Sie sind auf Seite 1von 39

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/320346805

Measuring mental workload: ocular astigmatism aberration as a novel objective


index

Article  in  Ergonomics · October 2017


DOI: 10.1080/00140139.2017.1395913

CITATIONS READS
0 78

5 authors, including:

Raimundo Jiménez Rodríguez David Cárdenas Vélez


University of Granada University of Granada
44 PUBLICATIONS   272 CITATIONS    117 PUBLICATIONS   607 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jesús Vera
University of Granada
23 PUBLICATIONS   37 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The search of ocular indices of mental load and fatigue View project

ADHD and visual function View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jesús Vera on 12 October 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Ergonomics

Fo
rP

Measuring mental workload: ocular astigmatism aberration


ee

as a novel objective index


rR

Journal: Ergonomics

Manuscript ID TERG-2017-0314.R1

Manuscript Type: Original Article


ev

Date Submitted by the Author: 15-Sep-2017

Complete List of Authors: Jimenez, Raimundo; University of Granada, Optics


ie

Cárdenas, David; University of Granada, Physical Education and Sport;


University of Granada, Sport and Health Mixed Institute
Gonzalez Anera, Rosario; University of Granada, Optics
w

Jiménez, José Ramón; University of Granada, Optics


Vera, Jesús; University of Granada, Optics; University of Granada, Sport
and Health Mixed Institute
On

ocular physiology, optical quality, astigmatism aberration, pupil size,


Keywords:
mental workload
ly

URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk


Page 1 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Title: Measuring mental workload: ocular astigmatism aberration as a novel objective index
4
5
6 Running head: Mental workload modulates astigmatism aberration
7
8
Authors: Raimundo Jiménez1, David Cárdenas2,3, Rosario G. Anera1, José R. Jiménez1, Jesús
9
10
11 Vera1,2.
12
13
Affiliations:
14
Fo
15
16 1. Department of Optics, University of Granada, Spain.
17
18
rP
19 2. Mixed University Sport and Health Institute (iMUDS), University of Granada, Spain.
20
21
22 3. Department of Physical Education and Sport, University of Granada, Spain
ee
23
24 Conflict of interest: No conflicting interests for any author.
25
26
rR

27 Corresponding author: Jesús Vera, Department of Optics, University of Granada, Campus de


28
29 Fuentenueva, 18001 Granada, Spain. Tel: +34 958244067; fax: +34 958248533. E-mail:
30
ev

31 jesusvv@correo.ugr.es
32
33
34
ie

35
36
37 Funding: This work was supported in part by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and
w

38
39 Competitiveness under grant [number DEP2013-48211-R]; and under grant [number FIS2013-
40
On

41 42204-R]; and CEMIX (Centro Mixto UGR-MADOC, Army of Spain) under grant [number
42
43 PIN 11].
44
45
ly

46 Acknowledgments: The authors thank all the volunteers who selflessly participated in the study
47
48 and the Indo Optical company (Barcelona, Spain) for the assignment of the Refractive
49
50 Power/Corneal Analyzer used in this study.
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
1 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 2 of 37

1
2
3 Abstract
4
5
6 This study assessed the effect of two perceptually matched mental tasks with different levels of
7
8 mental demand on ocular aberrations in a group of young adults. We measured ocular aberration
9
10 with a wave-front sensor, and total, internal, and corneal RMS (root mean square) aberrations
11
12 were calculated from Zernike coefficients, considering natural and scaled pupils (5, 4.5, and 4
13
14 mm). We found that total, internal, and corneal astigmatism RMS showed significant differences
Fo
15
16 between mental tasks with natural pupils (p<.05), and this effect was maintained with 5 mm
17
18 scaled pupils (total RMS astigmatism, p<.05). Consistently, pupil size, intraocular pressure,
rP
19
20 perceived mental load, and cognitive performance were influenced by the level of mental
21
22 complexity (p<.05 for all). The findings suggest that ocular astigmatism aberration, mediated by
ee
23
24 intraocular pressure, could be an objective, valid, reliable index to evaluate the impact of
25
26 cognitive processing in conjunction with others physiological markers in real world contexts.
rR

27
28
29
30
ev

31
32 Keywords: ocular physiology, optical quality, astigmatism aberration, pupil size, mental
33
34 workload.
ie

35
36
37
w

38
39
40 Practitioner Summary: The search continues for a valid, reliable, convenient method of
On

41
42 measuring mental workload. In this study we found ocular astigmatism aberration is sensitive to
43
44 the cumulative effect of mental effort. It shows promise of being a novel ocular index which
45
ly

46 may help to assess mental workload in real situations.


47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
2 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 3 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Introduction
4
5
6 Many activities require cognitive effort, across situations such as driving, air traffic control,
7
8 surgical interventions, military operations, team sports, among many others. In any one of these,
9
10 mental workload may be a concern. In addition to widely used subjective scaling techniques
11
12 (Nygren 1991; Paas et al. 2003; Hart and Staveland 1988; Annett 2002), numerous studies have
13
14 been conducted to find reliable and sensitive physiological biomarkers that objectively
Fo
15
16 characterize the level of mental workload (e.g. Paas et al., 2003; Young et al., 2015). These
17
18 physiological techniques include such systemic physiological measures as heart activity (e.g.
rP
19
20 heart-rate variability) (Luque-Casado et al. 2015; Mansikka et al. 2016), near infrared
21
22 spectroscopy (Durantin et al. 2014), brain activity (Di Stasi et al. 2015), electrodermal activity
ee
23
24 (Ghaderyan and Abbasi 2016) or hormonal changes (Qi et al. 2016). Because ocular functioning
25
26 is tightly linked to neurocognitive processes, researchers have drawn their attention to ocular
rR

27
28 variables as potential objective biomarkers of these processes. Thus, pupillary responses (De
29
30 Gee, Knapen, and Donner 2014; Wang and Munoz 2015; Klingner, Tversky, and Hanrahan
ev

31
32 2011), blink rate (Rosenfield et al. 2015), accommodative response (Davies, Wolffsohn, and
33
34 Gilmartin 2005; Vera et al. 2016), intraocular pressure (Brody, Erb, Veit, & Rau, 1999; Vera et
ie

35
36 al., 2016; Vera et al., 2017), critical flicker fusion (Luczak and Sobolewski 2005) or saccadic
37
w

38 eye movements (Di Stasi, Antolí, and Cañas 2011) have been well documented.
39
40
On

41 Although pupil size has been demonstrated to show a positive correlation with increasing
42
43 cognitive processing demand (De Gee, Knapen, and Donner 2014; Wang and Munoz 2015;
44
45
ly

Klingner, Tversky, and Hanrahan 2011), the pupil is directly dependent on the illumination
46
47 level, which leads to ceiling effects in dim surroundings (Wang and Munoz 2015). Also,
48
49 controversial findings have been reported concerning ocular accommodation during these
50
51 processes (Bullimore and Gilmartin 1988; Jainta, Hoormann, and Jaschinski 2008; Vera et al.
52
53 2016; Davies, Wolffsohn, and Gilmartin 2005), and few works have addressed the effect of
54
55 mental tasks on intraocular pressure (Brody et al., 1999; Vera et al., 2016; Vera et al., 2017).
56
57
Notably, these physiological parameters are closely linked, making it difficult to monitor them
58
59
60
3 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 4 of 37

1
2
3 independently under ecological conditions (Read et al. 2009; Brubaker 2003). Moreover, it
4
5 would be helpful to find a new ocular index which includes these interactions and is sensitive to
6
7 mentally demanding tasks.
8
9
10 The quality of images of the outside world projected on the retina is limited by ocular
11
12 aberrations, which reflect the imperfections of the ocular optic elements arranged along the
13
14 ocular axis, such as the anterior and posterior corneal surfaces, the aqueous humour, the
Fo
15
16 crystalline lens, and the vitreous humour. Ocular aberrations reduce image contrast and limit
17
18 spatial frequencies available for further stages of the visual processing. The optical performance
rP
19
20 of the eye is usually described in terms of wave aberrations, which refer to the deviations of a
21
22 wavefront exiting the pupil after progressing through the optics of the tested eye when
ee
23
24 compared to a reference wavefront that is aberration free. Also, there are different types of
25
26 aberrations which contribute to wavefront deformation from the ideal spherical shape (see
rR

27
28 Lombardo and Lombardo 2010 for review). The most common method for describing the
29
30 wavefront error of the eye is the normalized Zernike polynomial expansion. The weights of
ev

31
32 these polynomials on ocular aberrations are called the Zernike coefficients (cmn, corresponding
33
34 to the radial order n and the meridional frequency m), representing the magnitude of each
ie

35
36 particular ocular aberration present in the wave aberration (Thibos et al. 2002).
37
w

38
39 The number of aberrations per order increases as the order of the Zernike polynomials increases,
40
On

41 and wavefront aberrations can be further classified based on their Zernike order as lower (n ≤ 2)
42
43 or higher order aberrations (n ≥ 3). Both influence the visual performance of an individual but
44
45
ly

lower order aberrations, since are related to refractive error, can be easily alleviated by
46
47 prescribing compensatory spectacles, contact lenses, or refractive surgery (Martin et al. 2011).
48
49 However, higher order aberrations (HOA, n ≥ 3) cannot be easily corrected with these
50
51 procedures and an adaptive optics system, phase plates, customized contact lenses, and
52
53 customized refractive surgery are needed (Porter et al. 2006). Third order aberrations include
54
55 trefoil and coma. Fourth order includes quatrefoil, secondary astigmatism and spherical
56
57
aberration. Trefoil, coma, quatrefoil and secondary astigmatism are classified as a form of
58
59
60
4 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 5 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 irregular astigmatism which results in an irregular or distorted wavefront (Dai, 2008). Spherical
4
5 aberration is influenced by pupil diameter and ocular accommodation and become to be
6
7 important during dim lighting conditions, causing visual halos and night vision disturbances
8
9 (Applegate et al. 2007). The fifth, sixth, seventh and eighth order aberrations are usually very
10
11 small in magnitude and therefore negligible (Philip et al. 2012). However, wavefront aberrations
12
13 represented with Zernike polynomials and their coefficients can be difficult to understand, and
14
Fo
15 root mean square (RMS) is typically used as a global metric for optical quality (Pepose and
16
17 Applegate 2005). RMS represents the deviation of the wavefront from a plane wavefront
18
rP
19 represented as a single digit (in microns [µm]) and it can be determined individually for lower
20
21 or higher order aberrations or it can be calculated for the entire wavefront (Dai 2008). The
22
ee
23 higher the RMS wavefront error, the larger the wavefront aberration and the worse the optical
24
25 quality. The development of wavefront sensor allows the rapid, accurate, repeatable, and
26
rR

27 objective measurements of ocular aberrations, and RMS can be calculated using the information
28
29 given by the aberrometer once a measurement has been taken (Rozema, Van Dyck and
30
ev

31 Tassignon 2006). In addition, besides total ocular aberration, corneal and internal aberrations
32
33
can also be obtained separately with good precision (Holzer, Goebels, and Auffarth, 2006).
34
ie

35
36 As stated above, the changes in the activity of the autonomous nervous system as consequence
37
w

38 of cognitive processing are reflected in a variety of ocular physiological indices such as pupil
39
40 size (Wang and Munoz 2015), ocular accommodation (Davies, Wolffsohn, and Gilmartin 2005)
On

41
42 and intraocular pressure (Vera et al. 2017). Relevantly, ocular aberrations are highly dependent
43
44
of these components (e.g. pupil size, crystalline lens, intraocular pressure), and therefore, they
45
ly

46
are sensitive to any modification in these ocular variables (Lombardo and Lombardo 2010; Qu
47
48
et al. 2007). In this sense, we propose that ocular aberrations could be used to evaluate the
49
50
51
effect of mentally demanding task or predict the level of autonomous activation. To date, no
52
53 study has assessed the ocular optical quality, in terms of ocular aberrations, under conditions of
54
55 cognitive stress.
56
57
58
59
60
5 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 6 of 37

1
2
3 The present study aimed to assess the effect of mental task complexity on ocular aberrations,
4
5 using a wavefront sensor and considering three constant pupil diameters (5, 4.5, and 4 mm) and
6
7 constant accommodative-demand stimulus. Pupil size, intraocular pressure, subjective
8
9 questionnaires, and cognitive performance were also analysed with control purposes.
10
11 Participants performed two perceptually matched mental tasks (on different days and
12
13 counterbalanced order) but with different levels of mental complexity (3-back task and oddball).
14
Fo
15 We hypothesised that ocular aberrations could indicate changes in the activity of the
16
17 autonomous nervous system as a result of cognitive processing.
18
rP
19
20 Methods
21
22
Participants and ethical approval
ee
23
24
25
26 The study was approved by the University of Granada (Spain) Institutional Review Board (IRB:
rR

27
28 112/CEIH/2016) and the research followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. 14
29
30 participants were enrolled in this study, and they were screened according to the following
ev

31
32 inclusion criteria: (a) presenting static monocular visual acuity ≤0 log MAR (minimum angle of
33
34 resolution) in both eyes with their best optical correction, (b) belonging to asymptomatic group
ie

35
36 at Conlon (Conlon et al. 1999) and CISS (Borsting, Chase, and Ridder 2007) surveys and not
37
w

38 presenting any accommodative and binocular dysfunction, (c) presenting normal pupillary
39
40 reflex to light and no anisocoria (d) not taking any medication (except contraceptives), (e)
On

41
42 baseline intraocular pressure ≤ 21 mmHg (f) being healthy (not suffering any current illness or
43
44 mental disorder), and (e) scoring ≤ 3 on the Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS), which measures
45
ly

46 subjective levels of arousal and fatigue, before each of the two main experimental sessions
47
48 (Hoddes, Zarcone, and Dement 1972).
49
50
51 For the screening of potential participants, autorefraction was performed and the patient’s
52
53 previous clinical records were available at the time of testing and were used as a starting point
54
55 for subjective refraction in most cases. Subjective refraction was conducted using a Snellen
56
57 chart at 6 m on both eyes of each subject by the same investigator (JV) and was performed
58
59
60
6 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 7 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 using a trial frame and back vertex distance of 12 mm. The endpoint criteria were maximum
4
5 plus sphere and minimum minus cylinder power maintaining the best visual acuity, which was
6
7 recorded in logMAR (Lovie-Kitchin 2015). Visual function was evaluated following the
8
9 recommendations and normative data given by Scheiman and Wick (2008). Accommodative
10
11 function was tested with the amplitude of accommodation and accommodative response, while
12
13 the binocular function was assessed with the near point of convergence, negative and positive
14
Fo
15 fusional vergences and near stereoacuity. All participants fulfilled the inclusion criteria
16
17 established in the present study. Additionally, participants were instructed to abstain from
18
rP
19 alcohol consumption 24 h and from caffeine-based drinks 12 h before each experimental
20
21 session, and were asked to have at least 7 h of sleep the night prior to the study. We excluded
22
ee
23 two participants, one for sickness during the experimental session, and another for failures
24
25 during data collection. Thus, we analysed data from a total of 12 naïve participants (mean ± SD;
26
rR

27 age: 22.27 ± 2.69 years, 5 women, 7 men). Being this study first of its nature, no power
28
29 calculation was performed. An appropriate sample size was considered based on previous cohort
30
ev

31 studies, where statistically significant differences in ocular indices as consequence of cognitive


32
33
processing were found (Siegenthaler et al. 2014; Vera et al. 2016).
34
ie

35
36 Procedure
37
w

38
39 Participants took part in three sessions: (a) after signing the informed consent form and filling
40
On

41 visual questionnaires, the participants were given instructions about the mentally demanding
42
43 tasks and the NASA-TLX scale. Lastly, after a verbal explanation, they were given 5 min to
44
45
ly

practice with both tasks (3-back and oddball), so that all volunteers were familiar with them
46
47 before the commencement of the study. All participants performed the mentally demanding
48
49 tasks with best spectacle correction. The second (b) and the third session (c) constituted the
50
51 primary focus of the study in which two mental tasks with different levels of complexity were
52
53 performed. These were separated by a minimum of 24 h and a maximum of 48 h and both
54
55 sessions were scheduled at the same time of the day to avoid diurnal fluctuations that affect
56
57
58
59
60
7 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 8 of 37

1
2
3 arousal levels (Del Rio-Bermudez et al. 2014) and were performed in a counterbalanced order
4
5 (see Figure 1).
6
7
8 Stimuli and instruments
9
10
11 The two experimental sessions were identical except from the level of mental complexity
12
13 implemented (3-back, oddball). Both tasks lasted 33 min, and were displayed on a 19-in screen
14
(1920 x 1080 pixels) situated 2.5 m from the participant’s eyes in order to avoid possible visual
Fo
15
16
17 stress caused by prolonged near viewing (Jaschinsky, Heuer & Kylian, 1999; Vera et al., 2016).
18
rP
19 Visual stimuli subtended 7 min of arc, which corresponds to 0.85 logMAR visual acuity, and
20
21 was thus clearly visible for any participant of this study. Mentally demanding tasks were
22
interrupted for one min at minute 11 to assess the dependent variables. To make responses,
ee
23
24
25 participants held a clicker on their dominant hand and a distinctive sound was used as feedback
26
rR

27 for each response. The experiment was performed in a dimly lit laboratory. The illuminance of
28
29 the room was quantified (in the corneal plane) with an Illuminance meter (T-10, Konica
30
Minolta, Inc., Japan), and kept constant throughout the experiment [mean ± SD: 62.59 ± 1.09
ev

31
32
33 lux].
34
ie

35
36 Both tasks were divided into intervals consisting of 105 s long, and a rest period of 15s for the
37
w

38 first two intervals and 75 s for the third one. This sequence was maintained over the entire
39
40 session. All subjects performed 15 mentally demanding task intervals, and this was therefore the
On

41
42 total number of intervals considered to check cognitive involvement and performance.
43
44
45 Mentally demanding tasks
ly

46
47
48 The 3-back task (a 3-digit load version of the N-back task) (Owen et al. 2005) consisted of a
49
50 series of digits (1, 2, or 3), presented randomly, one at a time and at a rate of one digit every
51
52 2500 ms (each digit was presented for 1000 ms, and the inter-stimulus interval was 2000 ms). In
53
54 each trial, participants were asked whether the digit currently on screen was the same as the one
55
56 presented 3 positions earlier, and were instructed to press a button every time a match was
57
58 observed (participants did nothing if there was no match). This task thus requires keeping the
59
60
8 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 9 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 three last digits in working memory (working memory load), comparing every new digit with
4
5 the earliest of them (checking), incorporating the new item and discarding the earliest one for
6
7 further comparisons (updating). The task can be manipulated to augment or reduce the working
8
9 memory load, depending on the number of digits the participant must keep in mind (3 in the
10
11 current version of the task).
12
13
14 The oddball condition was designed to be perceptually identical to the 3-back task (Gomez and
Fo
15
16 Perea 2009). This condition allowed us to control the potential influence of stimulus setting
17
18 features (e.g. stimulus duration, inter-stimulus interval, see Luque-Casado et al., 2015). Before
rP
19
20 the task started, a single digit (1, 2 or 3) was presented on screen, and the participant was
21
22 instructed to press the button every time that digit appeared on the screen during that session,
ee
23
24 and to withhold the response for the other 2 digits. This task imposes little or no working-
25
26 memory load, but uses exactly the same stimuli as the 3-back task, requires vigilance during the
rR

27
28 whole session, and the same rate of response (on average, one response/3 trials).
29
30
ev

31 Ocular aberrations
32
33
34 Wavefront aberrations were measured before mentally demanding tasks, just after 11 min of
ie

35
36 mental effort, after finishing the 33 min of the mental task, and after 10 min of passive recovery.
37
w

38 Aberrometry was performed under mesopic conditions without any cycloplegia or mydriasis by
39
40 the OPD Scan III (Nidek Inc., Tokyo, Japan). The OPD-Scan lll is a five-in-one true refractive
On

41
42 work station combined with a topographer, wavefront aberrometer, keratometer, and
43
44 pupillometer, for which the repeatability and accuracy have been determined in previous studies
45
ly

46 (Burakgazi et al. 2006). Combined wavefront aberrometry and corneal topography can
47
48 differentiate between aberrations caused by the anterior cornea or by the internal ocular system.
49
50 Total ocular aberrations are the result of corneal and internal ocular aberrations.
51
52
53 The OPD-Scan III instrument software automatically calculates internal eye aberrations from
54
55 whole eye aberrometry and corneal topography data based on the assumption that the cornea
56
57 contributes 75% towards the total refractive power of the eye and the crystalline lens 25%. One
58
59
60
9 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 10 of 37

1
2
3 measurement from each eye was evaluated using software that follows the standards for
4
5 calculating and reporting the optical aberrations of eyes in terms of RMS. For each participant,
6
7 the seat height was adjusted, and their head and chin were placed properly. The device was
8
9 realigned before each imaging and this was repeated in case of map incompleteness or blinking
10
11 error. The measurements were taken across a naturally dilated pupil (mesopic pupil) and we
12
13 calculated the Zernike coefficients for the total ocular aberrations. Subsequently, internal and
14
Fo
15 corneal aberrations were exported in the corresponding Zernike coefficients, and total, internal,
16
17 and corneal aberrations were scaled to 5, 4.5, and 4 mm using the expression of Diaz et al.
18
rP
19 (2009). Higher order aberrations were measured out to the eighth Zernike order. The parameters
20
21 analysed included: 1) RMS from third to eight orders of total higher order aberrations (HOA);
22
ee
23 2) RMS of the total spherical aberration (SA); 3) RMS of total coma aberration (CA); 4) RMS
24
25 of total trefoil v (TA); and 5) RMS of total astigmatism aberration (AA).
26
rR

27
28 Subjective questionnaires (SSS and NASA-TLX)
29
30
The Stanford Sleepiness (SSS) is a 7-point Likert scale used to evaluate individuals’ self-
ev

31
32
33 reported activation, which ranges from 1 “very active, alert or awake” to 7 “very sleepy”
34
ie

35 (Hoddes, Zarcone, and Dement 1972). The SSS was filled upon arrival to the lab in both
36
37 experimental sessions. Meanwhile, the mental workload with the NASA-TLX scale was
w

38
39 assessed after cognitive tasks. The NASA-TLX is composed of six subscales: Mental demand
40
On

41 (How much mental and perceptual activity was required?), Physical demand (How much
42
43 physical activity was required?), Temporal demand (How much time pressure did you feel due
44
45
ly

to the rate of pace at which the tasks or task elements occurred?), Performance (How sucessful
46
47 do you think you were in accomplishing the goals of the task set by the experimenter?), Effort
48
49 (How hard did you have to work to accomplish your level of perfomance?), and Frustratition
50
51 level (How insecure, discouraged, irritated, stressed, and annoyed vs. secure, gratified, content,
52
53 relaxed, and pleasant did you feel during the task? The participants had to score each subscale
54
55 into 20 equals intervals anchored by a bipolar descriptor (e.g. Low/High); this score was
56
57
multiplied by 5, resulting in a final score between 0 and 100 (Hart and Staveland 1988).
58
59
60
10 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 11 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Cognitive-perfomance calculation
4
5
6 Regardless of the task (3-back or oddball) each response qualified as a hit (correct click), a false
7
8 alarm (or commission error, incorrect click), a correct rejection (correctly non-clicking), or a
9
10 miss (omission error, incorrectly not clicking). We calculated the hit rate h (correct button
11
12 pressings /total number of go trials) and the false alarm rate f (incorrect button pressings/total
13
14 number of no-go trials) for each block of the task (namely, the total number of intervals was
Fo
15
16 divided in 3 equal blocks of 5 intervals (15 intervals in total), and h and f were computed for
17
18 each of these three blocks) to check the cognitive performance. Larger h and smaller f values
rP
19
20 indicate better response discriminability between go and no-go trials. A composite measure of
21
22 discriminability can be computed as the arcsine of f minus the arcsine of h. The larger this
ee
23
24 difference of arcsines, the better the performance of the task (Perales et al. 2005).
25
26
rR

27
28 Experimental design and statistical analysis
29
30
A within-participant repeated-measures design was used to test the effect of mental-task
ev

31
32
33 complexity on ocular aberrations. The measurement moment (pre, block 1, block 2, and
34
ie

35 recovery) and mental task complexity (3-back, oddball) were the within-subject factors, and
36
37 HOA, SA, CA, TA, and AA RMSs were the dependent variables. We also analysed the pupil
w

38
39 diameter and intraocular pressure at the same points of ocular aberrations with control purposes
40
On

41 (see Supplementary Material), and determined the participant’s subjective mental workload via
42
43 standardized questionnaires and cognitive performance scores.
44
45
ly

46 A t-test for related samples considering mental task complexity (3-back, oddball) as the within-
47
48 subjects factor was carried out to analyse the effect of task complexity on cognitive performance
49
50 and perceived mental load (NASA-TLX).
51
52
53 Finally, we analysed total aberrations under mesopic conditions for the RMS of HOA, SA, CA,
54
55 TA, and AA. Then, for those total RMSs under mesopic conditions which showed statistical
56
57 significance (p < .05), we also evaluated corneal and internal aberrations, and pupil diameters
58
59
60
11 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 12 of 37

1
2
3 were scaled to 5, 4.5, and 4 mm. A repeated-measures ANOVA was performed using the
4
5 measurement moment (4) and the mental task complexity (2) as the within-participants factors
6
7 for each pupil diameter (mesopic, 5 mm, 4.5 mm, and 4 mm) and type of aberration (total,
8
9 internal, and corneal). We used the Greenhouse-Geisser adjustment to correct for the violation
10
11 of the sphericity assumption, and also the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was
12
13 used. The significance level was set at p < .05, and standardised effect size was reported by
14
Fo
15 means of the partial ƞ² for Fs and the Cohen’s d for t tests.
16
17
18 Results
rP
19
20
21 Cognitive-manipulation check
22
ee
23
24 Cognitive performance showed a significant difference between mental-task levels [t(11) = 7.47,
25
26 p < .01, d = 17.36]. The high load version (3-back) clearly displayed lower discriminability
rR

27
28 values than the low version (0.67 ± 0.02, and 1.49 ± 0.07, respectively). Still, the inspection of
29
30 confidence intervals (average +/- 2.7 standard errors) clearly shows that performance in the 3-
ev

31
32 back task was much above 0 (the level that would indicate random performance). Also, to
33
34 examine the effectiveness of mental level manipulation, we tested the subjective mental load
ie

35
36 reported in each experimental session with the NASA-TLX scale. The subjective result
37
w

38 demonstrated higher mental demand in the 3-back task (38.68 ± 3.82) than in the oddball
39
40 condition (15.07 ± 1.78) [t(11) = -7.66, p < .01, d = -8.43].
On

41
42
43 Effect of mental-task complexity on ocular aberrations
44
45
ly

46 High order RMS


47
48
Firstly, total HO RMS with natural pupils yielded no statistical significance for the mental task,
49
50
the measurement moment and the interaction [F(1,11) = 2.05, p = .18; F(3,33) = 1, p = .403; and
51
52
F(3,33) = 2.53, p = .103, respectively].
53
54
55 Spherical RMS
56
57
58
59
60
12 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 13 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Total spherical RMS with natural pupils displayed no significant differences for the mental task
4
5 [F(1,11) = 1.10, p = .316], and neither for measurement moment or the interaction [F < 1 in
6
7 both].
8
9
10 Coma RMS
11
12
13 For its part, total coma RMS exhibited no statistical differences for the mental task,
14
measurement moment, and the interaction [F < 1 in the three analysis].
Fo
15
16
17
Trefoil RMS
18
rP
19
20 Considering natural pupils, the total trefoil RMS showed no statistical significance for the
21
22 mental task, the measurement moment, or the interaction, [F(1,11) = 2.37, p = .152; F(3,33) =
ee
23
24
2.11, p = .118; and F(3,33) = 2.77, p = .059, respectively].
25
26
rR

27 Astigmatism RMS
28
29
30 Total astigmatism RMS with natural pupils proved to be significant for the mental task and the
ev

31
32 interaction [F(1,11) = 7.45, p = .02, ƞp² = .70; and F(3,33) = 3.17, p = .037, ƞp²=.68,
33
34 respectively], whereas no effect was found for the measurement moment [F < 1]. Due to the
ie

35
36 interactive effect, two separate ANOVAs showed that the 3-back task revealed a marginally
37
w

38 significant effect in total astigmatism RMS whereas the oddball condition showed no statistical
39
40 significance [F(3,33) = 2.39; p =.086, ƞp² = .36 vs. F(3,33) = .76, p = .487]. Internal astigmatism
On

41
42 RMS yielded a significant effect for the mental task complexity [F(1,11) = 4.33, p = .049, ƞp² =
43
44 .48] and no effect for the measurement moment or interaction [F(3,33) = 1.58, p = .223; and
45
ly

46 F(3,33) = 1.80, p = .167, respectively]. Similarly, corneal astigmatism RMS showed


47
48
significance for the mental task [F(1,11) = 4.89, p = .045, ƞp² = .52], and no effect was found for
49
50
the measurement moment or the interaction [F(3,33) = 1.52, p=.239; and F(3,33) = 2.25, p =
51
52
.101, respectively] (Figure 2).
53
54
55 Then, we analysed astigmatism RMS for a 5-mm pupil. Total astigmatism RMS yielded an
56
57 effect for the mental task and the measurement moment [F(1,11) = 5.02, p = .047, ƞp²=.53;
58
59
60
13 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 14 of 37

1
2
3 F(3,33) = 5.15, p < .01, ƞp²=.89, respectively] whereas no effect was found for the interaction [F
4
5 < 1]. Subsequently, the condition with high level of complexity (3-back) increased total
6
7 astigmatism RMS [F(3,33) = 3.36, p = .03, ƞp²=.71] against no changes for the oddball task
8
9 [F(3,33) = 1.16, p = .34]. Internal astigmatism RMS showed no effect for mental task,
10
11 measurement moment or the interaction of both factors [F(1,11) = 2.01, p = .184; F(3,33) =
12
13 1.61, p = .217; and F(3,33) = 1.99, p = .151, respectively]. Corneal astigmatism RMS also
14
Fo
15 showed no effect for any factor (mental task and measurement moment) or the interaction
16
17 [F(1,11) = 1.52, p = .243; F(3,33) = 1.37, p = .274; and F(3,33) = 1.20, p = .323, respectively].
18
rP
19
20 For pupil scaled to 4.5-mm, total astigmatism RMS showed an effect for the measurement
21
22 moment and a marginal effect for the mental task [F(3,33) = 4.46, p = .01, ƞp²=.84; and F(1,11)
ee
23
24 = 3.78, p = .078, ƞp²=.43, respectively] but no effect for the interaction [F(3,33) = 1.59, p = .22].
25
26 Separate analysis for each condition showed a significant effect for the 3-back condition,
rR

27
28 whereas no effect was detected for the oddball task [F(3,33) = 3.44, p=.028, ƞp²=.72; and
29
30 F(3,33) = 1.09, p = .366, respectively]. For its part, internal astigmatism RMS yielded no effect
ev

31
32 for the two main factors [F<1, for both] and for the interaction [F(3,33) = 1.82, p = .171].
33
34 Corneal astigmatism RMS also showed no effect for mental task, measurement moment, or
ie

35
36 interaction [F(1,11) = 1.08, p = .322; F(3,33) = 1, p = .389; and F(3,33) = 1.33, p = .282,
37
w

38 respectively].
39
40
On

41 Finally, total, internal, and corneal astigmatism RMS were calculated for 4-mm pupils. Total
42
43 astigmatism RMS demonstrated no effect for mental task, measurement moment and interaction
44
45
ly

[F(1,11) = 2.21, p = .165; and F(3,33)=1.76, p = .175; and F(3,33) < 1, respectively]. No effect
46
47 was found for either condition when analysed separately [F(3,33) = 1.41, p = .264 low-load
48
49 condition; and F(3,33) < 1 high-load condition]. Similarly, internal astigmatism RMS showed
50
51 no effect for any factor (mental task and measurement moment) nor for the interaction [F(1,11)
52
53 = 1.40, p = .261; F(3,33) < 1; and F(3,33) = 2.40, p = .125, respectively]. Corneal astigmatism
54
55 RMS also displayed no effect for either of the two main factors or the interaction [F(1,11) =
56
57
1.50, p = .246 for mental task; F(3,33) < 1 for the measurement moment; and F(3,33) = 2.11,
58
59
60
14 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 15 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 p=.158 for the interaction]. All the astigmatism RMSs with scaled pupil sizes are displayed in
4
5 Figure 3.
6
7
8 Discussion
9
10
11 Based on the fact that psychological stress has demonstrated to modify the ocular physiology
12
13 (e.g. pupil size [Wang and Munoz 2015], ocular accommodation [Davies, Wolffsohn, and
14
Gilmartin 2005], and intraocular pressure [Vera et al. 2017]). Here, we investigated the effect of
Fo
15
16
17 mental-task complexity on optical quality in terms of higher order aberrations RMSs, which are
18
rP
19 tightly linked to the different components and structures of the human eye such as the pupil size,
20
21 crystalline lens or intraocular pressure (Lombardo and Lombardo 2010; Qu et al. 2007). To do
22
this, participants performed two matched experimental sessions, where the only difference was
ee
23
24
25 the level of mental demand (3-back task and oddball version). We found that performing the
26
rR

27 condition with high level of complexity (3-back) impacts the total AA RMS and promotes
28
29 lowered optical quality, which was maintained when pupils were scaled to 5-mm.
30
ev

31
32 A successful mental task complexity manipulation was corroborated with the cognitive
33
34 performance score and subjective response (NASA-TLX), showing significant differences in the
ie

35
36 two measures, which indirectly indicated that the 3-back task was more difficult than the
37
w

38 oddball condition (Karavidas et al. 2010; Luque-Casado et al. 2015). In the same line, we have
39
40 used the pupil size, a classical physiological indicator of cognitive demand-effort (Wang and
On

41
42 Munoz 2015; Klingner, Tversky, and Hanrahan 2011) and intraocular pressure (Vera et al. 2017)
43
44 as control indices (see Supplementary Material in the Appendix). Our results show that pupil
45
ly

46 size and intraocular pressure are modulated by cognitive process, with greater pupil size and
47
48 higher intraocular pressure values at a high workload level (p < .05), and both quickly
49
50 diminishing once processing is terminated, in agreement with findings by numerous authors
51
52 (Hess and Polt 1964; Wang and Munoz 2015; Klingner, Tversky, and Hanrahan 2011; Vera et al.
53
54 2017; Brody et al. 1999). Due to the close relationship between pupil size and ocular
55
56 aberrations, which has been widely noted in the literature, higher aberrations with larger pupils
57
58
59
60
15 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 16 of 37

1
2
3 would be expected (Applegate et al. 2007; Wang et al. 2003). To avoid this interdependence,
4
5 which would limit the use of ocular aberrations in real-world contexts where illumination
6
7 conditions are uncontrollable (Wang and Munoz 2015), we have assessed the ocular aberrations
8
9 under different scaled pupil sizes, as we will discuss below.
10
11
12 Total ocular HOA, SA, CA, and TA RMSs showed no changes with the increase of mental-task
13
14 complexity with natural pupils. We found this effect only on AA RMS, with the amount of total
Fo
15
16 AA increasing with higher mental complexity. This variation was also found for internal and
17
18 corneal AA, demonstrating a dual effect from both. When pupils were scaled to 5 mm, similar
rP
19
20 results were found only for total AA, but no for internal and corneal AA. However, with 4.5 and
21
22 4 mm pupils, no effect of mental task complexity was detected. Our results agree with those of
ee
23
24 Liang (1997), who stated that AA play a substantial role in retinal-image quality when the pupil
25
26 is large but this effect is insignificant with small pupils.
rR

27
28
29 Ocular accommodation depends mainly on the accommodative-demand stimuli and also it has
30
been established that autonomous variations due to cognitive processing can play an additional
ev

31
32
33 role in the variation of accommodative response (Vera et al. 2016; Davies, Wolffsohn, and
34
ie

35 Gilmartin 2005). Corneal and lens changes occur during accommodation, which could affect the
36
37 aberration pattern. The change in corneal shape and curvature shows contradictory results
w

38
39 (Yasuda, Yamaguchi, and Ohkoshi 2003; Pierścionek, Popiołek-Masajada, and Kasprzak 2001;
40
On

41 Buehren et al. 2003), and despite these possible changes, it seems that the modification of
42
43 corneal HOA aberration is relatively small (He et al. 2003). On the other hand, variations in
44
45
ly

lower order aberrations (defocus and astigmatism) (Mutti, Enlow, and Mitchell 2001), and in
46
47 higher order aberrations (Atchison et al. 1995; Cheng et al. 2004; Ninomiya et al. 2002) occur
48
49 mainly because the shape, position, and refractive index gradient of the crystalline lens changes
50
51 (Garner and Yap 1997; Koretz, Cook, and Kaufman 2002). However, several authors have
52
53 indicated that high accommodative levels (greater than 3 diopters [D]) are necessary to induce
54
55 significant changes on the RMS of the total higher order (Atchison et al. 1995; Ninomiya et al.
56
57
2002), and spherical aberrations (Cheng et al. 2004). In the present study, the stimulus distance
58
59
60
16 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 17 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 was maintained constant with a small accommodative demand (0.4 D) over the entire
4
5 experiment, and thus, the possible changes in astigmatism aberrations could not be clearly
6
7 explained by dynamic changes in accommodation due to cognitive processing, since we have
8
9 found no significant changes in the others aberrations (e.g. high order or spherical) with respect
10
11 to level of mental task complexity.
12
13
14 Rosales et al. (2008) have shown, in rhesus monkey, that while accommodating a significant tilt
Fo
15
16 or vertical shift of the crystalline lens around the horizontal axis occurs, and it seems feasible
17
18 that it can be extrapolated in human eyes, with resulting changes in coma and astigmatism
rP
19
20 aberrations (Cheng et al. 2004). With respect to the coma aberration, the direction and
21
22 magnitude of the change varies greatly between subjects and no clear trends have been observed
ee
23
24 during accommodation (He, Burns, and Marcos 2000; Atchison et al. 1995), so that coma
25
26 aberration would not be an appropriate candidate to capture the effect of mental workload (He,
rR

27
28 Burns, and Marcos 2000; Atchison et al. 1995). In the present study, coma aberration did not
29
30 show any significant changes between tasks. The only ocular aberration that proved to be
ev

31
32 sensitive to mental task complexity manipulation was ocular astigmatism aberration even when
33
34 the pupil size was scaled up to 5 mm. Small pupils diminish the amount of astigmatism
ie

35
36 aberrations (Liang and Williams 1997), and similar to our case, we found that scaling pupils
37
w

38 under 4.5 mm masked the possible effect of the mental workload level in this parameter. Here,
39
40 we have not assessed any parameter of accommodative function (i.e. accommodative response)
On

41
42 in both experimental sessions, and therefore, no solid conclusions can be draw about the role of
43
44
ocular accommodation in the RMS astigmatism aberration changes found in the current study.
45
ly

46
Nevertheless, based on the available literature, which states that 3 D or more are necessary to
47
48
induce significant changes on ocular aberrations (Atchison et al. 1995; Ninomiya et al. 2002),
49
50
51
we consider that the influence of the possible accommodative changes induced by mental effort
52
53 may be minimal.
54
55 Another factor that may influence ocular aberrations is intraocular pressure (i.e. the pressure
56
57
exerted by the aqueous humor against the outer layer of the eye [Kiel et al. 2011]), which has
58
59
60
17 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 18 of 37

1
2
3 been demonstrated to fluctuate during cognitive tasks (Brody et al. 1999; Vera et al. 2016; Vera
4
5 et al. 2017). In this regard, Mierdel et al. (2004) reported an association between several Zernike
6
7 coefficients and changes of intraocular pressure. Along the same line, intraocular pressure has
8
9 been associated ocular aberrations, indicating some effect of the pressure exerted by the
10
11 contents of the eyeball (i.e. intraocular pressure) on the pattern of ocular aberrations (Asejczyk-
12
13 Widlicka and Pierscionek 2007; Qu et al. 2007). Increased intraocular pressure is produced by
14
Fo
15 impeded aqueous humor outflow, and this impeded outflow is asymmetrical, in other words,
16
17 some areas around the circumference of the aqueous humor outflow pathway present a higher
18
rP
19 blockage of outflow (i.e. high outflow resistance) than others (Carreon et al. 2016). The
20
21 intraocular pressure rise induced by mental effort would exacerbate this segmental (i.e.
22
ee
23 asymmetrical) outflow and provoke an asymmetric pressure of aqueous humor against the
24
25 ocular structures (e.g. cornea and lens shapes). It would lead to asymmetric changes along the
26
rR

27 optical axis, and thus, to ocular astigmatism aberrations. As a result, we conjecture that the
28
29 intraocular pressure increase found during the condition with high mental task complexity (see
30
ev

31 Supplementary Material) could have an effect on the astigmatism aberration changes obtained in
32
33
the present study.
34
ie

35
36 Numerous indices have been investigated to objectively evaluate the level of mental workload
37
w

38 (Di Stasi, Antolí, and Cañas 2011; Paas et al. 2003). The pupil response is considered one of the
39
40 most valid and reliable ocular biomarkers to assess mental workload (Wang and Munoz 2015;
On

41
42 Klingner, Tversky, and Hanrahan 2011; de Gee, Knapen, and Donner 2014), but it depends on
43
44
the illumination levels, presenting practical caveats in ecological settings outside well-
45
ly

46
controlled laboratory conditions. Therefore, other studies have recently focused their attention
47
48
on finding accurate ocular indices sensitive to cognitive load, such as accommodative response
49
50
51
(Vera et al. 2016; Davies, Wolffsohn, and Gilmartin 2005), intraocular pressure (Brody et al.
52
53 1999; Vera et al. 2017), and saccadic movements (Di Stasi, Antolí, and Cañas 2011). Notably,
54
55 the present work proposes a promising ocular candidate, i.e. astigmatism aberration with scaled
56
57 pupils up to 5 mm, to assess mental workload that is not dependent on surrounding illumination.
58
59
60
18 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 19 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Although the lapses between mental task performance and aberration measures are minimal, the
4
5 lack of having monitored the ocular aberrations measures during the experimental session may
6
7 be a limitation to our results. As Young et al. (2015) stated the main challenge is the application
8
9 of objective indices of mental workload in applied contexts (e.g., car drivers or aircraft pilots
10
11 [Borghini et al. 2014; Fallahi et al. 2016]), however, innovative solutions and development
12
13 engineering are needed to allow ocular aberrations monitoring during task performance in
14
Fo
15 laboratory and applied conditions. The investigation of new neuropsychological techniques has
16
17 been gaining popularity in recent years, and therefore future research is guaranteed in this
18
rP
19 regard. In addition, our results should be checked with larger sample size from different age
20
21 groups, and deepen in other RMSs aberrations such as trefoil aberration to assess mental
22
ee
23 workload, due to the marginal significant effect revealed in this study.
24
25
26 In summary, we have shown that the execution of a mentally demanding task modulates
rR

27
28 astigmatism aberrations. These findings may open new possibilities concerning the use of
29
30 astigmatism aberrations as an additional indicator of mental workload level, and suggest that
ev

31
32 ocular indices have the potential to capture the variation of person´s levels of activation. We
33
34 encourage future studies to test this objective index in complex and ecological settings (e.g.
ie

35
36 driving, flight operations, surgical procedures).
37
w

38
39
40
On

41
42
43
44
45
ly

46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
19 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 20 of 37

1
2
3
4
5
6 References
7
8
Annett, John. 2002. “Subjective Rating Scales: Science or Art?” Ergonomics 45 (14): 966–87.
9
10
11 doi:10.1080/00140130210166951.
12
13
Applegate, Raymond, William J Donnelly, Jason D Marsack, Darren E Koenig, and Konrad
14
Fo
15
Pesudovs. 2007. “Three-Dimensional Relationship between High-Order Root-Mean-
16
17
Square Wavefront Error, Pupil Diameter, and Aging.” Journal of the Optical Society of
18
rP
19
20
America. 24 (3): 578–87. doi:10.1364/JOSAA.24.000578.
21
22 Asejczyk-Widlicka, M, and BK Pierscionek. 2007. “Fluctuations in Intraocular Pressure and the
ee
23
24
Potential Effect on Aberrations of the Eye.” Brithish Journal of Ophthalmology 91: 1054–
25
26
59. doi:10.1136/bjo.2006.109793.
rR

27
28
29 Atchison, David A., Michael J. Collins, Christine F. Wildsoet, James Christensen, and Michael
30
ev

31 D. Waterworth. 1995. “Measurement of Monochromatic Ocular Aberrations of Human


32
33 Eyes as a Function of Accommodation by the Howland Aberroscope Technique.” Vision
34
ie

35
Research 35 (3): 313–23. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)00139-D.
36
37
w

38 Borghini, Gianluca, Laura Astolfi, Giovanni Vecchiato, Donatella Mattia, and Fabio Babiloni.
39
40 2014. “Measuring Neurophysiological Signals in Aircraft Pilots and Car Drivers for the
On

41
42 Assessment of Mental Workload, Fatigue and Drowsiness.” Neuroscience and
43
44 Biobehavioral Reviews 44: 58-75. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.10.003.
45
ly

46
47 Borsting, Eric, Christopher H Chase, and William H Ridder. 2007. “Measuring Visual
48
49 Discomfort in College Students.” Optometry and Vision Science 84 (8): 745–51.
50
51 doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e31812f5f51.
52
53
54 Brody, S, C Erb, R Veit, and H Rau. 1999. “Intraocular Pressure Changes: The Influence of
55
56 Psychological Stress and the Valsalva Maneuver.” Biological Psychology 51: 43–57.
57
58 doi:10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00012-5.
59
60
20 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 21 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Brubaker, Richard F. 2003. “Targeting Outflow Facility in Glaucoma Management.” Survey of
4
5 Ophthalmology 48 (Suppl 1): 17–20. doi:10.1016/S0039-6257(03)00003-1.
6
7
8 Buehren, Tobias, Michael J Collins, Jim Loughridge, Leo G Carney, and D Robert Iskander.
9
10 2003. “Corneal Topography and Accommodation.” Cornea 22 (4): 311–16.
11
12
13 Bullimore, MA, and B Gilmartin. 1988. “The Accommodative Response, Refractive Error and
14
Mental Effort: 1. The Sympathetic Nervous System.” Documenta Ophthalmologica.
Fo
15
16
17 Advances in Ophthalmology 69 (4): 385–97. doi:10.1007/BF00162751.
18
rP
19
Burakgazi, Ahmet Z, Bernard Tinio, Alejandro Bababyan, Kevin Kevork Niksarli, and Penny
20
21
22 Asbell. 2006. “Higher Order Aberrations in Normal Eyes Measured with Three Different
ee
23
24 Aberrometers.” Journal of Refractive Surgery 22 (9): 898–903.
25
26
Carreon, Teresia A, Genea Edwards, Haiyan Wang, and Sanjoy K Bhattacharya. 2016.
rR

27
28
“Segmental Outflow of Aqueous Humor in Mouse and Human.” Experimental Eye
29
30
Research. Advance online publication. doi:10.1016/j.exer.2016.08.001.
ev

31
32
33 Cheng, Han, Justin K Barnett, Abhiram S Vilupuru, Jason D Marsack, Sanjeev Kasthurirangan,
34
ie

35
Raymond Applegate, and Austin Roorda. 2004. “A Population Study on Changes in Wave
36
37
w

Aberrations with Accommodation.” Journal of Vision 4 (4): 272–80. doi:10.1167/4.4.3.


38
39
40 Conlon, Elizabeth G, William J Lovegrove, Eugene Chekaluk, and Philippa E Pattison. 1999.
On

41
42 “Measuring Visual Discomfort.” Visual Cognition 6 (6): 637–63.
43
44 doi:10.1080/135062899394885.
45
ly

46
47 Dai, Guang-Ming. 2008. Wavefront Optics for Vision Correction. Washington: SPIE press.
48
49
50 Davies, Leon Nicholas, James Stuart Wolffsohn, and Bernard Gilmartin. 2005. “Cognition,
51
52 Ocular Accommodation, and Cardiovascular Function in Emmetropes and Late-Onset
53
54 Myopes.” Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 46 (5): 1791–96.
55
56 doi:10.1167/iovs.04-0986.
57
58
59
60
21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 22 of 37

1
2
3 de Gee, Jan Willem, Tomas Knapen, and Tobias H Donner. 2014. “Decision-Related Pupil
4
5 Dilation Reflects Upcoming Choice and Individual Bias.” Proceedings of the National
6
7 Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111 (5): E618–25.
8
9 doi:10.1073/pnas.1317557111.
10
11
12 Del Rio-Bermudez, Carlos, Carolina Diaz-Piedra, Andrés Catena, Gualberto Buela-Casal, and
13
14 Leandro L. Di Stasi. 2014. “Chronotype-Dependent Circadian Rhythmicity of Driving
Fo
15
16 Safety.” Chronobiology International 31 (4): 532–41.
17
18 doi:10.3109/07420528.2013.876427.
rP
19
20
21 Di Stasi, Leandro L., Adoración Antolí, and José Juan Cañas. 2011. “Main Sequence: An Index
22
for Detecting Mental Workload Variation in Complex Tasks.” Applied Ergonomics 42 (6).
ee
23
24
25 Elsevier Ltd: 807–13. doi:10.1016/j.apergo.2011.01.003.
26
rR

27
28 Di Stasi, Leandro L., Carolina Diaz-Piedra, Juan Suárez, Michael B. McCamy, Susana
29
30 Martinez-Conde, Joaquín Roca-Dorda, and Andrés Catena. 2015. “Task Complexity
ev

31
32 Modulates Pilot Electroencephalographic Activity during Real Flights.” Psychophysiology
33
34 52: 951–56. doi:10.1111/psyp.12419.
ie

35
36
37 Díaz, José Antonio., José Fernández-Dorado, Carles Pizarro, and Josep Arasa. 2009. “Zernike
w

38
39 Coefficients for Concentric, Circular Scaled Pupils: An Equivalent Expression.” Journal of
40
On

41 Modern Optics 56 (1): 131–37. doi:10.1080/09500340802531224.


42
43
44 Durantin, G., J. F. Gagnon, S. Tremblay, and F. Dehais. 2014. “Using near Infrared Spectroscopy
45
ly

46 and Heart Rate Variability to Detect Mental Overload.” Behavioural Brain Research 259:
47
48 16–23. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.10.042.
49
50
Fallahi Majid, Majid Motamedzade, Rashid Heidarimoghadam, and Ali Reza Soltanian. 2016.
51
52
“Effects of Mental Workload on Physiological and Subjective Reponses during Traffic
53
54
55 Density Monitoring: A Field Study.” Applied Ergonomics 52: 95-103. doi:
56
57 10.1016/j.apergo.2015.07.009
58
59
60
22 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 23 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Garner, Leon F, and Maurice K H Yap. 1997. “Changes in Ocular Dimensions and Refraction
4
5 with Accommodation.” Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 17 (1): 12–17.
6
7
8 Ghaderyan, Peyvand, and Ataollah Abbasi. 2016. “An Efficient Automatic Workload Estimation
9
10 Method Based on Electrodermal Activity Using Pattern Classifier Combinations.”
11
12 International Journal of Psychophysiology 110: 91–101.
13
14 doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.10.013.
Fo
15
16
17 Gomez, Pablo, and Manuel Perea. 2009. “A Model of the Go/no Go Task.” Journal of
18
rP
19 Experimental Psychology 136 (3): 389–413. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.3.389.A.
20
21
22 Hart, Sandra G, and Lowell E Staveland. 1988. “Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load
ee
23
24 Index): Results of Empirical and Theorical Research.” Advances in Psychology 52:139–83.
25
26
He, J. C., S. A. Burns, and S. Marcos. 2000. “Monochromatic Aberrations in the Accommodated
rR

27
28
Human Eye.” Vision Research 40 (1): 41–48. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00156-X.
29
30
ev

31 He, Ji C, Jane Gwiazda, Frank Thorn, and Richard Held. 2003. “Wave-Front Aberrations in the
32
33 Anterior Corneal Surface and the Whole Eye.” Journal of the Optical Society of America
34
ie

35
20 (7): 1155–63. doi:10.1364/JOSAA.20.001155.
36
37
w

38 Hess, Eckhard H, and James M Polt. 1964. “Pupil Size in Relation to Mental Activity during
39
40 Simple Problem-Solving.” Science 143 (3611): 1190–92.
On

41
42
43 Hoddes, E, V Zarcone, and W Dement. 1972. “Development and Use of Stanford Sleepiness
44
45
ly

Scale (SSS).” Psychophysiology 9 (1): 150.


46
47
48 Holzer, Mike P, Sussane Goebels, Gerd U Auffarth. 2006. “Precision of NIDEK OPD-scan
49
50 measurements.” Journal of Refractive Surgery 22 (11): s1021-23. doi: 10.3928/1081-
51
52 597X-20061102-02.
53
54
55 Jainta, Stephanie, Joerg Hoormann, and Wolfgang Jaschinski. 2008. “Ocular Accommodation
56
57 and Cognitive Demand: An Additional Indicator besides Pupil Size and Cardiovascular
58
59
60
23 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 24 of 37

1
2
3 Measures?” Journal of Negative Results in Biomedicine 7: 6. doi:10.1186/1477-5751-7-6.
4
5
6 Jaschinski, Wolfgang, Herbert Heuer, and Hannegret Kylian. 1999. “A Procedure to Determine
7
8 the Individually Comfortable Position of Visual Displays relative to the Eyes” Ergonomics
9
10 42 (4): 535-49. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/001401399185450.
11
12
13 Karavidas, Maria Katsamanis, Paul M Lehrer, Shou-En Lu, Evgeny Vaschillo, Bronya
14
Vaschillo, and Andrew Cheng. 2010. “The Effects of Workload on Respiratory Variables in
Fo
15
16
17 Simulated Flight: A Preliminary Study.” Biological Psychology 84 (1): 157–60.
18
rP
19 doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.12.009.
20
21
22 Kiel, J.W, M Hollingsworth, M Rao, M Chen, and H.A Reitsamer. 2011. “Ciliary Blood Flow
ee
23
24 and Aqueous Humor Production.” Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 30 (1): 1–17.
25
26 doi:10.1016/j.preteyeres.2010.08.001.Ciliary.
rR

27
28
Klingner, Jeff, Barbara Tversky, and Pat Hanrahan. 2011. “Effects of Visual and Verbal
29
30
Presentation on Cognitive Load in Vigilance, Memory, and Arithmetic Tasks.”
ev

31
32
33 Psychophysiology 48 (3): 323–32. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01069.x.
34
ie

35
Koretz, Jane F, Christopher A Cook, and Paul L Kaufman. 2002. “Aging of the Human Lens:
36
37
w

Changes in Lens Shape upon Accommodation and with Accommodative Loss.” Journal of
38
39
the Optical Society of America 19 (1): 144–51.
40
On

41
42 Liang, Junzhong, and David R. Williams. 1997. “Aberrations and Retinal Image Quality of the
43
44 Normal Human Eye.” Journal of the Optical Society of America 14 (11): 2873.
45
ly

46
doi:10.1364/JOSAA.14.002873.
47
48
49 Lombardo, Marco, and Giuseppe Lombardo. 2010. “Wave Aberration of Human Eyes and New
50
51 Descriptors of Image Optical Quality and Visual Performance.” Journal of Cataract and
52
53 Refractive Surgery 36 (2): 313–31. doi:10.1016/j.jcrs.2009.09.026.
54
55
56 Lovie-Kitchin, Jan E. 2015. “Is It Time to Confine Snellen Charts to the Annals of History?”
57
58 Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 35 (6): 631–36. doi:10.1111/opo.12252.
59
60
24 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 25 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Luczak, Anna, and Andrzej Sobolewski. 2005. “Longitudinal Changes in Critical Flicker Fusion
4
5 Frequency: An Indicator of Human Workload.” Ergonomics 48 (15): 1770–92.
6
7 doi:10.1080/00140130500241753.
8
9
10 Luque-Casado, Antonio, José C. Perales, David Cárdenas, and Daniel Sanabria. 2015. “Heart
11
12 Rate Variability and Cognitive Processing: The Autonomic Response to Task Demands.”
13
14 Biological Psychology 113: 83–90. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.11.013.
Fo
15
16
17 Mansikka, Heikki, Petteri Simola, Kai Virtanen, Don Harris, and Lauri Oksama. 2016. “Fighter
18
rP
19 Pilots’ Heart Rate, Heart Rate Variation and Performance during Instrument Approaches.”
20
21 Ergonomics 59 (10): 1344–52.
22
ee
23
24 Martin, Jesson, Balamurali Vasudevan, Nikole Himebaugh, Arthur Bradley, and Larry Thibos.
25
26 2011. “Unbiased Estimation of Refractive State of Aberrated Eyes.” Vision Research 51
rR

27
28 (17): 1932-–40. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.07.006.
29
30
Mierdel, Peter, Hans Eberhard Krinke, Katharina Pollack, and Eberhard Spoerl. 2004. “Diurnal
ev

31
32
33 Fluctuation of Higher Order Ocular Aberrations: Correlation with Intraocular Pressure and
34
ie

35 Corneal Thickness.” Journal of Refractive Surgery 20 (3): 236–42.


36
37
w

Mutti, Donald O, Nicholas L Enlow, and G Lynn Mitchell. 2001. “Accommodation and Induced
38
39
with-the-Rule Astigmatism in Emmetropes.” Optometry & Vision Science 78 (1): 6–7.
40
On

41
42 Ninomiya, Sayuri, Takashi Fujikado, Teruhito Kuroda, Naoyuki Maeda, Yasuo Tano, Tetsuro
43
44 Oshika, Yoko Hirohara, and Toshifumi Mihashi. 2002. “Changes of Ocular Aberration
45
ly

46 with Accommodation.” American Journal of Ophthalmology 134 (6): 924–26.


47
48
doi:10.1016/S0002-9394(02)01856-1.
49
50
51 Nygren, Thomas E. 1991. “Psychometric Properties of Subjective Workload Measurement
52
53 Techniques: Implications for Their Use in the Assessment of Perceived Mental Workload.”
54
55 Human Factors 33 (1): 17–33. doi:10.1177/001872089103300102.
56
57
58 Owen, Adrian M., Kathryn M. McMillan, Angela R. Laird, and Ed Bullmore. 2005. “N-Back
59
60
25 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 26 of 37

1
2
3 Working Memory Paradigm: A Meta-Analysis of Normative Functional Neuroimaging
4
5 Studies.” Human Brain Mapping 25: 46–59. doi:10.1002/hbm.20131.
6
7
8 Paas, Fred, Juhani Tuovinen, Huib Tabbers, and Pascal W. M. Van Gerven. 2003. “Cognitive
9
10 Load Measurement as a Means to Advance Cognitive Load Theory.” Educational
11
12 Psychologist 38 (1): 63–71. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3801.
13
14
Pepose, Jay S, and Raymond A Applegate. 2005. “Making Sense out of Wavefront Sensing.”
Fo
15
16
17 American Journal of Ophthalmology 139 (2): 335–43.
18
rP
19
Perales, José C, Andrés Catena, David R Shanks, and José A González. 2005. “Dissociation
20
21
22 between Judgments and Outcome-Expectancy Measures in Covariation Learning: A Signal
ee
23
24 Detection Theory Approach.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
25
26 and Cognition 31 (5): 1105.
rR

27
28
Philip, Krupa, Aldo Martinez, Arthur Ho, Fabian Conrad, Jit Ale, Paul Mitchell, and Padmaja
29
30
Sankaridurg. 2012. “Total Ocular, Anterior Corneal and Lenticular Higher Order
ev

31
32
33 Aberrations in Hyperopic, Myopic and Emmetropic Eyes.” Vision Research 52 (1): 31–37.
34
ie

35 doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.10.018.
36
37
w

Pierścionek, B K, A Popiołek-Masajada, and H Kasprzak. 2001. “Corneal Shape Change during


38
39
Accommodation.” Eye 15: 766–69. doi:10.1038/eye.2001.246.
40
On

41
42 Porter, Jason, Abdul Awwal, Julianna Lin, Hope Queener, and Karen Thorn. 2006. Adaptive
43
44 Optics for Vision Science Principles, Practices, Design, and Applications. Hoboken, NJ:
45
ly

46
John Wiley & Sons.
47
48
49 Qi, Mingming, Heming Gao, Lili Guan, Guangyuan Liu, and Juan Yang. 2016. “Subjective
50
51 Stress, Salivary Cortisol, and Electrophysiological Responses to Psychological Stress.”
52
53 Frontiers in Psychology 7: 229. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00229.
54
55
56 Qu, Jia, Fan Lu, Jiang Xiu Wu, Qin Mei Wang, Chen Chen Xu, Xiang Tian Zhou, and Ji C. He.
57
58 2007. “Wavefront Aberration and Its Association with Intraocular Pressure and Central
59
60
26 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 27 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Corneal Thickness in Myopic Eyes.” Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery 33 (8):
4
5 1447–54. doi:10.1016/j.jcrs.2007.04.012.
6
7
8 Read, Scott A., M J Collins, H Becker, J Cutting, D Ross, A Savill, and B Trevor. 2009.
9
10 “Changes in Intracular Pressure and Ocular Pulse Amplitude with Accommodation.”
11
12 Brithish Journal of Ophthalmology 94 (3): 332–35.
13
14
Rosales, Patricia, Mark Wendt, Susanna Marcos, and Adrian Glasser. 2008. “Changes in
Fo
15
16
17 Crystalline Lens Radii of Curvature and Lens Tilt and Decentration during Dynamic
18
rP
19 Accommodation in Rhesus Monkeys.” Journal of Vision 8 (18): 1–12. doi:10.1167/8.1.18.
20
21
22 Rosenfield, M, S Jahan, K Nunez, and K Chan. 2015. “Cognitive Demand, Digital Screens and
ee
23
24 Blink Rate.” Computers in Human Behavior 51: 403–6. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.073.
25
26
Rozema Jos J; Van Dyck Dirk EM; Tassignon Marie-José. 2006. “Clinical Comparison of 6
rR

27
28
Aberrometers Part 2 : Statistical Comparison in a Test Group.” Journal of Cataract &
29
30
Refractive Surgery 32: 33–44. doi:10.1016/j.jcrs.2004.11.052.
ev

31
32
33 Scheiman, Mitchell, and Bruce Wick. 2008. Clinical Management of Binocular Vision:
34
ie

35
Heterophoric, Accommodative, and Eye Movement Disorders. Philadelphia: Lippincott
36
37
w

Williams & Wilkins.


38
39
40 Siegenthaler, Eva, Francisco M. Costela, Michael B. Mccamy, Leandro L. Di Stasi, Jorge Otero-
On

41
42 Millan, Andreas Sonderegger, Rudolf Groner, Stephen Macknik, and Susana Martinez-
43
44 Conde. 2014. “Task Difficulty in Mental Arithmetic Affects Microsaccadic Rates and
45
ly

46
Magnitudes.” European Journal of Neuroscience 39 (2): 287–94. doi:10.1111/ejn.12395.
47
48
49 Thibos, Larry N, Raymond A Applegate, James T Schwiegerling, and Robert Webb. 2002.
50
51 “Standards for Reporting the Optical Aberrations of Eyes.” Journal of Refractive Surgery
52
53 18 (5): S652–60.
54
55
56 Thibos, Larry N, and Xin Hong. 1999. “Clinical Applications of the Shack-Hartmann
57
58 Aberrometer.” Optometry & Vision Science 76 (12): 817–25.
59
60
27 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 28 of 37

1
2
3 Vera, Jesús, Carolina Diaz-Piedra, Raimundo Jiménez, José M Morales, Andrés Catena, David
4
5 Cardenas, and Leandro L Di Stasi. 2016. “Driving Time Modulates Accommodative
6
7 Response and Intraocular Pressure.” Physiology & Behavior 164: 47–53.
8
9 doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.05.043.
10
11
12 Vera, Jesús, Raimundo Jiménez, José Antonio García, and David Cárdenas. 2017. “Intraocular
13
14 Pressure Is Sensitive to Cumulative and Instantaneous Mental Workload.” Applied
Fo
15
16 Ergonomics 60: 313–19. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2016.12.011.
17
18
rP
19 Wang, Chin-An, and Douglas P Munoz. 2015. “A Circuit for Pupil Orienting Responses:
20
21 Implications for Cognitive Modulation of Pupil Size.” Current Opinion in Neurobiology
22
33: 134–40. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2015.03.018.
ee
23
24
25
26 Wang, Yan, Kanxing Zhao, Ying Jin, Yafei Niu, and Tong Zuo. 2003. “Changes of Higher Order
rR

27
28 Aberration with Various Pupil Sizes in the Myopic Eye.” Journal of Refractive Surgery 19
29
30 (2): S270–74.
ev

31
32
33 Yasuda, Akihiro, Tatsuo Yamaguchi, and K. Ohkoshi. 2003. “Changes in Corneal Curvature in
34
ie

35 Accommodation.” Journal of Cataract & Refractive Surgery 29 (7): 1297–1301.


36
37
w

Young, Mark, Karel A. Brookhuis, Christopher D. Wickens, and Peter A. Hancock. 2015. “State
38
39
of Science: Mental Workload in Ergonomics.” Ergonomics, 58 (1), 1-17. doi:
40
On

41
42
10.1080/00140139.2014.956151
43
44
45
ly

46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
28 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 29 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
Fo
15
16
17
18
rP
19
20
21
22
ee
23
24
25
26
rR

27
28
29
30
ev

31
32
33
34
ie

35
36
37 Figure Legends
w

38
39
40 Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the experimental setting for the two conditions. On panel A
On

41
42 (left side) the condition with high mental workload (3-back) is represented (in red). On panel B
43
44 (right side) the oddball condition is shown (in blue). Mental tasks were divided in intervals of
45
ly

46 105 s long with a 15-s rest periods between trials, excepting a 75-s rest period after the third
47
48 trail. This sequence was maintained for the entire experimental session. For the 3-back task
49
50 (panel A), participants read “press the button when the number on the screen matches the
51
52 number presented three positions before”. In our illustration, participants were asked to press
53
54 the button since the number (1) matched with number presented 3 positions before. For the
55
56 oddball condition (panel B), participants read the instructions “press the button when the
57
58
59
60
29 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 30 of 37

1
2
3 number X (from 1 to 3, in our example the number randomly chosen was 3) appears on the
4
5 screen”.
6
7
8
9
10 Figure 2. Effects of mental-task complexity on astigmatism RMS for total, internal, and corneal
11
12 aberrations with natural pupils. Data from the 3-back condition represented with black-filled
13
14 diamonds, and from the oddball task with open squares. The x-axis shows the four measurement
Fo
15
16 moments, with the measurement before mental workload, the measurements after 11 min and 33
17
18 min of mental workload, and the recovery measurement (10 min); # indicates statistical
rP
19
20 differences between task-complexity levels (p-value < .05). Error bars represent the standard
21
22 error (SE), and all values were calculated across participants (n=12).
ee
23
24
25 Figure 3. Effects of mental-task complexity on astigmatism RMS for total, internal, and corneal
26
rR

27 aberrations with 5-, 4.5-, and 4-mm scaled pupils. The top, middle, and bottom rows represent
28
29 astigmatism RMS in 5, 4.5, and 4 mm scaled pupils, respectively. The left, middle, and right
30
columns represent total, internal, and corneal astigmatism RMS aberrations, respectively. Data
ev

31
32
33 from the 3-back condition are represented with black-filled diamonds, and from the oddball task
34
ie

35 with open squares. The x-axis shows the four measurement moments, with the measurement
36
37 before mental workload, the measurements after 11 min and 33 min of mental workload, and the
w

38
39 recovery measurement (10 min). # indicates statistical differences between task-complexity
40
On

41 levels (p-value < .05). Error bars represent the standard error (SE). All values are calculated
42
43 across participants (n=12).
44
45
ly

46 Figure S1. The effect of mental task complexity on pupil size (Panel A) and intraocular
47
48 pressure (Panel B). Mean value for each measurement moment for the 3-back condition
49
50 (black-filled diamonds) and for the oddball condition (open squares) (left y-axis). The x-axis
51
52 shows the four measurement moments, with the measurement before mental workload, the
53
54 measurements after 11 min and 33 min of mental workload, and the recovery measurement (10
55
56 min). # and $ indicate statistical significance for the mental task and the interaction mental task
57
58
59
60
30 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 31 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 x measurement moment, respectively (p-value < .05). Errors bars represent the Standard Error
4
5 Mean across participants (n=12). All values are calculated across participants (n=12).
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
Fo
15
16
17
18
rP
19
20
21
22
ee
23
24
25
26
rR

27
28
29
30
ev

31
32
33
34
ie

35
36
37
w

38
39
40
On

41
42
43
44
45
ly

46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
31 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 32 of 37

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
Fo
15
16
17
18
rP
19
20
21
22
ee
23
24
25
Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the experimental setting for the two conditions. On panel A (left side) the
26 condition with high mental workload (3-back) is represented (in red). On panel B (right side) the oddball
rR

27 condition is shown (in blue). Mental tasks were divided in intervals of 105 s long with a 15-s rest periods
28 between trials, excepting a 75-s rest period after the third trail. This sequence was maintained for the entire
29 experimental session. For the 3-back task (panel A), participants read “press the button when the number
30 on the screen matches the number presented three positions before”. In our illustration, participants were
asked to press the button since the number (1) matched with number presented 3 positions before. For the
ev

31
32 oddball condition (panel B), participants read the instructions “press the button when the number X (from 1
to 3, in our example the number randomly chosen was 3) appears on the screen”.
33
34 185x100mm (120 x 120 DPI)
ie

35
36
37
w

38
39
40
On

41
42
43
44
45
ly

46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 33 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
Fo
15
16 Figure 2. Effects of mental-task complexity on astigmatism RMS for total, internal, and corneal aberrations
17 with natural pupils. Data from the 3-back condition represented with black-filled diamonds, and from the
18 oddball task with open squares. The x-axis shows the four measurement moments, with the measurement
before mental workload, the measurements after 11 min and 33 min of mental workload, and the recovery
rP
19 measurement (10 min); # indicates statistical differences between task-complexity levels (p-value < .05).
20 Error bars represent the standard error (SE), and all values were calculated across participants (n=12).
21
22 204x49mm (120 x 120 DPI)
ee
23
24
25
26
rR

27
28
29
30
ev

31
32
33
34
ie

35
36
37
w

38
39
40
On

41
42
43
44
45
ly

46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 34 of 37

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
Fo
15
16
17
18
rP
19
20
21
22
ee
23
24
25
26
rR

27
28
29
30 Figure 3. Effects of mental-task complexity on astigmatism RMS for total, internal, and corneal aberrations
ev

31 with 5-, 4.5-, and 4-mm scaled pupils. The top, middle, and bottom rows represent astigmatism RMS in 5,
32 4.5, and 4 mm scaled pupils, respectively. The left, middle, and right columns represent total, internal, and
33 corneal astigmatism RMS aberrations, respectively. Data from the 3-back condition are represented with
black-filled diamonds, and from the oddball task with open squares. The x-axis shows the four measurement
34
moments, with the measurement before mental workload, the measurements after 11 min and 33 min of
ie

35 mental workload, and the recovery measurement (10 min). # indicates statistical differences between task-
36 complexity levels (p-value < .05). Error bars represent the standard error (SE). All values are calculated
37 across participants (n=12).
w

38
39 219x150mm (120 x 120 DPI)
40
On

41
42
43
44
45
ly

46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 35 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3 Supplementary material
4
5
6 The effect of mental task complexity on pupil size response
7
8
Pupil size (PS) was obtained at the same measurement moments that ocular aberrations, using
9
10
11 the OPD Scan III (Nidek Inc., Tokyo, Japan). The same protocol used to measure ocular
12
13 aberrations was carried out for PS assessment (see 2.4.2 Ocular aberrations section for more
14
details).
Fo
15
16
17
Mental task complexity manipulation produced significant differences for PS. Mental task
18
rP
19
showed a significant effect [F(1,11)=4.859, MSE=.364, p=.048, ƞp²=.524], whereas the
20
21
22 measurement moment and the interaction displayed no signification [F(3,33)=2.47, MSE=.058,
ee
23
24 p=.079, ƞp²=.561; and F(3,33)=2.262, MSE=.098, p=.1, ƞp²=.521, respectively]. Two separate
25
26 one-way ANOVAs for each mental task showed that the 3 back task promoted a significant
rR

27
28 increase for PS [F(3,33)=4.047, MSE=.065, p=.015, ƞp²=.795] whereas the oddball condition
29
30 did not induce any significant change [F(3,33)=1.106, MSE=.098, p=.358] (see Figure S1, panel
ev

31
32 A).
33
34
ie

35 The effect of mental task complexity on intraocular pressure


36
37
w

We measured intraocular pressure with a portable rebound tonometer (Icare Tonometer, Tiolat,
38
39
Oy, INC. Helsinki, Finland) in both eyes, and the first measured eye was randomly selected,
40
On

41
using the same order for all subsequent IOP measures. Intraocular pressure was taken just before
42
43
44 each ocular aberrations assessment. This apparatus has been used in related investigations (see
45
ly

46 Vera, Jiménez, García, & Cárdenas, 2017 for a detailed description). Summing up, six rapid
47
48 consecutive measurements are taken against the central cornea while the participants are looking
49
50 at a distance target, and subsequently, the device displays the average intraocular pressure
51
52 reading.
53
54
55 In agreement with the study of Vera et al. 2017, intraocular pressure showed significance for the
56
57 measurement moment and the mental task x measurement moment [F(3,33) = 4.97, p < .001,
58
59
60
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Ergonomics Page 36 of 37

1
2
3 ƞp²= .31; and F(3,33) = 3.06, p = .042, ƞp² = .22, respectively], whereas the mental task did not
4
5 reach significance [F(1,11) = 1.30, p = .279]. Two separate one-way ANOVAs for each mental
6
7 task demonstrated significant differences for the 3 back task [F(3,33) = 5.92, p = .002, ƞp²=
8
9 .35], showing an intraocular pressure rise after performing the mental task, whereas the oddball
10
11 task did not show any change [F (3,33) < 1] (see Figure S1, panel B).
12
13
14
Fo
15
16
17
18
rP
19
20
21
22
ee
23
24
25
26
rR

27
28
29
30
ev

31
32
33
34
ie

35
36
37
w

38
39
40
On

41
42
43
44
45
ly

46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk
Page 37 of 37 Ergonomics

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
Fo
15
16
17
18
rP
19
20 Figure S1. The effect of mental task complexity on pupil size (Panel A) and intraocular pressure (Panel
21 B). Mean value for each measurement moment for the 3-back condition (black-filled diamonds) and for the
22 oddball condition (open squares) (left y-axis). The x-axis shows the four measurement moments, with the
ee
23 measurement before mental workload, the measurements after 11 min and 33 min of mental workload, and
24 the recovery measurement (10 min). # and $ indicate statistical significance for the mental task and the
25 interaction mental task x measurement moment, respectively (p-value < .05). Errors bars represent the
26 Standard Error Mean across participants (n=12). All values are calculated across participants (n=12).
rR

27
129x49mm (120 x 120 DPI)
28
29
30
ev

31
32
33
34
ie

35
36
37
w

38
39
40
On

41
42
43
44
45
ly

46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/terg E-mail: ergonomics@tandf.co.uk

View publication stats

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen