Sie sind auf Seite 1von 8

348 Mother Tongue Education: Standard Language

should read for pleasure and a full range of other Rather, pupils should be invited to participate in
functions. They should learn to write confidently authentic speech acts and literacy events in which
and develop their own voices; and they should ex- they encounter socially constructed conventions.
pand on their ability to speak and understand spoken Teachers can play a powerful role in demonstrating
language(s). In school, learners should feel free to the conventions in their own authentic language use
take risks in language and experiment with new and in mediating the students as they experience
forms, words and styles. None of this translates into the richness of language in its multiple forms and
the rigidity of traditional school standards. functions.

Supporting Language Development See also: Educational Linguistics; Language Policies: Po-
licies on Language in Europe; Language Policy in Multi-
Whether they teach in the mother tongue or in a lingual Educational Contexts.
national language, school language curricula should
support expansion of language, building on the moth-
er tongue of the learners. In language development at
all levels, there is constant disequilibrium between
Bibliography
the inventions of the learner and the conventions of Ferreiro E, Pontecorvo C, Ribeiro N & y Garcia-Hidalgo I
the language. Learners should be encouraged to take (1996). Caperucita Roja aprende a escribir: estudios psi-
risks as they learn a new language or as they use colingüı́sticos comparativos en tres lenguas. Barcelona
new forms of their first language or a new genre. In (Coleccion LEA): Gedisa.
this push and pull they move toward the conven- Read C (1975). Children’s categorization of speech sounds
tions of the language form and use it successfully. in English. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of
English.
Language learners should be encouraged to study lan-
Read C (1986). Children’s creative spelling. London, Bos-
guage variation in their own communities. Students ton: Routledge & Kegan.
take on the role of linguists as they inquire into UNESCO (1953). The use of vernacular languages in
language processes and their uses. education. Paris: UNESCO, 1953.
Schools have tended to suppress linguistic inven- UNESCO (2004). International mother language day 2004
tion pushing learners toward an oversimplified or press release. No. 2004-12.
misrepresented version of the conventional language. http://www.unesco.org

Motivation and Attitudes in Second Language Learning


R C Gardner, University of Western Ontario, London, Motivation Defined
Ontario, Canada
The term motivation is used in the area of language
ß 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. learning with many definitions, processes, and
measures proposed, and this variety is shared with
The concepts of motivation and attitudes have been the general area of psychology. In the early history
associated with individual differences in second lan- of psychology, motivation was considered variously
guage learning since at least the 1950s, and over the in terms of reinforcement, instincts, expectancy, va-
years numerous perspectives and issues have devel- lence, needs, and drive reduction. Recently, there has
oped. The corresponding research has been intensive been more of a focus on process-oriented concep-
and varied, and today there exist many different con- tualizations, including but not limited to curiosity,
structs and theoretical models seeking to explain in- self-determination, causal attributions, and goal
dividual differences in second language learning and setting.
use. The intent of this article is to consider the area in Some of the conceptualizations referred to above
general, to discuss some of the more dominant per- are represented in one form or another in the area of
spectives and theoretical models, and to review some motivation in second language learning, and there
of the issues that are of interest to researchers and have been calls for more attention to be given to
educators in this field. others (see, for example, Crookes and Schmidt,
Motivation and Attitudes in Second Language Learning 349

1991; Oxford and Shearin, 1994). Dörnyei and Ottó the bulk of studies in the area of second language
provided a formal definition of motivation as ‘‘the acquisition tend to recognize this interrelationship.
dynamically changing cumulative arousal in a person
that initiates, directs, coordinates, amplifies, termi- Theoretical Conceptualizations of
nates, and evaluates the cognitive and motor process- Motivation in Second Language Learning
es whereby initial wishes and desires are selected,
prioritised, operationalised, and (successfully or un- The definition of motivation proposed by Dörnyei
successfully) acted out’’ (1998: 65). This is a very and Ottó (1998) is important because it highlights the
comprehensive definition but it might not be accepted many features of motivation in terms of its dynamic
by all researchers. In fact, in his excellent book on nature, its continuity over time, and its implications
motivation and second language acquisition, Dörnyei for the task at hand. Similarly, Allport’s (1954) defi-
pointed out that most agreement would be obtained nition of attitude provided a clear insight into its
from researchers that motivation concerns the direc- dynamic nature, notwithstanding its overall continu-
tion and magnitude of behavior, and that it is ‘‘re- ity as well. It is clear that both constructs are com-
sponsible for why people decide to do something, plex, interrelated, and implicated in most behavioral
how long they are willing to sustain the activity, activities, and this is reflected in the research on indi-
and how hard they are going to pursue it’’ (2001: 8; vidual differences in second language acquisition.
italics in the original). This latter characterization is A close inspection of the literature will reveal that
closer to that proposed by Gardner, who defined although many researchers are concerned with the
motivation in second language acquisition as ‘‘the role played by attitudes and motivation, there are a
combination of effort plus desire to achieve the goal number of different trails that have been followed.
of learning the language plus favourable attitudes That is, there are conceivably a very large number of
toward learning the language’’ (1985: 10). possible attitudes (in terms of attitude objects) and
motives (in terms of reasons for doing an activity)
that might be investigated. Moreover, each of these
Attitudes Defined can be considered in terms of at least three different
The term attitude is also used quite extensively in the and distinct levels of analysis and generalization. In
language-learning literature, and it too has a very the area of motivation to learn another language,
large number of associated linkages. An early defini- these can be classified in terms of three perspectives,
tion proposed by Allport in 1935 characterized societal, activity-centered, and individual.
attitude as ‘‘a mental and neural state of readiness,
Perspective I
organized through experience, exerting a directive or
dynamic influence upon the individual’s response to The societal perspective of motivation focuses on
all objects and situations with which it is related’’ community-level reasons for learning a second lan-
(Allport, 1954: 45). Attitudes are considered to have guage. Because of different social and political exi-
cognitive, affective, and conative components and gencies or historical precedents, different cultures or
can be further characterized in terms of their direc- national groups have different pressures on them to
tion, magnitude, intensity, ambivalence, cognitive learn other languages or to attempt to revive or main-
complexity, embeddedness, flexibility, salience, and tain their own language. In fact, the societal level was
consciousness (cf., Scott, 1968). In terms of standard perhaps one of the first approaches to the concept of
attitude measurement techniques, however, the as- motivation in second language acquisition. In the lead
sessment of attitudes tends to make use of question- article of the inaugural issue of Language Learning,
naires that focus on evaluative reactions to the Marckwardt identified five motives for learning an-
attitude object. Thus, Gardner proposed that ‘‘an other language, and at least four of them focus more
individual’s attitude is an evaluative reaction to on societal reasons than on individual ones. The pri-
some referent or attitude object, inferred on the mary one, and clearly the more individualistic, is for
basis of the individual’s beliefs or opinions about the self-cultural development, viz., ‘‘a knowledge of mod-
referent’’ (1985: 9); reference to beliefs and opinions ern languages is one of the accomplishments of a
is made because of the way in which they are typically cultivated man’’ (1948: 3). The others refer to such
measured, though obviously attitudes also can be societal reasons as maintaining the ethnic identity of a
inferred on the basis of other aspects of behavior as minority group, assimilation of an ethnic minority,
well. Consideration of the two concepts, attitude and promotion of trade and commerce, and scientific and
motivation, makes it very clear that they are closely technical usefulness. The actual descriptions of the
interrelated. Motivation has attitudinal components, motives clearly place emphasis on why individuals
and attitudes have motivational implications, and should learn other languages for the good of the
350 Motivation and Attitudes in Second Language Learning

community. In all five cases, these motives can be seen with self-identity and ethnic relations. The other is
as pressures placed by society on individuals to learn described as ‘‘education-friendly’’ and focuses on the
another language. individual and classroom interactions. Both treat the
Societal motives are sometimes mistaken for in- motivation to learn the language in terms of relatively
dividual ones, either by the researcher or by the indi- stable affective characteristics. That is, motivation to
vidual student. Thus, when students state that their learn another language is seen as a general disposition
reason for studying a language is to obtain a degree, of the individual that nonetheless may be influenced
or to meet an academic requirement, etc., they are not by a number of external factors.
really reporting on a motive of their own, but rather One of the earliest studies based on the social mo-
are voicing pressures exerted by society. In some of tivation approach was conducted by Gardner and
the literature, these types are described as extrinsic Lambert (1959), based on Lambert’s seminal research
motives, but they are really pressures imposed by and theorizing on bilingualism (see, for example,
society. They could be considered motives that have Lambert, 1955, 1967). In their study, Gardner
been internalized by the individual if they are linked and Lambert demonstrated that two factors, lan-
to other aspects of motivation; otherwise they are best guage aptitude and motivation, were associated with
characterized as reflecting external pressures. The achievement in French among a sample of English-
important point is that motives are named on the speaking high school students. The motivation factor
basis of the reason for engaging in the particular was characterized by a high level of motivational
activity, but motivation involves a series of related intensity to learn French, favorable attitudes toward
consequences such as effort, persistence, interest, French Canadians, and a preference for integrative
and enjoyment. Without these, a reason is just a as opposed to instrumental reasons for learning
reason, not a motive. the language – the so-called integrative–instrumental
dichotomy, which subsequently became a topic of
Perspective II
much discussion (see below).
The second perspective treats motivation as a charac- There have been a number of models that have
teristic of the individual that is tied directly to the task developed from this tradition. One of the earliest
at hand. Constructs associated with this type of model was the socioeducational model of second language
are state motivation, which refers specifically to the acquisition (Gardner and Smythe, 1975; Gardner,
motivation to do well at a specific time (cf., Gardner 1985). The basic assumption underlying this model
et al., 2004; Heckhausen, 1991), and ‘‘on-line moti- is that language is a significant part of one’s self-
vation’’ (Boekaerts, 2002), which focuses on the task identity and that learning another language is much
itself and includes emotions, appraisals, and inten- more complex than learning another school subject
tions before the task begins and emotions, attribu- because it involves ‘‘the acquisition of skills or behav-
tions, and effort reported at the end of the task. iour patterns which are characteristic of another cul-
Elements of these constructs are represented in many tural community’’ (Gardner, 1985: 146). In the
models, but one model that focuses directly on the socioeducational model, second language learning is
task of language learning is that proposed by Dörnyei seen to take place in the context of a series of ante-
and Ottó (1998). It consists of three phases: preac- cedent experiential and biological factors (such as the
tional, actional, and postactional. It is a model that sociocultural milieu, and prior educational experi-
treats motivation in very conscious terms. Individuals ences, gender, etc.) which act on a host of individual
have objectives, set goals, perform acts, experience variables. These individual difference variables in
their outcomes, appraise their success, and behave turn act in conjunction with formal and informal
accordingly. It is an informative analysis of what language acquisition contexts to yield linguistic and
takes place at specific stages of learning and as such nonlinguistic outcomes.
provides very useful perspectives for individuals Although a number of individual difference vari-
concerned with explaining specific behaviors in lan- ables, such as language aptitude, intelligence, lan-
guage learning. It does not, however, deal with aspects guage anxiety, instrumental orientation, and
of motivation of which the individual is unaware language learning strategies, are considered in the
(unconscious features) or long-range motivation. socioeducational model, the major attention is direct-
ed toward the concept of integrative motivation. The
Perspective III
model proposes that two attitudinal factors, integra-
The third perspective has been characterized by tiveness and attitudes toward the learning situation,
Dörnyei (2001) as comprising two different approa- serve as supports for and influences on the motiva-
ches. One focuses on social motivation that treats tion to learn another language and that motivation
second language acquisition as a task closely linked and language aptitude are two primary variables
Motivation and Attitudes in Second Language Learning 351

influencing achievement. It also allows that other af- intergroup and individual motivational processes and
fective individual difference variables, such as lan- links them to six classes of variables. These are imme-
guage anxiety and an instrumental orientation, may diate social contact, fundamental needs, orientations,
influence second language learning. The constellation intention and engagement, L2 use, and linguistic and
of integrativeness, attitudes toward the learning situ- nonlinguistic outcomes. In this model, proficiency in
ation, and motivation has been described as reflecting the other language is seen as a linguistic outcome
an integrative motive to learn another language in that resulting from the interplay of contact with the lan-
the integratively motivated individual is perceived as guage with four fundamental needs, social identity,
one who has an open interest in the other language relatedness, autonomy, and competence leading to
community and other ethnic communities in general, three orientations, integrative, intrinsic, and extrin-
perceives the language learning context positively, and sic. These in turn can influence the willingness to
expresses a high degree of motivation to learn the communicate and the effort and persistence demon-
language. Associated with this model is the Attitude strated in learning the language, which in turn leads
Motivation Test Battery (AMTB), which is designed to language use and ultimately proficiency and some
to measure various features of the integrative motive possible changes in identity. As can be seen, this
as well as instrumental orientation and language anx- model is comparable to the others discussed above,
iety, and has been used in many investigations in this although, as with the willingness to communicate
area (see, for example, Gardner, 1985, 2000). model, it is perhaps more detailed in that it explicitly
Clément (1980) proposed an alternative model that identifies many variables that might be implicated in
considered many of the same variables, but focused language learning. The overlap is recognized by
more attention on the social context and the relative Noels, who claimed that ‘‘The primary contribution
vitality of the first- and second-language commu- of this approach lies in that it figuratively takes a
nities. Integrativeness and fear of assimilation were magnifying glass to look more closely at the nature
seen to influence the motivation to learn a second of orientations, how they are affected by significant
language in unilingual communities but to be others in different contexts, and how they may be
mediated by the frequency and quality of contact differentially related to L2 variables’’ (2001: 61).
and the resulting self-confidence in the language in These models vary in terms of parsimony, with the
multicultural contexts. Individual differences in mo- socioeducational and the social context models being
tivation resulted in competence in the language, the most parsimonious in that they involve fewer
which in turn could influence the individual’s level variables and processes than the others. They are all
of acculturation. In subsequent research, this model similar, however, in that each of them recognizes that
has been modified and extended to deal with accul- second language acquisition involves the acquisition
turation of ethnic minority group members (cf., of features that are characteristic of a different cul-
Clément, 1986; Noels et al., 1996). ture. This in turn could have implications for feelings
MacIntyre et al. (1998) proposed a Willingness to of self-identity for some individuals.
Communicate Model that is based on similar con- The education-friendly models place more of an
structs as these models, but viewed the ultimate goal emphasis on characteristics of the individual, and
as willingness to communicate rather than achieve- the implications this can have for learning, and focus
ment itself. This is an important development in this on variables that could be appropriate to any learning
area in that it stresses that the ultimate goal is not to situation. Dörnyei (2001) described a number of such
learn the language but to develop the competence and models, some of which are drawn from other fields of
the confidence to use it in communication. Like the psychology, including educational psychology. One
other models discussed in this section, it includes example of such a model is Dörnyei’s (1994) ‘‘extend-
variables associated with integrative motivation. ed motivational framework.’’ This model considers
Also, like Noel’s (2001) self-determination model motivation in terms of three levels, the language
(see below), it includes some personality characteris- level, the learner level, and the learning situation
tics, etc., but it places ultimate use of the language at level, and proposes that each of them can supplement
the pinnacle of a pyramid and actual learning further or cancel the motivation generated at the other levels.
down, whereas the other models treat ultimate The language level focuses on motivation associated
communication more or less as a given of learning with the language itself (i.e., the community, value of
the language. As many language teachers will note, knowing the language, etc.); the learner level refers to
however, this is not a foregone conclusion. characteristics of the student similar to the social
Noels (2001) has proposed a model of second motivation models; and the learning situation level
language acquisition based on Deci and Ryan’s focuses on motivation deriving from the curriculum,
(1985) self-determination model. It considers both the teacher, and the classroom.
352 Motivation and Attitudes in Second Language Learning

Another example of an education-friendly model is is implied in many of the models that individual
the social constructivist model proposed by Williams differences in motivation are responsible in part
and Burden (1997). This model distinguishes between for differences in achievement, but it has been ques-
internal factors, such as the individual’s intrinsic in- tioned whether this is the most meaningful cause–
terest, perceived value, feelings of competence, and effect sequence. One could equally argue that
attitudes about the language, and external factors, achievement in the language promotes motivation
such as significant others, the learning environment, and favorable attitudes (cf., Ellis, 1994). When deal-
and the social context. This model includes a large ing with the relationship between individual differ-
number of variables that could be implicated in the ences, however, it is not possible to demonstrate
language learning situation that would be of particu- simple causation of the nature that A causes B. The
lar interest to teachers and educators who wish to only way to demonstrate such causation is to ran-
consider various aspects that could influence an domly assign individuals to the various values of
individual’s level of achievement in the language. A and to assess whether this results in variation in
Each of these models is useful in helping us under- B (cf., Gardner, 2000). The attempt to use procedures
stand the learning process, in making us aware of such as structural equation modeling to demonstrate
variables that can influence learning, and in suggest- the direction of causation is not a solution. Such
ing perspectives that we might embrace. It is impor- ‘causal models’ are not tests of causation, but simply
tant to note, however, that the models are not tests of one way of explaining relationships in terms
contradictory. None makes a prediction that is not of regression coefficients. This is a big difference!
consistent with the others. The models do differ With characteristics that individuals bring with
somewhat in the specificity of the processes involved, them to any situation, random assignment is not pos-
varying from statements about how variables in the sible; hence, unequivocal conclusions about causa-
model influence or effect others to largely a descrip- tion are not possible. Though this may seem like a
tion of variables or classes of variables that can be major problem, in point of fact, the direction of cau-
involved. They also differ in terms of the amount of sality is immaterial. To the extent that there is a
research that has been devoted to testing specific pre- correlation between motivation and achievement in
dictions deriving from them. Clearly, there are a num- the population, this means that there are conditional
ber of different ways of conceptualizing motivation in (Bayesian) probabilities linking the two variables. For
second language acquisition and in the types of atti- example, a positive correlation demonstrates that
tude variables that might be implicated. An interest- there is an increased probability that an individual
ing question, however, is whether or not they capture with high levels of motivation will be more successful
different sources of variation in second language in learning the language than an individual with low
achievement. That is, each of them postulates the levels and there is also an increased probability that
importance of somewhat different constructs and the individual who is successful in learning the lan-
processes, but to the extent that they all are concerned guage will be more motivated to learn the language
with explaining the role of motivation in second lan- than an individual who is less successful.
guage learning, one might well ask whether any one This can be demonstrated by an example from a
model would contribute to prediction over another or study we recently conducted in Spain (Gardner and
whether the ultimate degree of prediction of achieve- Bernaus, 2004). In that study, a correlation of 0.40
ment would be essentially the same regardless of the was obtained between motivation assessed in the
model followed. If this is the case, the value in autumn and the following spring grades in English
the model rests not so much in accounting for varia- among a sample of 166 Level 2 secondary school
tion in achievement but in the nature of the variables students. Further analysis revealed that the probabil-
to be emphasized. ity that individuals scoring above the median in moti-
vation would be in the top 30% on achievement was
0.40, whereas the probability was 0.19 for those
Issues
scoring below the median on motivation (i.e., twice
There are many questions that have been raised by as great). Similarly, those above the median on
researchers and educators about the role of motiva- achievement were more likely to be in the top 30%
tion and attitudes in second language acquisition, and of motivation with a probability of 0.37, whereas
at least five of these have become issues that are the probability of those below the median on
relatively central to this area of research. achievement being in the top 30% on motivation
One very basic question deals with the direction of was 0.23 (again close to twice (1.61) as great).
causation between attitudes and motivation on the These are not certainties, of course, and could vary
one hand and language achievement on the other. It from sample to sample, but they do indicate clearly
Motivation and Attitudes in Second Language Learning 353

that a correlation as low as 0.40 has clear implica- Some of the questions concerning the lack of im-
tions about the conditional probabilities that operate portance of the role of attitudes and motivation in
when it is established that there is a correlation second language acquisition derive from individuals
between two variables. who focus only on the orientations (cf., Crookes and
A second, but related, issue involves the degree of Schmidt, 1991) rather than considering the other at-
relationship between the two classes of variables. It is titudinal and motivational variables. All of the social
true that a correlation of 0.40 between motivation motivation models include other attitude and motiva-
and achievement indicates that only 16% of the tion measures, however. In the socioeducational
variation of one of the variables is explained by model, for example, Gardner (1985, 2000) stressed
the other (cf., Ellis, 1994; Dörnyei, 2001), and as a that motivation, not orientations, is the major
consequence, some researchers consider low correla- affective variable implicated in second language
tions to be trivial. The first part of this statement is learning, but that it is supported and maintained by
true; the last part is arguable. Cohen described a integrativeness (as distinct from simply an integrative
correlation of 0.30 as reflecting a medium effect and orientation) and evaluation of the learning situation.
that such a relationship ‘‘would be perceptible to the He has proposed (Gardner, 2000) that many factors
naked eye of a reasonably sensitive observer’’(1988: might contribute to motivation, even though integra-
80). He noted too that ‘‘many of the correlation tiveness will always be important because of the link
coefficients encountered in behavioral science are of between language, ethnicity, and self-identity.
this magnitude’’ (1988: 80). Moreover, as indicated A fourth issue has to do with the distinction between
above, such a correlation still implies a level of second and foreign language learning. Inspection of the
conditional probabilities that are not trivial. literature reveals that there are at least two reasons for
A third issue is long-standing and deals with the making such a distinction, one based on the availability
distinction between integrative and instrumental of the language in the community (cf., Oxford and
orientations. In their initial study, Gardner and Shearin, 1994) and the other on the sociopolitical im-
Lambert (1959) classified students as integrative or portance of the other language (cf., Dörnyei, 2001). It
instrumental in their orientation to studying French has been proposed that motivational and attitudinal
as a second language based on the reason they ranked variables would play slightly different roles in the case
as most applicable to themselves. The results demon- of a second as opposed to a foreign language learning
strated that individuals who were integratively context, but to date there has been little evidence to
orientated were more motivated, had more favorable support such a hypothesis. For example, a meta-analy-
attitudes toward French Canadians, and had higher sis of research conducted by Gardner and associates
levels of achievement in French than those who were failed to detect an effect of bilingual vs. monolingual
instrumentally oriented. In subsequent research (e.g., settings on the relationship between motivation and
Gardner and Lambert, 1972; Gardner and Smythe, language achievement (Masgoret and Gardner, 2003).
1975), this dichotomous measure was dropped in Furthermore, research conducted in Hungary, in which
favor of separate measures of the applicability of English is clearly a foreign (as opposed to a second)
each type of orientation to language learning. Al- language, found significant relationships between inte-
though the results demonstrated that the two orienta- grative motivational types of variables and achieve-
tions were positively related to each other, many ment in English (Dörnyei and Clément, 2001).
other researchers and educators tended to emphasize Perhaps the most dominant issue currently is the
the relative superiority of one over the other (cf., distinction between motivation and motivating and
Crookes and Schmidt, 1991; Oxford and Shearin, the implications that the education-friendly vs. the
1994). A meta-analysis conducted by Masgoret and social motivational models have for this distinction.
Gardner (2003) demonstrated that achievement Education-friendly models highlight features that
tended to be more highly related to individual differ- educators might use to motivate students. Close in-
ences in an integrative orientation than in an instru- spection of these models will reveal, however, that
mental one but the differences were not large. In fact, many of the features discussed are common to virtu-
the mean correlations were relatively low over all, ally any subject from mathematics to history (i.e.,
though in the various samples, correlations with interest, intrinsic motivation, teaching techniques,
grades were as low as 0.21 and as high as 0.56. course content, causal attributions, feedback, arousal
For three different types of achievement measure of curiosity, feelings of competence, age, and gender).
(grades, objective measures, and self-ratings), the These are important attributes to be sure, but it
mean correlations ranged from 0.15 to 0.20 for inte- is important to determine whether these models con-
grative orientation and 0.08 to 0.16 for instrumental tribute anything to the prediction of achievement
orientation. or related behaviors such as the willingness to
354 Motivation and Attitudes in Second Language Learning

communicate that is not predicted by the social moti- may well be ineffective for some students, and it is
vation models. Clearly, there is nothing in the edu- important to conduct research to evaluate their effec-
cation-friendly models that is inconsistent with tiveness in motivating students to actually learn and
predictions from the social motivation models, or use the language. Recently, we investigated attitudinal
vice versa, and the social motivation models could and motivational changes over a year-long course, and
incorporate the other features without changing the results were instructive. Two studies were con-
their basic assumptions or postulates. ducted, one with Canadian university students of
The problem of motivating students is one that can French as a second language (Gardner et al., 2004)
be difficult in the context of second language learning and the other with EFL secondary school students in
if one accepts that such learning involves a construct Spain (Gardner and Bernaus, 2004). In both studies,
such as integrativeness. In the socioeducational model significant interactions were obtained between ulti-
(Gardner, 1985), integrativeness involves more than mate achievement in the course and time of testing for
just an integrative orientation. It refers to the individ- attitudes toward the learning situation but not for
ual’s cultural openness and interest in other commu- other attitudinal and motivational characteristics
nities and languages and favorable attitudes toward measured by the AMTB. In both studies, students
the target language group as well as an integrative who were ultimately successful in the course evi-
orientation. Integrativenss is a higher-order construct denced relative increases in attitudes toward the
with many levels. At one level, it can mean simply a learning situation, whereas students who were least
willingness or capacity to take on characteristics of successful showed declines and the middle students
another cultural community. At another, it can reflect showed virtually no change. That is, the degree of
a full desire to identify and integrate with the other success in the course had an effect only on reactions
community. Most language learners do not study to the course and/or the teacher. Admittedly, there
languages in order to become members of another was no attempt to investigate specific motivation-
cultural community (though some might), but at a al techniques, but the teachers in the study, like
minimum they must be able to make features (i.e., most teachers, were quite likely doing their best to
language) of another community part of their own teach the students and to motivate them. In both
behavioral repertoire, and there are individual studies, there was also a tendency for the measures
differences in the ability (or willingness) to do this. of integrativeness and motivation to become less pos-
Two examples from the literature illustrate possible itive over the course of the year, but these changes
extremes of an integrativeness dimension. One, de- generally were not moderated by ultimate success in
scribed by Nida (1956), was the case of an individual, the course. The implication is therefore that what
the son of immigrants, who identified strongly with goes on in the course can influence learning and reac-
the American way of life and hid his immigrant back- tions to the learning situation but not integrativeness
ground while growing up. Despite a superior language or motivation. It is possible, therefore, that educators’
program and a strong motivation to learn a foreign attempts to motivate students could result in im-
language to aid him in his life as a missionary, he was proved reactions to the course but not in more general
unsuccessful in learning the language. Nida proffered attitudes, motivation, or the willingness to use the
that his earlier rejection of his immigrant background language outside the classroom environment. Clearly,
made him incapable of learning a foreign language. At much more research is required to investigate the
the other extreme, Lambert (1955) described an issue of motivating students in language classes to
American graduate student majoring in French who determine whether or not it has the desired effect in
measured dominant in French on measures of bilin- terms of achievement and willingness to use the lan-
gualism. This student had an extreme attachment with guage. This is an important challenge for the future.
France, read French newspapers, etc., and planned to
move there when he graduated. Although these are
See also: Bilingualism and Second Language Learning;
extreme examples, they serve to define the potential
Identity: Second Language; Intercultural Pragmatics and
extremes of integrativeness from high negative to high Communication; Interlanguage; Language Attitudes; Sec-
positive and also indicate that the effects of integra- ond and Foreign Language Learning and Teaching.
tiveness on motivation might well be unconscious.
That is, individuals might want to learn a language
but because of emotional issues associated with eth- Bibliography
nicity simply may not be capable of doing so. Allport G W (1954). ‘The historical background of modern
The construct of integrativeness has implications for social psychology.’ In Lindsey G (ed.) Handbook of
the issue of motivating students. Educators may use social psychology, vol. 1, Theory and method. Cam-
procedures to motivate students, but these techniques bridge, MA: Addison-Wesley. 3–56.
Motivation and Attitudes in Second Language Learning 355

Boekaerts M (2002). ‘The on-line motivation question- Gardner R C & Lambert W E (1972). Attitudes and moti-
naire: A self-report instrument to assess students’ context vation in second language learning. Rowley, MA:
sensitivity.’ In Pintrich P R & Maehr M L (eds.) New Newbury House.
directions in measures and methods, vol. 12, Advances in Gardner R C & Smythe P C (1975). ‘Motivation and
motivation and achievement. Oxford: Elsevier Sciences second-language acquisition.’ The Canadian Modern
Ltd. 77–120. Language Review, 31, 218–230.
Clément R (1980). ‘Ethnicity, contact, and communicative Gardner R C, Masgoret A-M, Tennant J & Mihic L (2004).
competence in a second language.’ In Giles H, Robinson ‘Integrative motivation: Changes during a year-long in-
P & Smith P M (eds.) Language: Social psychological termediate-level language course.’ Language Learning
perspectives. Pergamon: Oxford. 147–154. 54, 1–34.
Clément R (1986). ‘Second language proficiency and accul- Heckhausen H (1991). Motivation and action. New York:
turation: An investigation of the effects of language status Springer.
and individual characteristics.’ Journal of Language and Lambert W E (1955). ‘Measurement of the linguistic
Social Psychology 5, 271–290. dominance of bilinguals.’ Journal of Abnormal and
Cohen J (1988). Statistical power analysis for the be- Social Psychology 50, 197–200.
havioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Lambert W E (1967). ‘A social psychology of bilingualism.’
Associates. Journal of Social Issues 23, 91–109.
Crookes G & Schmidt R W (1991). ‘Motivation: Reopening MacIntyre P D, Clément R, Dörnyei Z & Noels K A (1998).
the research agenda.’ Language Learning 41, 469–512. ‘Conceptualizing willingness to communicate in a L2:
Deci E L & Ryan R M (1985). Intrinsic motivation and A situational model of L2 confidence and affiliation.’
self-determination in human behavior. New York: Modern Language Journal 82, 545–562.
Plenum Press. Marckwardt A H (1948). ‘Motives for the study of modern
Dörnyei Z (1994). ‘Motivation and motivating in the for- languages.’ Language Learning 1, 1–11.
eign language classroom.’ Modern Language Journal 78, Masgoret A-M & Gardner R C (2003). ‘Attitudes, motiva-
273–284. tion, and second language learning: A meta-analysis of
Dörnyei Z (2001). Teaching and researching motivation. studies conducted by Gardner and associates.’ Language
Essex: Longman. Learning 53, 123–163.
Dörnyei Z & Clement R (2001). ‘Motivational character- Nida E A (1956). ‘Motivation in second-language learning.’
istics of learning different target languages: Results of a Language Learning 7, 11–16.
nationwide survey.’ In Dörnyei Z & Schmidt R (eds.) Noels K A (2001). ‘New orientations in language learning
Motivation and second language acquisition. Honolulu, motivation: Towards a model of intrinsic, extrinsic, and
HI: The University of Hawai’i, Second Language integrative orientations and motivation.’ In Dörnyei Z &
Teaching and Curriculum Center. 399–432. Schmidt R (eds.) Motivation and second language
Dörnyei Z & Otto I (1998). ‘Motivation in action: acquisition. Honolulu, HI: The University of Hawai’i,
A process model of L2 motivation.’ Working Papers Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center.
in Applied Linguistics. Vol. 4. Thames Valley University, 43–68.
London. 43–69. Noels K A, Pon G & Clément R (1996). ‘Language identity
Ellis R (1994). The study of second language acquisition. and adjustment: The role of linguistic self-confidence in
Oxford: Oxford University Press. the adjustment process.’ Journal of Language and Social
Gardner R C (1985). Social psychology and second lan- Psychology 15, 246–264.
guage learning: The role of attitudes and motivation. Oxford R L & Shearin J (1994). ‘Language learning moti-
London: Edward Arnold Publishers. vation: Expanding the theoretical framework.’ Modern
Gardner R C (2000). ‘Correlation, causation, motivation, Language Journal 78, 12–28.
and second language acquisition.’ Canadian Psychology Scott W A (1968). ‘Attitude measurement.’ In Lindzey G
41, 10–24. & Aronson E (eds.) The handbook of social
Gardner R C & Bernaus M (2004). The applicability of psychology, vol. 2. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
the Attitude/Motivation Test Battery to EFL students 204–273.
in Spain. Unpublished manuscript, The University of Williams M & Burden R L (1997). Psychology for
Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada. language teachers: A social constructive approach,
Gardner R C & Lambert W E (1959). ‘Motivational vari- vol. 2, Research methods. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
ables in second language acquisition.’ Canadian Journal University Press.
of Psychology 13, 266–272.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen