up to mixed employment and education. The timing of such
midlife. To cap off this active decade, investigators at transitions was important in determining their meaning
the Berkeley Institute completed a multifaceted study and implications. For example, adolescent marriage and
that revealed patterns of continuity and change in social parenting are coupled with more social and economic
The Life Course and Human Development 667

constraints than the same transitions occurring accord- and communities. In the classic Middletown studies
ing to a normative timetable, but late family formation (Lynd & Lynd, 1929, 1937), findings on families during
maximizes economic advantages and minimizes the dis- the 1920s seemed to have little relevance to family life in
ruptive effect of young children. However, these consid- the Great Depression. Life-course theory emerged in the
erations of context and timing—so richly descriptive of 1960s in response to these issues and to the challenge of
lives—were of little interest. In large part, this inatten- an aging population and the rapid growth of longitudinal
tion reflected the view that continuity of behaviors and studies. In the terminology of this chapter, the life course
psychological dispositions required little explanation refers most broadly to a theoretical orientation (or para-
aside from the label “stability.” digm) that encourages the study of changing lives in
Empirical studies of children into their adult and changing contexts. To use Robert Merton’s (1968) dis-
midlife years revealed major limitations to conventional tinction, a theoretical orientation establishes a common
knowledge of human development, which, in turn, posed field of inquiry by defining a framework that guides re-
major challenges for the future study of behavior: search in terms of problem identification and formula-
tion, variable selection and rationales, and strategies of
• To replace child-based, growth-oriented (“ontoge-
research design and analysis.
netic”) accounts of development with models that
Based in large measure on sociocultural theories of
apply to development and aging over the life course
age and social relations (Elder, 1975; Neugarten, 1968;
• To think about how human lives are organized and evolve
Ryder, 1965), the life course as a concept refers to a se-
over time, exhibiting patterns of constancy and change
quence of socially defined, age-graded events and roles
• To relate lives to an ever-changing society, with em- that defines, in large measure, the contours of biography.
phasis on the developmental effects of social change A sociocultural perspective gives emphasis to the social
and transitions meanings of age. Birth, puberty, and death are biological
As a whole, these challenges revealed a view of facts, but their meanings in the life course are social
human development that was advocated by proponents of facts or constructions. Age distinctions are expressed in
a contextualized psychology (e.g., Cairns & Cairns, expectations about the timing and order of a transition,
Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume) and many whether early, on time, or late. The life course can be
decades earlier by the Chicago school of sociology (Ab- historically linked to specific transitions and to the
bott, 1997), especially by William I. Thomas. With the meanings of cohort status (Riley, Johnson, & Foner,
close of the nineteenth century and through the first 1972). Birth year locates people in specific birth co-
decades of the twentieth century (a time of massive horts and thus according to particular social changes.
changes in U.S. society), Thomas made a persuasive The first author encountered such ideas about age and
case for studying change as “experiments of nature” in life course in the 1960s, just after arriving at the Insti-
the lives of immigrants and children. Inspired by tute of Human Development (UC Berkeley, 1962) to
Thomas and Znaniecki’s The Polish Peasant in Europe work with sociologist John Clausen on the Oakland
and America (1918–1920), researchers began to use Growth Study. The dramatic changes of families and in-
life-record data to investigate the impact of social dividual lives across the 1930s focused his energies on
change. Before most of the innovative longitudinal stud- the patterning of lives and links to a changing socioeco-
ies had been launched, Thomas urged, in the mid 1920s, nomic system. Codes that captured trajectories were
that priority be given to the “longitudinal approach to needed for people’s lives instead of the conventional
life history” (Volkart, 1951, p. 593). He claimed that codes for status at one point in time such as family so-
studies should investigate “many types of individuals cioeconomic status (SES) and personal achievement or
with regard to their experiences and various past peri- failure. The link between age and time provided an im-
ods of life in different situations” and follow “groups of portant step in this direction such as age-patterned
individuals in the future, getting a continuous record of events. This perspective on life changes suggested a way
experiences as they occur.” of thinking about the social construction of individual
Social transformations of the twentieth century lives, along with ideas from the life-history tradition of
raised many questions about historical variations in fam- the early Chicago School of Sociology. Thus, Children of
ily life and contexts beyond the family, including schools the Great Depression (Elder, 1974/1999) represents the
668 The Life Course and Human Development

published version of this initial effort to fashion a life- behavioral and social sciences, identifying important
course framework. problems and defining approaches from sociology
Since its inception, life-course studies have ex- (Elder, 1974/1999, 1975, 1985; Riley et al., 1972), de-
panded their purview beyond historical variations to mography (Ryder, 1965), history (Hareven, 1978, 1982;
include dynamic patterns of context in cohorts. These Modell, 1989), anthropology (Kertzer & Keith, 1984),
studies reveal remarkable diversity in cohorts with re- and both ecological (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and life-
spect to poverty experiences and economic fortunes, span developmental psychology (Baltes & Baltes, 1990).
residential mobility and the composition of neighbor- Major examples include:
hoods, family structure and household membership,
and experiences shaped by diverse stressors, employ- • Recognition of a life-course perspective on human de-
ment patterns, and immigration (Shanahan, Sulloway, velopment, extending from birth to maturity, and the
& Hofer, 2000). Each life is marked by social change in rapid growth of longitudinal studies that link child-
these respects, and the life-course framework has hood and the adaptations of later life (Phelps, Fursten-
proved useful in studying how these dynamisms shape berg, & Colby, 2002; Young, Savola, & Phelps, 1991).
lives and how the social aggregate of individual life • Life-history calendars for the collection of retrospec-
patterns affect social institutions. tive accounts of life events (Caspi et al., 1996; Freed-
We begin this chapter by viewing the emergence of man, Thornton, Camburn, Alwin, & Young-DeMarco,
life-course thinking as a response to some of the chal- 1988).
lenges we have noted, particularly those that stem from • New appreciation for the necessity of longitudinal,
following children into middle and old age. Life-span contextually rich data (Hofer & Sliwinski, 2002; Lit-
ideas in developmental psychology, life-cycle theory of tle, Bovaird, & Marquis, in press); appropriate statis-
role sequences, and concepts of the age-graded life tical techniques; and structural and dynamic, person-
course were prominent in this conceptual enterprise. By and variable-centered approaches (e.g., Bergman,
the end of the 1970s, a new synthesis, relating theory on Magnusson, & El-Khouri, 2003; Collins & Sayer,
relationships and age, achieved a theoretical orientation. 2001; Little, Schnabel, & Baumert, 2000).
The basic concepts and distinctions of life-course • Cross-disciplinary models of collaboration (Elder,
theory are surveyed in the next section, with emphasis Modell, & Parke, 1993), particularly with psychology
on the individual life course, its institutionalized path- and history, but now extending to exciting develop-
ways, developmental trajectories, and transitions. In ad- ments in subfields devoted to the study of physical and
dition, paradigmatic themes of life-course theory are emotional well-being (Halfon & Hochstein, 2002;
identified and illustrated by research projects. The Hertzman & Power, 2003; Kuh, Ben-Shlomo, Lynch,
themes include human agency and choice making in the Hallqvist, & Power, 2003). This chapter draws liber-
construction of lives, the timing of lives, linked or inter- ally from these sources in exploring the relevance of
dependent lives, and human lives in historical time and contemporary progress for studies of child, adoles-
place. This account explores distinctive contributions of cent, and adult development.
life-course theory to studies of children, adolescents, • A growing awareness that, beyond history and the
and young adults. The concluding section features newly differing experiences of cohorts, social change may
emerging developments in life-course study, with an em- refer to dynamic contextual patterns experienced
phasis on biological perspectives, and a promise of a within cohorts through diverse life histories (Shana-
theoretical framework that combines biology, social and han et al., 2000). Further, many contextual features
cultural influences, and psychology. are correlated, and their synergistic interactions are
critical to understanding time and place.

THE EMERGENCE OF LIFE-COURSE As is seen, these developments have relevance for the
THEORY: A HISTORICAL ACCOUNT study of the individual life course, its relation to chang-
ing social and historical conditions, and its implications
Over the past 30 years, advances in life-course theory for developmental processes. In this respect, life-course
and research have come from many quarters across the theory has much in common with interactionist think-
The Emergence of Life-Course Theory: A Historical Account 669

ing—which emphasizes interactions between person and agency of the aging organism (see also Schaie, 1965,
and context (see Magnusson & Stattin, Chapter 8, this with emphasis on the later years of aging), the multidi-
Handbook, this volume)—but it also attends to the orga- rectionality of life-span development, and the lifelong
nization and reorganization of social structures and interaction of person and social context. The concept of
pathways through life. As might be expected, life-course developmental task, perhaps first delineated by Robert
theory shares many objectives and concepts with the Havighurst (1949), also represents a way of viewing de-
ecology of human development (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; velopment across life stages. The concept alerts the ana-
Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Chapter 14, this Handbook, lyst to the possibility that specific experiences may be
this volume), including a multilevel concept of the envi- highly salient at different points in life, although empir-
ronment, from micro to macro. Life-course models also ical evidence for distinct psychosocial stages is not com-
share the ambition of life-span developmental psychol- pelling. The perceived or defined life course can change
ogy in rethinking the nature of human development and with aging through successive life reviews in which the
aging (Baltes, 1994; Baltes, Lindenberg, & Staudinger, past is assessed in light of the present. Staudinger (1989)
Chapter 11, this Handbook, this volume), but with par- has focused on “life review” as a way of studying intel-
ticular attention to links between changing contexts and lectual development across the life span.
lifelong development. The second strand (social relations) includes the
The principal traditions that led to life-course theory early work of W. I. Thomas on life histories (Thomas &
are illustrated in Figure 12.1: life-span concepts of de- Znaniecki, 1918–1920), G. H. Mead on socialization
velopment, social relations, and age and temporality. and the self (1934), Everett Hughes on work and the self
The first strand ( life-span concepts of human devel- (1971), Kurt Lewin on power dependence relations
opment) includes references to the pioneering work of (1948), and L. S. Vygotsky on language, the self, and so-
Erik Erikson’s (1950, 1963) psychosocial stages of de- cial relationships (1978; see also Clausen, 1968; Parke,
velopment and Paul Baltes’s (1997) process of selective Ornstein, Rieser, & Zahn-Waxler, 1994). The develop-
optimization with compensation. In addition, Richard ment of social role and self theories belongs in this tra-
Lerner (1982, 1991) has stressed the relative plasticity dition, and features the writings of sociologist Robert

Life-Span Concepts of Development Life Cycle and the Generations Age and Temporality

Psychosocial stage, adult stages Life cycle of social roles, Anthropology of age, age-grades,
of development generational succession expectations, concepts of age
status identity, proscriptive and
Multi-directionality of development Social roles, status, role-playing prescriptive age norms

Cumulative advantage, disadvantage Role transitions and sequences History of childhood and the family

Selective optimization with compensation Socialization as role/social learning Cohorts - Birth cohorts and
social change, structural lag
Life review, autobiographical memory Intergenerational relations, exchange
Age and life-course variations
Person-Context Interaction Social networks, capital
Transitions and trajectories

Life Course Theory


1960s to present

Figure 12.1 The emergence of life-course theory (1960s to present): research traditions and their concepts.
670 The Life Course and Human Development

Merton on role sets and reference groups (1968), Morris in which psychosocial transitions were affixed to age as
Rosenberg (1979) on self-esteem, and Urie Bronfen- if immutable to institutional change, such as the mid-
brenner on socialization (1970), to name a few. The life transition between ages 40 and 45. For Erikson,
field of intergenerational relations has expanded from Levinson, and other ontogenetic theorists, the starting
two to three and even four generations, with important point was a sequence of stages through which all per-
contributions to an understanding of three generations sons must pass.
from Reuben Hill (1970), Vern Bengtson and Laufer This perspective views the social context as a “scene
(1974), James Jackson (2000) and his three-generation or setting” through which the person—loaded with his
study of African Americans, and Ross Parke (Parke & or her “natural predispositions”—must pass. By con-
Ladd, 1992). trast, the life-course paradigm views the interplay of so-
A number of topics illustrate distinctive contribu- cial context and the organism as the formative process,
tions to a third strand (the study of age and temporality making people who they are. Individuals do not “de-
in lives). These include the early contributions to age- velop according to their natures” but, rather, they are
grading by social and cultural anthropologists and the continually produced, sustained, and changed by their
pioneering analysis of birth cohorts and generational social context (see Gottlieb, Wahlsten, & Lickliter,
units by Karl Mannheim in the 1920s (Elder, 1975). Chapter 5, this Handbook, this volume). Indeed, Mitter-
Also covered is Bernice Neugarten’s work on the social auer (1993) observes that, in many times and places, his-
psychology of age and the innovative sociological con- torians find little evidence of Erikson’s stages.
tributions of Matilda Riley and her collaborators. De- Proponents of life-span developmental psychology (a
mographers Uhlenberg (1974) and Hogan (1981) have field of inquiry first identified by name in 1969) ad-
produced creative analyses of cohort life patterns and dressed the challenges of such a circumscribed view by
age-grading. In social history, Hareven (1978, 1982) seeking a concept of development and aging across the
and Modell (1989), among others, have made imagina- life span that emphasizes cultural influences and
tive contributions to a historical understanding of the learned experiences or skills in patterns of aging. In
life course. The study of age expectations and time- theory, historical and cultural variations emerged as
tables in adolescent development is nicely illustrated particularly influential sources of adult adaptations
by Silbereisen’s programmatic study of youth affected and development. As Baltes (1979) observed, “It ap-
by German reunification (e.g., Silbereisen & Schmitt- pears that restricting developmental events to those
Rodermund, 1995; Silbereisen & Wiesner, 2000, 2002). which have the features of a biological growth concept
We turn now to a survey of each strand of influence of development is more of a hindrance than a help”
on life-course theory. (p. 265).
Paul Baltes (1993, 1994) has been a major figure in
Concepts of Human Development across the the conceptual articulation of life-span development
Life Span since the 1960s. More than most proponents of this per-
spective, he has interacted with life-course ideas and
A number of efforts in the psychological sciences were distinctions over the decades (see Baltes et al., Chapter
made during the post-World War II years to link child 11, this Handbook, this volume). The following proposi-
and adult developmental trajectories to social structure tions on life-span development are not new in them-
and changes in society, though typically from the per- selves but they add up to a distinctive perspective:
spective of a maturing or aging organism. Research
questions did not ask about the implications of environ- • Life-span development results from lifelong adaptive
mental change for the developing individual. Erik Erik- processes in which some are cumulative and continu-
son’s (1950, 1963) theory of psychosocial stages was ous, and others are discontinuous and innovative,
formulated with an eye to cultural variations, but he showing little connection to prior events or processes.
also largely saw the social system and culture from the • Ontogenetic development is local, specific, and time-
vantage point of the developing organism. Likewise, bound, so it is never fully adaptive. There is no pure
Daniel Levinson’s (1978) The Seasons of a Man’s Life advance or loss in development.
outlined a theory of life structure that ignored varia- • Age-graded influences are most important in the de-
tions in social structure and culture over historical time pendency years, childhood/adolescence and old age,
The Emergence of Life-Course Theory: A Historical Account 671

but history-graded and nonnormative influences are some attention to the role of social, cultural, and histor-
most consequential across the early and middle years ical forces in developmental processes.
of adulthood. However, concepts of life-span development gener-
• Changes occur in relation to positive and negative ally fail to apprehend social structure as a constitutive
events, gains, and losses, with the likelihood of ex- force in development. The problem stems from the life-
pected losses increasing. Biological resources decline span framework’s conceptualization of context, which
over the life span, but cultural resources may in- refers to age-graded, history-graded, or nonnormative
crease such as the cultivation of wisdom. influences: Age-graded influences shape individual de-
• Life-span development entails selection, optimization, velopment in largely normative ways for all persons,
and compensation. These mechanisms seek to maxi- history-graded influences shape development in differ-
mize gains and minimize losses or declines. Selective ent ways for different cohorts, and nonnormative influ-
optimization with compensation represents a “life- ences reflect idiosyncrasies (e.g., losing a leg in an
span model of psychological management that de- accident). Such a view has been unduly restrictive in
scribes how individuals can deal with the dual faced two senses. First, within-cohort variability largely re-
nature of human aging and the ubiquitous, age- flects nonnormative influences, which are not easily
related shift toward a less positive balance of gains subject to scientific study (Dannefer, 1984). As a result,
and losses” (Baltes, 1993, p. 590). the social basis for within-cohort differences becomes a
residual category. Second, as Mayer (2003) has noted,
The way these mechanisms or strategies work in life-span psychology views historical and nonnormative
later life is illustrated by an interview with the concert influences as idiographic (i.e., unique, nonrepeating),
pianist Arthur Rubenstein. When asked how he re- leaving only age-graded influences, which are thought to
mained a successful pianist in his late years, Ruben- be largely based in biology and age norms.
stein referred to three strategies: “(1) he performed Because the larger social forces that lead to age
fewer pieces, (2) he now practiced each more fre- norms are of little interest, within-cohort regularities
quently, and (3) he introduced more ritardandos in his in behavior are explained solely by personal attributes
playing before fast segments, so that the playing ( biology and internalized norms). In the final analysis,
sounded faster than it was” (Baltes, 1993, p. 590). The the study of contextual influences in cohorts is ham-
strategy of selection is illustrated by Rubenstein’s con- pered because it produces largely invariant patterns
centration on fewer pieces, the more frequent practice through such age-graded influences, or it cannot be
illustrates the use of optimization, and the increasing studied because of its seemingly random nature. De-
reliance on contrast in speed exemplifies a strategy of spite these conceptual difficulties, life-span studies
compensation. are beginning to investigate links between broader so-
This psychological model of successful aging has rel- cial contexts and individual functioning (e.g., Heck-
evance for successful development at all ages including hausen, 1999).
childhood and adolescence. Adaptations in adolescence The important issue to recognize is that there is not
can be viewed through the guidelines of selective opti- one optimum point of entry for studying human develop-
mization in which gains are maximized and risks, ment across the life span (see also Shanahan & Porfeli,
losses, or deprivations are minimized. Youth select ac- 2002). Indeed, the multilevel nature of human develop-
tivities in which they are competent (i.e., athletics, aca- ment invites different points of entry (each with specific
demics, or street life) and optimize benefits through an research questions) ranging from cultures and social
investment of resources, time, energy, and relationships. institutions to the human organism. Entry points fre-
Marsiske, Lang, Baltes, and Baltes (1995, pp. 35–36) quently link or cross adjacent levels in the developmen-
rightly claim that selective optimization with compen- tal process. Studies commonly employ different entry
sation is best understood as a metamodel for life-span points in the same research, although framed by a cen-
development because it applies broadly to aspects of the tral question. Thus, a project motivated by the impact
“developmental person-context matrix.” Life-span de- of rural change on children’s social and emotional de-
velopmentalists, such as Baltes, Schaie, and others, have velopment should be framed by an initial focus on some
undoubtedly enriched our thinking about development aspect of this social process such as the degree of eco-
and aging across the life course, and they have given nomic hardship and displacement. Inquiry would explore
672 The Life Course and Human Development

the process by which this change makes a difference in the cycle and minimizes age similarities across adjacent
children’s developmental experience. Parts of this study generations. In a rapidly changing world, parents,
might also investigate the determinants of specific emo- grandparents, and children share less culture and his-
tional or social outcomes and relevant protective re- torical experience.
sources in the family, a point of entry that centers on the The life cycle concept incorporates both socializa-
developmental status of the child. Still other entry tion and social control processes. The predominant roles
points might begin with the interchange of parents and of a life stage lock people into a set of normative expec-
child or with sibling relationships. Each point could be- tations and informal sanctions that provide direction
come a framing statement for an independent study. and discipline. Commitments to a line of action arise
over time through obligations to significant others
(Becker, 1961, 1964). Stable role relationships ensure a
How Lives Are Socially Organized: Roles,
measure of personal stability, just as entry into such re-
Cycles, and Age
lationships can stabilize a person’s life and minimize
The second column of Figure 12.1 refers to how an indi- involvement in unconventional and dangerous activities.
vidual’s life pattern is structured by multiple role se- Sampson and Laub (1993) observed, in their sample of
quences and their transitions. These transitions into and men from a low-income urban sample, that adult bonds
out of social roles across the life span entail both social to conventional figures and lines of activity defined a
and personal changes in status and identity (Glaser & route of escape from delinquency for a substantial num-
Strauss, 1971). In their field studies, anthropologists ber of men with a childhood history of delinquency and
have referred to a patterned role sequence from birth economic disadvantage.
to death as a “life cycle” (Kertzer & Keith, 1984). During the familistic post-World War II years, the
Changes in major roles, such as from youth to marriage life cycle became well known as the family cycle through
and parenthood, generally represent changes in a social the writings of Paul Glick and Reuben Hill; a set of or-
stage across the life cycle. dered stages of parenthood defined primarily by varia-
In concept, the life cycle views life organization tions in family composition and size (Elder, 1978).
through social relationships, particularly kin relation- Major transition points included courtship, engagement,
ships, and generational succession. A dominant concept marriage, birth of the first and last child, the children’s
of the life span from the early 1900s up to the 1960s, transitions in school, departure of the eldest and
life cycle generally referred to a sequence of social roles youngest child from the home, and marital dissolution
among individuals and families. A more precise social through the death of one spouse. Family life in this era
meaning of life cycle is a sequence of stages in parent- provided a better fit to this sequence of roles than it does
ing, from the birth of children through their departure today. Marriage and parenting have been uncoupled to a
from the home to their own children. The role sequence considerable extent (Bumpass & Lu, 2000). Children are
refers to a reproductive process that always applies to increasingly born prior to marriage or outside of mar-
human populations. In a life cycle of generational suc- riage altogether. In the United States, the prevalence of
cession, newborns are socialized to maturity, give birth divorce has led to multiple families in a person’s life
to the next generation, grow old, and die. The “cycle” is and to the likelihood that most children will experience
repeated from one generation to the next in a human a single parent household before they enter adulthood.
population (O’Rand & Krecker, 1990). The life-cycle concept and its family-cycle version
Life cycles as reproductive cycles vary greatly in the usefully knit together the full array of life stages and
pace of their revolutions. Early childbearing, shortly generations. They also provide insight into processes of
after menarche, accelerates the cycle and shortens the socialization and social control over the life span that
distance between the generations. When the eldest link the developing person and his or her career. And
daughter has a child before the age of 13, her mother yet, the life cycle’s focus on reproduction and parenting
may become a grandmother before the age of 30 and a has limited value as a way of viewing the lives and de-
great-grandmother before the age of 50. A sequence of velopmental trajectories of children and adults because
early childbearing across the generations weakens the it does not apply to never married, nonparent, or di-
generational and age basis for family authority and vorced persons, all of whom have become increasingly
social control. By contrast, late childbearing slows common (e.g., Fussell, 2002). The focus on a single ca-
The Emergence of Life-Course Theory: A Historical Account 673

reer also ignores the realities of multiple careers, and identity in terms of the new role and its predecessor, but
each person generally occupies multiple roles at the Ebaugh makes no explicit reference to timing across the
same time (whether spouse and parent or spouse and life span or to historical time. Judging from the analysis,
employee), but these concurrent roles are not part of one might conclude that it is not consequential whether
the life cycle’s scope. Consequently, the life or family an illness and death involves a child or a grandparent,
cycle did not orient research to the management or coor- whether family separation occurs in one’s 20s or 50s, or
dination of multiple roles such as marriage and work. whether a lay-off occurs at the beginning or the end of
By the end of the 1960s, a prime era for life-cycle re- one’s productive work life. On the contrary, the evi-
search, a survey by Young and Willmott (1973) con- dence suggests that timing matters because social
cluded that studies of work and family had proceeded timetables, age norms, and age-graded sanctions influ-
along separate paths with no significant effort to exam- ence individuals.
ine their interdependencies. This contrasts rather strik- In summary, role sequences and identity change, so-
ingly today with the flourishing study (with an emphasis cialization and social control, the life-cycle perspec-
on interlocking trajectories) of work and family relations tive, and generations are conceptual elements of a
(Blair-Loy, 2003; Crouter, Maguire, Helms-Erikson, & relationship view of life patterns and organization that
McHale, 1999; Drobnic, Blossfeld, & Rohwer, 1999; dates back to the nineteenth century. One of the earli-
Moen, 2003; van der Lippe & van Dijk, 2002). est proponents of this view, sociologist W. I. Thomas,
In addition, the life cycle is insensitive to temporal used life-record data to study the emigration of Polish
location and matters of timing. The concept depicts a peasants to European and U.S. cities around the turn of
sequence of social roles and transitions. Social roles are the century (Thomas & Znaniecki, 1918–1920). In this
ordered but are not temporally located in a person’s life. pioneering work, described as “ the greatest single
In the case of the family cycle, for example, each stage study done thus far by an American sociologist ” (Nis-
of parenting could be arrayed in a sequence but would bet, 1969, p. 316), the lives of immigrants embodied
not be bound by age or the temporal markers that come the discontinuities of the age; they were socialized for
with a perspective on the age-graded life course. A life- a world that had become only a memory. The societies
cycle model of a person’s life might locate marriage be- they left and entered—the Old World and the New—
fore the first birth, but it would not indicate whether the presented contrasting lines of genesis or primary se-
marriage occurred at 20 or 40 years. Sequence models quences of social roles for individual adaptation and
thus provide only part of the story on life context. development. Matters of social and historical time are
The kinship term of generation is part of a life-cycle clearly relevant to this project, and yet, Thomas and
perspective and shares its blindness to temporality. Znaniecki were largely insensitive to them.
Members of an ancestral generation do not occupy a For many years, the relationship and life-cycle per-
common historical location relative to events and long- spective offered a valuable way of thinking about the so-
range trends. A parent generation, for example, may cial patterning and interdependence of lives, although
have birth years that span 30 years, a period that could limited in a number of respects. During the 1960s, this
include eras of economic depression, global war, and approach began to converge with new understandings of
peace in the twentieth century. The greater the time age to form life-course models that combined the
span, the more diverse the historical experience of the virtues of both theoretical traditions: linked lives across
generation. With these points in mind, it is apparent that the life span and generations, and temporality through
generational role or position cannot offer a precise way an age-graded sequence of events and social roles, em-
of connecting people’s lives to the changes in society. bedded in a changing world. In addition, these models
Indeed, intergenerational studies are generally distin- were informed by life-span concepts of human develop-
guished by their insensitivity to historical time or loca- ment that underscored the agency of individuals in the
tion; generations are frequently studied in the timeless social construction of their lives and life courses.
realm of the abstract.
Temporal limitations of this kind are generally char-
Age and Life Course
acteristic of models based on role theory. Ebaugh’s
study of role exits (1988) makes this limitation very The importance of the 1960s in linking these theoretical
clear. The concept of “role exit ” involves a change of traditions had much to do with the appearance of new
674 The Life Course and Human Development

thinking about age, including an appreciation for its di- During the late 1950s and early 1960s, Bernice Neu-
verse meanings and consequences (see third column of garten directed a research program that featured a con-
Figure 12.1). These new thoughts include an emphasis on cept of normative timetables and individual deviations
subjective experiences with the age structures of society from such expectations (Neugarten & Datan, 1973). The
and the individual’s own construction of a life course, as timetable of the life course refers to social age, as de-
expressed in the pioneering work of Bernice Neugarten fined by people’s expectations regarding events. In the-
(Neugarten, 1996; Neugarten & Datan, 1973). In the Uni- ory, age expectations specify appropriate times for
versity of Chicago’s Committee on Human Development, major transitions, and violations of these expectations
post-World War II studies, such as the Kansas City proj- may lead to punitive responses from others. There is an
ect, were more successful than other efforts at the time in appropriate time for entering school, leaving home, get-
linking human development through the adult years to the ting married, having children, and retiring from the
social structures in which people lived. For example, labor force. Neugarten, Moore, and Lowe (1965) ob-
Neugarten and Peterson (1957) observed a relationship served a high degree of consensus on age norms across
between age-linked concepts of self and life stage by so- some 15 age-related characteristics in samples of middle-
cioeconomic position. Working-class people were older class adults. The data reveal general agreement among
when they entered the self-defined middle years of life, men and women on the appropriate age for a woman to
compared to upper-status people. Neugarten contributed marry and support the hypothesis that informal sanc-
to this early work by connecting socioeconomic careers tions are associated with relatively early and late mar-
to adult psychology, role transitions, and generations. We riage. Moreover, the women were aware if they were on
return to these important contributions in the context of time, late, or early with respect to marriage and other
age-based perspectives on the life course. Through the major role transitions.
innovative work of Norman Ryder (1965) and Matilda Although subsequent studies have extended this line
Riley (Riley et al., 1972), a more developed articulation of research (Settersten & Mayer, 1997), relatively little
of the relation between historical time and lives was pro- is known about age expectations and their boundaries
posed, as expressed through membership in age cohorts and related sanctions; thus, doubts have been raised
and successive age strata. For the first time, this work about age norms (Marini, 1984). These topics deserve
joined two relatively independent lines of research on age far more attention than they have received to date.
(Elder, 1975): (1) sociocultural and (2) cohort-historical. Some notions about the proper phasing of the life
course take the form of cognitive descriptions or pre-
Sociocultural Patterns in Human Experience dictions rather than normative accounts, whether pre-
The relevance of age for a sociocultural understanding scriptive or proscriptive. However, the process by
of life organization has evolved over many decades of which these descriptions or age expectations are con-
ethnographic study by anthropologists, as in research on structed, transmitted, and learned remains largely un-
age-grading and age-set societies (Kertzer & Keith, explored territory.
1984). This work has generally focused on age struc- For many decades, age-grades or categories were in-
tures in culture; however, the new inquiry explored indi- ferred as possessing common significance without evi-
vidual experiences of age and age-grading, giving fresh dence of their meaning to the individuals involved. At
insights to the social and psychological variability of what point do young children take the perspective of a
people’s lives. student? When do young adults begin to take an adult
Contrary to a structural view of age patterns in cul- standpoint and view themselves accordingly? Is the
tures (Eisenstadt, 1956; Kertzer & Keith, 1984), studies main transition point for an adult perspective marriage,
began to show that people of the same age do not march the birth of a child, or stable employment? Such ques-
in concert across major events of the life course; rather, tions were of interest in Neugarten’s research program,
they vary in the pace and sequencing of their transitions and she broke new ground in testing the proposition that
and they do so in ways that have real consequences for life stage is partially a function of one’s socioeconomic
family pressures, child socialization, and personal well- status and career.
being. This variation also appears in accounts of differ- In the mid-1950s, Neugarten found that men in the
ential aging among people who follow different social lower-economic strata were likely to perceive a more
trajectories. rapid passage through the major age divisions of life
The Emergence of Life-Course Theory: A Historical Account 675

than did middle-class men: Maturity, middle age, and Adjacent birth cohorts are most sharply differenti-
old age come earlier at a lower-economic strata, owing ated during rapid change, and represent a vehicle of
perhaps to class-linked occupational demands and social change when cohort differences arise. As succes-
stresses (Neugarten & Peterson, 1957). The man who sive cohorts encounter the same historical event, they
relies on mental skills in a sedentary occupation fore- do so at different stages in their life course. This means
sees a relatively long period of productivity, while the that adjacent cohorts bring different life experiences to
man who works with his hands expects a relatively the change. Consequently, the impact of the event is
short span of productive activity, followed by retire- contingent on the life stage of the cohort at the point of
ment. This research is one of the earliest contributions change. Ryder (1965) stressed this “life-stage princi-
to what is now called a “constructionist perspective on ple” in his account of cohort differences in the life
the life course” with its emphasis on human agency and course. As each cohort encounters a historical event,
choice making. whether depression or prosperity, it “is distinctively
Age distinctions order social roles (e.g., the sociocul- marked by the career stage it occupies” (p. 846). Exam-
tural perspective of the Neugarten studies), but they also ples include the differential age of military entry
order people through age or birth cohorts. Cohorts have among U.S. veterans who served in World War II. The
long been common to demographic research on marriage, age range spanned 20 years: Some recruits had just left
fertility, and divorce. However, these cohort studies high school while others were in their mid-30s with
were not carried out with an interest in the life course. families and careers.
From this vantage point, historical influence in life
Age Cohorts in Lives experience can take different forms in cohort studies.
Important theoretical work on the link between life One form is expressed as a cohort effect when social
patterns and changes in society began to appear in the change differentiates the life patterns of successive co-
1960s, featuring Ryder’s influential essay on “ The Co- horts such as the older and younger “children of the
hort as a Concept in the Study of Social Change.” The Great Depression” who were born in the 1920s. Consis-
most comprehensive pioneering work was authored by tent with the life-stage principle, younger children, and
Matilda Riley and her colleagues in Aging and Society especially the boys, were most adversely influenced by
(Riley et al., 1972). Riley and her colleagues viewed the economic stresses of the economic collapse (Elder,
age as a basis of stratification in historical experience 1974/1999). Cohort differences were also expressed in
and in role sequences across the life course. “Whereas the prevalence of a behavior or practice such as life-
socioeconomic strata are ranked to form a social hier- course reorganization or the proportion exposed to
archy, age strata are typically ordered by time ( hence, trauma in World War II.
are more akin to geological strata)” (p. 23). Birth year A cohort effect may also be expressed in a changing
indicates historical time, and chronological age ac- social mechanism, as expressed in the transition to
quires the meanings of social timing and life stage. parenthood across four birth cohorts of White women
Birth cohorts provide a link between historical change in upstate New York, United States (Forest, Moen, &
and the life course. Dempster-McClain, 1995): 1907 to 1918, 1919 to
Birth year or date of entry into a system (such as 1923, 1924 to 1928, and 1929 to 1933. The first cohort
school graduation or marriage) locates the individual came of age during the Great Depression, the second
according to historical time and related social changes: moved into adulthood during World War II, the third
With age peers in the cohort, this person is exposed to a made the transition in the early post-World War II
particular segment of historical experience as he or she years, and the fourth became parents during the 1950s.
moves across the sequence of age-graded roles. To grasp The study found employment before marriage to be a
the meaning and implications of birth year and cohort primary source of childbearing delay for women in the
membership, the analyst specifies the distinctive histor- first two cohorts. However, in the younger cohorts,
ical events and processes at the time, as well as charac- educational advancement played a significant role in
teristics of the cohort such as its size and composition. the delay of the first birth, far more than premarital
These characteristics are themselves a consequence of employment. Advanced education was becoming
historical changes in birth and death rates, immigration, increasingly important in the lives of women, including
and migration. women’s return to school following marriage and
676 The Life Course and Human Development

the birth of children, especially in the third and fourth strategy has more to offer in developmental and life-
cohorts. course implications because research is directed to the
In addition to cohort effects, history takes the form explication of a specific change process such as family
of a period effect when the influence of a historical adaptations to migration.
change is relatively uniform across successive birth co- Cohort membership has specific implications for
horts. Rodgers and Thornton (1985) conclude that lives when a particular cohort size is paired with avail-
“most of the changes in marriage rates observed during able economic opportunities. Richard Easterlin (1980)
this century are the consequences of period character- has pursued this issue in his account of postwar change
istics” (p. 21) rather than of differences between co- in the work lives of men. His point of departure was the
horts. They draw the same conclusion about rates of link between the supply of younger men and their rela-
marital dissolution and instability: “ the big picture is tive economic position, and between changing cohort
one of overwhelming historical effects that influenced size relative to options and life chances. Other things
all subgroups of the population substantially and sur- being equal, the greater the relative supply of young
prisingly equally” (p. 29). On divorce, they refer espe- male workers, the weaker their relative economic sta-
cially to the rising level up to the 1930s, the decline in tus and gains. Before 1960, the relatively small birth
the Great Depression era, a rapid recovery to the ex- cohorts of younger men experienced a wide range of
traordinary peak of divorce in the mid-1940s, and to advancement opportunities, and their relative eco-
the upward trend during the 1960s and 1970s. The nomic position (compared with older men) increased
precise factors in these period variations remain to be significantly. After 1960, the “ baby boom” cohorts
determined. began entering the young adult category, producing a
Efforts to disentangle these effects (cohorts and pe- labor surplus and restricting economic progress.
riod) and those associated with maturation or aging The behavior of these birth cohorts was symptomatic
have not advanced knowledge on historical change in of the “relative economic squeeze” they encountered.
lives. Cohort studies seldom address questions that The economic position of young men has deteriorated
specify a type of social change or the process that makes relative to that of older men, family formation has been
a difference. Life-span studies, for example, may assign delayed by increasing numbers of young adults, and the
environmental change to an error term or view cohorts employment rate of young women has increased more
as a test of the generational boundaries of behavioral rapidly than that of older women. Among young adult
outcomes, as in cohort sequential designs (Baltes, cohorts during this period, an upward trend in the di-
Cornelius, & Nesselroade, 1979). However, even when vorce, suicide, and crime rates is observed, as well as a
history is substantively important for understanding de- leveling off in the college enrollment rate, which has
velopmental change, this influence is likely to be opera- climbed steadily since the 1940s. Research inspired by
tionalized as a period or cohort effect that provides no Easterlin has led to mixed results, however, suggesting
clue as to the precise nature of the process. that the model may be over-simplified (Pampel & Pe-
Another approach to historical change—the compari- ters, 1995).
son of cohort subgroups—rests on the empirically docu- In part, this simplification reflects a problem shared
mented assumption that members of each birth cohort by cohort studies as a whole: when theory and research
are exposed differentially to trends and events. Not all focus on the cohort level, the linking mechanisms be-
children who lived through the Great Depression were tween lives and changing times are difficult to pin down.
exposed to severe hardships, and not all veterans of Cohorts can be merely “ black boxes” with no informa-
World War II were exposed to heavy combat. In the tion on causal dynamics and linkages. Behavioral differ-
Great Depression, families did not uniformly experience ences between cohorts also do not readily yield an
the economic decline, and family hardship did not affect understanding of the social or historical factors that ac-
all subgroups of children in the same way (Elder, count for them. Speculation frequently takes the place of
1974/1999). Though variations by subgroups in specific disciplined explication. The problem with cohort studies
birth cohorts represent a significant advance over cohort has much to do with exposure of people in a birth cohort
comparisons, even more valuable is a focus on exposure to varied environmental changes. Thus, some grade
to a particular contextual or historical change. This school children are exposed to the economic stress of a
The Emergence of Life-Course Theory: A Historical Account 677

plant closing, and other children are insulated from such reentry into the labor force, and the timing of material
stresses. In response to this social heterogeneity, more acquisitions. Consistent with a life-course model, he
studies are investigating specific types of differential noted that in periods of rapid change, “each generational
social change in birth cohorts. cohort encounters at marriage a unique set of historical
Before surveying one of these studies conducted by constraints and incentives which influence the timing of
Reuben Hill, we sum up the temporality of age by iden- its crucial life decisions, making for marked genera-
tifying meanings that have special relevance to human tional dissimilarities in life cycle career ” (1970, p. 322).
development: the ages of life or lifetime, the diverse The middle generation in Hill’s study followed this pat-
meanings of social time, and historical time. Life time is tern of cohort differentiation.
indexed by chronological age and refers to the stage or In one sense, the generational dimension of life-cycle
position in the developmental-aging process. From a de- analysis has helped to contextualize the individual life
velopmental standpoint, age alerts the investigator to course by emphasizing the social dynamic of “linked
subgroups that are differentially vulnerable to particu- lives”—parents and children, husband and wife, grand-
lar types of social change. The lifetime meaning of age parents and grandchildren, siblings and friends. Parents
requires specification of the variables it represents. So- and grandparents are not merely present at a stage in
cial time, such as the age patterned sequence of events, life, but rather appear as lifelong associates. From this
includes “ family time” across stages of parenting and vantage point, the life-cycle model made adult develop-
the generations. A normative concept of family time in- ment especially relevant to an understanding of child de-
dicates an appropriate time for leaving home, for mar- velopment, an important insight that has not been fully
riage, and for bearing children. Last, historical time realized in studies of children. Personal or social change
refers to a person’s location in history; membership in a in parents has developmental consequences for children;
birth cohort indicates this location. conversely, change in the behavior of children can alter
the behavior and psychology of parents (Crouter &
Life Cycle and Life Course Booth, 2003). In concept, children become active agents
In any period of theoretical transition, both old and new of their own life course.
models guide research. We see this mix of old and new This life-cycle contribution to the life-course proj-
in the 1960s. Hill’s (1970) three-generation study is a ect appears in a longitudinal study of Californians
case in point. The grandparent generation married be- from the Oakland Growth sample that were born in the
fore 1915 and the parent generation between the 1920s early 1920s, passed through adolescence in the de-
and mid-1930s. Hill made significant contributions to pressed 1930s, and were subject to the labor needs of
the life-cycle model in essays on the family cycle and World War II (Elder, 1974/1999). The central question
development, and he launched this multigenerational concerned the effects of the Great Depression on the
study to pursue issues of intergenerational continuity lives and development of the Oakland children. An in-
and change. However, the dramatic social changes at the tergenerational framework seemed entirely appropriate
time underscored the historical imprecision of genera- for addressing this question, with an emphasis on the
tional membership by placing the older and younger process by which economic hardship made a difference
members of each generation in different historical in the lives of children by changing family processes
epochs. Couples from the parent generation who mar- and socialization.
ried in the 1920s had significantly more children than But the dramatic changes of life experience from the
those who married in the depressed 1930s. The two 1920s into the late 1930s raised questions that could
groups were found to be sufficiently different in life not be addressed by the perspective. The effect of
course to constitute samples of different populations. change depended on many things including their expo-
The heterogeneity of generations on historical expe- sure to the event, their age or developmental stage, and
rience has led some analysts to identify cohorts in each the age of their parents. Fast-changing economic and
one, and Hill used this modification in his own work. He family circumstances called for relating them to the
investigated strategies of family management, which in- ages of parents and children. These observations made
cluded the timing of marriage and parenthood, the spac- the distinctions of birth cohort and life stage especially
ing of children, the husband’s and wife’s entry and relevant:
678 The Life Course and Human Development

At the time of maximum hardship in the early 30s, the Oak- As noted in this account, Children of the Great De-
land children were well beyond the dependency state of early pression (Elder, 1974/1999) began with concepts of the
childhood, with its consequences for intellectual and emo- life cycle and relationship tradition, such as role se-
tional development, and they reached the age of majority quences and generation, but soon turned to the analytic
after opportunities had improved through nationwide mobi-
meanings of age for linking family and individual expe-
lization for war. Persons born 10 years before the Oakland
rience to historical change (especially birth cohort and
children would have entered the labor force during the worst
life stage), and for identifying trajectories across the life
phase of the economic collapse, while the welfare of persons
in the 1929 cohort would have been entirely dependent on course, using a concept of age-graded events and social
conditions in their families. (Elder, 1974/1999, p. 16) roles. Both theoretical strands provide essential features
of life-course theory on matters of time, context, and
Family adaptations to economic hardship became a process. The life course is age-graded through institu-
set of linkages between the economic collapse of the tions and social structures and embedded in relation-
1930s and the developmental experience of children. In ships that constrain and support behavior. In addition,
place of static concepts of family life, the study turned people are located in historical settings through birth
to notions of the family economy and its multiple actors cohorts and they are linked across the generations by
as a way of thinking about the economic crisis and its kinship and friendship. The 25th anniversary edition of
implications for children. Through linked relationships Children of the Great Depression (Elder, 1974/1999) in-
and actors, changing economic roles and status shaped cludes another chapter, which compares the Oakland co-
the experience of children. Thus, children who acquired hort in life patterns to that of a younger birth cohort, the
paid jobs in the community became more socially inde- Berkeley Guidance study members, born in 1928 to
pendent than other youth. 1929. The Berkeley males were more adversely affected
An appraisal of growing up in the Great Depression re- by Depression hard times than all other gender/cohort
quired knowledge of life paths to adulthood, such as edu- subgroups in the comparison.
cation, marriage, work-life advancement, and military Contemporary theory on the life course and its social
service. Some youth escaped hardship through early work dimensions thus differs from perspectives of an earlier
and military service, others through higher education and era by joining the life cycle processes of social relation-
marriage. However, some outcomes have more to do with ships with the temporality and contextual aspects of
their timing than with mere occurrence: Marriage is an age. For examples of this shift, we need only compare
example. Hardship favored early marriage by diminishing Thomas and Znaniecki’s The Polish Peasant in Europe
the chances of higher education and by making home life and America (1918–1920) with its analysis of genera-
unappealing. Likewise, developmental theory suggested tions and lineages in a relatively timeless, abstract realm,
that the early work experience of adolescents would ac- to the birth cohort, age-graded life course, and intergen-
celerate their thinking about work and the timing of their erational themes of Family Time and Industrial Time
entry into adult work roles. (Hareven, 1982)—a study of successive worker cohorts
These and other conceptual issues made theoretical and their families in a large textile mill with declining
distinctions concerning the age-graded life course espe- economic prospects during the 1920s and 1930s. Though
cially useful to the study. Consider the sequence of explicitly historical, The Polish Peasant does not locate
events that link early adolescent work experience and the immigrants according to birth year and historical set-
adult work. Family hardship increased the involvement of ting, nor does it describe their life stage at the time of
boys in gainful employment and, through this experience, their emigration. Hareven’s study provides these mark-
advanced their social independence and sensitivity to ers and uses them to assess the implications of industrial
matters of vocation. This sensitivity took the form of an change for worker families (parents and children) in the
early vocational focus and work commitment that led to textile city of Manchester, New Hampshire.
work lives, which effectively countered any educational Through the integration of social relationship con-
handicap of family hardship, even among the sons of cepts and age-based distinctions, along with life-span
working class parents. In the end, family income losses concepts of the person and human organism (see Figure
did not adversely affect the occupational standing of 12.1), the life course became a vital, expanding field of
the Oakland men or the status that the Oakland women inquiry in the 1970s and 1980s. Both the individual life
achieved through marriage. course and a person’s developmental trajectory are inter-
Basic Concepts and Perspectives of the Individual Life Course 679

connected with the lives and development of others. Life- We turn now to some basic concepts and perspectives
course theory thus took issue with life-span studies that that center on the individual life course and develop-
viewed human development as an unfolding process, mental processes.
which was not coactive with social and cultural processes
in historical time. However, it is responsive to Lerner’s BASIC CONCEPTS AND PERSPECTIVES OF
(1991, p. 27) call for more attention to contextual vari- THE INDIVIDUAL LIFE COURSE
ability, and continues to be an emerging perspective on
developmental science (Cairns, Elder, & Costello, 1996; The individual life course and its relation to develop-
see also Ford & Lerner, 1992; Thelen & Smith, Chapter mental trajectories represent a common meeting ground
6, this Handbook, this volume) that extends across system for life-course theory and developmental science, with
levels and disciplines. its “perspective on individual functioning that empha-
The contextual perspective of the life-course frame- sizes the dynamic interplay among processes that oper-
work has much in common with Urie Bronfenbrenner’s ate across time frames, levels of analysis, and contexts”
ecology of human development, now called bio-ecological (Cairns et al., 1996). Building on advances since the
theory (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994), but it differs in 1960s, life-course theory has uniquely forged a concep-
emphasis on the temporal dimension of historical, fam- tual bridge between developmental processes, the life
ily, and life contexts. Bronfenbrenner’s Ecology of Human course, and ongoing changes in society based on the
Development (1979) proposed a multilevel view of the premise that age places people in the social structure
sociocultural environment, from macro to micro, but it and in particular birth cohorts.
did not include a temporal perspective on individual de- To understand this conceptual bridge, we turn to ele-
velopment across changing environments. In life-course mentary concepts. First, we begin with multiple levels
studies, this perspective includes age-graded social tra- of the life course, ranging from institutionalized path-
jectories or pathways as well as historical contexts. ways to cumulative patterns of context that shape the
Some years later, after making a case for the person- individual life course. Second, other important temporal
process-context model, Bronfenbrenner (1989, p. 201) concepts, such as trajectory, transition, and turning
noted a major lacuna in his work that also applied to point, are taken up with particular emphasis on the
Lewin’s original thinking—the dimension of time. To properties of social transitions. Third, we focus on link-
correct this limitation, he proposed the general concept ing mechanisms that have proven highly useful in the
of “chronosystem,” with its three interacting compo- study of contextual influences. Beginning with studies
nents over time: (1) the developing person, (2) the chang- of children who were born before the Great Depression,
ing environment, and (3) their proximal processes. research has revealed a set of mechanisms that link con-
Although this concept has not been widely adopted, text and the individual life course. These mechanisms
the ecological perspective itself has generated many help to flesh out the conceptual meaning of more gen-
contextual studies of child development (Moen, Elder, & eral principles of the life course.
Lüscher, 1995; see also Bronfenbrenner & Morris, The central role of agency in the shaping of lives was
Chapter 14, this Handbook, this volume). evident in these early studies, and subsequent research
Human development in life-course theory represents a has highlighted its connections to social context. While
process of organism-environment transactions over time the concept of agency encompasses many phenomena—
in which the organism plays an active role in shaping particularly when viewed through psychological con-
its own development. The developing person is viewed as structs such as motivations, values and aspirations, and
a dynamic whole, not as separate strands, facets, or do- personality—its full significance in life-course theory
mains such as emotion, cognition, and motivation. The is found in its dynamic interplay with social location
course of development is embedded in a dynamic system (Hitlin & Elder, in press). Finally, we consider issues
of social interchanges and interdependencies across and surrounding selection, which refers to the factors that
within levels. As noted by Bronfenbrenner (1996), this lead people to contexts and experiences (Caspi, 2004).
dynamic in life-course theory is illustrated well by the in- Every empirical study of lives enters an ongoing system
terlocking lives and developmental trajectories of family of exchanges between person and context. Selection and
members who are influenced differentially by their related concepts acknowledge this complexity and
changing world. encourage heightened sensitivity to the challenges that
680 The Life Course and Human Development

surround the identification of contextual influences. of the individual actor, some decision pressures and con-
Paradigmatic themes of life-course theory draw on straints are linked to federal regulation, some to the so-
these elementary concepts in highlighting distinctive cial regulations of an employer, and some to state and
conceptual orientations such as the relation between community legislation.
changing times and lives. Mayer (1986) had the nation-state in mind when he
identified important societal mechanisms, “ which im-
pose order and constraints on lives” (pp. 166–167).
Social Pathways, Cumulative Processes, and the
These include the cumulative effects of delayed transi-
Individual Life Course
tions, institutional careers, the historical circum-
Social pathways and cumulative patterns represent dy- stances associated with particular cohorts, and state
namic views of context. Pathways typically refer to se- intervention. Growth of the state in social regulation
quences of social positions in and between organizations counters the potentially fragmenting effects of social
and institutions. Institutionalized pathways generally differentiation. At the individual level, the state “legal-
have specified time boundaries, what Merton (1982, izes, defines and standardizes most points of entry and
1984) has called “socially expected durations.” The legis- exit: into and out of employment, into and out of marital
lated ages at maturity for voting and marriage can be status, into and out of sickness and disability, into and
viewed as marking off an accepted duration of depen- out of education. In doing so the state turns these tran-
dency. Social pathways are generally age-graded and thus sitions into strongly demarcated public events and acts
identify relatively early, on-time, and late transitions. as gatekeeper and sorter ” (p. 167). These are what
Children who are held back in school become aware of Buchmann (1989, p. 28) properly calls events in “ the
their lagging status on the educational ladder (Alexander, public life course.”
Entwisle, & Dauber, 1994), and company managers talk Multilevel accounts of the life course are well illus-
about the relation between age and grade in prospects for trated with cross-national studies of the transition to
promotion to senior rank (Sofer, 1970, p. 239). adulthood (Settersten, Furstenberg, & Rumbaut, 2005),
In addition to their age-graded nature, pathways particularly in relation to the social pathways from sec-
structure the direction that people’s lives can take. ondary school to work (Kerckhoff, 2003; Marshall,
Pallas (2003, p. 168–169) observes that pathways have Heinz, Krueger, & Verma, 2001). In Great Britain, sec-
distinct features that govern how strongly people’s tra- ondary school-leavers can follow a path to work that
jectories and behaviors are shaped including, for exam- consists of technical training programs or schools that
ple, the number of options a pathway leaves open in the provide credentials for a particular craft. With the free-
future, the extent of mobility that is likely to be experi- dom to make a wide range of choices, students also miss
enced, stigma and extrinsic rewards, and the importance opportunities and desirable job placements. Far more
of personal choice. Some pathways provide future op- structure is provided working-class German youth in a
portunities and chances for upward mobility based on secondary-level system that in theory joins industrial
personal motivation, while others effectively block training and education in an apprenticeship system. In
promising avenues irrespective of one’s efforts. principle, placement in a skilled craft is assured for
Pathways are also multilevel phenomena reflecting youth who complete their apprenticeships. In Japan, oc-
arrangements in place at levels of culture, the nation- cupational recruitment typically occurs in schools from
state, social institutions and organizations, and locale. the secondary-level to higher education, and the hiring
To varying degrees, people work out their life course in firm provides specific job training, not the schools or
established or institutionalized pathways. At the macro craft institutes. American adolescents encounter the
end of this multilevel system, governments generally es- least amount of articulation between schooling and
tablish pathways (Leisering, 2003). At micro levels, in- workplace. Vocational training in secondary schools is
stitutional sectors (economy, education, etc.) or local not closely linked to specific industries, their recruit-
communities (school systems, labor markets, and neigh- ment, and skill needs. In many less-developed countries,
borhoods) guide the pathways. Each system level, from youth are forced to leave school early to support their
macro to micro, socially regulates, in part, the decision families; in turn, their lowered educational attainment
and action processes of the life course, producing areas results in low wages, which forces their children to leave
of coordination or discord and contradiction (e.g., mar- school early as well (Shanahan, Mortimer, & Krueger,
riage, divorce, and adoption laws). At the primary level 2002). This intergenerational cycle of disadvantage il-
Basic Concepts and Perspectives of the Individual Life Course 681

lustrates how pathways from school to work can repro- first grade attributes—including temperamental factors,
duce across the generations. grades, and standardized test scores—predict educa-
In societies, role sequences become established or in- tional attainments at age 22 as well as similar factors
stitutionalized in the culture with the passage of time. measured at age 16 (Entwisle, Alexander, & Olson,
With respect to work, for example, Spilerman (1977) 2005). Interestingly, parental influence, assessed in
has used the term career lines to refer to pathways de- high school, greatly underestimates the effects that par-
fined by the differentiated and aggregated work trajec- ents have on their children’s attainments because much
tories or histories of individuals. In his view, career of it has already been converted into school outcomes.
lines are “shaped by the nature of industry structures Kerckhoff ’s (1993) study of connections between
(e.g., occupational distribution, mode of recruiting into school and work in Great Britain also shows patterns of
upper status slots such as promotion from below versus divergence. Regarding achievements, he nevertheless
hiring from outside the firm) and by the institutional de- finds continuity in students’ place in the school system
mography of the labor market ” (p. 552). In an expanding over time. Thus, early placement in an elite primary
market, these career lines extend across company and school is associated with the “ high road” to the univer-
industry boundaries. Career lines vary in their receptiv- sity. A low-status placement is frequently associated
ity to different times of entry: The trades frequently re- with the opposite path for students. At each stage of
quire early entry through a training program in contrast schooling, differences are magnified, with the transition
to the less age-graded nature of public school teaching from junior to secondary school producing the greatest
and service occupations. The selection and timing of ca- deflection. In young adulthood, at age 23, one’s occupa-
reer entry are major determinants of subsequent earn- tional prestige reflects a set of cumulative structural in-
ings and work trajectories. fluences that originated early in the life course.
Prior to entry into work, however, young people en- By considering pathways, we understand more com-
counter educational pathways. Studies of the educa- pletely the choices and actions that shape individual life
tional system in the United States reveal that these courses and their developmental implications. Accord-
pathways begin very early in life and that their effects ing to this perspective, the individual life course is de-
cumulate to produce marked differences among stu- termined over time by the general and specific dictates
dents and workers. Thus, drawing on data from the of social pathways. For the very young child, these path-
Beginning School Study in Baltimore, Entwisle, Alexan- ways often begin, in large measure, with the residential
der, and Olson (2003) have documented educational and socioeconomic histories of parents. Small differ-
pathways that begin to take form in the first grade. In a ences cumulate over time and, by young adulthood, re-
school where 88% of the students were on subsidy, sult in significant differences in achievements and
every first grade student received a failing mark in read- prospects. As with educational pathways, research is be-
ing in the first quarter. In low-SES schools more gener- ginning to show how career lines form at a young age
ally, the average first grade reading score was 1.64 and lead to diverging patterns. For example, sociologists
( below a C), in contrast to students in high-SES schools, have typically viewed the first job for pay after school
who averaged 2.15 (above a C). They also report that, completion as the onset of one’s work career, but studies
even controlling for family background and standard- are now identifying how earlier work experiences, start-
ized test scores, Black children received lower first ing at least in high school, bear on future achievements
grade reading and math scores, and these racial differ- and attainments (Mortimer, 2003).
ences were subsequently magnified. Ideally, studies of the developmental consequences of
While students of all races and socioeconomic groups life-course change take into account the potential con-
benefited from schooling to the same degree, low-SES straints and options associated with particular path-
students’ reading ability decreased during the summer ways. However, the realities of research are expressed in
vacation, while high-SES students’ reading improved. a disciplinary division of labor. Sociologists (Mayer,
Given initial differences in reading and math ability and 2004) and historians make use of a multilevel view of
these invidious summer trends, Entwisle et al. (2003) the life course in their historical and comparative stud-
concluded that “ the long-term persistence of early rank- ies. Both attend to issues of contextual variation. As his-
ings means that inequities visible in the first grade torian E. P. Thompson once said, “ the discipline of
translate into deficits all along the line” (p. 239). In- history is above all a discipline of context ” (Goldthorpe,
deed, recent studies drawing on this sample show that 1991, p. 212). Developmentalists in psychology may
682 The Life Course and Human Development

center on the impact of life-course change (Noack, durations. To adequately understand the covariations
Hofer, & Youniss, 1995) or simply ignore it altogether, between diverse spells and development, we need to
usually the latter. At least up to the 1990s, the typical know more about duration in interpersonal and develop-
longitudinal study of social development among chil- mental processes.
dren, as published in Child Development, measured the A particularly telling example of the complexity of
socioeconomic environment only at the beginning of the durations and their potential meaning is found in Mor-
research (Elder & Pellerin, 1995). Articles still recom- timer’s (2003) St. Paul longitudinal study of adolescent
mend the use of atemporal indicators that provide a employment. With monthly educational and employment
snapshot of families and children at a point in time, de- data, Mortimer and her colleagues developed a typology
spite growing evidence that the dynamic features of of work patterns through high school based on duration
families often are of consequence for children’s well- (whether the student worked more than 18 months
being and achievements (Shanahan et al., 2000). through the 48 months of high school) and intensity
Social pathways encompass cumulative processes, (during periods of employment, whether the student
which refer to long-term patterns of experiences that worked, on average, more than 20 hours per week). Mor-
sustain behavioral continuity or lead to change. Some timer, Staff, and Oesterle (2003) showed that ninth
cumulative processes reflect social experiences of long graders with higher educational promise—as indicated
duration. The concept of duration refers to the span of by grades and aspirations—opted for less intensive
time between changes in state. The full implications of work. Low intensity workers were also more likely to
long and short exposures to a situation depend on the na- save their earnings for college. In turn, “steady workers”
ture of the situation itself. For example, is divorce pre- ( high duration, low intensity) are more likely to earn a
ceded by a lengthy period of family conflict? Little is BA degree within 9 years of high school graduation than
known about the qualitative nature of experiences of high duration-high intensity workers. Indeed, among
long and short durations, though a lengthy involvement students with low levels of educational promise, those
tends to increase behavioral continuity through acquired who chose a steady work pattern were more likely to re-
obligations, investments, and habits (Becker, 1964). The ceive their BA than their low promise, high duration-
longer the duration of marriage, for example, the greater high intensity counterparts. Such findings suggest that
the chances for marital permanence (Cherlin, 1993); al- work of differing durations and intensity has distinct
ternatively, marital happiness is likely to decline at all meanings and consequences and highlight the mislead-
marital durations, with accelerated declines occurring ing nature of cross-sectional studies.
during the earliest and latest years of marriage (Van- The duration of children’s exposure to poverty is like-
Laningham, Johnson, & Amato, 2001). The connection wise complex. Although poverty durations in the early
between marital permanence and the length of marriage life course have been linked to numerous indicators of
has much to do with shared material assets (Booth, psychosocial adjustment and achievement—including
Johnson, White, & Edwards., 1986), which often pre- cognitive development (G. J. Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, &
vent divorce in spite of marital unhappiness (White & Klebanov, 1994), delinquency (Jarjoura, Triplett, &
Booth, 1991). Much more needs to be known about the Brinker, 2002), and, perhaps to a lesser extent, emotional
quality of marriages of differing durations and their de- outcomes (Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997)—little is
velopment implications (Hetherington, 1999). known about the mechanisms that account for such rela-
Duration of unemployment also increases the risk of tionships. The challenge of understanding how the dura-
permanent unemployment. The latter may involve ac- tion of poverty coincides with developmental experiences
quired ties to the unemployed as well as a de-skilling is complicated by the patterns of deprivation observed in
process in developing the incapacity to work. Indeed, national samples.
many forms of social class and economic hardship en- Between ages 1 and 17, about one-third of American
dure. In two British birth cohorts, Schoon and her col- children will spend at least 1 year in poverty (Rank &
leagues (2002) found “a stark chaining or continuity Hirschl, 1999). Of individuals experiencing poverty,
of risk factors” such that parental social class power- about half of Blacks and one-third of Whites will fall
fully predicts material deprivations through childhood, into poverty in 5 or more of the next 10 years (Stevens,
adolescence, and into young adulthood. Yet, great diver- 1999). Bane and Ellwood (1986) conclude that “ the av-
sity in socioeconomic experiences has been observed erage poor black child today appears to be in the midst
through the life course, producing complex patterns of of a poverty spell which will last almost 2 decades”
Basic Concepts and Perspectives of the Individual Life Course 683

(p. 21). However, most poverty spells turned out to be Characteristics of paid work are known to be associ-
comparatively shorter, owing in large measure to ated with psychological and physical functioning, al-
markedly improved earnings (Bane & Ellwood, 1986). though little research has investigated temporal patterns
Slightly more than 40% of poverty spells in the Bane of work characteristics. A notable exception is Amick
and Ellwood analysis ended within a single year. About and his colleagues (2002), who report that men who
70% were over within a period of 3 years. In addition to spend their work lives in jobs with low levels of control
the potentially complex patterns of poverty over the life have a 43% increase in chance of death when compared
course, the first transition into poverty—particularly if with men who have jobs with high levels of control over
unexpected—may be especially damaging to children’s their work life. Thus, multiple aspects of social class are
well-being (Oh, 2001). likely to matter for well-being, but such relationships
Besides the duration of poverty, time spent in various depend on the duration of experiences.
social classes may also be important to well-being and Many cumulative processes refer not to the duration
achievement but, once again, temporal patterns of social of a particular social circumstance but rather to the trig-
class may be complex. McDonough, Duncan, Williams, gering of chains of interrelated events, which have sig-
and House (1997) showed that low income (i.e., less nificant implications for later well-being and attainment
than $20,000 per household) that persisted 4 to 5 years (Rutter, 1989). Behavioral continuities across the life
raised the risk of mortality among adults significantly course are likely to be found in social interactions that
when compared to households experiencing this income are sustained by their consequences (cumulative) and by
level for a transitory period. Middle-income adults, the tendency of these styles to evoke maintaining re-
however, are at increased risk for mortality if their in- sponses from the environment (reciprocal; Caspi, Bem,
come fluctuates appreciably over a 5-year period. Draw- & Elder, 1989). In cumulative continuity, both individ-
ing on the British Birth Cohort of 1958, Power, Manor, ual dispositions and family values are likely to favor the
and Matthews (1999) show that lifetime patterns of so- choice of compatible environments, and this reinforces
cial class ( based on father’s occupational status at birth, and sustains the match. Thus, antisocial youth tend to
and then one’s own occupational status) predict poor affiliate with other problem youth, and their interaction
health at age 33. The risk of poor health increased for generally accentuates their behavior, producing over
men in the most advantaged (4%) to the least advantaged time what might be described as cumulative disadvan-
(19%) lifetime SES groups (see also Mare, 1990). tages (Cairns & Cairns, 1994; Sampson & Laub, 1997;
The duration effects for socioeconomic stressors may Simmons, Burgeson, Carlton-Ford, & Blyth, 1987).
be nonlinear, in which case a tipping point is reached be- Among problem youth from inner-city neighborhoods,
fore an adverse outcome is observed. Additional time those who were most negative toward their life chances
spent in deprivation may lead to a nonlinear increase in lacked the support of close kin and friends, did not have
distress. For example, Wadsworth, Montgomery, and Bart- a supportive older sibling, and were most likely to be in-
ley (1999) observed that British men who experienced a volved with deviant friends (Furstenberg, Cook, Eccles,
long period of unemployment (exceeding 3 consecutive Elder, & Sameroff, 1999).
years) reported less healthy profiles subsequently, al- Reciprocal continuity refers to a continuous inter-
though this effect was not observed for lesser durations. change between person and environment in which reac-
Durations of poverty, low income, and other forms of tion forms action and then by another cycle of action and
socioeconomic disadvantage are likely to be related to a reaction. As with cumulative continuity, the net result of
wide range of indicators of adjustment and achievement, reciprocal continuity is the cumulation of experiences
although very little is known about the linking mecha- that tend to maintain and promote the same behavioral
nisms. One plausible mechanism is exposure to stressors outcome. Baldwin (1895) refers to such interchanges
and the availability of supports and coping mechanisms. as “circular functions” in ontogeny. The ill-tempered
While material resources undoubtedly shape the stress outburst of an adolescent may provoke a cycle of
process (Link & Phelan, 1995), a detailed understand- parental rage and aggression, a widening gulf of irrita-
ing of how temporal patterns of social class link to tion, and, finally, parental withdrawal, which reinforces
stressors, supports, and coping mechanisms remains the adolescent’s initial aggression (Pepler & Rubin,
largely unaddressed. The quality of one’s paid work is 1991). Over time, the interactional experiences of ag-
another plausible candidate linking social class with gressive children can establish attitudes that lead them
long-term outcomes. to project interpretations on new social encounters and
684 The Life Course and Human Development

relationships, thereby ensuring behavior that affirms the illness, providing powerful evidence for cumulative ef-
expected behavior. Aggressive children generally expect fects. By implication, recent employment compensates,
others to be hostile and thus behave in ways that elicit at least in part, for prior spells of unemployment, as
hostility, confirming their initial suspicions and rein- does improvements in one’s occupational status (see
forcing their behavior. Poulton et al., 2002).
A growing body of evidence points to cumulative Experiences may cumulate because social circum-
processes linking early social experiences—particu- stances are largely stable or lead to “ functionally equiv-
larly forms of inequality—and later health and well- alent ” relationships with people and involvements with
being (Power & Hertzman, 1997). Drawing on the organizations and institutions. In the latter case, chains
Dunedin sample, for example, Poulton and his col- of interrelated experiences all encourage the same be-
leagues (2002) show that childhood socioeconomic sta- havioral outcomes.
tus ( based on occupational categories) is an important
predictor of physical health at age 26 even with adult so-
Trajectories, Transitions, and Turning Points
cioeconomic position controlled. Children growing up
in households marked by low socioeconomic conditions Social pathways and cumulations of experience present
have poorer health—defined, for example, by the body- temporally sensitive descriptions of context. Trajecto-
mass index and cardio-respiratory fitness—when com- ries provide a dynamic view of behavior and achieve-
pared with children who grow up in high socioeconomic ments, typically over a substantial part of the life span.
households, regardless of their adult socioeconomic Transitions refer to a change in state or states such as
status. The cumulative mechanisms that link such early when youth leave home. A substantial change in the
experiences with later physical well-being are not course of a behavioral trajectory, often during transi-
well documented, although plausible mechanisms in- tions, may represent a turning point.
clude health-related behaviors (van de Mheen, Stronks, Trajectories and transitions are elements of estab-
Looman, & Mackenbach, 1998). lished pathways, their individual life courses, and devel-
Can cumulating processes that lead to poor outcomes opmental patterns. Among individuals, social roles evolve
be neutralized by subsequent experiences? The possibil- over an extended span of time, as in trajectories of work
ity is not often entertained, and the answer is likely to or family; and they change over a short time span. The
depend on the processes being studied, their temporal latter may be marked by specific events such as children
properties, and indicators of well-being and achievement entering school for the first time, completing the first
at issue. However, the question reflects the interest in grade successfully, and graduating from high school.
dynamic views of context and contingency that life- Each transition, combining a role exit and entry, is em-
course analysis promotes. Research shows, for example, bedded in a trajectory that gives it specific form and
that marital transitions involving the loss of a partner meaning. Thus, work transitions are core elements of a
are associated with declines in adult health. Can subse- work-life trajectory, and births are important markers
quent remarriage improve matters? It likely does along a parental trajectory.
(Willitts, Benzeval, & Stansfeld, 2004), but its salutary Trajectories and transitions refer to processes that are
effects appear to diminish with the number of prior mar- familiar in the study of work careers and life events. The
ital transitions (Barrett, 2000). language of careers has a distinguished history in the
Similarly, unemployment is thought to diminish field of occupations and the professions, and it still rep-
adult health and subsequently the family support of resents one of the rare languages that depict a temporal
children. In what circumstances will reentry into the dimension or process. Career lines, as pathways, refer to
labor market lead to improvements in well-being? sequences of positions, while careers, as trajectories,
Drawing on a representative sample of English and refer to coinciding behaviors and achievements. Work
Welsh working men, Bartley and Plewis (2002) exam- careers have been defined as disorderly and orderly, and
ined the effects of occupational category and unem- achievements have been represented as career advance-
ployment status in 1971, 1981, and 1991—covering ment, whether early or late, rapid or slow (Wilensky,
most of the men’s working lives—on long-term illness. 1960). The term career has also been applied to the tra-
Unemployment in 1971 and 1981 and occupational jectories of marriage and parenthood (Hill, 1970). All of
class at all three time points had independent effects on these uses fall in the more inclusive definition of a life-
Basic Concepts and Perspectives of the Individual Life Course 685

course trajectory. The term does not prejudge the direc- deed, drawing on semiparametric models, researchers
tion, degree, or rate of change in its course. have uncovered evidence for unique trajectories of anti-
A developmental trajectory refers to change and con- social behavior (e.g., Nagin & Land, 1993). With greater
stancy in the same behavior or disposition over time, but use of such models there has been increasing apprecia-
consistency of measurement may be difficult to achieve tion for methodological issues (see Bauer & Curran,
in many cases, especially in the measurement of aggres- 2003 and accompanying exchanges; Eggleston, Laub, &
sion and dependency (Kagan & Moss, 1962). Neverthe- Sampson, 2004; Nagin, 2004) and theoretical nuance
less, trajectories of intraindividual change tell a (Nagin & Paternoster, 2000) that complicate the search
different story from life stories based on cross-section for qualitatively distinct types of behavioral trajecto-
analysis, and this concept is compatible with widely ries. Nevertheless, this approach raises exciting possibil-
shared views of development (Molenaar, Huizenga, & ities for linking behavioral patterns with change and
Nesselroade, in press; Tremblay, 2004). Further, the stability in context and experience.
modeling of trajectories has become increasingly so- The multiple role trajectories of life patterns de-
phisticated, offering the analyst increasing options for scribe strategies of coordination or synchronization.
thinking about patterns of change (e.g., Collins & Sayer, Various demands compete for the individual’s or fam-
2001; T. E. Duncan, Duncan, S. Strycker, Li, & An- ily’s scarce resources such as time, energy, and money.
thony, 1999; Singer & Willett, 2003). Goode (1960) argues that an individual’s set of relation-
Developmental trajectories are also integral to life- ships is both “ unique and overdemanding,” requiring
course theory, especially when they are studied as inter- strategies that minimize demands by scheduling and
dependent with the changing dynamics of social rescheduling transitions where possible. To cope with
trajectories. In a four-wave study of early adolescents, simultaneous, linked trajectories, the scheduling of
based on growth-curve models, Ge and his colleagues events and obligations becomes a basic task in managing
(Ge, Lorenz, Conger, Elder, & Simons, 1994) found that resources and pressures. The needs of children and fi-
(a) the trajectories of depressive symptoms increased nancial requirements, for example, play important roles
sharply among White girls, surpassing the symptom in determining work and leisure options.
level of boys at age 13; ( b) the increase for girls was The meaning of a transition has much to do with its
linked to their exposure to an increasing level of nega- timing in a trajectory. Consider the case of parenthood:
tive events; and (c) the initial warmth and supportive- the earlier the event, the greater the risk of social and
ness of a mother minimized the subsequent risk of health disadvantages for mother and child (Furstenberg,
depressed states and negative events among daughters. Brooks-Gunn, & Morgan, 1987). Early life transitions can
Studies such as these have inspired many efforts to in- have developmental consequences by affecting subsequent
terrelate developmental trajectories and context, al- transitions, even after many years and decades have
though frequently neglecting the changing nature of passed. They do so through behavioral consequences that
social circumstance. set in motion cumulative advantages and disadvantages,
Increasing attention is being devoted to the study of with radiating implications for other life domains. A Bal-
classes of behavioral trajectories based on the supposi- timore study of adolescent mothers who were followed
tion that people may be qualitatively distinct in their de- from 1966 to 1984 (Furstenberg et al., 1987), shows that
velopmental patterns (Bauer & Curran, 2004; Nagin, variations in personal resources (e.g., IQ) during adoles-
1999; Nagin & Tremblay, 2001). According to this per- cence affected their economic success by influencing how
spective, the population is heterogeneous with respect to they timed and ordered early events from marriage to ed-
behavioral trajectories; as such, distinct subgroups can ucation or employment. From the vantage point of this
be identified, and their covariates examined. For exam- study, the quality of transition experiences early in life
ple, Moffitt (1993; see also Moffitt, Caspi, Harrington, may foretell the likelihood of successful and unsuccessful
& Milne, 2002) hypothesized that aggregate patterns in adaptation to later transitions across the life course.
antisocial behavior conceal two distinct groups: (1) A Transitions to parenthood during adolescence in the
small percentage of youth engaged in antisocial behavior Baltimore panel raise another important general distinc-
at every stage of life (“life-course persistent ”), and (2) a tion: Life transitions can be thought of as a succession
larger percentage of youth engaged in antisocial behav- of mini-transitions or choice points. The transition from
ior during adolescence only (“adolescence-limited”). In- marriage to divorce is not simply a change in state, but
686 The Life Course and Human Development

begins with disenchantment and extends across divorce course dynamics and explanations. The process of be-
threats, periods of separation, and the filing of divorce coming an adolescent mother requires sexual activity,
papers. Different causal factors may operate at each failure to use, or ineffective use of, contraception, and,
phase of the process. The “origin” influences that in- once pregnant, the decision to bear and raise the child.
crease the risk of disenchantment are likely to differ Across these stages of the process, an agency “ has sev-
from those that sustain the process toward marital disso- eral possible points at which to target its interventions;
lution. In like manner, we can think of the transition to at initiation of sexual activity, at contraception use, or,
motherhood in adolescence as a multiphasic process in at the resolution of a pregnancy” (Hofferth, 1987, p. 78).
which each phase is marked by a choice point with op- The significance of this formulation becomes apparent
tions and social constraints. when we return to a time when unwed motherhood was
For example, young women may choose to engage in viewed simply as one transition, a concept that obscured
premarital sex or not, to use contraception or not, to the strategic points of preventive intervention along the
seek an abortion or not, and to marry the father or not. life course. Given their multiphasic nature, many transi-
Only a handful of options lead to an illegitimate birth. tions cover relatively long periods. The female pubertal
After the birth of a child, young women face other deci- transition, for example, begins before menarche and may
sions, such as whether to ask for their mothers’ help in last well beyond it. As Dorn and her colleagues (Dorn,
child care or to put the child up for adoption, to marry or Susman, & Ponirakis, 2003) observe, “Although menar-
to remain single, or to pursue educational and employ- che itself is an event (the first bleed), the integration of
ment possibilities or to enter the welfare system. The our findings of pre- and postmenarcheal hormone vari-
implications of having an illegitimate birth vary accord- ability support a conceptualization of menarche as an in-
ing to the options chosen. Some opportunities blend well tegral part of a longer-term biological process” (p. 300).
in favoring positive outcomes: They represent an adap- Similarly, the transition to adulthood—as indicated by
tive strategy, while others do not. demographic markers—begins when youth complete
Figure 12.2 presents a series of transitions that can school, which occurs at age 17 or 18 for most American
lead to unwed motherhood, as sketched from the Balti- youth, and ends with family formation, which may take
more study. Each choice point occurs at a different state place in the mid-30s, if at all (Fussell & Furstenberg,
in a young girl’s life and thus could involve varied life- 2005). Developmentalists tend to view transitions as dis-

Premarital sexual experience


among young, never-married females Nonexperience

Sex without contraceptives Contraceptive protection

Pregnancy Not Pregnant

Decision to have child out of wedlock Abortion or marriage

Decision not to marry after birth Marriage following birth

Unwed motherhood

Figure 12.2 The life course of unwed motherhood. As adapted from “Family Transitions, Cycles, and Social Change” (p. 41),
by G. H. Elder Jr., in Family Transitions, P. A. Cowan and M. Hetherington (Eds.), 1991, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Reprinted with
permission.
Basic Concepts and Perspectives of the Individual Life Course 687

crete events that occur in a relatively short period and, terns into high school. In the American school system,
consequently, very little is known about the sequence of pathways between middle school and high school are
minitransitions leading to full transitions. structured in different ways, affecting the proportion of
The two faces of a transition—leaving a state and en- one’s middle school classmates who attend the same high
tering a new state—can have different causal explana- school. Schiller’s (1999) study of how differing feeder
tions, as when divorce is followed by remarriage. patterns affect subsequent grades is revealing. Among
Moreover, “leaving a state” is part of the individual’s students receiving mostly Cs in middle school, high
history that shapes the meaning of the new role or situa- school math grades decrease as the proportion of one’s
tion (Wheaton, 1990). Leaving a conflicted, violent classmates in the same high school increases. The reverse
marriage would cast single status as a stress-relieving is true among students receiving mostly As in middle
status. The meaning and developmental implications of school: High school math grades increase as the propor-
a transition depend on the timing of the change relative tion of one’s classmates in the same high school in-
to norms and cognitive expectations. Judging from stud- creases. As Schiller notes, when middle school students
ies conducted to date (McLanahan & Sorensen, 1985; disperse into many high schools, opportunities seem to
Wortman & Silver, 1990), widowhood is most distress- open up for students at the bottom, as peer networks are
ing for young women, a time when the event is least ex- disrupted. Consistent with a turning point, the old social
pected or normative. By comparison, job loss during world is knifed off and new opportunities for growth and
young adulthood is relatively common, and entails less identity change present themselves.
of an emotional risk than in the later years. The concept of turning point also applies to the partic-
The apparent contrast between institutionalized tran- ular way people view their life trajectory—a subjective
sitions and personal, idiosyncratic “ transition experi- account of lived experience involves some degree of
ence” can misrepresent reality. In many cases, life change in situation, behavior, or meaning. Maruna and his
transitions are an institutionalized status passage in the colleagues’ (2001) exceptional interview study of desis-
life course of birth cohorts and a personalized transition tance among ex-convicts is one of a few research efforts
for individuals with a distinctive life and social history. to investigate the changing nature of the self during a
The latter may represent an individual working out of the turning point. Important themes in the life narratives of
former. These faces of a transition apply to the norma- desisters include acknowledging past crimes, understand-
tive transitions of life, from birth to school entry, mar- ing their genesis, and recasting the self as in control and
riage, parenthood, and retirement. Transitions of this with newfound purpose. Clausen (1995) has used de-
kind may seem more predictable and structured than tailed analyses of life histories to assess the subjective
nonnormative events, but all transitions can be sorted ac- turning points of people who have been part of a longitu-
cording to their structuredness or degree of external reg- dinal study for 60 or more years. Based on this work, he
ulation, duration, timing, predictability, and novelty. concludes that “one’s life does not have to take a different
Life transitions into different environments facilitate direction for a person to feel that a turning point has oc-
this process by representing potential turning points in a curred. But one must have a feeling that new meanings
trajectory for a troubled life course. Such turning points have been acquired, whether or not life experiences are
are sometimes referred to as “ knifing off ” past experi- much changed” (p. 371).
ences, which can allow for new opportunities and behav- The challenge to life-course study is to understand
ioral patterns. One example of a turning point is the the linkages among changing pathways and transitions,
desistance from criminal activity, a “ knifing off experi- life patterns, and developmental trajectories. A useful
ence” that involves a transition into new situations that first step in this direction leads to mechanisms that link
provide monitoring, social supports, growth experi- context and the individual life courses. Our next topic of
ences, and the emergence of a new self-identity (Laub & basic concepts and distinctions.
Sampson, 2003). Military service, gainful employment,
and marriage are all new role commitments that provide Selection, Endogeneity, and Contextual Effects
opportunities for a break from the past and social inte-
gration (see also Bouffard & Laub, 2004). Life-course analysis investigates the dynamic features
A further example of turning points—this time in an of social contexts and seeks to understand the relevant
educational trajectory—is found in a study of feeder pat- mechanisms by which time and place shape human
688 The Life Course and Human Development

development. Any study of context and behavior must Transitions of this kind generally accentuate the behav-
address the complex processes by which people select ioral effect of the selected dispositions, producing
and otherwise experience specific environments in the greater individual differences and heterogeneity be-
first place (Caspi, 2004). Consider life-events, which tween groups. Cairns and Cairns (1994, p. 117) observe
numerous studies identify as potent stressors. Virtually that social selection and accentuation go together in peer
all research on life-events views them as causes of dis- group formation. Once a group is formed in terms of se-
tress (e.g., depressive symptoms) with very little appre- lected attributes (e.g., aggressivity), the selected behav-
ciation that (a) distress can increase the likelihood of iors are accentuated. This process has obvious social
encountering life-events, and ( b) various circumstances implications when unruly behavior is involved and makes
( hypothetically, for example, working conditions, so- identifying “peer effects” particularly difficult.
cioeconomic status) may cause both life-events and de- Much research has examined the effects of paid work
pressive symptoms. By failing to account for these during high school on grades, neglecting the possibility
exogenous processes, the estimated effect of life-events that less academically engaged students may choose to
is likely to be biased. Moreover, this task involves im- work longer hours. Adjusting for such a selection pro-
portant issues of theory as well as methods. cess, the effect of work hours on grades is negligible or
Thoits’s (1994) study of life-events and mastery is insignificant (Schoenhals, Tienda, & Schneider, 1998;
instructive. Drawing on two measurement occasions, Warren, LePore, & Mare, 2000; see also Paternoster,
she classified the stress associated with major life- Bushway, Brame, & Apel, 2003, for the case of paid
events in the workplace and in one’s romantic life as (a) work and antisocial behaviors). The issue can also be
solved, ( b) unsolved but attempted to solve, and (c) un- viewed in experimental terms: When preexisting differ-
solved and did not try. Scores on a mastery scale at Time ences between people cannot be ruled out by random as-
1 were highest at Time 2 for adults who reported no signment (e.g., differing levels of school engagement),
problematic situations. Scores diminished significantly the “pure effect ” of the experimental manipulation
for adults who encountered life-events and solved them (e.g., hours per week of paid work during high school)
but were lowest for people who encountered life-events on the outcome (e.g., grades during high school) cannot
and did not solve them. Mastery predicted the subse- be determined with certainty.
quent experience of life-events and attempts to resolve In some instances, the problem can be addressed with
their attendant distress. Similarly, Shanahan and Bauer highly revealing randomized trials. What, for example,
(2005) show that low levels of mastery in high school in- are the implications of residential change? Perhaps mov-
crease the likelihood of life-events after graduation, ing from poor urban areas to more advantaged neighbor-
which in turn diminish mastery in young adulthood. In- hoods improves the lives of children. Do they profit
deed, growing evidence suggests that internalizing and from the change? The question is difficult to answer
externalizing symptoms increase the likelihood of en- given that certain types of families—those possessing
countering stressors (Aseltine, Gore, & Gordon, 2000; more resources—would be likely to move in the first
Hoffman & Cerbone, 1999; Kim, Conger, Elder, & place. Yet, a randomized study of the question became
Lorenz, 2003; Leadbeater, Kuperminc, Blatt, & Herzog, possible with the Moving to Opportunity (MTO)
1999). By implication, failure to account for such initial demonstration project, which has operated in five U.S.
differences might well overstate the true magnitude of cities (Baltimore, Boston, Chicago, Los Angeles, and
the effect of such stressors on personal well-being. New York) since 1994 (see Katz, Kling, & Liebman,
More generally, the challenge of preexisting differ- 2001; Ludwig, Duncan, & Hirschfield, 2001). Families
ences that lead people to contexts and promote behav- were eligible to participate if they had children and
ioral outcomes is pervasive in the study of human lived in public housing or Section 8 housing with a
development. Thus, high school students tend to enter neighborhood poverty rate of 40% or more. Interested
college environments that “are consistent ” with their families who applied were randomly assigned to one of
personal characteristics (Alwin, Cohen, & Newcomb, three groups: the experimental group (which received
1991), assortative processes create homophily in form- rent vouchers for housing in low poverty areas), a Sec-
ing friendships and heterosexual pairs (Caspi & Her- tion 8 comparison group (which received unrestricted
bener, 1990), and risk takers in military service are rent vouchers), and a control group (which did not re-
likely to end up in combat units (Gimbel & Booth, 1996). ceive rent vouchers). The design is especially helpful in
Life Transitions and Historical Change 689

learning about how neighborhoods affect well-being be- variable estimation). More important, the issue can
cause, in normal circumstances, specific types of fami- also be viewed through life-course theory. As George
lies live in specific types of neighborhoods, which (2003) notes, selection and endogeniety are less a
makes it difficult to disentangle the contributions of methodological nuisance than another way of describ-
families and neighborhoods. ing ideas that have long been central to life-course the-
In the typical move to a middle-class, European- ory, particularly pathways and agency. In a life course,
American suburb, the African American mothers and their contextual experiences most likely reflect prior cir-
children were involved in radically different worlds with cumstances. Such prior conditions and experiences
higher behavioral expectations and typically European- represent a substantive interest of life-course research,
American age-mates. If unemployed before the move, although they can also be viewed as potential threats to
African American mothers who moved to the suburbs identifying the relationship between present circum-
were more likely to find jobs and to engage in job stances and behavior.
searches, when compared to the city movers. In the
follow-up, the suburban minority students more often
followed a college track and attended a 2- or 4-year col- LIFE TRANSITIONS AND
lege. If not in college, they were nearly twice as likely as HISTORICAL CHANGE
city movers to be employed full-time with pay greater
than the minimum wage and job benefits. The suburban We have identified properties of life transitions that
adolescents were also far more likely to be engaged specify a way of thinking about social change and its
daily in activities with European-American students, psychosocial effects. According to this account, social
despite racial threats and harassment. change refers to a broad range of transitional phenomena
Before and after comparisons show that the transi- such as residential change. Additional contributions to
tion improved life chances, at least for the females. this perspective come from mechanisms that link transi-
Four to 7 years after baseline, girls’ mental health im- tions and life patterns to historical change, such as the
proved, although boys’ problem behaviors may have Great Depression and World War II, and to paradigmatic
worsened as a result of their families’ receiving the principles that define the life course as a theoretical ori-
MTO offer to move (G. J. Duncan, Clark-Kauffman, & entation. These mechanisms include the notion of life
Snell, in press). Girls in the experimental group also re- stage and its social roles, the social imperatives of new
ported less risky behavior and better educational out- roles or situations, the cycle of losing and regaining a
comes, while males exhibited more risky behaviors and sense of personal control in changing situations, and the
physical health problems (Kling & Liebman, 2004). The accentuation of individual dispositions in changing envi-
lack of advantage for boys in the MTO experimental ronments. Each of these mechanisms involves interde-
group is difficult to explain, although Kling and Lieb- pendent lives. The paradigmatic principles draw on
man speculate that boys in the experimental group may these mechanisms in charting the perspective of life-
have experienced stereotypes, relinquished fewer ties course study—the principles of lifelong development
to their old neighborhoods, and settled into peer groups and aging, human agency in making choices, the impor-
that exerted negative influences. Rabinowitz and tance of timing in lives, linked lives, and historical time
Rosenbaum (2000) provide valuable developmental in- and place (Elder, 1998b). These principles represent
sights on these transition experiences in their account more general theoretical themes that collectively define
of Chicago’s Gautreaux program, with its goal of en- the analytical scope of life-course theory.
abling families to leave public housing for suburbs and
city neighborhoods that were better off financially.
Linking Mechanisms
In many instances, randomized trials such as the
MTO are not possible or offer imperfect solutions Linking mechanisms refer to the processes by which
themselves (Kaufman, Kaufman, & Poole, 2003). In social change and behavioral development are interre-
such cases, statistical models may be helpful in deter- lated. They were originally documented in Children of
mining unbiased effects of social context. Yet, no sta- the Great Depression (Elder, 1974/1999), a study of co-
tistical solution is without assumptions and drawbacks horts born at opposite ends of the 1920s and occupied
(e.g., Bound, Jaeger, & Baker, 1995, on instrumental different life stages when the economy collapsed. The
690 The Life Course and Human Development

life-stage principle, noted above, suggests that young Another linking mechanism involves situational imper-
people of different ages are likely to be exposed to a dif- atives, the behavioral demands or requirements of a new
ferent slice of history. Indeed, the Oakland children situation. The more demanding the situation, the more in-
passed through adolescence during the worst years of dividual behavior is constrained to meet role expectations.
the Great Depression, but the Berkeley children became In emergency family situations, helpful responses become
teenagers in World War II. Consequently, job scarcity, an imperative for members, as in hard-pressed families
financial pressures, and emotional stress represented during the worst years of the Great Depression. Rachman
defining features of the Oakland cohort’s transition (1979) refers to these imperatives as “required helpful-
from childhood to young adulthood. By contrast, mem- ness.” The Oakland children were old enough in the early
bers of the Berkeley cohort were exposed to the “empty 1930s to be called on to meet the increased economic and
households” of World War II when parents worked from labor needs of their family, and a large number managed
sun up to sun down in home front industries. to earn money on paid jobs and to help in the household.
By encountering the Great Depression and other his- This money was often used to cover traditional family
torical events at different times in life, the Oakland and concerns such as school expenses.
Berkeley cohorts have different stories to tell about their In deprived families, girls generally specialized in
childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. The particular household chores, while boys were more often involved
sequence and timing of prosperity, economic depression, in paid jobs. This gender difference made girls more de-
and war distinguishes the developmental experiences of pendent on the family and generally fostered greater au-
the two birth cohorts. The concept “goodness of fit ” in tonomy among boys. Adolescent jobs in the 1930s
the match of person-environment is an important feature typically included what might be regarded as odd jobs in
of the life-stage principle and its implications for human the adult world, from waiting on tables and clerking to
development (see Eccles & Midgley, 1989, p. 9). delivering newspapers and running errands. Employ-
Consider the Berkeley males who entered the Great ment of this kind may seem developmentally insignifi-
Depression when they were highly dependent on family cant, though it carried the important implication that
nurturance and vulnerable to family instability. Eco- people counted on them—they mattered. Indeed, staff
nomic hardship came early in their lives and represented observers rated the working boys as more energetic and
a prolonged deprivational experience, from the economic efficacious than other boys on a set of scales. The flow
valley of the 1930s to the war years and departure from of influence was no doubt reciprocal. The more industri-
home. By comparison, the Oakland males were older and ous were likely to find jobs and success in work that
more independent when hardship hit their families. They would reinforce their ambition. With additional chores
assumed important roles in the household economy and at home, working boys experienced something like
entered adulthood with a more crystallized idea of their the obligations of adult status. To observers who knew
occupational goals. Despite some handicaps in education, them, they appeared to be more adult-oriented in values,
they managed to end up at midlife with a slightly higher interests, and activities when compared to other youth.
occupational rank. The life stage of the Oakland males Boys who managed both household chores and paid
represented a better fit in the match between person and jobs were most likely to think about the future and es-
environment when compared to the Berkeley males. pecially about a career. In adulthood, these youth were
The vulnerability of the younger Berkeley boys is con- more apt to have achieved a measure of clarity and self-
sistent with the results of other studies, which show that assurance in their work career when compared to other
family stressors are especially pathogenic for males in males. They also settled more quickly on a stable
early childhood (e.g., Rutter & Madge, 1976). How can line of work and displayed less floundering during their
we explain the accomplishments of the older Oakland 20s. Apart from level of education, this work life has
males up to middle age? One explanation is that they as- much to do with the occupational success and work
sumed valuable economic and household roles amid fam- ethic of men who grew up in deprived families during
ily hardships. Family experience of this kind enhanced the 1930s. The response of these young people to De-
their social independence and reduced their exposure to pression imperatives had enduring consequences for
family conflict. Another explanation centers on the mili- their lives and values.
tary experience of most of these young men. Service in the The developmental significance of adaptations to the
military made higher education more possible through the imperatives of new and challenging situations is ex-
GI Bill and frequently led to a supportive marriage. pressed across the life course and in other cultures.
Life Transitions and Historical Change 691

Consider the reunification of West and East Germany, centuation dynamic tends to amplify selection behaviors.
which produced dramatically new worlds, “almost from From this perspective, early transitional experiences be-
one day to the next, new laws, new institutions, and a come prologues for adult transitions that increase hetero-
new economic system governed the lives of the people of geneity over the life course. We see this development in
East Germany” (Pinquart & Silbereisen, 2004, p. 290). longitudinal studies of divorce and their increasing atten-
For the young, the school system changed almost tion to the behavioral changes initiated by it across the
overnight and students soon faced a dramatically differ- life course and the generations (Amato, 2000; Amato &
ent economy as well. Silbereisen launched a project to Cheadle, 2005). In children, as well as adults, the divorce
study these changes and their developmental effects transition appears to accentuate dispositions that were
soon after the collapse of East Germany—a cohort of present well before the event itself. For example, boys
East and West Germans from age 13 to 29 in 1991, fol- with behavior problems after a divorce were frequently
lowed up by a 1996 survey. Adaptations to the unifica- engaged in problem behavior before the divorce.
tion process were slow in coming among respondents in Selection and social causation processes are inter-
the study, and especially among those from former East woven in this accentuation process. Quinton and associ-
Germany such as young women of lower status. They re- ates (Quinton, Pickles, Maughan, & Rutter, 1993) show
ported later family transitions and financial self- this process in the history of a conduct disordered boy.
support in 1996, when compared to the West Germans. Conduct disorder is associated with conflicted, dysfunc-
Situational imperatives are elements of new situa- tional parenting. Children in these environments were
tions that characterize control cycles, which, as de- most at risk of choosing deviant friends when parents
scribed by W. I. Thomas (see Elder & Caspi, 1988), were in conflict. A harmonious family sharply reduced
refer to changing relations between expectations and re- this risk. The next step involved the selection of a de-
sources that affect a sense of personal control. A loss of viant partner, a process that occurred through a deviant
control stems from a process in which resources fall peer network. For girls, and especially those not in-
below expectations. This change motivates efforts to re- clined to plan, early pregnancy resulted from a deviant
store control by adjusting expectations, resources, or boyfriend. More future-oriented youth managed to avoid
both in terms of their relation. During the Great Depres- this outcome and were better able to establish stable re-
sion, heavy income loss tended to affect children, some- lations with nondeviant mates.
times adversely, through family adaptations to such As a whole, these linking mechanisms—life stage, sit-
deprivation in the Great Depression. These include the uational imperatives, control cycle, and the accentuation
reduction of family expenditures, the employment of dynamic—represent different understandings of the con-
more family members, and the lowering of living stan- nections among individual lives, developmental trajecto-
dards (Elder, 1974/1999). Equilibrium in these finan- ries, and the changing social world. They are embedded
cially strained families was achieved when expectations in a theoretical framework defined by paradigmatic prin-
matched resources. The psychology of this cyclical pro- ciples of the life course where they specify the dynamics
cess is well described by what Brehm and Brehm call at work. Consider, for example, the first principle on
“reactance” (1982). Feelings of reactance occur when- human development and aging as lifelong processes. New
ever one or more freedoms or expectations are elimi- transitions along the life course establish different life
nated or threatened. Such emotions spur efforts to stages and tend to accentuate the dispositions people
regain or preserve control. “It is the threat to control bring to the situation with its social imperatives in shap-
(which one had) that motivates an attempt to deal with ing behavior. A life transition also entails some loss of
the environment ” (p. 375). Once control is achieved, ex- personal control and motivates efforts to regain it.
pectations may be raised, thereby setting in motion an-
other round of equilibrating initiatives. Paradigmatic Principles of Life-Course Theory
The final mechanism, known as the accentuation dy-
namic, relates transition experiences to the individual’s The following principles emerged from studies of Chil-
life history of past events, acquired dispositions, and dren of the Great Depression (Elder, 1974/1999), subse-
meanings. When a transition heightens a prominent at- quent research, and the complex interplay of intellectual
tribute that people bring to the new role or situation, we and social forces at the time that stressed the impor-
refer to the change as an accentuation effect. Entry into tance of placing human development in a lifelong
new roles or situations is frequently selective, and the ac- context (Elder, Johnson, & Crosnoe, 2003). Collectively,
692 The Life Course and Human Development

they define the life course as a theoretical orientation proactive interactions. Evocative interactions refer to
that provides a framework for studying phenomena at the the process by which an individual’s appearance, behav-
nexus of social change, social pathways, and develop- ior, or personality elicits distinctive responses from oth-
mental trajectories. We begin with the principle of life- ers. Reactive interactions refer to people who encounter
span development. the same situation but interpret and respond to it differ-
ently. Examples include academic failure and the sepa-
The Principle of Life-Span Development: Human ration of parents, and their variable meaning at different
Development and Aging Are Lifelong Processes life stages. Proactive interactions refer to the selection
Over the years, the life span has been represented as a of environments such as friendships.
sequence of life stages, from infancy and early child- Transitional experiences across the life course in-
hood to old age. Each stage became an age-specific do- volve individual initiatives, situational constraints and
main for specialized study. However, we recognize now opportunities, the dispositions and prior experiences
that developmental and aging processes are most fully that people bring to new situations, and the influence of
understood from a lifelong perspective (Kuh, Power, others. Though many factors influence lives, young peo-
Blane, & Bartley, 1997). Behavior patterns at midlife ple play an important role in constructing their own lives
are not only influenced by current circumstances and by though the choices they make.
the anticipation of the future, but also by prenatal and
The Principle of Human Agency: Individuals
early childhood experiences. The biomarkers of predis-
Construct Their Own Life Course through
ease pathways extend back to the early years (Singer &
Choices and Actions They Take within the
Ryff, 2001) and, perhaps, even reflect intrauterine expe-
Opportunities and Constraints of History and
riences shaped by the mother’s context and experiences.
Social Circumstance
Long-term studies are documenting the relationship be-
tween late-life adaptation and the formative years of Elements of human agency have been prominent in stud-
life-span development. These include the national longi- ies of lives (see Haidt & Rodin, 1999; Thomas &
tudinal studies of birth cohorts in Great Britain, marked Znaniecki, 1918–1920) and are central to studies that
by birthdates of 1946, 1958, 1970, and 2000. They are relate lives to broader social contexts. People make
all scheduled to be followed into the later years of life choices in constrained situations that enable them to
(Ferri, Bynner, & Wadsworth, 2002). Such long-term exert a measure of control over their life course. These
longitudinal studies are still rare. choices ensure a degree of loose coupling between social
This temporal frame poses major challenges as well as transitions and life stages. Even during the economic
exciting opportunities. The longer a life is studied, the turmoil and distress of the 1930s, mothers found jobs
greater the risk of exposure to social change. The lives of amid scarce options, while many of their children car-
people in their 80s or 90s are thus most likely to reflect ried responsibilities in the home and community. When
the particular contours of a changing society. Longitudi- deprived parents moved their residence to cheaper quar-
nal data archives generally lack adequate information on ters and sought alternative forms of income, they were
change in social relationships, social organizations, and involved in a process of “ building a new life course.”
residential ecologies. However, the availability of geo- One part of this process appears in the response of
graphic codes with coordinates that map households for young people to needs in the family economy. As noted
users of large data sets now enable investigators to assess earlier, they were called on to meet the increased eco-
contextual changes and their effects on lives. nomic and labor needs of deprived households, and a
Another challenge posed by the principle of lifelong large number managed tasks in the family and earned
development and aging centers on the question of why money on paid jobs.
some behavioral patterns persist while others fade. The initiative of Depression youth from financially
What influences play a role in this change? We are just strained families is much the same among contemporary
beginning to identify mechanisms that drive patterns of youth in an Iowa longitudinal study of rural hardship
continuity and change. For example, Caspi and Bem (Elder & Conger, 2000). This pioneering study began in
(1990) identify three modes of interaction between per- the late 1980s with 451 farm and small town adolescents
son and situation that have relevance to individual conti- and their families in the north central region of Iowa,
nuity and change: (1) evocative, (2) reactive, and (3) United States. The adolescents are now in their late 20s.
Life Transitions and Historical Change 693

Children of deprived rural families in this region as- more “planful decisions” lead to greater success in work
sumed more responsibilities such as unpaid chores and and family through adulthood. Planful competence was
work on the farm. Boys and girls also sought paid work defined by three dimensions: (1) self-confidence, (2) de-
when faced by the time and labor pressures of both large pendability, and (3) intellectual investment. A competent
households and economically distressed farm families. adolescent is equipped with the self-discipline to pursue
Whether living on a farm or not, working adolescents chosen goals and has the ability to evaluate personal ef-
tended to describe themselves as industrious and effica- forts accurately as well as the intentions and actions of
cious, more so than other youth. Many of these young others, using an informed knowledge of self, others, and
people would eventually have to seek their fortunes in plausible options. Clausen found that the highly compe-
other communities, and we know that the movers were tent males in adolescence were most likely to achieve
among the more capable members of the younger rural a successful start through education, occupational ca-
generation. Youth with migration intentions had done reers, and family, apart from the influence of IQ and
well in school, but they perceived dismal life chances in class background. Moreover, this beginning anticipated
their local region. achievements across the life course, even into the 60s.
Choice making in migration is vividly expressed in The young men with a planful competence were more
Hagan’s (2001) account of American war resisters dur- likely to have stable marriages and careers and tended to
ing the Vietnam War, and their troubled decision to take find satisfaction and fulfillment during their final
the “northern passage” to Toronto, Canada, a legal sanc- decades. The planfully competent also ranked highest on
tuary from the American selective service system. the stability of their personality across the years.
Nearly 10,000 men chose to defy the military draft and Do these findings reflect the special circumstances
the counsel of their families, and some made this jour- of the study members’ early adult years—the beginning
ney with female friends. A majority continued their of World War II and an unparalleled era of prosperity?
protest of the war along the way and in their northern Postwar benefits for veterans encouraged them to obtain
community. Though many years removed from the Viet- a college education, but what if we stepped back a
nam crisis, former war resisters (a majority of whom decade of two so that both a Great Depression and
still reside in Canada) remembered the emotional com- global war loomed ahead? To do this, we turned to the
plexity and discord of their decision process. War re- Lewis Terman data archive (Holahan & Sears, 1995), a
sisters made their decision to settle in Canada after longitudinal study of the brightest Californians. This
countless appeals and protests, knowing the moral study of talented children was launched in the 1920s, a
stigma of their action in the American public. Inter- time when California’s economy seemed to offer unlim-
views recall this traumatic time and the process by ited opportunity. Half of the children were born before
which “each decision against service in the war ” con- 1911, the other half by the early 1920s. By selecting
structed a deviant path from the perspective of Ameri- only the most able of California’s children for the study,
can society. This was a different path from that followed Terman could direct his attention to great promise and
by siblings. They were more likely to be employed in the expected rise of talent to positions of accomplish-
human service and artistic professions, and they ended ment and leadership.
up with lower earnings, but this inequality stemmed But history changed this trajectory (Shanahan &
more from their prolonged involvement in war protests Elder, 2002; Shanahan, Elder, & Miech, 1997). The
and the world of activism, which also altered their sense older cohort had completed most of its post-high school
of self and relationships with family and friends. education by the stock market crash and looked ahead to
Does “planfulness” make a difference in the quality of a stagnant and declining labor market, while the younger
life choices and agency? In American Lives (1993), John men faced the prospects of going to college in the later
Clausen focused on this question, with emphasis on the years of the Depression decade. Lacking good job
formative adolescent years of Californians who were prospects, a substantial number of the older men stayed
members of the Oakland and Berkeley Guidance Studies. in graduate school, extending their list of degrees. By
He hypothesized that competent adolescents who think contrast, World War II reduced significantly the educa-
about the future with a sense of personal efficacy are tional opportunities of the younger men, while having no
more effective in making sound choices and in imple- impact on the education of the older men who were well
menting them during the transition to adulthood. These past the college years.
694 The Life Course and Human Development

With these different historical paths in mind, it is not most damaging during the early preschool years, owing
surprising that planful competence in adolescence had to heavy time demands; (2) duration of residence lessens
much greater relevance for the future of the younger the educational achievement of offspring by diminishing
men, when compared to the older cohort. The planful- social resources; and (3) boys are likely to be more im-
ness of the older men in adolescence had no effect paired by the change than girls, owing to modeling
on their chances for advanced education and career processes (see also McLanahan & Sandefur, 1994). Al-
achievement. In large part, this outcome reflects the though Krein and Beller designed precise measures of
process of “ warehousing” in which the young prolong the age and length of time a child lived in a single-parent
their stay in school during economically troubled times. household, the reports of family structure are retrospec-
School persistence had less to do with personal motiva- tive because the mothers were interviewed between the
tion than with a way of getting out of hardship situa- ages of 30 and 44. The offspring were interviewed when
tions. Life constraints and the timing of careers shape they were 14 to 24 years. However, such retrospective
the choices of human agency. reports are reasonably accurate.
The study found that timing mattered, along with du-
The Principle of Timing: The Developmental
ration and gender: (a) the adverse effect on education
Antecedents and Consequences of Life
was much greater for the preschool versus the later
Transitions, Events, and Behavior Patterns Vary
years, ( b) the adverse effect increased with the number
according to Timing in a Life Course
of years a child spent in a single-parent household, and
Lifelong processes of human development and human (c) the adverse effect was more negative for males than
agency underscore ways of thinking about the timing of for females. The strongest and most consistent timing
lives and their social contexts. As Bernice Neugarten and duration effects were obtained among White males,
(1968) has shown in her pioneering work, people do not with family income controlled. Black females and men
march through life in concert. They tend to vary by the were next in line on effects, followed at some distance by
age at which they pass through life transitions—when White females. Whether family income was controlled,
they begin and complete their schooling, enter a first job, the timing and duration of living in a single-parent house-
establish an independent domicile, share a household hold mattered least for White females. The meaning of
with a friend, marry, have children, see children leave this result was not pursued in the study, although these
home, and lose their first parent. They also vary in when young daughters of single-parent mothers may be pro-
they perceive themselves as young, middle age, and old. tected by maternal support and the model of a self-
In Children of the Great Depression (Elder, 1974/ sufficient woman. Little is known about the actual life
1999), some members of their cohort entered marriage history of family structure and the timing of a child’s
before their twentieth birthday, while others were still exposure to different phases.
unmarried a decade later. Early marriage tended to pro- Social disadvantage though family dissolution, er-
duce life disadvantages, from socioeconomic hardship to ratic parenting, and low socioeconomic status plays an
the loss of education. Early childbearing had similar important role in channeling youth toward involvement
consequences. All of these age variations or differences in antisocial behavior (Sampson & Laub, 1993). The
can make a difference by setting in motion a dynamic of early timing of this involvement increases the risk of a
cumulative events and processes. To illustrate this point, persistent pattern of such behavior. Thirty-five years
we turn to the ages at which children experience the ago Lee Robins (1966) found that antisocial boys were
breakup of their family and engage in behaviors that ini- likely to enter unskilled jobs and experience spells of
tiate a sequence of disadvantage. unemployment, short work weeks, and an unstable mar-
No time is a good time for a child’s loss of a parent riage. More recently, a study of ill-tempered boys in the
through separation or divorce, but the child’s age when Berkeley Guidance Study found them to be at risk for
such change occurs can make an important difference in the very same kind of disorganized life course (Caspi,
its consequences. To address the impact of a single-par- Elder, & Bem, 1987). They were not able to maintain so-
ent household, Krein and Beller (1988) matched mother- cial relationships or jobs. Sequences of adverse events
daughter and mother-son samples from the National are part of a cumulative process of life disadvantage.
Longitudinal Surveys to investigate three relevant hy- Just how this process unfolds is a subject of much theo-
potheses: (1) the transition to single-parent status is rizing and research. Patterson (1996) views the process
Life Transitions and Historical Change 695

as a cascade of secondary problems such as school fail- defined two groups with scales and interviews: childhood
ure, depressed mood, and parent rejection. An early his- and adolescent onset of antisocial behavior. With associ-
tory of antisocial behavior is linked to late adolescent ates, she has followed the males up to their 26th year
conduct through such processes. (Moffitt et al., 2002), a time that is still prior to the me-
From this research and studies into the adult years, dian age at first marriage for males in New Zealand. At
three markers along a disadvantaged life course emerge: this age, the childhood group shows the most elevated
(1) age at first arrest, (2) incarceration or jail time, and pattern of psychopathic personality traits, problems of
(3) unemployment. In combination, they underscore the mental health, violent and drug-related crime, substance
importance of “onset timing for deviant activities.” The dependence, and financial-work problems. Adolescent-
earlier the age at first arrest the greater the likelihood of onset delinquents were less extreme on these measures.
a criminal career (Farrington et al., 1990). Age at first Neither group of young people reveal turning points in
arrest is a reliable predictor of this future because it behavior, perhaps owing to their continuing status in the
sharply increases the likelihood of chronic, violent, and maturity gap before an established adulthood. Entry into
adult offending and the risk of incarceration. Though in- quality jobs and marriages may still produce such turning
carceration is a popular response to the crime problem, points, as Laub and Sampson (2003) have shown in their
jail time appears to be a large part of the problem itself, long-term study of delinquent youth and their divergent
owing to its role in expanding the disadvantaged popula- adult lives.
tion. Using both nationwide and local samples, Freeman In life-course theory, age at onset of antisocial be-
(cited by Sampson & Laub, 1996) reports that in all havior is most appropriately expressed as a continuous,
analyses “ having been in jail is the single most impor- time-dependent process. From research to date, one
tant deterrent to employment.” This finding held up even would expect causal factors during the early years to
with adjustments for individual differences that account feature neurological and social skill deficits as well as
for unemployment. impaired family processes and indications of extreme
An early onset trajectory is defined as a rule by a distress. In the later years, other factors, such as deviant
first arrest before the age of 14. Studies to date link peer activities, would come to the fore; but when they
early onset with an earlier onset of antisocial behavior become prominent may depend on the particular ecol-
(Patterson & Yoerger, 1996). Perhaps as early as age 6 ogy, whether the high-risk environment of the inner city
or 7, a breakdown in parenting processes increases coer- or the network closure of small rural communities. By
cive actions (e.g., talking back, explosive behavior, or employing this analytic model, empirical research and
hitting). The resulting interchange leads to fighting, not decisions based on potentially different criteria are
stealing, and truancy. Antisocial actions that are proto- likely to determine whether and when a break occurs
typic of delinquent acts, such as stealing from parents between types of causal influences.
and hitting them, increase the risk of delinquency Across the life course, the onset of antisocial behav-
through the medium of deviant friends. The later onset ior or its timing is generally viewed in a matrix of social
of deviant behavior includes more conventional youth relationships or linked lives. Indeed, change from a life
who are or might be seen as “ transitory delinquents.” of crime to a conventional lifestyle typically involves
They are more antisocial than uninvolved youth, but not change in a young person’s best friends, as prompted by
as antisocial as the early onset youth. Patterson and marriage or military service (Laub & Sampson, 2003).
Yoerger conclude that the most intriguing thing about
The Principle of Linked Lives: Lives Are
the late-onset boys is that they tend to be more deficient
Lived Interdependently and Social-Historical
in social skills than are the early onset boys. They re-
Influences Are Expressed through This Network
gard this as key to predicting which boys will persist in
of Shared Relationships
adult crime and which boys will not.
The profound life-course implications of early involve- The principles of timing and linked lives address in
ment in antisocial behavior continue to focus empirical complementary ways the temporality, process, and con-
work on the dual pathways hypothesis (Farrington & text of lives and human development. Interdependent
West, 1990; Moffitt, 1993; Moffitt, Caspi, Dickson, lives highlight the role of significant others in regulat-
Silva, & Stanton, 1996; Nagin, Farrington, & Moffitt, ing and shaping the timing of life trajectories through
1995). In the Dunedin longitudinal study, Moffitt (1993) a network of informal control. This network can be
696 The Life Course and Human Development

thought of as a “developmental context ” (Hartup & liefs, and they married men with similar beliefs, which
Laursen, 1991) and as a “convoy” of significant others sustained their own beliefs into the later years.
through life (Antonucci & Akiyama, 1995). Whatever Family changes are especially relevant to the princi-
the plans of an individual, these “significant others” ini- ple of linked lives and its implications. Hernandez
tiate or experience life transitions that produce transi- (1993) refers to a number of revolutionary family
tions in his or her own life. As Becker once observed changes in the lives of children and adults, including the
(1964), the expectations and informal sanctions of these decline in family size, migration off the land, the
“others” channel behavior and the life course in certain growth of women’s employment, divorce, and single
directions. parenting. Contemporary farm families in the Midwest-
Linked lives are expressed in Children of the Great ern region of the United States are characterized by
Depression (Elder, 1974/1999) across the generations, strong community and intergenerational ties, which are
in the parental marriage, and in the relationship of par- weaker for families of farm-reared parents in urban set-
ents and siblings. Older and younger siblings influence tings. A longitudinal study of families in the north cen-
each other directly through their encounters, whether tral region of Iowa documents this contrast and the
nurturant, competitive, or conflictual (Brody, 1996). greater social resources of young people with families
In an African American sample, Brody and associates who have ties to the land (Elder & Conger, 2000). These
(2003) found a significant link between the antisocial young people were among the most competent and re-
behavior of older and younger siblings, but it was sourceful adolescents in the study and their accomplish-
strongest in disadvantaged neighborhoods that pro- ments in school and social leadership had much to do
vided abundant opportunities for the younger sibling to with their social ties to family, church, and community.
express this behavior, when compared to siblings in af- They were also more engaged in joint activities with
fluent residential areas. Examples of an indirect path their parents and reported stronger relationships with
include the experience of parents with the eldest child grandparents and teachers. Their social responsibilities
that undermines or strengthens their sense of compe- fostered a sense of “mattering” to others, a feeling of
tence in parenting. A third potential sibling link in- significance because other people counted on them. In
volves the differential treatment of siblings by parents, this social world, linked lives regulated and empowered
relatives, or teachers. Little is known about continuity personal development, a social control that could also
and change in sibling relations from childhood into the take the form of barriers to residential change and ac-
adult years. cess to new opportunities in other places.
Marriage and the mutual regulatory influence of each The experiences and attributes of one generation,
partner illustrate both the process of timing through the such as parental work values, are passed on to the young
synchronization of lives and of the embeddedness of through relationships between the generations. For ex-
each family member’s life. For example, Caspi and Her- ample, drawing on the Youth Development Study, Ryu
bener (1990) investigated the influence of marital rela- and Mortimer (1996) found parental work experiences
tions on the developmental trajectories of husbands and and values to be correlated with the work values of chil-
wives. In “choosing situations that are compatible with dren. Mothers’ extrinsic work values (such as on money,
their dispositions and by affiliating with similar others, security) fostered similar values in the lives of their
individuals may set in motion processes of social inter- teenage and young adult daughters, and mothers with
change that sustain their dispositions across time and strong intrinsic values (including work autonomy and in-
circumstance” (p. 250). Among marriages with strong terest in job) were least likely to have daughters who
ties, they observed trajectories of parallel development valued extrinsic rewards such as high income and status.
over 20 years. Husbands and wives did not change to- For sons, the supportiveness of parents mattered more
ward greater resemblance in developmental trajectory, than parents’ actual work values and occupational expe-
but they did show a parallel course of development. rience. The more supportive the father and mother, the
When marriages dissolved, the former partners tended stronger the son’s intrinsic values. Intergenerational re-
to follow less parallel trajectories. A late-life follow-up lations are an important medium for the transmission of
of Bennington College graduates from the 1930s came to work values.
similar conclusions (Alwin et al., 1991). Women were Interdependent lives also extend beyond the family to
likely to select a college that matched their political be- friends, teachers, and neighbors. Theories of resilience
Life Transitions and Historical Change 697

commonly assume that positive influences can offset grandmother becomes a great-grandmother. Using data
negative influences (Luthar, 2003; Werner & Smith, on 41 female lineages from urban multigenerational
2001). A positive school environment of classmates and African American families in Los Angeles, Burton
teachers might compensate for a child’s punitive family (1985; Burton & Bengtson, 1985) has creatively explored
environment or a drug-infested neighborhood. Relevant the ripple effects of teenage pregnancy across the gener-
to these issues is a short-term longitudinal study of ado- ations. The age ranges of respondents in the early line-
lescents in Prince George’s County in the Washington, ages were 11 to 18 for the young mothers, 25 to 38 for the
DC, area (Cook, Herman, Phillips, & Settersten, 2002). grandmothers, and 46 to 57 for the great-grandmothers.
The influence of nuclear families, friendship groups, The other lineage units were judged on time in transi-
schools, and neighborhoods was assessed in the lives of tions. The age ranges for mothers, grandmothers, and
mainly African American and European-American stu- great-grandmothers were 21 to 26, 42 to 57, and 60 to
dents in the seventh and eighth grades during the early 73, respectively.
1990s. The quality of all four contexts had independent Role transitions that were “on time” were generally
and additive influences on adult success, defined by a welcomed. One 22-year-old mother commented that she
composite of school performance, social behavior, and had become a mother “ ‘at the right time’: I was ready,
mental health indicators. No reliable interaction effects my husband was ready, my mother was ready, my father
were observed among the four contexts. The effect of was ready, my grandmother couldn’t wait.” By compari-
any one context was not large, but the total contextual ef- son, early transitions multiplied social strains and depri-
fect proved to be substantial. vations in the family system, all reflecting the violation
Conventional approaches to the study of peers or of expectations. With few exceptions, the young mothers
friends, as linked lives, have viewed the relationship expected their own mothers to help care for their child.
only from the perspective of the child or adolescent. The However, this expectation never materialized in four-
perspective of the “other ” has seldom been assessed. fifths of the cases, in part because the mothers felt that
Studies have also ignored the developmental history of they were too young to become a grandmother. As one
friendship and peer experiences. Bearman and Brückner woman put it, “I can’t be a young momma and grand
(1999) address both of these deficiencies in their inves- momma at the same time. Something seems funny about
tigation of girl’s friendship and peer group as contribut- that, don’t you think?” A good many of the mothers re-
ing factors to sexual experiences in adolescence. Their sisted the grandparent identity because it conflicted
study, based on the National Longitudinal Study of Ado- with their availability as dating and sex partners.
lescent Health, provides evidence of the positive influ- The refusal of mothers to become grandmothers,
ence of peers at multiple levels. Both adolescent girls with their normative child care expectations, led most of
and their friends were classified as high risk or low risk the young mothers to shift these duties to their grand-
by school orientation and success as well as by evidence mothers, now the baby’s great-grandmother. Some of
of health-risk behavior such as drinking, skipping school, these women felt that the change made their life go by
and fighting. A girl’s own risk was less important for too fast. In the words of a woman beset by too many
her first sexual intercourse and pregnancy than the risk claims, “I ain’t got no time for myself. I takes care of ba-
of her male and female friends. Moreover, the age of a bies, grown children, and old people. I work too . . .”
girl’s friends tended to be more important than her own Some of these newly promoted great-grandmothers felt
age. Girls with older friends were more likely to engage they had to put their lives on hold until “ the older gener-
in sexual intercourse. Moreover, a girl’s circle of close ation died or the three younger generations grew up.”
friends and her wider peer network mattered more than The repercussions of this ill-timed pregnancy across the
her best friends. These effects were predominantly pro- generations underscore the price of interdependent lives
tective. Girls who had low-risk friends among their and the support they may provide.
close circle of friends or in their peer group were less
likely to have sex or experience a pregnancy. The Principle of Historical Time and Place:
A young girl’s pregnancy can have consequences that Individual Life Course Is Embedded in and Shaped
fundamentally change the lives of her mother and grand- by Historical Times and Places over a Lifetime
mother, among others. When a 13-year-old has a child, Children of the Great Depression (Elder, 1974/1999) is
her 28-year-old mother becomes a grandmother, and her based on children who were born and reared in a specific
698 The Life Course and Human Development

historical time and place, defined by culture, social in- relevant. This definition encouraged independence and
stitutions, and diversity of people—the 1920s, the San responsibility, separated recruits from the influence of
Francisco East Bay, California. The book also describes their home community and family, and allowed a degree
a very different Depression experience in Great Britain, of social autonomy in establishing new ties. Basic train-
Germany, and Japan. Even in the United States, condi- ing also promoted equality and comradeship among unit
tions during the Great Depression varied among cities, members, made prior identities irrelevant, required uni-
East and West, and between rural and urban places. In form dress and appearance, minimized privacy, and re-
view of this variation, the study’s generalizations are warded performance based on group achievement.
uncertain. Also uncertain are generalizations across A second distinctive feature involves “a clear-cut
historical time such as periods of economic depression break from the age-graded career,” a time-out in which
and recession. to sort out matters and make a new beginning. Military
One of the best examples of both historical and spa- duty legitimized a time-out from education, work, and
tial variations in the life course and human development family, and liberated the recruit from all conventional
comes from studies of lives during military times. The expectations for an age-graded career, such as expecta-
immediate years after World War II, for example, were tions regarding progress and life decisions.
hard times in many parts of Europe and Asia, unlike the Just being in the armed forces released the recruit
prosperity experienced in the United States. American from probing life-decision questions from parents (e.g.,
children who grew up in financially strained families Have you decided on a job or career? When will you be
during the Great Depression frequently saw military promoted or get married?). As Samuel Stouffer and his
service as a “ bridge to greater opportunity.” However, associates in the American Soldier study (1949, Vol. 2,
the age at which they entered the service made a differ- p. 572) noted, for many soldiers in World War II, “per-
ence in how it affected their lives. When appraised in haps for a majority, the break caused by Army service
terms of costs and benefits, military service for Ameri- [meant] a chance to evaluate where they had gotten and to
cans has favored the recruit who entered shortly after reconsider where they were going.” This time-out would
completing secondary school. This time of recruitment be far less timely for men and women who were mobi-
comes well before commitments to higher education, a lized in the midst of family and career responsibilities.
marriage partner, children, and a line of work. A third feature of mobilization offered a broadened
By contrast, later recruitment tends to disrupt all of range of developmental experiences and knowledge, in-
these activities. Empirical research (Clipp & Elder, cluding exposure to in-service skill training and educa-
1996; Elder, 1986, 1987; Elder, Shanahan, & Clipp, tional programs, as well as exposure to new interactional
1994; Sampson & Laub, 1993, 1996) has documented and cultural experiences through service itineraries that
the life-course advantages of early mobilization and the extended across the country and overseas. Out of such
disadvantages of relatively late entry, quite apart from experiences came a greater range of interpersonal con-
the mental health and mortality effects of wartime com- tacts, social models, and vocational skills. Horizons
bat. The disadvantages include family disruption, pro- were broadened and aspirations elevated. A veteran in-
longed father absence (Stolz, 1954), family discord, and terviewed just after World War II (Havighurst, Baugh-
divorce (Clipp & Elder, 1996), but these are not due to man, Burgess, & Eaton, 1951, p. 188) commented about
the mental and physical wounds of wartime combat. Ex- the remarkable diversity of his acquaintances in the ser-
posure to heavy combat markedly increased the likeli- vice and their influence on his views. As he put it, the
hood of emotional and behavioral problems after leaving experience “sort of opens up your horizons. . . . You
the service, but such problems were not concentrated start thinking in broader terms than you did before.”
among the late entrants. Before getting into the details Post-World War II veteran benefits, particularly the GI
of selected studies, we note some basic features of the Bill, gave significant support for these new aspirations.
transition to military service, in eras of World War II, The creation of new beginnings, a time-out or mora-
the Korean conflict, and the Vietnam War. torium to rethink and rework one’s future, and a broader
First, military service tended to pull young people range of skills, interpersonal contacts, and cultural ex-
from their past, however privileged or unsavory, and in periences do not exhaust important features of military
doing so it created new beginnings for developmental experience for new entrants, but in combination they de-
life changes. Basic training defined a recruit’s past as ir- fine a bridge to greater life opportunity and a potential
Life Transitions and Historical Change 699

turning point, especially for disadvantaged youth. As a fared well in their adult years, matching if not exceeding
total institution that presses from all angles, the mili- the occupational accomplishments of adults from more
tary is uniquely suited to recasting life trajectories. In- privileged backgrounds. However, the military experi-
deed, many years ago, Mattick (1960) found that young ence itself has remained largely a “ black box” and a
men paroled to the army had a much lower recidivism subject of informed speculation. What were the mecha-
rate than civil parolees. Features of basic training and nisms of developmental change? Sampson and Laub
the transition to military service have been adopted over (1996) provide some answers to this question in a com-
the years by social interventions, most especially by the pelling test of the early entry hypothesis. They use life-
Civil Conservation Corps in the 1930s. record data on a sample of approximately 1,000 men
Early entry into the military tends to minimize life who grew up in poverty areas of Boston ( birth years,
disruption and maximize such life-course benefits as 1925 to 1930), and more than 70% of the men served in
vocational education, skill training, and the GI Bill of the military. Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck (1968) origi-
support for veterans’ education and housing. In both the nally designed the longitudinal study of delinquency
Oakland Growth and Berkeley samples (1986, 1987), with a matched control sample—500 delinquents and
with birthdates in the 1920s, young men with disadvan- 500 controls.
tages of one kind or another were likely to join up as The delinquent sample of White males, ages 10 to 17,
soon as they could. Three types of disadvantage were es- was drawn from a population of youth who were com-
pecially consequential: (1) membership in an economi- mitted to one of two correctional schools in Massachu-
cally deprived family, (2) poor high school grades, and setts. The matched controls (on age, IQ, race-ethnicity,
(3) feelings of personal inadequacy during adolescence. and neighborhood deprivation) included 500 White
In combination, these factors predicted early entry into males from the Boston public schools, also ages 10 to 17.
military service and its pathway to personal growth and The two samples are treated in all analyses as indepen-
greater opportunity. Early entrants show greater life dent. From 1940 to 1965, the Gluecks collected a rich
benefits of the service up to the middle years than did body of life-history information on the study members.
later entrants. With a particular eye to experiences in the military,
Military service offered greater life development they assembled unparalleled details on the men’s ser-
among the early entrants in two ways. One route in- vice experience—their in-service training programs,
volved situational changes that made these recruits more special schools, exposure to the military justice system,
ambitious, assertive, and self-directed by midlife (Elder, and arrests. The life-record data on the sample of delin-
1986). The second route involved extensive use of gov- quents have also been coded by Sampson and Laub.
ernment educational and housing benefits through the Men in the delinquent and control samples typically
GI Bill. This important legislation, which has been entered the service at the age of 18 or 19 years, and most
likened to a Marshall Plan for America, was available to served over 2 years (more than 60% overseas). Consis-
recruits up through the age of 25. Early recruits in the tent with their history, men from the delinquent sample
Oakland and Berkeley cohorts were most likely to take were far more involved in antisocial conduct during their
advantage of these benefits for training and a college ed- service time than the controls (official misconduct,
ucation (Elder, 1986, 1987). number of arrests, dishonorable discharge), and they were
Though initially more disadvantaged, the early en- less likely to experience in-service training and benefits
trants at least matched the occupational standing of the from the GI Bill. Nevertheless, men from the delinquent
nonveterans at midlife, and the Berkeley males from de- sample were more likely to benefit from the service over
prived families showed greater developmental gains up to their life course, when compared to the controls, and
the age of 40. Using Q-sort ratings of personality in ado- this was especially true for men who entered the service
lescence and at midlife (Elder, 1986), the study found at an early age. In-service schooling, overseas duty, and
that the early entrants displayed greater change toward use of the GI Bill significantly enhanced job stability,
self-direction and confidence than the later entrants. The economic well-being, and occupational status, indepen-
mental health risks of combat exposure did not alter this dent of childhood differences and socioeconomic ori-
effect of life stage and timing in the two cohorts. gins. However, benefits of the GI Bill were larger for
Involvement in the military helped to account for why veterans with a delinquent past, especially when they
men from financially strained families in the 1930s have entered the military at an early age. The significant
700 The Life Course and Human Development

benefits of the GI Bill and overseas duty on socioeco- escape was conditional on historical time and place: Op-
nomic position were observed across the adult years up portunities and life itself were lost by countless youth in
to the age of 47. Europe and Asia.
In combination, these findings provide consistent
support for the life-course advantages of early entry into
Contributions of Life-Course Theory to the
World War II, and one study suggests that it applies as
Study of Human Development
well to the Korean War (Elder, 1986). However, ecologi-
cal context matters, as one might expect. The timing of In combination, these paradigmatic themes of life-
military service had very different effects in countries course theory identify its core features and potential
that lost World War II, specifically Japan and Germany. contributions to the study of human development. First,
West German males, born between 1915 and 1925 were this perspective places the field of study in a lifelong
drawn very heavily into military action (up to 97% of an framework. Human development and aging are lifelong
age cohort—Mayer, 1988, p. 234). These cohorts of vet- processes, expressed in continuity and change, and bio-
erans lost as many as 9 years of their occupational ca- logical, social, and psychological terms. The early years
reer in the war, suffered a high rate of imprisonment of child development have formative implications for
during and after the war, and experienced a mortality subsequent trajectories and healthy adaptations in later
rate of 25%. German children born around 1930 were life. In the course of aging, individuals change their en-
also hard hit by the war years, according to data from the vironment and social pathways by differentially inter-
German Life History Project (Mayer & Huinink, 1990, preting, selecting, and assigning meaning to situations
p. 220). The war disrupted their families and education and personal experiences. This process is expressed by
and they entered the labor market in a war-devastated the principle of human agency—life-course choices are
economy. Work placements were often poor, mixed with made in structured situations. Across the life course,
spells of joblessness, and advancement was unpredictable. pathways also shape the behavior of individuals through
Even the economic boom after the deprivational years of social demands and challenging options. Contexts and
recovery did not fully compensate this younger cohort for individuals thus become correlated. Transactional
its wartime losses in occupational achievement. A similar processes of this kind are established early in life and
story is told by these two birth cohorts (circa 1920 and contribute significantly to life-course continuity. Life
1930) in Japan (Elder & Meguro, 1987), except that the changes tend to occur when situational demands change
younger group was mobilized as students for work in the and pressures increase to alter one’s life course (e.g.,
fields and factories. A large number reported bomb-dam- marriage and military induction).
aged homes and a forced evacuation to the countryside. Few conceptual distinctions are more relevant to an
Today, military service occurs in a very different understanding of developmental change and the lives of
life course, marked by a later entry to adulthood for the children than the link between age and timing. Thus, the
college-oriented (Settersten et al., 2005). The nature of full negative impact of a lengthy dependence on welfare
this service has also changed in many places, from obli- for the educational progress of African American chil-
gation to voluntary. The aging of human societies has dren appears after the third grade (Guo, Brooks-Gunn,
extended the transition to young adulthood, from fam- & Harris, 1996). Cumulative dependence on welfare
ily origins in adolescence to the establishment of a markedly tends to increase the risk of grade retention
family in the early 30s or even later. In the United from the third to the ninth grades. Age and timing dis-
States, young people are entering adulthood at a time of tinctions also enable studies to relate children to the life
rising educational requirements and later family forma- course of significant people in their lives. Middle-aged
tion for the middle class especially. This contrasts with parents and their biographical experiences are an inte-
an accelerated timetable for the working class and fam- gral part of the adolescence of their children, and the ex-
ilies in poverty—their transition events tend to occur periences of youth figure prominently in the social
much earlier. world of their parents. In social meaning or function,
We have used military service to illustrate the role of parents remain parents for as long as they and their chil-
historical time and place in lives. Military service func- dren live. Likewise, the significant relationship of
tioned as a trajectory out of disadvantage in the lives of grandchildren and grandparents can have much to do
Depression youth who survived World War II, but this with the quality and pattern of their own lives. Lives
Integrating Biological Models with the Life Course: A Promising Frontier 701

and developmental trajectories are thus embedded in a insight of life-course theory. If behavioral development
moving system of intergenerational relationships. reflects ongoing exchanges between person and context,
As a theoretical orientation or framework, the life then its fully informed study necessarily involves longi-
course provides a conceptualization of the maturing in- tudinal views of both biological and social processes.
dividual’s changing environment and its developmental Thus, integration of the life course with biological mod-
relevance. This formulation locates children and their els of behavior represents an emerging and highly prom-
families in historical time and place. Cultural scripts ising area of study.
and social structure play an important role in organizing Such integration is facilitated by two considerations.
human lives as life courses, along with the actions of First, many topics of interest to life-course studies are
people and the internal forces of self-regulation. In the- linked in significant ways to biological processes.
ory, macrochanges influence developmental processes These topics include, for example, trajectories of physi-
by altering the individual life course through multilevel cal and mental health, the stress process, patterns of
processes and social structures. For children growing up aggression and deviance, sexual behavior, fertility, par-
in the Great Depression, hard times shaped their life enting, and manifold dimensions of aging and mortal-
course by changing their family’s experience and trajec- ity. Second, other topics are also likely to be associated
tory. Historical influences were filtered by their cohort with biological processes, albeit less conspicuously, in-
membership and social class in 1929 and by their actual cluding educational and occupational careers, patterns
exposure to severe income loss as expressed in family of close interpersonal relationships both within and be-
adaptations. Place also made a difference because the yond the family, and one’s involvement and status in
economic collapse hit the eastern half of the United organizations.
States before the western coast. Further, biological models of behavior have under-
gone nothing short of a paradigm shift in the past few
decades, moving from biological determinism to the
INTEGRATING BIOLOGICAL MODELS widespread assumption that “nature and nurture” inter-
WITH THE LIFE COURSE: A act in complex ways. This new view is consistent with
PROMISING FRONTIER propositions of systems theory (Lerner, Chapter 1, this
Handbook, this volume), several of which are especially
New developments in the study of behavior and biogra- relevant to a discussion of biology and the life course.
phy offer exciting possibilities for life-course studies. First, human behavior is the product of multiple levels of
Research on neighborhoods and communities (Morenoff, analysis, including, for example, levels characteristi-
2003; Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002), au- cally associated with sociology, psychology, biology,
tobiographical memory (e.g., Fivush & Haden, 2003), and anthropology. By extension, there is no a priori rea-
and subjective understandings of the self (Macmillan, son to believe that any one level will have special ex-
Hitlin, & Elder, in press) have become increasingly so- planatory value. For example, genes do not simply cause
phisticated. Substantial progress has also been made to- behavior (see Gottlieb et al., Chapter 5, this Handbook,
ward integrating the stress paradigm with life-course this volume) and, at the same time, behavior is not
principles (Elder, George, & Shanahan, 1996; Wheaton purely a result of social forces.
& Clarke, 2003). Advances such as these depend not only Second, all levels of analysis are characterized by
on novel data collection efforts but also on theory that plasticity, which refers to the range of possibilities
directs attention to behavioral patterns as they vary by (Lerner, 1984). Thus, each person’s behavior represents
place, time, and age. one set of possibilities from among a finite range of pos-
The study of biological processes and behavior illus- sibilities; similarly, every social order represents one
trates this need for integral models that encompass so- form of organization out of a range of possible social or-
cial settings and development. As explosive as interest ders. Third, although each level is likely to operate ac-
may be in biological studies of behavior, this work—en- cording to its own laws, the levels interact to produce
compassing evolutionary processes, metabolic processes behavior (Cairns, McGuire, & Gariepy, 1993). That is,
associated with the endocrine and immune systems, ge- systems theory assumes that many factors at multiple lev-
netics, and neuroscience—has not fully appreciated the els interact to form sets of “correlated constraints” that
importance of context and its dynamic features, the core include the behaviors of interest and their covariates.
702 The Life Course and Human Development

These behaviors and their covariates represent organized tal features or environmental features exert differential
systems, and ongoing reciprocal interactions among their control over genetic effects (Kendler & Eaves, 1986).
levels explain continuity and provide a map of opportuni- That is, genes may or may not be expressed depending
ties for change. on the context, or the effect of context may depend on
When viewed jointly, these principles define a central the genotype. Many students of human behavior, devel-
theme of genetic, metabolic, and evolutionary ap- opment, and aging believe that the study of GE interac-
proaches to behavior: Social and biological forces inter- tions will promote a better understanding of complex
act in complex and dynamic ways to define ranges of human behaviors (e.g., McClearn, Vogler, & Hofer,
likely behaviors. By itself, this theme acknowledges the 2001; McGue, 1999; Rowe, 2001; Rutter & Silberg,
importance of context and its interplay with biology. 2002; Sawa & Snyder, 2002; van Os & Marcelis, 1998;
Yet, a second overarching theme links biological models Wahlsten, 1999). Yet, few empirical examples of GE in-
of behavior to the life course more directly: Behavior re- teractions have been identified in the study of behav-
flects a lifetime of reciprocal exchanges between person ioral phenotypes in humans, and failures to find such
(including biological makeup) and context. This theme interactions have been noted (e.g., Heath et al., 2002;
acknowledges that behavior cannot be fully understood McGue & Bouchard, 1998, p. 12).
without reference to prior experience. To explore these Part of this discrepancy between the presumed com-
themes, we focus here on behavioral genetics and the monality of GE interactions and the infrequency of their
life course (for discussion of the life course and evolu- detection undoubtedly reflects methodological difficul-
tionary and endocrinological processes, see Shanahan, ties (e.g., power issues, levels of measurement, over-
Hofer, & Shanahan, 2003). reliance on cross-sectional designs), or over-simplified
Virtually all research on the life course has proceeded conceptualization and measurement of social context.
without considering the influence of genes on behavior, What is it about social context that would interact with
and, at the same time, behavioral genetics has proceeded the genotype to produce behavior? To date, four social
without regard to the sophisticated models of social con- processes have been identified (Shanahan & Hofer,
text that often characterize life-course research. Many 2005).
lines of research have now established, however, that First, social stressors may trigger a genetic diathesis,
genotypes do not produce behaviors in a simple way (see as is found, for example, in numerous studies of life-
Gottlieb et al., Chapter 5, this Handbook, this volume). events that trigger various forms of depression among
Rather, phenotypes are likely to reflect the cumulative people at high genetic risk for internalizing problems
history of the individual’s genotype (i.e., the genetic (e.g., Kendler & Kessler, 1995; Silberg & Rutter, 2001).
make-up of the organism), phenotype (i.e., any observ- Second, social context may compensate for a genetic
able feature of an organism, including its behavior), and diathesis, which means that the absence of notable stres-
context. Indeed, there is widespread appreciation among sors or the provision of an enriched environment may
behavioral geneticists that the links between genotypes prevent the expression of a genetic risk. For example,
and phenotypes are often heavily conditioned by social people at genetic risk for depression but not experiencing
location and personal experiences. What is insufficiently life-events typically do not exhibit depression. Studies of
appreciated is that the dynamic features of context often mice show that enriched settings can completely com-
determine its meaning for the person. Put differently, the pensate for genetic risks for cognitive tasks that are nec-
significance of social context for genetic expression will essary to negotiate a maze (Rampon, Tang, Goodhouse,
often depend on processes occurring in the life course as Shimuzu, & Tsien, 2000; Rampon & Tsien, 2000).
revealed by, for example, pathways, trajectories, transi- Third, social context may also prevent the expression
tions, turning points, and durations. One of the forefronts of a genetic risk through social control processes. Numer-
of behavioral genetics—gene-environment interactions— ous studies show that sources of social control—for ex-
provides a useful example of this principle. ample, religion, monitoring, anonymity, cultural values
and norms—lead to lowered levels of alcohol consump-
Mechanisms of Gene-Environment Interaction tion despite a genetic propensity or, conversely, high
levels of consumption in the absence of genetic in-
Gene-environment (GE) interactions occur when genes hibitors to drinking (e.g., Dick & Rose, 2001; Higuchi
alter the organism’s sensitivity to specific environmen- et al., 1994; Koopmans & Slutske, 1999). Finally, Bron-
Integrating Biological Models with the Life Course: A Promising Frontier 703

fenbrenner and Ceci’s (1994) bio-ecological model sug- the invidious nature of stressors is contingent on the
gests that proximal processes encourage the actualization prior, contemporaneous, and subsequent experiences of
of genetic potential: As proximal processes—enduring the person (Elder et al., 1996). In effect, life-events—
forms of social interactions characterized by progres- like all potential sources of stress, compensation, en-
sive complexity—improve, the genetic potential for hancement, and control—can only be understood in the
positive development is increasingly actualized. For ex- context of the life-course trajectories that embed them.
ample, Rowe and Jacobson (1999) showed that the heri- Indeed, while the vast majority of studies examine
tability of verbal intelligence is significantly greater the negative implications of life-events for well-being,
among high-education households than low. Their re- or the conditions in which such negative effects are at-
sults suggest that the genetic potential for verbal intelli- tenuated, life-events can actually have positive effects
gence is more fully realized in homes of better-educated on well-being, depending on prior circumstances. For
parents, which are assumed to provide enriched proxi- example, Wheaton (1990) shows that severe life-events
mal processes (e.g., Guo & Stearns, 2002). have a positive effect on psychological well-being if
they resolve an antecedent source of chronic distress.
Gene-Environment Interactions in the Life Course Thus, among adults who have lost a spouse, persons
with high levels of prior marital problems report signifi-
What is notable about all of these processes is that they cantly less distress than with low levels of prior marital
occur in the life course or are age-graded experiences problems. Similar patterns are observed for earlier di-
that form trajectories or pathways. How can the life vorce, premarital breakup, and a child moving out, with
course inform the study of genetic expression? First, qualified evidence for recent divorce, job loss, retire-
triggering, compensatory, social control, and proximal ment, and getting married. Wheaton concludes that role
processes are all mechanisms that occur over consider- histories often determine the meaning of a life-event
able periods, and thus must be studied with life-course (for other examples of the positive effects of life-events,
distinctions in mind. Second, the nature of these see also, Amato, Loomis, & Booth, 1995; Sweeney &
processes is multifaceted and will vary through the Horwitz, 2001).
phases of life: For example, the factors that constitute so- Furthermore, the experiences of early traumas (in-
cial control in childhood differ significantly from social cluding life-events) on later well-being are likely to be
control mechanisms in adolescence, which, in turn, dif- contingent on complex patterns of cumulative stressors.
fer from control in young adulthood. The importance of Turner and Lloyd (1995) report that the number of cu-
these themes—the dynamic and multidimensional nature mulative lifetime traumas significantly predicts onset of
of context—can be appreciated when considering exist- disorder (major depression or substance abuse) but not
ing approaches to context in behavioral genetic research. relapses of disorders. The number of traumas experi-
enced since the first onset, however, significantly pre-
Dynamic Patterns of Context
dicts relapses. Controlling recent life-events, the authors
The case of GE triggering interactions involving life- find that the number of post-onset traumas and chronic
events and depression illustrates the importance of con- stressors increases major depression and substance use,
ceptualizing and measuring context through time. As while the number of pre-onset traumas decreases the risk
noted, many studies show that life-events are significant of major depression. The authors conclude that the ef-
stressors that trigger depression in people with a ge- fects of life-events on distress will be significantly un-
netic susceptibility for that disorder. Such studies typi- derestimated if life-time patterns of both traumas and
cally ask people to indicate from a list of the life-events episodes of disorder are not taken into account (for the
that have occurred over a specified period. The total independent effects of earlier and later stressors, see
number of life-events experienced is then associated also, e.g., Ensel & Lin, 2000; Hayward & Gorman,
with depression. 2004; Poulton et al., 2002).
Yet, the magnitude of the relationship between life- With respect to experiences subsequent to the life-
events and indicators of distress like depression or de- event, research shows that the effects of life-events are
pressive symptoms is often modest (Turner, Wheaton, & often contingent on their implications for later life pat-
Lloyd, 1995), and the cumulative evidence shows that terns. For example, in their overview of research on
these modest associations are observed because, in part, childhood adversity and its effects on adult adjustment,
704 The Life Course and Human Development

McLeod and Almazan (2003) note that much of the ef- tions. Through adolescence, the importance of these
fect of parental loss (other than by parental separation) is sources may wane to some degree, and close interper-
mediated by subsequent experiences. Studies suggest sonal relationships and the workplace may increase in
that the provision of good child care, integration into and importance. Informal social control in young adulthood
achievements at school, good peer relations, and support- is often indicated by attachments to the labor force, mar-
ive, intimate relationships can all act to break the link be- riage, parenthood, military service, and religious and
tween parental loss in childhood and poor psychosocial civic involvements. Across the phases of life, these
outcomes in adulthood (e.g., Quinton & Rutter, 1988; sources of control tend to be positively interrelated, al-
Rutter, 1989). In adulthood, the effects of life-events are though, according to Sampson and Laub, youth experi-
often contingent on how the events are resolved. encing low informal control may have experienced an
While our review of the life-events literature is not increase in control during the transition to adulthood.
meant to be comprehensive, it underscores that the cross- Thus, social control refers to numerous, changing, and
sectional measurement of exposure to life-events repre- interrelated processes through the early life course. For
sents a crude proxy for increases in stress load that could example, it would be a mistake to suppose that measuring
actually trigger a genetic diathesis for psychosocial dis- parenting before age 5 could capture these complexities.
tress. Likewise, variables that represent other forms of
stressors—as well as potential sources of social compen- The Multidimensional Nature of Context
sation, control, and enhancement—are likely to acquire Empirical studies that demonstrate GE interactions al-
their meaning and impact only when viewed as part of a most invariably involve the interplay between an indica-
life-course trajectory. In an effort to enhance the accu- tor of genetic risk and a dimension of social experience.
racy and validity of their models, behavioral geneticists For example, depression is thought to be responsive to
are beginning to assess behavior in developmental terms. stressors, but research typically examines only one di-
A similarly dynamic orientation is necessary to capture mension of stressors (such as life-events). A focus on
the full significance of social context. one dimension of context is likely to underestimate the
In addition to the dynamic features of experiences, effect of contextual factors, which often operate as
the life course directs attention to the changing nature “correlated constraints,” or groups of variables that co-
of social processes through the phases of life. Social occur and work interactively. The multidimensionality
context is part of a “cascade of associations” (Johnston of context raises two possible sources of complexity.
& Edwards, 2002) or mediating mechanisms that makes First, high levels of interaction may characterize in-
certain behaviors more likely than others. As Rutter and dicators of triggering, compensation, control, and en-
Pickles (1991) observe, a GE interaction is not an expla- hancement. Highly stressful circumstances (capable of
nation but rather something to be explained. For exam- producing GE triggering interactions), constraining cir-
ple, Link and Phelan (1995) suggest that socioeconomic cumstances (capable of producing GE social control in-
status is a “ fundamental cause” of well-being, meaning teractions), and enhanced circumstances (capable of
that, as a rule, high socioeconomic status is associated producing GE enhancement or compensation interac-
with good health. Nevertheless, the mechanism by tions) are likely to reflect manifold aspects of context
which high socioeconomic status has these salutary ef- that exert their influence on the person as a set of
fects varies considerably depending on the time and variables, not individually. For example, Rutter (1990)
place. Socioeconomic status may promote manifold di- suggests that the presence of three or more risk factors
mensions of well-being and health through, for example, predicts maladjustment in an interactive fashion. Like-
preventative behaviors, monitoring and treatment, the wise, the developmental challenges posed by neigh-
amelioration of stressors, and/or the provision of stimu- borhoods marked by concentrated disadvantage are
lating, healthy environments. Perhaps all of these mech- numerous, intercorrelated, and likely to exert their neg-
anisms are at work, or perhaps their importance differs ative influences in nonadditive ways. Indeed, in their
at different points in life. overview of research on childhood adversities and their
Sampson and Laub’s research (1993) on informal so- implications for adulthood, McLeod and Almazan
cial control illustrates the multifaceted nature of this (2003) observe that:
mechanism. In childhood, informal control refers to a
complex range of parenting behaviors, peer relation- attempts to disaggregate the effects of clustered adversi-
ships, and connections to school and religious institu- ties may offer relatively little insight into processes of risk
Integrating Biological Models with the Life Course: A Promising Frontier 705

and resilience. The different clusters of events that chil- status, type of school, and percent of pledgers in the
dren experience have different meanings that are lost school—the controlling nature of the context can be
when those events are studied in isolation. (p. 401) more fully appreciated.
Second, the principles of equifinality and multifinal-
While behavioral geneticists have understandably been ity (which states the end phenotype may be the product
interested in gene-gene (GG) and gene-environment of multiple, distinct causal pathways and single causes
(GE) interactions, environmental factors may create may lead to multiple, distinct phenotypic expressions)
environment-environment (EE) interactions, whereby are relevant to links between social context and gene ex-
groups of contextual factors have nonadditive effects on pression. For example, Kendler, Gardner, and Prescott
behavior. Particularly, if only extreme settings will (2002) examined the interrelationships among 18 risk
moderate genetic expression (Scarr, 1992), then such en- factors over the life course (some of which were retro-
vironments are likely to involve EE interactions. Regard- spectively recalled) and depressive episodes. The best-
less of the presence of EE interactions, the high fitting, most parsimonious model included 64 paths
associations observed between contextual variables war- among risk factors and the occurrence of major depres-
rants caution when interpreting bivariate studies that in- sion, a simple but dramatic example of how multifaceted
terrelate one genotype and one contextual factor (e.g., mediational pathways can be. Distress is known to re-
Caspi & Sugden, 2003). This point is clear in a study of flect many different types of stressors, which can, in
sexual behavior among adolescents and their peers. turn, take on many different forms. Similarly, many
The case of virginity pledges among adolescents il- specific forms of social control that inhibit antisocial
lustrates the highly interactive nature of a form of social behavior have been identified, including warm and nur-
control in delaying age of first sexual intercourse. Draw- turant parenting, positive connections with schools and
ing on data from Add Health, Bearman and Brückner nonrelated adults, intimate interpersonal relations, mar-
(2001) observe that the risk of sexual initiation is 34% riage, parenthood, and meaningful ties to the labor mar-
lower among youth who took a virginity pledge than ket (e.g., Sampson & Laub, 1993). These considerations
among nonpledgers. The effect of pledging is contin- suggest that causal pathways involving social context and
gent, however, on other contextual features that, taken biological substrates may involve complex combinations
as a whole, establish controlling circumstances. The of different factors that lead to the same outcome.
others found the pledging effect to be stronger in early
and middle adolescence, but it did not prevent sexual
Future Directions for the Life Course and
intercourse until marriage. In addition, the effect of
Behavioral Genetics
pledging varied according to the type of school that
the student attends, and the percent of pledgers in the Taken together, these themes heighten sensitivity to the
school. In “socially open schools”—where many of the dynamic, highly contingent, and multidimensional na-
students report friendships and romantic relationships ture of social processes that are likely to be integral to
with students from other schools—pledging has no ef- genetic expression. The study of such themes would
fect if no other pledgers are present. In such schools, for transform how research typically proceeds in behavioral
every 1% additional same-sex pledgers, the rate of the genetics (Coll, Bearer, & Lerner, 2004). Behavioral ge-
transition to first intercourse is delayed by 2%. In so- netic research typically views social context in unidi-
cially closed schools—where friendships and romantic mensional, static terms, although life-course research
relationships are contained in the school—the opposite shows that formative social processes exhibit patterns of
is observed: with no other pledgers present, pledgers are change and constancy across the phases of life, and
much less likely to experience their sexual debut. When these dynamic patterns determine the salience of con-
other pledgers are present, the pledgers’ transition rate text for behavior. Integrating life-course models of so-
is higher than that of pledgers in schools with few cial processes with behavioral genetic studies thus
pledgers. For adolescents in schools with more than 30% presents exciting new opportunities for understanding
pledgers, a threshold is reached whereby pledging has no behavioral development in increasingly precise terms.
effect. That is, by itself, pledging status tells little of the Ideally, such integrative efforts will capitalize on a
story of how sexual behavior is controlled. When this long-standing interest of life-course research or the
form of social control is viewed as a constellation of comparison of people across differing social contexts.
variables that create EE interactions—encompassing Such an interest may involve the study of social change,
706 The Life Course and Human Development

which involves the transformation of context in people’s Each challenge was posed by the early longitudinal
lives, migration, or cross-national patterns. All of these studies, as they continued well into the adult and late
types of studies provide an advantage in the study of de- life years, and the mounting realization that much of
velopment by creating notable variability in context and the story of child development is written across the
behavior. The utility of these approaches is suggested by adult years. In addition, the challenges had much to do
behavioral genetic studies of alcoholism. For example, with the demographic and political pressures of an
Higuchi et al. (1994) show that while the suppressive ef- aging society.
fect of the ALDH2*2 genotype (i.e., homozygous for the Social theories of relationships and age converged in
null allele) inhibits alcoholism among the Japanese, the the 1960s with emerging concepts of life-span develop-
suppressive effect of the heterozygous genotype (i.e., ment to produce a theoretical orientation to the life
one null and one normal allele) has waned with succes- course. More than any other theoretical initiative, life-
sive cohorts. The authors speculate that social controls span developmental psychology has responded to the
on drunkenness have loosened in Japanese society first challenge by advancing a conceptual orientation
through the twentieth century. on human development and personality across the life
The functional polymorphism ADH2*2 also protects span. One result is a concept of ontogenetic develop-
against alcoholism, but its effects may be contingent on ment in which social structures and cultures merely es-
context. The fact that Jews drink less than other Cau- tablish behavioral settings. By contrast, life-course
casians is thought to reflect the fact that ADH2*2 is theory views human development as a coactive process
more prevalent in the former group. Among Jews, how- in which sociocultural, biological, and psychological
ever, the inhibitory effect of ADH2*2 may be contingent forces interact over time. Social structures and cultures
on environmental factors. Although drawing on a small are constituent elements in the developmental process.
sample, Hasin et al. (2002) report that the effect of People play an important role in shaping their life
ADH2*2 in suppressing alcohol consumption was less course and development, although choices and initia-
among Russian Jews who had been exposed to an envi- tives are always constrained by social forces and bio-
ronment of heavy drinking prior to immigration than logical limitations.
among Israeli Ashkenazi and Sephardic Jews who had In concept, and as discussed in this chapter, the indi-
not been exposed to such an environment. This research vidual life course provides a response to the second
illustrates how social change and the life course can be challenge, a way of thinking about life patterns or orga-
strategic in the study of genetic expression because they nization. Lives over time do not merely follow a se-
create “experiments of nature” that dramatically alter quence of situations or person-situation interactions.
the contextual forces thought to alter genetic expression. Instead, the life course is conceived as an age-graded
sequence of socially defined roles and events that are
enacted and even recast over time. It consists of multi-
CONCLUSIONS ple, interlocking trajectories, such as work and family,
with their transitions or changes in states. People gener-
The emergence of life-course theory and its elabora- ally work out their life course in relation to established,
tion over the past 30 years can be viewed through institutionalized pathways and their regulatory con-
prominent challenges to developmental studies that straints such as the curricula or tracks of a school, the
questioned traditional forms of thought and empirical age-graded expectations of a family, and the work ca-
work. They include: reers of a firm or culture.
The individual life course, developmental trajecto-
1. The necessity for concepts of development and per- ries and transitions (as psychobiological continuities
sonality that have relevance beyond childhood and and change), and established pathways are important el-
even adolescence ements in the life-course study of human development.
2. The need for a way of thinking about the social pat- Any change in the life course of individuals has conse-
terning and dynamic of lives over time, as they relate quences for their developmental trajectory, and histori-
to developmental processes cal change may alter both by recasting established
3. The increasing recognition that lives and develop- pathways. Thus, adultlike expectations for productive
mental trajectories may be transformed by a chang- work in World War II communities were lowered to-
ing society ward childhood to enable young people to fill needed
References 707

roles. By placing people in historical locations, life- Aseltine, R. H., Gore, S., & Gordon, J. (2000). Life stress, anger
course theory has oriented research to the third chal- and anxiety, and delinquency: An empirical test of general
strain theory. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41(3),
lenge, to understand the process by which societal 256–275.
changes make a difference in the primary world and de- Baldwin, J. M. (1895). Mental Development in the child and the race:
velopment of children. Methods and processes. New York: Macmillan.
This chapter on life-course theory represents the be- Baltes, P. B. (1979). Life-span developmental psychology: Some con-
ginning stage of a long journey toward understanding verging observations on history and theory. In P. B. Baltes &
O. G. Brim Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 2,
human development in ways that extend across individ- pp. 256–281). New York: Academic Press.
ual lives, the generations, and historical time. Just as the Baltes, P. B. (1993). The aging mind: Potential and limits. Gerontolo-
major themes of developmental psychology from a cen- gist, 33, 580–594.
tury ago seem to be regaining prominence in contempo- Baltes, P. B. (1994, August). Life-span developmental psychology: On
rary studies (Parke et al., 1994), including a renewed the overall landscape of human development. Invited address at
the 102nd Annual Convention of the American Psychological As-
interest in genetic influences, emotional regulation, and sociation (Division 7), Los Angeles.
the study of hormones, life-course theory can be viewed Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontoge-
through renewed priorities (e.g., social context and nesis: Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundations
change, life histories) that were once dominant in the of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–381.
past, particularly in the early Chicago School of Sociol- Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Successful aging: Perspec-
tives from the behavioral sciences. New York: Cambridge Uni-
ogy. These observed continuities, however, pale in rela- versity Press.
tion to the novel integrations and new directions of Baltes, P. B., Cornelius, S. W., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1979). Cohort
contemporary theory. Building on a wider net of cross- effects in developmental psychology. In J. R. Nesselroade & P. B.
disciplinary scholarship in developmental science, dis- Baltes (Eds.), Longitudinal research in the Study of Behavior and
Development (pp. 61–87). New York: Academic Press.
tinctions of time, context, and process have become
Bane, M. J., & Ellwood, D. T. (1986). Slipping into and out of
central to a life-course theory of child, adolescent, and poverty: The dynamics of spells. Journal of Human Resources,
adult development. The integration of biological models 21(1), 1–23.
with the life course represents a promising interdiscipli- Barrett, A. E. (2000, December). Marital trajectories and mental
nary frontier for this field of study. health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 451–464.
Bartley, M., & Plewis, I. (2002). Accumulated labour market disad-
vantage and limiting long-term illness: Data from the 1971–1991
REFERENCES Office for National Statistics’ Longitudinal Study. International
Journal of Epidemiology, 31, 336–341.
Abbott, A. (1997). Of time and space: The contemporary relevance Bauer, D., & Curran, P. J. (2003). Distributional assumptions of
of the Chicago school. Social Forces, 75(4), 1149–1182. growth mixture models: Implications for over-extraction of latent
trajectory classes. Psychological Methods, 8, 338–363.
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Dauber, S. L. (1994). On the suc-
cess of failure: A reassessment of the ef fects of retention in the pri- Bauer, D., & Curran, P. J. (2004). The integration of continuous and
mary grades. New York: Cambridge University Press. discrete latent variable models: Potential problems and promising
opportunities. Psychological Methods, 9, 3–29.
Alwin, D. F., Cohen, R. L., & Newcomb, T. M. (1991). Political atti-
tudes over the life-span: The Bennington women after fifty years. Bearman, P., & Brückner, H. (1999). Power in numbers: Peer ef fects
Madison: University of Wisconsin Press. on adolescent girls’ sexual debut and pregnancy. Washington, DC:
National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy.
Amato, P. R. (2000). Consequences of divorce for adults and chil-
dren. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1269–1287. Bearman, P., & Brückner, H. (2001). Promising the future: Virginity
pledges and first intercourse. American Journal of Sociology,
Amato, P. R., & Cheadle, J. (2005). The long reach of divorce: Di-
106(4), 859–912.
vorce and child well-being across three generations. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 67, 191–206. Becker, H. S. (1961). Notes on the concept of commitment. American
Journal of Sociology, 66(1), 32–40.
Amato, P. R., Loomis, S. L., & Booth, A. (1995). Parental divorce,
marital conf lict, and off-spring well-being during early adult- Becker, H. S. (1964). Personal change in adult life. Sociometry,
hood. Social Forces, 73, 895–915. 27(1), 40–53.
Amick, B. C., McDonough, P., Chang, H., Rogers, W. H., Pieper, Bengtson, V. L., & Laufer, R. S. (1974). Youth, generations, and so-
C. F., & Duncan, G. (2002). Relationship between all-cause mor- cial change. Journal of Social Issues, 30(2/3, Pts. I & II ).
tality and cumulative working life course psychosocial and physi- Bergman, L. R., Magnusson, D., & El-Khouri, B. M. (2003). Studying
cal exposures in the United States labor market from 1968 to individual development in an interindividual context: A person-
1992. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 370–381. oriented approach (Vol. 4). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (1995). Convoys of social relations: Blair-Loy, M. (2003). Competing devotions: Career and family among
Family and friendships within a life-span context. In R. Blieszner women executives. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
& V. H. Bedford (Eds.), Handbook of Aging and the Family Block, J. (with N. Haan). (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley,
(pp. 355–371). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. CA: Bancroft.
708 The Life Course and Human Development

Booth, A., Johnson, D. R., White, L. K., & Edwards, J. N. (1986). Di- Caspi, A. (2004). Life-course development: The interplay of social
vorce and marital instability over the life course. Journal of Fam- selection and social causation within and across the generations.
ily Issues, 7(4), 421–442. In P. L. Chase-Lansdale, K. Kiernan, & R. J. Friedman (Eds.),
Bouffard, L. A., & Laub, J. H. (2004). Jail or the army: Does military Human development across lives and generations: The potential
service facilitate desistance from crime. In R. Immarigeon & S. for change (pp. 8–27). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Maruna (Eds.), After crime and punishment: Pathways to of fender Caspi, A., & Bem, D. J. (1990). Personality continuity and change
reintegration (pp. 129–151). Devon, England: Willan. across the life course. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of person-
Bound, J., Jaeger, D. A., & Baker, R. M. (1995). Problems with instru- ality: Theory and research (pp. 549–575). New York: Guilford
mental variables estimation when the correlation between the in- Press.
struments and the endogenous explanatory variable is weak. Caspi, A., Bem, D. J., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1989). Continuities and
Journal of the American Statistical Association, 90(430), 443–450. consequences of interactional styles across the life course. Jour-
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1982). Psychological reactance: A the- nal of Personality, 57(2), 375–406.
ory of freedom and control. New York: Academic Press. Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Bem, D. J. (1987). Moving against the
Brody, G. (Ed.). (1996). Sibling relationships: Their causes and con- world: Life course patterns of explosive children. Developmental
sequences (Advances in Applied Developmental Psychology Se- Psychology, 23(2), 308–313.
ries). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Caspi, A., & Herbener, E. (1990). Continuity and change: Assortative
Brody, G. H., Ge, X., Kim, S. Y., Murry, V. M., Simons, R. L., marriage and the consistency of personality in adulthood. Journal
Gibbons, F. X., et al. (2003). Neighborhood disadvantage mod- of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 250–258.
erates associations of parenting and older sibling problem atti- Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Thornton, A., Freedman, D., Amell, J. W.,
tudes and behavior with conduct disorders in African American Harrington, H., et al. (1996). The life history calendar: A re-
children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, search and clinical assessment method for collecting retrospec-
211–222. tive event-history data. International Journal of Methods in
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1970). Two worlds of childhood: US and USSR. Psychiatric Research, 6, 101–114.
New York: Russell Sage. Caspi, A., & Sugden, K. (2003). Inf luence of life stress on depres-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Ex- sion: Moderation in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, 386–389.
periments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- Cherlin, A. J. (1993). Marriage, divorce, and remarriage. Cambridge,
versity Press. MA: Harvard University Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta Clausen, J. A. (1968). Socialization and society. Boston: Little, Brown.
(Ed.), Six theories of child development: Revised formulations and
Clausen, J. A. (1993). American lives: Looking back at the children of
current issues (pp. 185–246). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
the great depression. New York: Free Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1996). Foreword. In R. B. Cairns, G. H. Elder
Clausen, J. A. (1995). Gender, contexts and turning points in adults’
Jr., & E. J. Costello (Eds.), Developmental science (pp. ix-xvii).
lives. In P. Moen, G. H. Elder, Jr., & K. Lüscher (Eds.), Examin-
New York: Cambridge University Press.
ing Lives in Context: Perspectives on the geology of human devel-
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptu- opment (pp. 365–389). Washington, DC: American Psychological
alized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psy- Association.
chological Review, 101(4), 568–586.
Clipp, E. C., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1996). The aging veteran of
Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on World War II: Psychiatric and life course insights. In P. E.
children. Future of Children, 7(2), 55–71. Ruskin & J. A. Talbott (Eds.), Aging and post-traumatic stress
Buchmann, M. (1989). The script of life in modern society: Entry disorder (pp. 19–51). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
into adulthood in a changing world. Chicago: University of Press.
Chicago Press. Coll, C. G., Bearer, E. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (2004). Nature and
Bumpass, L., & Lu, H. H. (2000). Trends in cohabitation and implica- nurture: The complex interplay of genetic and environmental
tions for children’s family contexts in the United States. Popula- inf luences on human behavior and development. Mahwah, NJ:
tion Studies: A Journal of Demography, 54(1), 29–41. Erlbaum.
Burton, L. M. (1985). Early and on-time grandmotherhood in multi- Collins, L. M., & Sayer, A. G. (2001). New methods for the analysis of
generational Black families. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, change: Decade of behavior. Washington, DC: American Psycho-
University of Southern California. logical Association.
Burton, L. M., & Bengtson, V. L. (1985). Black grandmothers: Issues Cook, T. D., Herman, M., Phillips, M., & Settersten, R. A., Jr.
of timing and continuity of roles. In V. L. Bengtson & J. F. Robert- (2002). Some ways in which neighborhoods, nuclear families,
son (Eds.), Grandparenthood (pp. 61–77). Beverly Hills: Sage. friendship groups and schools jointly affect changes in early ado-
Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of youth lescent development. Child Development, 73, 1283–1309.
in our time. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Crosnoe, R., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2004). From childhood to the later
Cairns, R. B., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Costello, E. J. (1996). Developmen- years: Pathways of human development. Research on Aging,
tal science: Cambridge Studies in Social and Emotional Develop- 26(6), 623–654.
ment. New York: Cambridge University Press. Crouter, A. C., & Booth, A. (2003). Children’s inf luence on family
Cairns, R. B., McGuire, A. M., & Gariepy, J.-L. (1993). Developmen- dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships. Mahwah,
tal behavior genetics: Fusion, correlated constraints, and timing. NJ: Erlbaum.
In D. F. Hay & A. Angold (Eds.), Precursors and causes in devel- Crouter, A. C., Maguire, M. C., Helms-Erikson, H., & McHale, S. N.
opment and psychopathology (pp. 87–122). New York: Wiley. (1999). Parental work in middle childhood: Links between em-
References 709

ployment and the division of housework, parent-child activities, Elder, G. H., Jr. (1998a). The life course and human development. In
and parental monitoring. In T. L. Parcel (Ed.), Work and family: W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Hand-
Research in the sociology of work series (pp. 31–54). Greenwich, book of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human de-
CT: JAI Press. velopment (5th ed., pp. 939–991). New York: Wiley.
Dannefer, D. (1984). The role of the social in life-span developmental Elder, G. H., Jr. (1998b). The life course as developmental theory.
psychology, past and future: Rejoinder to Baltes and Nesselroade. Child Development, 69(1), 1–12.
American Sociological Review, 49(6), 847–850. Elder, G. H., Jr. (1999). Children of the great depression: Social
Dick, D. M., & Rose, R. J. (2001). Exploring gene-environment inter- change in life experience. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
(Original work published 1974)
actions: Socioregional moderation of alcohol use. Journal of Ab-
normal Psychology, 110(4), 625–632. Elder, G. H., Jr., & Caspi, A. (1988). Human development and social
change: An emerging perspective on the life course. In N. Bolger,
Dorn, L. D., Susman, E. J., & Ponirakis, A. (2003). Pubertal timing
A. Caspi, G. Downey, & M. Moorehouse (Eds.), Persons in con-
and adolescent adjustment and behavior: Conclusions vary by
text: Developmental processes (pp. 77–113). New York: Cam-
rater. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32(3), 157–167. bridge University Press.
Drobnic, S., Blossfeld, H.-P., & Rohwer, G. (1999, February). Dy- Elder, G. H., Jr., & Conger, R. D. (2000). Children of the land: Ad-
namics of women’s employment patterns over the family life versity and success in rural America. In Studies on Successful
course: A comparison of the United States and Germany. Journal Adolescent Development (John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur
of Marriage and the Family, 61, 133–146. Foundation Series on Mental Health and Development). Chicago:
Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic University of Chicago Press.
deprivation and early-childhood development. Child Development, Elder, G. H., Jr., George, L. K., & Shanahan, M. J. (1996). Psychoso-
65(2), 296–318. cial stress over the life course. In H. B. Kaplan (Ed.), Psychoso-
Duncan, G. J., Clark-Kauffman, E., & Snell, E. (in press). Resi- cial stress: Perspectives on structure, theory, life course, and
dential morbidity interventions as treatment for the sequelae of methods (pp. 247–292). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
neighborhood violence [Mimeo]. Evanston, IL: Northwestern Elder, G. H., Jr., Johnson, M. K., & Crosnoe, R. (2003). The emer-
University, Institute for Poverty Research. gence and development of the life course. In J. T. Mortimer &
M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 3–19).
Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C., Strycker, L. A., Li, F., & Anthony, A.
New York: Plenum Press.
(1999). An introduction to latent variable growth curve modeling:
Concepts, issues, and applications—Quantitative methodology se- Elder, G. H., Jr., & Meguro, Y. (1987). Wartime in men’s lives: A
Comparative Study of American and Japanese Cohorts. Interna-
ries. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 439–466.
Easterlin, R. A. (1980). Birth and fortune: The impact of numbers on
Elder, G. H., Jr., Modell, J., & Parke, R. D. (1993). Children in time
personal welfare. New York: Basic Books.
and place: Developmental and historical insights—Cambridge
Ebaugh, H. R. F. (1988). Becoming an ex: The process of role exit. Studies in Social and Emotional Development. New York: Cam-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. bridge University Press.
Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage-environment fit: Develop- Elder, G. H., Jr., Pavalko, E. K., & Hastings, T. J. (1991). Talent, history,
mentally appropriate classrooms for early adolescents. In R. E. and the fulfillment of promise. Psychiatry, 54, 251–267.
Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education Elder, G. H., Jr., & Pellerin, L. A. (April 1995). Social development
(Vol. 3, pp. 139–186). New York: Academic Press. research in historical perspective, 1965–89: Is it more longitudinal
Eggleston, E. P., Laub, J. H., & Sampson, R. J. (2004). Methodologi- and contextual today? Poster session presented at the biennial
cal sensitivities to latent class analysis of long-term criminal tra- meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development.
jectories. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 20(1), 1–26. Elder, G. H., Jr., Shanahan, M. J., & Clipp, E. C. (1994). When war
Eichorn, D. H., Clausen, J. A., Haan, N., Honzik, M. P., & Mussen, comes to men’s lives: Life course patterns in family, work, and
health [Special issue]. Psychology and Aging, 9(1), 5–16.
P. H. (1981). Present and past in middle life. New York: Aca-
demic Press. Ensel, M., & Lin, N. (2000). Age, the stress process, and physical
distress: The role of distal stressors. Journal of Aging and Health,
Eisenstadt, S. N. (1956). From generation to generation: Age groups
12, 139–168.
and social structure. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., & Olson, L. S. (2003). The first-
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1975). Age differentiation and the life course. An-
grade transition in life course perspective. In J. T. Mortimer &
nual Review of Sociology, 1, 165–190.
M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 229–250).
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1978). Family history and the life course. In T. K. Har- New York: Plenum Press.
aven (Ed.), Transitions (pp. 17–64). New York: Academic Press.
Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L., & Olson, L. S. (2005). First
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1985). Perspectives on the life course. In G. H. grade and educational attainment by age 22. American Journal
Elder Jr. (Ed.), Life course dynamics: Trajectories and transitions, of Sociology, 110, 1458–1502.
1968–1980(pp. 23–49). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1986). Military times and turning points in men’s Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New
lives. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 233–245. York: Norton.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1987). War mobilization and the life course: A co- Farrington, D. P., Loeber, R., Elliott, D. S., Hawkins, J. D., Kandel,
hort of World War II veterans. Sociological Forum, 2(3), 449–472. D. B., Klein, M. W., et al. (1990). Advancing knowledge about
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1991). Family transitions, cycles, and social change. the onset of delinquency and crime. In B. B. Lahey & A. E.
In P. A. Cowan and M. Hetherington (Eds.), Family Transitions Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 13,
(pp. 31–57). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 283–342). New York: Plenum Press.
710 The Life Course and Human Development

Farrington, D. P., & West, D. J. (1990). The Cambridge Study of Guo, G., & Stearns, E. (2002). The social inf luences on the realiza-
Delinquent Development: A long-term follow-up of 411 Lon- tion of genetic potential for intellectual development. Social
don males. In H. J. Kerner & G. Kaiser (Eds.), Kriminalitat: Forces, 80, 881–910.
Personlichkeit, Lebensgeschichte und Verhalten [Criminology: Per- Hagan, J. (2001). Northern passage: American Vietnam War resisters
sonality, Behavior and Life History] (pp. 115–138). New York: in Canada. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Springer-Verlag.
Haidt, J., & Rodin, J. (1999). Control and efficacy as interdiscipli-
Ferri, E., Bynner, J., & Wadsworth, M. (2002). Changing Britain, nary bridges. Review of General Psychology, 3(4), 317–337.
changing lives: Three generations at the turn of the century
Halfon, N., & Hochstein, M. (2002). Life course health development:
(Bedford Way Papers Series, No. 18). London: Institute of
An integrated framework for developing health, policy, and re-
Education.
search. Milbank Quarterly, 80(3), 433–479.
Fivush, R., & Haden, C. A. (Eds,) (2003). Autobiographical memory
Hareven, T. K. (1978). Transitions: The family and the life course in
and the construction of the narrated self: Developmental and cul-
historical perspective. New York: Academic Press.
tural perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hareven, T. K. (1982). Family time and industrial time. New York:
Ford, D. H., & Lerner, R. M. (1992). Developmental systems theory:
Cambridge University Press.
An integrative approach. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Forest, K. B., Moen, P., & Dempster-McClain, D. (1995). Cohort dif- Hartup, W. W., & Laursen, B. (1991). Relationships as developmental
ferences in the transition to motherhood: The variable effects of contexts. In R. Cohen & A. Siegel (Eds.), Context and develop-
education and employment before marriage. Sociological Quar- ment (pp. 253–279). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
terly, 36(2), 315–336. Hasin, D., Aharonovich, E., Liu, X., Mamman, Z., Matseoane, K.,
Freedman, D., Thornton, A., Camburn, D., Alwin, D., & Young- Carr, L., et al. (2002). Alcohol and ADH2 in Israel: Ashkenazis,
DeMarco, L. (1988). The life history calendar: A technique for Sephardic, and recent Russian immigrants. American Journal of
collecting retrospective data. Sociological Methodology, 18, 37–68. Psychiatry, 159, 1432–1434.
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Morgan, S. P. (1987). Havighurst, R. J. (1949). Developmental tasks and education.
Adolescent mothers in later life. New York: Cambridge Univer- Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
sity Press. Havighurst, R. J., Baughman, J. W., Burgess, E. W., & Eaton,
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Cook, T. D., Eccles, J., Elder, G. H., Jr., & W. H. (1951). The American veteran back home. New York:
Sameroff, A. (1999). Managing to make it: Urban families and Longmans, Green.
adolescent success. In Studies on Successful Adolescent Develop- Hayward, M. D., & Gorman, B. K. (2004). The long arm of child-
ment (John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Series on hood: The inf luence of early-life social conditions on men’s mor-
Mental Health and Development). Chicago: University of Chicago tality. Demography, 41(1), 87–107.
Press. Heath, A. C., Todorov, A. A., Nelson, E. C., Madden, P. A. F., Bu-
Fussell, E. (2002). Youth in aging societies. In J. T. Mortimer & cholz, K. K., & Martin, N. G. (2002). Gene-environment inter-
R. W. Larsen (Eds.), The changing adolescent experience: Societal action effects on behavioral variation and risk of complex
trends and the transition to adulthood (pp. 18–51). New York: disorders: The example of alcoholism and other psychiatric disor-
Cambridge University Press. ders. Twin Research, 5, 30–37.
Fussell, E., & Furstenberg, F. F., Jr. (2005). The transition to adult- Heckhausen, J. (1999). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Age-
hood during the twentieth century: Race, nativity, and gender. In normative and sociostructural constraints as adaptive challenges.
R. A. Settersten, Jr., F. F. Furstenberg, Jr., & R. G. Rumbaut New York: Cambridge University Press.
(Eds.), On the Frontier of Adulthood: Theory, Research, and Pub- Hernandez, D. J. (1993). America’s children: Resources from family,
lic Policy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. government, and the economy—The Population of the United
Ge, X., Lorenz, F. O., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Simons, States in the 1980s. New York: Russell Sage.
R. L. (1994). Trajectories of stressful life events and depressive Hertzman, C., & Power, C. (2003). Health and human development:
symptoms during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, Understandings from life-course research. Developmental Neu-
30(4), 467–483. ropsychology, 24(2/3), 719–744.
George, L. K. (2003). Life course research: Achievements and poten-
Hetherington, E. M. (Ed.). (1999). Coping with divorce, single parent-
tial. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the
ing, and remarriage: A risk and resiliency perspective. Mahwah,
life course (pp. 671–680). New York: Plenum Press.
NJ: Erlbaum.
Gimbel, C., & Booth, A. (1996). Who fought in Vietnam? Social
Higuchi, S., Matsushita, S., Imazeki, H., Kinoshita, T., Takagi, S., &
Forces, 74(4), 1137–1157.
Kono, H. (1994). Aldehyde dehydrogenase genotypes in Japanese
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1971). Status passage. Chicago: Aldine. alcoholics. Lancet, 343, 741–742.
Glueck, S., & Glueck, E. (1968). Delinquents and nondelinquents in Hill, R. (1970). Family development in three generations. Cambridge,
perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. MA: Schenkman Books.
Goldthorpe, J. H. (1991). The uses of history in sociology: Ref lec- Hitlin, S., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (in press). Agency: An empirical model
tions on some recent tendencies. British Journal of Sociology, of an abstract concept. In R. MacMillan (Ed.), Advances in life
42(2), 211–230. course research. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Goode, W. J. (1960). The theory of role strain, American Sociological Hofer, S. M., & Sliwinski, M. J. (2002). Understanding ageing: An
Review, 25(4), 483–496. evaluation of research designs for assessing the interdependence
Guo, G., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Harris, K. M. (1996). Parental labor- of ageing-related changes. Gerontology, 47, 341–352.
force attachment and grade retention among urban Black chil- Hofferth, S. L. (1987). Teenage pregnancy and its resolution. In S. L.
dren. Sociology of Education, 69, 217–236. Hofferth & C. Hayes (Eds.), Risking the future: Adolescent sexual-
References 711

ity, pregnancy, and childbearing (Vol. 2, pp. 78–92). Washington, Kuh, D., Power, C., Blane, D., & Bartley, M. (1997). Social pathways
DC: National Academy Press. between childhood and adult health. In D. Kuh & Y. Ben-Shlomo
Hoffman, J. P., & Cerbone, F. G. (1999). Stressful life-events and delin- (Eds.), A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology
quency escalation in early adolescence. Criminology, 37, 343–373. (pp. 169–198). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hogan, D. P. (1981). Transitions and social change: The early lives of Laub, J. H., & Sampson, R. J. (2003). Shared beginnings, divergent
American man. New York: Academic. lives. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Holahan, C. K., & Sears, R. R. (1995). The gifted group in later matu- Leadbeater, B. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Blatt, S. J., & Hertzog, C.
rity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. (1999). A multivariate model of gender differences in adoles-
Hughes, E. (1971). The sociological eye: Selected papers on work, self, cents’ internalizing and externalizing problems. Developmental
and the study of society (Vol. 1). Chicago: Aldine-Atherton. Psychology, 35, 1268–1282.
Jackson, J. S. (Ed.). (2000). New directions: African Americans in a Leisering, L. (2003). Government and the life course. In J. T. Mor-
diversifying nation. Washington, DC: National Policy Association. timer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course
Jarjoura, G. R., Triplett, R. A., & Brinker, G. P. (2002). Growing up (pp. 205–225). New York: Plenum Press.
poor: Examining the link between persistent childhood poverty and Lerner, R. M. (1982). Children and adolescents as producers of their
delinquency. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 18(2), 159–187. own development. Developmental Review, 2(4), 342–370.
Johnston, T. D., & Edwards, L. (2002). Genes, interactions, and the Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. Cambridge:
development of behavior. Psychological Review, 109, 26–34. Cambridge University Press.
Jones, M. C., Bayley, N., Macfarlane, J. W., & Honzik, M. E. (1971). Lerner, R. M. (1991). Changing organism-context relations as the
The course of human development: Selected Papers from the Longi- basic process of development: A developmental contextual per-
tudinal Studies, University of California, Berkeley, Institute of spective. Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 27–32.
Human Development. Waltham, MA: Xerox College Publishing. Levinson, D. J. (1978). The seasons of a man’s life. New York: Knopf.
Kagan, J., & Moss, H. A. (1962). Birth to maturity: A Study in Psy-
Lewin, K. (1948). Resolving social conf licts: Selected papers on group
chological Development. New York: Wiley.
dynamics. New York: Harper.
Katz, L. F., Kling, J., & Liebman, J. (2001). Moving to opportunity in
Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. (1995). Social conditions as fundamental
Boston: Early impacts of a housing mobility program. Quarterly
causes of disease [Special issue: Forty years of medical sociol-
Journal of Economics, 116(2), 607–654.
ogy: The state of the art and directions for the future]. Journal of
Kaufman, J. S., Kaufman, S., & Poole, C. (2003). Causal Inference Health and Social Behavior, 35, 80–94.
from randomized trials in social epidemiology. Social Science
Little, T. D., Bovaird, J., & Marquis, J. (in press). Modeling develop-
Medicine, 57(12), 2397–2409.
mental processes in context. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kendler, K. S., & Eaves, L. (1986). Models for the joint effect of
genotype and environment on liability to psychiatric illness. Little, T. D., Schnabel, K. U., & Baumert, J. (2000). Modeling longi-
American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 279–289. tudinal and multilevel data: Practical issues, applied approaches,
and specific examples. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (2002). Toward a
comprehensive developmental model for major depression in Ludwig, J., Duncan, G. J., & Hirschfield, P. (2001). Urban poverty
women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1133–1145. and juvenile crime: Evidence from a randomized housing-
mobility experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 116(2),
Kendler, K. S., & Kessler, R. C. (1995). Stressful life events, genetic
655– 680.
liability, and onset of an episode of major depression in women.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(6), 833–842. Luthar, S. S. (2003). Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the
Kerckhoff, A. C. (1993). Diverging pathways: Social structure and context of childhood adversities. New York: Cambridge Univer-
career def lections. New York: Cambridge University Press. sity Press.
Kerckhoff, A. C. (2003). From student to worker. In J. T. Mortimer & Lynd, R. S., & Lynd, H. M. (1929). Middletown. New York: Harcourt,
M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 251–267). Brace and Company.
New York: Plenum Press. Lynd, R. S., & Lynd, H. M. (1937). Middletown in transition: A Study
Kertzer, D. I., & Keith, J. (1984). Age and anthropological theory. in Cultural Conf licts. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Company.
Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Macmillan, R., Hitlin, S., & Elder, H. E., Jr. (in press). Construct-
Kim, K. J., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Lorenz, F. O. (2003). ing adulthood: Agency and subjectivity in the life course—Ad-
Reciprocal inf luences between stressful life-events and adoles- vances in life course research (Vol. 10). New York: Elsevier/JAI
cent internalizing and externalizing problems. Child Develop- Press.
ment, 74, 127–143. Mare, R. D. (1990). Socio-economic careers and differential mortal-
Kling, J. R., & Liebman, J. B. (2004). Experimental analysis of neigh- ity among older men in the United States. In J. Vallin, S. D’Souza,
borhood effects on youth. Princeton IRS Working Paper, No. 483. & A. Palloni (Eds.), Comparative Studies of Mortality and Mor-
Koopmans, J. R., & Slutske, W. S. (1999). The inf luence of religion bidity: Old and new approaches to measurement and analysis.
on alcohol use initiation: Evidence for genotype x environment in- London: Oxford University Press.
teraction. Behavior Genetics, 29(6), 445–453. Marini, M. M. (1984). Age and sequencing norms in the transition to
Krein, S. F., & Beller, A. H. (1988). Educational attainment of chil- adulthood. Social Forces, 63, 229–244.
dren from single-parent families: Differences by exposure, gen- Marshall, V. W., Heinz, W. R., Kruger, H., & Verma, A. (2001). Re-
der, and race. Demography, 25(2), 221–234. structuring work and the life course. Toronto, Ontario, Canada:
Kuh, D., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Lynch, J., Hallqvist, J., & Power, C. (2003). University of Toronto Press.
Life course epidemiology. Journal of Epidemiology and Commu- Marsiske, M., Lang, F. R., Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1995). Se-
nity Health, 57(10), 778–783. lective optimization with compensation: Life-span perspectives
712 The Life Course and Human Development

on successful human development. In R. A. Dixon & L. Bäck- bins (Eds.), Conf lict and consensus: A festschrift for Lewis A.
man (Eds.), Compensation for psychological deficits and de- Coser. New York: The Free Press.
clines: Managing losses and promoting gains (pp. 35–79). Mills, C. W. (1959). The sociological imagination. New York: Oxford
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. University Press.
Maruna, S. (2001). Making good: How ex-convicts reform and rebuild Mitterauer, M. (1993). A history of youth. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
their lives. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Modell, J. (1989). Into one’s own: From youth to adulthood in the United
Mattick, H. W. (1960). Parolees in the army during World War II.
States 1920–1975. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Federal Probation, 24, 49–55.
Moen, P. (2003). It’s about time: Couples and careers. Ithaca, NY:
Mayer, K. U. (1986). Structural constraints on the life course.
Cornell University Press.
Human Development, 29(3), 163–170.
Moen, P., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Lüscher, K. (Eds.). (1995). Examining
Mayer, K. U. (1988). German survivors of World War II: The impact
lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human develop-
on the life course of the collective experience of birth cohorts. In
M. W. Riley (Ed., with B. J. Huber & B. B. Hess) Social change ment—Essays in honor of Urie Bronfenbrenner. Washington, DC:
and the life course: Vol. I. Social structures and human lives American Psychological Association.
(pp. 229–246). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent
Mayer, K. U. (2003). The sociology of the life course and life- antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological
span psychology: Diverging or converging pathways. In Review, 100(4), 674–701.
U. M. Staudinger and U. Lindberger (Eds.), Understanding Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Dickson, N., Silva, P., & Stanton, W.
human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology (1996). Childhood-onset versus adolescent-onset antisocial con-
(pp. 463–481). Boston: Kluwer. duct problems in males: Natural history from age 3 to 18. Devel-
Mayer, K. U. (2004). Whose lives? How history, societies, and insti- opment and Psychopathology, 8, 399–424.
tutions define and shape life courses. Research in Human Develop- Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., & Milne, B. J. (2002).
ment, 1(3), 161–187. Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited anti-
Mayer, K. U., & Huinink, J. (1990). Age, period, and cohort in the social pathways: Follow-up at age 26 years. Development and
Study of the Life Course: A comparison of classical A-P-C analy- Psychopathology, 14(1), 179–207.
sis with event history analysis or farewell to lexis. In D. Magnus- Molenaar, P. C. M., Huizenga, H. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (in press).
son & L. R. Bergman (Eds.), Data quality in longitudinal research The relationship between the structure of intra-individual and
(pp. 211–232). New York: Cambridge University Press. inter-individual variability: A theoretical and empirical vindica-
McClearn, G. E., Vogler, G. M., & Hofer, S. M. (2001). Gene-gene tion of developmental systems theory. In U. M. Staudinger & U.
and gene-environment interactions. In Handbook of the biology of Lindenberger (Eds.), Understanding human development. Boston:
aging (pp. 423–444). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Kluwer Press.
McDonough, P., Duncan, G. J., Williams, D., & House, J. (1997). In- Morenoff, J. D. (2003). Neighborhood mechanisms and the spatial dynam-
come dynamics and adult mortality in the United States, ics of birth weight. American Journal of Sociology, 108, 976–1017.
1972–1989. American Journal of Public Health, 87(9), 1476–1483.
Mortimer, J. T. (2003). Working and growing up in America. Cam-
McGue, M. (1999). The behavioral genetics of alcoholism. Current bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Directions in Psychological Science, 8(4), 109–115.
Mortimer, J. T., & Shanahan, M. J. (2003). Handbook on the life
McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J. (1998). Genetic and environmental in- course. New York: Plenum Press.
f luences on human behavioral differences. Annual Review of Neu-
roscience, 21, 1–24. Mortimer, J. T., Staff, J., & Oesterle, S. (2003). Adolescent work and
the early socioeconomic career. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shana-
McLanahan, S. S., & Sandefur, G. (1994). Growing up with a single
han (Eds.), Handbook of the life course. New York: Plenum Press.
parent: What hurts, what helps. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
sity Press. Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A
semiparametric group-based approach. Psychological Methods,
McLanahan, S. S., & Sorensen, A. B. (1985). Life events and psycho-
4(2), 139–157.
logical well-being over the life course. In G. H. Elder Jr. (Ed.),
Life course dynamics: Trajectories and transitions, 1968–1980 Nagin, D. S. (2004). Response to “methodological sensitivities to la-
(pp. 217–238). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. tent class analysis of long-term criminal trajectories.” Journal of
McLeod, J. D., & Almazan, E. P. (2003). Connections between Quantitative Criminology, 20(1), 27–35.
childhood and adulthood. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan Nagin, D. S., Farrington, D. P., & Moffitt, T. E. (1995). Life-
(Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 391–411). New York: course trajectories of different types of offenders. Criminology,
Plenum Press. 33(1), 111–139.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Nagin, D. S., & Land, K. C. (1993). Age, criminal careers and popu-
Chicago Press. lation heterogeneity: Specification and estimation of a nonpara-
Merton, R. K. (1968). Social theory and social structure. New York: metric, mixed poisson model. Criminology, 31, 327–362.
Free Press. Nagin, D. S., & Paternoster, R. (2000). Population heterogeneity and
Merton, R. K. (1982). Socially expected durations. Paper presented at state dependence: State of the evidence and directions for future
the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association, San research. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 16(2), 117–144.
Francisco. Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Analyzing developmental tra-
Merton, R. K. (1984). Socially expected durations: A case study of jectories of distinct but related behaviors: A group-based method.
concept formation in sociology. In W. W. Powell & Richard Rob- Psychological Methods, 6(1), 18–34.
References 713

Neugarten, B. L. (1968). Middle age and aging: A reader in social of socioeconomic disadvantage and adult health: A Life-Course
psychology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Study. Lancet, 360, 1640–1645.
Neugarten, B. L. (Ed.) (with a foreword by D. A. Neugarten). (1996). Power, C., & Hertzman, C. (1997). Social and biological pathways
The meanings of age: Selected papers of Bernice L . Neugarten. linking early life and adult disease. British Medical Bulletin,
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 53(1), 210–221.
Neugarten, B. L., & Datan, N. (1973). Sociological perspectives on Power, C., Manor, O., & Matthews, S. (1999). The duration
the life cycle. In P. B. Baltes & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Life-span and timing of exposure: Effects of socioeconomic environment
developmental psychology: Personality and socialization on adult health. American Journal of Public Health, 89(7),
(pp. 53– 69). New York: Academic Press. 1059–1065.
Neugarten, B. L., Moore, J. W., & Lowe, J. C. (1965). Age norms, age Quinton, D., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., & Rutter, M. (1993). Part-
constraints, and adult socialization. American Journal of Sociol- ners, peers, and pathways: Assortative pairing and continuities
ogy, 70(6), 710–717. in conduct disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 5(4),
Neugarten, B. L., & Peterson, W. A. (1957). A Study of the American 763–783.
Age-Grade System. Proceedings from the Fourth Congress of the Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1988). Parenting breakdown: The making
International Association of Gerontology, 3, 497–502. and breaking of intergenerational links. Aldershot, England:
Nisbet, R. (1969). Social change and history. New York: Oxford Uni- Avebury.
versity Press. Rabinowitz, L. S., & Rosenbaum, J. E. (2000). Crossing the class and
Noack, P., Hofer, M., & Youniss, J. (1995). Psychological responses to color lines: From public housing to White suburbia. Chicago: Uni-
social change: Human development in changing environments. New versity of Chicago Press.
York: Walter de Gruyter. Rachman, S. (1979). The concept of required helpfulness. Behavior
Oh, H. J. (2001, February). An exploration of the inf luence of house- Research and Therapy, 17(1), 1–6.
hold poverty spells on mortality risk. Journal of Marriage and Rampon, C., Tang, Y.-P., Goodhouse, J., Shimuzu, M., Kyin, M., &
Family, 63, 224–234.
Tsien, J. M. (2000). Enrichment induces structural changes and
O’Rand, A. M., & Krecker, M. L. (1990). Concepts of the life cycle: recovery from nonspatial memory deficits in CA1 NMDAR1-
Their history, meanings and uses in the social sciences. Annual knockout mice. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 238–244.
Review of Sociology, 16, 241–262.
Rampon, C., & Tsien, J. Z. (2000). Genetic analysis of learning
Pallas, A. M. (2003). Educational transitions, trajectories, and path- behavior-induced structural plasticity. Hippocampus, 10, 605–609.
ways. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the
Rank, M. R., & Hirschl, T. A. (1999). The economic risk of childhood
life course (pp. 165–184). New York: Plenum Press.
in America: Estimating the probability of poverty across the forma-
Pampel, F. C., & Peters, H. E. (1995). The Easterlin effect. Annual tive years. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61(4), 1058–1067.
Review of Sociology, 21, 163–194.
Riley, M. W., Johnson, M. E., & Foner, A. (1972). Aging and society.
Parke, R. D., & Ladd, G. W. (1992). Family-peer relationship: Modes New York: Russell Sage.
of linkage. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Robins, L. (1966). Deviant children grown up. Baltimore:
Parke, R. D., Ornstein, P. A., Rieser, J. J., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1994). Williams & Wilkins.
A century of developmental psychology. Washington, DC: Ameri-
Rodgers, W. L., & Thornton, A. (1985). Changing patterns of first
can Psychological Association.
marriage in the United States. Demography, 22(2), 265–279.
Paternoster, R., Bushway, S., Brame, R., & Apel, R. (2003). The ef-
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
fect of teenage employment on delinquency and problem behav-
iors. Social Forces, 82(1), 297–335. Rowe, D. C. (2001). Assessing genotype-environment interactions and
correlations in the postgenomic era. In R. Plomin, J. C. DeFries,
Patterson, G. R. (1996). Some characteristics of a developmental
theory of early-onset delinquency. In M. F. Lenzenweger & J. J. G. E. McClearn, & P. McGuffin (Eds.), Behavioral genetics (4th
Haugaard (Eds.), Frontiers of developmental psychopathology ed., pp. 71–86). New York: Worth.
(pp. 81–124). New York: Oxford University Press. Rowe, D. C., & Jacobson, K. C. (1999). Genetic and environmental in-
Patterson, G. R., & Yoerger, K. (1996). A developmental model for f luences on vocabulary IQ: Parental education level as moderator.
late-onset delinquency. In D. W. Osgood (Ed.), Nebraska Sympo- Child Development, 70(5), 1151–1162.
sium on Motivation: Motivation and delinquency (pp. 119–177). Rutter, M. (1989). Pathways from childhood to adult life. Journal of
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(1), 23–51.
Pepler, D. J., & Rubin, K. H. (1991). The development and treatment Rutter, M. (1990). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms.
of childhood aggression. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. In J. E. Rolf, A. S. Masten, D. Cicchetti, K. H. Neuchterlein, & S.
Phelps, E., Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., & Colby, A. (2002). Looking at Weintraub (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of
lives: American Longitudinal Studies of the Twentieth Century. psychopathology (pp. 181–214). New York: Cambridge.
New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Rutter, M., & Madge, N. (1976). Cycles of disadvantage: A review of
Pinquart, M., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2004). Human development in research. London: Heinemann.
times of social change: Theoretical considerations and research Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. (1991). Person-environment interactions:
needs. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(4), Concepts, mechanisms, and implications for data analysis. In
289–298. T. E. Wachs & R. Plomin (Eds.), Conceptualization and measure-
Poulton, R., Caspi, A., Milne, B. J., Thomson, W. M., Taylor, A., & ment of organism-environment interaction (pp. 105–141). Wash-
Sears, M. R. (2002). Association between children’s experience ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
714 The Life Course and Human Development

Rutter, M., & Silberg, J. (2002). Gene-environment interplay in rela- Shanahan, M. J., Hofer, S. M., & Shanahan, L. (2003). Biological
tion to emotional and behavioral disturbance. Annual Review of models of behavior and the life course. In J. T. Mortimer & M. J.
Psychology, 53, 463–490. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 597–622). New
Ryder, N. B. (1965). The cohort as a concept in the Study of Social York: Plenum Press.
Change. American Sociological Review, 30(6), 843–861. Shanahan, M. J., Mortimer, J. T., & Kruger, H. (2002). Adolescence
Ryu, S., & Mortimer, J. T. (1996). The “occupational linkage hypoth- and adult work in the twenty-first century. Journal of Research in
esis” applied to occupational value formation in adolescence. In Adolescence, 12, 99–120.
J. T. Mortimer & M. D. Finch (Eds.), Adolescents, work, and fam- Shanahan, M. J., & Porfeli, E. (2002). Integrating the life course and
ily: An intergenerational developmental analysis (pp. 167–190). life-span: Formulating research questions with dual points of
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. entry. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 61, 398–406.
Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1993). Crime in the making: Pathways Shanahan, M. J., Sulloway, F. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2000). Conceptual
and turning points through life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- models of context in developmental studies. International Journal
sity Press. of Behavioral Development, 24, 421–427.
Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1996). Socioeconomic achievement Silbereisen, R. K., & Schmitt-Rodermund, E. (1995). German im-
in the life course of disadvantaged men: Military service as a migrants in Germany: Adaptation of adolescents’ timetables
turning point, circa 1940–1965. American Sociological Review, for autonomy. In P. Noack, M. Hofer, & J. Youniss (Eds.), Psy-
61(3), 347–367. chological responses to social change: Human development in
Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1997). A life-course theory of cumula- changing environments (pp. 105–125). Berlin, Germany: Walter
tive disadvantage and the stability of delinquency. In T. P. Thorn- de Gruyter.
berry (Ed.), Developmental theories of crime and delinquency: Silbereisen, R. K., & Wiesner, M. (2000). Cohort change in adoles-
Advances in criminological theory (Vol. 7, pp. 133–161). New cent developmental timetables after German unification: Trends
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. and possible reasons. In J. Heckhausen (Ed.), Motivational psy-
Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D., & Gannon-Rowley, T. (2002). Assess- chology of human development: Developing motivation and moti-
ing “neighborhood effects”: Social processes and new directions vating development (pp. 271–284). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
in research. Annual Review of Sociology, 28, 443–478. Silbereisen, R. K., & Wiesner, M. (2002). Lessons from research on
Sawa, A., & Snyder, S. H. (2002). Schizophrenia: Diverse approaches the consequences of German unification: Continuity and disconti-
to a complex disease. Science, 296, 692–695. nuity of self-efficacy and the timing of psychosocial transitions.
Schaie, K. W. (1965). A general model for the Study of Developmen- Applied Psychology: An International Review, 51, 290–316.
tal Problems. Psychological Bulletin, 64(2), 92–107. Silberg, J., & Rutter, M. (2001). Genetic moderation of environmen-
Schiller, K. S. (1999, October). Effects of feeder patterns on students’ tal risk for depression and anxiety in adolescent girls. British
transition to high school. Sociology of Education, 72, 216–233. Journal of Statistical Psychiatry, 179, 116–121.
Schoenhals, M., Tienda, M., & Schneider, B. (1998). The educational Simmons, R. G., Burgeson, R., Carlton-Ford, S., & Blyth, D. A.
and personal consequences of adolescent employment. Social (1987). The impact of cumulative change in early adolescence.
Forces, 77(2), 723–761. Child Development, 58(5), 1220–1234.
Schoon, I., Bynner, J., Joshi, H., Parsons, S., Wiggins, R. W., & Singer, B. H., & Ryff, C. D. (2001). New horizons in health: An inte-
Sacker, A. (2002). The inf luence of context, timing, and duration grative approach. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
of risk experiences for the passage from childhood to midadult- Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analy-
hood. Child Development, 73(5), 1486–1504. sis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford
Settersten, R. A., Jr., Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., & Rumbaut, R. (2005). University Press.
On the frontier of adulthood: Theory, research and public policy. Sofer, C. (1970). Men in mid-career: A Study of British Managers
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. and Technical Specialists. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni-
Settersten, R. A., Jr., & Mayer, K. U. (1997). The Measurement of versity Press.
Age, Age Structuring, and the Life Course. Annual Review of So- Spilerman, S. (1977). Careers, labor market structure, and socioeco-
ciology, 23, 233–261. nomic achievement. American Journal of Sociology, 83(3), 551–593.
Shanahan, M. J., & Bauer, D. (2005). Developmental properties of Staudinger, U. M. (1989). The Study of Life Review: An approach to
transactional models: The case of life-events and mastery from the investigation of intellectual development across the life-span.
adolescence to young adulthood. Development and Psychopathol- Berlin, Germany: Max-Planck-Institut.
ogy, 16, 1095–1117. Stevens, A. H. (1999). Measuring the persistence of poverty over
Shanahan, M. J., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2002). History, agency, and multiple spells: Climbing out of poverty, falling back. Journal of
the life course. In L. J. Crockett (Ed.), Agency, motivation, and Human Resources, 34(3), 557–588.
the life course (pp. 145–185). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Stolz, L. M. (1954). Father relations of war-born children. Stanford,
Press. CT: Stanford University Press.
Shanahan, M. J., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Miech, R. A. (1997). History and Stouffer, S. A., Lumsdaine, A. A., Lumsdaine, M. H., Williams,
agency in men’s lives: Pathways to achievement in cohort perspec- R. M., Jr., Smith, M. B., Janis, I. L., et al. (1949). The American
tive. Sociology of Education, 70(1), 54–67. soldier: Studies in Social Psychology in World War II. Princeton,
Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2005). Social context in gene- NJ: Princeton University Press.
environment interactions: Retrospect and prospect [Special Sweeney, M. M., & Horwitz, A. V. (2001). Infidelity, initiation, and
issue on the future of behavioral genetics and aging]. Journal of the emotional climate of divorce: Are there implications for men-
Gerontology: Social, 60B 65–76. tal health? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42, 295–309.
References 715

Thoits, P. A. (1994). Stressors and problem-solving: The individual as Wadsworth, M. E. J., Montgomery, S. M., & Bartley, M. J. (1999).
psychological activist. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, The persisting effect of unemployment on health and social well-
35(2), 143–160. being in men early in working life. Social Science and Medicine,
Thomas, W. I., & Znaniecki, F. (1918–1920). The Polish peasant in 48, 1491–1499.
Europe and America (Vols. 1–2). Boston: Badger. Wahlsten, D. (1999). Single-gene inf luences on brain and behavior.
Tremblay, R. E. (2004). The development of human physical aggres- Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 599–624.
sion: How important is early childhood. In L. A. Leavitt & Warren, J. R., LePore, P. C., & Mare, R. D. (2000). Employment
D. M. B. Hall (Eds.), Social and moral development: Emerging ev- during high school: Consequences for students’ grades in aca-
idence on the toddler years (pp. 221–238). New Brunswick, NJ: demic courses. American Educational Research Journal, 37(4),
Johnson and Johnson Pediatric Institute. 943–969.
Turner, R. J., & Lloyd, D. A. (1995). Lifetime traumas and mental Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (2001). Journeys from childhood to
health: The significance of cumulative adversity. Journal of midlife: Risk, resilience, and recovery. New York: Cornell Univer-
Health and Social Behavior, 36, 360–376. sity Press.
Turner, R. J., Wheaton, B., & Lloyd, D. A. (1995). The epidemiology Wheaton, B. (1990). Life transitions, role histories, and mental
of social stress. American Sociological Review, 60, 104–125. health. American Sociological Review, 55(2), 209–223.
Uhlenberg, P. (1974). Cohort variations in family life-cycle ex- Wheaton, B., & Clarke, P. (2003). Space meets time: Integrating tem-
periences of US women. Journal of Marriage and Family, 36, poral and contextual inf luences on mental health in early adult-
282–294. hood. American Sociological Review, 68, 680–706.
Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adaptation to life. Boston: Little, Brown. White, L. K., & Booth, A. (1991). Divorce over the life course: The
van de Mheen, H., Stronks, K., Looman, C. W., & Mackenbach, J. P. role of marital happiness. Journal of Family Issues, 12(1), 5–21.
(1998). Does childhood socioeconomic status inf luence adult Wilensky, H. L. (1960). Work, career, and social integration. Interna-
health through behavioral factors? International Journal of Epi- tional Social Science Journal, 12(4), 543–560.
demiology, 27(3), 431–437. Willitts, M., Benzeval, M., & Stansfeld, S. (2004). Partnership
van der Lippe, T., & van Dijk, L. (2002). Comparative research history and mental health over time. Journal of Epidemiology and
on women’s employment. Annual Review of Sociology, 28, Community Health, 58(1), 53–58.
221–241. Wortman, C. B., & Silver, R. C. (1990). Successful mastery of
van Os, J., & Marcelis, M. (1998). The ecogenetics of schizophrenia: bereavement and widowhood: A life-course perspective. In
A review. Schizophrenia Research, 32, 127–135. P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives
VanLaningham, J., Johnson, D. R., & Amato, P. (2001). Marital hap- from the behavioral sciences (pp. 225–264). New York: Cam-
piness, marital duration, and the u-shaped curve: Evidence from a bridge University Press.
Five-Wave Panel Study. Social Forces, 78(4), 1313–1341. Young, C. H., Savola, K. L., & Phelps, E. (1991). Inventory of Longi-
Volkart, E. H. (1951). Social behavior and personality: Contributions tudinal Studies in the Social Sciences. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
of W. I. Thomas to theory and social research. New York: Social Young, M. D., & Willmott, P. (1973). The symmetrical family. New
Science Research Council. York: Pantheon.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psy-
chological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
CHAPTER 13

The Cultural Psychology of Development:


One Mind, Many Mentalities
RICHARD A. SHWEDER, JACQUELINE J. GOODNOW, GIYOO HATANO, ROBERT A. LeVINE,
HAZEL R. MARKUS, and PEGGY J. MILLER

CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY: HOW IT DIFFERS THE EFFECTS OF EARLY


FROM OTHER APPROACHES TO CULTURE INTERPERSONAL EXPERIENCE 739
AND PSYCHOLOGY 719 THE SYMBOLIC MEDIATION OF EXPERIENCE:
Orienting Definitions 719 LANGUAGE AND COMMUNICATIVE CUSTOMS
WHY CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY IS NOT IN CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 740
CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 721 LANGUAGE AS PRACTICE 741
MULTIPLE, DIVERSE PSYCHOLOGIES 723 SOCIALIZATION THROUGH LANGUAGE 742
THE MEANING OF MEANING AND A CONTEXT FOR Narrative: Getting Those Stories Straight 744
CONTEXT IN CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 724 THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF 749
THE UNIT OF ANALYSIS PROBLEM 724 DEFINING AND LOCATING THE SELF AS DYNAMIC,
A Custom Complex Example: Who Sleeps by Whom MULTILEVEL, AND MULTIFACETED 750
in the Family 725 The Conceptual Self 751
More on the Custom Complex: The Intimate Association The Cultural Self 752
between a Mentality and a Practice Supported by a THE ONTOLOGICAL BASIS OF SELF 752
Cultural Community 727
The Person as an Individual 752
THE CLASSIFICATION OF PRACTICES 728 The Person as Relational 753
THE ANALYSIS OF MENTALITIES 730 CULTURAL DIVERSITY IN WAYS OF BEING 754
THE TWO SIDES OF CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 730 Selfways in Some European American Contexts 755
CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY’S THEORY OF MIND 731 Selfways in Some East Asian Cultural Contexts 758
CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY’S SPECIAL USE OF Selfways in Still Other Cultural Contexts 761
MENTAL STATE CONCEPTS 732 CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY AND
SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT IN THE DIVERGENT COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT 764
INTERPERSONAL WORLDS OF CHILDHOOD 733 Reexamining Thought and Action 764
THE SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF Hearts and Minds 768
CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCE 734 Persons and Contexts 772
Organizational Settings 734 CONCLUSION 778
Care-Giving Relationships 736 REFERENCES 779
Parental Practices 737
Age-Graded Activities 738
CULTURAL MENTALITIES CONCERNING
CHILDHOOD SOCIAL RELATIONS 738
Parental Models and Strategies 738

Cultural psychology, no longer a new field, may be more Herder and Vico pioneered comparative research with
accurately depicted as a renewed field (Jahoda, 1990, the aim of identifying the distinctive characteristics of
1992), approaching the study of mind from deep historical particular folk and historical traditions. Dilthey raised
antecedents in the work of eighteenth- and nineteenth-
century scholars such as Johann Gottfried von Herder, This chapter is a revision and update of a manuscript that was
Giovanni Vico, Wilhelm Dilthey, and Wilhelm Wundt. originally prepared when the coauthors were members of the

716
The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities 717

questions about the contrast between the natural science chology is the study of how individual persons think and
approach and the spiritual or moral science approach to act in the light of their particular goals, values, and pic-
human understanding and the explanation of behavior. tures of the world. This is a genre of psychological study
Wundt, who is often heralded as the father of modern based on a definition of the psychological (or of the
scientific psychology, also thought deeply about the lim- mental) as consisting of what individual persons want,
its of psychology as an experimental discipline and feel, think, know, and value. The cultural side of cul-
about its possibilities as an investigation of folk psy- tural psychology is the examination of socially assisted
chologies. Herder’s premise that “ to be a member of a processes of learning and schema activation associated
group is to think and act in a certain way, in the light of with becoming a member of a particular group. The dis-
particular goals, values, pictures of the world; and to cipline of cultural psychology gives special attention to
think and act so is to belong to a group” (Berlin, 1976, the particular wants, feelings, knowledge, reasoning,
p. 195) presents a starting point for the contemporary and values required for normative or competent partici-
discipline of cultural psychology. pation in the local customary practices of some histori-
Cultural psychology aims to document historical and cally identifiable community, especially (although not
cross-cultural diversity in the processes and products of exclusively) cultural communities that have a capacity to
the human mind. The psychological side of cultural psy- recruit new members through processes of kinship and
marriage and wish to perpetuate a particular way of life.
Cultural psychology has been experiencing a major re-
Social Science Research Council Planning Committee on Cul-
ture, Health and Human Development. We were able to develop vival since the early 1980s, owed in some significant
and undertake this cooperative project, involving an intellec- measure to developmentalists from several fields (e.g.,
tual division of labor and writing, as a result of our colloquies Bruner, 1990a, 1990b; Cole, 1990, 1996; Goodnow,
at SSRC. We are grateful to Diana Colbert of SSRC and Katia 1990a; A. Gottlieb, 2004; Greenfield, 1997; Greenfield,
Mitova of the University of Chicago who contributed in innu- Keller, Fuligni, & Maynard, 2003; Haidt, Koller, & Dias
merable and invaluable ways to the completion of this manu- 1993; Lave, 1990; R. A. LeVine, 1989, 2004; Levy, 1973,
script in its original form. We express our heartfelt gratitude 1984; Markus & Kitayama, 1991b; Menon, 2002; J. G.
to Frank Kessel, our Program Officer at SSRC, for his colle- Miller, 1984, 1994b; P. J. Miller & Hoogstra, 1992;
gial contributions not only to this chapter project but also to Much, 1992, 1993; Rogoff, 1990; Ross, Medin, Coley, &
the various activities of the Committee on Culture, Health,
Atran, 2003; Rozin & Nemeroff, 1990; Shweder, 1990a,
and Human Development over the years of its existence. The
1991, 1996a, 1996b, 2003a, 2003b; Shweder & Haidt,
skill, balance, and good cheer with which he shepherded the
2000; Shweder & LeVine, 1984; Stigler, Shweder, &
Committee’s activities are deeply appreciated. The Planning
Committee was supported by grants from the Health Program Herdt, 1990; Super & Harkness, 1997; Weisner, 1984,
of the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation and the 1987, 2001; Weisner & Lowe, in press; Wertsch, 1985,
W. T. Grant Foundation. The Center for Advanced Study in the 1992). The term cultural psychology has also become in-
Behavioral Sciences (where Shweder and Markus were Fellows creasingly popular among European “activity theorists”
during the 1995/1996 academic year) and the MacArthur (Boesch, 1991; Eckensberger, 1990, 1995; see also
Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Develop- Brandtstädter, Chapter 10, this Handbook, this volume),
ment (MIDMAC) provided intellectual and material assistance contextual psychologists of the sociohistorical school
in the preparation of the original review. The current updated (Cole, 1995; Rogoff, 1990; Wertsch, 1991; see also Cole,
revision of the chapter took place during the 2003/2004 aca- Chapter 15, this Handbook, Volume 2; Elder & Shanahan,
demic year. Richard A. Shweder, a Carnegie Scholar (2002),
Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume), anthropologists
wishes to express his gratitude to the Carnegie Foundation for
interested in the relationship of symbols and meanings to
its generous support. The authors wish to express their great
population-based differences in psychological function-
thanks as well to Michele Wittels, who skillfully contributed
to the coordination and processing of this revised and updated ing (D’Andrade, 1995; Howard, 1985; R. A. LeVine,
edition of the chapter. 1990a, 1990b; Levy, 1984; Lutz & White, 1986; Shore,
This chapter is dedicated to our friend and colleague 1996; Shweder & LeVine, 1984; G. M. White & Kirk-
Giyoo Hatano, in honor of his personality, his life, and his patrick, 1985), and among developmental, social, and
work. Even while his sudden and recent death deeply saddens cognitive psychologists in search of a unit of scientific
us, the memory of our kind, dedicated and elegant friend and analysis that is larger rather than smaller than the indi-
imaginative colleague makes us smile and warms our hearts. vidual person (Bruner, 1986, 1990a, 1990b; Cole, 1988,
718 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

1992; Goodnow, Miller, & Kessel, 1995; Kitayama & the last edition of the Handbook, itself signals a continu-
Markus, 1994; Medin, 1989; P. J. Miller & Hoogstra, ing appreciation of the value and relevance of cultural
1992; Nisbett, 2003; Nisbett & Cohen, 1996; Rogoff, psychology to developmental studies. The last edition
1990; Yang, 1997). was the first time that the Handbook of Child Psychology
Research in cultural psychology is now featured in included a chapter under the name cultural psychology.
several journals, most notably Culture, Mind and Activity, It should be acknowledged, however, that this chapter
Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, Culture and Psychol- continues a broader conversation about culture and indi-
ogy, Ethos: Journal of the Society for Psychological An- vidual development that began in previous editions of the
thropology, Psychological Review, The Journal of Handbook, beginning with Margaret Mead’s contribu-
Personality and Social Psychology, and Child Develop- tion to the first edition, published in 1931. The section
ment. Impressive collections of theoretical, methodologi- of this chapter on the interpersonal worlds of childhood
cal, and empirical papers have appeared (Goodnow et al., provides an update of Robert LeVine’s chapter in the
1995; Holland & Quinn, 1987; Jessor, Colby, & Shweder, third (1970) edition of the Handbook. And, the Labora-
1996; Kitayama & Markus, 1994; Rosenberger, 1992; tory of Comparative Human Cognition’s (LCHC) chap-
Schwartz, White, & Lutz, 1992; Shweder, 1991, 2003a, ter on culture and cognitive development in the fourth
2003b; Shweder & LeVine, 1984; Stigler et al., 1990; (1983) edition of the Handbook is an important prede-
G. M. White & Kirkpatrick, 1985). Important mono- cessor to this chapter, especially the section on cogni-
graphs and empirical studies have been published (D’An- tive development. We carry forward LCHC’s emphasis
drade, 1995; Fiske, 1991; Kakar, 1982; Kripal, 1995; on the semiotic mediation of experience and on a unit of
Levy, 1973; Lucy, 1992a; Lutz, 1988; Menon & Shweder, analysis that does not abstract the individual from his or
1994; J. G. Miller, 1984; P. J. Miller, 1982; Parish, 1991; her social and cultural context or focus exclusively on
Seymour, 1999; Shimizu & LeVine, 2001; Shwalb & what is “inside the skin” or “inside the head.”
Shwalb, 1996). A number of generative proposals have In this chapter, we selectively discuss the cultural psy-
been put forward for comparative research on culture and chology of individual development, with special attention
cognition (Cole, 1990; D’Andrade, 1995; Lave, 1990; to the way in which culture and psyche “make each other
Lucy, 1992a, 1992b; Shore, 1996), culture and emotion up” in the domains of self-organization, thinking, know-
(Kitayama & Markus, 1994; Mesquita & Frijda, 1992; ing, feeling, wanting, and valuing. The chapter is organ-
Russell, 1991; Shweder, 1991, 2003a, 2003b, 2004; ized into five sections: an introduction, which lays out
Shweder & Haidt, 2000; Wierzbicka, 1992, 1993), cul- major conceptual issues, followed by four topical areas—
ture and morality (Haidt et al., 1993; Jensen, 2005; J. G. the cultural organization of early experience, language
Miller, 1994a; Shweder, Mahapatra, & Miller, 1990; and socialization, self-development, and cognitive devel-
Shweder, Much, Mahapatra, & Park, 1997), and culture opment—although issues concerning moral development
and the self (Doi, 1981; Herdt, 1981, 1990; Kurtz, 1992; and the value-laden nature of mental functioning are ad-
Lebra, 1992; Markus & Kitayama, 1991a, 1991b, 2003; dressed throughout the chapter.
J. G. Miller, 1994b, 1997a; Shweder & Sullivan, 1990). We see these topical areas as paradigmatic in the
The field has been conceptualized, reconceptualized, and cultural psychology of development, yet we are also
reviewed from many perspectives: in a book-length his- keenly aware that several topics of vital interest receive
tory (Jahoda, 1992), in a book-length program for a cul- only passing and scattered attention—gender, play,
tural psychology rooted in sociohistorical theory (Cole, feelings and emotions, spirituality, and physical devel-
1996), in Handbook chapters (Greenfield, 1997; Green- opment. Without any pretense of representing all rele-
field et al., 2003; Markus, Kitayama, & Heiman, 1996; vant research agendas or conceptions of the field, we
J. G. Miller, 1997b), in the Annual Review of Psychology characterize some of the things cultural psychologists
(Shweder & Sullivan, 1993; Greenfield et al., 2003), in have learned about the interpersonal, ideational, and
the Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Masten, social communicative dimensions of psychological de-
1999), and in the Nebraska Symposium on Motivation velopment. In keeping with cultural psychology’s com-
(Markus & Kitayama, 2003). mitment to comparative inquiry within and across
Moreover, the publication of this chapter, which has cultures, we make a special effort to draw from the
been updated and revised after its initial publication in empirical record in a way that represents the range of
Cultural Psychology: How It Dif fers from Other Approaches to Culture and Psychology 719

cultural variety in psychological functioning across heritances of a cultural community (understandings and
human groups. behaviors are not always fully coordinated from either a
socialization or developmental point of view, and actions
do sometimes speak much louder than words), given the
CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY: HOW IT complexity and richness of culture, any genuine cultural
DIFFERS FROM OTHER APPROACHES TO community is always the beneficiary of both symbolic
CULTURE AND PSYCHOLOGY and behavioral inheritances (Shweder, 2003b).
In analyzing the concept of culture, most definitions
The main wager of cultural psychology is that rela- extant in the literature have tended to be either purely
tively few components of the human mental equipment symbolic in emphasis (culture as the beliefs and doc-
are so inherently constrained, hardwired, or fundamen- trines that make it possible for a people to rationalize
tal that their developmental pathway is fixed in ad- and make sense of the life they lead) or purely behav-
vance and cannot be transformed or altered through ioral in emphasis (culture as patterns of behavior that
cultural participation. The bet is that much of human are learned and passed on from generation to genera-
mental functioning is an emergent property that results tion). In our view, the most useful definitions of cul-
from symbolically mediated experiences with the be- ture try to honor both inheritances. Such definitions
havioral practices and historically accumulated ideas focus on units of analysis that are simultaneously sym-
and understandings (meanings) of particular cultural bolic and behavioral (e.g., Robert Redfield’s 1941 def-
communities. This was the bet of Herder and Vico in inition of culture as “conventional understandings,
the eighteenth century, of Wundt and Dilthey in the manifest in act and artifact, that characterize soci-
nineteenth century, and of Ruth Benedict, Margaret eties,” p. 132). Later in this chapter, we discuss in de-
Mead, Edward Sapir, and many other psychological an- tail a two-sided unit of analysis for cultural psychology
thropologists in the first half of the twentieth century. called the custom complex (J. W. M. Whiting & Child,
It is a bet that the renewed discipline of cultural psy- 1953), and we try to acknowledge and honor both the
chology, informed by contemporary research from sev- symbolic and behavioral inheritances of any cultural
eral disciplines, is still prepared to make today. community.
The symbolic inheritance of a cultural community
Orienting Definitions consists of its received ideas and understandings, both
implicit and explicit, about persons, society, nature, and
At least since the time of Herder and Vico in the eigh- the metaphysical realm of the divines. To illustrate,
teenth century, cultural psychology has been a label for ideas and understandings that are part of the symbolic
the reciprocal investigation of both the psychological inheritance of many enlightened secular folk in the Eu-
foundations of cultural communities and the cultural ropean American cultural region include:
foundations of mind. It has been a designation for the
study of how culture and psyche make each other up. Al-
• The understanding that infants are born innocent,
ternatively stated, cultural psychology is the study of all
naive, and free of any prior sins or inherited evils
the things members of different communities mentally
experience ( know, think, want, feel, value), and hence • The idea that individual wants, preferences, and
do, by virtue of being the kinds of beings who are the tastes matter and should be openly expressed and
beneficiaries, guardians, and active perpetuators of a accommodated
particular cultural tradition. • The belief that the main justification for rules, regu-
As a first approximation, we shall define culture as a lations, and any other forms of authority is to pro-
symbolic and behavioral inheritance received from out of mote social justice and enable individuals to pursue
the historical /ancestral past that provides a community their self-interest free of harm and to have the
with a framework for other-directed vicarious learning things they want
and for collective deliberations about what is true, beau- • The conviction that, other than human nature, the
tiful, good, and normal. Although it is important to material world is devoid of intentionality and has no
distinguish between the symbolic and the behavioral in- will of its own
720 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

• The doctrine that God and divinity are archaic no- them to be moral, healthy, natural, rational, benefit pro-
tions that should be displaced in the contemporary era moting, or at least normal.
• The related idea that the era in which we live is A noteworthy example of the combination of sym-
the most advanced, enlightened, and exceptional in bolic and behavioral approaches in cultural psychology
human history and should be classified and heralded is Alma Gottlieb’s recent book (2004) about reincarna-
as an age of reason tion beliefs in West Africa and their role in the pattern-
ing of infant development. Among the Beng people of
The behavioral inheritance of a cultural community Cote d’Ivoire, newborn children are comprehended and
consists of its routine or institutionalized family perceived as old souls—spiritually powerful, psycholog-
life, social, economic, and political practices. To ically complex, socially sophisticated, and retaining a
illustrate, a few of the routine or institutionalized memory of previous lives and halcyon times spent
family life practices that are popular among many dwelling in the abode of the spirits. They do not enter
rural folk in the South-Asian Hindu cultural region the world naive, at least not according to the Beng. Got-
include: tlieb’s The Afterlife Is Where We Come From offers an
eye-opening interpretation of the local cultural mean-
• Joint family living (adult brothers co-reside in the ings of developmental milestones such as the transition
same family compound or dwelling space with their from crawling to walking (which is actively discouraged
living parents and their wives marry in) by Beng parents) and the child’s early articulation of in-
• Co-sleeping arrangements of children with their parents telligible speech (which is greeted with anxiety). Her
• Separate eating arrangements for husband and wife study of Beng infant development and its connection to
(no family meal) local beliefs about reincarnation provides an expose of
• Sexual division of household tasks the dangers of presumptively universalizing culture-
specific ideals for human development, as she argues
• Time-out and seclusion for females during their men-
that infant development is not, and perhaps ought not to
strual period
be, the same wherever you go.
• Parental hand-to-mouth feeding of children long past
From the viewpoint of cultural psychology, the most
infancy and well into middle childhood
satisfactory definition of culture presupposes the exis-
• Prohibitions on premarital dating and sexuality tence of an active mental agent who not only is the re-
• Physical punishment for unruly or bad behavior cipient and guardian of a cultural tradition but also is
• Arranged marriage between young men and women motivated and engaged in some specific way of life.
of similar social status (primarily based on caste, Thus, our definition of culture emphasizes both symbols
local region, and relative wealth) and behavior. Such an approach also means that a major
prerequisite for conducting research in cultural psychol-
Of special import for the cultural psychology of individ- ogy is an imaginative capacity to suspend our disbelief
ual development is that human beings are the kinds of (e.g., one’s disbelief that the animating force in the
beings who benefit from and carry forward a cultural body of an infant is an old soul) and a willingness to set
tradition. They try to promote, promulgate, and share aside (at least temporarily) our own negative moral and
their understandings and practices with their children, emotional reactions (e.g., of anxiety, disapproval, indig-
their relatives, and their community at large. They are nation, or disgust) to other people’s understandings and
active agents in the perpetuation of their symbolic in- practices. To practice cultural psychology, we must be
heritance, largely because (among other motives) the willing and able to enter into other peoples’ conceptions
ideas and values that they inherit from the past seem to of what is right-minded, normal, beautiful, and true
them to be right-minded, true, dignifying, useful, or at (Shweder, 1996b), and we must at least try (we may fail,
least worthy of respect. but that is itself an informative outcome of the method-
They are also active agents in the perpetuation of ological effort) to translate their goals, values, and pic-
their behavioral inheritance. They try to uphold, en- tures of the world into an intelligible (and perhaps even
force, and require of each other some degree of compli- rationally defensible) account of their behavior.
ance with the practices of their community, largely Thus, cultural psychology is the study of the mental
because (among other motives) those practices seem to life of individuals in relation to the symbolic and behav-
Why Cultural Psychology Is Not Cross-Cultural Psychology 721

ioral inheritances of particular cultural communities. It Shweder et al., 1990) describe the different moral de-
is the study of the way culture, community, and the psy- velopmental pathways and patterns of moral judgment
che instantiate one another and are mutually sustaining, for children in societies privileging an “ethics of auton-
and, thus, how they become coordinated and make each omy” (where individualism, having the things you
other possible. A cultural tradition dies (it exists only in want, and harm, rights, and justice concepts predomi-
a canonical text or in an ethnographic book on a library nate) in contrast to societies privileging an “ethics of
shelf ) if there is no community that lives its doctrines, community” (where notions of duty, sacrifice, loyalty,
makes manifest its shared understandings, or inhabits and hierarchical interdependence and other social roles
its way of life. Similarly, some designated category of based on communitarian moral concepts predominate)
persons (e.g., Latinos, non-Hispanic Whites; residents or societies privileging an “ethics of divinity” (where
of the Pacific Islands; citizens of the United States) is notions of sanctity, purity, pollution, and the connec-
not a cultural community unless its members actively in- tion between the sacred order and the natural order
habit, think about, and hold each other accountable to predominate).
some symbolic and behavioral inheritance from out of A similar point is made by Greenfield (1997) who
some historical /ancestral past that they identify with as notes, “It is the human capacity to create shared mean-
a people, and claim as their own. ing that produces the distinctive methodological contri-
bution of cultural psychology.” She goes on to argue that
it is a mistake of modern psychology in general and
WHY CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY IS NOT modern cross-cultural psychology in particular to treat
CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY perspective (the shared meanings of a group is a type of
perspective) as a form of bias that should be eliminated
Many proponents of cultural psychology distinguish it from research procedures. She contrasts the methodol-
from cross-cultural psychology. This is what a few of ogy of cultural psychology with that of modern cross-
those authors have to say about the aims of a renewed cultural psychology as follows:
cultural psychology, and the ways in which it differs
from the discipline or research enterprise known as The methodological ideal of the paradigmatic cross-
cross-cultural psychology. cultural psychologist is to carry a procedure established in
Shweder and Sullivan (1993; also Shweder, 1990a) one culture, with known psychometric properties, to one or
identify the aim of cultural psychology as the study of more other cultures, in order to make a cross-cultural com-
parison (Berry, Poortinga, Segall, & Dasen, 1992). In con-
ethnic and cultural sources of psychological diversity
trast, the methodological ideal of the paradigmatic cultural
in self-organization, cognitive processing, emotional
psychologist is to derive procedures for each culture from
functioning, and moral evaluation. They describe cul- the lifeways and modes of communication of that culture.
tural psychology as a “project designed to reassess the This ideal explains why interpretive methods, especially
uniformitarian principle of psychic unity [which they ethnographic methods, have been so important to many cul-
associate with cross-cultural psychology] and aimed at tural psychologists. Ethnographic approaches were devised
the development of a credible theory of psychological originally by cultural anthropologists as a means of under-
pluralism.” They argue that performance or response standing other cultures on their own terms—not as projec-
differences between populations arise from differ- tions of the researcher’s own ethnocentric assumptions
ences in the normal meaning of stimulus situations and (Malinowski, 1922). The goal is to understand what people
materials across populations (the problem of “partial say and do from the perspective of insiders to the culture,
translation” or “limited commensurability”). They to render them intelligible within their own collectively
shared interpretive frameworks. From this standpoint, com-
suggest that a special feature of cultural psychology is
parisons within and across cultures make sense only when
its recognition that “ through the methodical investiga-
they are grounded in descriptions of the local meanings of
tion of specific sources of incommensurability in par- the people being studied. At the same time, these ap-
ticular stimulus situations (so-called thick description) proaches carry with them the reflexive recognition that re-
a culture’s distinctive psychology [the way people searchers too are members of particular communities and
think and act in the light of particular goals, values, cultures; that they may come to see their own local mean-
and pictures of the world] may be revealed.” For exam- ings in a new light by way of studying people who construe
ple, Shweder et al. (1997; also see Jensen, 2005; the world differently.
722 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

For further discussion of interpretive and ethno- ever it has diffused around the world. University stu-
graphic methods as applied to the study of children, see dents in Tokyo, Nairobi, New Delhi, and New York may
C. D. Clark (2003); Corsaro and Miller (1992); Erickson be far more like one another (and like the Western re-
(1986); Jessor et al. (1996); P. J. Miller, Hengst, and searcher) than they are like members of their respective
Wang (2003). societies whose life ways are embedded in less familiar
One useful metalanguage or theoretical framework indigenous understandings, institutions, and practices.
for the nonethnocentric identification and comparative Even if you have traveled 10,000 miles to get there, a
translation of culture-specific aspects of mental func- university setting in another land may be much closer
tioning has been developed by the anthropological lin- than you think.
guists Anna Wierzbicka and Cliff Goddard (Goddard, Much (1995) drives home this point with the follow-
1997, 2001; Goddard & Wiezbicka, 1994; Wierzbicka, ing observation:
1986, 1990, 1993, 1999; see also Shweder 2003a, 2004).
Wierzbicka and Goddard have identified a core set of It is especially important to be clear about one distinction.
semantically simple, intuitively obvious, universal folk Cultural psychology is not the same as “cross-cultural psy-
concepts (such as good, true, want, feel, do) that can chology,” which is a branch of experimental social, cogni-
then be used to elucidate the particular ways the mental tive and personality psychology. The chief distinction is
that most of what has been known as “cross-cultural psy-
states of members of different cultural groups vary. For
chology” has presupposed the categories and models that
example, in the domain of feelings and emotions those
have been available to participate in experiments or even
authors have effectively made the provocative point that to fill out questionnaires. . . . The argument often assumed
the contemporary American notion of “sadness” has to justify the tactic of studying mostly student behavior is
several cultural specific features (not even shared by based upon a sweeping and gratuitous universalist as-
various Northern European subcultures), and they have sumption—since we are all human, we are all fundamen-
proposed that the very idea of an emotion (in contrast to tally alike in significant psychological functions and
the idea of a feeling) is not a semantically simple, intu- cultural (or social) contexts of diversity do not affect the
itively obvious, or universal folk concept. important “deep” or “ hard wired” structures of the mind.
To return to Greenfield, one powerful (and somewhat There are several problems with this position. One is that
ironic) implication of her analysis would seem to be that there have been few if any satisfactory identifications of
the genuine existence of different cultural realities is deep, hard wired and invariant mental structures which
operate independently of the context or content of their
incompatible with the methodological assumptions of
functioning; the “method variance” problem in experi-
cross-cultural psychology. More specifically, if your re-
mental psychology is related to this fact. Another problem
search procedures and instruments travel readily and is that even though there may be certain biologically based
well (e.g., they are easy to administer and they display psychological foundations . . . this does not necessarily
the same psychometric properties from one test popula- mean (1) that they are invariant across individuals or pop-
tion to another) then you probably have not traveled far ulations or (2) that culture does not affect their develop-
enough into a truly alternative cultural world. ment as psychological structures and functions.
This may explain why long- and short-term field-
work, language learning, naturalistic observation, de- Whereas Greenfield and Much draw some methodologi-
tailed ethnography, and the analysis of the semantics cal contrasts between cultural versus cross-cultural psy-
and pragmatics of everyday discourse and communica- chology, J. G. Miller (1997b) envisions the difference
tion are central to the study of cultural psychology, yet between cultural psychology and cross-cultural psychol-
have played a minimal role in cross-cultural psychology. ogy in theoretical terms (although a similar theoretical
It may explain why much of the evidence in cross- point can be found in Greenfield and Much). She sug-
cultural psychology (yet relatively little of the evidence gests, “ The dominant stance within cultural psychology
in cultural psychology) is derived from observations in is to view culture and psychology as mutually constitu-
university laboratories, or from inventory or test proce- tive phenomena which cannot be reduced to each other.”
dures administered primarily to relatively cosmopolitan She adds that such a stance “contrasts with the tendency
university students in other lands. in cross-cultural psychology for culture to be conceptu-
The Western institution of the university carries with alized as an independent variable that impacts on the
it many features of an elite cosmopolitan culture wher- dependent variable of individual psychology.”
Multiple, Diverse Psychologies 723

Markus et al. (1996) carry forward this point. With gests that “ the nature of [human beings] is not, as has
an intent to simultaneously study the cultural origins long been supposed, static and unalterable or even unal-
of mind and the mental side of culture, they argue that tered; that it does not so much as contain even a central
“culture and psychology, regardless of the level at which kernel or essence, which remains identical through
they are analyzed, are interdependent and mutually ac- change; that the effort of [human beings] to understand
tive.” Markus et al. suggest: the world in which they find themselves and to adapt it
to their needs, physical and spiritual, continuously
The communities, societies, and cultural contexts within
which people participate provide the interpretive frame-
transforms their worlds and themselves” (p. xvi).
works—including the images, concepts, and narratives, as A second type of answer to those questions, to be de-
well as the means, practices and patterns of behavior—by veloped in this chapter, starts from the premise that any
which people make sense (i.e., lend meaning, coherence human nature that we are in a position to understand
and structure to their ongoing experience) and organize and render intelligible must have “a central kernel or
their actions. Although experienced as such, those orga- essence,” but it is rarely a strong constraint. According
nizing frameworks (also called cultural schemas, models, to this answer, the central kernel or essence of human
designs for living, modes of being) are not fully private nature consists of a heterogeneous collection of mutu-
and personal; they are shared. ally contradictory structures and inclinations, which are
differentially and selectively activated, brought “on-
Markus et al. go on to say:
line,” and given character and substance in the course of
Importantly, the contention here is that these group-based the historical experience of different cultural communi-
meanings and practices are not separate from observed be- ties. The motto “One mind, many mentalities: universal-
havior. They are not applied as interpretive frameworks ism without the uniformity” is the rallying cry for the
after “ behavior ” has occurred. Instead they are fully ac- interpretation of the claim that there may be multiple,
tive in the constitution of this behavior; they are the means
diverse psychologies rather than a single psychology.
by which people behave and experience, and thus should
This motto advertises a discipline founded on the
be taken into account in an analysis of this behavior. The
claim is that with respect to the psychological, the individ-
principle that the abstract potentialities and specific
ual level often cannot be separated from the cultural level. heterogeneous inclinations of the human mind are uni-
Many psychological processes are completely interde- versal but only gain character, substance, definition,
pendent with the meanings and practices of their relevant and motivational force (i.e., assume the shape of a func-
sociocultural contexts and this will result in systematic di- tioning mentality) as they are translated and trans-
versity in psychological functioning. It follows from this formed into and through the concrete actualities of some
perspective that there may be multiple, diverse psycholo- particular practice, activity setting, or way of life (Cole,
gies rather than a single psychology. 1990; D’Andrade, 1995; Goodnow et al., 1995; Green-
field, 1997; Greenfield et al., 2003; Lave, 1990; Markus
MULTIPLE, DIVERSE PSYCHOLOGIES et al., 1996; Much, 1992; Nisbett & Cohen, 1995; Ro-
goff, 1990; Shweder, 1991; Shweder & LeVine, 1984).
Perhaps, the central claim of cultural psychology (in The slogan connects current researchers in cultural psy-
contrast to other approaches to the study of conscious- chology with the intellectual ancestors of the field
ness and mental life) is that “ there may be multiple, di- (Vico, Herder, and others; Berlin, 1976) who, Kant- and
verse psychologies rather than a single psychology,” and Hegel-like, believed “Form without content is empty,
perhaps the central problematic of the field is to make content without form meaningless.”
sense of that provocative claim. Does such a claim entail For at least 200 years, a distinctive tenet of cultural
the denial of universals? If not, what universals of mind psychology has been the claim that the formal univer-
are entailed by cultural psychology? How are those uni- sals of mind and the content-rich particulars of any sus-
versals to be reconciled with the existence of diverse tainable mentality or way of life are interdependent,
psychologies across human populations without trivial- interactive, and give each other life. Scholars, such as
izing that diversity or treating it as mere content? Herder, Vico, and Wundt, scoured the historical record
Currently, there is no single answer that all cultural for successful (cohesive, shared, stable) fusions of form
psychologists would endorse. One type of answer, with a and content in which the human imagination has, of
pedigree stretching back to Vico (Berlin, 1976), sug- necessity, gone beyond the relatively meaning-barren
724 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

constraints of logic and mere sense perception to con- cause our theoretical language for psychological de-
struct an imaginative (and culture-specific) picture of scription will be contextual from the start. In part, cul-
the underlying nature of the world and it values, result- tural psychology involves the study of real things that do
ing in a mentality (the Homeric mentality, the Hindu not exist independently of some collectively shared
mentality, the Christian fundamentalist mentality) sup- point of view. Later in this chapter, we address in some
portive of a way of life. detail this issue, of dichotomies that need to be softened
They took as their data the great symbolic formations or recast (see also Overton, Chapter 2, this Handbook,
produced by human beings: myths, folk tales, language this volume).
patterns, naming systems, ethnoscientific doctrines, The distinction between cultural psychology and
and ethical, social, and religious philosophies. They also other contextual approaches in psychology is subtle, im-
took as their data the great behavioral formations portant, and easy to overlook because all approaches to
produced by human beings, including customary prac- psychology that emphasize context share much in
tices of various kinds: subsistence activities, games, common, especially their opposition to the idea that the
rituals, food taboos, gender roles, the division of labor, science of psychology is primarily the study of fixed,
and marriage rules. They interpreted those symbolic and universal, abstract processes or forms. Thus, cultural
behavioral formations as alternative manifestations, psychology shares with other contextual psychologies
substantializations, or instantiations of the disparate ab- the assumption that the mind of human beings ( know-
stract potentialities of the universal mind, which they ing, wanting, feeling, valuing, etc.) can only be realized
believed was the business of cultural psychology to char- through some situated or local process of “minding,”
acterize and to explain. which is always bounded, conditional, or relative to
something—shared meanings, goals, stimulus domain,
available resources, local artifacts, cognitive assistants,
THE MEANING OF MEANING AND A and so on. Beyond that general point of similarity, cul-
CONTEXT FOR CONTEXT IN tural psychology should be understood as a rather spe-
CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY cial type of contextual approach.
In the conception of cultural psychology developed in
In contemporary cultural psychology, the translation this chapter, the relevant contexts for the realization of
and transformation of one mind into many mentalities mind are the customs, traditions, practices, and shared
is typically conceptualized as a process by which con- meanings and perspectives of some self-monitoring and
texts and meanings become essential, active components self-perpetuating group. The primary emphasis is on
inside as well as outside the psychological system of contexts thought to be relevant for the realization of
individuals. In cultural psychology, this process is some- mind in the sense that such contexts are the means for
times described as the process by which culture and psy- transforming a universal mind into a distinctively func-
che make each other up. tioning mentality, a distinctive way that “people think
This insistence in cultural psychology that contexts and act in the light of particular goals, values and pic-
and meanings are to be theoretically represented as part tures of the world” (Berlin, 1976). In this approach,
of the psychological system and not simply as influ- cultural psychology is not coextensive with contextual
ences, factors, or conditions external to the psychologi- psychology (more on this in a moment). More important,
cal system distinguishes cultural psychology from other the contrast between inside and outside, person and con-
forms of psychology, which also think of themselves as text, and subjective perspective and external reality is
contextual (or situated). The aim in cultural psychology reconceptualized in cultural psychology as a process by
is not first to separate the psychological system from its which culture and psyche are constantly and continu-
nonpsychological context and then to invoke some type ously making each other up.
of external setting effect or outside situational influ-
ence on psychological functioning. The aim and the
challenge are rather to recast or soften the contrast be- THE UNIT OF ANALYSIS PROBLEM
tween person and context (inside versus outside, subjec-
tive perspective versus external reality) so that the very Just as the general field of psychology seems unsure
idea of a context effect will take on new meaning be- whether its proper subject matter should be the study of
The Unit of Analysis Problem 725

behavior or the study of consciousness or the study of tion between a mentality and a practice and a partial fu-
the mental life (which is a broader subject than the study sion of person /context, inside/outside, or subjective per-
of consciousness because it includes states of mind that spective/external reality.
are not in awareness), so too cultural psychologists do Examples of a custom complex are so commonplace
not always seem to agree on their proper unit of analysis. they are easy to overlook. They include the mentalities
Practitioners of cultural psychology study mentalities, associated with nursing on demand, co-sleeping in
folk models, practices, activity settings, situated cogni- a family bed, the family meal, enforcing strict Chris-
tions, and ways of life. It is not clear whether these units tian discipline, performing the ritual of “ what did
of analysis mentioned in the literature are different you do in school today,” or practicing ways to bolster
ways of speaking about the same intellectual object or self-esteem.
whether it is possible to combine them into a single unit
of analysis. A Custom Complex Example: Who Sleeps by
For the sake of clarity in this review, we adopt a pro- Whom in the Family
posal for a common unit of analysis for cultural psychol-
ogy put forward more than a generation ago (J. W. M. The mentality (what people know, think, feel, want,
Whiting & Child, 1953) in an exemplary collaboration value, and hence choose to do) intimately associated
between an anthropologist and a psychologist. Whiting with the practice of “ who sleeps by whom” in the family
and Child suggest combining mentalities and practices provides a paradigmatic example of a custom complex.
(the symbolic and behavioral inheritances of a cultural Who sleeps by whom in a family is a customary practice
community) into a single unit of analysis called the cus- invested with socially acquired meanings and with im-
tom complex, which “consists of a customary practice plications for a person’s standing (as moral, rational, or
and of the beliefs, values, sanctions, rules, motives and competent) in some consensus-sensitive and norm-
satisfactions associated with it ” (p. 27). If we adopt this enforcing cultural community.
proposal, cultural psychology can be defined as the Research on family life customs in different com-
study of the custom complex. munities in the United States (Abbott, 1992; Okami &
Although J. W. M. Whiting and Child introduced the Weisner, in press; Okami, Weisner, & Olmstead, 2002;
idea of a custom complex in 1953, its theoretical impli- Weisner, Bausano, & Kornfein, 1983) and around the
cations were not widely or fully appreciated at the time. world (Caudill & Plath, 1966; LeVine, 1989, 1990a,
Curiously, the idea was not taken up or carried forward 1990b; McKenna et al., 1993; Morelli, Rogoff, Oppen-
by psychological anthropologists working in the classi- heimer, & Goldsmith, 1992; Shweder, Balle-Jensen, &
cal tradition of the 1950s. It was not until the 1980s and Goldstein, 1995; J. W. M. Whiting, 1964, 1981) confirms
1990s, with the rebirth of a two-handed cultural psy- the existence on a worldwide scale of several divergent
chology focused on the way culture and psyche make custom complexes in this domain, each consisting of a
each other up, and with the return of an interest in “ac- network of interwoven and mutually supportive prac-
tivity settings” (Cole, 1992, 1995; Weisner, 1984, 1996, tices, beliefs, values, sanctions, rules, motives, and satis-
2001, 2002) and a “practice approach” to developmental factions. Indeed, on a worldwide scale, the European
studies (Goodnow et al., 1995), that J. W. M. Whiting American who-sleeps-by-whom custom complex is not
and Child’s conception gained currency and appeal. the one that communities most typically produce, repro-
If a custom complex “consists of a customary prac- duce, and enforce with the various formal and informal
tice and of beliefs, values, sanctions, rules, motives and powers (e.g., legal interventions, gossip, and effects on
satisfactions associated with it,” then the idea bears reputation) at their disposal.
some resemblance to the social psychologist’s idea of a The middle-class European American custom com-
personal “life space” (Lewin, 1943), to the sociologist’s plex includes the ritualized isolation of children during
idea of a societal “ habitus” (Bourdieu, 1972, 1990), and the night, the institution of bedtime, and the protection
to the historian’s idea of an epochal “mentality.” of the privacy of the sacred couple upheld by a cultural
Using the custom complex as a unit of analysis makes norm mandating the exclusive co-sleeping of the
it possible to conceptualize cultural psychology as the husband and wife. This European American custom
study of the way culture and psyche are socially pro- complex is typically associated with something like
duced and reproduced, resulting in an intimate associa- the following propositional attitudes, where knowing,
726 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

thinking, feeling, wanting, and valuing define the set of The cluster of propositional attitudes that lend au-
potential attitudes, and thus can be stated in proposi- thority to co-sleeping still need to be worked out for the
tional form: different culture regions of the world (although see
Morelli et al., 1992). The Japanese custom complex in-
I value autonomy and independence; I want my chil- cludes the propositional attitudes:
dren to become autonomous and independent adults; I
know that I can promote autonomy and independence I value and want to promote interdependency and
in infants and young children by having them sleep feelings of closeness and solidarity among members
alone; I value sexual intimacy with my spouse; I of the family; I know that co-sleeping will help chil-
know that a sleeping space is the most suitable site dren overcome feelings of distance and separation
for sexual intimacy with my spouse; I know that it from members of the family who are older or of a dif-
will not be possible to have sexual intimacy with my ferent sex.
spouse if the privacy of the spousal sleeping space is
violated; I know that children have erotic impulses The Oriya Hindu custom complex includes the proposi-
and a sexual fantasy life that should not be aroused or tional attitudes:
titillated by adults for the sake of the mental health
of the child; I don’t want to damage the mental health I highly value children as members of the family; I
of my children or make them unhappy and neurotic know that children are fragile, vulnerable, and needy
about sex or touching; I feel anxious about touching and therefore should not be left alone and unpro-
and having prolonged skin-to-skin contact with a tected during the night.
young child; therefore, infants and young children
should be trained, encouraged, and if necessary, Chastity anxiety and the chaperoning of adolescent fe-
forced, to sleep alone. males also play a part in the Oriya custom complex
(Shweder et al., 1995).
This custom complex is sanctioned, glorified, rational- Examples of the way local experts (pediatricians, ad-
ized, and enforced in innumerable ways in the European vice columnists, or social workers) rationalize, uphold,
American culture area, although nearly every one of and lend authority to the European American custom
those propositional attitudes is thought to be wrong, complex can be found in the responses of “Dear Abby”
bizarre, or beside the point by adults and children in and “Ann Landers” to the many letters they receive
many parts of Asia, Africa, and Central America, about the perceived problem of parent-child co-sleeping.
where children routinely and habitually co-sleep with The following, published May 26, 1994, in the Chicago
one or more of their parents and/or siblings and prefer Tribune, is a typical exchange between concerned adults
to do so even when more than ample sleeping space is in the European American cultural zone:
available for separate sleeping arrangements (Abbott,
1992; Brazelton, 1990; Caudill & Plath, 1966; Shweder Dear Abby: My niece—I’ll call her Carol—is a single
et al., 1995). mother with a 4-year-old son. (I’ll call him Johnny.)
In the early 1960s, Caudill and Plath (1966) discov- Carol just turned 40. Since the day Johnny was born, he
ered that (a) urban Japanese parents felt morally obliged has slept with his mother in a single bed. They go to bed
to provide their children with a parental sleeping part- between 8 and 10 o’clock every night and always have
ner, ( b) husbands and wives were willing to separate snacks and drinks in bed. They watch TV and cuddle until
from each other to do so, and (c) approximately 50% of Johnny falls asleep in his mother’s arms. Abby, this child
has never fallen asleep alone. Carol lives with her par-
11- to 15-year-old urban Japanese boys and girls slept in
ents, and there is no shortage of beds in their home. Re-
the same room as their mother or father or both. In an-
cently, Carol and Johnny visited me in my country home,
other example, Shweder et al. (1995) discovered from a and I gave them the bedroom with twin beds. The follow-
record of single-night sleeping arrangements in 160 ing morning, I discovered that Carol had pushed the beds
high-caste households in Orissa, India, that only 12% of together so she and Johnny wouldn’t be separated. I think
the cases matched the European American custom com- Carol’s emotional needs are taking precedence over what
plex in which husband and wife sleep together and sepa- is best for her son. He has no father, and his grandparents
rate from their children. have no say in his upbringing. I would appreciate your as-
The Unit of Analysis Problem 727

sessment of this situation. No city, please, and sign me, ioral inheritance of a cultural community). This analysis
Concerned Aunt. begins with the systematic identification, through ob-
Dear Concerned: You have good reason to be concerned, servation and interviews, of the routine or habitual fam-
You hit the nail on the head—Johnny doesn’t need to sleep ily life and social practices engaged in by members of
with his mother nearly as much as she needs to sleep with some self-monitoring and self-regulating group. Some
him. You would be doing Carol an enormous favor if you of these practices may surprise, disgust, or enrage an
advised her to get counseling in the rearing of her son. outside observer, although to the jaded eyes of the group
With all her good intentions, she is “(s)mothering” her son. members their own practices are likely to seem ordi-
Johnny’s pediatrician will be able to recommend the best nary, decent, and reasonable or at least “normal.”
counselor for Carol and Johnny. It is desperately needed. Each of the following practices, for example, is a com-
monplace way of being, at least for the members of the
Surprisingly little is known about the long-term particular cultural communities that uphold them. In one
effects of nighttime isolation or separation versus co- cultural world, a 2-year-old child gets in bed with his or
sleeping in any part of the world, which is a major la- her mother, unbuttons his or her mother’s blouse, suckles
cuna in the history of research in cultural psychology. at her breast, and sleeps by her side throughout the night.
Nevertheless, with the publication of an important lon- In another cultural world, each child in the family sleeps
gitudinal study by Okami et al. (2002; see also Okami & in a private sleeping space separated from the sleeping
Weisner, in press) there is now some empirical grounds space of all adults. In one cultural world, a woman brings
for being suspicious about any strong or generalized food home from the market and cooks it, and then she and
claims about the long-term effects on children of sleep- her husband consume the food together. In another cul-
ing alone versus co-sleeping with one or more parents. tural world, a man brings food home from the market, his
wife cooks it at home, and he consumes the food alone
and his wife eats separately and later. In one cultural
More on the Custom Complex: The Intimate
world, children are fostered by their parents to more
Association between a Mentality and a Practice
prosperous families in their society who subject these
Supported by a Cultural Community
children to ordeals of hardship, physical punishment, and
The concept of a custom complex presupposes an inti- demanding tests of loyalty, requiring them to work as
mate association between a mentality and a practice that family servants until they endure the ordeals and pass the
is supported, enforced, defended, and rationalized by tests (Bledsoe, 1990). Then the children are adopted and
members of some cultural community. When such an as- supported by those families and patronized and provided
sociation is in place, it will be the case that other mem- for throughout life. In another cultural world, however,
bers of the cultural community will judge the mentality parents get upset (even incensed) if another adult touches
associated with the practice to be normal and reason- their child, reprimands or scolds their child, makes stren-
able, while any actual participant in the practice will ex- uous demands of their child, or causes their child to suf-
perience the mentality associated with the practice to be fer abuse in any way.
under the skin, close to the heart, and self-relevant; the As noted earlier, the analysis of a custom complex
mentality will have become habitual, automatic, and can ends when one is able to spell out as comprehensively as
be activated without deliberation or conscious calcula- possible the things that the members of some group (tac-
tion—it will have become internalized. This intimate itly or explicitly, consciously or unconsciously) know,
(some might say experience-near) connection or partial think, feel, want, and value that explain and make intel-
fusion of a mentality and a practice does not, however, ligible the things that they do. Thus, the analysis begins
prohibit us from drawing an analytic distinction between with the identification of practices and it ends with the
the mentality and the practice that instantiates it. It does specification of a distinctive mentality.
not keep us from characterizing the custom complex as This interest in the distinctive mentality associated
two things intimately connected or partially fused. with the practices of a cultural community distinguishes
The study of a custom complex calls for the analysis cultural psychology from other approaches to the study of
of a two-sided thing—the intimate connection between a practice domains in which it is assumed that human activ-
mentality (the symbolic inheritance of a cultural com- ities come in natural domains or universal kinds (e.g., re-
munity) and one or more specific practices (the behav- ligion, economics, family life, schooling, or politics) and
728 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

that members of different cultural communities think and rium (the intimate association of a local mentality and a
behave more or less alike because of the strong con- cultural practice) has actually been achieved.
straints of each species of activity, regardless of commu-
nity. The idea of a custom complex invites a very THE CLASSIFICATION OF PRACTICES
different approach in which it is assumed that members of
various cultural communities have distinctive mentalities To conduct a relatively complete and systematic empiri-
associated with each of their practice domains (e.g., a cal study of a community’s cultural psychology, it is nec-
Taiwanese mentality of family life versus a New England essary to identify the members’ practices and categorize
mentality of family life), leading members of those cul- them into domains. Practices can be categorized in many
tural communities to engage in divergent patterns of be- ways, because any scheme of classification will depend
havior in ostensibly similar domains. largely on the investigator’s theory of human needs
The idea of a custom complex also invites cultural (physical, social, psychological, and spiritual) and the re-
psychologists to address the question of whether a par- search issues at hand.
ticular cultural community has a characteristic mental- One of the several ways practices can be classified into
ity (e.g., the Protestant mentality), which leaves its domains is from an ontogenetic perspective, with special
generalized mark on many domains in that community, reference to the development of mastery or expertise
thereby making, for example, Protestant economics, in some domain of psychological functioning ( knowing,
Protestant religion, and Protestant family life more like thinking, feeling, wanting, or valuing). Thus, practices
each other than like a parallel natural domain in an- might be identified and classified by reference to the par-
other cultural community. ticular substantive type of competence they promote (e.g.,
We emphasize, however, that cultural psychology practices promoting social sensitivity, practices promoting
does not presume the existence of global consistency or moral development, practices promoting cognitive devel-
thematic integration across all practice domains in a opment). For example, a recent study (Munroe, in press) of
culture. Even Ruth Benedict (1934) was quite aware 3- to 9-year-olds in four cultures (the Logoli of Kenya,
that many cultures are not patterned after some simple Newars of Nepal, Black Carib of Belize, and American
mold (Dionysian, Apollonian) or fundamentally inte- Samoans) produced the counter-intuitive and provocative
grated by a single theme (e.g., the work ethic). She finding that children are more willing to engage in oppo-
knew, as we know, that the degree to which a small set site gender sex role play and seem less threatened by sex
of core beliefs, goals, or motives can account for the role confusions in societies that have institutionalized
meaning and behavior of a people across the many do- adult patriarchal or patricentric practices such as male
mains of their life (family, work, and politics) is en- dominance, gender segregation, and a strict sexual divi-
tirely an open empirical issue. sion of labor. Although one can only speculate based on
There is no way to know in advance of years of re- the data presented in Munroe’s study, one might entertain
search in some particular cultural community whether the hypothesis that where gender is culturally sanctioned
their many practice domains all draw on the same men- as a basis for social organization the interest and capacity
tality. Nevertheless, even if a particular cultural commu- to imaginatively take the perspective of the other across
nity is not thematically integrated (one small set of core the gender division is more highly developed.
meanings revealed in many practice domains), the cus- Or a developmentalist might classify practices not so
tom complex is still a natural theoretical frame of analy- much according to the substantive competence (e.g., tak-
sis for cultural psychology. The idea defines a parameter ing the perspective of others) acquired but rather accord-
space for conceptualizing and modeling the ways that ing to types of processes of acquisition. Werker (1989;
culture and psyche make each other up, resulting, on a also G. Gottlieb, 1991), has generated a short list of hy-
worldwide scale, in multiple instances of a relatively pothetical ways that experience (read exposure to or ac-
stable or equilibrated condition in which a mentality and tive participation in a cultural practice) can affect the
a practice are mutually sustaining and reciprocally con- development of any mental skill or ability. She imagines
firmatory. Not all custom complexes are integrated in five kinds of processes:
the same way or cohere to the same degree. Neverthe-
less, the idea makes it possible for us to ask about the 1. Maturation (the practice made no difference; the
ways and degrees to which a relatively stable equilib- ability would have developed without it).
The Classification of Practices 729

2. Facilitation ( because of the practice, the ability was tural psychology project has general implications for
attained more quickly than otherwise would have claims about mental development that are quite indepen-
been the case). dent of any particular findings in any particular domain.
3. Induction (without the practice, there would have For example, comparative research by Ross et al.
been no ability at all in this domain). (2003) has suggested that cognitive developmental theo-
4. Attunement ( because of the practice, a higher level ries presumptively positing a universal anthropocentric
of ability was attained than otherwise would have stage in the development of children’s folk biological
been the case). knowledge (the idea that young children everywhere
initially project a naive human psychology onto nonhu-
5. Maintenance/ loss (the ability was preexisting but
man species) are more accurately viewed as local
would have been lost or deactivated if it had not been
descriptions of the course of mental development for
kept online through participation in the practice).
urban majority children who grow up with an impover-
At this early stage in the evolution of a cultural psy- ished experience of nonhuman nature. Native American
chology of individual development, we can only look for- Menominee children and rural children from main-
ward with excitement to the time when we will have in stream populations in the United States, whose involve-
hand the research designs, methodologies, and systemati- ment with plants and nonhuman animals is positively
cally collected bodies of evidence that will allow us to structured and mediated by cultural beliefs and prac-
classify practices in this way. We look forward to the time tices of various sorts, do not display the predicted uni-
when we will be able to distinguish between each of those versal developmental pattern and exhibit competences
five interpretations of the effects of participation in a cul- in ecological reasoning that are absent from the mental-
tural practice on the growth of a mental state or ability. ity of the urban, mainstream kids in the United States.
Cultural psychology is, however, not committed to a There are many other ways to classify cultural prac-
blank-slate learning theory (the blank-slate stance is a tices into domains. From the point of view of personal
straw person, and not even John Locke posited an en- and social identity, cultural practices might be identi-
tirely empty organism prior to learning from childhood fied and classified by the existential problems they ad-
experience) nor does it presuppose an induction theory dress. In any society, there are many existential
of mental development. Quite the contrary, much of the questions, which must be answered for the sake of both
current research in cultural psychology is quite compati- individual mental health and social coordination:
ble with (and may even presuppose) either an attunement
• Self practices answer: “What’s me or mine, and
or a maintenance/ loss account of the differential emer-
what’s not me or mine?”
gence, activation, or selective maintenance of particular
mental states. Our conception of cultural learning is dis- • Gender practices answer: “What’s male, and what’s
cussed later, especially in relationship to innate ideas. female?”
In this chapter, we can seldom choose between differ- • Disciplinary practices answer: “How are norms and
ent interpretations (maturation, attunement, mainte- rules to be enforced?”
nance/loss, etc.) of how participation in a cultural • Distributional practices answer: “How should bur-
practice affects the activation of a mental state or the dens and benefits be distributed?” (Shweder, 1982)
emergence of a mental skill. What we can do, however, as
an intermediary step in building a full-blown cultural psy- A closely related approach has been proposed by Fiske
chology of individual development, is point to some of the (1991, 1992), who argues that social life is comprised of
research and scholarship in cultural psychology that tries four social relationships: communal sharing, authority
to describe and explain the differential ontogenetic emer- ranking, equality matching, and market pricing. Fiske’s
gence, activation, and selective maintenance of what the scheme could readily be adapted and used in the classi-
“I’s” in different groups know, think, feel, want, value, fication of practices (practices promoting a sense of
and hence choose to do, including research about the commonality, the importance and legitimacy of hierar-
“self.” Later in this chapter, we examine one important chy, etc.). Some researchers may prefer to identify and
line of cultural psychological research on the development classify practices by the institutions in which they are
of an interdependent (sociocentric, collective) versus in- embedded (e.g., family-life practices, school-life prac-
dependent (autonomous, individualistic) self, but the cul- tices). Other researchers, with different intellectual
730 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

aims and inclinations, may prefer to classify practices domains for some groups in the United States (Markus &
according to the biological needs or physical survival Kitayama, 1991a, 1991b; Triandis, 1989, 1990).
functions they serve (e.g., eating practices, health prac- Thus, although cultural psychology is, in one major
tices, or sexual practices). sense, the study of the way culture and psyche make each
Still others may want to proceed emically (Pike, 1967), other up; in another closely related sense, it is also the
which involves letting the classification of practice do- study of the origin, structure, function, operation, and
mains go hand in hand with the specification of the men- social reproduction of that intimate association between
tality of a cultural community, in the anticipation of some a mentality and a practice known as the custom complex.
counterintuitive and astonishing results. In some cultural
communities, for example, among devout Brahmans in
India, there is a highly elaborated practice domain that THE TWO SIDES OF
might be labeled oblations, sacrifices, and sacramental of- CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY
ferings. It encompasses the daily preparation and con-
sumption of food and includes in the same general Cultural psychology is the study of the way culture and
practice domain other activities (e.g., prayer and animal psyche make each other up, resulting in the formation of
sacrifice) that would never naturally go together in the the custom complex, which is a unit of analysis for
mentality of a Western researcher. Among Hindu Brah- characterizing the way multiple, diverse psychologies
mans in India, food is not a personal preference system. emerge out of the abstract potentialities of a universal
Given the local culturally elaborated idea that eating is a mind. Psychological pluralism emerges, at least in part,
sacramental offering to a divinity (the self is conceptual- because peoples think and act in the light of particular
ized as a piece of divinity) residing in a temple (the human goals, values, and pictures of the world. Those factors
body), what you eat, how and by whom it has been pre- are rarely the same across cultural communities.
pared, and the conditions under which you eat is a mark of The cultural side of cultural psychology is the study
your moral standing and social status in the world. of the mentality-laden practices (including the symbolic
forms, communicative exchanges, rituals, mores, folk-
ways, and institutions) developed, promoted, promul-
THE ANALYSIS OF MENTALITIES gated, enacted, and enforced (and hence judged to be
customary, normal, legal, moral, or reasonable) by the
Mentalities are the other side of the custom complex. “I’s” (the subjects, agents, individuals, or selves) of par-
They can be investigated in the following ways: (a) by ticular groups.
analyzing the idea of a mentality into its component The psychological side of cultural psychology is the
parts: knowing, thinking, feeling, wanting, and valuing; study of practice-related mental states, the things that
( b) by modeling what some ideal or prototypical “I” the “I’s” (subjects, agents, individuals, selves) of partic-
(subject, agent, individual, or self ) who might be en- ular groups know, think, feel, want, value, and hence
gaged in this or that practice might know, think, feel, choose or decide to do to carry forward the normal prac-
want, and value; (c) by empirically determining the de- tices of their society.
gree of specificity or generality of those components of Based on those two sides of cultural psychology,
a mentality for actual agents across practice domains in which are fused in the idea of a custom complex, the aim
a cultural community (and perhaps across cultural com- of the discipline is to investigate precisely those cases
munities for a particular practice domain); and (d) by where the following three conditions hold:
pointing to broad patterns of generality for mentalities
when and where they exist. 1. A “practice” displays significant variation across
For example, there is good empirical reason to believe groups and differential patterning of within-group
that the mentality dubbed interdependency, sociocen- variations (e.g., there is a far greater probability of
trism, or collectivism supports and maintains a whole children and adults co-sleeping in a family bed in
array of practices both in and across domains for some South Asia and Africa than in Europe and the United
populations in Japan, while the mentality dubbed inde- States and the correlation between social status and
pendence, autonomy, or individualism supports and main- co-sleeping is not the same in South Asia and in the
tains a disparate array of practices both in and across United States).
Cultural Psychology’s Theory of Mind 731

2. The components of a mentality ( knowing, thinking, feel, want, value, and hence decide to do that are condi-
feeling, wanting, and valuing), such as feelings of tional, optional, or discretionary and are primed and
closeness, pleasure, and serenity versus feelings of activated through participation in the symbolic and be-
anxiety or agitation associated with skin-to-skin con- havioral inheritance of particular groups. In effect, cul-
tact between parent and child, display significant tural psychology is a discipline committed to the study
variation across groups and differential patterning of of patterns of psychological difference across groups or
within-group variation (e.g., European American subgroups and to the investigation of the emergence
males, in comparison to South Asian males, are more (and dissolution) of stable, relatively coherent, and inti-
likely to feel anxiety associated with skin-to-skin mate interconnections between cultural practices and
contact between parent and child and feelings of individual mental states.
closeness, pleasure, and serenity produced by skin- Any study of difference, however, presupposes many
to-skin contact between parent and child may be cor- commonalities, likenesses, or universals by which attri-
related with gender in the United States, but not in butions of difference become intelligible. A notable fea-
South Asia). ture of our conception of cultural psychology is that it
3. The distribution of the practice appears to be related presupposes certain universal truths about what is (and
to the distribution of the mentality, and vice versa. what is not) inherent in human psychological function-
ing. At a minimum, we are committed to a theory of
Thus, through the idea of a custom complex, cultural mind in which everywhere in the world human beings
psychology joins the study of individual mental states to are the kind of beings who have a mental life (who know,
the study of cultural practices. On the one hand, investi- think, and use language and other symbolic forms) and
gators explore those features of what individuals know, who feel, want, and value certain things, which is one
think, feel, want, value, and hence choose to do that are way to explain what they do (Donagan, 1987).
primed by, traceable to, or derivable from participation Even more deeply, we are committed to the view that
in the symbolic forms, communicative exchanges, ritu- psyche consists of certain mental powers. Most notable
als, mores, folkways, and institutions of some consensus- of these are (a) the representational power to form be-
sensitive or norm-enforcing group. liefs about other persons, society, nature, the divine, and
On the other hand, investigators look at the way in about means-ends connections of all sorts; and ( b) the
which the mentality-laden practices (the custom com- intentional power to affect an imagined future state of
plexes) of particular groups gain their credibility, rea- affairs by means of acts of the will, which is the human
sonableness, and motivational force from the very capacity to have a causal influence on the world through
psychological states that they have helped activate and acts of decision making and choice.
to which they have given life. Cultural psychology is If the power of representation is an essential feature
therefore the study of reciprocal connections between of the human psyche, then the human psyche can be
culture and psyche and of the various patterns or forms studied, at least in part, as a knowledge structure. If the
of coherency (custom complexes) that have arisen out of power of intentionality is an essential feature of the
their interactions. human psyche, then the human psyche can be studied, at
least in part, as inherently ends-sensitive, which is min-
imally what it means to be agentic or to have a free will.
CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY’S THEORY This view of the inherent powers of the psyche accords
OF MIND reasonably well with William James’s (1950) description
of the marks of the “mental.” According to James:
On a worldwide scale, there is well-documented diver-
The pursuance of future ends and the choice of means for
sity in the developmentally relevant cultural practices
their attainment are thus the mark and criterion of the pres-
that promote, sustain, and confirm what the “I’s” ence of mentality in a phenomenon. We all use this test to
of particular groups know, think, feel, want, value, and distinguish between an intelligent and a mechanical perfor-
hence choose to do. Consequently, cultural psychology mance. We impute no mentality to sticks and stones be-
is concerned not only with the inherent, mandatory, or cause they never seem to move for the sake of anything, but
fundamental aspects of the human mind but also, indeed always when pushed and then indifferently and with no sign
especially, with those parts of what people know, think, of choice. So we unhesitatingly call them senseless. . . . No
732 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

actions but such as are done for an end, and show a choice haps irresolvable) mind-body or mind-brain problem.
of means, can be indubitable expressions of Mind. (p. 1) Not all types of explanation of human behavior assume
that mind matters, in the sense of having causal effects
As noted earlier the anthropological linguists Anna on the body. Cultural psychology makes the assumption
Wierzbicka (1986, 1991) and Cliff Goddard (1997, that mental states are real, not epiphenomenal.
2001) have shown that the notion of a mental subject or This suggests one additional power inherent in the
agent (“I”) and mental state concepts such as to know, human psyche—the ability to translate or transform a
think, feel, want, and value (as good or bad) are lexical- self-conscious deliberative process into a routine, auto-
ized in all languages of the world and universally used matic, unconscious, or habitual process. This power to
in folk psychology to explain what people do. And it has turn a slow calculative process into a rapid response
been argued by Collingwood (1961, pp. 303, 306; see process prepares the individual to respond skillfully,
also Shweder et al., 1997), among many others, that at smoothly, and not self-consciously (indeed almost
least one basic sense of the folk psychology concept of a mindlessly) in particular ways in particular circum-
“cause” is the idea of “a free and deliberate act of a con- stances. When this translation or transformation is fully
scious and responsible agent ” that is best understood accomplished, the associated mentality comes to be inti-
through the ends the agent is trying to achieve and the mate and seems to be implicit in the practice.
means the agent believes are available for achieving As J. W. M. Whiting and Child (1953) pointed out
them. With respect to its picture of the component parts long ago, with respect to the beliefs implicit in a prac-
of a mentality, folk psychology and cultural psychology tice: “ The performer of a practice does not necessarily
presuppose pretty much the same picture of the univer- consciously rehearse the belief to himself at each per-
sal and inherent features of the human psyche. Those formance. [For example, a typical middle-class Euro-
marks of the mental include representation, intentional- pean American parent does not necessarily consciously
ity, knowing, thinking, feeling, wanting, valuing, and think to herself or himself ‘I know that I can promote
hence deciding to do something. autonomy and independence in infants and young chil-
Although cultural psychology is primarily concerned dren by having them sleep alone’ every time she or he
with the emergence and development of psychic plural- goes to bed at night.] If asked, however, she or he will
ism, it makes use of a restricted set of mental state con- generally be able to report immediately at least some of
cepts as a universal framework for understanding the the associated beliefs; in this case one may surmise that
organization of psychological differences between the rehearsal of the belief was not part of the stimulus pat-
“I’s” of different groups. The nature and organization tern for the present performance of the custom but
of such differences and the manner of their development rather a significant part of the stimulus pattern earlier in
are discussed in the following section. the development of the custom” (p. 28).
This comment by J. W. M. Whiting and Child is im-
portant for two reasons. First, it highlights the develop-
CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY’S SPECIAL USE mental process of becoming unconscious, whereas most
OF MENTAL STATE CONCEPTS developmental theorists, from Vygotsky to Piaget to
Kohlberg, privilege the developmental process of be-
In cultural psychology, mental state concepts are used to coming conscious or reflective. Whiting’s and Child’s
refer to the causal powers inherent in the mental nature implication that much of social behavior is habitual and
of human beings. Such concepts are not necessarily automatic and that social life would not be possible if
meant to be descriptions of bits of human consciousness this were not so accords well with the views of Bourdieu
or of deliberative awareness. (1972, 1990, 1991), Packer (1987), and others who are
One can use a mental state concept to explain what concerned with the difference between participating in
people do without necessarily assuming that the mental the world and consciously deliberating about it.
events in question are events in consciousness. What Bourdieu argues that as practices are repeated again
a person knows, thinks, wants, values is not always in and again, they come to be seen as part of a natural
front of that individual as a piece of awareness, even as it order, and their original explicit reasons for occurrence
plays a causal role in how the person acts. How that may be difficult to resurrect. Packer makes the point
causal process operates and produces its effects is a that development typically involves becoming more flu-
mystery that is at the heart of the unresolved (and per- ent at some activity and that this is not necessarily the
Social Development in the Divergent Interpersonal Worlds of Childhood 733

same as becoming more reflective about that activity (as the story of the progressive shift from deliberation and
any serious athlete surely knows; see also Keil, Chapter self-consciousness to mindless or intuitive fluency. It is a
14, this Handbook, Volume II ). developmental story that has rarely been acknowledged
The idea of the custom complex and the return of re- in child development studies, except perhaps by those in-
search interest to the study of routine or habitual prac- terested in the acquisition of physical skills such as
tice is an invitation to rethink some basic and classical walking down stairs, typing a letter, or hitting a golf ball.
ideas about the nature of development (on the intellec-
tual history of the idea of “ habit ” see Charles Camic,
1986). More needs to be said about the misguided idea SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT IN THE
that one can define progressive development as some DIVERGENT INTERPERSONAL WORLDS
standard formal criterion such as the shift from intuition OF CHILDHOOD
to reflection or from context-boundedness to context-
independence (Kessen, 1990). From the perspective of cultural psychology, the local
One can add to the classical image of progressive di- world of the child—especially in those dimensions
rectional change an indefinitely large series of other di- likely to affect behavioral and psychological develop-
chotomies. Somewhere or other in the vast literature ment—is largely mediated through culture-specific
on cognitive development, someone or other has argued mentalities and practices of child rearing. In document-
that the fully developed mind is complex (versus sim- ing variations across populations, cultural psychology
ple), complete (versus incomplete), explicit (versus first considers how responsible caregivers and educa-
tacit), impersonal (versus personal), taxonomic (versus tors, with special attention to the local ideas and mean-
associative), elaborated (versus restricted), concept- ings that support their behavior, routinely organize the
driven (versus percept-driven), detached (versus affect- child’s experience. If people think and act in the light of
laden), consistent (versus inconsistent), and so on. As particular goals, values, and pictures of the world, what
should be apparent from our discussion of the custom are the goals, values, and pictures of the world (the men-
complex and the developmental advantages of tacit un- tality) of members of different cultural communities?
derstandings, habits, and unreflective but fluent skills, Are there any generalizations that can be made about
cultural psychology is deeply suspicious of any attempt how and why differences arise in children’s worlds and
to define progressive development by universal (decon- how they are structured?
textualized) formal criteria. In some cases, cognitive As portrayed in the anthropological literature, varia-
development is the process of becoming less reflective tions in childhood worlds across human populations can
not more reflective. Again, at times, the accumulation of be roughly divided into three categories corresponding
tacit understanding is what intellectual growth is all to the material, social, and cultural conditions for child
about. It all depends. development (R. A. LeVine, 1989). First, material con-
The second reason for the importance of J. W. M. ditions include diet, housing, infant holding devices,
Whiting’s and Child’s (1953) comment is that it under- and forms of protection against disease and other health
scores the point that any adequate investigation into the risks. Second, social conditions include the family, peer
cultural psychology of a person or a people—any de- groups, and other aspects of the interpersonal environ-
scription of a custom complex—must characterize the ment. Third, cultural conditions refer to the local
level of consciousness of the mentality that is associated ideational models, combining beliefs and moral norms
with a particular cultural practice. Are the relevant be- that give meaning to all features of the child’s world as
liefs, values, motives, and satisfactions active without well as to the child’s development.
deliberation, active because of deliberation, reportable The focus in this section is on interpersonal aspects of
reflections, unavailable to reflection, and so on? When it the child’s world, as mediated by differing cultures
comes to participation in the custom complexes of any throughout the world. A considerable body of evidence on
particular cultural community, to what extent is the this subject has accumulated over the past 35 years (since
course of development from the deliberate to the auto- a review of the literature appearing in the third edition of
matic, from the self-conscious to the fluent, or from the the present work; see R. A. LeVine, 1970, and even more
explicated to the tacitly understood? At the very least, since Margaret Mead’s review in the Handbook’s first
the cultural psychology of development into the custom- edition in 1931), permitting some generalizations about
ary practices of any cultural community is likely to be the range of variation in children’s worlds and their
734 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

meanings. The interpersonal worlds of children from economic activities as well as for family residence. In
birth to adolescence in different cultural communities urban-industrial societies like the United States, in
vary widely along dimensions that can be described in which only 2% of adults engage in food production, chil-
quantitative and qualitative terms and that indicate diver- dren are more likely to be raised in home settings
gent pathways for behavioral and psychological develop- specifically designed for child care and segregated from
ment—particularly when analyzed from the perspectives adult economic activity.
of interactional theories of development. This difference between cultural worlds in which
First, we begin by describing how differing organiza- work and family have been merged versus cultural
tional settings, caregiving relationships, parental prac- worlds in which work and family have been separated
tices, and age-graded participation in activities provide (in some cases, as in the upper middle-class European
divergent patterns of socially and symbolically mediated American cultural area, with the family functioning
experience for children of different cultures. Second, we more or less like a Montessori School) makes a great
turn to the cultural mentalities that not only rationalize deal of difference for children. Where home is the set-
and legitimize these social patterns but also motivate ting for food or craft production, the attention of moth-
parental behavior. Third, we consider to what extent cul- ers is more often divided between child care and other
turally differentiated social experience during childhood demanding tasks. The family is then more likely to oper-
affects the psychological development of individuals— ate as a command hierarchy, with children at the bottom,
their attachments, skills, competence, preferences, rela- and children are more likely to be spectators of a wide
tionships, and emotional experience as adults. Finally, range of adult activities and to participate in them from
we attempt to generalize about universals and variations an early age (Rogoff, Mistry, Göncü, & Mosier, 1993).
in social development and their implications for develop- A family that functions as an economic production unit,
mental theory and research. like that of many Third World people today and prein-
dustrial Europe and North America, constitutes a dis-
tinctive world of childhood in which child labor is
THE SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF expected and children’s play and education must be ac-
CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCE commodated to the workplaces and routines of the home.
The actual amount of children’s labor contributions
In this section we discuss the character and composition in such families varies from one agricultural people to
of domestic groups and variations in their function, size, another (Nag, White, & Peet, 1978). Among those with
density, boundedness. low-level technology, like the peoples of sub-Saharan
Africa, children may have to work a great deal at tasks
they can do such as fetching water, herding animals, car-
Organizational Settings
ing for babies, and assisting in cultivation. This permits
For the first few years of life and often much longer, the adults to concentrate on the heavier or more skilled
children in most societies are raised in domestic groups tasks of hoeing, planting, weeding, harvesting, and food
(the normal residential homes of the adults who care for processing. Among peoples with a higher level of agri-
them). The functions of these groups, and their size, cultural technology including irrigation, draft animals,
composition, social density, and boundedness—all vari- and plows (e.g., rural villagers of India), the need for
able across cultures—influence the quantity and quality domestic labor is less and children may be indulged and
of social experience possible for a child in a given soci- have more free time. The actual utility of child labor in
ety. Many of these features, and the sociospatial arrange- a particular setting, however, depends on the specific
ment of the family as a domestic group as a whole, are crops cultivated, their seasonal cycles, the availability
often not matters of personal choice but are standardized of resources, such as water, and whether children can be
in local practice according to the dominant mode of eco- hired outside the family. When new technology is intro-
nomic production and prevailing ideas of morality. duced, the situation changes, and children may be freed
from labor, unless they are drafted into craft production
The Function of Domestic Groups at home or sent elsewhere as hired hands.
In societies with domestic agricultural or craft produc- In foraging (i.e., hunting-gathering) and fishing
tion, where every family engages in productive work at communities and among pastoral nomads, children also
home, children are raised in local settings designed for participate in productive activities at early ages ( by the
The Social Organization of Childhood Experience 735

standards of contemporary industrialized societies), but Among the Gusii of southwestern Kenya, a married
the degree to which they are confined or free to play in woman and her younger children live in a house by
the course of the day and the year varies with the themselves, but it is a unit embedded in a homestead
rhythm of the work cycle. Similar to agricultural com- owned by her husband or father-in-law, along with the
munities, domestic economic production largely deter- (nearby) houses of her parents-in-law, brothers-in-law,
mines the functional world of children’s social lives. and co-wives. If her husband is a polygamist, he may
live in the houses of his other wives all or part of the
The Size of Domestic Groups
time or even in a hut of his own separate from all of
The number of persons coresiding in domestic units is them, though near enough for children to bring him hot
extremely variable among human societies, and al- food from their houses.
though some of this variation depends on the definition Furthermore, as the children get older, they leave the
of the unit, it is certain that the nuclear family house- mother’s house to sleep in the house of an older brother
hold of Europe and North America is among the (for boys) or a grandmother (for girls), all within the
smallest in the world. Anthropologists have reported homestead. The Gusii mother-child household is the ele-
large domestic groups (up to and more than a hundred) mentary unit of family residence, but the homestead is
under a single roof or surrounded by a single wall in the basic unit of domestic social life from the viewpoint
places as diverse as New Guinea, lowland South Amer- of adults, and its male members form the nucleus of
ica, West Africa, and indigenous North America: Al- a local patrilineage (R. A. LeVine et al., 1994; R. A.
though such groups have internal social boundaries, LeVine & LeVine, 1966). This complex composition of
they certainly provide a child of any age with opportu- domestic groups is common to many nonindustrial soci-
nities for interacting with many and diverse persons eties and often means that the child grows up in a more
most of the time. complex residential environment than that of the average
This is also true, in a more limited way, of societies American child.
with extended or joint family structures in which the do-
mestic unit encompasses two or more nuclear families of The Social Density of Domestic Groups
two or more generations. B. B. Whiting and Whiting The interactive settings in which children spend their
(1975) pointed out that when adult women share cook- early lives—including those of eating, sleeping, work,
ing facilities and yard space, they are more likely to in- and play—vary widely in social density across cultures
teract with each other’s children and cooperate in child regardless of the size and compositional complexity of
care. The joint families of India are an example, as are domestic groups. Gusii children may grow up in a home-
the large compounds of the Yoruba of southwestern stead with as many as 58 inhabitants but spend all their
Nigeria, the smaller compounds of the Giriama of hours in and around their mother’s house, interacting
coastal Kenya (Wenger, 1989), and the Hausa of north- only with mother and older siblings during the pre-
western Nigeria (R. A. LeVine, LeVine, Iwanaga, & school years.
Marvin, 1970; Marvin, VanDevender, Iwanaga, LeVine, In contrast, Hausa children, in a much smaller com-
& LeVine, 1977). In all these environments, the sheer pound, may experience greater social density because
size of the domestic group guarantees that the child will the sharing of cooking facilities and yard space among
interact with a large number of women and children the Hausa women in a walled compound creates more
from infancy onward. crowded settings for daily interaction involving chil-
dren. The social density a child experiences, especially
The Composition of Domestic Groups
during the less mobile early years of life, depends not
In contemporary urban-industrial societies, the domestic only on the wealth or resources of the family but also on
group is coterminous with the household, and composi- the rules that govern family interaction. It seems hard
tion of households with children can usually be classified for Americans and Europeans to believe that people in
by whether one or both parents reside there and whether other cultures may enjoy, indeed prefer, crowded set-
there are other adults such as grandparents. It is more tings in which to eat, sleep, work, play, and even breast-
complicated among agrarian and other nonindustrial so- feed babies (Tronick, Morelli, & Winn, 1987), but such
cieties in which households as physical structures can be preferences are widespread among the world’s peoples,
situated in larger domestic units usually referred to as even when they have enough domestic space in which to
compounds or homesteads by anthropologists. carry on these activities in isolation.
736 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

The Boundedness of Domestic Groups fostering and adoption practices have been documented
Interactive patterns in the child’s world are constrained in Micronesia (Carroll, 1970) and West Africa (Bledsoe,
by the social boundaries recognized by adults. Bound- 1989). In these cases, young children are distributed
aries can be physical in form, like the mud walls of a among kin, often to mothers and sisters of the women
Yoruba or Hausa compound or the cultivated fields that who gave birth to them, after a period of breast-feeding
divide the mother-child households of a Gusii home- by the mother. There is usually no effort to disguise the
stead from each other. Boundaries can also be invisible original relationship, and children often go back to their
or conceptual barriers, as in the local traditions of inter- mothers after a period of years. Although some mothers
household visiting, greeting, and hospitality that limit do this because they feel obliged to meet the demands of
the interaction of children and adults in many Western their own mothers or sisters, they usually also feel that
and Japanese urban neighborhoods. the child will benefit from additional sponsorship, as
In urban India, by contrast, there are middle-class Goody (1982) has described for the fostering of older
apartment dwellings occupied by kin-related families children among the Gonja of Ghana. All these practices
whose children wander in and out of each other’s homes are infused with the assumptions of a kinship ideology
without such restriction. From the children’s perspec- in which children are seen as belonging to, and as poten-
tive, the permeability of the household and other domes- tial beneficiaries of, a descent group wider than the bio-
tic units in the immediate environment provides the logical parents. Mothers who do not care for their own
basis of a cognitive map of their social world. children are not viewed as irresponsible or neglectful in
these cultural communities.
In a much larger range of societies, children are
raised by their mothers, though often with help from
Care-Giving Relationships
others such as sibling caregivers, grandmothers and
Mothers are the primary caregivers of their children for other related adult women, and fathers or other men.
at least the first 2 years of life in most human societies, Sibling care of infants is widespread not only in sub-
but there are significant exceptions, and there is even Saharan Africa (where it is ubiquitous) but also in Ocea-
greater variation in the array of supplementary care- nia, Okinawa, and parts of Southeast Asia (Weisner,
givers who assist mothers and form relationships with 1982, 1987, 1989a, 1989b; Weisner & Gallimore, 1977).
young children. The ethnographic record as a whole does It is more frequent where mothers have extensive re-
not suggest that there is a single system for human child sponsibility for agriculture.
care but rather a range of parental patterns flexible The practice of sibling caretaking raises the question
enough to respond to and enable varying economic, de- of whether leaving infants in the care of 5- to 10-year-old
mographic, and technological conditions with diverse children, which would be considered criminal neglect in
care-giving arrangements that affect the interpersonal the United States, harms babies when it has achieved the
experience of the growing child. status of a custom complex and is the routine practice of
When women have a heavy workload due to a primary an entire population.
role in food production, then the resultant scarcity of fe- From the available evidence, the answer to this ques-
male labor may create a demand for supplementary care- tion is: No, babies are not harmed by this practice. For
giving arrangements. When children are scarce relative several reasons, first, 5-year-old children can be, and
to adult women (due to high rates of infertility, infant are, trained to be responsibly protective, if not necessar-
and child mortality, or contraception), adult women who ily sensitive, caregivers, particularly for babies carried
are infertile or postmenopausal may be eager to take on the back. Second, child care is largely conducted in
care of young children born to others. When wet nurses the open air during the day, and neighbors are within
or synthetic milk formulas become available, maternal earshot in case anything goes wrong. Third, the child
breast-feeding may decline. Thus, variations in caregiv- nurse is not expected to substitute for the mother in a
ing practices and relationships are generated by the dif- general sense, but simply to complement her care by pro-
fering conditions to which human populations adapt. tecting and feeding the baby for a few hours at a time.
There are some human populations in which a major- The mother breast-feeds during the day and sleeps with
ity of children under 2 years of age live with and are the baby at night, and infants raised under these condi-
cared for by someone other than their mothers. These tions become attached to their mothers.
The Social Organization of Childhood Experience 737

Finally, and in light of the foregoing, it seems that for periods of time during the day (S. LeVine, n.d.). As
the American or European American concern about Harkness and Super (1992) point out, fathers can be in
psychological harm is probably exaggerated. Babies can the presence of young children without interacting with
accommodate comfortably to sibling care, and back- them, and it is only when cultural practices and mental-
carrying as well as other widespread forms of tactile ities favor it, that fathers and other men will assume re-
stimulation promote both physical growth and psy- sponsibility for the care of children or engage them in
chosocial attachment during the 1st year (R. A. LeVine interaction. Infants become attached to their father and
et al., 1994, pp. 257–258). other men who interact with them, as they do to their
Furthermore, sibling care can initiate a strong lifelong mother, siblings, grandmothers, and other adult women
relationship between an older sister and younger brother, (Ainsworth, 1967).
which some cultures selectively promote. Among the
Hausa, the marriage of a sister’s son to the daughter of
Parental Practices
the brother she cared for as an infant is a preferred form
of cross-cousin marriage. Even in the short run, the rela- An important and culturally variable part of the child’s
tionship of the toddler to his sibling caregiver often intro- social environment is constituted by the customary activ-
duces the child to a larger group of children who become ities that parents and others arrange for them. Observa-
salient nonparental figures in his life. tional investigators of human and other primate offspring
Grandmothers and other adult women often play an have created a number of dichotomous categories to de-
important supplementary role in infant care, especially scribe these activities: Child-centered communications
where children are raised in large domestic groups. From versus those that do not include the child, distal (often
West Africa to India and China, grandmothers are not verbal) versus proximal (usually physical) stimulations,
only caregivers in the early years but also, as the child reciprocal or contingent vocalization versus unilateral
grows older, complements to the mother’s disciplinary speech to a child, positive versus negative emotional
role with their unconditional nurturance and emotional arousal, soothing versus stimulation, and sensitive versus
support. Children can, and often do, form intense and insensitive response to infant signals.
long-lasting relationships with other resident women in These dichotomies are behaviorally specified to be
extended family situations. unequivocally observable in differing contexts of pri-
Fathers and other men are more rarely observed as mate behavior, but they nevertheless seem to reflect
caregivers for young children, but there is variation European American middle-class preferences for
across human populations. Hewlett (1992) has provided child-centered, distal, verbal, reciprocal, emotionally
substantial data from diverse peoples. He distinguishes positive, stimulating, and sensitive patterns of parent-
between the father’s investment in the child, which may child interaction. Studies using these categories cross-
be indirect and consist of providing resources through culturally usually show that parents in other cultures
the mother, and involvement with the child, which refers exhibit some or all of these behaviors less frequently
to interaction. than middle-class European Americans (R. A. LeVine
Although paternal interaction with young children is et al., 1994; Richman et al., 1988; Richman, Miller, &
rare relative to that of the mother and other females, and LeVine, 1992).
it is unusual cross-culturally for males to be constant However valid these findings of difference in fre-
and responsible caregivers (as opposed to occasional quencies may be, they are only part of the story. Taking
playmates) for infants or toddlers, the range is quite con- European American custom complexes as the reference
siderable. Among the Dinka of the Sudan, for example, point for comparison almost inevitably means overlook-
the exclusion of men from attending the delivery of a ing activities and dimensions that are salient only in the
child is extended through the early years of a child’s other cultures. Without a complementary account of the
life, and the father only interacts with his older children mentality and point of view of the other culture, this is
(Deng, 1972). Aka pygmy fathers in Cameroon, how- grossly uninformative, like an African account that
ever, participate substantially in the care of young chil- might describe the American family as lacking cattle
dren (Hewlett, 1991), and among high-caste Hindu and agriculture.
farmers of the Katmandu Valley in Nepal, various men The findings may indicate that parents in the other
in the extended family take care of infants and toddlers culture are not committed to the same custom complex
738 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

in their observable practices and do not share the Euro- CULTURAL MENTALITIES CONCERNING
pean American mentality, but the findings do not de- CHILDHOOD SOCIAL RELATIONS
scribe what custom complex they are committed to and
what goals, values, and pictures of the world they are in Parents do not always try to control the interpersonal en-
fact and in practice following. To make sense of observ- vironments of their children in detail, particularly after
able differences in parents’ practices, it is necessary to the first 2 or 3 years, and when they try to, they are
describe the parents’ cultural models of social relations. often far from successful. Nevertheless, parents care
It is necessary to describe the mentalities that guide and about and can usually influence the settings in which
give meaning to their practices and to a child’s social their children interact with others, their caregivers and
participation. Some illustrations are provided in the companions, and the kinds of interactions that take
next section. place (B. B. Whiting & Edwards, 1988; B. B. Whiting &
Whiting, 1975). Thus, it matters what parents think and
feel about such things, and socially inherited beliefs,
Age-Graded Activities values, and pictures of the world frame what they think
and feel. Parents are culture bearers, and their models
In all societies, the social interaction of children is al- of childhood social relations are as variable as their
tered by their age-related participation in activities at culture’s conceptions of the good life and how to live it
home or school. The institution of schooling creates an (Harkness & Super, 1996; R. A. LeVine et al., 1994;
extreme form of age-grading. In most schools children, R. A. LeVine, Miller, & West, 1988).
from the ages of 5- to 8-years-old onward, tend to be
rigidly segregated by age from those older and younger
for many of their daytime activities. Parental Models and Strategies
The peer groups that result are neither natural nor
universal. In societies without schools, children’s rela- Parental behavior is symbolic action in Geertz’s
tionships with each other are formed among siblings or (1973b) sense of the term and reflects a local mentality
other multiage groups of juveniles (Konner, 1975). In about what parenthood and child development are and
these multiage groups, participants are much more ought to be, as formulated in the symbols of a particu-
sharply differentiated by authority and knowledge than lar culture. The local cultural mentality gives meaning
in school-based peer groups. In such groups, relation- to the actions of parents and children and motivates
ships among older and younger children may facilitate parents to promote certain behaviors and dampen oth-
the learning of skills by the younger, who observe ma- ers. A cultural mentality of child care has three compo-
ture practice performed by someone old enough to be nents: (1) moral direction, (2) a pragmatic design, and
more skilled but close enough in age to be easily imi- (3) customary scripts for interaction (R. A. LeVine
tated (Dunn, 1983). et al., 1994).
Sibling relations may also promote interpersonal
responsibility, cooperation, and sensitivity to the Moral Direction
vulnerability of others on the part of the elder children Cultural mentalities of child care are goal driven; they
(Schieffelin, 1990; Weisner, 1982, 1987, 1989b; B. B. are formulated by cultural concepts of virtue toward
Whiting & Edwards, 1988). Schools, alternatively, which a child’s behavioral development should move.
may foster interpersonal comparison and competi- The vernacular words (e.g., independence, autonomy,
tion among peers and, by obstructing the child’s obser- and self-reliance for the European American middle
vational access to mature practice, make learning more class) and the images associated with them that repre-
problematic and hence more self-conscious (Lave, sent virtuous goals of development help provide parents’
1990; R. A. LeVine, 1978; Scribner & Cole, 1973). rationales for their observable child-care practices.
Cultural variability in age-graded social activities is Research on comparative ethics and development,
widened further by specific combinations of siblings, however, has revealed that the humanly recognizable
school and work in the local environments of children, virtues or moral ends of life can be culturally organized
and culture-specific norms that elaborate or diminish in ways that do not privilege an “ethics of autonomy”
age ranking. (Haidt et al., 1993; Jensen, 1996, 2005; Shweder, 1990b;
The Ef fects of Early Interpersonal Experience 739

Shweder et al., 1990, 1997) and that in some societies an lowing a baby to cry more than a few seconds is experi-
“ethics of community” and/or an “ethics of divinity” enced by Gusii adults as an intolerable breach of care-
leads to an emphasis on alternative virtues and goals of giving norms.
development such as duty, respect, hierarchical interde-
pendency, purity, and sanctity.
Furthermore, each type of ethics highlights a partic- THE EFFECTS OF EARLY
ular view of the self. Shweder et al. (1997) argue that (a) INTERPERSONAL EXPERIENCE
the ethics of autonomy is associated with a conception
of the self as an individual preference structure, where What effects do cultural variations in interpersonal en-
the point of moral evaluation is to increase choice and vironments and symbolically mediated experience have
personal liberty; ( b) the ethics of community is associ- on the behavioral and psychosocial development of the
ated with a conception of the self as an office holder in child? A cultural community or population-level ap-
which a person’s role or station in life is intrinsic to proach provides a clearer picture of the effects of early
their identity; and (c) the ethics of divinity is associated experience than a focus on the psychology of individual
with a conception of the self as a spiritual entity con- differences (R. A. LeVine, 1990a). For example, chil-
nected to some sacred order of things and as the bearer dren who grow up in China obviously learn to speak
of a legacy that is elevated and pure. The meaning of Chinese, just as those who grow up in Turkey learn
child-care practices in any particular community, from Turkish. Less obvious, but well established by sociolin-
disciplinary practices to sleeping arrangements to the guistic investigators of child language, is that as young
practice of circumcision, is often most understandable children acquire a first language they also master the
with reference to the particular moral ends that justify communicative practices regulating interpersonal be-
and rationalize those practices in the minds of parents in havior in their communities (Ochs & Schieffelin, 1984;
that local cultural world (on initiation and circumcision Schieffelin & Ochs, 1986a, 1986b).
see, e.g., Kratz, 1994, pp. 341–347). The symbolic mediation of experience and commu-
nicative practices are discussed in a following section.
Pragmatic Design The main point of emphasis here is a very simple one:
For young children the development of communicative
Cultural mentalities of child care embody strategies not
competence reflects their early experience in a particu-
only for facilitating the child’s behavioral development
lar language environment and constitutes an important
in a morally virtuous direction but also for achieving
part of their early enculturation.
other ends (e.g., survival, health, and economic returns)
By age 3, children have culture-specific capacities for
and for overcoming obstacles to the attainment of all
and expectations of emotionally salient interpersonal
these ends. This utilitarian aspect of child-care mentali-
behavior, embedded in speech routines and other cus-
ties provides a practical value and convinces parents that
toms of face-to-face interaction, in the context of spe-
they are doing what is necessary and right.
cific relationships (Schieffelin, 1990). Their behavioral
development has taken a culturally distinctive character
Customary Scripts for Interaction
and direction, diverging from that of other cultures.
The moral and pragmatic aspects of a child-care mental- Relatively little research has been done on the
ity may or may not be explicitly formulated in general behavioral consequences of cultural variations in early
terms, but they are always represented in the social cus- social experience, but there is some evidence of
toms that guide the interaction of parents and other measurable effects. Social behaviors shown to differ
caregivers with young and older children. At this level of across culturally varying samples of children include
specificity in social interaction, for example, the script infant-mother attachment (Grossmann & Grossmann,
for responding to a baby’s cry among the Gusii of Kenya 1981, 1991; Grossmann, Grossmann, Spangler, Suess,
is an immediate soothing response. This response is & Unzner, 1985), attention-seeking (R. A. LeVine
seen as promoting the calmness and compliance of a et al., 1994; B. B. Whiting & Whiting, 1975), depend-
young child (the moral direction) as well as the child’s ence (Caudill & Schooler, 1973), cooperation (Thomas,
health and survival in the early months (part of the 1978), and gender orientation (B. B. Whiting & Ed-
pragmatic design). However, it is so customary that al- wards, 1988). In each of these cases, the evidence of
740 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

behavioral difference has been interpreted by the inves- The German evidence provides the starting point for
tigators to reflect the impact of the children’s prior ex- a cultural critique of the Bowlby-Ainsworth model of at-
perience in divergent cultural environments, although it tachment, especially its claims to have discovered the
is probably not possible at this time to choose between evolutionary origins of human social relationships and
different interpretations of this impact (e.g., facilita- the biological basis for judgments of optimality, normal-
tion, attunement, or maintenance/ loss, as discussed ity, and pathology in early development. As more de-
earlier; Werker, 1989). tailed and culturally informed evidence on behavioral
An example from infancy research is the Gross- development in diverse cultures accumulates, a cultural
manns’ (1981, 1991; Grossmann et al., 1985) study critique of developmental models may serve a useful
of infant-mother attachment in Bielefeld, North Ger- purpose, particularly if the models themselves continue
many. This German replication of Mary Ainsworth’s to ignore cultural variation in early social experience.
Baltimore study (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, At this point, it may not be possible to launch robust
1978) found that the majority of a nonclinical sample generalizations about the psychological effects of
of 12-month-olds was classifiable on the basis of the early interpersonal experience based on population-
videotaped Strange Situation as “insecurely” attached level comparisons across cultures. However, as the con-
to their mothers. Forty-nine percent of the sample was cepts and techniques for observing and recording infant
classified in the “A” category or “anxious-avoidant,” al- care and early communicative exchanges involving chil-
most twice as large a proportion as in American sam- dren have improved, and as comparative evidence has
ples. The Grossmanns related this departure from grown, so have the grounds for believing human behav-
American norms to the German mothers’ custom com- ioral development to be culturally divergent from the
plex—their mentality and practices. German mothers, in early years of childhood onward.
this region of Germany, prefer a greater physical and in-
terpersonal distance from their infants than Americans,
leaving them alone more often and sometimes pushing THE SYMBOLIC MEDIATION OF
them away. They would consider American infants rated EXPERIENCE: LANGUAGE AND
as “optimal” by attachment researchers to be spoiled. COMMUNICATIVE CUSTOMS IN
According to the Grossmanns’ interpretation, the CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY
culture-specific preferences of the German mothers was
based on a broader cultural mentality, even ideology, It is a major assumption of cultural psychology that one
emphasizing an ideal of pure independence that is even mind is transformed into many mentalities through the
more exaggerated than the European American ethics of symbolic mediation of experience and that the human
autonomy. For these mothers, this cultural ideology was conceptual capacities that support culture also support
translated into maternal practices that affected not only language use, which is the primary means by which the
their infants’ routine expectations for social interaction symbolic and behavioral inheritances of a cultural tradi-
and comforting but also their response to separation and tion are passed on to the next generation. It is primarily
reunion in the Strange Situation. Their interpretation of by means of language that human beings negotiate
their findings implies, though the Grossmanns do not divergent points of view and construct shared cultural
say so, that the profile of attachment ratings of Ameri- realities. In this section, we selectively discuss the role
can infants in the Strange Situation can be seen as re- of certain pragmatic forms of linguistic analysis in re-
flecting the culturally influenced parental practices of search on the cultural psychology of development.
European Americans rather than a universal norm for all As children learn language, they gain entry to exist-
human populations (LeVine & Norman, 2001). ing meaning systems and access to the tools for recreat-
If this is so, then claims of species-typical universal- ing and transforming those systems. In a wide-ranging
ity for attachment as observed in the Strange Situation review of the literature, Nelson (1996) concludes “lan-
should be considered premature. Infant reactions to re- guage and the surrounding culture take over the human
union with their mother after a brief separation at 12 mind” (p. 325), profoundly changing the nature of cog-
months of age can be reinterpreted as indicators of early nition and communication during the time from 2 to 6
enculturation to a cultural standard of interpersonal dis- years of age. Language is fundamental not only to mean-
tance mediated through parental practices of infant care. ing construction but to identity. Through its association
Language as Practice 741

with particular contexts, language comes to symbolize This distinction between language as tool and object
and belong to particular groups. Quite simply, there can of inquiry serves the interests of cultural psychology by
be no cultural psychology without language. promoting critical examination of the ways in which re-
This premise is traceable to many intellectual fore- searchers use language in conducting their research and
bears of contemporary cultural psychology. These in- by acknowledging the continued importance of under-
clude the eighteenth-century European philosophers who standing the referential function of language. At the
laid the groundwork for cultural psychology (Jahoda, same time, this distinction is limiting because it does
1992), Wilhelm Wundt and other nineteenth-century not readily encompass a third, rapidly growing set of
proponents of a “second” psychology (Cahan & White, studies of particular interest to cultural psychology.
1992), and Edward Sapir (Mandelbaum, 1951) the an- These studies focus on talk but they are not con-
thropological linguist whose works on language in social cerned with language development itself. Instead, they
life and on culture and personality anticipated many top- examine how talk contributes to constituting children’s
ics of current interest to cultural psychologists. experience in other developmental domains (Garvey,
We begin this section of the chapter by discussing the 1992) such as social development (e.g., Dunn, 1993),
conception of language that is most compatible with the self construction (e.g., Bruner, 1990a, 1990b), and peer
aims of cultural psychology and by identifying resources culture (e.g., Corsaro, 1992, 1997). These studies take
from allied fields of study that hold promise for deepen- talk seriously as a “substantive, structured, and struc-
ing our understanding of language in cultural life. We turing activity with intrinsic developmental signifi-
turn next to socialization, one of the fundamental prob- cance” (Packer, 1987, p. 253). An important implication
lems of cultural psychology, and review studies that of this perspective is that a particular social phenome-
have yielded important insights into the actual process non may be constituted in qualitatively different ways
of socialization by examining the forms and functions of within and across cultures and that these differences are
everyday discourse. We then single out oral narrative as created partly through talk.
a paradigm case of everyday discourse, organizing the Recent research on play deserves to be singled out
discussion around issues of diversity. Throughout this because it illustrates this point especially well and be-
section, the primary focus is on research with young cause it represents some of the most richly contextual-
children. In keeping with the comparative commitment ized research in developmental cultural psychology.
of cultural psychology, examples of variation within and Children use verbal and nonverbal means to frame their
across cultures are included wherever possible. play as nonliteral, to enact pretend roles, and to negoti-
ate pretend transformations with their play partners
(e.g., Garvey & Kramer, 1989; Sawyer, 1997). How-
LANGUAGE AS PRACTICE ever, local customs and belief systems vary dramati-
cally along many dimensions, including who children
The centrality of language to cultural psychology stems play with and how they communicate with one another
not only from historical precedent but also from the du- (e.g., C. D. Clark, 2003; Göncü, 1999; Göncü, Patt, &
ality of language. Unlike other domains, language is Kouba, 2002; Haight, Wang, Fung, Williams, & Mintz,
both a tool and an object of inquiry. On the one hand, 1999; Lancy, 1996; Schwartzman, 1978). For example,
the use of language as an instrument of inquiry is perva- when a child has long conversations with an invisible
sive; every study of human development depends on other, middle-class American parents are likely to as-
verbal communication in one way or another. Children sume that she is talking to an imaginary companion;
are questioned about the reasons for their moral judg- Hindu parents that she is talking to a real spiritual being
ments. Parents are interviewed about their child-rearing (M. Taylor & Carlson, 2000). In contrast to middle-class
beliefs. Verbal behaviors are incorporated into observa- European American children, whose mothers induct them
tional coding schemes. Experimental tasks have to be into pretense by prompting, elaborating, and modeling
explained to participants. On the other hand, language (Haight & Miller, 1993), Yucatec Mayan parents do not
serves as the object of inquiry in many studies that seek play with young children (Gaskins, 1999). When Mayan
to understand the nature and development of the lin- children engage in social pretense, they do so exclusively
guistic system itself, including its various subsystems with other children. Similarly, Indonesian and Mexican
(e.g., syntax, morphology). children are socialized into play by older siblings
742 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

(Farver, 1999). Although children in all these groups more fully later). These fields have been centrally con-
engage in pretend play, they do so according to local so- cerned with cross-cultural comparisons and hence are
cial and communicative conventions. As a result, play especially germane to the comparative mission of cul-
assumes different forms, takes on different signifi- tural psychology. Resting on the assumption that every-
cances, and likely makes different contributions to their day talk is a pervasive, orderly, and culturally organized
overall development. feature of social life in every culture, they seek to un-
Implicit in these studies is a conception of language derstand the diversity of language use in the conduct
that privileges the situated use of language and recog- and constitution of social life.
nizes that speaking is inherently polysemous when ex- These fields provide a rich set of conceptual,
tracted from context (Bauman & Sherzer, 1989; Duranti methodological, and empirical resources that cultural
& Goodwin, 1992; Hanks, 1996). This conception con- psychologists should exploit more fully. These include
trasts with the narrowly referential conception of procedures for grounding interpretations of commu-
language that holds sway in most research on human de- nicative practices in the public cues that participants
velopment and cross-cultural psychology. The view of systematically deploy in interaction and critiques of our
language that is most compatible with the aims of cul- own social scientific methods as communicative prac-
tural psychology does not reduce language to a represen- tices whose meaning may not be shared by the people
tational system or repository of knowledge. Rather, it we study. For example, C. Briggs’ (1986) analysis of in-
goes beyond grammatical and lexical meaning to include terviewing as a social and cultural practice is still
processes of indexical meaning that anchor utterances to timely and demonstrates the critical importance of
their linguistic and nonlinguistic contexts and to unspo- customizing interviewing to local metacommunicative
ken background assumptions. practices (see also P. J. Miller et al., 2003, for an appli-
Hanks (1996) describes speech as “a form of engage- cation of this approach).
ment in the world. . . .To speak is to occupy the world, Another important insight from these fields pertains
not only to represent it, and this occupancy entails vari- to the issue of context. The focus on naturally occurring
ous modes of expression, of which propositional mean- discursive practices has led to a much more dynamic
ing is only one” (p. 236). To speak is to create social conception of context and practice than is usually as-
realities—to play, tease, instruct, dominate, transform sumed in developmental studies. Contexts are treated
oneself, and so on. Just as language cannot be sealed off not as static givens, dictated by the social and physical
from social life, words cannot be sealed off from si- environment, but as ongoing accomplishments negoti-
lence or from gaze, posture, gesture, facial expression, ated by participants. This shift from static to dynamic is
and other practices of the body. From this perspective, signaled by the term contextualization, which focuses
speaking consists of practices that are organized be- attention on the interpretive processes participants
yond the sentence level into dialogues, genres, and mul- themselves use to determine which aspects of the ongo-
tichanneled performances. These larger communicative ing activity are relevant (e.g., Bauman & Briggs, 1990;
events, while serving as units of analysis, are them- Duranti & Goodwin, 1992). This conceptual innovation
selves multiply embedded in larger sociocultural con- offers a holistic conception of individual and context as
texts and networks of cultural practices. In contrast to an interlocking system in which the language practice
approaches that take the disembodied word, sentence, changes along with the person (see Goodnow et al.,
or text as the unit of analysis, this approach permits a 1995, for further discussion of this point).
deeper cultural analysis, for it recognizes that cultural
principles are expressed not just in the content of talk
but in the way that discourse is organized internally and SOCIALIZATION THROUGH LANGUAGE
in relation to larger events and sequences of talk.
Among the many intellectual currents that have fed The growing literature on language socialization de-
into practiced-centered views of language is sociohistor- serves further consideration because it arises from an
ical theory, with its focus on semiotically mediated intellectual project that is basic to cultural psychology.
activity (Cole, 1996; Wertsch, 1985) and linguistic an- Cultural psychology recognizes that child development
thropology, especially the fields of ethnography of com- is inextricably bound to the process of socialization—of
munication (e.g., Bauman & Sherzer, 1989; Hymes, orienting oneself in systems of meaning—and seeks to
1974) and language socialization (which is discussed understand the nature of this process as it is actually en-
Socialization through Language 743

acted by living, experiencing human beings. Cultural everyday life. Although the process of language social-
psychology is uniquely positioned, by virtue of its inter- ization was assumed to be lifelong, most research fo-
disciplinary character and commitment to meaning, to cused on the early years. In contrast to many domains of
claim socialization fully as its own—something that human development, some of the best documented cases
none of the social sciences has succeeded in doing. Be- were non-Western cultures (e.g., Ochs, 1988; Schieffe-
cause of the way in which human action has been parti- lin, 1990; Watson-Gegeo & Gegeo, 1990, 1999) and
tioned for study, socialization has remained marginal to working-class and minority groups in the United States
the intellectual agenda of any discipline. As a result, it (e.g., Heath, 1983; P. J. Miller, 1982).
has been difficult to devise an integrated conception of Research on language socialization has been the sub-
socialization that slights neither culture nor children. ject of numerous reviews (e.g., P. J. Miller & Hoogstra,
The field of language socialization provides an impor- 1992; Schieffelin & Ochs, 1986a) and collections (e.g.,
tant model of how to proceed with this task. Inspired by Bayley & Schecter, 2003; Corsaro & Miller, 1992; Schi-
Edward Sapir’s famous words, “Language is a great effelin & Ochs, 1986b). An important conclusion
force of socialization, probably the greatest that exists” emerging from this body of work is that there is enor-
(Mandelbaum, 1951, p. 15), this field rests on the prem- mous diversity in the cultural organization of caregiving
ise that children are not only socialized through language and language learning and that the pattern of sustained
but are socialized to use language (Ochs & Schieffelin, dyadic conversation and mutual negotiation of meaning
1984). Another touchstone is the Vygotskian idea that so- so familiar to many middle-class European Americans
ciocultural meanings are created by using language for is but one variant among many. As noted earlier, groups
particular purposes in socially defined activities (Vygot- differ in the physical and social ecology of child care, in
sky, 1934/1987; Wertsch, 1985). If language not only re- language ideologies and folk theories about the nature of
flects meaning but also constitutes meaning, then an children and development, in the practices used to en-
adequate theory of socialization must incorporate talk in courage mature speech, and in the principles that orga-
a principled way. nize interaction.
Such a theory confers three advantages. First, the For example, Kaluli mothers of Papua, New Guinea,
actual processes of socialization are rendered accessi- believe that infants do not understand and thus cannot be
ble through analysis of the forms and functions of conversational partners (Schieffelin, 1990). They do not
everyday discourse. Second, in keeping with a basic in- talk to infants; instead they face babies outwards so that
sight of modern developmental psychology, the child is they can be part of the social flow. When older siblings
accorded an active role through a focus on child and greet the baby, the mother speaks for the baby, using
caregivers’ mutual, negotiated participation in dis- language that is appropriate to the older child. Mothers
course practices (Brandtstädter, Chapter 10, this Hand- do not interpret or paraphrase infants’ vocalizations, a
book, this volume; Rogoff, 1990). Third, the fact that practice that reflects a dispreference for talking about
language practices systematically index social statuses another person’s thoughts or feelings. In the working-
and ideologies helps to explain the varied affective class African American community described by Heath
stances—eager acceptance, resistance, playfulness— (1983), multiparty talk is the norm and children are al-
that children assume as they attempt to invest cultural most never alone. Talk around the child, rather than talk
resources with meaning (Watson-Gegeo & Gegeo, directly to the child, is the primary linguistic resource
1999). Both the nonneutral, ideologically charged na- for novice learners. In a Mayan community in southern
ture of the socializing environment and the necessarily Mexico, both dyadic and “eavesdropper ” models of lan-
evaluative responses of the child-in-context are taken guage learning are practiced, and nonverbal interaction
into account (Goodnow, 1990a). plays an important role in organizing infants’ participa-
Beginning in the 1970s, led by the pioneering work of tion (de León, 2000).
Ochs and Schieffelin (1984), researchers began to trans- Coexisting with these and other differences are im-
late these ideas into a particular kind of empirical work portant similarities. For example, many groups socialize
designed to link macro- and microlevels of analysis. In children into elaborate forms of teasing and opposi-
an attempt to discover how communities structure chil- tional language (e.g., Briggs, 1998; Corsaro, Molinari,
dren’s entry into meaning, they combined ethnographic & Rosier, 2002; de León, 2000; Eisenberg, 1986). Even
fieldwork with the meticulous documentation of interac- more widespread is the use of explicit instruction to so-
tions between members and novices as they unfold in cialize young children into valued ways of acting, feeling,
744 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

and speaking (P. J. Miller & Hoogstra, 1992). This is cultural distinctiveness carries with it certain risks. One
one of the reasons we could suggest (in our initial dis- of the unintended consequences of the first phase of re-
cussion of the custom complex) that the course of pro- search on language socialization was to essentialize dif-
gressive development is sometimes from the reflective ferences across cultures, minimizing variation within
to the unreflective, from the explicit to the tacit. For ex- culture and across time (Ochs, 1999). Fortunately, an
ample, Watson-Gegeo and Gegeo (1990), found that emerging trend in recent research on language socializa-
Kwara’ae (Solomon Islands) parents used a symboli- tion is a focus on bilingual and multilingual communi-
cally powerful and emotionally intense discourse, ties and other situations in which languages and cultures
called “shaping the mind,” to pass on traditional knowl- come into contact (Bayley & Schecter, 2003; Garrett &
edge and encourage children to practice reasoning and Baquedano-López, 2002). Kulick’s (1992) study of lan-
argumentation. guage shift and language socialization in Gapun, Papua,
Research on language socialization also has revealed New Guinea is a landmark in this respect. Adults in this
that many of the most powerful socializing messages are small-scale society traditionally spoke the vernacular
implicit and unintended. They are conveyed through language as well as one or more other languages. How-
tacit routine organizations of time and space, with their ever, under the influence of modernization, Gapuners
associated routines and distributions of social actors, began to valorize the local Creole language. Kulick
and through contrastive distributions of language forms found that adults unwittingly interacted with children
and functions that index meaning. The implication is in ways that systematically denied them access to the
that research that depends exclusively on asking care- vernacular language, resulting in its rapid decline. This
givers about their socializing goals is likely to miss study not only linked two trajectories of change ( histor-
some of the deepest and most subtle dimensions of so- ical change and childhood socialization) but also
cialization—those pervasive and fundamental cultural showed that language ideologies linking gender, affect,
orderings that feel most deeply natural to participants and language played a critical role in imperiling the ver-
and are least likely to be reflected on. For example, nacular language.
Western Samoan children begin to learn about the social As scholars turn their attention to language socializa-
stratification that pervades their society not only by par- tion in heterogeneous situations, such as changing lan-
ticipating in interactions in which higher status care- guage policy in nation states, transnational migration,
givers direct lower status caregivers to care for them but postcolonial settings, and linguistically diverse institu-
also by observing how caregivers of different rank dis- tions (e.g., Fader, 2001; González, 2001; He, 2001;
tribute themselves in domestic spaces (Ochs, 1988). Sandel, 2003), they have begun to problematize impor-
In addition to establishing that there are diverse path- tant ideas. Garrett and Baquedano-López (2002) point
ways to communicative competence, studies of language out that such studies reveal that the boundaries of lan-
socialization also demonstrate that children come to em- guage communities are fuzzy, permeable, and shifting;
body diverse ways of being in the world. Particularly rel- community can no longer be defined as “geography, lan-
evant to cultural psychology are studies that focus on the guage(s) spoken, or broad presupposed social categories
socialization of affect. Like earlier work (e.g., P. J. Miller such as race or ethnicity, but in terms of mutual social
& Sperry, 1987; Schieffelin & Ochs, 1986a, 1986b), re- and interactive engagement ” (p. 347). Such studies un-
cent studies show that children are able to express affect derscore the need to know more about how children nav-
through customary communicative means from an early igate and interweave multiple languages, meaning
age and that they deploy a wide variety of communicative systems, and genres, a process that is also gaining atten-
resources, not just emotion state terms (e.g., Clancy, tion in studies of narrative.
1999). Some studies focus on caregivers’ styles of affec-
tive socialization (e.g., Cervantes, 2002). Others show
Narrative: Getting Those Stories Straight
how culturally salient affective experience—for example,
shame for Taiwanese children (Fung, 1999), interper- Narrative is a cultural universal, and one of the most
sonal danger for Inuit children (J. Briggs, 1998)—is con- powerful interpretive tools that human beings possess
stituted through children’s habitual participation in for organizing experience in time and for interpreting
recurring patterns of discourse. and valuing human action. The literature on narrative is
Welcome as such evidence is for developmental cul- vast, and the list of topics relevant to cultural psychol-
tural psychology, the project of mapping a landscape of ogy is long. These include the relationship between nar-
Socialization through Language 745

rative and memory (e.g., Neisser & Fivush, 1994), the “aim” a story at an offender. If taken to heart, the story
role of narrative in the construction of selves and iden- and the place with which it is associated will “stalk ” the
tities (e.g., Bruner, 1990a, 1990b; Gergen, 1991; Hol- offender and promote beneficial change.
land, Lachicotte, Skinner, & Cain, 1998; Wortham, Basso’s study illustrates three issues of plurality that
2001), narrative as a therapeutic technique in psy- apply to the study of oral narrative as a socializing
chotherapy and psychoanalysis (e.g., Polkinghorne, medium. First, the most obvious is the issue of narrative
1988; Spence, 1982), and narrative in family life (Pratt diversity across sociocultural groups. Basso describes a
& Fiese, 2004). Children’s narratives have been of keen distinctive cultural case that is sufficiently detailed to
interest to scholars for the light that they shed on lin- allow precise comparison and contrast with other cul-
guistic development (Berman & Slobin, 1994), peer tural cases. Second, although the study focuses primar-
culture (Corsaro, 1997; Goodwin, 1990), literacy (e.g., ily on one type of oral narrative—historical tales—it
Michaels, 1991; Wolf & Heath, 1992), and classroom situates them in the full range of native narrative cate-
life (Dyson & Genishi, 1994; Nicolopoulou, 1997). gories, including myths, sagas, and gossip. The plurality
Faced with this embarrassment of riches, we have cho- of oral narrative types in the culture is thus established.
sen, to focus on a single issue: the role of oral narrative Third, access to and ownership of narrative genres is so-
in early socialization. This focus maintains continuity cially distributed, introducing another source of intra-
with the previous discussion of socialization through cultural variation.
discursive practices while allowing exploration of cul-
Storytelling Begins Early
tural diversity in greater depth.
A question left unanswered by Basso’s account is how
Varieties of Oral Narrative
Western Apache narrative practices intersect with chil-
Keith Basso (1996) begins his classic paper on Western dren. For example, when do Western Apache begin to
Apache oral narratives with an interpretive puzzle. aim historical tales at errant children? An exciting trend
What did Western Apache elders mean when they made in recent developmental research is the strong interest in
the following statements: young children’s narratives. There is a growing body of
evidence that children from many cultural backgrounds
Our children are losing the land. It doesn’t go to work on
them anymore. They don’t know the stories about what
within and beyond the United States begin to tell stories
happened at these places. That’s why some get into trou- in conversation during the second or 3rd year of
ble. (p. 38) life (e.g., Eisenberg, 1985; Engel, 1995; McCabe & Pe-
terson, 1991; Meng, 1992; Ochs & Capps, 2001). At this
. . . . I recall stories of how it once was at that mountain.
early age, children step into the narrative practices
The stories told to me were like arrows. Elsewhere, hear-
of family and community, thereby laying claim to an
ing that mountain’s name, I see it. Its name is like a pic-
ture. Stories go to work on you like arrows. Stories make
important cultural resource for creating their own so-
you live right. Stories make you replace yourself. (p. 38) cialization (Bruner, 1990a, 1990b). Even linguistically
isolated deaf children, whose parents choose not to
Basso’s (1996) attempts to understand the signifi- expose them to a conventional sign system, were able
cance of these statements yielded one of the most com- to create gestured narratives (Van Deusen-Phillips,
prehensive accounts available of a group’s shared Goldin-Meadow, & Miller, 2001). These stories carried
understanding of how oral narrative functions in their echoes of culture-specific meaning, suggesting that con-
lives. Working in collaboration with informants whom versational narrative is a remarkably robust medium of
he had known for many years, Basso discovered that socialization.
Western Apache storytelling exploited two symbolic Personal storytelling not only emerges early in life
resources—land and narrative—for maintaining the but it also occurs habitually in a wide variety of commu-
moral order. nities, occurring especially frequently in working-class
Western Apache use stories about the early history of communities. For example, stories involving 21⁄2 -year-
the group to establish enduring ties between individuals olds occurred at average rates of 3 to 4 per hour in both
and features of the natural landscape. Because of these middle-class Taiwanese and middle-class European
bonds, people who have behaved improperly are moved American families (P. J. Miller, Wiley, Fung, & Liang,
to reflect on and correct their misconduct. At times, a 1997; Wiley, Rose, Burger, & Miller, 1998). Narratives
member of the community might find it necessary to accounted for one-quarter of 2-year-olds naturally
746 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

occurring talk in working-class African American fami- wanese families were much more likely than their Euro-
lies in the Black Belt of Alabama (Sperry & Sperry, pean American counterparts to tell stories in which they
1995, 1996). In working-class European American fami- cast the child protagonist as a transgressor. In keeping
lies in Chicago, 3-year-olds participated in co-narrations with local beliefs that parents should take every opportu-
at the remarkable rate of 6 times per hour (Burger & nity to correct young children, many of these stories oc-
Miller, 1999). When personal storytelling occurs so curred immediately after the focal child had committed a
abundantly, it gets woven, densely but almost invisibly, misdeed in the here and now. Families repeatedly invoked
into the fabric of young children’s social experience. moral and social rules, structured their stories to estab-
Moreover, regardless of where they occur, these small, lish the child’s misdeed as the point of the story, and con-
mundane stories are saturated with value and replete cluded their stories with didactic codas. By contrast, the
with culturally patterned messages. Stories vary within European American families enacted a self-favorability
and across cultures along a host of parameters that en- bias, erasing or downplaying children’s misdeeds. These
compass how the genre is defined and practiced (P. J. differences were also evident in parents’ beliefs about sto-
Miller & Moore, 1989). For example, in her classic rytelling (P. J. Miller, Sandel, Liang, & Fung, 2001) and
ethnography of neighboring working-class communities in pretend play (Haight et al., 1999).
in the Piedmont Carolinas, Heath (1983) found that the Wang, Leightman, and colleagues compared stories
European American residents of Roadville adhered to a elicited from Chinese and European American children
criterion of literal truth when narrating their personal ex- and reported similar findings. Chinese mothers from Bei-
periences. This contrasted with the African American jing showed a greater concern with moral rules and be-
community of Trackton, who strongly favored fictional havioral standards when co-narrating stories with their
embellishment. Trackton and Roadville also enacted op- 3-year-olds (Han, Leichtman, & Wang, 1998; Wang, Le-
posing norms toward and away from self-aggrandizement. ichtman, & Davies, 2000), and 6-year-olds told stories
Trackton children not only created bold and triumphant exhibited a parallel concern with moral correctness
self-protagonists but also asserted their rights to tell sto- (Wang & Leichtman, 2000).
ries by adroitly working their way into adult talk, com- Thus, although personal storytelling is a rich pur-
manding the floor, and receiving approbation for their veyor of values for European American children, the
verbal artistry. version of personal storytelling practiced by the Taipei
In their study of an African American community in and Beijing children leans more strongly in a didactic
rural Alabama, Sperry and Sperry (1995, 1996) found direction, reflecting and reinforcing larger systems of
that 2-year-olds produced more fantasy stories than fac- meaning that privilege moral education. Fung, Miller,
tual stories of past experience. “Both caregivers and and Lin (2004) link this didactic bias to Confucian
children enjoyed telling stories of escaping from ‘Nicou- discourses that valorize teaching, listening, and self-
dini,’ the ‘Boogabear,’ ‘Werewolf ’, or the spectral deer improvement, discourses that continue to circulate in the
who entered their home one misty evening. Families told complex mix of local and global influences that are re-
such stories easily and frequently, and children gathered shaping childrearing and education in contemporary
around to be thrilled by the imagined terror and to prac- Taiwan. Li (2002) found that Chinese college students
tice creating it themselves” (p. 462; Sperry & Sperry, viewed learning as a moral process, imbued with pur-
1996). Boys’ efforts to tell fantasy stories received pose, undertaken according to the virtues of diligence,
much more support than girls’, a finding that may help persistence, and humility, and encompassed by the
to explain how men in this community get to be so good larger project of self-perfection, but American college
at “ tall-bragging.” students saw learning as a neutral, mental process of
P. J. Miller, Fung, and their colleagues compared knowledge acquisition. The stories told by the Beijing
middle-class Taiwanese families in Taipei and middle- children and their mothers share a similar moral cast.
class European American families in Chicago and found In sum, studies of children’s early storytelling in
that in both cases narrators interpreted young children’s families and communities demonstrate that this narra-
past experiences in interpersonal terms, situating the tive genre is culturally differentiated from the begin-
child in relationship to other people (P. J. Miller et al., ning. Wherever personal storytelling is practiced with
1997; P. J. Miller, Fung, & Mintz, 1996). At the same young children, it takes on local color, absorbing values,
time, personal storytelling differed dramatically: Tai- affective stances, and moral orientations. As children
Socialization through Language 747

participate routinely in personal storytelling, they begin Fivush and Buckner (2000) conclude that by the end of
to carve out different versions of personal experience. the preschool years, girls talk much more about sadness
Personal storytelling thus highlights—and is implicated than do boys, and that such talk occurs both with par-
in—an early developmental moment in the co-creation ents and friends, possibly contributing to girls’ vulnera-
of person and culture. Particular frameworks of evalua- bility to depression.
tion and interpretation, linked to larger currents of cul-
Variability and Heterogeneity
tural meaning, operate again and again in oral stories,
while narrators and listeners create and respond to here- In addition to these ways in which early narrative varies
and-now social contingencies. Each co-narrated story, in and across cultures, a practice approach to narrative
each story aimed at or told around the child provides an- reveals even more sources and dimensions of variability.
other opportunity for the child to hear which experi- Ochs and Capps (2001) developed a dimensional ap-
ences are reportable and how these experiences should proach to conversational stories of personal experience,
be assessed. In this way, interpretive frameworks are arguing that personal narrative oscillates between the
not only reproduced but also repeatedly instantiated in narrators’ desire for coherence and their desire for au-
personally relevant terms. To return to the comparisons thenticity, but that research has tended to privilege the
between European Americans, on the one hand, and former. The “default ” story involves one active teller,
Taiwanese or Chinese, on the other, we catch a glimpse crafting a linearly sequenced, coherent account of a
of how culturally distinct selves might originate. A highly tellable event. Such stories are framed by a con-
need for positive self-regard may be rooted, in part, in stant moral stance and are easily detached from sur-
storytelling that is systematically biased toward self- rounding discourse. Researchers should not assume that
favorability, whereas an inclination to self-improvement this default version of storytelling is the only adult
may be rooted, in part, in the narration of misdeeds and model or endpoint of development. Ochs and Capps pro-
the explicit invocation of moral standards (see the fol- pose that development proceeds along two lines. Chil-
lowing section: “ The Development of Self ”). dren become able to report norm violations and to
Although comparative studies, informed by local produce temporally ordered, coherent narratives; at the
practices and understandings, are necessary for identi- same time they learn to use stories in a nonlinear manner
fying these kinds of divergent developmental pathways, to puzzle over events and weigh alternatives.
again we emphasize that it is important not to di- Ochs and Capps (2001) draw attention to a variety of
chotomize such differences, erasing similarities across overlooked or understudied aspects of storytelling that
groups and variations within groups. Evidence is accu- could enrich cultural psychologists’ understanding of
mulating, for example, that personal storytelling is es- narrative as a socializing medium. How do children use
pecially rich in gendered meanings (Nicolopoulou, narratives for problem solving; expressing, negotiating,
1997; Ochs & Taylor, 1995). Again, this differentiation or failing to negotiate divergent points of view; grap-
is apparent early in development. Fivush and her col- pling with moral dilemmas; pondering hypothetical sce-
leagues found that middle-class European American narios; envisioning the future? How are children’s
children’s oral narratives are gendered in numerous stories embedded in surrounding discourse and activity?
ways during the preschool years and beyond. For exam- What participant roles are available to children?
ple, when asked to elicit stories from their young chil- Much of the developmental research on preschoolers’
dren about specific emotions experienced in the past, stories has focused on children as co-narrators, produc-
mothers co-constructed more elaborate stories about ing stories about their experiences in collaboration with
sadness with their daughters than with their sons parents or other family members. Less is known about
(Fivush, 1993). Similar findings emerged when fathers children as listeners, recipients, overhearers, and eaves-
served as co-narrators. Even when parents were asked droppers. By way of illustrating the importance of these
to talk with their children about novel past events (with- other participant roles, consider two examples. In the
out being asked to elicit talk of specific emotions), working-class community of South Baltimore, adults
mothers and fathers narrated more emotion events with and older children told a great many stories of personal
their 3-year-old daughters, compared with their sons, experience to one another (P. J. Miller, 1994). Young
and this contrast was especially marked for sad events children were present as bystanders to these stories, free
(Kuebli & Fivush, 1992). From these and other studies, to tune in or not. Many of these stories fell toward the
748 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

default end of the continuum highlighted by Ochs and Preece, 1987; Sperry & Sperry, 1995) but also draws
Capps (2001) and were highly performed, riveting ac- attention to the many ways in which stories are embed-
counts, ranging from the hilarious (e.g., the bathtub fell ded in, are blended with, or otherwise keep company
through the floor with Aunt Sharon in it) to the harrow- with other stories, other genres, and other activities.
ing (e.g., violent encounters in school or on the street). For example, ethnographic studies have shown that op-
This activity not only modeled narrative virtuosity but positional talk is an important means by which working-
also provided curious youngsters with a constantly class African American children of various ages construct
updated source of information about the lives of signifi- identities, form friendships, and sustain peer culture
cant others, including their mothers’ experiences of and that narratives are often incorporated into opposi-
school, workplace, and male-female relations. tional talk as a way of instigating disputes and project-
Whereas this study illustrates what a rich and varied ing conflicting points of view (e.g., Goodwin, 1990;
socializing medium stories around the child can be, Shuman, 1986). The preschooler studied by Corsaro
C. E. Taylor (1995) offers a rare, penetrating analysis of et al. (2002) was already adept at this kind of talk and
how keenly children listen to stories told around them. collaborated with her Head Start peers in producing
In one case study of a middle-class family, the 5-year- pretend scenarios that drew on both oppositional and
old sister and 8-year-old brother were initially silent narrative elements. Fung and Chen’s (2001) study of
when their parents engaged in narrative conflict at the the socialization of shame in Taiwanese families
dinner table. Eventually, the sister protested that the revealed that naturally occurring events of shame in-
parents were fighting, a concern that they dismissed and cluded multiple episodes and traversed multiple tempo-
rationalized as the conversation continued to unfold. ral and spatial worlds. Young children were led to relive
This study not only shows how carefully children moni- and reflect on present and past transgressions and to
tor adult discourse but also serves as a powerful re- prepare for a better self in the future. Both of these
minder of two other important points: family narrative studies suggest that stories do not necessarily come
is sometimes unfriendly and threatening, exacerbating neatly packaged and clearly bounded in children’s
rather than solving problems, and children are socialized everyday experience. Rather, stories are embedded in
as much by the relationships that participants enact—in and overlap with other genres in heterogeneous and
this case, parental conflict—as they are by the content shifting configurations that illuminate again and again
of stories. particular threads of meaning for young participants.
In her analysis, C. E. Taylor (1995), like several of
The Dynamics of Narrative Practices
the scholars mentioned in this section of the chapter,
borrowed ideas from Mikhail Bakhtin, the Russian liter- Thus, when narratives are treated as situated practices,
ary scholar and philosopher of language. His vision of rather than disembodied texts, it becomes apparent that
language as culturally shaped and socially situated is storytelling is a dynamic process, emerging from partic-
compatible with the conception of language described ular circumstances, shaped by the interests of narrating
earlier (Bakhtin, 1981; Wertsch, 1991). Bakhtin (1986) participants, recurring in different combinations, and af-
stressed that speech is organized into genres associated fording children a range of participant roles. Children
with particular speech situations. He argued that speech engage repeatedly in networks of narrative practices,
is never free of generic constraints but that speakers can characterized by systematic variability and cross-cutting
achieve individuality of expression by creatively appro- redundancies. As argued earlier, this is one of the means
priating, combining, and reaccenting existing genres. As by which persons and cultures are cocreated. However,
researchers apply Bakhtinian constructs to the analysis narrative is also a means by which individuals affect
of children’s discourse, it is becoming clear that narra- change and transform identities (Holland et al., 1998;
tive sense-making involves juxtaposing and interweav- Wortham, 2001).
ing multiple, even conflicting voices and ideological The dynamic nature of narrative practices is espe-
perspectives and that every community affords such cially apparent when narrators tell the same story
heterogeneity (e.g., Cazden, 1993; Dyson, 1993; Hicks, repeatedly. Such stories can play a critical role in the
1994; Tobin, 2000; Wertsch, 1991; Wortham, 2001). lifelong process of socialization. Adults may be
This perspective not only underscores the impor- haunted, baffled, or sustained for decades by stories
tance of studying children’s narrative repertories (e.g., from their own or others’ lives (e.g., Coles, 1989; Fung,
The Development of Self 749

2003; Gone, 1999; Hudley, Haight, & Miller, 2003; THE DEVELOPMENT OF SELF
Steedman, 1986). As the person tells the story repeat-
edly to self and other, it accrues layer on layer of As noted earlier, the psychological side of cultural psy-
meaning and may be used to reinforce favored interpre- chology is the study of the things that the “I’s” (the
tations or to construct new interpretations. Repeated subjects, agents, or selves) of particular cultural com-
tellings and reinterpretations of personal experience are munities know, think, feel, want, and value, including
institutionalized in psychotherapy, Alcoholics Anony- what they know, think, feel, want, and value about the
mous, and religious conversion (Holland et al., 1998; self as a mental being capable of subjective experiences
Stromberg, 1993). Children as young as 2 years of age and of participation as an agent in a cultural commu-
develop intense attachments to particular stories, revis- nity. A powerful way in which culture and psyche con-
iting them again and again for weeks, months, and even stitute each other and come to influence individual
years (e.g., Alexander, Miller, & Hengst, 2001; P. J. behavior, we believe, is through our way of being a sub-
Miller, Hoogstra, Mintz, Fung, & Williams, 1993; Nel- ject or agent in a social world—what is often called
son, 1989; Wolf & Heath, 1992). The middle-class Euro- self-functioning.
pean American children in these studies treated their Indeed, the self can be conceptualized as a primary
special stories as resources for discussion and pretense locus of culture-psyche interaction and culture-specific
and used them to ponder problems and manage emo- being (see Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, Chapter
tions, activities that were supported by parents. These 11, this Handbook, this volume). It is where the individ-
studies suggest that from the time young children enter ual, a biological entity, becomes a meaningful entity—a
into narrative sense-making, they have the capacity to person, a participant in social worlds (P. J. Miller, 1994;
respond differentially to the ordinary narrative flow, Rogoff, Radziszewska, & Masiello, 1995; Weigert,
seizing certain stories for especially active and intense Teitge, & Teitge, 1990). Developing a sense of self as an
engagement. agentic, continuous entity with intentional powers may
Studies that trace the natural history of stories in appear to arise from highly personal idiosyncratic expe-
children’s lives are rare. This is unfortunate for cultural riences: A European American self is indeed defined to
psychology because the process of retelling and revising be individualized and distinct from other selves. Studies
stories goes to the heart of the socialization process. It highlight, however, that although the experience, struc-
is assumed to be transformative in theories ranging tures, and processes of self appear to be primarily indi-
from psychoanalysis to the Western Apache model of vidual creations, they are also cultural and historical
historical narrative. We need especially to know more constructions (Markus & Kitayama, 2003; J. G. Miller,
about how stories operate over the long term in chil- 2003; Oyserman & Markus, 1993).
dren’s lives. Before discussing the cultural psychology of the self,
Again, the Western Apache provide a compelling ex- we must acknowledge some of the issues and controver-
ample. Basso (1996) relates an incident in which an sies arising in this area. Several trends in philosophy,
adolescent girl arrived at a ceremonial in hair curlers, the social sciences, and world affairs have converged on
a violation of community standards. Some weeks later, self and identity as popular topics for research. Perhaps
when the girl was attending a party at her grand- most obviously, as the headlines of any newspaper con-
mother’s house, the grandmother narrated a historical firm, nationalism and a concern for maintaining and as-
story about an Apache policeman who suffered dire serting ethnic or cultural identity are now emerging as a
consequences because he had acted too much like a powerful focus in Eastern Europe, the Middle East,
white man. At the conclusion to the story, the girl left Africa, and Asia. In this climate of heightened ethnic
the party. When questioned by Basso, the grandmother identification and conflict, we can no longer afford to
explained her granddaughter’s sudden departure by say- ignore the role of cultural practices and mentalities as
ing that she had shot her with an “arrow.” Two years sources of personal and social identity or the role of
later, the young woman told Basso that she threw her self-management and self-regard in all aspects of social
curlers away after reflecting on her grandmother’s life, particularly social conflict (e.g., Crocker, Major,
story. Referring to the place where the Apache police- & Steele, 1998; Kakar, 1996).
man had lived, she said, “I know that place. It stalks me Perhaps less obviously, certain eternal disputes
everyday” (Basso, 1996, p. 57). about the character and causal role of personal identity
750 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

in psychological functioning have resurfaced. Several ated, and often regulated by others. Although cultural
quite disparate currents of thought, including skeptical psychologists acknowledge that the “I” (the subject) is
postmodernism, connectionist-parallel distributed pro- never fully determined by groups’ hegemonic ideologies
cess models in artificial intelligence, and Buddhist or interpersonal forces alone, they aim to clarify the ways
philosophical thought (Elster, 1987; Gergen, 1991; cultural mentalities and practices (including ideologies
Sass, 1992; Varela, Thompson, & Rosch, 1993) have of the self and symbolic products such as biographies of
concluded that the self is illusory or epiphenomenal community exemplars) can powerfully constitute an indi-
and plays no causal role in mental functioning, whereas vidual’s sense of self.
other have argued that the self is “multiple” or “pro- A cultural psychology perspective furthers our un-
tean” (Lifton, 1993). derstanding of a self ’s development by emphasizing that
We will not debate here whether the Buddhists are particular ways of representing and being a self, both as
right that the self is an illusion or whether the Hindus object and subject of experience, are grounded in norma-
are right that the self is the real component of pure tive understandings and behavioral routines of self hood
being. We will simply note that the very existence of in a given sociocultural and historical context (Benson,
human social and moral life seems intimately tied up 2001; Murphy-Berman & Berman, 2003; Oyserman &
with the evolution of a species whose central psycholog- Kemmelmeier, 1995; Oyserman & Markus, 1993).
ical makeup is defined by the existence of a causally From the perspective of cultural psychology, the self
active and somewhat unitary self (“One self per is grounded in the mentalities and practices (the custom
customer,” to use the philosopher Daniel Dennett’s complex) associated with being an “I” (a subject) in a
phrase; Flanagan, 1992): A self that is free, willful, self- particular community. Revisiting the insights of some of
regulating, morally responsible, and conscious; that is the field’s early theorists (e.g., Dewey, 1938; Erikson,
the initiator of action, author of texts, holder of rights; 1968) reveals that culture does not merely surround or
and that is the subject of evaluation and social scrutiny cover the “ universal” child but rather that culture com-
when questions about rationality, responsibility, nor- pletes the child (Bruner, 1990a, 1990b; Tomasello,
mality, and pathology arise. 1999). Culture provides the scripts for “ how to be” and
Putting aside “epiphenomenalism” (the self as un- how to participate as a member in good standing in the
real), the choice between “mechanistic” and “ vitalistic” cultural community and particular social contexts. Si-
conceptions of self seems uninviting (see Kapstein, multaneously, cultural psychologists recognize that chil-
1989). Contemporary mechanistic approaches to per- dren and adults actively constitute their own cultures,
sonal identity argue that our sense of self (e.g., our initiating changes in their relations with others and thus
sense of continuity over time) results merely from the in their immediate cultural settings (see Brandtstädter,
continuity of our memory for discrete mental states Chapter 10; Rathunde & Csikszentmihalyi, Chapter 9,
(perceptions, pleasure, and pain). Contemporary vitalis- this Handbook, this volume).
tic approaches argue that our sense of self is prewired
into the human brain. Neither view leaves much room for
social, interpersonal, or cultural processes in the con-
struction and maintenance of personal identity. DEFINING AND LOCATING THE SELF
Perhaps versions of these approaches can be made AS DYNAMIC, MULTILEVEL,
compatible with cultural psychology, which asserts that AND MULTIFACETED
the self is not fully reducible to either memory or brain
processes. A cultural psychology approach to personal The self can be defined as a multifaceted, dynamic sys-
identity examines the part of our sense of self that devel- tem that regulates and mediates behavior (Banaji &
ops through membership in some local cultural commu- Prentice, 1994; Markus & Wurf, 1986). Neisser (1988,
nity and symbolically mediated experiences with its 1991) explicitly constructs the self as a multileveled en-
practices. This approach examines the effects of labeling tity, defining five types of self-knowledge: (1) ecologi-
and stereotyping, dialogue and narrative, as well as cal, (2) private, (3) interpersonal, (4) conceptual, and
moral agency and social practice on self-functions such (5) what he labels “extended” (i.e., knowledge of the
as self-regard, self-confidence, and self-definition. It self over time). Neisser contends that regardless of dif-
looks at how the self is described, responded to, evalu- fering locations or beliefs, people are active, embodied
Defining and Locating the Self as Dynamic, Multilevel, and Multifaceted 751

agents in the natural and social environments, and thus From this theoretical perspective, the self becomes an
show evidence of ecological and interpersonal selves object of knowledge and it becomes natural to imagine
(Neisser & Jopling, 1997). that the self as represented or the self-concept is the
This multileveled self is believed to be broadly con- most significant aspect of individual experience.
sequential for individual experience: It provides the In research on development of the self-concept, the
blueprint, frame, and foundation for the psyche. What- critical indicator of the self has been visual self-
ever cultural form it assumes, a subject’s way of being a recognition as assessed by mirrors and photographs.
person affords and constrains what he or she feels, val- Summarizing the work in this paradigm, Bullock and
ues, assumes responsibility for, perceives and thinks Lütkenhaus (1990) conclude that self-recognition begins
about, as well as how he or she organizes, understands, between the ages of 9 and 16 months and by 2 years
and gives meaning to any experience. of age, most children recognize themselves in the mirror
The past decade has seen rapidly increasing interest and photographs. Some (e.g., M. Lewis, Sullivan,
in the nature, functioning, and development of self, and Stanger, & Weiss, 1989) have argued that self-awareness
much noteworthy theoretical activity. Several themes requires such self-recognition. Other studies have exam-
can be identified that raise challenging questions about ined speech for cognitive representations of self. During
what a self is, how to determine who has one, as well as their 2nd and 3rd years, children begin to use their
how and when the self emerges, functions, and develops. name, the pronouns “I” and “me,” and possessively
Many researchers advocate analyzing the self as not claim objects as theirs (L. E. LeVine, 1983; Van der
only an object of knowledge but also the subject of expe- Meulen, 1986).
rience. Attention to the embodied self, to the role of in- The idea that thinking about the self is the main ele-
tersubjectivity in constructing the self, to the situation, ment of self is also underscored by a large collection of
context, or niche in which the self participates and to studies that have explicitly tied the development of self
the self as socially engaged rather than isolated or to advancing cognitive capacities (Leadbeter & Dionne,
decontextualized is needed (Cole, 1999; Crook, 2003; 1981; Leahy & Shirk, 1985; Montemayor & Eisen, 1977;
Harter, 1996; Higgins & Parsons, 1983; Stern, 1985; Rosenberg, 1986). Many studies based on the Piagetian
Tomasello, Kruger, & Ratner, 1993). Cultural psychol- model of cognitive development have shown that as a
ogy research forcefully underscores such issues’ impor- child advances from preoperational thought to formal
tance and raises many complementary concerns (e.g., operations, the focus of self-definition shifts from con-
Greenfield & Cocking, 1994; Valsiner, 1988). crete, objective, and visible characteristics to abstract,
private features of the psychological interior. A widely
The Conceptual Self cited review by Harter (1983) notes:
Young children focus on concrete, observable aspects of
The self has been variously defined as the insider’s
self such as physical attributes and behaviors, whereas
grasp on the person, as the answer to the “ who am I” older children increasingly couch their self-descriptions in
question, and as a theory or set of schemas that provides terms of traits. With adolescence, there is a further shift
the individual with a sense of continuity. The focus has toward the use of abstractions and psychological processes
been on the self as object of knowledge (Allport, 1937; such as thoughts, attitudes, and emotions in defining the
Eder & Mangelsdorf, 1997; Epstein, 1973; Kihlstrom & self. (p. 305)
Cantor, 1984; Sullivan, 1940). Historically, despite
many theoretical statements to the contrary, the ten- Still other studies have shown a link between the
dency has been to reify the self and regard it as a thing, level of cognitive development and characteristics of the
as exemplified in thousands of studies on the self- self-concept such as hierarchical organization, integra-
concept, the self, or self-esteem. tion of opposing conceptions of the self, and the stabil-
Research on the development of self has tended to ity of self-conceptions across time and context (see
concentrate on what Neisser labels “ the conceptual Harter, 1990; Rosenberg, 1986, for reviews).
self.” Given the general European American tendency to Indeed, studies with people living in European
imagine the mind as the source of experience and thus American contexts demonstrate that such self-concepts
equate selves with minds, the emphasis on how children exist. These self-concepts include images and concep-
think about and represent the self is perhaps obvious. tions of the person in the present but also in the past and
752 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

in the future—narratives of what could have been and tion of being intelligent. Similarly, developing a self re-
what might be (Higgins, 1990; Markus & Nurius, 1986; quires incorporating the community’s definitions of
Oosterwegel & Oppenheimer, 1993). This self-concept being a self. Once self-development is considered in cul-
is also functional: It mediates behavior and is implicated tural context, it is almost immediately apparent that what
in all aspects of behavior from strong academic and ath- a self is and what it means to be an acceptable or good
letic performance to general well-being and life satis- self can vary dramatically from one cultural place to an-
faction. A negative self-concept of this type is related to other (Markus & Kitayama, 1991b; Shweder & Bourne,
delinquency, drug use, and depression (for a review see 1984). As C. Taylor (1989) has argued:
Bracken, 1996).
My self-definition is understood as an answer to the ques-
tion Who am I. And this question finds its original sense
The Cultural Self in the interchange of speakers. I define who I am by defin-
A cultural psychology approach emphasizes the need to ing where I speak from, in the family tree, in social space,
in the geography of social status and functions. We first
analyze more extensively the experience and under-
learn our languages of moral and spiritual discernment by
standing of self and to examine critically the cultural
being brought into an ongoing conversation by those who
presuppositions implicit in much research on the devel- bring us up. The meanings that the key words first had for
opment of self. As stated earlier, the cultural psychology me are the meanings they have for us, for me and my con-
approach to development is skeptical of most attempts to versation partners together. So I can only learn what
universally define progressive development using ab- anger, love, anxiety, the aspiration to wholeness, and so on
stract criteria, for instance, from behavior to traits or are through my and others’ experience for us in some com-
from context-dependent to context-free. mon place. (p. 35)
In studies of selves in non-European American cul-
tural contexts, one quickly confronts selves that are not
easily characterized by complex mental representation of THE ONTOLOGICAL BASIS OF SELF
traits, attributes, preferences, or possessions. It becomes
evident that studies on the development of self have pro- The study of self in psychology, as with many aspects of
ceeded primarily from one cultural viewpoint and drawn child development, has incorporated the European
on a set of invisible and untested assumptions about the American definition of being a person and has been
self as an idea or as an objectified and cognitively repre- firmly rooted in the ontology of individualism (Green-
sented entity. field & Cocking, 1994; Ho, 1993; Markus & Kitayama,
Comparative research in other cultural communities 1994a; Sampson, 1988). This ontology is extensively in-
suggests that selves are not primarily more-or-less stable corporated in most child-care practices and the main so-
concepts at all but rather a set of processes or ways of cietal institutions such as schools.
being. Moreover, in some groups, a description of self or
others in terms of internal, decontextualized attributes or
The Person as an Individual
qualities simply does not occur (Fajans, 1985; Hart, Feg-
ley, Hung Chan, Mulvey, & Fischer, 1993; Lillard, 1996; The Latin word “individual” means indivisible and
J. G. Miller, 1984; Ochs, 1988; Rosen, 1995; Shweder & whole, and the central tenet of individualism is the epis-
Bourne, 1984). Hart and Edelstein (1992) describe a study temological priority accorded to the separate, essen-
conducted with adolescents in Iceland in which one stu- tially nonsocial, individual. The person is assumed to
dent struggled mightily to answer the “ who am I” question exist independently and to enter into social relations
and finally in despair looked up from his blank question- based on need and by mutual consent with other individ-
naire and asked, “Are people meant to have these kinds of uals. The focus is on the individual rather than on the
thoughts about themselves?” In many cultural communi- social unit of which the individual is a part. The person
ties, the nature of self is referenced not to an internal self is cast as an entity whose behavior is determined by
but instead to a particular social context and characteriz- some amalgam of internal attributes apart from the ex-
ing a self outside that context is unnatural and irrelevant. ternal situation.
Goodnow (1990a, 1990b) has argued that cognitive Individualism is typically analyzed as the critical ele-
development involves learning the community’s defini- ment of Western society (e.g., Baumeister, 1987; Car-
The Ontological Basis of Self 753

rithers, Collins, & Lukes, 1987; Guisinger & Blatt, 1994; of practice and institutions that give it an objective real-
Sampson, 1985; Triandis, Bontempo, & Villareal, 1988), ity. This model is indeed powerful and practical for
and many analysts argue that the Enlightenment gave characterizing selves in European American contexts,
birth to the notion of the Kantian individual and the im- but it is not the only model of how to be. Other ontolo-
portance of individual reason and free will. Others sug- gies and ideologies of human nature exist that are as yet
gest that individualism shows the stamp of late industrial unrepresented in the literature on the development of
capitalism or results from a Cartesian categorization sys- self. Analyzing the self in cultural context brings these
tem that draws a sharp distinction between the self and other ontologies and ideologies of self to light.
others (Lebra, 1992). Another model of self contrasts significantly with in-
In the framework of individualism, it seems natural to dividualism and is pervasive throughout Japan, China,
assume that selves are objects and should be unified and Korea, Southeast Asia as well as much of South America
integrated, reflecting but not focusing on the concerns of and Africa (Triandis, 1989). According to this perspec-
others. Thus, a child’s central task is to progressively real- tive, the self is not and cannot be separate from others or
ize that he or she is separate from others and autonomous, the surrounding social context, but is experienced as in-
efficacious, in control of his or her actions. The idea of a terdependent with the social context: the self-in-
bounded individual separate from and not unduly influ- relation-to-other(s) is focal in individual experience
enced by others also leads to a consistency ethic in which (Markus, Mullally, & Kitayama, 1997; Triandis, 1989,
a good or authentic self is the same, unchanging self 1990). According to Kondo (1990), the self is funda-
across different situations. (See Fiske, Kitayama, Markus, mentally interdependent with others from a Japanese
& Nisbett, 1998; Gergen, 1968; Johnson, 1985; Markus & perspective, and understanding this Japanese sense of
Kitayama, 1994b; Morris, 1994; Shweder & Bourne, self requires dissolving the self-other or self-society
1984, for a more elaborate discussion.) boundary that forms such an obvious starting point in
The literature on the development of self is also re- European American formulations.
plete with unexamined presuppositions that reflect indi- An important imperative in this alternative way of
vidualism interwoven with other cultural and historical being is to avoid becoming separate and autonomous
assumptions. In this literature, interest persists in being from others and, instead, to fit in with others, to fulfill
a true rather than a false self, which probably echoes and create obligations and, in general, to become part of
Victorian concerns with secret or hidden parts of the various interpersonal relationships. Individuals are nat-
self (Baumeister, 1987; Harter, 1986). Currently, there urally understood to exist interdependently with others.
is also pervasive attention to raising children who feel Sharing, interweaving, or intersubjectivity is the estab-
good about themselves and have high self-esteem, aims lished cultural rule, not a mystical or magical project
that derive from societal appropriation of expressing (Ames, Dissanayake, & Kasulis, 1994). From this per-
rather than inhibiting feelings, actualizing the self, and spective, the individual is an open, communicating cen-
fulfilling our potential (Maslow, 1954). The literature ter of relationships and thus is intimately connected
on self development also incorporates presuppositions with other selves. From a Confucian perspective, groups
about what type of parenting practices produce these are not separate from individuals. The nature of individ-
proper or good selves. Thus, the child with high self- uals is to work through others, and to reveal themselves,
esteem is believed to be a product of accepting and ap- they must be parts of groups such as families, communi-
proving parenting that highlights a child’s successes ties, and nations (Tu, 1994). Moreover, sources of action
rather than failures (e.g., Coopersmith, 1967; Heine, are found in a person’s pattern of involvements with oth-
Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama, 1999). ers, rather than internal mental states or processes.
An interdependent view of self does not, as might be
The Person as Relational imagined from a European American perspective, result
in a merging of self and other, nor does it imply that peo-
The individualist model of self that provides the infra- ple lack a sense of themselves as agents originating their
structure for the field’s understanding of self is an obvi- own actions. This interdependent view requires a high
ous, natural model for European American researchers. degree of self-control, self-discipline, and agency to
This model is rooted in a set of Western philosophical effectively adjust oneself to various interpersonal con-
positions about human nature and in layers upon layers tingencies. Control, however, is directed primarily to
754 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

personal desires, goals, and emotions that can disturb the tions. We simply underscore that comparative research
harmonious equilibrium of interpersonal transaction. reveals divergent views of what the self is and should be
This understanding of self stands in contrast to a on a worldwide scale, and that these views critically un-
European American notion of control that entails assert- derpin self-relevant experiences.
ing our desires, goals, and emotions, and attempting to If the self functions as an interpretive, integrative, or
change features of the social situation. Hamaguchi (1985) orienting framework for individual behavior, then whether
for example, reports that for Japanese the “straightfor- one has a self shaped by prevalent European American or
ward claim of the naked ego” (p. 303) is experienced as Asian ontological traditions can matter greatly for indi-
childish. Self-assertion is viewed as a sign not of authen- vidual psychological processing. Comparing behavior con-
ticity, but instead of immaturity. This point is echoed in structed in a European American individualistic frame to
M. I. White and LeVine’s (1986) description of the behavior constructed in other cultural frames may illumi-
meaning of sunao, a term Japanese parents use to charac- nate how cultural processes are implicated in the etiology,
terize what they value in children: nature, and functioning of psychological systems, and how
“multiple, diverse psychologies” arise.
A child that is sunao has not yielded his or her personal
autonomy for the sake of cooperation: Cooperation does
not suggest giving up the self, as it may in the West: It im-
CULTURAL DIVERSITY IN WAYS OF BEING
plies that working with others is the appropriate way of
expressing and enhancing the self. Engagement and har- Much of the rapidly expanding literature relevant to cul-
mony with others is, then, a positively valued goal and the tural variation in the development of self does not focus
bridge—to open-hearted cooperation. (p. 58) directly on the nature and functioning of the psychologi-
cal self system as it has been operationalized in American
Being responsive to or influenced by others does not and European studies. Nevertheless, such comparative lit-
indicate inconsistency or false selves at work; rather, it erature is pertinent to research on the origins of culture-
reflects tolerance, self-control, flexibility, and maturity. specific selves.
In many Asian perspectives, children are assumed to be In examining the culture-specific nature of selves,
both naturally good and capable of developing the Markus et al. (1997) have described the custom com-
needed sensitivity to and empathy for others through en- plexes associated with being a person. They suggest that
couragement and example. The good child is believed to cultural and social groups in every historical period are
be a product of highly responsive parenting practices at- associated with characteristic patterns of sociocultural
tuned to the child. participation or, more specifically, with characteristic
Nevertheless, good parenting does not ignore a child’s ways of being a person in the world, which they call self-
failures, shortcomings, or transgressions. In Japan, chil- ways. Selfways are patterns or orientations, including
dren are encouraged to engage in self-reflection and ways of thinking, feeling, wanting, and doing, that arise
self-criticism as necessary steps to self-improvement from living one’s life in a particular sociocultural con-
and mastery (e.g., C. C. Lewis, 1995). Similarly, Chinese text structured by certain meanings, practices, and insti-
parents often use an explicitly evaluative, self-critical tutions. People do not live generally or in the abstract:
framework with their children as opposed to an overtly They always live according to some specific, substantive
self-affirming one (P. J. Miller et al., 1996). Chinese set of cultural understandings (goals, values, pictures of
caretakers claim that shaming as a caretaking practice the world). Selfways thus include important cultural
keeps children from falling into disgrace or losing their ideas, values, and understandings of what it means to be
all-important connection to others. a self and how to be a good self. Selfways, however, are
Much more could and must be said about these appar- not just matters of belief, doctrine, or ideology, but are
ently startling differences in ontological assumptions, to also manifest in everyday behavior, language practices,
which we are alerted when we conduct research in other patterns of caretaking, schooling, religion, work, the
cultural communities. A comprehensive investigation media, and social episodes, both formal and informal.
would not merely contrast “individualism” and “interde- The notion of selfways implies that every sense of
pendency,” but also draw on other ontologies that must self will be grounded in some shared meanings and cus-
exist on a worldwide scale. Nor should we imply a lack tomary practices and will necessarily bear some mean-
of variability in Japanese, Chinese, or Korean popula- ingful resemblance to similarly grounded selves. From
Cultural Diversity in Ways of Being 755

this perspective, to act, live, or function well in a given ticipation in the custom complexes characterizing Euro-
culture means practicing the underlying cultural views pean American and East Asian cultural contexts. We
of how to be. Thus, although any two American selves aim to highlight the diverse selfways that develop from
will obviously differ in countless ways, as will any two such participation. Most of the recent research has fo-
Japanese selves, cultural participation in either current cused on a contrast between patterns of cultural partici-
American or Japanese practices and institutions will pation that construe the person as an independent,
produce some critical similarities. autonomous entity and those that construe the person as
Speaking probabilistically and in central tendencies, an interdependent part of a larger social unit. Some re-
culturally acceptable European American selves are searchers suggest that variations on the interdependent
likely to be (a) separate, bounded, stable, and consis- pattern characterize about 70% of the world’s popula-
tent; ( b) attribute-based (e.g., based in traits, preferences, tion (Greenfield & Cocking, 1994; Triandis, 1989).
goals); (c) clear, confident, articulated, elaborated; (d) in
control; (e) different from others and uniqueness- Selfways in Some European American Contexts
oriented; (f ) particularly sensitive to positive regard,
self-enhancing; (g) success-oriented; and ( h) expressive Speaking generally and probabilistically, the European
and enthusiastic. Culturally acceptable East Asian American middle-class cultural region is characterized
selves are likely to be (a) connected; ( b) context-based; by selfways that promote independence of the self.
(c) relational, flexible, malleable, responsive to others’ Being a European American person requires the individ-
expectations, preferences, and feelings; (d) similar ualizing of experience. A person’s subjectivity is sensed
to others and concerned with fitting in; (e) particu- as a more-or-less integrated whole, configured by attri-
larly sensitive to potential inadequacy, self-critical; butes and values distinct from others’ or society’s (see
(f ) improvement- and mastery-oriented; (g) open, re- Geertz, 1984). The self is experienced as the individ-
ceptive; and ( h) fully engaged. ual’s meaningful center and is understood to be rooted
From the moment of birth (and even earlier in some in a set of internal attributes such as abilities, talents,
cultural contexts), individuals are given meaning and personality traits, preferences, subjective feeling states,
engaged as persons. Through this cultural participation, and attitudes. A major cultural task often mutually
they become selves. An infant’s mentality or way of pursued by caretakers, friends, and teachers is to contin-
being in the world is thus patterned according to the ually, progressively individualize the child. As re-
meanings and practices of a given cultural community, searchers become aware that conceptualizing the self as
and the communities, in turn, are maintained by these an object and describing one’s self in abstract psycho-
mentalities. A continuous cycle of mutual attunement logical terms are culture-specific tendencies rather than
and coordination connects psychological tendencies consequences of general cognitive development, they
with the social realities on which these tendencies are can investigate practices that afford these tendencies.
brought to bear. From our perspective, features of the Despite an explicit cultural emphasis on being nice
cultural system, such as the characteristic ways in which and caring and helpful (Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swi-
one is led to focus on and attend to others, can become dler, & Tipton, 1985; Deci & Ryan, 1990), development
directly incorporated into individual systems of experi- in the European American style is almost synonymous
encing and organizing the world. They become selfways. with individualizing and decontextualizing the self.
As Ingold (1986, 1991) characterizes it: Even as people seek and maintain interdependence with
others—social tasks that must be accomplished every-
Like organisms, selves become, and they do so within a where—they will maintain a sense of boundedness, rela-
matrix of relations with others. The unfolding of these re- tively greater separation from others, and being in
lations in the process of social life is also their enfolding control. Caring, connecting, and relationality will likely
within the selves that are constituted within this process, assume a more individually agentic form. Many cultural
in their specific structures of awareness and response—
practices that contribute to a sense of agency are so
structures which are, at the same time, embodiments of
much a part of everyday, domestic life that they are, for
personal identity. (p. 367)
all practical purposes, invisible.
In the following section, we briefly review studies In many English-speaking cultural communities, lan-
relevant to how selfways develop through cultural par- guage use itself helps create the decontextualized, agentic
756 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

“I.” Ikegama (1991) notes that English is a language involve autobiographies and personal narratives, and art
“ which focuses on the human being and which gives lin- projects focus on self-representation. Many middle-
guistic prominence to this notion, while Japanese is a class American children are thus continually encouraged
language which tends to suppress the notion of the and given chances to express themselves and present
human being, even if such a being is involved in the their ideas in speech and writing. A common elementary
event ” (p. 301). In characterizing an event, English fo- practice in school is show-and-tell: Children bring items
cuses on the particular person involved, but Japanese then stand before the class and tell a story about their
emphasizes the event as a whole, submerging the indi- object. All these everyday practices foster an objectifi-
vidual in it. Because the human subject is foregrounded cation of self and a sense of self as a source of action
in English, one might say “I have a temperature,” but in (Heine et al., 1999).
Japanese, one would say roughly “As for me, there is a Moreover, children are encouraged individually and
temperature” or “A temperature goes with me.” The En- collectively to think of themselves positively as stars,
glish phrase “John ran out of money” might become “As winners, above average, and the repositories of special
for John, money became null” in Japanese. qualities. It has become routine for every child who
Along with foregrounding the subject (the “I”) in En- plays on a soccer or basketball team to receive a trophy.
glish, American English speakers tend to be direct and This practice may discourage competition or invidious
assume that a speaker must make him- or herself clear to comparison among team members, but it underscores
listeners. This tendency begins early. Compared to Japa- the importance, not of the group, but of each individual.
nese mothers, American mothers talk more, and more Even as many current educators worry that they may
directly, to their children (Azuma, Kashiwagi, & Hess, have used praise too liberally in the past and try to focus
1981; Caudill & Weinstein, 1969). In a study of mother- on the learning process rather than on evaluating the
infant interaction patterns, Morikawa, Shand, and Ko- child (Damon, 1984, 1995; Damon & Hart, 1988),
sawa (1988) compared American and Japanese mothers teachers are persistently urged to find some unique as-
interacting with their 3-month-old infants. Americans pect of each child’s product.
mothers elicited more vocalizations and exhibited more American children are also encouraged to be inde-
expressions of positive affect, and vocalizations oc- pendent, autonomous, and self-determining (Bellah
curred while mothers looked at the baby and the baby et al., 1985). Families in European American middle
was happy and alert. This pattern contrasts with the class contexts often give infants their own beds and
Japanese studied who more often expressed negative af- bedrooms to foster autonomy (Shweder et al., 1995).
fect and did so while the baby was looking away. Similarly, most developmental markers center on au-
Direct, explicit verbal instruction characterizes tonomous activity—rolling over, sitting up, walking,
teacher-child interactions in the American cultural con- and eating by oneself.
text (Tobin, Wu, & Davidson, 1989; Wu, 1994). In such Additionally, American children are socialized to
interactions, the child’s distinctive attributes are identi- have distinct preferences. Long before the child is old
fied, then persistently noted and affirmed. Personal at- enough to answer, caretakers pose questions like “Do
tributes and abilities are assumed to define the self—to you want the blue cup or red cup?” Caretakers’ ques-
sustain one’s uniqueness and drive one’s current, past, tions signal to children that the capacity for independent
and possible actions. The explicit goal is the develop- choice is a necessary, desirable attribute (Markus & Ki-
ment of individuals’ potential. American institutions tayama, 1994a), and thereby instantiate an “ethics of
may realize the idea of defining attributes by grouping autonomy” that prevails in certain cultural communities
and tracking children according to ability (Stevenson & but not others (Haidt et al., 1993; Shweder et al., 1997).
Stigler, 1992). When resources permit, the curriculum Moreover, the availability of choice necessitates prefer-
is often individualized on the assumption that each child ences by which to make choices. Preschool settings are
has a unique learning style and pace. arranged such that children have an extensive choice of
The American schoolchild is objectified, made to feel activities and need not conform to the group except dur-
special, praised, encouraged, and complimented. In ing limited parts of the day (C. C. Lewis, 1995). Pre-
many schools, children may be a VIP or star for a day or sumably, such efforts to incorporate self-determination
a week, celebrate their birthdays, and be honored into the school day are designed to safeguard the child’s
(Markus & Kitayama, 1994a). Writing projects often intrinsic motivation.
Cultural Diversity in Ways of Being 757

This is an instance of culture and psyche making each low self-esteem matched the consensual self more
other up. Particular kinds of psychological processes closely. Such findings suggest that adolescents’ tendency
(e.g., construing the self as a positive, unique entity) to characterize themselves in abstract terms stems from
emerge through years of socialization and enculturation a particular collective idea of “ how to be” rather than
as an individual receives then perpetuates social prac- from increased cognitive ability. These findings are con-
tices and meanings recurrent in a given cultural commu- sistent with several other studies on cultural variation in
nity (Kitayama, Markus, Matsumoto, & Norasakkunkit, self-categorization (Cousins, 1989; Harter, 1983; Trian-
1997). These psychological processes, in turn, repro- dis, 1990), which suggest that internal features of the
duce those same cultural patterns. Becoming a self (a self—the traits, attributes, and attitudes—are privileged
meaningful cultural participant) in a European Ameri- and regarded as critical to self-definition.
can context thus involves maintaining an autonomous Furthermore, in a study comparing the self-efficacy
self separate from other selves and the social context. levels of children from Los Angeles, East and West
Those with such independent selves may be more at- Berlin, and Moscow, researchers (Little, Oettingen,
tuned to positive characteristics of the self, and may be Stetsenko, & Baltes, 1995) found that children from Los
especially motivated to discover them, express them in Angeles had the most optimistic self-efficacy beliefs,
public, and confirm them in private. They often develop while children from East Berlin had the most pes-
processes to buoy their own self-esteem. simistic beliefs about personal efficacy. The authors ar-
The data on European American selves in middle- gued that the higher self-efficacy ratings of the Los
class contexts support these generalizations. Examining Angeles elementary school students reflect the high lev-
earliest childhood memories and self-reports, Wang els of individualism and the low power distance between
(2001a) found that Americans described themselves students and teachers.
using individual attributes and reported lengthy, spe- Oettingen (1995) argues that efficacy depends
cific, self-focused, and emotionally elaborate memo- largely not only on one’s own evaluations in individual-
ries. Chinese self-reports were briefer and respondents ist cultures but also on evaluation by in-group members
described themselves by roles. Their memories centered in collectivist cultures. In cultures with a large power
on collective activities, general routines, and emotion- disparity between members, children will tend to treat
ally neutral events. parents and teachers as clear superiors. Children in cul-
In a series of studies with young children, Hart and tures with a lesser power disparity will have more op-
his colleagues (Hart, 1988; Hart & Edelstein, 1992) portunities to see themselves as “origins” of their own
asked American children to imagine a “person machine” actions. It would be interesting to know how those chil-
that makes the original person (respondent) disappear dren of Bielefeld, North Germany, who were reared to
but at the same time manufactures other people (copies be hyper-independent and unspoiled (Grossmann &
of the original) who receive some, but not all, of the Grossmann, 1981, 1991; Grossmann et al., 1985; and
original person’s characteristics. The respondent’s task see earlier discussion of culture-specific presupposi-
is to judge whether the new manufactured person with tions in attachment research) would have fared on a self-
the same physical attributes ( looks like respondent), efficacy index.
with the same social attributes ( has same family and As researchers develop methods of assessing the
friends), or with the same psychological attributes selves of ever-younger children, they have found that
(same thoughts and feelings) will be most like the origi- apprehending and experiencing the world through
nal person. They have found that by the ninth grade most bounded selfways begins quite early in individualistic
respondents believe the copy with the original’s psycho- cultural communities. European American children ap-
logical characteristics is most like the original. pear to have some sense of who they are and which at-
Consistent with a number of earlier studies of the de- tributes characterize them by 3 years of age (Eder &
velopment of the self-concept (e.g., Harter, 1983), Stein, Mangelsdorf, 1997), and by 4 years of age they show
Markus, and Moeser (1996) have found that 11- to 14- psychological tendencies that reflect the cultural em-
year-old European Americans asked to describe them- phasis on individualization and separation from others.
selves depict a consensual self whose attributes include They describe themselves as better than their peers in
being caring, friendly, nice, and worried. Moreover, the all domains, and studies with adults suggest that this
self-descriptions of adolescents with high rather than type of self-serving bias or false uniqueness correlates
758 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

positively with self-esteem (Josephs, Markus, & and is configured by constantly referencing the self to
Tarafode, 1992). In a cultural system organized around the situational setting or context.
promoting the individuality and uniqueness of the self, Throughout much of the world, the task of child rear-
a general proclivity to positive self-regard has positive ing is not the European American one of making a de-
social and psychological consequences. pendent baby into an independent adult, but instead one
of cultivating an unruly asocial baby into a civilized so-
Selfways in Some East Asian Cultural Contexts cial being (Caudill & Weinstein, 1969). The need to up-
hold interpersonal obligations as well as to maintain
A rapidly expanding literature in psychology, anthropol- connection and harmony with others is highly elabo-
ogy, and philosophy now provides an increasingly nu- rated in every sphere of social life. Caudill and Wein-
anced understanding of the cultural form of Japanese, stein (1986) find that Japanese mothers hold their 3- to
Chinese, and Korean selves. Systematic analyses of 4-month-old infants more and have more body commu-
Japanese settings reveal a pervasive concern with and nication with them than mothers in the United States.
attention to the relational side of social life and to indi- Furthermore, as noted earlier, co-sleeping and co-
vidual position in the social structure (Bachnik, 1994; bathing are common. Sleeping babies are rarely left
Lebra, 1993; Peak, 1987; Rosenberger, 1992). Markus alone in Japan. The close, fully interdependent mother-
and Kitayama (1991a, 1991b) suggest that Asian self- child dyad is particularly idealized in Japan, and many
ways emphasize the fundamental relatedness of individ- other relationships (e.g., between boss and subordinate)
uals to each other and that the relationship rather than are organized around this familial model. Greenfield
the individual may be a functional unit of conscious- and Cocking (1994) characterize interdependent and in-
ness, and claim: dependent developmental scripts as sharply contrasting,
intertwined but never fully balanced. From an East
Experiencing interdependence entails seeing oneself as
part of an encompassing social relationship and recogniz-
Asian cultural perspective, a self European American
ing that one’s behavior is determined by, contingent on, style—distinct, positive, and attribute-based—is not a
and to a large extent, organized by what the actor per- mature, fully civilized form of human agency. A
ceived to be the thoughts, feelings and actions of others in strongly held, clear sense of self signals childishness be-
the relationship. (p. 227) cause it entails failure to take full account of and show
sufficient regard for the relationships of which the self
Lebra (1994) argues that among the Japanese, empa- is a part. Agency Japanese style results from (a) the
thy is a psychological mainstay and must be understood sense that the self is afforded and appreciated through
if one is to comprehend almost any aspect of Japanese the relationships in which it participates, and ( b) the
behavior. Empathy (omoiyari) “refers to the ability and sense that the self must flexibly maintain and further
willingness to feel what others are feeling, to vicari- the welfare of those relationships.
ously experience the pleasure or pain that they are un- Such a sense of agency does not mean that the self is
dergoing, and to help them satisfy their wishes” (Lebra, passive or just going with the flow. Inter-individual har-
1976, p. 38). Lebra sees this focus as diametrically op- mony requires active attention. Mulder (1992), in de-
posed to the self-focus common in many European scribing Indonesian harmony (rukun), notes that it “does
American practices. not come as a gift but is the result of the active orienta-
This emphasis on empathy implies that Japanese tion toward mutual respect and adjustment to each
selves should not be conceptualized as lacking individu- other.” From this orientation, one must be empathic and
ality or a separate identity or that autonomy is unimpor- flexible to avoid hurting or embarrassing others by one’s
tant in Japan (Greenfield & Cocking, 1994; Kim, 1987; action. Oerter et al. (1996), in characterizing a Japanese
Oerter, Oerter, Agostiani, Kim, & Wibowo, 1996). It perspective on human nature, quotes a respondent as
does imply, however, that such empathic ways of being a claiming that “adulthood” means “good understanding,
self, which explicitly highlight the state of being-in- being flexible and following the general rules set by so-
relation, are different from selfways that emphasize and ciety. The more you grow as an adult, the smaller your
reify the individual. In this particular Japanese mode of own private fantasy world becomes. . . . You become
being, subjectivity is sensed as interdependence with a softer-minded, better fitted to the society, but you also
larger whole that includes both the person and others become less sensitive” (p. 41).
Cultural Diversity in Ways of Being 759

Interdependence characterizes many different aspects the primary aims of Japanese education and is fostered
of Japanese life. As noted previously, the Japanese lan- in many routine practices. Classroom walls are adorned
guage minimizes the person as an agent. Self-reference in with group goals, such as “let’s cooperate” or “let’s pool
Japanese is accomplished with an elaborated set of com- our strength” (C. C. Lewis, 1995).
municative customs based on individual status in each Similarly, Chinese parents and teachers consider de-
particular relationship. The Japanese word for self (jibun) veloping a sense of connectedness to be essential for
means “my share of the shared space between us.” children’s socialization. Child-rearing practices focus
Japanese-style interdependence emphasizes living cor- on obedience, reliability, proper behavior, social obliga-
rectly and self-improvement. tion, and group achievement (P. J. Miller et al., 1997;
Even maternal practices toward infants in Japan seem Mullen & Yi, 1995; Wu, 1996). In a study that content-
to involve communicating correct and expected ways to analyzed children’s stories and early memories, Wang
be. Caudill and Schooler (1973) noted that Japanese and Leichtman (2000) found that compared to American
mothers’ speech is apparently aimed to shape directly children, Chinese children showed greater orientation
their infants’ physical and emotional states in normative toward social engagement, a greater concern with moral
directions. They seem especially concerned with pre- correctness, greater concern with authority, a less au-
venting infants’ crying and with calming babies whose tonomous orientation, more expressions of emotion, and
serenity has been disturbed (Morikawa et al., 1988). more situational details. To illustrate these differences,
Compared to European American mothers, Japanese Wang and Leichtman provided the following examples
mothers appear especially alert to infant vocalizations, from 6-year-olds:
which they often interpret as signs of distress to
Memory 1 (American boy): When I didn’t get to get a toy.
which they must respond (Bornstein, Azuma, Tamis-
I like to get lots of Legos and there was an underwater one,
LeMonda, & Ogino, 1990). American mothers, by con- and it was a little big. It was a medium one and it had one
trast, seem to talk more to their infants, without trying to of the things I’ve been waiting for. But I forget what it was.
directly influence their infants’ behavior. Bornstein and
Memory 2 (Chinese boy): One day, my mom bought many
colleagues cite a Japanese mother saying to her 3-month-
flower seeds. They were alive. She planted them there. I
old baby who was looking away (“What is wrong with
stepped on them by accident. Then my mom scolded me
you?” and “Look at me”). Apparently, to reestablish a and spanked me twice. So I cried. I felt a little angry that
dyadic connection when the infants were gazing away she spanked me.
from them, Japanese mothers were more likely than
Americans to express negative affect, try to establish Differences by cultural context are especially evi-
mutual gaze, or seek information (see also Rothbaum, dent with respect to emotions (Mesquita, 2001; Tsai,
Pott, & Azuma, 2000; Rothbaum, Weisz, & Pott, 2000; Simenova, & Watanabe, 1999). In many middle-class
Shimizu & LeVine, 2001). European American contexts, emotions constitute an
Such East Asian parenting and teaching practices en- important aspect of the self and should be emphasized
courage interdependence through interaction and mu- and explained as one develops individuality. In Chinese
tual engagement. When American mothers and toddlers contexts, emotions result from the child’s relations with
interact with a new toy, American mothers typically significant others and serve to both encourage proper
focus on the object and draw the child’s attention to it. behavior in the child and reinforce a sense of connect-
Japanese mothers use the toy to engage the child in a re- edness (Wang, 2001b).
lational or interactive game and do much less talking, Studies of East Asian child-rearing and schooling
explaining, and questioning (Bornstein et al., 1990; Fer- practices also suggest an emphasis on knowing one’s
nald & Morikawa, 1993). According to Lewis’s (1995) place, role, station, and duties in the social order, partic-
survey of over 50 Japanese preschools, the focus is on ularly in Chinese cultural contexts that explicitly value
developing children’s connections to one another and self-improvement, order, and hierarchy. In a study of
engaging them in the pleasures of group life. Instead of Chinese American and European American mothers’ be-
celebrating individual success, special events recognize liefs about what matters for raising children, Chao (1992)
the whole group. Children routinely produce group pic- found that Chinese American mothers stressed sensitiv-
tures or storyboards, and no child may go to the play- ity to other’s expectations and the situation, while Euro-
ground until all are ready. Attention to others is among pean American mothers emphasized nurturing the child’s
760 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

sense of self. Here the former were primarily concerned quired to perform complicated activities, such as ar-
with the hierarchical, interdependent relationship be- ranging their lunch boxes or putting on their clothes for
tween self and others and the integrity that derives from outside activities, in the required way (Peak, 2001). As
doing your duty, but the latter were primarily concerned a child, being part of a family or a school group often
with furthering their children’s independence and en- means thinking about the social unit and your place in
couraging a strong, positive, even assertive, self-regard. it, and then doing what is proper for this situation. It
Chao (1993a, 1993b) found an emphasis on order and involves considerations such as “What do my parents or
respect for hierarchy among Chinese American mothers. my peers want me to do?” or “Did I do what they
They scored higher on scales of parental control, author- wanted me to do?” In a Japanese cultural context, a
itarianism, and what Chao calls “Chinese child-rearing sense of self is developed by being finely attuned to the
ideologies” than their European American counterparts. expectations of others, by not being left out of their
These Chinese American mothers were more likely to sympathy, and by making sure you are part of the so-
endorse items such as “I have strict, well-established cial process. Perceptually, cognitively, emotionally,
rules for my child,” “I do not allow my child to question and motivationally, others—the encompassing social
my decisions,” “I make sure I know where my child is unit, the group and its standards of excellence—are
and what he is doing at all times,” “I teach my child that important. Thus, the most useful kind of information
in one way or another punishment will find him when he about the self concerns your shortcomings, problems,
is bad,” “Mothers can teach children by pointing [out] or negative features. Self-criticism is encouraged in all
good behavior in other children,” “When children con- societal settings from the classroom to the boardroom.
tinue to disobey you, they deserve a spanking,” and Cultural participation entails discovering what may be
“Children should be in the constant care of their moth- lacking in your behavior and then closing the gap be-
ers or family members.” tween the actual and expected behavior (Kitayama
Similarly, Rohner and colleagues found that, in con- et al., 1997; Markus & Kitayama, 1994a, 1994b).
trast to Americans, Korean adolescents perceived In Japan, a constant focus on social expectations and
parental control as manifesting not hostility and mistrust meeting them appears to go hand in hand with a simulta-
but love and concern (Rohner, 1984; Rohner & Pettengill, neous focus on self-improvement and self-criticism.
1985). Notably, strictly controlling parental practices In studies comparing self-improving and self-enhancing
that aim to create a morally dutiful, disciplined sense of motivations (Heine et al., 1999, 2001), Japanese partici-
agency are also found in some European American con- pants who failed on an initial task persisted more on a
texts and merit further study. follow-up task than those who succeeded. In contrast,
Differences in selfways also come across in play prac- North Americans who failed persisted less on a follow-
tices. Farver, Kim, and Lee (1995) found that in free up task. In Japanese contexts, failures are important and
play among European American and Korean American diagnostic and thus serve to highlight where corrective
preschoolers, European American children primarily efforts are needed. The emphasis on self-improvement
described their own actions, rejected their partners’ as a virtue can be seen everywhere in Japanese life. An
suggestions, and used directives (e.g., “I am your king! advertisement urging Japanese workers to take vaca-
Do not obey the bad king! I’ll save you!”). Korean Amer- tions exhorted, “Let’s become masters at refreshing our-
ican children described their partners’ actions, used tag selves” (New York Times, May 1995).
questions, semantic ties, statements of agreement, and The desire for self-improvement has cognitive con-
polite requests (e.g., “He is a king, isn’t he? He’s the bad sequences: Many Japanese tend to focus on areas need-
guy, isn’t he? The good guy caught him, right?”). ing improvement while discounting positive aspects of
Overall, in many East Asian contexts, personal their performance. This tendency to discount the posi-
sense of well-being is tied less to meeting an individ- tive is often misinterpreted by European Americans as
ual’s goals (the ethics of autonomy) and more to doing self-depreciation, but in Japan it works very well to es-
what is required in a given situation, or doing some- tablish the person as a community member in good
thing the right or appropriate way (ethics of commu- standing. Humility might better describe this culturally
nity; for more on the ethics of autonomy and valued disposition. In contrast to European Americans,
community see Shweder et al., 1997). Already in the who often focus on a self ’s positive features and
first months of preschool, Japanese children are re- equate self-improvement with individual achievement,
Cultural Diversity in Ways of Being 761

Japanese are more sensitive to a self ’s negative fea- cultural practice. As Vygotsky (1987) suggests, “Every
tures in a given context. function in the child’s development appears twice; first
Further, Japanese practices are often framed in on the social level, and after on the individual level, first
terms of hitonami (average as a person). Difficult as it between people (interpsychological) and then inside the
may be for European Americans to believe, many Japa- child (intrapsychological)” (p. 57).
nese feel relieved to know that they are average because
being different entails the risk of being insensitive to
Selfways in Still Other Cultural Contexts
and not belonging in your community. From an interde-
pendent perspective, the self seems best described as a We have focused on the comparison between European
process of self-improvement that requires being sensi- American and East Asian children to highlight diver-
tive to the expectations of others and not disrupting har- gent selfways. Nevertheless, important variations in
mony or equipoise. selfways can also be found within cultural groups and
Studies of self in Asian contexts all point to self- are increasingly a focus of attention (Strickland, 2000).
criticism as integral to the self. Chinese respondents an- For example, Harwood and colleagues (Harwood &
swer the Twenty Statements Test using fewer positive Miller, 1991; Harwood, Schoelmerich, Ventura-Cook,
statements about themselves than Americans do (Bond Schultz, & Wilson, 1996) compared Anglo and Puerto
& Cheung, 1983; Karasawa, 1998). Ryff, Lee, and Na Rican mothers in the United States. They found that rel-
(1995) also found that Korean respondents were more ative to Puerto Rican mothers, both middle- and lower-
likely to endorse negative than positive statements about class Anglo mothers placed significantly greater value
themselves; European American respondents showed the on self-confidence and independence and significantly
reverse pattern. Stigler, Smith, and Mao (1985) found less value on obedience, the capacity for relatedness,
similar results among Chinese and United States ele- and proper demeanor. Miller, Potts, Fung, Hoogstra,
mentary school students’ perceptions of competence: and Mintz, 1990; Miller and Hoogstra, 1992; Miller,
Chinese students rated their competence lower in cogni- Mintz, Fung, Hoogstra, and Potts, 1992, report that au-
tive, physical, and general domains than did European tonomy is important in both working-class and middle-
Americans. class U.S. communities, but is fostered differently
These studies suggest that members in good standing according to social class. In a study of the co-narration
in Asian cultural contexts avoid calling attention to the of mothers and toddlers in two Midwestern communi-
self, deemphasize their own specialness, and adjust to ties, they found that middle-class mothers accorded
the immediate situations in which they participate. Even children speech and author privileges in creating stories
the universally necessary social tasks of individuation, about past events, and were less likely than working-
independence, and maintaining autonomy are grounded class mothers to challenge the toddlers’ versions of
in an appreciation of interdependence. reality. Working-class mothers also granted children
These orientations exist at odds with the very prac- speaker rights—and involved children in longer co-
tice of personal self-description and thus with com- narratives than middle-class children—but challenged
monly exported social science methods requiring people children more often to tell the “correct ” version rather
to evaluate and categorize the self. Interdependent (or than their own story. Similarly, recent ethnographic
sociocentric) and individualistic (or independent or ego- accounts of selves in working-class contexts find less
centric) mentalities and practices are distinct and may focus on self-confidence, self-expression, and develop-
require different methods of study. Considered in their ing potential and more emphasis on stability, integrity,
respective cultural contexts, notably, these two selfways and resilience (Harwood, Miller, & Irizarary, 1995;
are equally normal, reasonable, or viable ways of being, Kusserow, 1999; Snibbe & Markus, 2004).
although they are associated with patterned or system- A study by Rogoff et al. (1993) comparing U.S.
atic diversity in psychological functioning. middle-class and Guatemalan Mayan toddlers finds that
Both types—individualistic and interdependent self- autonomy also matters in the socialization of Mayan chil-
ways—involve the participation and support of others dren, particularly among toddlers who are accorded
and are saturated with cultural meanings. The instantia- special privileges and not expected to conform to
tion and realization of individualism is then a type of in- the same rules as older siblings. In contrast to the Ameri-
terdependence and a socially endorsed and constructed can children, however, older Mayan siblings cooperated
762 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

interdependently with the toddlers without caregiver in- portance of what Neisser (1988, 1991) terms the inter-
tervention, suggesting that among the Mayans developing personal self. Selves are constituted and develop in inter-
autonomy is associated with understanding that one is an action with specific others (J. M. Baldwin, 1911; M. W.
interdependent member of the community and is not as Baldwin & Holmes, 1987; Cooley, 1902; Hallowell,
completely autonomous as infants are allowed to be. 1955; Ingold, 1991; Rosaldo, 1984; Shweder & LeVine,
Only recently have investigators began to describe 1984). Echoing Mead (1934) and the early symbolic in-
how contexts other than European American and East teractionists, the self literature includes a growing ap-
Asian ones influence the constitution of self. For exam- preciation of the dynamic, socially constructed nature of
ple, a review of parenting among U.S. Latino families self. This idea has the appeal of potentially bridging the
finds that despite considerable within-group heterogene- gap between focusing primarily on the individual as a
ity, Latino parents tend to emphasize the main cultural cultural learner (Tomasello et al., 1993) versus on the
values of respeto and familismo (Harwood, Leyendecker, cultural collective of which the individual is an interde-
Carlson, Asencio, & Miller, 2002). Respeto refers to pendent part (Cole, 1995). This synthesis promises to get
maintaining proper demeanor, which involves knowing the person back in the practice and the practice back in
the proper decorum required in a given situation with the person.
people of a particular age, sex, and social status. Famil- Efforts to understand the mutual constitution of self
ismo refers to a belief system linking loyalty, reciproc- and other in the development of self, or just how it is that
ity, and solidarity toward members of the family, which selves and others make each other up, are ongoing. Out-
is seen as an extension of the self (Cortés, 1995). side of middle-class European American cultural com-
African American parenting, according to a review munities many people prefer crowded living conditions
by McAdoo (2002), must often address persistent issues and regard the physical presence of others, especially
that African American children confront, such as deval- family members, as essential to mental health and well-
uation of their own worth and future potential, inade- being. Peak (1987), writing about Japan, claims that be-
quate financial resources, and the challenge of teaching coming a person involves learning to appreciate the
children about race. Moreover, in African American pleasures of group life and living in human society.
contexts maintaining communal family traditions is es- Similarly, Ochs (1988) reports that Samoans are self-
sential. Coresidential extended families and their sup- conscious about their need for others to acknowledge
port systems are common and regarded as an important and sympathize with them. Menon’s (1995) interviews
survival system for African American families (Hatch- with Oriya Hindu women living in extended joint family
ett, Cochran, & Jackson, 1991). households reveal that in local moral worlds steeped in
Research in cultural psychology challenges some gen- an ethics of community the idea of living alone while
eralizations about self and self-development and signifi- sane and happy is almost a contradiction in terms (see
cantly strengthens others. In the next decade, these shifts also Kakar, 1978).
will likely result in new paradigms for studying the self. In much European American research on the devel-
Recent theoretical debates and discussions in psychology opment of self, others become relevant when selves
and anthropology aim to clarify and elaborate the gen- learn to take the perspectives of these others and get in-
eral propositions that the self is (a) constituted in inter- side their heads (Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1995), or as
action with others, ( b) collectively constructed through specific relationships are forged with particular others.
sociocultural participation, and (c) a product of history It is increasingly evident, however, that others have a
(see Elder & Shanahan, Chapter 12, this Handbook, this pervasive impact on any person’s psychological develop-
volume). Each of these propositions reflects a central ment throughout life in all cultural contexts. Even prior
claim of cultural psychology, namely, that processes of to birth, individuals are immersed in social relations and
self-functioning encompass not only a single psychology activities. Human infants only become selves through
but also multiple psychologies. their engagement in particular, culturally organized set-
tings (Markus et al., 1997; Weisner, 1982, 1984, 1987).
The Self Is Constituted in Interaction with Others Ever more investigators now assume that mutual involve-
It is an old idea that one cannot be a self by one’s self. ment of self and others is so fundamental to human
Although life in the middle-class European American functioning that others are automatically perceived as
cultural region has highlighted the conceptual self, stud- relevant to one’s sense of self. Gopnik (1993) refers to
ies of self in other cultural locations underscore the im- an innate bridge or intersubjectivity between self and
Cultural Diversity in Ways of Being 763

others. Infants are responsive to others’ affective ex- (1973a), “may finally be that we all begin with the natu-
pressions, and thus others are immediately expected, ral equipment to live a thousand kinds of life but end in
implicated, and involved in one’s becoming a self (see the end having lived only one” (p. 45). Every person par-
also Ingold, 1991). ticipates in combinations of significant cultural settings
or niches, which in contemporary American society
The Self Develops through could include specific groups, such as the family or
Sociocultural Participation workplace, as well as contexts defined by ethnicity, reli-
A cultural psychology perspective places considerable gion, profession, social class, gender, birth cohort, and
emphasis on what Kitayama and colleagues (Kitayama, sexual orientation. Some of the remarkable variation
Markus, Matsumoto, & Norasakkunkit, 1995; Markus among people arises at least in part because people are
et al., 1997) term the collective construction of self. The unlikely to participate in identical configurations of
concept is that selves develop in a dynamic, recursive group memberships. Even those living in similar config-
process in which sociocultural participation in a given urations of cultural contexts will diverge in the specifics
cultural system of meanings, practices, and institutions of their everyday, symbolically mediated experiences
affords characteristic tendencies of the self that further and due to prior, innate, received, or temperamental dif-
serve to integrate the person into the meanings and ferences in their sense of self will differentially attend
practices of a given cultural community (see also Bour- to, seek out, elaborate, or reflect some features of these
dieu, 1972; Giddens, 1984; Martin, Nelson, & Tobach, experiences and not others. Moreover, participation or
1995). This perspective emphasizes that from their ear- engagement in the activities of a given cultural setting
liest moments, selves arise from being a person in par- can assume divergent forms. Cultural participation can
ticular worlds. From a child’s earliest days, partial, be straightforward and unquestioning, resistant, or
incomplete, rudimentary gestures and vocalizations are ironic. Consequently, there is little danger that people of
“infused with specific meanings and significances cru- the same sociocultural and historical niches will be
cial to enabling the child to become a progressively clones of one another. Between-group differences do not
more competent partner ” (Bruner, 1993, p. 532). Chil- imply within-group homogeneity.
dren are immediately engaged in the settings of daily
The Self as a Historical Product
life and are subject to the specific normative expecta-
tions and the institutional entailments of what Super A cultural psychology approach to the development of
and Harkness (1986) label a “developmental niche.” self has led researchers to appreciate Bourdieu’s idea
People always live in culture-specific ways. To live oth- that processes of self are “ history turned into nature”
erwise is impossible. (1991, p. 7). Many Western researchers focusing on the
Super and Harkness’s theorizing is one of many at- self have participated in their discipline long enough
tempts to resolve the tension between psychology’s ex- now to have observed historical change in the European
cesses in viewing development as natural growth or an American cultural zone in the natural and normative
unfolding of abilities in stages and anthropology’s ex- self. In the late 1960s and early 1970s, there was a need
cesses in viewing development as cultural molding or to discover the true self and feelings. Currently, there is
conditioning. Super and Harkness claim that a child’s the need to say no not yes to experience and to create the
developmental experience is regulated by (a) the set- proper self. Many current self researchers were them-
tings—physical and social—in which the child lives; ( b) selves raised according to the dictates of Dr. Spock but
the customs of child care and child rearing; and (c) the as parents find his prescriptions rigid and inappropriate.
mentality of the caretakers. These three mutually inter- Similarly, American educators note that requiring chil-
active subsystems function together with other elements dren to be happy and feel good about themselves has
of the large culture and environment to constitute a produced a generation of children with high self-esteem
culture-specific child. and no basic skills. Programs under development aim to
Cultural psychology’s approach to the study of self raise the educational expectations for American chil-
does not deny the individuality, idiosyncrasy, and dren and to replace an emphasis on positive self-
uniqueness of the self observable in even the most tight- evaluation with an emphasis on building specific skills
knit and coherent collectives. Children do not become (Damon, 1995).
general people; they become particular persons or selves. General societal imperatives of “ the way to be”
One of the most significant facts about us, writes Geertz promulgated by the advertising industry and media have
764 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

a strong effect on nurturing practices and on both lay The opening material—on separations between
and scientific conceptions of self. In a comprehensive thought and action—focuses on links between ways of
historical overview of the American self, Cushman thinking and ways of acting, on the nature of activities
(1995, p. 24) argues that to understand the formation of or practices, and on the concept of participation. The
the American self one must understand the interplay be- section that follows—reexamining separations between
tween this nation and what it means to be American, be- hearts and minds—links ways of thinking to feelings,
tween what it means to be an American and what it values, and identities. To the analysis of cognitive devel-
means to be human, and between the construction of the opment, the argument runs, we need to bring the study
self and the construction of the country. It is of no small of accompanying feeling states. We need also to add the
significance to the renewed field of cultural psychology recognition that areas of competence and ways of learn-
that self researchers are beginning to heed Kessen’s ing differ in the extent to which they are seen as impor-
claim (1983): tant by the individual or by others, and in the extent to
which they are seen as appropriate to a person’s current
The study of children is not exclusively or even mainly a self or possible selves.
scientific enterprise in the narrow sense but stretches out For the third large concern—moving beyond tradi-
toward philosophy and history and demography. If we were tional separations between persons and contexts—the
to recognize such an expanded definition of child study, material is in two parts. Both stem from the challenge of
we might anticipate a new (science) whose object of study giving specific shape to the general recognition that per-
is not the true child or a piece of the true child but the sons and contexts make each other up. The first starts by
changing diversity of children. (pp. 37–38; see also Bron- considering changes in approaches to the specification
fenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner, Kessel, Kessen, &
of contexts, asking how these fit with views of cognitive
White, 1985; Kessel & Siegel, 1983; Lerner, Chapter 1,
development. Here we give special attention to accounts
this Handbook, this volume)
of contexts as both exerting pressure and allowing inno-
vation, as changing rather than static, as heterogenous
rather than monolithic and as composed of contested or
CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY AND competing positions, with room for individuals to nego-
COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT tiate what is to be learned or what may be questioned.
The later part starts by considering changes in accounts
In the previous sections, we have been considering some of cognition and cognitive development, asking how
selected content areas, using these to bring out features context enters into these. Here we give special attention
of cultural psychology and to demonstrate how these to accounts of cognition in contrasting domains (do-
alter our understanding and analysis of development, in- mains where biological predispositions are major and
fluencing concepts, methods, and research questions. domains where the bases is more one of gaining expert-
Those content areas have had to do with interpersonal ise), to the nature of expertise, and to the specific role of
relations during childhood, language and communica- language and communication in an area—the area
tion practices, and the development of self. The fourth known as “ theory of mind”—where age-related changes
and last area chosen is that known as cognitive develop- have been seen as biologically based, with little impact
ment. Terms with a cognitive cast to them—mind, men- from sociocultural experience.
tality, meanings, understandings, competencies—have The brief final comments then bring together some
already appeared throughout the chapter. The area of comments on the methods highlighted by a cultural psy-
cognitive development, however, gives them a particular chological perspective: Methods applicable to all as-
place and deepens their analysis. pects of development but particularly illustrated by
The material is necessarily selective. We have given studies of cognitive development.
preference, however, to proposals and issues that alter
our views not only of cognitive development but also of
Reexamining Thought and Action
development in general, and that focus on concepts, re-
searchable questions, and possible methods. Struc- The conventional assumption is that the mind comes
turally, the section is in three sections, each concerned first. Thought precedes action, at least as an ideal in
with ways of moving beyond constraining divisions. development. We then find it reasonable to account for
Cultural Psychology and Cognitive Development 765

what people do in cognitive terms. To take an example example. It takes the form of working from language and
from law, we find it reasonable to ask if children are old communicative practices, asking for example how these
enough to know the difference between right and wrong are associated with particular divisions between truth
and, given that knowledge, whether they can be held re- and fiction, particular distinctions among relationships
sponsible for what they do. We also see it as appropriate, or events, and particular ways of perceiving or repre-
in studies of development, to place our emphasis on senting the self. That direction is also very much evident
changes and variations in the way people think rather in analysis of the extent to which an understanding of
than on changes and variations in what they do. “mind” and of what others know or believe is shaped by
That conventional assumption has been questioned on the presence of lexical terms such as thoughts or beliefs,
several grounds. Noted especially in the previous edition by adults’ questions to children, and by games such as I
were two alternative proposals. The first is that we re- Spy or 20 Questions. In acquiring adults’ language, the
verse the usual order, with the flow now from actions to argument runs, children also acquire their theory of
thought. To take a well-known example, we begin by mind (e.g., Vinden, 1996; Vinden & Astington, 2000).
“doing gender.” We use, and see others use, different For a further example involving language, we turn to
names, games, clothes, spaces, and tasks for males and a study that is especially relevant to the under-explored
females. Those everyday practices provide the bedrock question: What ways of acting can substitute for one
for the category of gender. They lead us also to think of another? We surely learn to distinguish among relation-
gender distinctions as fixed and natural, and they move ships, for example, by more than one route, by involve-
development in the direction of less reflection rather ment in or exposure to more than one kind of practice.
than more. The nature of interchangeability, however, is so far
The second proposal is that we specify both contexts poorly understood. The content area in the study singled
and the course of development in activities, practices, out has to do with narrative development. It compares
and the nature of participation. In one society, for exam- hearing children whose parents are deaf with other chil-
ple, the main activities for children may be strongly age- dren. Narrative development turns out to be the same for
graded, with each school group or playgroup made up of both (Van Deusen-Phillips et. al., 2001). The narrative
children much the same age, and children usually dis- practices of people other than parents, it appears, are
tanced from adult activities. In another society, school- sufficient to model the expected structures.
based activities may play a minor role, younger and older Spoken language and communicative practices are by
children may be more often part of the same group, and no means the only practices to attract continuing atten-
children may be present while adults work, talk, or play. tion in the study of cognitive development. Anthropol-
The main directions seen as prompted by those pro- ogy and cultural psychology draw attention to a variety
posals took the form of closer looks at (a) links between of other tools, ranging from sticks to written scripts,
particular ways of thinking and particular ways of act- number systems, or maps: Tools that bring out the nature
ing and ( b) ways of specifying the nature of activities, of both cultural variations and historical changes. There
practices, and forms of participation. We now take up are by now many analyses of how various tools come to
those directions, asking what has been added since the be available, used, or altered and to shape the way think-
previous edition, and highlighting some particular gaps ing or problem solving proceeds. Several of these are
that remain. contained in the papers brought together by Cole, En-
Examining links between ways of thinking and ways geström, and Vasquéz (2000). A set of chapters edited
of acting. A view of actions and ideas as intrinsically in- by Hatano and Wertsch (2001) provides several further
terwoven is a core part of the concept of custom com- examples, ranging from the use of a globe in learning
plex. The challenge lies in finding ways to bring out the about astronomy (Schoultz, Säljö, & Wyndhamn, 2001)
forms of that interweaving. Two ways of doing so may be to the use of models or diagrams in planning construc-
distinguished. One starts from particular forms of activ- tions (Gauvain, 2001).
ity and asks about associated ways of thinking. The From these several sources, we abstract two points
other starts from ways of thinking and asks what prac- that may well be extended to the analysis of many prac-
tices or activities might establish or maintain them. tices. The first is that with extended use we are likely to
For the first method—starting from particular forms learn more than how to use a particular kind of tool. We
of activity—this chapter has already provided a major may also develop an understanding of it, a mental model
766 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

of how it works, and its possibilities (Oura & Hatano, A further possible circumstance has to do with repeti-
2001, describing a particular aspect of development in tions of the same message. Repetition in itself, however,
the course of learning to play the piano). The other is a is a concept still in need of unpacking. It may take the
strong reminder that all such understanding is socially form of everyone delivering the same message. There are,
shaped. What is written and used as a basis and purpose for example, experimental studies showing that consen-
for reading, for example, may in some groups cover a sus—especially consensus about the affective signifi-
wide range of material and of situations. In others, ma- cance of a particular view—tends to limit the degree of
terials and purposes may be strictly limited to writing monitoring for exceptions (Frijda & Mesquita, 1994).
certain kinds of letters, keeping certain kinds of ac- Repetition may also take the form of the same message
counts, or developing religious commitment (Scribner & emerging from several practices. Involvement in Western-
Cole, 1981). style schooling, for example, is low among Samoan chil-
For the second method—starting from a way of dren. Both adults outside school and the nature of school
thinking and asking about its possible background—we practices in Samoa, however, all convey the same mes-
single out, as a particular case, thinking that involves sage: the irrelevance of school for the children’s current
the emergence of change or innovation. or future ways of living (Watson-Gegeo, 1993).
There are, for example, clearly occasions when peo- In specifying activities and forms of participation or
ple develop ways of being original or creative rather nonparticipation, any approach to development that em-
than being only technically and reproductively expert. phasizes the importance of activities needs to move to-
That kind of development appears to be influenced by ward distinctions among them. Activities have been
the ways in which forms of teaching and definitions of distinguished by, for example, where they take place, the
skill allow some individuality of expression rather than people who are present, and the psychology of the peo-
only the strict following of prescriptive directions. ple present (e.g., their views of how development takes
Cooking and the performance of music, for example, place; e.g., Super & Harkness, 1986). They have also
often allow for some individuality of expression—at been distinguished by the tools or artifacts that are
least at some levels of competence more readily than do available or used (e.g., Gauvain, 2001), the patterns of
areas such as the feeding of animals or plants (Hatano & accompanying talk (Gutiérrez, 2002), and the forms and
Inagaki, 1992; Oura & Hatano, 2001). impacts of repetition (e.g., Hatano & Inagaki, 1992).
There are also clearly occasions when reflection, Practices, for example, are activities where repetition,
questioning, or resistance occurs rather than the simple by oneself or others, gives rise to the sense of the natu-
acceptance of a customary way of thinking or acting. ral or proper ways to act (e.g., Goodnow et al., 1995; P. J.
Most analyses of practice lead us away from exploring Miller & Goodnow, 1995).
that aspect of development. They emphasize instead the Activities may differ also in the extent to which they
extent to which the routine, everyday quality of prac- allow various forms of participation. We draw particu-
tices diminishes the likelihood that they will be re- lar attention to this aspect. One reason for doing so is
flected on, questioned, or resisted. That emphasis has that changes in participation have been proposed as
offered a powerful contrast to accounts of cognitive de- promising ways of characterizing the forms that devel-
velopment that emphasize people as making sense of the opment takes. They may then characterize both the
world in optimal scientific fashion: asking questions, shape and the bases of development. Another is that de-
being quickly alert to discrepancies, and investing effort scriptions of participation build on descriptions of ac-
in resolving ambiguities. It leaves open questions about tivities as joint and on distinctions among them as how
shifts in conventional views and practices, both across two or more people contribute to a task.
generations and in an individual’s lifetime. Currently, the most familiar form of attention to
For the circumstances that encourage questioning or changes in participation revolves around teacher-learner
resistance, we have so far little developmental evidence. or expert-novice relationships. Prompted, especially by
One relevant circumstance, however, seems likely to be Vygotskian theory, the course of development is often
what children learn about the fate or status of question- seen as one in which the expert provides guidance and
ers. They may be pointed out as odd people, as godless structures the task in ways that allow the novice to take
or lost souls whose afterlife is not what one would aspire over more and more responsibility for the task.
to. “Curiosity killed the cat,” it is said (more often than That description of a shift is a rich starting point, but
the rejoinder “Information brought it back ”). it needs, several expansions that involve (a) the kinds of
Cultural Psychology and Cognitive Development 767

relationships considered, ( b) the steps or processes in- In any joint activity, to take a second part of H.
volved, and (c) the nature of nonparticipation. Clark’s (1996) argument, we should specify the rules,
On the first score (the nature of relationships), we regulations, and etiquette that apply. In any team game,
need to continue questioning the benign and cooperative for example, people may be present as players, referees,
quality presented as typical of teacher-learner relation- coaches, fans, or observers. Each is expected to act
ships. Teachers or experts are not always eager to give within certain limits. Acting in violation of these limits
up their control and novices are not always eager to learn incurs penalties. For any game also, the number of peo-
or to take on responsibility (Goodnow, 1990b). Teacher- ple and the rules for participation define the game. Peo-
learner relationships are also not the only forms of rela- ple can play football with fewer than the number of
tionship that can apply. In some situations, for example, required players, for example, but then the game is no
people function or are expected to function more as a longer an official game. Varying the rules as to what
team. In still others, one person (e.g., a concert pianist) each piece may do in a game of chess is also possible but
may seem to take the only active part. Even here, how- the game is then no longer chess.
ever, the audience listens with expectations about what In any area, that kind of description suggests, the ac-
will be played and how pieces will be played. The per- quisition of skill may lie in learning both the rules of the
former will take those expectations into account and game and the limits to which they can be pushed. We
also try to persuade the audience that the choice of learn, for example, that some departures from the ex-
pieces or of interpretation is a reasonable or exciting one pected rules or procedures rule one out of the game
(Oura & Hatano, 2001). while others are tolerated. We learn also that failing to
For expansions on the second score (specifying steps observe the expected patterns (e.g., the patterns for con-
or processes in participation), we turn to proposals from versations) may not rule one out of the game but is likely
Rogoff and from H. Clark. Rogoff (2003) describes par- to result in fewer invitations to join in on later occasions.
ticipation as involving two processes. In one, people Learning the collaborative rules (Goodnow, 1996b)
seek to achieve some mutual understanding: for exam- seems likely to apply to many situations, from turn taking
ple, some mutual understanding of what each knows, to story telling, school tasks, or teamwork of any kind.
what each seeks, what each understands the task to be. How children learn such rules, however, is still far from
In the other, they seek to structure what each will do. clear. An intriguing beginning is Martini’s (1994) obser-
They offer choices, invite some actions rather than oth- vation that young children who spend a great deal of time
ers, or shape events so that some actions become more with older children (the older children are often their
likely than others. In effect, they engage in some “mu- minders) learn quickly that their presence is tolerated as
tual structuring of participation” (Rogoff, 2003, p. 287). long as they do not interrupt the older children’s games.
H. Clark’s (1996) analysis starts from conversation or Household tasks bring out a similar aspect of develop-
“language in use” as a prime example of “joint activity.” ment. They also bring out the extent to which what needs
His analysis is not developmentally oriented; however, it to be learned or negotiated are the reasons for participat-
does suggest several new directions for developmental re- ing, the expected styles for doing so, and the limits to
search. In any joint activity, Clark (1996) argues, we variations in what can be contributed (e.g., variations in
should ask what each person contributes and is expected what can be delayed, downsized, delegated, swapped,
to contribute, what they regard as their shared task, how substituted, or taken over; Fuligni, 2001; Goodnow,
they go about that task, and when some perceived limit to 1996c, 2004b).
what they can do is reached. In a conversation between a The third and last needed expansion that we single out
telephone operator and a caller, for example, one person has to do with the occurrence and the nature of nonpar-
seeks information and the other provides it. Each person ticipation. Most analyses of participation assume that
checks from time to time that the other has heard what participation always occurs, even at times—as in the ex-
was said, has heard accurately, that the information pro- ample of a concert pianist’s audience—when participa-
vided is what was sought and has been understood (in tion is not highly visible. What needs to be accounted for
essence, “are you with me?”). Moves toward establishing then are the timing and the shape of changes in the way
mutual understanding (Rogoff, 2003) are not only a way people participate. Nonparticipation, however, can also
of describing what people do but also an indication of a occur, and may be encouraged in some areas, with at-
particular competence whose acquisition we might well tempts at participation regarded as forms of interference
seek to trace. (Goodow, 1996a). Where it is encouraged, learning how
768 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

not to do so, in acceptable ways, may be a major part of identities, again summarizing very briefly the points
developmental skill (Goodnow, 2004a). made in the previous edition and asking what might
At this point, before we go further in attempts to now be added.
track down sources, the major need appears to be one of
differentiating among forms of nonparticipation. People Feelings
may, for example, be physically absent. They may stay The only point covered in the previous edition was the
away or drop out. More subtly, they may be present but widespread recognition of the need to avoid a two-box
unengaged in what occurs. Children may, for instance, approach to thinking and feeling. We now expand on that
participate in the sense that they stay in the classroom point in three ways.
(they do not drop out physically). They are, however, un- One is the use of feeling states as a way of differenti-
involved, uninvested, or not engaged ( behaviorally, cog- ating among shared points of view (D’Andrade, 1992).
nitively, or emotionally) in the formal work of the All members of a cultural group, for example, may hold
classroom. Increasingly, that kind of nonparticipation is the view that “everyone should help the poor ” or that
attracting both attention and proposals to the effect that “each of us can be whatever we want, provided we work
the difficulty may lie more in the nature of social and hard enough.” For some members of the group, however,
classroom contexts than in characteristics of the nonpar- that idea may be held at the level of a cliché. For others,
ticipants (e.g., Blumenfeld et al., in press). Delineating the idea may be held with a conviction that guides action
forms of nonparticipation, establishing how they come when a choice arises. For still others, the idea may be
about, and finding ways to change them seems to be one held at a level that initiates action: We seek ways to im-
of the next challenges to rise to in analyses of how plement it. How some ideas come to be held with various
thought and action are related to one another. levels of commitment is clearly a question about devel-
opment that still needs to be explored.
Hearts and Minds The second expansion has to do with occasions when
some ways of thinking or problem solving give rise to
Cultural anthropology has long contained an awareness strong negative emotions. These ways strike us as terri-
that matters of the mind cannot be separated from mat- bly wrong, as violating some basic principles, as—to
ters of the heart. Feeling cannot be regarded as a sepa- borrow from others—unthinkable, heretical, or taboo
rate state from thinking or treated simply as a force that (Fiske & Tetlock, 1997; Tetlock, Kristel, Elson, Green,
provokes thought (Shweder, 1992). Forms of compe- & Lerner, 2000). These feelings may well be an impor-
tence need to be regarded as the skills that communities tant part of our sense of a major difference between cul-
value (D’Andrade, 1981). Development needs always tures. They also suggest that people may be regarded not
to be framed by the social group into which one moves: only as “intuitive scientists” but also as “intuitive the-
for example, “Becoming a Kwoma” (J. W. M. Whiting, ologians”(Tetlock, McGraw, & Kristel, 2004). That
1941). At issue, to take a statement that has moved into kind of proposal is not only an interesting challenge to
popular discourse, is the battle for hearts and minds. any “cool scientist ” view of cognition but also nicely
Cognitive psychology has tended to cover a somewhat linked to a useful method: presenting people with errors
different set of concerns. A set of chapters on “affective of various kinds (essentially variations of expected pat-
minds,” edited by Hatano, Okada, and Tanabe (2001), terns) and exploring both judgments of seriousness and
illustrates two recurring lines of approach. One focuses the kinds of emotions reported.
on the ways in which emotions influence the nature of The third and last expansion to the place of feelings
processing, with effects ranging from what is attended attracts attention because it also challenges conven-
to or given processing priority to how far information is tional concepts and some particular methods. It has to
sought. The other focuses on the ways in which aspects do with variations in the level of attention paid to feel-
of cognition prompt various emotions, ranging from ing states as part of understanding others. A study of at-
fear in the face of perceived threat to the way pride, tention to two aspects of words provides an example.
shame, or embarrassment rest on some sense of self and Adult speakers of English and Japanese were presented
of standards. with words that were pleasant or unpleasant in meaning,
What does our cultural psychology perspective add and spoken with either a smooth, round tone or a harsh,
to such analyses of hearts and minds? To consider that constricted tone. They were then asked to ignore vocal
question, we take up the place of feelings, values, and tone and judge whether the words are pleasant or un-
Cultural Psychology and Cognitive Development 769

pleasant in meaning, or to ignore meaning and judge reflecting—for the Chinese preschoolers—a cultural
whether the tone was pleasant or unpleasant. Japanese model of learning in which a combination of “ heart and
speakers found the first task more difficult than English mind for wanting to learn” is central (Li, 2002, p. 246).
speakers did. English speakers found the second task For the acquisition of cognitive values, we turned in
more difficult than Japanese speakers did (Ishii & Ki- the previous edition to accounts that are essentially bor-
tayama, 2001). The course of development for that kind rowed from theories of social learning. The emphasis
of difference would now be of interest to establish, with was on what happens in dyadic interactions: on the way,
one possibility being that English speakers grow out of for example, that what is said is responded to, built on,
an initial sensitivity to tone and into a preoccupation ignored, scorned, or actively corrected. We drew espe-
with the lexical meaning of words. cially from accounts by Wertsch (1991) of how children
even in the early years of school learn to use “ the voice
Values of science”: sprinkling their stories or arguments with
At first glance, it may seem strange to use the term val- references to numbers, citing “evidence,” and talking
ues in relation to cognition. Values surely belong to ac- about the physical properties of objects rather their per-
counts of social or moral development. Cognitive values sonal meaning for the speaker. Noted also were several
are ubiquitous: embodied in distinctions between what closely observed accounts of what happens when chil-
we call basics and frills, significant problems and trivial dren bring to school ways of speaking and story telling
pursuits, elegant and pedestrian approaches to a prob- that are not in a teacher’s preferred style. They are
lem, original and regurgitated pieces of writing, and quickly made aware that their stories and their ways of
proper and odd ways of adding up numbers. story telling are not good, and teachers actively seek to
In the previous edition, we noted the ubiquity of cogni- dismantle (Michaels, 1991) the children’s styles.
tive values and raised the question: How are these ac- The ways in which we acquire cognitive values, how-
quired? What is the nature of such cognitive socialization ever, need not be limited to such direct interpersonal
(Goodnow, 1990a)? We can now offer three expansions: interactions. Values are also embodied in practices.
one related to where values occur, a second to how values Take, for example, a school timetable or a school curricu-
are acquired, and a third to the range of value tags or lum. In a timetable, some school subjects are allotted the
value judgments that are made. prime times of day, taught every day, and seldom
For the areas where values are salient, we focused in rescheduled. Others are given lower priorities: inevitably
the previous edition on the nature of the end product: on dropped or cut short whenever class juggling needs to
what is to be learned, what is said, understood, written, take place. In the material used for teaching, there may
or worked on. Values are also attached to particular ways be no references to a child’s country of origin, no images
of proceeding or learning. In many cultures, for example, of people from a child’s identity group, or no stories that
learning by watching is valued over learning by doing are part of a child’s heritage. It may not even occur to
(Rogoff, 2003). In any culture, teachers and students may kindergarten teachers that there is something absurd
also vary in the value they attach to various ways of about using stories such as Red Riding Hood in Samoan
learning. Teachers in many Western societies, for exam- classrooms (Watson-Gegeo, 1993). School subjects may
ple, often favor a process of learning by exploring dif- also acquire various status tags: Some are thought of a
ferences of opinion—by argument or by attempts at re- must-be-taken course, some are only for the brightest
solving different views. Pupils, however, often devalue (Latin and Greek once had this status), and some are
that procedure and avoid it on the grounds that it may best for the less bright (typing once had this status). Still
jeopardize their relationships with one another (Lampert, others seem to exist on sufferance: In most secular West-
Rittenhouse, & Crumbaugh, 1996). The values attached ern schools, for instance, religion or scripture—if taught
to play and to formal or academic learning provide a fur- at all—is usually an option, and it is often taught by
ther example. Chinese parents of preschoolers place far someone who is not part of the regular school staff. In
less value on play as a source of cognitive development many ways, its position parallels the low degree of atten-
than European Americans. Moreover, their preschoolers tion given in most current analyses of development to re-
themselves also place high value on learning in ways that ligious ways of viewing events and people (Hudley et al.,
suggest an “awareness of the unity of learning and moral- 2003). It is small wonder then that many of us are unpre-
ity” (Li, 2004, p. 126) that is slower to develop among pared for the significance of religious thought in many
European American children. The difference is seen as other parts of the world.
770 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

Direct social learning and embedding in the teaching about the developmental background to such distinctions
and linguistic practices of a group are then two lines of and the feelings associated with them. At the least, how-
cognitive socialization. To these, a third needs to be ever, these judgments at the far end of a scale of values
added. It has been considered so far in terms only of point to the need for expanding the range of value tags or
judgments about the work of adults, yet is extendable to value judgments among ways of thinking beyond any
children. It also points to a way in which practices may simple sorting into the more versus the less valued.
be linked to one another: a recurring concern in this
chapter. This core proposal is part of Bourdieu’s (1979) Identities
analysis of intellectual values—of “ taste” or “distinc- In the previous edition, we divided the argument for the
tion”: An analysis that brings out the extent to which need to link analyses of cognitive development to issues
some of the same criteria for judging value may apply in of identity into three ways, which break the line often
several situations. For example, we bring many of the drawn between analyses of cognitive development.
same dimensions to judgments about intellectual pro- The first noted that certain ways of thinking, problem
ductions that, we use for judgments about food, distin- solving, or expressing oneself mark one as a schooled or
guishing between rich and thin, refined or rough, and educated person, promoting the choice of some ways of
well or poorly presented. The essence of taste for the proceeding even when others are available (Nunes, 1995),
European middle-class, Bourdieu (1979) comments, and often provide the motivation needed to acquire a dif-
may well lie in the values it attaches to various forms of ficult skill (Hatano, 1995). The second was that changes
presentation, for both material and intellectual produc- in competence can lead to some changes in the identity
tions. That proposal not only helps illuminate an area of that others assign to us. A person may now, for example,
difference among social groups but also raises develop- be recognized as a legitimate member of some commu-
mental questions about the extent to which a common nity of practitioners (Lave & Wenger, 1991): a change in
presence makes values easier to pick up or more diffi- status noted as not always occurring. The third was that
cult to set aside in successive situations. the acquisition of competence covers only part of the
The third and last expansion in the area of values has story. Not acquiring certain forms of competence can
to do with the range of value judgments. We have so far also be a way of signaling identity (e.g., an identity as one
described various kinds of thinking, learning, or problem of “ the lads”; Willis, 1977) or a way of protecting one’s
solving only as being more versus less valued, more ver- identity, one’s sense of an essentially capable self (e.g.,
sus less privileged. That is surely an incomplete descrip- Cole & Traupmann, 1981). The critical skill may also lie
tion. At the least, we might extend the range by adding a in managing to maintain a double identity: for example,
term dispreferred from Ochs (1990, p. 299), which cov- an official identity, for teachers, as a reasonable child
ers what is actively discouraged. We might extend it also who obeys the rules and, for one’s peers, an identity as
by describing values as what is regarded as ideal, what is one who appears to stay in the letter of the rules but sub-
tolerated, what is discouraged, and what is regarded as tly bends or flouts them (Corsaro, 1992).
out of the question, impossible, intolerable (Goodnow, We have still a great deal to learn, as we noted in the
1995, 1996a), heretical, or unthinkable (Fiske & Tet- previous edition, about the strategic presentation of
lock, 1997; Tetlock et al., 2000). identity in the course of development and about what is
We have a great deal to learn about what are regarded seen as in keeping with an assigned identity (e.g., the
as the most serious violations of expected ways of think- identity of child or beginner). Not in keeping with some
ing. Building on Fiske’s (1991) account of social orienta- definitions of a child, for example, is a precocious
tions, Tetlock and his colleagues propose that the knowledge of sex or of money values. Not in keeping
underlying base is a violation of “spheres of exchange.” with some concepts of a beginner is the open and non-
With one example being the violation of a distinction be- deferential display of skill or ease.
tween what can be treated as a commodity (traded, sold, What might now be added? The expansions seen as
or discarded) and what cannot. All cultures, for exam- especially needed have to do with (a) the nature of
ple, draw distinctions between what can be sold and membership, ( b) asking which members of a group mat-
what cannot. They may differ in their views of what can ter, and (c) the implications of moves toward recogniz-
be sold (e.g., people, sex, loyalties), but the distinction is ing social and personal identity, multiple identities, and
always deeply felt. We have as well a great deal to learn both a current self and possible selves.
Cultural Psychology and Cognitive Development 771

On the first score (membership), developmentalists shame, and the development of the standards, rules, and
are now familiar with the argument that increasing com- goals that M. Lewis (1993) sees as their basis, depend
petence needs to be seen as involving not only a change then on the development of attachments and, presum-
in the individual but also in the individual’s coming to ably, the range of people that in any culture are re-
be an accepted, recognized member of a group. One garded as sources of praise or censure that should “cut
needed expansion on that argument consists of noting to the quick.”
that this second shift may not always happen. In a less- The last expansion (closer analyses of identity) takes
than-ideal world, acceptance may still be withheld. The several forms. Each suggests a different facet of what
official qualifications may never be enough. More develops and, in some cases, of conditions that shape
finely, we now need to take a closer look at the meanings cognitive development.
of terms such as community and membership. One is a distinction between social identity and per-
Rogoff (2003) provides one example of that move, sonal identity. Personal identity is identity captured by
distinguishing participation from membership. She de- asking whether people see themselves as competent or
scribes herself, for example, as having “participated for creative or as being the same person now that they were
several decades in a Mayan community in Guatemala, at an earlier age. Social identity refers to the social cat-
but people from that community (and I ) do not regard egories in which we place ourselves or others place us.
myself as a member of that community” (p. 83). We We may, for example, describe ourselves as Asian
may, she continues, do better to focus on “ the more dy- American, Chinese, or Cantonese, or as first-, second-,
namic concept of participation, rather than the categori- or third-generation. Others may assign a person to a
cal concept of membership” (p. 83). We may also need looser category, using a term such as Asian to cover
to think more about the defining features of a commu- everything from India to Japan. Interest in social identi-
nity. Communities need not, for example, be “limited to ties and social categories has a considerable history, es-
people who are in face-to-face contact or living in geo- pecially since the work of Tajfel (1981). Much of that
graphic proximity” (p. 81). They should, however, “ be work, however, has stayed in the realm of social psychol-
defined as groups of people who have some common and ogy and in references to the life of adults. Increasingly,
continuing organization, values, understanding, history, it shows signs of a much-needed move into analyses of
and practices. . . . A community involves people trying development.
to accomplish some things together, with some stability Seen as a mark of cognitive development, for example,
of involvement. . . . A community involves generations is an increasing skill in the description of oneself in ways
that move through it, with customary ways of handling that fit what is required in various situations and with
the transitions of generations” (pp. 80–81). Moreover, varied audiences (e.g., Banarjee, 2002). There are age
“participants in a community often continue to regard changes, for example, in the appropriateness of chil-
their involvement and their continuing relationships as dren’s answers on a task such as the following: “Imagine
central to their lives, whether this is expressed in affec- you are lost in a mall. I’m a detective and it’s my job to
tion or loyalty or efforts to avoid community ways” find you. I’ve never seen you before and I don’t have any
(p. 81). How people acquire these understandings of pictures of you. Tell me what I would need to know about
community and these feelings of belonging are ques- you to find you.” What may also develop with age or ex-
tions still to be pursued. perience is skill in shaping a presentation of identities to
On the second score (some people in a group matter suit one’s own strategic needs. To take an example from
more than others), we take as an example some propos- observations by Cooper, Garcia Coll, Thorne, and Orel-
als by Minsky (2000, 2001). Those proposals start by lana (in press) two girls in an Oakland school use to good
considering people as equipped by nature with “detec- purpose their mixed backgrounds (mixed is the term
tors” for praise or censure. The impact of praise or cen- used in the school and the playground). For one, the
sure, however, depends on the extent to which there is mother is described in this report as Chinese-American,
an attachment or close relationship between the person the father as Jewish. For the other, the mother is de-
making judgments and the person receiving them. The scribed as white, the father as Egyptian. Both girls were
remarks of a stranger, for example, generate less shame noted as using their mixed backgrounds as ways “ to
than do the remarks of a parent to whom the child feels avoid fights (mixed meant not on either side of ethnically
attached. The experience of emotions such as pride and charged conflicts) or avoid the slight stigma of being
772 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

white and build alliances (I told the Black kids that I’m Much of this work is in its early stages. What is clear,
Egyptian and that’s in Africa).” however, is that these directions of research promise to
A second expanded look at identity distinguishes be- yield a richer understanding of the ways in which issues
tween personal and collective identity. It is collective of identity, along with feelings and values, are part of
identity that is involved in the sense of “ we” (Thoits & cognitive development.
Virshup, 1997): reminiscent of J. W. M. Whiting’s
(1941) description of development as “Becoming a Persons and Contexts
Kwoma.” As Ruble and her colleagues point out, most of
the work on collective social or cultural identity has Salient in many areas of development is the issue of how
been with adults. In contrast, most of the work with to consider contributions from characteristics of both
children has been on the developing sense of gender and the individual and the social contexts in which they live.
gender constancy (Ruble et al., in press). There is some Surprisingly, an emphasis on both is still needed because
initial work on the extent to which a sense of racial con- there are still times when an emphasis on instruction or
stancy and a sense of gender constancy follow similar socialization seems to treat the individual as a blank
lines (Rhee, Cameron, & Ruble, cited by Ruble et al., in slate or a sponge that needs only to have information pro-
press). There is also the beginning of work on whether vided in absorbable amounts. There are also times when
and when a sense of collective identity in children has an emphasis on the individual as actively constructing
similar consequences to those observed in adults (e.g., meanings seems to treat the outside world as essentially
an effect on what is attended to and how information is blank, ignoring the history and the directiveness with
processed, or—a point taken up in research with adoles- which individuals must work or make sense:
cents by Yip and Fuligni, in press—an effect from the
strength of collective identity to successful coping with Once both contributions are fully acknowledged, questions
transitions such as changes in school). arise about how the two are interwoven. The first move has
been a recognition of the need to regard “persons” and
A third expansion is the recognition of identities as
“contexts” as influencing each other in ways that are not
multiple, especially when an individual occupies more
one-directional, static or linear. The second has been to-
than one social world: Most children occupy more than ward some ways of specifying interdependence. Persons
one world—home, school, and peers, for example. The and contexts have been described, for example, as “creat-
children of foreign-born parents have been regarded as ing each other ” (Briggs, 1992), “shaping each other ”
especially likely to occupy social worlds that differ (Cole, 1990), “making each other up” (Shweder, 1990a), or
markedly in their linguistic practices and values, with “co-constructing each other ” (Valsiner, 1994).
the inevitable development of a sense of being torn be-
tween two worlds. What they—and all children—may in- What are the specific meanings of such phrases, es-
stead develop is a set of border-crossing skills that make pecially in relation to cognitive development? To answer
it possible to negotiate a reasonably comfortable time in that question, we divide a large body of material into
more than one place (Phelan, Davidson, & Yu, 1988). two parts, varying in their starting points. The first
A fourth and last move in analyses of identity is a dis- starts from several descriptions of contexts and asks
tinction between a current self and future possible selves. what do these imply for the shape or course of develop-
What may matter most for performance or engagement in ment? The second starts from several descriptions of
school, for example, may be not only an image of oneself cognitive development and asks how do social or cul-
at present but also an image of what one might become, tural contexts enter into these pictures?
and an awareness of the steps that need to be taken to Both parts start with the recognition that any de-
achieve those futures. That kind of proposal not only scription of either term (context or individual develop-
makes intuitive sense but also is well supported by the re- ment) carries implications for the nature of the other.
sults of a California program designed to provide the chil- Ecological descriptions of context (e.g., the world as a
dren of immigrant families with both an understanding of set of nested circles starting from the home and working
the paths they needed to take from one point in school to outwards into parts of society) carry with them the con-
another (e.g., the particular levels in mathematics that notation of development as a journey, with the individual
they needed at various points) and a strengthened belief in discovering routes, acquiring navigational skills, or
their ability to move along the “pipeline,” in their “iden- finding helpful guides. Descriptions of the world as a
tity pathways” (Cooper, Dominguéz, & Rosas, in press). stage in which we all play roles or learn our place carry
Cultural Psychology and Cognitive Development 773

with them the implication that development involves ac- and for all other descriptions, we take it for granted that
quiring effective ways of self-presentation or emotional cultures, like people, are always changing.
management.
To take the reverse direction, descriptions of devel- Contexts as Linked Practices. We noted in the pre-
opment as coming to make sense of events or to discover vious edition (p. 913) that “it is always an open empirical
regularities imply a world that is not immediately com- question whether a mentality generalizes across many
prehensible, a world in which order or structure is hid- practice domains (the Hindu mentality) or is specific to a
den beneath a shifting surface appearance. Descriptions particular practice domain (the mentality of Hindu sleep-
of development as coming to make effective use of the ing arrangements).” That question is prominent also in
symbols or artifacts available imply worlds in which the present chapter. We are still, however, short of pro-
various tools are available, with some probably more ac- posals that consider the nature of linking among prac-
cessible or more promoted than others, varying over tices. We noted earlier a possible linking by ways of
groups or across time. Descriptions as activities and similar dimensions being used for evaluative judgments
changing forms of participation imply worlds that vary about what is produced, using as an example Bourdieu’s
in the opportunities they offer for participation or for (1979) analysis of “ taste” or “distinction” in judgments
establishing routines in what one does. In effect, one about food and about intellectual productions. A further
way of mapping part of the person /context universe al- example comes from Rogoff ’s (2003) analysis of the
ways suggests a way of mapping the other. Working from practices that go with the kinds of arrangement usually
such cross-mapping is likely to be more productive than found in Western schools. Occurring together are age-
trying to link analyses that use quite separate dimen- grading, a concern with readiness in relation to age, the
sions for the description of persons and contexts (Good- use of praise for a child’s interest or achievement, the
now, 2004a, 2004b, in press). asking of questions to which answers are known, particu-
lar kinds of demands by children for adult attention, and
Starting from Descriptions of Contexts the move of school type conversations into home settings.
There are by now many descriptions of contexts: a vari- In another grouping, Rogoff (2003) sees an emphasis on
ety especially brought out in a review by Cooper and particular kinds of relationships ( horizontal or vertical,
Denner (1998; see also Cole, Chapter 21, this Handbook, dyadic or multiparty in type) as related to differences in
Volume 2). “sleeping arrangements, discipline, cooperation, gender
As an opening step, we distinguish between descrip- roles, moral development, and forms of assistance in
tions by content and descriptions by quality. Descrip- learning” (p. 9).
tions of a culture as shared meanings and practices, or We now need further examples of such possible
as a warehouse of narratives, for example, place their groups, further indications of the occurrence and the ex-
emphases on content. Descriptions of contexts as always tent of linking, and a check on whether—where linked
changing, or as multiple and contested, place their em- practices occur—this makes a difference to the pattern
phasis on quality. of development. The indications of linking come so far
The descriptions we choose for particular comment from the way adults pattern a child’s social world. Do
are far from exhaustive, and the selection leaves us with children then shift easily from one linked practice to an-
a sense of regretted omissions. We would have liked, for other? Do they, for example, pick up a distinction em-
example, to give more space to descriptions of contexts bodied in one practice more easily when they have
that emphasize opportunities for children and families already experienced a linked practice rather than one
to establish the routine, everyday activities that are seen that is not linked?
as so crucial to development (e.g., Gallimore, Weisner,
Guthrie, Bernheimer, & Nihira, 1993; Weisner, 2002). Contexts as Warehouses for Narratives or Inter-
The descriptions chosen for closer comment, however, pretations. This way of viewing cultural contexts
strike us as offering some particular shifts in the way we picks up the argument that all social contexts need to be
think about the shape and course of development and as seen in historical terms. In essence, the argument is that
containing some particular gaps. the past provides a set of texts or narratives. Each gener-
We start with some examples of context descriptions. ation may then draw from the warehouse, add to it, or
The first two are descriptions of cultural contexts as rediscover forgotten treasures. Each generation may also
linked practices and warehouses of narratives. For both, bring, to the available set, attitudes ranging from respect
774 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

to indifference, disdain, or scorn. Among cultural psy- with the sentence—The sun rose over both worlds: the
chologists, Wertsch (1991) best exemplifies this ap- world Sven knew and the one he would be transported
proach to culture. More broadly, this concept is central to—we can readily hazard a guess that the background
to the work of Bakhtin, who noted the ways in which reading includes some form of science fiction. What
novelists such as Rabelais or Joyce broke the pattern of parts of a setting, a plot, or a collection of characters are
previous narratives: the one by covering topics previ- more readily taken over or more quickly transformed
ously regarded as not proper topics, the other by changes than others? What makes them more likely to be treated
in structure, even at the level of sentences (Kristeva, in some ways rather than others? All told, narrative se-
1980, provides a brief and readable account). In time, lection and transformation seems to capture especially
the argument continues, some ways of breaking up past well historical and cultural variations in what children
patterns become taken up by others. They then become encounter as well as processes of selection, rejection,
part of what Valsiner (1994) has described as a spiral of and transformation.
influence that alters the nature of what is available to
draw from or is regarded as a reasonable selection: Contexts as Shared Meanings or Practices.
These descriptions of contexts are especially prominent
Novels, we would add, are not the only kinds of texts or in anthropology in the form of emphases on the presence
narratives that have attracted this kind of attention and of cultural models (shared ways of viewing events or
that raise questions about aspects of selection or transfor- people), cultural practices (shared ways of proceeding),
mation. Martin-Barbero (1993), for example, is one
or custom complexes (an approach that combines both
scholar who has brought a similar style of analysis to the
actions and meanings). We have, throughout the chapter,
way cultural expressions in the form of art, music, film, or
television spread from one culture to another. It is not the
brought out several implications of these positions for
case, he argues, that cultures in areas such as South Amer- the study of development. We now add several others,
ica are “over-run” or “swamped” by the cultural expres- prompted by a focus on cognitive development and on
sions of the North. What occurs instead is a process of the quality of ideas or practices as being shared.
selection, utilization, and adaptation. In some anthropological work, the quality of shared-
ness has given rise to questions about the degree to
What are the implications of that kind of position for which there is consensus and about the position of peo-
the nature of cognitive development? One is that we may ple whose views are modal for the group as against being
now see development as including the acquisition of a out on the edge of the variations that occur (e.g., Rom-
stance toward the texts of the past. That stance may be ney, Weller, & Batchelder, 1986). That work is with
one of respect and a view that the best way to approach a adults but suggests a new way of viewing development.
problem or to provide evidence for one’s argument is to In any content area and at any age, what is the degree of
turn to the past. In contrast, it may be one of regarding consensus? Are there some areas or some ages when not
the past as having nothing to do with the present, or as being part of the consensus—to be out on the margin or
inevitably biased (the product, for example, of old peo- to disagree—is to be avoided, virtually seen as the kiss
ple). That stance, for reasons we still need to pin down, of death? What promotes an interest in coming to share
may well change over the course of development. the same understandings or to act in the same ways as
Overall, warehouse accounts of contexts remind us others? The usual emphasis is on the wish to understand
that the central processes are likely to take the form of others and to be understood by them in ways that make
selection and transformation. Turning those processes easy the communication of meanings. The reasons seem
into developmental form then becomes the challenge. likely to involve also the strategic presentation of self.
Children’s selections and transformations of narrative, The quality of sharedness prompts other questions
an earlier section of this chapter suggests, are likely to about what happens when there are clear departures
provide an especially productive starting point. They from everyone being of one mind or acting in the one
clearly offer a base for asking about the particular nar- way. How do children deal with such situations? We use
ratives or narrative styles that are held out for children the example of a California classroom that served as a
as the best or the ones they should take as models. We base for observing how children dealt with social cate-
may explore also the ways in which children come to in- gories (Cooper, Garcia Coll, et al., in press). In this
corporate the texts they read into what they themselves classroom, children came from several ethnic back-
produce. When a 10-year-old, for example, starts a story grounds, signaled in some cases by skin color and in oth-
Cultural Psychology and Cognitive Development 775

ers by the style of dress, with the most obvious dress sig- fashion, structuring a child’s world so that experience
nal being that of girls from Islamic families wearing a with different others is at least delayed (Goodnow,
veil or headscarf. Teased by another child for wearing 1997), or they may engage in more direct preparatory
something so different, the reply by one girl was “It’s work. When a child is seen as likely to encounter dis-
part of my tradition,” a reply taken from the school’s crimination, for example, parents may encourage pride
orientation—conveyed in many lessons—of respecting in the group’s own history, or teach specific ways of re-
others’ traditions. The shared meaning, in effect, was sponding to name-calling or other derogatory actions
used as a way of justifying an unshared practice. The se- (Hughes & Chen, 1999). Both kinds of experience
lection of the reply, and its quick effect, illustrate as- should affect the social categories, the stances toward
pects of shared meanings and practices for which other others, and the degree of reflectiveness about differ-
developmental examples might be sought. ences among people that children develop. We have a
great deal yet to learn about the nature of such effects.
Contexts as Multiple and Contested. No society They are likely to provide, however, some sharp exam-
is monolithic. In most industrialized societies, for exam- ples of how particular qualities of social /cultural con-
ple, there are usually to be found more than one religion, texts are related to some particular qualities of cognitive
political party, or form of schooling, more than one development.
class, and more than one country of origin. Some of the
Starting from Descriptions of
alternate forms may be known by adjectives that imply
Cognitive Development
their minority status (e.g., alternative medicine, alterna-
tive schooling, or independent film producers). It is not In the previous section, we started from descriptions of
only the presence of variety that matters but also the cultural contexts and asked about links to descriptions of
way in which these several segments compete or negoti- cognitive development, noting any suggested new ways of
ate with one another. The people in one group, for exam- considering development or new research questions. In a
ple, may regard the people in another as best avoided, change of method, we now reverse directions. We start
kept at arm’s distance, or suppressed. Where these ac- from some particular descriptions of cognitive develop-
tions do not achieve one’s purposes, some form of nego- ment and ask how cultural contexts enter the picture.
tiation or takeover needs to occur. Churches may unite, There are many ways of describing cognitive devel-
union activists may be pushed into management, or in- opment. In the previous edition, we noted three: (1) a
dependent film producers may be co-opted into studio shift from an initially weak or fluid state to one that was
affiliations and productions. better established or more smooth in its execution; (2) a
This way of viewing cultures is widespread in the area change in the nature of information processing: changes,
often known as cultural studies (the work cited by Martin- for example, in what is attended to or rehearsed; and (3)
Barbero, 1993, is from this field). Part of its attraction for a move toward dividing cognitive development into do-
the study of development is that it leads us away from a mains, with proposals for differences in the nature of
view of culture or context as a state or thing. The emphasis development by domain.
falls instead on the presence of various cultural groups, on The third direction is the one to which we gave most
their perceptions of each other, and on their relationships attention then and now. The focus reflects the extent to
with each other. In addition, recognition of the extent to which debates about domain specificity have been prime
which encounters with other people or other positions are sites for exploring questions highlighted at the start of
usually controlled is prompted. this chapter such as whether, in the area of cognitive de-
Control over access to knowledge is a long-standing velopment, there is a place for social or cultural factors
theme in sociological analyses (e.g., Bourdieu & (innate predispositions might offer a sufficient account
Passeron, 1977; Foucault, 1980). In developmental stud- of development), what that place might be, and how
ies, it is represented by studies comparing the under- given abilities and cultural circumstances might be
standing of animal biology that develops when children brought together.
can vary their approaches to feeding and care against In essence, the synthesis regards two kinds of do-
being restricted to the teachers’ prescriptive routines mains as varying considerably in the way cognitive
(Hatano & Inagaki, 1986). In a more social fashion, it is development proceeds. A distinction is drawn between
represented by questions about the nature of children’s core or privileged domains and other domains (Keil,
encounters with other people. Parents may act in cocoon 1984; Siegler & Crowley, 1994; Wellman & Gelman,
776 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

1998). First, in privileged domains, humans are geneti- expected to solve problems all by themselves. A study by
cally prepared to acquire knowledge systems that deal Takahashi and Tokoro (2002) of experienced amateur
with important aspects of the world. Second, in nonpriv- photographers (senior shutterbugs) brings out this point.
ileged domains, development relies on general learning Most senior shutterbugs explicitly expressed their ap-
mechanisms (Keil, 1984) or module acquisition modules preciation of their supporting networks: networks in-
(Sperber, 2002). In these domains, cognitive develop- volving peers, the instructor, and family members who
ment is usually conceptualized as the gaining of expert- provided transport, prepared lunchboxes, and so on.
ise. In both domains, sociocultural perspectives have Even when learners’ problem solving activities were ap-
something to offer. Because the two kinds of domain parently solo, other people entered the picture in the
have been conceptualized differently, suggestions from form of possible competitors or possible buyers of what
cultural psychology vary from one to the other. one produced. The senior shutterbugs, for example,
wanted to take at each moment shots that they and others
Nonprivileged Domains: Cognitive Development would regard as good. Their skills improved, unlike
as Expertise. Traditionally, expertise means the ac- school learning, almost as a by-product of keeping these
cumulation of rich and well-structured domain knowl- audiences in mind.
edge, consisting of “chunks” that can readily be used A third addition, again broadening our understanding
(Chi, Glaser, & Farr, 1988). There is also wide agree- of what is acquired in the course of gaining expertise,
ment among cognitive researchers that gaining expertise emphasizes that the acquisition of knowledge and skills
requires years of experience in solving problems in the is accompanied by socioemotional changes—for exam-
domain, carried out with concentration and often taking ple, changes in interest, values, and identity. That posi-
the form of deliberate practice (Ericsson, 1996). tion has been stated with particular strength in Lave’s
What does cultural psychology add to these character- (1991) argument that the practices that developing indi-
izations? A central addition, as we see it, is the elabora- viduals engage in are embedded in a community of prac-
tion of what expertise consists of and how it is acquired. titioners. All practices, it has come to be recognized,
We begin with the argument that through repeated involve socioemotional interactions as well as cognitive
participation in culturally organized practices, both divisions of labor. The process of gaining expertise is
children and lay adults acquire the skills and knowledge not likely to be an exception. It cannot be purely cogni-
needed to perform competently in those practices tive. In one example, Japanese students who develop
(Goodnow et al., 1995). To this we add, first, what mat- expertise in the use of the abacus do so in a national cli-
ters is not only the amount of time spent in repetition but mate of admired expertise and in the company of others
also the nature of what is repeated. Studies by Oura and who are also members of abacus clubs or competitive
Hatano (2001) with a group of nonprofessional pianists teams (Hatano, 1995). To take another, volunteers serv-
bring out this point especially. All of these pianists had ing at a soup kitchen for homeless people became not
started piano lessons at 6 years of age or younger. Some, only skilful at helping them but also, more important,
however, had reached a junior expert level. Others were more favorable toward and sympathetic to those people
still at a more novice level. Oura and Hatano (2001) (Youniss & Yates, 1997).
asked both to practice a short piece of music. Those who Finally, cultural perspectives help us as well to dis-
had stayed at the novice level tried only to perform ac- tinguish among domains in terms of the kind of expert-
curately and smoothly. Those who had reached the jun- ise required and the occurrence of innovation. Some
ior expert level checked and refined their performance domains of expertise are knowledge lean: Expertise in
from the perspective of an audience in mind. In effect, these domains is reproductive in nature. Other domains
the two differed in the practice in which they had en- are knowledge rich: In these, individuals, after gaining
gaged. The less successful students had expected to play expertise, are most likely to contribute to cultural
for the teacher who would evaluate how smoothly and change. Expert cooks, for example, may invent new
how accurately they played. In contrast, the successful dishes by combining a variety of materials and modes
students had practiced for playing in public, with an eye of cooking. The invented dishes may then be incorpo-
to ways of creating their own expression. rated into the culture of cooking, if the new productions
Second, we add that the process of gaining expertise attract a number of members constituting the commu-
is assisted by other people and artifacts: Novices are not nity of cooks.
Cultural Psychology and Cognitive Development 777

Conceptual Development in Core or Privileged veloping understanding of mental states of themselves


Domains. In these domains, cognitive development is and others. One such method is to analyze children’s
conceptualized quite differently from what happens in everyday conversation in families prior to a check on
nonprivileged domains. The accounts start from the ar- their understanding of beliefs as determinants of be-
gument that human beings are biologically predisposed, havior. For example, family differences in explanatory
prior to any experience, to attend to some events rather conversations have been found to be associated with
than others and to make some inferences rather than children’s later development of TOM (e.g., Dunn,
others (e.g., Keil, 1984). Coherent bodies of knowledge Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991; Dunn,
about important aspects of the world are then built on 2000). Another research strategy is to compare the TOM
these bases, with many researchers assuming that the performances of different populations of children.
acquisition of core domains of thought such as naive Convincing evidence for the importance of complex
physics, psychology, and biology is early, easy, and al- communication with significant others for the TOM de-
most universal. The ways in which this construction velopment, for example, is given by Peterson and Siegal
takes place may be varied. As Karmiloff-Smith (1992) (1995): Deaf children’s development of TOM is delayed
notes, the innate constraints may “potentiate learning by when their parents are hearing individuals who had to
limiting the hypotheses entertained” (p. 11), but they learn a sign language later in their life but is not delayed
may also place limits on what is readily learnable. One when the parents are fluent signers. Still another method
early assumption was that the evidence for predisposi- that can show the causal effect of linguistic interaction
tions tended to minimize the role of culture in earlier in the TOM development consists of training studies.
conceptual development, especially in the first few Lohmann and Tomasello (2003), for example, have
years (e.g., Carey & Spelke, 1994). demonstrated that 3-year-olds’ false belief understand-
How do cultural psychologists challenge views that ing is facilitated both by perspective-shifting discourse
exclusively emphasize human inheritance from evolu- and by sentential complement syntax that represents a
tion? The domain of naive psychology or theory of mind belief decoupled from reality.
(TOM) provides a nice illustration, bringing out how The domain of naive biology also contains both
cultural-psychological views change the characteriza- mainstream conventional studies of age-dependent de-
tion of the course of development as well as the research velopment and innovative sociocultural studies. For ex-
methodology. ample, among the studies reviewed by Inagaki and
In a large-scale meta-analysis of studies by Wellman, Hatano (2002), the majority started from a traditional
Cross, and Watson (2001), for example, many studies view of development and focused on the ages at which
emerged as focused on identifying age-dependent devel- children would acquire “autonomous” biology. A small
opmental patterns. The overall pattern of results is that number of studies, however ( harmonious with cultural-
younger children fail to, but children a few years older psychological views) showed that the age-dependent de-
correctly, recognize that people act in accordance with velopmental patterns observed among urban children
what they believe is the case, whether it corresponds (typical experimental participants) are not universal.
with the reality that the child has come to know. The Instead, the pattern varies with particular kinds of
overall assumption is that experience makes little dif- experience.
ference in this sequence. Most studies have shown that young children’s naive
An increasing number of innovative studies are com- biology is human-centered: The properties attributed to
patible with, if not influenced by, cultural-psychological other animals tend to be generalized from what is known
views. Some of these studies assume that attention to to be the case for humans. This tendency is weaker or
forms of communication with significant others may nonexistent, however, among children who have had fre-
yield different perspectives on the development of quent direct contact with nonhuman animals and plants
TOM. Some, as we noted earlier, assign particularly im- (Atran et al., 2001; Ross et al., 2003). Even among urban
portant roles in TOM development to the use of lan- children, active and enduring involvement in animal-
guage as a tool representing mental states (Astington & raising activity mitigates the human-centered nature of
Baird, in press). biology. Their enriched knowledge about animals they
In terms of research methods, these studies aim to have raised serves as an extra source for the analogical
connect children’s sociocultural experiences to their de- understanding of animals not yet experienced (Inagaki,
778 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

1990). Children’s understanding can also be influenced Making comparisons across cultures, although often
by cultural ideas about the categories in which humans, informative, is not the only method possible. The most
other animals, and plants can be placed (Hatano et al., informative route may be a concentration on a culture
1993; Stavy & Wax, 1989). In short, even young chil- and on some local practices or ways of thinking. Also
dren’s naive biology is instantiated differently in differ- valuable is working from a local practice and asking
ent sociocultural environments. what ideas accompany it or, conversely, starting with a
Moving beyond specific areas of knowledge are sev- stated or inferred belief and asking what practices
eral proposals for the need to take an interactive view of might be associated with it.
genetic predispositions and sociocultural facilitations or That aspect of methods (across or in cultures) is not
constraints. Gelman (1990), for example, proposes that only a pragmatic issue. It reflects also, as Cole (2001)
innate constraints are so skeletal that they always have has pointed out, the recognition that “all human groups
to be complemented socioculturally. Another possibility inhabit a world suffused with their predecessors’ his-
is that both types of constraints usually operate in a mu- tory . . . culture and cultural mediation are universal
tually facilitating or bootstrapping way, with innate features of human life and an integral part of human
constraints becoming less important as rich domain development. Consequently, the process of cultural me-
knowledge is acquired through cultural learning. Worth diation can be studied in a broad range of practices
particular note is the argument that, in a few years after within any large, demographic, culturally constituted
birth, children begin to learn in a uniquely human way, group” (p. 168).
exemplified by joint attention and imitation (Tomasello We hope it has been evident that we see great benefit
et al., 1993). in borrowing freely from both experimental and ethno-
Those several proposals go beyond a view of develop- graphic approaches, with psychologists perhaps benefit-
ment as shaped only by genetic predispositions or by so- ing from a fuller knowledge of how a variety of
ciocultural circumstances. People undoubtedly come ethnographic methods may be used (P. J. Miller et. al.,
equipped to make some distinctions—between inside 2003, is a useful source). Borrowing conceptual analy-
and outside or animate and inanimate objects. Cultures, ses from outside psychology is also important. We have
however, provide usable artifacts that are shared by a drawn on occasion from anthropology, sociology, and—
majority of people in a community or a subgroup. These less frequently than we might do—areas often known as
artifacts include physical facilities and tools, social in- cultural or narrative studies. We have regrettably made
stitutions and organizations, documented pieces of the least use of history, a deficit that Cole (2001) points
knowledge, and commonsense beliefs. They also include out as applying to many psychological analyses of the
the behavior of other people, interactions with them, cultural nature of human development. The historical
and social contexts created by them. Cognitive develop- analysis of how mind, body, and soul or heart have at
ment is best seen as interactively constrained by both various times been separated or merged with one an-
sociocultural circumstances as well as genetic predispo- other would in itself, for example, round out our steps in
sitions (Hatano & Inagaki, 2000). this chapter toward moving beyond current dichotomies.
Our hope is that the benefits of cultural psychology for
A Summary Comment on Cognitive the analysis of cognitive development lie not only in its
Development: Methods prompting fresh approaches to questions about what de-
The account just concluded—covering several ways in velops and how this takes place but also in enriching the
which people have explored the place of sociocultural repertoire of methods that we bring to those questions.
experience in cognitive domains often proposed as the
province of genetic predispositions—has brought to the
surface a variety of methods. Variety in method pro- CONCLUSION
vides a way of bringing this section on cognitive devel-
opment to a close and of underlining again some points It is the hope of all those who welcome the return of
about method that have been made in earlier sections of cultural psychology as a vibrant research enterprise
the chapter: in particular, the place of shifts in method that more and more social scientists from various
as one of the prime features of a cultural psychology home disciplines (psychology, anthropology, linguis-
perspective. tics, sociology) will become developmental experts on
References 779

the psychological functioning of members of particular Basso, K. (1996). Wisdom sits in places: Landscape and language
cultural communities around the world. Only then will among the Western Apache. Albuquerque: University of New
Mexico Press.
the many questions raised in this chapter begin to be
Bauman, R., & Briggs, C. L. (1990). Poetics and performance as crit-
answered. Only then will the abstract pluralistic idea ical perspectives on language and social life. Annual Review of
of “one mind, many mentalities” become substantial Anthropology, 19, 59–88.
and concrete, and thereby come fully to life. It is when Bauman, R., & Sherzer, J. (Eds.). (1989). Explorations in the
ethnography of speaking (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge Univer-
another culture’s taken-for-granted categories appear
sity Press.
to us to be counter-intuitive, or out of line with what Baumeister, R. F. (1987). How the self became a problem: A psycho-
we assume to be present early in life, that we are most logical review of historical research. Journal of Personality and
likely to experience the need to rethink our sense of Social Psychology, 52, 163–176.
what is natural. Bayley, R., & Schecter, S. R. (Eds.). (2003). Language socialization
in bilingual and multilingual societies. Tonawanda, NY: Multilin-
gual Matters Ltd.
Bellah, R. N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M., Swidler, A., & Tipton,
REFERENCES S. M. (1985). Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment
in American life. New York: Harper & Row.
Abbott, S. (1992). Holding on and pushing away: Comparative Benedict, R. (1934). Patterns of culture. New York: Houghton Miff lin.
perspectives on an eastern Kentucky child-rearing practice. Ethos,
Benson, C. (2001). The cultural psychology of self: Place, morality
1, 33–65.
and art in human worlds. New York: Routledge.
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda. Baltimore: Johns
Berlin, I. (1976). Vico and Herder. London: Hogarth Press.
Hopkins University Press.
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale: NJ: Erlbaum.
Patterns of attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situ-
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. (1992).
ation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications. Cambridge,
Alexander, K. J., Miller, P. J., & Hengst, J. A. (2001). Young chil- England: Cambridge University Press.
dren’s emotional attachments to stories. Social Development,
Bledsoe, C. (1989). Strategies of child-fosterage among Mende
10(3), 374–398.
grannies in Sierra Leone. In R. J. Lesthaeghe (Ed.), Reproduction
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. and social organization in sub-Saharan Africa. Berkeley: Univer-
New York: Holt. sity of California Press.
Ames, R. T., Dissanayake, W., & Kasulis, T. P. (1994). Self as person Bledsoe, C. (1990). No success without struggle: Social mobility
in Asian theory and practice. Albany: State University of New and hardship for foster children in Sierra Leone. Man, 25,
York Press. 70–88.
Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. (Eds.) (in press). Why language matters Blumenfeld, P., Modell, J., Bartko, T., Secada, W., Fredricks, J.,
for theory of mind. New York: Oxford University Press. Friedel, J., et al. (in press). School engagement of inner city stu-
Atran, S., Medin, D., Lynch, E., Vapnarsky, V., Ek, E. U., & Sousa, P. dents during middle childhood. In C. R. Cooper, C. García Coll,
(2001). Folkbiology doesn’t come from folkpsychology: Evidence T. Bartko, H. Davis, & C. Chapman (Eds.), Hills of gold: Rethink-
from Yukatek Maya in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cog- ing diversity and contexts as resources for children’s developmen-
nition and Culture, 1, 3–43. tal pathways. New York: Oxford University Press.
Azuma, H., Kashiwagi, K., & Hess, R. D. (1981). The inf luence of at- Boesch, E. E. (1991). Symbolic action theory and cultural psychology.
titude and behavior upon the child’s intellectual development. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press. Bond, M. H., & Cheung, T. S. (1983). College students’ spontaneous
Bachnik, J. M. (1994). Uchi/soto—Challenging our conceptualiza- self-concept: The effect of culture among respondents in Hong
tions of self, social order, and language. In J. M. Bachnik & C. J. Kong, Japan, and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Quinn Jr. (Eds.), Situated meaning: Inside and outside in Japanese Psychology, 14, 153–171.
self, society, and language (pp. 3–37). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Bornstein, M. H., Azuma, H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Ogino, M.
University Press. (1990). Mother and infant activity and interaction in Japan and in
Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogic imagination. Austin: University the United States: I. A comparative macroanalysis of naturalistic
of Texas Press. exchanges. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. Austin: 13(3), 267–287.
University of Texas Press. Bornstein, M. H., & Cote, L. R. (2004). Mothers’ parenting cogni-
Baldwin, J. M. (1911). The individual and society. Boston: Boston Press. tions in cultures of origin, acculturating cultures, and cultures of
Baldwin, M. W., & Holmes, J. G. (1987). Salient private audiences destination. Child Development, 75(1), 221–235.
and awareness of the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Bourdieu, P. (1972). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge, En-
chology, 52, 1087–1098. gland: Cambridge University Press.
Banaji, M., & Prentice, D. (1994). The self in social contexts. Annual Bourdieu, P. (1979). Distinction: A social critique of the judgment of
Review of Psychology, 45, 297–332. taste. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Banarjee, R. (2002). Audience effects of self-presentation in child- Bourdieu, P. (1990). A logic of practice. Stanford, CA: Stanford Uni-
hood. Social Development, 11, 487–507. versity Press.
780 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

Bourdieu, P. (1991). Language and symbolic power. Cambridge, MA: Japanese culture and behavior: Selected readings (pp. 201–246).
Harvard University Press. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
Bourdieu, P., & Passeron, J.-C. (1977). Reproduction in education, Cazden, C. B. (1993). Vygotsky, Hymes, and Bakhtin: From word to
society and culture. Beverly Hills: Sage. utterance and voice. In E. A. Forman, N. Minick, & C. A. Stone
Bracken, B. A. (1996). Clinical applications of a context-dependent, (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Sociocultural dynamics in children’s
multidimensional model of self-concept. In B. Bracken (Ed.), development (pp. 197–212). New York: Oxford University Press.
Handbook of self concept. New York: Wiley. Cervantes, C. A. (2002). Explanatory emotion talk in Mexican-immi-
Brazelton, T. B. (1990). Parent-infant co-sleeping revisited. Ab Initio: grant and Mexican-American families. Hispanic Journal of Behav-
An International Newsletter for Professionals Working with Infants ioral Sciences, 24, 139–164.
and Their Families, 1, 1–7. Chao, R. K. (1992). Immigrant Chinese mothers and European Amer-
Briggs, C. L. (1986). Learning how to ask: A sociolinguistic appraisal ican mothers: Their aims of control and other child-rearing as-
of the role of the interview in social science research. New York: pects related to school achievement. Dissertation Abstracts
Cambridge University Press. International, 53(6-A), 1787–1788.
Briggs, J. L. (1992). Mazes of meaning: How a child and a culture Chao, R. K. (1993a). Clarification of the authoritarian parenting
style and parental control: Cultural concepts of Chinese child
create each other. In W. A. Corsaro & P. J. Miller (Eds.), Inter-
rearing. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Re-
pretive approaches to child socialization (pp. 25–50). San Fran-
search in Child Development, New Orleans, LA.
cisco: Jossey-Bass.
Chao, R. K. (1993b). East and West concepts of the self ref lecting in
Briggs, J. L. (1998). Inuit morality play: The emotional education of a
mothers’ reports of their child rearing. Los Angeles: University of
three-year-old. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
California.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cam-
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.). (1988). The nature of
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bronfenbrenner, U., Kessel, F., Kessen, W., & White, S. (1985). To-
Clancy, P. (1999). The socialization of affect in Japanese mother-
ward a critical social history of developmental psychology. Amer-
child conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(4), 1397–1421.
ican Psychologist, 41, 1218–1230.
Clark, C. D. (2003). In sickness and in play: Children coping with
Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA:
chronic illness. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Harvard University Press.
Clark, H. (1996). Language in use. New York: Cambridge Univer-
Bruner, J. S. (1990a). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard sity Press.
University Press.
Cole, M. (1988). Cross-cultural research in the sociohistorical tradi-
Bruner, J. S. (1990b). Culture and human development: A new look. tion. Human Development, 31, 137–157.
Human Development, 33, 344–355.
Cole, M. (1990). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. In
Bruner, J. S. (1993). Do we “acquire” culture or vice versa: Reply to J. J. Berman (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 37.
M. Tomasello, A. C. Kruger, & H. H. Ratner, Cultural learning. Cross-cultural perspectives. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 163(3), 515–516.
Cole, M. (1992). Context, maturity, and the cultural constitution of
Bullock, M., & Lütkenhaus, P. (1990). Who am I? Self-understanding development. In L. T. Winegar & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Children’s de-
in toddlers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36(2), 217–238. velopment within social context: Vol. 2. Research and methodology
Burger, L. K., & Miller, P. J. (1999). Early talk about the past revis- (pp. 5–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ited: A comparison of working-class and middle-class families. Cole, M. (1995). The supra-individual envelope of development: Ac-
Journal of Child Language, 26, 1–30. tivity and practice, situation and context. In J. J. Goodnow, P. J.
Cahan, E. D., & White, S. H. (1992). Proposals for a second psychol- Miller, & F. Kessel (Eds.), Cultural practices as contexts for de-
ogy. American Psychologist, 47, 224–235. velopment: New directions for child development. San Francisco:
Camic, C. (1986). The matter of habit. American Journal of Sociol- Jossey-Bass.
ogy, 91, 1039–1087. Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline.
Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and con- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
ceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping Cole, M. (1999). Culture in development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E.
the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook
(pp. 169–200). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (4th ed., pp. 73–123). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carrithers, M., Collins, S., & Lukes, S. (1987). The category of the Cole, M. (2001). Remembering history in sociocultural research.
person: Anthropology, philosophy, history. Cambridge, England: Human Development, 44, 166–169.
Cambridge University Press. Cole, M., Engeström, Y., & Velasquéz, O. (2000). Mind, culture, and
Carroll, V. (Ed.). (1970). Adoption in Eastern Oceania. Honolulu: activity. New York: Cambridge University Press.
University of Hawaii Press. Cole, M., & Traupmann, K. (1981). Comparative cognitive research:
Caudill, W., & Plath, D. W. (1966). “ Who sleeps by whom? Parent-child Learning from learning disabled children. In W. A. Collins (Ed.),
involvement in urban Japanese Families.” Psychiatry, 29, 344–366. Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 14. Aspects of the
Caudill, W., & Schooler, C. (1973). Child behavior and child rearing development of competence (pp. 125–155). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
in Japan and the United States: An interim report. Journal of Ner- Coles, R. (1989). The call of stories. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.
vous and Mental Diseases, 157, 323–338. Collingwood, R. G. (1961). On the so-called idea of causation. In H.
Caudill, W., & Weinstein, H. (1969). Maternal care and infant behav- Morris (Ed.), Freedom and responsibility: Readings in philosophy
ior in Japan and America. Psychiatry, 32, 12–43. and law. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Caudill, W., & Weinstein, H. (1986). Maternal care and infant behav- Cooley, D. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New
ior in Japan and America. In T. S. Lebra & W. P. Lebra (Eds.), York: Scribners.
References 781

Cooper, C. R., & Denner, J. (1998). Theories linking culture and psy- Deng, F. M. (1972). The Dinka of the Southern Sudan. New York:
chology: Universal and community-specific processes. Annual Re- Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
view of Psychology, 49, 559–584. Dewey, J. (1938). Experience and education. New York: Macmillan.
Cooper, C. R., Dominguez, E., & Rosas, S. (in press). Soledad’s Doi, T. (1981). The anatomy of dependence. Tokyo: Kodansha.
dream: Immigration, children’s worlds, and immigration path- Donagan, A. (1987). Choice, the essential element in human action.
ways to college. In C. R. Cooper, C. García Coll, T. Bartko, London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
H. Davis, & C. Chapman (Eds.), Hills of gold: Rethinking diversity Dunn, J. (1983). Sibling relationships in early childhood. Child Devel-
and contexts as resources for children’s developmental pathways. opment, 54, 787–811.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Dunn, J. (1993). Young children’s close relationships: Beyond attach-
Cooper, C. R., García Coll, C., Thorne, B., & Orellana, M. F. (in ment. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
press). Beyond social categories: Studying four social processes
Dunn, J. (2000). Mind-reading, emotion understanding, and relation-
in immigration, ethnicity, and race in children’s lives at school. In
ships. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 142–144.
C. R. Cooper, C. García Coll, T. Bartko, H. Davis, & C. Chapman
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L.
(Eds.), Hills of gold: Rethinking diversity and contexts as re-
(1991). Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings
sources for children’s developmental pathways. New York: Oxford
and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child
University Press.
Development, 62, 1352–1366.
Coopersmith, S. A. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. San Fran-
Duranti, A., & Goodwin, C. (1992). Rethinking context: Language as an
cisco: Freeman.
interactive phenomenon. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Corsaro, W. A. (1992). Interpretive reproduction in children’s peer
Dyson, A. H. (1993). Social worlds of children learning to write in an
culture. Social Psychology Quarterly, 55, 160–177.
urban primary school. New York: Teachers College Press.
Corsaro, W. A. (1997). The sociology of childhood. Thousand Oaks, Dyson, A. H., & Genishi, C. (Eds.). (1994). The need for story: Cul-
CA: Pine Forge Press. tural diversity in classroom and community. Urbana, IL: National
Corsaro, W. A., & Miller, P. J. (Eds.). (1992). Interpretive approaches Council of Teachers of English.
to childhood socialization—New directions for child development: Eckensberger, L. H. (1990). From cross-cultural psychology to cul-
Vol. 58. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. tural psychology. Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of
Corsaro, W. A., Molinari, L., & Rosier, K. A. (2002). Zena and Human Cognition, 12, 37–52.
Carlotta: Transition narratives and early education in the United Eckensberger, L. H. (1995). Activity or action: Two different roads
States and Italy. Human Development, 45, 323–348. towards an integration of culture into psychology. Culture and
Cortés, D. E. (1995). Variations in familism in two generations of Puerto Psychology, 1, 67–80.
Ricans. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 249–255. Eder, R. A., & Mangelsdorf, S. (1997). The emotional basis of early
Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and personality development: Implications for the emergent self-
the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, concept. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of
56, 124–131. personality psychology. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C. (1998). Social stigma. In Eisenberg, A. R. (1985). Learning to describe past experiences in
D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & L. Gardner (Eds.), The handbook of social conversation. Discourse Processes, 8, 177–204.
psychology (Vol. 2, 4th ed., pp. 504–553). New York: McGraw- Eisenberg, A. R. (1986). Teasing: Verbal play in two Mexicano
Hill. homes. In B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socializa-
Crook, C. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. British tion across cultures (pp. 182–198). New York: Cambridge Univer-
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(4), 618–619. sity Press.
Cushman, P. (1995). Constructing the self, constructing America: A Elster, J. (1987). The multiple self. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
cultural history of psychotherapy. Boston: Addison-Wesley. University Press.
Damon, W. (1984). Peer interaction: The untapped potential. Journal Engel, S. (1995). The stories children tell: Making sense of the narra-
of Applied Developmental Psychology, 5, 331–343. tives of childhood. New York: Freeman.
Damon, W. (1995). Greater expectations: Overcoming the culture of Epstein, S. (1973). The self-concept revisited or a theory of a theory.
indulgence in America’s homes and schools. New York: Free Press. American Psychologist, 28, 405–416.
Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1988). Self understanding in childhood and Erickson, F. D. (1986). Qualitative methods in research on teaching.
adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (3rd
D’Andrade, R. G. (1981). The cultural part of cognition. Cognitive ed.). New York: Macmillan.
Science, 5, 179–195. Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.). (1996). The road to excellence. Mahwah,
D’Andrade, R. G. (1992). Schemas and motivations. In R. G. D’An- NJ: Erlbaum.
drade & C. Strauss (Eds.), Human motives and cultural models Erikson, E. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.
(pp. 23–44). New York: Cambridge University Press. Fader, A. (2001). Literacy, bilingualism and gender in a Hasidic com-
D’Andrade, R. G. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology. munity. Linguistics and Education, 12(3), 261–283.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Fajans, J. (1985). The person in social context: The social character
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1990). A motivational approach to self: of Baining “psychology.” In G. M. White & W. J. Kirkpatrick
Integration in personality. In R. A. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska (Eds.), Person, self, and experience (pp. 367–400). Berkeley: Uni-
Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 38. Perspectives on motivation versity of California Press.
(pp. 237–288). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Farver, J. M. (1999). Activity setting analysis: A model for examin-
de León, L. (2000). The emergent participant: Interactive patterns in ing the role of culture in development. In A. Göncü (Ed.), Chil-
the socialization of Tzotzil (Mayan) infants. Journal of Linguistic dren’s engagement in the world (pp. 99–127). New York:
Anthropology, 8(2), 131–161. Cambridge University Press.
782 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

Farver, J. M., Kim, Y. K., & Lee, Y. (1995). Cultural differences in Garrett, P. B., & Baquedano-López, P. (2002). Language socializa-
Korean- and Anglo-American preschoolers’ social interaction and tion: Reproduction and continuity, transformation and change.
play behaviors. Child Development, 66(4), 1088–1099. Annual Review of Anthrolopology, 31, 339–361.
Fernald, A., & Morikawa, H. (1993). Common themes and cultural Garvey, C. (Ed.). (1992). Introduction: Talk in the study of socializa-
variations in Japanese and American mothers’ speech to infants. tion and development [Invitational issue]. Merrill-Palmer Quar-
Child Development, 64(3), 637–656. terly, 38(1), iii–viii.
Fiske, A., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). The Garvey, C., & Kramer, T. (1989). The language of social pretend play.
cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & Developmental Review, 9, 364–382.
G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology Gaskins, S. (1999). Children’s daily lives in a Mayan village: A Case
(pp. 915–981). San Francisco: McGraw-Hill. Study of Culturally Constructed Roles and Activities. In A.
Fiske, A. P. (1991). Structures of social life: The four elementary Göncü (Ed.), Children’s engagement in the world (pp. 25–61).
forms of human relations. New York: Free Press. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Frame- Gauvain, M. (2001). Cultural tools, social interaction, and the devel-
work for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Re- opment of thinking. Human Development, 44, 126–143.
view, 99, 689–723. Geertz, C. (1973a). The impact of the concept of culture on the con-
Fiske, A. P., & Tetlock, P. E. (1997). Taboo trade-offs: Reactions to cept of man. In C. Geertz (Ed.), The interpretation of cultures.
transgressions that transgress spheres of exchange. Political Psy- New York: Basic Books.
chology, 18, 255–297. Geertz, C. (Ed.). (1973b). The interpretation of cultures. New York:
Fivush, R. (1993). Emotional content of parent-child conversations Basic Books.
about the past. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Geertz, C. (1984). From the natives’ point of view. In R. A. Shweder
Child Psychology: Vol. 26. Memory and af fect in development & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and
(pp. 39–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Fivush, R., & Buckner, J. P. (2000). Gender, sadness, and depres- Gelman, R. (1990). First principles organize attention to and learning
sion: The development of emotional focus through gendered dis- about relevant data: Numbers and the animate-inanimate distinc-
course. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social tion as examples. Cognitive Science, 14, 79–106.
psychological perspectives (pp. 232–253). New York: Cambridge Gergen, K. J. (1968). Personal consistency and the presentation of
University Press. self. In C. Gordon & K. J. Gergen (Eds.), The self in social inter-
Flanagan, O. (1992). Consciousness reconsidered. Cambridge, action (pp. 299–308). New York: Wiley.
MA: MIT Press. Gergen, K. J. (1991). The saturated self. New York: Basic Books.
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1995). Young children’s Giddens, A. (1984). The constitution of society. Oxford, England:
knowledge about thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research Polity Press.
in Child Development, 60(1, Serial No. 243). Gilmore, P., & Glatthorn, A. A. (Eds.). (1982). Children in and out of
Foucault, M. (1980). Power-knowledge: Selected interviews and other school: Ethnography and education. Washington, DC: Center for
writings. Brighton, Great Britain: Harvester Press. Applied Linguistics.
Frijda, N. H., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions Goddard, C. (1997). Constrastive semantics and cultural psychol-
of emotions. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and ogy: “Surprise” in Malay and English. Culture and Psychology,
culture (pp. 51–87). Washington, DC: American Psychological 2, 153–181.
Association. Goddard, C. (2001). Lexico-semantic universals: A critical overview.
Fuligni, A. (2001). Family obligation and academic motivation of Linguistic Typology, 5, 1–66.
adolescents from Asian, Latin American, and European back- Goddard, C., & Wierzbicka, A. (1994). Semantic and lexical univer-
grounds. In A. J. Fuligni (Ed.), Family obligation and assistance sals: Theory and empirical findings. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
during adolescence: Contextual variations and developmental im- Göncü, A. (Ed.). (1999). Children’s engagement in the world. New
plications (pp. 61–76). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. York: Cambridge University Press.
Fung, H. (1999). Becoming a moral child: The socialization of shame Göncü, A., Patt, J., & Kouba, E. (2002). Understanding young chil-
among young Chinese children. Ethos, 27, 180–209. dren’s play in context. In P. Smith & C. Hart (Eds.), Handbook of
Fung, H. (2003). When culture meets psyche: Understanding the social development. London: Blackwell.
contextualized self through the life and dreams of an elderly Gone, J. P. (1999). We were through as keepers of it: The “missing pipe
Taiwanese woman. Taiwan Journal of Anthropology, 1(2), narrative” and Gros Ventre cultural identity. Ethos, 27(4), 415–440.
149–175. González, N. (2001). I am my language: Discourses of women and
Fung, H., & Chen, E. C. (2001). Across time and beyond skin: Self children in the borderlands. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
and transgression in the everyday socialization of shame among Goodnow, J. J. (1990a). The socialization of cognition: What’s in-
Taiwanese preschool children. Social Development, 10, 419–436. volved. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cul-
Fung, H., Miller, P. J., & Lin, L. (2004). Listening is active: Lessons tural psychology: Essays on comparative human development
from the Narrative Practices of Taiwanese Families. In M. W. (pp. 259–286). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Pratt & B. H. Fiese (Eds.), Family stories and the life course Goodnow, J. J. (1990b). Using sociology to extend psychological ac-
(pp. 303–323). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. counts of cognitive development. Human Development, 33, 81–107.
Gallimore, R., Weisner, T., Guthrie, D., Bernheimer, L. P., & Nihira, Goodnow, J. J. (1995). Acceptable disagreement across generations.
K. (1993). Family responses to young children with developmen- In J. Smetana (Ed.), Parents’ socio-cognitive models of develop-
tal delays: Accommodation activity in ecological and cultural ment: New directions for child development (pp. 51–64). San Fran-
context. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 98, 185–206. cisco: Jossey-Bass.
References 783

Goodnow, J. J. (1996a). Acceptable ignorance, negotiable disagree- Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Spangler, G., Suess, G., & Unzner,
ment: Alternative views of learning. In D. Olson & N. Torrance L. (1985). Maternal sensitivity and newborns’ orientation re-
(Eds.), Handbook of psychology in education (pp. 345–368). Ox- sponses as related to quality of attachment in northern Germany.
ford, England: Blackwell. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Monographs of the Society for
Goodnow, J. J. (1996b). Collaborative rules: How are people sup- Research in Child Development: Growing points of attachment the-
posed to work with one another. In P. B. Baltes & U. M. ory and research, 50(Serial Nos. 1–2). Chicago: University of
Staudinger (Eds.), Interactive minds: Life-span perspectives on the Chicago Press.
social foundation of cognition (pp. 163–197). New York: Cam- Guisinger, S., & Blatt, S. J. (1994). Individuality and relatedness:
bridge University Press. Evolution of a fundamental dialect. American Psychologist, 49,
Goodnow, J. J. (1996c). From household practices to parents’ ideas 104–111.
about work and interpersonal relationships. In S. Harkness & C. Gutiérrez, K. D. (2002). Studying cultural practices in urban learn-
Super (Eds.), Parents’ cultural belief systems (pp. 313–344). New ing communities. Human Development, 45, 312–321.
York: Guilford Press.
Haidt, J., Koller, S., & Dias, M. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality,
Goodnow, J. J. (1997). Parenting and the “ transmission” and “inter- or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social
nalization” of values: From social-cultural perspectives to Psychology, 65, 613–628.
within-family analyses. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.),
Haight, W. L., & Miller, P. J. (1993). Pretending at home: Early de-
Handbook of parenting and the transmission of values
velopment in sociocultural context. Albany: State University of
(pp. 333–361). New York: Wiley.
New York Press.
Goodnow, J. J. (2004a, April). Development as changing participa-
tion. Paper presented at biennial meeting of Child Development Haight, W. L., Wang, X., Fung, H., Williams, K., & Mintz, J. (1999).
Association, Washington, DC. Universal, developmental, and variable aspects of young chil-
dren’s play: A cross-cultural comparison of pretending at home.
Goodnow, J. J. (2004b). Relational models theory: The domain of
Child Development, 70, 1477–1488.
household work. In N. Haslam (Ed.), Relational models theory:
Applications in research and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Hallowell, A. I. (1955). Culture and experience. Philadelphia: Univer-
sity of Pennsylvania Press.
Goodnow, J. J. (in press). Contexts, diversity, pathways: Linking and
expanding with a view to theory and practice. In C. R. Cooper, C. Hamaguchi, E. (1985). A contextual model of the Japanese: Toward a
García Coll, T. Bartko, H. Davis, & C. Chapman (Eds.), Hills of methodological innovation in Japan studies. Journal of Japanese
gold: Rethinking diversity and contexts as resources for children’s Studies, 11, 289–321.
developmental pathways. New York: Oxford University Press. Han, J. J., Leichtman, M. D., & Wang, Q. (1998). Autobiographical
Goodnow, J. J., Miller, P. J., & Kessel, F. (Eds.). (1995). Cultural memory in Korean, Chinese, and American children. Developmen-
practices as contexts for development: Vol. 67. New directions for tal Psychology, 34(4), 701–713.
child development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Hanks, W. F. (1996). Language and communicative practices. Boul-
Goodwin, M. H. (1990). He-said-she-said: Talk as social organization der, CO: Westview Press.
among black children. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1992). The cultural foundations of fa-
Goody, E. (1982). Parenthood and social reproduction. Cambridge, thers’ roles: Evidence from Kenya and the United States. In B. S.
England: Cambridge University Press. Hewlett (Ed.), Father-child relations: Cultural and biosocial con-
Gopnik, A. (1993). Psychopsychology. Consciousness and Cognition, texts. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
2, 264–280. Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (Eds.). (1996). Parents’ cultural belief
Gottlieb, A. (2004). The afterlife is where we came from: The culture systems. New York: Guilford Press.
of infancy in West Africa. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Hart, D. (1988). The adolescent self-concept in social context. In D.
Gottlieb, G. (1991). The experiential canalization of behavioral de- Lapsley & F. Power (Eds.), Self, ego, and identity: Integrative ap-
velopment. Developmental Psychology, 27, 4–13. proaches (pp. 71–90). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Greenfield, P. (1997). Culture as process: Empirical methodology for Hart, D., & Edelstein, W. (1992). The relationship of self-
cultural psychology. In J. W. Berry, Y. H. Poortinga, & J. Pandey understanding in childhood to social class, community type, and
(Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Vol. 1. Theory and teacher-rated intellectual and social competence. Journal of
method. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Cross-Cultural Psychology, 23(3), 353–365.
Greenfield, P., & Cocking, R. (1994). Cross-cultural roots of minor- Hart, D., Fegley, S., Hung Chan, Y., Mulvey, D., & Fischer, L. (1993).
ity child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Judgment about personal identity in childhood and adolescence.
Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Social Development, 2(1), 66–81.
Cultural pathways through development. Annual Review of Psy- Harter, S. (1983). Developmental perspectives on the self system.
chology, 54, 461–490. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol.
Grossmann, K. E., & Grossmann, K. (1981). Parent-infant attach- 4. Socialization, personality, and social development. New York:
ment relationships in Bielefeld. In K. Immelman, G. Barlow, L. Wiley.
Petrovich, & M. Main (Eds.), Behavioral development: The Harter, S. (1986). Processes underlying the construction, mainte-
Bielefeld interdisciplinary project. New York: Cambridge Uni- nance and enhancement of self-concept in children. In J. Suls &
versity Press. A. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspective on the self (Vol. 3,
Grossmann, K., & Grossmann, K. E. (1991). Newborn behavior, the pp. 136–182). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
quality of early parenting and later toddler-parent relationships in Harter, S. (1990). Causes, correlates and the functional role of global
a group of German infants. In J. K. Nugent, B. M. Lester, & T. B. self-worth: A life span perspective. In R. J. Sternberg & J. J. Kol-
Brazelton (Eds.), The cultural context of infancy (Vol. 2). Nor- ligian (Eds.), Competence considered (pp. 67–97). New Haven,
wood, NJ: Ablex. CT: Yale University Press.
784 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

Harter, S. (1996). Historical roots of contemporary issues involving Hewlett, B. S. (1991). Intimate fathers: The nature and context of
self-concept. In B. A. Bracken (Ed.), Handbook of self-concept. Aka Pygmy paternal infant care. Ann Arbor: University of
New York: Wiley. Michigan Press.
Harwood, R., Leyendecker, B., Carlson, V., Asencio, M., & Miller, A. Hewlett, B. S. (Ed.). (1992). Father-child relations: Cultural and
(2002). Parenting among Latino families in the US. In M. H. Born- biosocial contexts. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
stein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Social conditions and Hicks, D. A. (1994). Individual and social meanings in the classroom:
applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 21–46). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Narrative discourse as a boundary phenomenon. Journal of Narra-
Harwood, R., & Miller, J. (1991). Perceptions of attachment behav- tive and Life History, 4, 215–240.
ior: A comparison of Anglo and Puerto-Rican mothers. Merrill- Higgins, E. T. (1990). Self-state representations: Patterns of inter-
Palmer Quarterly, 3(4), 583–599. connected beliefs with specific holistic meanings and impor-
Harwood, R. L., Miller, J. G., & Irizarry, N. L. (1995). Culture and tance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 248–253.
attachment: Perceptions of the child in context. New York: Guil- Higgins, E. T., & Parsons, J. (1983). Social cognition and the social
ford Press. life of the child: Stages as subcultures. In E. T. Higgins, D. N.
Harwood, R. L., Schoelmerish, A., Ventura-Cook, E., Schulze, P., & Ruble, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social devel-
Wilson, S. (1996). Culture and class inf luences on Anglo and opment: A sociocultural perspective (pp. 15–62). New York: Cam-
Puerto-Rican mothers: Regarding long-term socialization goals bridge University Press.
and child behavior. Child Development, 62, 2446–2462. Ho, D. Y. (1993). Relational orientation in Asian social psychology.
Hatano, G. (1995). The psychology of Japanese literacy: Expanding In U. Kim & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Indigenous psychologies: Research
“ the practice account.” In L. Martin, K. Nelson, & E. Tobach and experiences in cultural context. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
(Eds.), Sociocultural psychology: Theory and practice of doing and Holland, D., Lachicotte, W., Jr., Skinner, D., & Cain, C. (1998). Iden-
knowing (pp. 250–275). New York: Cambridge University Press. tity and agency in cultural worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1986). Two courses of expertise. In H. versity Press.
Stevenson, H. Azuma, & K. Hakuta (Eds.), Child development and Holland, D. C., & Quinn, N. (Eds.). (1987). Cultural models in lan-
education in Japan (pp. 262–272). San Francisco: Freeman. guage and thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1992). Desituating cognition through the Howard, A. (1985). Ethnopsychology and the prospects for a cultural
construction of conceptual knowledge. In P. Light & G. Butter- psychology. In G. M. White & J. Kirkpatrick (Eds.), Person, self
worth (Eds.), Context and cognition: Ways of learning and know- and experience: Exploring specific ethnopsychologies. Berkeley:
ing (pp. 115–133). London: Harvester Wheatsheaf. University of California Press.
Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (2000). Domain-specific constraints of Hudley, E. V. P., Haight, W., & Miller, P. J. (2003). “Raise up a
conceptual development. International Journal of Behavioral De- child”: Human development in an African American family.
velopment, 24, 267–275. Chicago: Lyceum.
Hatano, G., Okada, N., & Tanabe, H. (Eds.). (2001). Af fective minds. Hughes, D., & Chen, L. (1999). The nature of parents’ race-related
Amsterdam: Elsevier. communications to children: A developmental perspective. In
L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A
Hatano, G., Siegler, R. S., Richards, D. D., Inagaki, K., Stavy, R., &
handbook of contemporary issues (pp. 467–490). Philadelphia:
Wax, N. (1993). The development of biological knowledge: A
Psychology Press.
multi-national study. Cognitive Development, 8, 47–62.
Hymes, D. (1974). Foundations in sociolinguistics: An ethnographic ap-
Hatano, G., & Wertsch, J. V. (Eds.). (2001). Cultural minds: Human
proach (pp. 29–66). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
Development, 44(Serial Nos. 2–3) [Special issue].
Press.
Hatchett, S. J., Cochran, D. L., & Jackson, J. S. (1991). Family life.
Ikegama, Y. (1991). “DO-language” and “BECOME-language”: Two
In J. S. Jackson (Ed.), Life in Black America (pp. 46–83). New-
contrasting types of linguistic representation. In Y. Ikegama
bury Park, CA: Sage.
(Ed.), The empire of signs: Semiotic Essays on Japanese Culture
He, A. W. (2001). The language of ambiguity: Practices in Chinese (Vol. 8, pp. 286–327). Philadelphia: Benjamins.
heritage language classes. Discourse Studies, 3(1), 75–96.
Inagaki, K. (1990). The effects of raising animals on children’s bio-
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in com- logical knowledge. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
munities and classrooms. New York: Cambridge University Press. 8, 119–129.
Heine, S. H., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2002). Young children’s naive thinking
Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological about the biological world. New York: Psychology Press.
Review, 106(4), 766–794. Ingold, T. (1986). Evolution and social life. New York: Cambridge
Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, University Press.
C., et al. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Ingold, T. (1991). Becoming persons: Consciousness and sociality in
Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving mo- human evolution. Cultural Dynamics, 4(3), 355–378.
tivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Ishii, K., & Kitayama, S. (2001). Spontaneous attention to emo-
Psychology, 81(4), 599–615. tional speech in Japan and the United states. In G. Hatano, N.
Herdt, G. (1981). Guardians of the f lutes. New York: McGraw-Hill. Okada, & H. Tanabe (Eds.), Af fective minds (pp. 243–248). Am-
Herdt, G. (1990). Sambia nosebleeding rites and male proximity to sterdam: Elsevier.
women. In J. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural Jahoda, G. (1990). Our forgotten ancestors. In J. J. Berman (Ed.), Ne-
psychology: Essays on comparative human development. New braska Symposium on Motivation, 1989: Cross-cultural perspec-
York: Cambridge University Press. tives (pp. 1–40). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
References 785

Jahoda, G. (1992). Crossroads between culture and mind: Continu- Kondo, D. (1990). Crafting selves: Power, gender, and discourses
ities and change in theories of human nature. Cambridge, MA: of identity in a Japanese workplace. Chicago: University of
Harvard University Press. Chicago Press.
James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover. Konner, M. J. (1975). Relations among infants and juveniles in com-
Jensen, L. A. (1996). Dif ferent habits, dif ferent hearts: Orthodoxy parative perspective. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Friend-
and progressivism in the United States and India. Unpublished ship and peer relations. New York: Wiley.
PhD thesis, University of Chicago, Illinois. Kratz, C. (1994). Af fecting performance: Meaning, movement and ex-
Jensen, L. A. (2005). Through two lenses: A cultural-developmental perience in Okiek women’s initiation. Washington, DC: Smithson-
approach to moral psychology. Available from JensenL@cua.edu. ian Institution Press.
Jessor, R., Colby, A., & Shweder, R. A. (Eds.). (1996). Ethnography Kripal, J. (1995). Kali’s child: The mystical and the erotic in the life and
and human development: Context and meaning in social inquiry. teachings of Rama Krishna. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kristeva, J. (1980). Desire in language: A semiotic approach to litera-
Johnson, F. (1985). The Western concept of self. In A. Marsella, ture and art. New York: Columbia University Press.
G. DeVos, & F. L. K. Hsu (Eds.), Culture and self. London: Tavis- Kuebli, J., & Fivush, R. (1992). Gender differences in parent-child
tock. conversations about past emotions. Sex Roles, 27, 683–698.
Josephs, R. A., Markus, H., & Tarafodi, R. W. (1992). Gender and Kulick, D. (1992). Language shifts and cultural reproduction: Social-
self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, ization, self, and syncretism in a Papua New Guinean village. New
391–402. York: Cambridge University Press.
Kakar, S. (1978). The inner world: A psychoanalytic study of child- Kurtz, S. N. (1992). All the mothers are one: Hindu India and the
hood and society in India. New York: Oxford University Press. cultural reshaping of psychoanalysis. New York: Columbia Uni-
versity Press.
Kakar, S. (1982). Shamans, mystics and doctors. Boston: Beacon Press.
Kusserow, A. S. (1999). De-homogenizing American individualism:
Kakar, S. (1996). The colors of violence: Cultural identities, religion
Socializing hard and soft individualism in Manhattan and
and conf lict. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Queens. Ethos, 27(2), 210–234.
Kapstein, M. (1989). Santaraksita on the fallacies of personalistic vi-
Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. (1983). Culture and
talism. Journal of Indian Philosophy, 17, 43–59.
cognitive development. In P. Mussen (Series Ed.) & W. Kessen
Karasawa, M. (1998). The cultural basis of self- and other-percep- (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. History, theory
tions: Self-criticism and other-enhancement in Japan. Unpub- and methods. New York: Wiley.
lished PhD dissertation, Shirayuri College, Tokyo, Japan.
Lampert, M., Rittenhouse, P., & Crumbaugh, C. (1996). Agreeing
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularit y. Cambridge, to disagree: Developing sociable mathematical discourse. In
MA: MIT Press. D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), Handbook of education and
Keil, F. C. (1984). Mechanisms in cognitive development and the human development: New models of learning, teaching, and
structure of knowledge. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Mechanisms of schooling (pp. 731–764). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.
cognitive development (pp. 81–99). New York: Freeman. Lancy, D. (1996). Playing on the mother ground: Cultural routines
Kessel, F. S., & Siegel, A. W. (Eds.). (1983). The child and other cul- for children’s development. New York: Guilford Press.
tural inventions. New York: Praeger. Lave, J. (1990). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and cul-
Kessen, W. (1983). The child and other cultural inventions. In F. S. ture in everyday life. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Kessel & A. W. Siegel (Eds.), The child and other cultural inven- Press.
tions. New York: Praeger. Lave, J. (1991). Situating learning in communities of practice. In
Kessen, W. (Ed.). (1990). The rise and fall of development. Worcester, L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives
MA: Clark University Press. on socially shared cognition (pp. 63–82). Washington, DC: Ameri-
Kihlstrom, J. F., & Cantor, N. (1984). Mental representations of the can Psychological Association.
self. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 1–47. Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate periph-
Kim, U. (1987). The parent-child relationship: The core of Korean eral participation. New York: Cambridge University Press.
collectivism. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Leadbeater, B. J., & Dionne, J. P. (1981). The adolescent’s use of for-
Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology, Newcastle, Australia. mal operational thinking in solving problems related to identity
Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (1994). Emotion and culture: Empiri- resolutions. Adolescence, 16, 111–121.
cal studies of mutual inf luences. Washington, DC: American Psy- Leahy, R., & Shirk, S. (1985). Social cognition and the development
chological Association. of the self. In R. Leahy (Ed.), The development of the self. New
Kitayama, S., Markus, H., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. York: Academic Press.
(1995). Individual and collective processes of self-esteem manage- Lebra, T. S. (1976). Japanese patterns of behavior. Honolulu: Univer-
ment: Self enhancement in the United States and self-depreciation sity of Hawaii Press.
in Japan. Kyoto, Japan: Kyoto University. Lebra, T. S. (1992). Culture, self, and communication. Ann Arbor:
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. University of Michigan.
(1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of Lebra, T. S. (1993). Culture, self, and communication in Japan and
the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self- the United States. In W. B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Communication in
criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Japan and the United States (pp. 51–87). Albany: State University
72(6), 1245–1267. of New York Press.
786 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

Lebra, T. S. (1994). Mother and child in Japanese socialization: A Li, J. (2004). “I learn and grow big”: Chinese preschoolers’ purposes
Japan-US comparison. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking for learning. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
(Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development 28, 116–128.
(pp. 259–274). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lifton, R. J. (1993). The Protean self. New York: Harper.
LeVine, L. E. (1983). Mine: Self-definition in 2-year-old-boys. De- Lillard, A. (1996). Body or mind: Children’s categorizing of pre-
velopmental Psychology, 19, 544–549. tense. Child Development, 67(4), 1717–1734.
LeVine, R. A. (1970). Cross-cultural study in child psychology. In Little, T. O., Oettingen, G., Stetsenko, A., & Baltes, P. B. (1995).
P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (3rd Children’s school performance-related beliefs: How do Ameri-
ed.). New York: Wiley. can children compare to German and Russian children. Berlin,
LeVine, R. A. (1978). Western schools in non-Western societies: Germany: Max Planck Institute for Human Development and
Psychosocial impact and cultural response. Teachers College Education.
Record, 79. Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the
LeVine, R. A. (1989). Cultural environments in child development. In development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child
W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow. San Development, 74, 1130–1144.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Lucy, J. A. (1992a). Grammatical categories and cognition: A case
LeVine, R. A. (1990a). Enculturation: A biosocial perspective on the study of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. New York: Cambridge
self. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: University Press.
Infancy to childhood. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lucy, J. A. (1992b). Language diversity and thought: A reformulation
LeVine, R. A. (1990b). Infant environments in psychoanalysis: A of the linguistic relativity hypothesis. New York: Cambridge Uni-
cross-cultural view. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt versity Press.
(Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human devel- Lutz, C. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Mi-
opment (pp. 454–474). New York: Cambridge University Press. cronesian Atoll and their challenge to Western theory. Chicago:
LeVine, R. A. (2004). Challenging expert knowledge: Findings from University of Chicago Press.
an African study of child care and development. In U. P. Gielen & Lutz, C., & White, G. (1986). The anthropology of emotions. Annual
J. P. Roopnaraine (Eds.), Childhood and adolescence in cross-cul- Review of Anthropology, 15, 405–436.
tural perspective. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Malinowski, B. (1922). Argonauts of the Western Pacific. New
LeVine, R. A., Dixon, S., LeVine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P. H., York: Dutton.
Keefer, C., et al. (1994). Child care and culture: Lessons from Mandelbaum, D. G. (Ed.). (1951). Selected writings of Edward Sapir
Africa. New York: Cambridge University Press. in language, culture, and personality. Berkeley: University of Cal-
LeVine, R. A., & LeVine, B. B. (1966). Nyansongo: A Gusii commu- ifornia Press.
nity in Kenya. New York: Wiley. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991a). Cultural variation in the self-
LeVine, R. A., LeVine, S., Iwanaga, M., & Marvin, R. (1970). Child concept. In J. Strauss & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), The self: Interdis-
care and social attachment in a Nigerian community: A prelimi- ciplinary approaches (pp. 18–48). New York: Springer-Verlag.
nary report. Paper presented at the American Psychological Asso- Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991b). Culture and the self: Impli-
ciation meetings. cations for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Re-
LeVine, R. A., Miller, P., & West, M. (Eds.). (1988). Parental behav- view, 98, 224–253.
ior in diverse societies. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994a). The cultural construction of
LeVine, R. A., & Norman, K. (2001). The infant’s acquisition of cul- self and emotion: Implications for social behavior. In S. Kitayama
ture: Early attachment re-examined from an anthropological per- & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of
spective. In C. Moore & H. Mathews (Eds.), The psychology of mutual inf luences (pp. 89–130). Washington, DC: American Psy-
cultural experience. New York: Cambridge University Press. chological Association.
LeVine, S. (n.d.). Caregiving in Godavari, Nepal, 1990 [Videotape]. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994b). The cultural shaping of emo-
tion: A conceptual framework. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus
Levy, R. I. (1973). Tahitians: Mind and experience in the Society Is-
(Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual inf lu-
lands. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
ences (pp. 339–351). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Levy, R. I. (1984). Emotion, knowing and culture. In R. A. Shweder Association.
& R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and
Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Models of agency: Sociocultural
emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
diversity in the construction of action. In R. A. Dienstbier (Se-
Lewin, K. (1943). Defining the field at given time. Psychological Re- ries Ed.) & V. Murphy-Berman & J. J. Berman (Vol. Eds.), Ne-
view, 50, 292–310. braska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 49. Cross-cultural
Lewis, C. C. (1995). Educating hearts and minds. New York: Cam- dif ferences in perspectives on the self (pp. 1–57). Lincoln: Uni-
bridge University Press. versity of Nebraska Press.
Lewis, M. (1993). Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, Markus, H. R., Kitayama, S., & Heiman, R. J. (1996). Culture and
shame, and guilt. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of “ basic” psychological principles. In E. T. Higgins & A. W.
emotions (pp. 563–573). New York: Guilford Press. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic princi-
Lewis, M., Sullivan, M., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self- ples. New York: Guilford Press.
development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, Markus, H. R., Mullally, P. R., & Kitayama, S. (1997). Collective
60, 146–156. self-schemas: The sociocultural grounding of the personal. In U.
Li, J. (2002). A cultural model of learning: Chinese “ heart and Neisser & D. Jopling (Eds.), The conceptual self in context: Cul-
mind for wanting to learn.” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol- ture, experience, self-understanding. Cambridge, England: Cam-
ogy, 33, 246–267. bridge University Press.
References 787

Markus, H. R., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psy- Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday so-
chologist, 41, 954–969. cial explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Markus, H. R., & Wurf, E. (1986). The dynamic self-concept: A 46, 961–978.
social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, Miller, J. G. (1994a). Cultural diversity in the morality of caring.
38, 299–337. Cross-Cultural Research, 28, 3–39.
Martin, L., Nelson, D., & Tobach, E. (1995). Sociocultural psychol- Miller, J. G. (1994b). Cultural psychology: Bridging disciplinary
ogy: Theory and practice of doing and knowing. New York: Cam- boundaries in understanding the cultural grounding of self. In
bridge University Press. P. K. Bock (Ed.), Handbook of psychological anthropology.
Martin-Barbero, J. (1993). Communication, culture and hegemony: Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.
From media to mediations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Miller, J. G. (1997a). Culture and self: Uncovering the cultural
Martini, M. (1994). Peer interactions in Polynesia: A view from the grounding of psychological theory. In J. G. Snodgrass & R. L.
Marquesas. In J. L. Roopmarine, J. E. Johnson, & F. H. Hooper Thompson (Eds.), The self across psychology: Self-recognition,
(Eds.), Children’s play in diverse cultures. Albany: State Univer- self-awareness, and the self concept (pp. 217–231). New York:
sity of New York Press. New York Academy of Sciences.
Marvin, R., VanDevender, T., Iwanaga, M., LeVine, S., & LeVine, R. Miller, J. G. (1997b). Theoretical issues in cultural psychology and
(1977). Infant-caregiver attachment among the Hausa of Nigeria. social constructionism. In J. W. Berry, Y. Poortinga, & J.
In H. McGurk (Ed.), Ecological factors in human development. Pandey (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Theoret-
Amsterdam: North-Holland. ical and methodological perspectives (Vol. 1). Boston: Allyn &
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper. Bacon.
Masten, A. S. (1999). Cultural processes of child development. Min- Miller, J. G. (2003). Culture and agency: Implications for psycho-
nesota Symposia on Child Development (29). logical theories of motivation and social development. In R. A.
McAdoo, H. P. (2002). African American parenting. In M. H. Born- Dienstbier (Series Ed.) & V. Murphy-Berman & J. J. Berman
stein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Social conditions and (Vol. Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 49. Cross-
applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 47–58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Cultural dif ference in perspectives on the self (pp. 59–99). Lin-
McCabe, A., & Peterson, A. (1991). Developing narrative structure. coln: University of Nebraska Press.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Miller, P. J. (1982). Amy, Wendy, and Beth: Learning language in
McKenna, J. J., Thoman, E. B., Anders, T. F., Sadeh, A., Schechtman, South Baltimore. Austin: University of Texas Press.
V. L., & Glotzbach, S. F. (1993). Infant-parent co-sleeping in an Miller, P. J. (1994). Narrative practices: Their role in socialization
evolutionary perspective: Implications for understanding infant and self construction. In U. Neisser & R. Fivush (Eds.), The re-
sleep development and the sudden infant death syndrome. Sleep, membering self: Construction and accuracy in the self-narrative
16, 263–282. (pp. 158–179). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Miller, P. J., Fung, H., & Mintz, J. (1996). Self-construction through
Chicago Press. narrative practices: A Chinese and American comparison of early
Mead, M. (1931). A handbook of child psychology (Carl Murchison, socialization. Ethos, 24, 237–279.
Ed.). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. Miller, P. J., & Goodnow, J. J. (1995). Cultural practices: Toward an
Medin, D. L. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American integration of development and culture. In J. J. Goodnow, P. J.
Psychologist, 89, 1969–1981. Miller, & F. Kessel (Eds.), Cultural practices as contexts for de-
Meng, K. (1992). Narrating and listening in kindergarten. Journal of velopment: New directions for child development (Vol. 67,
Narrative and Life History, 2(3), 235–276. pp. 5–16). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Menon, U. (1995). Receiving and giving: Distributivity as the source Miller, P. J., Hengst, J. A., & Wang, S.-H. (2003). Ethnographic meth-
of women’s wellbeing. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of ods: Applications from developmental cultural psychology. In P.
Chicago, IL. Camic, J. Rhodes, & L. Yardley (Eds.), Qualitative research in
Menon, U. (2002). Neither victim nor rebel: Feminism and the moral- psychology: Expanding perspectives in methodology and design.
ity of gender and family life in a Hindu temple town. In R. A. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Shweder, M. Minow, & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Engaging cultural Miller, P. J., & Hoogstra, L. (1992). Language as tool in the social-
dif ferences: The multicultural challenge in liberal democracies ization and apprehension of cultural meanings. In T. Schwartz,
(pp. 288–308). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. G. M. White, & G. A. White (Eds.), New directions in psycholog-
Menon, U., & Shweder, R. (1994). Kali’s tongue: Cultural psychology ical anthropology (pp. 83–101). New York: Cambridge Univer-
and the power of shame in Orissa, India. In S. Kitayama & H. R. sity Press.
Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual Miller, P. J., Hoogstra, L., Mintz, J., Fung, H., & Williams, K.
inf luence (pp. 241–284). Washington, DC: American Psychologi- (1993). Troubles in the garden and how they get resolved: A young
cal Association. child’s transformation of his favorite story. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.),
Mesquita, B. (2001). Culture and emotion: Different approaches to Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 26. Memory and
the question. In T. J. Mayne & G. A. Bonanno (Eds.), Emotions: af fect in development (pp. 87–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Current issues and future directions (pp. 214–250). New York: Miller, P. J., Mintz, J., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Potts, R. (1992).
Guilford Press. The narrated self: Young children’s construction of self in rela-
Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. H. (1992). Cultural variations in emo- tion to others in conversational stories of personal experience.
tions: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 179–204. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 45–67.
Michaels, S. (1991). The dismantling of narrative. In A. McCabe & Miller, P. J., & Moore, B. B. (1989). Narrative conjunctions of care-
C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 303–351). giver and child: A comparative perspective on socialization
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. through stories. Ethos, 17, 43–64.
788 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Mintz, J. (1990). Nelson, K. (Ed.). (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA:
Narrative practices and the social construction of self in child- Harvard University Press.
hood. American Ethnologist, 17(2), 292–311. Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence
Miller, P. J., Sandel, T., Liang, C.-H., & Fung, H. (2001). Narrating of the mediated mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.
transgressions in Longwood: The discourses, meanings, and para- Nicolopoulou, A. (1997). Children and narratives: Toward an interpre-
doxes of an American socializing practice. Ethos, 29(2), 1–27. tive and sociocultural approach. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative de-
Miller, P. J., & Sperry, L. L. (1987). The socialization of anger and velopment: Six approaches (pp. 179–215). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 1–31. Nisbett, R. (2003). The geography of thought. New York: Free Press.
Miller, P. J., Wiley, A., Fung, H., & Liang, C. (1997). Personal story-
Nisbett, R., & Cohen, D. (1995). The culture of honor: The psychol-
telling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and American
ogy of violence in the South. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
families. Child Development, 68, 557–568.
Nunes, T. (1995). Cultural practices and the conception of individual
Minsky, M. (2000). Attachments and goals. In G. Hatano,
differences: Theoretical and empirical considerations. In J. J.
N. Okada, & H. Tanabe (Eds.), Af fective minds (pp. 27–40).
Goodnow, P. J. Miller, & F. Kessel (Eds.), Cultural practices as
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
contexts for development (pp. 91–104). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Minsky, M. (2001). The emotion machine. New York: Pantheon.
Ochs, E. (1988). Culture and language development: Language acqui-
Montemayor, R., & Eisen, M. (1977). The development of self-
sition and language socialization in a Samoan village. Cambridge,
conceptions from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psy-
England: Cambridge University Press.
chology, 13, 314–319.
Ochs, E. (1990). Indexicality and socialization. In J. W. Stigler,
Morelli, G. A., Rogoff, B., Oppenheimer, D., & Goldsmith, D. (1992).
R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays
Cultural variations in infants’ sleeping arrangements: Question of
on comparative human development (pp. 287–308). New York:
independence. Developmental Psychology, 28, 604–613.
Cambridge University Press.
Morikawa, H., Shand, N., & Kosawa, Y. (1988). Maternal speech to
Ochs, E. (1999). Socialization. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology,
prelingual infants in Japan and the United States: Relationships
9(1/2), 230–233.
among functions, forms and referents. Journal of Child Language,
15, 237–256. Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (2001). Living narrative: Creating lives in
Morris, B. (1994). Anthropology of the self: The concept of the indi- everyday storytelling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
vidual in the west. Boulder, CO: Pluto Press. Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (1984). Language acquisition and social-
Much, N. C. (1992). The analysis of discourse as methodology for a ization: Three developmental stories. In R. Shweder & R. A.
semiotic psychology. American Behavioral Scientist, 36, 52–72. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Much, N. C. (1993). Personal psychology and cultural discourse:
Context analysis in the construction of meaning and a women’s Ochs, E., & Taylor, C. E. (1995). The father knows best dynamic in
devotional life in an Indian village. In J. Smith, R. Harre, & dinnertime narratives. In K. Hall & M. Bucholtz (Eds.), Gender
L. van Langenhove (Eds.), Rethinking psychology: Vol. 1. Concep- articulated: Language and the socially constructed self
tual foundations. London: Sage. (pp. 97–120). New York: Routledge.
Much, N. C. (1995). Cultural psychology. In J. Smith, R. Harre, & Oerter, R., Oerter, R., Agostiani, H., Kim, H., & Wibowo, S. (1996).
L. van Langenhove (Eds.), Rethinking psychology. London: Sage. The concept of human nature in East Asia: Etic and emic charac-
Mulder, N. (1992). Individual and society in Java: A cultural analysis teristics. Culture and Psychology, 2(1), 9–51.
(2nd ed.). Yogyakarta, Indonesia: Gajah Mada University Press. Oettingen, G. (1995). Cross-cultural perspectives on self-efficacy.
Mullen, M. K., & Yi, S. (1995). The cultural context of talk about the In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-ef ficacy in changing societies
past: Implications for the development of autobiographical mem- (pp. 149–176). New York: Cambridge University Press.
ory. Cognitive Development, 10, 407–419. Okami, P., & Weisner, T. (in press). Is parent-child bedsharing psy-
Munroe, R. H. (in press). Social structure and sex-role choices among chologically harmful? Data from the UCLA Family Lifestyles
children in four cultures. Cross-Cultural Research. Project. In J. McKenna (Ed.), Safe co-sleeping with baby: Scien-
Murphy-Berman, V., & Berman, J. J. (Eds.). (2003). Cultural differ- tific, medical and political perspectives.
ence in perspectives on the self. In R. A. Dienstbier (Series Ed.), Okami, P., Weisner, T. S., & Olmstead, R. (2002). Outcome corre-
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 49. Cross. Lincoln: Uni- lates of parent-child bedsharing: An 18-year longitudinal study.
versity of Nebraska Press. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 23(4),
Nag, M., White, B., & Peet, R. C. (1978). An anthropological ap- pp. 244–253.
proach to the study of the economic value of children. Current An- Oosterwegel, A., & Oppenheimer, L. (1993). The self-system: Devel-
thropology, 19, 293–306. opmental changes between and within self-concepts. Hillsdale,
Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psy- NJ: Erlbaum.
chology, 1, 35–59. Oura, Y., & Hatano, G. (2001). The constitution of general and specific
Neisser, U. (1991). Two perceptually given aspects of the self and mental models of other people. Human Development, 44, 144–159.
their development. Developmental Review, 11(3), 197–209. Oyserman, D., & Kemmelmeier, M. (1995, June). Viewing oneself in
Neisser, U., & Fivush, R. (1994). The remembering self: Construction light of others: Gendered impact of social comparisons in the
and accuracy in the self-narrative. New York: Cambridge Univer- achievement domain. Paper presented at the 7th annual convention
sity Press. of the American Psychological Society, New York.
Neisser, U., & Jopling, D. (Eds.). (1997). The conceptual self in con- Oyserman, D., & Markus, H. R. (1993). The sociocultural self. In
text: Culture, experience, self-understanding. Cambridge, En- J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 4,
gland: Cambridge University Press. pp. 187–220). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
References 789

Packer, M. J. (1987). Social interaction as practical activity: Implica- Rosaldo, M. Z. (1984). Toward an anthropology of self and feeling. In
tions for the study of social and moral development. In W. R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on
Kurtines & J. Gewirtz (Eds.), Moral development through social mind, self, and emotion (pp. 137–157). New York: Cambridge
interaction (pp. 245–280). New York: Wiley. University Press.
Parish, S. (1991). The sacred mind: Newar cultural representations of Rosen, L. (1995). Other intentions. Santa Fe, NM: School of Ameri-
mental life and the production of moral consciousness. Ethos, can Research.
19(3), 313–351. Rosenberg, M. (1986). Self-concept from middle child-hood through
Peak, L. (1987). Learning to go to school in Japan: The transition from adolescence. In J. Suls & A. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological per-
home to preschool life. Berkeley: University of California Press. spectives on the self (Vol. 3, pp. 107–136). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Peak, L. (2001). Learning to become part of the group: The Japanese Rosenberger, N. R. (1992). Japanese sense of self. New York: Cam-
child’s transition to preschool life. In H. Shimizu & R. A. bridge University Press.
LeVine (Eds.), Japanese frames of mind: Cultural perspectives on Ross, N., Medin, D., Coley, J. D., & Atran, S. (2003). Cultural and
human development (pp. 143–169). New York: Cambridge Uni- experiential differences in the development of folkbiological in-
versity Press. duction. Cognitive Development, 18, 25–47.
Peterson, C. C., & Siegal, M. (1995). Deafness, conversation and the- Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., & Azuma, H. (2000). The development of
ory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, close relationships in Japan and the United States: Paths of
459–474. symbolic harmony and generative tension. Child Development,
Phelan, P., Davidson, A. L., & Yu, H. C. (1998). Adolescents’ worlds: 71(5), 1121–1142.
Negotiating family, peers, and school. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J., & Pott, M. (2000). Attachment and culture:
Pike, K. L. (1967). Language in relation to a unified theory of the struc- Security in the United States and Japan. American Psychologist,
ture of human behavior. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton. 55(10), 1093–1104.

Polkinghorne, D. E. (1988). Narrative knowing and the human sci- Rozin, P., & Nemeroff, C. (1990). The law of sympathetic magic. In
ences. Albany: State University of New York Press. J. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology:
Essays on comparative human development. New York: Cambridge
Pratt, M. W., & Fiese, B. H. (2004). Family stories and the life course: University Press.
Across time and generations. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ruble, D. N., Alvarez, J., Bachman, M., Cameron, J., Fuligni, A.,
Preece, A. (1987). The range of narrative forms conversationally pro- Garcia Coll, C., et al. (in press). The development of a sense of
duced by young children. Journal of Child Language, 14, 353–373. “ we”: The emergence and implications of children’s collective
Redfield, R. (1941). The folk culture of Yucatan. Chicago: University identity. In M. Bennett & F. Sani (Eds.), The development of the
of Chicago Press. social self. East Sussex, England: Psychology Press.
Richman, A., LeVine, R. A., New, R. S., Howrigan, G., Welles- Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions.
Nystrom, B., & LeVine, S. (1988). Maternal behavior to infants Psychological Bulletin, 110, 426–450.
in five societies. In R. A. LeVine, P. Miller, & M. M. West Ryff, C., Lee, Y., & Na, K. (1995). Through the lens of culture: Psy-
(Eds.), Parental behavior in diverse societies: Vol. 40. New direc- chological well-being at midlife. Unpublished manuscript.
tions for child development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Sampson, E. E. (1985). The decentralization of identity: Toward a re-
Richman, A., Miller, P., & LeVine, R. A. (1992). Cultural and educa- vised concept of personal and social order. American Psycholo-
tional variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Psy- gist, 40, 1203–1211.
chology, 28, 614–621. Sampson, E. E. (1988). The debate on individualism: Indigenous psy-
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development chologies of the individual and their role in personal and societal
in social context. New York: Oxford University Press. functioning. American Psychologist, 43, 15–22.
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New Sandel, T. L. (2003). Linguistic capital in Taiwan: The KMT’s Man-
York: Oxford University Press. darin language policy and its perceived impact on language prac-
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J. J., Göncü, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided par- tices of bilingual Mandarin and Tai-gi speakers. Language in
ticipation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Mono- Society, 32, 523–551.
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(7, Sass, L. A. (1992). The epic of disbelief: The postmodernist turn in
Series No. 236). contemporary psychoanalysis. In S. Kvale (Ed.), Psychology and
Rogoff, B., Radziszewska, B., & Masiello, T. (1995). Analysis of postmodernism (pp. 166–182). London: Sage.
developmental processes in sociocultural activity. In L. M. Nel- Sawyer, R. (1997). Pretend play as improvisation: Conversation in the
son & E. Tobach (Eds.), Sociocultural psychology: Theory and preschool classroom. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
practice of doing and knowing. New York: Cambridge Univer- Schieffelin, B. B. (1990). The give and take of everyday life: Lan-
sity Press. guage socialization of Kaluli children. New York: Cambridge Uni-
Rohner, R. P. (1984). Handbook for the study of parental acceptance versity Press.
and rejection (Rev. ed.). Storrs: University of Connecticut. Schieffelin, B. B., & Ochs, E. (1986a). Language socialization. In
Rohner, R. P., & Pettengill, S. (1985). Perceived parental acceptance- B. Siegel (Ed.), Annual review of anthropology. Palo Alto, CA:
rejection and parental control among Korean adolescents. Child Annual Reviews.
Development, 56, 524–528. Schieffelin, B. B., & Ochs, E. (Eds.). (1986b). Language socialization
Romney, A. K., Weller, S. C., & Batchelder, W. H. (1986). Culture as across cultures. New York: Cambridge University Press.
consensus: A theory of culture and informant accuracy. American Schoneman, T. J. (1981). Reports of the sources of self-knowledge.
Anthropologist, 88, 313–332. Journal of Personality, 49, 284–294.
790 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

Schoultz, J., Säljö, R., & Wyndhamn, J. (2001). Heavenly talk: Dis- Shweder, R. A., & Haidt, J. (2000). The cultural psychology of the
course, artifacts, and children’s understanding of elementary as- emotions: Ancient and new. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.),
tronomy. Human Development, 44, 103–118. Handbook of Emotions. NY: Guildford Press.
Schwartz, T., White, G., & Lutz, C. (1992). New directions in psycho- Shweder, R. A., & LeVine, R. A. (1984). Culture theory: Essays on
logical anthropology. New York: Cambridge University Press. mind, self, and emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Schwartzman, H. (1978). Tranformations: The anthropology of chil- Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1990). Culture and
dren’s play. New York: Plenum Press. moral development. In J. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt
Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1973). The cognitive consequences of for- (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human devel-
mal and informal education. Science, 182, 553–559. opment. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cam- Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. big three of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the big
three explanations of suffering. In A. Brandt & P. Rozin (Eds.),
Seymour, S. (1999). Women, family and child care in India: A world in
Morality and health. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
transition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Shweder, R. A., & Sullivan, M. (1990). The semiotic subject of cul-
Shimizu, H., & LeVine, R. (2001). Japanese frames of mind: Cultural tural psychology. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality:
perspectives and human development. New York: Cambridge Uni- Theory and research (pp. 399–416). New York: Guilford Press.
versity Press.
Shweder, R. A., & Sullivan, M. (1993). Cultural psychology: Who
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture and the problem needs it? Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 497–523.
of meaning. New York: Oxford University Press.
Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1994). Constraints on learning in non-
Shuman, A. (1986). Storytelling rights: The uses of oral and written privileged domains. Cognitive Psychology, 27, 194–226.
texts by urban adolescents. New York: Cambridge University Press. Snibbe, A. C., & Markus, H. R. (2004). You can’t always get what you
Shwalb, D., & Shwalb, B. J. (1996). Japanese childrearing: Two gener- want: Social class, agency and choice. Unpublished manuscript.
ations of scholarship. New York: Guilford. Spence, D. P. (1982). Narrative truth and historical truth: Meaning
Shweder, R. A. (1982). Beyond self-constructed knowledge: The study and interpretation in psychoanalysis. New York: Norton.
of culture and morality. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 41–69. Sperber, D. (2002). In defense of massive modularity. In E. Dupoux
Shweder, R. A. (1990a). Cultural psychology: What is it. In J. W. (Ed.), Language, brain, and cognitive development (pp. 47–57).
Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Essays on comparative human development. Cambridge, England: Sperry, L. L., & Sperry, D. E. (1995). Young children’s presentation
Cambridge University Press. of self in conversational narration. In L. L. Sperry & P. A. Smi-
Shweder, R. A. (1990b). In defense of moral realism. Child Develop- ley (Eds.), Exploring young children’s concepts of self and other
ment, 61, 2060–2067. through conversation: Vol. 69. New directions for child develop-
Shweder, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cul- ment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
tural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Sperry, L. L., & Sperry, D. E. (1996). The early development of nar-
Shweder, R. A. (1992). Ghost busters in anthropology. In C. Strauss rative skills. Cognitive Development, 11, 443–465.
& R. G. D’Andrade (Eds.), Human motives and cultural models. Stavy, R., & Wax, N. (1989). Children’s conceptions of plants as liv-
New York: Cambridge University Press. ing things. Human Development, 32, 88–94.
Shweder, R. A. (1996a). “ The ‘mind’ of cultural psychology.” In Steedman, C. K. (1986). Landscape for a good woman: A story of two
P. Baltes & U. Staudinger (Eds.), Interactive minds: Life-span per- lives. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
spectives on the social foundations of cognition. New York: Cam- Stein, K., Markus, H., & Moeser, R. (1996). The sociocultural shap-
bridge University Press. ing of self-esteem of American adolescent girls and boys.
Shweder, R. A. (1996b). True ethnography: The lore, the law and the Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York:
lure. In R. Jessor, A. Colby, & R. A. Shweder (Eds.), Ethnography Basic Books.
and human development: Context and meaning in social inquiry. Stevenson, H. W. (1994). Moving away from stereotypes and precon-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ceptions: Students and their education in East Asia and the
Shweder, R. A. (2003a). Toward a deep cultural psychology of shame. United States. In P. M. G. R. R. Cocking (Ed.), Cross-cultural
Social Research, 70, 1401–1422. roots of minority child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Shweder, R. A. (2003b). Why do men barbecue? Recipes for cultural Stevenson, H. W., & Stigler, J. W. (1992). The learning gap: Why our
psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. schools are failing and what we can learn from Japanese and Chi-
Shweder, R. A. (2004). Deconstructing the emotions for the sake of nese education. New York: Summit Books.
comparative research. In A. S. R. Manstead, N. Frijda, & A. Fis- Stigler, J. W., Shweder, R. A., & Herdt, G. (Eds.). (1990). Cultural
cher (Eds.), Amsterdam Symposium: Feelings and emotions. Cam- psychology: Essays on comparative human development
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. (pp. 259–286). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Shweder, R. A., Balle-Jensen, L., & Goldstein, W. (1995). Who sleeps Stigler, J. W., Smith, S., & Mao, L. (1985). The self-perception of com-
by whom revisited: A method for extracting the moral goods im- petence by Chinese children. Child Development, 56, 1259–1270.
plicit in praxis. In J. J. Goodnow, P. J. Miller, & F. Kessell (Eds.), Strickland, B. R. (2000). Misassumptions, misadventures, and the
Cultural practices as contexts for development: New directions for misuse of psychology. American Psychologist, 55, 331–338.
child development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Stromberg, P. G. (1993). Language and self-transformation: A study
Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, L. (1984). Does the concept of the person of the Christian conversion narrative. Cambridge: Cambridge
vary cross-culturally. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), University Press.
Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp. 158–199). Sullivan, H. S. (1940). Conceptions of modern psychiatry. New
New York: Cambridge University Press. York: Wiley.
References 791

Super, C., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A con- Tronick, E. Z., Morelli, G., & Winn, S. (1987). Multiple caretaking
ceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International of Efe (Pygmy) infants. American Anthropologist, 89, 96–106.
Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 545–569. Tsai, J. L., Simenova, D., & Watanabe, J. (1999). Somatic and social:
Super, C., & Harkness, S. (1997). The cultural structuring of child Chinese Americans talk about emotion—Personality and Social
development. In J. Berry, P. R. Dasen, & T. S. Saraswathi (Eds.), Psychology Bulletin. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), The cultural origins
Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 3–29). Boston: of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Allyn & Bacon. Tu, W. (1994). Embodying the universe: A note on Confucian self-
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge, realization. In R. T. Ames, W. Dissanayake, & T. P. Kasulis
England: Cambridge University Press. (Eds.), Self as person in Asian theory and practice (pp. 177–186).
Takahashi, K., & Tokoro, M. (2002). Senior shutterbugs: Successful Albany: State University of New York Press.
aging through participation in social activities. International Valsiner, J. (1988). Child development within culturally structured en-
Society for Study of Behavioural Development Newsletter(Serial vironments: Social co-construction and environmental guidance in
No. 41, 9–11). development (Vol. 2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Taylor, C. (1989). Sources of the self: The making of modern identi- Valsiner, J. (1994). Co-construction: What is (and what is not) in a
ties. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. name. In P. van Geert & L. Mos (Eds.), Annals of theoretical psy-
Taylor, C. E. (1995). “You think it was a fight?”: Co-constructing (the chology (Vol. 10, pp. 343–368). New York: Plenum Press.
struggle for) meaning, face, and family in everyday narrative activ- Van der Meulen, M. (1986). Zelfconceptieproblemen bij jonge
ity. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 28(3), 283–317. kinderen: Een analyse van spontane, zelf beschouwende uit-
spraken. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie en haar
Taylor, M., & Carlson, S. M. (2000). The inf luence of religious beliefs
Grensgebieden, 41(6), 261–267.
on parental attitude about children’s fantasy behavior. In K. S.
Rosengren, C. N. Johnson, & P. L. Harris (Eds.), Imagining the im- Van Deusen-Phillips, S. B., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Miller, P. J.
possible (pp. 247–268). New York: Cambridge University Press. (2001). Enacting stories, seeing worlds: Similarities and differ-
ences in the cross-cultural narrative development of linguistically
Tetlock, P. E., Kristel, O. V., Elson, S. E., Green, M. C., & Lerner,
isolated deaf children. Human Development, 44, 311–336.
J. S. (2000). The psychology of the unthinkable: Taboo trade-offs,
forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. Journal of Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1993). The embodied
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 853–870. mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Tetlock, P. E., McGraw, A. P., & Kristel, O. V. (2004). Proscribed
forms of social cognition: Taboo tradeoffs, blocked exchanges, Vinden, P. G. (1996). Junin Quechua children’s understanding of
forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. In N. Haslam mind. Child Development, 67, 1701–1716.
(Ed.), Relational models theory: A contemporary overview Vinden, P. G., & Astington, J. W. (2000). Culture and understanding
(pp. 247–262). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. other minds. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J.
Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from de-
Thoits, P. A., & Virshup, L. K. (1997). Me’s and we’s: Forms and
velopmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 504–519). Oxford, En-
functions of social identities. In R. D. Ashmore & L. J. Jussim
gland: Oxford University Press.
(Eds.), Rutger Series on self and social identity: Vol.1. Self and
identity—Fundamental issues (pp. 106–133). New York: Oxford Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L . S. Vygotsky: Vol. 1.
University Press. Problems of general psychology. New York: Plenum Press. (Origi-
nal work published 1934)
Thomas, D. (1978). Cooperation and competition among children in
the Pacific Islands and New Zealand: The school as an agent of Walkerdine, V. (1988). The mastery of reason. London: Routledge &
social change. Journal of Research and Development in Educa- Kegan Paul.
tion, 12, 88–95. Wang, Q. (2001a). Culture effects on adults’ earliest childhood recol-
Tobin, J. (2000). Good guys don’t wear hats. New York: Teachers Col- lection and self-description: Implications for the relation between
lege Press. memory and the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 81(2), 220–233.
Tobin, J. J., Wu, D. Y. H., & Davidson, D. H. (1989). Preschool in
Wang, Q. (2001b). “Did you have fun?”: American and Chinese
three cultures: Japan, China, and the United States. New Haven,
mother-child conversations about shared emotional experiences.
CT: Yale University Press.
Cognitive Development, 16, 693–715.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cam-
Wang, Q., & Leichtman, M. D. (2000). Same beginnings, different
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
stories: A comparison of American and Chinese children’s narra-
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learn- tives. Child Development, 71(5), 1329–1346.
ing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.
Wang, Q., Leichtman, M. D., & Davies, K. I. (2000). Sharing memo-
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cul- ries and telling stories: American and Chinese mothers and their
tural contexts. Psychological Review, 93(3), 506–520. 3-year-olds. Memory, 8(3), 159–178.
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and Watson-Gegeo, K. A. (1993). Thick explanation in the ethnographic
collectivism. In J. Berman (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motiva- study of child socialization: A longitudinal study of the problem
tion (pp. 41–133). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. of schooling for Kwara’ae (Solomon Island) children. In W. A.
Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., & Villareal, M. (1988). Individu- Corsaro & P. J. Miller (Eds.), Interpretive approaches to childhood
alism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives on self- socialization (pp. 51–66). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
ingroup relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Watson-Gegeo, K. A., & Gegeo, D. W. (1990). Shaping the mind and
Psychology, 54, 323–338. straightening out conf licts: The discourse of Kwara’ae family
Tronick, E. Z., Morelli, G., & Ivey, P. K. (1992). The Efe forager in- counseling. In K. A. Watson-Gegeo & G. M. White (Eds.), Disen-
fant and toddler’s pattern of social relationships: Multiple and si- tangling: Conf lict discourse in Pacific societies (pp. 161–213).
multaneous. Developmental Psychology, 28, 568–577. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
792 The Cultural Psychology of Development: One Mind, Many Mentalities

Watson-Gegeo, K., & Gegeo, D. W. (1999). Remodeling culture in Wertsch, J. V. (1992). Keys to cultural psychology. Culture, Medicine
Kwara’ae: The role of discourse in children’s cognitive develop- and Psychiatry, 16(3), 273–280.
ment. Discourse Studies, 1(2), 227–245. White, G. M., & Kirkpatrick, J. (1985). Person, self and experience:
Weigert, A. J., Teitge, J. S., & Teitge, D. W. (1990). Society and iden- Exploring Pacific ethnopsychologies. Berkeley: University of Cal-
tity: Towards a sociological psychology. New York: Cambridge ifornia Press.
University Press. White, M. I., & LeVine, R. A. (1986). What is an li ko (good child)?
Weisner, T. S. (1982). Sibling interdependence and sibling caretak- In H. Stevenson, H. Azuma, & K. Hakuta (Eds.), Child develop-
ing: A cross-cultural view. In M. Lamb & B. Sutton-Smith (Eds.), ment and education in Japan (pp. 55–62). New York: Freeman.
Sibling relationships: Their nature and significance across the Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of dif ferent worlds.
lifespan. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Weisner, T. S. (1984). A cross-cultural perspective: Ecological niches Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. M. (1975). Children of six cultures.
of middle childhood. In A. Collins (Ed.), The elementary school Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
years: Understanding development during middle childhood Whiting, J. W. M. (1941). Becoming a Kwoma. New Haven, CT: Yale
(pp. 335–369). Washington, DC: National Academy. University Press.
Weisner, T. S. (1987). Socialization for parenthood in sibling care- Whiting, J. W. M. (1964). Effects of climate on certain cultural prac-
taking societies. In J. Lancaster, A. Rossi, & J. Altmann (Eds.), tices. In W. H. Goodenough (Ed.), Explorations in cultural an-
Parenting across the lifespan. New York: Aldine. thropology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Weisner, T. S. (1989a). Comparing sibling relationships across cul- Whiting, J. W. M. (1981). Environmental constraints on infant care
tures. In P. Zukow (Ed.), Sibling interaction across cultures. New practices. In R. H. Munroe, R. L. Munroe, & B. B. Whiting
York: Springer-Verlag. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural human development. New
Weisner, T. S. (1989b). Cultural and universal aspects of social sup- York: Garland.
port for children: Evidence from the Alaluyia of Kenya. In D. Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. (1953). Child training and personality.
Belle (Ed.), Children’s social networks and social supports. New New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
York: Wiley. Wierzbicka, A. (1986). Human emotions: Universal or culture-spe-
Weisner, T. S. (1996a). The 5–7 transition as an ecocultural project. cific? American Anthropologist, 88(3), 584–594.
In A. Sameroff & M. Haith (Eds.), Reason and responsibility: The Wierzbicka, A. (1990). Semantics of the Emotions [Special issue].
passage through childhood. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 10(2).
Weisner, T. S. (1996b). Why ethnography should be the most impor- Wierzbicka, A. (1991). Cross-cultural pragmatics: The semantics of
tant method in the study of human development. In R. Jesser, A. human interaction. Berlin, Germany: W. de Gruyter.
Colby, & R. A. Shweder (Eds.), Ethnography and human develop- Wierzbicka, A. (1992). Semantics, culture and cognition: Universal
ment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. human concepts in culture-specific configurations. New York: Ox-
Weisner, T. S. (2001). Anthropological aspects of childhood. In Inter- ford University Press.
national encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (Vol. Wierzbicka, A. (1993). A conceptual basis for cultural psychology.
3, pp. 1697–1701). Oxford, England: Elsevier. Ethos, 21(2), 205–231.
Weisner, T. S. (2002). Ecocultural understanding of children’s devel- Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures: Di-
opmental pathways. Human Development, 45, 275–281. versity and universals. Paris: Cambridge University Press.
Weisner, T. S., Bausano, M., & Kornfein, M. (1983). Putting family Wiley, A. R., Rose, A. J., Burger, L. K., & Miller, P. J. (1998). The
ideals into practice. Ethos, 11, 278–304. construction of autonomy through narrative practices: A compar-
Weisner, T. S., & Gallimore, R. (1977). My brother’s keeper: Child ative study. Child Development, 69, 833–847.
and sibling caretaking. Current Anthropology, 18, 169–180. Willis, P. (1977). Learning to labor: How working class kids get work-
Weisner, T. S., & Lowe, E. D. (in press). Globalization, childhood, ing class jobs. New York: Columbia University Press.
and psychological anthropology. In C. Casey & R. Edgerton Wolf, S. A., & Heath, S. B. (1992). The braid of literature: Chil-
(Eds.), A companion to psychological anthropology: Modernity dren’s worlds of reading. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
and psychocultural change. Oxford, England: Blackwell. Press.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of Wortham, S. (2001). Narratives in action: A strategy for research and
theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child analysis. New York: Teachers College Press.
Development, 72, 655–684. Wu, D. Y. H. (1994). Self and collectivity: Socialization in Chinese
Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1998). Knowledge acquisition in preschools. In R. T. Ames, W. Dissanayake, & T. P. Kasulis
foundational domains. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & D. Kuhn (Eds.), Self as person in Asian theory and practice. Albany: State
& R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. University of New York Press.
Cognition, perception and language (5th ed., pp. 523–573). New Wu, D. Y. H. (1996). Chinese childhood socialization. In M. H. Bond
York: Wiley. (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology (pp. 143–154). Hong
Wenger, M. (1989). Work, play and social relationships among chil- Kong, China: Oxford University Press.
dren in a Giriama community. In D. Belle (Ed.), Children’s social Yang, K.-S. (1997). Indiginizing westernized Chinese psychology. In
networks and social supports. New York: Wiley. M. H. Bond (Ed.), Working at the interface of culture: 18 lives in
Werker, J. (1989). Becoming a native listener. American Scien- social science. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
tist, 77, 54 –59. Yip, T., & Fuligni, A. J. (in press). Daily variation in ethnic identity,
Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. ethnic behaviors, and psychological well being among American
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. adolescents of Chinese descent. Child Development.
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Youniss, J., & Yates, M. (1997). Community service and social re-
University Press. sponsibility in youth. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
CHAPTER 14

The Bioecological Model of


Human Development
URIE BRONFENBRENNER and PAMELA A. MORRIS

OVERVIEW 795 PROXIMAL PROCESSES IN SOLO ACTIVITIES


DEFINING PROPERTIES OF THE WITH OBJECTS AND SYMBOLS 814
BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL 796 THE MICROSYSTEM MAGNIFIED: ACTIVITIES,
Proposition I 797 RELATIONSHIPS, AND ROLES 814
Proposition II 798 Effects of the Physical Environment on
FROM THEORY TO RESEARCH DESIGN: Psychological Development 814
OPERATIONALIZING THE The Mother-Infant Dyad as a Context of Development 815
BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL 799 BEYOND THE MICROSYSTEM 817
Developmental Science in the Discovery Mode 801 The Expanding Ecological Universe 818
Different Paths to Different Outcomes: Dysfunction Nature-Nurture Reconceptualized:
versus Competence 803 A Bioecological Interpretation 819
The Role of Experiments in the Bioecological Model 808 TIME IN THE BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL:
HOW DO PERSON CHARACTERISTICS INFLUENCE MICRO-, MESO-, AND
LATER DEVELOPMENT? 810 MACROCHRONOLOGICAL SYSTEMS 820
Force Characteristics as Shapers of Development 810 FROM RESEARCH TO REALITY 822
Resource Characteristics of the Person as Shapers THE BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL: A
of Development 812 DEVELOPMENTAL ASSESSMENT 824
Demand Characteristics of the Person as REFERENCES 825
Developmental Inf luences 812
THE ROLE OF FOCUS OF ATTENTION IN
PROXIMAL PROCESSES 813

The bioecological model, together with its correspon- sive generations, and through historical time, both past
ding research designs, is an evolving theoretical system and future. The term future raises a question: How is it
for the scientific study of human development over time possible to scientifically investigate phenomena that
(Bronfenbrenner, 2005). In the bioecological model, de- have not yet taken place? This question is hardly new;
velopment is defined as the phenomenon of continuity indeed, it pervades every field of scientific endeavor.
and change in the biopsychological characteristics of However, we are the only species that, over historical
human beings, both as individuals and as groups. The time, has developed the capacity to engage successfully
phenomenon extends over the life course, across succes- in scientific inquiry, and thereby, in many respects, has

We are especially grateful for the thoughtful criticisms of seminar, carefully reviewed a draft of this chapter, and made
earlier drafts of the manuscript generously provided by the many constructive suggestions. We have done our best to
following colleagues: Jay Belsky, Rick Canfield, Nancy Dar- meet the high standards that they commendably set. We wish
ling, Glen H. Elder Jr., Steven F. Hamilton, Melvin L. Kohn, to express gratitude to Richard M. Lerner and William
Kurt Lüscher, Phyllis Moen, Donna Dempster-McLain, Lau- Damon, the editors of the 1998 Volume and of that series as a
rence Steinberg, and Sheldon H. White. We owe particular whole, for their wise advice, encouragement, and patience.
thanks to Professor Susan Crockenberg and her students at Finally, a special thanks goes to our most severe and most
the University of Vermont who, in the course of a graduate constructive critic, Liese Bronfenbrenner.

793
794 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

been able to change the nature of the world in which we combined, along with new elements, into a more com-
live. As a result, within certain limits, we humans have plex and more dynamic structure.
altered the nature and course of our own development as The transition in the form and content of the model
a species (Bronfenbrenner & Evans 2000; Bronfenbren- actually took place over an extended period of time, an
ner & Morris 1998). expression that will become all too familiar to the reader
To place bioecological theory of human development (Bronfenbrenner, 2005). The transition from a focus on
into a larger context, it is important to recognize that the environment to a focus on processes was first intro-
many of the general perspectives advanced and elabo- duced in the context of Bronfenbrenner’s unpublished
rated in this theory are also parts of other related lines lectures, colloquium presentations, and contributions
of theoretical and empirical inquiry into human devel- to symposia. Not until 1986, did reference to an emer-
opment. Examples include life-span psychology (Baltes, gent new model first appear in print (Bronfenbrenner,
Lindenberger, & Staudinger, Chapter 11, this Handbook, 1986b). The following extended excerpt conveys both its
this volume), cultural psychology (Cole, 1995; Shweder spirit and intended substance. Because both of these at-
et al., Chapter 13, this Handbook, this volume), Magnus- tributes are relevant to the gradual evolution of the
son’s developmental theory of contextual-interactive model to its present form, we quote from the 1986 state-
holism (Magnusson & Stattin, Chapter 8, this Hand- ment at some length:
book, this volume), and, especially, the work of Robert
Cairns (Chapter 3, this Handbook, this volume), who
It is now more than a decade ago that, being somewhat
through communications and publications extending younger, I presumed to challenge the then-prevailing con-
over 3 decades, has played a major role in the evolution ventions of our field by describing the developmental re-
of the four defining properties of the bioecological search of the day as “ the study of the strange behavior of
model: (1) Process, (2) Person, (3) Context, and (4) children in strange situations for the briefest possible pe-
Time. Cairns is best known as the founder and principal riod of time” (Bronfenbrenner, 1974). Instead, I argued (as
protagonist of developmental science, and there are sev- if it were simply a matter of choice), we should be studying
eral excellent examples of his books and articles that development in its ecological context; that is, in the actual
have been most relevant to the evolution of the bioeco- environments in which human beings lived their lives. I
logical model (Bergman, Cairns, Nilsson, & Nysted, then proceeded to outline, in a series of publications, a con-
2000; Cairns, 1970; Cairns & Cairns, 1994). The spe- ceptual framework for analyzing development in context,
and to offer concrete examples of how various elements of
cific profile of the bioecological model of human devel-
the schema might be applied both to past studies and to
opment is its interdisciplinary and integrative focus on
studies yet-to-come. I also emphasized the scientific and
the age periods of childhood and adolescence and its ex- practical benefits of a closer linkage, in both directions, be-
plicit interest in applications to policies and programs tween developmental research and public policy (Bronfen-
pertinent to enhancing youth and family development. brenner, 1974, 1975, 1977a, 1977b, 1979a, 1979b, 1981).
In this chapter, we undertake to present the ecologi- Now, a dozen years later, one might think that I have good
cal model of human development that has been intro- reason to rest content. Studies of children and adults in
duced over the course of the prior two editions of this real-life settings, with real-life implications, are now com-
Handbook (Bronfenbrenner & Crouter, 1983; Bronfen- monplace in the research literature on human development,
brenner & Morris, 1998). The main focus of the 1983 both in the United States and, as this volume testifies, in
chapter was on the empirical and theoretical roots of a Europe as well. This scientific development is taking place,
model already in use, which centered on the role of the I believe, not so much because of my writings, but rather
because the notions I have been promulgating are ideas
environment in shaping development. In contrast, this
whose time has come. . . .
chapter is oriented toward the future. The present model
Clearly, if one regards such scientific developments as
introduces major theoretical innovations from the 1983 desirable, there are grounds for satisfaction. Yet, along
chapter in both form and content. The present formula- with feelings of gratification, I must confess to some dis-
tion makes no claim as a paradigm shift (if there be such content. My disquiet derives from two complementary
a phenomenon); rather, it continues a marked shift in the concerns. The first pertains to one of the main roads that
center of gravity of the model, in which features of ear- contemporary research has taken; the second, to some
lier versions are first called into question but then re- more promising pathways that are being neglected.
Overview 795

Alas, I may have to accept some responsibility for what reader to the present formulation of the biological
I regard as the wayward course. It is an instance of what model, a preview follows.
might be called “ the failure of success.” For some years, I
harangued my colleagues for avoiding the study of devel-
opment in real-life settings. No longer able to complain on OVERVIEW
that score, I have found a new bête noir. In place of too
much research on development “out of context,” we now
We begin with an exposition of the defining properties
have a surfeit of studies on “context without development.”
of the model, which involves four principal components
One cannot presume to make so brass an allegation
without being prepared to document one’s case. I am pre- and the dynamic, interactive relationships among them.
pared. (Bronfenbrenner 1986a, pp. 286–288) The first of these, which constitutes the core of the
model, is Process. More specifically, this construct en-
What followed was an early version of the newly evolv- compasses particular forms of interaction between or-
ing theoretical framework, but the purpose of the pres- ganism and environment, called proximal processes, that
ent chapter is better served by presenting the model in operate over time and are posited as the primary mech-
its current, albeit still-evolving, form now called the anisms producing human development. However, the
bioecological model. The term evolving highlights that power of such processes to influence development is
the model, along with its corresponding research de- presumed, and shown, to vary substantially as a func-
signs, has undergone a process of development during its tion of the characteristics of the developing Person, of
life course (Bronfenbrenner, 2005). The bioecological the immediate and more remote environmental Contexts,
model addresses two closely related but fundamentally and the Time periods, in which the proximal processes
different developmental processes, each taking place take place.
over time. The first process defines the phenomenon The sections that follow examine in greater detail each
under investigation—continuity and change in the of the three remaining defining properties of the model,
biopsychological characteristics of human beings. The beginning with the biopsychological characteristics of
second focuses on the development of the scientific the Person. This domain was given sequential priority to
tools—theoretical models and corresponding research fill a recognized gap in earlier prototypes of the ecologi-
designs required for assessing continuity and change. cal model. Thus, at midstage in the development of the
These two tasks cannot be carried out independently, present model, Bronfenbrenner criticized its theoretical
for they are the joint product of emerging and converg- predecessors and acknowledged his share of responsibil-
ing ideas, based on both theoretical and empirical ity for failing to deliver on an empirical promise:
grounds—a process called developmental science in the
Existing developmental studies subscribing to an ecologi-
discovery mode (Bronfenbrenner & Evans 2000,
cal model have provided far more knowledge about the na-
pp. 999–1000). In the more familiar verification mode,
ture of developmentally relevant environments, near
the aim is to replicate previous findings in other settings and far, than about the characteristics of developing indi-
to make sure that the findings still apply. By contrast, in viduals, then and now. . . . The criticism I just made also
the discovery mode, the aim is to fulfill two broader but applies to my own writings. . . . Nowhere in the 1979
interrelated objectives: monograph, nor elsewhere until today, does one find a
parallel set of structures for conceptualizing the charac-
1. Devising new alternative hypotheses and correspon- teristics of the developing person. (Bronfenbrenner,
ding research designs that not only question existing 1989a, p. 188)
results but also yield new, more differentiated, more
precise, replicable research findings and, thereby, Three types of Person characteristics are distin-
produce more valid scientific knowledge. guished as most influential in shaping the course of
2. Providing scientific bases for the design of effective future development through their capacity to affect
social policies and programs that counteract newly the direction and power of proximal processes through
emerging developmentally disruptive influences. the life course. First, dispositions can set proximal
This has been an explicit objective of the bioecologi- processes in motion in a particular developmental
cal model from its earliest beginnings. To orient the domain and continue to sustain their operation. Next,
796 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

bioecological resources of ability, experience, knowl- erations, as they affect and are affected by, processes
edge, and skill are required for the effective function- and outcomes of human development over the life
ing of proximal processes at a given stage of course. The treatment of this last topic draws on Elder
development. Finally, demand characteristics invite or and Shanahan, Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume.
discourage reactions from the social environment Our primary emphasis, however, is on the role of devel-
that can foster or disrupt the operation of proximal opmental processes and outcomes in producing large-
processes. The differentiation of these three forms scale changes over time in the state and structure of the
leads to their combination in patterns of Person struc- broader society over time, and the implications of those
ture that can further account for differences in the di- changes for the society’s future.
rection and power of resultant proximal processes and Before turning to the task at hand, it is important to
their developmental effects. make explicit three overarching orientations that define
These new formulations of qualities of the person the content and the structure of the chapter as a whole.
that shape his or her future development have had the First, we use the term development to refer to stability
unanticipated effect of further differentiating, expand- and change in the biopsychological characteristics of
ing, and integrating the original 1979 conceptualiza- human beings over the life course and across genera-
tion of the environment in terms of nested systems tions. There are no restrictive assumptions of change
ranging from micro to macro (Bronfenbrenner, 1979b). for the better or of continuity in the characteristics of
For example, the three types of Person characteristics the same person over time. Rather, these are issues to
previously outlined are also incorporated into the defi- be investigated.
nition of the microsystem as characteristics of parents, Second, from the perspective of the bioecological
relatives, close friends, teachers, mentors, coworkers, model, the forces producing stability and change in the
spouses, or others who participate in the life of the de- characteristics of human beings across successive gen-
veloping person on a fairly regular basis over extended erations are no less important than stability and change
periods of time. in the characteristics of the same person over his or
The bioecological model also introduces an even her lifetime.
more consequential domain into the structure of the mi- The third orientation is perhaps the most essential,
crosystem that emphasizes the distinctive contribution and the most difficult to achieve. It was Kurt Lewin
to development of proximal processes involving inter- (cited in Marrow, 1977) who said that there is nothing so
action not with people but with objects and symbols. practical as a good theory. But to be “good,” a theory
Even more broadly, concepts and criteria are introduced must also be “practical.” In science, a good theory is one
that differentiate between those features of the environ- that can be translated into corresponding research de-
ment that foster versus interfere with the development of signs that match the defining properties of the theory. In
proximal processes. Particularly significant in the latter the absence of such research designs—or worse yet, in
sphere is the growing hecticness, instability, and chaos the application of research designs that fail to match or
in the principal settings in which human competence even violate the defining properties of the theory—sci-
and character are shaped—in the family, child-care ence cannot move forward. Hence, we have sought, as we
arrangements, schools, peer groups, and neighborhoods. proceed through successive stages of theoretical formula-
The latter theme speaks to the fourth and final defin- tion, to specify, and, wherever possible, to illustrate the
ing property of the bioecological model and the one that properties of a research design that corresponds with, or
moves it farthest beyond its predecessor—the dimension at least approximates, the proposed theoretical structure.
of Time. The 1979 Volume scarcely mentions the term,
whereas in the current formulation, it has a prominent
place at three successive levels: (1) micro-, (2) meso-, DEFINING PROPERTIES OF THE
and (3) macro-. Microtime refers to continuity versus BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL
discontinuity in ongoing episodes of proximal process.
Mesotime is the periodicity of theses episodes across An early critical element in the definition of the bioeco-
broader time intervals, such as days and weeks. Finally, logical model is experience, which indicates that the
Macrotime focuses on the changing expectations and scientifically relevant features of an environment for
events in the larger society, both within and across gen- human development not only include its objective prop-
Defining Properties of the Bioecological Model 797

erties but also the way in which the properties are sub- cause the two sets of forces are interdependent and af-
jectively experienced by the person living in that envi- fect each other. Like their subjective counterparts, these
ronment. This equal emphasis on an experiential as well more objective factors also rely on their assessment of
as an objective view springs neither from an antipathy to corresponding theoretical models and associated re-
behaviorist concept nor from a predilection for existen- search designs, which evolved over time. These more
tial philosophic foundations but is dictated simply by the objective relationships are documented propositions
fact that very few of the external influences signifi- presented later (see too Bronfenbrenner & Evans 2000;
cantly affecting human behavior and development can Bronfenbrenner & Morris 1998). The first proposition
be described solely in objective physical conditions and specifies the theoretical model, and provides concrete
events (Bronfenbrenner & Evans 2000; Bronfenbrenner examples; the second foreshadows a corresponding re-
& Morris 1998). search design for their assessment.
Critical to the foregoing formulation is the word However, before proceeding with formal definitions,
solely. In the bioecological model, both objective and it may be useful to point out that traditionally such phe-
subjective elements are posited as driving the course of nomena as parent-child interaction—or, more generally,
human development; neither alone is presumed suffi- the behavior of others toward the developing person—
cient. Moreover, these elements do not always operate in have been treated under the more inclusive category of
the same direction. It is therefore important to under- the environment. In the bioecological model, a critical
stand the nature of each of these two dynamic forces, distinction is made between the concepts of environ-
beginning on the phenomenological or experiential side. ment and process, with the latter not only occupying a
Both of the terms are relevant because, while related to central position, but also having a meaning that is quite
each other, they are typically applied to somewhat dif- specific. The construct appears in Proposition I stipulat-
ferent spheres. Experiential is more often used in rela- ing the defining properties of the model. To place its
tion to cognitive development and pertains mainly to meaning in context, we cite Proposition II as well.
changes in how the environment is perceived at succes-
sive stages of the life course, beginning in early infancy
and proceeding through childhood, adolescence, adult- Proposition I
hood, and, ultimately, old age.
By contrast, experience pertains more to the realm of Especially in its early phases, but also throughout the life
feelings—anticipations, forebodings, hopes, doubts, or course, human development takes place through processes
personal beliefs. Feelings, emerging in early childhood of progressively more complex reciprocal interaction be-
and continuing through life, are characterized by both tween an active, evolving biopsychological human organism
stability and change: They can relate to self or to others, and the persons, objects, and symbols in its immediate ex-
especially to family, friends, and other close associates. ternal environment. To be effective, the interaction must
They can also apply to the activities in which we engage; occur on a fairly regular basis over extended periods of
time. Such enduring forms of interaction in the immediate
for example, those that we most or least like to do. But the
environment are referred to as proximal processes. Exam-
most distinctive feature of such experiential equalities is
ples of enduring patterns of proximal process are found in
that they are emotionally and motivationally loaded, en- feeding or comforting a baby, playing with a young child,
compassing both love and hate, joy and sorrow, curiosity child-child activities, group or solitary play, reading, learn-
and boredom, desire and revulsion, often with both polar- ing new skills, athletic activities, problem solving, caring
ities existing at the same time but usually in differing de- for others in distress, making plans, performing complex
grees. A significant body of research evidence indicates tasks, and acquiring new knowledge and know-how.
that such positive and negative subjective forces, evolving
in the past, can also contribute in powerful ways to shap- For the younger generation, participation in such in-
ing the course of development in the future (Bronfen- teractive processes over time generates the ability, moti-
brenner & Evans 2000; Bronfenbrenner & Morris 1998). vation, knowledge, and skill to engage in such activities
But these forces are not the only powerful ones at both with others and on your own. For example, through
work, other forces are more objective in nature. This progressively more complex interaction with their par-
presence does not mean, however, that the forces are ents, children increasingly become agents of their own
necessarily either more or less influential, mainly be- development, to be sure only in part.
798 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

Proximal processes are posited as the primary en- does not do the job, nor do activities that are often
gines of development (see Gottlieb, Wahlsten, & Lick- interrupted.
liter, Chapter 5, this Handbook, this volume; Tobach, 3. Why not? One reason is that, to be developmentally
1981; Tobach & Schneirla, 1968). A second defining effective, activities must continue long enough to
property, the fourfold source of these dynamic forces is become “increasingly more complex.” Mere repeti-
identified in Proposition II. tion does not work.
4. Developmentally effective proximal processes are
Proposition II not unidirectional; there must be influence in
both directions. For interpersonal interaction, this
means that initiatives do not come from one side
The form, power, content, and direction of the proximal
only; there must be some degree of reciprocity in
processes effecting development vary systematically as a
joint function of the characteristics of the developing per-
the exchange.
son, the environment—both immediate and more remote— 5. Proximal processes are not limited to interactions
in which the processes are taking place, the nature of the with people; they also can involve interaction with
developmental outcomes under consideration, and the so- objects and symbols. In the latter circumstance, for
cial continuities and changes occurring over time through reciprocal interaction to occur, the objects and sym-
the life course and the historical period during which the bols in the immediate environment must be of a
person has lived. kind that invites attention, exploration, manipulation,
elaboration, and imagination.
Propositions I and II are theoretically interdepend-
ent and subject to empirical test. An operational 6. The powerful moderating factors specified in Propo-
research design that permits their simultaneous inves- sition II produce substantial changes in the content,
tigation is referred to as the Process-Person-Context- timing, and effectiveness of proximal processes. In
Time (PPCT) model. particular:
Characteristics of the person actually appear twice a. As children grow older, their developmental
in the bioecological model—first as one of the four ele- capacities increase both in level and range;
ments influencing the form, power, content, and direction therefore, to continue to be effective, the corre-
of the proximal process, and then again as developmental sponding proximal processes must also become
outcomes—qualities of the developing person that more extensive and complex to provide for the
emerge at a later point in time as the result of the joint, future realization of evolving potentials. At the
interactive, mutually reinforcing effects of the four prin- same time, in view of the ongoing developmental
cipal antecedent components of the model. In sum, in the advance, the intervals between periods of “pro-
bioecological model, the characteristics of the person gressively more complex” activity can be in-
function both as an indirect producer and as a product of creasingly longer, although they must still occur
development (see Lerner, 1982, 2002; Lerner & Busch- on a “ fairly regular basis.” Otherwise, the pace
Rossnagel, 1981). of development slows, or its course may even
Finally, because in the bioecological model the con- reverse direction.
cept of proximal process has a specific meaning, it is b. The principal persons with whom young children
important that its distinctive properties be made ex- interact “on a fairly regular basis over extended
plicit. For present purposes, the following features of periods of time” are parents, but especially as
the construct are especially noteworthy: children get older, other persons—such as care-
givers, relatives, siblings, and peers—also func-
1. For development to occur, the person must engage in tion in this role. These are soon followed by
an activity. teachers or mentors in other activities, and then
2. To be effective, the activity must take place “on a by close friends of the same or opposite sex,
fairly regular basis, over an extended period of spouses or their equivalents, and coworkers, supe-
time.” For example, this means that with young chil- riors and subordinates at work. As the examples
dren, a weekend of doing things with Mom or Dad indicate, the involvement of persons functioning
From Theory to Research Design: Operationalizing the Bioecological Model 799

in this role is not limited to the formative years. relevant characteristics of the Person. The 1983 chapter
Borrowing a term from G. H. Mead (1934), we also made no reference to Time as a defining property of
refer to such persons as significant others. the theoretical model. In these and other respects to fol-
low, today’s bioecological model goes far beyond its
The foregoing constitute the principal elements of the predecessors both with respect to basic constructs and
emergent theoretical model. If so, the question arises in their bidirectional, synergistic interrelationships.
what sense is the model bioecological? Where and how
does biology come into the picture? We present three an-
swers to that question in an order of decreasing cer- FROM THEORY TO RESEARCH DESIGN:
tainty about their validity. The first is an unqualified OPERATIONALIZING THE
disclaimer. Little in the pages that follow speaks to the BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL
operation of biological systems within the organism.
By contrast, considerable scientific attention is ac- We have come to the point where it is both possible and
corded to characteristics of the person generally re- necessary to examine the requirements imposed by the
garded as biologically based that influence proximal bioecological model for corresponding research designs.
processes and their developmental outcomes. Finally, We begin with a concrete example of the latter.
the present model rests on the assumption that biologi- In the 1950s and 1960s, Cecil Mary Drillien (1957,
cal factors and evolutionary processes not only set limits 1964), a physician and professor of child life and health
on human development but also impose imperatives re- at the University of Edinburgh, carried out a 7-year lon-
garding the environmental conditions and experiences gitudinal investigation of psychological development in
required for the realization of human potentials. The po- two groups: 360 children of low birthweight and a con-
sition is taken that, to the extent that the necessary con- trol group selected “ by taking the next mature birth
ditions and experiences are not provided, such potentials from the hospital admission list ” (1957, p. 29). In her
will remain unactualized (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, follow-up assessments, the investigator found that chil-
1993, 1994a, 1994b). dren of low birthweight were more likely to exhibit
It is our belief that, when applied, the bioecological problems in physical growth, susceptibility to illness,
paradigm is scientifically productive. At the present impaired intellectual development, and poorer class-
time, however, its most distinguishing characteristic is room performance, with all of these tendencies being
not its proven scientific power, but its rarity. To be sure, more pronounced in boys (1964). In a comparison of
the rarity is hardly surprising, given the fact that suc- children’s school performance with what would have
cessive revisions of the emerging model began to be been expected on the basis of their scores on an intelli-
published only in the past several years (Bronfenbren- gence test, Drillien found that those of low birthweight
ner, 1989a, 1990, 1993, 1994, 1995; Bronfenbrenner & were especially likely to be working below their mental
Morris, 1998; Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994a). Paradox- capacity. In relation to this finding, the author com-
ically, some concrete examples nevertheless existed ments as follows: “In most cases, failure to attain a stan-
much earlier. They were the product of what Bronfen- dard commensurate with ability was associated with
brenner and Crouter referred to in the 1983 edition of problems of behavior, which were found to increase with
this Handbook as “latent paradigms”; that is, theoretical decreasing birthweight [and] to be more common in
models that were not explicitly stated, but were implicit males” (1964, p. 209).
in the research designs used in analyzing the data Figure 14.1 depicts the results. The figure does not ap-
(Bronfenbrenner & Crouter, 1983, pp. 373–376). In- pear in Drillien’s monograph, but was constructed from
deed, a partial precursor of the bioecological model ap- data presented in tables in that volume. It shows the im-
peared in the 1983 Handbook chapter under the rubric of pact of the quality of mother-infant interaction at age 2
a “person-process-context model.” In that chapter, how- on the number of observed problem behaviors at age 4 as
ever, what is meant by process is never specified, and a joint function of social class and three levels of low
the overwhelming majority of the examples cited do not birthweight—those underweight by a pound or more, not
include a proximal process component as defined in more than one pound, and those of normal birthweight.
Proposition I. The same holds true for developmentally Measures of maternal responsiveness were based on
800 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

12 mal process, still emerges as the most powerful predictor


of developmental outcome. In all instances, responsive
Average Score on Behavior Problems

Poor Process
10 maternal treatment reduces substantially the degree of
Good Process
behavioral disturbance exhibited by the child.
8
Herein lies the main justification for distinguishing
between proximal process on the one hand, and, on
6
the other, the environments in which the processes
4 occur; namely, in accord with Proposition I, proximal
processes turn out to be the most potent force influenc-
2 ing the developmental outcome (in this case, the fre-
quency of problem behaviors at 4 years of age).
0 Furthermore, as stipulated in Proposition II, the power
Nrm Low Vlow Nrm Low Vlow Nrm Low Vlow
High SES Middle SES Low SES of the Process varies systematically as a function of the
environmental Context (i.e., social class) and of the
Nrm = Normal Birthweight
Low = Between Normal and 5.5 lbs. characteristics of the Person (i.e., weight at birth). The
Vlow = 5.5 lbs. or Less process appears to have made its greatest impact on
young children growing up in the most disadvantaged
Figure 14.1 Effect of mother’s responsiveness on problem
behavior of child at age 4 by birthweight and social class. environment (i.e., the lowest socioeconomic level), but
in that environment, it is those who at birth were of nor-
mal weight who benefited most. Moreover, it was in this
observations in the home and interviews with the mother. same disadvantaged Context that, under high levels of
The investigator’s measure of social class was a compos- maternal responsiveness, birthweight showed its most
ite index that took into account not only parental income consistent effect, with the number of behavior problems
and education but also the socioeconomic level of the steadily rising as birthweight fell. Finally, across the
neighborhood in which the family lived. The quality of board, maternal responsiveness had the general result of
interaction was assessed by extent to which the mother decreasing or buffering against environmental differ-
was responsive to changes in the state and behavior of the ences in developmental outcome. Thus, at high levels of
infant. The measure of the developmental outcome was mother-child interaction, social class differences in
the frequency of reported behavior disturbances such as problem behavior became much smaller.
hyperactivity, overdependence, timidity, and negativism. From the perspective of developmental science, what
Our primary interest is not in the research findings, is most noteworthy about these findings is not their spe-
but in the extent to which the structure of the research de- cific content but that their simultaneous discovery was
sign corresponds with the defining properties of the bioe- made possible by a research design based on a theoretical
cological theoretical model. The first point to be noted in model that allowed for the emergence of patterns of this
this regard is that Proposition I defines Proximal form. Not only are the four key components of Process,
Processes as bidirectional. Drillien’s measure of process, Person, Context, and Time all represented but the design
however, was based only on the mother’s responsiveness also provides for the detection of the kinds of synergis-
to changes in the state and behavior of the infant, and no tic1 interdependencies among these components that are
data are reported that would permit calculating a comple- posited in the bioecological model as a dynamic theoret-
mentary measure of the infant’s responsiveness to ical system. Two specific examples of such interdepen-
changes in the state and behavior of the mother. This dencies are revealed in the analysis of Drillien’s data:
means that the operational measure available in Drillien’s
research taps only one side of the theoretical definition of 1. Proposition II stipulates that the developmental ef-
proximal process. For that reason, it appears likely that, fects of proximal processes vary as a joint function of
to the extent the infant’s contribution to reciprocal inter-
action carries any weight, the obtained results may un- 1
Synergism refers to “cooperative action of discrete agencies
derestimate the true magnitude of the observed effects. such that the total effect is greater than the sum of the two or
Nevertheless, as revealed in Figure 14.1, maternal re- more effects—taken independently” (Webster’s Third New
sponsiveness across time, a one-sided measure of proxi- International Dictionary).
From Theory to Research Design: Operationalizing the Bioecological Model 801

Person and Context; that is, the indirect effects of examples of the relation between theoretical and opera-
Person and Context on the relation of Process to out- tional models now before us, we can address what turns
come are not to be conceived as simply additive. out to be a complex and consequential question: What is
Consistent with this expectation is the finding that the function of research design in the bioecological
proximal processes had their greatest impact in the model? The first point to be made in this regard is that the
most disadvantaged environment but on the healthiest main function is not the usual one of testing for statistical
infant. The combination of Person and Context ex- significance. Rather, the research design must provide for
hibit a mutually reinforcing, multiplicative, indirect carrying out an equally essential and necessarily prior
effect on the power of proximal processes as the “en- stage of the scientific process: that of developing hypothe-
gines of development.” ses of suf ficient explanatory power and precision to war-
2. In Drillien’s research, the frequency of problem be- rant being subjected to empirical test. We are dealing with
haviors was assessed at two points in time—first science in the discovery mode rather than in the mode of
when the infants were 2-years-old, and then again at verification. In this earlier phase, theory plays an even
4-years-old. If one makes the not unreasonable as- more critical role. From its very beginnings, the bioeco-
sumption that mothers continued to interact with logical model, through its successive reformulations, rep-
their children over the intervening period, then the resents a sustained effort to meet this scientific need.
results shown in Figure 14.2 provide evidence for the What are the appropriate characteristics of research
effect of proximal processes that have taken place designs for developmental science in the discovery
over an extended period of time. Youngsters experi- mode? Finding an answer to this question is complicated
encing low levels of interaction with their mothers by the fact that, compared with the physical and natural
exhibited an accelerating increase in the number of sciences, developmental science is admittedly still in an
problem behaviors from 2 to 4 years of age, whereas earlier stage of development. Furthermore, because its
those exposed to substantially higher levels of this scope falls between the natural and the social sciences,
proximal process showed only a modest rise. the discovery process must to some extent be adapted to
the requirements of both. Perhaps in part for these rea-
Developmental Science in the Discovery Mode sons, we were unable to find any discussion of the issue
in the developmental literature. Under these circum-
What about the possibility that the preceding results are stances, we concluded that the best we could do was to
chance findings? Some of them are statistically signifi- try to make explicit the characteristics of the research
cant, yet others could not be tested because the variances designs that had been employed over the past several
needed for calculating error estimates were not reported. years to arrive at successively more differentiated for-
But that is not the principal issue at stake. With concrete mulations of the bioecological model.
These design characteristics depend on the con-
10
structs, and the possible relations between them, that
are posited in the theoretical model at its present stage
Average Score on Behavior Problems

Poor Process—Unresponsive Mothers of development. Both the constructs, and the possible in-
8
Good Process—Responsive Mothers terrelationships, have been indicated in Propositions I
and II, but as yet they appear in a relatively undifferen-
6 tiated form. For example, the directions of the expected
effects of Person and Context on proximal processes for
4 different types of outcomes are not specified. The rea-
son for such lack of specificity is that a more precise
2 formulation could not be deduced either from the theory
in its present, still evolving state, or induced from any
0 already available data (at least, to our knowledge).
High SES Mid SES Low SES High SES Mid SES Low SES Given these limitations, we concluded that an appropri-
Age 2 Age 4
ate design strategy at this point in the discovery process
Figure 14.2 Effect of mother’s responsiveness on problem could be one that involves a series of progressively more
behavior of child at ages 2 and 4 by social class. differentiated formulations and corresponding data
802 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

analyses, with the results at each successive step setting dismissing findings as Type I errors is further com-
the stage for the next round. The research designs em- pounded by the phenomenon of magnification of early
ployed must be primarily generative rather than confir- environmental differences over time. Thus, as illus-
matory versus disconfirming. trated by the escalating effects of proximal processes
In this generative process, implications derived from shown in Figure 14.2, changes in outcome associated
the theoretical model play a more prominent role than with a proximal process at Time 1 can be quite small and
those drawn from research findings, but the latter are nonsignificant statistically. Yet, as shown, they can be
also critical. Their importance is best conveyed by spec- powerful predictors of a marked increase in develop-
ifying a key feature of the corresponding research de- mental outcome several years later (in the likely event
sign: It must provide a structured framework for that the process continued to be maintained over the in-
displaying the emergent research findings in a way that tervening period).
reveals more precisely the pattern of the interdependen- At this point, a methodological note is in order. Sta-
cies that are obtained in the data available. Of primary tistical models widely used for the purpose of hypothe-
scientific interest are not those aspects of the observed sis testing are often ill-suited as operational models for
pattern already anticipated in the existing theoretical developmental investigations in the discovery mode.
model, but those features that point to more differenti- This is particularly true for models that control statis-
ated and precise theoretical formulations. These can tically solely for linear relationships among the factors
then be evaluated in the light of new evidence, and, if in the research design to obtain an estimate of the inde-
deemed scientifically promising, can be incorporated in pendent contribution of each factor in the statistical
the research design for a next step. The proposed strat- model to the outcome under investigation. The validity
egy for developmental investigations in the discovery of such analyses rests on what in mathematical statis-
mode involves an iterative process of successive con- tics is referred to as “ the assumption of homogeneity of
frontations between theory and data leading toward the regression.” To illustrate the assumption in its simplest
ultimate goal of being able to formulate hypotheses that general case: given a dependent variable y and two in-
both merit and are susceptible to scientific assessment dependent variables x1 and x2, then the relation be-
in the verification mode. tween x1 and y must be the same at all levels of x2. This
In presenting this definition of the discovery mode, assumption is often not met in developmental data. For
we acknowledge that, in actual scientific practice, it is example, when applied to the analysis shown in Figure
hardly likely to be a discovery. The process we have de- 14.2, it would require that the relation between proxi-
scribed, or something like it, is what scientists have al- mal process and frequency of problem behaviors be the
ways done. Our primary reason for seeking to make that same at every social class level, which is not the case.
process explicit was the belief that doing so could fur- Nor is this requirement likely to hold with respect to
ther the discovery process. But we also hope that the ex- any combination of the four defining properties of the
plication and examples of the discovery mode presented bioecological model. As Bronfenbrenner stated in his
in this chapter will have broader utility in developmen- 1979 monograph, “In ecological research, the principal
tal research. main ef fects are likely to be interactions” (p. 38, italics
To return to the task at hand, the proposed criteria in original).
have more specific implications for the critical role in Any research design based on a bioecological model
research design played by statistical analysis. First, in must allow for the possibility of such interactions. How-
the discovery phase, Type I errors can entail an even ever, it is also essential, especially in the discovery
greater risk than errors of Type II. To state the issue phase, that the particular interactions to be examined be
more broadly, dismissing as invalid a finding that points theoretically based, and that—if possible—their antici-
the way to a fuller and more precise explanation for the pated direction and form be specified in advance so that
phenomenon under investigation may result in a greater discrepancies between theoretical expectation and ob-
loss than that produced by accepting a finding that is served reality can be readily recognized and thus pro-
highly significant because of as yet undifferentiated and vide the basis for a next step in the typically slow,
thereby confounded factors producing the phenomenon iterative process of seeking more differentiated formu-
in question (e.g., the failure to distinguish Process from lations that merit further exploration both on theoretical
Context). The greater risk in the discovery process of and empirical grounds. In each case, the new formula-
From Theory to Research Design: Operationalizing the Bioecological Model 803

tion should be consistent with the existing theoretical across situations, whereas competence is defined as the
specifications of the bioecological model, but it also demonstrated acquisition and further development of
must take into account any old or new research findings knowledge and skills—whether intellectual, physical,
bearing on the issue. socioemotional, or a combination of them (e.g., learning
The foregoing criteria for research in the discovery how to care for a young infant involves all three).
mode do not imply neglect of the traditional issues of re- The preceding emergent formulation is based on the
liability and validity. These are honored in a somewhat following considerations. Most parents have the capac-
different, theoretically guided way. Essentially, the pro- ity and the motivation to respond to manifestations
cess is one of cross-validation at two levels. First, in a of physical or psychological distress on the part of
given study, the results at each successive stage of analy- their children. In deprived or disorganized environ-
sis are validated in the next, more differentiated formula- ments, such manifestations of dysfunction have been
tion. Second, the generalizations emerging from a given shown to be both more frequent and more severe (e.g.,
investigation are cross-validated against findings from in Drillien’s research), thus drawing on more of par-
other studies of theoretically related phenomena but with ents’ available time and energy. Accordingly, to the
a specific focus on the defining components of the bioe- extent that, in disadvantaged settings, parents are able
cological model. to engage in proximal processes, these are likely to
Before we proceed with concrete examples, it is im- have greater impact in reducing dysfunction rather
portant to emphasize that the criteria we have proposed than in enhancing their children’s knowledge about
and applied for conducting developmental science in the and skill in dealing with the external environment.
discovery mode represent a first attempt to construct a With respect to problems of dysfunction, in deprived
working model. Moreover, the working model is subject environments there is usually a match between young
to the curious qualification that it is itself the product of children’s needs and their parents’ capacity to meet
the same sequential design that it proposes. The criteria those needs. This does not mean, however, that chil-
were developed by examining the changes introduced at dren in such environments will end up functioning as
each successive stage in the evolution of the bioecological well as their age-mates growing up in more favorable
model to identify the theoretical and operational proper- circumstance, but rather that, over similar periods of
ties leading to improvement in the model’s predictive time, they will show greater improvement in control
power. The example that follows illustrates these concur- over their own problem behaviors as a function of
rent processes. parental responsiveness.
The situation in advantaged and stable environments
Different Paths to Different Outcomes: is rather different. Manifestations of dysfunction are
Dysfunction versus Competence likely to occur less often and to be less intense. Under
In this instance, our exploratory effort took as its point of these circumstances, parents are more apt to be at-
departure the stipulation in Proposition II that the effects tracted by and respond to the more frequent and more
of proximal processes vary systematically depending on gratifying signs of their children’s growing competence,
the developmental outcome. Once again, rather than tak- with the result that proximal processes may to be fo-
ing time to retrace our steps, we begin with where we cused mainly in this latter sphere. In addition, parents
ended up; namely, with the following initial formulation: living in a middle-class world are themselves more apt
to possess and exhibit the knowledge and skills they
The greater developmental impact of proximal processes
wish their children to acquire. They also have greater
on children growing up in disadvantaged or disorganized
environments is to be expected to occur mainly for out-
access to resources and opportunities outside the family
comes reflecting developmental dysfunction. By contrast, that can provide needed experiences for their children.
for outcomes indicating developmental competence, proxi- Taken together, the foregoing considerations led to the
mal processes are posited as likely to have greater impact formulation of the previously stated “proto-hypothesis.”
in more advantaged and stable environments. Because Drillien’s study of the influence of mother-in-
fant interaction dealt with only one developmental out-
The term dysfunction refers to the recurrent manifes- come, one has to look elsewhere for evidence that effects
tation of difficulties on the part of the developing per- of such processes vary depending on the nature of the out-
son in maintaining control and integration of behavior come under consideration. A rich data archive generously
804 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

6.9 formed about and set limits on their children’s activities


Mother and
Mother and outside the home. In the present study, it was assessed by
Grade Point Average (GPA)

Father
Stepfather
5.9 Single–Parent Mother a series of items in a questionnaire administered to ado-
Mean lescents in their school classes. All items referred to par-
4.9 Mean ents in the plural, with no distinction as to whether the
Mean
mother or the father was doing the monitoring. Levels of
3.9 parental monitoring, ranging from 0 to 12, are shown on
the horizontal axis, and grade point average (GPA) is
2.9 shown on the vertical. The markers to the right of each
0 2 4 6 8 10 12 M 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 M 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 M
1 3 5 7 9 11 1 3 5 7 9 11 1 3 5 7 9 11 curve record the mean GPA for each of the six groups.
Levels of Parental Monitoring Ranging from 0–12 Once again, the results reveal that the effects of prox-
imal processes are more powerful than those of the
Mothers with some education beyond high school
Mothers with no education beyond high school environmental Contexts in which they occur. In this in-
GPA Scale: stance, however, the impact of the Process was greatest
2 = Mostly D’s or less 6 = Mostly B’s in what emerges as the most advantaged ecological
3 = 1/2 C’s, 1/2 D’s 7 = 1/2 A’s, 1/2 B’s
4 = Mostly C’s 8 = Mostly A’s niche—families with two biological parents in which the
5 = 1/2 B’s, 1/2 C’s
mother had some education beyond high school. More-
over, the developmental effect of the proximal process on
school grades—a measure of competence—was stronger
Figure 14.3 Effect of parental monitoring on grades in
high school by family structure and mother’s level of educa- for families living in more advantaged socioeconomic
tion. Analyses and graph based on data archives generously circumstances. This finding is directly opposite to that
provided by Professors Stephen A. Small (University of Wis- revealed by the analysis of Drillien’s data, where the out-
consin) and Tom Luster (Michigan State University). come was one of psychological dysfunction (i.e., the fre-
quency of problem behaviors). At the same time, the
made available by Small and Luster (1990) from their principal finding from both studies documents the power-
statewide studies of youth at risk in Wisconsin met this ful effect of proximal processes on human development,
need.2 Figure 14.3 depicts the results from an analysis of a result consistent with the first defining property of the
the differential effects of parental monitoring on the aca- bioecological model stipulated in Proposition I.
demic achievement of high school students living in one of The reader may well ask why the data in each scatter-
the three most common family structures found in the gram were fitted to a curve with a declining slope rather
total sample of over 2,500 cases.3 The students were be- than simply with a straight line. In accord with the crite-
tween 14 and 16 years of age. It was also possible to strat- ria for research in the discovery mode, the introduction of
ify the sample by two levels of mother’s education, with the quadratic term was based on theoretical considera-
completion of high school as the dividing line. Parental tions. Higher levels of academic performance require
monitoring refers to the effort by parents to keep in- mastery of more complex tasks, and hence are more diffi-
cult to achieve. As a result, at each successive step, the
2
The analyses of data from the Wisconsin archive reported in same degree of active effort would be expected to yield a
this chapter were carried out in collaboration with Stephen A. somewhat smaller result. More specifically, for pupils
Small (University of Wisconsin) and Tom Luster (Michigan who are not doing so well in school, parental monitoring
State University) who designed and conducted the survey
from which the data are drawn. We are deeply indebted to into a closely corresponding research design can produce reli-
them for the theoretical thinking that underlies the construc- able findings even when there are relatively few cases in
tion of the survey. It is an excellent example of developmental some, or even all, the cells of the model. This comes about be-
science in the discovery mode. We are also grateful to Regina cause, in effect, the bioecological model requires, in its dis-
Cannon (then a graduate student at Cornell University) who covery phase, advance specification primarily not only of
carried out the statistical analyses with care and dispatch. main effects but also in the form and direction of their most
3 plausible interactions in the light of both the evolving theoret-
The large number of cases in this study should not be taken
to imply that the bioecological model can be applied only in ical model and the then available research evidence. This is
samples with a large N. As illustrated here, precision in the especially true for well-designed experiments. For examples,
formulation of the theoretical model and in its translation see pp. 808–809.
From Theory to Research Design: Operationalizing the Bioecological Model 805

can have a substantial effect by ensuring more stability of serve to reduce and act as a buffer against effects of dis-
Time and place so that some learning can occur. But for advantaged and disruptive environments.
superior school achievement, students would require in
addition high levels of motivation, focused attention, To turn from substance to method, the foregoing
prior knowledge, and—especially—actually working findings also demonstrate that tests of significance have
with the material to be learned. These are all qualities a place in research in the discovery mode, but, as with
that stability of Time and place by itself cannot provide. hypothesis verification, only after a specific theoretical
As can be seen in Figure 14.3, the relation between expectation has been formulated in advance.
parental monitoring and school grades shows a curvilin- In a discovery context, however, the aim is not to
ear trend. Moreover, in accord with criteria for research claim empirical validity for a particular theoretical for-
in the discovery mode (see pp. 801–803), both in its di- mulation but to indicate its plausibility for inclusion in
rection and form the trend corresponds with theoretical the research design at subsequent stages of exploratory
expectations in being more pronounced when the mother work. To be sure, doing so may result in a failure of
has some education beyond high school, especially in a replication. But not doing so risks missing potentially
two-parent family structure. A test for heterogeneity of important, theoretically guided research opportunities
regression confirms visual inspection. The differences in not yet recognized. Garmezy and Rutter (1983), in their
slopes between the two educational levels are highly sig- landmark studies of stress and coping in children’s
nificant (p ≤ .01), with the quadratic component emerg- development, did not differentiate between those protec-
ing as reliable only in the higher educational group.4 Also tive or disruptive forces emanating from the environ-
statistically significant are differences in school achieve- ment, and those inherent in the biopsychological
ment by family structure in each level of mother’s educa- characteristics of the person. As evidenced from the
tion, with students growing up in two-parent families analysis of Drillien’s data shown in Figure 14.1, these
getting the highest grades, and those from single-parent vectors do not always operate in the same direction.
families the lowest, a rank order corresponding to the Nevertheless, Garmezy and Rutter’s formulations and
power of the proximal process in each group as measured findings played a significant role in the early stages of
by the slopes of the associated regression coefficients. the process through which the bioecological model
Finally, a result not shown on the graph provides addi- reached its present, still-evolving form.
tional evidence pointing to another tentative generaliza- The still-evolving form imposes the obligation to take
tion. The first indication appeared in the analysis of advantage of existing opportunities for continued explo-
Drillien’s data, which, among other findings, revealed ration. With respect to the present inquiry, the next step in
that maternal responsiveness had the general effect of de- that process was once again to pose the question about the
creasing or buffering against environmental differences in extent to which the research design meets the defining
developmental outcome. Thus, at high levels of mother- properties of the bioecological model. At first glance, we
child interaction, social class differences in problem be- appear to be confronted with the same problem that we en-
havior became smaller. A similar pattern emerges for the countered with Drillien’s study. Proposition I defines
effects of parental monitoring on school grades. Across proximal processes as bidirectional. As previously noted,
the six groups shown in Figure 14.3, stronger parental Small and Luster (1990) defined parental monitoring as
monitoring was associated not only with a higher mean the effort by parents to keep informed about and set limits
on school performance but also with a lower standard de- on their children’s activities outside the home. As stated,
viation. These differences, too, were statistically reliable. such behavior implies influence from one side only—that
Hence the following working hypothesis: of the parents. An examination of the actual items used in
their questionnaire, however, revealed that they were of
For outcomes of competence, proximal processes not only
two kinds. Some were cast in the language of parental ex-
lead to higher levels of developmental functioning but also
pectation and prescription (e.g., “If I am going to be home
4
The degree of curvilinearity is measured by the correspon- late, I am expected to call my parent(s) to let them know”;
ding regression coefficients and not by difference in the “When I go out, my parent(s) ask me where I’m going”).
length of each curve from top to bottom. The latter is deter- By contrast, other items implied that the desired expecta-
mined by empty cells in the scatter plot below or above which tions or prescriptions were being met (e.g., “My parent(s)
entries for both monitoring level and GPA were available. know where I am after school”; “I tell my parent(s) who
806 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

I’m going to be with before I go out”). Although the first boys, a result that is paralleled by corresponding differ-
type of item is unidirectional, the second entails some de- ences in average GPA for the two sexes.5 In each of the
gree of reciprocity to the extent that the adolescent is three family structures, girls received higher grades than
providing the information desired by the parents. Accord- boys, with the difference being most pronounced in two-
ingly, we hypothesized that items of the second type parent households and lowest in single-mother families.
would show stronger relationships to developmental out- As seen in Figure 14.4, however, a distinctive feature
comes than those that described only the parents’ expecta- of the pattern for girls is a marked flattening
tions of how they wished their children to behave. of the curve, especially for daughters of single-parent
Separate analyses of scales based on each type of mothers. This result suggests that, in each of the
item provided substantial support for our working hy- three family structures, mothers may be pushing their
pothesis. Although responses to both types of questions already successful daughters to the point where con-
showed reliable effects on school performance, the rela- formity to maternal control no longer brings educational
tionships for the reciprocity scale were significantly returns, particularly when the mother is the only parent.
stronger and were much more likely to show curvilinear An analysis of data on students whose mothers had
effects. Accordingly, the latter was the scale used in an- no more than a high school education showed a similar
alyzing the results presented in Figure 14.3. general pattern, but the effects were less pronounced.
From the perspective of the biological model, the re- The influence of monitoring was appreciably weaker,
search design producing the results shown in that figure is and its greater benefit to girls was also reduced. Never-
missing an important Person component. It is a general theless, girls with less-educated mothers both in single-
finding in educational research that at the high school parent and in stepfamilies still had higher GPA scores
level female students score higher on measures of aca- than boys. This means that some other factor not yet
demic performance than do males. The question therefore identified must account for this difference.
arises: To what extent is this gender difference attributa- Although a number of possibilities come to mind re-
ble to variations in proximal process? Figure 14.4 pro- garding this unknown, regrettably the Wisconsin archive
vides a tentative answer to this question for students does not contain any data on the principal suspects. What
whose mothers had more than a high school education. In is available is information about another trail of discovery
each family structure, parental monitoring exerted a more that we have already begun to explore. Our successively
powerful effect on the school achievement of girls than of more differentiated working models, both conceptual and
operational, for assessing the effects of parental monitor-
7
ing on school achievement have provided increasing sup-
Average Grade in High School Subjects

Boys Girls port for the tentative hypothesis that, for outcomes
Mean Girls
6
Boys Girls reflecting developmental competence, proximal processes
Mean Mean Mean
Boys are likely to have greatest impact in the most advantaged
5 environments. But what about the other half of the original
Mean
Mean formulation: the complementary postulate that the greater
4 developmental impact of proximal processes growing up
in poor environments is to be expected to occur mainly for
3 outcomes reflecting developmental dysfunction?
Data from Small and Luster’s archive also provide
2
0 4 8 12 0 4 8 12 2 6 10 2 6 10 0 4 8 12 0 4 8 12
the opportunity for cross-validating this provisional
2 6 10 2 6 10 0 4 8 12 0 4 8 12 2 6 10 2 6 10
claim. In addition to measures of academic achievement,
Level of Parental Monitoring
2-Parent Mother and Single-Parent
the Wisconsin study also included information on
Family Stepfather Mother teenagers’ sexual activity. The decision to analyze this
High school students are between 14 and 16 years old. outcome in the context of a bioecological model was
prompted by Small and Luster’s (1990) finding that such
Figure 14.4 Parental monitoring and high school grades by
gender: mothers with more than a high school education.
5
Analyses and graph based on data archives generously pro- Within each pair, both means and regression coefficients
vided by Professors Stephen A. Small (University of Wiscon- were statistically significant, the latter confirming reliable
sin) and Tom Luster (Michigan State University). differences in slope.
From Theory to Research Design: Operationalizing the Bioecological Model 807

behavior varied systematically by family structure. Sex- 80


ual activity was measured by a single question: “Have

Percent Sexually Active


you ever had sexual relations with another person?” 60
Mean
This documentation of variations in sexual activity
Mean
by family structure takes on special significance in the 40
light of broader social changes taking place in the lives
of children, youth, and families in contemporary U.S.
20
society. Today, the United States has the highest rate of
teenage pregnancy of any developed nation, almost
0
twice as high as that of its nearest competitors (Bron- 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4
fenbrenner, McClelland, Wethington, Moen, & Ceci, Mother Completed Mother Had Some Education
High School Only Beyond High School
1996, p. 117). Adolescent sexual activity is also one of
the prominent elements in the so-called teenage syn- Daughter with Own Mother and Father
drome, an escalating pattern of co-occurring behaviors Daughter with a Single-Parent Mother
including smoking, drinking, early and frequent sexual Daughter with Mother and Stepfather

experience, adolescent pregnancy, a cynical attitude to-


ward education and work, and, in the more extreme Figure 14.5 Effect of monitoring on girls’ sexual activity
( high school students between 14 and 16 years of age).
cases, drugs, suicide, vandalism, violence, and criminal
acts (for references and successive summaries of the ev-
idence, see Bronfenbrenner, 1970, 1975, 1986a, 1989c,
that, for outcomes reflecting lack of control, proximal
1990, 1992; Bronfenbrenner et al., 1996; Bronfenbren-
processes have greater impact in poorer environments.
ner & Neville, 1994).
Tests for heterogeneity of regression confirm that this
In anticipating the effects of parental monitoring on
finding holds for each of the three family forms.
teenagers’ sexual activity, we were again confronted
Yet, as shown in Figure 14.6, the corresponding analy-
with the issue of the possible direction of influence. In
sis of the data for boys reveals the very opposite result.
relation to sexual activity as an outcome, however, some
Parental monitoring has a more powerful effect on boys
leverage for the resolution of the issue was provided be-
whose mothers have had more education rather than less.
cause each direction could be expected to produce oppo-
Once again, the finding holds in each family structure
site effects. On the one hand, if parental monitoring
and is confirmed by tests for heterogeneity of regression.
functions to defer sexual activity, then the more moni-
toring the less sexual activity. On the other hand, if the
parents begin to monitor only after the fact, the associa-
tion would be reversed, with monitoring occurring in re- 80
action to the adolescent’s behavior; hence, sexually
Percent Sexually Active

Group %
active adolescents would be monitored more. 60
Group %
The results of the analysis are shown in Figures 14.5
and 14.6.6 The most salient finding for both sexes is that 40
parental monitoring does substantially reduce adoles-
cents’ sexual activity. In many other respects, however, 20
the patterns for female and male adolescents are quite
different. The results for girls in Figure 14.5 show that 0
the effect of parental monitoring is stronger for daugh- 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4
Mother Completed Mother Had Some Education
ters of mothers with no education beyond high school— High School Only Beyond High School
a finding consistent with the working hypothesis
Sons in Biological Families
Sons of Single-Parent Mother
6
We are also indebted to Kristen Jacobson, now a doctoral Sons in Stepfamilies
student at Pennsylvania State University, for her ingenuity
and accuracy in translating into a common format data Figure 14.6 Effect of parental monitoring on boys’ sexual
archives recorded on different computing systems. activity ( high school students between 14 and 16 years of age).
808 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

This is not the only departure from the expecta- We offer these suggestions not for their relevance to
tions generated by the most recent working model. For this particular issue but to illustrate two additional emer-
example, there was not always correspondence between gent corollaries of the bioecological model:
the developmental power of proximal processes in a
given family structure and the percentage of sexually
1. The specific components of Process, Person, Con-
active adolescents in that structure: In stepfamilies in
text, and Time to be included in a given investigation
which the mother has only a high school education, ma-
should be those that, from a theoretical perspective,
ternal monitoring of daughters is as high as it is in two-
are maximally relevant to the research question under
parent families, but the percentage of sexually active
investigation and complementary to each other in re-
girls is even greater than that for single-parent mothers
lation to the given developmental outcome.
at the same educational level. The finding is consistent
2. From a theoretical perspective, the power of a PPCT
with research indicating that living in a family with a
design is most effectively enhanced by including more
stepfather entails a special developmental risk for girls
than one proximal process in the model.
(Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992).
And so, we find ourselves engaged in a next stage of the
discovery process in which we are seeking to develop a The next section leads to yet another corollary.
more differentiated formulation that, through a corre-
sponding research design, will be most effective in reduc-
The Role of Experiments in the
ing the observed empirical departures from expectations
Bioecological Model
based on the existing working model. The first step is to
ask an obvious question: What is most likely to account The examples considered thus far are essentially
for such discrepancies? Restating the question from the experiments of nature: They show how development is
perspective of the bioecological model, which of the four influenced by variations in the components of the bioe-
components is a likely suspect? It has to be somebody who cological model occurring in already existing societies.
is already on the scene. Parents are already there. Who They tell us nothing about whether, to what extent, or
else is around who could exert some influence on the sex- how these elements and their combinations can be
ual activity of high school students? The question answers changed. This limitation applies particularly to the most
itself—the peer group. And if it is indeed true that proxi- consequential component of the bioecological model—
mal processes are at least as powerful determinants of de- proximal processes. We know of no research bearing
velopment as either the characteristics of the person or of specifically on this question, but some indirect evidence
the environment, what might that process be? does exist. In research findings already presented, im-
A tentative first nomination is progressively more in- proving the quality of the environment has been shown to
tense interaction with peers who are already sexually ac- increase the developmental power of proximal processes.
tive. Among other considerations, this suggestion is guided The indirect evidence comes from experiments in which
by the possibility that peer pressure to engage in sexual ac- researchers have systematically introduced conditions
tivity and the prestige that such activity brings are likely to into the environment in which people lived that were hy-
be higher for boys from less educated families with the re- pothesized to enhance their psychological functioning
sult that parental monitoring is not as effective. With re- beyond existing levels.
spect to the other components in the model, given the Here are two examples at contrasting ages.
findings just reported, gender would still be a Person char-
acteristic of major importance. The choice of an appropri-
Environmental Dynamics in Old Age
ate environmental Context depends on the precise research
question being asked. Family structure would also still be The first example is Langer and Rodin’s oft-cited exper-
appropriate. But from the perspective of the bioecological imental intervention conducted with residents of a New
model, an option to consider would be the parents’ beliefs Haven nursing home for the aged (Langer & Rodin, 1976;
about the activities they wanted their adolescent son or Rodin & Langer, 1977). The contextual manipulation
daughter to engage in or refrain from, as well as the close- employed in this study is well summarized in the au-
ness of the parent-child relationship. thors’ words:
From Theory to Research Design: Operationalizing the Bioecological Model 809

The hospital administrator gave a talk to residents in the “workbook for parents” stressing the idea that “the infant
experimental group emphasizing their responsibility for learns most from the effects of its own behavior” (p. 113):
themselves, whereas the communication to a second, com-
parison group stressed the staff ’s responsibility for them
Caregivers were advised not to direct the child’s activities
as patients. To bolster the communication, residents in the
too much, but to give the child opportunity to find out
experimental group were offered plants to care for,
things for himself, to praise him for his efforts, and to re-
whereas residents in the comparison group were given
spond to his initiations of interaction. (p. 113)
plants that were watered by the staff. (Rodin & Langer,
1977, p. 897)
By contrast, mothers of infants in the “stimulation”
Residents were assigned at random to either the exper- group received a workbook that emphasized the impor-
imental or the control group. Data on psychological and tance of providing the infant with a great variety of per-
health characteristics were collected at three time points: ceptual experiences of all kinds, “ to point to and name
(1) just prior to the introduction of the experiment; (2) 3 objects and persons,” and “ to speak a lot to their in-
weeks later, when the experiment was formally ended; fants” (p. 112).
and (3) in a follow-up study conducted 18 months later. In the follow-up assessment conducted 3 months later,
The substantial effects of intervention found at the infants of mothers who had been encouraged to be re-
end of the experiment (Langer & Rodin, 1976) were still sponsive to their babies’ initiatives exhibited higher lev-
in evidence in the follow-up assessment. To be sure, be- els of exploratory behavior and were more likely to prefer
cause the residents were almost a year-and-a-half older, a novel object to one that was already familiar. The babies
the added age had taken some toll, but, nevertheless, also learned more quickly in a learning contingency task.
those in the “induced responsibility” group not only sig- Neither of the preceding investigations included
nificantly surpassed their controls, but were appreciably any systematic assessment of the activities in which the
better off, both psychologically and physically, than participants in the experiment subsequently engaged, of
they had been months earlier before the intervention had the balance between unidirectional and bidirectional be-
begun. In ratings by observers blind to the experimental havior in the two groups, or of any other specific feature
conditions, they were judged to be more alert, sociable, that could provide a measure of the extent to which prox-
and vigorous. The most striking results were seen in the imal processes were operating in each of the two con-
comparison of death rates between the two treatment trasting experimental conditions.
groups. Taking the 18 months prior to the original inter- In both of the preceding experimental studies,
vention as an arbitrary comparison period, in the subse- elegant as they are, the keystone of the bioecological
quent 18 months following the intervention, 15% in the model—a measure of proximal process—was not in-
“responsibility-induced” group died, compared with cluded in the research design. In addition, the demonstra-
30% in the control group. tion (in Figure 14.3) of the joint, indirect effects of
family structure and parents’ education on the relation of
Environmental Dynamics in Infancy proximal processes to school grades does only half the
A remarkable, independent cross-validation of Langer and job, for it provides no information on whether differences
Rodin’s principal hypothesis appears in the findings of in students’ personal characteristics (such as gender)
another intervention experiment—this one almost un- exert a similar indirect effect. Nevertheless, viewed from
known—that was carried out at about the same time with a the theoretical perspective of a bioecological model, all
sample of 100 9-month-old infants and their mothers in these findings are impressively consistent with expecta-
the Dutch city of Nijmegen (Riksen-Walraven, 1978). Al- tions derived from the model; the findings illustrate the
though this author, Marianne Riksen-Walraven, appears model’s practicability, and—perhaps most promising for
not to have been aware of Langer and Rodin’s work con- the future of developmental science—generate questions
ducted during the same period, one of the two interven- that, when answered, provide ways for enhancing the
tion strategies she employed with her sample of infants model’s scientific power. It is these questions and an-
was similar to that used in the New Haven study of elderly swers that are addressed in the following sections.
patients. Mothers, randomly assigned to what Riksen- Up to this point, our exposition has given primary at-
Walraven called the “responsiveness” group, were given a tention to the core concept of proximal process and its
810 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

key position in the bioecological model as a whole. We distinguish between these two propensities. We refer to
now proceed to a more detailed examination of each of the former as developmentally generative characteristics;
the other three defining properties of the model—Per- to the latter as developmentally disruptive.
son, Context, and Time. Examples of developmentally disruptive dispositions
come more readily to mind. At one pole, they include
HOW DO PERSON CHARACTERISTICS such characteristics as impulsiveness, explosiveness, dis-
INFLUENCE LATER DEVELOPMENT? tractibility, inability to defer gratification, or, in a more
extreme form, ready resort to aggression and violence; in
As already indicated, at midstage in the development of short, difficulties in maintaining control over emotions
the bioecological model, an effort was begun to arrive at and behavior. At the opposite pole are such Person attri-
some answers to this question, and it has continued up to butes as apathy, inattentiveness, unresponsiveness, lack
the present day. As before, rather than describe the suc- of interest in the surroundings, feelings of insecurity,
cessive stages in this emergent reconception, we present shyness, or a general tendency to avoid or withdraw from
it in its most recent, still-evolving form. activity.8 Persons exhibiting either of the preceding
Most developmental research treats the cognitive and propensities would find it difficult to engage in proximal
socioemotional characteristics of the person as dependent processes requiring progressively more complex patterns
variables; that is, as measures of developmental outcomes. of reciprocal interaction over extended periods of time.
Far less often are such characteristics examined as pre- By contrast, developmentally generative characteris-
cursors and producers of later development From the per- tics involve such active orientations as curiosity, tendency
spective of the bioecological model, their effectiveness in to initiate and engage in activity alone or with others, re-
the latter role derives from their capacity to influence the sponsiveness to initiatives by others, and readiness to
emergence and operation of proximal processes. defer immediate gratification to pursue long-term goals.
Accordingly, in an effort to identify such process-rel- We have found few investigations that shed light on the
evant Person characteristics, we applied the sequential developmental effects of either type of dynamic character-
design strategy described in the preceding section. Be- istics on proximal processes and their outcomes. A major
ginning with implications derived from the theoretical reason for this shortcoming is the absence of theoretical
model, which are then related to existing research find- constructs for conceptualizing their changing nature over
ings, successive applications of this strategy have re- the course of development from early infancy, through
sulted in the conceptualization of three kinds of adolescence, into and beyond early adulthood. The follow-
process-relevant Person characteristics, which, for con- ing framework is offered as an initial basis for meeting this
venience of brevity, we have labeled as Person forces, re- requirement beginning in the Person domain in greater
sources, and demands.7 need of conceptual definition—that of developmentally
generative characteristics. The corresponding structure
Force Characteristics as Shapers for developmentally disruptive Person qualities can then
of Development be derived as an inverted mirror image of the former.9

In the bioecological model, the characteristics of the


Developmentally Generative Dispositions in
Person most likely to influence future development
Life-Course Perspective
would be active behavioral dispositions that can set
proximal processes in motion and sustain their opera- The first and earliest manifestation of generative dispo-
tion, or—conversely—actively interfere with, retard, or sitions takes the form of what we call selective respon-
even prevent their occurrence. It is therefore useful to siveness. It involves differentiated response to, attraction
by, and exploration of aspects of the physical and social
environment.
7
As is documented later in this chapter (p. 819), the recently
8
renewed, and far stronger, claims by behavior geneticists for Depending on the available alternatives, withdrawal may be
the predominant role of genetic factors in determining both the only course left for dealing with an unbearable situation.
9
individual and group differences in all forms of human char- The material that follows represents a further development
acteristics are directly challenged by alternative explanations by the present authors of ideas first introduced in Bronfen-
and research findings derived from the bioecological model. brenner (1989).
How Do Person Characteristics Inf luence Later Development? 811

The next generative characteristic to evolve goes be- oneself as an active agent both in relation to the self
yond selective responsiveness to include the tendency to and environment, or, for short, directive beliefs. The
engage and persist in progressively more complex activ- oldest concept of this kind is Rotter’s construct and
ities; for example, to elaborate, restructure, and even to measure of “locus of control” (Rotter, 1966). Subse-
create new features in our environment—not only physi- quently, a more sophisticated formulation of the con-
cal and social but also symbolic. We refer to propensi- cept was introduced by Bandura (1977, 1982) under the
ties of this kind as structuring proclivities. rubric of self-efficacy. The principal distinction be-
The transition from one to the other of these dynamic tween these earlier constructs and their counterparts in
forms of orientation during early childhood is illustrated the bioecological model is that the latter are conceptu-
in successive publications from a longitudinal study of in- alized primarily not as characteristics of the person
fants being carried out by Leila Beckwith, Sarale Cohen, sufficient unto themselves but as directional disposi-
Claire Kopp, and Arthur Parmelee at UCLA (Beckwith & tions interacting synergistically with particular fea-
Cohen, 1984; Beckwith, Rodning, & Cohen, 1992; Cohen tures of the environment to generate successive levels
& Beckwith, 1979; Cohen, Beckwith, & Parmelee, 1978; of developmental advance.
Cohen & Parmelee, 1983; Cohen, Parmelee, Beckwith, & The closest approximation to an operationalized
Sigman, 1986). Their imaginative and careful work re- bioecological model in which directive beliefs function
veals a progressive sequence of such environmentally ori- as Person characteristics appears in a series of findings
ented dispositions from birth through 7 years of age. arising from a doctoral dissertation by Tulkin (1973,
Thus, immediately after birth, infants are especially re- 1977; Tulkin & Cohler, 1973; Tulkin & Kagan, 1972).
sponsive to vestibular stimulation ( being picked up and The investigator studied social class differences both in
held in a vertical position close to the body), which has the behaviors and the beliefs of mothers of 10-month-
the effect of soothing babies so that they begin to engage old girls. The research was conducted in the home, em-
in mutual gazing; by 3 months, visual exploration extends ploying both interviews and observations. Middle-class
beyond proximal objects, and the mother’s voice is most mothers were distinguished from their working-class
likely to elicit responses especially in the form of recipro- counterparts not only by higher levels of reciprocal in-
cal vocalizations. teraction with their infants, but also in their views about
From about 6 months on, the infant begins actively to what a 10-month-old could do, and about their own abil-
manipulate objects spontaneously in a purposeful way ities to influence their baby’s development; the more ad-
and to rearrange the physical environment. By now, both vantaged mothers attributed greater potentials both to
vocalization and gesture are being used to attract the their infants and themselves. In addition, the correla-
parents’ attention and to influence their behavior. In ad- tions between maternal behavior and attitudes were sub-
dition, there is a growing readiness, across modalities, stantially greater in middle-class than in lower-class
to initiate and sustain reciprocal interaction with a families. Several years later, Tulkin and a colleague
widening circle of persons in the child’s immediate en- (Tulkin & Covitz, 1975) reassessed the same youngsters
vironment. This is the emergence of what we call struc- after they had entered school. The children’s perfor-
turing proclivities. mance on tests of mental ability and language skill
A number of other investigations have yielded com- showed significant relationships to the prior measures of
parable findings, and have extended them to still other reciprocal mother-infant interaction.
activity domains; for example: individual differences in Perceptive readers may have detected a sleight of
children’s creativity in play and fantasy behavior (Con- hand in our analysis of Tulkin’s research when exam-
nolly & Doyle, 1984; MacDonald & Parke, 1984) or ined from the perspective of the bioecological model. In
Jean and Jack Block’s longitudinal studies of “ego re- that framework, we have been discussing characteristics
siliency” and “ego control” (J. H. Block & Block, 1980; of the developing Person that influence proximal
J. Block, Block, & Keyes, 1988). processes and their outcomes. In Tulkin’s work, the de-
The nature of the third and final class of develop- veloping Person is the infant. But the directive beliefs
mentally generative Person characteristics reflects the we have been discussing are those of the mother. The
increasing capacity and active propensity of children reason for the substitution is the following. Although, in
as they grow older to conceptualize their experience. It the line of work stimulated by Rotter and Bandura there
deals with what we call directive belief systems about are many investigations of the relation between personal
812 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

beliefs and development, to our knowledge there have A concrete example of a deficiency in developmen-
been no studies of the effect of personal beliefs on the tal resources has already been documented in the
proximal processes in which the developing person her- analysis of Drillien’s results depicted in Figure 14.1.
self or himself becomes engaged. To provide an example, Proximal processes exerted their most powerful effect
we resorted to a substitution of roles. on children growing up in the most disadvantaged envi-
The substitution also provides an opportunity to intro- ronment, but in that environment youngsters who at
duce a corollary formulation, the evidence for which ap- birth were of normal weight benefited most. Weight at
pears later in this and subsequent sections of this chapter: birth does not imply a directed propensity to engage in
or refrain from a particular kind of behavior. What it
In proximal processes involving interpersonal inter-
does represent is variation in the biological resources
action, the personal characteristics that influence the
available to engage in any activity requiring directed
power of the process and its effects are the same for all
parties involved.
activity or response over extended periods of time.
Thus, in the present instance, one plausible explanation
To return to the task at hand, we present a second for the observed asymmetric pattern is that, among
form of Person characteristic posited as affecting future families living in stressful environments, infants who
psychological growth—what we have called develop- are physically healthy from birth are more able to en-
mental resources. gage in reciprocal interaction than those who are bio-
logically impaired.
Resource Characteristics of the Person as This interpretation is called into question, however,
Shapers of Development by the corresponding results, shown in the same graph,
for infants raised under the most favorable socioeco-
These are Person characteristics that in themselves in- nomic circumstances. Infants of normal birthweight
volve no selective disposition to action, but constitute profited least from interaction with their mothers. How
biopsychological liabilities and assets that influence the might this paradox be resolved?
capacity of the organism to engage effectively in proxi- Even though the corresponding interaction term is
mal processes. In the first category are conditions that statistically significant, under normal circumstances
limit or disrupt the functional integrity of the organism. the preceding result would—and properly should—be
Some obvious examples include genetic defects, low called into question as a post hoc finding. But, in the
birthweight, physical handicaps, severe and persistent present instance, that is not quite the case. To be sure,
illness, or damage to brain function through accident or there was no a priori hypothesis predicting the precise
degenerative processes. By contrast, developmental as- pattern of the obtained results. The pattern is consis-
sets take the form of ability, knowledge, skill, and ex- tent, however, with several possibilities envisioned for a
perience that, as they evolve over most of the life third Person attribute posited as influencing proximal
course, extend the domains in which proximal processes processes and their developmental effects. And for sci-
can do their constructive work—thereby becoming an- ence in the discovery mode, post hoc findings that are
other source of the progressively more complex patterns theoretically relevant are not to be lightly dismissed.
of interaction constituting a defining property of proxi-
mal processes. Demand Characteristics of the Person
The similarity between the definitions for the two as Developmental Inf luences
types of developmental resources, and for the earlier
distinction between developmental outcomes reflecting The distinguishing feature of this last set of Person char-
dysfunction versus competence, derives from the fact acteristics affecting development is their capacity to in-
already noted that characteristics of the Person appear vite or discourage reactions from the social environment
on both sides of the bioecological equation. Develop- that can disrupt or foster processes of psychological
mental outcomes at Time 1 indirectly influence develop- growth: for example, a fussy versus a happy baby, attrac-
mental outcomes at Time 2 through their effect on tive versus unattractive physical appearance, or hyperac-
proximal processes during the intervening period. The tivity versus passivity. Half a century ago, Gordon
difference, therefore, lies not in the concepts themselves Allport (1937), borrowing a term originally introduced
but in their place in the bioecological model. by Mark A. May (1932), spoke of such characteristics as
The Role of Focus of Attention in Proximal Processes 813

constituting “personality” defined in terms of its “social


plausible answer for children of normal birthweight living
stimulus value.” Rephrasing this concept in terms of its
in the most favored circumstances is that their mothers
analog in contemporary theories of perception, we refer
would be responding primarily not to manifestations of
to such Person qualities as demand characteristics.
problem behavior, but of growing competence.
A striking example of the developmental effect of
such a feature emerges as a major finding in one of the
follow-up studies of children of the Great Depression
carried out by Elder and his colleagues (Elder, Van THE ROLE OF FOCUS OF ATTENTION IN
Nguyen, & Caspi, 1985). The investigators found that PROXIMAL PROCESSES
economic hardship adversely influenced the psychoso-
cial well-being of girls ( but not boys) through its ten- The preceding considerations, generated by a confronta-
dency to increase the rejecting behavior of fathers. The tion of data with theory, call for more differentiated for-
effects of rejection, however, varied inversely as a func- mulations in the existing bioecological model. Here is
tion of the daughter’s physical attractiveness. In the au- the first of two tentative responses to the call:
thors’ words, “Attractive daughters were not likely to be
maltreated by their fathers, no matter how severe the When a proximal process involves interaction with another
economic pressure. [The results] underscore the impor- person, the power of the bioecological model is substan-
tance of viewing economic decline in relation to both the tially enhanced by including in the research design a mea-
child’s characteristics and parenting behavior ” (p. 361). sure of the other person’s focus of attention on the
Here is a classic instance of the power of a PPCT model particular aspects of the behavior of the subject that are
in revealing the complex interactions between organism presumed, on theoretical and empirical grounds, to be most
closely related to the developmental outcome.
and environment that drive the process of development.
The concept of demand characteristics also intro-
For Drillien’s study, the measure of proximal process
duces a new perspective for interpreting the contrasting
is the mother’s responsiveness, but we do not know to
developmental effects of birthweight by social class
what particular behaviors of her baby she was respond-
shown in Figure 14.1. As noted earlier, at the lowest
ing. As already suggested, the aspect most relevant for
socioeconomic level it was the children of normal birth-
reducing future problem behaviors might be expressions
weight who benefited most from maternal responsive-
of distress. If so, a more precise conceptual and opera-
ness. But does that mean they were also the ones who got
tional definition of the proximal process in this study
the most attention from their mothers? Paradoxically,
would be the proportion of manifestations of distress
the picture turns out to be just the reverse. Only 14% of
that were responded to by successful efforts to reduce
these lower-class mothers were judged to be responsive
that distress.
to changes in their infants’ state or behavior, whereas
However, even though in the Drillien study the moth-
the percentage for mothers of low-birthweight babies
ers’ focus of attention was not known, the extent of her
was more than twice as high (averaging 37%). In short,
responsiveness was still a strong predictor of the out-
lower-class mothers were responding mainly to those in-
come. Even when the theoretical and operational re-
fants who most needed their attention, albeit with a
quirements of the bioecological model are not met in
lower return on their investment.
full, the results can still contribute to understanding the
But what characteristics of these babies were captur-
forces that shape human development.
ing the mothers’ attention? It appears likely that in this
A second, complementary tentative formulation de-
instance, the mothers were responding mainly to their in-
rives from the definition of proximal processes as bidi-
fants’ expressions of distress—behavior less apt to occur
rectional. Stated succinctly, it posits that the preceding
among those of normal birthweight. If we look at the cor-
formulation also holds in reverse:
responding data for families at the highest socioeconomic
level, we discover a rather different picture. Mothers are When a proximal process involves interaction with another
more responsive to the healthiest infants than to those of person, the power of the bioecological model is substan-
lowest birthweight, but as shown in Figure 14.1, they get tially enhanced by including in the research design a mea-
the least return for their pains. Once again a key question sure of the developing person’s focus of attention on the
becomes “What is capturing the mother’s attention?” A particular aspects of the behavior of the other person that
814 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

are presumed, on theoretical and empirical grounds, to be environmental Contexts as they affect developmental
most closely related to the developmental outcome.10 processes and outcomes.

PROXIMAL PROCESSES IN SOLO


THE MICROSYSTEM MAGNIFIED:
ACTIVITIES WITH OBJECTS AND SYMBOLS
ACTIVITIES, RELATIONSHIPS, AND ROLES
The foregoing considerations and complexities give
In addressing this topic, we return to the earliest formu-
added importance to those proximal processes that do
lation of the ecological model. Today, as then, “ the
not involve interpersonal interaction but instead focus on
ecological environment is conceived as a set of nested
progressively more complex reciprocal interaction with
structures, each inside the other like a set of Russian
objects and symbols. These are activities that can be car-
dolls” (Bronfenbrenner, 1979b, p. 3). The contemporary
ried on in the absence of other persons, and therefore the
definition of the innermost of these structures is similar,
magnitude and effectiveness of the proximal process are
but contains additional elements that link it to the “cen-
not influenced by another participant’s behaviors. One
ter of gravity” of the bioecological paradigm:
would therefore expect that the person’s own disposition
and resources would play a far stronger role in affecting A microsystem is a pattern of activities, social roles, and
the direction and power of the proximal process than in interpersonal relations experienced by the developing per-
the case of interpersonal interaction. Furthermore, such son in a given face-to-face setting with particular physi-
solo activities significantly change the processes in- cal, social, and symbolic features that invite, permit, or
volved, their outcomes, and the features of the environ- inhibit, engagement in sustained, progressively more com-
ment that become most relevant. The contrast in all three plex interaction with, and activity in, the immediate envi-
domains involves a focus on human relationships, on the ronment. (Bronfenbrenner, 1994, p. 1645)11
one hand, and tasks, on the other. To understand the de-
velopmental importance of this contrast requires a fuller We begin with consideration of the first feature of the
exposition of the features of the environment that influ- environment introduced in the foregoing definition.
ence proximal processes and their effects.
But before turning to this topic, we must give due Effects of the Physical Environment on
recognition to three other Person characteristics that Psychological Development
push us in the same direction. They are so pervasive in
affecting future development that their possible influ- The pioneering work in this sphere has been done by
ence routinely needs to be considered in relation to the Theodore Wachs. In 1979, he published a seminal paper
particular phenomenon under investigation. These are in which he showed a consistent pattern of relationships
the familiar demographic factors of age, gender, and eth- between certain features in the physical environment of
nicity. Another reason for this recommendation is that infants during the first 2 years of life and their cognitive
all three of these factors, although based on differing development over this same period. To permit examining
physical characteristics of the Person, also place that effects over time, data were grouped into successive 3-
person in a particular environmental niche that defines month blocks. The results are reported in the form of
his or her position and role in society. Recognition of correlations between characteristics of the environment
that ambiguity moves us to a change in focus from the at an earlier time and the developmental status of the in-
developmentally relevant characteristics of the Person to fants at a later time.
their counterparts in the structure and substance of From the complex results of the study, we focus on
those physical features in the environment that were
10
In terms of research design, both of the stated formulations most frequently and strongly associated with cognitive
are best assessed through direct observation, but, given the functioning. These included a physically responsive en-
clarity and contrasting nature of the predicted relationship,
valid measures can be obtained for older children and adults 11
The 1979 definition reads as follows: “A microsystem is a
through well-designed interviews, and even for young chil- pattern of activities, roles, and interpersonal relationships
dren from information provided by parents and other family experienced by the developing person in a given setting with
members. particular physical and material characteristics.”
The Microsystem Magnified: Activities, Relationships, and Roles 815

vironment, presence of sheltered areas, “ The degree to in the successive publications of the sociologist Melvin
which the physical set-up of the home permits explo- Kohn and his colleagues (for an integrative summary,
ration,” low level of noise and confusion, and “ the de- see Kohn & Slomczynski, 1990) demonstrating the pow-
gree of temporal regularity” (Wachs, 1979, p. 30). erful effect of work environments on intellectual devel-
Regrettably, few researchers have followed the ex- opment in adulthood. Of particular importance in this
citing scientific path that Wachs has been the first to regard turns out to be the complexity of the task that a
chart. Taken as a whole, his original and subsequent given job entails.
work (Wachs, 1987a, 1987b, 1989, 1990, 1991; Wachs At the conclusion of the preceding section, we called
& Chan, 1986) suggests two areas especially worthy of attention to a contrast that cuts across all four domains
further systematic investigation, in both conceptualiza- of Process, Person, Context, and Developmental Out-
tion and measurement. The first remains strictly in the come. The contrast in all four domains involves a pri-
realm of the physical environment. The second raises mary focus on relationships versus tasks. The findings
the issue of proximal processes as they relate to that of both Wachs and Kohn fall mainly in the latter cate-
environment. gory, but Drillien’s data on mother-infant interaction
In the first domain, Wachs’s findings point to two and infants’ problem behavior in lower-class families
general aspects of the physical environment that can af- fall mainly in the former (i.e., an increase in maternal
fect the course of cognitive development—one for bet- responsiveness functions as a buffer against problems in
ter, the other for worse. On the constructive side are this sphere of emotional and behavioral control).
objects and areas that invite manipulation and explo- But that is not the only effect of rising levels of prox-
ration, whereas instability, lack of clear structure, and imal process.
unpredictability of events undermine the developmental
process. From an ecological perspective, the existence
The Mother-Infant Dyad as a Context
of these countervailing forces in the physical environ-
of Development
ment leads to a new working hypothesis:
A substantial body of research indicates that such
Not only do developmentally generative features of the processes also foster the development of a strong emo-
surroundings have greater impact in more stable settings, tional attachment between mother and child, which
but they also function as a buffer against the disruptive in- increases the quality of future interaction between the
fluences of disorganizing environments. two parties (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978;
Bowlby, 1969, 1973). In addition, the more recent work
The second issue introduces an additional compo- in this sphere strongly suggests that, as a result of con-
nent into the research design. As stipulated in Proposi- tinuing reciprocity in the context of a close relationship,
tion I, proximal processes involve progressively more the infant begins to develop a definition of self that mir-
complex interactions not only with persons but also rors the form and content conveyed through the evolving
with objects and symbols. The question therefore again patterns of interchange between mother and child
arises as to what extent solitary activities involving ob- (Sroufe, 1990). Thus, proximal processes become the
jects and symbols—such as playing with toys, working measurable mechanisms for bringing about what in an
at hobbies, reading, or fantasy play—can also foster earlier era of developmental theory and research was
psychological development? And to what degree does called internalization.12 Moreover, this sequential pro-
involvement in both objects and symbols produce syner- cess does double duty. Though operating primarily on
gistic developmental effects in each domain? The an- the relationship side, it also furthers task performance.
swers to these questions are as yet unknown but are According to attachment theory, the emotionally
readily discoverable through the use of appropriate de- loaded patterns of interchange processes between the
signs that differentiate between measures of process infant and the primary caregiver become internalized
and of environmental structure. in the form of “internal working models” (Bowlby,
However, the most promising terra incognita for re-
search on the role of the physical environment in human 12
A resurgence of theoretical and research interest in this
development may well lie beyond the realm of childhood sphere has been stimulated by the elegant studies of Kochan-
in the world of adults. A preview of this promise appears ska and her colleagues.
816 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

1969, 1973). Such working models are representations these children expect and elicit positive interactions
of the infant in relation to others and become the basis with other social partners.
for the development of the self (Sroufe, 1990). Through This body of attachment theory and research has im-
interactions between the infant and the primary care- portant implications for the bioecological model. Its rel-
giver, the infant develops expectations of the care- evance is most succinctly conveyed in operational
giver’s behavior and complementary beliefs about him- terms, by assessing quality of attachment in different
or herself. For example, an infant who has experienced positions in the research design; for example, as an out-
a history of contingent responsiveness from a primary come at Time 2 of proximal processes at Time 1, or, al-
caregiver will develop a model of that caregiver as ternatively, in the form of strong versus weak contextual
available, and expect such behavior. That child will dyads at Time 1 moderating the power of a proximal pro-
also develop a complementary sense of self that he or cess to influence developmental outcomes at Time 2. The
she is worthy of responsive care. On the other hand, an latter design fits a long-standing proposition derived
infant who has experienced unresponsive care will de- from the bioecological model, which reads as follows:
velop a very different model of the relationship, ex-
In order to develop—intellectually, emotionally, socially,
pecting the caregiver to be unavailable. Such an infant
and morally—a child requires, for all of them, the same
is expected to develop a sense of self as unworthy of re-
thing: participation in progressively more complex recip-
sponsive care. rocal activity, on a regular basis over extended periods of
More generally, these internalized working models time with one or more other persons with whom the child
are seen as providing a framework for future interac- develops a strong, mutual, irrational attachment,13 and
tions, resulting in a repetition of the early attachment who are committed to that child’s development, preferably
relationship (Bowlby, 1973; Sroufe, 1990). The child for life. (Bronfenbrenner, 1989c, p. 5)
seeks, responds, and interprets events based on the
model that he or she has developed during infancy, and A second proposition goes a step further:
that model is adapted based on new experiences with the
The establishment and maintenance of patterns of progres-
environment. A child who has developed a secure at-
sively more complex interaction and emotional attachment
tachment relationship is likely to expect positive inter-
between caregiver and child depend in substantial degree
actions with teachers, and thus elicit responsive care on the availability and active involvement of another adult
reminiscent of his or her caregiver’s behavior. An inse- who assists, encourages, spells off, gives status to, and ex-
cure child, expecting rejection, will approach relation- presses admiration and affection for the person caring for
ships with increased hostility, ultimately resulting in and engaging in joint activity with the child. (Bronfen-
further experiences with rejection. brenner, 1989c, p. 11)
Support for these theoretical expectations comes
from a number of studies. For example, the quality of Taken together, the foregoing propositions present an
the child’s early attachment relationship with the important qualifier to the general finding that children
mother has been found to affect the child’s later func- growing up in single-parent families are at greater de-
tioning in social interactions with teachers and peers. velopmental risk than those in two-parent structures.
Thus, early proximal processes produce proximal What counts most is the quality of the relationships and
processes throughout development. Children judged as activities that take place in the family, and situations
securely attached in infancy have been shown to ap- can occur in which, from this perspective, quality over-
proach unfamiliar peers and adults more positively rides quantity (Hetherington & Clingempeel, 1992).
and with greater acceptance (Booth, Rose-Krasnor, Both propositions take on added importance because
McKinnon, & Rubin, 1994; Main & Weston, 1981; their relevance may extend beyond parental ties to close
Pastor, 1981). Furthermore, they have more positive relationships with other caregivers, relatives, peers,
relationships with peers and teachers in preschool teachers, mentors, coworkers, and supervisors. The
(Sroufe, Fox, & Pancake, 1983; Turner, 1991). Be- propositions may also apply beyond childhood and ado-
cause secure children have developed a positive inter-
nal working model in the context of a secure 13
What is meant by the term “irrational attachment ”? One
attachment relationship with a primary caregiver, answer: This is the first child you try to save in a fire.
Beyond the Microsystem 817

lescence to relationships in adulthood and old age. So held by the different “peer crowds” (e.g., “jocks,
far as we have been able to discover, these possibilities brains, nerds, preppies, or druggies”) in the nine high
still await systematic investigation in correspondingly schools included in their sample. Their subsequent
appropriate research designs. analysis revealed that “European-American young-
sters from authoritative homes are more likely to be-
long to peer crowds that encourage academic
BEYOND THE MICROSYSTEM achievement ” (Steinberg et al., 1995, p. 445).
On the basis of these and related findings, Steinberg
It is a basic premise of ecological systems theory that et al. (1995) formulated the following, new working
development is a function of forces emanating from hypothesis:
multiple settings and from the relations among these
settings. How can such multiple forces and their interre- There is a strong but indirect path between parenting prac-
lations be conceptualized, and what kinds of research tices and adolescent peer group affiliations . . . by foster-
designs can be employed to measure their combined ef- ing certain traits in their children, parents direct a child
fects? The first stage in such an expanded model of the toward a particular peer group. Thus to the extent that par-
environment involves what in ecological systems theory ents can influence characteristics by which adolescents
is called a mesosystem, defined as comprising the rela- are associated by peers with a crowd, parents can “con-
tionships existing between two or more settings; in trol” the type of peer group influences to which their child
short, it is a system of two or more microsystems. is exposed. . . . In essence, parents have a direct and pri-
Mesosystems and their operationalization in a research mary impact on adolescent behavior patterns—prosocial
design are best conveyed through a concrete example. as well as antisocial. Peer groups serve primarily to rein-
force established behavior patterns or dispositions.
Steinberg, Darling, and Fletcher (1995) reported on
(pp. 446–447)
what they described as “an ecological journey,” which
was the consequence of a deliberate decision made at
But when the investigators put their new hypothesis
the outset of their research. The initial focus of inves-
to the test, they were confronted by yet another unex-
tigation was on the impact of authoritative parenting
pected result:
on adolescents’ academic achievement. They had at
their disposal a range of data collected from a large
multiethnic, multiclass sample encompassing several When we attempted to apply this model to youngsters
from minority backgrounds, we were in for a shock. We
family structures. Under these circumstances, they
found that among Black and Asian students, there was no
concluded:
relation between parenting practices and peer crowd mem-
[I]t made no sense at all to control for ethnicity, social bership. (p 447)
class, or household composition in an attempt to isolate
“pure” process. No process occurs outside of a context. Once again, the researchers’ “multiple context model”
And if we want to understand context, we need to take it paved the way to solving the puzzle:
into account, not pretend to control it away. (Steinberg
et al., 1995, p. 424)
Why was there not significant relation between parenting
and peer group selection among minority youth? The an-
No sooner had the investigators embarked on this swer, we discovered, is that models of peer group selection
unconventional course than they encountered some un- that assume an open system, in which adolescents can se-
expected findings. The first of these occurred not in lect into any number of groups as easily as ordering food
the realm of environmental context but of developmen- off a restaurant menu, fail to take into account the tremen-
tal outcome. When they analyzed adolescents’ school dous level of ethnic segregation that characterizes the so-
performance, they found that, in contrast to youth cial structure of most ethnically mixed high schools in the
from European family backgrounds, Hispanic, United States. (pp. 447–448)
African, or Asian American youth did not benefit from
authoritative parenting. A first clue to this puzzle The authors’ findings with respect to specific minor-
emerged when the investigators identified the values ity groups are of considerable interest:
818 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

Although [African American] parents score highest on our Presumably, even an ecological model can only be
measure of parental involvement in schooling, [Black ado- taken so far, but Steinberg and his colleagues appear to
lescents] find it much more difficult to join a peer group be trying to push it to its limits—their next analysis
that encourages the same goal. (p. 449) moves from the parental network of the adolescent’s
peers to the neighborhood’s level of social integration.
By contrast: The measure of integration was based on a series of
questions about parents’ contact with their children’s
More often then not, Asian American students have no
friends, participation in community and social activi-
choice but to belong to a peer group that encourages and
ties, and ties to other families in the neighborhood. An
rewards academic excellence. . . . Asian Americans report
the highest level of peer support for academic achieve- analysis of the data revealed a modest effect of neigh-
ment. Interestingly, and in contrast to popular belief, borhood integration on ado-lescent development. How-
[their] parents are the least involved in their youngsters’ ever, this finding was qualified in an important way that
schooling. (p. 448) refocused attention on the key role played by family
processes. In the author’s words:

The Expanding Ecological Universe When we reran these analyses separately in neighbor-
hoods characterized by a high proportion of effective ver-
As if disappointed at not being confronted with yet an- sus noneffective parents, we find that . . . social
other unexpected finding, Steinberg and his colleagues integration only benefits adolescents whose families live
moved on to extend the ecological model to its next higher in neighborhoods characterized by good parenting. Social
systems level—that of the exosystem. The formal defini- integration into a neighborhood characterized by a high
tion of this environmental structure reads as follows: proportion of bad parents has a harmful effect on adoles-
cents’ school performance and behavior. (Steinberg et al.,
The exosystem comprises the linkages and processes tak- 1995, p. 457)
ing place between two or more settings, at least one of
which does not contain the developing person, but in A subsequent analysis revealed a second, equally
which events occur that indirectly influence processes critical but not surprising qualifier: “Living in a neigh-
within the immediate setting in which the developing per- borhood characterized by a high degree of social inte-
son lives. (Bronfenbrenner, 1993, p. 24) gration is only beneficial to an individual
adolescent if the child’s family is also socially inte-
The particular exosystem that Steinberg et al. (1995) grated” (p. 457).
undertook to investigate was “ the network of families Steinberg et al.’s final analysis adds psychological
that develops through the child’s peer relationships,” substance to social structure. By aggregating informa-
more specifically, “ the parenting practices of their tion on parenting practices and attitudes in a neighbor-
peers’ parents” (p. 450). The investigators’ analyses led hood, he and his associates were able to calculate a
to a series of interrelated findings as shown in these measure of the degree of consensus among parents in a
two examples: given neighborhood. Once again, the principal finding
emerging from the analysis was conditioned by a
Adolescents whose friends’ parents are authoritative earn psychological reality:
higher grades in school, spend more time on home-
work . . . have more positive perceptions of their academic
competence, and report lower levels of delinquency and High neighborhood consensus augments the association
substance use. between parenting and adolescent outcomes only when
Adolescents whose parents are already more authorita- the consensus is around good parenting. . . . In other
tive appear to benefit more from membership in a peer words, it is what parents agree about, not merely whether
network with other authoritatively reared youngsters than they agree, that makes the difference. (Steinberg et al.,
do adolescents in similar networks, but from less authori- 1995, p. 458)
tative homes. It appears that adolescents need certain
“ home advantages” in order to be able to take advantage of In this particular study, the investigators did not ex-
the social capital in their social networks. (Steinberg amine the extent to which the biopsychological charac-
et al., 1995, pp. 452–453) teristics of adolescents, or of their parents, influenced
Beyond the Microsystem 819

developmental processes and outcomes. Today, a grow- mental competence will be greater for those living in
ing body of researchers (e.g., Plomin, Reiss, Hethering- more disadvantaged and disorganized environments.
ton, & Howe, 1994) claims strong evidence for the view
that individual and group differences in a wide range of To test the preceding hypotheses, Bronfenbrenner and
developmental outcomes are mainly driven by differ- Ceci (1994b) reviewed literature on genetic inheritance:
ences in genetic endowment (“Ability Testing,” 1992;
Plomin, 1993; Plomin & Bergeman, 1991; Plomin & We have been able to find no studies of genetic inheritance
McClearn, 1993; Scarr, 1992). This claim is called into in contrasting environments that also contained data on
question, however, by alternative explanations and evi- proximal processes and hence would permit a direct test
of the previous hypotheses. Hence, most of the available
dence based on the bioecological model (see also
evidence is indirect.
Lerner, 1995, 2002, 2004a).
An indirect test can be carried out only when estimates
of heritability are reported for the same developmental
Nature-Nurture Reconceptualized: outcome in different environments. It is fortunate that
A Bioecological Interpretation there are several studies that meet this criterion. To begin
with, both Scarr-Salapatek (1971) and Fischbein (1980)
The theoretical argument is set forth in a series of hy- found support for the prediction that values of h2 for IQ
would be greater in higher than in lower social class
potheses, each accompanied by a corresponding re-
groups. Subsequently, a group of Norwegian investigators
search design (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994b).
(Sundet, Tambs, Magnus, & Berg, 1988) undertook to
clarify a series of earlier findings regarding secular trends
Hypothesis 1: Proximal processes raise levels of ef- over recent decades in heritability for measures of cogni-
fective developmental functioning, and thereby in- tive functioning. Using IQ scores as outcome data, the in-
crease the proportion of individual differences vestigators found some support for results of a previous
attributable to actualized genetic potential for such study of educational attainment (Heath et al., 1985) that
outcomes. This means that heritability ( h2) will be had shown an increase in h2 for twins born after 1940. The
trend for their mental test data, however, was considerably
higher when proximal processes are strong and lower
weaker. The authors offered the following interpretation
when such processes are weak.
of the observed similarity and contrast:
Hypothesis 2: Proximal processes actualize genetic
potentials both for enhancing functional competence This is probably due at least partly to the fact that the
and for reducing degrees of dysfunction. Opera- Norwegian government in the postwar period has of-
tionally, this means that as the level of proximal pro- fered loans to young people seeking education, thus en-
abling youngsters with poor parents to attend higher
cess is increased, indexes of competence will rise,
education. Such factors, together with a more positive
those of dysfunction will fall, and the value of h2 will
attitude toward education among poor people, would
become greater in both instances. tend to decrease the effect of familial environments and
1. The power of proximal processes to actualize ge- maximize genetic potential. (Sundet et al., 1988, p. 58)14
netic potentials for developmental competence (as
assessed by an increase in h2) will be greater in 14
Sundet (personal communication, March 17, 1993) re-
advantaged and stable environments than in those ported that, in response to a preliminary version of the article
that are disadvantaged and disorganized. by Bronfenbrenner and Ceci (1994), he and his colleagues un-
2. The power of proximal processes to buffer genetic dertook a preliminary analysis that yielded the following re-
potentials for developmental dysfunction will be sults: “For twins with mothers having the least education, the
greater in disadvantaged and disorganized environ- correlation between identical twins is .80, whereas the corre-
ments than in those that are advantaged and stable. lation for fraternal twins is .47. For the twins having mothers
with more education, these correlations are .82 and .39, re-
Hypothesis 3. If persons are exposed over extended spectively. As you will see, this yields a heritability estimate
periods of time to settings that provide developmental of .66 for the first group, whereas it is .86 for the second
resources and encourage engagement in proximal group. If I understand your [Hypothesis 2] correctly, this is in
processes to a degree not experienced in the other accordance with your predictions. However, the difference
settings in their lives, then the power of proximal between the two DZ [dizygotic] correlations does not seem to
processes to actualize genetic potentials for develop- reach statistical significance, although it is quite near.”
820 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

There are also a number of investigations that permit investigated the effect of environmental stability and
an indirect test of the hypothesized reverse pattern when change on the development of children through adoles-
the outcome is one of developmental dysfunction. For ex- cence and young adulthood, The “steadiness” versus “ un-
ample, Jenkins and Smith (1990) found that the positive steadiness” of family living conditions was measured by
effect of a good mother-child relationship on children’s
the frequency of such events as the following: the number
problem behavior was stronger in a troubled marriage than
of family moves, changes in day care or school arrange-
in a harmonious one. More generally, in a recent review,
ments, extent of family absence, incidence of divorce and
Rutter and Rutter (1992) concluded that the impact of pro-
tective factors in buffering developmental disorders is remarriage, and altered conditions of maternal employ-
greater in “circumstances of risk.” (p. 56) ment. Greater instability in the family environment was
associated with greater submissiveness, aggressiveness,
This concludes Process, Person, and Context as anxiety, and social problems among children in later
shapers of development, it is time to turn to Time. childhood and adolescence, leading to higher risks of vio-
lence and criminal behavior in early adulthood (Pulkki-
TIME IN THE BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL: nen, 1983; Pulkkinen & Saastamoinen, 1986). Moreover,
MICRO-, MESO-, AND the factor of stability of family living conditions ap-
MACROCHRONOLOGICAL SYSTEMS peared to be a stronger determinant of subsequent devel-
opment than was the family’s socioeconomic status.
Time, a defining property of the bioecological paradigm, Analogous findings for the contemporary U.S. scene
appears more than once in the model’s multidimensional were obtained by Moorehouse (1986) in a study of how
structure. Indeed, its first appearance, in the second stability versus change over time in the mother’s work
sentence of Proposition I, may have well gone unnoticed. status during the child’s preschool years affected pat-
Following the definition of proximal processes as in- terns of mother-child communication, and how these
volving progressively more complex reciprocal inter- patterns in turn influenced the child’s achievement and
action, the Proposition stipulates that to be effective, social behavior in the 1st year of school. A key analysis
the interaction must occur on a fairly regular basis. involved a comparison between mothers who had main-
Why this proviso? A first indication appears in the tained the same employment status over the period of
findings from Wachs’s research (1979) on the features the study, and those who had changed in either direc-
of the environment most frequently and strongly associ- tion: that is, to working more hours, fewer hours, or none
ated with individual differences in cognitive compe- at all. The results revealed that significant effects of
tence. Prominent among them were a physically work status were pronounced only in the group that had
responsive environment, presence of sheltered areas, in- changed their working status. Although the disruptive
stability and unpredictability of events, the “degree to impact was greatest among those mothers who had
which the physical set-up of the home permits explo- moved into full-time employment, it was still present
ration,” low level of noise and confusion, and “ the de- even for those who had reduced their working hours or
gree of temporal regularity” (p. 30). As noted earlier, it had left the labor force. Moorehouse concluded that “in-
follows from such findings that proximal processes can- stability, on the whole, is associated with less favorable
not function effectively in environments that are unsta- school outcomes than stability” (p. 103).
ble and unpredictable across space and time. In the framework of the discovery mode, we are once
It also follows that the cumulative effects at this again at a point where a series of findings from different
mesosystem level are likely to jeopardize the course of studies suggests yet another tentative formulation. The
human development. One reason for expecting such an corollary follows:
escalating effect is that, at this next higher level of en-
The degree of stability, consistency, and predictability
vironmental structure, similarly disruptive characteris-
over time in any element of the systems constituting the
tics of interconnected microsystems tend to reinforce ecology of human development is critical for the effective
each other. operation of the system in question. Extremes either of
The most informative research evidence bearing on disorganization or rigidity in structure or function repre-
this issue comes from a longitudinal study conducted by sent danger signs for potential psychological growth, with
the Finnish psychologist, Lea Pulkkinen (1983). Begin- some intermediate degree of system flexibility constitut-
ning when participating children were 8 years of age, she ing the optimal condition for human development. In re-
Time in the Bioecological Model: Micro-, Meso-, and Macrochronological Systems 821

search design, this proposition points to the importance of ding research designs, and examples of relevant research
assessing the degree of stability versus instability, with findings.
respect to characteristics of Process, Person, and Context, The first principle is that of historical time and place,
at each level of the ecological system. defined by Elder as follows: The life course of individu-
als is embedded in and shaped by the historical times and
This formulation also applies at the macrolevel to the
events they experience over their life time.
dimension of Time, both during the individual’s life
History is exploited as an experiment of nature.
course, and through the historical period in which the
The corresponding research design compares groups
person has lived (see Proposition II ). It was this observa-
similar in other respects who have been exposed, ver-
tion that gave rise to the first systematic formulation of
sus not exposed, to a particular historical event; for
what was to become the ecological model of human devel-
example, Elder’s studies of the Great Depression
opment. The formulation appeared almost 4 decades ago
(Elder, 1974; also see Elder, 1998; Elder & Shanahan,
in an article entitled “Socialization and Social Class
Chapter 12, this Handbook, this volume); military
through Time and Space” (Bronfenbrenner, 1958). In that
service and actual combat in World War II and Korea
article, Bronfenbrenner reanalyzed what appeared to be
(Elder, 1986; Elder, Shanahan, & Clipp, 1994);
contradictory findings on social class differences in pat-
the Iowa farm crisis (Conger & Elder, 1994; Elder,
terns and outcomes of child rearing. The analysis reveals
King, & Conger, 1996); urban inequality (Elder,
that when the obtained results were reorganized by the
Eccles, Ardelt, & Lord, 1995); and, Elder’s most re-
years in which the data were collected, the contradictory
cent work, research on youth sent to the countryside
findings disappeared. Instead, there was a systematic
during China’s cultural revolution (Elder, Wu, &
gradual change over the period just after World War II
Jihui, 1993).
until the late 1950s, with middle-class parents moving
The second principle, called timing in lives, states that
away from originally more authoritarian patterns toward
the developmental impact of a succession of life transi-
greater permissiveness and lower-class families going in
tions or events is contingent on when they occur in a per-
the opposite direction. Changes in patterns of child rear-
son’s life.
ing over historical time and their effects on development
Here an appropriate research design is one that com-
have been recurring themes in Bronfenbrenner’s work be-
pares early versus late arrivals at a particular transition
ginning in the late 1950s (1958) and continuing up to the
with respect to their subsequent life course. For exam-
present (Bronfenbrenner, 1970, 1975, 1990, 1994; Bron-
ple, Elder et al. (1994) reanalyzed follow-up data on
fenbrenner & Crouter 1982; Bronfenbrenner et al., 1996);
subjects from Terman’s 1925 classic Genetic Studies of
but in terms of theoretical and empirical contributions
Genius (all subjects with very high IQs) and were able
this work pales in comparison with that of Elder, begin-
to show marked differences in subsequent adult develop-
ning with his classic study, Children of the Great Depres-
ment depending on early versus late entrance into mili-
sion (Elder, 1974; see also Elder & Shanahan, Chapter
tary service during wartime. Some of the costs of late
12, this Handbook, this volume).
entry include:
As Bronfenbrenner has noted, Elder’s work on life-
course development played a significant role in the formu-
lation of the original ecological model (Bronfenbrenner, • A higher risk of divorce and separation
1979a, see especially pp. 266–285 and 273–285), and has • A work life of disappointment and loss of lifetime income
exerted even greater influence on the model’s subsequent • An accelerated decline of physical health, most no-
evolution in this same domain (Bronfenbrenner, 1986a, tably after the age of 50
1986b, 1989, 1993, 1995).
Because Elder’s contributions deservedly receive ex-
On the opposite side:
tended coverage in Chapter 12, this Handbook, this vol-
ume, we confine ourselves to the four defining principles
of life-course theory as presented in a recent formulation • For many men, and especially those who entered at
(Elder, 1998),15 along with implications for correspon- an early age, military service was a recasting expe-
rience. It provided a bridge to greater opportunity
15
For an earlier, but more comprehensive account, see Elder’s and an impetus for developmental growth up to the
Chapter 16, this Handbook, this volume. middle years.
822 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

One is reminded of Brutus’s fateful choice in re- FROM RESEARCH TO REALITY


sponse to Cassius’s urgings:
The fourth and last of Elder’s principles of life course
There is a tide in the affairs of men development he calls human agency. It states that “indi-
Which, taken at the flood, leads on to fortune; viduals construct their own life course through choices
Omitted, all the voyage of their life and actions they take within the opportunities and con-
Is bound in shallows and in miseries. straints of history and social circumstances.” A striking
—Shakespeare, Julius Caesar (IV. iii. 218–221)
example is his finding that the young men most likely to
volunteer early for service in World War II were often
those who came from the most deprived circumstances,
The third principle, linked lives, asserts that lives
but then benefited the most from the opportunities of
are lived interdependently and social and historical in-
training and experience that the military provided. Nev-
f luences are expressed through this network of shared
ertheless, he cautions that “Not even great talent and in-
relationships.
dustry can ensure life success over adversity without
The basic research design corresponding to this
opportunities” (Elder, 1997).
principle involves examining the differential impact
Finally, to Elder’s four principles, we add a fifth,
of historical events and role transitions on different
which in effect reverses the direction of his very first
members of the same family experiencing these
principle regarding the importance of historical changes
same events and transitions. In a study of mother-
in shaping the course of human development. Simply
daughter dyads in the broader historical context of
stated, the fifth principle asserts that changes over time
the societal changes in gender roles that have taken
in the four defining properties of the bioecological
place since World War II, Moen and Erickson (1995)
model are not only products but also producers of histor-
offered the following concluding comment, on the
ical change. To spell out the argument and evidence on
basis of their statistical analysis of data across two
which the principle is based: Periodically, since the late
generations:
1950s, Bronfenbrenner, together with colleagues, has
been publishing articles documenting changes over time
Conventional mothers embracing traditional gender roles
in three domains: child-rearing practices, the relation of
may find themselves with daughters who are in the van-
guard of the women’s movement. Some mothers may even these practices to child outcomes, and in family demo-
push their daughters to achieve what was impossible for graphics reported annually in the U.S. Census and other
themselves. The fact that mothers and daughters experi- government publications.
ence historical events and social changes from different One report of these analyses appears in a volume en-
vantage points means that their lives are differentially titled: The State of Americans: This Generation and the
touched by them and that their perspectives may well di- Next (Bronfenbrenner et al., 1996). The book consists of
verge. (p. 180) almost 300 pages and 150 graphs, but, for present pur-
poses, the principal findings can be summarized in 10
Environmental changes across historical time can points shown in Table 14.1. Considered as a whole, the
produce significant developmental changes in either findings constitute the basis for our proposed addition
direction. On the one hand, they can disrupt the timing to Elder’s four principles.
and duration of normative transitions during the life To illustrate, although proximal processes function
course, thus interrupting the sequence of learning ex- as the engines of development, the energy that drives
periences that are essential for meeting societal ex- them comes from deeper sources that take us back to
pectations as one gets older. On the other hand, they the experiential world of Proposition I (Bronfenbrenner
can offer to the person new, at once more stable et al., 1996; Bronfenbrenner & Evans, 2000; Bronfen-
and more challenging opportunities that enhance psy- brenner & Morris, 1998). Both subjective and objective
chological growth or even reverse a previously down- forces exert an especially strong influence on develop-
ward course (e.g., Elder’s 1974 studies of effects ment during the formative years (from early infancy to
of military enlistment on young men from poverty young adulthood). A substantial body of research over
backgrounds). the past century indicates that, 2 or 3 decades ago,
From Research to Reality 823

TABLE 14.1 Summary of Selected Findings jobs (in which overtime is increasingly required or ex-
1. Annual surveys over the past two decades reveal growing pected). The nature of this trend and its relevance
cynicism and disillusionment among American youth, ref lected for human development are conveyed in the idea that
in a loss of faith in others, in their government, in the basic to develop—intellectually, emotionally, socially, and
institutions of their society, and in themselves.
2. In the United States far greater percentages of youth and morally—a child requires, for all of these, the same
women are victims of homicide, with rates more than 10 times thing: participation in progressively more complex ac-
as high as those for any other developed country. tivities, on a regular basis over an extended period of
3. The young are not only likely to be the victims of murder, they
are also more likely to commit it. Youth and young adults (ages
time in the child’s life, with one or more persons with
18–25) now account for the majority of those arrested for whom the child develops a strong, mutual emotional
homicide. attachment, and who are committed to the child’s well-
4. The percentage of Americans in prison is four times higher
being and development, preferably for life (Bronfen-
than in other developed countries, and the number is rising
rapidly. brenner & Evans, 2000; Bronfenbrenner & Morris,
5. Despite recent gains made by youth from Black families. 1998; also see Lerner, 2004b). The establishment of a
American high school students are still far behind those from strong mutual emotional attachment leads to internal-
other developed countries in academic achievement. This
includes the top 10% of students in each nation. The trend ization of the parent’s activities and expressed feelings
already threatens our productivity and capacity to compete of affection. Such mutual ties motivate the child’s inter-
economically in the future. est and engagement in related activities in the immediate
6. The United States stands in first place in the percentage of
children growing up in single-parent families, which now
physical, social, and—in due course—symbolic environ-
includes over a quarter of all America’s children under 6 years ment that invite exploration, manipulation, elaboration,
of age. and imagination.
7. Families with children under 6, particularly single-parent
The establishment and maintenance of patterns of
mothers, are those who most seek—and desperately need—
a job. But they also have the highest unemployment rates. The progressively more complex interaction and emotional
proportion of Black mothers working full time is much higher attachment between parent and child depend, to a sub-
than for white mothers (in 1994, 76% vs. 29%). stantial degree, on the availability and involvement of
8. The percentage of U.S. children living in poverty today is
twice as high as that for any other developed nation. another adult, a third party, who assists, encourages,
9. Among developed nations, the incomes of rich versus poor spells off, gives status to, and expresses admiration and
families are farthest apart in the United States. We are rapidly affection for the person caring for and engaging in joint
becoming a two-class society.
10. Two-thirds of children in poverty live in families with working
activity with the child. It also helps, but is not absolutely
adult. Less than one-third of poor families with a young child essential, that the third party be of the opposite sex from
rely solely on welfare. that of the other person caring for the child, because this
We are indebted to our colleagues who, as coauthors of chapters of is likely to expose and involve the child in a greater vari-
the volume The State of Americans: This Generation and the Next, ety of developmentally instigative activities and experi-
provided the findings summarized in Table 14.1. Besides ourselves,
they include the following: Steven J. Ceci, Helen Hembrooke, Peter
ences (Bronfenbrenner et al., 1996). Where this is an
McClelland, Phyllis Moen, Elaine Wethington, and Tara L. White. attachment to two or more parent figures, each can serve
Source: From The State of Americans: This Generation and the Next, as a third party to the other.
by U. Bronfenbrenner, P. McClelland, E. Wethington, P. Moen, and
The research evidence for this idea comes mainly by
S. J. Ceci, 1996, New York: Free Press.
default. It was produced by demographic data document-
ing a rapid rise in the proportion of single-parent house-
these forces lay mainly in the family, with parents act- holds. The trend began in the 1980s, and then continued
ing as the principal caregivers and sources of emo- at an even faster rate through most of the 1990s. The
tional support for their children, and with other adult overwhelming majority of such homes were those in
family members living in the home being next in line. which the father was absent and the mother bore pri-
To a lesser extent, other relatives, family friends, and mary responsibility for the upbringing of the children.
neighbors also functioned in this role. A large number of investigations of developmental
However, there has been a marked change in this pat- processes and outcomes in families of this kind have
tern over the past 4 decades. Parents, and other adult since been conducted across a range of cultural and so-
family members as well, have been spending increasing cial class groups. The findings lead to two complemen-
amounts of time commuting to and working at full-time tary conclusions:
824 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

1. Even in families living in favorable socioeconomic velopmentally disruptive changes taking place in con-
circumstances, children of single-parent mothers, or temporary society.
of fathers, for whom no other person is acting reli- At a more general level, the research findings reveal
ably in a third party role are at greater risk for expe- growing chaos in the lives of families, in child care set-
riencing one or more of the following developmental tings, schools, peer groups, youth programs, neighbor-
problems: hyperactivity or withdrawal, lack of atten- hoods, workplaces, and other everyday environments in
tiveness, difficulty in deferring gratification, poor which human beings live their lives. Such chaos inter-
academic achievement, school misbehavior, and fre- rupts and undermines the formation and stability of re-
quent absenteeism. lationships and activities that are essential for
2. At a more serious level, there is the so-called teenage psychological growth. Moreover, many of the conditions
syndrome of behaviors that tend to be associated to- leading to that chaos are the often unforeseen products
gether: dropping out of school; involvement in so- of policy decisions made both in the private and in the
cially alienated or destructive peer groups; smoking, public sector. Today, in both of these arenas, we are con-
drinking, frequent sexual experience: adolescent sidering profound economic and social changes, some of
pregnancy; a cynical attitude toward work; and in the which threaten to raise the degree of chaos to even
more extreme cases—drugs, suicide, vandalism, vio- higher and less psychologically (and biologically) tolera-
lence, and criminal acts. Most of these effects are ble levels. The most likely and earliest observed conse-
more pronounced for boys than for girls (Bronfen- quences of such a rise are still higher levels of youth
brenner et al., 1996). crime and violence, teenage pregnancy and single par-
enthood,16 as well as reduced school achievement, and,
ultimately, a decline in the quality of our nation’s
Not all single-parent families, however, exhibited human capital (Bronfenbrenner et al., 1996).
these disturbed relationships and their disruptive ef- Thus, we have arrived at a point where the concerns
fects on children’s development. Systematic studies of of basic developmental science are converging with the
the exceptions have identified what may be described as most critical problems we face as a nation. That conver-
a general immunizing factor. For example, children of gence confronts us, both as scientists and as citizens,
single parents were less likely to experience develop- with new challenges and opportunities.
mental problems especially in families in which the
mother (or father) received strong support from other
adults living in the home. Also helpful were nearby rela- THE BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL: A
tives, friends, neighbors, members of religious groups, DEVELOPMENTAL ASSESSMENT
and, when available, staff members of family support
and child care programs. What mattered most was not In this chapter, we have undertaken two challenging
only the attention given to the child—important as this tasks, each an example of science in the discovery mode
was—but also the assistance provided to the single par- with developmental science as its subject matter. The
ent or by others serving in the supportive roles previ- first was to describe a next stage in the evolution of an
ously noted. It would seem that, in the family dance, it ecological theory of human development, first introduced
takes three to tango. more than 20 years ago. The second task was unintended,
But dancing is not the whole story. By the 1980s, the- but nevertheless begun, for this chapter also documents
ory and research in the ecology of human development early steps in the design of a third-generation model.
had documented an accelerating trend toward greater As one of those early steps, we found it necessary to
permissiveness in styles of child rearing in U.S. fami- spell out the requirements for conducting developmental
lies. At the same time, successive scientific investiga- research in the discovery mode. To our knowledge, this
tions had revealed progressively greater developmental is a first effort to do so systematically, and may there-
advantage for strategies that placed increased emphasis
on parental discipline and demand. The interpretation 16
Once again we emphasize that the relationships and activi-
that emerged from analyses of the available data sug- ties in which parent and child are involved can override the
gested that widespread application of these research influence of purely demographic factors such as mother’s age
findings would serve as an effective response to the de- and family structure (p. 1015).
References 825

fore receive—and deserve—more criticism than any The four defining components of the bioecological model
other section in the chapter. But at least readers will should be theoretically related to each other and to the de-
know what criteria we were trying to meet and will have velopmental outcomes under investigation. This means
a basis for assessing the validity of the proposed strat- that the choice of variables to represent each of the defin-
ing properties should be based on explicit assumptions
egy as reflected in the more differentiated theoretical
about their presumed interrelations.
and operational models emerging from the successive
confrontations between theory and data.
This may seem a disappointing conclusion for so long
Among the more promising products of this effort is
an exposition. Perhaps, even more in developmental sci-
the demonstration of the power of proximal processes as
ence than in other fields, the pathways to discovery are
the engines of development, and their systematic varia-
not easy to find. The trails are not marked, there are
tion as a function of the characteristics of both Person
many dead ends, the journey is far longer than expected,
and Context. We have also presented evidence that, in
and at the end, little may be there. What counts is what
accord with specifications of the bioecological model,
one learns along the way and passes on to future explor-
different pathways through space and time lead to dif-
ers of the uncharted terrain. Here are some final ideas
ferent outcomes. In this regard, distinctions between
for those of you whose work will fill the future land-
two types of outcome appear especially relevant: (1) be-
scape of developmental science. At this still early mo-
tween outcomes of competence versus dysfunction and
ment in the twenty-first century, we are left with a
(2) between activities focusing primarily on interper-
troubling question: From the perspective of the bioeco-
sonal relationships versus objects and symbols. A third
logical model, what is the prospect for the future devel-
potentially productive contrast speaks to the question of
opment of our species? The answer to that question lies
who develops and who doesn’t by identifying disposi-
with the willingness of the United States and other eco-
tional characteristics of the Person that are developmen-
nomically developed countries to heed the emerging les-
tally generative versus developmentally disruptive. Two
sons of developmental science. At the moment, it is
additional Person characteristics deemed consequential
difficult to know what the answer will be. The future
for development are also distinguished and illustrated.
could go either way. Given this alternative, it becomes
The first are resources in terms of ability and acquired
the responsibility of developmental science to communi-
knowledge and skill. The second are demand character-
cate such knowledge as we possess, and to do so in
istics that attract or encourage progressively more com-
words that can still find an echo. Here is a first draft:
plex interaction. An analogous taxonomy is proposed for
the quality of environments, accompanied by illustra- In the United States it is now possible for a youth, female
tions of their corresponding differential effects on prox- as well as male, to graduate from high school, or univer-
imal processes and outcomes. In each instance, the sity, without ever caring for a baby; without ever looking
evolving tentative hypotheses derived from successively after someone who was ill, old, or lonely; or without com-
more differentiated formulations based on the bioeco- forting or assisting another human being who really
logical model are accompanied by their operational needed help. The developmental consequences of such a
deprivation of human experience have not as yet been sci-
analogs in terms of corresponding research designs and
entifically researched. But the possible social implications
the findings generated by them.
are obvious, for—sooner or later, and usually sooner—all
The discovery process points also to the scientific
of us suffer illness, loneliness, and the need for help, com-
need and benefit of including, in research designs for the fort, and companionship. No society can long sustain it-
same subjects, two different developmental outcomes self unless its members have learned the sensitivities,
that complement each other. For theoretical reasons de- motivations, and skills involved in assisting and caring for
riving from the bioecological model, likely to be even other human beings.
more productive would be the inclusion in the same re-
search design of two different, but theoretically comple-
REFERENCES
mentary proximal processes.
Finally, in our view, the most scientifically promising Ability testing [Special section]. (1992). Psychological Science,
3, 266 –278.
formulation emerging from the discovery process docu-
Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, I., & Wall, S. (1978). Pat-
mented in this chapter is easily stated, but it is also one terns of attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situa-
that presents the greatest theoretical challenge: tion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
826 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychosocial interpretation. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986b). Recent advances in research on the ecol-
New York: Holt. ogy of human development. In R. K. Silbereisen, K. Eyferth, & G.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of be- Rudinger (Eds.), Development as action in context: Problem be-
havior change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. havior and normal youth development (pp. 286–309). New York:
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Springer-Verlag.
American Psychologist, 37, 122–147. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989a). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta
Beckwith, L., & Cohen, S. E. (1984). Home environment and (Ed.), Annals of child development: Six theories of child develop-
cognitive competence in preterm children during the first 5 ment-Revised formulations and current issues (pp. 187–249). Lon-
years. In Allen W. Gottfried (Ed.), Home environment and don: JAI Press.
early cognitive development (pp. 235–271). New York: Aca- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989b). The ecology of the family as a context
demic Press. for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental
Psychology, 22, 723–742.
Beckwith, L., Rodning, C., & Cohen, S. (1992). Preterm children
at early adolescence and continuity and discontinuity in mater- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989c). Who cares for children? Paris: United
nal responsiveness from infancy. Child Development, 63, Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization.
1198–1208. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1990). Oekologische sozialisationsforschung.
Bergman, L. R., Cairns, R. B., Nilsson, L.-G., & Nysted, L. (Eds.). In L. Kurse, C.-F. Graumann, & E.-D. Lantermann (Eds.),
(2000). Developmental science and the holistic approach. Mah- Oekologische psychologie: Ein handbuch in schlusselbegrif fen
wah. NJ: Erlbaum. (pp. 76–79). Munich, Germany: Psychologie Verlags Union.
Block, J., Block, J. H., & Keyes, S. (1988). Longitudinally foretelling Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Child care in the Anglo-Saxon mode. In
drug usage in adolescence: Early childhood personality and envi- M. E. Lamb, K. J. Sternberg, C. P. Hwang, & A. G. Broberg (Eds.),
ronmental precursors. Child Development, 59, 336–355. Child care in context (pp. 281–291). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-re- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1993). The ecology of cognitive development:
siliency in the organization of behavior. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Research models and fugitive findings. In R. H. Wozniak & K.
Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39–101). Fischer (Eds.), Scientific environments (pp. 3–44). Hillsdale, NJ:
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Erlbaum.
Booth, C. L., Rose-Krasnor, L., McKinnon, J., & Rubin, K. H. (1994). Bronfenbrenner, U. (1994). Ecological models of human develop-
Predicting social adjustment in middle childhood: The role of pre- ment. In T. Husen & T. N. Postlethwaite (Eds.), International en-
school attachment security and maternal style. Social Develop- cyclopedia of education (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 1643–1647). Oxford,
ment, 3(3), 189–204. England: Pergamon Press/ Elsevier Science.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New Bronfenbrenner, U. (1995). Developmental ecology through space
York: Basic Books. and time: A future perspective. In P. Moen, G. H. Elder Jr., & K.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Lüscher (Eds.), Examining lives in context: Perspectives on the
Basic Books. ecology of human development (pp. 619–648). Washington, DC:
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1958). Socialization and social class through American Psychological Association.
time and space. In E. E. Maccoby, T. M. Newcomb, & E. L. Hart- Bronfenbrenner, U. (2005). Making human beings human. Thousand
ley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology (pp. 400–425). New Oaks, CA: Sage.
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1993). Heredity, environment and
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1970). Children and parents (Report of Forum the question “How?”: A new theoretical perspective for the
15, pp. 241–255). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing 1990s. In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture,
Office. and psychology (pp. 313–324). Washington, DC: American Psy-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). Developmental research, public policy, chological Association.
and the ecology of childhood. Child Development, 45, 1–5. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994a). Nature-nurture reconcep-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1975). Reality and research in the ecology of tualized: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101,
human development. Proceedings of the American Philosophical 568–586.
Society, 119, 439–469. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994b). “The Bell Curve”: Are
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977a). Lewian space and ecological substance. today’s “New Interpreters” espousing yesterday’s science? Unpub-
Journal of Social Issues, 33, 199–212. lished manuscript, Cornell University, Department of Human De-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977b). Toward an experimental ecology of velopment and Family Studies, Ithaca, NY.
human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1982). Work and family
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979a). Contexts of child rearing. American through time and space. In S. Kamerman & C. D. Hayes (Eds.),
Psychologist, 34, 844–858. Children in a changing world (pp. 39–83). Washington, DC: Na-
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979b). The ecology of human development: Ex- tional Academy Press.
periments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1983). The evolution of en-
versity Press. vironmental models in developmental research. In W. Kessen
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1981). Die Oekologie der menschlichen Ent- (Series Ed.) & P. H. Mussen (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psy-
wicklung. Stuttgart, Germany: Klett-Cotta. chology: Vol. 1. History, theory, and methods (4th ed.,
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986a, July 23). A generation in jeopardy: Amer- pp. 357–414). New York: Wiley.
ica’s hidden family policy. Testimony presented to the committee Bronfenbrenner U., & Evans, G. W. (2000). Developmental science
on Rules and Administration, Washington, DC. in the 21st century: Emerging questions, theoretical models, re-
References 827

search designs, and empirical findings. Social Development, Elder, G. H., Jr., Van Nguyen, T. V., & Caspi, A. (1985). Linking fam-
9(1), 15–25. ily hardship to children’s lives. Child Development, 56, 361–375.
Bronfenbrenner, U., McClelland, P., Wethington, E., Moen, P., & Elder, G. H., Jr., Wu, W., & Jihui, Y. (1993). State-initiated change
Ceci, S. J. (1996). The state of Americans: This generation and and the life course in Shanghai, China (Unpublished project re-
the next. New York: Free Press. port). Chapel Hill, NC: Carolina Population Center.
Bronfenbrenner U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of develop- Fischbein, S. (1980). IQ and social class. Intelligence, 4, 51–63.
mental processes. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of Child Psy- Garmezy, N., & Rutter M. (1983). Stress, coping, and development in
chology (5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 993–1028). New York: Wiley. children. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Neville, P. R. (1994). America’s children and Heath, A. C., Berg, K., Eaves, L. J., Solaas, M. H., Corey, L. A., Sun-
families: An international perspective. In S. L. Kagan & det, J., et al. (1985). Educational policy and the heritability of ed-
B. Weissbourd (Eds.), Putting families first (pp. 3–27). San Fran- ucational attainment. Nature, 314, 734–736.
cisco: Jossey-Bass.
Hetherington, E. M., & Clingempeel, W. G. (1992). Coping with mar-
Cairns, R. B. (1970). Towards a unified science of development. Con- ital transitions. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
temporary Psychology, 15, 214–215. Development, 57(2/3).
Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways Jenkins, J. M., & Smith, M. A. (1990). Factors protecting children liv-
to youth in our time. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Univer- ing in disharmonious homes: Maternal reports. Journal of the
sity Press. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 60–69.
Cohen, S. E., & Beckwith, L. (1979). Preterm infant interaction with Kohn, M. L., & Slomczynski, K. M. (1990). Social structure and self-
the caregiver in the first year of life and competence at age two. direction: A comparative analysis of the United States and Poland.
Child Development, 50, 767–776. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.
Cohen, S. E., Beckwith, L., & Parmelee, A. H. (1978). Receptive lan- Langer, E. J., & Rodin, J. (1976). The effects of choice and enhanced
guage development in preterm children as related to caregiver- personal responsibility for the aged: A field experiment in an in-
child interaction. Pediatrics, 61, 16–20. stitutional setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Cohen, S. E., & Parmelee, A. H. (1983). Prediction of 5-year Stanford- 34, 191–198.
Binet scores in preterm infants. Child Development, 54, 1242–1253. Lerner, R. M. (1982). Children and adolescents as producers of their
Cohen, S. E., Parmelee, A. H., Beckwith, L., & Sigman, M. (1986). own development. Developmental Review, 2, 342–370.
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 7, 102–110. Lerner, R. M. (1995). The limits of biological inf luence: Behavioral
Cole, M. (1995). Culture and cognitive development: From cross-cul- genetics as the emperor’s new clothes [Review of the book The
tural research to creating systems of cultural mediation. Culture limits of family inf luence]. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 145–156.
and Psychology, 1, 25–54. Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development
Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1994). Families in troubled times: (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Adapting to change in rural America. Chicago: Aldine de Gruyter. Lerner, R. M. (2004a). Genes and the promotion of positive human
Connolly, J. A., & Doyle, A. (1984). Relation of social fantasy play to development: Hereditarian versus developmental systems per-
social competence in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, spectives. In C. Garcia Coll, E. Bearer, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.),
20, 797–806. Nature and nurture: The complex interplay of genetic and environ-
Drillien, C. M. (1957). The social and economic factors affecting the mental inf luences on human behavior and development (pp. 1–33).
incidence of premature birth. Journal of Obstetrical Gynecology, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
British Empire, 64, 161–184. Lerner, R. M. (2004b). Liberty: Thriving and civic engagement among
Drillien, C. M. (1964). Growth and development of the prematurely America’s youth. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
born infant. Edinburgh, Scotland: Livingstone. Lerner, R. M., & Busch-Rossnagel, N. A. (Eds.). (1981). Individual as
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1974). Children of the great depression. Chicago: producers of their development: A life-span perspective. New York:
University of Chicago Press. Academic Press.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1986). Military times and turning points in men’s MacDonald, K., & Parke, R. D. (1984). Bridging the gap: Parent-
lives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 233–245. child play interaction and peer interactive competence. Child De-
velopment, 55, 1265–1277.
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1998). Life course and development. In W. Damon
(Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child Main, M., & Weston, D. R. (1981). The quality of the toddler’s rela-
psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th tionship to mother and to father: Related to conf lict behavior and
ed., pp. 1–24). New York: Wiley. the readiness to establish new relationships. Child Development,
52, 932–940.
Elder, G. H., Jr., Eccles, J. S., Ardelt, M., & Lord, S. (1995). Inner city
parents under economic pressure: Perspectives on the strategies of Marrow, A. J. (1977). The practical theorist: The life and work of
parenting. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 771–784. Kurt Lewin. New York: Teachers College Press.
Elder, G. H., Jr., King, V., & Conger, R. D. (1996). Intergenera- May, M. A. (1932). The foundations of personality. In P. S. Achilles
tional continuity and changes in rural lives: Historical and (Ed.), Psychology at work (pp. 81–101). New York: McGraw-Hill.
developmental insights. International Journal of Behavioral Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of
Development, 10, 439–466. Chicago Press.
Elder, G. H., Jr., Shanahan, M. J., & Clipp, E. C. (1994). When war Moen, P., & Erickson, M. A. (1995). Linked lives: A transgenera-
comes to men’s lives: Life course patterns in family, work, and tional approach to resilience. In P. Moen, G. H. Elder Jr., & K.
health. Psychology and Aging, 9, 5–16. Luscher (Eds.), Examining lives in context: Perspectives on the
828 The Bioecological Model of Human Development

ecology of human development (pp. 169–210). Washington, DC: Sundet, J. M., Tambs, K., Magnus, P., & Berg, K. (1988). On the
American Psychological Association. question of secular trends in the heritability of intelligence test
Moorehouse, M. (1986). The relationships among continuity in mater- scores: A study of Norwegian twins. Intelligence, 12, 47–59.
nal employment, parent-child communicative activities, and the Terman, L. M. (1925). Genetic studies of genius: Vol. 1. Mental and
child’s social competence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cor- physical traits of a thousand gifted children. Stanford, CA: Stan-
nell University, Ithaca, NY. ford University Press.
Pastor, D. L. (1981). The quality of the mother-infant interaction and Tobach, E. (1981). Evolutionary aspects of the activity of the organ-
its relationship to toddlers’ initial sociability with peers. Devel- ism and its development. In R. M. Lerner & N. A. Busch-Ross-
opmental Psychology, 17(3), 326–335. nagel (Eds.), Individuals as producers of their development: A
Plomin, R. (1993). Nature and nurture: Perspective and prospect. In life-span perspective (pp. 37–68). New York: Academic Press.
R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and psy- Tobach, E., & Schneirla, T. C. (1968). The biopsychology of social
chology (pp. 459–486). Washington, DC: American Psychological behavior of animals. In R. E. Cooke & S. Levin (Eds.), Biologic
Association. basis of pediatric practice (pp. 68–82). New York: McGraw-
Hill.
Plomin, R., & Bergeman, C. S. (1991). The nature of nurture: Genetic
inf luence on “environmental” measures. Behavioral and Brain Tulkin, S. R. (1973). Social class differences in infants’ reactions
Sciences, 4, 373–427. to mothers’ and stranger’s voices. Developmental Psychology, 8(1),
137.
Plomin, R., & McClearn, G. E. (Eds.). (1993). Nature, nurture, and
psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Tulkin, S. R. (1977). Social class differences in maternal and infant
behavior. In P. H. Leiderman, A. Rosenfeld, & S. R. Tulkin
Plomin, R., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Howe, G. W. (1994).
(Eds.), Culture and infancy (pp. 495–537). New York: Academic
Nature and nurture: Genetic contributions to measures of the
Press.
family environment. Developmental Psychology, 30, 32–43.
Tulkin, S. R., & Cohler, B. J. (1973). Child-rearing attitudes and
Pulkkinen, L. (1983). Finland: The search for alternatives to aggres-
mother-child interaction in the first year of life. Merrill-Palmer
sion. In A. P. Goldstein & M. Segall (Eds.), Aggression in global
Quarterly, 19, 95–106.
perspective (pp. 104–144). New York: Pergamon Press.
Tulkin, S. R., & Covitz, F. E. (1975, April). Mother-infant inter-
Pulkkinen, L., & Saastamoinen, M. (1986). Cross-cultural perspec-
action and intellectual functioning at age six. Paper presented at
tives on youth violence. In S. J. Apter & A. P. Goldstein (Eds.),
the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Youth violence: Programs and prospects (pp. 262–281). New York:
Denver, CO.
Pergamon Press.
Tulkin, S. R., & Kagan, J. (1972). Mother-child interaction in the
Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (1978). Effects of caregiver behavior on ha-
first year of life. Child Development, 43, 31–41.
bituation rate and self-efficacy in infants. International Journal
of Behavioral Development, 1, 105–130. Turner, P. J. (1991). Relations between attachment, gender, and behav-
Rodin, J., & Langer, E. J. (1977). Long-term effects of a control-rele- ior with peers in preschool. Child Development, 62, 1475–1488.
vant intervention with the institutionalized aged. Journal of Per- Wachs, T. D. (1979). Proximal experience and early cognitive intel-
sonality and Social Psychology, 35, 897–902. lectual development: The physical environment. Merrill-Palmer
Rotter, J. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus exter- Quarterly, 25, 3–42.
nal locus of control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: Wachs, T. D. (1987a). The short-term stability of aggregated and
General and Applied, 80, 1–28. non-aggregated measures of infant behavior. Child Development,
Rutter, M., & Rutter, M. (1992). Developing minds: Challenge and 58, 796–797.
continuity across the life span. New York: Penguin Books. Wachs, T. D. (1987b). Specificity of environmental action as mani-
Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development fest in environmental correlates of infant’s mastery motivation.
and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1–19. Developmental Psychology, 23, 782–790.
Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). Race, social class, and IQ. Science, Wachs, T. D. (1989). The nature of the physical microenvironment:
174, 1285–1295. An expanded classification system. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
Small, S., & Luster, T. (1990, November). Youth at risk for parent- 35, 399–402.
hood. Paper presented at the Creating Caring Communities Con- Wachs, T. D. (1990). Must the physical environment be mediated by the
ference, Michigan State University, East Lansing. social environment in order to inf luence development: A further
Sroufe, L. A. (1990). An organizational perspective on the self. In D. test. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 11, 163–170.
Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to Wachs, T. D. (1991). Environmental considerations in studies with
childhood (pp. 281–307). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. non-extreme groups. In T. D. Wachs & R. Plomin (Eds.), Con-
Sroufe, L. A., Fox, N. E., & Pancake, V. R. (1983). Attachment and ceptualization and measurement of organism-environment inter-
dependency in developmental perspective. Child Development, action (pp. 44–67). Washington, DC: American Psychological
54, 1615–1627. Association.
Steinberg, L., Darling, N. E., & Fletcher, A. C. (1995). Authorita- Wachs, T. D., & Chan, A. (1986). Specificity of environmental ac-
tive parenting and adolescent adjustment: An ecological journey. tions as seen in physical and social environment correlates of
In P. Moen, G. H. Elder Jr., & K. Luscher (Eds.), Examining three aspects of 12-month infants’ communication performance.
lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human develop- Child Development, 57, 1464–1475.
ment (pp. 423–466). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
CHAPTER 15

Phenomenology and Ecological Systems


Theory: Development of Diverse Groups
MARGARET BEALE SPENCER

INTRODUCTION AND RATIONALE: A NEED FOR Inferences, Assumptions, and What Brown v. Board of
NEW THEORY 830 Education Should Have Considered: Contemporary
CONCEPTUAL SHORTSIGHTEDNESS IN THE Implications for Research, Practice, and
STUDY OF DIVERSE YOUTH: CHILDREN OF Theorizing 868
COLOR AND PRIVILEGE 833 Traditional Assumptions, Beliefs, and Values: The
Flaw One: Context Ignored 833 Conceptual Benefits of a Phenomenological Variant
Flaw Two: Racism Ignored 836 of Ecological Systems Theory Framework 872
Flaw Three: General Lack of a Developmental Cross-Disciplinary Contributions from Legal Studies 874
Perspective When Considering Youth of Color 838 AN EXAMPLE OF AFRICAN AMERICANS AND
Summary 843 TH E ACHIEVEMENT GAP: AN INTERFACE OF
STIGMATIZING CONTEXT, REACTIVE
PHENOMENOLOGICAL VARIANT OF ECOLOGICAL
COPING RESPONSES, AND TH E “ACTING
SYSTEMS THEORY: MULTIDISCIPLINARY,
WHITE” MYTH 878
DYNAMIC, SYSTEMIC INFLUENCES, AND
PROCESSES OF A SYNTHESIZED RECURSIVE Historical Overview of African American
FRAMEWORK 844 Achievement Motivation 879
The Foundational Role of Social Cognition and Linkages Black Response to Freedom: A Social Movement
to Phenomenology: Inference-Making, Perceptions, for Education 880
and Youths’ Everyday Experiences 844 RACISM AND HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 883
Systems Framework in an Ecological System 847 CONCLUSIONS AND PHENOMENOLOGICAL
CROSS-CUTTING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT VARIANT OF ECOLOGICAL SYSTEMS THEORY
THEMES OF PHENOMENOLOGICAL VARIANT INFLUENCED INTERPRETATIONS OF YOUTHS’
OF ECOLOGICAL SYSTEMS THEORY: SALIENT STAGE-SPECIFIC COPING OUTCOMES AS A
FOR THE DESIGN OF INTERVENTIONS AND THE FUNCTION OF NET VULNERABILITY 885
INTERPRETATION OF COPING OUTCOMES 853 Conceptual Advantages of Phenomenological Variant of
Importance of Broad Cultural Inclusiveness Ecological Systems Theory as a Dual Axis Coping
and Context 854 Outcome Interpretational Framework of Human
ECOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES: EXPLORING THE Development: Traditional and Limited Dual
BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL 864 Axis Perspectives 886
Ecological Systems Theory 865 Predicting Positive Outcomes in the Face
of Challenge 887
TWENTIETH-CENTURY SOCIAL SCIENCE
ASSUMPTIONS AND PRACTICES, JUDICIAL REFERENCES 888
DECISIONS, AND THEIR IMPLICATIONS FOR THE
NET VULNERABILITY OF DIVERSE YOUTH 868

This chapter’s organization serves to capture the dis- school through adolescence period, introduce PVEST as
tinctive contributions of phenomenological variant of a life-span model of human development. The next sec-
ecological systems theory (PVEST) to available human tion demonstrates why the theory promotes an identity-
development theorizing. Thus, following a brief intro- focused cultural ecological (ICE) perspective. It
ductory section, we provide a full presentation of the reviews (a) the fact of human vulnerability (i.e., the
PVEST framework, and, although applying it to the pre- presence of both risks and protective factors), ( b) the

829
830 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

normative experience of stress, (c) the need for reactive tional advantages of the PVEST framework and high-
coping responses, (d) the role of emergent (stable) iden- lights competing traditional frameworks while also ex-
tities, and (e) the links with the unavoidability of stage- plaining their limitations.
specific and patterned coping outcomes. As lives unfold
over time, stage-specific coping outcomes contribute to
the next period’s level of vulnerability. Thus, we also il- INTRODUCTION AND RATIONALE:
lustrate the dynamic character of PVEST. As a system A NEED FOR NEW THEORY
of experiences, the interpretation of patterned outcomes
(i.e., referred to as culture) are described as having im- An inclusive framework for understanding life-course
portant implications for group experiences (e.g., as development is long overdue. The narrowness of views
stigma, bias, or privilege) or individual supportive mod- concerning inclusiveness have made such a framework,
els for emulation (e.g., character development). at best, an illusive goal for child development. As used
To illustrate the themes noted and as a strategy for here, the term inclusive refers to a perspective relevant
comparing and contrasting the experiences of diverse to the contextual and cultural experiences of diverse
groups of youths, the third section affords an examina- groups. Thus, an inclusive framework suggests an au-
tion of several cross-cutting themes of human develop- thentic rendering of developmental processes for humans
ment established by the recursive character of PVEST, considered diverse as a function of characteristics such
which exemplifies the connectedness between individ- as race, gender, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, immi-
ual characteristics and context interactions. gration status, faith community, skin color, and nativity.
We point out that by diverse youth, we refer to all eth- PVEST (Spencer, 1995) provides a developmental,
nicities and races (i.e., unless we specify—diverse youth process-oriented, and context-sensitive focus. Thus, it af-
of color). This clarification is critically important be- fords unique conceptual contributions to developmental
cause Whites are often not considered under diversity psychology, specifically, and to the field of human de-
labels, given a frequent assumption that they represent velopment, more broadly. As a systems theory, PVEST
the norm rather than one ethnic experience among considers and includes the prior emphases noted along
many. The primary point is that, all groups have distinc- with a combined concern with culture, as lived and ex-
tive histories and responsive traditions that evolve into perienced at multiple levels of the environment, and in-
both unique yet similar cultural patterns. Accordingly, a dividuals’ own perceptions. Similar to Glen Elder’s
major influence described throughout the chapter for di- (1974) sociohistorical analysis of the impact of the
verse youth is the impact of distinctive conditions for Great Depression on human development, the PVEST
each and the role of history. framework acknowledges as a central tenet the historical
The fourth section utilizes the 1954 Supreme Court and contemporary impact of social policy (e.g., the
decision in Brown v. Board of Education for highlighting Brown v. Board of Education decision or the significant
and indicating the impact of policy change and its role in immigration policy shifts) on the social, cultural, and
determining long-term youth experiences generally and political context for the nation’s diverse youth and their
schooling specifically. Additionally, the interactions be- families. Considered together, the theory’s value em-
tween context and culture are presented and the contri- anates from simultaneous attentiveness to (a) multiple
butions of critical race theory (CRT) explored for layers of the environment, ( b) normal human processes
describing the individual-context-process links for that unfold in varied settings with multiple others, (c)
White youth. In parallel fashion, the implications of historical factors and social policy that are associated
White privilege for some and the implications of its ab- with long-standing and contemporary structural condi-
sence for others is analyzed. tions and social relations, and (d) cultural sensitivity to
The fifth section provides an illustration of the recur- the traditional ways the authentic everyday experience
sive processes for African American male adolescents of human development in context is interpreted.
as an example for understanding their stage-specific The specific combination and integration of factors
coping outcomes as a function of vulnerability (i.e., bal- indicated improves our understanding about the how of
ance between risks and protective factors) as demon- life-course human development while it acknowledges
strated from a PVEST perspective. Finally, the sixth both behavioral and genetic perspectives (e.g., Spencer
section of the chapter describes the distinct interpreta- & Harpalani, 2004). Accordingly, as a systems frame-
Introduction and Rationale: A Need for New Theory 831

work that takes into account the individual’s perspec- of perceptual processes in development (e.g., social cog-
tive, or phenomenology (see Rogers’ perspective as re- nition; children’s theory of mind) and reinforces the
viewed in Schultz, 1976), PVEST affords specificity need to integrate a phenomenological perspective (see
about the individual-context-process nexus as suggested Rogers & Kelly as reviewed in Schultz, 1976) to (a) ex-
by Bronfenbrenner’s (1977, 1989, 1992, 1993) ecologi- plain life-course developmental processes and thematic
cal systems theory. The integration imparts significant outcomes, ( b) explicate the role of cultural traditions, (c)
benefits. Foremost, given its linkages to both observed specify the contribution of history ( both for immigrants
and perceived context features, the framework provides and more long-term and indigenous groups; [see John-
enhanced understanding of foundational processes and son-Powell & Yamamoto, 1997]), and (d) indicate the
stage-specific outcomes of diverse groups and individu- critical contributions of context quality for individual-
als (see Spencer & Harpalani, 2004). Specifically, as context interactions. The expanded description of con-
indicated, it enhances the interpretation of disparate ap- text this model allows specifies how the role of historical
pearing developmental patterns and outcomes by expli- factors is important at several levels, and it acknowl-
cating the how of development for diverse groups (a) edges individual differences in levels of vulnerability
sharing what appears to be the same space and opportu- (i.e., the presence of risk and protective factors). The
nities, ( b) attempting behavioral responses to seemingly framework also enhances the interpretation of varied
parallel human development tasks and stage-specific ex- outcomes (i.e., productive and unproductive) experi-
pectations for competence, but frequently (c) demon- enced by diverse group members as each transitions
strating disparate behavioral outcomes in response to across contexts (see Figure 15.1). Additionally, the
myriad challenges. framework focuses on the roles mediating processes play
This chapter is intended to provide an updated de- between vulnerability levels and the coping outcomes in-
scription and explanation of PVEST, first published a dividuals experience as they transition across the life
decade ago (Spencer, 1995). Given the 10-year interim, course. The processes provide illumination as individu-
this update is informed by dozens of empirical publica- als confront both unique challenges and contextual
tions that describe analyses designed to demonstrate provocation (see Lee et al., 2003) while attending to the
components of the systems theory (e.g., Fegley, Spencer, sets of normative life-course developmental tasks ini-
Goss, Harpalani, & Charles, in press; Spencer, Dupree, tially described by Havighurst (1953).
Cunningham, Harpalani, Munoz-Miller, 2003; Spencer, Frequently, when attempting to disentangle and in-
Dupree, Swanson, & Cunningham, 1996, 1998; Spencer, terpret the complexities of the multiple coping trajecto-
Fegley, & Harpalani, 2003; Swanson, Spencer, Dell’An- ries possible, given the normative character of the
gelo, Harpalani, & Spencer, 2002). Additional pub- developmental tasks noted, several important contribut-
lished work utilizes the theory for clarifying conceptual ing factors are usually overlooked. For example, vulner-
themes and relationships (e.g., Lee, Spencer, & ability is often assumed to be the presence of risks only
Harpalani, 2003; Spencer, Dupree, & Hartmann, 1997; without an acknowledgment of protective factors. Worse
Spencer & Harpalani, 2004; Spencer & Jones-Walker, yet, particular outcomes are assumed for specific
2004). Finally, qualitative analyses derived from PVEST groups. For example, unproductive outcomes are tacit
(i.e., either single-method studies or multimethod publi- expectations for marginalized groups (see Figure 15.2).
cations that include empirical demonstrations) provide
helpful illustrations. Multimethod publications augment
and make obvious speculative theoretical linkages by Coping Outcomes
providing the voices or expressed meaning-making of
young people themselves (e.g., Spencer, Silver, Seaton, Net Vulnerability Level Unproductive Productive
Tucker, Cunningham, & Harpalani, 2001; Youngblood &
Spencer, 2002).
Risks Protective
This current and updated version of PVEST is consis- Factors
tent with established associations demonstrated between
young children’s normal cognitive, affectual, and social- Figure 15.1 Deterministic thinking: Traditionally linear
developmental processes (e.g., Spencer, 1982, 1983, hypothesized relationship between youth characteristics and
1985, 1990). It reasserts the important foundational role outcomes.
832 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

Coping Outcomes Coping Outcomes

Net Vulnerability Unproductive Productive Net Vulnerability Unproductive Productive

Risks Protective Risks Protective


Factors Factors

Figure 15.2 Deficit emphasizing: Perspective generally as- Figure 15.3 Creating new inclusive theorizing requires an-
sumed a priori for minorities. alyzing both protective factor presence as well as the nuanced
character of risks (e.g., unacknowledged privilege).

In the first published rendering of the theory, only generally ignored even though they certainly contribute
risks were explicitly indicated, although the buffering to a priori assumptions and inferences made about the
power of protective factors was inferred. However, given vulnerability of individuals and groups. Moreover, they
ongoing readings of the literature, a pattern has emerged. are often overlooked in the conduct of basic develop-
Assumptions of productive outcomes and successes (i.e., mental science. They are not only included here but also
as the standard applied to all) have often been implied for especially highlighted because they represent core per-
those considered to be empowered (i.e., males, middle- spectives and contribute to specific social dynamics,
income people, and generally Caucasians). Even if vulner- which include: (a) social stigma—the disparagement of
ability is acknowledged and coping products are specified groups and individuals based solely on particular person
for privileged youth, there is often a narrow focusing on characteristics (e.g., color, race/ethnicity, immigration
only productive outcomes and success. This results in a status, religion, and social class; see Jaret, 1995); ( b)
degree of conceptual shortsightedness, which produces privilege—the unacknowledged advantaging of some
very deterministic analyses, as suggested by Figure 15.1. that places others at a distinct disadvantage (see Ig-
Another pattern, however, is also evident. For those youth natiev, 1995; McIntosh, 1989); and (c) persistent eco-
perceived as marginal individuals (i.e., African Ameri- nomic and social inequities—the unfair burden and
cans, low-resource persons, Hispanics or subgroups of challenge to specific individuals, families or groups (see
Asian Americans—e.g., Hmong), too much emphasis is Darity & Myers, 1998; Jarrett, 1994, 1995).
often placed on unproductive coping products. Given the When all three are considered together, the combina-
lack of economic viability that often exists in communi- tion suggests the salient contributions of CRT for the ex-
ties where marginalized individuals live, countless protec- plicitly broadened framework. Although these themes
tive factors matter but are often overlooked. Protective have always been implicit in our view of the theory’s
factors as buffers have important implications for overall many strengths, the specific integration and inclusion of
level of vulnerability. Unfortunately, as suggested, protec- CRT allows for a more productive consideration of race.
tive factors are neither assumed nor frequently explored Originating in legal scholarship, CRT explicates how the
for those perceived as marginal. use of policies upheld by laws but shaped by biases and
Identity processes, as either the product of basic tem- perspectives concerning race are not unimportant to the
perament and/or particular socialization experiences, unfolding life course (e.g., Bonilla-Silva, 2001). CRT—
have been shown to serve specific protective functions the perspective acknowledging the historical centrality
(e.g., Spencer, 1983, 1988). Unfortunately, except for and involvement of law in upholding White supremacy
the copious self-esteem studies, they are infrequently in- and associated hierarchies of gender, sexual orientation,
ferred and considered in research and theorizing about in- immigration, faith community, and social class (see
dividual vulnerability level. Thus, as suggested in Figure Crenshaw, Gotanda, Peller, & Thomas, 1995)—illus-
15.3, this chapter and suggested framework also consider trates how law and policy are frequently shaped by race.
the numerous unique and shared protective factors avail- All have important implications for the flaws that con-
able to and accessed by diverse youth. tinue to hamper our research efforts to understand both
Accordingly, several essential factors are emphasized the processes and outcomes, which promote and protect
that allude to both risks and protective factors that are the well-being and development of diverse youth.
Conceptual Shortsightedness in the Study of Diverse Youth: Children of Color and Privilege 833

CONCEPTUAL SHORTSIGHTEDNESS IN Context has also been ignored. This appears particularly
THE STUDY OF DIVERSE YOUTH: relevant, when context creates a source of consonance
CHILDREN OF COLOR AND PRIVILEGE (i.e., inferences of mainly support), or, in contrast, dis-
sonance (i.e., a lack of individual-environment fit). Thus,
Shortsighted and erroneous perspectives about diverse when behavioral outcomes are viewed as problematic, the
youth fall into two major categories. The first major cat- pattern has been to not consider the context, but to infer
egorical error suggests that Caucasian children (i.e., or a relational pattern that localizes the problem in the indi-
assumptions about their development) represent the vidual. As suggested, negative inferences have more
standard of normalcy for all others. This group is most often than not been localized around human development
often the subject of normative studies conducted in de- experiences assumed for youth of color (see Kardiner &
velopmental and social sciences. The second category of Ovesey’s, 1951, Mark of Oppression; Pettigrew’s, 1964,
conceptual errors represents the perceptions about mi- Profile of the Negro American). The position of these
nority status youth or youth of color and their assumed treatises has been consistent in either pathologizing indi-
atypicality. This shortsightedness also includes refer- viduals or positing a style of analysis that associates par-
ences used to describe the group itself: Given the ticular behavioral patterns with specific characteristics
changes in American population statistics, the grouping of settings (see Elliott Liebow’s 1968 classic sociologi-
previously referred to as minorities is moving toward cal statement Tally’s Corner). An acknowledgment that
majority status and—although not widely—has more re- these early and significant social science contributions
cently been referred to as AHAANA (Asian, Hispanic, never placed the burden on a socially constructed context
African American, and Native American). An aspect of is important. Rather, more often than not, problematic
the privileged status associated with Caucasians is that behavioral correlates reported or inferred were linked
even the term diversity or diverse youth, unfortunately, with or credited to individuals who happened to live in
inconsistently elicits images of Whites. Frequently, ref- those contexts. The consistent assumption communicated
erencing diversity suggests a concern with adding mi- was that the pathology of the individuals themselves cre-
norities to a particular issue or task. For example, the ated the untoward character of the settings, however, at
role of diversity task forces is often to highlight issues the same time, enlightened and more sophisticated
that are inclusive of non-Whites. Implicit in the designa- analyses were available.
tion is that, without a gentle reminder, the tradition is to The critical and long-term insights provided by early
ignore non-Whites: Whites are assumed to represent nor- ecological psychologists such as Roger Barker, Herbert
mative development themes, and minorities (or the poor) Wright, and Paul Gump (see Barker & Wright, 1949,
are highlighted when adversity, pathology, deviance, or 1954; Gump & Sutton-Smith, 1955; Wright, 1967), and
problems are of topical interest. later broadened and refined by Urie Bronfenbrenner
In this chapter, we evoke a different approach. We use (see Bronfenbrenner, 1985, 1992, 1993; Bronfenbrenner
the term diverse youth in this chapter to include both Eu- & Crouter, 1983), existed and continue to provide criti-
ropean American (Caucasian) and AHAANA (frequently cal insights. Fieldwork by ecological psychologists in
marginalized) youth. Unlike the narrow and exclusively the United States and England obtained definitive find-
positive connotations associated solely with European ings concerning the reciprocal links between the indi-
Americans (Caucasians) and the more limited (and gen- vidual and context: Conceptual contributions by
erally negative) imagery linked with diverse youths’ of Bronfenbrenner illustrated the indisputable interactions,
color representation in the social sciences generally and and space psychologists such as Joachim Wohlwill
developmental science specifically, we posit a strategy (Wohlwill, 1985; Wohlwill & Heft, 1987) clearly expli-
that acknowledges several major conceptual defects that cated the reciprocal links further. Considered together,
our proposed framework seeks to remedy. given the different though parallel methodological and
conceptual strategies, they quite persuasively linked in-
dividual experiences with context character.
Flaw One: Context Ignored
Relatedly, if the character of context is socially con-
First, as consistently shared (e.g., Spencer 1985, 1995; structed (i.e., given provisions and omissions of support
Spencer et al., 2006), ecology has been frequently viewed as policy decisions and social traditions), then, from a
as a nonissue in the consideration of human development. CRT perspective, policies concerning (a) the owning of
834 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

slaves, ( b) the separations imposed on slave families appreciated, prevalent mind-set by scholars in develop-
(i.e., separating fathers from mothers and children), (c) mental psychology is that macrolevel conditions experi-
the use of immigrants and slaves for free labor (see enced at the micro-, meso-, and exolevels of social
Baron, 1971), and (d) the framed policies and enacted ecologies (i.e., generally reported as sample demograph-
traditions to limit the education of slave children (see ics) do not deserve serious attention. However, enough
Spencer, Cross, et al., 2003) or import immigrant men research, conceptual frameworks, and new theories
only as free laborers had long-term consequences: The demonstrate not if but how much these factors matter in
attendant decisions virtually guaranteed contextual con- the lives of children.
ditions as stigma-producing and peculiar situations that Further, the resistant and limiting shortsightedness
would foster problems for subsequent generations ( both described suggests that when these factors are brought
for those deemed marginal and those—although unac- to conscious awareness through journal policies and fed-
knowledged—deemed privileged). Accordingly, and as eral funding initiatives, the impact of the many levels of
suggested by the examples provided, the observations ecologies described by Bronfenbrenner (1979) and oth-
noted have special import to diverse youth of color and ers persist as merely acknowledged (i.e., as statistically
their families. manipulated control variables), but not as factors that
Frequently, these youngsters’ development is as- impact the questions posed, balanced character of the
sumed to be and studied as if it emanates from a vac- samples (or not), and constructs identified and mea-
uum of experience. Inferences about diverse youths’ of sured. Moreover, there has been a general disregard for
color development associated with broad structural, individuals’ own perceptions and meanings made from
physical, historical, and social contexts as well as inher- the multiple ecologies traversed, contexts psychologi-
ited conditions are overlooked. CRT’s emphasis on the cally experienced (see Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson,
methods by which attitudes about race influence deci- 1995), and limitations explicitly and subtly imposed
sions in the U.S. court system aids in excoriating the (see Chestang, 1972). Perceptions have important impli-
shortsightedness concerning guarantees of equity and cations for emotional reactions and subsequent analyses
equality of experience. However, in the social sciences inferred as human processes that fuel reactive (see
and child development literatures, the unequal condi- Stevenson, 1997) and stable repetitive coping responses.
tions and historical circumstances that diverse youth Patterns of shortsightedness remain for those producers
have inherited, grown up with, and psychologically de- of science following 3 decades of cogent critiques,
veloped in response to are, for the most part, inade- which highlighted major methodological and conceptual
quately considered or totally ignored. For example, shortcomings of the research enterprise (e.g., Banks,
considered as oversights are the numerous manifesta- 1976; Guthrie, 1976). For example, the continuing use
tions of symbolic and structural racism, economic of uneven comparison groups (e.g., low-income diverse
hardships, social inequities, and related barriers that youth compared to middle-income European American
appear to characterize the environments encountered by samples) has implications for the interpretation of find-
children and youth of color. Moreover, their stage of ings and perpetuation of social stigma (see Steele, 1997;
development (i.e., cognition-based meaning-making Steele & Aronson, 1995).
processes) determines the level of sophistication con- Frequently, the conceptual problems described ap-
cerning their own inferences about their situation. Fur- pear to be resistant to change. Even when the problem of
ther, given vulnerability level as a function of cultural unequal samples is not an issue, the measurement of
socialization, parental monitoring, and other available identified variables is frequently insufficient. Identified
protective factors, diverse youth reactively cope in variables and measures lack psychometric integrity
ways that may be either facilitating or exacerbating. across diverse groups, and inferred meanings represent
Protective factors often remain unnoticed in the de- an unaddressed conundrum: Constructs may lack con-
sign and conduct of developmental science, even though ceptual validity (i.e., meanings may not be shared).
an anthropologist would never purport to engage in Often, particular group members’ experiences at the
field-based research without knowing something about microsystem level may vary, given individual-context
the group of interest. This knowledge is assumed to go linked challenges, coping requirements (i.e., the degree
significantly beyond self-esteem knowledge (i.e., the to which social dissonance or consonance is experi-
construct of choice by many social scientists). Unfortu- enced), and reactive coping strategy availability (see
nately, the implicit, conceptually intrusive, although un- Stevenson, 1997). Further, because many research ini-
Conceptual Shortsightedness in the Study of Diverse Youth: Children of Color and Privilege 835

tiatives are well-funded as large multisite, longitudinal culties for adults caring for the very young (Jarrett,
data-gathering enterprises, particularly for young inves- 1994, 1995) through to the middle childhood period
tigators and dissertating students, strong encouragement (McLoyd, 1990). They may be further imperiled by bi-
is provided or incentives are made available for second- culturalism efforts (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton,
ary data analytic options—no matter how ill-conceived 1993). Struggles experienced include those that are en-
the study. Consequently, the predominance of inade- countered not only at home (Boyd-Franklin, 1989;
quate conceptualizations and data interpretations re- Boykin & Toms, 1985) but also at school (Ladson-
mains in the literature for significant periods. Billings, 1994; Lee et al., 2003). Their impact may be
exacerbated by gender when experienced by girls under
Deficit Perspectives and Stigma about Diverse dense and unsafe conditions (e.g., Ladner, 1972). Al-
Youth of Color though such struggles are particularly difficult for
As suggested in Figure 15.2, research that depicts di- males given the more valued status enjoyed by most
verse youth of color’s development in the context of males in the majority of the globe, thus providing high
family, school, community, and broader macrostruc- expectations for all males without necessarily condi-
tural determined conditions continues to be highly tions of attendant supports (Nanda, 1974). Problematic
deficit-oriented. For over a 50-year period, presented structural conditions persist, yet the majority of young
through a deficit-oriented, linear, and deterministic diverse youth of color manage to cope adaptively, al-
conceptual lens, diverse youth of color were shortsight- though their efforts are infrequently recognized and
edly viewed as pathological products of oppression factored into the design of research, selection of con-
(e.g., Kardiner & Ovesey, 1951), where only the nega- structs, specific questions posed, or the interpretation
tive outcomes associated with some of these youth were of findings.
studied. Such myopic tendencies continue to this day. Accordingly, a priori assumptions about diverse
The resiliency—health and mastery demonstrated in youth of color infer risks alone without also emphasizing
high-risk environments—associated with many youths’ and exploring the presence of protective factors. More-
coping efforts remains generally ignored. As noted in over, the frequent focus on risks and their linkages to
Figure 15.2, the monolithic perspective about youths’ problems leads social scientists to assume only unpro-
experiences is not only deterministic in nature but also ductive and homogeneous outcomes and labels such as
ignores the resilience of those who do succeed in spite castelike minorities (see Fordham & Ogbu, 1986; Ogbu,
of tremendous barriers. Given recent discourse on the 1985; and critiques by Spencer, 2001; Spencer, Cross,
topic of resiliency, the penchant to ignore resiliency et al., 2003; Spencer & Harpalani, under review). The
may be an effort to underemphasize the contributions of shortsighted perspective and concomitant assumptions
socially constructed conditions that privilege some and fuel deterministic assumptions, which may reinforce
undermine others (see Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000), thus, stereotypes (see Figure 15.2). Although infrequently
making particular diverse youths’ developmental tasks acknowledged for privileged youngsters (i.e., middle-
difficult, at best, or a double quandary, at worst (see income Caucasians), all humans are vulnerable at some
Boykin & Ellison, 1995). level (i.e., burdened with risks and buoyed by protective
For some, perhaps a triple quandary is more accurate factors). However, as suggested previously in Figure
when the special burden of gender is added and consid- 15.1, the degree of vulnerability can be determined only
ered for African American male youths’ experiences from an appreciation of both risks and protective fac-
(Boykin, 1986). In addition to the chronic struggles that tors. A narrow assessment of high vulnerability that is
are linked to race, ethnicity (see Allen, Spencer, & associated with socially constructed risk conditions but
Brookins, 1985; LaFromboise, 1988; Spencer, 1990; which frequently ignores protective factors epitomize
Spencer & Markstrom-Adams, 1990; Spencer & Dorn- poor science. There is a specific need to also understand
busch, 1990), and immigration status (Huang, 1989; available protective factors to achieve an accurate un-
Liu, Yu, Chang, & Fernandez, 1990), socioeconomic derstanding and assessment of vulnerability (Luther &
challenges frequently remain. Importantly, these chal- Becker, 2002; Luther & Latendresse, 2002).
lenges appear endemic, persistent, and are associated An analogy from the pharmaceutical sciences is eas-
with parenting conditions (i.e., work, housing and ily understood because the public would not use, or the
neighborhood adequacy, stable relationships, and par- medical establishment designate, a drug regimen without
enting time availability); they also present unique diffi- also understanding the other “ knowns” or protective
836 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

factors concerning its intended use. For example, infor- linked narrowly and often solely with untoward expecta-
mation related to safety and protection might include tions). Figure 15.1 noted another deterministic pattern
(a) establishing the number of milligrams of the drug’s used in the consideration of European American youths’
active ingredient per kilogram of the user’s body experiences.
weight, ( b) possessing no known allergies to the drug, As reported, the prevalent although simplistic per-
(c) not taking other drugs known to cause untoward in- spective noted in Figure 15.2 leaves the analogous issues
teractions with the drug intended for use, and (d) lack- ignored among European American youth (e.g., teen
ing the presence of concomitant medical conditions for pregnancy and abortion use, White male aggression, and
which the drug would be counter-indicated (e.g., a preg- high suicide rates). Further, when pursuing remedies for
nant woman drinking alcohol during the pregnancy). enhancing productive youthful outcomes, the situation
This illustration is helpful given scientific knowledge described (i.e., overlooking protective factors) for di-
concerning the teratogenic influence of many over the verse youth of color leaves practitioners with the narrow
counter, nonprescription drugs (e.g., aspirin and motion and erroneous assumption that untoward outcomes can
sickness remedies) on prenatal and neonatal develop- be remedied from a one-size-fits-all perspective. As
ment (Note: Teratogenic substances are those found to suggested by Figure 15.3, assumptions about inherent
have an adverse impact on fetus and infant health, protective factor presence and a total absence of ac-
growth, and development). Thus, it is not a new perspec- knowledged risks (i.e., in the case of privileged youth)
tive to undertake thorough understandings about individ- may exacerbate vulnerability level. Examples from Lit-
ual and group vulnerability in some fields. This is done tleton, Colorado, manifested by the Columbine High
by contemplating both anticipated risks and protective School killings by upper middle-income European
factors. This procedure appears relevant irrespective of American youths, clearly demonstrate the downside of
a specific issue (e.g., applying for a job, contemplating a privilege and reaffirm the salience of adaptive coping
vision screen for contact lenses or laser surgery, consid- skills for promoting healthy identity processes and
ering a medicinal regimen for health maintenance, or stage-specific competence (refer to Figure 15.3). Un-
contemplating marriage). Curiously so, and as illus- doubtedly, the appearance of resilience among low-
trated, a decision-making style that bears in mind both income, immigrants, and minority youth is not well-
risks and protective factors appears less prevalent when studied and is often misunderstood or overlooked (e.g.,
inferences concerning race, ethnicity, and socioeco- Fordham & Ogbu, 1986) as illustrated by Ogbu’s “act-
nomic disparity themes are at issue. ing White” assertions concerning patterns of under-
Unfortunately, for diverse youth of color, simplistic achievement for some African American and Hispanic
analyses and dependence on risk characteristics are youth. Unfortunately, given its broad presence in the
most often the norm rather than the analytic exception. popular media, Ogbu’s (1985) limited perspective fre-
As suggested, the approach is not only ultimately quently reinforces stigmatizing and stereotyping beliefs
stereotype-dependent but also tends to produce or rein- without affording an understanding of the outcomes and
force further stigma and stereotyping. In presenting al- mediating processes. As suggested, this penchant is
ternative and inclusive theorizing, we propose an most evident when matters of race, ethnicity, and social
improved and nuanced analysis. The conceptual strategy class are at issue. CRT would suggest that assumptions
described suggests improved interpretations of dis- concerning each are frequently made and impair the pos-
parate findings frequently obtained for youth who ap- sibility of equitable treatment.
pear to share family, school, and neighborhood contexts.
Additionally, a more analytic approach that recognizes,
Flaw Two: Racism Ignored
incorporates for consideration, and specifically eluci-
dates the role of protective factors in youths’ lives might Unaddressed contextual and psychological issues such as
facilitate improved understandings about high-risk pres- racism and class inequities may potentially impact child
ence along with patterns of positive coping outcomes and adolescent perceptions of self, others, and decision
(i.e., resiliency). The suggested approach might aid the making about the current coping responses expected and
identification of culture-sensitive and context-linked required given normative and unique challenges; also in-
supports and remedies. In sum, for youth of color, social fluenced are preparations for future life prospects. In
scientists too frequently err on the side of negative, lin- parallel fashion, although there are a few recent excep-
ear, and deterministic thinking (i.e., high risks are tions (e.g., Luthar, 2003; McIntosh, 1989), beliefs about
Conceptual Shortsightedness in the Study of Diverse Youth: Children of Color and Privilege 837

privilege (or its absence) are seldom acknowledged and mental, and countless challenges that accompany youths’
considered in the conceptualization and conduct of re- pursuit of competence in response to stage-specific de-
search on European American youth, while behaviors velopmental tasks (see Havighurst, 1953). Moreover, the
such as off-time sexual activity, behavior problems, un- lack of an authentic identity that is linked to stable cop-
derachievement, and aggressiveness are emphasized for ing processes may have implications for the character of
youth of color. contexts enjoyed (or not) by others; this is particularly
As stated previously, endemic and long-term important given the universal life course need to cope ef-
economic and social barriers are ignored frequently, fectively with stage-specific challenges and to maintain
leaving to be inferred that all problems are inherent balance, even when not generally expected. Even after
in the individual. Further, and most important, the “objective needs” have been satisfied, Robert White elo-
unacknowledged social consonance (i.e., optimal quently describes the importance of “effectance” moti-
individual-context fit) frequently experienced by Euro- vation in responding to the need to have an impact on the
pean Americans, given race-associated privileges, are outside world (i.e., to demonstrate competence; R.
imposed as the norm and expressed as the outcomes ex- White, 1959, 1960). Such a need has also been described
pected for all. Although for youth of color, supports af- as personal causation (see DeCharms, 1968). In sum, vo-
forded from protective factors (e.g., prior cultural litional history matters and a history of effective coping
socialization experiences including church engagement has implications for life-course demonstrated compe-
and very close parental monitoring) and manifested in- tence given numerous unique and normative challenges.
dividual or family level resiliency factors evident are in- An important challenge for many youngsters has been
frequently assumed. Critically important, the social significant changes in family structure (e.g., increase in
construction of inequality through discounting the in- two-parent working families for European Americans
equities noted within and between groups is infrequently and fewer three-generational family systems for youth
recognized. They are not only unacknowledged but also of color).
often overlooked in the actual design of research and Hetherington and Kelly (2003) and others have been
programming efforts. Worse, they are ignored in the prolific in describing the impact of divorce as a source of
evaluation of programs and attendant social policy de- challenge for European American youth. Inferred is that
velopment. Published review papers often list egregious a lack of experience with challenges and stress may have
scientific shortcomings (see Fisher et al. 2002); how- implications for the development of support-accessing
ever, the challenges implied for their continuing pres- abilities and the acquisition, honing, and exercise of ad-
ence and utilization in the making of public policy are equate coping skills. Although generally not addressed
seldom actually acted on as sanctions—when consider- in youth development studies, a consistently elevated
ing “ what ” gets funded, which research strategies are pattern of completed suicide rates for European Ameri-
modeled, why there is a continuing general absence of can males has not provoked the cause for alarm that the
research professors of color in academia, and how stu- statistics and their stability over decades actually de-
dents are taught to understand the issues. Not only do serve. Given the economic, social, and developmental
programs of research and attendant publications con- specific supports particularly available for European
tinue to ignore the conceptual concerns identified thus American males irrespective of social class versus youth
far but also fail, as indicated, to consider the special rel- of color (see Sullivan, 1989), broad research efforts fo-
evance for White youths’ developmental experience. cused on understanding this pattern’s etiology failed to
The issue of privilege is foundationally important par- solve the conundrum. As tallied across 5 decades in half-
ticularly to the human development of Western youth. decade analyses by Carroll and Tyler (2001) for the pe-
The absence of dissonance or appearance of maximal and riod 1940 to 1989, there are no differences for the 25- to
patterned social consonance (i.e., maximized individual- 34-year age range between Black and White males. This
context fit) may compromise the normal development of period represents the highest peak for Black young adult
important coping skills (i.e., the handling of normative males, however, suicide rates begin sooner and continue
concerns and the responses to unique challenges) and to increase through age 85 for European Americans. At
healthy identity processes among these youths. Positive that age, the average reported rate indicates three times
coping skills are important life-course acquisitions be- the rate evidenced in the early adult years. When analyz-
cause they provide psychological protection and stability ing the data trends, Carroll and Tyler (under review) sug-
across time and place given the unavoidable, develop- gest a gender by race interaction in the maladaptive
838 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

response to stress. At the same time, even though Euro- for youth of color. Alternatively, for European Ameri-
pean American males continue to enjoy the most lucra- can youth, the concomitant stress and challenges associ-
tive life-course averages, when earnings and number of ated with unacknowledged privilege and its downside
years of education completed are considered over the (e.g., inadequate coping opportunities that may be
several decades of the life course, these males have a linked with high rates of male suicides), unfortunately,
surprisingly high suicide rate. Although, attention has are seldom integrated into conceptual formulations of
been paid to the change in peak level of completed sui- development. Certainly, as indicated, the implications of
cides for Black young adult males (see Joe & Kaplan, the latter for the development of coping skills remain an
2002), the lack of anxiety or explanation for the parallel oversight in child psychology. The absence of publica-
lines of data for European Americans when compared tions on the downside of privilege is clearly demon-
against average life income, cumulative education strated when considering the several examples of
achieved, and completed suicides considered together school-based killings. These occasions are made similar
and examined over several decades is cause for alarm. given the unexpected demographics of the perpetrators;
one of the more recent examples, as noted, was the
Flaw Three: General Lack of a Developmental killings of fellow students at Columbine High School by
Perspective When Considering Youth of Color three youth from economically well-endowed families
and communities that were unaware of the threat within.
As indicated, the developmental literature dispropor-
tionately focuses on the experiences of European Amer- Domains of Human Development and Competence
icans. Alternatively (as suggested by Figure 15.2), Social science research with youth often lacks both cul-
theorizing about youth of color frequently underexam- tural understandings (see Lee et al., 2003) and aware-
ines developmental processes and overemphasizes risk ness of the af fective component in cognitive functioning
factors and unproductive stage-specific outcomes (e.g., generally (e.g., see DeCharms, 1968) and social compe-
early pregnancy, disproportionate incarceration rates, tence formation specifically (i.e., effectance motiva-
school failure, and aggression). The thematic patterns tion; R. White, 1959, 1960). The shortcomings are
are both interesting and troublesome because the nor- evident both in the design and the interpretation of
mative developmental thematic stressors experienced by scholarship on youth of color. Similarly, a general over-
youth of color, such as off-timed physical and social sight of cultural inclusiveness, including lived privilege
maturation and peer pressure issues, may be com- as culture as experienced by White youth, is lacking.
pounded by context character factors that are associated Minority children and adolescents developing in con-
with racial stigma and color-linked traditions that texts defined by a unique family structure and sets
assume equality of experience and exposures (see of cultural practices are (a) generally not well under-
Spencer, 1985; Spencer & Dornbusch, 1990; Spencer & stood, ( b) behaviorally misinterpreted, (c) burdened
Markstom-Adams, 1990). Normative child and adoles- with demonizing assumptions, and (d) frequently absent
cent developmental processes, such as early maturation, from developmental science concerned with normative
when considered in conjunction with encounters of race human processes. The oversights have implications for
and economic disparities (e.g., lack of job availability schooling experiences (i.e., both for the training of
for parents, under-funded schools, unsafe neighbor- teaching and administrative professionals), and the
hoods, inadequate schooling opportunities, and unac- general context-linked socialization experiences of rele-
knowledged racism) suggest inordinately stressful vance for competence formation and human develop-
contexts for achieving healthy normative social experi- ment more broadly. To sum, conceptually inclusive
mentation opportunities necessary for development. approaches to developmental science have important so-
Such conditions would be expected to have implications cietal implications regarding how policies are framed
for phenomena such as off-timed pregnancies, juvenile and implemented, how contexts are structured, and how
justice contact, and underachievement in school. Yet, socializing adults (e.g., teachers, administrators, and
apart from a few exceptions (see Spencer et al., 2001), service providers) actually deliver the supports intended
these perspectives are seldom acknowledged in publica- for youth of color. Good science representative of the
tions on the topic. In short, developmental, process-ori- breadth of human experiences facilitates the best prac-
ented, and context-linked analyses are not emphasized tices and policies. Accordingly, needed are viable and
Conceptual Shortsightedness in the Study of Diverse Youth: Children of Color and Privilege 839

broadly inclusive approaches to child development for Inferred or intended bias may serve both conscious
use in maximizing the best possible youth outcomes. and unconscious roles in human relations. Bias repre-
sents frequently unacknowledged adverse beliefs about
Need for Inclusive Approaches to particular individuals and is often experienced as the
Human Development invisible elephant in the room in that it potentially cre-
Particularly during childhood and adolescence, exposure ates extreme emotional discomfort. The emotional re-
to social factors experienced as challenges (e.g., social sponse may be apparent and different for those either
stigma)—particularly those creating bias (i.e., whether recognizing its presence or feigning its absence. Im-
inferred collectively by the group or experienced portantly, as described by Chestang (1972), in either
individually)—are important. Further, they have explana- case, the emotional reaction to the experience poten-
tory relevance for interpreting individuals’ stage-specific tially compromises stage-specific character formation
outcomes. For example, the function and experience of (i.e., as either specifically patterned behavioral re-
negative teacher perceptions experienced by youth help sponses or developmental processes more generally).
explain performance disparities documented both be- In many ways, given gradually emerging cognitive
tween and within diverse groups (see Spencer, 1999a; schemas beginning at infancy, the emotional response
Spencer & Harpalani, under review; Steele, 1997). For a may be not unlike the experience of marasmus (failure
significant length of time, inferences about teachers have to thrive syndrome). For infants, this syndrome sug-
aided the interpretation of gender differences in achieve- gests an expressive response to inadequate emotional
ment findings for European American students (see or physical care. Alternatively, when applied to the ex-
Dweck, 1978). Dweck’s research findings suggest that in perience of adults with highly vulnerable infants,
the primary grades, European American female students Kennell, Trause, and Klaus (1975) have described the
tend to internalize failure and externalize success. The difficulties encountered in emotionally bonding with
opposite pattern has been observed for elementary school infants deemed terminally ill and not expected to sur-
boys who learn early on to internalize success and exter- vive; in such cases, interventions are needed for maxi-
nalize failure. The findings are in keeping with observa- mizing infant-caregiver bonding. Accordingly, when
tions that boys and girls have very different school considering social relations between and among adults
experiences (Irvine, 1988). and children, the presence of bias may not only serve as
Because elementary teachers are often female, a source of negative stereotyping and shame for some
Dweck’s (1978) findings suggest that female teachers but also, as described by McIntosh (1989), may afford
may be more accurate when anticipating and evaluat- disproportionate protective factors (i.e., conceptual-
ing the behaviors of girls. Alternatively, boys tend to ized as privilege) for others. Thus, barring a few excep-
learn that elementary school teachers are in error at tions (e.g., Spencer, Brookins, & Allen, 1985), the
least half of the time when providing evaluative feed- dilemmas of individual-context consonance for some
back to them. Dweck (1978) suggests that boys (e.g., the privileging experiences afforded some Euro-
compensate by “discounting” the feedback and “cele- pean American youth) and the psychological experi-
brating” their successes by crediting only themselves. ence of social dissonance for others (i.e., more often
However, although not included in her study, other than not, experiences of youths of color; see Chestang,
conceptual analyses would suggest a quite different 1972) are not equivalent. The conundrum remains inad-
pattern if investigating the experiences of male youth equately integrated and considered in psychology’s
of color (see Irvine, 1988). These youngsters have the mainstream journals and for most theorizing about
added task of sorting out the contribution of teachers’ human development particularly for diverse youth,
racial attitudes and bias that become intertwined with specifically those of color (see Spencer, 1999). This
academic evaluation and behavioral feedback. Minus a conceptual shortcoming (i.e., of interpretation and ulti-
few exceptions (e.g., Dweck, 1978; Ladson-Billings, mate planned support as social policy and innovative
1994), student characteristics as linked with feedback program design) has implications for generalized
patterns, unfortunately, are infrequently and not seri- human development enhancement, basic human rights
ously considered as part of the context for evaluating infractions, and social justice. Specifically, when de-
the competence and character of youths’ social scribing diverse populations, the oversight has salient
relations. implications for the design of precise policy, the
840 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

incentive structure in support of best practices, the in- support may be different as a function of privileged in-
terpretations made of evaluation study outcomes, the dividuals’ level of personal vulnerability, which, histor-
character and process of juvenile justice system-based ically, may not have been an issue if maximized support
experiences and consequences, and decision-making had remained the standard. However, with the standard
processes about basic programs of research to be sup- of support, perhaps, being lowered to a level different
ported (e.g., the actual types and sets of questions from individual history, individual vulnerability charac-
posed). The specific points of view espoused in this teristics might provide ways of explaining expected dif-
chapter support greater conceptual inclusiveness that ferences. Discussion and exploration of such
goes beyond the mere inclusion of diverse samples in a within-group variation is infrequently addressed for
study’s design or program of research. As suggested, Caucasian youth. Variation of responses to challenge for
good human development theorizing representing real youth of color is generally not sought and perhaps is un-
time and valid human development experiences of dermined by the very design of research studies (e.g.,
diverse groups and their members serves important given the frequent comparison of middle-income Euro-
interpretational and decision-making functions. As a pean American youth with low-income youth of color).
strategy, it impacts the quality of child development Accordingly, when considering either within- or
and youth outcomes through implicit social engineering between-group differences, an individual’s accrued
as social policy. Such a strategy provides ways of artic- vulnerability represents the balance between risks and
ulating challenges and specifying precise supports re- available protective factors. On the one hand, risk
quired for facilitating best practices (i.e., as policy and types have implications for the character of challenges
application) while at the same time altering deleterious that one experiences or anticipates. On the other hand,
contexts that interfere with the promotion of produc- protective factors may vary, ranging from the intergen-
tive coping methods, which maximize the various ex- erational sharing of cultural traditions to unusually
pressions of positive youth outcomes as reactive coping high levels of accumulated wealth. Obviously, both
practices and stable healthy identities. would demonstrate different manifestations as each
Particularly for highly stigmatized groups, within- serves as a source of support. Further, it is important to
group variation is seldom acknowledged and, instead, acknowledge that both types of protective factors (e.g.,
homogeneity is often assumed. In contrast, between- cultural socialization versus intergenerational trans-
group differences frequently serve to further marginal- mitted accumulated wealth) have important implica-
ize or stigmatize outliers (i.e., those performing tions for both the specific nature of anticipated
significantly outside the mean). The sources of variabil- supports available and their character (e.g., stability
ity observed both within and between groups are broad and persistency or internal versus external origin).
and may be due to multiple human characteristics or Risks and protective factors may take a variety of
contextual experiences that operate as processes and forms given variations in race/ethnicity, gender, faith
mediate between an individual’s vulnerability level and community, body type, immigration status, skin color,
stage-specific coping outcome. For example, high per- privilege, health quality or disability status, cultural tra-
formance associated with privilege may be a valid ex- ditions, social class, and temperament. All are linked to
planation for some; alternatively, high performance for the character of the context and the individual’s history
youth of color may be due to significant levels of adap- of experiences and even the group’s history in the nation
tive coping and resilience. The implications of each may (e.g., generational experiences of immigrants as newcom-
be different when individuals of each type are con- ers and long-term adaptations of indigeneous groups such
fronted with similar challenges and equivalent supports. as Native Americans, Hispanic, and African American
Resilient youth may get better scaffolding from extra youth and families). When experienced as challenges or
supports, already have a history of dealing with chal- supports, the sources of variation within or between
lenges, and thus, possess well-developed coping skills. groups may be biologically inherent (e.g., due to the heri-
However, the same level of challenge for privileged tability of temperament, skin color, or body type) or so-
youth may be more disconcerting than expected because cially constructed (e.g., beliefs and biases concerning
a history of privileging experiences would not afford race/ethnicity, social class, and physical attractiveness).
similar opportunities for the developing of multiple cop- When considered together, as suggested, the noted basis
ing strategies. Similarly, responses to a standard level of of variation represents potential sources of risk or protec-
Conceptual Shortsightedness in the Study of Diverse Youth: Children of Color and Privilege 841

of youths’ cognition dependent perceptions that supports


Coping Outcomes the acquisition of unavoidable awareness of their own (i.e.,
assumed internal states) and social others’ perspectives.
Net Vulnerability Unproductive Productive For example, research on children’s awareness of race as a
social category (i.e., understandings or knowledge about
Risks Protective race) assessed frequently as racial attitudes and prefer-
Factors ence behavior demonstrate the complexity of youthful
meaning-making when cognition-dependent awareness is
? also considered (e.g., see Spencer, 1982, 1983, 1985;
Spencer & Markstrom-Adams, 1990). Specifically, the in-
sight affords greater understanding about the unfolding
processes of child and adolescent cognitive, social, and af-
fective capacities when linked with context-linked chal-
lenges (e.g., race biased treatment), given varying degrees
Figure 15.4 Non-deterministic theorizing: Acknowledges of human vulnerability (i.e., countless levels of race-
intervening mediating/moderating processes thus illustrating
associated contextual risks versus privilege-associated,
potentially diverse outcomes given unique individual-context-
process interactions. Caucasian-linked protective factors). Characteristics of
the context observed as numerous patterns of stable condi-
tions and the inferred meanings made about them are not
tion that are associated with some level of human vulner- lost on youth. Accordingly, PVEST as a context-linked
ability. The actual character of individual vulnerability is systems framework goes beyond merely linking human
unavoidably associated with the nature of the context and vulnerability (i.e., risk considered against protective fac-
its provision of either excessive challenges or supports. tors) linearly and deterministically with stage-specific
The level of vulnerability has implications for achieving coping products. Moreover, when considering stage-
stage-specific manifested competence (White, 1959, specific developmental tasks that are experienced as chal-
1960), resiliency (Anthony, 1974, 1987; Luthar, 2003), or lenges, the framework also acknowledges and specifies
under-development. In addition, and as described in Fig- the breadth of available supports possible; at the same
ure 15.4 as an example of nondeterministic theorizing, time, it recognizes the role of supports for productive
generally ignored or inadequately recognized (i.e., par- stage-specific coping products (see Figure 15.5).
ticularly as it relates to child development and adolescent The framework also focuses on the mediating/
experiences) is an emphasis and acknowledgment of the moderating function of net-stress responses and
individual’s awareness of (cognition-dependent) percep-
tions and stage-specific meaning-making processes.
Net Vulnerability Stage Specific Coping
Mediating Processes between Vulnerability and Product
Coping Outcomes
Risks Protective Unproductive Productive
From our conceptual perspective (as illustrated in Figure Factors
15.4), we hypothesize that intervening processes between
vulnerability level and coping outcomes go beyond an
OR
understanding of the “what” of the individual-context re-
lationship. A focus on the mediating processes aids in ex-
plaining the “how” of development (Spencer & Harpalani, Net Stress Net Stress
(Engagement) Level (Engagement) Level
2004), not merely the “what,” as eloquently illustrated by
Bronfenbrenner’s model (Bronfenbrenner, 1989). Individ-
uals’ awareness of (cognition-dependent) perceptions and Challenges Supports Challenges Supports
stage-specific meaning-making processes afford the con-
sideration of critical inferences salient for understanding Figure 15.5 Stress-focusing research exemplars: Stress con-
youths’ coping and identity-formation processes. Further, ditions may link with either individual vulnerability level or
it is the socially linked and maturation-based variability stage-specific coping outcomes.
842 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

1959, 1960). Accordingly, given both between- and


Net Stress Reactive Coping
(Engagement) Level Strategies within-group variations for diverse individuals and as-
sociated contextual heterogeneity, the resulting person-
context-processes system of mediating/moderating
Challenges Supports Maladaptive Adaptive
effects may vary. As depicted in Figure 15.5, it is inter-
esting that the literature often narrowly focuses stress
Figure 15.6 Stress and coping links: Emphasizes linkages questions (i.e., balances between challenges and sup-
between reactive coping methods and net level of stress ports) around vulnerability themes for some groups
experienced. (e.g., European Americans) while it provides a stress
focus as linked to outcomes of competence or failure for
reactive coping styles available through a history of so- others (e.g., diverse youth of color). We suggest that the
cialization (see Figure 15.6), and, consistent with ego approach taken often varies by ethnicity. For example,
psychology theorizing by Erikson (1959), it bears in being African American or poor is frequently linked
mind the critical function of emergent, stable, identity- with studies of stress for Blacks. Although divorce as a
formation processes (see Figure 15.7). significant stressor is often linked with particular cop-
The three mediating processes were illustrated previ- ing outcomes for European Americans (refer to Figure
ously and collectively (see Figure 15.4) and depicted as 15.5), the level of support provided by privilege as expe-
a large question mark and thick arrow between rienced by European American youth has just recently
vulnerability and stage-specific coping outcome. The become the focus of rigorous developmental science ef-
mediating processes assist with explanations about the forts concerned with maladaptive coping (see Luthar,
diversity of individual-context-process mediated links. 2003; Figure 15.6).
These associative relations provide the reason for Consistent with the emphases described and theorized
a simultaneous consideration of structural factors, cul- by Cross (1991) and Erikson (1959) about reference
tural influences, individuals’ perceptual processes about group orientation and broad ego processes, respectively,
the self, bidirectional interactions with others, and myr- the multiple functions of identity processes are founda-
iad daily life experiences that impact individual vulner- tional to this framework. Specifically, the central place-
ability, productive coping outcomes, and, for some, ment of identity processes is evident through its
manifested resiliency. The perceptions and patterned mediating links between human coping (i.e., net reactive
responses occur as individuals confront normative coping) with broad ego processes (i.e., stable emergent
developmental tasks while navigating countless contexts identities; see Spencer, 1985; Spencer & Dornbusch,
in pursuit of demonstrating competence given the un- 1990; Spencer & Markstrom-Adams, 1990; Swanson,
avoidable impact of effectance motivation (R. White, Spencer, & Petersen, 1998).
The central role of identity and self processes affords
the framework an ICE (identity-focused cultural ecolog-
ical) perspective, given the important and implicit func-
Emergent Identities:
Stable Coping Responses tion of social cognition, perceptual processes, and
context character (e.g., Spencer, 1984, 1985). As de-
scribed in Figure 15.7, negative identity processes for
Negative Positive
youth of color have been assumed to be a reactive coping
response to racial group membership; the relationship is
frequently assumed particularly for African American
youngsters. However, the assumptions have not been
confirmed with empirical data (see Hare & Castenell,
Reactive Coping
Strategies 1985; Spencer & Dornbusch, 1990; Spencer & Markstrom-
Adams, 1990). Beginning with the work of theorists
such as Kardiner and Ovesey (1951) and Pettigrew
Maladaptive Adaptive
(1964) and as inferred from the Brown v. Board of Edu-
cation decision (i.e., as in the citations of Kenneth and
Figure 15.7 Identity-emphasizing research. Mamie Clarks’ research as cited in Cross, 1991), the
Conceptual Shortsightedness in the Study of Diverse Youth: Children of Color and Privilege 843

youths’ own families, friends, neighbors, and extended


Stage-Specific kin represent the comparison others of salience for so-
Coping Outcomes
cial emotional functioning (i.e., inferred self valuing is
reflected from referent others) rather than externally
Unproductive Productive and negatively evaluated life-stage specific outcomes.
As particularly applied to youth of color, process-
oriented theorizing is needed that acknowledges context
and, at the same time, demonstrates mediating
processes and avoids the conceptual assumptions and
Emergent Identities:
Stable Coping Responses
erroneous conclusions of the past. Finally, unlike many
theories of human development that merely mention
context and acknowledge processes, our conceptual ap-
Negative Positive proach more specifically and directly demonstrates the
person-context-process view as espoused by Bronfen-
Figure 15.8 Outcome- and identity-emphasizing theoriz- brenner (1985, 1989). PVEST provides insights about
ing: Approach narrowly focuses on specific life stage coping the “ how” of development (see Spencer & Harpalani,
outcomes and makes assumptions about identity character. 2004), and, we believe, more directly links the daily ex-
periences of diverse people given individual character-
consistent assumption of psychopathology and negative istics and protective factors (i.e., by ethnicity, race,
identity processes continue to be associated with as- social status, religious faith, and physical attributes—
sumed internalized risk related to the stigma associated skin color or body type) with environmental quality (in-
with skin color and identifiability (i.e., and implications cluding the availability of supports and challenges) as
for biased treatment; see Figures 15.8 and 15.9; Cross, individuals traverse multiple contexts. Specifically,
1991, for a review). successful transitions require good person-context fit
It is interesting that identity processes for African for maximizing human processes and behavioral out-
American youth are not more carefully analyzed and in- comes (see Matute-Bianchi, 1986; Spencer &
terpreted given their often co-occurrence with appear- Harpalani, 2004). An approach of this type aids in in-
ances of unproductive stage-specific coping outcomes terpreting the “ how” or “ why not ” of youth outcomes as
(e.g., school failure). The lack of anticipated (i.e., nega- promulgated by positive youth development theorists
tive) linkage between self concept and unproductive out- included in this set of volumes such as Benson (Benson,
comes is due to the not unexpected and greater salience Scales, Hamilton, & Sesma, Chapter 16, this Handbook,
of own-group significant others for children’s psycho- this volume) and Lerner (Chapter 1, this Handbook, this
logical functioning and well-being. In other words, volume). The broad, dynamic, developmental, identity-
focused, and context-linked perspective suggested by
PVEST and relevant to numerous groups, to date, has
Net Vulnerability not characterized approaches to child and adolescent
development nor life course theorizing (see Spencer,
1999; Swanson & Spencer, 1999; Swanson et al., 1998).
Risks Protective
Factors
Summary
Emergent Identities: In summary, this chapter’s organization was structured
Stable Coping Responses
to delineate the advantages and contributions of PVEST
for maximizing the applicability of human development
Negative Positive theorizing for many ethnicities or diverse groups of hu-
mans ( both youth of color and European Americans).
Figure 15.9 Identity- and vulnerability-focusing approaches: The first section indicated the chapter’s focus on the
Perspective focuses mainly on individual vulnerability associ- period of preschool through adolescence and explained
ated with speculated identity statuses. why the theory affords human development a life-course
844 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

and identity-focused perspective that highlights cultural


patterns as linked to context character. The next section
afforded a detailed introduction to the PVEST frame-
work, and was followed by a discussion of the frame-
work’s useful dynamic character. Several cross-cutting
themes of human development were used for illustrating
the connectedness between individual characteristics Conception End of Life
Course
and context interactions, thus, providing a mechanisms

Middle Childhood

20–35 Young Adulthood


Early Childhood
for interpreting nuanced cultural patterns. Brown v.

Societal Elders
55–70 Mature Years
11–20 Adolescence
Toddlerhood

35–55 Middle Age


Infancy and
Board of Education was introduced as relevant for high-
lighting and demonstrating the impact of policy change
and its role in determining long-term youth experiences
and schooling. The next two text divisions introduce

6–11

70+
0–2

3–5
final sections that indicate the distinct interpretational
advantages of the framework.
Figure 15.10 Spiraling and interactive systemic processes:
An assumed “ unfolding” of human processes occurs across
the life course from conception to death.
PHENOMENOLOGICAL VARIANT OF
ECOLOGICAL SYSTEMS THEORY:
MULTIDISCIPLINARY, DYNAMIC, tions for more general competence as lives unfold
SYSTEMIC INFLUENCES, AND across the life course (see Figure 15.10), and, as
PROCESSES OF A SYNTHESIZED sources of social feedback, the social environment rep-
RECURSIVE FRAMEWORK resents an unavoidable and critical component of this
process (see Figure 15.11).
The meaning-making that individuals formulate about From the earliest interactions between infants and
their lives evolves from basic social and cognitive de- parenting adults, feedback impacts children’s behavior
velopment processes. These formulations become more on the environment and a reverse interaction occurs as
sophisticated with broadened social experiences and well (or consider the lack of feedback and the sequelae
increases in cognitive maturation (Flavell, 1968; of minimally responsive babies as described by Kennell,
Spencer, 1982, 1983, 1985; Spencer & Markstrom- Trause, & Klaus, 1975). Infants’ cognitive awareness
Adams, 1990). These relationships are better under- was described by Piaget (1926, 1967) as evolving
stood both from a dynamic framework that crosses schemata. At the same time, early ego functioning was
interdisciplinary boundaries (e.g., individual and group the subject of theories by Erikson (1968) and others as
experience, biological foundations, and cultural tradi-
tions), one that is indelibly linked to contextual forces,
Parents
and from a perspective that includes recognition of in- Grand- Siblings
dividuals’ information processing efforts available and parents
reflecting the opportunities and constraints of the var-
ious developmental periods.
Extended Child/ Peers
Kin Adolescent
The Foundational Role of Social Cognition and
Linkages to Phenomenology: Inference-Making,
Perceptions, and Youths’ Everyday Experiences
Media
More recent theorizing about children’s theory of mind Popular School
Culture Church
adds to our understanding about recursive and develop-
ment-dependent processes (Frye, 1992; Frye & Moore,
1991; Nguyen & Frye, 1999). We’ve learned that the Figure 15.11 Bidirectional influences of significant others
character of these recursive processes have implica- on youths’ development.
Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 845

not independent of cognitive development. Both domains Experienced as stigma, underanalyzed, or unsubstanti-
are involved in critically important personifications or ated assumptions may contribute as core features to
early self processes of human functioning. These linked context character for some (e.g., underresourced hous-
processes occur simultaneously in social contexts and ing and more general neighborhood conditions) and, as
serve critical roles for life-course development. Inde- stereotypes, produce additional risk that compromise
pendent of the domain stressed or the theoretical ap- self processes.
proach to which one subscribes, the salient implications
of social feedback for the development of general com- Self Processes of Diverse Youth
petence as defined by R. White (1959, 1960) is appar- The ecology of youth development may take on a partic-
ent. Children’s successful use of cognition-dependent ularly troubling hue when examined for culturally mar-
social cues reinforce and support effective management ginalized or diverse youth of color (e.g., Chestang,
of the environment and demonstration of stage-specific 1972). Youths’ knowledge of or inferences about the
competence. Given this recursive process, the feedback world have important implications for psychosocial
concerning a successful impact on the environment has functioning. As applied to the experiences of youth of
implications for subsequent efforts expended and is de- color, epistemology is seldom a concern in the litera-
scribed by R. White as “effectance motivation.” Thus, ture. Epistemology is a branch of philosophy concerned
as suggested by Figures 15.10 and 15.11, socializing with the nature of knowledge (see Piaget, as reviewed in
adults and others are critically important for these bidi- Hergenhahn & Olsen, 1993, p. 275). Relative to patterns
rectional processes. of achievement and minority status issues, the acting
White inferences from particular ethnographic assump-
Social Cognition and Development
tions and analyses (e.g., Fordham & Ogbu, 1986) pro-
Unavoidable inferences made from experiences may be vide an example. Although theoretically critiqued (e.g.,
linked to individuals’ meaning-making processes. They Spencer et al., 2001; Spencer & Harpalani, under re-
include the (a) perception of challenges and available view) and empirically demonstrated as incomplete (e.g.,
resources; ( b) exposure to modeled strategies for reac- see Steele & Aronson, 1995), the negative impact of the
tive coping, and (c) character of self processes inferred achievement-linked stigma on the ecology of learning
and enacted, which then contribute to stable emergent and development particularly for African American
identity processes. Following this line of thought, indi- youth is both unfortunate and undeniable. Specifically,
viduals’ life-stage specific coping products (i.e., as a unwarranted stigma makes it difficult for adolescents to
consequence of a stable identity providing consistency engage in positive character formation given that the
in coping responses over time and place) are linked to process is attempted under what Chestang (1972) de-
context character and which have societal implications scribes as “ hostile conditions” (see also, e.g., Phelan,
as outcomes for what Damon (2002) refers to as either Davidson, & Cao, 1991). Further, when applied to the
noble versus ignoble “deeds.” We suggest that the context-associated experiences of African American
processes frequently overlooked are imbedded in con- youth, correlates of stigma can contribute to youth feel-
texts that vary by risk level and the availability of pro- ing “missed, dissed, and pissed,” as described by
tective factors that make accessible specific types of Stevenson (1997).
supports. To illustrate, a priori assumptions imposed Social science has always benefited from analyses
from the outside matter and overlap with youths’ un- that provide perspectives of others outside the person.
folding sense of purpose (e.g., the media’s characteriza- However, we restate that a systems-oriented analysis
tion of particular youth as deviant or delinquent, while that acknowledges the critical and ever-present role of
ignoring the wrong doing or immature behavior of oth- the person’s own phenomenology or unique set of per-
ers, reinforces assumptions of privilege for some and ceptions is also needed. This perspective affords an
pathology for others). Risk level also matters as it is as- often-ignored or assumed analysis of the individual-
sociated with our human and unavoidable inference- context interface and, consequently, context and cultur-
making processes (i.e., social perceptions). Both the ally linked behavioral response. As suggested, the latter
unavoidable cognition-linked perceptual processes and is particularly salient because it is inherently ecological
a priori context-associated risk factors make a differ- and represents the character of experiences associated
ence for self processes (e.g., Spencer, 1981a, 1981b). with a particular context. Claude Steele and his
846 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

colleagues’ excellent illustrations provide an effective mental trajectories possible, unavoidably youngsters
vehicle for explaining achievement behavior and school infer supports, cope reactively with adversity in the mo-
orientation of a long-term stigmatized group who the ment (see Stevenson, 1997), and internalize emergent
media would infer lack a sense of purpose: African identities that have implications for stage-specific cop-
American students (e.g., Steele & Aronson, 1995). ing outcomes. Particularly for African American and
Because youths’ own perceptions of social contexts Hispanic youths, their social experiences along with an
( both past and present) and unique level of individual evolving sense of purpose may be highly stigmatizing
vulnerability coexist, it is not unexpected that associ- and compromising to the latter. For that reason, we view
ated net-level experiences of stress vary and matter. the theory-driven analysis proposed as a source of pro-
The noted associations are seldom acknowledged, ac- tection against the continuing penchant to misinterpret
counted for in the design of programs of research, or di- youthful efforts for demonstrating a sense of compe-
rectly measured. Further, these themes are not tence or achieving a sense of purpose albeit frequently
considered in a culturally sensitive, contextually under unacknowledged difficult circumstances tradi-
linked, or developmentally framed perspective. Con- tionally ignored in developmental science specifically
sidered together, the shortsightedness considerably and the social science more generally.
narrows the view of sense of purpose and suggests a Thus, PVEST utilizes an ICE perspective, integrat-
false dichotomy of noble versus ignoble purpose as ex- ing issues of culture, social and historical context, and
pressed behavior (see Spencer, Fegley, & Harpalani, normative developmental processes involved in identity
2003). Efforts that represent noble efforts in the face of formation and broad psychosocial processes (see
great adversity are generally unacknowledged, not rec- Dupree, Spencer, & Bell, 1997; Spencer, 1995; Spencer
ognized, or undervalued, given traditional assumptions & Harpalani, 2004; Spencer, Hartmann, & Dupree,
concerning representations of sense of purpose. Erro- 1997). While Scarr (1988) correctly argues that “ the
neously inferring a lack of purpose is more probable phenomenology of experiences is . . . correlated with the
than not for youth of color or those from low-resource genotype of the individual perceiver and processor ”
communities. Their attempts at orientation toward (quoted on p. 241), we believe that this view is far too
right action (see Spencer, 1999a) is frequently ham- unsophisticated; in the mold of much of the behavioral
pered by very challenging contexts and circumstances genetics research we have reviewed, it is an accurate but
(e.g., the multiple opportunities for adverse profiling oversimplified statement (see Spencer & Harpalani,
by police officers, social experiences in peer groups 2004). We submit that delineating the processes involved,
are generally viewed as suggesting gang membership, rather than a mere statement of obvious correlation, is
driving or riding in motor vehicles are frequently as- necessary for an understanding of nature-nurture inter-
sumed to be drug vans). Most important, European action in human development. A fuller understanding of
Americans of diverse groups do not generally en- process and context is necessary to understand how the
counter similar stigma; most youth enjoy the privileges genotype impacts development. We treat the genotype
associated with innocent forays and youthful experi- similarly to Bronfenbrenner’s, as it is an important and
ences associated with suburban and affluent communi- influential aspect in human development, but the expres-
ties without attendant and persistent concerns about sion of the genotype in context is actually a component
harassment based solely on race, ethnicity, class, faith of the developmental system.
community, national origin, or skin color. Contrary to
Phenomenology and Context Character
European Americans’ experiences and as encouraged
by Erikson (1968), diverse youth of color are not pro- The PVEST framework combines a phenomenological
vided opportunities for social experimentation without perspective with Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems
significant consequences and personal risks. theory (1989), linking context and perception. The crit-
As indicated, we view an ICE perspective (Spencer, ical role of phenomenology, among others of the period,
1995; Spencer et al., 2006) as a theoretical framework may be best represented by the personality theorizing of
of human development that is inclusive of culturally Carl Rogers who described the perspective during the
diverse youths’ potential breadth of experiences and first half of the twentieth century (e.g., see Schultz,
normative identity needs. Given the countless develop- 1976). In combining ecological perspectives with phe-
Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 847

nomenology, the integration provides an analytical Illustrated as a recursive system in Figure 15.12, the
mechanism for understanding the individual’s meaning- PVEST framework consists of five basic components
making processes that underlie identity development that form a dynamic theoretical arrangement.
and coping outcomes (Spencer, 1995; Spencer, Dupree, The first component, Net Vulnerability Level (1), con-
et al., 1997). PVEST serves as a model to examine nor- sists of individual, family, and community characteris-
mative human development—framed through the inter- tics that may serve as risk versus protective factors (or
action of identity, culture, and experience (given a both if considered at different developmental periods)
particular psycho-historical moment)—in fact, the sys- during an individual’s development. Net Vulnerability
tems framework is apropos to individuals of all ethnici- Level is so described because risks usually do not exist
ties and operates as a set of recursive processes without some protective factors: Accordingly, the ef-
throughout the life course. As suggested, PVEST utilizes fects of apparent risks may be offset or balanced by the
an ICE perspective, integrating issues of cultural con- presence or availability of protective factors (e.g., privi-
text with normative developmental processes. Each com- lege group membership, a particular cultural socializa-
ponent represents part of a system that is nestled in the tion history, skin color, facial features, body type,
multiple levels of youths’ contextual experiences. intellectual superiority, attractiveness, economic stabil-
ity, well-educated parents and protective extended fam-
ily networks, emotionally available and caring adults or
Systems Framework in an Ecological System
nonrelated kin, and an individual’s personal history of
As described, the framework acknowledges the critical productive coping during a prior period of stage-specific
role of perceptions (Spencer, 1995) and focuses on outcomes). We suggest that a net balance between evi-
identity formation while considering macro-, exo- and dent risk factors and accessible protective factors de-
mesolevel structural factors, cultural influences, indi- fines an individual’s net vulnerability level for a
vidual perceptions of the self, significant others, life particular period. The content and character of the pro-
experiences, and the environments in which individuals tective characteristics available at one stage (e.g., mid-
live and function. Identity formation takes place across dle childhood) may not remain adequate enough, without
the life course and is especially relevant for adoles- additions, when traversing another stage (e.g., adoles-
cents given their heightened self-consciousness. cence). Particularly for marginalized youth (e.g., youth
PVEST combines this emphasis on individual percep- of color, immigrants, and low-resource youths), identi-
tions with Bronfenbrenner’s ecological systems theory fied risk factors may include socioeconomic conditions,
(1979), thus linking context and perception. Consistent such as living in poverty, and imposed expectations
with classic ecological psychologists, Bronfenbren- based on race, immigration status, unstable family eco-
ner’s model provides a means for describing how multi- nomic status, and gender stereotypes.
ple levels of context can influence individual As illustrated in Figure 15.12, the bidirectional
development; additionally, PVEST directly illustrates arrow of the Net Vulnerability Level indicates an un-
life-course human development within context. In avoidable linkage between risks and protective factors
doing so, it allows an analysis of the meaning-making and suggests a particular level of vulnerability. The
processes that underlie identity development and be- transactional and dynamic relationship between risks
havioral outcomes (Spencer, 1995, 1999a; Spencer, and protective factors is apparent at different develop-
Dupree, & Hartmann, 1997). This is important be- mental stages as lives unfold with increasing complexity
cause, as reviewed earlier, most theories of develop- over time. The balance between risks and protective fac-
ment assume a priori deviance and psychopathology for tors and consequent level of vulnerability evokes a par-
highly vulnerable populations; yet they cannot explain ticular psychosocial status that can produce either worse
diverse outcomes for individuals in shared contexts or improved conditions dependent on or due to prior or
(e.g., siblings’ development in the same family, neigh- attendant experiences. Most important, as individuals
borhood, and socioeconomic status often manifest dif- transition across time and place, net vulnerability is it-
ferent life-stage outcomes—one sibling graduated from self recursively linked to its transformation as everyday
secondary school versus the other incarcerated in a ju- stress processes for individuals as each interfaces with
venile facility). multiple environments of varying character.
848 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

1 5
Net Vulnerability Stage-Specific Coping
Outcomes

Risks Protective
Factors Unproductive Productive

Normative Human Systemic


Processes: Individual at a
Specific Development Stage

2 4
Net Stress Emergent Identities
Engagement (Stable coping responses)

Challenges Supports Negative Positive

3
Reactive Coping Processes

Maladaptive Adaptive

Figure 15.12 Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST): Revised (2004) Process Emphasizing Version.

The second component of PVEST, Net Stress Engage- implications for an individual’s state of psychological
ment (2), refers to the actual experiences of challenge well-being. Thus, given the media-hyped stigma associ-
and support that can impinge on an individual’s well- ated with the achievement gap for African American
being. In contrast to the risk factors referred to in the youth (e.g., high achievement’s hypothesized associa-
Net Vulnerability Level, stressors are actualized risks tion with acting White beliefs or expectations for low
encountered that require some level of response because performance), the ways in which an achievement test sit-
they are experienced as specific challenges. Net Stress uation or other evaluative setting (e.g., work site) is
Engagement is recursively linked with Net Vulnerability framed or inferences about personal performance are
Level, and represents the balance between challenges communicated may represent significant sources of psy-
actually encountered and available supports accessible chological challenge or fuel assumptions about inherent
or available. Challenges and supports may be physically support possible (see Spencer & Harpalani, under re-
experienced or symbolically assessed and their signifi- view). What is interesting about the character and use of
cance inferred (e.g., assumptions about privilege ex- stigma is that it represents a social construction experi-
pected and supports available or perceptions about tests enced in particular and nonequivalent ways across
or confrontations to be weathered). groups. For example, Lesser and Stodolsky (1967, 1970)
Again, Steele (2004) describes the impact of identity describe the superior performance of Jewish Americans
or social stigma on performance outcomes. He describes over Caucasians and all other groups. However, the high
the inferred meaning of a stigma’s content that an indi- achievement efforts of other groups have never been de-
vidual may make (i.e., experienced as some level of scribed as “acting Jewish.” Similarly, when considering
challenge) concerning the character of the performance the high performance of Asian American students when
“expected”; the social experiments by Steele and his compared with Caucasians, Hispanics, and African
colleagues suggest that the effect can be nullified or its Americans, it remains curious that only African Ameri-
impact diminished by specific supports provided or can youths’ high achievement behavior is described as
through the reframing of expectations. The nature of the acting White. African American performance dispari-
“ balance achieved” (net stress level experienced) has ties are usually associated with deficit characteristics of
Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 849

self or the group although the lower performance of male into the family may be perceived as a source of
Caucasians when compared with Jewish children is significant support by the mother and other family mem-
never adversely depicted in parallel ways. Considering bers, particularly if he adds significant economic re-
the appearance of particularly structured, widely circu- sources to the household. However, the presence of this
lated, and socially constructed media images for sub- new adult male may be perceived differently by adoles-
groups of marginalized youth, it is not surprising that cent children in the household who are not biologically
the cultural socialization literature (see Arrington, related to this new adult male and his intended role as a
2002; Slaughter & Johnson, 1988; Spencer, 1983, 1990) source of support. For example, an adolescent male may
provides detailed illustrations of how culturally specific perceive this new presence as a loss of his own per-
strategies serve as exact types of support for precise di- ceived role as the man of the house; the source of sup-
verse group members. Jackson, Boostrom, and Hansen port may be actually inferred as a serious threat to the
(1993) suggest a group of positive qualities or virtues male youth’s inferred status or his very identity.
that individuals occupying particular systems possess, The scenario may evolve differently for adolescent
or contexts themselves exhibit (e.g., teachers, students, females who look to inferred safe adult males for feed-
or classrooms), which maximize the probability of find- back to reaffirm femininity and other aspects of gender
ing positive virtues. They suggest that the best strategy identity. However, as a nonbiologically linked male, the
for accomplishing this “is by adopting and maintaining a mother’s husband may or may not have the appropriate
sympathetic outlook, by going about the task biased in understandings about the daughter’s intent or need for
the direction of making positive errors, of seeing virtues specific and safe feedback. The adult male may (mis)in-
where they are not, rather than the other way terpret the girl’s efforts as a hyper-sexualized display.
around. . . . For many of the positive qualities of teach- He may not understand the teen’s actual interest in ob-
ers, students, and classrooms are subtly expressed and taining af firmation for her femininity. The transactional
are visible to those who look closely and who do so with outcomes from interactions with her mother’s spouse
a sympathetic eye” (Jackson, Boostrom, & Hansen, p. may serve to increase the girl’s risk due to increase
258). The unfortunate dilemma for marginalized young- challenges encountered in interactions with her mother.
sters is that the process described for seeing positively The girl’s mother may view her daughter’s behavior as
about others is usually reserved for those who enjoy a sexually alluring versus the girl’s actual intent (i.e., to
privileged status. Thus, diverse students also receive evoke feedback for the purpose of promoting psychosex-
ranges of feedback about their status and inferred social ual development).
station, which is not independent of the protective fac- Alternatively, as a second scenario, for youth devel-
tors available and contributes to youths’ level of net vul- oping in households burdened with economic insecu-
nerability (i.e., first component of PVEST). rity, a mother’s need to obtain work for maintaining the
As suggested, accessible or available social supports household economy might invite greater stress due to
are transformed protective factors described as the first expectations for shared household responsibilities.
component of PVEST. They aid youths’ transitioning Privileged youth unfamiliar with the strategies for suc-
through challenging experiences thus serving to reduce cessfully coping with schoolwork and, at the same time,
or offset the net level of stress experienced. In many contributing to household chores might find it difficult
ways, supports are actualized protective factors. to adjust to a change in lifestyle when a family is con-
Whereas risks and protective factors denote demograph- fronted with sudden economic downturns and attendant
ics or descriptors relevant to individuals, family mem- stressors; a youth from a privileged family history may
bers, or community, net stress level is the balance have less time to hone the necessary skills for accom-
between challenges and support. The character or qual- modating the new family stresses and attendant re-
ity of balance suggests the actual phenomenological ex- quired role expansions. Having to cope with the noted
periences of risk and protection in context. For example, role challenges concomitantly with traditional peer ex-
the addition of an adult male to a household may act as pectations and new family constraints may potentially
either a stress or a support depending on the context increase overall level of stress experienced in the mo-
and, perhaps, the gender or parentage of the offspring in ment when confronted by associated challenges (e.g.,
question. A single mother may marry the biological fa- cooking, cleaning, or child care responsibilities for a
ther of one of her children. The introduction of an adult younger sibling). It is not only the balance between the
850 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

current challenges, given accessible supports that im- appraisal all define our identity. As new challenges are
pact overall stress, but also the recursive links with net met, the stable method of responding has implications
vulnerability because the family situation is increasing for decision-making processes, options elected, and,
its level of effective risks with potential changes in its given individual-environment transactional influences
ability to maintain the prior protective factors. In sum, and the role of stereotyping and stigma, objective op-
given the recursive links between net vulnerability tional choices available.
evolving and net-stress level currently experienced, As reviewed in great detail elsewhere (e.g., Spencer
youth may respond by adopting an in-the-moment reac- & Markstrom-Adams, 1990; Swanson et al., 1998),
tive coping response. identity processes provide behavioral stability over time
In response to the challenges noted and in conjunc- and place and lay the foundation for future perceptions,
tion with available supports, the net level of stress ex- self appraisals, and behavior such as decision-making
perienced requires that the third component, Reactive processes. Given the sets of stage-specific developmen-
Coping Methods (3), is deployed. Figure 15.12 illus- tal tasks outlined by Havighurst (1953), the resulting
trates that reactive coping responses include problem- and ongoing problem-solving and decision-making be-
solving strategies that can be either adaptive or havior can yield adverse or productive Life-stage Spe-
maladaptive. For instance, given the prior illustration, cific Coping Outcomes (5). Figure 15.12 describes the
in response to decreased time and attention from par- fifth component and indicates that productive outcomes
ents and the need for adolescents to take on more fa- might include school engagement, positive family rela-
milial responsibilities, youth may engage in more risk tionships, adequate employment preparation, staying out
taking behavior (a maladaptive response) or seek more of jail, and low levels of high risk behavior. Alterna-
support through greater interaction with extended kin tively, adverse outcomes can include school dropout,
(e.g., grandparents) and non-kin adults (e.g., school poor school performance, illegal means of earning in-
counselor, teacher, or religious leader). Accordingly, come, poor health, incarceration, and teenage or out-of-
there will be options for positive or adaptive responses wedlock child bearing.
when particular issues surface in peer contexts, the The dynamic and ongoing recursive process of nego-
family system, or with social institutions such as tiating risks while identifying and garnering protective
schools. Conversely, models of maladaptive problem factors for decreasing vulnerability continues between,
solving would also be possible reactive coping re- within, and throughout each life stage. More specifi-
sponses (e.g., staying away from home or other sources cally, this recursive process occurs as individuals
of support, using drugs, dropping out of school, or be- encounter new stressors (i.e., through balancing chal-
coming committed to negative peer models). lenges with supports), establish more expansive reper-
As youth employ various reactive coping strategies toires for reacting to coping needs (i.e., given the
consistently over time and place, self-appraisal contin- availability of both maladaptive and adaptive coping
ues, and those strategies yielding desirable results (i.e., “opportunities”), and redefine how individuals view
objectively viewed as either positive or negative) are re- themselves, which also impacts how others view them.
peated. For example, negative peer approval might still As noted by Erikson (1968), unresolved issues in one life
feel comfortable and psychologically safe than the work stage influence future coping and identity-formation
involved in creating more adaptive solutions, which processes. Accordingly, PVEST aims not only to cap-
might require a change in peer relationships. The consis- ture this developmental process but also to place it in
tency of the reactive coping pattern has important impli- broader social contexts. As illustrated in Figure 15.13,
cations for psychosocial processes. As suggested in one could use the framework for testing any set of ori-
Figure 15.12, the reactive coping strategies become sta- enting issues as an entire system or as easily pursue its
ble coping responses, and, over time, yield Emergent use for examining variable relationships both within and
Identities (4). Thus, the fourth component of PVEST de- between only two or three components (e.g., Net Vulner-
fines how individuals view themselves in and between ability and Net Stress Experienced). As suggested by
their various contexts of development (e.g., family, the illustration, one could explore parent- or policy-level
school, peer group, and neighborhood). The combina- variables for Components 1 through 4 with an examina-
tion of factors such as cultural /ethnic background, un- tion of youth outcomes as Component 5. Indeed, Figure
derstandings about gender roles, and self and peer 15.13 illustrates several of the numerous constructs
1 5
Net Vulnerability Stage-Specific Coping
Outcomes

Risk Protective
Contributors Factors Unproductive Productive

Race/Ethnicity/Gender Competence
• Cultural values and beliefs • School status (active, withdrawn)
about family, gender roles,
• School performance (GPA/SAT-9 scores)
work, parenting, education
• Educational aspirations/fulfillment
• Adult support
• Sustainable employment
• Awareness of stereotypes
• Stable interpersonal relationships
Socioeconomic Factors
• Welfare history
Long-term vs. short-term 4
Recent welfare leavers
Emergent Identities
Never recipients
• Family/household composition
Two-parent (biological) family Negative Positive
Stepfamily
Single parent family
Cohabiting family
Extended family Adolescent Identities
Nonfamily • Early adultification

• Number and age of dependent • Self as learner


children/adults • Role identity within family
• Parental work readiness • Exaggerated sex-role presentation
Parental education Hypermasculinity (males)
Parental skills Hyperfemininity (females)
• Neighborhood/school context • Ethnic identity

2 3
Net Stress Reactive Coping Processes
Engagement

Challenges Supports Maladaptive Adaptive

Economic Stress Adolescent Perceptions and Coping Strategies


• Poverty level • Perceived interpersonal competence/ineptitude
due to parental reliance on adolescent
• Change in income
Increase/decrease in amount • Increase/decrease in interaction with non-custodial
of HH income parent, extended family, friends, neighbors, etc.
• Change in source of income • Re-orientation of educational and employment
aspirations
• Parental employment
Stable vs. unstable • Initiate/increase own employment
Full-time vs. part-time • Increase/decrease in religious participation
Type of work performed and practice
• Parental physical or mental disability • Engage/avoid risk-taking behavior
• School engagement/disengagement
• Expressive/inhibitive anger/aggression

Figure 15.13 Demonstration of Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST) application: Using frame-
work for analyzing the effects of specific economic policies and requirements for parents on adolescent academic and employ-
ment outcomes.

851
852 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

possible for examination in considering each of the five icantly in character as a consequence of particular social
components of the framework. constructions and individual characteristics, histories,
The illustrative possibilities would vary as a function and experiences.
of the populations of concern and specific developmental Most important, as illustrated in Figure 15.16, cop-
period of interest. Further, it is necessary to appreciate ing outcomes may differ both between groups and
the unique role gender plays because, for instance, when within groups (e.g., including ethnicities, races, and
studying outcomes among boys, the illustrative con- families). There are numerous differences in meaning-
structs represent unique experiences that are distinctive making processes expected for youth sharing the same
to boys across the components. As suggested by the intro- race, household, or classroom.
duction to the framework and illustrated in Figure 15.14, Our approach is quite different from those that antic-
PVEST is a synthesis of many contributing perspectives ipate homogenous group outcomes that are generally as-
and provides an ICE perspective of human development. sumed to represent either deficits or inferred privilege.
In examining the relationship between net vulnerabil- PVEST provides a systems framework for understanding
ity and net stress, PVEST explores the combined impact all possible variability. More important, given the im-
on coping processes specific to particular developmental pact of the context on individual or group attitudes and
stages. Given the contributions from multiple theoretical beliefs, the pattern of coping outcomes has the potential
perspectives, it aids our explanations and interpretive of contributing to the level of vulnerability. Figure 15.17
analyses of both similar and different outcomes. As illus- on page 854 illustrates the feedback loop created as a
trated in Figure 15.15, the outcomes of these interactions function of societal stereotype dependence thus demon-
and processes occur in nested ecologies that vary signif- strating the recursive relationship between the unique

PVEST ICE Net Vulnerability


and Stress Levels
Framework View of Human
Development:
Coping
Processes

Outcomes (coping abilities, skills and products):


Patterned and Diverse

Theoretical and Conceptual Formulations:

• Eriksonian theorizing about ego identity processes • Spencer’s notions about the social cognition/culture
cognition interface
• Du Bois’ notion of double consciousness
• Ecological psychology and Bronfenbrenner’s views
• Symbolic interactionists theorizing about
about context
phenomenological processes (e.g., Sullivan & Mead)
• Boykins’ notions about a “triple quandary”
• Competence and socialization perspectives
(e.g., Robert White & Brewster Smith) • Normal human development life-course theorizing
(e.g. Brim’s notions about continuity and change)
• Resiliency and vulnerability (e.g., J. Anthony)
• Historical perspectives (e.g., V.P. Franklin, John Hope
• Cross’ Reference Group Orientation (RGO) framework
Franklin, & Glen Elder; Brown vs. Board of Educ.)
• Chestang’s views of character development and
• White Privilege (McIntosh)
context quality
• Critical Race Theory (Crenshaw and others)

Figure 15.14 Acknowledging exemplar conceptual contributors to Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory
(PVEST) .
Cross-Cutting Human Development Themes of Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 853

Nested Ecologies

PVEST Identity-focused Cultural Vulnerability


Framework Ecological (ICE) Level and Stress
View of Human
Development
Coping
Processes

Figure 15.15 Advantages of framework: Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST) enhances inter-
pretations of vulnerability with coping processes as linked with diverse sets of life course outcomes specified for particular de-
velopmental periods.

and patterned outcomes produced and their subsequent gest critical cross-cutting themes of importance in the
impact on net vulnerability, net stress experienced, me- design and support of prevention and intervention
diating coping processes, and produced coping outcomes. strategies.
The modeled recursive feedback loop aids the ex-
planation suggested by Claude Steele concerning CROSS-CUTTING HUMAN DEVELOPMENT
achievement performance as linked to social stigma. THEMES OF PHENOMENOLOGICAL
Steele’s perspective (2004) indicates that anyone is VARIANT OF ECOLOGICAL SYSTEMS
privy to identity insecurity, and social contingencies THEORY: SALIENT FOR THE DESIGN OF
(i.e., the priming of social perceptions) have implica- INTERVENTIONS AND THE
tions for performance. Parallel with our own interpre- INTERPRETATION OF COPING OUTCOMES
tation, his views have important implications for the
character of interventions needed to impact youths’ It is not only symbolically important but also critical to
academic performance. All of the bidirectional rela- conceptualize the unfolding of lives in cultural context.
tionships between components of the framework sug- However, for groups such as urban youth of color, this is

Patterned and Diverse Outcome

Identity-focused
Vulnerability Level
Cultural
and Net Stress
PVEST Ecological
Framework (ICE)
View of Human Coping
Development Processes

Figure 15.16 Predicting diversity: Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST) demonstrating the
probability of both unique and patterned outcomes.
854 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

Coping (abilities and skills) OUTCOMES:


Either overlapping, unique or patterned feeds
back and contributes to net vulnerability level

Coping Includes
Use of Reactive
Net Vulnerability Strategies
PLUS AND
Net Stress Stable Identity
Formation
Processes

Figure 15.17 Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST) analyses afford an integration of individual
coping with context character: As an ICE perspective the framework suggests exacerbating influences (through an acknowledg-
ment of nested ecologies) that impact coping outcomes (i.e., either productive or adverse) given the experiences of net vulnera-
bility and stress level associated with coping processes (i.e., manifested as coping strategies and identity formation efforts).

not frequently the conceptual strategy employed as illus- of psychological science, the character of educational
trated by the ways in which we formulate questions, praxis, and life-course human development experience.
identify constructs, theorize about phenomena, interpret Considered from a PVEST perspective, all have implica-
results, conceptualize, and implement social policy. tions for the unique challenges experienced by specific
There are few places in which this phenomenon—the communities for some, and the occurrence of privilege
study of lives in context—is less apparent than in the and significant but unacknowledged support for others.
study of human development of marginalized youth in
Meaning and Experience of Culture in the
urban contexts. Further, when considered over time,
Consideration of Lives in Context
there are few developmental periods for which this
shortsightedness has had more dire consequences than in Developmental psychology came to life as a field of re-
the formulation of research, theory, practice, and policy search virtually 100 years ago at the turn of the 20th cen-
of relevance for middle childhood youth and adolescents. tury (S. H. White, 1996). Sheldon White notes that the
field’s birth followed an increase of nineteenth century
writings about children. He reports that their lives and
circumstances were examined philosophically, pedagogi-
Importance of Broad Cultural Inclusiveness
cally, medically, politically, autobiographically, statisti-
and Context
cally, didactically, sentimentally, and apocalyptically:
A succinct tracing of the history of psychological science There was a commitment to the scientific study of child
and the alternative advent of cultural-historical psychol- development. He goes on to report that, in the beginning,
ogy helps in highlighting the importance of emphasizing the approach stood insecurely on scattered facts strung
and integrating the meaning and experience of culture in together with much theorizing. Followers of Darwin put
the consideration of human development. Although gener- together observations of children, animals, cross-cultural
ally not acknowledged as a social construction, it is impor- beliefs and practices, psychopathology, and so on to
tant to describe and explore the exacerbating influence of sketch out an evolutionary picture of the human mind. It
urban character on our thinking about culture, the conduct was risky, speculative science. Sheldon White reports
Cross-Cutting Human Development Themes of Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 855

that Williams James, in the Principles of Psychology, periences, which represents yet another fallacy. S.H.
called it “ wild work ” (S. H. White, 1996, p. x). White also notes that:
S. H. White describes the late nineteenth and early
twentieth centuries as periods with organized coopera- The fact that some twentieth-century psychology has pro-
tive research enterprises that were modeled on the natu- duced findings that are only true within situational bound-
ral sciences and describes the emulation as a fledgling aries is not completely surprising. Before this century, a
discipline that yearned for status as a real science. S. H. number of distinguished philosophers argued that in order
to fully understand how the human mind works we will re-
White described it as “physics envy” because the physi-
quire two psychologies of different orders. We will need
cal sciences were well-formed and producing findings
the kind of naturalistic psychology with which we are fa-
that were intellectually interesting and practically useful
miliar, analyzing mental phenomena as constructions built
(S. H. White, p. xi). S. H. White’s description allows the out of sensations, ideas, associations, reflexes, or sensori-
inference of a current and historically similar situation. motor schemes. We will need also a less-familiar “second
There was a reasonable explanation for psychologists to psychology” describing higher-level mental phenomena as
imitate the particular pattern of cooperative activity; it entities given form by the language, myths, and social
provided the model for determining how far the approach practices in which the individual lives. Such a second psy-
would take it as a new science. By focusing on naturalis- chology would not be expected to yield universal findings.
tic questions and methods, psychologists sidestepped Since higher mental processes are formed by culture, they
some tricky and risky concerns (S. H. White, 1996, p. xi). differ from society to another. (1996, p. xii)
S. H. White’s analysis recognizes that the new psy-
chology was also saddled with unique and awkward re- In addition to Cole, S. H. White posits there has al-
strictions. The program of methods of the naturalistic ways been a lineage of cultural psychologists who have
scientific effort revealed particular patterns of organi- argued for:
zation of human perception, learning, and development,
An emphasis on mediated action in a context; the use of
and the rhetoric of that research program asserted that
the “genetic method” understood broadly to include his-
such patterns must be true for everyone everywhere
torical, ontogenetic, and micro-genetic levels of analysis;
(S. H. White, 1996, p. xi). Most important, S. H. White
the grounding of analysis in everyday life events; the as-
(1996) champions Michael Cole’s point of entry into the sumption that mind emerges in the joint activity of people
problems of twentieth-century psychological science as and is in an important sense, “co-constructed”; the as-
a reminder that aspects of the early assertion of univer- sumption that individuals are active agents in their own
sality were not true. He references Michael Cole’s work development; rejection of cause-effect, stimulus-response,
with the Kpelle in Africa in the 1960s that led to a explanatory science in favor of a science that emphasizes
salient observation: the emergent nature of mind in activity and that acknowl-
edges a central role for interpretation in its explanatory
Although tribal children classify, learn, remember, form framework; methodologies that draw upon the humanities
concepts, and reason in everyday life, they do not perform in as well as the social and biological sciences. (1996, p. xii)
a sophisticated manner on experimental procedures de-
signed for the study of age changes in those faculties. West- Sheldon White describes efforts expended for this
ern research procedures are grounded in a world in which
second psychology that began in the 1920s with work by
children go to school at 6 years of age and are surrounded by
Vygotsky who suggested the need for a human science
the life, language, and thought of a modern society. Much of
what we consider to be normal to child development is sim-
of psychology that could stand beside the existing natu-
ply a recognition of what usually happens when children ral science of the time. He argued that the need to “ un-
grow up in such a world. (S. H. White, 1996, p. xii) derstand” human mental life was deeply connected to
the manufactured objects in our world. Human beings
S. H. White’s point was that Cole’s work along with live in a world of human artifacts—tools, words, rou-
other cultural psychologists suggested significant diffi- tines, and rituals—changeling objects that are at once
culties in the use of traditional natural-scientific in- and the same time things the individual must deal with
quiries to compare people across cultures. An analogous and repositories of prior human thought and judgment
point may be made when considering the conjectures (1996, xiii). As reported by S. H. White, the integration
drawn from urban research contexts because a broadly and consideration of culture, for Michael Cole (as a
made assumption is that proximity suggests similar ex- cultural-historical psychology) means understanding
856 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

that psychology comes to life only when there are re- of African American male youths is described before
search procedures through which people can experience moving to the unique experiences of immigrants.
and know the world together. As noted by S. H. White,
Vygotsky asserts a critical philosophical position of im- The Cultural Context of African American Males
portance in this regard. “An artifact is an aspect of the S. H. White’s “alternative worlds” perspective is particu-
material world that has been modified over by the his- larly evident in the coping processes of African American
tory of its incorporation in goal-directed human action. boys growing up in socially constructed urban contexts,
By virtue of the changes wrought in the process of their particularly in public schools in low-resourced and un-
creation and use, artifacts are simultaneously ideal usually stigmatized social systems. Boykin (1986) sug-
(conceptual) and material” (p. xiv; emphases added). gests that the behaviors and practices evident in
S. H. White is quite clear that “artifacts” are the funda- contemporary communities where African American
mental constituents of culture. The growth of the human boys develop have their genesis in African cultures.
mind, in ontogeny and human history, must properly be These behaviors and practices manifest themselves dif-
understood as a co-evolution of human activities and ar- ferently, depending on the social, physical, historical, and
tifacts. The words we speak, the social institutions in economic contexts in which the boys interact. Although
which we participate, the man-made physical objects we this assertion has multiple political implications, it also
use, all serve as both tools and symbols. They exist in offers insight into the dynamic nature of culture. Simply
the world around us; they organize our attention and ac- by their contrast to mainstream culture, the behaviors and
tion in that world and, in the aggregate, they create “al- practices that characterize African American communi-
ternative worlds” (p. xiv). It would appear that including ties are assumed not to be cultural but, instead, deviant
the individual’s own reporting (i.e., as phenomenology) (Jarrett, 1994; Oyemade, 1985). Kottak (1987) notes that
of the particulars of “alternative worlds” would be criti- culture is learned and cultural learning is dependent on
cal for arriving at an understanding of specific meaning- “the uniquely human capacity to use symbols, signs that
making processes by individuals as they mature over have no necessary or natural connection to what they
time, transition across multiple settings and physical stand for. . . . A person born anywhere begins immedi-
spaces, and cope with the attendant contextual demands. ately through a process of conscious and unconscious
Accordingly, there are few places where this phenome- learning and interaction with others, to internalize, or in-
non is as evident as in the coping processes of youth grow- corporate, a cultural tradition through the process of en-
ing up in socially constructed urban contexts. There culturation” (p. 23). As Kottak implies, a great deal of
are few examples more poignant than the unique and co- culture is expressed in the human ability to give meaning
existing struggles of diverse urban youth and families in and value to a thing, activity, or event. The next section
their efforts to demonstrate resiliency in socially con- places emphasis on understanding the meanings that is
structed although infrequently acknowledged urban cul- given to experiences that appear to be unique to African
tural contexts. S. H. White (1996) suggests that a American children and families. Particularly illustrative
“cultural-historical approach to the study of mind dictates to this point are the findings of race awareness and racial
that when we study human development we must make the attitudes studies.
study of surrounding social practices part and parcel of From a set of programmatic studies of race aware-
our inquiry.” And relative to policy and programming is- ness, racial attitudes, and self-esteem findings from the
sues, “Similarly, if we want to change the pattern of a early childhood years to adolescence, Spencer and col-
human being’s activities, we need to address the sur- leagues have obtained quite interesting findings for
rounding situations in which those activities live” (p. xiv). Black boys. Beginning in the preschool stage (e.g.,
As scholars and researchers interested in human Spencer, 1970; Spencer & Horowitz, 1973), findings
development from a global perspective, there are few suggest that Black boys learn the cultural symbols for
context-concerns of more importance than the need to group membership at the same rate as girls. There are no
examine the assumptions and questions that guide the gender differences in preschoolers’ Eurocentric (i.e.,
conduct of the research industry and policy enterprise. White valuing and Black devaluing) evaluative judg-
A clear demonstration of this dilemma is the experience ments of things dark in hue and of pictures depicting
of immigrant youth (see Spencer, Harpalani, Cassidy, African American people: Three-year-olds learn the
et al., 2006). As an illustrative case, the cultural context symbols as effectively as 5-year-olds (see Spencer,
Cross-Cutting Human Development Themes of Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 857

1970, 1999b; Spencer & Horowitz, 1973). As indicated stage-specific processes, growth in value autonomy, and
by Spencer (2005), those cultural symbols and meanings expectations for positive outcomes in response to nor-
for “self processes” become increasingly more complex mative developmental tasks. Irrespective of societal sta-
for Black males by adolescence. tus, youthful performance expectations are generic
across groups and an equivalent level of competence and
The Unique Experiences of African American resiliency is anticipated for all peers independent of de-
Male Adolescents gree of special challenges experienced by specific groups.
As reviewed by Spencer (1999a), for African American For the most part, both entitlements and extra chal-
boys who are generally viewed with some degree of disso- lenges are ignored, although similar stage-specific posi-
nance and trepidation by the larger society (see Cunning- tive outcomes of competence, physical and psychological
ham, 1993), the task of managing an ego-supporting health status, and broad achievements are expected
identity while coping with generalized negative imagery for all. Generally lacking in our thinking, however, as we
is daunting. The dilemma may be parallel to male adoles- consider achievement-linked outcomes such as the
cents generally, however, African American males specif- achievement gap between Black and White academic
ically are expected to shoulder the traditional negative performance, is an appreciation of youths’ interpretive
stereotypes associated with male adolescence, along with framework and perceptions concerning (a) context risk
the added burden of enhanced, often unacknowledged and presence of protective factors, (b) attendant stress
negative imagery linked with minority status. Often over- and accessible supports, (c) available coping methods for
looked, negative stereotypes influence character and re- reactive deployment (used for guaranteeing psychological
actions to socialization efforts such as those extended health and physical survival), and (d) models of stable
both in and out of school. That these conditions and expe- coping processes (identities) and patterned coping out-
riences are not formally recognized (although are part of comes (e.g., including both achievement-relevant and
youths’ daily experiences) make the individual’s manage- moral identity-linked). An awareness of youths meaning-
ment of normative developmental tasks more challenging. making including assessments of resources and chal-
Specifically, those tasks associated with establishing a lenges have implications for how they traverse patterned
moral identity or academic (achievement-linked) identity and unique psychological and physical challenges. There
are particularly salient when coupled with attendant out- are also intriguing interactions between factors.
comes potentially more diverse and open to misinterpreta- The dilemma of competing allegiances (triple
tion. Accordingly, African American male adolescents, in quandary; see Boykin, 1986) and competing socializa-
their efforts to cope with normative developmental tasks tion contexts (see Allen, 1985; Boykin & Ellison, 1995;
with few resources and supports, may deploy coping Boykin & Toms, 1985; Brookins, 1985, 1988; Hare &
methods that prove to be less than constructive. The re- Castenell, 1985; Johnson, 1988) is rarely perceived as
sponsive methods deployed by African American youths being more evident than in the experiences of African
(i.e., what we generally term as coping methods) may be American males. The growth of value autonomy may be
emotionally comfortable for a short period. These strate- dramatically illustrated by urban youths’ pursuit and
gies are also used by privileged youth in their pursuits of acquisition of respect. For many boys growing up in low-
autonomy; however, given stereotyping they frequently resource communities, the demand and demonstration
further exacerbate an already challenging situation in the of independence and responsibility occur early (see
end when deployed by African American male youth. Holliday, 1985a, 1985b). The value and recognition of
The struggles associated with autonomy linked devel- both of these character qualities are evident in micro-
opmental tasks are quite challenging. Theorists suggest systems such as family, community, and church. The
that growth in value autonomy is linked to expected cog- recognition and the early assignment of characteristics
nitive changes. We suggest that these changes, although of maleness to African American boys is interesting.
infrequently considered for African American youths, Generally reported anecdotally, there is a penchant to
particularly males, require a sensitive and simultaneous refer to a male baby or toddler as “little man.” The
synthesis of context character. Necessary for considera- motivation for the term’s frequent usage is linked to an
tion in this synthesis includes both the process and prod- expressed and global valuing of maleness and may not
uct of individuals’ evolving understanding of the world represent a conscious effort to “adultify” male children.
as youth struggle with inequitable conditions, normative Instead, the widespread use of the term, particularly by
858 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

African American men, suggests an effort to short- nance created for African American males given social
circuit the use of the slavery-associated penchant of stigma is experienced as an early social challenge. As a
Whites to refuse adulthood status to Black men by refer- basic ego need, youths’ pursuit of respect may take on
ring to them as boys. Thus, from an African American greater importance than the highly shared and valued ac-
adult male’s perspective, a father’s use of “little man” quisition of academic achievement (Spencer, 1999a).
to his son may be solely one of endearment. The particu- Irvine (1988) observes teacher-student tensions begin-
lar language used connotes pride in his son and expecta- ning in the early grades for boys. Accordingly, in the ab-
tions for a future manly role. sence of close parental monitoring, if generalized respect
The purpose of such language is its attempt to neu- from the broader society and the school are not forthcom-
tralize the prevalent use of Black male stereotyping and ing, adolescent males’ reactive (or less constructive) cop-
undervaluing of Black males by the broader society. ing response may be problematic; specifically, the taking
Young children, however, are developmentally egocen- on of “ habitual right actions,” which are polar opposite to
tric (appropriately self-centered) and remain so until those generally valued by society and expected by
about 6 or 7 years of age (see Spencer, 1976, 1982). schools, may potentially have adverse academic perfor-
Therefore, taking the perspective of another can depend mance implications (see Cunningham, Swanson, Spencer,
on a combination of cognitive maturation and social ex- & Dupree, 2003). The awareness of inequities and need
perience. As a consequence, children hear, use, and un- for respect may complicate family dynamics particularly
derstand language and interpret its content from their when accompanied by biological-based normative chal-
own (appropriately) limited cognitive perspective. Ac- lenges such as off-timed maturational rate differences
cordingly, in the case of African American men and (Swanson, Cunningham, & Spencer, 2003).
boys, use of “little man” language may unavoidably re- Gender-intensified behavior, such as hypermasculin-
sult in children’s inferring a set of expectations to be- ity, may be seen by youth as potentially more effective
have and take on the responsibilities of manhood while in generating respect than the instrumentality- and
still a youngster. From a PVEST perspective, specific future-linked outcomes associated with academic
socialization strategies, expressed as reactive coping achievement. Another exacerbating but normative factor
methods, that are deployed by parents and intended to is the relatively late acquisition of a time-perspective (a
short circuit the internalization of prevalent stereo- true sense of the future), which is linked to the present
types, may themselves introduce errant self perceptions. and past as a cognitive construction and acquisition, and
Cultural socialization is in addition to the other tradi- is usually reserved until mid- or late-adolescence. Be-
tional socialization tasks for which all parents are re- fore then, young people require significant aid in under-
sponsible; thus, the additional need of contemporary standing and implementing the links between current
Black adults to responsively cope with historical condi- behavioral investments (e.g., studying and school en-
tions (e.g., the verbal strategy used by slave owners to gagement) and long-terms valued outcomes (e.g., sec-
render adult African American men to child status) ulti- ondary school graduation and successful career
mately makes child-rearing responsibilities particularly preparation). Given the normatively late acquisition of a
onerous for African American parents (see Spencer, time concept, it is not surprising that many male youth
1983, 1990). do not understand that the 12 years of primary and sec-
African American male adolescents’ pursuit of and ondary school preparation and academic engagement
sensitivity to obtaining respect from others (e.g., teach- provide a critically important opportunity; this is an-
ers, police officers, unknown citizens) can be problem- other reason why parental monitoring is closely linked
atic (see Spencer, 1999a). Teachers uniformly expect to academic outcomes for youth generally, and, given
student-like behavior from children irrespective of the the problem of stereotype threat described by Claude
problematic and frequently unacknowledged affective Steele and colleagues, this appears particularly salient
school climate experienced by many African American for African American youth (see Spencer et al., 1996;
and Hispanic children. Given particular cultural tradi- Spencer & Swanson, 2000). As a long-standing value
tions, gender appears to be an exacerbating factor for di- espoused by African Americans, documented both be-
verse youth, particularly males. Without carefully fore and after slavery (Spencer, Cross, Harpalani, &
internalized reactive coping strategies obtained from per- Goss, 2003), and integrated as a cultural value shared
sistent cultural socialization efforts by parents, the disso- through parental socialization efforts, education contin-
Cross-Cutting Human Development Themes of Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 859

ues to be highly valued by the African American com- South Asian Americans. Relative to the first component,
munity (Spencer, 1983, 1990). Educational success in- Net Vulnerability (the balance between risks and protec-
creases (not guarantees) the probability of acquiring the tive factors), the particular risks to South Asian Ameri-
long-term respect that hypermasculine reactive cans become salient when discussing the differences
coping behavior connotes and that Black boys and ado- between what constitutes individualistic societies, such
lescents desperately seek and need (Swanson, Cunning- as the United States, and collectivist cultures such as
ham, & Spencer, 2003). However, the variety of those of South Asian countries.
historical, structural, and contemporary barriers and Individualistic societies follow an ideology that re-
challenges make the educational process inopportune for volves around personal autonomy, independence, and
some youth. Similarly, the normative experiences of im- self-reliance. These societies aim to create individuals
migrants to the United States appear no less complex who are able to separate themselves from others and
and, from a PVEST perspective, suggest different from situational contexts. The independent self is con-
sources of risk that may contribute to youths’ vulnera- structed to be a fixed entity that does not change when
bility level. An inclusive and broad understanding of social situations change. “ The independent self-system
risks for diverse groups promotes an improved articula- thus seeks to display or assert attributes or features of
tion of basic supports required and culturally specific the self. The others in a social situation are important,
interventions necessitated for obtaining competence for but they are important primarily as standards of social
youth generally. A PVEST approach provides a cultur- comparison or for feedback that can validate the inner at-
ally sensitive mechanism for linking unique risks and tributes of the self ” (Markus & Kitayama, 1991, p. 22).
protective factors with context features experienced by Collectivist societies strive to emphasize obedience
specific groups—the individual-context analysis, given and conformity. Their primary goal is to shape individu-
evolving perceptual developments aids, determining the als into interdependent beings who are defined by their
level of vulnerability anticipated and the kinds of sup- relationships. The interdependent self is seen as being
ports required for obtaining the best possible outcomes. fluid (able to change when the social environment around
them changes), dynamic, and defined by roles according
Special Experiences of Immigrants to situation and context. “Such an interdependent self is
Given the cautions described by S. H. White (1996) con- not properly characterized as a bounded whole, because
cerning the need for a cultural-historical approach, this it changes structure with the nature of the particular so-
section explores various risk and protective factors that cial context ” (Markus & Kitayama, 1991, p. 23).
second-generation immigrants from collectivist soci- Given the drastic differences inherent in these two
eties may experience living in the United States. Focus- types of societies, it is highly probable that immigrants
ing particularly on the experiences of Asian Americans, from collectivist societies who move to the United States
specifically South Asian Americans, we emphasize that may experience many difficulties adjusting to their new
the problems and concerns addressed here are certainly surroundings. When individuals choose to immigrate, they
not indicative of all South Asian Americans. Consistent may come with practical knowledge of the country to
with Cole’s (1996) cultural psychology perspective and which they are moving. However many will not be ade-
S. H. White’s (1996) critique, we believe that an indi- quately versed in the cultural and societal values, beliefs,
vidual constructs his or her own worldview in his or her attitudes, and so on of the foreign country they are adopt-
own way. However, the construction is unavoidably ing as their new home. This circumstance may produce
linked to context character (Lee et al., 2003). As dis- significant problems for some immigrants, especially for
cussed elsewhere (Spencer, Harpalani, Cassidy, et al., those who may have difficulties in acculturation. Even
2006), using the phrase “South Asian Americans” or though these immigrants may be aware that they are going
“second-generation South Asian children,” we actually through intense emotional and mental changes, they may
refer to individuals who are specifically born and raised be reluctant to seek help from outsiders, including mental
in the United States and who have immigrant parents health professionals. This reluctance may be attributable
from the following countries in South Asia: India, Pak- to suspicion, doubt, fear, uncertainty, confusion, shame, or
istan, Sri Lanka, and Bangladesh. As illustrated previ- ignorance of Western psychology and psychologists. This
ously in Figure 15.12, PVEST provides a helpful analysis and decision to refrain from accessing objective
framework for exploring the development of identity in support may be acceptable for the immigrants themselves,
860 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

but what about their children? The acculturation process group constitutes an individual’s immediate social envi-
can be daunting for immigrants who may have already de- ronment. In South Asian countries, this may include the
veloped a strong sense of identity and self while living in family ( both nuclear and extended), friends, coworkers,
their mother country. However, the processes of accultura- and peers (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). The individual is
tion and identity development are heightened in intensity connected intimately with their particular in-group and
for second-generation immigrant children living in the therefore, according to Sethi, Lepper, and Ross (1999)
United States (Spencer, Harpalani, Cassidy, et al., 2006). “ the self and relevant in-group members may become
Theorizing by Mehta (1998) addresses these processes psychological entities prone to relatively similar inferen-
in the South Asian Indian American population and sug- tial, judgmental, attributional, motivational, and percep-
gests that “ the second generation has been exposed to tual biases” (p. 10). The collectivist self is thus defined
distinctly different language, goals, food, rituals, dress, according to his or her in-group. However, the process of
music, landscapes, and values than their parents. There is immigration disrupts the concept of an in-group.
a strong urge to retain ethnic identity while rapidly ac- Especially relevant in the case of children, when in-
quiring awareness of American values, partially due to fi- dividuals drastically change their social, societal, and
nancial gains” (p. 133). Because of this disparity between cultural environments by moving to another country, ex-
their ethnic identity and their national identity, South tended family, friends, coworkers, and peers are left be-
Asian American children are faced with what many have hind. These individuals must break the close bonds they
coined a clash of cultures: the collectivist ideologies from shared with their established in-group in South Asia and
their families constantly coming into conflict with indi- leave the stability and security of their lives to seek ful-
vidualistic ideologies from the surrounding environment. fillment of their goals and aspirations in another coun-
Second-generation South Asian Americans must grapple try. For people who are interdependent, the experience
with the exceedingly difficult task of juggling opposing of leaving their in-group can be very traumatic. The sev-
philosophies successfully to survive. From a PVEST per- ering of ties may be exceedingly difficult, especially if
spective, it is safe to say that protective factors in one a comparable kin network is not already established in
cultural setting may not serve as an effective source of the adopted country. From a PVEST perspective, to
support when acculturating to a new cultural niche. maximize supports for offsetting challenges, immi-
The tension from a clash of cultures that many South grants must learn to create their own kin networks with
Asian Americans experience is a significant risk other immigrants in their new environment. Although
contributor that increases vulnerability and net stress the friendships formed may be comforting and provide
and has implications for identity development (Spencer emotional, psychological, and perhaps financial support
et al., 2006). Similar to identifiable diverse youth, South they may not compare as favorably to those relationships
Asian youths’ physical features serve as a risk contribu- between family members or lifelong friends that were
tor because being easily identified can lead others to established in their country of origin. Accordingly, the
perceive them as foreign, even though they have been identification and acquisition of supports may them-
born and raised in the United States and are U.S. citi- selves introduce challenges that interfere with the posi-
zens. This dichotomy between the physical and internal tive balance sought for overall net stress level.
selves may have implications for disruptions in self- The character and experience of an in-group
image and self-esteem, which may interfere with com- may function differently in a foreign country. Triandis
petence strivings and mental health. Interestingly, given (1989) suggests that “collectivism is associated with
available protective factors, social supports, and models child-rearing patterns that emphasize conformity, obedi-
of adaptive coping methods, many youth still maintain a ence, and reliability. Such patterns are usually associated
healthy sense of self and demonstrate resiliency. with rewards for conformity to in-group goals, which
Similar to many families of color, a protective factor, leads to internalization of the in-group goals. Thus people
which also functions as a potential source of support in do what is expected of them even if that is not enjoyable”
the PVEST framework, is the South Asian immigrant (p. 513). However, characteristics that might suggest psy-
family unit. To understand the relevance of the self in re- chological protection in the country of origin might func-
lation to the family, which may function as a risk contrib- tion differently in the adopted country. For example, the
utor, it is important to discuss the concept of in-groups heavy emphasis on obedience and conformity, especially
and out-groups in a collectivist cultural context. An in- among individuals who are highly traditional and conser-
Cross-Cutting Human Development Themes of Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 861

vative about upholding the customs of their culture may groups, and the media. The latter teaches them to be
tend toward strict parenting. Accordingly, this poses a their own person, exert their autonomy and independ-
definite risk for South Asian American children living in ence, and follow their own aspirations and desires. They
an individualistic society that focuses on independence see that there is more than one option available and are
and autonomy (Spencer, Harpalani, Cassidy, et al., 2006). encouraged to choose what they feel is best for them-
The South Asian family’s economic status also poses selves (including what makes them happiest). However,
a risk and potential for experiencing a downside of South Asian American children are also expected to fol-
privilege. Mehta suggests that “Given the fact that low what their parents say and fulfill their obligations to
immigration from India has largely been restricted their families. “ The Anglo-American social and cultural
to the educated groups, this immigrant population in the values of self-sufficiency, autonomy, and personal re-
United States, unlike other immigrant groups, has sponsibility are often misinterpreted by immigrant
achieved remarkable economic prosperity” (Mehta, parents as selfish, and the parents place excessive re-
1998, p. 132). Already, Asian Americans are viewed as strictions on their children” (Mehta, 1998, p. 150).
the model minority in U.S. culture. The financial suc- Second-generation South Asian Americans therefore
cess of South Asian immigrants may invoke animosity in receive two completely different messages about con-
other racial and ethnic groups, which can lead to racial struction of self. This dilemma poses a serious challenge
conflicts, leaving South Asian American children vul- for successful identity development. The South Asian
nerable to threats, harassment, and potential violence. family unit can serve as a significant and comforting
Accordingly, from a PVEST perspective, protective fac- source of support, especially given its probable close-
tors generally available to offset more traditional risks ness and intimate nature. Alternatively, given the dia-
may not necessarily serve that purpose if the context metrically opposed identity messages received by youth,
presents other risk factors associated with stigma, families may also represent a significant source of
stereotypes, and associated stress. stress. Family friends who are a part of the South Asian
Gender may be another important risk contributor family’s kin network and the South Asian American’s
that enhances vulnerability. Given the varying expecta- peers from various social settings, including school and
tions and viewpoints in South Asian culture toward both neighborhood, could also serve as potential supports.
sexes, we can expect that the processes underlying iden- However, these supports can also become potential
tity development have the potential to be drastically dif- sources of pain and confusion, depending on how South
ferent for males and females. However, due to lack of Asian Americans choose to handle the decisions regard-
extensive research available with regards to this racial ing choice of spouse and career.
group, it may be a better strategy to focus on South In some South Asian families, it is possible for parents
Asian Americans as a collective whole. to accept and feel comfortable with their son’s or daugh-
Spencer, Harpalani, Cassidy, et al. (2006) suggest that ter’s choice to adapt and conform to their American sur-
the theoretically close relationships assumed for collec- roundings. It is still highly unlikely that conflicts with
tivist cultures are expected to serve as protective factors these more open-minded immigrant parents will never
that are transformed into sources of stress. However, two occur because most South Asian American children de-
of the most problematic, controversial, and emotionally scribe being caught between the two cultures as living an
charged areas in a South Asian American’s life are “American life from 9 to 5 and an Indian life from 5 to 9.
choice of career and romantic relationships because these This duality represents a very important feeling of split
are two of the most important decisions that directly af- identity in the second-generation immigrant from South
fect youths’ relationship with their parents. Mehta sup- Asia” (Mehta, 1998, p. 137). Ideally, these conflicts may
ports this assertion by including “sexual conflicts” and be minor and not hinder the South Asian American’s
“career conflicts” in her five proposed areas of develop- identity development, but it is not always the case.
mental conflicts (1998). However, more generally, the In most instances, children will reach a crossroad at
stressor can be any situation where immigrant parent ex- some point in their lives where they must choose one or
pectations conflict with U.S. societal expectations. the other. The question is which ideology do they follow,
Like other youth of color, South Asian American and how does this play out? Of importance is that
children must confront a complex social environment in PVEST elaborates the role of reactive coping strategies
the United States, including school, university, peer specifying salient implications for identity processes. It
862 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

may be this recursive stage between net reactive coping decisions, especially if subsequently South Asian Ameri-
strategies and emergent identity processes that the South can children regret having listened to their families and
Asian American’s self-perceptions prove to be most cru- are trapped in jobs or marriages that they find unbear-
cial. As described elsewhere, stable self-perceptions can ably dissatisfying and unpleasant. They may eventually
determine whether one uses or downplays certain abili- feel that they wasted their lives on the dreams and expec-
ties, emphasizes or draws attention away from certain tations of their family and culture and come to resent
physical attributes, adopts or suppresses certain behav- both (Spencer, Harpalani, Cassidy, et al., 2006).
iors, and engages in or shies away from certain activities Phoebe Eng, in her book entitled Warrior Lessons,
(Spencer, Hartmann, et al., 1997, p. 47). addresses this choice of many Asian American children:
If a South Asian American chooses to adapt to his or “When asked why we frequently forgo our own wants in
her environment by identifying fully as American and order to fulfill our parents’ expectations, many of us re-
rebelling against their parents to participate in normal spond with reasons that incorporate notions of filial
American cultural activities such as dating, going to the piety and reverence for our elders” (p. 25). With obser-
prom, or even choosing a career path that immigrant par- vations such as Eng’s, we can see how collectivist
ents do not deem as acceptable, this action may be thoughts learned from the family can influence South
viewed as either adaptive or maladaptive and thus Asian American children. The power of “ filial piety”
youths’ weighing of consequences is critical. On the one can be overwhelming and perhaps even suffocating at
hand, this coping strategy can definitely be seen as pos- times, especially considering these feelings can be com-
itive given the tremendous pressure from society to fit plicated further with guilt. Eng comments, “ ‘Filial
in with their peers. However, it can also be seen as nega- piety’ is often synonymous with payback—a [child’s]
tive, considering the tenacity of the bonds between fam- guilt and obedience in exchange for a [parent’s] undying
ily members, second-generation South Asian American but tacit support. Our guilt can come in many curren-
children must face the severe trauma of shaming their cies” (p. 26). These currencies, Eng continues, include
family if they follow the motto of American culture: words from immigrant parents that point out the many
“Following one’s own heart and pursuing one’s own sacrifices they may have made. The opportunity to grow
dreams.” Additionally, depending on the amount of con- up with privileges and luxuries that immigrant parents
servatism the family observes, South Asian American never had and worked hard to give becomes tainted with
children run the risk of losing their families altogether guilt, and this guilt can lead South Asian Americans to
because of exerting their independence. This sense of feel compelled or obligated to obey their parents.
shame and loss of the family happens because, accord- It is important to note that immigrant parents should
ing to their South Asian parents’ perspective, the action not be viewed as selfish or wrong. These views and atti-
of South Asian American children making their own tudes toward children are embedded within South Asian
choices in life becomes translated into exhibiting com- culture and are normal for collectivist societies. How-
plete disregard and disrespect for their elders and family ever when South Asian immigrants are placed within an
as well as displaying outright disobedience. individualistic framework, they may not realize that the
However, if South Asian American children conform same rules and social norms that they held in their
to what their families want for them in cases where they mother country cannot necessarily be applied to all situ-
internally would like to choose another option, this may ations. The amount of conservatism immigrant parents
also have negative and positive adaptive implications. By may observe can directly relate to whether or not they
obeying their parents, these children run the risk of fac- themselves have successfully acculturated and dealt
ing a life of unhappiness because they did not follow their with the immigration process.
own dreams. South Asian Americans may immediately South Asian Americans therefore choose to adapt in
experience this as they begin to feel disconnected from various ways, according to how they perceive they
their peers, particularly if they are not allowed to partic- should. They could go through one extreme, rebelling
ipate in activities and events that are paramount in Amer- against their parents and essentially their culture to
ican culture, such as going out with friends, dating, and identify completely as American, or through another by
so on. The relinquishment of personal desires is exceed- identifying completely with their ethnic culture and
ingly problematic when considering major life-altering denying their national identity. A third possibility is for
Cross-Cutting Human Development Themes of Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 863

South Asian Americans to choose neither. These three selves. These children feel connected to both their South
coping strategies can be maladaptive and lead to poten- Asian heritage and to their national identity as Ameri-
tial problems. A fourth possibility is that they may nego- can (Mehta, 1998).
tiate and balance both identities successfully, which is This leads to the final component of the PVEST
obviously the most positive and adaptive solution. model, “life-stage specific outcomes or coping products”
The next phase of the PVEST model involves emer- (Spencer, 1999a, p. 47). Depending on which emergent
gent identities. If South Asian Americans choose to identity eventually develops, the South Asian American
rebel, they may come to view themselves positively if can experience positive outcomes such as healthy rela-
they gain acceptance and support from their American tionships with their family, friends, kin network, peers
peers or friends. Mehta (1998) coins this identity as the from school or the neighborhood, and so on. They may
“Amerocentric identity” where they hold “strong Amer- experience healthy identity formation. However they
ican values and [have] little contact with their ethnic may also experience identity confusion, psychological
background” (p. 134). They are most likely to feel com- problems, disownment, and development of psychologi-
fortable in U.S. society, but feel disconnected to their cal disorders, which can lead to depression, even suicide.
families. South Asian American children may also view The plight of South Asian Americans is serious when
themselves negatively, especially if they are labeled as considering possible maladaptive coping strategies that
bad by their parents, which translates to meaning that lead to negative emergent identities and unproductive
they are a failure or have brought shame to the family. In outcomes. These problems must not be taken lightly or
extremely strict South Asian households, South Asian overlooked. More and more cases of psychological dis-
Americans may eventually be disowned or cast away turbance, psychopathology, and suicide in the South
from their families. They essentially take on the identity Asian immigrant community are surfacing. Although it
of being the black sheep of their family. is difficult to determine what the exact cause of these
If South Asian Americans conform to their family’s problems may be, it is safe to guess that conflicts with
expectations, they may develop a positive identity with marriage, love relationships, academics, and career play
relation to their parents. Their family and culture be- pivotal roles in troubling and complicating the lives of
come primary sources of support, and they are viewed South Asian Americans to a great extent.
and come to view themselves as the good and obedient Despite the negative stigma attached to mental health
child. However, these children may confront isolation and psychological problems in the South Asian immigrant
and possible humiliation or ridicule from their peers, es- community, more research needs to be conducted to in-
pecially if they focus entirely on studies and do not par- vestigate these issues. Additionally, clinical psycholo-
ticipate in social gatherings, activities, or events. Mehta gists must be cognizant of these problems when treating
(1998) refers to these individuals as having developed South Asian American clients. The issues brought up in
the “Ethnocentric identity” (p. 134). this section underscore the fact that it is imperative for
When South Asian Americans choose neither, they psychologists in the United States to be trained in ethnic
develop the “Compromised identity” which Mehta minority concerns and to be sensitive to the needs of their
claims is the most problematic. In this case, South Asian clients. By understanding the cultural context in which
Americans cannot connect to either their surroundings immigrant children live, psychologists can help South
or their family’s culture and become lost or alienated. Asian Americans develop healthy emergent identities that
Interestingly, this identity may develop if the South leave them with stable, positive coping products.
Asian immigrant parents have not dealt with accultura- Consistent for marginalized youth generally, immi-
tion successfully and they feel confused or conflicted grants specifically, and for youth who enjoy the appear-
about their own identity development since they immi- ance of consonant individual-context experiences (i.e.,
grated themselves (Mehta, 1998). although, as indicated, European Americans may be vul-
The “Bicultural identity,” which results from suc- nerable to the downside of privilege), context matters.
cessful negotiation of both national and ethnic identity, Integrated and implicated in multiple ways from a
develops in those children whose parents are willing to PVEST perspective and as applied uniquely to diverse
be flexible and open-minded toward U.S. culture and youths’ experiences, positive outcomes more frequently
have successfully adapted to the United States them- occur when programming, socialization ef forts, social
864 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

practices, and broad policies are linked to context char- proximal processes improve in quality for individuals in a
acter. Multiple perspectives about ecological contexts given population, these individuals will maximize their
are available, vary by emphasis, and provide unique con- genetic potentials, and observed differences in the popu-
tributions to understanding transactional individual- lation will be due to these differing genetic potentials.
context interactions. Bronfenbrenner and Ceci (1994) derive three propo-
sitions from their bioecological model, which they use
to formulate hypotheses. The first proposition delin-
eates the person-environment interaction in human de-
ECOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES:
velopment, stating “ human development takes place
EXPLORING THE BIOECOLOGICAL MODEL
through processes of progressively more complex recip-
rocal interaction between an active, evolving biopsycho-
As reviewed in several places (e.g., Spencer &
logical human organism and the persons, objects, and
Harpalani, 2004), Bronfenbrenner and Ceci (1994)
symbols in its immediate environment ” (p. 572). Bron-
present four distinguishing attributes of the bioecologi-
fenbrenner and Ceci (1994) note that this interaction
cal model. First, they define measurable mechanisms,
must be consistent over time to effectively facilitate de-
known as proximal processes, through which genetic
velopment. Consistent patterns of interaction are the
influences are actualized into observable phenomena.
proximal processes defined earlier. Examples of proxi-
Thus, the processes that mediate genetic and environ-
mal processes include a child’s interactions with parents
mental influences on human development are a
and peers, development of competencies and problem-
fundamental component of the model. Second, Bronfen-
solving skills, and acquisition of knowledge.
brenner and Ceci (1994) highlight the aforementioned
The second proposition of the bioecological model
variability of heritability, noting that the bioecological
states, “ The form, power, content, and direction of the
model stipulates system variation in heritability as a
proximal processes effecting development vary system-
joint function of proximal processes and characteristics
atically as a joint function of the characteristics of
of the environment in which these processes take place
(p. 570). With this second feature, the bioecological
model provides heritability as a measure—which ironi- Outcome
(Individual’s Phenotype)
cally is the same measure used in most behavioral ge-
netic studies. In the model, heritability is interpreted as High
the proportion of variance attributable to actualized ge- Highest
Heritability,
netic potential (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1993, 1994), as Moderately high Most competent
opposed simply to genetic influences. Bronfenbrenner Heritability,
and Ceci (1994) note that heritability matters; they More competent
view its most important contribution in social science as
those instances when researchers focus on its variabil-
Poor Environment Good Environment
ity. The variation in heritability allows a linkage be-
tween heritability and developmental functioning, with
the proximal processes defined in the first feature serv- Moderately low
ing as the conceptual bridge between the two. The third Heritability,
characteristic of the bioecological model is that it con- Lowest Less competent
Heritability,
siders variation in heritability as it relates to particular Least competent
developmental outcomes (see Figure 15.18). Low
The fourth feature is that the model simultaneously Levels of Proximal Processes
evaluates heritability and absolute level of developmental
competence. Bronfenbrenner and Ceci (1994) postulate Individual
(Genotype)
that improving the quality of proximal processes will lead
both to higher levels of heritability and to elevated levels Figure 15.18 Bioecological model. Figure adapted from
of developmental functioning. The reason for the latter is Bronfenbrenner and Ceci (1994). Source: From Spencer &
readily apparent: Heritability will increase because as Harpalani, 2004.
Ecological Perspectives: Exploring the Bioecological Model 865

the developing person, of the environment—both imme- Ecological Systems Theory


diate and more remote—in which the processes are
taking place, and of the nature of the developmental While the bioecological model lays out the relationship
outcomes under consideration” (Bronfenbrenner & between genetic influences, heritability, and proximal
Ceci, 1994, p. 572). With this precept, Bronfenbrenner processes as they take place in particular environments,
and Ceci (1994) introduce the factors—attributes of the ecological systems theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1979, 1989,
person and the environment, and the character of devel- 1993) focuses on characterizing levels of environmental
opmental outcomes being analyzed—that govern proxi- influence in terms of dynamic, interactive, systems of
mal processes and their impact. person-environment relationships (Figure 15.19).
As suggested by Figure 15.19 and carefully reviewed
With the third proposition, Bronfenbrenner and Ceci
by Spencer and Harpalani (2004), ecological systems
(1994) restate that proximal processes transform ge-
theory is organized hierarchically, involving interactive
netic potentials into actualized outcomes (phenotypes)
systems of increasing complexity embedded in the
and note that the factors that govern proximal processes
framework of human development. Bronfenbrenner
dictate their power to actualize genetic potentials. From
(1979, 1993) begins by transforming Lewin’s (1935)
these three propositions, Bronfenbrenner and Ceci
formulation that behavior is a coupled function of the
(1994) derive three hypotheses. First, they restate the
person and the environment. He substitutes develop-
view that effective proximal processes will increase her-
ment for behavior, stating that development is also a
itability by increasing the proportion of variation attrib-
function of the person and the environment over time.
utable to actualized genetic potential. Second, they
Also noted is the temporal contingence of developmen-
propose that in actualizing genetic potentials, proximal
tal function. Each successive developmental period is
processes work to both enhance competence and reduce
dependent on all previous periods of development.
dysfunction, increasing heritability in both cases. The
Bronfenbrenner (1993) notes that through most of de-
implications that follow from this hypothesis are:
velopmental psychology, theoretical constructs devised to
describe the characteristics of the person have not consid-
1. Proximal processes have more power to actualize ge- ered issues of context; developmental qualities of individ-
netic potentials for positive developmental outcomes uals are conceived without reference to the environments
in organized, advantaged environments than in incon- in which they are occurring (see Figure 15.19). Examples
sistent, disadvantaged environments. of such qualities include standardized psychological mea-
2. Proximal processes have more power to buffer ge- sures such as personality and IQ tests. This type of
netic potentials for negative developmental outcomes analysis reflects the personal attributes model (Bronfen-
in inconsistent, disadvantaged environments than in brenner, 1989) and has a narrow focus on the individual,
organized, advantaged environments. assuming that findings from standardized measures can
be generalized without attention to context. Bronfenbren-
These first two hypotheses essentially state that heri- ner (1993) questions the assumptions of environment gen-
tability, defined in terms of variance attributable to actu- eralizability that underlie the personal attributes model;
alized genetic potential, varies as a direct function of the he does recognize that these standardized measures are
quality of both proximal processes and the environment. useful, but he argues that research designs must simulta-
Bronfenbrenner and Ceci’s (1994) third hypothesis is that neously consider issues of social, cultural, and historical
proximal processes have a greater ability to actualize ge- context and incorporate context-oriented measures.
netic potentials for positive developmental outcomes for Conversely, the social address model (Bronfenbrenner,
individuals living in more inconsistent and disadvantaged 1989), which is the most common approach, considers
environments. Bronfenbrenner and Ceci (1994) cite a few only environmental factors, such as social class, family
studies that they interpret as supporting their first two size, and other demographic variables. The specific char-
hypotheses (Fischbein, 1980; Riksen-Walraven, 1978; acteristics of the environment, activities that occur in par-
Scarr-Salapatek, 1971), and they note the obvious impli- ticular environments, and the impact of these activities on
cations for intervention of the third hypothesis, which is individuals are all neglected in the social address model
derived from the first two. However, the authors also note (Bronfenbrenner & Crouter, 1983). The person-context
that the model still needs to be tested extensively. model examines both the individual and the context but
866 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

during the development of the person (Bronfenbrenner,


Macrosystem 1979, 1989, 1993). The mesosystem describes interac-
tions between the various microsystems in a person’s
Exosystem
life, essentially constituting the network of interper-
Mesosystem sonal relationships that overlap across the various set-
Microsystem tings. The exosystem entails more distal influences,
including the structure of the community where the
person resides and settings where the person is not di-
Individual
rectly involved. Finally, the macrosystem is comprised
Fam

ol

ng
Fam

ho
ly
Sc of the larger societal institutions, such as government,
i

etti
Neig
hborhood
i
ly

kS
economy, media, and so on, which lay the social and
Fr

en

or
i

ds W
historical context for development (Bronfenbrenner,
Social Services 1979, 1989, 1993).
Soc s Spencer and Harpalani’s (2004) analysis suggests
ia l, Cult ence
ural, Historical Influ
that ecological systems theory provides a dynamic,
contextually sensitive framework from which to ana-
Figure 15.19 Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Systems Theory. lyze environments and gene-environment interactions.
Source: From “Ecological Systems Theory” (pp. 187–248), It can be applied to shed light on the behavioral genetic
by U. Bronfenbrenner, in Annals of Child Development. R. theorizing noted earlier. For example, to the extent that
Vasta (Ed.), 1989, Greenwich, CT: JAI; and “ The Ecology of
they can occur, the active and passive effects of gene-
Cognitive Development ” (pp. 3–44), by U. Bronfenbrenner, in
Development in Context: Acting and Thinking Is Specific Envi- environment correlation (Plomin, DeFries, & Loehlin,
ronments, R. H. Wozniak & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), 1993, Hills- 1977; Scarr & McCartney, 1983) primarily involve the
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. lower levels of the model. Individuals may have some
ability to shape their own environments and those of
their relatives (perhaps based on their genetic makeup)
does not analyze the processes involved in development. at the level of the microsystem, and perhaps even at the
This model specifies ecological niches (Bronfenbrenner, mesosystem. However, individuals are by definition not
1989), but it does not delineate the processes by which de- involved in their exosystems of development, and
velopmental outcomes are attained. notwithstanding a few extraordinary examples (e.g.,
Bronfenbrenner’s (1979, 1989, 1993) ecological sys- Dr. Martin Luther King and Mahatma Gandhi), few
tems theory, in contrast, is a process-person-context can influence the macrosystem. Broad, societal influ-
model that, like the bioecological model, highlights ences such as structural racism (Spencer, Cross, et al.,
variability in developmental processes as a function of 2003; Swanson, Cunningham, & Spencer, 2003) and
the characteristics of the person and the environment. racial stereotyping (Harpalani, 1999) are filtered
Ecological systems theory is organized in four levels through macrosystems to impact the development and
of environment, which mediate person-environment experiences of minority youth. Regardless of their ge-
interaction: microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, and netic makeup, individuals can do little to eliminate
macrosystem (refer to Figure 15.19). The first level of these factors. Even more mundane situations of
Bronfenbrenner’s model, the microsystem, involves the stress—such as being ignored in commercial venues
interaction of the person with the immediate social and (e.g., not provided service) or followed and closely
physical environment: home, family, or school settings. monitored in a place of business may be cumulatively
All of the levels of environmental influence are filtered experienced as “microoppression”; given their pat-
through microsystems, where actual experiences take terned and persistent impact, the cumulative exposure
place. Proximal processes, earlier defined in the bioeco- can have a significant impact on stigmatized groups
logical model as the mechanisms through which ge- such as African Americans (e.g., Carroll, 1998).
netic influences are actualized into observable In addition to the uncontrollable effects of the
phenomena (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994; refer to macrosystem and their impact on everyday experiences,
Figure 15.18), are essentially patterns of person-envi- the ability to shape immediate environments is limited
ronment interactions in the microsystem and change for many youth. For example, Stevenson (1997) de-
Ecological Perspectives: Exploring the Bioecological Model 867

scribes how African American youth are “missed” and For twentieth-century America, there have been few
“dissed” by mainstream American society, and how this proximal processes left unaffected by the particular
treatment in conjunction with neighborhood factors re- gene expressions manifested as skin color (see Franklin,
lates to African American youth becoming “pissed” 1968). Additionally, there have been sets of attendant
while managing their anger. Black youth are missed as historical conditions created (Du Bois, 1903), socially
stereotypical media-based images distort the meanings constructed meanings made of race (Pettigrew, 1964),
of their social and affective displays—usually in nega- associated professional practices and perspectives pro-
tive terms. Hence, these unique cultural displays are de- duced (e.g., Kardiner & Ovesey, 1951), and policy deci-
valued and viewed with insolence—dissed. These are sions determined (see Crenshaw et al., 1995). However,
effects of the macrosystem that cannot be regulated by few policy changes up through the last century have been
individuals. In conjunction with these misrepresenta- more pregnant with meaning for youth of color than
tions, many Black youth reside in high-risk contexts Brown v. Board of Education. Consistent with the per-
where anger display may be an appropriate coping mech- spectives shared about immigrant youths’ experiences,
anism. Anger may indeed become a form of competence immigration policy has had parallel influences on the
for social and emotional viability in certain high-risk lives of American newcomers.
contexts, such as neighborhoods, which are microsys- Unfortunately, there is inadequate space for a thor-
tems. These displays may also be misconstrued or con- ough and integrated coverage of the many salient immi-
stitute inappropriate behavior in other microsystems gration policies of relevance for contemporary child
such as school settings. Hence, misrepresentation, disre- development outcomes of special relevance particularly
spect, and hazardous contextual factors at various eco- for marginalized youth. However, excellent reviews are
logical levels interact in creating the anger of Black available. Prashad’s (2000) analysis has particular
youth (i.e., pissed). Phelan et al. (1991) describe how salience for the contemporary experiences of South
dissonance between various microsystems—hence, at Asian Americans. Further, many of the early policy de-
the level of the mesosystem—can impinge on resiliency cisions remain especially significant for today’s new-
and health development. comers given the greater within-group diversity and
It is useful to consider the role of the genotype in eco- relevance of the historical timing of arrival for particu-
logical systems theory. Ecological systems theory fo- lar waves of immigrants. The within-group diversity of
cuses on delineating environmental influences rather values, beliefs, global economic factors, and perspec-
than genetic influences. The genotype (i.e., genetic tives along with inferences and stigma held by the
makeup), is an implicit component that Bronfenbrenner broader society (e.g., model minority stereotypes) have
obviously considers but does not identify explicitly in particular relevance for the experiences of all youth of
his model. This is probably because the genotype in- color and immigrants, particularly more recent arrivals
volves only the person and not person-environment in- (e.g., Hmong) and second-generation Asian Americans
teraction; it is not changed by environmental influences. (e.g., see Koshy, 1998).
Indeed, focus on the genotype in part leads behavioral Yet, independent of their actual authenticity, social con-
geneticists to often take a rather static view of human structions and inferences made about the psychosocial
development. However, as reviewed by Spencer and functioning of diverse group members matter. They repre-
Harpalani (2004), the important component in develop- sent the Zeitgeist held about diverse group members’ so-
ment may not be the genotype itself, but rather its ex- cial standing and inferred psychological well-being. Most
pression (i.e., phenotype). The expression of the important, from a PVEST perspective that acknowledges
genotype is dependent on environmental interaction, and the central role of ecological and phenomenological fac-
this expression cannot be determined accurately without tors, externally based assumptions and assessments of
actually observing the particular genotype being ex- salience to policy matters may actually function in opposi-
pressed in a given environment (Gottlieb, 1995). While tion to their intended role. As significant influences on pol-
the genotype is an important and measurable component icy decisions, particularly in the case of undervalued
in developmental analysis, its expression (and thus its (marginalized) youth and families, manifested policies
true impact) should be considered in conjunction with may inadvertently serve as sources of risk as opposed to
environmental influences and developmental processes. their desired and intended protective function (attenuating
Specifically, cognition-linked perceptions matter. the significant social vulnerability of particular citizens).
868 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

TWENTIETH-CENTURY SOCIAL SCIENCE roneously, their experiences are assumed to be homoge-


ASSUMPTIONS AND PRACTICES, nous as the minority case, and as suggested, they are
JUDICIAL DECISIONS, AND THEIR most frequently represented in the literature under cate-
IMPLICATIONS FOR THE NET gories of deviance, difference, problems, pathology, and
VULNERABILITY OF DIVERSE YOUTH deficit (see Spencer, Brookins, & Allen, 1985). More-
over, the two juxtapositions frequently ignore the multi-
As a basis for providing alternative and inclusive human ple contributions of the social and physical ecology:
development theorizing relevant to diverse youth, we Except for crediting diverse youth of color for the char-
described the benefits of PVEST. A major rationale acter of the context, and thus, the source for the group’s
supporting the framework’s use is its applicability to all high vulnerability ( higher risks than available protective
individuals irrespective of group demographics (e.g., factors).
economic status, race, ethnicity, nativity, and immigra- Unfortunately, as such, this latter perspective contin-
tion status). The framework’s inclusive character repre- ues to the disadvantage of some youth (i.e., immigrants,
sents an important benefit because the literature low-income families, and youth of color) because it com-
narrowly and frequently infers deviance, pathology, and municates the penchant that individuals are solely re-
problems for those categorized as different (i.e., from sponsible for their own social situations. The working
the accepted and nonstigmatizing norm of being Cau- notion reinforces the stereotype that the problem or
casian and middle income). As described, the early re- major sources of risk reside in individuals who deter-
search on Black children was conceptually flawed in ministically create their own environments. Thus, the
multiple ways (see Spencer, 2005). And at the same assumption is that they are responsible for their own dis-
time, traditional and widely disseminated child devel- advantaged situation. The stereotypes reinforced by
opment research efforts assumed the findings to be rep- narrow and under-representative scientific efforts have
resentative of all youths’ experiences (i.e., diversity failed to contribute to inclusive theorizing, proactive re-
within-group is generally not inferred). Specifically, search traditions, or culturally authentic programmatic
the operating perspective suggests that European applications that adequately address and enhance human
American experiences represent the normative standard development stage-specific outcomes for diverse groups.
for all. For the most part, process-oriented developmen- As a process-oriented, culturally sensitive and identity-
tal analyses have been restricted to middle income peo- focused framework that addresses the “ how” (which is
ple or European Americans. Conversely, problem- or important for application), PVEST aids in filling the
pathology-oriented outcome-focused studies have pri- long-term void described by S. H. White (1996), Cole
marily been assumed as representing the experiences of (1996), Lee et al. (2003), and others.
minorities in the social science literatures generally but S. H. White’s (1996) chronicling of the history and
continue to characterize the developmental sciences cultural shortcomings of contemporary research sug-
specifically. gests a significant lack of inclusive thinking. The ICE
perspective provided by PVEST in response, given S. H.
Inferences, Assumptions, and What Brown v. White’s critique, brings attention to the patterned lack
Board of Education Should Have Considered: of cultural competence and generally inadequate re-
Contemporary Implications for Research, search perspectives (including acknowledgment of the
Practice, and Theorizing broad diversity both within and between groups). Infre-
quently acknowledged, except to occasionally lament
The pattern of published research suggests specific as- their existence, the problematic conduct, character, and
sumptions such as an unacknowledged status of privi- interpretation of such programs of research remain en-
lege for some (i.e., and who represent what is thought of demic to the social sciences and have been highlighted
as normal for all), and an inferred homogenous experi- more recently as “outcome disparities,” particularly in
ence of atypicality for most others. Inferable from the the education and health literatures. As a consequence,
developmental sciences is that normal remains the given the shortcomings noted, social policies themselves
provenance of European Americans and middle-income have provided neither the conceptual leadership nor the
people. At the opposite end of the spectrum, marginal- social and psychological protective functions usually in-
ized group members are conceptualized as the other. Er- tended or inferred from good social policy.
Twentieth-Century Social Science Assumptions and Practices, Judicial Decisions, and Their Implications 869

At the same time, the broad privileges associated coping success and failure often occurs in the same
with middle and high socioeconomic status and Euro- family, where environmental influence may be similar
pean American group membership continue to be under- across siblings (one son becomes a medical professional
appreciated and more frequently generally ignored (see and a second becomes an inmate in jail). A particular
McIntosh, 1989). Luthar’s (2003) program of research strength of PVEST is its capacity for understanding the
with affluent Caucasian suburban youth provides the unique experiences of each sibling by including the con-
potential for new beginnings for understanding the sideration of phenomenology. Youths’ meaning-making
special character of vulnerability experienced by privi- can be totally different even if, for example, brothers are
leged youths. Similar to Roediger’s (2002) analysis, believed to be exposed to the same parental messages,
equally relevant is Moore’s (2002 and documentary, ecolevel structural conditions including the character of
http://www.bowlingforcolumbine.com) perspective con- neighborhood resources and challenges.
cerning school-based mass killings such as the one at Additionally, explicit experiences of cultural social-
Columbine High School. Given his analysis, evidence for ization and carefully developed strategies of parental
a downside of privilege begs attention. Generally speak- monitoring predict positively to healthy emergent identi-
ing, questions concerning the relationships between ties and developmental stage-specific coping products
privilege and underdeveloped coping skills are needed (e.g., completing school, obtaining employment, living a
for the design and implementation of intervention and healthy lifestyle; Spencer, 1983, 1990; Spencer et al.,
prevention strategies. Inadequate coping responses to 1996). Thus, given the variability possible, context-
many common but painful adolescent or childhood stres- linked challenges confronted, and coping processes
sors, such as teasing and marginalization, are inade- evoked (i.e., independent of shared demographics, child-
quately addressed in the literature, particularly as they rearing, and schooling environments), both within- and
relate to privilege, coping strategies, and completed sui- between-group diversity of outcomes is possible. Inclu-
cide rates. This oversight in the child and adolescent lit- sive conceptual frameworks should be amenable to and
eratures is especially troubling, given that White males effective in explaining the “ how” of (a) youths’ unique
on average have the highest completed suicide rate meaning-making processes that govern coping and iden-
among adolescents and individuals in middle adulthood tity processes, ( b) their active usage or self-restraint of
(see Carroll & Tyler, under review). On average, how- opportunity structures (e.g., optimizing or pushing back
ever, they are also expected to obtain the highest life from educational options provided through public school-
course earnings and complete the most years of educa- ing), and (c) children’s broad range of within-group
tion. Accordingly, high suicide rates among White behavioral variation. The wide spectrum of adaptive cop-
males in adolescence and middle adulthood (see Carroll ing prospects are not independent of the fact that youths,
& Tyler, under review) appears inconsistent with other as they transition across time and place, may be burdened
indicators of mean successes accrued. This disparity with stereotyping encounters or stigmatizing imagery
suggests the need for intervention /prevention strategies (i.e., of themselves or their referent group) and limita-
and analyses that differ from those assumed for margin- tions of proactive or adaptive coping experiences given
alized youths and particularly males. assumptions frequently associated with privilege, or its
Speculations about the challenges faced by youth of absence. Moreover, youths’ ensuing social epistemologies
color and low socioeconomic-resourced youth may be that result (i.e., given stage-specific developmental varia-
more readily associated with the lack of opportunity, tions in youths’ perceptual processes and phenomenol-
economic difficulties, and underserviced neighbor- ogy) promote further variability in coping strategies
hoods. Irrespective of the many challenges and the available and outcome options.
media exploitation of obvious coping failures, the ma- As inferred from Chestang (1972), the persistent en-
jority of young people successfully tackle the develop- counters with hostile environments are often based solely
mental tasks described by Havighurst (1953). Their on group-linked stigma such as race, gender, low socio-
successes are generally underacknowledged; in fact, economic resources, skin pigmentation, ethnicity, and
their resiliency amidst persistent challenges suggests immigration status. We suggest that how young people
that many demonstrate carefully honed adaptive coping make meaning and cope with challenges require dy-
skills. Resilient outcomes, however, are not solely de- namic, recursive, and culturally sensitive interpretive
pendent on environmental influence. The variability of frameworks. These conceptual advantages promote the
870 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

design, determination, and construction of the most ap- awareness of bias; Spencer, 1985, 1999b, 2001), and (c)
propriate and effective prevention /intervention supports. cultural embeddedness (e.g., Lee et al., 2003). The effi-
PVEST serves as an effective conceptual tool for de- cacy of a theory as a process-oriented conceptual tool
scribing the range of coping strategies possible and aids should aid in explaining the “ how” of outcomes, given
in determining the character of coping options that an in- the breadth of experiences possible and the persistence
dividual might use in the moment. Some responsive and character of situations under which many children
strategies might evoke jail-time sentencing decisions by struggle and have little control. For example, the tradi-
the judicial system (e.g., see Stevenson, 1997) or precip- tional literature on service learning and volunteerism
itate effectance motivation that can lead to youths’ high positively acknowledges and credits supports provided
levels of success and competence (e.g., see R. White, by youth only if provided to others or nonrelated groups.
1959, 1960). The range of reactive coping responses pos- For some children, the garnering of official approval,
sible may result in resiliency for some (i.e., positive out- support, and recognition for service learning provided to
comes in the face of inordinate challenge) or produce their immediate or extended family system and commu-
unfortunate labeling and unproductive coping products nity is equally important and should earn equivalent psy-
that lead to the oft-reported group disparities for a par- cho-social benefits and academic reward to youth when
ticular characteristic (e.g., academic underperformance, compared against service provided to unknown others
health status, special education placement, and dispro- (Spencer & Cassidy, 2004).
portionate sentencing to the criminal justice system). Given the salient role of context, PVEST satisfies
In contrast, the several mass school shootings over criteria for good theory as suggested by S. H. White
the last decades, most recently that at Columbine High (1996). Specifically, it accommodates the considera-
School (Moore, 2002 and documentary, http:// bowling- tion of traditional externally based information about
forcolumbine.com), certainly make the point of privi- study subject(s) such as neighborhood and community
leged youths’ understudied but apparent vulnerability, characteristics and assessments. These are frequently
which is consistent with the patterned suicide rates for obtained through “ windshield” observations of context
White males. Although generally lacking parallel media or similar strategies. This information resource is par-
coverage, which is frequently reserved for mishaps com- ticularly relevant for studies of older middle childhood
mitted by low-resource youth, particularly youth of youth and especially adolescents who transition across
color, these unfortunate situations also fall into the cate- neighborhoods as an aspect of the developmental task
gory of unproductive coping processes and outcomes. requiring the establishment of broader social relation-
Their patterned quality for privileged youth suggests a ships. Parental reports and other interview data sources
particular level and character of vulnerability. Although are also useful context-based information in answering
not generally discussed, these themes deserve theory- the “ how” question from a PVEST perspective.
driven scholarly analyses for informing intervention and Achievement testing process and outcome data repre-
prevention efforts. As most evidenced by stereotyping sent an additional context-linked data source on par
of marginalized youth, there is a potential downside to with teacher ratings. Considered together, PVEST pro-
bringing attention to a specific need because the result- vides an analysis, which includes the individual’s own
ing stigma increases individuals’ experiences of risk and view as well as the transactional contributions of the
underscores the need for additional protective factors. context; thus, it provides an ICE perspective that repre-
For example, the media acknowledges and highlights sents a valuing and recognition of the individual’s own
particular disparities, which further stigmatizes referent perspective or phenomenology considered from within
group members and can lead to situations of stereotype the cultural context.
threat (Steele, 1997, 2004; Steele & Aronson, 1995). Ac- The varied meaning-making, coping processes, and
cordingly, a theoretical perspective relevant to diverse emergent identities of youth may be associated with
youth (i.e., both privileged and those inordinately chal- several possible levels of environmental hostility or
lenged) should accommodate the exacerbating impact of racism, which are contributed to by social infusions of
(a) maturation-linked complexity (e.g., early versus late what Steele and colleagues (Steele, 2004; Steele &
physiologic maturation; Spencer, Dupree, Swanson, & Aronson, 1995) characterize as “stereotype threat ” or
Cunningham, 1998), ( b) transactional-determined dy- inferred hostility (see Chestang, 1972). Further, when
namism (i.e., individual-context interactions; e.g., considered developmentally, the responsive coping
Twentieth-Century Social Science Assumptions and Practices, Judicial Decisions, and Their Implications 871

styles may be highly heterogeneous in character and do May 31, 1955; and Bolling v. Sharpe, May 17, 1954.)
not necessarily include the internalization of negative Balkin (2001) instructed participants to draft an opin-
affect assumed by the larger society given the group’s ion (a majority opinion, concurrence, or dissent) based
social placement and denial of rights (see Spencer, on the material available in 1954. The contributors ad-
1990; Spencer & Markstrom-Adams, 1990; Swanson, dressed three sets of issues. Given feedback and overall
Spencer, Harpalani, Noll, Seaton, et al., 2003). From a case analysis, Balkin argues that Brown is one of the
policy perspective, the nuanced and developmental- most recognized and treasured court decisions in his-
sensitive perspectives published in the past 25 years tory. He states:
were not in place when the Courts determined Brown v.
Board of Education in 1954 (see Cross, 1991). As a The civil rights policy of the United States in the last half
major historical event, the decision’s salience and after- century has been premised on the correctness of Brown,
math greatly inf luenced social and educational policy, even if people often disagree (and disagree heatedly)
practice, and legal decisions. Most important, the deci- about what the opinion stands for. No federal judicial
sion continues having significant relevance today—50 nominee and no mainstream politician today would dare
suggest that Brown was wrongly decided. At most they
years post-Brown.
might suggest that the opinion was inartfully written, that
Shortcomings of Mid-Twentieth-Century Social it depended too much on social science literature, that it
Science Contributions to the Brown Decision did not go far enough, or that it has been misinterpreted by
legal and political actors to promote an unjust political
The research efforts of Kenneth and Mamie Clark agenda. The use made of Brown is often criticized, but the
(1939, 1940) were cited as a footnote in the U.S. idea of Brown remains largely sacred in American politi-
Supreme Court’s landmark Brown v. Board of Education cal culture. (Balkin, 2001, p. 4)
decision (see Lal, 2002). The published scholarship and
known programs of research available at the time were However, as recently analyzed (see Spencer, 2005),
used to support the notion that Black children developed the sentiment described was clearly not evident in the
a low self-esteem due to segregation, and that integra- first decade following the decision; indeed, there was
tion was necessary to mitigate feelings of inferiority. much upheaval over the decision. Many opponents ar-
The 50th anniversary of that decision suggests an appro- gued that it was more a sociological rendering than a
priate point to integrate, revisit, and reanalyze the legis- legal decision, claiming that it ignored history and legal
lation’s impact for youth development but particularly precedent. The South raised massive resistance claim-
for marginalized youths’ academic experiences and out- ing that Brown was an abuse of judicial power. However,
comes in U.S. school settings. The power of better the- Brown has gradually come to be highly esteemed.
ory for interpreting the Clark’s research without either Balkin (2001) claims that the decision falls in line with
castigating the researchers or pathologizing the children the U.S. “Great Progressive Narrative,” which states
is overdue. The case enlightens both the importance of that the United States through its Constitution and
training diverse investigators and having authentically through history is gradually reaching its social justice
inclusive human development theory for interpreting goals and that the United States is essentially a just so-
findings and enhancing the efficacy of policy decisions ciety and that through struggle and history these goals
for the common good. will be realized. The fame of Brown is ironic, in that
Jack Balkin’s (2001) edited volume, What Brown ver- today, many public schools remain segregated by race
sus Board of Education Should Have Said, reports on his and are often taught by teachers who have difficulty ed-
request of a group of constitutional scholars to rewrite ucating certain children, particularly those with whom
the opinion in Brown v. Board of Education. He asked they lack history (see Ladson-Billings, 1994). During
the specific question, “How would you have written the the 1970s and 1980s, southern schools were rapidly de-
Brown opinion in 1954, if you knew then what you segregated turning the south into one of the most inte-
know now about the subsequent history of the country grated parts of the country. However, more recently
and the progress of race relations in the past half cen- there has been resegregation of schools based primarily
tury?” (Balkin, 2001, p. ix; Note: It should be acknowl- on differential resources structured demographically
edged that the opinion called Brown is actually based along the poverty line, and the penchant has visible
on three opinions: Brown I, May 17, 1954; Brown II, racial consequences.
872 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

The pace of desegregation slowed in the middle education compared to that provided by public schools in
1970s, in part, because of Supreme Court decisions wealthier, Whiter suburban districts. As a major and
that failed to regulate desegregation across primarily consistent source of risk, the schooling contexts are not
White suburb and minority inner city boundaries. For equivalent for youth of color versus privileged youth.
example, Milliken v. Bradley, 1974, in Detroit, “ freed However, achievement gap language implies that it is an
white suburban districts from any legal obligation to individual’s own performance inadequacy that is the
participate in metropolitan desegregation efforts” culprit rather than sets of policy determined, con-
(p. 6). Despite these court implications, desegregation structed, and supported social inequities. Accordingly,
actually continues into the 1980s. As chronicled by inclusive and sensitive theorizing matters for producing
Balkin (2000), during the 1990s, however, the Supreme the most beneficial policies and practices.
Court actively restricted their supervision of school
districts with:
Traditional Assumptions, Beliefs, and Values:
• 1991, Board of Education v. Dowell: The Supreme The Conceptual Benefits of a Phenomenological
Court held that “courts could end desegregation or- Variant of Ecological Systems Theory Framework
ders in school districts that had attempted in good Given the process-emphasizing rendering of PVEST in
faith to comply, even if this would result in immediate Figure 15.12, good developmental theory appears im-
resegregation” (p. 6). The replacement of Justice portant because it explains how youth respond to exter-
Thurgood Marshal (major civil rights supporter) with nally imposed inequities and cope with adverse
Justice Clarence Thomas in 1991 gave further impe- ecological conditions or what Chestang (1972) refers to
tus to this trend of restricting court supervision. as character development efforts pursued in persist-
• 1992, Freeman v. Pitts: The Supreme Court held that ently challenging environments. Brown has come to
“courts could end some aspects of school desegrega- mean different things to different people: It has also
tion orders even if other aspects had never been fully been used in diverse ways by different groups. Brown
complied with” (p. 7). was implemented to support public equity in buses,
• 1995, Missouri v. Jenkins: The Supreme Court “over- pools, restaurants, and retail outlets (i.e., all important
turned an ambitious plan for magnet schools in contexts of socialization and youth development) and
Kansas City designed to attract white students back has become a symbol of equality generally, beyond
into inner city schools” (p. 7). racial equality. This is both interesting and informative
given the intent of the originating cases.
Two salient implications and consequences of these Brown opposed Plessy v. Ferguson, 1896, which al-
Supreme Court rulings ensued: (1) The federal court’s lowed for segregation of public spaces and gave justifi-
desegregation orders have been largely stopped, and (2) cation to Jim Crow. The only Justice to oppose this
districts technically subject to court orders face no en- decision was John Marshall Harlan who maintained
forcement activity. Thus, in the 1990s, there has been a that this country should not support a caste society and
tendency to resegregate. An implication is that schools that it should be color-blind. His words have been ap-
can be segregated by race as long as the status is not due plauded by civil rights supporters. However, more re-
to direct government impetus. In his analysis, Balkins cently, the differences between anti-classification and
(2001) states that “racial segregation today is the result anti-subordination have become more apparent and are
of a complicated mix of social, political, legal, and exceedingly important for youths’ developmental op-
economic factors, rather than the result of direct state portunities and experiences. As one example, there are
commands ordering racial separation . . . it remains those who support classification in affirmative action
overwhelmingly the case that minority children in cen- to promote Black equality (see Balkan, 2001).
tral cities are educated in virtually all-minority schools According to the anti-subordination approach, the
with decidedly inferior facilities and educational oppor- question is not whether the law classifies by race, but
tunities” (p. 7). Evident is that Brown called for the im- rather whether the law is doing anything to remedy sub-
portance of equal educational opportunity; however, it is ordination or whether it is doing something to enhance it.
evident that minority students do not receive an equal Anti-classification, because the law does not currently
Twentieth-Century Social Science Assumptions and Practices, Judicial Decisions, and Their Implications 873

directly impose classification, actually tends to work to Using Chestang’s (1972) analysis of the ecology, the
perpetuate Black inequality because it fails to acknowl- goal was to suggest a less hostile context of development
edge the dif ferential ways in which Blacks have suf fered for those suffering under Jim Crow practices. Brown
cumulative ef fects and continue to be burdened by social made sense as a blow to communism and to demonstrate
and economic subordination. Thus, this tends to sanctify the democratic tenets of our constitution and the ways
the many indirect practices that subordinate Blacks, the United States upholds these despite its lived societal
without directly classifying: “It encourages people to inconsistencies. Considered from a PVEST perspective,
explain persistent black inequality as the result of pri- the penchant produces a level of official deniability rel-
vate choices, cultural differences, or black inferiority ative to responsibility for diverse youths’ life course
rather than at least partially as the result of facially neu- coping patterns, processes, and outcomes.
tral legal policies that help preserve social stratifica- Many critics maintain that the view of the Supreme
tion” (Balkin, 2001, p. 13). The New York Times (Rimer Court as brave and a progressive, counter-majoritarian
& Arenson, 2004, June 24, Sec. A, P1) described analy- institution is largely a myth and that is does not work to
ses by two Harvard researchers, Skip Gates and Lani favor minorities in the face of majority opposition. As
Guanier. The researchers suggest that selective school Derrick Bell (cited in Balkin, 2001) maintains, Brown is
attendance by Blacks represent immigrant Blacks and congruent with his “interest convergence thesis,” which
not indigenous Blacks for whom the Brown decision was maintains that advancement for Blacks will only occur
targeted. The admissions profile favoring immigrants if these advancements are in White interests. Bell ar-
imply efforts to satisfy the law without considering its gues, “ending Jim Crow—at least formally—was in the
spirit and the variability of experience by ethnicity and interests of northern whites and the foreign policy es-
immigration status within race. The admissions profile tablishment. However, the convergence of interests be-
and its implementation by respective selective college’s tween blacks and white elites did not mean that whites
admissions office provides a powerful illustration of the had an interest in full social and economic equality for
broad manipulation of opportunity thus suggesting that blacks, and the limited convergence of interests that did
supports are not always supportive to those actually in- exist did not last. Actual desegregation remedies were a
tended to benefit. Selective postsecondary schools of long time in coming and fell far short of providing gen-
higher education such as Harvard, Yale, and Princeton uinely equal educational opportunities for blacks” (see
communicate a solid track record of admitting Blacks. Balkin, 2001, p. 22). From a child development and
However, the admissions procedures in practice at select parental socialization perspective, an understanding of
schools, in fact, admit Blacks of first- or second-genera- such nuanced analyses is required and itself represents
tion immigrant status whose academic record and an unfair burden for minority group and disenfranchised
schooling experiences lack the cumulative intergenera- parents. Appropriate use of the legal insights requires an
tional effects of long-term U.S. race-based hostilities, ability to translate complex policy-relevant information
which the Brown decision sought to remedy. into particular parenting strategies: For parenting of Eu-
The broad controversy between color-blindness and ropean American or well-resourced youth, a goal would
equal citizenship extends into every part of anti- be to counteract easily inferred privilege assumptions by
discrimination law and policy. Each side substantiates young people. For poor and marginalized youth, the goal
their claims in Brown. Balkin’s (2001) analysis of the would be to monitor and support youth development in
feedback suggests an important role for the politicized ways that would reinforce effectance motivation and
views of the moment. Actually, the Balkin perspective model adaptive coping strategies needed for responding
underscores the powerful role of the political-ecology to socially unacknowledged, unfair, and ethnicity-linked
and contends that the Court rulings coincide very challenges.
smoothly with the political climate and are not, as some Critics maintain that Brown is a symbol of how inef-
have argued, free from this context. Even Brown was not fective the Supreme Court is at promoting social change.
revolutionary in an isolated sense. From a global and They argue that the lack of implementation and the rever-
broad psycho-historical perspective, it was relevant to sal in the 1990s demonstrates the ineffectiveness of judi-
the Cold War climate and the image U.S. leaders wanted cial change. For example and as reported by Balkin
to present abroad to diminish criticism of Jim Crow: (2001), Rosenberg in his 1991 book, The Hollow Hope:
874 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

Can Courts Bring about Social Change? argues that cluding Cleveland, Dallas, Denver, Minneapolis, and so
Brown’s role in promoting equality has been overstated. on, “ were allowed by judges in the late 1990s to phase
Rosenberg suggests that enforcement did not proceed out or terminate court supervision” (p. 212). Hence, in-
until the Civil Rights Act of 1964 and rather that Brown tegration of schools vastly declined in the late 1990s.
instigated a forceful resistance from southern Democ- Several court decisions in the late 1990s ended ef-
rats. Also, critics oppose the view that Brown initiated forts to desegregate (e.g., busing) and also ended
the civil rights movement, naming the many grass-roots race-based decision-making in many schools, thus
efforts that proceeded Brown. Balkin (2001) claims that canceling affirmative action practices. Data indicate
legal precedent such as Brown is important in that it that gaps between Black and White achievements are
structures the discourse that can ensue. Brown provided a increasing again in the 1990s after slightly decreasing
symbol, linking the Constitution and rule of law to civil in the 1970s and 1980s. However, the data also suggest
rights and racial equality. Balkin maintains that Brown is that the gaps remain even in districts that are inte-
currently very important and the argument is not over grated and of a higher economic status. Family struc-
whether it was correct, but rather, contesting its mean- ture, income level, and educational level did not seem
ings and implications. Interestingly, 50 years later, the to alter these gaps.
argument is whether the underlying principle of constitu-
tional equality provided by Brown is anti-classification or Cross-Disciplinary Contributions from
anti-subordination. “Is the real evil of Plessy the classifi- Legal Studies
cation of persons by race or is it the subordination of one
race by another?” (Balkin, 2001, p. 55). Following the Like Balkin, Sarat (1997) argues that Brown chal-
Brown decision, there were no provisions for equalization lenged legal precedent and rearranged the ways in
and true integration, rather the specified legal doctrine which the law could work toward progressive change in
noted that states could not assign pupils based on race society. Sarat states: “Brown was at once a turning
alone. Many states adopted choice plans and the use of point and a source of resistance, a point of pride and an
private schools to avoid directly contesting Brown. object of vilification. Its legacy, like the legacy of all
Balkin states, “Indeed, because the schools were techni- great historical events, is, even today, contested and
cally ‘desegregated’ once overt assignments by race uncertain” (p. 5). Although not acknowledged, it is evi-
ended, one could argue that there was no continuing obli- dent that a strong legal position continues to be the
gation to equalize facilities between schools; he notes problem of subordination: Racism, stereotypes, and
that the situation might be even worse than it was before low expectations continue to haunt particularly His-
Plessy” (p. 65). Importantly, there has been little discus- panic and Black children as an integral and persistent
sion in the youth-focusing social sciences about the im- aspect of their social ecologies. Importantly, theories
pact of these events on the psychological context of child of development that guide the training of teachers in
development research, on everyday coping experiences in pre-kindergarten through 12th grade and beyond nei-
the daily lives of children, or on the myriad levels of ther adequately nor authentically represent the individ-
youths’ socialization contexts. ual-context experiences of diverse youth. Parents
In the 1990s, significant disillusionment surrounded frequently appear equally handicapped in providing the
Brown, given that a majority of Blacks still receive sub- necessary supports as counteractions against race-
standard education in deteriorating schools and that the linked challenges. Teachers and administrators not
push for desegregation for the most part has been re- only miss adequate training information about youth of
jected. In particular, public schools in urban areas con- color but also are frequently not accountable for ana-
tinue to be mostly Black: “In 1998–1999, 90% of the lyzing their own internalization of subordination be-
public school students in Chicago were African Ameri- liefs. Except for the scholarly thrust of the CRT
can or Latino; 83% lived in poverty-stricken house- initiatives, Whiteness studies (in general), and White-
holds. In Detroit, 90% of public schools students were privilege perspectives (specifically), there remains a
black and 70% were poor enough to qualify for free strong pattern in social science and developmental sci-
school lunches; more than one-half of the city’s students ence to inadequately consider race or to attempt to
did not graduate from high school” (Balkin, 2001, minimize or to statistically control for its impact on
pp. 211). In addition, many large school districts, in- youths’ coping processes.
Twentieth-Century Social Science Assumptions and Practices, Judicial Decisions, and Their Implications 875

Critical Race Theory (CRT): Contributions ployment and income support the growing concern that
and Limitations the slow racial advances of the 1960s and 1970s have
Another important development in scholarship on ended, and retrogression is well under way” (Bell,
racism, CRT (Delgado & Stefancic, 2001), draws its ori- 1999a, p. 2).
gins from legal studies and represents another histori- CRT, as a discipline, is based on the foundations laid
cally important innovation. CRT began as a critique of by critical legal studies and feminism and its intellec-
liberal post-civil rights ideology that de-emphasized the tual ties extend back to U.S. thinkers, such as W. E. B.
role of race in U.S. society. The CRT movement has Du Bois, and European theorists such as Gramsci and
drawn attention in a variety of academic disciplines, in- Derrida (Delgado & Stefancic, 2000, 2001). From a
cluding education, the humanities, and the social sci- PVEST perspective, it delineates the role of race for
ences. In parallel with sociological theorizing, CRT vulnerability and demonstrates the countless ways in
highlights racism as a normal component of society, not which race for youth of color increases net stress level
an aberration, and examines the subtle racial interests through its manifestation of subtle and explicit daily
and tangible consequences of legal decision-making and challenges borne by youth and their families. CRT ques-
policy in a society marked by racial hierarchy. Addition- tions the very foundations of legal reasoning, and al-
ally, CRT scholars often use rhetorical devices and nar- though CRT still predominantly produces new legal
rative storytelling, often autobiographical, to illustrate discourse, analysis is now applied more broadly in
everyday manifestations of racism and their connection fields, such as education, where the tenets of CRT are
to broader, structural forces. As a representation of a used to understand tracking, curriculum, and the
phenomenological perspective, CRT begins to link indi- history of IQ and achievement testing (Delgado & Ste-
vidual experiences with societal theories of racializa- fancic, 2001). Thus, from the child and adolescent de-
tion, integrating the structural, ideological, and material velopment perspective of the social sciences, CRT
expressions of racism in the routine course of daily life. provides an important and generally overlooked contex-
tual perspective by defining it in race-linked terms. CRT
Critical Race Theory: Tenets, Analytic Applica- is inherently nondevelopmental; however, it provides
tions, and Theoretical Renderings of Race. As a lenses for articulating the influence of racism and
movement that is both theoretical and activist oriented, stigma as a presence and potential impact on normative
CRT arose from the discipline of legal studies. CRT de- developmental tasks pursued by youths. Its inclusion
parts from many other disciplinary regimes as it at- and or consideration represents a significant variation
tempts to name the inequities in our current social from traditional child development approaches that at-
structure and then re-envision the categories and as- tempt to clarify the salience of race through statistical
sumptions that we as society use to frame our world. strategies that categorically defines the impact of race
Formally begun in the mid-1970s through the founding for hypothesized outcomes. Further, as a statement
work of Derrick Bell (e.g., Bell, 1995, 2000a, 2000b) about everyday practice as supported by the law, CRT
and Alan Freeman (e.g., Freeman, 1995), CRT poses specifies the differential character of race-based prac-
critiques that disrupt the blinders that social systems tices as experienced by marginalized youth. When con-
create in an attempt to make racism invisible. The per- sidered from a PVEST perspective, CRT provides a
spective forces an acknowledgment of the ways in which service to the social sciences by fully specifying the
discourse and social structure cooperate in the realiza- varied contributions to vulnerability level as experi-
tion of inequities and racism. enced by diverse youth. As supported from legal stud-
The CRT approach was situated in a particular histor- ies, the perspective specifies the contributions to the
ical period and arose primarily as a dialogue with and in high-risk context of development as experienced by
response to the civil rights movement. The theorists youth of color and, at the same time, specifies the im-
were critical of the changes brought about by this move- plicit protective character of racial group membership
ment, in particular, the lack of steady advancement in particularly for European American children.
racial reform in the United States. Regarding the more CRT incorporates two main concepts from the field
recent retrogression of progress that have been made of critical legal studies. First, legal indeterminacy ac-
since earlier legal gains (Delgado & Stefancic, 2000), knowledges the subjectivity of legal outcomes and main-
Bell (1999a) indicates that “statistics on poverty, unem- tains that “not every legal case has one correct outcome”
876 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

(Delgado & Stefacic, 2001, p. 5). The legal outcome these assumptions and the liberal notion of change in the
depends on whose interpretation is privileged, which is law through the process of gradualism. As alluded to
dictated by the power and authority of various interpre- previously in this chapter, CRT draws on a term created
tations. Second, favorable precedents tend to erode over by Derrick Bell, interest convergence, which describes a
time due to the ways in which lower courts interpret phenomenon in which Whites will only support Black
precedents using narrower definitions and because of advances if these advances serve White interests. Build-
the general lack of enforcement of legal doctrine (Del- ing on this concept, critical race theorists contend that
gado & Stefacic, 2001). the civil rights movement and the current legal structure
CRT also incorporates concepts and terminology do not foster structural change and that only through
from feminism. Specifically, it applies theoretical femi- drastic alterations to systems, structures, and ideologi-
nist views on power, the construction of social roles in cal foundations can true racial progress and equity
society, and its use of hegemony. As reported by Hall ensue. These linkages are too infrequently considered
(2000), the concept of hegemony was developed by both in the design and conduct of child and adolescent
Gramsci and is defined as “ total social authority” ac- research, theory, and policy and in their interpretation
quired through the combination of coercion and consent in the social sciences.
at the economic, political, ideological, intellectual, and Critical race theorists maintain that “structural de-
moral levels. CRT also works in the feminist premise terminism” in society impedes racial reform through a
that legal and social theory have practical consequences variety of means. Freeman argues that civil rights law
in society and that these consequences must be ad- enforces racial progress at a slow rate, creating a social
dressed (Armour, 2000; Delgado & Stefacic, 2001). control mechanism that insures enough change to pre-
Delgado and Stefancic (2000, 2001) summarize four vent civil uprising while not actually altering the status
underlying tenets of CRT. The first tenet maintains that quo (Delgado & Stefancic, 2001). The awareness of this
“[R]acism is normal, not aberrant, in American Soci- may have implications for differences in cultural social-
ety” (Delgado & Stefancic, 2000, p. xvi). As opposed to ization practices, thus also an implication for youths’
being relegated to the undifferentiated level of the ecol- cultural identity formation, manifestations as part of re-
ogy described by Bronfenbrenner as the macrosystem, active coping strategies for some, and implications for
instead, racism is conceptualized as an intricate part of youths’ degree of individual-contextual fit (e.g., see
the everyday life experiences in the United States and is Swanson et al. 1998).
woven into all institutions and social interactions. It is One of the tenets of CRT establishes that race is a
an ingrained aspect of our society to such an extent that product of social construction. Their analysis suggests
racist practices and interactions are deemed as normal. that prevalent definitions and assumptions in society are
Most important, as understood in the social sciences never innocent of the inequities shaped by power rela-
generally and child research specifically, the roots of tions. Youths’ social-cognitive maturation makes the
these injustices are ignored or frequently not perceived. awareness of inequities difficult to articulate but remain
Formal equal opportunity rules and laws target extreme, unavoidably experienced as either a source of risk or a
overt injustices but do not in any way address these mi- contributor of a protective factor. Thus, from a develop-
crolevel everyday expressions of racism. For example, mental perspective given maturing perceptions, CRT in-
legal doctrine requires the demonstration of intent of creases our understanding of youths’ vulnerability level,
racist action to litigate. However, this intent is often dif- coping processes, and stage-specific coping outcomes,
ficult to prove and racism is frequently built into the given the several developmental tasks requiring mastery.
fabric of U.S. social structures and institutions such that As an important conceptual contribution to child de-
a specific intent is not immediately apparent. From our velopment scholarship, CRT acknowledges the impor-
perspective, legal doctrine has no precedent for address- tance of context and uses legal studies to further specify
ing these forms of racism. its impact. For example, the legal system functions
The second tenet assumes a critique of liberalism. based on the premises of generalizability and essential-
According to Delgado and Stefancic (2000, 2001), liber- ized notions of right and wrong. These premises do not
als uphold the neutrality of the law and view it as objec- provide the legal space for consideration of the ways in
tive and morally accurate. However, CRT questions which individuals are inequitably influenced and shaped
Twentieth-Century Social Science Assumptions and Practices, Judicial Decisions, and Their Implications 877

by their specific contextual factors. CRT uses the term plores the historical process by which immigrants
intersectionality to acknowledge the complex, contra- earned their status as White. The process of American-
dicting, and cumulative ways in which race, gender, izing European immigrants (“White ethnics”) allowed
class, and sexual orientation affect an individual’s posi- them to be accepted as White rather than Irish or Polish.
tioning in larger social structures. CRT contends that Underlying premises of CRT are instituted to critique
racism exists and continues in our society because the the justice system, the educational system and policy af-
mind-set of the majority of people in the United States fecting the broader society.
has not changed, despite the civil rights movement. This CRT has also been useful to analyze the implications
lack of change in mind-set introduces an entrenched co- of various educational legal reforms including integra-
nundrum: Although unacknowledged, its impact is in- tion and affirmative action (which affects a broad
clusive of those individuals responsible for funding, range of institutions including the educational system).
designing, implementing, publishing, and interpreting Derrick Bell (2000b), in Serving Two Masters, looks at
social science research both for children of color and the outcomes of integration mandates and asks an im-
European American youth. Its use in the training of pro- portant question: Is racial balance in the schools enough
fessionals and their modeling of the content (e.g., in to provide educational equity? Bell contends that his-
youth and family service delivery and education prac- torically, since the 1930s, the approach of the National
tice) further contributes to its entrenched character, for- Association for the Advancement of Colored People
midability, and persistence. (NAACP) has been to eliminate racial segregation
Encompassed under the umbrella of CRT are other across society. This approach took precedence in the
subsidiary studies including critical White, Asian, and education system, particularly, with the passing of
Latino studies and feminist and gay critical studies. Brown in 1954. The subsequent lack of compliance with
Asian studies look critically at the model minority the legal precedent set by Brown led to a series of school
stereotype, while feminist and gay studies look at inter- litigation cases being filed. For some theorists, the lack
sectionality. Critical White studies examine the ways in of compliance has not been unexpected and, again, reaf-
which Whiteness is socially constructed and how groups firms the salience of history for contemporary and en-
have historically moved in and out of this category. For trenched practices.
example, certain ethnic populations in the United States W. E. B. Du Bois named the educational dilemma for
such as Jews, Italians, and Irish have moved into the cat- our society in 1935 and his concerns remain prevalent
egory of White, after being labeled as non-White during even today and, although infrequently acknowledged,
their early history in the United States. Such studies also are particularly relevant in the field of child develop-
explore the ways in which privileges associated with this ment. Integration has not proved to be the silver bullet
label structure power relations and how discourse for educational equity in our society, as predicted by
through literature and cultural forms powerfully rein- Du Bois, and more comprehensive changes need to be
forces stereotypes and the values associated with White- made in society before educational equity can be a real-
ness in contrast to color (Delgado & Stefancic, 2001). ity. Du Bois states:
In sum, unlike the traditional approach to child de-
velopmental research, critical White studies contribute [T]he Negro needs neither segregated schools nor mixed
by affording an opportunity to deconstruct and specify schools. What he needs is Education. What he must re-
the process of normal human development experienced member is that there is no magic, either in mixed schools
in context for members of diverse groups. Roediger or segregated schools. A mixed school with poor and un-
sympathetic teachers, with hostile public opinion, and no
(2002) looks at the social construction of Whiteness and
teaching of truth concerning black folk, is bad. A segre-
maintains that, historically, this category has been ig-
gated school with ignorant placeholders, inadequate equip-
nored. This continues to be the case particularly in the
ment, poor salaries, and wretched housing is equally bad.
adolescent and child development literatures. Roediger Other things being equal, the mixed school is the broader,
cites Fusco who contends that to ignore this category more natural basis for the education of all youth. It gives
tends to “redouble its hegemony by naturalizing it ” wider contacts; it inspires greater self-confidence; and
(Roediger, 2001, p. 327), thus Whiteness becomes fur- suppresses the inferiority complex. But other things sel-
ther ingrained as the normative standard. Roediger ex- dom are equal, and in that case, Sympathy, Knowledge,
878 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

and the Truth, outweigh all that the mixed school can AN EXAMPLE OF AFRICAN AMERICANS
offer. (Bell, 2000b, p. 243) AND THE ACHIEVEMENT GAP: AN
INTERFACE OF STIGMATIZING CONTEXT,
Critiques of Critical Race Theory and Their Im- REACTIVE COPING RESPONSES, AND THE
plications for Child Development Research. It is “ACTING WHITE” MYTH
important to note that CRT has not been without its
share of critiques. Delgado and Stefancic (2001) relay In their analysis and critique of the acting White myth,
general critiques of the CRT approach, and some schol- Spencer and Harpalani (under review) describe Ford-
ars in the broader discipline of legal studies have cri- ham and Ogbu’s (1986) position that one of the main
tiqued the CRT’s use of storytelling in the law. Their reasons that Black students do so poorly in school is that
critique rests on several premises, which are not unlike “ they experience inordinate ambivalence and affective
the standard research tensions between the use of quan- dissonance in regard to academic efforts and success”
titative versus qualitative research approaches in child (Fordham & Ogbu, 1986, p. 177). Fordham and Ogbu
development research: (a) These stories may not repre- further state that the cultural orientation in the Black
sent the typical experience of members of the larger community is that learning is equated to “acting White.”
group of individuals of color, ( b) narratives lack analyt- Although roundly critiqued by Bronfenbrenner (1985),
ical rigor, and (c) stories told by individuals of color sti- their three-group classification describes “autonomous”
fle wider discussion due to the general belief that these minorities as those who are minorities primarily in the
individuals have a superior understanding of race. Some numerical sense, “immigrant ” minorities as those who
scholars critique the perspective held by critical race arrived voluntarily with hopes of improving their lives,
theorists that truth is a social construct created to fur- and “castelike,” or involuntary minorities (e.g., Blacks,
ther the position of those in power. Native Americans, and Chicanos) as those who came to
Another critique of CRT is that it focuses on chang- America involuntarily through slavery or conquest
ing discourse and cultural forms of racism and does not (Fordham & Ogbu, 1986; Ogbu, 1986).
speak to the deeply ingrained structural /material Focusing on the castelike group, specifically Blacks,
racism, more strongly affecting the poor. This critique they purport to examine the expressive dimension of the
maintains that CRT focuses primarily on issues of iden- relationship between minorities and the dominant cul-
tity and does not incorporate a thorough enough class ture. In exploring the African American and European
analysis. Even in the discipline of CRT, theorists voice a American relationship, they focus on twin phenomena:
need to develop further theories that look at the inter- oppositional cultural identity and oppositional frame of
section between race and class. Also, critical race theo- reference (Ogbu, 1990). Their analysis concludes that
rists express the need to develop a broader global African Americans develop a sense of identity in oppo-
analysis of race, looking at the connections of sweat- sition to the social identity of Whites due to their treat-
shop concerns—the oppression of people of color ment in both the political and economic domains.
abroad working low-wage jobs—and the unemployment Fordham and Ogbu’s (1986) analysis suggests that an
of people of color in the United States (Delgado & Ste- oppositional frame of reference is used as a protective
fancic, 2001). device as a strategy to promote Black identity and foster
When considered together, Whiteness studies delin- the maintenance of Black / White cultural boundaries,
eating the impact of racism and privilege along with thus development of protective devices is aimed at pro-
CRT increase our understanding of proximal processes moting Black identity. They posit that the setting and
experienced by America’s diverse youth post-Brown. maintenance of boundaries reinforces solidarity and
Unfortunately, the links are seldom acknowledged and unity, suggesting that slavery reinforced this tendency
the oversight has important implications for the continu- through the creation of traditions such as beliefs con-
ing dissemination of stigma and unquestioned assump- cerning duty-bound relationships that extended beyond
tions concerning privilege. As elaborated, there are few family relations (e.g., fictive kin networks).
demonstrations of the interaction of these themes than They suggest an emphasis on group loyalty in situa-
in the acting White myth associated with African Amer- tions involving conflict and competition with the major-
ican youth achievement and coping patterns (see ity group. Fordham and Ogbu (1986) suggest that these
Spencer, Cross, et al., 2003, pp. 276–287). ideas influence children’s perception of their potential
An Example of African Americans and the Achievement Gap 879

for success (see also Fordham, 1988, 1996). As ana- At the start of the twentieth century, Carter G.
lyzed by Spencer, Cross, et al. (2003), Fordham and Woodson was surprised at the copious evidence con-
Ogbu’s contention is that, accordingly, Black students, cerning Black education (Woodson, 1919) that resulted
especially adolescents, attending both integrated and in his 454 page scholarly text. In thinking about freedom
predominantly Black schools face the burden of acting for slaves, in a theoretical sense, certain owners became
White. This line of reasoning suggests that Black curious as to whether emancipated Blacks could handle
youths’ academic prowess and success are controlled by freedom and the level of education demanded by the sta-
external and within-group factors. Thus, according to tus as a free person; consequently, certain individual
Fordham and Ogbu, “Apparently, Black children’s gen- slaves were turned into test cases. Bullock (1967) de-
eral perception that academic pursuit is ‘acting White’ scribes how John Chavis of North Carolina was selected
is learned in the Black community. The ideology of the as an experimental case and sent to Princeton. Chavis
community in regard to the cultural meaning of school- not only passed his tests with flying colors but also re-
ing is, therefore, implicated and needs to be reexam- turned to North Carolina to become a leading teacher for
ined” (p. 203). Overall, Fordham and Ogbu conclude the children of the planter class. Both Bullock (1967)
that Blacks have not historically valued education and and Woodson (1919) underscore that the atmosphere of
define academic achievement as a White cultural attrib- the revolutionary period stimulated a certain degree of
ute. There is major shortsightedness in the conceptual- leniency concerning the formal education of Blacks, and
ization of this work, which is consistent with the flaws the curiosities and whims of the planters resulted in
noted initially concerning the need for new theory. The highly educated slaves. A few became part of the popu-
first flaw is the absence of historical accuracy. lar culture of the times, including Phyllis Wheatley, the
Black poet, and Benjamin Banneker, the Black mathe-
matician and author of the first Farmer’s Almanac ever
Historical Overview of African American
written in the United States. It is during the revolution-
Achievement Motivation
ary period that the manumission of individual slaves
It is commonly assumed that slavery made achievement helped produce large clusters of free Blacks in Atlanta,
motivation impossible. As described, critiqued, and Charleston, and other major cities across the South; and
analyzed by Spencer, Cross, et al. (2003), the view sug- as these clusters of free Blacks formed communities, the
gests that from slavery forward, Blacks struggled not development of formal educational structures became an
only with external sources of oppression but also with immediate enterprise.
the effects of slavery, such as family dysfunctionality, Another ripple effect was the birth of the abolitionist
psychological hatred, high rates of criminality, and low movement. White leaders of this movement took the
achievement motivation: the pattern is inferred by likes of Frederick Douglass, assisted in his formal edu-
some to indicate a “mark of oppression” (see Kardiner cation, and turned him loose, as an icon for their move-
& Ovesey, 1951, as the classic illustration of the deficit ment. Beyond assisting individuals, the movement helped
assumption). However, historical evidence traces the found colleges where scores of Blacks could obtain a
evolution of Black achievement motivation. The record higher education. For those Whites who could not fathom
shows that from the end of slavery and well into the a nation that included Blacks, but whose moral compass
twentieth century, Blacks, as individuals and as a so- required that Blacks be educated, manumitted, and,
cial group, evidenced high achievement motivation, then, sent back to Africa (colonization), also created ed-
and we are hard-pressed to think of any White ethnic ucational opportunities for Blacks, including the con-
group, including Eastern European Jews, who evi- struction of schools.
denced a higher regard for achievement motivation for
the period from the end of the Civil War to the early
Black Response to Freedom: A Social
1930s. The insufficiency of efforts to translate such
Movement for Education
high achievement motivation into social mobility was
not linked to the legacy of slavery but to the larger soci- As delineated by Spencer, Cross, et al. (2003) as a his-
ety’s failure to cultivate, compliment, reinforce, and torical overview, the Black community enjoyed a critical
authenticate the numerous manifestations of Black mass of educated leaders, teachers, and potential educa-
achievement motivation. tional administrators. Du Bois estimated that 150,000 of
880 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

the 4 million slaves were able to read and write, and lit- seated achievement motivation that had its origins in
eracy was commonplace among free Blacks (Du Bois, slavery itself. As Anderson noted, “[B]lacks emerged
1935; p. 638). Thus, the source of the social movement from slavery with a strong belief in the desirability of
suggests ex-slaves themselves because they seemed to learning to read and write. This belief was expressed in
carry educational designs in their ragged pockets as they the pride with which they talked of the other ex-slaves
crossed over into freedom. After the Civil War, edu- who learned to read or write in slavery and in the esteem
cated Blacks and their White allies were dumfounded in which they held literate Blacks. It was expressed in
and even mildly shocked at the level of educational de- the intensity and frequency of their anger at slavery for
mands the ex-slaves immediately made on themselves keeping them illiterate” (Anderson, 1988; p. 5).
and their leaders (Anderson, 1988): “A teacher at Port W. E. B. Du Bois noted that the ex-slaves’ immediate
Royal declared that he could not set forth, in anything drive for education was one of the most amazing stories
like adequate terms, the eagerness and determination in the annals of Western history and culture (Du Bois,
with which the Black freedmen apply themselves, young 1935). Had this drive and achievement motivation been
and old, to the task of learning the alphabet [Another positively received, cultivated, protected, and sustained
teacher was greeted by] a motley assemblage [who were] by the larger society, within 100 years Blacks would
cold, dirty and half naked but eager to learn; [she found have outstripped the social mobility patterns of all
them less concerned with food and clothing] but anxious groups in modern American society, including Eastern
to feel sure that they would have the privilege of coming European Jews. As noted by Spencer, Cross, et al.
to school everyday” (Butchart, 1980, p. 169). Available (2003), one is hard-pressed to think of any White ethnic
research reported by Spencer, Cross, et al. (2003) indi- group who entered the United States at the turn of the
cate that the hunger for education came not simply from twentieth century with a collective achievement motiva-
the eyes of children as “many teachers conducted night tion that could match that exhibited by the ex-slaves on
school as well as day schools to accommodate the aspira- the heels of the Civil War. They suggest that those who
tions of [adult] workers” (Butchart, 1980; p. 170). In would try to draw a straight line connecting contempo-
sum, the drive for Black education was organic: It came rary problems in Black education with the legacy of
from the ex-slaves themselves. slavery, such as Fordham and Ogbu (1986), are ignorant
Just after the Civil War, John W. Alvord was ap- of this resilient history.
pointed the national superintendent of schools for the Reconstruction did not bring the ex-slaves land redis-
Freedman’s Bureau, and in 1866 he reported that Black- tribution, education, protection, liberty, the right to
controlled schools numbered 500 and were to be found vote, or social justice. During the bleak period between
all over the South. Just 3 years later, in 1869, Black 1900 to 1930, historians have recorded that Blacks
schools, now often associated with evolving Black would often double and triple tax themselves to build
churches, totaled 1,512, with 6,146 teachers and and sustain schools, despite not receiving a fair share of
107,109 pupils. The demand for education and the de- tax dollars already paid.
sire to be taught by Black teachers quickly strained the In effect, Black citizens of Mississippi, Georgia,
teaching force that first greeted the ex-slaves and their North Carolina and South Carolina, and other southern
children. To meet the demands being made by the ex- states reached deep into their grinding poverty and
slaves, educated Blacks, Northern societies, and the fed- found the wherewithal—the achievement motivation—
eral government helped to launch such new entities as which propelled them to support the education of their
Howard University and other Black colleges (Anderson, children. History does not support Fordham and Ogbu’s
1988; Bullock, 1967; Du Bois, 1935). Spencer, Cross, (1986) contention that Black communities have not val-
et al. (2003) suggest that the drive toward Black educa- ued education and that this devaluation is a reaction to
tion, after the Civil War, can only be given its due if White racism. The assumptions of Fordham and Ogbu
framed as a social movement—not a trend, not a drift, (1986) are woefully negligent of the resilience that
and not a contrivance imposed by White allies from the many African Americans displayed in the face of viru-
North. While educated free Blacks and White allies lent institutionalized oppression. Their shortsighted
where crucial elements, the sheer scope of the educa- perspectives have contributed to what Claude Steele
tional demands and depth of ex-slaves revealed a deep (2004) refers to as social and identity contingencies that
An Example of African Americans and the Achievement Gap 881

reinforce social stigma and dissonance in the individual- and Cooper (1996), which illustrate adaptive coping in
context match. the school context and show how peers of the same race
in different contexts interpret the same phenomena dif-
Developmental Considerations for Research on ferently. As noted earlier, such within-group variation
Black Academic Achievement is also largely neglected by Fordham and Ogbu (1986).
As analyzed by Spencer, Cross, et al. (2003), in addi- In a study of 562 Black adolescents, aged 11 to 16
tion to neglecting the resilient history of Black achieve- from a southeastern U.S. city, Spencer, Noll, Stolzfus,
ment motivation, the acting White myth also makes and Harpalani (2001) found that individuals with a Eu-
large conceptual errors in its analysis of racial identity rocentric orientation, as indicated by a high score in
and cultural influences on human development. For ex- Cross’s (1971, 1991) Pre-Encounter stage, exhibit lower
ample, the acting White hypothesis can be criticized academic achievement and lower self-esteem than those
on several grounds. It draws psychological inferences individuals who have a proactive Afrocentric orienta-
from sociohistorical data (Trueba, 1988), neglects tion—the Internalization stage. While individuals with
within-group variation, and fails to understand and a reactive Afrocentric orientation (Immersion-Emersion
capture the psychological and developmental processes stage) performed poorly, the study indicates that a
that lead to academic achievement and other outcomes. strong, proactive sense of Black cultural identity is as-
As noted by Obidah (2001), Ogbu (1990) does not ac- sociated with positive academic achievement for Black
count for variation in the so-called involuntary or youth. The study by Spencer et al. (2001) illustrates the
castelike minorities and fails to note that some mem- importance of considering both adaptive and maladap-
bers of the majority group demonstrate characteristics tive coping outcomes and shows how Black racial iden-
he associates with involuntary minorities (MacLeod, tity can be related to both. It also contradicts the claims
1987). Further, when considering Bronfenbrenner’s of Fordham and Ogbu (1986) and Fordham (1988,
(1989) ecological systems theory, it is apparent that 1996), who contend that Blacks must distance them-
Ogbu (1987; Fordham & Ogbu, 1986) limits much of selves from Black culture to achieve. As reviewed by
his analysis at the level of the macrosystem, narrowly Spencer, Cross, et al. (2003), it is interesting to note that
focusing on the social and historical conditions impact- Fordham and Ogbu’s (1986) paper was not the first aca-
ing African American academic achievement. He neg- demic article to use the phrase “acting White.” The ear-
lects the micro- and mesosystems, thereby failing to liest reference we found was a 1970 article by McArdle
understand how these larger conditions play out in and Young entitled “Classroom Discussion of Racial
everyday lives. The previous section that reviewed the Identity or How Can We Make It Without ‘Acting
history of Black achievement motivation illustrates White?’ ” The very question posed by McArdle and
well how Ogbu’s macrolevel analysis falls short in its Young (1970) implies that it is possible to make it with-
inferences about the attitudes and motivation of out acting White, an idea denied by Fordham (1988) and
African Americans toward education. Fordham and Ogbu (1986).
Further, as reported by Spencer, Cross, et al. (2003), Adolescents of all ethnicities engage in identity
Ford, Harris, Webb, and Jones (1994) note that Ford- searching and strive for acceptance and approval.
ham (1988) neglects the phenomenon of “code switch- Cross’s (1971; Cross et al., 1991) Immersion-Emersion
ing”—demonstrating different modes of cultural identity stage may be characterized by anti-White atti-
communication and style in different settings—and tudes. While this can be maladaptive, it is not an abnor-
that Black students can become bicultural rather than mal initial reaction to devaluation, particularly at this
“raceless” and maintain their ethnic identities. Bicul- salient developmental period. If Black youth perceive a
tural identity is one of the different modes of the Inter- classroom, school, or other setting as a context where
nalization stage that Cross (1995) has described in his they are devalued, they may cope by defining the expec-
model. It also represents a particular emergent identity tations of this context as acting White. However, this
in the PVEST framework, one that is not represented in is reflective of normative identity development
Fordham’s (1988, 1996) or Fordham and Ogbu’s processes rather than a cultural devaluation of educa-
(1986) work. Other coping options also sensitive to di- tion. As Spencer et al. (2001) note, “acting White has so
verse youth more generally are described by Datnow many potential variations based on multiple contextual
882 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

realities” (p. 28). The acting White phenomenon is not plore broader forms of discrimination and the ways in
responsible for Black academic underachievement, nor which racism is institutionally sanctioned and pro-
is it reflective of a broad cultural frame of reference, as duced through structural inequities.
Fordham and Ogbu (1986) suggest. It is simply one of Essed (2002) develops an understanding of racism
many coping responses to feeling devalued. Curiously, that synthesizes the ways in which microaggression
and acknowledged by Ogbu (1985), when describing works in conjunction with and through systemic in-
high academically performing, first-generation South equities. Essed defines “everyday racism” as:
Asian immigrants, this high achievement has never been
a process in which (1) socialized racist notions are inte-
described as acting White. As noted by Trueba (1988),
grated into meanings that make practices immediately de-
culturally based assumptions that represent sociohistor- finable and manageable, (2) practices with racist
ical assumptions are at best troubling. At worst, they implications become in themselves familiar and repeti-
serve as an additional source of stress and challenge to tive, and (3) underlying racial and ethnic relations are ac-
Black academic achievement and youths’ effectance tualized and reinforced through these routine or familiar
motivation. practices in everyday situations. (p. 190)
As reviewed by Harpalani and Spencer (2005), irre-
spective of whether racism functions as product of Furthermore, everyday racism is not necessarily com-
economic relations, sociological relations, or as a conse- municated through direct interactions with people of
quence of some configuration between the social and color. For example, journalists produce their everyday
economic race is defined by Omi and Winant (2002) as a products that perpetuate racist discourse and policy-
“concept which signifies and symbolizes social con- makers produce programs that can inadvertently per-
flicts and interests by referring to different types of petuate inequities. Essed maintains that “ when racist
bodies” (p. 123). Stoler (2002) complicates this notion notions and actions infiltrate everyday life and become
of race by contending that there is a historical diver- part of the reproduction of the system, the system re-
gence between racism as overt, biologically based, and produces everyday racism” (2002, p. 188). Hence,
psychologically based and the new racism as insidious, macro and micro forms of racism are extensions of one
cultural, and complex. There is a historical divergence another as microaggressions while racists are not al-
in capturing racism as a difference between the seen ways founded by intentional prejudice.
and the unseen, on noticeable differences or on more Omi and Winant (2002) refer to a “common sense”
subtle cultural differences. The selection of biological understanding, which they contend is a normative sys-
traits and/or cultural qualities for classification is a so- tem of ideas and practices—perpetuated through educa-
cial and historical process, not rooted as a biological tion, the media, policy, and so on. Similar to Essed’s
given. Over time, different groups of individuals have characterization of everyday racism, racial inequities
moved in and out of racial categories as economic and are perpetuated through these commonsense ways of
social conditions have changed. Hence, one can locate a relating and being in such a way that they are more in-
plurality of racisms constructed as a result of the com- sidiously woven into the fabric of society rather than ex-
plex relationship between specific social, cultural, and pressions of direct racial prejudice. However, the
economic factors (Hall, 2002; Omi & Winant, 2002; existence of this form of racism in society does not
Stoler, 2002). Omi and Winant (2002) characterize eliminate more direct forms of prejudice. Rather, Omi
“racial formation” as the “sociohistorical process by and Winant maintain that our understanding of racism
which racial categories are created, inhabited, trans- has broadened such that we now understand it to exist in
formed and destroyed” (p. 124). plural forms that function through diverse contexts and
Another historical way of categorizing racisms is ar- power dynamics.
ticulated by Omi and Winant (2002) who explain the It is through the development of this normative sys-
ways in which definitions and understandings have be- tem that hegemony operates. Hegemony defines the
come more complex only recently. They contend that ways in which relations of power frame the shape of
racism, before the 1960s, was understood using a rather racism. This form of racism, as discussed previously in
limited lens—expressed through interpersonal rela- this review, is related to the historical and temporal con-
tions and extending from individual prejudices. During text. This context is also shaped through power relations,
the 1960s, our understanding of racism expanded to ex- and the form of racial inequity is molded through the
Racism and Human Development 883

ways in which hegemony functions through coercion and forms. Goldberg (2002) contends: “ The rights others as
particularly, through consent. Omi and Winant (2002) a matter of course enjoy are yet denied people of color
argue that as racial rule transitions in many societies because black, Brown, red and yellow subjectivities
from dictatorship to democracy, rule is based more di- continue to be disvalued; and the devaluation of these
rectly on processes of consent rather than coercion, and subjectivities delimits at least the applicability of rights
beliefs concerning racial superiority and inferiority are or restricts their scope of application that people of
a part of the discourse. color might otherwise properly claim” (p. 299). Thus,
Cornel West (2002) has sought to locate the founda- the progressive subtlety and subjective character of
tion of the idea of Black racial inferiority in Western racial beliefs and their impacts concerning perceived
culture. His efforts include an attempt to characterize inferiority and superiority of particular societal mem-
the genealogy of modern racism, looking specifically at bers is not independent of developmental factors. Ac-
the cultural discourse. He makes links between the dis- cording, as a final theme and as reviewed by Harpalani
course associated with modernity and the ways in which and Spencer (2005), when considered especially for
White supremacy became substantiated as a dominant children and adolescents, it is important to detail the
discourse. West contends that modern discourse of the impact of denied rights as subjectively experienced in
late-seventeenth and eighteenth centuries in Western the course of human development.
Europe and during the Enlightenment was structured
such that the discourse of Black beauty and intelligence
as equivalent to White standards was silenced. The
modernity discourse of science and rationality fostered RACISM AND HUMAN DEVELOPMENT
the observation, comparison, ranking, and ordering of
physical characteristics of human bodies. The intellec- As described elsewhere (see Harpalani & Spencer,
tual ordering and co-option of classical norms of beauty 2005) and inferred from the integration and analysis
and culture made the equality of Black beauty and intel- presented, racism is an insidious and omnipresent phe-
ligence not only unreasonable but also barbaric. West nomenon that is translated through multiple levels of so-
(2002) argues that White supremacy is connected not cial, cultural, and historical contexts and affects a large
only to economic relations and psychological needs of impact on human lives. The traditional definition of
the White ruling class but also to the ways in which dis- racism as simply discrimination based on race does not
course is structured. Only certain notions of race are cover the vast range of ways in which this phenomenon is
conceivable based on the structuring of the dominant manifested. As experienced by youth, racism is signi-
modern discourse. fied not only by discriminatory behavior but also by
Goldberg (2002) complicates West’s analysis structural relationships, political ideologies, and institu-
through linking historical conceptions of moral subjec- tional practices, all of which are often viewed as norma-
tivity, the creation of racial exclusion, and modernity. tive components of our society and a critical aspect of
He maintains that only in the second half of the twenti- every day life for all. These structural and ideological
eth century has the concept of rights been claimed as components are highly institutionalized, thus affecting
“ vested and borne by the subject ” (p. 298). This con- individual experiences and life trajectories, not only by
ception of rights has fueled the critical attack on racial disadvantaging people of color but also by privileging
discourse. However, Goldberg cites MacIntyre’s char- White people. To help understand how racism operates,
acterization of “rights as a moral fiction,” which we integrate various theoretical traditions here, examin-
claims to rest justifications on objective notions of ing social, political, and cultural forces that shape his-
morality but in actuality the moral order is based on tory and society and the ways in which these forces
subjective standards that shift over time and space. impact individuals’ identities and everyday lives.
Goldberg points out the subjectivity of the moral order Classical opinions about racism, while acknowledg-
and how historically, morality has been used in diverse ing its damaging impact, tended to view this phenome-
ways to both justify and critique the racial order. Moral non as a set of aberrant attitudes and behaviors
categories are derived from the social structure and resulting from mistaken beliefs or miseducation. Newer
moral construction discourages certain racist expres- theories have critiqued this approach and provided a
sions while failing to recognize and discourage other broader, systemic delineation of racial hierarchy in
884 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

societies. To understand the salience of racism for Building on Omi and Winant’s (1994) ideological no-
human development and life-stage specific outcomes, it tions of racial formation, Bonilla-Silva (2001) outlines a
is first necessary to examine its broad, systemic char- theory of racism that is both structural and process-
acter. However, a societal approach to racism by itself oriented: the racialized social systems framework. In
is not sufficient for developmental science; it is also this framework, racialized social systems are societies
necessary to examine the different ways that youth that allocate material and social rewards differentially
cope with experiences of racism that present many by race, and such societies develop a particular racial
challenges for youth of color and unrecognized privi- structure: the set of social relations, cultural practices,
leges for White youth. To accomplish this end, both so- and tacit and explicit assumptions, based on physical
ciological theories of racism and contextually sensitive distinctions that govern the social construction of racial
frameworks for human development are useful, and groups in the society.
these can be integrated to understand how systemic Inclusive theories of human development, in
racism impacts the lives of youth. conjunction with systemic analyses of racism often
Among the most widely cited works on the sociohis- found in CRT, can help elucidate how racism impacts
torical formation of racism is Omi and Winant’s (1994) developmental outcomes. For example, Bronfenbren-
treatise, Racial Formation in the United States. This text ner’s (1989) ecological systems theory provides an ef-
outlines the various paradigms that sociologists in the fective framework to analyze the role of social, cultural,
twentieth century have employed to understand race and and historical context in human development, and thus
racism. The authors highlight the social construction of to illustrate the impact of racism at multiple levels. As
race: Racial groups do not reflect discrete biological reviewed by Harpalani and Spencer (2005), in its role as
categories, but are social and political divisions based a process oriented theoretical tool, ecological systems
on superficial physical features—divisions that are cre- theory offers mediating points between larger struc-
ated primarily for the purpose of economic and political tural processes and the immediate settings where
subordination (i.e., racism). Omi and Winant (1994) racism is encountered in everyday life. A final step for
note that in sociological theorizing, analyses of race and developmental scientists interested in the effects of
racism have often been subsumed under other para- racism is to employ an identity-focused, contextually
digms, including ethnicity (with an emphasis on culture sensitive, theory of human development that can illus-
and assimilation processes), class (with a Marxist orien- trate how bias (e.g., racism) impacts experiences, cop-
tation underscoring economic and labor divisions), and ing, identity formation, and outcomes from a
nation (with a focus on anti-colonial struggles and col- developmental perspective—through all stages of the
lective consciousness formed in response to these strug- life course. We posit that PVEST (Spencer, 1995) pro-
gles). Omi and Winant’s (1994) perspective centers on vides such a framework (see Figure 15.12).
racial formation, which they define as, “ the sociohistor- As suggested, PVEST links context and experience
ical process by which racial categories are created, in- with individual meaning-making and identity formation,
habited, transformed, and destroyed” (p. 55). Racial all from the perspective of human development. While
formation emphasizes the intersection of various influ- ecological systems theory provides a means for describ-
ences in this process of racialization and recognizes ing hierarchical levels of context, PVEST directly illus-
racial formation as a phenomenon that occurs at multi- trates life-course human development as influenced by
ple levels, with macrolevel structural manifestations these multiple levels of context. PVEST serves as a
that translate into individual everyday experiences at model to examine normative developmental processes—
the microlevel. Omi and Winant (1994) discuss racial framed through the interaction of identity, lived cultural
formation in terms of “ historically situated, racial proj- experiences, and tangible manifestations of racism, in-
ects,” which are posited interpretations and explana- cluding White privilege, for youth of all ethnicities. In
tions of the racial dynamics in a society and serve to doing so, it takes into account individual differences in
reallocate resources to various racial groups. Racism is experience, perception, and negotiations of stress and
constituted by those racial projects that “create or repro- dissonance. PVEST demonstrates a recycling through
duce structures of domination based on essentialist cat- the life span, as individuals encounter new risks and pro-
egories of race” (p. 194). tective factors, experience new stressors (potentially
Conclusions and Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 885

offset by supports), establish more expansive coping CONCLUSIONS AND


strategies, and redefine how they view themselves, PHENOMENOLOGICAL VARIANT OF
which also impacts how others view them. Thus, as elab- ECOLOGICAL SYSTEMS THEORY
orated in Harpalani and Spencer (2005), PVEST is a INFLUENCED INTERPRETATIONS OF
temporally contingent model; it illustrates how out- YOUTHS’ STAGE-SPECIFIC COPING
comes in prior stages impact developmental challenges OUTCOMES AS A FUNCTION OF
that individuals encounter and resolve in current situa- NET VULNERABILITY
tions. Many researchers have examined the relationship
of systemic racism to outcomes; however, PVEST high- One major certainty to be concluded from the multidisci-
lights the mediating processes between race, as a risk plinary literatures integrated and analyses provided in
factor, and tangible outcomes. As suggested by Figure demonstrating the benefits of PVEST is that any inclu-
15.20, essentially, it allows us to examine “ how” differ- sive theory of human development should bear a particu-
ent individuals cope with racism throughout their life- lar burden. Specifically, not only should the framework
course development. acknowledge and incorporate the major objectively iden-
Future research on racism, along with social, politi- tifiable expressions of human variation (e.g., race, gen-
cal, and legal attempts to mitigate its impact, should der, and unique life-course placement—an infant versus
take into account both the subtle, normative expressions an elder) that may differentially interface with context,
of this phenomenon and the different levels at which it is but also provide an adequate explanation of the “ how” of
manifested. Individual level interventions are neces- human development processes that leads to the “ what ” or
sary, but transformation of institutions and their prac- particularly patterned outcomes (see Figure 15.20).
tices will also be vital in the long run. The theories The uniquely structured and experienced processes of
noted along with many others, afford varying insights human development are inextricably linked to the ten-
on the nature of racism and its broad impacts on human sions produced between nuanced developmental tasks
lives. Developmental scientists have the option to draw pursued by the individual given context character and in-
on these frameworks in the production of better science, fluences of the psychohistorical moment, along with ex-
which allows all youth, regardless of race, to achieve pectations for competence. However, unavoidable
their full potential. tensions are also produced as a function of children’s
In summary, it is apparent that the several themes characteristics such as group membership and context
pursued have important implications for the content of quality; although infrequently noted, the latter continues
the invariable linear or deterministic thinking that to be linked to structural conditions associated with
continues to serve as shorthand analyses of youths’ race, racism, and White privilege. As indicated in the
life-course coping efforts. Accordingly, it is obvious synthesis statement, this unchanging dilemma is over-
that context, cultural traditions and normative human looked in the child psychology literature except for a pri-
developmental processes matter and interact while hu- ori assumptions of deviance, psychopathology, or
mans transition across time and place. Utilizing problems associated with youth of color. Further, this
PVEST as an organizing framework, this final section situation, experienced as stigmatization, is linked to his-
provides helpful heuristic devices for inferring more torical conditions and includes their perpetuation as val-
sensitive and valid conclusions about expected out- ues, beliefs, attitudes, contextual inequalities, and
comes. The strategy should diminish the dissemina- psychosocial experiences. Considered together, they
tion of stigma and stereotypes in the conduct of serve neither to perpetuate risks nor to promulgate pro-
developmental science: Interpretive frameworks repre- tective factors as net stress is experienced, reactive cop-
senting models, which afford best inferences concern- ing responses evoked, and emergent identities enacted.
ing both dual aspects implicit in youths’ Net Thus, recursively structured, they become associated
Vulnerability (i.e., levels of risks and protective fac- with particular stage-specific outcomes as young lives
tors)—given the experience of net stress and coping unfold given particular cultural traditions. Accordingly,
processes—that may be anticipated for a particular our introduction of PVEST as an ICE perspective sug-
stage-specific coping outcomes (i.e., unproductive ver- gests that the unfolding coping processes and consequent
sus productive options). coping products experienced at one stage serves as the
886 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

Reorienting As suggested by the literatures reviewed and synthe-


Processes sized, high risk assessments without the consideration of
1
available protective factors have generally represented the
5
Vulnerability Level Life Stage Outcomes
analyses of youth of color. At the same time, unacknowl-
edged White privileging views about European Americans
Protective Risk are generally not associated with estimates of vulnerabil-
Factors Contributors Productive Unproductive ity (i.e., only protective factors such as social class are
considered). As suggested and more problematic, the high
Goal Seeking
(Tertiary Coping and performance often associated with middle-income Euro-
Orienting) Processes pean Americans is assumed to be the expected norm for
all youth; outliers are considered deviant or atypical. As
4
Emergent Identities: illustrated, two groups are usually compared when con-
Proximal Stable Coping Responses ducting research with diverse youth; middle-income
Processes Whites are invariably compared with a group or groups of
Positive Negative low-resource marginalized group youth. This lack of
equivalent sample comparisons, as described in an early
Identity Formation section of the paper, remains a significant problem in the
(Secondary Coping)
Processes developmental literature on child development. However,
as illustrated in Figure 15.21 and when compared against
2 3 Figure 15.22, there remain conceptual shortcomings and
Net Stress Reactive Coping Strategies
(Engagement) Level simplistic assumptions when dual analyses of vulnerabil-
ity are provided.
Social Challenges Adaptive Maladaptive As suggested, as a dual axis model of vulnerability,
Supports
too frequently Quadrant I is assumed to be poor and mi-
Primary
Coping
Processes
RISK FACTOR LEVEL
Figure 15.20 Processes Emphasizing Phenomenological Vari- High Low
ant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST). Source: From
Spencer & Harpalani, 2004. Low Special Needs
(Not Evident)
or Deviant:
major source of the individual’s net vulnerability at the Atypical
PROTECTIVE
next stage given the ongoing experiences of individual- FACTOR
LEVEL
context interface. Thus, as a dynamic recursive theoreti- (Presence/
Experience)
cal framework, PVEST moves beyond narrow determin-
High Performance:
istic perspectives of human development. High Considered
(Significant the “Norm”
Presence)
Conceptual Advantages of Phenomenological
Variant of Ecological Systems Theory as a Dual
Axis Coping Outcome Interpretational Figure 15.21 Traditional and limiting dual axis Perspective:
Framework of Human Development: This view narrowly compares extremes. Source: From “What
Traditional and Limited Dual Axis Perspectives Does ‘Acting White’ Actually Mean?: Racial Identity, Adoles-
cent Development, and Academic Achievement among African
As illustrated in Figure 15.21, in general, traditional American Youth,” by M. B. Spencer and V. Harpalani, in Mi-
and limited views of human development may consider nority Status, Collective Identity and Schooling, J. U. Ogbu
(Ed.), submitted, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; Adapted from “Intro-
risk level and protective factor intensity for understand-
duction: The Syndrome of the Psychologically Vulnerable
ing human vulnerability. However, the approach is still Child” (pp. 3–10), by E. J. Anthony, in The Child in His Family:
generally underdeveloped when considering the experi- Children at Psychiatric Risk, E. J. Anthony and C. Koupernik
ences of diverse youth of color. (Eds.), 1974, New York: Wiley.
Conceptual Advantages of Phenomenological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory 887

RISK FACTOR LEVEL comes in the face of risk. Inferred from his definition is
that one can only be resilient given the presence of high
High Low
risk and its net-balancing with protective factors. Thus,
Low Quadrant I as described in Figure 15.23, lacking a high level of pro-
Quadrant II
(Not Evident) Special Needs ––– tective factors, Quadrant I youth can be labeled highly
Deviant: Atypical
(Assumed to be poor
(Assumed non-existent vulnerable; however, Quadrant II youth are low on risk
PROTECTIVE or ignored)
FACTOR
and/or minority status) factor level although generally lacking high levels of
LEVEL protective factors; accordingly, Figure 15.23 describes
(Presence/
Experience) Quadrant IV them as lacking in significant symptomatology but show
Quadrant III High Performing: overall “masked vulnerability.”
––– Considered the “Norm”
High (Assumed to be As indicated, Quadrant IV youth are generally those
(Assumed
(Significant middle income [MI]
non-existent) who have high levels of protective factors and low or no
Presence) or non-minority status)
discernible risks; problematically, they are often used as
the standard or norm for all; however, as indicated, they
Figure 15.22 Shortcomings and assumptions of a tradi- are described as “ untested” or actually possessing “ un-
tional dual axis model. Source: Adapted from “Introduction: determined vulnerability” as depicted in Figure 15.23 as
The Syndrome of the Psychologically Vulnerable Child”
the PVEST-linked vulnerability level and resiliency pre-
(pp. 3–10), by E. J. Anthony, in The Child in His Family:
Children at Psychiatric Risk, E. J. Anthony and C. Koupernik diction dual axis coping outcome model. Considered
(Eds.), 1974, New York: Wiley. carefully from the theoretical perspective of James An-
thony, as a major contributor to the PVEST framework,
“resiliency” can only be expected for Quadrant III Group
nority, on the one hand, and Quadrant IV is assumed to because, as defined by Anthony, resiliency is associated
be youth of middle-income and nonminority (or model with those experiencing significant levels of risk but con-
minority) status; on the other hand. Quadrant II is gen- comitantly have access to high levels of protective fac-
erally ignored in the literature except for recent efforts tors. As indicated, we believe that this dual axis model
by Luthar (2003) with extremely affluent suburban Eu- provides a clarifying heuristic device for understanding
ropean Americans. Alternatively, when considering levels of vulnerability and resiliency prediction estimates
Quadrant III, the major publication patterns in child without engaging in unfair and stigmatizing analyses.
psychology have overwhelmingly ignored this high risk
and high protective factor level group or assumed that it
does not exist. There are a few theorists that explore RISK FACTOR LEVEL
protective factors such as parental monitoring, cultural High Low
socialization, specific achievement enhancing program-
ming, and reference group identity as protective factors Low Special Needs Symptoms Not Evident/
(e.g., Spencer, 1983; Spencer, Fegley, & Harpalani, (Not Evident) Evident: Overlooked:
Quadrant I Quadrant II
2003; Spencer, Noll, et al., 2001; Swanson et al., 2002;
PROTECTIVE (Highly Vulnerable) (“Masked” Vulnerability)
Youngblood & Spencer, 2002). When considered to- FACTOR
gether, it appears that as a specific outcome oriented LEVEL
(Presence/
perspective of the PVEST framework, Figure 15.23 pro- Experience)
Untested:
vides important implications for interpreting resiliency Resilience Expected:
Quadrant IV
High Quadrant III
particularly as conceptualized by Anthony (1987). (Low Vulnerability)
(Undetermined
(Significant
Vulnerability)
Presence)
Predicting Positive Outcomes in the
Face of Challenge
Figure 15.23 PVEST-linked vulnerability level and re-
siliency predicting dual axis coping outcome model. Source:
One of the many strengths of James Anthony’s (1974)
Adapted from “Introduction: The Syndrome of the Psycho-
formulation of resiliency is its acknowledgment of the logically Vulnerable Child” (pp. 3–10), by E. J. Anthony, in
simultaneous interfacing of protective factors with risk. The Child in His Family: Children at Psychiatric Risk, E. J.
He defines resiliency as the attainment of positive out- Anthony and C. Koupernik (Eds.), 1974, New York: Wiley.
888 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

Unfortunately, this is not the orientation of most theories Barker, R. G., & Wright, H. F. (1954). Midwest and its children: The
of human development that consider the experiences of psychological ecology of an American town. Evanston, IL: Row
and Peterson.
diverse youth. Importantly, PVEST is not only applicable
Baron, H. (1971). The demand for Black labor: Historical notes on
to identifiable diverse young people and sensitive to their the political economy of racism. Radical American, 5(2), 1–46.
unique cultural and contextual niches but also serves as a Bell, D. A. (1992). Faces at the bottom of the well: The permanence of
broad life-course theory of human development, which racism. New York: Basic Books.
analyzes the systemic relationships between vulnerabil- Bell, D. A. (1995). Brown v. Board of Education and the interest con-
ity, stress level, coping processes (i.e., reactive, stable vergence dilemma. In K. Crenshaw, N. Gotanda, G. Peller, & K.
Thomas (Eds.), Critical race theory: The key writings that formed
coping as emergent identities), and stage-specific coping
the movement (pp. 20–28). New York: New Press.
outcomes as lives unfolds across the life course irrespec- Bell, D. A. (2000a). After we’ve gone: Prudent speculations on
tive of group membership (including privilege) and its so- America in a post-racial epoch. In R. Delgado & J. Stefancic
cially structured social standing. (Eds.), Critical race theory: The cutting edge (pp. 2–8). Philadel-
In sum, considered together, PVEST and its dual axis phia: Temple University Press.
Bell, D. A. (2000b). Serving two masters: Integration ideals and
coping outcome model of human development provide
client interests in school desegregation litigation. In R. Delgado
conceptual tools and heuristic devices for accounting for & J. Stefancic (Eds.), Critical race theory: The cutting edge
the diverse expressions of human development in cul- (pp. 236–246). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
tural context. Board of Education of Oklahoma City v. Dowell, 49.8 U.S. 237
(1991).
Bolling v. Sharpe, 347 U.S. 497 (1954).
Bonilla-Silva, E. (2001). White supremacy and racism in the post-civil
rights era. Boulder, CO: Lynne Rienner Publishers.
REFERENCES Boyd-Franklin, N. (1989). Black families in therapy: A multisystems
approach. New York: Guilford Press.
Allen, W. R. (1985). Race, income and family dynamics: A study of Boykin, A. W. (1986). The triple quandary and the schooling of Afro-
adolescent male socialization processes and outcomes. In M. B. American children. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The school achievement of
Spencer, G. K. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.), Beginnings: The minority children (pp. 57–92). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
social and af fective development of Black children (pp. 273–292).
Boykin, A. W., & Ellison, C. M. (1995). The multiple ecologies of
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Black youth socialization: An afrographic analysis. In R. L. Tay-
Allen, W. R., Spencer, M. B., & Brookins, G. K. (1985). Synthesis: lor (Ed.), African-American youth: Perspectives on their status in
Black children keep on growing. In M. B. Spencer, G. K. the United States (pp. 93–128). Westport, CT: Praeger.
Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.), Beginnings: The social and af-
fective development of Black children (pp. 301–314). Hillsdale, Boykin, A. W., & Toms, F. (1985). Black child socialization: A
NJ: Erlbaum. conceptual framework. In H. P. McAdoo & J. McAdoo (Eds.),
Black children: Social, educational, and parental environments
Anderson, J. D. (1988). The education of Blacks in the south,
(pp. 33–52). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
1860–1935. Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of
Anthony, E. J. (1974). Introduction: The syndrome of the psychologi-
human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531.
cally vulnerable child. In E. J. Anthony & C. Koupernik (Eds.),
The child in his family: Children at psychiatric risk (pp. 3–10). Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Ex-
New York: Wiley. periments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
Anthony, E. J. (1987). Risk, vulnerability, and resilience: An versity Press.
overview. In E. J. Anthony & B. J. Cohler (Eds.), The invulnerable Bronfenbrenner, U. (1985). Summary. In M. B. Spencer, G. K.
child (pp. 3–48). New York: Guilford Press. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.), Beginnings: Social and af fective
Armour, J. D. (2000). Race Ipsa Loquitur: Of reasonable racists, in- development of Black children (pp. 67–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
telligent bayesians, and involuntary negrophobes. In R. Delgardo Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). Ecological systems theory. In R. I. Vasta
& J. Stefancic (Eds.), Critical race theory (chap. 17). Philadel- (Ed.), Annals of Child Development: Vol. 6. Six theories of child
phia: Temple University Press. development (pp. 187–249). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Arrington, E. G. (2002). Negotiating race and racism: Black youth in Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). The process-person-context model in de-
racially dissonant schools. Dissertation Abstracts International, velopment research: Principles, applications and implications. Un-
62, 12. (UMI No. AA13031636) published manuscript, Cornell University, Department of Human
Balkin, J. M. (2001). What Brown versus Board of Education should Development and Family Studies, Ithaca, NY.
have said. New York: New York University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1993). The ecology of cognitive development. In
Banks, W. C. (1976). White preference in Blacks: A paradigm in R. H. Wozniak & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Development in context:
search of phenomenon. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 1179–1186. Acting and thinking is specific environments (pp. 3–44). Hillsdale,
Barker, R. G., & Wright, H. F. (1949). Psychological ecology and NJ: Erlbaum.
the problem of psychosocial development. Child Development, Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1993). Heredity, environment, and
20, 131–143. the question “How?”: A first approximation. In R. Plomin & G. E.
References 889

McClearn (Eds.), Nature, nurture, and psychology (pp. 313–324). Cross, W. E., Jr. (2001). Encountering nigrescence. In J. Ponterotto,
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. J. M. Casas, L. A. Suzuki, & C. M. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptu- multicultural counseling (pp. 30–44). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
alized in developmental perspective: A bioecological mode. Psy- Cross, W. E., Jr., & Vandiver, B. J. (2001). Nigrescence theory and
chological Review, 101(4), 568–586. measurement. In J. G. Ponterotto, et al. (Eds.), Handbook of mul-
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1983). The evolution of envi- ticultural counseling (pp. 371–393). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
ronmental models in developmental research. In P. H. Mussen Cunningham, M. (1993). Sex role inf luences of African American
(Series Ed.) & W. Kessen (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychol- adolescent males: A literature review. Journal of African Ameri-
ogy: Vol. I. History, theory, and methods (pp. 357–414). New can Male Studies, 1, 30–37.
York: Wiley. Cunningham, M., Swanson, D. P., Spencer, M. B., & Dupree, D.
Brookins, G. K. (1985). Black children’s sex role ideologies and oc- (2003). The association of physical maturation with family has-
cupational choices in families of employed mothers. In M. B. sles in African American adolescent males. Journal of Cultural
Spencer, G. K. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.), Beginnings: Social Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 9, 274–276.
and af fective development of Black children (pp. 257–272). Hills- Damon, W. (2002, February). The development of purpose during
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. adolescence. Paper presented at the Fuller Thrive Author Confer-
Brookins, G. K. (1988). Making the honor roll: A Black parent’s per- ence for beyond the Self: Perspectives on Transcendence and
Identity Development, Pasadena, CA.
spective on private education. In D. T. Slaughter & D. J. Johnson
(Eds.), Visible now: Blacks in private school (pp. 12–20). New Darity, W. A., Jr., & Myers, S. L., Jr. (1998). Persistent disparity:
York: Greenwood Press. Race and economic inequality in the United States since 1945.
Northampton, MA: E. Elgar.
Brown I. 347 U.S. 483 (1954).
Datnow, A., & Cooper, R. (1996). Peer networks of African-American
Brown II. 349 U.S. 294 (1955).
students in independent schools: Affirming academic success and
Bullock, H. A. (1967). A history of Negro education in the south: racial identity. Journal of Negro Education, 65(4), 56–72.
From 1619–present. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
DeCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal af fective de-
Butchart, R. E. (1980). Northern schools, southern Blacks, and re- terminant of behavior. New York: Academy Press.
construction: Freedmen’s education, 1862–1875. Westport, CT: Delgado, R., & Stefancic, J. (2000). Introduction. In R. Delgado &
Greenwood Press. J. Stefancic (Eds.), Critical race theory: The cutting edge
Carroll, G. (1998). Mundane extreme environmental stress and (pp. xv–xix). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
African American families: A case for recognizing different real- Delgado, R., & Stefancic, J. (2001). Critical race theory: An intro-
ities. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 29(2), 271–283. duction. New York: New York University Press.
Carroll, G., & Tyler, K. (under review). A portrait of African Ameri- Du Bois, W. E. B. (1903). The souls of Black folk. Greenwich, CT:
can suicide: An interaction of racism and stress. Fawcett Publications.
Ceci, S. J. (1994). Cognitive and social factors in children’s testi- Du Bois, W. E. B. (1935). Black reconstruction. New York: S. A. Russell.
mony. In B. D. Sales & G. R. VandenBos (Eds.), Psychology in lit- Dupree, D., Spencer, M. B., & Bell, S. (1997). The ecology of
igation and legislation (pp. 11–54). Washington, DC: American African American child development: Normative and non-norma-
Psychological Association. tive outcomes. In G. Johnson-Powell & Y. Yamamoto (Eds.),
Chestang, L. W. (1971). The dilemma of biracial adoptions. Social Transcultural child psychiatry: A portrait of America’s children
Work, 17, 100–105. (pp. 237–268). New York: Wiley.
Chestang, L. W. (1972). Character development in a hostile environ- Dweck, C. S. (1978). Achievement. In M. Lamb (Ed.), Social and per-
ment (Occasional Paper, No. 3). Chicago: University of Chicago, sonality development (pp. 114–130). New York: Holt Reinhart.
School of Social Service Administration. Elder, G. H., Jr. (1974). Children of the great depression. Chicago:
Clark, K. B., & Clark, M. P. (1939). The development of conscious- University of Chicago Press.
ness of self and the emergence of racial identity in Negro pre- Eng, P. (1999). Warrior lessons. New York: Pocket Books.
school children. Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 591–599. Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Is-
Clark, K. B., & Clark, M. P. (1940). Skin color as a factor in racial sues, 1, 1–171.
identification of Negro preschool children. Journal of Social Psy- Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.
chology, 2, 159–169.
Essed, P. (2002). Everyday racism: A new approach to the study of
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. racism. In P. Essed & D. T. Goldberg (Eds.), Race critical theo-
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ries: Test and context (pp. 176–194). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Crenshaw, K., Gotanda, N., Peller, G., & Thomas, K. (1995). Criti- Fegley, S. G., Spencer, M. B., Gross, T. N., Harpalani, V., & Charles,
cal race theory: Key writings that formed the movement. New N. (in press). Bodily self-awareness: Skin color and psychosocial
York: New Press. well-being in adolescence. In W. Overton & U. Mueller (Eds.),
Cross, W. E., Jr. (1971, July). The Negro-to-Black conversion experi- Body in mind, mind in body: Developmental perspectives on em-
ence. Black World, 13–27. bodiment and consciousness. Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
Cross, W. E., Jr. (1991). Shades of Black: Diversity in African-Ameri- Fischbein, S. (1980). IQ and social class. Intelligence, 4, 51–63.
can identity. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Fisher, C. B., Hoagwood, K., Boyce, C., Duster, T., Frank, D., Grisso,
Cross, W. E., Jr. (1995). The psychology of nigrescence. In J. Pon- T., et al. (2002). Research ethics for mental health science involv-
terotto, J. Casas, L. Suzuki, & C. Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of ing ethnic minority children and youths. American Psychologist,
counseling (pp. 93–122). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 57(12), 1024–1040.
890 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

Flavell, J. H. (1968). The development of role-taking and communica- Hetherington, E. M., & Kelly, J. (2003). For better or for worse: Di-
tion skills in children. New York: Wiley. vorce reconsidered. New York: Free Press.
Ford, D. Y., Harris, J. J., Webb, K. S., & Jones, D. L. (1994). Rejec- Holliday, B. G. (1985a). Developmental imperatives of social ecolo-
tion or confirmation of racial identity: A dilemma for high- gies: Lessons learned from Black children. In H. P. McAdoo &
achieving Blacks? Journal of Educational Thought, 28(1), 7–33. J. L. McAdoo (Eds.), Black children: Social, educational and
Fordham, S. (1988). Racelessness as a strategy in Black students’ parental environments. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
school success: Pragmatic strategy or pyrrhic victory? Harvard Holliday, B. G. (1985b). Towards a model of teacher-child transac-
Educational Review, 58(1), 54–84. tional processes affecting Black children’s academic achieve-
ment. In M. B. Spencer, G. K. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.),
Fordham, S. (1996). Blacked out: Dilemmas of race, identity and suc-
Beginnings: The social and af fective development of Black children
cess at Capital High. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
(pp. 117–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. U. (1986). Black students’ school success:
Huang, L. N. (1989). Southeast Asian refugee children and adoles-
Coping with the “ burden of ‘acting White.’ ” Urban Review,
cents. In J. T. Gibbs & L. N. Huang (Eds.). Children of color
18, 176–206.
(pp. 278–321). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Freeman, A. D. (1995). Legitimizing racial discrimination through
Ignatiev, N. (1995). How the Irish became White. New York: Rout-
antidiscrimination law: A critical review of Supreme Court doc- ledge & Kegan Paul.
trine. In. K. Crenshaw, N. Gotanda, G. Peller, & K. Thomas
Irvine, J. J. (1988). Black students and school failure. New York:
(Eds.), Critical race theory: The key writings that formed the
Greenwood Press.
movement (pp. 29–45). New York: New Press.
Jackson, P. W., Boostrom, R. E., & Hansen D. T. (1993). The moral
Freeman v. Pitts, 503 U.S. 467 (1992).
life of schools. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Frye, D. (1992). Causes and precursors of children’s theories of
Jaret, C. (1995). Contemporary racial and ethnic relations. New York:
mind. In D. Hay & A. Angold (Eds.), Precursors and causes in de-
HarperCollins.
velopment and psychopathology (pp. 145–168). West Sussex, En-
gland: Wiley. Jarrett, R. L. (1994). Living poor: Family life among single parent,
African American women. Social Problems, 41, 30–49.
Frye, D., & Moore, C. (1991). Children’s theories of mind: Mental
states and social understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jarrett, R. L. (1995). Growing up poor: The family experiences of so-
cially mobile youth in low-income African-American neighbor-
Goldberg, D. T. (2001). Race critical theories (P. Essed & D. T. Gold-
hoods. Journal of Adolescent Research, 10, 111–135.
bert, Eds.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Joe, S., & Kaplan, M. S. (2002, March). Firearm-related suicide
Goldberg, D. T. (2002). Modernity, race, and morality. In P. Essed & among young African American males. Psychiatric Services,
D. T. Goldberg (Eds.), Race critical theories: Text and context 53(3), 332–334.
(pp. 14–40). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Johnson, D. J. (1988). Racial socialization strategies of parents in
Gottlieb, G. (1995). Some conceptual deficiencies in “developmen- three Black private schools. In D. T. Slaughter & D. J. Johnson
tal” behavior genetics. Human Development, 38, 131–141. (Eds.), Visible now: Blacks in private schools (pp. 251–267). New
Gump, P. V., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1955). The “It ” in children’s York: Greenwood Press.
games. Group, 17, 3–8. Johnson-Powell, G., & Yamamoto, Y. (Eds.). (1997). Transcultural
Guthrie, R. (1976). Even the rat was White: A historical view of psy- child psychiatry: A portrait of American’s children. New York:
chology. New York: Harper & Row. Wiley.
Hall, S. (2000). In R. Delgardo, & J. Stefancic (Eds.), Critical race Kardiner, A., & Ovesey, L. (1951). The mark of oppression: Explo-
theory: An introduction. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. rations in the personality of the American Negro. New York: Norton.
Hall, S. (2002). Race, articulation, and societies structured in domi- Kennell, J., & Klaus, M. (1982). Parent-infant bonding (2nd ed.). St.
nance. In P. Essed & D.T. Goldberg (Eds.), Race critical theories: Louis: Mosby.
Text and context (pp. 38– 68). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Kennell, J. H., Trause, M. A., & Klaus, M. H. (1975). Evidence for a
Hare, B. R., & Castenell, L. A., Jr. (1985). No place to run, no place sensitive period in the human mother. In Ciba Foundation Sympo-
to hide: Comparative status and future prospects of Black boys. sium: Vol. 33. Parent-infant interaction (New series, pp. 87–102).
In M. B. Spencer, G. K Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.), Begin- Klaus, M. H., & Kennel, J. H. (1976). Maternal-infant bonding. St.
nings: The social and af fective development of Black children Louis, MO: Mosby.
(pp. 201–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Klaus, M. H., & Kennel, J. H. (1976). Maternal-infant bonding: The
Harpalani, V. (1999). A critique of Hoberman’s Darwin’s athletes: In- impact of early separation or loss on family development. St.
valid psychological inferences from a sociohistorical framework. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Pennsylvania, Philadel- Klaus, M., & Kennel, J. (1983). Parent-infant bonding. St.
phia, Graduate School of Education. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Harpalani, V., & Spencer, M. B. (2005). Racism. In C. B. Fisher & Klaus, P. H., Kennel, J. H., & Klaus, M. H. (1995). Bonding: Building
R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Applied developmental science: An encyclope- the foundations of secure attachment and independence. Reading,
dia of research, policies, and programs (pp. 905–909). Thousand MA: Addison-Wesley.
Oaks: Sage. Koshy, S. (1998). Category crisis: South Asian Americans and ques-
Havighurst, R. J. (1953). Human development and education. New tions of race and ethnicity. Diaspora, 7(3), 285–320.
York: McKay. Kottak, C. P. (1987). Cultural anthropology. New York: Random House.
Hergenhahn, B. R., & Olson, M. H. (1993). An introduction to theo- Ladner, J. A. (1972). Tomorrow’s tomorrow: The Black woman. New
ries of learning. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. York: Doubleday.
References 891

Ladson-Billings, G. (1994). The dream keepers: Successful teachers of Nanda, S. (1974). Cultural anthropology. New York: VanNostrand.
African-American children. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Nguyen, L., & Frye, D. (1999). Children’s theory of mind: Under-
LaFromboise, T. D., Coleman, H. L., & Gerton, J. (1993). Psycholog- standing desire, belief, and emotion with social referents. Social
ical impact of biculturalism: Evidence and theory. Psychological Development, 8, 70–92.
Bulletin, 114, 395–412. Obidah, J. E. (2001). In search of a theoretical framework. In R. O.
Lal, S. (2002). Giving children security: Mamie Phipps Clark and Mabokela & A. L. Green (Eds.), Sisters of the academy: Emergent
the racializing of child psychology. American Psychologist, Black women scholars in higher education (pp. 43–54). Sterling,
57(1), 20–28. VA: Stylus Publishing.
Lee, C., Spencer, M. B., & Harpalani, V. (2003). Every shut eye Ogbu, J. (1985). A cultural ecology of competence among inner-city
ain’t sleep: Studying how people live culturally. Educational Blacks. In M. B. Spencer, G. K. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.),
Researcher, 32, 6–13.
Beginnings: The social and af fective development of Black children
Lesser, G., & Stodolsky, S. (1967). Learning patterns in the disad- (pp. 45–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
vantaged. Harvard Educational Review, 37, 546–593.
Ogbu, J. U. (1986). The consequences of the American caste system.
Lesser, G., & Stodolsky, S. (1970). Equal opportunity for maximum In U. Neisser (Ed.), The school achievement of minority children:
development. In J. S. Coleman (Ed.), Equal educational opportu- New perspectives (pp. 19–56). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.
nity: Harvard Educational Review (pp. 127–138). Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press. Ogbu, J. U. (1987). Variability in minority school performance: A
problem in search of an explanation. Anthropology and Education
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York:
Quarterly, 18, 312–334.
McGraw-Hill.
Liebow, E. (1968). Tally’s corner: A study of Negro streetcorner men. Ogbu, J. U. (1990). Minority education in a comparative perspective.
Boston: Little, Brown. Journal of Negro Education, 59(1), 45–56.
Liu, W. T., Yu, E., Chang, C. F., & Fernandez, M. (1990). The mental Omi, M., & Winant, H. (1994). Racial formation in the United States
health of Asian American teenagers: A research challenge. In (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.
A. R. Stiffman & L. E. Davis (Eds.), Ethnic issues in adolescent Omi, M., & Winant, H. (2002). Racial formation. In P. Essed & D. T.
mental health (pp. 92–112). Newbury Park: Sage. Goldberg (Eds.), Race critical theories: Text and context (pp.
Luthar, S. S. (2003). Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the 123–145). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
context of childhood adversities. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer- Oyemade, U. J. (1985). The rationale for Head Start as a vehicle for
sity Press. the upward mobility of minority families: A minority perspective.
Luthar, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: Im- American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55(4) 591–602.
plications for interventions and social policies. Development and Pettigrew, T. F. (1964). A profile of the Negro American. Princeton,
Psychopathology, 12, 857–885. NJ: Van Nostrand.
MacLeod, J. (1987). Ain’t no makin’ it: Leveled aspirations in a low- Phelan, P., Davidson, A. L., & Cao, H. T. (1991). Students’ multiple
income neighborhood. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. worlds: Negotiating the boundaries of family, peer, and school
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Cultural variation in the cultures. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 22, 224–250.
self-concept. In G. R. Goethals & J. Strauss (Eds.), Multidisci- Piaget, J. (1926). The language and thought of the child. New York:
plinary perspectives on the self (pp. 18–48). New York: Springer- Harcourt, Brace and World.
Verlag.
Piaget, J. (1967). Six psychological studies. New York: Vintage.
Matute-Bianchi, M. E. (1986). Ethnic identity and patterns of school
success and failures among Mexican descendants and Japanese- Plessy v. Ferguson, 163 U.S. 537 (1896), pp. 550–551.
American students in a California high school: An ethnographic Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Loehlin, J. C. (1977). Genotype-
analysis. American Journal of Education, 95, 233–255. environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human
McArdle, C. G., & Young, N. F. (1970). Classroom discussion of behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 309–322.
racial identity or how can we make it without “acting White”? Prashad, V. (2000). The karma of Brown folk. Minneapolis, MN: Uni-
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 40(1), 135–141. versity of Minnesota Press.
McIntosh, P. (1989). White privilege: Unpacking the invisible knap- Riksen-Walraven, J. M. A. (1978). Effects of caregive behavior on
sack. Peace and Freedom, 10–12. habituation rate and self-efficacy in infants. International Journal
McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on Black of Behavioral Development, 1, 105–130.
families and children: Psychology distress, parenting, and socioe- Rimer, S., & Arenson, K. W. (2004, June). Top colleges take more
motional development. Child Development, 61, 311–346. blacks, but which ones? Retrieved December 6, 2005 from the
Mehta, P. (1998). The emergence, conf licts, and integration of the bi- New York Times website: http://select.nytimes.com /gst /abstract
cultural self. In S. Akhtar & S. Kramer (Eds.), The colors of .html?res=F00917FC355D0C778EDDAF0894DC404482
childhood (pp. 129–168). Northvale, NJ: Aronson. Roediger, D. R. (1991). The wages of whiteness: Race and the making
Milliken v. Bradley, 418 U.S. 717 (1974). of the American working class. New York: Verso.
Missouri v. Jenkins, 515 U.S. 70 (1995). Roediger, D. R. (1994). The abolition of whiteness. New York:
Moore, M. (2002). Stupid white men . . . and other sorry excuses for Verso.
the state of the nation! New York: Regan Books. Roediger, D. R. (2001). Whiteness and ethnicity in the history
Moore, M. (Director). (2002). Bowling for Columbine [Motion pic- of “ White Ethnics” in the United States. In P. Essed & D. T.
ture]. U.S. Columbia Tristar Home Video. Available from Goldberg (Eds.), Race critical theories: Text and context
http://www.bowlingforcolumbine.com. (pp. 325–343). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
892 Phenomenology and Ecological Systems Theory: Development of Diverse Groups

Sarat, A. (1997). Race, law and culture: Ref lections on Brown v. Spencer, M. B. (1999a). Social and cultural inf luences on school ad-
Board of Education. New York: Oxford University Press. justment: The application of an identity-focused cultural ecologi-
Scarr, S. (1988). How genotypes and environments combine: Devel- cal perspective. Educational Psychologist, 34, 43–57.
opment and individual differences. In N. Bolger & A. Caspi Spencer, M. B. (1999b). Transitions and continuities in cultural val-
(Eds.), Persons in context: Developmental processes—Human de- ues: Kenneth Clark revisited. In R. L. Jones (Ed.), African Ameri-
velopment in cultural and historical contexts (pp. 217–244). New can children, youth, and parenting (pp. 183–208). Hampton, VA:
York: Cambridge University Press. Cobb & Henry.
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own envi- Spencer, M. B. (2001). Identity, achievement orientation and race:
ronments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Devel- “Lessons learned” about the normative developmental experiences
opment, 54, 424–435. of African American males. In W. Watkins, et al. (Eds.), Race and
education: The role of history and society in educating African
Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). Race, social class, and IQ. Science,
American students (pp. 100–127). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn
174, 1285–1295. & Bacon.
Schultz, D. (1976). Theories of personality. Monterey, CA: Spencer, M. B. (2005). Crafting identities and accessing opportunity
Brooks/Cole. post Brown. American Psychologist, 60(8), 821–830.
Sethi, S., Lepper, M. R., & Ross, L. (1999). Independence from Spencer, M. B., Brookins, G. K., & Allen, W. R. (Eds.). (1985). Be-
whom? Interdependence with whom? Cultural perspectives on in- ginnings: Vol. 1. The social and af fective development of Black
groups versus outgroups. In D. Miller & D. Prentice (Eds.), Cul- children. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tural divides: Understanding and overcoming group conf licts Spencer, M. B., & Cassidy, E. (2004). A broader view of service
(pp. 273–301). New York: Sage. learning: Using a phenomenological approach to frame service
Slaughter, D. T., & Johnson, D. (1988). Visible now: Blacks in private learning among high-school-aged youth. Chapter prepared for the
schools. New York: Greenwood Press. Robert Wood Johnson Foundation.
Spencer, M. B. (1970). The ef fects of systematic social (puppet) and Spencer, M. B., Cross, Jr., W. E., Harpalani, V., & Goss, T. N.
token reinforcement on the modification of racial and color con- (2003). Debunking the “acting white” myth and posing new di-
cept-attitudes in preschool aged children. Unpublished master’s rections for research. In C. C. Yeakey & R. D. Henderson (Eds.),
thesis, University of Kansas, Lawrence. Surmounting all odds: Education, opportunity, and society in the
new millennium (pp. 273–304). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
Spencer, M. B. (1976, August). The competence model as a viable al-
ternative to IQ test gamesmanship. Proceedings of the Association Spencer, M. B., & Dornbusch, S. (1990). Challenges in studying mi-
of Black Psychologists, Chicago. nority youth. In S. Feldman & G. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold:
The developing adolescent (pp. 123–146). Cambridge, MA: Har-
Spencer, M. B. (1981a). Personal and group identity of Black chil-
vard University Press.
dren: An alternative synthesis. Genetic Psychology Monographs,
Spencer, M. B., Dupree, D., Cunningham, M., Harpalani, V., &
106, 59–84.
Munoz-Miller, M. (2003). Vulnerability to violence: A
Spencer, M. B. (1981b). Preschool children’s social cognition and contextually-sensitive, developmental perspective on African
cultural cognition: A cognitive developmental interpretation of American adolescents. Journal of Social Issues, 59(1) 33–49.
race dissonance findings. Journal of Psychology, 112, 275–286.
Spencer, M. B., Dupree, D., & Hartmann, T. (1997). A Phenome-
Spencer, M. B. (1982). Preschool children’s social cognition and cul- nological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST): A
tural cognition: A cognitive developmental interpretation of race self-organization perspective in context. Development and
dissonance findings. Journal of Psychology, 112, 275–286. Psychopathology, 9, 817–833.
Spencer, M. B. (1983). Children’s cultural values and parental child Spencer, M. B., Dupree, D., Swanson, D. P., & Cunningham, M.
rearing strategies. Developmental Review, 3, 351–370. (1996). Parental monitoring and adolescents’ sense of responsi-
Spencer, M. B. (1984). Black children’s race awareness, racial atti- bility for their own learning: An examination of sex differences.
tudes and self-concept: A reinterpretation. Journal of Child Psy- Journal of Negro Education, 65(1), 30–43.
chology and Psychiatry, 25(3), 433–441. Spencer, M. B., Dupree, D., Swanson, D. P., & Cunningham, M.
Spencer, M. B. (1985). Cultural cognition and social cognition as (1998). The inf luence of physical maturation and hassles on
identity factors in Black children’s personal-social growth. In African American adolescents’ learning behaviors. Journal of
M. B. Spencer, G. K. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.), Begin- Comparative Family Studies, 29, 189–200.
nings: The social and af fective development of Black children Spencer, M. B., Fegley, S., & Harpalani, V. (2003). A theoretical and
(pp. 215–230). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. empirical examination of identity as coping: Linking coping re-
Spencer, M. B. (1988). Self-concept development. In D. T. Slaughter sources to the self processes of African American youth. Journal
(Ed.), Perspectives on Black child development: New directions for of Applied Developmental Science, 7(3), 181–187.
child development (pp. 59–72). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Spencer, M. B., & Harpalani, V. (2004). Nature, nurture, & the
Spencer, M. B. (1990). Parental values transmission: Implications for question of “How?”: A phenomenological variant of ecological
the development of African-American children. In J. B. Stewart & systems theory. In C. Garcia-Coll, K. Kearer, & R. Lerner
H. Cheatham (Eds.), Interdisciplinary perspectives on Black fami- (Eds.), Nature and nurture: The complex interplay of genetic and
lies (pp. 111–130). Atlanta, GA: Transactions. environmental inf luences on human behavior and development
Spencer, M. B. (1995). Old issues and new theorizing about African (pp. 53–77). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
American youth: A phenomenological variant of ecological systems Spencer, M. B., & Harpalani, V. (under review). What does “acting
theory. In R. L. Taylor (Ed.), Black youth: Perspectives on their sta- White” actually mean? Racial identity, adolescent development,
tus in the United States (pp. 37–70). Westport, CT: Praeger. and academic achievement among African American youth. In
References 893

J. U. Ogbu (Ed.), Minority status, collective identity and school-


cents. In H. Fitzgerald, B. M. Lester, & B. Zuckerman (Eds.),
ing. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Children of color: Research, health and public policy issues
Spencer, M. B., Harpalani, V., Cassidy, E., Jacobs, C., Donde, S., (pp. 53–72). New York: Garland.
Goss, T., et al. (2006). Understanding vulnerability and
Swanson, D. P., Spencer, M. B., Dell’Angleo, T., Harpalani, V.,
resilience from a normative development perspective:Vol. 1. Im-
& Spencer, T. (2002). Identity processes and the positive
plications for racially and ethnically diverse youth (pp. 627–672).
youth development of African Americans: An explanatory
In D. Cicchetti & E. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Development and
framework. In G. Noam (Series Ed.) & C. S. Taylor, R. M.
Psychopathology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Lerner, & A. von Eye (Eds.), Pathways to positive youth devel-
Spencer, M. B., Hartmann, T. T., & Dupree, D. (1997). A Phenome- opment among gang and non-gang youth: New directions
nological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST): A for youth development (No. 95, pp. 73–99). San Francisco:
self-organization perspective in context. Development and Jossey-Bass.
Psychopathology, 9, 817–833.
Swanson, D. P., Spencer, M. B., Harpalani, V., Noll, L., Seaton, G., &
Spencer, M. B., & Horowitz, F. D. (1973). Effects of systematic so- Ginzberg, S. (2003). Psychosocial development in racially and
cial and token reinforcement on the modification of racial and ethnically diverse groups: Conceptual and methodological
color concept attitudes in Black and in White pre-school children. changes in the 21st century. Development and Psychopathology,
Developmental Psychology, 9, 246–254. 15, 743–771.
Spencer, M. B., & Jones-Walker, C. (2004). Interventions and ser- Swanson, D. P., Spencer, M. B., & Petersen, A. (1998). Identity for-
vices offered to former juvenile offenders reentering their com- mation in adolescence. In K. Borman & B. Schneider (Eds.), The
munities: An analysis of program effectiveness. Journal of Youth adolescent years: Pt. 1. Social inf luences and educational chal-
Violence and Juvenile Justice, 1(2), 88–97. lenges, ninety-seventh Yearbook of the National Society for the
Spencer, M. B., & Markstrom-Adams, C. (1990). Identity processes Study of Education (pp. 18–41). Chicago: University of Chicago
among racial and ethnic minority children in America. Child De- Press.
velopment, 61, 290–310. Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cul-
Spencer, M. B., Noll, E., Stoltzfus, J., & Harpalani, V. (2001). Iden- tural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–520.
tity and school adjustment: Revisiting the “acting White” assump- Trueba, H. T. (1988). Culturally based explanations of minority stu-
tion. Educational Psychologist, 36(1), 21–30. dents’ academic achievement. Anthropology and Education Quar-
Spencer, M. B., Silver, L. J., Seaton, G., Tucker, S. R., Cunningham, terly, 19(3), 270–287.
M., & Harpalani, V. (2001). Race and gender inf luences on teen West, C. (2002). A genealogy of modern racism. In P. Essed & D. T.
parenting: An identity-focused cultural-ecological perspective. In Goldberg (Eds.), Race critical theories: Text and context. Malden,
T. Urdan & F. Pajares (Eds.), Adolescence and education: General MA: Blackwell.
issues in the education of adolescents (pp. 231–268). Greenwich,
White, R. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of compe-
CT: Information Age Publishing.
tence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.
Spencer, M. B., & Swanson, D. P. (2000). Promoting positive out-
White, R. (1960). Competence and psychosexual development. In
comes for youth: Resourceful families and communities. In
M. R. Riley (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 3–32).
S. Danziger & J. Waldfogel (Eds.), Securing the future
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
(pp. 182–204). New York: Russell Sage Foundation Press.
White, S. H. (1996). Foreword. In M. Cole (Ed.), Cultural psychol-
Steele, C. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual
ogy: A once and future discipline (p. ix). Cambridge, MA:
identity and performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
Belknap-Harvard Press.
Steele, C. (2004, April). Endowed lectureship on stereotype threat.
Wohlwill, J. F. (1985). The conf luence of environmental and devel-
Sponsored by the Provost Office, University of Michigan.
opmental psychology: Signpost to an ecology of development?
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intel- Human Development, 23, 354–358.
lectual test performance of African-Americans. Journal of Per-
Wohlwill, J. F., & Heft, H. (1987). The physical environment and the
sonality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811.
development of the child. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Hand-
Steinberg, L. (1985). Adolescence. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. book of environmental psychology (Vol.1, pp. 281–328). New
Stevenson, H. C. (1997). Missed, dissed, and pissed: Making York: Wiley.
meaning of neighborhood risk, fear and anger management in Woodson, C. G. (1919). The education of the Negro prior to 1861: A
urban Black youth. Cultural Diversity and Mental Health, 3, history of the education of the colored people of the United States
37–52. from the beginning of slavery to the Civil War. Washington, DC:
Stoler, A. L. (2002). Racial histories and their regimes of truth. In P. Associated.
Essed & D. T. Goldberg (Eds.), Race critical theories: Text and Wright, H. F. (1967). Recording and analyzing child behavior. New
context. Malden, MA: Blackwell. York: Harper & Row.
Sullivan, M. (1989). Getting paid: Youth, crime and work in the inner Youngblood, J., & Spencer, M. B. (2002). Integrating normative iden-
city. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. tity processes and academic support requirements for special
Swanson, D., Cunningham, M., & Spencer, M. B. (2003). Black males’ needs adolescents: The application of an Identity-Focused Cul-
structural conditions, achievement patterns, normative needs, and tural Ecological (ICE) Perspective. Journal of Applied Develop-
“opportunities.” Urban Education Journal, 38, 608–633. mental Science, 6, 95–108.
Swanson, D. P., & Spencer, M. B. (1999). Developmental and cultural
context considerations for research on African American adoles-
CHAPTER 16

Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research,


and Applications
PETER L. BENSON, PETER C. SCALES, STEPHEN F. HAMILTON, and ARTURO SESMA JR.

DEFINING POSITIVE YOUTH DEVELOPMENT 895 Hypothesis Two 917


POSITIVE YOUTH DEVELOPMENT IN HISTORICAL Hypothesis Three 920
AND SOCIAL CONTEXT 898 Hypothesis Four 921
THE THEORY OF POSITIVE Hypothesis Five 923
YOUTH DEVELOPMENT 901 Hypothesis Six 926
Human Development 902 Hypothesis Seven 928
Conceptual Models of Positive Development 905 YOUTH DEVELOPMENT RESEARCH 928
The Theory of Context and Community Inf luence 910 Gender 928
The Theory of Context and Community Change 913 Age 928
RESEARCH SUPPORT FOR KEY POSITIVE YOUTH Race/ Ethnicity and SES 929
DEVELOPMENT HYPOTHESES 914 DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH 930
Overview of Positive Youth Development Research 915 CONCLUSIONS 932
How Much Explanatory Power Is Reasonable REFERENCES 933
to Expect? 915
Hypothesis One 916

Positive youth development is simultaneously a field interdisciplinary research, a policy approach, a philoso-
of research and an arena of practice. Linked more by phy, an academic major, a program description, and a
shared ideals than by formal membership or credentials, professional identity (e.g., youth development worker).
positive youth development includes a growing number of The “idea” of positive youth development reaches into a
programs, agencies, foundations, federal grant programs, number of fields, including child and adolescent devel-
policy initiatives, researchers, and youth-serving profes- opmental psychology, public health, health promotion,
sionals committed to promoting competent, healthy, and prevention, sociology, social work, medicine, and educa-
successful youth. Collectively, they have generated ideas, tion. Within the past few years, positive youth develop-
data, and resources. At the same time, they have un- ment has been a focal topic in a wide range of scholarly
leashed a wave of energy and action not unlike that of a journals, including The Annals of the American Academy
social movement, with a multitude of community actors of Political and Social Science (January, 2004), Preven-
connecting to a broad set of principles, concepts, and tion and Treatment (June, 2002), The Prevention Re-
strategies for increasing youth access to the kinds of rela- searcher (April, 2004), and the American Journal of
tionships, programs, settings, and activities known (or as- Health Behavior (July, 2003). Two established research
sumed) to promote healthy development. journals, Applied Developmental Science and New Direc-
Positive youth development is an umbrella term that tions in Youth Development, help to ground the field.
covers many streams of work. It is variously a field of Undergirding positive youth development is an im-
portant and growing line of scientific inquiry, includ-
The writing of this chapter was supported by a grant to ing theory, research, and a set of conceptual models
Search Institute from The Lilly Endowment. and frameworks that both guide and emerge from the

894
Defining Positive Youth Development 895

research. This chapter: (a) defines the concept of posi- plete—if not distorted—view of how organisms develop.
tive youth development; ( b) presents a broad theory of This ongoing debate is addressed in more detail in the
this sphere of human development; (c) examines empir- next section.
ical support for a series of theory-driven hypotheses; Second, Damon, like many other positive youth de-
and (d) proposes implications for theory reformulation, velopment advocates, holds up the centrality of com-
future research, and applications. munity as both an incubator of positive development as
well as a multifaceted setting in which young people
can exercise agency and inform the settings, places,
DEFINING POSITIVE people, and policies that in turn impact their develop-
YOUTH DEVELOPMENT ment. Finally, Damon notes that positive youth devel-
opment, in its efforts to identify the positive attitudes
As noted, the field of positive youth development en- and competencies that energize healthy developmental
compasses a vast territory of disciplines, concepts, and trajectories, is not afraid to identify values, moral per-
strategies. One recent review of positive youth develop- spectives, and religious worldviews as constructive de-
ment (Benson & Pittman, 2001a) suggests four distin- velopmental resources even though this “ flies in the
guishing features of this field. It is comprehensive in its face of our predominantly secular social-science tradi-
scope, linking a variety of: (1) ecological contexts (e.g., tions” (2004, p. 21).
relationships, programs, families, schools, neighbor- Several other accents or themes are increasingly
hoods, congregations, communities) to (2) the produc- prominent in the youth development literature. Two are
tion of experiences, supports, and opportunities known particularly germane for positioning this field in intel-
to (3) enhance positive developmental outcomes. Its pri- lectual and scientific space. A number of scholars
mary organizing principle is promotion (of youth access argue that the definition of developmental success most
to positive experiences, resources and opportunities, deeply entrenched in public policy and practice con-
and of developmental outcomes useful to both self and ceives of health as the absence of disease or pathology.
society). It is, as the term implies, developmental, with In recent decades, the dominant framework driving
emphasis on growth and an increasing recognition that federal, state, and local interventions with youth has
youth can (and should be) deliberate actors in the pro- been that of risk behaviors, including alcohol use,
duction of positive development. And it is symbiotic, tobacco use, other drug use, nonmarital pregnancy,
drawing into its orbit ideas, strategies, and practices suicide, antisocial behavior, violence, and school drop
from many lines of inquiry (e.g., resiliency, prevention, out (Benson, 1997; Hein, 2003; National Research
public health, community organizing, developmental Council & Institute of Medicine [NRCIM], 2002;
psychology). Takanishi, 1993). While positive youth development
Damon (2004; Damon & Gregory, 2003) argues that advocates readily accept that reductions in these
positive youth development represents a sea change in health-compromising behaviors are important markers
psychological theory and research, with observable con- of developmental success, there is simultaneously a
sequences for a variety of fields including education and growing interest in defining “ the other side of the
social policy. Three central themes are noted here. In coin”—that is, the attributes, skills, competencies, and
Damon’s view, positive youth development takes a potentials needed to succeed in the spheres of work,
strength-based approach to defining and understanding family, and civic life. This dichotomy is well captured
the developmental process. More precisely, it “empha- in the youth development mantra “problem free” is not
sizes the manifest potentialities rather than the sup- fully prepared (Pittman & Fleming, 1991). Accord-
posed incapacities of young people . . .” (2004, p. 15). ingly, an important aspect of current positive youth
There is more to this statement than initially meets the development science is the conceptualization and mea-
eye. It connotes a significant critique of mainstream surement of dimensions of positive developmental suc-
psychological inquiry that is quite ubiquitous in the pos- cess. Among these areas of work are efforts to define
itive youth development literature. This critique is that indicators of child well-being (Moore, 1997; Moore,
understandings of child and adolescent development Lippman, & Brown, 2004), thriving (Benson, 2003a;
have been so dominated by the exploration and remedia- Lerner, 2004; Lerner et al., in press; Scales & Benson,
tion of pathology and deficit that we have an incom- 2004; Theokas et al., 2004), and flourishing (Keyes,
896 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

2003). Within this inquiry on positive markers of 4. All youth benefit from these relationships, contexts,
success, an emerging issue has to do with expanding and ecologies. Support, empowerment, and engage-
the conceptualization of developmental success to in- ment are, for example, important developmental as-
clude not only what promotes individual well-being but sets for all youth, generalizing across race, ethnicity,
also what promotes the social good (Benson & Leffert, gender, and family income. However, the strategies
2001; Benson, Mannes, Pittman, & Ferber, 2004; and tactics for promoting these developmental assets
Damon, 1997; Lerner, 2004). can vary considerably as a function of social location.
In turn, this interest in positive indicators covaries 5. Community is a viable and critical “delivery system”
with an emerging accent on reconceptualizing the pop- for positive youth development.
ulation target for improving the lives of children and 6. Youth are major actors in their own development and
youth. This is the debate about “at-risk youth” versus are significant (and underutilized) resources for creat-
“all youth.” In the early stages of the term’s emer- ing the kinds of relationships, contexts, ecologies, and
gence, positive youth development tended to be posi- communities that enable positive youth development.
tioned as a strategy—complementary to reducing
risks—for preventing high-risk behaviors, particularly There are many published definitions of positive
among that subset of youth particularly susceptible to youth development. Indeed, most reviewers of the liter-
the potential harm of poverty and dysfunctional fami- ature and many authors of positive youth development
lies and/or communities. As work moves forward to research articles generate new definitions. This prolif-
expand the notions of health, well-being, and develop- eration of many definitions—as well as concomitant
mental success, and as these ideas merge with histori- lack of consensus on a particular definition—reflects
cal and sociological insights about pervasive societal both the relative newness of the field as well as its pro-
changes, the positive youth development field increas- foundly interdisciplinary nature. Each definition fo-
ingly calls for strategic national and community cuses on some combination (and the interactions among
investments to strengthen the developmental land- them) of the core constructs displayed in Figure 16.1.
scape more generally (Bumbarger & Greenberg, 2002; Figure 16.1 suggests that the core ideas in positive
Lerner, 2000; Lorion & Sokoloff, 2003). Ultimately, youth development include (A) developmental contexts
we might characterize this issue as whether the na- (i.e., places, settings, ecologies, and relationships with
tional priority should be to promote “good enough” de- the potential to generate supports, opportunities, and re-
velopment or to promote optimal development. In more sources); (B) the nature of the child with accents on in-
poetic language, Lorion and Sokoloff (2003) offer that herent capacity to grow and thrive (and actively engage
this choice is between “ fixing” troubled youth and the with supportive contexts); (C) developmental strengths
view that “all soil can be enriched and all moisture (attributes of the person, including skills, competencies,
and sunlight maximally used to nourish all flowers” values, and dispositions important for successful en-
(p. 137). gagement in the world); and two complimentary concep-
Several attempts have been made to articulate the tualizations of developmental success; (D) the reduction
core concepts and principles in the positive youth devel- of high-risk behavior; and (E) the promotion of thriving.
opment field (Benson & Pittman, 2001a, 2001b; Cata- The bidirectional arrows intend to convey the dynamic
lano, Berglund, Ryan, Lonczak, & Hawkins, 1999; nature of person-ecology interactions prominent in re-
Hamilton, Hamilton, & Pittman, 2004; NRCIM, 2002). cent expositions of positive youth development (Lerner,
A synthesis of these reviews suggests considerable con- 2003, 2004).
sensus on these six principles: We know of no definition that encompasses all of this
conceptual territory. But the fullness of these constructs
1. All youth have the inherent capacity for positive is evident when integrating a representative sample of
growth and development. published definitions. Several accent the nature of the
2. A positive developmental trajectory is enabled when child (B). Damon (2004), for example, offers that “ the
youth are embedded in relationships, contexts, and positive youth development perspective emphasizes the
ecologies that nurture their development. manifest potentialities rather than the supposed inca-
3. The promotion of positive development is further en- pacities of young people—including young people from
abled when youth participate in multiple, nutrient- the most disadvantaged backgrounds and those with the
rich relationships, contexts, and ecologies. most troubled histories” (p. 17).
Defining Positive Youth Development 897

Contexts Person Developmental Success

Work Place

Peers Family
Reduction in
View of
(b) (d) High-Risk
(a) the Child
Behaviors
Community

Congregrations School

Promotion
Developmental of Health
(c) Strengths (e) Well-Being
Programs Neighborhoods Thriving

Figure 16.1 Core positive youth development constructs.

Hamilton (1999; Hamilton et al., 2004) noted that ated a definition that named the objectives of positive
the term has been used in three ways. His first defini- youth development approaches. Hence, positive youth
tion reflects, like Damon, an articulation of the nature development seeks to promote one or more of the fol-
of the child (B in Figure 16.1): “ youth development has lowing: bonding, resilience, social competence, emo-
traditionally been and is still most widely used to mean tional competence, cognitive competence, behavioral
a natural process: the growing capacity of a young per- competence, moral competence, self-determination,
son to understand and act on the environment ” (Hamil- spirituality, self-efficacy, positive identity, belief in
ton et al., 2004, p. 3). His second definition picks up the the future, recognition for positive behavior, opportu-
role of contexts (A in Figure 16.1) in the development of nities for prosocial involvement, and prosocial norms.
strengths (C): “in the 1990s the term youth develop- This definition, then, focuses on describing the territo-
ment came to be applied to a set of principles, a philoso- ries of (C) developmental strengths and (E) well-being
phy or approach emphasizing active support for the in Figure 16.1.
growing capacity of young people by individuals, organ- In 2002, the National Research Council and Institute
izations and institutions, especially at the community of Medicine released the influential report, Community
level” (Hamilton et al., 2004, p. 4). Finally, youth de- Programs to Promote Youth Development (NRCIM,
velopment also refers to a “planned set of practices, or 2002). Though this report did not offer a clear definition
activities, that foster the developmental process in of the term, its focus was on defining (and advocating
young people” (Hamilton et al., 2004, p. 4). These prac- for) two of the constructs in Figure 16.1: “ the personal
tices occur within the context portion (A) of Figure and social assets” young people need “ to function well
16.1 and can be delivered via programs, organizations, during adolescence and adulthood” (p. 3) and the fea-
or community initiatives. tures of positive developmental settings. These two rep-
Catalano et al. (1999, 2004) conducted a major re- resent constructs C and A in Figure 16.1.
view of the positive youth development field with sup- Larson (2000) contrasts positive youth development
port from the National Institute of Child Health and with developmental psychopathology and suggests the
Human Development (NICHD). Among its purposes former is about “ how things go right ” while the latter
were to “research and establish both theoretical and focuses on “ how things go wrong.” Hence, his focus is
empirical definitions of positive youth development ” on positive youth development as a line of inquiry re-
(Catalano et al., 1999, p. ii). Arguing that no compre- garding “ the pathways whereby children and adoles-
hensive definition of the term could be found, they cre- cents become motivated, directed, socially competent,
898 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

compassionate and psycholocally vigorous adults” of Chicago’s experimental Community Youth Develop-
(p. 170). The pathways organically link contexts (A), de- ment Program, an initiative designed to identify and or-
velopmental strengths (C) and developmental success ganize community resources to better serve youth with
(D and E). In a similar vein, Lerner’s definition “special problems” or “special abilities” (Havighurst,
(Lerner, Fisher, & Weinberg, 2000) contrasts pathol- 1953).
ogy—reducing and asset-building approaches. “Prevent- Federal agencies dealing with juvenile delinquency
ing the actualization of youth risk behaviors is not the expanded on their earlier efforts and took another
same thing as taking actions to promote positive youth important conceptual step. In 1970, the Youth Develop-
development (e.g., the inculcation of attributes such as ment and Delinquency Prevention Administration
caring/compassion, competence, character, connection, ( housed in what was then the Department of Health,
and confidence). Similarly, programs and policies that Education, and Welfare) developed a delinquency pre-
prevent youth problems do not necessarily prepare youth vention program based on what keeps “good kids on
to contribute to civil society” (p. 12). track ” rather than the more prevalent question of the
Some recent definitions place additional accent on day (“ why do kids get into trouble?”; West, 1974). The
the processes and dynamics of designing and mobilizing federal answer to the question of why some youth suc-
developmental contexts (A in Figure 16.1) to enhance ceed had four components: a sense of competence, a
C, D, E, and their intersection. Benson and Saito (2001), sense of usefulness, a sense of belonging, and a sense of
for example, suggested that “ youth development mobi- power (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services
lizes programs, organizations, systems and communities Administration for Children and Families, 1996, p. 4).
to build developmental strengths in order to promote In these state and federal approaches to addressing
health and well-being” (p. 144). Finally, Small and “ troubled and troubling youth,” we see the early signs of
Memmo (2004) identify a variant on positive youth de- two cornerstones of contemporary youth development
velopment that places an important accent on mobilizing approaches: the primacy of context for shaping develop-
youth to shape their contexts and communities. Called ment and development understood in terms of strength
Community Youth Development (Hughes & Curnan, rather than deficit. Though such ideas hardly seem like
2000; Perkins, Borden, & Villarruel, 2001; Perkins, intellectual advances now, it is important to note how
Borden, Keith, Hoppe-Rooney, & Villarruel, 2003), this these ideas came to challenge historical and deeply en-
approach takes seriously the bidirectional arrow in Fig- trenched therapeutic models.
ure 16.1 connecting A with B and C. As we will see in Subsequently, a number of prominent foundations en-
the section on the theory of youth development, this tered the picture. In addition to major youth develop-
bidirectionality is a central feature of developmental ment grant programs at the Kellogg Foundation, the
systems theory and in particular, Lerner’s application of Lilly Endowment, and the Kauffman Foundation, the
this theory to positive youth development (Lerner, 2003, Carnegie Corporation of New York and the William T.
2004; Lerner, Brentano, Dowling, & Anderson, 2002). Grant Foundation sponsored and broadly disseminated
pivotal reports on the developmental trajectories of
POSITIVE YOUTH DEVELOPMENT IN American youth. Moving beyond the question of how so-
HISTORICAL AND SOCIAL CONTEXT ciety best deals with its so-called “at-risk youth,” these
influential reports began to document more persistent
Early uses of the term youth development can be found and pervasive issues about health and well-being of
in the literature on juvenile delinquency. In 1947, the American youth. To some extent, the reports expanded
Texas State Development Council was formed follow- the need for enhancing developmental supports and op-
ing a report from a blue-ribbon commission charging portunities to include most young people.
that the state-run schools for delinquent children were In 1985, the Carnegie Corporation launched the
failing. Embedded in the report was the suggestion that Carnegie Council on Adolescent Development. The con-
the causes of delinquency included environmental fac- cluding report, Great Transitions: Preparing Adoles-
tors with the implication that well-entrenched models cents for a New Century, sought to focus the national
of changing behavior by “ fixing the child” were in- spotlight on adolescence (Carnegie Council on Adoles-
sufficient. This new understanding of the contextual cent Development, 1995). The report, like many before
backdrop to individual development gained further mo- it, lamented not only the high rates of high risk behav-
mentum in a series of monographs from the University iors (e.g., alcohol use, illicit drug use, teen pregnancy)
Positive Youth Development in Historical and Social Context 899

and exposure to developmental threats (e.g., physical lems” more to environments and contexts than youth
and sexual abuse) among adolescents, but the emer- themselves, with a concomitant call for reforming
gence of alarming rates for these phenomena among 10- and/or transforming contexts; and mainstreaming the
to 15-year-olds. Unlike other reports on the health of need for change (i.e., the percentage of youth needing
youth, however, the Carnegie Council proposed solu- change goes far beyond the notion of “at-risk ” youth). A
tions based less on services to and treatment of youth corollary to these three strands is the oft-repeated idea
and more on altering the formative contexts of families, that youth are resources to be utilized rather than prob-
schools, community organizations, and the media. lems to be fixed.
Among key recommendations were reengaging families Several additional events have added direction and
with their adolescent children, designing developmen- momentum to the positive youth development move-
tally attentive schools, and transforming the media into ment. The first was a symbolic and galvanizing histori-
a socially constructive resource. And in a reaffirmation cal moment—the gathering of five living U.S. presidents
of the Carnegie Council’s early report, A Matter of (Carter, G. H. W. Bush, Clinton, Ford, and Reagan, rep-
Time: Risk and Opportunity in the Non School Hours resented by Nancy Reagan) with hundreds of influential
(Carnegie Corporation of New York, 1992), this 1995 delegates—for the President’s Summit on Youth in
report called for community investment in and expan- Philadelphia. This April, 1997 event offered an accessi-
sion of “safe, growth-promoting settings during the ble language of positive development—and a passionate
high-risk, after school time when parents are often not call to action—around five fundamental development re-
able to supervise their children and adolescents” (Ham- sources (or promises). These were: caring adults, safe
burg & Takanishi, 1996, p. 387). places and structured activities, community service, ed-
Several years earlier, the William T. Grant Founda- ucation for marketable skills, and a healthy start. This
tion released The Forgotten Half: Pathways to Success 1997 event became institutionalized with the formation
for America’s Youth and Young Families (1988). Its of America’s Promise, a not-for-profit community mobi-
focus was on ages 16 to 24 and the transition from ado- lization organization initially led by (Ret.) General
lescence to adulthood. In its words, “ half of our youth Colin Powell.
are in danger of being caught in a massive bind that can While this and other mobilization efforts gave im-
deny them full participation in our society and the full petus to the principles of positive youth development, a
benefit of their own talents” (p. 1). Like the Carnegie series of publications gave greater intellectual and
Council report, The Forgotten Half focused its recom- scientific attention to the youth development idea.
mendations on changing community and societal con- In 1998, the Youth Development Directions Project
texts. Among its specific recommendations: closer (YDDP) was conceived by the Youth Development
adult-youth relationships, opportunities to participate Funders Group at a meeting held at the Ewing Marian
in the life of community in activities valued by adults Kauffman Foundation in Kansas City. The purpose was
(including community service), and quality work expe- to take stock of the youth development field and lay out
riences that provide skill-building pathways to sustain- suggested direction for strengthening science, practice
able work. and policy. A number of organizations, including the
In combination, these two highly visible reports chal- Academy for Educational Development (Center for
lenged the common assumption that the “ youth prob- Youth Development and Research), Chapin Hall Center
lem” was confined to a small percentage of youth for Children at the University of Chicago, The Forum
needing special and targeted services to redeem them. for Youth Investment at the International Youth Foun-
Instead, portraits of youth emerge which suggested that dation, Public/ Private Ventures, and Search Institute
the developmental journey was fragile for a much larger participated in a 2-year learning and writing project,
percentage of youth. And both reports made bold calls culminating in one of the first efforts to capture the
for systemic change in how communities and their so- breadth and status of the field (Benson & Pittman,
cialization systems connect with young people. 2001b; Public/ Private Ventures, 2000).
By the 1990s, three ideas generally important in the Moreover, as already noted, influential federal re-
youth development field were gaining momentum. These ports had reviewed the field of positive youth develop-
are: identifying positive, developmental “ building ment. Both focused on the slice of youth development
blocks” which help youth stay on a successful develop- having to do with the creation of developmentally atten-
mental trajectory; attributing causality for “ youth prob- tive programs. One, initiated by the Board on Children,
900 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Youth and Families of the National Research Council, models focused primarily on reducing risk behaviors,
created a Committee on Community-Level Programs for example, are inadequate both theoretically and
for Youth that evaluated and synthesized the science strategically. Furthermore, models driven by risk,
of adolescent development with research on the quality deficit, and pathology may unintentionally become part
and efficiency of community programs designed to pro- of the problem (e.g., by negatively labeling youth and/or
mote healthy development, and resulted in the influen- fueling unfavorable stereotypes of youth). These ideas
tial report, Community Programs to Promote Youth have been discussed in a wide range of positive youth
Development (NRCIM, 2002). The second was the com- development publications (Catalano et al., 1999; Lerner,
prehensive review of positive youth development pro- 2004; Pittman, Irby, & Ferber, 2001; Roth & Brooks-
grams evaluations commissioned by the National Gunn, 2000; Villarruel, Perkins, Borden, & Keith,
Institute for Child Health and Human Development 2003). In one particularly important analysis, Larson
(Catalano et al., 1999). (2000) suggests that developmental psychology has
The rise of positive youth development as a field of spawned a much stronger tradition for understanding
science and practice has been fueled by two types of so- and treating psychopathology than for understanding
cial analysis. The first documents a series of pervasive and promoting pathways to developmental success. In
societal changes that inform and shape the processes of this regard, positive youth development advocates are
child and adolescent socialization. It is common in pub- sympathetic to positive psychology’s critique of the
lished treatises on positive youth development strategies dominance of pathology-oriented research and practice
to pinpoint the role of rapid social change in altering within mainstream psychology (Seligman & Csikszent-
youth access to developmental resources. In this exten- mihalyi, 2000).
sive literature, social changes hypothesized to under- The premise that positive youth development repre-
mine the capacity of family and community to generate sents a categorically different approach than so-called
developmental resources include: increasing parental ab- deficit, pathology, and risk models deserves deeper ex-
sence as a result of changes in the nature of work and the ploration. There is consensus that adolescent psychol-
dramatic increase in out-of home employment of moth- ogy, and applied youth areas have been dominated, in
ers; the rise of civic disengagement; the loss of shared recent decades, by explorations of “ youth problems.”
ideals about the goals of development; the growing priva- The social historian, Francis Fukuyama (1999), attri-
tization of recreation; increases in age segregation; the butes this, in part, to a logical outgrowth of rapid social
decrease in neighborhood cohesion; teenagers’ discon- change. When social institutions become less stable—as
nection from structured programming; the prevalence of in the United States beginning in the 1960s—govern-
negative stereotypes about youth; and the explosion in ments, he noted, inevitably begin to create and measure
media access by youth (see, e.g., Benson, 1997; Benson, indicators of social upheaval, and craft policy and pro-
Leffert, Scales, & Blyth, 1998; Damon, 1997; Dryfoos, grams to minimize social and personal problems as-
1990; Furstenberg, 2000; Garbarino, 1995; Lerner, sumed to emerge from social change (e.g., violence,
1995; Mortimer & Larson, 2002; Scales, 1991, 2001). In alcohol, and other drug use).
a particularly cogent analysis of these trends, Bronfen- Sociologist Frank Furstenberg (2000; Furstenberg,
brenner and Morris (1998) offered this summary: Modell, & Herschberg, 1976), argues that adolescence
becomes culturally defined as a lifestage when school-
The research findings presented here reveal growing chaos
ing replaces work as the major activity during youth.
in the lives of families, in child care settings, schools, peer
groups, youth programs, neighborhoods, workplaces, and
This, he suggests, occurred in the United States near the
other everyday environments in which human beings live middle of the twentieth century. The advent of full-time
their lives. Such chaos, in turn, interrupts and undermines education “establishes a youth-based social world that is
the formation and stability of relationships and activities age segregated, partially buffered from adult control,
that are necessary for psychological growth. (p. 1022) and relatively turned in on itself ” (2000, p. 897). Not
surprisingly, societies interpret the consequences of this
The second social analysis common in the youth de- upheaval in terms of “ youth problems.” Consequently,
velopment literature is a critique of deficit models and in line with Fukuyama’s analysis, cultural authori-
prominent in the service professions, policy, and re- ties focus major attention on behaviors and styles that
search. Indeed, it is a somewhat common refrain that contradict established social norms. And not surpris-
The Theory of Positive Youth Development 901

ingly, social scientific studies of youth follow suit, with cessfully with risk. As noted by Rutter (1987), protec-
a disproportionate focus on problem behaviors (Dry- tive factors operate only when risk is present.
foos, 1998; Larson, 2000; Roth & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; There are important points of overlap and of differ-
Steinberg & Lerner, 2004). This dominating theme in ence when comparing positive youth development with
youth research likely reflects the dual consequence of this major risk and protective factor approach to preven-
the cultural zeitgeist and the longer term hospitality in tion. The two approaches partially agree on develop-
mainline psychology to the study of pathology (Larson, mental goals. That is, both are dedicated to reducing
2000; Moore et al., 2004; Peterson, 2004). Furstenberg problem behaviors and negative outcomes. At the same
(2000) provided a cogent description of the implications time, however, positive youth development tends to place
of these social and scientific trends for the broader pub- as much or more focus on promoting additional ap-
lic perception of youth: proaches to health, including thriving skill-building and
competency (Bumbarger & Greenberg, 2002; Pittman &
Such an approach inevitably treats successful adolescents
Fleming, 1991). There is also some overlap in under-
and young adults as escape artists who manage to dodge
the hazards of growing up, rather than focusing on the
standing the processes and mechanisms involved in the
ways that young people acquire and master skills, con- production of successful development. Some of the so-
struct positive identities, and learn how to negotiate so- called protective factors that buffer risk and reduce neg-
cial roles simultaneously in the youth culture and adult ative outcomes also play a role in the production of
world. (p. 900) positive outcomes (Catalano, Hawkins, Berglund, Pol-
lard, & Arthur, 2002). Alternatively, positive youth de-
At first glance, it would appear that positive youth de- velopment research also identifies a series of additional
velopment represents a theoretical, research and practice supports, opportunities, and developmental assets
“paradigm shift ” from the prevention field—a multidis- whose identification emerges from investigations of en-
ciplinary area of inquiry, programming, policy, and vironmental and individual factors that promote compe-
practice with a substantial American history (Wanders- tence, achievement, growth, and thriving (Benson,
man & Florin, 2003; Weissberg, Kumpfer, & Seligman, 2003a; Lerner, 2004; Scales, Benson, Leffert, & Blyth,
2003). However, a considerable debate is underway about 2000). Hence, protective factors and the broader range
the conceptual overlap between prevention and positive of developmental resources central to positive youth de-
youth development (Benson et al., 2004; Bumbarger & velopment are not isomorphic.
Greenberg, 2002; Catalano & Hawkins, 2002; Roth & At another level, however, prevention and positive
Brooks-Gunn, 2003; Sesma, Mannes, & Scales, in press; youth development are grounded in quite different theo-
Small & Memmo, 2004). retical orientations and—though yoked by common in-
Prevention and prevention science are deeply rooted terest in the health of youth—spring forth from quite
in public health and epidemiological approaches to dis- different visions of youth potential and the developmen-
ease prevention (Bloom, 1996; Small & Memmo, 2004), tal, ecological, and social processes at play (Damon &
with a particular focus on crafting interventions before Gregory, 2003; Lerner, 2004).
the onset of significant problems, and with a focus on
populations known to be at risk for such onset (Durlak,
1998; Munóz, Mrazek, & Haggerty, 1996). This form of THE THEORY OF POSITIVE
prevention has been called primary prevention, in con- YOUTH DEVELOPMENT
trast to secondary and tertiary prevention (Caplan,
1964), or in more contemporary parlance, universal pre- A grand theory of positive youth development requires
vention in contrast to targeted prevention (Weissberg the integration of multiple theoretical orientations. In
et al., 2003). At the center of current prevention research part, this is because positive youth development is a
are the concepts of risk factors and protective factors “ bridging” field that touches multiple academic disci-
(Jessor, 1993; Jessor, Turbin, & Costa, 1998; Rutter, plines and spheres of practice. Three theoretical strands
1987). Risk factors are individual and/or environmental central to positive youth development are discussed in
markers which increase the probability of a negative this section, with primary emphasis on the first. These
outcome. Protective factors are safeguards identified in three are: human development, community organization
epidemiological research that help individuals cope suc- and development, and social and community change.
902 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Human Development development has begun. This historical progression of


practice, to research, to theory may not be the idealized
Central to positive youth development theory is a series scientific progression, but it is important here to iden-
of questions rooted squarely in the discipline of develop- tify how this evolutionary pattern critiques the hereto-
mental psychology. The overarching goals of this theory fore irrelevance of developmental psychology to the
are to explain: the capacity of youth to change and to massive number of people and organizations trying to
change in a direction that fosters both individual well- innovate strength-based youth work in the United States.
being and the social good; how and under what condi- As Larson (2000) put it, youth development evolved sep-
tions contextual and ecological factors contribute to this arately from development psychology “partly because
change (and how these factors are informed or influ- we psychologists have had little to offer ” (p. 171). Al-
enced by the developing person); and, the principles and ternatively, this progression may be an exemplar of the
mechanisms that are at play in maximizing the dynamic kind of citizen-scholar partnership needed to promote
and developmentally constructive interplay of context civil society (Lerner et al., 2000).
and individual. Essential to positive youth development theory is
The articulation of a developmental theory of posi- a generous view of human capacity and potential.
tive youth development is itself an ongoing and dy- Grounded initially in the views and values of profes-
namic process emerging several decades after the sionals and practitioners working with youth, this vi-
birthing of positive youth development as a field of sion of human nature identified the possibility of active
practice (Benson & Saito, 2001; Hamilton & Hamilton, and constructive contribution to the development of
2004; Larson, 2000; Zeldin, 2000). Zeldin (2000) pro- self, community, and society. As noted earlier in this
vides an important analysis of how the science of youth chapter, such a view is often characterized in youth de-
development emerged: velopment circles by describing young people as re-
sources to be nurtured versus problems to be managed.
[I]n hindsight, it is clear that positive youth development,
This view is an important starting point for a theory of
as a philosophy of service and as a field of study was initi-
ated and grounded in the expertise of practitioners, pri-
positive youth development, for it brings to the fore the
marily those working in nonprofit, community-based, notion that the individual—and not just the environ-
youth-serving organizations. Research was used primarily ment—is a prime actor in the shaping of positive devel-
to offer “empirical justification” for exemplary practice opmental trajectories.
that was already occurring in communities. (p. 3) Damon (2004), in an important essay titled “What Is
Positive Youth Development ” argues that this positive
An important step in growing the science of positive vision of youth potential has implications for research,
youth development was a “call to action” made by a team education, and social policy. He also sees this human na-
of researchers and leaders of youth development organi- ture assumption supported by three relatively recent
zations (Zeldin, 1995). Facilitated by the Academy for lines of inquiry: the research on resilience (Garmezy,
Educational Development, this 1995 document chal- 1983); the capacity of newborns to demonstrate empa-
lenged academicians—particularly those engaged in the thy (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998; Hoffman, 2000); and the
study of adolescence—to focus research on strength- universal capacity for moral awareness and prosocial be-
based models of adolescent development, identify and havior (Feshbach, 1983; Madsen, 1971). Damon also as-
study positive youth outcomes, and identify “ the day-to- serts that this human capacity for competence and
day developmental opportunities and supports that allow contribution is at play when seeking to explain how
young people to become competent and compassionate young people “learn and thrive in the diverse settings
individuals connected to their communities” (Zeldin, where they live.”
2000, p. 3). The essence of positive youth development theory is
The “golden age” of positive youth development re- explaining how such potentiality expresses itself. The
search began in the mid-1990s, with burgeoning litera- theory requires an appreciation of the dynamic inter-
tures on topics such as civic engagement, service play of person and context. Accordingly, the theory is
learning, connectedness, generosity, purpose, empower- most at home in a family of theoretical approaches con-
ment, and leadership. In the past few years, work posit- stituting the large metatheory known as developmental
ing the theoretical foundations of positive youth systems theory (Ford & Lerner, 1992; Gottlieb, 1997).
The Theory of Positive Youth Development 903

This metatheory includes several crucial assumptions An articulation of this point suggests that individual
and components that, in combination, positions human development cannot be explained by heredity or envi-
development in relational and contextual space, and that ronment alone (Gottlieb, Wahlsten, & Lickliter, 1998).
stand in contrast to earlier developmental theories that Evidence for this comes from studies where “geneti-
split development into such polarities as nature-nurture, cally identical individuals are reared in unusually uni-
biology-culture and individual—society (Lerner, 1998; form environments but nonetheless differ markedly in
Overton, 1998). phenotypic types” (Gottlieb et al., p. 253). While indi-
Although positive youth development theory is predi- vidual differences—stemming presumably from neither
cated on key concepts in developmental systems theory, genes nor contexts—can be a nuisance to theorists pre-
it includes several other core ideas having to do with the ferring reductionist understandings of development,
orchestration of bidirectional context-person relation- such so-called “noise” or “randomness” points to the
ships in order to maximize growth and development. “ third source” of development central in developmental
While positive youth development can happen naturally system theories.
(as in the adage that “positive youth development is what Positive youth development theory includes another
happens when families have a good day”), such adaptive dynamic feature of the organism that is consonant with
development regulations (Lerner, 1998, 2004) can be en- the process of self-organization but not readily inferred
couraged and engineered by the ways contexts are de- from it. And this is the concept of how persons act on
signed and the ways youth are engaged in that design. their contexts. Indeed, one of the core tenets in develop-
Central to the theory of positive youth development mental systems theory is the bidirectional nature of in-
are conceptions of the developing person, the contexts in fluences on development. That is, the “individual is both
which the person is embedded and the dynamic inter- the active producer and the product of his or her on-
action between the two. Following Lerner’s lead (1984, togeny . . .” (Brandtstädter, 1998, p. 800). Action theo-
1998, 2002, 2003), all of the multiple levels of organiza- ries of human development seek to explain these dual
tions engaged in human development—from biology and developmental regulation processes of the action of con-
personality disposition to relationships, social institu- texts on individuals and the action of individuals on
tions, culture, and history—are fused into an integrated their contexts. This process by which organisms engage,
system. Development has to do with changes in the rela- interact with, and alter their developmental contexts
tions among and between these multiple levels of organ- (e.g., peer group, family, school, and neighborhood) is
izations. Consonant with systems thinking in biology, not only a pivotal theoretical notion for positive youth
persons—through their dynamic interaction with devel- development, but is also “ the essential intellectual chal-
opmental contexts—experience pattern and order via lenge for developmental science” (Lerner, 2003, p. 228).
the process of self-organizing. This key dynamic of self- What processes guide how youth engage and act on
organization means that “pattern and order emerge from their contexts? There are a series of developmental
the interactions of the components of a complex system processes particularly salient during adolescence.
without explicit instructions, either in the organization Among these are identify formation and allied issues
itself or from the environment. Self-organization— around self-appraisal, meaning-making, and autonomy.
processes that by their own activities change them- Because of the centrality of these issues during adoles-
selves—is a fundamental property of living things” cence, positive youth development theory argues that
(Thelen & Smith, 1998, p. 564). At one level, this pro- adolescents bring particular energy to their relational
posed dynamic interaction of nature and nurture is a and social world. Their activity—as “co-producers” of
dramatic departure from earlier models of human devel- their development—is guided by three intertwined
opment which created a split between the two (Lorenz, processes, each of which is rooted in theoretical tradi-
1965; Skinner, 1938). At another level, however, the tions from within the broader “ family” of developmen-
concept of self-organization introduces, as Lerner sug- tal systems theories. Indeed, we think of these three as
gested (1976, 2003) a “ third source” of development: prime features of the “engine” of development. And in
the organism itself. Schneirla’s (1957, 1959) concepts of combination, the three make possible a purposeful
circular functions and self-stimulation were important search for positive (i.e., developmentally rich) contexts.
illustrations of the organism’s centrality and active par- Brandtstädter’s action theory of development empha-
ticipation in development. sizes the role of intentionality in guiding and regulating
904 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

one’s engagement with social and symbolic environ- ployees, and peers. Positive youth development theory
ments (1998, 1999). His assumption is that persons re- posits that adolescents will strive to find and/or create
flect on, learn from, and use feedback from their social optimizing settings even when their degrees of freedom
engagements creating behavioral intentions that guide are limited. These settings may be countercultural
subsequent behavior. While this proposed dynamic has and/or deemed by society to be out-of-bounds. This
currency across the life span, it is a hallmark of adoles- axiom is supported by the work of Heckhausen and her
cence. There are a range of possible constraints on how colleagues (Heckhausen, 1999; Heckhausen & Krueger,
the person self-regulates internal engagements with her 1993; Heckhausen & Schulz, 1995). As in the model of
or his social and symbolic worlds. As Brandtstädter sug- selection, optimization, and compensation (Baltes &
gests “ these constraints lie partly or even completely Baltes, 1990), she is concerned with the dialectic be-
outsides one’s span of control, but they decisively struc- tween possibility (i.e., plasticity) and constraint. She ar-
ture the range of behavioral and developmental options” gues that “primary control” (or the process of acting on
(1998, p. 808). the environment in order to make it more congruent with
In addition to intentionality, there are selection and ones needs) is a dominating human striving, particularly
optimization processes that also inform how persons during adolescence and young adulthood.
interact with their environments. Aligned with Baltes Lerner (1998, 2002, 2003, 2004; Lerner, Anderson,
and his colleagues (Baltes & Baltes, 1990; Baltes, Balsano, Dowling, & Bobek, 2003; Lerner et al., 2002)
Dittmann-Kohli, & Dixon, 1984; Baltes, Lindenberger, has been particularly productive and influential in con-
& Staudinger, 1998), positive youth development theory necting core ideas in developmental systems theories to
posits that youth select from a range of developmental the emerging field of positive youth development. His
supports and opportunities a subset that has psychologi- overarching view is that “changes across the life span
cal and social advantage for prioritized personal goals. are seen as propelled by the dynamic relations between
Selection, then, has to do with both one’s preferences the individual and the multiple levels of the ecology of
(e.g., to learn to play the flute, to find friends, to exper- human development (family, peer group, school, com-
iment with drama) and the ecologies one chooses to be munity, culture), all changing interdependently across
the primary crucibles for development. Optimization is time ( history)” (Lerner et al., 2002, pp. 13–14). His
“ the process of acquiring, refining, coordinating, and thinking about three core concepts—temporal embed-
applying goal-relevant means or resources” toward the dedness, plasticity, and developmental regulation—is
selected targets (Lerner, 2002, p. 224). Critical issues in central to the formation of positive youth development
the applied youth development world include: how well theory and deepens the assumptions of person-context
communities provide meaningful opportunities for opti- interactions described earlier.
mization; and how well communities make it possible Temporal embeddedness refers to the potentiality,
for youth to create optimization opportunities (e.g., to across the entire life span, for change in person-context
begin a new sports or arts program, or to attach oneself relations. This potentiality—yoked with our earlier dis-
to an appropriate mentor). cussion of the principles of self-organization and the ac-
The self-regulation of context engagement—even tive participation by the individual in shaping one’s
when buoyed with an internal press guided by intention- contexts—liberates us from the idea that biology, envi-
ality, selection, and optimization—creates something of ronment, or the combination of the two, is destiny.
a conundrum for those on whom the constraints on ac- Positive youth development—as theory and practice—
tion appear sizable. These constraints, which are well works in the optimistic arena offered by temporal em-
articulated in a number of life span and life course the- beddedness and by the relative plasticity (i.e., the
ories (e.g., Elder, 1974, 1980, 1998, 1999; Nesselroade, potential for systemic change) that derives from it. That
1977; Schaie, 1965), can have strong salience during is, temporality and relative plasticity mean that, “ the
adolescence. Youth, after all, both seek control and are potential to enhance human life” always exists (Lerner
controlled, with many agents in their lives who, by et al., 2002, p. 14).
virtue of position and power, can either suppress or en- Finally, Lerner links the concept of developmental
courage exploration, selection, and optimization. regulation to the promise of positive youth development.
Among this army of socialization agents are parents, By so doing, he gives the theory a way to understand
neighbors, teachers, youth workers, coaches, clergy, em- how individuals manage or shape their relations with
The Theory of Positive Youth Development 905

multiple contexts. Developmental systems theories de- of identity formation and of how self-appraisal processes
rive concepts of developmental regulation from the idea regarding one’s standing in multiple contexts (e.g.,
of relative plasticity. As persons actively regulate their schools) inform the processing of bidirectional person-
development, developmental change occurs in the mu- context transactions. Phenomenological variant of eco-
tual exchange between person and context. Adaptive logical systems th, then, integrates issues of historical
( healthy) developmental regulation occurs when there is and cultural context (e.g., race and gender stereotypes,
a balance between individual capacity or strengths and minority status) into normative developmental processes.
the “growth-promoting influences of the social world” This theory has been extensively utilized to understand
(Lerner, 2004, p. 44). the development of African American youth. New work
Positive youth development, then, occurs in the fu- is underway to understand the historical and cultural
sion of an active, engaged, and competent person with contexts informing the development of Latino/ Latina
receptive, supportive, and nurturing ecologies. The con- youth (Rodriguez & Morrobel, 2004).
sequences of these balanced interactions—particularly
when they are frequent and sustained—can be seen at Conceptual Models of Positive Development
both the individual and social level. Among these hy-
potheses are the advancement of individual thriving and A series of conceptual models have emerged to identify
the reduction of health-compromising behaviors (Ben- the positive developmental experiences that enhance
son, 1997; Benson et al., 1998; Lerner, 2004; Lerner & the fusion of person and context. A rich vocabulary has
Benson, 2003; Scales, Benson, et al., 2000). A common developed to describe these development-enhancing in-
vocabulary in positive youth development for describing gredients. Among these are supports, opportunities,
these effects is the five Cs: competence, confidence, developmental nutrients, developmental strengths, and
connection, character, and caring (or compassion; developmental assets.
NRCIM, 2002; Lerner, 2004; Lerner et al., 2000; Roth & One important research-based tradition informing
Brooks-Gunn, 2003) has written extensively about a these conceptualizations is that of resilience. Formal in-
“6th C” fueled by adaptive developmental regulations: quiry into resilience, or the development of positive adap-
contribution. In his frame, the six Cs are essential not tation in the context of significant adversity (Masten,
only for individual well-being but also for the creation 2001), took root during the 1960s and 1970s. In an effort
of healthy and civil society. to better understand maladaptive behavior, psychologists
Several recent lines of inquiry are congruent with and psychiatrists studied children believed to be at risk
this thinking. The goodness-of-fit model, for example, for pathology (e.g., children of a parent with schizophre-
demonstrates the adaptive consequences of good nia), and observed that some children were developing
matches between individual competencies and needs normally (Masten & Coatsworth, 1998). These early ef-
with the demands, features, and responsiveness of devel- forts at understanding “invulnerables” (Werner & Smith,
opmental settings, such as families and schools (Bogen- 1989) focused on personal qualities of the child, such as
schneider, Small, & Tsay, 1997; Chess & Thomas, 1999; self-esteem or high intelligence (Anthony, 1974). Eventu-
Galambos & Turner, 1999; Thomas & Chess, 1977). ally researchers came to understand that resilience was
Similarly, Eccles and her colleagues (Eccles, 1997; Ec- not a trait inherent in the child, but rather was a function
cles & Harold, 1996), employing a stage-environment fit of the child? environment interaction. This more ecologi-
model, demonstrate how embeddedness in developmen- cal approach led to the identification of three broad sets
tally appropriate environments such as schools influ- of protective factors implicated in fostering resilience:
ences motivation and academic achievement. (1) those within a child (cognitive abilities, easy tem-
As we note later in this chapter, the issue of diversity perament); (2) within the family (organized family envi-
is central to positive youth development. Spencer and her ronment, close parent-child relationships); and (3) within
colleagues (Spencer, 1995, 1999; Spencer, Dupree, & the broader social ecology (effective schools, relationship
Hartmann, 1997) provide a particularly important re- with a caring adult; Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000;
finement and extension of the kinds of ecological and Masten & Garmezy, 1985).
systems dynamics shaping the theory of youth develop- The primary mechanism through which resilience ap-
ment. Central to her phenomenological variant of ecolog- proaches attempt to facilitate positive development is via
ical systems theory (PVEST), Spencer uses the concept intervention and prevention programs. One exemplar of
906 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

this approach is Hawkins’ social development model and training. Connell, Gambone, and Smith (2001) posit
(Hawkins & Catalano, 1996). This model asserts that three major developmental resources: the ability to be
children who experience developmentally appropriate op- productive, the ability to connect, and the ability to nav-
portunities for active involvement in their families, igate. Zeldin (1995; Zeldin, Kimball, & Price, 1995)
schools, and communities, and are recognized for their identifies access to safe places, challenging experiences,
efforts are more likely to form positive bonds and attach- and caring people.
ments that inhibit deviant or problem behavior (Catalano The concept of developmental assets emerged in 1990
et al., 2003). According to these authors, the following (Benson, 1990, 1997, 2002, 2003a) and has triggered
salient protective factors are necessary for prevention: considerable research and a community change process
used in 700 cities in the United States and Canada. The
Community Protective Factors framework of developmental assets (see Table 16.1) is a
• Opportunities for prosocial community involvement theory-based model linking features of ecologies (exter-
• Rewards for prosocial community involvement nal assets) with personal skills and capacities (internal
assets), guided by the hypothesis that external and inter-
School Protective Factors nal assts are dynamically interconnected “ building
blocks” that, in combination, prevent high risk health
• Opportunities for prosocial school involvement
behaviors and enhance many forms of developmental
• Rewards for prosocial school involvement success (i.e., thriving).
Family Protective Factors As described in a series of publications (Benson,
1997, 2002; Benson et al., 1998), the framework estab-
• Opportunities for prosocial family involvement lishes a set of developmental experiences and supports
• Rewards for prosocial family involvement that are hypothesized to have import for all young people
• Family attachment during the 2nd decade of life. However, it has also been
hypothesized that developmental assets reflect develop-
Peer and Individual Protective Factors mental processes that have age-related parallels in in-
• Religiosity fancy and childhood (Leffert, Benson, & Roehlkepartain,
• Belief in a moral order 1997; Mannes, Benson, Kretzmann, & Norris, 2003;
Scales, Sesma, & Bolstrom, 2004).
• Social Skills
The framework synthesizes research in a number of
• Prosocial Peer Attachment fields with the goal of selecting for inclusion those de-
• Resilient Temperament velopmental resources that: (a) have been demonstrated
• Sociability to prevent high risk behavior (e.g., substance use, vio-
lence, dropping out of school), enhance thriving, or build
Within the community of scholars self-identifying as resilience; ( b) have evidence of generalizability across
youth development researchers, considerable attention social locations; (c) contribute balance to the overall
has been given to defining and conceptualizing develop- framework (i.e., of ecological and individual-level fac-
ment-enhancing processes, with a growing number of tors); and (d) for which it can be demonstrated that com-
publications dedicated to synthesizing the many frame- munities have the capacity to effect their acquisition.
works (Benson & Saito, 2001; NRCIM, 2002; Roth & Because the model, in addition to its theoretical and re-
Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Small & Memmo, 2004). Several search purposes, “is also intended to have practical signif-
publications have been influential in guiding practice icance for mobilizing communities” (Benson, 2002,
and policy. Pittman and her colleagues (Pittman, Irby, & p. 127), the assets are placed in categories that have con-
Ferber, 2000; Pittman, Irby, Tolman, Yohalem, & Fer- ceptual integrity and that can be described easily to the
ber, 2001) identified seven essential developmental re- people of a community. They are grouped into 20 external
sources: stable programs; basic care and services; assets ( health-promoting features of the environment) and
healthy relationships with peers and adults; high expec- 20 that are internal (skills, values, competencies, and self-
tations and standards; role models, resources and net- perceptions). The external assets are grouped into four
works; challenging experiences and opportunities to categories: (1) support, (2) empowerment, (3) boundaries
participate and contribute; and high-quality instruction and expectations, and (4) constructive use of time. The
TABLE 16.1 The Framework of Developmental Assets

Category External Assets Definition

1. Family support Family life provides high levels of love and support.
2. Positive family communication Young person and her or his parent(s) communicate positively, and
young person is willing to seek advice and counsel from parents.
3. Other adult relationships Young person receives support from three or more nonparent
Support adults.
4. Caring neighborhood Young person experiences caring neighbors.
5. Caring school climate School provides a caring, encouraging environment.
6. Parent involvement in schooling Parent(s) are actively involved in helping young person succeed in
school.
7. Community values youth Young person perceives that adults in the community value youth.
8. Youth as resources Young people are given useful roles in the community.
Empowerment
9. Service to others Young person serves in the community one hour or more per week.
10. Safety Young person feels safe at home, school, and in the neighborhood.
11. Family boundaries Family has clear rules and consequences and monitors the young
person’s whereabouts.
12. School boundaries School provides clear rules and consequences.
13. Neighborhood boundaries Neighbors take responsibility for monitoring young people’s
Boundaries and expectations behavior.
14. Adult role models Parent(s) and other adults model positive, responsible behavior.
15. Positive peer inf luence Young person’s best friends model responsible behavior.
16. High expectations Both parent(s) and teachers encourage the young person to do
well.

Category Internal Assets Definition

21. Achievement motivation Young person is motivated to do well in school.


22. School engagement Young person is actively engaged in learning.

Commitment to learning 23. Homework Young person reports doing at least one hour of homework every
school day.
24. Bonding to school Young person cares about her or his school.
25. Reading for pleasure Young person reads for pleasure three or more hours per week.
26. Caring Young person places high value on helping other people.
27. Equality and social justice Young person places high value on promoting equality and
reducing hunger and poverty.
28. Integrity Young person acts on convictions and stands up for her or his
Positive values beliefs.
29. Honesty Young person “ tells the truth even when it is not easy.”
30. Responsibility Young person accepts and takes personal responsibility.
31. Restraint Young person believes it is important not to be sexually active or to
use alcohol or other drugs.
32. Planning and decision making Young person knows how to plan ahead and make choices.
33. Interpersonal competence Young person has empathy, sensitivity, and friendship skills.
34. Cultural competence Young person has knowledge of and comfort with people of
Social competencies different cultural /racial /ethnic backgrounds.
35. Resistance skills Young person can resist negative peer pressure and dangerous
situations.
36. Peaceful conf lict resolution Young person seeks to resolve conf lict nonviolently.

(continued)

907
908 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

TABLE 16.1 Continued

Category External Assets Definition

37. Personal power Young person feels he or she has control over “ things that happen
to me.”
38. Self-esteem Young person reports having high self-esteem.
Positive identity
39. Sense of purpose Young person reports that “my life has a purpose.”
40. Positive view of personal future Young person is optimistic about her or his personal future.

Source: From All Kids Are Our Kids: What Communities Must Do to Raise Caring and Responsible Children and Adolescents, by P. Benson,
1997, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

internal assets are placed in four categories: (1) commit- chapter. The first has to do with the additive or cumula-
ment to learning, (2) positive values, (3) social competen- tive nature of the elements called assets. The assump-
cies, and (4) positive identity. The scientific foundations tion is that “ the more assets, the better.” The National
for the eight categories and each of the 40 assets are de- Research Council Report frames it this way: “adoles-
scribed in a series of publications (Scales & Leffert, cents with more personal and social assets . . . have a
1999, 2004; Scales et al., 2004). greater chance of both current well-being and future
The 2002 report from The National Research Council success” (NRCIM, 2002, p. 42). Benson and his col-
and Institute of Medicine, Community Programs to Pro- leagues (Benson, 2003a; Benson et al., 1998; Benson,
mote Positive youth Development (NRCIM, 2002), used Scales, & Mannes, 2003) refer to the longitudinal ex-
the concept of assets to describe the experiences, pression of this principle as the “ vertical pile up” of as-
supports, and opportunities “ which facilitate both suc- sets. Both streams of thought also contend that this
cessful passage through” adolescence and “optimal tran- principle of accumulated assets generalizes to multiple
sition into the next phase of life—adulthood” (p. 67). forms of behavior—from prevention of high risk behav-
Parallel to Search Institute’s distinction between exter- ior to the enhancement of positive outcomes such as
nal and internal assets, this national report used the lan- school success (Benson et al., 2003; NRCIM, 2002;
guage of “personal” and “social” assets. The authors Scales & Roehlkepartain, 2003).
used three types of empirical studies to identify assets: Closely related is the idea of the “pile up” of sup-
“studies linking the personal and social assets to indica- portive contexts. That is, positive development is also
tors of positive current development, studies linking enhanced when many settings collaborate—whether in-
these characteristics to indicators of future positive tentional or not—in generating the kinds of supports
adult development, and experimental studies designed to and opportunities known to promote assets. In the
change the asset under study” (p. 82). words of the National Research Council (2002),
The committee of scholars charged with creating this
Research shows that the more settings that adolescents ex-
report then identified 28 personal and social assets. Un-
perience reflecting these features, the more likely they
like Search Institute’s developmental asset taxonomy, are to acquire the personal and social assets linked to both
the 28 indicators are all personological in nature and do current and future well-being. (p. 43)
not include the same balance of contextual factors and
individual-level factors. Nonetheless, there is consider- Scales and Roehlkepartain (2004) have recently called
able overlap between the two taxonomies. Table 16.2 this the principle of “ horizontal pile up.” This concept is
displays the NRCIM taxonomy of personal and social as- similar to the idea of developmental redundancy
sets. It should be noted, however, that the committee (Benson, 1997; Benson et al., 1998). Recent work in the
also created a conceptual model of the “ features of pos- sociology of adolescence also speaks to this dynamic
itive developmental settings.” These provide some paral- (Furstenberg, 2000).
lel thinking to the concept of external assets. These A second hypothesis addresses the nature of assets
“ features” will be discussed in the next section. as relevant universally, although often experienced or
Embedded in both the developmental asset model and expressed differently across diversities. Among youth
the National Research Council report are three explicit development scholars, it is commonly assumed that
hypotheses, each of which will be evaluated later in this the elements in the conceptual models of nutrients/
The Theory of Positive Youth Development 909

TABLE 16.2 Personal and Social Assets That Facilitate Positive


Youth Development
Physical development:
–Good health habits
–Good health risk management skills
Intellectual development:
–Knowledge of essential life skills
–Knowledge of essential vocational skills
–School success
–Rational habits of mind—critical thinking and reasoning skills
–In-depth knowledge of more than one culture
–Good decision-making skills
–Knowledge of skills needed to navigate through multiple cultural
contexts
Psychological and emotional development:
–Good mental health including positive self-regard
–Good emotional self-regulation skills
–Good coping skills
–Good conf lict resolution skills
–Mastery motivation and positive achievement motivation
–Confidence in one’s personal efficacy
–“Planfulness”—planning for the future and future life events
–Sense of personal autonomy/responsibility for self
–Optimism coupled with realism
–Coherent and positive personal and social identity
–Prosocial and culturally sensitive values
–Spirituality or a sense of a “larger ” purpose in life
–Strong moral character
–A commitment to good use of time
Social development:
–Connectedness—perceived good relationships and trust with
parents, peers, and some other adults
–Sense of social plane/integration—being connected and valued by
larger social networks
–Attachment to prosocial /conventional institutions, such as school,
church, nonschool youth programs
–Ability to navigate in multiple cultural contexts
–Commitment to civic engagement
Source: From Community Programs to Promote Youth Development: Com-
mittee on Community-Level Programs for Youth, by the National Research
Council and Institute of Medicine, J. Eccles and J. A. Gootman (Eds),
Board on Children, Youth and Families, Division of Behavioral and Social
Sciences and Education, 2002, Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

resources/assets have currency for youth in all social lo- dom and capacity of people within particular racial, eth-
cations. This claim is particularly clear in both the Na- nic, religious, and economic groups (Hamilton et al.,
tional Research Council report and the research 2004).
undergirding the developmental asset model. At the same The third assumption is one that arguably is the
time, however, both models testify to the diversity of strongest point of theoretical consensus across scholars,
methods and procedures for promoting assets, and to the research programs, and practitioners within the positive
importance of creating strategies of asset-building that youth development field. This is the belief that assets
are crafted with deep sensitivity to the experience, wis- are enhanced when contexts and settings are configured
910 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

and organized in specific ways. Context matters and


contexts can be changed. This principle can be suc-
cinctly stated as: Theory of
Human
personal and social assets are enhanced by positive devel- Development
opmental settings. (NRCIM, 2002, p. 43)

Not surprisingly, then, there is a considerable re- Theory of Theory of


search tradition on how, and under what conditions, Context and Context and
Community Community
contexts and ecologies promote positive development. Influence Change
This body of work shifts the unit of analysis from the
person to contexts, environments, and communities.
Accordingly, it draws us into a number of fields beyond
developmental psychology in which such inquiry is Figure 16.2 A comprehensive theory of positive youth
development.
more at home. We suggest that a theory of person, con-
text, and their intersection such as suggested earlier
in this chapter is a necessary but not sufficient set
of ideas for delineating the territory, scope, and such concepts as youth leadership, service learning,
uniqueness of positive youth development. The major youth empowerment, and youth engagement.
lacuna in our discussion to this point is the idea of in-
tentional change. At the heart of positive youth devel- A comprehensive approach to positive youth-develop-
opment thinking and research is the question of how ment requires the integration of three theories: of human
the healthy/ balanced/adaptive fusion of person and development (which is the primary focus of this chap-
context can be enhanced. It is this idea—this possibil- ter), of context and community influence, and of how
ity of creating change—that has fueled practice for contexts and communities change. These three are dis-
several decades and, more recently, is fueling research played in Figure 16.2.
and policy.
A theory of positive youth development, then, is in- The Theory of Context and Community Inf luence
complete without incorporating the concept of inten-
tional change. Without doing so, we have a theory of There is an extensive and growing literature on the fea-
adolescent development—not positive youth develop- tures and dynamics of developmentally supportive con-
ment. Intentional change is the purposeful effort to en- texts. It is here that we reference the major contributions
hance the fusion of person and context in a healthy of Bronfenbrenner (1979; Bronfenbrenner & Morris,
direction. Because of the dynamic bidirectionality of 1998). His ecological theory of development has been
this interaction, there are three major points of potential instrumental in shaping the theory, research, and prac-
intervention. The three of these, in combination, in- tice of positive youth development. If we were to posit
crease the probability of adaptive developmental regula- the canon of youth development, the list would begin
tion. These are: with The Ecology of Human Development (1979). In this
work, he provides a highly influential definition that not
1. Increasing the developmental-attentiveness of contexts only supports a critical notion in current developmental
(to increase their capacity to nurture, support, and systems theory but also shaped a generation of scholar-
constructively challenge the developing person). ship. In his words:
2. Enhancing the skills and competencies of youth (to fur-
The ecology of human development involves the scientific
ther enable their “natural” capacity to engage with,
study of progressive, mutual accommodation between an
connect, change, and learn from their social contexts).
active, growing human being and the changing properties
3. Creating processes and opportunities to invite youth to of the immediate settings in which the developing person
actively exercise and utilize their capacity to engage lives, as this process is affected by relations between
with and change their social contexts. In practice and these settings, and by the larger contexts in which the set-
research, this form of intentional change travels under tings are embedded. (p. 21)
The Theory of Positive Youth Development 911

Among Bronfenbrenner’s many contributions is his teracts with “people, objects and symbols” in what they
conceptual formulation of the nature and dynamics of call “proximal processes,” which are “ the primary en-
developmental contexts. He portrays the nested systems gines of development ” (p. 996). Caring relationships are
that influence development as interdependent; their in- critical, not only in the family, but in all the settings youth
fluence is interactive; none stands or has its effects occupy. Likewise, youth need a range of challenging ac-
alone. What happens in a microsystem, such as a class- tivities in multiple settings. Both the people and the activ-
room, is influenced by tax policies and by the media, but ities foster development best when they provide an
those elements of the macrosystem are themselves inter- optimum balance of challenge and support. According to
preted through and influenced by microsystems. An Bronfenbrenner and Morris, the most powerful activities
important implication for youth development is that ef- and relationships are predictable and enduring.
fective efforts to enhance assets must change more than The classic account of how relationships promote de-
one system and level of system. Changing schools or velopment and learning is Vygotsky’s (1978) “zone of
even families will be less effective than changing multi- proximal development.” According to Vygotsky, devel-
ple systems (or settings). opment, in the sense of growing competence, occurs
Wynn (1997) and her colleagues conceive of the com- when the developing person is assisted by someone who
munity institutions influencing youth development as is already competent in accomplishing tasks she or he
“sectors” and focus on “primary supports” as a strong cannot do unaided. With experience, this assistance be-
but under-appreciated influence. Primary supports are comes unnecessary and the person can perform indepen-
voluntary; youth choose to participate and make choices dently. Several cognitive scientists have elaborated this
about what they will do and how. Primary supports af- notion, using the metaphor of “scaffolding” that is grad-
ford young people opportunities to take initiative and to ually withdrawn (e.g., Bruner, 1983; Rogoff, 1990). Al-
participate actively, in contrast to the passivity charac- though the metaphor is faulty (implying that scaffolding
terizing the role of student. Exemplars of primary sup- holds up a building until it is capable of standing on its
ports include “arts and after school programs; organized own), the idea is sound: the assistance of someone who
sports; community service and youth entrepreneurship is more advanced enables youth to gain competence, es-
opportunities; and the offerings of parks, libraries, mu- pecially if that person is skilled at knowing when to help
seums, and community centers” (p. 1). and when not to. Bronfenbrenner (1979), acknowledging
Consistent with Bronfenbrenner’s idea of the impor- Vygotsky, hypothesized that:
tance of links among systems, Wynn (1997) claims that
primary supports function best when they reinforce and Learning and development are facilitated by the participa-
link other sectors, especially families, schools, health- tion of the developing person in progressively more com-
care, and other services. Critical to effective primary plex patterns of reciprocal activity with someone with
supports are: high expectations; group problem solving; whom that person has developed a strong and enduring
concrete products and performances; prospects for ad- emotional attachment and when the balance of power
vancement and expanded opportunities; adults acting as gradually shifts in favor of the developing person. (p. 60)
caregivers, catalysts and coaches; membership; avail-
ability and continuity; respect and reciprocity; and adult Benson et al. (2003) enumerated five aspects of rela-
investment (pp. 5–7). tionships germane to positive youth development. First,
There are a growing number of such conceptual models supportive relationships with both immediate and ex-
for identifying developmental contexts that are potential tended family members have been shown, in multiple
sources for positive youth development (see, e.g., Benson studies and multiple demographic settings, to enhance
& Saito, 2001; Benson et al., 2003; Gambone, Klem, & developmental strengths and provide a protective buffer
Connell, 2002; Hamilton & Hamilton, 2004; Pittman against risk (Rhodes & Roffman, 2003). Second, sup-
et al., 2000). An important line of theory and research is portive relationships with nonparental adults can be
also emerging to explain how, and under what conditions, equally compelling in advancing positive development,
such contexts inform positive development. Several particularly during adolescence (Scales, Benson, &
themes are particularly central to positive youth develop- Mannes, 2002; Scales & Leffert, 2004; Scales, Leffert,
ment theory. Bronfenbrenner and Morris (1998) identify & Vraa, 2003). Third, the number of supportive adult
two of these themes. Development occurs as a person in- relationships may provide an additive impact: As the
912 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

number of nurturing relationships increase, probabili- challenging activities that reward them with a sense of
ties for the presence of developmental strengths such as successfully negotiating challenges that require intense
caring values, self-esteem, and a positive view of one’s concentration. This work helps to explain why some ac-
future also may increase (Benson, 1997). An additional tivities contribute more to building youths’ assets than
axiom about nonparental adults has to do with the sus- others. Activities such as playing chess, playing a musi-
tainability of relationships. It is reasonable to hypothe- cal instrument, or planning and carrying out a commu-
size that the strength-building capacity of nonparental nity service project build developmental assets more
adult connections increases proportionately with the than watching television or gossiping with friends.
length of the relationship. In another important statement of how activity con-
Fourth, exposure to positive peer influence—de- tributes to positive development, Larson (2000) posits
fined, for example, as peer modeling of prosocial and that the development of initiative is critical. Combining
achievement values—can both advance developmental intrinsic motivation and deep attention, initiative can
strengths and inhibit risk behaviors (Leffert et al., 1998; emerge from well-designed structured activities within
Scales, Benson, et al., 2000). Finally, the developmental sports, arts, and related youth development programs.
advantage of relationships is enhanced by three factors: The themes of relationships and developmentally ap-
their quality, their quantity, and their sustainability. propriate activity are “ front and center ” in most concep-
The second theme identified by Bronfenbrenner and tual models seeking to describe the essential features of
Morris (1998) has to do with the importance and cer- positive developmental contexts (Gambone & Arbreton,
tainty of activity. Csikszentmihalyi (1990) has com- 1997; McLaughlin, Irby, & Langman, 1994; Quinn,
pellingly made the case that certain kinds of activities 1999; Roth & Brooks-Gunn, 2000, 2003). In a synthesis
instigate development in his work on “ flow” or “ the psy- of this research, NRCIM (2002) identified eight features
chology of optimal performance.” Csikszentmihalyi has of programs, hypothesized to “expand the opportunities
documented the phenomenon of flow in people like rock for youth to acquire personal and social assets” (p. 8).
climbers, dancers, and others who engage in highly These are listed in Table 16.3. As noted earlier, these

TABLE 16.3 Features of Positive Developmental Settings


Feature Descriptors
Physical and Safe and health-promoting facilities, practice that increases safe peer group interaction and decreases unsafe
psychological safety or confrontational peer interactions
Appropriate structure Limit setting, clear and consistent rules and expectations, firm-enough control, continuity and predictability,
clear boundaries, and age-appropriate monitoring
Supportive relationships Warmth, closeness, connectedness, good communication, caring, support, guidance, secure attachment, and
responsiveness.
Opportunities to belong Opportunities for meaningful inclusion, regardless of one’s gender, ethnicity, sexual orientation or
disabilities; social inclusion, social engagement and integration; opportunities for sociocultural identity
formation; and support for cultural and bicultural competence
Positive social norms Rules of behavior, expectations, injunctions, ways of doing things, values and morals, and obligations for
service
Support for efficacy Youth-based, empowerment practices that support autonomy, making a real difference in one’s community,
and mattering and being taken seriously; practices that include enabling, responsibility granting, and meaningful challenge;
practices that focus on improvement rather than on relative current performance levels
Opportunity for Opportunities to learn physical, intellectual, psychological, emotional, and social skills; exposure to
skill building intentional learning experiences; opportunities to learn cultural literacies, media literacy, communication
skills, and good habits of mind; preparation for adult employment and opportunities to develop social and
cultural capital
Integration of family, school, Concordance; coordination and synergy among family, school and community.
and community efforts
Source: From Community Programs to Promote Youth Development: Committee on Community-Level Programs for Youth, by the National Re-
search Council and Institute of Medicine, J. Eccles and J. A. Gootman (Eds), Board on Children, Youth and Families, Division of Behavioral
and Social Sciences and Education, 2002, Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
The Theory of Positive Youth Development 913

eight features of positive development settings have vices delivery systems (Dorgan & Ferguson, 2004; Dry-
some conceptual overlap with the external assets in the foos, 1990; Mannes et al., 2003).
developmental asset framework (Benson, 1997; Benson The construct of social capital elucidates why com-
et al., 2003; Scales & Leffert, 1999, 2004). munity mobilization is important and points to some av-
The theory of positive youth development posits that enues for action. Coleman (1990, p. 304) describes
development is enhanced when contexts are configured social capital as contained in human relationships.
and organized in ways consonant with these develop- Human capital includes a person’s competencies. Just
mental principles. As already suggested, closely like human capital and financial capital, social capital
aligned with the “contexts can be changed” axiom is makes it possible for people to be productive, to accom-
the principle of “ horizontal pile-up.” This latter con- plish tasks. Coleman points out that social capital is
cept refers to the reinforcing, simultaneous experience greater in social networks with a high degree of “clo-
of ecological assets across the different context of a sure,” meaning that many people know each other, com-
young person’s total ecology, such as family, neighbor- municate, and trust each other (pp. 319–320).
hood, school, peer group, after-school programs, and Sampson and his colleagues (Sampson, 2001; Samp-
other co-curricular organizations. As suggested by son, Morenoff, & Earls, 1999; Sampson, Raudenbush, &
Benson et al. (2003): Earls, 1997) have identified community mechanisms
that facilitate the generation of social capital. Chief
Such multiple and redundant exposure to developmentally among these is the idea of collective efficacy, which sig-
rich ecologies fortifies the social space within which
nifies “an emphasis on shared beliefs in a neighbor-
young people can perceive themselves to be safe, sup-
hood’s conjoint capability for action to achieve an
ported and capable. Young people who experience such re-
dundancy ought to be even more likely than young people
intended effect, and hence an active sense of engage-
without such a horizontal pile-up of assets to enjoy protec- ment on the part of residents” (Sampson, 2001, p. 10).
tion from risk and to thrive. (p. 387) Benson and his colleagues (Benson, 1997; Benson et al.,
1998) have suggested that one important source of col-
This idea of “developmental redundancy” helps to lective efficacy is a shared community vocabulary of
fuel an additional and important concept in positive developmental assets aligned with a publicly shared un-
youth development: the viability of community as a set- derstanding of the capacity of social contexts to effect
ting for generating both ecological and internal assets. their acquisition.
This question of how communities inform development
has become a vibrant area of inquiry (Benson et al.,
The Theory of Context and Community Change
1998; Blyth, 2001; Booth & Crouter, 2001; Comer,
1980; Comer, Haynes, Joyner, & Ben-Avie, 1996; Con- The third formulation in a comprehensive theory of pos-
nell et al., 2001; Earls & Carlson, 2001; Hughes & Cur- itive youth-development focuses on the processes,
nan, 2000; Kretzmann & McKnight, 1993; Mannes strategies, and tactics that can directly or indirectly alter
et al., 2003; Sampson, 2001; Spencer, 2001). contexts and community. This is the least developed of
Using community as a unit of analysis, researchers the three theoretical foundations of the theory we envi-
have posited a number of community processes and dy- sion. One recent review of the science on “ how change
namics inferred to be important for creating the kinds of occurs” has argued that a compelling question emerging
relationships and developmentally rich contexts that from new discoveries about the dynamic and bidirec-
promote positive development. Scales and his colleagues tional sources of positive development has to do with:
(Scales et al., 2001, 2002, 2003) identify pro-child so-
the processes and procedures of increasing access to de-
cial norms in which engagement with children and ado-
velopmental nutrients/assets on a rather massive scale.
lescents is expected and supported. Some theorists posit
And truth be told, though all architects of developmental
the viability of shared ideals and expectations that unite nutrient models are deeply interested in application, the
multiple socializing systems in common purpose (Ben- science of how change occurs is in its infancy. We have in-
son, 1997; Damon, 1997). Zeldin (2002) points to the vested much more intellectual and research energy in
role of adults’ sense of community as an important pre- naming the positive building blocks of development and
cursor to engagement with youth. And several identify demonstrating their predictive utility for enhancing health
the role of strategic alignment among community ser- and academic outcomes than in studying the complex
914 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

array of strategies and procedures for moving the develop- velopmentally attentive community is also characterized
mental needle forward. (Benson, 2003b, p. 214) by indirect influences that support and sustain these
more direct resident and sector influences. These influ-
Thinking about such change is a complex enterprise. ences include policy, financial resources, and social
Because positive youth-development has a pronounced norms that promote adult engagement with the young
interest in application, a comprehensive theory of change (Scales et al., 2001, 2003).
is needed to guide both research and the change-making In turn, Benson et al. (2003) propose that the strate-
efforts already underway in hundreds of communities, gic targets for such communities are vertical pileup (in
organizations, and systems. Tying this theory and re- which youth develop many developmental assets), hori-
search agenda to the previous section on context and zontal pileup (in which youth experience asset-building
community influences suggests some of the concepts hy- in multiple contexts), and developmental breadth (ex-
pothesized to be central to this inquiry. Among these are tending, by purpose and design, the reach of asset-build-
building shared vision; activating collective and per- ing energy to all children and adolescents, not only those
sonal efficacy; promoting social trust; reframing how judged to be at “risk ” and served by traditional “preven-
citizens view youth; mobilizing adult-youth relation- tion” programs).
ships; creating effective cross-sector collaborations; and Accordingly, the five synergistic strategies they posit
enhancing relationships and developmentally appropri- for community change are:
ate activities within socializing systems and programs.
1. Engage adults: Community adults build sustained,
Many points of entry into this complex arena of
asset-building relationships with children and youth,
change have been proposed. Among these are social
both within and beyond family.
policy (Blum & Ellen, 2002; Halfon, 2003); social
norms (Scales et al., 2003); community building 2. Mobilize youth engagement: Adolescents use their
(Hyman, 2002; Mannes et al., 2003); schools (Gambone asset-building capacities with peers and with younger
et al., 2002); neighborhoods (Sampson, 2001); families children and in activities that help enhance the qual-
(Simpson & Roehlkepartain, 2003); the mobilization of ity of their community.
adults as change activists (Rhodes & Roffman, 2003); 3. Activate sectors: Families, neighborhoods, schools,
and the mobilization of youth as change activists (Earls congregations, and youth organizations activate their
& Carlson, 2002). asset-building potential.
Recently, two conceptual frames have been proposed 4. Invigorate programs: A community infrastructure of
to help guide theory and research on change. First, quality early childhood, after-school, weekend, and
Granger (2002) suggested two overarching constructs: summer programs is available and used by children
intervention strategies to enhance the will to change and and youth.
intervention strategies to enhance the capacity to 5. Inf luence civic decisions: Financial, leadership, media,
change. For the latter, he posits five key strategies: and policy resources are mobilized to support and sus-
human capital creation, redistribution strategies, in- tain the transformation needed for areas 1, 2, 3, and 4
vestment strategies, social capital creation, and effi- to emerge.
ciency strategies.
Second, Benson et al. (2003) proposed five interlock-
ing spheres of intervention. Grounded in organizational RESEARCH SUPPORT FOR KEY POSITIVE
systems theory, this model suggests that change in any YOUTH DEVELOPMENT HYPOTHESES
one sphere impacts each of the others. This assertion
bears theoretical affinity with core tenets in develop- The theory and practice of positive youth development
mental systems theory. This five-fold model is in the suggests several of key hypotheses. Later in this section,
service, theoretically, of creating a “developmentally at- we introduce and examine empirical support for seven
tentive community” (p. 389). Such a community is hypotheses, and offer perspectives on the implications
envisioned as one that marshals and activates the asset- of these principles both for understanding and promot-
building capacity of its residents ( both adults and ing positive youth development. Here, however, it is im-
youth), and sectors (family, neighborhoods, schools, portant to provide an overview of the nature and power
youth organizations, laces of work, congregations). A de- of the research base pertinent to these hypotheses.
Research Support for Key Positive Youth Development Hypotheses 915

Overview of Positive Youth Development Research 2003) for the broad theoretical connection between de-
velopmental assets and developmental outcomes, both
The research base supporting these hypotheses is plen-
concurrently and longitudinally. This is especially true
tiful, although uneven. The literature measuring devel-
when considering as an independent variable the cumu-
opmental resources is typified by variable-centered
lative number of assets young people experience, or
methods, a focus on isolated variables, use of cross-sec-
comparing those young people with relatively higher
tional samples, and linear-additive theory and analytic
and lower levels of assets.
strategies. What is needed are person-centered meth-
There is relative persuasiveness and consistency of
ods, a focus on patterns or clusters of variables, use of
positive findings in the literature on the explanatory
longitudinal samples, and dynamic nonlinear theory
power of positive youth development concepts. But
and analytic strategies (Lerner, Lerner, De Stefanis, &
what level of explanation is reasonable to expect devel-
Apfel, 2001).
opmental assets or nutrients to provide for complex
Developmental outcomes for youth also encompass outcomes? Luthar et al. (2000), for example, observe
processes that are as important as if not more important that studies whose findings rest on main effects often
than outcomes reflecting status points in time (e.g., cur- report effects of 10% to 20% for individual protective
rent use of alcohol, how much community service one factors. When interaction effects are necessary to ex-
contributes). Processes include reorganization (Sroufe, plain the workings of such assets, effect sizes are far
1979), being able to permanently make transitions smaller, in the 2% to 5% range. With both advocacy
(Baltes & Freund, 2003), and being on a path to a hope- and empirical work in recent years reflecting a shift
ful future (Lerner et al., 2002; Scales & Benson, 2004). from merely documenting the impact of developmental
Status outcomes may not adequately capture the nested nutrients to studying the processes and interactions
interactions of person and contexts over time, for exam- that suggest how those nutrients contribute to out-
ple, person-family and family community (Lerner, Fre- comes (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Heatherington, &
und, De Stefanis, & Habermas, 2001).
Bornstein, 2000; Davey, Eaker, & Walters, 2003;
Further, the literature says relatively little about the
Luthar et al., 2000), it may be expected that the size of
interaction of the combination of nutrients or resources
many reported effects will be disappointingly, but un-
young people experience. Most studies focus on just a
derstandably, limited.
handful of assets (especially parental /family assets and Ecological and developmental systems theory have
school orientation assets, with some emphasis on peers, become the predominant frames of theoretical reference
and more recently, on extracurricular and positive youth for the study of child and adolescent development
development program activities), and at best, how this (Lerner et al., 2002). Moreover, individual development
handful may interact. and broader community and social change processes in-
We illuminate the research support for the positive creasingly are linked in positive youth development
youth development hypotheses by focusing on a small frameworks (Benson et al., 1998, 2003; Connell & Ku-
number of outcomes for which positive youth develop- bisch, 2001; Hawkins & Catalano, 1996). These theoret-
ment theory is best explicated, and that appear to have ical formulations imply that effects derived from
strong research bases and broad constituencies of re- studies shaped by those theories and frameworks may be
searchers, practitioners, and policymakers dealing with quite modest, a conclusion supported in a recent review
them: Alcohol and other drug use; violence/anti-social by Wandersman and Florin (2003). All these factors
behavior; school success; and civic engagement. Much make it quite challenging scientifically to capture broad
but not all of the research cited herein pertains to those community change in the service of positive youth de-
four exemplar outcomes. velopment (Berkowitz, 2001).
With the preceding comments providing perspective
on the state-of-the-art in positive youth development re-
How Much Explanatory Power Is
search, we turn now to illustrating the evidence for each
Reasonable to Expect?
of the major positive youth development hypotheses, we
Hundreds of studies, cited in this chapter and in compre- can derive from our prior discussion of the theoretical
hensive reviews (Scales & Leffert, 2004; Scales et al., and practitioner bases of the concept of positive youth
2004), provide persuasive evidence (Miller & Thoresen, development.
916 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Hypothesis One 25 programs demonstrated significant effects on posi-


tive youth development outcomes, including improve-
The first hypothesis is termed the contextual change hy-
ments in interpersonal skills, quality of peer and adult
pothesis, and consists of two assumptions. First, contexts
relationships, self-control, problem solving, cognitive
can be intentionally altered to enhance developmental
competence, self-efficacy, commitment to school, and
success. And second, changes in these contexts change
academic achievement. In addition, 24 of the 25 showed
the person.
significant reductions in problem behaviors such as alco-
There is abundant evidence that ecological contexts hol and other drug use, school problems, aggressive be-
can be changed to promote positive youth development, havior, violence, and risky sexual behavior.
as well as a wealth of data about why such approaches In a review of more than 1,200 studies of outcomes in
have those positive effects. In most of this research, re- prevention programs for children and adolescents,
searchers have documented (usually, but not always) the Durlak (1998) identified eight common protective fac-
efficacy of intervention or prevention programs in pro- tors across programs successful in preventing behavior
viding youth with experiences that facilitate develop- problems, school failure, poor physical health, and preg-
mental outcomes. For example, from their review of 60 nancy among young people: Social support; personal
evaluations of youth development programs, Roth, and social skills; self-efficacy; good parent-child rela-
Brooks-Gunn, Murray, and Foster (1998) concluded: tionships; positive peer modeling; high quality schools;
effective social policies; and positive social norms. The
[Y]outh development programs are best characterized by resilience literature also suggests from the finding of
their approach to youth as resources to be developed rather “synchronous evidence” from multiple studies using dif-
than as problems to be managed, and their efforts to help fering measurements, that there are three critical kinds
youth become healthy, happy, and productive by increasing of protective factors: Close relationships with caring,
youths’ exposure to the external assets, opportunities and
supportive adults, often in primary care-giving roles; ef-
supports. (p. 427)
fective schools; and positive relationships with proso-
cial adults in the wider community (Luthar et al., 2000).
The Social Development Research Group at the Uni- In a meta-analysis of 177 primary prevention pro-
versity of Washington conducted one of the most wide- grams designed to prevent behavioral and mental health
ranging reviews of positive youth development programs problems among children and adolescents, Durlak and
(Catalano et al., 2004). They identified 161 programs Wells (1997) reported that most kinds of primary pre-
and discussed in detail 25 that were well-evaluated and vention programs (whether person- or environment-cen-
showed significant effects on behavioral outcomes. The tered, and whether universal or targeted) contributed
programs had to have one or more of the following ob- both to reducing problems and increasing competencies.
jectives about building developmental assets or nutri- However, only 15% of these programs attempted to
ents: Promote bonding; foster resilience; promote social change children’s environments, despite the emphasis of
competence; promote emotional competence; promote context in the major developmental systems and ecologi-
cognitive competence; promote behavioral competence; cal theories that are the foundation of the positive youth
promote moral competence; foster self-determination; development field.
foster spirituality; foster self-efficacy; foster clear and Developmental theories suggest that, because of the
positive identity; foster belief in the future; provide fusion of person and context, variations or alterations in
recognition for positive behavior; provide opportunities developmental context should be associated with varia-
for prosocial involvement; and foster prosocial norms. In tions or alterations in developmental outcomes. For ex-
addition, the programs had to address either multiple as- ample, theories regarding the development of anti-social
sets, or a single nutrient but across the multiple social behavior and violence typically posit several differing
domains of family, school, or community. Programs that trajectories. Children who are chronically high in anti-
addressed only a single asset in a single domain were ex- social behavior from childhood through adolescence, for
cluded. Competence, self-efficacy, and prosocial norms example, are seen as having biological or genetic vulner-
were addressed in all 25 programs, and most programs abilities that manifest themselves in attention and con-
dealt with at least 8 of the 15 nutrients. Most programs centration problems, which are associated both with
used positive outcome measures as well as reduction of early school failure and peer rejection (Moffitt, 1993).
problem behavior in their evaluations. Nineteen of the Poor parenting may also contribute to this pathway.
Research Support for Key Positive Youth Development Hypotheses 917

Those developing higher levels of antisocial behavior and if they were connected to youth organizations and
later in childhood or adolescence are seen as being in- religious institutions. That is, service was less an indi-
fluenced more by association with deviant peers vidual and spontaneous act and more the result of a web
(Dishion, Andrews, & Crosby, 1995; Patterson, Reid, & of asset-building relationships and norms that together
Dishion, 1992). A study of several hundred urban, elevated service to a shared social expectation.
mostly African American males followed from first One of the most impressive studies illustrating the
grade through seventh grade found evidence supporting power of changed contexts on personal change and de-
such differing pathways (Schaeffer, Petras, Ialongo, Po- velopmental success is the evaluation of Big Broth-
duska, & Kellam, 2003). Theoretically then, it is plausi- ers/ Big Sisters conducted by Public/ Private Ventures
ble that early efforts to improve family related assets, (Tierney, Grossman, & Resch, 1995). The investigators
social competencies, and school success all could have fashioned a true experiment by randomly assigning half
an ameliorative effect on the development of antisocial of those awaiting placement to a delayed-treatment con-
behavior trajectories. Indeed, Furlong, Paige, and Osher trol group while seeking placements for the other half.
(2003) note such evidence findings linking violence pre- Those in the treatment group demonstrated several ad-
vention with children’s connection to caring adults, so- vantages over the control group, including lower likeli-
cial /emotional skills, and appropriate instruction and hood of beginning to use drugs and alcohol or to have
academic supports that promote a sense of competence hit another person, along with better attitudes toward
and school success. school, better grades, and attendance. In addition, they
Similarly, in a study of school success, Gutman, reported improved relations with family and peers. The
Sameroff, and Eccles (2002) showed that developmental causal pathway of mentoring’s effects on school perfor-
assets may have both a promotive ( helpful for all youth) mance appears to have been through improved relations
and a protective function ( helpful for some youth under with parents (Rhodes, Grossman, & Resch, 2000).
conditions of risk). In their study of more than 800 In summary, intentional efforts to change contexts
seventh-grade African American students, these re- to improve developmental success among young people
searchers found that consistent discipline and parental largely have been shown to be effective. A cluster of
school involvement were related to higher GPAs and bet- intervention components including strengthened adult-
ter attendance for all youth, but not to math test scores. youth relationships, social norms around desired behav-
Peer support was a helpful resource, but only for math ior, development of social competencies, and provision
test scores, and then only for students who also were ex- of youth opportunities appears especially critical.
posed to multiple risks such as low maternal education
and family income. Parent promotion of democratic de- Hypothesis Two
cision making was related to higher GPAs and math test
scores for students who experienced multiple risks, but The youth action hypothesis is the second hypothesis.
it was high-risk students whose parents did not promote The three components that comprise it are: (1) Youth ac-
democratic decision making who had the greater school tion impacts contexts and the person. When youth take
success. The researchers reasoned that parents adapt action to improve the contexts in which they live, the im-
their parenting practices to the risk level of the environ- pact is enhanced because such action (properly guided
ment, with greater parental control more beneficial and including reflection) is developmentally enhancing
when children are living in high-risk environments. and, when successful, makes the target context(s) more
In a study of high school students, McLellan and beneficial to the actors and to other youth; (2) The im-
Youniss (2003) used the framework of identity develop- pact is cumulative because youth who take action are
ment theory to describe the developmental role played more likely than those who do not to take action in the
by different kinds of community service, that is, differ- future, which again enhances their personal develop-
ing service contexts. In their view, service provides ac- ment and the contexts they have changed; their example
cess to different “ transcendent systems of meaning” also encourages other youth to take action; and (3)
that enable young people to connect themselves with Processes for strengthening youth impact on context and
historical, religious, ethnic, or political traditions “of self—youth participation and leadership—can be de-
which they can legitimately feel a part ” (p. 57). Young signed and implemented.
people were more likely to volunteer if they were in net- Systems and ecological models of development hold
works in which their parents and friends did service, that individuals are both the products and producers of
918 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

their environments, and that it is the relation between of 6th, 8th, 10th, and 12th graders. They compared ado-
the individual and environment that influences develop- lescents who were “chronically interested” as they went
ment (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998; Hamilton et al., about their lives, versus adolescents who reported being
2004; Lerner, 2002; Zeldin, 2004). As Hamilton et al. habitually bored. The interested, engaged adolescents
(2004, p. 15) note: “Human beings develop through ac- had significantly higher global self-esteem, internal
tive engagement with their environment; by making locus of control, and optimism about their future, and
choices and shaping that environment, they also direct significantly less pessimism than the bored adolescents.
their own development.” Two related processes may be Hunter and Csikszentmihalyi (2003) reasoned that,
at work. Young people’s engagement may in fact alter over time, engaged adolescents will develop more inter-
how other people relate to them, and young people tak- nal resources of confidence and enthusiasm—more
ing action to improve their contexts may subjectively ap- “psychological capital”—than their disengaged peers,
praise those contexts more favorably. because they view themselves as more effective agents
Youth engagement extends beyond merely providing in constructing the flow of their lives. Moreover, their
opportunities for youth, but is rather a part of an intri- openness and interested connection to their experiences
cate dialectic that itself characterizes positive out- may both partly arise from their social capital in the
comes, or thriving (Lerner, Dowling, & Anderson, form of adults’ enhancing and guiding their interests,
2003). Lerner hypothesizes that when this bidirectional and also help create further social capital, as their very
process occurs under conditions of “ building a civil so- interested nature attracts others to them. Ryff and
ciety”—that is, when the ideals of equity, democracy, Singer (1998) also ascribe high importance to the effect
social justice, and personal freedoms are supported— that individuals’ perceptions of events or circumstances
and when youth see themselves as part of an activity or have on psychological coping and how “physiological
issue that is larger than themselves, this in turn impels cascades unfold” (p. 13) based on perceptions.
both healthy individual development as well as salutary These theoretical descriptions of social and psycho-
effects for the community (Lerner, Dowling, et al., logical capital, and the processes that link them, are
2003; see also Nakamura, 2001; Pancer, Rose-Krasnor, quite analogous to Benson et al.’s (1998) formulation of
& Loiselle, 2002). “external” and “internal” developmental assets being
‘Youth engagement’ is a multidimensional term, and key “ building blocks of success.” Similar too is Lerner,
loosely refers to activities and constructs such as posi- Wertlieb, and Jacobs (2003) elaboration of the recipro-
tive citizenship, volunteering, prosocial acts in the com- cal individual-context relations that are the heart of de-
munity, involvement, participation, community service, velopmental systems theory.
and youth voice (O’Donoghue, Kirshner, & McLaugh- Dworkin, Larsen, and Hansen (2003) also provide a
lin, 2002; Zaff & Michelsen, 2002). Central to all of theoretical explanation of how youth participation in
these terms is meaningful participation in an activity one kind of developmental context—extracurricular or
that links the individual, through action and commit- community-based activities—might positively influ-
ment, to the broader context (Nakamura, 2001; Pancer ence development through young people’s actions. They
et al., 2002). It is this last component—where the indi- postulated that such activities facilitate six different
vidual “ transcends self-interest ” (Lerner, Dowling, developmental processes: Identity exploration; the de-
et al., 2003; p. 176)—that separates youth engagement velopment of initiative (“ the capacity to direct atten-
from other extracurricular activities in which the youth tion and effort over time toward a challenging goal,”
may partake. p. 18) and goal-directed behavior; growth in emotional
The interplay of person and context means not only competencies; formation of new and varied peer net-
that change in context changes the person, but that young work connections; development of social skills; and
people’s actions inevitably alter the developmental con- the acquisition of social capital through developing
texts they experience, with related consequences, posi- relationships with nonfamily adults. Dworkin et al.
tively or negatively, for their developmental well-being concluded that a common thread connecting these
(and as well, the positive development of their commu- processes is that the young people participating in
nity—see below). For example, exploring the theoretical youth programs were developing a sense of agency and
importance of “engagement ” in living, Hunter and Csik- seeing themselves as producers of their own develop-
szentmihalyi (2003) studied a diverse national sample ment. This empirical conclusion provides support for
Research Support for Key Positive Youth Development Hypotheses 919

one of the basic tenets of both ecological and especially averages than did their noninvolved peers, even after
developmental systems theory, that children and youth controlling for initial levels of outcome.
help to construct their contexts and do not simply “in- Scales, Blyth, Berkas, and Kielsmeier (2000) com-
teract ” with them (Lerner, 2002). pared, over the course of a school year, social responsi-
Masten et al. (1999) used multiple methods to follow bility and academic success among middle-school
a group of 200 urban 8- to 12-year-olds for 10 years, in- students engaged in service-learning projects and a con-
vestigating pathways to resilience. Individuals were re- trol group of students. Youth action had significant ef-
silient if they were adequately competent in academic fects on young people’s social contexts: Youth in
achievement, conduct, and peer relations even when ex- service-learning projects were more likely to maintain
periencing high adversity. They experienced positive concern for others’ welfare than were control students.
adaptational systems much like those of low adversity Moreover, service-learning students, especially girls,
and competent peers, namely, adequate IQ, high parent- also declined significantly less then did control students
ing quality, high self-worth, and a cheerful, energetic in their frequency of talking with parents about school, a
outlook. Consistent with positive youth development contextual effect (parent involvement) related to positive
Hypothesis 2, competence in childhood longitudinally academic achievement.
predicted positive changes in parenting quality during In a study by Allen, Philliber, Herrling, and Kuper-
adolescence, and parenting quality in childhood longi- minc (1997), almost 700 high school students were ran-
tudinally contributed to positive changes in peer social domly assigned to a treatment group, which consisted of
competence during adolescence. Children’s own behav- structured volunteer community service time as well as
iors changed the kind of family context they experi- a related classroom-based curriculum, or to a control
enced, as reflected in parenting, and through that path group. Students involved in volunteer activities had sig-
altered another developmental context, that of later nificantly lower rates of course failure, school suspen-
peer relations. sions, and rates of pregnancy (ever been for females,
When youth provide community service, they partic- responsible for pregnancy for males) than did the con-
ipate in an activity that explicitly is intended to alter trol group.
both person and context. For example, Metz, McLellan, In a study of 972 urban, predominantly non-European
and Youniss (2003) studied 367 mostly European American seventh and eightg graders, O’Donnell and
American, middle-class, public high school students in colleagues (1999) found that students who participated
Boston, examining how different kinds of community in community service reported significantly less vio-
service facilitated civic development (e.g., concern lence than did control students, after controlling for ini-
with poverty, intention to vote, demonstrating for a tial levels of violence, gender, ethnicity, and socially
cause, future volunteering) over the course of a school desirable responses. Students who had only a violence-
year. Both social cause service (remedying a social prevention curriculum did not differ from controls at the
problem) and standard service (from coaching to raking 6-month follow-up, suggesting that participation in com-
leaves) were associated with greater future intentions munity service was critical to the changes in behavior.
to serve than was not participating in service. However, These studies illustrate the significance of different
social cause service during the school year was associ- kinds of youth engagement on changes in developmental
ated with greater concern for social issues and uncon- outcomes over time. In particular, given that in many
ventional civic involvement than was standard service cases these effects held after controlling for potential
or no service. selection-effect confounds (Atkins & Hart, 2003), these
Eccles and Barber (1999) examined the effects of results suggest that all youth, regardless of background,
10th-grade prosocial activity involvement (church in- can benefit from these kinds of experiences.
volvement and/or participating in volunteer and commu- Youniss and colleagues (Yates & Youniss, 1996;
nity service) on concurrent and future (2 years later) Youniss, McLellan, & Yates, 1997) offer a more spe-
risk behaviors and academic outcomes. Students en- cific model for how youth participation actively facili-
gaged in prosocial activities drank alcohol and used tates not only a sense of identity, but specifically, civic
marijuana less at both time points than did students not identity. They argue that “participatory action” during
engaged in these activities. In addition, involved stu- adolescent identity formation infuses one’s sense of
dents also had higher concurrent and future grade point self with a civic component; this civic component then
920 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

becomes an inextricable part of how an adolescent sees port of this hypothesis, studies consistently find con-
himself or herself (Youniss et al., 1997). stellations of developmental nutrients, including both
Youniss proposes three related consequences of youth internal and external factors, to be associated with vari-
engagement that work to influence identity formation. ous outcomes. For example, Dukes and Stein (2001)
First, participating in community activities “allows measured several protective factors, including: self-
youth to see society as a construction of human actors esteem, positive school attitudes, prosocial activities
with political and moral goals rather than as a distant, ( homework, clubs, service), purpose in life, and proso-
preformed object ” (Youniss et al., 1997). Second, by cial bonds (attitudes toward police officers). Outcomes
virtue of engaging in these kinds of activities, youth included drug use, delinquency, and weapons posses-
build a sense of agency regarding their own abilities to sion. A second-order factor comprising the assets pre-
influence their surrounding contexts. Third, community dicted significantly fewer of those problem behaviors
involvement instills in youth a sense of responsibility for among a sample of 13,000 6th to 12th grade students in
welfare of the community and its members (see also Colorado. Similarly, in the Add Health study, lower
Lerner, Dowling, et al., 2003). These processes have levels of violence were significantly predicted by par-
lifelong effects on the attitudes individuals hold and the ent-family connectedness, parental expectations for ed-
actions they take. Support for this hypothesis is pro- ucation (weakly), and school connectedness. However,
vided in Youniss et al. (1997). Retrospective accounts parent-adolescent activities or self-esteem did not pre-
indicated that participation in youth organizations dur- dict lower levels of violence (Resnick et al., 1997).
ing adolescence increased the likelihood of civic behav- Leffert et al. (1998) studied a sample of nearly
iors (e.g., membership in local civic, church, service, 100,000 youth from more than 200 U.S. communities.
and professional groups) 15 years later in adulthood. They reported that a cluster of four assets—positive
This is interpreted as indicating that youth engagement peer influence, peaceful conflict resolution, school en-
acts as a gateway to future civic involvement (Tolman & gagement, and safety—added 30% to the explained vari-
Pittman, 2001). ance of engagement in violence, compared to the 8%
The research thus shows the positive impact that explained by demographics.
youth action has on both person—young people them- Crosnoe, Erickson, and Dornbusch (2002) studied a
selves—and social context. However, most of this re- diverse sample of adolescents from nine California and
search concerns community service or service-learning Wisconsin high schools. They reported that “protection”
programs, which represent only one kind of “ youth ac- against delinquency and substance use existed among
tion” or leadership. One review of more than 800 studies adolescents who experienced warm relations with par-
concluded that youth “empowerment,” broadly con- ents, came from relatively well-organized households,
strued, is a relatively less represented area of research valued academic achievement, were engaged at school,
in positive youth development (Scales & Leffert, 2004). felt close to teachers, and performed well in school.
Catterall (1998) analyzed subsamples from the
Hypothesis Three National Educational Longitudinal Study of 1988 to ex-
plore the concepts of commitment resilience and aca-
The covariation hypothesis states that the Person factors demic resilience among 8th graders followed through
(e.g., achievement motivation) and context factors (e.g., 10th grade. Commitment resilience was the recovery by
caring school climate or school boundaries) covary and 10th grade of confidence in graduating among those
are mutually reinforcing. That is, ecological factors and who in 8th grade had “any degree of doubt ” about grad-
individual attributes tend to be directly related. Increas- uating. Academic resilience was the significantly better
ing assets of one kind tends to increase the other. performance in English of 10th-grade students who in
Developmental theory posits that person and context 8th grade had C or lower grades in that subject. Both
truly are mutually interactive. Thus, developmental as- kinds of resilience were fostered by a similar constella-
sets “in” the person, such as social competencies or pos- tion of positive assets. These assets included family in-
itive identity, should be found operating together with volvement in and supports for schooling (e.g., books in
developmental assets “outside” the person in their vari- the home, a place for studying, rules about TV watching
ous contexts (e.g., family, schools, peers, community) to [for academic resilience only]), teacher responsiveness
promote developmental well-being and thriving. In sup- ( listening and being interested in students), fairness of
Research Support for Key Positive Youth Development Hypotheses 921

school discipline policies, and student involvement in greater numbers of those assets. Horizontal pile-up also
school and extracurricular activities. implies contextual breadth, if not synergy, in the experi-
These studies illustrate the commonly observed link- ence of assets, as when the accumulation of assets expe-
age of both person and context factors in positive youth rienced in multiple ecological contexts (e.g., family,
development. Some assets, such as school bonding, also school, community, peer) is more strongly associated
well exemplify the often tenuous distinction between with positive outcomes than are assets experienced in
“internal” and “external” resources. School bonding is a only one context.
particularly important developmental asset, having been Jessor, Van Den Bos, Vanderryn, Costa, and Turbin’s
linked to positive outcomes such as reduced substance (1995) longitudinal study of seventh to ninth graders was
use, antisocial behavior, risky behaviors such as early one of the first to demonstrate, not only that an accumu-
sexual initiation, delinquency and, most substantially, lation of risk factors was associated with greater prob-
academic performance. Four dimensions of school lem behavior, but that a greater accumulation of
bonding have been identified: Attachment to school protective factors was associated with fewer problem be-
(youth care about their school), attachment to personnel havior. Implicit in their Protective Factor Index was the
(connection to school adults), school commitment (the representation of multiple contexts, including school,
priority of school for youth), and school involvement friends, family, and community elements. Gutman and
(participation in school activities). In the social devel- Midgely (2000) documented the multiplicative effects of
opment model, involvement is seen more as a contributor developmental assets on the academic achievement of
to school bonding than a result of it (Maddox & Prinz, African American students living in poverty and making
2003), but that the construct comprises both internal and the transition to middle school. Students with either
external dimensions underscores the covariation of per- family ( high parental involvement) or school protective
son and context influences on development. factors (perceived teacher support, or feelings of school
belonging) had higher GPAs in sixth grade than class-
mates who did not experience those nutrients. But stu-
Hypothesis Four
dents who had both family and school assets had higher
The fourth hypothesis, termed the “pile-up” hypothesis, GPAs than students who had only family or school assets
states that the total number of positive experiences but not both.
(i.e., a pile-up of assets) is concurrently related to both In a sample of more than 100,000 youth, Benson,
positive and negative outcomes. Moreover, assets are Scales, Leffert, and Roehlkepartain (1999) found that
functionally equivalent; it is the number of assets that each successive increase in a young person’s quartile
matters, not specific assets or combinations of assets, asset level, from asset-depleted (0 to 10 assets) to asset-
because context-person fusion creates an infinite diver- rich (31 to 40 assets) was associated with significantly
sity of combinations of assets that “matter most.” Re- more adolescent thriving (e.g., school success, overcom-
search provides considerable support for the first part of ing adversity) and significantly less risk behavior (e.g.,
this hypothesis, the “pile-up” effect associated with problem alcohol use, early sexual intercourse).
greater numbers of assets. But there is also considerable A subsequent analysis of a more diverse sample of
evidence that specific assets or clusters of assets matter 217,000 middle and high school students from more
more or less for specific youth (see also below under than 300 U.S. communities revealed the same evidence
the universality/diversity hypothesis), and depending of horizontal pile-up (Developmental assets, 2001).
on the developmental outcome the assets are hypothe- Young people with 0 to 10 assets report an average of 4.1
sized to predict. high-risk behavior patterns; those with 11 to 20 assets
The accumulation of developmental strengths repeat- report 2.3 risk patterns; those with 21 to 30 assets report
edly has been shown to add value over the positive ef- an asset of just 1 high-risk behavior pattern; and asset-
fects of a lesser number of strengths. As discussed in rich youth, and with 31 to 40 assets, report an average of
Benson et al. (2003) there are two manifestations of this just .3 high-risk patterns.
pile-up, horizontal and vertical. Horizontal pile-up is Hollister-Wagner, Foshee, and Jackson (2001) stud-
reflected in cross-sectional studies that document an ied how developmental assets (protective factors in their
increased association of assets and outcomes at a terminology) might promote resilience to aggression
single point in time when the young person experiences among adolescents. In their study of rural eighth and
922 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

ninth graders, the protective factors investigated were dicted actual achievement) and achievement ideology
importance of religion, self-esteem, closeness to one were stronger than the unique effects of any of the indi-
adult, relationship competence, constructive communi- vidual contexts (the combined effect on school conduct
cation skills, and constructive anger response. For fe- was comparable to the individual effect from teacher
males, but not for males, the researchers found that with support). This finding suggested that “ when students re-
each increase in the simple number of protective factors, ceive support from the family, church, and school simul-
the relationship between risk factors (e.g., having been taneously, the effects on their attitudes about self and
hit, witnessing parental violence) and reports of beating the importance of schooling are magnified” (Sanders,
up one’s peers weakened. Eighth and ninth-grade girls 1998, p. 402).
who had all six protective factors were about three times The effects of positive experience across multiple
less likely as girls with only two, and four times less contexts can be seen as well in Scales, Benson, et al.’s
likely as girls with no protective factors, to report beat- (2000) study of the relations among developmental
ing up someone. assets and thriving indicators. For example, among
Relationships with adults in school and community European American 6th to 12th graders, achievement
settings also provide valuable sources of protection from motivation alone explained 19% of the variance in
risk. For example, in the National Longitudinal Study on school success (self-reported grades). But school en-
Adolescent Health, Resnick et al. (1997) reported that gagement, time in youth programs, time at home, plan-
young people who experienced closer connections to ning and decision making, parent involvement in school,
their families and schools were significantly less likely and self-esteem added another 12% of variance (Scales,
than other adolescents to engage in a variety of risk-tak- Benson, et al., 2000). The Search Institute findings are
ing behaviors. Each of the contexts (family and school) consistent with those reported by Eccles, Early, Frasier,
by itself explained relatively modest portions (5% to Belansky, and McCarthy (1997). In their study of mid-
18%) of the variance across outcomes such as emotional dle school students, the explained variance of adolescent
distress, violence, and substance use. But when the ef- outcomes was “substantially increased” when all the
fects of the other context (family or school) and assets in contexts studied (family, school, and peers) were added
still other contexts (e.g., religious involvement) were in- into regressions, leading the researchers to conclude that
cluded, the contribution of these assets to outcome vari- positive experiences across contexts add “linearly and
ance increased by more than 50%. independently” to contribute to positive development.
In another report utilizing the Add Health data (U.S. Brody, Dorsey, Forehand, and Armistead (2002)
Department of Health and Human Services, 1999), the studied the contribution of supportive parenting and
extent of several positive behaviors among 7th to 12th classroom processes to the psychological adjustment of
graders was noted, such as getting B or higher averages, African American elementary and middle school stu-
involvement in extracurricular activities, and religious dents living in poverty in the South. For both grade co-
involvement once a month or more. The more positive horts, they found that students experiencing high
behaviors in which students engaged, the fewer the num- parenting ( high monitoring and a supportive, involved
ber of risk behaviors in which they engaged. mother-child relationship) or classroom quality ( high
Cumulative environmental risk has been shown to be levels of organization, clarity of rules, and involvement
predictive of internalizing and externalizing problems, of students) had better adjustment than students experi-
not only as the absolute number of risks increases, but as encing low quality in both contexts. However, students
the number of social domains (e.g., family, peer, school, experiencing high quality in both contexts had the best
neighborhood) being high-risk increases (Gerard & adjustment, as reflected in the highest self-regulation
Buehler, 2004). Sanders’s (1998) study of more than scores, and lowest externalization and depression scores.
800 urban African American students in the eighth Similar findings, among fifth and sixth graders, are
grade lends further support to the hypothesis that, anal- reported by Paulson, Marchant, and Rothlisberg (1998)
ogous to the findings for risk, strengths piling up across in a study of the effect of children having assets across
ecological domains magnify the protective and thriving contexts. Children with the highest achievement per-
effects of positive experiences in single contexts. ceived a consistency and congruence of parenting and
He reported that when all three support contexts— teaching styles, accompanied by high parental involve-
family, school, and church—were combined, the effect ment in school and a caring school climate. The assets
on academic self-concept (which most strongly pre- provided by family and school enabled those children to
Research Support for Key Positive Youth Development Hypotheses 923

enjoy more positive outcomes than children who experi- Thoresen, 2003) for the theoretical connection between
enced assets in only one of those contexts. assets and greater school success appears to be persua-
A test of the social development model (Catalano & sive, supported by scores of peer-reviewed studies. How-
Kosterman, 1996) found an acceptable fit to predicting ever, this conclusion pertains only when a number of
drug use among 590 17- and 18-year-olds on the basis of assets and other factors (e.g., teachers’ collective effi-
variables measuring prosocial and antisocial influences cacy) are operating together; rarely do single assets or
from fifth grade through middle school. In addition to other factors (excepting near tautologies such as previ-
prior drug use, the model includes such protective fac- ous grades predicting future grades) account for consid-
tors as: perceived opportunities and rewards for proso- erable variance in school success outcomes (Wang,
cial involvement ( knowing where to go to join clubs, Hartel, & Walberg, 1990). Benson et al. (1999), for ex-
participating in family decisions, having lots of chances ample, reported that in a sample of nearly 100,000 6th to
for extracurricular activities), reported involvement in 12th graders, each quartile increase in students’ levels
prosocial activities (including church attendance and of 40 developmental assets (i.e., from 0 to 10, 11 to 20)
membership in community groups), social competen- was associated with a significant improvement in self-
cies, attachment and bonding to prosocial others, and reported grades.
belief in the moral order (e.g., importance of telling the Similarly, Scales and Benson (2004) created a proso-
truth, whether it is okay to cheat). All the path coeffi- cial orientation measure by combining several items
cients for protective factors to drug use were significant tapping adolescents’ attitudes toward helping others,
and in the expected direction. and several items asking about intentions to help those
In a study of 12,500 9th to 12th graders from the in need, working to improve their school, or tutoring or
original Add Health study pool of 7th to 12th graders, coaching younger children over the next year. They then
Zweig, Phillips, and Lindberg (2002) reported that stu- examined the concurrent relation to prosocial orienta-
dents with higher levels of protective factors (e.g., deci- tion and the number of developmental assets adolescents
sion-making skills, participation in physical activities) reported. In a racially/ethnically diverse sample of more
consistently had significantly lower levels of behaviors than 5,000 6th to 12th graders, they found that each in-
such as sexual activity, alcohol use, binge drinking, crease in the quartile level of the asset domains studied
other drug use, fighting, and suicidal behaviors. (0 to 2, 3 to 5, 6 to 8, or 9 to 12 assets) was associated
Similarly, Jessor et al. (1998) examined risk and pro- with a significant increase in the mean score on proso-
tection especially among disadvantaged students, with cial orientation. They also found that, controlling for
disadvantage defined by low parental occupational sta- grade in school, race/ethnicity, and parental education,
tus, low parental education, and single-parent family both boys and girls with above average levels of proso-
structure. The outcome variables of interest were school cial orientation were nearly four times more likely to re-
engagement, low problem behavior, and a composite of port actual volunteer service of at least 1 hour per week
the two, labeled “Making It.” They reported that a pro- in the past year.
tective factor index contributed about as much to vari- Overall, the empirical evidence is consistent and
ance in the successful outcomes as did a risk factor index. strong for the theoretical relation between the number
For example, risk contributed 32% to the composite mea- of assets that adolescents experience and the positive
sure of Making It, compared to 26% for protection. developmental outcomes of both greater thriving and
Benson and Roehlkepartain (2004) studied the rela- lessened risk behaviors.
tion of assets to substance use among a cross-sectional
sample of more than 217,000 6th to 12th graders. They
Hypothesis Five
reported that young people with low levels of develop-
mental assets (0 to 10 of the 40 assets) were from 2.4 to The longitudinal hypothesis is defined as the fusion of
4.4 times more likely to engage in different kinds of al- context /person dynamics in the presence of high levels
cohol, tobacco, and other drug use than were students at of developmental assets results over time in (a) lessened
average or higher levels of assets (21 or more assets). risk behaviors; ( b) increased academic achievement;
The effects of assets were stronger than that of SES or (c) increased contribution; and (d) higher levels of other
living in a single-parent family. thriving indicators.
The pile-up effect is seen for other outcomes as well. Numerous studies have demonstrated the contribu-
For example, the overall level of evidence (Miller & tion that developmental assets make to positive youth
924 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

outcomes not only concurrently but also over time. For Phillips, and Settersten (2002) reported in their study
example, more than 30 longitudinal studies showing of changes in early adolescent development that the ef-
these relations are cited in comprehensive reviews of re- fects of individual contexts on development were gener-
search on developmental assets in adolescence (Scales & ally quite modest. However, the additive effects of
Leffert, 2004) and middle childhood (Scales et al., adolescents’ multiple positive contexts were consider-
2004). Although in all cases, the studies focus on only able, a result in alignment with other research showing
one or a small number of assets, not the entire range of the value of young people experiencing “redundancy”
40 assets identified by Search Institute, the results are of developmental assets across their ecologies (Benson
nevertheless consistent: The experience of developmen- et al., 2003).
tal assets contributes significantly to the likelihood of Ultimately, the most important “outcome” of positive
subsequent protection from high-risk behaviors and pro- development is more postive development. The findings
motion of thriving. of the Iowa Youth and Families Project are illustrative.
For example, Moore and Glei (1995) found that young The researchers (Conger & Conger, 2002) reported that
people who as children and adolescents participated sig- the assets of nurturant and involved parenting experi-
nificantly more than their peers in school clubs were es- enced in seventh grade helped young people have fewer
pecially likely to report positive outcomes in young emotional and behavioral problems and function more
adulthood (ages 18 to 22). Outcomes included closer re- competently during adolescence, even when dealing
lationships with their parents, and greater involvement with family economic adversity. But young people who
in community affairs or volunteer work. experienced those family assets during adolescence also
In a small (N = 100) sample of racially/ethnically di- were themselves more competent parents and more suc-
verse adolescents from low-income families, Way and cessful in their romantic relationships years later in
Robinson (2003) found, as predicted, that the asset of early adulthood (5 years posthigh school).
positive school climate contributed to higher levels of Gambone et al. (2002) created indices to measure
self esteem at 2 years later, over and above the positive several optimal adolescent developmental outcomes
effects of family and friend support. Masten et al. (young people are productive, connected, and can navi-
(1999) followed a sample of urban 8- to 12-year-olds for gate through their worlds effectively), and several opti-
10 years. They showed that even after controlling for IQ mal young adult outcomes (individuals are on a path to
and socioeconomic status (SES), the quality of parenting economic self-sufficiency, have healthy family and so-
in mid-adolescence predicted academic, conduct, and cial relationships, and are involved in the community).
social competence in late adolescence. Perhaps more About half of youth were doing well overall in young
striking, the quality of parenting in childhood predicted adulthood, but 69% of those with optimal developmental
social competence 10 years later in late adolescence. milestones in high school subsequently did very well,
Pettit, Bates, and Dodge (1997) reported similar lon- with a 41% greater chance of experiencing such optimal
gitudinal results in their 7-year study of more than 500 young adult outcomes. Moreover, young people who had
Tennessee and Indiana families with kindergartners. optimal levels of the developmental nutrients early in
The quality of supportive parenting children received as high school were much more likely to have the positive
kindergartners (e.g., parental warmth and involvement, developmental milestones later in high school. For ex-
proactive teaching, calm discussion) contributed a small ample, youth who had supportive relationships with par-
(1% to 3%) but unique amount of variance to the predic- ents, teachers, and friends early in high school were
tion of their functioning in both kindergarten and grade 100% more likely to have optimal developmental out-
six, including whether they exhibited problem behav- comes later in high school.
iors, were socially skillful, and performed well in Analysis by Search Institute (2004) of a longitudinal
school. This study was notable for showing that the pres- sample of 370 students in St. Louis Park, Minnesota,
ence of positive parenting, not merely the absence of from when they were in 7th to 9th grades to when they
harsh parenting, plays an important role in contributing were in 10th to 12th grades, showed that, in general, the
to child well-being in both the short- and longer-term. more assets students reported in 1998, the less they re-
Moreover, as for studies reflecting horizontal pile- ported risk-taking behavior patterns (e.g., driving and
up, experiencing assets in multiple contexts also is de- alcohol problems, school problems) and the more they
velopmentally advantageous over time. Cook, Herman, reported indicators of thriving (e.g., delayed gratifica-
Research Support for Key Positive Youth Development Hypotheses 925

tion, physical health) in 2001 (unpublished analyses of support, and boundaries and expectations. These re-
for this chapter; for study details see Scales, Benson, sults offer an additional provocative suggestion of the
Roehlkepartain, Sesma, & van Dulmen, in press; Scales role of developmental assets in protecting young people
& Roehlkepartain, 2003). These results were largely from ATOD risks.
maintained even when controlling for earlier levels of Participation in youth programs was found in both
the outcome variables. Additional perspective came the Scales, Benson, et al. (2000) and Scales and
from using a more person-centered analysis. Students Roehlkepartain (2003) Search Institute studies to be
who stayed stable or went up .5 SD in their assets over linked to school success. In a study focusing on the role
those 3 years had significantly fewer problem alcohol of such extracurricular programs on posthigh school
use or school problems, and more informal helping, educational achievement, Mahoney, Cairns, and Farmer
leadership, overcoming adversity, and school success (2003) utilized the Carolina Longitudinal Study to
than students who declined .5 SD in their assets. follow nearly 700 students annually from 4th grade
Moreover, both concurrently and longitudinally, each through 12th grade, interviewing them again when the
quartile increase in assets was associated with signifi- young people were 20. They found that consistency of
cantly higher GPA, and the longitudinal relations held extracurricular participation was significantly associ-
even when controlling for the effects of earlier GPA ated with both interpersonal competence over time, as
(Scales & Roehlkepartain, 2003). The difference in well as with educational aspirations in late adolescence,
mean GPA between asset-rich students (31 to 40 assets) and both of those factors were linked to educational sta-
and asset-depleted students (0 to 10 assets) was equiva- tus (whether in postsecondary education or not) at age
lent to the difference between a B+ and a C average. In 20. The researchers explained the theoretical basis for
addition, growth curve analysis showed a small but sig- such results by noting that the peer and adult relation-
nificant relationship between increase in assets and in- ships and skills associated with sustained extracurricu-
crease in GPA, such that mean GPA increased about 1⁄ 5 th lar activity participation promote social acceptance,
of a grade point over time for each increase of one asset. positive social identity development, less depressed
In a small study of 95 inner-city sixth to eighth mood and anti-social behavior, school engagement, and
graders (about 60% non-European American), Dubow, higher educational expectations.
Arnett, Smith, and Ippolito (2001) reported that the In an analysis of several waves of data from the Na-
asset of positive expectations for the future, as assessed tional Education Longitudinal Study of 1988, Zaff,
in September, significantly predicted lower levels of a Moore, Papillo, and Williams (2003) reported that vol-
problem behavior index in June, including using alcohol. unteering 2 years after high school was significantly
In addition, higher initial levels of perceived problem- more likely among students who enjoyed key develop-
solving efficacy and family support predicted increases mental assets from grades 8 to 12, such as having high
over the school year in positive expectations for the fu- levels of parental support and monitoring, positive peer
ture. In another example, the social development model influences, and attendance at religious services. More-
was applied to promote children’s bonding to school in over, if students consistently participated in extracurric-
Seattle. Children who received a program in Grade 5 ular activities during grades 8 to 12—regardless of
emphasizing the development of social competencies whether those activities were sports, schools clubs, or
and bonding to school experienced, by age 21, signifi- community clubs—they were twice as likely to volunteer
cantly more responsible sexual behavior, including fewer and to have voted in local or national elections 2 years
partners and less sexually transmitted diseases, than after high school as students with only occasional ex-
peers not exposed to the program (Lonczak, Abbott, tracurricular participation.
Hawkins, Kosterman, & Catalano, 2002). The overall pattern of these results suggests that de-
Benson and Roehlkepartain (2004) also conducted velopmental strengths provide some unique proportion
longitudinal analyses on a sample of middle school stu- of influence over time in addition to their much more
dents who reported abstaining from alcohol, tobacco, or substantial impact on concurrent developmental out-
drug (ATOD) use in 1997. Those who continued to ab- comes. In both their strong concurrent relations and
stain in high school 4 years later, compared to those who small to moderate longitudinal relations, they provide
began ATOD use, had significantly higher levels of as- support for the theoretical proposition that developmen-
sets in both 1997 and 2001, especially in the categories tal assets positively affect developmental trajectories.
926 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Scales and Roehlkepartain (2003) found that for


Hypothesis Six
every point higher students scored in 1998 on a develop-
The Community Hypothesis is based on the notion that mental assets factor reflecting connection to commu-
community is a viable focus for understanding and pro- nity, they were three times more likely than other
moting dynamics crucial for maximizing context /person students to be in the high GPA group (B+ or higher) in
relationships. By analogy to public health, the largest 2001. Assets in that factor included youth programs, re-
improvements in positive youth development will occur ligious community, service to others, creative activities,
in response to interventions/initiatives that are aimed at reading for pleasure, other adult relationships, and adult
changing communities more so than those aimed at indi- role models. The results of this study are provocative in
viduals. How the community is defined depends on the suggesting how a multiplicity of assets reflecting the de-
target(s) of the intervention /initiative. velopmental attentiveness of “community” may favor-
The inadequacy of individual treatment is related to ably affect young people’s school success.
the principles of public health and prevention. Despite Similarly, Greenberg et al. (2003) reviewed a wide
dramatic improvements in medical treatment, Kreipe, range of evidence that suggests the most effective
Ryan, and Seibold-Simpson (2004, p. 104) point out that school-based prevention and youth development data are
“Improved sanitation, work environments, and immu- those that “enhance students’ personal and social as-
nization programs as well as safety measures . . . have sets” and improve the school-community environment
done more to improve health than one-to-one medical (p. 467). The focus of effective approaches is not on nar-
treatment.” row programs addressing a single issue—programs that
Similarly, community mobilization to promote posi- often may be disruptive more than beneficial—but com-
tive youth development must address not only formal prehensive efforts that try simultaneously to build stu-
organizations and programs but also informal norms dents’ health, character, citizenship and community
and relationships. Studies show that youth do better in connection, school orientation, and academic perfor-
communities where adults share some basic values, mance. The American Psychological Association’s Task
norms, and expectations, including understandings Force on Prevention, Promoting Strength Resilience,
about what kind of behavior is acceptable and what to and Health in Young People, also endorses a broad ap-
do when someone crosses the line (Damon, 1997; proach that coordinates problem-prevention with efforts
Sampson et al., 1997). to build young people’s competence, relationships with
In this section, we refer to community as the inter- others, and contributions to the community (Weissberg
locking systems of contexts, ecologies, and settings that et al., 2003).
moderate developmental growth. Accordingly, there are Echoing the research presented earlier as relevant to
within this broad conception a wide range of influences the first positive youth development hypothesis (i.e.,
on development, including family, neighborhood, school, that contexts are modifiable, and that these changes in
playground, and congregation, the relationships inside contexts have consequences for youths’ developmental
and beyond these settings, and the policy, business and outcomes), a core of strategies repeatedly appears in re-
economic infrastructure of a community. ports of successful efforts. These include: building stu-
Tolan, Gorman-Smith, and Henry (2003) conducted a dents’ social-emotional learning repertoire, providing
6-year longitudinal study of several hundred African frequent opportunities for student participation, such as
American and Latino adolescent males and their pri- through community service, fostering caring, support-
mary caretakers. As predicted by bioecological theory ive relationships among students, teachers, and parents,
(Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998), they reported a com- and consistently rewarding positive social, health, and
plex relationship among community structural charac- academic behaviors through school-parent-community
teristics, neighborhood processes, parenting practices, collaborations.
and youths’ violent behavior. Neighborhood concen- Much of the source for the impact of community
trated poverty and high crime levels were found to pre- comes from adults outside young people’s own families.
dict the extent of perceived neighborhood problems and Recent research has documented clearly the value of
neighborliness, as well as directly to predict parenting formal mentoring relationships for young people
practices, such that high poverty and crime were related (DuBois, Holloway, Valentine, & Cooper, 2002; Rhodes
to more restrictive parenting, which reduced violence by et al., 2000). The more global influence of “other adult
limiting youths’ gang involvement. assets” that occur quite naturally in young people’s
Research Support for Key Positive Youth Development Hypotheses 927

lives, such as with neighbors, is potentially more far- engagement critically includes youth themselves. And
reaching but has been less well studied. The limited evi- from the lessons of CCYD, Gambone et al. (2002) have
dence suggests that only 15% of young people report articulated and demonstrated a convincing rationale for
experiencing a “rich” level of relationships with adults evaluating the opportunities such an initiative creates for
other than parents (Scales, 2003; Scales et al., 2002). youth rather than focusing solely on impact or outcomes
But the climate of social expectations is crucial. for individual youth. The “community action framework
Sixty-two percent of U.S. adults with strong social ex- for youth development ” (Connell et al., 2001) embeds
pectations for involvement are highly engaged with such opportunities in a theory of change that can be the-
other people’s children, versus 41% for those who feel oretically and empirically linked to desired outcomes,
only moderate expectations, 22% for those with mild some intermediate-term and some long-term.
expectations, and just 9% for those with weak social In a particularly useful analysis of community initia-
expectations for involvement (Scales, 2003). Conse- tives, Dorgan and Ferguson (2004) examined factors crit-
quently, although studies regularly demonstrate the ef- ical to the success (or lack of it) in the New Futures
fect of “community” as a source of developmental initiative and the New York City Beacons project (com-
assets, potentiation of the full range of possible posi- munity centers operating in public school buildings).
tive community impact on youth development requires Though the two initiatives had similar aspirations, they
attention to changing existing social norms about adult- were directed by quite different theories of change and
youth engagement. implementation strategies. The authors credit the partic-
Some of the more ambitious efforts to intervene at ular success of the Beacons project to a clear, understand-
the level of community have been initiated by national able, and politically compelling emphasis on co-locating
foundations. The Kellogg Youth Initiative Partnerships services, supports and opportunities in neighborhood
(KYIP) were launched in 1987 to assist three Michigan schools to create “safe havens” for youth. In addition, the
communities in expanding beyond investment in “ fixing Beacons’ focus on professionals working directly with
young people’s problems” to community collaborations youth and on the grassroots support of volunteers, par-
engaged in promoting youth potential. Combining ser- ents, and neighborhood residents led to faster achieve-
vice integration with youth development principles and a ment of goals than the New Futures approach of creating
focus on school reform, the Annie E. Casey Foundation collaboratives to plan and coordinate youth services and
in 1987 launched New Futures, a 5-year demonstration programs city-wide.
project in five cities with high percentages of high-risk A somewhat different theory of change undergirds
youth. In 1995, with funding from a consortium of foun- Search Institute’s national Healthy Community • Healthy
dations, Public/ Private Ventures (P/ PV) launched its Youth movement. With 600 communities currently en-
Community Change for Youth Development Initiative gaged (Benson, 2003a), this change strategy invites com-
(CCYD). The CCYD provided communities with a set munities to create multiple innovative “experiments” to
of research-based core principles and with strategies for transform contexts and ecologies with a particular eye
implementing them. Among the principles were adult to mobilizing asset-building adult and peer relation-
support and guidance and structured activities during ships. A number of studies are completed or ongoing in
nonschool hours. capturing both how transformative change is made and
None of these initiatives reported large and consis- the connection of these changes to adolescent health and
tent effects in terms of outcomes for youth. However, well-being (Mannes, Lewis, Hintz, Foster, & Nakkula,
new programs, organizations, and leaders demon- 2002; Whitlock & Hamiltion, 2003). A longitudinal
strated enduring impact. For example, 5 years after study in St. Louis Park, Minnesota provides suggestive
New Futures funding was ended, investigators (Hahn & evidence that sustained community-wide engagement
Lanspery, 2001) attributed “change that abides” to the with asset-building has population-level effects on sev-
“ripeness” of the communities for change, including eral measures of well-being (Roehlkepartain, Benson, &
leadership, a widespread recognition of problems, and Sesma, 2003).
utilization of other resources and initiatives with com- These studies generally support the broad hypothesis
patible goals. that describes “community” as an important focus of pos-
In a similar vein, a report from the Kellogg Founda- itive youth development efforts. However, much research
tion (n.d.) after the 1st decade of KYIP stressed the is needed to better understand how specific conceptual-
critical importance of engaging the community. Such izations of “community” operate to positively influence
928 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

young people, and how those effects may vary as a func- Gender
tion of varying realities of person-context fusion.
Studies consistently find that females report higher
levels of most developmental assets than do males,
Hypothesis Seven with the exception of self-esteem (see reviews by
Scales & Leffert, 2004; Scales et al., 2004). The con-
The Universality/ Diversity Hypothesis proposed that
sistency of such findings across studies and measures
there are developmental supports and opportunities that
provides evidence for the validity of this basic conclu-
enhance developmental success for all youth; strategies
sion. However, these systematic differences may be
and tactics for promoting them vary. Moreover, be-
produced by a lack of measures tapping potential as-
cause all youth need developmental assets, many com-
sets that may be more common among young men (e.g.,
munity-level interventions will benefit all or almost all
assertiveness, competitiveness). Reported gender dif-
youth. However, youth with few or no assets may re-
ferences in some assets also may be a result of system-
quire interventions targeted to them and their specific
atic response biases from young people responding in
needs. One of the functions of those extraordinary in-
gender-typed ways (e.g., girls’ greater reporting of
terventions is to enable those youth to benefit from
prosocial attitudes and behaviors—Eisenberg & Fabes,
more universal interventions.
1998). Apart from frequency differences, however, nu-
There are likely variations in the degree to which
merous studies suggest that assets may operate some-
developmental assets can explain developmental out-
what differently for males and females.
comes, and in which assets may be most critical in pro-
Huebner and Betts (2002) used social control theory
moting specific outcomes, depending on differences
to frame a study of 911 7th to 12th graders from a min-
among young people’s contexts and developmental
ing community in the southwest. They found that both
histories. But studies (e.g., reviews in Montemayor,
attachment bonds (connections to parents, unrelated
Adams, & Gullotta, 2000; Scales et al., 2004; Scales &
adults, and peers) and involvement bonds (time in
Leffert, 2004) suggest significant theoretical and prac-
school and nonschool activities, including time in reli-
tical insights relevant for most if not all groups of
gious activities, volunteering, and clubs or organiza-
young people in looking at their development through a
tions) predicted less delinquency and greater academic
strength-based lens.
achievement (self-reported grades). Involvement bonds
However, compared to the literature on developmen-
predicted delinquency more for males than females,
tal strengths and young people of differing gender, age,
and attachment bonds predicted grades more for fe-
racial /ethnic groups, and socioeconomic status, there is
males than for males.
a dearth of empirical work on relating developmental
Hollister-Wagner et al. (2001) studied resiliency with
strengths to other dimensions of diversity, such as sex-
regard to aggression ( beating up a peer). In a large sam-
ual orientation, family background, or differing expo-
ple of rural eighth and ninth graders, they found support
sure to violence. Goldfried and Bell (2003), for example,
for the role of protective factors in reducing violence for
describe literature on sexual minorities as essentially
females, but not males. The researchers reasoned that
“ignored” in mainstream psychology and adolescent de-
exposure to aggressive models, and social reinforcement
velopment. The available evidence suggests that at least
for aggression, is sufficiently stronger for males that
some developmental strengths, such as self-esteem and,
protective factors, although still positive, have a weaker
particularly, family support, seem to diminish or elimi-
influence on them.
nate differences in mental or behavioral health problems
among both sexual majority and minority youth (Blum,
Beuhring, & Rinehart, 2000).
Age

High school students consistently are found to report


YOUTH DEVELOPMENT RESEARCH fewer developmental assets than do middle school stu-
dents. For example, in a cross-sectional sample of more
We next briefly describe illustrative research pertaining than 217,000 6th to 12th graders, whose average number
to positive youth development as reflected across gen- of 40 assets was 19.3, 6th graders reported 23.1 assets,
der, age, SES, and race/ethnicity. 8th graders reported 19.6, and 10th graders reported
Youth Development Research 929

17.8 (Benson, 2001). Asset levels were somewhat higher ces of random halves of a single racial /ethnic group,
among 11th and 12th graders (to 18.1 and 18.3 assets, which would be expected to be highly similar.
respectively), but still remained lower than asset levels Rowe et al. (1994) did not investigate precisely how
among the younger students. Similarly, in a study of assets and outcomes were related across racial /ethnic
more than 5,000 6th to 12th graders in a mid-sized groups. Although developmental assets in general
Western city, Scales, Leffert, et al. (2003) reported that appear to have comparable positive relations with devel-
6th to 8th graders reported significantly more exposure opmental outcomes for most groups of youth, how par-
than 9th to 12th grade students to most assets, including ticular assets function to promote positive outcomes
positive relationships with unrelated adults and consis- may well vary depending on which dimensions of diver-
tency of expectations for behavior. In a longitudinal sity are examined. For example, Bean, Bush, McKenry,
study of 370 students, Roehlkepartain et al. (2003) re- and Wilson (2003) studied the relation of components of
ported that asset levels declined sharply across 6th to authoritative parenting to academic achievement (self-
8th grade, bottomed out between 9th to 11th grades, and reported grades) among 155 African American and
evidenced a slight rebound in the 12th grade. In another European American high school students. They found
analysis of the same longitudinal sample, Scales and that parental support, behavioral control, and psycholog-
Roehlkepartain (2003) reported that 41% of these stu- ical control had significantly different relations with
dents decreased at least .5 standard deviations in their grades, depending on parents’ race and gender. For
assets from middle school through high school. Another African American students, maternal support was sig-
34% stayed relatively stable, and only 24% increased at nificantly related to academic achievement, but the
least .5 standard deviations in asset levels over the mid- other components were not, and none of the parenting
dle school to high school period. components was significant for African American fa-
Similarly, Scales et al. (2004) found that fourth and thers. But for neither European American mothers nor
fifth graders reported more assets than did sixth graders fathers was support a significant contributor to achieve-
(26.6. and 26, respectively, versus 24.7 for sixth ment. For European American students, fathers use of
graders). Only for safety did sixth graders report higher greater behavioral control, and mothers use of greater
levels than fourth to fifth graders. Although longitudi- behavioral control and less psychological control, were
nal data are not yet available to confirm that those grade significant predictors of academic success.
differences result from declining assets as cohorts age, Sesma and Roehlkepartain (2003) examined develop-
the longitudinal results for older youth suggest that this mental assets and outcomes among 217,277 6th- through
interpretation is warranted. 12th-grade students (including 69,731 youth of color) sur-
veyed in 318 U.S. communities during the 1999/2000
school year. Across all racial /ethnic groups, greater num-
Race/ Ethnicity and SES
bers of developmental assets were associated with fewer
Drawing on seven national, state, and local studies with risk behavior patterns and more thriving indicators. These
racially/ethnically diverse adolescent samples, Rowe, relations held even after controlling for socioeconomic
Vazsonyi, and Flannery (1994) argued that developmen- status. For example, across all racial /ethnic groups, young
tal processes appeared similar across racial /ethnic people who engaged in none of 10 high-risk behavior pat-
categories in effects on outcomes such as IQ, achieve- terns averaged experiencing about 23 assets, whereas
ment, and social adjustment. The variables investi- those who reported engaging in 5 or more of the 10 risk
gated included parental involvement and monitoring, patterns said they experienced 15 or fewer of the develop-
self-efficacy, school self-esteem, parents’ school en- mental assets.
couragement, family communication, and attachment to At the same time, there were racial /ethnic differ-
teachers. The covariance matrices of the associations ences. For example, boundaries and expectations assets
between these developmental influences and outcomes (e.g., family rules, neighborhood social controls, and
had significant and similar goodness-of-fit indexes adult role models) were important for all youth in help-
across African American, Asian, Hispanic, and Euro- ing them avoid anti-social behavior, but were found to
pean American adolescents. The degree of similarity have especially strong preventive associations for Amer-
between racial /ethnic groups was no less than the de- ican Indian, Multiracial, and European American youth
gree of similarity found in comparing covariance matri- (Sesma & Roehlkepartain, 2003).
930 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Scales, Benson, et al. (2000) also reported that spe- There is little research on developmental assets
cific clusters of assets could explain from 19% to 32% of among mixed-race or multiracial adolescents. A recent
the variance in self-reported grades, over and above de- report drawing on the national Add Health dataset fo-
mographics, among six different racial /ethnic groups of cused solely on risk behaviors, and concluded that
students. The assets of achievement motivation, school mixed-race adolescents, regardless of which racial /eth-
engagement, time in youth programs, time at home, and nic groups were combined, tended to have higher inci-
personal power meaningfully contributed to variance in dences of health and behavioral risks than single race
grades for three or more of the six racial /ethnic groups. youth. Although impossible to confirm with those data,
In an interview study with 45 male African American the researchers speculated that the results were consis-
gang members and 50 similar youth connected to commu- tent with a theoretical explanation pointing to mixed-
nity organizations, Taylor et al. (2003) found that the race status increasing the stress those young people
nongang youth reported significantly more positive devel- faced (Udry, Li, & Hendrickson-Smith, 2003).
opmental experiences. However, across nine categories of In one study of positive development that included
positive attributes that reflect developmental assets, an self-described Multiracial youth, Scales, Benson, et al.
average of 28% of the gang members scored above the (2000) reported that clusters of developmental assets
mean for the nongang youth, suggesting that a reservoir of had significant explanatory power for concurrent indi-
developmental strengths may exist among even “deviant ” cators of thriving among 6,000 middle and high school
youth assets that supports their positive growth. For ex- youth across racial /ethnic groups (American Indian,
ample, more than one-third of the gang youth had more African American, Asian, Hispanic, Multiracial, and
positive relations with family and with school or educa- European American). For example, aggregating the in-
tion than the nongang youth, and a fifth had more positive dicators into an index of thriving, clusters of the assets
role models than did nongang youth. In a 1-year longitudi- explained from 47% of variance among American
nal analyses of this sample, Taylor et al. (2002) also Indian youth to 54% among Multiracial youth, over
reported a sizeable correlation (.67, p ≤ .01) between and above gender, grade, and level of maternal educa-
change in developmental assets from Time 1 to Time 2, tion. There were some differences across groups. For
and changes in individual growth in positive personal and example, experiencing supportive relationships with
social functioning. These findings point to two tentative adults other than parents was an important contributor
conclusions: (1) that the developmental assets that support to the thriving index for Multiracial, American Indian,
positive outcomes are not entirely absent even for young and European American youth, but reading for pleasure
people who currently are “embedded in a behavioral and was more important among African American and
social milieu marked by risks (e.g., gang violence, drugs, Hispanic youth. However, a core of assets was impor-
and poor familial support) [and] . . . ambient problems of tant across groups. Time spent in youth programs, cul-
poverty and racism” (p. 513), and (2) that enhancing de- tural competence, self esteem, personal power (a
velopmental assets may facilitate positive trajectories for construct akin to self-efficacy), achievement motiva-
a subset of such challenged youth. tion, and planning and decision-making skills each
In another study of several hundred gang and nongang meaningfully contributed to variance for at least two of
adolescents, Li et al. (2002) also found, as expected, that seven thriving indicators across at least three out of six
gang members on average reported fewer resilience fac- racial /ethnic groups.
tors in their lives. But like Taylor et al. (2003), Li et al.
also reported that gang and nongang youth were not sig-
nificantly different on a number of those contributors to DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
resilience, including social problem-solving skills, self-
esteem, physical activity, and academic performance. Although the past 10 years have seen a proliferation of
That is, both these studies suggest that individual and conceptual models seeking to articulate the necessary
ecological characteristics that promote health and thriv- ingredients for positive development and subsequent em-
ing exist among a substantial proportion of seemingly pirical tests of these models, on balance the state of our
“lost ” young people, representing a potentially valuable knowledge is disproportionately low compared with the
target of community actions to build better developmen- state of our unknowns. Our knowledge-base is relatively
tal paths for all young people. strong in the following areas:
Directions for Future Research 931

• Taxonomies of factors that are correlated with posi- explain delay of gratification in the school domain
tive outcomes. also explain delay of gratification in the peer domain?
• Cross-sectional research results affirming associations • Is there such a thing as too much of particular devel-
among relationships, opportunities, social norms, and opmental assets, such that they no longer are assets
positive developmental outcomes. and even become deficits or risk factors (e.g., family
• Knowledge that effective programs have the capacity support becoming enmeshment, or high expectations
to promote short-term changes in youth behaviors. becoming a factor that lessens perceived feelings of
competence)?
Lacunae in our knowledge base regarding developmental • Are there ceiling effects not yet revealed in the re-
assets include the following: search? For example, a close relationship with at least
one caring adult is clearly important, and probably
• Theories of change that articulate how youths, adults, having that with several adults is better, but what does
and community systems move toward greater devel- a dozen such relationships add that five or six does not?
opmental attentiveness. Whitlock (2003) reported, for instance, that youth re-
• Explorations of the transactional nature of commu- porting 9 or 10 of 10 possible developmental supports
nity-youth change; that is, examinations of both how did not have greater school connectedness than youth
community efforts ( both informal and program- reporting 7 or 8 of those supports. But developmental
matic) affect youth, as well as how youth in turn af- supports showed a continued linear relationship with
fect and help shape their ecology. community connectedness, that is, a ceiling effect was
• Empirical understanding of the significance of “in- not observed for community connectedness.
formal, natural, and nonprogrammatic capacity of • If all assets are not equal in their promotive and pro-
community” (Benson & Saito, 2001, p. 146). tective valence, then what are the bases on which
• Understanding of the variability in the delivery of de- some assets are considered more important than oth-
velopmental assets across diverse communities and ers, if not for all young people, then for some youth in
groups of people. While we can specify the necessary some situations for some outcomes?
ingredients, we still do not well understand how those • Do some assets function as “gateways” more than
ingredients “ work ” in culturally diverse settings. others, making it more likely that young people will
• Understanding of how broad, expansive models of experience additional assets that collectively pro-
community involvement and engagement interact with mote positive developmental outcomes? Scales and
more focused programmatic approaches (i.e., does Roehlkepartain (2004), for example, found that stu-
the presence of the former moderate the efficacy of dents who provided community service in middle
the latter?). school were significantly more likely to be “asset-
rich” in high school than were students who did not
In addition, the empirical literature offers to date contribute service.
only limited answers to the following more specific the- • Are some assets more critical for healthy develop-
oretical questions: ment at differing developmental points or stages? For
example, are high expectations from teachers and
• How is the theory connecting assets to thriving out- parents more critical in middle school and early ado-
comes the same as that linking assets to risk reduc- lescence, when increases in challenges to competency
tion outcomes, and how is it different? Relatedly, are beliefs are common, than in later adolescence? Simi-
“internal” asset categories such as positive values or larly, does the asset of cultural competence become
positive identity more properly thought of as indica- more important as children age and encounter in-
tors of well-being, that is, as outcomes? creasingly more diverse peers and adults?
• Is the role of developmental assets global, or depend- • In a related sense, do some assets have more impact
ent on the outcome of interest? during key developmental transitions than at other
• Is the effect of assets invariant across contexts, or times? For example, are young people feeling valued
does social domain make a difference in how assets and that they have useful roles more important assets
affect outcomes? For example, do the same assets that during the transition from elementary to middle
932 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

school and the transition from middle school to high youth development. Ryff and Singer (1998) struck a sim-
school than they are at other times during early and ilar chord in talking about research on “ health” among
later adolescence? older adults. Such research, they argued, routinely de-
• How many of developmental assets does one need, fines health by emphasizing the absence of negatives,
over what period of time, to contribute meaningfully such as being unable to dress and feed oneself, at the ex-
toward particular desired outcomes? pense of inquiring about the positive indicators of pur-
pose and engagement in life that actually may better
In addition to these questions, there exist issues re- predict health outcomes. To more accurately understand
garding the design of research within which the ques- health, they argued, questions should be asked about
tions may be addressed. Approaches such as the theory what persons did today “ that was meaningful or fulfill-
of change strategy for evaluating comprehensive positive ing,” or whether they “love and care for others” (p. 21).
youth development initiatives have been discussed for Several recent efforts have emerged in response to both
some time (see Connell & Kubisch, 2001; Connell, Ku- the relative lack of emphasis on measuring positive out-
bisch, Schorr, & Weiss, 1995). But only a limited number comes, and the lack of a common core of measures to be
of examples show such technology actually being ap- used across positive youth development studies. For exam-
plied, and a broad consensus does not exist about what ple, Search Institute, the Institute for Applied Research in
reasonable outcomes for community initiatives may be Youth Development at Tufts University, and the Fuller
(Berkowitz, 2001; Spilka, 2004). Theological Seminary, with strategic consultation from
Moreover, although the research to date shows prom- Stanford University and the Thrive Foundation for Youth,
ising results, the existing literature focuses almost recently embarked on a multiyear “ Thriving Indicator
exclusively on assessing the effects of community inter- Project ” with the goal of producing effective measure-
ventions on adolescent problem behaviors such as alco- ment tools and resources on thriving that would be widely
hol and other drug use, adolescent pregnancy, and used and developed from a foundation of deep science.
antisocial behavior. Few positive outcomes other than Initial activities have included a comprehensive re-
academic success tend to be measured in community, as view of the literature on thriving and related concepts,
contrasted with program, initiatives (Greenberg et al., and interviews with scholars, positive youth develop-
2003; Wandersman & Florin, 2003). ment practitioners, youth, and their parents that elicited
MacDonald and Valdivieso (2001) also observed that their views on what describes a thriving youth (King
deficit-oriented measures prevail in national tracking et al., in press). A group of core dimensions of thriving
systems. They described numerous possible positive is emerging (e.g., Theokas et al., in press) that will then
constructs and measures that are being or could be ap- serve as a lens to help focus development of thriving
plied in gathering data across four critical domains: measurement tools to be used in clinical, programmatic,
young people themselves, parent and nonparent adults, community change, and national tracking applications.
organizations that serve young people, and community- A similar effort, with the goal of developing and em-
level data on policies, resources, and services. bedding common measures of positive youth development
Weissberg et al. (2003) also note that despite an im- outcomes in state and federal data tracking systems, is
pressive literature now suggesting the effectiveness of being led by Child Trends. Scholars and policymakers are
strength-based approaches to prevention and youth de- recommending reliable, valid, and relatively brief mea-
velopment, there is a continuing need for evaluations of sures in areas such as prosocial orientation, religiosity,
multiyear, comprehensive youth development initiatives and social competencies (Moore & Lippman, 2004) that
that target multiple outcomes. Especially needed are in- could help track developmental strengths and contribute
vestigations of the mediating and moderating influences to a long-term re-shaping of child and youth policy.
on program or initiative effects, and how strength-based
approaches work similarly or differently across diversi-
ties of geography and circumstance. Finally, they noted CONCLUSIONS
the need for more standardized measures of core youth
development outcomes, so that results across different Despite differences in terminology and comprehensive-
studies can more readily be compared. ness, the similarities across models of positive youth de-
The lack of common positive measures of develop- velopment are apparent, and a substantial body of
ment decried by Weissberg et al. (2003) is not unique to research supports the hypotheses emerging from the
References 933

melding of positive youth development practice and a risk reduction and asset-building intervention strategies
variety of developmental and other theories. Positive work for youth in various social locations.
youth development is both caused and indicated by One of the major contributions of positive youth de-
whether a young person experiences adequate supports velopment theory and research is the identification of the
and opportunities. Doing so consistently and in multiple multiple contexts and settings that inform developmental
settings is particularly important. These experiences trajectories. As an applied field, positive youth develop-
help them develop key competencies, skills, values, and ment and its advocates face crucial decision points about
self-perceptions that adaptively self-regulating persons how and where to create intentional change. Though the
need in order to successfully shape and navigate life development of and/or enrichment of programs is the pri-
over time. mary locus of intervention, theory and research also
There are multiple sources of those developmental identify a much wider range of possibilities. Access to
nutrients or assets, including the proactive influence of developmental assets could also be advanced by, for ex-
young people on their own environments. Not just ge- ample, transforming socializing systems (e.g., schools
netic heritage, not just family, not just schools, congre- and neighborhoods) or mobilizing adults to create sus-
gations, peers, or any other influence create a young tained relationships with community youth.
person’s developmental path, but all do so operating to- It is here in this complex space of community and so-
gether, interactively, to form a system larger than the cietal change where new thinking is particularly needed.
sum of those parts. In practical terms, the research find- As noted at several points in this chapter, the least de-
ings that support this conclusion lead to two inevitable veloped part of positive youth development theory is
implications. that having to do with how intentional change can best
First, isolated programs working to change individual be understood (and practiced). The complexity of this
youth without changing the environments in which they issue (as well as the societal importance of promoting
live may have some limited, short-term success, but can- positive development) requires an interdisciplinary ap-
not be expected to support significant long-term positive proach, integrating multiple fields in common pursuit of
development, or especially, to radically alter the devel- how to enhance the dynamic fusion of ecological- and
opmental path of particularly vulnerable young people. individual-level strengths.
Multiple contexts of young people’s lives need to be This interdisciplinary research agenda should ini-
strengthened simultaneously to promote the systemic tially focus on developmental contexts as the unit of
supports needed for sustained and widespread positive analysis with inquiry into the strategies that enhance
development among all youth. the capacity and will of schools, neighborhoods,
Second, the nesting of young people in families and families, and congregations to nurture developmental
schools within neighborhoods and communities and strengths. And such inquiry will necessarily lead to im-
wider society means that a long-term commitment to portant research issues regarding the orchestration of
significant community mobilization around common change at multiple levels, including the strategies for
norms, values, and goals related to positive youth devel- creating developmentally attentive communities. Con-
opment is essential. No less an effort will attain the sonant with the theory of positive development, we hy-
breadth, depth, and permeation of culture with both pothesize that the most successful transformation in
formal and informal daily life to profoundly change the contexts and community will occur when youth are at
developmental odds for a critical mass of America’s the forefront in planning and implementing the change
young people. initiative.
Though research supports the efficacy of positive
youth development as an approach for changing these de-
velopmental odds, it is also clear that other approaches
REFERENCES
are necessary. Poverty, family violence, and abuse are
among a litany of risks that jeopardize development. It is Allen, J. P., Philliber, S., Herrling, S., & Kuperminc, G. P. (1997).
likely that reducing risks and promoting assets can be Preventing teen pregnancy and academic failure: Experimental
complimentary strategies for enhancing positive devel- evaluation of a developmentally based approach. Child Develop-
ment, 68, 729–742.
opmental outcomes. Theory and research is needed to
Anthony, E. J. (1974). The syndrome of the psychologically vulner-
better understand the interplay of risks and assets. In ad- able child. In E. J. Anthony & B. J. Cohler (Eds.), The invulnera-
dition, it is important to identify how combinations of ble child (pp. 103–148). New York: Guilford Press.
934 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Atkins, R., & Hart, D. (2003). Neighborhoods, adults, and the devel- sions, realities, and challenges (pp. vii–xii). Norwell, MA:
opment of civic identity in urban youth. Applied Developmental Kluwer Academic.
Science, 7, 156–165. Benson, P. L., & Pittman, K. (2001b). Trends in youth development:
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on Visions, realities, and challenges. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic.
successful aging: The model of selective optimization with com- Benson, P. L., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2004). Preventing alcohol, to-
pensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful bacco, and other drug use: Rethinking the role of community.
aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). New Search Institute Insights and Evidence, 2(1), 5.
York: Cambridge University Press. Benson, P. L., & Saito, R. N. (2001). The scientific foundations of
Baltes, P. B., Dittmann-Kohli, F., & Dixon, R. A. (1984). New per- youth development. In P. L. Benson & K. J. Pittman (Eds.),
spectives on the development of intelligence in adulthood: Toward Trends in youth development: Visions, realities, and challenges
a dual-process conception and a model of selective optimization (pp. 135–154). Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic.
with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim (Eds.), Life-span Benson, P. L., Scales, P., Leffert, N., & Roehlkepartain, E. (1999).
development and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 33–76). Orlando, FL: Aca- The fragile foundation: The state of developmental assets among
demic Press. American youth. Minneapolis, MA: Search Institute.
Baltes, P. B., & Freund, A. M. (2003). Human strengths as the or- Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., & Mannes, M. (2003). Developmental
chestration of wisdom and selective optimization with compensa- strengths and their sources: Implications for the study and prac-
tion. In L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology tice of community building. In R. M. Lerner, F. Jacobs, & D.
of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions Wertlieb (Eds.), Handbook of applied developmental science: Vol.
for a positive psychology (pp. 23–36). Washington, DC: American 1. Promoting positive child, adolescent, and family development
Psychological Association. through research, policies and programs—Applying developmental
Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life- science for youth and families: Historical and theoretical founda-
span theory in developmental psychology. In W. Damon & R. M. tions (pp. 369–406). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 1029–1143). Berkowitz, W. (2001). Studying outcomes of community-based coali-
New York: Wiley. tions. American Journal of Community Psychology, 29, 213–239.
Bean, R. A., Bush, K. R., McKenry, P. C., & Wilson, S. M. (2003).
Bloom, M. (1996 ). Primary prevention practices. Thousand
The impact of parental support, behavioral control, and psycho-
Oaks, CA: Sage.
logical control on the academic achievement and self-esteem of
African American and European American adolescents. Journal Blum, R. W., Beuhring, T., & Rinehart, P. M. (2000). Protecting
of Adolescent Research, 18, 523–541. teens: Beyond race, income, and family structure. Minneapolis,
MN: University of Minnesota, Center for Adolescent Health.
Benson, P. L. (1990). The troubled journey: A portrait of 6th to 12th
grade youth. Minneapolis, MN: Search Institute. Blum, R. W., & Ellen, J. (2002). Work group V: Increasing the capac-
ity of schools, neighborhoods, and communities to improve ado-
Benson, P. L. (1997). All kids are our kids: What communities must do
lescent health outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Health, 31(Suppl.
to raise caring and responsible children and adolescents. San
6), 288–292.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Blyth, D. A. (2001). Community approaches to improving outcomes for
Benson, P. L. (2002). Adolescent development in social and commu-
urban children, youth, and families. In A. Booth & A. C. Crouter
nity context: A program of research. New Directions for Youth De-
(Eds.), Does it take a village? Community ef fects on children, ado-
velopment, 95, 123–147.
lescents, and families (pp. 223–227). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Benson, P. L. (2003a). Developmental assets and asset-building com-
Bogenschneider, K., Small, S. A., & Tsay, J. C. (1997). Child, parent,
munity: Conceptual and empirical foundations. In R. M. Lerner
and contextual inf luences on perceived parenting competence
& P. L. Benson (Eds.), Developmental assets and asset-building
among parents of adolescents. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
communities: Implications for research, policy, and practice
ily, 59, 345–362.
(pp. 19–43). New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Press.
Benson, P. L. (2003b). Toward asset-building communities: How does Booth, A., & Crouter, A. C. (2001). Does it take a village? Com-
change occur. In R. M. Lerner & P. L. Benson (Eds.), Develop- munity ef fects on children, adolescents, and families. Mahwah,
mental assets and asset-building communities: Implications for re- NJ: Erlbaum.
search, policy, and practice (pp. 213–221). New York: Kluwer Brandtstadter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development.
Academic/ Plenum Press. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Hand-
Benson, P. L., & Leffert, N. (2001). Childhood and adolescence: De- book of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human de-
velopmental assets. In N. J. Smelser & P. G. Baltes (Eds.), Inter- velopment (5th ed., pp. 807–863). New York: Wiley.
national encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences Brandtstadter, J. (1999). Action and self-development: Theory and re-
(pp. 1690–1697). Oxford, England: Pergamon Press. search through the lifespan. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Benson, P. L., Leffert, N., Scales, P. C., & Blyth, D. A. (1998). Be- Brody, G. H., Dorsey, S., Forehand, R., & Armistead, L. (2002).
yond the “ village” rhetoric: Creating healthy communities for Unique and protective contributions of parenting and classroom
children and adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 2(3), processes to the adjustment of African American children living
138–159. in single-parent families. Child Development, 73(1), 274–286.
Benson, P. L., Mannes, M., Pittman, K., & Ferber, T. (2004). Youth Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cam-
development, developmental assets and public policy. In R. M. bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (1998). The ecology of develop-
(2nd ed., pp. 781–814). New York: Wiley. mental processes. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M.
Benson, P. L., & Pittman, K. (2001a). Moving the youth development Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoreti-
message: Turning a vague idea into a moral imperative. In P. L. cal models of human development (5th ed., pp. 993–1028). New
Benson & K. J. Pittman (Eds.), Trends in youth development: Vi- York: Wiley.
References 935

Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. New Connell, J. P., & Kubisch, A. C. (2001). Community approaches to
York: Norton. improving outcomes for urban children, youth, and families: Cur-
Bumbarger, B., & Greenberg, M. T. (2002). Next steps in advancing rent trends and future directions. In A. Booth & A. C. Crouter
research on positive youth development. Prevention and Treat- (Eds.), Does it take a village? Community ef fects on children, ado-
ment, 5(16). lescents, and families (pp. 177–201). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Caplan, G. (1964). Principles of preventive psychiatry. New York: Connell, J. P., Kubisch, A. C., Schorr, L. B., & Weiss, C. H. (1995).
Basic Books. New approaches to evaluating community initiatives: Concepts,
Carnegie Corporation of New York. (1992). A matter of time: Risk methods, and contexts. Washington, DC: Aspen Institute.
and opportunity in the non-school hours. Waldorf, MD: Carnegie Cook, T. D., Herman, M. R., Phillips, M., & Settersten, R. A., Jr.
Council on Adolescent Development. (2002). Some ways in which neighborhoods, nuclear families,
Carnegie Council on Adolescent Development. (1995). Great transi- friendship groups, and schools jointly affect changes in early ado-
tions: Preparing adolescents for a new century. New York: lescent development. Child Development, 73, 1283–1309.
Carnegie Corporation of New York. Crosnoe, R., Erickson, K. G., & Dornbusch, S. (2002). Protective
Catalano, R. F., Berglund, M. L., Ryan, J. A., Lonczak, H. S., & functions of family relationships and school factors on the deviant
Hawkins, J. D. (1999). Positive youth development in the United behavior of adolescent boys and girls: Reducing the impact of
States: Research findings on evaluations of positive youth devel- risky friendships. Youth and Society, 33(4), 515–544.
opment programs. Seattle, WA: Social Development Research Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal expe-
Group. rience. New York: Harper-Collins.
Catalano, R. F., Berglund, M. L., Ryan, J. A. M., Lonczak, H. S., & Damon, W. (1997). The youth charter: How communities can work to-
Hawkins, J. D. (2004). Positive youth development in the United gether to raise standards for all our children. New York: Free Press.
States: Research findings on evaluations of positive youth devel- Damon, W. (2004). What is positive youth development? Annals of the
opment programs. Annals of the American Academy of Political American Academy of Political and Social Science, 591(1), 13–24.
and Social Science, 591, 98–124. Damon, W., & Gregory, A. (2003). Bringing in a new era in the field
Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (2002). Response from authors to of youth development. In R. M. Lerner & P. L. Benson (Eds.), De-
comments on “Positive youth development in the United States: velopmental assets and asset-building communities: Implications
Research findings on evaluations of positive youth development for research, policy, and practice (pp. 47–64). New York: Kluwer
programs.” Prevention and Treatment, 5(article 20). Academic/ Plenum Press.
Catalano, R. F., Hawkins, J. D., Berglund, M. L., Pollard, J. A., & Davey, M., Eaker, D. G., & Walters, L. H. (2003). Resilience
Arthur, M. W. (2002). Prevention science and positive youth de- processes in adolescents: Personality profiles, self-worth, and
velopment: Competitive or cooperative frameworks? Journal of coping. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18(4), 347–362.
Adolescent Health, 31, 230–239. Dishion, T. J., Andrews, D. W., & Crosby, L. (1995). Antisocial boys
Catalano, R. F., & Kosterman, R. (1996). Modeling the etiology of and their friends in adolescence: Relationship characteristics, qual-
adolescent substance use: A test of the social development model. ity and interactional processes. Child Development, 66, 139–151.
Journal of Drug Issues, 26(2), 429–456. Dorgan, K. A., & Ferguson, R. F. (2004). Success factors in commu-
Catalano, R. F., Mazza, J. J., Harachi, T. W., Abbott, R. D., Haggerty, nity-wide initiatives for youth development. In S. F. Hamilton &
K. P., & Fleming, C. B. (2003). Raising healthy children through M. A. Hamilton (Eds.), The youth development handbook: Com-
enhancing social development in elementary school: Results after ing of age in American communities (pp. 271–300). Thousand
1.5 years. Journal of School Psychology, 41(2), 143–164. Oaks, CA: Sage.
Catterall, J. S. (1998). Risk and resilience in student transitions to Dryfoos, J. G. (1990). Adolescents at risk: Prevalence and prevention.
high school. American Journal of Education, 106(2), 302–334. New York: Oxford University Press.
Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1999). Goodness of fit: Clinical applica- Dryfoos, J. G. (1998). Safe passage: Making it through adolescence in
tions from infancy through adult life. Philadelphia: Brunner/ a risky society. New York: Oxford University Press.
Mazel. DuBois, D. L., Holloway, B. E., Valentine, J. C., & Cooper, H. (2002).
Coleman, J. S. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, MA: Effectiveness of mentoring programs for youth: A meta-analytic
Belknap Press. review. American Journal of Community Psychology, 30, 157–197.
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Heatherington, E. M., & Dubow, E. F., Arnett, M., Smith, K., & Ippolito, M. F. (2001). Predic-
Bornstein, M. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The tors of future expectations of inner-city children: A 9-month
case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218–232. prospective study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 21(1), 5–28.
Comer, J. (1980). School power. New York: Free Press. Dukes, R., & Stein, J. (2001). Effects of assets and deficits on the so-
Comer, J. P., Haynes, N. M., Joyner, E. T., & Ben-Avie, M. (1996). cial control of at risk behavior among youth. Youth and Society,
Rallying the whole village: The Comer process for reforming edu- 32(3), 337–360.
cation. New York: Teachers College Press. Durlak, J. A. (1998). Common risk and protective factors in success-
Conger, R. D., & Conger, K. J. (2002). Resilience in Midwestern fam- ful prevention programs. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
ilies: Selected findings from the first decade of a prospective, 68(4), 512–520.
longitudinal study. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 361–373. Durlak, J. A., & Wells, A. M. (1997). Primary prevention mental
Connell, J. P., Gambone, M. A., & Smith, T. J. (2001). Youth develop- health programs: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of
ment in community settings: Challenges to our field and our ap- Community Psychology, 25(2), 115–153.
proach. In P. L. Benson & K. J. Pittman (Eds.), Trends in youth Dworkin, J. B., Larson, R., & Hansen, D. (2003). Adolescents’ ac-
development: Visions, realities, and challenges (pp. 291–307). counts of growth experiences in youth activities. Journal of Youth
Boston: Kluwer Academic. and Adolescence, 32, 17–26.
936 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Earls, F., & Carlson, M. (2001). The social ecology of child health Gambone, M. A., Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2002). Finding out what
and well-being. Annual Review of Public Health, 22, 143–166. matters for youth: Testing key links in a community action frame-
Earls, F., & Carlson, M. (2002). Adolescents as collaborators: In work for youth development. Philadelphia: Youth Development
search of well-being. In M. Tienda & W. J. Wilson (Eds.), Youth in Strategies and Institute for Research and Reform in Education.
cities: A cross-national perspective (pp. 58–83) . New York: Cam- Garbarino, J. (1995). Raising children in a socially toxic environment.
bridge University Press. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Eccles, J. S. (1997). User-friendly science and mathematics: Can it Garmezy, N. (1983). Stress, coping and development in children. New
interest girls and minorities in breaking through the middle York: McGraw-Hill.
school wall. In D. Johnson (Ed.), Minorities and girls in school: Gerard, J. M., & Buehler, C. (2004). Cumulative environmental risk
Ef fects on achievement and performance: Vol. 1. Leaders in psy- and youth problem behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
chology (pp. 65–104). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 66, 702–720.
Eccles, J. S., & Barber, B. L. (1999). Student council, volunteering, Goldfried, M. R., & Bell, A. C. (2003). Extending the boundaries of
basketball, or marching band: What kind of extracurricular in- research on adolescent development. Journal of Clinical Child and
volvement matters? Journal of Adolescent Research, 14(1), 10–43. Adolescent Psychology, 32, 531–535.
Eccles, J. S., Early, D., Frasier, K., Belansky, E., & McCarthy, K. Gottlieb, G. (1997). Synthesizing nature-nurture: Prenatal roots of
(1997). The relation of connection, regulation, and support for au- instinctive behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
tonomy to adolescents’ functioning. Journal of Adolescent Re- Gottlieb, G., Wahlsten, D., & Lickliter, R. (1998). The significance
search, 12, 263–286. of biology for human development: A developmental psychobio-
Eccles, J. S., & Harold, R. D. (1996). Family involvement in chil- logical systems view. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M.
dren’s and adolescents’ schooling. In A. Booth & J. F. Dunn Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoreti-
(Eds.), Family-school links: How do they af fect educational out- cal models of human development (5th ed., pp. 233–273). New
comes? (pp. 3–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. York: Wiley.
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Meta-analysis of age and sex Granger, R. C. (2002). Creating the conditions linked to positive
differences in children’s and adolescents’ prosocial behavior. In youth development. New Directions for Youth Development, 95,
W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Hand- 149–164.
book of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality Greenberg, M. T., Weissberg, R. P., O’Brien, M. U., Zins, J. E.,
development (5th ed., pp. 701–778). New York: Wiley. Fredericks, L., Resnick, H., et al. (2003). Enhancing school-
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1974). Children of the great depression. Chicago: based prevention and youth development through coordinated so-
University of Chicago Press. cial, emotional, and academic learning. American Psychologist,
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1980). Adolescence in historical perspective. In J. 58(6/7), 466–474.
Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 3–46). Gutman, L. M., & Midgley, C. (2000). The role of protective factors
New York: Wiley. in supporting the academic achievement of poor African Ameri-
can students during the middle school transition. Journal of Youth
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1998). The life course and human development. In
and Adolescence, 29(2), 223–248.
W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Hand-
book of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human de- Gutman, L. M., Sameroff, A. J., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The aca-
velopment (5th ed., pp. 939–991). New York: Wiley. demic achievement of African American students during early
adolescence: An examination of multiple risk, promotive, and
Elder, G. H., Jr. (1999). Children of the great depression: Social change
protective factors. American Journal of Community Psychology,
in life experience (25th anniv. ed.). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
30(3), 367–399.
Feshbach, N. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and social behavior
Hahn, A., & Lanspery, S. (2001). Change that abides: A retrospective
in children. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial
look at five community and family strengthening projects, and
behavior (pp. 11–47). New York: Academic Press.
their enduring results. Waltham, MA: Brandeis University.
Ford, D. H., & Lerner, R. M. (1992). Developmental systems theory:
Halfon, N. (2003). Afterword: Toward an asset-based policy agenda
An integrative approach. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
for children, families, and communities. In R. M. Lerner & P. L.
Fukuyama, F. (1999). The great disruption: Human nature and the re- Benson (Eds.), Developmental assets and asset-building communi-
constitution of social order. New York: Free Press. ties: Implications for research, policy, and practice (pp. 223–229).
Furlong, M., Paige, L. Z., & Osher, D. (2003). The Safe Schools/ New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Press.
Healthy Students (SS/HS) Initiative: Lessons learned from imple- Hamburg, D. A., & Takanishi, R. (1996). Great transitions: Prepar-
menting comprehensive youth development programs. Psychology ing American youth for the 21st century—The role of research.
in the Schools, 40, 447–456. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6(4), 379–396.
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr. (2000). The sociology of adolescence and youth Hamilton, S. (1999). A three-part definition of youth development.
in the 1990s: A critical commentary. Journal of Marriage and the Unpublished manuscript, Cornell University College of Human
Family, 62, 896–910. Ecology, Ithaca, NY.
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Modell, J., & Herschberg, T. (1976). Social Hamilton, S. F., & Hamilton, M. A. (2004). The youth development
change and transitions to adulthood in historical perspective. handbook: Coming of age in American communities. Thousand
Journal of Family History, 1(1), 7–32. Oaks, CA: Sage.
Galambos, N., & Turner, P. (1999). Parent and adolescent tempera- Hamilton, S. F., Hamilton, M. A., & Pittman, K. (2004). Principles
ment and the quality of parent-adolescent relations. Merrill- for youth development. In S. F. Hamilton & H. M. A. Hamilton
Palmer Quarterly, 45, 493–511. (Eds.), The youth development handbook: Coming of age in Amer-
Gambone, M. A., & Arbreton, A. J. A. (1997). Safe havens: The con- ican communities (pp. 3–22). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
tributions of youth organizations to healthy adolescent develop- Havighurst, R. J. (1953). Human development and education. London:
ment. Philadelphia: Public/ Private Ventures. Longmans, Green.
References 937

Hawkins, J. D., & Catalano, R. F. (1996). Parents who care: A step- Leffert, N., Benson, P. L., & Roehlkepartain, J. L. (1997). Starting
by-step guide for families with teens. San Francisco: Jossey- out right: Developmental assets for children. Minneapolis, MN:
Bass/ Pfeiffer. Search Institute.
Heckhausen, J. (1999). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Age- Leffert, N., Benson, P. L., Scales, P. C., Sharma, A. R., Drake, D. R.,
normative and sociocultural constraints as adaptive challenges. & Blyth, D. A. (1998). Developmental assets: Measurement and
New York: Cambridge University Press. prediction of risk behaviors among adolescents. Applied Develop-
Heckhausen, J., & Krueger, J. (1993). Developmental expectations for mental Science, 2(4), 209–230.
the self and most other people: Age grading in three functions of Lerner, R. M. (1976). Concepts and theories of human development.
social comparison. Developmental Psychology, 29, 539–548. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York:
Psychological Review, 102, 284–304. Cambridge University Press.
Hein, K. (2003). Enhancing the assets for positive youth develop- Lerner, R. M. (1995). America’s youth in crisis: Challenges and op-
ment: The vision, values, and action agenda of the W. T. Grant tions for programs and policies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Foundation. In R. M. Lerner & P. L. Benson (Eds.), Developmen- Lerner, R. M. (1998). Theories of human development: Contemporary
tal assets and asset-building communities: Implications for re- perspectives. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner
search, policy, and practice (pp. 97–117). New York: Kluwer (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical mod-
Academic/ Plenum Press. els of human development (5th ed., pp. 1–24). New York: Wiley.
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implica- Lerner, R. M. (2000). Developing civil society through the promotion
tions for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University of positive youth development. Journal of Developmental and Be-
Press. havioral Pediatrics, 21, 48–49.
Hollister-Wagner, G. H., Foshee, V. A., & Jackson, C. (2001). Adoles- Lerner, R. M. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development
cent aggression: Models of resiliency. Journal of Applied Social (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology, 31(3), 445–466. Lerner, R. M. (2003). Developmental assets and asset-building com-
Huebner, A. J., & Betts, S. C. (2002). Exploring the utility of social munities: A view of the issues. In R. M. Lerner & P. L. Benson
control theory for youth development: Issues of attachment, in- (Eds.), Developmental assets and asset-building communities: Im-
volvement, and gender. Youth and Society, 34(2), 123–145. plications for research, policy, and practice (pp. 3–18). New York:
Hughes, D., & Curnan, S. P. (2000). Community youth development: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Press.
A framework for action. CYD Journal, 1(1), 7–13. Lerner, R. M. (2004). Liberty: Thriving and civic engagement among
Hunter, J. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2003). The positive psychology America’s youth. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
of interested adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, Lerner, R. M., Anderson, P. M., Balsano, A. B., Dowling, E. M., &
27–35. Bobek, D. L. (2003). Applied developmental science of positive
Hyman, J. B. (2002). Exploring social capital and civic engagement to human development. In R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, & J.
create a framework for community building. Applied Developmen- Mistry (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 6. Developmental
tal Science, 6(4), 196–202. psychology (pp. 535–558). New York: Wiley.
Jessor, R. (1993). Successful adolescent development among youth in Lerner, R. M., & Benson, P. L. (2003). Developmental assets and
high-risk settings. American Psychologist, 48(2), 117–126. asset-building communities: Implications for research, policy, and
practice. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic.
Jessor, R., Turbin, M. S., & Costa, F. M. (1998). Risk and protection
Lerner, R. M., Brentano, C., Dowling, E. M., & Anderson, P. M.
in successful outcomes among disadvantaged adolescents. Applied
(2002). Positive youth development: Thriving as the basis of per-
Developmental Science, 2(4), 194–208.
sonhood and civil society. New Directions for Youth Development:
Jessor, R., Van Den Bos, J., Vanderryn, J., Costa, F. M., & Turbin, Pathways to Positive Development Among Diverse Youth, 95, 11–33.
M. S. (1995). Protective factors in adolescent problem behavior:
Lerner, R. M., Dowling, E. M., & Anderson, P. M. (2003). Positive
Moderator effects and developmental change. Developmental Psy-
youth development: Thriving as the basis of personhood and civil
chology, 31(6), 923–933.
society. Applied Developmental Science, 7(3), 172–180.
Keyes, C. L. M. (2003). Complete mental health: An agenda for the
Lerner, R. M., Fisher, C. B., & Weinberg, R. A. (2000). Toward a sci-
21st century. In C. L. M. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing:
ence for and of the people: Promoting civil society through the ap-
Positive psychology and the life well-lived (pp. 293–312). Wash-
plication of developmental science. Child Development, 71, 11–20.
ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Lerner, R. M., Freund, A. M., De Stefanis, I., & Habermas, T. (2001).
King, P. E., Dowling, E. M., Mueller, R. A., White, K., Schultz, W., Understanding developmental regulation in adolescence: The use
Osborn, P., et al. (in press). Thriving in adolescence: The voices of the selection, optimization, and compensation model. Human
of youth-serving practitioners, parents, and early and late adoles- Development, 44, 29–50.
cents. Journal of Early Adolescence.
Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., Alermigi, J., Theokas, C., Phelps, E.,
Kreipe, R. E., Ryan, S. A., & Seibold-Simpson, S. M. (2004). Youth Gestsdottir, S., et al. (in press). Positive youth development, par-
development and health. In S. F. Hamilton & M. A. Hamilton ticipation in community youth development programs, and com-
(Eds.), The youth development handbook: Coming of age in Amer- munity contributions of fifth grade adolescents: Findings from
ican communities (pp. 103–126). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. the first wave of the 4-H Study of Positive Youth Development.
Kretzmann, J., & McKnight, J. (1993). Building communities from the Journal of Early Adolescence.
inside out: A path toward finding and mobilizing a community’s Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., De Stefanis, I., & Apfel, A. (2001). Un-
assets. Chicago: ACTA Publication. derstanding developmental systems in adolescence: Implications
Larson, R. W. (2000). Toward a psychology of positive youth develop- for methodological strategies, data analytic approaches, and
ment. American Psychologist, 55(1), 170–183. training. Journal of Adolescent Research, 16, 9–27.
938 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Lerner, R. M., Wertlieb, D., & Jacobs, F. (2003). Historical and theo- Masten, A. S., Hubbard, J. J., Gest, S. D., Tellegen, A., Garmezy,
retical bases of applied developmental science. In R. M. Lerner, N., & Ramirez, M. L. (1999). Competence in the context of ad-
D. Wertlieb, & F. Jacobs (Eds.), Handbook of applied developmen- versity: Pathways to resilience and maladaptation from child-
tal science: Vol. 1. Applying developmental science for youth and hood to late adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 11,
families—Historical and theoretical foundations (pp. 1–28). Thou- 143–169.
sand Oaks, CA: Sage. McLaughlin, M. W., Irby, M. A., & Langman, J. (1994). Urban sanc-
Li, X., Stanton, B., Pack, R., Harris, C., Cottrell, L., & Burns, J. tuaries: Neighborhood organizations in the lives and futures of
(2002). Risk and protective factors associated with gang involve- inner-city youth. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
ment among urban African American adolescents. Youth and Soci- McLellan, J. A., & Youniss, J. (2003). Two systems of youth service:
ety, 34(2), 172–194. Determinants of voluntary and required youth community ser-
Lonczak, H. S., Abbott, R. D., Hawkins, J. D., Kosterman, R., & vice. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 47–58.
Catalano, R. F. (2002). Effects of the Seattle Social Development Metz, E., McLellan, J., & Youniss, J. (2003). Types of voluntary ser-
Project on sexual behavior, pregnancy, birth, and sexually trans- vice and adolescents’ civic development. Journal of Adolescent Re-
mitted disease outcomes by age 21 years. Archives of Pediatric search, 18, 188–203.
and Adolescent Medicine, 156, 438–447. Miller, W. R., & Thoresen, C. E. (2003). Spirituality, religion,
Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and modification of behavior. Chicago: and health: An emerging research field. American Psycholo-
University of Chicago Press. gist, 58, 24–35.
Lorion, R. P., & Sokoloff, H. (2003). Building assets in real-world Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited
communities. In R. M. Lerner & P. L. Benson (Eds.), Developmen- antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological
tal assets and asset-building communities (pp. 121–156). New Review, 100, 674–701.
York: Kluwer Academic. Montemayor, R., Adams, G. R., & Gullotta, T. P. (2000). Advances in
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of re- adolescent development: Vol. 10. Adolescent diversity in ethnic,
silience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. economic, and cultural contexts. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Child Development, 71(3), 543–562. Moore, K. A. (1997). Criteria for indicators of child well-being. In
MacDonald, G. B., & Valdivieso, R. (2001). Measuring deficits and R. M. Hauser, B. V. Brown, & W. R. Prosser (Eds.), Indicators
assets: How we track youth development now, and how we should of children’s well-being (pp. 36–44). New York: Russell Sage
track it. In P. L. Benson & K. J. Pittman (Eds.), Trends in youth Foundation.
development: Visions, realities, and challenges (pp. 155–186). Moore, K. A., & Glei, D. (1995). Taking the plunge: An examination
Boston: Kluwer Press. of positive youth development. Journal of Adolescent Research,
Maddox, S. J., & Prinz, R. J. (2003). School bonding in children and 10(1), 15–40.
adolescents: Conceptualization, assessment, and associated vari- Moore, K. A., & Lippman, L. (2004). What do children need to f lour-
ables. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 6(1), 31–49. ish? Conceptualizing and measuring indicators of positive develop-
Madsen, M. C. (1971). Developmental and cross-cultural differences ment. New York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Press.
in the cooperative and competitive behavior of young children. Moore, K. A., Lippman, L., & Brown, B. (2004). Indicators of child
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 2, 365–371. well-being: The promise for positive youth development. The An-
Mahoney, J. L., Cairns, B. D., & Farmer, T. W. (2003). Promoting nals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science,
interpersonal competence and educational success through ex- 591, 125–147.
tracurricular activity participation. Journal of Educational Psy- Mortimer, J., & Larson, R. (2002). The changing adolescent experi-
chology, 95, 409–418. ence: Societal trends and the transition to adulthood. New York:
Mannes, M., Benson, P. L., Kretzmann, J., & Norris, T. (2003). The Cambridge University Press.
American tradition of community development: Implications for Munóz, R. F., Mrazek, P. J., & Haggerty, R. J. (1996). Institute of
guiding community engagement in youth development. In R. M. Medicine report on prevention of mental disorders: Summary and
Lerner, F. Jacobs, & D. Wertlieb (Eds.), Handbook of applied devel- commentary. American Psychologist, 51(11), 1116–1122.
opmental science: Promoting positive child, adolescent, and family Nakamura, J. (2001). The nature of vital engagement in adulthood. In
development through research, policies and programs: Vol. 1. Apply- M. Michaelson & J. Nakamura (Eds.), Supportive frameworks for
ing developmental science for youth and families—Historical and youth engagement: New directions for child and adolescent devel-
theoretical foundations (pp. 469–499). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. opment, No. 93. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Mannes, M., Lewis, S., Hintz, N., Foster, K., & Nakkula, M. (2002). National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. (2002). Com-
Cultivating developmentally attentive communities: A report on the munity programs to promote youth development: Committee on
first wave of the National Asset-Building Case Study Project. Min- community-level programs for youth (J. Eccles & J. A. Gootman,
neapolis, MN: Search Institute. Eds.) [Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education,
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in devel- Board on Children, Youth and Families]. Washington, DC: Na-
opment. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. tional Academy Press.
Masten, A. S., & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of Nesselroade, J. R. (1977). Issues in studying developmental change in
competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons adults from a multivariate perspective. In J. E. Birren & K. W.
from research on successful children. American Psychologist, Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 59–69).
53(2), 205–220. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Masten, A. S., & Garmezy, N. (1985). Risk, vulnerability and protec- O’Donnell, L., Stueve, A., San Doval, A., Duran, R., Atnafou, R.,
tive factors in developmental psychology. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Haber, D., et al. (1999). Violence prevention and young adoles-
Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in child clinical psychology (Vol. 8, cents’ participation in community youth service—Implications
pp. 1–52). New York: Plenum Press. for intervention. Journal of Adolescent Health, 24(1), 28–37.
References 939

O’Donoghue, J. L., Kirshner, B., & McLaughlin, M. (2002). Introduc- Rhodes, J. E., & Roffman, J. G. (2003). Nonparental adults as devel-
tion: Moving youth participation forward. New Directions for opmental assets in the lives of youth. In R. M. Lerner & P. L. Ben-
Youth Development, 96, 15–26. son (Eds.), Developmental assets and asset-building communities:
Overton, W. F. (1998). Developmental psychology: Philosophy, con- Implications for research, policy, and practice (pp. 195–212).
cepts, and methodology. In W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Boston: Kluwer Academic.
Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoreti- Rodriguez, M. C., & Morrobel, D. (2004). A review of Latino youth
cal models of human development (5th ed., pp. 107–187). New development research and a call for an asset orientation. Hispanic
York: Wiley. Journal of Behavioral Science, 26(2), 107–127.
Pancer, M., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Loiselle, L. (2002). Youth confer- Roehlkepartain, E. C., Benson, P. L., & Sesma, A. (2003). Signs of
ences as a context for engagement. New Directions for Youth De- progress in putting children first: Developmental assets among
velopment, 96, 47–64. youth in St. Louis Park, 1997–2001. Minneapolis, MN: Search
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. (1992). Antisocial boys: A Institute.
social interactional approach (Vol. 4). Eugene, OR: Castalia. Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development
Paulson, S. E., Marchant, G. J., & Rothlisberg, B. A. (1998). Early in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.
adolescents’ perceptions of patterns of parenting, teaching, and Roth, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). What do adolescents need for
school atmosphere: Implications for achievement. Journal of Early healthy development: Implications for youth policy. Social Policy
Adolescence, 18, 5–26. Report, 14(1), 3–19.
Perkins, D. F., Borden, L. M., Keith, J. G., Hoppe-Rooney, T., & Vil- Roth, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2003). What exactly is a youth develop-
larruel, F. A. (2003). Community youth development: A partner- ment program? Answers from research and practice. Applied De-
ship for creating a positive world. In F. A. Villarruel, D. F. velopmental Science, 7(2), 94–111.
Perkins, L. M. Borden, & J. G. Keith (Eds.), Community youth de-
Roth, J., Brooks-Gunn, J., Murray, L., & Foster, W. (1998). Promot-
velopment: Practice, policy, and research (pp. 1–23). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage. ing healthy adolescents: Synthesis of youth development program
evaluations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 8(4), 423–459.
Perkins, D. F., Borden, L. M., & Villarruel, F. A. (2001). Community
youth development: A partnership for change. School Community Rowe, D. C., Vazsonyi, A. T., & Flannery, D. J. (1994). No more than
Journal, 11, 39–56. skin deep: Ethnic and racial similarity in developmental process.
Psychological Review, 101(3), 396–413.
Peterson, C. (2004). Positive social science. Annals of the American
Academy of Political and Social Science, 591, 186–201. Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mecha-
nisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57(3), 316–331.
Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Supportive parent-
ing, ecological context, and children’s adjustment: A 7-year longi- Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1998). The contours of positive human
tudinal study. Child Development, 68, 908–923. health. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 1–28.
Pittman, K. J., & Fleming, W. P. (1991). A new vision: Promoting Sampson, R. J. (2001). How do communities undergird or undermine
youth development (Testimony of Karen J. Pittman before the human development? Relevant contexts and social mechanisms. In
House Select Committee on Children, Youth, and Families). Wash- A. Booth & A. C. Crouter (Eds.), Does it take a village? Commu-
ington, DC: Center for Youth Development and Policy Research. nity ef fects on children, adolescents, and families (p. 3). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Pittman, K. J., Irby, M., & Ferber, T. (2000). Unfinished business:
Further ref lections on a decade of promoting youth development. Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J., & Earls, F. (1999). Beyond social capi-
In Public/ Private Ventures (Ed.), Youth development: Issues, tal: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American
challenges and directions (pp. 17–64). Philadelphia: Public/ Pri- Sociological Review, 64, 633–660.
vate Ventures. Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. C. (1997). Neighbor-
Pittman, K. J., Irby, M., & Ferber, T. (2001). Unfinished business: hoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy.
Further ref lections on a decade of promoting youth develop- Science, 277, 918–924.
ment. In P. L. Benson & K. Pittman (Eds.), Trends in youth de- Sanders, M. G. (1998). The effects of school, family, and community
velopment: Visions, realities and challenges (pp. 3–50). Boston: support on the academic achievement of African American ado-
Kluwer Academic. lescents. Urban Education, 33, 385–409.
Pittman, K. J., Irby, M., Tolman, J., Yohalem, N., & Ferber, T. (2001). Scales, P. C. (1991). A portrait of young adolescents in the 1990s: Im-
Preventing problems, promoting development, encouraging engage- plications for promoting healthy growth and development. Min-
ment: Competing priorities or inseparable goals? Retrieved on Au- neapolis, MN: Search Institute.
gust 31, 2005, from www.forumforyouthinvestment.org. Scales, P. C. (2001). The public image of adolescents. Social Science
Public/ Private Ventures. (2000). Youth development: Issues, chal- and Modern Society, 38(4), 64–70.
lenges and directions. Philadelphia: Public/ Private Ventures. Scales, P. C. (2003). Other people’s kids: Social expectations and
Quinn, J. (1999). Where need meets opportunity: Youth development American adults’ involvement with children and adolescents. New
programs for early teens. Future of Children, 9(2), 96–116. York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Press.
Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K. E., Harris, Scales, P. C., & Benson, P. L. (2004). Prosocial orientation and com-
K. M., & Jones, J. (1997). Protecting adolescents from harm: Find- munity service. In K. A. Moore & L. Lippman (Eds.), What do
ings from the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health. children need to f lourish (pp. 339–356). New York: Kluwer Aca-
Journal of the American Medical Association, 278(10), 823–832. demic/ Plenum Press.
Rhodes, J. E., Grossman, J. B., & Resch, N. R. (2000). Agents of change: Scales, P. C., Benson, P. L., Leffert, N., & Blyth, D. A. (2000). Con-
Pathways through which mentoring relationships inf luence adoles- tribution of developmental assets to the prediction of thriving
cents’ academic adjustment. Child Development, 71, 1662–1671. among adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 4(1), 27–46.
940 Positive Youth Development: Theory, Research, and Applications

Scales, P. C., Benson, P. L., & Mannes, M. (2002). Grading grown-ups Simpson, A. R., & Roehlkepartain, J. L. (2003). Asset building in
2002: How do American kids and adults relate? Minneapolis, MN: parenting practices and family life. In R. M. Lerner & P. L. Ben-
Search Institute. son (Eds.), Developmental assets and asset-building communities:
Scales, P. C., Benson, P. L., Roehlkepartain, E. C., Hintz, N. R., Sul- Implications for research, policy, and practice (pp. 157–193). New
livan, T. K., Mannes, M., et al. (2001). The role of neighborhood York: Kluwer Academic/ Plenum Press.
and community in building developmental assets for children and Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton.
youth: A national study of social norms among American adults. Small, S., & Memmo, M. (2004). Contemporary models of youth
Journal of Community Psychology, 29(6), 703–727. development and problem prevention: Toward an integration of
Scales, P. C., Benson, P. L., Roehlkepartain, E. C., Sesma, A., Jr., & terms, concepts, and models. Family Relations, 53, 3–11.
van Dulmen, M. (in press). The role of developmental assets in Spencer, M. B. (1995). Old issues and new theorizing about African
predicting academic achievement: A longitudinal study. Journal American youth: A phenomenological variant of ecological systems
of Adolescence. theory. In R. L. Taylor (Ed.), Black youth: Perspectives on their sta-
Scales, P. C., Blyth, D. A., Berkas, T. H., & Kielsmeier, J. C. (2000). tus in the United States (pp. 37–69). Westport, CT: Praeger.
The effects of service-learning on middle school students’ social Spencer, M. B. (1999). Social and cultural inf luences on school ad-
responsibility and academic success. Journal of Early Adoles- justment: The application of an identity-focused cultural ecologi-
cence, 20(3), 332–359. cal perspective. Educational Psychologist, 34, 43–57.
Scales, P. C., & Leffert, N. (1999). Developmental assets: A synthesis
Spencer, M. B. (2001). Research on African-American adolescents.
of the scientific research on adolescent development. Minneapolis,
In J. V. Lerner & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Adolescence in America: An
MN: Search Institute.
encyclopedia (pp. 30–32). Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.
Scales, P. C., & Leffert, N. (2004). Developmental assets: A synthesis
Spencer, M. B., Dupree, D., & Hartmann, T. (1997). A Phenome-
of the scientific research on adolescent development (2nd ed.).
nological Variant of Ecological Systems Theory (PVEST): A
Minneapolis, MN: Search Institute.
self-organization perspective in context. Development and
Scales, P. C., Leffert, N., & Vraa, R. (2003). The relation of commu- Psychopathology, 9, 817–833.
nity developmental attentiveness to adolescent health. American
Spilka, G. (2004). On community-based evaluations: Two trends.
Journal of Health Behavior, 27(Suppl. 1), S22–S34.
Evaluation Exchange, 9(4), 6. (Harvard Family Research Project)
Scales, P. C., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2003). Boosting student Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development: Early
achievement: New research on the power of developmental assets. care, attachment, and subsequent developmental issues. American
Search Institute Insights and Evidence, 1(1), 1–10. Psychologist, 34, 834–841.
Scales, P. C., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2004). Service to others: A Steinberg, L., & Lerner, R. M. (2004). The scientific study of ado-
“gateway” asset for school success and healthy development. In lescence: A brief history. Journal of Early Adolescence, 24(1),
National Youth Leadership Council (Eds.), Growing to greatness: 45–54.
The state of service-learning report (pp. 26–32). Saint Paul: Na-
Takanishi, R. (1993). An agenda for the integration of research and
tional Youth Leadership Council.
policy during early adolescence. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Early
Scales, P. C., Sesma, A., & Bolstrom, B. (2004). Coming into their adolescence: Perspectives on research, policy, and intervention
own: How developmental assets promote positive growth in middle (pp. 457–469). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
childhood. Minneapolis, MN: Search Institute.
Taylor, C. S., Lerner, R. M., von Eye, A., Bobek, D. L., Balsano,
Schaeffer, C. M., Petras, H., Ialongo, N., Poduska, J., & Kellam, S. A. B., Dowling, E., et al. (2002). Stability of attributes of positive
(2003). Modeling growth in boys’ aggressive behavior across ele- functioning and of developmental assets among African American
mentary school: Links to later criminal involvement, conduct dis- adolescent gang members and community-based organization
order, and antisocial personality disorder. Developmental members. New Directions for Youth Development, 95, 35–55.
Psychology, 39, 1020–1035.
Taylor, C. S., Lerner, R. M., von Eye, A., Bobek, D. L., Balsano,
Schaie, K. W. (1965). A general model for the study of developmental A. B., Dowling, E., et al. (2003). Positive individual and social be-
problems. Psychological Bulletin, 64, 92–107. havior among gang and non-gang African American male adoles-
Schneirla, T. C. (1957). The concept of development in comparative cents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18, 496–522.
psychology. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development: An Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In W.
issue in the study of human behavior (pp. 78–108). Minneapolis: Damon (Series Ed.) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook on child
University of Minnesota Press. psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th
Schneirla, T. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of ed., pp. 563–634). New York: Wiley.
biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. R. Theokas, C., Almerigi, J., Lerner, R. M., Dowling, E. M., Benson,
Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 1–42). Lin- P. L., Scales, P. C., et al. (2004). Conceptualizing and modeling
coln: University of Nebraska Press. individual and ecological asset components of thriving in early
Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychol- adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence.
ogy: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55(1), 5–14. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New
Sesma, A., Jr., Mannes, M., & Scales, P. C. (in press). Positive adap- York: Brunner/ Mazel.
tation, resilience, and the developmental assets framework. In S. Tierney, P., Grossman, J. B., & Resch, N. L. (1995). Making a dif fer-
Goldstein & R. Brooks (Eds.), Handbook of resilience in children. ence: An impact study of Big Brothers/ Big Sisters. Philadelphia:
New York: Springer. Public/ Private Ventures.
Sesma, A., & Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2003). Unique strengths, shared Tolan, P. H., Gorman-Smith, D., & Henry, D. B. (2003). The develop-
strengths: Developmental assets among youth of color. Search In- mental ecology of urban males’ youth violence. Developmental
stitute Insights and Evidence, 1(2), 1–13. Psychology, 39, 274–291.
References 941

Tolman, J., & Pittman, K. (2001). Youth acts, community impacts: positive youth development through community-based program-
Stories of youth engagement with real results. Tahoma Park, MD: ming. Battle Creek, MI: Author. Retrieved September 22, 2004,
The Forum for Youth Investment. from http://www.wkkf.org/ Pubs/ YouthEd/KYIP/ Pub3174.pdf.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1999). Trends in W. T. Grant Foundation. (1988). The forgotten half: Pathways to suc-
the well-being of America’s children and youth. Washington, DC: cess for America’s youth and young families. New York: W. T.
U.S. Government Printing Office. Grant Foundation.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Administration for Wynn, J. (1997). Primary supports, schools, and other sectors: Impli-
Children and Families. (1996). Reconnecting youth and commu- cations for learning and civic life. Paper presented at the Harvard
nity: A youth development approach. Washington, DC: U.S. Gov- Project on Schooling and Children. Chicago, IL: Chapin Hall Cen-
ernment Printing Office. ter for Children.
Udry, J. R., Li, R. M., & Hendrickson-Smith, J. (2003). Health and Yates, M., & Youniss, J. (1996). A developmental perspective on com-
behavior risks of adolescents with mixed-race identity. American munity service in adolescence. Social Development, 5, 85–111.
Journal of Public Health, 93, 1865–1870. Youniss, J., McLellan, J., & Yates, M. (1997). What we know
Villarruel, F. A., Perkins, D. F., Borden, L. M., & Keith, J. G. (2003). about engendering civic identity. American Behavioral Scien-
Community youth development: Practice, policy, and research. tist, 40, 620– 631.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Zaff, J., & Michelsen, E. (2002). Encouraging civic engagement: How
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher teens are (or are not) becoming responsible citizens. Washington,
educational processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. DC: Child Trends.
Souberman, Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Zaff, J. F., Moore, K. A., Papillo, A. R., & Williams, S. (2003). Im-
Wandersman, A., & Florin, P. (2003). Community interventions and plications of extracurricular activity participation during ado-
effective prevention. American Psychologist, 58, 441–448. lescence on positive outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Research,
Wang, M. C., Hartel, G. D., & Walberg, H. J. (1990). What inf luences 18, 599–630.
learning? A content analysis of review literature. Journal of Edu- Zeldin, S. (1995). Community-university collaborations for youth de-
cational Research, 84, 30–43. velopment: From theory to practice. Journal of Adolescent Re-
Way, N., & Robinson, M. G. (2003). A longitudinal study of the ef- search, 10(4), 449–469.
fects of family, friends, and school experiences on the psycholog- Zeldin, S. (2000). Integrating research and practice to understand
ical adjustment of ethnic minority, low-SES adolescents. Journal and strengthen communities for adolescent development: An in-
of Adolescent Research, 18(4), 324–346. troduction to the special issue and current issues. Applied Devel-
Weissberg, R. P., Kumpfer, K. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2003). Pre- opmental Science, 4(Suppl. 1), 2–10.
vention that works for children and youth. American Psychologist, Zeldin, S. (2002). Sense of community and positive adult beliefs
58, 425–432. toward adolescents and youth policy in urban neighborhoods
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1989). Vulnerable but invincible: A and small cities. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31(5),
Longitudinal Study of Resilient Children and Youth. New York: 331–342.
Adams, Bannister and Cox. Zeldin, S. (2004). Youth as agents of adult and community develop-
West, D. D. (1974, December). Youth development: A new look at an ment: Mapping the processes and outcomes of youth engaged in or-
old concept. Youth Reporter, 5–8. ganizational governance. Applied Developmental Science, 8(2),
Whitlock, J. L. (2003). Voice, visibility, place, and power: Correlates 75–90.
of school and community connectedness among eighth, tenth and Zeldin, S., Kimball, M., & Price, L. (1995). What are the day-to-day
twelfth grade youth. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell experiences that promote youth development? An annotated bibli-
University. ography of research on adolescents and their families. Washington,
Whitlock, J. L., & Hamilton, S. F. (2003). The role of youth surveys DC: Academy for Educational Development, Center for Youth De-
in community youth development initiatives. Applied Developmen- velopment and Policy Research.
tal Science, 7(1), 39–51. Zweig, J. M., Phillips, S. D., & Lindberg, L. D. (2002). Predicting
W. K. Kellogg Foundation. (n.d.). Kellogg Youth Initiative Partner- adolescent profiles of risk: Looking beyond demographics. Jour-
ships 1978–1997: Lessons learned from the crucial first decade of nal of Adolescent Health, 31(4), 343–353.
CHAPTER 17

Religious and Spiritual Development


throughout the Life Span
FRITZ K. OSER, W. GEORGE SCARLETT, and ANTON BUCHER

A DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACH 943 CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT IN RELIGIOUS AND


IS RELIGIOUS AND SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT 972
RESISTANT TO THE POSTMODERN PATH? 944 Studies Carried Out from a
A HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE 946 Stage-Structural Approach 972
Religious and Spiritual Development as Maturing 947 Studies Carried Out from a
Religious and Spiritual Development as Coping: Cognitive-Cultural Approach 975
Psychoanalytic Theory 948 A Synthesis 976
Religious and Spiritual Development as Perfecting 951 RELIGIOUS AND SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT
DEFINING RELIGIOUS AND IN CONTEXT 976
SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT 952 The Family Context 977
Religion, Religiousness, and Spirituality 953 Religious Schools and Religious Education 980
Content, Form, and Function 954 Congregations 981
Stage and Structure 956 POSITIVE CORRELATES OF RELIGIOUS AND
STAGE-STRUCTURAL THEORIES 957 SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT 982
Cognitive-Stage Theories: Ronald Goldman 958 Health and Well-Being 982
Faith Developmental Theory: James Fowler 959 Moral Development 983
Stages of Religious Judgment and Reasoning: Fritz Oser Coping 984
and Paul Gmünder 961 Positive Correlates in Advanced Age 985
The Relational and Contextual Model of Development: NEGATIVE CORRELATES AND PATHOLOGY 985
Helmut Reich 965 Cults, Occultist Practices, and Terrorist Groups 987
STAGE-STRUCTURAL THEORIES: CONCERNS 965 Mental Disorders 989
ALTERNATIVES TO STAGE-STRUCTURAL Pathogenic (Toxic) Religious Beliefs, Practices
THEORIES 967 and Attitudes 989
Susan Kwilecki’s Substantive-Functional Approach 967 CONCLUSIONS 990
The Spiritual Child Movement 968 REFERENCES 991
Cognitive-Cultural Theories 970
Developmental Systems Theories 971

A young man meets his former professor and relates that is indeed a gift, a gift given by the two people involved
he has recently fallen in love. He says he believes his and by God. He also suggests that a development such as
falling in love is a matter of destiny, a gift given by this always has a hidden, transcendent meaning.
chance. The older man listens intently and then suggests In this sketch of a conversation, we are confronted
this experience cannot have happened by chance, that it with judgments about the meaning of an important life
event—falling in love. One judgment seems to have no
The authors would like to thank Kathryn Tabone, Mitchael connection to religion or spirituality, while the other
Steorts, and Alexis Gerber for their help in preparing the seems to be very much connected to religion. It is
manuscript. judgments such as these that theories of religious de-
Preparation of this chapter was supported by a grant from velopment must explain, and in such a way that we
the Templeton Foundation. understand individual change, transformations, pro-

942
A Developmental Approach 943

gression, and regression as well as commonalities ternative conceptions of endpoints of development—in-


across persons, groups, and ages. cluding the alternative of no definite endpoint.
Thirty years ago, theories of religious development Thus, this chapter aims to examine the emergence
avoided explaining issues expressed in our scene—issues and growth of religious and spiritual development
about faith and transcendence. Rather, theories of reli- from a variety of perspectives, but with a special em-
gious development applied a cognitive, Piagetian ap- phasis on those with a transformative view. It does so
proach to concepts and knowledge—for example, in using two overarching paradigms or frameworks. The
explaining how children understand the concept of God first is the organismic or cognitive—structural para-
(Goldman, 1964). digm associated with the stage theories of Piaget,
Religious thinking was seen as an application of gen- Werner, and Kohlberg. The second is the developmen-
eral cognitive structures, such that lower levels of reli- tal systems paradigm that has gained interest over the
gious thinking were taken as expressions of immature, past 2 decades (Lerner, 2002). In one sense, the devel-
magical, and egocentric thinking. In doing so, re- opmental systems paradigm includes the organismic
searchers on religious and spiritual development gave no paradigm such that they operate on different concep-
value to lower stage constructions of meaning. They saw tual levels. In theory the two are compatible, and we
nothing in the way lower stages can provide for a grow- use them in this way, as theoretically compatible.
ing religious and spiritual rationality. In this chapter, we However, in practice, those adopting a developmental
explore theories providing explanations of religious and systems paradigm have often settled on simple defini-
spiritual development quite different from that provided tions of religious and spiritual development. The
by Piagetian stage theory. strength and contribution of the organismic paradigm
Here is another story to help define our area of in- is that it forces attention on the problem of definition.
quiry: On arrival at a birthday party, a young woman no- This is a major reason for introducing the concept of
tices candles in front of every invited guest. After a stage. We use the organismic paradigm to better define
while, the host asks everyone present to concentrate on religious and spiritual development—and later on, use
the candles, to become quiet, to close their eyes and then mostly a developmental systems paradigm to discuss
to share a good wish for the birthday person. The room the literature on contexts, situatedness inf luences, sup-
is silent, and then, when wishes are shared, every word ports, outcomes, and ef fects.
is valued. What happens here is a form of going into as The chapter examines the following major questions
well as beyond oneself—which is what many take to be in particular:
something spiritual.
As with religious development, spiritual development • How should we define religious and spiritual devel-
is also related to change and transformation, to progres- opment?
sion and regression. However, if separated from reli-
• What is universal and what is culturally situated with
gious development, the contents of spiritual development
respect to religious and spiritual development?
seem less fixed, and the steps toward higher, more com-
• What triggers, causes, and supports religious and
plex levels seem less evident. There is today no theory of
spiritual development?
pure spiritual development. That is why in this chapter,
we explore alternative theories of spiritual growth, and • What evidence is there for religious and spiritual de-
why we use religious and spiritual as highly overlapping velopment promoting character development, avoid-
entities, which can be taken mostly together. ing risky behavior, and fostering thriving in general?
We believe that religious and spiritual development • What evidence do we have for religious and spiritual
is a normative task. As such, developing religiously development promoting physically and psychologi-
and/or spiritually is important because it transforms cally unhealthy behavior?
narrow-minded religious behavior and thinking into
open, fully integrated religiosity and spirituality, thus
avoiding central problems, including religious funda- A DEVELOPMENTAL APPROACH
mentalism, intolerance, and war. However, being mind-
ful of the challenges faced by any normative approach, To explain religious and spiritual development requires
we emphasize especially the challenge presented by al- explaining the antecedents and consequences of religious
944 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

and spiritual content and belief, religious and spiritual derived from nor reduced to something else. They have
practical life, and religious and spiritual structure their own meaning, roots, and core—like morality,
(Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 1998). It also re- mathematics, and music. Later on, we speak about reli-
quires explaining how an existential experience can be giousness as having a fundamental mother structure,
interpreted differently at different times in life and with which is generated, in part, from the universal experi-
different complexity, styles, and meaning-making capac- ence that life is fragile. Even at early ages, children ask
ities. Finally, it means explaining how religious and spir- questions about the meaning of death, about arbitrari-
itual development elucidates growth and change in ways ness and contingency, and about why there is misfortune
of being in the world that need not contradict the general and evil. This questioning reveals the religious mother
enlightening process of modern and postmodern society. structure, and it provides one expression of and impetus
Our approach is individual centered in that religious and for spiritual development.
spiritual behavior is seen as rooted in the person who
is immersed in and influenced by life events, learning,
and culture. IS RELIGIOUS AND SPIRITUAL
The question of how apparently simple levels of de- DEVELOPMENT RESISTANT TO THE
velopment are transformed into complex forms is cen- POSTMODERN PATH?
tral (Case, 1985; Fischer & Bidell, 1997; Fischer &
Rose, 1999; Pascual-Leone, 1983). We adhere to the dis- In 1791, Condorcet (1789/1976), the French Minister
continuous approach to development, but, in so doing, of Education, predicted that as the human intellect pro-
we differentiate highly according to lifestyles, denomi- gressed, religion would be dispensed with in a culture
nations, cultures, situations, intelligence, and ethno- largely dominated by science. In the nineteenth and
graphic experiences. We offer no stereotypic portraits twentieth centuries, leading thinkers agreed, among
of stages. Rather, we describe and explain how individu- them Karl Marx, Sigmund Freud, and James Leuba.
als in modern societies transform their religious atti- Marx dismissed religion as the “opium of the people”
tudes, judgments, and feelings in the life span. because he thought religion alienated people and legit-
Development is the focus—so much so that general dis- imized the exploitation of the proletariat. Freud re-
cussions about the psychology and sociology of religion garded religion as an obsessional neurosis, an infantile
(Brown, 1987; Durkheim, 1915/1995; Grom, 1992; illusion that humanity would have to overcome by mov-
Hood, Spilka, Hunsberger, & Gorsuch, 1996; Wulff, ing in the direction of “God as logos.” Leuba, one of the
1991) are avoided. founding fathers of the psychology of religion, consid-
Taking a developmental approach to religiousness and ered religiousness to be negatively correlated with ra-
spirituality has serious implications for designing inter- tionality, implying that religiousness is something to be
ventions to help. When religiousness and spirituality overcome.
make life uncontrollable and bound to blind faith, they However, at the beginning of the third millennium,
can easily become sources for inhuman behavior, intol- research tells us that Condorcet’s prognosis has not been
erance, and painful dependency on dysfunctional fulfilled. The opposite has happened. The classic secu-
groups. But when real development occurs, religiousness larization thesis, which claims there is a successive
and spirituality can be compared to any other develop- weakening of religiousness in the postmodern age, has
mental transformation producing losses and gains in a been refuted. In the United States, 96% of the popula-
flexible and complex human personality. tion professes to believe in God (Gallup, 1995) and 75%
Thus, religious and spiritual development contains of U.S. adolescents say they try to follow the teachings
the growth of the personality and identity in the broad- of their religion (P. King & C. Boyatzis, 2004). In most
est sense. Religious and spiritual knowledge, feelings, European countries, the numbers are markedly smaller,
belonging, self-efficacy, and identity—all are part of but even in a country such as France, with its distinct lay
development and change and must—to the extent that history, there are still approximately 67% who believe
research is available—be part of our overall develop- in God (Denz, 2002, p. 40).
mental frame. In Europe, in general, we see that persons in the new
In this chapter, we defend the thesis that religious millennium are less apt to belong to a denomination but
and spiritual thoughts, acts, and feelings can neither be still feel themselves to be religious and/or spiritual.
Is Religious and Spiritual Development Resistant to the Postmodern Path? 945

For example, in a large sociological study in Austria, Furthermore, in academia we now find a renewed in-
Zulehner, Hager, and Polak (2001) found that 94% of the terest in religion and spirituality as well as new theories
people in their sample felt strongly religious despite putting religion and spirituality at the very heart of
only 27% saying they are Christians, 30% saying they what it means to be human. We see this renewed interest
have a patchwork religiosity, 30% saying they are hu- especially in the psychology of religion, which long ago
manists, 13% saying they are atheists, and only a small cast off ties restricting it to church religiousness and, in
number saying they participate in active church life. In recent times, turned to discussing diverse forms of spir-
addition, the correlation between social engagement and ituality (Argyle, 2000; Beit-Hallahmi & Argyle, 1997;
religiosity was very high, a finding consistent with find- Hood, 1995; Hood et al., 1996; Wulff, 1991). And we
ings from the research of many, which shows that will- see that many scientists want to connect religious life
ingness to perform volunteer service is positively and the natural sciences through speculative reflection
associated with stable or upward religious developmen- (Reich, 2004). As for the new theories, we see them
tal trajectories (Donnelly, Matsuba, Hart, & Atkins, suggesting religious and spiritual development is indis-
2005; Youniss, McLellan, Su, & Yates, 1999). pensable to human functioning. For example, today,
If anything, the postmodern era manifests tenden- sociobiologists claim religiousness is determined genet-
cies to become more religious, not less. New religious ically and that religion was and still is an important fac-
movements, often of a fundamentalist nature, are moti- tor in evolutionary survival (Burkert, 1996; Daecke &
vating millions. More and more religious groups are Schnakenberg, 2000; Wilson, 1998). Religion, they
succeeding, among them totalitarian sects. And with argue, offers advantages of selection; sects especially
regard to forms of spirituality, the enormous variety serve as examples—since they achieve selection by
suggests that we are today in the midst of a spirituality strengthening group cohesiveness and because they fos-
“ boom.” ter aggression toward threatening entities outside the
The evidence for their being a spirituality boom group.
comes from several sources. There are the many new Thus renowned psychologists of religion (Hood et al.,
books on spirituality with titles such as Heart and 1996, p. 44) have asked, “Is religion in our genes?” In so
Home. Embracing the Spiritual in Everyday Life (Ben- doing, they are thinking not about specific religions,
ish, 2001) and The Way. Using the Wisdom of Kabbalah which are always marked and shaped in sociocultural
for Spiritual Transformation and Fulfillment (Berg, ways, but about a basic religious disposition, which ac-
2001). There are also now psychological journals devot- cording to Oerter (1980, p. 293), is activated when young
ing special issues to spiritual topics, for example, the children become conscious of their temporally restricted
Journal of Individual Psychology (2000, Vol. 3) and the existence, an experience that leads to a preoccupation,
journal, Applied Developmental Science (2004, Vol. 8). determined by nature, with religious and metaphysical
And even a cursory search on the Internet indicates problems. Spirituality, too, has been seen as humanity’s
tremendous interest in spirituality and spiritual develop- answer to its own finite nature (Socha, 1999).
ment. For example, launching a search for “spiritual de- Modern, scientifically based theories of religion and
velopment ” in databases such as PsycInfo will produce spirituality have explained religious and spiritual devel-
more hits than “religious development.” opment as a means of satisfying the human need to exer-
The variety and boom are, no doubt, related to the cise control, indeed, control over and above the mere
fact that religious freedom has been established as a immanence of the here-and-now and into the great tran-
constitutional right in the developed world so that reli- scendent realm beyond (Flammer, 1994).
gious and spiritual beliefs and affiliations can no longer Throughout human evolution, belief in the divine has
be imposed or prescribed. Connected with this is the worked because it has helped humans cope with threaten-
way in which spirituality has been released from the ing and difficult situations. In addition, religion has legit-
power of religious communities. As a result, many now imized authority and created security—through oaths,
are developing their own religious ideas and practices. for example, but also through the expectation of reciproc-
These ideas and practices can be rejected as “religion à ity between sacrifice and reward (Burkert, 1996).
la carte,” but they can also be seen positively as the re- As another example of new theories giving a positive
sult of a patchwork religiousness and spirituality that view of religious and spiritual development, Huntington
has emerged under the influence of postmodernism. (1998, p. 61) has theorized that every culture is based
946 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

on a religion, which, in the nature of a latent disposi- In the Christian faith tradition, the concepts of devel-
tion, retains its power to grow and spread even after it opment and stage have been used frequently to define
has been subjected to waves of secularization. Further- the religious and spiritual life—so frequently that one
more, cultural anthropologists point out that every cul- might suspect that current stage developmental theory is
ture produces a system of religious symbols, which rooted in the mind-set of this Western tradition. There
govern individuals’ relations with transcendence and are many examples. Ambrosias, one of the early church
provide the ultimate systems of authority. This means fathers (dec. 379), described four stages in the ascent to
“religion can be viewed as a precondition of culture, or God, and Benedict of Nursia (dec. 547) described nor-
as springing from the same roots as culture” (Ohlig matively 12 stages of humility.
2002, p. 101). Mysticism has also played a central role in develop-
However, discontinuity, boom, genetic issues, and ing the concept of religious and spiritual development,
new developments are not the only terms to use to ex- and, in recent times, some of the more famous stage
plain why Condorcet’s prognosis has been proven models of mystics have been compared to stage models
wrong. Today, there are continuities with the past in of modern theorists. Thus, M. J. Meadow (1992)
the way religiosity and spirituality are experienced. showed impressive parallels between Teresa of Avila’s
Children and young people continue, as they did in the seven stages leading to mystical union and Jane Lo-
past, to encounter religious phenomena such as church evinger’s model of ego development. Steele (1994) re-
buildings, saffron robed monks, religious sisters, constructed John of the Cross’s stages of spiritual
veiled Islamic schoolgirls, orthodox Jews with development in the light of transpersonal psychology,
yarmulkes and side locks, soccer players making the and Oser (2002) compared the steps of the mystical
sign of the cross, and people praying at a funeral. Fur- path in both Meister Eckhart and Margerethe Porete to
thermore, places of spirituality are still represented by the stages of religious judgment as presented by Oser
monasteries and convents, houses of retreat for spiri- and Gmünder (1991).
tual exercises, and remote places for tranquility and These prescientific descriptions of stages provide no
prayer. Spirituality also includes traditional ideas— systematic theory or description of age changes. Rather,
negative ideas having to do with suffering and asceti- they define an ideal for the total religious life. Neverthe-
cism but also positive ideas having to do with affirming less, they show that, early on, religious leaders made
life, being health conscious, and seeking happiness. frequent use of the metaphors of stage and step to give
In sum, religion and spirituality are alive and thriv- their own developmental accounts.
ing both in traditional ways and in ways not even imag- The phenomenon of religious development was also
ined by Condorcet. Now, more than ever, we need to obvious to Jean-Jacques Rousseau (1712–1768). In
understand this widespread and varied human phenome- Emile (1762/1979), his novel on education, Rousseau ob-
non—especially by understanding its development. served younger children’s propensity to imagine God as
a person as well as their not understanding the abstract
A HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE language of catechism. From these observations,
Rousseau advised that children be kept from religious
The scientific study of religious development dates back instruction until they reach adolescence. Centuries later,
to 1882, the year that G. Stanley Hall, in the context of Ronald Goldman would give similar advice but from a
his “Child Study,” investigated children’s religious Piagetian theoretical perspective.
imagination (see Huxel, 2000, 95 f ). However, the con- In the field of religious education, classical writers
cept of religious development has a much longer history. such as Salzmann, Basedow, Jean Paul, and Jean Calvin
The books of the dead in ancient Egypt and Tibet de- were finely attuned to children’s religious development
scribed steps leading to the gods and Nirvana respec- (Schweitzer, 1992). For example, Jean Calvin (1834,
tively. In the Bible, the Apostle Paul noted children’s p. 200) assigned the Old Testament to children and the
qualitatively different ways of being in the world when New Testament to adolescents, a practice that antici-
he wrote, “When I was a child, I spoke as a child, I un- pated G. Stanley Hall’s advice at the turn of the twenti-
derstood as a child, I thought as a child; when I became eth century (Hall, 1900, 1908). Calvin assumed that
an adult, I put away childish ways” (1 Corinthians 13:11, the vivid stories of the Old Testament matched the in-
New Revised Standard Version). terests and mental capacities of children just as the ab-
A Historical Perspective 947

stract messages of the New Testament did the same for from the psychology of religion would optimize religious
adolescents. instruction.
By the turn of the twentieth century, when psycholog- These early researchers and practical theologians in-
ical exploration of religious development began in vestigated children’s concepts of heaven (Barth, 1911),
earnest, the three main paradigms for explaining reli- God (Nobiling, 1929), religious doubt (Wunderle, 1932),
gious and spiritual development had already been well and religious ideas in general (Voss, 1926). Their central
established as: focus was on describing age differences rather than on
constructing theories. For most of these early investiga-
1. Religious and spiritual development as maturing tors, religious development was a maturational process
2. Religious and spiritual development as coping (Gesell, 1977; Kroh, 1965). Nowhere do we see this
3. Religious and spiritual development as perfecting more clearly than in the stage theory of G. Stanley Hall.

G. Stanley Hall
In the first paradigm, maturity, loosely defined, is the
telos toward which religious and spiritual development Hall (1904, 1923) argued that individual religious devel-
tends. Maturity is a value-laden concept, but the values opment recapitulates the religious development of the
made explicit in this paradigm are attainable by most. In species. So, religious development begins with uncon-
this paradigm, to mature religiously and spiritually is not sciousness and is followed by a stage of fetishism and
to become a saint. Rather, it is to become an adult in nature worship—the very features that define both an-
whatever ways being an adult is culturally defined. cient idolatrous religions and the religious concepts of
In the second paradigm, maturity is also a telos, but school children with their concrete and anthropomor-
the emphasis is more on the primitive and immature—so phic images of God.
that maturity means coping with personal limitations. Hall also argued that children need to work through
Here, we see a functional approach—with religious and these earlier (i.e., historically earlier) ways of being re-
spiritual development functioning to help us cope. We ligious before they can adopt more advanced, recently
also see health and maturity being used almost inter- acquired ways. So, as mentioned previously, he sug-
changeably. Nowhere is this paradigm used more exten- gested that the religious education of children should
sively than in psychoanalytic theory—so we concentrate focus on the Old Testament with its concrete images and
on psychoanalytic theory when discussing this paradigm. stories, and later on, during adolescence, on the New
The third paradigm explicitly embraces perfection as Testament with its abstract principles and values.
the goal of development. The telos here is entirely con- Hall’s influence and contribution was also in his pro-
ceptual—though occasionally the talk is of religious moting careful, empirical work as well as in his explain-
or spiritual exemplars. But even in the exemplars, the as- ing that adolescence is a significant time for spiritual
sumption of this paradigm is not that the exemplar is transformation.
perfect but that the exemplar points to what perfection Hall’s linking individual and phylogenetic religious
consists of, to the standard or ideal that defines develop- development has been critiqued and refuted (Gould,
ment, and allows us to evaluate and explain levels or 2003a). However, his influence on the psychology of re-
stages of development as approximations to perfection. ligious development has been enormous, not only be-
These three paradigms are used to organize the re- cause he got others to think developmentally, but also
mainder of this section. because he demonstrated that religious development is a
domain that should be studied scientifically. Hall’s phy-
Religious and Spiritual Development as Maturing logenetic theory was a maturationist theory in that it as-
sumed religious development unfolded in ways heavily
Research into religious development reached its first influenced by biology. This maturationist understanding
highpoint in the beginning decades of the twentieth cen- and emphasis continued long after Hall—even into the
tury, partly as a result of the work of American pioneers second half of the twentieth century.
in the psychology of religion, in particular, Hall, Leuba,
and Starbuck (see Huxel, 2000; Wulff, 1991, pp. 41–53). Gordon W. Allport
This era also produced extensive work in Germany, by The third example of religious and spiritual
practical theologians hoping that knowledge derived development as a process of maturing is the work of
948 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

G. W. Allport. In The Individual and His Religion (All- common focus on unconscious, intrapsychic conflicts.
port, 1950), Allport described two types of religious- Psychoanalytic theory has played a central role in defin-
ness, which form a developmental sequence. The first ing religious and spiritual development and its func-
type is the “self-interested” religiousness of younger tions. It has done so in both controversial and
children who see prayers as a means of getting material contradictory ways—with theorists from the same psy-
things. Children also imagine God in a concrete, an- choanalytic community often reaching opposite conclu-
thropomorphic manner. Thus, we have the precursor of sions based on common starting points. Nowhere do we
the well-established concept, “extrinsic religiousness.” see this more clearly than in the competing theories of
In the years preceding puberty, children’s egocen- Freud and Jung and in the radically different ways that
tric religiousness leads to disappointment (e.g., initi- Freudian, ego analytic, and object relations theorists
ated by denial of material goods, experiences of have evaluated religion and religious development.
theodicy). Subsequently, children form new and more
abstract religious concepts and shape a mature and Sigmund Freud
self-disinterested type of religiousness. In the best Sigmund Freud took issue repeatedly and in an influen-
case, religion becomes part of an adolescent’s own per- tial way with religiousness. In so doing, he stimulated
sonality and fulfils the criterion of functional auton- thousands of psychoanalytic studies of religion (Beit-
omy: religiousness becomes “intrinsic religiousness.” Hallahmi, 1996a; see also Shafranske, 1995; Wulff,
In the following decades, the distinction between ex- 1991, pp. 253–316). As early as 1907, he characterized
trinsic and intrinsic religiousness became one of the religiousness as an obsessional neurosis. For Freud, re-
most powerful distinctions in the psychology of religion. ligious concepts are illusions that provide comfort and
Hundreds of studies showed that intrinsic religiousness security to those threatened by the forces of nature, the
is positively correlated with a variety of desirable out- inevitability of old age and death, and the hostility
comes, such as a sense of well-being, whereas extrinsic of others. Their origins can be traced to protective per-
religiousness is positively correlated with undesirable sons with whom the child has close contact in early
outcomes such as being prejudiced (see the overview by childhood.
Wulff, 1991, pp. 217–242; see also Hood et al., 1996). The most important protective person was, for
In discussing earlier works using this first paradigm, Freud, the father, toward whom boys have hostile feel-
we see how religious and spiritual maturity can repre- ings during the Oedipal phase, resulting in the father
sent an achievable and worthwhile end. Furthermore, we image being projected into the transcendent realm. In
see how maturity has to do with individuals functioning his later writings, Freud put his theory of the psychic
well in their cultures and contributing to their societies. origins of God as follows: “ To begin with, we know that
These early works using this first paradigm can be God is a father-substitute; or, more correctly, that he is
seen as the forerunners of present-day research on reli- a copy of a father as he is seen and experienced in child-
gious maturity (Benson, Donahue, & Erickson, 1993). hood—by individuals in their own childhood . . .”
Furthermore, they can be seen as the forerunners of (Freud, 1923a, p. 85).
present-day research on resilience and positive youth However, in classic Freudian psychoanalysis, reli-
development as they relate to religious and spiritual de- gious development is not merely a matter of projection
velopment (Lerner, Dowling, & Anderson, 2003). Later but also of sublimating sexual libido. In an early paper
on, we have more to say about present-day research con- titled, “Obsessive Actions and Religious Practices,”
tinuing in this tradition of thinking about religious and Freud (1907) claimed that religiousness arose as a “re-
spiritual development as a maturing process. nunciation of certain impulses . . . within which . . . a
sexual element cannot normally be excluded.” Freud’s
student, Schroedter (1908), drew from this the general
Religious and Spiritual Development as
Coping: Psychoanalytic Theory conclusion that the origin of religiousness, in particular,
emotive and mystical forms of religiousness, resides in
Psychoanalytic theory is—if developmental—essen- the repression of erotic instincts. What is repressed, he
tially a theory of motivation. More precisely, it is a col- argued, reappears in a distorted form, as in acts of piety
lection of related theories connected by their sharing a that border on hysteria.
A Historical Perspective 949

Pfister (1911), one of the first theologians familiar the “shadow.” According to Jung, every individual has to
with Freudian theory, used Freudian theory to explain cope with what is suppressed, ignored, and neglected in
the religious development of Margarethe Ebner, a mys- personal life. In doing so, individuals develop toward
tic of the high Middle Ages. Ebner had the habit of wholeness, becoming a wholly integrated self. For this
lying next to a crucifix and experiencing the “sweet- process of becoming an integrated self (individuation),
est ” sensations—for Pfister, evidence of the sublima- religion plays a central role.
tion of unsatisfied sexuality. Pfister (1925) applied the The goal of the individual is to balance the demands
same kind of analysis to Count Ludwig von Zinzendorf of reality with personal needs. This balancing is
(1700–1760), founder of the Moravian Church. When achieved with the help of inherited archetypes in a col-
he was forty, Von Zinzendorf began worshipping the lective unconscious that is a part of every individual’s
wound in the side of Jesus—evidence, according to heritage. Numinous archetypes are structuring princi-
Pfister, for the sublimation of homosexual feelings. ples that are produced through dreams and cannot be
The constellation of unresolved Oedipal feelings, ho- produced by mind and reflection. Religions play a cen-
moerotic inclinations, sublimation, repression, and neu- tral role by effecting this balance in the way they relate
rosis in general was subsequently interpreted into dozens to the archetypes. Because religious myths are projec-
of biographies of prominent religious personalities (see tions of the archetypes, the messages of the soul and the
reviews by Bucher, 2004; Hitschmann, 1947; Zeligs, messages of religions are of similar form and can be
1974, on the religious development of biblical figures). translated each to the other. In doing so, individuals
Not surprisingly, the classical Freudian view of reli- cope better by coming to terms with their shadow.
gious development drew criticism on a massive scale Therefore, whereas for Freud development consists of
(see reviews by Beit-Hallahmi, 1996a; Meissner, 1984). putting religion behind us, for Jung, development consists
For example, Greve and Roos (1996) questioned the uni- of embracing and using religion to support development.
versal applicability of the Oedipus complex and argued
that the Oedipus story is, after all, a myth, not an accu- Erik Erikson
rate representation of a universal phenomenon. Freud’s Erikson represents a third variation on the psychoana-
reducing God to an earthly father also came under at- lytic theme with respect to religious and spiritual devel-
tack. For example, on the basis of clinical case studies, opment. As did Freud and Jung, Erikson focused on
Schjelderup and Schjelderup (1932) described how peo- unconscious intrapsychic conflicts to explain what fuels
ple develop their own mother religions, especially in development. However, Erikson’s emphasis was much
Eastern contexts. However, possibly the most damning more on how intrapsychic conflict reflects not only fa-
criticism of Freud’s psychology of religious develop- milial themes but also societal-cultural themes. In many
ment has been the criticism that there is no way to dis- ways, Erikson fits more comfortably into present-day
prove Freud’s claims because when claims cannot be discussions about context, culture, and development.
disproved, they fall outside the domain of science. With respect to Erikson and religious and spiritual
development, the two main concepts are “ basic trust ”
Carl Jung and “identity.” Developing basic trust was, for Erikson,
For Carl Gustav Jung, the son of a protestant minister, the central religious issue—not only trust in God but
religion played a positive role in both his theory and also trust in a universe that offers enough to make life
personal life. Still in the psychoanalytic tradition, Jung worth living.
provided a quite different focus than Freud’s—with his However, for Erikson, it is the struggle to achieve a
concept of individuation (1968). The concept of individ- personal identity that warranted special attention. For
uation explains individual development as a gradual in- Erikson, religion offered one of several ways in which
tegration of psychic structures, creating a self where individuals achieve identity. Nowhere is this better illus-
conscious and unconscious are fully integrated and trated than in Erikson’s (1958) influential book, Young
compatible. Man Luther, in which he shows how Luther used religion
As for his theory’s connection to religious and spiri- to shape and construct his identity.
tual development, Jung spoke about a “personal uncon- In Young Man Luther, Erikson demonstrated how reli-
scious” or hidden psychic realm that he referred to as gious ideology and religious institutions provide ways for
950 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

youth to separate themselves from old identifications the bankruptcy of an older one; it combined Yahwistic
with parents to achieve a new identity better suited to wrath with the benevolence of a God-in-Christ. But for all
weather the storms of adulthood and better suited to ren- his patent creativity, what reliance King had on the great
der them able to contribute to society. In Young Man illusionistic traditions of the Bible, black preaching, and
Luther, Erikson shows how Luther’s choice to become a Gandhian nonviolence! Absorption in these traditions had
become to him a sacred playing—with one sideways glance
monk, his obsessional behavior as a young monk, and, his
at the brutal facts of the realistic world and another keen
becoming a great reformer, all reveal his inner struggle to
glance at the ever-present explosive turbulence of the
remain identified with his father and his church while, at autistic world. (p. 179)
the same time, to distance himself to become his own
person with his own convictions and purpose in life.
The example of King provides insight into the ana-
Object Relations Theorists lytic concept of religious and spiritual maturity and the
role of religious-spiritual imagining in realizing matu-
For the past several decades, neo-analytic psychology rity. Pruyser implies that King’s religious imagining
has focused more and more on the quality of interper- grasped something true, though not something true in
sonal relations and on how interpersonal relations are the sense of scientific truth. Truth in this context is ob-
affected by internalized images of self and significant viously not about propositions that are either true or
others. Nowhere do we see this more clearly than in the false. Presumably, truth in this sense has more to do
work of those who identify themselves as object rela- with living lives that are “ true.”
tions theorists. Their central focus is on imagination What is missing, though, is just how to evaluate a re-
and illusion-making. ligious illusion as true. We are left wondering if Pruyser
While continuing Freud’s practice of calling religious means to suggest it is universal truths that are revealed
imaginings illusions, object relations theorists have given in religious imaginings or simply truths that are spe-
illusion-making a positive spin, which treats imagining as cific to particular groups and individuals. The following
a wedge between impulse and objective reality, a wedge personal story suggests perhaps it is both. Pruyser
that guards against the dangers of impulses and autistic (1991) writes,
thinking, on the one hand, and the slings and arrows of
misfortune and a stultifying status quo, on the other. In
I attended a denominational school . . . that stood for much
Paul Pruyser’s (1991) words:
greater Calvinist orthodoxy than did my family. Thus,
home and school presented me with two different religious
Illusion is not hallucination or delusion but can deteriorate and emotional worlds. The first was mellow, optimistic,
into them. Illusion formation is . . . a unique process that and forgiving; the second strict, somber and punitive—
derives from the imagination. It need not be captive to the both equally taking recourse to scripture. . . . There is
autistic process and cannot be locked into the reality test- nothing like such an upbringing to convince a young boy
ing procedures described by common sense. (p. 176) that religion is what you make it, that all of it is what I now
call “illusionistic.” Fortunately, home won over school,
Object relations theorists define imagining as a transi- undoubtedly because of its deeper roots in my childhood
tional phenomenon, where the boundaries between practicing of the transitional sphere. The hand of God,
subjective and objective become blurred. The partial much talked about in school, was closer to my mother’s
tender-and-firm hand than to the threatening and often
differentiation between what is inside and outside al-
slapping extremities of my teachers. (p. 180)
lows individuals to not only creatively manage but also
to creatively transform. Pruyser (1991) provides the
example of Martin Luther King Jr. and his “I have a In other words, for Pruyser—and for many trained in the
dream” speech to illustrate what is meant by religious object relations tradition—the decisive step in religious
imagining being a transitional phenomenon that cre- and spiritual development is choosing between alterna-
atively manages and transforms. He writes: tive imaginings. For Pruyser, the truth or value of imag-
ining lies not so much in its structure as in its content,
That speech straddled both the world of ideas and the and in the value of its content for supporting healthy
world of facts; it introduced a new paradigm after showing psychological development. The gentler, nurturing im-
A Historical Perspective 951

agery promoted in Pruyser’s home life seemed healthier development in terms of what is remarkable, so remark-
to Pruyser than the more punitive imagery promoted in able that one may never actually witness an example of
his school life. someone who reaches the endpoint, someone who is
But what exactly should one mean by “ healthier,” fully developed.
and is healthier to be equated with mature? Pruyser and At first glance, this third paradigm seems to revert
object relations theory in general are not specific with back to prescientific days when religious and spiritual
regard to how the concepts of healthy and religious- development meant climbing toward some state of mys-
spiritual development relate—and perhaps for good rea- tical union. However, the paradigm leads in a quite dif-
son. Health is not to be reified, turned into a “ thing”; ferent direction. It leads in the direction of offering
rather, it is to be used to assess functioning, and func- specific criteria for defining and evaluating develop-
tioning is a matter of fit between person and context. ment and for making explicit the often implicit or hid-
Object relations theory has also contributed by den values in all developmental models. Later on, there
broadening our understanding of the origins of the God will be extended examples to show how this paradigm
concept. For example, Rizzuto (1979) argued that Freud works. Here, and for the purpose of providing a histori-
picked too late a period in dating the beginnings of the cal perspective, we concentrate on a single example,
representation of God in the Oedipal phase. She located namely, the example of William James and his monu-
the origin of the representation of God in early child- mental work, The Varieties of Religious Experience
hood. Regardless of whether they are raised in a reli- (1902).
gious manner, children form a representation of God or The Varieties is arguably the single most influential
sacred figures such as the Buddha. These representa- book in the psychology of religion. Even after a century
tions or images may derive from the mother, father, or since it was written, readers find in it insights that are
combinations of significant others. Furthermore, the directly relevant to our times (Taylor, 2002). However,
quality of the relationships depicted in these images de- the Varieties is not normally thought of as a text on reli-
pends on the quality of early human relationships. gious and spiritual development. For one thing, there is
These representations or images provide, at best, pro- hardly any mention of children. For another, it gives us
tection. Later on, they may be held onto, updated, or religious types, but the types themselves seem ordered
discarded—to conform to official doctrines or to con- more along a horizontal plane than along a vertical, de-
tradict them. velopmental one. This seems especially true of James’s
In sum, classic and neo-psychoanalytic theory pro- two major types: “ The Healthy-Minded” or “once-born”
vides a variety of examples of work done in the paradigm and the “Sick Soul” or those who feel themselves in
of coping. Furthermore, these examples are among the need of being “ twice-born.”
forerunners of present-day studies, which also operate in Nevertheless, a closer reading of the Varieties shows
the paradigm of coping. We will have more to say about James to be very much a developmentalist as defined by
present-day studies working in the paradigm of coping, the third paradigm. In particular, James saw the sick
such as the case studies of Susan Kwilecki (1999). soul as having a more mature grasp of reality than that
seen in the healthy minded—and a greater potential for
developing spiritually. He shows this developmental or-
Religious and Spiritual Development dering of the two types when he turns from writing
as Perfecting about the once-born, healthy-minded to writing about
the sick soul in need of being twice-born:
[I]n contradistinction to the process of mere change (de-
velopment) is a movement toward perfection, as variously
Let us then resolutely turn our backs on the once-born and
as that idea may be constructed. (Kaplan, 1983a)
their sky-blue optimistic gospel; let us not simply cry out,
in spite of all appearances, “Hurrah for the Universe!—
In describing religious and spiritual development, the God’s in his Heaven, all’s right with the world.” Let us see
previous two paradigms stay close to tangible reality. rather whether pity, pain, and fear, and the sentiment of
Persons develop, and some mature, but development and human helplessness may not open a profounder view and
maturity are not remarkable. In contrast, this third put into our hands a more complicated key to the meaning
paradigm defines and assesses religious and spiritual of the situation. (James, 1902, pp. 135–136)
952 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

In short, the sick soul has a better grasp of reality and so type, saintliness, forecasts current stage theories, which
is in a better position to develop further. also use perfection as a telos, particularly the theories
And what might that development consist of ? Later of Fowler (1981) and Oser and Gmünder (1991). Later
on, James tells us when he discusses saintliness. After a on, we have more to say about these more current theo-
second (spiritual) birth, the previously sick soul is more ries operating in the paradigm of religious and spiritual
sensitive to the world’s contingencies. This is essential development as perfecting.
to growth, which shows how James can be seen as a de- From the point of view of today’s research, these
velopmentalist in the tradition of the third paradigm. three paradigms for thinking about religious develop-
Saintliness is James’s way of providing a telos, an ment operate in all discussions taking identity formation
ideal endpoint needed to define, evaluate, and explain as the main goal of development. As King (2003) states,
religious and spiritual development. He makes this clear “. . . identity development that emerges . . . within reli-
by characterizing his picture of saintliness as a compos- gion is an identity that transcends the self and can pro-
ite picture, an ideal type, rather than as a picture of spe- mote a sense of commitment that not only fosters
cific individuals. In short, James’s saint is a standard of individual well-being but promotes the good of society
perfection that helps define what it means to develop re- as well” (p. 197). These three paradigms thus are holis-
ligiously and spiritually. tic approaches to religious and spiritual development, a
James’s concept of saintliness is not the everyday point we return to repeatedly throughout the remainder
concept of a moral exemplar who uses religious lan- of the chapter.
guage. Rather, James’s concept of saintliness is essen-
tially religious or spiritual, not moral. In his words, to be
a saint is to have “a feeling of being in a wider life than DEFINING RELIGIOUS AND
that of this world’s selfish little interests; and a convic- SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT
tion, not merely intellectual, but as it were sensible, of
the existence of an Ideal Power ” (p. 272). He follows In Goethe’s (1974) Faust, when Gretchen asked Faust
this description with the added comment that the saint about his attitude toward religion, she was thinking of
has “a sense of the friendly continuity of the ideal power faith in the church, salvation of souls, and fear of hell.
with our own life, and a willing self-surrender to its Presumably, so was Faust. Were she to ask her question
control” (p. 273). For James, religious and spiritual today, she would have to imagine quotation marks
emotions are central. Morality is a by-product. around “religion” and do her best to interpret which of
In the Varieties, the saint is an ideal type defining the several meanings was embedded in Faust’s response.
endpoint of religious and spiritual development. For Today, one of the central problems in the psychology of
James, the experience of being a divided self, a self in religious and spiritual development—as mentioned in
need of being saved, a lost or sick soul was instrumental the beginning—is the problem of defining religious and
for developing toward saintliness. To develop religiously spiritual.
or spiritually meant that things might get worse, not bet- This problem of defining is nothing new. Almost a
ter—at least for a while. For James, simply becoming century ago, James Leuba (1912, p. 341) listed 48 dif-
mature and coping are not, by themselves, enough to de- ferent definitions of religion, which led him to con-
fine what can ultimately develop when we speak of reli- clude that attempts to define religion precisely border
gious and spiritual development. For James and anyone on megalomania. Since, the situation has become still
else working in this third paradigm, it matters little more difficult because spirituality also needs defini-
whether anyone actually reaches sainthood and perfec- tion and because the religious-spiritual landscape has
tion. What matters is that we can conceive of sainthood become more pluralistic. Generally accepted defini-
and perfection and in so conceiving, we can become bet- tions do not exist (Beile, 1998, p. 24), and the search
ter able to define, evaluate, and explain religious and for a common definition can proceed almost indefi-
spiritual development. nitely (Brown, 1987, p. 17). For this reason authorita-
Just as the work of previous writers working in the tive textbooks on the psychology of religion generally
other two paradigms forecast current research, so, too, avoid providing a working or operational definition
the work of James forecast current research and theory (Beit-Hallahmi, 1989, p. 11; Beit-Hallahmi & Argyle,
using this third paradigm. In particular, James’s ideal 1997, p. 5; Hood et al., 1996, p. 7).
Defining Religious and Spiritual Development 953

Nevertheless, even though there is no commonly ac- spiritual ideals even as they feel themselves not bound
cepted definition, there are commonly accepted distinc- by religious tradition (see story on p. 943).
tions that help define religious and spiritual development. In order not to exclude these people, Utsch (1998,
In particular, there are the commonly accepted distinc- p. 97) argued in favor of converting spirituality into an
tions between religion and religiousness, between reli- autonomous research subject separated from the subject
giousness and spiritual, and between content, form, and of religion. In his view, such a separation is justified
function. We discuss each of these distinctions to address since religion can exist without spirituality when it
the problem of definition. In addition, we discuss the con- “consists only of rules of behavior, of theology not re-
cepts of structure and stage as indispensable means for lated to experience, and of rites which have not been un-
defining and explaining the religious and spiritual devel- derstood” (p, 99). Spirituality is thus named ex negativo
opment of persons and not just of acts, thoughts, and feel- and defined as a subjective experience of what is existen-
ings considered separately. tially relevant for human beings.
However, a good many scholars retain the historical
connection between religion and spirituality by linking
Religion, Religiousness, and Spirituality
spirituality to the experience of transcendence. For ex-
Religion refers to institutions and systems consisting of ample, Pargament (1997) defined spirituality as “a
organizational structures, codes of behavior, and symbol highly individualized search for the sense of connected-
systems defining assumptions and beliefs designed to ness with a transcendent force” (p. 38). McFadden
create in people powerful, comprehensive, and enduring (1996) also included a religious element in her definition
world views and attitudes. As such, religion is primarily when she designated spirituality as a “motivational-
susceptible to sociological analysis. emotional phenomenon associated with a sense of mean-
Religiousness refers to subjective modes of experi- ingful integration within the self, with other persons and
encing and interpreting, making religiousness primar- the world, and with the Divine” (p. 387). Miller and
ily susceptible to psychological analysis. One and the Martin (1988) did something similar in saying, “Spiritu-
same religion can be reconstructed and experienced in ality entails the acknowledgment of a transcendent
quite different ways leading to quite different types of being, power, or reality greater than ourselves” (p. 14).
religiousness. The Catholic Church, for example, can In sum, spirituality can be identical with religiousness
be a source of fastidious anxieties for one person and a by its constituting a relationship to a divine or transcen-
secure, maternal home for another. Therefore, it is ul- dent power, or it can be independent of religiousness by
timately the individual who determines religiousness its stressing a more philosophical orientation. Put another
on the basis of his or her subjective experience: way, spirituality can refer to actions, which rekindle reli-
“Events or feelings are only ‘religious’ if a person de- giousness or to actions without religious reference such
fines them as such” (Stark, 1965, p. 99). Religiousness as relaxation exercises practiced by self-professed athe-
refers to much more than religious practice such as at- ists. Religiousness and spirituality are therefore not to be
tending religious services and joining religious institu- seen as in contrast to each other but rather as overlapping
tions. Many regard themselves as being religious even (Reich, Oser, & Scarlett, 1999, see also p. 943).
though they have left or avoided religious institutions This view of religiousness and spirituality as being
and religious practice. both independent and overlapping has been supported
Nor is it any less difficult to arrive at a definition of by several recent studies. For example, Dowling et al.
spirituality. For a good many experts, when it comes to (2004) used factor analytic and structural equation
spirituality, the problems of defining are even more dif- modeling to separate out spirituality from religiosity
ficult (Hood et al., 1996, p. 115). However, there is no factors contained in the questionnaire results of Search
way to evade the issue given the fact that at the present Institute’s database. They did so by equating spiritual-
time, there is scarcely a discussion of faith and scarcely ity with self-transcendence evidenced in the way indi-
a book on religion that does not use spirituality. Indeed, viduals become civic-minded and concerned about
the term spirituality is visibly eclipsing the term reli- contributing to society.
giousness. The increased use of spirituality derives to a In another study, Zinnbauer et al. (1997) asked
large extent from the loss of relevance of religious insti- a sample of 350 persons whether they understand
tutions and traditions. Many feel themselves bound by themselves as:
954 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

• Spiritual and religious (74%) The picture Smith (1998a) paints of faith-filled lives
• Spiritual but not religious (19%) emphasizes how individuals, by participating in the sym-
• Religious but not spiritual (4%) bols of their faith traditions have been transformed:
• Neither spiritual nor religious (3%)
To live religiously is not merely to live in the presence of
From these data, we see that the overwhelming ma- certain symbols, but to be involved with them or through
jority understood themselves as being both spiritual and them in a quite special way, a way that may lead far be-
religious (74%). Only 19% understood themselves as yond the symbols, that may demand of a person’s re-
being exclusively spiritual. Still, despite there being a sponse, and may affect one’s relations not only to them
relatively small percentage that understood themselves but to everything else: to oneself, to one’s neighbor and to
the stars. (p. 3)
to be exclusively spiritual, spiritual development is a po-
tentially powerful resource for positive human develop-
ment (Benson, Roehlkepartain, & Rude, 2003, p. 205). It But the main point here is that faith is about participa-
is, however, an understudied, complex, and multifaceted tion and response, not about belief. “One does not be-
concept, one that overlaps with many aspects of reli- lieve a symbol. Rather, one responds to it.” (Smith,
gious development, and one that is shaped by both indi- 1998a, p. 146).
vidual capacities and ecological influences—as we The consequences of conflating faith and belief have
discuss later. been to marginalize religion and to dismiss spirituality
It is the relatively small group that considers itself to as something less than rational:
be spiritual but not religious, which furnishes the popu-
lar stereotype that religion is antiquated for being insti- Belief became . . . the category of thought by which skep-
tutional and dogmatic, whereas spirituality is central for tics, reducing others’ faith to manageability, translated
that faith into mundane terms. They substituted for an in-
being personal and open. The wide-spread acceptance of
terest in it as faith an interest rather in the exotic mental
this stereotype has led Hill (2000) to warn against polar-
processes and conceptual framework of those whose lives
izing religiousness as bad and spirituality as good—be-
had been sustained and enriched by it. . . . What had been a
cause virtually all religions promote spirituality, and relation between the human and something external and
virtually all spiritual practices have at one time or an- higher . . . was transformed by the new thinking into a self-
other been promoted in religious traditions. subsistent, mundane operation of the mind. . . . To imagine
In the social sciences, the effort to distinguish reli- that religious persons “ believe” this or that is a way of
giousness from spirituality has not been matched by an dominating intellectually, and comfortably, what in fact
equal effort to specify what they have in common. Out- one does not truly discern. (W. C. Smith, 1998a, p. 144)
side the social sciences, there has been more said about
this issue. The work of Wilfred Cantwell Smith on faith With regard to the religious-spiritual distinction:
has been most influential (Smith, 1998a, 1998b). Faith need not refer to religious faith. Faith as involve-
Smith, the historian and long-time dean of compara- ment in a symbol system meant to define and support the
tive religious studies, has argued that faith is the central good life, such faith can be entirely secular faith. For
category in both religiousness and nonreligious spiritu- example, one legacy of classical Greece and Rome has
ality. As explained by him, faith refers to a person’s in- been a secular faith, which is, says Smith:
volvement in the symbols of a faith tradition rather than
to the symbols themselves. Faith is more a verb than a a living tradition with its own metaphysical underpinning,
noun. It is more about action and living a certain way its own great champions and even martyrs, its own institu-
than it is about something static such as a dogma, belief, tions, its own apprehension of or by transcendence, and,
or symbol. Smith points out that this meaning of faith is . . . its own type of faith. (1998a, p. 134)
much older than the newer meaning of faith as belief
(Smith, 1998a). It was the Enlightenment that spawned Using Smith’s conception of faith, we gain one way
the newer meaning and reduced religion to matters of of understanding what religiousness and spirituality
belief or to its alternative, feeling. have in common: They are both grounded in faith.
Defining Religious and Spiritual Development 955

Content, Form, and Function


tors? Given that there are no agreed on criteria, we
The last set of commonly accepted distinctions is the should not.
most difficult to explain, yet it may be the most crucial Nevertheless, content figures into analysis of reli-
for defining religious and spiritual development. Our gious and spiritual development in general and the de-
goal here is not simply to define what content, form, and velopment of prayer in particular—both to the extent
function mean with regard to religious and spiritual de- that content occasionally supports criteria for defining
velopment. Rather, our goal is also to define how these religious and spiritual maturity (e.g., acting in self-
three, taken together, point us in the direction of adopt- disinterested ways, pursuing noble purposes) and to the
ing a structural definition and explanation of religious extent that content reveals something about the overall
and spiritual development as development of persons. structuring of content, form, and function. So, for exam-
While there is general agreement over what we should ple, when the content of petitionary prayer includes
mean by content, form, and function, there is no such statements such as “If it be your will . . .,” then the con-
agreement over what we should mean by structure and tent makes explicit that the praying individual sees the
structural development. Beginning with what we can possibility of there being alternative perspectives or
agree on may help clarify what we should mean by reli- wills—a measure of decentering and therefore of devel-
gious and spiritual structural development. opment. It is reasonable to treat petitionary prayers that
To illustrate distinctions between content, form, and make this possibility explicit as being more developed
function, we use the phenomenon of personal prayer than petitionary prayers that do not, all other things
(Scarlett & Perriello, 1991). Personal prayer, as distinct being equal.
from the generally impersonal group prayers said during A better indicator of developmental differences has
religious services, offers a reasonable way to illustrate been, generally, differences in form. With respect to
distinctions, because personal prayer is both widespread prayer, there are the differences in types—petitionary,
and clearly religious in nature—particularly when indi- confessional, thanksgiving, and so on—and presumably
viduals invest their prayers with feeling. Furthermore, development has something to do with recognizing, un-
with age, personal prayers not only change but also de- derstanding, and being able to use each type. However,
velop. We focus mostly on petitionary personal prayers in each type, there are different forms that have differ-
because, of the various types of prayers, they are by far ent developmental meanings. For example, two confes-
the most common. sional type prayers may each contain specifications of
Prayer’s content refers to the specific themes, ideas, the same transgression, say, lying. However, only one of
and beliefs contained in prayer. Prayer may be about the two may specify a plan for becoming honest. Adding
healing, avoiding danger, hoping for success—almost the plan is more than adding content. It is making con-
anything and everything. It may be about gods, ances- fessional prayer into a more developed form, a form that
tors, or patron saints. It may be about something as small is more differentiated with respect to its parts. With the
and petty as finding a parking space or as large and example of prayer containing “If it be your will . . .,” the
noble as achieving world peace. added content is not clearly an added part making the
The variety of contents found in phenomena such as prayer into a more differentiated whole. The added con-
personal prayer points to both the value and limitations tent is, rather, an indicator of an underlying attitude and
of understanding religious and spiritual development in understanding of the relationship between person and
terms of its content only. On the one hand, content pro- the agency to which the prayer is directed.
vides a window on special circumstances, on what is on With regard to functions, prayer also provides exam-
an individual’s mind, and on how an individual may be ples. By definition, petitionary prayers serve the explicit
thinking in ways characteristic of his or her family, function of making requests. However, a closer inspec-
community, and culture. On the other hand, the infinite tion reveals petitionary prayers sometimes serve addi-
variety in content limits its value for defining develop- tional functions as well. Furthermore, these additional
ment in terms of content only. How, for example, should functions can sometimes help define development. For
we distinguish developmentally between prayer directed example, a child and an adult may both pray for a sick rel-
to a single divinity and prayer directed to gods or ances- ative to get well. However, the adult may add something
956 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

about not knowing what to do if things turn out for the critics and in so doing, have revitalized stage-structural
worse, if the sick relative dies. This little addition sug- theory. David Henry Feldman’s (in press) discussion
gests prayer serves the added function of helping the bears special mention.
praying individual to figure out, puzzle, or search for After outlining each of the main criticisms of Pi-
ways to understand or frame a crisis. This searching func- aget’s stage-structural theory, D. H. Feldman proposes
tion is hardly ever found in children’s prayers and may ways to answer the criticisms and retain stage as a use-
only occasionally be found in the prayers of adults— ful, even indispensable explanatory concept. His main
which tips us off to its being a function that can help de- suggestions include the following two, which we take to
fine development. be the most central.
Therefore, by analyzing and evaluating separately The first major criticism of stage-structural theories
the content, form, and functions of religious and spiri- such as Piaget’s is that they do not account for the wide
tual phenomena, such as prayer, we often gain a better variation in behavior one finds in any given stage. D. H.
understanding of religious and spiritual development. Feldman answers this criticism by suggesting we think
However, in doing so, we still are not able to define and of a stage as consisting of two substages—with the first
explain how individual persons develop. For this we substage devoted to constructing the stage and with the
need the concepts of stage and structure. second devoted to extending and applying the stage’s
system as widely as possible. Stages are marked by
their midpoints.
Stage and Structure
Using this way of thinking, stages emerge only grad-
The concepts of stage and structure offer indispensable ually and are only gradually replaced as the effort to ex-
means for defining and explaining the religious and spir- tend and apply begins to fail. In other words, stages
itual development of persons. They do so first in the way need not be thought of as structured wholes, which snap
they organize content, form, and function to reveal into place at particular moments in development. There
the full meaning of the religious and spiritual life. At- is an ebb and flow to their development, and variability
tending to content, form, and function separately or in in behavior or patterns of behavior, rather than under-
sequence can lead to overlooking the central character- mining the notion of stages, is another indicator of their
istics that distinguish mature from immature—the hid- development.
den structure that allows us to not only evaluate The second major criticism of stage-structural theo-
individual acts but also to evaluate the development of ries is that they do not provide adequate accounts of
persons. transition mechanisms. In the case of Piaget’s stage the-
Stage and structure also offer means to explain the ory, equilibration as a transition mechanism bears too
dynamic nature of religious and spiritual development. much of the burden for explaining transitions. D. H.
In the previously cited example of adding functions to Feldman answers this criticism by suggesting we keep
petitionary prayer, we see a transformation in the self ’s Piaget’s equilibration mechanism but give more empha-
structured way of being in the world—from being in the sis to other mechanisms to explain transitions—includ-
world as a self depending on some divine power to re- ing maturation and learning.
solve crises to being in the world as a self acting in con- Suggestions such as D. H. Feldman’s (in press) allow
cert with a divine power. The change from one to the us to hold on to the concepts of stage and structure to
other is neither arbitrary nor random. Rather, the change define and explain religious and spiritual development.
has profound meaning for adaptation—which is a major Once again, we find there is no reason to throw out the
reason why we judge the second way of structuring to be baby with the bath water.
more developed. In sum, content, form, and function are commonly
This view of stage and structure differs markedly accepted concepts and distinctions used for defining
from the views of current critics of stage theory—includ- and evaluating religious and spiritual development.
ing critics of religious and spiritual stage theories. Post- However, when used by themselves, they do not ade-
Piagetian critics have dismissed stage-structural theory quately account for the development of persons. Stage
primarily for not providing an adequate account of vari- and structure are less commonly accepted concepts.
ability and not accounting for transition processes. How- However, if used in ways that address the main criti-
ever, recently, a number of theorists have answered the cisms of stage-structural theories, stage and structure
Stage-Structural Theories 957

become indispensable concepts for defining and explain- They are frames of mind and heuristics of beliefs, not
ing the religious and spiritual development of persons. tangible realities.
The neo-Kantian philosopher, Ernst Cassirer, pro- Having explained the rationale for using the concepts
vides an example of how stage-structural analysis can be of stage and structure, we turn now to discussing spe-
indispensable in defining, evaluating, and explaining the cific stage-structural theories of religious and spiritual
religious and spiritual development of persons. For Cas- development.
sirer, at the lower stages of religious and spiritual devel-
opment, individuals live in a mytho-poetic world where
symbol and referent are fused and where there is no dis- STAGE-STRUCTURAL THEORIES
tinction between meaning and existence (Cassirer,
1955). Cassirer distinguishes between mythical and reli- The stage-structural theories to be discussed here are all
gious consciousness with the latter being more devel- in the constructivist tradition associated with the work
oped. However, myth (a tradition’s imagination) and of Jean Piaget. Because Piaget’s theory focused on epis-
religion are inseparable. Cassirer writes: temology and cognitive development, stage-structural
models of religious and spiritual development are often
If we attempt to isolate and remove the basic mythical criticized for being too cognitive. Critics assume that
components from religious belief, we no longer have reli-
any off-shoot of Piaget’s theory is also bound to be too
gion in its real, objectively historical manifestation; all
cognitive.
that remains is a shadow of it, an empty abstraction.
Yet, although the contents of myth and religion are inex-
It is ironic that few know about Piaget’s early reflec-
tricably interwoven, their form is not the same. And the tions on religious and spiritual development—what is
particularity of the religious form is disclosed in the generally understood to be a Piagetian approach to reli-
changed attitude which consciousness here assumes to- gious and spiritual development is not at all Piagetian.
ward the mythical image world. It cannot do without this We begin with a brief summary of Piaget’s actual ap-
world, it cannot immediately reject it; but seen through the proach to religious and spiritual development to better
medium of the religious attitude this world gradually frame the discussion of stage-structural theories.
takes on a new meaning. From his late teens until his early 30s, Piaget preoc-
The new ideality, the new spiritual dimension, that is cupied himself with the question of how one can be-
opened up through religion not only lends myth a new sig- lieve in God and remain objective (Reich, 2005). His
nification but actually introduces the opposition between
questioning led to a significant development in his
“meaning” and “existence” into the realm of myth. Reli-
thinking: to his distinguishing between a transcendent
gion takes the decisive step that is essentially alien to
myth: in its use of sensuous images and signs it recognizes
and immanent God. For Piaget, God initially meant the
them as such—a means of expression which, though they God of conservative theology—a transcendent and
reveal a determinate meaning, must necessarily remain in- mysterious God whose laws must be followed slav-
adequate to it, which “point ” to this meaning but never ishly. Over time and in the context of actively debating
wholly exhaust it. (p. 239) and discussing, Piaget developed a very different
meaning of God so that eventually God, for him, went
In Cassirer’s account, we see how important struc- inward. In rejecting transcendence in favor of imma-
tural differences can be for defining what is essential nence, Piaget came to identify God with the heart,
in religious and spiritual development. For Cassirer, it with the norms of reason, and with the internalized ex-
is the transformation from living in a mytho-poetic ample of Jesus.
world to living in a world where the myths and poetry Piaget’s model of religious development centered
of faith traditions become symbols pointing to truths to around a transformation from transcendent to imma-
live by. Others who engage in stage-structural analysis nent meanings of divinity. This transformation was far
find different ways to characterize what is essential. more than a cognitive transformation and certainly
Whatever the ways, the effort is the same, explaining more than developing faith in reason. To be sure, rea-
not only the development of acts, thoughts, and feelings son was there in his model but also the heart and a
but also explaining the development of persons. In gen- faith tradition. He makes this clear in a lecture he gave
eral, critics forget that stages and structures are heuris- in 1929, to members of the Swiss Christian Students
tic instruments for understanding this development: Association:
958 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

if, beyond men, one examines the currents of thought that book is ultimately derived from the pedagogy of religion:
propagate from generation to generation, immanentism ap- “ To know what a child is able to grasp intellectually is a
pears as the continuation of the impulse of spiritualization surer foundation for education than to know only what
that characterizes the history of the notion of divinity. The adults feel the child ought to grasp” (p. xi). Goldman re-
same progress is accomplished from the transcendental
ferred to the decisive work of Jean Piaget—not so much
God endowed with supernatural causality to the purely
to Piaget’s early work, “ The Child’s Conception of the
spiritual God of immanent experience, as from the semi-
World” (1929), which investigated children’s theories—
material God of primitive religions to the metaphysical
God. Now—and this is the essential point—to this progress and the stages of cognitive development. In so doing,
in the realm of intelligence corresponds a moral and social Goldman adopted the position that, “Religious thinking
progress, that is, ultimately an emancipation of inner life. is not different in mode and method from nonreligious
(Vidal, p. 287) thinking. Religious thinking is . . . the activity of think-
ing toward religion” (p. 3).
Piaget never developed a research program to test out Unlike earlier studies in the psychology of religion,
his developmental theory of religious and spiritual which merely described children’s religious concepts
development. Whatever the reasons may be for Piaget’s without explaining them, Goldman’s approach had the
abandoning his public reflections on religious and spir- advantage of treating diverse impressions of children in
itual development, his early discussions clearly indi- a theoretically based sequence. Goldman asked chil-
cate that a true Piagetian approach to religious and dren and young people about their religious concep-
spiritual development demands much more than attend- tions such as “God’s omnipotence,” “ the Bible,” and
ing to cognition and reasoning. “Jesus.” He also asked children about their interpreta-
However, in the early 1960s, when Piaget’s cognitive tions of three biblical stories: Moses and the burning
developmental theory was eclipsing all other theories of bush, the crossing of the Red Sea, and the temptation
cognitive development, Piaget’s early reflections were of Christ. He assigned children’s interpretations to
virtually forgotten. The result was an application of Pi- three stages of religious cognition and matched them to
aget that was not at all Piagetian and that was indeed Piaget’s stages of cognitive development—as indicated
overly cognitive. The clearest example is the work of in Table 17.1.
Ronald Goldman. From this summary table, we see that Goldman con-
ceptualized mature religious thought moving beyond the
“childish and immature religious ideas of children”
Cognitive-Stage Theories: Ronald Goldman
(p. 67) to be capable of seeing the symbolic-metaphorical
Goldman’s (1964) classic study “Religious Thinking structure of religious language in general and biblical
from Childhood to Adolescence” is a landmark in the re- language in particular. Using the findings of his research,
search on religious development. The main concern of the Goldman drew provocative consequences for the peda-

TABLE 17.1 Stages of Piaget and Goldman Compared

Piaget Goldman
Preoperational Intuitive Religious Thought
Religious contents comprehended unsystematically, fragmentarily, frequently in a magical way, God represented
anthropomorphously; frquent transductive conclusions: “ Why did Moses not go to the bush?” “Because there was
a sign saying ‘Keep off the Grass.’ ”

Concrete operational Concrete Religious Thought


Magical and animistic elements receding; religious concepts presented in a more coherent and objective way—
though their symbolic-metaphorical nature not yet apprehended: “ The burning bush had a fire behind it, it only
seemed to be burning.”

Formal operational Abstract Religious Thought


Religious contents now ref lected in a hypothetical-deductive manner with symbols recognized as symbols: “ The
burning bush is a symbol that God is there.”
Stage-Structural Theories 959

gogy of religion, notably that children should no longer be By far, the most common criticism of Goldman’s
confronted with the Bible because they glaringly misun- work has been directed at Goldman’s essentially nar-
derstand biblical texts. row cognitive focus. Critics have pointed out that reli-
Goldman inspired an abundance of studies replicat- gious consciousness includes far more than what is
ing his work (see reviews by Hyde, 1990, p. 15–63; Slee, implied in Goldman’s research, particularly with re-
1986a). Peatling’s (1974) multiple-choice test and his gard to the use of religious symbols (Godin, 1968b). A
devised “ Thinking about the Bible Test ” became widely special problem is that Goldman did not deal with the
known and used. Other psychologists of religion made development of religiousness, sui generis, but rather
use of the semi-clinical interview (Elkind, 1964), and with cognitive structures to the extent that they rub off
they also applied Piaget’s cognitive stages to religious on religious content.
concepts. The overall picture painted was that, with in- Probably as a reaction to Goldman’s overestimation
creasing age, concrete modes of thinking recede and are of cognitive processes, the first empirically based the-
replaced by more abstract religious thinking (e.g., Tam- ory of religious and spiritual development of the 1980s
minnen, 1976). was holistic in nature and included social, moral, reli-
However, even while Goldman inspired others to gious, spiritual, and identity formation parts and treated
apply Piagetian stage analysis to religious material, he faith as having a much broader meaning than belief. It is
also drew widespread criticism. His work was criticized this theory of James Fowler that we discuss next.
for its faulty methods (Langdon, 1969; Slee, 1986a,
1986b). For example, religious semantics can be more
Faith Developmental Theory: James Fowler
appropriately based on word choice procedures than on
interview questions foreign to children’s natural ways Fowler’s focus is broad because his view of faith is
of thinking (Murphy, 1978). Furthermore, research broad. Following the lead of theologians Paul Tillich, H.
showed that abstract religious thought coincides posi- Richard Niebuhr, and Wilfred Cantwell Smith, Fowler
tively not with the acceptance of religious, particularly (1981) defines faith as a universal quality of persons, a
biblical, contents but with rejection of that content quality defined by the way persons orient themselves
(Hoge & Petrillo, 1978), and children’s understanding in the cosmos and, at higher stages, in an ideal, yet-to-
of biblical content is more adequately explained by be-achieved or experienced ultimate environment. In
other stage-structural theories such as Oser and Gmün- Judeo-Christian thinking, this ultimate environment is
der’s (Bucher, 1991). defined as the “ kingdom of God,” the endpoint of
More recently, cognitive scientists informed by re- human history. Faith so defined is reflected in how indi-
search on “ theory of mind” as well as by cognitive an- viduals make or find meaning, how they define centers
thropology, have questioned Goldman’s work for of value and power, and how they adopt symbols and sto-
overstating differences between children’s and adult’s ries to reveal or express their faith. For Fowler, faith is
ways of thinking about religion. On the one hand, chil- far more than belief or reasoning.
dren’s thinking about counterintuitive religious con- Fowler’s (1981) focus is also broad because faith
cepts, such as the concept of an all-seeing God, must developmental theory is intent on capturing the overall
be judged in the context of children’s intuitive psychol- psychological development of persons. His theory bor-
ogy and in the context of what adults teach children rows heavily from Erikson, Piaget, and Kohlberg, and
about religious concepts. When so judged, children’s to a lesser extent from Sullivan, Stern, Rizzuto, and
thinking appears far more reasonable and sophisticated Kegan. His stages take into consideration an individ-
than Goldman’s theory leads us to believe (Boyer & ual’s development with respect to major developmental
Walker, 2000; Harris, 2000). On the other hand, adults tasks including identity achievement, cognitive devel-
often give theologically correct answers to questions opment, moral judgment, symbol formation, social
about religious concepts, but, in their own personal perspective taking, and locus of control. For Fowler,
lives, they function with religious concepts not alto- the development of faith and the development of per-
gether different than what Goldman ascribed to chil- sons are so intertwined as to be, to a large extent, one
dren (J. Barrett & Keil, 1996). We have more to say in the same.
about this line of criticism when discussing research At the heart of Fowler’s (1981) view of faith develop-
on children’s religious concepts. ment is the issue of individuation in a pluralistic society.
960 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

From this view, the central dilemma for individuals is TABLE 17.2 Fowler’s Stages of Faith Development
that of being true to ourselves, our feelings, intuitions, Stage One: Primal Faith (Infancy)
beliefs, and understandings, while, at the same time, Prelinguistic faith defined by the trust developing between infants
maintaining or constructing ethical, positive relation- and their caregivers and by the psychological bonds created
through affect attunement.
ships with increasingly diverse communities.
At first, this dilemma is played out in the family and Stage Two: Intuitive-Projective Faith (Early Childhood)
with one’s inherited extra-familial community and faith Faith defined by the images created to represent both threatening
and protective powers, by representations of God that derive from
tradition, then with self-consciously adopted communi-
experience with significant caregivers, by the awakening of moral
ties and faith traditions, and, ultimately, with the com- standards, and by continued reliance on and referencing to
munity of humankind. As with Oser and Gmünder’s caregivers.
(1991) stage theory, Fowler’s faith developmental the- Stage Three: Mythic-Literal Faith (Elementary School Years)
ory is meant to define an ideal sequence (paradigm of Faith defined by widening sources of authority to include those
perfection). Only a few reach the highest stage. outside the family so as to create a community of like-minded, by the
Fowler’s way of construing faith and human develop- appreciation of myths and narratives taken literally and valued for
their ability to explain and express faith, and by anthropomorphic
ment shows in the descriptions of his stages of faith images/conceptions of God.
development. Table 17.2 provides a brief overview of
Stage Four: Synthetic-Conventional Faith (Adolescence
those stages (Fowler, 1981). and Adulthood)
Fowler’s stage model was applied to transcripts Faith defined by the development of a worldview derived from
made following two-hour interviews with over 350 sub- conventionally or consensually sanctioned authorities, by self-
jects. The interviews made use of a “Faith Development identity forming a synthesis of the perceptions of others so that
identity is defined in terms of belonging (to family, ethnic group,
Instrument ” designed to elicit and support discussion sex role, religion) and/or possessing, and by undeveloped ability to
directly relevant to getting at subjects faith, as faith understand and identify with groups with worldviews different
was defined by Fowler. from one’s own.
The results of Fowler’s (1981) own research indi- Stage Five: Individuative-Ref lective Faith (Late Adolescence
cated that almost all of the preschoolers in the sample to Adulthood)
had an intuitive-projective faith; almost three quarters Faith defined by the self-conscious, explicit examination of
commitments, beliefs, and values leading to critical examination
of the elementary school children had a mythic-literal
of the individual’s faith heritage and a self-conscious choosing
faith, and almost half of the adolescents interviewed had of the faith tradition and community to which the individual
a synthetic-conventional faith. Among adults, the vari- belongs. Faith at this stage indicates the individual takes charge
ance was greater, with 40% younger than 30 having an of his/or life in defining self and commitments – often limiting
the individual’s ability to connect to faith-based groups and
individuative-reflective type of faith and 18% having a faith traditions and to take the perspective of alternative
synthetic-conventional type. Conjunctive faith was rare groups.
and occurred only after age 40. There was only one sub- Stage Six: Conjunctive Faith: (Average Age: About 30)
ject categorized as having a universalizing type faith. Faith defined as self-conscious commitment to ethical action
There have been at least 11 empirical studies repli- based on fully internalized principles and to a vision of meaning,
cating Fowler’s original study, and 26 others using the coherence, and value that fully accepts the limitations of the self
to understand fully and act purely. Faith at this stage is
faith development interview technique (Streib, 2001). maintained without the props of external authority and may be
Several cross-cultural studies provide inconclusive evi- described as paradoxical faith inasmuch as commitments are
dence about the suitability of Fowler’s stages for study- made in the context of self-doubt and despair that allows for
appreciation of alternative perspectives but threatens passivity
ing faith development in non-Western cultures. Part of
and inability to act.
the difficulty has been in using the faith development in-
Stage Seven: Universalizing Faith: (Average Age: Minimum
terview method. Attempts to devise a shorter method to
about 40)
measure faith development have not been successful.
Faith defined as overcoming paradox through identifying with the
Since conducting his study in the 1970s, Fowler ultimate conditions of existence (“ kingdom of God”). Faith at
(1981) has focused more on making faith developmental Stage Seven often appears to be simple compared to faith at Stages
theory useful for practitioners, particularly in the fields Five and Six. Self and other are, at Stage Seven, viewed and
understood from a cosmic, transcendent perspective which allows
of pastoral counseling and religious education. However, for the feeling of unity and meaning.
he has continued to modify the theory in response to
criticisms. Concerning gender bias and overemphasis on
Stage-Structural Theories 961

autonomy issues at stage four, Fowler has placed more ship to an ultimate being or reality. With contingency
emphasis on relational knowing to characterize faith at situations, there is ambiguity, which can be resolved
stage four, and in response to criticisms concerning cul- through a special kind of interpreting that Oser and
tural bias, Fowler has placed greater emphasis on the de- Gmünder (1991) identify as being religious or spiritual
constructing power of specific cultural contexts, which in nature. This chapter opened with such an example—
call for more emphasis on faith development being the example of one person interpreting another’s good
viewed in the context of culture and needing specific fortune as having a religious meaning. Other examples
conditions for development to proceed beyond the lower include interpretations of misfortune. Therefore, study-
stages. ing reactions to contingency situations offers one way
More recent stage-structural theories of development to understand the development of religious judgment
refer again to Piaget’s and Kohlberg’s conceptions of and reasoning.
growth; however, instead of considering religious and Oser and Gmünder’s (1991) focus was on the qualita-
spiritual experiences only as they reflect general stages tively different ways that individuals reason religiously
of logical thinking and cognition, these newer theories about the conflicts elicited by existential contingency
consider genuine religious experience, feelings, and con- situations. Contingency situations are not controllable in
victions as they reflect religious judgment, reasoning, human life. They stimulate the beginning of religious-
and cognition. Clear examples occur in the research of ness, but they also stimulate conflicts in persons, which
Oser, Gmünder, and Reich—which we discuss next. are defined as polar opposites needing to be differenti-
ated and coordinated. Each polar opposite defines some
issue that may be brought to bear when interpreting a
Stages of Religious Judgment and Reasoning:
contingency situation. As noted in Table 17.3, Oser and
Fritz Oser and Paul Gmünder
Gmünder list several polar opposites as central.
Goldman’s research was on judgment and reasoning These polar opposites can be used to reason about
about religious content and concepts. Oser and Gmün- how best to interpret and react to contingency situations.
der’s research is on religious judgment and reasoning They function in ways similar to the Kantian categories
about unspecified content (Oser & Gmünder, 1991). The of thinking (time, space, causality, etc.) needed to orga-
distinction is important. In Goldman’s case, judgment nize thinking in general.
and reasoning was the issue and religion was the content. From a genetic point of view, at lower stages of devel-
In Oser and Gmünder’s case, religiousness is the issue opment, individuals choose only one of the polar oppo-
and judgment and reasoning is a means to understanding sites. Either they are dependent, or they are free. Either
religiousness. In Goldman’s case, the focus is on the they are in a holy space or in a profane space—and so
cognitive domain. In Oser and Gmünder’s case, the forth. By adolescence, individuals usually consider both
focus is on the religious domain. Here, we see religious polar opposites simultaneously. There is trust and mis-
judgment and reasoning treated as a “mother structure” trust, transcendence and immanence, and so on. Fur-
or way of thinking that cannot be reduced to some other thermore, each polar opposite is seen as having meaning
way of thinking. in life. Here is transcendence, and there is immanence,
However, because religious judgment and reasoning and both can be at the same time. Later, at higher stages
constitute judgment and reasoning about something, of development, one polar opposite can be taken as a
the question arises as to what religious judgment and
reasoning are about. For Oser and Gmünder (1991), re- TABLE 17.3 Polar Opposites to Be
Coordinated for Religious
ligious judgment and reasoning are not about meaning-
Judgment and Reasoning
making of all life, but from a measurement point of
view are most apt to be about resolving ambiguities Freedom versus dependence
Transcendence versus immanence
elicited in particular kinds of situations, here described
Hope versus absurdity
as contingency situations.
Transparency versus opacity
Contingency situations elicit religious judgment and
Eternal versus ephemeral
reasoning primarily because they raise questions about Trust versus mistrust
who or what is ultimately in control and also about a va- Holy versus profane
riety of conflicts having to do with a person’s relation-
962 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

fundamental condition for the other, so, for example, the adapt to a wide variety of contexts or situations—not
experience of absurdity can become a condition for ex- just to the narrow set of contexts and situations experi-
periencing hope, and freedom can become a prerequisite enced in, say, childhood. Furthermore, development
for experiencing healthy dependency. here is assumed to help prevent negative dependency,
But what exactly does reasoning with the aid of seven dogmatic positions, intolerant behavior, and aggressive
polar opposites organize and accomplish? Oser and forms of religious expressions.
Gmünder’s (1991) answer is that it helps define how in- What development and adaptation mean differs ac-
dividuals understand their relationship to God or to cording to theoretical perspective, as we see in the next
whatever individuals owe a commitment. Because some section. Here, development is defined as structural de-
faith traditions speak of a collection of personal agencies velopment and approximations to an ideal endpoint (par-
(gods, ancestors, etc.) while others speak of an imper- adigm of perfection). In other theories, development
sonal force or reality (dharma, Tao), Oser and Gmünder may be defined more as content, intensity, and adapta-
referred to the person-“Ultimate” relationship—with ul- tion to specific circumstances (paradigm of coping).
timate referring to that which one owes commitment to Two further comments to clarify the stage theory of
ultimately. For example, devote Buddhists often treat religious judgment: First, though the focus here is on
dharma (cosmic law) as an Ultimate reality to which one contingency situations, the theory suggests that reli-
owes commitment (W. C. Smith, 1998a). gious judgment and reasoning may occur in a wide va-
A clear example of how the polar opposites figure riety of situations, not just contingency situations.
into reasoning about the person-Ultimate relationship William James’ saints were, after all, continually judg-
occurs when individuals, faced with contingency situa- ing and reasoning religiously. Second, though the the-
tions, reason about how free and/or how dependent they ory refers to religious judgment and reasoning, the
are with respect to a higher power or reality. Contin- term spiritual judgment and reasoning could work just
gency situations are, after all, situations that raise ques-
tions about control. The main assumption here is that
reasoning well about the person-Ultimate relationship
leads to more adaptive ways of living that serve not only TABLE 17.4 Stages of Religious Judgment in the Oser and
the individual but also those around the individual. Gmünder (1991) Model
This does not mean that less developed ways are nec- Stage 1: Orientation of Religious Heteronomy—Deus ex Machina
essarily less adaptive—because adaptation is always a God (the Ultimate) is understood as active and as intervening
unexpectedly in the affairs of the world; persons are conceived as
matter of the match between person and context. A
mostly reactive to God’s (the Ultimate’s) power and interventions.
child’s way of reasoning may be perfectly matched to
Stage 2: Orientation of Do et Des—“Give so you may receive”
his or her protective surroundings. The theory suggests
God (the Ultimate) is understood to be external, all-powerful, and
that it is adaptive for children to reason religiously as if
intervening, however, God (the Ultimate) now can be inf luenced
there are no apparent conflicts created by the presence through promises, prayers, and deeds.
of evil, tragedy, and injustice. However, it is not adaptive Stage 3: Orientation of Ego Autonomy and One-Sided
for adults to reason without taking into account these Responsibility
and other conflicts. In short, for adults, the standards The inf luence of God (the Ultimate) is consciously reduced. The
for adaptive behavior are higher. individual is conceived of as responsible for his or her own life.
The Ultimate Being (if accepted as real) has a separate and hidden
Furthermore, saying development is about adapting existence and responsibility/function.
does not mean that immature ways need to be discour-
Stage 4: Orientation of Mediated Autonomy
aged. On the contrary, the model suggests that most of
God (the Ultimate) is mediated through immanence—as in
the time, religious reasoning is directed toward adapting developing faith in a divine plan and living life accordingly. Social
and is open to being modified and transformed as the engagement becomes a form of religious/spiritual expression.
need arises. Refer back to the previous section’s com- Stage 5: Orientation of Unconditional Religiosity
ments about consolidating and expanding phases in The individual feels that he or she is always and unconditionally
stages as well as to transition mechanisms, and Oser and related to the Ultimate Being—so that each moment is or can have
a religious/spiritual dimension. Religious and spiritual reasoning
Gmünder’s stage approach becomes clearer. displays complete differentiation and coordination of the seven
What development of religious reasoning means is polar opposites.
that, with development, individuals are more able to
Stage-Structural Theories 963

as well—especially given the fact that certain kinds of a plane about to crash. The young man promises God that,
atheism and agnosticism retain notions of a person- if he survives the crash, he will forego a promising career
Ultimate relationship. and serve humanity in a third world country. Paul sur-
This brings us, at last, to the heart of the theory: to vives, and subjects are asked to respond to a series of
how the polar opposites become increasingly differen- questions to elicit reasoning about Paul’s promise,
tiated and coordinated and created stages defining how whether he should keep his promise toward God, what
individuals understand their relationship with an Ulti- will happen if he doesn’t, and how, in general, he should
mate being. Table 17.4 lists the stages and provides think and behave in reaction to his surviving the crash.
brief descriptions. A coding manual allowed subjects’ responses to the
Again, independent of the religious denomination Paul dilemma and to other hypothetical dilemmas to be
( jewish, catholics, protestants): evaluated in terms of the seven polar dimensions—and
allowed for subjects to be assigned to a particular stage
• At the lower stages, there is little or no effort to coor- of religious judgment. Here are typical responses to the
dinate the polar opposites. Either God (the Ultimate question of whether Paul should keep his promise:
Being) intervenes, or He (It) does not intervene.
Either we feel hopeful, or we do not feel hopeful. Stage One: “Paul has to keep his promise, otherwise
Either an event reveals God’s (the Ultimate Being’s) God will give him a stomach ache.”
will or that will remains inscrutable. And so it goes Stage Two: “God helped Paul so Paul now has to do
with the rest of the polar opposites, as individuals some good.” Or, “If Paul does not keep his promise,
continue to think in “Either . . . or . . .” terms. he has to do something else. He has to pray so that he
won’t be punished.”
• At middle stages, polar opposites become differenti-
Stage Three: “ This has nothing to do with God. Paul
ated and opposed to one another—without there
has to make his own decision. God, if he exists, has
being a way to coordinate or balance them. For exam-
other problems.”
ple, at a middle stage, individuals may see themselves
Stage Four: “Whatever decision Paul makes, he will
as free to make their own choices while, at the same
likely use his faith in God to act responsibly—be-
time, they may feel that God (or the Ultimate Being)
cause acting responsibly is really what is God’s will.
is simultaneously doing the same, possibly in opposi-
And, in the end, things will be okay.”
tion to what they are doing.
Stage Five: “Whether he goes (to serve in a third world
• At higher stages, the polar opposites interpenetrate
country) or does not go is unimportant; what is im-
one another so that, for example, freedom is achieved
portant is that Paul fulfills the larger promise to meet
through remaining dependent on God (the Ultimate
the many challenges he will face throughout life, in
Being); transcendence emerges indirectly through
the light of what he gathers to be God’s will. God is
concrete actions so that immanence and transcen-
present in every human communication and so it does
dence are intertwined, and transparency and opacity
not matter where exactly Paul ends up.”
become integrated as indicated by phrases such as,
“We see, but through a glass darkly.” (1 Corinthians
Again, no single answer or response was enough to as-
13:12, New Revised Standard Version). With respect
sign a subject to a particular stage. It was the collec-
to the polar opposites, what were originally experi-
tive response that determined how subjects were
enced as conflicting opposites come to be experi-
assigned to stages.
enced as integrated dualities that define religious and
The results of the original and subsequent studies
spiritual experience.
have proven the usefulness of this stage model for (a)
describing age changes, ( b) understanding religious be-
Oser and Gmünder’s (1991) stage theory of religious havior, (c) looking at conversion experiences, and (d) es-
judgment was developed, tested, and validated using a tablishing the value and significance of developing
semistructured clinical interview method that asked sub- religious judgment and reasoning.
jects to respond to hypothetical contingency situations. With respect to age changes, significant age trends
The best known of these situations is the “Paul dilemma.” were established with European samples, not only in
Subjects are first told the story of a young man caught in cross-sectional studies (Oser & Gmünder, 1991), but
964 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

also in longitudinal studies as well (Di Loreto & Oser, With respect to conversion experiences, conversions
1996). Together, the cross-sectional and longitudinal into open, but not into closed, narrow religious groups are
studies suggest that children and adolescents normally associated with stage development (Wenger Jindra, 2004).
progress through the first three stages of religious With respect to self-effectiveness in religious beliefs
judgment. After adolescence, the majority of adults re- (SEB), individuals at stage 3 show considerably less
main at stage 3. This finding may be explained, in part, SEB than do individuals at lower and higher stages (Rol-
by cognitive development being the prime transition lett & Kager, 1999). SEB is strongest at stage 2, which is
mechanism at lower stages (e.g., developing capacity to consistent with the overall stage 2 orientation emphasiz-
reason about contradictions) while emotional develop- ing the influence that individuals can have on God or the
ment figures more in the transitions at higher stages Ultimate Being.
(Beile, 1998). As for the value and significance of developing reli-
These findings were replicated in sociocultural gious judgment, three studies suggest that stages may in-
contexts as different as India (Hindu and Buddhist), fluence how individuals cope with adversity and come to
Tibet and Rhwanda (ancestor worship; Dick, 1981). think positively. In one study of parents with either
Another finding is that in adolescence, girls score Down syndrome or autistic children, parents at higher
higher than boys—though the differences are small and stages were more likely to use social support than were
disappear with the transition to adulthood (Reich, parents at stage 3 (Gnos, 2003). In a similar vein, another
1997; Schweitzer, 1992). study provided evidence that widows were overcoming
As for the usefulness of the stages for understanding sadness at a higher stage level than non-widows, suggest-
religious behavior, the stages shed light on individual ing that, for at least some widows, stage development
differences in (a) involvement in religious communi- may occur to cope with the death of a spouse (Zen-
ties, ( b) interpretations of religious texts, and (c) self- klusen, 2003). Finally, in one study, stimulating the de-
effectiveness in religious beliefs. velopment of thinking about content with spiritual
With respect to involvement in religious communi- meaning had direct effects on social consciousness and
ties, individuals at stages 1 and 2 are more likely to pre- on well-being (Blakeney & Blakeney, 2005).
fer religious communities that require unconditional Therefore, Oser and Gmünder’s (1991) stage theory
subservience from members, but individuals at higher has proven useful and in a number of ways. In particular,
stages are more likely to prefer liberal communities the theory has proven useful for understanding how reli-
(Zondag & Belzen, 1999). Furthermore, individuals at gious judgment and reasoning figure into overall reli-
stage 2 are more likely to have stronger ties to religious gious development.
institutions, attach greater importance to sacraments,
and expect more support from religion (Kager, 1995). Fowler and Oser and Gmünder’s Theories Compared
Finally, with respect to religious communities, persons The similarities between the theories of Fowler (1981)
at lower stages are more likely to prefer religious bond- and Oser and Gmünder (1991) are evident. Both are in-
ing relationships analogous to the mother-child relation- debted to genetic structuralism. Both are in the tradition
ship, but persons at higher stages are more likely to of the Enlightenment. Both describe a development that
prefer such relationships to take the form of cooperating leads from the particular to the universal and from het-
friends (Kager, 1995). eronomy to autonomy. Both establish significant age
With respect to interpretation of religious texts, trends, and the stages from both theories, to a certain
individuals at stages 1 and 2 tend to understand reli- extent, parallel one another. For example, in a direct
gious language literally rather than metaphorically or comparative study by Tamminnen (1994), results
symbolically (Zondag & Belzen, 1999)—and in a man- showed that in the adult age group, most subjects were at
ner consistent with their do ut des orientation. So, for stage 3 in Oser and Gmünder’s scheme and at the paral-
example, individuals at stage 2 tend to interpret mira- lel stage 4 in Fowler’s. Notwithstanding these similari-
cles reported in the Bible as pay-back for biblical fig- ties, there are differences (for a fuller discussion, see
ures being good, and parables such as the parable of Nipkow, Schweitzer, & Fowler, 1988).
the workers in the vineyard who received a full day’s First, Fowler (1981) incorporates into his scheme of
pay for working only an hour were interpreted as stages many different kinds of psychological elements,
showing that God rewards good work (Bucher, 1991). including moral and social elements, the development of
Stage-Structural Theories: Concerns 965

self-hood and self-identity, type of thinking, and com- RCR reasoning is useful in situations involving com-
prehension of symbols. In contrast, Oser and Gmünder peting, bona fide explanations. It is useful because of its
(1991) concentrate on religious matters only to the ex- underlying trivalent logic, which can lead to categorizing
tent they can be used to cope with life in the face of the competing explanations as being compatible, incompati-
Absolute or Divine. ble, or noncompatible. Noncompatible refers to instances
Fowler speaks of faith in a very general sense, when competing explanations are both (all) correct, de-
which has attracted the criticism that we are dealing pending on context.
here with an “everything and nothing view of faith” Using the construct of RCR and working in the
(Fernhout, 1986, p. 66). In contrast, Oser and Gmün- framework established by Oser and Gmünder (1991),
der (1991) speak of religious judgment for establishing Reich’s (1993) research has focused on how individuals
control in a situation of life through a person’s regulat- develop ways to resolve apparent contradictions having
ing the relationship ( bounding) between him- or her- religious meaning. Examples include the problem of
self and the Absolute. evil (How can there be a just and all-powerful God
Fowler (1981) views the normative goal of religious when there is evil and tragedy?) and the contrasting
education in terms of the biblical metaphor of the king- biblical and scientific accounts of how the universe
dom of God (or “reigning of God”), whereas Oser and came to be.
Gmünder (1991) view this goal as not determining the Reich’s (1993) research has demonstrated how indi-
characteristics of stages. viduals, with age and with development, resolve these
While Fowler’s (1981) approach may be more suit- apparent religious contradictions using RCR reasoning.
able for raising questions related to life history and exis- Development is described in the following stages:
tential themes, Oser and Gmünder’s (1991) approach is
more about the transformation dynamics of cognitive Stage One: Only one competing explanation is recog-
structures. Fowler is more interested in the implications nized.
of his stage theory for supporting pastoral care, while Stage Two: One or more competing explanations are
Oser and Gmünder are more interested in scientific is- recognized.
sues such as the issue of validating stages. Stage Three: Competing explanations are seen as
Approaches have their own history and their own needed for full understanding.
advantages and drawbacks. Oser and Gmünder’s Stage Four: The relation between competing explana-
(1991) approach is admittedly less explanatory and tions is analyzed, and the situation specificity of the
more sharply focused on God-man connectedness, but relative contributions of each is at least intimated.
this could be interpreted as a strength. Fowler’s (1981) Stage Five: An overarching theory or synopsis is con-
approach is more faith related and thus more holistic, structed to specify the complex and mutual rela-
and this, too, can be seen as strength. Therefore, to a tionships between competing explanations, and the
certain extent, these approaches or theories serve com- situation specificity of their explanatory weight.
plementary roles for understanding religious and spiri-
tual development. So, for example, stage 5 responses to the biblical versus
scientific explanations of the origins of the universe
The Relational and Contextual Model of and humankind might emphasize God’s setting in mo-
Development: Helmut Reich tion Darwinian evolution and/or the essentially moral-
spiritual meaning explained in biblical accounts versus
There are numerous religious phenomena calling for the essentially material-physical meaning explained in
more specialized reasoning than that explained in the scientific accounts.
Oser and Gmünder’s (1991) stage model. In particular, At the end of this focus on relational judgment and
there are religious dogmas and religious conflicts that reasoning as they both define religious and spiritual de-
raise intellectual challenges for any believer who feels velopment, we admit that they do not need to lead to re-
obligated to think logically and scientifically. These ducing religious and spiritual development to general
more specific challenges call for the development of a (e.g., Piagetian) stages of cognitive development. Fur-
specialized way of reasoning that Reich (1993) has thermore, we see that adopting such a focus allows for
named relational and contextual reasoning (RCR). precision in measuring and explaining.
966 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

STAGE-STRUCTURAL do indeed go beyond the evidence to overemphasize the


THEORIES: CONCERNS role of cognition.
However, overemphasis on cognition is not intrinsic
Stage-structural theories of development in general and to stage-structural theories. Indeed, Oser and Gmün-
religious and spiritual development in particular have der’s (1991) theory as well as Fowler’s (1981) include in
come under attack for several reasons. The main reasons their descriptions of stages many issues, conflicts, and
have to do with (a) the linear and normative treatment of concerns that are obviously social and emotional in na-
development, ( b) the emphasis on cognition, judgment, ture. For example, both theories place at the center of re-
and reasoning, (c) the emphasis on presumed universals ligious and spiritual development, not the ability to
to the exclusion of individual and cultural differences, think logically, but the ability to coordinate conflicts
and (d) the mostly positive/optimistic view of structural between individuation and making positive connections
development that overlooks pathology among those at to God (the Ultimate) and to communities of diverse be-
higher stages. A fair assessment of stage-structural the- lievers. Furthermore, and as Rest (1983) and Oser
ories must address these concerns—which we do in the (1988) have argued, it is impossible to disentangle emo-
next section on alternative approaches to religious and tion from cognition because there is no such thing as
spiritual development. reasoning without feeling. Finally, and following Pi-
Those who object to the linear and normative nature of aget’s lead, stage theorists such as Oser and Fowler as-
stage-structural theories generally equate development sume that emotion provides a functional “motor ” for the
with change over time. They may also place greater em- transition from one stage to another. Indeed, this seems
phasis than do stage-structural theorists on the short-term to be particularly the case for transitions between the
functions that faith plays in individuals’ lives. Together, upper stages (Rollett & Kager, 1999).
these two ways of thinking about religiousness and spiri- The third concern and criticism has to do with stage-
tuality lead us to note that stage-structural theories fail to structural theories’ emphasizing universals to the exclu-
capture the changes and consistencies in individuals’ ways sion of cultural differences. As we see in the next
of being religious and/or spiritual (Wulff, 1993). section, the focus on nonuniversals and on cultural
This criticism that stage-structural theories are too (communication) differences has become a main focus
linear and normative may even be found as an implied in recent times, often to the point of excluding discus-
criticism by the authors of stage-structural models. For sion of universals.
example, Oser and Gmünder (1991), as well as Fowler However, the choice to focus on universals is a legiti-
(1981), report that using their stages to describe age mate research focus. We need to focus on universals to
changes reveals patterns that are decidedly nonlinear— adequately define human development. We also need to
especially among adults. focus on those norms that need not be embodied but that
However, if the concept of development is distin- form indispensable means for defining what develop-
guished from the concept of change over time, as we ment means, or should mean. Stage-structural theories
think it should be, then the linear nature of stage- do just that: By focusing on structural development
structural models can be seen for its essential worth: rather than on content, we gain understanding of what
for contributing to the definition of religious and spiri- we should mean by “developing” and whether any norms
tual development and for contributing to our ability to for defining development can be said to be universal.
evaluate any given pattern of faith. A linear model is Furthermore, and as cross-cultural studies have shown,
needed for the purposes of defining and evaluating, there is at least enough empirical research to suggest
and age changes need not be linear for stage-structural that, when it comes to religious and spiritual develop-
models to be validated or useful (Kaplan, 1983b). ment, there are universals.
As for the criticism that stage-structural theories But does content not matter? This raises the last con-
overemphasize the role of cognition, judgment, and cern that stage-structural models are overly positive/
reasoning in religious and spiritual development, sev- optimistic. Our own view is that this concern is justi-
eral points apply. First, as we have seen, Goldman’s fied—despite the fact that there are discussions noting
(1964) theory and all other stage-structural theories the potential for problems even at the higher stages
that treat religious and spiritual development as being (Kohlberg, 1981). However, the main point here is about
merely an application of universal cognitive structures content, not structure. Content matters, particularly
Alternatives to Stage-Structural Theories 967

with respect to whether an individual’s or group’s im- development, explanation tends to be closer to describing
ages and beliefs support or undermine an ethical life. In- change over time, not only or mainly in the individual but
dividuals functioning at lower stages in stage-structural also in the individual’s transactions and participation in
models may show more compassion and be more sensi- contexts and cultures to function and adapt.
tive to issues of justice than many functioning at higher We look at four quite different approaches that share
stages as defined by structural criteria only. Put another a common understanding that religious and spiritual
way, there are spiritual problems at all stages—as we development need not and should not be thought of in
discuss later in the final section dealing with problems terms of stages and universal endpoints. Each criti-
of a religious and/or spiritual nature, such as the prob- cizes normative, stage-structural models for failing to
lem of religiously inspired terrorism. There is a need for explain the nonlinear changes that often occur in how
research in this area, to document and explain the nature religiousness and spirituality get expressed from birth
of problems at different stages of religious and spiritual to death. Each also criticizes stage-structural models
development. for not capturing the diverse ways that individuals ex-
Having provided a stage-structural framework for press themselves religiously and spiritually, for being
thinking about religious and spiritual development, we overly optimistic about the fruits of structural develop-
can move on to evaluating current empirical approaches ment, and for putting western, liberal values above all
in the light of this framework. Because this framework others. David Wulff (1993) sums up these criticisms
has to do with norms for defining the maturing of faith this way:
and of persons, the discussion of current approaches is
in terms of their not being about faith or persons, or The positing of religious development, especially in the
about development defined by norms. form of progressive and irreversible stages, requires the
assumption of religion-specific dispositions or structures
as well as of particular end-states representing the fullest
ALTERNATIVES TO STAGE- realization of the inborn potential. It is difficult to say,
STRUCTURAL TH EORIES however, of what these rarely observed end-states consist.
Furthermore, the construction of these states requires the
imposition of certain philosophical and theological views,
Is religious and spiritual development to be conceived
thus undermining any claims for universality. (p. 182)
of as a march toward a single, albeit complex and uni-
versal endpoint, thus following norms that push, pull, or
We review the contributions of these current ap-
define where individuals are to head if they are to de-
proaches to religious and spiritual development and
velop? Or is religious and spiritual development to be
their specific, as well as general, criticisms of norma-
conceived of as water flooding a hillside, as a branching
tive, stage-structural theories, even if they are in part
bush, or as some other metaphor that characterizes de-
speculative. We begin with the contributions and criti-
velopment in terms of multiple pathways leading to
cisms of Susan Kwilecki’s substantive-functional ap-
multiple endpoints?
proach (Kwilecki, 1999), which follows in the tradition
Development conceived of as an upward climb to-
of religious studies.
ward perfection is defined by approximations to perfec-
tion. Development conceived of as multidirectional
pathways leading to multiple endpoints is defined by
Susan Kwilecki’s
what happens to individuals as they react to unexpected
Substantive-Functional Approach
moments and as they participate in particular contexts
and cultures. Kwilecki’s work is with adults, not children, because,
These two conceptions of development lead to quite in her view, “childhood is not the period of consum-
different goals for the social scientist bent on explaining mate religious expression.” (1999, p. 264). She is,
religious and spiritual development. Using a normative therefore, critical of current research on religious and
conception of development, explanation tends to be about spiritual development, which she says, “meticulously
the structural development that underlies or defines explains the differences in . . . the religious conceptu-
“lower ” and “ higher,” “immature” and “mature,” and alization of a 5- and a 9-year-old, but does not address
about universals. Using a nonnormative conception of the spectacular variety of adult religious perspectives”
968 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

(p. 264). Because of this variety, Kwilecki defines the inherent ambiguity in religious imagining that is
religious and spiritual development by quantitative often missing in normative, stage-structural approaches.
measures. She writes, Kwilecki’s (1999) approach corrects deficiencies
found in normative, stage-structural approaches. But is
Growth or development in personal religion occurs her way of defining development entirely quantitative
. . . when ideas and experiences of the supernatural be- and without norms? In her writing, we can occasionally
come increasingly salient and functional to the individual. detect a normative and qualitative definition of develop-
. . . My criterion of religious development—the scope,
ment, one that seems sensible if not inevitable. Through-
depth, and pervasiveness of supernaturalism in life—is
out her writing, Kwilecki goes back and forth between
essentially quantitative. (pp. 32–33)
describing the ordinary individuals that are the focus of
her case studies and extraordinary religious and spiri-
For Kwilecki, what grows or develops has to do with
tual exemplars. In her admiration of ordinary examples
imagination more than with perception and reason:
of faith but especially in her admiration of exemplars,
Becoming religious . . . means realizing, and increasingly one can detect an implicit developmental approach de-
acting upon the realization, that ultimately our fate lies fined by qualitative differences. Whether writing about
with forces that transcend our generally most effective Mother Teresa, Black Elk, or the Zen nun Satori Myods,
tools of adaptation—the senses and reason. Attempting to Kwilecki admires the faithful’s ability to pursue noble
monitor critical but elusive powers, the religious are daily purposes, especially in the face of adversity—and al-
thrown upon the imagination, a faculty difficult to disci- ways she is taken with the faithful’s ability to imagine
pline and trust. Ongoing negotiations with unseen beings realities beyond appearances, their ability to remain op-
strain even the hominid capacity for symbolization. Not timistic, and their ability to use imagination to adapt.
everyone becomes religious to the same degree. (p. 31)
Her person-centered approach can, therefore, lay the
foundation for a normative developmental approach that
As lives lived imaginatively, the religious, for Kwilecki, is sensitive to culture, circumstances, and individual
are not to be evaluated using the cognitive-developmental personalities.
schemes derived from Piaget and Kohlberg. Kwilecki’s (1999) work focuses on adults because
With respect to defining religious and spiritual devel- she believes that it is in the adult years that faith takes
opment, Kwilecki’s (1999) work reminds us that the on its varied and sometimes magnificent forms. Her ap-
focus needs to be on whole persons functioning in com- proach is nonnormative in the way she defines religious
plex and ever-changing circumstances. To capture reli- and spiritual development in quantitative terms, as faith
gious and spiritual development, we cannot rely solely gaining strength. Her approach gives us development
on general description systems such as those found in without explicit norms, even if empirically not yet fully
stage-structural theories. We need to know the details of validated. The next example is of a current approach fo-
individuals’ lives, the challenges they face as well as cusing on children because it claims that stage-struc-
their inner thoughts, fantasies, and feelings. tural models have overlooked children’s rich and varied
Kwilecki’s (1999) work also shows us the value of spiritual experience. The focus here is on assessing chil-
certain forms of thinking and behaving that often are dren’s spirituality but without development.
explicitly or implicitly denigrated by stage-structural
theories. For example, in her case study of “Jack Mc-
The Spiritual Child Movement
Cullers,” a mechanic who reported he once received a
divine command to purchase a head gasket for a Toyota, In recent years, a number of psychologists and educators
when he did not own a Toyota or know anybody who did. have been writing about “ the spiritual child” (Coles,
Kwilecki’s response to the religious imaginings of Mc- 1990; Hart, 2005; Hay & Nye, 1998; Reimer & Furrow,
Cullers was: “What could be sillier than thinking that 2003), so many that we can discern a spiritual child
the ruler of the universe would waste time on interven- movement that is, in part, a reaction to stage-structural
tions and messages such as these? What could be lovelier theories of religious and spiritual development, which
than thinking that the ruler of the universe is so intimate are generally seen as following along the lines set down
and playful?” (personal communication; Kwilecki, Sep- by Goldman. As such, one of the main criticisms put
tember 2003). In short, in Kwilecki’s approach, we see forth by this group is that stage-structural theories are
Alternatives to Stage-Structural Theories 969

too cognitive. For example, David Hay (Hay & Nye, should form through being encouraged and supported)
1998) writes: the foundation of their religious, ethical, and spiritual
development.
the cumulative feeling I am left with after reviewing what
What is the evidence supporting this claim that chil-
we know about childhood spirituality is an uneasiness
dren have the capacity for experiences that are essentially
about the adequacy of developmental theory (meaning
stage-structural theories) to give an account of it. . . . The
spiritual? The evidence there is comes from interview
major problem (with stage theories) is their narrowness, studies of children, from retrospective paper and pencil
coming near to dissolving religion into reason and there- studies of adults, and from collections of anecdotes.
fore childhood spirituality into nothing more than a form Members of this group have been the first to point out
of immaturity or inadequacy. (pp. 50, 51) that their methods and the available evidence do not
meet rigorous scientific standards. Even with regard to
The idea that children are spiritual is not new. the issue of how to define children’s spirituality, they
Throughout history, faith traditions have, at some time admit that it is difficult, at best, and perhaps impossible
or another, found an innate spirituality in children—as to pin down children’s spirituality by means of defini-
demonstrated in the passage from Mark 10:15 (New Re- tion. There is, says this group, an inevitable subjectivity
vised Standard Version), “Whosoever shall not receive to discerning children’s spirituality. For example, Re-
the kingdom of God as a little child, he shall not enter becca Nye (1999) writes that what she observed as mo-
therein”; in the reincarnation beliefs surrounding the ments when the children she interviewed were relating
search for a new Dalai Lama (Thurman, 1991); and in their spiritual experiences were moments not captured
the treatment of babies by the Beng of Ivory Coast (Got- by the words appearing in transcripts. Those moments
tlieb, 2005). At various times, children have been seen were singled out because, during them, children
as having a natural and uncritical faith, a natural wis- “seemed to shift into another gear ” (Nye, 1999, p. 62)
dom and capacity for discerning the way things really when speaking about their experiences.
are, and a spirituality that comes from being old souls. Given the current evidence and methods, it might be
Therefore, what is new in the current spiritual child easy to dismiss this movement on the grounds that it is
movement is not the idea of children being spiritual but not grounded in solid scientific research. However, doing
the arguments attacking stage-structural theories for so ignores the real phenomena being discussed. The main
being too cognitive. question is not whether the phenomena discussed are
The spiritual child movement is a movement based on real but whether they warrant the designation of spiri-
the idea that spirituality is rooted in personal experi- tual. Other terms work equally well, terms used by the
ence, feeling, and biology. Those leading the movement leaders of this movement themselves—wonder, awe, wis-
do acknowledge there is a cognitive element to what they dom, and relational consciousness. Adding the term spir-
refer to as “spiritual experience.” To experience spiritu- itual runs the risk of adding a gratuitous interpretation.
ally requires at least the cognitive ability to step back Furthermore, by calling attention to children’s “spir-
and be aware—of the larger picture, of the mystery of itual experiences,” the group promoting the spiritual-
life, of there being something more than what is given by child movement has self-consciously de-emphasized the
the senses. But this ability is present at very young ages, role of judgment and reasoning and thinking in general,
as young as four by some accounts, but certainly by six. to the point of sometimes adopting an opposite extreme
The capacity for spiritual awareness is present at to that of Goldman. The previous quote by David Hay
young ages because, says this group, humans have might serve as an example.
evolved in ways that provide for this capacity (Hart, Finally, this group’s approach may be questioned for
2005; Hay & Nye, 1998). Spiritual capacity is inherited how development is conceived (or not conceived, as the
capacity, a product of brain development. It is given to case may be). It is one thing to point out moments of
children by biology rather than by parents, teachers, or awe, wonder, and wisdom in the lives of children and an-
culture—though for spirituality to develop, children other to define faith and its development. Of the two en-
need lots of encouragement and support. In sum, the terprises, the second seems more significant. Moments
spiritual child movement is bent on getting across one of awe, wonder, wisdom, and relational consciousness
main idea, namely, that children have the capacity for are not likely to shoulder much of the work of establish-
rich and varied spiritual experiences that form (or ing a foundation for religious and spiritual development.
970 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

At least, research has yet to show they do. And even if it children have a fairly well developed intuitive ontology
is not the moments themselves that are important but (Boyer & Walker, 2000; Harris, 2000).
rather the innate capacities they point to, we are still left This view of young children having a fairly well de-
with the question of how these innate capacities develop veloped intuitive ontology directly contradicts the older
into mature patterns of faith. Granted that what devel- Piagetian view, which characterizes young children’s
ops has to do with wonder, awe, and wisdom, we are left thinking as being prelogical. In this newer view, young
still with the task of figuring out the qualitative differ- children come off as neither rational nor irrational.
ences between the wonder, awe, and wisdom of a child Rather, they come off as adept at handling different sys-
and that of, say, John Muir speaking of mountains as tems of thinking about reality. For our purposes, two
“God’s cathedrals” (Cronon, 1997) and Gandhi demon- systems are central.
strating the wisdom of the Gujarati precept, “Return The first system is for thinking about everyday
good for evil” (Gandhi, 1993). events. This is the default system. It rests on direct ob-
In the following discussion of the next approach, we servation and on an innate push to find patterns and
also find an appreciation for how much children under- causal connections. It is not a system that looks for
stand—as well as an appreciation for the influence of magic and the counterintuitive. On the contrary, it is a
elders and culture. However, unlike the previous two ap- system that is thoroughly empirical. Preschoolers may
proaches, the next approach is grounded in experimental pretend, but in doing so, they call their pretense “make-
research done in post-Piagetian and postrationalist, cog- believe.” And when not pretending, if their causal infer-
nitive-developmental psychology—with its focus on do- ences seem irrational, it is more because they lack
main specific development and cultural diversity. information and experience than because their thought
processes are inherently irrational.
The other system is for thinking about the counterin-
Cognitive-Cultural Theories
tuitive. It comes into play more because of culture and
Stage-structural theories posit norms for defining devel- testimony of trusted caregivers than it does because of
opment, which make them susceptible to being insensi- children’s reflections on reality. Early on, children are
tive to cultural diversity. The wealth of information now exposed to counterintuitive ideas and counterintuitive
obtained from new disciplines such as cognitive anthro- worlds that make up their culture’s religious heritage,
pology suggests that no stage-structural theory can do and, remarkably, they have little difficulty taking on
justice to the rich variety of thinking and acting that is these ideas and worlds and making them their own
found in and among cultures. Rather than adopt the (Harris, 2003).
stairway to maturity model, today’s cognitive develop- They do so for a number of reasons. First, they trust
mental psychologists, like Darwin before them, have their caregivers and mentors—those giving testimony to
adopted the model of an ever-branching bush. the reality of the counterintuitive (Harris, 2000). Second,
Current cognitive-cultural theories (e.g., Johnson & because religious ideas are both counterintuitive and pre-
Boyatzis, 2005) search for competencies specific to sented as true, children find them arresting and memo-
nonuniversal domains rather than search only for com- rable (Atran & Norenzayan, 2005). Third, children are
petencies that cut across domains (e.g., object perma- able to keep a kind of double booking—with intuitive on-
nence). This search has provided a more nuanced view tology employed most of the time, and their newly ac-
of intellectual development. The word domain can have quired counterintuitive ontology employed when the
as its reference something quite broad, such as the do- occasion fits (Harris, 2003). Fourth, even when assimi-
main of physics, or something quite narrow such as the lating counterintuitive religious belief systems, children
domains of baseball and knock-knock jokes (Feldman, borrow from their intuitive ontology—making it possible
1980). It is the broad sense of domain that is used in cur- to draw inferences from the counterintuitive world to the
rent cognitive studies—and three broad domains in par- everyday and vice versa (Harris, 2000).
ticular have been the focus of research on children’s As can be seen in this account, the questions are
cognitive development: the physical, biological, and psy- about the development of distinctions and boundaries
chological (theory of mind) domains. Throughout their between separate domains. They are not questions about
early years, children are seen as developing intuitive stages leading in a particular direction. Furthermore,
knowledge in these three domains so that by age 4, most children older than four are characterized as being much
Alternatives to Stage-Structural Theories 971

more similar to adults in their basic thought patterns is, developmental systems theories are essentially rela-
than Piagetian characterizations lead us to believe. Fi- tional in nature. This means that development is located
nally, culture, not simply organismic development, ex- not in the person but in the ongoing transactions be-
plains age changes in what children believe. tween the person and his or her multilayered contexts.
What does this mean for the study and understand- Of concern here is the goodness of fit between person
ing of religious and spiritual development? For one and environment. Religiousness and spirituality relate to
thing it means rejecting theologically influenced devel- the extent that they provide ways to foster a better fit.
opmental models in favor of descriptions grounded Indeed, one of the main findings coming out of research
in cross-cultural field research. For another thing, it based on developmental systems theories is that reli-
means religious development is not a separate cognitive giousness and spirituality do indeed make it more likely
domain but rather a domain that draws on the cognitive for individuals to thrive because their behavior both
achievements designed originally for mundane tasks. improves and is rewarded by their contexts. In sum,
Finally, it means religious thinking is neither more thriving is the main interest; religious and spiritual de-
primitive nor more mature than other kinds of think- velopment are of interest only as they lead to thriving.
ing. It is simply different. And this relativity extends to Among the many types of studies linking religious
distinctions between religious traditions themselves so and spiritual development to thriving are those investi-
long as individuals in any given tradition develop basic gating faith-based communities and their role in helping
intuitive ontologies (physical, biological, psychologi- youth develop positively (Benson et al., 2003; King &
cal) and so long as the counterintuitive religious agents Boyatzis, 2004; King & Furrow, 2004; Roehlkepartain,
and worlds serve the usual adaptive functions of foster- 1995). A study by Regnerus and Elder serves as the
ing moral (communal) commitment and relieving exis- main example (Regnerus & Elder, 2003).
tential anxiety (Atran & Norenzayan, 2005). This study illustrates the developmental systems ap-
This is a composite view of current thinking about re- proach in several ways, first by its speaking about in-
ligious development from a cognitive-cultural perspec- volvement in a faith based community as a potential
tive. What are we to make of it? What are its strengths “resilient pathway.” The term resilient pathway reveals
and weaknesses? The main strength in this perspective the approach’s main assumption about plasticity as well
is providing a better account of how religious beliefs are as its main interest—what leads to thriving. The results
acquired. The main weakness is the perspective’s con- of this study show that in high risk communities, church
flating belief and faith—so much so that, at times, the attendance functions as a protective mechanism “stimu-
subject investigated seems to be something other than lating resilience in the lives of at-risk youth”—as shown
religious and spiritual development. For example, the by church attendance correlating positively with staying
perspective fails to distinguish differences between “on track ” in school.
children’s questions such as “Why don’t angels fall However, it is in Regnerus’ and Elder’s (2003) ex-
down to earth?” (Harris, 2000), and adults’ questions planation of the results rather than in the results
such as “Why do bad things happen to good people?” themselves that we see the main features of the devel-
(Kushner, 1981). The first question lacks moral signifi- opmental systems approach. Regnerus and Elder ex-
cance. The second question forces thinking about what plain the results as follows:
kind of universe we live in and how we ought to face in-
justice and adversity. While there may be disagreement The ritual action of attending worship services or cere-
about whether both kinds of questions fall in the domain monies, in contrast with theological differences that mark
of religious and spiritual development, it seems clear distinct religious affiliations and beliefs, appears to be a
process that operates independently of particular belief
that the second kind of question has more to do with
systems and organizational affiliations. Church atten-
what has previously been defined as faith.
dance may constitute—even if by accident—a form of so-
cial integration that has the consequence of reinforcing
Developmental Systems Theories values conducive to educational achievement and goal set-
ting . . . (In addition) . . . church attendance and doing
Developmental systems theories shift the focus from in- well in school require commitment, diligence, and routine.
dividuals to transactions between individuals and their The ritual practice of rising and going to church or mass,
various embedded contexts (Lerner et al., 2003). That and so forth—whether compelled by one’s own faith or
972 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

one’s parents’ demands—commits a youth to a practice • With age, children, adolescents, and adults maintain
and routine, a skill that translates into tools needed for conceptions of supernatural agencies that are both
academic success. (p. 646) anthropomorphic and nonanthropomorphic.
In other words, for the study’s at-risk youth, going to • With age, children, adolescents, and adults develop
church regularly meant they were exposed to values and intuitive ontologies having to do with what they can
good routines that could be transferred to school. perceive directly, which coexist with counterintuitive
Obviously, studies such as this study are much ontologies having to do with what they cannot per-
needed, especially for understanding how to support at- ceive directly. The acquisition of the latter constitutes
risk youth. But are they studies of religious and spiritual neither magical-egocentric nor logical-decentered
development, or are they simply studies of positive de- thinking.
velopment defined broadly? We think they are the latter
because they skirt the task of charting and explaining What is the evidence supporting each of these ap-
faith and its development. While these studies often ac- proaches? And in looking at the evidence, can we find a
knowledge that faith is a unique and important variable, way to reconcile the differences? To answer both ques-
they do not explain how faith develops. And in not ex- tions we look next at studies carried out from each of
plaining how faith develops, they leave out the inner these approaches—beginning first with studies carried
workings that define and explain the religious and spiri- out from a stage-structural approach. For the most part,
tual development of persons. we compare what the two sets of studies have to say
In subsequent sections we return to discussing devel- about development with respect to concepts of (a) super-
opmental systems theories and their contribution to natural agencies, ( b) religious institutions and prayer,
understanding the positive correlates of religious and and (c) death/ life after death.
spiritual development. However, in the next section, we However, in discussing studies of religious concepts
look more closely at the ongoing debate between stage- and their development, we remind readers that concepts
structural and cognitive-cultural theories—as we take can never be sorted neatly into religious and nonreli-
up the task of explaining concept formation in religious gious. Certain concepts may not appear to be associated
and spiritual development. with religious or spiritual development. Nevertheless,
by taking into account the context in which these con-
cepts are experienced, they become religious or spiri-
CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT tual concepts. For example, at the surface level, the
IN RELIGIOUS AND concepts of “ work ” and “ family” may not appear to
SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT have religious or spiritual meaning. However, by placing
these concepts in specific contexts their meaning be-
Nowhere do we see the contrast between stage-structural comes modified such that they take on a religious or
and cognitive-cultural approaches more clearly than spiritual meaning—as when Shakers speak of “conse-
when characterizing conceptual development in religious crated labor ” and Mormons speak of “ the eternal fam-
and spiritual development. Stage-structural approaches ily.” Meaning is tied not only to development but also to
provide the following composite picture: culture and faith tradition.

• With age, children go from having mostly anthropo-


morphic and concrete conceptions of supernatural
Studies Carried Out from a Stage-
agencies to having mostly symbolic and abstract
Structural Approach
conceptions.
• With age, children go from reliance on magical and Studies of religious conceptual development carried out
egocentric-imaginative reflecting, which includes from a stage-structural approach have largely followed
thinking about supernatural agencies and prayer, to in the rationalist tradition exemplified in the work of Pi-
more rational and decentered thinking. aget. In this tradition, children’s concepts reflect quali-
tatively different and less rational (when compared to
In contrast, cognitive-cultural approaches provide the adults’ concepts) ways of thinking. This can be seen
following composite picture: when examining a variety of religious concepts, the
Conceptual Development in Religious and Spiritual Development 973

most notable being those having to do with supernatural Do children’s drawings reflect their real conceptions
agencies, religious institutions, prayer, and death. of God? The answer is not clear. Children may repro-
duce in their drawing schemes what can be canonized,
Supernatural Agencies schemes that may persist even though their inner con-
By far the most studied religious concept has been the ceptions have changed (Freeman, 1980).
concept God. Children’s conceptions of God have been However, anthropomorphizing in childhood cannot
assessed in a variety of ways, most frequently by asking be attributed solely to the drawing method because,
children to draw what they picture in their minds when with respect to conceptions of God, other research
they hear the word God . Having children draw pictures methods have revealed a similar developmental trend.
of God, write letters to God, and other relatively un- For example, in interview studies, a number of investi-
structured and projective methods have shown them- gators have found that anthropomorphous depictions
selves useful for bringing out children’s conceptions of of the Divine dominate, more so than later on (Barnes,
God more readily than closed methods (Klepsch & 1892; Vogel, 1936). Similarly, Thun (1959), who
Logie, 1982, p. 36; Tamminnen, 1991, p. 160). recorded the classroom conversations of German ele-
These unstructured and projective methods are not mentary schoolchildren during religious instruction,
without their problems. In particular, they encourage an- and Deconchy (1967), who classified French chil-
thropomorphizing and, when used with children only, dren’s images of God by using a word association tech-
they can create biased comparisons based on children nique, found that children up to 11-years-old
and adults being assessed differently (J. Barrett, 2001). conceived of God anthropomorphously, while adoles-
Methodological issues notwithstanding, numerous cents conceived of God in ways that were both abstract
studies (Bucher, 1994; Hanisch, 1996; Heller, 1986; and vague. Hyde (1965), too, investigated adolescents’
Nye & Carlson, 1984; Pitts, 1977; Pnevmatikos, 2002) images of God with methods other than by using draw-
have confirmed the trend described by Harms (1944). ings and found a steady receding of anthropomor-
On the basis of more than 4800 drawings and conversa- phisms, delayed in the case of children who regularly
tions, Harms found that children, aged 3 to 6, produced went to church.
predominantly fairy-tale figures of God, while older In summary, numerous studies following in the stage-
children portrayed God in an anthropomorphous structural tradition have found that with age, children’s
manner. Only adolescents provided abstract-symbolic conceptions of God become less anthropomorphic and
conceptions. less concrete and more symbolic and abstract. Similar
Since Harm’s research, studies have repeatedly results have been found in studies of children’s concep-
demonstrated that young children typically see God as tions of religious institutions and prayer.
a friendly, smiling old man, often with a beard, and
often residing in heaven, with heaven depicted as a
place somewhere above the earth (Bucher, 1994; Gold- Conceptions of Religious Institutions,
man, 1964; Heller, 1986; Tamminnen, 1991, p. 195). A Prayer, and Death
number of surveys have shown that, with age, fewer Religiousness and spirituality are not continually being
and fewer children give anthropomorphous depictions invented anew. Rather, they are handed down by tradi-
of God, and more and more give symbols or metaphors tion in faith communities. Therefore, ideas possessed by
to represent God—such as God being represented as children of religious institutions and practices are of
light, nature, or love (Bucher, 1994; Goldman, 1964; particular interest not only to researchers but also to
Hanisch, 1996). Strict, or demonic images are rare teachers of religion. The central studies remain those
(Frielingsdorf, 1992). carried out by Elkind and his coworkers (1961, 1962,
According to this set of studies, children’s concep- 1963), in which Jewish, Catholic, and Protestant chil-
tions of God move from the anthropomorphic and con- dren were asked how they perceived their denomination
crete to the symbolic and abstract, albeit at different and religious community. The results supported the fol-
speeds in different religions. For example, Pitts (1977) lowing description of age changes:
found that Mormon children held on to anthropomor-
phous conceptions longer than average, while Jewish • Undifferentiated conceptions, according to which even
children did the opposite. a domestic cat could be regarded as Catholic (to age 7)
974 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

• Concretely differentiated conceptions (to age 9) in In a study of prayer and its development in adoles-
which religious affiliation was defined in terms of spe- cence, Scarlett and Perriello (1991) had subjects pro-
cific, concrete acts such as going to church or synagogue vide prayers for hypothetical situations calling for
• Abstractly differentiated conceptions (from age 10) prayer (e.g., praying for a dying friend). Their results
that made reference to beliefs and ideology showed that with age, adolescents prayed less for God to
intervene directly to make dramatic changes, such as
As for prayer, given the diversity and universality of curing a sick friend, and more for God to provide sup-
prayer, it is not surprising that the psychology of religion port and guidance. Furthermore, with age, adolescents
has, from its beginnings, investigated its development shifted from “ talking at ” God to “ talking with” God
(Brown, 1994; Hyde, 1990). Early investigations derive such that they engaged more in sharing feelings, ques-
from Pratt (1910), who characterized the development tions, and doubts and not just requests.
of prayer as leading from primitive and spontaneous Together, these studies of age changes in how chil-
supplication to more ritualized forms. dren and adolescents pray and think about prayer are
Goldman (1964) distinguished three stages in the de- reminiscent of William James’ (1902) words about the
velopment of the prayer concept. At the first stage, nor- development of prayer:
mally ending at age 9, children conceive of prayers like the belief is, not that particular events are tempered more
magic. Furthermore, younger children believe that God towardly to us by a superintending providence, as a reward
literally hears prayers and is compelled to accede to re- for our reliance, but that by cultivating the continuous
quests because the praying person has prayed properly sense of our connections with the power that made things
and is a good person. Later on, children believe that the as they are, we are tempered more towardly for their re-
effect of prayer is dependent on its content; if prayers ception. The outward face of nature need not alter, but the
contain only material and selfish wishes, they will not expressions of meaning in it alter. (p. 474)
likely be granted. In adolescence, development contin-
ues when prayer is seen as an expression of faith and as Death, or rather the possibility of life after death, is
a form of psychic-spiritual self-help. perceived by many as a religious phenomenon, which
Rosenberg (1989) investigated age changes in several means that children’s and young people’s conceptions
content areas of prayer: How (for instance, the physical po- of death should be taken into account (see reviews by
sition of the praying person), What (the content of prayer), Faulkner, 1993; Ramachers, 1994; Wittkowski, 1990).
Whom (the addressee of prayer or conception of God), and Early on, Anthony (1940) questioned 128 children
the reflective-subjective aspect of praying. Children and about their definitions of death and found that, with
adolescents were shown pictures of people praying and age, children only gradually come to realize that death
asked to associate freely to what they saw. The results refers to the cessation of life functions and is biologi-
showed that, with age, the “how” became less significant. cally necessary. Numerous studies since then have
For younger children, externalities such as covering the demonstrated that children between ages 3 and 5 do not
head during prayer were important, and supplications as understand death to be universal (mostly old people die,
well as anthropomorphous conceptions of God dominated. but not one’s parents), nor do they consider death to be
For adolescents, prayer required a more psychological con- irreversible. By age 7, children normally develop at least
ception, such that they saw prayer’s value as having to do a nascent understanding of the irreversibility and uni-
with its effect on the praying person him- or herself. versality of death so that most children, by age 9, pos-
Other studies have confirmed this trend of prayer de- sess a developed conception of death (Wittkowski,
veloping from being a magical speech act to a spiritual 1990, p. 58).
dialogue. For example, Long, Elkind, and Spilka (1967) Relatively little has been written about how the con-
distinguished three stages of prayer. Children younger cept of life after death changes over time (Tamminnen,
than nine regarded praying as essentially a way of asking 1991, p. 262). Barth (1911), in a survey of German
for things. Beginning around age 9, the relevance of ask- children, showed that children conceived of heaven as a
ing for things diminished, and prayer became an inner specific locality somewhere above the earth. Bur-
dialogue with God (Godin, 1968a; Thouless & Brown, gardsmeier (1951) found, in a survey of German chil-
1964). Brown (1967) also found that belief in the magi- dren of compulsory school age, that children conceived
cal-causative effect of prayer decreased with age. of heaven as a fantastic, beautiful, and sacred place and
Conceptual Development in Religious and Spiritual Development 975

then as air, firmament, and the residence of God, and, ing the box for the first time will think there is only ce-
finally, as a symbol. Blum (cited in Ratcliff, 1985) real in it, not rocks. Therefore, even young children have
found that children’s conceptions of life after death be- a theory of mind.
came vaguer with increasing age, and that age ac- However, when asked whether God will make the
counted for more of the variance than did religious same mistake, children, by age 5, say, “No.” Even young
background. These results were replicated by Tammin- children understand that God is no ordinary agent—that
nen (1991, p. 260–278), who found that young school- God has supernatural, counterintuitive powers. In short,
children depicted eternal life and heaven with above with respect to the concept of God, children are not as
average graphic detail (p. 278). Older children had dif- anthropomorphic as previously pictured. Rather, chil-
ficulties locating heaven in the cosmos, or else pro- dren make a clear distinction between persons and God.
vided theologically inspired ideas (e.g., “Heaven is The same holds true for children raised in cultures and
within us.”). faith traditions with supernatural, religious agents other
Once again, studies carried out following a stage- than God—such as Krishna and ancestors.
structural approach accentuate differences between The second question, about differences between
children’s, adolescents’, and adults’ religious concepts children’s and adults’ conceptions of God, has led to
as well as accentuate the usefulness of using the an- studies that show adults, as well as children, conceive
thropomorphic to nonanthropomorphic and concrete- of God anthropomorphously. J. Barrett and Keil’s
literal to abstract-symbolic criteria for describing and (1996) study is the one most cited. Barrett and Keil
evaluating religious and spiritual development. The found that their adult subjects, when asked directly
same is not the case with studies carried out following a about God’s nature, gave theologically correct an-
cognitive-cultural approach—as the subsequent dis- swers, answers that avoided defining God anthropo-
cussion explains. morphously. However, when asked to recount stories
involving God, the same adults added to or distorted
Studies Carried Out from a Cognitive- the stories by anthropomorphizing God—such as
Cultural Approach speaking of God as “not noticing” or as acting sequen-
tially. They spoke of God as acting in very human
The previous section provided an overview of the ways. Barrett and Keil concluded that adults, like chil-
cognitive-cultural viewpoint or approach. Here, we dren, develop and maintain conceptions of God that
look more closely at the evidence and arguments sup- combine their counterintuitive understanding of God as
porting this viewpoint and its characterization of a supernatural agent with their intuitive understanding
conceptual development in religious and spiritual de- of how normal agents act. This same combining of the
velopment. We begin with evidence and arguments re- intuitive and counterintuitive applies to other religious
lated to concepts of supernatural agencies. supernatural agencies as well (spirits and ancestors).
Two questions have framed studies carried out from With respect to prayer, Woolley’s research provides
the cognitive-cultural approach. The first is, “Are chil- a similar finding to that of Barrett and Keil (1996).
dren mostly anthropomorphic in their thinking about Woolley (2000) asked children, ages 3 to 8, to teach a
supernatural agencies, such as God—as stage-structural puppet to pray. She also interviewed the children about
theories lead us to believe?” The second is, “Are chil- their understanding of prayer, its effects, and why
dren’s concepts of supernatural agencies essentially prayers sometimes do not get answered. Her results
different from adults’ concepts of supernatural agen- share certain findings with studies carried out from a
cies—as stage-structural theories lead us to believe?” stage-structural approach. However, there were impor-
The first question has been addressed by asking chil- tant differences stemming from her comparing chil-
dren what they know about ordinary agents such as hu- dren’s understanding of prayer and their intuitive
mans. The starting point for discussion is Wimmer and knowledge of causation.
Perner’s (1983) study showing that children as young as 5 First, Woolley (2000) showed that children as young
understand that others’ thinking may differ from their as age 5 develop a mentalistic conception of prayer—
own depending on what information others have available much earlier than that reported by Long et al., 1967. Sec-
to them. For example, a 5-year-old discovering rocks in a ond, she showed that, by age 5, children begin to give up
cereal box will correctly predict that an adult encounter- belief in the causal powers of wishing, even as they begin
976 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

to believe in the causal powers of prayer. Therefore, chil- opposite. In contrast to counterintuitive scientific con-
dren’s belief in the efficacy of prayer cannot be attrib- cepts, counterintuitive religious concepts operate in par-
uted to their holding magical views of the causal powers allel to intuitive, nonreligious beliefs—at least for most
of thinking in general and wishing in particular. Their people, most of the time.
believing in prayer’s efficacy is, rather, a result of their
being taught or socialized. Woolley’s participants came
A Synthesis
from religious families. Furthermore, their belief in the
efficacy of prayers but not in the efficacy of wishes Where do these contrasting pictures leave us with re-
seemed to be tied to their understanding that prayer, un- spect to our present understanding and future research?
like wishing, involves an intermediary, God, who is a su- One possibility is that they force us to reject one in favor
pernatural agent. of the other. However, this possibility seems a poor
With respect to children’s conceptions of death and choice. On the one hand, the cumulative results of nu-
life after death, the same cognitive-cultural framework merous studies suggests that there are indeed important
applies. In the course of direct experience with death, differences in the way children as compared to adoles-
children construct their own, intuitive ontology about cents and adults give meaning to religious concepts.
death such that they understand death’s irreversibility and These differences are not simply the result of faulty re-
inevitability at much younger ages than stage-structural search methods and unevenness with respect to social-
theories lead us to believe (Slaughter, Jaakola, & Carey, ization. On the other hand, the results of careful and
1999). However, in the course of experiencing death, chil- thoughtful studies comparing intuitive and counterintu-
dren also acquire counterintuitive beliefs about death and itive ontologies demonstrate important similarities in
life after death—beliefs transmitted by culture and faith the way children, adolescents, and adults provide mean-
tradition through the testimonies of trusted others (Har- ing to religious concepts.
ris, 2000). We suggest that these two approaches are not so op-
Together, these cognitive-cultural accounts of chil- posite as they first appear to be, and that a synthesis is
dren’s religious concepts leave us with a quite different possible. Focusing on differences does not exclude fo-
picture of children’s capacities and of culture’s role in cusing on similarities. Furthermore, these two foci com-
religious and spiritual development. Whereas some re- plement rather than contradict one another. The strength
searchers see children as having limited capacity and of the cognitive-cultural approach lies in its providing
culture being responsible for helping children overcome better explanations for how religious beliefs are ac-
their natural propensity for magical thinking, others quired. The strength of the more normative, stage-struc-
conceive them as having considerable capacity and cul- tural approach lies in its providing possibilities for
ture being responsible for teaching or communicating explaining how religious beliefs become existentially
beliefs about what is counterintuitive. Therefore, at the relevant or, as Johnson and Boyatzis (2005) said, how
center of the newer picture is the distinction between in- religious concepts help with the task of connecting the
tuitive and counterintuitive ontology. self with a more valued reality. Put another way, the
However, because there is much that is counterintu- stage-structural approach may be more useful with re-
itive that is not learned in childhood, for example, the spect to explaining how beliefs become integrated with
Darwinian concept of evolution, why are religious coun- feelings and actions to form a pattern of faith that can
terintuitive concepts learned much earlier? Harris lead individuals to function at higher religious or spiri-
(2000) provides a compelling answer. Counterintuitive tual levels. After all, religious and spiritual exemplars
religious concepts, such as the concepts of gods, spirits, are exemplary largely because of their positive and pow-
and ancestors as supernatural agencies, do not require erful faith and not because of their concepts and beliefs.
children to give up their intuitive, commonsense con-
cepts of ordinary agents. In contrast, scientific con-
cepts, such as the Darwinian concept of evolution, do RELIGIOUS AND SPIRITUAL
indeed require children to give up their intuitive, com- DEVELOPMENT IN CONTEXT
monsense concepts. One cannot, for example, hold both
the intuitive, creationist view that species are immutable Religious and spiritual development always takes place
and the counterintuitive, Darwinian view that states the in multiple and layered contexts. Furthermore, each
Religious and Spiritual Development in Context 977

context has in itself parts operating together to form 1997; Hood et al., 1996). For example, Ozorak (1989)
systems—with the various systems themselves operat- found parents’ influence on their children’s religious de-
ing together to form larger systems. This observation velopment to be much greater than that of peers, as well
about contexts makes any attempt at causal explanation as more lasting than parents’ influence on their chil-
complex indeed—for clearly, in any final analysis, cause dren’s political development and ways of structuring
is to be found in the combination of a seemingly infinite leisure time. And, as indicated in Table 17.5, Cavalli-
number of causes (Lerner, 2002). Sforza (1982) found correlations that are consistent with
In the following discussion, when we speak of a par- the findings and interpretations of these other studies.
ticular cause, influence, outcome, or dependent variable, Similar results have come from self-report studies of
we do so with a developmental systems paradigm still in young adults. For example, the young adult interviewees in
mind. We use these concepts for what they are—still B. Hunsberger & Brown’s (1984) study reported that the
useful concepts for exploring how religiosity and spiri- strongest influence on their religious and spiritual devel-
tuality develop, even if they are not useful for fitting re- opment was their mother, followed by the church and their
ligiousness and spirituality into some procrustean bed. father, with friends and the media playing only a modest
They are useful, too, given the fact that our methodolo- role. B. Hunsberger (1995) also found that older subjects
gies lag behind progress in theorizing. considered their religious development to have been most
Now, we look at three main contexts for thinking influenced by their mothers, followed by their fathers and
about what influences or supports religious and spiritual the church, and then by the media, friends, and school.
development. These three are the family, the religious In a number of studies, mothers are credited with
school, and the congregation. We focus on these three having the primary influence on children’s religious and
because of their logical connection to religious and spir- spiritual development (Boyatzis et al., 2005). Several
itual development but also because they have been con- reasons seem to apply. First, mothers maintain a higher
texts that have been studied empirically. profile in matters of religious practices such as church
attendance and family prayer (Beit-Hallahmi & Argyle,
1997). Also, mothers are more likely than fathers to
The Family Context
converse with their children about religion. For example,
As several have noted, the family is the most powerful Strommen and Hardell (2000), in a study of mainline
influence on children’s religious and spiritual develop- protestant youth, found that mothers had conversations
ment (Boyatzis, Dollahite, & Marks, 2005). Its influ- with their children about faith and life issues nearly
ence is twofold. First, as an institution of socialization, two-and-a-half times more frequently than did fathers.
the family directly impacts the developing child. This Finally, mothers are most often the ones who introduce
occurs when practices, beliefs, traditions, and values are children to religious practices, for example, by leading
transmitted from parent to child. Second, children are them in family prayer and taking them to religious ser-
indirectly influenced in their religious and spiritual de- vices (Hood et al., 1996).
velopment by the attachments they form in their family. One of the most interesting findings has to do with
The Divine can function as an object of attachment, and the extent to which parents influence their children’s re-
the attachment a child forms with the Divine can occur ligious practices as compared to their religious beliefs.
either as a way to continue having a secure attachment or There are stronger correlations between the religious
as a means of compensating for an insecure attachment. practices of parents and those of their children than
In sum, parents and families influence the religious and there are between the religious beliefs of parents and
spiritual development of their children in direct and tar- those of their children. Gibson (Gibson, 1990) found
geted ways, especially through religious teachings and that parents’ church attendance correlated with that of
practices, but also in indirect and untargeted ways, espe-
cially through the way they foster attachments. TABLE 17.5 Parental and Peer Inf luence Compared

Parent-Child Friend-Child
Socialization and Practice
Religious behavior .57 .20
Empirical research has repeatedly demonstrated that Political behavior .32 .16
with regard to religious development, mothers and fa- Entertainment .16 .10
thers have the most influence (Beit-Hallahmi & Argyle, Sports played .13 .16
978 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

their young adult offspring more powerfully (r = .60) their parents (e.g., “When I had ideas of my own in mat-
than with their attitudes toward Christian beliefs (r = ters of religion, my mother/father took me seriously.”)
.50). Similarly, Ozorak (1989) found that family cohe- were significantly higher on the stage scale of religious
sion influenced children’s religious practices but not judgment (Oser & Gmünder, 1991) than were young
their beliefs. Beliefs, it seems, become increasingly indi- people with little or no religious encouragement.
vidual as children grow older. Furthermore, a more However, influence goes both ways, and children’s
powerfully individualized faith is accompanied by developmental level is a determining factor in their atti-
higher intellectual aptitude. tudes toward religion and in the religious climate of the
In the family, the available research suggests that re- family as a whole. For example, in a random sample of
ligious development is furthered by a number of factors, Swiss parents and children, Klaghofer and Oser (1987)
including a climate of support characterized by encour- found that children evaluated as being at stage 2 on the
agement to have independent views, encouragement to Oser and Gmünder (1991) scale had a more positive fam-
engage in discussion, and closeness between parents and ily climate than did children evaluated as being at stage
children. For example, closeness between parents and 3. This finding was expected, since persons at stage 3 at-
their children predicts higher positive correlations be- tribute a lower status to religiousness than do persons at
tween the religiousness of parents and the religiousness stages 4 and 2.
of their children. This still is the case when children be- Similarly, recent research has emphasized the posi-
come adolescents (Erickson, 1992). tive role played by conversational dialogues between
In two parent families, support also depends on parents and children on children’s religious and spiri-
whether parents share the same religious convictions tual development. In a study of diary entries recording
with each other. When parents share the same religious conversations between parents and children, Boyatzis
convictions, they influence the religiousness of their and Janicki (2003) found that children play an active
children more powerfully than when they hold opposing role in initiating, terminating, and driving religious con-
convictions (Hoge & Petrillo, 1982). Parents holding op- versations. Furthermore, they found that parents tended
posing convictions are associated with their offspring’s to ask questions that were open-ended, and the ques-
religious apostasy (Caplovitz & Sherrow, 1977). tions themselves were devoid of suggestions revealing
The phenomenon of giving up one’s religious origins parents’ personal views. In another study, of young Jew-
(apostasy) has been the subject of a number of studies ish adults, Herzbrun (1993) found that the frequency of
(Caplovitz & Sherrow, 1977; Hood et al., 1996). religious discussion in the family had an enduring influ-
Caplovitz and Sherrow (1977) identified familial strain ence on young people, especially on girls.
and dissociation from parents as the main psychological In sum, the collective picture painted of religious
antecedents of apostasy. L. Hunsberger (1980), too, socialization in the family supports a constructivist
found that the antecedents of apostasy included poor re- perspective and reveals a reciprocity process such
lations with parents. In addition, B. Hunsberger and that parents and children are inf luenced by what the
Brown (1984) found that apostates reported consider- other has to say (Boyatzis, Dollahite, & Marks, 2005).
ably less emphasis on religion in their childhood home Through conversations with parents, children are af-
as compared to matched controls who remained faithful forded the opportunity to puzzle over religious and spir-
to their original religion. Thus, apostasy is not so much itual matters and to construct their own personal views.
the result of an excess of religious teaching as it is a re- Further, the transmission of religious views from parent
sult of too little of it. In sum, apostasy can result from to child happens not as a result of didactic teaching so
not enough religious encouragement—which follows much as it happens as a result of parent and child co-
from Niggli’s (1988) study that demonstrated that reli- constructing their spiritual identities. Therefore, chil-
gious encouragement has indeed a significant effect on dren’s participation in their own religious development
religious development. supports rather than undermines continuity between
Niggli also developed scales to measure style of reli- generations.
gious instruction and their effects on religious develop- There is sufficient evidence to show the deep extent
ment: in particular, the styles of religious compulsion to which the family influences religious development,
and religious encouragement. In Niggli’s study, young particularly by the type of religiousness or religious in-
people who reported greater religious encouragement by struction practiced in the family. Most people remain in
Religious and Spiritual Development in Context 979

the denomination in which they are born and in which and child affect the relationship between person and
they grow up. Even in cases when young people turn to God in later life. As noted by Granqvist (1998) and
radical forms of religion, there are usually continuities Kirkpatrick (1995), God functions as an attachment fig-
with their parents’ religion. An example is the Jesus- ure because many believers imagine God as a safe haven
people movement, which made such a stir in the 1970s. and secure base who maintains proximity and is strong
Most of the followers came from fundamentalist Chris- in times of distress—all requirements for being an at-
tian families. Although these young people were re- tachment figure.
belling against their parents, they had internalized their Most of the attachment research on religious and
parents’ religious attitudes and were living them out, al- spiritual development has made use of the Ainsworth
beit in a radicalized form (Richardson, Stewart, & Sim- (e.g., Ainsworth, 1978) typology—to test for two possi-
monds, 1979). bilities. The first is that persons who, early on, devel-
Religious development is also influenced by parent- oped a secure attachment pattern with a caregiver tend
ing style. The available research suggests that parental to image a close and always protective God. This possi-
emphasis on obedience encourages a religious attitude bility is commonly referred to as the correspondence
according to which human nature is corrupt, sin is to be thesis. According to the correspondence thesis, a secure
punished, and the Bible must be interpreted literally attachment attained during early childhood prepares an
(Ellison & Sherkat, 1993). At the beginning of the individual to adopt a corresponding secure attachment
1960s, 66% of the children interviewed by Nunn (1964) with God later on.
reported that their parents threatened that God could The second possibility is that persons who, in early
punish them, and although this happened much more childhood, developed an insecure attachment pattern
frequently in socially disadvantaged families, it also (ambivalent or avoidant), tend to image God as a guaran-
took place when parents felt themselves to be powerless tor of protection and security. This possibility is com-
and in need of a coalition with a strong and vengeful monly referred to as the compensation thesis. According
God. Similarly, Potvin and Sloane (1985) found that to the compensation thesis, God becomes a surrogate at-
adolescents whose parents exercised powerful control tachment figure who compensates for the absence of an
and constraint were more likely to perceive God as pun- internalized secure attachment from childhood.
ishing deviant behavior. In a survey of 213 adults, Kirkpatrick and Shaver
(1990) found evidence for the compensation thesis.
Attachment They classified subjects according to the different at-
Images of God become even more relevant in research tachment types, and compared subjects’ images of
about attachment. The influence of parents on the reli- God, also taking into account subjects’ status with re-
gious and spiritual development of their children is not gard to extrinsic and intrinsic religiousness. Subjects
limited to the direct way in which they teach, encourage, assigned to the avoidant attachment type were clearly
and transmit faith traditions. There is a second, more the most religious. They scored highest on the loving
subtle, type in which parents influence their children’s God and intrinsic scales, on attendance at service, and
spiritual development indirectly, through the ways they on reporting a personal relationship with God. This
interact with their children and determine the quality of was especially the case for those reporting only infre-
their attachments. quent religious practice in their parents’ home (Kirk-
Ana-Maria Rizzuto (1979) made an important con- patrick & Shaver, 1990, p. 325). Furthermore, half of
tribution in studying this indirect influence. Through the avoidant group reported having experienced a sud-
the use of her God and family questionnaires, Rizzuto den conversion, whereas in the two other groups this
demonstrated how individuals’ images of God can be happened to only one person in nine. The differences
strongly related to their images of their relations to between the securely attached and the ambivalently at-
their parents. tached were markedly lower.
Since Rizzuto’s work, research on parents’ indirect In a later study, Kirkpatrick (1997) reproduced the
influence has continued outside the framework of psy- results of this first study using longitudinal data. Women
choanalytic theory. Most of this research has been con- were first interviewed to determine their attachment sta-
ducted using the constructs of attachment theory, tus, then, 4 years later, interviewed again about changes
looking at how early attachment patterns between parent in their religiousness. During the second interviews, 40%
980 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

of the women who had previously reported experiencing do not influence religious development and attitudes in
an avoidant attachment relationship with a parent, re- any powerful way (Hood et al., 1996).
ported that, since the first interview, they had found a Francis (1987a) questioned almost 5,000 young per-
“new relationship with God.” The percentage for those sons attending Catholic private schools in England, and
who had reported having a secure attachment relation- found that these schools had very little influence on stu-
ship was significantly lower. dents’ attitudes toward Christian beliefs. A more deci-
Grangvist (1998) also tested the compensation the- sive factor was the influence of the parental home. When
sis. In a study of 203 Swedish students, he collected in- the parental home follows the same religious educational
formation about the attachment behavior of mothers and choices as the denominational schools, the school’s ef-
fathers and about the remembered religiousness of both fect with regard to faith and later church practice is
parents. Combining ambivalent and avoidant groups into greater. Spencer (1971) concluded that in the absence of
one, insecurely attached group, Grangvist found that the reinforcement from the family, there is no reason to ex-
insecurely attached group scored higher on religious- pect that the religious school will modify values and
ness, on relationship with God, and on theism—but value-oriented behavior.
only when parents were reported to be not very reli- With regard to religious development, it seems that
gious. However, among subjects remembering their fa- religious schools possess not so much a compensatory or
thers as being highly religious, the securely attached independent function as a supplementary one, to the ex-
expressed greater religiousness and a closer relationship tent that they have any effect at all. The same conclusion
with God: Under these conditions, the correspondence was reached in studies carried out on the effect of reli-
thesis applied. gious schools in Australia (Hyde, 1990). Anderson (An-
The correspondence thesis is further supported by a dersen, 1988) found that the choice of a Catholic private
comparison of deity figures in cultures possessing clear school depended to a large extent on the religious and
differences in the behavior of parents. For example, philosophical attitudes of parents, especially mothers;
Lambert, Triandis, and Wolf (1959) demonstrated that the schools themselves changed the attitudes and prac-
in child-centered societies, where children are sur- tices of the pupils only marginally.
rounded by loving care, God and deity figures tend to As for explicit religious instruction in public schools,
be benevolent beings, but in cultures with rejecting par- practices differ depending on national policies. In
enting styles, God and other deity figures tend to be France, there is no teaching of religion at all in state
malevolent. schools, but in the United Kingdom, religious education
From these results, it becomes clear that the connec- is taught under the auspices of the government; in Ger-
tions between attachment experiences in early child- many, religious education is taught by and under the aus-
hood and religiousness later on are complicated and not pices of the Church but financed by the government.
straightforward. Religiousness can function not only in With respect to the content of religious education,
a compensatory manner but also as an extension of the opinions are divided about what constitutes religious
young child’s relationships with his or her parents. knowledge and the goals of religious education. The
However, the patterns are not random. They simply de- spectrum of expectations ranges from the imparting of
rive from more than one combination of antecedent general religious knowledge to the considerably more
conditions. ambitious aim of kindling faith and motivating religious
practice and church attendance.
The relatively few studies on the effects of religious
Religious Schools and Religious Education
education are in agreement that its popularity decreases
Most of the research on religious schools has been on markedly as children get older, especially in adoles-
Christian, denominational sponsored schools in the cence. On the basis of questioning more than 800
United States, Europe, and Australia. The main research schoolchildren in England, Francis (1987b), showed
questions have been about religious schools’ influence that religious education is the second most popular sub-
on pupils’ religious beliefs and practices. Differences ject among children in first grade ( junior year 1 in the
between school systems and student populations make United Kingdom)—only sports (“games”) is more pop-
comparisons difficult (e.g., Hyde, 1990). However, the ular, but by the time students are in ninth grade (sec-
composite picture drawn indicates that religious schools ondary year 4 in the United Kingdom), it is the least
Positive Correlates of Religious and Spiritual Development 981

popular subject. A study of 7,000 schoolchildren car- It is possible to use targeted teaching strategies as a
ried out in Germany on the popularity and perceived ef- means of significantly modifying religious development
ficacy of religious education yielded similar results. in positive ways, however, to do so requires engaging
The popularity of religious education is high in elemen- students in religious discussion to encourage them to
tary school, but in the 2nd year of high school it falls to question and modify their earlier religious interpreta-
the lowest third, with only physics and Latin being less tive patterns.
popular (Bucher, 2000).
With increasing age there is also a marked diminish-
Congregations
ing of the effects attributed to religious education; these
effects are highest in the area of general knowledge, in- Outside of the family, few contexts have a greater poten-
cluding knowledge of other religions, but the effects are tial for influencing and supporting religious and spiri-
small in matters of church practice and devotion. tual development than do congregations. Congregations
How can we account for such a significant drop in play a potentially important role in shaping religious be-
popularity and influence of religious education? A first liefs as they, “provide spiritual environments where
possible explanation is that religious education loses its young people can transcend their everyday concerns and
popularity because the popularity of the school as a experience connectedness with the divine and human
whole decreases with age. However, this explanation others. . . . Congregations may provide a distinct context
does not explain why the acceptance of other school sub- in which a young person can explore (spiritual) issues
jects (e.g., mathematics and English) remains constant. that are critical to commitment to identity” (King, 2003,
A second possible explanation is provided by Oser p. 200). Furthermore, congregations connect youth to
and Gmünder’s theory (Oser & Gmünder, 1991): Con- their community because they exist not on their own but
vinced that God is able to intervene in the world and that rather as a subcommunity in some broader community
He can be influenced through prayers—indications of (Roehlkepartain, 2005).
stage 2—schoolchildren are considerably better dis- Given this potential, it is surprising that there is so little
posed toward religious education and toward consider- empirical research on their influence on religious and spir-
ing religious education to be more relevant than are itual development. Congregations have, as Roehlkepartain
adolescents, whose religious judgment is apt to be at notes, slipped through the cracks in the divide between
stage 3 (Bucher, 2000, p. 128). At stage 2, children find psychology’s focus on individuals and sociology’s focus
it important to conform to God’s expectations, more so on institutions (Roehlkepartain, 2005).
than later on in adolescence when they have developed Congregational influence on the individual is bidirec-
to stage 3, the deistic stage, in which self-determination tional: The congregation shapes individual members,
is central. and individuals in the congregation shape the congrega-
However, whether religious education continues to tion’s goals, climate, and activities.
influence may depend on the character or methods of Roehlkepartain (2005) outlines the conditions and
that education. For example, in a quasi-experimental in- processes that are presumed to play a role in driving
tervention study, in which two experimental groups dis- spiritual development. First, congregations provide op-
cussed religious dilemmas over a long period of time, portunities for the formation of meaningful, positive re-
Oser and Gmünder (1996) found the average judgment lationships. Youths are able to forge relationships with
stage rose more significantly than did the average for a adults and peers who are invested in them and those re-
control group lacking in opportunities to discuss and lationships become the forum for the transmission and
problem solve collectively. Moreover, this difference re- construction of beliefs and values. Second, congrega-
mained constant in a follow-up survey carried out 6 tions establish a climate for supporting spiritual devel-
months later. A similar result was produced by Caldwell opment. Presumably, congregations that are welcoming
and Berkowitz (1987) who had high school students and warm and that are encouraging of thought and ser-
aged between 15 and 18 discuss religious dilemmas over vice are more likely to promote spiritual development
a period of 12 lessons. A subsequent test revealed that and faith maturity than those that do not.
for more than half of the students discussion sessions However, there seems to be a gap between congrega-
lead to significant development with respect to stages of tions’ actual influence and their potential for in-
religious judgment. fluence (e.g., Osmer & Schweitzer, 2003; Prell, 1995;
982 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

Roehlkepartain, 2005, Yust, 2003). The National Con- Health and Well-Being
gregational Life Survey (Woolever & Bruce, 2004)
found that 50% or fewer of the surveyed congregations A good number of studies have addressed the effects of
(n = 2,000) had effective caring for children and youth, religiosity and spirituality on health and physical and
and that a majority was reported to have fewer than 5 of psychological well-being. Plante and Sherman (2001)
the 10 strengths researchers examined. Based on these refer to at least 350 studies on physical health and 850
results, it appears that too few young people are spend- studies on mental health that treat religiosity as an inde-
ing time in congregations that are vibrant, spiritually pendent variable. The majority of these studies show
enriching, communities of faith (Roehlkepartain, that religious involvement and spirituality are associated
2005). with better health outcomes. Even when excluding those
However, these results are somewhat offset by the studies with serious methodological flaws, the evidence
more positive reports from young members of congre- suggests that religiosity and spirituality have significant
gations themselves. For example, C. Smith (2003) re- and positive effects on health and on expanding the life
ported that 62% of teenagers surveyed in one study span. Those who regularly frequent religious meetings
said congregations helped them think about important have a 30% reduced risk on mortality. This effect re-
matters, 75% said their congregation was both warm mains highly significant even after controlling for de-
and welcoming to teenagers, and 82% said that their mographic and socioeconomic variables. Furthermore,
congregation regularly provided opportunities for lead- persons who regularly attend religious services live
ership and service. longer (Oman, Kurata, Strawbridge, & Cohen, 2002).
Therefore, congregations present an interesting para- Undoubtedly, these positive correlations relate to re-
dox. They have tremendous potential for shaping and ligion’s promoting not simply participation at religious
guiding spiritual and religious development. However, services but also a healthy lifestyle. As measured by
they have not been the focus of a great deal of empirical participation and attendance at religious services, reli-
research, and what research there is suggests that we gious persons practice fewer health risk behaviors such
are a long way from determining any actual influence. as smoking, drinking, and sexual promiscuity.
Furthermore, the existing body of data on congrega- Powell, Shababi, and Thoresen (2003) demonstrated
tions is largely from samples that are both Christian this connection between religiosity and healthy lifestyle
and North American—and there is no scientific reason with specific regard for cardiovascular disease and can-
for assuming that results from these samples can be cer. Their findings suggest that the social support pro-
generalized elsewhere. vided by religious communities can reduce distress and
feelings of loneliness, both of which can undermine
physical health. A similar finding has been found with
POSITIVE CORRELATES OF RELIGIOUS respect to religious support and drugs and alcohol—with
AND SPIRITUAL DEVELOPMENT religion serving to help addicts avoid or turn away from
drugs and alcohol (Blakeney, Blakeney, & Reich, 2005).
What effects do religiousness and spirituality have on Meditation has long been a religious and spiritual
individuals’ health and sense of well-being? Does reli- practice for promoting health and well-being. Most reli-
giousness produce neurotic behavior, as Freud (1961) gious traditions have developed meditative practices, in
asserted, or does it increase the morality of believers, as part, for this reason. The effects of meditation are one
is generally assumed (Walker & Pitts, 1998)? Does spir- of the best investigated subjects in the psychology of re-
ituality, especially at higher stages of development, ex- ligion (Andresen, 2002) and the majority of studies re-
pand social consciousness and capacity to cope? In this port positive effects. Meditative practices, such as
section, we examine how religiousness and spirituality Transcendental Meditation and yoga, lower systolic and
relate to the following: diastolic blood pressure, increase nonrenal blood flow,
and decrease respiratory and heart rate (Andresen,
• Health and well-being
2002). These physiological effects reduce anxiety, de-
• Moral development and social conscience pression, and stress (Austin, 1997). The long-term prac-
• Coping tice of meditation produces more intense experiences of
• Adjusting to old age joy, meaning, love, and awareness (Andresen, 2002).
Positive Correlates of Religious and Spiritual Development 983

With regard to mental disorders and religious and morality being separate has been the research and
spiritual development, probably the single most consis- legacy of Lawrence Kohlberg.
tent finding has been that there is a negative correlation
between substance-related disorders and religiosity, es- Moral Judgment
pecially when religiosity is measured by involvement in
a faith-based community. Put another way, all other For more than 2 decades, Lawrence Kohlberg (1981) la-
things being equal, religious and spiritual development bored to have moral development considered as a sepa-
appears to be a significant protective factor preventing rate, autonomous domain—with the main engine of
an individual from developing a substance-related disor- moral development being those mundane conflicts that
der (Benson, 1992). confront children and adolescents daily. In doing so,
The quality or character of religiousness also affects Kohlberg argued against divine command theory and all
health, not just whether an individual is involved in a re- those who believe that morality derives from the pre-
ligious community. Intrinsic religiousness has stronger scriptions of religion. For most of Kohlberg’s career,
and more positive effects on physical and mental health moral development was independent of religious and
than extrinsic religiousness (Argyle, 2000). Batson, spiritual development. If anything, religion had, for him,
Schoenrade, and Ventis (1993) analyzed 115 studies and a potential stultifying effect because of those times
found that most studies reported that intrinsic religios- when religion suppresses the individual’s taking an ac-
ity correlated positively with health, especially mental tive role in distinguishing right from wrong.
health, but extrinsic religiosity correlated more nega- However, in his later years, Kohlberg softened his
tively (see also Plante & Bovccaccini, 1997). Further- stance toward religion as he confronted the meta-ethical
more, intrinsic religiosity positively correlated with a question of “Why be moral in an immoral world?”
sense of coherence, which itself enhances mental and Doing so led to his adopting a positive view of how reli-
physical well-being. In addition, individuals practicing gious development (or, more specifically, faith develop-
intrinsic religiosity are, on average, happier, especially ment) can support moral development. He came to
if they feel close to God (Pollner, 1989). believe that the primary function of religious structures
In sum, religiosity can positively influence physical is to validate and thereby support being moral. Religious
and mental health and well-being and happiness. Argyle structures do this by grounding morality not in promises
(1999, p. 366) discussed these findings as, in part, due of personal gain but in faith in human nature and in a
to the effect of the social support provided by the reli- cosmic order (Kohlberg, 1981).
gious communities. To an extent, Kohlberg saw religious development as
the by-product or outcome of moral development. There-
fore, just as logical structures are necessary but not suf-
Moral Development ficient for the development of moral structures, moral
structures are necessary but not sufficient for the devel-
In the history of social science research, religion has opment of religious (faith) structures (Kohlberg, 1981).
been depicted as serving only a weak influence on This reverses the usual way of relating moral and reli-
morality, or none at all. This view has been supported in gious development. Usually, morality is thought of as the
several well-known studies. For example, Hartshorne outcome of religious and spiritual development. This is
and May (1928) showed that neither religious affiliation the way that laypersons think, and it was also the way
nor religiously directed character education had any ef- that William James thought. In James’s (1902) discourse
fects on moral behavior. Darley and Batson (1973) on saintliness, he began not with morality but with the
found that almost two-thirds of the undergraduates they saint’s experience of being connected to an unseen,
observed entering a chapel to hear a sermon on the beneficent power. The by-product or “ fruit ” of this expe-
Good Samaritan, failed to help a man lying on the side- rience was, for James, a powerful and positive morality.
walk and in need of help. And in the current debate over How should we understand this relationship between
character education, there often is a determined effort moral development and religious and spiritual develop-
to keep religion out of the discussion (Damon, 2002; ment—in Kohlberg’s terms, in James’s terms, or of
Schwartz, 2002). However, by far the most important some alternative? And how do the findings of research
support for this view of religion’s relationship with help us decide?
984 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

The necessary but not sufficient hypothesis (Kohlberg Service


& Power, 1981, p. 227) has been tested by several stud- Reference to moral exemplars reminds us that moral de-
ies—with mixed results. Caldwell and Berkowitz (1987) velopment involves much more than judgment and rea-
found in their interviews with 50 Wisconsin students soning. It also involves action. One measure of moral
(mean age = 16.4 years) that students’ stages of moral development is service to others. How does religious
judgment were, as a rule, higher then their stages of reli- and spiritual development relate to community service
gious judgment, as measured by the Oser and Gmünder in those who have yet to become moral exemplars?
scales. In contrast, studies by Gut (1984) found that a Most of the research has been about adolescents and
quarter of her sample scored higher on stages of religious their involvement in religious institutions and commu-
judgment than on stages of moral judgment. Considering nity service.
inconsistent results such as these, more research is A number of studies have demonstrated a positive re-
needed to determine the exact relationships between the lationship between involvement in religious institutions
development of moral and religious judgment. and community service. For example, Youniss et al.
However, recent research on moral functioning as dis- (1999) found that high school students who valued reli-
tinguished from moral judgment, has shown a more inti- gion were vibrantly engaged in their schooling and in the
mate and subtle connection between moral development betterment of their communities.
and religious and spiritual development than that char- For adolescents, community service in the context of
acterized in discussions about stages of moral and reli- religious institutions seems to have a different and
gious judgment. For example, in a study of adult moral more positive meaning than community service in
exemplars, Colby and Damon (1992) found that the ma- other contexts. For example, Donnelly and his col-
jority of their sample acted out of some sense for the leagues (Donnelly et al., 2005) have demonstrated that
transcendent and that morality, spirituality, and identity adolescents doing community service in the context of
were fused in such a way as to sustain their lives of car- religious institutions as compared to those doing com-
ing. In other words, caring was, for them, who they were munity service in other contexts are more likely to do
or who they became. In a similar vein, Walker and his community service later on, in their adult lives.
colleagues found general support for William James’s
thesis that authentic religious experience is evidenced in
mature moral functioning (Walker, 2003; Walker &
Pitts, 1998; Walker, Pitts, Hennig, & Matsuba, 1995; Coping
Walker & Reimer, 2005).
One of the fundamental and undoubtedly universal func-
These findings refer to “authentic religious experi-
tions of religiousness is coping, especially in critical life
ence” and to “mature moral functioning.” But what about
events such as the death of a loved relative and serious
religious experience that may not be authentic or true to
illness. Certainly, long before psychologists studied cop-
the core values and beliefs of a faith tradition? The re-
ing processes, religions have disposed resources for fa-
sults show negative or ambiguous connections between
cilitating coping.
religion and morality: For example, a number of studies
have found a clear and positive correlation between ritual In the past decades, coping has become a very impor-
attendance, orthodox beliefs, and racial prejudice (Bat- tant concept in the psychology of religion (Pargament &
son et al., 1993). Other studies (B. Hunsberger, 1995) Brant, 1998). Pargament and Brant distinguished the
have found a curvilinear effect such that the most reli- following types of religious coping:
gious and the totally irreligious are the least prejudiced.
With regard to prejudice, there are no straightforward in- • Self-directed coping, as when subjects use God-given
fluences of religion on morality. Similarly, several stud- resources to individually solve their problems.
ies have found only a weak effect of religion in preventing • Deferring coping, as when individuals, especially in
delinquency (Hood et al., 1996). It seems that positive re- seemingly hopeless situations, give up control to a
lationships between moral development and religious and higher power or God, thereby paradoxically gaining
spiritual development are most evident at higher stages or control (Baugh, 1988).
when religious and spiritual experience becomes authen- • Collaborative coping, as when individuals appraise
tic and in accord with positive faith traditions. God as a helping partner.
Negative Correlates and Pathology 985

Out of the hundreds of studies on religious coping, most The obvious benefits produced by religiousness and
support the following points: spirituality in old age are integration and social support,
the creation of meaning and purpose, fostering a greater
• One of the most frequently used religious coping sense of control and maintaining better health. These
strategies is prayer. points can be summarized as follows:
• Religious coping is more frequent among females, older
individuals, blacks, and those who are less educated. • Religious communities offer integration and social
• If individuals have faith in a loving God, the outcomes support. Scales for loneliness are more powerfully
of religious coping are more apt to be less stress, less correlated in a negative way for the elderly than for
anxiety, a greater sense of well-being, and greater ca- younger people, and the benefits of church member-
pacity for handling difficulties than if they evaluate ship are greater for people in retirement or who have
negative life events as God’s punishment. been widowed (Argyle, 1987). Religious communi-
• Usually, the outcomes of collaborative religious cop- ties create a bolstering environment in which burden-
ing are more promising than are the outcomes of some life events, such as bereavements, can more
other styles. easily be coped with. Religious communities also
produce an atmosphere of acceptance, hope, and for-
But how does a person develop competencies in coping? giveness (Koenig, 1992). In her wide ranging Yale
And why do people prefer different coping styles? The Health and Aging Project, Idler (1994) found that
lack of explicitly developmental analyses (coping styles at being integrated with a religious community pre-
different stages of religious judgment, etc.) is a limitation dicted less depression and lower suicidal tendencies.
in this research—a limitation that makes it difficult to an- • Religions can additionally create an inner sense of se-
swer these and other important questions having to do with curity by offering meaningful rites and symbols (Ar-
coping and religious and spiritual development. gyle, 1987). They can provide a sense that life as a
whole, with its high-points, its lows, and its finiteness,
is meaningful. Religiousness is a protective factor
Positive Correlates in Advanced Age against problems of meaning (Dittmann-Kohli, 1990).
The considerable benefits of religiousness and spiritu- • Through the lens of religion, death, which looms ever
ality in advanced age warrant special attention. From more inexorably in old age, can be interpreted as the
his studies, Argyle (1987) concluded that for older peo- beginning of a better world, as the transition into
ple, religiousness and well-being are closely tied to- eternal life or as something natural and positive. As
gether. Levin (1997) also demonstrated that for many Meadow and Kahoe (1984) report that immortality
older people, religiousness has significant and positive seems to be the single item of faith that increases
effects. substantially with age.
Religiousness obviously has a striking effect on how • Religiousness has always been connected with con-
old age is experienced. For example, in one study, out of victions about control, even though we might believe
1,011 elderly men surveyed, a distinct majority said re- that the religious person is ceding control to a divine
ligiousness was more important to them than it was in being (control illusion). Well-being presupposes pos-
their middle years (Koenig, 1994). This seems to be es- sibilities of control. Baugh (1988) reduced this con-
pecially true for older women (Mc Fadden, 1996). The trol paradox to the formula, “Gaining control by
older the subjects are, the more powerful are their be- giving up control.” Ceding control to a higher power
liefs in a personal God, and the more their faith provides unburdens the spirit and makes it easier to cope. For
help with problems in living (Jörns, 1997). example, in the Duke longitudinal study, 100 elderly
Yet, religious activities diminish in old age (Blazer & participants, when asked how they dealt with stress-
Palmore, 1976). Many seniors, on account of their ful experiences (death of relatives, etc.), frequently
frailty and illness, are no longer capable of active partic- named having trust and belief in God (Koenig,
ipation in congregations. However, this lack is compen- George, & Siegler, 1988).
sated for by an increase in unorganized, private religious • Most religions prescribe a code of behavior, which
practice, especially prayer. promotes good physical health, and this correlates
986 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

highly with happiness and satisfaction with life concept of revelation, they have generally given one single
(Argyle, 1987, p. 176). The greater extent of well- meaning to revelation when there are several, including
being enjoyed by religious people in old age is also meanings that acknowledge multiple interpretations of sa-
explained by pointing out that they rarely smoke or cred texts and religious experience (Dulles, 1994).
drink alcohol and eat with greater moderation. Without making important distinctions between the
meanings of these and other central religious concepts,
religion easily appears to consist mostly of irrational be-
NEGATIVE CORRELATES lief and dogmatic, childish denial that truth is a matter of
AND PATHOLOGY interpretation and argument. Add a quantitative dimen-
sion to the discussion, and such negative views of religion
The preceding section has shown that spirituality and re- often turn into pathologizing religion whenever religion
ligiousness exert positive influences. The term religion, is taken seriously. One is reminded of Albert Ellis’s
however, also elicits images of September 11th, 2001 and (1980) remark, “ The less religious (people) are, the
of Jonestown, Guyana. We need to look at the evidence more emotionally healthy they will tend to be” (p. 637).
for negative and not just positive correlates. However, be- Failure to make meaningful distinctions has been one
fore doing so, we begin by discussing the negative biases problem. Making false distinctions has been another.
against religion, which, in the social sciences, have im- Perhaps the most obvious example of making a false dis-
peded progress in sorting out negative from positive. tinction is between science and religion, such that one is
With respect to religious and spiritual development, pitted against the other. This distinction often goes un-
the history of both psychological research and clinical challenged, despite the fact that the majority of religious
practice has been a history of negative bias and preju- persons who are also scientists find no contradiction in
dice. Bias and prejudice persist today but has perhaps being both, and despite the fact that only a small minor-
lessened—both because of a new tolerance for cultural ity of the religious people believe in unscientific theo-
diversity and because of empirical research that has ries such as creationism (Gould, 2003b).
challenged old negative stereotypes. Nevertheless, we This last reference to creationist theory brings up a
begin with a brief discussion of the most obvious nega- third prejudicial practice among researchers and clini-
tive biases and prejudices because their persistence cians—the practice of selecting unrepresentative exam-
presents a significant roadblock to understanding nega- ples to support arguments against religion. An example
tive correlates and pathology associated with religious of this practice occurred on the DSM III-R’s glossary of
and spiritual development. terms for defining mental disorders—where an inordi-
Negative bias is particularly evident in stereotyping— nate number of examples with religious content were
in collapsing meaningful distinctions into one negative given to illustrate what is meant by various mental disor-
category. Stereotyping the religious and a failure to make ders (Larson et al., 1993).
distinctions has been the hallmark of negative bias among A fourth prejudicial practice has been to report, as
research psychologists and clinicians. An overview of the objective fact, findings based on flawed, value-laden
literature indicates that rarely are distinctions made measures that are biased against religion. For example,
when discussing central religious concepts such as belief, on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory
faith, and revelation. Yet, there are many meanings for (MMPI ), answering positively to the statement, “I am
each of these three, and the differences matter. orthodoxly religious” lowers one’s overall score (Gart-
With regard to the concept of belief, research psycholo- ner, 1996). Such soft measures express the value system
gists and clinicians have customarily equated the meaning of a select group—often proponents of secular human-
of religious belief with the meaning of belief as it is used ism and self-actualizing theory (Maslow, 1971).
when speaking of, say, belief in trees and dogs, though the Researchers in religious psychology have been particu-
two meanings differ from one another (Blackstone, 1963). larly harsh on fundamentalist groups. Fundamentalists
With regard to the concept of faith, and, as previously dis- are routinely lumped together to form a negative stereo-
cussed, research psychologists and clinicians have cus- type consisting of character traits such as immature, dog-
tomarily equated faith with belief despite these two matic, rigid, and prejudiced. There is evidence to support
concepts having quite different meanings in the history of the claim that religious fundamentalism is indeed associ-
faith traditions (W. C. Smith, 1998a). With regard to the ated with higher levels of prejudice, authoritarism, and an
Negative Correlates and Pathology 987

“us versus them” mentality (Altemeyer, 2003), however, favors that benefit neither the group’s members nor so-
at least one study suggests that prejudice is not confined ciety (Barrett, 2001). The media sometimes adds nega-
to any one group and that groups of so-called healthy, tive terms such as brain-washing to characterize the
quest-orientated individuals are also prejudiced, espe- socialization of cult members. This negative definition
cially toward fundamentalists (Goldfried & Miner, 2002). of cults works well as an ideal type for identifying
The point is not that the religious are problem free or harmful cults, but the definition has been applied indis-
that there is no pathology associated with religious and criminately and to religious groups that do not fit the
spiritual development. We need not go to the opposite definition. As a result, discussions of cults are often
extreme. However, in the history of religious research polemical and groups of researchers are divided into
and clinical practice, there has been persistent negative “cult critics” and “cult sympathizers.”
bias against religion. Religious and spiritual develop- Cult sympathizers are more likely to use the terms
ment is a complex phenomenon that challenges us to new religious movements (NRM) and alternative reli-
make numerous distinctions and be scrupulous about ev- gions instead of cult, though these terms, too, have their
idence and measures. Pathology is real and does not re- problems. Certain groups considered by most to be cults
side in the eyes of the diagnostician, but pathology, as a are not new and in some contexts would not be alterna-
concept, needs to be used with care. tives to the mainstream. The International Society for
Having clarified some misunderstandings that have Krishna Consciousness, otherwise known as the Hare
impeded progress, we can turn now to recent research on Krishna, is an example (Daner, 1976). Nevertheless, new
psychopathology as it relates to religious and spiritual religious movements or alternative religions have be-
development. We discuss the subjects of cults, occultist come accepted terms. Cult sympathizers point out,
practices, terrorist groups, mental disorders, and patho- tongue in cheek, that the difference between a cult and a
genic (toxic) religious practices and beliefs—as they re- religion is about a million members (D. Barrett, 2001).
late to religious and spiritual development. There is also misunderstanding about the average age
of cult members. Cults are associated with “ youth reli-
gions,” which came into prominence in the 1970s. How-
Cults, Occultist Practices, and Terrorist Groups ever, the average age of cult members is estimated to be
With regard to cults, occultist practices, and terrorist between 25 and 40 (Schmitz & Friebe, 1992). For those
groups, the literature suggests two points in particular. in Bhagwan Shree Rajneesch, living in Oregon, the aver-
The first point is that problems of definition have cre- age age is 34 years (Richardson, 1995).
ated biases that impede the progress of research on Concerning the second point about who joins cults, the
cults. The second point is that there is no conclusive ev- available evidence suggests that members generally are
idence that members of so-called cults are any more de- no more pathological than are nonmembers (Richardson,
velopmentally disturbed or pathological than are 1995). In explaining why and how a person comes to join
individuals who do not join cults or terrorist groups. a cult, a more historical, less psychological approach may
work best because circumstances play a decisive role. For
example, a newly arrived freshman at college may, along
Cults and Occultist Practices with other freshman, feel lonely and disoriented but ad-
Historically, the term cult has had a neutral meaning— just without joining a cult, however if that freshman hap-
both in faith traditions and in the sociology of religion. pens to meet and talk with a cult member, he or she may
However, since the 1960s and especially since the well end up joining a cult (D. Barrett, 2001).
Jonestown and Waco tragedies (in Jonestown 912 people These words of caution about rushing to judge cults
of the Temple Sect died by an organized mass suicide; in aside, the extreme example of religion taking the wrong
Waco, 74 disciples of David Koresh perished by a mas- course is still taken to be the norm. A clear majority of Eu-
sacre), the term has been defined by the media and cer- ropeans live in a state of alert on account of religious cults
tain government agencies as referring to a particular (Schmidtchen, 1987), and in the United States, there is a
kind of group, one with a self-appointed, dogmatic, and similar negative view as well. Pfeiffer (1992) showed that
charismatic leader who promotes deceptive-coercive re- the overwhelming majority of American students (82%)
cruitment practices to ensnare individuals to join a total- described cult members purely in negative terms, deeming
itarian community organized to solicit funds and secure them to be less happy, less intelligent, and less free.
988 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

Of particular relevance to developmental psychology is times help members to cope. In a survey of 517 members
the question of what leads individuals to join cults. Sev- of the Unification Church and of Ananda Marga, Kuner
eral clinical studies conducted mostly with persons who (1981) showed that long-term membership produced “re-
had left cults, established that before joining cults, many socializing” and “therapeutic” effects. Schibilsky (1976)
members had serious family and nonfamily problems found that joining cults helped members cope with devel-
(Klosinski, 1996). By some accounts, life before joining a opmental tasks such as forming an identity and developing
cult was characterized by a history of poor relationships self-discipline. As another example of positive effects,
with both parents and peers (Silverstein, 1988). In her Wicca groups have been described as empowering women
sample, Rollett (1996) also found that young people with a and helping them to heal wounds inflicted on them in so-
high exposure to crises tended more strongly toward join- cieties where women, especially lesbians, have been dis-
ing cults. Others have characterized those who join cults empowered or hurt by homophobia (Warwick, 1995).
as persons who have suffered from the absence of a father Positive views of the effects of cults are sometimes
during childhood and who have difficulty dealing with the accompanied by the observation that cults help individ-
complexities of life (Ullman, 1982). This is consistent uals make a transition. People often belong to cults on a
with the finding of one study that showed that new con- temporary basis and experience them as “ havens”
verts to cults held stronger authoritarian values than did (Hood et al., 1996). In his thorough meta-analysis of
nonconverts (Shaver, Leneuaer, & Sadd, 1980). clinical studies on the psychological effects of joining
Popular empirical accounts often deliver the blanket cults, Richardson (1995) concluded that membership is
judgment that cults rob their members of freedom, individ- often therapeutic instead of harmful.
uality, and wealth. These accounts accuse cults of fostering Related findings on occultist practices have shown
dislocation from reality, thought paralysis, and regression that what may at first appear to be psychopathology of a
(Lademann-Priemer, 1998). The doctrines of many cults do religious or spiritual nature can be, on closer inspection,
seem to operate on lower developmental stages—as when a means for spiritual growth. For example, Streib (1999)
cults divide humanity into small groups of those who are described the occultist practices of the groups of Euro-
good and saved, from the great majority who are bad and pean adolescents he observed as “off-road religion,” im-
lost (Brickerhoff & MacKie, 1986). As another example, plying that they were age and stage appropriate for those
the Krishnas, who consider a doll to be the Divine itself, who have yet to achieve an adult identity. Furthermore,
seem to collapse the distinction between a religious symbol and contrary to popular negative views, the overwhelm-
and its referent (Cassirer, 1955; Fowler, 1981). ing majority of studies have shown that only a minority
However, negative characterizations of those joining of teenagers regularly perform occult practices, and for
cults may well be the result of interviewing mostly those most in this minority, occult practices are performed out
who have left cults because they were dissatisfied—the of curiosity and not from existential engagement.
research may be biased toward having a negative view Occult practices, rather than expressing pathology,
because the sample of former cult members is a biased can express religious affiliation and religious stage. Rol-
sample (Richardson, van der Lans, & Derks, 1986). lett (1992) demonstrated that catholic youth are more
As for the effects of joining cults, in the worst cases, attracted to occultism than are youth without any reli-
the results have been fatal, especially authoritarian gious denominations, and Bucher (1994) showed that
leaders or closed groups hinder development toward reli- adolescents at stage 2 of religious judgment according to
gious maturity and autonomy, respectively if they pro- Oser and Gmünder (1991) consider occult practices as
duce a childish and inflexible dependence. Fatal or being plausible, more so than do adolescents at stage 3.
otherwise, seriously harmful effects may happen, espe-
cially when cult members engage in occultist practices. Terrorist Groups
In the past decade, occult forms of religiousness among Similar to what was has been said about cults, what con-
adolescents have become, at least in Europe, a popular stitutes a terrorist group depends on one’s perspective
theme (Helsper, 1992). Headlines have covered tragic (Scarlett, 2003). Those identifying with or participating
deaths of young people after they performed occult in such groups define the groups very differently than
practices such as moving glasses and holding séances. outsiders—as aggrieved victims of violent injustice
However, some researchers have shown positive (Silke, 2003). For them, the group is all about justice and
effects of cults. Salzman (1953) found that cults some- freedom, not what is implied by the term terrorist group.
Negative Correlates and Pathology 989

Becoming a member of a terrorist group can be seen and • We need to focus on educating the general public about
experienced as entirely natural and normal—somewhat those social and political conditions to minimize the
akin to joining the army or police in times of war or crisis demonizing that continues the cycle of violence.
(Silke, 2003). In sum, terrorists, on average, have no ap-
preciable psychopathology and are average with respect
Mental Disorders
to level of education and socioeconomic status. It seems
that terrorism is more a group phenomenon than it is an As used here, the concept of a mental disorder is the
individual phenomenon—and should be studied as such. same as used in major classification systems such as the
There is a particular need for research on religion’s American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Sta-
role in stimulating terrorism and for understanding bet- tistical Manual (DSM). These classification systems
ter what leads to religiously sponsored evil. Kimball have a number of conceptual and practical advantages
(2002) provides a framework for explaining—using the over dimensional approaches (Cantwell & Rutter, 2002).
following list of symptoms for evaluating when religion However, they also have disadvantages. Professionals
is in danger of supporting evil: agree that classification systems should be used in con-
junction with dimensional approaches to correct for
• Inhibition of (religious) autonomy and freedom their inherent deficiencies. With this cautionary point
• Absolute truth claims made, we can proceed to discuss how the current litera-
ture treats religious and spiritual development with re-
• Blind obedience
spect to mental disorders as defined by classification
• Establishing an ideal time systems, particularly by the DSM-IV.
• Claiming the end justifies the means First, with regard to the DSM-IV itself, this revised
• Declaring Holy War form includes a V-code for “Other Conditions”—those
conditions that occasionally warrant a clinician’s help
What should we conclude from this overview of how the but which do not constitute mental disorders. “Medica-
literature discusses cults, occultist practices, and terrorist tion-induced movement ” disorder is one example, and
groups? If there is no more psychopathology, on average, “Religious or spiritual problem” is another.
within these cults and terrorist groups than without, are Including the V-Code “Religious or spiritual prob-
there no problems? Obviously, there are severe problems. lem” marks a step forward—because it says that prob-
There are cults that deceive, abuse, and kill, and terrorist lems with a religious or spiritual content should not be
groups inflict immeasurable harm and suffering on thou- automatically pathologized. However, some have ar-
sands of innocents. However, the problems do not fit neatly gued that the inclusion of religious and spiritual prob-
into the category of psychopathology—unless the category lems as one of several “other conditions” marginalizes
itself is expanded to fit just about everyone. With respect these problems when, in certain cases, they should not
to cults, the main conclusions are the following: be marginalized (Scott, Garver, Richards, & Hath-
away, 2003).
• We need to distinguish harmful cults from those new As for the other categories defining mental disorders,
religious movements that do no harm. In doing so, we the picture is more complex. Perhaps the best overall
can apply criteria such as deception, manipulation, characterization of the research is that with these other
coercion, and developmental stage but not without a categories (psychotic disorders, mood disorders, etc.),
careful examination of the evidence. religiosity and spirituality can define the content of the
• We need to develop reliable measures to evaluate disorders (e.g., having delusions with religious content),
cults in short- versus long-term functioning. but there is no evidence that religiosity and spirituality
cause these disorders. For example, the frequency of re-
With respect to terrorist groups, the main conclusions ligious delusions in groups of psychotic individuals
are the following: varies considerably depending on the group sampled—
as much as between 7% and 45% (Kingdom, Siddle, &
• We need to focus on changing the social and political Rathod, 2001)—suggesting psychotic individuals use
conditions leading to essentially normal religious in- whatever content is available in their culture to con-
dividuals joining terrorist groups. struct their delusions.
990 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

A further point about complexity is made with regard those without negative religious attributions. An avenging
to individuals with the same diagnosis but who differ from God brings about guilt feelings and can drive the self into
one another in their religiosity. For example, when accom- punishing itself if it perceives itself to be disobedient in
panied by strong religious faith, depression may have a the eyes of God (Hood 1992, p. 118). As another example
very different and more positive meaning with respect to and on the basis of clinical studies, Frielingsdorf (1992)
ability to function than depression without faith (Stone, described shocking examples of casuistry, where people
2000). Stage dependence should also be considered. lost all self-respect and felt themselves to be “like dirt” on
account of demonic images of God. He found that these
people were often unwanted and neglected as children.
Pathogenic (Toxic) Religious Beliefs, Practices
What can we conclude about the research reported on
and Attitudes
negative correlates and pathology related to religious and
Perhaps the best researched problem of this sort has been spiritual development? For one thing, we seem to be just at
that of prejudice and its relationship with religion. One of the beginning stage. To build an adequate research base,
the most consistent findings has been that moderate and we need to develop a more nuanced description system for
superficial levels of religious involvement predict high defining problems of a religious and spiritual nature. There
levels of prejudice (Allport & Ross, 1967). The distinc- are, for example, conceptual reasons for distinguishing be-
tion here is between extrinsic and intrinsic religious ori- tween different forms of problematic faith. There is idola-
entation or those who use religion for self-serving means trous faith, structurally immature faith, dysfunctional
such as making social connections versus those whose faith, and structurally developed faith whose content pro-
core identity is defined by their faith tradition. It is not motes evil (Scarlett, 2003). We need more research to es-
that religion fosters prejudice so much as it is how individ- tablish whether these and other distinctions can be used
uals use their religion. Just why intrinsic types are similar scientifically to further define problematic religious and
on measures of prejudice to nonreligious types is unclear. spiritual development. One of the main reasons for our
However, there are clear instances of religiously spon- adopting a normative, stage-structural analysis of religious
sored prejudice that should be distinguished from the and spiritual development becomes clear when discussing
psychological prejudice manifest in extrinsic religious problems relating to religiosity and spirituality. A norma-
orientation. For example, certain fundamentalist groups tive analysis is crucial for evaluating these problems and
find in the Bible grounds for considering homosexuality a for developing the right means to solve them.
sin. Religiously sponsored prejudice and the psychologi-
cal prejudice in extrinsic religious orientation are quite CONCLUSIONS
different in nature and should be discussed, explained,
and treated separately. This distinction between psycho- One goal for this chapter was to connect disparate voices
logical problems of individuals and problems associated in developmental science in general and in the study of
with a group’s thinking comes up when discussing cults spiritual and religious development in particular. Obvi-
and terrorist groups as well. ously, a field is enriched by there being different voices
Religious attributions (see Spilka & McIntosh, 1995) and alternative perspectives. Especially when the topics
can foster self-esteem and psychic equilibrium, but they in the study of religious and spiritual development have
can also weaken them. In particular, a person’s ability to been about culture, stages of development, and the rela-
cope can be impeded when critical events in life are attrib- tionship between belief and faith development, additional
uted to the punishment of God, which traditional religious perspectives and more research are needed. Nevertheless,
instruction used to encourage. Such attributions foster we have argued in this chapter that there are meaningful
feelings of anger, helplessness, shame, and fear (Parga- ways to attend to individual and cultural differences
ment, Ensing, & Falgout, 1990), and they undermine self- while still attending to what is universal and normative.
effectiveness (Di Loreto & Oser, 1996). For example, 13% We need to find ways to attend integratively to both di-
of the cardiac patients interviewed by Croog and Levine versity and general principles, and we also need to attend
(1972), convinced themselves that their illness was a pun- to matters of belief as well as to matters of faith.
ishment for earlier sins, and, in so doing, damaged their Another goal of this chapter was to make our own
ability to cope—as measured by their recovery time being position clear, and to explain why we believe that a con-
longer and their sense of well-being being poorer than for structivist and normative, stage-structural approach
References 991

will always be needed. Stage theory is indispensable for Allport, G. W. (1950). The individual and his religion: A psychologi-
defining religious and spiritual development, and it pro- cal interpretation. New York: Macmillan.
Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. (1967). Personal religious orientation and
vides possibilities for guiding interventions designed to
prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 432–443.
support religious and spiritual development. In addi- Altemeyer, B. (2003). Why do religious fundamentalists tend to be
tion, a stage-structural approach can provide ways to ex- prejudiced? International Journal for the Psychology of Religion,
plain universals in religious and spiritual development. 13(1), 17–28.
We contend that higher stages of positive religious Andersen, D. S. (1988). Values, religion, social class and the choice
of private school in Australia. International Journal of Educa-
and spiritual development provide adaptive functions. tional Research, 12, 351–373.
The current body of evidence seems to bear us out. The Andresen, J. (2002). Meditation meets behavioral medicine: The
main functions provided by mature, positive faith have story of experimental research on meditation. In I. J. A. R. K. For-
to do with giving humans hope for a better world, foster- man (Ed.), Cognitive models and spiritual maps (pp. 17–64).
Charlottesville, VA: Imprint Academic.
ing greater tolerance and respect for others, and provid-
Anthony, S. (1940). The child’s discovery of death. London: Rout-
ing motivation to work for justice, demonstrate care, ledge & Kegan.
and experience well-being even in the face of adversity. Argyle, M. (1987). The psychology of happiness. London: Methuen
However, the main goal of this chapter has been to show & Co.
Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happiness. In I. D.
the richness and importance of research and thinking in the
Kahnemann, E. Diener, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The
study of spiritual and religious development. This chapter foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 353–373). New York:
is the first on religious and spiritual development to appear Russell Sage Foundation.
in the Handbook of Child Psychology, although its inclusion Argyle, M. (2000). Psychology of religion: An introduction. New
York: Routledge.
was anticipated about a decade ago (Cairns, 1998; see also
Aronoff, J. S., & Lynn, S. (2000). Are cultic environments psycholog-
Cairns, Chapter 3; Lerner, Chapter 1, this Handbook, this ically harmful? Clinical Psychology Review, 20(1), 91–111.
volume), We hope and trust that it will not be the last— Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2005). Religion’s evolutionary land-
given that the study of religious and spiritual development scape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion.
is so central to our understanding of human development. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(6).
Furthermore, this field is expanding rapidly, and the Austin, J. A. (1997). Stress reduction through mindfulness meditation:
Effects on psychological symptomatology, sense of control, and spir-
next edition of the Handbook will likely be reporting on itual experiences. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 66, 97–106.
exciting new research findings in areas not even noted in Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life-
the present chapter. For example, there are beginning ef- span theory in developmental psychology. In W. Damon (Editor-
forts to connect research on brain development to religious in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child
psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th
and spiritual development, and, while there are currently ed., 1029–1144). New York: Wiley.
no good bridge theories (J. A. Feldman, in press) that ade- Barnes, E. (1892). Theological life of a Californian child. On Peda-
quately connect mind to brain to make the brain a truly gogical Seminary, 2, 442–448.
useful source of explanations for religious and spiritual Barrett, D. (2001). The new believers: A survey of sects, cults, and al-
ternative religions. London: Cassell.
development, those theories are likely to be in place by the
Barrett, J. (2001). Do children experience God as adults do. In J. An-
time of the next edition. The same predication may be for- dresen (Ed.), Religion in mind: Cognitive perspectives on religious
warded about other areas either not discussed at all in this belief, ritual, and experience (pp. 173–191). Cambridge, England:
chapter or reported on only slightly. For example, we ex- Cambridge University Press.
Barrett, J., & Keil, F. (1996). Conceptualizing a non-natural en-
pect that the research now being conducted on the role of
tity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology,
spirituality in youth development will yield a much better 31, 219–247.
account of interactions between organismic and contex- Barth, C. (1911). Der Himmel in der Gedankenwelt 10- und 11-
tual factors central to religious and spiritual development. jähriger Kinder. Monatsblätter für den Evangelischen Religionsun-
terricht, 4, 336–338.
In sum, the future of theory and research on religious and
Batson, C. D. V. W. L., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993). Reli-
spiritual development seems, at this writing, to be a bright gion and the individual. New York: Oxford University Press.
and scientifically productive one. Baugh, J. R. (1988). Gaining control by giving up control: Strategies
for coping with powerlessness. In I. W. R. Miller & J. E. Martin
(Eds.), Behavior therapy and religion (pp. 125–138). Newbury
REFERENCES Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Beile, H. (1998). Religiöse Emotionen und religiöses Urteil: Eine em-
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological pirische Studie über Religiosität bei Jugendlichen. Ostfildern:
study of the strange situation. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum. Schwabenverlag.
992 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1989). Prolegomena to the psychological study of Brown, L. B. (1994). The human side of prayer: The psychology of
religion. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell University Press. praying. Birmingham, AL: Religious Education Press.
Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1996a). Psychanalytic studies of religion: A criti- Bucher, A. (1991). Understanding parables: A developmental analy-
cal assessment and annotated bibliography. Westport, CT: Green- sis. In F. K. Oser & G.W. Scarlett (Eds.), New Directions for child
wood Press. development: Religious development in childhood and adolescence
Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1996b). Sense in nonsense: The psychoanalytic (pp. 101–105). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
approach to religion. In J. Rozenberg (Ed.), Sense and nonsense: Bucher, A. (1994). Ist Okkultismus die neue Jugendreligion? Eine
Philosophical, clinical and ethical perspectives. Jerusalem: Hebrew empirische Untersuchung an 650 jugendlichen. Archiv fur Reli-
University Magnes Press. gionpsychologie, 21, 248–266.
Beit-Hallahmi, B., & Argyle, M. (1997). The psychology of religious Bucher, A. (2000). Religionsunterricht zwischen Lernfach und
behavior, belief, and experience. New York: Routledge. Lebenshilfe: Eine empirische Untersuchung zum katholiscchen
Benish, G. (2001). Heart and home: Embracing the spiritual in every- Religionsunterricht in der Bundesrepublik Deutschland.
day life. New York: Kensington. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.
Benson, P. L. (1992). Religion and substance use. In I. J. Schumaker Bucher, A. (2004). Psychobiographien religioser Entwicklung
(Ed.), Religion and mental health (pp. 157–179). New York: Ox- Glaubensprofile zwischen Individualitat und Universalitat.
ford University Press. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.
Benson, P. L., Donahue, M. J., & Erickson, J. A. (1993). The faith Burgardsmeier, A. (1951). Gott und Himmel in der psychischen Welt
maturity scale: Conceptualization, measurement, and empiri- der Jugend: Vom dritten Schuljahr bis zum Ausgang des Volksschu-
cal validation. Research in the Social Scientific Study of Reli- lalters. Düsseldorf, Germany: Schwann / Patmos-Verlag.
gion, 5, 1–26.
Burkert, W. (1996). Creation of the sacred: Tracks of biology in early
Benson, P. L., Roehlkepartain, E. C., & Rude, S. P. (2003). Spiritual religions. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
development in childhood and adolescence: Toward a field of in-
Cairns, R. B. (1998). The making of developmental psychology. In
quiry. Applied Developmental Science, 7, 205–213.
W. Damon (Editor-in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. ed.), Handbook
Berg, M. (2001). The way: Using the wisdom of Kabbalah for spiritual of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human develop-
transformation and fulfillment. New York: Wiley. ment (5th ed., pp. 25–105). New York: Wiley.
Blackstone, W. (1963). The problem of religious knowledge. Engle- Caldwell, J. A., & Berkowitz, M. L. (1987). Die Entwicklung re-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. ligiösen und moralischen Denkens in einem Programm zum Reli-
Blakeney, C. D., & Blakeney, R. F. (2005). Delinquency: A quest for gionsunterricht. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 15, 157–176.
moral and spiritual integrity? In E. C. Roehlkepartain, P. King, L.
Calvin, J. (1834). Commentarius in Epistolam ad Ephesios. Corpus
Wagener, & P. Benson (Eds.), The handbook of spiritual develop-
Reformatorum, Berlin, 79, 141–240.
ment in childhood and adolescence (pp. 371–383). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage. Cantwell, D., & Rutter, M. (2002). Classification: Conceptual is-
sues and substantive findings. In S. B. Thielman (Ed.), Child
Blakeney, C. D., Blakeney, R. F., & Reich, K. H. (2005). Leaps of
and adolescent psychiatry (4th ed., pp. 3–19). Cambridge, MA:
faith: The role of spirituality in recovering integrity among Jew-
Blackwell Science.
ish alcoholics and drug addicts. Mental Health, Religion and Cul-
ture, 8, 1–19. Caplovitz, D., & Sherrow, F. (1977). The religious drop-outs: Apos-
Blazer, D., & Palmore, E. (1976). Religion and aging in a longitudinal tasy among college graduates. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
panel. Gerontologist, 16, 82–85. Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New
York: Academic Press.
Boyatzis, C., Dollahite, D., & Marks, L. (2005). The family context
of children’s spiritual and religious development. In E. C. Cassirer, E. (1955). The philosophy of symbolic forms: Vol. 2. Mythi-
Roehlkepartain, P. King, L. Wagener, & P. Benson (Eds.), The cal thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
handbook of spiritual development in childhood and adolescence Cavilli-Sforza, L. (1982). Theory and observation in cultural trans-
(pg. 297–309). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. mission. Science, 218, 19–27.
Boyatzis, C. J., & Janicki, D. (2003). Parent-child communication Colby, A., & Damon, W. (1992). Some do care: Contemporary lives of
about religion: A survey and diary assessment of unilateral trans- moral commitment. New York: Free Press.
mission and bi-directional reciprocity. Review of Religious Re-
Coles, R. (1990). The spiritual life of children. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
search, 44, 252–270.
Condorcet, J. A. (1976). Entwurf einer historischen Darstellung der
Boyer, P., & Walker, S. (2000). Intuitive ontology and cultural input
Fortschritte des menschlichen Geistes. Frankfurt, Germany:
in the acquisition of religious concepts. In K. Rosengren, C. John-
Suhrkamp. (Original work published 1789)
son, & P. Harris (Eds.), Imagining the impossible (pp. 130–156).
New York: Cambridge University Press. Cronon, W. (Ed.). (1997). John Muir: Nature writings. New York:
Brickerhoff, M. B., & MacKie, M. (1986). The applicability of so- Penguin Putnam.
cial distance for religious research. Review of Religious Research, Croog, S., & Levine, S. (1972). Religious identity and response to se-
28, 151–167. rious illness: A report on heart patients. Social Science and Medi-
Brown, L. B. (1967). Egocentric thought in petitionary prayer: A cine, 6, 17–32.
cross-cultural study. Journal of Social Psychology, 68, Daecke, M., & Schnakenberg, J. (2000). Gottesglaube—ein Selek-
197–210. tionsvorteil? Religion in der Evolution. München, Germany: Kaiser.
Brown, L. B. (1987). The psychology of religious belief. London: Aca- Damon, W. (Ed.). (2002). Bringing in a new era in character educa-
demic Press. tion. Stanford, CA: Hoover Institution Press.
References 993

Daner, F. (1976). The American children of Krsna: A study of the Faulkner, K. W. (1993). Children’s understanding of death. In A.
Hare Krsna movement. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Armstrong-Daily & S. Z. Goltzer (Eds.), Hospice Care for Chil-
Darley, J. M., & Batson, C. D. (1973). From Jerusalem to Jericho: A dren (pp. 9–21). London: Oxford University Press.
study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behav- Feldman, D. H. (1980). Beyond universals in cognitive development.
ior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 100–108. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Deconchy, J.-P. (1967). Structure génétique de l’idée de Dieu chez des Feldman, D. H. (in press). Piaget’s stages: The unfinished symphony
Catholics Français. Brussels, Belgium: Lumen Vitae Press. of cognitive development. New Ideas in Psychology.
Denz, H. (2002). Die europäische Seele: Leben und Glauben in Eu- Feldman, J. A. (in press). From molecules to metaphor: A neural the-
ropa. Wien: Czernin Verlag. ory of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fernhout, J. W. (1986). Where is faith? Searching for the core of the
Dick, A. (1981). Drei transkulturelle Erhebungen des religiösen
cube. In C. Dykstra & S. Parks (Eds.), Faith development and
Urteils: Eine Pilotstudie. Unveröffentlichte Lizentiatsarbeit,
Fowler (pp. 65–89). Birmingham, AL: Religious Education Press.
Freiburg, Switzerland: CH.
Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (1997). Dynamic development of psy-
Di Loreto, O., & Oser, F. (1996). Entwicklung des religiösen
chological structures in action and thought. In W. Damon (Editor-
Urteils und religiöser Selbstwirksamkeitsüberzeugung: Eine in-Chief ) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child
Längsschnittstudie. In F. Oser & K. H. Reich (Eds.), Eingebettet psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th
ins Menschsein: Beispiel Religion (pp. 69–87). Lengerich, Ger- ed., pp. 467–561). New York: Wiley.
many: Papst.
Fischer, K. W., & Rose, S. P. (1999). Rulers, models, and nonlinear
Dittmann-Kohli, F. (1990). Sinngebung im Alter. In P. Mayring & W. dynamics: Measurement and method in developmental research.
Saup (Eds.), Entwicklungsprozesse im Alter (pp. 145–166). In G. Savelsbergh, H. van der Maas, & P. van Geert (Eds.), Non-
Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer. linear developmental processes (pp. 197–212). Amsterdam, the
Donnelly, T., Matsuba, M. K., Hart, D., & Atkins, R. (2005). The re- Netherlands: Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences.
lationship between spiritual development and civic development. Flammer, A. (1994). Mit Risiko und Ungewissheit leben: Zur psychol-
In E. C. Roehlkepartain, P. E. King, L. M. Wagener, & P. L. Ben- ogischen Funktionalität und Religiosität in der Entwicklung. In G.
son (Eds.), Handbook of spiritual development in childhood and Klosinski (Ed.), Religion als Chance oder Risiko (pp. 20–34).
adolescence (pp. 239–251). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Bern, Switzerland: Huber.
Dowling, E., Gestsdottir, S., Anderson, P., von Eye, A., Almerigi, J., Flavell, J. H. (1972). An analysis of cognitive-developmental se-
& Lerner, R. (2004). Structural relations among spirituality, reli- quences. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 86, 279–350.
giosity, and thriving in adolescence. Applied Developmental Sci- Fowler, J. (1980). Faith and structuring of meaning. Morristown, NJ:
ence, 8 (1), 7–16. Silver Burdett.
Dulles, A. (1994). Models of revelation. Maryknoll, NY: Orbis Books. Fowler, J. (1981). Stages of faith: The psychology of human develop-
Durkheim, E. (1995). Elementary forms of the religious life (Karen ment and the quest for meaning. New York: Harper & Row.
E. Fields, Trans.). New York: Free Press. (Original work pub- Francis, L. (1987a ). Denominational schools and pupils’ attitudes
lished 1915) towards Christianity. British Educational Research Journal,
12, 145–152.
Elkind, D. (1961). The child’s conception of his religious denomina-
tion: Vol. 1. The Jewish child. Journal of Genetic Psychology, Francis, L. (1987b). The decline in attitudes towards religion among
99, 209–225. 8- to 15-year-olds. Educational Studies, 13, 125–134.

Elkind, D. (1962). The child’s conception of his religious denomina- Freeman, N. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children:
Analysis of spatial skills and drawing processes. London: Aca-
tion: Vol. 2. The Catholic child. Journal of Genetic Psychology,
demic Press.
101, 185–193.
Freud, S. (1907). Obsessive actions and religious practices. In J.
Elkind, D. (1963). The child’s conception of his religious denomina-
Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete works of Sig-
tion: Vol. 3. The Protestant child. Journal of Genetic Psychology,
mund Freud (pp. 432–451). London: Hogarth Press and the Insti-
103, 291–304.
tute of Psychoanalysis.
Elkind, D. (1964). Piaget’s semiclinical interview and the Study of Freud, S. (1923a). The ego and the id. Standard Edition, 19, 3–66.
the Spontaneous Religion. Journal for the Scientific Study of Reli-
Freud, S. (1923b). A seventeenth-century demonological neurosis.
gion, 4, 40–46.
Standard Edition, 19, 67–105.
Ellis, A. (1980). Psychotherapy and atheistic values: A response to
Freud, S. (1961). The future of an illusion. In J. Strachey (Ed.),
A. E. Bergin’s “Psychotherapy and religion values.” Journal of
The standard edition of the complete works of Sigmund Freud
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 639–653. (Vol. 21, pp. 1–56). London: Hogarth Press and the Institute of
Ellison, C. G., & Sherkat, D. E. (1993). Obedience and autonomy: Re- Psychoanalysis.
ligion and parental values reconsidered. Journal for the Scientific Frielingsdorf, K. (1992). Dämonische Gottesbilder: Ihre Entste-
Study of Religion, 32, 313–329. hung, Entlarvung und Überwindung. Mainz, Germany:
Erickson, J. A. (1992). Adolescent religious development and commit- Grünewald.
ment: A structural equation model of the role of family, peer Gandhi, M. K. (1993). Gandhi: An autobiography—The story of my
group, and educational inf luences. Journal for the Scientific Study experiments with truth. Boston: Beacon Press.
of Religion, 31(2), 131–152. Gartner, J. (1996). Religious commitment, mental health, and
Erikson, E. (1958). Young man Luther: A study in psychoanalysis and prosocial behavior: A review of the empirical literature. In
history. New York: Norton. E. Shafranske (Ed.), Religion and the clinical practice of
994 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

psychology (pp. 301–319). Washington, DC: American Psycho- Harris, P. L. (2003). Les dieux, les ancêtres et les enfants [Gods, an-
logical Association. cestors and children]. Terrain, 40, 81–98.
Gesell, A. (1977). The child from five to ten. New York: Norton. Hart, T. (2005). Spiritual experiences and capacities of children and
Gibson, H. (1990). The relationship between social class and attitude youth. In P. L. Benson, P. E. King, L. M. Wagener, & E. C.
towards Christianity among 14- and 15-year old adolescents. Per- Roehlkepartain (Eds.), The handbook of spiritual development in
sonality and Individual Dif ferences, 11, 631–635. childhood and adolescence (pp. 163–178). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gnos, C. (2003). Bewältigungsverhalten, religiöses Urteil und Hartshorne, H., & May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the nature of charac-
Werthaltungen. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of ter: Vol. 1. Studies in deceit. New York: Macmillan.
Freiburg, Educational Department, Freiburg, Switzerland. Hay, D., & Nye, R. (1998). The spirit of the child. London: Fount.
Godin, A. (1968a). From cry to word: Contributions toward a psy- Helsper, W. (1992). Okkultismus, die neue Jugendreligion? Die Sym-
chology of religion. Brussels, Belgium: Lumen Vitae Press. bolik des Todes und des Bösen in der Jugendkultur. Opladen, Ger-
Godin, A. (1968b). Genetic development and the symbolic function: many: Leske & Budrich.
Meaning and limits of the work of Goldman. Religious Education, Herzbrun, M. (1993). Father-adolescent religious consensus in the
63, 439–445. Jewish community: A preliminary report. Journal for the Scien-
Goethe, J. W. (1974). Faust: Erster Teil. Frankfurt am Main, Ger- tific Study of Religion, 32, 163–166.
many: Insel. Hill, P. C. (2000). Conceptualizing religion and spirituality: Points of
Goldfried, J., & Miner, M. (2002, December). Quest religion and the commonality, points of departure. Journal for the Theory of Social
problem of limited compassion. Journal for the Scientific Study of Behavior, 30, 51–77.
Religion, 41, 685–695. Hitschmann, E. (1947). New varieties of religious experience: From
Goldman, R. (1964). Religious thinking from childhood to adoles- William James to Sigmund Freud. In G. Roheim (Ed.), Psycho-
cence. New York: Seabury. analysis and the social sciences (pp. 195–233). New York: Inter-
Gottlieb, A. (2005). Non-western approaches to spiritual de- national Universities Press.
velopment among infants and young children. In P. L. Benson, Hoge, D., & Petrillo, D. (1978). Development of religious thinking in
P. E. King, L. M. Wagener, & E. C. Roehlkepartain (Ed.), The adolescence. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 17,
handbook of spiritual development in childhood and adoles- 139–154.
cence (pp. 150–162). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hoge, D., & Petrillo, D. (1982). Transmission of religious and social
Gould, S. J. (2003a). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Har- values from parents to teenage children. Journal of Marriage and
vard University Press. the Family, 44, 569–580.
Gould, S. J. (2003b). The hedgehog, the fox, and the magister’s pox: Hood, R. W. (1992). Sin and guilt in faith traditions: Issues of self-
Mending the gap between science and the humanities. New York: esteem. In J. Schumaker (Ed.), Religion and Mental Health
Harmony Books. (pp. 110–121). New York: Oxford University Press.
Grangvist, P. (1998). Religiousness and perceived childhood attach- Hood, R. (Ed.). (1995). Handbook of religious experience. Birming-
ment: On the question of compensation or correspondence. Jour- ham, AL: Religious Education Press.
nal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 37, 350–367. Hood, R., Spilka, B., Hunsberger, B., & Gorsuch, R. (1996). The
Greve, W., & Roos, J. (1996). Der Untergang des Ödipuskomplexes: psychology of religion: An empirical approach. New York: Guil-
Argumente gegen einen Mythos. Bern, Switzerland: Huber. ford Press.
Gut, U. (1984). Analyse der vorraussetzungen der stufen des moralis- Hunsberger, B. (1995). Religion and prejudice: The role of religious
chen und des reliosen urteils. Zurich, Switzerland: Benzinger. fundamentalism, quest, and right-wing authoritarianism. Journal
Hall, G. S. (1900). The religious content of the child-mind. In I. N. M. of Social Issues, 51, 113–129.
Butler (Ed.), Principles of religious education (pp. 161–189). New Hunsberger, B., & Brown, L. B. (1984). Religious socialization, apos-
York: Longmans, Green. tasy, and the impact of family background. Journal for the Scien-
Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence: Its psychology and its relation to tific Study of Religion, 23, 239–251.
physiology, anthropology, sociology, crime, religion, and educa- Hunsberger, L. (1980). A re-examination of the antecedents of apos-
tion. Boston: Appleton. tasy. Review of Religious Research, 25, 21–38.
Hall, G. S. (1908). A glance at the phyletic background of genetic Huntington, S. P. (1996). The clash of civilsations. New York: Simon
psychology. American Journal of Psychology, 19, 149–212. & Schuster.
Hall, G. S. (1923). Life and confessions of a psychologist. New York: Huntington, S. P. (1998). Kampf der Kulturen: Die Neugestaltung der
D. Appleton. Weltpolitik im 21—Jahrhundert. München, Germany: Siedler.
Halperin, D. (Ed.). (1983). Psychodynamic perspectives on religion, Huxel, K. (2000). Die empirische Psychologie des Glaubens: His-
sect, and cult. Littleton, MA: PSG. torische und systematische Studien zu den Pionieren der Religion-
Hanisch, H. (1996). Die zeichnerische Entwicklung des Gottesbildes spsychologie. Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.
bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. Stuttgart, Germany: Calwer. Hyde, K. (1965). Religious learning in adolescence. Edinburgh, Scot-
Harms, E. (1944). The development of religious experience in chil- land: Oliver & Boyd.
dren. American Journal of Sociology, 50, 112–122. Hyde, K. (1990). Religion in childhood and adolescence. Birmingham,
Harris, P. L. (2000). On not falling down to earth: Children’s AL: Religious Education Press.
metaphysical questions. In K. S. Rosengren, C. N. Johnson, & Idler, E. (1994). Cohesiveness and coherence: Religion and the health
P. L. Harris (Eds.), Imagining the impossible: Magical, scien- of the elderly. New York: Taylor & Frances.
tific, and religious thinking in children (pp. 157–178). Cam- James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience. New York:
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Longmans, Green.
References 995

James, W. (1961). The principles of psychology (Rev. ed.). New York: Koenig, H., George, L., & Siegler, I. (1988). The use of religion and
Holt. (Original work published 1892) other emotion-regulating coping strategies among older adults.
Jarvis, G., & Northcott, H. (1987). Religion and differences in mor- Gerontologist, 28, 303–310.
bidity and mortality. Social Science and Medicine, 25, 813–824. Kohlberg, L. (1981). Essays on moral development: Vol. 1. The philos-
Johnson, C. N., & Boyatzis, C. J. (2005). Cognitive-cultural foun- ophy of moral development. San Francisco: Harper & Row.
dations of spiritual development. In E. C. Roehlkepartain, Kohlberg, L., & Power, C. (1981). Moral development, religious
P. E. King, L. M. Wagener, & P. L. Benson (Eds.), The hand- thinking, and the question of a seventh stage. Zygon, 16, 203–258.
book of spiritual development in childhood and adolescence Kroh, O. (1965). Entwicklungspsychologie des Grundschulkindes.
(pp. 211–223). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.
Jörns, K. P. (1997). Die neuen Gesichter Gottes: Was die Menschen Kuner, W. (1981, September). Jugendsekten: Ein Sammelbecken für
heute wirklich glauben. München, Germany: Beck. Verrückte? Psychologie Heute, 8, 53–61.
Jung, C. G. (1968). Concerning the archetypes, with special refer- Kushner, H. (1981). When bad things happen to good people. New
ence to the anima concept. In M. F. H. Read & G. Adler (Eds.), York: Avon Books.
The collected works of C. G. Jung (2nd ed., Vol. 9, pp. 54–72).
Kwilecki, S. (1999). Becoming religious. Cranbury, NJ: Associated
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
University Press.
Kager, A. (1995). Die Stufen des religiösen Urteils in Ordensgemein-
Lademann-Priemer, G. (1998). Warum faszinieren Sekten? Psychologis-
schaften. Eine Pilotstudie: Unveröffentliche Magisterarbeit an
che Aspekte des Relgionsmissbrauchs. München, Germany: Claudius.
der Universität Wien.
Kaplan, B. (1983a). Genetic-dramatism: Old wine in new bottles. In Lambert, W., Triandis, L., & Wolf, M. (1959). Some correlates of be-
liefs in malevolence and benevolence of supernatural beings: A
S. Wapner & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Toward a holistic developmental
cross-societal study. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
psychology (pp. 53–74). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
58, 162–169.
Kaplan, B. (1983b). Ref lections on culture and personality from the
perspective of genetic-dramatism. In S. Wapner & B. Kaplan Langdon, A. (1969). A critical examination of Dr. Goldman’s re-
(Eds.), Toward holistic developmental psychology (pp. 95–110). search study on religious thinking from childhood to adolescence.
Journal of Christian Education, 12, 37–63.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kimball, C. (2002). When religion becomes evil. San Francisco: Larson, D. B., Thielman, S. B., Greenwold, M. A., Lyons, J. S., Post,
Harper & Row. S. G., Sherrill, K. A., et al. (1993). Religious content in the DSM-
III-R glossary of technical terms. American Journal of Psychiatry,
King, P. E. (2003). Religion and identity: The role of ideological, social,
150(12), 1884–1885.
and spiritual contexts. Applied Developmental Science, 7, 196–203.
Lerner, R. (2002). Concepts and theories of human development (3rd
King, P. E., & Boyatzis, C. (2004). Exploring adolescent spiritual and
ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
religious development: Current and future theoretical and empiri-
cal perspectives. Applied Developmental Science, 8(1), 2–6. Lerner, R., Dowling, E., & Anderson, P. (2003). Positive youth devel-
opment: Thriving as the basis of personhood and civil society. Ap-
King, P. E., & Furrow, J. L. (2004). Religion as a resource for positive
plied Developmental Science, 7(3), 172–180.
youth development: Religion, social capital, and moral outcomes.
Developmental Psychology, 40(5), 703–713. Leuba, J. (1912). A psychological study of religion. New York: Macmillan.
Kingdom, D., Siddle, R., & Rathod, S. (2001). Spirituality, psychosis, Levin, J. S. (1997). Religious research in gerontology, 1980–1994: A
and the development of “normalising rationales.” In I. Clarke systematic review. Journal of Religious Gerontology, 10, 3–31.
(Ed.), Psychosis and spirituality (pp. 177–198). London: Whurr. Lindenthal, J. J., Myers, J. K., Pepper, M. P., & Stern, M. S. (1970).
Kirkpatrick, L. (1995). Attachment theory and religious experience. Mental status and religious behavior. Journal for the Scientific
In I. R. W. Hood (Ed.), Handbook of religious experience Study of Religion, 9(2), 143–149.
(pp. 446–475). Birmingham, AL: Religious Education Press. Long, D., Elkind, D., & Spilka, B. (1967). The child’s conception of
prayer. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 6, 101–109.
Kirkpatrick, L. (1997). A longitudinal study of changes in religious
belief and behavior as a function of individual differences in Maslow, A. H. (1971). The farther reaches of human behavior. New
adult attachment style. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, York: Viking.
36, 207–217. May, A. (1997). Psychopathology and religion in the era of “enlight-
ened science”: A case report. European Journal of Psychiatry,
Kirkpatrick, L., & Shaver, P. (1990). Attachment theory and religion:
11(1), 14–20.
Childhood attachments, religious beliefs, and conversion. Journal
for the Scientific Study of Religion, 29, 315–334. McFadden, S. H. (1996). Religion and spirituality. In J. E. Birren
(Ed.), Encyclopedia of Gerontology (Vol. 2, pp. 387–397). San
Klaghofer, R., & Oser, F. (1987). Dimensionen und Erfassung des re-
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
ligisen Familienklimas. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 15, 190–206.
Meadow, M. J. (1992). Personality maturity and Teresa’s interior cas-
Klepsch, M., & Logie, L. (1982). Children draw and tell. New York: tle. Pastoral Psychology, 40(5), 293–302.
Brunner/ Mazel.
Meadow, M. J., & Kahoe, R. D. (1984). Psychology of religion: Reli-
Klosinski, G. (1996). Psychokulte: Was Sekten für Jugendliche so at- gion in individual lives. New York: Harper & Row.
traktiv macht. München, Germany: Beck. Meissner, W. W. (1984). Psychoanalysis and religious experience.
Koenig, H. (1992). Religion and mental health in later life. In J. F. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Schumaker (Ed.), Religion and mental health (pp. 155–174). New Miller, W. R., & Martin, J. E. (1988). Spirituality and behavioral psy-
York: Oxford University Press. chology: Toward integration. In W. R. Miller & J. E. Martin (Eds.),
Koenig, H. G. (1994). Aging and God: Spiritual pathways to mental Behavior therapy and religion: Integrating spiritual and behavioral
health in midlife and later years. New York: Haworth Pastoral Press. approaches to change (pp. 13–23). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
996 Religious and Spiritual Development throughout the Life Span

Murphy, R. J. L. (1978). A new approach to the Study of the De- Pascual-Leone, J. (1983). Growing into human maturity: Toward a
velopment of Religious Thinking in Children. Educational Stud- metasubjective theory of adulthood stages. In P. B. Baltes &
ies, 4, 19–22. O. G. J. Brim (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol.5,
Niggli, A. (1988). Familie und religiöse Erziehung in unserer Zeit: pp. 117–156). New York: Academic Press.
Eine empirische Studie über elterliche Erziehungspraktiken und Pfeiffer, J. E. (1992). The psychological framing of cults: Schematic
religiöse Merkmale bei Erzogenen. Frankfurt, Germany: Lang. representations and cult evaluations. Journal of Applied Social
Nipkow, K. E., Schweitzer, F., & Fowler, J. W. (1988). Glaubensen- Psychology, 22, 531–544.
twicklung und Erziehung. Gütersloh, Germany: Gerd Mohn. Pfister, O. (1911). Hysterie und Mystik bei Margaretha Ebner
Nobiling, E. (1929). Der Gottesgedanke bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. (1291–1351). Zentralblatt für Psychoanalyse und Psychotherapie,
In W. Gruehn (Ed.), Atrchiv für Religionspsychologie und Seelen- 1, 468–485.
führung (Vol. 4, pp. 43–216). Leipzig, Germany: Eduard Pfeiffer. Pfister, O. (1925). Die Frömmigkeit des Grafen Ludwig von Zinzen-
Nunn, C. Z. (1964). Child-control through a “coalition with God.” dorf: Eine psychoanalytische Studie. Zweite verbesserte Auf lage,
Child Development, 34, 417–432. Leipzig, Germany und Wien: Franz Deuticke.
Nye, R. (1999). Relational consciousness and the spiritual lives of Piaget, J. (1928). Immanence et transcendence. In J. Piaget & J. de la
children: Convergence with children’s theory of mind. In H. Harpe (Eds.), Deux types d’attitude religieuse: Immanance et tran-
Reich, F. K. Oser, & W. G. Scarlett (Eds.), Psychological studies scendence (pp. 5–40). Editions de L’Association Chretienne dE’-
on spiritual and religious development (pp. 57–82). Lengerich, tudiants de Suisse Romande, Depot, Geneva, Switzerland: Labor.
Germany: Pabst Science.
Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. New York:
Nye, W. C., & Carlson, J. S. (1984). The development of the concept
Harcourt.
of God in children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 145, 137–142.
Pitts, V. P. (1977). Drawing pictures of God. Learning for Living,
Oerter, R. (1980). Moderne Entwicklungspsychologie. Donauwörth, Ger-
16, 123–131.
many: Auer.
Ohlig, K. H. (2002). Religion in der Geschichte der Menschheit: Die Plante, T. G., & Bovccaccini, M. T. (1997). The Santa Clara strength
Entwicklung des religiösen Bewusstseins. Darmstadt, Germany: of religious faith questionnaire. Pastoral Psychology, 45, 375–387.
Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft. Plante, T. G., & Sherman, A. (Eds.). (2001). Faith and health: Psy-
Oman, D., Kurata, J. H., Strawbridge, W. J., Cohen, R. D. (2002). chological perspectives. New York: Guilford Press.
Religious attendance and cause of death over 31 years. Interna- Pnevmatikos, D. (2002). Conceptual changes in religious concepts of
tional Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 32, 69–89. elementary schoolchildren: The case of the house where God
Oser, F. (1988a). Genese und Logik der Entwicklung des religiösen lives. Educational Psychology, 22, 93–112.
Bewusstseins: Eine Entgegnung auf Kritiken. In K. E. Nipkow, Pollner, M. (1989). Divine relations, social relations, and well being.
F. Schweitzer, & J. W. Fowler (Eds.), Glaubensentwicklung und Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, 30, 92–104.
Erziehung (pp. 48–88). Gütersloh, Germany: Güterloher Ver-
Potvin, R. H., & Sloane, D. M. (1985). Parental control, age, and reli-
lagshaus Gerd Mohn.
gious practice. Review of Religious Research, 27, 3–14.
Oser, F. (1988b). Wieviel Religion braucht der Mensch? Erziehung
Powell, L. H., Shahabi, L., & Thoresen, C. E. (2003). Religion and
und Entwicklung zur religiösen Autonomie. Gütersloh, Ger-
spirituality: Linkages to physical health. American Psycholo-
many: Güterloher Verlagshaus Gerd Mohn.
gist, 58, 36–52.
Oser, F. (1991). The development of religious judgment. In F. Oser &
W. G. Scarlett (Eds.), Religious development in childhood and Pratt, J. B. (1910). An empirical study of prayer. American Journal of
adolescence (pp. 5–26). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Religious Psychology and Education, 4, 48–67.
Oser, F. (2002). Mystische und psychologische Stufen der religiösen Prell, R.-E. (1995). Ref lections on the social science of American
Entwicklung: Inkompatibel, analog, ergänzungsbedürftig? Ein Jews and its implications for Jewish education. In A. Aron, S.
spekulativer Vergleich. In W. Simon (Ed.), Meditatio (pp. 67–80). Lee, & S. Rossel (Eds.), A congregation of learners: Transform-
FS für Günter Stachel, Münster, Germany: Lit-Verlag. ing the synagogue into a learning community (pp. 139–153). New
Oser, F., & Gmünder, P. (1991). Religious judgement: A developmental York: URJ Press.
approach. Birmingham, AL: Religious Education Press. Pruyser, P. (1991). Forms and functions of the imagination in reli-
Osmer, R. R., & Schweitzer, F. (2003). Religious education between gion. In H. N. Maloney & B. Spilka (Eds.), Religion in psychody-
modernization and globalization: New perspectives on the United namic perspective (p. 179). New York: Oxford University Press.
States and Germany. Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans. Ramachers, G. (1994). Entwicklung und Bedingungen von
Ozarak, E. (1989). Social and cognitive inf luences on the develop- Todeskonzepten bei Kindern. Frankfurt-am-Main, Germany:
ment of religious beliefs and commitment in adolescence. Journal Peter Lang.
for the Scientific Study of Religion, 28, 448–463. Ratcliff, D. (1985). The development of children’s religious concepts:
Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religious coping: Theory, Research review. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 1, 35–43.
research, practice. New York: Guilford Press. Regnerus, M., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2003). Staying on track in school:
Pargament, K., & Brant, C. (1998). Religion and coping. In H. Koenig Religious inf luences in high- and low-risk settings. Journal for the
(Ed.), Handbook of religion and mental health (pp. 112–129). Scientific Study of Religion, 42(4), 633–649.
Boston: Academic Press. Reich, H. (1993). Integrating different theories: The case of religious
Pargament, K. I., Ensing, D. S., & Falgout, K. (1990). God help me: development. Journal of Empirical Theology, 6(1), 39–49.
Religious coping effects as predictors of the outcomes of signifi- (Reprinted from The psychology of religion: Theoretical ap-
cant negative life events. American Journal of Community and proaches, pp. 105–113, by B. Spilka & D. M. McIntosh, Eds.,
Psychology, 18, 793–824. 1997, Boulder, CO: Westview)
References 997

Reich, H. (1997). Do we need a theory for the religious development opment in childhood and adolescence (pp. 63–76). San Francisco:
of women. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 7, Jossey-Bass.
67–86. Schibilsky, M. (1976). Religiöse Erfahrung und Interaktion: Die
Reich, H. (2004, November). The role of cognition in religious devel- Lebenswelt Jugendlicher Randgruppen. Stuttgart, Germany:
opment: The contribution of Relational and Contextual Reasoning Kohlhammer.
(RCR) to religious development. Unpublished doctoral disserta- Schjelderup, H., & Schjelderup, K. (1932). Über drei Haupttypen der
tion, Universität Utrecht, Theologische Fakultät. religiösen Erlebnisformen und ihre psychologische Grundlage.
Reich, H. (2005). Jean Piaget’s views on religion. Unpublished manu- Berlin, Germany: W. de Gruyter.
script, University of Fribourg, Switzerland. Schmidtchen, G. (1987). Sekten und Psychokultur: Reichweite und At-
Reich, H., Oser, F. O., & Scarlett, W. G. (1999). Spiritual and reli- traktivität von Jugendreligionen in der Bundesrepublik Deutsch-
gious development: Transcendence and transformations of the land. Freiburg, Germany: Herder.
self. In K. H. Reich, F. O. Oser, & W. G. Scarlett (Eds.), Psycho- Schmitz, E., & Friebe, S. (Eds.). (1992). Die neuen Jugendreligionen:
logical Studies on Spiritual and Religious Development: Vol. 2. Öf fentliche Akzeptanz und Konversionsmotive. Göttingen, Ger-
Being human—The case of religion. Scottsdale, AZ: Pabst Science. many: Hogrefe.
Reimer, K. S., & Furrow, J. L. (2003). A qualitative exploration of re- Schroedter, T. (1908). Erotogenese der Religion. Zeitschrift für Religion-
lational consciousness in Christian children. Pasadena, CA: Fuller spsychologie: Grenzfragen der Theologie und Medizin, 1, 445–455.
Theological Seminary, Graduate School of Psychology.
Schwartz, A. (2002). Transmitting moral wisdom in an age of the au-
Rest, J. R. (1983). Morality. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & J. Flavell tonomous self. In W. Damon (Ed.), Bringing in a new era in charac-
& E. Markman (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. ter education (pp. 1–22). Stanford, CA: Hoover Institution Press.
Cognitive development (4th ed., pp. 556–629). New York: Wiley.
Schweitzer, F. (1992). Die Religion des Kindes: Zur Prob-
Richardson, J., Stewart, M., & Simmonds, R. (1979). Organized mira- lemgeschichte einer religionspädagogischen Grundfrage.
cles. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books. Gütersloh, Germany: Gerd Mohn.
Richardson, J. T. (1995). Clinical and personality assessment of par- Scott, S., Garver, S., Richards, J., & Hathaway, W. (2003). Religious
ticipants in new religions. The International Journal for the Psy- issues in diagnosis: The V-Code and beyond. Mental Health, Reli-
chology of Religion, 5, 145–170. gion and Culture, 6(2), 160–173.
Richardson, J. T., van der Lans, J., & Derks, F. (1986). Leaving and Shafranske, E. P. (1995). Freudian theory and religious
labeling: Voluntary and coerced disaffiliation from new religion experience. In R. W. Hood Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of religious ex-
movements. Social Movement, Conf lict, and Change, 8, 385–393. perience (pp. 200–230). Birmingham, AL: Religious Education
Rizzuto, A.-M. (1979). The birth of the living God: A psychoanalytic Press.
study. Chicago: Chicago University Press. Shaver, P., Leneuaer, M., & Sadd, M. (1980). Religousness, conver-
Roehlkepartain, E. C. (1995). Youth development in congregations: An sion, and subjective well-being: The “ healthy-minded” religion
exploration of the potential and barriers. Minneapolis, MN: of modern American women. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Search Institute. 137, 1563–1568.
Roehlkepartain, E. C. (2005). Congregations: Unexamined crucibles Silke, A. (Ed.). (2003). Terrorists, victims, and society: Psychological
for spiritual development. In E. C. Roehlkepartain, P. E. King, perspectives on terrorism and its consequences. Southern Gate, En-
L. M. Wagener, & P. L. Benson (Eds.), The handbook of spiritual gland: Wiley.
development in childhood and adolescence (pp. 324–336). Thou- Silverstein, S. M. (1988). A study of religious conversion in North
sand Oaks, CA: Sage. America. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs,
Rollett, B. (1992). Religiöse Entwicklung und Interesse an Jugendsek- 114, 261–305.
ten. Forschungsbericht Universität Wien: Abteilung für Entwick- Slaughter, V., Jaakola, R., & Carey, S. (1999). Constructing a co-
lungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie. herent theory: Children’s biological understanding of life and
Rollett, B. (Ed.). (1996). Religiose Entwicklung im Jugendalter. Göt- death. In M. Siegal & C. Peterson (Eds.), Children’s understand-
tingen, Germany: Hogrefe. ing of biology, health, and ethics (pp. 71–96). Cambridge: Cam-
Rollett, B., & Kager, A. (Eds.). (1999). Post-modern religiousness: A bridge University Press.
prerogative of the “new religion”? Religious emotions and reli- Slee, N. (1986a). Goldman yet again: An overview and critique of
gious development—Finding of a pilot study. Lengerich, Ger- his contribution to research. British Journal of Religious Educa-
many: Papst. tion, 8, 33–84.
Rosenberg, R. (1989). Die Entwicklung von Gebetskonzepten. In Slee, N. (1986b). A note on Goldman’s methods of data analysis with
A. Bucher & K. H. Reich (Ed.), Entwicklung von Religiosität special reference to scalogram analysis. British Journal of Reli-
(pp. 175–198). Fribourg, Germany: Universitätsverlag. gious Education, 8, 168–175.
Rousseau, J. J. (1979). Emile: Or, On Education. New York: Basic Smith, C. (2003). Religious participation and network closure among
Books. (Original work published 1762) American adolescents. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,
Salzman, L. (1953). The psychology of religious and ideological con- 42(2), 259–267.
version. Psychiatry, 16, 177–187. Smith, W. C. (1998a). Faith and belief: The dif ference between them.
Scarlett, W. G. (2003 July). Healthy and pathological spirituality. Oxford, England: Oneworld Publications.
Paper presented at the Association for Moral Education, Smith, W. C. (1998b). Patterns of faith around the world. Boston:
Krakow, Poland. Oneworld Publications.
Scarlett, W. G., & Perriello, L. (1991). The development of prayer in Socha, P. (1999). The existential human situation: Spirituality as a
adolescence. In F. Oser & W. G. Scarlett (Eds.), Religious devel- way of coping. In H. K. Reich, F. Oser, & W. G. Scarlett (Eds.),
998 References

Psychological studies on spiritual and religious development: Vol. Walker, L., & Reimer, K. S. (2005). The relationship between moral
2. Being human—The case of religion. Langerich, Germany: Pabst and spiritual development. In E. C. Roehlkepartain, P. E. King, L.
Science Publishers. Wagener, & P. Benson (Eds.), The handbook of spiritual develop-
Spencer, A. E. (1971). The future of Catholic education in England ment in childhood and adolescence (pp. 224–238). Thousand
and Wales. London: Catholic Renewal Movement. Oaks, CA: Sage.
Spilka, B., & McIntosh, D. N. (1995). Attribution theory and reli- Warwick, L. (1995). Feminist Wicca: Paths to empowerment.
gious experience. In R. W. Hood Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of religious Women and Therapy, 16(2/3), 121–133.
experience: Theory and practice (pp. 421–445). Birmingham, AL: Wenger Jindra, I. (2004). Konversion und Stufentransformation: Ein
Religious Education Press. kompliziertes Verhältnis. Münster, Germany: Waxmann.
Stark, R. A. (1965). A taxonomy of religious experience. Journal for Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York:
the Scientific Study of Religion, 5, 97–116. Alfred A. Knopf.
Steele, S. (1994). The multistage paradigm and the spiritual path of Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representa-
John of the cross. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 26, 55–80. tion and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young chil-
Stone, M. (2000). Psychopathology: Biological and psychological cor- dren’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.
relates. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, Wink, P. D. M. (2002). Spiritual development across the adult life
28(2), 203–235. course: Findings from a longitudinal study. Journal of Adult De-
Streib, H. (1999). Off-road religion? A narrative approach to fun- velopment, 9(1), 79–94.
damentalist and occult orientations. Journal of Adolescence, Wittkowski, J. (1990). Psychologie des Todes. Darmstadt, Germany:
22(2), 255–267. Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft.
Streib, H. (2001, September). Faith development research at 20 years. Woolever, C., & Bruce, D. (2004). Beyond the ordinary: 10 strengths of
Paper presented at the 14th Conference of the International As- U.S. congregations. Louisville, KY: Westminster/John Knox Press.
sociation for the Psychology of Religion, Soesterberg, The Woolley, J. (2000). The development of beliefs about direct mental-
Netherlands. physical causality on imagination, magic, and religion. In K. S.
Strommen, M. P., & Hardel, R. A. (2000). Passing on the faith: A Rosengren, C. N. Johnson, & P. L. Harris (Eds.), Imagining the
radical new model for youth and family. Winona, MN: Saint impossible: Magical, scientific, and religious thinking in chil-
Mary’s Press. dren (pp. 99–129). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Tamminnen, K. (1976). Research concerning the development of reli- Press.
gious thinking in Finnish students. Character potential, 7, 206–219. Wulff, D. M. (1991). Psychology of religion: Classic and contempo-
Tamminnen, K. (1991). Religious development in childhood and youth. rary views. New York: Wiley.
Helsinki, Finland: Suomalainen Tiedeakatemia. Wulff, D. M. (1993). On the origins and goals of religious develop-
Tamminnen, K. (1994). Comparing Oser’s and Fowler’s developmen- ment. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion,
tal stages. Journal of Empirical Theology, 7, 75–112. 3(3), 181–186.
Taylor, C. (2002). Varieties of religion today: William James revisted. Wunderle, G. (1932). Glaube und Glaubenszweifel moderner Jugend:
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Das ja und nein katholischer Schüler und Schülerinnen zur
Glaubensdarbietung. Düsseldorf, Germany: Pädagogischer Verlag.
Thouless, R. H., & Brown, L. B. (1964). Petitionary prayer: Belief in
its appropriateness and causal efficacy among adolescent girls. Youniss, J., McLellan, J. A., Su, Y., & Yates, M. (1999). Religion,
Lumen Vitae, 23, 123–136. community service, and identity in American youth. Journal of
Adolescence, 22, 243–253.
Thun, T. (1959) Die Religion des Kindes: Eine Untersuchung nach
Klassengesprächen mit katholischen und evangelischen Kindern in Yust, K.-M. (2003, June). Challenges of faith formation in children’s
der Grundschule. Stuttgart, Germany: Ernst Klett. ministries. Paper presented at the Children’s Spirituality Confer-
ence, Christian Perspectives, Chicago, IL.
Thurman, R. (1991). Wisdom and compassion: The scared art of
Tibet. New York: Thames and Hundson. Zeligs, D. F. (1974). Psychoanalysis and the Bible: A study in depth of
seven leaders. New York: Bloch Publishing.
Ullman, C. (1982). Cognitive and emotional antecedents of reli-
gious conversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Zenklusen, B. (2003). Witwen—Erlebter Verlust durch den Tod und
43, 183–192. religiöses Urteil: Eine Untersuchung über den Zusammenhang
zwischen den religiösen Stufen und dem erlebten Verlust des
Vidal, F. (1987). Jean Piaget and the liberal Protestant tradition. In
Ehepartners durch den Tod. Unpublished master’s thesis,
M. Ash & W. Woodward (Eds.), Psychology in twentieth-century
University of Freiburg, Educational Department, Freiburg,
thought and society (pp. 271–294). Cambridge University Press.
Switzerland.
Vogel, P. (1936). Ein Beitrag zur Religionspsychologie des Kindes.
Zinnbauer, B. J., Pargament, K., Reye, B., Butter, M., Blavich, E.,
Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 36, 311–466.
Hipp, T., et al. (1997). Religiousness and spirituality: Unfuzzing
Voss, T. (1926). Die Entwicklung der religiösen Vorstellungen. the fuzzy. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 36(4),
Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 57, 1–86. 549–564.
Walker, L. (2003). Morality, religion, spirituality: The value of saint- Zondag, H. J., & Belzen, J. A. (1999). Between reduction of uncertainty
liness. Journal of Moral Education, 32(4). and ref lection: The range and dynamics of religious judgment. In-
Walker, L., & Pitts, R. (1998). Naturalistic conceptions of moral ma- ternational Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 9, 63–81.
turity. Developmental Psychology, 34, 403–419.
Zulehner, P., Hager, I., & Polak, R. (2001). Kehrt die Religion wieder:
Walker, L., Pitts, R., Hennig, K., & Matsuba, M. K. (1995). Reason- Religion im Leben der Menschen 1970–2000. Ostfildern:
ing about morality and real-life problems. In M. Killen & D. Hart Schwabenverlag.
(Eds.), Morality in everyday life: Developmental perspectives
(pp. 371–407). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Author Index

Abbott, A., 667 Allard, T. T., 283 Ames, R. T., 753


Abbott, R. D., 906, 925 Allen, G. E., 222 Amick, B. C., 683
Abbott, S., 276, 725 Allen, J. J. B., 628 Amorim, K., 202
Abel, L., 230 Allen, J. P., 919 Anastasi, A., 33, 43, 416
Abeles, R. P., 551 Allen, M. J., 183 Anderman, E., 503, 505, 506
Abelson, R. P., 325, 372, 522, 524, Allen, P., 178 Anders, T. F., 725
541, 542, 543 Allen, V. L., 408, 427 Andersen, C., 45, 72
Abraham, J. D., 591, 611, 644 Allen, W. R., 379, 835, 839, 857, Andersen, D. S., 980
Abramson, L. J., 547 868 Anderson, A. R., 532
Ackerman, P. L., 600, 605, 607 Allport, A., 537 Anderson, D. I., 384
Acredolo, L., 54, 280, 300 Allport, G., 59, 119, 409, 432, 433, Anderson, J. D., 880
Adams, G. R., 928 751, 813, 947, 989 Anderson, J. E., 125, 126, 130, 147
Adams, M. D., 7, 9, 223 Alm, M., 415 Anderson, J. R., 339, 340, 347, 606
Adlai-Gail, W. S., 481 Alm, P. O., 415 Anderson, N. H., 605
Adler, M. J., 490 Almasy, L., 233, 239 Anderson, P., 898, 904, 915, 918,
Adolfsson, R., 448 Almazan, E. P., 704 920, 948, 971
Adolph, K. E., 134, 246, 279 Almerigi, J., 12, 13, 895 Andersson, T., 404, 417, 445
Aebli, H., 522, 532 Alonso, J. M., 238 Andreas, J. B., 335
af Klinteberg, B., 415 Alpern, L., 377 Andresen, J., 982
Agarwal, G. C., 296 Alpert, N., 638 Andrews, D. W., 917
Aggen, S. H., 632 Alpert, R., 149 Angell, J. R., 414, 415, 416
Agostiani, H., 758 Alsaker, F. D., 412 Angleitner, A., 620, 623, 633
Aharonovich, E., 706 Altaha, M., 242, 245 Angoff, W. H., 417
Ahern, F., 614 Altemeyer, B., 986 Angold, A., 156
Ainsworth, M. D., 151, 153, 377, Altman, I., 488 Anni, H., 237
384, 406, 422, 500, 737, 740, Altmann, J., 507 Anokhin, K. V., 216, 247
815, 979 Alvarez, J., 772 Anscombe, G. E. M., 75, 76
Aitken, J. K., 439 Alwin, D., 668, 688, 696 Anthony, A., 685
Ajzen, I., 410, 522 Amanatides, P. G., 223 Anthony, E. J., 841, 886, 887, 905
Akiyama, H., 696 Amann-Gainotti, M., 197 Anthony, S., 974
Albersheim, L., 377 Amari, S., 300 Antonucci, T. C., 587, 696
Alberts, J. A., 246 Amato, P., 682, 691, 703 Apel, R., 688
Alberts, J. R., 242 Amatruda, C. S., 132 Aperia, A., 424
Aldwin, C. M., 635, 640, 641 Amaya-Williams, M., 549 Apfel, A., 915
Alexander, K. J., 749 Amell, J. W., 668 Appelbaum, M. I., 30
Alexander, K. L., 680, 681 Ames, C., 505 Appley, M. H., 407
Alibali, M., 349, 365 Ames, L. B., 297 Apter, M., 469, 480

999
1000 Author Index

Arbreton, A. J. A., 912 Axel, R., 404 586, 589, 590, 591, 593, 594,
Archer, S. L., 620, 622 Axelrod, R., 624 597, 602, 604, 610, 611, 619,
Arcus, D., 438, 442 Ayer, A. J., 73, 74 624, 634, 635, 637, 638, 668,
Ardelt, M., 607, 821 Ayoub, C., 316, 317, 321, 326, 327, 671, 904
Ardila, R., 57 328, 334, 335, 349, 350, 357, Baltes, P. B., 5, 11, 12, 167, 182,
Arend, R. A., 501 365, 376, 377, 378, 379, 380, 267, 405, 408, 417, 418, 419,
Arendt, H., 537 381 421, 433, 436, 437, 441, 442,
Arenson, K. W., 873 Azuma, H., 756, 759 443, 445, 448, 449, 473, 491,
Argyle, M., 945, 952, 977, 982, 983, 493, 520, 527, 528, 530, 539,
985 Babigian, H., 427 547, 548, 551, 552, 553, 569,
Armistead, L., 922 Bach, G. R., 149 570, 571, 572, 573, 574, 575,
Armour, J. D., 876 Bachman, M., 772 576, 577, 578, 579, 580, 581,
Armstrong, K., 452 Bachnik, J. M., 758 582, 583, 584, 585, 586, 587,
Arn, I., 416 Bäckman, L., 448, 548, 553, 578, 588, 589, 590, 591, 592, 593,
Arnett, M., 925 580, 593, 597, 601, 602, 609, 594, 595, 596, 597, 598, 599,
Arnheim, R., 470 610, 612 600, 601, 602, 603, 604, 605,
Aronoff, J. S., 991 Bacon, S., 624 606, 607, 608, 609, 610, 611,
Aronson, J., 834, 839, 845, 846, 870, Baddeley, A. D., 601 612, 613, 614, 615, 616, 617,
871 Bader, J. S., 237 618, 619, 620, 621, 624, 625,
Arrington, E. G., 849 Badley, E. M., 595 626, 627, 630, 632, 634, 636,
Arrington, R. E., 135 Baer, D., 150, 521 637, 638, 639, 640, 641, 643,
Arsenio, W. F., 25 Bahrick, L., 240, 241, 243, 245, 284 644, 668, 669, 670, 671, 676,
Arterberry, M. E., 241 Baillargeon, R., 61, 278, 303, 324, 757, 904, 915, 944
Arthur, M. W., 901 333, 339, 340, 341, 344, 345, Baltes-Götz, B., 554, 578
Aschersleben, G., 418, 436, 583, 353 Baltissen, R., 626, 628
596, 598, 599, 600, 609, 615, Baird, J., 777 Balzer, W., 531, 532
616 Bakan, D., 485 Bamberg, M., 185
Aseltine, R. H., 688 Bakeman, R., 500 Banaji, M., 315, 322, 327, 376, 379,
Asencio, M., 762 Baker, R. M., 689 380, 410, 750
Asendorpf, J. B., 20, 623 Bakhtin, M., 80, 200, 495, 748 Banarjee, R., 771
Ash, M., 171, 185 Baldwin, A., 143, 146 Bandura, A., 149, 150, 151, 264,
Ashman, S. B., 416 Baldwin, B. T., 126 364, 411, 424, 453, 481, 520,
Aslin, R., 195, 240, 605 Baldwin, D., 287, 308 522, 527, 536, 537, 539, 543,
Aspinwall, L., 416, 585, 607, 626 Baldwin, J. M., 2, 37, 39, 40, 60, 95, 547, 572, 587, 620, 634, 639,
Assanand, S., 634 107, 108, 109, 110, 111, 112, 811
Astington, J. W., 765, 777 113, 121, 122, 172, 187, 188, Bane, M. J., 682, 683
Astuti, R., 61 192, 199, 247, 248, 365, 494, Banich, M. T., 612
Atchley, R. C., 539, 544, 551 495, 501, 507, 508, 683, 762 Banker, H., 213, 245
Atkins, R., 49, 919, 945, 984 Baldwin, M. W., 762 Banks, M. K., 420
Atkinson, J. W., 522 Bales, K. R., 223 Banks, W. C., 834
Atnafou, R., 919 Bales, R. F., 496 Baquedano-López, P., 744
Atran, S., 717, 729, 777, 970, 971 Balkenius, C., 288 Barbano, H. E., 637
August, D. L., 548 Balkin, J. M., 871, 872, 873, 874 Barbee, A. H., 574
Austad, S. N., 576 Ballard, D., 287, 288, 305 Barber, B. L., 919
Austin, G., 321 Balle-Jensen, L., 725, 726, 756 Barbu-Roth, M. A., 384
Austin, J. A., 982 Balsano, A. B., 904, 930 Bargh, J., 63, 522, 554, 584, 621
Austin, J. L., 523 Baltes, B. B., 591, 594, 643 Bar-Joseph, Z., 237, 238
Averill, J. A., 534, 540 Baltes, M., 449, 527, 548, 553, 570, Barker, M., 135
Avia, M. D., 620, 633 576, 579, 580, 581, 582, 583, Barker, R., 146, 421, 481, 833
Author Index 1001

Barkow, J. H., 623 Bausano, M., 725 Bengtson, V. L., 551, 622, 637, 670,
Barlow, C., 223 Bavelier, D., 279 697
Barnes, E., 135, 973 Bayer, U., 540 Benigni, L., 168
Barnes, H., 425, 426 Bayles, K., 498 Benish, G., 945
Baron, H., 834 Bayley, N., 134, 666 Ben-Shlomo, Y., 668
Baron-Cohen, S., 348 Bayley, R., 743, 744 Benson, C., 750
Barone, M., 226 Bean, R. A., 929 Benson, F. B., 612
Barrell, B. G., 183 Bearer, E., 1, 7, 156, 705 Benson, P., 12, 895, 896, 899, 900,
Barrett, A. E., 684 Bearer, R., 416 901, 902, 905, 906, 908, 911,
Barrett, D., 987 Bearman, P., 697, 705, 920, 922 912, 913, 914, 915, 918, 920,
Barrett, J., 959, 972, 975 Beasley, A. B., 223 921, 922, 923, 924, 925, 927,
Barrett, K. C., 321 Beattie, O., 538, 541 929, 930, 931, 948, 954, 971,
Barrett, L. F., 370, 372 Beatty, J., 172 982
Barron, F., 484 Beaudet, L., 223 Benton, L. A., 576
Barsalou, L. W., 284, 303 Beaver, A. P., 135 Benzeval, M., 684
Barth, C., 947, 974 Becker, B., 905, 915, 916 Bereiter, C., 349
Bartko, T., 768 Becker, H. S., 672, 682, 696 Berg, C. A., 597, 600, 613
Bartley, M., 683, 684, 692 Beckmann, J., 522, 536, 544 Berg, K., 819
Bartok, J., 376 Beckwith, L., 811 Berg, M., 945
Barton, S., 437, 582 Bee, H., 469 Berg, S., 614, 615
Bartsch, K., 301 Beer, R. D., 305 Bergeman, C. S., 819
Bashore, T. R., 601 Beier, M. E., 605 Bergenn, V. W., 128
Basinger, O., 245 Beile, H., 952, 963 Berger, C. R., 525
Basov, M., 194 Beilin, H., 342, 343 Berglund, M. L., 896, 897, 900, 901,
Basseches, M., 495, 646 Beit-Hallahmi, B., 945, 948, 949, 916
Basso, K., 745, 749 952, 977, 991 Bergman, L. R., 5, 49, 156, 417,
Batchelder, W. H., 774 Bekhterev, V. M., 200 420, 431, 432, 438, 442, 444,
Bates, E., 10, 279, 303, 304, 306, Belansky, E., 922 445, 446, 447, 449, 587, 589,
356, 357, 597, 605 Belfiore, P. J., 537 611, 668, 794
Bates, J. E., 498, 626, 627, 628, 629, Belfrage, H., 415 Bergson, H., 177, 193, 197
924 Belknap, J. K., 223, 224 Berkas, T. H., 919
Bates, K. E., 235 Bell, A. C., 928 Berkowitz, M. L., 981, 983
Bateson, G., 145 Bell, D. A., 875, 877, 878 Berkowitz, W., 915, 932
Bateson, P., 41, 46, 156, 421 Bell, M. A., 348, 384 Berlin, I., 717, 723, 724
Bateson, W., 222, 235, 239 Bell, R. Q., 134, 406, 425, 434, 545, Berlyne, D. E., 469, 508
Batson, C. D., 982, 983, 984 623 Berman, J. J., 750
Batterman, N., 534 Bell, S., 500, 846 Berman, R., 745
Battey, J. F., Jr., 224 Bellah, R. N., 755, 756 Bermudez, J. L., 54
Bauer, D., 685, 688 Beller, A. H., 694 Berndt, T. J., 426
Bauer, I. O., 233, 239 Bellugi, U., 213, 214 Bernheimer, L. P., 773
Baugh, J. R., 984, 985 Belnap, N. D., Jr., 532 Bernstein, J. H., 335
Baughman, J. W., 698 Belsky, J., 44, 426, 500 Bernstein, N. A., 298
Bauman, K. E., 920, 922 Belzen, J. A., 964 Berry, J. W., 721
Bauman, R., 742 Bem, D. J., 612, 642, 683, 692, Berry, T. D., 246
Baumeister, R. F., 543, 544, 549, 694 Bertelson, P., 240
619, 640, 752, 753 Ben-Avie, M., 913 Bertenthal, B. I., 246, 280, 302, 384
Baumert, J., 668 Benedict, R., 375, 376, 728 Berthier, N., 302
Baumrind, D., 406, 425, 485, 496, Benes, F., 385 Berthier, W. E., 241
527 Benetka, G., 170 Berzonsky, M., 537
1002 Author Index

Betts, S. C., 928 Bloom, B., 530, 587 Borstlemann, L. J., 90


Beuhring, T., 928 Bloom, L., 1, 24, 50 Bosman, E. A., 597, 605, 609
Bialystok, E., 571, 577, 596, 597, Bloom, M., 901 Bossom, J., 54
601, 602 Blossfeld, H.-P., 589, 673 Bothell, D., 339, 340, 347
Bickhard, M. H., 50 Bluck, S., 641 Bott, H., 126, 136
Biddle, F. G., 231 Blum, R. W., 914, 920, 922, 928 Botuck, S., 243
Bidell, T., 154, 155, 199, 315, 319, Blumberg, B., 289 Bouchard, T. J., Jr., 226, 614, 615,
325, 336, 341, 342, 343, 344, Blumberg, H. P., 409 702
347, 365, 383, 944 Blumenfeld, P., 768 Boudreau, J. P., 242
Biggs, J., 323 Blyth, D., 12, 683, 900, 901, 905, Bouffard, L. A., 687
Bigi, L., 548 906, 908, 912, 913, 915, 918, Bound, J., 689
Bijou, S. W., 150, 521 919, 920, 922, 925, 930 Bourdieu, P., 195, 518, 522, 725,
Binet, A., 100, 101, 102, 103, 104, Bobek, D. L., 904, 930 732, 763, 770, 773, 775
115, 118, 122, 132 Bock, H. H., 446 Bourgeois, J.-P., 382, 384, 385
Birch, H., 241 Bode, C., 620 Bourne, L., 752, 753
Birns, B., 143 Boehm, S. L., II, 235 Bovaird, J., 668
Biro, D., 192 Boesch, E. E., 24, 38, 49, 67, 188, Bovccaccini, M. T., 982
Birren, J. E., 491, 493, 551, 573, 195, 199, 200, 201, 517, 519, Bowen, K. R., 605
576, 581, 601, 640, 646 522, 525, 597, 717 Bowlby, J., 37, 151, 153, 265, 406,
Bishop, K. M., 225 Bogdahn, U., 609 500, 507, 815, 816
Bishop, Y. M. M., 446 Bogenschneider, K., 905 Bowman, R. E., 231
Bizzi, E., 298 Bohan, J. B., 549 Boyatzis, C., 944, 970, 971, 976,
Bjorklund, D., 41, 240, 600, 601, Bohman, M., 441 977, 978
605 Bohner, G., 639 Boyce, C., 837
Black, I. B., 215 Boje, D. M., 485 Boyd-Franklin, N., 835
Black, J. E., 235 Boklage, C. E., 232 Boyer, P., 959, 970
Blackhart, G. C., 629 Bolk, L., 506 Boykin, A. W., 835, 856, 857
Blackmore, C., 244 Bolles, C., 423 Bracken, B. A., 752
Blackstone, W., 986 Bolstrom, B., 906, 908, 915, 924, Bradley, R. M., 242
Blaich, C. F., 213 928, 929 Bradway, R., 589
Blair, C., 231 Bonanno, G. A., 621 Brainard, M. S., 244
Blair-Loy, M., 673 Boncinelli, E., 410 Brainerd, C. J., 332, 333, 531
Blakeney, C. D., 964, 982 Bond, M. H., 761 Brame, R., 688
Blakeney, R. F., 964, 982 Bond, T. G., 30, 350, 351, 352 Brammer, G. L., 415
Blanchard-Fields, F., 597, 606, 620, Bonilla-Silva, E., 832, 884 Branchi, I., 239
622, 636 Bontempo, R., 753 Branco, A. U., 174, 176, 178, 193,
Blane, D., 692 Boomsma, D. I., 447, 613, 614 195
Blaney, P. H., 554 Booth, A., 677, 682, 688, 703, Brand, M., 517, 522
Blanton, H., 636 913 Brandtstädter, J., 6, 51, 411, 517,
Blatt, S. J., 688, 753 Booth, C. L., 816 519, 520, 524, 525, 526, 527,
Blavich, E., 953 Borden, L. M., 13, 898, 900 528, 529, 530, 532, 534, 536,
Blazer, D., 540, 551, 552, 985 Boring, E. G., 2, 91 537, 538, 539, 540, 541, 543,
Bledsoe, C., 727, 736 Borkenau, P., 442 545, 548, 549, 550, 551, 552,
Blehar, M., 153, 377, 384, 740, 815 Borkowski, W., 362 554, 555, 570, 578, 579, 581,
Bleicher, J., 64 Born, D. E., 219 582, 584, 585, 587, 593, 620,
Blenkinsop, T. G., 320 Bornstein, M., 1, 4, 7, 10, 33, 36, 622, 628, 634, 635, 636, 637,
Block, J., 6, 49, 352, 446, 450, 478, 182, 612, 759, 779, 915 638, 641, 903, 904
480, 481, 530, 574, 619, 621, Borsboom, D., 431 Brant, C., 984
639, 666, 811 Borst, S., 376 Braver, T. S., 386
Author Index 1003

Brazelton, T., 499, 545, 726 Broughton, J., 39, 60, 90, 108, 112, Bull, J. J., 231
Breazeal, C., 289 113, 494, 549 Bullock, D., 200, 248, 317, 318, 320,
Bredy, T. W., 236 Brouwer, C., 237 321, 330, 332, 339, 341, 344,
Brehm, J. W., 538, 544, 553, 691 Brower, A. M., 551 351, 354, 357
Brehm, S. S., 691 Brown, A. L., 317, 332, 349, 590, Bullock, H. A., 879, 880
Brehmer, Y., 604 605 Bullock, M., 545, 751
Breinlinger, K., 263 Brown, B., 488, 895, 901 Bulman-Fleming, B., 225, 230, 231
Bremner, J. G., 300 Brown, G. G., 409 Bumbarger, B., 896, 901
Brenner, S., 233 Brown, G. W., 424 Bumpass, L., 672
Brent, S. B., 269, 492, 574 Brown, J., 500, 639, 777 Bunge, M., 31, 57, 520, 528
Brentano, C., 898, 904, 915 Brown, L. B., 944, 952, 973, 974, Burgardsmeier, A., 974
Brentano, E., 409 977, 978 Burge, B., 201
Brentano, F., 51 Brown, T., 370 Burger, L. K., 745, 746
Bressler, R., 226 Bruce, D., 981 Burgeson, R., 683
Bretherton, I., 153, 377, 500 Brückner, H., 697, 705 Burgess, E. W., 698
Breuer, J., 115, 122, 380 Bruner, J., 153, 321, 325, 473, 494, Burgess, R. L., 647
Brewer, W. F., 102 507, 508, 517, 519, 521, 522, Burkert, W., 945
Breytspraak, L. M., 551 526, 530, 545, 546, 717, 741, Burkhart-Kasch, S., 235
Brickerhoff, M. B., 988 745, 750, 763, 911 Burns, B. D., 610
Brickman, P., 553 Brunstein, J. C., 541, 639 Burns, J., 930
Bridgman, P., 74 Brunswik, E., 213, 220, 246, 421 Burt, C., 129, 235
Briggs, C. L., 742 Brush, S. G., 170 Burton, L. M., 697
Briggs, J. L., 743, 744, 772 Bryant, P. E., 278 Burton, R. V., 150, 157
Brim, O. G., Jr., 5, 526, 527, 539, Buchanan, C. M., 429, 503 Busch, V., 609
544, 554, 569, 571, 573, 578, Buch-Bartos, K., 639 Busch-Rossnagel, N. A., 6, 14, 316,
579, 582, 584, 587, 619, 622, Bucher, A., 949, 959, 964, 973, 980, 492, 517, 528, 635, 798
639, 644 981, 988 Bush, K. R., 929
Brinker, G. P., 682 Buchmann, M., 680 Bushnell, E., 241, 242, 282
Broberg, G., 432 Bucholz, K. K., 702 Bushway, S., 688
Brody, G., 428, 696, 922 Buchsbaum, D., 289 Busnel, M. C., 232
Bronfenbrenner, U., 2, 5, 10, 11, 13, Buchsbaum, H. K., 317 Buss, A. H., 537, 648
14, 44, 50, 146, 156, 177, 195, Buck, K. J., 223, 224 Buss, D. M., 7, 41, 623
267, 341, 349, 408, 416, 421, Buck, L., 404 Butchart, R. E., 880
427, 480, 494, 503, 525, 535, Buckle, C. E., 227 Butler, S. C., 302
586, 613, 668, 669, 670, 679, Buckley, K. W., 130, 131, 132 Butter, M., 953
702, 703, 764, 793, 794, 795, Buckner, J. P., 747 Butterworth, G., 545
796, 797, 799, 802, 807, 810, Buckner, R. I., 601 Buunk, B. P., 640
814, 816, 818, 819, 821, 822, Budde, M., 602, 616 Bynner, J., 682, 692
823, 824, 833, 834, 841, 843, Buddin, B., 365 Byrd, M., 600
846, 847, 864, 865, 866, 878, Budwig, N., 185 Byrne, M. D., 339, 340, 347
881, 884, 900, 910, 911, 912, Buehler, C., 922 Byrnes, J., 12
918, 926 Bühler, C., x, 129, 135, 136, 139,
Brookins, G. K., 379, 835, 839, 857, 147, 167, 184, 517, 521, 541, Cabeza, R., 597, 602, 603, 616,
868 571, 620 627
Brooks, D. R., 46, 47 Bühler, K., 96, 98, 200 Cabral, E. A., 195
Brooks-Gunn, J., 13, 154, 408, 411, Bühler, R., 640 Cacioppo, J. T., 628, 629, 630
412, 430, 545, 546, 549, 682, Buhrmann, K., 242 Cahan, E. D., 91, 108, 109, 113,
685, 700, 900, 901, 905, 906, Buikhuisen, W., 410 119, 120, 741
912, 916 Buitelaar, J. K., 415 Cain, C., 745, 748, 749
1004 Author Index

Cairns, B., 154, 155, 406, 419, 426, Carlson, S. M., 741 Caudill, W., 725, 726, 739, 756, 758,
427, 433, 436, 441, 447, 683, Carlson, V., 762 759
688, 794, 925 Carlton-Ford, S., 683 Cavilli-Sforza, L., 977
Cairns, R., 3, 6, 49, 90, 94, 99, 108, Carmichael, L., xi, xii, xiv, 90, 142, Cazden, C. B., 748
109, 111, 114, 132, 151, 152, 147 Ceci, S. J., 408, 586, 614, 679, 702,
154, 155, 156, 171, 176, 185, Carnochan, P., 320, 325, 370, 371, 703, 799, 807, 819, 821, 822,
197, 202, 213, 214, 219, 230, 372 823, 824, 864, 865, 866, 889
240, 249, 316, 347, 405, 406, Caron, A. J., 470 Celniker, S. E., 223
408, 414, 415, 416, 417, 419, Caron, R. F., 470 Cerbone, F. G., 688
426, 427, 432, 433, 436, 439, Carpendale, J. I. M., 63, 67, 68 Cerella, J., 601
440, 441, 447, 482, 521, 679, Carpenter, P. A., 601 Cervantes, C. A., 744
683, 688, 701, 794, 991 Carpenter, W. B., 93 Cervone, D., 455, 619, 620, 640
Calamandrei, G., 216, 247 Carr, L., 706 Challman, R. C., 136
Caldwell, J. A., 981, 983 Carriger, M. S., 613 Chalmers, D., 466
Callan, P., 340 Carrithers, M., 753 Champagne, D. L., 236
Calverley, R., 381 Carroll, G., 867 Chan, A., 373, 374, 815
Calvert, G., 240 Carroll, J. B., 127, 132 Chandler, M., 27, 61, 269, 492
Calvin, J., 946 Carroll, V., 736 Chang, C. F., 835
Camburn, D., 668 Carstensen, L., 551, 553, 570, 579, Chang, H., 683
Cameron, A. M., 547 580, 591, 620, 629, 630, 639 Changeux, J.-P., 219
Cameron, J., 772 Carus, F. A., 527, 571 Chao, R. K., 759, 760
Camic, C., 733 Carvalho, A. A., 202 Chapman, M., 61, 188, 197, 198,
Campbell, B. H., 537 Carver, C. S., 410, 522, 536, 537, 343, 517, 547, 601, 606
Campbell, D. T., 13, 349 539, 540, 541, 543, 554, 620, Charles, N., 831
Campbell, J. D., 150, 157, 633 638, 639 Charles, S. T., 579, 620, 630, 639
Campbell, J. H., 247 Carver, L. J., 416 Charlesworth, B., 575
Campos, J., 24, 25, 246, 280, 302, Casaer, P., 449 Charlesworth, W. R., 94
321, 384 Case, R., 187, 199, 323, 333, 342, Charness, N., 367, 597, 606, 610
Camras, L. A., 372 343, 344, 348, 350, 353, 357, Chartrand, T. L., 554
Cannon, W. B., 416 368, 369, 384, 545, 549, 601, Chase, W. G., 610
Cantor, N., 541, 544, 545, 549, 551, 602, 944 Chassein, B., 539
620, 636, 639, 751 Caspi, A., 233, 408, 415, 428, 429, Chaudhary, N., 168, 200
Cantwell, D., 989 431, 434, 571, 613, 620, 621, Chaudhuri, A., 237
Cao, H. T., 845, 867 622, 623, 625, 628, 633, 642, Chaudhuri, J., 12
Caplan, G., 901 668, 679, 683, 684, 685, 688, Cheadle, J., 691
Caplovitz, D., 978 691, 692, 694, 695, 696, 703, Cheek, P. R., 553
Capps, L., 745, 747, 748 705, 813 Chen, A., 223
Caprara, G. V., 438 Cassidy, E., 833, 846, 856, 859, 860, Chen, E. C., 748
Capriotti, R., 627, 629 861, 862, 870 Chen, H. M., 238
Capron, C., 232 Cassidy, J., 377 Chen, L., 775
Carbonari, J. P., 641 Cassirer, E., 58, 59, 956, 988 Cheng, C., 332, 354, 356
Cardon, L. R., 612, 614 Castenell, L. A., Jr., 842, 857 Cheng, M.-F., 216
Care, N. S., 522 Castiglione, C. M., 227 Cheng, P. C.-H., 605, 609
Carey, S., 61, 302, 338, 344, 345, Catalano, R. F., 896, 897, 900, 901, Cherif, C., 227, 228
364, 372, 532, 777, 975 906, 915, 916, 923, 925 Cherlin, A. J., 682
Carlier, M., 213, 220, 227, 228 Cattell, A. K. S., 446 Chernoff, J. M., 184
Carlo, G., 633 Cattell, J., 102 Cherny, S. S., 613
Carlson, J. S., 603, 973 Cattell, R. B., 446, 583, 596, 600, Chesler, E. J., 235
Carlson, M., 415, 437, 913, 914 605, 613, 618, 622, 627 Chess, S., 477, 491, 493, 526, 623,
Carlson, R., 441 Catterall, J. S., 920 905
Author Index 1005

Chestang, L. W., 834, 839, 845, 870, Coelho, N. E., Jr., 199 Connolly, J. A., 811
871, 872, 873 Cohen, D., 156, 637, 717, 723 Connolly, K. J., 167, 405
Cheung, T. S., 761 Cohen, J. D., 601 Conrad, H., 598
Chi, M. T., 263, 582, 606, 776 Cohen, L. B., 278, 470 Conrad, T., 385
Chiara, G., 640 Cohen, M. R., 19 Conway, A. R. A., 602
Child, I., 719, 725, 732 Cohen, N. H., 261 Cook, G., 190
Chin, H. R., 224 Cohen, N. J., 236, 612 Cook, M., 293
Chinen, A. B., 491 Cohen, R. D., 982 Cook, T. D., 683, 697, 924
Chisholm, R. M., 523 Cohen, R. L., 688, 696 Cooke, J., 260
Chomsky, N., 61, 303, 315, 338, Cohen, S. E., 500, 587, 811 Cookson, K., 28, 62
339, 340, 344, 346 Cohen, S. H., 587 Cooley, C. H., 111, 113, 485, 501
Christou, C., 343, 353 Cohler, B. J., 811 Cooley, D. H., 762
Chu, A. M., 238 Coie, J. D., 427 Coons, H., 424
Chudacoff, H. P., 524 Colby, A., 5, 156, 348, 449, 668, Cooper, B. P., 604
Chugani, H. T., 602 718, 722, 983 Cooper, C. R., 496, 771, 772, 773, 774
Church, M. A., 639 Colcombe, S., 604, 612 Cooper, H., 926
Church, R. B., 365 Cole, E., 276 Cooper, R., 881
Churchill, J. D., 420 Cole, M., 31, 66, 153, 200, 322, 347, Coopersmith, S. A., 753
Churchland, P., 282, 303 356, 522, 531, 576, 586, 597, Copeland, P., 624
Cianciolo, A. T., 607 644, 717, 718, 723, 725, 738, Coppotelli, H., 427
Cicchetti, D., 156, 248, 317, 377, 742, 751, 762, 765, 766, 770, Corbetta, D., 286, 293, 294, 295,
578, 637, 835, 905, 915, 916 772, 778, 794, 859, 869 296, 297
Cicero, M. T., 572 Coleman, H. L., 835 Corbit, J. D., 423
Clancy, P., 744 Coleman, J. C., 538, 543 Corcos, D. M., 296
Clark, C. D., 721, 741 Coleman, J. S., 401, 410, 421, 913 Corey, L. A., 819
Clark, C. L., 377 Coles, R., 748, 968 Cornelius, S. W., 448, 539, 586,
Clark, G. N., 500 Coley, J. D., 717, 729, 777 611, 676
Clark, H., 767 Coll, C. G., 705 Cornford, F. M., 56
Clark, J. E., 196 Collingwood, R. G., 732 Cornoni-Huntley, J., 637
Clark, K. B., 378, 871 Collins, L. M., 446, 668, 685 Corrigan, R., 332
Clark, M. K., 378 Collins, P. F., 628, 629 Corsaro, W. A., 721, 741, 743, 745,
Clark, M. P., 871 Collins, R. L., 225, 539 748, 770
Clarke, A. D. B., 527 Collins, S., 753 Cortés, D. E., 762
Clarke, A. M., 527 Collins, W. A., 1, 7, 10, 33, 36, 535, Cosmides, L., 41, 623
Clarke, P., 701 550, 580, 915 Costa, F. M., 901, 921, 923
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., 530 Collis, K., 323 Costa, P. T., Jr., 378, 488, 619, 620,
Clark-Kauffman, E., 689 Columbus, R., 244 621, 632, 633, 635, 637
Clausen, J. A., 574, 666, 669, 687, Colvin, C. R., 622, 639 Costall, A., 121
693 Comer, J., 913 Costanzo, P. R., 411, 439
Clayton, V. P., 491, 493 Commons, M. L., 323 Costello, E. J., 347, 679
Clifton, R., 241, 302 Compayré, G., 96, 98 Costigan, K. A., 629
Clingempeel, W. G., 808, 816 Condon, S. M., 496 Cote, L. R., 779
Clipp, E. C., 698, 821 Condorcet, J. A., 944 Cotman, C. W., 584, 594
Cloninger, C. R., 441, 620, 629 Conger, K. J., 924 Cott, C., 595
Clyman, R. B., 317 Conger, R. D., 428, 685, 688, 692, Cottrell, L., Jr., 145, 150, 930
Coats, E. J., 604, 639 696, 821, 924 Coulombre, A. J., 142
Coatsworth, J. D., 905 Connell, J. P., 548, 635, 906, 911, Courtier, J., 101
Cochran, D. L., 762 913, 914, 915, 924, 927, 932 Cousins, S. D., 757
Cochrane, C., 231 Connell, M. W., 366 Covitz, F. E., 811
Cocking, R., 542, 751, 752, 755, 758 Connelly, C., 238 Cowdry, E. V., 573
1006 Author Index

Cowen, E. L., 427 Csordas, T. J., 49 Davis, F. C., 238


Coyle, T. R., 364 Culver, C., 62 Davis, J. L., 282
Coyne, J. C., 553 Cunningham, C. L., 223, 224 Dawkins, R., 619
Crabbe, J. C., 223, 224, 225, 235, 239 Cunningham, M., 831, 838, 857, Dawson, G., 364, 416
Craig, I. W., 233 858, 859, 867, 869, 870, 881 Dawson, T. L., 30, 79, 323, 332,
Craik, F. I. M., 571, 577, 596, 597, Curnan, S. P., 898, 913 333, 334, 350, 351, 352
601, 602 Curran, P. J., 685 Dawson-Tunik, T. L., 387
Craik, K., 500 Cushman, P., 764 Deacon, T. W., 382
Cranston, M., 555 Dearing, R. L., 371
Crapo, L. M., 603 Dabholkar, A. S., 602 Deary, I. J., 616
Craven, R. G., 634 Daecke, M., 945 de Beer, G., 93, 94, 99
Crawford, C., 179 Dahlstrom, W. G., 127 DeCasper, A. J., 232
Cremin, L. A., 120 Dailey, M. E., 385 DeCharms, R., 837, 838
Crenshaw, K., 832, 867 Dalton, T. C., 128 Deci, E. L., 755
Crick, F., 170, 282, 403 Damasio, A., 38, 48, 49, 282, 285, Decker, S., 554
Crick, N. R., 25 370, 372, 386, 412, 415, 421, Deconchy, J.-P., 973
Criqui, M. H., 638 468 de Frias, C. M., 448
Crits-Christoph, P., 376, 377 Damasio, H., 421 DeFries, J., 340, 347, 498, 632, 866
Crocker, J., 542, 749 Damon, W., 2, 6, 11, 12, 27, 38, 63, de Gelder, B., 240
Crockett, L. J., 517 67, 69, 73, 336, 496, 533, 550, de Geus, E. J. C., 614
Croker, S., 605, 609 576, 756, 763, 845, 895, 896, Degrelle, H., 213
Cronbach, L. J., 104, 440, 446, 574, 900, 901, 913, 926, 983 Dehaene, S., 342
589 D’Andrade, R., 524, 576, 717, 718, DeKay, W. T., 623
Cronon, W., 969 723, 768 deLeeuw, N., 263
Croog, S., 990 Daner, F., 987 de León, L., 743
Crook, C., 751 Daniels, M., 230, 231 Delgado, R., 875, 876, 877, 878
Crosby, L., 917 Dannefer, D., 517, 519, 540, 586, Dell, T. C., 602
Crosnoe, R., 666, 691, 920 671 Dell’Angleo, T., 831, 887
Cross, D., 301, 777 Danziger, K., 170 Delle Fave, A. D., 471
Cross, W. E., Jr., 834, 835, 842, 843, Darity, W. A., Jr., 832 DelVecchio, W. F., 632
859, 867, 871, 878, 879, 880, Darley, J. M., 983 Dembo, T., 146
881, 882, 889 Darling, N. E., 817, 818 Demetriou, A., 26, 343, 353
Crossin, K. L., 216 Darlington, R. B., 444 Demick, J., 39, 69, 70
Crott, H. W., 616 Darwin, C., 159, 401, 412, 433 Dempster, F. N., 601
Crouch, C. H., 340 Daselaar, S. M., 602, 616 Dempster-McClain, D., 675
Crouter, A. C., 177, 416, 673, 677, Dasen, P. R., 721 Deng, F. M., 737
794, 799, 821, 833, 866, 913 Daston, L., 172 Dengler, D., 377
Crowell, J. A., 377 Datan, N., 586, 674 Denner, J., 773
Crowley, K., 72, 86, 364, 365, 590, Datnow, A., 881 Dennett, D., 51, 519, 521
603, 605, 775 Dauber, S. L., 680 Denney, N. W., 590
Crumbaugh, C., 769 Davey, M., 915 Dennis, A., 445
Crusio, W. E., 238 Davey, T. C., 613 Dent, C. H., 240
Crutchfield, F. L., 223 David, J. P., 641 Denz, H., 944
Csikszentmihalyi, I. S., 477 Davidson, A. L., 772, 845, 867 Depue, R. A., 628, 629
Csikszentmihalyi, M., 184, 423, Davidson, D., 521, 756 de Ribaupierre, A., 449
467, 468, 469, 470, 473, 477, Davidson, P., 67, 343, 533 Derks, F., 988
478, 479, 481, 484, 491, 497, Davidson, R. J., 627, 628, 629, 630 Derryberry, D., 555
498, 502, 504, 505, 506, 526, Davies, D. R., 537 Descartes, R., 56, 336, 337
538, 541, 900, 912, 918 Davies, K. I., 746 De Stefanis, I., 915
Author Index 1007

Detterman, D. K., 234 Dollahite, D., 977, 978 Duncan, G., 5, 156, 682, 683, 688,
Devlin, B., 230, 231 Dollard, J., 144, 149 689
De Weerth, C., 291 Dominguez, E., 772 Duncan, S. C., 685
Dewey, J., 68, 69, 119, 120, 195, Donagan, A., 731 Duncan, T. E., 685
467, 470, 478, 497, 502, 508, Donahue, E. M., 634 Dunér, A., 425
750 Donahue, M. J., 948 Dunn, J., 413, 422, 738, 741, 777
Diacoyanni-Tarlatis, I., 373, 374 Donaldson, G., 417 Dupree, D., 831, 846, 847, 858, 859,
Diamond, A., 275, 283, 302, 348, Donde, S., 833, 846, 856, 859, 860, 862, 869, 870, 905
353, 613 861, 862 Duran, R., 919
Dias, M., 717, 718, 738, 756 Dong, H., 255 Duranti, A., 742
Diaz, I., 241 Dong, W., 604 Durham, W. H., 575, 576, 580, 596,
Diaz, R. M., 549 Donnelly, T., 945, 984 605, 644
DiBerardino, M. A., 219 D’Onofrio, B., 614 Durkheim, E., 944
Dick, A., 54, 61, 963 Doob, L. W., 144, 149 Durkin, K., 638
Dick, D. M., 233, 239, 428, 614, 702 Dorgan, K. A., 913, 927 Durlak, J. A., 901, 916
Dickinson, C. M., 590 Dorn, L. D., 686 Durnaret, A., 232
Dickson, M. W., 644 Dornbusch, S., 835, 838, 842, 920 Duster, T., 837
Dickson, N., 695 Dörner, J., 634 Duval, S., 537
Dickstein, S., 441 Dörner, O., 538 Duveen, G., 201
DiClemente, C. C., 641 Dorow, J., 235 Duyme, M., 232
Diedrich, F. J., 301 Dorsett, J. G., 644 Dweck, C. S., 517, 839
Diehl, M., 620, 634, 638 Dorsey, S., 922 Dworkin, J. B., 918
Diener, E., 630, 633 Douglass, S., 339, 340, 347 Dyer, A. B., 244
Diguer, L., 377 Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., 628 Dyson, A. H., 745, 748
Di Loreto, O., 963, 990 Dowdney, L., 426
Dilthey, W., 521 Dowling, E., 12, 895, 898, 904, 915, Eaker, D. G., 915
Dimsdale, J. E., 438 918, 920, 930, 932, 948, 971 Eales, B. A., 231
Dionne, J. P., 751 Doyle, A., 811 Earls, F., 13, 415, 437, 913, 914, 926
Di Paula, A., 634 Drachman, D. B., 142 Early, D., 922
DiPietro, J. A., 629 Draganski, B., 609 Easterbrooks, M. A., 547
Diriwächter, R., 171, 176, 185, 195 Drake, D., 12, 912, 920 Easterlin, R. A., 676
Dishion, T., 427, 917 Draper, P., 44 Eaton, W. H., 698
Dissanayake, W., 753 Dray, W. H., 75 Eaves, L., 702, 819
Dittmann-Kohli, F., 550, 591, 596, Dreher, E., 550, 551 Ebaugh, H. R. F., 673
620, 634, 639, 904, 985 Driesch, H., 211, 218 Ebbinghaus, H., 103, 132
Dixon, R. A., 267, 347, 436, 548, Drillien, C. M., 799 Ebner, N. C., 579, 592, 631
551, 553, 569, 571, 578, 580, Drobnic, S., 673 Eccles, J. S., 12, 410, 411, 428, 429,
582, 583, 591, 593, 596, 606, Drummond, W. B., 119 503, 504, 506, 683, 690, 821,
611, 626, 632, 904 Dryfoos, J. G., 900, 901, 913 905, 917, 919, 922
Dixon, S., 735, 737, 738, 739 DuBois, D. L., 926 Eckenhoff, M. F., 382, 384, 385
Dodart, J.-C., 223 Du Bois, W. E. B., 867, 877, 880 Eckensberger, L. H., 38, 67, 200,
Dodd, B., 240 Dubow, E. F., 925 201, 517, 526, 717
Dodds, A. E., 190 Ducret, J.-J., 197 Edelman, G. M., 7, 27, 38, 46, 47,
Dodge, K. A., 25, 335, 416, 427, 924 Dudek, B. C., 235 48, 49, 216, 219, 240, 249, 260,
Doi, T., 718 Duke, K., 237 282, 285, 303, 410, 421, 434,
Dolan, C. V., 447, 613 Dukes, R., 920 582
Dolan, R. J., 606 Dulles, A., 986 Edelstein, W., 343, 348, 449, 526,
Dolby, R. G. A., 170 Dumais, S. T., 537 579, 752, 757
Dolcos, F., 602, 616 Dunbar, K., 368 Edenberg, H. J., 233, 239
1008 Author Index

Eder, R. A., 751, 757 Emler, N., 428 Fagan, J. F., 613
Edey, M. A., 507, 508 Emmerich, W., 7 Fagen, A. P., 340
Edwards, C. P., 321, 356, 364, 379, Emmons, R. A., 541, 631, 639 Fagen, R. M., 508
738, 739 Endler, N. S., 406, 419 Fairbairn, W. R. D., 60
Edwards, J. N., 682 Eng, P., 862 Fajans, J., 752
Edwards, L., 219, 222, 228, 249, 704 Engel, B. T., 627, 629 Falgout, K., 990
Efklides, A., 412, 413 Engel, S., 745 Farber, S. L., 233
Eggleston, E. P., 685 Engeström, Y., 765 Farde, L., 601
Eibach, R., 35 Engle, R. W., 597, 602, 605, 609, Farmer, E. M. Z., 156
Eichorn, D. H., 345, 348, 387, 574, 610 Farmer, M. E., 224
666 Engler, R., 187 Farmer, T. W., 156, 925
Eilers, R., 245 Ennis, M., 25, 30, 52 Farnham, S., 315, 322, 327, 376,
Eisen, M., 751 Ensel, M., 703 379, 380
Eisenberg, A. R., 743, 745 Ensing, D. S., 990 Farr, M. J., 776
Eisenberg, N., 902, 928 Entin, E. E., 543 Farrar, M. J., 321, 325, 333
Eisenstadt, S. N., 674 Entwisle, D. R., 680, 681 Farrington, D. P., 695
Eisner, T., 440 Ephron-Wertheim, T., 533 Farver, J. M., 742, 760
Ek, E. U., 777 Eppler, M. A., 246 Faulconer, J. E., 436
Ekman, P., 62, 373, 374, 628, 629 Epstein, D., 612 Faulkner, K. W., 974
Elbert, T., 609 Epstein, S., 411, 549, 751 Fawcett, P., 415
Elbogen, E. B., 633 Erickson, F. D., 721, 722 Feather, N. T., 522
Elder, G. H., Jr., 4, 6, 7, 13, 90, 347, Erickson, J. A., 948, 977 Featherman, D. L., 442, 581, 589,
428, 429, 438, 571, 573, 574, Erickson, K. G., 920 591
586, 587, 665, 666, 667, 668, Erickson, K. I., 611 Feffer, M., 380
670, 672, 674, 675, 676, 677, Erickson, M. A., 822 Fegley, S., 49, 752, 831, 846, 887
678, 679, 682, 683, 685, 686, Ericsson, K. A., 367, 415, 553, 582, Fein, G. G., 508, 530
688, 689, 691, 692, 693, 694, 593, 597, 605, 607, 609, 776 Feinberg, S. E., 446
696, 697, 698, 699, 700, 701, Erikson, E., 28, 37, 38, 60, 383, 469, Feinglos, M. N., 231
703, 813, 821, 822, 830, 904, 493, 508, 538, 541, 571, 579, Feldlaufer, H., 504
971 620, 621, 622, 625, 632, 669, Feldman, D. H., 956, 970
El-Khouri, B. M., 444, 446, 447, 670, 750, 842, 845, 846, 850, Feldman, J. A., 991
448, 668 949 Feldman, M. W., 7
Elkind, D., 958, 973, 974, 975 Erikson, J. M., 493 Feldman, S., 544
Ellen, J., 914 Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., 230 Felser, G., 550
Ellias, M. F., 616 Escobar, A., 171 Fenichel, O., 117
Elliot, A. J., 639 Essed, P., 882 Fenigstein, A., 537
Elliott, D. S., 695 Essex, C., 174 Fensham, P., 423
Ellis, A., 537, 986 Essex, M. J., 637 Fentress, J. C., 433, 437
Ellis, L. A., 634 Estes, W. K., 146, 159, 329, 365 Fenwick, R. G., 225
Ellis, M. J., 508 Evans, C. A., 223 Ferber, T., 896, 900, 901, 906, 911
Ellison, C. G., 978 Evans, G. W., 794, 795, 797, 822, Féré, C., 100
Ellison, C. M., 835, 857 823 Ferguson, M. W. J., 231
Ellwood, D. T., 682, 683 Eyferth, K., 517 Ferguson, R. F., 913, 927
Elman, J., 10, 22, 279, 303, 306, Eyler Zorilla, L. T., 409 Fergusson, L. L., 433, 447
357, 596, 597, 605 Eysenck, H. J., 415, 499 Ferland, M. B., 241
Elson, S. E., 768, 770 Fernald, A., 759
Elster, J., 536, 750 Fabes, R. A., 902, 928 Fernandez, M., 835
Emde, R. N., 317, 422, 432, 527 Fabricius, W. V., 198 Fernhout, J. W., 964
Emerson, P. E., 151, 153 Fader, A., 744 Ferrar, M. J., 199
Author Index 1009

Ferrer-Caja, E., 582 Flannery, D. J., 929 Fox, N. A., 348, 384
Ferri, E., 692 Flash, T., 298 Fox, N. E., 816
Feshbach, N., 902 Flavell, E. R., 762 Fox, P. W., 613
Festinger, L., 146, 538 Flavell, J., 2, 153, 332, 335, 341, Fraley, R. C., 373, 374, 377
Fetters, L., 293 346, 532, 548, 549, 600, 606, Francis, L., 98
Field, T., 496, 499, 500, 501 762, 844, 993 Frank, D., 837
Fiese, B. H., 441, 745 Fleeson, W., 541, 620, 638, 639, 641 Frank, L., 125
Figueiredo, L. C., 199 Fleishman, E. A., 352 Frankel, C., 75
Filipp, S.-H., 545, 551, 620, 626, Fleming, C. B., 906 Frankel, W. N., 224, 225
634, 638, 640, 641, 642 Fleming, W. P., 895, 901 Frankfurt, H. G., 525
Fillmore, C. J., 522 Fletcher, A. C., 817, 818 Franks, M. M., 634
Finch, C. E., 225, 575, 576, 584, 593 Flint, J., 224 Franz, S. I., 102
Fink, R. P., 335 Flom, R., 240, 245 Frasier, K., 922
Finkel, D., 613 Florin, P., 901, 915, 932 Fravel, D. L., 441
Fischbein, S., 819, 865 Flynn, J. R., 603, 611 Frazier, L. D., 636
Fischer, K., 30, 79, 154, 155, 187, Fodor, J., 303, 325, 339, 340, 343, Fredericks, L., 926, 932
199, 200, 213, 248, 291, 315, 344, 368 Fredricks, J., 768
316, 317, 319, 320, 321, 322, Fogel, A., 195, 240, 362 Fredrickson, B. L., 470, 638
323, 324, 325, 326, 327, 328, Folkman, S., 552, 635 Freedman, D., 499, 668
329, 330, 332, 333, 334, 335, Folkow, B., 628 Freeman, A. D., 875
336, 339, 340, 341, 342, 343, Foner, A., 573, 586, 587, 667, 668, Freeman, N., 241, 973
344, 347, 348, 349, 350, 351, 674, 675 Freeman, W. J., 273
352, 353, 354, 356, 357, 358, Fontaine, J. R. J., 374 Freeman-Moir, D. J., 60, 90, 108,
359, 360, 361, 362, 363, 364, Ford, D. H., 11, 30, 156, 177, 179, 112, 494
365, 366, 367, 368, 369, 370, 181, 182, 195, 196, 211, 213, Frese, M., 522
371, 372, 375, 376, 377, 378, 219, 228, 248, 249, 259, 267, Freud, A., 144
379, 380, 381, 382, 383, 385, 270, 432, 442, 446, 481, 522, Freud, S., 94, 114, 115, 116, 122,
386, 387, 388, 389, 521, 532, 528, 622, 679, 902 379, 380, 508, 948, 982, 993
533, 944 Ford, D. Y., 881 Freund, A., 570, 575, 576, 578, 579,
Fischer, L., 752 Fordham, S., 835, 836, 845, 878, 584, 585, 590, 591, 592, 593,
Fishbein, H. D., 247 879, 880, 881, 882 594, 595, 597, 601, 602, 605,
Fishbein, M., 410, 522 Forehand, R., 922 606, 608, 609, 620, 622, 630,
Fisher, C. B., 12, 156, 837, 898, 902, Foreman, N., 242, 245 631, 632, 635, 636, 638, 641,
905 Forest, K. B., 675 915
Fisher, D. M., 275, 292 Forgas, J. P., 406, 412, 422 Frey, K. S., 640
Fisher, L. M., 646 Forster, J. M., 641 Freyd, J., 277
Fisk, A. D., 603 Forsythe, A. B., 500 Friebe, S., 987
Fisk, J. E., 604 Foshee, V. A., 921, 928 Friedel, J., 768
Fiske, A., 718, 729, 753, 768, 770 Foss, B. M., 153 Friedes, D., 241
Fiske, D. W., 13 Foster, C., 552 Friedman, H. S., 623, 638, 652
Fiske, J., 99 Foster, K., 927 Friedman, J. M., 226
Fiske, S. T., 639 Foster, W., 916 Friedman, S. L., 542
Fitch, W. T., 338 Foucault, M., 775 Frielingsdorf, K., 973, 990
Fitts, P. M., 296 Foulkes, D., 380 Frierson, T., 441
Fivush, R., 701, 745, 747 Foushee, R., 242 Fries, J. F., 603
Flammer, A., 945 Fowler, J., 952, 959, 960, 964, 965, Friesen, W. V., 373, 374, 629, 630
Flanagan, C., 429, 503 966, 988 Frieze, I. H., 546
Flanagan, O., 750 Fox, B. A., 498 Frijda, N. H., 321, 370, 371, 534,
Flanders, M., 296 Fox, C. M., 30, 350, 351, 352 718, 766
1010 Author Index

Frith, C. D., 606 Gariépy, J.-L., 219, 220, 240, 249, Getzels, J., 484
Frohring, W., 604 433, 447, 701 Geuze, R., 449
Frye, D., 844 Gärling, T., 526 Gewirtz, J. L., 150
Fujita, F., 633 Garmezy, N., 578, 637, 805, 902, Ghisletta, P., 589. 598, 606, 611,
Fukuyama, F., 900 905, 919, 924 615, 616, 617, 624
Fuligni, A., 717, 718, 723, 767, 772 Garraghty, P. E., 420 Giaever, G., 238
Fulker, D. W., 613, 615 Garrett, H. E., 616 Gianutsos, J., 7
Fumerton, R., 78, 79 Garrett, L., 223 Gibbons, F. X., 640, 696
Funder, D. C., 619 Garrett, P. B., 744 Gibbs, J., 348
Fung, H., 551, 741, 744, 745, 746, Gartner, J., 986 Gibson, E. J., 241, 246, 287, 303
748, 749, 754, 759, 761 Gärtner, K., 225 Gibson, H., 977
Furby, R., 589 Garver, S., 989 Gibson, J. J., 63, 65, 241, 246, 309,
Furlong, M., 917 Garvey, C., 139, 741 316, 321, 518, 519, 546
Furrow, J. L., 968, 971 Garvik, B., 419 Gibson, K. R., 247
Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., 5, 156, 449, Garwood, M., 627, 629 Gibson, W. A., 446
668, 680, 683, 685, 686, 700, Gaser, C., 609 Giddens, A., 526, 763
900, 901, 908 Gaskins, S., 741 Gierer, A., 260, 261, 262
Furth, H. G., 67 Gasser, M., 291 Gigerenzer, G., 171, 172, 596, 605,
Fussell, E., 672, 686 Gatenby, D., 289 623
Gatz, M., 630 Gignac, M. A. M., 595
Gabrielian, S., 333, 334 Gauvain, M., 321, 765, 766 Gilbert, D. T., 520, 554
Gadamer, H. G., 58, 59, 63, 64, 65, Ge, X., 428, 685, 696 Gilbert, J. A., 103
73, 75 Geertz, C., 518, 522, 738, 755, 763 Gilbert, S. F., 46
Gage, F. H., 236, 420 Gegeo, D. W., 743, 744 Gilkey, L., 490
Gaines, B., 538 Gehlen, A., 518, 522, 580 Gillespie, A., 199, 201
Galambos, N., 408, 905 Geiselmann, B., 576 Gillett, G., 414
Galanter, E., 522, 536 Gelman, R., 54, 263, 278, 333, 341, Gilligan, C., 492
Galitski, T., 402 342, 344, 345, 346, 364, 372, Gilmore, P., 782
Gallagher, D., 641 778 Gilovich, T., 35
Gallese, V., 49, 53 Gelman, S. A., 343, 775, 776 Gimbel, C., 688
Gallimore, R., 736, 773 Genishi, C., 745 Ginzberg, S., 871
Gallistel, C. R., 263 Gentner, D., 368 Giorgi, A., 465
Galton, F., 102, 122, 437 George, L., 652, 689, 701, 703, 985 Giot, L., 237
Gambone, M. A., 906, 911, 912, Geppert, U., 546 Glaser, B. G., 672
913, 914, 924, 927 Gerard, J. M., 922 Glaser, D., 416
Gandhi, M. K., 969 Gerber, G. K., 237, 238 Glaser, R., 582, 776
Gangestad, S., 438 Gergen, K. J., 31, 517, 527, 530, Glatthorn, A. A., 782
Gannon-Rowley, T., 701 550, 745, 750, 753 Glei, D., 924
Garbarino, J., 900 Gergen, M. M., 550 Gleitman, L., 282
Garcia, C. A., 225 Gerlai, R., 238 Glenberg, A., 284, 287, 303
Garcia, R., 60, 343, 532, 605 Gershkoff-Stowe, L., 276, 279, 291, Glendinning, A., 429
Garcia Coll, C., 1, 156, 416, 771, 292 Gleser, G. C., 446
772, 774 Gershman, E. S., 427 Glotzbach, S. F., 725
Garcia-Mila, M., 45, 72 Gerton, J., 835 Glueck, E., 699
Gardner, C. O., 705 Gerzén, M., 418 Glueck, S., 699
Gardner, H., 321, 343, 367, 484, Gesell, A., 132, 133, 134, 297, 947 Gmünder, P., 946, 952, 959, 961,
485 Gest, S. D., 156, 919, 924 963, 964, 965, 966, 978, 980,
Gardner, J., 243, 247, 469 Gestsdottir, S., 12, 13, 895 981, 988
Gardner, W. P., 498 Getz, K., 379 Gnos, C., 964
Author Index 1011

Gobet, F., 605, 609 Goossens, L., 604 Grant, R. J., 236
Gocayne, J. D., 223 Gopher, D., 602 Gray, E. K., 632
Goddard, C., 722, 732 Gopnik, A., 762 Gray, J., 289
Goddard, H. H., 103, 126 Gordon, C., 551, 622, 637 Gray, J. A., 629
Godin, A., 959, 974 Gordon, J., 688 Gray, J. R., 386
Goethe, J. W., 993 Gordon, W., 334 Gray, L., 245
Gogate, L., 284 Gore, J. C., 409 Gray, R. D., 219, 249
Goldberg, D. T., 883 Gore, S., 688 Grayling, A. C., 79
Goldberg, L. R., 604, 619, 621 Gorman, B. K., 703 Grebogi, C., 277
Goldberg, S., 153 Gorman-Smith, D., 926 Green, F. L., 548, 762
Goldberger, A. L., 273 Gorsuch, R., 944, 945, 948, 952, Green, M. C., 768, 770
Golden, G. Y., 655 953, 977, 978, 980, 984, 988 Green, S. H., 385
Goldfield, B. A., 348 Goss, T., 833, 834, 835, 846, 856, Greenberg, G., 2, 7, 36, 156
Goldfried, J., 986 859, 860, 861, 862, 867, 878, Greenberg, M. T., 896, 901, 926,
Goldfried, M. R., 928 879, 880, 881 932
Goldin, P. R., 409 Gotanda, N., 832, 867 Greenberger, E., 426
Goldin-Meadow, S., 213, 349, 364, Gottesman, I. I., 471, 613 Greenfield, P., 717, 718, 721, 723,
365, 745, 765 Gottfried, A. E., 503 751, 752, 755, 758
Goldman, A. I., 525 Gottlieb, A., 717, 720, 969 Greeno, J. G., 346
Goldman, D., 227 Gottlieb, D. E., 365 Greenough, W. T., 235, 236, 604
Goldman, R., 943, 958, 966, 973 Gottlieb, G., 7, 9, 27, 39, 40, 46, 53, Greenspan, R. J., 223, 224, 230
Goldman-Rakic, P., 281, 382, 384, 94, 95, 99, 111, 142, 156, 160, Greenwald, A. G., 315, 322, 327,
385 169, 170, 177, 178, 187, 210, 376, 379, 380, 410, 538, 619
Goldsmith, D., 725, 726 211, 215, 217, 218, 219, 220, Greenwold, M. A., 986
Goldsmith, H. H., 321 221, 222, 228, 231, 232, 233, Gregory, A., 12, 901
Goldstein, K., 187, 190 241, 242, 243, 244, 245, 246, Greve, W., 520, 523, 538, 539, 548,
Goldstein, M. H., 266 247, 248, 259, 296, 318, 341, 549, 551, 552, 553, 554, 578,
Goldstein, W., 725, 726, 756 408, 416, 417, 436, 449, 519, 579, 584, 589, 620, 634, 637,
Goldthorpe, J. H., 681 529, 584, 603, 605, 728, 867, 641, 949
Gollin, E. S., 527 902, 903 Griffin, J. J., Jr., 538
Gollwitzer, P. M., 522, 540, 544, Gottmann, J. M., 179 Griffin, P., 200
553, 584, 622, 639 Gould, O. N., 606 Griffiths, P. E., 219, 249
Göncü, A., 498, 499, 734, 741, 761 Gould, S., 6, 8, 42, 46, 92, 93, 94, Grimm, J., 572
Gone, J. P., 749 106, 116, 122, 222, 474, 506, Grisso, T., 837
González, N., 744 507, 508, 519, 529, 576, 605, Groeben, N., 523
Goode, W. J., 685 947, 986 Groffmann, K. I., 572
Goodenough, F. L., 125, 126, 127, Goulet, L. R., 569, 571, 573, 589, Groos, K., 508
135, 136, 147 604 Gross, C. G., 7
Goodfield, J., 59 Graber, M., 303 Gross, J. J., 630
Goodhouse, J., 702 Graf, P., 437, 597, 644 Gross, T. N., 831
Goodnow, J., 321, 413, 421, 441, Graham, M. A., 553 Grossberg, S., 318, 332, 357
535, 550, 717, 718, 723, 725, Graham, S., 8, 449 Grossman, J. B., 917, 926
742, 743, 752, 766, 767, 768, Gramer, M., 438 Grossmann, K., 739, 740, 757
769, 770, 773, 775, 776 Granger, R. C., 914 Grossmann, K. E., 739, 740, 757
Goodwin, B., 46, 261, 341, 451 Grangvist, P., 979 Grotevant, H. D., 38, 441, 496
Goodwin, C., 105, 742 Granic, I., 240 Gruber, H., 363, 367, 587
Goodwin, M. H., 745, 748 Granier-Deferre, C., 232 Grun, P., 227
Goodwyn, S. W., 54 Granott, N., 321, 349, 363, 364, 365, Grusec, J. E., 151
Goody, E., 736 366, 367, 368, 369 Guardo, C. J., 549
1012 Author Index

Guenther, F. H., 318, 357 Hakim-Larson, J., 633, 636 Haring, M. J., 551
Guerra, G., 185 Haldane, J. B. S., 225 Harkness, S., 430, 449, 526, 717,
Guilford, J. P., 486, 487 Hale, S., 601 737, 738, 763, 766
Guisinger, S., 753 Halevy, E., 58 Harlow, H. F., 151, 152
Gullotta, T. P., 928 Haley, A., 613 Harlow, R. E., 639
Gump, P. V., 833 Halfon, N., 668, 914 Harman, G., 78
Gunnar, M. R., 547 Halford, G. S., 323, 342, 344, 384, Harmon, R. J., 527
Güntürkün, O., 609 601 Harmon-Jones, E., 629
Guo, G., 700, 703 Hall, G. S., 91, 94, 96, 105, 106, Harms, E., 973
Guo, S. W., 231 107, 121, 123, 125, 129, 144, Harold, R. D., 905
Gurwitsch, A., 194 167, 383, 573, 946, 947 Harpalani, V., 830, 831, 833, 834,
Gustafson, S. B., 411, 427, 430, Hall, S., 876, 882 835, 838, 839, 841, 843, 845,
439, 447 Hallett, D., 27 846, 848, 856, 859, 860, 861,
Gut, U., 983 Hallowell, A. I., 762 862, 864, 865, 866, 867, 869,
Guthke, J., 603 Hallqvist, J., 668 870, 871, 878, 879, 880, 881,
Guthrie, D., 773 Halperin, D., 994 882, 883, 884, 885, 886, 887
Guthrie, E. R., 131, 143 Halpern, C. T., 246, 247 Harper, L. V., 134
Guthrie, R., 834 Halverson, H. M., 292 Harré, R., 49, 414, 451, 523
Gutiérrez, K. D., 766 Hamagami, F., 583, 589, 616 Harrington, H., 233, 668, 685
Gutiérrez Peña, E., 5, 156 Hamaguchi, E., 754 Harris, C., 930
Gutman, L. M., 917, 921 Hambrick, D. Z., 597, 602, 605, Harris, D. B., 569, 573, 580
Guttman, L., 350 609, 610 Harris, J. J., 881
Gutzmann, H., 603, 612 Hamburg, D. A., 899 Harris, K. M., 700, 920, 922
Hamburger, V., 92, 217, 252 Harris, P. L., 345, 959, 970, 971,
Haan, N., 574, 666 Hamer, D., 624 976
Haber, D., 919 Hamerle, R., 589 Harris, T. O., 424
Habermas, J., 66 Hamilton, M. A., 896, 897, 902, Harris, W. A., 235
Habermas, T., 915 909, 911, 918 Harrison, C. R., 612
Hadamard, T., 484 Hamilton, S., 896, 897, 902, 909, Harrison, J., 283
Haden, C. A., 701 911, 918, 927 Hart, D., 27, 49, 67, 69, 550, 752,
Haeckel, E., 93, 94, 123 Hammerstein, P., 575, 584, 587, 756, 757, 919, 945, 984
Hagan, J., 693 603, 623 Hart, L. M., 468
Hagen, E. H., 575, 584, 603 Han, J. J., 746 Hart, T., 296, 968, 969
Hager, I., 945 Hand, H. H., 350, 378 Hartel, G. D., 923
Hagerman, S., 536 Hanisch, H., 973 Hartelman, P. A., 357
Hagestad, G. O., 524, 550 Hanks, W. F., 742 Harter, S., 109, 154, 330, 332, 352,
Haggerty, K. P., 906 Hanmer, T. J., 492 365, 379, 411, 525, 526, 544,
Haggerty, R. J., 901 Hansen, D., 918 545, 546, 549, 633, 751, 753,
Hagne, I., 387 Hanson, N. R., 74, 75, 76, 343 757
Hahn, A., 927 Hansson, R. O., 591, 611, 644 Hartman, J. L., 419
Hahn, H., 602 Harachi, T. W., 906 Hartmann, T., 831, 846, 847, 862,
Hahn, S., 612 Harackiewicz, J. M., 543 905
Haidt, J., 692, 717, 718, 738, 756 Haraway, M. M., 7 Hartshorne, H., 137, 147, 983
Haig, J., 632 Hardel, R. A., 977 Hartup, W. W., 419, 427, 696
Haight, W., 741, 746, 749, 769 Harding, S., 36 Hartwell, L., 419
Hailman, J. P., 95, 121 Hardy-Brown, K., 498 Harvey, J. H., 641
Haith, M. M., 613 Hare, B. R., 842, 857 Harwood, R., 761, 762
Haken, H., 271 Hareven, T. K., 668, 670, 678 Hasher, L., 601
Author Index 1013

Hasin, D., 706 Heil, F. E., 525, 543, 550 Herzbrun, M., 978
Hastings, C. T., 620, 634, 638 Heiman, R. J., 718, 722, 723 Herzog, A. R., 619, 633
Hastings, T. J., 666 Hein, A., 54 Hess, B., 437
Hatano, G., 413, 421, 441, 765, 766, Hein, K., 895 Hess, R. D., 756
767, 768, 770, 775, 776, 777, Heine, S. H., 753, 756, 760 Hess, T. M., 606
778 Heine, S. J., 760 Hetherington, E. M., 1, 7, 10, 32, 33,
Hatchett, S. J., 762 Heinz, W. R., 680 36, 571, 574, 682, 808, 816,
Hathaway, W., 989 Heinze, H., 630 818, 837
Haugaard, J. J., 441 Hektner, J., 468 Hettema, P. J., 409
Hauser, M. D., 338 Held, R., 54 Hewlett, B. S., 737
Hauser, S., 471, 497 Helmchen, H., 11, 576, 614 Heydens-Gahir, H. A., 591, 595
Havighurst, R. J., 83, 469, 541, 573, Helmreich, R. L., 485 Hicinbothom, G., 213
586, 669, 698, 850, 869, 898 Helms-Erikson, H., 673 Hicks, D. A., 748
Hawkins, J. D., 695, 896, 897, 900, Helson, H., 423 Higgins, E. T., 327, 371, 525, 538,
901, 906, 915, 916, 925 Helson, R., 619, 633, 638 545, 549, 751, 752
Hay, D., 156, 968, 969 Helsper, W., 988 Higgins, R. L., 538, 539
Hayek, F. A., 517 Hempel, C. G., 73 Higuchi, S., 702, 706
Hayes, D. S., 427 Hencke, R., 316, 350, 378 Hilgard, E. R., 380
Hayes, J. R., 367 Henderson, B. B., 498 Hilgard, J., 128
Hayes-Roth, B., 542 Hendrickson-Smith, J., 930 Hill, P. C., 954
Hayes-Roth, F., 542 Hendry, L. B., 429 Hill, R., 670, 677, 684
Hayhoe, M., 287, 288 Henfry, A., 212 Hinde, R. A., 96, 156, 219, 249,
Hayley, J., 145 Hengst, J. A., 722, 742, 749, 778 421, 426, 445
Hayne, H., 286 Hennig, K., 984 Hintz, N., 913, 914, 927
Haynes, N. M., 913 Henri, V., 101, 102, 103, 118, 132 Hipp, T., 953
Hayward, M. D., 703 Henry, D. B., 926 Hirata, S., 169, 192
Hazan, C., 441 Henry, K. R., 231 Hirsch, J., 5, 7, 9, 11
He, A. W., 744 Heppen, J. B., 631 Hirschfeld, L. A., 343
Hearne, S., 155 Herbener, E., 688, 696 Hirschfield, P., 688
Hearst, E., 90 Herbst, D., 187 Hirschl, T. A., 682
Heath, A. C., 234, 702, 819 Herbst, J. H., 633 His, W., 211
Heath, S. B., 743, 745, 746, 749 Herder, J. G., 518 Hitlin, S., 679, 701
Heatherington, E. M., 915 Herdt, G., 717, 718 Hitschmann, E., 949
Heatherton, T. F., 544 Hergenhahn, B. R., 845 Hitzemann, R., 223, 224
Hebb, D. O., 469, 600 Herlitz, A., 611 Ho, D. Y., 752
Heber, M., 498 Herman, M., 697, 924 Ho, M.-W., 7, 225, 247
Heckhausen, H., 522, 544, 546, 547, Hermans, H. J., 201, 620, 638 Hoagwood, K., 12, 13, 837
548, 549 Hernandez, D. J., 696 Hobart, C. J., 633, 636
Heckhausen, J., 517, 528, 539, 548, Herrling, S., 919 Hobfoll, S. E., 593
551, 552, 578, 579, 586, 590, Herrmann, U., 296 Hobson, R. P., 38, 67
620, 621, 626, 631, 634, 635, Herrnstein, R. J., 7, 155 Hochstein, M., 668
640, 641, 671, 904 Herschberg, T., 900 Hock, H. S., 277
Heffler, B., 418 Hershberger, S. L., 613 Hodge, J. A., 227
Heft, H., 833 Hertenstein, M. J., 384 Hodgson, D. M., 628
Hegel, G. W. F., 34, 59, 61, 62, 530 Hertzman, C., 668, 684 Hofer, M., 152, 682
Heider, K., 373, 374, 376 Hertzog, C., 587, 589, 600, 604, Hofer, S. M., 340, 347, 364, 583,
Heidmets, M., 517 612, 613, 632, 688 597, 668, 682, 702
Heidrich, S. M., 640 Herzberg, P. Y., 641 Hofferth, S. L., 686
1014 Author Index

Hoffman, E. P., 225 Horney, K., 145 Hurford, J., 288


Hoffman, J. P., 688 Horowitz, F. D., 1, 7, 9, 10, 130, Hurlock, E. B., 132
Hoffman, L. W., 233 419, 856, 857 Husén, T., 432
Hoffman, M. L., 902 Horowitz, H., 54, 60 Husserl, E., 467
Hogan, A. E., 323, 332, 348, 385, Horvath, J. A., 606 Hutchings, M. E., 244
387 Horwitz, A. V., 703 Hutchins, E., 267
Hogan, D. P., 670 Houdé, O., 601 Huttenlocher, P. R., 282, 385, 602
Hogan, N., 298 House, J., 552, 683 Huxel, K., 946, 947
Hogan, R., 19, 80, 139 Howard, A., 717 Huxhold, O., 591, 597, 603
Hogarty, P. S., 345, 348, 387 Howe, G. W., 818 Huxley, J. S., 506
Hoge, D., 959, 978 Howrigan, G., 737 Huxley, T. H., 212
Höhn, E., 521 Hoyer, W. J., 597, 600 Hyde, K., 958, 973, 980
Holahan, C. K., 574, 638, 666, 693 Hrebickova, M., 620, 633 Hyman, J. B., 914
Holden, C., 413 Hsu, A. Y. C., 631 Hymes, D., 742
Holden, M. T. G., 183 Hua, J. Y., 320
Holland, D., 718, 745, 748, 749 Huang, L. N., 835 Iacono, W. G., 615
Holland, J. H., 362 Hubbard, E. M., 384 Ialongo, N., 917
Holland, P. W., 446 Hubbard, J. J., 919, 924 Ickes, W., 543
Holliday, B. G., 857 Hubel, D. H., 382, 422, 424 Ide, E., 760
Holliday, S. G., 492 Huber, H. P., 438 Ideker, T., 402
Hollingworth, H. L., 129, 573, 596 Hudley, E. V. P., 749, 769 Idler, E., 985
Hollister-Wagner, G. H., 921, 928 Hudspeth, W. J., 388 Ignatiev, N., 832
Holloway, B. E., 926 Huebner, A. J., 928 Iinuma, K., 387
Holmberg, D., 633 Hugdahl, K., 628, 629, 630 Ikegama, Y., 756
Holmes, J. G., 762 Hughes, D., 775, 898, 913 Imanishi, K., 179, 186
Holt, L. E., 131 Hughes, E., 669 Imazeki, H., 702, 706
Holt, R. A., 223 Huinink, J., 700 Immordino-Yang, M. H., 321, 323,
Hommel, B., 418, 436, 583, 596, Huizenga, H. M., 176, 177, 183, 325, 333, 335, 352, 353, 367,
597, 598, 599, 600, 602, 610, 184, 442, 607, 614, 685 388
616 Huizinga, J., 508 Inagaki, K., 766, 775, 777, 778
Honeycutt, H., 41, 179, 241, 416, Huizink, A. C., 415 Inghilleri, P., 471
417 Hull, C. L., 131, 143, 149, 408 Ingold, T., 41, 46, 49, 755, 762, 763
Honzik, M. E., 666 Hultsch, D. F., 603, 632 Ingvar, M. I., 604
Honzik, M. P., 574, 666 Humble, K., 415 Inhelder, B., 334, 532, 534
Hood, B., 302 Humle, T., 192 Inoue-Nakamura, N., 192
Hood, K. E., 156, 213, 214, 219, Humphreys, L. G., 613 Intieri, R. C., 598
230, 240, 249, 415, 482 Hundeide, K., 169 Ippolito, M. F., 925
Hood, L., 402 Hundert, E. M., 62, 79 Irby, M., 900, 906, 911, 912
Hood, R., 944, 945, 948, 952, 953, Huneycutt, W. S., 244 Irion, J. C., 636
977, 978, 980, 984, 988, 990 Hung Chan, Y., 752 Irizarry, N. L., 761
Hoogstra, L., 717, 743, 744, 749, Hunsberger, B., 944, 945, 948, 952, Irvine, J. J., 839, 858
761 953, 977, 978, 980, 984, 988 Irvine, S., 413
Hooker, C. A., 197 Hunsberger, L., 978 Irwin, C. E., 496
Hooker, K., 620, 636 Hunt, D. E., 425 Isaacowitz, D. M., 579, 620, 630,
Hopkins, B., 416, 449 Hunt, J. M., 153, 345, 350, 384, 387, 639
Hoppe-Graff, S., 44 422 Isabella, R. A., 500
Hoppe-Rooney, T., 898 Hunt, S. P., 216, 247 Ishii, K., 769
Horn, J. L., 127, 132, 340, 347, 364, Hunter, J. P., 918 Israel, Y., 237
417, 583, 597, 600, 613, 618 Huntington, S. P., 945, 994 Ivey, P. K., 791
Author Index 1015

Ivinson, G., 201 Jensen, D. D., 130 Jones, S., 259, 604
Iwanaga, M., 735 Jensen, L. A., 718, 721, 738 Jones-Walker, C., 831
Izard, C. E., 62, 63, 499 Jensen, P., 12, 13 Jopling, D., 751
Izzo, L. D., 427 Jernigan, T. L., 213, 214 Jordan, J., 579
Jersild, A. T., 126, 128 Jörns, K. P., 985
Jaakola, R., 975 Jersild, C. L., 126 Josephs, I. E., 199
Jackendoff, R., 61 Jessberger, S., 236 Josephs, R. A., 758
Jacklin, C. N., 422 Jessor, R., 425, 718, 722, 901, 921, Josephsson, S., 612
Jackson, C., 921, 928 923 Joshi, H., 682
Jackson, D. C., 627, 628, 629, 630 Jessor, S. L., 425 Joyner, E. T., 913
Jackson, D. D., 145 Jiang, C. H., 255 Julien, J.-P., 223
Jackson, J., 54, 670, 762 Jiang, M., 237 Jung, C. G., 479, 949
Jacob, F., 437 Jihui, Y., 821 Juraska, J. M., 236
Jacobs, C., 833, 846, 856, 859, 860, Joanen, T., 231 Just, M. A., 601
861, 862 Joas, H., 190
Jacobs, F., 156, 169, 918 Joe, S., 838 Kaas, J. H., 283
Jacobs, J., 103 Johanson, D. C., 507, 508 Kafatos, F. C., 440
Jacobson, K., 263, 703 Johansson, B., 615 Kagan, J., 5, 49, 378, 415, 417, 438,
Jacobson, M., 221 Johansson, S. E., 434 442, 523, 527, 528, 531, 546,
Jaeger, D. A., 689 Johansson, T., 430 549, 574, 584, 612, 613, 619,
Jahoda, G., 200, 716, 718, 741 John, E. R., 388 622, 623, 628, 644, 666, 685,
Jahrsdoerfer, R., 245 John, O., 618, 634 811
Jain, E., 630 Johnson, C. N., 970, 976 Kager, A., 964, 966
James, W., 68, 69, 111, 123, 161, Johnson, D., 849 Kagitcibasi, C., 168
409, 411, 412, 414, 466, 497, Johnson, D. J., 857 Kahana, B., 526
502, 509, 549, 731, 951, 974, Johnson, D. R., 682 Kahana, E., 526
983 Johnson, F., 753 Kahlbaugh, P., 494
Janet, P., 188, 189 Johnson, M., 48, 54, 287, 319, 340, Kahn, R. L., 551, 580, 587
Janicki, D., 978 506, 573, 586, 587 Kahneman, D., 540
Janis, I. L., 698 Johnson, M. E., 667, 668, 674, 675 Kahoe, R. D., 985
Janney, J. E., 129, 573 Johnson, M. H., 10, 279, 303, 306, Kail, R., 597, 601
Janoff-Bulman, R., 553, 639 418, 597, 602, 605, 613 Kainz, H. P., 54, 59
Janson, H., 411 Johnson, M. K., 357, 691 Kakar, S., 468, 718, 749, 762
Jantsch, E., 178 Johnson, M. L., 348, 383 Kalicki, B., 550
Jaret, C., 832 Johnson, P. M., 636 Kalin, N. H., 627, 628, 629, 630
Jarjoura, G. R., 682 Johnson, T. E., 576 Kalverboer, A. F., 416, 449
Jarrett, R. L., 832, 835, 856 Johnson, T. R. B., 629 Kamin, L. J., 127, 129
Järvelä, S., 412 Johnson-Laird, P. N., 500 Kaminski, G., 533
Jarvis, G., 994 Johnson-Powell, G., 831 Kamlah, W., 555
Jasievicz, J., 438 Johnston, T. D., 219, 222, 226, 228, Kamm, K., 294
Jeannerod, M., 298 242, 246, 249, 704 Kandel, D. B., 427, 695
Jeeves, M., 410 Jolly, A., 507 Kandel, E. R., 441
Jelicic, H., 6 Jones, D. L., 881 Kane, M. J., 601, 602, 603
Jenkins, E., 321, 325, 365 Jones, E., 115, 116 Kanfer, F. H., 536
Jenkins, J. J., 100 Jones, G., 605, 609 Kanowski, S., 576
Jenkins, J. M., 820 Jones, H. E., 127, 131, 598 Kant, I., 61, 339, 368
Jenkins, W. M., 283 Jones, J., 920, 922 Kanwisher, N., 345
Jennings, H. S., 102, 121 Jones, M. C., 126, 130, 131, 134, Kaplan, A., 74
Jennings, S., 378 666 Kaplan, B., 60, 190, 432, 951, 966
1016 Author Index

Kaplan, M. S., 838 Kelvin, P., 438 King, A. P., 218, 266
Kaprio, J., 428 Kemmelmeier, M., 750 King, P. E., 932, 944, 952, 971
Kapstein, M., 750 Kempermann, G., 236, 420, 584, King, P. M., 333, 358, 386
Karasawa, M., 761 604, 610, 612 King, V., 821
Kardiner, A., 833, 835, 842, 867, Kendler, H. H., 154 Kingdom, D., 989
879 Kendler, K. S., 702, 705 Kingstone, A., 242
Karli, P., 408, 411 Kendler, T. S., 154, 348 Kinney, L., 629
Karmel, B. Z., 243, 247 Kennedy, B., 320, 329, 330, 332, Kinney, M. D., 152
Karmiloff-Smith, A., 10, 43, 61, 348, 349, 354, 356, 357, 358, Kinoshita, T., 702, 706
279, 303, 306, 357, 597, 605, 359, 360, 361, 362, 438 Kiraly, M., 237
777 Kennel, J., 839, 844, 890, 891 Kirkpatrick, J., 499, 717, 718
Karoly, P., 536, 537, 544, 641 Kenny, P., 242, 245 Kirkpatrick, L., 979
Kasai, H., 235 Kent, R. D., 266 Kirkwood, T. B. L., 225, 576, 597,
Kaschak, M., 284, 287, 303 Kent, T., 66 623, 628
Kashiwagi, K., 756 Kerckhoff, A. C., 680, 681 Kirschner, S. R., 199
Kastenbaum, R., 541 Kermoian, R., 384 Kirshner, B., 918
Kasulis, T. P., 753 Kern, D., 539 Kirson, D., 372, 373
Kath, W. L., 320 Kerr, J., 153 Kitayama, S., 354, 375, 717, 718,
Katz, B. B., 240 Kerr, M., 406, 410, 425, 428, 430 722, 723, 730, 749, 752, 753,
Katz, D., 139 Kertzer, D. I., 668, 672, 674 754, 756, 757, 758, 760, 762,
Katz, L. F., 688 Kessel, F., 717, 718, 723, 725, 742, 763, 769, 859, 860
Katz, R., 139 764, 766, 776 Kitchener, K. S., 332, 333, 351, 358,
Katzir-Cohen, T., 335 Kesselring, F., 520 386
Kaube, H., 606 Kessen, W., 94, 96, 107, 733, 764 Kitchener, R. F., 264, 347
Kauffman, S., 46, 47, 318, 320, 341, Kessler, R. C., 552, 573, 702 Kivnick, H. Q., 493
362 Kestenbaum, V., 466, 467, 468, 502 Klackenberg, G., 426, 450
Kaufman, J., 409, 689 Keverne, E. B., 216, 247 Klackenberg-Larsson, I., 411
Kaufman, S., 689 Key, C. B., 127 Klaczynski, P. A., 429
Kavsek, M., 612 Keyes, C. L. M., 633, 895, 896 Klaghofer, R., 978
Kawai, M., 169, 192 Keyes, S., 811 Klahr, D., 339, 346, 364
Kawamura, S., 169 Kidd, J. R., 227 Klauer, T., 551, 626, 634
Keefer, C., 735, 737, 738, 739 Kidd, K. K., 227 Klaus, C. R., 636
Kegan, R., 474, 475, 478, 482, 497, Kielsmeier, J. C., 919 Klaus, M., 839, 844, 890, 891
545 Kiesner, J., 428 Klaus, P. H., 891
Keil, F., 533, 534, 775, 776, 777, Kihlbom, M., 434 Klebanov, P. K., 682
959, 975 Kihlstrom, J. F., 410, 549, 751 Klein, M. W., 695
Keith, J., 13, 668, 672, 674, 898, Kim, C. J., 238 Klein, R., 538, 545
900 Kim, H., 758 Klem, A. M., 911, 914, 924, 927
Kellam, S., 917 Kim, K. J., 688 Klepsch, M., 972
Keller, E. F., 236 Kim, S. K., 237 Kliegl, R., 530, 577, 585, 591, 593,
Keller, H., 717, 718, 723 Kim, S. Y., 696 601, 602, 603, 611, 612
Keller, M., 526 Kim, U., 758 Kline, J. P., 630
Keller, T. A., 601 Kim, Y. K., 760 Kling, J., 688, 689
Kellman, P. J., 241 Kimball, C., 988 Klinger, E., 536, 538, 540, 552, 553
Kelly, G. A., 490 Kimball, M., 906 Klintsova, A., 235
Kelly, J., 837 Kindermann, T., 173, 179 Klix, F., 611, 623
Kelso, J. A. S., 273, 275, 276, 277, King, A., 265, 290 Kloep, M., 428
282, 306, 320, 403, 437 King, A. J., 244 Klopfer, P. H., 95, 121
Author Index 1017

Klosinski, G., 987 Kramer, T., 741 Kuhl, P. K., 241


Kluckhohn, C., 7 Krampe, R., 415, 585, 591, 594, Kuhlen, J. E., 129
Knight, C. C., 329, 334, 335, 349, 597, 601, 602, 604, 605, 606, Kuhlen, R. G., 573
351, 378, 379 607, 609, 618 Kuhn, C. M., 231
Knorr Cetina, K., 170 Krampen, G., 522, 525, 543, 548, Kuhn, D., 45, 72, 213, 292, 494, 590
Knudsen, E. I., 244 550 Kuhn, T., 2, 76, 340, 343
Knudsen, P. F., 244 Krasnogorski, N. I., 130 Kukla, A., 532
Koch, C., 282 Kratz, C., 739 Kulick, D., 744
Koenig, H., 985 Krause, S. R., 323 Kumpfer, K. L., 901, 926, 932
Koeske, R. D., 605 Krauss, S. P., 629 Kunda, Z., 523, 538, 554, 555
Koestler, A., 421 Kray, J., 602 Kuner, W., 988
Kofsky, E., 334, 351 Krebs, D. L., 179 Kunzmann, U., 579, 607, 608, 609,
Kogan, N., 632 Krebs, H. A., 181, 182 626, 630, 635
Kohlberg, L., 112, 155, 332, 333, Krecker, M. L., 672 Kuo, H. H., 139
343, 348, 427, 528, 531, 533, Krein, S. F., 694 Kuo, Y., 489
534, 549, 966, 983 Kreipe, R. E., 926 Kuo, Z.-Y., 46, 96, 99, 141, 142,
Kohli, M., 586 Kreppner, K., 118, 406, 425, 426 219, 221, 240, 264, 265, 408
Kohn, M. L., 606, 815 Kretzmann, J., 906, 913, 914 Kuperminc, G. P., 688, 919
Koivisto, P., 448 Krewer, B., 200 Kupersmidt, J. B., 335, 427
Kojima, H., 182, 499 Kripal, J., 718 Kupkova, M., 320
Kokko, K., 550 Kris, E., 479 Kupperbusch, C. S., 631
Kolaric, G. C., 408 Kristel, O. V., 768, 770 Kurata, J. H., 982
Kolb, D. A., 467 Kristeva, J., 774 Kurtz, K. J., 368
Koller, S., 717, 718, 738, 756 Kroh, O., 947 Kurtz, S. N., 718
Komatsushiro, M., 387 Krueger, A. C., 311 Kushner, H., 971
Kondo, D., 753 Krueger, F., 186 Kusserow, A. S., 761
Konishi, M., 219 Krueger, J., 539, 640, 904 Küster, U., 546
Konner, M. J., 738 Kruger, A. C., 751, 762, 778 Kwan, V. S. Y., 618, 632
Kono, H., 702, 706 Kruger, H., 680 Kwilecki, S., 951, 967, 968
Koopmans, J. R., 702 Krüger, L., 172 Kyin, M., 702
Kopp, C. B., 546, 549 Kruglanski, A. W., 537, 541, 555,
Kornfein, M., 725 583, 584 Labouvie, G. V., 584, 590, 600, 603
Kosawa, Y., 756, 759 Kruhl, J. H., 320 Labouvie-Vief, G., 492, 493, 578,
Koshland, D. E., Jr., 225 Krupat, E., 423 579, 580, 581, 582, 583, 584,
Koshy, S., 868 Krus, D. J., 334 597, 619, 621, 629, 633, 636
Koslowski, B., 499, 545 Kruse, A., 587 Lachicotte, W., Jr., 745, 748, 749
Kosslyn, S. M., 395, 628, 629, 630 Krushel, L., 216 Lachman, M., 551, 571, 573, 609,
Kosterman, R., 923, 925 Kuang, B., 237 620, 634, 635, 636, 655
Kottak, C. P., 856 Kubey, R., 184 LaCrosse, J., 427, 528
Kottler, M. L., 213 Kubisch, A. C., 915, 932 Ladd, G. W., 670
Kouba, E., 741 Kubovy, M., 242 Lademann-Priemer, G., 987
Kouzes, J. M., 485 Kuczynski, L., 422, 424 Ladner, J. A., 835
Kovach, J. K., 247 Kuebli, J., 747 Ladson-Billings, G., 835, 839, 872
Kovacs, S. L., 54 Kuenne, M. R., 154 LaFromboise, T. D., 835
Kracke, B., 412 Kugler, P. N., 273, 274 Lagercrantz, H., 424
Kraemer, P., 242 Kuh, D., 668, 692 Laing, R. D., 54
Kramer, A. F., 602, 604, 612 Kuhl, J., 522, 536, 544 Laird, J. L., 172
Kramer, D. A., 492, 495 Kühl, K.-P., 603, 612 Lakatos, I., 70, 74, 76
1018 Author Index

Lakoff, G., 54, 287, 319, 340, 344, Layton, W. M., Jr., 225 228, 232, 240, 248, 249, 259,
369, 373, 506 Lazarus, R. S., 321, 370, 371, 408, 267, 270, 316, 347, 348, 408,
Lal, S., 871 540, 552, 635, 641, 655 409, 415, 416, 418, 421, 425,
Laland, K. N., 7 Leadbeater, B. J., 688, 751 426, 428, 432, 436, 442, 471,
Lalonde, C. E., 27 Leahy, R., 751 477, 481, 490, 491, 492, 506,
Lamb, M., 4, 321, 486, 547 Leavitt, H. J., 485 507, 508, 517, 519, 526, 527,
Lambert, W., 980 Lebiere, C., 339, 340, 347 528, 529, 530, 535, 569, 570,
Lamborn, S. D., 333 Lebra, T. S., 718, 753, 758 571, 574, 575, 580, 581, 582,
Lamiell, J. T., 183 Lecanuet, J. P., 232 585, 587, 589, 603, 614, 621,
Lamkin, C. A., 246 Lecci, R., 641 622, 623, 626, 635, 644, 669,
Lampert, M., 769 Lecours, A. R., 385 679, 701, 705, 798, 819, 895,
Lampl, M., 348, 383 LeDoux, J., 48, 412, 415 896, 898, 900, 901, 902, 903,
Lancy, D., 741 Lee, B., 546 904, 905, 915, 918, 919, 920,
Land, K. C., 685 Lee, C., 831, 835, 838, 859, 869, 930, 943, 948, 971, 976
Landau, B., 282 870 Leshner, A. I., 449
Landesman, C., 522 Lee, S. M., 226 Lesser, G., 848
Landy, F. J., 423 Lee, T. I., 237, 238 Leuba, J., 952
Lane, J., 190 Lee, Y., 760, 761 Leung, C., 760
Lane, P. C. R., 605, 609 Leffert, N., 12, 896, 900, 901, 905, Leung, M.-C., 155
Lang, F. R., 570, 580, 582, 591, 593, 906, 908, 911, 912, 913, 914, Levelt, W. J. M., 583
595, 597, 604, 611, 671 915, 918, 920, 921, 922, 923, Levenson, R. W., 627, 629, 630,
Langdon, A., 959 924, 925, 928, 929, 930 631, 638
Langer, E. J., 808, 809 Lefford, A., 241 Leventhal, H., 63
Langer, J., 54, 189 Lehman, D. R., 753, 756, 760 Leventhal, T., 13
Langer, S., 97 Lehman, H. C., 601 Levi-Montalcini, R., 433
Langman, J., 912 Lehrman, D. S., 46, 152, 155, 219, Levin, H., 149
Langston, C. A., 551 226, 264, 415 Levin, J. S., 985
Lanspery, S., 927 Leichtman, M. D., 746, 759 LeVine, B. B., 735
Lanz, P., 523 Leiderman, P. H., 735, 737, 738, Levine, J. M., 587
Lariviere, W. R., 235 739 LeVine, L. E., 751
Larsen, A., 425, 426 Leisering, L., 680 LeVine, R., 356, 413, 421, 441, 469,
Larson, D. B., 986 Leisse, T. J., 238 717, 718, 723, 725, 733, 735,
Larson, R., 184, 485, 496, 897, 900, Lemerise, E. A., 25 737, 738, 739, 740, 754, 759,
901, 902, 912, 918 Leneuaer, M., 987 762
Latash, M. L., 296 Lenk, H., 517, 523, 532 Levine, S., 245, 416, 990
Latham, G. P., 544 Leont’ev, A. N., 188, 200, 522, LeVine, S., 735, 737, 738, 739
Latour, B., 32, 37, 39 533, 546 Levins, R., 84
Laub, J. H., 5, 156, 672, 683, 685, Leopold, L., 226 Levinson, B., 603
687, 693, 695, 698, 699, 704, LePore, P. C., 688 Levinson, D. J., 469, 550, 619, 670
705 Lepper, M. R., 860 Lévi-Strauss, C., 468
Laudan, L., 70, 74, 76, 79 Leppert, J., 415 Levy, R. I., 717, 718
Laufer, R. S., 670 Lerner, J. S., 768, 770 Lewin, K., 2, 14, 118, 145, 146, 181,
Launier, R., 552 Lerner, J. V., 12, 13, 425, 477, 526, 182, 194, 195, 268, 402, 432,
Laursen, B., 696 623, 895, 915 442, 669, 725, 865
Lautrey, J., 353, 590 Lerner, R. M., 1, 2, 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, Lewis, C. C., 754, 756, 759
Lave, J., 200, 546, 717, 718, 723, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 21, 23, 30, Lewis, J., 177
738, 770, 776 36, 39, 43, 51, 52, 156, 169, Lewis, M., 27, 62, 154, 195, 240,
Lawrence, J. A., 190, 413, 549, 576, 177, 179, 180, 181, 182, 195, 280, 447, 499, 545, 546, 549,
597, 644 196, 199, 211, 213, 219, 226, 751, 771
Author Index 1019

Lewis, S., 927 Lindenthal, J. J., 995 Lovallo, W. R., 627, 628, 629
Lewkowicz, D. J., 26, 27, 240, 241, Lindzey, G., 144 Lövdén, M., 606, 611, 612, 613,
242, 243, 245, 247, 469 Link, B. G., 683, 704 614, 615, 616
Lewontin, R., 7, 10, 42, 44, 46, 47, Linville, P. W., 554, 634 Love, G. D., 636
53, 84, 225, 507, 508, 519 Lipan, O., 238 Lovejoy, A. O., 56
Leyendecker, B., 762 Lipp, H. P., 238 Lowe, E. D., 717
Leykin, I., 238 Lippitt, R., 146 Lowe, J. C., 674
Lezza, A. M. S., 227 Lippman, L., 895, 901, 932 Lowe, R. C., 334, 351
Li, F., 685 Lipps, T., 199 Lu, H. H., 672
Li, J., 322, 356, 370, 372, 375, 376, Lipsitt, L. P., 128, 527, 572, 575, Lubart, T. I., 353
746, 769 580, 581, 582, 586 Luborsky, E., 377
Li, K. Z. H., 595, 597, 600, 602 Little, B. R., 541 Luborsky, L., 376, 377
Li, P. W., 7, 9 Little, T., 630, 635, 636 Luckmann, T., 199
Li, R. M., 930 Little, T. D., 442, 668 Lucy, J. A., 718
Li, S.-C., 408, 418, 420, 436, 451, Little, T. O., 757 Ludwig, J., 5, 156, 688
570, 574, 575, 576, 578, 583, Littman, R. A., 90 Luhmann, N., 32
584, 585, 586, 590, 591, 594, Liu, W. T., 835 Lukas, K. E., 602
595, 596, 597, 598, 599, 600, Liu, X., 706 Lukes, S., 753
601, 604, 603, 608, 609, 610, Lloyd, D., 242, 703 Lumley, M. A., 629
616, 628, 632, 644, 743 Lockard, J. S., 624 Lumsdaine, A. A., 698
Li, X., 930 Locke, B. Z., 637 Lumsdaine, M. H., 698
Li, Y., 237 Locke, E. A., 544 Lumsden, C. J., 247
Liang, C., 745, 746, 759 Locke, J., 337, 414 Lund, J., 237
Liben, L. S., 54 Lockhart, D. J., 255 Lundberg, U., 417
Licht, R., 388 Loeb, J., 146 Lupski, J. R., 225
Lickliter, R., 39, 41, 179, 213, 240, Loeber, R., 695 Luria, A. R., 31, 66, 139, 412, 413,
241, 242, 243, 244, 245, 246, Loehlin, J. C., 633, 866 441, 449, 519, 522, 544, 546
247, 284, 416, 417, 903 Loevinger, J., 497, 620 Lüscher, K., 679
Lidberg, L., 415 Löfgren, L., 450 Luster, T., 803, 805, 806
Lieberman, M., 348 Logie, L., 972 Luthar, S. S., 697, 835, 836, 841,
Liebman, J., 688, 689 Lohmann, H., 777 842, 869, 887, 905, 915, 916
Liebow, E., 833 Loiselle, L., 918 Lütkenhaus, P., 545, 751
Lienert, G. A., 446, 615 Lonczak, H. S., 896, 897, 900, Lutz, C., 717, 718
Lifton, R. J., 750 916, 925 Lykken, D. T., 614, 624
Lightfoot, C., 167 London, I. D., 248 Lynam, D., 408
Lillard, A., 752 Long, D., 974, 975 Lynch, C. L., 332, 351, 358, 386
Lin, L., 746 Looman, C. W., 684 Lynch, E., 777
Lin, N., 703 Loomis, A. M., 135 Lynch, J., 668
Lindberg, L. D., 923 Loomis, S. L., 703 Lynch, M., 245
Lindberg, U., 406, 434 Lopez, D., 608 Lynd, H. M., 667
Linden, M., 576, 579, 635, 640 Lord, S., 506, 821 Lynd, R. S., 667
Lindenberger, U., 11, 167, 405, 418, Lorenz, F. O., 685, 688 Lynn, S., 991
436, 442, 569, 570, 571, 572, Lorenz, K., 142, 226, 235, 264, Lyons, J. S., 986
577, 583, 585, 589, 590, 591, 506, 903 Lyons, N. P., 492
595, 596, 597, 598, 599, 600, Lorion, R. P., 896 Lyons-Ruth, K., 377
601, 602, 604, 605, 606, 608, Lotman, J. M., 200 Lyra, M. C. D. P., 195, 362
609, 610, 611, 612, 613, 614, Lott, M. T., 227
615, 616, 617, 618, 626, 627, Louis, B., 603 Maarouf-Veray, F., 227, 228
634, 904, 944 Lourenço, O., 77, 342, 343, 606 Maas, I., 11, 579, 636, 641
1020 Author Index

Maccoby, E. E., 1, 7, 10, 33, 36, Maher, B. A., 143 Markus, H., 354, 375, 413, 421, 441,
149, 152, 419, 422, 425, 426, Maher, W. B., 143 545, 549, 618, 619, 633, 717,
496, 915 Mahoney, J., 156, 416, 420, 620, 925 718, 722, 723, 730, 749, 750,
MacCombie, D. J., 213, 214 Maier, G. W., 541 752, 753, 754, 756, 757, 758,
MacDonald, G. B., 932 Maier, N. R. F., 2 760, 761, 762, 763, 859, 860
MacDonald, K., 811 Maier, S. F., 131, 435 Marquis, D. B., 131
MacDonald, S. W. S., 603 Main, M., 377, 499, 545, 816 Marquis, J., 668
Mace, F. C., 537 Maitland, S. B., 598 Marriott, M., 468
Mace, W., 306 Major, B., 749 Marrow, A. J., 796
Macfarlane, J. W., 666 Malatesta, C. W., 62 Marrs, G. S., 385
Machado, A., 77, 342, 343, 606 Malatesta, C. Z., 62 Marschak, M., 521, 541
Maciel, A. G., 608, 621 Malinowski, B., 721 Marsh, H. W., 634
Maciel, D. A., 193 Mameli, M., 41 Marshall, E., 238
MacIntyre, A., 476 Mamman, Z., 706 Marshall, V. W., 680
Mack, K. J., 247 Mandelbaum, D. G., 741, 743 Marsiske, M., 552, 570, 578, 579,
Mack, P. A., 247 Mandl, H., 587 580, 582, 583, 591, 592, 594,
Mackenbach, J. P., 684 Mandler, G., 63 595, 597, 606, 611, 612, 625,
MacKenzie, M. J., 448 Mandler, J. M., 61, 604 627, 631, 637, 638, 639, 641,
MacKie, M., 988 Mangelsdorf, S., 751, 757 644, 671
MacLeod, J., 881 Mangold, O., 213 Martin, A., 409
Macmillan, R., 701 Manke, B., 32 Martin, D. I. K., 225
Macomber, J., 263 Manly, B. F., 446 Martin, G. M., 576
Madden, P. A. F., 702 Manners, J., 638 Martin, J. A., 496
Maddox, S. J., 921 Mannes, M., 896, 901, 906, 908, Martin, J. E., 953
Madge, N., 690 911, 913, 914, 915, 921, 924, Martin, L., 763
Madsen, M. C., 902 927 Martin, L. L., 541, 555
Madsen, R., 755, 756 Manor, O., 683 Martin, L. R., 623, 638, 652
Maehr, M., 503, 505, 506 Manstead, A. S. R., 540 Martin, N. G., 702
Maercker, A., 587, 607, 608, 609 Manturzewska, M., 441 Martin-Barbero, J., 774, 775
Maffei, M., 226 Mao, L., 761 Martini, M., 499, 767
Maggs, J. L., 408 Maraganore, A., 328, 381 Marton, F., 423
Magnus, K., 632 Marcelis, M., 702 Maruna, S., 687
Magnus, P., 819 Marcet, B., 227, 228 Marvin, R., 735
Magnuson, K. A., 5, 156 Marchant, G. J., 922 Marx, J. L., 260
Magnusson, D., 6, 7, 11, 14, 36, 37, Marcia, J. E., 496 Mascolo, M. F., 521
39, 49, 156, 202, 219, 249, 401, Marcoen, A., 604 Mascolo, M. J., 322, 370, 372, 376
403, 404, 405, 406, 407, 408, Marcovitch, S., 26, 27 Masiello, T., 749
411, 413, 415, 416, 417, 418, Marcus, D. E., 549 Maslow, A., 467, 476, 479, 753, 986
419, 420, 422, 423, 425, 427, Marcus, G. F., 320 Mason, W. A., 99, 152
430, 431, 432, 433, 434, 435, Mare, R. D., 683, 688 Massimini, F., 471
438, 439, 440, 442, 443, 444, Mareschal, D., 345 Masten, A. S., 621, 718, 905, 919,
445, 446, 447, 448, 449, 450, Margolis, J., 53 924
535, 571, 575, 582, 587, 589, Marini, M. M., 674 Masters, J. C., 152
611, 621, 626, 642, 644, 668 Markam, S. S., 374 Matas, L., 501
Magnusson, R., 387 Markey, F. V., 126 Mateer, F., 98, 102, 119, 130
Magovcevic, M., 190 Markman, E., 548 Mathew, A., 293
Maguire, M. C., 673 Marks, L., 241, 977, 978 Mathis, C., 223
Mahapatra, M., 718, 721, 732, 739, Markstrom-Adams, C., 835, 838, Matousek, M., 388
756, 760 841, 842, 844, 850, 871 Matseoane, K., 706
Author Index 1021

Matsuba, M. K., 945, 984 McClintock, M. K., 415 Mead, G. H., 65, 113, 190, 494, 501,
Matsumoto, H., 375, 757, 760, 763 McCord, J., 413, 427 669, 762, 798
Matsushita, S., 702, 706 McCormack, M. A., 217 Mead, M., 718, 733
Matsuzaki, M., 235 McCrae, R. R., 378, 488, 619, 620, Meadow, M. J., 946, 985
Matsuzawa, T., 177, 192 621, 632, 633, 635, 637 Meakin, P., 261
Matthews, S., 683 McCrone, J., 413 Meaney, M. J., 236
Mattick, H. W., 699 McCubbin, J. A., 231 Medawar, P., 416, 576
Maturana, H., 181, 520, 582 McCubin, H. I., 425, 426 Medin, D., 537, 717, 729, 777
Matute-Bianchi, M. E., 843 McDade, L., 244 Medina, N., 438
Maughan, B., 691 McDonough, P., 683 Medler, M., 620, 622, 634
Maurer, D., 283 McDougall, W., 119, 130 Mees, U., 534
Mauro, V. P., 216 McEwen, B. S., 419 Meguro, Y., 700
May, A., 609, 995 McFadden, S. H., 953, 985 Mehta, P., 860, 861, 862, 863
May, M. A., 813, 983 McFarland, C., 540 Meigs, A., 183
May, M. S., 137, 147 McGeer, T., 287 Meinz, E. J., 602
Mayer, A. K., 634, 640 McGrath, S. K., 613 Meissner, W. W., 949
Mayer, K. U., 11, 551, 571, 573, McGraw, A. P., 768 Mel, B. W., 320
584, 586, 589, 615, 671, 674, McGraw, M., 128, 135 Mellenbergh, G. J., 431
680, 681, 700 McGraw, M. B., 140, 142 Meller, J., 438
Maynard, A., 717, 718, 723 McGraw, M. G., 281 Mellner, C., 448
Mayr, E., 56, 402, 403, 420, 433, McGue, M., 226, 233, 615, 624, 702 Mellon, R., 242, 243
434, 451 McGuire, A. M., 701 Mellott, D., 315, 322, 327, 376, 379,
Mayr, U., 585, 601, 602, 629, 630 McGuire, C. U., 549 380
Mazur, E., 340 McGuire, C. V., 354 Meltzoff, A. N., 240, 241
Mazza, J. J., 906 McGuire, M. T., 415 Memmo, M., 898, 901, 906
Mazziotta, J. C., 602 McGuire, S., 32 Mendelson, M. J., 241
McAdams, D. P., 620 McGuire, W. J., 354, 549 Meng, K., 745
McAdoo, H. P., 762 McHale, S. N., 673 Menon, U., 717, 718, 762
McArdle, C. G., 881, 882 McIntosh, D. N., 990 Meredith, M. A., 240, 244, 283, 589
McArdle, J. J., 5, 156, 583, 589, McIntosh, P., 832, 836, 839, 869 Merleau-Ponty, M., 48, 467
611, 612, 616, 624 McIntosh, W. D., 555 Mernissi, F., 184
McAuley, E., 612 McKenna, J. J., 725 Merrick, S., 377
McBride, T., 244, 245 McKenry, P. C., 929 Merton, R. K., 667, 670, 671
McCabe, A., 745 McKinney, M. L., 222 Merzenich, M. M., 283
McCall, R. B., 30, 345, 348, 387, McKinnon, J., 816 Meschke, L. L., 442
433, 448, 530 McKnight, J., 913 Mesquita, B., 718, 759, 766
McCall, R. M., 600, 601, 612 McLanahan, S. S., 687, 694 Messick, S., 80
McCarthy, D., 138, 139, 140 McLaughlin, G. H., 323 Metcalfe, J., 602
McCarthy, K., 922 McLaughlin, M., 912, 918 Metten, P., 225, 235
McCarthy, T., 65, 66, 69 McLellan, J., 917, 919, 920, 945, Metz, E., 919
McCartney, K., 235, 519, 606, 615, 984 Mey, G., 184
619, 866 McLeod, J. D., 704 Meyer, A., 335
McClearn, G., 340, 347, 614, 615, McLin, D., 299, 300, 301, 306 Meyer, J. S., 542
702, 819 McLoyd, V. C., 8, 835 Meyer, J. W., 586
McClelland, J. L., 303, 596 McNamara, K. J., 222 Meyer-Probst, B., 417
McClelland, P., 807, 821, 822, 823, McNamara, S., 428 Meyers, E. W., 7, 9
824 McNemar, Q., 127 Meyerson, I., 189
McClenny, B. D., 629 McRae, K., 284 Miao, C.-H., 368
McClintock, E., 425 Meacham, J. A., 493, 517 Miccery, T., 444
1022 Author Index

Michaels, S., 745, 769 Mitterauer, M., 670 Morgan, J. J. B., 130
Michel, G., 7 Mizuno, T., 387 Morgan, S. P., 685
Michell, J., 172 Mjchard-Vanhée, C., 228 Morikawa, H., 756, 759
Michelsen, E., 918 Modell, J., 6, 7, 90, 668, 670, 768, Morris, B., 753
Mickler, C., 635 900 Morris, P., 50, 177, 421, 525, 794,
Middleton, D., 498 Moen, P., 673, 675, 679, 807, 821, 797, 799, 822, 823, 900, 910,
Midgley, C., 429, 503, 504, 505, 822, 823, 824 911, 912, 918, 926
506, 690, 921 Moeser, R., 757 Morrison, F. J., 154
Miech, R. A., 693 Moessinger, P., 198 Morrobel, D., 905
Mikhailov, A., 437 Moffitt, T., 233, 408, 415, 431, 668, Mortimer, J., 665, 680, 681, 682,
Milevsnic, R., 216, 247 685, 695, 916 696, 900
Miller, A., 547, 762 Moghaddam, F. M., 199 Moshman, D., 199
Miller, D. B., 213 Mogil, J. S., 235 Mosier, C., 498, 499, 734, 761
Miller, D. T., 540 Molander, B., 448, 609 Moskowitz, G. B., 544
Miller, E. K., 601 Moldin, S. O., 224 Moss, H., 528, 574, 622, 666, 685
Miller, G. A., 522, 536 Molenaar, I. W., 30 Moulines, C. U., 531, 532
Miller, J., 38, 66, 434, 717, 718, Molenaar, P. C., 5, 156, 176, 177, Mower, G. D., 217
721, 722, 739, 749, 752, 761 183, 184, 206, 290, 347, 357, Mowrer, O. H., 144, 149
Miller, N. E., 144, 149 362, 442, 447, 605, 606, 612, Moya, C. J., 522, 523
Miller, P., 413, 421, 441, 737, 738 613, 614, 647, 685 Mrazek, P. J., 901
Miller, P. H., 364, 548 Molina, J. C., 242 Mroczek, D. K., 632
Miller, P. J., 717, 718, 721, 722, Molina, M., 241, 245 Much, N. C., 717, 718, 721, 722,
723, 725, 741, 742, 743, 744, Molinari, L., 743, 748 723, 732, 739, 756, 760
745, 746, 747, 749, 754, 759, Mondloch, C. J., 283 Muchisky, M., 276
761, 765, 766, 769, 770, 776, Monroe, W. S., 135 Mueller, E., 139
778 Monsour, A., 330, 332 Mueller, R. A., 932
Miller, P. M., 356 Montada, L., 540 Mueller, R. H., 108, 109, 110, 111,
Miller, W. R., 915, 923, 953 Montagu, A., 506, 507 112, 113, 114
Mills, C. W., 665 Montemayor, R., 751, 928 Mueller, U., 51, 53, 63, 67, 68
Mills, P., 438 Montessori, M., 504, 505 Muir, J., 228
Mills, W., 95, 96, 123, 128 Montgomery, S., 402, 683 Muir, R., 153
Milne, B. J., 684, 685, 703 Moody, S. A., 221 Mulatu, M. S., 606
Minch, E., 181 Moore, B., 432, 746 Mulder, E. J. H., 415
Minear, M., 616 Moore, C., 7, 844 Mulder, N., 758
Miner, M., 986 Moore, D. R., 244, 425 Mullally, P. R., 753, 754, 762, 763
Minsky, M., 771 Moore, D. S., 224, 228 Mullen, M. K., 759
Minton, H. L., 125, 127 Moore, J. W., 674 Müller, A. W., 520
Mintz, J., 741, 746, 749, 754, 761 Moore, K. A., 895, 901, 924, 925, Müller, F., 223
Minuchin, P., 426 932 Müller, U., 238
Mischel, H. N., 544 Moore, M., 869, 870, 891 Müller, V., 604
Mischel, T., 523 Moorehouse, M., 820 Müller-Brettel, M., 571
Mischel, W., 151, 352, 401, 406, Moreland, R. L., 423 Müller-Hill, B., 7
432, 481, 544, 602, 621, 622, Morelli, G., 725, 726, 735, 791 Mulvey, D., 752
623 Moreno, A., 178 Mumme, D. L., 24, 25
Mishler, E. G., 5, 156 Moreno, M., 183 Munakata, Y., 44, 45, 302, 303,
Miskimins, R. W., 554 Morenoff, J., 701, 913 305
Mistretta, C. A., 242 Morgan, C. L., 94, 96, 111, 112, 121, Munholland, K. A., 377
Mistry, J., 498, 499, 734, 761 170, 176, 177, 180 Munn, N. L., 96, 130
Mitrushina, M., 613 Morgan, H. J., 373 Munóz, R. F., 901
Author Index 1023

Munoz-Miller, M., 831 Neter, E., 639 Nosek, B. A., 315, 322, 327, 376,
Munroe, R. H., 728 Neugarten, B. L., 524, 550, 569, 379, 380
Munroe, R. L., 143, 144, 145 573, 667, 674, 675, 693 Nosten-Bertrand, M., 220, 228
Mural, R. J., 7, 9 Neumann, E.-M., 590 Novacek, J., 635
Murchison, C., 90, 97, 125, 147 Neville, H. J., 279 Novikoff, A. B., 2
Murdaya, U., 373, 374 Neville, P. R., 807 Novikoff, B., 440
Murphy, L. B., 136 New, R. S., 737 Nowak, A., 362, 447
Murphy, R. J. L., 959 Newcomb, T. M., 688, 696 Nowlis, V., 149
Murphy-Berman, V., 750 Newell, A., 306, 338 Nozick, R., 555
Murray, C., 7, 155 Newman, D., 200 Nucci, L. P., 27, 69
Murray, H., 7 Newman, J. L., 346 Nunes, T., 770
Murray, J. D., 179, 262 Newmann, J. B., 551 Nurius, P., 545, 752
Murray, L., 916 Newport, E. L., 5, 33, 61, 605 Nurmi, J.-E., 429, 448, 550, 551,
Murry, V. M., 428, 696 Neyer, F. J., 633 636
Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A., 298 Nguyen, L., 844 Nuttin, J. R., 542
Mussen, P., 90, 109, 574, 666 Ni, L., 238 Nyberg, L., 448, 597, 602, 604, 615
Muxen, M., 425, 426 Nicholls, J. G., 547 Nye, R., 968, 969
Myers, J. K., 995 Nicolis, G., 195 Nye, W. C., 973
Myers, S. L., Jr., 832 Nicolopoulou, A., 745, 747 Nyman, B., 418
Niedenthal, P., 545, 551 Nysted, L., 794
Na, K., 761 Nietzsche, F., 476
Nadeau, J. H., 224, 225 Nieuwenhuys, O., 169 Oakes, L. M., 278
Nag, M., 734 Niggli, A., 978 Oates, G., 606
Nagel, E., 26, 70, 528 Nihira, K., 773 Oberauer, K., 601
Nagel, S., 426 Nijhout, H. F., 42, 233 Obeyesekere, G., 200
Nagin, D. S., 447, 685, 695 Nilges, P., 554 Obidah, J. E., 881
Nakahara, H., 235 Nilsson, L.-G., 448, 794 O’Brien, M. U., 926, 932
Nakamura, J., 468, 469, 470, 477, Ninio, A., 325, 344 Ochs, E., 499, 739, 743, 744, 745,
484, 491, 918 Nipkow, K. E., 964 747, 748, 752, 762, 770
Nakkula, M., 927 Nisbet, R., 56, 59, 673 O’Connor, C., 372, 373
Nanda, S., 835 Nisbett, R., 538, 717, 723, 753 O’Connor, E. E., 327, 377, 379, 381
Neal, L. J., 549 Nisenbaum, L. K., 238 O’Connor, T. G., 441
Neckerman, H. J., 427, 433, 447 Noack, P., 427, 682 O’Doherty, J., 606
Neely, A. S., 604, 612 Noam, G., 328, 376, 377, 381, 579 Odom, D. T., 237, 238
Neimeyer, R., 521 Nobiling, E., 947 O’Donnell, L., 919
Neisser, U., 745, 750, 751, 762 Noë, A., 287 O’Donoghue, J. L., 918
Nelesen, R. A., 438 Noel, M., 213 Oerter, R., 533, 546, 550, 551, 758,
Nelson, C. A., 409, 420, 594 Noguchi, J., 235 945
Nelson, D., 763 Noll, E., 845, 881, 887 Oesterle, S., 682
Nelson, E. C., 234, 702 Noll, L., 871 Oettingen, G., 442, 639, 757
Nelson, K., 320, 321, 740, 749 Norasakkunkit, V., 757, 760, 763 Ogbu, J., 429, 835, 836, 845, 878,
Nemeroff, C., 717 Nordhaus, W. D., 553 879, 880, 881, 882
Nesselroade, C. S., 607, 612 Norem, J. K., 551 Ogden, T. H., 54
Nesselroade, J. R., 2, 5, 156, 176, Norem-Hebeisen, A. A., 549 Ogilvie, D. M., 631
177, 182, 183, 184, 441, 442, Norenzayan, A., 970, 971 Ogino, M., 759
446, 447, 448, 539, 570, 580, Norman, K., 740 Oh, H. J., 683
586, 587, 588, 589, 600, 607, Norris, L., 622 Ohlig, K. H., 946
610, 612, 614, 616, 619, 624, Norris, T., 906, 913, 914 Öhman, A., 410
630, 631, 632, 676, 685, 904 Northcott, H., 994 Okada, N., 768
1024 Author Index

Okami, P., 725, 727 43, 45, 49, 51, 52, 53, 54, 57, Parmelee, A. H., 811
Okamoto, Y., 333, 342, 353, 357, 59, 60, 61, 63, 70, 71, 72, 74, Parsons, J., 751
369 75, 79, 80, 346, 405, 528, 532, Parsons, S., 682
Okun, M. A., 551, 641, 652 535, 549, 580, 586, 903 Parsons, T., 496, 522
Olah, A., 413 Ovesey, L., 833, 835, 842, 867, 879 Parten, M. B., 136
O’Leary, D. D. M., 434 Oyama, S., 36, 39, 40, 42, 43, 187, Parton, D. A., 150
Oliveira, Z. M. R., 195 211, 219, 228, 246, 249, 259, Parziale, J., 321, 349, 363, 364, 365,
Oliver, I., 605, 609 527 368, 369
Oliveri, M. E., 426 Oyemade, U. J., 856 Pascual-Leone, J., 199, 339, 384,
Oller, D. K., 245, 266 Oyserman, D., 749, 750 492, 581, 600, 601, 606, 944
Olmstead, R., 725, 727 Ozaki, H., 225 Pasquale, E., 413
Olson, D. H., 425, 426 Ozarak, E., 977 Passeron, J.-C., 518, 775
Olson, G. M., 545 Ozer, D. J., 446 Pastor, D. L., 816
Olson, L. S., 681 Pastoret, C., 228
Olson, M. H., 845 Pack, R., 930 Pasupathi, M., 609, 620, 627, 628,
Olson, S. L., 498 Packer, M., 438, 732, 741 629, 630, 634
Oman, D., 982 Pagano, P., 167 Pate, E., 261
Omi, M., 882, 883, 884 Paget, K., 376 Paternoster, R., 685, 688
O’Neil, R., 426 Pagliari, C., 616 Paterson, D. G., 100
Oosterwegel, A., 752 Paige, L. Z., 917 Patt, J., 741
Opitz, J. M., 46 Paikoff, R. L., 408, 412, 430 Patterson, G. R., 151, 425, 694, 695,
Oppenheim, R. W., 142 Pakstis, A. J., 227 917
Oppenheimer, D., 725, 726 Palincsar, A. S., 349 Paul, S. M., 223
Oppenheimer, J. M., 94 Pallas, A. M., 680 Paulhus, D. L., 624
Oppenheimer, L., 67, 545, 752 Palmiter, R. D., 238 Paulson, S. E., 922
O’Rahilly, R., 223 Palmore, E., 985 Pauly, P. J., 130
O’Rand, A. M., 672 Pampel, F. C., 676 Pavalko, E. K., 666
O’Regan, J. K., 287 Pancake, V. R., 816 Pavot, W., 633
O’Reilly, R. C., 305 Pancer, M., 918 Peak, L., 758, 760, 762
Oreland, L., 415 Pandey, R. B., 468 Pearlin, L. I., 473
Orellana, M. F., 771, 774 Pantev, C., 609 Peck, S. C., 428, 429
Ornstein, P. A., 89, 90, 108, 132, Pantoja, A. P., 195 Pedersen, N. L., 614, 615, 624, 632
154, 573, 669, 707 Papillo, A. R., 925 Pederson, A., 427
Ortiz, J., 415 Papousek, H., 507, 545 Peet, R. C., 734
Osborn, H. F., 111, 112 Papousek, M., 507, 545 Peirce, C. S., 177, 196
Osborn, P., 932 Parasuraman, R., 537 Pellegrini, A. D., 41
Oser, F., 946, 952, 953, 959, 961, Pargament, K., 953, 984, 990 Peller, G., 832, 867
963, 964, 965, 966, 978, 980, Paris, S. G., 102, 151 Pellerin, L. A., 682
981, 988, 990, 996 Parish, S., 718 Pennak, S., 575, 597, 603, 611, 612
Osgood, C. E., 327, 374 Parisi, D., 10, 357, 596, 597, 605 Pennebaker, J. W., 541
Osher, D., 917 Park, D., 597, 615 Pennisi, E., 236
Osiewacz, H. D., 576 Park, L., 718, 721, 732, 739, 756, Penuel, W., 201
Osmer, R. R., 981 760 Pepler, D. J., 683
Ostendorf, F., 442, 620, 633 Park, R., 615 Pepper, M. P., 995
O’Sullivan, M., 373, 374 Parke, R. D., 6, 7, 89, 90, 151, 573, Pepper, S. C., 30, 68, 69, 70, 74,
Ott, E., 277 668, 669, 670, 707, 811 339, 340
Oura, Y., 766, 767, 776 Parker, S. T., 247 Perdue, S., 385
Overton, W. F., 1, 5, 8, 18, 21, 25, Parkhill, J., 183 Peréz, B., 100
26, 28, 30, 31, 32, 34, 36, 39, Parkinson, B., 540 Perkins, D. F., 13, 898, 900
Author Index 1025

Perkins, D. N., 353, 367 332, 333, 334, 340, 341, 342, Poole, C., 689
Perkins, P., 247 343, 345, 361, 362, 363, 364, Poortinga, Y. H., 374, 721
Perlmutter, M., 519, 540, 571, 574, 365, 366, 383, 412, 422, 470, Popper, K., 74
579, 584 473, 478, 488, 504, 508, 521, Popper, K. M., 542
Perner, J., 582, 605, 975 531, 532, 533, 534, 545, 546, Popper, K. R., 410
Perret-Clermont, A.-N., 201 547, 606, 845, 957, 958, 996 Porfeli, E., 671
Perriello, L., 955, 974 Pick, A., 241 Porges, S. W., 628
Persson, J., 387 Pickens, J., 241 Port, R. F., 320
Pervin, L., 410, 422, 543, 619, 622 Pickles, A., 425, 426, 691, 704 Portales, A. L., 629
Perz, C. A., 642 Pieper, C. F., 683 Porter, T., 172
Peters, H. E., 676 Pierce, C. S., 77 Posner, B. Z., 485
Petersen, A., 411, 415, 429, 842, Pietromonaco, P., 63 Posner, M. I., 613
843, 850, 876 Pike, K. L., 730 Post, S. G., 986
Petersén, I., 387, 388 Pimley, S., 635 Pott, M., 759
Peterson, A., 223, 745 Pinker, S., 48, 61 Pötter, U., 600
Peterson, C., 638, 777, 901 Pinquart, M., 24, 691 Potts, R., 761
Peterson, D. R., 425 Pipp, S., 333, 351, 378 Potvin, R. H., 979
Peterson, J., 118, 126, 127, 132, Pirttilä-Backman, A.-M., 333 Poulin, F., 427
582, 591 Pittman, K., 895, 896, 899, 900, Poulton, R., 684, 703
Peterson, R. C., 137 901, 906, 909, 911, 918, 920 Powell, L. H., 982
Peterson, W. A., 674, 675 Pitts, R., 982, 984 Power, C., 668, 683, 684, 692, 983
Petersson, K. M., 604, 609 Pitts, V. P., 973 Powers, W. T., 522, 541
Peto, J., 424 Plante, T. G., 982 Prasada, S., 302
Petras, H., 917 Plath, D. W., 725, 726 Prashad, V., 867
Petrill, S. A., 615 Plato, 339, 368 Pratkanis, A. R., 619
Petrillo, D., 959, 978 Platsidou, M., 343, 353 Pratt, J. B., 973
Pettengill, S., 760 Platt, S. A., 231 Pratt, M. W., 745
Pettigrew, T. F., 833, 842, 867 Plessner, H., 609 Predmore, S. C., 538
Pettit, G. S., 924 Plewis, I., 684 Preece, A., 748
Pfeiffer, J. E., 987 Plomin, R., 7, 32, 234, 340, 347, Prell, R.-E., 981
Pfeiffer, R., 287 413, 422, 498, 519, 529, 585, Prentice, D., 750
Pfister, O., 948, 949 614, 615, 623, 625, 633, 648, Prescott, C. A., 705
Phelan, J., 683, 704 659, 818, 819, 866 Pressey, S. L., 129, 573
Phelan, P., 772, 845, 867 Plunkett, K., 10, 345, 357, 596, 597, Preyer, W., 94, 96, 97, 98, 99, 100,
Phelps, E., 5, 12, 13, 156, 292, 449, 605 142, 438
668, 895 Pnevmatikos, D., 973 Pribram, K. H., 388, 522, 536
Phelps, M. E., 602 Poddiakov, A. N., 193 Price, L., 906
Philliber, S., 919 Poduska, J., 917 Price-Williams, D., 334
Phillippe, J., 101 Poincaré, H., 424 Prigogine, I., 177, 179, 269, 482,
Phillips, M., 697, 924 Polak, R., 945 582
Phillips, R. J., 223 Polk, T. A., 615 Prince, S. E., 601, 616
Phillips, S. D., 923 Polkinghorne, D. E., 745 Prinz, R. J., 921
Phillips, S. J., 196 Pollard, J. A., 901 Prinz, W., 418, 436, 583, 596, 598,
Phillips, T. J., 223, 224, 235 Pollner, M., 983 599, 602, 610, 616
Piaget, J., 2, 37, 40, 41, 47, 51, 60, Polsky, A., 320 Proctor, R. N., 7
63, 70, 93, 111, 113, 135, 136, Pondy, L. R., 485 Proenca, R., 226
138, 139, 143, 176, 197, 198, Ponirakis, A., 686 Prosch, H., 57
241, 265, 278, 282, 285, 303, Pontecorvo, C., 201 Pruyne, E., 386
305, 314, 315, 321, 323, 324, Pook, P., 287, 288 Pruyser, P., 950
1026 Author Index

Psaltis, C., 201 Ratner, H. H., 751, 762, 778 Reuter-Lorenz, P., 575, 582, 594,
Pulkkinen, L., 411, 550, 621, 820 Rau, L., 149 608, 610, 644
Pullen, S. M., 606 Raudenbush, S. W., 13, 913, 926 Revenson, T. A., 641
Purves, D., 221, 235 Rawls, J., 550 Reye, B., 953
Putnam, F., 380 Raya, P., 200, 248, 316, 317, 320, Reynolds, C. A., 624, 630, 632
Putnam, H., 54, 55, 57, 63, 68, 69, 321, 328, 330, 332, 344, 354, Reynolds, G., 243
74, 75, 521, 532, 534 381 Reznick, J. S., 348
Rayner, R. A., 130 Rhodes, J. E., 911, 914, 917, 926
Qin, Y., 339, 340, 347 Raynor, J. O., 543 Rhymer, R. M., 446
Quetelet, A., 572 Raz, N., 602, 612 Ricceri, L., 239
Quine, W. V., 74, 114, 538 Rebok, G. W., 542 Ricco, R. B., 532
Quinn, J., 912 Recanzone, G., 283 Richards, D. D., 778
Quinn, N., 718 Reddy, M., 340 Richards, F. A., 323
Quinton, D., 691, 704 Redfield, R., 719 Richards, J., 989
Reed, E. S., 65, 306 Richards, M. H., 485, 496
Rabbitt, P. M. A., 590 Reedy, M. N., 551, 622, 637 Richardson, D., 284, 287, 288
Rabe-Hesketh, S., 5, 156 Rees, E., 582 Richardson, J., 978, 987, 988
Rabiner, D. L., 25 Reese, H. W., 1, 26, 30, 31, 32, 34, Richardson, K., 240
Rabinowitz, L. S., 689 441, 527, 528, 535, 570, 572, Richman, A., 735, 737, 738, 739
Rachman, S., 670 575, 580, 581, 582, 586, 588, Richters, J. E., 402
Radell, P. L., 241, 242, 243, 245, 589, 603, 614 Rickman, M., 630
247 Reeve, R., 317, 332 Ricks, D., 427, 528
Radke-Yarrow, M., 422, 424 Regnerus, M., 971 Ricoeur, P., 68, 73, 74, 75, 76
Radziszewska, B., 749 Reich, H., 945, 953, 957, 965 Ridderinkhof, K. R., 601
Raff, R. A., 46 Reich, K. H., 964, 982 Ridley-Johnson, R., 292
Raftopoulos, A., 26 Reid, J. B., 917 Rieckmann, N., 595, 611
Raglioni, S. S., 241 Reimer, K. S., 968, 984 Riediger, M., 579, 591, 636, 638
Raichle, M. E., 386 Reinert, G., 91, 96, 97, 98, 100, 521, Riegel, K., 196, 421, 436, 490, 520,
Raine, A., 415 571, 572, 600, 616 573, 575, 580, 581, 586, 606
Raizada, R., 357 Reinhardt, J. M., 421 Rieser, J. J., 89, 573, 669, 707
Rakic, P., 382, 384, 385 Reinshagen, H., 526 Rigney, D. R., 273
Rakotovao, R., 230 Reis, A., 609 Riksen-Walraven, J. M., 809, 865
Raleigh, M., 415 Reischies, F. M., 576 Riles, L., 238
Ram, N., 2, 5, 156 Reisenzein, R., 534 Riley, K., 526
Ramachers, G., 974 Reiss, D., 32, 426, 818 Riley, M. S., 346
Ramirez, M., III, 334 Renkl, A., 587 Riley, M. W., 573, 584, 586, 587,
Ramirez, M. L., 919, 924 Renner, G., 527, 536, 539, 548, 552, 667, 668, 674, 675
Rampon, C., 235, 236, 255, 702 555, 634, 636 Rimer, S., 873
Randall, J. H., 72 Repacholi, B., 377 Rinaldi, N. J., 237, 238
Rank, M. R., 682 Resch, N. L., 917 Rinehart, P. M., 928
Rao, R., 287, 288 Resch, N. R., 917, 926 Rittenhouse, P., 769
Rapaport, D., 117 Resnick, H., 926, 932 Ritzen, M., 424
Rappolt-Schlichtman, G., 316, 328 Resnick, L. B., 201, 587 Rizzuto, A.-M., 951, 979
Rasch, G., 323, 350, 352 Resnick, L. R., 533 Robbins, M. A., 616
Ratcliff, D., 974 Resnick, M. D., 920, 922 Robert, F., 237, 238
Rathod, S., 989 Rest, J., 548, 966 Roberts, B., 620, 632, 633, 634
Rathunde, K., 466, 467, 471, 478, Retzinger, S., 376 Robertson, H. A., 216, 247
479, 481, 491, 492, 497, 498, Reuchlin, M., 590 Robertson, S. S., 273
501, 502, 503, 504, 505 Reuman, D., 429, 503 Robin, D. J., 241
Author Index 1027

Robins, L., 450, 694 Rose, A. J., 745 Rowe, D. C., 7, 8, 702, 703, 929
Robins, R., 49, 412, 634 Rose, D., 335 Rowe, J. W., 551, 580, 584
Robinson, D., 101, 357, 411 Rose, H., 41 Rowell, J. A., 198
Robinson, M. G., 924 Rose, K. M., 631 Rozeboom, W. W., 79
Rochat, P., 241 Rose, L. T., 335 Rozin, P., 717
Rochberg-Halton, E., 526 Rose, R. J., 428, 614, 702 Rubel, E. W., 219
Rockstroh, B., 609 Rose, S., 41, 216, 227, 228, 236, Rubin, K. H., 508, 683, 816
Rodgers, W. L., 676 241, 247 Ruble, D. N., 549, 640, 772
Rodin, J., 551, 692, 808, 809 Rose, S. A., 240, 247 Rude, S. P., 954, 971
Rodkin, P., 154, 441 Rose, S. P., 323, 332, 335, 364, 382, Ruderman, A., 365
Rodning, C., 811 383, 387, 388, 389, 944 Rudinger, G., 449, 517, 587, 589,
Rodriguez, M. C., 905 Rose-Krasnor, L., 816, 918 611
Rodriguez, M. L., 621 Rosen, K. M., 217 Rudman, L., 315, 322, 327, 376,
Rodriguez-Zas, S. L., 235 Rosen, L., 752 379, 380
Roeder, K., 230, 231 Rosenbaum, J. E., 689 Rudolph, L., 172
Roediger, D. R., 877, 891, 892 Rosenberg, F., 550 Ruff, H. A., 240
Roehlkepartain, E., 908, 913, 914, Rosenberg, M., 550, 553, 670, 751 Ruhland, R., 348, 351, 352, 358, 387
921, 923, 925, 926, 927, 929, Rosenberg, R., 974 Ruiselova, Z., 430
931, 954, 971, 981 Rosenberger, N. R., 718, 758 Rumbaut, R., 680, 700
Roehlkepartain, J. L., 906, 914 Rosenblatt, J. S., 152 Runco, M. A., 487
Röesler, H.-D., 417 Rosenblum, L. A., 499 Rushton, J. P., 7, 8
Roffman, J. G., 911, 914 Rosier, K. A., 743, 748 Russell, E. S., 227
Rogers, C., 473, 476, 479, 482 Rösler, F., 575, 582, 608, 610, 644 Russell, G., 550
Rogers, S. C., 183 Ross, A. O., 150 Russell, J., 51, 79, 370, 372, 718
Rogers, W. H., 683 Ross, D., 104, 171 Russell, R. W., 409, 432
Rogoff, B., 65, 66, 67, 168, 185, Ross, G., 545 Rustak, B., 216, 247
200, 316, 321, 322, 344, 347, Ross, J., 989 Rutter, M., 32, 196, 340, 347, 417,
356, 364, 494, 498, 499, 501, Ross, L., 538, 860 425, 449, 527, 528, 578, 621,
545, 546, 717, 723, 725, 726, Ross, M., 640 683, 690, 691, 702, 704, 805,
734, 743, 749, 761, 767, 769, Ross, N., 717, 729, 777 820, 901, 989
771, 773, 911 Ross, W. D., 55, 56 Ryan, J. A., 896, 897, 900, 916
Rohner, R. P., 760 Rossetti-Ferreira, M. C., 195, 202 Ryan, R. M., 755
Rohwer, G., 589, 673 Rotenberg, E. J., 200, 248, 317, 320, Ryan, S. A., 926
Roland, E., 410 321, 330, 332, 344, 354 Rybash, J. M., 597, 600
Rollett, B., 964, 966, 987, 988 Roth, J., 900, 901, 905, 906, 912, Rychlak, J. F., 490
Romanes, G. J., 94, 95, 96, 120 916 Rydelius, P.-A., 424
Rommetveit, R., 525 Rothbart, M. K., 613, 623, 627, 628, Ryder, N. B., 588, 667, 668, 674,
Romney, A. K., 774 629, 630 675
Ronca, A. E., 246 Rothbaum, F., 552, 759 Ryder, R. G., 622
Rönkä, A., 411 Rothermund, K., 538, 540, 548, 552, Ryff, C., 401, 429, 573, 579, 587,
Roodin, P. A., 597, 600 554, 555, 620, 622, 634, 636, 620, 633, 637, 638, 640, 692,
Roos, J., 949 637, 639 761, 918, 932
Rorty, R., 58 Rothlisberg, B. A., 922 Ryu, S., 696
Rosa, A., 188 Rott, C., 636
Rosaldo, M. Z., 762 Rotter, J., 151, 422, 811 Saarni, C., 24, 25
Rosas, S., 772 Roubertoux, P., 213, 220, 227, 228 Saastamoinen, M., 820
Rosch, E., 46, 47, 53, 69, 246, 303, Rousseau, J. J., 946 Sabini, J., 35, 522
373, 466, 530, 531, 533, 534, Roux, W., 211 Sacker, A., 682
750 Rovee-Collier, C., 286 Sadd, M., 987
1028 Author Index

Sadeh, A., 725 Savola, K. L., 5, 156, 668 Schiller, K. S., 687
Sadovnik, A., 501 Sawa, A., 702 Schindler, I., 627, 628, 636, 638,
Saffran, J. R., 605 Sawyer, R., 741 639
Saha, A. K., 261 Saxe, G. B., 54 Schjelderup, H., 949
Saile, H., 554 Saxe, R., 345 Schjelderup, K., 949
Saint-Hilaire, E. G., 211 Sayer, A. G., 668, 685 Schlick, M., 73, 74
Saito, R. N., 902, 906, 911, 931 Scales, P., 12, 895, 900, 901, 905, Schlosberg, H., 374
Säljö, R., 765 906, 908, 911, 912, 913, 914, Schmidt, G., 600
Salmela-Aro, K., 448 915, 918, 919, 920, 921, 922, Schmidt, J., 468
Salomon, G., 353, 367 923, 924, 925, 926, 927, 928, Schmidt, K., 377
Salthe, S. N., 222 929, 930, 931 Schmidt, L. A., 415
Salthouse, T. A., 528, 553, 571, 577, Scardamalia, M., 349 Schmidt, L. R., 590
590, 598, 600, 601, 602, 606, Scarlett, W. G., 953, 955, 974, 990 Schmidt, R. A., 296
609, 610, 611, 627 Scarr, S., 36, 43, 65, 234, 235, 247, Schmidtchen, G., 987
Salvatore, S., 167 248, 519, 529, 606, 613, 614, Schmidt McCollam, K., 447, 607
Salzman, L., 988 615, 619, 819, 846, 866 Schmiedek, F., 591, 597, 603
Sameroff, A., 109, 269, 357, 408, Scarr-Salapatek, S., 247, 819, 865 Schmitt-Rodermund, E., 670
428, 429, 432, 441, 448, 535, Schaal, S., 288 Schmitz, B., 595
683, 917 Schacter, D. L., 605 Schmitz, E., 987
Sampson, E. E., 49, 752, 753 Schadewaldt, W., 610 Schmitz, U., 550, 554
Sampson, R., 5, 13, 156, 666, 672, Schaeffer, C. M., 917 Schnabel, K. U., 668
683, 693, 695, 698, 699, 701, Schaffer, H. R., 151, 153 Schnakenberg, J., 945
704, 705, 913, 914, 926 Schaffler, P., 377 Schneider, B., 506, 688
Samuelson, P. A., 553 Schaie, K. W., 448, 527, 528, 551, Schneider, E. L., 551
Samuelson, S., 277 572, 573, 575, 583, 584, 587, Schneider, K., 294, 296, 546
Sandblom, J., 604 588, 589, 597, 598, 603, 610, Schneider, S. K., 539
Sandefur, G., 694 611, 612, 613, 644, 669, 904 Schneider, W., 449, 478, 537, 605
Sandel, T., 744, 746 Schallenberger, M. E., 106, 123, Schneirla, T. C., 2, 26, 33, 46, 96,
Sanders, M. G., 922 135 121, 141, 217, 219, 225, 247,
San Doval, A., 919 Schank, R. C., 325, 372, 522, 524, 265, 408, 415, 422, 423, 469,
Sanford, E. C., 573, 596 541, 542, 543 471, 495, 574, 798, 903
Sanford, N., 451 Schechtman, V. L., 725 Schoelmerish, A., 761
Sangsue, J., 370 Schecter, S. R., 743, 744 Schoenhals, M., 688
Sanislow, C. A., 231 Scheff, T. J., 376 Schoenrade, P., 982, 984
Sansone, L., 543 Scheibel, A., 385 Scholnick, E. K., 28, 62, 542
Santostefano, S., 53, 63 Scheier, C., 53, 240, 246, 287, 299, Scholz, J. P., 276
Sapolsky, R. M., 415, 438 300 Schoneman, T. J., 789
Sarat, A., 874 Scheier, M. F., 410, 522, 536, 537, Schöner, G., 53, 240, 246, 273, 276,
Sarbin, T. R., 441 539, 540, 541, 543, 554, 620, 277, 298, 299, 300, 306
Sareen, J., 409 638, 639 Schönpflug, U., 534
Sarigiani, P. A., 429 Scherer, H., 590 Schooler, C., 606, 739, 759
Sarkar, S., 222, 231 Scherer, K. R., 370, 371 Schoon, I., 682
Sarris, E. G., 191 Scherer, M., 540 Schore, A. N., 377
Sass, L. A., 750 Scherer, U., 370 Schorr, L. B., 932
Sato, T., 185 Schibilsky, M., 988 Schoultz, J., 765
Satz, P., 614 Schieffelin, B., 499, 738, 739, 743, Schroeder, D. C., 183
Sauck, C., 176 744 Schroedter, T., 948, 964
Saudino, K. J., 32 Schiff, M., 232 Schrott, L. M., 417
Savage, A., 616 Schiller, J., 320 Schuierer, G., 609
Author Index 1029

Schulenberg, J. E., 612 Seligman, M. E. P., 131, 423, 473, Sherrod, L., 586
Schultheiss, O. C., 541 540, 547, 638, 900, 901, 926, Sherrow, F., 978
Schultz, D., 831, 847 932 Sherry, D. F., 605
Schultz, L. H., 376, 377 Sells, S. B., 410, 422 Sherzer, J., 742
Schultz, P. G., 255 Selman, R., 376, 377, 526, 533, 548, Shiffrin, R. M., 478, 537
Schultz, W., 932 549, 550 Shillito, E., 242
Schulz, R., 548, 552, 554, 578, 590, Selye, H., 415, 416 Shils, E. A., 522
620, 634, 636, 641, 904 Semel, S., 501 Shimizu, H., 718, 759
Schulze, P., 761 Senders, S. J., 241 Shimuzu, M., 702
Schunk, D., 536 Senn, M. J. E., 125 Shinn, M., 164, 438
Schurr, T. G., 227 Sesma, A., Jr., 901, 906, 908, 915, Shinn, P., 238
Schütz, A., 199, 522 924, 925, 927, 928, 929 Shirk, S., 751
Schwartz, A., 983 Sethi, S., 860 Shirley, M. M., 128, 135
Schwartz, C., 409 Setiadi, B., 374 Shishler, R. J., 601
Schwartz, G. E. R., 630 Settersten, R. A., Jr., 571, 573, 584, Shoda, Y., 621, 623
Schwartz, J. C., 356, 372, 373, 374, 586, 633, 674, 680, 697, 700, Shore, B., 717, 718
376 924 Shostak, M., 184
Schwartz, J. E., 623, 638, 652 Sewertzoff, A., 187 Shrager, J., 602
Schwartz, J. H., 441 Sewney, V. D., 111 Shrout, P. E., 279
Schwartz, M. S., 341 Seymour, B., 606 Shucksmith, J., 429
Schwartz, N., 412 Seymour, S., 718 Shultz, T. R., 348, 356, 357, 382
Schwartz, T., 718 Shafranske, E. P., 948 Shuman, A., 748
Schwartzman, H., 741 Shahabi, L., 982 Shwalb, B. J., 743
Schwarz, B., 442 Shakow, D., 117 Shwalb, D., 743
Schwarz, N., 539, 639, 640 Shamakina, I. Y., 216, 247 Shweder, R., 66, 67, 200, 519, 527,
Schweder, R. A., 413, 421, 441 Shanahan, L., 702 576, 597, 717, 718, 719, 720,
Schweitzer, F., 946, 964, 981 Shanahan, M., 170, 248, 449, 665, 721, 722, 723, 725, 726, 729,
Scott, J. P., 151, 152, 153 666, 668, 671, 680, 682, 688, 732, 738, 739, 752, 753, 756,
Scott, S., 989 693, 698, 701, 702, 703, 821 760, 762, 768, 772
Scribner, S., 200, 531, 738, 766 Shand, N., 756, 759 Siddle, R., 989
Scriven, M., 419 Shannon, L. W., 443 Siegal, M., 777
Scriver, C. R., 224 Shanon, B., 51, 56 Siegel, A. W., 764
Searle, J., 32, 34, 56, 57, 524 Sharma, A., 12, 912, 920 Siegler, I., 632, 985
Sears, H. A., 408 Shatz, C., 215 Siegler, R. S., 28, 44, 45, 62, 72, 86,
Sears, M. R., 684, 703 Shaver, P., 320, 325, 356, 370, 371, 100, 101, 303, 321, 325, 336,
Sears, P., 146, 149, 574 372, 373, 374, 376, 377, 979, 352, 364, 365, 533, 582, 590,
Sears, R. R., 90, 91, 123, 125, 128, 987 603, 605, 775, 778
144, 149, 150, 155, 574, 666, Shaw, J. C., 306 Siepmann, M., 35
693 Shaw, L., 242 Sigman, M., 500, 811
Seaton, G., 831, 838, 871, 881 Shaw, R. E., 306 Sigvardsson, S., 441
Secada, W., 768 Shea, M. C., 537 Sijtsma, K., 30
Secord, P. F., 523 Sheets-Johnstone, M., 53 Sikström, S., 600
Segall, M. H., 721 Sheldon, K. M., 639 Silbereisen, R. K., 24, 412, 427,
Segalowitz, S. J., 415 Shepard, B., 62 428, 442, 517, 670, 691
Seibold-Simpson, S. M., 926 Sherkat, D. E., 978 Silberg, J., 702
Seidel, F., 213 Sherman, A., 982 Silke, A., 988
Seifer, R., 500 Sherman, M., 127 Silva, P., 408, 628, 695
Seifert, A. E., 540 Sherman, T., 545 Silver, L. J., 831, 838, 881
Sejnowski, T. J., 282 Sherrill, K. A., 986 Silver, R. C., 642, 687
1030 Author Index

Silverberg, S. B., 579 Small, B. J., 632, 633 Solaas, M. H., 819
Silvern, L., 323, 344 Small, S., 803, 805, 806, 898, 901, Solomon, B., 190
Silverstein, S. M., 987 905, 906 Solomon, G. E. A., 61
Simão, L. M., 200, 201 Smedslund, J., 173, 176, 532, 533 Solomon, J., 377
Simenova, D., 759 Smith, C., 408, 540, 981 Solomon, R. L., 131, 423
Simmel, G., 195 Smith, E. E., 537 Somsen, R. J. M., 388
Simmonds, R., 978 Smith, F., 612 Sorell, G. T., 436
Simmons, R. G., 683 Smith, J., 11, 408, 473, 491, 493, Sorensen, A. B., 586, 687
Simmons, W. L., 554 530, 542, 550, 569, 571, 574, Sorensen, S., 415
Simner, M. L., 242 575, 577, 581, 582, 585, 590, Soto-Faraco, S., 242
Simon, H., 191, 306, 338, 526, 610 591, 593, 594, 597, 604, 606, Sousa, P., 777
Simon, T., 103 607, 608, 611, 612, 614, 621, Sovran, T., 187
Simons, R. L., 428, 685, 696 625, 630, 634, 635, 636, 637, Sowarka, D., 580, 603, 612
Simonton, D. K., 471, 472, 484, 610 638, 642, 644 Spalding, D. A., 95
Simpson, A. R., 914 Smith, K., 925 Spangler, G., 739, 740, 757
Singer, B., 637, 692, 918, 932 Smith, L., 39, 53 Spanoudis, G., 343, 353
Singer, D., 5 Smith, L. B., 156, 195, 240, 243, Spaulding, W., 634
Singer, J. D., 328, 359, 685 246, 259, 271, 275, 277, 278, Spear, L. P., 420
Singer, J. L., 620 284, 287, 289, 291, 292, 299, Spear, N., 242
Singer, M. I., 441 300, 301, 302, 303, 306, 332, Spearman, C., 235
Singer, T., 577, 578, 584, 589, 598, 347, 348, 357, 605, 607, 622, Speed, W. C., 227
604, 606, 611, 615, 616, 617 903 Speer, J. R., 548
Singer, W., 597, 628 Smith, L. D., 44, 73 Spelke, E., 5, 33, 61, 241, 263, 278,
Singh, G., 227 Smith, M. A., 820 315, 325, 338, 339, 340, 342,
Singh, I., 328, 381 Smith, M. B., 698 344, 345, 346, 348, 353, 372,
Sinnott, J., 492 Smith, M. J., 524 604, 605, 777
Sjövall, B., 188 Smith, M. R., 616 Spelt, D. K., 131
Skeels, H. M., 127 Smith, R. S., 697, 905 Spemann, H., 140
Skinner, B. F., 44, 47, 67, 131, 143, Smith, S., 320, 761 Spence, C., 242
150, 329, 338, 363, 364, 521, Smith, T. J., 906, 913, 927 Spence, D. P., 745
605, 903 Smith, W. C., 954, 962, 986 Spence, J., 240, 485
Skinner, D., 745, 748, 749 Smoller, D., 242 Spence, M. J., 232
Skinner, E. A., 436, 547, 548, 554, Smyth, M., 174 Spencer, A. E., 980
635 Smythe, W. E., 174, 199 Spencer, H., 93, 196
Skorpen, C. G., 630 Sneed, J. D., 531, 532 Spencer, J., 293, 294, 295, 296, 297,
Skrondal, A., 5, 156 Snell, E., 689 299, 302, 306
Slaughter, D. T., 849 Snibbe, A. C., 761 Spencer, M. B., 8, 379, 830, 831,
Slaughter, V., 975 Snidman, N., 378, 438, 442, 623 832, 833, 834, 835, 838, 839,
Slee, N., 958, 959 Snow, C., 325, 344 841, 842, 843, 844, 845, 846,
Sliwinski, M. J., 668 Snyder, C. R., 538, 539 847, 848, 849, 850, 856, 857,
Sloane, D. M., 979 Snyder, M., 438, 537 858, 859, 860, 861, 862, 863,
Slobin, D., 344, 745 Snyder, S. H., 702 864, 865, 866, 867, 868, 869,
Slomczynski, K. M., 815 Snyder, S. S., 552 870, 871, 876, 878, 879, 880,
Slomkowski, C., 777 Soares da Silva, A. P., 202 881, 882, 883, 884, 885, 886,
Slotta, J. D., 263 Sofer, C., 680 887, 905, 913
Slovak, K., 441 Sokol, B. W., 27 Spencer, T., 831, 887
Slunecko, T., 185 Sokoloff, H., 896 Sperber, D., 776
Slutske, W. S., 702 Sokolowski, M. B., 223, 224, 226, Sperry, D. E., 746, 748
Sluyter, F., 227, 228 228, 229, 230 Sperry, L. L., 744, 746, 748
Author Index 1031

Sperry, R. W., 217, 247 Steele, S., 946 Strauman, T., 538, 545
Spiegel, D., 628, 629, 630 Stefancic, J., 875, 876, 877, 878 Strauss, A. L., 672
Spilerman, S., 681 Stein, B. E., 240, 244, 283 Strauss, B., 242
Spilka, B., 944, 945, 948, 952, 953, Stein, J., 35, 920 Strauss, S., 361, 533
974, 975, 977, 978, 980, 984, Stein, K., 757 Strawbridge, W. J., 982
988, 990 Stein, M. B., 409 Streib, H., 960, 988
Spilka, G., 932 Stein, S. A., 323 Strelau, J., 623
Spiro, A., III, 632, 640 Steinberg, L., 1, 7, 10, 33, 36, 44, Streri, A., 241, 245
Spiro, M. E., 379 426, 817, 818, 893, 901, 915 Strickland, B. R., 761
Spivey, M., 284, 287, 288 Steinhagen-Thiessen, E., 11, 615 Strohman, R. C., 249
Spock, B., 145 Stenberg, C., 321 Stromberg, P. G., 749
Spoont, M. R., 629 Stengers, I., 269, 582 Strommen, M. P., 977
Spranger, E., 520, 521 Stent, G. S., 222 Stronks, K., 684
Spring, B., 424 Stepanova, A. N., 238 Strout, S., 179
Spruston, N., 320 Stern, D., 751 Strycker, L. A., 685
Spudich, J. L., 225 Stern, D. C., 420 Stryker, M. P., 437
Sroufe, L. A., 24, 27, 62, 370, 377, Stern, D. M., 494, 499, 500 Stryker, S., 551
432, 482, 501, 815, 816, 915 Stern, M. S., 995 Stuart, J. M., 237
Srull, T. K., 544 Stern, W., 2, 37, 40, 118, 119, 248, Stueve, A., 919
Stace, W. T., 35, 55 406, 421, 425, 438 Stumpf, C., 409
Staff, J., 682 Sternberg, P. W., 436 Su, Y., 945, 984
Standing, E. M., 504, 505 Sternberg, R. J., 28, 44, 62, 353, Suci, G. J., 327, 374
Stanger, C., 751 413, 475, 492, 506, 537, 579, Suess, G., 739, 740, 757
Stangor, C., 549 578, 600, 605, 606, 608, 613, Suess, P. E., 629
Stanley, J. C., 349 618, 622 Sugarman, S., 26
Stansfeld, S., 684 Sterns, H. L., 644 Sugden, K., 233, 705
Stanton, B., 930 Stetsenko, A., 442, 757 Suh, E., 630
Stanton, J. M., 620, 634, 638 Stevens, A. H., 682 Sullivan, H. S., 37, 145, 150,
Stanton, W., 695 Stevenson, H. C., 834, 845, 846, 751
Stark, R. A., 953 867, 870 Sullivan, M., 66, 200, 718, 721,
Stattin, H., 7, 11, 14, 36, 37, 39, 49, Stevenson, H. W., 151, 756, 790 751, 837
401, 406, 408, 410, 411, 415, Stewart, J., 187, 199, 315, 319, 320, Sullivan, T. K., 913, 914
425, 426, 427, 428, 430, 432, 324, 325, 330, 332, 334, 365, Sullivan, W. M., 755, 756
450 366, 367 Sulloway, F. J., 93, 94, 95, 115, 668,
Staudinger, U., 11, 167, 405, 416, Stewart, M., 978 682
419, 552, 569, 570, 572, 577, Stigler, J. W., 717, 718, 756, 761 Sully, J., 111, 114, 119
578, 579, 583, 585, 587, 596, Stigsdotter, A., 612 Suls, J., 641
597, 598, 605, 606, 607, 608, Stock, W. A., 551 Sundet, J., 819
609, 610, 614, 618, 619, 620, Stodolsky, S., 848 Suomi, S. J., 1, 53, 152
622, 625, 627, 628, 631, 633, Stoler, A. L., 883 Super, C., 449, 526, 717, 737, 738,
634, 635, 636, 637, 638, 639, Stoltzfus, J., 845, 881, 887 763, 766
641, 644, 669, 904, 944 Stolz, L. M., 698 Surwit, R. S., 231
Stavy, R., 778 Stone, A., 546 Susman, E. J., 408, 415, 622, 686
Stearns, E., 703 Stone, M., 989 Sutton, K. J., 635, 640
Stearns, P. N., 177 Storch, K.-F., 238 Sutton-Smith, B., 508, 833
Stecher, L. I., 126 Stouffer, S. A., 698 Svanborg, A., 629
Steedman, C. K., 749 Strack, F., 539, 640 Swann, W. B., 538, 553
Steele, C., 538, 749, 834, 839, 845, Strang, P., 589 Swann, W., Jr., 538
846, 848, 853, 870, 871, 881 Stratton, P., 179 Swanson, C. C., 179
1032 Author Index

Swanson, D., 831, 842, 843, 850, Teichmann, H., 417 Thomson, W. M., 684, 703
858, 859, 867, 869, 870, 871, Teitge, D. W., 749 Thoresen, C. E., 915, 923, 982
876, 887 Teitge, J. S., 749 Thorndike, E. L., 95, 137
Swanson, K. R., 179 Tellegen, A., 919, 924 Thorne, B., 771, 774
Sweeney, M. M., 703 Terman, L. M., 103, 127, 128, 821 Thorngate, W., 183
Swidler, A., 755, 756 Terr, L. C., 380 Thornton, A., 668, 676
Swijtink, Z., 172 Tesch-Römer, C., 415, 609 Thouless, R. H., 974
Sylva, K., 507 Tesla, C., 777 Thun, T., 973
Symons, L. A., 241, 242, 245 Tesman, J. R., 62 Thurman, R., 969
Szeminska, A., 342 Tesser, A., 362, 537, 541, 555 Thurstone, L. L., 137
Tetens, J. N., 421, 518, 570, 571, Tian, H. J., 233, 239
Tabor, L. E., 348 572, 574, 600, 618 Tice, D. M., 544
Taine, H., 97 Tetlock, P. E., 768, 770 Tiedemann, D., 164
Tajfel, H., 771 Thalberg, J., 517, 523 Tienda, M., 688
Takagi, S., 702, 706 Thaler, D. S., 597 Tierney, P., 917
Takagi, T., 387 Thatcher, R. W., 281, 348, 382, 383, Tinbergen, N., 96
Takahashi, K., 776 386, 387, 388, 389 Tinker, E., 24, 50
Takanishi, R., 895, 899 Thelen, E., 27, 39, 53, 54, 134, 156, Tipper, S. P., 555
Takata, T., 760 195, 240, 243, 246, 259, 271, Tipton, S. M., 755, 756
Tambs, K., 819 275, 276, 278, 284, 286, 289, Tiret, L., 230
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., 759 290, 291, 292, 293, 294, 295, Titzer, R., 275, 284, 291, 299, 300,
Tamminnen, K., 958, 964, 972, 973, 296, 297, 299, 300, 301, 302, 301, 306
974 303, 304, 306, 332, 347, 348, Tobach, E., 2, 7, 10, 36, 46, 156,
Tanabe, H., 768 356, 357, 408, 430, 605, 606, 415, 469, 495, 518, 763, 798
Tang, Y.-P., 255, 702 622, 903 Tobin, J., 748, 756
Tangney, J. P., 371, 372 Theokas, C., 6, 12, 13, 895 Todd, J., 293
Tannenbaum, P., 327, 374 Thielman, S. B., 986 Todd, P. M., 596, 605
Tanner, J. M., 417 Thieman, T. J., 102 Todd, R. D., 437
Tanner, L., 233 Thoits, P. A., 688, 772 Todorov, A. A., 702
Tapaswi, P. K., 261 Thom, R., 261 Tokoro, M., 776
Tarafodi, R. W., 758 Thomae, H., 417, 438, 526, 569, Tolan, P. H., 926
Tatarkiewicz, W., 555 573, 636 Tolman, E. C., 131, 143, 220, 409
Tatsuoka, M. M., 334 Thoman, E. B., 725 Tolman, J., 906, 920
Taub, E., 609 Thomas, A., 477, 491, 493, 526, Tomarken, A. J., 629
Taylor, A., 233, 684, 703 623, 905 Tomasello, M., 311, 412, 575, 576,
Taylor, B., 415 Thomas, D., 126, 135, 136, 739 597, 750, 751, 762, 777, 778
Taylor, C., 23, 24, 48, 63, 72, 75, 79, Thomas, K., 832, 867 Tomiyasu, U., 385
539, 540, 752, 951 Thomas, M. S. C., 279 Tomkiewicz, S., 232
Taylor, C. E., 747, 748 Thomas, S. A., 238 Tomkins, S., 441
Taylor, C. S., 930 Thomas, W. I., 135, 422, 471, 667, Tomlinson, W. T., 242, 245
Taylor, E., 466, 501 669, 673, 678, 692 Tomlinson-Keasey, C., 623, 638
Taylor, H. F., 233 Thompson, E., 46, 47, 53, 69, 246, Toms, F., 835, 857
Taylor, M., 741 303, 466, 750 Tononi, G., 38, 48, 410
Taylor, S. E., 365, 538, 542, 554, 639 Thompson, H., 132, 414 Tonooka, R., 192
Teasdale, E. J. D., 547 Thompson, L., 585, 614, 615 Tooby, J., 41, 623
Teasley, S. D., 587 Thompson, R. A., 412 Toomela, A., 192, 199, 200
Tecott, L. H., 223 Thompson, R. F., 101 Topoff, H. R., 7
Tees, R. C., 245 Thompson, S. C., 423, 553 Törestad, B., 219, 249, 404, 405, 413,
Tees, R. L., 241, 242, 244 Thompson, W. W., 616 423, 445, 449, 587, 589, 611
Author Index 1033

Toth, S. L., 317 Udry, J. R., 930 van der Weele, C., 231
Toulmin, S., 56, 59, 74, 75, 524, 527 Uhlenberg, P., 670 Van Deusen-Phillips, S. B., 745, 765
Touwen, B. C. L., 348 Ullman, C., 987 VanDevender, T., 735
Toynbee, A., 472 Ulrich, B. D., 27, 259, 290 van Dijk, L., 673
Trabasso, T. R., 278 Ulrich, B. O., 240 van Dijk, M., 320, 323, 329, 348
Tracy, J. L., 49, 412 Ulvund, S. E., 613 Vandiver, B. J., 889
Tran, V., 370 Umerez, J., 178 van Dulmen, M., 925
Traupmann, K., 770 Unzner, L., 546, 739, 740, 757 van Geert, P., 170, 195, 291, 315,
Trause, M. A., 839, 844 Updegraff, R., 127 320, 323, 329, 347, 348, 351,
Treboux, D., 377 Urbano, R., 245 352, 356, 357, 358, 359, 360,
Tremblay, R. E., 447, 685 Uttal, D. H., 35 362, 382, 383, 384, 387, 447
Tretjakoff, I., 223 Uvnäs-Moberg, K., 416 van Gelder, T., 320
Trevarthen, C., 439, 500, 507 Uzgiris, I. C., 345, 350, 384, 387 Van Heck, G. L., 406, 422
Triandis, H. C., 730, 753, 755, 757, van Heerden, J., 431
860 Vaidya, J. G., 632 VanLaningham, J., 682
Triandis, L., 980 Vaihinger, H., 199 van Leeuwen, H. M. P., 388
Trickett, E., 13 Vaillant, G., 469, 637, 644, 666 van Lieshout, C. F. M., 419
Triplett, R. A., 682 Valdivieso, R., 932 Van Nguyen, T., 428, 813
Trivers, R. L., 624 Valentine, J. C., 926 van Os, J., 702
Tronick, E. Z., 735, 791 Valiant, G., 376 Van Parys, M., 329, 334, 349, 378,
Trost, M. A., 427 Vallacher, R. R., 362, 447, 541 379
Trudeau, E. J., 323 Valle, V. A., 546 van ‘t Klooster, B. J., 388
Trudewind, C., 546 Valsiner, J., 5, 20, 21, 27, 28, 38, 63, Van Tol, H. H. M., 227
Trueba, H. T., 881, 882 65, 90, 108, 109, 112, 113, 114, Vapnarsky, V., 777
Truly, T. L., 196 130, 156, 167, 168, 169, 170, Varela, F., 46, 47, 53, 69, 246, 303,
Tryon, W. W., 12 171, 172, 173, 174, 175, 176, 466, 520, 582, 750
Tsai, J., 631, 759 177, 178, 179, 180, 182, 183, Vazsonyi, A. T., 929
Tsay, J. C., 905 184, 185, 187, 188, 189, 190, Velasquéz, O., 765
Tsien, J. M., 702 191, 192, 193, 194, 195, 196, Venter, J. C., 7, 9
Tsien, J. Z., 235, 236, 702 197, 198, 199, 200, 201, 206, Ventis, W. L., 982, 984
Tu, W., 753 247, 248, 356, 405, 413, 449, Ventura-Cook, E., 761
Tucker, D., 248, 492 517, 522, 526, 535, 545, 546, Vereijken, B., 279
Tucker, J. S., 623, 638, 653 549, 576, 597, 644, 751, 772, Verhaeghen, P., 598, 601, 604, 611,
Tucker, S. R., 831, 838, 881 774 615, 616, 617
Tuholski, S. W., 602 van Aken, M. A. G., 352 Verma, A., 680
Tulkin, S. R., 811 van de Mheen, H., 684 Veronneau, S., 238
Tuma, S., 589 Vandenberg, B., 508 Verschure, P. F. M. J., 357
Turbin, M. S., 901, 921, 923 Vandenbergh, J., 218 Vicary, J. R., 429
Turiel, E., 343, 533 van der Lans, J., 988 Vico, G. B., 520
Turkewitz, G., 242, 243, 245, 247, van der Lippe, T., 673 Vidal, F., 998
283, 469 van der Maas, H., 290, 320, 347, Viitanen, M., 612
Turkheimer, E., 613 357, 362 Viken, R. J., 428
Turnball, R., 407 Van der Meulen, M., 751 Villa-Komaroff, L., 217
Turnbull, W., 540 van der Molen, M. W., 388, 601 Villareal, M., 753
Turner, P., 816, 905 Vanderryn, J., 921 Villarruel, F. A., 13, 898, 900
Turner, R. J., 703 van der Veer, R., 63, 65, 90, 108, Villenas, S., 183
Turvey, M., 273, 274, 306 109, 112, 113, 156, 170, 175, Vinden, P. G., 765
Tyler, S. A., 522 187, 188, 189, 190, 191, 199, Vinsel, A., 488
Tyson, R., 179 200, 247, 248, 522 Virshup, L. K., 772
1034 Author Index

Vishton, P., 604 234, 235, 323, 341, 347, 416, Watkins, L. R., 435
Viswanathan, N., 238 702, 903 Watson, D., 492, 632
Viteles, M. S., 118 Wainwright, P., 228 Watson, J., 777
Vogel, P., 973 Waismann, F., 532 Watson, J. B., 2, 91, 121, 129, 130,
Vogler, G. M., 702 Walberg, H. J., 923 131, 165, 171, 403, 405, 521
Volet, S., 412 Waldron, M., 614 Watson, J. S., 545
Volkart, E. H., 667 Waldrop, M., 470, 482 Watson, M. W., 335, 378, 379
von Baer, K. E., 91, 92, 93, 187, Walker, L., 982, 984 Watson-Gegeo, K. A., 743, 744, 766,
211, 212 Walker, S., 959, 970 769
von Bertalanffy, L., 2, 32, 93, 94, Walker-Andrews, A. S., 241, 243 Watt, L. M., 551
141, 214, 215, 257, 268, 269, Walker-Andrews, A. W., 284 Watts, C. L., 376
347, 357, 434 Walkerdine, V., 791 Wax, N., 778
von Cranach, M., 522 Wall, S., 153, 377, 384, 740, 815 Way, N., 924
Vonèche, J. J., 111 Wallace, D. C., 227 Wayment, H. A., 639
von Ehrenfels, C., 185 Wallace, J. G., 339, 346, 364 Weakland, J. H., 145
von Eye, A., 5, 156, 426, 442, 446, Wallbott, H. G., 371 Weaver, L., 620, 635
447, 464, 529, 930 Wallon, H., 143, 189, 366 Webb, A. G., 612
von Hofsten, C., 292, 293, 604 Walters, L. H., 915 Webb, K. S., 881
von Knorring, A.-L., 441 Walters, R. H., 149, 150, 151, 572 Webster, P. S., 552
von Oertzen, T., 589, 591, 603, Wamboldt, F. S., 441 Wechsler, A., 385
606 Wandersman, A., 901, 915, 932 Wegner, D. M., 541
Von Uexküll, J. J., 179 Wang, L., 332, 354, 356, 372, 375 Weigert, A. J., 749
von Wright, G. H., 51, 75, 76, 79 Wang, M. C., 923 Weiler, I. J., 235
Voss, T., 947 Wang, P. P., 213, 214 Weinberg, R. A., 898, 902, 905
Voyat, G., 198 Wang, Q., 746, 757, 759 Weinberger, D. R., 604
Vraa, R., 911, 913, 914, 929 Wang, S.-H., 722, 742, 778 Weiner, B., 534
Vrba, E. S., 605 Wang, X., 741, 746 Weiner, H., 436, 437
Vreeke, G. J., 32 Wapner, S., 39, 69, 70, 432 Weinert, F. E., 582, 586, 605
Vroom, V. H., 522 Warburton, D. M., 407 Weinstein, H., 756, 758
Vuyk, R., 198 Ward, M. C., 499 Weir, M. W., 583
Vygotsky, L., 26, 60, 66, 113, 139, Ward, R. A., 553 Weismann, A., 218
140, 186, 191, 192, 199, 200, Warr, P., 602 Weisner, T., 717, 725, 727, 736, 738,
288, 292, 303, 321, 330, 332, Warren, D. H., 242 762, 773
349, 363, 364, 421, 496, 501, Warren, H. C., 101 Weiss, C. H., 932
508, 519, 522, 526, 546, 553, Warren, J. R., 688 Weiss, M., 751
598, 603, 669, 743, 761, 911 Warren, N., 531 Weiss, P., 181, 215
Wartofsky, M., 56, 59, 70, 534 Weissberg, R. P., 901, 926, 932
Wachs, T. D., 223, 233, 423, 814, Warwick, L., 988 Weisz, J., 547, 552, 759
815, 820 Washburn, R. W., 134 Welch, R. B., 242
Waddington, C., 99, 141, 194, 247, Wasserman, S., 278 Welford, A. T., 321, 601
260, 261, 268, 276, 417, 582, Watanabe, A., 387 Weller, S. C., 774
592 Watanabe, J., 759 Welles-Nystrom, B., 737
Wadsworth, M., 683, 692 Watanabe, K., 169, 192 Wellman, B. L., 127
Wagner, D., 539 Waterman, A. S., 620, 622, 633 Wellman, H. M., 301, 343, 548,
Wagner, R. K., 537, 606 Waters, E., 153, 377, 384, 740 597, 600, 605, 609, 775, 776,
Wahl, H.-W., 579, 590 Waters, I., 815 777
Wahlsten, D., 39, 223, 224, 225, Waters, P. J., 224 Wells, A., 481, 916
226, 227, 228, 229, 230, 231, Watkins, J. W. N., 529 Wells, L. E., 551
Author Index 1035

Wenger, E., 546, 770 Whiting, J. W. M., 149, 719, 725, Wilson, S. M., 929
Wenger, M., 735 732, 735, 738, 739 Wilson, W. H., 183
Wenger Jindra, I., 964 Whitlock, J. L., 927, 931 Wimmer, H., 975
Wentura, D., 541, 548, 551, 552, Wibowo, S., 758 Winant, H., 882, 883, 884
554, 555, 570, 593, 620, 637, Wickens, C. D., 131 Winch, P., 75, 524
638 Wickens, D. D., 131 Winegar, L. T., 176, 195
Wentzel, K., 504 Wicklund, R. A., 537, 538, 540, 553 Winer, B. J., 32
Werboff, J., 152 Wiedl, K. H., 603 Wingard, D. L., 623, 638
Werker, J., 728, 740 Wienbruch, C., 609 Wink, P., 633, 638, 998
Werner, E. E., 621, 697, 905 Wierzbicka, A., 718, 722, 732 Winn, S., 735
Werner, H., 27, 37, 40, 60, 93, 118, Wiese, B. S., 595 Winnicott, D. W., 37, 54, 60
189, 190, 364, 383, 580, 603, Wiesel, T. N., 382, 422, 424 Wisden, W., 216, 247
634, 635 Wiesner, M., 670, 725 Wispé, L., 199
Wertlieb, D., 156, 169, 918 Wigfield, A., 12, 410, 429, 503 Witasek, S., 199
Wertsch, J., 31, 38, 66, 200, 201, Wiggins, R. W., 682 Witherington, D., 384
322, 494, 522, 526, 546, 717, Wilensky, H. L., 684 Witter, R. A., 551
742, 743, 748, 765, 769, 774 Wiley, A., 745, 746, 759 Wittgenstein, L., 19, 35, 75, 80, 175,
West, C., 883 Wiley, E. O., 46 341, 373, 535
West, D. D., 898 Wilkening, F., 605 Wittkowski, J., 974
West, D. J., 695 Willett, J. B., 5, 156, 328, 357, 359, Wohlwill, J., 22, 94, 114, 334, 346,
West, M., 265, 290, 738 589, 685 351, 357, 417, 423, 833
West, M. J., 218, 266 Williams, D., 683 Wolf, M., 335, 980
West, M. M., 356 Williams, E. M., 54 Wolf, S. A., 745, 749
Westen, D., 327 Williams, H. M., 127 Wolf, T. H., 100, 101
Westerhof, G. J., 620 Williams, K., 741, 746, 749 Wolfer, D. P., 238
Weston, D. R., 816 Williams, R. M., Jr., 698 Wolff, P. H., 259, 432
Wethington, E., 807, 821, 822, 823, Williams, R. N., 436 Wolfram, S., 362
824 Williams, R. W., 224 Wollheim, R., 551
Whalen, S., 481, 497, 502 Williams, S., 630, 925 Wolpert, L., 260
Wheaton, B., 687, 701, 703 Williams, W. M., 606 Wong, A. H. C., 227
Wheeler, L. R., 127 Williamson, P. A., 492 Wong, P. T. P., 551
Wheeler, R. E., 629 Willis, P., 770 Wong, W. H., 238
Wheeler, S., 569, 571, 573 Willis, S. L., 575, 584, 585, 590, Wood, D., 498, 545
Whitbourne, S. K., 620, 633 597, 598, 604, 611, 612 Wood, I. C., 216
White, B., 734 Willis, S. W., 584 Wood, J. V., 640
White, G., 717, 718 Willitts, M., 684 Wood, P. K., 332, 351, 358, 386
White, K., 932 Willmott, P., 673 Woodcock, R. W., 583
White, L. K., 682 Wills, C., 432 Woodruff, D. S., 640
White, M. I., 754 Wills, T. A., 539 Woodson, C. G., 879
White, R., 146, 422, 546, 621, 837, Wilmington, D., 245 Woodward, K. M., 630
838, 841, 842, 845, 870 Wilson, E. O., 95, 141, 247, 337, Woolever, C., 981
White, S., 45, 90, 91, 104, 105, 107, 945 Woolley, J., 975
155, 573, 741, 764, 854, 855, Wilson, G., 247 Wortham, S., 745, 748
856, 859, 869, 870 Wilson, J., 323, 332, 351, 352, 429 Wortman, C. B., 553, 642, 687
Whitelaw, E., 225 Wilson, M., 323, 425, 426 Wozniak, R., 466, 501
Whiteman, R., 409 Wilson, R. S., 235 Wozniak, R. H., 535
Whiting, B. B., 149, 321, 356, 364, Wilson, S., 761 Wozniak, R. J., 90, 108, 109, 111,
735, 738, 739 Wilson, S. G., 235 121
1036 Author Index

Wranik, T., 370 Yasumatsu, N., 235 Zazzo, R., 143


Wright, H. F., 833 Yates, E., 576 Zecevic, N., 382, 384, 385
Wright, R. A., 544, 553 Yates, K., 261 Zeigarnik, B., 194
Wright, S., 216 Yates, M., 776, 919, 920, 945, 984 Zelazo, P. D., 53
Wrosch, C., 635 Yi, S., 759 Zeldin, S., 902, 906, 913, 918
Wu, D. Y. H., 756, 759 Yip, T., 772 Zélény, M., 520
Wu, S., 356, 373, 374, 376 Yoerger, K., 695 Zeligs, D. F., 949
Wu, W., 821 Yohalem, N., 906 Zelinski, E. M., 575, 584, 594
Wugalter, S. E., 446 York, P., 441 Zenderland, L., 105
Wulff, D. M., 944, 945, 947, 948, Yorke, J. A., 277 Zenklusen, B., 964
966, 967 Young, C. H., 5, 156, 668 Zernicke, R. F., 294, 296
Wunderle, G., 947 Young, K., 127 Zetterblom, G., 425
Wundt, W., 91, 121, 166, 374, 414 Young, M. D., 673 Zhang, J. H., 409
Wurf, E., 619, 620, 750 Young, N. F., 881, 882 Zhang, Y., 226
Wyer, R. S., Jr., 544 Youngblade, L., 777 Ziegler, G., 438
Wyndhamn, J., 765 Youngblood, J., 831, 887 Zigler, E., 2, 14
Wynn, J., 911 Young-DeMarco, L., 668 Zimbardo, P. G., 438
Wynne, L. C., 131 Younger, B. A., 470 Zinnbauer, B. J., 953
Wynshaw-Boris, A., 223 Youniss, J., 63, 67, 682, 776, 917, Zins, J. E., 926, 932
919, 920, 945, 984 Zirkel, S., 541
Xie, H., 154, 155 Yu, C., 287, 305 Zittoun, T., 183, 201
Xie, Y., 323, 332, 351, 352 Yu, E., 835 Zivin, G., 519, 546
Xuei, X. L., 233, 239 Yu, H. C., 772 Zlatev, J., 288
Yurko, K. H., 638 Znaniecki, F., 471, 667, 669, 673,
Yakovlev, P. I., 385 Yushak, M., 438 678, 692
Yamakoshi, G., 192 Yust, K.-M., 981 Zoghbi, H. Y., 225
Yamamoto, Y., 831 Yuwiler, A., 415 Zohar, A., 45, 72
Yamauchi, B., 305 Yuwiler, J., 415 Zondag, H. J., 964
Yamauchi, N., 387 Zondermann, A. B., 637
Yan, Z., 187, 199, 291, 315, 319, Zacks, R. T., 601 Zuckerman, M., 623
320, 324, 325, 330, 332, 334, Zaff, J., 918, 925 Zukow-Goldring, P., 240
365, 366, 367 Zahn-Waxler, C., 89, 573, 669, 707 Zulehner, P., 945
Yang, K.-S., 717 Zajonc, R. B., 63, 423 zur Oeveste, H., 446
Yarrow, M. R., 150, 157 Zank, S., 590 Zweig, J. M., 923
Subject Index

Abduction, 64 development of control-related beliefs/motivations,


Abduction-transcendental argument, 77–80 546–548
Abused children, developmental pathways, 380 intentional action, developmental aspects of,
Academic/school success, 881–883, 917 545–546
Accentuation dynamic, 691 intentional self-development and the developing
Accommodative processes, 198, 478, 553–555 self, 548–551
Action, 51–53, 516–556. See also Dynamic preserving personal continuity and identity:
development of action and thought assimilative and accommodative processes,
concepts, 521–527 551–555
conceptual constituents, 522–524 personal regulation of developmental processes,
constitutive rules, 524 519–520
constraints of, 524 polyvalence of, 524–526
context of, 526–527 regulative rules, 524
control-system theories of, 522 rise of perspective on human development, 516–521
cultural regulation of development, 518–519 social-constructivist concepts; activity theory, 522
developmental diversity and regularity, action- structural theories of, 521–522
theoretical reconstructions, 527–535 Act psychologists, 409
construction and deconstruction of developmental Adaptation:
coherence, 528–529 and appraisal, 371
developmental plasticity, 529–530 emotion and, 371
invariance and universality in development, selective; development as, 580–582
530–535 Adaptation level theory, 423
weak and strong constraints, 529–530 Adaptive capacity, development as change in, 580–584
experience and, 52–53 Adolescence:
explicating, 522–524 academic/employment outcomes, 851
historical notes, 520–521 African American males, 857–859
intentional self-development and personal control antisocial behavior limited to, 685
over development, 535–544 parenting, 496–498
intention / behavior and, 51–53 transitions to parenthood during adolescence,
motivational theories, 522 685–686
overview/introduction, 555–556 Adultomorphic perspective, 26
personal control over development, 544–555 Affective state, and developmental sequences, 334
action resources and perceived control, 548 African Americans:
developing sense of control and personal agency, academic achievement research, 881–883
547–548 achievement gap, 878–883

1037
1038 Subject Index

African Americans (Continued) Attachment, 151–153, 376–378, 500–501, 740, 979–980


achievement motivation, historical overview, Attractors, 196, 272–274, 289
879–880 Attributions, religious, 990
AHAANA (Asian, Hispanic, African American, and Attunement, 729
Native American), 833, 834 Authoritarian regimes, 472
cultural context, 856–857 Autopoietic systems, 181–182
social movement for education, 880–883
unique experience, male adolescents, 857–859 Bean bag concept of genome, 42
Age: Becoming/ being, 56
advanced (See Older adults) Behavior:
cohorts, 675–677 expressive-constitutive and instrumental-
life course and, 673–679 communicative functions, 22–25, 28
as marker of maturation rate, 417–418 fossilization of, 195
positive youth development, 928–929 holistic-interactionistic model, 414
temporality and, 669 Behavioral ecology, 44
Agency: Behavioral /emotional development, historical
action theory, and development of sense of personal, perspective, 126, 129–132
547–548 Behavioral genetics and life course, future directions,
child as active agent, 21 705–706. See also Genetics
interaction and, 318–319 Behavioral indicators of life mechanics of personality,
living systems as agentive, 318 629–631
person-agent, 50–51, 53–54, 409 Behavioral inheritance, cultural psychology, 719, 720
principle of human agency, 692–694 Bimanual coordination, 297–298
Agency/communion (polar dimensions of complexity), Bioecological model of human development, 793–825,
485 864–868
AHAANA (Asian, Hispanic, African American, and defining properties, 796–799
Native American), 833, 834 developmental assessment of, 824–825
Alcohol, tobacco, or drug (ATOD) use, 925 environmental dynamics:
Allocation of resources in personality functioning, 625 in infancy, 809–810
Analogy as mechanism of development, 45 in old age, 808–809
Animal models and molecular genetics, 223–224 expanding ecological universe, 818–819
A-not-B task, 287, 299–303 experience, 796–797
Anticipatory preadaptation, 197–198 experiments, 808–810
Antisocial behavior: microsystem, 814–820
adolescence-limited, 685 mother-infant dyad as a context of development,
biology and, 415 815–817
early onset, 695 nature-nurture and, 819–820
life-course persistent, 685 overview/introduction, 793–796
Anxiety, experiential perspective, 472 person characteristics and later development, 810–813
Appraisal, in emotion, 371 demand characteristics, 812–813
Approach/avoidance goals, personality development, 631 developmentally generative dispositions inlife-
Approach/withdrawal theory, 469 course perspective, 810–812
Argument from precocity, 344–345 force characteristics, 810–811
Arousal reversals, 479 resource characteristics, 812
“As-if ” structures unfolding in time, 198–199 physical environment, and psychological
Assimilation /assimilative activity, 198, 478, 552–553 development, 814–815
Associative competition as mechanism of development, process, 795
45 proposition I, 797–798
Atomism, 30, 31, 57 proposition II, 798–799, 800–801
Subject Index 1039

proximal processes, 795 Cartesian dualist framework, 337–338


focus of attention in proximal processes, 813–814 Causality:
in solo activities with objects and symbols, 814 action patterns and, 76–77
PVEST (phenomenological variant of ecological developmental, coaction, 218–219
systems theory) and, 864–868 holistic-interactionist model, 435–436
diagram, 864 split mechanical explanation, 72
ecological systems theory, 865–868 systemic, in biological and psychological worlds,
research design and, 799–810 180–182
developmental science in discovery mode, 801–803 Causal models, metatheoretical level, 435
dysfunction versus competence, 803–808 biological model, 435
from research to reality, 822–824 environmentalistic model, 435
time: mentalistic model, 435
micro-, meso-, and macrochronological systems, Cautiousness, balance of mature faith and, 493
820–822 Central conceptual structure, 353
proximal processes, 795 Cerebral asymmetry, 629–630
Biological constraints, dynamic systems, 305 Change:
Biological constructivism, 65 developmental, 22–25
Biological determinism, 43 mechanisms of, dynamic systems, 304–305
Biological embodiment, 48–49 transformational, 23, 25–28, 42
Biological models: variational, 23, 25–28, 42
holistic interactionistic model, 435 Chaos:
life course and, 701–706 edge of, 482
Biological roots, developmental psychology, 92 theory, 197
Biological standpoint, nature-nurture example, 36 Child-centered pattern, 499
Biology. See also Developmental psychobiological Child movement, spiritual, 968–970
systems view: Child psychology, 166–169
antisocial behavior and, 415 blinders of adult psychological concerns, 167–169
cerebral asymmetry, 629–630 versus developmental psychology, 166–167
co-evolution, culture, 575–578 Child study, historical perspective, 119
intellectual development, life span and, 595–619 (see Choice points, 685–686
also Cognitive development) Chunking, 478
life span development and, 575–578, 595–619 Clocks/rulers, 350–352
neuroscience/ brain, 305–306, 382–389, 602, 628–630 Coaction, 218–219
personality development and, 623–625 Co-constructivism, developmental biocultural, 585
person-environment interaction processes, 414–416 Cognition:
well-being and, 415–416 emotion and, 320–336, 370–372
Bio/social interactionism, 43 postformal, 492
Board of Education v. Dowell, 872 Cognitive-cultural theories, religious/spiritual
Boredom, 472, 483 development, 970–971, 975–976
Boundaries/expectations, positive youth development Cognitive development. See also Intellectual
and, 907 functioning across life span:
Brain /neuroscience, 305–306, 382–389, 602, 628–630. constructivist theory (Piaget), 303
See also Biology dynamics systems perspectives, 283–284, 303,
Bridging, 368–370, 498 341–342
Brown v. Board of Education, 868–872, 874 multimodal processes in, 283–284
social framework and dynamic systems (Vygotsky),
Career: 303
lines, 681 stage debate and discovery of variability in,
trajectories of marriage and parenthood, 684–685 341–342
1040 Subject Index

Cognitive development and cultural psychology, in later life, 482–493


764–778 neoteny and, 506–509
hearts and minds, 768–772 organized, 32
feelings, 768–769 parent-child interaction and growth of, 496–501
identities, 770–772 polar dimensions of, 484–489
values, 769–770 agency and communion, 485
methods, 778 divergent and convergent thinking, 486–487
persons and contexts, 772–778 energy and quietude, 488–489
cognitive development as expertise, 776 extroversion and introversion, 488
conceptual development in core or privileged iconoclastic and traditional, 489
domains, 777–778 passive investment and detached objectivity, 486
contexts as linked practices, 773 playfulness and discipline, 487–488
contexts as multiple and contested, 775 psychological; development and, 480–482
contexts as shared meanings or practices,774–775 social context, importance of, 494–496
contexts as warehouses for narratives or teacher-child interaction and growth of, 501–506
interpretations, 773–774 wisdom and, 491–493
nonprivileged domains, 776 Connectionism (dynamic systems), 303
thought and action, 764–768 Connections among growers, 359–361
Cognitive neuroscience approaches, 602 Constructive web, development as, 319–320, 325–329
Cognitive reemergence, modern era, 153–154 Constructivist /constructivism:
Cognitive-stage theories (Goldman), 958–959 alternative, 346–347
Coherence, developmental, 528–529 social and biological, 65
Cohort effects, 611–612, 675–677 theory of cognitive development, 303
Collectivist culture and self-concept, 354 Context, life course development and, 703–705
Columbine High School, 869, 870 Contextual change hypothesis, positive youth
Communication: development, 916–917
communicative action, 24 Contextual /ecological theorists, dynamic systems,
expressive-constitutive and instrumental- 266–271
communicative functions of behavior, 22–25, 28 Control:
language and communicative customs in cultural action theory, 544–555
psychology, 740–749 (see also Language) action resources and perceived control, 548
Communion /agency (polar dimensions of complexity), developing sense of control and personal agency,
485 547–548
Community: development of control-related beliefs/motivations,
change, theory of context and, 913–914 546–548
influence, theory of context and, 910–913 future selves, life plans, and cultural scripts,
Community Change for Youth Development Initiative 550–551
(CCYD), 927 intentional action, developmental aspects of,
Community hypothesis, positive youth development, 545–546
926–928 intentional self-development, 548–551
Compensation. See Selective optimization with ontogeny of conceptual self and internalized
compensation (SOC) model control, 549–550
Competence: dynamic systems, and levels of, 298
versus dysfunction, 803–808 personality development and beliefs about,
versus performance, 278, 346 635–636
Complexity: Control cycles, 691
dialectical thinking and optimal experience, Control-system theories of action, 522
489–493 Conventional interactionism, 43
foundations of, in child development, 493–506 Conventionalism, 71
Subject Index 1041

Coping: selfways (ways of being), cultural diversity in,


personality development and, 635–636, 641–642 754–764
PVEST (phenomenological variant of ecological social organization of childhood experience, 734–738
systems theory), 848 symbolic inheritance, 719
religious/spiritual development as, 948–951, 984 symbolic mediation of experience, 740–749
Core conflictual relationship script, 377 language as practice, 741–742
Cortical connections, cycle of growth of, for each level narrative, 744–749
of skill development, 389 socialization through language, 742–749
Covariation hypothesis, positive youth development, theory of mind, 731–732
920–921 two sides of, 730–731
Creationist theory, 986 unit of analysis problem, 724–730
Creativity in later life, study of, 473, 484 Cultural standpoint, 38
Critical race theory (CRT), 830, 875–878 Culture in human development:
Cross-cultural psychology versus cultural psychology, academic achievement and, 504
721–723 action perspectives, 517, 518–519
Cult /occultist practices, 987–988 age-related decrease in efficiency of, 577–578
Cultural practice theory, 200 age-related increase in need for, 576–577
Cultural psychology of development, 716–779 developmental epistemology, 200–202
attachment, 740 dialogical models of the self, 201
attunement, 729 experiential perspectives, 468–469
behavioral inheritance, 719, 720 generalization, 442–443
classification of practices, 728–730 life span development:
cognitive development and, 764–778 co-evolution with biology, 575–578
hearts and minds, 768–772 intellectual development, 595–618
persons and contexts, 772–778 metatheories, split /relational, 66–68
thought and action, 764–768 personality development, 618–625
versus cross-cultural psychology, 721–723 perspectives on person-environment system,
custom complex, 725–728, 730 430–431
definitions, 719–721 PVEST (phenomenological variant of ecological
disciplinary practices, 729 systems theory), and meaning/experience of,
distributional practices, 729 854–859
early interpersonal experience, effects, 739–740 variation in small-scale developmental sequences,
example, who sleeps by whom in the family, 725–727 334
facilitation, 728 Cumulative continuity, 683
gender practices, 729 Cumulative disadvantages, 683
induction, 729 Custom complex, 725–728, 730
local world of the child, 733–734
maintenance/ loss, 729 Data:
maturation, 728 collected/derived, 172–173
meaning of meaning and context, 724 methods for collection, person-environment
mentalities, 730, 738–739 perspective, 440–441
mental state concepts, special use of, 732–734 Death, conceptions of, 973–975
multiple, diverse psychologies, 723–724 Decalage, Piagetian, 278, 343
versus other approaches to culture and psychology, Decomposition, principles of, 31
719–721 Defense mechanisms, 538
overview/introduction, 716–718, 778–779 Deficits:
processes, 728 as catalysts for progress/growth, 579–580
self-development, 749–754 perspectives, PVEST (phenomenological variant of
self practices, 729 ecological systems theory), 832, 835–836
1042 Subject Index

Deictic pointers, 288 foundations of developmental thinking, 177–179


Demand characteristics of the person as developmental functional structuring, centrality of, 185–186
influences, 812–813 general models for developmental science, 186–195
Depression, core conflictual relationship script for, 377 coordination of the self and the other (Mead),
Detached objectivity/passive investment (polar 190–193
dimensions of complexity), 486 developmental logic (genetic logic), Baldwin and,
Determinism, biological and social, 43 187–188
Deterministic thinking, PVEST (phenomenological dialectical synthesis (Vygotsky), 191
variant of ecological systems theory) and, 831 differentiation and equilibration: dynamics of
Development: structures, 187
concept of, 22–33 differentiation model (Werner), 189–190
expressive-constitutive and instrumental- hierarchy of psychological functions (Janet), 188–189
communicative functions of behavior, processes in the field of life space, 194–195
22–25, 28 theory of dynamic structural forms (Basov), 194
nature of change, 23, 25–28 topological psychology (Lewin), 194
unified, 28–30 methodology and, 201–202
what changes, 22–25 multilevel nature of developmental processes,
framing research agenda, 12–13 178–179
mechanisms of, 45 new discipline of developmental science in the
positive perspective, 11–12 making, 169–170
study of, 1–14 overview/introduction, 166–167, 201–202
taxonomy of theories, 304 science, basics of, 170–177
“ when does it end”: special axiomatic features of development, 177–178
contemporary advances, 155 Developmental manifold concept, 217–218
historical theme, 122–123 Developmental principle, 92–94
Developmental epistemology, 166–202. See also Developmental psychobiological systems view,
Epistemological-ontological issues; Epistemology 210–249. See also Biology
child psychology, blinders of, 167–169 broader implications, 247–248
monocultural assumptions, 168 developmental behavior genetics, 221–239 (see also
reducing complexity to socially accepted norms, Genetics)
168–169 features, 221
culture in human development, 200–202 historical perspective, 140–142, 211–221
developmental systems analysis; rethinking Morgan’s birth of experimental embryology, 211–214
Canon, 179–185 coaction, 218–219
dynamic hierarchies in developmental processes, developmental causality, 218–219
182–183 developmental manifold concept, 217–218
systemic causality in biological /psychological experience defined as functional activity, 220
worlds, 180–182 influences of sensory stimulation on genetic
variability as phenomenon versus error, 183–185 activity, 217
dynamic approaches in developmental psychology, systems versus mechanico-reductive and vitalistic-
195–200 constructive viewpoints, 214–217
development through “as-if ” structures unfolding triumph of epigenesis over preformation, 211
in time, 198–199 triumph of probabilistic epigenesis over
development through disequilibriae (Peirce, predetermined epigenesis, 219–220
Bergson, and Piaget), 196–197 intersensory development, 239–247
dynamic systems theory, 195–196 (see also assumptions underlying traditional approaches,
Dynamic systems theory) 241–243
post-Piagetian and post-Vygotskian models, 199–200 developmental psychobiological systems approach,
progressing equilibration, 196 243–245
Subject Index 1043

system characteristics of (structured organisms social and personality development, 135–136


and structure environments), 245–247 social neobehaviorism, 143–144
traditional approaches to, 240–241 theoretical trends, 143–146
overview/introduction, 210, 221, 248–249 modern era, 147–157
probabilistic epigenesis, 248 attachment, discovery and loss, 151–153
Developmental psychology, making of, 89–157 cognitive reemergence, 153–154
biological roots, 92 social learning: rise, decline, and reinvention,
child study, 119 148–151
developmental principle, 92–94 modern era: contemporary advances in historical
developmental psychopathology, 114–117 themes, 154–156
developmental theory, 118–119 knowledge and consciousness, 154
development and history, 90–92 morality and perfectibility of humans, 155
education, 119–120 nature and nurture, 155
embryology and evolution, 92 ontogeny and phylogeny, 154–155
emergence (1882–1912), 96–104 social applications, 155–156
embryos and infants, 96–100 thoughts and actions, 154
memory and intelligence, 100–104 when does development end, 155
evolution and development, 94–96 psychoanalysis/ Freud, 114–117
foundational period, themes of, 120–124 trends, 117–120
knowledge and consciousness, 120–121 United States, new psychology in, 104–107
morality and the perfectibility of humans, 123 Developmental systems:
nature and nurture, 122 analysis, rethinking Morgan’s Canon, 179–185
ontogeny and phylogeny, 122 defining features of theories, 3, 10–11
relations between thoughts and actions, 121–122 example of metatheory nested within relational
social applications, 123–124 metatheory, 39
when does development end, 122–123 Developmental theory, making, 107–114, 118–119
interdisciplinary science, toward, 156–157 Developmental thinking, foundations of, 177–179
making developmental theory (Baldwin), 107–114 Developmental trajectory, 589, 685. See also
critical evaluation, 111–114 Trajectory(ies)
mental development and social ontogeny, Diachronic perspective, 405
109–111 Dialectical synthesis (Vygotsky), 191
metaphysics and development, 108–109 Dialectical thinking and optimal experience, 489–493
sociogenesis, 110–111 Dialogical models of the self, 201
middle period (1913–1946), 124–147 Differentiation and equilibration, dynamics of
behaviorism and learning, 126, 129–132 structures, 187
comments, 146–147 Differentiation model (Werner), 189–190
developmental psychobiology and ethology, Disciplinary practices, 729
140–142 Discipline/playfulness (polar dimensions of
emotional development, 126 complexity), 487–488
ecological psychology, 145–146 Discontinuity in development, 26
field theory, 145–146 Disequilibriae, development through (Peirce, Bergson,
growth and physical maturation, 126, 132–135 and Piaget), 196–197
institutions and development, 125–126 Dissociation, 379–382
language and cognition development, 138–140 Distal environments, 428
longitudinal study, 126, 128–129 Distributional practices, 729
mental testing, 125, 126–127 Diversity:
moral development, 136–138 action-theoretical reconstructions, 527–535
psychoanalysis, 144–145 from deficit to diversity in developmental science,
research methods, 126 6–7
1044 Subject Index

Diversity (Continued) stage debate and the discovery of variability in


development of diverse groups (see Phenomenological cognitive development, 341–342
variant of ecological systems theory (PVEST)) structure-as-form paradigm, 340–341
fundamental asset of human development, 11 dynamic structuralism, 314–320
intraindividual change, interindividual differences in dynamic nature of psychological structure,
intraindividual change, and fundamental 317–318
substantive significance of, 3 dynamic structure in living systems, 318–319
universality/diversity hypothesis, positive youth form, defined, 315
development, 928 structure, defined, 315
Domain specificity theory, 343–344 structure/form problem, 315
Down syndrome, 214, 500 variability in “ the middle of things”; exampleof
Drawing Hands (Escher), 33–34, 36, 37 representing social interactions, 315–317
DSM III-R glossary of terms for defining mental variation and order in development; the
disorders, and religious content, 986 constructive web, 319–320
Dualism, 57, 58 emotions and dynamic organization of activity and
Duration, concept of, 682 development, 370–382
Dynamic development of action and thought, attachment, working models, and temperament,
313–390 376–378
building structures; transition mechanisms, culture and emotion, 374–375
microdevelopment, and new knowledge, developmental shifts in emotions about self in
363–370 family roles, 378–379
bridging, process of building new knowledge, development of emotional splitting and
368–370 dissociation, 379–382
construction processes, from micro to macro, dynamic shifts of positive/negative biases in webs,
364–370 378
growth curves, chaotic/scalloping/stable, 367 emotionally organized development, 376–382
growth curves, novice/intermediate/expert, 367 emotion and cognition together, 370–372
multiple dimensions of learning and development, families of emotions, dimensions, and cultural
367–368 variations, 373–376
relations between micro- and macrodevelopment, hierarchy of emotion categories (diagram), 374
363–364 one-way effects on developmental webs for shame
shapes of growth curves in construction and and honor, 376
generalization of new skills, 365–367 organizing effects of emotions, 372–376
shift of focus in transitional states, 365 scripts, 372–373
cognitive and emotional development, dynamic methodology, dynamic structural analysis,
structure in, 320–336 (see also Cognitive 347–363
development) building and testing models of growth and
crisis of variability and the Cartesian synthesis in development, 356–363
developmental science, 336–347 clocks, rulers, and repeated measures, 350–352
Cartesian dualist framework, 337–338 equilibration, disturbance, and the shapes of
competence/performance models, 346 development, 361–363
constructivist alternative, 346–347 guidelines for developmental research, 349–356
domain specificity theory, 343–344 logistic growth, 357–359
explaining variability versus “explaining away,” nonlinear dynamic models of growth and
342–346 development, 357
modern synthesis in psychology; nativism and single context assumption—not, 349
empiricism together, 338–340 single-level, single-competence assumption—not,
neo-nativist movement, 344–346 349
reasserting stage theory, 343 single-person assumption—not, 349
Subject Index 1045

single-shape assumption—not, 349 mathematical formalisms and simulations,


starting in the middle of things; implications for 306–307
design, 348–349 mechanisms of change, 304–305
nature/nurture; growth cycles of psychological and mental representations, 306
brain activity, 382–388 nativist theory of language development
overview/introduction, 313–314 (Chomsky), 303
stability and variability in development, 388–390 sensorimotor bases of higher cognition, 306
Dynamic force fields (Lewin), 268 social interaction, 305
Dynamic hierarchies in developmental processes, structuring role of external information, 305
182–183 taxonomy of developmental theories, 304
Dynamic order in hierarchical complexity, 179 theory of cognitive development in social
Dynamic skill theory, 332–336 framework (Vygotsky), 303
Dynamic structural forms, theory of (Basov), 194 theory of perception and perceptual development
Dynamic systems theory, 195–196, 258–307 (Gibson), 303
adaptation of Waddington’s epigenetic landscape, overview/introduction, 258–259, 307
276 pendulum as dynamic system, 272, 273
attractors and dynamic stability, 272–274 phase-space diagram of development (Waddington),
bimanual coordination, 297–298 261
cognitive development, multimodal processes in, rationale, 307
283–284 reach, learning to, 292–297, 298
contextual and ecological theorists, 266–271 research, approach to, 289–292
development as selection, 284–286 characterizing the behavioral attractor states, 289
development from perspective of, 276–297 describing dynamic trajectory of collective
dynamic epigenetic landscape, 276–277 variable, 289–290
dynamic force fields (Lewin), 268 identifying collective variable of interest, 289
embodiment, 287–289 identifying points of transition, 290
embryology, lessons from (form from a formless identifying potential control parameters, 290–292
egg), 260–263 manipulating putative control parameters to
epigenesis in developmental psychobiology, 263–266 experimentally generate transitions, 291–292
epigenetic landscape, 261, 281 skill, defining, 298–307
experience-dependent plasticity, 282–283 soft assemblies, 274–275
exploration, 286–287 synergetics, 271
fluctuations/transitions, 275–276 systems theory as metaphor, 268–271
general systems theories, 267–271 Dysfunction versus competence, 803–808
intellectual heritage; developmental process, 259
layered levels of analysis, 279–281 Ecological perspectives:
model of levels of control, 298 behavioral ecology, 44
mountain stream metaphor, 263 biology and (see Bioecological model of human
multicausality, 281–282 development)
nested timescales, 277–289 history of ecological psychology, 145–146
order from complexity, 271–272 phenomenological variant (see Phenomenological
and other theories of development, 303–304 variant of ecological systems theory (PVEST))
biological constraints, 305 Edge of chaos, 482
brain development as source of constraints, Education:
305–306 academic/school success, 881–883, 917
connectionism, 303 educational pathways, 681
constructivist theory of cognitive development historical perspective, development and, 119–120
(Piaget), 303 Montessori and traditional school environments,
dynamics as source of structure and change, 306 compared, 503–506
1046 Subject Index

Education (Continued) Empiricism /materialism /objectivism, 57–58


religious schools, 980–981 Empowerment, positive youth development and, 907
teacher-child interaction and growth of complexity, Enabling, affective/cognitive (supportive/challenging),
501–506 497
EEG graphs, 387, 388 Encoding as mechanism of development, 45
Elderly. See Older adults Endpoints, developmental, 26
Embodiment, 47–54, 287–289 Energy/quietude (polar dimensions of complexity),
biological, 48–49 488–489
defined, 47–49 Engagement, youth, 918
developmental oriented embodied action metatheory, Environment(s), 420–431
47–54 actual /perceived, 422
dynamic systems, 287–289 as changing stage for individual functioning and
person, biology, culture, 48 development, 428–429
person-centered, 49–54 concept of context, 421
sociocultural, 49 conceptual and empirical analyses of, 422
Embryogenesis, 19 cross-cultural perspective, 430–431
Embryology, 96–100, 211–214, 260–263 in developmental research, 421–422
Emergence zones, 363 distal, 428
Emotion: formal and informal societal regulations, 428
attachment, working models, and temperament, social and economic conditions, 428
376–378 family as, 425–426
culture and, 374–375 formative and eliciting events, 424
developmental shifts in, about self in family roles, optimal, 423–424
378–379 peers, 426–428
development of emotional splitting and dissociation, physical, 422
379–382 proximal dynamic person-environment systems, 425
dynamic organization of activity and development, significant events, 424–425
370–382 social-cultural, 422
dynamic shifts of positive/negative biases in webs, as source of information and stimulation, 422–423
378 synchronization, 429–430
emotionally organized development, 376–382 Environmental dynamics, bioecological model:
emotion and cognition together, 370–372 infancy, 809–810
families of; dimensions, and cultural variations, old age, 808–809
373–376 Environmentalistic model, 435
hierarchy of emotion categories (diagram), 374 Epigenesis, 211, 219–220, 248, 263–266
mediating mental system, 412 Epigenetic landscape, 261, 276–277, 281
one-way effects on developmental webs for shame Epigenetic robotics, 288
and honor, 376 Epiphenomenalism, 750
organizing effects of, 372–376 Epistemological-ontological issues, 54–70
person-environment and, 412 culture and development in split and relational
process (diagram), 371 metatheories, 66–68
scripts, 372–373 empiricism /materialism /objectivism, 57–58
in self-evaluation, 540 Marxist split tradition, 65–66
Emotional development: methodology, 70–80
Lewis’s three scales of, 280 modernity and elaboration of relational metatheory,
positive youth development, 909 58–65
Emotional discomfort, 839 Hegel, 59–60, 61–63
Emotional splitting, 379–382 hermeneutics, 63–65
Emotional tone and reactivity, personality development Kant, 60–61
and, 630–631 Leibniz, 58–59
Subject Index 1047

phenomena-noumena split, 61 optimal arousal, human nature and, 469–471


phenomenological constructivism and realism, 63 optimal experience theory, 474–482
relational developmental reconciliation of mind flow and development, 479–480
and nature, 61–63 Montessori educational model and, 504–505
modernity and rise of the split tradition, 56–58 optimal experience of flow, 477–479
Plato and Aristotle and relational developmental other perspectives on self-environment
tradition, 55–56 equilibrium, 476–477
pragmatism, 68–70 phenomenological extension of Piaget, 474–475
split mechanical explanation, 70–75 psychological complexity and development,
Epistemology: 480–482
defined, 54 overview/introduction, 465–466, 509–510
developmental (see Developmental epistemology) parenting, 495–501
principles (terminology), 21 in adolescence, 496–498
Equifinality principle, 182, 185, 213 in childhood, 498–499
Equilibration, 196, 361–363, 474 in infancy, 499–501
Equilibrium, 21, 198, 475–477 play, syntelic character of play, 507–509
Ethology, 140–142 self-regulation of experience, potential, 471–472
Events: social context, importance of, 494–496
formative and eliciting, 424 teacher-child interaction and growth of complexity,
significant, 424–425 501–506
Evolution, development and: Experiments, role in bioecological model, 808–810
history, 94–96 Exploration, 286–287
relational history and relational models, 39–41 Expressive-constitutive and instrumental-
split approaches, 41–47 communicative functions of behavior, 22–25, 28
Evolutionary perspective: Extroversion /introversion (polar dimensions of
analogy with, 168–169 complexity), 488
intellectual functioning, mechanics/pragmatics, 604
selection benefits decreasing with age, 575–576 Facilitation (cultural psychology), 728
versus synchronic/diachronic, 405 Faith developmental theory (Fowler), 959–961
Executive function of personality, 621 Family(ies):
Exosystem, 818 in context, 426
Experience(s): as environment in the socialization process, 425–426
bioecological model, 796–797 reciprocity in relations and levels of analysis,
dynamic systems and, 282–283 425–426
as functional activity, 220 religious/spiritual development, 977–980
heterogeneity of, 188 time dimension and, 426
plasticity and, 282–283 “Feeling into” others (EinFühlung) intersubjectivity,
Experience Sampling Method (ESM), 468, 503 199
Experiential perspective on development, 465–510 Feelings, 768–769
child development, foundation of complexity in, Field theory, 145–146
493–506 Flow, 467–471, 477–480, 505
complexity in later life, examples, 482–493 absence of, 470
dialectical thinking and optimal experience, development and, 479–480
489–493 experiences, 467–468
dimensions, 484–489 Montessori and, 505
wisdom, 491–493 optimal arousal of, 469–471
developing person in context, 468–469 optimal experience of, 477–479
historical context, 466–468 organizing construct, 471
ideal outcomes of adult development, 472–474 Force characteristics as shapers of development,
neoteny, 506–509 810–811
1048 Subject Index

Form, 185, 315, 340–341 heredity/environment, separating effects of, 228–236


Fossilization of behavior, 195 documenting norm of reaction, 231–232
Foundationalism, 31, 55, 56 hardware-software distinction, 235–236
Frames of reference: heritability and additivity, 229–231
individual-ecological, 174 in humans, 232–233
individual-socioecological, 174–175 scope of gene-environment interactions, 233–235
inter-individual, 173–174 heritability, changes in across life span, 614–615
intra-individual, 173 implications of new molecular biology, 236–239
organizing knowledge construction, 173–175 molecular markers, 238–239
Freeman v. Pitts, 872 targeted mutations, 238
Fully functioning person (Rogers), 476 Germany, West / East, 691
Functional interaction, 434–436 Goals:
causality in, 435–436 action theory, 540–544, 553–555
nonlinearity, 435 adjusting, 553–555
reciprocity and interdependence, 434–435 chronic, 543–544
Functional pleasure, 475 developmental, 540–544
Functional structuring, centrality of, 185–186 personal life investment (PLI ) and, 636
selection of, and life priorities, 638–639
Gain-loss dynamic, development as, 582–584 semantic and procedural specification, 542–543
Gender: God. See Religious and spiritual development
cultural psychology, 729 Goodness of fit, 481
life course and principle of timing, 694 Great Depression, 678, 690, 692, 693, 696, 698
positive youth development, 928 Growth curves, 356–363
Generalization, 325, 365–367, 441–443, 446–447 Growth/physical maturation, historical themes, 126,
General systems theories, 267–271 132–135
Generations, life course development / life cycle and, Guided participation as universal process, 498–499
669, 673
Genetics, 221–239 Hardware-software distinction, 235–236
bean bag concept of genome, 42 Harmonious-equipotential systems, 213
development and, 221–239 Health and well-being, religious/spiritual development
animal models, 223–224 and, 982–983
approaches to genetic analysis of development, Heredity. See Genetics
221–225 Hermeneutics, 63–65
individual development, 221 Heterarchy, 195
population approach, 221–222 Hierarchy of psychological functions (Janet), 188–189
single gene and multiple factor research on Historical cohorts, 586–587
individual differences, 224–225 Historical time and place, principle of, 697–700
sources of individual differences, 225 Holism, 32–33
ecological systems theory, genotype in, 867 Holistic-interactionistic model, 431–439. See also
gene-environment interactions in the life course, Person-environment relations, holistic-
702–705 interactionistic approach
gene expression arrays, 237–238 Hypotheses testing, theory-driven versus pseudo-
gene tally, 236–237 empirical, 173
heredity, 225–228
at conception, 225–226 ICE. See Identity-focused cultural ecological (ICE)
dissecting heredity, 228 perspective
non-Mendelian, 227–228 Iconoclastic/traditional (polar dimensions of
operational definition of, 227 complexity), 489
studying difference in, 226–227 Ideal outcomes of adult development, 472–474
Subject Index 1049

Identity(ies): processing speed, 601


cultural psychology, and cognitive development, search for determinants of mechanic development,
770–772 600–601
positive, and positive youth development, 908 working memory, 601
preserving; assimilative and accommodative heritability across life span, changes in, 614–615
processes, 551–555 intellectual growth during adulthood, stage
research emphasizing (phenomenological variant of conceptions versus functionalist approaches,
ecological systems theory), 842, 843 606–607
Identity-focused cultural ecological (ICE) perspective, malleability/plasticity, 611–612
829, 846–847, 852 normative versus person-specific pragmatic
Identity moment of analysis, 33–34 knowledge, 605–606
Identity of opposites, 33–36 old age, mechanics/pragmatics in, 615–617
Idiosyncratic influences, nonnormative, 586–587 overview/introduction, conclusions, 617–619
Immediate feedback cycle, 191 prototypical examples:
Immigrants, special experiences of, 859–864 adult age differences in upper limits of short-term
Induction, 72–73, 729 memory (serial word recall), 603–604
Infancy: speed and knowledge in aging typists, 610–611
complexity and parenting in, 499–501 stability in, 612–613
effective of mother’s responsiveness, 800, 801 predicting childhood intelligence on basis of infant
environmental dynamics in, 809–810 behavior, 613
mother-infant dyad as context of development, relative interindividual stability after infancy,
815–817 613–614
object permanence, 345 two-component model (mechanics versus
personality development, and links to old age, pragmatics), 596–600
621–622 cognitive mechanics, 596–597
predicting childhood intelligence on basis of behavior cognitive pragmatics, 597
in, 612 divergence in life span trajectories between
Inference-making perceptions, 844–847 mechanics and pragmatics, 597–598, 600
Inhibition and interference, 601 relations to other theories, 600
In medias res, 198, 315–317, 318, 348–349, 371 varieties of mechanic/pragmatic interdependence,
Institutions and development, 125–126 609–611
Instrumentalism, 71 age of peak performance in complex skills,
Instrumentalism-conventionalism, 74–75 609–610
Intellectual development, and positive youth mechanic-pragmatic interdependence, evidence at
development, 909 cortical level, 609
Intellectual functioning across life span, 595–619. See wisdom as expertise, 676–609
also Cognition; Cognitive development: Intellectual heritage, developmental process (dynamic
biology and culture of, 596–600 systems), 259
cognitive intervention work: activation of learning Intelligence and memory, 100–104
potential among older adults, 612 Intentional action, developmental aspects of, 545–546
cohort effects, period effects, and environmental Intentionality, 903–904
change, 611–612 Intentional self-development, 535–544, 548–551
crystallized pragmatics of cognition, 599, 604–609 activities of, 535–536
evolutionary perspective, mechanics and pragmatics attentional focus, 537–538
in, 604 complexity and differentiation, 537
fluid mechanics of cognition, 600–604 and the developing self, 548–551
age-based differences; testing the limits, 603–604 goals, 540–544
cognitive neuroscience approaches, 602–603 protective and defensive mechanisms, 538
inhibition and interference, 601–602 self-evaluation processes, 538–540
1050 Subject Index

Intentional self-development (Continued) Life-course theory, 665–707


self-observation and self-monitoring, 536–538 age, 672–679
structure and process, 535–536 cohorts, 675–677
Interactionism, 57 temporality and, 669
bio/social, 43 basic concepts and perspectives, 679–689
conventional, 43 selection, endogeniety, and contextual effects,
Interest convergence, 876 687–689
Interindividual differences, in developmental social pathways, cumulative processes, and the
trajectories, 589 individual life course, 680–684
Internal feed-forward cycle, 191 trajectories, transitions, and turning points,
International nature of contemporary psychology, 171 684–687
Interpersonal experience, effects of early, 739–740 biological models, integrating, 701–706
Intersensory development, 239–247 future directions, 705–706
developmental psychobiological systems approach, gene-environment interactions, 702–705
243–245 concepts of human development across the life span,
system characteristics (structured organisms and 670–672
structure environments), 245–247 contributions to study of human development,
traditional approaches, 240–243 700–701
Intransitive hierarchies in real life, 183 cycles, 672–673
Introversion /extroversion (polar dimensions of generations, 669, 673
complexity), 488 historical account, 668–679
life cycle, 669, 677–679
Jung (religious/spiritual development), 949 life transitions and historical change, 689–701
linking mechanisms, 689–691
Kant, 60–61 overview/introduction, 665–668, 706–707
Kellogg Youth Initiative Partnerships (KYIP), 927 paradigmatic principles, 691–700
“Knowing” (dynamic systems), 303 historical time and place, 697–700
Knowledge and consciousness: human agency, 692–694
contemporary advances, 154 life-span development, 669, 692 (see also Life span
historical theme, 120–121 development)
linked lives, 695–697
Language: timing, 694–695
acquisition of, person-environment perspective, relationship view of life patterns, 673
412 research traditions and concepts, 669
and cognition development ( history), 138–140 roles, 672–673
culture and, 740–749 social organization of lives, 672–673
narrative, 744–749 sociocultural patterns in human experience, 674–675
as practice, 741–742 Life space, 194–195
socialization through, 742–749 Life span development, 569–644
Learning: concluding comments, 643–644
commitment to, and positive youth development, 907 historical background, 571–574
growth from novice to expert, 367 level 1 (overall architecture; first view from
multiple dimensions of, 367–368 perspectives of biological and cultural co-
Leibniz, 58–59 evolution), 574, 575–578
Levels of discourse in understanding a domain of age-related decrease in efficiency of culture,
inquiry, 20 577–578
Levels of organization, developmental age-related increase in need for culture, 576–577
systemstheories, 3 evolutionary selection benefits decreasing with
Life-course persistent antisocial behavior, 685 age, 575–576
Subject Index 1051

level 2 ( life span changes in relative allocation of Living systems, dynamic structure in, 318–319
resources to distinct functions of development), Logic, developmental /genetic (Baldwin), 187–188
574, 578–580 Logistic growth, 357–359
deficits as catalysts for progress, 579–580 Logos/mythos, 492
growth versus resilience (maintenance) versus Longitudinal hypothesis, positive youth development,
regulation of loss, 578–579 923–925
level 3 (metatheoretical propositions about life span Longitudinal study, historical perspective, 126,
developmental theory), 574, 580–591 128–129
cross-sectional to longitudinal to sequential, Loss, regulation of, 578–579
587–589 Love:
development as gain-loss dynamic, 582–584 and discipline, parenting behaviors, 495
development as selection and selective adaptation prototypical script for adult, 373
(optimalization), 580–582 Love of fate, 476–477
experimental simulation of development, 589–590
family of theoretical propositions characteristic of Macro-model of developmental influences, 586–587
life span developmental psychology, 581 Maintenance/ loss, 578–579, 729
focus on plasticity and age-associated changes in Malleability, in intellectual functioning, 611–612
plasticity, 584–586 Mammalian coat coloration, 262
macro-model of developmental influences, Manifold concept, developmental, 217–218
586–587 Marginalized youth, frequently, 833
methodological developments, 587–591 Marxist split tradition, 65–66
ontogenetic and historical contextualism as Mastery, life-events and, 688
paradigm, 586 Materialism:
reformulating concept of development from dialectical, 31
functionalist perspective, 580–584 empiricism /objectivism and, 57–58
selective age-related change in adaptive capacity, Materialist ontology, 55
580–582 Mathematical formalisms and simulations, dynamic
testing-the-limits paradigm, 590–595 systems, 306–307
level 4 (systemic and overall theory; selective Maturation:
optimization with compensation), 574, 591–595 concept of, 27
age gradients, 594–595 cultural psychology and, 728
compensation, 593–593 religious/spiritual development as, 947–948
definitional frames, 592 Meaning of meaning and context, 724
definition of selection, optimization, and Measurement models, 443–447
compensation, 591–593 individuality and generalization, 446–447
dual-task research and behavioral indicators, measurement model 1 (MM1), 443–445
594–595 measurement model 2 (MM1), 445–446
empirical evidence, 594 pattern analysis of environments, 447
examples from proverbs and questionnaire items, pattern analysis of individuals, 446
592 Mechanico-reductive and vitalistic-constructive
management and mastery of life tasks, 595 viewpoints; systems versus, 214–217
optimization, 593 Mechanics. See Intellectual functioning across life
selection, elective and loss-based, 593 span; Personality development across the life span
level 5 examples, 575–617 Mediating mental system, 409–414
overview/introduction, 569–571, 574 comments, 414
toward psychological theories of; five levels of development of, 413
analysis, 574–575 emotions, 412
Lines of sight, 36 language and language acquisition, 412
Linked lives, principle of, 695–697, 822 motivation, 412–413
1052 Subject Index

Mediating mental system (Continued) relational scientific, 74–80


self-consciousness, self-perceptions, and self- abduction-transcendental argument, 77–80
evaluations, 411–412 causality and action patterns, 76–77
subconscious processes, 410 interpretation and observation, 75–76
values, valuations, and norms, 410–411 split mechanical explanation, 70–75
Memes, 472 causal explanation (step 2), 72
Memory and intelligence, 100–104 induction of interpretation free hypotheses,
Mental development and social ontogeny, 109–111 theories, laws (step 3), 72–73
Mental disorders, religious/spiritual development and, instrumentalism-conventionalism, 74–75
989 positivism and neopositivism, 73–74
Mentalistic model, 435 reduction description (step 1), 71–72
Mentalities, cultural, 730, 738–739 terminology, 20
Mental representations, dynamic systems, 306 testing the limits, 590
Mental state concepts, special use of, cultural Microdevelopment:
psychology, 732–734 construction processes: from micro to macro,
Mental testing, 125, 126–127 364–370
Mesosystem, 820 relations between micro- and macrodevelopment,
Metamethod, 21 363–364
Metaphysics, 54, 108–109 Microgenesis, 19
Metatheories, 20–21 Microgenetic investigations, 190
levels of discourse in understanding a domain of Microsystem:
inquiry, 20 beyond, 817–820
relational, 3, 5–6, 19, 32–39 defined, 866
holism, 32–33 magnified, 814–817
identity of opposites, 33–36 Middle of things (in medias res), 198, 315–317, 318,
implications for developmental science, 5–6 348–349, 371
opposites of identity, 36–37 Military service, transition to, 698–700
synthesis of wholes, 37–39 Mind:
split, 19, 30–32 and nature (Hegel’s relational developmental
Method of double stimulation (MeDoSt), 191–192 reconciliation of ), 61–63
Methodology: as specialized living system, 318
building/testing models of growth and development, theory of, 731–732
356–363 worldviews and self-perceptions, 452
connections among growers, 359–361 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI ),
equilibration, disturbance, and the shapes of 986
development, 361–363 Missouri v. Jenkins, 872
logistic growth, 357–359 Model /paradigm (terminology), 21
nonlinear dynamic models of growth and Models of growth/development, building/testing,
development, 357 356–363
consistencies, two kinds, 176 connections among growers, 359–361
cross-sectional to longitudinal to sequential, equilibration, disturbance, and the shapes of
588–589 development, 361–363
cycle, 175–177, 201 logistic growth, 357–359
as epistemic cycle, 175–177 nonlinear dynamic models of growth and
experimental simulation of development, 589–590 development, 357
guidelines for developmental research, 349–356 Modern era, 147–157
metamethods, 20 attachment, discovery and loss, 151–153
objectivity through subjectivity, 176–177 cognitive reemergence, 153–154
Subject Index 1053

contemporary advances in historical themes, 154–156 Natural selection, 42


knowledge and consciousness, 154 Nature and nurture:
morality and perfectibility of humans, 155 bioecological interpretation, 819–820
nature and nurture, 155 biological determinism, 43
ontogeny and phylogeny, 154–155 bio/social interactionism, 43
social applications, 155–156 contemporary advances, 155
thoughts and actions, 154 contrast, 167
when does development end, 155 conventional interactionism, 43
social learning: rise, decline, and reinvention, 148–151 hereditary and environmental factors in individual
Modernity: development, 416–418
and elaboration of relational metatheory, 58–65 historical theme, 122
and rise of the split tradition, 56–58 joining; growth cycles of psychological and brain
Molecular markers, 238–239 activity, 382–388
Moments of analysis, 33–34 clusters of discontinuities, 383
Monad, 58 concurrence of independent growers, 384
Monism, 58 cycles of discontinuities forming levels and tiers, 385
Montessori and traditional school environments, cycles of reorganization in development, 385–388
compared, 503–506 domain specificity of activities and localization of
Moral development: brain functions, 384
historical perspective, 136–138 emergence of neural networks and action control
religious/spiritual development, 983–984 systems, 385
Morality and perfectibility of humans: epigenesis of action, feeling, thought, and brain,
contemporary advances, 155 382–383
historical theme, 123 growth cycles in brain, 387–388
Morgan’s Canon, 179–185 hierarchical growth of action and thought, 386–387
Motivation, person-environment and, 412–413 principles for understanding growth patterns of
Motivational theories of action, 522 brain and behavior, 383–385
Mountain stream metaphor, 263 social determinism, 43
Moving to Opportunity (MTO), 688–689 strategies, 43
Multicausality, dynamic systems, 281–282 Neighborhoods:
Multidisciplinarity and need for change-sensitive assessments, phenomenological variant of ecological
methodologies, 3 systems theory, 870
Multiple context model, 817 social integration, 818
Mutual interweaving, 200 well-being and, 689
Mythos/ logos, 492 Neo-Darwinian:
metatheory and mechanisms of development
Narrative: (variational change) diagram, 44
children’s, variability in, 316–317 synthesis, 41
cultural psychology: Neo-nativist movement, 344–346
dynamics of practice, 748–749 Neoteny, 506–509
getting stories straight, 744–749 Neuroscience/ brain, 305–306, 382–389, 602, 628–630.
storytelling beginning early, 745–747 See also Biology
variability and heterogeneity, 747–748 New York City Beacons project, 927
varieties of oral, 745 Nice-mean social interactions:
Nativism: developmental web, 326
modern synthesis, empiricism and, 338–340 developmental web biased toward nice interactions,
neo-nativist movement, 344–346 328
theory of language development (Chomsky), 303 stories, 330
1054 Subject Index

Nondeterministic theorizing, 841 Optimal /functional levels, 386


Nonlinearity: Optimism, 3
dynamic models of growth and development, 357 Optimization. See Selective optimization with
holistic-interactionist model, 435 compensation (SOC) model
Normative versus person-specific pragmatic Optimization processes, 904
knowledge, 605–606 Order from complexity, 271–272
Novelty, 181–182, 433–434 Order parameters, 271
Novice/intermediate/expert skill use, growth curves, Organization, holistic-interactionist model, 437–439
367 Orthogenetic principle, 60, 189–190
Overaccommodation, 478
Objectivism, 57–58 Overassimilative mode, 478
Objectivity through subjectivity, 176–177
Object permanence in infants, 345 Paratelic, 479
Object relations theorists, religious/spiritual Parenthood, transitions to during adolescence, 685–686
development and, 950–951 Parenting:
Older adults: bioecological model, mother-infant dyad, 800, 801,
cognitive intervention work; activation of learning 815–817
potential among, 612 complexity and (experiential perspective), 496–501
complexity in later life, examples, 482–493, 615–617 adolescents, 496–498
covariance dedifferentiation, 615–616 children, 498–499
directionality dedifferentiation, 615 infants, 499–501
environmental dynamics, 808–809 cultural psychology; models/strategies, 738–739
maintenance of divergence in explanatory customary scripts for interaction, 739
corrrelational patterns, 616–617 moral direction, 738–739
mechanics and pragmatics in very old age, 615–617 pragmatic design, 739
religious/spiritual development and, 985 “Parent persons,” 499
speed and knowledge in aging typists, 610–611 Parsimony, principle of, 179–185
Ontogenesis, 19, 24 Pars pro toto error, 168
Ontogenetic and historical contextualism as paradigm, Partially ordering scaling, 334
586 Passive investment and detached objectivity (polar
Ontogeny and phylogeny: dimensions of complexity), 486
contemporary advances, 154–155 Pathology:
historical theme, 122 developmental psychopathology, historical
Ontology: perspective, 114–117
defined, 54 religious/spiritual development and, 985–990
ontological principles, 21 cult and occultist practices, 987–988
and the Real, 54–55 mental disorders, 989
Open systems, 271–272 terrorist groups (religious/spiritual development),
Opposites of identity, 36–37 988–989
Optimal arousal, human nature and, 469–471 toxic beliefs/practices/attitudes, 989–990
Optimal environments, 423–424 Pattern analysis:
Optimal experience theory, 474–482 of environments and person-environment systems,
flow and development, 479–480 447
Montessori educational model and, 504–505 of individuals, 446
optimal experience of flow, 477–479 Pattern formation, 285
other perspectives on self-environment equilibrium, Peers/peer groups, 426–428
476–477 Pendulum as dynamic system, 272, 273
phenomenological extension of Piaget, 474–475 Perception and perceptual development, theory of
psychological complexity and development, 480–482 (Gibson), 303
Subject Index 1055

Perfecting, religious/spiritual development as, 951–952 Personal regulation of developmental processes (action
Person-agent, 50–51, 53–54, 409 theory), 519–520
Personal attributes model, 866–867 Personal /social assets facilitating positive youth
Personal control. See Control development, 909
Personality consistency, person-environment Person-centered embodiment, action, and development,
interaction, 418–419 49–54
Personality development across the life span, 619–643 Person characteristics, and later development
allocation of resources, 625 ( bioecological model), 810–813
approach/avoidance goals, 630–631 demand characteristics, 812–813
approaches to study of, 619–620 developmentally generative dispositions in life-
behavioral indicators, development of, 630–632 course perspective, 810–812
biology and culture, illustrations of role of, 623–625 force characteristics, 810–811
cerebral asymmetry, 629–630 resource characteristics, 812
continuity and change, life-long transactional Person-context model, 866
adaptation, 626–637 Person-environment relations, holistic-interactionistic
development of neurophysiological indicators of approach, 400–452
life mechanics of personality, 628–630 classical interactionism, 406
dynamic interaction of mechanics/pragmatics, cultural and cross-cultural research, 449
627–628 environment, 402–403, 420–431
evidence on development of life pragmatics of distal, 403
personality, 632–637 proximal, 403
mechanics and pragmatics of life as relevant to general approaches to, 405–409
personality functioning, 626–627 goal of scientific psychology, 403–404
mechanics of life as relevant to personality holistic-interactionistic model, 404–405, 431–439
functioning, 626–627 biological model, 435
pragmatics of life as relevant to personality causal models, metatheoretical level, 435
functioning, 627 environmentalistic model, 435
summary, 637 functional interaction, 434–436
control and self efficacy beliefs, 635 mentalistic model, 435
coping, 635–636, 641 nonlinearity, 435
emotional tone and reactivity, 630–631 organization, 437–439
foci/facets, summarizing, 642–643 reciprocity and interdependence, 434–435
goal system and personal life investment, 636 synthesis; integration of processes, 439
key features of life span approach, 620–623 temporality, 436–437
executive function of personality, 622 transformation, emergence, and novelty in
providing links from infancy to old age, 622–623 structures and processes, 433–434
search for general-purpose mechanisms, variables/individuals, 431–433
621–622 longitudinal design, 448–449
links from infancy to old age, 622–623 measurement models, 443–447
neurophysiological indicators of life mechanics of methodological implications, 439–447
personality, 628–630 generalization, 441–443
protecting/optimizing characteristics, 643 mechanisms, 440
reserve capacities of self and personality nature of phenomena, levels of analysis, 440
development, 637–641 statistics, 447–448
self-concept development, 634 tools in acquisition of information, 440–441
self-regulatory processes, 620, 635–637 toward a developmental science, 439–440
self-system approach, 619–620 multidisciplinary collaboration, 449
trait approach, 618, 632–633 multivariate approach, 448
Personal life investment (PLI ), 636 overview/introduction, 400–402, 450–452
1056 Subject Index

Person-environment relations, holistic-interactionistic example of African Americans and achievement gap


approach (Continued) (interface of stigmatizing context, reactive
perspectives, synchronic/diachronic/evolutionary, coping, responses, and the “acting white” myth),
405 878–883
prevention, treatment, intervention, 449–450 Black response to freedom; social movement for
propositions, 401, 407 education, 880–883
research strategy, 448–450 developmental considerations for research on
unidirectional models, 405–406 Black academic achievement, 881–883
worldviews and self-perceptions, mind and, 452 historical overview of African American
Person as organizing principle for scientific inquiry, achievement motivation, 879–880
409–420 immigrants, special experiences of, 859–864
active agency, 409 importance of broad cultural inclusiveness and
behavior in holistic-interactionistic model, 414 context, 854–864
biological age, a marker of maturation rate, 417–418 interpretations of youths’ stage-specific coping
biological factors in interaction processes, 414–416 outcomes as function of net vulnerability,
antisocial behavior, 415 885–886
well-being, 415–416 intervention design, 853–864
individuality, the developing person, 419–420 meaning/experience of culture, 854–856
mediating mental system, 409–414 overview/introduction, 829–833, 848, 885–886
development of, 413 positive youth development and, 905
emotions, 412 predicting positive outcomes in face of challenge,
language and language acquisition, 412 887–888
motivation, 412–413 racism and human development, 883–885
self-consciousness, self-perceptions, and self- rationale; need for new theory, 830–832
evaluations, 411–412 social science assumptions/practices, judicial
subconscious processes, 410 decisions, (twentieth century), 868–878
values, valuations, and norms, 410–411 cross-disciplinary contributions from legal studies,
nature-nurture issue, 416–418 874–878
personality consistency, 418–419 inferences, assumptions, and what Brown v.
Person standpoint, 37, 38, 49–50 Board of Education should have considered,
Phase-space diagram of development (Waddington), 868–872
261 shortcomings of social science contributions to the
Phenomena-noumena split, 61 Brown decision, 871–872
Phenomenological constructivism and realism, 63 traditional assumptions, beliefs, and values,
Phenomenological extension of Piaget, 474–475 872–874
Phenomenological variant of ecological systems theory study of diverse youth; children of color and
(PVEST), 829–888, 905 privilege, 833–844
conceptual advantages of, 872–874, 886–888 deficit perspectives and stigma, 835–836
critical race theory, 875–878 domains of human development and competence,
cross-cutting human development themes, 853–864 838–839
cultural context of African American males, flaw of context ignored, 833–836
856–857 flaw of general lack of a developmental
demonstration, application (diagram), perspective when considering youth of color,
parent /adolescent academic/employment 838–843
outcomes, 851 flaw of racism ignored, 836–838
as dual axis coping outcome interpretational mediating processes between vulnerability and
framework, 886–888 coping outcomes, 841–843
ecological perspectives; exploring the bioecological need for inclusive approaches to human
model, 864–868 development, 839–841
Subject Index 1057

synthesized recursive framework, 844–853 Positive human development perspective,


bidirectional influences of significant others on features/implications of, 11–12
youths’ development, 844 Positive youth development, 894–933
foundational role of social cognition and linkages age, 928–929
to phenomenology, 844–847 boundaries/expectations, 907
inference-making perceptions, 844–847 commitment to learning, 907
phenomenology and context character, 846–847 core constructs, 897
self processes of diverse youth, 845–846 defining, 895–898
social cognition and development, 845 developmental assets, framework of, 907–908
youth’s everyday experiences, 844–847 empowerment, 907
systems framework in ecological system, 847–853 gender, 928
emergent identities, 848, 850 historical and social context, 898–901
life-stage-specific coping outcomes, 848, 850 intellectual development, 909
net stress, 848 overview/introduction, 894–895, 932–933
net vulnerability, 847, 848 personal /social assets, 909
reactive coping methods, 848, 850 physical development, 909
unique experience of African American male positive developmental settings, features of, 912
adolescents, 857–859 positive identity, 908
Phenomenology (Hegel), 62 positive values, 907
Phylogenesis, 19 protective factors, 906
Physical development, positive youth development and, psychological /emotional development, 909
909 race/ethnicity and SES, 929–930
Physical environment, 814–815 research, 928–930
“Pile-up” hypothesis, positive youth development, research support for key hypotheses, 914–928
921–923 community hypothesis, 926–928
Plasticity: contextual change hypothesis, 916–917
age-associated changes in, 584–586 covariation hypothesis, 920–921
developmental; weak and strong constraints, 529–530 how much explanatory power is reasonable to
malleability in intellectual functioning across expect, 915
historical and ontogenetic time, 611–612 longitudinal hypothesis, 923–925
relative, 3, 904 overview, 915
and temporality in human development, 3 “pile-up” hypothesis, 921–923
Play: universality/diversity hypothesis, 928
cultural differences, 760 youth action hypothesis, 917–920
syntelic character of, 507–509 social competencies, 907–908
Playfulness/discipline (polar dimensions of social development, 909
complexity), 487–488 support, 907
Pluralistic universe, 58 theory, 901–914
versus dualism, 58 conceptual models of positive development,
versus monism, 58 905–910
Points of view, 36 context and community change, 913–914
Polarities, complexity and, 482, 484–489 context and community influence, 910–913
agency and communion, 485 human development, 902–905
divergent and convergent thinking, 486–487 Positivism /neopositivism, 71, 73–74
energy and quietude, 488–489 Post-modern path, and religious/spiritual development,
extroversion and introversion, 488 944–946
iconoclastic and traditional, 489 Poverty:
passive investment and detached objectivity, 486 duration of exposure to, 682–683
playfulness and discipline, 487–488 gender, 690
1058 Subject Index

Pragmatic feedback cycle, 191 Quantitative trait locus (QTL), 224–225


Pragmatics. See Intellectual functioning across life Quietude/energy (polar dimensions of complexity),
span; Personality development across the 488–489
life span
Pragmatism, 68–70 Race development, 108
Prayer, 955, 973–975. See also Religious and spiritual Race/ethnicity, positive youth development and,
development 929–930
Precocity, argument from, 344–345 Race theory, critical (CRT), 830, 875–878
Primacy of social forces, 31 Racism, 836–838, 883–885
Primary/secondary process thinking, 478 Random selection, 42
Principle of parsimony, 179–185 Reach, learning to; dynamic systems approach to,
Probabilistic epigenesis, 219–220, 248 292–297, 298
Probabilistic states, Ford and Lerner’s model of average speed of reaching, 295
developmental change as a series of, 270 changes in straightness/smoothness of reach
Problem-finding mode, 479 trajectories of four infants, 294
Process(es): collective variable dynamics, 293
bioecological model, 795 first transition, 293–297
cultural psychology, 728 model of levels of control, 298
Processing speed, 600 shifts in control during first year, 296–297
Process-person-context model, 866 Reading:
Process-Person-Context Time (PPCT) model, 798 developmental pathways of good and poor readers,
Progressing equilibration, 196 335
Protective factors, positive youth development, 906 task profiles for normative developmental sequence
Proximal dynamic person-environment systems, for reading words, 351
425 Real, the (capital R), 54–55
Proximal processes, 363, 795, 866 Reciprocal continuity, 683
focus of attention in, 813–814 Reciprocity and interdependence, holistic-interactionist
in solo activities with objects and symbols, 814 model, 434–435
Psychoanalysis/ Freud, 114–117, 144–145, 948–949 Reduction description, 71–72
Psychological space landscapes, 268 Reductionism, 7–10, 56–57
Psychological structure: Reflective judgment, development of, 386
dynamic nature of, 317–318 Reflective level of analysis, 21
as dynamic skill, 321–325 Regulation, adaptive developmental, 905. See also Self-
common ruler for skill development, 322–325 regulation
context specificity and culture, 322 Relational approach to inquiry (diagram), 49
generalization through construction, 325 Relational /contextual model of development (Reich), 965
integration and interparticipation, 321–322 Relational developmental tradition, Plato and Aristotle
self-organization, mutual regulation and growth, and, 55–56
322 Relational metatheory, 3, 5–6, 19, 32–39
Psychological synthesis, 181 holism, 32–33
Psychological tension, 188–189 identity of opposites, 33–36
Psychopathology. See Pathology implications for developmental science, 5–6
Public/ Private Ventures (P/ PV), 927 opposites of identity, 36–37
PVEST. See Phenomenological variant of ecological synthesis of wholes, 37–39
systems theory (PVEST) Relational scientific methodology, 74–80
abduction-transcendental argument, 77–80
Qualitative change, 26 causality and action patterns, 76–77
Quantification in context, 172 interpretation and observation, 75–76
Subject Index 1059

Relational standpoints in psychological inquiry stages of Piaget and Goldman compared, 958
(person / biology/culture), 37 stages of religious judgment and reasoning (Oser
Relationship view of life patterns, 673 and Gmünder), 961–965
Religious and spiritual development, 942–991 stage-structural theories, alternatives to, 967–972
attachment, 979–980 cognitive-cultural theories, 970–971
conceptual development in, 972–976 developmental systems theories, 971–972
congregations, 981–982 spiritual child movement, 968–970
in context, 976–982 substantive-functional approach (Kwilecki),
as coping (psychoanalytic theory), 948–951 967–968
Erikson, 949–950 studies:
Freud, 948–949 cognitive-cultural approach, 975–976
Jung, 949 conceptions of religious institutions, prayer, and
object relations theorists, 950–951 death, 973–975
definitions, 952–957 stage-structural approach, 972–975
content, form, and function, 954–956 supernatural agencies, 972–973
religion, religiousness, and spirituality, 953–954 synthesis, 976
stage and structure, 956–957 Reserve capacities of self and personality development,
developmental approach, 943–944 637–641
family context, 977–980 baseline/developmental, 585
historical perspective on, 946–952 evidence in indicators of personality structure, 638
as maturing, 947–948 evidence in self-regulatory processes, 641
negative correlates and pathology, 985–990 Resilience, growth versus, 578–579
cult and occultist practices, 987–988 Resource allocation, life span changes in, 578–580
mental disorders, 989 Resource characteristics of the person as shapers of
pathogenic/toxic beliefs/practices/attitudes, development, 812
989–990 Retroduction, 64
terrorist groups, 988–989 Robot, Lego (microdevelopment of understanding), 366
overview/introduction, 942–943 Robotics, epigenetic, 288
as perfecting, 951–952 Rulers/clocks, 350–352
positive correlates of, 982–985
advanced age and, 985 Sampling, historically structured, 184–185
coping, 984 Scaffolding, 330
health and well-being, 982–983 Scalloping, 366, 367
moral development, 983–984 Schools. See Education
moral judgment, 983–984 Science, basics of, 170–177
service, 984 conventionalism, 71
post-modern path and, 944–946 data collection, 172–173
prayer, personal, 955 hypotheses testing, theory-driven versus pseudo-
religious schools/education, 980–981 empirical, 173
socialization and practice, 977–979 instrumentalism, 71
stage-structural theories, 957–967 intellectual interdependency of social sciences:
cognitive-stage theories (Goldman), 958–959 episodic growth spurts, 170–171
concerns/attacks on, 965–967 international nature of contemporary psychology,
faith developmental theory (Fowler), 959–961 171
Fowler and Oser and Gmünder’s theories methodologies, 71
compared, 964–965 organizing knowledge construction; frames of
relational and contextual model of development reference, 173–175
(Reich), 965 pathways to objectivity, 171–173
1060 Subject Index

Science, basics of (Continued) goals, 540–544


positivism, 71 processes of self-evaluation, 538–540
quantification in context, 172 protective and defensive mechanisms, 538
social interdependency of psychology, 170–171 self-observation and self-monitoring, 536–538
split tradition versus relational tradition, 71 structure and process, 535–536
statistics as a form of inductive logic, 172–173 cultural psychology of, 749–754
Scientific nature of developmental inquiry, 64 sociocultural participation and, 763
Scripts: Self-efficacy concept, 481
cultural, 550–551 Self-evaluations, 411–412
customary scripts for interaction, 739 action theory and, 538–540
emotions and dynamic organization of activity and adaptive value of, 639
development, 377 goal of, 638–641
shame, adult, 372 lifetime comparisons, 640–641
Search Institute, 927 social comparisons, 640
Selection: Self-functioning, 749
development as, 284–286 Self-in-Relationships (SiR) Interview, 331, 354
and endogeniety, and contextual effects, 687–689 diagram constructed by 15-year-old Korean girl, 355
Selective optimization with compensation (SOC) Self-organization, 19
model, 591–595 Self-perceptions, 411–412, 452
age gradients, 594 Self practices, 729
compensation, 593–594 Self processes of diverse youth (PVEST), 845–846
definitions, 591–593 Self-regulation:
dual-task research and behavioral indicators, 595 capacity for; complex person, 481
empirical evidence on SOC theory, 594–595 of context engagement, 904
examples from proverbs and questionnaire items, 592 of experience, 471–472
management and mastery of life tasks, 595 processes for, 619, 634–636
optimization, 593 Self-system approach, personality development,
selection, elective and loss-based, 593 619–620
Selective responsiveness, 810–811 Selfways (ways of being), cultural diversity in,754–764
Self: self as historical product, 763–764
conceptual, 549–550, 751–752 self develops through sociocultural participation,
coordination of the self and the other (Mead), 763
190–193 self is constituted in interaction with others,
cultural, 752 762–763
dialogical models of, 201 in some East Asian cultural contexts, 758–761
future selves, life plans, and cultural scripts, 550–551 in some European American contexts, 755–758
as historical product, 763–764 in some other cultural contexts, 761–764
interaction with others, and, 762–763 Sensitive periods, learning experiences and, 430
ontogeny of conceptual self and of internalized Sensorimotor bases of higher cognition, 306
control, 549–550 Sensory stimulation, influences on on genetic activity,
as unreal, 750 217
Self-actualization (Maslow), 467, 476 Service, religious/spiritual development and, 984
Self-concept development, 634 Shame:
Self-consciousness, 411–412 hierarchy of categories, in Chinese, 375
Self-correcting teaching style, 490 prototypical script for adult, 372
Self-development: Significant others, bidirectional influences of, on
action theory, and intentional, 535–544 youths’ development (phenomenological variant of
attentional focus, 537–538 ecological systems theory), 844
complexity and differentiation, 537 Situational imperatives, 690, 691
Subject Index 1061

Skill(s): Social science assumptions/practices, judicial


A-not-B task, 287, 299–303 decisions, 868–878
challenge and, 483 Social stigma, 832
defining, 298–307 Societal /economic conditions, 428
dynamic, 321, 329–336 Societal regulations, formal /informal, 428
growth curves, 365–367 Sociocultural embodiment, 49
optimal experience of flow and, 477–478 Sociocultural patterns in human experience, 674–675
scale, 323, 324–325 Sociocultural variation and frames of meaning, 356
Skin color, 867 Socioeconomic status (SES):
Sleeping arrangements, family, 725–727 positive youth development and, 929–930
Social address model, 866 reading ability and, 681
Social applications: Sociogenesis, 110–111
contemporary advances, 155–156 Soft assemblies (dynamic systems), 274–275
historical theme, 123–124 Spirituality. See Religious and spiritual development
Social cognition, phenomenological variant of Split mechanical explanation, 70–75
ecological systems theory, 844–847 Split metatheory, 19, 30–32
Social competencies (positive youth development), Split neo-Darwinian metatheory:
907–908 developmental applications, 43–46
Social constructivism, 65, 522 diagram, 42
Social determinism, 43 flawed story of change, 46–47
Social development: Split versus relational tradition, 71
divergent interpersonal worlds of childhood, Splitting:
733–734 emotional, 379–382
historical perspectives, 135–136 foundationalism and, 31, 56, 57
positive youth development and, 909 Stage theory:
Social dissonance, 839 cognitive development, 341–342, 343
Social forces, primacy of, 31 religious/spiritual development, 956–957 (see also
Social integration, neighborhoods, 818 Religious and spiritual development, stage-
Social interaction (dynamic systems), 305 structural theories)
Social interdependency of psychology, 170–171 Standpoints, 36, 38
Social learning, 148–151 Statistical models, methodological note, 802
Social neobehaviorism, 143–144 Statistics as form of inductive logic, 172–173
Social organization: Strategies of coordination, 685
of childhood experience, 734–738 Strategy choice as mechanism of development, 45
age-graded activities, 738 Stress. See Phenomenological variant of ecological
care-giving relationships, 736–737 systems theory (PVEST)
organizational settings, 734–736 Structural theories of action, 521–522
parental practices, 737–738 Structural transposition, 186
domestic groups: Structure:
boundedness of, 736 defined, 315, 956–957
composition of, 735 form and, 315, 340–341
function of domestic groups, 734–735 religious/spiritual (See Religious and spiritual
size of, 735 development, stage-structural theories)
social density of, 735 Structure breaking and problem finding, 482–483
roles, cycles, and age ( life course development), Structuring role of external information, dynamic
672–673 systems, 305
Social pathways, and individual life course, 680–684 Subconscious processes, 410
Social relations, cultural mentalities concerning Substantive-functional approach (Kwilecki), 967–968
childhood, 738–739 Supernatural agencies, 972–973
1062 Subject Index

Synaptogenesis as mechanism of development, 45 Trait personality, 618, 631–633


Synchronic perspective, 405 Trajectory(ies), 195–196, 289–290, 589, 684–687
Synchronization, 429–430, 685 Transformational versus variational change, 23,
Synchrony, developmental, 331–333 25–28, 42
Synergetics, 271 Transition(s):
Synthesis: dynamic systems, 275–276, 290
holistic-interactionist model, 439 identifying points of, 290
nativism /empiricism, 338–340 life course development, 684–687, 689–701
neo-Darwinian /modern, 41–42 mechanisms, microdevelopment, and new knowledge,
of wholes, 19, 37–39 363–370
System(s): shift of focus, 365
defined, 19 Transitive/intransitive relations, 182–183
dynamic (See Dynamic systems theory) Turning points, 684–687
versus mechanico-reductive and vitalistic- Typists, speed and knowledge in aging, 610–611
constructive viewpoints, 214–217
theory as metaphor, 268–271 Unemployment, 682, 684
Unified concept of development, 28–30
Teacher-child interaction and growth of complexity, Unit(s) of analysis:
501–506. See also Education centrality of transfer and, 186
Teleology, subjective/objective, 26 holistic look at, 186
Temperament, 376–378 problem (cultural psychology), 724–730
Temporality. See Time synthetic, 186
Tension, 188–189 United States, new psychology in, 104–107
Terrorist groups, 988–989 Universality/diversity hypothesis, positive youth
Theoretical level of discourse, 21 development, 928
Theory of mind, 731–732 Universality/invariance in development; action-
Theory of neuronal group selection (TNGS), 285 theoretical account of, 530–535
Thinking, divergent /convergent (polar dimensions of conceptual implications, 533–535
complexity), 486–487 constructive and conventional implications, 533
Thoughts and actions. See also Dynamic development developmental universals as empirical regularities,
of action and thought: 530–531
cognitive development and cultural psychology, developmental universals as structural implications,
764–768 531–532
contemporary advances, 154 formal implications, 532–533
historical theme, 121–122 Unwed motherhood, life course of, 686
Time:
clocks/rulers, 350–352 Values:
in bioecological model: micro-, meso-, and cultural psychology, 769–770
macrochronological systems, 820–822 positive youth development, 907
nested timescales, dynamic systems, 277–289 valuations, and norms (person-environment),
periods, proximal processes, 795 410–411
temporality: Variability:
age and, 669 dynamic skills and, 329–336
holistic-interactionist model, 436–437 explaining versus “explaining away,” 342–346
and plasticity in human development, 3 inter-systemic, 184
Timing, principle of; life course development,694–695 intra-systemic, 184
Topological psychology (Lewin), 194 as phenomenon not “error,” 183–185
Traditional /iconoclastic (polar dimensions of Variable approach to inquiry (diagram), 50
complexity), 489 Variable/person-centered standpoints, 49–50
Subject Index 1063

Variational versus transformational change, 23, Williams syndrome, 214


25–28, 42 Wisdom:
Vietnam War resisters, 693 complexity and, 491–493
Vitalistic-constructive viewpoints; systems versus as expertise in fundamental pragmatics of life,
mechanico-reductive and, 214–217 606–608
Vulnerability, mediating processes between coping Working memory, 601
outcomes and, 841–843. See also Phenomenological Working model, 500
variant of ecological systems theory (PVEST) Worldviews, 19, 21, 452

Webs, constructive, 319–320, 325–329 Youth action hypothesis, 917–920


Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised Youth development. See Positive youth development
(WISC-R), 213, 214
Well-being, biology and, 415–416 Zone of proximal development (ZPD), 192–193, 498
“Whole” issue of development, 186

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen