Sie sind auf Seite 1von 5

Review

Folia Phoniatr Logop 2017;69:38–42 Published online: December 15, 2017


DOI: 10.1159/000479181

Neurobiological Bases of Autism


Spectrum Disorders and Implications for
Early Intervention: A Brief Overview
Kakia Petinou Despo Minaidou
Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Cyprus University of Technology, Limassol, Cyprus

Keywords goals could potentially improve intervention outcomes in


Neurobiology · Autism spectrum disorder · Joint attention · terms of improving social communication skills in young-
Eye gaze · Early intervention sters with ASD. © 2017 S. Karger AG, Basel

Abstract
Objectives: To better understand the pathogenesis of au- Introduction
tism spectrum disorder (ASD) and implications for interven-
tion, the current paper reports on research related to the Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex neuro-
neurobiological underpinnings of ASD and the implication developmental impairment that affects children and fam-
for early intervention with a focus on the importance of joint ilies regardless of language background, religion, and so-
attention and eye gaze behaviors. Participants and Meth- cioeconomic status around the world. ASD presents as
ods: An overview is provided on the available research find- one of the most prevalent neurobiological disorders of
ings from the fields of social neuroscience and experimental early childhood with affected individuals forming a het-
psychology specific to brain development, brain pathology, erogeneous group with severity and manifestation of
eye gaze, and joint attention behaviors. Results: The results symptoms varying from mild to severe [1]. Based on the
of the review converge towards the existence of aberrant Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders V
brain connections and atypical brain morphology areas, (DSM-5) [2], one area of particular challenge for children
which in complex and dynamic ways hinder the prioritiza- with ASD deals with social pragmatic skills and issues re-
tion of social information. Consequently, the atypical social lated to theory of mind (ToM) which is defined as the
interaction skills exhibited by infants at risk for developing ability to reflect and decode what others know and believe
ASD are traced in the malformation of respective brain con- [3, 4]. Furthermore, individuals with ASD exhibit prob-
nections. Conclusions: Given the importance of neurobio- lems in the linguistic subdomain of pragmatics described
logical findings and their mapping onto early social prag- as the ability to exhibit appropriate use of language for
matic skills, early intervention goals need to focus on in- social purposes (e.g., turn taking, humor appreciation,
creasing appropriate eye gaze skills and joint attention. Such processing of metaphorical versus concrete language, us-

© 2017 S. Karger AG, Basel Kakia Petinou


Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Cyprus University of Technology
Vragadinos Street 15
E-Mail karger@karger.com
CY–3041 Limassol (Cyprus)
www.karger.com/fpl E-Mail kakia.petinou @ cut.ac.cy
age of greetings, reading facial expression, etc.) [2, 5]. A improving social communication skills via joint attention
recent review by Westby [6] discussed the neurobiologi- and eye contact. It is noted that the current report pro-
cal underpinnings related to ASD vis-à-vis affective and vides general rather than exhaustive information related
social ToM under the umbrella term of social neurosci- to the neurobiological and intervention interphase in
ence as advanced by Cacioppo and Bertson [7] and Bar- ASD.
on-Cohen [8].
In the last 10 years, the field of social neuroscience has
highlighted the relationship between neuroanatomical Research Related to Brain Development, Eye Gaze
brain regions and their involvement in atypical social be- Skills, and Joint Attention in ASD
havior as evidenced in ASD pathogenesis and symptom-
atology [9–11]. Such a relationship appears to operate in Neuroscience findings serve to elucidate the develop-
a bidirectional manner. That is, the ability to exhibit ap- ment of ASD over time. To date, research has shown that
propriate social behavior skills (i.e., using appropriate eye aberrant brain activity and atypical brain morphology
contact behaviors) influences neurobiological events. In during infancy appear to underline the manifestation of
turn, neuroanatomical, neurochemical, and neurophysi- ASD symptomatology. Deviant brain growth in ASD oc-
ological events occurring in cortical and subcortical brain curs during a critical age period of development when the
areas trigger and determine the person’s ability to under- formation and connectivity of cerebral circuits are at their
stand the meaning behind pragmatic aspects of commu- prime, in the most productive and optimum stage of syn-
nication. Such skills include the decoding of facial expres- aptic activity. Aberrant brain growth might disrupt the
sions, the appropriate use of eye contact, and joint atten- formation of typical cerebral connectivity necessary for
tion [12, 13]. Data suggest that the infant’s attention to a the operation of optimum neural pathways, a process that
person’s face is mediated by the subcortical area of the might eventually lead to autism [9, 10, 17].
visual neural pathway (e.g., from the retina to the occipi- Information on brain development in children with
tal lobe) [11, 14]. It appears that appropriate use of eye ASD stems from systematic research developed in the last
contact in infancy strengthens the establishment of such 10 years. These findings provide information regarding
neural pathways. In turn, reciprocal use of eye contact the neurobiological, neuroanatomical, and genetic un-
contributes to the appropriate response by the child dur- derpinnings of ASD. Findings from functional magnetic
ing joint attention instances occurring when 2 people resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shed light on the
share interest in an object or event. Findings from neuro- nature and pattern of brain activity and growth in ASD
science research map directly onto the early diagnostic infants [18]. Specifically, Weigelt et al. [17] and Cour­
markers evident in ASD high-risk infants even as early as chesne et al. [18] detected remarkable growth patterns
in the first months of the child’s life. Diagnostic biomark- and changes in cerebellar brain areas, including the size
ers include poor eye contact, lack of affective reciprocity, of the cerebral vermis, defined as a region of the brain re-
atypical eye gaze behavior, and lack of joint attention sponsible for motor control and cognitive functions. Spe-
skills [11, 13–15]. This information proves to be an in- cifically, toddlers with ASD showed occurrences of atyp-
valuable diagnostic criterion for clinical assessment and ical brain overgrowth during the first and second year of
intervention; specifically, joint attention skills which life. Aberrant brain overgrowth was noted in the cerebel-
emerge during the first year of life (around the age of 9 lar, cerebral, and limbic structures. These areas are re-
months) and become established by the age of 18 months sponsible for the process and the use of higher-order cog-
are important. Note that such skills appear to be among nitive, pragmatic, emotional, social, and language func-
the earliest diagnostic markers for possible ASD diagno- tions. In subsequent investigations, Courchesne and
sis in high-risk infants [12, 14, 16]. colleagues [10, 18] and Weigelt et al. [17] revealed that
On a par with research reports, theory-motivated and excessive brain growth was followed by abnormal decel-
evidenced-based findings can be adopted when develop- eration of cerebral areas [18]. These findings seem to pro-
ing specific therapy intervention goals targeting the im- vide evidence for the existence of complex and multiple
provement of eye contact, eye gaze, and joint attention modalities that are abnormal in ASD, possibly due to neu-
skills. In the sections to follow we provide (a) a brief over- roanatomical lesions in multiple brain regions involving
view of research findings related to brain development, the number of cell bodies in the prefrontal cortex. Inter-
eye gaze skills, and joint attention in toddlers with ASD estingly, the role of the prefrontal cortex and temporal
and (b) suggestions related to early intervention goals for parietal junction has been shown to be active during ex-

ASD Neurobiology and Intervention Folia Phoniatr Logop 2017;69:38–42 39


DOI: 10.1159/000479181
perimental tasks that require processing of events related ability of factors that might contribute to language out-
to people’s emotions, thoughts, and beliefs (affective and comes (e.g., cognitive ability, presence of familial histo-
cognitive ToM, respectively) [15]. Support for the contri- ry). On parallel lines, reports from MRI studies indicated
bution of specific areas in the brain responsible for pri- that high-risk ASD infants showed delayed responses
oritizing social information comes from research projects when shifting eye gaze during tasks that measure eye
focusing on the involvement of the corpus callosum [9, movements and eye contact. The researchers attributed
19]. One property that underlies the initial steps of social this delay to atypical connectivity patterns observed in the
interaction is that of appropriate reciprocal eye contact splenium, a brain structure found in the area of the cor-
followed by skills of joint attention. In these 2 areas, in- pus callosum responsible for connecting the right and left
fants who develop ASD and infants at risk for developing cerebral hemispheres [19]. Comparable investigations re-
ASD (e.g., with a significant familial history for ASD) ap- lated to eye gaze behaviors in infants who were high risk
peared to lag behind on the expression and development for developing ASD have been reported by Jones and Klin
of appropriate prelinguistic social skills [1, 2]. Based on in 2013 [14] and Klin and Jones in 2008 [22]. The inves-
diffusion tensor imaging, Wolff et al. [19] in 2012 indi- tigators indicated that the young high-risk participants
cated that the expression of ASD “communication-so- preferred looking at a person’s mouth rather than looking
cial” phenotypes is possibly preceded by atypical develop- at the eyes, a pattern not observed in the neurotypical
ment of cerebral white matter pathways during infancy controls. Overall, eye gaze and eye fixation behaviors dif-
[14]. Such findings offer insight regarding the gradual ferentiate high-risk from low-risk ASD infants after the
rather than the sudden onset of ASD pathogenesis. This age of 2 months. The findings suggested that neurobio-
gradual occurrence appears to be “unfolding” during in- logical reorganization followed different patterns in the 2
fancy rather than emerging during the second year of life study groups. In summary, neuroimaging investigations
after a period of unremarkable development. Further- of ASD have advanced our understanding of atypical
more, recent data on the importance of cerebellar path- brain structure, morphology, and function. Researchers
ways predicted later sensory responsiveness in toddlers have identified abnormalities in brain regions involved in
with ASD symptoms [20]. Specifically, the findings sug- social behavior, eye gaze, and eye contact. Recent reports
gested that restricted and repetitive behaviors on a par have also proposed new ways of identifying ASD behav-
with ASD symptomatology at age 2 years were associated iors through the use of optometry, since this method al-
with structural properties of callosal and cerebellar white lows the examination of specific visual behaviors consis-
matter pathways measured during infancy and toddler- tent with ASD symptomatology. Given the difficulties re-
hood. They further suggested that repetitive behaviors garding visual processing difficulties and atypical eye
seen in ASD and atypical sensory integration response gaze and eye contact behaviors manifested in ASD, the
patterns co-occurred and share common brain-behavior authors supported such aberrant visual difficulties to oc-
relationships. cur in ASD toddlers during the process of optometric ex-
Appropriate attention shifting in early infancy is para- amination [23].
mount to later social pragmatic and cognitive develop- Collectively, experimental paradigms using MRI and
ment. In addition, joint attention skills that emerge as neuroimaging have shed light on the aberrant behaviors
early as at 10 months of age are the “building blocks” for manifested in ASD, as well as have improved our under-
later social, pragmatic, and cognitive development [5, 8, standing of early symptomatology of ASD. Overall, con-
20]. One of the most significant prelinguistic social com- verging evidence supports the neurobiological under­
municative challenges seen in youngsters with ASD is the pinning regarding the pathogenesis, etiology, and inte-
inappropriate use of eye contact, eye gaze, and joint at­ grated functional and structural measures across brain
tention [11, 12, 21]. Pierce et al. [21] have shown that regions in ASD high-risk infants and toddlers [7]. We
toddlers with ASD exhibited atypical patterns of visual know from research data that there is a clear difference
preference as indexed by eye tracking behaviors. Fur­ between ASD and non-ASD subjects regarding the re-
thermore, when compared to their neurotypical peers, sponse of integrated brain regions responsible for eye
children with ASD preferred watching geometric as op- gaze and joint attention processing. The following section
posed to social interaction vignettes projected on a screen discusses possible clinical implications related to brain
[20, 21]. The authors proposed that unusual looking pref- behaviors in children with ASD with a particular focus on
erences and the lack of typical visual fixation behaviors eye gaze and joint attention, as these skills are considered
might act as early biomarkers of ASD, despite the vari- developmental precursors to affective ToM.

40 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2017;69:38–42 Petinou/Minaidou


DOI: 10.1159/000479181
Clinical Implications and Early Intervention However, it is important that clinicians become aware of
different types of joint attention and eye contact skills.
Despite contradictory and unequivocal findings, data Discerning the different types of communication acts be-
converge towards the existence of brain regions that con- comes of paramount importance. Since, in general, chil-
tribute to the lack of social skills and social prioritizing dren with ASD are not particularly interested in these
skills seen in ASD. Future longitudinal studies on brain kinds of social communication behaviors, early focused
development can contribute to clarifying the mechanisms intervention might increase their ability to begin to pri-
regarding ASD etiology, symptomatology, and variability oritize social information [5, 6]. Consequently, when
of social behaviors. Consequently, evidence-based infor- dealing with ASD, it is important to be aware of the non-
mation can lead to best clinical practices with a major fo- exhibited behaviors in addition to those behaviors exhib-
cus on bringing about positive and promising communi- ited by the child. The present overview of the neurobio-
cation outcomes. Given the plethora of available infor­ logical bases of ASD suggests that equivocality exists in
mation regarding the neurobiological substrate of ASD, the early diagnosis and early intervention of this perva-
targeted early intervention becomes imperative. Advances sive disorder. Through continued research into the patho-
in neuroimaging methods and large-scale collaborations genesis of ASD, maximum positive outcomes can be real-
will also allow for an integrated approach via linking neu- ized as characterized by a positive impact on the child, the
robiological information and early intervention regimes. family, and the educational/social system.
Given the early communication challenges associated Consequently, early intervention goals may focus on in-
with possible manifestations of ASD, one might hypoth- creasing social skills through joint attention in a way that
esize that by targeting the improvement of eye contact the child’s communicative potential is maximized and
and joint attention skills during infancy positive neuro- brain connectivity is improved. Clinical opportunities aris-
developmental and linguistic outcomes could emerge. ing from current neurobiological findings should be imple-
Atypical patterns of visual fixation, visual preference, and mented in terms of improving assessment and intervention
visual shifting can be used as both clinical markers and practices to improve long-term language outcomes [23].
clinical goals for ASD. This is because research findings
suggest that these behaviors map onto specific brain areas
and affect cerebral connectivity [23]. Based on what we Conclusion
know on social pragmatic deficits and current pragmat-
ics-based language development theory, clinicians can Overall, neuroscience research findings have shed
use natural settings to elicit and motivate communication light on how the brain functions during processing tasks
with emphasis on interaction involving shared control, requiring social and cognitive affective behaviors. As a
turn taking, and reciprocity in a rich and positive manner. result, the neurobiological underpinnings of ASD, even
Given that the most significant behavioral impact of ASD though not very well understood, point towards the exis-
is in the processing of social information, such as eye gaze, tence of certain brain areas which are responsible for ear-
facial expression, and speech, a primary focus of interven- ly developing communication skills, including eye con-
tions should be on facilitating these behaviors. Among tact, eye gaze, and joint attention behaviors, which pose a
these social behaviors, joint attention is a particularly particular challenge to youngsters with ASD. Given the
challenging interactive behavior for toddlers with ASD. aberrant brain functions which map onto early social
These children exhibit difficulty orienting and attending pragmatic behaviors in humans, early intervention might
to a social partner, shifting gaze between people and ob- eventually allow neurobiological reorganization that
jects, sharing affect or emotional state with another per- could reverse the negative developmental outcomes re-
son, following the gaze and point of another person, and lated with ASD [24]. Such areas of research await further
being able to draw another person’s attention to objects investigation as this arena of research and clinical frame-
or events for the purpose of sharing experiences [21, 22]. work remains underinvestigated, albeit promising in
Clearly, a high priority in the early intervention of chil- terms of favorable communication outcomes [25].
dren with ASD is for them to learn to engage and respond
and react to joint attention routines. As clinicians we are
interested in joint attention skills when targeting early in- Disclosure Statement
tervention goals because these skills form a critical foun-
dation for social, cognitive, and language development. The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

ASD Neurobiology and Intervention Folia Phoniatr Logop 2017;69:38–42 41


DOI: 10.1159/000479181
References
  1 Gillon G, Hyter Y, Dreux F, Ferman S, Hus Y, 11 Tomalski P, Csibra G, Johnson MH: Rapid 19 Wolff JJ, Gu H, Gerig G, Elison JT, Styner M,
Petinou K, Segal O, Tumarova T, Vogindrou- orienting toward face-like stimuli with gaze- Gouttard S, Botteron KN, Dager SR, Dawson
kas I, Westby C, Westerveld M: International relevant contrast information. Perception G, Estes AM, Evans AC, Hazlett HC, Kosto-
survey of speech-language pathologists’ prac- 2009;38:569–578. poulos P, McKinstry RC, Paterson SJ, Schultz
tices in working with children with autism 12 Sullivan M, Finelli J, Marvin A, Garrett-May- RT, Zwaigenbaum L, Piven J: Differences in
spectrum disorder. Folia Phoniatr Logop er E, Bauman M, Landa R: Response to joint white matter fiber tract development present
2017;69:8–19. attention in toddlers at risk for autism spec- from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism.
  2 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnos- trum disorder: a prospective study. J Autism Am J Psychiatry 2012;169:589–600.
tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor- Dev Disord 2007;37:37–48. 20 Elison JT, Wolff JJ, Heimer DC, Paterson SJ,
ders, ed 5. Arlington, American Psychiatric 13 Lombardo MV, Pierce K, Eyler LT, Barnes Gu H, Styner M, Gerig G, Piven J: Fronto­
Publishing, 2013. CC, Ahrens-Barbeau C: Different functional limbic neural circuitry at 6 months predicts
  3 Premack DG, Woodruff G: Does the chim- neural substrates for good and poor language individual differences in joint attention at 9
panzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci outcome in autism. Neuron 2014;86:567–597. months. Dev Sci 2013;16:186–197.
1978;1:515–528. 14 Jones W, Klin A: Attention to eyes is present 21 Pierce K, Marinero S, Hazin R, McKenna B,
  4 Baron-Cohen S: The development of a theory but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later Barnes C, Malige A: Eye tracking reveals ab-
of mind in autism: deviance and delay? Psy- diagnosed with autism. Nature 2013; 504: normal visual preference for geometric im-
chiatr North Am 1991;14:33–51. 427–431. ages as an early biomarker of an autism spec-
 5 Tager-Flushberg H: Risk factors associated 15 Shamy-Tsoory S, Aharon-Peretz J, Perry D: trum disorder subtype associated with in-
with language in autism spectrum disorder: Two systems for empathy: a double dissocia- creased symptom severity. Biol Psychiatry
clues to understanding mechanisms. J Speech tion between emotional and cognitive empa- 2016;79:657–666.
Lang Hear Res 2016;59:143–154. thy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventrome- 22 Klin A, Jones W: Altered face scanning and
  6 Westby CE: Social-neuroscience and theory dial prefrontal lesions. Brain 2009; 132: 617– impaired recognition of biological motion in
of mind. Folia Phoniatr Logop 2014;66:7–17. 627. a 15-month-old infant with autism. Dev Sci
  7 Cacioppo JT, Bertson GG: Social psychologi- 16 Bakeman R, Adamson L: Coordinating atten- 2008;11:40–60.
cal contributions to the decade of the brain: tion to people and objects in mother-infant 23 Press J, Richman J: The role of optometry in
doctrine of multi-level analysis. Am Psychol and peer infant interaction. Child Dev 1984; early identification of autism spectrum disor-
1992;47:1019–1028. 55:1278–1289. ders. Optm Vis Dev 2009;40:141–149.
  8 Baron-Cohen S: Mindblindness: An Essay on 17 Weigelt S, Koldewyn K, Kanwisher N: Face 24 Koegel LK: Communication and language in-
Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, identity recognition in ASD: a review of be- tervention; in Koegel RL, Koegel LK (eds):
MIT Press, 1995. havioral studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev Teaching Children with Autism: Strategies
  9 Wolff JJ, Piven J: On the emergence of autism: 2012;36:1060–1084. for Initiating Positive Interactions and Im-
neuroimaging findings from birth to pre- 18 Courchesne E, Mouton PR, Calhoun ME, Se- proving Learning Opportunities. Baltimore,
school. Neuropsychiatry 2013;3:209–222. mendeferi K, Ahrens-Barbeau C, Hallet MJ, Paul H Brookes, 1995, pp 17–32.
10 Dinstein I, Pierce K, Eyler L, Solso S, Malach Barnes CC, Pierce K: Neuron number and size 25 National Research Council: Educating Chil-
R, Behrmann M, Courchesne E: Disrupted in prefrontal cortex of children with autism. dren with Autism. Washington, DC, The Na-
neural synchronization in toddlers with au- JAMA 2011;306:2001–2010. tional Academies Press, 2001.
tism. Neuron 2011;70:1218–1225.

42 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2017;69:38–42 Petinou/Minaidou


DOI: 10.1159/000479181

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen