Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26

DOI 10.1007/s12671-010-0038-x

ORIGINAL PAPER

Coping with Cigarette Cravings: Comparison of Suppression


Versus Mindfulness-Based Strategies
Jenny Rogojanski & Lisa C. Vettese & Martin M. Antony

Published online: 2 December 2010


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Abstract Mindfulness- and acceptance-based therapies Keywords Smoking . Mindfulness . Craving . Substance
have been explored within the context of addiction abuse . Acceptance
treatment, with some preliminary success. The current
empirical study investigated the effectiveness of a brief
suppression versus mindfulness-based strategy for coping Introduction
with cigarette cravings. Participants (N =61; M age=
40.34 years, SD=12.42) were randomly assigned to using It has been estimated that approximately 1.1 billion people
one of the two coping strategies to help them manage smoke worldwide, a statistic that is expected to rise to over
cravings during an experimental cue exposure to cigarettes. 1.6 billion by the year 2025 (Ghadirian 2004). Smoking
Participants completed self-report measures of self-efficacy, contributes to over 45,000 mortalities annually in Canada
craving, negative affect, depression, and nicotine depen- (Ellison et al. 1995), and approximately 438,000 deaths per
dence before and after the cue exposure and at a 7-day year in the USA, making it the third leading cause of death
follow-up assessment session. Participants in both condi- in North America, and costing over 75 billion dollars in
tions reported significantly reduced amount of smoking and direct medical expenses each year (Centers for Disease
increased self-efficacy in coping with smoking urges at the Control and Prevention 2005). Consequently, smoking-
7-day follow-up. However, only participants in the mind- related death is considered to be the leading preventable
fulness condition demonstrated reductions in negative cause of mortality in both the USA (Substance Abuse and
affect, depressive symptoms, and marginal reductions in Mental Health Services Administration 2004) and Canada
their level of nicotine dependence. These findings suggest (Ghadirian 2004) and is associated with a decrease in
that, although both conditions were associated with lifespan among smokers of approximately 8 years (Peto et
improvements on smoking relevant outcomes, only mind- al. 1996).
fulness had beneficial effects on reported nicotine depen- Despite the severity of health risks associated with
dence and emotional functioning over the course of the smoking, relatively few smokers attempt to quit. Although
study. These findings provide preliminary support for the approximately 80% of smokers report wanting to quit, only
use of mindfulness-based strategies for coping with 43% report having made a recent attempt (Weiss-Gerlach et
smoking urges, as these strategies appear to provide some al. 2008). Of those who do make a quit attempt, between
additional benefits not obtained when coping with smoking 85% and 95% resume smoking within 12 months of
cravings through suppression. cessation without formal intervention (Garvey et al. 1992).
Similarly, of those who receive a formal smoking cessation
intervention, more than 50% resume smoking within
30 days, and 70% (Fiore et al. 2000) to 90% (Niaura et
J. Rogojanski : L. C. Vettese : M. M. Antony (*) al. 1999) resume smoking within their first year of quitting.
Department of Psychology, Ryerson University,
Numerous factors have been proposed to account for the
350 Victoria Street,
Toronto, ON M5B 2K3, Canada difficulties that smokers experience when attempting to quit
e-mail: mantony@psych.ryerson.ca smoking. Among these are the uncomfortable experiences
Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26 15

of nicotine dependence and the associated symptoms of more likely to smoke than individuals without MDD
nicotine withdrawal and craving (DiFranza and Wellman (Glassman et al. 1990). Similarly, those with mood
2005). Nicotine craving is possibly one of the most difficult disorders are far more likely to fail at their smoking
symptoms for smokers to tolerate, particularly because of cessation efforts than those without mood disorders
its early onset, perseverance, and severity (Sommese and (Covey et al. 1990; Glassman 1993; Glassman et al.
Patterson 1995). Research suggests that craving is central to 1990), and several studies indicate that, in a subset of
the maintenance of nicotine addiction and is possibly the smokers, smoking cessation can precipitate clinical de-
factor most responsible for the high rate of relapse to pression (e.g., Covey et al. 1997). Furthermore, increase in
smoking following a cessation attempt (e.g., Bagot et al. depressive symptoms is associated with smoking relapse
2007; Baker et al. 1986; Shiffman 1991; United States following initial cessation (Covey et al. 1997), particularly
Department of Health and Human Services 1988; West and among smokers with past MDD (Hall et al. 1993;
Grunberg 1991). Whereas smokers who experience low Thorsteinsson et al. 2001). Conversely, abstinence from
levels of craving are at a 60% risk for smoking relapse, this smoking beyond the point at which withdrawal symptoms
risk rises to 90% for those individuals who experience have abated is associated with lower levels of depressive
severe craving (Stapleton 1998). symptoms (United States Department of Health and
Furthermore, the relationship between self-efficacy or Human Services 1990).
confidence in one's ability to cope with cravings, and Taken together, these variables contribute to the diffi-
smoking relapse is also well-established (Baer et al. 1986). culty that most smokers experience when attempting to
For example, one study found a positive correlation quit, making smoking cessation interventions a significant
between self-efficacy in coping with cravings while challenge. More recently, researchers have begun to suggest
participating in a smoking cessation program and successful that mindfulness- and acceptance-based interventions may
cessation attempts 6 months later (Stuart et al. 1994). enhance smoking cessation outcomes by targeting some of
Similarly, another study showed self-efficacy to be a the variables that pose a challenge to nicotine cessation (e.g.,
significant predictor of latency to first cigarette following Gonzalez et al. 2009; Vidrine et al. 2009). Mindfulness, a
introduction to a strategy for coping with cravings and therapeutic approach that was initially described in its
number of cigarettes smoked during a 7-day follow-up application for patients with chronic pain (Kabat-Zinn
period (Bowen and Marlatt 2009). 2005), is commonly defined as “paying attention in a
Negative affect, defined as the acute experience of particular way: on purpose, in the present moment, and
negative emotional states such as anger, sadness (depres- nonjudgmentally” (Kabat-Zinn 1994, p. 4). Mindfulness
sion), fear, and anxiety, has also been implicated in nicotine skills can be developed through meditative practices that
addiction (Carmody et al. 2007). Empirical findings suggest encourage individuals to focus their attention on a target of
that stress and negative affect play a large role in the onset interest, while maintaining nonjudgmental awareness of
of tobacco use among adolescents, as well as the transition the present moment (Baer 2003). When phenomena enter
from experimentation to nicotine dependence (Brown et al. into an individual's awareness during mindfulness prac-
1996; Kassel et al. 2003; Orlando et al. 2001). According to tice, it is important for the phenomena to be attended to,
the “negative affect model” of nicotine dependence, the without passing judgment on the nature of those phenom-
initiation and maintenance of smoking addiction is due, in ena. Recently, mindfulness-based strategies have been
part, to the use of cigarettes as a means of avoiding or explored within the context of addiction treatment (e.g.,
reducing these aversive mental states (Carmody et al. Davis et al. 2007; Marlatt et al. 2004; Pollack et al. 2002;
2007). This high negative affective state can be understood Toneatto et al. 2007; Vernig and Orsillo 2009; Vettese
as exceeding the negative affect that is characteristic of 2008; Vieten 2005).
typical withdrawal symptoms and therefore, is a factor Studies suggest that smokers tend to rely on avoidance
that incrementally predicts smoking relapse above and strategies to cope with stress, a coping style that has been
beyond withdrawal alone (Myrsten et al. 1977). Research shown to be predictive of future relapse (Kassel et al. 2003;
indicates that smokers who experience high levels of Wegner and Zanakos 1994). Conversely, mindfulness
negative affect have less success in quitting smoking practices are conceptually analogous to interoceptive
(Anda et al. 1999). They also have worse withdrawal exposure practices (Carmody et al. 2007), such that they
symptoms (Breslau et al. 1992) and a higher frequency of facilitate emotional approach coping and acceptance of
relapse (Brandon et al. 1990). present moment experience, as opposed to avoidance or
Moreover, severity of nicotine dependence has been suppression of that experience. This is accomplished by
found to positively correlate with severity of mood encouraging individuals to engage in moment-by-moment
symptoms (Lerman et al. 1996). Studies suggest that awareness of negative affective cues without engaging in
individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) are maladaptive responding (Baer 2002), which, in turn, builds
16 Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26

a tolerance to such cues and creates a new, more adaptive significant decrease in thought suppression as compared
conditioned response to negative affect (Breslin et al. with individuals in a treatment-as-usual condition, which
2002). Specifically, mindfulness requires one to be aware partially mediated the effectiveness of the course. Individ-
of his or her thoughts and feelings without reacting to them uals who frequently use suppression strategies have also
or attempting to control or change them, which can act as a been found to have lower levels of self-esteem, are less
form of covert exposure and translate into increased self- successful at mood repair, and have more depressive
efficacy (Hayes et al. 1999). Research suggests that symptoms than nonsuppressors (Gross and John 2003).
mindfulness can enhance general feelings of self-efficacy Interestingly, over one quarter of individuals use thought
and control (Bishop 2002), including in the context of suppression strategies at least some of the time when they
addiction. Mindfulness may be particularly useful in are dealing with cravings to smoke (Bowen and Marlatt
decreasing the avoidance of thoughts and increasing 2007). Thus, given the prevalent use of suppression
confidence to resist smoking urges, thereby reducing the strategies by the general population and its distinction from
likelihood of relapse to smoking (Carmody et al. 2007). It mindfulness-based approaches, such strategies serve as an
has been suggested that the experience of repeatedly informative comparison to mindfulness for coping with
observing rather than reacting to one's urges may facilitate cravings.
a greater sense of control over the actual decision to use a This study expands upon research conducted by Bowen
substance (Breslin et al. 2002). This reduced reactivity, in and Marlatt (2009) on the effects of a mindfulness strategy
turn, may reduce the likelihood of relapse to drug use for coping with smoking urges, which examined the
among addicted populations. effectiveness of a brief mindfulness-based instruction set
In addition to facilitating an increased sense of self- on urges for cigarettes and smoking behavior following a
efficacy, the rationale for integrating mindfulness-based cigarette cue exposure. Results from this study suggested
strategies into smoking cessation treatment comes from that participants receiving brief mindfulness-based instruc-
correlational and experimental data showing higher levels tions, in contrast to a comparison group of individuals
of mindfulness to be related to lower levels of negative asked to deal with their cue-evoked cravings as they usually
affect and depression symptomatology. For example, would, smoked fewer cigarettes over a 7-day follow-up
research has shown that adherence to a meditation practice period. Whereas Bowen and Marlatt’s (2009) study com-
is positively associated with smoking cessation outcomes pared a controlled mindfulness condition to the coping
and reductions in emotional distress (Davis et al. 2007), as strategies that participants used independently without an
well as reduced symptoms of depression (e.g., Ree and experimental manipulation; the current research included a
Craigie 2007). In addition, mindfulness training has been controlled comparison group, specifically an experimental-
shown to correlate with improvements in positive and ly manipulated suppression condition. In addition, the
negative affect and maintenance of these gains at 4 and 8- current research altered several components of the previous
month follow-up (Davidson et al. 2003). study, which debriefed participants regarding their treat-
The aim of the current study was to examine, among ment conditions prior to a 7-day follow-up phone assess-
nicotine dependent individuals, the influence of a ment. Within the current study, the researcher met with
mindfulness-based strategy versus a suppression strategy participants in person to complete a 7-day follow-up
on the experience of craving, self-efficacy in coping with assessment of smoking behaviors and debriefed participants
smoking urges, emotion-based triggers to smoking behavior only after the follow-up assessment to reduce response bias
(depression, negative affect), and actual amounts of associated with knowing one's respective condition mem-
smoking behavior. Suppression was selected as the com- bership. Furthermore, whereas the previous researchers
parison condition to mindfulness because, as a coping conducted the study interventions in a group context,
strategy, suppression is phenomenologically opposite to including the delivery and practice of the coping strategies,
mindfulness. It is an emotion-focused mental control the current study implemented greater experimental control
strategy, whereby an individual deliberately tries to prevent by conducting all assessments individually. Finally, a
a particular thought from coming to mind (Watkins and community sample rather than a student sample was used
Moulds 2007). Suppression techniques have also been in the present research to increase the external validity of
found to be iatrogenic in studies of substance use, as they the findings.
can be counterproductive and result in a rebound effect, In the current study, we hypothesized that, from
whereby cravings are actually intensified (e.g., Haaga and pretreatment to 7-day follow-up, individuals in a mindful-
Allison 1994; Salkovskis and Reynolds 1994; Toll et al. ness condition would report improvements on the main
2001). Furthermore, Bowen and colleagues (2007) have outcomes of negative affect, depressed mood, craving, and
shown that alcohol-dependent individuals participating in a self-efficacy in coping with smoking cravings, and that
course of Vipassana mindfulness meditation reported a these improvements would be greater than those experi-
Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26 17

enced by individuals in a suppression condition. In daily experience. Participants respond to a four-point


addition, it was expected that at a 7-day follow-up Likert-type scale to questions such as, “it is easy for me
assessment, participants in the mindfulness condition would to concentrate on what I am doing.” In the present study,
consume fewer cigarettes and demonstrate lower nicotine the CAMS-R was found to have relatively low to adequate
dependence. internal consistency (Cronbach's α=.72 for attention sub-
scale, 0.57 for present focus subscale 0.76 for acceptance/
nonjudgment subscale, and 0.67 for awareness subscale).
Method Depression was assessed using the depression subscale
of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales—21-item version
Participants (DASS-21; Lovibond and Lovibond 1995), a measure of
depression, anxiety, and stress/tension over the past week.
Participants included in the present study were 61 adults Items are rated on a four-point severity/frequency scale and
(41% female, 72% white) over the age of 18 years (M= include statements such as, “I couldn’t seem to experience
40.34, SD=12.42) who self-identified as cigarette smokers. any positive feelings at all.” The DASS-21 yielded
Participants were recruited from a large metropolitan Cronbach's alphas of 0.92 for the depression subscale in
Canadian city and a large Canadian university through the current study.
flyer postings, Internet classifieds, and newspaper adver- Nicotine dependence was measured using the Fagerstrom
tisements, and received monetary compensation for attend- Test of Nicotine Dependence (FTND; Heatherton et al. 1991).
ing the two study sessions. In order to be included in this The FTND is a brief, six-item scale asking individuals to
study, participants were required to indicate that they: (a) respond to multiple choice type questions, with each answer
have smoked an average of ten or more cigarettes per day corresponding to a score and higher scores indicating higher
over the past month; (b) do not have significant difficulty levels of dependence. An example of items includes, “how
reading, speaking, or writing in English; (c) do not have soon after you wake up do you smoke your first cigarette?”
significant difficulty using a computer to respond to The FTND has been shown to produce reliable and valid
questionnaires; (d) were over the age of 18; and (e) had scores under a variety of conditions (Shadel and Shiffman
thought about cutting back on their smoking, or tried to quit 2005) that predict smoking cessation outcomes (Fagerstrom
smoking in the past. Participants' expired breath carbon and Schneider 1989). However, data from the present study
monoxide (CO) level was measured at baseline, which was demonstrated low reliability (Cronbach's α=−.44), which
used to biologically confirm their smoking status. The may be explained by the relatively low nicotine depen-
mean level of expired breath CO for the sample was 16.75 dence of study participants, as well as the small number
(SD=8.53) parts per million (ppm). Moreover, 81.97% of of items included in this scale. Although the internal
the sample yielded a CO level of 10 ppm or above, a cutoff consistency was quite low for this scale in the current
level that is frequently used in smoking studies to identify study, it is one of the most commonly used measures of
heavy smokers (e.g., Fonder et al. 2005). N=49, or 80.33% nicotine dependence.
of the sample was retained at follow-up. Negative affect was assessed using the negative affect
subscale of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule
Measures (PANAS; Watson et al. 1988). The PANAS is a 20-item
self-report measure consisting of words describing a variety
The current study included six separate assessment points: of feelings and emotions, ten positive and ten negative, that
one set of baseline measures prior to the cue induction participants are required to rate on a five-point scale. Within
procedure, three brief assessments of craving during the cue the current study, the PANAS was highly internally
induction procedure, one set of measures immediately consistent, yielding a Cronbach's alpha of 0.84 for the
following the cue induction procedure, and a final set of negative affect scale.
follow-up measures administered during the second study Participants' level of self-efficacy was measured using
session. Prior to the cue exposure procedure, participants the Relapse Situation Efficacy Questionnaire (RSEQ;
were asked to provide their demographic and smoking Gwaltney et al. 2001), a 43-item self-report measure
behavior information, including expired breath CO level. assessing participants' confidence in their ability to resist
All questionnaires were completed on a computer. the temptation to smoke in a wide variety of contexts. Items
Mindfulness was assessed using the Cognitive Affective make up seven separate factors and ask participants to rate
Mindfulness Scale—Revised (CAMS-R; Feldman et al. their confidence level to refrain from smoking when in a
2007). The CAMS-R is a 12-item self-report measure of variety of triggering moods and situations on a four-point
attention, awareness, present-focus, and acceptance/non- scale. Within the current study, Cronbach's alpha values
judgment with respect to thoughts and feelings in general were as follows: 0.76 for the low arousal factor, 0.93 for the
18 Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26

negative affect factor, 0.88 for the positive affect factor, was selected to be consistent with Bowen and Marlatt’s
0.87 for the restrictive situation factor, 0.79 for the idle time (2009) study. To eliminate differences between participants
factor, 0.80 for the social situation/food factor, and 0.43 for based on length of smoking abstinence, participants were
the craving factor. instructed to smoke one cigarette 30 min prior to arriving
To ensure that participants in both study conditions for their first study session (Erblich and Bovbjerg 2004).
viewed their respective coping strategies as being equally On average, participants reported that they had smoked
credible, the Credibility/Expectancy Questionnaire (CEQ; their last cigarette 47.98 min prior to the study session. A
Devilly and Borkovec 2000) was administered at the end of longer smoking deprivation period was not used in the
the first study session. The CEQ is a brief measure of current study, as a number of studies have suggested that
treatment expectancy and rationale credibility frequently smoking deprivation alone may raise nicotine craving to
used in clinical outcome studies and consists of two levels at which cue reactivity effects are no longer
separate factors: expectancy and credibility. The scale noticeable due to ceiling effects (e.g., Fonder et al. 2005;
includes items such as, “at this point, how logical does Sayette et al. 2001; Tidey et al. 2005).
the therapy offered to you seem?” which are rated on either During the first study session, participants engaged in a
a nine-point or a 0 to 100% scale (set at 10% intervals). 20-min smoking cue exposure while simultaneously receiv-
Within the current study, the CEQ yielded a Cronbach's ing either mindfulness instructions (to accept the thoughts
alpha of 0.89 for the credibility factor and standardized and feelings that arise in a mindful way) or suppression
alphas of 0.88 for the expectancy factor, and 0.91 for the instructions (to distance themselves from the experience by
entire scale. actively avoiding the thoughts and feelings that arise). The
Participants' amount of smoking over the course of cue exposure required the experimenter to place a tray on
7 days prior to their first study session, as well as during the the table in front of the participant, with an upturned
7-day follow-up period, was assessed using the Timeline opaque bowl used to cover the participant's preferred brand
Follow-back (TLFB; Brown et al. 1998; Sobell et al. 1980), of cigarettes, a lighter with no lighter fluid, and an ashtray.
an interview commonly used to determine the number of The instructions for the cue exposure were audio-recorded
cigarettes an individual has consumed over a predetermined and played for each participant through stereo headphones
period of time. The interviewer first uses a calendar to to ensure consistency. The cue exposure instructions were
identify events of personal interest (e.g., holidays, birth- based on those used by Bowen and Marlatt (2009), such
days, illnesses, etc.) as anchors that help participants with that participants were instructed to lift the bowl and use the
recall. Participants are then asked to report the number of materials underneath to simulate cigarette smoking. How-
cigarettes they consumed daily over the past 7 days, starting ever, participants were not permitted to light the cigarette
with the present day. Research indicates that the TLFB has and were asked to instead imagine that the cigarette had
good test-retest reliability and is strongly correlated with been lit. Although many studies involving in vivo cue
individuals’ daily smoking diaries (Brown et al. 1998). exposure to cigarettes require participants to actually light
Throughout the cue induction procedure, participants the cigarette (e.g., Havermans et al. 2003; Miranda et al.
were asked to complete several Visual Analogue Scales 2008), the methodology for the present study was altered to
(VAS) as a measure of their current-moment craving for a avoid cue reactivity complications that may arise due to the
cigarette. The VAS was comprised of a 100 mm horizontal release of nicotine from cigarette smoke. Specifically, we
line anchored by word descriptors at each end of the line. thought that the release of nicotine from a lit cigarette might
Participants were asked the question “How strong is your result in less craving than an unlit cigarette that does not
urge to smoke right now?” and were asked to place a allow for the release of nicotine.
vertical mark across the line to indicate how much craving During the cue exposure, half of participants (n=31)
they are experiencing at the current moment. The response received instructions to accept their present-moment expe-
options ranged from “No urge at all” to “Very severe urge.” rience in a nonjudgmental way. They were instructed to
A score is determined using a ruler to measure where the notice their thoughts and urges, without reacting to them or
participant drew a mark along the 100 mm horizontal line. attempting to make them go away. Additionally, they were
asked to imagine that their craving is a wave that reaches a
Procedure peak and then naturally subsides if one is able to wait it out
without succumbing to the temptation to smoke. These
Participants were screened over the telephone to ensure that instructions were based on the urge surfing technique
study inclusion criteria were met, and those eligible were described by Marlatt and Gordon (1985) and Davis and
randomly assigned to either a mindfulness (n=31) or colleagues (2007). Participants in the suppression condition
suppression (n=30) condition. The study consisted of two (n=30) received instructions that encouraged them to
sessions spaced 7 days apart. The 7-day follow-up period forcefully subdue their present-moment experience by
Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26 19

attempting to stop and ignore their thoughts and urges. The any of the primary outcome variables (see Table 1 for a
instructions given to both groups were delivered by the summary of group means and standard deviations) or on
same voice on the audio-recordings and balanced for length any key demographic variables. Furthermore, there were no
of the instructions and the number of times that words significant differences between participants who did not
pertaining to smoking were used (e.g., smoking, urges, return for the second session and those who completed both
cravings, cigarette). Participants were assessed for amount study sessions. On average, participants smoked 16.42 (SD=
of smoking, level of nicotine dependence, negative affect, 7.41) cigarettes per day over the course of 7 days prior to
depression, and craving, as well as self-efficacy to abstain their participation in the study. Additionally, the mean score
from smoking, prior to and following the cue exposure on the FTND was 4.57 (SD=1.35) out of ten, which
procedure. At the 7-day follow-up session, participants constitutes a low level of nicotine dependence, and there
were assessed for changes on the aforementioned variables, were no significant differences between the study conditions
as well as their smoking behaviors over the past 7 days. on mean FTND scores, t(59)=−0.80, p>.05 (M=4.43, SD=
Notably, participants were not instructed to continue to 1.45 for suppression condition; M=4.71, SD=1.24 for
practice their respective strategy over the course of the mindfulness condition). According to cutoff scores sug-
follow-up period. gested by Fagerstrom et al. (1991), at baseline 6.56% of
participants were classified as having very low nicotine
dependence, 40.98% as low dependence, 26.23% as
Results moderate dependence, and 26.23% as high nicotine
dependence. No participants scored in the very high
Comparisons between the suppression and mindfulness dependence range (a score of 8–10). There were no
conditions revealed no significant differences at baseline on significant group differences on prestudy amounts of

Table 1 Baseline measures separated by study condition

Measure Suppression condition Mindfulness condition t values df

M SD M SD

TLFB 17.59 7.89 15.29 6.84 1.22 59


FTND 4.43 1.45 4.71 1.24 −0.80 59
CAMS-R Total 33.97 6.51 33.76 7.03 0.12 59
Attention 8.60 2.54 8.39 1.99 0.37 59
Present focus 8.97 1.85 8.84 2.44 0.23 55.84
Awareness 8.00 2.30 8.39 2.17 −0.68 59
Acceptance 8.40 2.33 8.15 2.45 0.42 59
DASS-21 depression 9.03 10.87 11.00 10.63 −0.70 57
PANAS negative 19.29 6.17 20.04 9.15 −0.37 59
RSEQ total 1.88 0.44 1.93 0.57 −0.37 58
Negative affect 1.45 0.52 1.52 0.73 −0.43 59
Positive affect 2.27 0.76 2.20 0.71 0.37 59
Restrictive situation 2.39 0.65 2.45 0.82 −0.34 59
Idle time 1.71 0.45 1.77 0.75 −0.38 59
Social/food 1.61 0.45 1.66 0.69 −0.35 59
Low arousal 1.97 0.57 2.00 0.61 −0.19 59
Crave 1.78 0.60 1.78 0.72 0.00 58

TLFB timeline follow-back, FTND Fagerstrom test of nicotine dependence, CAMS-R Total total score of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness
Scale—revised, Attention Attention subscale of Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale, Present focus Present focus subscale of Cognitive and
Affective Mindfulness Scale, Awareness Awareness subscale of Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale, Acceptance Acceptance subscale of
Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale, DASS-21 Depression Depression subscale of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale, PANAS Negative
Negative affect subscale of the Positive and Negative Affect Scale, RSEQ Total Total score of the Relapse Situation Efficacy Questionnaire,
Negative Affect Negative affect subscale of the Relapse Situation Efficacy Questionnaire, Positive Affect Positive affect subscale of the Relapse
Situation Efficacy Questionnaire, Restrictive Situation Restrictive situation subscale of the Relapse Situation Efficacy Questionnaire, Idle Time
Idle time subscale of the Relapse Situation Efficacy Questionnaire, Social/food Social/food situations subscale of the Relapse Situation Efficacy
Questionnaire, Low Arousal Low arousal subscale of the Relapse Situation Efficacy Questionnaire, Crave Craving subscale of the Relapse
Situation Efficacy Questionnaire
20 Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26

smoking, t(45)=−0.71, p=.48 or poststudy ratings of main effect of time, F(2, 90)=6.81, p=.002, partial η2 =.13,
strategy usefulness, t(36.52)=−0.86, p=.39. indicating that participants increased in self-efficacy in
coping with smoking urges across the study. However,
Treatment Credibility there were no significant group differences in change in
abstinence self-efficacy, F(2, 90)=0.72, p=.49, partial
There were also no group differences on perceived η2 =.02. Additional analyses conducted on each of the
treatment credibility and expectancy, t(59) = −1.57, seven subscales of the RSEQ further revealed no significant
p=.12. Thus, both groups viewed their study condition as group effects.
providing them with an equally credible intervention.
Furthermore, there were no significant differences between Craving Participants' ratings of their cravings immediately
conditions on participants' percentage of time using the following the cue induction procedure indicated that this
craving strategy over the course of the follow-up period procedure was ineffective in increasing participants' sub-
(58.06% for mindfulness condition, 56.67% for suppression jective craving for a cigarette. This was the case in both the
condition), χ2(1)=0.01, p=.93. Participants reported using suppression condition, t(29)=0.67, p=.51 and the mindful-
their respective strategy an average of 3.68 days during the ness condition, t(30)=0.65, p=.52. Additionally, changes in
follow-up period (M=3.88, SD =1.78 for mindfulness cravings were assessed using the VAS from immediately
condition; M=3.48, SD=2.06 for suppression condition) following the cue-induction procedure, which was method-
and gave the strategy a mean rating of 4.59 on a ten-point ologically designed to induce craving, and immediately
scale in terms of its likelihood of improving their ability to following delivery of the mindfulness or suppression
cope with nicotine cravings (M = 4.83, SD = 1.44 for coping strategy. Contrary to hypotheses, there were no
mindfulness condition; M=4.35, SD=2.31 for suppression significant main effects of time, F(1, 59) = 0.01,
condition). p=.94, partial η2 =.00 or time by condition, F(1, 59)=
0.09, p=.76, partial η2 =.01. Therefore, contrary to hypoth-
Main Hypotheses eses, these findings indicate that neither the mindfulness
nor the suppression craving reduction interventions were
The primary hypotheses for this study pertained to group successful in reducing cravings immediately following the
effects for the mindfulness versus suppression condition on cue induction procedure.
post-intervention smoking amounts, self-efficacy, craving,
nicotine dependence, affect, and depression. Hypotheses Negative Affect Changes in negative affect were assessed
were tested using a mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA), using the negative affect subscale of the PANAS from
with measures of the aforementioned outcome variables at baseline to follow-up. Analyses revealed no significant
either two (baseline and follow-up) or three (baseline, main effect of time, F(1, 45)=0.12, p=.73, partial η2 =.003.
immediately following intervention, and follow-up) time However, the mixed ANOVA revealed a significant time by
points serving as the within-subjects factor and condition condition interaction, F(1, 45) = 8.11, p = .007, partial
(mindfulness or suppression) serving as the between- η2 =.15, such that the negative affect score of participants
subjects factor. Statistical significance was set at p<.05. in the suppression condition increased by an average of
2.09 points, while the score of participants in the
Amount of Smoking Change in amount of smoking was mindfulness condition decreased by an average of 2.68
assessed using participants' scores on the TLFB at baseline points. In order to assess whether participants' negative
and follow-up. There was a significant main effect of time, affect scores changed significantly from baseline to follow-
F(1, 47)=52.50, p<.001, partial η2 =.53, such that all up, paired-samples t-tests were conducted for each condi-
participants reported smoking an average of 3.41 fewer tion. Within the mindfulness condition, participants' nega-
cigarettes at the follow-up period, when compared with their tive affect score at baseline (M=20.09, SD=9.82) decreased
baseline amount of smoking. However, contrary to hypothe- significantly by the follow-up assessment (M=17.42, SD=
ses, there was no significant interaction between time and 8.86), t(23)=2.86, p=.009, r=.51. However, this was not
condition, F(1, 47)=1.98, p=.17, partial η2 =.04, indicating evident within the suppression condition, (M=19.29, SD=
no significant differences between study conditions in 6.84 at baseline; M = 21.38, SD = 8.83 at follow-up),
amount of smoking at follow-up. t(22)=−1.49, p=.15, r=.30.

Self-Efficacy Change in self-efficacy to abstain from smok- Depression Group differences in change on depression were
ing in a variety of contexts was assessed using participants' assessed using the depression subscale of the DASS-21 at the
scores on the RSEQ as assessed at three time points over 7-day follow-up. Analyses revealed no significant main effect
the study. As hypothesized, analyses revealed a significant of time, F(1, 44)=1.05, p=.31, partial η2 =.02, but did show
Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26 21

a significant time by condition interaction, F(1, 44)=8.09, effectiveness of suppression strategies would suggest
p=.007, partial η2 =.16. Paired-samples t-tests indicated otherwise (e.g., Haaga and Allison 1994; Toll et al. 2001).
that, while the depression scores for the suppression As such, there are a number of plausible explanations for
condition did not change significantly from baseline (M= why group differences on the aforementioned variables may
10.61, SD=11.59) to follow-up (M=12.70, SD=10.63, not have been evident within the current study. First, given
t(22)=−1.46, p=0.16, r=0.30), they reduced significantly the moderate sample size of this pilot study, it is possible
in the mindfulness condition (M=12.70, SD=11.11 at that the analyses conducted lacked sufficient statistical
baseline versus M=8.26, SD=8.58 at follow-up), t(22)= power to detect an effect. However, given the relatively
2.47, p=.02, r=.47. small effect sizes of the null findings, it is unlikely that
reduced power can fully account for these results. Alterna-
Nicotine Dependence Group differences in changes on tively, given the relatively brief nature of the intervention
self-reported nicotine dependence on the FTND from strategies taught in the study conditions, it is possible that
baseline to follow-up were evaluated. A mixed ANOVA participants were not given the appropriate “dose” of
revealed a marginally significant interaction between time treatment necessary to cause immediate change, at least to
and condition, F(1, 45)=3.46, p=.07, partial η2 =.07. Post a degree that between-group differences would be evident.
hoc paired-samples t-tests indicated that participants' Researchers have noted that there is a paucity of empirical
nicotine dependence scores were significantly reduced research on the relationship between the duration and
from baseline to follow-up in the mindfulness condition frequency of mindfulness practice and outcomes and as
(M=4.74, SD=1.15 at baseline; M=4.25, SD=1.48 at such, little is known about whether mindfulness practices
follow-up), t(23)=2.22, p=.04, but not in the suppression can be successfully delivered using practice times shorter
condition (M = 4.43, SD = 1.50 at baseline; M = 4.61, than 45 min (Vettese et al. 2009). However, one study
SD=1.59 at follow-up), t(22)=−0.61, p=.55. found that participation in a series of 10-min mindfulness
exercises, as part of a mindfulness course for clinical
psychologists in training, was associated with greater levels
of self-reported mindfulness at post-intervention (Moore
Discussion 2008). As such, it would not be unreasonable to suspect
that the intervention administered in the current study
The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects would result in changes in state mindfulness within the
of a brief mindfulness-based versus suppression strategy for mindfulness condition.
coping with craving on nicotine use, dependence, and self- Another possible reason why between-group differences
efficacy in coping with urges among cigarette smokers. on these variables may not have been detected may involve
Findings from the current study indicate that all participants the cue induction procedure that participants engaged in
reported greater levels of self-efficacy to refrain from prior to learning their respective craving reduction strategy.
smoking across a variety of contexts from baseline to During the cue induction procedure, participants were
follow-up, regardless of study condition. Furthermore, all asked to pay close attention to a cigarette and other cues
participants, regardless of study condition, exhibited reduc- associated with smoking by using their sense of sight,
tions in smoking quantity from baseline to follow-up. This touch, smell, and taste. Given that all participants engaged
is consistent with the findings of Bowen and Marlatt in this cue induction procedure, it is possible that this
(2009). Surprisingly, although the craving reduction strat- exercise increased all participants' present moment aware-
egies were introduced immediately following a cue induc- ness, which is a core feature of mindfulness (Kabat-Zinn
tion procedure to evaluate whether they can effectively 1994). As such, this exposure procedure alone may have
attenuate the experience of craving immediately as well as been more powerful than the respective strategy that the
over a follow-up period, no significant immediate reduc- participant engaged in subsequently, reducing the expected
tions in self-reported craving were found. This suggests that differences between the conditions.
receiving the mindfulness-based intervention did not Furthermore, the suppression strategy itself may have
change participants' levels of craving to a greater degree given participants mixed messages regarding coping with
than the suppression condition, at least as captured by this their cravings, such that, on the one hand, participants were
self-report measure. asked to pay attention to what they were doing, while, on
The equivalence between the mindfulness and suppres- the other hand, they were told to suppress thoughts
sion conditions in terms of their impacts on self-efficacy associated with that experience. In most research involving
and amount of smoking was surprising. While it is possible a suppression strategy, participants are instructed to first
that both strategies may be effective at attenuating cigarette pay attention to a given thought or stimulus for a number of
smoking in the short-term, research on the counter- minutes and then actively suppress their thoughts regarding
22 Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26

that stimulus (e.g., Clark et al. 1991). However, within the activation, which has been linked with more adaptive
current study, cue induction and coping strategy instruc- responding to, and faster recovery following, negative
tions were interwoven such that participants were instructed events. Similarly, Arch and Craske (2006) found that a
to use their respective coping strategy while engaging in the brief, focused breathing exercise that is an analog of
cue exposure. This may have caused some confusion mindfulness led to increased behavioral willingness to
among participants in the suppression condition, which tolerate unpredictable, negative stimuli. Previous research
may have resulted in them using mindfulness-based skills also indicates that mindfulness training is associated with
despite being in the suppression condition. As such, the improved symptoms of depression (e.g., Ree and Craigie
suppression condition may not have been adequately 2007), which is also consistent with the present investiga-
differentiated from the mindfulness condition, as both tion. The reduction in negative affect and depression may
involved deliberate exposure to thoughts, emotions, and also have contributed to participants' reduced nicotine
visceral experiences related to craving. Future research dependence within the mindfulness condition, given that
could attempt to remedy these issues by instructing these emotional states were associated with increases in the
participants repeatedly to suppress any and all urges to urge to use a substance. As such, it is possible that
smoke, and compare this intervention to others, such as participants in the mindfulness condition felt less of an
those used in the current research. urge to smoke because they were not experiencing as many
In contrast to the similarity of findings across conditions of the aversive symptoms associated with nicotine with-
with respect to the above smoking variables, a number of drawal, which resulted in them feeling less dependent on
interesting between-group findings did emerge. As hypoth- cigarettes.
esized, participants' nicotine dependence scores decreased Furthermore, research findings indicate that the mood state
from baseline to follow-up in the mindfulness condition, that existed during the initial suppression of a thought will
but not in the suppression condition. Furthermore, negative become reinstated when that thought is later expressed
affect decreased from baseline to follow-up for participants (Wenzlaff et al. 1991). Thus, given that the cue induction
in the mindfulness condition, but not in the suppression procedure used in the current study was designed to induce
group. In fact, participants in the suppression condition craving, which is associated with increases in subjective
reported an increase in negative affect from baseline to negative affect (e.g., Leventhal et al. 2007), one would expect
follow-up, although this increase was not statistically that individuals who were asked to suppress thoughts related
significant. This is inconsistent with findings from Bowen to their craving during the cue induction would experience a
and Marlatt’s (2009) study, which did not find significant reinstatement of these emotions when those thoughts arose
differences between conditions on negative affect. A similar later. This may explain why individuals in the suppression
pattern of results was observed in participants' depression condition reported some increase in their negative affect and
scores. Specifically, individuals in the mindfulness condi- depression, whereas individuals in the mindfulness conditions
tion reported significantly fewer symptoms of depression at reported a decrease on these measures.
follow-up, as compared with that at baseline. However, The current study is only the second empirical investiga-
participants in the suppression condition displayed the tion to directly compare the effectiveness of a brief,
opposite pattern of results, such that their depression scores mindfulness-based strategy for coping with smoking urges
increased at follow-up, although this again was not a to an alternative strategy, namely a suppression-based
statistically significant change. Taken together, these find- strategy. The isolation and direct comparison of these two
ings suggest that although all study participants showed strategies is a significant strength of the current methodology
similar improvements in terms of amount of smoking and because it allowed for an investigation of the impact of these
abstinence self-efficacy at follow-up; participants in the two particular strategies without the additive effects of other
mindfulness condition also reported feeling better at follow- variables typically included within a larger treatment protocol.
up, as indicated by their self-reported decrease in negative Additionally, both strategies were face valid and viewed as
affect and depression, and their reported reduction in equally credible forms of intervention by the participants.
subjective nicotine dependence. Furthermore, the cue induction and craving interventions
These findings are consistent with those of previous were pre-recorded, which allowed for consistency between
research indicating that mindfulness training is associated participants and study conditions, and all instructions were
with a reduction in negative affect and mood disturbance. counterbalanced between conditions for length of instructions
For example, previous research suggests that mindfulness and the number of times that smoking-related words were
training programs can lead to more adaptive responding to stated within the instruction sets. Finally, the current study
negative stimuli in the environment. Davidson and col- included a biological measure of expired breath CO to verify
leagues (2003) found that mindfulness meditation training participants' smoking status, a variable that was not
was associated with greater left-sided anterior brain measured in previous research.
Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26 23

While findings from the current study provide some extensive mindfulness training that also incorporates a control
preliminary support for the use of mindfulness-based condition is warranted. Similarly, future research would
strategies for smoking, there are several limitations to benefit from including a longitudinal design and including
consider. First, these findings are based on a relatively an at-home practice component and homework check, to
small sample of individuals who were not necessarily allow for a more extensive evaluation of the effectiveness of
treatment-seeking, which may limit the external validity of the interventions over time and with practice.
the findings. Some participants also reported a relatively Finally, given that both the suppression and mindfulness
low level of nicotine dependence at baseline, which may strategy yielded similar outcomes in terms of smoking amount
have contributed to some of the null findings in the current and self-efficacy to refrain from smoking at follow-up, future
study. Additionally, all measures used within the current research could investigate what specific individual difference
study were self-reported, and the methodology did not factors predict who will benefit more from which coping
incorporate a manipulation check following the craving strategy. It may be the case that some individuals are simply
reduction intervention to ensure that participants under- better suited for one strategy versus the other, and identifying
stood the instructions of their respective craving reduction the factors that contribute to these differences could be
strategy. Furthermore, some of the measures used were extremely useful, clinically. For example, a study conducted
found not to be highly internally consistent. It is possible by Barnier and colleagues (2004) suggests that individuals
that the fact that we recruited individuals who had thought with a repressive coping style, which is characterized by low
about cutting back on their smoking or attempted to quit in scores on self-report measures of anxiety and high scores on
the past resulted in significant heterogeneity among the measures of defensiveness, are highly effective at using
sample. The utilization of a treatment-seeking sample in thought suppression strategies. They also tend to rate their
future replications of this study would likely address this suppression attempts as being less effortful than others and
issue. Finally, approximately 20% of the sample did not experience no rebound effect of those thoughts. These, and
return for the second session. This was likely due to the fact other personality characteristics, could be explored to
that participants did not initially receive enough compen- elucidate variables that might help match individuals with
sation for attending the second session to offset their travel particular craving coping strategies.
costs. Once compensation for attending the second session Overall, findings from the current study provide prelim-
was increased, retention rates improved. inary support for the use of mindfulness-based strategies for
In light of these limitations, the findings from the current coping with smoking urges, as well as evidence for the
investigation warrant replication with a larger and less benefits for suppression of smoking urges. Both mindful-
heterogeneous sample, which would ideally substantiate the ness and suppression were associated with smoking fewer
development of mindfulness-based interventions for nicotine cigarettes and greater self-efficacy in refraining from
dependence and other addictive problems. It would be smoking in the days following learning these brief
beneficial for future research to incorporate physiological strategies. Interestingly, only participants in the mindfulness
measures of the stress associated with craving in order to condition reported reductions in negative affect, depressive
corroborate the self-report measures used in the current study. symptoms, and subjective nicotine dependence. Thus, the
Additionally, given that some of the self-report measures used use of mindfulness-based techniques for coping with
within the present study were shown to not be highly cigarette cravings appears to provide some additional
internally consistent, the current findings should be interpreted benefits not obtained when coping with smoking cravings
cautiously until future replication. Furthermore, given that the through suppression. Future studies comparing more
cue induction procedure may have inadvertently confounded extensive mindfulness and suppression interventions for
the instructions given to participants in the suppression smoking cessation and with participants who are specifi-
condition, it is recommended that future research utilize an cally seeking treatment for smoking cessation, would help
alternative to an instruction-based cue induction procedure. to clarify further the relative merits of mindfulness versus
For example, it may be possible to use a virtual reality suppression strategies in coping with cravings, as well as
smoking cue induction paradigm to induce craving (e.g., smoking and emotion-based outcomes.
Traylor et al. 2008), rather than an in vivo cue exposure.
Moreover, it would be useful to study whether including
more extensive mindfulness and suppression training would References
yield greater differences between conditions. For example,
Davis and colleagues (2007) recently conducted a pilot
Anda, R. F., Croft, J. B., Felitti, V. J., Nordenberg, D., Giles, W. H.,
study of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction for smokers,
Williamson, D. F., et al. (1999). Adverse childhood experiences
which was shown to be effective at a 6-week follow-up and smoking during adolescence and adulthood. Journal of the
assessment. Thus, a larger comparative trial with more American Medical Association, 282, 1652–1658.
24 Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26

Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M.,
Emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F., et al. (2003). Alterations in brain and
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1849–1858. immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psycho-
Baer, J. (2002). Student factors: Understanding individual variation in somatic Medicine, 65, 564–570.
college drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 14, 40–53. Davis, J. M., Fleming, M. F., Bonus, K. A., & Baker, T. B. (2007). A
Baer, R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A pilot study on mindfulness based stress reduction for smokers.
conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology: Science BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 7, 1–7.
and Practice, 10, 125–143. Devilly, G. J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2000). Psychometric properties of
Baer, J. S., Holt, C. S., & Lichtenstein, E. (1986). Self-efficacy and the credibility/expectancy questionnaire. Journal of Behavior
smoking reexamined: Construct validity and clinical utility. Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 31, 73–86.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 846–852. DiFranza, J. R., & Wellman, R. J. (2005). A sensitization-homeostasis
Bagot, K. S., Heishman, S. J., & Moolchan, E. T. (2007). Tobacco model of nicotine craving, withdrawal, and tolerance: Integrating
craving predicts lapse to smoking among adolescent smokers in the clinical and basic science literature. Nicotine & Tobacco
cessation treatment. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 9, 647–652. Research, 7, 9–26.
Baker, T. B., Morse, E., & Sherman, J. E. (1986). The motivation to Ellison, L. F., Mao, Y., & Gibbons, L. (1995). Projected smoking-
use drugs: a psychobiological analysis of urges. Nebraska attributable mortality in Canada, 1991–2000. Chronic Diseases
Symposium on Motivation, 34, 257–323. in Canada, 16, 84–89.
Barnier, A. J., Levin, K., & Maher, A. (2004). Suppressing thoughts of Erblich, J., & Bovbjerg, D. H. (2004). In vivo versus imaginal
past events: are repressive copers good suppressors? Cognition smoking cue exposures: Is seeing believing? Experimental and
and Emotion, 18, 513–531. Clinical Psychopharmacology, 12, 208–215.
Bishop, S. R. (2002). What do we really know about mindfulness- Fagerstrom, K. O., & Schneider, N. G. (1989). Measuring nicotine
based stress reduction? Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 71–83. dependence: a review of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire.
Bowen, S., & Marlatt, G. A. (2007, November). Effects of Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 159–182.
mindfulness-based strategies on urges, negative affect, and Fagerstrom, K. O., Heatherton, T. F., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1991).
smoking. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Nicotine addiction and its assessment. Ear, Nose, & Throat
Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies, Philadelphia, PA. Journal, 69, 763–765.
Bowen, S., & Marlatt, G. A. (2009). Surfing the urge: brief Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J.
mindfulness-based intervention for college student smokers. (2007). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: the development
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, 666–671. and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness
Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., Dillworth, T. M., & Marlatt, G. A. (2007). Scale—revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology and
The role of thought suppression in the relationship between Behavioral Assessment, 29, 177–190.
mindfulness meditation and alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors, 32, Fiore, M. C., Bailey, W. C., Cohen, S. J., Dorfman, S. F., Gritz, E. R.,
2324–2328. Heyman, R. B., et al. (2000). Treating tobacco use and
Brandon, T. H., Tiffany, S. T., Obremski, K. M., & Baker, T. B. dependence. Clinical practice guideline. Rockville, MD: United
(1990). Postcessation cigarette use: the process of relapse. States Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health
Addictive Behaviors, 15, 105–114. Service.
Breslau, N., Kilbey, M. M., & Andreski, P. (1992). Nicotine Fonder, M. A., Sacco, K. A., Termine, A., Boland, B. S., Seyal, A. A.,
withdrawal symptoms and psychiatric disorders: Findings from Dudas, M. M., et al. (2005). Smoking cue reactivity in
an epidemiologic study of young adults. The American Journal schizophrenia: effects of a nicotinic receptor antagonist. Biolog-
of Psychiatry, 149, 464–469. ical Psychiatry, 57, 802–808.
Breslin, F., Zack, M., & McMain, S. (2002). An information- Garvey, A. J., Bliss, R. E., Hitchcock, J. L., Heinold, J. W., & Rosner,
processing analysis of mindfulness: implications for relapse B. (1992). Predictors of smoking relapse among self-quitters: a
prevention in the treatment of substance abuse. Clinical Psychol- report from the normative aging study. Addictive Behaviors, 17,
ogy: Science and Practice, 9, 275–299. 367–377.
Brown, R. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., & Wagner, E. F. Ghadirian, P. (2004). Sleeping with a killer: The effect of smoking on
(1996). Cigarette smoking, major depression, and other psychi- human health. A report from Tobacco Control Programme,
atric disorders among adolescents. Journal of the American Health Canada
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1602–1610. Glassman, A. H. (1993). Cigarette smoking: Implications for
Brown, R. A., Burgess, E. S., Sales, S. D., Evans, D. M., & Miller, I. psychiatric illness. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 150,
W. (1998). Reliability and validity of a smoking timeline follow- 546–553.
back interview. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 12, 101–112. Glassman, A. H., Helzer, J. E., Covey, L. S., Cottler, L. B., Stetner, F.,
Carmody, T. P., Vieten, C., & Astin, J. A. (2007). Negative affect, Tipp, J. E., et al. (1990). Smoking, smoking cessation, and major
emotional acceptance, and smoking cessation. Journal of depression. Journal of the American Medical Association, 12,
Psychoactive Drugs, 39, 499–508. 1546–1549.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2005). Smoking— Gonzalez, A., Vujanovic, A. A., Johnson, K. A., Leyro, T. M., &
attributable mortality, years of potential life lost, and economic Zvolensky, M. J. (2009). The role of mindful attention in regard
costs—United States, 1997–2001. Washington, DC: CDC. to the relation between negative affect reduction outcome
Clark, D. M., Ball, S., & Pape, D. (1991). An experimental expectancies and emotional vulnerability among adult cigarette
investigation of thought suppression. Behaviour Research and smokers. Cognitive Therapy Research, 33, 645–656.
Therapy, 29, 253–257. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two
Covey, L. S., Glassman, A. H., & Stetner, F. (1990). Depression and emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relation-
depressive symptoms in smoking cessation. Comprehensive ships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychiatry, 31, 350–354. Psychology, 85, 348–362.
Covey, L. S., Glassman, A. H., & Stetner, F. (1997). Major depression Gwaltney, C. J., Shiffman, S., Norman, G. J., Paty, J. A., Kassel, J. D.,
following smoking cessation. The American Journal of Psychi- Gnys, M., et al. (2001). Does smoking abstinence self-efficacy
atry, 154, 263–265. vary across situations? Identifying context-specificity within the
Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26 25

relapse situation efficacy questionnaire. Journal of Consulting Peto, R., Lopez, A. D., Boreham, J., Thun, M., Heath, C., & Doll, R.
and Clinical Psychology, 69, 516–527. (1996). Mortality from smoking worldwide. British Medical
Haaga, D. A. F., & Allison, M. L. (1994). Thought suppression and Bulletin, 52, 12–21.
smoking relapse: a secondary analysis of Haaga (1989). The Pollack, M. H., Penava, S. A., Bolton, E., Worthington, J. J., III,
British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 327–331. Allen, G. L. A., Farach, F. J., Jr., et al. (2002). A novel cognitive-
Hall, S. M., Munoz, R. F., Reus, V. I., & Sees, K. L. (1993). Nicotine, behavioural approach for treatment-resistant drug dependence.
negative affect, and depression. Journal of Consulting and Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 23, 335–342.
Clinical Psychology, 61, 761–767. Ree, M. J., & Craigie, M. A. (2007). Outcomes following
Havermans, R. C., Debaere, S., Smulders, F. T. Y., Wiers, R. W., & mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in a heterogeneous sample
Jansen, A. T. M. (2003). Effects of cue exposure, urge to smoke, of adult outpatients. Behaviour Change, 24, 70–86.
and nicotine deprivation on cognitive performance in smokers. Salkovskis, P. M., & Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17, 336–339. smoking cessation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 193–201.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance Sayette, M. A., Martin, C. S., Wertz, J. M., Shiffman, S., & Perrott, M.
and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behaviour A. (2001). A multi-dimensional analysis of cue-elicited craving
change. New York, NY: Guilford Press. in heavy smokers and tobacco chippers. Addiction, 96, 1419–
Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., & Fagerstrom, K. 1432.
(1991). The fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence: a revision of Shadel, W. G., & Shiffman, S. (2005). Assessment of smoking
the fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire. British Journal of behavior. In D. M. Donovan & G. A. Marlatt (Eds.), Assessment
Addiction, 86, 1119–1127. of addictive behaviors (2nd ed., pp. 113–154). New York, NY:
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness Guilford Press.
meditation in everyday life. New York, NY: Hyperion. Shiffman, S. (1991). Refining models of dependence: Variations
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2005). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your across persons and situations. British Journal of Addiction, 86,
body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness: 15th Anniversary. 611–615.
New York, NY: Delta Trade Paperback/Bantam Dell. Sobell, M. B., Maisto, S. A., Sobell, L. C., Cooper, A. M., Cooper, T.
Kassel, J. D., Stroud, L. R., & Paronis, C. A. (2003). Smoking, stress, C., & Sanders, B. (1980). Developing a prototype for evaluating
and negative affect: correlation, causation, and context across alcohol treatment effectiveness. In L. C. Sobell, M. B. Sobell, &
stages of smoking. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 270–304. E. Ward (Eds.), Evaluating alcohol and drug abuse treatment
Lerman, C., Audrain, J., Orleans, C. T., Boyd, R., Gold, K., Main, D., effectiveness: Recent advances (pp. 129–150). Elmsford, NY:
et al. (1996). Investigation of mechanisms linking depressed Pergamon Press.
mood to nicotine dependence. Addictive Behaviors, 21, 9–19. Sommese, T., & Patterson, J. C. (1995). Acute effects of cigarette
Leventhal, A. M., Waters, A. J., Boyd, S., Moolchan, E. T., Lerman, smoking withdrawal: A review of the literature. Aviation Space
C., & Pickworth, W. B. (2007). Gender differences in acute and Environmental Medicine, 66, 164–167.
tobacco withdrawal: effects on subjective, cognitive, and phys- Stapleton, J. (1998). Cigarette smoking prevalence, cessation, and
iological measures. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharma- relapse. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 7, 187–203.
cology, 15, 21–36. Stuart, K., Borland, R., & McMurray, N. (1994). Self-efficacy, health
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative locus of control, and smoking cessation. Addictive Behaviors, 19,
emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress 1–12.
scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2004).
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335–343. Results from the 2003 national survey on drug use and health:
Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (1985). Relapse prevention: National findings (Office of Applied Studies, NSDUH Series H–
Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors. 25, DHHS Publication No. SMA 04–3964). Rockville, MD.
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Thorsteinsson, H. S., Gillin, J. C., Patten, C. A., Golshan, S., Sutton,
Marlatt, G. A., Witkiewitz, K., Dillworth, T. M., Bowen, S. W., Parks, L. D., Drummond, S., et al. (2001). The effects of transdermal
G. A., Macpherson, L. M., et al. (2004). Vipassana meditation as nicotine therapy for smoking cessation on depressive symptoms
a treatment for alcohol and drug use disorders. In S. C. Hayes, V. in patients with major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology,
M. Follette, & M. M. Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and 24, 350–358.
acceptance: Expanding the cognitive-behavioral tradition. New Tidey, J. W., Rohsenow, D. J., Kaplan, G. B., & Swift, R. M. (2005).
York, NY: Guilford Press. Subjective and physiological responses to smoking cues in
Miranda, R., Rohsenow, D. J., Monti, P. M., Tidey, J., & Ray, L. smokers with schizophrenia. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 7,
(2008). Effects of repeated days of smoking cue exposure on 421–429.
urge to smoke and physiological reactivity. Addictive Behaviors, Toll, B. A., Sobell, M. B., Wagner, E. F., & Sobell, L. C. (2001). The
33, 347–353. relationship between thoughts suppression and smoking cessa-
Moore, P. (2008). Introducing mindfulness to clinical psychologists in tion. Addictive Behaviors, 26, 509–515.
training: an experimental course of brief exercises. Journal of Toneatto, T., Vettese, L. C., & Nguyen, L. (2007). The role of
Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 15, 331–337. mindfulness in the cognitive-behavioural treatment of problem
Myrsten, A., Elgerot, A., & Edgren, B. (1977). Effects of abstinence from gambling. Journal of Gambling Issues, 19, 91–100.
tobacco smoking on physiological and psychological arousal levels Traylor, A. C., Bordnick, P. S., & Carter, B. L. (2008). Assessing
in habitual smokers. Psychosomatic Medicine, 39, 25–38. craving in young adult smokers using virtual reality. American
Niaura, R., Britt, D. M., Borrelli, B., Shadel, W. G., Abrams, D. B., & Journal on Addiction, 17, 436–440.
Goldstein, M. G. (1999). History and symptoms of depression United States Department of Health and Human Services (1988). The
among smokers during a self-initiated quit attempt. Nicotine & health consequences of smoking. In Nicotine addiction: A report
Tobacco Research, 1, 251–257. of the US surgeon general. Washington, DC: Author.
Orlando, M., Ellickson, P. L., & Jinnett, K. (2001). The temporal United States Department of Health and Human Services (1990).
relationship between emotional distress and cigarette smoking Psychological and behavioral consequences and correlates of
during adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Consulting smoking cessation. In Health benefits of smoking cessation: A report
and Clinical Psychology, 69, 959–970. of the US surgeon general (pp. 517–578). Rockville, MD: Author.
26 Mindfulness (2011) 2:14–26

Vernig, P. M., & Orsillo, S. M. (2009). Psychophysiological and self- Watkins, E. R., & Moulds, M. (2007). Revealing negative thinking in
reported emotional responding in alcohol-dependent college recovered major depression: a preliminary investigation. Behav-
students: The impact of brief acceptance/mindfulness instruc- iour Research and Therapy, 45, 3069–3076.
tions. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 38, 174–183. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
Vettese, L. C. (2008, April). Mindfulness, mental health and addictive validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the
behaviour in outpatient substance use treatment. Paper presented PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
at the Conference for Integrating Mindfulness-Based Interven- 54, 1063–1070.
tions into Medicine, Health Care, and Society, Worcester, MA. Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. I. (1994). Chronic thought suppression.
Vettese, L. C., Toneatto, T., Stea, J. N., Nguyen, L., & Wang, J. J. (2009). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 615–640.
Do mindfulness meditations participants actually practice? And does Weiss-Gerlach, E., Franck, M., Neuner, B., Gentilello, L. M.,
it make a difference? A review of the empirical evidence. Journal of Neumann, T., Tonnesen, H., et al. (2008). Motivation of trauma
Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 23, 198–225. patients to stop smoking after admission to the emergency
Vidrine, J. I., Businelle, M. S., Cinciripini, P., Li, Y., Marcus, M., Waters, department. Addictive Behaviors, 33, 906–918.
A. J., et al. (2009). Associations of mindfulness with nicotine Wenzlaff, R. M., Wegner, D. M., & Klein, S. B. (1991). The role of
dependence, withdrawal, and agency. Substance Abuse, 30, 318–327. thought suppression in the bonding of thought and mood.
Vieten, C. (2005). A mindfulness-based approach to relapse preven- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 500–508.
tion [Abstract]. Tobacco-related disease research program. West, R. J., & Grunberg, N. E. (1991). Implications of tobacco use as
Abstract for grant #14IT-0178. an addiction. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 485–488.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen