Sie sind auf Seite 1von 382

Resilience and Mental Health:

Challenges Across the Lifespan


Resilience and
Mental Health
Challenges Across the Lifespan
Edited by
Steven M. Southwick
Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven

Brett T. Litz
Boston University School of Medicine, Boston

Dennis Charney
The Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York

Matthew J. Friedman
Dartmouth University School of Medicine, White River Junction
c a mb rid g e u n ive r si t y pres s
Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town,
Singapore, São Paulo, Delhi, Tokyo, Mexico City
Cambridge University Press
he Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 8RU, UK

Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York

www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521898393

© Cambridge University Press 2011

his publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception


and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements,
no reproduction of any part may take place without the written
permission of Cambridge University Press.

First published 2011

Printed in the United Kingdom at the University Press, Cambridge

A catalog record for this publication is available from the British Library

Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication data


Resilience and mental health: challenges across the lifespan / [edited by]
Steven M. Southwick ... [et al.].
p. ; cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-0-521-89839-3 (hardback)
1. Mental health. 2. Stress (Psychology) 3. Adaptability (Psychology) I. Southwick, Steven M. II. Title.
[DNLM: 1. Mental Health. 2. Resilience, Psychological. 3. Adaptation, Psychological. 4. Stress Disorders,
Traumatic–prevention & control. 5. Stress, Psychological–psychology. WM 105]
RA790.R447 2011
616.89–dc23 2011016713

ISBN 978-0-521-89839-3 Hardback

Cambridge University Press has no responsibility for the persistence or


accuracy of URLs for external or third-party internet websites referred to
in this publication, and does not guarantee that any content on such
websites is, or will remain, accurate or appropriate.

Every efort has been made in preparing this book to provide accurate and up-to-date information
which is in accord with accepted standards and practice at the time of publication. Although case
histories are drawn from actual cases, every efort has been made to disguise the identities of the
individuals involved. Nevertheless, the authors, editors, and publishers can make no warranties that
the information contained herein is totally free from error, not least because clinical standards are
constantly changing through research and regulation. he authors, editors, and publishers therefore
disclaim all liability for direct or consequential damages resulting from the use of material contained
in this book. Readers are strongly advised to pay careful attention to information provided by the
manufacturer of any drugs or equipment that they plan to use.
Contents
List of contributors page vii
Preface xi

Section 1 Pathways to resilience 1 Section 3 Resilience in families,


Section editor: Brett T. Litz communities, and societies 149
1 Neurobiology of resilience 1 Section editor: Fran H. Norris
Adriana Feder, Dennis Charney, and Kate Collins 10 Family resilience: a collaborative approach in
2 Resilience in the face of stress: emotion response to stressful life challenges 149
regulation as a protective factor 30 Froma Walsh
Allison S. Troy and Iris B. Mauss 11 Community resilience: concepts, assessment,
3 Cognitive factors and resilience: how and implications for intervention 162
self-efficacy contributes to coping with Fran H. Norris, Kathleen Sherrieb, and
adversities 45 Betty Pfeferbaum
Charles C. Benight and Roman Cieslak 12 Trauma, culture, and resiliency 176
4 Personality factors in resilience to Carl C. Bell
traumatic stress 56
Mark W. Miller and Kelly M. Harrington
Section 4 Specific challenges 189
5 Social ties and resilience in chronic Section editor: Matthew J. Friedman
disease 76
Denise Janicki-Deverts and Sheldon Cohen 13 Loss and grief: the role of individual
differences 189
6 Religious and spiritual factors in Anthony D. Mancini and George A. Bonanno
resilience 90
David W. Foy, Kent D. Drescher, and Patricia 14 Reorienting resilience: adapting resilience for
J. Watson post-disaster research 200
Jennifer Johnson and Sandro Galea
15 Rape and other sexual assault 218
Section 2 Resilience across the Heidi S. Resnick, Constance Guille,
lifespan 103 Jenna L. McCauley, and Dean
Section editor: Dennis Charney G. Kilpatrick

7 Resilience in children and adolescents 103 16 The stress continuum model: a military
Ann S. Masten, Amy R. Monn, and organizational approach to resilience and
Laura M. Supkof recovery 238
William P. Nash, Maria Steenkamp,
8 Toward a lifespan approach to resilience and Lauren Conoscenti, and Brett T. Litz
potential trauma 120
George A. Bonanno and Anthony D. Mancini 17 Resilience in the face of terrorism: linking
resource investment with engagement 253
9 Resilience in older adults 135 Stevan E. Hobfoll, Brian Hall, Katie J. Horsey, and
Diane L. Elmore, Lisa M. Brown, and Joan M. Cook Brittain E. Lamoureux

v
Contents

18 Resilience in the context of poverty 264 21 Childhood resilience: adaptation, mastery,


John C. Buckner and Jessica S. Waters and attachment 307
Angie Torres, Steven M. Southwick, and
19 Resiliency in individuals with serious mental
Linda C. Mayes
illness 276
Piper S. Meyer and Kim T. Mueser 22 Military mental health training: building
resilience 323
Carl Andrew Castro and Amy B. Adler
Section 5 Training for resilience 289 23 Public health practice and disaster resilience:
Section editor: Steven M. Southwick
a framework integrating resilience as a worker
20 Interventions to enhance resilience and protection strategy 340
resilience-related constructs in adults 289 Dori B. Reissman, Kathleen M. Kowalski-Trakoler,
Steven M. Southwick, Robert H. Pietrzak, and and Craig L. Katz
Gerald White

Index 359

vi
Contributors

Amy B. Adler Sheldon Cohen


Clinical Research Psychologist and Chief of Science, Director of the Laboratory for the Study of Stress,
US Army Medical Research Unit, Europe, Walter Immunity and Disease, Carnegie Mellon University,
Reed Army Institute of Research, Heidelberg, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Germany
Kate Collins
Carl C. Bell Graduate Assistant, Mount Sinai School of Medicine,
President/CEO, Community Mental Health Council, New York, USA
Inc. and Clinical Professor of Psychiatry and Public
Health, University of Illinois, Chicago, IL, USA Lauren Conoscenti
National Center for PTSD, VA Boston Healthcare
Charles C. Benight System and Boston University, Boston, MA
Department of Psychology and Trauma, Health, and
Hazards Center, University of Colorado, Colorado Joan M. Cook
Springs, CO, USA Assistant Professor, Psychiatry, Yale School of
Medicine and National Center for PTSD, West Haven,
George A. Bonanno CT, USA
Professor of Education and Psychology, Teachers
College, Columbia University, New York, USA Kent D. Drescher
Health Science Specialist, Palo Alto VA Healthcare
Lisa M. Brown System and National Center for PTSD, Menlo Park,
Associate Professor, Department of Aging and Mental CA, USA
Health, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
Diane L. Elmore
John C. Buckner Associate Executive Director, Public Interest
Staf Scientist, Department of Psychiatry, Children’s Government Relations Oice, American
Hospital Boston, Boston, MA, USA Psychological Association, Washington, DC, USA
Carl Andrew Castro Adriana Feder
Director, Military Operational Medicine Research Assistant Professor, Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of
Program, US Army Medical Research and Material Medicine, New York, USA
Command, Fort Detrick, MD, USA
David W. Foy
Dennis Charney Professor, Department of Psychology, GSEP-
Dean and Professor of Psychiatry, Neuroscience and Pepperdine University, Encino, CA, USA
Pharmacology and Systems herapeutics, Mount
Sinai School of Medicine, New York, USA Matthew J. Friedman
Professor of Psychiatry and of Pharmacology and
Roman Cieslak Toxicology and Executive Director of the National
Department of Psychology, Warsaw School of Social Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, US
Psychology and Trauma, Health, and Hazards Center, Department of Veterans Afairs, Dartmouth Medical
University of Colorado, Colorado Springs, CO, USA School, White River Junction, VT, USA

vii
List of contributors

Sandro Galea Brittain E. Lamoureux


Gelman Professor, Department of Epidemiology, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA
Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia
University, New York, USA Brett T. Litz
Massachusetts Veterans Epidemiological Research
Constance Guille and Information Center, National Center for PTSD,
Clinical Instructor of Psychiatry, National Crime VA Boston Healthcare System and Boston University,
Victims Research and Treatment Center, Medical Boston, MA, USA
University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, USA
Anthony D. Mancini
Brian Hall Teachers College, Columbia University, New York,
Research Study Coordinator, Rush Medical College USA
and Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA
Ann S. Masten
Kelly M. Harrington Distinguished McKnight University Professor,
VA Boston Healthcare System, National Center for University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA
PTSD and Division of Psychiatry, Boston University
School of Medicine, Boston, MA, USA Iris B. Mauss
Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology,
Stevan E. Hobfoll University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA
Professor and Chairperson, Department of Behavioral
Sciences, Rush Medical College, Chicago, IL, USA Linda C. Mayes
Chair of the Directorial Team at the Anna Freud
Katie J. Horsey Centre, London and Arnold Gesell Professor of Child
Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA Psychiatry and Psychology, Yale Child Study Center,
New Haven, CT, USA
Denise Janicki-Deverts
Research Fellow, Carnegie Mellon University, Jenna L. McCauley
Pittsburgh, PA, USA Instructor, Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences,
National Crime Victims Research and Treatment
Jennifer Johnson Center, Medical University of South Carolina,
Research Assistant, Center for Global Health, Charleston, SC, USA
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Piper S. Meyer
Craig L. Katz Department of Psychology, University of North
Disaster Psychiatry Outreach and Department Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School of Medicine,
New York, USA Mark W. Miller
VA Boston Healthcare System, National Center for
Dean G. Kilpatrick PTSD and Division of Psychiatry, Boston University
Director, National Crime Victims Research and School of Medicine, Boston, MA, USA
Treatment Center and Vice Chair for Education,
Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina, Amy R. Monn
Charleston, SC, USA Researcher, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis,
MN, USA
Kathleen M. Kowalski-Trakofler
Pittsburgh Research Laboratory, Oice of Mining Kim T. Mueser
Safety and Health Research, National Institute for Departments of Psychiatry and of Community and
Occupational Safety and Health, US Centers for Family Medicine, Dartmouth Psychiatric Research
Disease Control and Prevention, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Center, Concord, NH, USA

viii
List of contributors

William P. Nash Steven M. Southwick


Captain, Medical Corps, US Navy (Retired), VA Professor, Yale University School of Medicine
San Diego Healthcare System, and University of National Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder,
California, San Diego, CA, USA and VA Connecticut Healthcare System, New Haven,
CT, USA
Fran H. Norris
Research Professor of Psychiatry and of Community Maria Steenkamp
and Family Medicine, Department of Veterans Afairs Clinical Psychology, Boston University, Boston, MA,
National Center for PTSD and National Consortium USA
for the Study of Terrorism and Responses to
Terrorism, Dartmouth Medical School, White River Laura M. Supkoff
Junction, VT, USA Researcher, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis,
MN, USA
Betty Pfefferbaum
National Consortium for the Study of Terrorism and Angie Torres
Responses to Terrorism, National Child Traumatic Postdoctoral Fellow, Yale Child Study Center, New
Stress Network of the Terrorism and Disaster Center, Haven, CT, USA
University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center,
Allison S. Troy
Oklahoma City, OK, USA
Department of Psychology, University of Denver,
Robert H. Pietrzak Denver, CO, USA
Assistant Professor of Psychiatry, Yale University
Froma Walsh
School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Firestone Professor Emerita and Co-Director of the
Dori B. Reissman Chicago Center for Family Health, University of
Oice of the Director, National Institute for Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
Occupational Safety and Health, US Centers for
Jessica S. Waters
Disease Control and Prevention and Commissioned
Research Assistant, Department of Psychiatry,
Medical Corps of the US Public Health Service,
Children’s Hospital Boston, Boston, MA, USA
Washington DC, USA
Patricia J. Watson
Heidi S. Resnick
National Center for PTSD and VA Medical and
Professor, National Crime Victims Research and
Regional Oice Center, White River Junction, VT,
Treatment Center, Charleston, SC, USA
USA
Kathleen Sherrieb
Gerald White
Department of Veterans Afairs National Center for
Co-founder, Survivor Corps, Washington, DC, USA
PTSD and National Consortium for the Study of
Terrorism and Responses to Terrorism, Dartmouth
Medical School, White River Junction, VT, USA

ix
Preface

Humans are remarkably resilient in the face of crises, (oten called “recovery”); and functioning well in spite
traumas, disabilities, attachment losses, and ongoing of trauma-related psychopathology (functional resili-
adversities. In fact, resilience to stress and trauma may ence). Clearly these outcome-related deinitions of
be the norm rather than the exception. However, to resilience difer markedly from one another.
date, most research in the ield of traumatic stress has Process-oriented deinitions of resilience include
focused on neurobiological, psychological, and social cognitions, emotional reactions, and behaviors that are
factors associated with trauma-related psychopath- adaptive in response to stress and trauma. For example,
ology and deicits in psychosocial functioning. While active coping and seeking social support have been
much has been learned in these areas of research, par- described as resilient processes that facilitate resist-
ticularly about post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), ance to, or recovery from, traumatic stressors. Other
far less is known about resilience to stress and healthy processes shown to be adaptive in relation to stress and
adaptation to stress and trauma. trauma include the capacity to generate positive emo-
he study of resilience is enormously challenging. tions, to accept that which cannot be changed, and to
he irst hurdle involves deinition. Currently, there reframe the negative into positive.
is no single agreed-upon deinition of resilience in Resilience is multidimensional in nature. Conse-
the clinical or scientiic literature. In a review of the quently, people who are faced with adversity can
published literature on risk, vulnerability, resistance, exhibit competence in some domains but not others.
and resilience, Layne and colleagues (2007) described For example, traumatized adults may demonstrate
the lack of precision and numerous terminological resilience in areas such as work but falter in other areas
inconsistencies in the meanings of these concepts, such as family relationships. Typically at-risk or resili-
and identiied at least eight distinct meanings for the ent individuals display an unevenness of functioning
term “resilience.” For example, deinitions of resilience across domains. As noted by Masten and colleagues
have ranged from symptom-free functioning follow- (Chapter 7), it is unrealistic to expect that anyone, no
ing trauma exposure (Bonanno et al., 2006) to positive matter how resilient, will consistently perform at a uni-
adaptation despite adversity (Garmezy, 1993), and even formly high or low level across all areas of their life.
to enhanced psychobiological regulation of stress/fear- Consequently, some researchers deine resilience as
related brain circuitry, neurotransmitters, and hor- excellence in one salient domain with at least average
mones (Charney, 2004). he American Psychological adjustment in other domains.
Association (2010) has deined resilience as “the pro- In some cases resilience and related constructs have
cess of adapting well in the face of adversity, trauma, been viewed as enduring because they are believed to be
tragedy, threats or even signiicant sources of threat.” present even when the individual is not challenged by
Further complicating the study of resilience is the stress. In other cases, factors associated with resilience
notion that resilience can be conceptualized as an out- have been referred to as risk-activated moderating fac-
come in the face of stress and trauma or as a process that tors because they only become fully activated when the
mediates the response to stress and trauma. Examples individual is challenged. Masten ofers the analogy of
of resilience as an outcome include symptom-free an automobile airbag that inlates during a collision but
functioning (deined as “resistance” by some research- not during everyday driving. For example, some forms
ers); not meeting criteria for PTSD following traumatic of social support, such as assistance by emergency per-
exposure; developing symptoms of trauma-related psy- sonnel and social services, may only become activated
chopathology, but recovering from those symptoms in the immediate atermath of a traumatic event.

xi
Preface

In addition to being multidimensional, resilience is stress. he chapters, which are organized into ive sec-
dynamic rather than static. Resilience trajectories may tions, summarize the current literature on the adaptive
be uneven, with some people demonstrating resilience responses to stress from various relevant ields and
at one age but not another, or in one circumstance but domains.
not another. Some researchers have conceptualized Section 1 introduces the reader to state of the art
resilience as one of a set of possible trajectories follow- advances in theory and empirical research on pathways
ing severe stress or trauma. For example, Norris and to resilience, approaching this discussion from multiple
colleagues (2009) analyzed two population-based lon- perspectives. Decades of research on mental disorders,
gitudinal datasets that were collected ater the terror psychosomatic disease, and abnormalities in social
attacks of September 11, 2001 and the 1999 loods in behavior have led to advances in our understanding of
Mexico and found ive diferent symptom trajectories the biological, psychological, and social processes and
among study participants. hey speciically diferenti- mechanisms that are associated with enduring distress
ated what they called resilience (i.e., initial moderate or and malfunction in the face of stress and trauma. he
severe symptoms followed by a sharp decrease) from ultimate value of the study of pathways to resilience is
resistance (i.e., no symptoms or mild and stable symp- to develop universal, selective, and targeted programs
toms) and recovery (i.e., initially moderate or severe to prevent damaging responses to stress and adversity
symptoms followed by a gradual decrease). in at-risk groups.
Resilient processes are not strictly intrapsychic and he pathways to resilience and positive outcomes
biological; rather they relect the transaction or inter- ater exposure to stress are multidimensional and
action between the person and his or her environment. multisystemic and typically require complex multi-
For example, stressful events are processed through variate modeling. he study of human resilience also
perception and subjective appraisal, which, in turn, requires an interdisciplinary approach, which has
moderates impact. he subsequent response (e.g., a been rare in this relatively young ield. Nevertheless,
coping behavior) to the stressor is shaped and con- the chapters in this section will help the reader to
strained by environmental (e.g., social supports) as well appreciate the state of the art advances in theory
as personal (e.g., biological) factors and resources. he and empirical research pertaining to diferent per-
individual who adapts to stress does not do so in iso- spectives of pathways to resilience, which is a good
lation but rather in the context of available resources, starting place toward the goal of developing a more
other human beings, families, speciic cultures and paradigmatic and interdisciplinary approach. he
religions, organizations, and communities and soci- chapters review the full range of salutogenic factors
eties, all of which adapt to challenges as well. that moderate and mediate the relationship between
Stresses and losses can hamper or reduce resources exposure to acute stress, trauma, and chronic adver-
that would otherwise be employed to manage and sity and the multidimensional outcomes (biological,
recover from stressors. For example, death of a loved spiritual, social, behavioral, and cognitive). hese
one can greatly constrain social and relational resources include neurobehavioral and neurohormonal factors,
when those providing solace and support to others social and interpersonal variables, spiritual practices,
must also cope with his/her own grief and loss. Stress social cognition, emotion regulation strategies, and
can also adversely afect sleep and wellness behaviors personality variables.
that are needed to restore biological, psychological, Section 2 examines developmental determinants
and functional capacities. of resilience across the lifespan, from infancy to old
Readers should bear in mind that the type and age. Although there are both conceptual diferences
degree of stress and trauma typically have a marked and similarities in the concept of resilience across the
impact on resilience processes and outcomes. At the lifespan, there is little research comparing responses to
extreme end of stress and trauma, even the best trained traumatic stress as a function of development. What
elite athletes, professionals, and warriors have a limit information that does exist suggests that children cope
beyond which they can no longer function adequately, as well as, if not better than, adults. Furthermore, fac-
at least for a period of time. tors such as attachment relationships, social support,
his edited textbook on resilience has brought religion, intelligence and problem-solving ability, and
together experts from a broad array of scientiic ields cognitive lexibility promote resilience in both chil-
whose research has focused on adaptive responses to dren and adults.

xii
Preface

Scientists interested in human resilience are as well as family resilience, is inextricably linked to the
beginning to study much larger systems, at the level community’s ability to prepare for, respond to, and adapt
of society, culture, and government to enhance adap- to adverse conditions. Furthermore, just as individuals
tive responses to traumas, such as natural disasters and families are embedded in communities, so too are
or pandemics, where many systems are involved that communities embedded in larger systems of inluence.
impact resilience in individuals, families, and commu- Among the most important of these is culture. Recovery
nities. However, a great deal of work remains to fully from trauma involves the reconstruction of meaning,
understand how resilience can be facilitated through- and constructs of meaning are inseparable from cul-
out life. Further investigation is needed to determine ture. Culture exerts a strong inluence on the multitude
the best methods for fostering resilience in children, of protective factors discussed throughout this text,
adults, and the elderly. Resilience is multifaceted and including personality, optimism, cognitive styles and
the interplay needs to be better understood among a attributions, worldview, social support, beliefs about
number of variables including type and severity of illness and health, and healing practices.
trauma, genetic predisposition, psychosocial context, he chapters in Section 4 focus on challenges
personality, social support and relationships, family, to resilience when dealing with speciic adversities,
community, and culture. Ultimately, the goal is to iden- including loss and grief, disasters, rape and assault,
tify a personalized roadmap to help people of all ages to combat, terrorism, poverty, and chronic mental illness.
maintain and enhance resilience and experience per- Previous sections of this book have addressed general
sonal growth in the face of the challenges of daily life, cross-cutting attributes of resilience that would be
stressful life events, and major traumas. applicable to almost any situation. In addition, how-
Section 3 describes the impact of social context, in ever, there are speciic skill sets that may be useful in
the form of family, community and society, on adap- some contexts, but not in others. In this section, the
tation to adversity. Since the earliest days of research reader learns how diferent challenges demand more
on human resilience, it has been understood that indi- speciic manifestations of resilience. In short, the dif-
viduals are embedded in social networks that can aug- ferent challenges posed by bereavement, disasters, rape,
ment or undermine their capacity to thrive in the face combat, terrorism, poverty, and chronic mental illness
of adversity. he resilience of children, for example, appear to require diferent, and more context-speciic,
depends highly on the strength of their bonds with expressions of resilience. For example, as with service
parents and others who nurture their development. men and women returning from Iraq and Afghanistan,
Ecologies of human behavior are oten portrayed as what constitutes adaptive and resilient behavior in a
nested systems that exert increasingly complex lay- war zone may be dysfunctional or disruptive in a post-
ers of inluence (Bronfenbrenner, 1977). Microsystems deployment, marital, or domestic context. Resilience
are the primary settings, such as families, schools, and following bereavement or rape may also be diferent, in
places of work; mesosystems are intersecting microsys- some respects, than resilience in coping with poverty
tems; and macrosystems are the matrices of rules, laws, or chronic mental illness.
and cultural norms that shape the nature of society’s In Section 4 the reader learns that even in the con-
political, economic, legal, and educational systems. text of painful loss, the bereaved retain a capacity for
Together capturing such an ecological perspective generative experiences, positive emotions, and the
on resilience, the three chapters in Section 3 summarize capacity to derive comfort from others while overcom-
current knowledge about the inluences of family, com- ing loss. Section 4 also considers that resilience, in the
munity, and culture on individual well-being. Beyond face of disasters, is expressed at the individual, family,
this, these chapters explore the resilience of families and and community level, and that resilience evaluations
societies themselves. he family, for example, is more need to accommodate these various contexts, ideally
than just a resource for individual resilience but is itself synergistically and comprehensively.
a functional unit that may exhibit resilience as a whole. he challenges associated with coming to terms
Varied and evolving family structures and life courses with sexual assault are enormous. Section 4 describes
create challenges and opportunities for resilience. the advantages of using approach-oriented coping
In the face of collective stressors, such as disas- strategies for this group of survivors, and points out
ters, survivors are connected and dependent upon one that these strategies are at the core of early interven-
another’s coping strategies, and individual resilience, tions designed to promote recovery and resilience.

xiii
Preface

Terrorism presents yet another special challenge to the adults who adapt well in the face of adversity, to date,
modern world. Resilient responses or variables that there have been relatively few methodologically rigor-
indicate successful navigation of these challenges are ous investigations of actual strategies and techniques
discussed in the context of conservation of resources and designed to enhance resilience.
engagement theory, where resilience is characterized by Some resilience-enhancing interventions have been
“engagement” (i.e., a persistent, pervasive, and positive developed to strengthen coping skills in stress- and
afect-motivational state), “dedication” (i.e., a commit- trauma-exposed individuals, both with and without
ment to key life tasks), “absorption” (i.e., the sense of psychiatric disorders. Others focus on preventing the
full involvement and excitement over life tasks), and development of stress- and trauma-related morbidity.
“vigor” (i.e., high levels of energy and mental resilience While most resilience-enhancing programs have been
when meeting life challenge). Resilience in the face of designed for speciic at-risk populations (e.g., ireight-
combat and operational experiences during war also ers, police, military personnel), some are more general
requires a unique vantage point and conceptual frame- in nature. Other relevant interventions include those
work, where operational resilience (e.g., successful that have not speciically been designed to enhance
performance, mission readiness) needs to be distin- resilience but that enhance aspects of functioning, such
guished from psychological resilience. as optimism and social support, which are known to be
Resilience in the context of poverty, which is an associated with resilience.
enduring source of adversity, powerlessness, stress, and Children, in general, are remarkably resilient. heir
limited opportunities for personal and social advance- resilience develops as a set of abilities and processes
ment is both complex and understudied. In Section including positive attachment, the capacity to attract
4, the reader learns that poverty should be studied at social support, self-motivating rewards critical for
the individual and community level. For individuals, mastery and self-eicacy, efective modulation of the
resilience is understood as self-regulation and adaptive stress response, and successful monitoring and regula-
lexibility in the service of goal-directed behavior. At tion of emotions. hese abilities and processes form the
the community level, this translates into social cohe- foundation for resilience in adulthood. As with adults,
sion and collective eicacy. stress tends to enhance resilience so long as the stress
Finally, Section 4 discusses methods that may be is manageable and not overwhelming. Interventions
used to mobilize resilience and optimize functional designed to enhance resilience must address capacities
capacity among individuals with chronic mental illness. and skills that are appropriate to the physical, emo-
Using a recovery model, which eschews the traditional tional, and cognitive maturity of the child.
psychopathology model, the focus of these methods is he speciic approach to resilience training oten
on improving well-being through empowerment and depends on anticipated stressors. Consequently, resili-
enhancement of autonomous function. ence training for a prospective ireman will difer from
As these chapters attest, although resilience con- training for a prospective humanitarian worker. For
sists of general attributes that serve individuals well in example, the US Army Battlemind Training System,
almost any context, there are also very speciic expres- a mental health resilience-building program, is spe-
sions of resilience that are context dependent and that ciically designed to prepare service members for the
may require diferent sets of adaptive capacity. Mental mental challenges of training, operations, combat, and
toughening and combat readiness for a marine may transitioning home. It addresses military-speciic con-
require diferent biopsychosocial capabilities than the cerns about relevance, practicality, utility, user accept-
coping strategies needed by a recently raped individual, ability, and stigma.
than acquisition of social capital by an impoverished When evaluating public health practices to
inner city youth, or than the capacity to generate posi- enhance disaster resilience, it is important to adopt
tive emotions by a homeless individual with chronic an ecological framework that addresses interdepend-
schizophrenia. encies between people and the contexts in which they
he inal section brings together what is currently respond. herefore, it is useful to consider a wide range
known about enhancing resilience and includes chap- of interventions including engineering design; safety
ters speciically devoted to children, military members, codes and standards; legislation; land use management;
and disaster workers. While much is known about cog- interagency planning and coordination; public edu-
nitive styles, emotions, and behaviors of children and cation; leadership training; worker training; medical,

xiv
Preface

emotional, and cognitive readiness; regulation of oper- just as resilience continually changes and adapts for
ational tempo and degree of worker exposure; as well as each individual and context, the study of resilience is
close monitoring of medical and psychological status continually changing and improving as scientists and
of those who have been exposed to the disaster. researchers learn more.
In future eforts to develop efective interventions
to enhance resilience, it will be important to address References
a number of critical questions. Is the intervention
American Psychological Association (2010). he road to
designed to be administered prior to stress and trauma resilience. Washington, DC: American Psychological
exposure as a means to prevent trauma-related morbid- Association, http://www.apa.org/helpcenter/road-
ity or is it designed to enhance resilience in survivors resilience.aspx (accessed June 25, 2010).
of trauma? Is the intervention designed for a speciic Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D.
population and a speciic type of stressor? What skills (2006). Psychological resilience ater disaster: New York
and strengths will be targeted for development by the City in the atermath of the September 11th terrorist
intervention? Is there suicient scientiic evidence to attack. Psychological Science, 17, 181–186.
support the association between these skills and resili- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of
ence, and how will mastery of these skills be assessed? human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531.
Will the intervention be most efective using a class- Charney, D. S. (2004). Psychobiological mechanisms of
room-based model or a scenario-based model? What resilience and vulnerability: implications for successful
are the most appropriate control groups? What are the adaptation to extreme stress. American Journal of
most relevant outcome measures for the intervention? Psychiatry, 161, 195–216.
hese are just some of the critical issues that will need Garmezy, N. (1993). Children in poverty: resilience despite
to be addressed in order to further advance interven- risk. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 56,
tions designed to enhance resilience. 127–136.
In summary, each of the ive sections in this edited Layne, C. M., Warren, J. S., Watson, P. J., & Shalev, A. Y.
textbook examine adaptive responses to trauma, (2007). Risk, vulnerability, resistance, and resilience:
spanning from factors that contribute to and pro- toward an integrative conceptualization of posttraumatic
adaptation. In T. K. M. Friedman & P. Resick (eds.),
mote resilience, to populations and societal systems in Handbook of PTSD: Science and practice (pp. 497–520).
which resilience is employed, to speciic applications New York: Guilford Press.
and contexts of resilience, and interventions designed
Norris, F. H., Tracy, M., & Galea, S. (2009). Looking for
to better enhance resilience. he reader is reminded resilience: understanding the longitudinal trajectories
that this textbook aims to review relevant concepts of responses to stress. Social Science and Medicine, 68,
pertaining to adaptive responses to trauma, but that 2190–2198.

xv
Section1
Section Pathways to resilience
Chapter
Neurobiology of resilience

1 Adriana Feder, Dennis Charney, and Kate Collins

Introduction researchers have had diiculty operationalizing resili-


ence on a neurobiological level. Some neuroscientists
Resilience is commonly conceptualized as the abil-
have focused on the capacity to experience stress with-
ity to adapt and thrive despite experiencing adversity
out developing mental illness (Conrad & Hammen,
(Masten et al., 1995; Elder, 1998; Masten & Coatsworth,
1989; Tiet et al., 1998; New et al., 2009); others have
1998). A resilient individual has been tested (Rutter,
placed less emphasis on the development of psychiatric
2006) and continues to demonstrate healthy psycho-
symptoms and, instead, focus on the ability to recover
logical and physiological stress responses (McEwen,
from a mental illness with (or without) treatment
2003; Charney, 2004). For most, it is possible to con-
(Nitschke et al., 2009).
jure an image of such a person: a woman who chooses
We believe that a diferent archetype will be neces-
to work with sexual assault survivors ater she herself is
sary as neuroscience progresses and scientists come to
raped; a child growing up in poverty who earns a schol-
deine neural health with increasingly accurate bio-
arship to college; a hurricane survivor who rebuilds her
logical assays, and become less dependent on behav-
own home and helps to revitalize her community.
ioral measures. In order to articulate the formulation
For over three decades, scientists have worked to
of resilience employed in this chapter, we return to its
identify the states and traits characteristic of resilience,
original meaning as deined by physicists: the capabil-
with the aim of developing more efective and more
ity of an object to resume its original size and shape
diverse evidence-based prophylactic and treatment
ater deformation. While maintaining a clear parallel
interventions to combat the deleterious impact of stress
with the deinition of resilience utilized in psycho-
on human body and brain. While the value of under-
social investigation, this signiication is more easily
standing the neurobiological substrates of resilience
translated into a neurobiological model: acute adapta-
has always been appreciated, the lack of tools available
tions in neural systems in response to stress constitute
to assess the integrity of neural structure and function
deformation, and the ability of those systems to resume
has impeded progress. Hence, early research on resili-
optimal or pre-stress operations is resilience.
ence focused on illuminating the psychological and
social determinants of stress resistance (Rutter, 1985;
Masten & Coatsworth, 1998; Masten, 2001; Bonanno, Allostasis and resilience
2004). Homeostasis is the maintenance of the small set of
While recent advances in scientiic technology have physiological states that must be rigidly preserved to
made the exploration of biological processes associ- ensure survival. Even minute changes in variables such
ated with resilient phenotypes more feasible, it remains as temperature, osmolarity, pH, or oxygen tension
constrained by reliance on behavioral observations and will result in death. In contrast, a multitude of other
self-report to identify resilient individuals. Because parameters can be, and oten are, altered. Such modii-
we have very few direct measures of neural health, a cations constitute the body’s eforts to respond to and
person’s descriptions of his or her internal experience counteract stressors that could disturb homeostasis.
and the degree of functional impairment observed Sterling and Eyer (1988) introduced the term allosta-
must serve as its proxy. Hence, it is not surprising that sis to describe this dynamic regulation of secondary

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

1
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Table 1.1 Key terminology.

Term Deinition
Homeostasis Maintenance of those states essential to survival
(pH, osmolarity, temperature, oxygen tension, etc.)
Stress Stimulus or stimuli that threaten homeostasis
Allostasis Dynamic regulation of non-essential set-points in response to stress in order to preserve critical
variables
Allostatic load The damage the body incurs as a result of allostasis
Resilience The degree to which the body is able to minimize allostatic load

set-points in defense of homeostasis. Subsequently, his, in turn, stimulates increases in the production
McEwen and Stellar (1993) observed that, while essen- and release of the glucocorticoids cortisol and dehy-
tial to survival, any allostatic deviation from the opti- droepiandrosterone (DHEA) from the adrenal gland
mal internal milieu takes a toll on the body. hey coined (Rosenfeld et al., 1971). he glucocorticoids travel
the phrase “allostatic load” to refer to the physiological throughout the body inciting the physiological changes
cost of adaptation to stressors. In this context, the required to cope with the stressor.
severity of the allostatic load, as a measure of the brain’s
inability to resume its pre-allostatic state, is inversely Allostatic contribution
proportional to the degree of resilience. hat is, Cortisol promotes arousal, attention, and memory for-
resilience is the capacity to minimize allostatic load. mation and increases blood glucose and blood pressure,
Table 1.1 summarizes these deinitions. while suppressing growth, reproductive, and immune
In this chapter, we begin by reviewing the systems processes. By comparison, DHEA, its sulfated deriva-
most directly involved in the acute stress response, high- tive (DHEA-S), and its metabolites have antiglucocorti-
lighting both their contribution to allostasis and their coid and antiglutamatergic properties in several tissues,
allostatic load. he genetic, physiological, psychological, including the brain (Browne et al., 1992), and appear
and environmental factors that appear to contain allo- to support memory and cognition (Rose et al., 1997).
static load and, therefore, promote resilience are also hey deter corticosteroid-induced hippocampal neuro-
delineated. Subsequently, we describe other variables toxicity by interfering with the glucocorticoid receptor
whose allostatic role is less clear but are known to modu- (GR) uptake in the hippocampus (Kimonides et al., 1998;
late the intensity and eicacy of the acute processes and, Bastianetto et al., 1999; Morin & Starka, 2001), and they
therefore, to inluence resilience. We conclude with a amplify long-term potentiation of hippocampal neu-
discussion of emerging integrated models of resilience rons, likely by modulating transmission at the N-methyl-
and the potential applications of this work. d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor (Chen et al., 2006).

Acute stress-response systems Allostatic load


Chronic excessive cortisol exposure is thought to
The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis increase vulnerability to hypertension, immunosup-
he hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, pression, osteoporosis, insulin resistance, truncal
composed of the hypothalamus, pituitary, and adrenal obesity, dyslipidemia, dyscoagulation, atherosclerosis,
glands, is the primary mechanism responsible for cardiovascular disease (Whitworth et al., 2005), anxiety,
coordinating the body’s response to stress. When a and depressive disorders (Carroll et al., 2007). A substan-
stimulus is perceived as threatening, the paraventricu- tial body of literature exploring cortisol dysregulation in
lar nucleus of the hypothalamus releases corticotropin- major depressive disorder (MDD) and post-traumatic
releasing hormone (CRH) and arginine-vasopressin stress disorder (PTSD) has emerged in the past few
into the portal vessels. hese subsequently trigger the decades (reviewed by Handwerger, 2009). Handwerger
production and secretion of adrenocorticotropic hor- concluded that the available literature links the develop-
mone (ACTH) from the anterior lobe of the pituitary. ment of PTSD ater single-trauma exposure to chronic

2
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

(if not acute) reductions in basal cortisol levels, relative axis is predictive of the degree of resilience (de Kloet
suppression of the cortisol response in the low-dose et al., 2005). hat is, its successful acute activation
dexamethasone suppression test, and heightened cor- when triggered by a threat, and subsequent and timely
tisol responses in anticipation of, and in response to, deactivation when danger has passed, are adaptive and
stress. hese indings support a hypothesis that the GRs promote physical and mental health. Conversely, both
of the HPA axis are hypersensitive in single-trauma hypo- and hyperactive HPA axis activity are associated
PTSD. However, as Handwerger (2009), notes, patients with psychological and physical illness. Hence, factors
with PTSD and histories of chronic or multiple traumas that enhance the functioning of the feedback mecha-
such as child abuse oten display diferent patterns of nisms that regulate the axis are likely to promote resili-
cortisol dysregulation than either healthy volunteers or ent phenotypes.
individuals with PTSD precipitated by a single adult-
hood trauma. She posits that this mediator may account Single nucleotide polymorphisms
for conlicting indings in the literature (e.g., Baker et al., Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) that inlu-
2005; Inslicht et al., 2006; Wheler et al., 2006). ence HPA reactivity have been identiied in the genes
In addition to the exact impact of trauma history, coding for the mineralocorticoid receptor (MR),
what remains unclear is the timeline of the onset of cor- GR, gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) A receptor,
tisol dysregulation observed in patients with PTSD. Is α-adrenoceptor (Masten et al., 1995), μ-opioid recep-
it a pre-existing risk factor or does it emerge only in the tor, as well as the serotonin transporter, catechol-
atermath of the trauma? Studies showing that gluco- O-methyltransferase (COMT), monoamine oxidase
corticoid administration inhibits traumatic memory A, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and
formation (Bierer et al., 2006) support a vulnerabil- angiotensin-converting enzyme. Comprehensive
ity model. For example, patients undergoing surgery reviews are available elsewhere (Derijk & de Kloet,
and/or hospitalization in the intensive care unit who 2008; Derijk, 2009) but many SNPs will be briely dis-
are pre-treated with stress doses of glucocorticoids are cussed in this chapter.
less likely to have traumatic memories of their hospital
stay ater discharge than patients treated with placebo Ratio of dehydroepiandrosterone to cortisol
(Brunner et al., 2006; Schelling et al., 2006; Weis et al., he ratio of DHEA and its derivatives to cortisol has
2006). In contrast, studies showing that individuals proved to be predictive of adaptive responses to stress
with elevated cortisol at the time of trauma are more in multiple populations (Morgan et al., 2004). he
likely to be subsequently diagnosed with PTSD could ratio of DHEA-S to cortisol was positively correlated
be cited as evidence of a post-hoc model (Baker et al., with performance during rigorous survival training
2005; Inslicht et al., 2006). (Morgan et al., 2004) and negatively correlated with
Handwerger’s (2009) review also concluded that dissociative symptoms (Korte et al., 2005). his ratio is
40–60% of adults with MDD display hypercortisolemia also negatively correlated with the severity of negative
and non-suppression in the dexamethasone suppres- mood symptoms in women with PTSD (Rasmusson
sion test. his pattern may be even more common in et al., 2004). Secretion of DHEA in response to ACTH
psychotic than non-psychotic depression. In contrast, injections in these same women was highest in those
when changes in cortisol levels following exposure to reporting the mildest symptoms (Rasmusson et al.,
acute stressors are assessed, no clear pattern distin- 2004). Higher DHEA plasma levels have been posi-
guishes patients with MDD from controls. A recent tively correlated with improvement and efective
study with a very large sample (1588) also documented coping in an investigation of veterans being treated
statistically signiicant morning basal cortisol elevation for PTSD (Morgan et al., 2004; Yehuda et al., 2006a)
in patients with present or a history of MDD but failed and negatively correlated with depressive symptoms
to diferentiate between those with MDD and healthy (Goodyer et al., 1998; Gallagher & Young, 2002; Young
volunteers in cortisol response to the dexamethasone et al., 2002).
suppression test (Vreeburg et al., 2009). he inverse relationships observed between DHEA
levels and psychopathology prompted researchers to
Factors promoting resilience test its viability as a treatment. Use of DHEA outper-
Several animal models and some investigations in formed placebo in 145 patients with human immuno-
humans suggest that the relative eiciency of the HPA deiciency/acquired immunodeiciency syndrome

3
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

(HIV/AIDS) being treated for MDD (Rabkin et al., striatum, and only CRH-2 in the choroid plexus, certain
2006): 51% of patients randomized to DHEA reported hypothalamic nuclei, the nucleus prepositus, and the bed
symptom relief in comparison to only 31% of those nucleus of the stria terminalis (Sanchez et al., 1999). Mice
assigned to the placebo group when assessed with the bred to be deicient in either CRH-1 or CRH-2 display
Hamilton Depression Rating Scale and the Clinical contradictory behavioral proiles when exposed to stress.
Global Impression Scale. he CRH-1-deicient mice demonstrate less anxiety-like
behavior and fail to mount an adequate stress response in
Integrity of feedback mediated by glucocorticoid and comparison with controls, while CRH-2-deicient mice
mineralocorticoid receptors evince more anxiety-like behavior and are hypersensi-
Functional variants have been identiied in humans of tive to stress (Bale et al., 2000; Coste et al., 2000). Trials
the genes for brain MRs and GRs, which are involved, of CRH-1 antagonists indicate that this receptor acti-
respectively, in setting the threshold and in regulat- vates the behavioral, endocrine, and visceral responses
ing the termination of the HPA axis response to stress to stress; in contrast, CRH-2 generally serves to dampen
(de Kloet et al., 2007). For example, carriers of the these efects (Tache & Bonaz, 2007). However, CRH-2
N363S variant of GR were shown to exhibit higher does have some anxiogenic efects; it appears to enhance
cortisol responses to the Trier Social Stress Test, a the CRH-1-mediated suppression of feeding behavior,
stress-inducing public speaking and mental arith- for example, and to augment stress-induced behaviors
metic task (Krishnan et al., 2007). Interestingly, four via action in the lateral septum of the amygdala (Bakshi
SNPs of FKBP5 (rs9296158, rs3800373, rs1360780, et al., 2002). What is more, stress appears to modify CRH
and rs9470080), a gene coding for a “chaperone” pro- receptor expression in the rodent dorsal raphe nucleus,
tein that regulates GR sensitivity, were found to inter- increasing the CRH-2 and decreasing the CRH-1 avail-
act with severity of childhood abuse in the prediction ability (Waselus et al., 2009). herefore, additional
of PTSD symptoms in adults (Bradley et al., 2008). research will be required to illuminate the complex inter-
Another study demonstrated an association between actions between these receptors.
genetic variation in FKBP5 and ineicient recovery of
HPA axis activity ater the Trier Social Stress Test in Allostatic load
healthy participants, identifying a potential risk factor As described above, CRH-1 signaling appears to be pri-
for chronically elevated cortisol levels and, ultimately, marily anxiogenic. Exposure to excessive CRH during
stress-related psychopathology (Derijk & de Kloet, development appears to have a lasting and deleterious
2008). impact on mental and social health. In adults, increased
levels of CRH in cerebrospinal luid have been linked
to PTSD and major depression (Bremner et al., 1997;
Extrahypothalamic corticotropin-releasing Baker et al., 1999; Nemerof, 2002). he data from
hormone studies measuring plasma CRH levels in patients with
PTSD are mixed (Voisey et al., 2009).
Allostatic contribution
In addition to its role in the HPA pathway, CRH also Factors promoting resilience
contributes to the inhibition of a variety of neuroveg- Early life stress has been linked to chronically high
etative functions, such as food intake, sexual activ- levels of CRH in human and animal studies (Heim &
ity, growth, and reproduction. Increased activity of Nemerof, 2001), providing yet more evidence that
extrahypothalamic CRH-containing neurons in the environmental stability during childhood equips a
amygdala appears to activate fear-related behaviors, while body to resist damage when stressed. Yet genetic fac-
those in the cortex may reduce reward expectation. tors appear to mediate the impact of childhood trauma
Two G-protein-coupled CRH receptors have been on CRH system integrity. A recent study in two inde-
characterized, CRH-1 and CRH-2. Both are expressed pendent populations found that polymorphisms and
in the pituitary, the hippocampus, the amygdala, and haplotypes of the gene for CRH (e.g., a haplotype
throughout the neocortex (with greater density in the formed by three SNPs in intron 1) afected the inlu-
prefrontal, cingulate, striate, and insular cortices). ence of child abuse on depressive symptoms in adult-
Only CRH-1 has been detected in the locus coeruleus, hood, with certain alleles (rs7209436, rs242940) and
the nucleus of the solitary tract, the thalamus, and the haplotypes exerting a protective efect (Berton et al.,

4
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

2006). In addition, scientists were able to reverse social memory prevents the process of memory reconsolida-
impairments associated with developmental exposure tion and appears to permanently impair that memory
to high levels of CRH by administering a CRH antag- (Debiec & LeDoux, 2006).
onist to the dorsal raphe of rodents (Lukkes et al.,
2009), suggesting that these compounds may be efect- Allostatic load
ive treatments for people sufering from social anxiety Although locus coeruleus activity is important for
stemming from early adverse experiences. mounting an efective stress response, hyperactivity or
sustained activity of the locus coeruleus is likely dele-
terious and is associated with depression, anxiety dis-
Monoamines orders, fear, intrusive memories, and an increased risk
Monoamines (norepinephrine [NE], serotonin [5-HT], of hypertension and cardiovascular disease (Charney
and dopamine) were the original neuromodulators et al., 1987, 1992; Southwick et al., 1997; Wong et al.,
implicated in emotion regulation. Hence, it is hardly 2000; Geracioti et al., 2001). For example, both plasma
surprising that they have been identiied as players in and cerebrospinal luid levels of NE were found to be
allostasis. elevated in patients with PTSD (Yehuda et al., 1995;
Baker et al., 1997).
Norepinephrine he neuron response in the locus coeruleus to
he locus coeruleus is co-activated with the HPA axis stress-related stimuli is mediated in part by α2-
in response to stress. his is a nucleus located within adrenoceptors, which are inhibitory autoreceptors
the dorsal wall of the rostral pons in the lateral loor of localized to the cell bodies of locus coeruleus neurons.
the fourth ventricle, and the primary site of NE gen- hese α2-adrenoceptors serve an important role in pro-
esis in the brain. Having the hypothalamus, amygdala, viding negative feedback and containment of the NE
and prefrontal cortex as both aferents and eferents, response to stress. Antagonism of α2-adrenoceptors
the locus coeruleus plays a key role in regulating emo- with idazoxan or yohimbine increases the response
tional and physiological reactions to stimuli. of locus coeruleus neurons to excitatory stimuli with-
out altering their baseline iring rate (Simson & Weiss,
Allostatic contribution 1988). Neumeister and colleagues (2005) found that
When provoked by stress, the locus coeruleus con- humans homozygous for a polymorphism that com-
tributes to the excitation of the HPA axis and the promises the integrity of α2-adrenoceptor function had
sympathetic nervous system, while inhibiting the higher levels of NE at rest and more sustained increases
parasympathetic nervous system, resulting in a state in NE, heart rate, and anxiety in response to a yohim-
of arousal. Further inhibition of the prefrontal cortex bine challenge compared with non-carriers.
allows instinctual “ight or light” behaviors to dom- Exposure to stressors from which the animal can-
inate, unchecked by the more nuanced cognitions of not escape results in behavioral deicits termed learned
the forebrain (Charney & Bremner, 1999). Researchers helplessness. he learned helplessness state is regarded
recently demonstrated that NE levels in the prefrontal as an animal model of MDD and is associated with
cortex also, in part, determine the dopaminergic depletion of NE (Bremner et al., 1996). he depletion
response to stress. Higher levels of NE appear to favor of NE during exposure to an inescapable stressor may
dominance of mesolimbic dopamine signaling and function as a blockade of α2-adrenoceptors and pre-
active coping, lower levels of NE leading to mesocorti- cipitate hypersensitization of locus coeruleus neurons
cal dopamine signaling and passive coping. to stimuli (Simson & Weiss, 1988).
he activation of the HPA and locus coeruleus–NE
systems under acute stress also facilitates the encoding Factors promoting resilience
and relay of aversively charged emotional memories, Variables that promote eiciency and/or prevent
beginning at the amygdala. Animal studies have shown hypersensitivity or chronic activation of the NE sys-
that injections of NE into the amygdala enhance mem- tem would likely result in resilience. For example, the
ory consolidation; in contrast, blocking NE activity levels of COMT, which degrades NE and dopamine,
during stress impedes the encoding of fearful memor- would inluence the quality and duration of NE activa-
ies. In rats, blocking the lateral nucleus of the amyg- tion. Polymorphisms in the gene that codes for COMT
dala to the efects of NE during reactivation of a fearful have been identiied and found to afect cognition and

5
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

anxiety levels. Individuals with low functioning COMT Allostatic contribution


variants (hence, higher levels of circulating NE) tend to Such investigations of 5-HT-impaired subjects have
have better attention and working memory, but higher demonstrated that 5-HT tempers physiological respon-
anxiety (Heinz & Smolka, 2006). Individuals with low sivity to threats in part via modulation of amygdala
functioning COMT also exhibit higher neural respon- and prefrontal activation (Inoue et al., 1993; Cools
siveness in the limbic system and visuospatial attention et al., 2005; Heinz et al., 2005; Pezawas et al., 2005).
system when presented with unpleasant stimuli, sug- Stress exposure triggers more activity in those regions
gesting that they have heightened reactivity to poten- in 5-HT-impaired individuals than in controls (Cools
tial threats (Smolka et al., 2005). et al., 2008). Furthermore, multiple studies in humans
here is also evidence linking severe stress in early and animals have revealed a negative association
life to hyperfunctioning of the locus coeruleus–NE between tonic 5-HT levels and negative bias (reviewed
system in adulthood. In one study of police recruits, by Cools et al., 2008). hat is, 5-HT-impaired individ-
participants watched aversive videos and subsequently uals have more diiculty perceiving and processing
gave saliva samples. hose recruits with a history of positive and rewarding stimuli than aversive stimuli.
childhood trauma had signiicantly higher levels of a his 5-HT impairment has also been linked to impul-
salivary metabolite of NE than did control peers (Otte sivity. In animals, 5-HT impairment results in an inabil-
et al., 2005). ity to refuse or ignore rewarding stimuli. In humans,
Beta-blockers (e.g., propranolol), agents that pre- it renders individuals less likely to refuse or ignore a
vent stimulation of β-adrenoceptors by both NE and stimulus that was once rewarding, even if it is no longer
epinephrine, inhibit some aspects of memory consoli- rewarding. hese 5-HT-impaired subjects also make
dation when administered to animals immediately more commission errors on go–no-go cognitive tests.
before or ater a learning task. hese indings sug- hese tests ask participants to act (“go”) in some situ-
gested that treatment with beta-blockers in the acute ations and to refrain from acting (“no-go”) in others.
atermath of trauma exposure might prevent PTSD. he 5-HT-impaired subjects are as likely as controls to
Propranolol, in comparison with placebo treatment, act when acting is appropriate. However, they are more
was associated with greater reductions in physiological likely to act even when acting is not appropriate, dem-
reactivity when recalling memories of trauma three onstrating a failure of inhibitory mechanisms (reviewed
months later (Pitman et al., 2002). Unfortunately, sub- by Cools et al., 2008). his 5-HT impairment is also
sequent similar investigations have failed to replicate thought to contribute to the pathophysiology of MDD,
the promising results (Orr et al., 2006). PTSD, and other anxiety disorders. Although this was
originally inferred from the serendipitous discovery
Serotonin that agents that increased 5-HT transmission were
Serotonin has been implicated in the regulation of a oten efective antidepressants, decades of additional
number of complex processes including anxiety, fear, research have documented more direct evidence to sup-
mood, aggression, and impulse control. However, port this hypothesis. For example, the density of 5-HT1A
because 5-HT has at least 17 unique receptors and add- receptors is reduced in depressed patients when they
itional enzymes and proteins that inluence its metab- are depressed as well as in remission (Belda & Armario,
olism and release, it remains exceedingly diicult to 2009), and density is also decreased in patients with
understand exactly how it exerts its inluence (Cools panic disorder (Bogdan & Pizzagalli, 2006).
et al., 2008; Dayan & Huys, 2009). Researchers have Given that 5-HT impairment is associated with
approached the study of 5-HT by exposing humans or increased physiological responsivity to stress, increased
animals believed to have compromised 5-HT signaling attention to negative stimuli, and impulsivity, as well
(5-HT-impaired subjects) to stressors and observing as with MDD and PTSD, it is possible to deduce that
the results. In humans, genetic testing can identify efective 5-HT neurotransmission would contribute
individuals with speciic polymorphisms known to to allostasis in situations where tranquility, optimism,
impair the function of their 5-HT system for partici- and/or behavioral control are adaptive.
pation in experiments. Alternatively, the quantity of
5-HT available can be reduced via nutritional manipu- Allostatic load
lation (tryptophan depletion). Animals can be genetic- here is no evidence that physiologically appropri-
ally or physically modiied. ate levels of 5-HT are harmful. However, agents that

6
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

facilitate its neurotransmission have a clear allostatic even following the restoration of the receptors
load. Common side-efects of selective serotonin (Heisler et al., 1998; Parks et al., 1998; Gross et al.,
reuptake inhibitors such as luoxetine and citalopram 2002). Reductions in these same 5-HT1 receptors in
include sexual dysfunction, such as reduced desire, dei- adulthood create only a state-dependent anxious
cits in the ability to achieve orgasm, or quality of orgasm; phenotype, reinstating the receptors restores original
nausea; dry mouth; headache; diarrhea; rash; nervous- behavior. hese results suggest that altered function
ness, agitation, and restlessness; increased sweating; of 5-HT receptors early in life can produce long-term
and weight gain. Serotonin syndrome, one of the most abnormalities in the regulation of anxiety behaviors
severe of the possible adverse efects, is characterized by (Gross et al., 2002) and underscores the importance of
the presence of symptoms in three domains: cognitive early childhood environment in inclining an individ-
(mental confusion, hypomania, hallucinations, agita- ual to resilience or vulnerability to mood and anxiety
tion, headache, coma); autonomic (shivering, sweat- disorders.
ing, hyperthermia, hypertension, tachycardia, nausea, Interactions between the 5-HT and other neu-
diarrhea); and somatic (myoclonus [muscle twitching], romodulating systems also appear to be relevant to
hyperrelexia [manifested by clonus], tremor). As indi- the eicacy of its signaling. For example, increases
cated by the range of risk imposed by the symptoms in adrenal steroid release, characteristic of an active
listed in each domain, cases can range from mild to HPA axis, result in a corresponding decrease in 5-HT1
severe (Boyer & Shannon, 2005). receptor density and mRNA levels by activating MRs
(Lopez et al., 1998). here may also be important func-
Factors contributing to resilience tional interactions between 5-HT1A and benzodiazep-
Given the wealth of data implicating suboptimal 5-HT ine receptors. One study of 5-HT1A knockout mice
signaling in the pathophysiology of mood and anxiety reported a downregulation of benzodiazepine GABA
disorders, any factors that promote the health of this α1- and α2-receptor subunits as well as benzodiazepine-
system would likely support resilience. Numerous stud- resistant anxiety in the elevated-plus maze (Sibille
ies have explored the role of genetics, and speciically et al., 2000). However, a subsequent study did not rep-
a well-known polymorphism in the gene for the sero- licate these results using mice with a diferent genetic
tonin transporter (5HTTLPR) in conferring vulner- background (Pattij et al., 2002), raising the possibility
ability to depression. hree genotypes are possible: two that genetic background can afect functional interplay
copies of the long allele (LL), two copies the short allele between 5-HT1A and benzodiazepine systems.
(SS), or one of each (SL). A handful of studies found
that that the short allele renders individuals more likely Dopamine
to develop MDD ater stressful life experiences (Caspi With ive known receptors, dopamine is another mul-
et al., 2003; Gillespie et al., 2005). However, two recent titasking neuromodulator, involved in numerous psy-
meta-analyses concluded that the relationship between chological and physiological processes (psychosis and
these variables is not signiicant (Munafo et al., 2009; movement among the most well known). Its function
Risch et al., 2009). Verhagen and colleagues (2009) in the experience of reward and pleasure has been most
suggest that the efect of the 5HTTLPR polymorphism thoroughly investigated in mood and anxiety disorders.
on MDD is co-dependent on the presence of co-mor- he “mesocorticolimbic” dopamine pathway, com-
bid disorders and sex. In their work, the S-allele of the posed of neurons in the ventral tegmental area, ventral
5-HTTLPR polymorphism has been associated with striatum (nucleus accumbens), amygdala, medial pre-
signiicantly lower rates of particular lifetime co-mor- frontal cortex, ventral pallidum and mediodorsal thal-
bid disorders. herefore, they argue that the presence amus, is thought to be most relevant to reward. his
of co-morbid psychiatric disorders should be taken circuit is linked to brain regions involved in attention,
into account to clarify the association of the 5HTTLPR goal-directed behavior, motivation, reinforcement,
polymorphism with MDD phenotypes. emotion regulation, cognitive function, and social
In animal models, embryonic and early post- interactions. Dopamine neuron activity in the ventral
natal shutdown of expression of a type of 5-HT recep- tegmental area is dependent upon expectation of and
tor (5-HT1), via either genetic (e.g., knockout mice) receipt of reward: the neurons are activated in response
or environmental (e.g., high levels of juvenile CRH) to a reward (e.g., food, sex, social interaction), or even
means, produces an anxiety phenotype that endures, the expectation of a reward, and are inhibited by an

7
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

aversive stimulus or the absence of an expected reward dissatisfaction with social rewards in healthy males was
(Carter et al., 2009). (However, certain dopaminergic associated with increased activation in prefrontal (top-
neurons are also activated by aversive stimuli, suggest- down control) areas during performance of a monet-
ing their involvement in mood regulation more gener- ary task (Siegrist et al., 2005).
ally [Carter et al., 2009].) Hence animal models suggest Patients with PTSD are both less likely to expect a
that the response of the dopaminergic system changes reward and experience less satisfaction upon receipt of
as a function of the nature and duration of the stress a reward than controls (Hopper et al., 2008). Further,
experienced by an animal. functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) investi-
gations suggest that they demonstrate less activation in
Allostatic contribution the nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex in
Exposure to brief, novel stressors appears to result in an response to reward (Sailer et al., 2008). However, their
immediate increase in mesolimbic dopamine release activation does not difer from controls in response to a
(Puglisi-Allegra et al., 1991; Pascucci et al., 2007). loss or disappointment. In rodents, exposure to a social
hese changes in signaling serve to arouse the animal defeat paradigm (time in a cage with a larger aggres-
and catalyze coping behaviors. If the animal deter- sive animal) increased activity of dopamine neurons
mines that it is possible to behaviorally modulate the in the ventral tegmental area via increased activity-
dose or intensity of the stressor, increased mesolimbic dependent release of BDNF onto nucleus accumbens
dopamine release can be maintained regardless of the neurons. Animals that recovered normal functioning
severity or duration of exposure (Puglisi-Allegra et al., ater the exposure upregulated potassium channels in
1991). he animal continues to struggle as long as the the ventral tegmental area, minimizing the increase in
behavior is rewarded. neuronal excitability and in BDNF release (Eisch et al.,
However, if coping tactics are inefective (and the 2003; Meaney & Szyf, 2005). hose that continued to
stress is uncontrollable), the animal does not receive the demonstrate anxious behaviors did not (Eisch et al.,
reward it originally expected. Mesolimbic dopamine 2003, Krishnan et al., 2008).
transmission will drop of to lower than normal levels, In a study conducted by Pizzagalli and colleagues
contributing to the inhibition of behavioral responses (2009), subjects with MDD, in contrast to controls,
to stress (Puglisi-Allegra et al., 1991; Pascucci et al., showed attenuated responses to reward in the let
2007). his may serve as a survival mechanism in situ- nucleus accumbens and the caudate bilaterally.
ations where conservation of energy better serves the However, there were no group diferences in these
animal than continuing a futile struggle. regions in response to neutral or negative outcomes.
In addition, Belda and Armario (2009) explored In this same sample, self-reported severity of anhedo-
the impact of dopamine antagonists on HPA axis activ- nia and depressed mood group were associated with
ity with immobilization stress in rats. Both D1 and D2 reduced caudate volume bilaterally. In imaging inves-
receptor antagonists reduced HPA axis activity and tigations, individuals with a history of MDD, but no
the duration of activation, indicating that dopamine present clinically signiicant depressive symptoms,
stimulates HPA axis activity during and ater stress via were less responsive to pleasant visual stimuli in the ven-
these receptors. tral striatum than were controls. In contrast, they were
more responsive to aversive visual stimuli in the caud-
Allostatic load ate nucleus. When the visual stimuli were paired with
Stress reduces sensitivity to reward in humans (Bogdan corresponding olfactory stimuli, subjects with MDD
& Pizzagalli, 2006), and altered reward circuitry appears evinced relatively less activity in the prefrontal cortex
to characterize both MDD (Nestler & Carlezon, 2006) (McCabe et al., 2009). Adolescents with MDD have also
and PTSD. Indeed, an increasing number of studies been shown to mount a weaker striatal response but a
implicate the mesocorticolimbic circuit in both resili- more robust dorsolateral and medial prefrontal cortex
ence and vulnerability to developing mood and anx- response than controls during reward anticipation and
iety symptoms. Special forces soldiers, considered reward outcome (Forbes et al., 2009). Altered activa-
highly resilient because of their ability to perform well tion of reward circuits in depressed compared with
even under extreme stress, showed increased reactivity healthy adolescents was associated with self-reports of
of reward-processing regions compared with healthy reduced positive afect in naturalistic settings (Forbes
civilian controls (Vythilingam et al., 2009). Conversely, et al., 2009). Diferential reward system function has

8
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

also been demonstrated in children of depressed versus behavior, circadian rhythms, cardiovascular symp-
never-depressed parents (Monk et al., 2008). toms, and the immune and stress responses (Hökfelt
et al., 1998). here are at least four metabotropic,
Factors contributing to resilience G-protein-coupled NPY receptors expressed in the
States and traits that facilitate efective functioning of human brain (Y1, Y2, Y3, Y5) (Blomqvist & Herzog,
mesolimbic dopamine signaling and buttress the cir- 1997) whose activation precipitates inhibitory
cuitry of reward might promote active coping and responses such as the inhibition of cyclic AMP accu-
decrease vulnerability to MDD and anxiety. For example, mulation (Lin et al., 2004).
the persistence of the state of reduced mesolimbic dopa-
minergic transmission that results from uncontrollable Allostatic contribution
stress appears to be, in part, genetically determined; here are important interactions between NPY and
some strains of mice become sensitized to the reduction CRH (Heilig et al., 1994; Britton et al., 2000): NPY
in mesolimbic dopamine and re-initiate coping behav- counteracts the anxiogenic efects of CRH at various
iors, while others will evince progressively fewer and locations within the stress–anxiety circuit, includ-
fewer coping behaviors (Puglisi-Allegra et al., 1990). In ing the amygdala, hippocampus, hypothalamus, and
humans, genes afecting the integrity of dopamine sig- locus coeruleus. Activation of the Y1 receptor inhib-
naling inluence severity of PTSD. An SNP (C957T) in its several metabolic and behavioral stress responses,
the gene for the D2 receptor (DRD2) has been linked to including gastrointestinal efects, anxious behavior,
PTSD risk. Individuals with PTSD are more likely to and decreased sleep (Eva et al., 2006).
carry the C allele compared with the controls (Voisey
et al., 2009). In a study of combat veterans with PTSD, Allostatic load
the A1+ allele of DRD2 was associated with increased No known health risks are associated with excess expos-
PTSD symptoms and higher co-morbid anxiety and ure to NPY. However, knockout models have revealed
depression (Lawford et al., 2006). he A1+ allele has that each of the NPY receptors have unique purposes.
previously been linked to social dysfunction in children, For example, while mice without any NPY display
compromised visuospatial functioning in adolescents, increased levels of anxious behavior (spending less time
and increased family stress (Lawford et al., 2006). in the open ield on the open ield test) and increased
he A2 allele of DRD2 appears to confer MDD risk. startle amplitude (Bannon et al., 2000), Y2 receptor
Occurrence of stressful life events was associated with knockout mice display less anxious behavior than con-
increased risk of subsequent depressive symptoms in trols, suggesting that Y2 receptors may be anxiogenic.
individuals with the A2/A2 genotype. No such associ- Mice bred to overexpress NPY are more vulner-
ation was detected in participants with A1/A1 or A1/ able to developing obesity when maintained on a high-
A2 genotypes (Elovainio et al., 2007). sucrose diet, and as their age advances (Kaga et al.,
Exposure to stress during development also appears 2001). hey are also relatively hypersensitive to ethanol
to alter functioning of dopaminergic circuitry. Adult (hiele et al., 1998).
rats exposed to social defeat as adolescents had lower
levels of medial prefrontal cortex dopamine, increased Factors promoting resilience
NE in the dentate gyrus, and decreased NE in the dor- In general, increased levels of NPY seem to promote
sal raphe (Watt et al., 2009). resilience and reduce anxiety in both animal models
he Val158Met COMT polymorphism also and humans. In a study of special forces soldiers, who
appears to contribute to variability between individ- are considered to be highly stress resilient, higher NPY
uals in neural responses to reward anticipation, even in levels during rigorous military training were associated
healthy individuals. with better performance (McGaugh, 2004). Other stud-
ies found higher plasma and cerebrospinal luid NPY in
Neuropeptides combat-exposed veterans without PTSD than in those
with PTSD (Sah et al., 2009). hese indings in humans
Neuropeptide Y are consistent with recent studies in rats: central admin-
Neuropeptide Y (NPY), composed of 36 amino acid istration of NPY in rats inhibits the development, and
residues and one of the most abundant neuropep- promotes the extinction, of fear conditioning, with NPY
tides, is involved in regulation of feeding and sexual antagonists exerting the opposite actions. hese efects

9
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

are mediated at least in part via the amygdala (Morgan with increased NE release in the central nucleus of the
et al., 2000). Moreover, intra-amygdala NPY admin- amygdala (Khoshbouei et al., 2002).
istration promotes resilient responses to stress, in the
form of reduced anxiety-like behaviors in response to Allostatic load
acute restraint (Yehuda et al., 2006b). he GAL-3 receptor has anxiogenic efects. Galanin
Elevated NPY is negatively correlated with depres- acting at the GAL-3 receptor may precipitate decreases
sion and anxiety in human and animal models (Heilig in 5-HT signaling by hyperpolarizing 5-HT neurons in
et al., 1993, 2004; Karlsson et al., 2005). A variety of the dorsal raphe nucleus and reducing levels of 5-HT
antidepressant drugs increase NPY levels in humans in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, leading to
and animals (Nikisch et al., 2005; Goyal et al., 2009; increased vulnerability to anxiety and isolation. In
Bjornebekk et al., 2010). Electroconvulsive therapy animal models, GAL-3 antagonists have increased
has produced similar elevations in animal studies prosocial behaviors and decreased anxiety and depres-
(Mikkelsen & Woldbye, 2006; Nikisch & Mathe, 2008). sion-like behaviors (Swanson et al., 2006; Kozlovsky
et al., 2009).
Galanin
Galanin is a neuropeptide composed of 30 amino acid Factors promoting resilience
residues and with three known receptors (GAL-1, In humans, certain polymorphisms of the gene for the
GAL-2, GAL-3). All are thought to be involved in GAL-3 receptor have been linked with anxiety and alco-
allostasis (Walton et al., 2006). A dense galanin iber holism (Belfer et al., 2006). In a recent study exploring
system originates in the locus coeruleus and innervates an animal model of galanin functioning in PTSD, rats
the hippocampus, hypothalamus, amygdala, and pre- were exposed to a predator. hose rats who exhibited
frontal cortex among other areas (Perez et al., 2001). minimal anxiety-like symptoms ater the exposure
Galanin-releasing neurons are thought to be located were observed to have a lasting upregulation of galanin
immediately adjacent to the central amygdala and mRNA in the C area of the hippocampus in the pre-
appear to be activated by non-noradrenergic aferents frontal cortex, while those who became symptomatic
that themselves are activated by NE (Barrera et al., evinced galanin mRNA downregulation in the same
2006). areas (Kozlovsky et al., 2009). In addition, those symp-
tomatic animals treated with galnon displayed fewer
Allostatic contribution anxiety-like behaviors and upregulated their galanin
Both GAL-1, an autoreceptor (Sevcik et al., 1993; mRNA. Immediate post-exposure treatment with gal-
Xu et al., 2001), and GAL-2 appear to be anxiolytic. non signiicantly reduced prevalence rates of extreme
Knockout mice for GAL-1 and GAL-2 evince increased responders, reduced trauma-cue freezing responses,
anxiety-like behavior (Holmes et al., 2003; Rajarao corrected the corticosterone response, and increased
et al., 2007; Lu et al., 2008a). Galnon, a non-speciic CA1 expression of 5-HT1A and BDNF mRNA com-
galanin agonist, has been shown to elevate GABA lev- pared with controls treated with saline.
els in the rat amygdala and results in anxiolytic efects
in some (open-maze and hyperthermia), but not all
(mouse tail suspension, rat forced swim) animal mod-
Agents modulating the efficacy and
els (Unschuld et al., 2008). Galanin reduces NE, 5-HT, intensity of the acute stress response
and dopamine levels in the prefrontal cortex via inhib-
ition (Holmes & Picciotto, 2006). Hence, scientists Sex hormones (gonadal steroids)
have proposed that galanin recruitment during stress Epidemiological studies have repeatedly demonstrated
may serve as a bufer, minimizing the intensity of the that women are twice as likely to sufer from depressive
anxiety experienced as a result of NE over-activation and anxiety disorder. It appears that this gender dis-
(Karlsson & Holmes, 2006). Galanin-overexpressing parity in risk and resilience is mediated in part by the
transgenic mice are unresponsive to the anxiogenic impact of sex hormones on HPA axis activity.
efects of the α2-adrenoceptor antagonist yohimbine.
Consistent with this observation, galanin injected dir- Testosterone
ectly into the central nucleus of the amygdala blocked Testosterone, a steroid hormone from the andro-
the anxiogenic efects of stress, which is associated gen group derived from cholesterol, induces protein

10
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

synthesis and growth of tissues with androgen recep- was more likely to result in reduced depression severity
tors. It can also act, ater conversion to estradiol, via than placebo and noted that the efect was particularly
the estrogen receptors. In men, who have much higher strong in patients with hypogonadism or HIV/AIDS
levels of testosterone than women, it is produced pri- (Zarrouf et al., 2009). Hence, one reasonable hypoth-
marily in the Leydig cells in the testes; in women, it is esis is that adequate supplies of testosterone, by redu-
produced in the thecal cells of the ovaries, and in the cing physiological and psychological stress reactivity,
placenta during pregnancy. Testosterone increases serve as a bufer. hose with lower basal levels (includ-
muscle mass and bone density, facilitates maturation ing women) would be more vulnerable because the
of the sex organs and development of secondary sex stress-induced decrease in testosterone would make
characteristics in men, and promotes libido. them prone to more severe, and therefore more costly,
Testosterone administration in women reliably stress responses.
reduces physiological reactivity to stress, as evinced However, as with most agents that impact allostatic
by decreases in skin conductance, heart rate, and star- load, there can be too much of a good thing. High levels
tle (Hermans et al., 2006, 2007), as well as attention of testosterone are associated not only with product-
bias toward and conscious recognition of angry faces ive feelings of competence but also with mood lability
(van Honk et al., 1999, 2005; van Honk & Schutter, and increased propensity for violence. Studies of the
2007; Wirth & Schultheiss, 2007). It also increases neuropsychiatric efects of androgen administration
activity of brain areas associated with aggression and in healthy men are few but informative. Some have
impulse control (amygdala, hypothalamus, brainstem, observed statistically signiicant, if not always clinic-
and Brodmann area 47 of the orbitofrontal cortex) ally signiicant, increases in mood lability and distract-
(Hermans et al., 2008). A proile of high testosterone ibility (Su et al., 1993); others have not (O’Connor,
and low cortisol in the absence of testosterone admin- 2002). Emergence of hypomania and mania has also
istration is also associated with increased activity in been noted (Su et al., 1993; Pope et al., 2000). Increased
these regions compared with controls. In addition, levels of testosterone have been found in male prison
testosterone levels are known to correlate positively inmates with frequent episodes of violent behavior
with aggression (Zitzmann & Nieschlag, 2001; Bender (Forbes et al., 2009; McCabe et al., 2009). While tes-
et al., 2006; Yu & Shi, 2009), social status (Flugge et al., tosterone’s ability to reduce conscious recognition of
2001; Anestis, 2006; Setchell et al., 2008; Czoty et al., emotions such as anger might serve to control fear
2009), athletic success (Edwards et al., 2006; Edwards and facilitate active responses to threat (van Honk &
& O’Neal, 2009; Oliveira et al., 2009), and the subject- Schutter, 2007), this same efect might also predispose
ive feelings of success, dominance, and social connect- individuals to antisocial behavior. here is also some
edness, all experiences that are known to be associated evidence to suggest that pairing reduced levels of cor-
with resilience (as discussed above). tisol with increased levels of testosterone may augment
However, stress is associated with reductions in tes- its efect and contribute to criminal and aggressive ten-
tosterone levels, which is observed both in soldiers ater dencies (Terburg et al., 2009).
a psychologically and physically stressful training exer-
cise and in marathon runners following a diicult race Estrogens
(Morgan et al., 2004; Franca et al., 2006). Hypogonadism Estrogens, steroid hormones produced through
is associated with depressive symptoms, while testos- enzymatic modulation of androgens, act primarily via
terone replacement is associated with improvement in estrogen receptors. In women, who have much higher
mood and relief from other depressive symptoms, and levels than men, estrogens are produced primarily in
decreases in CRH-stimulated release of cortisol and the ovarian follicles, the corpus luteum and the pla-
the cortisol-to-ACTH ratio (Daly et al., 2001). Some centa during pregnancy; in men, testosterone is con-
men with a polymorphism within the gene encoding verted into estradiol to act in the skeletal and central
the androgen receptor that makes them less susceptible nervous systems. Estrogens promote the development
to androgen efects are at a higher risk for developing of female secondary sex characteristics and regulate
MDD (Zitzmann, 2006; Colangelo et al., 2007). While the menstrual cycle in women, and they contribute
the results of studies testing the antidepressant potential to sperm maturation in men. In addition, they sup-
of testosterone have been inconsistent, a recent meta- port the health and functioning of many other body
analysis concluded that testosterone administration systems.

11
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Estrogens can either potentiate or inhibit the peptide systems (Table 1.2). hese interactions also
body’s stress response. Normally menstrual cycling play a role in determining the allostatic load. Further
female rodents have higher basal and stress-induced research is needed to speciically determine the mecha-
levels of ACTH and corticosterone than males, as do nisms, and ultimate importance, of these relationships.
proestrous compared with diestrous females (Young What is understood about the very complex role of
et al., 2001; Solomon & Herman 2009). Administration estrogens in the central nervous system was recently
of superphysiological doses of estrogens to gonadally reviewed by Solomon & Herman (2009). hey con-
intact female rats or estradiol to ovariectomized rats cluded that stable, physiological doses of estrogens in
raises ACTH and corticosterone levels (Carey et al., gonadally intact females appear to be largely protect-
1995). However, physiological doses of estradiol have ive, while luctuation and instability in estrogen levels
been shown to reduce the ACTH response to acute confer vulnerability.
stress in female rats, while estrogen antagonists had
the opposite efect (Young et al., 2001). Proestrous
compared with diestrous females evince less anxious Brain-derived neurotrophic factor
behavior. Neurotrophins are proteins that promote survival,
In women, physiological doses of estrogens con- development, and function of neurons by regulat-
strain stress-induced activation of neurons in the ing growth, and associated metabolic functions such
frontal cortex, hippocampal, and paraventricular as protein synthesis, and neurotransmitter produc-
nucleus. Estrogens have been shown to blunt HPA tion. Hence, they play a role in determining the brain’s
axis responses to psychological stress in postmeno- response to stressors. Brain-derived neurotrophic fac-
pausal women (Komesarof et al., 1999; Cucinelli et al., tor is highly produced in the brain and is known for
2002) and to blunt the ACTH response to CRH in its support of adult hippocampal function in rodent
postmenopausal women with high levels of body fat. models.
In addition, eight weeks of estrogen supplementation Studies have demonstrated that stress leads to
in perimenopausal women blunted increases in sys- a reduction of BDNF in the rodent hippocampus.
tolic and diastolic blood pressure, cortisol, ACTH, Further, chronic antidepressant treatment can reverse
plasma epinephrine and NE, and in NE responses of this efect (Gutman et al., 2008). Postmortem studies
the entire body to stress (Young, 1996). Pregnancy and suggest that a parallel process occurs in the human
lactation, states associated with high levels of estro- hippocampus. In contrast, in the rodent nucleus
gens, also result in decreased ACTH and cortisol sig- accumbens, chronic stress increases BDNF levels
naling. Conversely, premenstrual, postpartum, and (Duman & Monteggia, 2006; Sajdyk et al., 2008). his
premenopausal states are associated with an increased change in BDNF concentrations results in a propor-
prevalence of MDD and anxiety disorders (reviewed by tional increase in depressive-like behaviors such as
Douma et al., 2005). passive coping, social avoidance, and anhedonia.
It is clear that estrogens are able to modulate the hat is, the more symptomatic an animal, the larger
degree of stress response and, consequently, the degree the increase in BDNF concentration in the nucleus
of allostatic load. Whether or not estrogens enhance or accumbens, while “resilient” animals show no parallel
suppress stress responsivity appears to depend, in part, increase. Humans with depression also show increased
on the targeted receptor. Two unique estrogen recep- BDNF levels in the nucleus accumbens (Eisch et al.,
tors have been identiied: ERα and ERβ (Green et al., 2003).
1986; Kuiper et al., 1996). Systemic or paraventricular It is unclear what role exactly BDNF plays in resili-
nucleus administration of agents that selectively target ence. As with other factors, its impact is diferent in dif-
ERα, in concert with a stressor, precipitated increases ferent contexts. In mice, an SNP (G196A, Val66Met)
in ACTH, corticosterone, and paraventricular nucleus in the gene encoding BDNF is associated with reduced
fos RNA; agents that selectively target ERβ have the hippocampal volume and signiicantly impairs BDNF’s
opposite impact. Hence, ERβ are thought to underlie intracellular traicking and activity-dependent release
estrogen’s protective beneits, and vice versa (Weiser (Heinz & Smolka, 2006). Mice expressing the A allele
et al., 2008; Solomon & Herman 2009). evince more anxiety-like behavior and impaired hip-
Estrogens are also known to interact with the pocampal-dependent learning. Yet, they are more
monoaminergic (McEwen, 2002), GABA, and neuro- resilient to chronic stress (Eisch et al., 2003; Heinz &

12
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

Table 1.2 Estrogen’s influence on other stress-responsive systems.

System Interaction Reference


5-HT Increased expression of the gene for Bethea et al., 2002
tryptophan hydroxylase and protein
production
Increased 5-HT2A receptor binding Moses et al., 2000
Decreased serotonin transporter Pecins-Thompson et al., 1998
expression
Decreased 5-HT1A mRNA and 5-HT1A Bethea et al., 2002
binding in both presynaptic (dorsal
raphe) and postsynaptic sites
Decreased levels of inhibitory G Raap et al., 2000; Mize et al., 2001
proteins involved in intracellular signal
transduction mediated by the 5-HT1A
receptor
Norepinephrine and dopamine Modulated α1-adrenoceptor expression Colucci et al., 1982
Modulated β-adrenoceptor receptor Klangkalya & Chan, 1988
expression
Modulated norepinephrine and Alfinito et al., 2009
dopamine release
GABA Increased GABAA benzodiazepine Maggi & Perez, 1986
receptor expression
Neuropeptides Increased neuropeptide Y release and Hilke et al., 2009
gene expression
Increased galanin release and gene Hilke et al., 2005
expression
5-HT, serotonin; GABA, gamma-aminobutyric acid.

Smolka, 2006). hese data underscore the complex For example, rats raised with low levels of mater-
efects of BDNF on the brain, which are highly speciic nal nurturing evince increased methylation of the
to the brain region. promoter region for GR at the binding site for nerve
growth factor-inducible factor in the hippocam-
Epigenetic mechanisms pus, an epigenetic change associated with reduced
Chromosomes are constructed from chromatin, which GR expression (Weaver et al., 2004; Meaney & Szyf,
consists of DNA, RNA, and proteins. he most abun- 2005). his epigenetic change emerges in the irst
dant of these proteins are histones, which serve as the week of life and persists into adulthood. he rats, both
framework around which DNA is wound. When chro- as pups and adults, have a more robust corticosterone
matin structure is modiied, stable changes in gene response to stress and express lower levels of GR in
expression result, which can be passed on to subse- the hippocampus compared with genetically identical
quent generations when they occur in germ cells. hese controls raised with more nurturing mothers. hey
“epigenetic” events impact gene expression by altering are also more anxious and are less nurturing to their
DNA transcription rather than sequence, and oten own ofspring.
involve chemical modiication of histones via acetyl- Perhaps even more dramatic evidence of epigenetic
ation or methylation, or of DNA via methylation (Kim inluences on resilience comes from comparisons of
et al., 2007). he oten durable up- and downregulation genetically identical rats raised not in purposefully dif-
of the stress-response systems following trauma appear ferent environments but in very controlled and nearly
to be mediated by epigenetic change. identical environments. hese animals do not always

13
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

demonstrate the same response (behaviorally or epi- HPA axis eiciency. A neural circuit of coordinated
genetically) to the same stressors. For example, when a signaling between the amygdala, hippocampus and
population of inbred mice is exposed to a social defeat ventromedial prefrontal cortex) is thought to be asso-
paradigm (Pezawas et al., 2005; Munafo et al., 2008), ciated with fear and fear-related learning (Dedovic
some demonstrate behavioral changes indicative of et al., 2005; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, 2009). Animal
anxiety and depression, while others do not (Eisch et al., and human studies suggest that the amygdala is the
2003; Krishnan et al., 2007). In one study, the resilient primary site of fear-conditioning acquisition (Rauch
animals not only failed to demonstrate many of the et al., 2006). hat is, the amygdala facilitates the learn-
changes in gene expression seen in the ventral tegmen- ing that allows cues associated with a fearful stimulus
tal area–nucleus accumbens of vulnerable mice but (e.g., footshock, predator odor) to become aversive
also evinced distinct changes in gene expression absent themselves (Felmingham et al., 2007; Liberzon &
in their vulnerable counterparts (Eisch et al., 2003). Sripada, 2008). Contextual and temporal aspects of
For example, resilient mice induced several potassium fear conditioning are processed by the hippocam-
channel subunits in dopamine neurons in the ventral pus (Davis & Whalen, 2001). Both amygdala- and
tegmental area. his change served as prophylaxis hippocampus-dependent fear conditioning in animals
against a stress-triggered increase in ventral tegmental has been related to long-term potentiation and other
area excitability and subsequent release of BDNF into forms of synaptic plasticity. Accordingly, blockade of
the nucleus accumbens (Eisch et al., 2003; Meaney & NMDA glutamate receptors in the amygdala blocks
Szyf, 2005). Resilience has also been associated with cue-associated fear conditioning. In the hippocampus,
the induction of ∆FosB in neurons in the ventrolateral it blocks context-dependent fear conditioning (Bast,
region of the periaqueductal gray in the midbrain. he 2007). Reactivation of memories (i.e., re-exposure to
∆FosB suppressed the production of the neuropep- the cue or context) can lead to either reconsolidation
tide substance P in these cells, which, in turn, reduced (further strengthening of the memory) or to extinc-
substance P transmission to target regions such as the tion, with extinction generally requiring more inten-
nucleus accumbens (Berton et al., 2007). Interestingly, sive training. Extinction of fear conditioning involves
in some of the vulnerable animals, chronic, but not not only the amygdala but also the ventromedial pre-
acute, treatment with imipramine reversed the efect frontal cortex; activation in these structures, as well
(Wilkinson et al., 2009). as thickness of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, has
hese indings buttress the view that resilience is been associated with extinction success (Rauch et al.,
not simply the absence of maladaptive changes that 2006; Yehuda & LeDoux, 2007). In addition, emerging
occur in vulnerable individuals but also is mediated evidence suggests that this area of the brain is the site
by a unique set of adaptive changes. In addition, the of the neural processing required to adapt to context
absence of any obvious external variables that deter- when assessing the degree of threat associated with
mined which of the identical animals were resilient and a particular stimuli. In a recent fMRI study, partici-
which were not suggests that some epigenetic changes pants were repeatedly exposed to stimuli that changed
that inluence an organism’s resilience and vulnerabil- from threatening to safe, and vice versa. Activity in
ity to stress are unpredictable. It is important to note the ventromedial prefrontal cortex was correlated
that the maladaptive or adaptive nature of any epigen- with success in modifying neural and physiological
etic changes, or even genetic polymorphisms, is likely responses accordingly (Delgado et al., 2006).
context dependent. Belsky and colleagues (2009) pro- he experience of fear, and more speciically the
posed that, “putative ‘vulnerability genes’ or ‘risk alleles’ ability to learn to associate stimuli with danger, is
might, at times, be more appropriately conceptualized essential to ensuring that the stress response is timely,
as ‘plasticity genes’, because they seem to make individ- adequate, and facilitates survival. However, in order to
uals more susceptible to environmental inluences – prevent hyperactivity of the HPA axis, it is also import-
for better and for worse.” ant that an individual be able to extinguish fear-related
memories, so that stimuli that are only dangerous in
some contexts do not continue to be interpreted physio-
Neural circuitry of fear logically as valid threats. herefore, the integrity of the
he ability to distinguish between threatening and neural circuitry of fear and fear learning would pro-
non-threatening stimuli is essential to ensure optimal mote resilience. While the genetic and environmental

14
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

Trigger: A woman passes her friend on the street. Figure 1.1 Elements of emotion and
She waves and says hello. Her friend doesn't make eye the impact of cognitive interpretation
contact trigger.
Interpretation A Interpretation B Interpretation C
My friend ignored me My friend ignored me My friend didn't see me. He
purposefully. He must be angry purposefully. He is such a would have responded if he
with me self-centered jerk! had seen me

Physiological Physiological response/ Physiological response/


response/awareness awareness awareness
Guilt, depression Anger, hopelessness
Resignation, disappointment
I must have done something My friend is trying to hurt me.
I wish he had seen me. It
wrong. I am a bad person No one can be trusted. I will
would be nice to talk to him
always be alone

Behavior Behavior Behavior


Call friend to arrange a
Avoid friend, other social contact Call friend and berate him
meeting

Outcome Outcome Outcome


Vulnerable Vulnerable Resilient

variables that determine the functional capacity of this external trigger, an interpretation of the trigger, an
circuit have yet to be identiied, several treatments appraisal of the interpretation, a physiological reac-
for PTSD attempt to inhibit fear-conditioning learn- tion, and (possibly) conscious awareness and overt
ing and enhance extinction learning. For example, behavior (Power & Dalgleish, 1997). Consider the fol-
because long-term potentiation is known to be lowing sequence: a woman passes an old friend on the
involved in extinction learning, a partial agonist of the street and waves hello. he friend does not make eye
NMDA receptor (d-cycloserine) that facilitates long- contact or respond in any way and continues walking
term potentiation is administered during prolonged (trigger). A number of possible interpretations, and the
exposure therapy (Goldstein et al., 1996). Blockade of resulting consequences, that provide an apt illustration
β-adrenoceptors in the amygdala can also block cue- of how emotions can inluence stress response are out-
dependent fear conditioning, and beta-blockers have lined in Figure 1.1.
been tested in patients exposed to trauma, with mixed he transient and/or context-dependent experi-
results (Baker et al., 1999; Morrow et al., 1999). In add- ence of negative emotion serves a purpose and pro-
ition, fear conditioning and its extinction are regulated motes successful adaptation. An attentional bias
by activation of GRs in the hippocampus and perhaps toward stimuli that signal danger and mobilization of
in the amygdala, which suggests the possible use of physiological resources support the arousal necessary
glucocorticoids in the treatment of trauma (Lemieux for life-saving responses to threat such as “ight and
& Coe, 1995). However, whether β-adrenoceptor and light” (Isen et al., 1987; Lazarus, 1991; Folkman &
GR levels or functioning difers in resilient versus non- Moskowitz, 2000). However, chronic negativity is tax-
resilient individuals has not yet been studied. ing to the body. A recent meta-analysis of 729 studies
found that hostility, aggression, and type A behavior
are associated with increased cardiovascular activity
Psychological processes in response to stress, while anxiety, neuroticism, and
A number of psychosocial states and traits have been generalized negative afect are associated with poorer
repeatedly associated with resilience, including posi- cardiovascular recovery following stress (Chida &
tive emotions, optimism, active coping, social support, Hamer, 2008).
and prosocial behavior. All of these can be understood In contrast, positive states and traits are associated
as strategies that support the eicacy and reduce the with reduced HPA axis reactivity (Chida & Hamer,
intensity and duration of stress responsivity. 2008), as well as faster cardiovascular recovery from
negativity-induced stressors (Levenson, 2003), and
Positive and negative emotions have been associated with physical and psychological
heorists conceptualize emotion as a multifaceted risk only when very unrealistic (e.g., manic delusions
entity with the following components: an internal or of grandeur and invincibility leading to poor decision

15
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

making). he broaden and build theory of Fredrikson optimism, and meaning-making, and the nature of
(2001) posits that the impact of positive emotions is social interactions (competence and prosocial behav-
cumulative; repeated positive emotional experiences ior). hese are now discussed in more detail.
over time prime the system for optimal response to
negative stimuli by expanding physical, psychological, Reframing/reappraisal
intellectual, and social resources. he experience of In general, the ability to use one’s cognitive facilities
positive emotions can also tune the degree of stress to frame an event in a relatively positive light pro-
response by supporting interpretations of events that motes a quick recovery from distress. Neurobiological
are associated with safety (Figure 1.1). mechanisms that underlie some of these processes
Given the protective capacity of positivity and the include memory suppression, memory consolida-
necessary but taxing nature of negative emotions, it tion, and cognitive control of emotion (Ochsner et al.,
also follows that resources that promote positive emo- 2004; Goldin et al., 2008). One mechanism of emo-
tions and/or quick recovery and general afective lexi- tion regulation – cognitive reappraisal – has received
bility (emotion regulation) would promote resilience particular attention. Functional MRI studies have
(Masten & Coatsworth, 1998; Siegrist et al., 2005). A shown increased activation in lateral and medial pre-
neural model of emotion regulation consisting of ven- frontal cortex regions and decreased amygdala activa-
tral and dorsal systems has been described, with vari- tion during reappraisal, with increased activation in
ous patterns of abnormalities associated with a range lateral prefrontal cortex associated with reappraisal
of psychiatric disorders (Hyman et al., 2006; Nestler & success (Ochsner et al., 2004; Goldin et al., 2008). It
Carlezon, 2006). Studies in mood and anxiety disor- has, therefore, been hypothesized that the prefrontal
ders have most consistently identiied abnormalities cortex regulates the intensity of emotional response
in amygdala, hippocampus, subgenual anterior cingu- by modulating activation of the amygdala. An fMRI
lated cortex, and prefrontal cortex function (Johnstone study using mediation analysis demonstrated that the
et al., 2007). ventrolateral prefrontal cortex acts on both the amyg-
In healthy individuals, diferential amygdala dala and the nucleus accumbens, resulting in opposite
reactivity to negative stimuli could represent an inter- behavioral responses: the pathways through the amyg-
mediate phenotype associated with diferential vul- dala and the nucleus accumbens were associated with
nerability to anxiety and depressive disorders (Jabbi reduced and increased reappraisal success, respect-
et al., 2007). Indeed, several studies have linked the ively (Wager et al., 2008). hese indings are consist-
short allele of 5HTTLPR and the A allele of COMT ent with animal studies, which have established that
with higher anxiety levels, vulnerability to negative the amygdala and nucleus accumbens work in concert
mood, increased amygdala reactivity to negative stim- to regulate an individual’s responses to both negative
uli, and diferential amygdala–cortical functional and positive emotional stimuli. Consequently, vari-
coupling (Phillips et al., 2003a, 2003b; Gillespie et al., ability in the function of these two pathways might
2005; Munafo et al., 2008). Furthermore, diferences underlie individual diferences in emotional response
in corticolimbic connectivity suggest a possible gen- and regulation in stressful contexts. Greater use of
etic predisposition toward inlexible emotion process- reappraisal in everyday life has also been linked to
ing (Phillips et al., 2003a). As mentioned above, recent greater prefrontal cortex activation and lower amyg-
imaging studies have shown evidence of multiple gene dala activation to negative stimuli, suggesting a pos-
interactions on limbic reactivity to unpleasant stim- sible central mechanism through which reappraisal
uli (Jabbi et al., 2007). In addition, studies of healthy could promote successful coping and reduce the risk of
children and young adults at high familial risk for mood disorder onset (Drabant et al., 2006). A further
depression and controls at low familial risk for depres- fMRI study found that resilient women with a history
sion have yielded evidence of volumetric diferences of sexual trauma were more successful at cognitively
and diverging neural responses to emotional stimuli enhancing emotional responses to aversive pictures
between the groups (Munn et al., 2007; Monk et al., compared with women with PTSD ater sexual trauma
2008; Wolfensberger et al., 2008). and healthy, non-traumatized controls. his increased
he speciic strategies of emotion regulation that capacity to enhance emotional responses was associ-
have been linked to resilience include the active coping ated with increased prefrontal cortex activation (New
internal mechanisms of reframing/reappraisal, humor, et al., 2009).

16
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

Humor bufer against negative feelings and their consequences,


Humor is a second strategy known to contain the and which is associated with resilience. A sense of pur-
threatening nature of stressful situations through cog- pose and an internal framework of beliefs about right
nitive reappraisal. Identiied as one of the most mature and wrong are characteristic of resilient individuals
defense mechanisms, humor may lessen the likelihood (Southwick et al., 2005; Alim et al., 2008). Religious
of developing stress-induced depression (Deaner & and spiritual beliefs and practices might also facilitate
McConatha, 1993; horson & Powell, 1994; Manne recovery and inding meaning ater trauma (Southwick
et al., 2003). Humor, it has been suggested, may reduce et al., 2005). Brain imaging studies are beginning to
depressive symptoms by reducing tension and discom- identify the neural correlates of human morality (Raine
fort (Vaillant, 1992), and by attracting social support & Yang, 2006).
(Silver et al., 1990). A network of subcortical regions
that constitute core elements of the dopaminergic Active coping
reward system are activated during humor-related Reframing/reappraisal, humor, optimism, and mean-
tasks (Mobbs et al., 2003; Moran et al., 2004). In an ing-making are all active coping strategies, in that an
event-related fMRI study of healthy volunteers, Mobbs individual expends cognitive efort to shape his or her
et al. (2003) found that funny cartoons, in compari- own emotional response. Active coping, including ight
son with non-funny cartoons, elicited activation of and light (rather than freeze) responses are associated
the amygdala, ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens, with a more transient activation of the HPA axis (Korte
ventral tegmental area, anterior thalamus, and subad- et al., 2005), although the relationship between HPA
jacent hypothalamus. A time-series analysis showed axis activity and active or passive coping might not be
that activity in the nucleus accumbens increased with straightforward, as positive associations have also been
degree of humor intensity. he nucleus accumbens has found (Lu et al., 2008b). Physical exercise, which can be
been repeatedly linked to psychologically and pharma- viewed as a form of active coping, has positive efects
cologically mediated rewards, and the amygdala has on mood, attenuates stress responses, and is thought to
been associated with processing of positive emotions, promote neurogenesis (reviewed by aan het Rot et al.,
laughter, and reward magnitude, in addition to its well- 2009).
known role in fear and fear-related behaviors (Mobbs
et al., 2003; Moran et al., 2004). Social competence and prosocial behavior
In addition to these internal mechanisms, the nature of
Optimism one’s social interactions can also promote or constrain
Optimism, the inclination to adopt the most hope- positive emotionality. For instance, an fMRI study of
ful interpretation of any event, is a trait that inlu- married women demonstrated that holding hands with
ences emotion regulation and is associated not only their husband attenuated neural responses to the threat
with resilience (Block & Kremen, 1996; Klohnen, of receiving a shock, a response that was proportional
1996) but also with greater life satisfaction (Chang to the quality of their relationship (Coan et al., 2006).
et al., 1997) and increased psychological and physical Resilient individuals are oten altruistic and are
health (Aleck & Tennen, 1996; Goldman et al., 1996). able to attract and make use of social support (Masten
Interestingly, brain imaging studies have linked the & Coatsworth, 1998; Southwick et al., 2005; Maier
cognitive and emotional experiences of optimism to et al., 2006; Lyons & Parker, 2007). It seems likely that
increased activity in the amygdala and anterior cingu- empathic capacity would facilitate social competence
lated cortex (Sharot et al., 2007) his pattern contrasts and prosocial behavior. Signaling by oxytocin, a neuro-
with evidence of reduced anterior cingulated cortex peptide, has been linked with empathic skill (Domes
activity in MDD. his is hardly surprising given that et al., 2007). In animal models, central release of oxy-
hopelessness and helplessness, polar opposites of opti- tocin and vasopressin regulates anxiety and social
mism, are characteristic symptoms of depression. behavior (Storm & Tecott, 2005). In rodent species,
oxytocin and vasopressin increase social recognition,
Meaning-making pair bonding, and ailiation (Insel, 1997). In humans,
he ability to ind meaning in even the most distressing administration of intranasal oxytocin to healthy men
events is another cognitive strategy that can efectively was associated with increased trust. his change may

17
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

be mediated by the amygdala (Kosfeld et al., 2005). In proven efective in eliciting durable increases in resili-
a separate investigation, oxytocin reduced amygdala ence to work-related and terrorism-related stress
activation in response to fear-inducing visual stimuli, (Butler et al., 2009; Willert et al., 2009). Certain forms
and reduced connectivity between the amygdala and of psychotherapy appear to enhance psychological
brainstem areas mediating autonomic and behavioral attributes associated with resilience (Lee et al., 2004).
fear responses (Kirsch et al., 2005). It has been pro- In addition, the US military has identiied minimiza-
posed that oxytocin and vasopressin may enhance the tion of the psychiatric burden of war as a high prior-
reward value of social stimuli and reduce potential fear ity and is just beginning the largest-scale study to date
responses (Skuse & Gallagher, 2009). of psychosocial resilience-enhancing tactics (Reed &
“Mirror neurons,” cortical neurons that ire simi- Love, 2009).
larly both when an animal performs a task themselves It is likely that new interventions will emerge with
and when the animal observes another animal of the further improvements in technology. For example, new
same species performing that task, are also poten- pharmacological agents that inluence the function of
tially relevant to the experience of empathy (Rizzolatti the HPA axis, as well as the monoamine, neuropeptide,
& Craighero, 2004). his system, in conjunction with and other neurochemical stress-response systems,
limbic brain regions, may play a role in understand- could be developed. It is even possible that individuals
ing others’ emotions and intentions (Schulte-Ruther could be trained to modulate their own brain activity
et al., 2007). In a recent study, children rated as more using real-time fMRI-based neurofeedback (Weiskopf
empathic and socially skilled than their peers evinced et al., 2004; Caria et al., 2007). As the body of resilience
greater activation in presumed mirror neuron and scholarship and its impact on the academic and clin-
associated limbic areas during imitation of emotional ical communities continues to grow, there appears to
faces (Pfeifer et al., 2008). Adults engaged in tasks that be much cause for hope.
require thinking about the mental states of both self
and other activate the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. References
Patients with lesions in this area have deicits in social
aan het Rot, M., Collins, K. A., & Fitterling, H. L. (2009).
emotions such as shame, guilt, and empathy.
Physical exercise and depression. Mount Sinai Journal of
Medicine, 76, 204–214.
Conclusions Aleck, G. & Tennen, H. (1996). Construing beneits from
his chapter has used an allostatic framework to adversity: Adaptational signiicance and dispositional
deine resilience and to explore its neurobiological underpinnings. Journal of Personality, 64, 899–922.
substrates. Given the preponderance and com- Alinito, P. D., Chen, X., Mastroeni, R., Pawlyk, A. C., &
plexity of questions still unanswered, it is clear that Deecher, D. C. (2009). Estradiol increases catecholamine
years of further study will be needed to fully under- levels in the hypothalamus of ovariectomized rats during
stand the dynamic mechanisms that support resili- the dark-phase. European Journal of Pharmacology, 616,
ent phenotypes. However, a model of resilience has 334–339.
begun to emerge from the knowledge acquired so far. Alim, T. N., Feder, A., Graves, R. E., et al. (2008). Trauma,
hroughout life, genetic and environmental agents resilience, and recovery in a high-risk African-American
(and perhaps some stochastic epigenetic events) population. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1566–1575.
interact with and reshape the circuitry responsible Anestis, S. F. (2006). Testosterone in juvenile and
for and inluenced by the stress response. Because the adolescent male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): efects
of dominance rank, aggression, and behavioral style.
integrity of these dynamic neural systems involved in
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 130, 536–545.
fear, reward, emotion regulation, and social behavior
determines the degree of resilience, resilience can also Baker, D. G., West, S. A., Orth, D. N., et al. (1997).
Cerebrospinal luid and plasma beta-endorphin in
be understood as a luid state, rather than a static trait.
combat veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder.
Consequently, it seems likely that individuals could Psychoneuroendocrinology, 22, 517–529.
become more resilient with exposure to enhancing
Baker, D. G., West, S. A., Nicholson, W. E., et al. (1999).
protective factors. Serial CSF corticotropin-releasing hormone levels
Indeed, clinicians have begun to design, imple- and adrenocortical activity in combat veterans with
ment, and evaluate translational strategies to improve posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of
resilience. Already, group-based interventions have Psychiatry, 156, 585–588.

18
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

Baker, D. G., Ekhator, N. N., Kasckow, J. W., et al. (2005). protein in the dorsal raphe of macaques. Biological
Higher levels of basal serial CSF cortisol in combat Psychiatry, 47, 562–576.
veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. American Bierer, L. M., Tischler, L., Labinsky, E., et al. (2006). Clinical
Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 992–994. correlates of 24-h cortisol and norepinephrine excretion
Bakshi, V. P., Smith-Roe, S., Newman, S. M., Grigoriadis, among subjects seeking treatment following the World
D. E., & Kalin, N. H. (2002). Reduction of stress-induced Trade Center attacks on 9/11. Annals of the New York
behavior by antagonism of corticotropin-releasing Academy of Sciences, 1071, 514–520.
hormone (CRH2) receptors in lateral septum or CRH1 Bjornebekk, A., Mathe, A. A., & Brene, S. (2010). he
receptors in amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, antidepressant efects of running and escitalopram
2926–2935. are associated with levels of hippocampal NPY and
Bale, T. L., Contarino A., Smith, G. W., et al. (2000). Mice Y1 receptor but not cell proliferation in a rat model of
deicient for corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor-2 depression. Hippocampus, 20, 820–828.
display anxiety-like behaviour and are hypersensitive to Block, J. & Kremen, A. M. (1996). IQ and ego-
stress. Nature Genetics, 24, 410–414. resiliency: Conceptual and empirical connections
Bannon, A. W., Seda, J., Carmouche, M., et al. (2000). and separateness. Journal of Personality and Social
Behavioral characterization of neuropeptide Y knockout Psychology, 70, 349–361.
mice. Brain Research, 868, 79–87. Blomqvist, A. G. & Herzog, H. (1997). Y-receptor subtypes –
Barrera, G., Hernandez, A., Poulin, J. F., et al. (2006). how many more? Trends in Neuroscience, 20, 294–298.
Galanin-mediated anxiolytic efect in rat central Bogdan, R. & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2006). Acute stress reduces
amygdala is not a result of corelease from noradrenergic reward responsiveness: Implications for depression.
terminals. Synapse, 59, 27–40. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 1147–1154.
Bast, T. (2007). Toward an integrative perspective on Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience:
hippocampal function: From the rapid encoding Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive
of experience to adaptive behavior. Reviews in the ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist,
Neurosciences, 18, 253–281. 59, 20–28.
Bastianetto, S., Ramassamy, C., Poirier, J., & Quirion, R. Boyer, E. W. & Shannon, M. (2005). he serotonin syndrome.
(1999). Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) protects New England Journal of Medicine, 352, 1112–1120.
hippocampal cells from oxidative stress-induced damage.
Brain Research, MolecularBrain Research, 66, 35–41. Bradley, R. G., Binder, E. B., Epstein, M. P., et al. (2008).
Inluence of child abuse on adult depression: Moderation
Belda, X. & Armario, A. (2009). Dopamine D1 and D2 by the corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor gene.
dopamine receptors regulate immobilization stress- Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 190–200.
induced activation of the hypothalamus–pituitary–
Bremner, J. D., Krystal, J. H., Southwick, S. M., & Charney,
adrenal axis. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 206, 355–365.
D. S. (1996). Noradrenergic mechanisms in stress and
Belfer, I., Hipp, H., McKnight, C., et al. (2006). Association anxiety: I. preclinical studies. Synapse, 23, 28–38.
of galanin haplotypes with alcoholism and anxiety in two
Bremner, J. D., Licinio, J., Darnell, A., et al. (1997). Elevated
ethnically distinct populations. Molecular Psychiatry, 11,
CSF corticotropin-releasing factor concentrations
301–311.
in posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of
Belsky, J., Jonassaint, C., Pluess, M., et al. (2009). Psychiatry, 154, 624–629.
Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Molecular
Britton, K. T., Akwa, Y., Spina, M. G., & Koob, G. F. (2000).
Psychiatry, 14, 746–754.
Neuropeptide Y blocks anxiogenic-like behavioral action
Bender, N., Heg, D., Hamilton, I. M., et al. (2006). he of corticotropin-releasing factor in an operant conlict
relationship between social status, behaviour, growth and test and elevated plus maze. Peptides, 21, 37–44.
steroids in male helpers and breeders of a cooperatively
Browne, E. S., Wright, B. E., Porter, J. R., & Svec, F. (1992).
breeding cichlid. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 173–182.
Dehydroepiandrosterone: Antiglucocorticoid action in
Berton, O., McClung, C. A., Dileone, R. J., et al. (2006). mice. American Journal of Medical Science, 303, 366–371.
Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine
Brunner, R., Schaefer, D., Hess, K., et al. (2006). Efect of
pathway in social defeat stress. Science, 311, 864–868.
high-dose cortisol on memory functions. Annals of the
Berton, O., Covington, H. E. 3rd, Ebner, K., et al. (2007). New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 434–437.
Induction of deltaFosB in the periaqueductal gray by stress
Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., Azarow, J., et al. (2009).
promotes active coping responses. Neuron, 55, 289–300.
Psychosocial predictors of resilience ater the September
Bethea, C. L., Mirkes, S. J., Shively, C. A., & Adams, M. R. 11, 2001 terrorist attacks. Journal of Nervous and Mental
(2002). Steroid regulation of tryptophan hydroxylase Disorders, 197, 266–273.

19
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Carey, M. P., Deterd, C. H., de Koning, J., Helmerhorst, F., & Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006).
de Kloet, E. R. (1995). he inluence of ovarian steroids Lending a hand: Social regulation of the neural response
on hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal regulation in the to threat. Psychological Science, 17, 1032–1039.
female rat. Journal of Endocrinology, 144, 311–321. Colangelo, L. A., Sharp, L., Kopp, P., et al. (2007). Total
Caria, A., Veit, R., Sitaram, R., et al. (2007). Regulation of testosterone, androgen receptor polymorphism,
anterior insular cortex activity using real-time fMRI. and depressive symptoms in young black and
Neuroimage 35, 1238–1246. white men: he CARDIA male hormone study.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 951–958.
Carroll, B. J., Cassidy, F., Natolowitz, D., et al. (2007).
Pathophysiology of hypercortisolism in depression. Acta Colucci, W. S., Gimbrone, M. A. Jr., McLaughlin, M. K.,
Psychiatric Scandinavica Supplement, 433, 90–103. Halpern, W., & Alexander, R. W. (1982). Increased
vascular catecholamine sensitivity and alpha-adrenergic
Carter, R. M., Macinnes, J. J., Huettel, S. A., & Adcock, R. A.
receptor ainity in female and estrogen-treated male
(2009). Activation in the VTA and nucleus accumbens
rats. Circulation Research, 50, 805–811.
increases in anticipation of both gains and losses.
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 21. Conrad, M. & Hammen, C. (1989). Role of maternal
depression in perceptions of child maladjustment. Journal
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moitt, T. E., et al. (2003). of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 663–667.
Inluence of life stress on depression: Moderation
by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, Cools, R., Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., et al. (2005).
386–389. Individual diferences in threat sensitivity predict
serotonergic modulation of amygdala response to fearful
Chang, E., Maydeu-Olivares, A., & D’Zurilla, T. J. (1997). faces. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 180, 670–679.
Optimism and pessimism as partially independent
constructs: Relationship to positive and negative Cools, R., Roberts, A. C., & Robbins, T. W. (2008).
afectivity and psychological well-being. Personality and Serotoninergic regulation of emotional and behavioural
Individual Diferences, 23, 433–440. control processes. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12, 31–40.
Charney, D. S. (2004). Psychobiological mechanisms of Coste, S. C., Kesterson, R. A., Heldwein, K. A., et al.
resilience and vulnerability: Implications for successful (2000). Abnormal adaptations to stress and impaired
adaptation to extreme stress. American Journal of cardiovascular function in mice lacking corticotropin-
Psychiatry, 161, 195–216. releasing hormone receptor-2. Nature Genetics, 24,
403–409.
Charney, D. S. & Bremner, J. D. (1999). he neurobiology of
anxiety disorders. In D. S. Charney, E. J. Nestler, & Cucinelli, F., Soranna, L., Barini, A., et al. (2002). Estrogen
B. S. Bunney (eds.), Neurobiology of mental illness treatment and body fat distribution are involved in
(pp. 494–517). New York: Oxford University Press. corticotropin and cortisol response to corticotropin-
releasing hormone in postmenopausal women.
Charney, D. S., Woods, S. W., Goodman, W. K., & Heninger, Metabolism, Clinical and Experimental, 51, 137–143.
G. R. (1987). Neurobiological mechanisms of panic
Czoty, P. W., Gould, R. W., & Nader, M. A. (2009).
anxiety: Biochemical and behavioral correlates of
Relationship between social rank and cortisol and
yohimbine-induced panic attacks. American Journal of
testosterone concentrations in male cynomolgus
Psychiatry, 144, 1030–1036.
monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Journal of
Charney, D. S., Woods, S. W., Krystal, J. H., Nagy, L. M., Neuroendocrinology, 21, 68–76.
& Heninger, G. R. (1992). Noradrenergic neuronal
Daly, R. C., Su, T. P., Schmidt, P. J., et al. (2001).
dysregulation in panic disorder: he efects of
Cerebrospinal luid and behavioral changes ater
intravenous yohimbine and clonidine in panic disorder
methyltestosterone administration: Preliminary
patients. Acta Psychiatric Scandinavica, 86, 273–282.
indings. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 172–177.
Chen, L., Dai, X. N., & Sokabe, M. (2006). Chronic Davis, M. & Whalen, P. J. (2001). he amygdala: Vigilance
administration of dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 13–34.
(DHEAS) primes for facilitated induction of long-
term potentiation via sigma 1 (sigma1) receptor: Dayan, P. & Huys, Q. J. (2009). Serotonin in afective control.
Optical imaging study in rat hippocampal slices. Annual Reviews in the Neurosciences, 32, 95–126.
Neuropharmacology, 50, 380–392. de Kloet, E. R., Joels, M., & Holsboer, F. (2005). Stress and
Chida, Y. & Hamer, M. (2008). Chronic psychosocial factors the brain: From adaptation to disease. Nature Reviews in
and acute physiological responses to laboratory-induced the Neurosciences, 6, 463–475.
stress in healthy populations: A quantitative review of de Kloet, E. R., Derijk, R. H., & Meijer, O. C. (2007).
30 years of investigations. Psychological Bulletin, 134, herapy insight: Is there an imbalanced response of
829–885. mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors in

20
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

depression? Nature Clinical and Practical Endocrinology Elovainio, M., Jokela, M., Kivimaki, M., et al. (2007).
and Metabolism, 3, 168–179. Genetic variants in the DRD2 gene moderate the
Deaner, S. L. & McConatha, J. T. (1993). he relation of relationship between stressful life events and depressive
humor to depression and personality. Psychological symptoms in adults: Cardiovascular risk in Young Finns
Reports, 72, 755–763. study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 391–395.
Debiec, J. & LeDoux, J. E. (2006). Noradrenergic signaling in Eva, C., Serra, M., Mele, P., Panzica, G., & Oberto, A.
the amygdala contributes to the reconsolidation of fear (2006). Physiology and gene regulation of the brain
memory: Treatment implications for PTSD. Annals of the NPY Y1 receptor. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 27,
New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 521–524. 308–339.
Dedovic, K., Renwick, R., Mahani, N. K., et al. (2005). he Felmingham, K., Kemp, A., Williams, L., et al. (2007).
Montreal Imaging Stress Task: Using functional imaging Changes in anterior cingulate and amygdala ater
to investigate the efects of perceiving and processing cognitive behavior therapy of posttraumatic stress
psychosocial stress in the human brain. Journal of disorder. Psychological Science, 18, 127–129.
Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 30, 319–325. Flugge, G., Kramer, M., & Fuchs, E. (2001). Chronic
Delgado, M. R., Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2006). subordination stress in male tree shrews: Replacement
Extending animal models of fear conditioning to of testosterone afects behavior and central alpha(2)-
humans. Biological Psychology, 73, 39–48. adrenoceptors. Physiology and Behavior, 73, 293–300.
Derijk, R. H. (2009). Single nucleotide polymorphisms Folkman, S. & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive afect and
related to HPA axis reactivity. Neuroimmunomodulation, the other side of coping. American Psychology, 55,
16, 340–352. 647–654.
Derijk, R. H. & de Kloet, E. R. (2008). Corticosteroid Forbes, E. E., Hariri, A. R., Martin, S. L., et al. (2009).
receptor polymorphisms: Determinants of vulnerability Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive
and resilience. European Journal of Pharmacology, 583, afect in adolescent major depressive disorder. American
303–311. Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 64–73.
Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Michel, A., Berger, C., & Franca, S. C., Barros Neto, T. L., Aqresta, M. C., Lotufo,
Herpertz, S. C. (2007). Oxytocin improves “mind- R. F., & Kater, C. E. (2006). Divergent responses of
reading” in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 731–733. serum testosterone and cortisol in athlete men ater a
marathon race. Arquivos Brasileiros de Endocrinologia e
Douma, S. L., Husban D. C., O ’ Donnell, M. E., Barwin,
Metabologia, 50, 1082–1087.
B. N., & Woodend, A. K. (2005). Estrogen-related mood
disorders: Reproductive life cycle factors. Advances in Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). he role of positive emotions in
Nursing Science, 28, 364–375. positive psychology. he broaden-and-build theory of
positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
Drabant, E. M., Hariri, A. R., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., et al.
(2006). Catechol O-methyltransferase val158met genotype Gallagher, P. & Young, A. (2002). Cortisol/DHEA ratios in
and neural mechanisms related to afective arousal and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 26, 410.
regulation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 1396–1406. Geracioti, T. D. Jr., Baker, D. G., Ekhator, N. N., et al. (2001).
Duman, R. S. & Monteggia, L. M. (2006). A neurotrophic CSF norepinephrine concentrations in posttraumatic
model for stress-related mood disorders. Biological stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158,
Psychiatry, 59, 1116–1127. 1227–1230.
Edwards, D. A., Wetzel, K., & Wyner, D. R. (2006). Gillespie, N. A., Whitield, J. B., Williams, B., Heath, A. C., &
Intercollegiate soccer: Saliva cortisol and testosterone are Martin, N. G. (2005). he relationship between stressful
elevated during competition, and testosterone is related life events, the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR)
to status and social connectedness with teammates. genotype and major depression. Psychological Medicine,
Physiology and Behavior, 87, 135–143. 35, 101–111.
Edwards, D. A. & O’Neal, J. (2009). Oral contraceptives Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2008).
decrease saliva testosterone but do not afect the rise he neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and
in testosterone associated with athletic competition. suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63,
Hormones and Behavior, 56, 195–198. 577–586.
Eisch, A. J., Bolanos, C. A., de Wit, J., et al. (2003). Brain- Goldman, S. L., Kraemer, D. T., & Salovey, P. (1996). Beliefs
derived neurotrophic factor in the ventral midbrain- about mood moderate the relationship of stress to
nucleus accumbens pathway: A role in depression. illness and symptom reporting. Journal of Psychosomatic
Biological Psychiatry, 54, 994–1005. Research, 41, 115–128.
Elder, G. H. Jr. (1998). he life course as developmental Goldstein, L. E., Rasmusson, A. M., Bunney, B. S., & Roth,
theory. Child Development, 69, 1–12. R. H. (1996). Role of the amygdala in the coordination
21
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

of behavioral, neuroendocrine, and prefrontal cortical variation of the serotonin transporter. Nature
monoamine responses to psychological stress in the rat. Neuroscience, 8, 20–21.
Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 4787–4798. Heisler, L. K., Chu, H. M., Brennan, T. J., et al. (1998). Elevated
Goodyer, I. M., Herbert, J., & Altham, P. M. (1998). anxiety and antidepressant-like responses in serotonin
Adrenal steroid secretion and major depression in 8- to 5-HT1A receptor mutant mice. Proceedings of the National
16-year-olds, III. Inluence of cortisol/DHEA ratio at Academy of Sciences of the USA, 95, 15049–15054.
presentation on subsequent rates of disappointing life Hermans, E. J., Putman P., Baas, J. M., Koppeschaar, H. P.,
events and persistent major depression. Psychological & van Honk, J. (2006). A single administration of
Medicine, 28, 265–273. testosterone reduces fear-potentiated startle in humans.
Goyal, S. N., Upadhya, M. A., Kokare, D. M., Bhisikar, S. M., Biological Psychiatry, 59, 872–874.
& Subhedar, N. K. (2009). Neuropeptide Y modulates Hermans, E. J., Putman P., Baas, J. M., et al. (2007).
the antidepressant activity of imipramine in olfactory Exogenous testosterone attenuates the integrated
bulbectomized rats: Involvement of NPY Y1 receptors. central stress response in healthy young women.
Brain Research, 1266, 45–53. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 1052–1061.
Green, S., Walter, P., Kumar, V., et al. (1986). Human Hermans, E. J., Ramsey, N. F., & van Honk, J. (2008).
oestrogen receptor cDNA: Sequence, expression and Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to
homology to v-Erb-A. Nature, 320, 134–139. social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in
Gross, C., Zhuang, X., Stark, K., et al. (2002). Serotonin 1A humans. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 263–270.
receptor acts during development to establish normal Hilke, S., heodorsson, A., Fetissov, S., et al. (2005).
anxiety-like behaviour in the adult. Nature, 416, 396–400. Estrogen induces a rapid increase in galanin levels
Gutman, A. R., Yang, Y., Ressler, K. J., & Davis, M. in female rat hippocampal formation: Possibly a
(2008). he role of neuropeptide Y in the expression nongenomic/indirect efect. European Journal of
and extinction of fear-potentiated startle. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 2089–2099.
Neuroscience, 28, 12682–12690. Hilke, S., Holm, L., Man, K., Hökfelt, T., & heodorsson,
Handwerger, K. (2009). Diferential patterns of HPA activity E. (2009). Rapid change of neuropeptide Y levels and
and reactivity in adult posttraumatic stress disorder and gene-expression in the brain of ovariectomized mice
major depressive disorder. Harvard Reviews of Psychiatry, ater administration of 17β-estradiol. Neuropeptides, 43,
17, 184–205. 327–332.
Heilig, M., McLeod, S., Brot, M., et al. (1993). Anxiolytic- Hökfelt, T., Broberger, C., Zhang, X., et al. (1998).
like action of neuropeptide Y: Mediation by Y1 receptors Neuropeptide Y: Some viewpoints on a multifaceted
in amygdala, and dissociation from food intake efects. peptide in the normal and diseased nervous system.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 8, 357–363. Brain Research Reviews, 26, 154–166.
Heilig, M., Koob, G. F., Ekman, R., & Britton, K. T. (1994). Holmes, A. & Picciotto, M. R. (2006). Galanin: A novel
Corticotropin-releasing factor and neuropeptide Y: Role therapeutic target for depression, anxiety disorders and
in emotional integration. Trends in Neuroscience, 17, drug addiction? CNS Neurology Disorders and Drug
80–85. Targets 5, 225–232.
Heilig, M., Zachrisson, O., horsell, A., et al. (2004). Holmes, A., Kinney, J. W., Wrenn, C. C., et al. (2003).
Decreased cerebrospinal luid neuropeptide Y (NPY) Galanin GAL-R1 receptor null mutant mice display
in patients with treatment refractory unipolar major increased anxiety-like behavior speciic to the elevated
depression: Preliminary evidence for association with plus-maze. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 1031–1044.
preproNPY gene polymorphism. Journal of Psychiatric Hopper, J. W., Pitman, R. K., Su, Z., et al. (2008). Probing
Research, 38, 113–121. reward function in posttraumatic stress disorder:
Heim, C. & Nemerof, C. B. (2001). he role of childhood Expectancy and satisfaction with monetary gains and
trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety losses. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 802–807.
disorders: Preclinical and clinical studies. Biological Hyman, S. E., Malenka, R. C., & Nestler, E. J. (2006).
Psychiatry, 49, 1023–1039. Neural mechanisms of addiction: he role of reward-
Heinz, A. & Smolka, M. N. (2006). he efects of catechol related learning and memory. Annual Reviews in the
O-methyltransferase genotype on brain activation Neurosciences, 29, 565–598.
elicited by afective stimuli and cognitive tasks. Reviews Inoue, T., Koyama, T., & Yamashita, I. (1993). Efect of
in the Neurosciences, 17, 359–367. conditioned fear stress on serotonin metabolism in the
Heinz, A., Braus, D. F., Smolka, M. N., et al. (2005). rat brain. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 44,
Amygdala–prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic 371–374.

22
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

Insel, T. R. (1997). A neurobiological basis of social Klohnen, E. C. (1996). Conceptual analysis and
attachment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 726–735. measurement of the construct of ego-resiliency. Journal
Inslicht, S. S., Marmar, C. R., Neylan, T. C., et al. (2006). of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1067–1079.
Increased cortisol in women with intimate partner Komesarof, P. A., Esler, M. D., & Sudhir, K. (1999).
violence-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Annals of Estrogen supplementation attenuates glucocorticoid
the New York Academy of Sciences, 1071, 428–429. and catecholamine responses to mental stress
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). in perimenopausal women. Journal of Clinical
Positive afect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal Endocrinology and Metabolism, 84, 606–610.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131. Korte, S. M., Koolhaas, J. M., Wingield, J. C., & McEwen,
Jabbi, M., Korf, J., Kema, I. P., et al. (2007). Convergent B. S. (2005). he Darwinian concept of stress: Beneits of
genetic modulation of the endocrine stress response allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-ofs
involves polymorphic variations of 5-HTT, COMT and in health and disease. Neuroscience and BioBehavioral
MAOA. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 483–490. Reviews, 29, 3–38.
Johnstone, T., van Reekum, C. M., Urry, H. L., Kalin, N. H., Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., &
& Davidson, R. J. (2007). Failure to regulate: Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans.
Counterproductive recruitment of top-down prefrontal- Nature, 435, 673–676.
subcortical circuitry in major depression. Journal of Kozlovsky, N., Matar, M. A., Kaplan, Z., Zohar, J., &
Neuroscience, 27, 8877–8884. Cohen, H. (2009). he role of the galaninergic system in
Kaga, T., Inui, A., Okita, M., et al. (2001). Modest modulating stress-related responses in an animal model
overexpression of neuropeptide Y in the brain leads of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 65,
to obesity ater high-sucrose feeding. Diabetes, 50, 383–391.
1206–1210. Krishnan, V., Han, M. H., Graham, D. L., et al. (2007).
Karlsson, R. M. & Holmes, A. (2006). Galanin as a Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and
modulator of anxiety and depression and a therapeutic resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell,
target for afective disease. Amino Acids, 31, 231–239. 131, 391–404.
Karlsson, R. M., Holmes, A., Heilig, M., & Crawley, J. N. Krishnan, V., Han, M. H., Mazei-Robison, M., et al. (2008).
(2005). Anxiolytic-like actions of centrally-administered AKT signaling within the ventral tegmental area
neuropeptide Y, but not galanin, in C57BL/6J mice. regulates cellular and behavioral responses to stressful
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 80, 427–436. stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 691–700.
Khoshbouei, H., Cecchi, M., Dove, S., Javors, M., & Kuiper, G. G., Enmark, E., Pelto-Huikko, M., Nilsson, S.,
Morilak, D. A. (2002). Behavioral reactivity to stress: & Gustafsson, J. A. (1996). Cloning of a novel receptor
Ampliication of stress-induced noradrenergic activation expressed in rat prostate and ovary. Proceedings of the
elicits a galanin-mediated anxiolytic efect in central National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 93, 5925–5930.
amygdala. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 71, Lawford, B. R., Young, R., Noble, E. P., Kann, B., & Ritchie, T.
407–417. (2006). he D2 dopamine receptor (DRD2) gene is
Kim, J. M., Stewart, R., Kim, S. W., et al. (2007). Interactions associated with co-morbid depression, anxiety and social
between life stressors and susceptibility genes dysfunction in untreated veterans with post-traumatic
(5-HTTLPR and BDNF) on depression in Korean elders. stress disorder. European Psychiatry, 21, 180–185.
Biological Psychiatry, 62, 423–428. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-
Kimonides, V. G., Khatibi, N. H., Svendsen, C. N., relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46,
Sofroniew, M. V., & Herbert, J. (1998). 819–834.
Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and DHEA-sulfate Lee, V., Cohen, S. R., Edgar, L., Laizner, A. M., & Gagnon,
(DHEAS) protect hippocampal neurons against A. J. (2004). Clarifying “meaning” in the context of
excitatory amino acid-induced neurotoxicity. Proceedings cancer research: A systematic literature review. Palliative
of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 95, Support Care, 2, 291–303.
1852–1857. Lemieux, A. M. & Coe, C. L. (1995). Abuse-related
Kirsch, P., Esslinger, C., Chen, Q., et al. (2005). Oxytocin posttraumatic stress disorder: Evidence for chronic
modulates neural circuitry for social cognition and fear neuroendocrine activation in women. Psychosomatic
in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 11489–11493. Medicine, 57, 105–115.
Klangkalya, B. & Chan, A. (1988). he efects of ovarian Levenson, R. W. (2003). Blood, sweat, and fears: he
hormones on beta-adrenergic and muscarinic receptors autonomic architecture of emotion. Annals of the New
in rat heart. Life Science, 42, 2307–2314. York Academy of Sciences, 1000, 348–366.

23
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Liberzon, I. & Sripada, C. S. (2008). he functional McEwen, B. S. (2002). Estrogen actions throughout
neuroanatomy of PTSD: A critical review. Progress in the brain. Recent Progress in Hormonal Research, 57,
Brain Research, 167, 151–169. 357–384.
Lin, S., Boey, D., & Herzog, H. (2004). NPY and Y receptors: McEwen, B. S. (2003). Mood disorders and allostatic load.
Lessons from transgenic and knockout models. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 200–207.
Neuropeptides, 38, 189–200. McEwen, B. S. & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the individual:
Lopez, J. F., Chalmers, D. T., Little, K. Y., & Watson, S. J. (1998). Mechanisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal
A.E. Bennett Research Award. Regulation of serotonin 1A, Medicine, 153, 2093–2101.
glucocorticoid, and mineralocorticoid receptor in rat and McGaugh, J. L. (2004). he amygdala modulates the
human hippocampus: Implications for the neurobiology of consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing
depression. Biological Psychiatry, 43, 547–573. experiences. Annual Reviews in the Neurosciences, 27, 1–28.
Lu, X., Ross, B., Sanchez-Alavez, M., Zorrilla, E. P., & Meaney, M. J. & Szyf, M. (2005). Environmental
Bartfai, T. (2008a). Phenotypic analysis of GalR2 programming of stress responses through DNA
knockout mice in anxiety- and depression-related methylation: Life at the interface between a dynamic
behavioral tests. Neuropeptides, 42, 387–397. environment and a ixed genome. Dialogues in Clinical
Lu, A., Steiner, M. A., Whittle, N., et al. (2008b). Conditional Neuroscience, 7, 103–123.
mouse mutants highlight mechanisms of corticotropin- Mikkelsen, J. D. & Woldbye, D. P. (2006). Association of
releasing hormone efects on stress-coping behavior. the 5-HT2A receptor gene polymorphism 102T/C with
Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 1028–1042. ischemic stroke. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40,
Lukkes, J., Vuong, S., Scholl, J., Oliver, H., & Forster, G. 153–159.
(2009). Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor Mize, A. L., Poisner, A. M., & Alper, R. H. (2001). Estrogens
antagonism within the dorsal raphe nucleus reduces act in rat hippocampus and frontal cortex to produce
social anxiety-like behavior ater early-life social rapid, receptor-mediated decreases in serotonin
isolation. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 9955–9960. 5-HT(1A) receptor function. Neuroendocrinology, 73,
Lyons, D. M. & Parker, K. J. (2007). Stress inoculation- 166–174.
induced indications of resilience in monkeys. Journal of Mobbs, D., Greicius, M. D., Abdel-Azim, E., Menon, V., &
Trauma and Stress, 20, 423–433. Reiss, A. L. (2003). Humor modulates the mesolimbic
Maggi, A. & Perez, J. (1986). Estrogen-induced up-regulation reward centers. Neuron, 40, 1041–1048.
of gamma-aminobutyric acid receptors in the CNS of Monk, C. S., Klein, R. G., Telzer, E. H., et al. (2008).
rodents. Journal of Neurochemistry, 47, 1793–1797. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens activation to
Maier, S. F., Amat, J., Baratta, M. V., Paul, E., & Watkins, L. R. emotional facial expressions in children and adolescents
(2006). Behavioral control, the medial prefrontal cortex, at risk for major depression. American Journal of
and resilience. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 8, Psychiatry, 165, 90–98.
397–406. Moran, J. M., Wig, G. S., Adams, R. B. Jr., Janata, P., & Kelley,
Manne, S., Duhamel, K., Ostrof, J., et al. (2003). Coping W. M. (2004). Neural correlates of humor detection and
and the course of mother’s depressive symptoms during appreciation. Neuroimage, 21, 1055–1060.
and ater pediatric bone marrow transplantation. Morin, R. & Starka, L. (2001). Neurosteroid
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent 7-hydroxylation products in the brain. International
Psychiatry, 42, 1055–1068. Reviews in Neurobiology, 46, 79–95.
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic. Resilience processes Morgan, C. A. 3rd, Wang, S., Southwick, S. M., et al.
in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. (2000). Plasma neuropeptide-Y concentrations in
Masten, A. S. & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). he development humans exposed to military survival training. Biological
of competence in favorable and unfavorable Psychiatry, 47, 902–909.
environments. Lessons from research on successful Morgan, C. A. 3rd, Southwick, S., Hazlett, G., et al. (2004).
children. American Psychologist, 53, 205–220. Relationships among plasma dehydroepiandrosterone
Masten, A. S., Coatsworth, J. D., Neemann, J., et al. (1995). sulfate and cortisol levels, symptoms of dissociation,
he structure and coherence of competence from and objective performance in humans exposed to acute
childhood through adolescence. Child Development, 66, stress. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 819–825.
1635–1659. Morrow, B. A., Elsworth, J. D., Rasmusson, A. M., & Roth,
McCabe, C., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2009). Neural R. H. (1999). he role of mesoprefrontal dopamine
representation of reward in recovered depressed patients. neurons in the acquisition and expression of conditioned
Psychopharmacology (Berl), 205, 667–677. fear in the rat. Neuroscience, 92, 553–564.

24
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

Moses, E. L., Drevets, W. C., Smith, G., et al. (2000). Efects Orr, S. P., Milad, M. R., Metzger, L. J., et al. (2006). Efects
of estradiol and progesterone administration on human of beta blockade, PTSD diagnosis, and explicit threat on
serotonin 2A receptor binding: A PET study. Biological the extinction and retention of an aversively conditioned
Psychiatry, 48, 854–860. response. Biological Psychology, 73, 262–271.
Munafo, M. R., Brown, S. M., & Hariri, A. R. (2008). Otte, C., Neylan, T. C., Pole, N., et al. (2005). Association
Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and between childhood trauma and catecholamine response
amygdala activation: A meta-analysis. Biological to psychological stress in police academy recruits.
Psychiatry, 63, 852–857. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 27–32.
Munafo, M. R., Durrant, C., Lewis, G., & Flint, J. (2009). Parks, C. L., Robinson, P. S., Sibille, E., Shenk, T., & Toth, M.
Gene X environment interactions at the serotonin (1998). Increased anxiety of mice lacking the serotonin
transporter locus. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 211–219. 1A receptor. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Munn, M. A., Alexopoulos, J., Nishino, T., et al. (2007). Sciences of the USA, 95, 10734–10739.
Amygdala volume analysis in female twins with major Pascucci, T., Ventura, R., Latagliata, E. C., Cabib, S., &
depression. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 415–422. Puglisi-Allegra, S. (2007). he medial prefrontal cortex
Nemerof, C. B. (2002). Recent advances in the neurobiology determines the accumbens dopamine response to stress
of depression. Psychopharmacological Bulletin, 36(Suppl 2), through the opposing inluences of norepinephrine and
6–23. dopamine. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2796–2804.
Nestler, E. J. & Carlezon, W. A. Jr. (2006). he mesolimbic Pattij, T., Groenink, L., Hijzen, T. H., et al. (2002).
dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biological Autonomic changes associated with enhanced
Psychiatry, 59, 1151–1159. anxiety in 5-HT(1A) receptor knockout mice.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 380–390.
Neumeister, A., Charney, D. S., Belfer, I., et al. (2005).
Sympathoneural and adrenomedullary functional efects Pecins-hompson, M., Brown, N. A., & Bethea, C. L. (1998).
of alpha2C-adrenoreceptor gene polymorphism in healthy Regulation of serotonin re-uptake transporter mRNA
humans. Pharmacogenetics and Genomics, 15, 143–149. expression by ovarian steroids in rhesus macaques. Brain
Research, Molecular Brain Research, 53, 120–129.
New, A. S., Fan J., Murrough, J. W., et al. (2009). A functional
magnetic resonance imaging study of deliberate emotion Perez, S. E., Wynick, D., Steiner, R. A., & Mufson, E. J.
regulation in resilience and posttraumatic stress (2001). Distribution of galaninergic immunoreactivity
disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 656–664. in the brain of the mouse. Journal of Comparative
Neurology, 434, 158–185.
Nikisch, G. & Mathe, A. A. (2008). CSF monoamine
metabolites and neuropeptides in depressed patients Pezawas, L., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Drabant, E. M., et al. (2005).
before and ater electroconvulsive therapy. European 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-
Psychiatry, 23, 356–359. amygdala interactions: A genetic susceptibility mechanism
for depression. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 828–834.
Nikisch, G., Agren, H., Eap, C. B., et al. (2005).
Neuropeptide Y and corticotropin-releasing Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M.
hormone in CSF mark response to antidepressive (2008). Mirroring others’ emotions relates to empathy
treatment with citalopram. International Journal of and interpersonal competence in children. Neuroimage,
Neuropsychopharmacology, 8, 403–410. 39, 2076–2085.
Nitschke, J. B., Sarinopoulos I., Oathes, D. J., et al. (2009). Phillips, M. L., Drevets, W. C., Rauch, S. L., & Lane, R.
Anticipatory activation in the amygdala and anterior (2003a). Neurobiology of emotion perception, I: he
cingulate in generalized anxiety disorder and prediction neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biological
of treatment response. American Journal of Psychiatry, Psychiatry, 54, 504–514.
166, 302–310. Phillips, M. L., Drevets, W. C., Rauch, S. L., & Lane, R.
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., et al. (2004). For (2003b). Neurobiology of emotion perception, II:
better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the Implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biological
cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Psychiatry, 54, 515–528.
Neuroimage. 23, 483–499. Pitman, R. K., Sanders, K. M., Zusman, R. M., et al. (2002).
O’Connor, D. B. (2002). Exogenous testosterone, aggression, Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic
and mood in eugonadal and hypogonadal men. stress disorder with propranolol. Biological Psychiatry,
Physiology and Behavior, 75, 557–566. 51, 189–192.
Oliveira, T., Gouveia, M. J., & Oliveira, R. F. (2009). Pizzagalli, D. A., Holmes, A. J., Dillon, D. G., et al. (2009).
Testosterone responsiveness to winning and Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to
losing experiences in female soccer players. rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 1056–1064. disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 702–710.

25
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Pope, H. G. Jr., Kouri, E. M., & Hudson, J. I. (2000). Efects Rose, K. A., Stapleton, G., Dott, K., et al. (1997). Cyp7b, a
of supraphysiologic doses of testosterone on mood and novel brain cytochrome P450, catalyzes the synthesis of
aggression in normal men: A randomized controlled neurosteroids 7alpha-hydroxy dehydroepiandrosterone
trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 133–140; and 7alpha-hydroxy pregnenolone. Proceedings of the
discussion 155–156. National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 94, 4925–4930.
Power, M. & Dalgleish, T. (1997). Cognition and emotion Rosenfeld, R. S., Hellman, L., Rofwarg, H., et al. (1971).
from order to disorder. New York: Taylor and Francis. Dehydroisoandrosterone is secreted episodically and
Puglisi-Allegra, S., Kempf, E., & Cabib, S. (1990). Role of synchronously with cortisol by normal man. Journal of
genotype in the adaptation of the brain dopamine system Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 33, 87–92.
to stress. Neuroscience, BioBehavioral Reviews, 14, 523–528. Rutter, M. (1985). Resilience in the face of adversity.
Puglisi-Allegra, S., Imperato, A., Angelucci, L., & Cabib, S. protective factors and resistance to psychiatric disorder.
(1991). Acute stress induces time-dependent responses British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 598–611.
in dopamine mesolimbic system. Brain Research, 554, Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts
217–222. for scientiic understanding. Annals of the New York
Raap, D. K., DonCarlos, L., Garcia, F., et al. (2000). Estrogen Academy of Sciences, 1094, 1–12.
desensitizes 5-HT(1A) receptors and reduces levels of Sah, R., Ekhator, N. N., Strawn, J. R., et al. (2009). Low
G(z), G(i1) and G(i3) proteins in the hypothalamus. cerebrospinal luid neuropeptide Y concentrations in
Neuropharmacology, 39, 1823–1832. posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66,
Rabkin, J. G., McElhiney, M. C., Rabkin, R., McGrath, P. J., 705–707.
& Ferrando, S. J. (2006). Placebo-controlled trial of Sailer, U., Robinson, S., Fischmeister, F. P., et al. (2008).
dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) for treatment of Altered reward processing in the nucleus accumbens and
nonmajor depression in patients with HIV/AIDS. mesial prefrontal cortex of patients with posttraumatic
American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 59–66. stress disorder. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2836–2844.
Raine, A. & Yang, Y. (2006). Neural foundations to moral Sajdyk, T. J., Johnson, P. L., Leitermann, R. J., et al. (2008).
reasoning and antisocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Neuropeptide Y in the amygdala induces long-term
Afective Neuroscience, 1, 203–213. resilience to stress-induced reductions in social responses
Rajarao, SJR., Platt, B., Sukof, S. J., et al. (2007). Anxiolytic- but not hypothalamic-adrenal-pituitary axis activity or
like activity of the non-selective galanin receptor agonist, hyperthermia. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 893–903.
galnon. Neuropeptides, 41, 307–320. Sanchez, M. M., Young, L. J., Plotsky, P. M., & Insel, T. R.
Rasmusson, A. M., Vasek J., Lipschitz, D. S., et al. (1999). Autoradiographic and in situ hybridization
(2004). An increased capacity for adrenal DHEA localization of corticotropin-releasing factor 1 and
release is associated with decreased avoidance and 2 receptors in nonhuman primate brain. Journal of
negative mood symptoms in women with PTSD. Comparative Neurology, 408, 365–377.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 1546–1557. Schelling, G., Roozendaal, B., Krauseneck, T., et al. (2006).
Rauch, S. L., Shin, L. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2006). Eicacy of hydrocortisone in preventing posttraumatic
Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder stress disorder following critical illness and major
and extinction: Human neuroimaging research – past, surgery. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
present, and future. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 376–382. 1071, 46–53.
Reed, J. & Love, S. (2009). Army developing master Schulte-Ruther, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Fink, G. R., &
resiliency training. Military Health System News, August Pieke, M. (2007). Mirror neuron and theory of mind
17. Washington, DC: Department of Defense, http:// mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions:
www.health.mil/News_And_Multimedia/News/ A functional magnetic resonance imaging approach
detail/09–08–17/Army_Developing_Master_Resiliency_ to empathy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19,
Training.aspx (accessed February 16, 2011). 1354–1372.
Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., et al. (2009). Interaction Setchell, J. M., Smith, T., Wickings, E. J., & Knapp, L. A.
between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), (2008). Social correlates of testosterone and
stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta- ornamentation in male mandrills. Hormones and
analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, Behavior, 54, 365–372.
301, 2462–2471. Sevcik, J., Finta, E. P., & Illes, P. (1993). Galanin receptors
Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004). he mirror-neuron inhibit the spontaneous iring of locus coeruleus
system. Annual Reviews in the Neurosciences, 27, neurones and interact with μ-opioid receptors. European
169–192. Journal of Pharmacology, 230, 223–230.

26
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., & Phelps, E. A. and SNAP 398. Proceedings of the National Academy of
(2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Sciences of the USA, 102, 17489–17494.
Nature, 450, 102–105. Tache, Y. & Bonaz, B. (2007). Corticotropin-releasing
Sibille, E., Pavlides, C., Benke, D., & Toth, M. (2000). factor receptors and stress-related alterations of gut
Genetic inactivation of the serotonin(1A) receptor in motor function. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 117,
mice results in downregulation of major GABA(A) 33–40.
receptor alpha subunits, reduction of GABA(A) receptor Terburg, D., Morgan, B., & van Honk, J. (2009). he
binding, and benzodiazepine-resistant anxiety. Journal of testosterone–cortisol ratio: A hormonal marker for
Neuroscience, 20, 2758–2765. proneness to social aggression. International Journal of
Siegrist, J., Menrath, I., Stocker, T., et al. (2005). Diferential Law Psychiatry, 32, 216–223.
brain activation according to chronic social reward hiele, T. E., Marsh, D. J., St. Marie, L., Bernstein, I. L.,
frustration. Neuroreport, 16, 1899–1903. & Palmiter, R. D. (1998). Ethanol consumption and
Silver, J. M., Sandberg, D. P., & Hales, R. E. (1990). New resistance are inversely related to neuropeptide Y levels.
approaches in the pharmacotherapy of posttraumatic Nature 396, 366–369.
stress disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 51(Suppl), horson, J. A. & Powell, F. C. (1994). Depression and sense
33–38; discussion 44–46. of humor. Psychological Reports, 75, 1473–1474.
Simson, P. E. & Weiss, J. M. (1988). Altered activity of the Tiet, Q. Q., Bird, H. R., Davies, M., et al. (1998). Adverse life
locus coeruleus in an animal model of depression. events and resilience. Journal of American Academy of
Neuropsychopharmacology, 1, 287–295. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 1191–1200.
Skuse, D. H. & Gallagher, L. (2009). Dopaminergic- Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation
neuropeptide interactions in the social brain. Trends in of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature
Cognitive Science, 13, 27–35. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 10, 397–409.
Smolka, M. N., Schumann, G., Wrase, J., et al. (2005). Unschuld, P. G., Ising, M., Erhardt, A., et al. (2008).
Catechol-O-methyltransferase VAL158MET genotype Polymorphisms in the galanin gene are associated with
afects processing of emotional stimuli in the amygdala and symptom severity in female patients sufering from panic
prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 836–842. disorder. Journal of Afective Disorders, 105, 177–184.
Solomon, M. B. & Herman, J. P. (2009). Sex diferences Vaillant, G. E. (1992). Ego mechanisms of defense: A guide for
in psychopathology: Of gonads, adrenals and mental clinicians and researchers. Washington, DC: American
illness. Physiology and Behavior, 97, 250–258. Psychiatric Press.
Southwick, S. M., Krystal, J. H., Bremner, J. D., et al. van Honk, J. & Schutter, D. J. (2007). Testosterone reduces
(1997). Noradrenergic and serotonergic function conscious detection of signals serving social correction:
in posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Implications for antisocial behavior. Psychological
Psychiatry, 54, 749–758. Science, 18, 663–667.
Southwick, S. M., Vythilingam, M., & Charney, D. S. (2005). van Honk, J., Tuiten, A., Verbaten, R., et al. (1999).
he psychobiology of depression and resilience to stress: Correlations among salivary testosterone, mood, and
Implications for prevention and treatment. Annual selective attention to threat in humans. Hormones and
Reviews in Clinical Psychology, 1, 255–291. Behavior, 36, 17–24.
Sterling, P. & Eyer, J. (1988). Allostasis: A new paradigm to van Honk, J., Peper, J. S., & Schutter, DJLG. (2005).
explain arousal pathways. In S. R. Fisher (ed.), Handbook Testosterone reduces unconscious fear but not
of life stress, cognition, and health (pp. 629–649). New consciously experienced anxiety: Implications for the
York: Wiley. disorders of fear and anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 58,
Storm, E. E. & Tecott, L. H. (2005). Social circuits: 218–225.
Peptidergic regulation of mammalian social behavior. Verhagen, M., van der Meij, A., Janzing, J. G., et al. (2009).
Neuron, 47, 483–486. Efect of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in the serotonin
Su, T. P., Pagliaro, M., Schmidt, P. J., et al. (1993). transporter gene on major depressive disorder and
Neuropsychiatric efects of anabolic steroids in male related comorbid disorders. Psychiatric Genetics, 19,
normal volunteers. Journal of the American Medical 39–44.
Association, 269, 2760–2764. Voisey, J., Swagell, C. D., Hughes, I. P., et al. (2009). he
Swanson, C. J., Blackburn, T. P., Zhang, X., et al. (2006). DRD2 gene 957C>T polymorphism is associated with
Anxiolytic- and antidepressant-like proiles of the posttraumatic stress disorder in war veterans. Depression
galanin-3 receptor (Gal3) antagonists SNAP 37889 and Anxiety, 26, 28–33.

27
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Vreeburg, S. A., Hoogendijk, W. J., van Pelt, J., et al. (2009). Willert, M. V., hulstrup, A. M., & Hertz, J. (2009). Changes
Major depressive disorder and hypothalamic–pituitary– in stress and coping from a randomized controlled
adrenal axis activity: Results from a large cohort study. trial of a three-month stress management intervention.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 617–626. Scandinavian Journal of Work and Environmental Health,
Vythilingam, M., Nelson, E. E., Scaramozza, M., et al. 35, 145–152.
(2009). Reward circuitry in resilience to severe trauma: Wirth, M. M. & Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). Basal testosterone
An fMRI investigation of resilient special forces soldiers. moderates responses to anger faces in humans.
Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 172, 75–77. Physiology and Behavior, 90, 496–505.
Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., Wolfensberger, SPA., Veltman, D. J., Hoogendijk, WJG.,
& Ochsner, K. N. (2008). Prefrontal-subcortical Boomsma, D. I., & de Geus, EJC. (2008). Amygdala
pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. responses to emotional faces in twins discordant or
Neuron, 59, 1037–1050. concordant for the risk for anxiety and depression.
Walton, K. M., Chin, J. E., Duplantier, A. J., & Mather, R. J. Neuroimage, 41, 544–552.
(2006). Galanin function in the central nervous system. Wong, M. L., Kling, M. A., Munson, P. J., et al. (2000).
Current Opinions in Drug Discovory and Development, 9, Pronounced and sustained central hypernoradrenergic
560–570. function in major depression with melancholic features:
Waselus, M., Nazzaro, C., Valentino, R. J., & Van Relation to hypercortisolism and corticotropin-releasing
Bockstaele, E. J. (2009). Stress-induced redistribution of hormone. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
corticotropin-releasing factor receptor subtypes in the of the USA, 97, 325–330.
dorsal raphe nucleus. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 76–83. Xu, Z. Q., Tong, Y. G., & Hökfelt, T. (2001). Galanin
Watt, M. J., Burke, A. R., Renner, K. J., & Forster, G. L. enhances noradrenaline-induced outward current on
(2009). Adolescent male rats exposed to social defeat locus coeruleus noradrenergic neurons. Neuroreport, 12,
exhibit altered anxiety behavior and limbic monoamines 1779–1782.
as adults. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123, 564–576. Yehuda, R. & LeDoux, J. (2007). Response variation
Weaver, I. C., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., et al. (2004). following trauma: A translational neuroscience approach
Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature to understanding PTSD. Neuron, 56, 19–32.
Neuroscience, 7, 847–854. Yehuda, R., Boisoneau, D., Lowy, M. T., & Giller, E. L. Jr.
Weis, F., Kilger, E., Roozendaal, B., et al. (2006). Stress doses (1995). Dose–response changes in plasma cortisol
of hydrocortisone reduce chronic stress symptoms and lymphocyte glucocorticoid receptors following
and improve health-related quality of life in high-risk dexamethasone administration in combat veterans with
patients ater cardiac surgery: A randomized study. and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of
Journal of horacic and Cardiovascular Surgery, 131, General Psychiatry, 52, 583–593.
277–282. Yehuda, R., Brand, S. R., Golier, J. A., & Yang, R. K. (2006a).
Weiser, M. J., Foradori, C. D., & Handa, R. J. (2008). Clinical correlates of DHEA associated with post-
Estrogen receptor beta in the brain: From form to traumatic stress disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
function. Brain Research Reviews, 57, 309–320. 114, 187–193.
Weiskopf, N., Scharnowski, F., Veit, R., et al. (2004). Yehuda, R., Brand, S., & Yang, R. K. (2006b). Plasma
Self-regulation of local brain activity using real-time neuropeptide Y concentrations in combat exposed
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Journal veterans: Relationship to trauma exposure, recovery
of Physiology, 98, 357–373. from PTSD, and coping. Biological Psychiatry, 59,
660–663.
Wheler, G. H., Brandon, D., Clemons, A., et al. (2006).
Cortisol production rate in posttraumatic stress disorder. Young, A. H., Gallagher, P., & Porter, R. J. (2002). Elevation
Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 91, of the cortisol–dehydroepiandrosterone ratio in drug-
3486–3489. free depressed patients. American Journal of Psychiatry,
159, 1237–1239.
Whitworth, J. A., Williamson, P. M., Mangos, G., & Kelly, J. J.
(2005). Cardiovascular consequences of cortisol excess. Young, E. A. (1996). Sex diferences in response
Vascular Health and Risk Management, 1, 291–299. to exogenous corticosterone: A rat model of
hypercortisolemia. MolecularPsychiatry, 1, 313–319.
Wilkinson, M. B., Xiao, G., Kumar, A., et al. (2009).
Imipramine treatment and resiliency exhibit similar Young, E. A., Altemus, M., Parkison, V., & Shastry, S.
chromatin regulation in the mouse nucleus accumbens (2001). Efects of estrogen antagonists and agonists on
in depression models. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, the ACTH response to restraint stress in female rats.
7820–7832. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25, 881–891.

28
Chapter 1: Neurobiology of resilience

Yu, Y. Z. & Shi, J. X. (2009). Relationship between levels Zitzmann, M. (2006). Testosterone and the brain. Aging
of testosterone and cortisol in saliva and aggressive Male, 9, 195–199.
behaviors of adolescents. Biomedical and Environmental
Zitzmann, M. & Nieschlag, E. (2001). Testosterone levels in
Science, 22, 44–49.
healthy men and the relation to behavioural and physical
Zarrouf, F. A., Artz, S., Griith, J., Sirbu, C., & Kommor, M. characteristics: Facts and constructs. European Journal of
(2009). Testosterone and depression: Systematic review and Endocrinology, 144, 183–197.
meta-analysis. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 15, 289–305.

29
Section
Section1 Pathways to resilience
Chapter
Resilience in the face of stress: emotion

2 regulation as a protective factor


Allison S. Troy and Iris B. Mauss

Introduction (Figure 2.1). he present chapter will review relevant


literatures and suggest that there is indeed evidence to
Everyone experiences stress at one time or another –
support this thesis. More speciically, we will propose
from major events such as the death of a loved one,
that a speciic type of emotion regulation, cognitive
to more minor stressors such as inancial diicul-
emotion regulation, holds particular promise for con-
ties. Not surprisingly, exposure to stress is generally
tributing to resilience.
associated with a wide range of negative outcomes,
including decreased well-being, increased incidence
of disease, post-traumatic stress disorder, general- Scope of the chapter
ized anxiety disorder, and major depressive disorder he concepts we examine here are complex: the topics
(Dohrenwend & Dohrenwend, 1974; Kendler et al., of stress, mental health, and emotion regulation each
1999; Monat et al., 2007). However, not all individ- have generated a vast body of research. To maintain
uals who are exposed to even high levels of stress conceptual clarity, the chapter will focus on speciic
develop such negative outcomes. In fact, recent evi- types of stressor, speciic types of emotion regulation,
dence suggests that a considerable number of individ- and speciic types of relationship between these con-
uals exhibit resilience, which is commonly deined as structs and resilience. his is achieved as follows.
maintained or improved mental health in the face of he irst way in which the present review is nar-
stress, ater short disruptions (if any) to normal func- rowed is by focusing on stressful life events from
tioning (Freitas & Downey, 1998; Rutter, 1999; Luthar among the various types of stressor that can afect
et al., 2000; Bonanno, 2005). Note that this deinition, people (Sarason et al., 1978; Lu, 1994; Ensel & Lin,
which we adapt here, conceptualizes resilience as a 1996; Mazure et al., 2000; Tennant, 2002). Common
potential outcome ater exposure to stress rather than stressful life events include relatively minor events,
a psychological trait that leads to positive outcomes such as a disagreement with a spouse, to more major
(c.f. Norris et al., 2008). events, such as the unexpected loss of a job, a serious
It, therefore, appears that, in the face of compar- illness or injury of oneself, or the death of a friend or
able stressors, some individuals exhibit signiicantly family member. Because this deinition of stress expos-
impaired functioning while others show impressive ure includes stressors that, arguably, everyone has
resilience. Understanding the factors that govern the experienced one time or another, the present model of
great individual variance in outcomes ater stress is resilience is fairly general in its implications. However,
important for understanding mental health and for the stressors will not include traumatic events such as
developing interventions and prevention programs abuse, exposure to crime, or exposure to war, or events
that foster resilience. What factors, then, might pre- that could be considered “positive” stressors such as
dict resilience? One key to this question might lie in marriage or the birth of a child, because, based on the
the fact that stressful events are inherently highly emo- current literature, it is not clear whether the proposed
tional (Sarason et al., 1978; Lazarus, 1999). For this model of resilience would generalize to these types of
reason, people’s ability to regulate emotions may be a stressor. While it is possible that cognitive emotion
critically important factor in determining resilience regulation ability is also an important contributor

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

30
Chapter 2: Resilience in the face of stress

Figure 2.1 Proposed moderation


Cognitive emotion regulation of the relationship between stressful
ability life events and resilience by cognitive
Selective emotion regulation ability. Each of the
Cognitive links in the model is indicated by a letter
attention reappraisal
control (A–E). Links D and E are positioned to
imply that D usually occurs earlier in the
emotion generative process than E, and
D E that E operates by directly changing
appraisals; however, this is not meant to
imply a strict time course with regard to
cognitive emotion regulation. The links
Stressful Adaptive
Appraisal Resilience are described in detail in the text.
life events A B emotional C
responses

to resilience with regard to trauma and positive as well as evidence that cognitive emotion regulation
events, more research is needed in order to test this is a moderator of the relationship between stress and
hypothesis. outcomes. Studies of the main efects of emotion regu-
Second, the proposed framework will focus on lation do not directly test resilience because they do
emotion regulation ability as a moderator, such that not explicitly consider the context of stress. However,
people with high cognitive emotion regulation abil- evidence of this type will be included for two reasons.
ity should exhibit increased likelihood of resilience First, the literature to date has simply focused more
ater stress exposure compared with those low in on the main efects of emotion regulation. Second,
cognitive emotion regulation ability (Figure 2.1). and more importantly, we believe that the relationship
Emotion regulation has also been proposed as a medi- between cognitive emotion regulation and positive
ator in the context of adjustment to stress (Schwartz & outcomes is likely to generalize to stressful contexts. In
Proctor, 2000; McCarthy et al., 2006; Silk et al., 2007). addition, where research has directly tested the inter-
According to such mediator models, stress exposure actions between emotion regulation and stress, it is
leads to emotion dysregulation, which, in turn, leads described as evidence that directly supports the mod-
to negative outcomes. Both mediator and moder- erator model.
ator models are useful in examining the relationship In order to lay the groundwork for the present
between stress and mental health. he present chapter review, appraisal theories of emotion and cognitive
focuses on emotion regulation as a moderating factor emotion regulation are discussed.
because a moderator model lends itself more clearly
to understanding individual diferences in resili- A framework for resilience: appraisal
ence. One implication of this approach is that emo- theory and cognitive emotion
tion regulation ability is conceptualized as a relatively
independent variable that is not strongly afected by regulation
stress exposure. his is not to say, however, that this Appraisal theories are commonly used in the ields of
ability is set in stone. Rather, we believe that the abil- emotion, emotion regulation, and coping to under-
ity to use cognitive emotion regulation is shaped and stand individuals’ emotional reactions (Lazarus &
inluenced by a number of situational and individual Folkman, 1984). he central point of appraisal theor-
factors, and it can, in turn, be lexibly applied across ies is that “the way we evaluate an event determines
a wide range of emotional contexts. According to this how we react emotionally” (Lazarus, 1999, p. 87). In
model of resilience, however, the ability to use cogni- other words, it is not a particular event that causes a
tive emotion regulation is most important in the con- particular emotion but, rather, it is a person’s subjective
text of stress. appraisals of the event that lead to an emotional reac-
In order to provide evidence for the moderator tion (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Ortony et al., 1988;
model of resilience shown in Figure 2.1, empirical Scherer, 1988; Lazarus, 1991). In this way, appraisals
evidence that supports main efects of cognitive emo- have generally been deined as the meaning and sig-
tion regulation on positive outcomes will be reviewed, niicance that a person assigns to an event or stimulus.

31
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

In support of appraisal theories, research has found context of stress, this could involve changing one’s
that people exposed to comparable events, either in appraisal to another less-threatening or more positive
the laboratory or in a naturalistic setting, will display interpretation of the event (Gross & hompson, 2007).
a wide variety of emotional reactions depending on Because CR most oten occurs ater an initial appraisal
their appraisal of the event (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; of an event has taken place, it is depicted as occurring
Smith & Ellsworth, 1987; Scherer & Ceschi, 1997; Siemer slightly later in the emotion generation process than
et al., 2007; Figure 2.1, links A and B). Because apprais- AC (Gross, 1998a; Figure 2.1, link E). We acknowledge
als appear to play such an important role in the gen- that it is possible for CR to occur either earlier or later
eration of an emotion, emotion regulation strategies in the generation of emotion than described in this def-
that target appraisals should be particularly efective. inition. In the proposed model, however, the temporal
Consequently, once an individual’s appraisal of a situ- sequence of these strategies is not critical for determin-
ation has changed, so will the emotional reaction. ing their efectiveness. Instead, we believe that the abil-
Given the centrality of appraisals in the generation ity to change appraisals is what ultimately determines
of emotion, cognitive emotion regulation strategies the efectiveness of a given strategy. Going back to the
that entail appraisal change may provide particularly public speaking example, one could initially look out
efective ways to manage emotions. Cognitive emotion at the huge audience and make the appraisal that this
regulation has been broadly deined as changing one’s audience appears quite uninterested in the impend-
attention to or one’s appraisals of a situation in order ing talk and is, therefore, threatening. hen, one could
to change an emotion’s duration, intensity, or both reappraise the situation by telling oneself that the few
(Ochsner & Gross, 2005). his deinition is in contrast people who are paying such close attention could have
to non-cognitive types of emotion regulation such as great feedback that could really help to improve the
expressive suppression, which consists of changing speech in the future, thereby decreasing anxiety. By
only the outward expression of an emotion (Gross & using CR to change appraisals of a threatening situ-
hompson, 2007). ation, individuals can efectively transform their ensu-
Two groups of cognitive emotion regulation strat- ing emotions (Figure 2.1, links E and B).
egies have shown particular promise as efective ways Clearly, there is some overlap between AC and CR.
to manage emotions: attention control (AC) and cog- Because attention and appraisals appear to be critically
nitive reappraisal (CR) (Ochsner & Gross, 2005). linked, it may be that the use of one of these strategies
Attention control consists of selectively attending makes it more likely that the other strategy will also
toward or away from certain stimuli (either internal be used (Totterdell & Parkinson, 1999). For example,
or external) in order to change their emotional impact it may become much easier for someone to engage
(Ochsner & Gross, 2005). Note that internal stimuli are in a positive reappraisal of the public-speaking situ-
included in this deinition, which refers to people’s own ation if he or she is selectively attending to the friendly
thoughts and feelings. Selective attention to certain people in the front row. Conversely, as he or she is busy
aspects of a situation changes individuals’ appraisals, reappraising the audience by considering how truly
which, in turn, changes individuals’ emotional states. unthreatening everyone is, he or she may be more
For example, consider someone who is faced with giv- likely to notice the friendly people sitting in the front
ing a speech to a large crowd of people and chooses to row and less likely to notice the people sleeping in the
focus on how friendly and engaged everyone in the irst back. Although AC and CR are, therefore, by no means
row looks. his person’s appraisal of the situation will mutually exclusive, they will be described separately to
be less threat provoking than that of someone who maintain conceptual clarity. To acknowledge the fact
instead focuses on the people who are asleep in the back that these two types of cognitive emotion regulation
row. Essentially, by iltering the afective information that are not completely separable, they are pictured as over-
enters one’s awareness in a stressful situation, the apprais- lapping circles in Figure 2.1.
als one makes will be less threatening, in turn leading to In sum, as illustrated in Figure 2.1, the appraisal–
fewer negative emotions (Figure 2.1, links A, B, and D). theoretical framework suggests that AC and CR may
In this way, AC is conceptualized as a moderator of the be linchpin processes in contributing to resilience.
relationship between stressful life events and appraisals. Speciically, these types of strategy for cognitive emo-
Cognitive reappraisal involves reframing a situ- tion regulation should contribute to resilience (link C)
ation in order to change its emotional impact. In the by helping individuals to decrease negative emotional

32
Chapter 2: Resilience in the face of stress

responses (links D and E). he following sections will 2008). his is similar to the contention of Campbell-
review evidence to support this model. Sills and Barlow (2007, p. 556) that distraction is simply
“a ‘band-aid’ approach rather than a long-term solu-
Empirical findings: attention control tion to excessive anxiety and/or depression.” Indeed,
there is evidence to suggest that distraction may be not
and resilience just inefective but even detrimental to long-term cop-
heoretically, if a person does not attend to negative ing. For example, research has found that individuals
stimuli, this person will make appraisals of the environ- who use avoidance coping strategies such as distrac-
ment that are less threatening. In turn, these changed tion when they are stressed are more likely to develop
appraisals will lead to fewer negative emotional reac- depression in the long run (Felsten, 2002; Powers et al.,
tions (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Gross & hompson, 2002; Holahan et al., 2005).
2007). For this reason, it has been widely hypothe- Given the negative long-term consequences asso-
sized that the ability to distract oneself from negative ciated with distraction, perhaps its opposite – focusing
stimuli, including one’s own negative feelings, is an on what is wrong in a given situation – could lead to more
important protective factor against long-term negative positive outcomes. his negative focus is oten referred
outcomes. In the context of depression, for example, to as rumination, which has been deined as repetitively
Nolen-Hoeksema (1991, p. 577) hypothesized that focusing on oneself, one’s negative emotions, and their
depressed individuals must “be distracted from their anticipated negative consequences (Nolen-Hoeksema,
ruminative thoughts long enough for their depressed 1991; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Unfortunately,
mood to be relieved substantially.” Laboratory studies this ruminative response style in the face of stress
of distraction support this hypothesis. For example, in decreases the likelihood of resilience. For example,
studies with depressed patients as well as normative among people who are already depressed, those who
samples, participants who distracted themselves from engage in rumination about their own depressed mood
their sad moods by completing an emotionally neu- experience signiicantly longer depressive episodes,
tral task were able to attenuate their negative feelings which suggests that rumination leads to a vicious
(Morrow & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1990; Erber & Tesser, cycle of negative emotions that are diicult to escape
1992). Similarly, in a group of depressed adolescents, (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). he use of rumination has
Park and colleagues (2004) found that participants also been associated with higher levels of anxiety and
induced to use distraction by reading about 45 neu- symptoms of post-traumatic stress (Nolen-Hoeksema
tral items (boats, kettle, etc.) decreased their depressed et al., 2008). his relationship between rumination and
mood compared with participants who read about negative outcomes also seems to be present in the con-
more emotional, self-focused items (feelings, body text of stress. In a study of caregivers for terminally ill
sensations, etc.). patients, for example, results indicated that caregivers
While these laboratory studies suggest that dis- who used more rumination in response to their nega-
traction staves of depressed mood, the positive efects tive moods were more depressed than caregivers who
associated with distraction seem to wear of with time, used less rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1994).
giving those who use distraction only short-term ben- Such correlational studies, of course, do not allow
eits. For example, Kross and Ayduk (2008, study 2) for inferences about the causal role of rumination.
found that ater a laboratory sadness induction dis- Importantly, however, a prospective study by Nolen-
traction decreased depressed mood immediately ater Hoeksema and Morrow (1991) provided some sup-
the emotion induction. However, in follow-up sessions port for the notion that rumination is a risk factor for
one day and seven days ater the induction, the pro- depression. heir questionnaire study obtained partici-
tective efects of distraction had disappeared and lev- pants’ reports of ruminative response style and depres-
els of self-reported depressed mood had signiicantly sive symptoms. By chance, 14 days later, the Loma
increased relative to other experimental groups that Prieta earthquake hit the area close to where these
had not used distraction. he authors explained these data had been collected. Participants who had previ-
results by pointing out that distraction does nothing ously reported using more of a ruminative response
to change how emotional experiences are dealt with in style were signiicantly more depressed at follow-up
future situations; consequently, the utility of distraction sessions 10 days and 50 days following the earthquake
appears to be limited to the short term (Kross & Ayduk, compared with individuals who reported using less of

33
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

a ruminative response style. hese results were signii- Furthermore, a sample of children at high risk for
cant even when controlling for depressed mood before depression selectively attended to negative facial
the earthquake, suggesting that rumination indeed expressions ater a negative mood induction, whereas
had a causal role in depressed mood. Several other children who were not at risk for depression selectively
prospective longitudinal studies using both adults and attended to positive facial expressions during an emo-
children have found a similar pattern: individuals who tional faces dot-probe task (Joormann et al., 2007).
use rumination during periods of stress are more likely hese indings suggest that negative attentional bias is
to develop depressive disorders and to experience not simply a side-efect of current depression, but may
more prolonged periods of depression in the long-run constitute a vulnerability to depression.
(reviewed by Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Taken together, these indings suggest that there is
More recent laboratory studies that have manipu- a main efect between AC and mental health outcomes.
lated the use of rumination have added further sup- Speciically, distraction, rumination, and negative
port to the contention that rumination plays a causal biases in attention are associated with vulnerabilities to
role in the occurrence of negative mood states. For negative outcomes over time. In addition, the indings
example, Singer and Dobson (2007) found that remit- reviewed above could have important implications in
ted depressed patients who were instructed to rumin- the context of stress and resilience – if an individual
ate ater a negative emotion induction had higher levels cannot efectively disengage attention away from nega-
of depressed mood compared with participants who tive aspects of themselves or the situation, the apprais-
were instructed to use distraction. Other studies using als of a stressor could become much more threatening,
normative samples have also found that instructions to the resulting negative emotions could become more
ruminate ater negative mood inductions exacerbate intense and long lasting, and resilience will become
negative mood states compared with those who were more diicult to attain. At the other extreme, however,
not instructed to ruminate (Broderick, 2005; Ray et al., those who completely distract themselves away from
2008). Overall, it seems that when people in stressful negative information enjoy short-term beneits but
situations repetitively focus their attention on their appear ill-equipped to efectively cope with further
own negative emotions, resilience becomes a less likely exposure to stressors later on.
outcome. Indeed, research using the emotional Stroop task,
In addition to the studies on rumination, research which measures individuals’ ability to ignore irrelevant
on attentional biases in depression suggest that a bias emotional material, has provided evidence that pre-
towards external negative afective stimuli (i.e., nega- existing diferences in AC can prospectively predict
tive stimuli other than one’s own emotional states) adjustment to stress. For example, MacLeod and Hagan
might also decrease the likelihood of resilience and (1992) found that women who displayed the most
lead to increased vulnerability to negative outcomes. In pronounced bias towards negative information later
several studies using paradigms such as the dot-probe reported the greatest amount of distress upon learn-
task, researchers have found that clinically depressed ing that they had been diagnosed with cervical path-
patients (Mogg et al., 1995; Mathews et al., 1996) as ology. Similarly, MacLeod (1999) found that a Stroop
well as individuals with induced or naturally occurring measure predicted Singaporean students’ emotional
dysphoria (Bradley et al., 1997) displayed a negative adjustment ater migrating to Australia. Speciically,
attentional bias, attending relatively more to negatively the students with the most pronounced negative atten-
valenced emotional stimuli than to neutral or positive tional bias reported the greatest levels of anxiety ater
stimuli. his negative attentional bias in depression has immigration.
also been found using other tasks such as the dichotic hese prospective studies support the hypothesis
listening task (Ingram et al., 1994) and an afective that individual diferences in AC can exert a moder-
interference task (Gotlib et al., 2005). ating efect on the relationship between stress and
In addition, using a negative afective priming outcomes over time (Figure 2.1, link D). Speciically,
task to measure selective attention to afective words, maladaptive forms of AC such as negative attentional
Joormann (2004) found an attentional bias towards biases appear to result in heightened negative afective
irrelevant negative distractors in participants who responses (Figure 2.1, link D) and are associated with
were currently dysphoric, as well as in non-depressed more vulnerability to negative outcomes and less resili-
individuals with a previous history of depression. ence (Figure 2.1, link C).

34
Chapter 2: Resilience in the face of stress

Conversely, then, individuals who use adaptive AC hese indings on optimists have led to the “pragmatic
should respond to a stressor with an attenuated nega- information-processing hypothesis,” which posits that
tive afective response, making resilience more likely. it is not the valence of information that drives selective
However, what constitutes “adaptive AC”? he indings attention in optimists but rather the level of personal
reviewed so far suggest a dilemma: directing attention relevance that will determine whether afective infor-
away from the negative aspects of a situation (as is done mation is attended to or not (Abele & Gendolla, 2007).
in distraction), on the one hand, is only adaptive in the With these indings in mind, it is not surprising that
short term but seems to have deleterious consequences optimists tend to exhibit greater levels of well-being
in the longer term. On the other hand, focusing on the and lower rates of depression than pessimists (Scheier
negative aspects of a situation (as is done in rumination) & Carver, 1993). By iltering out unneeded negative
appears to lead to deleterious consequences in almost information, the optimists’ appraisals of these stim-
all contexts. One answer to this dilemma might lie in uli may be less disturbing, leading, in turn, to fewer
the degree and context rather than in the kind of AC negative emotions. Although this phenomenon has
strategies used. Perhaps it is just maladaptive to com- not been examined in the context of high stress, it is
pletely and inlexibly distract oneself from or completely plausible that optimists avoid the negative efects of
and inlexibly focus on negative stimuli. In contrast, a distraction by attending to negative information when
more lexible employment of some of both strategies – it is relevant. At the same time, when negative informa-
selectively focusing attention – could lead to resilience tion becomes irrelevant, perhaps because it has been
in the face of stress. Additionally, the maladaptive processed, optimists may efectively direct attention
types of AC described above are stimulus-driven strat- away from it. Hypothetically, by attending away from
egies. Perhaps more goal-driven strategies based on an unneeded information in the environment, individ-
individual’s present goals and needs would be more uals with this type of selective attention would display
adaptive. In this way, selective AC could be employed less-threatening appraisals of the stressor in the irst
lexibly across a wide range of contexts in order to suit place, thus leading to an attenuated negative emotional
an individual’s current needs instead of according to response and resilience.
particular stimulus contingencies. Does this mean that if someone does not naturally
Indeed, studies of selective attention support possess the ability to selectively focus attention away
this hypothesis. For example, recent research using from negative stimuli, that person is doomed if stres-
eye-gaze tracking has found that individuals who sors arise? Recent studies that have examined training
are high in trait optimism – that is, individuals who paradigms for selective attentional biases suggest that
are generally characterized by high well-being – dis- the answer to this question is negative. hese studies
play an attentional bias away from negative mater- have shown that, with training, pre-existing attentional
ial (Isaacowitz, 2005). At irst glance, this looks like bias patterns can be changed (either acquired or atten-
distraction. However, research from Aspinwall and uated). For example, in a laboratory study, participants
Brunhart (1996) suggests that optimists are not using were taught to acquire attentional biases either toward
chronic pollyannaism to simply ilter out all negative or away from afectively negative versus neutral stimuli
information; instead, optimists appear to attend only to using a computer task (MacLeod et al., 2002, study 1).
what is relevant for their own well-being. For example, Ater completing the training procedure, participants
they found that when optimists who engage in habit- in the negative attention condition exhibited greater
ual sun tanning were given health information about bias towards negative stimuli while participants in
damage from ultraviolet radiation, they spent more the neutral attention condition exhibited greater bias
time reading the threatening information, whereas towards neutral stimuli. A more recent study of atten-
optimists who did not habitually sun tan did not select- tional bias training extended the indings of MacLeod
ively attend to the same information. he optimists et al. by showing that positive attentional biases can
also displayed better recall for the relevant health risk also be taught (Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2008). hese
information than for irrelevant information, adding positive biases also have observable behavioral efects
to the contention that optimists do selectively attend on subsequent tasks, such that the participants who
to negative material if it is relevant (Aspinwall & had been trained to attend to positive stimuli subse-
Brunhart, 1996). More recent research on optimism quently looked less at negative images during a stress
has replicated these results (Abele & Gendolla, 2007). induction (Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2008).

35
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

hese indings are important in suggesting that that can be improved with practice. Furthermore, they
people’s selective attention to emotional stimuli seem provide support for the hypothesis that selective AC
to be changeable. Such training procedures may have plays an important role with regard to adaptive emo-
important therapeutic value for promoting resilience, tion regulation and resilience.
particularly among individuals with pre-existing nega- Clearly, more research is needed on the clinical util-
tive attentional biases. In a follow-up study, MacLeod ity of AC. As mentioned above, blind pollyannaism is
and colleagues (2002, study 2) indeed found that partic- not the answer to coping with stress. Rather, research
ipants who had learned to exhibit a negative attentional suggests that selective AC is most adaptive, with atten-
bias responded to a stress induction with a pronounced tion paid to relevant but not irrelevant negative stimuli.
increase in negative emotions. Participants who had It is unclear, however, how people coping with real-
been trained to attend away from the negative stimuli world stressors decide which negative information is
and towards neutral stimuli did not exhibit this increase “irrelevant” and could, therefore, be safely ignored.
in negative emotion. he authors concluded that the One promising idea is that “relevant” information, in
acquired attentional biases contributed causally to these the context of stress, can best be conceptualized as the
diferences in negative emotions. hese indings are aspects of the stressor that are changeable, and there-
very important in providing evidence that individual fore subject to active problem solving and coping.
diferences in selective AC might inluence emotional Unchangeable aspects of a stressor could perhaps be
adjustment to a stressor (Figure 2.1, link D). considered “irrelevant” and iltered out using selective
It is promising to think that people who exhibit mal- AC, thereby reducing the amount of negative informa-
adaptive patterns of AC can be led down a more resilient tion that enters awareness. his hypothesis has not yet
path by learning to attend to diferent kinds of stimulus been tested, however.
in the environment. Indeed, recent work by Siegle and Taken together, the evidence reviewed in this sec-
colleagues (2008) suggests that training in selective AC tion supports the moderating efect of AC on the rela-
may be an efective treatment component for depres- tionship between stress and resilience. Speciically,
sion. hey used an adjunctive intervention for depres- strategies such as distraction and rumination appear
sion called cognitive control training – importantly, to be maladaptive: by indiscriminately diverting atten-
the protocol included AC training, in which patients tion away from negative stimuli (as in distraction),
learnt to selectively attend to certain sounds coming or indiscriminately focusing attention on negative
out of speakers, while ignoring irrelevant sounds. Ater stimuli (as in rumination), individuals who use these
receiving two weeks of this AC training (in the con- strategies are more vulnerable to outcomes such as
text of cognitive control training), patients exhibited depression in the context of stress. Although these two
greater improvements in depressive symptoms than diferent strategies appear to be opposites, ironically,
patients who received treatment as usual (Siegle et al., they both lead to negative outcomes over time, per-
2008). Notably, the AC training consisted of short ses- haps because of the cumulative aspect of inefectively
sions (15 minutes) that used non-afective stimuli such regulated negative emotions. he AC strategies that
as bird sounds. his suggests that this particular type of promote resilience, by comparison, appear to involve
AC training may be tapping into a generic ability that is selectively attending away from irrelevant negative
not simply limited to afective stimuli. In turn, this type stimuli and towards more positive or neutral stimuli,
of generic ability may make it more likely that people suggesting that a more modulated attentional response
can use it in a lexible and context-dependent way. (instead of the extreme approaches of distraction and
Although these training paradigms have not been rumination) is more adaptive in the context of stress.
examined in the context of long-term adjustment to herefore, by iltering out unnecessary negative infor-
stress, the optimism literature discussed above suggests mation in a goal-directed way (Figure 2.1, moder-
that the lexible use of these attentional biases across ation, link D), it becomes more likely that individuals
contexts would be most adaptive. For example, individ- will make less-threatening appraisals of stressful situ-
uals who display biases away from irrelevant negative ations (Figure 2.1, link A) and, as a result, will experi-
information but who attend to relevant negative infor- ence fewer negative emotions (Figure 2.1, link B), in
mation would be most likely to be resilient in the long turn increasing the likelihood of resilience (Figure 2.1,
term. Taken as a whole, these training studies suggest link C). he next section will examine the main efects
that selective AC is best conceptualized as an ability and the moderating efects of another type of cognitive

36
Chapter 2: Resilience in the face of stress

emotion regulation strategy, CR, in the context of stress 1990; Gross, 1998b; Jackson et al., 2000). Subsequent
and resilience (Figure 2.1, link E). research using self-report trait measures of reappraisal
have extended Gross’ indings by examining the afect-
Empirical findings: cognitive ive consequences of individual diferences in CR use.
For example, Mauss and colleagues (2007) found that
reappraisal and resilience individuals who reported frequently using CR as an
In contrast to AC, CR usually takes place ater an ini- emotion regulation strategy – relative to those who
tial appraisal has been made (Figure 2.1). Oten, CR reported not using this strategy – experienced less anger
involves reframing an emotionally negative situation and an adaptive pattern of physiological responding in
in a more positive way to decrease feelings of negative a laboratory anger provocation.
emotion (Gross, 1998a; Gross & hompson, 2007). Extending these indings beyond the laboratory,
Notably, however, CR can also be used to increase Gross and John (Gross & John, 2003; John & Gross,
the experience of positive emotions (Folkman & 2004) have examined the consequences of CR use for
Moskowitz, 2000; Shiota, 2006; Krompinger et al., individuals’ well-being in daily life. hey found that
2008). Because of its direct impact on appraisals, CR individuals who report frequently using CR (“reap-
has been widely hypothesized to be an adaptive strat- praisers”) also report greater overall well-being.
egy, particularly in the context of stress – by reapprais- Together, these studies suggest that there are individ-
ing a stressor in a less negative and/or more positive ual diferences in CR use, and that those who report
way, individuals can change their emotional reaction using CR are able to efectively downregulate their
to the stressor for the better. experience of negative emotion. In addition, reap-
Neuroimaging studies of reappraisal have praisers also seem to be rewarded with increases in
supported the hypotheses that this strategy is efect- positive outcomes over time, which supports the claim
ive in changing the activation of brain regions asso- that CR has an important main efect on resilience.
ciated with emotion, such as the amygdala and the Speciically, those who use CR across a wide range of
insula. hese areas of the brain can be either increased negative emotional contexts are more likely to experi-
or decreased in activity in accordance with the goal ence positive outcomes and less likely to experience
of reappraisal (Ochsner & Gross, 2005, 2008; Urry negative outcomes.
et al., 2006; Eippert et al., 2007; Goldin et al., 2008). Studies that have examined the upregulation of
For example, when reappraisal was used to decrease positive emotions using CR have provided converging
negative emotions, results indicated that the experi- evidence. For example, the study of individual difer-
ence of negative emotion was reduced, and activity in ences in CR and responding to an anger induction
the amygdala and insula was decreased (Goldin et al., also showed that individuals who reported frequently
2008). In addition, neuroimaging studies comparing using CR experienced more positive emotions during
distraction to reappraisal have found that these two laboratory anger induction (Mauss et al., 2007). In the
strategies are supported by diferent areas of the brain, context of high stress, Folkman and Moskowitz (2000)
suggesting that people who claim to use reappraisal noted that, among caregivers for people with AIDS,
are not simply distracting themselves from negative those who reported frequently using reappraisal con-
stimuli (Kalisch et al., 2005, 2006). sistently experienced more positive emotions both
Behavioral and autonomic-physiological studies during caregiving and ater the death of the patient.
lend further support to the hypothesis that CR can Speciically, these caregivers used positive reappraisal
efectively change the experience of emotions with- to change the meaning of the negative situations they
out negative “side-efects.” For example, Gross (1998b) were experiencing. For example, many thought about
showed emotional ilm clips to undergraduates and how their eforts were beneiting their patients. In
asked them to reappraise their emotions, suppress another study on daily stressors among undergradu-
emotional behaviors, or simply watch the ilm. Results ates, Shiota (2006) found that the self-reported trait
from this and similar laboratory studies indicate that use of positive reappraisal was positively associated
individuals instructed to use CR report experien- with positive afect. hese studies together support the
cing less negative emotion than the other experimen- notion that CR can be efectively used to increase one’s
tal groups, but show no maladaptive physiological experience of positive emotions even in highly stress-
responding (Lazarus et al., 1965; Dandoy & Goldstein, ful situations.

37
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Although research on CR has focused consid- Taken together, it seems that CR confers advan-
erably less on increases in positive emotion than on tages to those who use it, suggesting that this regula-
decreases in negative emotion, some studies suggest tion strategy may be an important factor in resilience.
that experiencing positive emotions in the face of Speciically, the use of CR appears to change apprais-
stress is an important facet of achieving resilience. In als of a stressor, thus leading to an attenuated negative
support of this point, resilient individuals are more emotional reaction (as depicted in Figure 2.1, links
likely to ind positive meaning in the stressors they E and B). his adaptive emotional reaction, in turn,
experience (Moskowitz, 2001; Tugade & Fredrickson, appears to increase the likelihood of resilience (Figure
2004). Fredrickson and colleagues (2003, p. 367) pos- 2.1, link C). However, some open questions still
ited that this is because resilient people “use positive remain. First, the eicacy of CR in the context of high
emotions strategically or intelligently to achieve their life stress is not yet well understood. Although some
superior coping outcomes.” his strategic use of posi- studies have examined CR in stressed populations (c.f.
tive emotional experience is likely based, in part, on Carrico et al., 2005; Pakenham, 2005), most of these
adaptive emotion regulation strategies such as CR. For studies included only one speciic stressor, such as
the remainder of the chapter, the term CR will be used having HIV. his makes it diicult to know whether
to refer to both the upregulation of positive emotions these indings would generalize to other types and
and the downregulation of negative emotions, unless intensities of stressor. Also, apart from a few notable
otherwise noted. exceptions (Garnefski et al., 2001, 2003; Garnefski &
he studies reviewed so far suggest that CR is asso- Kraaij, 2006), most research on CR has not examined
ciated with positive outcomes in healthy populations. its relationship to negative outcomes such as depres-
Additional research suggests that CR also explains sion and anxiety symptoms, and even fewer studies
variation in mental health. For example, across a ser- have examined the potential moderating efects of CR
ies of studies, Garnefski and colleagues have found a on resilience versus negative outcomes. his makes it
consistent negative association between self-reported diicult to know whether CR can indeed bufer people
use of CR and depression (Garnefski et al., 2001, 2003; from developing negative outcomes in the context of
Garnefski & Kraaij, 2006). In addition, several lon- high life stress.
gitudinal studies have found that this relationship Additionally, nearly all of the existing research on
remains robust over time. In one study using a sam- CR has relied on self-report questionnaire measures of
ple of people aged 67 years and older, the negative rela- the frequency of CR use. Although these trait measures
tionship between CR and depression was replicated at of CR have proven very useful, we argue that speciic-
a follow-up session two and a half years ater the irst ally the ability to efectively and lexibly manage emo-
session (Kraaij et al., 2002). In another longitudinal tions across diferent contexts serves as an important
study that examined CR in the context of high stress, and strong moderator of the relationship between
Pakenham (2005) investigated outcomes in a sample stress and depression. It is, therefore, important to
of people caring for patients with multiple sclerosis. measure CR ability in addition to frequency of CR
Results indicated that self-reported use of CR had a use. One last limitation of research that has relied on
bufering efect on the relationship between stress and self-report measures of CR is the the confounds inher-
negative outcomes, including depression. ent in self-reports, such as self-presentational biases.
In order to better understand the efects of CR in the Going forward, it is important to use more objective
context of high stress, Carrico and colleagues (2005) laboratory measures of CR that are less inluenced by
conducted a longitudinal, experimental intervention self-report biases.
using cognitive-behavioral stress management with In our recent research, we sought to address these
a sample of highly stressed males with HIV. Over the open questions (Troy et al., 2010). We tested the idea
treatment period of 10 weeks, men who received the that CR ability could act as a moderator of the rela-
intervention showed signiicant decreases in depres- tionship between stress and depression – serving as a
sive symptomatology relative to the control group, and protective factor for those who are high in CR abil-
this decrease was mediated by self-reported increases ity, and acting as a vulnerability factor for those who
in the use of positive reframing, a strategy similar to are low in CR ability. To test this hypothesis, a labora-
CR, suggesting that CR was the mechanism of positive tory measure of CR ability was developed that meas-
change ater the intervention. ures changes in two afective domains: self-reported

38
Chapter 2: Resilience in the face of stress

emotion, speciically sadness, and physiological is an important factor contributing to adjustment to


responding. In order to measure CR ability in the stress. Taken as a whole, the literature on stress, resili-
laboratory, a sadness induction was conducted using ence, and CR supports a scientiic model of resilience
short ilm clips. Ability in CR was quantiied by cal- in which the ability to use CR serves as a moderator of
culating changes in sadness (using self-report and the relationship between stress and resilience (Figure
physiological changes) from a sad baseline ilm clip to 2.1, link D) Speciically, CR can be used to change
a subsequent ilm clip in which the participants were appraisals of a stressor, thus leading to an attenuated
instructed to reappraise. Speciically, participants were negative emotional response (Figure 2.1, link B). his
given instructions that asked them to think about the downregulated emotional response is, in turn, associ-
emotional situation depicted in the ilm in a more posi- ated with resilience (Figure 2.1, link C).
tive way. hese instructions gave speciic examples of More work is needed in this area to improve under-
how to reappraise, such as trying to imagine the unex- standing of the causal mechanisms, and further studies
pected good outcomes that the characters in the ilm examining changes in emotions besides sadness, both
clip could experience. Decreases in feelings of sadness positive and negative, are also warranted. he results
and physiological indices of negative emotion when of a recent study suggest that the ability to efectively
instructed to reappraise indexed the ability to use CR. regulate one’s emotion, such as sadness, may also
his laboratory measure of CR ability was used with extend to the ability to efectively regulate other nega-
a community sample of 90 women who had experi- tive emotions, and even positive emotions such as joy,
enced a stressful life event in the past three months. although this study did not look speciically at CR abil-
he women also illed out questionnaires that measure ity (Mikolajczak et al., 2008). Additionally, the present
life stress and depressive symptoms. review suggests that some but not all individuals are
he results indicated that CR ability moderated able to use CR to regulate emotions in the context of
the relationship between intensity of life stress and stress. his raises important questions. For example,
depressive symptoms. Speciically, at high levels of why are some people quite good at using CR while
stress, women who were high in CR ability exhibited others appear unable or unwilling to use this strategy?
signiicantly lower levels of depressive symptoms than What processes support efective emotion regulation?
women who were low in CR ability. Indeed, they were While there is some preliminary evidence to suggest
statistically indistinguishable from women who expe- that speciic aspects of cognitive control may support
rienced low levels of life stress. At low levels of stress, efective cognitive emotion regulation (Mikels et al.,
CR ability did not have a moderating efect on depres- 2008), this question remains to be fully explored. In
sive symptoms. his interaction was found using the addition, it remains unclear whether training para-
self-reported as well as the physiological indices of CR digms (similar to those used in the selective attention
ability. Additionally, nearly all participants reported literature) may allow those who are low in CR ability to
that they tried very hard to use reappraisal when they increase their ability with practice. Future research on
were instructed to do so. his inding, combined with the plasticity of CR ability is needed in order to answer
the signiicant interactions using both self-report and this open question.
physiological changes, supports the conclusion that the An additional area for future research lies in the
observed results are not simply the product of demand heterogeneity of processes involved in reappraisal.
characteristics or some other form of emotion regula- Reappraisal has been described, so far as the refram-
tion such as distraction. ing of an emotional situation in a more positive way
Together, these results suggest that CR ability has in order to change its emotional impact. However,
important implications for resilience. First, CR only there are several other ways in which reappraisal can
afected individuals’ well-being in the context of high be applied. For example, reappraisal has been used in
stress. Second, the fact that this sample contained other studies to reframe an emotional situation in a
a wide range of stressors and levels of stress suggests more objective, detached way (Gross, 1998b; Ochsner
that CR may be an adaptive regulatory strategy across et al., 2004), to imagine that an emotional event is being
many types of stressful situation. In addition, by using observed from a great distance (Ayduk & Kross, 2008),
a laboratory measure of CR ability, this study is one of or to imagine that the situation is not real (Deveney
the irst to demonstrate that an ability speciically to & Pizzagalli, 2008). Research examining these other
downregulate negative emotions by using reappraisal forms of reappraisal has found that they too appear

39
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

to be adaptive ways to decrease negative emotions. Conclusions


Indeed, one study that compared the utility of difer-
Exposure to stress is an emotional experience for
ent reappraisal strategies found that there are multiple
most people and, on average, stress exposure has
ways in which CR can be used to efectively down-
been linked to impaired mental health outcomes
regulate negative emotions (Ochsner et al., 2004). In
such as depression. here is, however, wide vari-
the context of stress and resilience, it remains unclear
ation in people’s adjustment to stress – many people
whether certain reappraisal strategies are more adap-
exhibit resilience ater a stressor, while others experi-
tive than others. It may be, for example, that distancing
ence negative long-term outcomes. his variance in
reappraisal is particularly adaptive for the downregula-
adjustment to stress suggests that there must be some
tion of negative emotions in the context of uncontrol-
endogenous factors that serve a protective role: those
lable stressors whereas positive reappraisal strategies
who possess these factors are more likely to experi-
may be more adaptive in the context of stressors that
ence resilience, while those who do not are more
can be changed or controlled, in that this approach may
vulnerable to negative outcomes. Because of the emo-
facilitate appraisals that lead to more active coping and
tional nature of stressors, there has been an increasing
problem solving in the face of stress. Further work on
focus on individual diferences in emotion regula-
optimal matching of speciic reappraisal strategies to tion as one such potential protective factor. Based on
diferent emotional contexts will help to shed light on appraisal theories of emotion, which argue that it is
these hypotheses. one’s appraisal of a stressor that leads to an emotional
Lastly, the model outlined in Figure 2.1 has the reaction (Figure 2.1, links A and B), we have put forth
potential to inform future clinical interventions. For a framework for resilience (Figure 2.1). In this frame-
example, several existing clinical interventions such work, the ability to use cognitive emotion regulation
as cognitive-behavioral stress management and cog- to adaptively change appraisals of a stressor moder-
nitive-behavioral therapy target cognitive change as a ates the relationship between stress and resilience. By
mechanism for the prevention of psychological disor- changing one’s appraisals of a stressor, the resulting
ders as well as for growth. One important component emotions can be changed in adaptive ways, leading to
of these therapies is to challenge distorted appraisals increased chances of resilience even in highly stress-
and replace them with more realistic, positive apprais- ful situations (Figure 2.1, link C).
als of a situation. his practice clearly overlaps with To support the proposed framework, two difer-
CR (Campbell-Sills & Barlow, 2007). he research ent types of cognitive emotion regulation that appear
reviewed above lends additional empirical support to be particularly efective at changing appraisals in the
to these interventions. Based on this research, more- context of stress have been explored. First, a review of
over, it seems that interventions could be particularly studies of AC suggested that adaptive strategies in the
efective in highly stressed individuals before path- context of stress consist of using AC selectively and lex-
ology develops. People who are highly stressed and ibly according to an individual’s current goals rather
low in emotion regulation ability may be particularly than in rigid, inlexible ways. For example, it appears
vulnerable, but also particularly responsive to treat- to be maladaptive to attend to negative stimuli when
ments that are aimed at improving CR ability. Using they are irrelevant to one’s well-being but it is adaptive
the laboratory paradigm described above, such vulner- to do so when they are relevant. People who are able to
able people could be relatively easily identiied. Clearly, lexibly and efectively use this type of AC appear to be
more research on CR ability (both basic and applied) more resilient in the long term, while people who use
is warranted in order to answer these open empirical maladaptive, stimulus-driven types of AC such as dis-
questions. Overall, however, as research in the area of traction and rumination are more vulnerable to long-
CR, stress, and resilience progresses, it is our hope that term negative outcomes such as depression (Figure 2.1,
more treatments might be informed by or improved link D).
upon using indings from this ield. Moving forward, Second, the role of CR, another form of cognitive
it is our hope that researchers and clinicians examin- emotion regulation, was examined. his consists of
ing emotion regulation can work together to ind the reframing emotional stimuli to decrease their negative
optimal way to promote resilient emotion regulation emotional impact. he literature suggests that CR can
among at-risk populations. be used to change the intensity of negative emotions in

40
Chapter 2: Resilience in the face of stress

response to stressful situations. In the context of high Campbell-Sills, L. & Barlow, D. H. (2007). Incorporating
life stress, individuals who report frequently using CR emotion regulation into conceptualizations and
are less likely to exhibit depression. In addition, recent treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In J. J. Gross
(ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 542–560).
work suggests that CR ability moderates the relation-
New York: Guilford Press.
ship between stress and depression. In this way, CR
ability may be playing an important protective role for Carrico, A. W., Antoni, M. H., Weaver, K. E., Lechner, S.
C., & Schneiderman, N. (2005). Cognitive–behavioural
those individuals who are able to efectively reappraise stress management with HIV-positive homosexual
in the context of stress (Figure 2.1, link E). men: Mechanisms of sustained reductions in depressive
In summary, it appears that the ability to use both symptoms. Chronic Illness, 1, 207–215.
types of cognitive emotion regulation serves as a pro- Dandoy, A. C. & Goldstein, A. C. (1990). he use of
tective factor against the development of negative out- cognitive appraisal to reduce stress reactions: A
comes in the context of stress. People who are able to replication. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5,
use cognitive emotion regulation strategies in adap- 275–285.
tive ways when experiencing stress are able to efect- Deveney, C. M. & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2008). he cognitive
ively manage the intensity of their negative emotions consequences of emotion regulation: An ERP
by changing the appraisals that they make. When these investigation. Psychophysiology, 45, 435–444.
negative emotions are efectively and adaptively down- Dohrenwend, B. S. & Dohrenwend, B. P. (eds.) (1974).
regulated, resilience becomes more likely. Stressful life events: heir nature and efects. New York:
Wiley.
Acknowledgements Eippert, F., Veit, R., Weiskopf, N., et al. (2007). Regulation
of emotional responses elicited by threat-related stimuli.
he authors would like to thank Betsy App, Benjamin Human Brain Mapping, 28, 409–423.
Hankin, Jutta Joormann, Jeremy Reynolds, Amanda
Ensel, W. M. & Lin, N. (1996). Distal stressors and the life
Shallcross, and members of the Emotion Regulation
stress process. Journal of Community Psychology, 24,
Laboratory at the University of Denver for their feed- 66–82.
back on a drat of this chapter.
Erber, R. & Tesser, A. (1992). Task efort and the regulation
of mood: he absorption hypothesis. Journal of
References Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 339–359.
Abele, A. E. & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2007). Individual Felsten, G. (2002). Minor stressors and depressed mood:
diferences in optimism predict the recall of personally Reactivity is more strongly correlated than total stress.
relevant information. Personality and Individual Stress and Health, 18, 75–81.
Diferences, 43, 1125–1135. Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a
Aspinwall, L. G. & Brunhart, S. M. (1996). Distinguishing process: Study of emotion and coping during three stages
optimism from denial: Optimistic beliefs predict of a college examination. Journal of Personality and Social
attention to health threats. Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 150–170.
Psychology Bulletin, 22, 993–1003. Folkman, S. & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Stress, positive
Ayduk, O. & Kross, E. (2008). Enhancing the pace of emotion, and coping. Current Directions in Psychological
recovery: Self-distanced analysis of negative experiences Science, 9, 115–118.
reduces blood pressure reactivity. Psychological Science, Freitas, A. L. & Downey, G. (1998). Resilience: A dynamic
19, 229–231. perspective. International Journal of Behavioral
Bonanno, G. A. (2005). Resilience in the face of potential Development, 22, 263–285.
trauma. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin,
135–138. G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crises?
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Lee, S. C. (1997). Attentional A prospective study of resilience and emotions following
biases for negative information in induced and naturally the terrorist attacks on the United States on September
occurring dysphoria. Behaviour Research and herapy, 11, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
35, 911–927. 365–376.
Broderick, P. C. (2005). Mindfulness and coping with Garnefski, N. & Kraaij, V. (2006). Relationships between
dysphoric mood: Contrasts with rumination and cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive
distraction. Cognitive herapy and Research, 29, symptoms: A comparative study of ive speciic samples.
501–510. Personality and Individual Diferences, 40, 1659–1669.

41
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Critchley, H. D., et al. (2005).
life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional Anxiety reduction through detachment: Subjective,
problems. Personality and Individual Diferences, 30, physiological, and neural efects. Journal of Cognitive
1311–1327. Neuroscience, 17, 874–883.
Garnefski, N., Boon, S., & Kraaij, V. (2003). Relationships Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Herrmann, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2006).
between cognitive strategies of adolescents and Neural correlates of self-distraction from anxiety and a
depressive symptomatology across diferent types of life process model of cognitive emotion regulation. Journal of
event. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 401–408. Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1266–1276.
Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Kendler, K. S. Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1999).
he neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and Causal relationship between stressful life events and
suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry, the onset of major depression. American Journal of
63, 577–586. Psychiatry, 156, 837–841.
Gotlib, I. H., Yue, D. N., & Joormann, J. (2005). Selective Kraaij, V., Pruymboom, E., & Garnefski, N. (2002).
attention in dysphoric individuals: he role of afective Cognitive coping and depressive symptoms in the
interference and inhibition. Cognitive herapy and elderly: A longitudinal study. Aging and Mental Health,
Research, 29, 417–432. 6, 275–281.
Gross, J. J. (1998a). he emerging ield of emotion Krompinger, J. W., Moser, J. S., & Simons, R. F. (2008).
regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Modulations of the electrophysiological response to
Psychology, 2, 271–299. pleasant stimuli by cognitive reappraisal. Emotion, 8,
Gross, J. J. (1998b). Antecedent- and response-focused 132–137.
emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for Kross, E. & Ayduk, O. (2008). Facilitating adaptive
experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of emotional analysis: Distinguishing distanced-analysis
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237. of depressive experiences from immersed-analysis and
Gross, J. J. & John, O. P. (2003). Individual diferences in two distraction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34,
emotion regulation processes: Implications for afect, 924–938.
relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion.
Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. American Psychologist, 46, 352–367.
Gross, J. J. & hompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Lazarus, R. S. (1999). Stress and emotion: A new synthesis.
Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (ed.), Handbook of New York: Springer.
emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). New York: Guilford Press. Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and
Holahan, C. J., Moos, R. H., Holahan, C. K., Brennan, P. L., & coping. New York: Springer.
Schutte, K. K. (2005). Stress generation, avoidance coping, Lazarus, R. S., Opton, E. M. Jr., Nomikos, M. S., & Rankin,
and depressive symptoms: A 10-year model. Journal of N. O. (1965). he principle of short-circuiting of threat:
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 658–666. Further evidence. Journal of Personality, 33, 622–635.
Ingram, R. E., Bernet, C. Z., & McLaughlin, S. C. (1994). Lu, L. (1994). University transition: Major and minor life
Attentional allocation processes in individuals at risk for stressors, personality characteristics and mental health.
depression. Cognitive herapy and Research, 18, 317–332. Psychological Medicine, 24, 81–87.
Isaacowitz, D. M. (2005). he gaze of the optimist. Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). he construct
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 407–415. of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for
Jackson, D. C., Malmstadt, J. R., Larson, C. L., & Davidson, future work. Child Development, 71, 543–562.
R. J. (2000). Suppression and enhancement of emotional MacLeod, C. (1999). Anxiety and anxiety disorders. In
responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology, 37, T. Dalgleish & M. Powers (eds.), he handbook of cognition
512–522. and emotion (pp. 447–477). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
John, O. P. & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy MacLeod, C. & Hagan, R. (1992). Individual diferences in
emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual the selective processing of threatening information, and
diferences, and life span development. Journal of emotional responses to a stressful life event. Behaviour
Personality, 72, 1301–1333. Research and herapy, 30, 151–161.
Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: he role of MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G.,
inhibitory processes. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 125–147. & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional
Joormann, J., Talbot, L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Biased vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association
processing of emotional information in girls at risk for through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias.
depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 135–143. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 107–123.

42
Chapter 2: Resilience in the face of stress

Mathews, A., Ridgeway, V., & Williamson, D. A. (1996). Norris, F. H., Stevens, S. P., Pfeferbaum, B., Wyche, K. F.,
Evidence for attention to threatening stimuli in & Pfeferbaum, R. L. (2008). Community resilience as
depression. Behaviour Research and herapy, 34, 695–705. a metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for
Mauss, I. B., Cook, C. L., Cheng, J. Y. J., & Gross, J. J. disaster readiness. American Journal of Community
(2007). Individual diferences in cognitive reappraisal: Psychology, 41, 127–150.
Experiential and physiological responses to an anger Ochsner, K. N. & Gross, J. J. (2005). he cognitive control of
provocation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242–249.
66, 116–124.
Ochsner, K. N. & Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion
Mazure, C. M., Bruce, M. L., Maciejewski, P. K., & Jacobs, regulation: Insights from social cognitive and afective
S. C. (2000). Adverse life events and cognitive- neuroscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
personality characteristic in the prediction of major 17, 153–158.
depression and anti-depressant response. American
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., et al. (2004). For
Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 896–903.
better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the
McCarthy, C. J., Lambert, R. G., & Moller, N. P. (2006). cognitive down and up-regulation of negative emotion.
Preventive resources and emotion regulation Neuroimage, 23, 483–499.
expectances as mediators between attachment and
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). he cognitive
college students’ stress outcomes. International Journal of
structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University
Stress Management, 13, 1–22.
Press.
Mikels, J. A., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Beyer, J. A., &
Fredrickson, B. L. (2008). Emotion and working Pakenham, K. I. (2005). Relations between coping and
memory: Evidence for domain-speciic processes for positive and negative outcomes in carers of persons with
afective maintenance. Emotion, 8, 256–266. multiple sclerosis (MS). Journal of Clinical Psychology in
Medical Settings, 12, 25–38.
Mikolajczak, M., Nelis, D., Hansenne, M., & Quoidbach, J.
(2008). If you can regulate sadness, you can probably Park, R. J., Goodyer, I. M., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Efects
regulate shame: Associations between trait emotional of induced rumination and distraction on mood and
intelligence, emotion regulation and coping eiciency overgeneral autobiographical memory in adolescent
across discrete emotions. Personality and Individual major depressive disorder and controls. Journal of Child
Diferences, 44, 1356–1368. Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 996–1006.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Williams, R. (1995). Attentional Powers, D. V., Gallagher-hompson, D., & Kraemer, H. C.
bias in anxiety and depression: he role of awareness. (2002). Coping and depression in Alzheimer’s caregivers:
British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 17–36. Longitudinal evidence of stability. Journal of Gerontology,
57B, 205–211.
Monat, A., Lazarus, R. S., & Reevy, G. (eds.) (2007). he
Praeger handbook on stress and coping, Vol. 1. Westport, Ray, R. D., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2008). All in the
CT: Praeger. mind’s eye? Anger rumination and reappraisal. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 133–145.
Morrow, J. & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Efects of responses
to depression on the remediation of depressive afect. Rutter, M. (1999). Resilience concepts and indings:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519–527. Implications for family therapy. Journal of Family
herapy, 21, 119–144.
Moskowitz, J. T. (2001). Emotion and coping. In T. J. Mayne
& G. A. Bonanno (eds.), Emotions: Current issues and Sarason, I. G., Johnson, J. H., & Siegel, J. M. (1978).
future directions (pp. 311–336). New York: Guilford Press. Assessing the impact of life changes: Development of the
life experiences survey. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and
Psychology, 46, 932–946.
their efects on the duration of depressive episodes.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582. Scheier, M. F. & Carver, C. S. (1993). On the power of
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective positive thinking: he beneits of being optimistic.
study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 26–30.
ater a natural disaster: he 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Scherer, K. R. (1988). Criteria for emotion-antecedent
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 115–121. appraisal: A review. In V. Hamilton, G. H. Bower, & N. H.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Frijda (eds.), Cognitive perspectives on emotion and
Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. motivation (pp. 89–126). New York: Kluwer Academic/
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92–104. Plenum Press.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirski, S. (2008). Scherer, K. R. & Ceschi, G. (1997). Lost luggage: A ield
Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological study of emotion antecedent appraisal. Motivation and
Science, 3, 400–424. Emotion, 21, 211–235.

43
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Schwartz, D. & Proctor, L. J. (2000). Community violence Smith, C. A. & Ellsworth, P. C. (1987). Patterns of appraisal
exposure and children’s social adjustment in the school and emotion related to taking an exam. Journal of
peer group: he mediating roles of emotion regulation Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 475–488.
and social cognition. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Tennant, C. (2002). Life events, stress and depression: A
Psychology, 68, 670–683. review of recent indings. Australian and New Zealand
Shiota, M. N. (2006). Silver linings and candles in the dark: Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 173–182.
Diferences among positive coping strategies Totterdell, P. & Parkinson, B. (1999). Use and efectiveness
in predicting subjective well-being. Emotion, 6, of self-regulation strategies for improving mood in a
335–339. group of trainee teachers. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 4, 219–232.
Siemer, M., Mauss, I., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Same situation –
diferent emotions: How appraisals shape our emotions. Tugade, M. M. & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient
Emotion, 7, 592–600. individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from
negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality
Siegle, G. J., Ghinassi, F., & hase, M. E. (2008). and Social Psychology, 86, 320–333.
Neurobehavioral therapies in the 21st century: Summary
Troy, A. S., Wilhelm, F. H., Shallcross, A. J., & Mauss, I. B.
of an emerging ield and an extended example of
(2010). Seeing the silver lining: Cognitive reappraisal
cognitive control training for depression. Cognitive
ability moderates the relationship between stress and
herapy and Research, 31, 235–262.
depression. Emotion, 10, 783–795.
Silk, J. S., Vanderbilt-Adriance, E., Shaw, D. S., et al. Urry, H. L., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., et al. (2006).
(2007). Resilience among children and adolescents at Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are
risk for depression: Mediation and moderation across inversely coupled during regulation of negative afect and
social and neurobiological contexts. Development and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among
Psychopathology, 19, 841–865. older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 4415–4425.
Singer, A. R. & Dobson, K. S. (2007). An experimental Wadlinger, H. A. & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2008). Looking
investigation of the cognitive vulnerability to depression. happy: he experimental manipulation of a positive
Behaviour Research and herapy, 45, 563–575. visual attention bias. Emotion, 8, 121–126.

44
Section
Section1 Pathways to resilience
Chapter
Cognitive factors and resilience: how

3 self-efficacy contributes to coping with


adversities
Charles C. Benight and Roman Cieslak

We are disturbed not by events, but by the views ging stressful demands can be traced back to works
which we take of them of Sigmund Freud and his successors. hese authors
(Epictetus, stoic philosopher, c. 55–135) wrote in depth about how people manage extreme
experiences using more or less adaptive cognitive
Introduction defensive mechanisms. he later work of Victor Frankl
Human ability to survive and to thrive in challenging poignantly describes the importance of perceptions in
or threatening situations has always fascinated people, surviving the horror of his three-year imprisonment in
inspiring artists and philosophers. People who proved a Nazi concentration camp (Frankl, 1962). Since these
their resilience, personal strengths, or moral virtues (as early approaches to stress and resilience, the cognitive
philosophers used to say) have been the focus of admir- revolution in psychology changed our understanding
ation and pride. hey have been gloriied as heroes in of the stress and coping process (Lazarus, 1966).
innumerate folktales and immortalized in countless his chapter focuses on the contemporary cog-
works of art. he stoic philosophers, however, argued nitive factors deemed critical in understanding
that ordinary people may face extraordinary circum- resilience. We agree with Luthar and colleagues (2000,
stances and remain undisturbed by challenging situ- p. 543), who stated that “resilience refers to a dynamic
ations. he quotation from Epictetus that opens this process encompassing positive adaptation within the
chapter introduces a major topic in this chapter: the role context of signiicant adversity.” We also argue that
of cognitive processes in human resilience. Although resilience is more a process than an outcome of indi-
there are many diferences in cognitive approaches to vidual coping eforts or a single factor (e.g., personality
understand resilience, all of them echo Epictetus’ aph- trait) contributing to efective coping with adversities.
orism that perception of reality and processing of vital he risk in studies of resilience lies in the broad search
information are critical components of coping with for an agreed deinition while the underlying processes
adversities. remain unknown. Following an old logic principle
Many deinitions and measures capturing the con- ascribed to Ockham, this chapter will avoid multiplying
cept of resilience have been proposed and are discussed entities beyond necessity. Instead, we will demonstrate
in other chapters in this book, including physiological how existing approaches focusing on cognitive factors
resistance (Selye, 1936), temperamental or personality vital to coping with adversities might be easily applied
predispositions (Kobasa, 1979; Scheier & Carver, 1985; through theoretically available models to advance our
Antonovsky, 1987; Strelau, 1998), trajectories of psy- knowledge of human resilience.
chological distress outcomes (Bonanno, 2004), coping Among these approaches, Bandura’s social cogni-
lexibility (Cheng, 2003), and self-perceptions of ability tive theory (SCT) is a viable framework to understand
to manage one’s life (Davidson et al., 1982). he focus resilience (Bandura, 1997). Other cognitive approaches
in this chapter relates to the role of cognitive determi- that might be applied to the study of resilience will also
nants of successful adaptation. be briely introduced. he focus will then be on SCT,
he irst cognitive psychological conceptualization presenting its primary concepts and studies showing
of factors relevant for surviving and adapting to chan- its usefulness in studies of resilience. Some possible

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

45
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

developments of this theory, which, in our opinion, here are an overwhelming number of studies sup-
could help in the understanding of the multidimen- porting the transactional model of stress (see Folkman
sional and complex process of adaptation and coping & Moskowitz, 2004). Recently, research has demon-
with adversities, will also be discussed. his chapter strated that appraisals made during stressful situations
focuses on adult adaptation to adversity and does not have an impact not only on psychological response
include how children might it into these models. but also on physiological response to stressors. For
example, there is a strong efect of anticipatory cog-
nitive appraisals on the cortisol stress response (Gaab
Cognitive approaches to resilience et al., 2005). Primary and secondary appraisals may
he process of regulating between environmental explain up to 35% of the variance for cortisol reactivity
demands and available resources is seen as critical for among people who were exposed to a standard stress-
human functioning in many theories of stress (Lazarus ful situation (Gaab et al., 2005). Importantly, Epel et
& Folkman, 1984; Hobfoll, 1989, 2001) and stress- al. (1998) suggested that cognitive appraisals that lead
related models (House, 1981; Karasek & heorell 1990; to a challenge approach to adversities may actually
Demerouti et al., 2001; Siegrist et al., 2004). Although the strengthen the physical and psychological resilience of
primary focus here is on cognitive factors derived from the individual.
SCT (such as self-eicacy beliefs) and the dynamic pro- Other cognitive processes such as causal attri-
cess involved in managing environmental demands and butions about an adverse event may be important to
resources, it is important to consider other theories that consider when understanding resilience. Causal attri-
also acknowledge the crucial role of human cognitions butions may refer to the stability, controllability, and
in coping with adversities and adaptation processes. generalizability related to the stressor. For example, a
he transactional theory of stress (Lazarus & stressor attributed to unstable, controllable, and spe-
Folkman, 1984) lists stress appraisals as key cognitive ciic causes can lead to a sense of challenge in dealing
mediators and distinguishes between primary appraisal with the adversity (Abramson et al., 1978).
and secondary appraisal. In primary appraisal, indi- herefore, although not typically discussed as resili-
viduals evaluate how important a speciic demand ence, previous research utilizing cognitive processes
is for their own well-being, judging if it is irrelevant such as appraisals and attributions conceptualizes suc-
(i.e., demands are not perceived as linked to a per- cessful coping as central to positive adaptation. Inherent
son), positive (i.e., the transaction may lead to positive in these models is an interactive process whereby an
consequences without taxing or exceeding resources), individual regulates his/her behavior through ongoing
or stressful (i.e., the transaction may lead to positive interactions with the adverse environment.
or negative consequences, but resources need to be Carver and Scheier (1981) discussed an interactive
engaged). A stressful situation may be further appraised control feedback system to understand this self-reg-
as harm/loss, threat, or challenge (Lazarus & Folkman, ulation. his system targets individualized appraisals
1984). If the perception is that the consequences of a of success or failure in reaching important self-goals,
speciic stress transaction may be positive, despite the leading to cognitive and behavioral adjustments
amount of resources invested, the stressful situation is through a cybernetic feedback system. More recently,
interpreted as a challenge. Harm/loss or threat apprais- Carver (1998) developed a more complex model of dif-
als, contrastingly, are made under situations where ferent trajectories for understanding functional and
stress demands exceed available resources, leading to dysfunctional responses to adverse events, including
negative outcomes. Perceiving adversity as a challenge resilience. his model assumes four diferent possible
is a hallmark characteristic of resilience. trajectories following an adverse event. he trajector-
he perception of available resources is called sec- ies include succumbing, survival with impairment,
ondary appraisal. hese appraisals relate to an evalu- resilience, thriving. It is noteworthy that Carver (1998)
ation of available physical, social, psychological, and introduced thriving as a possible outcome separate
material resources and the ability to use them (self- from resilience, and that environmental engagement
eicacy) in dealing with environmental demands and goal valence are also important to consider. Carver
(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Speciic coping strategies emphasized perceptions of personal conidence as
are undertaken as a result of both primary and second- critically important to consider. He stated (p. 256),
ary appraisals. “people with a sense of conidence and mastery move

46
Chapter 3: Cognitive factors and resilience

onward and upward and people with suicient doubt behaviors is self-eicacy. Self-eicacy is part of the
sufer continued or repeated declines.” As with sec- self-evaluative cognitive process and is deined as the
ondary appraisal described by Lazarus and Folkman perceived capability to enact a certain behavior. Self-
(1984), a sense of competence to meet adversity eicacy perceptions have been found to be highly
demands provides a sense of control that is critical for predictive of behavior across a vast array of human
healthy adaptation (Epel et al., 1998). Bandura (1997) functioning (e.g., athletics, education, health, work per-
and others have repeatedly shown this to be a central formance, stress [Bandura, 1997]). Within the context
variable in understanding human behavior. Based on of stress and coping, self-eicacy perceptions have been
this, we argue that a sense of control to manage adver- labeled coping self-eicacy. Consequently, coping self-
sity is a necessary condition for resilience (or thriving) eicacy in the context of severe adversity is deined as the
to occur. To understand this process more thoroughly, perceived capability for managing post-traumatic recov-
the SCT framework is utilized to examine the mecha- ery or stressful demands (Benight & Bandura, 2004).
nisms that drive resilience. Self-eicacy perceptions are developed through
interactive feedback, with success or failure as one
Social cognitive theory approach strives toward valued goals. Self-eicacy perceptions
are enhanced through mastery experiences, verbal
to resilience persuasion of valued social connections, observa-
Social cognitive theory is a broad theory of human tions of relevant others’ successes, and adequate self-
behavior (Bandura, 1997). It emphasizes the import- management of physical arousal. Conversely, failure,
ance of interactions among three primary factors that negative social feedback, inadequate social models,
predict future behavior: the environment, the person, and unmanageable anxiety lead to derailment of self-
and behavior (Figure 3.1). hrough dynamic interac- eicacy beliefs. hese perceptions are highly predict-
tions with environmental conditions, human beings are ive of motivational behaviors such as goal setting and
able to be strategic in adapting to changing demands. behavioral perseverance following environmental
Self-regulative behavior is accomplished through impediments (Bandura, 1997). Clearly, it could be
feedback systems both internally (cognitive appraisal argued that such cognitive appraisals play an import-
processes) and externally (changes in environmental ant role in positive trajectories following adversity
conditions). Self-evaluation is a critical component in (i.e., the resilience or thriving trajectories described
the feedback process to ascertain successful or unsuc- by Carver [1998]). Other researchers have also cited
cessful achievements of desired goals. By interpreting the importance of perceptions of conidence in under-
one’s successes and failures, individuals alter their cog- standing resilience and optimal functioning (Ford &
nitions and behaviors in order to shape desired envir- Smith, 2007).
onmental conditions. We argue that self-appraisals Self-eicacy can be viewed as a more general con-
play a central role in resilience. struct or as a domain-speciic construct. General self-
A key construct within this self-regulatory dynamic eicacy is a trait-like factor that relects one’s beliefs
interplay between the person, the environment, and about one’s own capabilities in dealing with demands
across diferent situations. Although general self-ei-
cacy may be seen as a personality trait (e.g., Schyns &
Behavior von Collani, 2002), it may be also argued that cross-
situational coherence in self-eicacy appraisals might
be explained without referring to a personality trait
construct (Cervone, 1997, 2000). In contrast to gen-
eral self-eicacy, task- or domain-speciic self-eicacy
Human adaptation is more proximally related to a target task, behavior, or
goal. Individuals’ beliefs about their ability to perform
a particular task, execute a speciic behavior, or achieve
a goal predict health behavior change (Luszczynska &
Individual factors Environment Schwarzer, 2003), teachers’ performances (Skaalvik &
Figure 3.1 Interactions among environmental, individual, and Skaalvik, 2007), and managing computers (Compeau
behavioral factors predict human adaptation. & Higgins, 1995), to name a few.

47
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Self-eicacy perceptions are not the only cognitive demands from such adversities as a lood, terrorism,
factors inluencing predictors of stress-response or or a hurricane, all three types of resource (i.e., envir-
post-traumatic outcomes. As discussed above, stress onmental, personal, and behavioral) will be used to
appraisals, attributions, perception of controllabil- ofset the environmental demands to achieve positive
ity, or general self-eicacy have all been shown to be functioning. Of course, internal demands (e.g., intru-
predictive of behaviors. However, speciic self-eicacy sive thoughts) must also be managed through available
perceptions have been shown to be stronger predic- psychosocial resources.
tors of outcomes than these other cognitive processes In the context of this three-dimensional model of
(Bandura, 1997). self-regulation, self-eicacy beliefs would remain a
As mentioned above, coping self-eicacy relates to crucial mechanism predicting successful adaptation
beliefs about one’s own abilities to manage demands (i.e., resilience). In a study testing this expectation, both
speciic to coping with trauma or stressful events demands- and resources-related self-eicacy should
(Benight & Bandura, 2004). A demand becomes stress- be considered (e.g., “I am capable to work efectively
ful when there is a lack of adequate resources to deal even with time pressure” and “I am capable to get sup-
with this demand (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Hobfoll, port from friends when needed,” respectively). Our
1989). he process of regulation between demands and laboratory has recently shown that a measure assessing
resources is seen as critical for human functioning in self-eicacy for speciic job demands and for resource
many theories of stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; utilization mediates the efects of work stressors on job
Hobfoll, 1989, 2001). We argue that the classical self- burnout (Lua, 2008).
regulation mechanism described in SCT (Figure 3.1) Figure 3.3 depicts a model that might be used to test
might be extended by incorporating the regulating the efect of demands and resources on selected resilient
mechanisms between demands and resources (Figure adaptation outcomes. Rather than solely focusing on
3.2). In other words, the self-regulation mechanisms the speciic measures of resilience or thriving outcomes,
among behavioral, personal, and environmental fac- or on indices of resources or demands, the model high-
tors may be viewed as the dynamic interplay between lights the mediational role of self-eicacy perceptions
demands and resources. his approach to self-regu- in understanding the dynamics of resilient coping with
lation could be compared with a three-dimensional adversities. he dynamic nature of the model is repre-
graph, in which multivectorial relationships represent sented by showing that changes in critical variable levels
the mechanisms discussed above. For example, for across time, both before and ater exposure to traumatic
resilient individuals coping with environmental or stressful events, are important to consider.
As SCT suggests, the ability to generate positive
self-eicacy beliefs is not static but changes over time
Behavioral and as environmental conditions change (c.f. Mischel
resources
& Shoda, 1995). In other words, one can develop self-
eicacy beliefs to cope with a particular demand, but
Behavioral demonstrate a lack of such beliefs about one’s ability to
demands cope with other demands as environmental conditions
change. his approach toward understanding the adap-
tation ater exposure to traumatic or stressful events
focuses on the mechanisms underlying the process of
Personal
rsonal
rs
resources
essources
resilient adaptation and requires monitoring the levels
of critical components of the coping process and the
Personal changes in these factors (i.e., demands, resources, con-
demands text-speciic coping self-eicacy, and various aspects of
Environmental functioning as indices of adaptation).
resources Rather than drawing possible trajectories of recov-
ery resulting from coping with adversities, the model
Environmental attempts to identify dynamic factors that trigger, boost,
demands and maintain efective coping through the utilization
Figure 3.2 Three-dimensional model of self-regulation. and cultivation of internal and external resources. In

48
Chapter 3: Cognitive factors and resilience

Post-event
Figure 3.3 The mediating role of
Pre-event resources Post-event
resources (time 1) resources (time n)
self-efficacy in coping with adversities.
(time 0)
∆ ∆

Pre-event demands Post-event Post-event


(time 0) demands (time 1) demands (time n)


Self-efficacy Self-efficacy ∆ Self-efficacy
(time 0) Event (time 1) (time n)

Pre-event Post-event Post-event


outcome (time 0) outcome (time 1) outcome (time n)
∆ ∆

our view, coping self-eicacy is an active, crucial cog- (e.g., a house), conditions (e.g., marriage), personal
nitive component that can facilitate a sense of control characteristics (e.g., social skillfulness), and ener-
to inluence essential social and personal resources gies (e.g., education). Signiicant evidence exists that
that are relevant to maintaining, regaining, or enhan- demonstrates the importance of considering environ-
cing well-being. For example, the beneicial efect of mental resources in successful adaptation to adversity
social support on depressive symptoms can be more (Hobfoll, 1998). A more thorough overview of how
thoroughly explained when self-eicacy is considered self-eicacy may modify the efects of all these kinds
as a mediating factor (Fiori et al., 2006). Modern ana- of resource on diferent aspects of humans function-
lytic techniques such as latent growth curve modeling ing is beyond the scope of this chapter; the reader is
and structural equation modeling allow us to test these referred to Benight and Bandura (2004) for a deeper
more sophisticated theoretical approaches to under- analysis. Here some empirical evidence is presented
standing adaptation to adversity. that supports the notion of self-eicacy as a relevant
mediator between stress characteristics, resources,
The importance of the environmental and diferent outcomes, beginning irst with trau-
matic stress.
context
As discussed above, self-eicacy may modify the rela-
tionship between resources and demands as people Self-efficacy and traumatic demands
confront adversities. hese demands may vary, ran- Research on coping self-eicacy perceptions as a pre-
ging from external demands caused by chronic stress dictor of psychological distress following trauma has
at work or daily stressors to internal demands resulting found these perceptions to be predictive of psycho-
from an exposure to a traumatic event. In our model logical adjustment within a variety of traumatic con-
(Figure 3.3), we argue that one should always control texts such as natural disasters (e.g., hurricanes, ires,
for the efects of speciicity of these demands when loods, and earthquakes; Benight et al., 1997, 1999a,
investigating the efect of resources on well-being or 1999b; Cheever & Hardin, 1999; Benight & Harper
other domains of stress outcomes. 2002; Sumer et al., 2005), motor vehicle trauma (Benight
In addition to environmental demands, environ- et al., 2008), domestic violence (Benight et al., 2004),
mental resources must also be considered. Following death of a spouse (Lindstrom, l997; Benight et al., 2001),
the deinition of Hobfoll (2001, p. 339), resources are terrorist attacks (Benight et al., 2000), and military com-
“objects, personal characteristics, conditions, or ener- bat (Solomon et al., 1988, 1991). Coping self-eicacy
gies that are valued in their own right, or that are val- also predicted positive outcomes (e.g., post-traumatic
ued because they act as conduits to the achievement growth) following Hurricane Katrina in a sample of
or protection of valued resources.” Hobfoll (2001) HIV-positive survivors with signiicant post-traumatic
grouped resources into several categories: objects stress symptoms (Cieslak et al., 2009).

49
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Data also support the role of coping self-eicacy as self-eicacy perceptions may also be important in this
a mediator between negative post-trauma cognitions context. Research indicates that the same moderating
(such as negative beliefs about self and the world) and efect could be ascribed to self-eicacy. Results have
psychological distress among survivors of child sexual shown that self-eicacy perceptions appear to buf-
abuse and motor vehicle trauma (Cieslak et al., 2008). fer the efects of high job demands and low levels of
A recent meta-analysis indicated that self-eicacy has control on important occupational outcomes such as
medium to large efects on post-traumatic adaptation physiological reactivity (Schaubroeck & Merritt, 1997)
indices (Luszczynska et al., 2009), a testament to the and job burnout (Salanova et al., 2002). In addition, the
importance of this construct. match between level of self-eicacy and environmen-
Collectively, these studies demonstrate that the tal control may also be important to consider. Among
level of self-perceptions of coping capability follow- workers who report high self-eicacy, for example, low
ing a trauma is important to consider. Positive coping levels of control (under highly demanding conditions)
self-eicacy (conidence) to manage stress adaptation have also increased blood pressure (Schaubroeck &
demands provides an internal sense of control that Merritt, 1997) and increased symptoms of burnout
promotes positive cognitions about the self, increases (Salanova et al., 2002).
motivation to respond to ongoing demands, assists in Although these studies tested the moderating
self-management of emotions, and promotes efective efects of self-eicacy, it may be that self-eicacy per-
decision making, in other words “resilience” (Bandura, ceptions serve as a mediating mechanism rather than
1997; Carver, 1998). Alternatively, lower levels of cop- as a moderator (i.e., the cognitive mechanism through
ing self-eicacy beliefs (i.e., conidence) lead survivors which these other variables inluence important out-
of trauma into a sense of increasing despair as recovery comes). Several workplace stress studies testing the
demands accumulate, leading to looding of uncon- mediating role of self-eicacy perceptions have been
trolled emotions, signiicant negative self-scrutiny, supportive. Brouwers and Tomic (2000) tested longi-
avoidance, and eventual capitulation (Carver, 1998). tudinal relationships between teachers’ burnout and
Turning attention now to general stress adaptation work-related self-eicacy and found that perceived
rather than traumatic stress, the results continue to be self-eicacy mediated the efects of emotional exhaus-
consistent. tion on personal accomplishment and depersonal-
ization (important occupational outcomes). Further,
among teachers working in stressful conditions caused
Self-efficacy and other stressful demands by recent organizational changes, self-eicacy in deal-
he above-mentioned studies provide evidence that ing with organizational changes was found to predict
high coping self-eicacy beliefs might reduce the dele- burnout symptoms, even ater controlling for demo-
terious efects of trauma-related demands on psycho- graphic variables (Evers et al., 2002). Self-eicacy also
logical distress. Outside the realm of trauma, a similar mediated the efects of work stressors on job dissatis-
efect might be observed when other types of stressor faction and turnover intention (Jex & Gudanowski,
are concerned (e.g., demands at work). Indeed, recent 1992). Finally, in probably the most extensive study to
results provide evidence for the crucial role of self-ei- date testing self-eicacy as a prime work stress medi-
cacy when added to well-known models of work stress ator, Perrewé et al. (2002) conducted a study in nine
and burnout. countries and found that work role stress (i.e., role con-
he job demands–control model is probably one lict and role ambiguity) was indirectly related to burn-
of the most frequently tested models in occupational out through self-eicacy perceptions. Although these
health psychology. In this model, work strain (i.e., studies speak to the challenges of occupational stress,
work stress) is a result of working in the conditions the focus has been on negative outcomes such as burn-
where job demands are high and the latitude of con- out or physiological stress markers. Future research
trol over the job is low (Karasek, 1979). In more recent is necessary to identify positive outcomes relating to
developments of this model (i.e., in the demands– resilience or thriving in these contexts.
control–support model), social support (i.e., social he reviewed evidence on level of self-eicacy per-
resource) has moderated the interaction of high ceptions is supportive of the importance of these beliefs
demands and low control on job strain (Johnson & in managing environmental demands and efectively
Hall, 1988; Karasek & heorell, 1990). Importantly, utilizing critical adaptational resources (Figure 3.3).

50
Chapter 3: Cognitive factors and resilience

Yet this view fails to take into account the dynamic predictive of physical and psychological improvements
nature of human resilience. Also in Figure 3.3, one can (Riazi et al., 2004).
see that changes in critical factors across time are vital In conclusion, research has supported the assump-
to consider. Attention is now given to evidence testing tion that mastery experiences in high-anxiety environ-
the value of changes in self-eicacy perceptions across ments translate into an increase of coping self-eicacy
time in understanding resilience. perceptions across time, which, in turn, results in
important positive biopsychosocial implications.
Changes in self-efficacy beliefs and However, self-eicacy beliefs do not exist suspended in
a shell of a human being void of characteristic traits.
psychological outcomes Personality interacts with self-regulatory processing
Whereas the literature described above supports the during times of adversity.
role of levels of coping self-eicacy, there is also some
evidence that the dynamic changes in self-perceptions Personality factors, self-efficacy, and
are also important to consider in psychological adap-
tation. Indeed, if self-eicacy increases through posi- resilience
tive management of resources in response to critical Personality traits (e.g., hardiness, internal locus of con-
demands, it would suggest an increasing sense of mas- trol, etc.) are oten regarded as an important resource
tery over the adaptation process. A decrease in self- in determining coping responses to adversity. here
eicacy beliefs would suggest an increasing sense of is a link between personality traits and self-eicacy. A
defeat and self-doubt. meta-analysis that tested the relationship between the
In a study examining recovery ater motor vehicle “big ive” traits (extraversion, neuroticism, agreeable-
accident trauma (Benight et al., 2008), early changes ness, conscientiousness, and openness) and self-ei-
in coping self-eicacy perceptions (within the irst cacy demonstrated that these personality traits may
month following an accident) were predictive of lower explain up to 49% of self-eicacy variance (Judge &
post-traumatic distress three months later, even ater Ilies, 2002). Another meta-analysis showed that gen-
controlling for the other predictors of distress (such as eral self-eicacy (a more trait-like construct) is related
responsibility for accident, involvement in litigation, to job performance and job satisfaction (average cor-
peritraumatic dissociation, seven-day post-traumatic rected correlations were 0.23 and 0.45, respectively).
distress, and coping self-eicacy level). Based on these hese results suggest that people with more resili-
results, future investigations should consider changes ent personalities may appraise adverse events as less
in self-eicacy perceptions over time when attempting stressful and may have stronger beliefs about their
to model the resilience process following trauma. he capabilities to cope with these events, which, in turn,
model depicted in Figure 3.3 shows that both levels of afects their behaviors and well-being (i.e., a mediating
and changes in self-eicacy may predict resilient func- model).
tioning ater a traumatic event. his mediating efect of self-eicacy related to
Evidence for the importance of changes in self-ei- personality, stress, and its health outcomes can be
cacy outside the trauma realm also exists. For example, supported. For example, the efect of personality fac-
one study experimentally manipulated self-eicacy tors (self-esteem, optimism, and control) on adjust-
perceptions related to managing a phobic threat and ment ater abortion (i.e., distress, well-being, and
found that change in coping self-eicacy perceptions decision satisfaction) was mediated by pre-abortion
(resulting from this manipulation) was a signiicant stress appraisals and self-eicacy (Major et al., 1998).
predictor of immunological functioning (Wiedenfeld Similarly, Chen et al. (2000) found the efect of state-
et al., 1990). In another study, the efectiveness of a like factors, such as general self-eicacy and cognitive
group intervention for coping with HIV depended on ability, on learning performance was mediated by spe-
changes in self-eicacy, namely changes in coping self- ciic self-eicacy beliefs. Benight et al. (1999b) found
eicacy following a 10-week intervention mediated that coping self-eicacy perceptions mediated between
the efects of the intervention on stress and burnout optimism and psychological adjustment following
(Chesney et al., 2003). Among patients with another Hurricane Opal.
chronic illness (multiple sclerosis), both changes in Within the workplace environment, self-eicacy
eicacy perceptions and baseline levels of eicacy were (together with optimism and self-esteem) mediated

51
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

the efects of job resources (e.g., autonomy and social the human process of perseverance or its counter-
support) on work engagement, a positive dimension of part “giving up.” More recently, Ford and Smith (2007)
work-related well-being (Xanthopoulou et al., 2007). outlined the beneit of utilizing an integrated or uni-
Job-related self-eicacy fully mediated the relationship ied approach to understanding optimal human func-
between support from the organization and work– tioning by including key components of motivation:
home conlict (Erdwins et al., 2001). Self-eicacy also goals, self-eicacy, emotions, and environmental con-
signiicantly predicted psychological detachment (a text. However, resilience cannot be separated from
positive outcome) from work during of-work hours self-regulatory mechanisms. It is our suggestion that
(Sonnentag & Kruel, 2006). resilience research would greatly beneit from multiple
hese studies suggest that a multifactorial model theoretical approaches that elucidate how critical cog-
is necessary that includes trait factors and the mecha- nitive self-regulatory processes such as self-eicacy
nisms that translate these characteristics into efect- serve as catalysts for successful managing of environ-
ive adaptation (i.e., efective utilization of resources mental demands and available resources.
in managing stressful environmental demands).
Although the presented empirical evidence for a References
model of this type is compelling, signiicant chal-
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D.
lenges lie ahead for advancing our understanding of (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and
the mechanisms involved in promoting resilience. reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87,
49–74.
Challenges for cognitive approaches to Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unravelling the mystery of health:
How people manage stress and stay well. San Francisco,
investigating resilience CA: Jossey-Bass.
his chapter has emphasized the role of self-eicacy Bandura, A. (1997). Self-eicacy. he exercise of control.
cognitions in coping with adversities, a concept derived New York: Freeman.
from SCT and its recent developments. A theoretical
Benight, C. C. & Bandura, A. (2004). Social cognitive theory
framework for future research has also been proposed of posttraumatic recovery: he role of perceived self-
(Figures 3.2 and 3.3), emphasizing the mediating eicacy. Behaviour Research and herapy, 42, 1129–1148.
role of domain-speciic self-eicacy and the need for Benight, C. C. & Harper, M. L. (2002). Coping self-eicacy
a multifactorial model. It has been argued that both perceptions as a mediator between acute stress response
demands and resources should be considered when and long-term distress following natural disasters.
testing the mediating efect of self-eicacy. Further, a Journal of Traumatic Stress, 15, 177–186.
variety of aspects of human functioning should be con- Benight, C. C., Antoni, M. H., Kilbourn, K., & Ironson, G.
sidered among the outcomes variables. To understand (1997). Coping self-eicacy bufers psychological and
the role of cognitive resilience factors in more detail, physiological disturbances in HIV-infected men following
longitudinal and experimental studies testing aspects a natural disaster. Health Psychology, 16, 248–255.
of this dynamic model of adaptation are necessary. Benight, C. C., Ironson, G., Klebe, K., et al. (1999a).
Only these designs will allow for testing how changes Conservation of resources and coping self-eicacy
in, and levels of, critical catalyst variables promote predicting distress following a natural disaster: A causal
model analysis where the environment meets the mind.
optimal adaptation in the fact of adversities in diferent
Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 12, 107–126.
domains of functioning.
Although this chapter targeted the role of self- Benight, C. C., Swit, E., Sanger, J., Smith, A., & Zeppelin, D.
(1999b). Coping self-eicacy as a prime mediator of
regulation cognition in understanding resilience, we
distress following a natural disaster. Journal of Applied
acknowledge that studying other motivational pro- Social Psychology, 29, 2443–2464.
cesses may lead to a more comprehensive picture of
Benight, C. C., Freyaldenhoven, R. W., Hughes, J., et al.
factors contributing to human resilience. he inlu- (2000). Coping self-eicacy and psychological distress
ential work leading to the learned helplessness the- following the Oklahoma City bombing. Journal of
ory (Seligman, 1975), the attribution theory (Weiner, Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1331–1344.
1974), the modiied attribution theory (Abramson Benight, C. C., Flores, J., & Tashiro, T. (2001). Bereavement
et al., 1978), and the self-determination theory (Deci coping self-eicacy in cancer widows. Death Studies, 25,
& Ryan, 1985) may elucidate our understanding of 97–125.

52
Chapter 3: Cognitive factors and resilience

Benight, C. C., Harding-Taylor, A., Midboe, A. M., & Durham, Compeau, D. R. & Higgins, C. (1995). Computer self-
R. (2004). Development and psychometric validation eicacy: Development of a measure and initial test. MIS
of a Domestic Violence Coping Self-Eicacy Measure Quarterly, 19, 189–211.
(DV-CSE). Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 505–508. Davidson, L. M., Baum, A., & Collins, D. L. (1982). Stress
Benight, C. C., Cieslak, R., Molton, I. R., & Johnson, L. E. and control-related problems at hree Mile Island.
(2008). Self-evaluative appraisals of coping capability and Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 12, 349–359.
posttraumatic distress following motor vehicle accidents. Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 677–685. selfdetermination in human behavior. New York: Plenum
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Press.
Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli,
ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, W. B. (2001). he Job Demands–Resources Model of
59, 20–28. burnout. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 499–512.
Brouwers, A. & Tomic, W. (2000). A longitudinal study of Epel, E. S., McEwen, B. S., & Ickovics, J. R. (1998).
teacher burnout and perceived self-eicacy in classroom Embodying psychological thriving: Physical thriving in
management. Teaching and Teacher Education, 16, 239–253. response to stress. Journal of Social Issues, 54, 301–322.
Carver, C. S. (1998). Resilience and thriving. Issues, models, Erdwins, C. J., Bufardi, L. C., Casper, W. J., & O’ Brien,
and linkages. Journal of Social Issues, 54, 245–266. A. S. (2001). he relationship of women’s role strain to
Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self- social support, role satisfaction, and self-eicacy. Family
regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. Relations, 50, 230–238.
New York: Springer. Evers, W. J. G., Brouwers, A., & Tomic, W. (2002). Burnout
Cervone, D. (1997). Social-cognitive mechanisms and and self-eicacy: A study on teachers’ beliefs when
personality coherence: Self-knowledge, situational implementing an innovative educational system in the
beliefs and cross-situational coherence in perceived self- Netherlands. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
eicacy. Psychological Science, 8, 43–50. 72, 227–243.
Cervone, D. (2000). hinking about self-eicacy. Behavior Fiori, K. L., Antonucci, T. C., & Cortina, K. S. (2006). Social
Modiication, 24, 30–56. network typologies and mental health among older
adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,
Cheever, K. H. & Hardin, S. B. (1999). Efects of traumatic 61B, 25–32.
events, social support, and self-eicacy on adolescents’
Folkman, S. & Moskowitz, J. T. (2004). Coping: Pitfalls and
self-health assessments. Western Journal of Nursing
promise. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 745–774.
Research, 21, 673–684.
Ford, M. E. & Smith, P. R. (2007). hriving with social
Chen, G., Gully, S. M., Whiteman, J. A., & Kilcullen, B. N.
purpose: An integrative approach to the development of
(2000). Examination of relationships among trait-like
optimal human functioning. Educational Psychologist,
individual diferences, state-like individual diferences,
42, 153–171.
and learning performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,
85, 835–847. Frankl, V. (1962). Man’s search for meaning: An introduction
to logotherapy. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
Cheng, C. (2003). Cognitive and motivational processes
underlying coping lexibility: A dual-process model. Gaab, J., Rohleder, N., Nater, U. M., & Ehlert, U. (2005).
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 425–435. Psychological determinants of the cortisol stress
response: he role of anticipatory cognitive appraisals.
Chesney, M. A., Chambers, D. B., Taylor, J. M., Johnson, L. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 599–610.
M., & Folkman, S. (2003). Coping efectiveness training
for men living with HIV: Results from a randomized Hobfoll, S. (1998). Stress, culture, and community: he
clinical trial testing a group-based intervention. psychology and philosophy of stress. New York: Plenum
Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 1038–1046. Press.
Cieslak, R., Benight, C. C., & Lehman, V. C. (2008). Coping Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new
self-eicacy mediates the efects of negative cognitions attempt at conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist,
on traumatic distress. Behaviour Research and herapy, 44, 513–524.
46, 788–798. Hobfoll, S. E. (2001). he inluence of culture, community,
and the nested-self in the process: Advancing
Cieslak, R., Benight, C. C., Schmidt, N., et al. (2009).
conservation of resources theory. Applied Psychology: An
Predicting posttraumatic growth among Hurricane
International Review, 50, 337–421.
Katrina survivors living with HIV: the role of self-
eicacy, social support, and PTSD symptoms. Anxiety, House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading,
Stress, and Coping, 22, 449–462. MA: Addison-Wesley.

53
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Jex, S. M. & Gudanowski, D. M. (1992). Eicacy beliefs examination of role stressors, general self-eicacy,
and work stress. An exploratory study. Journal of and burnout. Journal of International Management, 8,
Organizational Behavior, 13, 509–517. 163–187.
Johnson, J. V. & Hall, E. M. (1988). Job strain, work place Riazi, A., hompson, A. J., & Hobart, J. C. (2004). Self-
social support and cardiovascular disease: A cross- eicacy predicts self-reported health status in multiple
sectional study of a random sample of the Swedish sclerosis. Multiple Sclerosis, 10, 61–66.
working population. American Journal of Public Health, Salanova, M., Peiró, J. M., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2002). Self-
78, 1336–1342. eicacy speciicity and burnout among information
Judge, T. A. & Ilies, R. (2002). Relationship of personality to technology workers. An extension of the job demand-
performance motivation: A meta-analytic review. Journal control model. European Journal of Work and
of Applied Psychology, 87, 797–807. Organizational Psychology, 11, 1–25.
Karasek, R. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, Schaubroeck, J. & Merritt, D. E. (1997). Divergent efects of job
and mental strain: Implications for job redesign. control on coping with work stressors: he key role of self-
Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 285–308. eicacy. Academy of Management Journal, 40, 738–754.
Karasek, R. & heorell, T. (1990). Healthy work. New York: Scheier, M. F. & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping,
Basic Books. and health: Assessment and implications of generalized
Kobasa, S. Q. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219–247.
health: An inquiry into hardiness. Journal of Personality Schyns, B. & von Collani, G. (2002). A new occupational self-
and Social Psychology, 37, 1–11. eicacy scale and its relation to personality constructs and
Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping organizational variables. European Journal of Work and
process. New York: McGraw-Hill. Organizational Psychology, 11, 219–241.
Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression,
coping. New York: Springer. development, and death. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
Lindstrom, T. C. (1997). Immunity and health ater Selye, H. (1936). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous
bereavement in relation to coping. Scandinavian Journal agents. Nature, 138, 32.
of Psychology, 38, 253–259. Siegrist, J., Starke, D., Chandola, T., et al. (2004). he
Lua, H. J. (2008). he mediating role of work stress and measurement of efort–reward imbalance at work:
burnout management self-eicacy in the Job Demand- European comparisons. Social Science and Medicine, 58,
Resources model. Master’s thesis, Warsaw School of 1483–1499.
Social Psychology, Warsaw. Skaalvik, E. M. & Skaalvik, S. (2007). Dimensions of teacher
Luszczynska, A. & Schwarzer, R. (2003). Planning and self-eicacy and relations with strain factors, perceived
self-eicacy in the adoption and maintenance of breast collective teacher eicacy, and teacher burnout. Journal
self-examination: A longitudinal study on self-regulatory of Educational Psychology, 99, 611–625.
cognitions. Psychology and Health, 18, 93–108. Solomon, Z., Weisenberg, M., Schwarzwald, J., &
Luszczynska, A., Benight, C. C., & Cieslak, R. (2009). Mikulincer, M. (1988). Combat stress reaction and
Self-eicacy, psychological, and somatic health-related posttraumatic stress disorder as determinants of
outcomes of collective trauma: A systematic review. perceived self-eicacy in battle. Journal of Social and
European Psychologist 14, 51–62. Clinical Psychology, 6, 356–370.
Luthar, S. S. Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). he construct Solomon, Z., Benbenishty, R., & Mikulincer, M. (1991). he
of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for contribution of wartime, pre-war and post-war factors
future work. Child Development, 71, 543–562. to self-eicacy: A longitudinal study of combat stress
reaction. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 4, 345–361.
Major, B., Richards, C., Cooper, M. L., Cozzarelli, C., &
Zubek, J. (1998). Personal resilience, cognitive appraisals, Sonnentag, S. & Kruel, U. (2006). Psychological detachment
and coping: An integrative model of adjustment to from work during of-job time: he role of job stressors,
abortion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, job involvement, and recovery-related self-eicacy.
735–752. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology,
15, 197–205.
Mischel, W. & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-afective system
theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, Strelau, J. (1998). Temperament: A psychological perspective.
dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality New York: Plenum Press.
structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268. Sumer, N., Karanci, A. N., Berument, S. K., & Gunes, H.
Perrewé, P. L., Hochwarter, W. A., Rossi, A. M., et al. (2002). (2005). Personal resources, coping self-eicacy, and
Are work stress relationships universal? A nine-region quake exposure as predictors of psychological distress

54
Chapter 3: Cognitive factors and resilience

following the 1999 earthquake in Turkey. Journal of on components of the immune system. Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 18, 331–342. Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1082–1094.
Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribution Xanthopoulou, D., Bakker, A. B., Demerouti, E., &
theory. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Schaufeli, W. (2007). he role of personal resources
Wiedenfeld, S. A., O’Leary, A., Bandura, A., et al. (1990). in the Job Demands–Resources model. International
Impact of perceived self-eicacy in coping with stressors Journal of Stress Management, 14, 121–141.

55
Section
Section1 Pathways to resilience
Chapter
Personality factors in resilience to

4 traumatic stress
Mark W. Miller and Kelly M. Harrington

Introduction emotionality/neuroticism (NEM), and constraint/


impulsivity (CON).
he study of individual diferences in resilience to
Positive emotionality/extraversion refers to indi-
traumatic stress has received unprecedented attention
vidual diferences in the capacity to experience positive
in recent years from investigators in the ield of post-
emotions and tendencies towards active involvement in
traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) but there remains a
the social and work environments and it is represented
lack of consensus regarding the deinition, measure-
in other models as extraversion (Eysenck & Eysenck,
ment, and conceptualization of the construct. Trait
1975; Costa & McCrae, 1985). Occurrence of PEM has
personality psychologists have grappled with similar
been linked conceptually to individual diferences in
issues since the seminal work of Jack Block (1961) on
sensitivity to signals of reward and to functioning of
the construct of ego resilience over 50 years ago. In the
the neurobiological system underlying appetitive-
process, they have developed comprehensive models
of personality and psychometrically sophisticated approach behavior (i.e., the behavioral activation sys-
tools for the measurement of its traits that can poten- tem) (Zuckerman, 1983; Gray, 1987; Panksepp, 1992),
tially inform and advance the study of resilience. he including the midbrain dopamine system (Berridge,
primary purpose of this chapter is to review the lit- 2003). Because of this, individuals high in PEM (extra-
erature on personality factors involved in resilience to verts) are more susceptible to the experience of positive
traumatic stress and to outline a model for conceptu- afect than their low PEM counterparts (introverts).
alizing this interface. In line with this, trait measures of PEM/extraversion
Contemporary models of personality aim to have been found to predict self-ratings of positive
identify the structure and basis for behavioral mood and the strength of responses to positive mood
traits – deined as individual diferences in patterns manipulations in the laboratory (Costa & McCrae,
of thoughts, feelings, and actions that are consist- 1980; Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991; Gross & John, 1995).
ent across developmental periods and environmental Low trait PEM has been shown to prospectively pre-
contexts. Personality models difer considerably with dict the development of major depression/dysthymia
regard to the factor structure, number, and deinition (e.g., Holohan & Moos, 1991; Verkerk et al., 2005) and
of speciic traits. Because of this, research on person- is widely conceptualized as the personality vulnerabil-
ality traits that confer risk or resilience to the develop- ity factor that distinguishes depression from anxiety
ment of post-traumatic psychopathology has yielded a (Watson et al., 1988a, 2005; Brown et al., 1998). High
complicated collection of studies examining disparate PEM, by comparison, is associated with higher levels
constructs and measures. To provide coherence and of happiness and life satisfaction (Schimmack et al.,
organization to this literature, this chapter will focus 2004), greater achievement and leadership (Ross et al.,
on three broadband personality dimensions described 2003; Bono & Judge, 2004), and diminished risk for the
by Tellegen (1985, 2000) that are also represented with development of social phobia and/or cluster C person-
subtle deinitional variations in most other contem- ality disorders (Jylhä et al., 2009).
porary trait models of personality. For this reason, Negative emotionality/neuroticism refers to dispo-
they are known as the “big three” personality factors: sitions toward negative mood and emotion and a ten-
positive emotionality/extraversion (PEM), negative dency towards conlictual interactions with others. It is

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

56
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

statistically and conceptually orthogonal to PEM, syn- Additional support for the validity of these higher-
onymous with neuroticism (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975; order personality dimensions comes from evidence
Costa & McCrae, 1985), and subsumes traits that relate that they correspond closely to dimensions of tempera-
to anxiety, alienation, and aggression. Occurrence of ment identiied in studies on infancy and early child-
NEM is thought to be linked to sensitivity of the neuro- hood (Rothbart et al., 1994). Behavior genetics studies
biological system underlying defensive behavior (the have shown them to have substantial heritabilities (e.g.,
behavioral inhibition system) (Zuckerman, 1983; heritability estimates in the 0.40–0.60 range [Tellegen
Gray, 1987; Panksepp, 1992; Tellegen, 2000), including et al., 1988; Robinson et al., 1992]) and scales measuring
the central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of these constructs evidence long-term stability in adult-
the stria terminalis (Davis & Lee, 1998; LeDoux, 2000). hood (e.g., 6- to 12-year test–re-test reliability ater age
heorists have reasoned that “because signals of pun- 30 on the order of 0.70–0.80 [Costa & McCrae, 1977,
ishment are the source of negative afect, and because 1992; Watson & Walker, 1996]). Cross-cultural studies
neurotics are sensitive to signals of punishment, neu- and research on the behavior of non-human primates
rotics should therefore be more susceptible than stable also support the universality and evolutionary founda-
or non-neurotic individuals to negative afect” (Larsen tion of these traits (McCrae & Costa, 1997; Capitanio,
& Ketelaar, 1991, p. 134). Evidence for the validity of 1999; Capitanio & Widaman, 2005).
this proposition comes from multiple sources includ-
ing studies showing that individuals with high NEM Conceptual and methodological
report more frequent and intense negative moods (e.g.,
Costa & McCrae, 1980) as well as heightened emotional challenges
reactivity to negative laboratory mood inductions (e.g., he existing body of research on personality factors
Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991). he trait NEM is ubiquitous involved in resilience to traumatic stress is limited by the
in the ield of personality assessment, has been repre- fact that studies have focused primarily on PTSD and
sented in every major model of personality, and is con- other measures of psychopathological adjustment as the
ceptualized as the primary personality vulnerability for outcome of interest, so relatively little is known about
the development of a spectrum of problems variously factors that may predict resilient or non-pathological
referred to as the “internalizing” (Krueger, 1999), “dis- outcomes. However, since the personality constructs of
tress” (Clark & Watson, 1991; Mineka et al., 1998), or interest are conceptualized as bipolar dimensions, it is
“emotional” (Watson, 2005) disorders (i.e., the anx- logical to view personality resilience factors as the polar
iety and depressive diagnoses in DSM-IV [American opposites of established vulnerability factors. hat is,
Psychiatric Association, 1994]). if high NEM is identiied as a PTSD vulnerability fac-
Most models of personality also posit the existence tor, then, by inference, low NEM can be conceptual-
of a separate disinhibition–constraint dimension – ized as a resilience factor. An important caveat to this
referred to here as CON – that involves tendencies assumption is that it is predicated on the notion that
anchored by planfulness versus spontaneity, restraint resilience is the absence of post-traumatic psychopath-
versus recklessness, and harm avoidance versus risk ology; however, this basic deinitional matter remains a
taking. he trait CON has been labeled by other theo- source of controversy among resilience researchers (c.f.
rists as psychoticism (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975), nov- Bonanno, 2004; Agaibi & Wilson, 2005).
elty seeking (Cloninger, 1987), and impulsivity (Buss A number of methodological issues unique to the
& Plomin, 1975), and it is thought to relect individ- study of personality and resilience to traumatic stress
ual diferences in functioning of the brain regulatory present formidable obstacles to research in this area.
system that governs behavioral restraint, including the Prospective longitudinal studies employing pre- and
prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex (Miller post-trauma assessments are the method of choice for
& Cohen, 2001). Numerous studies have implicated studying personality and resilience because they per-
low CON as the primary personality risk factor for mit examination of whether traits existed diferentially
the development of “externalizing” or impulse con- between individuals with resilient versus non-resilient
trol disorders, including substance-related problems, outcomes prior to trauma exposure. Unfortunately,
antisocial personality disorder, and attention-deicit obtaining an adequate sample of individuals who
hyperactivity disorder (Krueger, 1999; Krueger et al., did not have PTSD at an initial assessment, but who
2001; Patrick & Bernat, 2006). developed the disorder by a second point in response

57
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

to an interim event, necessitates a very large sample it is impossible to draw etiological inferences from
followed over an extended period of time, making cross-sectional studies of personality traits measured
this type of research expensive, time consuming, and post-trauma. For this reason, the review that follows
rare. Because of this, some investigators have resorted will focus primarily on studies that used pre- and post-
to studying populations at high risk for trauma expos- trauma prospective designs.
ure (e.g., active duty military personnel [Bramsen
et al., 2000]), but this approach has its own limitations Research on personality and resilience
involving the external generalizability of study ind-
ings to other populations and the validity of baseline to trauma
assessments obtained during stressful baseline (e.g.,
pre-deployment) intervals. Pre-trauma prospective studies
Post-trauma prospective designs in which trauma- Table 4.1 lists prospective longitudinal studies that
exposed individuals are assessed shortly ater trauma compared the pre-trauma personality traits of individ-
exposure and followed longitudinally to examine fac- uals who exhibited resilient versus psychopathological
tors that afect the course of the post-traumatic adjust- outcomes following exposure to a traumatic event. All
ment (e.g., who recovers versus who develops a chronic of them found signiicant associations between char-
condition) are becoming more common. Recent stud- acteristics linked to NEM measured prior to trauma
ies have followed trauma-exposed individuals over exposure and the subsequent development of PTSD
intervals ranging from months to years following in samples ranging from combat veterans to civilians.
motor vehicle accidents (e.g., Koren et al., 1999; Bryant O’Toole et al. (1998) examined the military records of
et al., 2000), sexual assaults (e.g., Zoellner et al., 1999), 641 Vietnam veterans and found that those who devel-
and combat (Wolfe et al., 1999). he advantage of this oped combat-related PTSD scored signiicantly higher
approach is that it permits investigators to identify var- on a measure of neuroticism administered at the time of
iables present immediately ater trauma exposure that enlistment than those who never developed the disor-
precede the onset of symptoms and/or that predict the der. Similarly, Bramsen et al. (2000) assessed 572 soldiers
course of the disorder. he drawback is that scores on before and ater deployment on a peacekeeping opera-
personality measures administered post-trauma may tion and found that a pre-deployment measure of NEM
be contaminated by transient correlates of the acute (i.e., negativism or “a negative, dissatisied, and hostile
traumatic reaction and it may be impossible to difer- attitude toward others and life in general” [Bramsen
entiate phasic adjustment reactions from traits that et al., 2000, p. 1116]) predicted scores on a self-report
existed prior to the trauma. measure of PTSD symptomatology post-deployment,
he latter issue is the primary problem with cross- even ater statistically controlling for the severity of
sectional studies – by far the most common in the peacekeeping stressors, demographic variables, and
ield. Personality traits measured post-trauma may other pre-morbid personality characteristics.
relect (1) enduring characteristics that were evident Studies with civilian samples have yielded similar
prior to the event, (2) correlates of transient stress- indings. In the largest such study, 2085 participants
related symptomatology, (3) permanent alterations in an Australian epidemiological survey were assessed
in personality that occur as a consequence of trauma before and ater a disastrous bushire (Parslow et al.
exposure, or (4) any combination of these. Although 2006). Results showed that individuals who screened
there is substantial evidence for the longitudinal sta- positive for PTSD ater the ire had produced signii-
bility of personality (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1977, 1992; cantly higher scores on the Neuroticism Scale of the
Watson & Walker, 1996), trait scales are also known to Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (Eysenck
be susceptible to contamination by mental health state et al., 1985) at the initial assessment several years before.
at the time of measurement (Kerr et al., 1970; Bianchi Similarly, in a study of 81 university students exposed
& Fergusson, 1977; Ingham et al., 1986; Duncan-Jones to a terrorist attack, Gil (2005) found that scores on
et al., 1990), and patients with anxiety and depressive the Harm Avoidance Scale (Cloninger, 1992) (which
disorders have been shown to respond diferently to is highly [r = 0.68] correlated with NEM/neuroticism;
personality inventories during the experience of a dis- e.g., Stewart et al., 2005) measured two weeks before
order compared with ater the remission of symptoms the trauma were positively associated with the pres-
(Hirschfeld et al., 1983; Reich et al., 1986). As a result, ence of PTSD symptoms six months ater the attack.

58
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

Table 4.1 Pre-trauma prospective studies of personality and post-traumatic adjustment.

Personality PTSD
Study Sample assessment assessment Method Findings
Bramsen 572 male military Dutch version of Self-Rating Dutch MMPI Pre-deployment
et al., 2000 peacekeepers the MMPI Inventory for administered prior neuroticism and
PTSD to deployment on paranoia/psychotic
a peacekeeping ideation predicted
mission; PTSD post-deployment
assessed less than 3 PTSD
years later
Engelhard 118 women with Neuroticism Scale PSS-SR Scale mailed Pre-trauma
et al., 2003 recent pregnancy from EPQ within 12 weeks of neuroticism
loss pregnancy; PTSD predicted PTSD
assessed ~1 month symptoms,
after pregnancy loss particularly arousal
symptoms, after
pregnancy loss; this
relation between
neuroticism and
PTSD symptoms was
no longer significant
after controlling for
pre-trauma arousal
levels
Gil, 2005 81 male and TPQ SCID TPQ administered Pre-trauma harm
female survivors 2 weeks prior to avoidance (~high
of a terrorist and 1 month after NEM) was positively
explosion a terrorist bus associated with risk
explosion; PTSD for PTSD; pre-trauma
assessed 6 months novelty seeking
after explosion was negatively
associated with risk
for PTSD
Gil & Caspi, 180 male and TPQ SCID TPQ administered Participants with
2006 female survivors 2 weeks prior to PTSD scored higher
of a terrorist and 1 month after on harm avoidance
explosion a terrorist bus (~NEM) and lower
explosion; PTSD on novelty seeking
assessed 6 months than participants
after explosion without the disorder
both before and
after the terrorist
attack
Lee et al., 150 male veterans Psychosomaticism DSM-III- Psychosomaticism Pre-war
1995 of World War II Scale developed referenced rating assessed prior to psychosomaticism
for this study and self-report military enlistment; (NEM) predicted
measures PTSD assessed in the development
developed for 1946 and 1988 of PTSD in veterans
this study exposed to low
levels of combat
intensity

59
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Table 4.1 (cont.)

Personality PTSD
Study Sample assessment assessment Method Findings
O’Toole 641 male Vietnam Self-description AUSCID-IV Personality data Participants with
et al., 1998 veterans Inventory (highly (interview drawn from combat-related
correlated derived from enlistment records; PTSD scored higher
with Eysenck’s the SCID PTSD PTSD assessed ~20 than participants
neuroticism) module) years after the war without the disorder
on a measure
of neuroticism
administered at the
time of entry into the
service
Parslow 2085 male and EPQ-R TSQ EPQ-R administered Pre-trauma
et al., 2006 female Australian 3–4 years before the neuroticism
civilians Canberra bushfires; predicted PTSD
PTSD assessed symptoms following
12–82 weeks after the bushfires
the bushfires
Schnurr 131 male Vietnam MMPI MISS and SCID MMPI administered Pre-war scores
et al., 1993 and Vietnam-era prior to military on the MMPI
veterans enlistment; MISS Hypochondriasis,
and SCID collected and Paranoia Scales
~20 years after the (high NEM) as well as
war psychopathic deviate
(low CON) predicted
those who went on
to develop PTSD
symptoms
DSM-III, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edn (American Psychiatric Association, 1980); EPQ, Eysenck Personality
Questionnaire (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975); EPQ-R, Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (Eysenck et al., 1985); MISS, Mississippi Scale
for Combat-related PTSD (Keane et al., 1988); MMPI, Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (Hathaway & McKinley, 1967); PSS-SR,
Posttraumatic Symptom Scale self-report version (Foa et al., 1993); SCID, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM (Spitzer et al., 1987); TPQ,
Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (Cloninger, 1987); TSQ, Trauma Screening Questionnaire (Brewin et al., 2002); CON, constraint/
impulsivity; NEM, negative emotionality/neuroticism; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.

Two signiicant limitations of these studies should by elevated neuroticism and introversion at the time
be noted. First, most of them only measured a single of hospital discharge were more likely to be diag-
dimension of personality so little is known about the nosed with PTSD at 4 and 12 months post-discharge.
possible direct or interactive efects of other dimen- Similarly, Carlier et al. (1997) assessed the personal-
sions. Second, it is plausible that the presence of ity characteristics of police oicers two weeks ater
pre-trauma anxiety or mood disorders at the irst traumatic exposure in the line of duty and found that
assessment may have contributed to the observed asso- introversion, but in this case not neuroticism, pre-
ciations between measures of NEM and the subsequent dicted the presence of PTSD symptoms three months
development of PTSD. later, even ater controlling for trauma severity, emo-
tional exhaustion during the trauma, social sup-
Post-trauma prospective studies port, and emotional expressivity. Holeva and Tarrier
As summarized in Table 4.2, post-trauma prospective (2001) reported that neuroticism scores on the
studies suggest a possible role for not only NEM but Eysenck Personality Questionnaire obtained within
also PEM in resilience to traumatic stress. Fauerbach one month of a traumatic motor vehicle accident pre-
et al. (2000) found that burn survivors characterized dicted PTSD deinition four to six months later, even

60
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

Table 4.2 Post-trauma prospective studies of personality and post-traumatic stress disorder.

Personality PTSD Personality-PTSD


Study Sample assessment assessment Method findings
Bennett et al., 89 male and PANAS-NA (trait) PDS Negative effect Negative effect trait
2002 female patients measured in at time 1 predicted
with recent hospital; PDS PTSD symptom
myocardial completed 3 severity at time 2
infarction months later
Carlier et al., 262 male and Sensation- SI-PTSD Participants Introversion
1997 female police seeking, assessed 2 measured 2 weeks
officers neuroticism and, weeks, 3 months, post-trauma was a
extraversion and 12 months significant predictor
following of the presence
traumatic events of PTSD symptom
experienced on severity at 3 months
the job
Fauerbach et al., 70 male and NEO-FFI SCID Participants Neuroticism and
2000 female burn completed the introversion at
survivors NEO at hospital time 1 predicted
discharge; PTSD subsequent PTSD
assessed at diagnosis
discharge, and 4
and 12 months
later
Holeva & Tarrier, 265 male and EPQ Penn Inventory EPQ was Neuroticism and
2001 female survivors administered psychoticism at time
of a motor vehicle 2–4 weeks after 1 predicted PTSD
accident the accident symptom severity at
and PTSD was time 2
assessed 4–6
months after
Lawrence & 158 male and NEO-FFI DTS NEO was Higher neuroticism
Fauerbach, 2003 female burn administered scores at time 1
survivors during predicted PTSD
hospitalization; diagnosis at time
DTS administered 2 (statistical trend);
at 1 and 6 months extraversion,
after discharge openness,
agreeableness, and
conscientiousness
at time 1 were
not significant
predictors of PTSD
at time 2
McFarlane, 1992 290 male EPQ DIS EPQ was Neuroticism at time
firefighters completed 29 1 predicted PTSD
months after diagnosis at time 2
the fire and
the DIS was
administered 13
months later

61
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Table 4.2 (cont.)

Personality PTSD Personality-PTSD


Study Sample assessment assessment Method findings
Nightingale & 39 male and NEO-FFI Self-report Participants were Although
Williams, 2000 female survivors inventory assessed 1 and 6 analyses did not
of a motor vehicle weeks after the directly address
accident accident the relationship
between the NEO
and PTSD, openness,
extraversion, and
agreeableness
were shown to
be associated
with attitudes
towards emotional
expression, which, in
turn, predicted PTSD
symptom severity at
time 2
Punamaki et al., 86 male and Arabic CPTSD-RI EPQ administered Higher neuroticism
2001 female Palestinian translation during the symptoms at time
children exposed of EPQ Intifada in 1993 1 predicted PTSD
to political (Neuroticism (time 1) and at at 3-year follow-
violence Scale) (El 3-year follow-up up; neuroticism
Khalek, 1978) (time 2); CPTSD- symptoms decreased
RI administered significantly from
at time 2 time 1 to time 2
Qouta et al., 2007 65 male and Arabic CPTSD-RI EPQ administered Higher neuroticism
female Palestinian translation during the at time 1 (middle
adolescents of EPQ Intifada in 1993 childhood) predicted
exposed to (Neuroticism (time 1); CPTSD- PTSD severity at time
military violence Scale) (El Khalek, RI administered at 3 (adolescence)
1978) 7-year follow-
up (time 3)
Roca et al., 1992 29 male and NEO-FFI SCID NEO was Individuals
female burn administered who endorsed
survivors near the time re-experiencing
of hospital symptoms at
discharge; SCID follow-up had lower
administered 4 openness scores at
months later discharge
CPTSD-RI, Children’s Post-traumatic Stress Disorder Reaction Index (Frederick et al., 1992); DIS, DSM-III-R Diagnostic Interview Schedule
(Robins et al., 1989); DTS, Davidson Trauma Scale (Fontana & Rosenheck, 1994); EPQ, Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Eysenck &
Eysenck, 1975); NEO-FFI, NEO Five Factor Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992); PANAS-NA, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, negative
affect (Watson et al., 1988b); PDS, Post-traumatic Diagnostic Scale (Foa et al., 1997); SCID, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM (Spitzer
et al., 1987); SI-PTSD, Structured Interview for PTSD (Davidson et al., 1989); PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder.

ater controlling for the inluence of variables such In one of the more sophisticated studies of this type,
as previous accident history, accident severity, the Lawrence and Fauerbach (2003) used structural equa-
presence of acute stress disorder, and peritraumatic tion modeling to examine the relationship between
dissociation. several predictor variables (i.e., personality measured

62
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

with the NEO Five Factor Inventory [NEO-FFI; Costa interesting (commitment, as opposed to alienation),
& McCrae, 1992], coping strategies, social support, appraise stressors as changeable (control, as opposed
and chronic stress) and PTSD symptoms in a sample to powerlessness), and see change as a normal aspect
of male and female burn survivors. Results showed of life and an opportunity for growth (challenge, as
that higher neuroticism scores during hospitalization opposed to threat). Hardiness is thought to moderate
predicted greater PTSD symptoms at one month post- the impact of stress on physical health, mental health,
discharge, and avoidant coping was found to mediate and other indices of stress adaptation and has been
roughly half of the variance of this association. Also shown to predict (1) the physical health of men (Kobasa
of note, extraversion (PEM) was found to inluence et al., 1981) and women (Lawler & Schmied, 1992) over
PTSD symptoms through indirect efects mediated by a ive year period of time, (2) the mental health, per-
active coping and social support. Finally, in two related formance, and probability of graduating from stressful
studies (Punamaki et al., 2001; Qouta et al., 2007), military training programs (Westman, 1990; Florian
Palestinian children were followed prospectively ater et al., 1995), and (3) the mental health of disaster assist-
exposure to military violence. he Arabic transla- ance workers (Bartone et al., 1989).
tion of the Junior Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Many cross-sectional studies of trauma-exposed
Neuroticism Scale (El Khalek, 1978) was administered individuals have reported signiicant associations
to a group of Palestinian children during the height between hardiness and PTSD symptoms. For example,
of the Intifada violence and PTSD symptoms were King and colleagues used structural equation mod-
assessed at follow-up visits ater three and seven years eling to analyze data from 1632 veterans collected for
during more peaceful conditions. Results showed that the National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study
higher neuroticism symptoms at baseline predicted (Kulka et al., 1990) in a pair of studies that examined
the development of PTSD at the three-year follow-up the relative impact on the development of PTSD of
(Punamaki et al., 2001) and at the seven-year follow-up hardiness, pre-war risk factors, war-zone stressors,
(Qouta et al., 2007). post-war life stressors, and other post-war recovery
and resilience factors. In both studies, hardiness was
indexed by an 11-item scale constructed from the
Hardiness Readjustment Study’s interview items. he structural
Although not derived from the tradition of trait person- equation model in the irst study (King et al., 1998)
ality research, an important subset of resilience studies revealed evidence of a direct negative association
has focused on a related construct termed hardiness, between hardiness and PTSD symptom severity as
developed originally by Kobasa (1979). Hardiness is well as an indirect efect of hardiness on PTSD medi-
conceptualized as a constellation of personality char- ated by social support. In other words, hardy individ-
acteristics that function as a resilience resource dur- uals showed less-severe PTSD and were more likely
ing encounters with stressful life events and it relates to efectively utilize support from others, which,
to how individuals perceive and cope with such events. in itself, was associated with lower PTSD sever-
Although it has not generally been conceptualized as ity. Similar results were observed in a second study
a temperament-based dimension of behavior, there is using a diferent combination of predictor variables
a theoretical and empirical basis for relating it to PEM (King et al., 1999). Other work suggests that the inlu-
and NEM. ence of hardiness may be conditional on the sever-
Hardiness is thought to relect a combination of ity of trauma exposure. For example, Bartone (1999)
adaptive personality characteristics complemented assessed hardiness, combat exposure, and PTSD
by an early learning history characterized by rich, symptoms in Gulf War veterans shortly ater the war.
rewarding, and varied experiences and includes three Multiple regression analyses were used to predict
facets: commitment, the sense of meaning, purpose, PTSD severity, revealing a main efect of hardiness
and perseverance attributed to one’s existence; control, and a signiicant interaction between hardiness and
one’s sense of autonomy and perceived ability to inlu- combat exposure. hese results indicated that highly
ence one’s destiny and manage experiences; and chal- hardy veterans were less symptomatic than those with
lenge, one’s tendency to perceive change as an exciting low hardiness overall, and this diference was more
opportunity for growth. Hardy individuals tend to pronounced for individuals reporting the highest lev-
view potentially stressful situations as meaningful and els of combat exposure.

63
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Several recent studies have employed more inform- on hardiness tend to feel alienated from themselves
ative longitudinal designs to study the inluence of and others, powerless, and in need of security – char-
hardiness on the adaptation to stress. For example, acteristics that are strongly associated with NEM and
Adler and Dolan (2006) assessed deployment stres- general measures of maladjustment. Further research
sors, hardiness, and depression in a large sample of is needed to speciically address the extent to which
military peacekeepers during and ater deployment to measures of hardiness provide incremental validity
a crisis in Kosovo. As in Bartone’s (1999) study, ana- beyond personality traits (or vise versa) in predicting
lyses showed that hardiness moderated the inluence post-traumatic adjustment.
of deployment-related stress on depression symptoms,
with these efects evident only for soldiers exposed to A conceptual model for the
high levels of deployment stress. Only one published
study has measured hardiness at more than one time
interface between personality and
point, which is useful because it permits examin- post-traumatic adjustment
ation of the stability of the construct. Vogt et al. (2008) his chapter has examined the literature on person-
assessed US marine recruits at the beginning and end ality factors involved in resilience to traumatic stress.
of basic training and found that men who were hardier Studies have been discussed in relation to the “big
at time 1 reported lower stress reactions at time 2, while three” dimensions of personality – PEM, NEM, and
men who reported more stress reactions at time 1 were CON – that are represented in most contemporary
less hardy at time 2, which points to the malleability of trait models of personality. Numerous studies sup-
the construct. port the conclusion that NEM/neuroticism is the pri-
mary personality risk/resilience factor inluencing
Relating hardiness to the big three post-traumatic adjustment. Individuals high in NEM
describe themselves as nervous, tense, and easily upset.
personality dimensions hey are prone to worry, report feeling vulnerable and
Investigators who have examined the relationship sensitive, and experience negative emotional states
between hardiness and trait measures of omnibus per- such as anger, distress, and guilt at a high frequency
sonality inventories have suggested that hardiness rep- and intensity even under everyday life conditions.
resents the inverse of NEM (Funk & Houston, 1987; On the other end of the spectrum, individuals low in
Rhodewalt & Zone, 1989; Funk, 1992), or a combin- NEM describe themselves as emotionally stable, rarely
ation of low NEM and high PEM (Parkes & Rendall, nervous or anxious, and with the capacity to remain
1988). Evidence for the latter comes from several calm in stressful situations and to recover quickly from
sources. For example, Maddi and colleagues (2002) negative experiences. When these characteristics are
investigated the construct validity of the Hardiness combined with a disposition to be cheerful and enthu-
Scale by examining its relationship to the Minnesota siastic, take pleasure in and value close interpersonal
Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) and ties, and to be assertive, hardworking, and inluential
the NEO-FFI and found negative correlations greater (i.e., high PEM), the proile that emerges is that of an
than 0.6 between hardiness and several MMPI-2 scales individual who is highly resilient to stress, loss, adver-
including the Infrequency (F), Social Introversion (Si), sity, trauma, and the development of post-traumatic
Posttraumatic Stress (PK), Low Self-esteem (LSE), psychopathology.
and Welsh Anxiety Scales (A; Maddi et al., 2002). Figure 4.1 illustrates the big three dimensions
his study also found that the NEO Neuroticism and of personality and their hypothesized relations with
Extraversion Scales were correlated with total hardi- resilience versus vulnerability to the development of
ness at levels of −0.46 and +0.47, respectively, and post-traumatic psychopathology. All three form the
similar results have been reported by other investiga- foundation of the model. he median of each dimen-
tors (Parkes & Rendall, 1988; Ramanaiah et al., 1999). sion is located in the center of the igure; high and low
Other work has shown that the relationship between extremes on each dimension are toward the periphery
hardiness and health outcomes disappears when of the igure. Characteristics associated with vulner-
measures of NEM are removed (Funk & Houston, ability to the development of psychopathology are
1987; Rhodewalt & Zone, 1989). hese indings sug- located in the upper (i.e., high NEM) part of the igure,
gest collectively that individuals who obtain low scores while factors associated with resilience are in the lower

64
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

High NEM versus internalizing co-morbidity (c.f. Krueger et al.,


Ex 1998). Across these studies, externalizers produced
g ter
izin na
rnal liz personality proiles deined by low CON coupled with
Inte Vulnerability ing Low CON
Low PEM
high NEM. Individuals in this subgroup were charac-
terized by the tendency to express post-traumatic dis-
tress outwardly through antagonistic interactions with
others and behavior that conlicts with societal norms
and values. hey endorsed elevated levels of anger and
aggression, substance-related disorders, and DSM-IV
Cluster B personality disorder features. In contrast,
internalizers were characterized by personality proiles
ce High PEM deined by high NEM combined with low PEM and ten-
High CON si lien
Re dencies to direct their post-traumatic distress inwardly
through shame, self-defeating/deprecating and anx-
Low NEM ious processes, avoidance, depression, and withdrawal.
Individuals of this type were characterized by high rates
Figure 4.1 The hypothesized interactive model of relationships
between the “big three” dimensions of personality and resilience of co-morbid major depression and panic disorder, and
versus vulnerability to the development of post-traumatic schizoid and avoidant personality disorder features.
psychopathology. NEM, negative emotionality; PEM, positive Although etiological inferences about the extent to
emotionality; CON, constraint/impulsivity.
which these subtypes relect the inluence of pre-morbid
personality were limited by the cross-sectional methods
part. While NEM is viewed as the primary personality employed in these studies, evidence for the longitudi-
risk/resilience factor for post-traumatic psychopath- nal stability of personality traits in other research (e.g.,
ology, low PEM and low CON are conceptualized as Costa & McCrae, 1977, 1992; Watson & Walker, 1996)
moderating factors that inluence the form and expres- and indications of greater pre-military delinquency in
sion of the post-traumatic adaptation (c.f. Miller, 2003, the externalizing group in two of the studies (Miller
2004). Individuals characterized prior to trauma by high et al., 2003, 2004) are consistent with the notion that
NEM combined with low PEM are thought to be predis- these subtypes relect the inluence of pre-morbid traits
posed towards an internalizing form of post-traumatic on the expression of post-traumatic symptomatology.
response, characterized by marked anhedonia, depres- It is also possible, however, that these subtypes
sion, and social withdrawal. In contrast, pre-morbid represent the manifestation of personality traits that
high NEM combined with low CON is expected to pre- were altered as a consequence of the development of
dict an externalizing form of post-traumatic response, PTSD. Successful personality development requires
characterized by tendencies towards impulsivity, learning to inhibit pathological dispositional tenden-
aggression, antisociality, substance-related disorders, cies, including dispositions towards the internalization
and higher rates of the “dramatic–emotional” Cluster versus externalization of distress. For the individual
B personality disorders. with elevated trait NEM, this necessitates developing
Results from a recent series of cluster-analytic stud- the capacity to regulate and inhibit the expression of
ies of personality proiles of combat veterans and sexual intense negative afectivity. We hypothesize that under
assault survivors provide preliminary support for these periods of intense stress, and during the experience
propositions (Miller et al., 2003, 2004; Miller & Resick, of PTSD, these normal self-regulatory processes are
2007). In each of these studies, cluster analyses of per- compromised. his results in an unrestrained system
sonality proiles of individuals with PTSD yielded evi- driven by basic dispositional tendencies and impaired
dence for a three-group typology. he irst subgroup was in the ability to adjust efectively to ongoing social and
a lower pathology or “simple PTSD” cluster comprising environmental demands. Individuals with a dispos-
individuals with less-severe PTSD symptoms, low levels ition towards internalization (i.e., low PEM combined
of co-morbidity, and normal-range personality proiles with high NEM) resort to withdrawing from the world
compared with individuals in the other two clusters. through social avoidance and passivity, and their post-
he other two more pathological or “complex PTSD” traumatic adjustment is characterized by negative self-
clusters difered on variables related to externalizing referential or ruminative thoughts, guilt, anxiety, and

65
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

depression. In contrast, individuals with a propensity hard. When these traits co-occur with low NEM the
towards externalizing behaviors (i.e., low CON com- result is a combination of characteristics that resem-
bined with high NEM) move against the world becom- ble both hardiness and a related construct termed
ing at odds with others through antisocial behavior, mental toughness (Clough et al., 2002). Clough and
antagonism, hostility, and aggression. colleagues (sports psychologists) developed the con-
Consistent with this alternative hypothesis, prior struct of mental toughness to capture the personality
theorists have conjectured that personality traits are attributes of elite athletes, who tend to be social and
most inluential and evident in circumstances involv- outgoing, calm and relaxed, with low anxiety and “an
ing stress and signiicant life re-adjustment (Caspi & unshakeable faith that they control their own destiny”
Bem, 1990). Prior studies have shown that the nega- (Clough et al., 2002, p. 38). Mental toughness has been
tive traits of irritable and explosive men were inten- shown to predict physical endurance (Crust & Clough,
siied during the severe economic stress of the Great 2005) and achievement in professional sports (Golby
Depression (Elder & Caspi, 1988), and the coping styles & Sheard, 2004). Like hardiness, mental toughness
of entrepreneurs whose businesses were destroyed by is highly correlated with neuroticism and extraver-
natural disaster were accentuated during the atermath sion (r = −0.64, 0.45, respectively [Horsburgh et al.,
(Anderson, 1977). Similarly, Allport et al. (1941), who 2009]). Not surprisingly, these traits are also the per-
studied the experiences of individuals during the Nazi sonality characteristics selected for in elite military
revolution, concluded (pp. 7–8) that “very rarely does units (e.g., US Navy SEALS [Braun et al., 1994]) and
catastrophic… change produce catastrophic altera- have been found to predict leadership across a wide
tions in personality… Where change does take place it range of military and other professional settings (for a
seems invariably to accentuate trends clearly present in comprehensive review, see Judge et al. [2002]). Finally,
the pre-[trauma] personality.” analyses in the study of Horsburgh et al. (2009), which
compared the similarity of mono- and dizygotic twins
High positive emotionality/extraversion and on a measure of mental toughness, showed that 52%
of variability in this trait could be attributed to genetic
resilience efects, underscoring the role of temperament and con-
he orientation of Figure 4.1 emphasizes low NEM as stitutional factors in the etiology of resilience-related
the primary personality resilience factor, but what role traits. hese indings also beg the question, what are
might high PEM play in also conferring resilience to the mechanisms by which these genetic efects confer
traumatic stress? In the model proposed by Tellegen risk/resilience to the development of post-traumatic
(1985, 2000), PEM is composed of four traits. he irst psychopathology? Findings from three extant areas
two are associated with communal positive emotional- of research – conditionability, coping style, and social
ity: well-being, the tendency to be optimistic, hopeful, support – provide possible answers to this question
cheerful, and interested and engaged in one’s activities; and are reviewed below.
and social closeness, the disposition toward social com-
munion and interpersonal connectedness. Individuals Mediators of the link between
with high levels of these traits attract and main-
tain close relationships with family, friends, and co- personality and resilience
workers, and they experience greater perceived social
support. his, in turn, has been shown repeatedly to Conditionability
be one of the most robust psychosocial moderators of Low NEM has been conceptualized as a relative
post-traumatic adjustment (Connell & D’Augelli, 1990; insensitivity of the neurobiological system underlying
King et al., 1998; Lawrence & Fauerbach, 2003). defensive behavior (the behavioral inhibition system
Tellegen’s two other PEM traits involve tenden- [Gray, 1987], see also Zuckerman, 1983; Panksepp,
cies towards agency and eicacy in the social and 1992; Tellegen, 2000) and evidence points to the cen-
work domains (i.e., agentic positive emotionality): tral nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the
social potency is the tendency towards being persua- stria terminalis as integral components of this system
sive, inluential, enjoying social visibility, and being (Davis & Lee, 1998; LeDoux, 2000). One manifestation
in charge; achievement relates to being ambitious, of variation in functioning of this system is individ-
persistent, enjoying challenging tasks, and working ual diferences in conditionability – individuals who

66
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

are most susceptible to aversive conditioning may avoidance coping (behaviors or emotions that avoid or
be predisposed to acquire the intense and persistent ignore the source of the stress) (Folkman & Lazarus,
conditioned emotional responses that are central to 1980; Folkman, 1984; Herman-Stahl et al., 1995).
the psychopathology of PTSD (Keane et al., 1985; Foa Individuals high in NEM (or with the high NEM/low
et al., 1989). Evidence in support of this hypothesis PEM combination) have been found to be more likely to
comes from many sources. First, aversive conditioning utilize the latter two, less adaptive, forms of coping and
and the acquisition of punishment expectancies has are less likely to employ problem-focused/approach
been shown to be enhanced in individuals high in trait coping strategies (e.g., McCrae & Costa, 1986; Parkes,
anxiety (i.e., NEM) (Spence & Taylor, 1951; Spence & 1986; Bolger, 1990; Endler & Parker, 1990; Gomez et al.,
Farber, 1953; Spence & Beecrot, 1954; Spence et al., 1999; Volrath & Torgersen, 2000).
1954; Zinbarg & Mohlman, 1998) and introversion Investigators have also identiied a link between
(i.e., low PEM) (Franks, 1957; Shagass & Kerenyi, 1958; dysfunctional coping and the development and/or
Costello, 1967; Mangan, 1967; Kantorowitz, 1978; maintenance of PTSD. For example, in one longitudi-
Paisey & Mangan, 1988; Gupta & Shukla, 1989; Weyer, nal study of combat-exposed Israeli soldiers, Solomon
1989). Second, by deinition, high NEM is associated et al. (1988) showed that PTSD symptomatology was
with enhanced reactions to negative afect-evoking predicted by the over-utilization of emotion-focused
situations, which, in turn, facilitates conditioning. For coping and underutilization of problem-focused cop-
example, high NEM has been shown to be associated ing strategies. Along the same lines, Morgan et al.
with greater nervousness/negative afectivity during (1995) found that neuroticism (NEM) was positively
stressful laboratory procedures (e.g., Carver & White, associated with use of emotion-focused coping strate-
1994; Miller & Patrick, 2000) and enhanced acoustic gies and that both predicted PTSD severity in a sam-
startle relex potentiation (an index of defensive emo- ple of civilian lood survivors. Similar indings have
tional responding) during processing of threatening been observed in other samples of civilians (Amir
stimuli when compared with subjects with low NEM et al., 1997) and combat veterans (Nezu & Carnevale,
(Wilson et al., 2000; Craske et al., 2009). Finally, to 1987; Blake et al., 1992) with PTSD. he implication
the extent that individual diferences in condition- of these indings is that individuals with high NEM
ability relect an enduring characteristic, evidence who experience a traumatic event may have a stronger
that patients with PTSD are more conditionable than disposition towards avoidance of trauma-related stim-
trauma-exposed individuals without the disorder (Orr uli than others. As a result, they would be expected to
et al., 2000) would also support this hypothesis. have fewer opportunities to experience the beneits
of exposure to corrective information (Foa & Kozak,
1986) and other cognitive–afective processing of the
Coping style traumatic experience that is believed to be essential for
Another mechanism by which personality can trans- positive post-traumatic adjustment.
late into resilience to traumatic stress is via coping
style. Coping has been deined as the cognitions and
behaviors that a person uses to reduce stress (Billings Social support
et al., 1983; Folkman, 1984) and habitual coping strat- Other research suggests that social support plays a
egies are believed to moderate the impact of stressful key role in mitigating the negative efects of stress on
life events on functioning (Pearlin & Schooler, 1978; mental health (e.g., Cohen & Wills, 1985). Low PEM
Billings & Moos, 1981). Although coping is conceptu- and high NEM have both been associated with low
alized as a process that varies as a function of contextual scores on measures of social support (e.g., Bolger &
demands, there is also evidence that coping styles tend Eckenrode, 1991; Morgan et al., 1995; Lara et al., 1997;
to be consistent over time and predicted by personality Caspi, 2000) and evidence suggests that each charac-
traits (McCrae & Costa, 1986; Volrath et al., 1995). teristic may be associated with a unique form of social
Many investigators have distinguished between impairment. Speciically, investigators have suggested
problem-focused or approach coping (the channeling that PEM, but not NEM, is associated with measures of
of resources to solving the stress creating problem), actual number of social contacts (Bolger & Eckenrode,
emotion-focused coping (the easing of tension though 1991), whereas high NEM relates to a tendency to
denial or changing one’s attitude towards it), and underestimate the amount of social support that is

67
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

available (Stokes & McKirnan, 1989) and is associated crucial etiological factor is the nature and severity
with a propensity to experience conlict with members of the trauma exposure, and in future work it will be
of the social network (Caspi, 2000). important to develop models that better account for
Research on psychosocial factors that contribute the interaction between personality and trauma inten-
to the development of PTSD has shown that social sity. In such work, it may be useful to draw from the
support bufers the adverse psychological efects of models of diathesis–stress inluences in depression
stressful life events in combat veterans (Stretch, 1985, outlined by Monroe and Simons (1991) and to exam-
1986) and sexual assault survivors (e.g., Ruch & Leon, ine whether the personality–trauma interaction is
1983; Popiel & Susskind, 1985; Golding et al., 1989). best represented by an additive or interactive model.
Conversely, inadequate social support has been impli- In an additive model, trauma severity and the loading
cated in the etiology of delayed-onset PTSD (Buckley of the personality trait (i.e., the diathesis) summate to
et al., 1996). Impaired social support is known to co- determine the probability of developing PTSD. Two
occur with PTSD (Kulka et al., 1990; Boscarino, 1995; independent efects are postulated: (1) the likelihood
King et al., 1998) and a meta-analysis of 14 separate of PTSD increases with the severity of the trauma,
risk factors for PTSD showed that lack of social support and (2) the probability of developing the disorder
was second only to trauma severity in the proportion of increases as a function of the vulnerability. In this
variance accounted for in predicting concurrent PTSD model individuals with a low diathetic loading (e.g.,
symptomatology (Brewin et al., 2000). Consequently, low NEM) would require more extreme levels of stress
social support may represent a third mechanism by to achieve the same likelihood of developing the dis-
which these personality characteristics translate into order as someone with higher levels of the diathesis,
risk versus resilience to the development of PTSD fol- and vice versa.
lowing trauma exposure. Alternatively, in an interactive model, personality
traits would interact with the stressor to determine the
probability of developing PTSD such that at certain lev-
Conclusions and future directions els of trauma intensity vulnerable individuals would be
Although this chapter has focused primarily on the at disproportionately high risk for the disorder. Figure
inluence of personality factors it is important to rec- 4.2 depicts such a model. At low levels of NEM, PTSD
ognize that post-traumatic adaptations relect the con- is unlikely to occur in response to even high levels of
luence of an array of environmental and individual trauma severity. However, with high NEM, individ-
diference factors and that personality traits account uals are at high risk for the development of the dis-
for only a portion of the variance in any outcome. One order across levels of trauma intensity. he slopes of

Figure 4.2 An interactive model of the


Maximum relationship between negative emotionality
(NEM) and the intensity of a traumatic
PTSD risk stressor. PTSD, post-traumatic stress
disorder; Mod, moderate.
High
Trauma severity

Mod–high
Moderate
Low–mod

Low

Minimum

0 20 40 60 80 100
NEM percentile

68
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

the demarcation lines diverge across increasing levels Agaibi, C. E. & Wilson, J. P. (2005). Trauma, PTSD, and
of the x-axis indicating a synergistic, as opposed to addi- resilience. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 6, 195–216.
tive, relationship between the variables. Individual dif- Allport, G. W., Bruner, J. S., & Jandorf, E. M. (1941).
ferences in the response to trauma are most apparent at Personality under social catastrophe: Ninety life-histories
low levels of stress. Extreme levels of stress place virtu- of the Nazi revolution. Character and Personality: A
Quarterly for Psychodiagnostic and Allied Studies, 10,
ally anyone at a moderate-high or high risk for PTSD,
1–22.
regardless of what temperamental resources exist. At the
lowest levels of traumatic stress, however, there is a much American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders, 3rd edn.
wider range in the probability of developing PTSD. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
At this early stage in the development of research
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic
in this area, it is unclear whether personality trait dif-
and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn.
ferences in resilience are more likely to be observed in Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
response to low or high levels of stress. Some investi-
Amir, M., Kaplan, Z., Efroni, R., et al. (1997). Coping styles
gators have suggested that individual characteristics in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) patients.
can be expected to play a greater role in inluencing the Personality and Individual Diferences, 23, 399–405.
nature and severity of stress reaction under conditions Anderson, C. R. (1977). Locus of control, coping behaviors,
of lower stress intensity (e.g., Rabkin & Struening, and performance in a stress setting: A longitudinal study.
1976; Foy et al., 1987; McCranie et al., 1992; Lee et al., Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 446–451.
1995) and that extreme degrees of stress should pro- Bartone, P. T. (1999). Hardiness protects against war-related
duce symptoms of psychiatric disability in virtually all stress in army reserve forces. Consulting Psychology
who are exposed to them (Hocking, 1970). Others have Journal: Practice and Research, 51, 72–82.
suggested that the inluence of individual diferences Bartone, P. T., Ursano, R. J., Wright, K. M., & Ingraham, L. H.
on the development of PTSD may be more pronounced (1989). he impact of a military air disaster on the health
at high levels of stress exposure (e.g., Bartone, 1999; of assistance workers: A prospective study. Journal of
Casella & Motta, 1990). Clearly more work is needed in Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 317–328.
this area to test these alternative hypotheses. Bennett, P., Owen, R. L., Koutsakis, S., & Bisson, J. (2002).
Evidence that certain combinations of personality Personality, social context and cognitive predictors of
traits (i.e., high PEM with low NEM) may be most reli- post-traumatic stress disorder in myocardial infarction
ably linked to resilience underscores the importance of patients. Psychology and Health, 17, 489–500.
moving beyond single-scale assessments of individual Berridge, K. C. (2003). Pleasures of the brain. Brain and
traits towards more comprehensive multidimensional Cognition, 52, 106–128.
assessments of personality. It would also be beneicial Bianchi, G. N. & Fergusson, D. M. (1977). Short term mental
for investigators interested in constructs such as con- state and scores on the Eysenck Personality Inventory.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 131, 306–309.
ditionability, coping, and social support to examine
the extent to which these behaviors relect the inlu- Billings, A. G. & Moos, R. H. (1981). he role of coping
ence of higher-order personality traits and to attempt responses and social resources in attenuating the stress of
life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 139–157.
to relate these more speciic constructs to a broader
understanding of individuals’ personality functioning Billings, A. G., Cronkite, R. C., & Moos, R. H. (1983).
Social-environmental factors in unipolar depression:
across social, cognitive, afective, and biological levels
Comparisons of depressed patients and nondepressed
of analysis. controls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 119–133.
Blake, D. D., Cook, J. D., & Keane, T. M. (1992). Post-
Acknowledgement traumatic stress disorder and coping in veterans who are
seeking medical treatment. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
his work was supported in part by a VA Merit Review
48, 695–704.
Award to Dr. Miller.
Block, J. (1961). Ego identity, role variability, and
adjustment. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 25,
References 392–397.
Adler, A. B. & Dolan, C. A. (2006). Military hardiness Bolger, N. (1990). Coping as a personality process: A
as a bufer of psychological health on return from prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social
deployment. Military Medicine, 171, 93–98. Psychology, 59, 525–537.

69
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Bolger, N. & Eckenrode, J. (1991). Social relationships, rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of
personality, and anxiety during a major stressful event. Primatology, 65, 289–294.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 440–449. Carlier, I. V. E., Lamberts, R. D., & Gersons, B. P. R. (1997).
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Risk factors for posttraumatic stress symptomatology in
Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive police oicers: A prospective analysis. Journal of Nervous
ater extremely aversive events?. American Psychologist, and Mental Disease, 185, 498–506.
59, 20–28. Carver, C. S. & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition,
Bono, J. E. & Judge, T. A. (2004). Personality and behavioral activation, and afective responses to
transformational and transactional leadership: A meta- impending reward and punishment: he BIS/BAS scales.
analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 901–910. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.
Boscarino, J. A. (1995). Post-traumatic stress and associated Casella, L. & Motta, R. W. (1990). Comparison of
disorders among Vietnam veterans: he signiicance characteristics of Vietnam veterans with and without
of combat exposure and social support. Journal of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Reports, 67,
Traumatic Stress, 8, 317–336. 595–605.
Bramsen, I., Dirkzwager, A. J. E., & van der Ploeg, H. M. Caspi, A. (2000). he child is father of the man: Personality
(2000). Predeployment personality traits and exposure to continuities from childhood to adulthood. Journal of
trauma as predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms: Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 158–172.
A prospective study of former peacekeepers. American Caspi, A. & Bem, D. J. (1990). Personality continuity and
Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1115–1119. change across the life course. In L. A. Pervin (ed.),
Braun, D. E., Prusaczyk, W. K., Goforth, H. W., & Pratt, N. C. Handbook of personality: heory and research
(1994). Personality proiles of US Navy sea–air–land (pp. 549–575). New York: Guilford Press.
(SEAL) personnel. [US Naval Health Research Center Clark, L. A. & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety
Technical Report NHRC 94–8.] San Diego, CA: US Naval and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic
Health Research Center. implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100,
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta- 316–336.
analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical
in trauma-exposed individuals. Journal of Consulting and description and classiication of personality traits.
Clinical Psychology, 68, 748–766. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–588.
Brewin, C. R., Rose, S., Andrews, B., et al. (2002). Brief Cloninger, C. R. (1992). he Temperament and Character
screening instrument for post-traumatic stress disorder. Inventory. (Available from C. R. Cloninger, Washington
British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 158–162. University School of Medicine, Department of
Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). Psychiatry, PO Box 8134, St. Louis, MO, 63110, USA.)
Structural relationships among dimensions of the Clough, P. J., Earle, K., & Sewell, D. (2002). Mental
DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders and dimensions of toughness: he concept and its measurement. In
negative afect, positive afect, and autonomic arousal. I. Cockerill (ed.), Solutions in sport psychology
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 179–192. (pp. 32–45). London: homson.
Bryant, R. A., Harvey, A. G., Guthrie, R. M., & Moulds, M. L. Cohen, S. & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the
(2000). A prospective study of psychophysiological bufering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.
arousal, acute stress disorder, and posttraumatic stress Connell, C. M. & D’Augelli, A. R. (1990). he contribution
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 341–344. of personality characteristics to the relationship between
Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Hickling, E. J. (1996). social support and perceived physical health. Health
A prospective examination of delayed onset PTSD Psychology, 9, 192–207.
secondary to motor vehicle accidents. Journal of Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1977). Age diferences in
Abnormal Psychology, 105, 617–625. personality structure revisited: Studies in validity, stability,
Buss, A. H. & Plomin, R. (1975). A temperament theory of and change. Aging and Human Development, 8, 261–275.
personality development. New York: Wiley. Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Inluence of
Capitanio, J. P. (1999). Personality dimensions in adult extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being:
male rhesus macaques: Prediction of behaviors across Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and
time and situation. American Journal of Primatology, 47, Social Psychology, 38, 668–678.
299–320. Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1985). he NEO Personality
Capitanio, J. P. & Widaman, K. F. (2005). Conirmatory Inventory Manual. Odessa, Fl: Psychological Assessment
factor analysis of personality structure in adult male Resources.

70
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Trait psychology comes coping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
of age. In T. B. Sonderegger (ed.), Nebraska symposium 687–695.
on motivation 1991: Psychology and aging. Current theory Foa, E. B. & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of
and research in motivation, 39 (pp. 169–204). Lincoln, fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological
NE: University of Nebraska Press. Bulletin, 99, 20–35.
Costello, C. G. (1967). Extraversion, neuroticism, and the Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., & Rothbaum, B. O. (1989).
classical conditioning of word meaning. Psychonomic Behavioral/cognitive conceptualizations of post-
Science, 8, 307–308. traumatic stress disorder. Behavior herapy, 20,
Craske, M. G., Waters, A. M., Nazarian, M., et al. (2009). 155–176.
Does neuroticism in adolescents moderate contextual Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Dancu, C. V., & Rothbaum, B. O.
and explicit threat cue modulation of the startle relex? (1993). Reliability and validity of a brief instrument
Biological Psychiatry, 65, 220–226. for assessing post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of
Crust, L. & Clough, P. J. (2005). Relationship between Traumatic Stress, 6, 459–473.
mental toughness and physical endurance. Perceptual Foa, E., Cashman, L., Jaycox, L., & Perry, K. (1997). he
and Motor Skills, 100, 192–194. validation of a self-report measure of post-traumatic
Davidson, J. T. R., Smith, R., & Kudler, H. S. (1989). Validity stress disorder: he Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale.
and reliability of the DSM-III criteria for post-traumatic Psychological Assessment, 9, 445–451.
stress disorder: Experience with a structured interview. Folkman, S. (1984). Personal control and stress and coping
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 336–341. processes: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Personality
Davis, M. & Lee, Y. (1998). Fear and anxiety: Possible roles and Social Psychology, 46, 839–852.
of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in
Cognition and Emotion, 12, 277–305. a middle-aged community sample. Journal of Health and
Duncan-Jones, P., Fergusson, D. M., Ormel, J., & Horwood, Social Behavior, 21, 219–239.
L. J. (1990). A model of stability and change in minor Fontana, A. & Rosenheck, R. (1994). Posttraumatic stress
psychiatric symptoms: Results from three longitudinal disorder among Vietnam theater veterans: A causal
studies. Psychological Medicine, Monograph Supplement model of etiology in a community sample. Journal of
18, iii–28. Nervous and Mental Disease, 182, 677–684.
Elder, G. H. & Caspi, A. (1988). Economic stress in lives: Foy, D. W., Resnick, H. S., Sipprelle, R. C., & Carroll, E. M.
Developmental perspectives. Journal of Social Issues, 44, (1987). Premilitary, military, and postmilitary factors in
25–45. the development of combat-related posttraumatic stress
El Khalek, A. S. B. G. (1978). Eysenck personality inventories. disorder. Behavior herapist, 10, 3–9.
[In Arabic.] Alexandria: Dar El Maaref. Franks, C. M. (1957). Personality factors and the rate
Endler, N. S. & Parker, J. D. A. (1990). Multidimensional of conditioning. British Journal of Psychology, 48,
assessment of coping: A critical evaluation. Journal of 119–126.
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 844–854. Frederick, C., Pynoos, R. S., & Nader, K. (1992). Child Post-
Engelhard, I. M., van den Hout, M. A., & Kindt, M. (2003). Traumatic Stress Disorder Reaction Index (CPTSD-RI).
he relationship between neuroticism, pre-traumatic Los Angeles, CA: University of California at Los Angeles
stress, and post-traumatic stress: A prospective study. Department of Psychiatry.
Personality and Individual Diferences, 35, 381–388. Funk, S. C. (1992). Hardiness: A review of theory and
Eysenck, H. J. & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1975). Eysenck Personality research. Health Psychology, 11, 335–345.
Questionnaire manual. San Diego, CA: Educational and Funk, S. C. & Houston, B. K. (1987). A critical analysis
Industrial Testing Service. of the Hardiness Scale’s validity and utility. Journal of
Eysenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J., & Barratt, P. (1985). A Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 572–578.
revised version of the psychoticism scale. Personality and Gil, S. (2005). Pre-traumatic personality as a predictor of
Individual Diferences, 6, 21–29. post-traumatic stress disorder among undergraduate
Fauerbach, J. A., Lawrence, J. W., Schmidt, C. W., Munster, students exposed to a terrorist attack: A prospective
A. M., & Costa, P. T. (2000). Personality predictors of study in Israel. Personality and Individual Diferences, 39,
injury-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of 819–827.
Nervous and Mental Disease, 188, 510–517. Gil, S. & Caspi, Y. (2006). Personality traits, coping style,
Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., & Taubman, O. (1995). and perceived threat as predictors of posttraumatic
Does hardiness contribute to mental health during a stress disorder ater exposure to a terrorist attack: A
stressful real-life situation? he roles of appraisal and prospective study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 904–909.

71
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Golby, J. & Sheard, M. (2004). Mental toughness and comorbidity among patients with DSM-IV major
hardiness at diferent levels of rugby league. Personality depressive disorder. Journal of Afective Disorders 114,
and Individual Diferences, 37, 933–942. 110–121.
Golding, J. M., Siegel, J. M., Sorenson, S. B., Burnam, M. A., Kantorowitz, D. A. (1978). Personality and conditioning of
& Stein, J. A. (1989). Social support following sexual tumescence and detumescence. Behaviour Research and
assault. Journal of Community Psychology, 17, 92–107. herapy, 16, 117–123.
Gomez, R., Holmberg, K., Bounds, J., Fullarton, C., & Keane, T. M., Zimering, R. T., & Caddell, J. M. (1985). A
Gomez, A. (1999). Neuroticism and extraversion as behavioral formulation of posttraumatic stress disorder
predictors of coping styles during early adolescence. in Vietnam veterans. Behavior herapist, 8, 9–12.
Personality and Individual Diferences, 27, 3–17. Keane, T. M., Caddell, J. M., & Taylor, K. L. (1988). he
Gray, J. A. (1987). he psychology of fear and stress, 2nd edn. Mississippi Scale for combat-related posttraumatic stress
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. disorder: hree studies in reliability and validity. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 85–90.
Gross, J. J. & John, O. P. (1995). Facets of emotional
expressivity: hree self-report factors and their correlates. Kerr, T. A., Schapira, K., Roth, M., & Garside, R. F. (1970).
Personality and Individual Diferences, 19, 555–568. he relationship between the Maudsley Personality
Inventory and the course of afective disorders. British
Gupta, S. & Shukla, A. P. (1989). Verbal operant
Journal of Psychiatry, 116, 11–19.
conditioning as a function of extraversion and
reinforcement. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 39–44. King, D. W., King, L. A., Foy, D. W., Keane, T. M., &
Fairbank, J. A. (1999). Posttraumatic stress disorder in
Hathaway, S. R. & McKinley, J. C. (1967). MMPI manual,
a national sample of female and male Vietnam veterans:
revised edn. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Risk factors, war-zone stressors, and resilience-recovery
Herman-Stahl, M. A., Stemmler, M., & Peterson, A. C. variables. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 164–170.
(1995). Approach and avoidance coping: Implications
King, L. A., King, D. W., Fairbank, J. A., Keane, T. M., &
for adolescent mental health. Journal of Youth and
Adams, G. A (1998). Resilience-recovery factors in
Adolescence, 24, 649–665.
post-traumatic stress disorder among female and male
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Clayton, P. J., et al. Vietnam veterans: Hardiness, postwar social support,
(1983). Assessing personality: Efects of the depressive and additional stressful life events. Journal of Personality
state on trait measurement. American Journal of and Social Psychology, 74, 420–434.
Psychiatry, 140, 695–699.
Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and
Hocking, F. (1970). Extreme environmental stress and its health: An inquiry into hardiness. Journal of Personality
signiicance for psychopathology. American Journal of and Social Psychology, 37, 1–11.
Psychotherapy, 24, 4–26.
Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., & Courington, S. C. (1981).
Holeva, V. & Tarrier, N. (2001). Personality and Personality and constitution as mediators in the stress–
peritraumatic dissociation in the prediction of PTSD in illness relationship. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
victims of road traic accidents. Journal of Psychosomatic 22, 368–378.
Research, 51, 687–692. Koren, D., Arnon, I., & Klein, E. (1999). Acute stress
Holohan, C. J. & Moos, R. H. (1991). Life stressors, personal response and posttraumatic stress disorder in traic
and social resources, and depression: A 4-year structural accident victims: A one-year prospective, follow-up
model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 31–38. study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 367–373.
Horsburgh, V. A., Schermer, J. A., Veselka, L., & Vernon, Krueger, R. F. (1999). he structure of common mental
P. A. (2009). A behavioural genetic study of mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 921–926.
toughness and personality. Personality and Individual Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1998).
Diferences, 46, 100–105. he structure and stability of common mental disorders
Ingham, J. G., Kreitman, N. B., McMiller, P., Sashidharan, (DSM-III-R): A longitudinal-epidemiological study.
S. P., & Surtrees, P. G. (1986). Self-esteem, vulnerability, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 216–227.
and psychiatric disorder in the community. British Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2001). he
Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 175–185. higher-order structure of common DSM mental
Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M. W. (2002). disorders: Internalization, externalization, and their
Personality and leadership: A qualitative and quantitative connections to personality. Personality and Individual
review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 765–780. Diferences, 30, 1245–1259.
Jylhä, P., Melartin, T., & Isometsä, E. (2009). Relationships Kulka, R. A., Schlenger, W. E., Fairbank, J. A., et al. (1990).
of neuroticism and extraversion with axis I and II Trauma and the Vietnam War generation: Report

72
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

on the indings from the National Vietnam Veterans Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory
Readjustment Study. New York: Brunner/Mazel. of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of
Lara, M. E., Leader, J., & Klein, D. N. (1997). he association Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.
between social support and course of depression: Is Miller, M. W. & Patrick, C. J. (2000). Trait diferences in
it confounded with personality? Journal of Abnormal afective and attentional responding to threat revealed
Psychology, 106, 478–482. by emotional Stroop interference and startle relex
Larsen, R. J. & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and modulation. Behavior herapy, 31, 757–776.
susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Miller, M. W. & Resick, P. A. (2007). Internalizing and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140. externalizing subtypes in female sexual assault survivors:
Lawler, K. A. & Schmied, L. A. (1992). A prospective study Implications for the understanding of complex PTSD.
of women’s health: he efects of stress, hardiness, locus Behavior herapy, 38, 58–71.
of control, type A behavior, and physiological reactivity. Miller, M. W., Greif, J. L., & Smith, A. A. (2003).
Women and Health, 19, 27–41. Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire proiles of
Lawrence, J. W. & Fauerbach, J. A. (2003). Personality, veterans with traumatic combat exposure: Internalizing
coping, chronic stress, social support and PTSD and externalizing subtypes. Psychological Assessment, 15,
symptoms among adult burn survivors: A path analysis. 205–215.
Journal of Burn Care and Rehabilitation, 24, 63–72. Miller, M. W., Kaloupek, D. G., Dillon, A. L., & Keane, T. M.
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual (2004). Externalizing and internalizing subtypes of
Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155–184. combat-related PTSD: A replication and extension using
the PSY-5 scales. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113,
Lee, K. A., Vaillant, G. E., Torrey, W. C., & Elder, G. H.
636–645.
(1995). A 50-year prospective study of the psychological
sequelae of World War II combat. American Journal of Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1998). Comorbidity
Psychiatry, 152, 516–522. of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annual Review
of Psychology, 49, 377–412.
Maddi, S. R., Khoshaba, D. M., Persico, M., et al. (2002). he
personality construct of hardiness II: Relationships with Monroe, S. M. & Simons, A. D. (1991). Diathesis-stress
comprehensive tests of personality and psychopathology. theories in the context of life stress research: Implications
Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 72–85. for the depressive disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 110,
Mangan, G. L. (1967). Studies of the relationship between 406–425.
neo-pavlovian properties of higher nervous system Morgan, I. A., Matthews, G., & Winton, M. (1995).
activity and western personality dimensions: IV. A factor Coping and personality as predictors of posttraumatic
analytic study of extraversion and lexibility and the intrusions, numbing, avoidance and general distress:
sensitivity and mobility of the nervous system. Journal of A study of victims of the Perth lood. Behavioural and
Experimental Research in Personality, 2, 124–127. Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 251–264.
McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. (1986). Personality, coping Nezu, A. M. & Carnevale, G. J. (1987). Interpersonal
and coping efectiveness in an adult sample. Journal of problem solving and coping reactions of Vietnam
Personality, 54, 385–405. veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of
McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait Abnormal Psychology, 96, 155–157.
structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, Nightingale, J. & Williams, R. M. (2000). Attitudes to
52, 509–516. emotional expression and personality in predicting
McCranie, E. W., Hyer, L. A., Boudewyns, P. A., & Woods, post-traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Clinical
M. G. (1992). Negative parenting behavior, combat Psychology, 39, 243–254.
exposure, and PTSD symptom severity: Test of a person– Orr, S. P., Metzger, L. J., Lasko, N. B., et al. (2000). De novo
event interaction model. Journal of Nervous and Mental conditioning in trauma-exposed individuals with
Disease, 180, 431–438. and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of
McFarlane, A. C. (1992). Avoidance and intrusion in Abnormal Psychology, 2, 290–298.
posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Nervous and O’Toole, B. I., Marshall, R. P., Schureck, R. J., & Dobson, M.
Mental Disease, 180, 439–445. (1998). Risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder
Miller, M. W. (2003). Personality and the etiology and in Australian Vietnam veterans. Australian and New
expression of PTSD: a three-factor model perspective. Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 32, 21–31.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 373–393. Paisey, T. J. & Mangan, G. L. (1988). Personality and
Miller, M. W. (2004). Personality and the development and conditioning with appetitive and aversive stimuli.
expression of PTSD. PTSD Research Quarterly, 15, 1–7. Personality and Individual Diferences, 9, 69–78.

73
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Panksepp, J. (1992). A critical role for “afective behavior: A twin study. Developmental Psychology, 28,
neuroscience” in resolving what is basic about basic 1030–1037.
emotions. Psychological Review, 99, 554–560. Roca, R. P., Spence, R. J., & Munster, A. M. (1992).
Parkes, K. R. (1986). Coping in stressful episodes: he role Posttraumatic adaptation and distress among adult
of individual diferences, environmental factors, and burn survivors. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149,
situational characteristics. Journal of Personality and 1234–1238.
Social Psychology, 51, 1277–1292. Ross, S. R., Rausch, M. K., & Canada, K. E. (2003).
Parkes, K. R. & Rendall, D. (1988). he hardy personality Competition and cooperation in the ive-factor model:
and its relationship to extraversion and neuroticism. individual diferences in achievement orientation.
Personality and Individual Diferences, 9, 785–790. Journal of Psychology, 137, 323–337.
Parslow, R. A., Jorm, A. F., & Christensen, H. (2006). Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994).
Associations of pre-trauma attributes and trauma A psychobiological approach to the development
exposure with screening positive for PTSD: Analysis of temperament. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs (eds.),
of a community-based study of 2085 young adults. Temperament: Individual diferences at the interface of
Psychological Medicine, 36, 387–395. biology and behavior (pp. 83–117). Washington, DC:
Patrick, C. J. & Bernat, E. M. (2006). he construct American Psychological Association.
of emotion as a bridge between personality and Ruch, L. O. & Leon, J. J. (1983). Sexual assault trauma and
psychopathology. In R. F. Krueger & J. L. Tackett trauma change. Women and Health, 8, 5–21.
(eds.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 174–209).
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C.
New York: Guilford Press.
(2004). Personality and life satisfaction: A facet-level
Pearlin, L. I. & Schooler, C. (1978). he structure of coping. analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30,
Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19, 2–21. 1062–1075.
Popiel, D. A. & Susskind, E. C. (1985). he impact of rape: Schnurr, P. P., Friedman, M. J., & Rosenberg, S. D. (1993).
Social support as a moderator of stress. American Journal Premilitary MMPI scores as predictors of combat-related
of Community Psychiatry, 13, 645–676. PTSD symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150,
Punamaki, R., Qouta, S., & El- Sarraj, E. (2001). Resiliency 479–483.
factors predicting psychological adjustment ater political Shagass, C. & Kerenyi, A. B. (1958). Neurophysiologic
violence among Palestinian children. International studies of personality. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 256–267. Disease, 126, 141–147.
Qouta, S., Punamaki, R., Montgomery, E., & El Sarraj, E. Solomon, Z., Mikulincer, M., & Flum, H. (1988). Negative
(2007). Predictors of psychological distress and positive life events, coping responses, and combat-related
resources among Palestinian adolescents: Trauma, child, psychopathology: A prospective study. Journal of
and mothering characteristics. Child Abuse and Neglect, Abnormal Psychology, 97, 302–313.
31, 699–717.
Spence, K. W. & Beecrot, R. S. (1954). Diferential
Rabkin, J. G. & Struening, E. L. (1976). Life events, stress, conditioning and level of anxiety. Journal of Experimental
and illness. Science, 194, 1013–1020. Psychology, 48, 399–403.
Ramanaiah, N. V., Sharpe, J. P., & Byravan, A. (1999). Spence, K. W. & Farber, I. E. (1953). Conditioning
Hardiness and major personality factors. Psychological and extinction as a function of anxiety. Journal of
Reports, 84, 497–500. Experimental Psychology, 45, 116–119.
Reich, J., Noyes, R., Coryell, W., & O’Gorman, T. W. (1986). Spence, K. W. & Taylor, J. (1951). Anxiety and strength
he efect of state anxiety on personality measurement. of the UCS as determiners of the amount of eyelid
American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 760–763. conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42,
Rhodewalt, F. & Zone, J. B. (1989). Appraisal of life change, 183–188.
depression, and illness in hardy and nonhardy women. Spence, K. W., Farber, I. E., & Taylor, E. (1954). he relation
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 81–88. of electric shock and anxiety to level of performance in
Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Cottler, L. B., & Goldring, E. eyelid conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
(1989). National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic 48, 404–408.
Interview Schedule, version 3, revised. St. Louis, MO: Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., & Gibbon, M. (1987).
Washington University Department of Psychiatry, Structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R, version
Robinson, J. L., Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Corley, R. NP-V. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute,
(1992). he heritability of inhibited and uninhibited Biometrics Research.

74
Chapter 4: Personality factors in resilience to traumatic stress

Stewart, M. E., Ebmeier, K. P., & Deary, I. J. (2005). Watson, D. (2005). Rethinking the mood and anxiety
Personality correlates of happiness and sadness: disorders: A quantitative hierarchical model for DSM-V.
EPQ-R and TPQ compared. Personality and Individual Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 522–536.
Diferences, 38, 1085–1096. Watson, D. & Walker, L. M. (1996). he long-term stability
Stokes, J. P. & McKirnan, D. J. (1989). Afect and the social and predictive validity of trait measures of afect. Journal
environment: he role of social support in depression of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 567–577.
and anxiety. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson (eds.), Anxiety Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988a). Positive
and depression: Distinctions and overlapping features and negative afect and their relation to anxiety and
(pp. 253–284). New York: Academic Press. depressive disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97,
Stretch, R. H. (1985). Posttraumatic stress disorder among 346–353.
US Army reserve Vietnam and Vietnam-era veterans. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. J. (1988b).
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, Development and validation of brief measures of positive
935–936. and negative afect: the PANAS scales. Personality and
Stretch, R. H. (1986). Incidence and etiology of post- Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
traumatic stress disorder among active duty personnel. Watson, D., Gamez, W., & Simms, L. J. (2005). Basic
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16, 464–481. dimensions of temperament and their relation to anxiety
Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and and depression: A symptom-based perspective. Journal
their relevance to assessing anxiety with an emphasis on of Research in Personality, 39, 46–66.
self-report. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (eds.), Anxiety Westman, M. (1990). he relationship between stress
and the anxiety disorders (pp. 681–706). Hillsdale, NJ: and performance: he moderating efect of hardiness.
Erlbaum. Human Performance, 3, 141–155.
Tellegen, A. (2000). Manual for the Multidimensional Weyer, G. (1989). Conditioned and unconditioned
Personality Questionnaire. Minneapolis, MN: University cardiovascular reactivity: Predictability and stability over
of Minnesota Press. time. Personality and Individual Diferences, 10, 633–652.
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., et al. (1988). Wilson, G. D., Kumari, V., Gray, J. A., & Corr, P. J. (2000).
Personality similarity in twins reared apart and he role of neuroticism in startle reactions to fearful and
together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, disgusting stimuli. Personality and Individual Diferences,
1031–1039. 29, 1077–1082.
Verkerk, G. J., Denollet, J., van Heck, G. L., van Son, M. J., & Wolfe, J., Erickson, D. J., Sharkansky, E. J., King, D. W.,
Pop, V. J. (2005). Personality factors as determinants of & King, L. A. (1999). Course and predictors of
depression in postpartum women: A prospective 1-year posttraumatic stress disorder among Gulf War veterans:
follow-up study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 632–637. A prospective analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Vogt, D. S., Rizvi, S. L., Shipherd, J. C., & Resick, P. A. (2008). Psychology, 67, 520–528.
Longitudinal investigation of reciprocal relationship Zinbarg, R. E. & Mohlman, J. (1998). Individual diferences in
between stress reactions and hardiness. Personality and the acquisition of afectively valenced associations. Journal
Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 61–73. of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1024–1040.
Volrath, M. & Torgersen, S. (2000). Personality types Zoellner, L. A., Foa, E. B., & Brigidi, B. D. (1999).
and coping. Personality and Individual Diferences, 29, Interpersonal friction and PTSD in female victims of
367–378. sexual and nonsexual assault. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
Volrath, M., Torgersen, S., & Alnaes, R. (1995). Personality 12, 689–700.
as a long-term predictor of coping. Personality and Zuckerman, M. (1983). Biological bases of sensation seeking,
Individual Diferences, 18, 117–125. impulsivity, and anxiety. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

75
Section
Section1 Pathways to resilience
Chapter
Social ties and resilience in

5 chronic disease
Denise Janicki-Deverts and Sheldon Cohen

Introduction psychological distress (e.g., anxiety, depression) and


loss of role function (i.e., social, sexual, vocational)”
Social ties are thought to afect mental and physical
(Helgeson & Cohen, 1996, p. 136). Physiological adjust-
health by inluencing emotions, cognitions, and behav-
ment is taken to mean slowed biological progression of
ior (Cohen, 1988, 2004). In the case of mental health,
disease, with progression being operationalized as the
the hypothesis is that aspects of social relationships
reappearance of manifest disease expression (e.g., can-
regulate these three response systems by preventing
cer recurrence, second heart attack in those with heart
the occurrence of the kinds of extreme response that
disease) and disease-related mortality.
are associated with dysfunction. his regulation occurs
he chapter will begin by introducing two import-
through communication of social expectations, of
ant components of social relationships – social support
appropriate norms, of rewards and punishments, and
and social integration – describing approaches that
through the provision of coping assistance (Caplan,
have been taken for their measurement and discussing
1974; Cassel, 1976; hoits, 1986). In the case of phys-
models that have been proposed to describe the man-
ical health, the hypothesis is that social ties inluence
ner by which these components of social relationships
behaviors with implications for health such as diet,
deliver their beneicial efects. he subsequent discus-
exercise, smoking, alcohol consumption, sleep, and
sion will focus on potential psychological, behavioral,
adherence to medical regimens. Moreover, the failure
and physiological mechanisms by which social ties
to regulate emotional responses can trigger health-rel-
might be translated into better adjustment, using stud-
evant changes in the responses of the neuroendocrine,
ies from chronic disease research as speciic examples.
immune, and cardiovascular systems (Cohen, 1988;
hen, evidence will be presented suggesting that social
Cohen et al., 1994; Uchino, 2006).
ties promote individuals’ physical and psychological
During times of stress, social ties can be thought to
adjustments to chronic disease. Finally, the chapter
operate by these same mechanisms to promote resili-
will end with a brief discussion of the literature on
ence, or positive adaptation in the face of adversity
social relationship interventions designed to promote
(Cohen & Wills, 1985; Luthar, 2006). People undergo-
adjustment in persons with chronic disease, as well as
ing the stress of being diagnosed with a serious chronic
possible future directions for social ties and resilience
illness are vulnerable to both physical and psycho-
research.
logical adversity. Patients with chronic disease must
cope with the physical threats of worsening health, loss
of physical function, and the potential of a reduced life-
Social support and social integration:
span, as well as with psychological threats such as feel- definitions and measurement
ings of isolation, loss of self-esteem, and loss of social
and occupational role function. Social support
his chapter focuses on resilience as it pertains to Social support refers to a social network’s provision
psychological and physiological adjustment to chronic of psychological and material resources intended to
disease. Psychological adjustment is taken to mean beneit an individual’s ability to cope with stress (Cohen,
“adaptation to disease without continued elevations of 2004). Support is oten diferentiated in terms of three

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

76
Chapter 5: Social ties and resilience in chronic disease

types of resource: instrumental, informational, and communality and identiication with one’s social roles
emotional (e.g., House & Kahn, 1985). Instrumental (Brissette et al., 2000). he concept of social integra-
support involves the provision of material aid, for tion is rooted in the seminal work of Durkheim (1951),
example inancial assistance or help with daily tasks. originally published in 1897, on social conditions and
Informational support refers to the provision of rele- suicide. Durkheim proposed that stable social struc-
vant information intended to help the individual to ture and widely held norms are protective and serve to
cope with current diiculties and typically takes the regulate behavior.
form of advice or guidance in dealing with one’s prob- Measures of social integration can be categorized
lems. Emotional support involves the expression of according to whether they are role based (focus on
empathy, caring, reassurance, and trust and provides number of relationships), participation based (focus on
opportunities for emotional expression and venting. frequency of interaction or engagement with social net-
Such typologies of support provide a basis for deter- work members), or perception based (focus on extent
mining whether the efectiveness of diferent kinds of to which individuals perceive that they are embedded
support difers by the nature of stressful events or by in a stable social structure and identify with network
the characteristics of persons sufering adversity. members and social positions) (Brissette et al., 2000).
In general, measures of social support focus either Some social integration measures (e.g., Berkman &
on individuals’ perceptions of available support from Syme, 1979) are complex and include two or even all of
members of their social networks or on individuals’ the types of assessment described above.
accounts of support that was actually provided by/
received from their network members. Interestingly, Main effects and buffering effects
measures of received support tend to correlate poorly
Two models have been proposed to describe how social
with perceived support measures. Moreover, perceived
ties confer beneits on mental and physical health out-
support has been associated with greater beneits than
comes: the main efects model and the bufering model
support that was actually received. Explanations for
(House, 1981; Cohen & Wills, 1985; Cohen, 1988).
this phenomenon include the fact that support gener-
he main efects model purports that social ties have
ally is sought during times of stress and that asking for
an overall positive efect on health regardless of the
or simply needing support may be detrimental to one’s
support recipient’s current stress level. he bufering
self-esteem (Cohen & Wills, 1985; Uchino, 2004). An
model suggests that social ties confer positive efects for
additional explanation is that the support provided by
health by protecting individuals from the potentially
one’s network members may not meet the individual’s
detrimental efects of stress during times of adversity
speciic needs. Hence the matching hypothesis, which
(Cohen & Wills, 1985). Which model more accurately
proposes that support will be most efective if the
describes the association of social ties with health out-
support provided matches the needs of the recipient
comes depends on the component of social relation-
(Cohen & McKay, 1984; Cutrona & Russell, 1990). On
ships that is being measured. Reviews of the research
the one hand, for example, an individual who recently
on social ties and psychological distress among phys-
experienced the death of a close other would beneit
ically healthy individuals suggest that main efects
more from receiving emotional or belonging support
emerge when social ties are being measured in terms
than from receiving material assistance. On the other
of social integration and that bufering efects are
hand, although emotional support tends to be well
observed when social ties are being measured in terms
received in most circumstances, for an individual who
of perceived availability of social support (Cohen &
has just lost his or her job, instrumental support in the
Wills, 1985). However, whether it is appropriate to test
form of inancial assistance may prove to be a more
for bufering efects when examining the potential ben-
efective support provision.
eits of social ties for persons with chronic disease may
be subject to question, as all participants are experien-
Social integration cing high levels of stress related to their illness.
Social integration is a multidimensional construct
thought to include a behavioral component – active Summary
engagement in a wide range of social activities or rela- Two important features of social relationships – social
tionships – and a cognitive component – a sense of support and social integration – factor importantly

77
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

in individuals’ abilities to cope with stressful situ- Others’ willingness to help and/or the enhanced abil-
ations. Social support can be distinguished according ity to cope that results from receiving help increases
to whether the support provision is informational, feelings of personal control and self-esteem. As noted
instrumental, or emotional in nature. Social inte- above, such feelings may inluence health through
gration can be categorized in terms of the number of increased motivation to perform health behaviors or
social relationships in which one is involved, the fre- through suppression of neuroendocrine responses and
quency by which one interacts, or the number of types enhanced immune function. Even the mere perception
of relationship in which one participates. Social sup- that help is available may similarly trigger these pro-
port is thought to bufer individuals from the negative cesses. Findings from several studies of persons facing
efects of stressor exposure, whereas social integration life-threatening diseases such as cancer have demon-
is thought to operate according to a main efects model strated a positive association between social relation-
wherein being more highly integrated results in better ships and psychological factors. For example, Crothers
adjustment, regardless of one’s current stress levels. In and colleagues (2005), in a sample of patients receiv-
both cases, just the perception that one is supported ing treatment for various types of cancer, found that
and/or is embedded in a diverse social network may greater satisfaction with social support and greater
be suicient for the positive efects of social ties to be perceived closeness with the primary support pro-
observed. vider were associated with higher levels of positive
relative to negative afect and higher levels of hope.
Mechanisms by which social ties Similarly, Schroevers and colleagues (2003), also in a
sample of persons with various types of cancer, found
promote adjustment to stress that greater perceived emotional and informational
support was positively related to higher self-esteem.
Psychological mechanisms In a sample of patients ater surgery for gastrointes-
here are several theoretical perspectives suggest- tinal cancer, Schulz and Mohamed (2004) examined
ing that social integration increases feelings of self- whether received support during the period surround-
esteem, of self-identity, and of control over one’s ing tumor removal surgery would be associated with
environment. Social integration is presumed to pro- greater self-eicacy and greater cancer-related beneit
vide a source of generalized positive afect, a sense of inding. Having received higher relative to lower levels
predictability and stability in one’s life situation, and of support during the period surrounding the surgery
recognition of self-worth because of demonstrated was associated with greater beneit inding 11 months
ability to meet normative role expectations (e.g., later. However, received support was only weakly and
hoits, 1985; Cohen, 1988). hese positive psycho- non-signiicantly correlated with concurrent self-ei-
logical states are presumed to be facilitative because cacy (Schulz & Mohamed, 2004).
they lessen psychological despair (e.g., hoits, 1985), hough not as extensive as the research on social
result in greater motivation to care for oneself (e.g., ties and psychological outcomes in cancer patients,
Cohen & Syme, 1985), or result in a benign neuroen- research conducted in samples of persons with other
docrine response (e.g., Uchino, 2006). serious diseases also has provided evidence for the psy-
A related psychological model assumes that it is chological beneits of social relationships. For example,
social isolation that causes disease, rather than social among recent heart transplant recipients, those who
integration that enhances health (House et al., 1988). reported stronger attachments to their social net-
his approach assumes that isolation increases nega- work members within the irst few days ater surgery
tive afect and feelings of alienation, and decreases also reported lower levels of anger and depression,
feelings of control. Alternatively, one can merely view and higher levels of optimism and life satisfaction six
social isolation as a stressor in and of itself. months later (Bohachick et al., 2002). Similarly, in a
In regard to social support, availability of aid or sample of older men with HIV, Chesney and colleagues
of tangible or economic resources from one’s social (2003) found that higher levels of perceived social sup-
network could reduce the probability of potentially port, operationalized as a composite of emotional and
stressful events being appraised as threatening or tangible support, were associated cross-sectionally
harmful and hence could reduce the behavioral and with less psychological distress, and greater positive
afective concomitants of a more negative appraisal. afect.

78
Chapter 5: Social ties and resilience in chronic disease

exercise in cancer survivors has been associated with


Behavioral mechanisms greater levels of physical and psychological well-being
Several models have been proposed to describe how (reviewed by Pinto & Trunzo, 2005). In regard to the
social relationships might inluence individuals’ stress- lack of association with dietary changes, this inding
coping behaviors (Cohen et al., 1994). hese inluence among cancer patients contrasts with indings from
processes carry the potential to afect the behaviors studies conducted with other patient populations, such
of individuals with chronic disease in such a way as as people with diabetes (reviewed by Gallant, 2003)
to promote both physiological and psychological and people with heart failure (e.g., Sayers et al., 2008),
adjustment. wherein presence of support has been found to correl-
In regard to social integration, one possibility is ate with maintaining a better diet. It is possible that the
that having a wide range of network ties presumably null efect of social support on dietary changes in can-
provides multiple sources of information and hence an cer patients may relect the nature of the disease and
increased probability of having access to an appropri- its treatment. Because of the nausea frequently expe-
ate information source. Information could inluence rienced while undergoing chemotherapy, the goal of
health-relevant behaviors or help in avoiding stressful support providers may be to encourage the patient to
or high-risk situations. For example, network members eat anything at all, regardless of healthfulness.
could provide information regarding access to med- In addition to social ties promoting healthful
ical services or regarding the beneits of behaviors that behaviors among persons with chronic disease, social
positively inluence health and well-being. Another ties have been shown to increase patients’ adherence
possibility is that socially integrated persons are sub- to medical regimens. DiMatteo (2004) conducted a
ject to social controls and peer pressures that promote meta-analysis of 122 correlational studies conducted
health behaviors (e.g., exercise, better diet, not smok- between 1948 and 2001 that examined the association
ing, moderating alcohol intake). Being embedded in a of social support and social integration with patient
social network also increases individuals’ motivation adherence to medical regimens. he results suggested
to care for themselves. that social ties were associated with better adherence.
Social controls and peer pressures also might con- A comparison of efects for social support versus social
stitute a pathway by which social support could inlu- integration indicated that support correlated better
ence individuals’ coping behaviors. Such inluence with adherence than did integration.
processes would promote health to the extent that
normative coping behaviors are efective in reducing
perceptions of stress, non-adjustive behavioral adapta-
Physiological mechanisms
tions, and negative afective responses. Inappropriate In addition to having an indirect efect on physiological
norms, however, could lead to less-efective coping resilience via behavioral mechanisms, it also is possible
and hence greater risk of poor psychological and phys- that social ties inluence health outcomes directly via
ical adjustment. In so far as social coping norms have three major physiological pathways: cardiovascular,
been internalized, or that individuals expect others to immune, and neuroendocrine (Uchino, 2006).
encourage them to behave in a socially appropriate and
healthful manner, the mere perception of such encour- Cardiovascular system
agement could inluence coping behaviors in the same he cardiovascular system responds to stress by
manner as actually receiving it. increasing heart rate and blood pressure. Physiological
In their review of psychosocial factors and health evidence suggests that exaggerated cardiovascular
behaviors in cancer survivors, Park and Gafey (2007) responses to stress are associated with the development
concluded that social relationships positively inlu- of hypertension and cardiovascular disease (Manuck,
ence health behavior change following a diagnosis of 1994). Moreover, among persons with coronary heart
cancer. However, indings are not consistent across disease (CHD), heightened cardiovascular reactivity
categories of health behaviors. More precisely, higher in response to acute stress has been associated with
levels of social support consistently predict increases brief episodes of cardiac ischemia (i.e., reduced oxygen
in exercise among cancer survivors but show little asso- supply to the heart through constriction or blockage
ciation with dietary changes. he positive inluence of coronary arteries [Rozanski et al., 1988]). herefore,
of support on exercise levels is important, as regular one mechanism by which social ties may protect

79
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

individuals from development or progression of car- One frequently employed assessment of immune
diovascular disease is by dampening the cardiovascu- function involves measuring the amount of anti-
lar stress response. body an individual produces in response to being
his proposal has been tested in the laboratory by inoculated with an inactive virus (i.e., receiving a
assigning individuals to an experimental condition, vaccination). Production of more antibodies to the
wherein they complete a stressful task in the presence inoculated virus strain signiies a better functioning
of a supportive friend or confederate, or to a control of the immune system. Glaser and colleagues (1992)
condition, wherein individuals complete the same task adopted this technique to examine whether social sup-
but in the absence of a supportive other. he expect- port was associated with immune function in second
ation is that individuals in the support condition would year medical students, a group generally found to
show reduced cardiovascular reactivity (i.e., smaller report elevated levels of stress. Students who reported
increases in blood pressure and heart rate) relative to higher levels of perceived social support developed
those in the control condition. Findings from this lit- more speciic antibody in response to a hepatitis B
erature have been mixed, with some studies showing vaccination relative to those reporting less support
lower reactivity in the support condition (e.g., Kamarck (Glaser et al., 1992). Similarly, college freshmen who
et al., 1990), some showing higher reactivity (e.g., reported smaller social networks mounted lower anti-
Allen et al., 1991), and some showing no diference body responses to a component of the inluenza vac-
between conditions (e.g., Anthony & O’Brien, 1999). cine relative to freshman with larger social networks
Methodological diferences are thought to explain (Pressman et al., 2005).
much of the inconsistency between studies (Kamarck Cohen and colleagues (1997) took a diferent
et al., 1998). For example, social support has a greater approach to examining the association of social ties
attenuating efect on reactivity when it is received from with immune function by exposing 276 healthy men
a friend rather than a stranger (Christenfeld et al., 1997) and women to a common cold virus and then exam-
or from a woman rather than a man (Glynn et al., 1999). ining whether extent of social integration assessed at
Moreover, social support appears to be most efective the beginning of the study predicted the likelihood
in dampening cardiovascular reactivity to acute stress of developing a clinical cold during the ive days fol-
when the stressor is associated with high social threat lowing virus exposure. he presence of a cold was
(Kamarck et al., 1995). determined by the amount of mucus production and
In addition to inluencing cardiovascular reactiv- the nasal mucociliary clearance time (i.e., the length
ity to stress, social ties may afect the cardiovascular of time necessary for the upper respiratory tract to
system’s capacity to recover from stress. Comparatively clear itself of inhaled particles). hose individuals
little research has been done in the area of cardiovas- who reported having a more diverse social network
cular recovery, and even less on the inluence of social (i.e., greater number of diferent types of relationship)
ties on recovery. However, existing evidence provides were less likely to develop a cold relative to those who
some indication that higher levels of perceived social reported less diverse networks.
support from one’s existing network may promote bet-
ter recovery from acute laboratory stressors (e.g., Roy Neuroendocrine system
et al., 1998). Neuroendocrine activity is estimated primarily
by measuring end-products of the hypothalamic–
Immune system pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA) and the sympathetic–
he inability of the immune system to mount an appro- adrenomedullary system. he products of these two
priate response to infection or injury has been impli- neuroendocrine systems function as mediators of
cated in the pathophysiology of several chronic diseases allostasis, the active process by which the body main-
including cardiovascular disease (Libby, 2002), type 1 tains a state of internal balance in response to a chan-
diabetes mellitus (Knip & Siljander, 2008), and some ging environment (McEwen & Stellar, 1993); that is,
types of cancer (Dunn et al., 2002). A growing body of the two systems function to promote physiological
research provides suggestive evidence of a link between resilience in the face of potentially adverse environ-
greater social support and social integration and bet- mental stimuli. he “ight or light” response, a con-
ter immune function (reviewed by Uchino [2006] and stellation of behavioral and biological adaptations
Uchino et al. [1996]). that are directed toward promotion of survival in

80
Chapter 5: Social ties and resilience in chronic disease

the face of an acute aversive stimulus, is one example Summary


of a function that depends upon activation of these
hree mechanisms by which social ties have been
systems. In the short term, then, excitation of these
proposed to inluence adjustment to stress involve
systems clearly is adaptive. Over longer periods of
psychological, behavioral, and physiological fac-
time, however, prolonged or repeated activation of
tors, respectively. Psychological mechanisms include
these two systems can lead to allostatic load, the wear
enhancement of self-esteem, self-identity, and con-
and tear on the body and brain that results from dys-
trol over one’s environment, as well as reduction in the
regulation of allostatic mechanisms (McEwen, 2007).
probability of appraising a stressful event as threatening
Allostatic load has been thought to contribute to the
or harmful. Behavioral mechanisms include encour-
pathophysiology of many if not most chronic diseases,
agement to engage in health-promoting behaviors and
and it has been suggested to be a pathway through
discouragement from engaging in risky behaviors.
which psychosocial factors, including social support,
Physiological mechanisms include decreased stress
might inluence susceptibility to and exacerbation of
responsivity of the cardiovascular, immune, and neu-
disease (McEwen, 2007).
roendocrine systems.
Despite the fact that cardiovascular and immune
activities are thought to be regulated in part by neu-
roendocrine mediators, research on social ties and Social ties and resilience:
neuroendocrine activity is relatively limited in com- the evidence
parison with that on social ties and cardiovascular and he role of social ties in the promotion of psychological
immune functions (Uchino, 2006). Findings from the and physiological adjustment to chronic disease has
few existing studies in this area suggest that support- been examined extensively in individuals with CHD,
ive social relationships may have a positive inluence those with cancer, and to a lesser extent, those infected
on neuroendocrine regulation, but these efects may be with HIV. he following section provides a brief over-
moderated by other factors, including gender (Seeman view of the association of social ties, measured in
& McEwen, 1996; Uchino, 2006). For example, social multiple ways, to psychological and physiological
support has been associated with lower levels of plasma adjustment to chronic disease.
and urinary epinephrine and norepinephrine, hor-
mones that are secreted in response to activation of the
sympathetic–adrenomedullary system. In a study of Social support and psychological
married and cohabiting couples, self-reported support adjustment
from one’s partner was associated with lower plasma
norepinephrine in women but not men (Grewen et al., Coronary heart disease
2005). In healthy older adults, both social support Across CHD studies, adjustment has been operational-
(emotional, instrumental) and social integration (num- ized in terms of depression (e.g., Brummett et al., 1998;
ber of social ties) were associated with lower levels of Frasure-Smith et al., 2000), quality of life (Bennett et al.,
urinary epinephrine and norepinephrine among men 2001), and anxiety (reviewed by Duits et al., 1997).
but not women (Seeman et al., 1994). Social support Signiicant interest has surrounded predictors of
also has been associated with lower levels of cortisol, depression in patients with CHD because depression,
the primary efector of the hypothalamic–pituitary– which has a prevalence rate of approximately 19.8% in
adrenal axis. Among women with metastatic breast heart attack survivors (hombs et al., 2006), has been
cancer, salivary cortisol concentrations were found found to predict re-hospitalization (Levine et al., 1996)
to decrease with increasing perceived social support and shorter survival times (Frasure-Smith et al., 1993;
(Turner-Cobb et al., 2000). Both Grewen et al. (2005) Barth et al., 2004). In a sample of patients with CHD
and Seeman et al. (1994) measured cortisol in addition who had been hospitalized for cardiac catheterization
to epinephrine and norepinephrine. Findings for cor- (a medical procedure used to diagnose and treat some
tisol mirrored those for epinephrine and norepineph- heart conditions), Brummett and colleagues (1998)
rine: social support was associated with lower cortisol examined whether in-hospital measures of perceived
concentrations in married/cohabiting women but not social support predicted depression symptoms one
men (Grewen et al., 2005) and in healthy older men but month later. Using structural equation modeling, the
not women (Seeman et al., 1994). authors detected a direct path between social support

81
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

(a composite of instrumental, informational, and Human immunodeficiency virus


emotional support) and later depression symptoms, A few studies that were conducted in the early 1990s
such that higher levels of support were associated with examined the association between social ties and
fewer symptoms. his association remained signii- psychological adjustment among persons with HIV.
cant when in-hospital measures of depression were Findings from these studies were generally supportive
included in the model. Similarly, Frasure-Smith and of the hypothesis that social ties promote psychological
colleagues (2000) found that among heart attack sur- adjustment to the stress of living with HIV-positive
vivors at one year who were depressed at baseline (i.e., status. For example, higher levels of perceived social
when in hospital), higher levels of perceived social sup- support were found to be related cross-sectionally to
port at baseline predicted greater declines in depres- lower levels of depression (Hays et al., 1992) and psy-
sion symptoms assessed one year later. Interestingly, chological distress (Blaney et al., 1991) and to higher
when the authors included two measures of social levels of social adjustment (Pakenham et al., 1994).
integration (frequency of social interaction and liv- Similar evidence was reported in a recent prospect-
ing alone or not) into their regression model, all three ive study. Johnson and colleagues (2001) examined
social relationship measures emerged as independ- whether perceived social support could predict sub-
ent predictors of changes in depression symptoms sequent changes in psychological adjustment among
(Frasure-Smith et al., 2000). HIV-positive men. Lower levels of perceived support
at baseline were related to higher concurrent help-
Cancer
lessness and symptoms of depression and to greater
Helgeson and Cohen (1996) reviewed the extant increases in hopelessness and depression six months
research examining the role of social relationships in later. Moreover, additional analyses indicated that the
the promotion of psychological adjustment among changes in hopelessness over time mediated the asso-
persons with cancer. From their review of existing cor- ciation between baseline social support and changes in
relational research, the authors concluded that emo- depression symptoms (Johnson et al., 2001).
tional support is both the type of support that is most
desired by cancer patients and the type that has the Summary
strongest associations with psychological adjustment.
Existing literature on social ties and psychological
More recent research is consistent with these indings.
adjustment to CHD, cancer, and HIV strongly suggest
For example, Helgeson et al. (2004) followed a sample
that perceived social support promotes psychological
of women with breast cancer from 4 to 55 months ater
resilience to chronic physical disease. hat social ties
diagnosis and examined whether the patients fell into
should protect individuals with chronic disease from
distinct trajectories of social–psychological and phys-
developing depression symptoms, speciically, may
ical adjustment, and whether social resources (among
have important implications for physical as well as psy-
other variables) assessed at four months ater diagno-
chological well-being.
sis could distinguish between adjustment trajectories.
Social resources in this study were operationalized as
a composite of two measures: perceived availability of Social support and physiological
emotional, informational, and tangible support from adjustment
friends, family, and partner; and frequency of negative
interactions with friends, family, and partner (reverse- Coronary heart disease
scored). Findings indicated that social resources dif- Lett and colleagues (2005) reviewed the existing litera-
ferentiated between two trajectories. Women with ture on social ties and disease progression in patients
higher levels of social support followed a trajectory with CHD, and concluded that social ties exert a posi-
characterized by high levels of psychological adjust- tive main efect on progression of CHD such that
ment at four months that were maintained throughout patients with deicient social relationships are two to
the follow-up period; whereas women with lower levels four times more likely to experience a second heart
of social support followed a trajectory characterized by attack or CHD-related death relative to persons with
lower levels of adjustment at four months and that con- adequate social relationships. Findings were consistent
tinued to decline throughout the follow-up (Helgeson across various indicators of perceived social support
et al., 2004). (i.e., emotional support, marital quality, instrumental

82
Chapter 5: Social ties and resilience in chronic disease

support, general availability of needed support) and explanation for this unexpected inding may be that
social integration (i.e., social network size, marital sta- their emotional support measure might be interpreted
tus, participation in clubs or social/recreational activ- as a measure of received rather than perceived support.
ities), with neither social relationship domain emerging For example, an individual may talk to her conidant
as the more prognostic. (e.g., a sister who lives out of town) only a few times per
By comparison, tests of whether social support is year. However, if she feels that her sister is always avail-
beneicial to patients with CHD because it bufers the able to provide her with a sense of caring or reassur-
negative efects of CHD-related depression have pro- ance, her levels of perceived support may match or
vided mixed results. In a study of social support and even exceed those of someone who interacts with her
mortality in patients with cardiac disease, Frasure- conidant more frequently.
Smith and colleagues (2000) found an interaction
between perceived social support and depression such Human immunodeficiency virus
that high levels of perceived support were protective Ironson and Hayward (2008) examined 11 prospect-
against mortality only in those patients who also were ive social relationship studies in their review of posi-
depressed. Contrariwise, Lett and colleagues (2007) tive psychosocial factors and disease progression in
found that higher levels of perceived support predicted persons infected with HIV. Disease progression was
longer survival times among patients with CHD who deined as development of AIDS, appearance of AIDS-
had lower rather than higher levels of depression symp- related disease complications, or physiological evi-
tomatology, thus suggesting that the moderating efect dence of worsening immune function (e.g., declines
of depression on the association of perceived support in CD4 T-cell numbers, immune cells that are afected
and CHD survival is complex and in need of further by HIV). Of these studies, ive found that having social
examination. ties was predictive of less-severe disease progression,
whereas the remaining six found no signiicant efect
Cancer of social ties. Of the positive studies, two reported buf-
Helgeson et al. (1998) reviewed studies examining ering efects of social ties in that efects were observed
the association of social ties with cancer survival and only among those with more severe disease. In one
recurrence. he authors identiied three types of social case, having a larger social network was associated with
relationship measure that were examined in this litera- increased survival among men who had progressed
ture: marital status, social integration, and emotional to AIDS, but not among other HIV-infected men
support. he marital status literature was inconsistent, (Patterson et al., 1996). In the second case, perceived
with some studies reporting increased survival time social support interacted with CD4 T-cell levels, such
among married persons, some reporting decreased that higher levels of perceived support was associated
survival time, and some reporting no efect of marital with less disease progression one year later only among
status. Studies that examined recurrence as the out- men who were the most immunocompromised at the
come were similarly inconsistent. Findings for studies beginning of the study (Solano et al., 1993).
that either focused on perceived emotional support
or on characteristics of the social network were more Summary
encouraging, with various markers of social integra- Findings across three diferent chronic diseases pro-
tion (e.g., frequency of engagement in activities involv- vide suggestive evidence of a role for social ties in pro-
ing social interaction) showing a positive association moting physiological resilience to disease progression.
with recurrence and survival time. Moderating factors such as disease severity or depres-
A well-controlled prospective study by Kroenke sion may inluence the efectiveness of social relation-
and colleagues (2006) found a link between social ship factors on physiological outcomes.
integration and survival among women with breast
cancer. Speciically, women with more social ties (i.e., Promoting resilience through social
close relatives, friends, or living children) exhibited
increased survival relative to women with fewer ties. relationship interventions
Interestingly, the authors found no association between Despite the suggestive evidence that social relation-
their measure of socioemotional support (frequency of ships may play an important role in promoting psy-
communication with a conidant) and survival. One chological and physiological resilience in the face of

83
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

chronic disease, there are surprisingly few experi- which was conducted with patients with prostate can-
mental studies testing the possibility that interven- cer, only those men with the lowest self-esteem, the
tions aimed at improving social relationships, either by lowest prostate-speciic self-eicacy, and the highest
increasing the diversity of individuals’ social networks depression symptoms at baseline showed improved
or by increasing their levels of perceived functional psychological adjustment ater participating in a psy-
support, would be beneicial to persons with chronic choeducational support group intervention (Helgeson
diseases. Moreover, the intervention studies that et al., 2006).
do exist seldom draw inspiration from the evidence
reported in the correlational literature described in the Physiological adjustment
preceding sections. Speciically, while the correlational
studies have found that characteristics of natural social Coronary heart disease
networks were protective, intervention studies have he very few studies that have attempted to curb CHD
generally manipulated support by facilitating interac- progression (i.e., recurrent cardiac event or mortality)
tions with strangers (Helgeson & Cohen, 1996; Cohen by improving patients’ perceptions of social support
& Janicki-Deverts, 2009). have, for the most part, been unsuccessful. One mul-
tisite trial in which nurses regularly called and visited
patients to provide social support actually found nega-
Psychological adjustment tive efects of the intervention on women and no bene-
Most of the intervention studies aimed at improving it to men (Frasure-Smith et al., 1997). Another trial
psychological adjustment to chronic disease have been conducted with heart attack survivors used cognitive-
carried out with cancer patients, comparing partici- behavioral therapy in an attempt to increase patients’
pation in therapy groups together with other cancer perceptions of social support from existing natural
patients to usual care. Because the therapy is conducted networks (Berkman et al., 2003). Although patients in
in groups, these studies are generally referred to as tests the intervention group reported greater support than
of the efectiveness of social support. Evidence to sup- those in the control group, there was no efect on sub-
port the efectiveness of social support interventions sequent heart attacks or mortality.
on psychological adjustment to cancer has been incon-
sistent (Helgeson & Cohen, 1996; Ross et al., 2002), Cancer
with recommendations for their use largely being ten- In regard to disease progression, although two early
tative (Newell et al., 2002). In many cases, studies that studies did ind beneicial efects of group psycho-
have demonstrated positive efects of social support on therapy on cancer survival (Spiegel et al., 1989; Fawzy
psychological adjustment have done so only for cer- et al., 1993), this work has been criticized in terms of
tain subsets of the studied populations. For example, design and data interpretation (Fox, 1998; Coyne et al.,
results from a supportive–expressive group interven- 2007). Moreover, more recent work (Ilnyckyj et al.,
tion trial found that patients with breast cancer who 1994; Cunningham et al., 1998) including studies con-
had been randomized to the support condition showed ducted at multiple sites with larger samples (Goodwin
lower levels of negative afect following the interven- et al., 2001; Spiegel et al., 2007), failed to replicate
tion than women randomized to the control condition the early results. Goodwin and colleagues (2001), for
(Goodwin et al., 2001). However, this association was example, examined whether supportive–emotional
qualiied by a signiicant group-by-distress interaction group therapy would improve survival among women
such that only women reporting the highest levels of with metastatic breast cancer. Although the interven-
distress at baseline beneited from the support inter- tion improved mood among the women who were the
vention. Similar evidence for social support beneiting most distressed at the beginning of the study, the inter-
only certain individuals is provided by two studies con- vention had no efect on length of survival (Goodwin
ducted by Helgeson and colleagues (Helgeson et al., et al., 2001).
2000, 2006). In the irst study, women with early-stage
breast cancer received beneit from support groups Human immunodeficiency virus
designed to provide informational and emotional sup- To date, only one published study has examined
port only if they were lacking strong external support whether a social support intervention can inluence
networks (Helgeson et al., 2000). In the second study, disease progression among persons with HIV. Simoni

84
Chapter 5: Social ties and resilience in chronic disease

and colleagues (2007) examined whether participa- physically healthy persons, the association of social
tion in a peer-support intervention would be associ- integration with main efects and social support with
ated with less-severe declines in immune function bufering efects appears not to be as reliable among
among HIV-positive patients who were receiving persons with chronic disease. However, it is likely that
highly active antiretroviral therapy. he intervention tests for bufering efects may not be appropriate when
inluenced neither immune function nor the second- examining the efects of social ties among persons with
ary outcomes of adherence to the therapy regimen chronic disease, as all participants are experiencing
and satisfaction with received social support. he null high levels of stress associated with their illness.
indings of this study are diicult to interpret. In the Potential mechanisms through which social
broadest sense, one might argue that, based on the ties might inluence adjustment include promotion
indings of this study, social support interventions are of positive psychological states, encouragement of
not efective in attenuating HIV progression. However, healthy behaviors and adherence to medical regimens,
because the intervention was not successful at improv- and attenuation of excessive physiological responses
ing patients’ satisfaction with their social support, it to stress. hough discussed separately, psychological,
cannot be known whether a more efective support behavioral, and physiological pathways are far from
intervention might have had a greater impact on HIV mutually exclusive. One could envision a situation
progression. wherein the boost to self-esteem received from a sup-
portive social network could motivate a cancer patient
Summary to engage in more health-promoting behaviors. Having
a companion available to exercise with might encour-
At present, evidence for social support interventions
age better adherence to an exercise regimen in a person
providing psychological and physiological beneits to
with CHD. Appraisal support or even the perception
persons with chronic disease is less than convincing.
that social support of any kind is available can act to
However, there are relatively few studies, and many of
reduce the perceived stressfulness associated with
these may have employed inadequate interventions or
disease-related adversity, and this, in turn, could result
failed to account for confounding inluences. Given
in a dampening of physiological reactivity that other-
that voluntary social ties have been related more con-
wise might contribute to further progression of disease
sistently to better psychological well-being than have
processes.
obligatory ties (Helgeson & Cohen, 1996; Uchino,
Although the association between social support
2004), the contrived nature of social support manipu-
and better adjustment is readily observed when sup-
lations may be the fundamental reason for their appar-
port is naturally occurring, attempts at producing
ent lack of efectiveness. Consequently, developing and
similar beneicial efects through social support inter-
testing interventions designed to improve patients’
ventions have largely been unsuccessful, particularly
existing social networks and perceptions of avail-
when physiological adjustment is the outcome of
able support from existing networks seems the next
interest. Perhaps the key to improving the efects of
logical step. However, with the exception of the study
support interventions is for future research to enhance
of Berkman et al. (2003), the efectiveness of interven-
the existing natural support resources of patients with
tions of this sort on improving psychological and phys-
chronic disease rather than to introduce new sources
ical outcomes among persons with chronic disease
of support that may not be easily assimilated into
remains relatively unexplored.
these individuals’ lives. Emphasis might be placed,
for example, on encouraging chronically ill persons
Conclusions and future directions to recognize the breadth and diversity of their exist-
Whether measured in terms of the size and diversity of ing networks and the extent to which they feel they
one’s social network or in terms of the speciic resources have been supported by network members before they
provided by one’s social network members, social rela- became ill. Creating an association between social
tionships are associated with the promotion of resili- support resources and a time when ill persons were
ence in persons with chronic diseases, as indicated by healthier may reduce potential losses in self-esteem
the beneicial efects of social ties on psychological and oten associated with receiving help, increase feelings
physiological adjustment. In contrast to the literature of self-worth and self-eicacy, and ultimately promote
on social support and psychological distress among resilience.

85
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

older men living with HIV infection. Journal of Acquired


References Immunodeiciency Syndromes, 33(Suppl. 2), s185–s193.
Allen, K. M., Blascovich, J., Tomaka, J., & Kelsey, M. (1991). Christenfeld, N., Gerin, W., Linden, W., & Pickering, T. G.
Presence of human friends and pet dogs as moderators (1997). Social support efects on cardiovascular
of autonomic responses to stress in women. Journal of reactivity: Is a stranger as efective as a friend?
Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 582–589. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 388–398.
Anthony, J. L. & O’ Brien, W. H. (1999). An evaluation
Cohen, S. (1988). Psychosocial models of social support
of the impact of social support manipulations on
in the etiology of physical disease. Health Psychology, 7,
cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory stressors.
269–297.
Behavioral Medicine, 25, 78–87.
Cohen, S. (2004). Social relationships and health. American
Barth, J., Schumacher, M., & Herrmann-Lingen, C. (2004).
Psychologist, 59, 676–684.
Depression as a risk factor for mortality in patients with
coronary heart disease: A meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Cohen, S. & Janicki-Deverts, D. (2009). Can we improve
Medicine, 66, 802–813. our physical health by altering our social networks?
Bennett, S. J., Perkins, K. A., Lane, K. A., et al. (2001). Social Perspectives in Psychological Science, 4, 375–378.
support and health-related quality of life in chronic heart Cohen, S. & McKay, G. (1984). Social support, stress, and
failure patients. Quality of Life Research, 10, 671–682. the bufering hypothesis: A theoretical analysis. In A.
Berkman, L. F. & Syme, L. S. (1979). Social networks, host Baum, J. E. Singer, & S. E. Taylor (eds.), Handbook of
resistance, and mortality: A nine-year follow-up study psychology and health, Vol. IV (pp. 253–267). Hillsdale,
of Alameda County residents. American Journal of NJ: Erlbaum.
Epidemiology, 109, 186–204. Cohen, S. & Syme, S. L. (1985). Issues in the study and
Berkman, L. F., Blumenthal, J., Burg, M., et al. (2003). Efects application of social support. In S. Cohen & S. L. Syme
of treating depression and low perceived social support (eds.), Social support and health (pp. 3–22). San Diego,
on clinical events ater myocardial infarction: he CA: Academic Press.
Enhancing Recovery in Coronary Heart Disease Patients Cohen, S. & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and
(ENRICHD) randomized trial. Journal of the American the bufering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98,
Medical Association, 289, 3106–3116. 310–357.
Blaney, N. T., Goodkin, K., Morgan, R. O., et al. (1991). Cohen, S., Kaplan, J. R., & Manuck, S. B. (1994). Social support
A stress-moderator model of distress in early HIV-1 and coronary heart disease: Underlying psychologic
infection: Concurrent analysis of life events, hardiness and biologic mechanisms. In S. A. Shumaker & S. M.
and social support. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 35, Czajkowski (eds.), Social support and cardiovascular disease
297–305. (pp. 195–221). New York: Plenum Press.
Bohachick, P., Taylor, M. V., Sereika, S., Reed, S., & Anton, Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P., Rabin, B. S., &
B. B. (2002). Social support, personal control, and Gwaltney, J. M. Jr. (1997). Social ties and susceptibility
psychosocial recovery following heart transplantation. to the common cold. Journal of the American Medical
Clinical Nursing Research, 11, 34–51. Association, 277, 1940–1944.
Brissette, I., Cohen, S., & Seeman, T. E. (2000). Measuring Coyne, J. C., Stefanek, M., & Palmer, S. C. (2007).
social integration and social networks. In S. Cohen, Psychotherapy and survival in cancer: he conlict
L. Underwood, & B. Gottlieb (eds.), Measuring and between hope and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 133,
intervening in social support: A guide for social and health 367–394.
scientists (pp. 53–85). New York: Oxford University Press.
Crothers, M. K., Tomter, H. D., & Garske, J. P. (2005). he
Brummett, B. H., Babyak, M. A., Barefoot, J. C., et al. (1998). relationships between satisfaction with social support,
Social support and hostility as predictors of depressive afect balance, and hope in cancer patients. Journal of
symptoms in cardiac patients one month following Psychosocial Oncology, 23, 103–118.
hospitalization: A prospective study. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 60, 707–713. Cunningham, A. J., Edmonds, C. V., Jenkins, G. P., et al.
(1998). A randomized controlled trial of the efects of
Caplan, G. (1974). Support systems and community mental group psychological therapy on survival in women with
health. New York: Behavioral Publications. metastatic breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 7, 508–517.
Cassel, J. (1976). he contribution of the social environment Cutrona, C. E. & Russell, D. W. (1990). Type of social
to host resistance. American Journal of Epidemiology, support and speciic stress: Toward a theory of optimal
104, 107–123. matching. In I. G. Sarason, B. R. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce
Chesney, M. A., Chambers, D. B., Taylor, J. M., & Johnson, (eds.), Social support: An interactional view
L. M. (2003). Social support, distress, and well-being in (pp. 319–366). New York: Wiley.

86
Chapter 5: Social ties and resilience in chronic disease

DiMatteo, M. R. (2004). Social support and patient Hays, R. B., Turner, H., & Coates, T. J. (1992). Social support,
adherence to medical treatment: A meta-analysis. Health AIDS-related symptoms, and depression among gay
Psychology, 23, 207–218. men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60,
Duits, A. A., Boeke, S., Taams, M. A., Passchier, J., & 463–469.
Erdman, R. A. (1997). Prediction of quality of life ater Helgeson, V. S. & Cohen, S. (1996). Social support
coronary artery bypass grat surgery: A review and and adjustment to cancer: Reconciling descriptive,
evaluation of multiple, recent studies. Psychosomatic correlational, and intervention research, Health
Medicine, 59, 257–268. Psychology, 15, 135–148.
Dunn, G. P., Bruce, A. T., Ikeda, H., Old, L. J., & Helgeson, V. S., Cohen, S., & Fritz, H. L. (1998). Social ties
Schreiber, R. D. (2002). Cancer immunoediting: and cancer. In J. C. Holland & W. Breitbart (eds.), Psycho-
From immunosurveillance to tumor escape. Nature Oncology (pp. 99–109). New York: Oxford University Press.
Immunology, 3, 991–998. Helgeson, V. S., Cohen, S., Schulz, R., & Yasko, J. (2000).
Durkheim, E. (1951). Suicide. New York: Free Press Group support interventions for women with breast
(originally published in 1897). cancer: Who beneits from what? Health Psychology, 19,
Fawzy, F. I., Fawzy, N. W., Hyun, C. S., et al. (1993). 107–114.
Malignant melanoma: Efects of an early structured Helgeson, V. S., Snyder, P., & Seltman, H. (2004).
psychiatric intervention, coping, and afective state on Psychological and physical adjustment to breast cancer
recurrence and survival 6 years later. Archives of General over 4 years: Identifying distinct trajectories of change.
Psychiatry, 50, 681–689. Health Psychology, 23, 3–15.
Fox, B. H. (1998). A hypothesis about Spiegel et al.’s 1989 Helgeson, V. S., Lepore, S. J., & Eton, D. T. (2006).
paper on psychosocial intervention and breast cancer Moderators of the beneits of psychoeducational
survival. Psycho-Oncology, 7, 361–370. interventions for men with prostate cancer. Health
Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., & Talajic, M. (1993). Psychology, 25, 348–354.
Depression following myocardial infarction. Journal of House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading,
the American Medical Association, 270, 1819–1825. MA: Addison-Wesley.
Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., Prince, R. H., et al. (1997). House, J. S. & Kahn, R. L. (1985). Measures and concepts
Randomised trial of home-based psychosocial nursing of social support. In S. Cohen & S. L. Syme (eds.), Social
intervention for patients recovering from myocardial support and health (pp. 83–108). New York: Academic
infarction. Lancet, 350, 473–479. Press.
Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., Gravel, G., et al. (2000). House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social
Social support, depression, and mortality during the relationships and health. Science, 241, 540–545.
irst year ater myocardial infarction. Circulation, 101, Ilnyckyj, A., Farber, J., Cheang, M., & Weinerman, B. (1994).
1919–1924. A randomized controlled trial of psychotherapeutic
Gallant, M. P. (2003). he inluence of social support on intervention in cancer patients. Annals of the Royal
chronic illness self-management: A review and directions College of Physicians and Surgeons of Canada, 272, 93–96.
for research. Health Education and Behavior, 30, 170–195. Ironson, G. & Hayward, H. (2008). Do positive psychosocial
Glaser, R., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Bonneau, R., Malarkey, W., factors predict disease progression in HIV-1?
& Hughes, J. (1992). Stress-induced modulation of the Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 546–554.
immune response to recombinant hepatitis B vaccine. Johnson, J. G., Alloy, L. B., Panzarella, C., et al. (2001).
Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 22–29. Hopelessness as a mediator of the association between
Glynn, L. M., Christenfeld, N., & Gerin, W. (1999). Gender, social support and depressive symptoms: Findings of a
social support, and cardiovascular responses to stress. study of men with HIV. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 234–242. Psychology, 69, 1056–1060.
Goodwin, P. J., Leszcz, M., Ennis, M., et al. (2001). he efect Kamarck, T. W., Manuck, S. B., & Jennings, J. R. (1990).
of group psychosocial support on survival in metastatic Social support reduces cardiovascular reactivity
breast cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 345, to psychological challenge: A laboratory model.
1719–1726. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, 42–58.
Grewen, K. M., Girdler, S. S., Amico, J., & Light, K. C. Kamarck, T. W., Annunziato, B., & Amateau, L. M. (1995).
(2005). Efects of partner support on resting oxytocin, Ailiation moderates the efects of social threat on stress-
cortisol, norepinephrine, and blood pressure before and related cardiovascular responses: Boundary conditions
ater warm partner contact. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, for a laboratory model of social support. Psychosomatic
531–538. Medicine, 57, 183–194.

87
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Kamarck, T. W., Peterman, A. H., & Raynor, D. A. (1998). Patterson, T. L., Shaw, W. S., Semple, S. J., et al. (1996).
he efects of the social environment on stress-related Relationship of psychosocial factors to HIV disease
cardiovascular activation: Current indings, prospects, progression. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 18, 30–39.
and implications. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 20, Pinto, B. M. & Trunzo, J. J. (2005). Health behaviors during
247–256. and ater a cancer diagnosis. Cancer, 104(Suppl. 11),
Knip, M. & Siljander, H. (2008). Autoimmune mechanisms 2614–2623.
in type 1 diabetes. Autoimmunity Reviews, 7, 550–557. Pressman, S. D., Cohen, S., Miller, G. E., et al. (2005).
Kroenke, C. H., Kubzansky, L. D., Schernhammer, M. D., Loneliness, social network size, and immune response
Holmes, M. D., & Kawachi, I. (2006). Social networks, to inluenza vaccination in college freshmen. Health
social support, and survival ater breast cancer diagnosis. Psychology, 24, 297–306.
Journal of Clinical Oncology, 24, 1105–1111. Ross, L., Boesen, E., Dalton, S., & Johansen, C. (2002). Mind
Lett, H. S., Blumenthal, J. A., Babyak, M. A., et al. and cancer. Does psychosocial intervention improve
(2005). Social support and coronary heart disease: survival and psychological well-being? European Journal
Epidemiologic evidence and implications for treatment. of Cancer, 38, 1447–1457.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 869–878. Roy, M. P., Steptoe, A., & Kirschbaum, C. (1998). Life events
Lett, H. S., Blumenthal, J. A., Babyak, M. A., et al. (2007). and social support as moderators of individual diferences
Social support and prognosis in patients at increased in cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity. Journal of
psychosocial risk recovering from myocardial infarction. Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1273–1281.
Health Psychology, 26, 418–427. Rozanski, A., Bairey, N., Krantz, D. S., et al. (1988). Mental
Levine, J. B., Covino, N. A., Slack, W. V., et al. (1996). stress and the induction of silent myocardial ischemia
Psychological predictors of subsequent medical care in patients with coronary artery disease. New England
among patients hospitalized with cardiac disease. Journal Journal of Medicine, 318, 1005–1012.
of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation, 16, 109–116. Sayers, S. L., Riegel, B., Pawlowski, S., Coyne, J. C., & Samaha,
Libby, P. (2002). Atherosclerosis: he new view. Scientiic F. F. (2008). Social support and self-care of patients with
American, 286, 47–55. heart failure. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 35, 70–79.
Luthar, S. S. (2006). Resilience in development: A synthesis Schroevers, M. J., Ranchor, A. V., & Sanderman, R. (2003).
of research across ive decades. In D. Cicchetti & he role of social support and self-esteem in the presence
D. J. Cohen (eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. and course of depressive symptoms: A comparison
3: Risk, disorder, and adaptation, 2nd edn (pp. 740–795). of cancer patients and individuals from the general
New York: Wiley. population. Social Science and Medicine, 57, 375–385.
Manuck, S. B. (1994). Cardiovascular reactivity in Schulz, U. & Mohamed, N. E. (2004). Turning the tide:
cardiovascular disease: “Once more unto the breach.” Beneit inding ater cancer surgery. Social Science and
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 1, 4–31. Medicine, 59, 653–662.
McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress Seeman, T. E. & McEwen, B. S. (1996). Impact of social
and adaptation: Central role of the brain. Physiological environment characteristics on neuroendocrine
Reviews, 87, 873–904. regulation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 459–471.
McEwen, B. S. & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the individual. Seeman, T. E., Berkman, L. F., Blazer, D., et al. (1994).
Mechanisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal Social ties and support and neuroendocrine function,
Medicine, 153, 2093–2101. MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Annals of
Newell, S. A., Sanson-Fisher, R. W., & Savolainen, N. J. Behavioral Medicine, 16, 95–106.
(2002). Systematic review of psychological therapies for Simoni, J. M., Pantalone, D. W., Plummer, M. D., & Huang, B.
cancer patients: Overview and recommendations for (2007). A randomized controlled trial of a peer support
future research. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, intervention targeting antiretroviral medication
94, 558–584. adherence and depressive symptomatology in HIV-
Pakenham, K. I., Dadds, M. R., & Terry, D. J. (1994). positive men and women. Health Psychology, 26, 488–495.
Relationships between adjustment to HIV and both Solano, L., Costa, M., Salvati, S., et al. (1993). Psychosocial
social support and coping. Journal of Consulting and factors and clinical evolution in HIV-infection: A
Clinical Psychology, 62, 1194–1203. longitudinal study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 37,
Park, C. L. & Gafey, A. E. (2007). Relationships between 39–51.
psychosocial factors and health behavior change in Spiegel, D., Bloom, J., Kraemer, H., & Gottheil, E. (1989).
cancer survivors: An integrative review. Annals of Efect of psychosocial treatment on survival of patients
Behavioral Medicine, 34, 115–134. with metastatic breast cancer. Lancet, ii, 888–891.

88
Chapter 5: Social ties and resilience in chronic disease

Spiegel, D., Butler, L. D., Giese-Davis, J., et al. (2007). Efects Turner-Cobb, J. M., Sephton, S. E., Koopman, C., Blake-
of supportive-expressive group therapy on survival of Mortimer, J., & Spiegel D. (2000). Social support and
patients with metastatic breast cancer: A randomized salivary cortisol in women with metastatic breast cancer.
prospective trial. Cancer, 110, 1130–1138. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 337–345.
hoits, P. A. (1985). Social support processes and Uchino, B. N. (2004). Social support and physical health:
psychological well-being: heoretical possibilities. In Understanding the health consequences of relationships
I. G. Sarason & B. Sarason (eds.), Social support: heory, (pp. 9–32). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
research, and applications (pp. 51–72). he Hague: Uchino, B. N. (2006). Social support and health: A review
Martinus Nijhof. of physiological processes potentially underlying links
hoits, P. A. (1986). Social support as coping assistance. to disease outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29,
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 377–387.
416–423. Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K.
hombs, B. D., Bass, E. B., Ford, D. E., et al. (2006). (1996). he relationship between social support and
Prevalence of depression in survivors of acute physiological processes: A review with emphasis on
myocardial infarction. Journal of General Internal underlying mechanisms and implications for health.
Medicine, 21, 30–38. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488–531.

89
Section
Section1 Pathways to resilience
Chapter
Religious and spiritual factors in resilience

6 David W. Foy, Kent D. Drescher, and Patricia J. Watson

Introduction attacks of September 11 2001, more than 90% of those


surveyed reported that they coped by “turning to reli-
his chapter will examine how religion/spirituality
gion,” second only to “talking with others,” which was
plays an important role as a resource used by most
endorsed by 98% (Schuster et al., 2001). However, des-
people in coping with the immediate, as well as
pite the widely recognized positive aspects of religion
longer-term, consequences of highly stressful or trau-
or spirituality, there are large gaps in our scientiic
matic experiences. First, working deinitions of the key
knowledge of the dynamic processes of spirituality that
concepts of resilience and religion/spirituality will be
could explain these relationships.
given. Distinctions are made between deinitions for
As a starting point, promising areas for empirical
general communications and operational deinitions
inquiry have been identiied. It has been suggested that
suitable for clinical and research purposes. Spirituality
spirituality ofers a positive meaning-making frame-
is conceptualized as a dynamic process that is an inte-
work for coping (Park, 2005). In addition, enhanced
gral and inseparable part of humanity. A current
social support and efective cognitive processing of
conceptual model of spirituality as being multidimen-
stressful events have also been proposed as mecha-
sional in nature is presented, and core dimensions are
nisms by which one’s spirituality might be involved
described. Findings from a selective review of current
(McIntosh et al., 1993).
studies on religion and/or spirituality and resilience
are presented. Four key obstacles, or “spiritual red
lags,” are identiied, and a group therapy module for
General definitions of religion/spirituality
addressing them is presented. Finally, conclusions and resilience
about our current knowledge, as well as recommenda- Religion has long been a topic of discussion and research
tions for future clinical and research applications are within the ield of psychology, dating back to William
made. James’ writings in the twentieth century (James, 1997).
Spirituality is acknowledged as an important part Pargament, a prominent researcher in the ield, has put
of life by most individuals. Annual Gallup polls con- forth a brief and powerful deinition of religion as, “a
sistently show that more than 90% of the US popula- search for signiicance in ways related to the sacred”
tion report a “belief in God,” and approximately 70% (Pargament, 1997). Relatedly, Larson and colleagues
report ailiation with a faith community and attend- (1997), deined spirituality as the “multidimensional
ing religious services. In addition, religion or spiritu- space in which everyone can be located.” A core elem-
ality has been consistently linked to positive mental ent of transcendence is found in these deinitions for
(Nooney & Woodrum, 2002) and physical (Powell et al., both religion and spirituality. A deinition of spiritual-
2003) health functioning, as well as increased longev- ity that we have found useful in clinical work is that of
ity (Oxman et al., 1995). When mental health services Drescher and colleagues (2004): “an individual’s under-
are sought, clergy are most frequently the irst point of standing of, experience with, and connection to that
contact, with more than 40% seeking counseling from which transcends the self.” In popular usage, distinc-
them rather than mental health providers (Weaver tions are oten made such that many people may pro-
et al., 1997). In the immediate atermath of the terrorist fess to be “spiritual, but not religious.” An individual’s

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

90
Chapter 6: Religious and spiritual factors in resilience

spirituality may be realized in a religious context or it six-stage model of spiritual development has been
may be highly personalized and entirely distinct from described (Hagberg & Guelich, 2005) that identiies
religion of any sort. Of the two, spirituality is broader the following key stages:
and more individual, while religion contains within it Stage 1: he recognition of the divine or transcendence
the element of a shared understanding within a group/ Stage 2: Joining the religious or spiritual community
community context. Stage 3: Leading the community
One way of seeing religion might be as a corporate/ Stage 4: Inward journey, re-evaluation of personal
community expression of individual spiritual experi- spirituality
ence. Our preferred deinition of religion is “a system Stage 5: Journey outward, reclaim service based on
of beliefs, values, rituals, and practices shared in com- internal connection
mon by a social community as a means of experiencing Stage 6: Life of connection to divine.
and connecting with the sacred or divine” (Drescher
et al., 2004). While there are meaningful distinctions In using this model, it should not be assumed that
between religion and spirituality, and while they are not stages are attained in a straightforward, linear fashion.
synonymous, there is also much overlap. Accordingly, Rather, the starting point, movement in a forward or
for purposes of this chapter, religion and spirituality backward direction, and the ultimate end point, may
will be considered as a single construct, religion/spir- vary from person to person depending on biological
ituality (RS). limiting factors, familial and relational history, life
Resilience generally refers to the human ability experiences, and, probably, other factors.
to withstand stressful challenges and retain or regain During the irst stage, there is the initial connec-
normal functioning. Consequently, individuals dis- tion with God or the divine, where the sense of awe
play resilience when they manifest positive adapta- and divine sovereignty is experienced. Learning the
tion under extenuating circumstances. he American language, beliefs, and traditions of a faith commu-
Psychological Association Task Force on Promoting nity compose key elements of the second stage. he
Resilience in Response to Terrorism deines it as “the third stage involves personal productivity within the
process of adapting well in the face of adversity, trauma, faith community, oten including teaching and other
tragedy, threats, or even signiicant sources of stress.” service roles. Re-examining personal beliefs and
It is generally accepted that resilience is common and connection with the divine characterize the “inward
derives from the basic human ability to adapt to new journey,” or fourth stage. Maturing in spiritual devel-
situations (Masten, 2001). opment marks the inal two stages, during which indi-
viduals may reassess and internalize their connections
Spirituality and resilience as dynamic to the divine. In these later stages, relationships with
religious communities may be re-evaluated and re-
processes deined in ways that relect and promote more mature
Individuals’ experiences of RS are oten described as spirituality, reaching beyond narrow constraints dic-
part of a “spiritual journey” that involves a lifelong tated by a faith community. Along the course of indi-
quest for meaning and direction. A physical journey viduals’ spiritual journeys, life experiences represent
is dynamic because it occurs across both space and opportunities for challenge and doubt, as well as
time. Similarly, a spiritual journey occurs throughout meaning and support, and these experiences are well
the course of a person’s life, across whatever stages of documented across both Western and Eastern tradi-
physical or emotional development that occur, and tions (Eriksson, 2008).
it may involve multiple and changing dimensions of he varying facets of trauma exposure include (1)
spiritual experience. From a research perspective, this the nature and intensity of the trauma (i.e., natural ver-
means a unidimensional measurement of spirituality sus human caused, intentional versus accidental), (2)
captured during a cross-sectional (i.e., single time the personal risk and resilience characteristics of the
point) period of data collection may miss an enor- victim, (3) the emotional and instrumental support
mous wealth of information about the interaction of context before, during, and ater the event, and (4) the
individual life history and spirituality, some of which level of predictability and controllability perceived
may be predictive of important outcomes in the phys- by the victim. hese facets may all interact diferently
ical, emotional, or psychological arenas. Recently, a with the victim’s spirituality depending on the stage

91
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

of spiritual development and on the available spirit- time, but with trauma, continuing adversities, or with-
ual support and familiar spiritual coping behaviors out adequate supports to deal with such occurrences,
utilized. his interaction will then produce unique they can show developmental deterioration or gaps in
appraisals about the trauma itself and the individ- key resilient capacities that continue throughout life
ual’s degree of self-eicacy in managing its efects, and (Rutter, 1998).
it may result in varying levels of need to reconstruct While developmental approaches view current
core beliefs, values, meanings, and assumptions about adaptive capacities as a function of an individual’s
the world and the individual’s place in it. he ultimate history of successful adaptations to stressful condi-
impact of the trauma may lead to either maturation or tions (i.e., successful coping with earlier mild stres-
disintegration of the person’s spiritual development, sors can serve to inoculate children against the efects
which may, in turn, result in either resilient personal of later major stressors), contextual factors play an
growth or development of physical or psychological equally large role in producing positive outcomes. For
diiculties. example, high-competence children raised in high-
Resilience is also currently conceptualized in a risk environments do worse than low-competence
more dynamic perspective (e.g., Richardson, 2002). children raised in low-risk environments (Samerof
he study of resilience has evolved through three et al., 1998). herefore, resilience is also a function of the
phases of inquiry, starting with a basic search for resili- family’s ability, and other aspects of the social environ-
ent qualities: that is those traits, assets, and protective ment’s ability, to bufer the efects of high-risk, adverse
factors associated with positive outcomes. A second circumstances. For example, a core theme in resilient
phase introduced a dynamic view that featured resili- children under diverse risk conditions is the presence
ence as both a disruptive and a re-integrative process of a strong, supportive relationship with at least one
that involves using resilient qualities. he third phase adult that shows a sustained degree of continuity and
(current) deines resilience as “the force that drives a consistency (Werner, 2000). While resilience is not
person to grow through adversity and disruptions” “ixed” forever, in order to achieve and sustain resilient
(Richardson, 2002). adaptation, children must receive support from adults
Many empirical studies of resilience exemplify in their environments.
the irst phase in attempting to identify qualities that Not surprisingly, this inding is relected as well in
distinguish resilient from non-resilient individuals in the literature on spiritual development, with a num-
the wake of a destabilizing experience. Well-designed ber of studies showing a relationship between early
longitudinal studies of high-risk children in diverse attachment to primary caregivers and later quality of
geographic locations have been very inluential in relationship with God. Whereas secure attachments
identifying characteristics of resilient individuals tend to lead to stronger, more positive internalized
(Werner, 1982; Rutter, 1985; Garmezy, 1991). More images of God, insecure attachments in childhood
recently, studies in the ield of positive psychology lead to either an insecure attachment to God or a need
have identiied several RS-related factors associated to depend on God strongly under adverse situations
with resilience, including morality and self-control (Hall, 2007). Garbarino and Bedard (1996, p. 467)
(Baumeister & Exline, 2000), forgiveness (McCullough, additionally described that “the experience of child-
2000), and hope (Snyder, 2000). hood traumatization functions as a kind of ‘reverse
Developmentally, resilience is not ixed in time and religious experience’, a process combining overwhelm-
is not exhibited in the same way at diferent develop- ing arousal and overwhelming cognitions that threat-
mental stages. For example, resilient toddlers would ens core ‘meaningfulness’ for the child” and can create
show behaviors that relect mastery of that psycho- spiritual crisis. his trauma can conceivably impact
social stage of development, such as a strong autonomy the child’s resilience primarily in the mastery of tasks
as well as secure attachment to their mothers, whereas at their particular developmental stage. For example,
resilient school-age children would show a capacity to trauma literature emphasizes that very early child-
master educational skills and interact well with their hood trauma has the most profound efects on trust.
peers and adults in the school setting. Furthermore, However, the type and extent of the trauma, if severe
resilient adolescents would show facility with multiple enough, can also overwhelm and deteriorate previ-
roles and activities and a strong self-image in the face of ously mastered stages (Perry & Pollard, 1998; Anda
peer pressure. Children might excel at a given point in et al., 2006). he recent convergence in developmental

92
Chapter 6: Religious and spiritual factors in resilience

research on competence, resilience, trauma, behav- individuals evidencing tonic let frontal activation are
ioral and emotional problems, brain development, and more apt than individuals showing tonic right frontal
prevention science underscores the importance for activation to ‘‘organize limited resources in support
building resilience into human development in mul- of goal-approaching behaviors’’ (Sutton & Davidson,
tiple stages, levels, and contexts: within the child, the 1997). hey also suggest that taking an active role in
family, the community, and their interactions. Young life and appropriately engaging sources of motivation,
children who have attachment bonds with competent behaviors that are characteristic of those involved in
and loving caregivers, the stimulation and nutrition active religious and spiritual programs, may contrib-
required for healthy brain development, opportun- ute to higher levels of well-being, sense of meaning,
ities to learn and experience the pleasure of mastering and self-conidence (Urry et al., 2004). Moreover,
new skills, and the limit setting or structure needed they have found that seasoned Buddhist monks who
to develop self-control typically manifest resilience in are meditating on compassion while hooked up to
the face of adversity, as long as their fundamental pro- electroencephalography sensors show a striking
tective skills and relationships continue to operate and increase in gamma waves in the let prefrontal cortex,
develop (Luthar, 2005). an area correlated with reported feelings of well-being
Certain types of trauma, as well as unique aspects (Lutz et al., 2004). When applying these indings to a
of the traumatic experience, may create developmen- training program, they found that workers in a high-
tal barriers to resilience. Early childhood abuse, for technology company who took a two-month training
example, particularly when perpetrated within the program in meditation showed signiicant changes in
family, can dramatically change a child’s ability to brain activity, declines in anxiety, beneicial changes
engage in future loving and trusting relationships, in immune function, as well as positive impact on
and thus diminish the possibility of the social support emotional regulation (Davidson et al., 2003). Along
needed for resilient responding. Such trauma can also another line, research has shown that caring relation-
damage the child’s view of him/herself as a worthwhile ships, such as those found among members of faith
and competent person and thus impair the ability to communities, can provide essential social support
experience self-conidence and ultimately resilience. in recovery from life crises (Charuvastra & Cloitre,
Traumas that involve personal violation such as rape 2008).
and battering can also damage self-esteem and self-
eicacy. Combat is unique as a traumatic experience Multidimensional representation of
in that it involves not only direct experience of trauma
and witnessing but also calls for inlicting trauma upon religion/spirituality
an enemy. hese combat-speciic factors can introduce Major obstacles in the advancement of the scientiic
strong elements of guilt and shame, which may warp study of RS have included lack of interest in RS as a
an individual’s self-esteem and cause them to withdraw primary study variable by researchers, and, in the
from social relationships. relatively few studies that did include it as a variable,
he process by which resilience is achieved has the use of non-standard, single-item measures of RS.
been the focus of many studies, from which ive key Reviews of studies published during the 1980s and
factors have been identiied. he indings of these stud- 1990s found that only 1–3% of scholarly journal art-
ies show that primary factors in resilience are (1) caring icles in psychology, psychiatry, and medicine included
relationships within and outside the family that create spiritual variables (Larson et al., 1986; Weaver et al.,
love and trust, provide role models, and encourage and 1997). Additionally, within this small body of litera-
reassure; (2) the capacity to make realistic plans and ture, the majority of studies utilized ad hoc, non-stand-
implement them; (3) self-conidence; (4) communica- ard RS measures, or used a single item as the measure
tion and problem-solving skills; and (5) the capacity to of religious or spiritual experience. hese limitations
manage emotions (Masten, 2001). constrict scientiic study of RS by impeding replication
It is notable that RS presents aspects that are eas- of indings across studies through problems with basic
ily found in each of the ive resilience factors. For measurement.
example, scientists have recently begun to map the To address these measurement limitations, a
brain regions related to both well-being and posi- national work group was formed, with its members
tive emotions such as empathy. hey have found that selected because of their expertise and prominence in

93
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

measurement of religious or spiritual constructs. he well-being, hope, happiness, self-esteem and quality
group was supported by the Fetzer Institute in collab- of life. Measures for negative adjustment ranged from
oration with the National Institute of Aging, part of the anxiety, depression, and symptoms of post-traumatic
US National Institutes of Health. Based on the consen- stress disorder (PTSD) to social dysfunction, suicidal-
sus that RS was best represented as a multidimensional ity, and trait anger.
construct, the group developed a measure, the Brief Bivariate correlations between religious coping
Multidimensional Measure of Religion/Spirituality (positive and negative) measures and psychological
(BMMRS), which taps into 12 domains of religion adjustment (positive and negative) were averaged to
and spirituality that have been empirically linked to obtain efect size estimates for indings for the four
mental and physical health. hese domains include key relationships: positive religious coping and posi-
daily spiritual experience, meaning, values, beliefs, tive adjustment; positive coping and negative adjust-
forgiveness, private religious practices, RS coping, ment; negative coping and positive adjustment; and
religious support, RS history, commitment, organiza- negative coping and negative adjustment. he strong-
tional religiousness, and religious preference. During est average efect size estimate (0.33) was obtained for
the creation of the measure, care was taken in concep- the 29 studies examining the relationship between
tualization and wording to maximize its potential use positive coping and positive adjustment. From 38
with a wide variety of religious groups. Whenever pos- studies examining correlations between positive
sible, use of the speciic term God was avoided. When coping and negative adjustment, a small, yet signii-
it was unavoidable, the instructions direct individuals cant, efect size estimate of −0.12 was obtained. For
uncomfortable with the term God to replace it with the 16 studies of negative coping and positive adjust-
their own transcendent or higher power. Preliminary ment, a non-signiicant efect size estimate (0.02) was
studies support the theoretical basis for the BMMRS found. Finally, the 22 studies of negative coping and
and indicate acceptable indings for its measures of negative adjustment produced a signiicant moder-
reliability and validity. What remains is for future stud- ate efect size estimate of 0.22. Taken together, ind-
ies to use it to improve understanding of the complex ings from this meta-analysis (Ano & Vasconcelles,
relationship between religion, spirituality, and adjust- 2005) showed that both forms of religious coping are
ment or health (Idler et al., 2003). related to psychological adjustment to stress. Positive
religious coping is related to both positive and nega-
Selective review of studies on tive forms of adjustment in the directions expected,
whereas negative coping is only signiicantly related
religion/spirituality and resilience to negative adjustment.
While the study of RS and resilience is still in an early Findings from more recent studies were consist-
phase of development, there have been a growing num- ent with those of Ano and Vasconcelles (2005). For
ber of studies represented in the literature since the late example, a study of help-seeking military veterans
1990s. Here we will focus initially on indings from a found signiicant associations between lack of for-
meta-analytic study (Ano & Vasconcelles, 2005) that giveness (along with negative religious coping) and
incorporated results from 49 studies meeting inclusion worse mental health outcomes (PTSD and depres-
criteria from the 109 studies initially identiied. In this sion) (Witvliet et al., 2004). Similarly, loss of faith
study, Pargament’s (1997) model of religious coping was found to be associated with worse mental health
was used to distinguish between positive and negative outcomes (i.e., greater utilization of mental health ser-
uses of RS to deal with life crises. Ten positive strat- vices) among military veterans in treatment for PTSD
egies were identiied, ranging from forgiveness and (Fontana & Rosenheck, 2004). Most recently still, in a
seeking spiritual direction, to inding spiritual connec- study of religiously active trauma survivors, positive
tion and benevolent religious reappraisal. Examples relationships were found between a measure of posi-
of negative strategies included spiritual discontent, tive religious coping, seeking spiritual support, and
demonic reappraisal, passive deferral, interpersonal post-traumatic growth. In the same study, a negative
religious discontent, and reappraisal of God’s pow- religious coping indicator, religious strain, was sig-
ers. Psychological adjustment was also dichotomized niicantly related to post-traumatic symptoms (Harris
into positive and negative categories. Examples of et al., 2008). herefore, there is consistency of indings
positive indicators included acceptance, emotional of a complex relationship between religious coping

94
Chapter 6: Religious and spiritual factors in resilience

and psychological adjustment following stress, such Loss of faith refers to a crisis-related reaction that ini-
that positive religious coping is related to both positive tially involves confusion or disillusionment about one’s
adjustment and lower levels of negative adjustment, spiritual beliefs in light of the signiicant life crisis just
while negative religious coping is related to negative encountered. When the experience cannot be readily
adjustment. assimilated by the individual’s existing cognitive inter-
pretive framework or schema, then accommodation,
Spiritual “red flags” or change in the schema, is required. Frequently, indi-
Under ideal circumstances, one’s spiritual beliefs and viduals resolve such a spiritual struggle by over-accom-
practices would be ready resources in coping with life modation, a process of discarding or setting aside their
crises. hey would help to bufer the immediate efects previously held spiritual beliefs. In the short term, the
of severely stressful experiences, promote cognitive cognitive conlict is resolved; however, a longer-term
processing of their meaning, and facilitate inding their consequence is the loss of the positive coping potential
appropriate place in one’s life narrative. Unfortunately, found in spiritual beliefs and faith practices. Among
surviving trauma oten results in shattering of adap- combat veterans in treatment for PTSD, “diiculty
tive illusions and sustaining beliefs, along with intense reconciling my faith with combat experiences” was
emotions and arousal, and an endangered sense of endorsed by more than 75% of respondents (Drescher &
agency and control. Survivors may feel like strangers Foy, 1995).
to themselves and the world around them. he very Negative religious coping, as we have used the term,
core or sustaining elements of the self may have been is characterized by questions or tensions about God’s
severely threatened. Consequently, key aspects of sur- presence, power, and character; strong anger at God;
vivors’ spirituality may become casualties of the trau- discontent with one’s faith community and its clergy;
matic experience, rendering them unavailable for use and punitive appraisals of negative experiences (e.g.,
as coping resources. God is punishing me for my sins) (Drescher & Foy,
Given the indings regarding negative religious 2008).
coping and negative psychological adjustment, what Guilt is represented in three forms: acts of commis-
elements of spirituality may be responsible for this sion, acts of omission, and survivor guilt experienced
relationship? he use of the term spiritual “red lags” when others are killed and the survivor is spared.
dates back to a seminal study by Pargament and col- Survivor guilt oten involves unresolved issues of per-
leagues (1998). In their original study, three prob- ceived randomness and fairness, as well as the persist-
lematic domains or “red lags” were identiied in the ent belief that others who died were more deserving
context of religious coping with life crises: choosing of life or had more to live for. In addition, cognitive
inappropriate goals or ends, using inappropriate distortions about the degree of personal culpability for
means, and conlict with one’s spiritual beliefs and tragic outcomes are oten critical elements that may
values. In their original terminology, “wrong direc- serve to exacerbate and perpetuate this particular spir-
tion,” “wrong road,” and “against the wind” were used itual “red lag.”
to describe the “red lags” (Pargament et al., 1998). Lack of forgiveness represents the inability to posi-
Here we have extended the use of the term to refer tively resolve perceptions of having forgiven others,
to four speciic RS-related obstacles or diiculties fre- feeling forgiven by God, and having forgiven one-
quently encountered by trauma survivors during their self for acts of omission or commission related to life
recovery (Drescher & Foy, 2008). hese obstacles may experiences with tragic outcomes. As noted above,
be problematic as RS issues per se, and/or they may lack of forgiveness has been found to be associated
impede progress toward more general psychological with more severe PTSD and depression among com-
trauma processing and recovery. he “red lags” we bat veterans in treatment for PTSD (Witvliet et al.,
have identiied are loss of faith, negative religious 2004). he four “red lags” oten co-occur and so they
coping, guilt, and lack of forgiveness. hese indices are more frequently found in combination than in
of spiritual struggling or “stuck points” are empiric- isolation (Drescher & Foy, 2008). In a sense, trauma
ally based, having been identiied by studies of trauma survivors who exhibit spiritual “red lags” may appear
survivors, many of which were cited in the previous to be “stuck” or “derailed” in their journey of trauma
section of this chapter. processing and recovery.

95
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Spirituality group therapy module hroughout the pilot testing process, several main
themes emerged, from which the current eight sessions
What systematic eforts have been made to address
in the module were drawn.
the spiritual “red lags” found so frequently among
trauma survivors receiving mental health services? A • What is spirituality? How can I ind connection
concerted efort has been made to develop a standard- within and beyond myself?
ized group therapy or modular approach to incorpor- • heodicy: how does one think about God and the
ate RS issues into combat PTSD treatment (Drescher world around ater trauma?
et al., 2004). he Trauma and Spirituality group at the • Forgiveness: of self and others; feeling forgiven by
Menlo Park treatment program, under the auspices God.
of the US Department of Veterans Afairs (VA), has • Pursuit of what matters. Where do I ind my
its roots in the rich clinical expertise of highly experi- values?
enced psychotherapists in that program working with • How can I ind meaning as a trauma survivor?
combat veterans with PTSD. he Menlo Park program
Feedback received over time from group members and
features a group therapy approach, employing a var-
staf suggested that these themes were important and
iety of therapy groups aimed at the speciic treatment
were not being addressed in other groups within the
and rehabilitation issues presented by many veterans
program at that time. herefore, the clinical aims of the
with chronic PTSD. Earlier experiences in other treat-
intervention were to help veterans to understand better
ment groups had shown that many veterans expressed
how their spirituality was afected by their traumatic
a great deal of rage when religious or spiritual top-
life experiences, and to promote awareness of their
ics were mentioned. Some veterans reported feeling
spirituality as a potential healing resource.
that military chaplains had deceived or let them down
From a research and dissemination perspective,
during their war zone service, and consequently, they
it was important to standardize the group interven-
had been reticent to seek counsel from VA chaplains
tion and evaluate its efectiveness. Accordingly, three
or other clergy since then. Data collected in the PTSD
primary components of a manual set were planned,
residential program indicated that 74% of residents
including a facilitator’s manual, a participant’s work-
reported having diiculty reconciling their reli-
book, and an assessment packet to evaluate the mod-
gious beliefs with the traumatic events they saw and
ule. he primary purpose of creating the manual set
experienced in the war zone and 51% reported that
was to provide a systematic approach to incorporating
they abandoned their religious faith in the war zone
spirituality into trauma treatment, while allowing for
(Drescher & Foy, 1995). Consequently, several spiri-
the empirical investigation of this group treatment.
tuality-related issues relevant to treatment had been
his also provides a guideline for other treatment pro-
identiied that were not being addressed by other
grams or individual clinicians to incorporate spiritu-
treatment groups.
ality into their interventions with trauma survivors.
Pilot work on the Trauma and Spirituality Group
he purpose of the facilitator’s manual is to provide
Module began in 2000. he format was designed to bal-
group leaders with clinical strategies and guidelines
ance educational presentation of content with group
for implementing the group treatment. he facilitator’s
discussion and interactive small-group opportunities
manual details the eight-session group intervention
to enhance interpersonal sharing among the members.
using a predominantly present-centered approach. he
A guiding principle in designing the group experience
eight session themes are:
was that it would be a forum for the safe discussion and
interpersonal processing of diicult and emotionally 1. What is spirituality?
laden issues related to spirituality, broadly deined. In 2. Building connections (including spiritual
that respect, it was to be diferent from other groups practices)
in the program that relied upon a psychoeducational 3. Spiritual practices
format (i.e., teaching “correct” ways to view relevant 4. heodicy (the “problem of evil”)
issues). In order to standardize group procedures, the 5. Hostility and forgiveness
plan was to develop a manual set (module) consisting 6. Forgiveness of self
of a facilitator’s guide, a member’s workbook, and an 7. Values
evaluation packet. 8. Making meaning.

96
Chapter 6: Religious and spiritual factors in resilience

he participant’s workbook is a session-by-session encouragement while they take on the challenges of


guide designed to accompany the facilitator’s manual. trauma processing work.
he organization of the workbook follows the facilita-
tor’s manual outline. It is a practically oriented volume Other notable religion/spirituality
that serves several purposes. First, it provides systematic
and consistent implementation. Second, it establishes a interventions
framework of practical applications for group partici- A review and meta-analysis of 14 forgiveness
pation and support, guiding group members through intervention studies found that those receiving a
their treatment process. hird, the workbook provides process-based form of intervention showed signiicant
members with practical information about the use of improvements by forgiving more, increasing their posi-
spirituality as a healing resource. Finally, the materials tive afect and self-esteem, and reducing their negative
presented can be used as a reference that includes spir- afect (Lundahl et al., 2008). he review suggested that
itual readings, healing stories, relevant deinitions and results were better for individually administered inter-
a journal for their own spiritual experiences. ventions than for a group format. Additionally, mul-
he assessment packet includes measures for three tiple sessions, administered over a sustained period
components. he irst component evaluates each mem- of time, were associated with better outcomes. Most
ber’s knowledge gain for the material presented in each of the studies in the review were conducted with indi-
session. A second component assesses the facilitator’s viduals reporting interpersonal betrayal. he applic-
adherence to the session guidelines in the facilitator’s ability of these interventions for more severe trauma
manual. he third component measures members’ survivors has not been established, nor has the spe-
changes in religiosity and spirituality, and it is admin- ciic issue of self-forgiveness been addressed (Lundahl
istered before the irst group session and ater the inal et al., 2008).
session. More detailed descriptions of the Trauma Another example of a mental health treatment
and Spirituality Group Module development have based upon principles of Eastern spirituality is the
been provided by Drescher et al. (2004). he facilita- work being done utilizing the construct of mindful-
tor’s manual, participant’s workbook, and assessment ness. Kabat-Zinn (1990) introduced a commonly
packet are available in electronic form and may be used deinition of mindfulness as “intentional, non-
requested from the authors. judgmental awareness.” he mindfulness construct
Why might a group approach to address trauma and has been incorporated into several behavioral treat-
spirituality be especially appropriate? Along with its core ments for both PTSD and depression, including
symptoms, chronic PTSD frequently involves social dis- acceptance and commitment therapy and dialectical
connection and isolation. Using a group format provides behavior therapy. Acceptance and commitment ther-
positive social supports for its members while stimulat- apy has been identiied as an empirically supported
ing the use and further development of their interper- treatment for depression, and evaluation of its eicacy
sonal connections and skills. Members are accountable for PTSD is ongoing. Dialectical behavior therapy
to their peers, the other group members, for providing a was originally created as a treatment for borderline
therapeutically safe and respectful group environment personality disorder and has been utilized with sev-
where diversity in religious backgrounds and spiritual eral other disorders. here is empirical support for
beliefs and practices is encouraged. Using a group for- its use in combination with exposure treatment in
mat is also consistent with current concepts about how efectively addressing issues related to PTSD (Cahill
spirituality might promote trauma recovery. et al., 2009). Two additional approaches using mind-
For example, McIntosh et al. (1993) suggested fulness have recently been reviewed. One is mindful-
three potential healing pathways through which spir- ness-based cognitive therapy (Coelho et al., 2007),
ituality could ameliorate some of the impact of a trau- which is a group treatment incorporating mindful-
matic event. hese pathways were enhanced social ness meditation with elements of cognitive therapy
support, changed cognitive processing of the event, for depression. Ater examining four relevant stud-
and enhanced sense of meaning attached to the trau- ies, including two randomized clinical trials and one
matic event. herapeutically safe groups can provide non-randomized trial, the authors concluded that
opportunities for members to receive support and there is suicient initial evidence to support the use

97
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

of this therapeutic approach for individuals sufering Implications for future clinical
recurrent bouts (more than three) of severe depres-
sion. Another approach is called mindfulness-based and research applications
stress reduction, which is a group program running here is a very clear clinical implication that can be
over 8–10 weeks and utilizing mindfulness meditation drawn from the current scientiic knowledge about RS
to alleviate sufering associated with a variety of phys- and resilience: clinicians need to promote the use of
ical efects associated with mental health problems. positive religious coping methods by those clients whose
A recent meta-analysis of studies using mindfulness- religious backgrounds and practices are consistent with
based stress reduction (Grossman et al., 2004) found them. A irst step for trauma clinicians is learning what
a medium sized positive efect for mental health out- the positive religious coping methods actually are. Much
comes, including depression and anxiety. of the empirical work on identifying these practices has
been accomplished by Pargament and his colleagues
(1998), where a full explanation of each positive prac-
Conclusions tice is given. Examples of these practices include ben-
It is encouraging that progress is being made with evolent reappraisal, or seeking a lesson from God in the
respect to identifying key RS elements associated with event; seeking spiritual support, or searching for comfort
resilience. Recent studies have shown that positive reli- in God’s care; active religious surrender, or coping as one
gious coping consistently shows a strong relationship can and then leaving the outcome with God; and seeking
to positive adjustment ater crises. Progress has also spiritual connection, or recognizing that one is part of a
been made in operationally deining positive religious larger spiritual force (Peres et al., 2007). Description of
coping. Examples of positive religious coping activities Eastern practices related to well-being, such as compas-
signiicantly linked to positive psychological adjust- sion, mindfulness, and challenging one’s source of iden-
ment in the Western traditions include religious puri- tity, can also be found (Ricard, 2007).
ication, forgiveness, seeking support from clergy and/ A second clinical implication is also clear: clini-
or faith community members, collaborative religious cians need to be aware of spiritual “red lags” that may
coping, religious surrender, and benevolent religious be part of the problem their clients are experiencing
reappraisal (Ano & Vasconcelles, 2005). Universal in their recovery attempts. Loss of faith and cognitive
compassion, self-relection, mindfulness, and detach- distortions about lack of forgiveness and guilt per se
ment from desire and negative emotion have been may present formidable obstacles in the path to trauma
linked to well-being in Eastern traditions (Davidson recovery, and they may disallow the use of positive reli-
et al., 2003). Equally important is the empirical iden- gious and other coping practices that might otherwise
tiication of spiritual obstacles or “red lags” – loss of promote recovery. For clients mired in these “red lags,”
faith, guilt, and lack of forgiveness – that are correlated clinicians might consider referral to a member of the
with negative psychological adjustment. Although the clergy for help in their resolution.
importance of RS as a resource in coping with life cri- More generally, trauma clinicians need to inquire
ses is widely acknowledged, the dynamic processes by about their clients’ religious backgrounds and spiritual
which the coping is accomplished remain to be identi- beliefs and practices as part of their intake assessment.
ied and understood. Clients’ RS strengths and resources can be identiied,
With the exception of studies of forgiveness and as well as possible spiritual “red lags.” Clinicians need
mindfulness/compassion interventions, the empir- not share their clients’ religious backgrounds, prac-
ical validation of RS-speciic interventions to promote tices, and beliefs, but they do need to be aware of them,
trauma recovery or resilience is in its infancy. To date, show respect for them, and encourage the use of posi-
there are no published controlled trials. Considerable tive coping practices associated with them.
progress has been made, however, on the development Several pressing research implications are also
of a testable intervention, a Trauma and Spirituality apparent. Perhaps the most important research issue for
Group Module. Standardized elements for the module future studies concerns measurement of RS. here needs
are available, including a facilitator’s manual, a partici- to be consistency across studies employing operational
pant’s workbook, and an assessment packet for evalu- measures of RS. In this area as well, Pargament and his
ating each session and the overall module. colleagues (2000) have made a major contribution by

98
Chapter 6: Religious and spiritual factors in resilience

developing a standardized measure of positive and nega- Foa, T. M. Keane, M. J. Friedman, & J. A. Cohen (eds.),
tive religious coping (RCOPE) that can be adjusted for Efective treatments for PTSD: Practice guidelines from
various needs. he BMMRS (Idler et al., 2003) is another the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, 2nd
edn. New York: Guilford Press.
well-designed and standardized instrument that taps 12
RS dimensions, including short versions of Pargament’s Charuvastra, A. & Cloitre, M. (2008). Social bonds
positive and negative religious coping questionnaire. and posttraumatic stress disorder. Annual Review of
Psychology, 59, 301–328.
Beyond the use of standardized instruments that pro-
vide consistent operational deinitions for RS, future Coelho, H. F., Canter, P. H., & Ernst, E. (2007). Mindfulness-
based cognitive therapy: Evaluating current evidence
studies also need to assess both positive and negative
and informing future research. Journal of Consulting and
aspects of religious coping, as well as other dimensions Clinical Psychology, 75, 1003–1005.
of RS. Designing studies to include multiple dimensions
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., et al. (2003).
of RS will allow relationships between RS dimensions to Alterations in brain and immune function produced by
be empirically examined directly. mindfulness meditation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65,
Regarding the experimental designs of future 564–570.
studies, there needs to be an emphasis on using lon- Drescher, K. D. & Foy, D. W. (1995). Spirituality and trauma
gitudinal designs that allow temporal and causal rela- treatment: Suggestions for including spirituality as
tionships between study variables to be addressed. a coping resource. National Center for PTSD Clinical
Most currently published studies are cross-sectional in Quarterly, 5, 4–5.
design and can provide no information about tempor- Drescher, K. D. & Foy, D. W. (2008). When they come
ality and causality in the relationships found between home: Posttraumatic stress, moral injury, and spiritual
variables of interest. However, the consistent ind- consequences for veterans. Relective Practice: Formation
ings across these preliminary studies can inform the and Supervision in Ministry, 28, 85–102.
hypotheses to be tested in more rigorously designed Drescher, K. D., Ramirez, G., Leoni, J. J., et al. (2004).
future studies. Spirituality and trauma: Development of a group therapy
A inal research implication regards the need for module. Group Journal, 28, 71–87.
studies to evaluate the eicacy of RS interventions for Eriksson, C. B. (2008). Religion as a risk and resilience factor.
trauma survivors. Ultimately, randomized controlled In Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the International
trials, comparing diferent forms of intervention (or Society for Traumatic Stress, Chicago, November, 2008.
wait-list controls), are needed. In the interim, manual- Fontana, A. & Rosenheck, R. (2004). Trauma, change in
ized interventions can be developed and pilot tested. strength of religious faith, and mental health service use
among veterans treated for PTSD. Journal of Nervous and
As a start, pre-treatment and post-treatment measures
Mental Disease, 192, 579–584.
can be administered to treatment groups as they are
conducted. When possible, untreated similar groups Garbarino, J. & Bedard, C. (1996). Spiritual challenges to
children facing violent trauma. Childhood: A Global
could receive the same assessments to provide scores Journal of Child Research, 3, 467–478.
to compare with those in the treatment group.
Garmezy, N. (1991). Resiliency and vulnerability to adverse
developmental outcomes associated with poverty.
References American Behavioral Scientist, 34, 416–430.
Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Bremner, J. D., et al. (2006). he Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H.
enduring efects of abuse and related adverse experiences (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health
in childhood: A convergence of evidence from beneits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic
neurobiology and epidemiology. European Archives of Research, 57, 35–43.
Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 256, 174–186. Hagberg, J. O. & Guelich, R. A. (2005). he critical journey:
Ano, G. G. & Vasconcelles, E. B. (2005). Religious coping Stages in the life of faith, 2nd edn. Salem, WI: Sheield
and psychological adjustment distress: A meta-analysis. Publishing.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61, 461–480. Hall, T. (2007). Psychoanalysis, attachment, and spirituality.
Baumeister, R. F. & Exline, J. J. (2000). Self-control, morality, Part I: he emergence of two relational traditions.
and human strength. Journal of Social and Clinical Journal of Psychology and heology, 3, 14–28.
Psychology, 19, 29–42. Harris, J. I., Erbes, C. R., Engdahl, B. E., et al. (2008).
Cahill, S. P., Rothbaum, B. O., Resick, P. A., & Follette, V. M. Christian religious functioning and trauma outcomes.
(2009). Cognitive behavioral therapy for adults. In E. B. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 17–29.

99
Section 1: Pathways to resilience

Idler, E. L., Musick, M. A., Ellison, C. G., et al. (2003). Pargament, K. I. (1997). he psychology of religion and coping:
Measuring multiple dimensions of religion and heory, research, practice. New York: Guilford Press.
spirituality for health research: Conceptual background Pargament, K. I., Zinnbauer, B. J., Scott, A. B., et al. (1998).
and indings from the 1998 General Social Survey. Red lags and religious coping: Identifying some religious
Research on Aging, 25, 327–365. warning signs among people in crisis. Journal of Clinical
James, W. (1997). he varieties of religious experience. Psychology, 54, 77–89.
New York: Touchstone. Pargament, K. I., Koenig, H. G., & Peres, L. M. (2000).
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the he many methods of religious coping: Development
wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and and initial validation of the RCOPE. Journal of Clinical
illness. New York: Delacorte. Psychology, 56, 519–543.
Larson, D. B., Pattison, E. M., Blazer, D. G., Omran, A. R., & Park, C. L. (2005). Religion as a meaning-making
Kaplan, B. H. (1986). Systematic analysis of research on framework in coping with life stress. Journal of Social
religious variables in four major psychiatric journals, Issues, 61, 707–730.
1978–1982. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, Peres, J. F. P., Moreira-Almeida, A., Nasello, A. G., & Koenig,
329–334. H. G. (2007). Spirituality and resilience in trauma
Larson, D. B., Swyers, J. P., & McCullough, M. E. (1997). victims. Journal of Religion and Health, 46,
Scientiic research on spirituality and health: A consensus 343–350.
report. Rockville, MD: National Institute for Healthcare Perry, B. D. & Pollard, R. (1998). Homeostasis, stress,
Research. trauma, and adaptation: A neurodevelopmental view
Lundahl, B. W., Taylor, M. J., Stevenson, R., & Roberts, K. D. of childhood trauma. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric
(2008). Process-based forgiveness interventions: A meta- Clinics of North America, 7, 33–51.
analytic review. Research on Social Work Practice, 18, Powell, L. H., Shajhabi, L., & horesen, C. E. (2003).
465–478. Religion and spirituality: Linkages to physical health.
Luthar, S. S. (2005). Resilience at an early age and its impact American Psychologist, 58, 36–52.
on child psychosocial development. In R. E. Tremblay, Ricard, M. (2007). Happiness: A guide to developing life’s most
R. G. Barr, & R. De V. Peters (eds.), Encyclopedia on important skill. New York: Little Brown.
early childhood development (pp. 1–6). Montreal: Centre Richardson, G. E. (2002). he metatheory of resilience and
of Excellence for Early Childhood Development, resiliency. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58,
http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/ 307–321.
LutharANGxp.pdf (accessed February 26, 2011).
Rutter, M. (1985). Resilience in the face of adversity:
Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Ricard, M., & Protective factors and resistance to psychiatric disorder.
Davidson, R. J. (2004). Long-term meditators self-induce British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 598–611.
high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental
practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Rutter, M. (1998). Developmental catch-up, and deicit,
of the USA, 101, 16369–16373. following adoption ater severe global early privation.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes Disciplines, 39, 465–476.
in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238.
Samerof, A. J., Bartko, W. T., Baldwin, A., Baldwin, C., &
McCullough, M. E. (2000). Forgiveness as a human strength: Seifer, R. (1998). Family and social inluences on the
heory, measurement, and links to well-being. Journal of development of child competence. In M. Lewis &
Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 43–55. C. Feiring (eds.), Families, risk, and competence
McIntosh, D. N., Silver, R. C., & Wortman, C. B. (1993). (pp. 161–185). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Religion’s role in adjustment to a negative life event: Schuster, M. A., Stein, B. D., Jaycox, L. H., et al. (2001). A
coping with the loss of a child. Journal of Personality and national survey of stress reactions ater the September
Social Psychology, 65, 812–821. 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. New England Journal of
Nooney, J. & Woodrum, E. (2002). Religious coping and Medicine, 345, 1507–1512.
church-based social support as predictors of mental Snyder, C. R. (2000). he past and possible futures of hope.
health outcomes: Testing a conceptual model. Journal for Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19,
the Scientiic Study of Religion, 41, 359–368. 11–28.
Oxman, T. E., Freeman, D. H., & Manheimer, E. D. (1995). Sutton, S. K. & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain
Lack of social participation or religious strength and asymmetry: A biological substrate of the behavioral
comfort as risk factors for death ater cardiac surgery in approach and inhibition systems. Psychological Science,
the elderly. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 5–15. 8, 204–210.

100
Chapter 6: Religious and spiritual factors in resilience

Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., et al. (2004). Making Werner, E. E. (2000). Protective factors and individual
a life worth living: neural correlates of well-being. resilience. In J. P. Shonkof & S. J. Meisels (eds.),
Psychological Science, 15, 367–372. Handbook of early childhood intervention. 2nd edn,
Weaver, A. J., Samford, J., Kline, A. E., et al. (1997). What do (pp. 115–132). New York: Cambridge University Press.
psychologists know about working with the clergy? An Witvliet, C. V. O., Phillips, K. A., Feldman, M. E., &
analysis of eight APA journals: 1991–1994. Professional Beckham, J. C. (2004). Posttraumatic mental and
Psychology: Research & Practice, 28, 471–474. physical health correlates of forgiveness and religious
Werner, E. E. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible: A coping in military veterans. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
longitudinal study of resilient children and youth. 17, 269–273.
New York: McGraw-Hill.

101
Section 2 Resilience across the lifespan
Chapter
Resilience in children and adolescents

7 Ann S. Masten, Amy R. Monn, and Laura M. Supkof

Introduction A small but highly inluential group of scientists in


child development, clinical psychology, and psychiatry
In the sciences concerned with human development, recognized the potential importance of understanding
resilience can be deined as the capacity of a dynamic, such resilience in high-risk children, both for theory and
living system to endure or recover from a major dis- practice. hese pioneers, including Norman Garmezy,
turbance and continue to develop in healthy ways. Lois Murphy, Michael Rutter, and Emmy Werner,
Resilience is a broad systems concept that generally inspired the descriptive irst wave of resilience research,
refers to the capacity of any dynamic system to adapt which focused on deining and measuring resilience to
or recover from a signiicant threat to its function or identify the most promising factors associated with the
viability. It can be applied to an individual, a family, an phenomena that fell under this rather large conceptual
organization, or an ecosystem. Interest in resilience umbrella (Masten & Wright, 2009). Subsequent waves
emerged independently in the ecological and social of research have focused on explanatory processes, with
sciences around the same time (late 1960s and early the goals of understanding the processes by which resili-
1970s), probably as a result of common roots in general ence occurs (wave two), whether it can be promoted
systems theory (Masten & Obradović, 2008). through intervention (wave three), and how it works
Judging from ancient tales of young heroes and across levels of human function (wave four), from the
heroines, people have been intrigued with the idea of molecular level of cells to the macro level of societies
overcoming adversity for millennia. he science of (Masten, 2007; Masten & Wright, 2009).
resilience in children, however, emerged in the 1960s he early work on resilience in children and youth
as investigators, searching for the causes of mental was part of a broader movement that has revolutionized
illnesses such as schizophrenia and behavioral prob- theory and practice in mental health, contributing to
lems such as delinquency, began to study children at the emergence of the integrative perspective of develop-
risk for psychopathology (Luthar, 2006; Masten, 2007, mental psychopathology in the 1970s (Cicchetti, 1984,
2011). he rationale behind this approach was to begin 2006; Masten, 2006a). he developmental psychopath-
research before disorders were evident in order to study ology framework represented a synthesis of two great
their causes and course of development (Garmezy & traditions in science, one focused on problems and
Streitman, 1974; Watt et al., 1984; Obradović et al., mental illness and the other focused on normal devel-
2011). When investigators began to study groups of opment (Masten, 2006a). Longitudinal studies of chil-
children believed to be at risk, the striking variability in dren at risk required ideas, methods, and investigators
the life course among the individual children became from both traditions. Soon a synergistic merger was
apparent. he diversity in outcomes of children who underway, with the promise of bridging developmental
shared risk factors was impressive, particularly when and clinical perspectives to improve the understanding
followed forward in time. Investigators were con- of pathways toward and away from psychopathology
fronted with the puzzle of numerous young people in over the life course. New textbooks and journals fol-
“high-risk” groups who were showing positive mental lowed, with a focus on delineating the new conceptual
health and development. approach of developmental psychopathology.

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

103
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

A central tenet of the new framework was the idea viability of a dynamic system. It is an inferential con-
that contrasting variations in adaptation – successful cept because the deinition is conditional with respect
and unsuccessful, normal and deviant, resilient and to two major stipulations: there must be “good adap-
maladaptive – are mutually informative for under- tation” by some deinition and there must have been
standing the course of human development in its exposure to “serious risk” of some kind. Judging or
varying forms, pathological and healthy (Sroufe assessing good adaptation is typically based on expec-
et al., 2005; Cicchetti, 2006; Masten, 2006a). In devel- tations for developmental level. Expectations for infant
opmental psychopathology, it was just as important and adult behavior clearly difer. However, the threat
to understand pathways to competence and posi- side of the equation also has a developmental context
tive mental health as it was to understand the path- because the nature of threat exposure and the per-
ways to problems and disorders. Proponents of this ception of threat also vary with developmental level.
approach argued that the study of competence and Infants and adolescents experience diferent kinds of
positive development, particularly among those at adversity and experience the same objective threat in
risk for psychopathology, was informative for under- diferent ways.
standing, preventing, and treating symptoms and Resilience also is a dynamic concept, which means
disorders. hey also argued that a developmental that it refers to how well a system is (or has been) actively
perspective was essential for understanding adapta- adapting or developing in relation to a signiicant chal-
tion and behavior in a developing organism. hus, the lenge. Deinitions variously refer to the capacity for,
developmental psychopathology movement strongly outcomes of, or processes involved in the dynamics
supported research on positive aspects of develop- of resilience. Generally, resilience must be manifested
ment, including resilience. (i.e., there is an observable track record of doing alright
his chapter highlights the concepts and ind- during or ater adversity), although presumably it is
ings from the developmental research on resilience in possible to predict resilience based on evidence from
young people, spanning childhood to adulthood. he other people. In other words, investigators typically
irst section describes how resilience has been deined are reluctant to identify people as “resilient” who have
in developmental research, including discussion of never been exposed to serious adversity, although they
salient controversies and issues in deining resili- might be willing to predict better odds for resilience
ence for research. he second section highlights core based on similarities to other people who have mani-
models and approaches that guided the developmen- fested resilience.
tal research on resilience. he third section highlights he overarching goal of research on resilience in
indings from the broad literature on resilience in chil- children has been to understand how resilience hap-
dren and youth, discussing the “short list” of widely pens, with an eye toward promoting and protecting
reported protective factors for resilience and what this positive adaptation in development. Identifying resili-
list may signify about the core adaptive systems for ence is a key irst step, but it is also important to work
resilience in human development. he fourth section out what makes a diference for better adaptation and
presents a resilience framework for intervention based how. Consequently, it is also important to attend to
on the main evidence, with general guidelines for the predictors of resilience among those exposed to
applying current evidence to help young people who hazards: the promotive and protective factors associ-
are sufering now and cannot wait for scientists to learn ated with good adaptation during or following ser-
all the answers. he resilience framework includes the ious threats. he predictors of resilience also would be
ive “Ms” of mission, models, measures, methods, and expected to change with development, as resources and
multiple levels. Concluding comments highlight new adaptive systems develop, mature, and decline.
horizons of research on resilience in the early decades his section of the chapter discusses developmen-
of life. tal approaches to the deinition of resilience, includ-
ing the two core components of adaptation and threat
Defining resilience from a that are required to identify resilience, as well as the
meaning of promotive and protective inluences that
developmental perspective are invoked to explain how resilience comes about.
Resilience broadly refers to good adaptation in the Key deinitional issues and controversies are also
context of signiicant threats to the development or discussed.

104
Chapter 7: Resilience in children and adolescents

Defining good adaptation in development (such as low birth weight or neglect),


individual traumatic experiences (such as abuse), and
developmental resilience research mass disasters (such as hurricanes or war). What all
In research on human development, good adapta- of these various risk factors have in common is their
tion is oten deined from a normative perspective, established potential to disrupt or harm normal func-
with respect to typical development and expected tion or development.
achievements across the lifespan for people of a given It was quickly apparent to the researchers examin-
age, gender, culture, and period in history (Masten & ing early risk that risk factors or negative life experi-
Coatsworth, 1998). hese expectations for compe- ences oten pile up in the lives of children and youth
tence based on normative development have been (Obradović et al., 2011). Divorce, for example, was
termed developmental tasks (Havighurst, 1972; Masten not a simple event but a complex sequence of experi-
et al., 2006a). For a school-age child, these expecta- ences oten accompanied by chronic family conlict,
tions might include obedience to parents, following economic hardship, moving, and loss. Children with
community norms for public conduct, going to school one major risk factor, such as an abusive parent, oten
and behaving appropriately in the classroom, learning faced other risks too. As a result, attention shited away
what is being taught in school, and getting along with from the study of isolated risk factors to concentrate on
other people, including peers and teachers. As chil- cumulative risk. Methods of tallying or combining risk
dren mature, developmental tasks also change. School factors were developed, ranging from simple counts
work is more challenging and children are expected of negative life events to complex ratings of adversity
to follow societal rules without constant supervision. based on clinical interviews (e.g., Obradović et al.,
In adolescence and early adulthood, there are new 2011).
domains of developmental tasks, including expecta- Risk gradients were created by plotting adaptive
tions for work, romantic relationships, parenting, and function (positive or negative) as a function of risk
civic engagement. level. As risk level rises, the average level of positive
In psychiatry, good adaptation might be judged in adaptive behavior declines and problems rise (depend-
reference to normal mental health or the absence of ing on what is being plotted). Consequently, studies of
mental health disorders or impairment. In this case stressful life events and disaster oten ind a dose gradi-
too, judgments about adaptive function take develop- ent where the children who were more highly exposed
mental expectations into account, oten in the form (more adversity) show more problems (Pine et al.,
of diagnostic criteria that consider impairment in life 2005; Masten & Osofsky, 2010; Obradović et al., 2011).
tasks (e.g., going to school or learning to read) and Risk gradients have been illustrated for many health
developmental level (see Masten et al., 2006a). In the and behavior outcomes plotted as a function of various
diagnostic criteria for attention-deicit hyperactivity indicators of cumulative risk (Obradović et al., 2011).
disorder, for example, an individual must show symp- Resilience theorists have pointed out that it is oten
toms of inattention or hyperactivity–impulsivity “to a arbitrary to designate these gradients as “risk gradi-
degree that is maladaptive and inconsistent with devel- ents” because, in many cases, high risk also indicates
opmental level” (American Psychiatric Association, low assets or resources (Masten A.S. et al., 2009a). A
2000, p. 92). high-risk score, for example, might be based on the fol-
Good adaptation or healthy function is not lowing risk factors: single-parent household, a parent
enough to deine resilience, however. here also must who is poorly educated, low birth weight, poor parent-
be some kind of challenge to the organism with the ing skills, low income, and a disadvantaged neigh-
potential to disrupt or destroy normative function or borhood with high unemployment. A low-risk score,
development. in this case, is likely to indicate that a child has more
resources in the form of two better educated parents,
Defining threats in developmental more family income, and a neighborhood with more
social capital, better schools, and greater safety.
resilience research Also of greatest interest to resilience investigators
Many kinds of potential threat to the lives of children was the striking variability in adaptive function within
and youth have been studied under the rubric of resili- levels of risk. hey observed individuals exposed to high
ence, including well-established risk factors for normal levels of risk or adversity who showed good adaptive

105
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

behavior, thus appearing to be “of gradient” cases in protecting children when risk is high for child mal-
terms of prediction. hese resilient individuals were treatment or neglect. In contrast, good parents may
doing better than expected based on their level of risk, take all kinds of routine action to raise healthy children
or “beating the odds.” Similarly, there were individuals under normal conditions, but also spring into emer-
who were doing worse than expected at low levels of gency action when a child’s life is threatened. Parents
risk, although these “vulnerable” individuals (poor are a multipurpose adaptive system for child rearing,
adaptation in the context of low risk) appear to be less with many promotive and protective roles.
common than the resilient individuals (see Masten Under circumstances of threat, individual people
et al., 1999). may call on or attract help and resources from self,
he risk side of the resilience equation also must be other people, community organizations, and the envir-
considered from a developmental perspective because onment. hese resources and access to them all change
the nature of exposure and the repercussions of the as a function of development and as contexts change
threat are inluenced by the development of the indi- across the life course. For example, the individual
vidual. For example, younger children are less likely to problem-solving capabilities of children improve as the
be kidnapped and forced to be a child soldier and less brain matures and they learn more cognitive skills (i.e.,
likely to understand the full import of war (Masten & human capital develops). Older children and youth
Osofsky, 2010). he loss of parents in a war zone, how- also develop connections and friendships in the com-
ever, may have much greater signiicance for a young munity that extend the resources they have to call upon
child who is unable to survive without the care of in times of threat (social capital develops). At the same
others than it may have for an older youth. Responses time, older children and youth have greater mobility
to threat also are inluenced by development because and access to more extensive networks of informa-
the capacities of individuals for adaptation also change tion. All of these changes have the potential to alter
with development. the resources and tools available for adaptation in the
face of threat. However, it is important to remember
Promotive and protective factors in that sometimes development brings greater vulner-
ability to threat as well as greater exposure to adversity.
developmental resilience research Development can be a two-edged sword when it comes
From the outset, resilience investigators were looking to adaptive behavior. In adolescence, greater aware-
for factors that might counteract, bufer, or ameliorate ness of threats and greater exposure to adversity may
the efects of adversity and other kinds of threat – what- accompany developmentally based improvements
ever made it possible for some young people to over- in thinking abilities, knowledge, physical skills, and
come adversity when others did not. Promotive factors mobility.
or inluences encompass those assets, resources, rela-
tionships, or strengths that appear to contribute in
positive ways to good adaptation and development, Definitional issues and controversies
regardless of risk level. Protective factors, by compari- here have been a number of issues and controversies
son, show a special efect in the context of high threat, about the meaning of resilience in research on human
moderating the consequences of threat for adapta- development. Consideration of these issues generally
tion or development. Sometimes it is diicult to tell has had a positive efect on the quality of the science,
the diference between these two types of efect, either oten clarifying confusing variations in terminology
because the low-risk groups are missing (only high-risk from meaningful diferences in substance. Several key
groups are studied) or because the same factor (e.g., an examples are highlighted here.
efective parent) seems to have both types of efect. he
diference between promotive and protective functions Who decides on the criteria for resilience?
is most striking for the protective factors that only are To study resilience, it was necessary to deine and
functional when risk is high. Airbags in automobiles measure both “good adaptation” and “threat,” as
and antibodies to infectious agents that form with vac- noted above. Resilience research has been criticized
cination are classic examples of protective efects that for inconsistencies across studies of these criteria and
work only in speciic high-risk conditions. Crisis nur- measures, which does make it diicult to aggregate
sery services are designed to operate in a similar way, indings. hese deinitions by diferent investigators,

106
Chapter 7: Resilience in children and adolescents

both conceptual and operational, involved many deci- resilience, focused on multiple criteria, tend to identify
sions and, in the end, judgment calls about the best cri- the protective factors with the broadest efects, such as
teria and measures for the purpose at hand. Many issues parenting or general intellectual skills.
are embedded in these decisions. Should resilience be
deined in terms of developmental task achievement Time issues: resilience versus recovery and resistance
or subjective well-being, or both? If a person does not he time frame for deining resilience also has gen-
have a diagnosable mental illness, does this mean they erated some debate. Some would argue that when
are doing well? When young people live in bicultural recovery from a traumatic disturbance takes a long
contexts (e.g., immigrant youth), should bicultural time, it should be called recovery and not resili-
criteria be applied in deining resilience? Should the ence (e.g., Bonanno, 2004), while others encompass
criteria for resilience vary as a function of the severity a wide range of resistance and recovery patterns
of adversity exposure? What does resilience mean in a under the conceptual umbrella of resilience (e.g.,
child soldier? What are the best ways to composite risk Masten, 2001, 2011). Still others diferentiate resist-
factors and stressful life experiences? Is subjective stress ance (maintaining good function despite trauma
appraisal more important than objective exposure? exposure) from resilience (disturbance in function
hese are just a small sample of questions that con- followed by rapid recovery) from recovery (when the
vey some of the issues involved in deining and meas- return to good function takes longer). Norris et al.
uring the adaptive behavior and risk components of (2008) have provided an overview of such difer-
resilience. Given the potential for confusion, the con- ences. he important point is probably to note that
clusions in this literature have shown remarkable con- deining resilience requires a time perspective. he
sistency, as discussed in a subsequent section. nature of the risk or adversity also may be import-
ant, since some adversities have acute onset and short
Specificity issues: narrow versus broad definitions? duration while others are chronic and extended over
A related set of issues were focused on whether to deine time. Many investigators note that the same person
resilience in narrow or broad ways, in one domain or may appear to be maladaptive at one point in life and
in multiple domains simultaneously. Variable-focused doing well at a diferent point. Consequently, it could
approaches (discussed below) tended to focus on one depend on when the assessment of adaptive function
particular outcome domain and its predictors (e.g., is done as to whether a person is viewed as resilient or
school achievement and the predictors of “academic maladaptive. In any event, there is considerable inter-
resilience”), whereas person-focused approaches (also est in the possibilities of “late emerging resilience” or
discussed below), such as case studies and the identi- “late bloomers” in developmental resilience research,
ication of resilient subgroups of children in high-risk relecting dramatic turnarounds in the life course.
samples, tended to consider multiple domains of func- Clearly, there are individual cases of young people
tion in identifying resilient people. Variable-focused whose lives were going in a bad direction in adoles-
studies highlighted the fact that doing well can be cence who become successful young adults (Werner &
domain speciic and that a given protective factor may Smith, 1992; Masten et al., 1999, 2006b). It also is clear
be more important in one domain than another. he that there are dramatic declines in some areas of bad
speciicity of this approach can be illuminating for behavior during the transition from adolescence to
closing in on clues to the processes involved in spe- adulthood ater a sharp increase in the same behaviors
ciic aspects of adaptation. In person-focused stud- during the adolescent years; such behaviors include
ies, resilience was akin to a diagnosis of doing well in antisocial behavior, alcohol use and dependence, or
spite of risk or adversity. As in other kinds of diagnosis, risk-taking behavior (Miller et al., 1998; Masten et al.,
multiple criteria made sense. A child who excelled in 2008; Steinberg, 2008).
one domain (e.g., having friends) but failed miserably
in other domains (e.g., school achievement) was not System issues: capacity, process, or outcome; internal or
usually identiied as resilient. Resilience in person- external adaptation; resiliency or resilience?
focused work typically meant that the person was Other deinitional issues in resilience science probably
doing adequately well across all the domains that mat- have their origins in the challenges of deining prop-
ter to stakeholders such as parents or society. Not sur- erties of dynamic systems. Deinitions of resilience
prisingly, these broad approaches to the deinitions of vary, for example in referring to resilience in terms of

107
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

capacity, process, or outcome, or all three aspects. In Person-focused and variable-focused


ecology theory, where resilience is strongly embed-
ded in systems theory, resilience nearly always refers models of resilience
to the capacity of a system for change and resuming Resilience investigators oten combined person-
development ater a disturbance. Deinitions of resili- focused with variable-focused approaches in their
ence in the social sciences also vary in their focus on studies. Person-focused approaches initially included
internal versus external adaptation or both, a variation case examples of individuals judged to be resilient and
that likely relects another systems issue. Living sys- also studies that compared resilient subgroups of high-
tems have a dual nature with respect to adaptation, in risk children with their less adaptive peers with similar
that they must maintain internal coherence or equilib- risk level. Such people inspired the pioneers to study
rium while growing and developing and they must also resilience. Person-focused approaches also included
adapt to the environments in which they live, which analyses of individual pathways over time and studies
are also continually changing (Masten et al., 2006a). of groups of children with life patterns or trajector-
Some investigations have deined resilience strictly in ies that illustrated good function during or following
terms of external adaptation (e.g., how well a child is adversity exposure. Recent advances in multivariate
functioning in the family, school, and with peers) while statistics and growth models have made it possible to
others focus only on internal well-being (e.g., how well identify such patterns in longitudinal data. Figure 7.1
the child is feeling and thinking) and still others on both illustrates a variety of resilience pathways, represent-
aspects of adaptation. Some investigators deine resili- ing various pathways in the course of adaptive behavior
ence in terms of internal and external symptoms rather over time before, during, and ater a signiicant threat
than competence (e.g., the absence of internalizing and to development, both for an acute disturbance (a trau-
externalizing disorders or clinical levels of these symp- matic experience or disaster) and for an extended,
tom dimensions). Finally, there has been considerable chronic period of adversity.
controversy about the use of the term “resiliency” and Variable-focused approaches concentrated on the
whether resilience can be considered a trait of the indi- relation among measured variables in the lives of indi-
vidual. We would concur with those who object to this viduals, oten linking adaptive outcome measures to
term, and argue that resilience usually involves draw- risk or adversity measures, with analyses centered on
ing on resources or systems that extend well beyond identifying other variables that helped to predict bet-
the individual, embedded in attachment relationships, ter outcomes in relation to risk posed by the measured
family systems, and emergency response systems in the adversities. Analytically, these approaches oten relied
community. he capacity for resilience in human func- on multivariate statistics, such as regression analysis
tion, as in other dynamic living systems, is far more or structural equation modeling, that attempted to test
complex than the property of a material (e.g., a rubber hypotheses about potential contributors to resilience
band or steel) to withstand stress and recover its form and their role.
without breaking. Figure 7.2 illustrates some of the major efects tested
in studies of resilience, including intervention efects.
Models and methods in developmental Risk factors are deined as direct, negative predictors of
adaptive behavior. In Figure 7.2, several risk efects are
research on resilience illustrated. Risk 0, which normally has a negative efect
he goals of resilience research called for new mod- on adaptive behavior, is totally prevented by some inter-
els, measures, and methods because positive aspects vention (e.g., premature birth is prevented by good pre-
of adaptation and assessment had been relatively natal care). Risk 1 has a direct, negative efect on adaptive
neglected in the early research on psychopathology behavior (e.g., child abuse predicts worse mental health
and risk, while normative studies of child development and school function). he efect of risk 2 is moderated
had largely ignored individual diferences. Among the (reduced) by a naturally occurring protective factor while
important contributions of resilience research was a the efect of risk 3 is attenuated or eliminated by an inter-
renewed interest in the concepts and tools for assess- vention that also functions as a moderator of risk. Risk
ing competence, environmental assets, and adaptive 4 has an indirect efect on adaptive behavior by under-
systems, as well as promotive and protective efects mining an important asset (asset 2) for adaptive behav-
(Masten & Obradović, 2006). ior (e.g., economic hardship or divorce undermines

108
Chapter 7: Resilience in children and adolescents

Optimal
Acute Figure 7.1 Examples of resilience
pathways in the context of acute trauma
trauma
or chronic adversity. In acute trauma
(top), pathway A shows resistance, B
C shows breakdown and recovery, and C
shows post-traumatic growth. In chronic
adversity (bottom), pathway A shows
Adaptive function

resistance, B shows breakdown and rapid


recovery, and C shows breakdown with
Average

A later recovery.

B
Maladaptive

Time
Optimal

Chronic adversity
Adaptive function
Average

A
B

C
Maladaptive

Time

good parenting, which, in turn, afects adaptive behav- as a risk factor for school achievement. A child’s IQ is a
ior). In this case, asset 2 is said to function as a mediator good predictor of school achievement, probably along
of risk 4 efects on adaptive behavior. Interventions can a continuum that extends from low to high scores.
be designed to protect asset 2 (e.g., providing support In contrast, there are some experiences and re-
for parents under economic stress or in the course of sources that appear to function only in one way. Negative
divorce). Interventions also can be designed to increase life experiences such as injury in a car accident are gen-
the assets available; asset 3 (e.g., a tutor) has been added erally viewed only as a risk factor. Genetic anomalies
through intervention in Figure 7.2. that alter development only in negative ways also could
Oten, what is called a risk factor or a promotive be viewed as pure risk factors. Infections similarly are
factor (asset) is arbitrary, because the actual variable viewed as risk factors for health. Positive examples of
that is measured is a continuously distributed bipolar pure assets or promotive factors – those when present
indicator of variation, with a negative and positive pole. seem to confer advantages in life but when lacking are
Intelligence quotient (IQ) test scores provide a classic not necessarily harmful – might include non-essential
example, in that high IQ can be viewed as a promotive talents (e.g., musical or athletic talent), a good sense of
factor for learning in school while low IQ can be viewed humor, a helpful neighbor, or a nearby library.

109
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Preventive intervention Protective factor


(risk moderator)
Intervention to
moderate risk
Risk 0
(prevented) Risk 1 Risk 2 Risk 3

Adaptive behavior over time


+ + +

Asset 1 Asset 2 Asset 3


(mediator)

Intervention
Risk 4 Intervention to to add asset
protect asset

Figure 7.2 Resilience models showing potential effects of risk factors, assets (also known as promotive factors), protective factors
(moderating risk effects), and interventions strategies that target risks or assets. Risk 0 is prevented by intervention. Risk 1 has a direct negative
effect on adaptive behavior while asset 1 has a direct positive effect. The risk 2 effect is reduced or eliminated by a naturally occurring
protective factor that functions as a moderator of risk for the adaptive behavior under study. Risk 3 is moderated by an intervention designed
to reduce the effects of risk 3. Risk 4 has a negative effect on adaptive behavior through its effects on asset 2 (which is a mediator of risk 4
effects on adaptive behavior). Intervention can be directed at protecting asset 2 in the context of risk 4. Asset 3 is added through intervention
and has a positive effect on adaptive behavior.

Variables that appear to moderate risk also fre- [2006] for a more extended discussion of these models
quently work both ways and so could be viewed as with respect to parenting). Some of the most efective
vulnerability or protective factors in the context of intervention programs to promote achievement and
an interaction model, depending on the emphasis or prevent antisocial behavior in child development have
perspective. Intellectual skills and parenting quality, focused on parent education or training (Patterson
both widely implicated as key moderators of risk in the et al., 2004). Recent eforts to apply this approach to
lives of children, probably work both ways, conferring disaster planning have argued for targeting parents
greater vulnerability when they are low or poor and and teachers as “irst responders” who could learn to
greater protection when they are high (Masten, 2001, mitigate the consequences of disasters on children
2011; Luthar, 2006). through advance training (Pine et al., 2005; Masten &
Other multivariate models focus attention on Obradović, 2008).
mediating efects, where risk factors damage children Resilience models have had a transformative efect
through their efects on key promotive or protect- on intervention, by underscoring the signiicance of
ive factors. For example, if a life event such as a car positive inluences on adaptive behavior and devel-
accident or assault causes brain damage in a child opment. Positive models have great appeal to stake-
(lowering IQ) or in a parent (lowering the quality of holders, which may explain why these positive models
parenting available), the developmental outcomes are taking hold in school counseling (Galassi & Akos,
that depend on good IQ or good parenting may be 2007), prevention science (Miller et al., 1998), social
harmed as well. welfare (Flynn et al., 2006), and other areas of inter-
Intervention models are also built around these vention and policy (Luthar & Cicchetti, 2001).
basic models of resilience, as noted in Figure 7.2.
Intervention efects can be aimed to build up assets or
resources in an attempt to counterbalance risks; they Research on competence and cascades
can target risk factors by preventing them from hap- Another key contribution of the resilience researchers
pening or they can target key promotive or protective was the attention they brought to theory and assess-
factors (such as parenting) in an efort to protect these ment of individual diferences in positive adaptation
key advantages for development (see Masten & Shafer and development (Masten & Obradović, 2006). Such

110
Chapter 7: Resilience in children and adolescents

Table 7.1 Examples of widely reported resilience factors for children and implicated adaptive systems.

Promotive/protective factors Implicated adaptive systems


Good caregiving Attachment, family
Close relationships with other competent and prosocial people Attachment
Intelligence and problem-solving skills Learning and cognition systems of the brain
Self-regulation skills Regulatory systems for self-control, executive functions
of the brain
Self-efficacy and positive self-perceptions Mastery motivation
Hope, faith, and religious affiliations Meaning-making systems of belief, religion
Effective schools Community education system

variations in positive function had been neglected in important clues to the adaptive tools and systems for
favor of methods to study either typical (normative resilience (Masten, 2001). Table 7.1 provides examples
or average) development or problems and symptoms. of the factors oten found in resilience studies and also
Methods for assessing and tracking competence in the adaptive systems that may explain the power of
age-salient developmental tasks were developed and these factors to facilitate adaptive behavior or recovery
greater attention was given to the variability in the in the context of challenge.
normal range for social competence, work or roman- Masten (2001) concluded that, in most situations,
tic competence, civic engagement, and other domains resilience depends on the operation of fundamen-
of competence across the lifespan. Renewed attention tal adaptive systems that are the legacy of biological
was given to theories about adaptation, developmen- and cultural evolution, rather than rare or extraor-
tal tasks, and competence, which had roots deep in the dinary talents, resources, or eforts. Masten hypoth-
history of science (Masten et al., 2006a). esized that the capacity for resilience is generated by
he complex conceptual and empirical connections the “ordinary magic” of these systems when they are
between competence and psychopathology in devel- operating normally and, therefore, that the greatest
opment was re-examined (Masten et al., 2006a). With lasting threats to children occur when the capacity
powerful new statistical tools and longitudinal data on of these protective systems is damaged, impaired, or
positive development as well as symptoms, it was pos- destroyed. Catastrophic adversity can overwhelm all
sible to test ideas about developmental cascades, or the human adaptive capacity, even on a regional scale, as
spreading efects of good and poor adaptation from happens in major disasters. As the threat subsides and
one domain to another over time (Masten et al., 2005; adaptive capacity is restored, adaptive processes begin
Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). Longitudinal data clearly to support recovery.
indicated the high return on investment in programs he basic adaptive systems that support individ-
in early childhood that promoted competence as these ual resilience extend well beyond the individual. All
programs prevented problems developing (Heckman, living systems are interdependent with respect to the
2006). functioning of the systems in their environments.
Moreover, humans are highly social, with lives embed-
ded in many levels of social and cultural organization
Highlights of the research findings on to enhance adaptability and the quality of life.
resilience in children and youth Each of these adaptive systems implicated for
In light of the extraordinary range in resilience research human development and resilience has a long history
with respect to the populations and adversities stud- of research, in some cases across disciplines and lev-
ied, as well as conceptual and methodological varia- els of analysis, although there is less study of exactly
tions, the indings have been remarkably consistent how they work in the context of varying threats. Only
(Garmezy, 1985; Luthar, 2006; Masten, 2001, 2011). a few of the adaptive systems are described here, with a
Many studies converge on a “short list” of factors with focus on those that are being targeted in interventions
strong ties to resilience in children and youth, afording to promote resilience.

111
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Attachment relationships in resilience thinking capabilities for solving problems. Historically,


intelligence has been described as the mental or cog-
From the earliest days of resilience science, investiga-
nitive activity associated with efective adaptation
tors noted the essential role of close relationships for
(Masten et al., 2006a). It is not surprising from this per-
resilience, including the attachments among family
spective to observe that good cognitive skills have been
members, romantic partners, friends, and also spirit-
implicated in many resilience studies of both children
ual relationships. Every review of the evidence since
and adults (Masten, 2001; Luthar, 2006). he capabil-
then has corroborated the central importance of rela-
ities of the human brain to learn, think, and come up
tionships for resilience. he role of these relationships
with solutions to problems about what is going on and
appears to be multifaceted, providing emotional secur-
what to do clearly improve with brain development,
ity, arousal regulation, practical help, physical defense,
education, and experience. Until this adaptive tool is
and access to resources through additional relationships
fully grown and operational, attachment relationships
(social capital). A powerful biological system is impli-
with adults and social organizations function to extend
cated by the data on relationships, initially delineated
the cognitive capacities for resilience of an individual.
by John Bowlby (1969) in his attachment theory on the
Books, computers, and information processing on the
bonds that form between a caregiving parent igure and
Internet also have expanded human capacity for prob-
a child during infancy. his system has efects on behav-
lem solving and inding adaptive solutions to speciic
ior of both the caregiver and the child. Once an attach-
threats.
ment bond has formed, for example, the child and the
parent will both respond to perceived danger by seeking
proximity and experience anxiety or panic if contact is Motivation to adapt: agency, self-efficacy,
blocked. An attachment igure can provide comfort to and the mastery motivation system
a frightened child and also serves as a secure emotional In 1959, Robert White published a now classic paper
base for venturing out in the world when there is no dan- on competence, positing a motivation system that
ger. If a toddler ventures too far or becomes frightened rewards and reinforces adaptive behavior leading to
for some reason, the child will rush back to the secure competence, thus yielding pleasure for perceived suc-
base for comfort. Children typically form attachment cess in eforts to adapt. Pleasure in mastery, he argued,
bonds with other family members and caregivers, pets, can be observed in humans and other similar species
or snuggly objects; when they grow older, people form as they explore the world and try to solve problems or
strong attachment relationships with friends, romantic make things happen. White posited that humans are
partners, and spiritual igures. Loss of attachment igures motivated to adapt to the environment and experience
can induce profound grief, but also can leave a young pleasure when they succeed. Later scholars elaborated
child unprotected from threats. When the primary care- on reward systems for adaptive behavior in theor-
giver of a child is lost or injured, it is crucial that someone ies about self-eicacy (Bandura, 1997) and intrinsic
else takes on the protective roles of the primary attach- motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000). People enjoy learn-
ment igure. As people mature, they form attachment ing to walk, to throw a ball, to solve riddles, and to get
bonds with friends and partners that are more sym- jokes. Very powerful reward systems motivate and
metrically balanced with respect to mutual comforting reinforce eforts to adapt and people who believe they
and care than the relationships of a parent with a child. will be successful can anticipate these rewards and also
Much of the research on the role of close relationships tend to persist in the face of failure. Parents and teach-
for adults in resilience has focused on the role of “social ers are well aware of mastery motivation for learning
support,” but the fundamental nature of the adaptive and performance, and they oten seek to provide small
system underlying the protective efects of relationships experiences of success to enhance the motivation for
is likely to be a mature form of attachment. efort and achievement. Studies in resilience research
of late bloomers, individuals who turn their lives in
Intelligence and problem-solving systems better directions in late adolescence or early adulthood,
suggest that the motivation to change plays a key initi-
of the human central nervous system ating role in change (Masten et al., 2006b). his system
Another widely implicated system (or set of systems) for may be shut down by neglect or adversity, particularly
human adaptation is the central nervous system and its experiences that remove any sense of control over the

112
Chapter 7: Resilience in children and adolescents

environment and opportunities to experience agency development, reinforcement, and support of self-regu-
in the world. he apathy observed among infants lation in adaptive behavior.
with neglect in orphanages or among animals in early
learned helplessness experiments may be related to the Cultural systems in resilience
extinction of this motivational system (Seligman &
he rituals, routines, values, and practices of religions
Beagley, 1975; Zeanah et al., 2006). Interventions
and other cultural systems that have evolved around
that provide opportunities for children and youth to
the world appear to play many roles in potential sup-
experience success, or to help or lead others in recovery
port of resilience. he great religions of the world,
eforts ater disaster, may have positive efects on the
through their values, rituals, and practices, appear to
mastery motivation system.
engage and nurture many of the fundamental protect-
ive systems for individual resilience. hese include
Self-regulation systems attachment relationships (both human and spiritual),
emotion or arousal regulation (e.g., through rules for
Efective responses to challenges oten require the cap-
acity to regulate and coordinate one’s own behavior in living, prayer, or meditation practices, and rituals for
major life passages), and they also imbue life with mean-
the interest of adaptive goals, including the regulation
of attention, arousal, emotions, and impulses. Self- ing and value (Crawford et al., 2006). Cultural groups
based on ethnicity or nationality also have developed
regulation skills have been widely implicated in resili-
ence studies. In the early history of psychology, the values, advice, wisdom, rituals, and practices to support
adaptive responses to adversity, although culturally
capacity to regulate oneself in the interest of adaptive
behavior was described as a major function of the “ego” based protective systems have been relatively neglected
in the research on individual resilience (Luthar, 2006;
in psychoanalytic theory (Masten et al., 2006a). More
recently, self-regulation has been examined in theories Masten & Wright, 2009). here is growing interest in
adaptive systems embedded in cultural practices that
about executive function and the underlying neural sys-
tems associated with the capacity and skills to control serve to support the development of resilience capacity
in communities, families, and individuals.
and coordinate one’s own attention, thinking, planning,
and actions (Rothbart et al., 2007). Self-regulation sys-
tems develop over time with brain maturation (particu- Other adaptive systems
larly as the prefrontal cortex develops), socialization, Many other adaptive systems have evolved in human
and experience. Self-regulatory skills are widely viewed life, ranging from schools to emergency services,
as important tools for successful school entry in young and it is not feasible to consider all the possibil-
children (Blair, 2002), and interventions to improve ities here. Efective schools and good teachers have
these skills with high-risk children show very prom- been widely implicated as protective for high-risk
ising efects (Diamond et al., 2007). Evidence supports children. Schools provide an important context
the developmental hypothesis that parents play an for promoting the capacity for resilience in chil-
important role in the development of self-regulation, dren because schools are charged with educational
including their functioning as co-regulators of infant goals that enhance the development of fundamen-
and child behavior until children develop the capabil- tal protective systems such as problem solving and
ities for self-regulation (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Brody self-regulation systems (Masten et al., 2008; Masten
et al., 2002; Eisenberg et al., 2005). As noted above, & Motti-Stefanidi, 2009). Schools also aford many
attachment relationships, and particularly parent– opportunities for the development of protective
child bonds in childhood, serve many regulatory func- relationships and mastery motivation. he Strength-
tions in child development, helping children to learn Based School Counseling movement (Galassi &
impulse control and modulate their emotions. Later, Akos, 2007) has the goal of enhancing the role of
peers inluence self-regulation as they support or dis- schools in building resilience capacity. Additionally,
courage risk-taking behaviors, thrill seeking, or self- because children spend so much time in school,
control (Steinberg, 2008). here appears to be integral members of staf must be viewed as irst responders
connections between self-regulation and social regu- in disaster planning. Social welfare systems also play
lation over the life course. Similarly, institutions, reli- a large role in the lives of some children, and some of
gions, and other cultural practices also inluence the these systems now actively have the goal of promoting

113
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

resilience in human development (see Flynn et al., methods: consider strategies that reduce risk, boost
2006). here also is growing discussion of building resources, mobilize/protect fundamental adaptive
resilient communities (Norris et al., 2008), along with systems, and generate positive cascades
increasing interest in a more integrated approach to multilevel approaches: consider multiple systems that
resilience that brings together knowledge from bio- may inluence human resilience.
logical, social, communication, organizational, and
hese features relect lessons gleaned from the study of
large-scale ecosystem sciences to address problems
resilience that can be applied to practice or programs
that cut across traditional disciplinary boundaries,
aimed at promoting competence, preventing psycho-
such as preparing for large-scale natural or human-
pathology, or planning for disaster response (Masten &
generated disasters (Masten & Obradović, 2008;
Powell, 2003; Masten, 2006b; Masten & Wright, 2009).
Longstaf, 2009).
Each of these features also is enhanced by an under-
standing of development.
A resilience framework for practice
and policy Mission
he study of resilience always had the ultimate goal of At the heart of a resilience-oriented approach is the
helping people faced with adversities and challenges concept of promoting positive outcomes. Positive goals
that had the potential to undermine healthy func- and achievements are not only appealing to stakehold-
tion and development. As knowledge about resili- ers and congruent with reward systems (e.g., mastery
ence expanded, the complexity and enormity of the motivation) but also marking progress on such goals
scientiic quest became more apparent. Clearly, there may improve morale, build self-eicacy of the young
remains much research ahead to understand the pro- people and those implementing the program, and
cesses that result in resilience. Yet there are children document progress that might be missed with a sin-
and adults who are sufering now in the wake of disas- gular focus on symptoms or problems. Problems and
ters and other kinds of adversity. hey cannot wait for sufering are not ignored, but the goals are deined in
a more complete explication of resilience. herefore, it positive terms of promoting or supporting health, com-
is reasonable to consider an evidence-informed, resil- petence, achievements, or other positive objectives.
ience-guided framework for practice and policy, while Ideally, these goals are guided by a solid understanding
acknowledging that further research will undoubt- of developmental pathways, tasks, and progressions
edly improve the knowledge base about how resilience related to the desired outcomes.
works and how to efectively promote resilience.
he ideas, results, and perspectives from research Models
on resilience in children has had a transformative Resilience research has generated new models of
inluence on theories and practices for intervention, behavior and change that place greater emphasis on
shiting the paradigm for intervention away from positive components and processes that had been
deicit-focused models to more positive and strength- overlooked in earlier models of intervention focusing
focused approaches (Cicchetti et al., 2000; Luthar & exclusively on risks, vulnerabilities, symptoms, and
Cicchetti, 2001; Nation et al., 2003; Akos & Galassi, disorders. More inclusive models can generate more
2008; Masten & Wright, 2009). complete measurement strategies and lead to new
theories and strategies for change that can be tested
Components of a resilience in intervention experiments. Prevention science and
programs aimed at positive youth development among
framework: the five Ms high-risk youth have adopted these broader models
he core features of a resilience-oriented approach to in experimental designs that serve to test resilience
intervention can be described in terms of ive “Ms” theory about protective or promotive efects (e.g.,
mission: frame positive goals Beardsley et al., 1997; Forgatch & DeGarmo, 1999;
models: include strengths, promotive and protective Cicchetti et al., 2000; Wolchik et al., 2002; Greenberg,
processes in models of change 2006; Masten A.S. et al., 2006a, 2009a). Prevention
measures: include assessments of positive predictors, and intervention strategies based on developmentally
outcomes, and change informed resilience-oriented models have proven to be

114
Chapter 7: Resilience in children and adolescents

efective not only for promoting competence and resili- here is increasing interest in the possibility of “repro-
ence but also for preventing and treating problems. gramming” cognitive systems to improve executive
Measures and methods of intervention oten parallel function skills, discussed in the concluding section of
models of change and, therefore, the new look of these this chapter. Finally, there is growing attention to stra-
models resulted in diferent strategies of intervention tegic timing of interventions to increase the return on
and assessment. investment and the spread of positive efects, building
on knowledge of development and the potential of cas-
Measures cading consequences of well-timed improvements in
adaptive behavior. Developmental research shows that
When the pioneers began their studies of resilience, they
competence begets competence; this inding explain
faced the challenge of inadequate measures, because
why early intervention focused on developmental task
the emphasis had been on assessing the problem side of
achievement has such lasting and cost-efective conse-
behavior. Resilience research focused greater attention
quences (Heckman, 2006).
on reliable, valid, and developmentally attuned assess-
ment of positive behavior and outcomes (Masten &
Obradović, 2006). As a result, new and improved meth- Multilevel approaches
ods for assessing competence and positive aspects of he nature of dynamic, developing systems and the
development were developed. Nonetheless, there con- evidence on resilience converge on the conclusion that
tinues to be insuicient attention to culturally suitable resilience can be studied and promoted across many
measures of competence over the life course for assess- levels of analysis and also that a multidisciplinary and
ing achievements and progress in development. multiple-level perspective may be necessary to harness
the full potential for change in human development
Methods (Masten, 2007). heoretically, efective changes at one
level or location in interdependent systems will rever-
Resilience models and indings suggested several basic
berate through interactions to inluence other levels and
strategies for resilience promotion and also under-
other systems. he leverage for change may be greater at
scored the important of developmental perspectives
one level or location than another, as well as at one time
on change. Preventing risk factors from occurring or
in development or history than another. he chapters of
lowering cumulative risk exposure was a basic strategy,
this volume attest to the multiple levels and disciplines
and the stunning success of eforts to prevent prema-
of resilience science. hose developing policies and
ture birth and low birth weight in the twentieth cen-
practices to promote resilience in young people at risk
tury showed what risk-focused eforts could achieve.
are beginning to consider the potential of multiple-level
his strategy also relects the recognition of “safety
approaches, whether one is reforming the child welfare
irst” priorities; it does not make sense to build a new
systems (e.g., Flynn et al., 2006) or preparing for disas-
school if there are still landmines in the ground from
ter (e.g., Masten & Obradović, 2008).
war. Targeting risk exposure can take many forms,
ranging from treating depression in new mothers to
averting multiple foster home placements. he second Conclusions and future directions
basic resilience-oriented strategy of boosting resources As the second decade of the century begins, the fourth
or access to resources also takes many forms, from the wave of resilience science is swelling on the horizon and
provision of food and tutors to the building of schools overtaking earlier waves of research (Masten, 2007).
and libraries or adding a case manager. he third strat- he new wave is characterized by integrative goals and
egy is focused on mobilizing or improving fundamen- methodological advances that are opening new oppor-
tal adaptive systems, in an efort to restore or boost tunities for studying the processes of resilience across
the most powerful and universal protective systems an unprecedented range of levels, from the molecular
for positive development and recovery. Again, this to the global. In studies of resilience in human devel-
approach may take many forms, from adding parents opment, there is keen interest in neural plasticity and
or mentors to a child’s life, providing opportunities to the possibility of interventions to alter how the brain
succeed or develop talent, to supporting cultural tra- develops and functions in processing information and
ditions that provide children with adaptive tools and responding to stress (Curtis & Cicchetti, 2003; Cicchetti
opportunities for relationships with prosocial adults. & Curtis, 2006; Gunnar et al., 2006; Lester et al., 2006;

115
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Belsky & Pluess, 2009). he idea that “plasticity” or and a second is planned for 2011. Ecology and social
“sensitivity to context” develops and changes, and may sciences share a focus on resilience in developing,
be shaped by nurture and reshaped by intervention, is living systems.
opening new avenues of theory and empirical investi- In short, there remains much to do. Nonetheless,
gation with profound implications for interventions the compelling regularities in the indings on resilience
to promote resilience (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Belsky & in human development to date are encouraging in two
Pluess, 2009; Obradović & Boyce, 2009). With new ways. First, the indings ofer a strong set of clues about
tools to measure genes and their status (“on” or “of ”), “hot spots” for integration and further study of proc-
investigators are searching for genetic moderators of esses in the fourth wave of research (Masten, 2007).
risk with protective efects, with an eye to future inter- Second, the indings provide guidance for policy mak-
ventions that may facilitate resilience through pre- ers and those charged with helping young people in
venting or promoting gene expression (Kim-Cohen & the near future, while further evidence on resilience
Gold, 2009). accrues. Children and youth cannot wait for all the
At the level of social interaction, new method- data. It is encouraging to know that there are robust
ologies are also making it possible to study social indings in the initial waves of data collection that
experience at the level of neural function (e.g., social point to reasonable strategies for action in promoting
neuroscience studies of peer rejection with brain resilience. It is also reassuring that resilience does not
imaging technology [Masten & Eisenberger, 2009; seem to require rare and extraordinary capabilities,
Masten C.L. et al., 2009b]). In addition, the dynamics but instead depends on common and ordinary human
of dyadic interaction (among peers or parent–child) adaptive systems that develop and change, and can
can be studied through “state-space grids” that depict be nurtured, protected, and restored, with impressive
the reciprocal interaction in relationships based on power for recovery and resilience.
observational coding (Dishion & Piehler, 2007; Lewis,
2000). hrough the use of this and other new tools Acknowledgements
that allow for observation and analysis at the micro- Preparation of this chapter was supported in part by
social level, it is becoming possible to examine in detail the National Science Foundation with a grant to Ann
how parents may scafold the development of self- Masten (No. 0745643) and Graduate Fellowships to
regulation through the co-regulation they provide. It Amy Monn and Laura Supkof. Any opinions, conclu-
is also possible to study how prosocial peers may shape sions, or recommendations expressed in this chapter
their mutual social competence by the positive model- are those of the authors and do not necessarily relect
ing and reinforcement they provide each other. hese the views of the National Science Foundation.
methodological advances that capture social dynamics
are producing new insights into the ways that relation- References
ships inluence the development of competence and
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S.
resilience.
(1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Developmental scientists also are beginning to
connect with investigators who study the behavior of Akos, P. & Galassi, J. P. (2008). Strengths-based school
counseling. Professional School Counseling, Special
much larger systems, such as communities and eco- issue 12.
systems, to tackle common conceptual and methodo-
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and
logical issues and also to address problems like disaster
statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn, text
or pandemics where many systems are involved in the revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
efects or resilience in individuals, families, and com-
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-eicacy:he exercise of control.
munities (Masten & Obradović, 2008). Integrating New York: Freeman.
resilience science across scale and disciplines is just at
Beardsley, W. R., Salt, P., Versage, E. M., et al. (1997).
early stages. Disciplinary “cross-talk” has the poten-
Sustained change in parents receiving preventive
tial to bring innovative ideas and methods across interventions for families with depression. American
ields to accelerate progress. he irst international Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 510–515.
conference on resilience science with a broad, inte- Belsky, J. & Pluess (2009). he nature (and nurture?) of
grative agenda across disciplines and scale was held plasticity in early human development. Perspectives on
in 2008 in Stockholm (known as “Resilience2008”) Psychological Science, 4, 345–351.

116
Chapter 7: Resilience in children and adolescents

Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave
emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of child longitudinal study. Child Development, 76, 1055–1071.
functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, Flynn, R. J., Dudding, P. M., & Barber, J. G. (eds.) (2006).
111–127. Promoting resilience in development: A general framework
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: for systems of care. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press.
Have we under-estimated the human capacity to thrive Forgatch, M. S. & DeGarmo, D. S. (1999). Parenting through
ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, change: An efective prevention program for single
59, 20–28. mothers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. New York: Basic Books. 67, 711–724.
Boyce, W. T. & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to Galassi, J. P. & Akos, P. (2007). Strength-based school
context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the counseling: Promoting student development and
origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development achievement. Mahway, NJ: Earlbaum.
and Psychopathology, 17, 271–301.
Garmezy, N. (1985). Stress-resistant children: he search for
Brody, G. H., Murry, V. M., Kim, S., & Brown, A. C. (2002). protective factors. In J. E. Stevenson (ed.), Recent research
Longitudinal pathways to competence and psychological in developmental psychopathology (pp. 213–233). [Journal
adjustment among African-American children living in of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Book Supplement 4.]
rural single-parent households. Child Development, 73, Oxford: Pergamon Press.
1505–1516.
Garmezy, N. & Streitman, S. (1974). Children at risk: the
Cicchetti, D. (1984). he emergence of developmental search for the antecedents to schizophrenia: Part 1.
psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1–7. Conceptual models and research methods. Schizophrenia
Cicchetti, D. (2006). Development and psychopathology. Bulletin, 8, 14–90.
In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (eds.), Developmental Greenberg, M. T. (2006). Promoting resilience in children
psychopathology, Vol. 1: heory and method, 2nd edn (pp. and youth: Preventive interventions and their interface
1–23). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. with neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of
Cicchetti, D. & Curtis, W. J. (2006). he developing Sciences, 1094, 139–150.
brain ad neural plasticity: Implications for normality, Gunnar, M. R. & Fisher, P. A. for the he Early Experience,
psychopathology, and resilience. In D. Cicchetti & Stress, and Prevention Network (2006). Bringing basic
D. Cohen (eds.), Developmental psychopathology, research on early experience and stress neurobiology
Vol. 2: Developmental neuroscience, 2nd edn (pp. 1–64). to bear on preventive interventions for neglected and
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology,
Cicchetti, D., Rappaport, J., Sandler, I., & Weissberg, R. P. 18, 651–677.
(eds.) (2000). he promotion of wellness in children and Havighurst, R. J. (1972). Developmental tasks and education,
adolescents. Washington, DC: CWLA Press. 3rd edn. New York: McKay.
Crawford, E., Wright, M. O. D., & Masten, A. S. (2006). Heckman, J. J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics
Resilience and spirituality in youth. In P. L. Benson, of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312,
E. C. Roehlkepartain, P. E. King & L. Wagener (eds.), 1900–1902.
he handbook of spiritual development in childhood and
adolescence (pp. 355–370). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Kim-Cohen, J. & Gold, A. L. (2009). Measured gene–
environment interactions and mechanisms promoting
Curtis, J. & Cicchetti, D. (2003). Moving resilience on resilient development. Current Directions in Psychological
resilience into the 21st century: heoretical and Science, 18, 138–142.
methodological considerations in examining the
biological contributors to resilience. Development and Lester, B. M., Masten, A., & McEwen, B. (eds.) (2006).
Psychopathology, 15, 773–810. Resilience in children. Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences, 1094, 139–150.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., homas, J. & Munro, S. (2007).
Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, Lewis, M. D. (2000). he promise of dynamic systems
318, 1387–1388. approaches for an integrated account of human
development. Child Development, 71, 36–43.
Dishion, T. J. & Piehler, T. F. (2007). Peer dynamics in
the development and change of child and adolescent Longstaf, P. H. 2009. Managing surprises in complex systems:
problem behavior. In A. S. Masten (ed.), Multilevel Multidisciplinary perspectives on resilience. Ecology and
dynamics in developmental psychopathology: Pathways to Society, 14, 49, http://www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol14/
the future (pp. 151–180). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. iss1/art49/ (accessed February 24, 2011).
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., et al. (2005). Luthar, S. S. (2006). Resilience in development: A synthesis
Relations among positive parenting, children’s efortful of research across ive decades. In D. Cicchetti &

117
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

D. J. Cohen (eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. resilience. In A. Clarke-Stewart & J. Dunn (eds.), Families
3: Risk, disorder, and adaptation, 2nd edn (pp. 739–795). count: Efects on child and adolescent development (pp.
New York: Wiley. 5–25). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Luthar, S. S. & Cicchetti, D. (2001). he construct of Masten, A. S. & Wright, M. O’D. (2009). Resilience over
resilience: Implications for interventions and social the lifespan: Developmental perspectives on resistance,
policies. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 857–885. recovery, and transformation. In J. W. Reich, A. J. Zautra,
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes & J. S. Hall (eds.), Handbook of adult resilience (pp. 213–
in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. 237). New York: Guilford Press.
Masten, A. S. (2006a). Developmental psychopathology: Masten, A. S., Hubbard, J. J., Gest, S. D., et al. (1999).
Pathways to the future. International Journal of Competence in the context of adversity: Pathways to
Behavioral Development, 31, 47–54. resilience and maladaptation from childhood to late
adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 11,
Masten, A. S. (2006b). Promoting resilience in development: 143–169.
A general framework for systems of care. In R. J. Flynn,
P. Dudding, & J. G. Barber (eds.), Promoting resilience in Masten, A. S., Roisman, G. I., Long, J. D., et al. (2005).
child welfare (pp. 3–17). Ottawa: University of Ottawa Developmental cascades: Linking academic
Press. achievement, externalizing and internalizing symptoms
over 20 years. Developmental Psychology, 41, 733–746.
Masten, A. S. (2007). Resilience in developing systems:
Progress and promise as the fourth wave rises. Masten, A. S., Burt, K. B., & Coatsworth, J. D. (2006a).
Development and Psychopathology, 19, 921–930. Competence and psychopathology in development.
In D. Ciccheti & D. J. Cohen (eds.), Developmental
Masten, A. S. (2011). Risk and resilience in development.
psychopathology, Vol. 3: Risk, disorder and adaptation,
In P. D. Zelazo (ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental
2nd edn (pp. 696–738). New York: Wiley.
psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, in press.
Masten, A. S., Obradović, J., & Burt, K. (2006b). Resilience
Masten, A. S. & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Editorial:
in emerging adulthood: Developmental perspectives
Developmental cascades. Development and
on continuity and transformation. In J. J. Arnett &
Psychopathology, 22, 491–495.
J. L. Tanner (eds.), Emerging adults in America:
Masten, A. S. & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). he development Coming of age in the 21st century (pp. 173–190).
of competence in favorable and unfavorable Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
environments: Lessons from successful children. Press.
American Psychologist, 53, 205–220.
Masten, A. S., Herbers, J. E., Cutuli, J. J., & Lafavor, T. L.
Masten, A. S. & Motti-Stefanidi, F. (2009). Understanding (2008). Promoting competence and resilience in the
and promoting resilience in children: Promotive and school context. Professional School Counseling, 12,
protective processes in schools. In T. B. Gutkin & 76–84.
C. R. Reynolds (eds.), he handbook of school psychology,
Masten, A. S., Cutuli, J. J., Herbers, J. E., & Gabrielle-Reed,
4th edn (pp. 721–738). New York: Wiley.
M. J. (2009a). Resilience in development. In C. R. Snyder,
Masten, A. S. & Obradović, J. (2006). Competence and & S. J. Lopez (eds.), he handbook of positive psychology,
resilience in development. Annals of the New York 2nd edn (pp. 117–131). New York: Oxford University
Academy of Sciences, 1094, 13–27. Press.
Masten, A. S. & Obradović, J. (2008). Disaster preparation Masten, C. L. & Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Exploring the
and recovery: Lessons from research on resilience in pain of social rejection in adults and adolescents: A
human development. Ecology and Society, 13, 9, http:// social cognitive neuroscience perspective. In M. Harris
www.ecologyandsociety.org/vol13/iss1/art9/ (accessed (ed.), Bullying, rejection, and peer victimization: A social
February 26, 2011). cognitive neuroscience perspective (pp. 53–78). New York:
Masten, A. S. & Osofsky, J. (2010). Disasters and the impact Springer.
on child development: Introduction to the special Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., et al.
section. Child Development, 81, 1029–1039. (2009b). Neural correlates of social exclusion during
Masten, A. S. & Powell, J. L. (2003). A resilience framework adolescence: Understanding the distress of peer
for research, policy, and practice. In S. S. Luthar (ed.), rejection. Social Cognitive and Afective Neuroscience, 4,
Resilience and vulnerabilities: Adaptation in the context of 143–157.
childhood adversities (pp. 1–25). New York: Cambridge Miller, G. E., Brehm, K., & Whitehouse, S. (1998).
University Press. Reconceptualizing school-based prevention for
Masten, A. S. & Shafer, A. (2006). How families matter in antisocial behavior within a resiliency framework. School
child development: Relections from research on risk and Psychology Review, 27, 364–379.

118
Chapter 7: Resilience in children and adolescents

Nation, M., Crusto, C., Wandersman, A., et al. (2003). What development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55,
works in prevention: Principles of efective prevention 68–78.
programs. American Psychologist, 58, 449–456. Seligman, M. E. P. & Beagley, G. (1975). Learned
Norris, F. H., Stevens, S. P., Pfeferbaum, B., Wyche, K. F., & helplessness in the rat. Journal of Comparative and
Pfeferbaum, R. L. (2008). Community resilience as Physiological Psychology, 88, 534–541.
a metaphor, theory, set of capacities and strategy for
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A.
disaster readiness. American Journal of Community
(2005). he development of the person: he Minnesota
Psychology, 41, 127–150.
Study of Risk and Adaptation from birth to adulthood.
Obradović, J. & Boyce, W. T. (2009). Individual diferences New York: Guilford Press.
in behavioral, physiological, and genetic sensitivities to
contexts: Implications for development and adaptation. Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective
Developmental Neuroscience, 31, 300–308. on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28,
78–106.
Obradović, J., Shafer, A., & Masten, A. S. (2011). Risk
in developmental psychopathology: Progress and Watt, N. F., Anthony, E. J., Wynne, L. C., & Rolf, J. E. (eds.)
future directions. In L. C. Mayes & M. Lewis (eds.), he (1984). Children at risk for schizophrenia: A longitudinal
environment of human development: A handbook of theory perspective. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
and measurement. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Press, in press. Werner, E. E. & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds:
Patterson, G. R., DeGarmo, D. S., & Forgatch, M. S. (2004). High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY:
Systematic changes in families following prevention Cornell University Press.
trials. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 621–633. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: he concept
Pine, D. S., Costello, J., & Masten, A. S. (2005). Trauma, of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.
proximity, and developmental psychopathology: Wolchik, S. A., Sandler, I. N., Millsap, R. E., et al. (2002).
he efects of war and terrorism on children. Six-year follow-up of preventive interventions for children
Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 1781–1792. of divorce: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the
Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Posner, M. I. (2007). American Medical Association, 288, 1874–1881.
Executive attention and efortful control: Linking Zeanah, C. H., Smyke, A. T., & Settles, L. D. (2006).
temperament, brain networks, and genes. Child Orphanages as a developmental context for early
Development Perspectives, 1, 2–7. childhood. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (eds.),
Ryan, R. M. & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory Blackwell handbook of early childhood development
and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social (pp. 424–454). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

119
Section
Section2 Resilience across the lifespan
Chapter
Toward a lifespan approach to

8 resilience and potential trauma


George A. Bonanno and Anthony D. Mancini

Introduction little evidence of more than transient disruptions in


functioning.
As much as we might wish it otherwise, bad things
his chapter briely traces the history of the con-
happen: war, natural disaster, the death of close
struct of psychological trauma and then describes
friends and relatives, serious accidents, senseless
recent empirical research on individual variation. he
abuse or violence at the hand of others, and so on.
most common or prototypical outcome trajectories that
Any of these things can and all too oten do happen,
have been elucidated in recent longitudinal research on
and at every stage of life. Epidemiological data indi-
PTEs are considered and the growing evidence for the
cate that most adults experience at least one and usu-
human capacity to thrive even ater the most diicult
ally several potentially traumatic events (PTE) during
of experiences is addressed. he chapter then moves
the course of their lives (Norris, 1992; Kessler et al.,
on to enumerate both individual diferences and con-
1995; Breslau et al., 2000), and that most children are
textual factors that have been associated with a resilient
also exposed to such experiences (Copeland et al.,
outcome among children and adults. his capacity is
2007). It is important to note, however, that life event
examined from a lifespan perspective, situating resili-
research typically relies on retrospective accounts,
ence in the context of developmental factors. Finally,
which more than likely underestimate the frequency
the implications of resilience across the lifespan are
of PTEs. Indeed, a recent study that measured life
consider for therapeutic intervention .
events among college students over a four-year period
using a weekly internet survey reported an average of Historical conceptions of
six PTEs per student (Lalande & Bonanno, 2011).
Perhaps because acutely aversive events are so psychological trauma
dreaded, both clinicians and the lay public tend to Practically since the beginnings of psychology and
assume that they will almost always result in lasting psychiatry as formal disciplines, researchers, theo-
emotional damage. he available evidence, however, rists, and practitioners have looked to violent or life-
suggests a more complex and far more encouraging threatening events as antecedents to psychological
picture. To emphasize the pronounced individual and physiological dysfunction (Ellenberger, 1970;
diferences in the way people react to adversity, we Lamprecht & Sack, 2002). However, the primary
emphasize that such events are only “potentially trau- source of dysfunction, even in the case of extreme
matic” (Norris, 1992; Bonanno, 2004), for the simple stressor events, was generally situated within the indi-
reason that not everyone experiences them as trau- vidual, as for example in Kardiner’s (1941) concept of
matic. Most people in fact cope with PTEs remark- traumatic neuroses. he great wars of the twentieth cen-
ably well (Bonanno, 2004, 2005; Bonanno & Mancini, tury, and in particular the global conlicts surround-
2008). Although some do, in fact, endure lasting emo- ing World War II, brought increasing awareness of the
tional diiculties, the vast majority of people exposed caustic impact of extreme stress on human functioning
to extreme adversity recover a semblance of their nor- (Keegan, 1976). Nevertheless, it was not until late in
mal level of functioning within several months to sev- the twentieth century that consensus began to emerge
eral years ater the event, and many if not most show that extremely aversive events by themselves could be

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

120
Chapter 8: A lifespan approach to resilience and trauma

the primary source of trauma-related dysfunction. In et al., 1984, p. 18). In similar fashion, Bowlby (1980,
1980, the American Psychiatric Association (1980) irst p. 138) considered the “prolonged absence of conscious
formalized post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) as a grieving” as a type of disordered mourning, while,
legitimate diagnostic category, illing an enormous gap according to Rando (1993, p. 158), bereaved individ-
in public health knowledge about the impact of trauma. uals who do not experience intense distress following
At the same time, the PTSD category also helped to a loss are in a state of denial and have “a powerful abil-
consolidate and promote a surge of new research on ity to block out reality.” Indeed, the failure to grieve in
traumatic stress (McNally, 2003). As a result, our under- response to bereavement was thought to relect under-
standing of the neurobiology, etiology, and treatment lying psychopathology, because it suggested that the
of PTSD has advanced considerably (Foa & Rothbaum, person is inhibiting or dissociating from negative feel-
1998; Ozer et al., 2003; Dalgleish, 2004). ings (Middleton et al., 1993) or lacked suicient attach-
he advances generated by the nearly exclusive ment to the deceased (Fraley & Shaver, 1999). Bereaved
emphasis on PTSD have, however, come at a cost. When people who failed to display overt grief or distress
viewed from the perspective of the lay and professional were presumed to be avoiding the “tasks” of grieving
literatures, this emphasis has tended to obscure the (Worden, 1991; Rando, 1993) and were commonly
diversity in people’s responses to PTEs. Indeed, the expected to have more severe diiculties or otherwise
impact of PTEs is still commonly seen in relatively delayed reactions.
simplistic terms as a binary distinction between path-
ology versus the absence of pathology. his simpliied Trajectories of individual
view has resonated, with lingering controversies about differences following potentially
the sometimes elusive distinction between genuine
psychological trauma and malingering, an issue with traumatic events
particular relevance to war-related PTSD. hroughout he discrepancy between clinical/cultural expecta-
the twentieth century, warfare had been plagued by an tions and the empirical reality of how people behave
enduring tension about the proper time and place for in the atermath of acute adversity can be attributed at
diagnosis or treatment (Shepard, 2001; Lamprecht & least in part to misunderstandings about the nature of
Sack, 2002). During the most recent Iraq war, these the underlying variability in change across time. Most
issues once again lared into the open. For example, traditional approaches to trauma, including the binary
one recent study of returning US Army and Marine distinction between pathology versus the absence of
Corps soldiers involved in combat operations in Iraq pathology, are couched in the broader assumption that
and Afghanistan reported that many soldiers wanted aversive life events produce a single homogeneous dis-
but did not seek treatment out of fear of the stigma still tribution of change over time (Muthén, 2004; Duncan
associated with treatment for psychological diiculties et al., 2006). Indeed, even non-categorical approaches
in the armed forces (Hoge et al., 2004). to trauma, such as those that assess variability in the
his same type of oversimpliication has plagued average level of functioning over time, have continued
our understanding of bereavement, where there has to embody the assumption that the underlying distri-
long been confusion over distinctions between grief, bution is homogeneous.
healthy functioning, and denial. As was the case for In stark contrast to this approach, recent con-
psychological trauma more broadly, bereavement ceptual (Bonanno, 2004; Mancini et al., 2011a) and
researchers and theorists have tended to view acute statistical (Curran & Hussong, 2003; Muthén, 2004;
and chronic grief reactions as normative while ofer- Jung & Wickrama, 2008) advances have dramatically
ing little insight about possible resilience to loss. his underscored the natural heterogeneity of human stress
emphasis on dysfunction has perhaps grown out of response. Empirical studies using this approach indi-
wider cultural assumptions about the appropriate way cate that most of the variability in response to PTEs can
to grieve (Archer, 2001; Bonanno et al., 2005a). For be captured by four prototypical trajectories of adjust-
example, a report summarizing the views of bereave- ment across time: chronic dysfunction, gradual recov-
ment experts in the 1980s stated that it was commonly ery, delayed reactions, and a relatively stable trajectory
assumed, particularly by clinicians, “that the absence of healthy functioning or resilience (Bonanno, 2004).
of grieving phenomena following bereavement repre- hese trajectories are illustrated in Figure 8.1 and elab-
sents some form of personality pathology”(Osterweis orated below.

121
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Figure 8.1 Prototypical patterns of


disruption in normal functioning across
time following potentially traumatic
events (PTE). (Adapted with permission
severe

Chronic from Bonanno, 2004.).


Disruptions in normal functioning

moderate

Delayed
mild

Recovery

Resilience

PTE 1 year 2 years


Time since event

he initial research in this area used relatively Chronic dysfunction


basic mathematical algorithms based on means and
It is well established that only a relatively small sub-
standard variation to assess individual diferences in
the trajectory of adjustment across time. Despite the set of those exposed to PTEs will eventually develop
simplicity of this approach, the same basic longitu- chronic pathological reactions. Post-traumatic stress
dinal and prospective trajectories have been identiied disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000)
across studies in response to bereavement (Bonanno is typically observed, for example, in only 5–10% of
et al., 2002a, 2005b), disaster (Bonanno et al., 2005c), exposed individuals (Kessler et al., 1995). he propor-
and radiation treatment for breast cancer (Deshields tion of any sample will vary, of course, depending on
et al., 2006). More recent studies have beneited from the type and degree of exposure to PTEs. In exposure
advances in latent growth mixture modeling (LGMM; to exceptionally acute or corrosive events, the propor-
Muthén, 2004), a sophisticated statistical approach tion of full-blown PTSD reactions may reach higher
that is uniquely suited to identifying multiple unob- levels, sometimes as high as one third of the sample.
served trajectories in the data. he LGMM approach For example, a survey of a population obtained by
has been applied to a wide variety of longitudinal random-digit dialing one month ater the September
phenomena, including drinking among college stu- 11 2001 (9/11) terrorist attack in New York City esti-
dents (Greenbaum et al., 2005), childhood aggression mated that 7.5% of Manhattan residents met criteria
(Schaefer et al., 2003, 2006), acclimation to retirement for PTSD and another 17.4% subsyndromal PTSD
in late life (Pinquart & Schindler, 2007), and develop- (Galea et al., 2002). However, among those physically
mental learning trajectories (Boscardin et al., 2008). injured in the attack, PTSD prevalence rose to 26%
he LGMM has also now been applied to the study (Bonanno et al., 2006). In another study reanalyzing
of variability following PTEs, and has identiied the a representative sample of 1200 Vietnam veterans,
same basic longitudinal trajectories in the atermath chronic PTSD was estimated at 9% overall but among
of disease epidemic (Bonanno et al., 2008), bereave- veterans with the highest levels of combat exposure
ment (Mancini et al., 2011a), and traumatic injury the prevalence estimate rose to 28% (Dohrenwend
(DeRoon-Cassini et al., 2010). et al., 2006).

122
Chapter 8: A lifespan approach to resilience and trauma

In studies of bereavement, the proportion of the to a PTE was rare and would occur only in people with
individuals displaying more severe and chronic grief, exceptional emotional strength (Casella & Motta,
or complicated grief, exhibits a similar bandwidth. 1990; McFarlance & Yehuda 1996; Tucker et al., 2002).
Generally around 10% to 15% of those in bereaved And as noted above, bereavement theorists have per-
samples will evidence complicated grief reactions sistently regarded the relative absence of grief as a form
(Bonanno & Kaltman, 2001). As was the case for PTEs of denial or hidden psychopathology (Middleton et al.,
in general, however, complicated grief tends to be more 1993). here is now compelling evidence, however,
prevalent following more extreme losses, such as when that a genuine and enduring resilience is not rare but
the loss was from a violent cause (Zisook et al., 1998; common and neither a sign of exceptional strength
Kaltman & Bonanno, 2003) or when the lost loved one nor of psychopathology, but rather a fundamental fea-
was a child (Bonanno et al., 2005a). ture of normal coping skills evident in both children
here are relatively fewer data available on trauma and adults (Masten, 2001; Bonanno, 2004). Moreover,
and grief reactions among children. However, the although resilience and recovery are sometimes con-
data that are available suggest similar levels of com- lated, the available longitudinal evidence, such as the
plicated grief in children (Christ, 2000) but somewhat LGMM analyses mentioned above, clearly demonstrate
less PTSD in children than in adults (Copeland et al., that resilience and recovery can be mapped as dis-
2007). As will be discussed in greater detail below, it is crete and empirically separable outcome trajectories.
important to keep in mind that the question of adjust- Distinctions between resilience and recovery have been
ment is in many ways more complex among children. identiied, for example, following loss (Bonanno et al.,
For example, children exposed to aversive events may 2002a), treatment for neoplastic disease (Deshields
fail to evidence PTSD or complicated grief but may et al., 2006), disease epidemic (Bonanno et al., 2008),
manifest decrements in other areas, such as increased terrorist attack (Bonanno et al., 2005c), and traumatic
behavioral problems, substance use, academic prob- injury (DeRoon-Cassini et al., 2010).
lems, or peer conlict .
Development of the construct of
Delayed reactions psychological resilience
As mentioned above, one of the axiomatic assumptions Much of the original theorizing on resilience can be
in the bereavement literature has been that the absence traced to developmental psychologists and psychia-
of overt signs of grieving will eventually manifest as trists during the 1970s. hese pioneering researchers
delayed grief reactions (Deutsch, 1937; Bowlby, 1980; documented the large number of children who despite
Parkes & Weiss, 1980; Osterweis et al., 1984; Rando, growing up in caustic socioeconomic circumstances
1993; Sanders, 1993). Despite the prevalence of this (e.g., poverty, chronic abuse) nonetheless tended
belief, solid empirical evidence for delayed grief has yet to reach normal healthy developmental milestones
to be produced (Wortman & Silver, 1989; Bonanno & (Murphy & Moriarty, 1976; Rutter, 1979; Garmezy,
Kaltman, 1999), even in longitudinal studies explicitly 1991; Werner, 1995). A surprising feature of this work
designed to capture the phenomenon (Middleton et al., was that it showed resilience in children at risk to be
1996; Bonanno & Field, 2001). By contrast, delayed common (Masten, 2001). Whereas traditional deicit-
PTSD reactions following PTEs have been detected, usu- focused models of development had assumed that only
ally at low frequencies (e.g., Buckley et al., 1996; Bonanno children with remarkable coping ability could thrive
et al., 2005c). he way delayed PTSD manifests, however, in such adverse contexts, a growing body of evidence
does not necessarily conform to the traditional idea of began to suggest that resilience is a result of normal
denied symptoms turning into full-blown pathology. human adaptational mechanisms (Masten, 2001). As
Rather, delayed PTSD reactions are more accurately noted above, however, most of this research focused on
characterized as subthreshold PTSD that tends to worsen enduring aversive contexts, rather than isolated PTEs.
over time (Buckley et al., 1996; Brewin et al., 2008). Although at the time of these studies the construct
of resilience had not yet “trickled up” to the adult lit-
Resilience and recovery erature, the adult literature had periodically included
Until recently, it was widely assumed that the endur- reports of widespread resilience in the atermath of
ing absence of trauma symptoms following exposure isolated PTEs (e.g., Janis, 1951; Rachman, 1978). More

123
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

recently, however, as the resilience construct has gained individuals may have diiculty sleeping, or experience
currency among trauma researchers, the diferences intrusive thoughts or memories of the event for sev-
between resilient outcomes in adults and children eral days or even weeks, but most can still manage to
have begun to become apparent (Bonanno, 2004, 2005; adequately perform the tasks of their daily life or car-
Bonanno & Mancini, 2008). Some of the key difer- eer, or care for others. his is not to say, of course, that
ences seem to hinge on the temporal and sociocontex- people showing resilient outcomes were not upset, dis-
tual characteristics of stress and adaptation at diferent turbed, or unhappy about the occurrence of the event.
points in the lifespan. For developing children, the Our point is merely that, as undesirable as PTEs might
deinition of healthy adaptation is a complex issue be, many people cope with such events extremely well
(Luthar et al., 2000; Masten, 2001). For example, chil- and are able to continue meeting the normal daily
dren at risk may evidence competence in one domain demands of their lives.
but fail to meet long-term developmental challenges
in other domains (Luthar et al.,1993). By contrast, for
adults exposed to a PTE this situation is arguably more Resilience across the lifespan
straightforward (Bonanno, 2004, 2005). Despite the conceptual diferences and similarities in
Most but certainly not all of the PTEs adults might the concept of resilience across the lifespan, there are
be confronted with can be classiied as isolated stres- surprisingly few comparative data on trauma and grief
sor events (e.g., an automobile accident) that occur reactions among children and adults. Nonetheless,
in a broader context of otherwise normative (i.e., low although there is variability across studies, the avail-
stress) circumstances. here may be concomitant stres- able data on the whole suggest that children cope as
sors accompanying or extending the PTE (e.g., endur- well if not better than adults. A study of children in the
ing health problems or change in inancial situation), atermath of Hurricane Andrew, a category 4 storm,
but this level of variability can typically be measured observed elevated PTSD but at rates not dramatically
with a reasonable degree of reliability (e.g., Bonanno dissimilar to those observed in adults: 39% of the chil-
et al., 2005b, 2007). Finally, because developmental dren met PTSD criteria one month ater the storm.
considerations are less pronounced in adults, responses Chronic PTSD measured 10 months ater the storm
to PTEs can usually be assessed in terms of deviation was observed in 18% of the children (La Greca et al.,
from or return to normative (baseline) functioning 1996). Although this study did not report data on resili-
(Carver, 1998) rather than in terms of more abstract ence, it can be surmised that approximately 44% of the
developmental milestones. children from the same sample evidenced minimal or a
Based on these considerations, Bonanno (2004, complete absence of PTSD symptoms at three months,
pp. 20–21) deined resilience in adults as the ability of suggesting that a substantial proportion most likely
individuals “in otherwise normal circumstances who showed resilience. Consistent with the adult litera-
are exposed to an isolated and potentially highly dis- ture, the most robust predictor of PTSD scores at three
ruptive event such as the death of a close relation or a months was an exposure variable, accounting for 15%
violent or life-threatening situation to maintain rela- of the variance. hese indings would seem to suggest
tively stable, healthy levels of psychological and phys- that children, when compared with adults, are equally
ical functioning … as well as the capacity for generative and perhaps more vulnerable to PTSD. However, this
experiences and positive emotions.” By contrast, the study used a self-report measure that does not map
more traditional recovery pathway is characterized by directly onto a PTSD diagnosis. he researchers also
readily observable elevations in psychological symp- employed a convenience sample.
toms that endure for at least several months before By contrast, studies using representative sam-
gradually returning to baseline, pre-trauma levels. ples and diagnostic interviews have suggested PTSD
A key point is that even resilient individuals may prevalence rates for children that are equal to if not
experience at least some form of transient stress reac- substantially lower than adults. For example, another
tion. However, these reactions are usually mild to investigation of Hurricane Andrew found that among
moderate in degree, relatively short term, and do not children aged 12 to 17 years, only 2.9% of males and
signiicantly interfere with their ability to continue 9.0% of females met criteria for a PTSD diagnosis
functioning (Bonanno et al., 2002a, 2005b; Bisconti (Garrison et al., 1995). Interestingly, increased age was
et al., 2006; Ong et al., 2006). For example, resilient associated with higher rates of PTSD, underlining the

124
Chapter 8: A lifespan approach to resilience and trauma

role of developmental factors. A similar prevalence reactions among New Yorkers to the 9/11 attacks indi-
of PTSD was observed (4.5%) in a study of child and cated a nearly linear increase in resilience from younger
adolescent survivors of the 1999 earthquake in Ano to older adults (Bonanno et al., 2007). Moreover, in a
Liosia, Greece (Roussos et al., 2005). More recently, full regression model that controlled for demographic
an epidemiological investigation of PTSD among and other predictor variables, adults 65 years of age or
children and young adults aged 9 to 16 years found older were over three times as likely as young adults to
even lower prevalence rates using a diagnostic inter- be resilient ater the 9/11 attacks. By contrast, a study
view measure and a representative probability sam- of long-term adjustment of Hong Kong residents who
ple (Copeland et al., 2007). In a longitudinal design had been hospitalized for severe acute respiratory syn-
spanning eight years and encompassing exposure to drome (SARS) failed to ind a link between resilience
a variety of potentially extreme stressors (violence, and age. It is not clear, however, whether the null efects
interpersonal loss, sexual trauma), remarkably, less of age relected the nature of the stressor, or possible
than 0.5% of children in the sample met full criteria cultural variation.
for the PTSD diagnosis. One type of stressor that has consistently shown
When considered together, these indings suggest, an age-related coping advantage is bereavement.
albeit preliminarily, that children cope extremely well Research on grief reactions in older adults indicate that
with isolated PTEs. It remains to be determined, how- they are generally less intense than those of younger
ever, whether the phenomenology of PTSD in children adults (Sherbourne et al., 1992; Sanders, 1993; Nolen-
is diferent from that in adults (Perrin et al., 2000). In Hoeksema & Ahrens, 2002); one obvious reason is
addition, as noted above, the question of adjustment in that the loss may be viewed by older adults as a more
children is in many ways more complex than among normative, and hence less disturbing, consequence of
adults owing to the developmental issues involved. the later stages of life (Neugarten, 1979). Indeed, other
hese conclusions, therefore, should be tempered with common late-life stressors, such as disability, appear
caution. to have more long-lasting and deleterious efects than
However, from the other end of the developmental bereavement (Reich et al., 1989).
continuum, older adulthood, we also ind evidence of An additional factor is that older bereaved persons
enhanced resilience. Again, while there is some vari- are more likely to have provided high levels of care to a
ability across studies, the available evidence generally disabled spouse, resulting in increased caregiver strain
suggests that age bufers against harmful reactions to but perhaps the opportunity for relief during bereave-
potential trauma. Phifer (1990) reported, for example, ment (Bonanno et al., 2002a). Research by Schulz et al.
that ater high exposure to a severe lood older adults (2001) has shown, in fact, that elderly bereaved people
were less vulnerable to psychological distress than who experienced stressful caregiving reported declines
were middle-aged adults with similar levels of expos- in psychological distress ater the spouse had died.
ure. Other studies have also suggested that older adults However, spousal illness by itself, even in the absence
experience fewer symptoms than younger adults in of caregiver strain, may be enough to produce this pat-
response to various natural disasters, including a tor- tern. In the Bonanno et al. (2002a) prospective study
nado (Bell, 1978) and the Bufalo Creek dam collapse of conjugally bereaved older adults, mentioned above,
(Green et al., 1990). In their meta-analysis of PTSD risk 10% of the sample evidenced a similar trajectory of
factors, Brewin and colleagues (2000) reported that elevated depression prior to bereavement followed
older age is modestly associated with reduced PTSD. It by improvement following the spouse’s death loss.
is important to stress, however, that this efect showed Although individuals showing the improved pattern
heterogeneity across samples. As a counterexample, a did not report greater caregiving strain relative to other
comparison of the reactions of younger and older adults bereaved individuals, their spouses were nonetheless
to the 1988 earthquake in Armenia failed to reveal dif- markedly more likely to have been ill in the years prior
ferences in overall PTSD symptoms (Goenjian et al., to their death. he depression–improvement pattern
1994). was also observed in a recent study of bereavement
Few studies have explicitly examined resilience among HIV-positive gay men who had been caring for
across adult and older adult groups. he available data, a partner who died of AIDS (Bonanno et al., 2005b).
however, are partially consistent with the idea that Again, the improvement pattern was associated spou-
age serves a bufering efect. Epidemiological data on sal illness but not caregiver burden per se.

125
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

A more general mechanism that may inform resili- However, these efects usually disappear when socio-
ence to PTEs among older adults is their increased abil- economic status is statistically controlled. A recent
ity to regulate emotion. he ratio of positive to negative exception, however, was a inding that among New
afect, for example, has been shown to increase linearly Yorkers exposed to the 9/11 attacks, ethnic Chinese
with age (Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998; Charles et al., 2001). were considerably more likely to be resilient (Bonanno
In addition, older adults also generally show a super- et al., 2007).
ior capacity to regulate their own emotional experi- Numerous theorists have delineated a crucial role
ence (Lawton et al., 1992) and report more complex for social and personal resources in the ways adults
or mixed emotional states (Labouvie-Vief et al., 1989). cope with stress (Murrell & Norris, 1983; Holahan &
hese abilities, in turn, suggest that older adults may Moos, 1991; Hobfoll, 2002) here is also consider-
be particularly competent managers of their emotional able research linking resources or change in resources
states and may be better able to manage the distress with adjustment following PTEs (Freedy et al., 1992;
related to loss or other PTEs. Kaniasty & Norris, 1993; Norris & Kaniasty, 1996;
Ironson et al., 1997). Recent research on resilience to
A heterogeneous array of resilience- trauma has further highlighted the particular import-
ance of maintaining full-time employment and of a
promoting factors range of social resources (e.g., social support, social
Research on the factors that promote resilient out- network size) (Bonanno et al., 2007, 2008).
comes following PTEs is still in an embryonic stage. here is also strong evidence linking PTSD with
Yet, an emergent conclusion even at this early junc- increased life stress prior to and following the marker
ture is that resilience is multiply determined. In other traumatic event (Brewin et al., 2000; Kubiak, 2005).
words, there are not one or two dominant factors that Recent evidence suggests an even stronger relationship
might make a person resilient – nor does resilience between the relative absence of current and prior life
appear to be an exclusive property of the individual. stress and resilience to trauma (Bonanno et al., 2007).
Rather, multiple, independent factors, some personal
and some situational and contextual, contribute jointly Personality: flexible and pragmatic coping
to the development of resilient outcomes.
Perhaps the most imperative, but poorly understood
hat a multiplicity of factors might promote resili-
conclusion from this emergent research is the idea that
ence is not a new idea. his same conclusion was
resilience does not depend solely on trait-like char-
already evident decades earlier following the initial
acteristics of the person. In part, the confusion arises
pioneering research into the nature of resilience in
from the fact that there are several unique person-
children. Among the factors implicated in that research
centered factors that can be reliably associated with
were person-centered variables (e.g., temperament of
resilience. In attempting to place these indings in their
the child) and sociocontextual factors (e.g., support-
proper perspective, however, we recall Mischel’s (1969)
ive relations, community resources) (Cowen, 1991;
famous if not timeless observation that personality
Werner, 1995; Rutter, 1999). Research on resilience
rarely explains more than 10% of the actual variance in
in adults following PTEs has suggested a similar con-
people’s behavior across situations. And so is the case
clusion (Hobfoll, 2002; Bonanno, 2005; Bonanno &
with the research on personality and resilience. What
Mancini, 2008). Multivariate modeling of resilient out-
has yet to be done, and should be imperative for future
comes has, for example, revealed upwards of 13 unique
research endeavors, is to examine the predictive role
and independent predictive factors (e.g., Bonanno
of personality in the context of broader, multivariate
et al., 2007).
models that could help to ascertain the relative contri-
bution of these measures.
Demographic and contextual factors In an attempt to move toward this type of integra-
A prosaic group of predictors can be found in simple tive understanding, we have elsewhere parsed per-
demographic variation. Resilience to trauma has been sonality factors into two broad categories, which we
associated with male gender, older age, and greater have labeled lexible adaptation and pragmatic coping
education (Bonanno et al., 2007). Racial/ethnic minor- (Bonanno, 2005; Mancini & Bonanno, 2006; Bonanno &
ity status is oten considered as a risk factor for PTSD. Mancini, 2008). he construct of pragmatic coping

126
Chapter 8: A lifespan approach to resilience and trauma

stems from the fact that highly stressful life events this confound requires a design in which worldviews
oten pose very speciic coping demands. Successfully are assessed a priori, that is before the PTE occurs. For
meeting these demands may require a highly pragmatic obvious reasons, this type of prospective design poses
or “whatever it takes” approach that is single minded practical and methodological diiculties. Indeed, to
and goal directed. Although pragmatic coping can our knowledge, only a few studies have as yet managed
arise in response to the situational demands, it has to meet such a condition. In the prospective study of
also been observed as a consequence of relatively rigid spousal loss discussed above, Bonanno et al. (2002a)
personality characteristics, such as repressive coping identiied pre-loss predictors of a resilient outcome;
(Weinberger et al., 1979) and the habitual use of self- among these were stronger a-priori beliefs in the
enhancing attributions and biases, otherwise known world’s justice and more accepting attitudes toward
as trait self-enhancement (Paulhus, 1998). A compel- death. We recently provided additional conirmation
ling inding with these pragmatic styles is that although for the role of a-priori beliefs by showing that favor-
they have been associated with some genuine costs able worldviews were related to adjustment over time
(e.g., narcissism) they consistently predict superior only among bereaved persons and not among non-
adjustment in the face of loss or other PTEs (Bonanno bereaved controls (Mancini et al., 2011b). In one of
et al., 1995, 2002b, 2005c; Coifman et al., 2007). the few investigations of PTSD to use pre-event data,
It should be stressed, however, that the majority of Bryant and Guthrie (2007) assessed ireighters dur-
people who exhibit resilient outcomes are genuinely ing their training and then collected a second wave of
healthy people who appear to possess a capacity for data four years later. Similar to the indings on bereave-
behavioral elasticity or lexible adaptation to impinging ment, more negative pre-event beliefs, and in particu-
challenges. A core aspect of this kind of lexibility is the lar beliefs about the self, were associated with elevated
capacity to shape and modify one’s behavior to meet PTSD symptoms four years later, accounting for 20% of
the demands of a given stressor event. his capacity the variance in those symptoms.
can be observed early in development, yet can change
over time as a result of the dynamic interplay of per- Positive emotions
sonality and social interactions with key attachment
One of the most robustly studied resilience factors in
igures (Block & Block, 2006). Practically speaking,
recent years has been positive emotion. It is widely
then, lexibility is a personality resource that helps to
acknowledged that positive emotions provide an
bolster resilience to aversive events, such as childhood
array of adaptive beneits, both for everyday life and
maltreatment (Flores et al., 2005), but it may also be
in response to stressful events (Lazarus et al., 1980;
enhanced or reduced by developmental experiences
Keltner & Bonanno, 1997; Tugade & Fredrickson, 2004;
(Shonk & Cicchetti, 2001). Preliminary research on
Fredrickson & Losada, 2005; but see also Bonanno et al.,
lexibility in adults suggests that the construct does
2007). However, the link between positive emotion and
eventually become stable and predicts resilience to
adjustment is particularly prominent in aversive con-
PTEs (Fredrickson et al., 2003; Bonanno et al., 2004;
texts. In a recent experimental study (Papa & Bonanno,
Westphal et al., 2010).
2008), for example, the expression of positive emotion
following exposure to a sad ilm predicted better psy-
Worldviews (a-priori beliefs) chological adjustment two years later. By contrast, posi-
A broad-based predictor of resilient outcomes has to tive emotional expressions following an amusing ilm
do with a-priori beliefs or worldviews. In the most were unrelated to long-term adjustment. Not surpris-
general terms, worldviews form our most abstract and ingly, then, the expression of positive emotions have
generalized conceptions of the extent that life is fair, consistently been shown to be a critical pathway toward
benevolent, and predictable (Rubin & Peplau, 1975; healthy adaptation (Bonanno, 2004). For example,
Janof-Bulman, 1992; Park & Folkman, 1997). Because bereaved individuals who exhibited genuine laughs and
worldviews are widely thought to be inluenced by smiles when describing a recent loss were found to be
extremely stressful events (Schwartzberg & Janof- better adjusted over the irst several years of bereave-
Bulman, 1991; Janof-Bulman, 1992), it has historic- ment (Bonanno & Keltner, 1997), and also evoked more
ally been diicult to untangle their direct efects on favorable responses in observers (Keltner & Bonanno,
coping. he only feasible methodological solution to 1997). Moreover, resilient bereaved people also reported

127
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

the fewest regrets about their behavior with the spouse, identifying factors that might inform childhood resili-
or about things they may have done or failed to do when ence to isolated PTEs. In many cases, it may be possible
he or she was still alive. Finally, resilient individuals were to identify resilience factors as the inverse of identiied
less likely to search to make sense of or ind meaning in risk factors (Bonanno, 2004). Indeed, as Masten (2001)
the spouse’s death, suggesting that they are less likely to has pointed out, risk and resilience factors oten index
engage in rumination, which has negative consequences a bipolar characteristic that can be either positive or
for adjustment to loss (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1997). negative, such as parenting (good or poor) or emotional
Positive emotions also serve a critical function in stability (high or low). Although research on resilience
adapting to more extreme and acute events. In a pro- in adults does not always conform to this simple bipolar
spective study on reactions to the 9/11 terrorist attacks scheme (e.g., Bonanno et al., 2007), such a scheme may
among college students, Fredrickson et al. (2003) found be more plausible in predicting childhood outcomes
that positive emotions, such as love, interest, and grati- because of the longer-term developmental consider-
tude, fully mediated the relation between pre-event ego ations. Proceeding in this way, some characteristics of
resilience, a trait-like personality characteristic, and children that are associated with better outcomes can
post-event depression and growth experiences. In the be identiied, although again we hasten to point out the
study of Bonanno et al. (2005c) of high-exposure sur- paucity of prospective research in this area.
vivors of the 9/11 attacks, the trait self-enhancers who One study that met this criterion came from the
had coped so efectively in that study were also more programmatic investigation of children’s responses to
likely to have experienced positive afect when talking Hurricane Andrew carried out by La Greca and col-
about the event. league (1998). hese researchers had obtained data 15
An ongoing and particularly compelling issue in months in advance of the hurricane and then at three
this research pertains to the question of how positive and seven months ater the hurricane. Among the pre-
emotions facilitate coping. he available empirical and dictors of positive post-hurricane outcome were low
theoretical literature suggests two independent but pre-hurricane anxiety and better pre-hurricane school
equally viable mechanisms may be at work. One is that performance, suggesting that emotional stability and
positive emotions quiet or “undo” negative emotions, academic achievement function as resilience factors.
which, in turn, helps to reduce levels of distress fol-
lowing PTEs (Keltner & Bonanno, 1997; Fredrickson, Implications for intervention across
1998, 2001). A second mechanism is that positive emo-
tions seem to facilitate coping by contagious inluence the lifespan
on others and, more indirectly, by increasing the avail- he evolving literature on resilience in the face of PTEs
ability of social resources (Malatesta, 1990; Keltner & holds important implications for possible interven-
Bonanno, 1997; Fredrickson, 2001). here is consistent tion across the lifespan. Most prominent is the gen-
empirical support for the role of both of these mecha- eral rebuttal to the nearly axiomatic assumption that
nisms. However, what has not yet been clear is the psychotherapeutic intervention for PTEs is invariably
extent to which each may be involved in coping with beneicial. he study of resilience advances the coun-
PTEs. Recently, Papa and Bonanno (2008) sought to terargument that such an assumption is misguided and
clarify the issue using an unusual approach of embed- that sweeping interventions can potentially even lead
ding an experimental methodology within a broader, to harm (McNally et al., 2003; Bonanno, 2004). For
long-term ield study of real-world adjustment among most people, intrinsic recovery processes will restore
college students following the 9/11 terrorist attack. equilibrium relatively soon ater exposure. Early
heir results showed clearly that both the undoing and interventions, such as critical incident stress debrief-
social facilitation components of positive emotion each ing, targeted indiscriminately at persons immediately
independently promoted adjustment over a two-year ater exposure to a PTE are not of questionable eicacy
period following the attack (Papa & Bonanno, 2008). but have been shown in several studies to exacerbate
trauma reactions, possibly by interfering with natural
recovery processes (Mayou et al., 2000; Litz et al., 2002;
Resilience factors specific to childhood McNally et al., 2003). Although research on early inter-
Given the paucity of research on trauma among ventions has up to now been conined to adults, we
children, relatively little progress has been made in see no reason to think that children are more likely to

128
Chapter 8: A lifespan approach to resilience and trauma

beneit from such early interventions or that children be candidates for treatment. It is inarguable that there
would necessarily be inure to the potential iatrogenic are efective treatments for PTSD, particularly with
efects of such interventions. Indeed, because children respect to exposure treatment (Institute of Medicine,
are more suggestible, it would appear likely that the 2007). However, this fact does not preclude the possi-
processes that give rise to deterioration efects from bility of iatrogenic efects, as witnessed in blanket early
early interventions may indeed be more and not less interventions following PTE.
salient for children.
With regard to grief counseling, although opinion Conclusions
is somewhat mixed (Larson & Hoyt, 2007), we would
It is our hope that continued research on resilience
maintain that the balance of evidence has not been
will foster an increased understanding of the normal
favorable to traditional models of grief counseling
human adaptability that characterizes all age groups,
(Bonanno, 2004; Bonanno & Lilienfeld, 2008). Meta-
and focus on the factors that might both promote or
analyses of the efectiveness of grief counseling have
prevent resilience at each stage of life. How resilience
consistently indicated small and oten non-signiicant
changes and how the factors that promote it might
efects, which are substantially less than typically
continue, disappear, or evolve across the lifespan are
observed for other forms of psychotherapeutic inter-
topics for future research. he study of resilience in
vention (Allumbaugh & Hoty, 1999; Kato & Mann,
the face of PTEs is nascent. Its promise, however, is
1999; Currier et al., 2008). Moreover, some studies
enormous.
have found no long-term beneits for grief counseling.
Previously we have argued that, given the increasing
awareness that resilience is the modal response to loss, References
grief-related interventions should only be targeted Allumbaugh, D. L. & Hoty, W. T. (1999). Efectiveness of
toward those who evidence obvious signs of endur- grief therapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling
ing dysfunction and express a clear desire for help Psychology, 46, 370–380.
(Bonanno & Mancini, 2006; Mancini & Bonanno, American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic
2006; Bonanno & Lilienfeld, 2008). Indeed, recent and statistical manual of mental disorders, 3rd edn.
studies of grief interventions that adopted just such a Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
targeted approach (e.g., Shear et al., 2005; Boelen et al., American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic
2007) have shown markedly more promising results and statistical manual, 4th edn revised (DSM-IV-TR).
than were previously observed. We would suggest Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
that similar guidelines should obtain when consider- Archer, J. (2001). Broad and narrow perspectives on grief:
ing treatment for bereaved children. Indeed, a recent A commentary on Bonanno and Kaltman (1999).
meta-analytic examination of grief counseling with Psychological Bulletin, 127, 554–560.
children showed essentially the same pattern of over- Bell, B. D. (1978). Disaster impact and response:
all poor efect sizes as observed with adults (Currier Overcoming the thousand natural shocks. Gerontologist,
et al., 2008). Consequently, rather than assuming that 18, 531–540.
all children will beneit, it would seem to be the wiser Bisconti, T. L., Bergeman, C. S., & Boker, S. M. (2006). Social
course to reserve treatment for those children experi- support as a predictor of variability: An examination of
the adjustment trajectories of recent widows. Psychology
encing genuine and persistent diiculties and not just
and Aging, 21, 590–599.
an expectable reaction to loss.
Although the story is somewhat more compli- Block, J. & Block, J. H. (2006). Venturing a 30-year
longitudinal study. American Psychologist, 61, 315–327.
cated for adults and children who have experienced
other types of acutely aversive experience, the study of Boelen, P. A., de Keijser, J., van den Hout, M. A., & van
den Bout, J. (2007). Treatment of complicated grief: A
resilience makes it clear that PTSD alicts only a small
comparison between cognitive-behavioral therapy and
minority of those exposed. his appears to be the case supportive counseling. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
in childhood, adulthood, and old age. For this reason, Psychology, 75, 277–284.
we would again argue that only those persons at any Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience:
age who might evidence genuine dysfunction in the Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive
face of PTEs, as deined by recurring symptoms and ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist,
interference with social roles and obligations, should 59, 20–28.

129
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Bonanno, G. A. (2005). Resilience in the face of loss and Bonanno, G. A., Moskowitz, J. T., Papa, A., & Folkman, S.
potential trauma. Current Directions in Psychological (2005b). Resilience to loss in bereaved spouses, bereaved
Science, 14, 135–138. parents, and bereaved gay men. Journal of Personality and
Bonanno, G. A. & Field, N. P. (2001). Examining the delayed Social Psychology, 88, 827–843.
grief hypothesis across 5 years of bereavement. American Bonanno, G. A., Rennicke, C., & Dekel, S. (2005c). Self-
Behavioral Scientist, 44, 798–816. enhancement among high-exposure survivors of the
Bonanno, G. A. & Kaltman, S. (1999). Toward an integrative September 11th terrorist attack: Resilience or social
perspective on bereavement. Psychological Bulletin, 125, maladjustment? Journal of Personality and Social
760–776. Psychology, 88, 984–998.
Bonanno, G. A. & Kaltman, S. (2001). he varieties of grief Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D.
experience. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 705–734. (2006). Psychological resilience ater disaster: New York
City in the atermath of the September 11th terrorist
Bonanno, G. A. & Keltner, D. (1997). Facial expressions of attack. Psychological Science, 17, 181–186.
emotion and the course of conjugal bereavement. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 126–137. Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D.
(2007). What predicts psychological resilience ater
Bonanno, G. A. & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2008). Let’s be realistic: disaster? he role of demographics, resources, and life
When grief counseling is efective and when it’s not. stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75,
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 39, 671–682.
377–378.
Bonanno, G. A., Ho, S. M. Y., Chan, J., et al. (2008).
Bonanno, G. A. & Mancini, A. D. (2006). Bereavement- Psychological resilience and dysfunction among
related depression and PTSD: Evaluating interventions. hospitalized survivors of the SARS epidemic in Hong
In L. Barbanel & R. Sternberg (eds.), Psychological Kong: A latent class approach. Health Psychology, 27,
interventions in times of crisis (pp. 37–55). New York: 659–667.
Springer.
Boscardin, C. K., Muthén, B., Francis, D. J., & Baker, E. L.
Bonanno, G. A. & Mancini, A. D. (2008). he human (2008). Early identiication of reading diiculties using
capacity to thrive in the face of potential trauma. heterogeneous developmental trajectories. Journal of
Pediatrics, 121, 369–375. Educational Psychology, 100, 192–208.
Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Holen, A., & Horowitz, M. J. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss, Vol 3: Loss: Sadness
(1995). When avoiding unpleasant emotions might and depression. New York: Basic Books.
not be such a bad thing: Verbal–autonomic response
dissociation and midlife conjugal bereavement. Journal Breslau, N., Davis, G. C., Peterson, E. L., & Schultz, L. R.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 975–989. (2000). A second look at comorbidity in victims
of trauma: A posttraumatic stress disorder-major
Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., Lehman, D. R., et al. depression connection. Biological Psychiatry, 48,
(2002a). Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A 902–909.
prospective study from preloss to 18-months postloss.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta-
1150–1164. analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder
in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and
Bonanno, G. A., Field, N. P., Kovacevic, A., & Kaltman, S. Clinical Psychology, 68, 748–766.
(2002b). Self-enhancement as a bufer against extreme
adversity: Civil war in Bosnia and traumatic loss in the Brewin, C. R., Scragg, P., Robertson, M., et al. (2008).
United States. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Promoting mental health following the London
28, 184–196. bombings: a screen and treat approach. Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 21, 3–8.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., &
Coifman, K. (2004). he importance of being lexible: Bryant, R. A. & Guthrie, R. M. (2007). Maladaptive self-
he ability to both enhance and suppress emotional appraisals before trauma exposure predict posttraumatic
expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Science, 15, 482–487. Psychology, 75, 812–815.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Zhang, N., & Noll, Buckley, T. C., Blanchard, E. B., & Hickling, E. J. (1996).
J. G. (2005a). Grief processing and deliberate grief A prospective examination of delayed onset PTSD
avoidance: A prospective comparison of bereaved secondary to motor vehicle accidents. Journal of
spouses and parents in the United States and the People’s Abnormal Psychology, 105, 617–625.
Republic of China. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Carver, C. S. (1998). Resilience and thriving: Issues, models,
Psychology, 73, 86–98. and linkages. Journal of Social Issues, 54, 245–266.

130
Chapter 8: A lifespan approach to resilience and trauma

Casella, L. & Motta, R. W. (1990). Comparison of Foa, E. B. & Rothbaum, B. O. (1998). Treating the trauma of
characteristics of Vietnam veterans with and without rape: Cognitive behavioral therapy for PTSD. New York:
posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Reports, 67, Guilford Press.
595–605. Fraley, R. C. & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Loss and bereavement:
Charles, S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz, M. (2001). Age- Attachment theory and recent controversies concerning
related diferences and change in positive and negative “grief work” and the nature of detachment. In J. Cassidy
afect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social & P. R. Shaver (eds.), Handbook of attachment: heory,
Psychology, 80, 136–151. research, and clinical applications (pp. 735–759). New
Christ, G. H. (2000). Healing children’s grief: Surviving a parent’s York: Guilford Press.
death from cancer. New York: Oxford University Press. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions?
Coifman, K. G., Bonanno, G. A., Ray, R. D., & Gross, J. J. Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–319.
(2007). Does repressive coping promote resilience? Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). he role of positive emotions in
Afective–autonomic response discrepancy during positive psychology: he broaden-and-build theory of
bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–226.
Psychology, 92, 745–758. Fredrickson, B. L. & Losada, M. F. (2005). Positive afect and
Copeland, W. E., Keeler, G., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. the complex dynamics of human lourishing. American
(2007). Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress in Psychologist, 60, 678–686.
childhood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 577–584. Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin,
Cowen, E. L. (1991). In pursuit of wellness. American G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis?
Psychologist, 46, 404–408. A prospective study of resilience and emotions following
the terrorist attacks on the United States on September
Curran, P. J. & Hussong, A. M. (2003). he use of latent
11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
trajectory models in psychopathology research. Journal
84, 365–376.
of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 526–544.
Freedy, J. R., Shaw, D. L., Jarrell, M. P., & Masters, C. R.
Currier, J. M., Neimeyer, R. A., & Berman, J. S. (2008). he
(1992). Towards an understanding of the psychological
efectiveness of psychotherapeutic interventions for
impact of natural disasters: An application of the
bereaved persons: A comprehensive quantitative review.
conservation resources stress model. Journal of
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 648–661.
Traumatic Stress, 5, 441–454.
Dalgleish, T. (2004). Cognitive approaches to posttraumatic
Galea, S., Ahern, J., Resnick, H., et al. (2002). Psychologial
stress disorder: he evolution of multirepresentational
sequelae of the September 11 terrorist attacks in New
theorizing. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 228–260.
York City. New England Journal of Medicine, 346,
DeRoon-Cassini, T. A., Mancini, A. D., Rusch, M. D., & 982–987.
Bonanno, G. A. (2010). Psychopathology and resilience
Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience and vulnerability to adverse
following traumatic injury: A latent growth mixture
developmental outcomes associated with poverty.
model analysis. Rehabilitation Psychology, 55, 1–11.
American Behavioral Scientist, 34, 416–430.
Deshields, T., Tibbs, T., Fan, M. Y., & Taylor, M. (2006).
Garrison, C. Z., Bryant, E. S., Addy, C. L., et al. (1995).
Diferences in patterns of depression ater treatment for
Posttraumatic stress disorder in adolescents ater
breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 15, 398–406.
Hurricane Andrew. Journal of the American Academy of
Deutsch, H. (1937). Absence of grief. Psychoanalytic Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 1193–1201.
Quarterly, 6, 12–22.
Goenjian, A. K., Najarian, L. M., Pynoos, R. S., et al. (1994).
Dohrenwend, B. P., Turner, J. B., Turse, N. A., et al. (2006). Posttraumatic stress disorder in elderly and younger
he psychological risks of Vietnam for US veterans: A adults ater the 1988 earthquake in Armenia. American
revisit with new data and methods. Science, 313, 979–982. Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 895–901.
Duncan, T. E., Duncan, S. C., & Strycker, L. A. (2006). An Green, B. L., Lindy, J. D., Grace, M. C., et al. (1990). Bufalo
introduction to latent variable growth curve modeling: Creek survivors in the second decade: Stability of stress
concepts, issues, and applications, 2nd edn. Mahwah, NJ: symptoms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60,
Erlbaum. 43–54.
Ellenberger, H. F. (1970). he discovery of the unconscious: Greenbaum, P. E., Del Boca, F. K., Darkes, J., Wang, C.-P., &
History and evolution of dynamic psychiatry. New York: Goldman, M. S. (2005). Variation in the drinking
Basic Books. trajectories of freshmen college students. Journal of
Flores, E., Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2005). Predictors Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 229–238.
of resilience in maltreated and nonmaltreated Latino Hobfoll, S. E. (2002). Social and psychological resources and
children. Developmental Psychology, 41, 338–351. adaptation. Review of General Psychology, 6, 307–324.

131
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Hoge, C. W., Castro, C. A., Messer, S. C., et al. (2004). La Greca, A. M., Silverman, W. K., & Wasserstein, S. B.
Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health (1998). Children’s predisaster functioning as a predictor
problems, and barriers to care. New England Journal of of posttraumatic stress following Hurricane Andrew.
Medicine, 351, 13–22. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66,
Holahan, C. J. & Moos, R. H. (1991). Life stressors, personal 883–892.
and social resources, and depression: A 4-year structural Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe, M., & Bulka, D. (1989). Speaking
model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 31–38. about feelings: Conceptions of emotion across the life
Institute of Medicine (2007). Treatment of posttraumatic span. Psychology and Aging, 4, 425–437.
stress disorder: An assessment of the evidence. Washington, LaLande, K. M. & Bonanno, G. A. (2011). Retrospective
DC: National Academies Press. memory bias for the frequency of potentially traumatic
events. Psychological Trauma, in press.
Ironson, G., Wynings, C., Schneiderman, N., et al. (1997).
Posttraumatic stress symptoms, intrusive thoughts, Lamprecht, F., & Sack, M. (2002). Posttraumatic stress
loss, and immune function ater Hurricane Andrew. disorder revisited. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 222–237.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 128–141. Larson, D. G. & Hoyt, W. T. (2007). What has become
Janis, I. L. (1951). Air war and emotional stress. New York: of grief counseling? An evaluation of the empirical
McGraw Hill. foundations of the new pessimism. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice, 38, 347–355.
Janof-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: towards a
new psychology of trauma. New York: Free Press. Lawton, M., Kleban, M. H., Rajagopal, D., & Dean, J. (1992).
Dimensions of afective experience in three age groups.
Jung, T. & Wickrama, K. A. (2008). An introduction to latent
Psychology and Aging, 7, 171–184.
class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling.
Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 302–317. Lazarus, R. S., Kanner, A. D., & Folkman, S. (1980).
Emotions: A cognitive–phenomenological analysis. In
Kaltman, S. & Bonanno, G. A. (2003). Trauma and
R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (eds.), heories of emotion.
bereavement: Examining the impact of sudden and
Emotions: theory, research, and experience, Vol. 1
violent deaths. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 131–147.
(pp.189–217). New York: Academic Press.
Kaniasty, K. & Norris, F. H. (1993). A test of the social Litz, B. T., Gray, M. J., Bryant, R. A., & Adler, A. B. (2002).
support deterioration model in the context of natural Early intervention for trauma: Current status and future
disaster. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, directions. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9,
395–408. 112–134.
Kardiner, A. (1941). he traumatic neuroses of war. New Luthar, S. S., Doernberger, C. H., & Zigler, E. (1993).
York: Paul B. Hoeber. Resilience is not a unidimensional construct: Insights
Kato, P. M. & Mann, T. (1999). A synthesis of psychological from a prospective study of inner-city adolescents.
interventions for the bereaved. Clinical Psychology Development and Psychopathology, 5, 703–717.
Review, 19, 275–296. Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). he construct
Keegan, J. (1976). he face of battle. Harmondsworth, UK: of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for
Chaucer Press. future work. Child Development, 71, 543–562.
Keltner, D. & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter and Malatesta, C. Z. (1990). he role of emotions in the
dissociation: Distinct correlates of laughter and smiling development and organization of personality. In R. A.
during bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social hompson (ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation,
Psychology, 73, 687–702. Vol. 36 (pp. 1–56). Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., & Press.
Nelson, C. B. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder in Mancini, A. D. & Bonanno, G. A. (2006). Resilience in
the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General the face of potential trauma: Clinical practices and
Psychiatry, 52, 1048–1060. illustrations. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 971–985.
Kubiak, S. P. (2005). Trauma and cumulative adversity in Mancini, A. D., Bonanno, G. A., & Clark, A. E. (2011a).
women of a disadvantaged social location. American Stepping of the hedonic treadmill: Individual diferences
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 75, 451–465. in response to major life events. Journal of Individual
Diferences, in press.
La Greca, A. M., Silverman, W. K., Vernberg, E. M., &
Prinstein, M. J. (1996). Symptoms of posttraumatic Mancini, A. D., Prati, G., & Bonanno, G. A. (2011b).
stress in children ater Hurricane Andrew: A prospective Do shattered worldviews lead to complicated grief?
study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, Prospective and longitudinal analyses. Journal of Social
712–723. and Clinical Psychology, 30, 184–215.

132
Chapter 8: A lifespan approach to resilience and trauma

Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes diferent demographic groups. Journal of Consulting and
in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. Clinical Psychology, 60, 409–418.
Mayou, R. A., Ehlers, A., & Hobbs, M. (2000). Psychological Norris, F. H. & Kaniasty, K. (1996). Received and perceived
debrieing for road traic accident victims. British social support in times of stress: A test of the social
Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 589–593. support deterioration deterrence model. Journal of
McFarlane, A. C. & Yehuda, R. (1996). Resilience, Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 498–511.
vulnerability, and the course of posttraumatic reactions. Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., Bisconti, T. L., & Wallace, K. A.
In A. C. M. B. A. van der Kolk, & L. Weisaeth (eds.), (2006). Psychological resilience, positive emotions, and
Traumatic stress (pp. 155–181). New York: Guilford Press. successful adaptation to stress in later life. Journal of
McNally, R. J. (2003). Progress and controversy in the Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 730–749.
study of posttraumatic stress disorder. Annual Review of Osterweis, M., Solomon, F., & Green, F. (1984). Bereavement:
Psychology, 54, 229–252. Reactions, consequences, and care. Washington, DC:
McNally, R. J., Bryant, R. A., & Elhers, A. (2003). Does National Academies Press.
early psychological intervention promote recovery from Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2003).
posttraumatic stress? Psychological Science in the Public Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and
Interest, 4, 45–79. symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Middleton, W., Moylan, A., Raphael, B., Burnett, P., & Bulletin, 129, 52–73.
Martinek, N. (1993). An international perspective on Papa, A. & Bonanno, G. A. (2008). Smiling in the face of
bereavement related concepts. Australian and New adversity: Interpersonal and intrapersonal functions of
Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 27, 457–463. smiling. Emotion, 8, 1–12.
Middleton, W., Burnett, P., Raphael, B., & Martinek, N. Park, C. L. & Folkman, S. (1997). Meaning in the context
(1996). he bereavement response: A cluster analysis. of stress and coping. Review of General Psychology, 1,
British Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 167–171. 115–144.
Mischel, W. (1969). Continuity and change in personality. Parkes, C. M. & Weiss, R. S. (1980). Recovery from
American Psychologist, 24, 1012–1018. bereavement. New York: Basic Books.
Mroczek, D. K. & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). he efect of age on Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic
positive and negative afect: A developmental perspective adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A mixed
on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
Psychology, 75, 1333–1349. 1197–1208.
Murphy, L. B. & Moriarty, A. E. (1976). Vulnerability, coping, Perrin, S., Smith, P., & Yule, W. (2000). Assessment and
and growth. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. treatment of PTSD in children and adolescents. Journal
Murrell, S. A. & Norris, F. H. (1983). Resources, life events, of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 41, 277–289.
and changes in psychological states: A prospective Phifer, J. F. (1990). Psychological distress and somatic
framework. American Journal of Community Psychology, symptoms ater natural disaster: Diferential
11, 473–491. vulnerability among older adults. Psychology and Aging,
Muthén, B. (2004). Latent variable analysis: Growth mixture 5, 412–420.
modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. Pinquart, M. & Schindler, I. (2007). Changes of life
In D. Kaplan (ed.), Handbook of quantitative methodology satisfaction in the transition to retirement: a latent-class
for the social sciences (pp. 345–368). Newbury Park, CA: approach. Psychology and Aging, 22, 442–455.
Sage. Rachman, S. J. (1978). Fear and courage. New York:
Neugarten, B. L. (1979). Time, age, and the life cycle. Freeman.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 887–894. Rando, T. A. (1993). Treatment of complicated mourning.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. & Ahrens, C. (2002). Age diferences Champaign, IL: Research Press.
and similarities in the correlates of depressive symptoms. Reich, J. W., Zautra, A. J., & Guarnaccia, C. A. (1989). Efects
Psychology and Aging, 17, 116–124. of disability and bereavement on the mental health and
Nolen-Hoesksema, S., McBride, A., & Larson, J. (1997). recovery of older adults. Psychology and Aging, 4, 57–65.
Rumination and psychological distress among bereaved Roussos, A., Goenjian, A. K., Steinberg, A. M., et al. (2005).
partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, Posttraumatic stress and depressive reactions among
855–862. children and adolescents ater the 1999 earthquake in
Norris, F. H. (1992). Epidemiology of trauma: Frequency Ano Liosia, Greece. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162,
and impact of diferent potentially traumatic events on 530–537.

133
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Rubin, Z. & Peplau, L. A. (1975). Who believes in a just Shepard, B. (2001). A war of nerves: Soldiers and psychiatrists
world? Journal of Social Issues, 31, 65–89. in the twentieth century. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Rutter, M. (1979). Protective factors in children’s responses University Press.
to stress and disadvantage. In M. W. Kent & J. E. Rolf Sherbourne, C., Meredith, L., Rogers, W., & Ware, J. (1992).
(eds.), Primary prevention of psychopathology: Social Social support and stressful life events: Age diferences
competence in children, Vol. 3 (pp. 49–74). Lebanon, NH: in their efects on health-related quality of life among the
University Press of New England. chronically ill. Quality of Life Research, 1, 235–246.
Rutter, M. (1999). Resilience concepts and indings: Shonk, S. M. & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Maltreatment,
Implications for family therapy. Journal of Family competency deicits, and risk for academic and
herapy, 21, 119–144. behavioral maladjustment. Developmental Psychology,
Sanders, C. M. (1993). Risk factors in bereavement 37, 3–17.
outcome. In M. S. Stroebe, W. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson Tucker, P., Pfeferbaum, B., Doughty, D. E., et al. (2002).
(eds.), Handbook of bereavement: heory, research, and Body handlers ater terrorism in Oklahoma City:
intervention (pp. 255–267). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Predictors of posttraumatic stress and other symptoms.
University Press. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 72, 469–475.
Schaefer, C. M., Petras, H., Ialongo, N., Poduska, J., & Tugade, M. M. & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient
Kellam, S. (2003). Modeling growth in boys’ aggressive individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from
behavior across elementary school: Links to later negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality
criminal involvement, conduct disorder, and antisocial and Social Psychology, 86, 320–333.
personality disorder. Developmental Psychology, 39, Weinberger, D. A., Schwartz, G. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1979).
1020–1035. Low-anxious, high-anxious, and repressive coping styles:
Schaefer, C. M., Petras, H., Ialongo, N., et al. (2006). A Psychometric patterns and behavioral and physiological
comparison of girls’ and boys’ aggressive–disruptive responses to stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88,
behavior trajectories across elementary school: 369–380.
prediction to young adult antisocial outcomes. Journal of Werner, E. E. (1995). Resilience in development. Current
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 500–510. Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 81–85.
Schulz, R., Beach, S. R., Lind, B., et al. (2001). Involvement Westphal, M., Seivert, N., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010).
in caregiving and adjustment to death of a spouse: Expressive lexibility. Emotion, 10, 92–100.
Findings from the caregiver health efects study. Journal
of the American Medical Association, 285, 3123–3129. Worden, J. W. (1991). Grief counseling and grief therapy: A
handbook for the mental health practitioner, 2nd edn. New
Schwartzberg, S. S. & Janof-Bulman, R. (1991). Grief and York: Springer.
the search for meaning: Exploring the assumptive worlds
of bereaved college students. Journal of Social and Clinical Wortman, C. B. & Silver, R. C. (1989). he myths of coping
Psychology, 10, 270–288. with loss. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
57, 349–357.
Shear, K., Frank, E., Houck, P. R., & Reynolds, C. F. (2005).
Treatment of complicated grief: a randomized controlled Zisook, S., Chentsova-Dutton, Y., & Shuchter, S. R. (1998).
trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, PTSD following bereavement. Annals of Clinical
2601–2608. Psychiatry, 10, 157–163.

134
Section 2 Resilience across the lifespan
Chapter
Resilience in older adults

9 Diane L. Elmore, Lisa M. Brown, and Joan M. Cook

Introduction adults makes this subgroup of the population exceed-


ingly important to policy makers, service providers,
Older adulthood is a developmental stage at the end
clinicians, and researchers.
of the lifespan when an individual experiences a num- his chapter will focus on issues of resilience in
ber of signiicant changes in life circumstances. hese older adults. First, deinitions of resilience and his-
age-associated events include changes in physical torical perspectives on its investigation in older adults
appearance and body composition, which can result will be reviewed. hen, a brief general background
in increased vulnerability to both acute and chronic regarding older adult mental health and aging will be
physical illness as well as functional limitations such as presented. Next, an overview of the resilience of older
decreased mobility and diminished sensory capacities. adults in the face of stressful life events will be pro-
In addition, older people generally experience numer- vided. Fourth, an overview of resilience and recovery
ous losses and stressors, such as moving to a ixed from trauma in older adults will be discussed. Lastly,
income, increasing expenses, and loss of retirement areas for continued scientiic and clinical investiga-
investments; death of family members, friends and tion related to resilience among older adults will be
loss of social network; alterations in social position; highlighted.
changes in housing and work; and spousal caregiving
and widowhood. he ability to adapt positively to these
types of stressful life event and to other adversities is
Definitions and historical
likely an important factor in “successful” aging. perspectives
Currently, older adults are among the fastest grow- he impact of trauma on older adults appears to be a
ing subgroups of the population in the USA as in many complicated relationship with a number of potentially
other countries. Recent estimates suggest that there are exacerbating and bufering variables that can impact
approximately 37 million people aged 65 years and over outcomes. While a great deal of work has been done
in the USA, accounting for over 12% of the total popula- to identify predictors of post-traumatic stress dis-
tion (US Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related order (PTSD), few eforts have focused on predictors
Statistics, 2008). By the year 2030, the number of indi- of resilience (Bonanno, 2004). A number of key varia-
viduals age 65 years and over is expected to nearly dou- bles have been discussed as potentially inluencing the
ble to 71.5 million, accounting for approximately 20% response to traumatic events, including gender; type
of the US population (US Federal Interagency Forum of trauma; a history of prior trauma; personality traits;
on Aging-Related Statistics, 2008). social support; interpersonal relationships; sense of
In addition to the increase in size of the older purpose; humor; self-eicacy; and interactions of age
adult population, the aging community is also becom- with social, economic, cultural, and historical contexts
ing increasingly racially and ethnically diverse and is (Breslau et al., 1998; Norris et al., 2002a; Boardman et al.,
enjoying greater educational attainment than previous 2008). Bonanno (2004) suggested that more work was
generations of older people (US Federal Interagency needed to assess whether the inversion of the predic-
Forum on Aging-Related Statistics, 2008). Both the tors of PTSD may allow for the prediction of resilient
increase in number and the growing diversity of older functioning.

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

135
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Since the late 1990s, the concept of resilience has Services, 1999). Additionally, approximately 3% of
received increasing attention, yet its exact deinition older adults have PTSD, while the prevalence is 10%
is still being debated. Foster (1997) posited that one’s among middle-aged adults and 9% among younger
ability to function in the face of stress and adversity is adults (Norris, 1992).
inluenced by genetics, life experiences and learning, While these data may imply that a majority of older
and individual diferences in resources. Some of these adults enjoy good mental health, the existing litera-
genetic, environmental, and individual variables may ture suggests that the mental health problems of older
be particularly important to older adults, including the adults are oten underreported and, therefore, under-
trauma-speciic variables (e.g., severity and duration), diagnosed (Brown et al., 2007a). Further complicating
social support, and societal reaction to the particular detection is a subsyndromal presentation of symptoms
trauma. in this population. Although the person’s symptoms
Hardy et al. (2004, p. 260) conceptualized resilience are not suicient to warrant a formal psychiatric diag-
as a response to a stressful event, rather than a per- nosis, the presence of subthreshold symptoms can still
sonality trait, and identiied several possible resilient adversely afect quality of life, morbidity, and mortality
responses, including “a small initial decremental efect and may require clinical attention.
of the event, a rapid recovery, minimal negative long-
term consequences, and positive long-term conse- Resilience and recovery from trauma
quences of the event.” Bonanno (2004, p. 20) theorized
Traumatic experiences can impact individuals across
that resilience is the ability “to maintain relatively sta-
the lifespan. For some, such experiences are associated
ble, healthy levels of psychological and physical func-
with negative psychological outcomes, such as PTSD
tioning” in the face of loss and trauma. He described
(Norris, 1992; Norris et al., 2002b, 2002c). While most
recovery as a distinct concept represented by a return
attention in the loss and trauma literature has focused
to pre-event functioning following a period of clinical
on the maladaptive outcomes associated with traumatic
or subclinical psychopathology. He asserted that the
experiences, not all individuals experience signiicant
distinctions between resilience and recovery are crit-
negative psychosocial outcomes following trauma. In
ical, as they can inform the appropriate need for and
fact, a growing body of research suggests that most
timing of clinical intervention (Bonanno, 2004).
people are highly resilient and able to fully recover
Although the investigation of resilience and its
from serious adverse life events (US Department of
potential correlates and predictors has received lim-
Health and Human Services, 2001; Bonanno, 2004). A
ited attention in older adults, the importance of related
number of key variables have been discussed as poten-
concepts in the early stages of the ield of gerontology
tially inluencing the response to traumatic events,
should be noted. hese include successful aging, posi-
including age.
tive adjustment to aging, personality traits, and mor-
To date, relatively limited attention in the trauma
ale and life satisfaction. he relationship between these
literature has focused exclusively on older adults. What
earlier age-related concepts and resilience will be dis-
literature does exist identiies older adults as a group
cussed throughout this chapter.
that is either primarily vulnerable or mainly resilient to
the negative consequences of traumatic stressors (e.g.
Mental health and aging Kilijanek & Drabek, 1979). Further, most of the litera-
ture related to the impact of trauma on older adults
In order to understand resilience in older adults, it is
focuses on psychopathology and the negative efects
useful to be aware of some basic facts regarding aging
of such events; the small body of resilience research
and mental health. Community-based prevalence esti-
does present some possible explanations for resilient
mates suggest that approximately 5% of older adults
responses in the face of adversity.
meet criteria for either an anxiety or a depressive dis-
order (Gallo & Lebowitz, 1999). It is estimated that
approximately 20% of those aged 55 years or older Theories of resilience and aging
experience some type of mental disorder, and that A number of theories have been developed to explain
13% of those aged 65 years and over meet criteria the response of older adults to traumatic events. Among
for a mental disorder other than dementia (Robins & the theories that may help in understanding resili-
Regier, 1991; US Department of Health and Human ence in older adults are those based on vulnerability,

136
Chapter 9: Resilience in older adults

inoculation, burden, mortality efects, and maturation. of older adults following signiicant loods, Norris and
A brief overview and critical summary of these theor- Murrell (1988) examined the concepts of both direct
ies in respect to their relevance to older adults are given tolerance and cross-tolerance in older adults. heir
below. investigation suggested that prior experience with
loods as well as prior exposure to other traumatic
Vulnerability theory events moderated the impact of the event. Interestingly,
Early research on trauma suggested that disasters stronger support was found for the relationship with
had a disproportionate efect on older adults. It was direct tolerance than for cross-tolerance (Norris &
hypothesized that this group experienced a greater Murrell, 1988). Similarly, in a study of adults aged 30
sense of deprivation relative to their losses and, con- to 102 years following the 1994 Northridge earthquake,
sequently, more psychological distress than younger Knight et al. (2000) found that greater prior earthquake
adults (Kilijanek & Drabek, 1979). However, this was exposure was associated with lower post-earthquake
not borne out by other research (Fields, 1996). Some depression scores. hese indings are consistent with
evidence inds support for the vulnerability hypothesis, the inoculation hypothesis.
in which older adult survivors of prolonged stress (e.g., Norris and Murrell (1988) cautioned that, while the
Holocaust survivors, prisoners of war) are thought to inoculation hypothesis may hold true for older adults
be at greater risk because of their increasing age in con- who experience intermittent stressors, similar results
junction with the accumulation of greater numbers of may not be expected following prolonged or extended
traumas resulting from their longer lives (Solomon & stress. In fact, other studies of aging survivors, which
Prager, 1992; Danieli, 1997). Older adults have a var- are discussed later in this chapter, have not found sui-
ied and extensive accumulation of life experiences that cient support for this hypothesis but instead have sug-
afect short- and long-term vulnerability following gested that prior prolonged stressors may be associated
exposure to traumas (Phifer & Norris, 1989; Brown with increased vulnerability to subsequent traumas
et al., 2007b). (Bremner et al., 1992; Solomon & Prager, 1992; Yehuda
et al., 1995; Breslau et al., 1998; Green et al., 2000;
Stress inoculation hypothesis Nishith et al., 2000).
he stress inoculation hypothesis was posited by
Eysenck (1983), who asserted that earlier trauma fos- Burden hypothesis
ters resilience to subsequent traumas. Speciically, two he burden perspective is another theory that may be
paths were suggested: direct tolerance, in which prior relevant to understanding resilience among some older
exposure to a speciic stressor may lessen the efect of adults. his hypothesis posits that middle-aged indi-
that stressor in the future, and cross-tolerance, in which viduals are the most afected by trauma because they
prior exposure to a particular stressor may lessen the experience the greatest disruptions and demands on
efect of a diferent stressor in the future (Eysenck, 1983). their time as providers of care to both younger and
According to Norris and Murrell (1988), the stress older generations and as breadwinners. In a study of
inoculation theory is particularly relevant to under- adults following Hurricane Hugo, hompson et al.
standing the efects of trauma on older adults because of (1993) found a curvilinear interaction between post-
their rich history of experience. Evidence that supports disaster distress and age, with middle-aged adults
the inoculation hypothesis suggests that older adults experiencing the highest symptom levels. his study
may respond better following traumatic events because provides support for the burden hypothesis.
of their experience with trauma earlier in life (Knight
et al., 2000). Despite a more severe or direct exposure Maturation hypothesis
to the event, poorer health status, and fewer social and Another explanation for resilience among older adults
economic resources, a lifetime of learning how to cope is the maturation hypothesis, which suggests that older
with taxing events as well as fewer currently unresolved adults have more mature coping styles and this makes
stressful experiences are believed to promote adapta- them less emotionally reactive to stressful events
tion to disasters in older adults (Phifer, 1990). (Knight et al., 2000). However, this theory was not
Several studies have found support for the inocu- supported by research that examined depressed mood
lation hypothesis among aging survivors of trauma prior to and ater the 1994 Northridge earthquake. Age
(Norris & Murrell, 1988; Knight et al., 2000). In a study did not moderate the relationship between damage

137
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

exposure and rumination. As noted above, the inocu- since the mid 1990s. In a sample of community-
lation hypothesis was supported by the indings from dwelling adults over 60 years of age, a self-rating of suc-
this study. cessful aging was signiicantly correlated with higher
resilience, greater activity, and greater number of close
The mortality effect friends – despite some having chronic physical illnesses
he mortality efect has also been posited as a possible and some type of disability (Montross et al., 2006).
explanation for the resilience that is observed among In a sample of community-dwelling predominately
older adults who have experienced trauma. his efect Caucasian women over age 60 years, exploratory factor
refers to the fact that many of the less resilient survivors analysis using the Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale
of trauma may have died before reaching older adult- (Connor & Davidson, 2003) yielded four factors: (1)
hood, thus leaving a healthier group of aging individ- personal control and goal orientation, (2) adaptation
uals behind (Creamer & Parslow, 2008). and tolerance for negative afect, (3) leadership and
trust in instincts, and (4) spiritual coping (Lamond
Summary et al., 2008). Consistent with the indings of Montross
While these hypotheses are important in understand- et al. (2006), self-rated successful aging was positively
ing the possible relationships between age and trauma, associated with resiliency. Other predictors of resili-
overly simplistic explanations that characterize the ency were higher emotional well-being, optimism,
entire population of older adults do not provide an social engagement, and fewer cognitive complaints.
accurate scientiic or clinical picture. Recent reviews hese results are similar to those obtained in younger
of the literature suggest that such diferential classiica- samples. However, the authors posited that the con-
tions of older adults may be unhelpful and may have struct of resilience difers between younger and older
deleterious efects for older adults (Norris et al., 2002a; adults (Lamond et al., 2008, p. 152): in younger adults
Cook & Elmore, 2009). resilience may be construed as “problem- or task-
Bonanno (2004) suggested that individuals may focused active coping” whereas in older adults it may
be more resilient to extremely aversive events than be better characterized as “acceptance and toleration
commonly thought. He asserted that theorists in the of negative afect.”
loss and trauma ields may underestimate and mis- In a sample of community-residing older adults, a
understand resilience, as most of the evidence in the wide range of reactions associated with resilience were
ield is derived from samples of individuals who have displayed in response to a stressful event. Resilience was
had negative outcomes following trauma. According associated with a range of demographic, clinical, func-
to Bonanno (2004), most who experience a traumatic tional, and psychosocial factors (Hardy et al., 2004).
event will display healthy responses indicative of a In particular, high resilience was associated with male
resilience trajectory. Bonanno et al. (2005a) asserted gender, living with others, high grip strength, inde-
that the resilient pathway is far more common than the pendence in ability to perform instrumental activities
recovery trajectory. herefore, caution should be taken of daily living, having few depressive symptoms, and
to ensure that clinical interventions do not interfere having good to excellent self-rated health. he authors
with or undermine the natural resilience of the major- suggested that resilience is a key factor for older adults
ity of survivors (Bonanno, 2004). in maintaining their well-being as well as a potentially
important predictor of recovery from future illness and
Resilience in daily life and disability (Hardy et al., 2004).
he relationship between resilience, sense of coher-
in the face of stressful events ence, purpose in life, and self-transcendence to per-
In general, older adults experience stressful life events ceived physical and mental health was examined in
at a much greater frequency than younger adults individuals aged 85 years or older (Nygren et al., 2005).
(Hughes et al., 1988). Indeed, some events that were here was a high correlation between scores on sev-
previously infrequent across the lifespan become eral of the resilience-like scales suggesting some core
increasingly normative. A review of the literature dimension of inner strength.
revealed that a growing number of researchers are In an examination of resilience in the face of
examining the role of resilience and aging. Below is a daily stressors and recent widowhood, Ong and col-
brief synopsis of several studies that were published leagues (2004, 2006) found that the occurrence of

138
Chapter 9: Resilience in older adults

daily positive emotions moderated stress reactivity in the community is their aging spouse (American
and mediated stress recovery. Just as the research con- Psychological Association, 1997). While caregiving
ducted by Nygren et al. (2005) suggested that resili- can be extremely stressful and result in negative out-
ence was related to some aspect of inner strength, this comes for some, others appear to be quite resilient to
study found that a stable facet of personality, some- the potential deleterious efects of caregiving (Schulz
times referred to as stress tolerance or neuroticism, et al., 1997; Beach et al., 2000). he caregiving literature
predicted a weaker association between negative and makes a unique contribution to our understanding of
positive emotions during periods of heightened stress resilience. Several recent studies that examined resili-
trait resilience. heir indings revealed that experien- ence and caregiving are discussed.
cing positive emotions over time helped high-resilient In a longitudinal investigation of caregivers of
individuals to recover from daily stress. someone with dementia, who were in various stages
Results from the population-based Heidelberg related to providing care (e.g., institutionalization, care-
Centenarian Study challenged the concept that psycho- recipient death), high baseline resilience was associ-
logical resilience may end in very advanced age (Jopp & ated with less-frequent institutionalization and loss to
Rott, 2006). Indeed, centenarians reported high levels follow-up as well as more frequent care-recipient mor-
of happiness, a substantial proportion of which was tality (Gaugler et al., 2007). High resilience at baseline
accounted for by basic resources such as health and was also associated with the following variables: female
social network. Some resource efects were mediated care recipients, providing care for a longer duration,
through self-referent beliefs (e.g., self-eicacy) and spending more time providing care, and greater util-
attitudes toward life (e.g., optimistic outlook). ization of formal and informal resources (Gaugler et al.,
Qualitative research also yielded similar results 2007). Furthermore, contextual care-recipient status
to the quantitative studies described above. A series and resource indicators were also associated with stress
of interviews were conducted with women between resistance or resilience in caregivers for a person with
the ages of 70 and 80 years to determine factors that dementia. In contrast, higher baseline resilience was
contribute to resilience (Kinsel, 2005). Seven factors associated with caregivers being white, having inde-
emerged as contributing to resilience, including social pendent activities of daily living dependencies, living
connectedness, facing challenges “head-on,” spirit- with care recipients, and having greater education and
ual grounding, curiosity, and helping others (Kinsel, income; it was also associated with greater cognitive
2005). impairment in care recipients (Gaugler et al., 2007).
In addition to daily stressors, there are a number Qualitative research has also examined aging, care-
of speciic stressors that are oten increasingly encoun- giving, and resilience. hree to ive in-depth interviews
tered with age. hese include caregiving and widow- were conducted over the course of several years with
hood. Further, there are also traumatic stressors that older African-Americans who had chronic illness
are particularly salient for the current and emerging (Becker & Newsom, 2005). heir experiences and per-
cohort of older adults, such as being a veteran of ceptions of racism inluenced how they viewed and
World War II, the Holocaust, and the Korean conlict. coped with their illnesses. he cultural beliefs and
Consideration of such historical sociopolitical stressors mores held by the older women placed a high value on
may be helpful in identifying potential generational resilience, independence, and survival.
traumas. he following provides an overview of some Interviews with women over the age of 85 years
key research indings associated with these events as from various ethnic, racial, and socioeconomic back-
they relate to resilience in older adults. grounds who were currently dealing with chronic ill-
ness revealed several factors that appeared to be related
Caregiving and illness to the resilience (Felten, 2000). hese included deter-
mination to overcome adversity and previous experi-
Currently, approximately 80% of older adults live with
ence with hardship.
at least one chronic health condition, which can oten
result in limitations in daily activities and health-
related quality of life (Centers for Disease Control and Widowhood
Prevention & the Merck Company Foundation, 2007). Not surprisingly, widowhood becomes increasingly
he most likely caregiver of an impaired older adult prevalent as individuals age. For most older adults the

139
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

death of a loved one is a signiicant loss and life stres- Afect and somatic symptoms were strongly associated
sor. Here three studies on widowhood are reviewed with resilience.
that examined resilience as it relates to bereavement. In
a sample of middle-aged and older bereaved spouses, Holocaust survivors
parents, and gay men who had lost a partner, the qual-
Much attention in the trauma literature has focused
ity of their relationship with the deceased was not a fac-
on the impact of the Holocaust on the health and well-
tor that inluenced resilience (Bonanno et al., 2005b).
being of the survivors, who would have had a wide range
Resilient people were rated as “well adjusted” prior to
of ages at the time of the trauma. Today, the remaining
the person’s death and as possessing positive traits.
survivors of the Holocaust are rapidly aging. Studies
In a prospective investigation of older adults who
of Holocaust survivors provide unique insight into
had experienced the death of a spouse, Bonanno et al.
the efects of trauma on the normative aging process
(2002) identiied ive unique trajectories of bereave-
(Danieli, 1994). In a study of aging Israeli Holocaust
ment outcome, one of which was resilience. he
survivors that was conducted during the Persian Gulf
resilient group showed remarkably healthy proiles
War, Solomon and Prager (1992) found that the study
in reacting to and processing the loss. In particular,
participants perceived higher levels of danger, reported
resilient individuals showed diferences in coping and
more symptoms of acute distress, and displayed higher
meaning-making and showed little signs of struggling
levels of both state and trait anxiety compared with
with or denying the loss.
non-survivors. his study did not support the inocula-
In a sample of older bereaved spouses, ive out of
tion hypothesis and instead suggested that these aging
six people adjusted well over time, and one in three
survivors may have been more vulnerable because of
showed substantial resilience without negative con-
their prior trauma. Dougall et al. (2000) suggested that
sequences (Ott et al., 2007). his resilient pattern was
prior trauma history could sensitize the individual to
characterized by low levels of grief and depression and
new stressors, thus potentiating their efects. Exposure
high quality of life.
to prolonged, extreme stress at developmentally cru-
cial points in time may result in permanent efects
Former combatants and prisoners of war that increase vulnerability to future traumatic events
Recent estimates suggest that two of every three men throughout the lifespan.
aged 65 years and over in the USA and Puerto Rico are Yehuda and colleagues (1995) investigated the
veterans (US Federal Interagency Forum on Aging- impact of cumulative lifetime trauma and recent stress
Related Statistics, 2008). Many of these men were likely on current PTSD symptoms in Holocaust survivors.
exposed to stressors or traumas associated with war Greater cumulative trauma and recent stress was
zone-related duty or other stressful aspects of their reported by survivors with PTSD than those without
military service. Consequently, older veterans are a PTSD. In her work with Holocaust survivors and their
particularly valuable group from which to gather data families over several decades, Danieli (1994) found that
on resilience. adaptational styles and post-Holocaust adjustment dif-
In a large sample of aging veterans, 40% of whom fered substantially among survivor families. She identi-
had experienced combat, most identiied more positive ied four adaptational styles of survivor families: victim
efects of their military service, including enhanced families, ighter families, numb families, and “those
coping skills and self-esteem and increased independ- who made it.” Families considered in the “those who
ence and self-discipline, than undesirable efects (Aldwin made it” category appeared to display greater resilience
et al., 1994). In fact, in those who had experienced com- than those in some of the other groups.
bat, perceiving positive beneits from this experience A recent study compared the responses of female
mitigated the deleterious efects. In essence, their posi- survivors of the Holocaust with those of non-Holo-
tive perception bufered their experience of distress. caust survivor women following the September 11,
In a national sample of former American prison- 2001 (9/11) terrorist attacks (Lamet et al., 2009). A
ers of war, Engdahl and colleagues (1993) examined non-linear relationship was found between stress and
psychosocial functioning as measured by positive and coping, with Holocaust survivors reporting both more
negative afect, somatic symptoms, captivity trauma, symptoms of PTSD as well as higher levels of resilience
and resilience. Data were collected in 1967 and in 1985. compared with the non-Holocaust survivor group.

140
Chapter 9: Resilience in older adults

Other trauma groups Older adults who react to but quickly bounce back
from traumatic stressors are sometimes considered
A phone survey conducted following the 9/11 attacks
to be responding in a maladaptive fashion. However,
found that resilience was predicted by the age of the
Boerner et al. (2005) contended that “doing well” fol-
study participant (Bonanno et al., 2007). Participants
lowing an event like the death of a loved one may be a
who were 65 years of age and older were three time more
normal response compared with the response that is
likely to be resilient than a younger cohort of partici-
a cause for concern and intervention. While it may be
pants (18–24 years), and less likely to have PTSD. Other
challenging for loss and trauma experts, as well as soci-
factors associated with resilience included male gender,
ety as a whole, to fully understand rapid recoveries fol-
Asian race/ethnicity, and lack of major life stressors (i.e.,
lowing tragic events, a growing body of research does
that person had experienced no prior traumatic events
support the occurrence of such outcomes.
or recent life stressors or additional traumas since 9/11).
Examples from the aging literature can also help
Higher levels of education were not predictive of resili-
us to understand resilience in older adults. In a study
ence. hose who were less likely to be resilient included
related to age and memory for positive, negative, and
people who had experienced loss of income because of
neutral images, Charles et al. (2003) found age-asso-
the terror attack, had a chronic disease, or reported lower
ciated reductions in memory for negative images. he
levels of perceived social support. he authors noted
authors suggested that older adults put less efort into
that their indings were consistent with earlier work
remembering negative information that was deemed
conducted by developmental theorists (Garmezy, 1991;
less important to their everyday functioning, in
Werner, 1995; Rutter, 1999) who posited that resilience
order to reserve energy for other more central issues.
results from a “cumulative mix of person-center variables
herefore, it is plausible that traumatized older adults
(e.g., disposition, personality) and sociocontextual (e.g.,
may minimize the efects of the traumatic event and
family interaction, community support systems) risk and
appear highly resilient when relaying their experience
protective factors” (Bonanno et al., 2007, p. 769).
to others because they spent less time and efort encod-
In a qualitative investigation of coping strategies
ing negative information. Further, social comparison
and mental health among low-income older African-
theory suggests that older adults may compare them-
Americans who experienced Hurricane Katrina,
selves with age peers as a way to reinterpret their pre-
expressions of spirituality were found to bufer the
sent situation in a more positive manner (Frieswijk
atermath and promote emotional resilience (Lawson
et al., 2004). In large-scale disasters, such as hurricanes
& homas, 2007). Regular communication with a
and earthquakes, older adults may compare themselves
“Higher Power,” daily reading of the Bible and various
with others and downplay the extent of trauma or dis-
devotional materials, as well as helping others as a con-
ruption they are experiencing (Brown et al., 2007b).
sequence of faith were spiritual coping strategies that
Although this cognitive appraisal may be psychologic-
promoted resilience.
ally protective, minimizing or downplaying their losses
and troubles may be problematic when needed services
Older adults and resilience are not sought – or accepted when ofered.
he empirical evidence speciic to older adults and Additionally, the relationship between personality
trauma reveals that most older adults, like their traits and resilience deserves attention in older adult-
younger counterparts, will recover rapidly without hood. he “ive factor model” is a personality inven-
intervention in the atermath of trauma (Phifer & tory that is widely used by researchers to describe
Norris, 1989; Norris, 1992; Norris et al., 2002a; Shalev, traits (John & Srivastava, 1999; Morey, 2007). he
2002; Bonanno et al., 2004; Boerner et al., 2005). inventory encompasses ive dimensional measures of
Unfortunately, because much of the research related personality traits: openness, conscientiousness, extra-
to trauma and older adults has focused on clinical sam- version, agreeableness, and neuroticism (OCEAN).
ples of individuals sufering from psychopathology or Neuroticism is closely associated with resilience across
dysfunction, relatively little is known about the ways the lifespan (van Os et al., 2001; Ong et al., 2006).
in which resilience is attained or fostered. he follow- Neuroticism relects a person’s stress tolerance, sta-
ing section provides an overview of the current evidence bility of emotions, and ability to adapt in response to
related to resilience following trauma and describes how change and evolving environmental demands (Costa
it may apply to the growing population of older adults. & McCrae, 1992). Not unexpectedly, current research

141
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

indicates that people with poor stress tolerance are less trauma (e.g., incarceration in a concentration camp or
resilient than those who are highly adaptive (van Os prisoner of war camp) (Norris et al., 2002b).
et al., 2001; Ong & Bergeman, 2004; Ong et al., 2006). When seeking to understand the responses of older
McCrae and Costa (2003, pp. 218–219) noted that adults to traumatic events, it is also important to rec-
“Neuroticism is the aspect of personality most relevant ognize that other stressful events may have occurred
to adjustment, and those high on this dimension are earlier in life and have adversely afected the person’s
likely to show evidence of maladjustment at all ages… developmental trajectory. Earlier traumatic events
and more likely to use inefective coping mechanisms that can later increase vulnerability of older adults
such as hostile reactions, passivity, wishful thinking, include those that occurred during childhood (e.g.,
and self-blame in dealing with stress.” child abuse), early in adulthood (e.g., military com-
A variety of pathways have been proposed that bat), or during older adulthood (e.g., elder abuse,
could result in resilience (Bonanno et al., 2005a). traumatic loss). For the current cohort of older adults,
Bonanno (2004) identiied several distinct dimensions the lack of knowledge regarding the potential psycho-
related to resilience: hardiness, self-enhancement, logical and developmental impact of traumatic events
repressive coping, and positive emotions. He asserted that occurred during their youth may have reduced
that hardiness is characterized by three dimensions, the possibility of seeking and obtaining appropriate
including “being committed to inding meaningful post-trauma attention and care. Older individuals may
purpose in life, the belief that one can inluence one’s have been encouraged to minimize or keep their trau-
surroundings and the outcome of events, and the matic experiences private and may have been discour-
belief that one can learn and grow from both positive aged from positive opportunities to share, potentially
and negative life experiences” (Bonanno, 2004, p. 25). recover, and to grow from their experiences.
Self-enhancement was described as an overly If trauma was experienced during middle adult-
positive and sometimes unrealistic self-perception hood or older adulthood, the afected person may have
(Bonanno, 2004). It has been suggested that self-bias encountered other beneits or challenges. For example,
may be helpful in promoting well-being following during middle adulthood, many people are at the height
trauma, but that it is also associated with more negative of their productivity as well as at their greatest burden
qualities such as narcissism (Paulhus, 1998; Bonanno, threshold. Middle-aged adults are oten working, car-
2004). As noted above, personality traits are relatively ing for children, and sometimes assisting with the care
stable over the lifespan. Personality styles likely inlu- needs of aging parents. A traumatic event during this
ence interactions with the social environment, cop- time is oten characterized by tremendous respon-
ing, and adaptation. How one perceives or cognitively sibility and demands that may lead to more negative
appraises a traumatic event can magnify or minimize responses. his experience is consistent with the bur-
the degree of stress and shape the meaning a person den hypothesis as described above.
may attach to the event. It is a complex interaction Traumas during older adulthood bring yet another
among personal and environmental factors that evolve set of possible outcomes that may be inluenced by
with life experience and aging. the developmental characteristics of this time period.
Consistent with hypotheses discussed above, individ-
Trauma-specific variables uals who experience trauma during older adulthood
may respond resiliently or recover relatively rapidly as
here are a number of variables speciic to the particu-
a result of their prior successful experience with such
lar trauma that may lead to more or less favorable out-
traumatic events. Others may be less likely to ward of
comes among older adults, including the timing and
the negative efects of trauma because of decreases in
severity of the trauma. he type of trauma experienced
physical or cognitive functioning, which may be com-
may have an impact on the response of older adult sur-
pounded by fewer social and economic resources.
vivors (e.g., elder abuse as opposed to vehicle accident).
he severity of a trauma is thought to impact responses
among survivors across the lifespan (Yehuda et al., Social support
1995). Some have suggested that emotional responses A great deal of research has focused on the role of social
to single traumatic events (e.g., natural disaster) may dif- support as an important factor related to an individ-
fer from those experienced following more prolonged ual’s ability to cope following stressful life events. he

142
Chapter 9: Resilience in older adults

issue of social support may be particularly import- Because it is likely that resilience is multifaceted and
ant when considering the impact of trauma on older related to a number of variables, including genetic
adults, as much has been written about the efects of underpinnings, environmental context, personality,
diminishing social networks as part of the aging pro- social support and relationships, and culture, a com-
cess. Death, relocation (e.g., move to a nursing home prehensive biopsychosocial framework measured with
or retirement community), and increased functional multiple methods would likely yield the most fruitful
limitations (e.g., inability to drive or ambulate without information on resilience in older adults (Inui, 2003).
assistance) may reduce contact with signiicant others Understanding how to maintain and enhance resili-
and close friends. ence in the face of daily life hassles, stressful events, and
Norris and Murrell (1990) found that social net- major traumas is an important area to address in future
works bufer the efects of bereavement in older adults. investigations. Because recent research has suggested
In a sample of community-residing older adults, for- that some people may use maladaptive coping strat-
mal volunteering was associated with more positive egies to be resilient (Bonanno, 2004), it is important for
afect and moderated the negative efect of having role- clinical researchers to develop healthful interventions
identity absences in major life domains (e.g., partner, for use by at-risk populations. Lamond et al. (2008)
employment, and parental) on feelings of purpose in suggested the need for investigations of psychosocial
life (Greenield & Marks, 2004). herefore, appropri- and behavioral interventions that may help to teach
ately seeking social support and formally volunteering resilience in older adults. Speciically, they recom-
may be ways to help older adults to feel engaged and mended that attention be paid to interventions aimed
connected and thus possibly more resilient. at enhancing problem-solving skills, such as problem-
solving therapy.
Society’s response to the trauma Shortly ater the 9/11 terror attacks, the American
Psychological Association produced a fact sheet
he response of society to a particular traumatic
intended to assist psychologists who were seeking to
event can signiicantly impact post-trauma outcomes.
foster resilience in older adults (Zeiss et al., 2003).
Although limited empirical research has focused on
Suggested tips for promoting resilience in critical
this particular topic, existing evidence suggests the
times included maintaining a routine, taking good
potential importance of society’s reaction in the ater-
care of oneself, engaging in pleasurable activities, ind-
math of trauma (Danieli, 1994; Solomon, 1995). History
ing supportive people, setting a plan and following
attests to the fact that some survivors are revered and
through, and inding meaning or purpose. Although it
treated as heroes, while others receive societal dis-
is important to teach older adults how to handle their
approval or indiference. For example, World War
reactions to a major catastrophic event, it is equally
II veterans were largely treated with admiration and
important to teach older adults how to be resilient
respect by American society, even being referred
when encountering everyday stressors.
to as “the greatest generation.” In contrast, many of
Lastly, a concept that may be related to resilience
those who served in the Vietnam War returned to the
is the notion of post-traumatic growth (Westphal &
USA to chanting protestors and an indiferent citi-
Bonanno, 2007). Unlike recovery, which is an even-
zenry. Accounts from Holocaust survivors in both the
tual return to baseline functioning with or without
USA and Israel indicate that they oten encountered
intervention, or resilience, which is the capability to
a disbelieving society and people who discouraged
resist or remain undamaged by stressful or traumatic
them from sharing the horrors of their experiences
events, post-traumatic growth is deined by Tedeschi
(Solomon, 1995).
and Calhoun (2004) as a positive adaptation or devel-
opment that results in event-related personal growth.
Future research and clinical directions hey suggested that fewer changes in growth following
A great deal of work remains to fully understand how trauma may be evident in older adults because they may
resilience is manifested in older adults. Research needs be less open to change and may have already learned
to be conducted that investigates prevalence, phenom- many life lessons. Additional research is needed to
enology, and assessment and that determines the best determine the conditions under which post-traumatic
methods for fostering resilience in an aging population. growth may occur in older adults.

143
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B. & Nesse, R. M. (2004).


Conclusions Prospective patterns of resilience and maladjustment
During the course of a lifetime, most older adults will during widowhood. Psychology and Aging, 19, 260–271.
experience at least one stressful or traumatic event. At Bonanno, G. A., Rennicke, C., & Dekel, S. (2005a). Self-
present, there is an emphasis on screening and treat- enhancement among high-exposure survivors of the
ing those who have been exposed to a major stres- September 11th terrorist attack: Resilience or social
sor. However, recent research suggests that it may be maladjustment? Journal of Personality and Social
beneicial to foster resilience in populations prior to Psychology, 88, 984–998.
a stressor (e.g., strengthening social support in pre- Bonanno, G. A., Moskowitz, J. T., Papa, A., & Folkman, S.
paring for the death of a loved one) and to create and (2005b). Resilience to loss in bereaved spouses, bereaved
support recovery environments ater an event (e.g., parents, and bereaved gay men. Journal of Personality and
assisting people with chronic health conditions who Social Psychology, 88, 827–843.
might be at greater risk for adverse outcomes). A hall- Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D.
mark of successful aging is the ability to adapt and (2007). What predicts psychological resilience ater
recover from adverse life events. As the older adult disaster? he role of demographics, resources, and life
stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75,
population continues to grow and diversify, the need 671–682.
to enhance resilience in this subgroup of the popula-
Bremner, J. D., Southwick, S., Brett, E., et al. (1992).
tion takes on new importance.
Dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder
in Vietnam combat veterans. American Journal of
References Psychiatry, 149, 328–332.
Aldwin, C. M., Levenson, M. R., & Spiro, A. (1994). Breslau, N., Kessler, R. C., Chilcoat, H. D., et al. (1998).
Vulnerability and resilience to combat exposure: Can Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the
stress have lifelong efects? Psychology and Aging, 9, community: he 1996 Detroit area survey of trauma.
34–44. American Journal of Psychiatry, 55, 626–632.
American Psychological Association (1997). What Brown, L. M., Rothman, M., & Norris, F. (2007a). Issues
practitioners should know about working with older adults. in mental health care for older adults during disasters.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Generations, 31, 25–30.
Press. Brown, L. M., Cohen, D., & Kohlmaier, J. (2007b). Older adults
Beach, S. R., Schulz, R., Yee, J. L., & Jackson, S. (2000). and terrorism. In B. Bongar, L. M. Brown., L. Beutler,
Negative and positive health efects of caring for a P. Zimbardo, & J. Breckenridge (eds.), Psychology of
disabled spouse: Longitudinal indings from the terrorism (pp. 288–310). New York: Oxford University Press.
caregiver health efects study. Psychology and Aging, 15, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention & the Merck
259–271. Company Foundation (2007). Public health and aging:
Becker, G. & Newsom, E. (2005). Resilience in the face of trends in aging: United States and worldwide. Morbidity
serious illness among chronically ill African Americans and Mortality Weekly Report, 52, 101–106.
in later life. Journals of Gerontology, 60B, S214–S223. Charles, S. T., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003).
Boardman, J. D., Blalock, C. L., & Button, T. M. (2008). Sex Aging and emotional memory: he forgettable nature of
diferences in the heritability of resilience.Twin Research negative images for older adults. Journal of Experimental
and Human Genetics, 11, 12–27. Psychology, 132, 310–324.
Boerner, K., Wortman, C. B., & Bonanno, G. A. (2005). Connor, K. M. & Davidson, J. R. T. (2003). Development of
Resilient or at risk? A 4-year study of older adults who a new resilience scale: he Connor–Davidson Resilience
initially showed high or low distress following conjugal Scale (CD-RISC). Depression and Anxiety, 18, 76–82.
loss. Journals of Gerontology, 60B, 67–73. Cook, J. M. & Elmore, D. L. (2009). Disaster mental health
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: in older adults: Symptoms, policy and planning. In
Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive Y. Neria, S. Galea, & F. Norris (eds.), he mental health
ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, consequences of disasters (pp. 233–263). New York:
59, 20–28. Cambridge University Press.
Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., Lehman, D. R., et al. Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO
(2002). Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A prospective Personality Inventory (NEO–PI–R) and NEO Five-Factor
study from pre-loss to 18 months post-loss. Journal of Inventory (NEO–FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL:
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1150–1164. Psychological Assessment Resources.

144
Chapter 9: Resilience in older adults

Creamer, M. & Parslow, R. (2008). Trauma exposure Greenield, E. A. & Marks, N. F. (2004). Formal volunteering
and posttraumatic stress disorder in the elderly: A as a protective factor for older adults’ psychological well-
community prevalence study. American Journal of being. Journals of Gerontology, 59B, S258–S264.
Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 853–856. Hardy, S. E., Concato, J., & Gill, T. M. (2004). Resilience
Danieli, Y. (1994). As survivors age: Part 1. National Center in community-dwelling older persons. Journal of the
for PTSD Clinical Quarterly, 4, 1, 3–7. American Geriatrics Society, 52, 257–262.
Danieli, Y. (1997). As survivors age: An overview. Journal of Hughes, D. C., Blazer, D. G., & George, L. K. (1988). Age
Geriatric Psychiatry, 30, 9–26. diferences in life events: A multivariate controlled
analysis. International Journal of Aging and Human
Dougall, A. L., Herberman, H. B., Delahanty, D. L., Inslicht,
Development, 27, 207–220.
S. S., & Baum, A. (2000). Similarity of prior trauma
exposure as a determinant of chronic stress responding Inui, T. S. (2003). he need for an integrated biopsychosocial
to an airline disaster. Journal of Consulting and Clinical approach to research on successful aging. Annals of
Psychology, 68, 290–295. Internal Medicine, 139, 391–394.
Engdahl, B. E., Harkness, A. R., Eberly, R. E., Page, W. F., John, O. P. & Srivastava, S. (1999). he big-ive trait
& Bielinski, J. (1993). Structural models of captivity taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical
trauma, resilience, and trauma response among perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (eds.),
former prisoners of war 20 to 40 years after release. Handbook of personality: heory and research, Vol. 2 (pp.
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 28, 102–138). New York: Guilford Press.
109–115. Jopp, D. & Rott, C. (2006). Adaptation in very old age:
Eysenck, H. J. (1983). Stress, disease, and personality: he Exploring the role of resources, beliefs, and attitudes
inoculation efect. In C. L. Cooper (ed.), Stress research for centenarians’ happiness. Psychology and Aging, 21,
(pp. 121–146). New York: Wiley. 266–280.
Felten, B. S. (2000). Resilience in a multicultural sample of Kilijanek, T. S. & Drabek, T. E. (1979). Assessing long-term
community-dwelling women older than age 85. Clinical impacts of a natural disaster: A focus on the elderly.
Nursing Research, 9, 102–123. Gerontologist, 19, 555–566.
Fields, R. B. (1996). Severe stress in the elderly: Are Kinsel, B. (2005). Resilience as adaptation in older women.
older adults at increased risk for posttraumatic stress Journal of Women and Aging, 17, 23–39.
disorder? In P. E. Ruskin & J. A. Talbott (eds.), Aging and Knight, B. G., Gatz, M., Heller, K., & Bengtson, V. L.
posttraumatic stress disorder (pp. 79–100). Washington, (2000). Age and emotional response to the Northridge
DC: American Psychiatric Press. earthquake: A longitudinal study. Psychology and Aging,
Foster, J. R. (1997). Successful coping, adaptation 15, 627–634.
and resilience in the elderly: An interpretation of Lamet, A., Szuchman, L., Perkel, L., & Walsh, S. (2009). Risk
epidemiologic data. Psychiatric Quarterly, 68, 189–219. factors, resilience, and psychological distress among
Frieswijk, N., Buunk, B. P., Steverink, N., & Slaets, J. P. J. holocaust and nonholocaust survivors in the post-9/11
(2004). the interpretation of social comparison and its environment. Educational Gerontology, 35, 32–46.
relation to life satisfaction among elderly people: Does Lamond, A. J., Depp, C. A., Allison, M., et al. (2008).
frailty make a diference? Journal of Gerontology, 59, Measurement and predictors of resilience among
P250–P257. community-dwelling older women. Journal of Psychiatric
Gallo, J. J. & Lebowitz, B. D. (1999). he epidemiology of Research, 43, 148–154.
common late-life mental disorders in the community: Lawson, E. J. & homas, C. (2007). Wading in the waters:
themes for the new century. Psychiatric Services, 50, Spirituality and older Black Katrina survivors. Journal of
1158–1168. Health Care for the Poor and Underserved, 18, 341–354.
Gaugler, J. E., Kane, R. L., & Newcomer, R. (2007). McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in
Resilience and transitions from dementia caregiving. adulthood: A ive-factor theory perspective. New York:
Journals of Gerontology, 62B, P38–P44. Guilford Press.
Garmezy, N. (1991). Resilience and vulnerability to adverse Montross, L. P., Depp, C., Daly, J., et al. (2006). Correlates of
developmental outcomes associated with poverty. self-rated successful aging among community-dwelling
American Behavioral Scientist, 34, 416–430. older adults. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14,
Green, B. L., Goodman, L. A., Krupnick, J. L., et al. (2000). 43–51.
Outcomes of single versus multiple trauma exposure Morey, L. C. (2007). Personality Assessment Inventory,
in a screening sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
271–286. Assessment Resources.

145
Section 2: Resilience across the lifespan

Nishith, P., Mechanic, M. B., &. Resnick, P. A. (2000). Prior Phifer, J. F. (1990). Psychological distress and somatic
interpersonal trauma: he contribution to current PTSD symptoms ater natural disaster: Diferential vulnerability
symptoms in female rape victims. Journal of Abnormal among older adults. Psychology and Aging, 5, 412–420.
Psychology, 109, 20–25. Phifer, J. F. & Norris, F. H. (1989). Psychological symptoms
Norris, F. H. (1992). Epidemiology of trauma: Frequency in older adults following natural disaster: Nature,
and impact of diferent potentially traumatic events on timing, duration, and course. Journals of Gerontology, 44,
diferent demographic groups. Journal of Consulting and S207–S217.
Clinical Psychology, 60, 409–418. Robins, L. N. & Regier, D. A. (1991). Psychiatric disorders in
Norris, F. H. & Murrell, S. A. (1988). Prior experience as a America: he Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study. New
moderator of disaster impact on anxiety symptoms in York: Free Press.
older adults. American Journal of Community Psychology, Rutter, M. (1999). Resilience concepts and indings:
16, 665–683. Implications for family therapy. Journal of Family
Norris, F. H. & Murrell, S. A. (1990). Social support, life events, herapy, 21, 119–144.
and stress as modiiers of adjustment to bereavement by Schulz, R., Mittelmark, M., Burton, L., Hirsch, C., &
older adults. Psychology and Aging, 5, 429–436. Jackson, S. (1997). Health efects of caregiving: he
Norris, F. H., Kaniasty, K. Z., Conrad, M. L., Inman, G. L., & caregiver health efects study: An ancillary study of
Murphy, A. D. (2002a). Placing age diferences in cultural the cardiovascular health study. Annals of Behavioral
context: A comparison of the efects of age on PTSD Medicine, 19, 110–116.
ater disasters in the United States, Mexico, and Poland. Shalev, A. Y. (2002). Acute stress reactions in adults.
Journal of Clinical Geropsychology, 8, 153–173. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 532–543.
Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., & Watson, P. J. (2002b). Solomon, Z. (1995). From denial to recognition: Attitudes
60 000 disaster victims speak: Part II. Summary and toward Holocaust survivors form World War II to the
implications of the disaster mental health research. present. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 215–228.
Psychiatry, 65, 240–260.
Solomon, Z. & Prager, E. (1992). Elderly Israeli Holocaust
Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., Watson, P. J., et al. (2002c). survivors during the Persian Gulf War: A study of
60 000 disaster victims speak: Part I. An empirical review psychological distress. American Journal of Psychiatry,
of the empirical literature, 1981–2001. Psychiatry, 65, 149, 1707–1710.
207–239.
Tedeschi, R. G. & Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Posttraumatic
Nygren, B., Alex, L., Jonsen, E., et al. (2005). Resilience, sense growth: Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence.
of coherence, purpose in life and self-transcendence in Psychological Inquiry, 15, 1–18.
relation to perceived physical and mental health among
the oldest old. Aging and Mental Health, 9, 354–362. hompson, M. P., Norris, F. H., & Hanacek, B. (1993).
Age diferences in the psychological consequences of
Ong, A. D. & Bergeman, C. S. (2004). he complexity of Hurricane Hugo. Psychology and Aging, 8, 606–616.
emotions in later life. Journals of Gerontology, 59B,
P117–P122. US Department of Health and Human Services (1999).
Mental Health: A report of the Surgeon General. Rockville,
Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., & Bisconti, T. L. (2004). MD: US Department of Health and Human Services.
he role of daily positive emotions during conjugal
bereavement. Journals of Gerontology, 59B, P168–P176. US Department of Health and Human Services (2001).
Older adults and mental health: Issues and opportunities.
Ong, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., Wallace, K. A., & Bisconti, Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human
T. L. (2006). Psychological resilience, positive emotions, Services, Administration on Aging.
and successful adaptation to the stress in later life.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, US Federal Interagency Forum on Aging-Related Statistics
730–749. (2008). Older Americans 2008: Key indicators of well-
being. Washington, DC: Government Printing Oice.
Ott, C. H., Lueger, R. J., Kelber, S. T., & Prigerson, H.
G. (2007). Spousal bereavement in older adults: van Os, J., Park, S. B. G., & Jones, P. B. (2001). Neuroticism,
Common, resilient, and chronic grief with deining life events and mental health: Evidence for person–
characteristics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, environment correlation. British Journal of Psychiatry,
195, 332–341. 178, s72–s77.
Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic Werner, E. E. (1995). Resilience in development. Current
adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A mixed Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 81–85.
blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, Westphal, M. & Bonanno, G. A. (2007). Posttraumatic
1197–1208. growth and resilience to trauma: Diferent sides of the

146
Chapter 9: Resilience in older adults

same coin or diferent coins? Applied Psychology: An Zeiss, A. M., Cook, J. M., & Cantor, D. W. (2003). Fostering
International Review, 56, 416–426. resilience in response to terrorism: For psychologists
Yehuda, R., Kahana, B., Schmeidler, J., et al. (1995). Impact working with older adults. [Report to American
of cumulative lifetime trauma and recent stress on Psychological Association Task Force on Resilience in
current posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in Response to Terrorism.] Washington, DC: American
Holocaust survivors. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, Psychological Association Press, http://www.apa.org/pi/
1815–1818. aging/older-adults.pdf (accessed March 12, 2011).

147
Section
Section3 Resilience in families, communities, and societies
Chapter
Family resilience: a collaborative approach

10 in response to stressful life challenges


Froma Walsh

Introduction circumstances, dealing with chronic illness, or recov-


ering from catastrophic life events, trauma, and loss.
Family resilience can be deined as the ability of
Resilience came to be seen in terms of an interplay
families to withstand and rebound from disruptive life
of multiple risk and protective processes over time,
challenges, strengthened and more resourceful (Walsh,
involving individual, family, and larger sociocultural
2003, 2006). Building on studies of family stress, cop-
inluences (Rutter, 1987). Individual vulnerability or
ing, and adaptation, and research on well-functioning
the impact of stressful conditions could be outweighed
family systems, family resilience is seen to involve
by mediating inluences.
dynamic processes that foster positive adaptation of
he crucial inluence of signiicant relationships
the family unit and its members in the context of sig-
stood out across studies. Reports of children who
niicant adversity (Walsh, 1996; Luthar et al., 2000).
thrived despite a parent’s mental illness or maltreat-
his chapter presents core principles of resilience
ment noted that their resilience was oten nurtured in
derived from a family systems orientation, highlight-
strong bonds with mentors, such as coaches and teach-
ing sociocultural and developmental perspectives. he
ers, who had invested in the child. Yet, the narrow focus
assessment of family resilience is described, identify-
on parental deicits led many to dismiss the family as
ing key processes that can be targeted to strengthen
dysfunctional and to only consider outside resources.
family resilience in intervention and prevention
In mental health and child development literature,
eforts. Program applications in training and services
families were thought to contribute to risk, but not to
are described briely to illustrate the broad utility of
resilience.
this systemic approach for intervention and preven-
Family systems theory, research, and practice have
tion eforts.
broadened our recognition of the potential family
resources for individual resilience in the network of
Individual resilience: a systemic relationships, from couple and sibling bonds to rela-
perspective tionships with extended family members. Family
assessment and intervention with troubled and at-risk
Most resilience theory, research, and intervention
youth aim to identify and involve family members who
approaches since the early 1980s have been individu-
are invested in them and can support their best eforts,
ally focused, relecting the dominant medical and
believe in their potential, and encourage them to make
mental health paradigm and the cultural ethos of the
the most of their lives (Ungar, 2004). Even in multi-
“rugged individual” (Walsh, 1996). Early studies of
stressed families, strengths and potential can be found
resilience sought to identify innate or acquired per-
alongside vulnerabilities and limitations (Walsh, 2003,
sonal traits that made some children less vulnerable to
2006).
the impact of extreme conditions or the damage of par-
ental pathology.
An interactive view of resilience emerged as The concept of family resilience
research was extended to a wide range of adverse he concept of family resilience expands focus beyond
conditions – such as growing up in impoverished a dyadic view – seeing a family member as a resource

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

149
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

for individual resilience – to a systemic perspective on render some families or communities more at risk.
risk and resilience in the family as a functional unit Symptoms of family members may be generated by a
(Walsh, 1996). A basic premise is that stressful life crisis event, such as a sexual assault or a tragic loss, or
challenges have an impact on the whole family and, in by the wider impact of a large-scale disaster. Family
turn, key family processes mediate the recovery – or distress is exacerbated by unsuccessful attempts to
maladaptation – of all members and the family unit. cope with an overwhelming situation, whatever its
he family response to adversity is crucial. Major stres- source. he family, peer group, community resources,
sors can derail the functioning of a family system, with school, work setting, and other social systems are seen
ripple efects for all members and their relationships. as nested contexts for nurturing and sustaining resili-
Key processes and extrafamilial resources enable the ence. A multidimensional, holistic approach addresses
family system to rally in times of crisis, to bufer stress, the varied contexts, identiies common elements in a
to reduce the risk of dysfunction, and to support opti- crisis situation, and also takes into account each fam-
mal adaptation. ily’s unique perspectives, resources, and challenges.
he concept of family resilience extends theory
and research on family stress, coping, and adaptation Family transformations in changing
(McCubbin & Patterson, 1983; Boss, 2001; Patterson,
2002). It entails more than managing stressful condi- societies
tions, shouldering a burden, or surviving an ordeal. he concept of family resilience is particularly timely as
It involves the potential for personal and relational our world grows increasingly unpredictable and fam-
transformation and growth that can be forged out of ilies face unprecedented challenges. With profound
adversity. Tapping into key processes for resilience, social, economic, and political upheavals over recent
families that have been struggling can emerge stronger decades, families have been undergoing rapid trans-
and more resourceful in meeting future challenges. formation (Walsh, 2012a, 2012b). To understand fam-
Members may develop new insights and abilities. A ily challenges and resources, it is important to consider
crisis can be a wake-up call, heightening their attention the implications of the following trends: varied family
to core values and important matters. It oten becomes structures and changing gender roles, increasing cul-
an opportunity for families to reappraise life prior- tural diversity and economic disparity, and varying
ities and stimulates greater investment in meaningful expanded family life course. Eforts to strengthen fam-
relationships. In studies of strong families (Stinnett & ily resilience must be relevant to this growing diversity
DeFrain, 1985), many report that through weathering and complexity.
a crisis together their relationships were enriched and
became more loving than they might otherwise have Varied family structures and gender roles
been. Over the past century, multigenerational kinship net-
works living in close proximity in traditional rural
societies have fragmented with industrialization,
Sociocultural and developmental urbanization, and modernization. Amidst economic
contexts of family resilience and social changes of recent decades, the intact nuclear
A family resilience framework is grounded in fam- family with traditional gender roles, idealized in mid-
ily systems theory, combining ecological and devel- twentieth century modern society, currently accounts
opmental perspectives to view family functioning in for only a small band on the spectrum of contempor-
relation to its broader sociocultural context and multi- ary families. Increasingly, individuals and couples
generational family life cycle passage. construct a wide variety of household and kinship
Risk and resilience are viewed in light of multiple, arrangements and eschew traditional patriarchal gen-
recursive inluences involving individuals, families, and der patterns in favor of a more egalitarian partnership
larger social systems. hese can be seen from biological, in marriage, family life, and childrearing. Increasing
psychological, social, or spiritual system orientation. rates of cohabitation, divorce, single-parent house-
Symptoms of distress may be primarily biologically holds, stepfamilies, and gay/lesbian-headed families
based, as in serious illness or neurological vulnerabil- has led to concerns about their pathogenic inluence
ities, but are also inluenced by sociocultural variables, for children. However, since the late 1990s, research
such as barriers of poverty and discrimination that has clearly shown that families can function well and

150
Chapter 10: Family resilience

children can thrive in a variety of family structures that involving emotional, behavioral, and substance abuse
are stable, nurturing, and inancially secure (Lansford problems (Figley, 1998a). Ripple efects throughout
et al., 2001; Oswald, 2002; Johnson-Garner & Meyers, the family network produced distress for children and
2003; Green, 2004; Greene et al., 2012). What matters other family members. Family involvement in treat-
most are efective family processes. ment was deemed essential to address this family stress
and, in turn, to enable the family to support the optimal
Cultural diversity and economic disparity recovery of the returned serviceperson. Currently, with
Cultural norms and values shape family belief systems, the high rate of PTSD, suicide, traumatic brain injury,
organization, and communication. McCubbin et al. other severe injuries, and marital distress for deployed
(1998) have studied varied pathways in resilience in and returning combat troops, eforts to strengthen
ethnic minority, native, and immigrant families. As family resilience are a high priority in programs for US
societies become more culturally diverse, families are military families (MacDermid, 2010).
becoming increasingly multiethnic, multiracial, and Family resilience-oriented services are also becom-
multifaith, requiring mutual understanding and accept- ing a valuable approach in community recovery eforts
ance of diferences (Walsh, 2009a). Socioeconomic in the wake of natural disasters, such as Hurricane
conditions strongly impact family risk and resilience. Katrina, and in regions sufering ongoing conlict,
A wide disparity between the rich and the poor have political persecution, and/or terrorist attacks (Cohen
severe consequences for the welfare of growing num- et al., 2002; Hernandez, 2002; Landau, 2007; Walsh,
bers of families worldwide. Declining economic con- 2007; Girwitz et al., 2008; Rowe & Liddle, 2008; Vigil &
ditions, job dislocation, and persistent unemployment Geary, 2008; Knowles et al., 2010).
have a devastating impact on family functioning and
stability, fueling conlict, violence, divorce, and home- Developmental perspective: family
lessness. Conditions of discrimination, neighborhood
decay, poor schools, crime, violence, and inadequate life cycle orientation
healthcare worsen life chances for parents and their A family life cycle orientation views the family as a sys-
children. tem moving forward over the life course of all members
and across the generations (McGoldrick et al., 2011).
Varying, expanded life course his developmental perspective is essential to under-
Medical advances and the aging of societies have stand and foster family resilience: (1) families navigate
increased the number of four- and ive-generation varied pathways in resilience with emerging challenges
families, with concomitant challenges of chronic ill- over time; (2) a pile-up of multiple stressors can over-
ness and caregiving (Walsh, 2011). Two or more com- whelm family resources; (3) the impact of a crisis may
mitted couple relationships are increasingly common vary in relation to its timing in individual and family
over time, with periods of cohabitation and single liv- life cycle passage; and (4) a family’s past experiences
ing. Children and their parents are likely to transition of adversity and response can generate catastrophic
in and out of several household and kinship arrange- expectations or can serve as models of resilience.
ments over their life course. For resilience, families
need to bufer transitions and learn how to live success- Varied pathways in resilience
fully in complex arrangements. Most major stressors are not simply a short-term sin-
gle event but rather a complex set of changing condi-
Stressful events in the broader social tions with a past history and a future course (Rutter,
1987). Family resilience involves varied adaptational
context: family impact and resilience pathways over time, from the approach to a threaten-
In the early study of family stress, the family impact of ing event, through disruptive transitions, subsequent
combat-related trauma in returning military veterans shockwaves in the immediate atermath, and long-term
was a focal concern (Figley, 1989). Secondary trau- reorganization. For example, in adaptation to loss, the
matization and burnout were experienced by spouses, immediate and long-term recovery for all members and
not only in hearing about their loved one’s past trauma their relationships will be inluenced by how a family
but even more through ongoing interactions impacted approaches the loss, facilitates emotional sharing and
by post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms meaning-making, efectively reorganizes, and fosters

151
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

reinvestment in life pursuits (Walsh & McGoldrick, change, and losses over the generations (Beavers &
2004). Likewise, the experience of divorce proceeds Hampson, 2003). his perspective helps members to
from an escalation of pre-divorce tensions through see disruptive events and transitions also as milestones
disruption and reorganization of households and par- on their shared life passage.
ent–child relationships; most experience transitional A family resilience approach focuses on family
upheaval again with remarriage and stepfamily inte- functioning and adaptation around stressful life events
gration (Greene et al., 2012). Given such complexity, and transitions (Lavee et al., 1987; Walsh, 2009a). Some
no single coping response is invariably most successful; researchers have examined variables in risk and resili-
diferent strategies may prove useful in meeting emer- ence around predictable, normative stressors, such as
ging challenges. Some approaches that are functional the transition to parenthood (Cowan & Cowan, 2012).
in the short term may become rigid and dysfunctional More attention is directed to family processes for resili-
over time. For example, with a sudden illness, a family ence associated with the disruptive impact of unantici-
must mobilize resources and pull together to meet the pated, untimely events, such as the multiple transitions
crisis, but later the family must shit gears with chronic with divorce (Gorell Barnes, 1999; Greene et al., 2012),
disability and attend to the needs of other family mem- the death of a child, or loss of a parent raising children
bers over a longer time (Rolland, 1994, 2012). Research (Greef & Human, 2004; Greef & van der Merwe, 2004;
on efective family processes for resilience over time in Walsh, 2009a). here is growing interest in family resili-
the context of loss, divorce, or illness can guide inter- ence with medical crises and adaptation to unfolding
ventions. Brief modules can be timed to help families at phases and transitions in chronic illness and disability
various steps or transitions along their journey, helping (Campbell, 2003; Rolland, 2012).
them to integrate what has happened and meet antici-
pated challenges ahead. Intergenerational legacies
he convergence of developmental and multigen-
Pile-up of stressors erational strains heighten the risk for dysfunction.
Some families may do well with a short-term crisis Distress increases exponentially when current stressors
but buckle under the cumulative strains of persistent reactivate painful issues from the past (McGoldrick
or recurrent challenges, as with prolonged joblessness et al., 2011). Unresolved conlicts and losses may sur-
or ongoing complex trauma for those living in a con- face when similar challenges are confronted. Family
lict zone. A pile-up of internal and external stressors members may lose perspective, conlate immediate
can overwhelm the family, heightening vulnerabil- situations with past events, and either become over-
ity and risk for subsequent problems. Multistressed, whelmed by or cut of from painful feelings and con-
underresourced families, most oten in impoverished tacts. Multigenerational anniversary patterns can be
minority communities, are oten blamed for their identiied: many families function well until they reach
diiculties. In contrast to problem-focused interven- a point in the life cycle passage that had been traumatic
tions, which too oten increase parents’ sense of deicit a generation earlier, particularly when a child reaches
and despair, recent strengths-oriented family therapy the same age that a parent experienced trauma. Family
approaches are intended to airm and enhance family stories of past adversity and how they inluence future
conidence and competencies (Combrinck-Graham, expectations, from catastrophic fears to a hopeful out-
2005; Minuchin et al., 2006; Walsh, 2006). Intervention look, can be illuminating. Legacies of resilience can
approaches using a strengths-oriented family system also be found in adaptive response to adversity across
also treat complex problems of family violence and sex- the generations (Hauser, 1999).
ual abuse, oten intertwined with substance abuse (e.g.,
Sheinberg & Fraenkel, 2001). Assessing family functioning and
Stressful transitions resilience
Functioning and symptoms of distress are assessed in he assessment of family functioning is fraught
the context of the multigenerational family system as it with dilemmas (Walsh, 2012b). Views of normality
moves forward across the life cycle (McGoldrick et al., and health are socially constructed. Clinicians and
2011). Well-functioning families tend to have an evolu- researchers bring their own assumptive maps into every
tionary sense of time and a continual process of growth, evaluation and intervention, embedded in cultural

152
Chapter 10: Family resilience

norms, professional orientations, and personal expe- should be explored in terms of (1) their impact on the
riences. Moreover, with recent social and economic family system, its members, and their relationships;
transformations and a growing multiplicity of family and (2) how the family attempted to handle the prob-
kinship arrangements, no single model of family lem situation, that is their proactive stance, immediate
health or resilience its all families and situations. response, and long-term strategies.
Since the late 1980s, systems-oriented family process Whereas genograms are most oten used to focus
research has provided important empirical grounding on problematic family patterns, such as illness, con-
for assessment of healthy family functioning (Beavers lict, or estrangement, a resilience-oriented approach
& Hampson, 2003; Epstein et al., 2003; Olson & Gorall, also searches for positive inluences, past, present, and
2003). Yet, family typologies tend to be static and potential. Inquiry is made about resourceful ways a
acontextual; many ofer a snapshot of interaction pat- family had dealt with past adversity and about models
terns within the family but lack multisystemic and of resilience in the kin network that might be drawn on
developmental perspectives in relation to family chal- to inspire eforts to master current challenges. Families
lenges, resources, and constraints. Since families most are also encouraged to make eforts to repair conlicts
oten seek help in periods of crisis, clinicians must be and reconnect with estranged loved ones who could
cautious not to label their distress relexively or to see contribute to child and family resilience.
diferences from research norms as family pathology.
A family resilience framework ofers several advan- The Walsh family resilience framework:
tages. By deinition, it focuses on family strengths
under stress, in crisis, and facing adversity (Walsh, key processes
2003). Second, it is assumed that no single model of he Walsh family resilience framework (Box 10.1) was
healthy functioning its all families or their situations. developed as a conceptual map of key processes to guide
Functioning is assessed in context: relative to each assessment and intervention in clinical and community
family’s values, structure, resources, and life chal- practice. his framework is informed by two decades of
lenges. Families forge varied pathways through adver- social science and clinical research on well-functioning
sity, itting their cultural orientation, their emerging family systems and on individual and family resilience.
challenges, their life cycle passage, and their personal Synthesizing indings, nine processes for resilience are
strengths and resources. identiied within three domains of family functioning:
family belief systems, organization patterns, and com-
munication processes. hese processes can be assessed
Use of genograms, timelines and targeted to strengthen family capacities to rebound
Individual and family symptoms of distress are assessed from crises and master persistent life challenges (Walsh,
in sociocultural and developmental contexts. A family 2003, 2006). Interventions aim to build family strengths
genogram and timeline are essential tools for clinical as problems are addressed, thereby reducing risk and
assessment to schematize relationship information, vulnerability. As the family becomes more resourceful,
track systems patterns over time, and guide interven- members gain ability to meet future challenges.
tion planning (McGoldrick et al., 2008).
Particular attention should be paid to the timing of Family belief systems
symptoms of distress: their co-occurrence with recent Family resilience is fostered by shared beliefs that help
or threatening disruptive events in the family (Walsh & members make meaning of their stressful situations,
McGoldrick, 2004). For example, a son dropping out facilitate a positive, hopeful outlook, and provide tran-
from school may be precipitated by his father’s job loss scendent or spiritual, values, practices, and purpose.
and related family tensions, although family members Families can be helped to gain a sense of coherence
may not initially note any connection. Frequently, indi- (Antonovsky & Sourani, 1988; Antonovsky, 1998),
vidual symptoms coincide with stressful transitions, recasting a crisis as a shared challenge that is com-
such as family separation or stepfamily formation, that prehensible, manageable, and meaningful to tackle.
require boundary shits and re-deinition of roles and Normalizing and contextualizing members’ distress
relationships. It is important to attend to non-custodial as natural or understandable in their crisis situation
parents and the extended kin network beyond the can depathologize their reactions and reduce blame,
immediate household. he impact of stressful events shame, and guilt. Drawing out and airming family

153
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

Box 10.1 Key processes in family resilience • build financial security and balance work/family
strains
Belief systems • use institutional supports (workplace,
1. Make meaning of adversity: healthcare, family policy).
• view resilience as relationally based rather than
“rugged individual”
• normalize, contextualize adversity and distress Communication/problem solving
• encourage sense of coherence: view crisis as 7. Clarity:
a meaningful, comprehensible, manageable • clear, consistent messages (words and actions)
challenge
• examine ambiguous information, truth seeking/
• use causal/explanatory attributions: truth speaking.
expectations.
8. Open emotional expression:
2. Stimulate a positive outlook:
• share range of feelings (joy and pain; hopes and
• encourage hope, optimistic bias, confidence in
fears)
overcoming odds
• have mutual empathy; tolerance for differences
• affirm strengths and build on potential
• seize opportunities: active initiative and • encourage pleasurable interactions, humor,
perseverance (can-do spirit) respite.
• master the possible, accept what cannot be 9. Collaborative problem solving/preparedness:
changed, live with uncertainty. • creative brainstorming, resourcefulness
3. Transcendence and spirituality: • shared decision making, conflict management:
• larger values, purpose negotiation, fairness
• spirituality: faith, practices, congregation; • focus on goals, take concrete steps, build on
connectedness with nature success, learn from failure
• inspiration: envision new possibilities, creative • proactive stance: prevent problems, avert crises,
expression, social action prepare for future challenges.
• transformation: learning, change, and growth
from adversity.

Structural/organizational patterns strengths in the midst of diiculties counters a sense of


helplessness, failure, and despair as it reinforces shared
4. Flexibility:
pride, conidence, and a “can do” spirit. he en-courage-
• open to change: rebound, reorganize, adapt to
ment of family members bolsters eforts to take initia-
fit new challenges
tive and persevere in eforts to overcome barriers. For
• find stability during disruption: continuity,
resilience, family members focus eforts on mastering
dependability, follow-through
the possible, accepting that which is beyond their con-
• strong authoritative leadership: nurturance,
trol. Spiritual resources, such as shared faith, practices
protection, guidance.
of prayer or meditation, and religious/congregational
5. Connectedness: involvement strengthen family bonds and resilience
• mutual support, collaboration, and (Walsh, 2009b, 2010).
commitment
• respect individual needs, differences, and Family structure/organization
boundaries Family resilience is fostered by lexible structure, con-
• reconnection, reconciliation of wounded nectedness, mutual support, and teamwork in facing
relationships. life challenges. In navigating disruptive changes and
6. Social and economic resources: structural reorganization, families need to counter-
• mobilize kin, social, and community networks; balance lexibility with stability, particularly reassur-
use models and mentors ing children and other vulnerable family members by
providing security, continuities, and dependability.

154
Chapter 10: Family resilience

Mutual support and teamwork facilitate resilience. Box 10.2 Family resilience orientation: princi-
Extended kin and social networks can be lifelines for ples for practice
support. Institutional policies (e.g., workplace, health-
• Relational view:
care) that support family functioning are vital for dis-
tressed families to thrive.
• a deficit view of families shifted to one of strengths
• families seen as challenged by adversity, with
Family communication processes potential for repair and growth.
Family resilience is facilitated through clear informa- • Systemic orientation:
tion about their adverse situation and options, open • interaction of biological, psychological, social,
emotional expression and pleasurable interactions for and spiritual influences
renewal, and collaborative problem solving. Families • crisis/stress impacts family system: family
become more resourceful when interventions shit response influences recovery of all members,
from a crisis-reactive mode to a proactive stance, relationships, and family unit
anticipating and preparing to meet future challenges. • contextual view of crisis, symptoms of distress,
Most important, interventions help families in prob- and adaptation.
lem-saturated situations to envision a better future and • Developmental perspective:
take concrete steps toward achieving their goals. • timing of symptoms and disruptive family
challenges
Practice applications of a family • pile-up of stressors, persistent adversity
resilience orientation approach • varying adaptational challenges over time, varied
A family resilience orientation can be applied usefully pathways in resilience
with a wide range of crisis situations, disruptive transi- • fit with individual and family developmental
tions, and persistent life challenges. Interventions util- phases/transitions
ize principles and techniques common among many • multigenerational patterns.
strength-based family systems practice approaches,
but attend more centrally to the impact of signiicant
stressors and aim to strengthen the family resources Box 10.3 Practice guidelines to strengthen
and potential for positive adaptation. his approach also family resilience
recognizes that families may forge varied pathways for
• Convey conviction in potential to overcome
resilience over time, depending on their adverse situation
adversity through shared efforts.
and their values, resources, and challenges. Principles
guiding this approach are outlined in Box 10.2. • Use respectful language, framing to humanize and
contextualize distress:
Family resilience-oriented practitioners serve
as compassionate witnesses and facilitators, helping • view as understandable, common in adverse
family members to share with each other their experi- situation (normal response to abnormal
conditions)
ence of adversity; to overcome silence, secrecy, shame,
or blame; and to build mutual empathy and support.
• decrease shame, blame, and pathologizing of
Respect for family strengths in the midst of sufering family and individual members.
readily engages so-called “resistant” families, who • Provide safe haven for family members to share
are oten reluctant to come for mental health services pain, fears, challenges:
out of concerns that they will be judged as disturbed • compassionate witness for suffering and struggle
or deicient and blamed for their problems. Instead, • facilitate their communication, mutual empathy,
family members are respectfully regarded as essential support, collaboration.
members of the healing team for recovery and resili- • Identify and affirm strengths, courage alongside
ence. Where they may have faltered, they are viewed as vulnerabilities, constraints.
struggling with an overwhelming set of challenges and • Draw out potential for mastery, healing, and growth.
their best intentions are airmed. Intervention eforts
• Tap into kin, community, and spiritual resources to
are directed to master those challenges through their master challenges.
shared eforts. (Box 10.3 gives practice guidelines.)

155
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

Box 10.3 (cont.) • serious illness, disability, end-of-life issues, and


caregiving challenges (e.g., “resilient partners” couples
• View crisis as opportunity for learning, change, and groups for couples living with multiple sclerosis)
growth.
• complicated and traumatic loss, healing, and
• Shift focus from problems to possibilities: resilience
• gain mastery, healing, and transformation out of • major disasters: family and community recovery
adversity and resilience
• reorient future hopes and dreams. • refugee trauma and resilience
• Integrate adverse experience – and resilient response • complex trauma and family resilience in war-torn
– into fabric of individual and relational life passage. regions
• positive adaptation with divorce and stepfamily
integration
A family resilience orientation can be valuable in • job loss and economic hardship: family resilience
guiding clinical training, research, and community workshops for displaced workers in partnership
projects. A systemic assessment may lead to a variety with transition services
of interventions or it may combine individual, couple, • family–school partnership programs: for at-risk
family, and multifamily group modalities depending youth
on the relevance of diferent system levels to inter- • gay and lesbian couples and families: overcoming
vention aims. Putting an ecological view into prac- challenges of stigma.
tice, interventions may involve collaborative eforts
of families with community agencies, the workplace, A few brief programmatic descriptions are ofered
schools, healthcare providers, and other larger sys- here from our Center and from the work of colleagues
tems. Resilience-based family interventions can be elsewhere, to illustrate the potential utility of this
adapted to a variety of formats including periodic approach. hese illustrate working with families and
family consultations or more intensive family ther- communities.
apy. Psychoeducational multifamily groups empha- Transitional stresses of job loss
size the importance of social support and practical
information, ofering concrete guidelines for crisis One family resilience-based program addressed transi-
management, problem solving, and stress reduction tional adjustment of displaced workers when jobs were
as families navigate through stressful periods and face lost through factory closure or company downsizing.
future challenges. herapists or group leaders may he Chicago Center designed family resilience-based
help families to clarify speciic stresses they are dealing psychoeducational workshops and counseling ser-
with and to develop efective coping strategies, meas- vices in partnership with a community-based agency
uring success in small increments and maintaining that specialized in job retraining and placement ser-
family morale. Brief, cost-efective psychoeducational vices. Rebounding from job loss involves dealing with
“modules” timed for critical phases of an illness or prolonged transitional stresses of retraining and job
life challenge encourage families to accept and digest search, and then gaining and retaining new employ-
manageable portions of a long-term coping process ment. Job and income loss, as well as anxiety and
(Rolland, 1994). uncertainty about re-employment success, oten fuel
depression, substance abuse, and marital/family con-
Examples of family resilience-based lict. Parental roles have to be realigned and the value of
the displaced worker to the family has to be broadened
programs from “breadwinner,” particularly for men whose self-
Since the early 1990s, this family resilience metaframe- esteem is linked to that role. his pile-up of stress over
work has guided the development of clinical and com- many months, in turn, reduces the ability of spouses
munity-based professional training, consultation, and and family members to support workers’ eforts. In one
services at the Chicago Center for Family Health (www. case, with the closing and relocation of a large cloth-
cchchicago.org). Collaborative programs have been ing manufacturing plant, over 1800 workers lost their
designed to address a wide range of family challenges jobs. Most were African-American or Latino bread-
(Walsh, 2002, 2006, 2007; Rolland & Walsh, 2006): winners for their families; many were single parents,

156
Chapter 10: Family resilience

with limited education or skills for employment in the worlds (Falicov, 2007). To foster collaboration and to
changing job market. Resilience-focused workshops develop local resources, facilitators from their commu-
addressed the personal and familial impact of losses nity were trained to co-lead groups and to be available
and transitional stresses and rallied family members as as urgent needs might arise. his approach was experi-
a resource team to support the best eforts of the dis- enced as respectful and empowering.
placed worker. Group topics focused on worker/family
challenges and keys to resilience, such as identifying Kosovar Family Professional Educational Collaborative
constraining beliefs and negative self-appraisals; iden- he success of the CAFES and TAFES initiative led
tifying and airming strengths, such as pride in doing to development of the Kosovar Family Professional
a job well and qualities of dependability and loyalty. Educational Collaborative (KFPEC), an ongoing
New competencies and beneits were generated by partnership in Kosovo between local mental health
realigning household and childrearing tasks, devel- professionals and several teams of American family
oping mutually supportive bonds, and mobilizing therapists. he aim of this project was to enhance the
extended family and community resources. capacities of mental health professionals and parapro-
fessionals to address the overwhelming service needs
Multifamily groups for refugees in their war-torn region by strengthening family cop-
he value of a community-based family resilience ing and recovery in the wake of widespread trauma and
approach with refugees and in war-torn communi- loss. In describing the value of this approach, Rolland
ties was demonstrated in projects developed by the and Weine (2000, p. 35) noted,
Chicago Center in collaboration with the Center on he family, with its strengths, is central to Kosovar life,
Genocide, Psychiatry, and Witnessing at the University but health and mental health services are generally not
of Illinois (Weine et al., 2004; Walsh, 2006). In 1998– oriented to families. Although “family” is a professed part
1999, multifamily groups were designed for Bosnian of the value system of international organizations, most
and Kosovar refugees who had sufered atrocities and programs do not deine, conceptualize, or operationalize
traumatic loss of loved ones, homes, and communities a family approach to mental health services in any sub-
in the Serbian “ethnic cleansing” campaign. he fam- stantial or meaningful ways. Recognizing that the psy-
ily resilience approach was sought out because many chosocial needs of refugees, other trauma survivors, and
refugees were sufering symptoms of PTSD but were vulnerable persons in societies in transition far exceed
the individual and psychopathological focus that con-
not utilizing mental health services because of feelings
ventional trauma mental health approaches provide, this
of shame and stigmatization they associated with psy- project aims to begin a collaborative program of family
chiatric diagnoses of PTSD and mental disorders, and focused education and training that is resilience-based
because of the narrow focus on individual symptoms of and emphasizes family strengths.
pathology. he community responded enthusiastically
to the family-centered resilience orientation to foster he consultants, sharing a multisystemic, resil-
recovery and positive adaptation. ience-oriented approach to address family challenges,
his program, called CAFES for Bosnians and encouraged Kosovar professionals to adapt the frame-
TAFES for Kosovars (Cofee/Tea and Family Education work and develop their own practice methods to
and Support), utilized a nine-week multifamily group best it local culture and service needs. he approach
format. Families readily participated because it tapped emphasized the importance of meeting with families
into the strong family-centered cultural values and to hear their stories, bearing witness to atrocities suf-
was located in an accessible neighborhood store- fered, and eliciting the strengths and resources in fam-
front, where they felt comfortable. Ofering a safe ily belief systems, organization, and communication
and compassionate setting to share stories of sufer- processes. Interviews revealed that their Islamic teach-
ing and struggle, it also airmed family strengths and ings and the inspiration of family models and mentors
resources, such as their courage, endurance, and faith; were powerful wellsprings in resilience. One family
strong kinship networks; deep concern for loved ones; drew strength from the mother’s faith in Allah and her
and determination to rise above their tragedies to forge courage in carrying on ater witnessing the murder of
a new life. heir eforts were encouraged to bridge her husband and sons. An uncle shared stories with his
cultures and, to the extent possible, to sustain kinship nephews of the strong leadership and bravery of their
ties and gain a sense of belonging in both old and new deceased father and grandfather to inspire their best

157
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

eforts for the future. In many families, strong cohe- of workers who were missing ater the 9/11 attacks
siveness and adaptive role lexibility enabled members in concert with their labor unions. Multifamily group
to assume new responsibilities to ill in missing func- meetings were held in their union hall. Multilingual,
tions. Although their grief was immense, their resili- multiracial intervention teams were attuned to
ence was remarkable. As one family member related, the cultural diversity of families. Group leaders
“Everyone belongs to the family and to the family’s helped families to share anguishing experiences and
homeland, alive or dead, here or abroad. Everyone communicated their basic premise: when a loved
matters and everyone is counted upon” (Becker et al., one remains missing, it is the situation of ambigu-
2000, p. 29). ous loss that is abnormal, not distressed family mem-
bers. he group interactions and mutual support were
Traumatic loss and major disaster empowering, with several widows taking on leader-
here is increasing recognition among trauma and ship roles over time.
bereavement specialists of the intertwining of trauma,
loss, grief, and resilience (Figley, 1998b; Bonanno,
2004; Litz, 2004; Malkinson et al., 2005). In contrast Research challenges and
to treatment programs focusing on individuals, multi- opportunities
systemic resilience-oriented approaches build healing he very lexibility of the concept of resilience, the
networks that facilitate child, family, and community complexity of systemic assessment, and the varied
resilience (Walsh, 2007). hese types of program cre- applications and intervention formats do pose daunt-
ate a safe haven for family and community members to ing challenges for family assessment and intervention
share both deep pain and positive strivings. hey can research. Given cultural and family diversity, and the
help families and communities to expand their vision of probability that some processes may be more useful
what is possible through collaboration, not only to sur- than others in dealing with varied challenges, ind-
vive trauma and loss, but to regain their spirit to thrive. ings from a particular study may not be generalizable
to diverse populations and life challenges. hat said, a
Lower Manhattan Community Recovery Project
number of recent and ongoing qualitative studies in
Following the September 11 2001 (9/11) terrorist many parts of the world are making progress in apply-
attacks, neighborhood schools closed and families ing this family resilience framework to clarify which
were displaced for several months; parents and chil- variables matter most for family resilience in a range
dren were distressed on re-entry to homes and schools of adverse situations, such as immigration, living with
where they had experienced the trauma. Saul and cancer, and death of a child, and in interventions to
colleagues (Landau & Saul, 2004) organized multi- strengthen poor, fragile families to prevent child aban-
family groups and parent–teacher networks in lower donment to the streets.
Manhattan neighborhoods directly afected. Later,
they included local agencies, shopkeepers, and other
residents. he process of family and community con- Conclusions
nectedness provided a matrix of healing and support. A family resilience orientation involves a crucial shit
A neighborhood resource center and ongoing forums in emphasis from family deicits to family challenges,
evolved to share experiences of trauma and resili- with conviction in the potential inherent in family
ence, mobilizing concerted action in recovery eforts. systems for recovery and growth out of adversity. By
hey formed a disaster preparedness initiative and targeting interventions to strengthen key processes for
developed such projects as a video narrative archive, resilience, families become more resourceful in deal-
a theater of witness project, a community website, a ing with crises, weathering persistent stresses, and
computer education program for seniors, and art and meeting future challenges. his conceptual framework
music projects, with long-lasting positive impact. can usefully be integrated with many strengths-based
practice models and applied with a range of crisis situ-
Family meetings as a community intervention ations, with respect for family and cultural diversity.
for ambiguous loss Resilience-oriented services foster family empower-
Another team of family systems-oriented therapists, ment as they bring forth shared hope, develop new and
co-led by Boss (Boss et al., 2003), worked with families renewed competencies, and strengthen family bonds.

158
Chapter 10: Family resilience

Figley, C. (ed.) (1998b). he traumatology of grieving. San


References Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Antonovsky, A. (1998). he sense of coherence: An Girwitz, A., Forgatch, M., & Wieling, E. (2008). Parenting
historical and future perspective. In H. McCubbin, practices as potential mechanisms for child adjustment
E. hompson, A. hompson, & J. Fromer (eds.), Stress, following mass trauma. Journal of Marital and Family
coping and health in families: Sense of coherence and herapy, 34, 177–192.
resiliency (pp. 3–20). housand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gorell Barnes, G. (1999). Divorce transitions: Identifying
Antonovsky, A. & Sourani, T. (1988). Family sense of risk and promoting resilience for children and their
coherence and family adaptation. Journal of Marriage parental relationships. Journal of Marital and Family
and the Family, 50, 79–92. herapy, 25, 425–441.
Beavers, W. R. & Hampson, R. B. (2003). Measuring family
Greef, A. P. & Human, B. (2004). Resilience in families in
competence. In F. Walsh (ed.), Normal family processes:
which a parent has died. American Journal of Family
Growing diversity and complexity, 3rd edn (pp. 549–580).
herapy, 32, 27–42.
New York: Guilford Press.
Greef, A. P. & van der Merwe, S. (2004). Variables
Becker, C., Sargent, J., & Rolland, J. S. (2000). Kosovar
associated with resilience in divorced families. Social
Family Professional Education Collaborative. AFTA
Indicators Research, 68, 59–75.
Newsletter, 80, 26–30.
Green, R.-J. (2004). Risk and resilience in lesbian and gay
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience.
couples. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 290–292.
American Psychologist, 59, 20–28.
Greene, S., Anderson, E., Forgatch, M. S., DeGarmo, D. S.,
Boss, P. (2001). Family stress management: A contextual
& Hetherington, E. M. (2012). Risk and resilience ater
approach, 2nd edn. housand Oaks, CA: Sage.
divorce. In F. Walsh (ed.), Normal family processes:
Boss, P., Beaulieu, L., Wieling, E., & Turner, W. (2003). Growing diversity and complexity, 4th edn. New York:
Healing loss, ambiguity, and trauma: A community- Guilford Press, in press.
based intervention with families of union workers
Hauser, S. T. (1999). Understanding resilient outcomes:
missing ater the 9/11 attack in New York City. Journal of
Adolescent lives across time and generations. Journal of
Marital and Family herapy, 29, 455–467.
Research on Adolescence, 9, 1–24.
Campbell, T. (2003). he efectiveness of family
interventions for physical disorders. Journal of Marital Hernandez, P. (2002). Resilience in families and
and Family herapy, 29, 263–281. communities: Latin American contributions from the
psychology of liberation. Journal of Counseling and
Cohen, O., Slonim, I., Finzi, R., & Leichtentritt, R. (2002). herapy for Couples and Families, 10, 334–343.
Family resilience: Israeli mothers’ perspectives. American
Journal of Family herapy, 30, 173–187. Johnson-Garner, M. Y. & Meyers, S. A. (2003). What
factors contribute to the resilience of African-American
Combrinck-Graham, L. (2005). Children in families at children within kinship care? Child and Youth Care
risk: Maintaining the connections, 2nd edn. New York: Forum, 32, 255–269.
Guilford Press.
Knowles, R., Sasser, D., & Garrison, M. E. B. (2010). Family
Cowan, P. A. & Cowan, C. P. (2012). Normative family resilience and resiliency following Hurricane Katrina. In
transitions, couple relationship quality and and healthy R. Kilmer, V. Gil-Rivas, R. Tedeschi, & L. Calhoun (eds.),
child development. In F. Walsh (ed.), Normal family Helping families and communities recover from disaster.
processes: Growing diversity and complexity, 4th edn. New Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
York: Guilford Press, in press. Press.
Epstein, N., Ryan, C., Bishop, D., Miller, I., & Keitner, G. Landau, J. (2007). Enhancing resilience: Families and
(2003). he McMaster model: A view of healthy family communities as agents for change. Family Process, 46,
functioning. In F. Walsh (ed.), Normal family processes: 351–365.
Growing diversity and complexity, 3rd edn (pp. 581–607).
New York: Guilford Press. Landau, J. & Saul, J. (2004). Facilitating family and
community resilience in response to major disasters. In
Falicov, C. (2007). Working with transnational immigrants: F. Walsh & M. McGoldrick (eds.), Living beyond loss: Death
expanding meanings of family, community and culture. in the family, 2nd edn (pp. 285–309). New York: Norton.
Family Process, 46,157–172.
Lansford, J. E., Ceballo, R., Abby, A., & Stewart, A. J. (2001).
Figley, C. R. (1989). Helping traumatized families. San Does family structure matter? A comparison of adoptive,
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. two-parent biological, single-mother, stepfather, and
Figley, C. R. (1998a) Burnout in families: he systemic cost of stepmother households. Journal of Marriage and the
caring. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Family, 63, 840–851.

159
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

Lavee, Y., McCubbin, H. I., & Olson, D. H. (1987). he Rolland, J. S., & Walsh, F. (2006). Facilitating family
efect of stressful life events and transitions on family resilience with childhood illness and disability. [Special
functioning and well-being. Journal of Marriage and the issue on the family.] Pediatric Opinion, 18, 1–11.
Family, 49, 857–873. Rolland, J. S. & Weine, S. (2000). Kosovar Family
Litz, B. (2004). Early intervention for trauma and traumatic Professional Educational Collaborative. American Family
loss. New York: Guilford Press. herapy Newsletter, 79, 34–35.
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). he construct Rowe, C. L. & Liddle, H. A. (2008). When the levee breaks:
of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for Treating adolescents and families in the atermath
future work. Child Development, 71, 543–562. of Hurricane Katrina. Journal of Marital and Family
MacDermid, S. M. (2010). Family risk and resilience in the herapy, 34, 132–148.
context of war and terrorism. Journal of Marriage and Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective
Family, 72, 537–556. mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57,
Malkinson, R., Rubin, S., & Witztum, E. (2005). Terror, 316–331.
trauma, and bereavement: Implications for theory and Sheinberg, M. & Fraenkel, P. (2001). he relational trauma of
therapy. In Y. Danieli, D. Brom, & J. Sills (eds.), he incest: A family-based approach to treatment. New York:
trauma of terrorism: Sharing knowledge and shared care – Guilford Press.
An international handbook (pp. 467–481). New York:
Stinnett, N. & DeFrain, J. (1985). Secrets of strong families.
Haworth.
Boston, MA: Little, Brown.
McCubbin, H. & Patterson, J. M. (1983). he family stress
Ungar, M. (2004). he importance of parents and other
process: he double ABCX model of adjustment and
caregivers to the resilience of high-risk adolescents.
adaptation. Marriage and Family Review, 6(Suppl.),
Family Process, 43, 23–41.
7–37.
Vigil, A. M. & Geary, D. C. (2008). A preliminary
McCubbin, H., McCubbin, M., McCubbin, A., & Futrell, J.
investigation of family coping styles and psychological
(eds.) (1998). Resiliency in ethnic minority families: Vol. 1
well being among adolescent survivors of Hurricane
Native and immigrant families, Vol. 2 African-American
Katrina. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 176–180.
families. housand Oaks, CA: Sage.
McGoldrick, M., Gerson, R., & Petry, S. (2008). Walsh, F. (1996). he concept of family resilience: Crisis and
Genograms: Assessment and intervention, 3rd edn. New challenge. Family Process, 35, 261–281.
York: Norton. Walsh, F. (2002). A family resilience framework:
McGoldrick, M., Carter, B., & Garcia Preto, N. (2011). he Innovative practice applications. Family Relations, 51,
expanded family life cycle: Individual, family, and social 130–137.
perspectives, 4th edn. Needham Heights: Allyn & Bacon. Walsh, F. (2003). Family resilience: A framework for clinical
Minuchin, P., Colapinto, J., & Minuchin, S. (2006). Working practice. Family Process, 42, 1–18.
with families of the poor, 2nd edn. New York: Guilford Walsh, F. (2006). Strengthening family resilience, 2nd edn.
Press. New York: Guilford Press.
Olson, D. H. & Gorell, D. (2003). Circumplex model of Walsh, F. (2007). Traumatic loss and major disaster:
marital and family systems. In F. Walsh (ed.), Normal Strengthening family and community resilience. Family
family processes: Growing diversity and complexity, 3rd Process, 46, 207–227.
edn (pp. 514–544). New York: Guilford Press. Walsh, F. (2009a). Family transitions: Challenges and
Oswald, R. F. (2002). Resilience within the family networks transitions. In M. Dulcan (ed.), Textbook of child and
of lesbians and gay men: Intentionality and redeinition. adolescent psychiatry. Washington, DC: American
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 64, 374–383. Psychiatric Press.
Patterson, J. (2002). Integrating family resilience and family Walsh, F. (ed.) (2009b). Spiritual resources in family therapy.
stress theory. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 64, New York: Guilford Press.
349–373.
Walsh, F. (2010). Spiritual diversity: Multifaith perspectives
Rolland, J. S. (1994). Families, illness and disability: An in family therapy. Family Process, 49, 330–348.
integrative treatment model. New York: Basic.
Walsh, F. (2011). Families in later life: Challenges,
Rolland, J. S. (2012). Mastering family challenges in serious opportunities, and resilience. In B. Carter &
illness and disability. In F. Walsh (ed.), Normal family M. McGoldrick (eds.), he expanded family life cycle,
processes: Growing diversity and complexity, 4th edn. New 4th edn (pp. 261–277). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn &
York: Guilford Press, in press. Bacon.

160
Chapter 10: Family resilience

Walsh, F. (2012a).he “new normal family”: Diversity and Walsh, F. & McGoldrick, M. (2004). Loss and the family:
complexity. In F. Walsh (ed.), Normal family processes: A systemic perspective. In F. Walsh & M. McGoldrick
Growing diversity and complexity, 4th edn. New York: (eds.), Living beyond loss: Death in the family, 2nd edn
Guilford Press, in press. (pp. 3–26). New York: Norton.
Walsh, F. (2012b). Clinical views of family normality, Weine, S., Muzuravic, N., Kulauzovic, Y., et al. (2004).
health, and dysfunction. In F. Walsh (ed.), Normal family Family consequences of refugee trauma. Family Process,
processes: Growing diversity and complexity, 4th edn. New 43, 147–160.
York: Guilford Press, in press.

161
Section
Section3 Resilience in families, communities, and societies
Chapter
Community resilience: concepts,

11 assessment, and implications for


intervention
Fran H. Norris, Kathleen Sherrieb, and Betty Pfeferbaum

Introduction sections. he irst section provides our perspective


on the meaning of resilience, including deinitions
Although stress research has emphasized individual
that work across levels of analysis (individual, family,
well-being, many types of stress are experienced col-
organization, community, society). he second section
lectively: the events bring harm, pain, and loss to large
describes the adaptive capacities theorized to yield
numbers of people simultaneously. Natural disasters,
community resilience. his is followed by an outline
terrorist attacks, war, political oppression, epidemics,
of measurement strategies and challenges in assess-
and economic recessions happen to whole communi-
ing capacities, including some of our own pilot work.
ties and, sometimes, whole societies. his is not to say
he concluding section makes recommendations for
that all exposed individuals experience the event iden-
intervention and raises issues that need to be addressed
tically; in a disaster, one person may lose a loved one,
in future research.
while another loses a home, and another only a few pos-
sessions. Nor is this to say that all exposed individuals
respond identically; a person’s psychological, social, Resilience as a process
and material resources powerfully shape his or her cap- Our perspective on the broader meaning of resilience
acity to cope and function efectively. When stress per- has shaped our thinking about its speciic application
vades the community, however, these factors tell only to communities. he concept of resilience appears in a
part of the story. To have an ecologically valid under- variety of disciplines, including physics and engineer-
standing of mass trauma, we must recognize that survi- ing (Gordon, 1978; Bodin & Wiman, 2004), biology and
vors are connected and dependent upon one another’s ecology (e.g., Holling, 1973), sociology (e.g., Adger, 2000;
coping strategies. heir attributions and actions relect Godschalk, 2003), and psychology (Werner & Smith,
a host of social inluences, social comparisons, and 1982; Rutter, 1993; Bonanno, 2004). Across domains
emergent norms. hey help each other but also com- of concern, most deinitions of resilience emphasize a
pete for scarce resources. Household preparedness is capacity for successful adaptation in the face of a dis-
vital, but one household can no more prepare for dis- turbance, stress, or adversity. Attempting to integrate
aster than it could, on its own, protect itself from crime various deinitions across levels of analysis, Norris and
or disease, educate its children, or keep the roads safe. colleagues (2008, p. 130) deined resilience as “a process
Consequently, an individual’s resilience is inextricably linking a set of adaptive capacities to a positive trajectory
linked to the community’s ability to prepare for, respond of functioning and adaptation ater a disturbance.” his
to, and adapt to adverse conditions. Simply put, when deinition of resilience encompasses two primary con-
problems are shared, so must be solutions. ceptions that are important for our discussion here.
In recognition of such interdependencies, “com- First, in this deinition, resilience emerges from
munity resilience” has emerged as a key concept for adaptive capacities, but it is not synonymous with
disaster readiness, although by no means limited to those capacities. Resilience is not a trait that an indi-
this one goal (Norris et al., 2008). his chapter will vidual or community invariably has or does not have.
explore the concept of community resilience in some Post-event trajectories are contingent upon both the
depth. Broadly, the chapter is organized into four capacities and the stressor. Resilience occurs when

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

162
Chapter 11: Community resilience

resources are suiciently strong to bufer or counteract Capacities for resilience


the efects of a stressor such that a return to function-
What we primarily seek to discover in resilience
ing, adapted to the altered environment, occurs. he
research is the capacity for resilience – those resources
more severe the stressor, the stronger the resources
that increase the likelihood of adaptation as manifest in
must be to create resilience. here is perhaps no person
psychological or population wellness. Resilience rests
or community that would always exhibit resilience nor
on both the resources themselves and the dynamic
a person or community that would never exhibit resili-
attributes of those resources; we use the term “adaptive
ence. he current emphasis on resilience is essentially
capacities” to capture this combination. Drawing upon
a reframing or evolution of stress theory, now decades
the earlier work of Bruneau et al. (2003), we deine adap-
old, in which stress outcomes are viewed as the prod-
tive capacities as resources with one or more dynamic
uct of stressors interacting with risk and protective fac-
attributes: robustness, the ability of the resource to
tors. Importantly, however, the contemporary frame of
withstand stress without sufering degradation; redun-
resilience directs attention to the potential of individ-
dancy, the extent to which elements are substitutable
uals and communities to adjust and stay well in the face
in the event of disruption or degradation; and rapid-
of threats, losses, and challenges.
ity, the speed with which the resource can be accessed
Second, resilience is manifest in outcomes of inter-
and used. By posing this deinition, we aimed to inte-
est, but it is not synonymous with those outcomes. he
grate resilience perspectives with evidence showing
deinition of resilience as a process implies that it is not
that resources are not static – they evolve, strengthen,
observed or measured directly, but it is evident in the
weaken, and rebound – and these trajectories are of
patterns of change observed ater signiicant stress.
interest in their own right (Hobfoll, 1988). Reaching a
Norris et al. (2009) outlined six possible trajectories
better understanding of the impact of critical incidents
for post-traumatic stress symptoms, of which resili-
on community resources is one of the most critical and
ence was just one, the others being resistance, recovery,
complex challenges for future research.
relapsing/remitting, delayed dysfunction, and chronic
On the basis of their review of the literature, Norris
dysfunction. In analyses of two four-wave datasets col-
et al. (2008) identiied four primary sets of resources
lected from population-based samples ater the 1999
thought to yield community resilience: economic devel-
loods/mudslides in Mexico and the 2001 terrorist
opment, social capital, information and communica-
attacks in New York, all of the hypothesized trajectories
tion, and community competence. hese sets are far
except one (relapsing/remitting) occurred with meas-
from orthogonal, but they are also far from synonym-
urable frequency in one or both of the samples.
ous. Much has been written about these concepts, which
While the analyses conducted by Norris et al. (2009)
are introduced here only briely. here are innumerable
focused on one particular outcome (post-traumatic
possible linkages between elements in these sets (and
stress), “wellness” provides a more complete criterion
between these elements and population wellness) that
for assessing human adaptation (Cowen, 1983, 1994,
could be researched empirically across communities.
2000; Norris et al., 2008), Wellness goes beyond the
mere absence of psychopathology to include healthy
patterns of behavior, adequate role functioning, and Economic development
high quality of life. Community-level adaptation can Economic development encompasses three key elem-
be understood as “population wellness,” deined as ents relating to the level of economic resources, the
high and non-disparate levels of mental and behavioral diversity of those resources, and the equity of their
health, role functioning, and quality of life in constitu- distribution. Land and raw materials, physical cap-
ent populations. However, it is important not to confuse ital, accessible housing, health services, schools,
resilience, the process, with wellness, the outcome. A and employment opportunities create the essential
resilient trajectory could be observed for one outcome resource base of a resilient community (Godschalk,
(e.g., post-traumatic stress) but not for another (e.g., 2003; Pfeferbaum et al., 2007). Because of extensive
quality of life). he outcomes of interest vary across interdependencies at the macroeconomic level, eco-
levels of analysis, as do the speciic resources that inlu- nomic resilience depends not only on the capacities
ence the patterns of change, but the basic nomencla- of individual businesses but also on the capacities of
ture of adaptive capacities, observed trajectories, and all the entities that depend on them and on which they
adequate functioning applies to all. depend (Rose, 2004, 2005).

163
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

Community resilience depends on both the vol- For crisis response, an important dimension of
ume of economic resources and their diversity. social capital is the presence of interorganizational
Communities that are dependent on a narrow range of networks that are characterized by reciprocal links,
resources are less able to cope with change that involves frequent supportive interactions, overlap with other
the depletion of that resource, a state that is sometimes networks, the ability to form new associations, and
referred to as “resource dependency” (Adger, 2000). cooperative decision-making processes (Goodman
Extreme events, such as droughts, loods, or infesta- et al., 1998). Comfort (2005, p. 347) noted that uncer-
tions, increase the risk of being dependent on particu- tainty oten leads to eforts to broaden the “scope of
lar resources and, therefore, decrease resilience. Cutter actors, agents, and knowledge that can be marshaled.”
et al. (2006) described one community that was par- More generally, this trend necessitates networked as
ticularly devastated by Hurricane Katrina in August opposed to hierarchical systems for disaster response.
2005 because residents were almost totally reliant on Longstaf (2005) highlighted the importance of “key-
the shrimping industry, on which the storm had a tre- stones” or “hubs”: “super-connected” network mem-
mendous impact. bers who link one network to another (see also Fullilove
Societies do not allocate environmental risk & Saul, 2006). Despite the many values of dense net-
equally, oten making the poorest communities the works, they are also more complex and, therefore, more
weakest links in hazard mitigation (Tobin & Whiteford, uncertain. he eiciency of hubs may actually decrease
2002; Cutter et al., 2003; Godschalk, 2003). Wisner resilience because the entire system fails if the hub is
(2001, p. 251) argued that mitigation plans in develop- compromised (Allenby & Fink, 2005). Systems will be
ing countries oten fail to address the “root causes of highly vulnerable if there is little redundancy for these
disaster vulnerability, namely, the economic and polit- connective functions.
ical marginality of much of the population and envir- Longstaf (2005) also noted the tendency to want
onmental degradation.” Poor communities not only are individuals, groups, and organizations to come
at greater risk for death and severe damage but are also together tightly to resist danger. “Tight coupling”
oten less successful in mobilizing support ater disas- occurs when change in one component engenders a
ters (Bolin & Bolton, 1986; Tobin & Whiteford, 2002). response from other components. his is not always
Ideally, the distribution or mobilization of support bad, but tight coupling can also increase danger in
follows the “rule of relative needs,” wherein the most some circumstances and can lead to premature con-
support goes to those who need it the most. Oten, vergence on solutions. “Loosely coupled” systems
however, the distribution of support follows the “rule may be better at responding to local changes since any
of relative advantage,” because one’s embeddedness in change they make does not require the whole system
the community, political connections, and social class to respond (Longstaf, 2005). he happy medium may
determine the availability and accessibility of resources be loosely coupled organizations (to better respond to
(Kaniasty & Norris, 1995). he capacity to distribute local needs) that are able to coordinate or collaborate
post-disaster resources to those who most need them (to facilitate access to their resources). he failure of
seems vitally important for community resilience. relief organizations to work together results in “cracks”
in the post-disaster service delivery network, whereas
an efective service delivery system provides a com-
Social capital plete set of services and linkages in which such cracks
he basic idea of social capital is that individuals do not appear (Gillespie & Murty, 1994).
invest, access, and use resources embedded in social Social capital also encompasses the more familiar
networks to gain returns (Bourdieu, 1985; Lin, 2001). concept of social support, which refers to social inter-
Social capital theorists debate the roles of self-inter- actions that provide individuals with actual assistance
est and status attainment and whether social capital and embed them into a web of social relationships
should be conceived as an individual, collective, or perceived to be loving, caring, and readily available
multilevel asset (Wellman & Frank, 2001). heorists in times of need (Barrera, 1986). Strong social sup-
have also debated the extent to which people actively port is vital for community resilience following crises
aim to increase their social capital (through invest- (Goodman et al., 1998; Ganor & Ben-Lavy, 2003; Tse &
ment) or whether, conversely, it arises from structural Liew, 2004; Pfeferbaum et al., 2005). he overall pat-
positions, families, and friendships (Kadushin, 2004). tern of help utilization resembles a pyramid with its

164
Chapter 11: Community resilience

broad foundation being the family, followed by other natural environments. In the worst of cases, people are
primary support groups, such as friends, neighbors, displaced from neighborhoods and communities in
and co-workers, followed by formal agencies and other which they are deeply rooted. he impact of displace-
persons outside of the recipient’s immediate circle ment ater disasters has oten been profoundly adverse
(Kaniasty & Norris, 2000). Received support typically (e.g., Erikson, 1976; Oliver-Smith, 1986; Norris et al.,
shows a mobilization pattern by increasing in the ater- 2005a), raising the possibility that place attachment
math of disasters and correlating positively with sever- could, in some circumstances, impair rather than facili-
ity of exposure. It protects against erosion of perceived tate individual resilience. However, place attachments
support, which is a powerful protective factor for men- increase the likelihood that the community as a whole
tal health (Norris & Kaniasty, 1996). has the will to rebuild (Manzo & Perkins, 2006).
Another critical function of social networks is Citizen participation is the engagement of commu-
social inluence. In emergencies, people look to similar nity members in formal organizations, including reli-
others to help them to make decisions about appropri- gious congregations, school and resident associations,
ate behaviors. his idea, oten characterized as “emer- neighborhood watches, and self-help groups (Perkins
gent norms,” is among the oldest to be found in the et al., 2002). It is widely believed to be a fundamental
sociology of disasters (Fritz & Williams, 1957). For element for community resilience (Goodman et al.,
example, the greater one’s social ties, the more likely 1998; Ganor & Ben-Lavy, 2003; Pfeferbaum et al.,
one is to receive information about recommendations 2007). Empowering community settings are character-
to evacuate. Evacuation is oten the only available ized by inspired, committed leadership and by oppor-
strategy to save lives and reduce personal injuries. In tunities for members to play meaningful roles (Maton
an analysis of evacuation decisions before Hurricanes & Salem, 1995). Quarantelli (1989) summarized the
Hugo and Andrew, Riad et al. (1999) found that resi- results of a series of observational studies of citizen
dents with stronger social support were twice as likely groups that emerge with respect to hazardous waste
to evacuate as were residents with weaker social sup- sites, noting that they tend to have a small active core,
port. he important dimension was perceived support a larger supporting circle that can be mobilized for
(e.g., ability to borrow money, get a ride, have a place to speciic tasks, and a greater number of nominal mem-
stay), not merely the number of ties. bers. Most groups exist in conlict. heir goals are oten
For the most part, social support captures helping vague and loty at the outset but become more speciic
behaviors within family and friendship networks, but and achievable over time. Edelstein (1988) observed
social capital also encompasses relationships between that leaders of citizen groups are usually those who
individuals and their larger neighborhoods and com- have strongest attachments to place.
munities (Perkins et al., 2002). hree key social psycho-
logical dimensions of social capital are, therefore, sense
of community, place attachment, and citizen participa- Information and communication
tion. Sense of community is an attitude of bonding (trust Information may be the primary resource in technical
and belonging) with other members of one’s group or and organizational systems that enable adaptive per-
locale (Perkins et al., 2002), including mutual con- formance (Comfort, 2005). By communication, we refer
cerns and shared values. It is reliably believed to be an to the creation of common meanings and understand-
attribute of resilient communities (Ahmed et al., 2004; ings and the provision of opportunities for members to
Landau & Saul, 2004; Tse & Liew, 2004; Pfeferbaum articulate needs, views, and attitudes. Pfeferbaum et al.
et al., 2007). Place attachment is closely related to one’s (2007), Goodman et al. (1998), and Ganor and Ben-
sense of community. It implies an emotional connec- Lavy (2003) have all argued that good communication
tion to one’s neighborhood or city, somewhat apart is essential for community resilience or capacity.
from connections to the speciic people who live there Information and communication become vital in
(Altman & Low, 1992; Manzo & Perkins, 2006). Place emergencies. People need accurate information about
attachment oten underlies citizens’ eforts to revitalize the danger and behavioral options, and they need it
a community (Perkins et al., 2002) and, therefore, may quickly. Public adherence to recommendations can-
be essential for community resilience. Place attachment not be taken for granted, particularly when there is
may be of special note for disaster recovery, as these marked uncertainty about exposure, consequences
events have spatial parameters and will harm built and of exposure, or the risks involved with following the

165
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

recommendations (Reissman et al., 2005). On the basis reciprocity and charity, were abandoned. here were
of her review, Longstaf (2005) argued that informa- widespread beliefs that some community members
tion increases survival only if it is “correct and cor- had prospered at the expense of others. In the three less
rectly transmitted.” In emergencies, when there is little distressed communities, residents shared a belief that
time to check information, it is also important that customs and social practices would return to normal as
the sender of the information be trusted. Closer, local soon as economic conditions improved. Most import-
sources of information are more likely to be relied upon antly, they had created a collective story that empha-
than unfamiliar, distant sources. In fact, Longstaf con- sized their resistance to the violence.
cluded (2005, p. 62), “A trusted source of information is
the most important resilience asset that any individual
or group can have” (emphasis in original). Similarly, Community competence
the Working Group on Governance Dilemmas in Community competence is the capacity for meaning-
Bioterrorism Response (2004) concluded that trusted ful, intentional action (Brown & Kulig, 1996/97). here
communication treats the public as a capable ally, appears to be high consensus that critical relection
invests in public outreach, and relects the values and and problem solving are fundamental prerequisites for
priorities of local populations. community resilience (Goodman et al., 1998; Bruneau
Communication infrastructure is also a valuable et al., 2003; Ganor & Ben-Lavy, 2003; Pfeferbaum et al.,
resource. On the basis of their experiences in New 2007). Endangered communities must be able to learn
York City ater the September 11 2001 (9/11) terrorist about their risks and options and work together lex-
attacks, Draper et al. (2006) maintained that it is advan- ibly and creatively to solve problems. Longstaf (2005)
tageous for a lifeline (or hotline) system to be in place argued that the capacity to acquire trusted and accurate
beforehand. hese communication systems can be information, to relect on that information critically,
ramped up ater the disaster to coordinate and deploy and to solve emerging problems is far more import-
volunteers, and later they provide a central means for ant for community resilience than is a detailed security
the public to learn about and access services (see also plan, which rarely foresees all contingencies (see also
Norris et al., 2006). he media also can be engaged Comfort, 2005; Handmer & Dovers, 1996).
to publicize available services and educate the public Cottrell (1976, p. 197) described a competent com-
about typical reactions to disaster (e.g., Gist & Stolz, munity as one in which “the various component parts
1982; Norris et al., 2006). of the community: (1) are able to collaborate efectively
he remaining element in this set, less structural in identifying the problems and needs of the commu-
than the others, is the presence of communal narra- nity; (2) can achieve a working consensus on goals and
tives that give the experience shared meaning and pur- priorities; (3) can agree on ways and means to imple-
pose (Sonn & Fisher, 1998). Couto (1989) described ment the agreed upon goals; and (4) can collaborate
how “group formulations” (narratives and symbols) efectively in the required actions.” Cottrell proposed
became a mechanism for empowerment in Aberfan, that these competencies arose from commitment to
South Wales, ater a horriic environmental disaster the community as a relationship worthy of substantial
took the lives of 104 school children and 20 adults. efort, articulateness, communication, participation,
Writing about their own experiences in the atermath of and means for debate, discussion, and decision mak-
the 9/11 terrorist attacks in lower Manhattan, Landau ing. Many of these conditions were previously con-
and Saul (2004) concluded that community recovery sidered as aspects of social capital or information and
depends partly on collectively telling the story of the communication, so it might be said that social capital
community’s experience and response. In an extraor- and communication are prerequisites for community
dinary anthropological study of six Guinean commu- competence.
nities attacked by Sierra Leonean and Liberian forces Collective action may depend heavily on the pres-
(Abramowitz, 2005), symptoms of post-traumatic ence of “collective eicacy”: community members’
stress were much higher in three of the communities trust in the efectiveness of organized community
than in the others. In the three more distressed com- action (Perkins & Long, 2002). Sampson et al. (1997)
munities, respondents shared the feeling that gov- deined collective eicacy as a composite of mutual
ernment and non-governmental organizations had trust and shared willingness to work for the common
neglected them. Social rituals and practices, including good of a neighborhood. Paton and Johnston (2001)

166
Chapter 11: Community resilience

proposed that an initial focus on promoting collect- single community or a small set of communities, par-
ive eicacy would increase the likelihood of achieving ticipatory self-assessments may be most appropriate.
success in working with a community to adopt mitiga- When the goal is to assess multiple communities using
tion strategies. Collective eicacy is highly related to a standardized metric, secondary analysis of social
empowerment (Perkins et al., 2002), a process through indicator and other publicly available data appears to
which people lacking an equal share of valued resources be a promising approach. Each of these approaches is
gain greater access to and control over those resources described in more detail below.
(Rappaport, 1995). A particular relevant discussion of
empowerment for our purposes was presented by Rich Participatory approaches
et al. (1995), who examined the dynamics of commu-
he Community Assessment of Resilience tool (CART)
nity empowerment ater discovery of environmental
developed by Pfeferbaum and colleagues (2006) con-
hazards. he authors reasoned that meaningful partici-
stitutes a community-based participatory approach to
pation in environmental action groups can be empow-
community resilience assessment and intervention.
ering and, conversely, that lack of opportunity for such
Community-based participatory research promotes
participation can be disempowering. According to
collaboration between investigators and those being
Rich and colleagues, the efectiveness of a community’s
studied. hese approaches can be used to investigate
response to a hazard is shaped by a combination of
complex health, public health, or social concerns that
resources (such as suicient education to understand
occur disproportionately in communities marginal-
the technical issues, money to hire lawyers or scientiic
ized with respect to health education and resources
advisors), a culture that permits challenges to author-
or to address challenging issues such as disparities in
ity, institutions that provide a basis for coordinating a
access to healthcare and outcomes (Meyer, 2000; Israel
response, and political mechanisms that involve citi-
et al., 2006; Cargo & Mercer, 2008; Flicker et al., 2008).
zens in decision making. In their model, empowerment
Inspired by the desire to stimulate community-based
progresses through a sequence of formal empower-
problem solving, CART not only builds on the princi-
ment (mechanisms for citizen input), intrapersonal
ples of community-based participatory research it also
empowerment (feelings of personal competence and
promotes these principles by involving community
conidence), instrumental empowerment (ability to
representatives in both assessment and community-
participate in and inluence decision, as determined by
resilience building exercises.
knowledge, material resources, and persuasiveness),
he CART process begins by surveying community
and substantive empowerment (ability to reach deci-
representatives about community resilience domains
sions that solve problems). he last of these stages is
tested in community assessment studies and about
virtually synonymous with our broader meaning of
additional speciic concerns identiied and reined in
community competence.
conjunction with community partners. he CART sur-
vey instrument incorporates four overlapping interre-
Assessment of capacities lated domains that characterize communities. hree of
Broadly, there are three approaches to the assessment the domains are common to all of CART eforts: con-
of capacities for community resilience. Population- nection and caring, which includes relatedness, shared
based surveys that are designed to allow responses values, participation, support systems, equity, justice,
from individual respondents to be aggregated to hope, and diversity; resources, which includes natural,
meaningful social units (neighborhoods, counties) physical, inancial, human, and social resources; and
have high potential but are considerably expensive. transformative potential, which includes data collec-
Existing national surveys are representative at the state tion, analysis of community assets and shortcomings,
level but cannot be used to describe smaller units of and skill building that create the potential for commu-
analysis. Consequently, in our work, we have focused nity change. A fourth domain relates to the speciic
on two very diferent, but perhaps complementary, condition or adversity that concerns the community
approaches: community-based participatory methods such as terrorism and disaster management, commu-
and secondary analysis of social indicator data. he nity violence, or poverty.
appropriate strategy may depend upon the scope of the Community-based participatory approaches
analysis. At the local level, when the goal is to assess a emphasize relationships to promote trust and mutual

167
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

understanding, improve interactions, and foster col- proile incorporating indings by and across the CART
lective problem solving between researchers and par- domains (connection and caring, resources, trans-
ticipants (Israel et al., 1998; Cargo & Mercer, 2008; formative potential, and disaster management or other
Cashman et al., 2008; Christopher et al., 2008). he community concern). he initial survey is followed by
CART approach relies on community partners to a series of meetings in which the community partici-
convene either a homogeneous or a heterogeneous pants review and interpret survey indings within the
mix of community leaders, neighborhood groups, context of their community and concerns. Upon com-
selected professionals, residents, or other stakehold- pletion of the community resilience assessment phase,
ers to complete the community assessment and CART participants work together through group processes
group activities. Rather than traditional method- to explore the meaning of community, perceptions of
ology in which investigators study individuals whose the community resilience domains in application to
sole input is to provide data (Israel et al., 1998; Meyer, speciic community concerns, and potential resilience-
2000; Macaulay & Nutting, 2006), community-based enhancing actions. Based on survey results and know-
participatory approaches strive to improve the pro- ledge of their community, group members identify
cess and results by engaging the community in virtu- community strengths and weaknesses, opportunities
ally all aspects of the work (Israel et al., 1998; Macaulay for improvement, external factors that might facilitate
& Nutting, 2006; Westfall et al., 2006; Cargo & Mercer, or impede change, and areas of disagreement. Group
2008; Flicker et al., 2008). Relying on the community’s members then establish goals related to issues they
collective social experience and cultural perspective, choose to address, and they engage in strategic plan-
community representatives can help to generate and ning to prepare strategies and an action plan to enhance
reine theory, frame questions that address the com- community resilience in support of the goals they have
munity’s concerns, and enhance the validity of research established. Strategies are then ielded, evaluated, and
constructs (Israel et al., 1998; Cargo & Mercer, 2008). reined. Plans may be disseminated among community
Community partners help to design recruitment strat- members and organizations, which may require train-
egies and assessment and intervention processes that ing and other support to implement the strategies.
consider the needs and values of those involved, and Several considerations arise in using community-
they can assist in data collection by identifying and based participatory research approaches. Colleagues,
reaching out to potential participants (Macaulay & potential partners, and funders may need to be con-
Nutting, 2006; Cashman et al., 2008). hey can advance vinced of the value, validity, and reliability of this
the translation of research into practice; and they can approach (Israel et al., 1998). Community-based partici-
promote the adoption and dissemination of results patory approaches do require a long-term commitment
(Macaulay & Nutting, 2006; Cargo & Mercer, 2008; of time and human capital to establish and nurture rela-
Cashman et al., 2008). he community can beneit tionships with community partners, to conduct commu-
from this approach directly if newly generated know- nity assessments, and to incorporate stakeholder input
ledge is used to expand or enrich existing practices, ser- throughout the process (Israel et al., 1998; Macaulay &
vices, programs, or policies (Israel et al., 1998; Kemmis & Nutting, 2006; Westfall et al., 2006; Cashman et al., 2008;
McTaggart, 2005; Cargo & Mercer, 2008; Flicker et al., Christopher et al., 2008). We have been fortunate to
2008). Our CART community partners are engaged identify partners who endorse the approach and appre-
across many of these activities. ciate the work we are promoting. While this approach is
Designed to stimulate communication, ana- time and labor intensive, our experience has been posi-
lysis, and action, and to foster community participa- tive and appears to support our ultimate goal of fostering
tion and collaboration, community self-awareness, community resilience building.
critical analysis, and skill development, the CART
assessment and intervention relect the principles of
community-based participatory methodology. To ini- Social indicator research
tiate the process, CART community participants (e.g., he second potential approach to assessing commu-
a mix of community leaders, neighborhood members, nity resilience relies on publicly available data to cre-
selected professionals, and/or representatives of com- ate a metric that can be applied across communities.
munity organizations) complete the CART survey. he In the disaster ield, an excellent example is the Social
results provide information to generate a community Vulnerability Index (SOVI; Cutter et al., 2003). Cutter

168
Chapter 11: Community resilience

and colleagues conducted factor analyses with data unions. Putnam (2000) included additional struc-
from all US counties to create the SOVI. It contains 11 tural indicators measuring volunteerism, voting par-
factors representing income, age, race/ethnicity, occu- ticipation, service and participation in clubs, local
pation, commercial establishment density, single-sec- organizations and community projects, and num-
tor industry, housing, and infrastructure dependence; ber of non-proit organizations per population size.
these factors are used to depict community vulner- Rupasingha et al. (2006) proposed a county-level model
ability. Although social vulnerability should be related to measure social capital based on their deinition of
(inversely) to community resilience, it is conceptually social capital as participation in associational activ-
distinct, suggesting that new measures were needed. ities. heir indicators included associational densities,
he social indicator approach has been explored presidential voter rates, census response rates, and per
using the State of Mississippi as the pilot case, with capita non-proit organizations. hey argued that the
county as the unit of analysis (Sherrieb et al., 2010). degree of participation and density provides a good
In reviewing the literature related to the measurement measure of social capital diferences between regions.
of the capacities included in the Norris et al. (2008) hrough a multistep process beginning with a
model, it became apparent that “economic develop- “wish list” of measures and continuing through search-
ment” and “social capital” had structural characteris- ing for and selecting indicators and validating indices,
tics that were possible to measure with secondary data, Sherrieb et al. (2010) selected 10 indicators to form an
but the same was not true for “information and com- index of economic development: (1) employment rate,
munication” and “community competence.” Sherrieb (2) median household income, (3) number of medical
et al. (2010) described previous measures of economic doctors per 10 000, (4) corporate tax revenues per 1000
development and social capital in some detail, noting population, and (5) percentage creative class occupa-
both their strengths and limitations with regard to util- tions (measuring resource level); (6) income equity
ity for inclusion in a measure of community resilience. (inverse of the Gini coeicient), (7) racial diferences
here has been considerable prior work on measure- in percentage with less than a high school education
ment of economic development. Well-known indices (measuring resource equity); and (8) net gain/loss
of economic development include (1) the gross domes- rate in businesses over a year, (9) occupational diver-
tic product, which provides the total value in national sity (percentage of occupations in the non-dominant
currency (dollars here) of all economic production in industry for the area), and (10) urban inluence (meas-
a given area in a year; (2) the Human Development uring resource diversity). Likewise, they selected seven
Index, which has been used by the United Nations indicators to form an index of social capital: (1) ratio
Development Programme (launched in 1990) as the of households with married parents with children to
marker for comparison of economic development households with single female parents with children
across nations since 1990; and (3) the Social Health (measuring social support); (2) density of sports/arts
Index, which is derived from ive potential indicators organizations per 10 000, (3) density of civic organi-
of social disadvantage: in the USA, rates of unemploy- zations per 10 000, (4) voter percentage in 2004 presi-
ment, poverty, high school drop out, violent crime, and dential election, (5) religious adherents per 1000
Medicaid recipients (for further discussion of these (measuring social participation); and (6) net migra-
measures, see Anderson, 1991; Horn, 1993; Shaw- tion rate per 1000 in a three-year period, and (7) the
Taylor, 1999; McGillivray, 2007). inverse of the property crime rate (measuring commu-
Likewise, the literature deining, measuring, and nity bonds).
modeling social capital is vast. Several measurements In their initial research, Sherrieb et al. (2010) vali-
of social capital use an aggregate measurement derived dated their measures against the SOVI and aggregated
from survey data. For example, Putnam (2000) and survey data from 21 Mississippi counties. (he avail-
Kawachi et al. (1997) used measures from the General ability of these survey data was their primary reason
Social Survey conducted by the US National Opinion for using Mississippi as the pilot case.) Economic
Research Center to determine social trust and organ- development and social capital were moderately cor-
izational participation at the state level. Structural related with each other (r = 0.37), but only 14% of the
social capital indicators used by Kawachi et al. included variance was shared by the three concepts, social cap-
per capita number of groups and associations such as ital, economic development, and SOVI. More than
churches, sports groups, political groups, and labor 50% of the variance in each measure was unique. hese

169
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

data support the premise that the three attributes share more geographically diverse sample of counties and
some common characteristics yet are distinct con- a set of survey measures (such as the CART survey)
cepts. he Mississippi survey had included measures developed speciically to capture the capacities thought
of collective eicacy, which were aggregated to the to underlie community resilience.
county level. Social capital was highly correlated with
the aggregated collective eicacy measures (r values of
0.62 to 0.77), but the economic development and SOVI Conclusions and implications
measures were not. “Community preparedness” was recently legislated as
he distributions of scores were also consistent with one of four cross-cutting “target capabilities” required
what is known about diferent regions of Mississippi. for federal emergency management (Post-Katrina
For example, the rural region bordering the Mississippi Emergency Management Reform Act of 2006 [P.L.
River, known as the Delta historically, has been notable 109–295]). Representing a paradigm shit from earlier
for its high rates of poverty. his area has some of the disaster mitigation approaches, community prepared-
highest rates for infant, adolescent, and adult mortality ness calls for the use and integration of all the com-
in the USA (Woods, 1998). he region is also in the top munity’s resources into disaster planning, including
highest 20% for social vulnerability when comparing those that exist outside of government as well as inside.
the counties within the state and within the nation with Our work in identifying the adaptive capacities that
SOVI (Hazards and Vulnerability Research Institute, underlie community resilience is consistent with this
2008). he new community resilience measures pro- approach. While the distinction between community
vided comparable results as that region was in the low- resilience and community preparedness is not com-
est 20% for social capital, economic development, and pletely clear, a resilience perspective takes us beyond
community resilience. making plans for a disaster toward building strengths
Ultimately, the utility of measures of community in a community that will facilitate the process of resili-
resilience (or more correctly, capacity for community ence regardless of the nature of the shared adversity.
resilience) depends upon the ability of these measures Consequently, translation of our indings into policy
to identify areas in need of strengthening. Areas low recommendations involves a discussion of changes in
in community resilience and high in risk for disasters, the socioeconomic structure of at-risk communities.
terrorist attacks, or other collective threats are par- he four sets of adaptive capacities provide a guide
ticularly important for eforts to build capacity. For for enhancing community resilience. We repeat here
example, Harrison County (Biloxi/Gulfport) was iden- the recommendations described in more detail by
tiied by Sherrieb et al. (2010) as being at high risk for Norris et al. (2008).
disasters but low in social capital. Consequently, know- First, to increase their resilience to disaster, commu-
ledge about the distribution of capacities and disaster nities must develop economic resources, reduce risk and
risk can help to target speciic interventions to facilitate resource inequities, and attend to their areas of greatest
resilience. social vulnerability. Godschalk (2003) recommended
Despite their potential utility and feasibility, there that urban hazard mitigation activities be integrated
are inherent limitations of archival data. Indicators with activities related to economic development and
that might be good measures of a concept are not social justice, thereby “achieving the multiple objectives
always available, and there is a lag in the availabil- needed for a resilient system.” Eforts to create economic
ity of data. Not only did Sherrieb et al. (2010) omit diversity increase the probability that the community
entire constructs, they were unable to measure the can withstand adversity or surprise. Ater a disaster,
cognitive characteristics of social capital such as trust, residents and grass-roots leaders should be vigilant to
reciprocity, norms, and values although the literature the equity of resource distribution. Competent com-
indicates that these are fundamental aspects of social munities may inluence these dynamics through cre-
capital. Despite this limitation, their indices of social ative problem solving and political action.
capital and community resilience correlated with sur- Second, to access social capital, one of the primary
vey measures of collective eicacy, which addressed resources of any community, local people must be engaged
some of those cognitive aspects. We believe the study meaningfully in every step of the mitigation process.
of Sherrieb et al. (2010) points to a useful method for Enabled by professional practitioners, as necessary,
future research that could be conducted with a larger, community members must assess and address their

170
Chapter 11: Community resilience

own vulnerabilities to hazards, identify and invest in uncertainty and relies on timely and trusted sources of
their own networks of assistance and information, and information for rapid decision making as opposed to
enhance their own capacities to solve problems created rigid plans (Longstaf, 2005). In contrast to command
by “known or unknown unknowns” (e.g., Brown & and control styles, problem-solving approaches allow
Kulig, 1996/97; Coles & Buckle, 2004; Longstaf, 2005; for innovation and localized variations in response.
Pfeferbaum et al., 2007). As we reviewed here, the Similarly, Godschalk (2003, p. 139) envisioned that
CART process is one structured approach that engages “the public and private organization of a resilient city
communities in this process. Non-indigenous practi- would both plan ahead and act spontaneously …hey
tioners best foster recovery by providing settings and would eschew simple command and control leader-
resources that allow the community to take charge of ship, preferring to develop networks of leadership and
the direction of change (van den Eynde & Veno, 1999; initiative. hey would set goals and objectives, but be
Landau & Saul, 2004; Perez-Sales et al., 2005; Fullilove prepared to adapt these in light of new information and
& Saul, 2006). learning.”
hird, pre-existing organizational networks and rela- Our primary hope is to foster creative think-
tionships are the key to rapidly mobilizing emergency and ing about how various pathways between economic
ongoing support services for disaster survivors. Loosely development, social capital, information and commu-
coupled but cooperative systems appear to provide the nication, and community competence shape disaster
best combination of linkages and lexibility (Gillespie readiness and recovery. Of course, there is then the
& Murty, 1994; Longstaf, 2005). A series of case studies question of how strongly the various adaptive cap-
of mental health system responses to major disasters, acities in this network contribute to the wellness of
including the Oklahoma City bombing (Norris et al., constituent populations. To date, research on the out-
2005b), the 9/11 terrorist attacks (Norris et al., 2006), comes of community resilience is meager. Some stud-
and many others (Elrod et al., 2006) repeatedly revealed ies have examined how individual-level perceptions of
that developing organizational networks, coalitions, community resilience (Kimhi & Shamai, 2004; Pooley
and cooperative agreements ahead of time is crucial, et al., 2006), sense of community (Paton et al., 2001),
and that organizational plans should indicate how key or collective eicacy (Benight, 2004) correlate with
constituencies will be involved. Program directors individual-level outcomes, but no study, to our know-
relied upon pre-existing relationships perhaps more ledge, has truly examined how independently assessed
than any other single resource to implement programs community resources inluence the post-disaster well-
quickly. To work together ater a disaster, systems must ness of constituent populations.
understand and trust each other, which is challenging he “prevention paradox” (Rose, 1981, 2001) is
if they have not worked together before. extremely important for future judgments regarding
Fourth, interventions are needed that boost and the relative inluence (and signiicance for policy) of
protect naturally occurring social supports in the ater- individual and community resilience resources. Efects
math of disasters. Fostering natural supports helps to that seem small in analyses of individuals may be quite
ensure that communities and families retain the cap- large when extrapolated to populations. Traditional
acity to exchange emotional and instrumental support risk (or protective) factor research is almost assured
(Landau & Saul, 2004). Social support interventions are to ind that individual-level resilience resources (e.g.,
most efective when they build social skills and mutual self-eicacy) have stronger efects within a study popu-
support (Hogan et al., 2002). Ideally, post-disaster sup- lation than do community-level resilience resources
port interventions furnish participants with know- (e.g., collective eicacy). his makes perfect sense from
ledge, attitudes, and skills that can be used to recruit an ecological perspective, which distinguishes between
their own supports (Layne et al., 2001). proximal and distal inluences on individual resilience
Fith, communities must plan for the unexpected; and wellness, in turn. But proximal determinants pro-
this means that communities must exercise lexibility tect only certain individuals, whereas distal efects pro-
and focus on building efective and trusted information tect everyone. If the underlying cause of an illness can
and communication resources that function in the face be removed from the population, susceptibility of indi-
of unknowns. Uncertainty is almost certain to exist viduals within the population ceases to matter (Rose,
ater disasters. he most adaptive disaster manage- 2001). Moreover, when the community itself is endan-
ment strategy is one that acknowledges complexity and gered, it does not merely function in the background as

171
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

the context in which individuals lourish or do not. To Brown, D. & Kulig, J. (1996/97). he concept of resiliency:
paraphrase Trickett (1995), in this case, the commu- heoretical lessons from community research. Health
nity must become the igure, not the ground. and Canadian Society, 4, 29–52.
Bruneau, M., Chang, S., Eguchi, R., et al. (2003). A
framework to quantitatively assess and enhance the
Acknowledgements seismic resilience of communities. Earthquake Spectra,
his research was supported by the US Department of 19, 733–752.
Homeland Security through the National Consortium Cargo, M. & Mercer, S. L. (2008). he value and challenges
for the Study of Terrorism and Responses to Terrorism of participatory research: Strengthening its practice.
(grant N00140510629). However, any opinions, ind- Annual Review of Public Health, 29, 325–350.
ings, and conclusions or recommendations in this Cashman, S. B., Adeky, S., Allen, A. J. 3rd, et al. (2008). he
document are those of the authors and do not neces- power and the promise: Working with communities to
sarily relect views of the US Department of Homeland analyze data, interpret indings, and get to outcomes.
Security. American Journal of Public Health, 98, 1407–1417.
Christopher, S., Watts, V., McCormick, A. K. H. G., &
References Young, S. (2008). Building and maintaining trust in a
community-based participatory research partnership.
Abramowitz, S. (2005). he poor have become rich, and American Journal of Public Health, 98, 1398–1406.
the rich have become poor: Collective trauma in the
Guinean Languette. Social Science and Medicine, 61, Coles, E. & Buckle, P. (2004). Developing community
2106–2118. resilience as a foundation for efective disaster recovery.
Australian Journal of Emergency Management, 19, 6–15.
Adger, W. (2000). Social and ecological resilience: Are they
related? Progress in Human Geography, 24, 347–364. Comfort, L. (2005). Risk, security, and disaster
management. Annual Review of Political Science, 8,
Ahmed, R., Seedat, M., van Niekerk, A., & Bulbulia, S. 335–356.
(2004). Discerning community resilience in
disadvantaged communities in the context of violence Cottrell, L. Jr. (1976). he competent community. In
and injury prevention. South African Journal of B. Kaplan, R. Wilson, & A. Leighton (eds.), Further
Psychology, 34, 386–408. explorations in social psychiatry (pp. 195–209). New York:
Basic Books, Inc.
Allenby, B. & Fink, J. (2005). Toward inherently secure and
resilient societies. Science, 309, 1034–1036. Couto, R. (1989). Catastrophe and community
empowerment: he group formulations of Aberfan’s
Altman, I. & Low, S. (eds.) (1992). Place attachment. New survivors. Journal of Community Psychology, 17,
York: Plenum Press. 236–248.
Anderson, V. (1991). Alternative economic indicators. New Cowen, E. (1983). Primary prevention in mental health:
York: Routledge. Past, present, and future. In R. Felner, L. Jason,
Barrera, M. (1986). Distinctions between social support J. Moritsugu, & S. Farber (eds.), Preventive psychology:
concepts, measures, and models. American Journal of heory, research, and practice in community intervention
Community Psychology, 14, 413–445. (pp. 11–25). New York: Pergamon Press.
Benight, C. (2004). Collective eicacy following a series Cowen, E. (1994). he enhancement of psychological
of natural disasters. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 17, wellness: Challenges and opportunities. American
401–420. Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 149–179.
Bodin, P. & Wiman, B. (2004). Resilience and other stability Cowen, E. (2000). Community psychology and routes to
concepts in ecology: Notes on their origin, validity, and psychological wellness. In J. Rappaport & E. Seidman
usefulness. ESS Bulletin, 2, 33–43. (eds.), Handbook of community psychology (pp. 79–99).
Bolin, R. & Bolton, P. (1986). Race, religion, and ethnicity in Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.
disaster recovery. Boulder, CO: University of Colorado. Cutter, S., Boruf, B., & Shirley, W. L. (2003). Social
Bonanno, G. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: vulnerability to environmental hazards. Social Science
Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive Quarterly, 84, 242–261.
ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, Cutter, S., Emrich, C., Mitchell, J., et al. (2006). he long
59, 20–28. road home: Race, class, and recovery from Hurricane
Bourdieu, P. (1985). he forms of capital. In J. Richardson Katrina. Environment, 48, 10–20.
(ed.), Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of Draper, J., McCleery, G., & Schaedle, R. (2006). Mental
education (p. 248). New York: Greenwood. health services support in response to September 11: he

172
Chapter 11: Community resilience

central role of the Mental Health Association of New Hobfoll, S. (1988). he ecology of stress. New York:
York City. In Y. Neria, R. Gross, R. Marshall, & E. Susser Hemisphere.
(eds.), 9/11: Mental health in the wake of terrorist attacks Hogan, B., Linden, W., & Najarian, B. (2002). Social support
(pp. 282–310). New York: Cambridge University Press. interventions: Do they work? Clinical Psychology Review,
Edelstein, M. (1988). Contaminated communities: he social 22, 381–440.
and psychological impacts of residential toxic exposure. Holling, C. (1973). Resilience and stability of ecological
Boulder, CO; Westview Press. systems. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 4, 1–23.
Elrod, C., Hamblen, J., & Norris, F. (2006). Challenges in Horn, R. V. (1993). Statistical indicators for the economic and
implementing disaster mental health programs: State social sciences. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
program directors’ perspectives. Annals of the American Press.
Academy of Political and Social Science, 604, 152–170.
Israel, B. A., Schulz, A. J., Parker, E. A., & Becker, A. B.
Erikson, K. (1976). Loss of communality at Bufalo Creek. (1998). Review of community-based research: Assessing
American Journal of Psychiatry, 133, 302–305. partnership approaches to improve public health. Annual
Flicker, S., Savan, B., Kolenda, B., & Mildenberger, M. Review of Public Health, 19, 173–202.
(2008). A snapshot of community-based research in Kadushin, C. (2004). Too much investment in social capital?
Canada: Who? what? why? how? Health Education Social Networks, 26, 75–90.
Research, 23, 106–114.
Israel, B. A., Kreiger, J. W., Viahov, D., et al. (2006).
Fritz, C. & Williams, H. (1957). he human being in Challenges and facilitating factors in sustaining
disasters: A research perspective. Annals of the American community-based participatory research partnerships:
Academy of Political and Social Science, 309, 42–51. Lessons learned from the Detroit, New York City, and
Fullilove, M. & Saul, J. (2006). Rebuilding communities Seattle Urban Research Centers. Journal of Urban Health,
post-disaster in New York. In Y. Neria, R. Gross, 83, 1022–1040.
R. Marshall, & E. Susser (eds.), 9/11: Mental health in Kaniasty, K. & Norris, F. (1995). In search of altruistic
the wake of terrorist attacks (pp. 164–177) New York: community: Patterns of social support mobilization
Cambridge University Press. following Hurricane Hugo. American Journal of
Ganor, M. & Ben-Lavy, Y. (2003). Community resilience: Community Psychology, 23, 447–477.
Lessons derived from Gilo under ire. Journal of Jewish Kaniasty, K. & Norris, F. (2000). Help-seeking comfort and
Communal Service, Winter/Spring, 105–108. receiving social support: he role of ethnicity and context
Gillespie, D. & Murty, S. (1994). Cracks in a postdisaster of need. American Journal of Community Psychology, 28,
service delivery network. American Journal of 545–582.
Community Psychology, 22, 639–660. Kawachi, I., Kennedy, B. P., Lochner, K., & Prothrow-
Gist, R. & Stolz, S. (1982). Mental health promotion and the Stith, D. (1997). Social capital, income inequality,
media: Community response to the Kansas City hotel and mortality. American Journal of Public Health, 87,
disaster. American Psychologist, 37, 1136–1139. 1491–1498.
Godschalk, D. (2003). Urban hazard mitigation: Creating Kemmis, S. & McTaggart, R. (2005). Participatory action
resilient cities. Natural Hazards Review, 4, 136–143. research: Communicative action and the public sphere.
Goodman, R., Speers, M., McLeroy, K., et al. (1998). In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (eds.), he SAGE
Identifying and deining the dimensions of community handbook of qualitative research (pp. 559–603). housand
capacity to provide a basis for measurement. Health Oaks, CA: Sage.
Education and Behavior, 25, 258–278. Kimhi, S. & Shamai, M. (2004). Community resilience and
Gordon, J. (1978). Structures. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin. the impact of stress: Adult response to Israel’s withdrawal
from Lebanon. Journal of Community Psychology, 32,
Handmer, J. & Dovers, S. (1996). A typology of resilience: 439–451.
Rethinking institutions for sustainable development.
Industrial and Environmental Crisis Quarterly, 9, Landau, J. & Saul, J. (2004). Facilitating family and
482–511. community resilience in response to major disaster. In
F. Walsh & M. McGoldrick (eds.), Living beyond loss:
Hazards and Vulnerability Research Institute (2008). Social Death in the family (pp. 285–309). New York: Norton.
vulnerability maps and data. Columbia, SC: Hazards &
Vulnerability Research Institute, University of South Layne, C., Pynoos, R., Saltzman, W., et al. (2001). Trauma/
Carolina, http://webra.cas.sc.edu/hvriapps/SOVI_ grief-focused group psychotherapy: School-based
Access/SOVI_search.aspx?Region =state&State=Missi postwar intervention with traumatized Bosnian
ssippi&Search=Submit+Query (accessed February 18, adolescents. Group Dynamics: heory, Research, and
2011). Practice, 5, 277–290.

173
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

Lin, N. (2001). Social capital: A theory of social structure and of responses to stress ater major disasters. Social Science
action. New York: Cambridge University Press. and Medicine, 68, 2190–2198.
Longstaf, P. (2005). Security, resilience, and communication Oliver-Smith, A. (1986). he martyred city. Albuquerque,
in unpredictable environments such as terrorism, natural NM: University of New Mexico Press.
disasters, and complex technology. Cambridge, MA: Paton, D. & Johnston, D. (2001). Disasters and communities:
Program for Information Resources Policy, Harvard Vulnerability, resilience, and preparedness. Disaster
University. Prevention and Management, 10, 270–277.
Macaulay, A. C. & Nutting, P. A. (2006). Moving the Paton, D., Millar, M., & Johnston, D. (2001). Community
frontiers forward: Incorporating community-based resilience to volcanic hazard consequences. Natural
participatory research into practice-based research Hazards, 24, 157–169.
networks. Annals of Family Medicine, 4, 4–7.
Perez-Sales, P., Cervellon, P., Vazquez, C., Vidales, D.,
McGillivray, M. (2007). Human well-being: Issues, concepts & Gaborit, M. (2005). Post-traumatic factors and
and measures. In M. McGillivray (ed.), Human well- resilience: he role of shelter management and
being: Concept and measurement (pp. 1–22). New York: survivors’ attitudes ater the earthquakes in El Salvador
Palgrave MacMillan. 2001. Journal of Community and Applied Social
Manzo, L. & Perkins, D. (2006). Finding common ground: Psychology, 15, 368–382.
he importance of place attachment to community Perkins, D. & Long, D. (2002). Neighborhood sense of
participation and planning. Journal of Planning community and social capital: A multi-level analysis. In
Literature, 20, 335–350. A. Fisher, C. Sonn, & B. Bishop (eds.), Psychological sense
Maton, K. & Salem, D. (1995). Organizational of community: Research, applications, and implications
characteristics of empowering community settings: (pp. 291–318). New York: Plenum Press.
A multiple case study approach. American Journal of Perkins, D., Hughey, J., & Speer, P. (2002). Community
Community Psychology, 23, 631–656. psychology perspectives on social capital theory and
Meyer, J. (2000). Qualitative research in health care: Using community development practice. Journal of the
qualitative methods in health related action research. Community Development Society, 33, 33–52.
British Medical Journal, 320, 178–181. Pfeferbaum, R. L., Pfeferbaum, B., Wyche, K., Norris, F., &
Norris, F. & Kaniasty, K. (1996). Received and perceived Reissman, D. (2006). Community Assessment of Resilience
social support in times of stress: A test of the social Tool. Oklahoma City, OA: Terrorism and Disaster Center
support deterioration deterrence model. Journal of of the National Child Traumatic Stress Network, http://
Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 498–511. www.oumedicine.com/Workiles/College%20of%20
Norris, F., Baker, C., Murphy, A., & Kaniasty, K. (2005a). Medicine/AD-Psychiatry/CART_description_060509.
Social support mobilization and deterioration ater pdf (accessed March 2, 2011).
Mexico’s 1999 lood: Efects of context, gender, and Pfeferbaum, B., Reissman, D., Pfeferbaum, R. L., Klomp,
time. American Journal of Community Psychology, 36, R. W., & Gurwitch, R. H. (2007). Building resilience to
15–28. mass trauma events. In L. Doll, S. Bonzo, J. Mercy, & D.
Norris, F., Watson, P., Hamblen, J., & Pfeferbaum, B. Sleet (eds.), Handbook on injury and violence prevention
(2005b). Provider perspectives on disaster mental interventions (pp. 347–358). New York: Kluwer Academic.
health services in Oklahoma City. Journal of Aggression, Pooley, J., Cohen, L., & O ’ Connor, M. (2006). Links
Maltreatment, and Trauma, 10, 649–661. between community and individual resilience: evidence
Norris, F., Hamblen, J., Watson, P., et al. (2006). Toward from cyclone afected communities in north west
understanding and creating systems of postdisaster care: Australia. In D. Paton & D. Johnston (eds.), Disaster
A case study of New York’s response to the World Trade resilience: An integrated approach (pp. 161–173).
Center disaster. In E. C. Ritchie, P. Watson, & Springield, IL: Charles C. homas.
M. Friedman (eds.), Interventions following mass violence Putnam, R. (2000). Bowling alone. New York: Simon &
and disasters: Strategies for mental health practices Schuster.
(pp. 343–364). New York: Guilford Press. Quarantelli, E. (1989). Characteristics of citizen groups
Norris, F., Stevens, S., Pfeferbaum, B., Wyche, K., & which emerge with respect to hazardous waste sites. In
Pfeferbaum, R. (2008). Community resilience as a D. Peck (ed.), Psychosocial efects of hazardous toxic waste
metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for disposal on communities (pp. 177–195). Springield, IL:
disaster readiness. American Journal of Community Charles C. homas.
Psychology, 41, 127–150. Rappaport, J. (1995). Empowerment meets narrative:
Norris, F., Tracy, M., & Galea, S. (2009). Looking for Listening to stories and creating settings. American
resilience: Understanding the longitudinal trajectories Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 795–807.

174
Chapter 11: Community resilience

Reissman, D., Spencer, S., Tanielian, T., & Stein, B. (2005). Sonn, C. & Fisher, A. (1998). Sense of community:
Integrating behavioral aspects into community Community resilient responses to oppression and
preparedness and response systems. In Y. Danieli, D. change. Journal of Community Psychology, 26, 457–472.
Brom, & J. Sills (eds.), he trauma of terror: Sharing Tse, S. & Liew, T. (2004). New Zealand experiences: How is
knowledge and shared care (pp. 707–720). Binghamton, community resilience manifested in Asian communities?
NY: Haworth Press. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 2,
Riad, J., Norris, F., & Ruback, R. B. (1999). Predicting 1–8.
evacuation in two major disasters: Risk perceptions, Tobin, G. & Whiteford, L. (2002). Community resilience
social inluence, and access to resources. Journal of and volcano hazard: he eruption of Tungurahua and
Applied Social Psychology, 29, 918–934. evacuation of the Faldas in Ecuador. Disasters, 26, 28–48.
Rich, R., Edelstein, M., Hallman, W., & Wandersman, A. Trickett, E. (1995). he community context of disaster
(1995). Citizen participation and empowerment: he and traumatic stress: An ecological perspective from
case of local environmental hazards. American Journal of community psychology. In S. Hobfoll & M. DeVries
Community Psychology, 23, 657–676. (eds.), Extreme stress and communities: Impact and
Rose, A. (2004). Deining and measuring economic intervention (pp. 11–25). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.
resilience to disasters. Disaster Prevention and van den Eynde, J. & Veno, A. (1999). Coping with disastrous
Management, 13, 307–314. events: An empowerment model of community healing.
Rose, A. (2005). Analyzing terrorist threats to the economy: In R. Gist & B. Lubin (eds.), Response to disaster:
A computable general equilibrium approach. In P. Psychosocial, community, and ecological approaches (pp.
Gordon, J. Moore, & H. Richardson (eds.), Economic 167–192). Philadelphia, PA: Bruner/Mazel.
impacts of terrorist attacks (pp. 196–217). Cheltenham, Wellman, B. & Frank, K. (2001). Network capital in a
UK: Edward Elgar. multilevel world: Getting support from personal
Rose, G. (1981). Strategy of prevention: Lessons from communities. In N. Lin, K. Cook, & R. Burt (eds.), Social
cardiovascular disease. British Medical Journal, 282, capital: heory and research (pp. 233–273). New York:
1847–1951. Aldine de Gruyter.
Rose, G. (2001). Sick individuals and sick populations. Werner, E. E. & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable but invincible:
International Journal of Epidemiology, 30, 427–432. A longitudinal study of resilient children and youth. New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Rupasingha, A., Goetz, S. J., & Freshwater, D. (2006). he
production of social capital in US counties. Journal of Westfall, J. M., Van Vorst, R. F., Main, D. S., & Herbert, C.
Socio-Economics, 35, 83–101. (2006). Community-based participatory research in
practice-based research networks. Annals of Family
Rutter, M. (1993). Resilience: some conceptual Medicine, 4, 8–14.
considerations. Journal of Adolescent Health, 14,
626–631. Wisner, B. (2001). Risk and the neoliberal state: Why post-
Mitch lessons didn’t reduce El Salvador’s earthquake
Sampson, R., Raudenbush, S., & Earls, F. (1997). losses. Disasters, 25, 251–268.
Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of
collective eicacy. Science, 277, 918–924. Woods, C. (1998). Development arrested: he blues and
plantation power in the Mississippi Delta. New York:
Shaw-Taylor, Y. (1999). Measurement of community health: Verso.
he Social Health Index. New York: University Press of
Working Group on Governance Dilemmas in Bioterrorism
America.
Response (2004). Leading during bioattacks and
Sherrieb, K., Norris, F., & Galea, S. (2010). Measuring epidemics with the public’s trust and help. Biosecurity
capacities for community resilience. Social Indicators and Bioterrorism: Biodefense Stategy, Practice, and
Research, 99, 227–247. Science, 2, 25–39.

175
Section
Section3 Resilience in families, communities, and societies
Chapter
Trauma, culture, and resiliency

12 Carl C. Bell

Introduction Protective factors that cultivate resiliency and pro-


tect against trauma fall in to several categories (Bell
he generic principles for recovery from trauma
et al., 2005). Protective factors are biological (genetic
and loss involve the reconstruction of meaning, the
such as intellectual ability, personality/temperamen-
rebuilding of hope, and the sense of empowerment tal traits, and toughness), psychological (intrapsychic
necessary to regain control over one’s being and life attributes: adaptive mechanisms such as ego resiliency,
(Herman, 1992). Constructs of meaning are insepar- motivation, humor and hardiness, and perceptions of
able from culture. Furthermore, genetic inluences that self; emotional attributes: emotional well-being, life
put individuals at risk for developing traumatic symp- satisfaction, optimism, happiness, trust, dispositional
toms cannot be adequately understood without taking optimism, dispositional hope, and life satisfaction;
into consideration their interdependence with envir- cognitive attributes: cognitive styles and processing,
onmental factors (Binder et al., 2008), such as culture. causal attribution such as an internal locus of control
his chapter highlights the interface between trauma, and blame, worldview or philosophy of life, change
culture, and resiliency. in philosophy of life, assumptive world, and wisdom;
Apfel and Simon (1996), Masten and Coatsworth and spiritual attributes: spiritual well-being, spirit-
(1998), and Wolin and Wolin (1996) outlined the ual development, existential issues, and attributes of
characteristics of resiliency as (1) resourcefulness; post-traumatic growth), social (interpersonal skills,
(2) ability to attract and use support; (3) curiosity and interpersonal relationships, connectedness, and social
intellectual mastery; (4) compassion, with detach- support), and environmental (such as positive life
ment; (5) conviction of one’s right to survive; (6) ability events and socioeconomic status). Culture intersects
to remember and invoke images of good and sustain- with these protective factors by facilitating practices
ing igures; (7) ability to be in touch with afects, not that cultivate toughness, motivation, humor, hardi-
denying or suppressing major afects as they arise; (8) ness, perceptions of self, optimism, trust, dispositional
goal to live for; (9) vision of the possibility and desir- hope, dispositional optimism, life satisfaction, cog-
ability of restoration of civilized moral order; (10) the nitive styles and processing, causal atribution (e.g.,
need and ability to help others; (11) ability to con- internal locus of control and blame), worldview or
ceptualize; (12) an afective repertory; (13) altruism philosophy of life, change in life philosophy, assump-
toward others; and (14) turning “traumatic helpless- tive world perspectives, wisdom, spiritual well-being,
ness” into “learned helpfulness.” In addition to these spiritual development, existential equipoise, interper-
characteristics of resiliency, there are more culturally sonal skills, interpersonal relationships, connected-
bound characteristics of resiliency (Bell, 2001) such as ness, and ainity for social support.
(1) having a sense of “Atman” (true self); (2) develop-
ing kokoro (heart), also known as “indomitable ight-
ing spirit” (Bell & Suggs, 1998); (3) having a totem, an
Cultural considerations for
animal spirit that lives inside; and (4) being able to cul- understanding resiliency
tivate chi, Chinese for internal energy (Cohen, 1997; Culture is a complex, shared system of meaning con-
Bell, 2000, 2008a). sisting of learned behavior and meanings that are

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

176
Chapter 12: Trauma, culture, and resiliency

socially transferred in various life-activity settings for on the group as a collective: “I am, because we are” is the
purposes of individual and collective adjustment and key philosophy (Chun et al., 2006). Locus of control is a
adaptation (Marsella et al., 2008). Accordingly, cul- very pertinent personality dimension to coping. Cross-
ture shapes and constructs our realities by contribut- cultural studies have revealed that individuals from
ing to our worldviews, perceptions, and orientations individualistic cultures, such as European-Americans
with ideas, morals, and preferences (Marsella, 2005). and New Zealanders, tend to have a stronger sense of
Further, culture changes and, as it is transmitted gen- internal locus of control than individuals from collect-
eration to generation, it is shaped by various inside and ivistic cultures, such as the Japanese (Bond & Tornatzky,
outside contexts and dynamics. his system of mean- 1973; Mahler, 1974), the Chinese (Hamid, 1994), Asian
ing consists of norms, beliefs, and values that proscribe Indians (Chandler et al., 1982), and individuals from
behavior. As recovery from trauma intimately involves Zambia and Zimbabwe (Munro, 1979).
reconstruction of meaning, consideration of resili- Some authors have noted that cultural, racial, and
ency must take into account a person’s speciic cultural ethnic people of color are more adversely efected
norms, beliefs, and values, and cultural dynamics (e.g., by various traumas (Bell, 2008b; Norris & Alegria,
the subject’s culture of origin and the extent of the sub- 2008). For example, Latinos and non-Hispanic blacks
ject’s acculturation or level of ethnic identiication). were more adversely afected by Hurricaine Andrew
Further, ‘‘factors associated with ethnicity and culture than were non-Hispanic whites (Perilla et al., 2002).
strongly inluence individuals’ vulnerability and resili- Following the September 11, 2001 terrorist attacks,
ence; determine their coping styles, cognitive response Galea et al. (2005) found cultural, racial, and ethnic
to stress, and the nature of social support; shape their people of color were more likely to met criteria for
psychopathology, their experiencing of distress, and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) than non-
their clustering of symptoms; and inluence the course Latino white respondents. Additionally, cultural,
and outcome of psychiatric conditions’’ (Charney et al., racial, and ethnic people of color within the USA are
2002, p. 69). When evaluating another’s status, Hansen less likely than whites to seek mental health treatment
(2002) suggested consideration of multicultural compe- until their symptoms are more severe. Further, they are
tencies such as knowing the person’s history of victim- less likely to seek treatment from mental health spe-
ization and manifestations of such issues as oppression, cialists as they are more inclined to use primary care
prejudice, marginalization, and their psychological or to use informal sources of support (Department
sequelae; certainly these dynamics can inluence resili- of Health and Human Services, 2001). Also, it is less
ence in negative and positive ways. Further, as family clear whether acculturation is a beneit (Marshall &
structures, gender roles, values, and beliefs difer across Orlando, 2002) or detriment (Vega et al., 1998) to resili-
cultures and yet afect personality formation, develop- ency. Issues of acculturative stress have been identiied
mental outcomes, and manifestations of mental illness as strategies of non-dominant groups, such as integra-
and wellness, it is important to consider these dynamics tion, assimilation, separation, or marginalization; and
of resilience or vulnerability as well. of dominant groups, such as multiculturalism, melt-
Unfortunately, in an American culture highly ing pot, segregation, or exclusion (Berry, 2006).
charged with individualism, one of the most import- Some researchers suggest that ethnic, racial, and
ant, yet neglected, aspect in the ield of stress and cop- cultural people of color seem to be more resilient
ing (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) is the context of culture than whites. For example, Ryf et al. (2003) reported
(Chun et al., 2006). Currently, the cultural elements that blacks report higher levels of psychological well-
of individualism and collectivism are the most widely being than whites, and, ater controlling for education,
studied (Triandis, 1995). Individualism and collectiv- income, perceived discrimination, and other demo-
ism consist of a set of values, attitudes, and behaviors graphic variables, blacks would have an even better level
that vary based on the value placed on the individual of psychological well-being than whites if it were not
versus the individual’s group (Hofstede, 1980). In indi- for the inding that blacks experienced more discrim-
vidualistic cultures, the individual is the hub of atten- ination than whites. Keyes (2007) found similar results
tion and emphasis. Accordingly, individual freedoms, and these observations are consistent with the obser-
rights, and privileges are of utmost importance: “I vation that African-American women (despite their
think, therefore I am” is the key philosophy. In collect- status in contemporary society resulting from sexism
ivistic cultures, by comparison an emphasis is placed and discrimination) have the lowest suicide rates in the

177
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

USA (Goldsmith et al., 2002). Unfortunately, there is a and ethnic groups (Sue & Sue, 1999). his aspect of
dearth of research on cultural, racial, and ethnic people European and European-American culture also makes
of color (Department of Health and Human Services, it diicult to appreciate diversity within racial groups.
2001), so it is diicult to achieve clarity in issues of Achenbach (2008, p. 25) underscored this observation
acculturation and resiliency. by noting
Other authors have studied issues of hardiness Multicultural encounters can spawn both beneits and
(Maddi & Harvey, 2006). “Hardy attitudes” are com- risks. For the study and treatment of psychopathology,
mitment, control, and challenge, and “hardy skills” the increasing salience of cultural variations forces us
involve interacting with signiicant others in a fashion to question the generalizability of our mainly Western
that enhances one’s sense of social support. Hardiness concepts. Until recently, much of the professional lit-
involves self-care aimed at maintaining a level of erature related to psychopathology was written pri-
marily by Westerners from a handful of societies. hese
arousal that is optimal for “hardy coping” and social
Westerners have written mainly about Westerners and
interaction eforts. Self-eicacy, ego strength, opti-
for Westerners.
mism, resilience, post-traumatic growth, religious-
ness, endurance, patience, humility, and lexibility are Unfortunately, monocultural ethnocentrism oten
aspects of hardiness. he notion of hardiness appears precludes the appropriate use of exposure to mul-
to be present in all cultural, racial, and ethnic groups; tiple cultures as a creative source of possible coping
consequently, it can be considered a generic aspect of responses.
human nature (Maddi & Harvey, 2006).
Religious and spiritual coping are imbued with Genetic/environmental interactions
various aspects of culture. Culture shapes how people Major depressive disorders have been linked to levels of
seek signiicance, how events are interpreted to have serotonin in the brain, and many antidepressant drugs
meaning for people, how familiar people are with the act by blocking reuptake of serotonin by acting directly
coping process, how they use speciic modes of cop- on the brain serotonin transporter (5-HTT). Caspi and
ing, and what vehicles they use for transformation of colleagues (2002) have examined gene–environmental
trauma (Pargament, 1997). Culture determines how interactions in the development of depression by look-
people ind meaning and gain control, comfort, intim- ing at genetic variants of this transporter. One variant,
acy, and closeness to God (regardless of how God is 5HTTLPR, moderated the inluence of early childhood
deined) so they can transform their traumatized lives maltreatment and stressful life events on the devel-
(with the inherent sense of helplessness that accom- opment of depression. Kaufman and Henrich (2000)
panies trauma) into helpfulness and purpose (Klaassen noted clinical studies of individuals with a history of
et al., 2006). Further, in order successfully to achieve abuse which suggested that the availability of a car-
a communal aspect to sufering and coping, cultural ing and stable parent or alternative guardian is one of
guidelines are necessary to shape community behav- the most important factors that distinguishes abused
ior. Culture shapes how people engage in active coping: individuals with good developmental outcomes from
planning, seeking instrumental social support, seeking those with more deleterious outcomes. Kaufman and
emotional support, suppressing competing activities, colleagues (2006) also looked at the efect of social sup-
practicing religion, reinterpreting events as positive and ports in moderating for genetic risk of depression in
growth producing, accepting the trauma, focusing on maltreated children, showing that maltreated children
and venting emotions, seeking calmness, denying psy- with positive social supports had depression scores that
chologically and behaviorally avoiding, using alcohol/ were only slightly greater than control subjects regard-
drugs, and engaging in humor (Carver et al., 1989). less of genotype. his indicates that the quality and
availability of social supports is an extremely potent
Monocultural ethnocentrism factor in determining risk for depression in maltreated
Monocultural ethnocentrism is deined as the belief in children – with the efect greatest for those maltreated
one “right” culture and valuing that culture over others children with the most vulnerable genotypes (Kaufman
(Taylor, 2006). Monocultural ethnocentrism (usu- et al., 2006). Clearly, cultural context has the potential
ally European-centrism) makes it diicult to appre- to inluence parenting styles and the quality of social
ciate cultural diferences between the cultural, racial, supports in a maltreated child’s life.

178
Chapter 12: Trauma, culture, and resiliency

he interaction between the parents and ofspring Further, there is a wealth of psychiatric research
(homas & Chess, 1977; Rettew et al., 2006) can either be indicating a relationship between degree of obsessive–
protective or a risk factor for illness. herefore, taking a compulsive disorder, culture, and religious identity
developmental psychopathology, family-based, gene– and practice (Greenberg & Witztum, 1994; Chia, 1996;
environment approach should better equip researchers Raphael et al., 1996; Texcan & Millet, 1997; Shooka
and clinicians alike to understand etiopathology and et al., 1998; Tek & Ulug, 2001; Abramowitz et al., 2002;
ultimately treatment. he key is to prevent risk factors Greenberg & Sheler, 2002; Sica et al., 2002; Rassin &
from becoming predictive factors by understanding Koster, 2003).
protective factors (Bell, 2007). We should aim to devise Our group and others have been struck by what
strategies to keep children well, try particularly hard appears to be the protective qualities of family-based
to prevent at-risk children from getting ill, and inter- activities and wonder whether the data are already
vene with those who are already ill. Unfortunately, the strong enough to support a greater role for family-
major focus in mental health has been those who are based approaches aimed at increasing family cohesion
already ill. as a strategy for health and resilience promotion (Bell
et al., 2008; National Research Council & Institute of
Religiosity as an environmental influence Medicine, 2009).
that can impact genetic expression
he concept that being raised in a religious household Specific cultural, racial, and ethnic
is protective has long been a core belief of religious dis- considerations for cultivating
ciplines. High levels of religious involvement predicts
a reduce risk of substance misuse (Larson & Wilson, resiliency
1980; Payne et al., 1991; Koenig et al., 1994; Gorsuch,
1995), and Heath et al. (1999) found a protective efect African-American resiliency systems
of religious involvement and values against adolescent African-Americans have a number of characteristics
alcohol use. Spirituality has also been conceptualized that cultivate resiliency: historical experiences, cul-
by some as a genetic component of personality (Luby tural traits such as family characteristics, cultural
et al., 1999; Cloninger, 2004). In a population-based values, ethnic orientation, strong kinship bonds,
adult twin sample, Kendler et al. (2003) found that gen- communal orientation, strong religious orientation,
eral religiosity was inversely and signiicantly linked to importance of tradition, strong work ethic, individ-
nicotine dependence, alcohol dependence, drug mis- ual and academic achievement, respect for elders,
use and dependence, and adult antisocial behavior. harmony with nature, holistic thinking, adherence
However, general religiosity was signiicantly related to mainstream American cultural norms and values,
to panic disorder. religion, communication styles, food, holidays, and
Maes et al. (1999) found that genetic factors could health considerations (Billingsley, 1992; Jagers &
explain the association between church attendance and Mock, 1995; Hill, 1997; Sue & Sue, 2003; Carswell &
alcohol use in males, but in females, the association is Carswell, 2008).
primarily linked to shared environmental factors and Racial socialization is a protective factor in African-
genotype–environment covariance. In the Netherlands American families (Klonof & Landrine, 1999; Brook
Twin Register adult twin sample, Koopmans et al. et al., 2007; Corneille & Belgrave, 2007; Nasim et al.,
(1999) examined gene–environment interactions in 2007). Resiliency in African-American families is
the inluence of religiosity on initiation of drinking strongly associated with facilitating self-eicacy,
in females and found a variance of 0% between high parental supervision that is protective (Donenberg
religiosity and initiation of drinking and 40% between et al., 2002), being future oriented, and spiritual-
low religiosity and initiation. Subjects with a religious ity (McCreary et al., 2006). Nobles (1986) suggested
upbringing and who participate in church activities that black families give their children a sense of his-
scored lower on Sensation Seeking Questionnaire tory, sense of family, and sense of spirituality, and
scales, with religiosity being associated with reduced Staples (1976) noted the unique characteristic of the
genetic inluence on disinhibition, particularly in black family as being the non-kinship extended fam-
males (Boomsma et al., 1999). ily: both of which are protective. Research inds the

179
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

most common racial socialization message given to Arab-American resiliency systems


African-American youth was the importance of the
Arab, Muslim, and Middle Eastern people are very
work ethic and achievement. Racial and ethnic pride
family oriented, with religious involvement and piety,
messages were next, and the third was race-related
including faith, prayer, and spiritual practices, being
lessons about black heritage and historical traditions
highly valued (Abi-Hashem, 2008). In addition, com-
(hornton et al., 1990). Racial socialization mes-
munity, hospitality, truthfulness, thankfulness and
sages are generally shown to have a protective efect
gratitude, respect, esteem, contentment and satisfac-
(Stevenson et al., 1997; Fischer & Shaw, 1999). Miller
tion, generosity, dignity and honor, clear gender roles
and MacIntosh (1999) have asserted that racial iden-
for the male and female, saving face and avoiding pub-
tity can create resilience by enhancing collective self-
lic shame, working hard and providing for your family,
esteem. However, more research needs to be carried
social obligations and relational duties, patience, and
out on how African-American racial socialization
endurance are all very highly valued (Abi-Hashem,
processes are protective, as research results are mixed.
2008). hese cultural, racial, and ethnic groups use
For example, Arroyo and Zigler (1995) found that the
many metaphors, proverbs, and sayings containing
more students endorsed racelessness, the more anx-
cultural and spiritual wisdom (Abi-Hashem, 2008).
iety and depression they reported. However, these
Traditional therapies such as religious oaths, shrine
same students showed a greater sense of self-eicacy
visitation, purposeful prayers, evil eye protection,
on achievement attitudes. Similarly, Anglin and Wade
script writings, psychic activities, and natural ther-
(2007) found that racial socialization positively con-
apies are common resiliency-cultivating approaches
tributes to academic adjustment, while internalized
to life.
Afrocentric racial identity was negatively related to
overall college adjustment.
Asian cultural resiliency systems
American-Indian/Alaska Native resiliency Religion and philosophy vary across cultures, and
religious and philosophical beliefs profoundly
systems inluence how people cope (Tweed & Conway,
Like African-Americans, American-Indian/Alaska 2006). Further, people within the same culture have
Natives have a great many challenges that cultivate diferent approaches to sufering, adaptation, and the
their resiliency, for example the genocide and ter- environment, resulting in diferent coping strategies.
rorism perpetrated against American-Indians in the here is a vast array of cultural subgroups within
eighteenth and nineteenth centuries (Johnson et al., the general Asian culture, and these subgroups are
2008). Similar to African-Americans, but to a greater inluenced to varying degrees by the three major
extent, death rates from crime and disease are high religious and philosophies of Asian culture: Taoism,
in American-Indians. Further, substance abuse is a Confucianism, and Buddhism.
huge problem, with suicide rates being a close second he ubiquity of change in Chinese culture was
to European-American rates (Goldsmith et al., 2002). explored by Ji (2001), who found that Chinese partici-
Native-American key values that foster resilience pants in her study were more likely to airm that change
involve an emphasis on spirituality; sacredness of all was ubiquitous in the world than were the European-
living things and respect for nature, realized by cooper- American participants. his high expectation of
ation and harmony with nature; cyclical thinking; change and of change in the direction of change (rever-
interdependence between people; sharing and social sal of trends) coheres with the Taoist tradition of China
responsibility; extended family; and respect for elders. (Chen, 2006a, 2006b). Taoism teaches a philosophy of
Humor is a major part of many American-Indian cul- life in terms of how, regardless of the circumstances, to
tures and these cultures gravitate toward approaches live a contented, serene life with equipoise. In addition,
that emphasize values and strengths (Lafromboise et al., in the tradition of sophisticated Asian culture, Taoism
1990). A major strength for American-Indians is that also proscribes mind/body practices that encourage
their medicine is oriented toward healing through breathing exercises that cultivate physical and mental
restoring the balance and harmony with the body and well-being. Lao-tzu was clear that yin and yang rule
world using strategies involving spirituality, such as rit- nature thus “Fortune owes its existence to misfortune,
ual puriication in sweat lodges. and misfortune is hidden in fortune” (Chen, 2006b).

180
Chapter 12: Trauma, culture, and resiliency

Individuals wise in Taoism know it is not either/or, but provide inner serenity and compassion. Buddhism
rather both/and. is based on catvāri āryasatyāni (“four noble truths”)
Confucian tradition teaches the value of perse- (Byrom, 1976; Chen, 2006a). (1) Dukkha, the truth of
verance in response to diiculty; hence people from sufering: – life has sufering (craving or greed; and
Asian cultures tend to believe in the utility of efort. aversion or hatred; ignorance or delusion). (2) Tanha,
Confucianism also emphasized family piety, a moral the truth of arising of sufering: sufering comes from
duty to respect and serve elders, ancestor worship, dig- craving and aversion. (3) Nirvana, the truth of liber-
niied expression, reserved manner of speaking, chaste ation from sufering: freedom from sufering is possible
temperament, and industriousness (Kaplan & Huynh, by transforming craving or greed, aversion or hatred,
2008); consequently, the tendency to catastrophize is ignorance or delusion by following the right path. (4)
minimized. (Research has shown clear links between Magga, the truth of the eightfold path: right speech,
catastrophizing and a lack of self-eicacy and risk of right action, right living, right efort, right mind-
developing PTSD [Bryant & Guthrie, 2005].) fulness, right meditation, right thought, and right
Buddhism, a multifaceted religion originating in understanding hese eight paths can be classed as
India, has been an inluential cultural force in Asia “morality,” “meditation” (requiring regular eforts to
for more than 2500 years (Chen, 2006a). he conser- practice mindfulness or bare attention in both formal
vative heravada (southern Buddhism) – prevalent meditation and in day to day life), and “wisdom” (the
in hailand, Sri Lanka, Burma, and Laos – adheres to capacity to be non-attached to craving or greed, aver-
the early scriptures believed to embody the authentic sion or hatred, ignorance or delusion) (Chen, 2006a).
teachings of the Buddha. his form of Buddhism is hese attributes are exempliied by various apho-
called Hinayana (“lesser vehicle”) by its critics because risms: “hose who know themselves are enlightened”
it mainly focuses on enlightenment of its practitioners. “hose who conquer themselves are strong” “he
By comparison, the reportedly more evolved and adap- greatest woes comes from not knowing contentment;
tive Mahayana Buddhism (“great vehicle”), dominant the greatest of faults comes from craving for gains” “A
in China, Taiwan, Japan, Korea, and Vietnam, stresses sage is free from excessive pursuit, extravagance and
the ideal of Bodhisattvas: enlightened beings who vow arrogance” (Ajaya, 1997).
to save all sentient beings from sufering. he Lotus
Sutra, a central feature of Mahayana Buddhism, delves Caribbean black resiliency system
into the nature of the mind, and “ultimate reality,” sug-
he 10 key values of Eastern Caribbean people that
gesting emptiness, is the highest possible stage in the
help to cultivate resiliency are the extended family, a
evolution of human consciousness, because the mind is
strong work ethic, multiculturalism and blacks’ experi-
pure and “empty” of thoughts and feelings, thus allow-
ence with racism, the value of education, spirituality,
ing transcendence of dualistic consciousness (Chen,
respect (as the culture is very authoritarian), veneration
2006a). From this expanded state of consciousness (Bell,
of elders and ancestors, communalism, creativity, and
1980) comes wisdom, unconditional love, compassion,
disaster as a result of negative actions (Dudley-Grant &
and a loss of the fear of death. Zen (Chan) developed
Etheridge, 2008).
out of Mahayana Buddhism and practices enlighten-
ment through “direct seeing” of the true nature of the
mind. Reportedly, Zen was introduced to China in the Asian Indian cultural resiliency system
sixth century by an Indian monk named Bodhidharma here is a great deal of diversity within the Indian
(Oyama, 1966) or Ta Mo (Smith, 1964), who sought community that is complicated by difering levels of
enlightenment through intuitive insights into the “self- acculturation to Western society. Although Hinduism
nature” (true self). Bodhidharma was also extremely is most widely practiced, there are ive other major
inluential in the development of Asian martial arts. religions, and most emphasize transcendence, holistic
Buddhist practices provide more than stress values, collectivism, self-realization, and transform-
reduction, they provide a pathway that is free from ation of the self (Prashantham, 2008). Developing a
life’s troubles and sorrows. Like Christianity, efect- sense of “Atman” or “true self ” is one way in which many
ive coping in Buddhism requires spiritual/personal Asian Indians cultivate their resiliency (Bell, 2001).
transformation, which is cultivated by mental and Asian Indian cultural values that cultivate resilience
physical discipline leading to enlightenment to include humility, respect, responsibility, politeness,

181
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

morality, tolerance, spirituality, holistic perception, Native Hawaiian resiliency systems


hospitality, and non-violence (Prashantham, 2008).
Native Hawaiian values that cultivate resiliency include
Folk religions that emphasize karma are a large fac-
aloha (afectionate, compassionate, and loving senti-
tor in encouraging people to take the “high road,” and
ments), aina (the environment nourishing the body,
displays of anger (behind anger is hurt) are frowned
mental health, and relationships with the spiritual
upon because it disrupts harmony. Metaphors are
world), ha’aha’a (modesty or humility, interconnect-
very important forms of communication in India, and
edness, respect, and caring), kokua (helping other),
allegories are frequently used to convey truth. As in
lokahi (harmony), mana (divine or spiritual power),
other cultures, Asian Indians have their own healing
ohana (family), alaka’i (leadership), pono (justice),
and strength-based strategies such as yoga, ayurveda,
and kela (excellence) (McCubbin et al., 2008). Social
naturopathy, siddha treatment, homeopathy, unani
relationships are very important to Hawaiians and
treatment, tantra, shamans, reiki, pranic healing,
their values support these relationships. Ho’oponopono,
kalari, and marma (Prashantham, 2008). Family
aimed at restoring harmony, is another culturally spe-
is very important to Asian Indians and resiliency-
ciic resiliency-cultivating strategy of Hawaiian people
cultivating practices that emphasize mind/body/sprit
(Rezentes, 1996). Native Hawaiians have three pat-
it exactly into their cosmology. he capacity to take
terns of resilience: coherence or the family’s capacity of
on responsibility for relatives outside of the nuclear
developing a sense of trust, predictability, and manage-
family is another feature of the culture that assists in
ability; problem-solving communication; and the fam-
resiliency. Finally, beliefs in karma may lead to quicker
ily’s shared ethnic identity (hompson et al., 1995).
acceptance of the inevitable, thus the catastrophizing
resulting in increased risk for developing PTSD can be
avoided (Bryant & Guthrie, 2005). General issues
Rather than simply approaching trauma from a
diagnostic perspective, it is important to consider cul-
Latino/Native cultural resiliency systems tural aspects of salutogenesis as seeking to cope with
Like other cultural, racial, and ethnic groups, there is trauma worldwide. Essential considerations in under-
a great deal of diversity among Latin-Americans and, standing the interface between trauma, culture, and
as with other groups, the generalizations presented resiliency include traditional healing practices, thera-
may have exceptions. Most Latinos have a strong value peutic contexts, culturally based medical practices,
for familia and extended kinship systems, suggesting shamanic practices, assumptive belief systems about
strong regards for collectivism and interdependence. illness and health, perspectives on psychobiology of
In addition, there is a strong Catholic inluence within traumatic stress (e.g., mind–body relationships), a
Latino cultures. Values that cultivate resiliency include culture’s implicit psychological and behavioral princi-
familismo (family ties), compadrazco (friendship), and ples, the range of healer roles and practices, individual
personalismo (warm personal relationships); other verses collective practices, and religious and spiritual
relationship values such as respecto (respect), conianza involvement in healing and recovery (Wilson, 2008).
(trust), caridad (caring), tener valor (courage), and Marsella (2005) outlined the generic salutogenic aspects
fortaleza (fortitude); and spiritual values such as espe- of all cultures as beliefs, catharsis, confession, cultural
ranza (hope) and fe (faith) (Padilla & Borrero, 2006; re-embeddedness or separation, re-deinition of prob-
Arredondo et al., 2008). Working with such cultures lems or self, empathy, expression and verbalization of
requires awareness of the role of the family, the recog- problems, faith, forgiveness, hope, identity develop-
nition of religion, and use of indigenous resources. ment and awareness, information exchange, insight,
Don Miguel Ángel Ruiz (1997) provides an example interpretation of events, locus of control alterations,
of a Latino/Native cultural resiliency system in his out- mobilization of endorphin and immune system, social
line of the four agreements from his tradition of the supports, and so on. hroughout history, civilizations
southern Mexican Toltec people: aggreement one, “be have experienced trauma and, accordingly, have devel-
impeccable with your word”; aggreement two, “don’t oped practices, techniques, wisdom, and other skill
take anything personally”; aggreement three, “don’t sets to address this issue and to cultivate resiliency in
make assumptions”; and aggreement four, “always do preparation for the inevitable. In an insightful article,
your best.” Wilson (2004) described the “abyss experience,” which

182
Chapter 12: Trauma, culture, and resiliency

he deines as being confrontation with evil and death References


(i.e., the traumatic experience), experience with soul
death and with non-being (i.e., absence of self/iden- Abi-Hashem, N. (2008). Arab Americans: Understanding
their challenges, needs, and struggles. In A. J. Marsella,
tity), a sense of abandonment by humanity (i.e., loss
J. L. Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski (eds.),
of connectedness), ultimate aloneness and despairing Ethnocultural perspectives on disaster and trauma:
(i.e., separation and isolation), and cosmic challenge Foundations, issues and applications (pp. 115–173). New
of meaning (i.e., spirituality and a sense of the mys- York: Springer.
tical). Kohut (1966, p. 264) stated “Man’s capacity to Abramowitz, J. S., Huppert, J. D., Cohen, A. B., et al. (2002).
acknowledge the initeness of his existence and to act Religious obsessions and compulsions in a non-clinical
in accordance with this painful discovery may well be sample: he Penn Inventory of Scrupulosity (PIOS).
his greatest psychological achievement….” Behaviour Research and herapy, 40, 825–838.
Marsella (2005) noted that all healing subcultures Achenbach, T. M. (2008). Multicultural perspectives on
have (1) a set of assumptions about the nature of the developmental psychopathology. In J. J. Hudziak (ed.),
problems that matches their worldview and construc- Developmental psychopathology and wellness (pp. 23–47).
tion of reality; (2) a set of assumptions about the con- Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
text, settings, and requirements for healing to occur; Ajaya, S. (1997). Psychotherapy east and west: A unifying
(3) a set of assumptions and procedures to elicit par- paradigm. Honesdale, PA: Himalayan International
Institute.
ticular expectations, emotions, and behaviors; (4) a set
of requirements for activity and participation levels Anglin, D. M. & Wade, J. C. (2007). Racial socialization,
and/or roles for patient, family, and therapist; and (5) racial identity, and Black students’ adjustment to college.
Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13,
speciic requirements for therapist training and skills 207–215.
expertise criteria. Unfortunately, as Satcher’s report
Apfel, R. J. & Simon, B. (eds.) (1996). Mineields in their
Mental health: culture, race, and ethnicity highlighted
hearts (pp. 9–11). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
(Department of Health and Human Services, 2001),
there is a dearth of research on mental health issues for Arredondo, P., Bordes, V., & Paniagua, F. A. (2008).
Mexicans, Mexican Americans, Caribbean, and other
various cultural, racial, and ethnic minority groups in Latin Americans. In A. J. Marsella, J. L. Johnson,
the USA, and even less on resiliency and protective fac- P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski (eds.), Ethnocultural
tors in these groups (Bell & Williamson, 2002). perspectives on disaster and trauma: Foundations, issues
and applications (pp. 299–320). New York: Springer.
Conclusions Arroyo, C. G. & Zigler, E. (1995). Racial identity, academic
In the West, in order fully to understand how trauma achievement, and the psychological well-being of
economically disadvantaged adolescents. Journal of
interfaces with culture and resiliency we need to over- Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 903–914.
come our monocultural ethnocentrism. We need
Bell, C. C. (1980). States of consciousness. Journal of the
to learn how to respect other cultures’ resiliency-
National Medical Association, 72, 331–334.
cultivating strategies and stop being critical of other
cultures. To achieve this goal, we must develop a higher Bell, C. C. (2000). Eight pieces of brocade. Chicago, IL:
Community Mental Health Council.
tolerance for what we do not understand because it is
not familiar and become comfortable with diversity. Bell, C. C. (2001). Cultivating resiliency in youth. Journal of
Adolescent Health, 29, 375–381.
We need a higher awareness that our culture is just one
of many, and that there are similarities and commonal- Bell, C. C. (2007). Keeping promises: Ethics and principles
ities among cultures, not solely diferences. In order to in psychiatric practice. In Aspatore Books Staf (ed.),
he art and science of psychiatry (pp. 7–38). Boston, MA:
understand how diferent cultures cultivate resiliency, Aspatore Books.
we must seek to study and research diferent cultural
Bell, C. C. (2008a). Asian martial arts and resiliency. Ethnicity
strategies to create protective factors, strength, and
and Inequities in Health and Social Care, 1: 11–17.
resiliency in their groups. “Wisdom is achieved largely
Bell, C. C. (2008b). Should culture considerations inluence
through man’s ability to overcome his unmodiied nar-
early intervention? In R. Ursano & M. Blumenield
cissism and it rests on his acceptance of the limitations (eds.), Early psychological intervention following mass
of his physical, intellectual, and emotional powers” trauma: Present and future directions (pp. 127–148).
(Kohut, 1966, p. 268). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

183
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

Bell, C. C. & Suggs, H. (1998). Using sports to strengthen Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. & Weintraub, J. (1989). Assessing
resiliency in children – training “heart”: Using sports to coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal
create resiliency. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267–283.
of North America, 7, 859–865. Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moitt, T. E., et al. (2002). Role of
Bell, C. C. & Williamson, J. (2002). Psychiatric services genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children.
across the millennium: A celebration of 50 years of Science, 297, 851–854.
Psychiatric Services journal–special populations. Chandler, T. A., Shama, D. D., Wolf, F. M., & Planchard, S. K.
Psychiatric Services, 53, 419–424. (1982). Multiattributional causality: A ive cross-national
Bell, C. C., Richardson, J., & Blount, M. A. (2005). Suicide samples study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 12,
prevention. In J. R. Lutzker (ed.), Preventing violence: 207–221.
Research and evidence-based intervention strategies (pp. Charney, D. S., Barlow, D. H., Botteron, K., et al. (2002).
217–237). Washington, DC: American Psychological Neuroscience research agenda to guide development of a
Press. pathophysiologically based classiication system. In D. A.
Bell, C. C., Bhana, A., Petersen, I., et al. (2008). Building Regier, D. J. Kupfer, & M. B. First (eds.), Research agenda
protective factors to ofset sexually risky behaviors for DSM V (pp. 31–84). Washington DC: American
among black South African youth: a randomized control Psychiatric Press.
trial. Journal of the National Medical Association, 100, Chen, Y. H. (2006a). Coping with sufering: he Buddhist
936–944. perspective. In P. T. P. Wong & L. C. J. Wong (eds.),
Berry, J. W. (2006). Acculturative stress. In P. T. P. Wong & Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and
L. C. J. Wong (eds.), Handbook of multicultural perspectives coping (pp. 73–90) New York: Springer.
on stress and coping (pp. 287–298). New York: Springer. Chen, Y. H. (2006b). he way of nature as a healing power.
Billingsley, A. (1992). Climbing Jacob’s ladder: he enduring In P. T. P. Wong & L. C. J. Wong (eds.), Handbook
legacy of African-American families. New York: Simon & of multicultural perspectives on stress and coping
Shuster. (pp. 91–103). New York: Springer.
Binder, E. B., Bradley, R. G., Liu, W., et al. (2008). Chia, B. H. (1996). A Singapore study of obsessive-
Association of FKBP5 polymorphisms and childhood compulsive disorder. Sinagapore Medical Journal, 37,
abuse with risk of posttraumatic stress disorder 402–406.
symptoms in adults. Journal of the American Medical Chun, C. A., Moos, R. H., & Cronkite, R. C. (2006).
Association, 299, 1291–1305. Culture: A fundamental concept for the stress and
Bond, M. H. & Tornatzky, L. G. (1973). Locus of control in coping paradigm. In P. T. P. Wong & L. C. J. Wong (eds.),
students in Japan and the United States: Dimensions and Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and
levels of response. Psychologia: An International Journal coping (pp. 29–53). New York: Springer.
of Psychology in the Orient, 16, 209–213. Cloninger, C. R. ( 2004). Feeling good: he science of well-
Boomsma, D. I., deGeus, E. J., van Baal, G. C., et al. (1999). being. New York: Oxford University Press.
A religious upbringing reduces the inluence of genetic Cohen, K. S. (1997). he way of qigong. New York: Ballantine
factors on disinhibition: Evidence for interaction Books.
between genotype and environment in personality. Twin
Research, 2, 115–125. Corneille, M. A. & Belgrave, F. Z. (2007). Ethnic identity,
neighborhood risk, and adolescent drug and sex
Brook, J. S., Duan, T., Brook, D. W., & Ning, Y. (2007). attitudes and refusal eicacy: he urban African
Pathways to nicotine dependence in African American American girls’ experience. Journal of Drug Education,
and Puerto Rican young adults. American Journal of 37, 177–190.
Addictions. 16, 450–456.
Department of Health and Human Services (2001). Mental
Bryant, R. & Guthrie, R. M. (2005). Factor for posttraumatic health: Culture, race, and ethnicity. A supplement to
stress: A study of trainee ireighters. Psychological “Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon General”.
Science, 16, 749–752. Rockville, MD: US Department of Mental Health and
Byrom, T. (trans.) (1976). he Dhammapada: he sayings of Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health
the Buddha. New York: Vintage Books. Services Administration.
Carswell, S. B. & Carswell, M. A. (2008). Meeting the Donenberg, G., Wilson, H. W., Emerson, E., & Bryant, F. B.
physical, psychological, and social needs of African (2002). Holding the line with a watchful eye: he impact
Americans following disaster. In A. J. Marsella, J. L. of perceived parental permissiveness and parental
Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski (eds.), Ethnocultural monitoring on risky sexual behavior among adolescents
perspectives on disaster and trauma: Foundations, issues in psychiatric care. AIDS Education and Prevention, 14,
and applications (pp. 39–71). New York: Springer. 138–157.

184
Chapter 12: Trauma, culture, and resiliency

Dudley-Grant, G. R. & Etheridge, W. (2008). Caribbean Jagers, R. J. & Mock, L. O. (1995). he Communalism Scale
Blacks (Haitians, Jamaicans, Virgin Islanders, Eastern and collectivistic–individualistic tendencies: Some
Caribbean): Responses to disasters in cultural context. In preliminary indings. Journal of Black Psychology, 21,
A. J. Marsella, J. L. Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski 153–167.
(eds.), Ethnocultural perspectives on disaster and trauma: Ji, L. J. (2001). Culture, change, and prediction. Psychological
Foundations, issues and applications (pp. 209–240). New Science, 12, 450–456.
York: Springer.
Johnson, J. L., Baldwin J., Haring, R. C., et al. (2008).
Fischer, A. R. & Shaw, C. M. (1999). African Americans’ Essential information for disaster management and
mental health and perception of racist discrimination: trauma specialists working with American Indians. In
Moderating efects of racial socialization experiences A. J. Marsella, J. L. Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski
and self-esteem. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46, (eds.), Ethnocultural perspectives on disaster and trauma:
395–407. Foundations, issues and applications (pp. 73–113). New
Galea, S., Vlahov, D., Tracy, M., et al. (2005). Hispanic York: Springer.
ethnicity and post-traumatic stress disorders ater a Kaplan, A. S. & Huynh, U. K. (2008). Working with
disaster: evidence from a general population survey Vietnamese Americans in disasters. In A. J. Marsella, J. L.
ater September 11, 2001. Annals of Epidemiology, 14, Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski (eds.), Ethnocultural
520–531. perspectives on disaster and trauma: Foundations, issues
Goldsmith, S. K., Pellmar, T. C., Kleinman, A. M., et al. for and applications (pp. 321–349). New York: Springer.
the Committee on Psychopathology and Prevention Kaufman, J. & Henrich, C. (2000). Exposure to violence and
of Adolescent and Adult Suicide and the Board on early childhood trauma. In C. Zeanah Jr. (ed.), Handbook
Neuroscience and Behavioral Health, National Institute of infant mental health (pp. 195–207). New York: Guilford
of Medicine (2002). Reducing suicide: A national Press.
imperative. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Kaufman, J., Yang, B. Z., Douglas-Palumberi, H., et al.
Gorsuch, R. L. (1995). Religious aspects of substance abuse (2006). Brain derived neurotrophic factors–5-HTTLPR
and recovery. Journal of Social Issues, 5, 65–83. gene interactions and environmental modiiers of
Greenberg, D. & Sheler, G. (2002). Obsessive-compulsive depression in children. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 673–680.
disorder in ultra-orthodox Jewish patients: A Kendler, K. S., Aggen, S. H., Jacobson, K. C., et al. (2003).
comparison of religious and non-religious symptoms. Does the level of family dysfunction moderate the impact
Psychological Psychotherapy, 75, 123–130. of genetic factors on the personality trait of neuroticism?
Greenberg, D. & Witztum, E. (1994). Inluence of cultural Psychological Medicine, 33, 817–825.
factors on OCD: Religious symptoms in a religious Keyes, C. L. M. (2007). Promoting and protective mental
society. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Relations Science, health as lourishing: A complementary strategy for
31, 211–220. improving national mental health. American Psychologist
Hamid, P. N. (1994). Self-monitoring, locus of control, and 62, 95–108.
social encounters of Chinese and New Zealand students. Klaassen, D. W., McDonald, M. J., & James, S. (2006).
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 25, 353–368. Advance in the study of religious and spiritual coping.
Hansen, N. D. (2002). Teaching cultural sensitivity in In P. T. P. Wong & L. C. J. Wong (eds.), Handbook of
psychological assessment: a modular approach used multicultural perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 105–
in a distance education program. Journal of Personality 132). New York: Springer.
Assessment, 79, 200–206. Klonof, E. A. & Landrine, H. (1999). Acculturation and
Heath, A. C., Madden P. A. F., Grant, J. D., et al. (1999). alcohol use among Blacks: he beneits of remaining
Resiliency factors protecting against teenage alcohol culturally traditional. Western Journal of Black Studies,
use and smoking: Inluences of religion, religious 23, 211–216.
involvement and values, and ethnicity in the Missouri Koenig, H., George, L., Meador, K., et al. (1994). Religious
Adolescent Female Twin Study. Twin Research, 2, practices and alcoholism in a southern adult population.
145–155. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 45, 225–231.
Herman, J. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Harper Kohut, H. (1966). Forms and transformations of narcissism.
Collins. Journal of American Psychoanalysis Association, 14,
Hill, R. B. (1997). he strengths of African American families: 243–272.
Twenty-ive years later. Washington, DC: RB Books. Koopmans, J. R., Slutske, W. S., van Baal, G. C. M., et al.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: Comparing (1999). he inluence of religion on alcohol initiation:
values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across Evidence for genotype X environment interaction.
nations. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Behavioral Genetics, 29, 445–453.

185
Section 3: Resilience in families, communities, and societies

Lafromboise, T. D., Trimble, J. E., & Mohatt, G. V. (1990). socialization and identity as protective factors. Social
Counseling intervention and American Indian tradition: Work Research, 23, 159–169.
An integrative approach. Counseling Psychologist,18, Munro, D. (1979). Locus-of-control attribution: Factors
628–654. among Blacks and Whites in Africa. Journal of Cross-
Larson, D. & Wilson, W. P. (1980). Religious life of Cultural Psychology, 10, 157–172.
alcoholics. Southern Medical Journal, 73, 723–727. Nasim, A., Belgrave, F. Z., Jagers, R. J., Wilson, K. D., &
Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and Owens, K. (2007). he moderating efects of culture on
coping. New York: Springer. peer deviance and alcohol use among high-risk African-
Luby, J. L., Svrakic, D. M., McCallum, K., et al. (1999). American adolescents. Journal of Drug Education, 37,
he Junior Temperament and Character Inventory: 335–363.
Preliminary validation of a child self-report measure. National Research Council & Institute of Medicine
Psychological Reports, 83, 1127–1138. (2009). Preventing mental, emotional, and behavioral
Maddi, S. R. & Harvey, R. H. (2006). Hardiness considered disorders among young people: Progress and possibilities.
across cultures. In P. T. P. Wong & L. C. J. Wong (eds.), Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and Nobles, W. (1986). African psychology: Toward its reclamation,
coping (pp. 409–426). New York: Springer. reascension, and revitalization. Oaland, CA: Institute for
Maes, H. H., Neale, M. C., Martin, N. G., et al. (1999). the Advanced Study of Black Family Life and Culture.
Religious attendance and frequency of alcohol use – Norris, F. H. & Alegria, M. (2008). Promoting disaster
same genes or same environment: A bivariate extended recovery in ethnic-minority individuals and
twin kinship model. Twin Research, 2, 169–179. communities. In A. J. Marsella, J. L. Johnson, P. Watson,
Mahler, I. (1974). A comparative study of locus of control. & J. Gryczynski (eds.), Ethnocultural perspectives on
Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the disaster and trauma: Foundations, issues and applications
Orient, 17, 135–139. (pp. 15–35). New York: Springer.
Marsella, A. J. (2005). Culture and conlict: Understanding Oyama, M. (1966). his is karate. San Francisco: Japan
and negotiating diferent cultural constructions of reality. Publications.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 651–673. Padilla, A. M. & Borrero, N. E. (2006). he efects of
Marsella, A. J., Johnson, J. L., Watson, P., & Gryczynski, J. acculturative stress on the Hispanic family. In P. T. P.
(2008). Essential concepts and foundations. In Wong & L. C. J. Wong (eds.), Handbook of multicultural
A. J. Marsella, J. L. Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 299–317). New York:
(eds.), Ethnocultural perspectives on disaster and trauma: Springer.
Foundations, issues and applications (pp. 3–14). New Pargament, K. I. (1997). he psychology of religion and
York: Springer. coping. New York: Guilford Press.
Marshall, G. N. & Orlando, M. (2002). Acculturation Payne, I., Bergin, A., Bielema, K., et al. (1991). Review of
and peritraumatic dissociation in youth adult Latino religion and mental health: Prevention and enhancement
survivors of community violence. Journal of Abnormal of psychological functioning. Prevention Human Services,
Psychology, 111, 166–174. 9, 11–40.
Masten, A. S. & Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). he development Perilla, J. L., Norris, F. H., & Lavizzo, E. A. (2002).
of competence in favorable and unfavorable Ethnicity, culture, and disaster response: Identifying
environments. American Psychologist, 52, 205–220. and explaining ethnic diferences in PTSD six months
McCreary, M. L., Cunningham, J. N., Ingram, K. M., & ater Hurricane Andrew. Journal of Social and Clinical
Fife, J. E. (2006). Stress, culture, and racial socialization. Psychology, 21, 20–45.
In P. T. P. Wong & L. C. J. Wong (eds.), Handbook of Prashantham, B. J. (2008). Asian Indians: Cultural
multicultural perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 487– considerations for disaster workers. In A. J. Marsella,
513). New York: Springer. J. L. Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski (eds.),
McCubbin, L. D., Ishikawa, M. E., & McCubbin, H. I. Ethnocultural perspectives on disaster and trauma:
(2008). he Kanaka Maoli: Native Hawaiians and their Foundations, issues and applications (pp. 175–207).
testimony of trauma and resilience. In A. J. Marsella, New York: Springer.
J. L. Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski (eds.), Raphael, F. J., Rani, S., Bale, R., et al. (1996). Religion,
Ethnocultural perspectives on disaster and trauma: ethnicity and obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Foundations, issues and applications (pp. 271–298). New International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 42, 38–44.
York: Springer. Rassin, E. & Koster, E. (2003). he correlation between
Miller, D. B. & MacIntosh, R. (1999). Promoting resilience thought-action fusion and religiosity in a normal sample.
in urban African American adolescents: Racial Behaviour Research and herapy, 41, 361–368.

186
Chapter 12: Trauma, culture, and resiliency

Rettew, D. C., Stanger, C., McKee, L., et al. (2006). obsessions and compulsions in East Turkey. [In French.]
Interactions between child and parent temperament and Encephale, 23, 342–350.
child behavior problems. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47, homas, A. & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and
412–420. development. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Rezentes, W. C. (1996). Ka lama kukui (Hawaiian hompson, E. A., McCubbin, H. I., hompson, H. I.,
psychology): An introduction. Honolulu, HI: A’ali’I Books. & Elver, K. M. (1995). Vulnerability and resiliency
Ruiz, D. M. A. (1997). he four agreements: A practical guide in Native Hawaiian families under stress. In H. I.
to personal freedom. San Rafael, CA: Amber-Allen. McCubbin, E. A. hompson, H. I. hompson, & J. E.
Ryf, C. D., Keyes, C. L. M., & Hughes, D. (2003). Status Fromer (eds.), Resiliency in ethnic minority families:
inequalities, perceived discrimination, and eudaimonic Native and immigrant American families (pp. 115–132).
well-being: Do the challenges of minority life hone Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin System Center for
purpose and growth? Journal of Health and Social Excellence in Family Studies.
Behavior, 44, 275–291. hornton, M. C., Chatters, L. M., Taylor, R. J., & Allen, W. R.
Shooka, A., al-Haddad, M. K., & Raees, A. (1998). OCD (1990). Sociodemographic and environmental
in Bahrain: a phenomenological proile. International correlates of racial socialization by Black parents. Child
Journal of Social Psychiatry, 44, 147–154. Development, 61, 401–409.
Sica, C., Novara, C., & Sanavio, E. (2002). Religiousness and Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism.
obsessive–compulsive cognitions and symptoms in an Boulder, CO: Westbiew Press.
Italian population. Behaviour Research and herapy, 40, Tweed, R. G. & Conway, L. G. (2006). Coping strategies and
813–823. culturally inluenced beliefs about the world. In P. T. P.
Smith, R. (ed). (1964). Secrets of Shaolin Temple boxing. Wong & L. C. J. Wong (eds.), Handbook of multicultural
Rutland, VT: Charles E. Tuttle. perspectives on stress and coping (pp. 133–153). New York:
Springer.
Staples, R. (1976). Introduction to black sociology. New York:
McGraw-Hill. Vega, W. A., Kolody, B., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., et al. (1998).
Lifetime prevalence of DSM–III–R psychiatric disorders
Stevenson, H. C., Reed, J., Bodison, P., & Bishop, A. (1997).
among urban and rural Mexican Americans in
Racism stress management: Racial socialization beliefs
California. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 771–778.
and the experience of depression and anger in African
American Youth. Youth and Society, 29, 197–222. Wilson, J. P. (2004). he abyss experience and traumatic
complex: A Jungian perspective of PTSD and
Sue, D. W. & Sue, D. (1999). Counseling the culturally
dissociation. Journal of Trauma in Dissociation, 5,
diferent: heory and practice, 3rd edn. New York: Wiley.
43–68.
Sue, D. W. & Sue D. (2003). Counseling the culturally diverse:
heory and practice, 4th edn. New York: Wiley. Wilson, J. P. (2008). Culture, trauma, and the treatment
of post-traumatic syndromes: A global perspective. In
Taylor, J. F. (2006). Ethnocentric monocuturalism In A. J. Marsella, J. L. Johnson, P. Watson, & J. Gryczynski
Y. Jackson (ed.), Encyclopedia of multicultural psychology (eds.), Ethnocultural perspectives on disaster and trauma:
(p. 203). housand Oaks, CA: Sage. Foundations, issues and applications (pp. 351–375). New
Tek, C. & Ulug, B. (2001). Religiosity and religious York: Springer.
obsessions in obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Wolin, S. & Wolin, S. J. (1996). he challenge model:
Research, 104, 99–108. Working with strengths in children of substance-abusing
Texcan, E. & Millet, B. (1997). Phenomenology of obsessive- parents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
compulsive disorders. Forms and characteristics of America, 5, 243–256.

187
Section
Section4 Specific challenges
Chapter
Loss and grief: the role of individual

13 differences
Anthony D. Mancini and George A. Bonanno

Introduction level of functioning. he most problematic reaction


is found among those with a persistent syndrome of a
It is an unfortunate but inevitable fact of life that
sometimes incapacitating distress that may take years
virtually all of us must face: people we are close to die.
to resolve. his pattern, oten described as “complicated
Despite this universality, researchers and theorists
grief ” (Bonanno et al., 2007), is relatively rare, typically
have long assumed that bereavement almost always
occurring in 10–15% of grievers (Bonanno & Kaltman,
results in signiicant, and sometimes incapacitating,
2001). Two other types of bereavement response
distress. Curiously, the absence of distress ater loss has
have also emerged recently, although they typic-
itself been considered pathological and a likely har-
ally characterize only a small proportion of grievers.
binger of future diiculties (Middleton et al., 1993).
hese include a chronic form of distress that predated
When bereaved persons fail to display the expected
the loss and is exacerbated in its atermath (Bonanno
distress reaction, some have maintained that they are
et al., 2002; Mancini et al., 2011) and dramatic improve-
suppressing their grief (Middleton et al., 1993) or lack
ment following loss (A.D. Mancini, I. Galatzer-Levy, &
an attachment to their spouse (Fraley & Shaver, 1999).
G.A. Bonanno, unpublished data). Although each is
Indeed, emotional expression following loss – particu-
quite uncommon (5–10%), these patterns have been
larly negative emotions – has long been considered
conirmed using diferent methods and samples, sug-
cathartic, a necessary ingredient of healthy adjustment
gesting that they are veridical.
(Freud, 1957). Perhaps for this reason, bereaved people
his chapter considers the implications of these
who appear outwardly resilient and who resume their
diferent patterns of grief response for how we under-
lives with minimal disruptions have oten been thought
stand the grieving process. To address this issue, we
to possess extraordinary coping abilities.
irst review grief ’s varying manifestations. We then
However, this idea has increasingly come under ire
describe empirical indings on grieving across the
(Bonanno, 2004). In fact, most bereaved people experi-
three primary trajectories, focusing on qualitative dif-
ence relatively transient disruptions in their ability to
ferences. We conclude by considering the therapeutic
function efectively. Furthermore, research increas-
implications of these diferences in grieving and poten-
ingly shows that there is a marked diversity in how
tial cultural variations.
people respond to loss. Indeed, it appears that three
primary patterns or trajectories adequately describe
most people’s response to interpersonal loss. he lar- The phenomenology of grief
gest category, usually from 50% to 60%, is characterized How does the experience of grief manifest itself?
by stable, healthy levels of psychological and physical Evidence increasingly suggests that four broad cat-
functioning relatively soon ater a loss, or “resilience” egories of disrupted functioning characterize the nor-
(Bonanno, 2004; Mancini & Bonanno, 2006). A second mative experience of loss (Bonanno & Kaltman, 2001):
category of bereaved persons (20–25%) displays more (1) cognitive disorganization, (2) dysphoric emotions,
acute and persistent levels of distress but gradually they (3) health deicits, and (4) disruptions in social and
too recover their bearings and return to their former occupational functioning.

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

189
Section 4: Specific challenges

Cognitive disorganization Some research also suggests that bereavement is


associated with a concerted and enduring search to
In the initial atermath of loss, some cognitive disor-
place the loss in a meaningful context. In a study of indi-
ganization in adjusting to altered life circumstances
viduals who lost a child or a spouse in a car crash, for
has been widely noted, although this disorganization
example, the search for meaning ater a loss was found
is by no means characteristic of all bereaved persons.
to unfold over the course of years (Lehman et al., 1987).
Among the components of this disorganization are a
As late as four to seven years ater the sudden loss of a
sense of confusion and preoccupation with the loss, as
spouse or child, the vast majority of bereaved individ-
well as having diiculty accepting the reality of their
uals continued to talk about the loss; to review memor-
loss, a sense of derealization and disorganization, and
ies, thoughts, or mental pictures of the deceased; or to
preoccupation with memories of the deceased (Parkes
ask themselves the questions “Why me?” or “Why my
& Weiss, 1983; Shuchter & Zisook, 1993). For example,
spouse/child?”. Disconcertingly, 68% of the bereaved
a study of 350 conjugally bereaved individuals found
spouses and 59% of the bereaved parents reported that
that even two months ater the loss up to 70% of the
they had not found any meaning or made any sense at
sample still found it “hard to believe” that their spouses
all of the loss. Indeed, a number of studies suggest that
had actually died, and approximately half (49%) con-
the search for meaning is usually associated with dis-
tinued to have this diiculty into the second year of
tress and may not help the person to resolve their grief.
bereavement (Zisook & Shuchter, 1993). Despite these
However, a recent study examining cancer survivors
continuing preoccupations, only a small proportion of
suggested that adaptation may be facilitated by mak-
the bereaved showed more extreme cognitive diicul-
ing meaning out of the experience, as opposed to only
ties, such as diiculties concentrating (20%) or making
searching for meaning. In other words, a persistent
decisions (17%).
search for meaning in the atermath of an acute stres-
Not surprisingly, bereaved persons are also com-
sor will only serve adaptive ends when it eventuates in
monly preoccupied with the memory of the deceased,
a “meaning made” (Park et al., 2008).
especially soon ater the loss. Particularly within the
irst year ater the loss, it is common for bereaved
persons to experience sudden recollections of the Dysphoric emotions
deceased that are not prompted by external events. Although bereavement obviously results in sadness, a
hese memories are oten described as “unbidden,” variety of dysphoric emotions have also been linked to
and, when they persist over a long period of time, they loss, most oten centering around anger, irritability, hos-
can be a source of considerable distress. In the study tility, sadness, fear, and guilt (Bowlby, 1980; Raphael,
of Horowitz and colleagues (1997), for example, most 1983; Osterweis et al., 1984). However, the frequency
bereaved persons (72%) reported such experiences of these emotions is somewhat in doubt. Shuchter and
even at six months ater the loss. A more potentially Zisook (1993) used self-report measures to assay emo-
problematic aspect of this continued engagement tional experiences in the irst year ater the loss, inding
with the memory of the deceased is relected in that only a small portion of participants (less than 15%)
changes in identity ater loss. For example, a substan- endorsed emotions commonly linked to grieving, such
tial proportion of bereaved persons reported (87%) as anger, guilt, and fear. his may be accounted for by
experiencing that “a piece of me is missing” (Zisook the insuiciency of retrospective self-report measures
& Shuchter, 1993). Moreover, many of the bereaved to record ephemeral emotional experiences accurately.
(55%) found themselves doing things more like their Indeed, a greater prevalence of dysphoric emotion dur-
deceased spouses, or even becoming more like their ing conjugal bereavement was observed in a subsequent
deceased spouses (39%). In another report, some study that measured facial expressions of emotion as
bereaved individuals (14%) even experienced recur- they occurred during an interview six months ater the
rent thoughts that their own death would follow, or loss (Bonanno & Keltner, 1997). Using this immediate,
mirror, their spouse’s death (Horowitz et al., 1997). non-verbal assessment, anger and sadness were exhib-
Given that these indings were observed in samples ited by almost two thirds of the bereaved participants,
of middle-aged, and presumably healthy, individuals, and contempt and disgust by approximately one third.
they ofer particularly compelling evidence of the A more complex and mixed syndrome of dys-
potential impact of loss on cognitions about the self. phoric emotion is found in the common experience

190
Chapter 13: Loss and grief

of yearning for the deceased. Unsurprisingly, yearn- perceived health, and poorer health relative to others
ing is a basic component of grief. Parkes (1970, p. 451) of the same age.
concluded that “the central and pathognomonic fea- In addition to these potential sequelae of loss, sev-
ture of grief ” is an intense “pining” for the deceased, eral studies have also examined the possible role of
such that “without it grief cannot truly be said to have neuroendocrine activity, such as changes in cortisol or
occurred and when present it is a sure sign of a per- serum growth hormone levels, during bereavement.
son grieving.” However, it is worth noting that yearn- Although there is no evidence that neuroendocrine
ing is a complex state, consisting of mixed positive and activity is altered signiicantly during bereavement or
negative thoughts and feelings related to the lost loved that it inluences the course of grieving (Kim & Jacobs,
one. Indeed, a critical point here is that although grief 1993), evidence does suggest a link between grief and
may evoke a range of emotional responses, grief is not a relatively short-lived compromise in immune func-
synonymous with emotion. Whereas emotions are tioning (Irwin et al., 1987). his linkage, however,
ephemeral responses to internal and environmental may be mediated by symptomatology in response to
stimuli that condition certain action tendencies, grief the loss. For example, Zisook and colleagues (1994)
is distinguished by its relatively enduring nature and examined immune functioning variables in a sample of
by its recruitment of more long-term coping eforts to middle-aged conjugally bereaved women and in a sam-
manage distress. ple of matched controls. here were no signiicant dif-
A similar dysphoric feature commonly associ- ferences between the bereaved women at two months
ated with grieving is intense loneliness. Shuchter and ater loss and their married counterparts on any of the
Zisook (1993) reported that 59% of the widows and immune variables. However, when the bereaved sam-
widowers they questioned during the irst two months ple was separated into depressed and non-depressed
of bereavement experienced loneliness, while 37% felt widows, depressed widows had a lower concentra-
lonely even when around other people. By the second tion of T-cells, lower natural killer cell activity, and a
year of bereavement, these proportions dropped to 39% trend toward lower lymphocyte stimulation responses
and 23%, respectively; in contrast, only 3% of a match compared with the non-depressed widows. As compel-
married sample endorsed experiencing any form of ling as the link between grief-related depression and
loneliness. Horowitz and colleagues (1997) reported a short-term suppression of immune functioning may
similar indings, with 59% of their sample experien- be, it has not yet been demonstrated that the depres-
cing loneliness at six months ater loss, and 38% experi- sive aspects of grief exert a longer-term inluence on
encing loneliness at 14 months ater loss. immune functioning, or that the short-term suppres-
sion efects produce any lasting health consequences
(Irwin & Pike, 1993). Additional research will be neces-
Health deficits sary to address this question further.
here is considerable evidence that the stress of inter- One dramatic and unfortunate consequence of
personal loss also can carry a signiicant health cost. bereavement is its impact on mortality, an association
his has been observed most commonly in the form of that appears to be particularly robust in the early months
increased doctor visits and complaints about general ater a loss. In a striking demonstrations of the bereave-
health, but more serious health problems have also been ment–mortality efect, researchers in Finland exam-
identiied. A number of studies have suggested that ined longitudinal data on 95 647 conjugally bereaved
interpersonal loss is associated with increased health individuals in Finland during their irst four years of
diiculties, including shortness of breath, palpitations, bereavement (Kaprio et al., 1987). Compared with the
digestive diiculties, loss of appetite, restlessness, and average expected mortality rate in Finland, the con-
insomnia (Lindemann, 1944; Horowitz, 1986). One jugally bereaved individuals showed an overall 6.5%
well-designed study compared health outcomes in eld- higher likelihood of dying. his increase was smaller
erly bereaved two months ater loss and a comparison (3.2% increase) for deaths by natural causes (e.g.,
group matched for socioeconomic and demographic cardiovascular disease). However, deaths by natural
variables (hompson et al., 1984). Compared with causes were almost twice as high as the normal expect-
the matched control participants, the bereaved par- ancy rate in the irst week of bereavement. Deaths by
ticipants still reported worsened or new illnesses, more violent causes (e.g., traic accidents) were even more
severe illnesses, increased use of medications, poorer likely (93% increase) among the bereaved individuals.

191
Section 4: Specific challenges

Finally, bereaved individuals showed considerably proportion of bereaved persons still reported diicul-
more deaths (242% increase) by suicide. Indeed, it is ties in developing new intimate relationships even six
widely thought that bereavement increases the risk of months ater a loss (Horowitz et al., 1997). However,
dying, and this increased risk of mortality extends to all by 14 months, only about a third of the sample (32%)
types of bereavement, particularly in the early months reported such diiculties.
ater the loss and particularly among younger bereaved
individuals (Stroebe & Stroebe, 1993). Trajectories of loss and grief
Given that the experience of loss and grief takes on
Disrupted social and occupational such varying manifestations, what are the implica-
functioning tions of a trajectory approach for our understanding
of individual diferences in grieving? As discussed
In addition to its cognitive, emotional, and somatic
above, empirical evidence increasingly supports three
manifestations, grief has also been associated with
primary or prototypical patterns of grief reaction:
disruptions in social and occupational functioning.
resilience, recovery, and chronic or complicated grief.
hese diiculties have been observed most commonly
Beyond the straightforward diference in the duration
in the form of social withdrawal and isolation, and a
of grief symptoms across time, what does a trajectory
concomitant diiculty in assuming or maintaining
approach suggest for our understanding of the qualita-
ordinary social and occupational roles. As with the
tive aspects of grief? In other words, is the grief experi-
other features of grief, disruptions in social and occu-
ence across the trajectories diferent in kind or just
pational functioning are common for most but not all
in duration and intensity? his question is taken up
bereaved individuals in the initial atermath of a loss
by irst describing each trajectory pattern and briely
(Lindemann, 1944; Parkes & Weiss, 1983).
reviewing evidence for their prevalence before moving
Bereaved individuals also may struggle in their
on to consider available empirical evidence for how the
roles as parents or in their careers (Parkes & Weiss,
manifestations of grief described above would likely
1983). Several studies have shown that widowers report
difer qualitatively across grief trajectories.
greater diiculties in their work roles both outside and
inside the home, greater diiculties in managing spare
time, and greater diiculties with their family roles Resilience
(Tudiver et al., 1991). Similar indings were observed he most prevalent response to loss is characterized by
in a study conducted four to seven years ater the sud- relatively stable psychological and physical function-
den death of a spouse or child. he conjugally bereaved ing as well as the capacity for generative experiences
portion of the sample was still less likely to look for- and positive emotions. Elsewhere, we have described
ward to doing things with others, less conident that this capacity as resilience, and increasing evidence sup-
they could handle or cope with a serious problem or ports its distinctiveness as a trajectory in response to
major change in their life, and rated the general state of acute stress (Mancini & Bonanno, 2006; Bonanno &
their lives more negatively, compared with a matched Mancini, 2008). In addition to bereavement, resili-
married group. However, the bereaved parents in this ence has been observed in response to a wide variety of
study showed similar but non-signiicant trends toward acute stressors, including the September 11 2001 ter-
the same results. rorist attack (Bonanno et al., 2006), traumatic injury
Bereavement also afects work performance and (DeRoon-Cassini et al., 2010), divorce (Mancini et al.,
initiating and maintaining relationships. One study 2011), and disease epidemic (Bonanno et al., 2008).
found that over a third (36%) of bereaved persons In the bereavement literature, an initial study
reported dissatisfaction in their work performance at focused on how people coped with the premature death
seven months, and that 28% still endorsed this prob- of a spouse at midlife (Bonanno et al., 1995). Although
lem at 13 months (Shuchter & Zisook, 1993). Although using relatively small samples, this study demonstrated
these percentages show that dissatisfaction with work that a stable pattern of low distress over time, or resili-
may not be as widespread as other grief-related dii- ence, could be clearly distinguished from the more
culties, it was still more common among bereaved indi- conventionally understood pattern of recovery. More
viduals relative to the 10% of the married comparison recent studies have provided additional evidence for the
sample who reported such diiculties. A substantial resilience trajectory. In an unusual study that followed

192
Chapter 13: Loss and grief

a large sample of older married persons over the course memory and assumes a more benign and even com-
of many years, the subset of persons who experienced forting presence in the person’s life. Bonanno et al.
the loss of their spouse showed surprisingly low levels (2002) found that resilient people are particularly
of clinical depression, with approximately half report- likely to experience positive emotions in tandem with
ing minimal to no symptoms at any point in the study memories of the deceased. Speciically, resilient people
(Bonanno et al., 2002). In another sample that included were better able than other bereaved participants to
bereaved parents, bereaved spouses, and bereaved gay gain comfort from talking about or thinking about the
men, more than half reported minimal to no symptoms spouse. For example, they were more likely than other
across time (Bonanno et al., 2005a). We found further bereaved people to report that thinking about and talk-
conirmation for resilience as a distinct and common ing about their deceased spouse made them feel happy
trajectory in response to loss in a recent study using a or at peace (Bonanno et al., 2004a). Moreover, this cap-
latent growth mixture modeling framework, a sophis- acity remained stable over time but only for those in the
ticated statistical approach that permits the model- resilient trajectory.
ing of divergent trajectories of response across time Although the resilient bereaved may experienced
(Muthén, 2004). In an analysis of over 16 000 German some cognitive disorganization related to the loss,
citizens who were followed over 20 years, 60% of per- recent evidence suggests that they largely maintain
sons who lost their spouse reported only a slight dip in their equilibrium. For example, one characteristic
life satisfaction and returned to baseline levels a year of resilient people is a remarkable continuity in their
ater the loss (Mancini et al., 2011). identities. We found evidence for this continuity in a
What does the resilient trajectory suggest for our recent study in which we compared resilient and more
understanding of the grief response? It has long been symptomatic bereaved persons with a matched sample
assumed that the failure to grieve is indicative of an of married persons (A.D. Mancini, I. Galatzer-Levy, &
underlying but unseen psychopathology (Freud, 1957; G.A. Bonanno, unpublished data). Participants com-
Bowlby, 1980). Indeed, some have gone so far as to sug- pleted a task in which they identiied traits associated
gest that the appearance of mental health, when associ- with their identity (e.g., “kind” or “thoughtful”). Once
ated with a defensive disavowal of unpleasant aspects of the list was inalized, the researcher then asked the
the self, can be illusory (Shedler et al., 1993). Consistent participant to indicate the extent that each trait had
with the illusion of mental health paradigm, there is a changed since the loss. When compared with bereaved
long history of associating resilience to loss with nega- persons with higher levels of symptoms, resilient indi-
tive characteristics, including defensive denial, a lack viduals reported relatively little identity change at both
of attachment to the spouse, and a delayed and more 4 and 18 months ater loss, and the degree of change
severe reaction to the loss. did not difer from that reported by non-bereaved
Contrary to these speculations, an ample litera- participants. Likewise, in qualitative examinations of
ture now suggests that the grief of resilient people is the bereavement experience, the resilient bereaved
not marked by an absence of feeling. For example, reported a stable and continuous sense of who they are,
when asked about their experiences soon ater the loss, a resource that appears to bufer them from the many
about 75% of those showing a resilient outcome tra- changes entailed by the loss of a loved one (Bauer &
jectory reported experiencing intense yearning (pain- Bonanno, 2001; Mancini & Bonanno, 2006).
ful waves of missing spouse) as well as pangs of intense In terms of health functioning, preliminary evi-
grief at some point in the earliest months of bereave- dence suggests that the resilient bereaved also experi-
ment (Bonanno et al., 2002). What is more, all but one ence less-marked physiological consequences. To
of the bereaved people showing the resilient trajectory explore individual diferences in somatic complaints
reported having experienced intrusive and unbidden over time, for example, Bonanno and colleagues (1999)
thoughts about the loss that they could not get out of categorized individuals according to their level of som-
their mind, and that they found themselves ruminat- atic symptoms across three time points, extending to
ing, or going over and over what happened when the 25 months ater the loss. Two thirds of participants had
spouse died, in the earliest months of bereavement. either low levels of somatic complaints at each assess-
However, it appears that these aspects of grief are ment or reported initial elevations of symptoms that
simply less debilitating and less persistent, in part dropped to normal levels between the irst and second
because the loss is more quickly transmogriied into year. In a more recent analysis, self-reported health

193
Section 4: Specific challenges

dysfunction strongly diferentiated the resilient from bereavement, because it would appear to relect an
trajectories of chronic grief and chronic high distress appropriate expression of the negative feelings associ-
that predated the loss (Mancini et al., 2011), further ated with the loss. Such expression is oten presumed
suggesting that the resilient bereaved experience fewer to facilitate grief ’s resolution. As discussed above, how-
health problems following loss. Although it is likely ever, evidence suggests that resilience is most charac-
that resilience would also attenuate or even nullify the teristic of psychological health. Indeed, there is no
mortality–bereavement link, this question has not been evidence that grief-related symptoms early on in the
directly investigated and is thus an important topic of bereavement experience are prognostic of better out-
future research. comes (Bonanno et al., 2002, 2004b).
A hallmark of resilient bereaved people is their What is the bereavement experience like for people
ability to continue to express and experience positive exhibiting this trajectory? At this point, there have been
emotions even soon ater a loss. For example, resili- only a few direct examinations of recovery (Bonanno
ent individuals tend to exhibit more genuine laughs et al., 2002, 2004b), making it somewhat diicult to
and smiles when speaking about the loss (Bonanno & characterize the grief experiences of such persons.
Keltner, 1997), and also evoke more favorable responses Nevertheless, some aspects of their experience can be
in observers (Keltner, 1997). Moreover, resilient deduced from extant research. Bereaved persons who
bereaved people also have fewest regrets about their show the recovery pattern typically struggle with mod-
behavior with the spouse, or about things they may erate levels of symptoms and experience diiculties
have done or failed to do when he or she was still alive. carrying out their normal tasks at work or in the care
Perhaps as a result, resilient individuals are less likely to of loved ones, but they somehow manage to struggle
search to make sense of or ind meaning in the spouse’s through these tasks and slowly but gradually begin to
death, suggesting that they are less likely to engage in return to their pre-loss or baseline level of functioning,
rumination, which is strongly associated with negative usually over a period of one or two years. Symptoms
afective states (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1997). are relatively acute in the early stages of the loss. When
In sum, the resilient bereaved appear to experience compared with resilient persons, those in the recovery
a grief course marked by initial experiences of yearning trajectory usually have more feelings of depression, dif-
and sadness but few disruptions in their ability to meet iculty enjoying activities, and sleeping symptoms early
everyday obligations at work and in social relationships. on, as well as experiencing more yearning and feelings
In addition to experiencing relatively few health prob- of shock and despair about the loss. Although these
lems ater the loss, the resilient bereaved also appear to symptoms typically resolve by 18 months, persons in
retain a sense of a continuous self in spite of the loss and the recovery trajectory do struggle to a greater extent
to experience a sense of comfort from memories of the than, for example, resilient persons. hey also appear to
deceased that remains stable across time. experience fewer positive emotions in their experience
of grief (Bonanno et al., 2004a). For example, persons
Recovery who demonstrate the recovery pattern see a decline
over time in their capacity to derive comfort from posi-
he term recovery connotes a trajectory in which nor-
tive memories of the deceased (Bonanno et al., 2004a).
mal functioning temporarily gives way to threshold
his decline stands in sharp contrast to the resilient,
or subthreshold psychopathology (e.g., symptoms of
whose memories of the deceased continue to provide
depression or post-traumatic stress disorder [PTSD]),
undiminished comfort across time.
usually for a period of at least several months, and then
gradually returns to pre-event levels. his recovery pat-
tern has long been considered the modal response to Chronic grief
loss and other acute stressors (Brickman & Campbell, he most problematic reaction to loss is characterized
1971). However, the empirical evidence has over- by a persistent syndrome of grief-related distress that
whelmingly shown that recovery typically character- can last for years ater the loss. Although this type of
izes only a minority of bereaved persons, at most 20% reaction generally characterizes a small proportion of
(Bonanno et al., 2002; Mancini et al., 2011). grievers, it can have profound efects on people’s abil-
According to a more traditional perspective, ity to function (Prigerson et al., 1995, 1996). heorists
recovery might be seen as the healthiest response to have long argued that chronic grief should be viewed

194
Chapter 13: Loss and grief

as a separate form of psychopathology (Lichtenthal conscious awareness (Ehlers, 2006). he persistent and
et al., 2004), independent of diagnoses for major oten distressing nature of the deceased’s felt presence
depression and PTSD. Until recently, evidence in sup- in the griever’s life, along with the yearning that it occa-
port of a diagnostic category for chronic or “compli- sions, appears to set in train a variety of consequences
cated” grief had been equivocal. One critical question for adjustment, which mark chronic grief as distinct
that had not been adequately addressed is whether from other types of grieving.
chronic grief has incremental validity in predicting Although most grievers experience some intrusive
functional outcomes over and above the predictive cap- memories of the deceased or cognitive disorganization
acity of depression and PTSD symptoms. We recently soon ater a loss (for example, confusing someone on
investigated this question and conirmed that grief the street with the deceased), these experiences typic-
symptoms, when entered simultaneously into a regres- ally rapidly diminish in their intensity and frequency.
sion equation with depression and PTSD symptoms, For chronic grievers, however, the intrusive memor-
were associated with unique variance across diferent ies continue and may be accompanied by marked dis-
outcome measures (heart rate change and global func- tress and autonomic arousal (Zisook et al., 1998). In
tioning) and in two separate samples (Bonanno et al., this way, chronic grief can resemble a syndrome with
2007). herefore, chronic grief seems to be an inde- strong parallels to PTSD (Ehlers, 2006). In accord with
pendent form of psychopathology. this, a cardinal feature of chronic grief is avoidance.
How does chronic grief difer from the recovery Chronic grievers are particularly likely, for example,
pattern? An obvious but perhaps principal difer- to deploy avoidant strategies to manage feelings and
ence between the conventional recovery pattern and reminders associated with the loss (Boelen et al.,
chronic grief reactions is the duration of symptoms 2006a). hey may, for example, not talk to old friends
and their impact on functioning. However, duration of or avoid places associated with the deceased. hey may
symptomatology does not appear to be the only factor try to inoculate themselves against feelings related to
to distinguish chronic reactions; severity of symptoms the loss, try to push them out of awareness, or redirect
even in the initial months of bereavement also appears their thoughts to mitigate their preoccupation with the
to inform such reactions. Recent research has shown, deceased. hese avoidant strategies for regulating the
for example, that bereaved individuals who ultimately thoughts and feelings associated with the loss are typ-
developed chronic reactions struggle with acute symp- ically inefective, however, because they oten reinforce
tom levels in the early months of bereavement. hese the link between the memory of the deceased and the
can include a dramatically reduced ability to perform cognitive–afective structures with which it is associ-
well at work, to maintain relationships with friends or ated (Boelen et al., 2006b).
intimates, and to meet parenting obligations. hese One avoidant strategy with particularly nega-
diiculties may persist for years ater the loss, but at tive implications for bereavement is suppression, or
a minimum should endure for at least one year ater inhibiting emotional–expressive behavior while feel-
bereavement to warrant the label “chronic grief.” ing emotional arousal. Although research has yet to
uncover a direct link between the use of suppression
Qualitative aspects of grief also differentiate chronic and chronic grief, it would appear likely, based on
grief from the other trajectories the central role of avoidance in chronic grievers’ rep-
Although yearning typically characterizes all mourn- ertoire of coping behaviors, that suppression is oten
ers, chronic grievers appear to experience a particu- enacted in managing troubling feelings. Because of the
larly acute form of it, one that has debilitating efects on cognitive resources required, the use of suppression
their functioning. Parkes and Weiss (1983) identiied a inhibits the person’s ability to absorb new information
“high yearning” group of conjugally bereaved individ- and, in this way, can impair interpersonal functioning
uals who appeared to yearn for their deceased spouses (Gross & John, 2003), depriving the person of helpful
“constantly,” “frequently,” or “whenever inactive.” resources. Consider, for example, a chronic griever
Pining like this is oten a dominant feature of the day to who is preparing to attend a memorial for his or her
day experience of chronic grievers. Indeed, the slight- loved one. To avoid being overwhelmed, the person
est peripheral cue – passing a restaurant associated may try to suppress the behavioral manifestations of
with the deceased, for example – can activate the mem- his or her feelings, a coping strategy that entails sig-
ory of the deceased, even when this cue has not entered niicant cognitive resources and that is more likely to

195
Section 4: Specific challenges

enhance autonomic arousal (Gross & John, 2003). As by grief theorists, is no longer tenable. Indeed, it has
a consequence, the person would not only experience become increasingly clear that grief interventions are
the event as more stressful but would also be deprived only appropriate for a minority of grievers, primarily
of some of the beneicial aspects of social interaction, those in the chronic grief trajectory. his position was
including more cognitive integration of the loss. recently underscored in an authoritative meta-analysis
Somewhat surprisingly, suppression and avoidance of grief interventions (Currier et al., 2008). Perhaps the
of the loss may also co-occur with an anxious preoccu- central inding of this meta-analysis is that studies that
pation with the negative feelings related to the loss. One screen for more serious forms of grief show substan-
unfortunate consequence of this preoccupation is that tially stronger efects. In fact, in the absence of such
chronic grievers’ continuous expressions of pain may screening, grief interventions show small efect sizes,
drive away potential avenues of social support (Coyne substantially less than are observed in traditional ther-
et al., 1988; Kelly & McKillop, 1996; Keltner & Bonanno, apies, and these efects are not retained at follow-up.
1997). his possibility received preliminary support Moreover, given the heterogeneity of grief, it would
in a recent study in which untrained observers were appear that it is necessary to employ a variety of clinical
asked to rate their subjective reactions to videotapes techniques tailored to the speciic experiences of the
of conjugally bereaved individuals as they described griever, as opposed to generic therapeutic procedures
their loss to an interviewer (Keltner & Bonanno, 1997). that would be presumed to help all grievers. It is worth
hose bereaved participants who were perceived by noting that this mandate stands in sharp contrast to
the observers as less well adjusted also evoked in the traditional notions that grief is “work” and requires
observers greater frustration and less compassion. In emotional expression or that grief proceeds in preor-
a subsequent study (Capps & Bonanno, 2000), another dained stages (Wortman & Silver, 2001).
group of untrained judges were shown narrative tran- Although resilient persons are not appropriate can-
scripts of these interviews. he more the bereaved par- didates for intervention, their coping strategies may
ticipants described negative thoughts and emotions ofer some intriguing avenues for clinical interven-
during the interview, the more the judges reported tion (for a longer treatment of this issue, see Mancini
they would be inclined to avoid the participant. As can & Bonanno [2006]). For example, one characteristic
be seen, chronic grievers are in something of a double of resilient people is their ability to regulate emotion
bind. If they suppress behaviors associated with their lexibility in response to situational demands. his
emotions, the beneits they derive from social inter- capacity gives them a distinct coping advantage when
action are sharply curtailed. Conversely, if they express faced with acute adversity (Bonanno et al., 2004b). We
their feelings of pain too much to their social network, are currently exploring how such skills might inform
they may erode the very relationships they are count- our understanding and treatment of chronic grief.
ing on for support. Although no formal treatment approach exists to teach
these kinds of skill directly, we suspect that coping
lexibility might be learned or with practice improved
Conclusions and clinical implications among individuals who lack such abilities. Another
his chapter has focused on individual diferences in clinical implication of the present review concerns the
grieving, describing the widely varying manifestations techniques used to regulate aversive emotions associ-
of grief in response to loss. Based on a growing body of ated with loss. As discussed, the use of suppression, par-
research, three primary trajectories of loss (resilience, ticularly for chronic grievers, carries with it a number
recovery, and chronic grief) and two secondary trajec- of untoward complications (Gross & John, 2003). An
tories (improvement and chronic distress) were identi- alternative technique is cognitive reappraisal, which is
ied that appear to characterize the experiences of most antecedent focused (designed to anticipate the emo-
grievers. he empirical evidence for qualitative difer- tion) and involves construing a potentially stressful
ences in the grief experience of persons in the diferent event in benign or growth-oriented terms in order to
grief trajectories was then considered. blunt its emotional impact (Gross & John, 2003). his
What are the implications of the heterogeneous may be a particularly important skill for chronic griev-
nature of grief for clinical interventions? One obvi- ers, who are more vulnerable to distress under circum-
ous implication is that a one-size its all approach stances that activate their associations to the deceased.
for grief interventions, once commonly endorsed Although such techniques are standard components of

196
Chapter 13: Loss and grief

clinical intervention, given the indings on individual broader community. By contrast, Americans bereaved
diferences in resilience, it seems worth emphasizing persons who report a continuing bond to the deceased
their potentially important role in interventions with tend to do quite poorly (Bonanno et al., 2005a), under-
bereaved persons. lining the cultural imperative of a more individualistic
One additional question is the role of contextual society. hese indings are consistent with the notion
factors, such as the nature of the loss, caregiving strain, that adaptation may be linked to normative cultural
or material resources, and their impact on the grief values regarding appropriate grieving and views of the
experience. For example, although it might appear self. Moreover, these data raise the intriguing question
intuitive that sudden losses would be associated with of whether resilience has diferent meanings in difer-
more chronic grief, data have largely been equivocal on ent cultural contexts or, perhaps even more intriguing,
this question. However, one well-designed study, using whether diferent cultures may learn from each other
a representative sample, appropriate control variables, about efective and not so efective ways of coping with
and prospective data, found that the sudden loss of a extreme adversity.
loved one is not, by itself, associated with worse adjust-
ment (Carr et al., 2001). However, there is evidence that References
losses from violent causes (suicide, homicide, or vio-
Bauer, J. J. & Bonanno, G. A. (2001). I can, I do, I am: he
lent accident) are associated with more symptomatol- narrative diferentiation of self-eicacy and other self-
ogy and lower levels of resilience (Kaltman & Bonanno, evaluations while adapting to bereavement. Journal of
2003). When the analysis controls for the violent nature Research in Personality, 35, 424–448.
of a loss, the impact of sudden losses is no longer sig- Boelen, P. A., van den Hout, M. A., & van den Bout, J.
niicant. Caregiving strain, by contrast, appears not (2006a). A cognitive-behavioral conceptualization
to be related to resilience per se, but it does predict an of complicated grief. Clinical Psychology: Science and
unusual trajectory of improved functioning follow- Practice, 13, 109–128.
ing loss (Bonanno et al. 2002). Additional evidence Boelen, P. A., van den Bout, J., & van den Hout, M. A.
for the crucial role of contextual factors was found in (2006b). Negative cognitions and avoidance in emotional
a recent analysis of a large panel dataset. Using a latent problems ater bereavement: A prospective study.
class framework, we not only validated the resilient tra- Behaviour Research and herapy, 44, 1657–1672.
jectory empirically but we also found that one of the Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience:
factors associated with resilience was less reduction in Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive
income following loss (Mancini et al., 2011). ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist,
59, 20–28.
A inal important question is the role of ethnic
and cultural variations in resilience during bereave- Bonanno, G. A. & Kaltman, S. (2001). he varieties of grief
experience. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 705–734.
ment. Western, independence-oriented countries
tend to focus more heavily than collectivist countries Bonanno, G. A. & Keltner, D. (1997). Facial expressions of
on the personal experience of grief. Cushman (1990) emotion and the course of conjugal bereavement. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 126–137.
has described a pervasive assumption in the West of a
“bounded, interior self ” in which autonomy and self- Bonanno, G. A. & Mancini, A. D. (2008). he human
capacity to thrive in the face of potential trauma.
reliance are primary values and which stands in oppos-
Pediatrics, 121, 369–375.
ition to Eastern notions of a collectivist self. What
are the implications of such cultural beliefs for cop- Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Holen, A., & Horowitz, M. J.
(1995). When avoiding unpleasant emotions might
ing with extreme adversity? Although we cannot take not be such a bad thing: Verbal–autonomic response
up this issue in detail, preliminary evidence suggests dissociation and midlife conjugal bereavement. Journal
that coping strategies do hold diferent consequences of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 975–989.
for adjustment in diferent cultures. For example, Bonanno, G. A., Znoj, H., Siddique, H. I., & Horowitz, M. J.
bereaved people in China recover more quickly from (1999). Verbal–autonomic dissociation and adaptation
loss than do bereaved Americans, and for the Chinese, to midlife conjugal loss: A follow-up at 25 months.
coping is enhanced by a continuing psychological Cognitive herapy & Research, 23, 605–624.
bond with the deceased (Bonanno et al., 2005b), per- Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., Lehman, D. R., et al.
haps because the self is construed in connection to the (2002). Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A prospective

197
Section 4: Specific challenges

study from preloss to 18-months postloss. Journal of bereaved persons: A comprehensive quantitative review.
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1150–1164. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 648–661.
Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., & Nesse, R. M. (2004a). Cushman, P. (1990). Why the self is empty: Toward a
Prospective patterns of resilience and maladjustment historically situated psychology. American Psychologist,
during widowhood. Psychology and Aging, 19, 260–271. 45, 599–611.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & DeRoon-Cassini, T., Mancini, A. D., Bonanno, G. A., &
Coifman, K. (2004b). he importance of being lexible: Rusch, M. (2010). Psychopathology and resilience
he ability to both enhance and suppress emotional following traumatic injury: a latent growth mixture
expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological model analysis. Rehabilitation Psychology, 55, 1–11.
Science, 15, 482–487. Ehlers, A. (2006). Understanding and treating complicated
Bonanno, G. A., Moskowitz, J. T., Papa, A., & Folkman, S. grief: What can we learn from posttraumatic stress
(2005a). Resilience to loss in bereaved spouses, bereaved disorder? Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 13,
parents, and bereaved gay men. Journal of Personality and 135–140.
Social Psychology, 88, 827–843. Fraley, R. C. & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Loss and bereavement:
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Nanping, Z., & Attachment theory and recent controversies concerning
Noll, J. G. ( 2005b). Grief processing and deliberate “grief work” and the nature of detachment. In J. Cassidy
grief avoidance: A prospective comparison of bereaved & P. R. Shaver (eds.), Handbook of attachment: heory,
spouses and parents in the United States and the People’s research, and clinical applications (pp. 735–759). New
Republic of China. Journal of Consulting and Clinical York: Guilford Press.
Psychology, 73, 86–98. Freud, S. (1957). Mourning and melancholia. In J. Strachey
Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D. (ed.), he standard edition of the complete psychological
(2006). Psychological resilience ater disaster: New York works of Sigmund Freud, Vol. 14 (pp. 152–170). London:
City in the atermath of the September 11th terrorist Hogarth Press.
attack. Psychological Science, 17, 181–186. Gross, J. J. & John, O. P. (2003). Individual diferences in two
Bonanno, G. A., Neria, Y., Mancini, A., et al. (2007). Is emotion regulation processes: Implications for afect,
there more to complicated grief than depression and relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and
posttraumatic stress disorder? A test of incremental Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.
validity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 342–351. Horowitz, M. J. (1986). Stress response syndromes, 2nd edn.
Bonanno, G. A., Ho, S. M. Y., Chan, J. C., et al. (2008). Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
Psychological resilience and dysfunction among Horowitz, M. J., Siegel, B., Holen, A., et al. (1997).
hospitalized survivors of the SARS epidemic in Hong Diagnostic criteria for complicated grief disorder.
Kong: A latent class approach. Health Psychology, 27, American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 904–910.
659–667.
Irwin, M. R. & Pike, J. (1993). Bereavement, depressive
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss. New York: Basic Books. symptoms, and immune function. In M. S. Stroebe,
Brickman, P. & Campbell, D. T. (1971). Hedonic relativism W. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson (eds.), Handbook of
and planning the good society. In M. H. Appley (ed.), bereavement: heory, research, and intervention (pp. 160–
Adaptation level theory: A symposium (pp. 287–302). New 171). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
York: Academic Press. Irwin, M. R., Daniels, M., Weiner, H., et al. (1987). Immune
Capps, L. & Bonanno, G. A. (2000). Narrating bereavement: and neuroendocrine changes during bereavement.
hematic and grammatical predictors of adjustment to Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 10, 449–465.
loss. Discourse Processes, 30, 1–25. Kaltman, S. & Bonanno, G. A. (2003). Trauma and
Carr, D., House, J. S., Wortman, C., Neese, R., & Kessler, R. C. bereavement: Examining the impact of sudden and
(2001). Psychological adjustment to sudden and violent deaths. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 131–147.
anticipated spousal loss among older widowed persons. Kaprio, J., Koskenvuo, M., & Rita, H. (1987). Mortality
Journals of Gerontology 56B, S237–S248. ater bereavement: A prospective study of 95 647
Coyne, J. C., Wortman, C. B., & Lehman, D. R. (1988). he bereaved persons. American Journal of Public Health,
other side of support: Emotional overinvolvement and 77, 283–287.
miscarried helping. In B. H. Gottlieb (ed.), Marshaling Kelly, A. E. & McKillop, K. J. (1996). Consequences of
social support: Formats, processes, and efects (pp. 305– revealing personal secrets. Psychological Bulletin, 120,
330). housand Oaks, CA: Sage. 450–465.
Currier, J. M., Neimeyer, R. A., & Berman, J. S. (2008). he Keltner, D. (1997). Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the
efectiveness of psychotherapeutic interventions for distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and

198
Chapter 13: Loss and grief

shame. In P. Ekman & E. L. Rosenberg (eds.), What the Parkes, C. M. (1970). he irst year of bereavement: a
face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous longitudinal study of the reaction of London widows to
expression using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) the death of their husbands. Psychiatry, 33, 444–467.
(pp. 133–160). London: Oxford University Press. Parkes, C. M. & Weiss, R. S. (1983). Recovery from
Keltner, D. & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter bereavement. New York: Basic Books.
and dissociation: Distinct correlates of laughter and
Prigerson, H. G., Maciejewski, P. K., Reynolds, C. F. 3rd,
smiling during bereavement. Journal of Personality and
et al. (1995). Inventory of Complicated Grief: A scale
Social Psychology, 73, 687–702.
to measure maladaptive symptoms of loss. Psychiatry
Kim, K. & Jacobs, S. C. (1993). Neuroendocrine changes Research, 59, 65–79.
in bereavement. In M. S. Stroebe, W. Stroebe, & R. O.
Prigerson, H. G., Bierhals, A. J., Kasl, S. V., et al. (1996).
Hansson (eds.), Handbook of bereavement: heory,
Complicated grief as a disorder distinct from
research, and intervention (pp. 143–159). Cambridge,
bereavement-related depression and anxiety: A
UK: Cambridge University Press.
replication study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
Lehman, D. R., Wortman, C. B., & Williams, A. F. (1987). 1484–1486.
Long-term efects of losing a spouse or child in a motor
Raphael, B. (1983). Recovery from bereavement. New York:
vehicle crash. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Basic Books.
52, 218–231.
Lichtenthal, W. G., Cruess, D. G., & Prigerson, H. G. (2004). Shedler, J., Mayman, M., & Manis, M. (1993). he illusion of
A case for establishing complicated grief as a distinct mental health. American Psychologist, 48, 1117–1131.
mental disorder in DSM-V. Clinical Psychology Review, Shuchter, S. R. & Zisook, S. (1993). he course of normal
24, 637–662. grief. In M. S. Stroebe, W. Stroebe, & R. O. Hansson
Lindemann, E. (1944). Symptomatology and management of (eds.), Handbook of bereavement: heory, research, and
acute grief. American Journal of Psychiatry, 101, 141–149. intervention (pp. 23–43). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Mancini, A. D. & Bonanno, G. A. (2006). Resilience in
the face of potential trauma: Clinical practices and Stroebe, M. S. & Stroebe, W. (1993). he mortality of
illustrations. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, bereavement: A review. In M. S. Stroebe, W. Stroebe, &
971–985. R. O. Hansson (eds.), Handbook of bereavement: heory,
research, and intervention (pp. 175–195). Cambridge,
Mancini, A. D., Bonanno, G. A., & Clark, A. (2011). UK: Cambridge University Press.
Stepping of the hedonic treadmill: latent class analyses
of individual diferences in response to major life events. hompson, L. W., Breckenridge, J. N., Gallagher, D., et al.
Journal of Individual Diferences, in press. (1984). Efects of bereavement on self-perceptions of
physical health in elderly widows and widowers. Journal
Middleton, W., Moylan, A., Raphael, B., Burnett, P., & of Gerontology, 39, 309–314.
Martinek, N. (1993). An international perspective on
bereavement related concepts. Australian and New Tudiver, F., Hilditch, J., Permaul, J. A., et al. (1991). A
Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 27, 457–463. comparison of psychosocial characteristics of new
widowers and married men. Family Medicine, 23,
Muthén, B. (2004). Latent variable analysis: Growth mixture 501–505.
modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data. In
D. Kaplan (ed.), Handbook of quantitative methodology for Wortman, C. B. & Silver, R. C. (2001). he myths of coping
the social sciences (pp. 345–368). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. with loss revisited. In M. S. Stroebe, R. O. Hansson,
W. Stroebe & H. Schut (eds.), Handbook of bereavement
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., McBride, A., & Larson, J. (1997).
research: Consequences, coping, and care (pp. 405–429).
Rumination and psychological distress among bereaved
Washington, DC: American Psychological Press.
partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
855–862. Zisook, S. & Shuchter, S. R. (1993). Major depression
associated with widowhood. American Journal of
Osterweis, M., Solomon, F., & Green, F. (1984). Bereavement:
Geriatric Psychiatry, 1, 316–326.
Reactions, consequences, and care. Washington, DC:
National Academy Press. Zisook, S., Shuchter, S. R., Irwin, M., & Darko, D. F. (1994).
Bereavement, depression, and immune function.
Park, C. L., Edmondson, D., Fenster, J. R., & Blank, T. O.
Psychiatry Research, 52, 1–10.
(2008). Meaning making and psychological adjustment
following cancer: he mediating roles of growth, life Zisook, S., Chentsova-Dutton, Y., & Shuchter, S. R. (1998).
meaning, and restored just-world beliefs. Journal of PTSD following bereavement. Annals of Clinical
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 863–875. Psychiatry, 10, 157–163.

199
Section
Section4 Specific challenges
Chapter
Reorienting resilience: adapting resilience

14 for post-disaster research


Jennifer Johnson and Sandro Galea

Introduction Disaster research


he past few years have seen an increase in research his section introduces and orients readers who are
interest about resilience ater speciic traumatic events. not familiar with the disaster literature. Disasters are
One of the more interesting developments in the peer- quite common occurrences worldwide. It is estimated
reviewed literature about the topic is a broadening of that nearly 500 disasters occur every year (excluding
the term “resilience,” extending it from its original droughts and war) in which around 50 000 people
formulation pertaining to individual experiences die, 74 000 are injured, 5 million are displaced, and
and individual reactions (Bonanno, 2004; Butler et al., 80 million are afected (Norris et al., 2005). Norris
2007; de Mel et al., 2008; Ganzel et al., 2008) to con- (2005, p. 141) aptly describes the challenges that dis-
siderations of populations or groups who collectively asters pose: “For the survivors, disasters may engen-
experience traumatic events (Bruneau et al., 2003; der an array of stressors, including threat to one’s life
Kendra & Wachtendorf, 2003; Vale & Campanella, and physical integrity, exposure to the dead and dying,
2005; Norris et al., 2008). his chapter focuses on the bereavement, profound loss, social and community
particular challenges that arise when considering disruption, and ongoing hardship.”
resilience of communities in the context of collect- Disasters are costly – both in terms of human life
ively experienced traumatic events, typically called (Figure 14.1) and inancial loss (Figure 14.2). his is
disasters. his extension is referred to as a “multilevel” illustrated with two examples of the inancial and life
conception of resilience, recognizing that in this for- losses in two very diferent disasters. he Indian Ocean
mulation resilience is both an “individual level” and a tsunami of 2004 caused around 230 000 deaths and
“community level” phenomenon. material loss in excess of US$10 billion, not counting
Although the term disaster represents a broad the indirect costs, clean-up, or the cost of morbidity
range of human experiences, disasters have in com- and mortality (Smolka, 2006). he tsunami had an
mon their disruption of collectively experienced nor- impact on countries more than 5 000 km away from
mal life. his chapter irst briely introduces disaster its source (United Nations Oice for the Coordination
public health research before going on to discuss the of Humanitarian Afairs, 2005). Less than one year
origins and application of resilience as a multilevel ater the tsunami, Hurricane Katrina struck the US
concept. A perspective is ofered on the meaning and Gulf Coast, resulting in an estimated 1800 known
uses of resilience in disaster research and the chal- fatalities (Knabb et al., 2005) and an estimated total
lenges of applying the concept of multilevel resilience economic loss of $125 billion (Risk Management
to disaster settings. his is followed by a brief review Solutions, 2005). he damages from this disaster were
of the special challenges disasters pose across the mul- far reaching, including impaired oil-reining capacity
tiple levels of human organization – from the molecu- through destroyed oil rigs, destruction of transporta-
lar to the ecological. Finally, resilience is reoriented in tion arteries through damaged bridges and roads, and
terms of disasters and future research directions are displacement for thousands of residents into shelter
suggested within this rapidly evolving and dynamic or temporary housing (Knabb et al., 2005). Although
concept. these two settings and disasters are very diferent, the

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

200
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

loss experienced in each will impact afected individ- he increases in occurrence and cost of natural dis-
uals and the communities for generations to come. asters are likely caused by a combination of factors: con-
Of particular concern, the rate and cost of disasters centration of populations in urban areas (Klein et al.,
have increased in the last half-century. he Center for 2003; Smolka, 2006; James et al., 2008), development
Research on the Epidemiology of Disasters reports an of risky coastal/valley regions (Smolka, 2006; James
increase in the number of natural disasters since the et al., 2008), increasing complexity of modern societies
early 1980s, particularly for hydrological and meteoro- and technology (Smolka, 2006), and global warming/
logical disaster types (Scheuren et al., 2008). Figure climate change (Klein et al., 2003; Smolka, 2006; James
14.1 shows a breakdown of the number of natural dis- et al., 2008). Human settlement in hazardous areas
asters by number of victims from 1987 to 2006; the oten destroys local ecosystems that act as protection
number of disasters increases for each category over against natural disasters (Mileti, 1999). Moreover,
this period (Hoyois et al., 2007). Smolka (2006) showed some hazard mitigation eforts only postpone the inev-
an increase in economic and insured losses from nat- itable and oten put more life and property at risk when
ural disasters from 1950 to 2005, even ater adjusting the eventual event occurs (Mileti, 1999). In addition,
for inlation (Figure 14.2). the increasing reliance on and interconnectedness of

180 Figure 14.1 Number of natural disasters


by number of victims. (Based on Figure 10
160 in Hoyois et al., 2007. Reproduced with
permission from EM-DAT: The OFDA/
Number of disasters

140 CRED International Disaster Database –


120 Université catholique de Louvain –
Brussels (www.emdat.be).)
100
80
60
40
20
0
1987 1989 1991 1993 1995 1997 1999 2001 2003 2005

<1000 1000–9999 1 000 000–9 999 999


10 000–99 999 100 000–999 999 >10 000 000

200 Figure 14.2 Economic losses from


large-scale natural disasters 1950–2005,
180 adjusted for inflation. (Reproduced, with
160 permission, from figure 1, Smolka, 2006.)

140
$US billion

120

100

80

60

40

20

0
50

53

56

59

62

65

68

71

74

77

80

83

86

89

92

95

98

01

04
19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

19

20

20

201
Section 4: Specific challenges

technology puts the world at far greater risk of severe For a number of reasons, disaster research is less
damage from technological disasters (Mitchell & empirically informed than many other ields of research
Townsend, 2005). For example, the Nimda computer today. Abramson and colleagues (2007) conducted an
virus, which struck 85 000 servers worldwide about assessment of medical and public health literature on
one week ater the September 11, 2001 (9/11) terror- disasters, inding 32% of articles to be reviews or com-
ist attack, led to far more Internet congestion, outages, mentary, 24% case studies, 12.2% surveys, 8.3% pro-
and economic damage than the infrastructure damage gram or policy evaluation, 6.6% quasi-experimental/
from the 9/11 terrorist attacks (Mitchell & Townsend, observational, 6.6% secondary data analysis, and less
2005). than 10% key informant interview, clinical trial, epi-
he incidence and consequences of disasters are demiological investigation, operational research, or
far from equally distributed across regions of the focus group. In summary, these authors found about
world. Asian nations, particularly population-dense as much review and commentary on the subject as
India and China, were the hardest hit by natural dis- original research. Disasters are particularly diicult
asters in 2007, representing 37% of reported natural to study for a number of reasons. Most disasters are
disasters, 90% of reported victims, and 46% of eco- unanticipated, yet study planning, institutional review
nomic damages from natural disasters (Scheuren et al., board approvals, and funding all take time to obtain.
2008). India and China both ranked in the highest 10 In addition, existing records may be damaged in the
countries for number of disasters, number of disaster disaster and records taken just ater the disaster may
deaths, and economic damages from disaster in 2006 be of far lower quality owing to confusion and lack
as well (Hoyois et al., 2007). In addition to death and of coordination in an overwhelmed system. Lastly,
inancial cost, disasters are oten associated with psy- research in less wealthy nations may be challenged by
chopathology for exposed individuals. he severity of lack of ongoing surveillance and lack of existing health
psychological impairment following natural disasters infrastructure.
is higher in less wealthy nations than in more wealthy
ones, while the pattern of impairment severity follow-
ing technological disasters is the opposite (Norris et al.,
Resilience: origins of a multilevel
2002). concept
Disaster research in public health aims to iden- he concept of resilience has gained much momen-
tify the short- and long-term outcomes of disasters as tum since the late 1990s. With roots in the engineer-
well as the distribution of those outcomes in popula- ing ield, the term has come to represent individual
tions. Disaster research is heavily informed by disasters psychological, family, social, and ecological processes.
occurring in more wealthy nations. A recent meta- Below, we briely review the origins of resilience and
analysis on behavioral health outcomes following ter- highlight its importance as a multilevel concept in dis-
rorist events found 88 of 111 published articles on this aster research.
topic were in North American populations, while only Resilience began as a technical engineering con-
5 of 227 worldwide terrorist incidents were located cept meaning the speed of return to equilibrium ater
in North America (DiMaggio & Galea, 2006). Of 160 displacement (Bodin & Wiman, 2004) or the “ability
articles on psychosocial consequences of disasters to store strain energy and delect elastically under a
reviewed by Norris and colleagues (2002), 91 (57%) load without breaking or being deformed” (Gordon,
were from the USA and territories, 46 (29%) from other 1978, p 129). While this deinition is speciic and eas-
wealthy countries, and 23 (14%) were from less wealthy ily deined and measured, it has limited applicability to
nations. Disaster research is also unequally distributed human systems, which are both proactive and reactive,
by disaster type. In the Norris review, over 55% of the have multiple possible equilibrium states, and evolve
published literature was on the consequences of nat- over time.
ural disasters, 34% on technological disasters, and 11% Ecological deinitions of resilience vary. Holling
on mass violence (Norris et al., 2002). his is likely a deined resilience as the persistence of a system and
relection of the frequency of disaster occurrence, the its ability to absorb disturbance without damaging or
severity of consequences, and the media attention and altering the relationships within it (Holling, 1973).
public awareness of each type of event (e.g., a drought Klein and colleagues (2003, p. 40) deined resilience by
versus a bombing). two components: “the amount of disturbance a system

202
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

can absorb and still remain within the same state or populations faces a number of challenges that we will
domain of attraction and the degree to which the sys- return to below.
tem is capable of self-organization.” hese deinitions
are applicable to human systems in that they allow The use and meaning of the concept of
for multiple stable states and for a dynamic, evolving
system. In addition, they deine the system’s resilience resilience in disaster research
through its relationships, rather than physical proper- he concept of resilience is important in disaster
ties, further increasing applicability and relevance to research for a number of reasons. First, a return to nor-
human populations. malcy is appropriate and unambiguously desirable in
In medicine, the term resilience was originally many post-disaster contexts. he infrastructure and
co-opted by developmental psychologists and used to institutions of a community, including such things as
describe children who, despite adverse genetic or envir- electricity, clean water, gas, sewage, and health ser-
onmental conditions, exceeded expectations of psy- vices, are oten easily objectiied and measured in both
chological adaptation and wellness (Butler et al., 2007). pre- and post-disaster setting. hey are also related to
At its most extreme, resilience has been suggested to an issue that individual members of the community are
be synonymous with invulnerability (Rutter, 1993). most concerned with: is it safe to go home? Return to
Applied to broader psychology, resilience has been normalcy has profound importance in terms of indi-
deined as the ability to endure temporary upheaval viduals’ outlooks on life (de Mel et al., 2008), daily
with no apparent disruption in ability to function at functioning (Jordan et al., 2004), and work perform-
work or in relationships (Bonanno, 2004). Bonanno ance (Boscarino et al., 2006).
articulated a trajectory of resilience that is distinct he concept of resilience may also serve a func-
from recovery, where temporary psychopathology fol- tional purpose. As Vale and Campanella (2005, p. 340),
lows trauma and is gradually overcome. Egeland and noted, “In a sense, the notion of a resilient city is a soci-
colleagues (1993) deined resilience as a process: “the etally and economically productive form of denial.”
development of competence despite severe or perva- he denial of pathology following disaster may pave
sive adversity.” hese deinitions of resilience are spe- the way for the return of investment in a community
ciic to human psychology and easily measured by on all levels. Former Federal Emergency Management
various pre-ordained levels of functioning or compe- Agency administrator James Lee Witt is quoted as say-
tence (e.g., hours worked, quality of life). However, ing, “all disasters are political” (Abramson et al., 2007).
this deinition does not allow for the multiple possible Demonstration of resilience may be an end in and of
equilibrium states in community or group systems. itself, particularly where administrators need to prove
Nor does it adequately capture community factors or their worth.
feedback between multiple actors. When the salience of the resilience concept in both
Community resilience, a relatively new concept, is social and political terms is recognized, it is then not
gaining in importance in disaster research today. Like surprising that resilience has tremendous utility in both
Egeland, Norris and colleagues (2008, p. 131) deined disaster research and disaster planning. Two key works
community resilience as a process rather than an out- in the area are those of Norris and colleagues (2008)
come: “a process of linking a set of adaptive capacities and Bruneau and colleagues (2003). Norris et al. (2008)
to a positive trajectory of functioning and adaptation discusses how disasters, in addition to shocking the sys-
in constituent populations ater disturbance.” Mileti tem oten simultaneously damage the very resources
(1999, p. 4) described a similar concept, “Sustainability necessary for a resilient response. Bruneau and col-
means that a locality can tolerate – and overcome – leagues (2003, pp. 737–738) outlined four character-
damage, diminished productivity, and reduced quality istics thought to predict community resilience: “ability
of life from an extreme event without signiicant out- to withstand a given level of stress or demand without
side assistance.” his conceptualization of resilience sufering degradation or loss of function” (robustness),
examines the function of systemic features, rather than “the extent to which elements, systems, or other units
individual features, making it a potentially very useful of analysis exist that are substitutable” (redundancy),
concept for the purposes of identifying potential ave- “the capacity to identify problems, establish priorities,
nues for community-level intervention. However, the and mobilize resources when conditions exist that
expansion of resilience to consider disaster-afected threaten to disrupt some element” (resourcefulness),

203
Section 4: Specific challenges

and “the capacity to meet priorities and achieve goals women, children, and the elderly (Nishikiori et al.,
in a timely manner in order to contain losses and avoid 2006), and people with mental disorders, physical dis-
future disruption” (rapidity). Although the utility of a abilities, recent hospitalization, or low income (Chou
resourcefulness factor has recently been challenged in et al., 2004). Because of the unexpected nature of many
the prediction of community resilience, the other three disasters, individuals are unable to control their expos-
have become generally accepted in recent literature on ure to many of these risk factors for death and injury.
the subject (Norris et al., 2008). Survivors with permanent injuries may have ongoing
Bruneau et al. (2003) continued to specify examples medical needs (Rathore et al., 2007; Tauqir et al., 2007)
of these measures, such as partial power restored to and resilience means moving on in the face of disabil-
households within one hour, for social performance ity, where a “return to normalcy” may not be possible.
of rapidity. Other factors are more challenging to Disaster-related stress is associated with physio-
evaluate (operationalize, quantify, and judge) in the logical and anatomical changes in survivors. Exposure
post-disaster setting. Moreover, disaster planners are to the Oklahoma City bombing was associated with
ideally interested in the efectiveness of pre-disaster neurological reactivity six to seven years later (Tucker
community characteristics to predict post-disaster et al., 2007). hree to four years ater the 9/11 terror-
resilience. To continue the example above, what pre- ist attacks, proximity to the attacks was associated with
disaster aspects of a community’s social infrastructure gray matter volume changes in brain areas related to
make power restoration within one hour post-disaster mental disorders (Ganzel et al., 2008). Animal mod-
possible? While more empiric evidence is needed to els support the role of stress-induced brain changes
inform disaster planning eforts, these are clearly very (Helmreich et al., 1999). Disaster exposure is also asso-
good places to start. ciated with short-term death from cardiac conditions
(Leor et al., 1996; Gold et al., 2007); higher resting heart
Disasters as a special challenge to rate, serum cholesterol, and triglycerides (Trevisan
et al., 1992); and long-term higher resting heart rate
multiple levels of functioning (Bland et al., 2000) and serum uric acid levels (Trevisan
Unlike many traumatic events, disasters act all at once et al., 1997), particularly for individuals who have lost
on individuals, families, and communities. his sec- inancial or social resources in the disaster (Trevisan
tion of the chapter provides a brief overview of the et al., 1997; Bland et al., 2000). Exposure to disasters
multilevel consequences of disasters that challenge our in utero is associated with changes to the adult physio-
multilevel concept of resilience. Disaster threats to the logical proile, including cortisol and testosterone
individual include direct threat of injury and death, levels (Huizink et al., 2008), glucose tolerance, athero-
physiological changes, mental health consequences, genic lipid proile, and ibrinogen concentrations
and indirect and ongoing risk of injury. hreats to the (Roseboom et al., 2001). In these ways, disaster-asso-
family include property loss and inancial/employment ciated stress may have long-term or lifelong impacts,
concerns. hreats to the community include cultural/ even generations later.
identity conceptualization, economic, and political
concerns. Psychological health
he psychological impacts of disasters are oten more
Physical health heterogeneous, widespread, and longer lasting than
Disasters are oten characterized by their capacity for their physical impact. Vicarious grief ater disasters
causing mass injury and death. For those who have is disproportionate to their burden, compared with
perished, resilience is moot. Direct threat of death and more chronic problems of disease or malnutrition
injury depends on disaster type (Llewellyn, 2006), tim- (Chochinov, 2005). Mental health problems follow-
ing, and severity (Peleg et al., 2002), as well as individual ing disasters have been reported by those directly
circumstances such as proximity, location, and quality afected (e.g., injured [Hoven et al., 2003] or relocated
of infrastructure (Peleg et al., 2002). Risk of injury or [Soeteman et al., 2007]), those indirectly afected (e.g.,
death also depend on demographic characteristics, witnessing burial of corpses [Ghafari-Nejad et al.,
where those with lower power and resources are gen- 2007]), and those unafected or vicariously afected by
erally at highest risk of an adverse event, for example disaster (e.g., living in the same country [Silver et al.,

204
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

2002] or exposed to the media [Wayment, 2004]). through their persistence, lack of individual control
Speciic mental health outcomes following disaster over exposure, and their delayed or unanticipated
include post-traumatic stress disorder (Galea et al., impact. hese indirect risks may also exact a toll on
2005), depression (Armenian et al., 2002; Bromet & psychological well-being.
Havenaar, 2007), anxiety (Grievink et al., 2007), sleep
problems (Dirkzwager et al., 2006; Grievink et al., Rescue and clean-up operations
2007), behavioral problems (Reijneveld et al., 2003;
In order to respond adaptively to disaster, communi-
Chemtob et al., 2008), suicidality (Chou et al., 2003;
ties must mobilize rescue and clean-up personnel. Yet
Loganovsky et al., 2008), and decreased quality of life
these same people that lead a community to a resilient
(Wang et al., 2000; Slottje et al., 2007).
response are, themselves, at a higher risk for persistent
he trajectory of mental illness following disasters
physical and psychological impairment. he impact on
is tremendously heterogeneous and may last years to
rescue and clean-up personnel is highlighted because
decades (homas, 2006; Chou et al., 2007; Tucker et al.,
they are part of a resilient response and, at the same
2007; Loganovsky et al., 2008). Mental health trajec-
time, prone to negative outcomes as individuals. Of the
tory following a disaster may be inluenced by other
2801 people killed in the 9/11 terrorist attacks, 343 were
forms of recovery (e.g., inancial [Nandi et al., 2004]);
ire and 60 law enforcement personnel (14%) (Bradt,
perceived support (Tang, 2007); demographic factors
2003). Time spent on the disaster scene has been asso-
such as age, race, gender, or education level (Bonanno
ciated with occupational exposure and symptomology,
et al., 2007); recent or past life stressors (Bonanno et al.,
oten in a dose–response manner (Feldman et al., 2004).
2007); genetic background (Kilpatrick et al., 2007); or
Rescue and clean-up personnel may also be exposed to
time alone (de Mel et al., 2008). As with risk for phys-
unusual industrial chemicals (e.g., petroluorochemi-
ical harm, the probability of a resilient trajectory of
cals used in ire-ighting foams [Tao et al., 2008]).
mental health is inluenced by many factors outside
Disaster-associated exposures may lead to chronic
individual control.
health problems for rescue and clean-up personnel.
For example, rescue and police oicers responding to
Environmental health the Tokyo subway sarin attack showed chronic prob-
lems, with memory decline three to four years later in a
In addition to the direct risk of physical injury and
death, disasters may also produce environmental con- dose–response manner with exposure level (Nishiwaki
et al., 2001). One year ater the 9/11 terrorist attacks,
ditions with protracted or even delayed health impact.
Reduced air quality is associated with worsening or workers showed lung function decline equivalent to 12
years of age-related decline (Banauch et al., 2006). In
new respiratory complaints following volcanic erup-
tions (Michaud et al., 2004; Shimizu et al., 2007), forest addition to short- and long-term physical risks, rescue
ires (Moore et al., 2006), haze disasters (Kunii et al., and clean-up workers have shown excess risk of sui-
cide (Rahu et al., 1997), chronic anxiety, depression,
2002), and bombing incidents (Lioy et al., 2002). Mold
somatic symptoms, obsessive–compulsive symptoms,
following hurricanes and lood disasters may result in
infections and allergic or hypersensitivity reactions interpersonal sensitivity, hostility, and sleeping prob-
(Metts, 2008). Reduced water quality following storms lems (Huizink et al., 2006). While rescue and clean-up
workers are essential to disaster response and recovery,
and looding may be associated with an increased
they bear a great challenge to their individual health
incidence of infectious disease (Kondo et al., 2002).
Industrial disasters may cause soil contamination and resilience in that their exposure far exceeds that of
bioaccumulation (Meharga et al., 1999). Agricultural most bystanders and may continue for months or years
ater the disaster.
contamination with radioactive substances may ind
its way into milk and foodstufs, wood, mushrooms,
berries, and game (Alexakhin et al., 2007). Ater Displacement
Chernobyl, consumption of agricultural foodstufs Displacement is a far more common direct consequence
from the afected region was a leading source of acci- of disasters than death or injury. Loss of shelter leaves
dental irradiation (Alexakhin et al., 2007). hese few people vulnerable to disease (Kondo et al., 2002) and
examples provide a brief glimpse of the environmen- heat or cold (Llewellyn, 2006). hose in temporary liv-
tal challenges to health resilience that disasters pose ing conditions may sufer from chronic undernutrition

205
Section 4: Specific challenges

(Jayatissa et al., 2006), mental illness, the worsening Disasters and the community: relationships
of chronic diseases, and lack of access to medical care
for years ater disaster (Shehab et al., 2008). Primary between people and place
concerns for displaced populations include economic In addition to destruction of the physical environ-
security; physical and economic access to food; rela- ment in which communities are situated, disasters
tive freedom from disease and infection; access to clean pose several challenges to other elements of commu-
water, air, and non-degraded land; security from vio- nity, including concepts of identity and relationship
lence and threats; security of cultural identity; and with the environment. Disasters may shatter trad-
protection of their basic human rights (Kett, 2005). itional assumptions of security, predictability, and
Displacement ater disaster poses a major challenge to trust (Walsh, 2007). he following quote from a focus
physical and mental well-being; displaced persons have group in Tamil Nadu, India following the Indian Ocean
oten lost family members and economic resources and tsunami shows how cultural conceptualizations of the
are vulnerable to physical and mental health assaults, relationship between people and nature can be altered
sometimes over a protracted time period. by disaster (Rajkumar et al., 2008, p. 847): “We have
been taught in our childhood how to survive during
Disasters and the family storms and cyclones. We would be alert only when we
were sailing over the sea. On the shore, we would be
In addition to the hazards that disasters pose to indi-
relaxed like sleeping in our mother’s lap. his is the irst
viduals who come in contact with these events, families
time we learned that the sea might hit us even when we
also sufer disaster losses that in many ways difer from
are on the shore.”
the losses experienced by individuals. Families suf-
Individual self-assessments of a community’s cap-
fer property loss ater disasters through severe direct
acity to deal successfully with the ongoing challenge
damage (Madamala et al., 2007), disaster sequelae (e.g.,
may have an impact on the relationship between disas-
ires ater the Kobe earthquake [Shaw & Goda, 2004]),
ter threat and mental health of its members (Kimhi &
and post-disaster involuntary resettlement programs
Shamai, 2004).
(Badri et al., 2006). For small business owners, dam-
Post-disaster resettlement and changes in day to
age to the workplace predicts non-return (Madamala
day living further disrupt the cultural relationship
et al., 2007). Relocation may also be motivated by avoid-
between people and place (Oliver-Smith, 1991; Dugan,
ance of traumatic reminders (Salcoglu et al., 2008). In
2007). Temporary living situations that separate dis-
addition to property, loss of family “irreplaceables”
aster survivors from their communities are associated
(lost sentimental possessions) is common ater disas-
with adverse mental health consequences (Perez-Sales
ter (Galea et al., 2008). Family loss is associated with
et al., 2005). Interestingly, Perez-Sales and colleagues
a number of mental health problems, including post-
showed that when emergency shelters organized tent
traumatic stress disorder (Armenian et al., 2000; Chou
assignment by community of origin, survivors had
et al., 2007) and suicide (Chou et al., 2003). Even threat-
fewer feelings of humiliation and emotional discom-
ened loss is predictive of distress (O’Neill et al., 1999).
fort. While the preservation of community groups is
Displaced families oten have to change their pri-
ideal ater disaster, it is not always possible. Challenges
mary source of income, particularly those families ori-
to the cultural relationship with place, nature, and
ginally employed in agriculture or commercial activities
people pose serious challenges to the cultural resilience
(Badri et al., 2006). Families reliant on individuals suf-
of communities following disaster.
fering permanent disaster-related injury may require a
substantial transition in employment to maintain their
inancial security (Gul et al., 2008). Displaced families Economic and political impact
may be forced to spend more on their households in Disasters also pose a challenge to the economic and
an unfamiliar environment (Badri et al., 2006). his political well-being of communities. Loss of public
increased expenditure, combined with job transition infrastructure, utilities, and communications follow-
and loss of savings, may result in increased borrowing ing disaster are common challenges to businesses and
for daily needs and debt. Because property and posses- residents within and around disaster-afected commu-
sions may be a family’s main source of income or form nities (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
of investment, their loss in disasters oten produces 2006). Restoration of transportation methods, such as
profound economic vulnerability. railways, roads, and harbors, ater disasters is essential
206
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

to economic restoration and may take years to accom- travel), but these measures are by no means compre-
plish (Shaw & Goda, 2004). Shared public resources hensive. he next section reviews the limitations and
such as hospitals, universities, and schools are oten challenges to applying resilience theory to disaster
damaged and are sometimes permanently closed in settings.
the wake of disaster (Guidry & Margolis, 2005; Schultz
et al., 2007; Ayyala & Sacks, 2008) Lastly, disasters may Challenges in applying resilience to
have large-scale economic and political consequences
for entire countries, particularly where they disrupt
disasters
locations of centralized power (e.g., the Mexico City he earlier part of this chapter provided a brief intro-
earthquake [Davis, 2005]). duction to disaster research and looked at the his-
Relocation caused by disasters disrupts the eco- tory, uses, and meaning of the term resilience before
nomic and political relationships between people and giving an overview of the dimensions along which
place in terms of resource availability, access to employ- disaster disruption acts to challenge individual, fam-
ment and labor resources, territoriality, leadership ily, and community well-being. Yet, adaptation and
structures, and intergroup relations (Oliver-Smith, continued existence are far more common outcomes
1991). Resettlement also afects the political and eco- than extinction following disaster. Clearly, then, resili-
nomic environment of host communities where dis- ence is an apposite construct for disaster research.
placed persons settle (Badri et al., 2006). However, thinking of resilience as a multilevel con-
Where disasters are associated with mass demo- struct poses particular challenges, especially where
graphic shit, they may result in the restructuring of communities have actually changed shape through
class systems within communities. Ater the Indian drastic demographic shit and physical change to the
Ocean tsunami, socioeconomic and cultural difer- environment. he expansion of resilience to consider
ences between isherman and lower castes in India disaster-afected communities is associated with a
were reduced, because both groups sufered prop- number of challenges, including (1) the identiication
erty loss and lived as neighbors in emergency shelters of the population of interest, (2) the quantiication of
(Rajkumar et al., 2008). In addition, because ishermen “baseline” state, and (3) the measurement of adapta-
were at greater risk of death (Nishikiori et al., 2006), the tion in a meaningful way. Lastly, this form of resilience
tsunami resulted in the opening of lucrative isherman is concerned with population-level functioning and is,
jobs to survivors of lower castes (Rajkumar et al., 2008). therefore, potentially insensitive to the heterogeneity
Ater the Manjil earthquake, resettled communities of impact among subgroups within the system. hese
saw an increase in women working outside the home issues are discussed in more detail below.
because of a lack of home-related farm work and an
overall decreased reliance on agriculture for household Defining community
income (Badri et al., 2006). Many of these examples One of the principal challenges faced by the expansion
showcase the power of disaster-afected communities of resilience to a multilevel construction is the dein-
to adapt through large-scale demographic and eco- ition of community. In social sciences, the community
nomic changes. Community adaptations to altered has been deined as a group of people bound by social,
economic and political circumstances may be delayed, functional, cultural, or circumstantial connection
however, by prolonged uncertainty in the atermath of (Chaskin, 1997). Communities are dynamic, negoti-
disaster or reduced overall assets (De Silva & Yamao, ated, voluntary, and neither totally inclusive nor exclu-
2007). sive (Chaskin, 1997). Norris and colleagues (2008, p.
At other times, disasters may bring to light under- 128) more broadly deined the term “community” to
lying vulnerability and result in more accurate future better suit the purposes of community resilience the-
estimations of risk. Ater the 9/11 terrorist attacks, ory: “Communities are composed of built, natural,
insurance premiums on prominent buildings in New social, and economic environments that inluence
York City increased by as much as 300% as the down- one another in complex ways.” However, communities
town area was seen as an area of concentrated risk (Vale may span large geographic regions (e.g., the Jewish-
& Campanella, 2005). American community). In addition, populations are
Some measures of economic resilience are fairly not merely the sum of individuals, just as population
intuitive (e.g., return of power, restoration of railway health is not the sum of individual health (Reidpath,

207
Section 4: Specific challenges

120 Figure 14.3 Defining baseline as


“quality of infrastructure” after disaster.
t0, time of an earthquake when damage
100 to community infrastructure results in a
50% fall; t1, time at which the community
Quality of infrastructure (%)

returns to close to 100% quality of


infrastructure. (Based on data in Bruneau
80
et al., 2003.)

60

40

20
t0 t1

0
Time

2005). Lastly, the boundaries that deine community Return to what baseline?
are likely to vary from observer to observer.
Another challenge in the extension of resilience to the
Compounding these challenges, disasters inluence
community is the deinition of a baseline level of func-
areas that do not necessarily match the boundaries
tioning. Bruneau and colleagues (2003) provided a
observers may ascribe to communities. For example,
framework for assessing earthquake resilience (Figure
injury and death in the Indian Ocean tsunami was
multicontinental – including the Seychelles, Maldives, 14.3). In this diagram, t0 is the time of an earthquake
Somalia, India, Bangladesh, Myanmar, hailand, when damage to community infrastructure results in
Malaysia, and Indonesia (United Nations Oice for the a 50% drop. Over time, restoration projects repair the
Coordination of Humanitarian Afairs, 2005). Even damage and the community returns to 100% qual-
when destruction and death are isolated to a single ity of infrastructure by time t1. he authors go on to
city, the impact may be felt over an entire country. For describe several illustrative performance measures,
example, students at Northern Arizona University who such as percentage of homes with power immediately
experienced no personal bereavement in the 9/11 ter- ater an earthquake and time required to initiate and
rorist attacks showed disaster-focused distress, includ- complete critical response tasks (e.g., ire-ighting,
ing grief, survivor guilt, and intrusive thoughts of the search and rescue).
disaster (Wayment, 2004). Exposure to media, medi- However, measures of functionality are funda-
ated by perceived similarity to victims, was associ- mentally set by an a-priori understanding of “normal”
ated with disaster-focused distress nearly six months function that frequently are dependent on disciplinary
ater the attacks (Wayment, 2004). Hurricane Katrina perspectives. For example, economists measure loss
caused direct damage to numerous communities in the to economic output (e.g., Rose, 2006), civil engineers
states of Louisiana and Mississippi, with hurricane- assess transportation network coverage and accessi-
related deaths reported in the parishes of Orleans, St. bility (Chang & Nojima, 2001), and religious leaders
Bernard, and Jeferson (Brunkard et al., 2008). he measure the recovery of faith and spiritual life (Ford
majority of deaths occurred in the Orleans parish, et al., 2003). Research on these diferent community
speciically in the lower ninth ward, in Lakeview and traits may show diferential recovery trajectories for
Gentilly, adjacent to Lake Pontchartrain (Brunkard the same community. his heterogeneity of recovery
et al., 2008). he multidimensional impact of Hurricane path is likely to occur across the multilevel conceptu-
Katrina crossed state, city, and neighborhood bound- alizations of resilience. For example, recovery of live-
aries. Researchers interested in community resilience lihood for individual business owners may not relect
following disasters are challenged to deine their unit their mental health recovery (de Mel et al., 2008). It is
of analysis in a meaningful way. possible that resilience of some types of functionality

208
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

Figure 14.4 Hypothetical extreme


A. 50% dead, flood disaster in Wayne County, MI.
displaced,
migrated
Disaster
Community I Community II

B. 50% survive and Adaptation


return to the and growth
community

may by asynchronous with, and may even be in con- Flood Return to pre-flood level

K-12 schools/1000
trast to, recovery in other areas.

enrolled students
0.75
Resilience for whom?
Further compounding these challenges, a return to a
community-wide baseline may actually belie the true Recovery
heterogeneity of disaster impact on subgroups within
the same community. Because resilience relies on the Time
pre-existing resources of a community, the rebound of Figure 14.5 Educational system resilience (public schools, K-12)
that community may relect, or even worsen, pre-exist- following hypothetical flood disaster in Wayne County, MI.
ing disparities. Vale and Campanella (2005, pp. 12–13)
posed some very diicult questions on this subject:
What we call ‘recovery’ is also driven by value-laden his example uses a hypothetical number of oper-
questions about equity. Who sets the priorities for the ational K-12 public schools per 1000 enrolled students
recovering communities? How are the needs of low- in the county as a measure of system functioning
income residents valued in relation to the pressing claims (Figure 14.5), based on the Census American Factinder
of disrupted businesses? Who decides what will be and Wayne County Regional Educational Service
rebuilt where, and which voices carry forth the dominant Agency for construction of these data. From the sys-
narratives that interpret what transpires? Who gets dis- tem standpoint, it would appear that the community
placed when new facilities are constructed in the name has responded resiliently, with a return to the pre-lood
of recovery?
(baseline) level of education system functioning. When
Community resilience is fundamentally concerned broken down by school district (Figure 14.6), however,
with the persistence of the system. he processes that we see that the demography of diferent districts has
lead to system persistence, however, are not univer- actually changed. While Detroit and Dearborn display
sally aligned with resilience processes in individuals a return to baseline school-to-student ratio ater the
or within subgroups of communities. A simplistic, disaster, many of the families living below poverty sim-
hypothetical example uses an extreme lood disaster in ply never moved back to Detroit ater the disaster. he
Wayne County, MI (Figure 14.4). Ater the disaster, 50% number of students decreased by 40 000, and yet the
of pre-disaster residents have been killed, displaced, or community shows resilience. How should we describe
migrated; while 50% of residents survive the disaster this community’s response to disaster? Do those 40 000
and return to the community, adapt, and grow. To what students and their families enter our equation?
degree do researchers care about those who preserve hese questions are related to the more gen-
the system (B) more than those who are no longer con- eral question of how we wish to use the concept of
tributing to the system (A)? Does it matter what group resilience. If we want to use the term as a theoretical
A individuals are doing now (e.g., contributing to other objective – a term used to motivate leaders to make
communities, in temporary housing, dead)? To what their communities more equitable, diversify their eco-
degree were the two groups similar or diferent in the nomic base, and cultivate social networks and collect-
irst place? What might determine whether a given ive eicacy in the name of disaster preparedness – then
individual falls into group A or B? How do we describe its current deinition may suice. However, if we are
the resilience of this community? tempted to use it in an evaluative way to assess how

209
Section 4: Specific challenges

Figure 14.6 Educational system resilience following hypothetical flood disaster in Wayne County, MI by school system. K, indicates 1000.

communities respond to disasters based on measured white, while those who moved were disproportionately
quantities and qualities of functioning and to inform black; likewise, the age distribution of residents who
future responses based on generalizable conclusions, stayed and moved were diferent, with non-movers
then the operationalization of this concept needs to being older. In other situations, disasters may cause
incorporate a distributional perspective as well as one mass resettlement of entire populations. For example,
focused on system wellness. ater the Manjil earthquake, residents of two villages,
Hurricane Katrina provides a well-documented Balklor and Jamal-Abad, were relocated and integrated
example of the devastation disaster causes and the into a larger village, Ab-Bar, causing dramatic and com-
mass population shit that occurs as a result. Ater its plex changes to all three groups (Badri et al., 2006).
irst landfall in Louisiana, Hurricane Katrina moved Just as individual outcomes ater disasters are
northeast, making its second landfall in Hancock inequitably distributed, this pattern is replicated on a
County, Mississippi, destroying more than a third of higher level of analysis: “here is never a single, mono-
homes in the county (Centers for Disease Control and lithic vox populi that uniformly airms the adopted
Prevention, 2006). he US Census shows that New resilience narrative in the wake of disaster… marginal-
Orleans residents who moved ater Hurricane Katrina ized groups or peoples are generally ignored in the nar-
were very diferent from those who stayed (Figures 14.7 rative construction process” (Vale & Campanella, 2005,
and 14.8; Koerber, 2006). Koerber shows that those p. 341). Just as subcommunities do not have equitably
who stayed in New Orleans were disproportionately distributed pre-disaster resources, they do not equitably

210
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

100 Figure 14.7 Racial makeup


of residents in the New Orleans
19.8 metropolitan statistical area (MSA)
29.1
80 35.7 before Hurricane Katrina (January–
Racial makeup (%)

August 2005) and after (September–


56.7 December 2005). FEMA area, area
62.3
60 covered by the Federal Emergency
Management Agency. (Based on data
in Koerber, 2006.)
40 68.8
58.6
54.6

20 35.7
27.8

0
January– Non-movers Movers within Movers to Movers to
August 2005 New remainder remainder
Orleans MSA FEMA area USA
September–December 2005

White alone, not Hispanic Black or African–American alone, not Hispanic

Age Distribution of New Orleans MSA Residents Figure 14.8 Age distribution
of residents in the New Orleans
100 metropolitan statistical area (MSA) before
12.6 9.8 8.7 8.2
90 15.3 Hurricane Katrina (January–August 2005)
80 21.7 18.7 and after (September–December 2005).
23.2
27.2 FEMA area, area covered by the Federal
Age distribution (%)

70 33.0 Emergency Management Agency. (Based


60 30.7 on data in Koerber, 2006.)
28.3 28.5
50
28.0
40 23.9
30 19.3 18.9
18.2
13.4
20 10.2

10 18.9 20.4 21.8 23.5


17.6
0
January–August Non-movers Movers within Movers to Movers to
2005 New remainder of remainder of
Oreleans MSA FEMA area of the USA
September–December 2005

1–14 years 15–24 years 25–44 years 45–64 years 65 years and over

share disaster impact or resilience processes ater dis- Defining value-neutral resilience and failure
aster. For example, researchers found that a school’s It has been suggested that disasters are a catalyst for
probability of being looded one week ater Hurricane positive social change (Paton, 2006). Others assert that
Floyd depended on race and income level of students – disasters are not usually accompanied by changes to
schools in low-income areas with a majority of black the pre-disaster establishment (Vale & Campanella,
students were twice as likely to be looded as non-low- 2005). Are there situations where resilience may actu-
income schools with a majority of non-black students ally worsen social problems or increase future vulner-
(Guidry & Margolis, 2005). In addition to the higher ability? Many researchers have denounced the utility of
risk of looding, schools in low-income areas will have a universally positive conceptualization of resilience:
fewer resources to deal with the health consequences of “Are cities resilient because their recovery has followed
looding, such as respiratory illness. some particular identiiable model, or are they resilient

211
Section 4: Specific challenges

only because interpreters choose to deine resilience in of disasters (Klein et al., 2003). We suggest, however,
such luid and malleable ways” (Vale & Campanella, that disasters expose pre-existing vulnerabilities and
2005, p. 335). For evaluation purposes, we must be capacities of individuals and communities that indeed
equipped with a value-neutral conceptualization of provide insight about positive trajectories following
resilience. disruption. he abandonment of disaster resilience as
Davis (2005) provides a compelling case study on an evaluative construct would result in a missed oppor-
the Mexico City 1985 earthquake: the bulk of damage tunity for the assessment of disaster preparedness and
was located in the city center, including death, injury, response. he challenge today is to deine resilience as
and damage to buildings; while disruption of power and a uniied concept that can be used to compare expe-
telecommunications extended to businesses outside riences across contexts, despite the heterogeneity of
the city. While survivors inside the city had emergency disasters. We propose three main ways forward that
shelter and medical needs, inancial and large-scale may address some of the challenges identiied here. We
manufacturers outside the city required phones and intend these to inform future work in the area and to
electricity to continue business. Davis argued that encourage discussion and debate about ways forward.
the city’s response prioritized economic and political First, the community level of analysis is both mean-
interests over eforts to provide emergency shelter and ingful and practical in resilience research. However,
medical needs and, later, housing reconstruction for both communities and disasters are geographically
displaced residents. “heir ‘resilience’ – expressed as an difuse. In addition, disaster exposure is more dimen-
insistence on continuing business as usual – may pre- sional than it is dichotomous. Disaster researchers are
vent them from acting in ways that most constructively challenged to deine the boundaries of communities
promote recovery and reconstruction” (Davis, 2005, in ways that relect identity and shared environment,
p. 275). Davis’ recognition of a resilient process with rather than convenience. Research that quantiies indi-
both positive and negative consequences for individ- viduals as either “afected” or “unafected” by disaster,
uals and subgroups is highly unusual in resilience lit- usually based on some threshold level of exposure, may
erature. his approach should be encouraged in future contradict the true gradient nature of disasters on pop-
research. ulations. Research in this area should aim to identify a
Lastly, we suggest that multilevel resilience must be simple way to assess disaster impact on individuals and
deined in a way that allows for failure. he technical communities across multiple dimensions.
homologue of resilience failure would mean a material Second, aggregate measures of system performance
that breaks or deforms under strain. Examples of eco- (e.g., return of water or power) are truly useful qual-
logical resilience failure include the extinction of a spe- ities for assessing community functioning. A return
cies, a reduction in the diversity of a ecological system, to aggregate baseline functioning ater the disruption
or decreased stability of the system. A psychological of disaster may be an appropriate measure for some
homologue of resilience failure would be persistent dis- dimensions of system resilience. However, aggregate
ruption in quality of life or ability to function. A failure values oten belie the diferential impact of disasters
of community resilience would connote a negative tra- on subgroups within the community. he concept of
jectory of community functioning and adaptation or community resilience must be sensitive to the het-
increased social vulnerability ater disaster. If we want erogeneity of disaster impacts, resilience processes,
to evaluate the degree to which communities respond and outcomes among subgroups within a community
with resilience to disaster, we must identify levels of while maintaining the assessment of shared, systemic
quantiiable characteristics that describe both a resili- functioning.
ent and a non-resilient response in meaningful ways. hird, the common deinition of resilience as a
“return to normalcy” is appealing. However, a uni-
versally positive conceptualization of resilience is
Reorienting resilience misleading. Where system resources ater disaster are
his chapter has identiied some of the major chal- meager, diferent dimensions of system resilience may
lenges in the application of resilience to disaster set- work in competition with one another. Where one
tings, particularly for the concept of community dimension of the community functioning (e.g., eco-
resilience. hese challenges have led some to question nomic) is prioritized over other important elements
the usefulness of the concept of resilience in context (e.g., temporary housing), demonstrated resilience in

212
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

that prioritized dimension may belie an increase in Banauch, G. I., Hall, C., Weiden, M., et al. (2006).
overall system vulnerability. he concept of resilience, Pulmonary function ater exposure to the World Trade
then, must be both multidimensional and value neu- Center collapse in the New York City ire department.
American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care
tral. In addition, resilience must be deined in a way
Medicine, 174, 312–319.
that allows for failure. If we wish to use resilience as
a way to evaluate disaster response, disaster planners Bland, S. H., Farinaro, E., Krogh, V., et al. (2000). Long
term relations between earthquake experiences and
and researchers should negotiate concrete, measurable coronary heart disease risk factors. American Journal of
outcomes at each level of a system that indicate suc- Epidemiology, 151, 1086–1090.
cessful resilience and deine what level of system per-
Bodin, P. & Wiman, B. L. B. (2004). Resilience and other
formance indicates failed resilience. stability concepts in ecology: Notes on their origin,
validity, and usefulness. ESS Bulletin, 2, 33–43.
Conclusions Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience:
his chapter has outlined the challenges that disas- Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive
ters pose to the resilience of individuals, families, and ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist,
communities and discussed the ways in which disas- 59, 20–28.
ters may transform the way researchers and theorists Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D.
think about resilience. hree main ways forward were (2007). What predicts psychological resilience ater
disaster? he role of demographics, resources, and life
outlined in the hope of inspiring new discourse on this
stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75,
burgeoning topic. Resilience is both theoretically com- 671–682.
pelling and useful in the ield of disaster planning and
Boscarino, J. A., Adams, R. E., & Figley, C. R. (2006).
research. However, much development is needed to
Worker productivity and outpatient service use ater the
truly realize the potential of resilience as an evaluative September 11th attacks: Results from the New York City
tool in these ields. terrorism outcome study. American Journal of Industrial
Medicine, 49, 670–682.
References Bradt, D. A. ( 2003). Site management of health issues in the
2001 World Trade Center disaster. Academic Emergency
Abramson, D. M., Morse, S. S., Garrett, A. L., & Redlener, I.
Medicine, 10, 650–660.
(2007). Public health disaster research: Surveying the
ield, deining its future. Disaster Medicine and Public Bromet, E. J. & Havenaar, J. M. (2007). Psychological and
Health Preparedness, 1, 57–62. perceived health efects of the Chernobyl disaster: A
20-year review. Health Physics, 93, 516–521.
Alexakhin, R. M., Sanzharova, N. I., Fesenko, S. V.,
Spiridonov, S. I., & Panov, A. V. (2007). Chernobyl Bruneau, M., Chang, S. E., Eguchi, R. T., et al. (2003). A
radionuclide distribution, migration, and framework to quantitatively assess and enhance the
environmental and agricultural impacts. Health Physics, seismic resilience of communities. Earthquake Spectra,
93, 418–426. 19, 733–752.
Armenian, H. K., Morikawa, M., Melkonian, A. K., et al. Brunkard, J., Namulanda, G., & Ratard, R. (2008).
(2000). Loss as a determinant of PTSD in a cohort of Hurricane Katrina deaths, Louisiana, 2005. Disaster
adult survivors of the 1988 earthquake in Armenia: Medicine and Public Health Preparedness, 2, 215–223.
Implications for policy. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Butler, L. D., Morland, L. A., & Leskin, G. A. (2007).
102, 58–64. Psychological resilience in the face of terrorism. In
Armenian, H. K., Morikawa, M., Melkonian, A. K., et al. B. Bongar, L. Brown, L. Beutler, J. Breckenridge &
(2002). Risk factors for depression in the survivors P. Zimbardo (eds.), Psychology of terrorism (pp. 400–417).
of the 1988 earthquake in Armenia. Journal of Urban New York: Oxford University Press.
Health: Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine, 79, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2006). Rapid
373–382. community needs assessment ater Hurricane Katrina:
Ayyala, R. & Sacks, J. G. (2008). Tulane ater Katrina. Hancock county, Mississippi, September 14 – 15, 2005.
Ophthalmology, 115, 922–923. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 55, 234–236.
Badri, S. A., Asgary, A., Etekhari, A. R., & Levy, J. (2006). Chang, S. E. & Nojima, N. (2001). Measuring post-disaster
Post-disaster resettlement, development and change: transportation system performance: he 1995 Kobe
A case study of the 1990 Manjil earthquake in Iran. earthquake in comparative perspective. Transportation
Disasters, 30, 451–468. Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 35, 475–494.

213
Section 4: Specific challenges

Chaskin, R. J. (1997). Perspectives on neighborhood and Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 157,
community: A review of the literature. Social Science 572–578.
Review, 71, 521–547. Galea, S., Nandi, A., & Vlahov, D. (2005). he epidemiology
Chemtob, C. M., Nomura, Y., & Abramovitz, R. A. (2008). of post-traumatic stress disorder ater disasters.
Impact of conjoined exposure to the World Trade Center Epidemiologic Reviews, 27, 78–91.
attacks and to other traumatic events on the behavioral Galea, S., Tracy, M., Norris, F., & Cofey, S. F. (2008).
problems of preschool children. Archives of Pediatrics and Financial and social circumstances and the incidence
Adolescent Medicine, 162, 126–133. and course of PTSD in Mississippi during the irst two
Chochinov, H. M. (2005). Vicarious grief and response to years ater Hurricane Katrina. Journal of Traumatic Stress,
global disasters. Lancet, 366, 697–698. 21, 357–368.
Chou, F. H., Wu, H. C., Chou, P., et al. (2007). Epidemiologic Ganzel, B. L., Kim, P., Glover, G. H., & Temple, E. (2008).
psychiatric studies on post-disaster impact among Chi- Resilience ater 9/11: Multimodal neuroimaging
Chi earthquake survivors in Yu-Chi, Taiwan. Psychiatry evidence for stress-related change in the healthy adult
and Clinical Neurosciences, 61, 370–378. brain. Neuroimage, 40, 788–795.
Chou, Y. J., Huang, N., Lee, C. H., et al. (2003). Suicides ater Ghafari-Nejad, A., Ahmadi-Mousavi, M., Gandomkar, M.,
the 1999 Taiwan earthquake. International Journal of & Reihani-Kermani, H. (2007). he prevalence of
Epidemiology, 32, 1007–1014. complicated grief among Bam earthquake survivors in
Chou, Y. J., Huang, N., Lee, C. H., et al. (2004). Who is at Iran. Archives of Iranian Medicine, 10, 525–528.
risk of death in an earthquake? American Journal of Gold, L. S., Kane, L. B., Sotoodehnia, N., & Rea, T. (2007).
Epidemiology, 160, 688–695. Disaster events and the risk of sudden cardiac death: A
Davis, D. E. (2005). Reverberations: Mexico city’s 1985 Washington state investigation. Prehospital and Disaster
earthquake and the transformation of the capital. In Medicine, 22, 313–317.
L. J. Vale & T. J. Campanella (eds.), he resilient city: How Gordon, J. (1978). Structures. Harmondsworth, UK:
modern cities recover from disaster (pp. 255–280). New Penguin Books.
York: Oxford University Press.
Grievink, L., van der Velden, P. G., Stellato, R. K., et al.
de Mel, S., McKenzie, D., & Woodruf, C. (2008). Mental (2007). A longitudinal comparative study of the physical
health recovery and economic recovery ater the and mental health problems of afected residents of the
tsunami: High-frequency longitudinal evidence from irework disaster Enschede, the Netherlands. Public
Sri Lankan small business owners. Social Science and Health, 121, 367–374.
Medicine, 66, 582–595.
Guidry, V. T. & Margolis, L. H. (2005). Unequal respiratory
De Silva, D. A. & Yamao, M. (2007). Efects of the tsunami health risk: Using GIS to explore hurricane-related
on isheries and coastal livelihood: A case study of looding of schools in eastern North Carolina.
tsunami-ravaged southern Sri Lanka. Disasters, 31, Environmental Research, 98, 383–389.
386–404.
Gul, S., Ghafar, H., Mirza, S., et al. (2008). Multitasking
DiMaggio, C. & Galea, S. (2006). he behavioral a telemedicine training unit in earthquake disaster
consequences of terrorism: A meta-analysis. Academic response: Paraplegic rehabilitation assessment.
Emergency Medicine, 13, 559–566. Telemedicine Journal and e-Health, 14, 280–283.
Dirkzwager, A. J., Kerssens, J. J., & Yzermans, C. J. (2006). Helmreich, D. L., Watkins, L. R., Deak, T., et al. (1999).
Health problems in children and adolescents before he efect of stressor controllability on stress-
and ater a man-made disaster. Journal of the American induced neuropeptide mRNA expression within the
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 94–103. paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. Journal of
Dugan, B. (2007). Loss of identity in disaster: How do you Neuroendocrinology, 11, 121–128.
say goodbye to home? Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 43, Holling, C. S. (1973). Resilience and stability of ecological
41–46. systems. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 4, 1–23.
Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1993). Resilience as Hoven, C. W., Duarte, C. S., & Mandell, D. J. (2003).
process. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 517–528. Children’s mental health ater disasters: he impact of the
Feldman, D. M., Baron, S. L., Bernard, B. P., et al. (2004). World Trade Center attack. Current Psychiatry Reports,
Symptoms, respirator use, and pulmonary function 5, 101–107.
changes among New York City ireighters responding to Hoyois, P., Below, R., Scheuren, J., & Guha-Sapir, D. (2007).
the World Trade Center disaster. Chest, 125, 1256–1264. Annual disaster statistical review: Numbers and trends
Ford, C. A., Udry, J. R., Gleiter, K., & Chantala, K. (2003). 2006. Brussels: Centre for Research on the Epidemiology
Reactions of young adults to September 11, 2001. of Disasters.

214
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

Huizink, A. C., Slottje, P., Witteveen, A. B., et al. Kunii, O., Kanagawa, S., Yajima, I., et al. (2002). he 1997
(2006). Long term health complaints following the haze disaster in Indonesia: Its air quality and health
Amsterdam air disaster in police oicers and ire- efects. Archives of Environmental Health, 57, 16–22.
ighters. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 63, Leor, J., Poole, W. K., & Kloner, R. A. (1996). Sudden cardiac
657–662. death triggered by an earthquake. New England Journal of
Huizink, A. C., Bartels, M., Rose, R. J., et al. (2008). Medicine, 334, 413–419.
Chernobyl exposure as stressor during pregnancy
Lioy, P. J., Weisel, C. P., Millette, J. R., et al. (2002).
and hormone levels in adolescent ofspring. Journal of
Characterization of the dust/smoke aerosol that
Epidemiology and Community Health, 62, e5.
settled east of the World Trade Center (WTC) in lower
James, J. J., Subbarao, I., & Lanier, W. L. (2008). Improving Manhattan ater the collapse of the WTC 11 September
the art and science of disaster medicine and public 2001. Environmental Health Perspectives, 110, 703–714.
health preparedness. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 83,
Llewellyn, M. (2006). Floods and tsunamis. Surgical Clinics
559–562.
of North America, 86, 557–578.
Jayatissa, R., Bekele, A., Piyasena, C. L., & Mahamithawa, S.
Loganovsky, K., Havenaar, J. M., Tintle, N. L., et al. (2008).
(2006). Assessment of nutritional status of children
he mental health of clean-up workers 18 years ater
under ive years of age, pregnant women, and lactating
the Chernobyl accident. Psychological Medicine, 38,
women living in relief camps ater the tsunami in Sri
481–488.
Lanka. Food and Nutrition Bulletin, 27, 144–152.
Jordan, N. N., Hoge, C. W., Tobler, S. K., et al. (2004). Mental Madamala, K., Campbell, C. R., Hsu, E. B., Hsieh, Y. H., &
health impact of 9/11 Pentagon attack: Validation of a James, J. (2007). Characteristics of physician relocation
rapid assessment tool. American Journal of Preventive following Hurricane Katrina. Disaster Medicine and
Medicine, 26, 284–293. Public Health Preparedness, 1, 21–26.
Kendra, J. M. & Wachtendorf, T. (2003). Elements of Meharga, A. A., Osborna, D., Pain, D. J., Sanchez, A., &
resilience ater the World Trade Center disaster: Naveso, M. A. (1999). Contamination of Donana food-
Reconstituting New York City’s emergency operations chains ater the Aznalcollar mine disaster. Environmental
centre. Disasters, 27, 37–53. Pollution, 105, 387–390.
Kett, M. (2005). Displaced populations and long term Metts, T. A. (2008). Addressing environmental health
humanitarian assistance. British Medical Journal, 331, implications of mold exposure ater major looding.
98–100. American Association of Occupational Health Nurses
Journal, 56, 115–120; quiz 121–122.
Kilpatrick, D. G., Koenen, K. C., Ruggiero, K. J., et al. (2007).
he serotonin transporter genotype and social support Michaud, J. P., Grove, J. S., & Krupitsky, D. (2004). Emergency
and moderation of posttraumatic stress disorder and department visits and “vog”-related air quality in Hilo,
depression in hurricane-exposed adults. American Hawai’i. Environmental Research, 95, 11–19.
Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1693–1699. Mileti, D. (1999). Disasters by design: A reassessment of
Kimhi, S. & Shamai, M. (2004). Community resilience and natural hazards in the United States. Washington, DC:
the impact of stress: Adult response to Israel’s withdrawal Joseph Henry Press.
from Lebanon. Journal of Community Psychology, 32, Mitchell, W. J. & Townsend, A. M. (2005). Cyborg agonistes:
349–451. Disaster and reconstruction in the digital electronic era.
Klein, R. J. T., Nicholls, R. J., & homalla, F. (2003). In W. J. Mitchell & A. M. Townsend (eds.), he resilient
Resilience to natural hazards: How useful is this concept? city: How modern cities recover from disaster (pp. 313–
Environmental Hazards, 5, 35–45. 335). New York: Oxford University Press.
Knabb, R. D., Rhome, J. R., & Brown, D. P. (2005). Tropical Moore, D., Copes, R., Fisk, R., et al. (2006). Population
cyclone report: Hurricane Katrina. Miami, FL: National health efects of air quality changes due to forest ires in
Hurricane Center, http://www.nhc.noaa.gov/pdf/TCR- British Columbia in 2003: Estimates from physician-
AL122005_Katrina.pdf (accessed February 10, 2011). visit billing data. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 97,
Koerber, K. (2006). Migration patterns and mover 105–108.
characteristics from the 2005 ACS gulf coast area special Nandi, A., Galea, S., Tracy, M., et al. (2004). Job loss,
products: Detailed tables. Washington, DC: Housing and unemployment, work stress, job satisfaction, and the
Economic Household Statistics Division, US Census persistence of posttraumatic stress disorder one year
Bureau. ater the September 11 attacks. Journal of Occupational
Kondo, H., Seo, N., Yasuda, T., et al. (2002). Post-lood: and Environmental Medicine, 46, 1057–1064.
infectious diseases in Mozambique. Prehospital and Nishikiori, N., Abe, T., Costa, D. G., et al. (2006). Who
Disaster Medicine, 17, 126–133. died as a result of the tsunami? Risk factors of mortality

215
Section 4: Specific challenges

among internally displaced persons in Sri Lanka: A International Medical Society of Paraplegia, 45, 658–663.
retrospective cohort analysis. BMC Public Health, 6, 73. doi:10.1038/sj.sc.3102023
Nishiwaki, Y., Maekawa, K., Ogawa, Y., et al. (2001). Efects Reidpath, D. D. (2005). Population health. More than
of Sarin on the nervous system in rescue team staf the sum of the parts? Journal of Epidemiology and
members and police oicers 3 years ater the Tokyo Community Health, 59, 877–880.
subway Sarin attack. Environmental Health Perspectives,
Reijneveld, S. A., Crone, M. R., Verhulst, F. C., & Verloove-
109, 1169–1173.
Vanhorick, S. P. (2003). he efect of a severe disaster
Norris, F. H., Friedman, M. J., Watson, P. J., et al. (2002). on the mental health of adolescents: A controlled study.
60 000 disaster victims speak: Part I. An empirical review Lancet, 362, 691–696.
of the empirical literature, 1981–2001. Psychiatry, 65,
Risk Management Solutions (2005). RMS combines real-
207–239.
time reconnaissance with risk models to estimate Katrina
Norris, F. H., Baker, C. K., Murphy, A. D., & Kaniasty, K. losses. Newark, CA: Risk Management Solutions,
(2005). Social support mobilization and deterioration www.rms.com/newspress/pr_091905_hukatrina_
ater Mexico’s 1999 lood: Efects of context, gender, and lossmethodology.asp (accessed February 10, 2011).
time. American Journal of Community Psychology, 36,
Rose, A. (2006). Economic resilience to disasters: Toward a
15–28.
consistent and comprehensive formulation. In D. Paton,
Norris, F. H., Stevens, S. P., Pfeferbaum, B., Wyche, K. F., & D. Johnston (eds.), Disaster resilience: An integrated
& Pfeferbaum, R. L. (2008). Community resilience as approach (pp. 226–248). Springield, IL: Charles C.
a metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for homas.
disaster readiness. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 41, 127–150. Roseboom, T. J., van der Meulen, J. H., Ravelli, A. C., et al.
(2001). Efects of prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine
Oliver-Smith, A. (1991). Successes and failures in post- on adult disease in later life: An overview. Molecular and
disaster resettlement. Disasters, 15, 12–23. Cellular Endocrinology, 185, 93–98.
O’Neill, H. K., Evans, B. A., Bussman, M. D., & Strandberg, Rutter, M. (1993). Resilience: Some conceptual
D. K. (1999). Psychological distress during the red river considerations. Journal of Adolescent Health, 14, 626–
lood: Predictive utility of the conservation of resources 631, 690–696.
model. Applied Behavioral Science Review, 7, 159–169.
Salcoglu, E., Basoglu, M., & Livanou, M. (2008).
Paton, D. (2006). Disaster resilience: Building capacity to Psychosocial determinants of relocation in survivors of
co-exist with natural hazards and their consequences. the 1999 earthquake in Turkey. Journal of Nervous and
In D. Paton & D. Johnston (eds.), Disaster resilience: An Mental Disease, 196, 55–61.
integrated approach (pp. 3–10). Springield, IL: Charles
C. homas. Scheuren, J., le Polain de Waroux, O., Below, R.,
Guha-Sapir, D., & Ponserre, S. (2008). Annual disaster
Peleg, K., Reuveni, H., & Stein, M. (2002). Earthquake statistical review: he numbers and trends 2007. Melin,
disasters: Lessons to be learned. Israel Medical Belgium: Jacofset.
Association Journal, 4, 361–365.
Schultz, C. H., Koenig, K. L., & Lewis, R. J. (2007). Decision-
Perez-Sales, P., Cervellon, P., Vazquez, C., Vidales, D., & making in hospital earthquake evacuation: Does
Gaborit, M. (2005). Post-traumatic factors and resilience: distance from the epicenter matter? Annals of Emergency
he role of shelter management and survivors’ attitudes Medicine, 50, 320–326.
ater the earthquakes in El Salvador (2001). Journal of
Community and Applied Social Psychology, 15, 368–382. Shaw, R. & Goda, K. (2004). From disaster to sustainable
civil society: he Kobe experience. Disasters, 28, 16–40.
Rahu, M., Tekkel, M., Veidebaum, T., et al. (1997). he
Estonian study of Chernobyl cleanup workers: II. Shehab, N., Anastario, M. P., & Lawry, L. (2008). Access to
incidence of cancer and mortality. Radiation Research, care among displaced Mississippi residents in FEMA
147, 653–657. travel trailer parks two years ater Katrina. Health Afairs,
27, w416–w429.
Rajkumar, A. P., Premkumar, T. S., & haryan, P. (2008).
Coping with the Asian tsunami: Perspectives from Tamil Shimizu, Y., Dobashi, K., Hisada, T., et al. (2007). Acute
Nadu, India on the determinants of resilience in the face impact of volcanic ash on asthma symptoms and
of adversity. Social Science and Medicine, 67, 844–853. treatment. International Journal of Immunopathology and
Pharmacology, 20(Suppl. 2), 9–14.
Rathore, M. F., Rashid, P., Butt, A. W., et al. (2007).
Epidemiology of spinal cord injuries in the 2005 Pakistan Silver, R. C., Holman, E. A., McIntosh, D. N., Poulin, M., &
earthquake. Spinal Cord: he Oicial Journal of the Gil-Rivas, V. (2002). Nationwide longitudinal study of

216
Chapter 14: Reorienting resilience after disaster

psychological responses to September 11. Journal of the homas, C. R. (2006). Psychiatric sequelae of disasters.
American Medical Association, 288, 1235–1244. Journal of Burn Care and Research, 27, 600–605.
Slottje, P., Twisk, J. W., Smidt, N., et al. (2007). Health- Trevisan, M., Jossa, F., Farinaro, E., et al. (1992). Earthquake
related quality of life of ireighters and police oicers 8.5 and coronary heart disease risk factors: A longitudinal
years ater the air disaster in Amsterdam. Quality of Life study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 135, 632–637.
Research, 16, 239–252. Trevisan, M., O’ Leary, E., Farinaro, E., et al. (1997). Short-
Smolka, A. (2006). Natural disasters and the challenge of and long-term association between uric acid and a
extreme events: Risk management from an insurance natural disaster. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 109–113.
perspective. Philosophical Transactions. Series A, Tucker, P. M., Pfeferbaum, B., North, C. S., et al. (2007).
Mathematical, Physical, and Engineering Sciences, 364, Physiologic reactivity despite emotional resilience
2147–2165. several years ater direct exposure to terrorism. American
Soeteman, R. J., Yzermans, C. J., Kerssens, J. J., et al. (2007). Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 230–235.
Health problems presented to family practices in the United Nations Oice for the Coordination of
Netherlands 1 year before and 1 year ater a disaster. Humanitarian Afairs (2005). Indian Ocean earthquake-
Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine, 20, tsunami 2005, lash appeal. New York: United Nations.
548–556.
Vale, L. J. & Campanella, T. J. (2005). he resilient city: How
Tang, C. S. (2007). Trajectory of traumatic stress symptoms modern cities recover from disaster. New York: Oxford
in the atermath of extreme natural disaster: A study University Press.
of adult hai survivors of the 2004 southeast Asian
earthquake and tsunami. Journal of Nervous and Mental Walsh, F. (2007). Traumatic loss and major disasters:
Disease, 195, 54–59. Strengthening family and community resilience. Family
Process, 46, 207–227.
Tao, L., Kannan, K., Aldous, K. M., Mauer, M. P., &
Eadon, G. A. (2008). Biomonitoring of Wang, X., Gao, L., Zhang, H., et al. (2000). Post-earthquake
perluorochemicals in plasma of New York state quality of life and psychological well-being: Longitudinal
personnel responding to the World Trade Center disaster. evaluation in a rural community sample in northern
Environmental Science and Technology, 42, 3472–3478. China. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 54,
427–433.
Tauqir, S. F., Mirza, S., Gul, S., Ghafar, H., & Zafar, A.
(2007). Complications in patients with spinal cord Wayment, H. A. (2004). It could have been me: Vicarious
injuries sustained in an earthquake in northern Pakistan. victims and disaster-focused distress. Personality and
Journal of Spinal Cord Medicine, 30, 373–377. Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 515–528.

217
Section
Section4 Specific challenges
Chapter
Rape and other sexual assault

15 Heidi S. Resnick, Constance Guille, Jenna L. McCauley, and


Dean G. Kilpatrick

Introduction physical health compared with non-victims (Steketee


& Foa, 1987; Resick, 1993; Resnick et al., 1993, 1997;
his chapter describes the prevalence of rape and the
Kessler et al., 1995; Kilpatrick et al., 1997, 2003). In par-
risk of associated mental health problems, including
ticular, indings from epidemiological studies indicate
deinition of key terms such as rape. he particular
that rape or completed sexual assault, compared with
focus of the chapter is on post-traumatic stress disorder
other traumatic events, is associated with greatest risk
(PTSD) as a primary mental health problem associ-
of PTSD (Kilpatrick et al., 1989; Norris, 1992; Resnick
ated with rape. Major methodological approaches to
et al., 1993; Kessler et al., 1995). While estimates vary,
the study of rape, related risk, resilience, and associ-
approximately half of rape victims develop PTSD
ated mental health problems are described, and advan-
(Breslau et al., 1998); so not all women who experi-
tages and disadvantages and implications of the major
ence rape develop emotional problems, but, given
approaches are discussed. Findings from the litera-
the high prevalence of rape and the substantial cost
ture related to risk and protective factors for PTSD are
associated with this adverse event, it is not surprising
reviewed in general, as well as what is known speciic-
that researchers have focused on predictors of PTSD
ally about risk and protective factors for PTSD given
among rape victims. A signiicant consequence of this
exposure to rape. he discussion is concluded by con-
focus is that little is understood about the characteris-
sidering the implications for future research related to
tics of those who appear to endure rape with minimal
etiology and/or intervention in relation to the study of
adverse sequelae. A better understanding of the human
resilience.
capacity to maintain healthy, symptom-free function-
ing or resilience in the face of signiicant adversity is of
Prevalence of rape great importance to the study of PTSD. It is useful to
Rape is a common problem in many countries, includ- identify protective factors as well as risk factors for psy-
ing America. Carefully conducted epidemiological chopathology, both to understand resilience in the face
studies estimate the proportion of women who have of known risk factors and to identify potentially modi-
been raped is between 12.6% and 16.1% in the USA iable risk factors that might be targeted in interven-
(Kilpatrick et al., 1992, 2007a; Tjaden & hoennes, tions promoting resilience (e.g., providing secondary
2000). Based on the most recent estimates and the 2005 prevention once a woman has been sexually assaulted).
US Census data, an estimated 20.2 million women have For example, knowing characteristics of rape that ele-
been raped, with an estimated 1.1 million women who vate risk may help to identify those who are resilient
have been raped within a given year (Kilpatrick et al., even under high-risk conditions and to identify indi-
2007b). Furthermore, the incidence of rape is likely to vidual, system response, or social response factors that
be increasing, with recent lifetime prevalence estimates produce more positive outcomes under those condi-
of rape being higher than those obtained in the early tions. Consequently, the major objective of this chap-
1990s. here is substantial evidence to support signii- ter is to review what is known about risk and protective
cant untoward consequences of rape. Women who are factors for PTSD ater exposure to rape, with particu-
victims of rape are at a signiicantly increased risk for lar emphasis on factors related to resilience. Prior to
mental health problems, substance abuse, and poor this review, important terms will be deined and key

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

218
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

methodological issues identiied that should be con- Rape acknowledgement. Not all women who have
sidered in a review of rape and resilience and how well experienced an event that meets the legal
extant research is capable of providing information deinition of rape deine what happened as rape,
on resilience. Our review will draw on what existing and, therefore, they do not think of themselves as
reviews of the literature tell us about risk and pro- rape victims.
tective factors for PTSD in general, as well as what we Unacknowledged rape. An event that has the legal
know about risk and protective factors for PTSD given elements of rape but that is not perceived by the
exposure to rape speciically. victim to have been a rape. Unacknowledged
rape victims are victims of rape who do not
Definition of terms acknowledge that what happened to them was a
rape.
Rape
Rape is a form of sexual assault that is deined as a Resilience
felony crime in the USA and many other nations.
As described by Bonanno (2004), resilience is deined
he crime of rape has several major elements. First,
as the absence of an emotional disorder following
rape involves some type of sexual penetration of the
exposure to a potentially traumatic event. In the con-
victim’s vagina, mouth, or anus. Second, the sexual
text of this chapter, resilience is deined as either failure
activity must be unwanted by the victim. hird, the
to develop PTSD ater a rape occurs or rapid recovery
tactics used by the perpetrator must involve (1) use
from PTSD if PTSD does develop ater a rape occurs.
of force or threat of force or other harm to the vic-
Although this deinition of resilience will be used, it
tim, (2) using alcohol or drugs to render a victim too
is important to note that true resilience is much more
incapacitated to control his or her behavior or protect
than the absence of psychopathology (e.g., post-trau-
themself, or (3) taking advantage of a victim who is
matic growth). However, there is so little research on
incapacitated or unconscious and who, therefore, is
these other measures of resilience that this is the dein-
unable to protect themself. Although rape victims can
ition that will be used.
be male or female, in this chapter they will be referred
to as female.
Forcible rape. he tactic used by the perpetrator is
Methodological issues
force or threat of force or other harm to the victim
or someone else (e.g., the perpetrator threatens
Characteristics of an ideal study of
to hurt the victim’s children if the victim does not post-rape resilience
cooperate). Ideal studies do not exist, but if they did, here is what
Drug- or alcohol-facilitated rape. he tactic used by the they would look like. First, the sample of rape victim
perpetrator is to deliberately give the victim drugs studies would be representative of the population of
or alcohol without her permission in an attempt rape victims of interest. his would ensure that both
to get her high or drunk and then to commit an the victims and the rapes they experienced are repre-
unwanted sexual act. sentative of all rape victims and all rape cases. Second,
Incapacitated rape. he victim voluntarily uses there would be careful measurement of all pre-rape,
alcohol or drugs and is passed out, or awake rape, and post-rape variables that are thought to be
but too drunk or high to consent or control her risk or protective factors for PTSD or related problems.
behavior. hird, PTSD and related problems would be measured
Statutory rape. he perpetrator has sexual penetration before the rape as well as a suicient number of times
with someone who is deined by law as too young aterwards to permit assessment of the trajectory of
to be capable of giving consent. PTSD and related problems before, immediately ater
Attempted rape. his is a type of sexual assault that the rape, and several time thereater. Fourth, an appro-
occurs when no sexual penetration as deined priate comparison group of non-victims of rape would
above occurs but an unsuccessful attempt by the be included and assessed to make it possible to estab-
perpetrator to commit a rape as deined above has lish the extent to which PTSD and other problems are
occurred. rape related (i.e., their prevalence is signiicantly higher

219
Section 4: Specific challenges

among rape victims than among non-victims). Fith, particularly because many such studies involve longi-
the longitudinal PTSD data would be used to identify tudinal assessments ater the rape. However, it has long
the trajectories of changes in PTSD status ater a rape to been known that the majority of women who are raped
determine victims who are resilient versus those who do not report the crime to police (e.g., Kilpatrick, 1983;
are not resilient. Kilpatrick et al., 1992) or seek services from rape crisis
A research study with these elements would be centers (Kilpatrick & Veronen, 1983). To the extent that
ideally suited to address post-rape resilience for several there are major diferences between samples including
reasons. First, because it would include a representative the types of victim (and their cases) participating in
sample of all rape victims as opposed to a biased sample these studies and the population of all rape victims and
that excluded some important types of victim, any ind- cases, such studies might be expected to yield biased
ings would be much more generalizable. Second, lon- information about resilience following rape.
gitudinal assessment of PTSD status before and ater
rape would permit trajectories of change to be tracked, Unacknowledged rape and how it can
thereby establishing which rape victims are resilient.
hird, comprehensive measurement of potential risk bias participation in rape research
and protective factors would permit the examination here is ample evidence that many victims of expe-
of a wide range of biopsychosocical risk and protective riences that meet the legal deinition of rape do not
factors related to resilience or lack of same. perceive what happened to them as a rape (Kilpatrick
Not surprisingly, no studies meet all these cri- et al., 2007b). hese unacknowledged rape victims are
teria. In general, extant studies fall into two groups. unlikely to report cases to police, to seek services from
he irst group of studies are epidemiological and rape crisis centers or other service agencies, or to par-
involve screening for history of rape among repre- ticipate in research projects that are recruiting for “rape
sentative, probability samples of female adolescents, victims,” because they do not think what happened to
young adults, or adult women living in households them was a rape and they do not think of themselves as
or colleges (Tjaden & hoennes, 2000; Fisher et al., rape victims. As noted, even if the rape is acknowledged
2003; Kilpatrick et al., 2007b). Such studies are gen- as such, the vast majority of rape victims do not report
erally retrospective, not longitudinal, and they rarely the assault to police or other authorities, particularly
involve collection of biological risk or protective fac- if drugs or alcohol were involved in the rape (Resnick
tor measures. A major strength of these studies is that et al. 2000). For example, in a recent study of a national
their samples of rape victims are much more likely to household probability sample of 3001 adult women and
be representative of the population of all rape victims a sample of 2000 women recruited nationwide from
from which they are drawn. An additional strength colleges, Kilpatrick and colleagues (2007b) found that
is that such studies are more likely to capture the full 18% of women in the general population sample and
range of changes in PTSD and other mental problems 11.5% of women in the national college student sample
that occur as a result of a rape because they include reported a lifetime history of rape including forcible,
victims who are having few problems as well as those incapacitated rape or drug- and alcohol-facilitated
who are having many problems. A inal strength of rape. Among those who experienced forcible rape,
these studies is that they are more likely to include only 18% of women in the general population sample
a representative distribution of risk and protective and 16% of the college women reported the incident to
factors. police or other authorities. An even smaller percentage
he second group of studies involves obtaining data of the general population sample (10%) and the college
from the subset of rape victims who report crimes to women sample (7%) reported rapes that occurred as a
police, seek services from rape crisis centers or other result of drug or alcohol facilitation or incapacitation.
victim assistance agencies, or who respond to advertise- hese data conirm the belief that most rape victims do
ments seeking rape victims to participate in research not report their cases to police.
on rape. he last method for recruiting victims yields If there are diferences in cases that are acknowl-
only a convenience sample, from which results can- edged as rapes or not, cases that are reported to police
not be generalized. Samples obtained from the subset or not reported, and among rape victims who do and do
of rape victims who report their crimes and/or who not participate in rape research, this could have a sub-
seek assistance can provide valuable information, stantial impact on the characteristics of the rape victims

220
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

and rape cases that have been included in various stud- approaching that found by Rothbaum et al. (1992) at
ies. Likewise, if some of the same variables that predict 12 weeks post-rape among those who had reported an
whether a rape case is acknowledged, or reported, or assault to police, hospital, or rape crisis personnel. Most
that result in victims seeking services, also increase importantly, these data demonstrate that rape victims
risk of PTSD and/or related disorders, then some of who report their cases to police were more likely to
what we think we know about risk and resilience ater have PTSD than those who do not report.
rape may have been obtained from biased samples of As reviewed by Fisher et al. (2003), demographic
rape victims that do not include the full range of rape characteristics such as older age at time of assault and
cases and rape victims. For example, Rothbaum et al. assault characteristics such as injury or other indica-
(1992) found that 94% of a sample of rape victims who tors of severity are risk factors for reporting to police,
reported to police, sought medical care, or rape crisis self-acknowledgement of rape (Layman et al., 1996;
center services met diagnostic criteria for PTSD at two Conoscenti & McNally, 2006), and seeking post-rape
weeks ater the assault while 50% continued to meet medical services (Resnick et al., 2000). Furthermore,
criteria three months later. In contrast, Resnick et al. as reported by Kilpatrick et al. (2007b), positive pre-
(1993) found that 32% of women who had ever been dictors of reporting of a most recent or only rape
raped in a national household probability sample of within the general population sample included ver-
US adult women met lifetime PTSD criteria. However, bal threat, injury, or fear of death or injury. herefore,
prevalence of lifetime PTSD was 45% among victims indings indicate that women who self-acknowledge
of either rape or physical assault when such incidents rape, report rape, or are seen in medical settings are
included both fear of death/injury or actual injury also more likely to have experienced the types of rape
(Resnick et al., 1993). Consequently, Resnick and col- that greatly increase the risk of PTSD. Speciically, both
leagues suggested that diferences in PTSD prevalence injury and fear of death or injury have consistently
across studies might relate to such diferences in assault been identiied as risk factors for PTSD within studies
or other characteristics that distinguish rape victims or that have evaluated outcomes associated with a range
rape incidents that are or are not reported to author- of traumatic events including rape (Kilpatrick et al.,
ities. Consistent with this notion regarding diferences 1989; Resnick et al., 1993). hese data demonstrate the
in PTSD or other outcomes among those reporting to biases inherent to convenience samples of rape victims
police or other authorities, acknowledgement of rape and underscore the need to study all victims of rape
has been found to be associated with greater symptoms among the general population.
of PTSD (Layman et al., 1996; Zinzow et al., 2010) and
physical health complaints (Conoscenti & McNally, Reviews of the extant literature:
2006).
To further investigate whether women who report
risk and protective factors for post-
rape to police or other authorities are more likely to also traumatic stress disorder in general
meet criteria for PTSD, we conducted analyses within Although much is known concerning risk factors for
the original National Women’s Study sample of 4008 PTSD, research examining protective factors is in its
women. Women who had experienced at least one rape relative infancy. However, important information can
incident as an adult were asked in reference to a irst or be distilled from understanding which factors increase
most recent rape assault whether they had reported the risk of psychopathology following a traumatic event.
incident to police or other authorities. Occurrence of One of the irst attempts to summarize factors that
PTSD was assessed with the National Women’s Study impact later development of PTSD was by Shalev and
PTSD module (Resnick et al., 1993) using DSM-IV colleagues (1996). Ater reviewing 38 studies, they
criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). identiied several risk factors for PTSD including pre-
Of 218 women who had experienced an adult rape, trauma vulnerability (e.g., psychiatric family history,
23.9% had reported one or more lifetime rape incident sex, genes, neuroendocrine factors, personality traits,
to police or other authorities. hose who reported an early trauma, negative parenting experiences, and
assault to police were signiicantly more likely to meet lower education), peritraumatic events (e.g., magni-
criteria for lifetime PTSD (44.2% and 28.9%, respect- tude of the trauma, preparation for the event, imme-
ively) (chi square = 4.23 [df 1, n =218]; p < 0.05). hese diate reactions to the event including dissociation
data are interesting and indicate a prevalence of PTSD and coping responses), and post-trauma factors (e.g.,

221
Section 4: Specific challenges

symptom trajectory, social support, other life stress). their meta-analysis on mainly two types of predictor:
he identiication of these risk factors is laudable and personal characteristics relevant for psychological pro-
served as a basis for subsequent explorations of indi- cessing of the trauma and outstanding features of the
vidual vulnerability factors that have attempted to traumatic event or its efects. Speciically, seven risk
overcome some of the limitations of these initial data, factors were explored, including prior trauma, prior
including generalizability, heterogeneity of studies, psychological adjustment, family history of psycho-
and inconsistency in replication of risk factors (Brewin pathology, perceived life threat during the trauma,
et al., 2000). post-trauma social support, peritraumatic emotional
Simultaneously, Brewin et al. (2000) and Ozer et al. responses, and peritraumatic dissociation. he results
(2003), conducted independent meta-analyses of risk revealed similar efect sizes among comparable predic-
factors for PTSD in trauma-exposed adults. he irst tors to those found by Brewin et al. (2000), and that,
meta-analysis (Brewin et al., 2000) included studies overall, peritraumatic psychological processes, not
of multiple types of trauma among military and civil- prior characteristics, were the strongest predictors of
ian populations, with combined sample sizes of 1149 PTSD. Consistent with the indings of Brewin et al.
to 11 000 subjects. In addition to identifying the abso- (2000), Ozer et al. (2003) also found that predictors var-
lute and relative efect sizes of 14 risk factors for PTSD, ied based on study sample and characteristics (i.e., type
including multiple pre-trauma variables (age, gender, of traumatic event, time elapsed since the event, type of
socioeconomic status, education, race, family psychi- sample, and method of PTSD assessment). While each
atric history, intelligence, childhood adversity, and pre- of these variables moderated at least two of the predic-
vious trauma), peritrauma and post-trauma variables tors, the most consistent and salient variable was type
(trauma severity, social support and life stress), they of event. Interpersonal violence, classiied as civilian
also examined the moderating efects of various sample assault, rape, and domestic violence, compared with
and study characteristics. Overall Brewin et al. (2000) violence that occurred in the military, increased the
found that, when both military and civilian popula- efect sizes of almost all the predictors, including his-
tions were combined, the risk of PTSD was enhanced tory of prior trauma, prior psychological adjustment,
to a small extent by pre-trauma characteristics (efect family history of psychopathology, and perceived life
sizes ranging from 0.5 to 0.19) and to a somewhat lar- threat during the trauma. hese indings suggest that
ger extent by peritraumatic and post-traumatic experi- the characteristics of the individual must be consid-
ences (efect sizes ranging from 0.23 to 0.40). However, ered in combination with the nature of the traumatic
the extent of efect for the majority of predictors dif- event when attempting to determine who will become
fered by sample, with most of the pre-trauma variables symptomatic following a traumatic event. herefore,
predicting PTSD in military veterans compared with in order to understand the risk factors for the devel-
civilian samples. Very few of the pre-trauma predictors, opment of PTSD, it may be helpful to study traumatic
with the exception of prior psychiatric history, child- events that are more homogeneous in nature.
hood abuse and family psychiatric history, had efects
of similar magnitude across studies, with the efect of
some pre-trauma predictors, including gender, age at
Reviews of the extant literature:
trauma, and race, disappearing in certain subsets of risk and protective factors for post-
studies. hese data suggest that attempts to identify a traumatic stress disorder specifically
universal set of pre-trauma predictors of PTSD that is
valid across diferent traumatized groups is premature for exposure to rape
and it underscores the complex interaction between Steketee and Foa (1987) conducted an early review of
the person exposed and the nature of exposure in pre- the extant rape literature that addressed risk factors
dicting PTSD. As suggested by Brewin et al. (2000), for short-term and longer-term reactions and that
these data serve as a springboard to begin to investigate evaluated risk factors in terms of demographics, rape
the dynamic between pre-trauma characteristics and characteristics, initial acute responses, and post-rape
how these interact with peritrauma and post-trauma stressors including interaction with the criminal just-
factors in the development of PTSD. ice system. As with the reviews discussed above, the
Independent of Brewin’s conclusions, Ozer et al. information is organized in terms of pre-assault, peri-
(2003) began exploring this interaction and focused assault, and post-assault risk factors with a range of

222
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

outcomes. As noted in the review by Steketee and Foa some rape victims were recruited via epidemiological
(1987) and subsequent reviews (Resnick et al., 1991; approaches while others were recruited from clinical
Resick, 1993), rape has been associated with a range or community-based populations and are subject to
of problems including anxiety, depression, substance the aforementioned biases. In addition, certain demo-
abuse, poor self-esteem, physical health complaints, graphic characteristics can have both positive and
and problems in social and sexual functioning – in add- negative efects. For example, being married does not
ition to PTSD. Findings from early reviews are noted necessarily imply a positive and supportive relation-
here and updated indings regarding risk factors are ship, and may, in fact, be an added stressor for some.
provided. here is a relatively greater emphasis in this Furthermore, education may be moderated by other
review on predictors of PTSD, with resilience viewed as demographic factors such as socioeconomic status
non-PTSD, given the primary focus of many reports on and, therefore, account for some of these contradictory
PTSD as an outcome. indings.
One demographic characteristic that appears to be
Pre-trauma factors associated with a consistent predictor of poorer post-rape function-
ing is lower economic status (Kilpatrick et al., 1985;
post-rape functioning Resnick et al., 2007). Given that history of rape and
A number of pre-assault characteristics have been physical assault may be prevalent among those with
shown to inluence post-rape functioning. Although reduced economic resources and in fact may contrib-
the majority of studies have focused on the relationship ute to lower resources (Bassuk et al., 1996; Byrne et al.,
between pre-assault characteristics and post-assault 1999), it may be important to develop strategies to
dysfunction, valuable information can be gained from increase economic resources among women at risk for
these studies that may increase our understanding victimization and in the post-assault period. Similar to
of factors associated with post-trauma adjustment. social support (discussed below in reference to post-
Overall, the pre-assault characteristics most heavily assault factors), baseline economic and social support
investigated in post-rape functioning are (1) demo- may also be conceptualized as pre-assault risk factors
graphic information, (2) prior assault history, and (3) related to either resilience or poor adjustment post-
prior psychological functioning or prior psychiatric assault.
history. As noted above, these studies are oten retro- he impact of demographic characteristics, as well
spective in nature and the populations studied likely as assault and post-assault characteristics, on cur-
do not accurately represent all rape victims. he limi- rent PTSD symptoms were evaluated in a study of 600
tations discussed above should be kept in mind when female sexual assault survivors recruited from college,
reviewing the extant literature that follows. community, and mental health agencies (Ullman et al.,
2007b). All forms of sexual victimization were included,
Demographics including completed rape, attempted rape, sexual coer-
Demographic characteristics are some of the most cion, and unwanted sexual contact occurring during
heavily investigated factors in the development of adolescence or adulthood (age 14 years and over) and
PTSD, but their role in predicting recovery from sex- childhood (aged under 14 years). Among the demo-
ual assault is unclear. he extant literature investigat- graphic variables, race, education, and marital status
ing demographic factors as a predictor of PTSD among were not related to PTSD symptoms, but older age at the
sexual assault survivors is inconsistent (Ullman et al., time of the assault was related to less severity of PTSD
2007a), with some studies highlighting these factors symptoms. hese indings may suggest that adults,
as correlates and others not inding an association compared with teenagers, are somehow more resilient
(Koss et al., 2002). For example, two studies have found when faced with sexual assault. It is possible that, with
older age to be protective (Ullman et al., 2007a), with age, comes more experience in dealing with traumatic
less severe PTSD symptoms, but several earlier studies events. Also, adults are likely to have more resources
reviewed found no efect of age (Steketee & Foa, 1987). and better-developed coping strategies to handle this
he indings reviewed by Steketee and Foa (1987) on experience compared with young adults. One caveat to
efects of education and marital status and response these data, however, is that participants were a conveni-
to rape are also mixed. hese contradictory results ence sample of volunteers from the community who
may relect diferences in sampling approach, because responded to advertisements and notices for women

223
Section 4: Specific challenges

who had a history of unwanted sexual experiences. As cortisol levels and PTSD in this small sample of recent
noted above, such women are self-acknowledged vic- rape victims. However, as noted, women with prior
tims, who may difer in terms of assault characteristics assault histories had lower cortisol levels post-rape and
and other variables from those within more represen- were also subsequently found to have higher prevalence
tative samples. Consequently, these demographic risk of PTSD. Although dysfunction of the hypothalamic–
factors and other predictors of PTSD may not general- pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis in the context of trauma
ize to more representative samples. is not completely understood, low cortisol levels may
In a longitudinal study of a nationally represen- relect an altered stress response as the result of prior
tative sample of 3006 women, Acierno et al. (1999) trauma. It is also possible that an attenuated stress-
identiied separate risk factors for PTSD ater rape response system may confer additional risk for PTSD
and ater physical assault. Whereas demographic vari- following subsequent victimization, or it may correlate
ables did not predict post-rape PTSD, lower education with other factors that increase risk of PTSD following
did predict post-assault PTSD among physical assault a subsequent assault.
victims. hese data highlight another important area Altered HPA activity and glucocorticoid receptor
of investigation in the study of PTSD, which is revic- sensitivity have been shown to be associated with PTSD
timization. Demographic variables such as young age (Yehuda, 2001; Yehuda et al., 2004) and there is increas-
and minority status increased the risk of being raped ing evidence that the interaction of biological (i.e.,
or physically assaulted, respectively (Acierno et al., genetic) and environmental (i.e., childhood traumatic
1999). Given that history of prior assault is a risk fac- events) factors may predict adult PTSD. Binder et al.
tor for PTSD, it is important to identify those at risk (2008) examined variants in the gene FKBP5 (a regu-
for revictimization in an efort to intervene to avoid lator of glucocorticoid receptor sensitivity) in a sample
further traumatic event exposure and its devastating of low-income, predominately African-American sub-
consequences. jects recruited from an inner city general medical clinic.
hese results showed that the variants in FKBP5 were
Prior assault history associated with higher levels of adult PTSD symptoms,
One of the most consistent indings from epidemio- but only among individuals exposed to two or more
logical and clinical samples is that many survivors of types of child abuse (including sexual abuse). here
rape also have histories of other traumatic events, and was no efect of genotype for individuals who experi-
multiple traumatic events are associated with increased enced only one type of child abuse or no child abuse.
risk of PTSD (Kramer & Green, 1991; Resnick et al., While the topic of childhood abuse is beyond the scope
1995; Ozer et al., 2003; Ullman et al., 2007a; Cougle of this chapter, these results demonstrate that the com-
et al., 2009). For example, Resnick et al., (1995) found bination of genotype and exposure to traumatic events,
a PTSD prevalence of 67% three months ater rape in this case early life trauma, inluences adult PTSD
among women with a prior history of sexual or phys- outcomes. hese indings are important and highlight
ical assault compared with 23% among women without the need to consider childhood traumatic event expos-
that history. hese indings are consistent with earlier ure, severity of diferent types of traumatic event, and/
studies suggesting that those with a history of multiple or multiple exposures as potential risk factors for the
rapes, compared with a single incident rape, showed development of PTSD in the atermath of rape within
greater distress following a recent assault and reported studies of potential genetic risk factors. Such research
more intense nightmares and fears one year ater the may also shed light on the association between prior
assault (Resick, 1993). Resnick et al. (1995) found that traumatic event exposure and PTSD.
women with a prior history of assault also had lower Prior assault history is not only associated with
cortisol levels in the immediate hours following a rape PTSD but also with a number of other psychiatric con-
compared with women without such histories. hese ditions and treatment responses. Resnick et al. (2007)
data highlight important biological diferences among found that prior history of rape was a signiicant pre-
women with a history of prior assault and women with- dictor of PTSD, depression, and other anxiety symp-
out such a history and are consistent with most studies, tom frequency following a recent rape, and that prior
but not all, that found low cortisol levels among adults history moderated the efects of a brief secondary pre-
with chronic PTSD (reviewed by Pervanidou, 2008). ventive intervention designed to prevent or reduce
Resnick et al. (1995) did not ind associations between PTSD, depression, and substance abuse. Speciically,

224
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

frequency of PTSD and depressive symptoms were sig- and post-crime PTSD. Speciically, participants who
niicantly lower among women with a prior history of met criteria for depression with age of onset before
rape who were exposed to a video intervention prior exposure to crime were signiicantly more likely to meet
to the rape examination that contained information criteria for post-crime PTSD only among those whose
about the examination and modeling and information attacks included fear of death, sustained injury, or rape.
about post-rape coping strategies than in those who Prior mental health history was not related to crime
received standard care. Women without a prior his- exposure itself and did not appear to be a vulnerability
tory of rape appeared to be more resilient. In addition, factor for PTSD in the absence of these crime severity
among women without a prior history of rape, those in characteristics. hese indings are partially consistent
the video group reported somewhat higher frequency with Koss et al. (2002), who examined the psycho-
or intensity of PTSD and general anxiety symptoms at logical, physical, and social impact of rape among a
six weeks ater rape assessment but not at six months large university-based sample of women. hey found
ater rape. a negative inluence on post-rape recovery with “psy-
chological problem history,” deined as self-reported
Prior psychological functioning (or mental behavioral problems (i.e., acting out, referral to juvenile
health history) services, substance abuse) as well as a history of psychi-
he majority of early studies, but not all, have found atric hospitalization, suicide attempts, and pharma-
prior psychological functioning to play an important cological and psychological treatment. However,
role in the immediate, recent, and long-term post-rape psychological problem history had direct efects on
adjustment periods (Resick, 1993). As reviewed by social maladjustment, self-blame, and assault severity,
Resick, indings from most but not all early longitu- which are all factors that have been shown to associate
dinal studies of recent rape victims indicated that prior with poor post-rape adjustment and PTSD. hese ind-
history of mental health problems was associated with ings suggest that the negative impact of psychological
greater distress within the irst few days post-assault or problem history is possibly moderated by maladaptive
increased distress or prevalence of post-rape criteria coping strategies (i.e., self-blame), poor social skills, or
for a psychiatric condition at one month ater rape or at assault characteristics (i.e., assault severity) and is not
longer-term follow-ups. hese study samples included necessarily a predictor of poorer post-assault adjust-
primarily rape victims who reported to police, rape cri- ment. In addition, the role of prior or complex assault
sis centers, or other authorities. history and its role in inluencing adjustment prior to a
Other research has demonstrated high prevalence subsequent rape needs to be considered and may have
of rape and physical assault among those with serious important treatment implications. Consequently, those
mental illness, including mood disorders, schizophre- with signiicant prior traumatic event history may also
nia, PTSD, and substance abuse (Goodman et al., 2001; beneit from intervention addressing responses and
Cusack et al., 2004). Findings indicate that the vast problems associated with such events. Post-rape inter-
majority (87–91%) of patients in the US public men- ventions aimed at improving social skills and address-
tal health system report a history of exposure to one ing issues of self-blame may impart resilience among
or more serious traumatic events (Cusack et al., 2004) rape victims.
and more than one third may experience a past year
incident of physical or sexual assault (Goodman et al.
2001). Data from these and other studies indicate that
Peritraumatic factors associated with
there may be a cyclical relationship between mental post-rape functioning
health problems and victimization by assault, such that his section considers factors linked to the assault time
history of assault increases risk of mental health prob- frame, such as assault characteristics, peritraumatic
lems, which may also increase risk of new victimization reactions, rape acknowledgement, and post-assault
and associated worsening of mental health problems. concerns.
hose with serious mental illness may be particularly
vulnerable to assault victimization. Assault characteristics
Findings from Resnick et al. (1992) indicated an In their early review of the literature related to rape,
interaction between retrospectively assessed pre-crime Steketee and Foa (1987) stated that most extant stud-
diagnosis of depression, crime severity characteristics, ies reported no signiicant associations between rape

225
Section 4: Specific challenges

characteristics or situational factors and PTSD. In add- and almost 50% met criteria at three months post-rape.
ition, indings were mixed across the small number of As reviewed by Resick (1993), indings do not support
studies that found associations between assault char- the notion that stranger rape is more severe than rapes
acteristics and post-rape indicators of functioning. by known assailants in terms of negative mental health
Some studies found that the more violent or poten- outcomes. In fact, rapes by known assailants including
tially injurious the assault the poorer the adjustment dating or marital partners appear to be similar in terms
over time, in terms of mood, psychiatric problems, and of negative mental health consequences (Kilpatrick
physical health complaints, while another set of stud- et al., 1988; Koss et al., 1988). Data from more repre-
ies found that severity was related to better self-esteem sentative sample studies indicate that injury as a part of
and sexual functioning. As suggested by Steketee and an assault increases risk of current PTSD among those
Foa (1987), the mixed indings might be attributed to with history of rape, controlling for other predictors
the possibility that increased severity may relate difer- (Acierno et al., 1999). Similarly, Kilpatrick et al. (1989)
entially to the varied outcomes studied. Consequently, examined a large probability sample of community
they suggested that higher threat or more violent women who were assessed for a range of crime-expo-
assaults might be associated with negative mental sure histories and found that the experiences of injury,
health outcomes in some studies but with less guilt and fear of death or injury, and the crime type of rape were
hence some positive functioning as observed in other all signiicant predictors of PTSD. Kilpatrick et al.
studies. his could be seen as consistent with indings found that rape was the only type of crime studied that
reported by Ullman et al. (2007a) that indicated that was a signiicant predictor of PTSD even ater control-
life threat during a sexual assault was signiicantly posi- ling for injury and threat characteristics. A majority
tively correlated with positive social reactions while (78%) of women reporting all three history character-
also being positively correlated with PTSD. istics met criteria for PTSD. Resnick et al. (1993), in a
A more complicated issue is that life threat was also large national sample of women, found that both injury
positively correlated with negative social reactions. It is and fear of death or injury were predictors of lifetime
likely that this is a function of the fact that more severe PTSD among women with histories of rape or phys-
assaults (in terms of injury or perceived life threat) are ical assault. Ullman and colleagues (Ullman & Filipas,
more likely to be self-acknowledged as rape and are 2001; Ullman et al., 2007a), using a range of strategies
also more likely to be reported, which increases the to recruit women with sexual assault histories from the
likelihood of others knowing about the incident and community and various service agencies, also found
responding in negative as well as positive ways. In fact, that perceived life threat was a signiicant correlate of
in an earlier study, Ullman and Filipas (2001) found PTSD among victims of sexual assault. However, other
that injury and life threat were positively correlated assault characteristics studied, including ofender vio-
with the number of support providers told about the lence (Ullman et al., 2007a) or injury (Ullman & Filipas,
assault and with the average number of negative social 2001), were not related to PTSD. Consequently, one of
reactions. he numbers of support providers and the the more consistent assault time frame predictors of
average number of negative social reactions were also PTSD may in itself be a perception or response, albeit a
signiicantly positively correlated with each other. response that is highly correlated with objective threat
he lack of any reported diferences in the major- characteristics such as injury as a result of assault (e.g.,
ity of studies and/or inconsistent indings observed Ullman & Filipas, 2001).
may also have related to the selective nature of samples
studied. Early studies of functioning post-rape typically Peritraumatic reactions
included women identiied through rape crisis or hos- Consistent with the inding that the subjective percep-
pital programs serving recent victims (e.g., Kilpatrick tion of fear of death or injury during assault is predictive
et al., 1979; Calhoun et al., 1982; Frank & Stewart, 1984). of PTSD or other negative outcomes, results of numer-
he more restricted range of characteristics of reported ous studies indicate that early distress responses post-
cases and/or an extremely high prevalence of PTSD may rape are strong positive predictors of PTSD or other
make it diicult to identify assault-related risk factors long-term patterns of anxiety or distress (Kilpatrick
in these samples. In the Rothbaum et al. (1992) longi- et al., 1985; Girelli et al., 1986; Rothbaum et al., 1992).
tudinal study of recent rape victims, 94% met all but he studies by Kilpatrick et al. (1985) and Rothbaum
duration criteria for PTSD at two weeks ater assault et al. (1992) indicated that distress at two weeks ater

226
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

rape positively predicted ongoing distress and PTSD at distress. Modeling of successful coping with examin-
three months ater rape. Other data indicate that anx- ation procedures was included to possibly help prepare
iety or distress reported at the time of rape or immedi- women to deal with the upcoming potential stressor.
ately aterwards may be a risk factor for later anxiety he pattern of symptoms over time among rape
and PTSD symptoms (Girelli et al., 1986; Resnick victims is also very important to think about in terms
et al., 2005). However, possible emotional avoidance, of resilience or recovery issues. Signiicant decreases in
or reduced acute reactions in the early atermath of PTSD or anxiety by three months ater rape have been
assault, has also been hypothesized as a risk factor for reported based on longitudinal studies (Kilpatrick
later problems in functioning (Gilboa-Schechtman & et al., 1979; Rothbaum et al., 1992). Kilpatrick and
Foa, 2001). Dunmore et al. (2001) found that retro- Calhoun (1988) suggested that it was critical to keep
spectively reported cognitive reactions that included information about the naturalistic course of recovery
mental defeat, confusion, and detachment were pre- in mind when evaluating the eicacy of treatment pro-
dictive of PTSD severity at later assessments among grams implemented soon ater a rape, because a sig-
victims of sexual or physical assault. Several studies niicant proportion of individuals would be likely to
have identiied panic reactions as common responses recover over time.
in the atermath of rape, or other assault, that are asso-
ciated with later PTSD (Resnick et al., 1994; Falsetti Rape acknowledgement
et al., 1995; Bernat et al., 1998; Kilpatrick et al., 1998). As noted above, self-acknowledgement of rape has
he study of panic and other acute reactions and cog- been found to be associated with more stereotypic
nitions appears to be important given the inding that or violent assaults (Koss, 1985), greater symptoms of
subjective factors, including perceived life threat, were PTSD (Layman et al., 1996), and physical health com-
more predictive of psychological distress than object- plaints (Conoscenti & McNally, 2006). More recently,
ive assault characteristics among victims of interper- Kilpatrick et al. (2007b) assessed how oten forcible,
sonal violence (Weaver & Clum, 1995). drug- or alcohol-facilitated or incapacitated rapes
Understanding emotional and cognitive perception were acknowledged as rape. Speciically, women were
variables that commonly occur during sexual assault asked in reference to up to two separate rape incidents
and that are predictive of PTSD or other problems whether they perceived the incident as unpleasant but
post-assault should inform development of secondary not a crime, a crime but not a rape, or rape. Results
and tertiary preventive interventions. For example, as within the college sample indicated that forcible rape
part of the video intervention developed by Resnick incidents were most likely (51%) to be perceived as
and colleagues (2005), information about panic reac- rape. One third (32%) of such incidents were perceived
tions during the assault was included in order to nor- as a crime but not a rape, and 15% were seen as unpleas-
malize such reactions and provide an explanation of the ant but not a crime. Within the college sample, only
functional underpinnings of physical sensations that 25% of incidents that involved unwanted penetration
might occur during an extremely stressful event, and but that involved incapacitation through either vol-
to address the possibility that conditioned responses untary or administered substances were perceived as
might occur later with reminders. he rationale for rape; 40% were seen as crime other than rape, and 31%
including this component was that this might normal- were perceived as unpleasant but not a crime. Parallel
ize such reactions and reduce fear of panic sensations data in the general population sample indicated that
in and of themselves. In addition, potential cognitions 71% of forcible incidents were perceived as rape, 18%
related to blame and concern about negative responses were perceived as crime, and 7% were perceived as
from others within the formal medical setting were unpleasant incidents. Substance-related rapes were
addressed by providing positive models of both rape perceived as rape in 45% of the cases; 28% were per-
crisis and hospital personnel responding in a support- ceived as crime, and 22% were perceived as unpleasant
ive way. Another major rationale for the development incidents.
and use of a video intervention designed to reduce acute Some previous studies have examined associations
distress and later psychopathology post-rape was the between rape acknowledgement and PTSD without
hypothesis that many components of the forensic rape controlling for assault factors such as perceived life
examination (e.g., pelvic examination) might serve threat or force, which relate to both acknowledgement
as strong rape-related cues, potentially raising acute and PTSD (e.g., Layman et al., 1996). A recent study

227
Section 4: Specific challenges

by Littleton and Henderson (2009) of 346 rape victims mental health consequences ater controlling for such
recruited from a large university sample found that assault characteristics is equivocal at this point, but it
39% of rapes were acknowledged. Acknowledgement may be relevant to consider as a cognitive variable that
was associated with more violent assaults and more could be addressed in a constructive way in the context
frequent PTSD symptoms; however, in a structural of secondary prevention.
equation model controlling for assault severity,
acknowledgement was not a signiicant predictor of Concerns in the aftermath of assault
PTSD. Further, the efects of assault characteristics Data from household probability samples (Kilpatrick
on PTSD were not direct but rather were associated et al., 1992, 2007b) indicate that victims of rape report
indirectly with PTSD via the association of more vio- post-assault concerns about health that include fear of
lent assaults with more maladaptive coping strategies, pregnancy, HIV/AIDS, or other sexually transmitted
including wishful thinking, social withdrawal, and self- diseases. Such concerns were reported by at least 40%
criticism; these, in turn, were predictive of PTSD. In of women who had been raped in the previous ive-year
a comparison of acknowledged and unacknowledged period in the National Women’s Study (Kilpatrick et al.,
rape and non-rape victims in a volunteer community 1992) and by 50% or more of rape victims in the general
sample, Conoscenti and McNally (2006) found that household probability study of forcible and drug- and
acknowledged and unacknowledged rape victims did alcohol-facilitated or incapacitated rape (Kilpatrick
not difer from each other on a standardized measure et al., 2007b). Both studies also found that concerns
of PTSD symptom frequency. Both unacknowledged about negative evaluation by family members or others
and acknowledged rape victims reported more health were reported as or more frequently than concerns
complaints than non-victims, while acknowledged about health. In a more select sample of those report-
rape victims reported more health symptoms than ing the assault to police or other authorities, Resnick
unacknowledged victims. and colleagues (2002) found that concerns about HIV
Zinzow et al. (2010) reported indings within the infection from the assault were reported by a major-
general population sample of 3001 women participat- ity of women. Greater concern regarding health issues
ing in the aforementioned study of forcible and drug- such as sexually transmitted diseases may relate to for-
and alcohol-facilitated rape. History of both forcible mal reporting and was associated with receipt of post-
rape and drug/alcohol-facilitated rape was associated rape medical care (Resnick et al., 2000). Programs that
with increased prevalence of PTSD compared with facilitate women receiving basic healthcare needs and
prevalence among non-rape victims. Zinzow et al. also addressing their health and other concerns might pro-
examined characteristics of a most recent or only rape mote resilience following assault. Outreach to those
incident in association with PTSD diagnosis within not seeking post-assault services might reduce barriers
the subsample of 556 rape victims and found posi- to those who do not currently report assault.
tive correlations between rape acknowledgement and
peritraumatic fear, injury, memory of event, force, and
forcible rape. Acknowledgement of an incident as rape
Post-rape factors associated with
and/or as a crime was inversely related to incapacitated post-rape functioning
or drug/alcohol-facilitated rapes. Acknowledgement Several post-assault factors have been shown to have
of most recent rape as either a rape or as a crime was a varying degrees of inluence on post-rape resilience.
risk factor for PTSD diagnosis, controlling for assault In general, the factors receiving the most empirical
characteristics that had been associated with PTSD attention include social support, victim coping strat-
on a univariate level. Although, as noted by Littleton egies, experience of additional traumas/life stres-
and Henderson (2009), there are some mixed ind- sors, and accessing post-rape resources such as crisis
ings regarding association between acknowledgement counseling or psychological treatment. As with the
and mental health correlates of rape, controlling for personal and rape characteristics considered in this
characteristics of assault, it does appear that acknow- chapter, the research on these post-rape factors has
ledgement tends to be positively correlated with those been conducted with a wide range of populations, in a
characteristics of assault that increase risk of PTSD. wide range of settings; the limitations of the extant lit-
Whether acknowledgement or perception of more erature mentioned at the outset of this chapter should
severe victimization is a unique predictor of negative be kept in mind.

228
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

Social support 2001). Borja and colleagues (2006) examined the role
of formal positive, informal positive, formal negative,
he literature on social support has several ways of
and informal negative social reactions in contributing
deining the reactions received by rape victims from
to the post-rape adjustment of over 100 college women.
either formal of informal recipients of disclosures.
Results of that study indicated that only informal nega-
Positive social reactions or social support reactions are
tive reactions signiicantly increased women’s report
those in which the rape survivor feels that the response
of PTSD symptoms, whereas positive social support
to disclosure is positive or neutral, whereas nega- from both formal and informal sources were correlated
tive social reactions have been deined as reactions in with better adjustment/post-traumatic growth. None
which the victim feels blamed, disbelieved, or unsup- of the various forms of social reactions were signii-
ported (reviewed by Ullman, 1999). Positive and nega- cantly associated with general psychological distress.
tive social reactions are distinguishable from lack of his study highlights the complexities of the relation
social support, which is most oten considered to be between social support and post-rape adjustment in
a separate factor with respect to resilience and recov- suggesting that both valence (positive versus negative)
ery. In general, negative social reactions to disclosure and source (formal versus informal) of support are
have been consistently associated with lower levels of important considerations.
resilience, greater use of unhealthy coping styles such he literature on social support has several limita-
as avoidance and self-blame, and more severe levels of tions worthy of note. First, it is limited by its reliance on
PTSD symptoms (Andrews et al., 2003; Ullman et al., data from non-representative (most oten community,
2007a, 2007b; reviewed by Ullman, 1999). Further, but at times college) women who self-identify as “rape
measures of perceived lack of social support have been victims.” As previously discussed, sampling meth-
associated with poorer recovery (Brewin et al., 2000), odology can limit the populations to which research
whereas perceived satisfaction with level of social sup- indings can be generalized and applied. Another limi-
port received has been shown to bufer against initial tation of this area of research is that it lacks standard
risk for the development of PTSD (Andrews et al., operational deinitions of the various types of support
2003). and oten relies heavily (or exclusively) on subjective
he data regarding the impact of positive social sup- perceptions of supportive/unsupportive reactions.
port are much less straightforward, and more mixed hat is, similar reactions may be interpreted as posi-
with respect to indings. Initial research on the role of tive by some and interpreted as negative reactions
social support found that victim-perceived supportive by others (Ullman, 1999). Since almost all studies of
reactions were associated with fewer reported phys- social support have relied on retrospective self-report,
ical health symptoms and more positive overall phys- it is also possible that respondent’s current evaluation
ical health ratings (Kimmerling & Calhoun, 1994). of the social support they received post-rape has been
Although Ullman (1999) noted that some of the extant inluenced by their present level of adjustment. hat
literature found positive social support to be protective, is, women reporting higher levels of current symp-
a signiicant portion of the literature reported non-sig- toms may be more likely to remember and highlight
niicant indings regarding the impact of positive social the negative social reactions they received in the more
support on the recovery process. More recent research immediate rape atermath Even given these limita-
with self-identiied survivors of adult rape has, surpris- tions, we may still cautiously draw two main conclu-
ingly, found greater frequency of social contact, posi- sions from the social support literature: (1) perception
tive social support, and higher levels of global support of positive support and that the level of support meets
were associated with higher levels of PTSD symptoms the individual need of the rape survivor are associated
(Ullman et al., 2007a, 2007b). While this might suggest with recovery and resilience; and (2) negative reactions
that support somehow leads to PTSD symptoms, it may are seldom, if ever, associated with resilience and are
simply relect the fact that more symptomatic women oten associated with poorer general recovery.
seek more help, as has been noted in other studies.
Finally, the literature suggests that person and rape Coping
characteristics not only inluence to whom women dis- When confronted with an adverse life experience
close their rape experience but also from whom they such as rape, women may employ a wide array of cop-
are most likely to receive support (Ullman & Filipas, ing strategies, and may use them to varying degrees

229
Section 4: Specific challenges

either in tandem or succession. When reading the hird, as mentioned above with respect to social
summary of research on post-rape coping and resili- reactions and social support, victims’ self-blame has
ence presented below, it is important to keep in mind also been shown to have a positive correlation with
that various measures of coping strategies exist, some higher levels of post-rape psychopathology. Self-blame
measuring dominant coping styles (e.g., Frazier & (e.g., there is something wrong with me as a person that
Burnett, 1994), while others measure relative fre- led to my victimization; it was my fault I was raped)
quency of use of an array of coping strategies (e.g., has, with high level of consistency, been associated with
Gutner et al., 2006). However, despite these nuances greater levels of PTSD symptomology. Frazier and col-
in measurement, several general conclusions may be leagues (Frazier, 2003; Frazier et al., 2004, 2005) have
drawn from the existing literature on coping strategies speciically examined behavioral self-blame (which
and post-rape resilience. they refer to as past control, or the notion that the vic-
First, approach-oriented coping strategies have tim’s actions were to blame for the assault), consistently
generally been found to be associated with fewer post- inding that it was associated with higher levels of dis-
traumatic stress symptoms and recovery, include cog- tress. In addition, the correlation between measures of
nitive restructuring, expressed emotion, seeking social self-blame and higher levels of psychological distress,
support, counseling, positive distancing, and keeping in turn, have been shown to inluence health outcomes
busy (Frazier & Burnett, 1994; Valentiner et al., 1996; when examined in a cross-sectional sample of women
Gutner et al., 2006). Frazier and colleagues (2005) lon- with rape experiences (Koss et al., 2002).
gitudinally surveyed a sample of women reporting Finally, there is an important conceptual issue that
their rape experience to an emergency medical facility must be considered with respect to any potential rela-
and found that perceptions of control over the recovery tionships that are found to exist between coping or self-
process were signiicantly associated with greater use blame and PTSD. Although it is generally assumed that
of cognitive restructuring; this, in turn, was associated avoidant coping strategies and/or behavioral self-blame
with less reported distress post-rape. Similar results are causative risk factors for PTSD, one can also make
were found in a volunteer community sample of female the case that people who develop PTSD have a greater
adult rape survivors, with greater perceptions of con- need than those who are resilient to use a variety of
trol being associated with fewer PTSD symptoms coping strategies (including avoidant ones) than their
(Ullman et al., 2007a). counterparts who are not coping with PTSD symp-
Second, avoidance-oriented coping strategies have toms. Likewise, it could be argued that self-blame is a
generally been found to be associated with greater self-protective cognitive coping strategy that is more
PTSD symptom severity and lower levels of resilience likely to occur among people who develop PTSD than
(Cohen & Roth, 1987; Santello & Leitenberg, 1993; among those who do not. Consequently, it is equally
Ullman, 1999; Ullman et al., 2007b). he term avoidance plausible to hypothesize that PTSD causes maladap-
coping has been used in a litany of research to apply to tive coping and self-blame as it is to hypothesize that
a range of coping strategies with the common element maladaptive coping and/or self-blame causes PTSD.
of disengagement or withdrawal. For example, among Clearly, only well-designed longitudinal research can
a community-recruited sample of self-identifying rape establish which of these hypotheses is correct.
victims, Frazier and Burnett (1994) found that the most
unhelpful coping strategies (those associated with Additional stress and trauma
higher mental health symptom levels) include avoid- Women’s experience of rape does not occur in isolation
ance strategies such as staying home and withdrawing from additional life stresses. In fact, some evidence
from others. Similarly, wishful thinking, which may suggests that women experiencing sexual assault vic-
be viewed as a form of cognitive withdrawal, was asso- timization are at increased risk for the additional
ciated with higher levels of PTSD symptoms among life stress produced by unemployment, divorce, and
an adult clinical sample of rape victims (Valentiner reduced income (Byrne et al., 1999). It is likely that this
et al., 1996). Research sampling college survivors of notable resource loss has a bidirectional relationship
rape has contributed similar indings of poorer adjust- with post-rape distress. Research supports post-rape
ment among women employing higher levels of dis- distress as a contributor to women’s loss of resources
engagement coping strategies (Santello & Leitenberg, (Monnier et al., 2002). In turn, general life stress has
1993). been shown to be one of most consistent predictors of

230
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

PTSD development in meta-analysis, with medium to the potential to yield potentially biased information
large efect sizes (Brewin et al., 2000). Among a group because of the methodological approaches used (e.g.,
of rape survivors, Resnick et al. (2007) found that hav- typically retrospective nature of epidemiological stud-
ing lower economic resources was a risk factor for ies and non-generalizable results from convenience
poorer functioning ater rape. Given that history of samples typically restricted to volunteer samples or
rape and physical assault are prevalent among those those reporting the assault to police or other author-
with reduced economic resources and, in fact, may con- ities). In order to ill in the gaps in our understanding
tribute to lower resources (Bassuk et al., 1996; Byrne of factors that promote resilience among rape victims,
et al., 1999), it may be important to develop strategies future studies need to address these limitations.
to increase economic resources among women at First, as described in the methodological section of
risk for victimization and in the post-assault period. this chapter, an ideal study of risk and resilience would
Similar to social support (discussed below in reference include a large representative sample of women who
to post-assault factors), baseline economic and social would be assessed for “baseline” characteristics includ-
support may also be conceptualized as pre-assault risk ing both psychological and biological markers thought
factors related to either resilience or poor adjustment to be important for risk or protection against PTSD.
post-assault. his sample would then be followed over time to allow
Subsequent exposure to another traumatic event for assessment of traumatic events, including rape, as
may appear to decrease resilience among rape sur- well as for PTSD and related problems in those who
vivors. Much empirical evidence exists that many experienced rape compared with those who did not.
women have multiple experiences of rape (Messman his would allow for the identiication of potential risk
& Long, 1996; Breitenbecher & Scarce, 1999; Rich and protective factors related to resilience in a large
et al., 2004). Women with a history of prior rape are representative sample of women. Ultimately, these
at 2 to 4.5 times greater risk for future victimization results would be more generalizable than the conveni-
than women without a rape history, making prior rape ence samples.
history one of the strongest risk factors for subsequent Second, despite employing an adequate study
assault (Gidycz et al., 1995, Humphrey & White, 2000). design to identify key risk and resilience factors among
Women with a history of multiple victimizations are at rape victims, these data may still be limited by rape vic-
greater risk for lacking satisfying social relationships tims’ tendency to avoid acknowledging rape. As noted
and utilizing escapism/avoidant coping techniques, above, many victims who meet the legal deinition of
both of which have been previously identiied as risk rape do not acknowledge what happened to them as
factors for poorer adjustment (Ellis et al., 1982; Proulx a rape. Given the indings that rape acknowledgement
et al., 1995). is related to characteristics of assault that are predict-
Several factors should be considered when con- ive of PTSD or other negative mental health outcomes,
sidering the role of additional life stress and trauma in studies that ask participants if they have been raped or
women’s resilience against rape. First, direct prospective sexually assaulted, rather than using behavioral dei-
examinations of the contribution of additional life stress nitions, and studies that gather volunteer respondents
and subsequent trauma exposure to the development of (all of whom will identify as having an unwanted sex-
PTSD (or other relevant mental health problems) have ual assault history) may include primarily or more
yet to be conducted. Second, the literature on revic- predominantly sexual assault victims who have expe-
timization is somewhat confused by the fact that many rienced higher rates of injury and perceived life threat
studies deine revictimization as at least one incident of and, in turn, may observe higher prevalence of PTSD.
childhood sexual victimization and subsequent victim- In order to avoid this potential bias, when researchers
ization in adulthood, whereas other studies have deined are inquiring about rape experiences among partici-
revictimization more in line with the discussion of pants, it is important to employ behavioral deinitions
multiple victimizations presented in this chapter. of rape as opposed to asking participants if they have
been raped or sexually assaulted.
Also, as highlighted throughout this chapter, the
Conclusions and future directions majority of the literature has focused on factors that
he majority of studies investigating risk and resilience predict post-rape PTSD or other psychopathology. he
factors among rape victims, although laudable, have identiication of key risk factors for psychopathology

231
Section 4: Specific challenges

can be used to study resilience in the face of known the direct impact of rape crisis advocates who provide
risk factors as well as to identify potentially modii- information and support to rape victims during the
able risk factors that may be targeted in interventions medical examination, through the criminal justice
to promote resilience or that constitute secondary process, or in the period ater rape. However, the lim-
prevention eforts once a woman has been sexually ited information available suggests that such services
assaulted. A second line of investigation would be to are helpful to survivors (reviewed by Campbell et al.,
identify protective factors among a cohort of those 2005; Martin et al., 2007; Decker & Naugle, 2009).
that have thrived in the face of adversity. In addition, More information is available regarding secondary
more research should focus on risk factors and poten- prevention or early intervention approaches (primarily
tial interventions for other problems such as substance targeting the individual and her cognitive or behavioral
abuse. coping strategies) designed to be implemented soon
ater a rape with the goal of reducing acute distress and
Using what we know to foster greater preventing or reducing longer-term problems such as
resilience and design early interventions PTSD. Treatment implemented ater an individual
already has PTSD would be considered tertiary inter-
to speed recovery vention. Kilpatrick and colleagues (2007b) reviewed
Little is known about strategies that might promote strategies and indings pertaining to rape-related sec-
resilience ater rape. Such approaches might include ondary and tertiary interventions. hey noted that
public education about realistic characteristics of propranolol is an example of a pharmacological early
rape and about functional coping strategies in the intervention that has been evaluated in small stud-
atermath of rape. he latter type of information ies with victims of diferent traumatic events (e.g.,
about how to report an incident or what to do in the Famularo et al., 1988; Pitman et al., 2002; Vaiva et al.,
immediate atermath is oten put forth by rape cri- 2003). hey suggested that the initial promising ind-
sis centers but it is heavily focused on criminal just- ings showing relatively lower prevalence of PTSD or
ice system or evidence-gathering issues. Information lower intensity of physiological responding need to be
designed to reduce blame or shame by addressing replicated within larger samples. Mixed indings have
true characteristics of rape may also be disseminated been observed related to psychosocial early interven-
by service agencies, but these are typically given to tions, which include cognitive-behavioral approaches
women who have already been victimized. Outreach delivered within hours, days, or weeks ater a rape. For
to college campuses and community education events example, Kilpatrick and Veronen (1983) found symp-
are also oten conducted by rape crisis agencies, but tom improvement among victims of rape who received
the potential impact of such educational approaches four to six hours of an early cognitive-behavioral skills-
in terms of resilience following rape has not been based intervention between 6 and 21 days ater an
empirically studied to our knowledge. More wide- assault, but improvement was not greater than that seen
spread public education programs targeting potential in the control group. Foa and colleagues (1995) found
social support providers or recipients of disclosure that a brief multisession cognitive-behavioral interven-
have also not been evaluated. Such approaches might tion delivered within one month of an assault (includ-
promote resilience by reducing negative social reac- ing rape) was associated with reduced prevalence of
tions and increasing positive supportive responses. PTSD at the end of treatment but not at a follow-up
Rape crisis agencies oten conduct education with at 5.5 months ater the assault. A subsequent study by
formal service providers such as police, with the goal Foa and colleagues (2006) comparing brief cognitive-
of reducing negative responses. Similarly, sexual behavioral therapy, assessment control, or supportive
assault response teams, including nurses and other counseling implemented within one month ater an
criminal justice personnel, attempt to provide such assault found reduced symptoms of PTSD and gen-
education across disciplines in order to improve the eral anxiety associated with the cognitive-behavioral
quality of services provided and/or the investigation treatment relative to the supportive counseling condi-
of rape cases. Empirical evaluation of the impact of tion when assessed at three months ater the assault.
such approaches on treatment of rape victims or per- Diferences between groups were not observed at long-
ceived formal responses by rape victims is lacking. er-term follow-up. Findings from the study by Foa and
Additionally, minimal information is available about colleagues were seen as consistent with the possibility

232
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

that early cognitive-behavioral intervention might understand the content of the video intervention and
accelerate recovery post-assault. it may have been more salient to them given their prior
As described above, Resnick and colleagues (1999, history of assault and/or subsequent reactions includ-
2005) developed a psychoeducational cognitive- ing PTSD.
behavioral intervention delivered in a video format and he review of risk and resilience factors related
designed to be shown prior to the rape examination to diferential outcomes following rape may identify
that is routinely conducted whenever a rape victim which approaches or components may be useful for
reports the assault to police or seeks post-rape med- integrating with other promising interventions to pro-
ical services. he video has two major components, the mote resilience or recovery. here are a number of such
irst of which provides information about the medical strategies that can be envisaged. Wider dissemination
examination and modeling of a woman successfully of information about characteristics of rape and help-
going through procedures. In addition, hospital and ful responses to others who have experienced assault
rape crisis personnel are introduced and positive mes- could be evaluated to see if such information leads to
sages are conveyed by these individuals regarding the attitude and/or behavior change, greater resilience, or
fact that the assault was not the individual’s fault but more positive support/reduced negative support in
that they have control over their recovery. his compo- the atermath of rape. Strategies to enhance perceived
nent was designed to reduce anxiety related to the med- control or eicacy would appear to be useful and could
ical examination and to provide a positive supportive perhaps be taught as general ways of coping with stres-
context at the time of the exam. he second compo- sors. Greater emphasis might be given to improving
nent of the video includes psychoeducation and mod- social support of victims in the atermath of assault
eling of coping strategies in the atermath of assault, and such a strategy could then be evaluated empiric-
which include in vivo graduated exposure, behavioral ally. Assistance with economic or other resources in
activation, identiication of cues or situations that may the atermath of rape could be instigated, similar to the
be associated with drug or alcohol use, and promo- components augmenting the treatment strategy used
tion of activities and contexts that do not involve sub- by Zatzick and colleagues (2004) for patients with trau-
stance use. Information about acute reactions of panic matic injuries. Finally, additional attention should be
and a learning theory model of possible later reactions given to developing and evaluating potentially efective
to reminders of assault is presented in order to try to early interventions and exploring possible individual
prevent fear of physiological arousal sensations and to diferences in response to interventions to determine
provide a model for understanding the rationale for in which approaches may be useful given identiied risk
vivo exposure exercises. or resilience factors.
A randomized clinical trial compared the video
intervention with standard care among a group of rape
victims who were seen for standard post-rape med-
Acknowledgements
ical care. Data indicated that, among women with a Support for writing was provided by NIDA Grant R01
prior history of rape, those in the video group reported DA023099–02 (Prevention of Postrape Drug Abuse:
signiicantly lower frequency of PTSD symptoms Replication Study and Training) and Grant 5 T32
and severity of depression symptoms at assessment MH018869–22 (Traumatic Stress Across the Lifespan:
approximately six weeks ater rape compared with A Biopsychosocial Training Program) (principal inves-
women in the standard care sample. Women without tigator, Dean Kilpatrick).
a prior history of rape appeared to be more resilient
in terms of reported symptom frequency and inten- References
sity over time. No diferences in symptoms of psycho-
Acierno, R., Resnick, H., Kilpatrick, D. G., Saunders, B., &
pathology were observed related to intervention at Best, C. L. ( 1999). Risk factors for rape, physical
longer-term follow-up assessment at approximately six assault, and posttraumatic stress disorder in women:
months post-rape (Resnick et al., 2007). Results of this examination of diferential multivariate relationships.
study indicated a signiicant moderating efect of prior Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 13, 541–563.
history of rape. It was hypothesized that women with American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic
a prior history of assault, who are also at risk of more and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn.
severe problems following a new incident, might also Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

233
Section 4: Specific challenges

Andrews, B., Brewin, C., & Rose, S. (2003). Gender, social Conoscenti, L. M. & McNally, R. J. (2006). Health
support, and PTSD in victims of violent crime. Journal of complaints in acknowledged and unacknowledged rape
Traumatic Stress, 16, 421–427. victims. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 372–379.
Bassuk, E. L., Weinreb, L. F., Buckner, J. C., et al. (1996). he Cougle, J. R., Resnick, H., & Kilpatrick, D. G. (2009). Does
characteristics and needs of sheltered homeless and low prior exposure to interpersonal violence increase risk
income housed mothers. Journal of the American Medical of PTSD following subsequent exposure. Behaviour
Association, 276, 640–646. Research and herapy, 47, 1012–1017.
Bernat, J. A., Ronfeldt, H. M., Calhoun, K. S., & Arias, I. Cusack, K. J., Frueh, B. C., & Brady, K. T. (2004). Trauma
(1998). Prevalence of traumatic events and peritraumatic history screening in a community mental health center.
predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms in a Psychiatric Services, 55, 157–162.
nonclinical sample of college students. Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 11, 645–664. Decker, S. E. & Naugle, A. E. (2009). Immediate intervention
for sexual assault: A review with recommendations and
Binder, E. B., Bradley, R. G., Liu, W., et al. (2008). implications for practitioners. Journal of Aggression,
Association of FKBP5 polymorphisms and childhood Maltreatment and Trauma, 18, 419–441.
abuse with risk of posttraumatic stress disorder
symptoms in adults. Journal of the American Medical Dunmore, E., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2001). A prospective
Association, 299, 1291–1305. investigation of the role of cognitive factors in persistent
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) ater physical
Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and herapy, 39,
Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive 1063–1084.
ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist,
59, 20–28. Ellis, E., Atkeson, B., & Calhoun, K. (1982). An examination
of diferences between multiple-and single-incident
Borja, S., Callahan, J. L., & Long, P. J. (2006). Positive and victims of sexual assault. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
negative adjustment and social support of sexual assault 91, 221–224.
survivors. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19, 905–914.
Falsetti, S. A., Resnick, H. S., Dansky, B. S., Lydiard, R. B., &
Breitenbecher, K. H. & Scarce, M. (1999). A longitudinal
Kilpatrick, D. G. (1995). he relationship of stress to panic
evaluation of the efectiveness of a sexual assault
disorder: Cause or efect? In C. M. Mazure (ed.), Does
education program. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14,
stress cause psychiatric illness? (pp. 111–147). Washington,
459–478.
DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Breslau, N., Kessler, R. C., Chilcoat, H. D., et al. (1998).
Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the Famularo, R., Kinscherf, R., & Fenton, T. (1988).
community: the 1996 Detroit Area Survey of Trauma. Propranolol treatment for childhood posttraumatic
Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 626–632. stress disorder, acute type: a pilot study. Archives of
Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 142, 1244–1247.
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta-
analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder Fisher, B. S., Daigle, L. E., Cullen, F. T., & Turner, M. G.
in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and (2003). Reporting sexual victimization to the police and
Clinical Psychology, 68, 748–766. others: Results from a national-level study of college
women. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 30, 6–38.
Byrne, C. A., Resnick, H. S., Kilpatrick, D. G., Best, C. L., &
Saunders, B. E. (1999). he socioeconomic impact of Foa, E. B., Hearst-Ikeda, D., & Perry, K. J. (1995). Evaluation
interpersonal violence on women. Journal of Consulting of a brief cognitive-behavioral program for the
and Clinical Psychology, 67, 362–366. prevention of chronic PTSD in recent assault victims.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63,
Calhoun, K. S., Atkeson, B. M., & Resick, P. A. (1982). A 948–955.
longitudinal examination of fear reactions in victims of
rape. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29, 655–661. Foa, E. B., Zoellner, L. A., & Feeny, N. C. (2006). An
evaluation of three brief programs for facilitating
Campbell, R., Patterson, D., & Lichty, L. F. (2005). he
recovery ater assault. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19,
efectiveness of sexual assault nurse examiner (SANE)
29–43.
programs: A review of psychological, medical, legal, and
community outcomes. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 6, Frank, E. & Stewart, B. D. (1984). Depressive symptoms in
313–329. rape victims. Journal of Afective Disorders, 7, 77–85.
Cohen, L. J. & Roth, S. (1987). he psychological atermath Frazier, P. A. (2003). Perceived control and distress
of rape: Long-term efects and individual diferences following sexual assault: A longitudinal test of a new
in coping. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5, model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
525–534. 1257–1269.

234
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

Frazier, P. & Burnett, J. (1994). Immediate coping strategies In C. R. Figley (ed.), Trauma and its wake (pp.113–141).
among rape victims. Journal of Counseling and New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Development, 72, 633–639. Kilpatrick, D. G., Best, C. C., Saunders, B. E., & Vernon, L. J.
Frazier, P., Steward, J., & Mortensen, H. (2004). Perceived (1988). Rape in marriage and in dating relationships: How
control and adjustment to trauma: A comparison across bad is it for mental health? Annals of the New York Academy
events. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, of Sciences, 528, 335–344.
303–324. Kilpatrick, D. G., Saunders, B. E., Amick-McMullan, A.,
Frazier, P., Mortensen, H., & Steward, J. (2005). Coping et al. (1989). Victim and crime factors associated with
strategies as mediators of the relations among perceived the development of crime-related post-traumatic stress
control and distress in sexual assault survivors. Journal of disorder. Behavior herapy, 20, 199–214.
Counseling Psychology, 52, 267–278.
Kilpatrick, D. G., Edmunds, C. N., & Seymour, A. K. (1992).
Gidycz, C., Hanson, K., & Layman, M. (1995). A prospective Rape in America: A report to the nation. Arlington, VA:
analysis of the relationships among sexual assault National Victim Center and the Medical University of
experiences: An extension of previous indings. South Carolina.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 19, 5–29.
Kilpatrick, D. G., Acierno, R., Resnick, H. S., Saunders, B., &
Gilboa-Schechtman, E. & Foa, E. B. (2001). Patterns Best, C. L. ( 1997). A 2-year longitudinal analysis of the
of recovery from trauma: the use of intraindividual relationship between violent assault and substance use in
analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 392–400. women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65,
Girelli, S. A., Resick, P. A., Marhoefer-Dvorak, S., & Hutter, 834–847.
C. K. (1986). Subjective distress and violence during Kilpatrick, D. G., Resnick, H. S., Freedy, J. R., et al. (1998).
rape: heir efects on long-term fear. Violence and he posttraumatic stress disorder ield trial: Emphasis on
Victims, 1, 35–45. criterion A and overall PTSD diagnosis. In T. Widiger,
Goodman, L., Salyers, M., & Mueser, K., et al. (2001). Recent A. Frances, H. Pincus, et al. (eds.), DSM-IV sourcebook,
victimization in women and men with severe mental Vol. 4 (pp. 303–344). Washington, DC: American
illness: Prevalence and correlates. Journal of Traumatic Psychiatric Press.
Stress, 14, 615–633. Kilpatrick, D. G., Ruggiero, K. J., Acierno, R., et al. (2003).
Gutner, C., Rizvi, S., Monson, C., & Resick, P. (2006). Violence and risk of PTSD, major depression, substance
Changes in coping strategies, relationship to the abuse/dependence, and comorbidity: Results from the
perpetrator, and posttraumatic distress in female crime national survey of adolescents. Journal of Consulting and
victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19, 813–823. Clinical Psychology, 71, 692–700.
Humphrey, J. A. & White, J. W. (2000). Women’s Kilpatrick, D. G., Resnick, H. S., Ruggiero, K. J.,
vulnerability to sexual assault from adolescence to young Conoscenti, L. M., & McCauley, J. (2007a).
adulthood. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27, 419–424. Drug-facilitated, incapacitated, and forcible rape: A
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M., & national study. Final report submitted to the National
Nelson, C. B. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder in Institute of Justice, May 2007 [NCJ 219181]. Rockville,
the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General MD: National Institute of Justice.
Psychiatry, 52, 1048–1060. Kilpatrick, D. G., Amstadter, A. B., Resnick, H. S., &
Kilpatrick, D. G. (1983). Rape victims: Detection, assessment, Ruggiero, K. J. (2007b). Rape related PTSD: Issues and
and treatment. Clinical Psychologist, 36, 92–95. interventions. Psychiatric Times, 24, 7.
Kilpatrick, D. G. & Calhoun, K. S. (1988). Early behavioral Kimmerling, R. & Calhoun, K. S. (1994). Somatic
treatment for rape trauma: Eicacy or artifact. Behavior symptoms, social support, and treatment seeking among
herapy, 19, 421–427. sexual assault victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 62, 333–340.
Kilpatrick, D. G. & Veronen, L. J. (1983). Treatment for rape
related problems: Crisis intervention is not enough. In Koss, M. P. (1985). he hidden rape victim. Psychology of
L. H. Cohen, W. L. Claiborn, & C. A. Spector (eds.), Crisis Women Quarterly, 48, 61–75.
intervention (pp. 165–185). New York: Human Sciences Koss, M. P., Dinero, T. E., & Seibel, C. A. (1988). Stranger
Press. and acquaintance rape: Are there diferences in
Kilpatrick, D. G., Veronen, L. J., & Resick, P. A. (1979). he victim’s experience? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 12,
atermath of rape: Recent empirical indings. American 1–24.
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 49, 658–669. Koss, M. P., Figueredo, A. J., & Prince, R. J. (2002).
Kilpatrick, D. G., Veronen, L. J., & Best, C. L. (1985). Factors Cognitive mediation of rape’s mental, physical, and
predicting psychological distress among rape victims. social health impact: Tests of four models in

235
Section 4: Specific challenges

cross-sectional data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Resnick, H. S., Kilpatrick, D. G., Dansky, B. S., Saunders, B. E.,
Psychology, 70, 926–941. & Best, C. L. (1993). Prevalence of civilian trauma and
Kramer, T. L. & Green, B. L. (1991). Posttraumatic stress posttraumatic stress disorder in a representative national
disorder as an early response to sexual assault. Journal of sample of women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Interpersonal Violence, 6, 160–173. Psychology, 61, 984–991.
Layman, M. J., Gidycz, C. A., & Lynn, S. J. (1996). Resnick, H. S., Falsetti, S. A., Kilpatrick, D. G., & Foy, D. W.
Unacknowledged versus acknowledged rape victims: (1994). Associations between panic attacks during rape
situational factors and posttraumatic stress. Journal of assaults and follow-up PTSD or panic-attack outcomes. In
Abnormal Psychology, 105, 124–131. Proceedings of the 10th Annual Meeting of the International
Society of Traumatic Stress Studies, Chicago, November.
Littleton, H., & Henderson, C. E. (2009). If she is not a
victim, does that mean she was not traumatized? Violence Resnick, H. S., Yehuda, R., Pitman, R. K., & Foy, D. W.
Against Women, 15, 148–167. (1995). Efect of previous trauma on acute plasma
cortisol level following rape. American Journal of
Martin, S. L., Young, S. K., Billings, D. L., & Bross, C. C. Psychiatry, 152, 1675–1677.
(2007). Health care-based interventions for women who
have experienced sexual violence. Trauma, Violence, and Resnick, H. S., Acierno, R., & Kilpatrick, D. G. (1997).
Abuse, 8, 3–18. Health impact of interpersonal violence. Section II:
Medical and mental health outcomes. Behavioral
Messman, T. L. & Long, P. J. (1996). Child sexual abuse and Medicine, 23, 79–85.
its relationship to revictimization in adult women: A
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 16, 397–420. Resnick, H. S., Acierno, R., Holmes, M., Kilpatrick, D., &
Jager, N. (1999). Prevention of post-rape psychopathology:
Monnier, J., Resnick, H., Kilpatrick, D., & Seals, B. (2002). Preliminary indings of a controlled acute rape treatment.
he relationship between distress and resource loss Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 13, 359–370.
following rape. Violence and Victims, 17, 85–92.
Resnick, H. S., Holmes, M. M., Kilpatrick, D. G., et al.
Norris, F. H. (1992). Epidemiology of trauma: Frequency (2000). Predictors of post-rape medical care in a national
and impact of diferent potentially traumatic events on sample of women. American Journal of Preventive
diferent demographic groups. Journal of Consulting and Medicine, 19, 214–219.
Clinical Psychology, 60, 409–418.
Resnick, H. S., Monnier, J., Seals, B., et al. (2002). Rape-
Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2003). related HIV risk concerns among recent rape victims.
Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17, 746–759.
symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 129, 52–73. Resnick, H. S., Acierno, R., Kilpatrick, D. G., Holmes, M.
(2005). Description of an early intervention to prevent
Pervanidou, P. (2008). Biology of post-traumatic stress substance abuse and psychopathology in recent rape
disorder in childhood and adolescence. Journal of victims. Behavior Modiication, 29, 156–188.
Neuroendocrinology, 20, 632–638.
Resnick, H. S., Acierno, R., Waldrop, A. E., et al. (2007).
Pitman, R. K., Sanders, K. M., Zusman, R. M., et al. (2002). Randomized controlled evaluation of an early
Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic intervention to prevent post-rape psychopathology.
stress disorder with propranolol. Biological Psychiatry, Behaviour Research and herapy, 45, 2432–2447.
51,189–192.
Rich, C. L., Combs-Lane, A. M., Resnick, H. S., & Kilpatrick,
Proulx, J., Koverola, C., Fedorowicz, A., & Kral, M. D. G. (2004). Child sexual abuse and adult sexual
(1995). Coping strategies as predictors of distress in revictimization. In L. J. Koenig, L. S. Doll, A. O’Learey,
survivors of single and multiple sexual victimization & W. Pequegnat (eds.), From child sexual abuse to adult
and nonvictimized controls. Journal of Applied Social sexual risk: Trauma, revictimization, and intervention (pp.
Psychology, 25, 1464–1483. 49–68). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Resick, P. A. (1993). he psychological impact of rape. Rothbaum, B. O., Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Murdock, T., &
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8, 223–255. Walsh, W. (1992). A prospective examination of post-
Resnick, H. S., Kilpatrick, D. G., & Lipovsky, J. A. (1991). traumatic stress disorder in rape victims. Journal of
Assessment of rape-related posttraumatic stress disorder: Traumatic Stress, 5, 455–475.
stressor and symptom dimensions. Journal of Consulting Santello, M. & Leitenberg, H. (1993). Sexual aggression
and Clinical Psychology, 3 561–572. by an acquaintance: Methods of coping and later
Resnick, H. S., Kilpatrick, D. G., Best, C. L., & Kramer, T. L. psychological adjustment. Violence and Victims, 8,
(1992). Vulnerability–stress factors in development of 91–104.
posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Nervous and Shalev, A. Y., Peri, T., Canetti, L., & Schreiber, S. (1996).
Mental Disease, 180, 424–430. Predictors of PTSD in injured trauma survivors: a

236
Chapter 15: Rape and other sexual assault

prospective study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, Vaiva, G., Ducrocq, F., Jezequel, K., et al. (2003). Immediate
219–225. treatment with propranolol decreases posttraumatic
Steketee, G. & Foa, E. B. (1987). Rape victims: Post- stress disorder two months ater trauma. Biological
traumatic stress responses and their treatment: A Psychiatry, 54, 947–949.
review of the literature. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 1, Valentiner, D., Foa, E., Riggs, D., & Gershuny, B. (1996).
69–86. Coping strategies and posttraumatic stress disorder in
female victims of sexual and nonsexual assault. Journal of
Tjaden, P. & hoennes, N. (2000). Prevalence and
Abnormal Psychology, 105, 455–458.
consequences of male-to-female and female-to-male
partner violence as measured by the National Violence Weaver, T. L. & Clum, G. A. (1995). Psychological distress
Against Women Survey. Violence Against Women, 6, associated with interpersonal violence: A meta-analysis.
142–161. Clinical Psychology Review, 15, 115–140.
Ullman, S. (1999). Social support and recovery from sexual Yehuda, R. (2001). Biology of posttraumatic stress disorder.
assault: A review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 4, Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 62, 41–46
343–358. Yehuda, R., Golier, J. A., Halligan, S. L., Meaney, M., &
Ullman, S. E. & Filipas, H. H. (2001). Predictors of PTSD Bierer, L. M. (2004). he ACTH response to
symptom severity and social reactions in sexual assault dexamethasone in PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry,
victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 369–389. 161, 1397–1403
Ullman, S., Filipas, H. H., Townsend, S. M., & Starzynski, Zatzick, D., Roy-Byrne, P., Russo, J., et al. (2004). A
L. L. (2007a). Psychosocial correlates of PTSD symptom randomized efectiveness trial of stepped collaborative
severity in sexual assault survivors. Journal of Traumatic care for acutely injured trauma survivors. Archives of
Stress, 20, 821–831. General Psychiatry, 61, 498–506.
Ullman, S., Townsend, S. M., Filipas, H. H., & Zinzow, H., Resnick, H., Amstadter, A., et al. (2010). Drug
Starzynski, L. L. (2007b). Structural models of the and alcohol-facilitated, incapacitated, and forcible
relations of assault severity, social support, avoidance rape in relation to mental health in a national sample
coping, self-blame, and PTSD among sexual assault of women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25,
survivors. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 23–37. 2217–2236.

237
Section
Section4 Specific challenges
Chapter
The stress continuum model: a military

16 organizational approach to resilience


and recovery
William P. Nash, Maria Steenkamp, Lauren Conoscenti, and Brett T. Litz

Introduction or paradigmatic approach to conceptualizing the mili-


tary and extra-military processes and functions that
Resilience in the face of adversity is vital for military
may lead to resilient outcomes, and even by the lack of
service members to survive potential threats to their
a consensus deinition of resilience. Uniform methods
own lives and safety and to accomplish assigned mis-
of measuring resilience processes or outcomes also do
sions, oten for the sake of others’ survival and welfare.
not yet exist. he little empirical research that has been
he ability of service members to bounce back from
conducted in the military has signiicant internal and
operational stress may also determine how successfully
external validity problems, greatly limiting their power
they reintegrate with their families and communities
to inform prevention or intervention practices, which
ater returning from deployment, whether they can
is the basic goal of resilience research.
continue to work in military professions, and whether
To promote clarity about the concept of resilience in
they develop potentially disabling mental disorders or
the military, and to foster future research, this chapter
other serious behavioral problems. Military organiza-
begins by considering the concept of resilience in the
tions have long traditions of selecting, training, and
military as comprising of diferent classes of positive
sustaining service members to endure intense and per-
stress outcome. he chapter then considers potential
sistent operational stress without losing their abilities
metrics for each and briely surveys existing evidence
to function on the battleield, but other indices of resili-
that the military has been successful at developing and
ence have only recently attracted the sustained inter-
sustaining each aspect of resilience in its service mem-
est of the military. As military organizations develop
bers. he goal is to identify the greatest ongoing chal-
programs to promote a broader spectrum of desired
lenges for military resilience program development.
stress outcomes, they are faced with a choice between
One recently developed, evidence-informed approach
expecting traditional resilience-building methods to
to promoting resilience and recovery is then described
meet untraditional objectives and creating entirely
that speciically emphasizes military ecological val-
novel approaches to resilience.
idity: the Combat and Operational Stress Control
Academic interest in the psychological, biological,
(COSC) program in the US Marine Corps and its par-
social, and personality-trait diferences associated
allel in the US Navy, the Operational Stress Control
with successful adaptation to combat and operational
(OSC) program. he rationale, conceptual framework,
experiences has increased rapidly since the late 1990s.
and intervention strategies common to both programs
Underlying recent studies in this area has been the
are outlined. he chapter concludes with a call for well-
assumption that the incidence of various mental health
designed outcome studies of programs that attempt to
and functional problems associated with combat and
ill the current gaps in military resilience programs.
operational experiences might be reduced if modi-
iable risk and resilience factors could be identiied
and then targeted in military prevention programs. Military resilience challenges
Unfortunately, research and translational programs Almost any mission with which a military unit may
to enhance resilience in members of the armed ser- be tasked – whether assaulting an opposing military
vices have, so far, been limited by the lack of a uniied force, countering a civilian insurgency, conducting

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

238
Chapter 16: The stress continuum model

peacekeeping, or providing humanitarian assistance in Troops in battle who are disabled by stress, even briely,
the wake of a disaster – can expose service members or who lose their ability to remain calm and make
to mortal danger, loss, and moral compromise with an rational decisions in the face of chaos lose operational
intensity and relentlessness hard to imagine in most efectiveness and may quickly become physical cas-
other settings (e.g., Litz, 2007; Nash, 2007a; Litz et al., ualties. Some of the historically highest physical cas-
2009). Returning from deployment exposes service ualty rates have been experienced by ground combat
members to an entirely diferent set of adaptive chal- units engaged in disorganized and panicked retreats.
lenges, some of which may be just as overwhelming Similarly, the success of western nations and deployed
as those experienced during deployment. Examples military units to prevail in modern, asymmetrical war-
include the stress of coming home to a broken family, fare against non-governmental insurgencies or terror-
a lost civilian job, or inancial ruin. Both during and ist groups depends on their abilities to remain focused
ater deployment to an operational theater, service on constructively “winning the hearts and minds” of
members face the challenge of mourning losses, ind- populations even while repeatedly enduring attacks
ing meaning in experiences that seem senseless, and from within the population’s ranks – attacks that have
making peace with enduring memories of death and great potential to provoke terror, horror, helplessness,
destruction. Resilience challenges for military service rage, and a desire to seek vengeance rather than to build
members and the organizations that support them, hospitals, schools, and sewage systems. he weapons of
therefore, arguably encompass at least three broad global terrorism, including the growing threat of rogue
forms: (1) operational resilience, which may be deined nuclear incidents, directly target the resilience of soci-
as the ability to maintain occupational role function- eties and their governments.
ing and psychological performance during operational At the unit level, the desired outcomes of oper-
deployments despite stressor exposures, and perhaps ational resilience include mission efectiveness, readi-
despite internal distress and conlict (see Litz, 2005); ness, and force preservation. At the individual level,
(2) post-deployment resilience, which may be deined the simplest desired outcome is to avoid becoming a
as the ability to reacquire and maintain efective role stress casualty (i.e., being incapacitated) by continu-
functioning in largely non-military settings ater ing to maintain at least the minimum required abil-
returning from deployment, and so again to be a pro- ity to function in assigned operational military roles.
ductive member of a family and civilian society; and Stated more positively, the objectives of individual
(3) psychological resilience, which may be deined as and unit-level operational resilience are performance
the ability to adapt physically, mentally, and spiritually enhancement despite hardships and losses, and the
to operational stressor exposures, and the sometimes development and maintenance of the time-honored
lasting changes they engender, without developing a military traits of courage, fortitude, and valor (e.g.,
signiicant mental disorder or behavioral problem. Moran, 1967 [originally published 1945]). Operational
hough obviously interrelated, these three broad resilience has been so central to the efectiveness of
resilience challenges in the military have very difer- ighting forces that methods for promoting it have
ent historical and scientiic bases, and they depend been in existence since ancient times. In the Roman
on diferent methods for promotion and metrics for army (Phang, 2008), for example, the goal of all train-
assessment. Each will now be briely described in its ing and discipline was to develop virtus (courage in
historical and scientiic context. battle) without ferocia (madness), and animus (coni-
dence) and impetus (enthusiasm) without excessive ira
(anger). he same warrior ideal of controlled aggres-
Operational resilience sion, summoned by external authority but restrained
Extreme stress is not an incidental byproduct of armed by internal moral values, can be found in military
conlict but rather one of its central features and imme- cultures throughout history (French, 2003). Much
diate objectives. Success in traditional warfare has the same tools are available to the military today as
oten been determined by which side possesses the were in use in ancient times to promote operational
greater ability to endure and continue ighting despite resilience: pre-induction selection, tough and realistic
the potentially overwhelming challenges and hard- training, the building of unit cohesion through shared
ships each side intentionally inlicts upon the other successes in the face of shared hardships, and engaged
(von Clausewitz, 1982 [orginally published 1832]). leadership (Nash et al., 2011).

239
Section 4: Specific challenges

At its most basic level, operational resilience is easy American Civil War soldiers and veterans (Dean, 1997).
to measure in service members because the primary he ancient Greeks understood that war – particularly
target outcome is simple and easy to quantify over the the brutal, close-up variety they practiced – could
ixed period of observation of each deployment. Most darken the psyches of surviving veterans with a miasma
simply, operational resilience may be quantiied as the (pollution) that was diicult, at best, to wash away, and
inverse of the incidence of disabling combat or oper- that could result in illness, death, or the destruction of
ational stress casualties in a deployed force. A deployed families long ater the war had ended (Meagher, 2006).
unit that has sufered a 10% stress casualty rate may be Like many other ancient Greek insights, however, this
said to be 90% operationally resilient. At the less basic understanding of the post-deployment consequences
level of optimum individual performance despite stress of combat stress was somehow forgotten, only to be
load, metrics for operational resilience are less clear slowly and painfully rediscovered over the past cen-
and simple, and may not be easily collected. tury. he world was shocked by the murders over six
How successful has the military been at promot- weeks in 2002 of four military wives by their active
ing operational resilience in its service members and duty husbands at Fort Bragg, North Carolina, home of
units? Using the simple metric of the rates of stress the army’s special operations command (Starr, 2002).
casualties requiring medical evacuation, the US mili- hree of the four assailants had recently returned from
tary has enjoyed considerable and sustained success combat operations in Afghanistan.
in promoting operational resilience. Ater World War Unlike the military’s approach to operational resili-
II, when as many as 10% of all troops were disabled by ence, tools for promoting post-deployment resilience
“battle fatigue” or some mental health diagnosis, stress have changed considerably over the centuries. Public
casualties rates fell to approximately 3.7% during the puriication rites and rituals to remove the stain of war
Korean War, and as low as 1.2% in Vietnam (Nash, have long been used to promote reintegration into civil
2007b). Shorter and more predictable tour lengths and society ater war. In ancient Greece, public perform-
generally lower physical casualty rates were credited ances of dramas of homecoming, written for audi-
for some of this apparent improvement in operational ences of veterans by playwrights who, like Sophocles
resilience over time. According to the 2005 report of and Euripides, were veterans themselves, may have
the US Army Mental Health Advisory Team (MHAT; been crucial for the socialization of returning warriors
the MHAT-II report), the last annual MHAT report to (Meagher, 2006). In the USA, organized programs to
include stress-casualty data, behavioral health diagno- promote post-deployment resilience have only recently
ses accounted for 7.1% of all medical evacuations from become a priority. Troops returning from the US Civil
Iraq (527 of 7415) between March and September 2003 War, World War I, and the Vietnam War were notori-
(the initial invasion), and 6.0% of all medical evacua- ously let to fend for themselves to too great an extent
tions from Iraq during the same months the following unless they qualiied for government-supplied inancial
year (Mental Health Advisory Team, 2005). Given that or medical assistance. Despite the myriad of readjust-
approximately 181 356 soldiers deployed to Iraq during ment problems experienced by Vietnam combat vet-
2003 (Hoge et al., 2006), the stress casualty rate among erans, US military services only irst implemented
the personel during the initial invasion of Iraq was only ongoing programs to promote post-deployment resili-
527 (0.29%). By any standard, the modern US military ence, such as “Warrior Transition” in the Marine Corps
may be considered highly successful at promoting and and “Battlemind” in the US Army, in the wake of the
maintaining operational resilience. Fort Bragg murders in 2002. Both Warrior Transition
and Battlemind, as initially conceived, are single-
session psychoeducational classes delivered to service
Post-deployment resilience members immediately before or ater they return from
It is likely that returning service members and veterans a theater of war (see Nash et al., 2011). Another method
have always faced signiicant challenges readapting to in use by the military to promote post-deployment
civilian life, and that operational stress has always fol- resilience is a brief “decompression” period in a loca-
lowed them home to their families and communities tion intermediate between war zone and home, such
ater they have participated in armed conlict. hese as the “hird Location Decompression” currently
challenges are clearly described in ancient Greek tra- employed by Canadian forces (Marin, 2004), among
gedies (Shay, 1994, 2002) and in the diaries and letters of others. Perhaps most important for post-deployment

240
Chapter 16: The stress continuum model

resilience is the provision by military organizations of from both operational and post-deployment resilience
ongoing support for their own service members and because the methods available for promoting it, and the
veterans by peers and leaders, as well as a full range of possible metrics for assessing it, may be very diferent.
social and health programs. Notable in this regard is Observable post-exposure behaviors, for example, may
the US Marine Corps’ Marine for Life program (http:// be very poor indicators of levels of psychological resili-
www.m4l.usmc.mil). ence because even extreme distress may be invisible
Metrics suggesting a lack or deiciency of post- to all but the most intimate of social contacts, and the
deployment resilience may be drawn from the rates of impacts of physical, psychological, social, or spiritual
occurrence of a number of objective post-deployment changes resulting from stressor exposures may be long
behaviors. Examples include post-deployment vehicle delayed. By deinition, psychological resilience is more
accidents, substance misuse or abuse, family violence, than merely meeting minimum standards of behav-
divorce, and post-deployment suicide. How suc- ior during circumscribed periods of time. To a much
cessful has the US military been at promoting post- greater extent than operational or post-deployment
deployment resilience indexed this way? A survey of resilience, psychological resilience must be viewed as a
recent news articles on the subject gives anecdotal evi- process rather than a state or trait, and studied longitu-
dence that a lot more work needs to be done in this area. dinally rather than in cross-section.
For example, in an eerie echo of the 2002 Fort Bragg he military has only recently focused its resilience-
murders, three female soldiers were killed in 2008 by building eforts on the promotion of psychological
their military husbands or boyfriends, also at Fort resilience, at least in part because of its long tradition
Bragg (Burleigh, 2008). A series of homicides at Fort of viewing the qualities that contribute to operational
Carson, Colorado, between November 2008 and May resilience as suicient to also ensure long-term psy-
2009, triggered an epidemiological investigation (US chological health and well-being in service members
Army Center for Health Promotion and Preventive ater they returned home. In the traditional warrior
Medicine, 2009). Less anecdotal evidence for a cur- ethos, qualities like courage and fortitude have been
rent gap in post-deployment resilience promotion also seen as the primary, if not sole, determinants of psy-
comes from the news media, oten quoting govern- chological resilience as well as resilience on the battle-
ment reports, regarding the rising rates in the military ield. he logical lip-side of this belief has been that
of suicide, substance abuse, domestic violence, and those who lacked psychological resilience ater return-
divorce (e.g., Anon, 2009; Stewart, 2009; Tyson, 2009). ing from a theater of war must also have been lacking
Unfortunately, data regarding the prevalence of these in courage and fortitude (Nash, 2007b). Such attitudes
post-deployment problems in military populations are might lead to the conclusion that stress disorders such
sparse in the current professional medical and psycho- as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are not legit-
logical literature. Nevertheless, it is clear that current imate illnesses or injuries, even among those who pro-
gaps exist in the promotion of post-deployment resili- vide healthcare in the military. In a recent survey of a
ence in the military. self-selected but large sample of convenience of 310
army healthcare providers at Fort Hood, Texas, 18%
endorsed having little or no conidence that PTSD was
Psychological resilience a “real illness caused by military service” (Stahl, 2009).
Psychological resilience, as deined here, is most closely Regardless of attitudes and traditional beliefs, the
related to the concept of resilience commonly found goal of psychological resilience programs must be to
in the mental health literature – that is, the ability to prevent long-term psychological distress or dysfunc-
maintain a stable, healthy level of psychological and tion and, instead, to encourage psychological health,
physical functioning despite exposure to losses, poten- strength, and well-being. hese are the clear goals set
tially traumatic events, or other extreme stressors (e.g., for US military psychological health programs by the
Bonanno, 2004). Since levels of psychological and phys- Department of Defense Task Force on Mental Health,
ical functioning must strongly inluence behaviors and chartered by Congress in 2006. In its report back to
role performance both during and ater deployment, Congress in 2007, the Task Force established four over-
psychological resilience is fundamental to the other arching goals for military psychological health pro-
two aspects of resilience discussed above. But psycho- grams: (1) build a culture of support for psychological
logical resilience deserves to be discussed separately health in the military; (2) ensure the availability to

241
Section 4: Specific challenges

service members and their families of a full continuum for use in the US Navy as the OSC program (Marine
of excellent care; (3) provide suicient resources to Corps Combat Development Command & Navy
achieve these ends; and (4) empower line military Warfare Development Command, 2010). Although
leaders to plan and coordinate integrated prevention, these two service-speciic programs difer somewhat,
identiication, and treatment eforts (US Department they share common goals, guiding assumptions, core
of Defense Task Force on Mental Health, 2007). concepts, and tools for intervention. he presentation
How well has the military met the psychological of the program is structured in a way that we feel can
resilience challenge? Does the low rate of battleield lead to measurement strategies, testable predictions,
combat stress casualties during the current conlicts and empirical research, and can hopefully serve as a
in Iraq and Afghanistan signal a low rate of mental model for the ield.
disorders among its veterans? According to recent epi-
demiological studies, psychological resilience remains Program overview and goals
a signiicant problem for the military. he rate of PTSD
he COSC and OSC programs are comprehensive,
among service members and veterans who have served
system-wide occupational stress management pro-
in the wars in Iraq and Afghanistan is in the range
grams whose primary goal is to promote resilience of all
10–18%, and the prevalence does not diminish over
the three types described above, including operational
time (Litz & Schlenger, 2009). In addition, PTSD is but
resilience during deployments, post-deployment
one of many adverse mental health outcomes possible
resilience ater returning, and psychological resilience
in those exposed to combat; others include depressive,
throughout military careers and beyond. herefore,
anxiety, and substance use disorders. Another source of
COSC and OSC target wellness and the prevention of
data regarding psychological resilience in the military
mental disorders rather than the enhancement of per-
is the reports of the US Army’s MHAT teams, char-
formance. A secondary goal for these programs is to
tered by the Army Surgeon General every year since
provide a common language and framework to inte-
2003 to assess the mental health of soldiers currently
grate all resilience and recovery eforts within US Navy
deployed to Iraq and Afghanistan. he MHAT stud-
and Marine Corps units under the leadership of military
ies have reported that among US Army soldiers cur-
commanders and their chains of command. he COSC
rently deployed to Iraq, the prevalence of signiicant
and OSC have been developed and implemented step-
mental disorder symptoms, whether of post-traumatic
wise, beginning approximately in 1999.
stress, depression, or anxiety, has varied from a low of
12.6% in 2005 to a high of 20% in 2007 (Mental Health
Advisory Team, 2003, 2005, 2006a, 2006b, 2008, 2009). Guiding assumptions and rationale
Year-to-year variations correlated well with recent lev- he following are the key underlying assumptions and
els of combat exposure and other risk factors rather the rationales that guide the COSC and OSC programs,
than in response to institutional resilience building or along with the rationale for each.
other intervention programs . Assumption 1. he stress states lie along a broad
spectrum for members of the military services,
from wellness and thriving, at one end, to illness
The US Navy and Marine Corps and disability, at the other, with important
approach to resilience and recovery intermediate stress states signaling varying levels
Every military service branch has responded to the of risk for role impairment and long-term mental
resilience challenges described above by developing disorders. here are many more possible stress
and ielding new programs designed to reduce the outcomes than the extreme states of aliction or
rates of adverse stress outcomes and, instead, to pro- resistance. Furthermore, stress states are not ixed
mote wellness and optimal performance. Rather than over time but evolve as trajectories.
survey these myriad new programs, none of which has Rationale. Epidemiological research has oten
yet been proven efective by well-designed outcome or focused on dichotomous outcomes from stressor
program-evaluation studies, we turn now to a descrip- exposures, such as the presence or absence of
tion of one particular approach to resilience and recov- PTSD, for example. However, when symptom
ery that was recently developed in the US Marine burden among stress-exposed service members
Corps as its COSC program, and subsequently adapted and veterans is assessed using continuous rather

242
Chapter 16: The stress continuum model

than dichotomous measures, it is clear that levels support the efectiveness of indicated prevention
of clinical and subclinical distress and dysfunction interventions, which target individual subclinical
are distributed broadly at any point in time distress and dysfunction, rather than selective or
(Shalev, 2002; Bonanno et al., 2006), and that universal prevention interventions targeting high-
symptoms change along identiiable trajectories as risk groups or entire populations, respectively
time progresses (e.g., Bonanno, 2004; Dickstein (Feldner et al., 2007). Furthermore, since even
et al., 2010). so-called resilient individuals oten experience
Assumption 2. Regardless of how strong, capable, at least brief periods of subclinical distress and
and well prepared someone is prior to stressor dysfunction in the atermath of trauma or loss
exposure, anyone can be stressed beyond their (Bonanno, 2004), and early symptom burden is a
adaptive capacity. Given suicient exposure marker of risk for future pathology (Shalev, 2002),
to stressors, many individuals will express recovery may be more relevant to prevention
at least subclinical distress and dysfunction, eforts than resilience.
even though few will develop chronic mental Assumption 4. Whereas certain aspects of resilience
disorders. are determined by pre-existing individual traits
Rationale. Pre-exposure preparation programs have and capabilities, promoting resilience of all
not yet been found to efectively prevent adverse types throughout the lives and careers of service
stress outcomes such as PTSD in trauma-exposed members and their families is only possible by
individuals (Whealin et al., 2008). In studies of leveraging all available systemic resources, including
risk and resilience factors for PTSD, among the those in the physical, psychological, social, and
greatest determinants of mental health outcomes spiritual domains. One of the resources that is
have been the cumulative burden of stressor most crucial for the promotion and maintenance
exposures (e.g., Bonanno et al., 2007; King et al., of resilience in military units and families is
2008) and peri-exposure psychological processes their organic leadership. he responsibility for
(Ozer et al., 2003), rather than pre-exposure resilience and recovery programs in the military
factors. he inding by army researchers in World resides with line leaders at all levels; it cannot be
War II that every soldier had a breaking point delegated to medical, mental health, or religious
deined by duration of continuous exposure to ministry personnel.
combat (Appel & Beebe, 1946) has been echoed Rationale. he early literature on resilience
by the indings of recent researchers that the risk correlated the ability to bounce back quickly
for mental disorder symptoms increases with each from adversity with individual risk and
month of deployment (Mental Health Advisory protective factors such as age, gender, ethnicity,
Team, 2008). and enduring personality traits such as hardiness
Assumption 3. To be maximally efective, mental (Kobasa et al., 1982; Tat et al., 1999). However,
disorder prevention programs in the military evidence has accrued that positive outcomes
must intervene not only before service members in the face of adversity depend on complex
are exposed to potentially toxic stressors, but also interactions between the individual and the
ater they develop even subclinical levels of distress environment, with signiicant roles played by
or dysfunction in the context of trauma, loss, or availability of resources and social systems such
other extreme stress. In order to provide targeted as families, communities, and cultures (e.g., Tat
interventions for symptomatic individuals, et al., 1999; Waller, 2001; Hobfoll et al., 2009).
prevention must also include efective methods
for identifying who, at any given moment, is
symptomatic. Military psychological health Core concept: the stress continuum model
programs must promote both resilience and A prerequisite for implementing an organization-wide
recovery. efort in the military to promote resilience and recov-
Rationale. Using the nomenclature endorsed ery, and to prevent adverse stress outcomes, is a lan-
by the Institute of Medicine Committee on guage and classiication system for stress that can be
Prevention of Mental Disorders (Mrazek & employed equally by military leaders, service members,
Haggerty, 1994), the greatest evidence exists to chaplains, family members, and medical and mental

243
Section 4: Specific challenges

health professionals. Every member of the organiza- increase individuals’ resistance to hardship and adver-
tion must hold a similar concept of the stress states sity may be said to “grow the green zone” for them: that
to be promoted or prevented, and each must share a is, increase the range of stress challenges that they are
similar understanding of the words used to describe able to endure without becoming distressed or dys-
these states. Such commonalities of language and con- functional in any signiicant way. Military training,
ception are not easily achieved among the many stake- social cohesion, and leadership are also engineered
holders of resilience and stress prevention in a military to enhance the ability of service members to bounce
organization, given the many cultural, educational, quickly back to the green zone once the source of per-
and professional diferences that divide them. To turbing stress has been removed.
meet this challenge, the commanding generals of the Attributes and behaviors characteristic of the green
US Marine Corps’ three air–ground–logistics Marine zone include high levels of physical and cognitive per-
Expeditionary Forces (MEFs) convened a working formance, remaining calm and steady both physically
group in 2007 consisting of Marine leaders, chap- and emotionally, sustained conidence in oneself and
lains, and medical and mental health professionals. others, and behaving ethically and morally. Other green
he charter for this Tri-MEF COSC Working Group zone characteristics include receiving adequate and
charged it with developing a multidisciplinary but restful sleep, maintaining proper nutrition and phys-
leader-oriented, organization-wide, stigma-reducing ical itness, retaining a sense of humor, and remaining
approach to resilience, wellness, and prevention that engaged socially and spiritually.
was consistent with both the warrior ethos and current he green zone is the zone of stress resistance rather
science. he result of the Working Group’s delibera- than stress resilience, in that green zone stress, by def-
tions was the combat and operational stress continuum inition, causes only minimal changes in perceived
model, a heuristic that divided the spectrum of possible distress and functional abilities. Individuals in the
stress states into four color-coded zones designated green zone do not need to bounce back because they
green (ready), yellow (reacting), orange (injured), are not signiicantly bent. herein lies the limitation
and red (ill). Although the boundaries between these of the green zone, and the reason that military train-
four stress zones are neither sharp nor easily deined ing intentionally and repeatedly stresses service mem-
by available metrics, the conceptual deinition of each bers well beyond their levels of green zone comfort. As
zone was believed by the multidisciplinary members of with physical training in preparation for athletic com-
the Tri-MEF Working Group to have strong face valid- petition, training must repeatedly and increasingly
ity and be consistent with current evidence and theory challenge individuals slightly beyond their current
(Nash, 2011). Importantly, the line Marine leaders that capacities in order to develop greater strength, power,
commissioned the Tri-MEF Working Group saw the and endurance.
continuum model as a way to integrate and coordinate
the resilience eforts of line commanders and caregivers Reacting: the yellow zone
throughout the organization, throughout unit deploy- he yellow zone is the zone of mild and temporary
ment cycles and throughout the careers of individual distress or changes in functioning owing to stress. By
service members. What follows is a brief description of deinition, yellow zone stress reactions are always tem-
the four zones of the model. porary and reversible since yellow zone stress does
not signiicantly exceed individuals’ coping capaci-
Ready: the green zone ties. Each individual’s yellow zone, therefore, is deined
he green zone is the zone of adaptive coping, optimal by their current level of resilience in body, mind, and
functioning, and personal well-being. he green zone is spirit – their current ability to bounce back from hard-
not the absence of stress, for the lives of marines, sailors, ship and adversity of various levels of intensity. It is
and their family members are seldom without stress, crucial for service members to acquire and maintain
but rather its efective mastery without the experience broad yellow zone stress capacities because the yellow
of signiicant distress or impairment in physical, social, zone is where most operational challenges are met.
or occupational functioning. he ability to remain in Hence, military training repeatedly and intentionally
the green zone under stress, and to return quickly to pushes service members into their yellow zones. While
it once afected by stress, are two crucial aspects of preparing for a war zone deployment, entire military
resilience. Training and experiences of mastery that units are led through yellow zone training challenges

244
Chapter 16: The stress continuum model

of ever-increasing diiculty. Like athletes, waright- cumulative stresses of military operations. Some individ-
ers seek to inish their training regimens at near-peak uals may have behavioral disorders that existed prior to
capacity. deployment or disorders that were irst present during
As deined, yellow zone stress causes subjective dis- deployment, and need BH intervention beyond the inter-
ventions for COSR.
tress, decrements in functioning, or both. Subjective
distress in the yellow zone may include emotions of Implied in this deinition of COSR is the conceptu-
anxiety, fear, anger, or sadness. Yellow zone cognitions alization of combat stress, dating back to the early years
may include worrying or fantasies of either quitting of World War I, as not genuine expressions of injury or
or retaliating against those who are inlicting current illness induced by stress but rather as always temporary
stressors. Changes in physical functioning in the yellow and reversible, except in those individuals weakened by
zone may include high levels of physiological arousal, pre-existing disorders of character or mental function-
causing a rapid heart rate and breathing, sweating, and ing (see Lerner, 2003). he same symptoms of distress
tremulousness. hey may also include diarrhea, nausea, or dysfunction, if experienced somewhere other than
or other physical symptoms of strain. Cognitive changes in a theater of war, might be conceived very diferently,
in the yellow zone are those associated with very high and might receive very diferent treatment. In fact, the
and sustained levels of physiological arousal, such as same symptoms as those considered a normal COSR
distractibility, poorly sustained attention, slowed recall, during deployment might be diagnosed and treated as
and poor problem solving. Behaviors characteristic of PTSD if they were still evident ater the deployment
the yellow zone may include irritability, diiculty fall- ended.
ing asleep, or changes in appetite, levels of enjoyment, he orange zone of “stress injury” was established
motivation, enthusiasm, or social connectedness. to address some of the drawbacks of the traditional
he deining characteristics of yellow zone distress view of combat stress as essentially all yellow zone, no
and changes in functioning are that they are always matter how disabling. In particular, a stress zone was
mild and temporary, and they always disappear com- needed that represented subclinical or pre-clinical lev-
pletely once the source of stress is no longer present. els of distress or dysfunction interposed between the
Yellow zone stress is, by deinition, a level of stress to yellow zone of normal, necessary, and transient stress
which a particular individual is resilient and, therefore, reactions and the more severe and diagnosable mental
able to rebound from without incurring lasting dam- disorders (the red zone). Without a stress zone between
age to body, mind, or spirit. Yellow zone stress does normal and disordered, there could be no target for the
not leave a mental or emotional scar. Of course, as each early indicated prevention interventions that might
individual endures repeated challenges over time, and spell the diference between recovery and chronic
as resources for resilience are depleted, yellow zone disability. Also, the traditional view of psychological
capacities may dwindle and resilience may be progres- resilience as determined solely by the same qualities
sively lost. necessary for functional operational resilience, such as
courage and fortitude, has increased the stigma associ-
Injured: the orange zone ated with being damaged by the stress of military ser-
he orange zone represents the irst signiicant depart- vice, and erected barriers of shame and denial between
ure in the stress continuum model from more trad- injury and care (Nash et al., 2009).
itional views of combat and operational stress, which Orange zone stress is deined as more persistent and
tended to conceive of any and all experiences of distress severe distress or dysfunction resulting from stressors
or dysfunction in the context of military operations as that exceed, in intensity or duration, the functional lim-
normal and transient, regardless of severity or persist- its of individuals’ biological, psychological, social, and
ence. For example, the COSC doctrinal publication spiritual coping machinery. Whereas yellow zone stress
FM4–02–51 deines “combat and operational stress represents a bending under force, orange zone stress is
reaction” (COSR) as follows (US Army, 2006, pp. 1–5): conceived to represent a literal wound in the body, mind,
his term can be applied to any stress reaction in the or spirit caused by stress. If one conceives of an injury
military unit environment. Many reactions look like as a disruption in normal integrity that resulting in a
symptoms of mental illness (such as panic, extreme anx- decrement in normal functioning, then the evidence in
iety, depression, hallucinations), but they are only tran- support of the concept of literal, rather than igurative,
sient reactions to the traumatic stress of combat and the injuries under stress comes from many sources. For

245
Section 4: Specific challenges

example, a disruption of sustaining attachments caused mental disorders, they can only be diagnosed by health
by the death of someone or something cherished may professionals. Nevertheless, commanders, unit leaders,
result in at least short-term, if not enduring, changes peers, and family members can and should be aware of
in self-concept and relationship to the world (Papa the characteristic symptoms of stress illnesses so that
et al., 2008; Prigerson et al., 2009). Similarly, to the they can identify them and make appropriate referrals
extent internalized cognitive schemata form the struc- as soon as possible. he most widely recognized stress
ture of personality and other mental functioning, illness is PTSD, but well-recognized mental disorders
events that violate deeply held assumptions, beliefs, or arising from stressor exposures in vulnerable individ-
values can cause enduring psychological, social, and uals include depressive and anxiety disorders, and sub-
spiritual dysfunction (Janof-Bulman, 1992; Litz et al., stance abuse and dependence.
2009). here is also accruing evidence from both
pre-clinical and clinical studies that severe stress can The five core leader functions for
cause a number of lasting changes in brain structure
and function (e.g., Bremner, 2006; Heim & Nemerof, psychological health
2009; Martin et al., 2009). High levels of cortisol in the he combat and operational stress continuum model
brain seen in stress have been implicated in the loss of described above is broad in its scope, encompassing
glutamate neurons in the prefrontal cortex and hippo- all conceivable responses and outcomes to stress, both
campus in circuits important for the control of physio- for service members and their families. Clearly, no one
logical arousal, emotion, and cognition; these circuits group of individuals can manage the entire stress con-
act through metabolic pathways similar to those tinuum as deined. At let end of the continuum – the
changed in head trauma, stroke, and a wide variety of green and yellow zones – the activities of line leaders
degenerative brain diseases (Kruman & Mattson, 1999; predominate to promote resiliency. Here, universal and
McEwen, 2000, 2008; Giza & Hovda, 2001). selective prevention are paramount. At the far right of
Although the clinical literature has not yet eluci- the continuum, the red zone is the purview of medical
dated the causes and efects of orange zone stress, the and mental health professionals, supported by chap-
lessons of history teach that there are at least four dis- lains and other social and spiritual support personnel.
tinct yet overlapping sources of stress injury: (1) life Individual service members and family members bear
threat, (2) loss, (3) moral compromise, and (4) cumu- responsibility for maintaining their own psychological
lative wear and tear. he stress continuum model, as health across the stress continuum, including build-
employed by the OSC and COSC programs, alerts ing their own resiliency, managing their own stress
leaders and caregivers to the possibility of orange zone reactions, and recognizing and getting help for stress
stress in the wake of speciic events that are potentially injuries and illnesses when needed. To promote resili-
traumatic, potentially morally injurious, or involve the ence, recovery, and reintegration across the stress con-
loss of cherished persons or things. It also draws atten- tinuum, leaders in the US Marine Corps and Navy have
tion to cumulative stress, from all sources over months developed and promulgated a set of ive core leader
or years, as a fourth possible source of stress injury. functions for psychological health: (1) strengthen, (2)
Orange zone stress is a subclinical and pre-clinical mitigate, (3) identify, (4) treat, and (5) reintegrate.
state from which most individuals are expected to
recover (e.g., Shalev, 2002). However, the orange zone Strengthen
is also a marker of risk, both for possible failure of role he irst core function for leaders is to strengthen ser-
performance (loss of operational resilience) and future vice members before they are exposed to operational
mental disorders or behavioral problems (loss of psy- stressors. Individuals enter military service with a set
chological resilience). he risk indexed by orange zone of pre-existing strengths and vulnerabilities based
stress is conceived to persist for an indeinite period of on genetics, prior life experiences, personality style,
time ater acute distress and dysfunction fade. family supports, and a host of other factors that may
be largely immutable. However, centuries of experi-
Ill: the red zone ence in military organizations, as well as a number of
he red zone is the zone of diagnosable mental disor- research studies, have demonstrated that commanders
ders arising in individuals exposed to combat or other of military units can do much to enhance the resilience
operational stressors. Because red zone illnesses are of unit members and their families through a number

246
Chapter 16: The stress continuum model

of universal prevention interventions. hese interven- facing line leaders hoping to strengthen their unit
tions fall into the three broad categories of training, members through training is to deliver training that
unit cohesion, and leadership. is adequately tough and realistic without making it so
Tough, realistic training develops physical and tough that it inlicts orange zone injuries on the train-
mental strength and endurance, enhances warighters’ ing ield. Another challenge is to recognize the limits
conidence in their ability as individuals and as mem- of pre-exposure universal prevention interventions.
bers of units to cope with the challenges they will face, Military psychological health programs must also
and inoculates them to the stressors they will encoun- provide selective interventions for yellow zone stress
ter. Exactly how pre-exposure to stress enhances resili- reactions, and indicated interventions for orange zone
ence is not well understood, but emerging evidence stress injuries.
suggests that both psychological and biological mecha-
nisms are involved. Well-trained service members have Mitigate
lower heart rates and higher levels of neuropeptide Y, he second core function is to mitigate stressors
a neurotransmitter that promotes calmness in the face throughout deployment cycles in order to reduce the
of severe stress (Morgan et al., 2000; Eaton et al., 2007). stress burden placed on service members and their
hey also face familiar challenges with greater coni- families. Optimal mitigation of stress requires balan-
dence and less anxiety-induced loss of mental focus or cing competing priorities. On the one hand is the need
dissociation (Morgan et al., 2001). to intentionally subject service members to stress in
Unit cohesion, deined broadly as mutual trust and order to train and toughen them, and to accomplish
support in a social group, is developed through shar- assigned missions while deployed. On the other hand
ing adversity over time in a group with a stable mem- are the imperatives to reduce or eliminate stressors that
bership. Two-way communication, both horizontally are not essential to training or mission accomplishment
among peers and vertically between leaders and sub- and to ensure adequate sleep, rest, and restoration to
ordinates, is essential to unit cohesion. Most leaders allow recovery between challenges. Resilience, courage,
know how to build cohesive units given enough time and fortitude can be likened to leaky buckets that are
and unit stability, but a too-common challenge is to try constantly being drained by stress. To keep them from
to maintain unit cohesion in the face of rotations into running dry, these buckets must be frequently reilled
and out of the unit, including casualties and combat through the provision of all necessary physical, psy-
replacements. Certainly, the unit rotation policies cur- chological, social, and spiritual resources. Mitigation
rently practiced in the US military are more conducive activities include physical interventions such as ensur-
to unit cohesion than the individual rotations common ing adequate sleep, rest, and nutrition, and maintain-
during the Vietnam era, but individual augmentees and ing physical health and itness. hey also can include
members of reserve or National Guard units may still social and spiritual replenishment activities such as
be disadvantaged regarding this important ingredient shared successes, recreation, religious practices, and
to strengthening. ater-action reviews as a means by which leaders can
Although complex and multifaceted, leadership is reinforce ethical standards and give meaning to sacri-
an essential factor for the strengthening of unit mem- ices and losses. Given the correlation between length
bers and families. Leaders strengthen unit members by of deployment and stress, the most important mitiga-
teaching and inspiring them, keeping them focused on tion strategies may be simply limiting the duration of
mission essentials, instilling conidence, and providing deployments and increasing the length of dwell time
a model of ethical and moral behavior. Another cru- between deployments.
cial way in which leaders enhance the resilience of their
unit members is by providing a resource of courage Identify
and fortitude on which unit members can draw during Since even the best pre- and peri-exposure prevention
times of challenge (Moran, 1967). eforts cannot eliminate all signiicant post-exposure
Military organizations in the USA are currently distress and dysfunction, efective psychological health
investing a great deal in new methods to augment promotion requires continuous monitoring of stres-
pre-exposure strengthening through enhanced it- sors and stress outcomes. Operational leaders must
ness, neurocognitive training, and stress inoculation know the individuals in their units, including their spe-
using sensory immersion trainers. One challenge ciic strengths and weaknesses, and the nature of the

247
Section 4: Specific challenges

challenges they face both in the unit and in their home he irst continuous aid action, check, is merely the
lives. Leaders must recognize when individuals’ coni- imperative to assess and reassess individuals exposed
dence in themselves or their peers or leaders is shaken, to potential orange zone stressors, in order to recognize
or when units have lost cohesion because of casualties, current stress zone, needs, and risks. he check action
changes in leadership, or challenges to the unit. Most takes into account two critical components of the stress
importantly, every unit leader must know which stress continuum model: the diferential risks posed by the
zone each unit member is in at every moment, day to four zones of the stress continuum, and the trajectories
day. Service members cannot be depended upon to over which symptoms change over time. here are few
recognize their own stress reactions, injuries, and ill- settings in which longitudinal assessment is as easy to
nesses, particularly while deployed to operational set- perform by involved leaders, caregivers, and peers as
tings. he external focus of their attention and their in military units, given the closeness and involvement
denial of discomfort, necessary to thrive in an arduous that characterizes such organizations. he second con-
environment, make it diicult for them to recognize tinuous aid action, coordinate, makes maximum use
their own stress states. In addition, stigma can be an of the extended support system intrinsic to military
insurmountable barrier to admitting stress problems, organizations by connecting those in need with avail-
once recognized, to someone else. Efective indicated able resources.
prevention depends on leaders, caregivers, peers, and he two primary aid actions, cover and calm, are
family members recognizing orange zone stress in tools for acute crisis response. Cover means to get to
others. For this reason, education and training on stress cover, or to make safe, whatever that may require. Calm
zone identiication is aforded to all personnel groups actions target physiological arousal, emotional inten-
in the US Navy and Marine Corps at various points in sity, and cognitive disorganization. Primary aid is sel-
their careers, particularly before and ater operational dom required, but when it is needed, the need is acute.
deployments. herefore, everyone at every level of the organiza-
tion should be trained in both verbal and non-verbal
Treat actions for primary aid.
he fourth leader function for psychological health Secondary aid actions are designed to promote
promotion is to treat orange and red zone stress once recovery and healing from orange zone stress over a
identiied. he operative verb, “treat,” may imply clin- longer period of time, and they are intended to be per-
ical care provided by a licensed medical or mental formed mostly by senior leaders and caregivers, since
health professional. But in the context of the OSC and they require the greatest communication skills, author-
COSC programs, what is meant is not clinical care but ity, and responsibility. he connect action of COSFA
intervention of any kind, at any level. Viewed in this merely means to promote social support, especially
way, the treatment function is the responsibility of from peers and family members, in the atermath of
everyone in the organization, since efective interven- orange zone stress. he competence action includes all
tions for subclinical states may be provided by almost mentoring, teaching, and counseling activities geared
anyone trained to provide such help. toward the recovery of functional skills in all important
To ill the gap in intervention strategies lying areas, including self-care, social interacting, and occu-
between routine leadership and clinical mental health- pational performance. he inal secondary aid action,
care, the US Navy and Marine Corps developed a set conidence, targets obstacles to self-esteem and hope in
of indicated prevention tools targeting orange zone the wake of orange zone stress, particularly high lev-
stress in operational environments. his interven- els of guilt, shame, or blame. Restoration of conidence
tion is known as combat and operational stress irst aid oten depends on more realistic self-appraisal, but it
(COSFA) and provides evidence-informed strategies also may require forgiveness of self and others for per-
to promote recovery (see Hobfoll et al., 2007) in the ceived failures.
hands of line leaders, non-mental healthcaregivers, he treatment function of the COSC and OSC pro-
and others (Nash et al., 2008). he COSFA intervention grams also encompasses referral, consultation, and
includes seven actions, divided into three levels of care: case management to ensure higher levels of care are
(1) continuous aid (check and coordinate), (2) primary obtained when they are needed. Finally, this treatment
aid (cover and calm), and (3) secondary aid (connect, function challenges leaders at all levels to remove the
competence, and conidence). obstacles to care posed by stigma in all its forms.

248
Chapter 16: The stress continuum model

Reintegrate here is a broad need for program evaluation


he inal leader function for psychological health, research across the spectrum of resilience training
reintegrate, seeks to conserve precious personnel eforts in the military. We argue that useful research
resources by returning individuals to duty as soon and can only come from cogent and well-articulated con-
as completely as possible as they recover and heal from ceptual frameworks. We urge decision makers, devel-
stress. he reintegration imperative challenges military opers, and researchers to lay out the goals, assumptions,
line leaders and medical support personnel to continu- and rationale for each component of their respective
ously monitor the functional capacities and self-coni- models, as we have done in this chapter. his will help
dence of service members as they recover, and to apply to address the big challenges in the ield, which are to
rational standards to decisions regarding itness for operationally deine resilience; to gain a consensus
duty and worldwide deployability. For stress-afected about what “pre-clinical” states of distress or dysfunc-
individuals to be efectively reintegrated into their units, tion entail, and how to measure these states; to agree
stigma must be continuously addressed, and the coni- on the key constructs of interest and to derive or cull
dence of both the stress-afected person and their peers measures of various predictor variables and outcomes;
and small unit leaders must be restored. his process to conduct longitudinal research so that causal infer-
may take months to bring to successful conclusion. In ences can be generated about risk and resilience factors
those cases in which substantial recovery and return to in the military; and to use this information to conduct
full duty is not anticipated, the challenge for operational ongoing program evaluation.
commanders is to assist service members as they transi-
tion to civilian life and Veterans Afairs care. References
Anon. (2009). Divorces rising in military. New York
Conclusions Times, November 28, 2009, http://www.nytimes.
com/2009/11/28/us/28brfs-DIVORCESRISI_BRF.html
he US Navy and Marine Corps have developed paral- (accessed January 8, 2010).
lel programs (COSC and OSC) to promote resiliency
Appel, J. W. & Beebe, G. W. (1946). Preventive psychiatry: an
and recovery in their members, not only to preserve epidemiologic approach. Journal of the American Medical
operational functional capacities but also to promote Association, 131, 1469–1475.
post-deployment functioning and long-term health Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience:
and well-being. he conceptual foundation for these Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive
programs is as the combat and operational stress con- ater extremely aversive events? American Psychologist,
tinuum model, a heuristic that promotes communica- 59, 20–28.
tion and integration of resilience and recovery eforts Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D.
throughout the organization. he model classiies (2006). Psychological resilience ater disaster: New York
stress states in four color-coded stress zones, each rep- City in the atermath of the September 11th terrorist
resenting a diferent level of risk for failure of role per- attack. Psychological Science, 17, 181–186.
formance and a future mental disorder. Leaders in the Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D.
US Navy and Marine Corps bear responsibility for sev- (2007). What predicts psychological resilience ater
eral core functions necessary to promote psychological disaster? he role of demographics, resources, and life
stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75,
health across the stress continuum. Unique to these 671–682.
programs and the stress continuum model on which
Bremner, J. D. (2006). Stress and brain atrophy. CNS and
they are based is a major focus on identifying subclin-
Neurological Disorders, 5, 503–512.
ical or pre-clinical stress injuries in order to apply indi-
Burleigh, N. (2008). he Fort Bragg murders. People, 70,
cated prevention interventions when needed.
December 15, 2008, http://www.people.com/people/
Although the COSC and OSC programs were archive/article/0,20245668,00.html (accessed January 8,
devised based on available evidence, the programs have 2010).
yet to be proven efective by well-designed outcome or Dean, E. T. (1997). Shook over hell: Post-traumatic stress,
program evaluation studies. Program evaluation stud- Vietnam, and the civil war. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
ies are needed to validate the major components of University Press.
these programs, including the stress continuum model, Dickstein, B. D., Suvak, M., Stein, N., Adler, A. B., & Litz, B. T.
the core leader functions, and COSFA. (2010). Examining variability in the natural course of

249
Section 4: Specific challenges

PTSD: predictors of symptom trajectory. Journal of Litz, B. T. & Schlenger, W. E. (2009). PTSD in service
Traumatic Stress, 23, 331–339. members and new veterans of the Iraq and Afghanistan
Eaton, K., Sallee, F. R., & Sah, R. (2007). Relevance of wars: a bibliography and critique. PTSD Research
neuropeptide Y (NPY) in psychiatry. Current Topics in Quarterly, 20, 1–7.
Medicinal Chemistry, 7, 1645–1659. Litz, B. T., Stein, N., Delaney, E., et al. (2009). Moral injury
Feldner, M. T., Monson, C. M., & Friedman, M. J. (2007). and moral repair in war veterans: A preliminary model
A critical analysis of approaches to targeted PTSD and intervention strategy. Clinical Psychology Review, 29,
prevention: Current status and theoretically derived 695–706.
future directions. Behavior Modiication, 31, 80–115. Marin, A. (2004). From tents to sheets: an analysis of the,
French, S. E. (2003). Code of the warrior: Exploring warrior C.F. experience with third location decompression ater
values past and present. Lanham, MD: Rowman & deployment. [Report from the Ombudsman, Canadian
Littleield. Forces, July 29, 2004.] Ottawa: Ombudsman for the
Canadian Forces, http://www.ombudsman.forces.gc.ca/
Giza, C. C. & Hovda, D. A. (2001). he neurometabolic rep-rap/sr-rs/tld-dtl/app-ann-01-eng.asp (accessed
cascade of concussion. Journal of Athletic Training, 36, January 18, 2010).
228–235.
Marine Corps Combat Development Command & Navy
Heim, C. & Nemerof, C. B. (2009). Neurobiology of Warfare Development Command (2010). Combat and
posttraumatic stress disorder. CNS Spectrums, operational stress control. [MCRP 6–11C/NTTP 1–15M.]
14(Suppl. 1), 13–24. Quantico, VA: Marine Corps Combat Development
Hobfoll, S. E., Watson, P., Bell, C. C., et al. (2007). Five essential Command.
elements of immediate and mid-term mass trauma Martin, E. I., Ressler, K. J., Binder, E., & Nemerof, C. B.
intervention: Empirical evidence. Psychiatry, 70, 283–315. (2009). he neurobiology of anxiety disorders: brain
Hobfoll, S. E., Palmieri, P. A., Johnson, R. J., et al. (2009). imaging, genetics, and psychoneuroendocrinology.
Trajectories of resilience, resistance, and distress during Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 32, 549–575.
ongoing terrorism: he case of Jews and Arabs in
McEwen, B. S. (2000). he neurobiology of stress: from
Israel. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77,
serendipity to clinical relevance. Brain Research, 886,
138–148.
172–189.
Hoge, C. W., Auchterlonie, J. L., & Milliken, C. S. (2006).
McEwen, B. S. (2008). Central efects of stress hormones in
Mental health problems, use of mental health services,
health and disease: Understanding the protective and
and attrition from military service ater returning
damaging efects of stress and stress mediators. European
from deployment to Iraq or Afghanistan. Journal of the
Journal of Pharmacology, 583, 174–185.
American Medical Association, 2295,1023–1032.
Meagher, R. E. (2006). Herakles gone mad: Rethinking
Janof-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a
heroism in an age of endless war. Northampton, MA:
new psychology of trauma. New York: Free Press.
Olive Branch Press.
King, L. A., King, D. W., Bolton, E. E., Knight, J. A., &
Vogt, D. S. (2008). Risk factors for mental, physical, Mental Health Advisory Team (2003). Operation Iraqi
and functional health in Gulf War veterans. Journal of Freedom (OIF). [Report, December 16, 2003.]
Rehabilitation Research and Development, 45, 395–408. Washington, DC: US Army Medical Department, http://
www.armymedicine.army.mil/reports/mhat/mhat.html
Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., & Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness (accessed August 30, 2009).
and health: a prospective study. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 42, 168–177. Mental Health Advisory Team (2005). Operation Iraqi
Freedom (MHAT-II). [Report, January 30, 2005.]
Kruman, I. I. & Mattson, M. P. (1999). Pivotal role of Washington, DC: US Army Medical Department, http://
mitochondrial calcium uptake in neural cell apoptosis www.armymedicine.army.mil/reports/mhat/mhat.html
and necrosis. Journal of Neurochemistry, 72, 529–540. (accessed August 30, 2009).
Lerner, P. (2003). Hysterical men: War, psychiatry, and the Mental Health Advisory Team (2006a). Operation Iraqi
politics of trauma in Germany, 1890–1930. Ithaca, NY: Freedom 04–06 (MHAT-III). [Report, May 29, 2006.]
Cornell University Press. Washington, DC: US Army Medical Department, http://
Litz, B. T. (2005). Has resilience to severe trauma been www.armymedicine.army.mil/reports/mhat/mhat.html
underestimated? American Psychologist, 60, 262. (accessed January 18, 2010).
Litz, B. T. (2007). Research on the impact of military trauma: Mental Health Advisory Team (2006b). Operation Iraqi
Current status and future directions. Military Psychology, Freedom 05–07 (MHAT-IV). [Final Report, December 17,
19, 217–238. 2006.] Washington, DC: US Army Medical Department,

250
Chapter 16: The stress continuum model

http://www.armymedicine.army.mil/reports/mhat/ & M. Friedman (eds.), Posttraumatic stress reactions:


mhat.html (accessed January 18, 2010). Caring for the veterans of the global war on terror
Mental Health Advisory Team (2008). Operation Iraqi (pp. 193–214). New York: Guilford Press.
Freedom 06–08: Iraq, Operation Enduring Freedom 8. Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2003).
[Afghanistan Report February 14, 2008.] Washington, Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and
DC: US Army Medical Department, http://www. symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychological
armymedicine.army.mil/reports/mhat/mhat.html Bulletin, 129, 52–73.
(accessed August 30, 2009). Papa, A., Neria, Y., & Litz, B. T. (2008). Traumatic bereavement
Mental Health Advisory Team (2009). Operation Iraqi in war veterans. Psychiatric Annals, 38, 686–691.
Freedom 07–09 (MHAT-VI). [Report, May 8, 2009.] Phang, S. E. (2008). Roman military service: Ideologies of
Washington, DC: US Army Medical Department, http:// discipline in the late republic and early principate. New
www.armymedicine.army.mil/reports/mhat/mhat.html York: Cambridge University Press.
(accessed August 30, 2009).
Prigerson, H. G., Horowitz, M. J., Jacobs, S. C., et al. (2009).
Moran, C. M. W. (1967). he anatomy of courage. Boston: Prolonged grief disorder: Psychometric validation
Houghton Milin. [Original published in 1945.] of criteria proposed for DSM-V and ICD-11. PLoS
Morgan, C. A., Wang, S., Mason, J., et al. (2000). Hormone Medicine, 6, e1000121.
proiles in humans experiencing military survival Shalev, A. Y. (2002). Acute stress reactions in adults.
training. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 891–901. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 532–543.
Morgan, C. A., Hazlett, G., Wang, S., et al. (2001).
Shay, J. (1994). Achilles in Vietnam: Combat trauma and the
Symptoms of dissociation in humans experiencing
undoing of character. New York: Scribner.
acute, uncontrollable stress: a prospective investigation.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1239–1247. Shay, J. (2002). Odysseus in America: Combat trauma and the
trials of homecoming. New York: Scribner.
Mrazek, P. J. & Haggerty, R. J. (eds.) (1994). Reducing risks
for mental disorders: Frontiers for preventive intervention Stahl, S. M. (2009). Crisis in army psychopharmacology
research. Washington, DC: National Academies Press and mental health care at Fort Hood. CNS Spectrums, 14,
for the Committee on Prevention of Mental Disorders, 677–684.
Institute of Medicine. Starr, B. (2002). Fort Bragg killings raise alarm about stress:
Nash, W. P. (2007a). he stressors of war. In C. R. Figley & No connection established to assailant’s Afghanistan duty.
W. P. Nash (eds.), Combat stress injuries: heory, research, CNN.com, July 27, 2002, http://archives.cnn.com/2002/
and management (pp. 11–31). New York: Routledge. US/07/26/army.wives (accessed January 8, 2010).
Nash, W. P. (2007b). Combat/operational stress adaptations Stewart, P. (2009). US Army suicides set to hit new high in
and injuries. In C. R. Figley & W. P. Nash (eds.), Combat 2009. Reuters, November 17, 2009, http://www.reuters.
stress injuries: heory, research, and management com/article/idUSN1752246 (accessed January 8, 2010).
(pp. 33–63). New York: Routledge. Tat, C. T., Stern, A. S., King, L. A., & King, D. A. (1999).
Nash, W. P. (2011). US Marine Corps and Navy combat and Modeling physical health and functional health status:
operational stress continuum model: A tool for leaders. In the role of combat exposure, posttraumatic stress
E. C. Ritchie (ed.), Operational behavioral health (pp. 193– disorder, and personal resource attributes. Journal of
204). Washington, DC: Borden Institute. Traumatic Stress, 12, 3–23.
Nash, W. P., Westphal, R. J., Watson, P., & Litz, B. T. (2008). Tyson, A. S. (2009). Army’s suicide rate “horrible,”
Combat and operational stress irst aid (COSFA): general says. Washington Post, November 18, 2009,
A toolset for military leaders. In Proceedings of the http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/
Psychological Health and Traumatic Brain Injury Warrior article/2009/11/17/AR2009111703426.html (accessed
Resilience Conference of the Defense Centers of Excellence, January 8, 2010).
Fairfax, VA, November 2008. US Army (2006). Combat and operational stress control. [FM
Nash, W. P., Silva, C., & Litz, B. T. (2009). he historical 4–02.51.] Washington, DC: Department of the Army,
origins of military and veteran mental health stigma, http://www.fas.org/irp/doddir/army/fm4–02–51.pdf
and the stress injury model as a means to reduce it. (accessed February 17, 2011).
Psychiatric Annals, 39, 789–794. US Army Center for Health Promotion and Preventive
Nash, W. P., Krantz, L., Stein, N., Westphal, R. J., & Litz, B. Medicine (2009). Epidemiologic consultation 14-HK-
(2011). Two approaches to meeting the challenges OB1U-09: Investigation of homicides at Fort Carson,
of mental health prevention in the military: Army Colorado, November 2008–May 2009. Washington, DC:
Battlemind and the Navy–Marine Corps Stress US Army Center for Health Promotion and Preventive
Continuum. In J. Ruzek, J. Vasterling, P. Schnurr, Medicine, http://www.armymedicine.army.mil/reports/

251
Section 4: Specific challenges

FinalRedactedEpiconReport14July2009.pdf (accessed Waller, M. A. (2001). Resilience in ecosystemic context:


January 18, 2010). evolution of the concept. American Journal of
US Department of Defense Task Force on Mental Health Orthopsychiatry, 71, 290–297.
(2007). An achievable vision: Report of the Department of Whealin, J. M., Ruzek, J. I., & Southwick, S. (2008).
Defense Task Force on Mental Health. Falls Church, VA: Cognitive behavioral theory and preparation for
Defense Health Board. professionals at risk for trauma exposure. Trauma,
von Clausewitz, C. (1982). On war. New York: Penquin Violence, and Abuse, 9, 100–113.
Books. [Original published in 1832.]

252
Section
Section4 Specific challenges
Chapter
Resilience in the face of terrorism: linking

17 resource investment with engagement


Stevan E. Hobfoll, Brian Hall, Katie J. Horsey, and Brittain E. Lamoureux

Introduction and indirect exposure takes a heavy toll on such


Since the terrorist attacks in Washington and New populations (Stein et al., 2004; Somer et al., 2005;
York on September 11, 2001 (9/11), there have been a Shalev et al., 2006).
lurry of studies on the impact of terrorism and war. At the same time, there is new interest in how people
Not surprisingly, research has focused on the negative may respond resiliently in the face of terrorism and war.
health and mental health impact of these events. In par- Bonanno and colleagues (Bonanno, 2005; Bonanno
ticular, research has focused on how direct and indir- et al., 2005, 2006, 2007) noted that following the attacks
ect exposure to terrorism and war relate to increased on the World Trade Center the majority of individuals
post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and depression did not develop signiicant symptoms, and even those
(Galea et al., 2002; Bleich et al., 2003; Panamaki et al., who initially showed signs of disorder recovered fairly
2005; Hobfoll et al., 2006a, 2006b; Agronick et al., 2007). quickly. Likewise, Norris et al. (2007) and Layne et al.
he major goal of terrorism in particular, and in many (2007) have encouraged the discussion and study of
senses the purpose of war, is to impact the enemy’s what resiliency might look like and how it might bet-
civilian population psychologically to motivate them ter be studied following mass casualty. Such interest is
to capitulate to demands, or to make them psycho- not new as the study of resiliency has long interested
logically sufer for perceived wrongs they have done. psychology and psychiatry. Several seminal theorists
Indeed, this second goal of terrorism, to inlict harm who witnessed the events of World War II, including
without a clear political goal, is increasingly the stance concentration camps, and the following period noted
taken by terrorists. Whether it is Hamas, Hezbollah, Al the importance of salutogenic processes (Frankl, 1963;
Qa’ida or the Tamil Tigers, it is not always clear what Caplan, 1964), but their work was seldom matched
they are actually asking for, or if there is agreement with empirical study, even if their ideas were rich and
among them as to what “victory” would even look like. potentially heuristic.
Rather, they oten strike out to cause the “enemy” to Several recent studies have examined resilience in
sufer in exchange for their sufering. the face of terrorism and war. Galea et al. (2002), in their
he study of traumatic psychological injury has examination of Manhattan residents following the 9/11
continued value. In particular, we know little about terrorist attacks, found that over 40% did not report
how people react to ongoing terrorism (Bleich et al., any PTSD symptoms. Examining the same sample,
2003; Hobfoll et al., 2006a), circumstances where Bonanno et al. (2006) noted that 65.1% of their sample
populations must quickly evacuate (Palmieri et al., reported no or one symptom of PTSD in the six months
2008), or to diferent kinds of attack (shootings, stab- following the World Trade Center attacks. Even among
bings, bioterrorism, etc.). Nor do we know much those highly exposed in this sample, around one third
about instances where attacks are of large scale, even remained resilient using these strict criteria.
though in places like Iraq terrorist attacks have oten his chapter will map out two theoretical frame-
been of major, repeated scale. Studies of national works that have been employed in diferent areas of
samples in regions of ongoing terrorism and war are stress research and apply them to an understanding of
particularly important to study because both direct resiliency in the face of mass casualty and terrorism in

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

253
Section 4: Specific challenges

particular. One, conservation of resources (COR) theory Conservation of resources theory next states several
(Hobfoll, 1988, 1989, 2001) has been used across the key principles that have been supported today in liter-
continuum of levels of stress. his theory has been par- ally hundreds of studies of stress and trauma (Hobfoll
ticularly applied to traumatic stress (Hobfoll, 1991) & Lilly, 1993; Hobfoll, 2001):
and has been used to aid an understanding of disaster principle 1: the primacy of resource loss
(Freedy et al., 1994; Norris & Kaniasty, 1996; Benight principle 2: resource investment
et al., 1999) and terrorism (Hobfoll et al., 2006a, 2007; principle 3: the salience of gain under situations of
Hall et al., 2008; Palmieri et al., 2008). Engagement resource loss.
theory, in contrast, has been most successfully applied
to work-related stress (Schaufeli et al., 2002), and is a hese principles give rise to three corollaries, which
major emerging theory within the positive psychology will be described as they arise.
movement (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). he
two theories it extremely well, in part, because engage- Principle 1: the primacy of resource loss
ment thinking was informed by COR theory, but also he irst principle of COR theory is that resource loss
because COR theory speaks to how stress operates on is disproportionately more salient than resource gain.
individuals, groups, and communities and engage- Resources include object resources (e.g., car, house),
ment theory speaks to outcomes that are related to condition resources (e.g., employment, marriage), per-
human resiliency in the face of challenge. his difers sonal resources (e.g., key skills and personal traits such
greatly from how resiliency has generally been exam- as self-eicacy and self-esteem), and energy resources
ined, which is most oten in terms of the lack of the (e.g., credit, knowledge, money). he disproportion-
psychopathology or resiliency-related resources that ate impact of resource loss is seen in both the degree
help to prevent or limit psychopathology. Instead, and the speed of impact, as losses have large impact
engagement theory speaks to positive outcomes that and typically also have rapid impact. It might appear at
are true markers of resilient, positive responding. odds to again emphasize resource loss when this chap-
ter focuses on resiliency. However, stating the primacy
Conservation of resources theory of loss emphasizes the main task of resilient respond-
and resiliency ing, that is to ofset the powerful, usually rapid, and
oten long-term impact of resource loss.
Conservation of resources theory is based on several Resource loss is primary across all stress levels.
principles and corollaries that must be delineated to However, there are both quantitative and qualitative
understand the theory and move forward in applying diferences under conditions of traumatic stress, and
it to mass casualty responding and terrorism and war more speciically when trauma occurs communally
responding in particular. Unlike other stress theories, in situations of mass trauma (Hobfoll, 1991, 1998).
COR theory emphasizes the centrality of both loss and First, the process of resource loss following terrorism
gain cycles, and an understanding of both is critical to and war occurs with even greater rapidity, as people’s
understanding potential resiliency. world can literally be altered fundamentally and at core
he theory begins with the tenet that individuals levels of life, family, and society. Further, as terrorism
strive to obtain, retain, foster, and protect those things and war occur on the level of community, the extent of
they centrally value. hey employ key resources in resource breakdown afects the full continuum of soci-
order to conduct this self, social, and societal regula- ety, resulting in decrements of reserves for both indi-
tion. his tenet places COR theory in the context of a viduals and individual families, and across families and
motivational theory that goes beyond stress and chal- organizations. Indeed, if great enough or chronic, the
lenge, suggesting instead that this is the normal course breakdown may deplete the reserves established for
of human responding. It needs also be stated that what treatment and intervention, which can become quickly
is centrally valued is universal and includes health, overwhelmed.
well-being, peace, family, self-preservation, and a posi-
tive sense of self, even if the core elements of sense of
self difer culturally. his also means that humans will Principle 2: resource investment
exist in, build, foster, and protect social and societal he second principle of COR theory is that people must
systems that enable these same valued ends. invest resources in order to protect against resource

254
Chapter 17: Resilience in the face of terrorism

loss, recover from losses, and gain resources. he irst withhold resource reserves, and consequently tend to
corollary of COR theory relates to this: be conservative in their resource investment. Even in
corollary 1: those with greater resources are less instances of massive loss, there is motivation and logic
vulnerable to resource loss and more capable of in withholding resources for the unknown challenges
orchestrating resource gain; conversely, those with that are yet to come. For those who are resource rich,
fewer resources are more vulnerable to resource the threat to their reserves will require a greater degree
loss and less capable of resource gain. of loss or more long-standing loss. Yet, terrorism and
war, if directly impacting even those who are resource
he principle and the corollary related to resource rich, can result in depletion of the resources that might
investment and gain are central to any understanding have contributed to gain cycles. For those who lack
of resiliency. hey posit that resiliency is an active pro- basic resource reserves on social, economic, or emo-
cess and one that demands personal, social, and soci- tional levels, this situation will be more immediate and
etal resources. It is a balancing act and demands hard may even occur on note of the threat of terrorism or
work in the service of coping, particularly in situations disaster, before losses actually occur.
such as terrorism and war, where there are oten signii- his suggests that one of the major challenges of
cant and rapid losses and oten no clear end to threat. resiliency processes will be to respond with resource
Given that terrorism and war can result in profound investment at major levels, which implies having
resource loss, the means of protection of resources the resources and the willingness to spend or invest
or their reinstatement in the aid of resiliency is par- resources. A consequence of this is that those with
ticularly challenging. Indeed, it may oten be beyond major resource reservoirs will be more likely to be resili-
individuals’ self-capacity, at least initially, and require ent and that those who are willing to, or are directed
major inlux of social support or formal therapeutic to, efectively spend and invest resources will do best.
and resource intervention from government and non- Implied in this is a level of strategy of resource invest-
governmental organizations that respond to disaster, ment, and to the extent that terrorism and war cause a
war, and terrorism. his means that the reconstitution challenge at unpracticed ways of coping, the successful
of resources to stabilize individuals and families oten pattern of resiliency responding may have a large cre-
demands resources that are part of functional com- ative element.
munity processes: what Iscoe (1974) called competent
communities. he therapist, intervention apparatus, Principle 3: the salience of gain
and aid agencies are major resources that can provide Principle 3 is paradoxical. Although resource loss is
a variety of support that is outside the armamentarium more potent than resource gain, the salience of gain
of individuals, families, or organizations, and that may increases under situations of resource loss (Hobfoll
act directly or as advocates. et al., 1999). he paradoxical increase in saliency of
Gain cycles that follow large disasters or mass cas- resource gain is accentuated during traumatic situations
ualties and foster resiliency are potentially deceptive. and is a critical insight as to the substance and even the
Kaniasty and Norris (1995) have noted that, follow- counterintuitive strength of resiliency eforts. his fol-
ing a brief period of altruistic sharing of resources, lows because, under conditions of high loss, even eforts
people and social systems may become more neutral that result in small gains may elicit positive expectancy
about sharing resources, or even become competitive and hope, and lead to further goal-directed eforts. In
for resources. Moreover, for those who are most disen- this manner, resource gains that under less-stressful cir-
franchised in a society, the altruistic period of resource cumstances would be appraised as trivial may object-
sharing may not reach them. Families and care agencies ively ofer a lifeline to survival (e.g., “I am not alone,”
may attempt to take care of their own and their com- “Rescuers are on there way,” “I now see what I need to
munities, but if they are cut of from the major bank of do, even if it will be diicult”) or may be imbued with
community resources, their eforts may be insuicient meaning (e.g., “People still care,” “God is with me”).
to sustain resiliency processes. When turning to engagement theory, below, it
he processes of resource gain to aid resiliency are will be increasingly clear how the processes of invest-
also challenged by the nature of resource conservation. ing resources can result in positive outcomes that can
Speciically, COR theory states that individuals, fam- counterbalance and even at times ofset the negative
ilies, and organizations have a strong motivation to impact of traumatic events such as war and terrorism.

255
Section 4: Specific challenges

his is not to romanticize these processes, as resiliency the atermath of the Northridge earthquake, Sattler
may come in tandem with or interlaced with troubling (2006) reported how individuals subject to second-
emotions and thoughts. ary stressors, such as living in shelters, continued to
experience resource loss and loss spirals that were dif-
Resource loss and gain spirals icult to break. Secondary resource strain and loss will
continue to impact psychological outcomes, such as
he irst two principles of COR theory concerning loss
PTSD, and challenge recovery (Pynoos et al., 2004).
primacy and investment of resources, in turn, lead to
Psychosocial resource losses, as well as material losses
two key further corollaries, which pertain to resource
and losses to the community on service and structural
loss and gain spirals (Hobfoll, 1988, 1998):
levels, follow in the wake of initial losses and mediate
corollary 2: those who lack resources are not only more
the relationship between disaster (e.g., lood) expos-
vulnerable to resource loss, but initial loss begets
ure and psychological distress (Smith & Freedy, 2000).
future loss
Because secondary stressors can lead to a taxing of per-
corollary 3: while those who possess resources are sonal resources beyond that of the original trauma, the
more capable of gain and initial resource gain demand on resources continues until resources can be
begets further gain, because loss is more potent gained and loss halted. Seen this way, resiliency is not
than gain, loss cycles will be more impactful and a single state but a trajectory, which must be followed
more accelerated than gain cycles. for individuals.
Understanding these changes the approach to trauma In war zones and where terrorism is ongoing, the
from one that might be more static to a more dynamic potency of resource loss over resource gain is clear,
moving target of challenges that require ongoing eforts illustrating the great challenge to resiliency. At the
in the service of coping and resiliency eforts. same time as individuals, families, organizations, and
In the context of major stress and traumatic com- the society itself make eforts to cope with the changing
munity stress, corollary 2 is paramount (Norris & reality, the losses may continue to mount and prior
Kaniasty, 1996) as loss cycles are central to mass cas- avenues for coping and gaining resources may be inter-
ualty and occur with both force and increasing momen- rupted or lost (Klingman, 2006). War presents a unique
tum. Particularly where people are broadly impacted situation in which resources are lost both as a conse-
beyond individual loss, where many families are quence of attacks and also through their withdrawal
afected and social structures become overburdened for redirection to ighting the enemy. his can impact
or even lost amidst attacks, resource loss cycles can healthcare, transportation, food, medication, social
become overwhelming. For resiliency, this means that support networks, jobs, inances, and basic access to
eforts to halt loss cycles will require great energy, sig- services.
niicant resources, and will be ongoing. In a mass cas- Corollary 3 suggests that resiliency processes will
ualty disaster or terrorist attack, individuals may lose be stretched as losses occur. However, at the same
the very social, personal, and societal resources that time, eforts to strive for resiliency will continue and
are key to their normal coping processes, now required be fostered if resources are available or can be fostered
with accelerating need (Klingman & Cohen, 2004). through informal or formal support eforts.
For example, if a family already under inancial strain Resiliency resources are important in understand-
loses the primary breadwinner, or where the general ing resource gain in the face of trauma. Resiliency
economy has collapsed as in Palestine, trauma results resources again must be considered on the level of
in loss cycles that undermine the structure and avail- individuals, families, and community. Key resiliency
ability of even primary resources such as food, shelter, resources that function to ofset psychological distress
and, in the case of a highly unsafe area, lives. Perhaps and aid well-being include self-eicacy, deeply imbued
even more fundamental in such cycles is the realistic sense of optimism, self-esteem, social support, and
understanding that one cannot even protect one’s fam- higher socioeconomic status and social status in the
ily given the randomness of attack. community. As we turn to engagement theory, below,
Recent research has illustrated the critical nature these will become clearer as they are integral to the
of resource loss spirals subsequent to natural disasters process of engagement, both as a process and in being
or terrorist attacks that have led to many casualties. In engaged as an outcome.

256
Chapter 17: Resilience in the face of terrorism

Defensive responding under conditions informs the potential for understanding the resiliency
process, even in terms of engagement in the face of
of resource lack traumatic stress. Engagement can be deined as a per-
he fourth corollary of COR theory is fundamental for sistent, pervasive, and positive afective–motivational
an understanding of mass casualty trauma: state of fulillment in individuals who are reacting to
corollary 4: those who lack resources are likely to challenging circumstances (Schaufeli et al., 2002).
adopt a defensive posture to conserve their Engagement, in turn, is conceptualized as a product of
resources. three dimensions – vigor, dedication, and absorption.
Dedication is seen as the commitment to key life
Following terrorism and during periods of war and tasks. In the case of terrorism and war, it includes dedi-
the consequent onslaught of resource loss, individuals, cation to family, work, organizations, society and the
families, and organizations will oten adopt a defen- preservation of the self. Absorption is deined as the
sive posture. Under such circumstances a new “logic” sense of full involvement and even excitement over
occurs – the logic of defense. his has been understood life tasks. When we are absorbed in a major challenge,
on the individual level where denial, projection, sub- we oten lose the sense of time, and problem solving is
limation, and rationalization take place, but it is less maximized. In contrast, to the extent that people are
well understood under situations of mass casualties. worried about what might happen or has happened
However, we can be guided by some understanding of they become less absorbed in the task before them and
underlying processes. People tend to follow a defen- are likely to be less capable of performing complex
sive course in a logical sequence when there are known, tasks. Vigor, in turn, refers to high levels of energy and
practiced, accepted avenues of response. his means that mental resilience when meeting life challenge.
people are practiced when they are addressing circum- Shirom (2004) suggested that vigor is the funda-
stances that they have gone through before. However, mental element of this process and that, if it occurs,
people are generally not prepared on individual, family, the issues of absorption and commitment are not con-
and organizational levels (including government) for sequential. When addressing the consequences of ter-
situations of war and terrorism. Even in regions such as rorism and war, however, the issues of absorption and
Israel, where practiced coping might be expected, many commitment may be more fundamental, as it is critical
circumstances are so new and diferent as to trap people that individuals and the society at large continue their
into using strategies that have poor it. involvement and sustaining of key life tasks.
Defensive coping is probably more integral to
resiliency than might irst be assumed. We tend to
place defensive coping in a negative light and active,
Engagement as both process and outcome
proactive coping in a positive light. As Breznitz (1983) Absorption, commitment, and being vigorous are both
indicated, there are many levels of defensive coping processes and outcomes. We can speak of people acting
and they are fundamental to successful adaptation to in an absorbed manner and being committed to tasks
extreme circumstances. People need to titrate the full in their lives. We can likewise view people as acting in
weight of their losses and the challenges that await a vigorous manner, full of energy and active, and they
them, and some degree of optimistic bias can aid these can also report a sense of vigor. Both the process and
eforts (Carver & Scheier, 1998). It is possible, however, the outcomes aid understanding and should be incor-
that those who are more resilient will be less destruc- porated in future study of resiliency following terror-
tive in their defensive patterns and remain more pro- ism and war. In some ways, being both process and
social; however, this premise, like much in this chapter, outcome is also relective of depression and anxiety,
is largely speculative. as these describe and indeed are deined by processes.
his perhaps just underscores the key point made by
Lazarus and Folkman (1984), that the stress process is
Engagement theory and resiliency transactional and multidirectional with multiple feed-
he positive psychology revival has led to interest in back loops and no true outcome point.
engagement as the counterbalance to the burnout and Engagement involves central afective components
other distress-indicative processes (Schaufeli et al., that results from processes that center on peoples’
2002). his work on engagement is instructive and intrinsic energetic resources, more speciically emotional

257
Section 4: Specific challenges

robustness, cognitive agility, and physical vigor that under optimal conditions such engagement results
(Hobfoll & Shirom, 2001; Shirom, 2004). Seen this way, in the peak experience of low, where people are fully
engagement has emotional, cognitive, and behavioral wrapped up in their activity. his might help to explain
components that are integrally tied to resource invest- why resiliency may be self-perpetuating, as the success
ment and its consequences, and which can be quickly of withstanding stress and remaining actively involved
drained by loss cycles. It is important to note that resili- in life tasks is self-rewarding in addition to its extrinsic
ency and distress are also not polar opposites. We and social and material rewards.
others have oten depicted them as such (Hobfoll et al., Informal and formal support from family, friends,
2009), by calling resiliency and resistance the absence organizations, professional agencies, and govern-
of symptoms, which is one aspect of resilience. Surely, ment bodies aid engagement by adding key resources
engagement and psychological distress are negatively at critical junctures following terrorism and war. his
related. However, when confronting momentous positive inlux of resources must also be strategic, as
events, distress and engagement, and even high levels resilient individuals are likely to be self-suicient oten
of vigor, may be simultaneously experienced. hey also yet at other times be quite open to and needy of aid.
may wax and wane, with times of engagement follow- On a more mesosocial level, public mental health
ing distress as people reinsert themselves in addressing intervention and actions of government to compen-
instrumental, emotional, and cognitive tasks. To the sate for losses and help to actualize people’s use of what
extent that resources remain available and are not crit- resources they have (e.g., making loans available for
ically depleted in loss cycles, they may be reinvested in rebuilding, making sure insurance companies pay their
resiliency processes. debts to consumers) are critical actions to aid resiliency.
In this regard, Shirom (2004) has posited that the We should not fall into the trap of considering resili-
cognitions and behaviors that are closely interrelated ency to be a trait or action of the individual, isolated
with the changes in energetic resources are part of self. Concerning the allocation of suicient adequate
autonomous biobehavioral systems. Hence, one biobe- resources, non-governmental and government organi-
havioral system inluences the withdrawal response zations can aim at compensating lack or loss of individ-
that accompanies PTSD, depression, and high anxiety ual, family, and business resources (Hobfoll, 2001). A
levels. At the same time, a second system facilitates and positive interaction between the individual, the family,
accompanies an approach-oriented behavior facilita- and organizations will spread resiliency, as gain spirals
tion system that produces persistence and resilience- are shared between these social–structural levels. hat
characterizing engagement. Evidence shows that these is, where families do better, individuals do better, and
two biobehavioral systems, too, operate quite inde- where individuals at work do better, the organization is
pendently from one another (Shirom, 2004), which is more resilient.
another reason why distress and engagement should
be seen as potentially co-occurring, if negatively
associated.
Distinguishing resiliency from
he engagement process underscores that, just as traumatic growth
depression and PTSD are typiied by a withdrawn or Our research has illustrated that attempts at post-trau-
agitated energetic state, engagement is typiied by a matic and peritraumatic growth are common, but oten
positive energetic state matched with a positive feel- backire. hat is, our studies have shown that in most
ing tone. Energetic states are key to human function- instances, those who report greater post-traumatic
ing. hroughout life, people’s energetic state needs to growth do worse, and continue to do worse than those
be consonant and facilitative of their life tasks to aid who get on with life tasks (Hobfoll et al., 2007). he
in decision making, activity level, information pro- attempt to grow from trauma is fraught with pitfalls as
cessing, and social action (Gaillard & Wientjes, 1994). it leads to expectations that are diicult to achieve, par-
Both consciously and unconsciously, there is a con- ticularly when we are speaking of types of trauma that
stant taxing of the energetic state in order to react to may be chronic and where there is little that individuals
life threat and opportunities. Csikszentmihalyi (1997) can do other than to get on with their lives.
has argued that this is mainly not thoughtful or within Resiliency is in many ways a lower bar than trau-
consciousness, and further that when we are engaged it matic growth. As we have already outlined in this chap-
is inherently a pleasant process. He further has argued ter, resiliency involves staying involved in resource gain

258
Chapter 17: Resilience in the face of terrorism

cycles and minimizing resource loss cycles. It involves “ability to maintain relatively stable, healthy levels of
keeping psychological distress to a minimum and pre- psychological… functioning” (Bonanno, 2005, p. 20)
serving functioning. Even when we speak of engage- in the face of highly disruptive, threatening events.
ment, this involves achieving a state that is normal in Lacking symptoms means that “ill health” or what
most people’s lives but that becomes interrupted when Antonovsky (1979) termed “dis-ease” was not occur-
people experience high levels of distress. ring. However, in this chapter we are adding to this the
In this way, the resource gain cycles that people concept of engagement as a way to better capture the
should hope to achieve are the normal ones of their spirit of Bonanno’s thinking.
lives and the extraordinary ones demanded by mass A third trajectory was one that would relect sus-
casualties and personal trauma. he goal is to return tained lack of resilience: the chronic distress trajectory.
to the engagement that is common in most people’s his trajectory relects some level of symptoms of dis-
lives when they are doing well. hey are committed to order even if these symptoms are low in intensity. his
their work, families, friends, and even the hobbies and is quite diferent to a chronic disorder, which would
pastimes that they are passionate about. When doing mean meeting diagnostic criteria for a disorder or at
these activities they are absorbed in them fully. hat least having high levels of symptoms, which is the state
is they commit their energetic, cognitive, and emo- that has typically been studied. he inal trajectory is
tional resources to the tasks that involve them. his termed the delayed distress trajectory, and it is char-
requires extra efort and will power during and follow- acterized by initial resistance that is lost, again even if
ing trauma, but again it is for the most part a return to only low symptom levels develop (Bonanno et al., 2007;
fairly normal levels of functioning. Layne et al., 2007).
Likewise, achieving a sense of vigor is a normal A sizable minority of individuals displayed a resist-
healthy state. It may not be characteristic of all people ant trajectory (22.1%), although this is lower than earl-
at all times, but it can and is oten achieved even ier writing on resiliency might have led us to expect
by those with low levels of psychological distress. (Bonanno et al., 2006). Further, a small group (13.5%)
Sustaining post-traumatic growth, in contrast, may of individuals showed a resilience trajectory, such that
be an extraordinary state as it appears to be achieved they initially were symptomatic but became relatively
with positive efect on other aspects of life by rela- free of symptoms over time. he chronic distress tra-
tively few individuals, at least when facing terrorism jectory was by far the most common pattern, seen in
and war. 54.0% of the sample. Finally, a small group of individ-
uals were initially resistant but became symptomatic
Distinguishing engagement over time (10.3%). hese rates compare more closely
to those of another study in Israel (Bleich et al., 2006),
trajectories from distress trajectories which found that 14.4% of an Israeli sample were resist-
Borrowing largely from seminal work of Bonanno ant, as deined by an absence of symptoms assessed at
et al. (2007) and Norris et al. (2007), we have exam- one time point. However, these results relect much less
ined four key distress-symptom trajectories over time resiliency and resistance than Bonanno et al. (2006)
ater exposure to ongoing terrorism and war in Israel. found among Manhattan residents following the 9/11
his study was during a relative lull in violence, but as attacks: more than 50% of their sample was resistant
is oten the case in terrorism and war, ongoing threats and even their most exposed groups never fell below
imbue the situations with peritraumatic elements and 30% showing resistance.
reminders as well. he irst trajectory contained indi- Returning to our sample, the resistance trajectory
viduals who never develop symptoms of disorder, was predicted demographically by being male, hav-
termed the resistance trajectory (Layne et al., 2007). A ing higher income and education, being secular (ver-
second trajectory was characterized by initial symp- sus being traditionally religious), and being a member
toms, followed by recovery: the resilience trajectory. of the majority ethnic group (Jewish versus Arab).
he resilience trajectory was seen as characterized by Consistent with COR theory, experiencing less psy-
improvement to levels that indicate absence of psy- chosocial resource loss at either time point predicted
chological symptoms from even low levels of earlier the resistance trajectory. Moreover, maintaining high
symptoms. In using the general term resilience, we fol- levels of social support was related to greater likelihood
lowed Bonanno’s conceptualization of resilience as the of displaying resistance.

259
Section 4: Specific challenges

Predictors of the resilience trajectory were similar Csikszentmihalyi’s (1997) concept of low have always
to those predicting resistance. Speciically, the resili- been diicult to study empirically. he aspect of low, of
ence trajectory was associated with higher income and losing track of time, whereby individuals ind that time
being Jewish versus being Arab. Again, consistent with lies or does not move will need to be distinguished
COR theory, psychosocial resource loss at a second time from the painful processes in PTSD related to dissoci-
point was associated with lower likelihood of having a ation. Indeed, the element of how the low of time is
resilience trajectory. Illustrating that traumatic growth experienced, positively or negatively, may be the key
and engagement are likely not similar processes, lower element. In the case of commitment, we would need to
(not higher) traumatic growth at time point two was rely on people’s assessment of how committed they felt,
associated with the resilience trajectory. Finally, there and, for example, there are many demand characteris-
was a borderline signiicant association of high social tics regarding saying that one is committed to family.
support predicting this resilience pattern. Although
the indings for resistance and resilience trajectories
were not identical, there was clear support for the asso- How engagement and distress
ciation of these resiliency-related processes with lower may interact
resource loss, and that possessing greater resources Following from COR theory, we can speculate on a
(e.g., having majority status, higher income, and greater number of principles that, while speculative, it the
social support from friends) was associated with these existing research on the interaction of resource loss
more favorable outcomes (Hobfoll, 1989, 2002). and gain and are worthy starting points. Hence, what
We believe our indings on resistance and resili- follows discusses three speculative principles of COR
ence trajectories add to the existing body of knowledge theory relating speciically to engagement:
on resiliency; however, at the same time, our examin-
engagement principle 1: engagement processes are a
ation of the lack of symptoms is not the same as positive
fundamental aspect of human motivation and are,
well-being. Not developing symptoms in the face of
therefore, also primed by role behavior and role
chronic terrorist attack and threat and war is no small
obligations
achievement and is integrally related to resiliency.
engagement principle 2: resource loss markedly limits
Nevertheless, the study of engagement processes goes
engagement and absorption
signiicantly further. Indeed, the ability to resist symp-
engagement principle 3: gain cycles and engagement
tom development may be a product of the ability to
processes are mutually iterative and expansive.
become engaged. Hence, when individuals are initially
afected by terrorism and war, or other trauma, many he irst engagement principle deals with the pro-
recover reasonably healthy levels of functioning and cesses of engagement. Individuals who face traumatic
low levels of distress in a reasonable amount of time, and highly challenging circumstances will attempt to
and they may well do so by being engaged, as well as remain, or re-immerse themselves, in life activities
becoming more capable of engagement as their symp- with increasing levels of absorption and commitment.
toms subside (Frankl, 1963; Caplan, 1964; Antonovsky, hese will include activities relating to family, self,
1979; Norris et al., 2007). his should be distinguished work, and society. his absorption and commitment
from recovery as it is normally deined, which is to go will result in decreased psychological distress and ill
from diagnostic or high levels of symptoms of a dis- health and increased sense of well-being, including a
order to subclinical levels. In such conceptualizations, sense of vigor.
considerable symptoms may still exist and there is no People’s involvement in life tasks is consistent with
evidence about health processes like engagement. their role behavior, which is deeply ingrained. Further,
In our current studies in Israel and Palestine, we life tasks have many survival and obligation-based
are adding the examination of resiliency markers. demand characteristics. Hence, unless people become
Following Shirom (2004), we feel that vigor is an essen- extremely despondent or withdrawn, they will have a
tial element of engagement. We hope also to examine drive to continue their involvement in life tasks. his
absorption and commitment, albeit there are diicult may become robotic at high levels of distress, but even
issues to resolve in their study. First, it may also be dif- such robotic activity will positively contribute to vigor,
icult for research to distinguish between absorption albeit in small amounts. It follows from principle 2 of
and rumination. Second, the time elements relating to COR theory that these small amounts of increases in

260
Chapter 17: Resilience in the face of terrorism

vigor can have enormous potential, as resource gains stressors may be how even individuals who are deeply
have increased weight and meaning when losses are distressed remain engaged. A parent who is deeply
high. depressed but continues to nurture and care for his
he second engagement principle considers the or her children, the soldier who is deeply grieving but
efect of resource loss on engagement and absorption. continues to ight with his comrades, the employee who
Resource loss cycles will limit individuals’ ability to goes to work with PTSD because work is important to
sustain absorption and commitment to life tasks and as feed his or her family and because work keeps society
such will limit the sense of vigor and diminishment of going are all showing remarkable resiliency of a kind
psychological stress and ill health. that we have only begun to appreciate.
Because resource loss is more powerful than
resource gain, and because resource loss has such
powerful impact, it will be diicult to sustain or
References
reassert absorption and commitment in life tasks Agronick, G., Steuve, A., Vargo, S., & O’ Donnell, L. (2007).
under high levels of resource loss. Rather than a linear, New York City young adults’ psychological reactions to
9/11: Findings from the Reach for Health Longitudinal
all or nothing process, however, this will appear as a
Study. American Journal of Community Psychology, 39,
seesaw process, as there is a strong human motivation 79–90.
to be involved, absorbed, and committed to life tasks,
Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health, stress, and coping. San
and learned helplessness is the exception, even when
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
resource loss is massive in amount or kind.
he third engagement principle considers the Benight, C. C., Ironson, G., Klebe, K., et al. (1999).
Conservation of resources and coping self-eicacy
interaction of gain cycles and engagement processes. predicting distress following a natural disaster: A causal
Resource gain cycles will contribute to increased model analysis where the environment meets the mind.
absorption and commitment in life tasks and a sense Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 12, 107–126.
of vigor. his, in part, will come from the direct asso- Bleich, A., Gelkopf, M., & Solomon, Z. (2003). Exposure
ciation of absorption and commitment, in that when to terrorism, stress-related mental health symptoms,
people are absorbed and committed they build and and coping behaviors among a nationally representative
contribute to material, personal, social, condition, and sample in Israel. Journal of the American Medical
energy resources. It will also occur because, as resource Association, 290, 612–620.
gains are made, more energies and attention will be Bleich, A., Gelkopf, M., Melamed, Y., & Solomon, Z. (2006).
available for absorption and commitment in life tasks. Mental health and resiliency following 44 months of
In this sense, absorption and commitment involve terrorism: A survey of an Israeli national representative
both physical actions and cognitive and emotional task sample. BMC Medicine, 4, 21.
attention. Bonanno, G. A. (2005). Clarifying and extending the
construct of adult resilience. American Psychologist, 60,
265–267.
Conclusions Bonanno, G. A., Rennicke, C., & Dekel, S. (2005). Self-
It would be a step backwards to study engagement with- enhancement among high-exposure survivors of the
out regard to distress. We would expect that engage- September 11th terrorist attack: Resilience or social
maladjustment? Journal of Personality and Social
ment will co-occur with stress. At the same time, as Psychology, 88, 984–998.
individuals are able to become increasingly engaged,
Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D.
their psychological distress should diminish. his pro-
(2006). Psychological resilience ater disaster- New York
cess may well be iterative with many loops, as opposed City in the atermath of the September 11th terrorist
to linear. Even the most resilient individuals will experi- attack. Psychological Science, 17, 181–186.
ence dark moments when in the midst or atermath of
Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D. (2007).
disasters with mass casualties. Engagement, however, What predicts psychological resilience ater disaster? he
should be integral to the ability to stay involved in role of demographics, resources, and life stress. Journal of
meaningful life processes, to derive joy from them, and Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 671–682.
to bounce back following diicult times. Breznitz, S. (1983). Anticipatory stress reactions. In
Some of the most interesting aspects of engage- S. Breznitz (ed.), he denial of stress (pp. 225–255). New
ment following terrorism, warfare and other traumatic York: International Universities Press.

261
Section 4: Specific challenges

Caplan, G. (1964). Principles of preventative psychiatry. in Israel. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
New York: Basic Books. 74, 207–218.
Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of Hobfoll, S. E., Tracy, M., & Galea, S. (2006b). he impact of
behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press. resource loss and “traumatic growth” on probable PTSD
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding low, the psychology of and depression following terrorist attacks. Journal of
engagement with everyday life. New York: Basic Books. Traumatic Stress, 19, 867–878.
Frankl, V. (1963). Man’s search for meaning. Boston: Beacon. Hobfoll, S. E., Hall, B. J., Canetti-Nisim, D., et al. (2007).
Reining our understanding of traumatic growth in the
Freedy, J. R., Saladin, M. E., Kilpatrick, D. G., Resnick, H. S., face of terrorism: Moving from meaning cognitions
& Saunders, B. E. (1994). Understanding acute to doing what is meaningful. Applied Psychology, 56,
psychological distress following natural disaster. Journal 345–366.
of Traumatic Stress, 7, 257–273.
Hobfoll, S. E., Palmieri, P. A., Johnson, R. J., et al. (2009).
Gaillard, A. W. K. & Wientjes, C. J. E. (1994). Mental load Trajectories of resilience, resistance and distress during
and work stress as two types of energy mobilization. ongoing terrorism: he case of Jews and Arabs in Israel.
Work and Stress, 8, 141–152. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 77, 138–148.
Galea, S., Ahern, J., Resnick, H., et al. (2002). Psychological Iscoe, I. (1974). Community psychology and the competent
sequelae of the September 11 terrorist attacks in New community. American Psychologist, 29, 607–613.
York City. New England Journal of Medicine, 346,
982–987. Kaniasty, K. & Norris, F. H. (1995). In search of altruistic
community: Patterns of social support mobilization
Hall, B. J., Hobfoll, S. E., Palmieri, P., et al. (2008). he
following hurricane Hugo. American Journal of
psychological impact of impending forced settler
Community Psychology, 23, 447–477.
disengagement in Gaza: Trauma and posttraumatic
growth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 22–29. Klingman, A. (2006). Children and war trauma. In
A. Renniger & I. E. Sigel (eds.), Handbook of child
Hobfoll, S. E. (1988). he ecology of stress. New York:
psychology, Vol.4: Child psychology in practice, 6th edn
Hemisphere.
(pp. 619–652). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new
Klingman, A. & Cohen, E. (2004). School-based
attempt at conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist,
multisystemic interventions for mass trauma. New York:
44, 513–524.
Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press.
Hobfoll, S. E. (1991). Traumatic stress: A theory based on
Layne, C. M., Warren, J., Shalev, A., & Watson, P. (2007).
rapid loss of resources. Anxiety Research, 4, 187–197.
Risk, vulnerability, resistance, and resilience: Towards
Hobfoll, S. E. (1998). Stress, culture, and community: he an integrative conceptualization of posttraumatic
psychology and philosophy of stress. New York: Plenum adaptation. In M. J. Friedman, T. M. Kean, & P. A. Resick
Press. (eds.), PTSD: science and practice: A comprehensive
Hobfoll, S. E. (2001). he inluence of culture, community, handbook (pp. 497–520). New York: Guilford Press.
and the nested-self in the stress process: Advancing Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and
conservation of resources theory. Applied Psychology, 50, coping. New York: Springer.
337–370.
Norris, F. H. & Kaniasty, K. (1996). Received and perceived
Hobfoll, S. E. (2002). Social and psychological resources and social support in times of stress: A test of the social
adaptation. Review of General Psychology, 6, 307–324. support deterioration deterrence model. Journal of
Hobfoll, S. E. & Lilly, R. S. (1993). Resource conservation Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 498–511.
as a strategy for community psychology. Journal of Norris, F. H., Stevens, S. P., Pfeferbaum, B., Wyche, K. F., &
Community Psychology, 21, 128–148. Pfeferbaum, R. L. (2007). Community resilience as
Hobfoll, S. E. & Shirom, A. (2001). Conservation of a metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for
resources theory. In R. Golembiewski (ed.), Handbook of intervention. American Journal of Community Psychology,
organizational behavior (pp. 57–80). New York: Dekker. 41, 127–150.
Hobfoll, S. E., Lavin, J., & Wells, J. D. (1999). When it rains Palmieri, P. A., Canetti-Nisim, D., Galea, S., Johnson, R. J., &
it pours: he greater impact of resource loss compared Hobfoll, S. E. (2008). he psychological impact of the
to gain on psychological distress. Personality and Social Israel–Hezbollah war on Jews and Arabs in Israel: he
Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1172–1182. impact of risk and resilience factors. Social Science and
Hobfoll, S. E., Canetti-Nisim, D., & Johnson, R. J. (2006a). Medicine, 67, 1208–1216.
Exposure to terrorism, stress-related mental health Panamaki, R. L., Komproe, I. H., Qouta, S., Elmasri, M. &
symptoms, and defensive coping among Jews and Arabs de Jong, J. T. V. M. (2005). he role of peritraumatic

262
Chapter 17: Resilience in the face of terrorism

dissociation and gender in the association between terror: A study of two communities in Israel. American
trauma and mental health in a Palestinian community Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 667–673.
sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 545–551. Shirom, A. (2004). Feeling vigorous at work? he construct
Pynoos, R. S., Steinberg, A. M., Grete, D., et al. (2004). of vigor and the study of positive afect in organizations.
Reverberations of danger, trauma and PTSD on In P. L. Perrewe & D. Ganster (eds.), Research in
group dynamics. In B. Sklarew, S. W. Twemlow & organizational stress and well-being, Vol. 3 (pp. 135–165).
S. M. Wilkinson (eds.), Analysts in the trenches: Streets, Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
schools, war zones (pp. 1–22). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Smith, B. W. & Freedy, J. R. (2000). Psychosocial resource
Press. loss as a mediator of the efects of lood exposure on
Sattler, D. N. (2006). Family resources, family strains, and psychological distress and physical symptoms. Journal of
stress following the Northridge earthquake. Stress, Crisis, Traumatic Stress, 13, 349–357.
and Trauma, 9, 187–202.
Somer, E., Ruvio, A., Soref, E., & Sever, I. (2005). Terrorism,
Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., Gonzáles-Romá, V., & distress and coping: High versus low impact regions and
Bakker, A. B. (2002). he measurement of burnout and direct versus indirect civilian exposure. Anxiety, Stress
engagement: A conirmatory factor analytic approach. and Coping, 18, 165–182.
Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 71–92.
Stein, B. D., Elliott, M. N., Jaycox, L. H., et al. (2004).
Seligman, M. E. P. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive A national longitudinal study of the psychological
psychology. American Psychologist, 55, 5–14. consequences of the September 11, 2001 terrorist attacks:
Shalev, A. Y., Tuval, R., Frenkiel-Fishman, S., Hadar, H., & Reactions, impairment, and help-seeking. Psychiatry:
Eth, S. (2006). Psychological responses to continuous Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 67, 105–117.

263
Section
Section4 Specific challenges
Chapter
Resilience in the context of poverty

18 John C. Buckner and Jessica S. Waters

Introduction areas where poverty is widespread. For individuals,


being poor heightens one’s risk of experiencing a nega-
Resilience has been deined as the manifestation of tive outcome of some sort, such as the development of a
positive outcomes in the face of some form of adver- signiicant physical or mental health disorder. Living in
sity (Luthar et al., 2000; Masten, 2001), poverty being poverty can also increase the likelihood of facing adver-
one such adversity. Poverty, “the state or condition of sities and experiences that are much less common for
having little or no money, goods, or means of support,” those with greater inancial resources. Such hardships
has meaning in both relative and absolute terms. hose can take the form of negative life events, which tend to
who are deined as poor in an aluent country like the be discrete in nature (e.g., being the victim of crime or
USA may not seem impoverished, on a more absolute violence), or chronic life strains, which are more episodic
basis, compared with those living in extreme poverty in and enduring (e.g., experiencing hunger, economic
regions such as sub-Saharan Africa. Nonetheless, indi- insecurity, residential instability, and other adversities
viduals who are poor in a more relative sense may still related to the lack of material resources). Although it is
experience signiicant distress through the compara- no surprise that the quantity of life events and chronic
tive wealth they see around them. strains experienced by persons living in poverty tends
Poverty in developed countries indexes a range of to be higher than for more inancially advantaged indi-
social ills that extend beyond the absolute or relative viduals, it is noteworthy that the qualitative nature of
lack of money and goods. Crowded living conditions, these events and strains oten stands apart from what
community and family violence, drug traicking, high individuals with middle and or higher incomes would
crime rates, a weaker civil infrastructure, and higher realistically encounter. For example, while children of
prevalence rates of mental and substance use disor- all backgrounds may experience physical illness, the
ders are issues that are more commonly found in poor divorce of their parents, or some such adversity, chil-
communities. hese social conditions are better able to dren growing up in poor communities are much more
fester in impoverished settings and can feed on them- likely to become homeless, witness a violent crime,
selves, creating a downward spiral or “vicious circle,” or have a family member incarcerated (Masten et al.,
wherein one problem provokes the emergence or wors- 1993; Huston et al., 1994; McLoyd, 1998; Buckner et al.,
ening of another. For example, a high crime rate in a 1999, 2004). Most oten children living in poverty have
neighborhood may lead to businesses deciding to avoid little or no control over such severe negative events.
it as a place of investment, leading to higher unemploy- In addition to the distinction between negative
ment and further crime in that area. events and chronic strains, stressors have also been dif-
Both the manifestations and the ramiications of ferentiated as to whether they are “positive,” “tolerable,”
poverty can be described at diferent levels of analysis. or “toxic” in nature (National Scientiic Council on the
At a community or population level, poverty is associ- Developing Child, 2007). Positive stressors are usually
ated with increased rates of crime, higher prevalence of modest in intensity and represent situations that pose
mental health and substance use disorders, and higher challenges, which can lead to successes or growth (e.g.,
rates of disease. Also, rates of unemployment, school inal examinations, music recitals, athletic competi-
dropout, and teenage pregnancy are oten higher in tions, applying to college, etc.). Tolerable stressors are

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

264
Chapter 18: Resilience in the context of poverty

more severe but are time limited, thus allowing a per- was conducted by Emmy Werner and colleagues, who
son to recover once the stressor has ended (e.g., living followed a cohort of 698 children born in 1955 to fam-
through a natural disaster, experiencing the serious, but ilies with limited income living on the island of Kauai
brief, illness of a loved one). A toxic stressor is extraor- in Hawaii. he Kauai Longitudinal Study (Werner &
dinary in nature and/or severe and prolonged, thereby Smith, 1982, 1992, 2001) followed these children from
overwhelming a person’s ability to cope. Children as before birth into adulthood (data were collected begin-
well as adults who live in poverty oten have many more ning in the prenatal period, at birth and at ages 1, 2,
toxic stressors in their lives and fewer positive stressors 10, 18, 32, and 40 years). he investigators designated
than those who are more aluent. about 200 of their study participants as high-risk chil-
Poor individuals and families also lack the mater- dren as they were born into low-income families with
ial resources with which to mitigate some of the efects troubled family environments. About two thirds of
of stressors. Events or strains that may be tolerable these youngsters developed signiicant behavioral
for some individuals – if material resources can be diiculties by childhood, the other third growing up
brought to bear to curtail or bufer their efects – may without any signiicant learning or behavioral prob-
be toxic to persons who have more limited options lems. he Kauai study focused intently on describing
at their disposal. For example, a ire or natural disas- the characteristics of this latter group of children, who
ter could displace middle-income families from their the investigators deemed to be resilient.
homes for a period of time, but they may have savings he extensive longitudinal nature of this study
they can access or insurance to claim that can readily aforded an opportunity to examine resilience at vari-
restore them to permanent housing. Facing a similar ous stages of development and to identify internal char-
predicament, low-income families without such safety acteristics and external factors associated with these
nets would likely ind themselves having to live in a adaptive outcomes. In terms of internal characteristics,
homelessness shelter for an extended period of time those evidencing positive adaptation, that is resilience,
or move into crowded housing with extended family tended to have easy temperaments as infants and were
or friends (Bassuk et al., 1996). Also, individuals with successful in eliciting positive attention from adults.
limited inancial resources are oten one pay check (or hey were characterized by their caregivers as active,
unexpected major expense) away from inancial crisis, alert, responsive, and sociable babies (Werner, 1993).
which can lead to eviction from housing and a result- Similar attributions were used to describe resilient
ing cascade of other negative events. youngsters when they reached their pre-school years.
Resilience in children has been deined as “achiev- By middle childhood, resilient children began to dem-
ing desirable outcomes in spite of signiicant challenges onstrate good problem-solving and communication
to adaptation or development” (Masten & Coatsworth, skills as well as a relective cognitive style, good impulse
1998, p. 37). he prerequisite for evidencing “resili- control, ability to concentrate, and lexibility in their
ence” is to have faced a major adversity of some sort. approaches to dealing with stress. Teachers noted them
Without such hardship, a child who manifests “desir- as sociable but also quite independent. When children
able outcomes” would be said to be evidencing “com- in the Kauai study reached adolescence, those eviden-
petence.” he remainder of this chapter summarizes cing resilience had a more internal locus of control and
studies of resilience that have taken place in impover- higher self-esteem. Resilient youths of both sexes also
ished settings: studies focusing on children, on adults/ tended to be somewhat androgynous in their personal-
families, and on entire communities. he chapter then ity traits and interests. In both adolescence and young
attempts to identify common themes about resilience adulthood, these resilient individuals seemed to have
and poverty emerging from these diferent studies. internalized a positive set of core values and were more
responsible, nurturing, and socially perceptive than
Studies of resilience among children their less well-adapted peers (Werner, 1993).
In terms of factors external to the child, such as fam-
living in poverty ily structure and characteristics, resilient children in the
Of the many published studies of resilience involving Kauai study tended to have better educated and more
children and adolescents, relatively few have examined competent caregivers. Having a mother who worked at
children’s resilience in the context of poverty. One of a steady job appeared to be a prominent protective fac-
the most well-known studies of resilience in children tor for resilient girls. Resilient children typically had

265
Section 4: Specific challenges

the opportunity to establish a close bond with at least cross-sectional investigations that were conducted
one older person in their lives who provided them with approximately a decade apart on diferent samples
stable care and positive attention. his was not always (Parker et al., 1990; Wyman et al., 1991, 1999; Cowen
a parent; a close tie with a grandparent or older sibling et al., 1992; Magnus et al., 1999). he later study of
(usually a sister) appeared to be suicient to promote youngsters (aged seven to nine years) had many of
resilience in some children (Werner & Smith, 2001). the same indings as the original investigation, which
Beyond caregivers and family, ties to adults in school focused on those aged 9–12 years. he investiga-
(e.g., a favorite teacher) or in broader community who tors posited a range of inner resources and parenting
could serve as positive role models was an important variables that would diferentiate “stress-resistant”
external resource for participants in the Kauai study, (i.e., resilient) from “stress-afected” youths. Among
both in childhood and adolescence. internal attributes examined in the later study, resili-
Another longitudinal study of resilience involv- ent children scored higher on measures of perceived
ing a sample of low-income youths was the Harvard competence, self-esteem, empathy, and “realistic
Medical School Study of Adult Development, reported control” attributions (Magnus et al., 1999). hese ind-
by Felsman and Vaillant (1987). his investigation, ings largely replicated their earlier results. Similarly,
which took place over a 40-year period, focused on a as reported by Wyman et al. (1999) the two stud-
group of 500 males selected from inner city schools ies had similar indings with regard to temperament
in Boston, who were originally enrolled to represent a and parenting as predictors of resilience status. Like
non-delinquent control group for a prospective study the Kauai study, resilient children were more likely to
of juvenile delinquency (Glueck & Glueck, 1950). his be rated by their parents as having had an easy going
cohort was re-interviewed at multiple time points in temperament as toddlers. Resilient children were also
adulthood (ages 25, 31, and 47 years). At age 47, men more likely to have parents who felt competent in
in the top and bottom quartiles of mental health were their roles, were consistent in disciplinary practices,
compared. Intelligence (as measured in adolescence) evidenced an authoritative approach to parenting,
and parental socioeconomic status were not useful and were responsive to their child’s emotional needs
predictors of outcomes in mid-life. Rather, those who (Wyman et al., 1999). One strength of the Rochester
demonstrated competence as adolescents (e.g., held study is its replication of similar indings at two
part-time job, participated in school and extracurricu- points in time with diferent samples. A limitation is
lar activities, did well in school) were much more likely the authors’ decision to report indings pertaining to
to evidence positive outcomes at age 47. Also, a rating inner resources and external (e.g., parenting) factors
of “childhood environmental strengths” (e.g., based on as they relate to resilience in separate articles rather
relationships with family members and lack of child- than considering the relative predictive power of these
hood emotional problems) was positively associated variables in a single multivariate model.
with good outcomes in adulthood. Buckner et al. (2003) conducted a study compar-
In two related studies, Luthar and colleagues ing 45 resilient with 70 non-resilient youths from
(Luthar, 1991; Luthar et al., 1993) studied resilience families with extremely low incomes in Worcester,
and competence among inner city youths, many from Massachusetts. A third of these school-aged children
a low socioeconomic background, in New Haven, had been homeless within the past two years and all
Connecticut. Among ninth grade students identiied were from households with incomes below the poverty
as resilient (on the basis of experiencing high stress but line. Resilience was operationally deined in a multidi-
evidencing good social competence), it was common mensional manner using well-established instruments
to ind that these youths were experiencing diiculties that measured children’s emotional well-being, behav-
in other spheres of adjustment, particularly internaliz- ior, competence, and level of functioning. Children
ing problems such as symptoms of anxiety and depres- deemed resilient showed positive adjustment in each
sion. his observation suggests caution in applying the of these realms, whereas those determined to be non-
term “resilient” to children upon an assessment of only resilient evidenced signiicant problems in one or more
one domain of functioning. of these areas. Although participants in this study all
he Rochester Resilience Project is another study lived below the poverty line, there was still substan-
of urban children, many of whom were from low- tial variation in the quantity of negative events and
income families. his project entailed two separate chronic strains they had experienced in recent years.

266
Chapter 18: Resilience in the context of poverty

Because these adversities were predictive of outcomes self-regulation (and executive functions) did not begin
in expected directions, it was necessary to statistically in earnest until the mid-to-late 1990s, so researchers
control for them in order to understand the independ- who conducted the pioneering studies on resilience
ent contributions of inner and external resources for in children did not deliberately assess and examine
predicting resilience. this variable as a predictor. Interestingly, if the ind-
While this study was limited to a cross-sectional ings from the Kauai Longitudinal Study are examined
comparison of children, a decided strength was its through a self-regulation “lens,” it is clear that many of
extensive assessment battery, which was made up of the attributes used to characterize resilient children in
data collected directly from the child as well as from this study relect adept self-regulatory capacities (e.g.,
a parent and an external rater. In combination with strong problem-solving skills, a relective cognitive
multivariate analyses, this allowed the investigators to style, good impulse control, an ability to concentrate,
examine the relative contribution of an array of vari- and lexibility in dealing with stressful circumstances).
ables, relecting both inner and external resources of It is highly likely that, if a precise measure of self-
a child, in predicting their resilience status. Among regulation could have been employed in this pioneer-
inner resources, self-esteem and, particularly, self- ing longitudinal investigation (unfeasible at the time
regulation skills emerged as independent predictors because the construct had yet to be formed), Werner
of resilience. Likewise, among external resources that and colleagues would have found it very efective in
were examined, parental monitoring stood out as a distinguishing resilient from non-resilient children.
predictor, when controlling for all other explanatory How is it that self-regulation skills may be import-
variables. Perhaps surprisingly, children’s social sup- ant in dealing with stress in an adaptive manner?
port did not predict resilience status. he parental Aspinwall and Taylor (1997) argued that self-regula-
monitoring variable tapped into a parent’s proclivity to tory capacities are behind proactive means of coping
pay close attention to the whereabouts of a child when with stress. An individual who copes in a proactive
away from home and to who the child was with. Of manner tries to anticipate stressors before they occur,
note, the non-verbal intelligence of a child, while asso- analyzes how to prevent them, and plans a course of
ciated with resilience status in bivariate analyses, was action if a stressor is unavoidable. he net result may be
not a predictor of resilience status in multivariate mod- a reduction in the number of stressors experienced (or
eling. Instead, when in the same model, self-regulation a diminution in their impact), thereby leading to better
(which was positively associated with intelligence) was psychosocial adjustment (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1997).
the much more potent predictor. Self-regulation skills may also be important in adap-
tively dealing with stressors ater they have occurred.
For example, good emotion regulation, which is a facet
Self-regulation and resilience of self-regulation, may help to overcome uncontrol-
Over the years, intelligence is a variable that has been lable adversities to which one can only react (Eisenberg
found to be predictive of resilience in children across a et al., 1997). Unpreventable losses, such as the death of
range of studies. More recently, there has been a growing a loved one through illness, are a type of event that may
interest in self-regulation as an important predictor of require good emotion-regulation skills in order to deal
positive outcomes in children (Masten & Coatsworth, with it efectively. Good self-regulation skills are prob-
1998; Buckner et al., 2003; Dishion & Connell, 2006). ably useful in handling adversities of diferent types,
Both theory and recent empirical indings are support- although they might be especially helpful for those liv-
ive of the argument that self-regulation skills may be an ing in poverty, particularly if they can mitigate control-
important inner resource for children, including those lable stressors (e.g., turn what would otherwise be toxic
living in poverty (Buckner et al., 2009). stressors into more tolerable negative events).
Self-regulation refers to an integrated set of meta- A methodological note to mention for future
cognitive skills that draw from both executive function research on resilience in children, particularly within
and emotion-regulation capacities, which are invoked the context of poverty, is the need to account for sub-
in the service of accomplishing both proximal and stantial variation in stressors that can be experienced
distal goals. While associated with intelligence, self- by children living within the same impoverished con-
regulation is a somewhat separate construct that may text (or more generally who have experienced a com-
have closer links to adaptive functioning. Research on mon adverse event). Such variation in the nature and

267
Section 4: Specific challenges

quantity of stress experienced can partly explain dif- symptoms of anxiety and depression among both
ferences in outcomes seen in children (or adults). If adults and children.
not measured and accounted for in the study design or McCubbin and colleagues (McCubbin & McCubbin
statistical analyses, such diferences in adverse experi- 1988; McCubbin et al., 1998) have studied factors
ences can act as a confounding variable(s), and distort leading to resilient outcomes in families. Qualities
or misrepresent study indings. For example, as men- of resilient families they identiied include cohesion,
tioned above, in their study of resilience among chil- commitment, warmth, afection, and emotional sup-
dren belonging to families with very limited income in port for family members. Several research studies have
Worcester, Massachusetts, Buckner et al. (2003) found examined risk and protective factors for family home-
that children deemed to be resilient had experienced lessness. In the context of extreme poverty, the ability
fewer negative life events and chronic strains over the to remain in permanent housing can be looked at as
past year than youths who were non-resilient. hese a resilient outcome for low-income families. Findings
researchers statistically controlled for those diferences from studies in diferent locales are consistent in iden-
in order to identify more meaningfully internal char- tifying resources, speciically Section 8 housing vouch-
acteristics and external resources that (also) distin- ers and certiicates in the USA, as a key protective factor
guished the resilient from the non-resilient children. in keeping families in permanent housing (Bassuk
et al., 1997; Shinn et al., 1998).
Studies of resilience among adults and
families living in poverty Resilient communities
here are comparatively few published studies of Once again, in contrast to the more substantial litera-
resilience involving adults and families who are eco- ture regarding individual resilience in children, rela-
nomically disadvantaged. In a review of the literature, tively few studies have explored factors associated with
Bonanno and Mancini (2008) cited several factors resilience at the community level (whether impover-
linked to resilient outcomes in the face of traumatic ished or not). Nevertheless, drawing from the ields of
events, including male gender, older age, cognitive sociology, community development, and community
ability, and greater education. hey also cited “adaptive psychology, several themes emerge regarding con-
lexibility” as an inner resource that helps some indi- structs that may inluence community resilience in the
viduals to manage traumatic events. By this they mean, context of poverty.
“the capacity to shape and modify one’s behavior to Focusing on geographic communities as opposed
meet the demands of given stressor event” (Bonanno & to communities of interest, community has been
Mancini, 2008, p. 372). (It should be noted that the deined as “a collection of both people and institutions
ability to be lexible in one’s problem-solving approach occupying a spatially deined area inluenced by eco-
[to select a response that best its a given situation] is logical, cultural, and sometimes political forces” (Park,
a component of self-regulation.) Social resources are 1916, cited in Sampson et al., 2002, p. 445). In practice,
also a predictor of resilient outcomes among persons many researchers use the local boundaries identiied
who have experienced a natural disaster (Bonanno by the US census to deine neighborhoods or commu-
et al., 2007). nities. Residents of a community tend to live within
In a study of risk and resilience in Denver, involv- reasonable proximity one to another, and to share phys-
ing members of low-income families, Wadsworth ical and social resources. As such, the net worth of indi-
and Santiago (2008) found that child family members viduals in the same community is oten similar, which
appeared more negatively impacted than parents by in poorer communities results in a phenomenon known
poverty-related stressors that families experienced. as “concentrated disadvantage” (Sampson, 2004).
Use of primary control coping (problem solving, emo- Endemic, enduring poverty, and the resulting con-
tional expression, emotion regulation) and secondary centrated disadvantage, presents considerable obsta-
control coping (acceptance, cognitive restructuring, cles to community health and well-being and is oten
distraction, and positive thinking) by study partici- associated with other negative factors such as higher
pants was linked to better outcomes for family mem- rates of crime, school dropout, and infant mortality
bers of all ages. High secondary control coping in the (Sampson, 2004). As with individuals, the efects of
face of poverty-related adversities was linked to lower this type of long-term strain on a community difer

268
Chapter 18: Resilience in the context of poverty

from those of a sudden event, and they are oten harder Social capital
to gauge. In addition, a relative lack of economic and
Social capital “refers to features of social organization,
institutional resources leads poor communities to
such as networks, norms, and trust that facilitate coord-
experience greater diiculty when confronted with an
ination and cooperation for mutual beneit” (Putnam,
acute stressor, such as a natural disaster or a terrorist
1995, p. 67). his sociological construct has been tied
attack (Suarez-Ojeda & Autler, 2003).
Numerous indicators can be used to measure the to resilience in impoverished areas. For example, the
resilience of communities in the face of poverty or con- 2004 Paciic Ocean tsunami in Sri Lanka and Hurricane
centrated disadvantage. One process oten associated Katrina’s 2005 landfall in New Orleans reveal striking
with resilient communities includes economic growth, diferences in local community response (Munasinghe,
or the amelioration of poverty itself. However, other 2007). In spite of the limited material resources and
desirable outcomes, such as the health of residents or infrastructure in the poorer nation of Sri Lanka, a rally-
the survival of local customs and values, can be used ing of local eforts began within hours of the natural
to characterize resilient communities independent of disaster. Well before national or international reinforce-
economic change. Furthermore, community resilience ments arrived, the residents of the region were searching
can be demonstrated by either the presence of positive for survivors, administering medical care, and provid-
outcomes or by the absence of negative outcomes. As ing food, water, and shelter to those who were displaced.
such, many studies have identiied resilient communi- In New Orleans, by contrast, citizens tended to wait for
ties by their avoidance of the undesirable social condi- and rely on the slower and less efective response of the
tions oten associated with poverty, for example, high national government. As a result, the American com-
crime rates or other forms of social disorder. munities in the Gulf Coast area were, overall, not as
In spite of growing interest in this ield, few successful at organizing relief eforts as their Sri Lankan
researchers have demonstrated empirical associations counterparts. Although these case studies focus on
between community-level variables and community responses to natural disaster, it is worth noting that the
resilience. As there are a relatively small number of strength of the social networks and established patterns
studies addressing community resilience in the face of of providing mutual aid of communities in Sri Lanka
poverty, anecdotal examples and case studies are men- led to a more efective response to acute need. In other
tioned here in addition to empirical research. Some of words, these qualities helped the Sri Lankans to react
the studies do not identify poverty as the main adver- more efectively to the natural disaster despite the more
sity faced by a community, but rather as a pre-existing absolute poverty experienced in their country com-
condition when the adversity in question (e.g., natural pared with the Gulf Coast of the USA.
disaster) arises. However, it is reasonable to assume In her pivotal work, All Our Kin, Stack (1974) pro-
that the same processes that build resilience against vided a case study of an urban African-American com-
one community stressor may strengthen resistance to munity that exempliies the adaptive nature of social
the efects of another. Also, the sparse nature of the lit- capital in the face of poverty. According to Stack (p. 32),
erature in this area and inconsistency in how terms are “black families living in ‘he Flats’ need a steady source
deined sometimes makes it diicult to discern import- of cooperative support to survive”; they have, thus
ant distinctions between the community level variables developed a system for sharing and swapping resources
investigated. based on a valuing of mutual aid and trust, resulting in a
Despite these limitations, a number of studies have network of individuals, friends, and families bound by
linked certain constructs to the resilience of impover- both need and kinship. As described by Stack (1974),
ished communities. We have identiied several con- the expectation that one will share resources, the social
structs that are repeatedly discussed as pertinent to pressure to repay favors, and the networks that link
community resilience. hese can be grouped loosely donors and recipients provide evidence of how strong
into three categories: social capital helps residents of this community to sur-
social capital: social and institutional resources vive despite entrenched poverty.
cohesion: collective sense of togetherness and
connection Cohesion
collective eicacy: the ability to take empowered While social capital comprises the social resources,
action). norms, and common values that unite members of

269
Section 4: Specific challenges

a community, cohesion denotes a collective sense dislocation. Erikson posited that the “loss of commu-
of togetherness and connection to the community. nality” caused by the Bufalo Creek lood was central
Buckner (1988) described cohesion as a collective-level to the ensuing negative outcomes experienced by resi-
variable for which the individual-level “psychological dents of the community. he disruption of cohesion
sense of community” (a feeling of belonging and con- through the fragmentation of communities, and the
nection to other individuals) is a major indicator. hat resulting loss of community resilience, demonstrates
is to say, the presence of a psychological sense of com- the importance of this construct in maintaining the
munity among many members of a community sug- health and functioning of impoverished communities.
gests a larger phenomenon, namely “cohesion,” which
transcends the individual. In reviewing the literature,
Sampson et al. (2002) noted that the construct of cohe- Collective efficacy
sion has been found to be related to positive outcomes A third factor that appears to contribute to community
in impoverished communities in several studies. One resilience is the willingness and ability to take collective
investigation, based on the results of the British Crime action; this quality has been described with terms such
Survey from 1984 to 1992, demonstrated a negative as collective eicacy (Sampson et al., 1997), empower-
association between neighborhood cohesion and both ment (Norris et al., 2008), and citizen participation
burglary rates and social disorder, as measured by resi- (Burkey, 1993). Sampson (2004, p. 108) described col-
dent ratings of problems such as litter in the streets lective eicacy as “an emphasis on shared beliefs in a
or the presence of loiterers (Markowitz et al., 2001). neighborhood’s capability for action to achieve an
Steptoe and Feldman (2001) found supporting results intended efect, coupled with an active sense of engage-
in their survey of London residents, which indicated ment on the part of residents.” he concept of collect-
that greater social cohesion was signiicantly correlated ive eicacy appears to be distinct from but related to
with lower levels of a cluster of neighborhood prob- social cohesion and capital. Collective eicacy can
lems, including vandalism and noise disturbance; this depend on context and the speciic task at hand, but
efect remained even when controlled for neighbor- it generally indicates a community’s ability to actively
hood socioeconomic status and individual inancial address needs and take action. As Sampson and col-
or material deprivation. Similarly, Fagg and colleagues leagues deine collective eicacy as the combination
(2008), analyzing data from a national survey in of social cohesion, trust, and informal social control,
England, found less psychological distress among indi- their results give further support to the hypothesis that
viduals living in areas with greater social cohesion even cohesion increases community resilience to poverty.
ater controlling for the efects of poverty. hese studies Using data from the Project on Human Devel-
provide examples of the positive impact of social cohe- opment in Chicago Neighborhoods, which included
sion on impoverished communities. a cross-sectional study of over 300 Chicago communi-
he ethnographic study of Erikson (1976), which ties, Sampson et al. (1997) found that levels of violent
described the collective trauma resulting from the loss crime, as measured by homicide rates and resident
of community following the Bufalo Creek lood of reports of victimization, were negatively associated
1972, serves as a noteworthy example of the potential with the construct of “collective eicacy” when con-
outcomes when social cohesion is adversely impacted. trolling for prior violence, individual characteristics,
Prior to the lood that destroyed their community, the and measurement error. More precisely, collective
citizens of Bufalo Creek exhibited, despite minimal eicacy was shown to diminish the strength of the
economic resources, a strong “communality”; in other relationship between concentrated disadvantage and
words, the community evidenced high cohesion and homicide; although poorer neighborhoods did evi-
social capital as demonstrated by the routine practice dence higher rates of homicide, collective eicacy
of mutual assistance. However, ater the disaster, the appeared to be a protective factor, leading to lower than
inhabitants of this mining community found them- expected levels of violence (Sampson & Raudenbush,
selves separated from friends, living in areas assigned 1999; Morenof et al., 2001).
by relief workers with no regard for prior social connec- he construct of collective eicacy also appears
tions, and wielding little control over their situations. applicable to the Sri Lankan response following the
Many residents experienced symptoms of depression, tsunami. he swit and concerted action taken by local
anxiety, and trauma from the disaster and the resulting citizens ater the disaster attested to a high level of

270
Chapter 18: Resilience in the context of poverty

collective eicacy in response to the disaster, facilitated could shed light on the underpinnings of community
by pre-existing social networks. In contrast, the resi- resilience.
dents of Bufalo Creek exhibited a less efective response As discussed above, researchers who have studied
to the disaster. Erikson suggested that the lack of self- community resilience have sometimes deined similar
determination in the resettlement of their villages, and terms in diverse manners and used diferent terms to
the resulting inability to take collective action, contrib- describe overlapping phenomena. Similarly, distinc-
uted to the depression and isolation experienced by so tions between the characteristics that allow commu-
many residents of Bufalo Creek. hese observations nities to be resilient and the deinition of community
seem to indicate that the residents’ sense of collective resilience itself are oten blurred. For example, some
eicacy was impaired by the circumstances following researchers (Ahmed et al., 2004) conceive of social
the lood. It is important to note that not all nearby cohesion as an indicator of community resilience itself
citizens were directly afected by the tsunami and so rather than as an independent predictor. Further clari-
were in a position to ofer aid; in contrast, nearly all of ication is needed regarding the indicators of commu-
the residents of Bufalo Creek sufered signiicant loss nity resilience and the factors that distinguish resilient
of property and loved ones. his distinction may have from non-resilient communities. Interdisciplinary col-
contributed to the contrast between the two popula- laboration would help both to unify constructs across
tions in ability to take action when faced with a need disciplines and to highlight important diferences in
in the face of natural disaster. Many practitioners of the conceptual frameworks of psychology, sociology,
psychology and community development attempt and community development. Finally, further investi-
to foster the collective eicacy form of active citizen gation could shed light on processes that speciically
participation by promoting self-government, encour- protect communities from the adverse outcomes asso-
aging democratic processes, or otherwise empowering ciated with poverty. Nonetheless, the studies referenced
residents (Burkey, 1993). It is clear that collective ei- here ofer an intriguing foundation for future research
cacy is viewed by these professionals as a positive asset, on the characteristics of resilient communities and the
enhancing the resilience of the communities they work processes that facilitate this resilience.
in. However, more research is needed to clarify the rela-
tionship between collective eicacy and the resilience Conclusions
of communities to poverty, with the hope of informing
Attributes of individuals that may be associated with
future interventions.
resilient outcomes for children and adults oten have
no clear parallels or do not make sense when examin-
Community resilience: summary ing community-level characteristics that are linked to
he three constructs discussed (social capital, cohe- resilience. It is, therefore, a challenge to draw compari-
sion, and collective eicacy) have been shown, through sons across studies that have looked at resilience using
empirical investigation and case studies, to be linked to diferent levels of analysis. Nonetheless, it is tempt-
community resilience in the context of poverty. Beyond ing to point out that qualities that serve the purpose
these variables, many people have hypothesized about of facilitating adaptive, goal-directed behavior have
factors that might protect resilience in impoverished been found to be linked to resilient outcomes at both
communities. Suggested constructs include shared the individual and the community level. For individ-
spirituality or consciousness (Sonn & Fisher, 1998), uals, this attribute is self-regulation, while at the com-
community hope (Ahmed et al., 2004), collective self- munity level it is social cohesion/collective eicacy. As
esteem (Suarez-Ojeda & Autler, 2003), and citizen par- mentioned above, individuals invoke their self-regula-
ticipation (Norris et al., 2008). Further research may tory capacities in the service of goal-directed behavior
provide empirical basis for these ideas, but it is possible (Karoly, 1993) and good self-regulation skills may be
that they are tied to the variables above. For example, fundamental to efectively coping with stress, either by
citizen participation can reasonably be assumed to be problem solving or through emotion regulation. Such
related to collective action and social capital, while skills have been linked to resilient outcomes as well as
shared spirituality could be a byproduct of having to adaptive coping for impoverished youths (Buckner
achieved a high degree of social cohesion. Studies et al., 2003, 2009), although additional research is
assessing relationships between these constructs needed to buttress these indings. For communities,

271
Section 4: Specific challenges

the capacity of residents to come together to efectively of intervention in order to promote resilience. For
address common problems or crises is a consistent example, there is a growing interest in developing inter-
theme in the literature. Such capability seems particu- ventions to teach self-regulation skills in children as a
larly important when responding to natural disasters, means of promoting positive outcomes (Blair, 2002;
but it likely extends to dealing with the more chronic Diamond et al., 2007; Buckner et al., 2009). Eforts to
adversity of poverty. promote community organization and social cohe-
To survive in conditions of poverty, individ- sion might be analogous at the family and community
uals must play close attention to the acquisition and levels.
maintenance of basic resources. he stressors that As eforts to promote resilient outcomes in impov-
are part and parcel to poverty can deplete the inner erished settings oten take the form of individual and
and external resources that promote resilient out- family-level interventions, program developers and
comes. Conservation of resources (COR) theory, as society at large should be sensitive to what is realistic
articulated by Hobfoll (1988, 1989; see also Chapter in a context having few economic resources and oppor-
17), seems particularly well suited to the examination tunities. A pitfall when viewing resilience in the context
of resilience in the context of poverty. his approach of poverty is the illusion that if individuals or commu-
uses a comprehensive theory of stress and adaptation nities possess the right combination of attributes they
based on the central tenet that individuals strive to can thrive despite economic hardship. A “pull yourself
obtain, build, and protect things that they value (i.e., up by your own bootstraps” philosophy that leads to
resources) and that psychological stress and distress individual-level strengths-based interventions is not
ensue when these resources are lost, threatened with suicient. he vast majority of impoverished people
loss, or if individuals fail to replenish resources ater sufer greatly from their lack of material resources
having made a signiicant investment of some form and the social consequences of poverty. he longer
(Hobfoll, 1989). he COR theory highlights the pur- a community remains in poverty the more diicult it
poseful, goal-directed manner in which individuals, becomes to overcome the vicious cycle that has taken
families, and communities struggle to combat the root. Impoverished communities, wherever they may
stressors engendered by poverty. For example, strong be, oten require external forms of assistance to become
social ties within a community do not just happen more viable and break the proverbial “cycle of poverty.”
spontaneously; rather, they are constructed over time Such external assistance could take the form of
in a deliberate manner by those individuals involved. helping to build social cohesion and collective eicacy
Social capital is a resource that low-income individuals to respond to community needs. It is also likely that
and families seek to garner and maintain over time to external economic aid through federal and state gov-
guard against both personal and possibly collective ernment programs is also needed. In the USA, commu-
disaster. While wealthier individuals can draw from nity development block grants represent aid targeted at
savings, a home equity line of credit, or insurance pol- the community, whereas assistance programs such as
icies to ward of a personal crisis, impoverished indi- the earned income tax credit, food stamps, Section 8
viduals must rely on the “good credit” they may have housing assistance, Medicaid, TANF (cash assistance),
established with relative, friends, and neighbors when and energy assistance are important means by which
faced with comparable diiculties. his highlights the low-income individuals and families receive vital eco-
relative importance of community organization and nomic support to address their basic needs.
social cohesion as counterbalances to a lack of monet- A key virtue of the concept of resilience is its
ary resources for persons living in poverty. In spite of strengths-based approach. Promoting both individual
its relevance, we are unaware of any attempts to apply and community-level factors linked to positive out-
COR theory to this topic. Future research on resilience comes in the face of poverty is a noble task. he struc-
in the context of poverty should consider the applic- tural problems that engender poverty can sometimes
ability of COR theory in order to develop hypotheses feel overwhelming to address. Yet these factors, whether
and explain the mechanisms that engender successful they be community violence, lack of afordable hous-
adaptation despite adversities. ing, racial discrimination, insuicient educational and
Whether examining resilience at the individual, employment opportunity, or lack of basic resources,
family, or community level, factors associated with must be targeted in order to see resilient outcomes at
resilient outcomes may represent interesting targets the individual level. Having said this, structural eforts

272
Chapter 18: Resilience in the context of poverty

to alleviate poverty and interventions to promote fac- low-income youths. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
tors leading to resilient outcomes among individuals 79, 19–30.
or families are not mutually exclusive; both types of Burkey, S. (1993). People irst: A guide to self-reliant,
endeavor have important roles to play in improving the participatory rural development. London: Zed Books.
lives of people living in poverty as well as the commu- Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., Wyman, P. A., et al. (1992). Test
nities in which they live. comparisons among stress-afected, stress-resilient, and
nonclassiied 4th-6th grade urban children. Journal of
Community Psychology, 20, 200–214.
References
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., homas, J., & Munro, S. (2007).
Ahmed, R., Seedat, M., van Niekerk, A., & Bulbulia, S. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science,
(2004). Discerning community resilience in 318, 1387–1388.
disadvantaged communities in the context of violence
and injury prevention. South African Journal of Dishion, T. J. & Connell, A. (2006). Adolescents’ resilience as
Psychology, 34, 386–408. a self-regulatory process: Promising themes for linking
intervention with developmental science. Annals of the
Aspinwall, L. G. & Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in time: Self- New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 125–138.
regulation and proactive coping. Psychological Bulletin,
121, 417–436. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Guthrie, I. K. (1997). Coping
with stress: he role of regulation and development. In
Bassuk, E. L., Browne, A., & Buckner, J. C. (1996). Single S. A. Wolchik & I. N. Sandler (eds.), Handbook of
mothers and welfare. Scientiic American, 275, 60–67. children’s coping: Linking theory and intervention
Bassuk, E. L., Buckner, J. C., Weinreb, L., et al. (1997). (pp. 41–70). New York: Plenum Press.
Homelessness in female-headed families: Childhood Erikson, K. T. (1976). Everything in its path: Destruction of
and adult risk and protective factors. American Journal of community in the Bufalo Creek lood. New York: Simon &
Public Health, 87, 241–248. Schuster.
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition Fagg, J., Curtis, S., Stansfeld, S. A., et al. (2008). Area social
and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization fragmentation, social support for individuals and
of children’s functioning at school entry. American psychosocial health in young adults: Evidence from a
Psychologist, 57, 111–127. national survey in England. Social Science and Medicine,
Bonanno, G. A. & Mancini, A. D. (2008). he human 66, 242–254.
capacity to thrive in the face of potential trauma. Felsman, J. K. & Vaillant, G. E. (1987). Resilient children as
Pediatrics, 121, 369–375. adults: A 40-year study. In E. J. Anthony & B. J. Bertram
Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D. (eds.), he invulnerable child (pp. 289–314). New York:
(2007). What predicts psychological resilience ater Guilford Press.
disaster? he role of demographics, resources, and life
Glueck, S. & Glueck, E. (1950). Unraveling juvenile
stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75,
delinquency. New York: he Commonwealth Fund.
671–682.
Hobfoll, S. E. (1988). he ecology of stress. Washington, DC:
Buckner, J. C. (1988). he development of an instrument to
Hemisphere.
measure neighborhood cohesion. American Journal of
Community Psychology, 16, 771–791. Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new
attempt at conceptualizing stress. American Psychologist,
Buckner, J. C., Bassuk, E. L., Weinreb, L. F., & Brooks, M. G.
44, 513–524.
(1999). Homelessness and its relation to the mental
health and behavior of low-income school-age children. Huston, A., McLoyd, V. C., & Garcia Coll, C. (1994).
Developmental Psychology, 35, 246–257. Children and poverty: Issues in contemporary research.
Child Development, 65, 275–282.
Buckner, J. C., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. (2003).
Characteristics of resilient children living in poverty: Karoly, P. (1993). Mechanisms of self-regulation: A systems
he role of self-regulatory processes. Development and view. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 23–52.
Psychopathology, 15, 139–162. Luthar, S. S. (1991). Vulnerability and resilience: A study
Buckner, J. C., Beardslee, W. R., & Bassuk, E. L. (2004). of high-risk adolescents. Child Development, 62,
Exposure to violence and low income children’s mental 600–616.
health: Direct, moderated, and mediated relations. Luthar, S. S., Doernberger, C. H., & Zigler, E. (1993).
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 74, 413–423. Resilience is not a unidimensional construct: Insights
Buckner, J. C., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. (2009). Self- from a prospective study on inner-city adolescents.
regulation and its relations to adaptive functioning in Development and Psychopathology, 5, 703–717.

273
Section 4: Specific challenges

Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). he construct disaster readiness. American Journal of Community
of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for Psychology, 41, 127–150.
future work. Child Development, 71, 543–562. Park, R. (1916). Suggestions for the investigations of human
Magnus, K. B., Cowen, E. L., Wyman, P. A., Fagen, D. B., & behavior in the urban environment. American Journal of
Work, W. C. (1999). Correlates of resilient outcomes Sociology, 20, 577–612.
among highly stressed African-American and white Parker, G. R., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., & Wyman, P. A.
urban children. Journal of Community Psychology, 27, (1990). Test correlates of stress-afected and stress-
473–488. resilient outcomes among urban children. Journal of
Markowitz, F. E., Bellair, P. E., Liska, A. E., & Liu, J. (2001). Primary Prevention, 11, 19–35.
Extending social disorganization theory: Modeling Putnam, R. D. (1995). Bowling alone: America’s declining
the relationships between cohesion, disorder, and fear. social capital. Journal of Democracy, 6, 65–78.
Criminology, 39, 293–320.
Sampson, R. J. (2004). Neighborhood and community:
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes Collective eicacy and community safety. New Economy,
in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. 11, 106–113.
Masten, A. S. & Coatsworth, D. (1998). he development of Sampson, R. J. & Raudenbush, S. W. (1999). Systematic
competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: social observation of public spaces: A new look at
Lessons from research on successful children. American disorder in urban neighborhoods. American Journal of
Psychologist, 53, 205–220. Sociology, 105, 603–651.
Masten, A. S., Miliotis, D., Graham-Bermann, S. A., Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997).
Ramirez, M., & Neemann, J. (1993). Children in Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of
homeless families: Risks to mental health and collective eicacy. Science, 277, 918–924.
development. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Sampson, R. J., Morenof, J. D., & Gannon-Rowley, T.
Psychology, 61, 335–343. (2002). Assessing “neighborhood efects”: Social
McCubbin, H. I. & McCubbin, M. A. (1988). Typologies processes and new directions in research. Annual Review
of resilient families: Emerging roles of social class and of Sociology, 28, 443–478.
ethnicity. Family Relations, 37, 247–254. Shinn, M., Weitzman, B. C., Stojanovic, D., et al. (1998).
McCubbin, H. I., McCubbin, M. A., hompson, A. I., & Predictors of homelessness among families in New York
hompson, E. A. (1998). Resiliency in ethnic families: A City: From shelter request to housing stability. American
conceptual model for predicting family adjustment and Journal of Public Health, 88, 1651–1657.
adaptation. In H. I. McCubbin, E. A. hompson, Sonn, C. C. & Fisher, A. T. (1998). Sense of community:
A. I. hompson, & J. E. Fromer (eds.), Resiliency in Native Community resilient responses to oppression and
American and immigrant families, Vol. 2 change. Journal of Community Psychology, 26, 457–472.
(pp. 3–48). housand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Stack, C. B. (1974). All our kin: Strategies for survival in a
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and black community. New York: Harper & Row.
child development. American Psychologist, 53, 185–204.
Steptoe, A. & Feldman, P. J. (2001). Neighborhood problems
Morenof, J. D., Sampson, R. J., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2001). as sources of chronic stress: Development of a measure
Neighborhood inequality, collective eicacy, and the of neighborhood problems, and associations with
spatial dynamics of homicide. Criminology, 39, 517–560. socioeconomic status and health. Annals of Behavioral
Munasinghe, M. (2007). he importance of social capital: Medicine, 23, 177–185.
Comparing the impacts of the 2004 Asian tsunami on Suarez-Ojeda, E. N. & Autler, L. (2003). Community
Sri Lanka, and Hurricane Katrina 2005 on New Orleans. resilience: A social approach. In E. H. Grotberg (ed.),
Ecological Economics, 64, 9–11. Resilience for today: Gaining strength from adversity
National Scientiic Council on the Developing Child (2007). (pp. 189–210). Westport, CT: Praeger.
he science of early childhood development: Closing the gap Wadsworth, M. E. & Santiago, C. D. (2008). Risk and
between what we know and what we do. Cambridge, MA: resiliency processes in ethnically diverse families in
Center for the Developing Child, Harvard University, poverty. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 399–410.
http://developingchild.harvard.edu/library/reports_ Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, and recovery:
and_working_papers/science_of_early_childhood_ Perspectives from the Kauai Longitudinal Study.
development/ (accessed February 27, 2011). Development and Psychopathology, 5, 503–515.
Norris, F. H., Stevens, S. P., Pfeferbaum, B., Wyche, K. F., & Werner, E. E. & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable but not
Pfeferbaum, R. L. (2008). Community resilience as a invincible: A study of resilient children. New York:
metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for McGraw-Hill.

274
Chapter 18: Resilience in the context of poverty

Werner, E. E. & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: resilience in urban children who have experienced major
High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: life-stress. American Journal of Community Psychology,
Cornell University Press. 19, 405–426.
Werner, E. E. & Smith, R. S. (2001). Journeys from childhood Wyman, P. A., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., et al. (1999).
to midlife: Risk, resilience, and recovery. Ithaca, NY: Caregiving and developmental factors diferentiating
Cornell University Press. young at-risk urban children showing resilient versus
Wyman, P. A., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., & Parker, G. R. stress-afected outcomes: A replication and extension.
(1991). Developmental and family milieu correlates of Child Development, 70, 645–659.

275
Section
Section4 Specific challenges
Chapter
Resiliency in individuals with

19 serious mental illness


Piper S. Meyer and Kim T. Mueser

Introduction followed by a description of strategies that speciically


target resiliency skills for improving long-term out-
For many years the original term for schizophrenia,
comes. he chapter concludes with a discussion of how
dementia praecox (or premature dementia) coined by
a resiliency-focused approach to treatment could fur-
Kraepelin in 1919 (1971) conveyed the assumption
ther shit attention to important but neglected areas
that the course of this disease and other serious men-
in serious mental illness, such as the experience of
tal disorders was invariably a chronic and deteriorat-
positive emotions, thereby improving quality of life
ing one. People with schizophrenia were informed that
in spite of the potential persistence of symptoms and
there was little they could do with their lives, that they
impairments.
should give up their hopes and dreams (Deegan, 1990),
they should focus on following their treatment recom-
mendations, but they had little role to play in actual The recovery paradigm
decision making about their care. Over the past several Recovery from schizophrenia and other serious men-
decades there has been a sea change in the long-term tal disorders was once thought of strictly in medical
perspective on schizophrenia, and more broadly on the terms that emphasized the complete remission of all
concept of recovery from serious mental illness. hese symptoms and associated impairments related to the
new perspectives have been largely brought about by deining characteristics of the illness (Liberman et al.,
the rise of the mental health consumer movement 2002; Andreasen et al., 2005). More recently, there has
(Chamberlin, 1978; Davidson et al., 2009), and the been a shit away from such deinitions and towards
development of new and more personally meaningful alternative conceptualizations that are more person-
deinitions of recovery that instill hope, empowerment, ally meaningful to consumers (i.e., individuals with
and responsibility in individuals with a psychiatric ill- a serious psychiatric disorder) and are not explicitly
ness, and involve the individual as an active participant tied to the deining characteristics of their speciic
in his or her own treatment. As new perspectives on disorders. here are several factors that contributed
recovery have assumed an increasingly dominant role to consumers’ dissatisfaction with traditional medical
in the treatment of serious mental illness, the import- deinitions of recovery from mental illness that led to
ance of resiliency has been recognized, both in terms a re-conceptualization of the word, and the emergence
of understanding how people meet and cope with of the recovery paradigm of treatment.
the many challenges of their disorder and in terms of
helping them to bolster their ability to learn and grow Limitations of medical definitions
from their experiences in order to experience a full and
rewarding life (Roe & Chopra, 2003). of recovery
his chapter begins with a brief review of the evolv- A common experience of consumers was being
ing concept of recovery, and its relevance for elevating informed by well-intentioned but misguided mental
considerations of resiliency to the understanding and health professionals that recovery from severe mental
improvement of outcomes in serious mental illness. illnesses such as schizophrenia was impossible. Mental
Next, the role of resiliency as both an adaptive and a health consumers were oten told that their future
protective factor in serious mental illness is considered, prospects of a meaningful life were bleak, that their

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

276
Chapter 19: Resiliency in serious mental illness

psychiatric illness was chronic, and that they would be to other important areas, such as social relationships,
best advised to give up on any personal life goals that role functioning, and satisfaction with life. Rather than
required signiicant efort or competence (Deegan, focusing on symptoms and deicits, consumers argued
1990; Mead & Copeland, 2000). A natural consequence that they should be more actively involved in identi-
of this message for consumers was initially an over- fying the goals of treatment, and that successful treat-
whelming sense of loss or disbelief, oten followed by ment requires a shit in focus away from pathology
resentment about being told what they could and could and towards improved psychological and psychosocial
not accomplish with their lives. functioning, as deined by the consumer (Madera, 1988;
Reactions against such information and advice Deegan, 1992). As a part of questioning traditional
provided by professionals were ampliied as evidence deinitions of recovery based on psychopathology,
emerged that symptomatic and functional recovery consumers advocated for a newer, more nuanced, and
over the lifespan of people with serious mental illness more personally meaningful deinition of recovery.
was actually much more common than previously One widely cited deinition of recovery is
believed. For example, long-term outcome studies of “Recovery involves the development of a new mean-
schizophrenia have shown that between 40 and 70% of ing and purpose in one’s life as one grows beyond the
individuals demonstrate partial or complete remission catastrophic efects of mental illness” (Anthony, 1993,
of symptoms and impairment over their lives (Ciompi, p. 21). Another inluential deinition is given by Deegan
1980; Harding et al., 1987; Harrison et al., 2001). (1988, p. 13):
Medical recovery from serious mental illness does, in Recovery is a process, a way of life, an attitude and a way
fact, occur. of approaching the day’s challenges. It is not a perfectly
linear process. At times our course is erratic and we falter,
slide back, regroup, and start again… he need is to re-
Objections to traditional hierarchical establish a new and valued sense of integrity and purpose
decision making within and beyond the limits of the disability; the inspir-
ation is to live, work, and love in a community in which
Another factor contributing to a new perspective on
one makes a signiicant contribution.
recovery was the growing dissatisfaction among con-
sumers to the traditional hierarchical decision-making Although a wide range of diferent deinitions of
process that dominated psychiatric treatment of ser- recovery have been ofered (Davidson et al., 2005a;
ious mental illness until quite recently. Increasingly, Slade, 2009), most deinitions are based on one or two
consumers refused to buy the adage that “the doctor core elements: the subjective experience of the pro-
knows best” and began to demand more respect and cess of recovering and functional aspects of recov-
involvement in making decisions about their own ery. As illustrated in the two deinitions of recovery
treatment (Blaska, 1990; Campbell, 1996), in line with provided above, recovery is subjectively experienced
the trend towards shared decision making across the as an intensely personal process that is not deined by
medical spectrum (Wennberg, 1988). Consumers the attainment of the speciic end point but rather by
demanded to be recognized by mental health profes- the development of meaning and sense of purpose in
sionals as active participants in their own treatment one’s life. Most subjective deinitions of recovery place
and to be aforded a lead role in the decision-making a high priority on the consumer’s own perception of
process (Carling, 1995). Furthermore, they insisted on whether he or she is in recovery, and what that means
a role not only in making treatment decisions but also to the individual.
in deining the goals of intervention, challenging the In addition to emphasis on the experiential pro-
traditional focus of much treatment on symptoms and cess of recovery, descriptions of recovery oten refer to
relapse, as described below. changes in speciic functional outcomes. For example,
the Deegan (1988) quotation above deines the over-
Broadening the focus of treatment riding motive of recovery as “the inspiration is to live,
work, and love in a community in which one makes a sig-
beyond psychopathology niicant contribution” – a clear reference to functional
Historically, much of psychiatric treatment was outcomes such as independent living, quality of social
oriented towards the minimization of symptoms, relationships, work, and being a member of a commu-
relapses, and impairments, with less attention paid nity. Consequently, recovery can be conceptualized

277
Section 4: Specific challenges

as including both subjective aspects, related to the 1988, p. 726). Empowerment has become an import-
process of coming to grips with and growing past the ant recovery theme as a process involving respecting
experience of having developed a mental illness, and oneself, demanding respect from others, expecting and
objective aspects that tap quality of functioning in demanding collaboration from treatment providers,
areas such as social relationships, work, and commu- and possessing a strong conviction in one’s own sense
nity integration. of agency and self-determination.

Themes of recovery Coping


In addition to new and more personally meaningful Although recovery is not deined by the psychiatric
deinitions of recovery, several themes of recovery have illness per se, a common theme in writings about
emerged from consumers’ writings on the topic (Ralph, recovery is the importance of coping strategies for min-
2000). Common recovery themes include hope and imizing the efects of symptoms or other aspects of the
optimism, self-respect and self-determination, coping, illness on day to day functioning (Deegan & Afa, 1995;
and openness to discovery and new experiences. hese Macdonald et al., 1998; Roe et al., 2006). One consumer
are briely described below. wrote the following eloquent description of the import-
ance of coping in her life (Leete, 1989, p. 197):
Hope and optimism More than by any other one thing, my life has been
Numerous writings on recovery emphasize that hope changed by schizophrenia. For the past 20 years I have
and optimism are the wellspring from which motiv- lived with it and in spite of it – struggling to come to
ation to change emerges (Fisher, 1992; Deegan, 1996; terms with it without giving in to it. Although I have
fought a daily battle, it is only now that I have some
Perry et al., 2007). At the most basic level, the belief
sense of conidence that I will survive my ordeal. Taking
that change is possible is critical if individuals are to responsibility for my life and developing coping mecha-
invest the time and efort necessary to improve their nisms has been crucial to my recovery.
lives. Deegan and colleagues (Deegan 1996; Perry et al.,
2007) noted that, when people with serious mental ill-
ness themselves have no hope, others around them can
Openness to discovery and new experiences
form a “conspiracy of hope” that is capable of touching Consumers oten describe the experience of recovery as
and ultimately inspiring hope for those who previously similar to taking a journey or even being on an adven-
had none. ture (Slade, 2009). he development of a serious men-
tal illness can be a life-shattering event that challenges
Self-respect and self-determination an individual’s outlook on the world and on himself or
herself. he experience can present a threat to the indi-
he emphasis in traditional psychiatric treatment on
vidual’s self-identity, and evoked anxiety about what is
hierarchical decision making, combined with coer-
perceived to be a less predictable and uncertain future.
cive treatment and cultural and institutional stressors
Accepting the fact of having a mental illness requires
related to mental illness (e.g., social extrusion and
the development of a new sense of identity, and an
stigma), has led to personal empowerment and choice
openness to the experience of recovery and the many
as core themes of recovery (Miller, 1990; Beale &
surprises it may bring (Ralph & Corrigan, 2005). While
Lambric, 1995; Fisher et al., 1996). Consumers have
these experiences involve challenges, they also provide
oten expressed resentment at not being treated as
opportunities for personal transformation and growth
genuine partners in their own treatment and a feeling
(Roe, 2001; Roe & Chopra, 2003). Viewed from such a
of lack of respect from treatment providers, family,
perspective, the recovery process is a learning oppor-
and society at large. Lack of respect frequently trans-
tunity that can enrich the individual’s life rather then
lates to low levels of self-esteem and self-stigma when
deprive him or her of valuable opportunities.
individuals adopt negative societal attitudes towards
mental illness as their own (Estrof, 1989; MacInnes &
Lewis, 2008). Empowerment has been deined as “a pro- Summary
cess by which individuals gain mastery or control over he themes described here indicate that recovery is a
their own lives and democratic participation in the multidimensional experience that combines elements
life of their community” (Zimmerman & Rappaport, of both process and outcome. he positive orientation

278
Chapter 19: Resiliency in serious mental illness

of this new conceptualization of recovery, including individual coping skills serve to moderate the noxious
the recognition that people can and do recover their efects of stress, providing some protection. While the
lives and establish a sense of purpose ater developing focus of outcome of the stress–vulnerability model is
a serious mental illness, suggests that resiliency is of on psychopathology, resiliency factors (coping, social
central interest to understanding and promoting the support) are alluded to for their role in reducing the
recovery process. negative efects of stress.
However, taking a resiliency perspective could
Resiliency in serious mental illness broaden both the options for treatment and the goals
of treatment. Instead of only focusing on resiliency fac-
Resiliency is oten deined in terms of two key com-
tors such as coping skills and social support, a wider
ponents: the ability to bounce back from adversity
range of resiliency factors could be developed to min-
or trauma, and the positive adaptations in response
imize the efects of stress on biological vulnerability
to a stressful event, such as developing new insight
(Edward & Warelow, 2005). Further, a broader var-
or strengths that improve functioning or well-being
iety of resiliency factors becomes relevant when the
(Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000; Masten, 2001; Atkinson
focus of treatment shits from reducing symptoms and
et al., 2009). If recovery is deined as the process of
relapses to facilitating the recovery process and achiev-
adapting to and growing past disability, and the devel-
ing recovery goals such as improved functioning, sense
opment of personally meaningful goals, resiliency
of purpose, and well-being.
can be understood as the attributes, strategies, and
here are numerous resilient qualities described in
resources that enable a person to establish a rewarding
the literature. Some mentioned oten include personal
life. Recovery is a subjective experience shaped by one’s
optimism, faith, subjective well-being, creativity, grati-
personal goals, whereas the strengths and resources
tude, forgiveness, perseverance, adaptability, tolerance,
that an individual builds upon and further develops
self-reliance, and viewing one’s life as meaningful; other
are the tools that facilitate engagement in the process
resources that contribute to resiliency include social
of recovery and progress towards recovery goals.
and family support (Richardson, 2002; Atkinson et al.,
In the context of recovery, resiliency then becomes
2009). Integrating resiliency into treatment for people
a bridge between the past qualities, skills, and strat-
with serious mental illness begins by eliciting resili-
egies used to overcome adversity and a person’s dreams
ent qualities such as strengths and resources, along
and aspirations for the future. Resiliency is one process
with understanding deicits and weaknesses. hrough
that facilitates recovery and helps clinicians to promote
eliciting consumers’ own resiliency stories in personal
individual competencies, focusing on healthy adjust-
narratives that focus on overcoming past challenges,
ment and enhancing adaptation (Tedeschi & Kilmer,
people can recall and rediscover the strengths they
2005). Resilient skills and strategies developed in, or
used to deal with setbacks in the past and build on them
employed in, past stressful experiences have the poten-
to spring back from their current predicament. hese
tial to help consumers to broaden the use of their own
stories help people to visualize a path to their goals and
natural coping strategies. People who are helped to
provide examples of coping strategies based on their
strengthen their resilient qualities may learn to apply
strengths. Resiliency stories ofer a sense of hope to
these strengths in diferent situations to promote their
people with serious mental illness, a common feature of
conidence, competence, and well-being.
both recovery and resiliency. Hope is critical to people
pursuing their own personal pathways to recovery and
Resiliency factors achieving their goals. Hope is also an agent for change
he most inluential theory associated with the through helping people to conceptualize and sustain
development and course of mental illness is the motivation for personal goals (Lopez et al., 2004). A
stress–vulnerability model. his states that the devel- person with serious mental illness experiences periods
opment of symptoms and/or relapses is an interaction when his or her illness interferes with life and periods
between biological and environmental or stress fac- when the illness does not. Re-discovering resilient
tors (Zubin & Spring, 1977; Nuechterlein & Dawson, qualities from the past helps individuals to identify
1984). Biologically, alcohol and drug use can worsen strengths that help them “to bounce back.” Questions
biological vulnerability, while medication can reduce such as “Tell me about a diicult time in your life” and
it. his model also states that social support and “What helped you get through that time?” can be used

279
Section 4: Specific challenges

to help people to feel more hopeful about future possi- resilient qualities, and the insight and growth that
bilities for recovery (Tedeschi & Kilmer, 2005). occur as a result of adversity. As resiliency theory is now
in its third wave, researchers are trying to identify the
Resiliency in traditional mental health motivational forces that underlie eforts to be resilient.
Research has suggested that the sources of this motiv-
treatment ation may stem from ecological triggers such as a belief
he concept of resiliency was rarely evoked in the con- in God, thoughts or feelings that cause neuropeptide
text of early, pathology-oriented treatment approaches releases in the brain, or increases in cells that fortify the
for serious mental illness, as described above. When immune system (Richardson, 2002).
the dominant belief was that people could not get bet- he consumer movement has faced some similar
ter, the concept of resiliency and of “springing back” challenges and could learn something from the devel-
from a chronic, disabling illness seemed irrelevant. opment of resiliency theory. As the deinition of recov-
As a new and more personally meaningful deinition ery has broadened, so has the exploration of individual
of recovery has emerged from the consumer move- themes across deinitions of recovery (Rodgers et al.,
ment, the relevance of resiliency has become apparent. 2007; Bonney & Stickley, 2008). his, in turn, has led to
Recovery from serious mental illness is now under- the development of models of the recovery process as
stood as involving personal growth, expanding abil- the consumer movement has moved into a partnership
ities, and developing a meaningful life despite illness with mainstream mental health services (Andresen
(Davidson et al., 2005b). he ability to achieve the et al., 2003). Transforming mental health systems to
goals of recovery implies the presence of individual support recovery requires a better understanding
resilience. Similarly, the common themes of recovery, of the motivation for recovery, such as identiication
including hope, optimism, and self-determination, are and fostering of recovery goals, and of how resilient
frequently used to characterize resiliency (Brodkin & qualities and resources can help consumers to achieve
Coleman, 1996; Andresen et al., 2003; Geanellos, 2005; those goals. Such resources may include, but are not
Atkinson et al., 2009). limited to, becoming a contributing member of one’s
As recovery became a legitimate focus of treat- community, connecting with friends and family, and
ment for people with serious mental illness, attention establishing meaning and purpose in life. Researchers
shited towards developing services that were more in have begun to examine one part of recovery through
line with the goals of recovery. Recovery-oriented ser- helping consumers to establish personally meaningful
vices have been described as being person centered, goals that can enhance motivation, but understanding
strengths based, and informed by the needs of the indi- the motivation that is integral to the recovery process is
vidual (Rodgers et al., 2007). his deinition is inform- still in its early stages (Mueser et al., 2002; Farkas et al.,
ative to programs but provides little detail about how to 2005; Clarke et al., 2006). Recovery is deined by a per-
transform services to become more recovery oriented. son’s goals and their desire and motivation for recov-
he relevance of resiliency as a personal attribute to ery. Resilient qualities and resources can help a person
achieving the goals of recovery suggests that resiliency to access their strengths and build new skills that foster
theory may ofer some insight into strategies to further the attainment of recovery goals.
promote the transformation of mental health services
to make them more recovery focused. Resilience strategies in existing treatment
Research on resiliency theory has progressed
through three stages to develop recommendations for interventions
building resiliency in treatment. he irst wave focused Interventions that incorporate a resilience framework
on identifying the resilient qualities in people that help into the treatment model have the potential to improve
them to bounce back from adversity. Some of the most the outcomes for people with serious mental illness.
common resilient qualities that had helped people to Consumers who enhance existing resilient qualities
survive adversity were identiied from longitudinal and learn new resiliency skills have an increased cap-
studies of children over 30 years, children of parents acity to make greater progress towards their recovery
with schizophrenia, and studies of character traits in goals (Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000; Tedeschi & Kilmer,
adults. he second wave described the process through 2005). he following paragraphs summarize a num-
which individuals might develop and strengthen ber of treatment strategies that contribute to the

280
Chapter 19: Resiliency in serious mental illness

development of resilient qualities and resources. hese problems, individuals are able to develop a resiliency
strategies represent a selection of what is currently skill to better enable them to face challenges or adver-
used in the treatment of serious mental illness and sity, and reduce stress in their lives. As people learn
some approaches that have not typically been applied problem-solving skills, they oten experience rapid
in this population but have strong potential for pro- success, feel better about themselves, and become
moting work towards recovery. Descriptions will focus more motivated to change. Within the framework of
not only on how the strategies reduce distress but also the stress–vulnerability model, these skills serve as a
on building competence and positive experiences. protective factor that moderates the efects of stress.
In the illness management and recovery program,
Resilience strategies targeting the effects of stress consumers are taught a behavioral problem-solving
Treatment strategies commonly used for people with model at the beginning of treatment that focuses on
serious mental illness have tended to focus on man- clearly deining the problem, generating alternative
aging or reducing psychopathology. Some of these solutions, and trying out the solution best suited to
strategies have been inluenced by the stress–vulner- the individual (Mueser et al., 2002). hese skills are
ability model, and aim at helping people to reduce the essential in helping an individual to achieve his or
efects of stress and the likelihood of stress-induced her recovery goal, by presenting a framework that he
relapses. hree strategies that are commonly used in or she can use when faced with a challenge or obs-
treatment are enhancing coping skills, teaching prob- tacle. Problem-solving skills are also a key ingredient
lem-solving skills, and building social support (Mueser in skills training interventions. People with serious
et al., 2002). Coping is one of the fundamental com- mental illness can identify and describe problems in
ponents in the resiliency process, such that a person their life and use a problem-solving model to ind a
who is resilient has coped successfully in spite of adver- manageable solution (Liberman et al., 2001). hese
sity (Tusaie & Dyer, 2004). Research has shown that strategies have been efective at helping people to
50–100% of those with serious mental illness spontan- function more independently and to manage their
eously use coping strategies to deal with their illness in symptoms better (Kopelowicz et al., 2006).
response to stressors or symptoms, but the strategies Another key factor that moderates stress while
that they choose are not always efective (Garcelan & also serving as a protective factor is social support.
Rodriguez, 2002; Farhall et al., 2007). Coping strategies By decreasing interpersonal stress and increasing
are oten taught to people with serious mental illness social support, people can increase their resilient
because they can be learnt and serve as protective fac- qualities and resources. Social skill deicits are com-
tors that moderate against intense emotion. Individuals mon among people with serious mental illness and
who are able to recognize stressful situations can use strongly associated with the course and outcome of
efective coping strategies before their symptoms illness (Mueser & Bellack, 1998). Studies have shown
become overwhelming (Garcelan & Rodriguez, 2002). that social competence is also highly correlated with
Cognitive-behavioral strategies that emphasize cog- increased life satisfaction in people with serious men-
nitive restructuring also have a goal of enhancing tal illness (Salokangas et al., 2006). Social skills train-
coping skills by linking thoughts and feelings about ing uses social learning techniques to teach people
symptoms, normalizing symptoms, and identifying how to communicate better their needs and emotions
the negative beliefs and assumptions about diagnosis to help them to meet their goals, and this has been
(Fowler et al., 1995; Morrison et al., 2004). he focus of shown to improve outcomes, including treatment
treatment is the individual meaning attached to symp- adherence, social role functioning, and self-eicacy
toms along with the person’s understanding and cop- (Kurtz & Mueser, 2008). As individuals are better able
ing strategies for symptoms (Turkington et al., 2006; to interact with others and improve social relation-
Tai & Turkington, 2009). Fostering the use of efective ships, so they build social supports that serve as a buf-
coping strategies in treatment can increase the indi- fer against stress and future adversities (Bebbington
vidual’s resiliency in response to stress or persistent & Kuipers, 1992), leading to increased resiliency.
symptoms. Additionally, social support can lead to an increased
Problem solving is another skill oten taught support network that provides assurance that the per-
to people with serious mental illness (Falloon son is not alone and ofers some people meaning and
et al., 2007). By taking steps to identify and resolve purpose in life (Neenan, 2009).

281
Section 4: Specific challenges

Resilience strategies targeting deficits and incorporate a person’s strengths builds resiliency
he next group of treatment strategies describes meth- by highlighting individual competencies and build-
ods aimed less at decreasing psychopathology and ing resources. hese activities also provide direction
more on the common deicits associated with serious for treatment and inform the goal-setting process. For
mental illness. hree areas that have received some example, if people have diiculty setting goals, encour-
attention include improving self-esteem, inding rec- aging them to ind an absorbing activity initially can
reational interests and activities, and setting and work- lead to a more meaningful goal later.
ing towards a meaningful goal. People with serious Goals are what give life meaning and purpose.
mental illness are oten disempowered by hierarchical Setting and pursuing goals gives someone’s life direc-
treatment decision making that ignores their voice, tion, boosts self-esteem and conidence, adds struc-
stigmatizing attitudes that marginalize their potential ture and meaning to daily life, helps in accomplishing
for contribution, and self-stigma that limits their own tasks by breaking them down into smaller steps, and
eforts to take control of their lives. All lead to feelings encourages social connections (Lyubomirsky, 2008).
of hopelessness, worthlessness, and low self-esteem. People build resilient qualities and resources by setting
Treatments that target improving self-esteem may lead and working towards goals. Goal setting is an import-
to improvements in coping and competence, which do ant part of many treatment interventions designed to
serve as protective factors against relapse. One program increase problem-solving abilities, remediate weak-
for people with serious mental illness that targeted self- nesses, create hope, achieve more efective coping,
esteem by focusing on improving self-worth, compe- and facilitate the recovery process (Clarke et al., 2006).
tence, and self-determination found improvements in Traditionally, most treatment interventions have
coping abilities and self-esteem (Lecomte et al., 1999). helped consumers to set goals in order to remediate
People who have a healthy self-esteem feel more coni- deicits, but there has been a shit more recently toward
dent in coping with stressful situations and, as a result, ensuring that the goals set are personally meaningful
are going to be more resilient. Interventions such as to the consumer. Consumers, in fact, have stated how
the one described above focus on helping people to feel important goals are to their recovery process (Marshall
better about their lives and giving them tools to cope et al., 2007). Treatment programs such as the illness
more efectively with adversity in the future. management and recovery program view goals as crit-
Having an absorbing interest can be a source of ical to the rehabilitation of people with serious men-
social support, giving life purpose and meaning, and tal illness and as one of the core features of recovery to
improved well-being, both of which are connected to help people to achieve something meaningful in their
increased resiliency. A hobby or recreational activ- lives (Mueser et al., 2002).
ity provides a distraction from daily life but can also
remind people how to enjoy life. Such an activity ofers Novel resiliency treatment strategies
people the opportunity to become an expert, to increase Other resilience-building strategies have rarely been
their happiness, and to pursue goals related to recovery used to treat people with serious mental illness.
in their lives (Neenan, 2009). People with serious men- Savoring and mindfulness, gratitude, practicing acts
tal illness oten have little recreation in their lives, com- of kindness, and humor are all strategies that focus on
pounded by the lack of motivation that can be associated increasing positive emotions and well-being, rather
with their illness. Strategies to enhance leisure and rec- than remediating weaknesses or the ability to man-
reational activities can lead to increased motivation age stress. Positive emotions such as hope and opti-
towards recovery as people experience more positive mism have been found to bufer against depression and
emotions. Interventions that have focused on increas- have been linked to better life outcomes, mental and
ing leisure and recreational activities have used both physical health, more efective coping, and increased
a skills training and a recreational therapy approach lifespan (Seligman, 2002; Lyubomirsky et al., 2005a).
for individuals with serious mental illness and have Positive emotions have an important lasting efect
found that they are not only interested in inding leis- on a person’s ability to build resilience such that over
ure activities but are also motivated to change behav- time these experiences aggregate into life-changing
iors to maintain these activities ater treatment (Roder resources. he repeated experience of positive emo-
et al., 1998). Finding activities that are both absorbing tions can become habitual, leading to an increase in the

282
Chapter 19: Resiliency in serious mental illness

personal resources that can be used to cope more efect- practice of these skills. Consumers who use savoring
ively in times of stress. In fact, research has shown that and mindfulness can build resources by experiencing
short-term efects of positive emotions lead to long- more positive emotions that can enhance resilient
term physical, social, and psychological resources, qualities and resources.
including resiliency (Cohn et al., 2009). Although Gratitude is appreciating the things that are import-
interventions have not traditionally used these strat- ant to a person and stimulates concern for others.
egies for people with serious mental illness, engaging When the focus of the gratitude is another person,
in pleasurable activities has been related to improved expressing the associated positive feelings to that per-
ability to regulate depressive symptoms, leading to bet- son makes them feel appreciated and valued, in much
ter self-esteem in this population and suggesting that the same way the person expressing the gratitude feels.
positive emotions can play an important role in recov- Enhancing gratitude may serve to build resilient qual-
ery (Davidson, 2003). ities and resources because it is associated with the
Savoring can be described as the thoughts that development of social connections and coping skills
enhance experiences from the past, present, and future. that foster well-being in the face of stress (McCullough
It has been deined as an ability to enjoy positive expe- et al., 2001). Research has shown that gratitude is
riences and the thoughts and behaviors associated with incompatible with negative emotions and may ofer
building, engaging in, and lengthening these experi- protection against psychiatric disorders (Bono &
ences (Bryant & Verof, 2007; Lyubomirsky, 2008). In McCullough, 2006). Teaching skills to increase a sense
essence, savoring allows a person to bring the positive of gratitude, and inhibit negative emotions, could be
emotions from past experiences, a current situation, particularly important for people with serious mental
or anticipating future events into the present moment. illness given the amount of depression associated with
People who are better able to experience pleasure in the schizophrenia (Drake & Cotton, 1986; Häfner et al.,
present moment and to hold onto those feelings experi- 2005) and, by deinition, with major mood disorders.
ence less depression or stress (Lyubomirsky, 2008). he lives of those afected are oten characterized by
Savoring has been shown to improve well-being and multiple setbacks, and at times it may be diicult for
self-concept (Bryant et al., 2005). Speciic strategies have them to ind the blessings in their daily lives; however,
been developed to enhance savoring, including sharing enhancing gratitude may be even more important in
good news with others, replaying happy moments, and addressing these issues given its incompatibility with
taking pleasure in ordinary experiences (Lyubomirsky, negative emotions and its role in building of social rela-
2008). Mindfulness can be considered an aspect of tionships. Feeling grateful for the good things in one’s
savoring where a person is open to the present experi- life and sharing this with others can buoy resilience and
ence or has increased and enhanced attention to the serve to bufer the efects of whatever stress the individ-
present moment (Bryant & Verof, 2007). Mindfulness ual is under.
also has been associated with increases in positive Another strategy that encourages the building
emotions, and use of mindfulness over time creates of positive emotions is practicing acts of kindness.
improvements in mood and decreases in stress (Brown Social relationships are a key ingredient to well-being
& Ryan, 2003). People with serious mental illness oten and, as discussed above, can serve as a bufer against
have negative trait afectivity (Blanchard et al., 1998), stress-induced relapses (Buchanan, 1995). Research
and strategies that enhance the experience of savoring has suggested that when people help others they rate
and mindfulness ofer skills to build positive emotions their moods as more positive (Lyubomirsky et al.,
into their daily life. he skills of savoring are relatively 2005a). Indeed, the process of assisting others ben-
easy to learn and can be applied in situations to reduce eits the helper as much as the person helped (known
the efects of stress and symptoms on functioning. as the helper’s principle) (Gartner & Riessman, 1977;
People can learn to replay the happy moments from the Madera, 1988; Campbell, 1989, 1997; Carpinello
past, relish positive events in the present, and anticipate et al., 1992). People with serious mental illness are oten
positive experiences from their dreams. People with dependent upon other people for many things in their
negative symptoms such as anhedonia oten have par- life, such as inancial help, housing, and transportation.
ticular deicits in the ability to anticipate and reminisce As a result, they may experience feelings of a loss of
about pleasurable events (Burbridge & Barch, 2007; control in their lives and may not have as many oppor-
Gard et al., 2007), and could beneit from deliberate tunities to help other people. he beneits of practicing

283
Section 4: Specific challenges

acts of kindness, such as feeling more positive about development of a broader deinition of recovery has
one’s community, increased sense of cooperation, and led to a wider range of treatment options for people.
increased awareness of the good things in one’s life, To help an individual to achieve recovery more fully,
seem even more relevant to people with serious mental treatments need to focus on how to improve well-being
illness. Practicing acts of kindness may also result in outside of the psychopathology model. Interventions
strengthening feelings of conidence, control, and opti- that include strategies to build gratitude, savoring
mism about the future (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005b). and mindfulness, practicing acts of kindness, and
he beneits of having a sense of humor have been humor generate, enhance, and prolong positive emo-
linked to increases in psychological well-being as well tions, thus building a greater sense of well-being in
as being a means to cope more efectively with stressful the process of recovery rather than only minimizing
situations. Speciically, using ailiative humor, a non- the dysfunction. hese strategies represent a picture
threatening humor style used to enhance relationships, of treatment that is focused both on enhancing well-
may facilitate the development of social connections being by building positive emotions and on reducing
and improved well-being (Kuiper & McHale, 2009). the impact of stress on illness. All of these strategies
Furthermore, the ability to laugh at oneself and one’s recognize that the treatment of the illness must also
own human failures can promote health psychological address how a person pursues well-being and happi-
adjustment, even in the face of loss or other stres- ness, because remittance of symptoms does not always
sors (Vaillant, 1977; Taylor et al., 2000). Research has result in positive well-being and outcomes (Fava et al.,
shown that people with serious mental illness display 2007). Treatments that address a person’s meaningful
some deicits in detecting humor when the situation is goals and positive life activities and experiences help
presented but still are able to appreciate humor (Tsoi the person more readily to embrace recovery and build
et al., 2008). herefore, developing a deeper sense of resiliency.
humor may be adaptive for people with serious mental
illness. Adaptive humor strategies focused on efect- References
ive coping, relating to other people, and looking at a
Andreasen, N. C., Carpenter, W. T. Jr., Kane, J. M. et al.
situation from a diferent perspective can be taught as
(2005). Remission in schizophrenia: Proposed criteria
skills to build resilient qualities and serve as a bufer and rationale for consensus. American Journal of
against stressors. Teaching humor also gives people Psychiatry, 162, 441–449.
a chance to experience positive emotions as part of Andresen, R., Oades, L., & Caputi, P. (2003). he experience
treatment and the recovery process (Davidson et al., of recovery from schizophrenia: Towards an empirically
2006). validated stage model. Australian and New Zealand
Journal of Psychiatry, 37, 586–594.
Anthony, W. A. (1993). Recovery from mental illness: he
Conclusions guiding vision of the mental health service system in the
Resilient qualities and resources are directly associ- 1990s. Psychosocial Rehabilitation Journal, 16, 11–23.
ated with helping a person to move past a stressful Atkinson, P. A., Martin, C. R., & Rankin, J. (2009).
or traumatic experience while building resources to Resilience revisited. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental
face future adversities. A person with serious mental Health Nursing, 16, 137–145.
illness is faced with many diicult stressors, which Beale, V. & Lambric, T. (1995). he recovery concept:
can be compounded by illness-related symptoms. he Implementation in the mental health system – A report by
most commonly used elements of treatment have pri- the Community Support Program Advisory Committee.
Columbus, OH: Department of Mental Health, Oice of
marily focused on helping to overcome the deicits
Consumer Services.
through strategies such as coping, problem solving,
and increasing social support. hese treatment meth- Bebbington, P. E. & Kuipers, L. (1992). Life events and
social factors. In D. J. Kavanagh (ed.), Schizophrenia: An
ods mainly build resilient qualities and resources by overview and practical handbook (pp. 126–144). London:
moderating the efects of stress on symptoms and in Chapman & Hall.
daily life. As treatment components move away from
Blanchard, J. J., Mueser, K. T., & Bellack, A. S. (1998).
the psychopathology model, a few interventions have Anhedonia, positive and negative afect, and social
used strategies to target self-esteem, inding absorb- functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24,
ing activities, and pursuing meaningful goals. he 413–424.

284
Chapter 19: Resiliency in serious mental illness

Blaska, B. (1990). he myriad medication mistakes in Clarke, S. P., Oades, L. G., Crowe, T. P., & Deane, F. P. (2006).
psychiatry: A consumer’s view. Hospital and Community Collaborative goal technology: heory and practice.
Psychiatry, 41, 993–997. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 30, 129–136.
Bonney, S. & Stickley, T. (2008). Recovery and mental Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., Mikels, J. A., &
health: A review of the British literature. Journal of Conway, A. M. (2009). Happiness unpacked: Positive
Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 15, 140–153. emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience.
Emotion, 9, 361–368.
Bono, G. & McCullough, M. E. (2006). Positive responses
to beneit and harm: Bringing forgiveness and gratitude Davidson, L. (2003). Living outside mental illness: Qualitative
into cognitive psychotherapy. Journal of Cognitive studies of recovery in schizophrenia. New York: New York
Psychotherapy, 20, 147–158. University Press.
Brodkin, A. & Coleman, M. (1996). What makes a child Davidson, L., Lawless, M. S., & Leary, F. (2005a). Concepts
resilient? Instructor, 105, 28–29. of recovery: competing or complementary? Current
Opinion in Psychiatry, 18, 664–667.
Brown, K. W. & Ryan, R. M. (2003). he beneits of being
present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well- Davidson, L., Harding, C., & Spaniol, L. (2005b). Recovery
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, from severe mental illnesses: Research evidence and
822–848. implications for practice, Vol 1. Boston, MA: Center for
Psychiatric Rehabilitation of Boston University.
Bryant, F. B. & Verof, J. (2007). Savoring: A new model of
positive experience. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Davidson, L., Shahar, G., Lawless, M. S., Sells, D., &
Tondora, J. (2006). Play, pleasure, and other positive life
Bryant, F. B., Smart, C. M., & King, S. P. (2005). Using the events: “Non-speciic” factors in recovery from mental
past to enhance the present: Boosting happiness through illness? Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes,
positive reminiscence. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 69, 151–163.
227–260.
Davidson, L., Tondora, J., Lawless, M. S., O ’ Connell, M. J., &
Buchanan, J. (1995). Social support and schizophrenia: A Rowe, M. (2009). A practical guide to recovery-oriented
review of the literature. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 9, practice: tools for transforming mental health care. New
68–76. York: Oxford University Press.
Burbridge, J. A. & Barch, D. M. (2007). Anhedonia and the Deegan, P. E. (1988). Recovery: he lived experience of
experience of emotion in individuals with schizophrenia. rehabilitation. Psychosocial Rehabilitation Journal, 11,
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 30–42. 11–19.
Campbell, J. (1989). he Well Being Project: Mental health Deegan, P. E. (1990). Spirit breaking: When the helping
clients speak for themselves, Vol. 6. Sacramento, CA: professionals hurt. Humanistic Psychologist, 18,
California Network of Mental Health Clients. 301–313.
Campbell, J. (1996). Toward collaborative mental health Deegan, P. E. (1992). he Independent Living Movement
outcomes systems. New Directions for Mental Health and people with psychiatric disabilities: Taking back
Services, 71, 69–78. control over our own lives. Psychosocial Rehabilitation
Campbell, J. (1997). How consumers/survivors are Journal, 15, 3–19.
evaluating the quality of psychiatric care. Evaluation Deegan, P. E. (1996). Recovery and the conspiracy of
Review, 21, 357–363. hope. In Proceedings of the Sixth Annual Mental Health
Carling, P. J. (1995). Return to community: Building support Conference of Australia and New Zealand, Brisbane,
systems for people with psychiatric disabilities. New York: Australia.
Guilford Press. Deegan, P. E. & Afa, C. (1995). Coping with voices: Self-help
Carpinello, S. E., Knight, E., & Jatulis, L. L. (1992). A study strategies for people who hear voices that are distressing.
of the meaning of self-help, self-help group processes, Lawrence, MA: National Empowerment Center.
and outcomes. In Proceedings of the hird Annual Drake, R. E. & Cotton, P. G. (1986). Depression,
Meeting of the National Association of State Mental Health hopelessness and suicide in chronic schizophrenia.
Program Directors, Alexandria, VA. British Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 554–559.
Chamberlin, J. (1978). On our own: Patient-controlled Edward, K.-L. & Warelow, P. (2005). Resilience: When
alternatives to the mental health system. New York: coping is emotionally intelligent. Journal of the American
Hawthorne. Psychiatric Nurses Association, 11, 101–102.
Ciompi, L. (1980). Catamnestic long-term study on the Estrof, S. E. (1989). Self, identity, and subjective experiences
course of life and aging of schizophrenics. Schizophrenia of schizophrenia: In search of the subject. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 6, 606–618. Bulletin, 15, 189–196.

285
Section 4: Specific challenges

Falloon, I. R. H., Barbieri, L., Boggian, I., & Lamonaca, D. Kopelowicz, A., Liberman, R. P., & Zarate, R. (2006). Recent
(2007). Problem solving training for schizophrenia: advances in social skills training for schizophrenia.
Rationale and review. Journal of Mental Health, 16, Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32, S12–S23.
553–568. Kraepelin, E. (1971). Dementia praecox and paraphrenia.
Farhall, J., Greenwood, K. M., & Jackson, H. J. (2007). [R. M. Barclay, trans.] New York: Krieger.
Coping with hallucinated voices in schizophrenia: Kuiper, N. A. & McHale, N. (2009). Humor styles as
A review of self-initiated strategies and therapeutic mediators between self-evaluative standards and
interventions. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 476–493. psychological well-being. Journal of Psychology:
Farkas, M., Gagne, C., Anthony, W., & Chamberlin, J. Interdisciplinary and Applied, 143, 359–376.
(2005). Implementing recovery oriented evidence Kurtz, M. M. & Mueser, K. T. (2008). A meta-analysis
based programs: Identifying the critical dimensions. of controlled research on social skills training for
Community Mental Health Journal, 41, 141–158. schizophrenia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Fava, G. A., Ruini, C., & Belaise, C. (2007). he concept of Psychology, 76, 491–504.
recovery in major depression. Psychological Medicine, 37, Lecomte, T., Cyr, M., Lesage, A. D., et al. (1999). Eicacy of a
307–317. self-esteem module in the empowerment of individuals
Fisher, D. B. (1992). Humanizing the recovery process. with schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Resources, 4, 5–6. Disease, 187, 406–413.
Fisher, W. A., Penney, D. J., & Earle, K. (1996). Mental health Leete, E. (1989). How I perceive and manage my illness.
services recipients: heir role in shaping organizational Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15, 197–200.
policy. Administration and Policy in Mental Health, 23, Liberman, R. P., Eckman, T. A., & Marder, S. R. (2001).
547–553. Training in social problem solving among persons with
Fowler, D., Garety, P., & Kuipers, E. (1995). Cognitive schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services, 52, 31–33.
behaviour therapy for psychosis: heory and practice. Liberman, R. P., Kopelowicz, A., Ventura, J., & Gutkind, D.
Chichester, UK: Wiley. (2002). Operational criteria and factors related to
Garcelan, S. P. & Rodriguez, A. G. (2002). Coping strategies recovery from schizophrenia. International Review of
in psychotics: Conceptualization and research results. Psychiatry, 14, 256–272.
Psychology in Spain, 6, 26–40. Lopez, S. J., Snyder, C. R., Magyar-Moe, J. L., et al. (2004).
Gard, D. E., Kring, A. M., Gard, M. G., Horan, W. P., & Strategies for accentuating hope: Positive psychology in
Green, M. F. (2007). Anhedonia in schizophrenia: practice (pp. 388–404). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Distinctions between anticipatory and consummatory Luthar, S. S. & Cicchetti, D. (2000). he construct of
pleasure. Schizophrenia Research, 93, 253–260. resilience: Implications for interventions and social
Gartner, A. & Riessman, F. (1977). Self-help in the human policies. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 857–885.
services. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). he how of happiness: A scientiic
Geanellos, R. (2005). Adversity as opportunity: Living approach to getting the life you want. New York: he
with schizophrenia and developing a resilient self. Penguin Press.
International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 14, Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005a). he beneits
7–15. of frequent positive afect: does happiness lead to
Häfner, H., Maurer, K., Trendler, G., et al. (2005). success? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803–855.
Schizophrenia and depression: Challenging the Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., & Schkade, D. (2005b).
paradigm of two separate diseases – A controlled study Pursuing happiness: he architecture of sustainable
of schizophrenia, depression and healthy controls. change. Review of General Psychology, 9, 111–131.
Schizophrenia Research, 77, 11–24.
Macdonald, E. M., Pica, S., McDonald, S., Hayes, R. L., &
Harding, C. M., Brooks, G. W., Ashikaga, T., Strauss, J. S., & Baglioni, A. J. J. (1998). Stress and coping in early psychosis:
Breir, A. (1987). he Vermont longitudinal study of Role of symptoms, self-eicacy, and social support in
persons with severe mental illness: I. Methodology, study coping with stress. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172(Suppl.
sample and overall status 32 years later. American Journal 33), 122–127.
of Psychiatry, 144, 718–726. MacInnes, D. L. & Lewis, M. (2008). he evaluation of a
Harrison, G., Hopper, K., Craig, T., et al. (2001). Recovery short group programme to reduce self-stigma in people
from psychotic illness: A 15- and 25-year international with serious and enduring mental health problems.
follow-up study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 15,
506–517. 59–65.

286
Chapter 19: Resiliency in serious mental illness

Madera, E. J. (1988). Seven principles in self help: Richardson, G. E. (2002). he metatheory of resilience
Understanding how self-help groups help. New Program and resiliency. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58,
Initiatives in Mental Health, May, 3–4 [Newsletter 307–321.
published by the New Jersey State Division of Mental Roder, V., Jenull, B., & Brenner, H. D. (1998). Teaching
Health & Hospitals.] schizophrenic patients recreational, residential and
Marshall, S. L., Crowe, T. P., Oades, L. G., Deane, F. F., & vocational skills. International Review of Psychiatry, 10,
Kavanagh, D. J. (2007). A review of consumer 35–41.
involvement in evaluations of case management: Rodgers, M. L., Norell, D. M., Roll, J. M., & Dyck, D. G.
Consistency with a recovery paradigm. Psychiatric (2007). An overview of mental health recovery. Primary
Services, 58, 396–401. Psychiatry, 14, 76–85.
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes Roe, D. (2001). Progressing from patienthood to
in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. personhood across the multidimensional outcomes in
McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S. D., Emmons, R. A., & schizophrenia and related disorders. Journal of Nervous
Larson, D. B. (2001). Is gratitude a moral afect? and Mental Disease, 189, 691–699.
Psychological Bulletin, 127, 249–266. Roe, D. & Chopra, M. (2003). Beyond coping with mental
Mead, S. & Copeland, M. E. (2000). What recovery means to illness: Toward personal growth. American Journal of
us: Consumers’ perspectives. Community Mental Health Orthopsychiatry, 73, 334–344.
Journal, 36, 315–328. Roe, D., Yanos, P. T., & Lysaker, P. H. (2006). Coping with
Miller, J. S. (1990). Mental illness and spiritual crisis: psychosis: An integrative developmental framework.
Implications for psychiatric rehabilitation. Psychosocial Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194, 917–924.
Rehabilitation Journal, 14, 29–45. Salokangas, R. K. R., Honkonen, T., Stengard, E., &
Morrison, A. P., Renton, J. C., Dunn, H., Williams, S., & Koivisto, A.-M. (2006). Subjective life satisfaction and
Bentall, R. P. (2004). Cognitive therapy for psychosis: living situations of persons in Finland with long-term
A formulation-based approach. New York: Brunner- schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services, 57, 373–381.
Routledge. Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the
Mueser, K. T. & Bellack, A. S. (1998). Social skills and new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting
social functioning. In K. T. Mueser & N. Tarrier (eds.), fulillment. New York: Free Press.
Handbook of social functioning in schizophrenia (pp. Slade, M. (2009). Personal recovery and mental illness: A
79–96). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. guide for mental health professionals. Cambridge, UK:
Mueser, K. T., Corrigan, P. W., Hilton, D., et al. (2002). Cambridge University Press.
Illness management and recovery for severe mental Tai, S. & Turkington, D. (2009). he evolution of cognitive
illness: A review of the research. Psychiatric Services, 53, behavior therapy for schizophrenia: Current practice
1272–1284. and recent developments. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35,
Neenan, M. (2009). Developing resilience: A cognitive- 865–873.
behavioural approach. New York: Routledge/Taylor & Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, J. E., &
Francis. Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psychological resources,
Nuechterlein, K. H. & Dawson, M. E. (1984). A heuristic positive illusions, and health. American Psychologist, 55,
vulnerability/stress model of schizophrenic episodes. 99–109.
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 10, 300–312. Tedeschi, R. G. & Kilmer, R. P. (2005). Assessing strengths,
Perry, B. M., Taylor, D., & Shaw, S. K. (2007). “You’ve got resilience, and growth to guide clinical interventions.
to have a positive state of mind”: An interpretative Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 36,
phenomenological analysis of hope and irst episode 230–237.
psychosis. Journal of Mental Health, 16, 781–793. Tsoi, D. T. Y., Lee, K. H., Gee, K. A., et al. (2008). Humour
Ralph, R. O. (2000). Review of recovery literature: A synthesis experience in schizophrenia: Relationship with executive
of a sample of recovery literature 2000. Portland, ME: dysfunction and psychosocial impairment. Psychological
Edmund S. Muskie Institute of Public Afairs, University Medicine, 38, 801–810.
of Southern Maine. Turkington, D., Dudley, R., Warman, D. M., & Beck, A. T.
Ralph, R. O. & Corrigan, P. W. (eds.) (2005). Recovery (2006). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for schizophrenia:
in mental illness: Broadening our understanding of A review. Focus, 4, 223–233.
wellness. Washington, DC: American Psychological Tusaie, K. & Dyer, J. (2004). Resilience: a historical review of
Association. the construct. Holistic Nursing Practice, 18, 3–10.

287
Section 4: Specific challenges

Vaillant, G. E. (1977). Adaptation to life. Boston: Little, empowerment. American Journal of Community
Brown. Psychology, 16, 725–750.
Wennberg, J. E. (1988). Improving the medical decision- Zubin, J. & Spring, B. (1977). Vulnerability: A new view
making process. Health Afairs, 7, 99–105. of schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86,
Zimmerman, M. A. & Rappaport, J. (1988). Citizen 103–126.
participation, perceived control, and psychological

288
Section
Section5 Training for resilience
Chapter
Interventions to enhance resilience and

20 resilience-related constructs in adults


Steven M. Southwick, Robert H. Pietrzak, and Gerald White

Introduction trauma exposure (Bonanno et al., 2006). In general,


resilience may be understood as “the process of adapt-
Most people are exposed to at least one signiicant ing well in the face of adversity, trauma, tragedy,
trauma during their lifetime. Individuals employed in threats, or even signiicant sources of stress. Further,
high-risk occupations, such as soldiers, policemen, and it may be understood as the ability to ‘bounce back’
iremen, are typically exposed to many traumas. While from diicult experiences” (American Psychological
numerous training programs have been developed with Association, 2010, p. 2).
the goal of increasing resilience and ability to cope with Resilience to stress has been associated with a host
traumatic stress, very few programs and interventions of neurobiological factors (Charney 2004, Southwick
have been subjected to rigorous scientiic evaluation. As 2005, Haglund et al., 2007). he neurobiology of resili-
a result, currently there exists little empirical evidence ence, which is highly complex, involves numerous
to support speciic approaches to enhancing resilience brain regions and circuits, as well as neurotransmit-
and preventing the development of psychopathology in ter and endocrine systems. Some of the more import-
the face of stress and trauma. Nevertheless, given recent ant factors and systems that have been studied to date
interest in the development and enhancement of resili- include the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, the
ence (Meichenbaum & Jaremko, 1983; Meichenbaum, sympathetic nervous system, the dopamine-mediated
1985; Maddi, 2002; Davidson et al., 2005; Defense reward system, and the amygdala, hippocampus, and
Centers of Excellence, 2009), we believe there is value in prefrontal cortex. hese factors and circuits have been
reviewing published research on interventions designed described in detail in an earlier chapter.
to enhance constructs that are related to resilience, such Resilience has also been associated with a number
as hardiness, coping self-eicacy, and stress inocula- of psychosocial factors that appear to have a protect-
tion, as well as factors that are associated with resilience, ive role in highly challenging and stressful situations.
such as optimism and social support. As noted in earl- Some of the best studied psychosocial factors include
ier chapters, there is no single deinition and no agreed realistic optimism and positive emotions, active prob-
upon criteria for resilience. his makes it challenging to lem-focused coping, high levels of positive social sup-
evaluate existing treatments and interventions designed port, altruism, religious/spiritual practice, attention to
to enhance resilience. physical health and exercise, cognitive lexibility (e.g.,
he Preface gives a variety of deinitions of resili- cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, positive explanatory
ence. Resilience has been deined in terms of protective style), disciplined focus on skill development, commit-
factors associated with growing up in disadvantaged ment to a valued cause or purpose, and the capacity to
environments (Garmezy, 1974; Garmezy & Streitman, extract meaning from adverse situations and from life
1974), successful adaptation to adversity (Luthar et al., in general (Southwick, 2005).
2000), a “class of phenomena characterized by good Interventions designed to enhance resilience, or
outcomes in spite of serious threats to adaptation or constructs associated with resilience, can be directed
development” (Masten, 2001, p. 228), enhanced psy- at the individual, the family, the organization, or the
chobiological regulation of stress hormones (Charney, community. Further, interventions can be delivered
2004), and symptom-free functioning following at various points in time prior to (i.e., preparation/

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

289
Section 5: Training for resilience

prevention), during or ater stress/trauma exposure. to be used as an outcome measure (Connor &
Training administered prior to stress/trauma expos- Davidson, 2003). For example, in a randomized
ure is generally designed to enhance performance trial of patients with post-traumatic stress
during stressful life events or traumas, with the intent disorder (PTSD), treatment with venlafaxine was
to lessen or even prevent the development of stress- associated with increased scores on dimensions
related morbidities. Such training typically focuses of resilience relecting hardiness, persistence/
on increasing familiarity with stressful scenarios that tenacity, social support, and faith (Davidson et al.,
one might encounter, as well as with potential emo- 2008). Abbreviated 10- (Campbell-Sills & Stein,
tional and physiological reactions to these stressors; 2007) and 2-item (Vaishnavi et al., 2007) versions
increasing perceptions of predictability and control- of the CD-RISC are also available and may be
lability of both external and internal trauma-related easily incorporated into treatment studies.
stimuli; developing skills relevant to the speciic stres- he Response to Stressful Experience Scale (RSES).
sor/trauma (e.g., intensive combat training), as well as his 22-item self-report measure was developed
more generic stress management skills (e.g., diaphrag- by the National Center for PTSD and assesses a
matic breathing, muscular-relaxation training); and broad range of behaviors, thoughts, and actions
exposing individuals to intensive, realistic training that individuals characteristically employ when
(e.g., scenario-based training), which may be delivered responding to stress, adversity, or trauma (Johnson
in graduated doses of intensity (Whealin et al., 2008). et al., 2011). he measure consists of a total score
and six subscales: active coping, meaning-making,
cognitive lexibility, spirituality, self-eicacy, and
Measurement of resilience restoration. To date, the RSES has been used to
Several measures of resilience and constructs related characterize behaviors, thoughts, and actions that
to resilience (e.g., hardiness, self-eicacy, meaning- may confer protection against traumatic stress and
making) have been developed and are described below. related psychopathology in military populations.
he Dispositional Resilience Scale-15 (DRS-15). his For example, higher scores on the RSES were
is a brief, valid, and reliable 15-item self-report found to be negatively associated with PTSD
instrument that provides a measure of hardiness symptom severity in military personnel on active
(Bartone, 1995, 2007). It assesses three dimensions duty, as well as in treatment-seeking veterans
of resilience related to hardiness: commitment (Johnson et al., 2011).
(i.e., tendency to engage fully in life activities), he Resilience Scales for Children and Adolescents
perceived control (i.e., perceived ability to exercise (RSCA). his 64-item self-report measure
control over life circumstances), and challenge assesses resilience-related attributes of children
(i.e., tendency to enjoy challenges). his measure, and adolescents, including sense of mastery,
which is derived from longer 30- and 45-item sense of relatedness, and emotional reactivity
versions of the Dispositional Resilience Scale (Prince-Embury, 2008). he RSCA consists
(DRS; Bartone et al., 1989), has been applied in of three global scales: (1) sense of mastery
military and occupational settings. Scores on (optimism, self-eicacy, adaptability); (2) sense of
this measure have been shown to be negatively relatedness (trust, support, comfort, tolerance);
associated with perceived occupational stress and (3) emotional reactivity (sensitivity, recovery,
and psychological dysfunction (Bartone et al., impairment). Each global scale comprises
1989; Steinhardt et al., 2003; Bartone, 2006, 2007; 20–24 questions, and there are 10 subscales. he
McCalister et al., 2006). RSCA has been used to screen for psychological
he Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC). vulnerability in children and adolescents. Higher
his 25-item self-report measure assesses a scores on the emotional reactivity scale have been
broad range of resilient characteristics, including found to be associated with greater symptoms
hardiness, personal competence, tolerance of of depression, anxiety, anger, and disruptive
negative afect, acceptance of change, personal behavior, while higher scores on the sense of
control, and spirituality (Connor & Davidson, mastery and sense of relatedness scales were
2003). he CD-RISC is a psychometrically negatively associated with these variables (Prince-
sound measure of resilience that was designed Embury, 2008).

290
Chapter 20: Interventions to enhance adult resilience

Measures of resilience may have diferent applications 1982) and the scale has undergone a number of revi-
in studies of resilience-enhancing interventions. For sions designed to shorten the original measure (e.g.,
example, the CD-RISC may be most useful as an out- the DRS) (Bartone et al., 1989; Bartone, 2006).
come measure in interventions purporting to enhance Summarizing several decades of research, Maddi
resilience (e.g., Vaishnavi et al., 2007; Davidson et al., (2007, p. 61) described hardiness as “a pattern of atti-
2008; Steinhardt & Dolbier, 2008), while the DRS-15, tudes and skills that provides the courage and strat-
RSES, and RSCA may be most useful in assessing egies to turn stressful circumstances from potential
characteristics of resilience (e.g., hardiness, self- disasters into growth opportunities instead.” He also
eicacy, sense of mastery) that may bufer or protect wrote that hardiness requires the “courage and motiv-
against traumatic stress and related psychopathology ation to face stressors accurately rather than to deny
(Steinhardt et al., 2003; McCalister et al., 2006; Prince- or catastrophize” (Maddi, 2005, p. 261). Such courage
Embury, 2008; Johnson et al., 2011). Given that and motivation may well lead to active coping, prob-
research on the measurement of resilience is in its early lem solving, and the giving and receiving of social sup-
stages, additional studies are needed to validate these port. In a study of a nationally representative sample
instruments in a broader range of stress- and trauma- of 1632 Vietnam veterans, King and colleagues found
exposed populations; to evaluate their similarities and that higher level of hardiness in veterans was negatively
diferences in assessing aspects of resilience; to deter- related to PTSD symptoms and that hardiness was
mine their relative sensitivity in predicting response associated with increased resistance to PTSD through
to resilience-enhancing interventions; and to evaluate its association with functional social support (King
their role in mediating treatment-related change, as et al., 1998). Studies in a variety of populations have
well as in assessing the efects of these interventions. shown that hardiness is positively associated with good
mental and physical health (Westphal et al., 2008), that
hardiness is a signiicant modiier or bufer of combat
Interventions designed to enhance stress (Bartone, 1993; Westphal et al., 2008) and occu-
resilience pational stress (Kobasa & Puccetti, 1983; Kobasa et al.,
his section describes three interventions designed 1985; Contrada, 1989), and that hardy individuals tend
speciically to enhance resilience: hardiness training, to appraise or evaluate potentially negative events as
stress inoculation training (SIT), and psychoeduca- less threatening (Kobasa et al., 1981, 1982) and remain
tional resilience enhancement training. more optimistic about their ability to cope with the
situation (Allred & Smith, 1989) compared with less
hardy individuals.
Hardiness training Khoshaba and Maddi (2001) have developed a train-
Psychological hardiness is a construct that was devel- ing program to increase hardiness. he program takes
oped to describe an inner resource (Florian et al., place in small groups led by a trainer, involves weekly
1995) or constellation of personality characteristics sessions, and addresses the core elements of the hardi-
(Westphal et al., 2008) associated with good health ness construct. Trainees are taught techniques to handle
and optimal performance under conditions of high stress and practice exercises designed to enhance atti-
stress (Eid & Morgan, 2006). As deined by Kobasa tudes of commitment, control, and challenge (Maddi &
et al. (1982), hardiness is composed of three primary, Khoshaba, 2005; Maddi, 2008). In addition, they learn
interrelated components: (1) control, or the belief that problem-solving skills. In what is termed transform-
one can inluence events in life; (2) commitment, or ational coping, trainees learn to broaden their perspec-
the ability to feel deeply involved in one’s existence, tive and deepen their understanding of the stressful
relationships, activities, and self; (3) challenge, or the experience they face, and then develop and carry out a
tendency to view adverse events and change as chal- decisive course of action. Finally, trainees learn how to
lenges rather than threats (Maddi & Khoshaba, 2005). build two-way social support relationships where they
Hardiness is believed to develop early in life and remain give and receive encouragement and assistance from
relatively stable over time, although changes in hardi- others (Maddi & Khoshaba, 2005).
ness are possible (Kobasa et al., 1982). As noted above, he efects of hardiness training have been exam-
Kobasa and Maddi constructed the Hardiness Scale ined in working adults and college students. In 1981,
to measure this attribute (Kobasa, 1979; Kobasa et al., the US Federal Court ordered the deregulation of the

291
Section 5: Training for resilience

American Telephone and Telegraph (AT&T) monop- between the individual and the environment. For any
oly. Maddi and colleagues (2006) studied the efects given life event, each person will interpret or appraise
of this deregulation, as well as the efects of hardiness that event in his or her own unique manner depend-
training at Illinois Bell Telephone (IBT) (a subsidy of ing on a complex array of biological, developmental,
AT&T) from 1975 to 1987. Massive layofs and a dis- psychological, social, and spiritual factors. he indi-
mantling of long-standing norms and policies resulted vidual experiences the event as stressful when he or
in high levels of stress among most employees. his she believes that the skills and resources needed to
longitudinal study of over 450 male and female IBT deal with the event exceed his or her ability. hus, in
employees examined the diferences between the one the transactional model, appraisal of environmental
third who thrived (i.e., one third of the employees took demands and of one’s capacity to handle and cope with
the changes as opportunities to grow and they rose to those demands determines how an individual responds
the top of the company, with some even starting com- to stress.
panies of their own) compared with the remaining two he SIT approach was originally developed as a
thirds who demonstrated overt signs of stress during treatment for anxiety by Meichenbaum and colleagues
the transition (e.g., physical and mental health prob- (Meichenbaum, 1975, 1985; Meichenbaum & Jaremko,
lems, marital problems). he thriving group demon- 1983). It was later adapted by Kilpatrick and colleagues
strated the “three Cs” of commitment, control, and (1982) to treat trauma survivors. he program typic-
challenge to a signiicantly greater degree than the ally consists of 8 to 22 weekly sessions and is composed
other workers. of what Meichenbaum describes as three overlap-
Based on indings from this initial study of IBT ping phases (Meichenbaum & Defenbacher, 1988).
employees (Maddi & Kobasa, 1984; Maddi & Khoshaba, Beginning with the conceptual and educational phase,
2005), Maddi and colleagues (2006) developed and a collaborative therapeutic relationship is established
applied a hardiness training method (HardiTraining) to and the neurobiological and psychological impact of
enhance hardiness in this population of IBT employees. trauma and stress-related symptoms is discussed and
Compared with employees who did not receive hardi- normalized. Individuals are taught to self-monitor by
ness training, those who completed training experi- learning to recognize emotional, behavioral, phys-
enced greater reductions in stress, depression, anxiety, ical, and cognitive responses to trauma-related cues
and blood pressure, and had better job satisfaction and or triggers. hen, signs of strength and resilience
job performance (Maddi & Khoshaba, 2005). Of the are identiied to help to facilitate changes in how
workers who completed the training, 93% reported an stressors are perceived (i.e., challenging rather than
improvement in their ability to cope with stressful situ- overwhelming).
ations, with these group diferences persisting at a six- During the second phase (the skills acquisition and
month follow-up. rehearsal phase), individuals are taught new coping
Hardiness training has also been applied in college skills and are helped to consolidate skills they already
students where it has been associated with increases in possess. Examples of coping skills include relaxation
grade point averages and retention in school (Maddi training, thought stopping, guided self-dialogue, iden-
et al., 2002; Maddi & Khoshaba, 2005). Hardiness train- tiication and replacement of irrational and maladap-
ing has been recommended as a potential method to tive thinking, and problem solving.
enhance performance, leadership, and health in mili- In the third phase of SIT, the individual is encour-
tary personnel, and to help with recovery from stress- aged to practice the skills acquired in the irst two
related diiculties (Maddi, 2007). To date, however, phases through role playing, modeling, imagery
we are unaware of any studies that have examined the rehearsal, and behavioral rehearsal in situations that
eicacy of hardiness training in this population. are stressful and anxiety provoking. hese stressful
situations are graduated (i.e., graded in vivo expos-
ure) so that they become increasingly challenging (i.e.,
Stress inoculation training creating an inoculating efect). Once one situation is
Stress inoculation training is based on a transactional mastered, the individual moves on to the next most
model of stress and coping that was irst described by challenging one. hroughout this phase, emphasis is
Lazarus and Folkman (1984). In this model, stress is placed on accurately discriminating dangerous from
viewed as a bidirectional and dynamic transaction safe situations, managing fear, problem solving, and

292
Chapter 20: Interventions to enhance adult resilience

staying actively engaged in the situation through the Interventions designed to enhance
use of relaxation, cognitive restructuring, guided self-
dialogue, and other coping strategies. here is also a constructs related to resilience
focus on extending into the future what was learned his section describes three interventions designed
during the training program through booster sessions speciically to enhance constructs related to resilience:
and follow-up. social support interventions, learned optimism train-
he SIT approach has been studied primarily in ing, and well-being therapy. A number of other con-
female victims of sexual assault (Foa et al., 1991, 1999). structs, such as altruism and positive religious coping,
Results of these studies suggest that SIT is associated have also been associated with resilience, but will not
with reductions in PTSD and depressive symptoms be reviewed in this chapter.
that are largely maintained at follow-up one year later.
Magnitudes of these reductions are comparable to
those associated with prolonged exposure and with the Social support interventions
combination of prolonged exposure and SIT (Foa et al., A large body of literature has examined the relation-
1999). ship between social support, resilience, and physical
and mental health. Higher perceived social support,
which has been conceptualized as an individual’s per-
Psychoeducational resilience enhancement ception or experience of helpful social interactions, is
College is oten seen as a period of rapid growth but also associated with better physical functioning (Moak &
of psychological vulnerability. Students are faced with Agrawal, 2010), as well as decreased symptoms of
many new challenges at a time when they have not fully PTSD (King et al., 1998; Brewin et al., 2000; Ozer et al.,
developed their coping abilities. To help to address this 2008) and depression (Charuvastra & Cloitre, 2008).
period of vulnerability, Steinhardt and Dolbier (2008) For example, in a nationally representative sample of
designed a four-week psychoeducational interven- 34 653 US adults, Moak and Agrawal (2010) found that
tion to enhance resilience to stress as well as decrease high perceived social support was negatively associ-
psychological and psychosomatic symptoms by redu- ated with depression, generalized anxiety disorder,
cing maladaptive coping mechanisms, fostering adap- and social phobia, as well as with physical health prob-
tive coping skills, and enhancing factors that protect lems. Higher perceived social support also bufered
against the deleterious efects of stress (e.g., positive the relationship between traumatic life events and
afect, self-esteem). his study was conducted during psychopathology.
a period of increased academic stress (i.e., the inal he importance of social support has also been
weeks of classes). Students were randomized into the examined in military samples. For example, as
experimental group (30) or the waitlist control group described above, King and colleagues (1998), in a
(27). he intervention consisted of four sessions of two nationally representative sample of 1632 Vietnam vet-
hours each that were psychoeducational in nature: (1) erans, found that hardiness and post-war social support
transforming stress into resilience, (2) taking respon- were negatively associated with PTSD symptoms, and
sibility, (3) focusing on empowering interpretations, that functional social support (i.e., emotional susten-
and (4) creating meaningful connections. Ater the ance and instrumental assistance) accounted for a sub-
intervention, the experimental group had signiicantly stantial amount of the indirect efect of hardiness on
higher resilience scores (CD-RISC), lower negative PTSD. More recently, a study of 272 Iraq/Afghanistan
psychological symptom scores, higher scores on efect- veterans found that greater perceptions of unit sup-
ive coping strategies, and higher scores on measures of port were associated with greater psychological resili-
positive afect, self-esteem, and self-leadership com- ence (CD-RISC scores), and that both psychological
pared with the waitlist control group. Results of this resilience and post-deployment social support were
study suggest that this intervention may be useful in inversely associated with PTSD symptoms, even ater
managing stress and enhancing resilience. Additional adjusting for combat exposure (Pietrzak et al., 2010).
research is needed to examine the durability of these To address the inding that low perceived social
changes over time, as well as whether the changes asso- support in patients with heart disease may be related
ciated with this intervention generalize to other life to cardiac morbidity and mortality, investigators in
stressors. the Enhancing Recovery in Coronary Artery Heart

293
Section 5: Training for resilience

Disease (ENRICHD) research group developed a enhancing resilience to stress, numerous studies have
treatment designed to enhance social support in examined the impact of social support interventions
2481 patients who had recently experienced an acute on a variety of physical and mental health outcomes.
myocardial infarction (Writing Committee for the In a comprehensive review of the literature, Hogan
ENRICHD Investigators, 2003). A psychosocial inter- and colleagues (2002) reviewed 100 studies that were
vention based on cognitive-behavioral therapy was designed to test the efect of interventions to enhance
used to address cognitions, emotions, and behaviors social support on a broad range of physical and men-
associated with low perceived social support. he inter- tal health outcomes. Social support interventions had
vention included counseling to address behavioral been delivered in group, individual, and combined for-
and social skill deicits, as well as social outreach and mats, with outcomes comparable across these modal-
social network development. he primary focus was ities. Individuals who participated in these intervention
to strengthen and improve existing social networks studies were drawn from several populations, including
as well as to create new relationships. At a six-month individuals with cancer, substance abusers, as well as
follow-up assessment, the intervention group reported those preparing for surgery. he authors of this review
signiicantly greater improvement in depression and found that 83% of studies reported at least some bene-
social isolation, but no diferences in cardiac event- its of social support relative to no treatment or another
free survival were observed. Results of this study active treatment, beneits including reductions in psy-
suggest that a psychological intervention based on chological distress, depression, and substance abuse.
cognitive-behavioral methods may be useful in redu- A number of research and treatment programs for
cing depressive symptoms and enhancing perceptions individuals with alcohol dependence include strat-
of social support in individuals who recently sufered egies designed to change the patient’s social network
an acute myocardial infarction, but that this inter- from one that reinforces drinking to one that supports
vention may not afect rates of recurrent infarction or sobriety. Examples include Alcoholics Anonymous
death. Additional research has also been done on the role (AA), the community reinforcement approach, and
of social support in myocardial infarction (Lett et al., the UK Alcohol Treatment Trial Social Behavior and
2007, 2009). Network herapy Study (Litt et al., 2007). Studies by
In addition to the robust positive relationship Bond and colleagues (Kaskutas et al., 2002, 2009; Bond
between health and the receiving of social support, et al., 2003) have found that beneits from AA appear
there is emerging evidence for a positive association to be mediated, at least in part, by changes in social
between health and the giving of social support. In a network. Similarly, Litt and colleagues (2007) devel-
ive-year, prospective study of 846 older married adults oped a manualized treatment for the Network Support
who had experienced a recent myocardial infarction, Project that consists of once weekly outpatient sessions
Brown et al. (2003) compared the efects on longevity over 12 weeks. he program focused on attending AA
of receiving versus providing support. Results indi- meetings as a way to avoid drinking and to make new
cated reduced risk of mortality among individuals who acquaintances, assertiveness training, engaging in en-
reported providing support to others. his was true joyable activities other than drinking, re-establishing
whether individuals provided emotional support to a contact with relatives and friends who do not drink,
spouse or instrumental support (e.g., housework, child and exploring ways to change one’s social network.
care, etc.) to neighbors, friends, and relatives. When Peer-support and self-help interventions, particularly
providing support was taken into consideration, there for reducing speciic symptoms, have been found to be
was no efect of receiving support on mortality. Taken efective in enhancing well-being, improving coping
together, these results suggest that providing social sup- skills, and expanding social networks.
port may have a positive efect on longevity and that this Litt and colleagues (2007) tested the network sup-
efect may be independent of received social support. port intervention in 210 alcohol-dependent individ-
Interventions designed to increase social support uals, who were randomized to one of three outpatient
have been shown to enhance physical and mental health treatments: network support, network support plus
and may be useful in promoting resilience to stress contingency management, and case management.
(King et al., 1998; Kaniasty & Norris, 2008). While we Over two years of follow-up, individuals who received
are aware of no published studies that have speciically network support treatment reported no change in
examined the efect of social support interventions on number of drinking friends but an increase in the

294
Chapter 20: Interventions to enhance adult resilience

number of abstinent people in their social network; by recording whether and to what degree each of these
they also experienced a better drinking outcome com- relationships involves conlict. Conlicts are subse-
pared with a control group. Changes in social network quently solved through assistance and encourage-
were associated with increases in self-eicacy and cop- ment while carrying out an action plan that includes
ing, which, in turn, predicted better long-term drink- responding to feedback.
ing outcomes. It is important to note that not all studies have
Many studies evaluating the association between reported that social support interventions yield bene-
social support and physical and mental well-being icial mental and physical health outcomes. In some
have measured perceived social support, or an indi- cases, relying on others for support may increase
vidual’s appraisal of being reliably connected to others, dependence, feelings of guilt and anxiety, and the feel-
as opposed to actual enacted social support (Barrera, ing that one is a burden to others (Hogan et al., 2002,
1986). he mechanism by which increased perceived Litt et al., 2007). It is also not clear which elements of
social support might increase physical and psycho- social support are most important for enhancing health
logical well-being as well as resilience is not well under- and functioning. While most studies have focused on
stood. Brand and colleagues (1995) began to address receiving support, very few have evaluated the efects of
these questions using a 13-week preventive psycho- providing social support on health outcomes. Given the
educational intervention they developed to enhance great variability in subject populations, methodology,
perceived social support in 51 community residents interventions, assessments, and outcome measures
with low perceived social support. he intervention, employed in studies of social support interventions, it
which involved training in social skills and cognitive is diicult to draw speciic conclusions from an ana-
reframing, focused on recognizing one’s positive qual- lysis of these studies (Hogan et al., 2002). Nevertheless,
ities, correcting cognitive distortions about oneself, accumulating evidence from both epidemiological and
reconceptualizing relationships with family members, treatment research suggests that interventions designed
addressing negative cognitive biases when interpret- to enhance social support may be helpful in enhancing
ing supportive behaviors, and improving social com- resilience to stress as well as factors associated with
petence. Compared with a waitlist control group, the resilience, such as coping self-eicacy.
active treatment group reported increased perceived
social support from family, but not friends. Results fur-
ther indicated that increases in perceived social sup- Learned optimism training
port may be mediated by changes in self-esteem and he teaching of optimistic thinking has been operation-
frequency of self-reinforcement. alized in a 12-week program called Learned Optimism
he giving and receiving of social support consti- (Seligman, 1991). his program is based largely on
tutes an important aspect of Maddi’s hardiness train- Beck and colleagues’ cognitive models of treatment for
ing, described above (Maddi & Khoshaba, 2005). depression (Beck et al., 1979). In Learned Optimism,
Building two-way social support involves the exchange an individual learns to recognize the connections
of assistance and encouragement, as well as minimiz- between adversity, beliefs, and consequences in every-
ing destructive competition and overprotection. In the day life. He or she begins by keeping a diary to record
hardiness model, critical elements of social support examples of daily adversities, even minor ones such
include empathy, sympathy, and showing appreci- as disagreeing with a co-worker. he writing exercise
ation for the other person. Communicating faith in the begins by describing what happened as objectively as
other person’s ability to handle problems and helping possible without evaluating the event. Next, thoughts
with responsibilities when the other person is over- and beliefs about the adverse event are recorded.
whelmed, yet providing some breathing room for him/ Finally, the individual describes consequences of the
her to deal with problems, is an important aspect of this event (i.e., feelings and/or actions) and his or her sub-
model. Resources such as expertise and contacts are sequent beliefs about it.
also ofered when needed. Concrete steps to increase In an intervention study designed to prevent anx-
social support include creating a social interaction iety and depression, Seligman and colleagues recruited
map, which involves recording the names, frequency 231 college students who were assessed to be at risk
of contact, degree of interaction and intimacy for all based on their scores in the bottom quartile (i.e., the
of the important people in one’s life. his is followed most pessimistic explanatory style) of the Attributional

295
Section 5: Training for resilience

Style Questionnaire (Seligman et al., 1999). Half of the of mood and anxiety disorders, its primary goal is to
students were randomized into a control group, which engender positive outcomes (i.e., well-being, resili-
completed an initial assessment but did not participate ence) rather than to alleviate psychological distress.
in the prevention workshop, while the other half com- Well-being therapy is based on Ryf ’s multidimen-
pleted the same assessment and then participated in sional model where well-being is viewed as more than
the eight-week prevention workshop. Workshop train- the absence of psychological distress (Ryf, 1989). In
ers all had experience working at the Beck Center for this model, well-being incorporates six dimensions:
Cognitive herapy. he workshop covered a number autonomy, personal growth, environmental mastery,
of cognitive-behavioral topics, including the cognitive purpose in life, positive relations, and self-acceptance
theory of change, identiication of automatic negative (Ryf, 1989; Fava, 1999; Fava & Ruini, 2003).
thoughts and beliefs, empirical hypothesis testing to Well-being therapy has been described as struc-
question and dispute automatic negative thoughts and tured, directive, and problem oriented (Fava, 1999;
irrational beliefs, and generation of more constructive Fava & Ruini, 2003). It is generally conducted over
interpretations. he workshop also covered behavioral eight sessions of 30–50 minutes that are spaced one to
activation techniques such as creative problem solving two weeks apart. he initial sessions focus on learning
and assertiveness training, training in interpersonal to recognize personal episodes of well-being, identi-
skills, relaxation training, and the application of these fying the context in which these episodes occur, and
coping skills to a variety of situations. Participants were rating the intensity of each episode, even if the episode
followed for three years. During the study period, par- is very brief in duration. Patients record their observa-
ticipants who completed the workshop reported fewer tions in a structured diary.
symptoms of anxiety and depression than participants In the intermediate phase of treatment, the indi-
in the control group, although there was no diference vidual learns to recognize beliefs and thoughts that
in frequency of major depressive episodes. he work- trigger premature interruption of the episode of well-
shop group also experienced signiicant improvements being. For example, an individual may observe that
in measures of hopelessness, dysfunctional attitudes, he is feeling positive about a comment from a friend,
and cognitive explanatory style. At 6–30 months ater but also notices that the positive feeling is short lived
entry into the study, participants in the workshop because he imagines that the friend feels sorry for him
group reported fewer illness-related visits to student and that the compliment was not genuine but instead
health centers, fewer doctor visits, and fewer symp- an attempt to make him feel better. As in traditional
toms of physical illness (Seligman et al., 1999). cognitive-behavioral therapies, the individual learns
he Penn Resiliency Program has tested a similar to challenge irrational beliefs and automatic thoughts,
intervention to prevent depression in a large group but in this case the irrational beliefs are related to epi-
of US middle school children (Cutuli et al., 2006). sodes of well-being rather than to episodes of distress.
he intervention was based on a failure model where he therapist also assigns the individual to engage in
repetitive failures in important domains of life are pleasurable activities and tasks and to self-monitor
seen as contributing to later symptoms of depression. these experiences.
Over the course of 12 sessions, children learned cog- In the inal phase, the therapist teaches the indi-
nitive-behavioral techniques to challenge inaccurate vidual about the six dimensions in Ryf ’s multidimen-
negative self-perceptions and learned techniques to sional model and provides guidelines for enhancing
regulate emotions and enhance social skills. While the these dimensions (Ryf, 1989; Fava, 1999; Fava &
intervention was described as generally beneicial to Ruini, 2003). Individuals learn to take greater control
all participants, it was most efective for young adoles- of their environment (environmental control); recog-
cents with elevated levels of conduct problems. nize and acknowledge self-improvement (personal
growth); search for a sense of meaning and develop
life goals (purpose in life); become more assertive
Well-being therapy and resistant to social pressures (autonomy); recog-
Well-being therapy is a short-term structured psycho- nize unrealistically high self-standards and develop a
therapeutic intervention designed to enhance well- more accepting and positive attitude toward self (self-
being. While it shares many therapeutic features found acceptance); and to value and strive for closer, warmer,
in cognitive-behavioral therapies for the treatment and more trusting relationships (positive relations

296
Chapter 20: Interventions to enhance adult resilience

with others). hroughout this phase of treatment, cor- Preparatory interventions


rection of irrational and automatic thoughts as well as
Many organizations whose personnel routinely face
errors in thinking and alternative interpretations are
potential traumatic stressors have developed prepara-
emphasized.
tory training programs to provide information and
he formal study of well-being therapy is in the
teach skills to facilitate optimal performance under
preliminary stage. So far, well-being therapy has
conditions of high stress. Examples of these programs
been used in a number of small studies for treating
the residual phase of afective disorders (Fava et al., include military basic and advanced training, police
1998a), the prevention of recurrent depression (Fava and ireighter training academies, and irst-responder
et al., 1998b) and residual symptoms in remitted training. While these programs are widely employed
patients with panic disorder with agoraphobia (Fava and believed to be useful, there is little scientiic evi-
et al., 2001). For example, in a small controlled study dence supporting their efectiveness (Whealin et al.,
of patients who had been treated successfully for 2008).
afective disorders but who continued to experience Preparatory interventions are designed to provide
residual symptoms, patients were randomized either information and to familiarize and expose personnel
to cognitive-behavioral treatment or well-being to stressful situations that they are likely to encounter
therapy (Fava et al., 1998a). While both treatments in their occupations. hese programs are thought to
resulted in a signiicant reduction in depression and a increase familiarity; increase controllability and pre-
signiicant improvement in well-being, the improve- dictability of internal emotions and physical sensations,
ments in the well-being therapy group were signii- and promote the development of greater perceptions
cantly greater, particularly in the area of personal of control and self-eicacy (Salas & Cannon-Bowers,
growth. Well-being therapy has also been found to 2001; Salas et al., 2006; Whealin et al., 2008).
increase psychological well-being, decrease distress, One of the most common preparatory interven-
and improve long-term outcomes in individuals with tions is scenario-based training. Scenari-based train-
generalized anxiety disorder (Fava et al., 2005) and ing involves learning through doing, where trainees
PTSD (Belaise et al., 2005). apply what they have learned in the classroom to
Well-being therapy has also been tested in chil- realistic and challenging situations (Salas & Cannon-
dren and adolescents. In a sample of 111 junior high Bowers, 2001; Salas et al., 2006; Whealin et al., 2008).
school students, Ruini and colleagues (2006) imple- It is theory based and requires careful scripting of sce-
mented a four-session version of well-being therapy narios so that trainees must have mastered the infor-
that included psychoeducation, cognitive-behavioral mation and skills they have been taught if they are to
techniques, and Ryf ’s model of psychological well- succeed.
being. Participation in the program was associated Scenario-based training has been shown to be an
with increased well-being and decreased anxiety efective form of learning that can be adapted to a wide
among the students. In a subsequent intervention variety of settings and learning tasks. hese include
adapted for adolescents, 227 high school students were practice of learned skills, assessment of accurate skill
randomized to a well-being intervention or an atten- acquisition, and real-time constructive feedback. Salas
tion-placebo protocol (Ruini et al., 2009). Both inter- and colleagues (Salas & Cannon-Bowers, 2001; Salas
ventions involved six weekly sessions of two hours each et al., 2006), who have written extensively on scenario-
conducted in a group format. he well-being therapy based training, believe that it is ideal for training in
included cognitive-behavioral techniques and focused military tasks and for skill acquisition and perform-
on the six dimensions of Ryf ’s model. In the well-be- ance that involves complex team work. Scenario-based
ing therapy group, signiicant improvement (although training is commonly used with professionals such as
a modest efect size) was reported in personal growth police oicers and iremen who are involved in highly
and physical well-being, while signiicant reductions stressful work. Research has shown that scenario-based
were noted in somatization, anxiety, and physiological training can be used to enhance test light performance
anxiety. When taken together, these studies provide (Gopher et al., 1994), train pediatric critical care cli-
encouraging preliminary evidence that it may be pos- nicians (Nishisaki et al., 2009), and improve skills and
sible to enhance psychological well-being and possibly operating-room performance among surgeons (Gof,
resilience through school-based interventions. 2010).

297
Section 5: Training for resilience

Seven interrelated stages have been described in disabilities, including victims of particular weapons:
scenario-based training (Cannon-Bowers & Salas, the 1997 Landmine Ban Treaty, the 2008 Convention
1998). on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities, and the 2009
Step 1: collect past performance data and conduct a Cluster Munitions Convention. White and Rutherford
skills inventory to determine what skills trainees have worked in over 20 war-afected countries to train
have already acquired survivor leaders as co-advocates and peer role mod-
Step 2: determine the competencies and tasks that els. When the Landmine Survivors Network later grew
the scenario-based training will be designed to into the Survivors Corp, White and Rutherford stated,
enhance “We focus on the unique contributions and leader-
Step 3: develop training objectives based on ship of conlict survivors because we believe no one
competencies and job/task needs; objectives is better positioned to break cycles of violence and
can be based on either the need to accomplish a victimization than those who have survived war.” For
speciic task or the need to develop skills that are over a decade, this survivor network helped victims of
transferrable to a variety of settings war rebuild their lives and communities. he signa-
Step 4: embed scenarios, which should be carefully ture peer-support programs connected survivors with
crated with the goal of promoting training survivor role models to ofer the encouragement and
objectives and targeting speciic desired motivation critical to helping new survivors to ind
competencies hope, get jobs, and get on with their lives.
Step 5: develop performance measures that assess he challenge is to transform victims into sur-
performance and training efectiveness vivor leaders. hrough the support of those around
them (peers and family), these traumatized people
Step 6: provide constructive and timely feedback so
mentor, teach, coach, and support others on their
that the trainee can learn rapidly and correctly, as
path to survivorship. White deines “survivorship”
well as improve performance
simply as, “choosing to live positively and dynam-
Step 7: modify future training programs to eliminate
ically in the face of death, disaster and disability.
inefective components while retaining and possibly
Survivorship is a healthy and pragmatic approach to
improving efective components.
quality living even ater traumatic adversity or loss.”
Scenario-based training is most efective when partner- His philosophy is shared by many types of survi-
ships are formed between subject matter experts and vors, from genocide and torture to breast cancer and
experts in the science of learning, and when trainees bereavement, from war and disability to addiction
practice with close supervision, guidance, and imme- and human betrayal.
diate feedback. White himself survived a landmine explosion and
lost his right leg at the age of 20 while studying abroad
as an undergraduate in Israel. Ten years ater recovery,
Case example: landmine survivors White began his professional outreach to other war
his inal section of the chapter presents a more in- victims. He has since observed, in countries as diverse
depth description of a speciic program to foster as Bosnia–Herzegovina, El Salvador, Ethiopia, Jordan,
resilience. he program was developed by Survivor and Vietnam, that it is not uncommon for trauma sur-
Corps and was primarily targeting survivors of war vivors to get stuck for years in a victim mentality where
trauma. they tend to pity themselves, resent their circumstances,
In 1995, Jerry White and Ken Rutherford, who live in the past, and/or blame others. Even worse, they
both lost limbs to landmine explosions, founded might become the next victimizers: “his was done to
Landmine Survivors Network, the irst international me, now I will do it to the next person.” White believes
organization created by and for survivors to prevent that a victim-minded person is generally inlexible,
future mine injuries and ofer peer support to vic- stuck in his or her grievances, and seemingly unable to
tims of the weapon. White and Rutherford are recog- let go, ind hope, or move forward. Over time, a victim’s
nized leaders of the International Campaign to Ban intense focus on their own personal sufering can inter-
Landmines and co-laureates of the 1997 Nobel Prize fere with his or her ability to take positive action, relate
for Peace. hey helped to drat three major inter- to others in a healthy manner, or participate more fully
national treaties to address the rights of people with in daily life.

298
Chapter 20: Interventions to enhance adult resilience

Ater establishing more than a dozen peer-support painful facts, it is oten necessary to repeat this pro-
programs working with thousands of victims of land- cess of telling the traumatic story in the presence of an
mines and violence, White believes each survivor has active listener, perceived to be empathetic.
something to teach about resilience. Drawing from his was the case for Zainab Salbi, who grew up in
personal experience and lessons learned in the ield, he Iraq as the daughter of Saddam Hussein’s pilot (Salbi &
recommends ive “steps” to help trauma survivors to Buckland, 2005). Her family lived close to the inner
tap their innate resilience and grow stronger. circle of power in the shadows of dictatorship. Being
1. Face facts: accept what has happened, the sufering close to Saddam was oten harrowing and horrifying,
and loss and Zainab spent years suppressing the facts of her life.
2. Choose life: live for the future, not in the past Since childhood, Zainab’s mother had always told her
3. Reach out: connect to others who have “been to “erase from your memory” anything too scary, like
there” the kidnapping and killing of friends who were also in
4. Get moving: set goals and take action for a healthy Saddam’s employ. Zainab was a grown woman in her
recovery thirties before she started to face these facts, a little
at a time. And it was only ater Saddam Hussein was
5. Give back: be thankful for what you do have,
captured in 2003, that the “dam inally broke.” Zainab
contribute to your community.
experienced a nervous breakdown, telling a female para-
hese ive steps to overcoming crisis do not necessarily medic, “Nothing is wrong. Everything is going right in
take place in a speciic order; not everyone experiences my life. I just don’t know how to stop crying.” (Salbi &
each step; some steps may evolve over the course of Buckland, 2005, p. 231). Zainab relected (p. 5):
years; and sometimes the trauma survivor may tackle I wanted to make myself whole again. I wanted to come
more than one step simultaneously. he following clean. I wanted to do my job without feeling like a hypo-
material, excerpted interviews and recommendations crite. But I had been afraid for so long I didn’t know how
were adapted from the book by Jerry White, Getting up to get rid of the layers of fear inside me. Because I had sur-
when life knocks you down: ive steps to overcoming a life vived by hiding my past, even from myself, I had never
crisis (2008). really pieced together the story of my own life. Which
of the things that had happened to me were causes, and
which were the efects?
The five steps to overcoming a life crisis
Writing about her experiences proved cathartic, “It felt
Face facts like the heavy, dark stone in my chest inally passed
When something threatens our very being and way of through me; it’s gone now,” she said, “Taking the time
life, we oten try to deny or avoid it. We may actively to piece together my personal story and my family’s his-
avoid memories of the trauma or forget important tory has given me new energy” to face the future with
aspects of what happened; we may shut down emo- more optimism and resilience (White, 2008, p. 49).
tionally and numb ourselves; we may detach emotion-
ally from our family, friends, and environment. Rape Choose life
survivors may shower repeatedly hoping to wash away Trauma survivors have the ability to choose life, even
the violation. War-injured veterans may drink or take though this choice may be enormously diicult, par-
drugs to numb painful memories. Individuals perman- ticularly in the immediate atermath of the trauma. In
ently paralyzed and in wheelchairs may pray fervently 1989, Jesús Martinez was 17 and living through civil
for years to walk again. his is natural. However, the war in El Salvador. He had a job in the city and had
Survivor Corps approach encourages survivors to “face to take a bus to work. Because of roadblocks set up by
facts” by acknowledging what has happened and then guerillas, Jesús had to get of his bus, walk around the
inding a way to live with it. roadblock, and get on another bus to continue his jour-
Talking through a personal trauma is one way to ney to work. White (2008, p. 61) quotes him as saying:
help to integrate the experience. hat is why Survivor I was a teenager, and not aware of the danger of mines.
Corps trained peer outreach workers to listen actively Everyone just got of the buses and walked on the side of
to other trauma survivors who describe, in their own the road. I don’t know why I was walking where no one
words, their traumas and subsequent attempts to cope. else was, but I remember someone saying there might be
In order to facilitate acceptance and assimilation of the mines. I didn’t think about it much.

299
Section 5: Training for resilience

Walking along the side of the road, he stepped on a mental and substance abuse conditions make use of
mine that blew of both his legs on the spot. he explo- empathy and empowerment to help support and inspire
sion was so powerful that others walking near him were recovery.
wounded. He never lost consciousness, but he did lose
hope. He went on to say Cancer peer-support groups have been shown to
enhance emotional functioning, quality of life, strat-
I fell in a hole. Both of my legs were blown of. I had blood
in my mouth. My arm was wounded, and really, I thought
egies for coping with illness, and attitudes toward treat-
it was all a bad dream. I tried to kill myself with an explo- ment (Glajchen & Magen, 1995). Peer support has been
sive that was lying on the ground near me. It didn’t used efectively for large numbers of people struggling
explode when I picked it up. When the soldiers arrived, with a host of health issues and disabling conditions,
I took a gun from one of them and begged him to kill me including physical and sexual abuse, substance abuse,
with it. I remember being so desperate to die, and saying breast cancer, chronic medical illnesses, and landmine
over and over, “Please kill me.” injuries.
Given the fundamental importance of social sup-
Jesús did not die that day. Instead he chose life (White, port, particularly in communities fragmented by war,
2008, p. 62): Survivor Corps spent most of its programmatic energy
he healing process was very diicult for me. here was and resources building networks of peer supporters
the healing of my body, and also the trauma in my mind. as a way for survivors to reconnect. By 2002, Survivor
I am thankful that I had the support of my family. It was
Corps had pioneered peer-to-peer-support networks
very important for me to meet other disabled people, and
seeing how they lived their lives. Sports had always been
in Bosnia–Herzegovina, Jordan, El Salvador, Ethiopia,
a part of my life, and I was very happy when I saw people Mozambique, and Vietnam. More recently, it has
practicing wheelchair sports in the hospital. It has been trained partner survivor groups in Colombia, Rwanda,
a long journey from wanting to die to getting where I am Burundi, and the USA.
now. I am very happy and excited about many aspects of
my life. I have a wife, my children, a family, my parents Get moving
and siblings, and a very wide circle of friends.
Climbing out of horriic crises oten requires Herculean
Initially, Jesús could not imagine living his life as a efort, physically and emotionally. To assist the survivor
severely physically disabled young man. But somehow in their eforts to move forward, the Survivor Corps
he was able to reach deep inside, ind the courage and approach required that each survivor developed his
power to reframe his tragic experience, and ultim- or her own individual action plan. he survivor must
ately chose to embrace his life. For many survivors, identify his or her life priorities (objectives) in the areas
this choice is supported by faith, spirituality, religion, of health, livelihood, and community, and then, with
hope, and/or an optimistic outlook. he survivor who the help of a peer-support worker, develop a realistic
chooses life understands that he or she is far more than plan with concrete steps to reach each objective.
their scars and circumstances. Creating an individual action plan, believing that
the objectives can be achieved, and successfully com-
Reach out pleting concrete steps tends to engender hope and build
Reaching out to peers, friends, and family is a key to self-conidence. By measuring these objectives and the
building resilience. Many survivor groups, from can- completion of speciic steps, peer-support workers can
cer to disability to veterans, attest to the power of peer more efectively evaluate progress and, when needed,
support to build resilience. Mental Health America re-evaluate and change objectives. Developing and
(2008) concurs that peer support is working on the individual action plan calls for specii-
city of objectives, identiication of activities, and meas-
… a unique and essential element of recovery-oriented
urable outputs relevant to the survivor’s life goals – all
mental health and substance abuse systems. MHA
[Mental Health America] calls on states and communities achievable within a reasonable period of time. he indi-
to incorporate peer-support services into community- vidual action plan is a contract of sorts that is used as a
based mental health and substance abuse services, both guide to track each survivor’s progress. Understandably,
as stand-alone entities and in conjunction with other livelihood or work objectives tend to become the strong-
services. he provision of mental health and substance est focus for survivors with disabilities living in poverty.
abuse support services by persons who have experienced Survivors know well how work afects their self-esteem,

300
Chapter 20: Interventions to enhance adult resilience

standing in the community, structure for the day, social lost his right leg. He wondered how he would feed his
companionship, and sense of purpose. wife and six-month-old son. His young family had
no home. So Ramiz moved in with his parents, but he
Give back avoided his family, drank heavily, and fell into a deep
Gratitude (thanking people who have helped) and depression. hen Ramiz met Adnan, another amputee
generosity (giving more than taking) both promote and peer outreach worker. Adnan listened for hours
resilience. he acts of thanking and giving indicate to Ramiz and gently encouraged him to get back to
the survivor has begun to emerge from the depths of his life, his responsibilities. Ramiz told Adnan he felt
their trauma. Survivor Corps hired survivors who have overwhelmed with the responsibility of taking care of
struggled with their disabilities but found positive ways his family. But Adnan persisted, showing up weekly in
to cope, and then trained these survivors to visit and lis- support of his fellow amputee survivor.
ten to newer victims. By serving as an inspiration and It took over a year, but inally peer-to-peer support
motivating force for other survivors, veteran survivors began to pay of. Ramiz joined a survivor economic
help to accompany newer survivors out from the dark- support group and attended meetings for six months.
ness of their isolation. Veteran survivors beneit as well Armed with a business plan and new conidence,
when they see how far they have come and rediscover Ramiz built a greenhouse and a thriving tomato busi-
their own strength. Survivor Corps observed that the ness. He became known in the community as honest
survivor who learns to focus energy on more than their and hardworking, and people lined up to buy his prod-
own survival is more likely to thrive. hat is why com- uce. White (2008, p. 124) quotes him as saying:
munity service has been incorporated as a principle I feel like I have eliminated that horrible feeling of uncer-
mode of recovery. tainty and insecurity. If it weren’t for this opportunity, I
Community service refers to service that an indi- would still be wasting my days drinking and my future
vidual survivor or group of survivors provides for would be uncertain. hanks to the support from my
the beneit of his or her community. he objective is community, my family has a strong husband and father
to provide meaningful and productive opportunities again, and with my new business, our future is no longer
for survivors to contribute to other members of the uncertain.
community. By moving from beneiciary to benefactor, But Ramiz took his growth even a step further. He
the survivor no longer feels like a social burden but, planned to build a second greenhouse and hire other
instead, a contributing member of the community. survivors. He also gave out produce to his neighbors
Making a diference in the lives of other survivors and in need: “he day I irst donated 200 kilograms of fresh
community members tends to evoke a sense of satisfac- tomatoes to the local orphanage was an incredible
tion, pride, and personal growth. By performing com- moment for me. Ater so many years, I could now help
munity service, a survivor can change the community’s others in need. I was no longer the beneiciary, but the
perception of survivors and motivate other survivors benefactor” (White, 2008, p. 125).
and community members to become active in worth-
while community projects. It is expected that the sur-
vivor will decide on a service project within the irst 18 The challenge of monitoring and evaluation
months of working on their individual action plan and Increasingly, civil society organizations use a version of
complete their service project before graduating from peer support in eforts to promote resilience. However,
their peer-supported program. While some survivors very few organizations deine or operationalize “peer
are ready to participate in a community service project support” and very few measure its eicacy, which has
early in their recovery process, many need more time limited the impact of these peer-support interventions
to heal and recover irst and, therefore, do not partici- within the clinical and scientiic community.
pate until later in the process. In partnership with the Centers for Disease Control
Giving is a particularly healing practice for indi- and Prevention, Survivor Corps set out to measure the
viduals who have survived war and witnessed the frag- eicacy of its program using the Medical Outcomes
mentation and destruction of their communities. he Study 36-Item Short Form Health Survey (SF-36®;
act of giving is a sign of resilience. http://www.sf-36.org/tools/sf36.shtml), a commonly
In 1994, Ramiz stepped on a landmine in Bosnia– used and well-validated instrument that measures
Herzegovina. White (2008, p. 124) tells his story. He self-perceived health-related quality of life. he SF-36

301
Section 5: Training for resilience

has been has been translated into numerous languages social supports, by involving local citizens in commu-
and used to assess self-perceived health in many coun- nity preparedness and responses to adversity, and by
tries across a wide cross-spectrum of socioeconomic preparing for the unexpected.
contexts. Survivors entering Survivor Corps programs Interventions designed to enhance resilience
were administered the SF-36 at the beginning of the can be targeted at preventing risk factors; bolstering
program and again when they “graduated” or discon- resources; developing skills; supporting cultural, reli-
tinued from the program, up to two years ater entry. gious, and spiritual rituals; promoting organizations
Preliminary data analysis with 280 survivors suggests that focus on developing competence (i.e., schools, ire
that the Survivor Corps approach has signiicant posi- and police departments, disaster response organiza-
tive efects on subjective assessments of physical and tions, and the military); limiting stressful/traumatic
mental well-being. exposure; and providing rapid evidence-based treat-
ment for traumatized individuals. Interventions to
Conclusions enhance resilience can be directed to the individual,
his chapter has described a broad range of interven- the family, and/or the community and may involve
tions that have been designed to enhance resilience or the bolstering of internal and external resources. As
constructs associated with resilience, such as social noted by Masten and others (Chapter 7), interventions
support. hese interventions are delivered in various can be initiated at one or more levels within a multi-
formats (e.g., in a classroom setting, over the Internet, level model. Finally, interventions to enhance resili-
or as part of a therapy modality), are commonly psy- ence should take into account other critical variables
choeducational and cognitive-behavioral in nature, such as age, physical and mental health, and cultural
and have been applied in a diverse range of popula- dynamics and norms, as well as personal beliefs and
tions. Although some of these interventions have values. For example, speciic cultural and spiritual
received preliminary empirical support (e.g., psych- practices may enhance resilience in one group of indi-
oeducational resilience enhancement training, well- viduals but not in another.
being therapy), the majority have received little or no he second part of this chapter presented a more
such support. in-depth description of a program developed by an
While the interventions described have been international organization, Survivor Corps, to foster
designed to foster resilience in individuals, interven- resilience in war-traumatized individuals. Survivor
tions to enhance resilience can be targeted toward fam- Corps built a global network of survivors and part-
ilies, organizations, and communities. For example, ners, helping thousands of victims of war to heal and
the Walsh family resilience framework (Chapter 10) rebuild their communities. Peer support formed the
focuses on fostering family strengths in response to core delivery vehicle in the program to foster and
stressful life events, adversity, and life transitions. It strengthen coping mechanisms that are known to be
does so in three domains of family life: organizational associated with resilience. Survivors themselves rec-
processes, family belief systems, and communication ommend building resilience through ive steps, which
processes. Walsh and colleagues stress the variety and include facing the facts, choosing life, reaching out to
complexity of families and the ways in which they func- connect with others, getting moving (e.g., setting goals
tion, and believe that no single strategy for enhancing and taking action), and giving back.
resilience applies to all families. In conclusion, research on resilience-enhancing
Just as resilience difers from one individual to interventions is in its infancy, but growing rapidly. More
another and from one family to another, communi- research is needed to reine measurement of resilience
ties difer in their capacity to respond and adapt to and related constructs, to examine factors that mediate
adversity. Norris and colleagues (Chapter 11) have the development of resilience in intervention stud-
proposed that community resilience can be bolstered ies, and to carefully consider outcome variables (e.g.,
by developing economic resources and using these reductions in psychopathology, functioning, coping,
resources to assist areas of greatest social vulnerability self-eicacy) that are sensitive to change. Most import-
by coordinating relevant organizational networks (e.g., antly, controlled trials are needed to examine the ei-
ire, police, mental health, rescue and recovery) prior cacy of resilience-enhancing interventions in a broad
to adverse events, by supporting naturally occurring range of populations.

302
Chapter 20: Interventions to enhance adult resilience

Brown, S. L., Nesse, R. M., Vinokur, A. D., & Smith, D. M.


References (2003). Providing social support may be more beneicial
Allred, K. D. & Smith, T. W. (1989). he hardy personality: than receiving it: results from a prospective study of
cognitive and physiological responses to evaluative mortality. Psychological Science, 14, 320–327.
threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, Campbell-Sills, L. & Stein, M. B. (2007). Psychometric
257–266. analysis and reinement of the Connor–Davidson
American Psychological Association (2010). he road to Resilience Scale (CD-RISC): Validation of a 10-item
resilience. Washington, DC: American Psychological measure of resilience. Journal of Trauma and Stress, 20,
Association, http://www.apa.org/helpcenter/road- 1019–1028.
resilience.aspx (accessed June 25, 2010). Cannon-Bowers, J. A. & Salas, E. (eds.) (1998). Making
Barrera, M. Jr. (1986). Distinctions between social support decisions under stress: Implications for individual and
concepts, measures, and models. American Journal of team training. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Community Psychology, 14, 413–445. Association.
Bartone, P. T. (1993). Psychosocial predictors of soldier Charney, D. S. (2004). Psychobiological mechanisms of
adjustment to combat stress. In Proceedings of the hird resilience and vulnerability: implications for successful
European Conference on Traumatic Stress, Bergen, adaptation to extreme stress. American Journal of
Norway, June. Psychiatry, 161, 195–216.
Bartone, P. T. (1995). A short hardiness scale. In Proceedings Charuvastra, A. & Cloitre, M. (2008). Social bonds
of the 103rd Annual Convention of the American and posttraumatic stress disorder. Annual Review of
Psychological Association, New York, July. Psychology, 59, 301–328.
Bartone, P. T. (2006). Resilience under military operational Connor, K. M. & Davidson, J. R. (2003). Development of a
stress: Can leaders inluence hardiness? Military new resilience scale: the Connor–Davidson Resilience
Psychology, 18(Suppl.), S131–S148. Scale (CD-RISC). Depression and Anxiety, 18, 76–82.
Bartone, P. T. (2007). Test-retest reliability of the Contrada, R. J. (1989). Type A behavior, personality
dispositional resilience scale-15, a brief hardiness scale. hardiness, and cardiovascular responses to stress. Journal
Psychological Reports, 101, 943–944. of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 895–903.
Bartone, P. T., Ursano, R. J., Wright, K. M., & Ingraham, L. H. Cutuli, J. J., Chaplin, T. M., Gillham, J. E., Reivich, K. J., &
(1989). he impact of a military air disaster on the health Seligman, M. E. (2006). Preventing co-occurring
of assistance workers. A prospective study. Journal of depression symptoms in adolescents with conduct
Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 317–328. problems: the Penn Resiliency Program. Annals of the
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 282–286.
Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press. Davidson, L., Payne, V. M., Connor, K. M., et al. (2005).
Belaise, C., Fava, G. A., & Marks, I. M. (2005). Alternatives Trauma, resilience and saliostasis: efects of treatment
to debrieing and modiications to cognitive behavior in post-traumatic stress disorder. International Clinical
therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychotherapy Psychopharmacology, 20, 43–48.
and Psychosomatics, 74, 212–217. Davidson, L., Baldwin, D. S., Stein, D. J., et al. (2008).
Bonanno, G. A., Galea, S., Bucciarelli, A., & Vlahov, D. Efects of venlafaxine extended release on resilience
(2006). Psychological resilience ater disaster: New York in posttraumatic stress disorder: an item analysis of
City in the atermath of the September 11th terrorist the Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale. International
attack. Psychological Science, 17, 181–186. Clinical Psychopharmacology, 23, 299–303.
Bond, J., Kaskutas, L. A., & Weisner, C. (2003). he Defense Centers of Excellence (2009). Defense Centers of
persistent inluence of social networks and alcoholics Excellence for Psychological Health and Traumatic Brain
anonymous on abstinence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Injury: Annual Report 2009. Silver Springs MD: Defense
64, 579–588. Centers of Excellence.
Brand, E. F., Lakey, B., & Berman, S. (1995). A preventive, Eid, J. & Morgan, C. A. 3rd (2006). Dissociation, hardiness,
psychoeducational approach to increase perceived social and performance in military cadets participating in
support. American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, survival training. Military Medicine, 171, 436–442.
117–135. Fava, G. A. (1999). Well-being therapy: conceptual and
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta- technical issues. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 68,
analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder 171–179.
in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and Fava, G. A. & Ruini, C. (2003). Development
Clinical Psychology, 68, 748–766. and characteristics of a well-being enhancing

303
Section 5: Training for resilience

psychotherapeutic strategy: well-being therapy. Journal Haglund, M. E., Nestadt, P. S., Cooper, N. S., Southwick, S. M.,
of Behavioral herapy and Expimental Psychiatry, 34, & Charney, D. S. (2007). Psychobiological mechanisms
45–63. of resilience: Relevance to prevention and treatment
Fava, G. A., Rafanelli, C., Cazzaro, M., Conti, S., & Grandi, S. of stress-related psychopathology. Development and
(1998a). Well-being therapy. A novel psychotherapeutic Psychopathology, 19, 889–920.
approach for residual symptoms of afective disorders. Hogan, B. E., Linden, W., & Najarian, B. (2002). Social
Psychological Medicine, 28, 475–480. support interventions: Do they work? Clinical
Fava, G. A., Rafanelli, C., Grandi, S., Conti, S., & Belluardo, P. Psychological Reviews, 22, 383–442.
(1998b). Prevention of recurrent depression with Johnson, D. C., Polusny, M. A., Erbes, C. R., et al. (2011).
cognitive behavioral therapy: preliminary indings. Development and initial validation of the Response to
Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 816–820. Stressful Experiences Scale (RSES). Military Medicine,
176, 161–169.
Fava, G. A., Rafanelli, C., Ottolini, F., et al. (2001).
Psychological well-being and residual symptoms in Kaniasty, K. & Norris, F. H. (2008). Longitudinal linkages
remitted patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia. between perceived social support and posttraumatic
Journal of Afective Disorders, 65, 185–190. stress symptoms: sequential roles of social causation
and social selection. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21,
Fava, G. A., Ruini, C., Rafanelli, C., et al. (2005). Well-being
274–281.
therapy of generalized anxiety disorder. Psychotherapy
and Psychosomatics, 74, 26–30. Kaskutas, L. A., Bond, J., & Humphreys, K. (2002). Social
networks as mediators of the efect of Alcoholics
Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., & Taubman, O. (1995). Does
Anonymous. Addiction, 97, 891–900.
hardiness contribute to mental health during a stressful
real-life situation? he roles of appraisal and coping. Kaskutas, L. A., Bond, J., & Avalos, L. A. (2009). 7-year
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 687–695. trajectories of Alcoholics Anonymous attendance and
associations with treatment. Addictive Behavior, 34,
Foa, E. B., Rothbaum, B. O., Riggs, D. S., & Murdock, T. B.
1029–1035.
(1991). Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder
in rape victims: A comparison between cognitive- Khoshaba, D. M. & Maddi, S. R. (2001). HardiTraining.
behavioral procedures and counseling. Journal of Newport Beach, CA: Hardines Institute.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 715–723. Kilpatrick, D. G., Veronen, L. J., & Resick, P. A. (1982).
Foa, E. B., Dancu, C. V., Hembree, E. A., et al. (1999). A Psychological sequelae to rape: Assessment and
comparison of exposure therapy, stress inoculation treatment strategies. In D. M. Dolays & R. L. Meredith
training, and their combination for reducing (eds.), Behavioral medicine: Assessment and treatment
posttraumatic stress disorder in female assault victims. strategies (pp. 473–497). New York: Plenum Press.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, King, L. A., King, D. W., Fairbank, J. A., Keane, T. M., &
194–200. Adams, G. A. (1998). Resilience-recovery factors in
Garmezy, N. (1974). Children at risk: the search for the post-traumatic stress disorder among female and male
antecedents of schizophrenia. Part II: Ongoing research Vietnam veterans: hardiness, postwar social support, and
programs, issues, and intervention. Schizophrenia additional stressful life events. Journal of Personality and
Bulletin, 1, 55–125. Social Psychology, 74, 420–434.
Garmezy, N. & Streitman, S. (1974). Children at risk: the Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and
search for the antecedents of schizophrenia. Part I. health an inquiry into hardiness. Journal of Personality
Conceptual models and research methods. Schizophrenia and Social Psychology, 37, 1–11.
Bulletin, 1, 14–90. Kobasa, S. C. & Puccetti, M. C. (1983). Personality and
Glajchen, M. & Magen, R. (1995). Evaluating process, social resources in stress resistance. Journal of Personality
outcome, and satisfaction in community-based cancer and Social Psychology, 45, 839–850.
support groups. In M. J. Galinsky & J. H. Schopler Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., & Courington, S. (1981).
(eds.), Support groups: Current perspectives on theory and Personality and constitution as mediators in the stress-
practice (pp. 27–40). New York: Haworth Press. illness relationship. Journal of Health and Social Behavior,
Gof, B. A. (2010). Training and assessment in gynaecologic 22, 368–378.
surgery: the role of simulation. Best Practice in Research Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., & Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness
and Clinical Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 24, 759–756. and health: a prospective study. Journal of Personality and
Gopher, D., Weil, M., & Bareket, T. (1994). Transfer of skill Social Psychology, 42, 168–177.
from a computer game trainer to light. Human Factors, Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., Puccetti, M. C., & Zola, M. A.
36, 387–405. (1985). Efectiveness of hardiness, exercise and

304
Chapter 20: Interventions to enhance adult resilience

social support as resources against illness. Journal of training. In C. Spielberger & I. Sarason (eds.), Stress and
Psychosomatic Research, 29, 525–533. anxiety, Vol. 2 (pp. 237–264). New York: Wiley.
Lazarus, R. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and Meichenbaum, D. (1985). Psychology practitioner
coping. New York: Springer. guidebooks: Stress inoculation training. New York:
Lett, H. S., Blumenthal, J. A., Babyak, M. A., et al. (2007). Pergamon Press.
Social support and prognosis in patients at increased Meichenbaum, D. & Jaremko, M. E. (eds.) (1983). Stress
psychosocial risk recovering from myocardial infarction. reduction and prevention. New York: Plenum Press.
Health Psychology, 26, 418–427.
Meichenbaum, D. H. & Defenbacher, J. L. (1988). Stress
Lett, H. S., Blumenthal, J. A., Babyak, M. A., et al. (2009). inoculation training. Counseling Psychologist, 16, 69–90.
Dimensions of social support and depression in
Mental Health America (2008). Position Statement 37: he
patients at increased psychosocial risk recovering from
role of peer support services in the creation of recovery-
myocardial infarction. International Journal of Behavioral
oriented mental health systems. Alexandria, VA: Mental
Medicine, 16, 248–258.
Health America (accessed February 28, 2011). http://
Litt, M. D., Kadden, R. M., Kabela-Cormier, E., & Petry, N. www.nmha.org/go/position-statements/37 (accessed
(2007). Changing network support for drinking: initial February 28, 2011).
indings from the network support project. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 542–555. Moak, Z. B. & Agrawal, A. (2010). he association between
perceived interpersonal social support and physical and
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). he construct mental health: results from the national epidemiological
of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for survey on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of
future work. Child Development, 71, 543–562. Public Health, 32, 191–201.
Maddi, S. R. (2002). he story of hardiness: Twenty years of Nishisaki, A., Hales, R., Biagas, K., et al. (2009). A multi-
theorizing, research, and practice. Consulting Psychology institutional high-idelity simulation “boot camp”
Journal: Practice and Research, 54, 173–185. orientation and training program for irst year pediatric
Maddi, S. R. (2005). On hardiness and other pathways to critical care fellows. Pediatric Critical Care Medicine, 10,
resilience. American Psychology, 60, 261–262; discussion 157–162.
265–267. Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2008).
Maddi, S. R. (2007). Relevance of hardiness assessment and Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and
training to the military context. Military Psychology, 19, symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psychological
61–70. Trauma: heory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 1(Suppl.
Maddi, S. R. (2008). he courage and strategies of hardiness 1), 3–36.
as helpful in growing despite major, disruptive stresses. Pietrzak, R. H., Johnson, D. C., Goldstein, M. B., et al.
American Psychologist, 63, 563–564. (2010). Psychosocial bufers of traumatic stress,
Maddi, S. R. & Khoshaba, D. M. (2005). Resilience at work depressive symptoms, and psychosocial diiculties in
New York: AMACOM. veterans of Operations Enduring Freedom and Iraqi
Freedom: the role of resilience, unit support, and
Maddi, S. R. & Kobasa, S. C. (1984). he hardy executive: postdeployment social support. Journal of Afective
Health under stress. Homewood, IL: Dow Jones-Irwin. Disorders, 120, 188–192.
Maddi, S. R., Khoshaba, D. M., Jensen, K., et al. (2002). Prince-Embury, S. (2008). he Resiliency Scales for
Hardiness training for high-risk undergraduates. Children and Adolescents: Psychological symptoms, and
NACADA Journal, 22, 45–55. clinical status in adolescents. Canadian Journal of School
Maddi, S. R., Harvey, R. H., Khoshaba, D. M., et al. (2006). Psychology, 23, 41–56.
he personality construct of hardiness, III: Relationships Ruini, C., Belaise, C., Brombin, C., Cafo, E., & Fava, G. A.
with repression, innovativeness, authoritarianism, and (2006). Well-being therapy in school settings: a pilot
performance. Journal of Personality, 74, 575–597. study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 75, 331–336.
Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic. Resilience processes Ruini, C., Ottolini, F., Tomba, E., et al. (2009). School
in development. American Psychologist, 56, 227–238. intervention for promoting psychological well-being
McCalister, K. T., Dolbier, C. L., Webster, J. A., Mallon, in adolescence. Journal of Behavioral herapy and
M. W., & Steinhardt, M. A. (2006). Hardiness and support Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 522–532.
at work as predictors of work stress and job satisfaction.
Ryf, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it?
American Journal of Health Promotion, 20, 183–191.
Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-
Meichenbaum, D. (1975). A self-instructional approach to being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
stress management: A proposal for stress inoculation 1069–1081.

305
Section 5: Training for resilience

Salas, E. & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (2001). he science Steinhardt, M. A., Dolbier, C. L., Gottlieb, N. H., &
of training: a decade of progress. Annual Reviews in McCalister, K. T. (2003). he relationship between
Psychology, 52, 471–499. hardiness, supervisor support, group cohesion, and job
Salas, E., Priest, H. A., Wilson, K. A., & Burke, C. S. stress as predictors of job satisfaction. American Journal
(2006). Scenario-based training: Improving military of Health Promotion, 17, 382–389.
mission performance and adaptability. In A. B. Adler, Vaishnavi, S., Connor, K., & Davidson, J. R. (2007). An
C. A. Castro, & T. W. Britt (eds.), Military life: he abbreviated version of the Connor–Davidson Resilience
psychology of serving in peace and combat, Vol. 2: Scale (CD-RISC), the CD-RISC2: psychometric
Operational stress. Westport: Praeger Security properties and applications in psychopharmacological
International. trials. Psychiatry Research, 152, 293–297.
Salbi, Z. & Buckland, L. (2005). Between two worlds – Escape Westphal, M., Bonanno, G. A., & Bartone, P. T. (2008).
from tyranny: Growing up in the shadow of Saddam. New Resilience and personality. In B. J. Lukey & V. Tepe (eds.),
York: Penguin. Biobehavioral resilience to stress (pp. 219–244). Boca
Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism. New York:
Pocket Books. Whealin, J. M., Ruzek, J. I., & Southwick, S. (2008).
Cognitive-behavioral theory and preparation for
Seligman, M. E. P., Schulman, P., DeRubeis, R. J., & Hollon, professionals at risk for trauma exposure. Trauma,
S. D. (1999). he prevention of depression and anxiety. Violence, and Abuse, 9, 100–113.
Prevention and Treatment, 2, 8.
White, J. (2008). Getting up when life knocks you down: Five
Southwick, S. M., Vythilingam, M., & Charney, D. S. (2005). steps to overcoming a life crisis. New York: St. Martin’s
he psychobiology of depression and resilience to stress: Press.
Implications for prevention and treatment. Annual
Writing Committee for the ENRICHD Investigators (2003).
Reviews of Clinical Psychology, 1, 255–291.
Efects of treating depression and low perceived social
Steinhardt, M. A. & Dolbier, C. (2008). Evaluation of a support on clinical events ater myocardial infarction:
resilience intervention to enhance coping strategies and the Enhancing Recovery in Coronary Heart Disease
protective factors and decrease symptomatology. Journal Patients (ENRICHD) Randomized Trial. Journal of the
of the American College of Health, 56, 445–453. American Medical Association, 289, 3106–3116.

306
Section 5 Training for resilience
Chapter
Childhood resilience: adaptation, mastery,

21 and attachment
Angie Torres, Steven M. Southwick, and Linda C. Mayes

Introduction able to provide support and protection, and bufer the


impact of the experience.
Exposure to adversity, challenge, and day to day stress
How challenging circumstances promote adapta-
is an inevitable aspect of nearly every child’s devel-
tion or are suiciently toxic to lead to enduring changes
opment. For some children, these challenges are acute,
in the neural circuitry of stress-response systems is a
uncontrollable, and immediately overwhelming.
critical question for studies of childhood resilience.
Others are exposed on a daily basis to chronic stressors
his chapter focuses on how the relationships in a
such as poverty, parental psychopathology, and envir-
child’s life serve to moderate and bufer the potentially
onmental chaos. Still others experience the stress of
detrimental impact of stress and adversity, as well as
moving to a new school or a new home and grow adap-
how these relationships may promote the development
tively from the experience. he ability to cope with, even
grow in response to, novel or threatening situations is of skills needed to master subsequent stress and adver-
essential to healthy development and to survival. sity. here is an explicit decision not to frame the discus-
he capacity to respond to and manage novelty sion in terms of children who are more or less resilient
and potential threat is based in brain circuits whose but rather in terms of those environmental conditions
development is inluenced by multiple experiences and experiences that potentially enhance every child’s
beginning in the irst years of life. Experiences or ability to respond adaptively to and learn from stress
environmental conditions that activate these circuits (Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000). he environment is deined
are considered stressors and, under optimal or usual as primarily the child’s parenting and home caregiving
conditions, the body’s response to such stressors pro- environment but also, particularly as children mature,
motes learning so that the child has developed a set of their caregiving environments outside of the home,
adaptive responses for subsequent exposures. including schools and communities, which may also
At the same time, indings from a growing body of be positively supportive. Accumulating data suggest
research suggest that exceptionally stressful experi- that environmental/caregiving conditions are critical
ences early in life may have long-term consequences for establishing “set-points” for stress-system acti-
for a child’s cognitive, social, and emotional health, and vation and also for the ability of the system – and the
long-term consequences for both physical and mental body – to return to baseline ater a stressful condition
health (Gunnar & Vazquez, 2006). So-called “toxic has resolved or the child is no longer in the situation.
stress” (Loman & Gunnar, 2010) may lead to a detri- he later part of the chapter reviews those pre-
mental impact on developing brain architecture and on vention and intervention programs that, while not
the physiological regulatory systems that help children expressly focused on “promoting resilience,” have
to respond to and learn from challenge and adversity. positive efects on children’s stress-regulatory abilities
What makes a situation detrimental or toxic relates to through their impact on children’s caregiving environ-
whether the stressful experience is controllable, how ment, their ability to develop social supports and net-
oten and how long the stress-response system has been works, their ability to adaptively seek out and use social
activated (e.g., chronic stress), and whether or not the relationships, and their ability to master challenges and
child has a dependable, stable set of relationships that is develop a strong sense of self-eicacy. he discussion is

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

307
Section 5: Training for resilience

framed particularly on the concept of allostasis and of adaptation to the world around them. hese include a
allostatic load (McEwen, 2000). motivation to learn; the ability to regulate emotions,
Allostasis refers to the ability to maintain stabil- including aggressive impulses; the capacity to soothe
ity throughout change or to maintain homeostasis. the self; the ability to solve problems under stress;
Adaptation in response to stressful situations involves knowing the diference between right and wrong; hav-
activation of neural, endocrine, and immune mecha- ing the capacity to form and sustain friendships and
nisms that permit the body to respond to the challenge intimate relationships; and, ultimately, to care for a
and then to return to a homeostatic baseline. Allostatic child as a parent. Each of these capacities, in turn, pro-
load refers to the physiological impact of chronic acti- motes positive, healthy adaptation to challenge and
vation of neural, endocrine, and immune systems in adversity.
response to chronic stress. he higher the allostatic In the earliest interactions with parents, there is an
load, or the longer the activation of the body’s stress essential reciprocity of exchanges – what some have
response, the greater the damage to the body in the called “serve and return” (Shonkof & Phillips, 2000) –
long run. he main hormonal mediators of the stress in which infants and young children naturally make
response, cortisol and epinephrine, have both protect- bids for interactions through their babbling, facial
ive and damaging efects. In the acute situation, both expressions, and gestures. Adults respond oten with
are essential for adaptive response and maintaining the same kind of vocalizations, gestures, or expres-
homeostasis in the face of challenge. However, if acti- sions – sometimes even more marked for emphasis.
vated frequently and for long periods of time (i.e., an he consistency, reliability, and afective tone of these
increase in allostatic load), the same system that under types of early exchange are fundamental to creating
a lower allostatic load is protective becomes damaging relationships in which the infant’s social communica-
and can accelerate a number of disease processes, tion abilities are nurtured and their exploration and
including cardiovascular disease, diabetes, depression, learning about the world around them is supported.
and anxiety. Accumulating evidence also underscores the import-
Systems for maintaining homeostasis develop in ance of these early interactions for the development of
early childhood and are particularly sensitive to the the brain, particularly in enhancing neural networks
allostatic load placed on the child at any given time and in supporting learning (summarized by Shonkof
in early development. hat is, a greater allostatic load & Phillips, 2000). Perhaps most salient in the efects of
earlier in childhood may permanently alter the respon- early relationships appears to be the long-term impact
siveness of stress-regulatory systems throughout life. on stress reactivity and allostatic capacities in the face
Hence, those experiences (e.g., caregiving relation- of challenge. Chronic adversity in young children’s lives
ships) and conditions that moderate a child’s allostatic is related to greater permeability and reactivity to stress
load are critical to the long-term development of adap- in later childhood and adolescence (Nachmias et al.,
tive homeostasis. Converging research indings from 1996; Loman & Gunnar, 2010), whereas secure, con-
many sources suggest that early care and attachment sistent caregiving is related to more lexible allostatic or
relationships are the major developmental contribu- stress-response capacities, which, in turn, may facili-
tors to moderating the allostatic load in a child’s life tate children in growing from stress and challenge.
(Loman & Gunnar, 2010). In turn, when allostatic load Attachment perspectives ofer models for concep-
is appropriately moderated, children develop a strong tualizing how relationships or “internalized working
sense of self-eicacy and mastery, which helps to mod- models” can be stress bufering and can enhance posi-
erate future adversity. tive adaptation (Southwick et al., 2008). Attachment
perspectives are essentially models of stress and emo-
tion regulation (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999) and of how
Attachments as stress regulating children (and adults) use important persons in their
Healthy development depends on the quality and sta- lives to downregulate negative afective states and
bility of a child’s relationships with the caring adults in upregulate positive or rewarding states. In this way,
his or her life, including parents, other family caregiv- attachment perspectives are readily integrated into
ers, and adults outside the family. Such relationships lay neurobiological models of the balance between stress
the foundation for a range of developmental outcomes regulation and reward systems that are relevant to
and skills that signiicantly impact children’s lives and processes with negative outcomes, such as drug use

308
Chapter 21: Childhood resilience

and addiction (Brady & Sinha, 2005), and positive adaptation to challenge is based largely on the security
outcomes, such as enhanced mastery and self-eicacy. of one’s internalized working models or social schema
here are a number of key features of an attachment of relationships.
perspective relevant to allostasis and stress reactivity: his notion also ties into the idea of “good enough
how early caregiving does or does not enhance secure parenting,” where caregivers provide suicient protec-
and trusting attachments in infants, how these early tion, emotional bufering, and responsiveness, which
experiences of being cared for shape templates of what change with the developmental needs of the child,
one might expect generally from relationships with but at the same time provide the child with suicient
others, and how relationships with others do and do exposure to the challenges and stressors that promote
not provide expected safety and security. In the lan- growth and mastery. As children mature, their capacity
guage of attachment theory, these early templates are to tolerate negative afective experiences and meet
thought of as “internalized working models”; in social both challenge and acutely stressful situations changes.
cognitive theory, they are considered as social schemas he source of that change is not only maturation but
or models for social interactions. also how parents bufer the environment just enough
Secure models of attachment emerge from early that the child is not overwhelmed but at the same time
relationships that are appropriately contingent to a has the experience of mastery which, in turn, contrib-
child’s needs and bufer the child from overwhelming utes to a sense of individuality, eicacy, coping, and
stress but at the same time give the child suicient chal- optimism.
lenge and opportunities to solve problems and regulate Related capacities shaped by early care and relation-
his or her own feelings; this allows the child to begin to ships that are relevant to response and adaptation to
develop a sense of autonomy and individuality as well challenge include lexibility; imagination, or the abil-
as agency and mastery. Importantly, too-perfect con- ity to cognitively reframe or think through alternative
tingency or too much protection is just as distorting possibilities for an experience or considered choice;
of relationship schemas as too little or neglectful care and curiosity, as a manifestation of lexibility. Each of
with frequent exposure to overwhelming stress and these abilities is, in turn, central to positive adaptation
chaos. From these early experiences and developing or resilience. Further, research has shown that secure
templates, children begin to develop a perspective on children are more curious, more lexible, and gener-
the world of relationships and other persons as trust- ally have greater access to imagination and an adap-
worthy, responsive, caring, and helpful, or as frighten- tive playfulness than insecure children (Sroufe, 2005;
ing, unsafe, uncaring, and not reliable under challenge Sroufe et al., 2005), and that these diferences are based
or extreme stress. in developmental changes in speciic neural systems.
Indeed, these templates of social relationships
come into play for the child or the adult particularly in Neurobiological models of caregiving
times of stress. hat is, at times of distress and fright,
children and adults call on their individual schemas of and stress reactivity
how important caregivers have responded in the past. here are now considerable supporting data from
If a child’s internalized map is secure, he or she is able pre-clinical models for the relationship between early
to use real people in the external world for comfort experiences and individual diferences in caregiving
and for companionship during stress – and is also able behavior as these afect individual diferences in chil-
to learn from others around in these challenging cir- dren’s ability to modulate their response to stress and
cumstances. If that template is insecure or disorgan- adversity (Kaufman et al., 2000; Southwick et al., 2008).
ized, the individual child or adult is let more isolated, Developing animals that are forced to confront over-
more anxious in the face of challenge, and hence with whelming and uncontrollable stressors tend to display
increased allostatic load and dysfunctional allostasis an exaggerated or sensitized sympathetic nervous sys-
mechanisms; these, in turn, perpetuated and activated tem and/or hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA)
again at the next stressful time. hese circumstances axis response to stress as adults (reviewed by Kaufman
also interfere with a child’s ability to develop a sense et al. [2000] and Southwick et al. [1999]). For example,
of mastery of the speciic challenge and an individual separating rat pups or infant monkeys from their
sense of eicacy for their capacity to respond adap- mother for substantial periods of time results in long-
tively in the future. In this point of view, positive term increases in corticotropin-releasing hormone

309
Section 5: Training for resilience

(CRH), reduced central benzodiazepine binding, of the adult ofspring (Denenberg et al., 1969; Levine,
altered neuron development (reduced iber dens- 1975; Francis et al., 1999). Although many of the recent
ities) in some areas of the brain, increases in anxiety- data are from rodent studies, investigations in social
like behaviors, impaired cognitive performance, and primates also highlight the importance of early moth-
decreases in social interaction (reviewed by Kaufman ering in determining how the daughters will mother
et al., 2000). (Harlow, 1963; Suomi et al., 1983). his complex pro-
Primate studies show similar efects of early uncon- gramming through maternal care also appears to inlu-
trolled or chronic stress, including studies in rhesus and ence aspects of learning and memory.
squirrel monkeys exposed to varying degrees of mater- Models relating maternal care to developing neural
nal deprivation (Suomi, 1997; Levine, 2005). Rhesus stress-response systems and to subsequent parenting
infants reared only with peers showed larger cortisol use both experimental manipulations and naturally
responses to psychological stressors as adults and were occurring variations. Repeated handling of pups in con-
more reactive (Higley et al., 1992); they also consumed junction with prolonged maternal separations induces
more alcohol when given access (Fahlke et al., 2000). deranged maternal behavior, including a reduction
In a non-human primate brain imaging study, Rilling in licking and grooming by the rat dams and reduced
and colleagues (2001) showed that separation of juven- maternal aggression. Similarly, the adult ofspring
ile rhesus monkeys from their mothers was associated show increased neuroendocrine responses to acute
with activation of cortical brain circuits that are sus- restraint stress and airpuf startle, including elevated
ceptible to stress hormones. levels of mRNA for CRH in the paraventricular nucleus
Indeed, the behavioral proiles of mistreated mon- and elevated plasma levels of adrenocorticotropic hor-
keys (normative reactivity with normal care versus mone (ACTH), corticosterone and mRNA for steroid.
high-stress reactivity with disrupted maternal care) are hese animals also show an increased acoustic startle
remarkably similar to Bowlby’s original description of response and enhanced anxiety or fearfulness to novel
anxious versus secure attachment in human infants environments (Ladd et al., 2000, 2004).
(Suomi, 1995). In addition, studies of human infants In contrast, developing animals exposed to mild to
reared in orphanages, malnourished, and/or exposed moderate stressors that are under their control and that
to prenatal stress suggested similar long-term efects they can master tend to become stress inoculated, with
on stress-regulatory HPA systems, which endured into a reduced overall response to future stressors (reviewed
adulthood (reviewed by Gunnar, 2000). Consequently, by Kaufman et al., 2000) and more eicient mastery of
it appears that under early exposure to chronic, uncon- novelty and/or future challenges. Repeated handling of
trollable stress individuals develop stress-sensitized rodent pups in conjunction with brief maternal sepa-
systems that hyperrespond to future stressors with rations induces more licking and grooming by the rat
exaggerated physiological and biochemical responsive- dams (Liu et al., 1997). As adults, the ofspring of moth-
ness (Kaufman et al., 2000). Further, it is also clear that ers that exhibited more licking and grooming of pups
the efects of early maternal deprivation in primates during the irst 10 days of life showed reduced plasma
may be diicult to reverse, such that many maternally ACTH and corticosterone responses to acute restraint
deprived monkeys are able to function normally as stress, as well as increased hippocampal glucocortic-
adults under normal conditions but are unable to cope oid receptor mRNA expression, and decreased levels of
with psychosocial stressors (Suomi et al., 1976). hypothalamic CRH mRNA (Plotsky & Meaney, 1993;
As discussed earlier in this chapter, parenting is Liu et al., 1997). Subsequent studies have shown that
key to bufering children from uncontrollable or over- the ofspring of these mothers with high licking and
whelming stress. Recent advances in our understand- grooming activity also show reduced acoustic startle
ing of the genetic, epigenetic, and neurobiological responses and enhanced spatial learning and mem-
substrates of maternal behavior in model mammalian ory (Ladd et al., 2000; Liu et al., 2000). It appears in
species are relevant to processes contributing to resili- these models that, in the face of controllable stressors,
ent and non-resilient adaptations. Pre-clinical stud- enhanced maternal care serves to facilitate more robust
ies suggest that the nature of the maternal behavior stress-response capacities as adults.
in the days following birth serves to establish the the Naturally occurring variations in licking, groom-
level of HPA responsiveness to stress in pups, as well ing, and arched back nursing have also been associ-
as “programming” the subsequent maternal behavior ated with the development of individual diferences

310
Chapter 21: Childhood resilience

in behavioral responses to novelty in adult ofspring. and the infant. Building on the early work of Bowlby
Adult ofspring of the nursing mothers with low levels and colleagues (Bowlby, 1969), eforts to characterize
of licking, grooming, and arched back show increased this reciprocal interaction between caregiver and infant
startle responses, decreased open-ield exploration, and to assess its impact have provided a powerful the-
and longer latencies to eat food provided in a novel oretical and empirical framework for social and emo-
environment (Francis et al., 1999). Importantly, these tional development (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999). Since
diferences pertain in cross-fostering designs when of- the 1970s, clear evidence has emerged that signiicant
spring of mothers with high licking and grooming habit disturbances in the early parent–child relationship
are cross-fostered to mothers with a low habit and vice (relected in such things as child abuse and neglect or
versa. In the cross-fostering experiments, ofspring of insecure attachments) contribute to an increased risk
mothers with a high licking and grooming habit cared for developing both internalizing and externalizing
for by mothers with a low habit show similar increased disorders over the entire life (Sroufe et al., 1999; Sroufe,
startle and decreased open-ield exploration to those 2005). While early adversity and insecure attachment
of the ofspring of the mothers with a low licking and may not be a proximal cause of later psychopathology,
grooming habit (Francis et al., 1999). it appears to confer risk.
Furthermore, Francis and co-workers (1999) dem- Conversely, longitudinal studies of high-risk
onstrated that the inluence of maternal care on the infants suggest that the formation of a special relation-
development of stress reactivity was mediated by ship with a caring adult in the perinatal period confers
changes in gene expression in regions of the brain that a degree of resiliency and protection against the devel-
regulate stress responses. For example, adult ofspring opment of psychopathology later in life (Werner, 1997).
of dams with high licking, grooming, and arched back Similar to the indings in rodents by Liu, Francis, and
nursing habit showed increased mRNA expression for colleagues (Liu et al., 1997; Francis et al., 1999), there is
the hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor and brain- accumulating evidence indicating that human caregiv-
derived neurotrophic factor, and increased produc- ers’ levels of response to their children can be traced
tion of the N-acetyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor in part to the caregivers’ own childrearing histor-
subunit and cholinergic innervation of the hippocam- ies and attachment-related experiences (Miller et al.,
pus. In the paraventricular nucleus, there is decreased 1997). Caregivers’ attachment-related experiences
CRH mRNA. hese adult pups also show a number are encoded in “internal working models,” described
of changes in receptor density in the locus ceruleus, above, and establish styles of emotional communica-
including increased α2-adrenoceptors, reduced GABAA tion that either bufer the individual in times of stress
receptors, and decreased CRH receptors (Caldji et al., or contribute to maladaptive patterns of afect regula-
2000). tion and behavior (Bretherton & Munholland, 1999).
In sum, despite genetic constraints, data from ani-
mal studies indicate that the interval surrounding the
birth of the rat pup or the rhesus infant is a critical Self-efficacy, mastery, and stress
period in the life of the animal that likely has enduring Self-eicacy, deined as a positive belief in one’s abil-
neurobiological and behavioral consequences. In par- ity to perform optimally in a situation, has wide-
ticular, the nature of early caregiving experiences can reaching efects in children’s (and adult’s) functioning.
have enduring consequences on individual diferences Perceived self-eicacy impacts how children feel, their
in subsequent anxiety regulation and patterns of stress motivation to learn and meet challenges, and their
response, occurring through speciic neuropharmaco- perception of their world and day to day experiences.
logical mechanisms (reviewed by Weaver et al., 2004). For example, individuals with a strong sense of self-
Turning to humans, increasing clinical and epi- eicacy approach novel tasks or diicult situations as
demiological data support the view that exposure to challenges that can be mastered, rather than as threats
early adverse environments underlies vulnerability to or dangers to be avoided. Indeed, individuals with a
altered physiological responses to stress and the later strong sense of eicacy may seek out challenges, set
expression of mood and anxiety disorders (Brown goals, and maintain a high commitment to these goals.
et al., 1987; Ambelas, 1990; Kendler et al., 2002, 2006). In the face of failure or set-back, individuals with high
Among the most important early environmental inlu- perceived self-eicacy redouble their eforts to master
ences is the interaction between the primary caregiver the situation at hand. hey also approach stressful or

311
Section 5: Training for resilience

uncertain situations with a perception that they will be Far less is known about the so-called stress-inoc-
able to meet whatever the situation requires. In turn, ulation efect in humans. In one study, pediatric inpa-
such an eicacious outlook diminishes the experience tients with previous positive separation experiences
of stress and lowers the likelihood of the detrimental such as staying with grandparents for short periods of
experience of chronic stress. Consequently, perceived time experienced less stress during their hospital stay
self-eicacy relects both a response to challenges that (Stacey, 1970). Childhood exposure to mild stress has
have been met and mastered and a capacity that facili- also been associated with reduced heart rate and blood
tates stress management. pressure responses during stressful laboratory tests in
As discussed above, early adversity and stress that adolescents (Boyce & Chesterman, 1990). Norris and
is well bufered by parents not only contributes to the colleagues have also reported better psychiatric out-
development of a well-regulated stress-response sys- comes among adult survivors of natural disaster who
tem but also to a developing sense of mastery and self- had experienced similarly traumatic events earlier in
eicacy in the child; these, in turn, promote positive their lives (Basoglu et al., 1997; Knight et al., 2000).
adaptation to subsequent adversity and stress. In other he idea that manageable and controlled doses of
words, not all stress and adversity is associated with a stress can have “steeling” or inoculating efects has
negative impact on development. Instead, early expos- been incorporated into a number of clinical thera-
ure to controlled stress may enhance mastery and cop- peutic approaches both for the treatment of and the
ing skills later in development (Southwick et al., 2008; prevention of trauma efects. Wells et al. (1986) dem-
Dienstbier & Zillig, 2009). As noted above, controlled onstrated that preoperative stress inoculation was
exposure to stress as a means to enhance future adap- associated with less postoperative pain and anxiety, as
tation and allostasis capacities is oten referred to as has been the inding now in a number of perioperative
stress inoculation. For example, in studies of squirrel educational programs for children and families (Kain
monkeys, Parker et al. (2004) found that brief intermit- et al., 1996, 1998, 2007).
tent maternal separation in postnatal weeks 17–27 led While the literature speciically linking manageable
to diminished anxiety responses on subsequent expos- stressful experiences to increased mastery in children
ure to novel environments. “Stress-inoculated” mon- is still emerging, there are more indings regarding
keys were also found to have lower basal plasma ACTH the factors associated with a well-developed sense of
and cortisol, as well as lower stress-induced cortisol. eicacy and mastery in children and adults (Bandura
In follow-up at 18 months of age, these same mon- et al., 2001; hompson et al., 2004; Jones & Prinz, 2005;
keys performed better on a response-inhibition task Davis-Kean et al., 2008). Notably these include, among
than their age-matched, non-stress-inoculated peers others, supportive caregiving environments that pro-
(Parker et al., 2005). vide encouragement to meet age-appropriate chal-
Similarly, as also addressed above, rodents reared lenges, support during times of failure and stress, and
in a nurturing environment have been found to dem- opportunities to practice and subsequently succeed. As
onstrate enhanced tolerance to stress in adulthood children grow up, a similarly supportive community
(reviewed by Lyons & Parker, 2007). Rat pups that (e.g., teachers, coaches, counselors) and peer networks
received 15 minutes of handling per day during the irst provide key contexts for developing and enhancing
three weeks of life were less reactive to stress and less self-eicacy. Hence, as with the promotion of a range of
fearful in novel environments as adults than the adults capacities central to positive adaptation to stress, par-
that had not been handled as pups (Ladd et al., 2005). ental care and social networks are key.
hey also had reduced ACTH and corticosterone
responses to stress and demonstrated a more rapid Intervention approaches that enhance
return of corticosterone levels to baseline ater expos-
ure to stress. Further, cross-fostering studies, also cited capacities for healthy allostasis
above, suggest that even ater stress-induced neuro- We suggest that, while there are few to no explicit
biological and behavioral alterations have occurred it “resilience promoting” interventions for children,
may be possible to modify these alterations by subse- interventions that seek to enhance parents’ abil-
quent supportive, maternal caregiving and/or pharma- ity to care appropriately for their children are in and
cological interventions (Caldji et al., 1998; Kuhn & of themselves resilience-enhancing interventions
Schanberg, 1998). through their efects on children’s stress modulation

312
Chapter 21: Childhood resilience

or capacities for healthy allostasis. Interventions that have argued that one key element is the length of
promote children’s mastery of challenges or enhance time between the irst and second pregnancies by the
emotional regulatory skills also have efects on capaci- mothers participating in the home visitation program
ties for responding to adversity and are, thus, likely to (Olds et al., 1999; Eckenrode et al., 2000). On average,
promote positive adaptation to challenge. his section the time to the second pregnancy was more than 60
of the chapter discusses intervention areas, with spe- months in the experimental group that participated in
ciic examples of approaches that may be considered the home visitation program and less than 40 months
to have a direct impact on children’s ability to respond in the comparison group. With a delay of subsequent
adaptively to stress and challenge – and hence increase pregnancy, there is more time for the mother to attend
their likelihood of reduced allostatic load and more to her developing infant and less family disruption
resilient adaptation. hese interventions are divided and stress, which can oten accompany the rapid birth
into those that work directly with parents in an efort of a second child. Over the three separate randomized
to diminish parental stress, and thus child stress, and control trials with diferent populations and contexts,
those that focus on the child’s broader environment, there was less abuse and improved infant emotional
schools, and community – speciically interventions and language development in the intervention group
that focus on mastery, self-eicacy, and efective use of (Olds, 2006).
social relationships. he division of the interventions Another model program that also began prenatally
into those for parents and those taking place in com- and continued through to the child’s second birth-
munities and schools moves from the more proximal day is Minding the Baby (Slade et al., 2005), a model
approaches that impact the child’s day to day caregiv- that integrates advanced practice nursing and mental
ing to eforts that ofer children more role models and healthcare in home visits for irst time at-risk parents
a broader community of caring adults, and that pro- and builds on the principles from the Olds model as
mote life skills (e.g., mastery and self-eicacy skills) to well as the attachment-based approaches of Heinicke
strengthen, both directly and indirectly, children’s abil- and colleagues (2006). he Minding the Baby pro-
ity to respond to stress and challenge. gram aims to enhance the capacity of a young par-
ent to understand her infant’s mental and emotional
needs as well as her own needs as a parent. Based in
Interventions with parents attachment theory and the notion of parental relect-
A comprehensive review of the range of parenting ive functioning (Fonagy & Target, 1997), the program
interventions reported in the clinical and research lit- is directed speciically toward young parents living in
erature is beyond the scope of this chapter. he discus- communities afected by the many stressors associ-
sion here focuses on two intervention models that have ated with poverty. he intervention approach helps
demonstrated efective outcomes and target infancy mothers to keep both the physical and emotional
and early childhood. Since the mid 1980s, several needs of their infants in mind as they provide daily
intervention trials have suggested that well-designed care. he approach acknowledges the parent’s own
intervention programs may have signiicant efects on emotional and cognitive needs as a parent and expli-
both children’s health and their psychological adapta- cit attention is given to the parent’s own mental health
tion, particularly in the areas of emotional regulation needs. Early indings suggest that this very intensive
and stress reactivity (Olds et al., 2007). For example, relationship-focused approach may be particularly
perhaps the most well-studied set of early interven- beneicial for families where chronic stress and early
tions included home visits by nurses that began pre- adversity precludes response to less-intensive inter-
natally and continued for 30 months ater birth. his ventions, and that this model may have a signiicant
has shown a number of positive outcomes as late as 15 impact on both parental and child emotional regula-
years of age (Olds et al., 1997, 1998, 1999). Outcomes tory abilities (Olds et al., 2007).
include a reduction in the number of subsequent In sum, indings from selective early intervention
pregnancies, use of welfare, child abuse and neglect, programs suggest that these programs are likely to
and criminal behavior on the part of low-income, reduce a variety of maladaptive outcomes later in devel-
unmarried mothers 15 years ater the birth of the irst opment, as well as improving proximal infant outcomes
child. Although the mechanism by which these efects such as language/emotion development and enhanced
are achieved remains in doubt, Olds and colleagues attachment security. Less clear is the impact of these

313
Section 5: Training for resilience

early interventions on the later rates of depression and Emotional literacy


anxiety disorders as the children reach adolescence. Learning to regulate emotions is an essential skill that
What seems clear from the range of early intervention dramatically impacts one’s ability to manage stress and
programs for parents and infants is that starting early, trauma. Emotions are oten associated with high levels
even prenatally, is most efective. Further, the efective- of physiological and psychological arousal. When faced
ness of the intervention may be facilitated by target- with a challenge or stressor, the child who underreacts
ing not just parental education or child care practices emotionally may allocate too few internal and exter-
but the developing relationship between parent and nal resources to solve the problem. On the other hand,
child, and even more speciically those stressors and the child who overreacts emotionally may be physiolo-
challenges that come from parenting children and that gically and psychologically looded or overwhelmed,
may impede efective parenting, including responding disrupting his or her ability to process information and
efectively to a child’s distress. make good decisions.
he ability to regulate emotions derives from a
Interventions in schools and community complex interaction of genetics, development, and
Interventions based in schools and communities environment. A number of school-based programs
can have a critical impact on children, particularly have been developed to help children to improve what
those living in poverty and/or with parental discord. some have termed “emotional literacy.” For example,
Also, as children’s peer networks broaden, there are Maurer and colleagues (2004) have designed an emo-
greater opportunities for learning from others and tional literacy program for middle school children that
for learning mastery and emotional regulation skills. teaches students to recognize and evaluate their own
he focus here is on selected examples of school- and and others’ thoughts, feelings, and actions. his class-
community-based interventions that are organized room-based program, which is recommended for ith
around facilitating mastery and improved coping and through eighth grade, conforms to the guidelines of
emotional regulatory skills. the Collaborative for Academic, Social and Emotional
Several decades of research show strong evidence Learning (2002) and focuses on social and emotional
for the role of schools and school-related programs learning as well as academics. One of its goals is to
(e.g., sports, community programs for students) in pro- teach children to cope more adaptively with modern-
moting mastery and self-eicacy in children and youth day stresses, and, in the process, to decrease behaviors
(Masten et al., 1990; Wang & Gordon, 1994; Rutter & that can be destructive.
Maughan, 2002; Condly, 2006; Luthar, 2006). Research he program has six steps: introducing feeling
on how schools efectively promote childhood mastery words, designs and personiied explanations, real
and eicacy parallels studies in families, where posi- world association, personal/family association, class-
tive efects are linked to stable and nurturing relation- room discussions, and creative writing assignments.
ships, a supportive climate in the face of challenge, high hese steps are intended to help students to learn to
expectations, and an expectable structure with consist- control their emotions by understanding the relation-
ent rules (Masten, 2006). Schools present many poten- ship between thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, thus
tially positive opportunities for children to experience increasing self- and social awareness and learning to
challenge and master failure as well as to succeed, learn choose emotional and behavioral responses that are
from role models, and beneit from mentors and sup- adaptive rather than destructive. An important goal of
portive adult relationships. Further, for at-risk stu- the program is to teach students to express their feel-
dents, schools ofer a relatively asset-rich opportunity ings more freely, through writing and through conver-
compared with their homes or the communities in sation, in an environment that is challenging yet caring
general. hese include ater-school programs, libraries, and supportive. Another goal is to promote school,
sports, recreational activities in the community, and family, and community partnerships.
opportunities to explore talents in, for example, music, A second example of a program for strengthening
athletics, and writing. Speciic examples of school- and social and emotional competence in young children
community-based programs that particularly facilitate is the Incredible Years Teacher program for teach-
developing positive self-eicacy and mastery in chil- ers and the accompanying Child Training Programs
dren follow. (Webster-Stratton et al., 2008). he program was

314
Chapter 21: Childhood resilience

originally developed to treat children aged three statistically and clinically signiicant improvement
to seven years with early-onset conduct problems in non-compliant and aggressive behaviors as well as
and oppositional deiant disorder, but it has been in social problem-solving strategies compared with
expanded to be used as a preventive intervention children in the control group. At one year follow-up,
by school teachers for young students. Its goal is to most of the improvements were maintained. In a 30-
strengthen the capacity of young children to man- lesson school-based study involving 120 classrooms,
age their emotions and behaviors and to develop the Webster-Stratton and colleagues (2008) randomly
social skills needed to make meaningful relationships. assigned schools to intervention or control conditions.
hese skills are seen as critical for success in cognitive Results indicated that the intervention had a robust
and academic preparedness, success in school, and efect on changing teachers’ approaches to classroom
for dealing with stress, particularly for children who management and resulted in signiicantly fewer crit-
are exposed to multiple life stressors. ical statements by teachers to students. Compared
he intervention involves four days of training with children in the control school group, those in the
workshops where teachers learn skills related to class- intervention group showed signiicant improvements
room management strategies (e.g., methods to develop in conduct problems, social competence, and emotion
positive relationships with students and parents, regulation. he intervention had a particularly large
efective use of encouragement and praise, incentive efect in schools with high conduct problems and very
programs targeted to speciic prosocial skills, and dis- low levels of school readiness. Both teachers and par-
cipline plans when needed), promoting self-regulation ents in the intervention schools reported being very
of emotions in students, teaching class members how satisied with the program.
to solve problems, and promoting social competence
among students. In addition to teacher training the Team sports
intervention follows the Dina Dinosaur Social Skills In addition to its well-known positive efects on phys-
and Problem Solving Curriculum for children, which ical health and development, regular physical activity,
includes the following seven units: learning school and in some cases participation in team sports, has
rules, how to be successful in school, emotional liter- also been associated with psychological health. Studies
acy including perspective taking and empathy, inter- have found a positive relationship between regular
personal problem solving, anger management, social physical activity and self-esteem, perceptions of com-
skills, and communication skills. Lessons are typically petence, life satisfaction, and well-being (Steptoe &
delivered in a large group circle of students, two to Butler, 1996; Fox, 2000; Valois et al., 2004, Pedersen &
three times each week in 15–20 minute time periods, Seidman 2004; Bailey, 2006). For example, in a study
followed by practice activities of approximately 20 of 247 urban adolescent girls, Pedersen and Seidman
minutes in a smaller group setting. Teachers also pro- (2004) found that achievement in team sport during
mote skill development throughout the course of the early adolescence was positively associated with self-
day during meals, recess, and so on. Parents are regu- esteem in middle adolescence. In addition, reduced
larly informed about the concepts and skills that are depression, anger, anxiety, and stress have been asso-
being taught to the children and are asked to help their ciated with regular physical activity (Hassman et al.,
child to complete Dina Dinosaur home activity books. 2000; Valois et al., 2004). Sports programs, with their
Randomized controlled studies (Webster-Stratton focus on strengths and resilience, have even been
& Hammond, 1997, Webster-Stratton & Reid 2004; employed as psychosocial interventions in countries
Webster-Stratton et al., 2004, 2008) of the Incredible that have sufered from disasters and wars (Colliard,
Years Child Training Program have been conducted in 2005; Heninger & Meuwly, 2005).
both clinic-based and school-based populations. For In addition to increasing physical toughness, team
example, in one clinic-based study (Webster-Stratton sports can build heart and enhance mental toughness
et al., 2001), chidren aged four to eight years and with (Bell & Suggs, 1998). To be a successful team member,
early-onset conduct problems were randomly assigned the young athlete must master a set of physical and
to receive training using the Incredible Years Dinosaur emotional skills, endure challenging and unpleasant
Social Skills and Problem Solving Curriculum or training sessions, learn to control negative emotions
to be in a waitlist control group. Ater treatment, and cooperate during times of high stress, learn to “go
children in the intervention group demonstrated the extra mile,” sacriice personal goals for team goals,

315
Section 5: Training for resilience

and learn to face and then tolerate the stress of com- adolescents. Sport can be detrimental when it excludes
petition. his requires dedication, hard work, and some youth or groups of youth, when it focuses on
discipline (Bell & Suggs, 1998; Toler & Butterbaugh, excessive negative feedback and criticism, and when
2005). expectations are inappropriate or excessive, as may be
Team sports provide a powerful venue for learning the case with unrealistic outcome-driven parenting and
lessons in ethical and moral behavior under condi- coaching (Toler & Butterbaugh, 2005, Bailey, 2006).
tions of high arousal and stress. To be fully successful In some cases, sport can also exploit youth (Toler &
in team sports, children must learn to maintain eth- Butterbaugh, 2005).
ical standards in the context of hard work, determin-
ation, frustration, anger, and the drive to win (Toler Outdoor education programs
& Butterbaugh, 2005). hey must grapple with numer- Outdoor education/adventure programs may also have
ous moral issues such as fair play, sportsmanship, self- a role in enhancing resilience. hese programs typically
aggrandizing behavior, integrity, and character (Stuart, are physically and emotionally challenging and empha-
2003; Toler & Butterbaugh, 2005). hey must also size the importance of character, teamwork, problem
resist the temptation to bend or exploit the rules or to solving, skill building, endurance, discipline, and cour-
win at all costs. hese lessons are oten diicult to learn, age. In some programs, altruism is also encouraged. To
particularly if parents and coaches have not themselves date, there have been hundreds of studies measuring
fully embraced a mature and ethical approach to team the eicacy of outdoor education programs and at
sports. least ive published meta-analyses. he most compre-
Physical activity and team sports each have been hensive meta-analysis, which was conducted by Hattie
associated with academic performance (Sallis et al., and colleagues (1997), included 96 studies from 1968
1999; Fox, 2000). For example, in a survey of over 4000 to 1994 with 12 057 unique participants; 72% of pro-
middle and high school students, Fox and colleagues grams lasted between 20 and 26 days. Programs were
(Fox, 2000) found that team sport participation was located in wilderness or backcountry settings where
associated with higher grade point average among both small groups (generally less than 16 participants) were
boys and girls, and that physical activity was associated taught a variety of skills and then challenged by physic-
with higher grade point average among girls but not ally and mentally demanding objectives that required
boys. here is also some evidence that increased phys- intense problem solving and decision making among
ical activity at school, even if it means reduced time for group members.
academic activities, may result in improved academic he meta-analysis carried out by Hattie et al. (1997)
performance (Shephard, 1997). evaluated the following six main outcome variables:
As noted earlier in this chapter, higher levels of leadership, self-concept, academic achievement, per-
positive social support are strongly associated with sonality, interpersonal skills, and adventuresome-
resilience. Social development, social competence, ness. Adventure programs had the greatest immediate
and the fostering of prosocial skills and behaviors are impact on a number of dimensions that have been
among the most positive efects of team sport par- associated with resilience, including assertiveness,
ticipation (Toler & Butterbaugh, 2005; Bailey, 2006). emotional stability, lexibility, conidence, self-eicacy,
Children and adolescents learn the importance of problem solving, achievement motivation, internal
communicating with peers and coaches, playing by the locus of control, and maturity. he overall efect size was
rules, cooperating and sharing, and placing the goals of small to moderate (0.34). However, for those programs
the team above their own personal goals. Team mem- that included a long-term follow-up evaluation, the
bers oten develop strong and supportive relationships overall efect size increased by an additional 0.17. his
with fellow team members and coaches. Team sports raised the overall long-term efect size to 0.51, which
also tend to foster peer respect and relationships across is generally considered to be moderate. he greatest
a broad range of ethnic, racial, and cultural groups increase was seen for self-concept, which increased
(Toler & Butterbaugh, 2005). Inclusion on a team pro- from 0.28 immediately ater the program to 0.51 at
vides an opportunity to create social networks and may long-term follow-up. In a review of outdoor educa-
create a sense of belonging (Bailey, 2004). tion meta-analytic studies, Neill (2002) suggested that
It is important to note that sport is not always the relatively large increase in self-concept over time
positive and resilience enhancing for children and might indicate a “sleeper efect,” meaning that changes

316
Chapter 21: Childhood resilience

in self-concept may begin during a program and then courageous. Next children are asked to ind examples
continue to develop ater the program. of such heroes in their own families and communi-
While it is diicult to draw irm conclusions about ties. Finally, students identify a need in their commu-
speciic mediators of success in outdoor/adventure nity and work to address it though a service-learning
programs, the available evidence suggests that out- project. In this way, they themselves become morally
comes are related to the speciic organizations running courageous heroes. As of 2007, the Girafe Project had
the programs (e.g., Outward Bound Australia appears reached more than 30 000 US students in K-12 schools
to be particularly efective), the duration of the pro- (www.girafe.org).
grams (i.e., programs greater than 20 days appear to be
more successful than programs less than 20 days), and Penn Resiliency Program
perhaps age (i.e., self-concept may change more among he Penn Resiliency Program is described in Chapter
younger compared with older adolescents). here is 20 and so is only touched upon briely here. As noted
additional evidence to suggest that educational/adven- above, optimism, particularly realistic optimism, has
ture programs have positive efects on academic per- been associated with resilience in a host of studies
formance and may be particularly efective for speciic involving numerous populations of adults and children
populations with emotional, behavioral, and psycho- (Southwick et al., 2005). he Penn Resiliency Program
logical problems (Cason & Gillis 1994; Neill, 2002), is largely based on the cognitive models of treatment
including juvenile delinquents. for depression developed by Beck and colleagues
While the construct of resilience has not speciic- (1979) and on Seligman’s learned optimism training,
ally been assessed before and ater outdoor education/ where the participant learns to monitor beliefs about
adventure programs, the available published data sug- everyday adversity and to connect those beliefs to con-
gest that a number of these programs have a positive sequences in his or her daily life. For example, in one
impact on domains associated with the capacity to deal study of middle school children, Cutuli and colleagues
with adversity. Further, unlike many education pro- (2006) used a 12-session cognitive-behavioral inter-
grams, the positive efects experienced during these vention to teach children how to challenge inaccurate
programs may trigger further positive change ater self-perceptions and perceived failures. he interven-
adolescent participants have let the program. tion was particularly helpful for young adolescents
with conduct problems.
Training in moral courage
Just as moral courage, altruism, and giving to others
have been associated with resilience in adults, they also Conclusions
appear to be associated with resilience in children. In his chapter has primarily focused on caregiving rela-
her classic study of at-risk children on the Hawaiian tionships and environments (e.g., home and school) as
island of Kauai, Emmy Werner (1992) found that chil- key to children’s response to adversity and challenge.
dren who were most likely to lead successful lives as Positive, nurturing, supportive relationships moderate
adults were those who helped others in meaningful and bufer the potentially detrimental impact of stress
ways (e.g., assisting a family member, a neighbor, or and adversity and consequently may promote enhanced
some other community member). Similarly, Zimrin mastery skills for subsequent stress challenge. When
(1986) reported that Israeli children who had been relationships promote self-eicacy and mastery, they
physically abused fared better if they assumed respon- aford children skills for responding to stress and chal-
sibility for someone else, such as a sibling or a pet. lenge that permit a more positive or “resilient” adapta-
Many parents and teachers instruct children about tion. Promoting enhanced self-eicacy and an ability to
the value of ethical and moral principles and altru- weather adversity, even to ind positive opportunity in
ism. One example of a structured approach to teaching negative circumstances, requires also that children be
moral courage is the Girafe Project, whose motto is protected from chronic, toxic stress. Environmental/
“moving people to stick their necks out for the com- caregiving conditions are critical for establishing “set-
mon good.” In a classroom setting, students are intro- points” for stress-system activation and also for the allo-
duced to the stories of “heros” who have been morally static ability of the system – and the body – to return to
courageous. hese stories are discussed in detail so baseline ater a stressful condition has resolved or the
that children can learn what it means to be morally child is no longer in the situation. he concept of stress

317
Section 5: Training for resilience

inoculation requires far more careful study in humans heory, research, and clinical implications (pp. 89–111).
as to how stress and challenge can enhance children’s New York: Guilford Press.
self-eicacy and also prepare the stress-response sys- Brown, G. W., Bifulco, A., & Harris, T. O. (1987). Life events,
tem for future adversity. hat said, a number of inter- vulnerability and onset of depression: some reinements.
ventions ranging from speciic eforts to enhance British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 30–42.
parental abilities to working with children in schools Caldji, C., Tannenbaum, B., Sharma, S., et al. (1998).
and communities are examples of eforts that impli- Maternal care during infancy regulates the development
of neural systerns mediating the expression of fearfulness
citly, and sometimes explicitly, promote mastery and
in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
stress-weathering abilities in children. In the future, of the USA, 95, 5335–5340.
it will be important to consider more explicitly how
Caldji, C., Francis, D., Sharma, S., Plotsky, P. M., &
interventions and educational programs for children Meaney, M. J. (2000). he efects of early rearing
can be designed to target key capacities for positive environment on the development of GABAA and central
adaptation to and mastery of challenge and adversity. benzodiazepine receptor levels and novelty-induced
fearfulness in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22,
219–229.
References Cason, D. & Gillis, H. L. (1994). A meta-analysis of outdoor
Ambelas, A. (1990). Life events and the onset of mania. adventure programming with adolescents. Journal of
British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 450–451. Experiential Education, 17, 40–47.
Bailey, R. (2004). Evaluating the relationship between Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Handbook of attachment.
physical education, sport and social inclusion. Education New York: Guilford Press.
Reviews, 56, 71–90. Collaborative for Academic, Social and Emotional Learning
Bailey, R. (2006). Physical education and sport in schools: a (2002). Guidelines for social and emotional learning:
review of beneits and outcomes. Journal of School Health, Quality programs for school and life success (pp. 1–4).
76, 397–401. Chicago, IL: Collaborative for Academic, Social and
Emotional Learning, http://www.casel.org/downloads/
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Vittorio Caprara, G., & Safe%20and%20Sound/2A_Guidelines.pdf (accessed
Pastorelli, C. (2001). Self-eicacy beliefs as shapers February 28, 2011.)
of children’s aspirations and career trajectories. Child
Development, 72, 187–206. Colliard, C. (2005). Mission Report. Evaulation of the Tdh
psychosocial programme of recreational centres in Bam,
Basoglu, M., Mineka, S., Paker, M., et al. (1997). Iran. Geneva: Center for Humanitarian Psychology.
Psychological preparedness for trauma as a protective
factor in survivors of torture. Psychological Medicine, Condly, S. J. (2006). Resilience in children: A review
27,1421–1433. of literature with implications for educators. Urban
Education, 41, 211–236.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979).
Cutuli, J. J., Chaplin, T. M., Gillham, J. E., Reivich, K. J.,
Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford
& Seligman, M. E. (2006). Preventing co-occurring
Press.
depression symptoms in adolescents with conduct
Bell, C. C. & Suggs, H. (1998). Using sports to strengthen problems: the Penn Resiliency Program. Annals of the
resiliency in children: Training “Heart”. Child and New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 282–286.
Adolescent Psychology Clinics, 7, 859–865.
Davis-Kean, P. E., Huesmann, L., Jager, J., et al. (2008).
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. Changes in the relation of self-eicacy beliefs and
London: Hogarth Press. behaviors across development. Child Development, 79,
Boyce, W. T. & Chesterman, E. (1990). Life events, social 1257–1269.
support, and cardiovascular reactivity in adolescence. Denenberg, V. H., Rosenberg, K. M., Paschke, R., & Zarrow,
Journal of Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, 11, M. X. (1969). Mice reared with rat aunts: efects on plasma
105–111. corticosterone and open ield activity. Nature, 221, 73–74.
Brady, K. T. & Sinha, R. (2005). Co-occurring mental and Dienstbier, R. A. & Zillig, L. M. P. (2009). Toughness. In S. J.
substance use disorders: the neurobiological efects Lopez & C. R. Snyder (eds.), Oxford handbook of positive
of chronic stress. American Journal of Psychiatry, psychology, 2nd edn (pp. 537–548). New York: Oxford
162,1483–1493. University Press.
Bretherton, I. & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal working Eckenrode, J., Ganzel, B., Henderson, C. R. Jr., et al. (2000).
models in attachment relations: A construct revisited. In Preventing child abuse and neglect with a program of
J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (eds.), Handbook of attachment: nurse home visitation: the limiting efects of domestic

318
Chapter 21: Childhood resilience

violence. Journal of the American Medical Association, Kain, Z. N., Mayes, L. C., and Caramico, L. A. (1996).
284, 1385–1391. Preoperative preparation in children: A cross-sectional
Fahlke, C., Lorenz, J. G., Long, J., et al. (2000). Rearing study. Journal of Clinical Anesthesia, 8, 508–514.
experiences and stress-induced plasma cortisol as Kain, Z. N., Mayes, L. C., Caramico, L. A., et al. (1998).
early risk factors for excessive alcohol consumption Preoperative preparation programs in children: a
in nonhuman primates. Alcoholism: Clinical and comparative examination. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 87,
Experimental Research, 24, 644–650. 1249–1255.
Fonagy, P. & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and relective Kain, Z. N., Caldwell-Andrews, A. A., Mayes, L. C., et al.
function: their role in self-organization. Development& (2007). Family-centered preparation for surgery
Psychopathology, 9, 679–700. improves perioperative outcomes in children: a
Fox, K. (2000). he efects of exercise on self-perceptions randomized controlled trial. Anesthesiology, 106, 65–74.
and self-esteem. In S. Biddle, K. Fox, & S. Boutcher Kaufman, J., Plotsky, P. M., Nemerof, C. B., & Charney, D. S.
(eds.), Physical activity and psychological well-being (2000). Efects of early adverse experiences on brain
(pp. 88–117). London: Routledge. structure and function: Clinical implications. Biological
Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Psychiatry, 48, 778–790.
Nongenomic transmission across generations of Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (2002).
maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Toward a comprehensive developmental model for major
Science, 286, 1155–1158. depression in women. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Gunnar, M. R. (2000). Early adversity and the development 159, 1133–1145.
of stress reactivity and regulation. In C. A. Nelson (ed.), Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. (2006).
he Minnesota symposia on child psychology, Vol. 31: he Toward a comprehensive developmental model for major
efects of early adversity on neurobehavioral development depression in men. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163,
(pp. 163–200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 115–124.
Gunnar, M. & Vazquez, D. M. (2006). Stress neurobiology Knight, B. G., Gatz, M., Heller, K., & Bengtson, V. L.
and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & (2000). Age and emotional response to the Northridge
D. Cohen (eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. earthquake: A longitudinal analysis. Psychology and
2: Developmental neuroscience, 2nd edn (pp. 533–577). Aging, 15, 627–634.
New York: Wiley. Kuhn, C. M. & Schanberg, S. M. (1998). Responses to
Harlow, H. F. (1963). he maternal afectional system of maternal separation: Mechanisms and mediators.
rhesus monkeys. In H. L. Rheingold (ed.), Maternal International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16,
behavior in mammals (pp. 254–281). New York: Wiley. 261–270.
Hassman, P., Koivula, N., & Uutela, A. (2000). Physical Ladd, C. O., Huot, R. L., hrivikraman, K. V., et al. (2000).
exercise and psychological well-being: a population Long-term behavioral and neuroendocrine adaptations
study in Finland. Preventive Medicine, 30, 17–25. to adverse early experience. Progress in Brain Research,
Hattie, J., Marsh, H. W., Neill, J. T., & Richards, G. E. (1997). 122, 81–103.
Adventure education and outward bound: Out-of-class Ladd, C. O., Huot, R. L., hrivikraman, K. V., Nemerof, C. B.,
experiences that make a lasting diference. Reviews of & Plotsky, P. M. (2004). Long-term adaptations in
Educational Research, 67, 43–87. glucocorticoid receptor and mineralocorticoid receptor
Heinicke, C. M., Goorsky, M., Levine, M., et al. (2006). mRNA and negative feedback on the hypothalamo-
Pre- and postnatal antecedents of a home-visiting pituitary-adrenal axis following neonatal maternal
intervention and family developmental outcome. Infant separation. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 367–375.
Mental Health Journal, 27, 91–119. Ladd, C. O., Huot, R. L., hrivikraman, K. V., & Plotsky, P. M.
Heninger, J.-P. & Meuwly, M. (2005). Movement, games and (2005). Diferential neuroendocrine responses to
sports: Developing coaching methods and practices for chronic variable stress in adult Long Evans rats
vulnerable children in the Southern hemisphere. Lausanne: exposed to handling–maternal separation as neonates.
Foundation Terre des Hommes. Psychoneuroendocrinrology, 30, 520–533.
Higley, J. D., Suomi, S. J., & Linnoila, M. (1992). A Levine, S. (ed.) (1975). Psychosocial factors in growth and
longitudinal assessment of CSF monoamine metabolite development. London: Oxford University Press.
and plasma cortisol concentrations in young rhesus Levine, S. (2005). Developmental determinants of sensitivity
monkeys. Biological Psychiatry, 32, 127–145. and resistance to stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30,
Jones, T. L. & Prinz, R. J. (2005). Potential roles of parental 939–946.
self-eicacy in parent and child adjustment: A review. Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., et al. (1997). Maternal
Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 341–363. care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and

319
Section 5: Training for resilience

hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses to stress. Olds, D. L., Eckenrode, J., Henderson, C. R. Jr., et al. (1997).
Science, 277, 1659–1662. Long-term efects of home visitation on maternal
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Day, J. C., Francis, D. D., & Meaney, M. J. life course and child abuse and neglect. Fiteen-year
(2000). Maternal care, hippocampal synaptogenesis and follow-up of a randomized trial. Journal of the American
cognitive development in rats. Nature Neuroscience, 3, Medical Association, 278, 637–643.
799–806. Olds, D., Henderson, C. R. Jr., Cole, R., et al. (1998). Long-
Loman, M. & Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Early experience and term efects of nurse home visitation on children’s
the development of stress reactivity and regulation in criminal and antisocial behavior: 15-year follow-up of
children. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American
867–876. Medical Association, 280, 1238–1244.
Luthar, S. S. (2006). Resilience in development: A synthesis Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R. Jr., Kitzman, H. J., et al. (1999).
of research across ive decades. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Prenatal and infancy home visitation by nurses: recent
Cohen (eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 3: indings. Future Child, 9, 44–65, 190–191.
Risk, disorder, and adaptation, 2nd edn (pp. 739–795). Olds, D. L., Sadler, L., & Kitzman, H. (2007). Programs
New York: Wiley. for parents of infants and toddlers: Recent evidence
Luthar, S. S. & Cicchetti, D. (2000). he construct of from randomized trials. Journal of Child Psychology and
resilience: Implications for interventions and social Psychiatry, 48, 355–391.
policies. Developmental Psychopathology, 12, 857–885. Parker, K. J., Buckmaster, C. L., Schatzberg, A. F., &
Lyons, D. M. & Parker, K. J. (2007). Stress inoculation- Lyons, D. M. (2004). Prospective investigation of stress
induced indications of resilience in monkeys. Journal of inoculation in young monkeys. Archives of General
Traumatic Stress 20, 423–433. Psychiatry, 61, 933–941.
Masten, A. S. (2006). Promoting resilience in development: Parker, K. J., Buckmaster, C. L., Justus, K. R., Schatzberg,
A general framework for systems of care. In R. J. Flynn, A. F., & Lyons, D. M. (2005). Mild early life stress
P. Dudding, & J. G. Barber (eds.), Promoting resilience in enhances prefrontal-dependent response inhibition in
child welfare (pp. 3–17). Ottawa: University of Ottawa monkeys. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 848–855.
Press. Pedersen, S. & Seidman, E. (2004). Team sports achievement
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and self-esteem development among urban adolescent
and development: Contributions from the study of girls. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28, 412–422.
children who overcome adversity. Development and Plotsky, P. M. & Meaney, M. J. (1993). Early, postnatal
Psychopathology, 2, 425–444. experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing
Maurer, M., Brackett, M. A., & Plain, F. (2004). Emotional factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content
literacy in the middle school: A 6-step program to promote and stress-induced release in adult rats. Brain Research,
social, emotional and academic learning. Port Chester, Molecular Brain Research, 18, 195–200.
NY: Dude. Rilling, J. K., Winslow, J. T., O’Brien, D., et al. (2001). Neural
McEwen, B. S. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: correlates of maternal separation in rhesus monkeys.
implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 146–157.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 108–124. Rutter, M. & Maughan, B. (2002). School efectiveness
Miller, L., Kramer, R., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., & indings, 1979–2002. Journal of School Psychology, 40,
Weissman, M. (1997). Intergenerational transmission 451–475.
of parental bonding among women. Journal of the Sallis, J. F., McKenzie, T. L., Kolody, B., et al. (1999). Efects
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, of health-related physical education on academic
36, 1134–1139. achievement: project SPARK. Research Quarterly in
Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M. R., Mangelsdorf, S., Parritz, R., Exercise and Sport, 70, 127–134.
& Buss, K. A. (1996). Behavioral inhibition and stress Shephard, R. J. (1997). Curricular physical activity and
reactivity: Moderating role of attachment security. Child academic performance. Pediatric Exercise Science, 9,
Development, 67, 508–522. 113–126.
Neill, J. T. ( 2002). Meta-analytic research on the outcomes Shonkof, J. P. & Phillips, D. (eds.) (2000). From neurons
of outdoor education. In Proceedings of the 6th Biennial to neighborhoods: he science of early childhood
Coalition for Education in the Outdoors Research development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press
Symposium, Bradford Woods. for the Committee on Integrating the Science of Early
Olds, D. L. (2006). he nurse-family partnership: an Childhood Development.
evidence based preventative intervention. Infant Mental Slade, A., Sadler, L. S., & Mayes, L. C. (2005). Minding the
Health Journal, 27, 5–25. baby: Enhancing parental relective functioning in a

320
Chapter 21: Childhood resilience

nursing/mental health home visiting program. In Suomi, S. J., Mineka, S., & DeLizio, R. D. (1983). Short-
L. J. Berlin, Y. Ziv, L. Amaya-Jackson, & M. T. Greenberg and long-term efects of repetitive mother-infant
(eds.), Enhancing early attachments: heory, research, separations on social development in rhesus monkeys.
intervention, and policy (pp. 152–177). New York: Developmental Psychology, 19, 770–786.
Guilford Press. Toler, I. R. & Butterbaugh, G. J. (2005). Developmental
Southwick, S. M., Bremner, J. D., Rasmusson, A., et al. overview of child and youth sports for the twenty-irst
(1999). Role of norepinephrine in the pathophysiology century. Clinical Sports Medicine, 24, 783–804.
and treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological
hompson, A. H., Barnsley, R. H., & Battle, J. (2004). he
Psychiatry, 46, 1192–1204.
relative age efect and the development of self-esteem.
Southwick, S. M., Vythilingam, M., & Charney, D. S. (2005). Educational Research, 46, 313–320.
he psychobiology of depression and resilience to stress:
Valois, R. F., Zullig, K. J., Huebner, E. S., & Drane, J. W.
Implications for prevention and treatment. Annual
(2004). Physical activity behaviors and perceived life
Reviews of Clinical Psychology, 1, 255–291.
satisfaction among public high school adolescents.
Southwick, S. M., Ozbay, F., & Mayes, L. C. (2008). Journal of School Health, 74, 59–65.
Psychological and biological factors associated with
Wang, M. C. & Gordon, E. W. (1994). Educational resilience
resilience to stress and trauma. In H. Parens, H. P. Blum, &
in inner-city America: Challenges and prospects. Hillsdale,
S. Akhtar (eds.), he unbroken soul: Tragedy, trauma, and
NJ: Erlbaum.
resilience (pp. 130–151). Lanham, MD: Jason Aronson.
Sroufe, L. A. ( 2005). Attachment and development: Weaver, I. C., Diorio, J., Seckl, J. R., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M. J.
a prospective, longitudinal study from birth to (2004). Early environmental regulation of
adulthood. Attachment and Human Development, 7, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression:
349–367. characterization of intracellular mediators and potential
genomic target sites. Annals of the New York Academy of
Sroufe, L. A., Carlson, E. A., Levy, A. K., & Egeland, B. Sciences, 1024, 182–212.
(1999). Implications of attachment theory for
developmental psychopathology. Developmental Webster-Stratton, C. & Hammond, M. (1997). Treating
Psychopathology, 11, 1–13. children with early onset conduct problems: A
comparison of child and parent training interventions.
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65,
A. (2005). Placing early attachment experiences in 93–109.
developmental context: he Minnesota Longitudinal
Study. In K. E. Grossmann, K. Grossmann, & E. Waters Webster-Stratton, C. & Reid, J. M. (2004). Strengthening
(eds.), Attachment from infancy to adulthood: he major social and emotional competence in young children,
longitudinal studies (pp. 48–70). New York: Guilford he Foundation for Early School Readiness and
Press. Success: Incredible years classroom social skills
problem-solving curriculum: Infants and Young
Stacey, M. (1970). Hospitals, children, and their families: Children, 17, 96–113.
report of a pilot study. London: Routledge & Paul.
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M. J., & Hammond, M.
Steptoe, A. & Butler, N. (1996). Sports participation (2001). Social skills and problem solving training for
and emotional wellbeing in adolescents. Lancet, 347, children with early-onset conduct problems: Who
1789–1792. beneits? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42,
Stuart, M. E. (2003). Moral issues in sport: the child’s 943–952.
perspective. Research Quarterly in Exercise and Sport, 74,
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M. J., & Hammond, M. (2004).
445–454.
Treating children with early-onset conduct problems:
Suomi, S. J. (1995). Inluence of Bowlby’s attachment theory Intervention outcomes for parent, child, and teacher
on research on non-human primate biobehavioral training. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
development. In S. Goldberg, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (eds.), Psychology, 33, 105–124.
Attachment theory: social, developmental, and clinical
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M. J., & Stoolmiller, M.
perspectives (pp. 185–201). Hillside, NJ: Analytic.
(2008). Preventing conduct problems and improving
Suomi, S. J. (1997). Early determinants of behaviour: school readiness: Evaluation of the incredible years
evidence from primate studies. British Medical Bulletin, teacher and child training programs in high-risk
53, 170–184. schools. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49,
Suomi, S. J., Delizio, R., & Harlow, H. F. (1976). Social 471–488.
rehabilitation of separation-induced depressive Wells, J. K., Howard, G. S., Nowlin, W. F., & Vargas, M. J.
disorders in monkeys. American Journal of Psychiatry, (1986). Presurgical anxiety and postsurgical pain and
133, 1279–1285. adjustment: Efects of a stress inoculation procedure.

321
Section 5: Training for resilience

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, Werner, E. E. (1997). Vulnerable but invincible: high-risk
831–835. children from birth to adulthood. Acta Paediatrica
Werner, E. E. (1992). he children of Kauai: Resiliency Supplement, 422, 103–105.
and recovery in adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Zimrin, H. (1986). A proile of survival. Child Abuse and
Adolescent Health, 13, 262–268. Neglect, 10, 339–349.

322
Section
Section5 Training for resilience
Chapter
Military mental health training: building

22 resilience
Carl Andrew Castro and Amy B. Adler

For learning to take place with any kind of eiciency Training. To be sure, the task confronting us was by no
students must be motivated. To be motivated, they means an easy one. Imagine, a brigade combat team
must become interested. And they become inter- comprising over 3500 soldiers getting ready to deploy
ested when they are actively working on projects to combat for over a year. You have one hour. What
which they can relate to their values and goals in are you going to tell them about the psychological
life. demands of combat in order to prepare and sustain
Gus Tuberville, President, Penn College them for that year? hat was our task.
Prior to Battlemind Training, none of the services
in the US military had developed an empirically based
Introduction mental health training program for preparing service
here is no debate that combat places tremendous psy- members for the psychological demands of combat.
chological and physical demands on those involved. Of course, some service members did receive men-
And as we learn more about how combat afects the tal health training prior to deploying, but this mental
psychological well-being of those involved, a set of health training was generic and non-speciic to the
central questions emerge. What can we do to prepare actual demands of the combat environment, and it
service members for the psychological demands of was not conducted in a systematic manner (McKibben
combat? What can we do to sustain the mental health et al., 2009). Typically, the mental health training con-
and well-being of those deployed in a combat envir- sisted of “home-grown” PowerPoint presentations con-
onment? What can we do to facilitate the return of taining theoretical information on stress that had little
these service members from the combat environment or questionable value to service members getting ready
to home? In short, what do service members need to to go into combat. For example, these home-grown
know about how combat can afect them? mental training eforts typically included a discussion
In response to these questions, the US Army of the Yerkes–Dodson model of stress, along with a
developed the Battlemind Training System, a mental discussion of the distinction between eustress, astress,
health resilience building program (US Army Medical and distress, material of interest perhaps to irst-year
Command, 2007). his system established several fun- graduate students but not necessarily interesting to sol-
damental principles of mental health training, iden- diers about to deploy to Iraq or Afghanistan.
tiied key implementation principles, and deined Even more remarkable, soldiers and marines
several important terms. hroughout this chapter, the would receive exactly the same PowerPoint presen-
Battlemind Training System will be used as an exem- tations about “combat stress” that they had received
plar to highlight how a military mental health training prior to deploying upon returning home from a year-
program can be created that employs theses principles long deployment. hat is, mental health training was
of mental health training and implementation. not tailored to where the service members were in
Before beginning a discussion of these key prin- the deployment cycle. It is reasonable to assume that
ciples and terminology, it will be useful to provide the psychological needs of service members would
an historical perspective on the status of mental be diferent upon returning home from combat than
health training in the US military prior to Battlemind when going into combat. While this assumption seems

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

323
Section 5: Training for resilience

obvious now, it was not until the development of the Once Saint let his post upon retirement, the
Battlemind Training System that the content was keyed Battlemind concept languished and, therefore, initially
to the deployment cycle and the concept was acted did not continue to have an impact on how the US Army
upon. Indeed, subsequent mental health training pro- prepared for combat. In 2005, the Battlemind Training
grams developed by both the US Marines and US Air guide was “re-discovered” and re-shaped as a mental
Force are now also linked to the phase of the deploy- health training program for soldiers and junior lead-
ment cycle (e.g., the Marine Corps’ Warrior Resilience ers (e.g., Castro, 2006; Castro et al., 2006a). Numerous
Program). training modules were developed that encompassed
One of the fundamental questions that existed at the entire deployment cycle, providing soldiers and
the time the Battlemind Training System was being leaders with mental health training and education
developed was whether mental health training was tailored for where they were in the deployment cycle.
efective in either preventing or reducing mental health Scientiic rigor was also applied where possible to evalu-
problems, particularly for soldiers deploying to and ate whether the various Battlemind Training modules
from combat. Indeed many mental health profession- accomplished the intended goals (Adler et al., 2007,
als were understandably skeptical of such an approach. 2009a; Mental Health Advisory Team, 2008; homas
here was little evidence in the scientiic literature to et al., 2007). Equally important, however, at the dir-
suggest that large-scale one-hour programs of any sort ection of the Secretary of the Army, the Battlemind
could have any signiicant impact on the resilience of Training System was integrated into the institutional
service members (Sharpley et al., 2008). Consequently, army via the Deployment Cycle Support Program and
the mental health training that was developed should the army’s formal oicer and non-commissioned oi-
have measurable, long-term efects. hat is, the men- cer career training courses (Department of the Army,
tal health training needed to be based on scientiic 2007). For this reason, Battlemind Training has been
evidence, but should also be validated scientiically to chosen to illustrate the concepts of developing and
demonstrate its eicacy. Insisting on scientiic valid- implementing mental health training within an occu-
ation is critical on several levels. First, it demonstrates pational health context.
that the intervention provides some beneit and is Recently, the Battlemind Training System was
therefore worth time and personnel resources. Second, selected as one of the three programs to be integrated
it demonstrates the limits to the efects of such training into Comprehensive Soldier Fitness (CSF), a broad US
so that important outcomes can be targeted in other Army initiative (Cornum et al., 2011). his initiative
ways. hird, it provides a standard that can be used to ensures in-depth resilience training that integrates
guide decision making for senior leaders faced with fundamental resilience skills based on work from
a myriad of well-intentioned programs and requests the Pennsylvania Resiliency Program, performance
for funding and access to troops. In efect, additional psychology techniques from the Center for Enhanced
validated training can ensure that opportunists are less Performance, and resilience skills focused on the mili-
likely to be successful in selling unsubstantiated train- tary occupational context (the former Battlemind
ing to the military. Training System). he training and implementation
principles discussed in this chapter are also exempli-
ied by the eforts taken with CSF, including the need
A brief history of Battlemind Training for evidence-based training and program evaluation
he Battlemind concept was originally articulated (Lester et al., 2011).
by General Crosbie Saint in a senior leader training
guide (Battlemind guidelines for battalion command- Definitions
ers) while he was serving as the Commanding General Before beginning a discussion of the principles of men-
of the US Army, Europe (Saint, 1992). In this guide, tal health training and implementation considerations,
Saint emphasized the role of the senior leader in pre- it is useful to provide a few key deinitions in order to
paring the soldier for the human dimensional aspects clarify what is being discussed and prevent confusion.
of combat. Saint’s message described Battlemind as “a
warrior’s fortitude in the face of danger.” Consequently, Mental health training
Battlemind was originally a concept created by the he central concept to be deined is mental health
warighter for the warighter. training. he Technical Cooperation Program Tech-

324
Chapter 22: Military mental health training

nical Panel 13 (TP-13) deined it as: “Mental health Similarly, the panel rejected deinitions that focused
training is an intervention primarily designed to on resilience as a collection of attributes or risk/pro-
mitigate mental health problems and/or promote tective factors that confer susceptibility/resistance to
good mental health through skills and knowledge. It the harmful efects of adversity, such as the presence or
is armor for the mind. Mental health training/educa- absence of childhood adversity. While such attributes
tion involves prevention, or early intervention, not are interesting and important as covariates, the panel
clinical treatment” (Castro, 2008).1 Two things are was mainly interested in the psychological or behav-
important to note in this deinition. First, the focus ioral mechanisms by which these attributes confer risk or
is on training, which implies that the training must protection. he driving consideration here is the prag-
contain a skills component. hat is not to say men- matic aspect of being better able to enhance resilience
tal health training does not include education; it using psychological or behavioral interventions, and
most certainly will have educational components, but to examine potential interactions between individual
education is not the exclusive focus. Second, mental characteristics and interventions to enhance resilience
health training is not clinical treatment. So, mental (Zamorski, 2008).
health training should not include a discussion of As the paragraph above suggests, the panel included
mental health diagnoses nor should it involve treat- behavioral aspects of psychological process in its dein-
ment intervention suggestions. ition of resilience. his deinition relects the fact that
the panel viewed resilience as a latent construct that
Battlemind is inferred through the preservation of well-being and
Battlemind is the warrior’s inner strength to face the real- functioning in response to adversity. he panel felt
ities of the environment with courage, conidence, and that other deinitions imply more than is really known
resilience.2 his means meeting the mental challenges of about resilience by focusing on one or more speciic
training, operations, combat, and transitioning home. processes, which may indeed contribute to resilience
Warriors with Battlemind take care of themselves, their but are not necessarily the only such processes. here may
buddies, and those they lead. Two key components of come a time when the processes that confer such pro-
Battlemind are self-conidence and mental toughness; tection will be understood suiciently well that resili-
strengths that all soldiers must have to successfully ence can be deined as such processes rather than as the
perform in combat and to navigate between training, consequence of their application, but the ield is not yet
operations, combat, and home. Both of these concepts ready for such distinctions.
comprise a behavior and an attitude. Self-conidence he latency of the resilience construct means that its
involves taking calculated risks (the behavior) and hav- presence can only be convincingly demonstrated ater
ing the belief in one’s self to handle future challenges exposure to adversity. Nevertheless, it seems likely that
(the attitude). Mental toughness demands overcoming resilience must exist in some form prior to the experi-
obstacles and setbacks (the behavior) and maintaining ence of adversity, even if it cannot yet be demonstrated
positive thoughts during times of adversity and chal- convincingly. he panel conceptualized resilience
lenge (the attitude). he goal of Battlemind Training is as being processes speciic to the person, rather than
to build and foster self-conidence and mental tough- the sum total of factors (including environmental and
ness (Castro, 2004, 2005). social factors) that contribute to the capacity to experi-
ence adversity without long-lasting harm (Zamorski,
2008). Environmental and social factors (such as social
Resilience support) are certainly important factors that merit
Resilience has many deinitions, spanning many study, but the panel’s interest in such factors was in the
dimensions, including psychological, spiritual, phys- role that such factors play as covariates, mediators, or
ical, and emotional, among others. For the purposes moderators.
of this chapter, the deinition developed by TP-13 Resilience is important for both traumatic and non-
(Castro, 2008) is adopted, deining resilience from a traumatic stressors, although the former has attracted
psychological perspective: “Resilience comprises the much more attention, particularly in the military con-
sum total of the psychological processes that permit text. Similarly, the distress or dysfunction that occurs
individuals to maintain or return to previous levels of in response to adversity can take many forms beyond
well-being and functioning in response to adversity.” the well-known symptom complex of post-traumatic

325
Section 5: Training for resilience

stress disorder (PTSD). For example, depression, risk- mental health training, like Battlemind Training, that
taking behavior, acting out, alcohol abuse, and other can increase resilience and mental itness. Both resili-
behaviors can emerge following stressful experiences, ence and mental itness can be measured as a way
and the impact of these behaviors on quality of life and to determine the eicacy of mental health training.
functioning may be greater than PTSD. Despite the Resilience, however, can only be inferred in the pres-
importance of broadening the way in which outcomes ence of signiicant stressors, whereas mental itness can
are considered, the TP-13 panel viewed the meas- be measured directly by the presence of skills and cap-
urement of resilience as the assessment of those psy- acity. Both resilience and mental itness, however, dem-
chological processes that contribute to recovery ater onstrate the potential for mental health training to have
adversity and did not exclusively focus on outcomes. an impact on well-being. Efective mental health train-
While measurement of the outcomes of distress and ing is not only based on the content of the training but
dysfunction are critical for the validation of resilience also on overarching principles that can be used to guide
measures, such outcome assessment should not be con- the development of mental health training content.
fused with the processes inherent in resilience itself. For
example, if an individual is resilient, then they may not Fundamental principles of mental
experience depression in the presence of some stres-
sor. his lack of depression is not resilience but rather a health training
marker that a resilient process has occurred. he foundations of efective mental health training
he concept of resilience has become increas- rest on several fundamental principles (Box 22.1). In
ingly attractive to mental health specialists. It is con- developing the mental health training modules for the
sistent with the positive psychology movement (e.g., Battlemind Training System, care was taken to adhere
Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) and provides to each of these principles. One could easily argue that
researchers with an alternative approach to under- these principles are not unique to mental health train-
standing why some individuals falter under stress, ing, but to all good training, regardless of the topic
by focusing on why some individuals do not. For the or domain. However, explicitly stating these princi-
purposes of this discussion, we do not distinguish ples can guide the development of new modules, thus
between resilience training and mental health train- contributing to the coherency of an integrated train-
ing; good mental health training should be the same ing system. Furthermore, without being cognizant of
thing as good resilience training. To make a distinc- these speciic principles, one or more of them may be
tion at this point would be to imply a level of specii- more likely to be overlooked or violated by those who
city, which neither concept can yet claim. he speciic attempt to develop mental health training. It should be
terms may be adopted for meeting the needs of speciic kept in mind that, while the principles of mental health
organizational goals. training and the principles of implementation are dis-
cussed separately, these principles certainly inluence
Mental fitness each other. In the following sections, each is discussed
A new term that is emerging and that also requires a in the context of the US Army. Other organizations,
working deinition is mental itness. Mental itness can however, should be able to easily adapt these concepts
be viewed as the psychological equivalent of physical for application with their population.
itness and can be deined as “an individual’s capacity to
sustain mental health and performance in a demanding
environment” (Adler, 2009, p. 5). In contrast to resili- Strength-based approach
ence, mental itness is not a process but a characteristic Efective mental health training should build on skills
or state of an individual that can be measured before, and strengths that soldiers and military families already
during, or ater mental health training. possess. A strength-based approach explicitly rejects a
deicit or medical model. Practically, being strength-
Integrating the definitions based means providing a positive approach that sets
he four key deinitions in this chapter – mental health the expectation of success for the individual and does
training, Battlemind, resilience, and mental itness – not reinforce stereotypes that individuals are weak or
are clearly interrelated. From the perspective of an will become sick as a result of some stressful experience
organization such as the military, the goal is to develop like deployment.

326
Chapter 22: Military mental health training

Box 22.1 Fundamental principles of mental to include a list of mental health symptoms or include
health training and implementation a discussion of mental health diagnosis, as is found in
many military mental health training programs.
Principles of mental health training here is a ine line, however, between avoiding a
• Strength based medical model in which symptoms are prescribed,
or at least elucidated, and providing individuals with
• Relevant purpose and content
enough information about typical reactions so that
• Based on experience
they know what is normal and what might be a sign
• Explanatory that professional help is warranted. his balance must
• Team-based approach be maintained throughout the training and continu-
• Action-based strategies ally re-examined. One way to maintain this balance is
• Developmental approach to obtain feedback from participants about their per-
• Comprehensive and integrated training ception of the training message and to ensure that the
• User acceptability training addresses how existing strengths and new
skills can be applied, and that it provides suicient time
• Evidence-based and validated methodology
to practice those skills before they are needed.

Principles of implementation
• Integration into the organizational culture
Relevant purpose and content
• Appropriate in timing
All training should have a clear purpose or object-
ive, and all the training content should support that
• Quality control
purpose. Consistent with this principle, the content
• Train-the-trainer program for mental health training should be based on docu-
• Exportable and scalable mented needs. In the case of the Battlemind Training
• Training guidelines System, the topics covered in the training modules
• Refresher training were based on extensive surveys and interviews with
• Mobile training teams deployed and re-deploying soldiers, as well as with
• Sustainability family members. Rather than making assumptions
• Program improvement
about what soldiers experienced, or what they needed
to know, the training was informed by an ongoing sys-
• Policy
tematic assessment. Feedback from this research then
• Leader supported inluenced the development of the content of each of
• Verifiable claims the Battlemind Training modules. By using this kind
• Packaging and multimedia of rigorous approach, the training can avoid being the
• Ownership product of a trainer’s idiosyncratic experience, which
can lead to a training program of limited value.
One example from Battlemind Training’s pre-de-
he strength-based approach also explicitly builds on ployment module illustrates this point. he purpose
existing skills and abilities. In the case of the Battlemind of the module provided right before deployment is to
Training System, individuals are reminded of the skills prepare soldiers mentally for what to expect in a com-
that they already possess, and new skills tap into these bat environment, and to remind them about speciic
existing skills. For example, the training emphasizes actions that they can take to mitigate the impact of these
the importance of building relationships back home. experiences on their psychological health. he need
he individuals are reminded that they already have for this pre-deployment module emerged from focus
the skills to build relationships, as evidenced by the groups with soldiers who described not being prepared
strong bonds they have formed with their battle bud- mentally for what they encountered on deployment.
dies. Existing strengths and skills provide a scafold by Moreover, the content of this preparation was based
which new skills and information can contribute to the on surveys and focus group interviews with soldiers
resilience of an individual. In addition, since a medical when they were asked about their deployment experi-
model is rejected, mental health training does not need ences. hus, in pre-deployment Battlemind Training,

327
Section 5: Training for resilience

the focus is on what soldiers can expect to encounter this mixed reaction can help them to understand and
in the deployed environment that may afect them, and normalize it. he development of explanations for such
not on how the deployment is going to change them, or complex and conlicted reactions requires professional
on what to expect when they return from deployment. expertise in behavioral health.
Certainly these other issues are important, but they are
reserved for when soldiers return home as part of the A team-based approach
post-deployment modules. Relevance refers not only
he military organization is fundamentally based on
to content but to the timing of the training so that the
teams, on leadership, and on unit cohesion. Any men-
training matches the needs of the group at that time.
tal health training with the military needs to integrate
these fundamental components of the organization.
Based on experience Military mental health training should take advantage
Good mental health training should also include sce- of the natural camaraderie and hierarchy that exists
narios and situational training that reinforces the within all military cultures. For example, in Battlemind
information and skills being trained (hompson & Training, unit cohesion is one of the core elements of
McCreary, 2006). For every skill or educational point all of the training modules. Each module teaches how
addressed in the training, there should be a real-world to look ater other unit members and uses this buddy
example that can be used to reinforce that point. In the focus as a way to also increase self-awareness. he
case of the Battlemind Training, examples should be Battlemind Training modules also highlight the role
used that the soldiers and/or families can relate to and of leaders and the leaders’ responsibilities for ensur-
that use the language of the military. hese examples ing that their subordinates get the mental healthcare
should be based on experiences of soldiers, not on the they need. he training is typically also provided in a
experiences of the trainer. When trainers are men- unit context in order to reinforce the material across
tal health professionals, their personal examples may the group. Battlemind trainers also report anecdotally
undermine their credibility. he trainers may appear that unit members will comment to one another dur-
misguided if they appear to think that their experi- ing the group training and point out particular reac-
ence of deployment or stress mirrors the experience tions that relate to a unit member, interacting in a way
of a junior soldier on patrol, a non-commissioned oi- that enhances the relevance of the material.
cer in logistics, or an oicer in command of a combat
arms unit. he Battlemind Training System sought to Action-based strategies
overcome this problem by developing detailed speaker
Mental health training should address speciic actions
notes that contain numerous real-world examples from
individuals can take, not just be a theoretical descrip-
experienced soldiers that the mental health trainer
tion of stress responses. hese actions should be able
could use in conducting the training.
to guide the behavior of soldiers and spouses. he
Battlemind Training System is designed to focus on
Explanatory such behaviors, and future iterations of Battlemind
Good mental health training is explanatory; it high- Training modules are being ielded to test the ei-
lights conlicted or misunderstood reactions that cacy of teaching speciic skills. In keeping with the
service members might experience. he Battlemind team-based approach mentioned above, these actions
Training System includes numerous examples of how include behaviors that soldiers can take to help them-
soldiers might experience mixed or contradictory selves, their buddies, and those they lead.
reactions and emphasizes that these reactions are nor- One of the key components to teaching action-
mal. For example, while soldiers are happy to be home based strategies is the need for lexible and adaptive
from a long combat deployment, they also oten report coping in response to a myriad of potential stressors.
being angry and on edge. he training normalizes this he training needs to speciically advance the idea that
dual experience and explains that while many soldiers there are diferent types of stressor and which coping
report being happy to see their family and friends, mechanism is best depends on how much direct con-
they are oten angry about being deployed for a year or trol the individual has over the stressor. For many mili-
angry about how they were treated during the deploy- tary personnel, signiicant stressors are outside of their
ment. Providing soldiers with the words to understand direct control and so they need to practice action-based

328
Chapter 22: Military mental health training

strategies that are not “action” in the sense of getting framework, mental health vocabulary, and actions
rid of the stressor. he action may involve a change in should be consistent across the training system.
cognitive coping, a reduction in physiological arousal, Although the irst module of the Battlemind
seeking social support, or acceptance. Redeining Training System developed was an immediate post-
action as incorporating each of these kinds of skill, and deployment intervention, it was clear that a broader
emphasizing the need to match the appropriate coping approach was needed. For example, the development
response to the situation, is a key part of an integrated team recognized that the training needed to include
mental health training system. a follow-up post-deployment module and a spouse
module, prior to deployment. Later, based on feedback
Developmental approach from training and from army leaders, the Battlemind
Training System was expanded to target leadership,
Efective training builds on prior training or upon
medics, mental health providers, and soldiers in vari-
existing strengths and skills and progressively adds
ous career courses, beginning with basic combat train-
new concepts and skills. Ideally, we believe that a men-
ing. By conceptualizing mental health training as an
tal health training system should strive to develop skills
integrated system, the lesson plans can build on one
of increasing complexity, beginning with simple con-
another and can reinforce the points of each training
cepts. For example, the training can introduce a sim-
module as exempliied by the CSF initiative.
ple approach to cognitive restructuring in managing
At this point in the development of the Battlemind
the stressors of basic combat training while waiting
Training System, the modules provide a comprehensive
until later in the career of a soldier to teach how cogni-
and integrated mental health training approach. For
tive restructuring can be used to manage a high-stress
example, the deployment-related modules build upon
environment such as combat deployment. Another
and reinforce each other. he pre-deployment mod-
example of the developmental approach is to introduce
ules prepare soldiers and leaders to deploy to a com-
the concepts of PTSD without detailing the complex-
bat environment (McGurk et al., 2007); the in-theater
ities of the diagnostic criteria. he initial training could
psychological debrieings sustain soldiers throughout
include an overall appreciation for how PTSD-related
the deployment and reinforce Battlemind Training
reactions can interfere with getting along with friends,
principles (Adler et al., 2009b); the post-deployment
family, and at work without discussing the disorder
module prepares soldiers and leaders for the reintegra-
itself. his approach avoids the temptation of present-
tion process immediately upon returning home from
ing PTSD criteria in an oversimpliied manner that
combat (Adler et al., 2009a; homas et al., 2007); and
might inadvertently lead soldiers to think that they
the follow-up module allows soldiers and leaders to re-
have PTSD if they have only a few PTSD symptoms. In
assess their mental health status to determine if they are
subsequent courses for certain personnel, such as lead-
in need of additional mental healthcare (Adler et al.,
ers or medics, more information could be presented
2007). Spouse training was also developed for before
about symptoms, symptom clusters, and time course.
and ater deployment (see also Chapter 16). Finally,
Such information underscores the need for content to
modules have been developed that target medical
be informed by experts in mental health, as will be dis-
providers; leadership in junior, mid-level, and senior
cussed under implementation principles.
leaders (non-commissioned oicers and oicers); and
soldiers irst entering military service. he Battlemind
Comprehensive and integrated training Training System was the irst training program in the
Implicit throughout this chapter is the assumption that military to take a comprehensive approach to mental
mental health training needs to be more than a single health training across the entire deployment and life
session. Mental health training should not be one-of cycle.
training that occurs only once a year, only when the he Battlemind Training System is well recognized
service member gets ready to deploy, or only when the and has been a vehicle for communicating deployment-
service member returns from deployment. It needs to related mental health literacy among a wide range of
provide the target population with an integrated and military personnel (e.g., Slone & Friedman, 2008). he
comprehensive system that builds skills, reinforces demand for these deployment and life cycle modules
concepts, and targets areas of relevance to the group at by the military has at time presented the development
the right time. In addition, of course, the fundamental team at the Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

329
Section 5: Training for resilience

with a dilemma: wait for group randomized trials to be here are many possible markers of a successful
completed or derive the essential elements from mod- mental health training program. Typically, in order to
ules that have been studied, implement the training, assess a program’s efectiveness, the outcomes should
and then follow this implementation with program match the intent of the program. Historically, much
evaluation and randomized trials to improve training. of the research on early interventions has focused on
In order to meet the immediate need, the development PTSD symptoms, and other researchers have criticized
team, along with members of the Battlemind Training this limited view (Deahl et al., 2001). For our purposes,
Oice at the Army Medical Department Center and military mental health training outcomes should
School, produced a series of Battlemind Training include measures of (1) attitude, (2) skill attainment,
modules. (3) mental health itness, (4) training satisfaction, and
(5) unit climate and leadership.
First, in terms of attitudes, mental health train-
User acceptability ing should target self-conidence in handling chal-
Mental health training must be perceived to be use- lenges and monitoring positive thoughts during times
ful by those being trained in order for the training of adversity. Second, in terms of skill sets, outcomes
to become accepted into the organizational culture. should address the speciic skills and knowledge
Even if the training is eicacious, if it is not face valid, addressed in the training. For example, training may
the audience does not accept it, and the trainers do address knowledge about anxiety management and
not support it, then the training quality is likely to providing buddy aid. hird, in terms of mental health
deteriorate or drit and resentment may preclude itness, outcome indicators should include measures
it from being helpful. In the case of the Battlemind of distress that go beyond traditional PTSD symptoms
Training System, surveys and interviews with soldiers as well as measures of positive well-being. Outcomes
and spouses ater training found that the modules of relevance to the organization should be included,
were relevant and useful. However, while user accept- such as aggression, sleep, relationship qualities, and
ability is necessary, it is not suicient for establish- risk-taking behaviors. Fourth, as mentioned above,
ing good mental health training (Iverson et al., 2008; measures of training satisfaction and user acceptabil-
Sharpley et al., 2008; McKibben et al., 2009). In order ity should be included. Fith, measures of unit climate
to demonstrate that mental health training improves should be included because the training can have an
mental itness, randomized controlled studies must impact on the way the unit climate is perceived and
be conducted. because the training can have an impact on the lead-
ership itself. herefore, these measures should address
the degree to which mental health training may have
Evidence-based and validated had an impact on cohesion and leadership quality.
methodology Indeed research has found that the Battlemind Training
What does it mean to say “evidence based?” he mater- System increased soldier perception of organizational
ial in the training needs to be based on research evi- support (homas et al., 2007). Similarly, the training
dence and the training itself needs to be validated. should also assess the degree to which leaders support
his validation extends beyond satisfaction ratings or the mental health skills and training provided by the
demonstration of changes from pre- to post-training. organization. Without support from the leadership,
he standard needed for demonstrating mental health the training will likely be less efective.
training eicacy is a randomized controlled trial. For One outcome measure that may be considered but
training that is conducted in intact groups, random- that is diicult to interpret is whether or not individ-
ization needs to occur at the level of the group, not the uals seek help for clinical services as a function of train-
individual. herefore, group randomized trials are ing. On the one hand, if seeking help for mental health
needed (Donner & Klar, 2004; Varnell et al., 2004). his problems increases, then this outcome may be an
approach can be diicult, time consuming, and com- indicator of greater openness to help-seeking, greater
plex statistically but the end result is evidence assessing awareness of mental health problems, and less stigma
the training’s efectiveness. Exactly what these studies associated with seeking care. On the other hand, seek-
assess as markers of efectiveness depends on the goal ing help for mental health problems could be an indi-
of the training. cator of greater distress. Consequently, without a clear

330
Chapter 22: Military mental health training

rationale, this outcome can lead to results that are dif- size can be considered practically meaningful from a
icult to disentangle. public health perspective. Future studies will examine
To date, there have been three group randomized the impact of Battlemind Training on speciic initia-
controlled trials conducted with post-deployment tives such as managing intrusive thoughts (Shipherd
Battlemind Training. In these studies, platoons & Salters-Pedneault, 2008), expressive writing (Adler
recently returned from a year in Iraq were randomly et al., 2008), and speciic coping skill attainment (e.g.,
assigned to either a Battlemind Training condition or Adler, 2009).
a comparison condition. For the immediate post-de-
ployment training studies (Adler et al., 2009a; homas Fundamental principles of
et al., 2007), the comparison condition was traditional implementation
stress education. Follow-up surveys were adminis-
tered about four months later. For the study in which When mental health training content is being devel-
Battlemind Training was provided four months ater oped and validated, how the training will be imple-
returning home, follow-up surveys were administered mented should be considered. While distinct, the
six months later (Adler et al., 2009). In terms of training training content and processes (the implementation
satisfaction, all Battlemind Training interventions have strategy) dramatically inluence each other. he key
received high ratings in terms of user acceptability. implementation principles regarding mental health
In terms of attitudes, these studies have found training in organizations are given in Box 22.1 and
that soldiers reported fewer stigma-related attitudes discussed below. While this is not an exhaustive list of
toward seeking help for mental health problems ater all the implementation issues that need to be consid-
participating in Battlemind Training. In terms of men- ered when developing mental health training, it does
tal health itness, for the most part, these studies have represent the common issues that arise.
found that Battlemind Training is associated with fewer
PTSD symptoms (Adler et al., 2007, 2009a; homas Integration into the organizational culture
et al., 2007), depression symptoms (Adler et al., 2007, Military mental health training must be integrated into
2009a), anger problems (Adler et al., 2007), and sleep the organization’s culture (homas & Castro, 2003). In
problems (Adler et al., 2009a). In the Adler et al. (2009a) the case of the US Army, this means that Battlemind
study, efects on PTSD symptoms and sleep problems needs to be called “training” and not “education,” as the
were found only for those soldiers reporting high levels military leadership understands and promotes “train-
of combat experiences, although main efects for inter- ing” but not necessarily “education.” In fact, many mili-
vention condition were found for depression symp- tary leaders view education as an unnecessary use of
toms. Main efects were found for the other two group valuable soldier time. his distinction does not mean
randomized controlled trials. In terms of unit climate, that mental health training cannot contain educational
homas et al. (2007) reported that Battlemind Training material, but it does mean that the mental health train-
resulted in improved perceived organizational support ing needs to involve skill strengthening or skill devel-
relative to traditional stress education. opment. In the context of a military academy, however,
While efect sizes were small (e.g., Cohen’s d the terminology may need to emphasize “education”
values of 0.20–0.30), these studies demonstrate the rather than “training.” In other nations, for example
potential for single-session mental health interven- Germany and the Netherlands, the two concepts are
tions to improve the adjustment of soldiers returning regarded as sequential. he irst phase of the program
from combat. Small efect sizes are typically found in has to be termed “education” and the second phase in
ield research (Bliese et al., 2011) because the over- which the skills are practiced is termed “training.” he
all model error is larger than in experimental studies larger issue is that the language used to describe and
(McClelland & Judd, 1993), and a small efect size may promote the program needs to make sense within the
have important practical signiicance. For example, organizational context.
while an efect size of 0.21 is usually considered small, Mental health training should also be conducted
Rosenthal and Rubin (1982) argued that the efect size within existing units, preferably at the platoon or com-
represents a success rate of 45% versus 55% between pany level in order to optimize the impact of small
two groups. When this efect is assessed in the con- group dynamics and leadership. Conducting mental
text of training large numbers of soldiers, the efect health training in small, pre-existing groups ensures

331
Section 5: Training for resilience

group members will have the opportunity to share In the Battlemind Training System, mental health
their experiences with each other, and feel comfortable training modules are distinct for each phase of the
enough to do so. Training conducted in large groups development cycle and are focused on the speciic
in an auditorium or gymnasium runs the risk of being needs of the soldier and spouse. his tailored approach
too impersonal, and too large for focused skill develop- was an outcome based on the scientiic evidence
ment to occur. In general, mental health training con- obtained by the development team at the Walter Reed
ducted in large groups is likely to become educational Army Institute of Research regarding soldier and fam-
or didactic in nature, with little interaction or sharing ily deployment-related health and the team’s real-world
of the group members’ personal experiences. In the experience working with soldiers before, during, and
case of Battlemind Training, the training was deliber- ater combat deployments. he team’s multidisciplin-
ately designed to be conducted in small groups, with ary expertise (psychiatry, psychology, sociology, and
group members sharing their experiences. he role social work) and experience enabled the development
of the trainer is to facilitate these group interactions. of the US Army’s irst integrated and comprehensive
he degree to which group size actually inluences mental health training program.
the eicacy of the training remains unclear. Indeed,
homas et al. (2007) compared small and large group Quality control
Battlemind Training and did not ind reliable difer-
ences between the two types. Any standardized training program requires a robust
quality control program to ensure that the training
is being conducted as intended. Constant vigilance
Appropriate in timing is required to ensure that the content of the mental
Mental health training should be relevant to the health training is maintained; the content should not
deployment cycle or the stage of professional devel- be altered nor should additional material be inserted
opment. his point has been made above but it into the training modules. Further, the mental health
deserves repeating. For example, mental health train- training needs to be conducted using the procedures
ing designed to be given prior to deploying should not that have been validated. For the Battlemind Training
be given during the deployment, nor should mental System, this would mean that that the training needs
health training designed to be given at post-deploy- to be conducted in small, intact groups, with a trained
ment be given during the deployment or prior to facilitator to guide the group interactions. A mental
deploying. Since the content of mental health training health training quality control program should sys-
should vary depending on the phase of the deploy- tematically ensure that trainers are prepared to con-
ment, it stands to reason that the training cannot be duct the training, that the training materials and lesson
used interchangeably. plans are clear and detailed, and that the training con-
When training programs are promoted as use- ducted remains consistent. Maintaining quality train-
ful regardless of when they are implemented in the ing can be diicult in a large organization such as the
deployment cycle phase, the training itself becomes military. Mechanisms such as refresher courses, team
suspect in terms of its relevance to the organizational teaching, training evaluation, and spot checking by a
context. Training that is independent of the deploy- mobile team responsible for training quality can facili-
ment cycle appears to underestimate the importance of tate quality sustainment over time.
the diferent stressors associated with the deployment
cycle phases and how these stressors inluence the well- Train-the-trainer program
being of service members. Creating a one-size-its-all he irst step in preventing drit in the content and
approach to the training cycle is not likely to meet implementation is to develop a train-the-trainer pro-
the needs of service members and families, as their gram in which each mental health trainer receives
needs change depending on the phase of the deploy- formal training and certiication which conirms that
ment. Proponents and/or developers of such global they are capable of delivering the training program to
approaches to mental health training fail to recognize standard. At a minimum, a train-the-trainer program
the organizational context and may not have ensured should include the training material, a detailed course
that the needs of the service members are adequately syllabus, and detailed speaker notes. A train-the-trainer
addressed. course should also include practice for the individuals

332
Chapter 22: Military mental health training

who are being certiied as trainers and an evaluation best manage these conlicting preferences. Regardless
of their training performance. Only ater the individ- of who these trainers are, they will need to acknow-
ual being trained has shown competence in giving the ledge what they are not. If they are not experts in men-
mental health training, and in answering anticipated tal health, they can talk about how the material has
questions regarding the training, should they be cer- been developed by mental health professionals expe-
tiied. A train-the-trainer program, however, does not rienced in working with the military. If they have not
preclude the need for a quality control program, as drit been deployed, they can talk about how the material
in training can still occur. has been developed based on what soldiers have said is
Obviously during the development of a mental important to know. Even if the trainer has deployment
health training program, it must be decided who will experience, this experience can be misleading – estab-
be conducting the training. For example, will the men- lishing a link to the service members that is not really
tal health training be conducted by behavioral health- there. For example, most trainers with deployment
care providers, chaplains, experienced combat arms experience are not likely to have been a junior enlisted
service members, or some other group? his decision combat arms soldier out on patrol every day. he train-
needs to be addressed early in the training develop- the-trainer course needs to underscore the reality that
ment process because the decision regarding who con- the trainers cannot be everything to all people. he
ducts the training is likely to shape the content of the trainers can acknowledge what they do bring to train-
training material. Regardless of what decision is made ing in terms of their expertise, but most importantly
regarding who conducts the mental health training, they can bring their level of commitment, their enthu-
it should be noted that, while individuals may have a siasm, and their professionalism.
personal preference for one type of trainer, in terms of
Battlemind Training at least, trainer efects were not
associated with diferential rates of training eicacy. Exportable and scalable
In one study, for example, trainers included behavioral Regardless of who conducts the mental health training,
health oicers, enlisted soldiers with behavioral health a mental health training program must be designed
training, and civilians (Adler et al., 2009a). Some had with the average trainer in mind, not the ideal trainer.
combat deployment experience; others did not. In the Whatever the mental health training program, it
immediate atermath, when individuals rated the train- must be exportable and scalable. For example, a men-
ing atmosphere and their satisfaction with the training, tal health training program will be of little utility to
there were diferences in the way trainers were rated. large organizations such as the military if only a hand-
herefore, at this point, there were indeed statistical ful of people in the world are capable of conducting
diferences across trainers in the post-session ratings. the training or if it takes years to train others to con-
However, when outcomes were assessed four months duct the training. Surprisingly, this obvious criterion
later, there were no signiicant diferences across train- for a useful military mental health program is oten
ers. he training material itself drove the efects, not not considered or is dismissed by various advocates
the trainer. his distinction is important because when of mental health training programs, who insist that
training is implemented in an organization on a large trainers of various mental health training programs
scale it will be diicult to ensure that only the best require many years of experience. In one example,
trainers are used; the training material itself needs to outside experts ofering a training program to the US
be the key ingredient rather than a trainer’s personal Army patiently explained that the only people able to
style. Personal style cannot be dictated in a set of lesson provide the speciic training would have to embrace
plan instructions. the training on a personal level and spend years of
In examining the question of who should provide their life attaining “deep experience” with the mater-
the training, we have also noted that soldiers have dif- ial before they could train others, and this experience
ferent opinions on what constitutes an ideal mental might be obtained in remote sites in other countries.
health trainer. Some soldiers say they would prefer an Even if the training content could be validated, such
experienced combat veteran; others prefer knowing approaches to mental health training are of ques-
that the information is being provided by an expert tionable value to the military because they cannot be
in mental health. Consequently, the train-the-trainer implemented across a million or more soldiers in any
program should address how individual trainers can kind of realistic time frame.

333
Section 5: Training for resilience

Training guidelines should certainly also be developed. We are unaware of


refresher courses being integrated into other organiza-
Another means to ensure that mental health training
tion-based mental health training programs, although
is conducted consistently across military installations
a mental health course in Australian recruit training is
and over time is to develop clear guidelines as to how
being developed to integrate a “booster” session mid-
the training is to be conducted. In military language,
way through basic training.
this approach to training is known as establishing “task,
condition, and standard.” Task, conditions, and stand-
ards are applied to every form of training conducted Mobile training teams
in the military, regardless of the type. he task compo- Implementing mental health training on a large scale
nent of this approach speciies exactly what needs to can be facilitated through the use of mobile training
be accomplished. he conditions specify the context or teams. hese teams can train service members directly
the variables in a situation that may afect performance. but, more importantly, they can also conduct train-
Finally, the standards delineate the markers of success. the-trainer courses to certify other trainers. In the
Consequently, a “task, condition, standard” approach case of the Battlemind Training System, for example,
to mental health training details the exact training the Battlemind Training Oice at the Army Medical
that is to be conducted, who is to be trained, who is Department Center and School successfully used
to conduct the training, the environment in which mobile training teams to accomplish both of these
the training is conducted, and the means for assess- objectives. he new CSF also uses mobile training
ing the efectiveness of the training. his standardized teams.
approach to mental health training will also facilitate
a rigorous quality control program that can evaluate if Sustainability
the training is being conducted as intended.
Whatever mental health training program is adopted,
Here again, some proponents of mental health
it must be sustainable and supportable. he import-
training programs proposed for use in the mili-
ance of the trainer and standardization has already
tary state that their training should not or cannot be
been discussed in some detail. Equally important is
subjected to such rigid procedures. On inspection,
how long the training needs to be in order for it to be
proposed mental health training programs that can-
efective in increasing mental itness. In the military
not be standardized are programs that have not been
context, training time is a valuable commodity that
scientiically validated. Indeed, there is a common
must compete with a myriad of other demands. he
ground between the rigor required in a military’s “task,
modules contained in the Battlemind Training System
condition, standard” approach and the rigor required
were deliberately developed to be given in one-hour
of a randomized trial, in which the methods have to
sessions in order to integrate the modules into busy
be stated clearly so as to facilitate replication and crit-
unit schedules. In developing the modules, the devel-
ical review. It appears that the non-standardized pro-
opment team recognized that a mental health training
grams tend to drit in their approach from one set of
program could easily be developed that could consume
procedures to another, or from one trainer to another.
upwards of 40 hours of training time. However, it was
Needless to say, such non-standardized programs can-
felt that commanders would resist a program that was
not be expanded on a large scale, nor are they easily
so time consuming, particularly if this time occurs
amendable to scientiic investigation.
during the pre-deployment train-up phase.
he fact that a one-hour session has been found
Refresher training in three studies to reduce mental health symptoms
Like most efective training, mental health training related to combat exposure supports the feasibility
should contain refresher training modules. Service of this economical approach. he key question now
members should not be repeatedly subjected to the becomes how the one-hour modules can be enhanced
exact same mental health training material because the to increase efectiveness. Although one-hour mod-
training will inevitably become stale, which will likely ules are efective, judicious use of additional training
blunt its efectiveness. Various modules provided over should be tested in order to improve the eicacy of the
time and over the course of the deployment cycle serve entire system. Whatever system results, it must remain
to reinforce the key principles, but refresher modules cognizant of the issue of time as well as other resources,

334
Chapter 22: Military mental health training

such as personnel, equipment, and coordination. If too 2006b). Consequently, their explicit and implicit sup-
many resources are required, then the training pro- port for a program can mean the diference between a
gram may not be sustainable in the long run. supportive training environment and one in which the
training is conducted as a way to “check the block” –
Program improvement simply meeting a speciic organizational requirement.
Leaders can demonstrate their support for mental
Along with ensuring that the training program can
health training by attending the training themselves,
be sustained in terms of implementation, there also
by emphasizing the importance of the mental health
needs to be a vigorous system for continually assessing
training to their subordinates, and by ensuring that
whether the mental health training program is achiev-
the training is a priority on the unit calendar. Research
ing its stated goals and to identify how the program can
evidence demonstrating the eicacy of mental health
be improved. Unfortunately, military leaders may be
training can directly impact the leader’s endorsement
reluctant to commit resources for program assessment
of the program. If scientiic indings can be provided
and improvement. Without such a program in place,
to leaders showing them that mental health training
however, there will be no systematic, ongoing analyses
is efective in increasing the mental itness of their
to ensure that the mental health needs of service mem-
unit, then they are more likely to support the train-
bers and families are still being met. Such improve-
ing. Obtaining high-proile endorsements from
ment eforts are not only important for ensuring the
senior enlisted service members and oicers can also
program remains efective but also provide another
enhance the acceptability of mental health training.
way to ensure that service members do not become
he Battlemind Training System received endorse-
bored by the same material.
ments from senior Army leaders and has served to
change the Army culture regarding mental health
Policy training. Similarly, CSF has received Army support
Even the most efective mental health training can- from high levels.
not be successful without a parallel efort on the part
of the organization to institutionalize its implementa-
tion (homas & Castro, 2003). In the case of the mili- Verifiable claims
tary, policy must be developed that supports and directs Credibility requires that any claims made regard-
that mental health training be conducted. here must ing mental health training be consistent with verii-
also be guidance issued that describes how the training able facts that are based on scientiic evidence (for a
will be implemented (see the discussion of the “task, review of military mental health prevention eforts,
condition, standard” approach above). In short, orders see Castro et al. [2004] and Mulligan et al. [2010]).
must be given that mandate that military mental health Unfortunately, developers or proponents of various
training occur. Otherwise, such training will be let up mental health training programs sometimes make
to the discretion of each commander, usually with the unsubstantiated claims regarding what their training
result that the training is not conducted to the stand- program can achieve. hese unsubstantiated claims
ard that has been shown to be efective, or it will not range from preventing PTSD to improving decision
be conducted at all. Regarding the Battlemind Training making in combat. Perhaps the proponents for these
System, the Secretary of the Army mandated that it programs become caught up in an enthusiastic attempt
should be conducted for all deploying and re-deploying to implement their work and neglect the lack of scien-
soldiers and that training opportunities be provided to tiic evidence. Perhaps they are more focused on being
spouses. he CSF initiative was implemented with simi- sure to present their product in a way that is relevant
lar policy directives. to the organization and they underestimate the degree
to which scientiic evidence matters. Regardless of
Leader supported their motivation, the end efect is that proponents of
Whether the training is mandated or not, leaders are these unsubstantiated claims are able to prey upon the
critical for the successful implementation of mental hope, fear, and ignorance of unit leaders to gain access
health training. Leaders at all levels play an import- to service members, usually with the unit paying for
ant role in the mental health of service members, both the training. Such unethical behavior should not be
directly and indirectly (Britt et al., 2004; Castro et al., tolerated by the organization or by the professionals

335
Section 5: Training for resilience

promoting their training programs. Leaders need to be and deployment are equally complex and nuanced.
provided with realistic expectations about what men- Consequently, the content of the mental health train-
tal health training will achieve and what it will not; the ing program needs to belong to military behavioral
organization needs to make decisions based on science healthcare experts. he content of mental health train-
in order to diferentiate between efective training and ing must be determined by behavioral health experts.
another good idea. hey are the subject matter experts, not the com-
mander, not the chaplains, not the policy makers, and
Packaging and multimedia not others who are interested in helping service mem-
bers. Obviously, these individuals are expert in their
Efective mental health training needs good packaging.
own areas and their input is invaluable for ensuring
While this implementation principle may sound super-
that the training material addresses issues in a way that
icial, it is critical because it ultimately means that the
is relevant to the audience. In fact, the training devel-
information will be presented in such a way that the
opment process should actively solicit input from a
organization and the individual service member are
variety of domains, such as operational leadership, the
more likely to accept it. Furthermore, good packaging
chaplaincy, military families, and soldiers themselves.
through catchy slogans or the use of easy-to-remember
However, caring about service member mental health
phrases and acronyms enhances the degree to which
or having been deployed to a combat environment
individuals are likely to remember the training con-
does not make one an expert in mental health training;
tent. he use of humor can also make mental health
the behavioral care experts should own the content of
training more engaging. Still, care must be exercised to
the training.
avoid the training becoming so slick that the trainees
are put of by the training or the style distracts from the
training objective or message. Conclusions and future directions
Wherever possible, multimedia (e.g., interactive
computer simulations, video scenarios, music, gam- Lessons learned
ing technology) should be considered in developing a
Despite using the Battlemind Training System through-
mental health training program. While there is no clear
out this chapter as a way to illustrate mental health
evidence that mental health training is more efective if
training and implementation principles, it is import-
it employs multimedia, the training is likely to be more
ant to note that the training system encountered sig-
engaging, which increases the likelihood that individ-
niicant implementation diiculties. hese problems
uals will attend to the training content. Appropriately
included launching many modules before scientiic
incorporated and researched multimedia training can
validation could be conducted, not having a system in
also enhance standardization of training. Multimedia
place to certify instructors and track who received the
approaches also help to underscore a central tenet of
training, and not focusing enough on building speciic
training: training should be conducted to engage the
resilience skills. Other mental health training programs
three fundamental types of learner: visual learners, who
have also been ielded prior to proper validation stud-
prefer to be able to see the point written out or visually
ies being conducted and without a full implementation
depicted in a diagram; auditory learners, who prefer
plan. Such diiculties are part of ielding programs in a
to listen and discuss the information; and experiential
real-world setting in which senior leaders demand that
learners, who prefer to practice a concept and grapple
something be done immediately.
with some task related to the concept.
At some level, these stumbling blocks may be a sign
that the mental health training is so meaningful to the
Ownership leadership that they are willing to direct its implemen-
he inal implementation principle reviewed here is tation without all the training pieces being inalized
ownership: who actually controls the content of the and validated. It may also partly relect a lack of com-
training program and retains the right to revise it. munication to leadership about the importance of
Mental health and mental itness issues facing service such plans and studies.
members and their families are complex and varied. We also recognize that even with dedicated
How to build resilience and increase mental itness in resources to develop and validate mental health train-
order to help these people meet the demands of combat ing, it is diicult to create training that is interesting,

336
Chapter 22: Military mental health training

meaningful, and useful. Group randomized trials training that can be provided to moderate the impact of
take years to execute, and the team efort and expert- the demands, the inluence of the occupational context
ise required should not be underestimated. No doubt, on the way in which demands are appraised, and the
it is for these reasons that there are few examples of way in which demands afect outcomes. hese organ-
validated mental health training modules. izational factors include multiple levels of moderators
To counteract this common problem, the ielding from the individual, small group, leader, and organiza-
of mental health training should routinely include tion (Castro & Adler, 2005). By incorporating an occu-
a plan to conduct validation research and to address pational perspective into the model, we can take into
the broader implementation issues identiied in this account factors that can be taught in a mental health
chapter. he awareness of the importance of address- training program, such as successful coping strat-
ing these concerns appears to be increasing. To date, egies and behaviors, the role of professional identity,
for example, programs such as CSF and the Australian and inding meaning in one’s job. In this way, adopt-
Defence Force’s BattleSMART program are being ing an occupational health framework can facilitate
developed with such plans. the development of mental health training targeted
speciically on mental health issues inherent within a
military context.
The way ahead his kind of occupational health model does not
here are numerous occupational hazards associated mean that military mental health training should
with serving in the military. One of the greatest phys- exclusively focus on deployment-related mental health
ical hazards associated with combat are bullets, frag- problems. he focus on mental health problems should
ments, or other explosive munitions; therefore, service be broad enough to address other topics that can
members are required to wear body armor for protec- adversely impact on service member readiness, such
tion. However, there is a limit to the extent that such as family readiness and resilience. Both service-related
body armor can protect. Soldiers and marines are still and non-service-related mental health training can be
wounded and die from bullets and fragments even part of a comprehensive, integrated program.
when they wear their body armor. Similarly, mental At present, mental itness does not enjoy the same
health problems are also an occupational hazard for focus that physical itness does. Still, the concept of
service members in combat. While mental health train- mental itness is included in the Soldier’s Creed: “I am
ing can reduce the symptoms associated with PTSD, it disciplined, physically and mentally tough, trained and
certainly cannot protect every service member from proicient in my warrior tasks and drills.” By including
developing a diagnosis of PTSD. Like body armor, “mentally tough,” the US Army explicitly assumes that
there are limits as to what mental health training can mental itness is a necessary and important element to
achieve – and no matter how much we would like to being a soldier. Within the army, soldiers are required to
think otherwise, there are limits to what organizations conduct physical itness training three times per week
can do to prevent the negative mental health efects of and are given time out of the duty day to do so. hey are
combat. One useful approach to enhance what can be also encouraged to do physical training on their own
done to prevent PTSD symptoms and other mental for the other two days of the week. Physical itness is
health problems associated with deployment is to view taken so seriously in the army that soldiers must pass
these mental health disorders within an occupational a physical itness examination twice a year, and their
health framework. scores on this examination are used to determine pro-
Much like the soldier adaptation model developed motion potential. If soldiers receive the maximum
by Bliese and Castro (2003), an occupational health score possible on the physical itness examination
framework for the military proposes a model for exam- then they are oten given a four-day pass. Can we ever
ining the way in which occupational demands have an envision a time when mental itness is viewed on par
impact on individual adaptation. While many pre-ex- with physical itness? When training time is regularly
isting, individual and personality factors may inluence allotted to maintain mental itness? When soldiers are
the relationship between occupational demands and tested to ensure they are mentally it and when they are
outcomes, the focus of an occupational health model formally recognized for this resilience? hrough dein-
is on the nature of the occupational stressors (their ing what we mean by terms such as resilience, mental
duration, variability, intensity, predictability, etc.), the itness, and mental health training, and by establishing

337
Section 5: Training for resilience

fundamental principles of mental health training and Castro, C. A. (2004). How to build Battlemind. NCO Journal,
implementation, we can build toward delivering a sys- April, 23–24.
tem of mental health training for service members that Castro, C. A. (2005). Building Battlemind. Countermeasures,
is integrated, relevant, and efective. 26, 6–8.
Castro, C. A. (2006). Military courage. In T. W. Britt, A. B.
Acknowledgement Adler, & C. A. Castro (eds.), Military life: the psychology of
serving in peace and combat, Vol. 4: Military culture (pp.
he views expressed in this chapter are those of the 60–78). Westport, CT: Praeger.
author and do not relect the oicial position of the
Castro, C. A. (2008). Symposium of the Annual Meeting of
Walter Reed Institute of Research, the US Army, or the the International Military Testing Association on Military
US Department of Defense. Resiliency: Perspectives from an international technical
panel. Amsterdam, September.
References Castro, C. A. & Adler, A. B. (2005). Preface to the special
Adler, A. B. (2009). Protocol 1543: Basic training and mental issue. Military Psychology, 17, 131–136.
itness study: enhancing performance and mental health. Castro, C. A., Engel, C. C., & Adler, A. B. (2004). he
Silver Spring, MD: Walter Reed Army Institute of challenge of providing mental health prevention and
Research. early intervention in the US military. In B. T. Litz (ed.),
Adler, A. B., Castro, C. A., Bliese, P. D., McGurk, D. & Early intervention for trauma and traumatic loss (pp.
Milliken, C. (2007). he eicacy of Battlemind Training 301–318). New York: Guilford Press.
at 3–6 months post-deployment. In Symposium of Castro, C. A., Hoge, C. W., & Cox, A. L. (2006a). Battlemind
the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological training: building soldier resiliency. In Proceedings of
Association on the Battlemind Training System: RTO-MP-HFM-134, Human dimensions in military
Supporting soldiers throughout the deployment cycle, San operations: Military leaders strategies for addressing stress
Francisco, August. and psychological support (Paper 42, pp. 42–1–42–6).
Adler, A. B., Bliese, P. D., McGurk, D., et al. (2008). Writing Neuilly-sur-Seine, France: NATO Research and
about the transition from combat to home: Initial Technology Association.
analysis. In Proceedings of the Force Health Protection Castro, C. A., homas, J. L., & Adler, A. B. (2006b). Towards
Conference, Albuquerque, August. a liberal theory of military leadership. In A. B. Adler,
Adler, A. B., Bliese, P. D., McGurk, D., Hoge, C. W., & Castro, C. A. Castro, & T. W. Britt (eds.), Military life: the
C. A. (2009a). Battlemind debrieing and battlemind psychology of serving in peace and combat, Vol. 2:
training as early interventions with soldiers returning Operational stress (pp. 192–212). Westport, CT: Praeger.
from Iraq: Randomization by platoon. Journal of Cornum, R., Matthews, M. D., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2011).
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 7, 928–940. Comprehensive soldier itness: Building resilience in a
challenging institutional context. American Psychologist,
Adler, A. B., Castro, C. A., & McGurk, D. (2009b). Time-
66, 4–9.
driven Battlemind psychological debrieing: A group-
level early intervention in combat. Military Medicine, Deahl, M. P., Srinivasan, M., Jones, N., Neblett, C., &
174, 22–28. Jolly, A. (2001). Commentary. Evaluating psychological
debrieing: Are we measuring the right outcomes?
Bliese, P. D. & Castro, C. A. (2003). he soldier adaptation
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 527–529.
model (SAM): applications to peacekeeping research.
In T. W Britt & A. B. Adler (eds.), he psychology of Department of the Army (2007). Deployment cycle support
the peacekeeper: Lessons from the ield (pp. 185–204). (DCS) directive. Washington, DC: Department of the
Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing. Army, http://www.apd.army.mil/pdiles/ad2007_02.pdf
(accessed February 16, 2011).
Bliese, P. D., Adler, A. B., & Castro, C. A. (2011). he
deployment context: Psychology and implementing Donner, A. & Klar, N. (2004). Pitfalls and controversies in
mental health interventions. In A. B. Adler, P. D. cluster randomized trials. American Journal of Public
Bliese, & C. A. Castro (eds.), Deployment psychology: Health, 94, 416–422.
evidence-based strategies to promote mental health in the Iversen, A. C., Fear, N. T., Hacker Huges, J., et al. (2008).
military (pp. 103–124). Washington, D.C.: American Risk factors for post-traumatic stress disorder among
Psychological Association. UK armed forces personnel. Psychological Medicine, 29,
Britt, T. W., Davison, J., Bliese, P. D., & Castro, C. A. (2004). 1–12.
How leaders can inluence the impact that stressors have Lester, P. B., McBride, S. A., Bliese, P. D. & Adler, A. B.
on soldiers. Military Medicine, 169, 541–545. (2011). Bringing science to bear: An empirical

338
Chapter 22: Military mental health training

assessment of the Comprehensive Soldier Fitness homas, J. L. & Castro, C. A. (2003). Organizational
program. American Psychologist, 66, 77–81. behavior and the US peacekeeper. In T. W Britt &
McClelland, G. H. & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical A. B. Adler (eds.), he psychology of the peacekeeper:
diiculties of detecting interactions and moderator lessons from the ield (pp. 127–146). Westport, CT:
efects. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 376–390. Greenwood.
McGurk, D., Castro, C. A., homas, J. L., & Hoge, C. W. homas, J. L., Castro, C. A., Adler, A. B., et al. (2007). he
(2007). Pre-deployment Battlemind Training. In eicacy of Battlemind at immediate post deployment
Symposium of the Annual Meeting of the American reintegration. In Symposium of the Annual Meeting of the
Psychological Association on the Battlemind Training American Psychological Association on the Battlemind
System: Supporting soldiers throughout the deployment Training System: Supporting soldiers throughout the
cycle, San Francisco, August. deployment cycle, San Francisco, August.
McKibben, E. S., Britt, T. W., Hoge, C. W., & Castro, C. A. hompson, M. M. & McCreary, D. R. (2006). Enhancing
(2009). Receipt and rated adequacy of stress mental readiness in military personnel. In A. B.
management training is related to PTSD and other Adler, C. A. Castro, & T. W. Britt (eds.), Military life:
outcomes among Operational Iraqi Freedom veterans. he psychology of serving in peace and combat, Vol. 2:
Military Psychology, 21(Suppl. 2), S68–S81. Operational stress (pp. 54–79). Westport, CT: Praeger
Security International.
Mental Health Advisory Team (2008). Operation Iraqi
Freedom 06–08: Iraq, Operation Enduring Freedom 8. US Army Medical Command (2007). Operations Order
[Afghanistan Report February 14, 2008.] Washington, 07–48: Implementation of the Battlemind Training System.
DC: US Army Medical Department, http://www. Washington, DC: US Army Medical Command.
armymedicine.army.mil/reports/mhat/mhat.html Varnell, S. P., Murray, D. M., Janega, J. B., & Blitstein, J. L.
(accessed August 30, 2009). (2004). Design and analysis of group-randomized trials:
Mulligan, K., Fear, N. T., Jones, N. Wesseley, S. & Greenberg, A review of recent practices. American Journal of Public
N. (2011). Psycho-educational interventions designed Health, 94, 393–399.
to prevent deployment-related psychological ill-health Zamorski, M. (2008). Deining resilience: An international
in armed forces personnel: A review. Psychological perspective. In International Military Testing Association
Medicine, 41, 673–686. Meeting Symposium on Military Resiliency: Perspectives
Rosenthal, R. & Rubin, D. B. (1982). A simple, general from an International Technical Panel, Amsterdam,
purpose display of magnitude of experimental efect. September.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 166–169.
Saint, C. E. (1992). Battlemind guidelines for battalion Endnotes
commanders. United States Army, European OPE, and 1. he concept of mental health training was a topic of the sympo-
Seventh Army Training Manual, Heidelberg, Germany : sium Technical Panel (TP) 13 (Castro, 2008), which examined the
US Army European OPE. psychological health and operational efectiveness domains. he
symposium was part of the activities of the Technical Cooperation
Seligman, M. E. P. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive
Program, which is an international collaboration between the
psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, USA, Canada, Australia, New Zealand, and the UK. he TP-13
5–14. members who prepared the deinitions of training and resilience
Sharpley, J. G., Fear, N. T., Greenberg, N., Jones, M., & and who participated in dicussions regarding fundamental prin-
Wessely, S. (2008). Pre-deployment stress brieing: does it ciples of mental health training are Mark Zamorski, Carl Castro,
have an efect? Occupational Medicine, 58, 30–34. Neil Greenberg, Amy Adler, Paul Cawkill, Delwyn Neill, Kerry
Sudom, and Alan Twomey.
Shipherd, J. C. & Salters-Pedneault, K. (2008). Attention, 2. Note that this deinition difers from the initial deinition pre-
memory, intrusive thoughts, and acceptance in PTSD: sented in post-deployment Battlemind Training. he initial def-
An update on the empirical literature for clinicians. inition was “Battlemind is the Soldier’s inner strength to face fear
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 15, 349–363. and adversity in combat with courage.” he deinition presented in
Slone, L. B. & Friedman, M. J. (2008). Ater the war zone: this chapter more accurately relects the scope of the Battlemind
A practical guide for returning troops and their families. Training System. It was co-developed by Carl Castro, Amy Adler,
Cambridge, MA: Da Capo Press. Dennis McGurk, and Michael Rinehart in 2009.

339
Section
Section5 Training for resilience
Chapter
Public health practice and disaster

23 resilience: a framework integrating


resilience as a worker protection strategy
Dori B. Reissman, Kathleen M. Kowalski-Trakoler, and Craig L. Katz

Introduction purview of both disciplines has varied over time and


in response to events, legislative initiatives, and lead-
he disciplines of emergency management and public
ership styles. For the purposes of this chapter, a disas-
health share objectives in hazard identiication and the
ter is deined as a serious disruption in the functioning
application of control or prevention strategies in the
of a local jurisdiction that poses a signiicant level of
advent of disasters. his chapter deines resilience as
threat to life, health, property, or the environment and
it pertains to workers converging at a disaster site to
requires outside assistance to manage or cope with the
perform response, recovery, or clean-up operations. A
event (International Federation of Red Cross and Red
conceptual framework will be applied to disaster situ-
Crescent Societies, 2000). In common parlance, disas-
ations to highlight intervention avenues to preserve or
ters are local events and directly impact communities.
enhance the resilience of the workers. Possible inter-
ventions include worker training and education, med- Community resilience
ical and emotional support services, disaster safety
he term community resilience is used in multiple,
management, and alignment of organizational culture,
and oten vaguely deined, ways. Emergency manage-
policy, and procedures.
ment uses the term “disaster-resilient community” to
The disciplines of public health and describe buildings, bridges, and roadways designed to
withstand expected weather and geological hazards.
emergency management Public policy circles use the term resilience to encour-
Public health is “the science and art of preventing age a degree of self-suiciency for deined jurisdictions
disease, prolonging life and promoting health and to provide immediate emergency services and adequate
eiciency through organized community efort” food, water, medical help, and law enforcement to
(Winslow, 1920). Epidemiology is the basic science preserve social order in the face of disaster. National
of public health research, policy, and practice; which preparedness planning has applied the concept of
provides methods to study factors afecting the health resiliency to macro-level systems redundancy, equip-
and illness of populations (Institute of Medicine, ment back-up, and alternative sources for raw mate-
2002). Modern public health practice requires alli- rials, inal goods and services, including information
ance among many disciplines and emotionally charged management (US Department of Homeland Security,
points of view, particularly when health and safety 2009). However, these approaches focus on harden-
become challenged by disasters. Public health and ing the human-built environment and enhancing the
emergency management share the objectives of iden- systems that sustain people, but they do not address
tifying hazards and managing risk, particularly before human behavior directly. he disaster literature is full
a disaster strikes (Haddow & Bullock, 2006). When of accounts where poor planning and lack of prepared-
looking across these disciplines, risk would be deined ness for the emotional, social, and behavioral conse-
as susceptibility to death, injury, and illness to people; quences were likely to have delayed or compromised
damage or destruction of the human-built or natural recovery eforts (Flynn, 2003). Disaster events have
environment; and disruption or stoppage of services been associated with health-risk behaviors, exacerba-
that support people from a population perspective. he tion of chronic medical conditions, incitementof new

Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan, ed. Steven M. Southwick, Brett T. Litz, Dennis Charney, and
Matthew J. Friedman. Published by Cambridge University Press. © Cambridge University Press 2011.

340
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

psychiatric disturbances, and unexplained physical and/or safety of community members can be inlu-
symptoms associated with decrements in function or enced through interventions aimed at deined social
quality of life (Katz et al., 2002; Fullerton et al., 2003; networks, local governance, and critical infrastructure
Institute of Medicine, 2003; Engel, 2004). However, pathways supplying products and services needed to
inadequate planning, personnel, and resources for this sustain a community in the face of disaster (Schoch-
arena continue to plague emergency managers (Flynn, Spana, 2008). A recent review of the literature across
2006). several disciplines led Norris and colleagues (2008,
In 2006, the term community resilience was dir- p. 130) to deine community resilience as “a process
ectly introduced in the Pandemic Inluenza and linking a set of adaptive capacities to a positive trajec-
All Hazards Preparedness Act (Public Law number tory of functioning and adaptation ater a disturbance”
109–417) and the Homeland Security Presidential (such as a disaster). heir framework deined adapta-
Directive 21 (US Department of Homeland Security, tion in terms of an ability to function and they went
2007). Community resilience was contextualized on to comment (p. 132) that post-disaster function is
(paragraph 20) as: “not necessarily superior in level or character or efect-
Where local civic leaders, citizens, and families are edu- iveness to pre-event functioning; it is simply diferent.”
cated regarding threats and are empowered to mitigate his is a departure from other conceptualizations of
their own risk, where they are practiced in responding to collective resilience, which incorporates the elem-
events, where they have social networks to fall back upon, ent of learning and growth from the disaster, a trans-
and where they have familiarity with local public health formative process catalyzed by the disaster experience
and medical systems, there will be community resilience (Brown & Kulig, 1996/97; Tedeschi et al., 1998; Paton
that will signiicantly attenuate the requirement for add- & Johnston, 2006; Masten & Obradović, 2008). Norris
itional assistance.
and colleagues (2008) provided a theoretical model
his led the US Department of Health and Human that deines economic development (continuity of
Services (DHHS) to convene a federal advisory com- business, availability of jobs), social capital (collective
mittee (the National Biodefense Science Board) for value of social networks), information and communi-
assistance with strategy and implementation. A special cation, and community competence (ability to address
expert subcommittee was initiated to provide guidance the challenges) as the dynamic domains of adaptive
to the Secretary of the DHHS about operationalizing capacity at the collective community level. heir model
community resilience as a health protection strat- utilizes an ecological framework of the interdependen-
egy (US Department of Homeland Security, 2007).1 cies between people, institutions, and organizations to
A report capturing the deliberations of this expert mobilize resources to adapt to the new reality created
advisory body was passed to DHHS in November 2008 by the disaster. It incorporates the process of collective
(Disaster Mental Health Subcommittee of the National problem solving and a common understanding about
Biodefense Science Board, 2008); it began with the fol- respective roles and responsibilities of various private,
lowing paragraph: public, and volunteer entities in advance of a disaster.
Disaster mental and behavioral health…, includes the In a book on disaster resilience, Paton and Johnston
interconnected psychological, emotional, cognitive, (2006, p. 8) deine the challenge of adapting to the new
developmental, and social inluences on behavior and reality as “a measure of how well people and societies
mental health and the impact of those factors on pre- can adapt to a changed reality and capitalize on the new
paredness, response, and recovery from disasters or possibilities ofered.”
traumatic events. hese factors directly and indirectly
inluence individual and community risks for health A framework to guide interventions
and safety outcomes such as substance use and abuse,
aggression and non-adherence to public health recom- promoting resilience in disaster
mendations (e.g., medication regimens, infection con- workers
trol, and evacuation or restricted movement), and the
he remainder of this chapter will focus on the resili-
success of emergency response strategies and public
directives…
ence of workers (ailiated and volunteer) converging
at disaster sites to perform response, recovery, and/or
Community resilience is sometimes used in multiple, clean-up operations. In essence, the concept of commu-
and oten vaguely deined ways to imply that the health nity will be targeted to the workers, their organizations,

341
Section 5: Training for resilience

Table 23.1 The Haddon matrix template.

Physical Sociocultural
Host Agent or vehicle environment environment
Pre-event
Event
Post-event

and the dynamic disaster work environment. he will- and how it is transmitted (vehicle), and deining the
ingness and ability for workers to comply with safe circumstances or settings that bring the host and agent
work practices is greatly inluenced by each worker’s together resulting in the adverse health or safety out-
awareness of the hazards, the existence of a safety plan come (environment). he rows represent time phases
and availability of personal protective equipment, the (i.e., before, during, and ater) in relation to a deined
culture of the responding and afected organizations, event (e.g., car crash, infectious disease, or disaster).
and the peculiarities of the incident itself (Jackson his systematic approach has broad application
et al., 2004; Qureshri et al., 2005; Zohar, 2010). As such, for public health practice (Institute of Medicine, 2003;
it will be important to integrate resilience within the Runyan, 2003; Barnett et al., 2005). he following text
organizational culture and the activities designed to outlines how to use the Haddon matrix to deine a
protect the health and safety of workers (Flynn, 2006; health or safety outcome under evaluation. he chap-
Reissman et al., 2006a; Ursano et al., 2006; Reissman & ter will then give two case studies where the Haddon
Howard, 2008). he approach to interventions that pre- matrix is applied to two disasters to explore possible
serve or enhance disaster resilience for workers must interventions to preserve or enhance resilience for
be tailored to the work organization, the anticipated workers involved in disaster response, recovery, and
disaster scenario, and the training and experience of clean-up operations.
the workers themselves. his requires systematic ana- he disaster literature describes the following
lysis and evaluation as part of intervention planning. potential outcomes: health-risk coping behaviors
he analysis beneits from combining expertise in a (smoking, drinking, recklessness), exacerbation of
public health practice framework: speciically applying underlying medical conditions (e.g., hypertension,
classic models of causation from epidemiology within peptic ulcer disease, asthma, chronic anxiety, thought
the ields of occupational health and safety and behav- or mood disorders), and the incidence of new anxiety
ioral and psychological health. and mood disorders (Katz et al., 2002; Fullerton et al.,
2003; Institute of Medicine, 2003; Engel, 2004). For
The Haddon matrix: a tool for developing the purpose of this chapter, the rows can be assigned
according to the disaster cycle in emergency man-
interventions to enhance resilience in the agement: preparedness and mitigation (pre-event),
disaster worker community response (during the event), and recovery (post-
Designing interventions for public health practice event). he adverse health and safety outcomes of con-
requires the development of a logic model to ascer- cern are stipulated as inluenced by perceived threats
tain reasonable opportunities for intervention and to to resilience: the “agent”, such as traumatic loss (e.g.,
evaluate the impact of the resources applied. William sudden death of a co-worker or loved one), harm (e.g.,
Haddon, Jr. developed such a model to enable a sys- serious injury), or change (e.g., loss of job/income,
tematic multidisciplinary analysis of the contributing home/place) resulting from the disaster. As mentioned
factors to injury from traic crashes, which enabled a above, the “environment” is deined collectively as the
comprehensive program of highway safety to be devel- work site(s) associated with the disaster event. So,
oped. his is known as the Haddon matrix (Haddon, in looking more closely at the host, one could deine
1968, 1970, 1972) and is illustrated in Table 23.1. he intrapersonal and interpersonal factors for the worker
columns capture the contributing factors using basic as having a bearing on vulnerability and resilience.
epidemiology as the underpinning: namely identifying he environment is broken into a physical (human-
who is harmed (host), what is causing the harm (agent), built or natural landscape) and the social and cultural

342
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

components. he sociocultural aspects may include Box 23.1 Lessons from prior disaster responses
the interaction within (co-worker or management
• It is difficult to prepare responders for everything
support, communication, resources, safety and admin-
they might encounter
istrative policies) and between (coordination and con-
trol) the work organizations involved in the disaster, • Even seasoned responders can face situations
and issues that cause uneasiness and distress
the community and workforce behavioral norms and
beliefs, and the inluence of regulatory aspects (laws • It is not unusual for responders to be asked to
and enforcement). work outside their areas of expertise
here are several typologies used to classify dis- • Concerns about family and friends rank high on
asters for the purpose of studying mental and behav- responders’ lists of priorities
ioral response. Disasters may be caused by forces of • Timely, accurate, and candid information should
nature or by human action. Examples of natural forces be shared to facilitate decision making
include extreme weather events (hurricanes, torna- • Managers, at every level, need to consider the
dos, cyclones), geological disturbances (earthquakes, health, safety, and resiliency of workers on the
volanic eruptions), or severe infectious epidemics job as part of situation awareness and for staged
(severe acute respiratory syndrome, pandemic inlu- planning (implies needs for occupational health
enza). Technological or industrial disasters may occur and wellness monitoring)
from human neglect, error, or by deliberate and harm- • Resiliency is an integral component of
ful actions. People who perceive a loss of control (no occupational safety and health, which requires
pre-planning to maximize worker recovery
warning, unable to avoid, lack of coping skills) or have
inadequate social support in relation to the disaster • Self-care plans and peer-support activities are
are more likely to have more severe or longer lasting essential to mission completion
psychopathology (Fullerton et al., 2003; Kaniasty & • Everything possible should be done to safeguard
Norris, 2004). Box 23.1 provides some key observa- responders’ physical and emotional health
tions gleaned from prior disasters, which serve as a • Responders do not need to face response
context for the case studies to be discussed in the next challenges alone; they may share their
section. experiences with buddies, teammates, family
members, and colleagues
• It is particularly difficult for responders to
CASE 1: the World Trade Center maintain emotional distance when they witness
terrorist attack on September 11, 2001 the deaths of children
At least 40 000 individuals are estimated to have • Organizational differences among groups of
responded to the World Trade Center (WTC) site responders and cultural differences between
following the September 11 2001 (9/11) terrorist victims and responders can impede the timely
and efficient provision of emergency services
attacks, encompassing a broad array of traditional irst
responders, workers as diverse as laborers and morgue • Individuals may be thrust into leadership roles for
staf, and ailiated and unailiated volunteers (Herbert which they have had little to no formal training
et al., 2006). hese responders encountered well-char-
acterized physical hazards (including chemical tox-
ins) and emotional traumatogens. It is for this reason relevant to non-responders as well. he 9/11 respond-
and others that their experience provides an ideal lens ers also have had access to a unique long-term medical
through which to look at the public health dimensions follow-up program, the information from which has
of resilience amid catastrophe. led to much published discussion about issues rele-
Public health observations about the responder vant to the well-being of the responder community in
population may in fact be particularly generalizable the event of future large-scale catastrophic events.
to other catastrophic events because of the vast size Although not intended to be exhaustive, Table 23.2
of this population and its associated heterogeneity. As and the following discussion exempliies the applica-
responders are members of the broader “disaster com- tion of the Haddon matrix to the subject of resilience
munity” that grows up around a disaster (Wright et al., in responders. When the 9/11 rescue and recovery
1990), lessons regarding their resilience should be response is overlaid with the Haddon matrix, the

343
Section 5: Training for resilience

Table 23.2 Haddon matrix of public health measures promoting the resilience of responders to the World Trade Center attacks.

Host Physical Social


(WTC responders) Agent environment environment
Pre-event Integrate routine Emergency response Biohazard training Promote workplace
mental health services training, including clear environment of
into employee health role definition openness and
programming support around
mental health and
resilience
Event Train and deploy Institute policies for Promote/require Promote a
providers of psychological monitoring and use of personal disaster response
first aid; identify high-risk limiting exposure to protective environment
responders Ground Zero equipment that emphasizes
responder,
workplace, and
family well-being
Post-event Establish a system for System of financial Establish a system Involve families in
long-term mental health support and vocational for long-term long-term mental
follow-up and care rehabilitation for physical health health follow-up
affected responders follow-up and care and care

boxes highlight true accomplishments, missed oppor- Agent


tunities, and lessons learned regarding public health Adequate training in disaster response is essential for
measures relevant to resiliency. ensuring not only an efective response but also the
well-being of responders themselves. An analysis of
Pre-event 28 962 registrants in the WTC Health Registry who
identiied themselves as having been rescue or recovery
Host workers at the WTC site found that rates of post-trau-
Planning in the pre-disaster peroid afords the oppor- matic stress disorder (PTSD) were lowest in police and
tunity to deploy a robust cohort of responders in the highest in unailiated volunteers (Perrin et al., 2007).
event of disaster. A responder base that begins from a his seems to underscore the importance of training
healthy position is best positioned to manage itself and and experience in bolstering mental health and coping
the response in a healthy and efective manner (Bills during disaster. Indeed, the study also found that risk
et al., 2008). In a sample of the irst 1138 participants of PTSD was heightened if responders reported hav-
in a medical/mental health screening and treatment ing performed tasks at “Ground Zero” atypical for their
program for 9/11 responders, ultimately known as the occupation.
WTC Medical Monitoring Program, only 3% reported Public health-oriented management of future dis-
ever having previously seen a mental health profes- aster responses would, therefore, include better gate-
sional (Smith et al., 2004). keeping around unailiated volunteers ill prepared to
Ensuring better access to mental health services handle what awaits them. In addition, emergency man-
to responders pre-disaster should enhance resiliency agement agencies can work to ensure that partner agen-
when disaster strikes. To achieve this requires a system cies provide fully trained and credentialed responders
of capable and available mental health providers who and that these responders adhere to their roles as best
can be reimbursed for their services. his may be easier as circumstances permit.
for disaster response agencies that rely on employees
rather than volunteers, but voluntary agencies could Physical environment
also mandate receiving proof of a “mental health check he attacks on the WTC generated exposure to phys-
up” for all potential volunteers. ical contaminants, which has been the subject of much

344
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

subsequent discussion (Herbert et al., 2006). he toxins recovery work at the WTC, despite anecdotal reports
derived from the thousands of liters of exploded jet fuel of the availability of these services. One organization,
and the tons of coarse and ine particular matter gener- Disaster Psychiatry Outreach, did “staf ” Ground Zero
ated by the collapse of the towers themselves, airborne ater the 9/11 attacks with volunteer psychiatrists,
exposure to which was perpetuated by days to weeks although minimal documentation is available regard-
of smoldering ires. Emergency response training must ing the interventions (McQuistion & Katz, 2001).
include consideration of the potential biohazards to he experience of 9/11 responders has suggested
which responders may be exposed. Although it may be the need for a comprehensive mental health program
impossible to anticipate, or at least meaningfully edu- that spans the immediate and long-term atermath of
cate about, all possible physical exposures, anyone who disaster and that includes real-time surveillance, treat-
trains to participate in disaster response should at least ment, and study of service utilization and impact (Bills
be trained to ask two general questions: what are the et al., 2008). his program should smoothly link imme-
potential physical exposures and who is monitoring diate and long-term mental health services (Garakani
occupational safety? his will be particularly import- et al., 2004). Psychological irst aid was widely prom-
ant in the event of a nuclear, chemical, or biological ter- ulgated in the atermath of 9/11, providing a standard
rorist attack, although it may be those very cases where for basic mental health intervention that public health
the issue is most salient. Emergency response agencies authorities may implement in future disasters. In keep-
can mandate basic “all-hazards” training to be an elem- ing with a key aspect of psychological irst aid, and in
ent of the training programs of their partner agencies. the setting of scarce resources, future peri-event men-
tal health interventions can be targeted at higher-risk
Social environment groups within the responder and general population.
Even if employers provide for capable, available, and A host of well-documented risk factors for developing
afordable pre-event mental health services for disaster post-disaster psychopathology have been established
responders, another hurdle remains – stigma. he chal- in the general psychiatric literature (Garakani et al.,
lenge therein lies to create a workplace environment 2004) and in the 9/11 responder literature (Katz et al.,
that promotes mental well-being and characterizes 2009). hese span prior psychiatric history, extent of
the action of seeking out mental health professionals exposure to a disaster such as 9/11, and availability of
as not only acceptable but also desirable. his chal- post-disaster social supports; this suggests a picture of
lenge likely extends beyond the purview or capacity of who among a disaster-afected population may be less
any one employer to that of a community as a whole, resilient and need more assistance.
including its departments of health or mental health.
A discussion of de-stigmatization is beyond the scope Agent
of this paper, but, for example, models for addressing he disaster literature is replete with evidence that
workplace depression have been developed (Bilsker “dose of exposure” to a disaster or other trauma cor-
et al., 2007). relates with likelihood of sufering psychiatrically in
its atermath (Katz et al., 2002). his association has
Event also been shown in 9/11 responders, where expos-
ure was relected in such dimensions as having dir-
Host ectly witnessed the attacks, been exposed to the dust
he evidence base for mental health interventions in cloud, lost someone, or spent more time at Ground
the acute atermath of disaster is limited, leading to reli- Zero (Bills et al., 2008). Regarding the last, among the
ance on general principles for intervention, including WTC Health Registry’s 28 962 responders, arriving at
promoting a sense of safety, calm, hope, self-eicacy, the WTC site soon ater 9/11 and total duration of time
and connectedness (Hobfoll et al., 2007). Consensus spent working at the site (from 1 to 200 days) were each
exists around a set of basic interventions known as an exposure variable that correlated with likelihood of
“psychological irst aid” for addressing the psycho- having PTSD in all responders except police (Perrin
logical equivalent of the bumps and scrapes of trauma et al., 2007).
(Brymer et al., 2006). However, little is known about Emotional exposure represents a multifaceted con-
the nature and extent of mental health services pro- struct that includes at least one major modiiable fac-
vided to responders in the course of their rescue and tor – duration of exposure. Communities need their

345
Section 5: Training for resilience

responders to rush in quickly. However, the scientiic family tensions may become exacerbated (Katz et al.
literature suggests that limiting responders’ subse- 2006a). In addition, it must be considered whether the
quent duration of exposure at places like the WTC site immersion in disaster response represents, at least in
should promote the resilience of the responders and, part, an unhealthy light from personal problems (Katz
therefore, be in the interest of the community that has a & Nathaniel, 2002).
stake in their well-being. Responders’ time of exposure It should be possible for response agencies to
to a disaster could be monitored according to at least “preach” balance to their responders in the course of
two parameters: total time spent on-site and how much training and emergency response planning, thereby
time of occurs within that time period. While further making it easier to “practice” balance during the inher-
study will be required to calibrate the former thresh- ently unbalanced state of eventual disaster. Attempts
old, common sense should be of more use in guiding can be made to foster disaster response environments
public health decision making on the latter. that accentuate the need for decisive action without
siphoning excessively from the responder’s self, home,
Physical environment or workplace. Speciic approaches to this include invit-
One of the major exposures sufered by WTC respond- ing responders to examine their motivations, devis-
ers, toxins, has already been alluded to. In a study of ing “buddy systems” for responders to look ater one
the irst 9442 WTC responders who participated in another in the course of a disaster, and prioritizing lit-
a medical and mental health screening program, the eral lines of communication between responders and
WTC Worker and Volunteer Screening Program, 69% their home environments.
reported new or worsened respiratory symptoms at
the time of working at the WTC site and 59% reported Post-event
persistent complaints at the time of their examination
up to two and a half years later (Herbert et al., 2006). Host and physical environment
As with emotional exposure, earlier time of arrival he WTC Worker and Volunteer Medical Screening
correlated with respiratory symptoms and indings. Program and the associated WTC Worker and
Likewise, in the WTC Health Registry’s cohort, time Volunteer Mental Health Screening Program were
of arrival as well as duration of exposure at the WTC established in July 2002 through a combination of fed-
site correlated with the likelihood of newly diagnosed eral and philanthropic funding in order to screen WTC
asthma (Wheeler et al., 2007). responders for physical and mental health conditions
Importantly, the WTC Health Registry study found associated with the disaster (Herbert et al., 2006; Katz
that delays in use of respiratory masks, and to a lesser et al., 2006a). Originally funded as a one-year program,
degree total days without using masks, correlated with what is now called the WTC Medical Monitoring and
the likelihood of developing asthma (Wheeler et al., Treatment Program (MMTP) has evolved into an
2007). he authors of the study concluded that timely ongoing multiyear program that includes physical and
and appropriate use of respiratory masks, along with mental health screenings, follow-up examinations, on-
programs that enforce this usage, should reduce future site treatment services, and case management services
respiratory exposures in the event of disasters that (Katz et al., 2006b). A similar but separate program
involve airborne contaminants. his relects a hard- exists for ireighters from the Fire Department of New
learned lesson from 9/11 that can inform future public York. As of May 2008, over 24 000 WTC responders had
health planning around disasters. undergone initial screenings across the consortium of
non-ire department clinical sites in the metropolitan
Social environment New York City area, funded by the National Institutes
In the necessary rush to respond and help ater a dis- of Occupational Safety and Health. Details of the clin-
aster such as 9/11, there is great potential for neglect ical programming are described elsewhere (Smith
of life beyond the disaster site. Self, family, and work- et al., 2004; Herbert et al., 2006; Katz et al., 2006a).
place may all be neglected in the course of becoming To the best of our knowledge, the MMTP and its
immersed in rescue and recovery eforts. When the forerunner, the WTC Worker and Volunteer Medical
length of a disaster response spans months, as it did at Screening Program, represent programs of unprece-
the WTC site, this neglect may have signiicant ill con- dented scope and magnitude in addressing the med-
sequences in any or all of these domains. For example, ical and mental health needs of disaster responders.

346
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

Congress appropriated $323 million to support both problems such as PTSD is necessary, particularly given
responder and non-responder WTC medical surveil- the modest efect and evidence base for most pharma-
lance and treatment programs between 2003 and 2008 cological and psychotherapeutic treatment modalities
(Gerberding, 2008), relecting the magnitude and the (Shalev et al., 1996).
cost of these programs. he MMTP represents a post- One clinical center within the MMTP has created
event public health safety net to capture 9/11-associated a family intervention program based on a recent New
health and mental health problems arising despite York State Oice of Mental Health initiative for involv-
whatever pre-event and event-related health measures ing families in the treatment of patients with chronic
were undertaken, and because of those measures that mental illness (Salerno et al., 2008; McKay, personal
perhaps were overlooked. Although future research communication, 2009). Having such a specialized unit
will be needed to determine the full impact of these within a broader medical/mental health program may
WTC programs, the MMTP serves as the best available serve as a model for broad-based public health plan-
working model for addressing post-event resiliency ning around future disasters.
through public medical and mental healthcare.

Agent
CASE 2: the Sago Mine disaster on
We are not aware of any peer-reviewed publications January 2, 2006
on the issue of disability related to working or volun-
teering as a 9/11 responder. However, clinical obser- Background
vations from the MMTP by one of the authors (CLK) It is useful for the reader to have some understanding
suggest that it is a major issue for many patients, with of the mine environment in order to appreciate the
workers’ compensation claims constituting a frequent inherent danger of the workplace and relate to the case
dimension of both physical and mental healthcare. study. A mine is broadly deined as an opening or exca-
he inability to work as a result of their exposure at the vation in the earth created for the purpose of extract-
WTC site deprives 9/11 responders of both income and ing minerals (hrush et al., 1968). Coal is mined either
fulillment. on the surface of the earth or underground. To extract
As of 2006, Congress had awarded $105 million coal from an underground mine, the coalbed (or seam)
to New York State in support of 9/11-related worker must be accessed from the surface. he term “portal”
compensation claims (Gerberding, 2008). A portion of is generally given to any entrance that provides access
this funding has also gone toward planning for future to a coal mine. In hilly terrain, such as is found in the
disasters. he public health impact of providing work- Appalachian coalields in the eastern USA, the coal
ers’ compensation on the resilience of 9/11 responders may form an outcrop on a hillside, which allows direct
remains to be investigated but can be predicted to be a entry to the coal seam via a horizontal tunnel or “drit”
signiicant ingredient in post-event recovery. Clinical opening. At other locations, it may be possible to open
observations suggest that, whatever the magnitude a “slope” tunnel that angles down from the surface and
of this impact it could be increased by better pair- intersects with the coal seam. If the coal seam is too
ing of compensation with vocational rehabilitation deep for a slope to be feasible, a “shat” must be con-
programming. structed. he shat, which may be 20 feet (12.5 m) or
more in diameter, extends vertically from the surface
Social environment to the seam and is accessed via a large elevator. Once
he need for better connections between respond- inside the underground mine, miners are transported
ers and their families during a catastrophic event has to various worksites underground, sometimes miles
already been discussed. In a similar fashion, families away. he transportation system is installed for moving
should be involved in post-event mental health treat- workers, supplies, equipment, and the extracted coal.
ment programming for responders. he rationale is In an underground coal mine, workers must con-
twofold: the mental health problems of responders ater tend with poorly illuminated work areas that can be
9/11 may afect their partners and children, while men- dusty, uneven, and wet and muddy depending upon the
tal health issues in the family may exacerbate the 9/11- amount of water present. Work areas can be extremely
related mental health issues of the responders. Experts conined, particularly in mines with a low seam coal
have generally agreed that a multifaceted approach to height (i.e., less than 5 feet [1.5 m] high). In low seams,

347
Section 5: Training for resilience

coal miners work on their knees or backs during their and Health Administration 1998a, 1998b; Kowalski-
entire shit. To extract the coal, miners must operate Trakoler et al., 2009).
large, noisy, technically sophisticated equipment in During a ive-month period in 2006, three under-
these conined work areas. he only lighting available ground coal mining incidents in the USA resulted in
comes from the miner’s battery-operated cap lamp and the deaths of 19 miners. All three incidents received
from localized sources on various mining machines. In nationwide attention, particularly the Sago Mine dis-
addition, the coal mining process releases dangerous aster in West Virginia. hese incidents represented a
gases, including methane, which is highly explosive departure from the recent trends in underground coal
and needs to be monitored. Miners may also have to mining safety (Figure 23.1). Before 2006, the number
deal with carbon monoxide should there be a ire or of mining disasters had decreased from a high of 20
explosion underground and be aware of oxygen dei- in 1909 to an average of one every four years during
ciency in locations that may not be adequately venti- 1985–2005.
lated. As per federal regulation, miners are required to
carry emergency respirators – self-contained self-res- The events of the Sago Mine disaster
cuers (SCSRs) – whenever they are inside the mine. he On Monday, January 2, 2006, at approximately 6:30
SCSRs are closed system breathing devices designed to a.m., an explosion occurred at Wolf Run Mining
provide the miner with a protected oxygen supply that Company’s Sago Mine in West Virginia, leading to the
lasts one hour. Miners are mandated to receive appro- death of 12 miners. hirteen miners were working in the
priate training on when and how to properly use the vicinity of the explosion. One miner was working alone
SCSRs. he SCSR is for emergency escape from the about 1500 feet (450 m) from the others and was killed
mine when the air is thought to be contaminated with instantly by the forces from the explosion. he other 12
toxic gas (i.e., carbon monoxide). were members of a coal production crew: of this group,
In summary, the mining environment is danger- 11 perished from carbon monoxide poisoning. he sole
ous and the hazards are dynamic in nature – constantly survivor from the production crew was able to give tes-
changing as the mining process creates new space timony in the ensuing investigations conducted by the
(Kowalski-Trakoler & Barrett, 2003). Our example, Mine Safety and Health Administration (Gates et al.,
the Sago Mine disaster in West Virginia, is a drit mine 2007) and the West Virginia Oice of Miners’ Health,
opening into the Middle Kittanning coal seam through Safety, and Training (2006). hese oicial reports con-
ive drit openings. Battery-powered track-mounted licted in some aspects in the description of events.
personnel carriers (mantrips) and locomotives were What is understood is that following the explosion
used to move the men and materials throughout the the crew attempted to escape by their transport vehicle
mine. Coal was transported to the surface by a series of (mantrip) but soon encountered debris that made their
conveyor belts. route impassable. he crew then entered their primary
escapeway on foot, a tunnel that would lead them to a
Historical trends portal and out of the mine. hey walked approximately
he safety of miners is dependent upon many inter- 1000 feet (305 m) through a potentially deadly atmos-
related factors including knowledge of the dynamic, phere before donning their SCSRs. hey returned to
ever-changing environment, the ability to recognize their original location at the coal face and erected a
and respond to hazards, training, experience, and barricade of plastic sheeting – which is part of standard
communication. In the USA, mine operators, federal training when escape is not possible. Reportedly, the
and state mine safety agencies, and researchers have miners took of their SCSRs while they were building
looked at numerous aspects of miner injuries and the barricade.
fatalities with regard to mitigating future incidents Ater the barricade was inished, all 12 men went
and increasing resilience amongst the population. behind it to wait for rescue teams to arrive. he only
he most intense eforts in this area have occurred survivor reported that there were not enough SCSRs
following major mine emergencies. Mining has a to go around since ater a time four miners were not
history of disasters. From 1900 to 2007, some 11 612 able to make their’s work, so individuals shared with
underground coal mine workers died in 514 difer- each other, trading of SCSRs between them. Besides
ent mining disasters, with most disasters resulting removing and replacing mouthpieces while sharing
from explosions or ires (DeMarchi, 1997; Mine Safety the apparatus, members of the crew also removed their

348
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

1000 25

US Bureau of Mines (1910) Fatalities (11 606 total)


Incidents (513 total)

800 20
Number of fatalities

600 15

Number of incidents
400 10
Federal Coal Mine Health
Federal Mine Safety &
& Safety Act (1969)
Health Act (1977)

Mine Improvement &


200 New Emergency 5
Response Act (2006)

0 0
1900

1905

1910

1915

1920

1925

1930

1935

1940

1945

1950

1955

1960

1965

1970

1975

1980

1985

1990

1995

2000

2005
Year
Figure 23.1 Underground coal mining disaster incidents and fatalities from 1900 to 2006. A mining disaster is an incident with five or more
fatalities. (Data from http://www.cdc.gov/niosh/mining/statistics/disasters.htm MSHA, accessed February 22, 2011.)

mouthpieces to talk. he West Virginia Oice of Miners’ the logistics of escape and rescue in an underground
Health, Safety, and Training Report of Investigation coal mine are complex, involving time and distance
concluded that the 11 miners succumbed to carbon to the portal. All miners are trained in emergency
monoxide poisoning. response. It takes time to assemble a mine rescue team,
Given the way that the SCSRs were utilized dur- who sometimes must travel a distance, assemble, be
ing the building of the barricade, it seems apparent briefed, and don their equipment. here is a basic mine
that they were not used correctly, at least not in that rescue protocol that has developed over the years based
instance. Evaluation of SCSRs removed from the mine on rescue eforts made during previous mine disasters,
revealed all were working. Lack of expectations and with a focus on the safety of the rescuers, monitoring of
training in how the devices work may have had some- the environment such as toxic gases, and constructing
thing to do with their misuse, which, in turn, led to the roof support as needed; consequently forward progress
miners’ inability to utilize the equipment fully. In add- can be slow. However, each mine disaster is unique and
ition, carbon monoxide poisoning may have afected presents its own challenges.
the miners’ reasoning and decision-making abilities To complicate matters, and create a second tra-
and may have contributed to how they utilized the gedy, families and the media were told that “all are okay
SCSRs during the building of the barricade. behind barricade – 12 men” (West Virginia Oice of
he irst mine rescue teams arrived about four and Miners’ Health, Safety, and Training, 2006). he dev-
a half hours ater the explosion, but they did not reach astating news came to the families and media over an
the barricaded miners until 40 hours ater the explo- hour later, that there was only one survivor and that
sion. his proved to be a critical issue. In mining, the he was in serious condition. Communications were
miners themselves are actually the “irst responders” as problematic because of the need to pass information

349
Section 5: Training for resilience

Table 23.3 Application of the Haddon matrix to the Sago Mine disaster, 2006.

Host Social environment


(underground coal Agent/vehicle (safety culture at
miners; mine emergency (death, injury, loss Physical mine/in the industry;
response personnel of income, penalty environment mine policies;
including mine rescue, fines, psychological (underground coal legal environment;
mine incident command) consequences) mine) community norms)
Pre-event Promote mental health Develop site Prevention through Promote workplace
and resilience behaviors; emergency plans engineering; support around mental
travel escape routes; teach (mandated); monitor gases; health and resilience;
proper behavior and use emergency response design emergency provide community
of personal protective training, know the communication and with ways to support
equipment in mine law; clearly marked protocols; provide each other; provide
emergency response escapeways; assigned equipment at specific disaster mental
roles; teach decision- mandated intervals health resources.
making points to (i.e., SCSRs, refuge Develop cooperation
escapees; security and shelters, etc.) with local emergency
media plan rescue teams, state and
federal personnel
Event On-site response Mandates and policies Monitor mine Provide information
immediate; trapped miners provide structure for conditions; build (shift/fatigue) and
follow escape procedures; response barricade; utilize support to incident
mine rescue protocol and refuge chamber command; provide
incident monitoring take protection and support
leadership; psychological for families and
first aid community
Post-event Long-term mental health Official report; cause; Evaluate cause; Involve miners, families,
follow-up and care; SCSR lessons learned; rescue make appropriate mine rescue, and
training; policy evaluation; engineering community in long-
develop interventions interventions to make term mental health
the mine safe; close follow-up; facilitate
mine; evaluation of policies;
cooperation; positive
support; MINER Act
passed
SCSR, self-contained self-rescue.

through multiple individuals. On the surface, there Analysis of the disaster using the Haddon
were two pager phones in the mine oice, one in the
dispatcher’s building and one just inside the mine matrix
portal, in addition to one near the mine rescue truck. To determine mine emergency prevention and inter-
here were over 100 people on the surface, many with vention strategies, the Haddon matrix is helpful in
cell phones. Although the site was being secured by organizing and examining the various aspects of pre-
police, the national media had set up observation posi- event, event, and post-event activities (Table 23.3).
tions for their camera crews; what was not known at here are more pre-event activities than in most other
the time was that some of these media crews had direc- emergency situations, mainly because of legal man-
tional listening devices pointed at the mine portal and dates and constraints, both state and federal, concern-
could hear the pager phone, further complicating oi- ing types of hazard, enforcement of safety regulations,
cial communication from the command center to the allowable technology, and particularly mandated
afected families. training in the mining industry. While the discussion

350
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

of the WTC disaster was covered by considering events or state police arrive on scene. In addition, mines des-
before, during, and ater the attack, the mine disaster ignate an area for families to gather and provide space
here is better illlustrated by considerations based on for media at a separate location. he essential goals are
the matrix column heads – host, agent, physical envir- to protect incident command in order to focus on the
onment, and social environment – at various stages emergency, provide for the families, and maintain oi-
within the disaster pathway. cial communications. Sago showed the industry that
with the advent of sophisticated technology, revisiting
Host security of both property and information communi-
he population or host for the Sago disaster is deined cation is important.
as the actors involved. All played a part in the event Post-event, the matrix can put into perspective
response from the moment the event was identiied. the need for SCSR training and the development of a
he actors are highly dependent upon each other. switch-over procedure from one SCSR unit to another,
Underground coal miners are actually their own irst as the units are designed to last one hour. here is also
responders, as they may be miles from the surface. It need for expectations training for miners so that they
may takes hours to assemble a mine rescue team, com- understand what they can anticipate when wearing
mand center, and support personnel. herefore, the a working unit. here were two ex-Sago miners who
host or responder is deined broadly to include the committed suicide eight months ater the disaster. he
escaping miners, the mine oice initial response per- company had provided “grief counseling” ater the dis-
sonnel (dispatcher), the eventual incident command aster and renewed the ofer ater the two suicides. here
(mine management, federal, state, union personnel) is no formal ongoing disaster mental health counseling
and the mine rescue teams, plus of course the miners’ ater a mine disaster. he Sago disaster emphasized the
families and the community. Pre-event for the host is need for such a service and the Mine Safety and Health
focused on training for emergencies, including under- Administration has recognized this need.
standing escapeways, practicing with equipment, and
reviewing emergency plans and procedures. he Sago The physical environment
miners completed mandated training, yet during the Much is known about the pre-event physical environ-
event they struggled with the use and operation of their ment in most mine disasters. Speciic mine hazards
SCSRs. Mine rescuers, who are highly trained, took 40 such as ires and explosions or inundations of gas
hours to reach the miners because of the environment or water are well known and continuously studied.
and the established protocols for advancement in toxic Underground coal mines are inspected by both federal
air. and state inspectors regularly. Maintaining a safe min-
ing environment is a key component to a safe and resili-
Agent ent workforce. At Sago, the explosion was most likely
he loss or harm (or agent) that results from an inci- caused by a lightning strike on the surface. It is believed
dent such as Sago is substantial. At Sago, there were 12 that energy from the lightning propagated through the
deaths; one injured miner; closure of the mine, resulting earth to a sealed, abandoned working area, igniting
in loss of livelihood for many in the community; legal methane gas that had accumulated in the old work-
and inancial penalties for the mine company; and long- ings, blasting out the walls between the abandoned and
and short-term mental health consequences. Pre-event active mine areas. During the event, the mine rescue
development of site-speciic emergency plans is man- team monitors the air in addition to drilling surface
dated. All miners must complete emergency response bore holes to lower sensors. he major focus of post-
training annually. Mine operators are expected to event interventions in the mining industry are engin-
know the law. Teaching miners decision-making eering controls. he irst line of defense is to engineer
points in escape should be an important component in out the hazard in the environment.
the training. During the event, escapeways should be
clearly identiied and emergency roles assigned. Sago The social environment
Mine provided the lesson that extensive security and Less is known about the pre-event social environ-
media plans need to be in place. Mines have plans in the ment: the safety climate at the mine. Most miners work
event of an emergency to cordon-of the mine prop- within a crew and buddy system and are taught to stay
erty. Mine personnel provide this function until local together in the event an escape is necessary. he Sago

351
Section 5: Training for resilience

crew exempliied their training. As the Haddon matrix feature in the MINER Act, which called for installation
deines social environment, policies and the legal envir- and maintenance of lame-resistant directional lifelines
onment are included, which are well deined in the in escapeways. Mine operators must install wireless
mining industry. herefore, the social environment two-way communication and tracking systems between
pre-event at Sago Mine can be deined and understood underground and surface workers. Congress subse-
more than in many other types of disaster. Mines are in quently passed an emergency supplemental appropri-
rural locations and most members of the mining com- ation to accelerate implementation of (1) emergency
munity live in rural or small communities. his provides oxygen supplies, (2) underground refuge chambers, and
a support system during and ater an event – one which (3) communications and tracking systems.
is already in place through community interaction. Communication quality surfaced as a major issue
in the Sago disaster, in the mine, mine to surface, and
personal communication with families and media.
Lessons from the Sago Mine disaster Since Sago, resources have been applied to the devel-
Examination of the Sago Mine disaster utilizing the opment of wireless communications, and procedures
Haddon matrix helps to deine the response strengths have been put in place to communicate with families
and to focus on key issues to target for intervention with and secure the media. Evaluations of mine rescue pol-
the goal of developing a more resilient host, including icies are ongoing. Of major interest in terms of devel-
miners, mine operators, mine emergency response and oping a resilient workforce is the need for training on
command personnel, and the overall mining commu- psychological aspects of human response and behav-
nity itself. Clearly as the model illuminates, further iors in disaster, in addition to a program such as psy-
training on SCSR use is warranted. In addition, since chological irst aid during the event and aterwards
SCSRs last approximately one hour, switching SCSRs (Brymer et al., 2006).
in a toxic environment became an issue ater Sago. Why
some miners thought their SCSRs did not function
when testing ater the incident showed that the units Conclusions
were not empty remains a concern. he tools for impacting resilience at the workforce
Within six months of the Sago disaster, the US (population) level share strategies with those used for
Congress enacted the 2006 Mine Improvement and occupational health and safety and public health prac-
New Emergency Response Act (MINER Act, 2006). tice, such as leadership training, public education,
his legislation contained provisions to improve safety, worker training, legislation, engineering design codes
health, preparedness, and emergency response in US and standards, land use management, and research
mines. he MINER Act requires mine operators to regarding human behavior and intervention efect-
develop and maintain an emergency response plan. iveness. During the pre-event period, workers must
Prior to the Sago incident, mines were only required to acquire and maintain appropriate education, training,
provide miners with a single self-contained breathing and certiication (credentials) speciic to their poten-
apparatus afording the wearer one hour of protection. tial deployment roles. Medical, emotional, and cogni-
Regulators felt miners needed to be provided with suf- tive readiness are important dimensions of workforce
icient quantities of breathing apparatus to give them health protection planning. his may be enhanced by
at least two hours of protection in the event of a pro- improving coping skills through training and by build-
longed escape. he legislation requires mines to store ing social support networks before the disaster strikes.
extra emergency breathing apparatus along escape- Leadership training is a critical element of force pro-
ways in suicient quantities that miners will have oxy- tection as team cohesion, buddy relations, and opera-
gen breathing apparatus available to them the entire tions are all leadership dependent.
length of their escapeways. he Mine Safety and Health In addition, the employers can implement organ-
Administration developed SCSR switchover training izational changes to preserve and maintain worker
along with expectations training, based on a study by resilience by encouraging and supporting safe work
researchers at the National Institutes of Occupational practices and adopting policies that better enable
Safety and Health (Kowalski-Trakoler et al., 2008) workers to balance competing work and life demands.
he inability of trapped miners to communicate with Boxes 23.2 and 23.3 are organizational tools for use pre-
rescuers during the Sago Mine disaster led to another event to improve leadership and resiliance in workers,

352
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

Box 23.2 Organizational Leadership Self-Assess Box 23.3 Organizational actions supporting
ment Tool: adapting existing worker support resiliency for essential service workers
services to provide enhanced workforce support
Actions supporting worker resiliency at the organiza-
during public health emergencies
tional level require dedicated resources and an align-
The Organizational Leadership Self-Assessment Tool is ment of management practices and administrative
designed to help state and local health departments, policies. The following list provides guidance about
healthcare facilities, and first-responder organizations how to achieve this level of support for both work-
to develop plans for ensuring the availability of work- force and organizational resiliency:
force support services during an influenza pandemic. • monitoring workforce needs for stress
These plans may be developed by existing employee management and healthcare
assistance programs, occupational health services, or • planning for continuity of medical care
human resource departments and can be used as the (specialists, medicines) for workers with chronic
basis for other disaster scenarios. The tool consists of a illnesses (physical and psychological)
series of questions. • monitoring for emerging needs (e.g., stress-related,
• What are the most essential or mission critical depression, grief, idiopathic medical conditions)
functions performed by your organization? How • providing education about stress-related conditions
might they be impacted by pandemic influenza? and anticipated health and safety concerns
What is your organization’s plan for protecting
• providing family assistance programs (most
workers and maintaining essential functions?
coping will come from family/friends/faith and
What is your organization’s role in the response to
programs within workplaces and schools)
pandemic influenza?
• providing/receiving leadership, management,
• What psychosocial issues might arise within your and supervisory training
organization and workforce because of pandemic
• anticipating needs and work pace over time (think
influenza (i.e., consider increases in workload
of the metaphor of a marathon not a sprint)
[absenteeism], alternative work strategies such
as telework, just-in-time training, currency of • providing/receiving grief leadership, ceremony,
information, exchange of ideas, grief leadership support (fatality management)
to help with death of co-workers or others in • improving perceptions of collective efficacy (i.e.,
personal life, management flexibility, increased ability to handle problems as a team) through
challenge of disseminating current, accurate, education, training, and drills
timely information, etc.)? • promoting integrated health, safety, and security
• What significant aspects of your organization’s culture/climate (hardiness, resiliency)
structure, business practices, workforce • using role modeling by leadership
demographics, and culture should be considered • enabling workers to balance work and home
in planning for pandemic influenza? demands through flexible work schedules and
• What hurdles might your organization encounter other administrative policies
in attempting to provide services to your • reinforcing organizational commitment to
workforce during different phases of pandemic worker safety and health through appropriate
influenza? What strategies might be employed to supervision, training, and access to services
overcome those hurdles (e.g., alternative service • implementing continuity of information and
delivery methods)? communication systems (FEMA Emergency
• How might your organization assist your covered Management Guide for Business and Industry,
workforce in preparing for and dealing with these http:www.fema.gov/business/guide/index.shtm;
psychosocial issues? Continuity of Operations, http:www.fema.gov/
• How might your organization assist its executive government/coop/index.shtm#0)
leaders in preparing for and dealing with these • ensuring continuity of essential operations
psychosocial issues? (organizational resiliency)
• What might your organization be doing now • ensuring redundancy and cross-training of critical
to promote the resiliency of your workforce tasks/roles providing alternative worksites and
and coping ability in the event of a pandemic innovative use of technology to maintain operations.
emergency (prepare and mitigate)? Sources: Reissman et al., 2006a; Ursano et al.,
Source: Reissman et al., 2006b. 2006.

353
Section 5: Training for resilience

Box 23.4 The Worker Resiliency Program presented to demonstrate how the principles presented
in this chapter have been applied to a particular work-
The Worker Resiliency Program was created by the place setting and organizational culture. Organizational
US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC)
changes may include lexible schedules, portable com-
as an internal workforce strategy in 2004, during the
munication devices (and air time), leadership and team
humanitarian response to the Indian Ocean earth-
quake and tsunami, to safeguard the health, safety, building, regulating the operational tempo or staf
and resiliency of staff members deployed to danger- rhythm, monitoring worker health, fatigue and safety
ous locations. It was further expanded during the 2005 practices, and providing services to meet work-related
responses to Hurricane Katrina along the Gulf Coast in physical and emotional healthcare needs. Supervisors
the USA and to a large outbreak of Marburg hemor- should be aware that fatigue and sleep deprivation can
rhagic fever in Angola. The program, which continues have serious negative consequences in the workplace
to evolve, currently performs the following activities: and lead to recklessness or unsafe work practices dur-
• identifies and anticipates stressors that ing an emergency, particularly if shit length and dur-
responders are likely to encounter ation of mandated service are long and rest periods are
• develops field resources and conducts pre- inadequate (Caruso et al., 2006). Fatigue management
deployment briefings on how to use self-care becomes increasingly important when workers face
strategies to minimize anticipated stressors multiple or hazardous work demands, threats to per-
• ensures access to healthcare and counseling sonal safety, and/or traumatic and psychologically chal-
services during and after deployment lenging experiences, such as witnessing mass fatalities
• recommends organizational strategies to assist or handling afected persons who are angry or emotion-
responders deployed to harsh settings, such ally distraught.
as administrative leave, just-in-time training, It is helpful to anticipate needs for psychological
tactical logistical assistance, clarifying mission interventions within the workplace and do the home-
assignments
work required to ensure that potential techniques are
• provides support materials for families of those based on empirically defensible and evidence-based
deployed using Internet technology and a peer-
practices – and are conducted by qualiied individ-
support (family buddy) system
uals. It is also important to tailor any intervention
• assists in interim and after-action reporting
strategy to the ecology of the workplace and work-
• supports development of routine training force (e.g., when and how the intervention unfolds)
courses for team leaders and deployment
(Hobfoll et al., 2007). hese are all essential elements
personnel
for organizational continuity. he cost of not doing
• facilitates external expert consultation with senior
this can be quite high, including loss of specialized
leadership and employee assistance professionals
workers, with a need to recruit and train new staf;
on strategic and operational policies, training, and
behavioral healthcare practices interim loss of productivity; worker compensation
costs for job-related injury, illness, or disability; and
• institutes an integrated health, safety, and
resiliency function as part of command staffing other potentially cascading organizational efects (e.g.,
within the CDC incident management system loss of morale). Efective disaster safety management
• supports a pilot field-support program, ranging requires appropriate infrastructure and interagency
from field support by peers (buddy) and tactical planning and coordination before the emergency
deployment, safety and resiliency team members, arises. Pre-deployment and preparedness activities
with linkage to CDC emergency operations for shape and inluence the overall success of the emer-
technical support and situation awareness gency response eforts, and, in turn, the disaster recov-
• provides field training involving the basics of ery process. As seen in the WTC disaster, workers who
safety, peer support, and psychological first aid. assume roles to which they are not accustomed, or
for which they are psychologically unprepared, may
experience signiicant interpersonal and organiza-
respectively. Box 23.4 outlines the Worker Resiliency tional stresses (Perrin et al., 2007). he same may be
Program developed by the US Centers for Disease true for workers who shoulder extra responsibilities
Control and Prevention to support its workers during to ensure the continuity of businesses, governmental
a disaster and in its atermath. his program is being oices, or critical community services (e.g., energy,

354
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

food, and water supply; waste management; telecom- boards/nbsb/meetings/Documents/dmhreport-inal.pdf


munications; and transportation). (accessed February 15, 2011).
Finally, expert consultation (advisor) to the inci- Engel, C. C. (2004). Somatization and multiple idiopathic
dent or crisis leadership regarding the resilience, physical symptoms: Relationship to traumatic events
health, and safety of the afected workers would and posttraumatic stress disorder. In P. P. Schnurr &
greatly beneit the eiciency of the combined opera- B. L. Green (eds.), Trauma and health: Physical health
consequences of exposure to extreme stress (pp. 191–216).
tions. Behavioral expertise in a command staf advis- Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
ory role may also serve to minimize decision-making
Flynn, B. W. (2003). Leadership, mental health, and
errors stemming from the impact of the traumatic
bioterrorism. In Bioterrorism and public health:
events on the leaders themselves.2 Psychologically Communities and leaders preparing for psychological and
and behaviorally informed advice is likely to improve behavioral responses to a biological attack. Bethesda, MD:
information analyzed about when and why leadership Center for the Study of Traumatic Stress.
should implement major decisions: such as when to Flynn, B. W. (2006). Facing the future: Who will own readiness
transition from response (rescue and life safety activ- for our emerging threats. In Proceedings of the 22nd
ities) to recovery (managing the losses) operations. Annual Rosalynn Carter Symposium on Mental Health
here are cascading and enduring consequences that Policy, Atlanta, http://www.cartercenter.org/documents/
can occur when such a transition is delayed (Jackson nondatabase/sym06_lynn.ppt (accessed August 30, 2009).
et al., 2004). Fullerton, C. S., Ursano, R. J., Norwood, A. E., &
Holloway, H. H. (2003). Trauma, terrorism, and disaster.
In R. J. Ursano, C. S. Fullerton, & A. E. Norwood (eds.),
References Terrorism and disaster: Individual and community mental
Barnett, D. J., Balicer, R. D., Lucey, D. R., et al. (2005). A health interventions (pp. 1–22). New York: Cambridge
systematic analytic approach to pandemic inluenza University Press.
preparedness planning. PLoS Medicine, 2, 1235–1241. Garakani, A., Hirschowitz, J., & Katz, C. L. (2004). General
Bills, C., Levy, N., Sharma, V., et al. (2008). he mental disaster psychiatry. Psychiatric Clinics of North America,
health of workers and volunteers responding to the 27, 391–406.
events of 9/11: a review of the literature. Mount Sinai Gates, R. A., Phillips, R. L., Urosek, J. E., et al. (2007). Report
Journal of Medicine, 75, 115–127. of investigation: Fatal underground coal mine explosion,
Bilsker, D., Gilbert, M., & Samra, J. (2007). Anti-depressant January 2, 2006, Sago Mine, Wolf Run Mining Company,
skills at work: Dealing with mood problems in the Tallmansville, Upshur County, West Virginia. [ID No.
workplace. Vancouver: British Columbia Mental Health 46–087910.] Arlington, VA: Mine Safety and Health
and Addiction Services, Provincial Health Services Administration, US Department of Labor.
Authority, British Columbia, http://www.comh.ca/ Gerberding, J. L. (2008). World Trade Center health
antidepressant-skills/work/ (accessed July 31, 2009). efects. [Testimony to the Committee on Energy
Brown, D. & Kulig, J. (1996/97). he concept of resiliency: and Commerce, Subcommittee on Health of the US
heoretical lessons from community research. Health House of Representatives.] Atlanta, GA: Centers for
and Canadian Society, 4, 29–52. Disease Control and Prevention, http://www.hhs.gov/
asl/testify/2008/07/t20080731d.html (accessed on
Brymer, M., Jacobs, A., Layne, C., et al. (2006). Psychological
September 5, 2009).
irst aid: Field operations guide, 2nd edn. Los Angeles, CA:
National Child Traumatic Stress Network and National Haddon, W. Jr. (1968). he changing approach to the
Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. epidemiology, prevention, and amelioration of trauma:
he transition to approaches etiologically rather than
Caruso, C. C., Bushnell, T., Eggerth, D., et al. (2006). Long
descriptively based. American Journal of Public Health,
working hours, safety, and health: Toward a national
58, 1431–1438.
research agenda. American Journal of Industrial Medicine,
49, 9300–9342. Haddon, W. Jr. (1970). On the escape of tigers: an ecologic
note. American Journal of Public Health, Nations Health,
DeMarchi, J. (1997). Historical mining disasters (pp. 34–44).
60, 2229–2234.
Beckley, WV: National Mine Health and Safety Academy,
Mine Safety and Health Administration. Haddon, W. Jr. (1972). A logical framework for categorizing
highway safety phenomena and activity. Journal of
Disaster Mental Health Subcommittee of the National
Trauma, 12, 193–207.
Biodefense Science Board (2008). Disaster mental health
recommendations. Washington, DC: National Biodefense Haddow, G. D. & Bullock, J. A. (2006). he historical context
Science Board, http://www.phe.gov/Preparedness/legal/ of emergency management. In Introduction to emergency

355
Section 5: Training for resilience

management, 2nd edn (pp. 1–18). Burlington MA: recovery workers: cases and observations. Psychiatric
Elsevier Butterworth-Heinemann, Services, 57, 1335–1358.
Herbert, R., Moline, J., Skloot, G., et al. (2006). he World Katz, C. L., Levin, S., Munro, S., Pandya, A., & Smith, R.
Trade Center disaster and the health of workers: Five (2009). Prevalence and predictors of psychiatric
year assessment of a unique medical screening program. symptoms in Ground Zero iron workers: the atermath
Environmental Health Perspectives, 114, 1853–1858. of the World Trade Center attacks of September 11, 2001.
Hobfoll, S. E., Watson, P. J., Bell, C., et al. (2007). Five Psychiatric Bulletin, 33, 49–52.
essential elements of immediate and mid-term mass Kowalski-Trakoler, K. M. & Barrett, E. A. (2003).
trauma intervention: Empirical evidence. Psychiatry, 70, he concept of degraded images applied to hazard
283–315. recognition training in mining for reduction of lost-time
Institute of Medicine (2002). he future of the public’s health injuries. Journal of Safety Research, 34,
in the 21st century. Washington, DC: National Academies 515–523.
Press. Kowalski-Trakoler, K. M., Vaught, C., & Brnich, M. J.
Institute of Medicine (2003). Institute of Medicine, National (2008). Expectations training for miners using self-
Academy of Sciences. A. Stith Butler, A. M. Panzer, & contained self-rescuers. Journal of Occupational and
L. R. Goldfrank (eds.), Preparing for the psychological Environmental Hygiene, 5, 671–677.
consequences of terrorism: A public health strategy. Kowalski-Trakoler, K. M., Alexander, D. A., Brnich, M. J.,
Washington, DC: National Academies Press. McWilliams, L., & Reissman, D. B. (2009). Underground
International Federation of Red Cross and Red Crescent coal mining disasters and fatalities: United States,
Societies (2000). Introduction to disaster management. 1900–1906. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 57,
Geneva: International Federation of Red Cross and Red 1379–1383.
Crescent Societies, http://www.ifrc.org/Docs/pubs/ Masten, A. S. & Obradović, J. (2008). Disaster preparation
disasters/resources/corner/dp-manual/all.pdf (accessed and recovery: Lessons from research on resilience in
July 11, 2009). human development. Ecology and Society, 13, 1–9.
Jackson, B. A., Baker, J. C., Ridgely, M. S., Bartis, J. T., & McQuistion, H. & Katz, C. L. (2001). he September 11,
Linn, H. I. (2004). Protecting emergency responders, 2001 disaster: some lessons learned in mental health
Vol. 3: Safety management in disaster and terrorism preparedness. Emergency Psychiatry, 7, 61–64.
response. [DHHS publication 2004–144.] Santa Monica,
CA: RAND Corporation for the National Institute for MINER Act (2006). Mine Improvement and New
Occupational Safety and Health, http://www.cdc.gov/ Emergency Response Act of 2006, Pub. L. No.
niosh/docs/2004–144/ (accessed July 11, 2009). 108–236 (S 2803), http://www.msha.gov/MinerAct/
MinerActSingleSource.asp (accessed February 28, 2011).
Kaniasty, K. & Norris, F. H. (2004). Social support in the
atermath of disasters, catastrophes, and acts of terrorism: Mine Safety and Health Administration (1998a). Historical
altruistic, overwhelmed, uncertain, antagonistic, and summary of mine disasters in the United States, Vol. II:
patriotic communities. In R. Ursano, A. Norwood, & Coal Mines 1959–1998. Arlington, VA: Mine Safety
C. Fullerton (eds.), Bioterrorism: psychological and public and Health Administration, US Department
health interventions (pp. 200–229). Cambridge, UK: of Labor.
Cambridge University Press. Mine Safety and Health Administration (1998b). Historical
Katz, C. L. & Nathaniel, R. (2002). Disasters, psychiatry, and summary of mine disasters in the United States, Vol. I: Coal
psychodynamics. Journal of the American Academy of Mines 1810–1958. Arlington, VA: Mine Safety and Health
Psychoanalysis, 30, 519–530. Administration, US Department of Labor.
Katz, C. L., Pellegrino, L., Pandya, A., Ng, A., & DeLisi, L. Norris, F. H., Stevens, S. P., Pfeferbaum, B., Wyche, K. F.,
(2002). Research on psychiatric outcomes subsequent to & Pfeferbaum, R. L. (2008). Community resilience as
disasters: a review of the literature. Psychiatry Research, a metaphor, theory, set of capacities and strategy for
110, 201–217. disaster readiness. American Journal of Community
Katz, C. L., Smith, R., Herbert, R., Levin, S., & Gross, R. Psychology, 41, 127–150.
(2006a). he World Trade Center worker/volunteer Paton, D. & Johnston, D. (eds.) (2006). Disaster resilience: An
mental health screening program. In Y. Neria, R. Gross, integrated approach. Springield, IL: Charles C. homas.
R. Marshall, & E. Susser (eds.), 9/11: Public mental health Perrin, M. A., DiGrande, L., Wheeler, K., et al. (2007).
in the wake of a terrorist attack (pp. 355–377). New York: Diferences in PTSD prevalence and associated risk
Cambridge University Press. factors among World Trade Center rescue and recovery
Katz, C. L., Smith, R. P., Silverton, M., et al. (2006b). A workers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 9,
mental health program for Ground Zero rescue and 1385–1394.

356
Chapter 23: Public health practice and disaster resilience

Qureshri, K., Gerson, R. R. M., & Sherman, M. F., et al. US Department of Homeland Security (2007). Homeland
(2005). Health care workers ability and willingness to Security Presidential Directive 21. Public health and
report to duty during catastrophic disasters. Journal of medical preparedness. Washington, DC: US Department
Urban Health, 82, 378–388. of Homeland Security, http://www.dhs.gov/xabout/laws/
Reissman, D. B. & Howard, J. (2008). Responder safety gc_1219263961449.shtm (accessed September 5, 2009).
and health: Preparing for future disasters. Mount Sinai US Department of Homeland Security (2009). Homeland
Journal of Medicine, 75, 135–141. Security Presidential Directive 7. National infrastructure
Reissman, D. B., Schreiber, M., Klomp, R. W., et al. protection plan: Partnering to enhance protection
(2006a). he virtual network supporting the front and resiliency. Washington, DC: US Department of
lines: Addressing emerging behavioral health problems Homeland Security, http://www.dhs.gov/xlibrary/assets/
following the tsunami of 2004. Military Medicine, 171, NIPP_Plan.pdf (accessed September 5, 2009).
40–43. West Virginia Oice of Miners’ Health, Safety, and Training
Reissman, D. B., Watson, P. J., Klomp, R. W., Tanielian, T. L., (2006). Report of investigation into the Sago Mine
& Prior, S. D. (2006b). Pandemic inluenza preparedness: explosion. Charleston, WV: West Virginia Oice of
adaptive responses to an evolving challenge. Journal of Miners’ Health, Safety, and Training.
Homeland Security and Emergency Management, Wheeler, K., McKelvey, W., horpe, L., et al. (2007). Asthma
3, 13. diagnosed ater11 September 2001 among rescue and
Runyan, C. W. (2003). Introduction: back to the future: recovery workers: Findings from the World Trade Center
Revisiting Haddon’s conceptualization of injury Health Registry. Environmental Health Perspectives, 115,
epidemiology and prevention. Epidemiological Review, 1584–1590.
25, 60–64. Winslow, C. E. A. (1920). he untilled ield of public health.
Salerno, A., Margolies, P., & Cleek, A. (2008). Wellness and Modern Medicine, 2, 183–191.
self-management personal workbook, 2nd edn. New York: Wright, K. M., Ursano, R. J., Bartone, P. T., & Ingraham,
New York State Oice of Mental Health and the Urban L. H. (1990). he shared experience of catastrophe:
Institute for Behavioral Health. an expanded classiication of the disaster community.
Schoch-Spana, M. (2008). Editorial: Community American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 60, 35–42.
resilience for catastrophic health events. Biosecurity and Zohar, D. (2010). Safety climate: Conceptual and
Bioterrorism: Biodefense Strategy, Practice, and Science 6, 2. measurement issues. In J . Campbell Quick &
Shalev, A. Y., Bonne, O., & Eth, S. (1996). Treatment of L. E. Tetrick (eds.), Handbook of occupational health
post-traumatic stress disorder: A review. Psychosomatic psychology, 2nd edn (Ch. 8). Washington, DC: American
Medicine, 58, 165–182. Psychological Association.
Smith, R. P., Katz, C. L., Holmes, A., et al. (2004). Mental
health status of World Trade Center rescue and recovery
workers and volunteers: New York City, July 2002– Endnotes
August 2004. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 53, 1. he National Biodefense Science Board was established by the
812–815. Secretary of the US Department of Health and Human Services
(DHHS) pursuant to section 319M of the Public Health Services
Tedeschi, R. G., Park, C. L., & Calhoun, L. G. (1998).
Act (42 USC. 247d-7f) as added by section 402 of the Pandemic
Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual issues. In R. Tedeschi
and All Hazards Preparedness Act (PAHPA) (Pub. L. 107–417),
& L. Calhoun (eds.), Posttraumatic growth: Positive
efective December 19, 2006. he Board provides expert advice
changes in the atermath of crisis (pp. 1–22). Mahwah, NJ: and guidance to the Secretary on scientiic, technical, and other
Earlbaum. matters of special interest to HHS regarding current and future
hrush, P. W. & staf of the U. S. Bureau of Mines (1968). chemical, biological, nuclear, and radiological agents, whether
A dictionary of mining, mineral and related terms. naturally occurring, accidental, or deliberate. he Board may also
Washington, DC: Bureau of Mines. provide advice and guidance to the Secretary on other matters
related to public health emergency preparedness and response. It
Ursano, R. J., Vineburgh, N. T., Giford, R. K., Benedek, is an established Federal Advisory Committee with both legal and
D. M., & Fullerton, C. S. (2006) A leadership document discretionary authorities.
to inform planning, response and policy for workplace 2. Under the Homeland Security Presidential Directive 21, paragraph
preparedness and behavioral risk management 31 (US Department of Homeland Security, 2007), the Secretary of
of disaster and terrorism. Bethesda, MD: Center Health and Human Services, in coordination with the Secretaries
for the Study of Traumatic Stress, http://www. of Defense, Veterans Afairs, and Homeland Security, was directed
centerforthestudyotraumaticstress.org/downloads/ to establish a Federal Advisory Committee for Disaster Mental
CSTS%20Leadership%20Planning%20for%20 Health. he directive stated that the committee shall consist of
Workplace%20Preparedness.pdf (accessed July 11, 2009). appropriate subject matter experts and, within 180 days ater its

357
Section 5: Training for resilience

establishment, shall submit to the Secretary of Health and Human the Disaster Mental Health Subcommittee was established under
Services recommendations for protecting, preserving, and restor- the National Biodefense Science Board, an established Federal
ing individual and community mental health in catastrophic Advisory Committee with both legal and discretionary author-
health event settings, including pre-, intra-, and post-event edu- ities providing advice and guidance to the Secretary of Health and
cation, messaging, and interventions. To execute this directive, Human Services.

358
Index

5-HT, see serotonin burden perspective, 137 training, 35–36


5-HTTLPR, see serotonin transporter inoculation perspective, 137 avoidance, 195–196, 230
gene maturation hypothesis, 137–138
absorption, 257 mortality hypothesis, 138 baseline function, 208–209
abyss experience, 182–183 vulnerability theory, 137 Battlemind, 325
acceptance and commitment therapy, aging population, 135 Battlemind Training System, 323–324
97 increased diversity, 135, comprehensiveness, 329
acculturation, 177 see also older adults future directions, 336–338
achievement, 66 Alaska Natives’ resiliency systems, 180 history, 324
active coping, 17 alcohol dependence, 294–295 implementation, 331–336
acts of kindness, 283–284 allostasis, 1–2, 80, 308 exportability and scalability, 333
acute stress response enhancement in children, 312–313 integration into organizational
modulating agents, 10–18 emotional literacy, 314–315 culture, 331–332
brain-derived neurotrophic factor, interventions in schools and leader support, 335
12–13 communities, 314 mobile training teams, 334
epigenetic mechanisms, 13–14 interventions with parents, ownership issues, 336
neural circuitry of fear, 14–15 313–314 packaging and multimedia, 336
psychological processes, 15–18 moral courage training, 317 policy, 335
sex hormones, 10–12 outdoor education programs, quality control, 332
systems, 2–10 316–317 refresher training, 334
corticotropin-releasing hormone, Penn Resiliency Program, 317 sustainability, 334
4–5 team sports, 315–316 timing, 332
dopamine, 7–9 allostatic load, 2, 81, 308 training guidelines, 333–334
hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal American-Indian/Alaska Native train-the-trainer program,
axis, 2–4 resiliency systems, 180 332–333
neuropeptides, 9–10 amygdala veriiable claims, 335–336
norepinephrine, 5–6 role in fear, 14 mental health training
serotonin, 6–7 stress response and, 16 action-focused, 328
adaptive capacities, 163 a-priori beliefs, appraisal theory, 31–32, developmental nature, 328–329
adaptive systems, 111–114 46 evidence-based, 330–331
attachment relationships, 112 primary appraisal, 46 experience-based, 327–328
cultural systems, 113 secondary appraisal, 46 explanatory nature, 328
improving or mobilizing, 115 self-appraisal, 47 relevant purpose and content, 327
intelligence and problem-solving Arab American resiliency systems, 180 strength-based, 327
systems, 112 arginine vasopressin, 4–5 team-based approach, 328
motivation to adapt, 112–113 Asian cultural resiliency systems, user-acceptability, 329–330
self-regulation systems, 113 180–181 program improvement, 334–335
adolescents Asian Indian cultural resiliency bereavement, 121, 189
well-being therapy, 297, systems, delayed reactions, 123
see also children attachment relationships, 112 grief counseling, 129
adrenocorticotropin hormone, 2 attention control, 32–33, 40 individual diferences, 189
estrogens and, 12 adaptive, 35 ethnic and cultural diferences,
α2-adrenoceptor, 5 cognitive reappraisal, 37–40 197
African-Americans’ resiliency systems, distraction, 36 implications of, 196–197
179–180 resilience and, 33–37 variation in grief reactions, 123
aging, see also older adults rumination, 36 older adults, 125–126, 139–140
mental health and, 136 selective attention, 35–36 positive emotions and, 127–128
resilience theories, 136–138 attentional bias, 34 trajectories

359
Index

bereavement (cont.) competence and cascades, cognitive reappraisal, 32–33, 40–41


chronic grief, 194–196 166–167 resilience and, 37–40
recovery, 219 intervention models, 110 collective eicacy, 47–49, 270–271
resilience, 192–194, see also grief person-focused approaches, 108 collectivism, 177
beta-blockers, 6 variable-focused approaches, combat, see warfare
Bosnian refugees, 157 108–110 combat and operational stress
brain-derived neurotrophic factor, developmental tasks, 105 continuum model, 243–246
12–13 grief in, 123 core leader functions, 246–249
breast cancer, beneits of social ties, intervention issues, 128–129 Combat and Operational Stress Control
82, 83 grief counseling, 129 program. see US Marine Corps
resilience promotion, 84 PTSD, 124–125 combat and operational stress irst aid,
Brief Multidimensional Measure of resilience, 103–104, 111–114, 123, 248
Religion/Spirituality (BMMRS), 265 commitment, 63
93–94, 99 attachments and, 308–309 communication, community resilience
Buddhism, 181 poverty efects, 265–268 and, 165–166
Bufalo Creek lood, 270, 271 resilience development communities
burden hypothesis, 137 good adaptation, 105 deinition, 207–208, 268
promotive and protective factors, disaster impact on, 206
CAFES program, 157 106 Community Assessment of Resilience
cancer, beneits of social ties, 82, 83 threats, 105–106 (CART), 167–168
resilience promotion, 84 resilience framework, 114–115 community competence, 166–167
cardiovascular stress response, 79–80 resilience promoting factors, 128 community preparedness, 170
social ties and, 79–80 self-eicacy community resilience, 162, 203, 209–
caregiving, 139 promotion of, 314 210, 340–341
stress reactivity in children and, stress relationships, 311–312 assessment of capacities, 167–170
309–311 stress reactivity related to caregiving, participatory approaches,
Caribbean black resiliency system, 309–311 167–168
181 trauma responses, 123 social indicator research, 168–170
catechol-O-methyl-transferase, 5–6 well-being therapy, 297 enhancement, 170–172
causal attributions, 46 Chinese culture, 180 multiple meanings of, 340–341
CDC Worker Resiliency Program, 354 chronic disease, 122–123 poverty efects, 268–271
challenge, 63 older adults, 139 cohesion, 269–270
change, models of, 114–115 physiological adjustment, 76 collective eicacy, 270–271
Chicago Center for Family Health psychological adjustment, 76 social capital, 269
programs, 156 social ties and predictors of, 203–204
Bosnian and Kosovar refugees, 157 behavioral efects, 79 resources, 163
job loss stresses, 156–157 evidence for efects, 81–83 community competence,
Kosovar Family Professional physiological beneits, 79–81, 166–167
Educational Collaborative, 82–83 economic development,
157–158 psychological beneits, 78, 81–82 163–164
children, 307–308 resilience promotion, 83–85 information and communication,
adaptive systems, 111–114 chronic grief, 194–195 165–166
attachment relationships, 112 qualitative aspects, 195–196 social capital, 164–165
cultural systems, 113 chronic life strains, 264 competence, 166–167
intelligence and problem-solving citalopram, 7 complicated grief, 189
systems, 112 citizen participation, 165 Comprehensive Soldier Fitness (CSF),
motivation to adapt, 112–113 coal mining, see mining 324, 335
self-regulation systems, 113 cognitive approaches, 45, 46–47 conditionability, 66–67
allostasis enhancement, 312–313 challenges, 52 Confucianism, 181
emotional literacy, 314–315 see also social cognitive theory Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale
interventions in schools and cognitive-behavioral stress (CD-RISC), 290
communities, 314 management, 38, 40 conservation of resources theory,
interventions with parents, cognitive-behavioral therapy, 40 254–257, 272
313–314 following rape, 233 defensive responding with resource
moral courage training, 317 serious mental illness, 281 lack, 257
outdoor education programs, cognitive disorganization, 190 engagement/distress interactions,
316–317 cognitive emotion regulation, 32–33, 260–261
Penn Resiliency Program, 317 40–41 primacy of resource loss, 254
team sports, 315–316 attention control, 32–37, 40 resource investment, 254–255
developmental research, 108–111 cognitive reappraisal, 32–33, 40–41 resource loss and gain spirals, 256

360
Index

constraint/impulsivity, 57 depression, economic development, 163–164


see also personality factors attentional bias and, 34 index of, 169
control, 63 distraction and, 33 measurement, 169–170
coping self-eicacy, 47, 48–49 in chronic disease, beneits of social economic disparity, 151
demands and, 50–51 ties, 81–82 economic impact of disasters, 206–207
traumatic demands, 49–50 rumination and, 33–34 elderly, see older adults
coping strategies see also manic depressive disorder emergency management, 170–172,
active coping, 17 developmental cascades, 111 340
avoidance coping, 67 developmental psychopathology, see also disaster management
emotion-focused coping, 67 103–104 emotion regulation, 16, 30
following rape, 229–230 developmental tasks, 105 active coping, 17
personality and, 67 dexamethasone suppression test, 3 humor, 17
pragmatic coping, 126–127 dialectical behavior therapy, 97 meaning-making, 17
problem-focused/approach coping, disaster management, 170–172 moderator model, 31
67 resilience promotion in disaster optimism, 17
recovery and, 278 workers, 341–343, 352–355 promotion in children, 314–315
religious coping, 94–95, 98 Haddon matrix, 342–343 reframing/reappraisal, 16
negative religious coping, 95 Sago Mine disaster, 347–352 social competence, 17–18
with serious mental illness, 281 World Trade Center case study, see also cognitive emotion
coronary heart disease, 79 343–347 regulation
beneits of social ties, 81–83 see also emergency management emotional literacy, 314–315
resilience promotion, 84 disaster research, 200–202 emotional support, 77
corticotropin-releasing hormone, 4–5 resilience concept in, 203–204 emotions, 15–18
allostatic contribution, 4 disasters, 200, 203–204, 340 dysphoric, following bereavement,
allostatic load, 4 challenges in applying resilience, 190–191
factors promoting resilience, 4–5 207–213 elements of, 15
neuropeptide Y interaction, 9 baseline function, 208–209 see also emotion regulation; negative
receptors, 4 deining communities, 207–208 emotions; positive emotions
cortisol, 2 deining value-neutral resilience empathy, 17–18
allostatic contribution, 2 and failure, 211–212 empowerment, 278
DHEA ratio, 3–4 resilience for whom, 209–211 engagement, 257
dysregulation, 2–3 multilevel consequences of, as both process and outcome,
social ties and, 81 204–207 257–258
critical incident stress debrieing, 128 community impact, 206 engagement versus distress
cultural diversity, 151 displacement, 205–206 trajectories, 259–260
cultural systems, 113 economic and political impact, engagement/distress interactions,
African-American, 179–180 206–207 260–261
American-Indian/Alaska Native, 180 environmental health, 205 engagement theory, 254, 257
Arab-American, 180 family impact, 206 Enhancing Recovery in Coronary
Asian cultural, 180–181 physical health, 204 Artery Heart Disease
Asian Indian, 181 psychological health, 204–205 (ENRICHD), 293–294
Caribbean black resiliency system, rescue and clean-up operations, environmental context
181 205 demands, 50–51
Hawaiian Native, 182 typologies, 343 traumatic demands, 49–50
Latin-American, 182 see also disaster management genetic–environmental interactions,
culture, 176–178 displacement, 205–206 178–179
monocultural ethnocentrism, 178 Dispositional Resilience Scale-15 religiosity as an environmental
resiliency and, 177–178 (DRS-15), 290 inluence, 179
in trauma recovery, 182–183 distraction, 33, 36 epigenetic changes, 13–14
see also cultural systems; ethnic distress epinephrine (E)
considerations engagement/distress interactions, social ties and, 81
cumulative risk, 105 260–261 estrogens, 11–12, 13
trajectories, 259–260 stress response and, 11–12
dedication, 257 dopamine, 7–9 ethnic considerations
dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA), 2 allostatic contribution, 8 African-American resiliency
allostatic contribution, 2 allostatic load, 8–9 systems, 179–180
cortisol ratio, 3–4 factors contributing to resilience, 9 American-Indian/Alaska Native
treatment, 3–4 dopamine D2 receptor gene resiliency systems, 180
demands, 50–51 (DRD2), 9 Arab-American resiliency systems,
traumatic demands, 49–50 dysphoric emotions, 190–191 180

361
Index

ethnic considerations (cont.) GAL-3 receptor, 10 homeostasis, 1, 2


Asian cultural resiliency systems, gender roles, 150–151 hope, 278
180–181 genetic approaches, 116 HPA, see hypothalamic–pituitary–
Caribbean black resiliency system, genetic–environmental interactions, adrenal axis
181 178–179 Human Development Index, 169
Hawaiian Native resiliency system, religiosity as an environmental humor, 17, 284
182 inluence, 179 Hurricane Andrew, 124–125, 128
Latin-American cultural resiliency genograms, 153 Hurricane Katrina, 141, 200–201, 208,
systems, 182 Girafe Project, 317 210
see cultural systems glucocorticoids, 2 hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis,
executive function, 113 glucocorticoid receptor sensitivity, 2–4, 80–81
goal-setting, with serious mental allostatic contribution, 2
faith, loss of illness, 282 allostatic load, 2–3
see also religion/spirituality good adaptation, 105 factors promoting resilience, 3–4
families gratitude enhancement, 283 axis feedback, 4
disaster impact on, 206 grief cortisol to DHEA ratio, 3–4
structure, 150–151 children, 123 single nucleotide polymorphisms,
family resilience, 149–150 complicated grief, 189 3
assessment of family function, delayed reactions, 123
152–155 individual diference implications, immune function, 80
genograms, 153 196–197 Incredible Years Teacher and Child
Walsh Family Resilience phenomenology, 189–192 Training Programs, 314–315
Framework, 153–155 cognitive disorganization, 190 Indian Ocean tsunami, 200
developmental perspective, dysphoric emotions, 190–191 Sri Lankan response, 269, 270–271
151–152 health deicits, 191–192 individualism, 177
family life cycle stressors, 152 social and occupational information, community resilience
intergenerational legacies, 152 dysfunction, 192 and, 165–166
pile-up of stressors, 152 trajectories, 192–195 informational support, 77
varied pathways in resilience, chronic grief, 194–196 inoculation perspective, 137
151–152 recovery, 219 stress inoculation training, 292–293
family resilience-oriented practice, resilience, 192–194 instrumental support, 77
155–158 variation in grief reactions, 123 intelligence, 112
approaches to traumatic loss and contextual factors, 197 IQ, 109
major disaster, 158 see also bereavement intergenerational legacies, 152
Chicago Center for Family Health grief counseling, 129 intervention models, 110
programs, 156 gross domestic product (GDP), 169 IQ, 109
key processes, 154 guilt, 95
poverty efects, 268 Job Demands–Control model (JD-C),
research challenges and Haddon matrix, 342–343 50
opportunities, 158 application to Sago Mine disaster, job loss, 156–157
sociocultural context, 150–151 347–352
family impact of broader social application to World Trade Center Kauai Longitudinal Study, Hawaii,
trauma, 151 disaster, 343–347 265–266
family transformations in hardiness, 63–64, 142, 178, 291 Kosovar Family Professional
changing societies, 150–151 personality trait relationships, 64 Educational Collaborative,
fear training for resilience enhancement, 157–158
conditioning, 14–15 291–292 Kosovar refugees, 157
neural circuitry of, 14–15 HardiTraining, 292
ight or light response, 80 Hawaiian resiliency system, 182 Landmine Survivors Network, 298–302
FKBP5 gene, 4 health latent growth mixture modeling, 122
lexible adaptation, 127 bereavement impact on, 191–192 Latin-American cultural resiliency
luoxetine side-efects, 7 disaster impact on, 204 systems, 182
forgiveness, lack of, 95 environmental health, 205 learned helplessness, 5
intervention studies, 97 psychological health, 204–205 learned optimism training, 295–296
social support and, 293–294 life course, 151
galanin, 10 hippocampus role in fear, 14 locus coeruleus, 5
allostatic contribution, 10 HIV, beneits of social ties, 82, 83 allostatic contribution, 5
allostatic load, 10 resilience promotion, 84–85 allostatic load, 5
factors promoting resilience, 10 Holocaust survivors, 140 loneliness, 191

362
Index

loss of faith, 95 Minding the Baby program, 313 inoculation perspective, 137
Lower Manhattan Community mining, 347–348 maturation hypothesis, 137–138
Recovery Project, 158 disaster incidents, 349 mortality hypothesis, 138
safety history, 348 vulnerability theory, 137
manic depressive disorder (MDD) see also Sago Mine disaster trauma recovery, 136–138
cortisol dysregulation, 2–3 mirror neurons, 18 operational resilience, 239–240
5-HTTLPR polymorphism and, 7 monocultural ethnocentrism, 178 Operational Stress Control (OSC)
reward responses, 8 moral courage training, 317 program, see US Navy
maturation hypothesis, 137–138 mortality hypothesis, 138 optimism, 17, 35, 278
meaning-making, 17 mortality, following bereavement, learned optimism training,
mediating efects, 110 191–192 295–296
mental itness, 326 motivation to adapt, 112–113 Organizational Leadership Self-
mental health training, 324, 326–331 Assessment Tool, 353
action-focused, 328 National Vietnam Veterans outdoor education programs, 316–317
developmental nature, 328–329 Readjustment Study, 63 oxytocin, 17–18
evidence-based, 330–331 negative attentional bias, 34
experience-based, 327–328 negative emotions, 15 Penn Resiliency Program, 296, 317
explanatory nature, 328 negative emotionality/neuroticism, personality factors, 51–52, 56–57
implementation, 331–336 56–57 future research, 68–69
exportability and scalability, 333 post-traumatic adjustment and, hardiness and, 63–64
integration into organizational 64–65 mediators of the link with resilience,
culture, 331–332 see also emotions; personality 66–68
leader support, 335 factors conditionability, 66–67
mobile training teams, 334 negative life events, 264 coping style, 67
ownership issues, 336 networks, 164 social support, 67–68
packaging and multimedia, 336 neural circuitry of fear, 14–15 older adults, 141–142
policy, 335 neural plasticity, 115 post-trauma prospective studies,
quality control, 332 neuroendocrine activity, 80–81 60–63
refresher training, 334 social ties and, 81 post-traumatic adjustment
timing, 332 neuropeptide Y, 9–10 relationships, 64–66
training guidelines, 333–334 allostatic contribution, 9 pre-trauma prospective studies,
train-the-trainer program, allostatic load, 9 58–60
332–333 factors promoting resilience, 9–10 research challenges, 57–58
relevant purpose and content, 327 neuroticism, 56–57, 141–142 resilience promotion, 126–127
strength-based approach, 327 post-traumatic adjustment and, person-focused approaches, 108
team-based, 328 64–65 place attachment, 165
user-acceptability, 329–330 norepinephrine, 5–6 political impact of disasters, 206–207
mental illness, see serious mental illness allostatic contribution, 5 positive emotions, 15–16
mental toughness, 66 allostatic load, 5 mechanisms of efects, 128
Mexico City earthquake, 207, 212 factors promoting resilience, 5–6 positive emotionality/extroversion,
military personnel, 238, 323 social ties and, 81 56
resilience challenges, 238–242 resilience relationship, 66
operational resilience, 239–240 occupational stress, 50 resilience promotion, 127–128
post-deployment resilience, older adults, 125, 126, 135 serious mental illness treatment,
240–241 mental health problems, 136 282–283
psychological resilience, 241–242 resilience in, 138–143 acts of kindness, 283–284
social support importance, 293 bereavement and, 139–140 gratitude enhancement, 283
US Navy and Marine Corps caregiving and, 139 savoring, 283
approach, 242–249 chronic illness and, 139 sense of humor, 284,
core leader functions, 246–249 former combatants and prisoners see also emotions; personality
program goals, 242 of war, 140 factors
rationale, 242–243 future directions, 143 positive goals, 114
stress continuum model, 243–246, Holocaust survivors, 140 post-deployment resilience, 240–241
see also Battlemind Training in daily life, 138–139 post-traumatic growth, 143
System personality factors, 141–142 versus resiliency, 258–259
mindfulness, 97–98, 283 social support and, 142–143 post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD),
mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, trauma-speciic variables, 142 121
97–98 resilience theories, 136–138 children, 123, 124–125
mindfulness-based stress reduction, 98 burden perspective, 137 coping dysfunction, 67

363
Index

post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) positive emotions, 127–128 recreational interests, with serious
(cont.) worldview, 127 mental illness, 282
cortisol dysregulation, 2–3 propranolol, 6 redundancy, 163
delayed reactions, 123 prosocial behavior, 17–18 reframing, 113
dopamine and, 9 protective factors, 106, 176 see also cognitive reappraisal
following rape, 218–219, 222–223 psychoeducational resilience religion; religion/spirituality
future research, 231–232 enhancement, 293 Buddhism, 181
methodological issues, 219–220, public health, 340 Confucianism, 181
221 see also disaster management; deinition, 90, 91
peritraumatic factors, 225–228 emergency management religiosity as an environmental
post-assault factors, 230–231 inluence, 179
pre-trauma factors, 223–225 racial variation, 177–178 Taoism, 180
resilience promotion, 232–233 see also ethnic considerations religion/spirituality (RS), 90, 91, 178
Holocaust survivors, 140 rape dynamic nature of, 91
intervention issues, 129 consequences of, 218–219 trauma response and, 91–92
older adults, 125 deinitions, 219 future directions, 98–99
personality factors, 65–66 future research directions, 231–233 interventions, 97–98
hardiness, 63, methodological issues, 219–221 spirituality group therapy module,
prevention, 6 ideal study, 219–220 96–97
reward responses, 8 unacknowledged rape, 220–221 measurement of, 98–99
risk factors, 221–222 peritraumatic factors associated with multidimensional representation,
social support relationship, 68 post-rape function, 225–228 93–94
trauma intensity signiicance, acknowledgement of rape, religious coping, 94–95, 98
68–69 227–228 negative religious coping, 95
variation in trauma response, 122 assault characteristics, 225–226 resilience relationships, 94–95
potentially traumatic events, 120, 122 peritraumatic reactions, 226–227 relilience development, 93
heterogeneous trajectories following post-assault concerns, 228 spiritual “red lags”, 95, 98
exposure, 121–123 post-assault factors associated with guilt, 95
chronic dysfunction, 122–123 post rape function, 228–231 lack of forgiveness, 95
delayed reactions, 123 additional stresses and trauma, loss of faith, 95
resilience and recovery, 123 230–231 negative religious coping, 95
intervention implications across the coping strategies, 229–230 spiritual development, 92
lifespan, 128–129 social support, 229 model, 91
resilience promoting factors pre-trauma factors associated with rescue and clean-up operations, 205
demographic and contextual post-rape function, 223–225 resilience, 30, 45, 136, 289, 325–326
factors, 126 demographics, 223–224 across the lifespan, 124–126
in children, 128 mental health history, 225 as a process, 162–163
personality, 126–127 prior assault history, 224–225 bereavement and, 192–194
positive emotions, 127–128 prevalence, 218–219 capacities for, 163
worldview, 127 PTSD risk and protective factors, children, 103–104, 111–114, 123,
see also trauma 222–223 265
poverty, 264–265 resilience promotion, 232–233 attachments and, 308–309
resilience and, 271–273 rapidity, 163 poverty efects, 265–268
adults and families, 268 RCOPE, 99 community, see community
children, 265–268 reappraisal, 16, see also cognitive resilience
communities, 268–271 reappraisal construct development, 123–124
pragmatic coping, 126–127 recovery, 107, 123, 124 deinition, 2, 91, 103, 202–203
preparatory interventions, 297–298 culture signiicance, 182–183 controversies, 106–108
primary appraisal, 46 deinitions, 277–278 criteria, 106–107
prisoners of war, 140, see also warfare following bereavement, 219 developmental approaches, 104
problem-solving systems, 112 from serious mental illness, 276 following rape, 219
problem-solving therapy, 281 limitations of medical deinitions, good adaptation, 105
Project on Human Development in 276–277 narrow versus broad deinition,
Chicago Neighbourhoods, 270 themes, 278–279 107
promotive factors, 106, 126–128 coping, 278 threats, 105–106
demographic and contextual factors, hope and optimism, 278 time frame, 107
126 openness to new experiences, 278 deinitions, 325
in children, 128 self-respect and self- development, 93
personality, 126–127 determination, 278 late-emerging resilience, 107

364
Index

promotive and protective factors, variable-focused approaches, side-efects, 7


106 108–110 self-appraisal, 47
trauma impact, 120 see also resiliency self-blame, following rape, 230
dynamic perspective, 92–93, 104 Resilience Scales for Children and self-determination, 278
enhancement interventions, 289– Adolescents (RSCA), 290 self-eicacy, 47–49
290, 291–293, 302 resiliency, 108 changes in, 51
case example, 298–302 African-American resiliency demands and, 50–51
children, 312–317 systems, 179–180 traumatic demands, 49–50
disaster workers, 341–343, American-Indian/Alaska Native environmental context, 49–51
352–355 resiliency systems, 180 in children
hardiness training, 291–292 Arab-American resiliency systems, promotion of, 314
psychoeducational resilience 180 stress relationships, 311–312
enhancement, 293 Asian cultural resiliency systems, personality factors, 51–52
stress inoculation training1052 180–181 self-enhancement, 142
enhancement of related constructs, Caribbean black’ resiliency system, self-esteem, improvement with serious
293–298 181 mental illness, 282
learned optimism training, characteristics of, 176 self-regulation, 46–47, 113
295–296 cultivation, 179–182 children living in poverty, 267
social support interventions, culture and, 177–178 social cognitive theory, 47–49
293–295 in serious mental illness, 279 see also emotion regulation
well-being therapy, 296–297 resiliency factors, 279–280 self-respect, 278
family, see family resilience Hawaiian Native resiliency system, sense of community, 284
framework, 31–33, 114–115 182 sense of humor, 284
measures, 115 Latin-American cultural resiliency September 11 terrorist attack, New
methods, 115 systems, 7–9 York, 122, 141, 208
mission, 114 versus post-traumatic growth, 258– family meetings as community
models of change, 114–115 259, see also resilience intervention, 158
multilevel approaches, 115 resistance, 107 Haddon matrix application, 343–347
future directions, 115–116 resource dependency, 164 Lower Manhattan Community
in disaster research, 203–204 resources, boosting, 115 Recovery Project, 158
individual resilience, 149 Response to Stressful Experiences Scale serious mental illness, 276
measurement, 290–291 (RSES), 290 objections to traditional hierarchical
multilevel concept, 200, 202–203 reward system decision making, 277
older adults, 138–143 altered responses, 8–9 recovery, 276
bereavement and, 139–140 motivation to adapt, 112–113 limitations of medical deinitions,
caregiving and, 139 risk factors, 108–110 276–277
chronic illness and, 139 cumulative risk, 105 themes, 278–279
former combatants and prisoners in development, 105–106 resiliency in, 279
of war, 140 mediating efects, 110 resiliency factors, 279–280
future directions, 143 PTSD, 221–222 treatment, 280
Holocaust survivors, 140 following rape, 222–223 broadening the focus, 277–278
in daily life, 138–139 risk gradients, 105 novel strategies, 282–284
resilience theories, 136–138 risk prevention, 115 resilient strategies, 280–281
social support and, 142–143 robustness, 163 targeting deicits, 282
trauma recovery and, 136–138 Rochester Resilience Project, 266 targeting the efects of stress, 281
trauma-speciic variables, 142 rumination, 33–34, 36 serotonin (5-HT), 6–7
predictors of, 104 allostatic contribution, 6
preparatory interventions, Sago Mine disaster, 348–353 allostatic load, 6–7
297–298 Haddon matrix application, 350 factors promoting resilience, 7
promotion, see promotive factors savoring, 283 receptor dysfunction, 7
religion/spirituality relationships, scenario-based training, 297–298 serotonin transporter gene
93, 94–95 schizophrenia, 276, see also serious (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism, 7
reorienting, 212–213 mental illness sexual assault, see rape
research, 1 schools, 113 single nucleotide polymorphisms
competence and cascades, secondary appraisal, 46 (SNPs), 3
166–167 selective attention, 35–36 social capital, 164–165
developmental research, 108–111 training, 35–36 index of, 169
intervention models, 110 see also attention control (AC) measurement, 169–170
person-focused approaches, 108 selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors poverty efects, 269

365
Index

social closeness, 66 state-space grids, 116 trauma


social cognitive theory, 7, 47–49 stress, 2 heterogeneous trajectories following
social competence, 17–18 cardiovascular response, 79–80 exposure, 121–123
Social Health Index, 169 social ties and, 79–80 chronic dysfunction, 122–123
social indicator research, 168–170 disaster-related, 204 delayed reactions, 123
social integration, 77–78 preparatory interventions, 297–298 resilience and recovery, 123
behavioral efects, 79 reactivity in children related to historical conceptions of
physiological efects, 80 caregiving, 309–311 psychological trauma, 120–121
psychological beneits, 78 self-eicacy relationships in recovery
see also social ties children, 311–312 culture signiicance, 182–183
social isolation, 78 transactional theory of, 46 in older adults, 136–138
social networks, 164 workplace stress, 50 societal response to, 143
social potency, 66 see also acute stress response; stress trauma-speciic variables, 142
social support, 66, 67–68, 76–78 inoculation theory see also potentially traumatic
behavioral efects, 79 stress inoculation theory, 137 events
community resilience and, 164–165 stress inoculation training, 292–293 Trauma and Spirituality Group
emotional support, 77 stress vulnerability model, 279 Module, 96–97
following rape, 229 stressful life events, 30–31 trauma intensity, 68–69
informational support, 77 Stroop measure, 34 traumatic demands, 49–50
instrumental support, 77 Study of Adult Development, Harvard Trier Social Stress Test, 4
interventions, 293–295 Medical School, 266
matching hypothesis, 77 suppression, 195–196 US Marine Corps Combat and
older adults, 142–143 survivorship, 298 Operational Stress Control
physiological efects, 80, 81 sympathetic–adrenomedullary system, (COSC) program, 242–249
evidence for in chronic disease, 80–81 core leader functions, 246–249
82–83 goals, 242
resilience promotion, 84–85 TAFES program, 157 rationale, 242–243
psychological beneits, 78 Taoism, 180 stress continuum model, 243–246
evidence for in chronic disease, team sports, 315–316 US Navy Operational Stress Control
81–82 terrorist events, 202 (OSC) program, 242–249
resilience promotion, 84 engagement versus distress core leader functions, 246–249
serious mental illness and, 281 trajectories, 259–260 goals, 242
see also social ties engagement/distress interactions, rationale, 242–243
social ties, 76, 85 260–261
attachment relationships, 112 resilience and, 202 vigor, 257
behavioral efects, 79 conservation of resources theory, vulnerability theory, 137
bufering efects, 77 254–257
evidence for efects in chronic engagement theory, 257–258 Walsh Family Resilience Framework,
disease, 81–83 versus post-traumatic growth, 153–155, 158, 302
main efects, 77 258–259 communication, 155
physiological efects, 79–81, 82–83 see also September 11 terrorist family belief systems, 153–154
cardiovascular system, 79–80 attack, New York family organization, 154–155
immune system, 80 testosterone, 10–11 warfare, 323
neuroendocrine system, 80–81 stress response and, 11 former combatants and prisoners of
psychological beneits, 78, 81–82 threats, 105–106 war, 140
resilience promotion, 83–85 tight coupling, 164 PTSD, 121
physiological adjustment, 84–85 training hardiness relationship, 63–64,
psychological adjustment, 84 hardiness training, 291–292 see also military personnel
see also social integration; social learned optimism training, well-being, 66
support 295–296 well-being therapy, 296–297
Social Vulnerability Index (SOVI), mental health, 324, 326, 331 wellness, 163
168–170 implementation, 331–336 widowhood, 139–140,
spirituality, 141 moral courage, 317 see also bereavement
deinition, 90–91 resilience enhancement, 290 workplace stress, 50
interventions, 97–98 scenario-based training, 297–298 World Trade Center terrorist attack.
group therapy module, 96–97 stress inoculation training, 292–293 see September 11 terrorist
spiritual development, 92 Training and Doctrine Command, 335 attack, New York
model, 91 transactional theory of stress, 46 worldview, 127
see also religion/spirituality transformational coping, 291 resilience promotion, 127

366

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen