Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/51604002

Effect of Chronic Iron Deficiency on Neuropsychological Domains in Infants

Article  in  Journal of child neurology · March 2012


DOI: 10.1177/0883073811416867 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
11 148

4 authors:

Beatriz Beltrán-Navarro Esmeralda Matute


University of Guadalajara University of Guadalajara
18 PUBLICATIONS   13 CITATIONS    102 PUBLICATIONS   1,338 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Edgar vasquez-garibay Daniel Zarabozo


University of Guadalajara University of Guadalajara
208 PUBLICATIONS   833 CITATIONS    38 PUBLICATIONS   259 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Appetitive traits and their relationships to weight in a Mexican population: Validation of the Adult Eating Behaviour Questionnaire - Esp View project

Bone mineral density and nutritional status in children with quadriplegic cerebral palsy View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Edgar vasquez-garibay on 17 November 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal http://jcn.sagepub.com/
of Child Neurology

Effect of Chronic Iron Deficiency on Neuropsychological Domains in Infants


Beatriz Beltrán-Navarro, Esmeralda Matute, Edgar Vásquez-Garibay and Daniel Zarabozo
J Child Neurol published online 29 August 2011
DOI: 10.1177/0883073811416867

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jcn.sagepub.com/content/early/2011/08/26/0883073811416867

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Journal of Child Neurology can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jcn.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jcn.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at SAGE Publications on September 7, 2011


Original Article
Journal of Child Neurology
000(00) 1-7
Effect of Chronic Iron Deficiency on ª The Author(s) 2011
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Neuropsychological Domains in Infants DOI: 10.1177/0883073811416867
http://jcn.sagepub.com

Beatriz Beltrán-Navarro, MSc1, Esmeralda Matute, PhD1,


Edgar Vásquez-Garibay, PhD2, and Daniel Zarabozo, PhD1

Abstract
The aim of this study was to assess the effects of chronic iron deficiency on neuropsychological traits in infants. We established
the nutritional iron status and assessed the neuropsychological characteristics of 58 Mexican 14- to 18-month-old infants. The
Bayley Scales of Infant Development, preschool language scales and an environmental sound perception task designed expressly
for the study, were used. The infants’ mothers were asked to fill out 2 questionnaires concerning their child’s sociodemographic
background. Six different neuropsychological domains were analyzed. Results showed that the chronic iron deficiency group did
show significantly lower scores on language, environmental sound perception, and motor measures, when compared with infants
with normal nutritional iron status at 6 and 14 to 18 months. Our conclusion is that the development of language and motor skills
and environmental sound perception appeared to be sensitive to the effects of chronic iron deficiency in infants.

Keywords
iron deficiency, neuropsychological development, infants, anemia

Received June 2, 2011. Accepted for publication June 19, 2011.

Iron deficiency is the most prevalent childhood nutritional With respect to iron deficiency anemia, study results are
disorder worldwide.1,2 It can be related to such factors as a diet more consistent and show a broader cognitive, motor and beha-
low in iron, impaired intestinal iron absorption, and parasitic vioral impairment in infants. However, they cannot be consid-
infections. Since iron intervenes in numerous metabolic pro- ered conclusive. Several studies of infants indicate that iron
cesses, including oxygen transportation, adenosine triphosphate deficiency anemia is associated with significantly lower perfor-
production, metabolism of neurotransmitters, DNA synthesis, mances on the mental,3,4,9-12 motor,4,9,10,12 and behavioral
and myelin production, several health processes could be affected tasks3,11,12 included in the Bayley Scales of Infant Development,
in children who have a low concentration of this element. Iron the Denver Developmental Screening Test,4 in observations of
deficiency is classified in 3 stages: (1) iron depletion, a decrease motor activity recorded in the home,7 in a motor development
in the normal reserve that has no functional consequences; (2) iron assessment battery,8 and in behavioral observations measuring
deficiency without anemia, biochemical changes reflected in the the duration and frequency of free play in both the labora-
normal seric ferritin reserve; and, (3) iron deficiency anemia, sub- tory13,14 and at home.14 In addition, the functional isolation
normal concentrations of seric ferritin and hemoglobin. hypothesis has been explored in infants with iron deficiency
Previous findings related to the effects of iron deficiency with- anemia. This hypothesis, derived from research on general-
out anemia on cognitive development in infants are inconsistent. ized undernutrition, holds that a decrease in the activity of
Some studies indicate no association, but low performance malnourished infants leads them to seek less stimulation from
scores on the Bayley scales of infant development3; whereas oth- their physical and social environments. In response to the
ers have found lower performance among infants on the mental,4
motor,5 and behavioral6 tests included in those scales. In more
recent studies, an adverse effect of iron deficiency without anemia 1
Instituto de Neurociencias, Universidad de Guadalajara, Guadalajara, Jalisco,
on motor development has been reported more consistently, based Mexico
on observations of motor activity at home7 and using motor devel- 2
Instituto de Nutrición Humana, Universidad de Guadalajara, Guadalajara,
opment assessment tools such as the Peabody Developmental Jalisco, Mexico
Motor Scale, the infant neurological international battery, and
Corresponding Author:
the Bayley behavioral rating scale; or through more informal Esmeralda Matute, PhD, Universidad de Guadalajara, Instituto de Neurociencias,
assessments of the gross motor development milestones and a Francisco de Quevedo 180, C.P. 44130, Guadalajara, Jalisco, Mexico
sequential/bi-manual coordination toy retrieval task.8 Email: ematute@cencar.udg.mx

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at SAGE Publications on September 7, 2011


2 Journal of Child Neurology 000(00)

infants’ behavior, caregivers thus tend to offer less stimula- per deciliter) 11 and seric ferritin (micrograms per liter) 12; for the
tion. Over time, these altered behavior patterns can impede iron deficiency without anemia group, hemoglobin (grams per decili-
ter) 11 and seric ferritin (micrograms per liter) <12; and for the iron
the child’s normal acquisition of environmental information
deficiency anemia group, hemoglobin (grams per deciliter) <11 and
and adversely affect her/his development.14 seric ferritin (micrograms per liter) <12. The hospital’s laboratory
Although existing studies of the effects of chronic iron defi- determined the concentrations of serum ferritin using immunoab-
ciency on infant abilities are few, some have found significantly sorbent assay, while hemoglobin concentrations were measured using
lower scores on the Bayley mental3,10,12 and motor12 scales in an automated blood cell counter.
In this study, 58 infants aged 14 to 18 months were included.
infants who have been affected by iron deficiency anemia for
According to their iron nutritional status, the infants were grouped as
more than 3 months. As we have seen, most of those studies used follows: normal iron nutritional status, n ¼ 30; iron deficiency without
standardized developmental tests, such as the Bayley scales, and anemia, n ¼ 17; iron deficiency anemia, n ¼ 9; iron deficiency without
took global scores as the principal measure of outcomes.3,4,9-12 hemoglobin value n ¼ 2. No significant sociodemographic differences
Few, however, have done analyses of specific items related to were found among these groups. In all 4 groups, the majority of the
infants were boys, while the caregiver was most often the mother. Most
language,12,15 attention,16 recognition memory,16,17 object per-
of the children were living with both parents; the majority of the fathers
manence,17 and gross motor development.7,8,10,12,18 were employees, while the mothers were homemakers. Mean parents’
In this study, the effects of chronic iron deficiency on neu- age and parents’ number of years of schooling were also similar among
ropsychological traits were assessed in 14- to 18-month-old groups (Table 1).
infants who had been diagnosed with iron deficiency 1 year To determine the effect of chronic iron deficiency (the presence of
iron deficiency on both blood tests: at ages 6 and 14–18 months) on
earlier. To evaluate them, we used Aylward’s general neuro-
cognitive development, our main objective, 2 groups were subse-
psychological design19 for early neuropsychological assessment. quently formed on the basis of the presence or absence of chronic iron
Our hypothesis was that, even at such an early age, the effects of deficiency at both ages tested: thus, group 1 included 25 infants that
chronic iron deficiency on neuropsychological development presented normal iron nutritional status at 6 and 14–18 months (here-
would be detectable, but that no underperformance would be inafter the always normal iron nutritional status group), while group 2
was made up of 6 infants that presented iron deficiency with or with-
found when infants at this age (14 to 18 months) presented iron
out anemia at both 6 and 14–18 months (hereinafter the chronic
deficiency for the first time. iron deficiency group). In the latter group, 1 infant presented iron
deficiency without anemia at both ages, 3 infants showed iron defi-
ciency without anemia at 6 months and iron deficiency anemia at
Methods 14–18 months, and 2 infants had iron deficiency without anemia at
6 months and presented iron deficiency but lacked the hemoglobin
Selection and Description of Participants value at 14–18 months old. No significant sociodemographic dif-
Subjects were recruited from a previous study20 (n ¼ 154) conducted ferences were found between the 2 groups (Table 2). Twenty-
at ‘‘Doctor Juan I. Menchaca’’ Civil Hospital in Guadalajara, Jalisco, seven infants were not taken into account in the second analysis,
Mexico. In Monterrosa et al20 research, infants were followed from as 15 of them presented a disparity between the iron concentration
birth to 6 months in a nutritional evaluation. We invited all parents results on the 2 blood tests. Of the remaining 12 infants, 5 pre-
of Monterrosa et al20 sample that could be reached by telephone or sented anemia without iron deficiency at 6 months, and no seric
at home when their infants were 14 to 18 months old. A total of ferritin or hemoglobin values were available for 7 of them at age
74 families were contacted (48%), as the rest were no longer living 6 months.
at the addresses recorded for the earlier study, but of those 74 only
58 mothers (38% of the total of Monterrosa et al20 sample) gave their
written consent for the blood venipuncture test required to determine
Assessment
their infants’ current nutritional iron status (serum ferritin and hemo- Each child was assessed using the following tests:
globin), and for the neuropsychological assessment. The Ethics (1) The Bayley Scales of Infant Development.21 This test was used
Committee/Institutional Review Boards of the Instituto de Neuro- to measure the mental and motor development and to evaluate the
ciencias (Neuroscience Institute) at Universidad de Guadalajara behavior of the infants. Mental, motor, and behavioral standard scores
(University of Guadalajara) and the ‘‘Doctor Juan I. Menchaca’’ were computed.
Civil Hospital approved the study design. (2) Preschool language scales22 were used to measure receptive
The inclusion criteria for Monterrosa et al20 study were: first or and expressive language ability. Three raw scores related to auditory
second child, physiological gestation 37 weeks, birth weight comprehension, expressive communication and total language were
2500 grams, no perinatal pathology, no congenital malformations taken into account. We used the raw scores because this test has not
or genetic alterations, no prenatal infections, Apgar 7 at 5 minutes, been extensively used in Mexican populations so no normative data
and no chronic illness or neonatal hypoxia sequelae. Mothers’ ages is available for the country.
ranged from 18 to 33 years (mean, 25.5 years) and no iron deficiency, (3) An environmental sound perception task designed expressly
with or without anemia, had occurred during their pregnancies. The for this study to evaluate the infants’ ability to identify nonverbal
infants had been given a blood venipuncture test at 6 months to verify auditory stimuli. Ten physical toys or objects (dog, bird, telephone,
their nutritional iron status (seric ferritin and hemoglobin concentra- rooster, car, crying baby, rattle, keys, cat, train) and the recordings
tion) but no neuropsychological assessments were conducted at that (5 seconds each) of their respective sounds were used: dog bark-
time, nor were their developmental milestones registered. All the ing, bird singing, telephone ringing, rooster crowing, car starting,
infants that presented iron deficiency with or without anemia at that baby crying, rattle shaking, keys moving, cat meowing, and train
time received oral iron therapy (daily doses of 15 mg/kg of ferrous whistling. The 10 objects were placed together in front of the
sulfate for 2 months). child, who was then asked to listen to the sounds one-by-one and
The diagnostic biochemical criteria used by the previous study20 point to the object that was related to it. One point was given for
and in this study, were those proposed by the World Health Organiza- each correct match, so the maximum score was 10. Before using
tion2: for the normal iron nutritional status group, hemoglobin (grams this task in this study, 14 sounds were pretested with a pilot group

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at SAGE Publications on September 7, 2011


Beltrán-Navarro et al 3

Table 1. Sociodemographic Characteristics of Groups Aged 14 to 18 Months

NINS n¼30 ID Without Anemia n¼17 IDA n¼9 ID Without Hemoglobin Value n¼2 w2 P
Categorical variables
Gender, no. of boys 17 10 4 1 0.55 .90
Caregiver, % mother 83.33 76.47 77.77 50 1.43 .69
People living with the child, % both 80 76.47 88.88 50 1.64 .65
parents
Parents’ marital status, % 46.66 41.17 22.22 0 3.09 .37
cohabitation
Fathers’ occupation, % employee 93.33 100 88.88 100 1.82 .61
Mothers’ occupation, % homemaker 70 76.47 77.77 100 1.07 .78
Continuous variables: means, standard deviation
NINS n¼30 ID without anemia n¼17 IDA n¼9 ID without hemoglobin value n¼2 K P
Infants’ age in months 15.67 (1.26) 16.17 (1.27) 15.98 (1.81) 14.22 (0) 0.93 .62
Fathers’ age in years 26.20 (4.90) 26.35 (5.06) 26.67 (2.91) 22.50 (2.12) 0.59 .74
Mothers’ age in years 23.33 (3.49) 23.18 (3.12) 23.22 (2.90) 21 (1.41) 0.02 .98
Fathers’ schooling in years 9.20 (3.54) 9.75 (2.86) 8.56 (2.55) 7.50 (2.12) 0.97 .61
Mothers’ schooling in years 9.87 (3.24) 9.71 (3.36) 8.89 (3.25) 9 (4.24) 1.94 .37

Abbreviations: NINS, normal iron nutritional status group; ID without anemia, iron deficiency without anemia group; IDA, iron deficiency anemia group; ID with-
out hemoglobin value, iron deficiency without hemoglobin value group.

Table 2. Sociodemographic Characteristics of Groups at 6 and 14 to wing. Each child’s mother was present in the room throughout the
18 Months Old evaluation process. It is important to note that this was a blind study,
so the psychologist in charge of assessing the infants had no knowl-
Always NINS Chronic ID edge of their iron nutritional status. The assessment sessions lasted
n¼25 n¼6 w2 P 2 hours. Finally, the blood venipuncture process was carried out by
a trained pediatric nurse on the same day as the neuropsychological
Categorical variables assessment, but in a different room. The psychologist was not present.
Gender, no. of boys 13 2 0.67 .65
Caregiver, % mother 88 66.66 1.62 .20
People living with the 84 66.66 0.93 .33
child, % both parents
Statistics
Parents’ marital status, 48 16.66 1.95 .16 All data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social
% cohabitation Sciences (SPSS) for Windows, release 11.5.0.23 Standard scores
Fathers’ occupation, 92 100 0.51 .47 for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development and raw scores for
% employee the preschool language scale were obtained. Later, the raw scores
Mothers’ occupation, 76 83.33 0.14 .70 from the tasks included in the tests were converted into percen-
% homemaker tages and arranged in 6 neuropsychological domains: environmen-
Continuous variables: means, standard deviation tal sound perception, receptive language, expressive language, fine
Always Chronic ID U P motor abilities, gross motor abilities, and executive function
NINS n¼25 n¼6 (namely, A-not-B, problem solving, and tool use) following the
Infants’ age in months 15.65 (1.27) 15 (1.37) 44.50 .12 general neuropsychological design proposed by Aylward19 for
Fathers’ age in years 26.20 (5.24) 23.50 (2.34) 54.50 .30 early neuropsychological assessment. Cronbach a analysis was
Mothers’ age in years 23.20 (3.48) 22.67 (4.32) 60 .44 then applied and found internal consistency reliability among the
Fathers’ schooling in years 8.64 (2.81) 8.83 (1.94) 74 .95 items in each 1 of the 6 neuropsychological domains, as follows: envi-
Mothers’ schooling in 9.52 (2.97) 10 (4.42) 72 .87 ronmental sound perception, a (0.6727); receptive language,
years a (0.7809); expressive language, a (0.7650); fine motor abilities,
a (0.5541); gross motor abilities, a (0.7579); and executive function,
Abbreviations: Always NINS, always normal iron nutritional status group; a (0.6320).
Chronic ID, chronic iron deficiency group. Two analyses were carried out, the first included n ¼ 56 infants
divided into 3 groups: (1) normal iron nutritional status, n ¼ 30; (2)
iron deficiency without anemia, n ¼ 17; and, (3) iron deficiency ane-
mia, n ¼ 9. In the second analysis (n ¼ 31), 2 groups were compared:
group 1 was the always normal iron nutritional status, n ¼ 25; and
of 26 infants (10 girls, 16 boys) aged 14 to 22 months (mean, 16 group 2 was the chronic iron deficiency, n ¼ 6.
months), used to assess the adequacy of this task for this age The categorical variables were analyzed using chi square, while the
range. A mean of 5 (standard deviation 2.36) correct answers was continuous variables were examined with the Mann Whitney-U test
obtained. Ten of the 14 sounds were chosen, as the other 4 proved for the 2 groups, and the Kruskal-Wallis test for the 3 groups. The
difficult to identify at this age. level of significance was set at P  .05.
Evaluations of the infants and interviews with their mothers were Because we had only 2 small groups for the second analysis, we
all conducted by the first author in a spacious, well-lit room, free of used not only statistical significance to evaluate the results, but also
distractions, in ‘‘Doctor Juan I. Menchaca’’ Civil Hospital’s nutrition strengthen the statistical analysis by applying the effect size of

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at SAGE Publications on September 7, 2011


4 Journal of Child Neurology 000(00)

Table 3. Assessment Scores, Means, and Standard Deviationa

NINS n¼30 ID Without Anemia n¼17 IDA n¼9 K P


Bayley Scales of Infant Development: standard scores
Mental 96.60 (9.94) 94.29 (12.98) 94.44 (19.09) 0.44 .79
Motor 111.67 (10.20) 110 (9.40) 107.89 (10.38) 0.82 .66
Behavioral 55.63 (26.52) 55.76 (26.97) 47.89 (42.56) 0.20 .90
Preschool language scale: raw scores
Auditory comprehension 22.30 (1.74) 22.35 (1.27) 22.33 (0.86) 1.23 .53
Expressive communication 22.53 (2.81) 22.41 (3.39) 21.67 (3.24) 0.70 .70
Total language 44.83 (3.83) 44.76 (3.88) 44 (3.74) 0.54 .76
Neuropsychological domains: raw scores converted to % of correct answers
Environmental sound perception 44.67 (24.17) 39.41 (19.51) 23.33 (18.02) 5.34 .06
Receptive language 60.58 (17.75) 56.39 (17.15) 56.85 (13.15) 1.53 .46
Fine motor abilities 54.17 (13.39) 52.94 (11.46) 50 (7.07) 1.34 .50
Gross motor abilities 65.34 (15.82) 65.22 (8.67) 61.11 (18.95) 0.63 .72
Expressive language 47.50 (18.32) 45 (19.28) 39.44 (19.43) 1.41 .49
Executive function 80 (22.28) 69.41 (33.25) 66.66 (31.62) 1.70 .42

Abbreviations: NINS, normal iron nutritional status group; ID without anemia, iron deficiency without anemia group; IDA, iron deficiency anemia group.
a
Standard deviations are presented within parentheses.

Spearman r2 (squared r), a standardized, objective measure of deficiency without anemia, and iron deficiency anemia)
the magnitude of an observed effect. showed no differences among them.
Previous studies of infants have reported negative effects of
iron deficiency anemia on scores attained on the Bayley mental
Results
scale,3,4,9,10-12,15 the Denver Developmental Screening Test,4 and
In the first analysis, no differences among groups were found the Bayley motor scale.4,9,10,12 Moreover, those studies have
(Table 3). observed certain behavioral peculiarities that led to the infants
The second analysis compared the scores obtained by the studied being characterized as fearful,3 insecure,3,14 unhappy,14
always normal iron nutritional status group (n ¼ 25) with those cautious,3,11 less active, indecisive, less participatory, irritable,
of the chronic iron deficiency group (n ¼ 6). This comparison less attentive,12,14,25 with tendencies to isolation,14,25 and difficult
showed that the latter had achieved significantly lower scores to calm once angry or upset (eg, after crying).25
on the Bayley motor scale, in expressive communication, and In contrast, findings for iron deficiency without anemia
on the total language subscale of the preschool language scale. groups have been inconclusive, neither entirely consistent nor
Later analysis of the neuropsychological domains showed that coincident. On the 1 hand, Lozoff et al3 found that iron defi-
the chronic iron deficiency group had lower scores than the ciency without anemia is not associated with low scores on
always normal iron nutritional status group on environmental developmental tests, nor with behavioral alterations, and that
sound perception, receptive language, gross motor abilities and only the iron deficiency anemia condition affects cognitive,
expressive language. To reinforce the statistical analysis, motor and behavioral development in infants. Similarly, Dei-
Spearman r2 (r squared) was applied to measure the effect size nard et al6 reported no differences between infants with moder-
of chronic iron deficiency. This measure showed an effect of ate and severe iron deficiency without anemia compared with a
medium size, per Cohen,24 for environmental sound perception normal iron nutritional status group, though variations in their
and expressive language; an effect larger than small size, also behavioral characteristics were seen. Finally, Oski et al26 iden-
per Cohen,24 was evident in the scores for receptive language tified only a trend toward lower scores on the Bayley mental
and gross motor abilities (Table 4). It is worthwhile to note the scale in the iron deficiency without anemia group.
importance of our findings of the effect size, since the analysis Contrary to those results, Akman et al4 and Baptista-
was conducted with such a small sample. González et al5 found that iron deficiency without anemia does
indeed affect cognitive development, and that its effect is
already evident during infancy. Moreover, this latter work5
Discussion pointed out that the degree of cognitive impairment increases
The main aim of this study was to ascertain if an early chronic iron with the severity of the iron deficiency condition.
deficiency effect on cognitive, motor, and behavioral develop- The fact that we found no differences among groups can be
ment is observable when infants are 14 to 18 months old. related to the similarity of the sociodemographic context and
The first analysis was conducted to determine whether iron perinatal background of the 3 groups included in our study (see
deficiency presented for the first time at 14 to 18 months of age Methods, and Table 1).
affects neuropsychological development, but the comparison of Indeed, several of the studies that found an effect of iron defi-
3 groups of infants (normal iron nutritional status, iron ciency anemia on cognitive, motor, or behavioral development

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at SAGE Publications on September 7, 2011


Beltrán-Navarro et al 5

Table 4. Assessment Scores, Means, and Standard Deviationa

Always NINS n¼25 Chronic ID n¼6 U P Spearman r2


Bayley Scales of Infant Development: standard scores
Mental 97.04 (8.96) 94.67 (17.09) 68.00 .72 0.004
Motor 112.72 (10.58) 102.50 (7.06) 32.00 .03* 0.155
Behavioral 56.40 (27.37) 33.67 (32.51) 44.50 .12 0.077
Preschool language scale: raw scores
Auditory comprehension 22.32 (1.65) 22 (0.89) 53.00 .25 0.044
Expressive communication 22.92 (2.73) 19.83 (1.47) 22.00 .007* 0.239
Total language 45.24 (3.81) 41.83 (1.47) 38.00 .01* 0.187
Neuropsychological domains: raw scores converted to % of correct answers
Environmental sound perception 44.40 (24.84) 10 (15.49) 18.00 .004* 0.300
Receptive language 60.93 (17.71) 47.05 (8.31) 33.00 .03* 0.150
Fine motor abilities 55.40 (13.06) 45 (7.07) 39.50 .07 0.107
Gross motor abilities 65.91 (16.16) 48.88 (19.06) 36.00 .04* 0.140
Expressive language 49.80 (17.99) 29.16 (7.35) 19.50 .005* 0.300
Executive function 78.40 (23.03) 63.33 (34.44) 57.00 .34 0.030

Abbreviations: Always NINS, always normal iron nutritional status group; Chronic ID, chronic iron deficiency group.
a
Standard deviations are presented within parentheses.
*P < .05.

have also identified an association between iron deficiency ane- 3 months of iron supplementation. Treatment failure found in this
mia and certain demographic differences,10 including family study can be related to the discontinuation or irregular administra-
composition,3,10 the intellectual and educational characteristics tion of treatment, as Mexico has a high incidence of interruptions
of mothers (lower intelligence quotient,10 fewer years of school- of therapeutic treatments for various diseases (55.5%–60%),
ing3,12), and some personality traits of the infants themselves especially chronic ones, complex treatments, treatments of more
(eg, higher timidity).17 than 7 days’ duration, and in pediatric patients.27
It is important to mention that the World Health Organiza- Concerning the comparison of the task scores between the
tion2 reported a disparity in the prevalence of iron deficiency 2 groups, infants with chronic iron deficiency underperformed
without anemia/iron deficiency anemia in preschool children the always normal iron nutritional status group on 4 of the
between developed (10%–20%) and developing countries 6 neuropsychological domains that were assessed: environ-
(30%–80%). The differences in the prevalence of iron defi- mental sound perception, gross motor, receptive language, and
ciency without anemia/iron deficiency anemia between these expressive language. It is noteworthy that the scores achieved
types of countries could be related to variations in the socio- on the scales used to evaluate these 2 groups all fell within the
economic and cultural background that characterized each one. normal range, a pattern that has been observed in other stud-
Thus, the association between iron deficiency (with or with- ies.4,10,12 According to the World Health Organization,2 infants
out anemia) and socioeconomic indicators of poverty can that presented iron deficiency will have a delay in their psycho-
represent confounding variables for interpreting the effect of motor development, and when they reach school age they will
iron deficiency (with or without anemia) on psychomotor devel- show impaired performance in tests related to language, motor
opment, because when family and biological variables are con- skills, and coordination, equivalent to a 5- to 10-point deficit in
trolled for, the effect of iron deficiency (with or without anemia) intelligence quotient. However, to our knowledge, mental defi-
decreases, as in our study. It is important to consider that risk ciency in those children has never been reported.
factors do not normally occur in isolation and that a combination Lozoff et al3,10 and Walter et al12 already reported a negative
of a larger number of such factors increases the probability effect of chronic iron deficiency (3 months) on cogni-
that infants will achieve lower scores in cognitive, motor, tive,3,10,12 motor12 and language12 development in infants. Upon
or behavioral development. doing follow-up on their samples, they found that this effect was
The second analysis was conducted to explore our main still observable in school performance during childhood and
objective; that is, to determine whether early chronic iron defi- even adolescence.28-32
ciency affects cognitive, motor, and behavioral development. In infancy, language and motor skills undergo rapid deve-
One unpredicted finding was that at 6 months of age, 7 infants lopment, while in this same period we also see advances in
presented iron deficiency and at 14 to 18 months, 6 of those chil- other cognitive domains; for example, executive functions.
dren still had that deficiency, despite having received oral iron While our study found no differences in the tasks related to exec-
therapy (daily doses of 15 mg/kg of ferrous sulfate for 2 months). utive functions (A-not-B, problem solving, tool use), Carter
The literature contains reports that between 36%10 and et al17 reported lower scores on the object permanence task,
57.89%12 of infants with moderate iron deficiency anemia fully though not on the A-not-B test, in infants from 9 to 10 months
corrected their iron and hemoglobin values after receiving with iron deficiency anemia. In general, greater variability in

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at SAGE Publications on September 7, 2011


6 Journal of Child Neurology 000(00)

the expression of cognitive development is characteristic of venipuncture blood process and determining the infants’ iron nutri-
younger age groups when a cognitive domain is just beginning tional status. We are very grateful to the infants and their parents for
to develop; thus, our results suggest that at the ages we studied, their voluntary participation in the study. Research was carried out at
it is too early to perceive the effects of chronic iron deficiency on the Instituto de Neurociencias (Neuroscience Institute) of Universidad
de Guadalajara (University of Guadalajara), the Instituto de Nutrición
functions that attain higher levels of development later in life,
Humana (Human Nutrition Institute) of Universidad de Guadalajara
such as executive functions. Hence, we would expect that the
(University of Guadalajara) and the ‘‘Doctor Juan I. Menchaca’’ Civil
effects of chronic iron deficiency on those functions would Hospital, in Guadalajara, Jalisco, México. Some sections of this article
appear at more advanced ages, so longitudinal studies covering were presented at the International Neuropsychological Society’s Mid-
a wider age range are needed to assess the effect of infantile year Meeting in 2008.
chronic iron deficiency on executive functions.
During the last trimester of pregnancy and the first 2 years of Author Contributions
postnatal life, rapid brain growth occurs. This is called a
The details of each author’s contribution to the study are as follows:
‘‘growth spurt,’’ a period in which the central nervous system BB-N: This research was for her MSc thesis. She contacted partici-
is extremely vulnerable to nutritional deficiencies. Although pants’ mothers, evaluated the subjects, built the databases, did the sta-
there is a greater chance of recovery in early stages of develop- tistical analysis and literature search, and prepared the first draft and
ment, it is probable that in these stages chronic iron deficiency amendments to the text. EV-G: A pediatrician with a PhD in nutrition,
does affect brain plasticity significantly and its effects would he served as the nutritional expert. He was the coordinator of the Mon-
appear primarily in a lag in language and motor development. terrosa et al20 study, and provided information from that work, as well
At older ages, the effects of iron deficiency might be mani- as space in the ‘‘Doctor Juan I. Menchaca’’ Civil Hospital’s nutrition
fested in other, more complex, and sophisticated cognitive wing. Finally, he made arrangements with the hospital’s laboratory for
functions. the analyses of the infants’ iron nutritional status. DZ was the statisti-
In conclusion, our study has produced 4 main findings. First, cal analysis expert for the study. He provided advice and reviewed the
Methodology and Statistical Analysis sections of the study. EM was
we found that almost all the infants that presented iron defi-
the Project Director, participated actively in all aspects of the work,
ciency at 6 months still had it at 14 to 18 months. Second, and contributed directly to the article.
chronic iron deficiency has an effect on cognitive and motor
development that is evident during infancy and expressed in
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
poor performance on tasks related to language, motor abilities
and environmental sound perception. Third, no effect of The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
nonchronic iron deficiency was evident, suggesting that when
iron deficiency is nonchronic it has adverse effects on a child’s
neuropsychological development only when combined with Funding
certain environmental and biological variables. This could The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship,
explain the fact that, in some studies, nonchronic iron defi- and/or publication of this article.
ciency is related to low scores. Finally, the observable effect
of the actual scores achieved were all within the broad range Ethical Approval
of normal; from a neurodevelopmental perspective none would Participants’ mothers gave their written consent for both the blood
be considered significantly delayed or justify a developmental venipuncture test required to determine their infants’ current nutri-
diagnosis at this moment of development. However, evaluating tional iron status (seric ferritin and hemoglobin), and the neuropsycho-
other aspects that can be examined at older ages, such as logical assessment. The Ethics Committee/Institutional Review
mathematics skills, reading, memory, attention, and executive Boards of both the Instituto de Neurociencias (Neuroscience Institute)
functions could reflect a greater impact on cognition. Thus, the at the Universidad de Guadalajara (University of Guadalajara) and the
‘‘Doctor Juan I. Menchaca’’ Civil Hospital approved the study design.
results for the age group studied in this research cannot be
generalized to other cohorts, due to the absence of cognitive
functions that only become evident later in life. Longitudinal References
studies over a wider age range can provide information on 1. UNICEF, WHO. Prevention and Control of Iron Deficiency Anemia
changes in the manifestations of the effects of chronic iron defi- in Women and Children. Geneva, Switzerland: UNICEF/WHO;
ciency on neuropsychological development in relation to the 1999.
age of children. 2. WHO. Iron Deficiency Anaemia: Assessment, Prevention, and Con-
trol a Guide for Programme Managers. Geneva, Switzerland:
Acknowledgments WHO; 2001.
3. Lozoff B, Wolf AW, Jimenez E. Iron-deficiency anemia and infant
The authors gratefully acknowledge Jissela Alfaro from the Instituto
de Nutrición Humana (Human Nutrition Institute) of Universidad de development: effects of extended oral iron therapy. J Pediatr.
Guadalajara (University of Guadalajara), in Guadalajara, Jalisco, 1996;129:382-389.
Mexico, for her assistance in recruiting the participants. We thank the 4. Akman M, Cebeci D, Okur V, et al. The effects of iron deficiency
nurses and laboratory assistants at the ‘‘Doctor Juan I. Menchaca’’ Civil on infants’ developmental test performance. Acta Paediatr. 2004;
Hospital in Guadalajara, Jalisco, Mexico, for performing the 93:1391-1396.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at SAGE Publications on September 7, 2011


Beltrán-Navarro et al 7

5. Baptista-González HA, Rosenfeld F, Ramı́rez A, et al. Diferencias 20. Monterrosa E, Frongillo E, Vásquez-Garibay E, et al. Predomi-
en el crecimiento y neurodesarrollo en el lactante menor deficiente nant breast-feeding from birth to six months is associated with
de hierro no anémico. Bol Med Hosp Infant Mex. 2004;61:9-18. fewer gastrointestinal infections and increased risk for iron defi-
6. Deinard A, Gilbert A, Dodds M, Egeland B. Iron deficiency and ciency among infants. J Nutr. 2008;138:1499-1504.
behavioral deficits. Pediatrics. 1981;68:828-833. 21. Bayley N. In: Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed. San
7. Olney D, Pollitt E, Kariger P, et al. Young Zanzibari children with Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1993.
iron deficiency, iron deficiency anemia, stunting, or malaria have 22. Zimmerman IL, Steiner VG, Pond RE. In: Preschool Language
lower motor activity scores and spend less time in locomotion. Scale. 4th ed. Spanish. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
J Nutr. 2007;137:2756-2762. Corporation; 2002.
8. Shafir T, Angulo R, Jing J, et al. Iron deficiency and human motor 23. Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) for Windows.
development. Early Hum Dev. 2008;84:479-485. Rel. 11.5.0. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc.; 2002.
9. Idjradinata P, Pollitt E. Reversal of developmental delays in iron 24. Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. London, England:
deficient anemic infants treated with iron. Lancet. 1993;341:1-4. Sage; 2009.
10. Lozoff B, Brittenham GM, Wolf AW. Iron deficiency anemia and 25. Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, et al. Behavioral and devel-
iron therapy: effects on infant developmental test performance. opmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy
Pediatrics. 1987;79:981-995. full-term infants. Pediatrics. 2003;112:846-854.
11. Walter T, Kovalskys J, Stekel A. Effect of mild iron deficiency on 26. Oski FA, Honig AS, Helu B, Howanitz P. Effect of iron therapy
infant mental development scores. J Pediatr. 1983;102:519-522. on behavior performance in non-anaemic iron-deficient infants.
12. Walter T, de Andraca I, Chadud P, Perales CG. Iron deficiency Pediatrics. 1983;71:877-880.
anemia: adverse effects on infant psychomotor development. 27. Reyes H, Guiscafré H, Muñoz O, et al. Factores asociados a
Pediatrics. 1989;84:7-17. incumplimiento terapéutico y costo del desperdicio de antimicro-
13. Lozoff B, Klein N, Prabucki KM. Iron-deficient anemic infants at bianos en infección respiratoria alta y diarrea aguda. Fármacos.
play. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1986;7:152-158. 1998;1:10-13.
14. Lozoff B, Klein N, Nelson E, et al. Behavior of infants with iron 28. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, et al. Poorer behavioral and devel-
deficiency anemia. Child Dev. 1998;69:24-36. opmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron
15. Lozoff B, Brittenham GM, Viteri E, et al. Developmental deficits deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105:1-11.
in iron-deficient infants: effects of age and severity of iron lack. 29. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Smith JB. Double burden of iron deficiency
J Pediatr. 1982;101:948-952. in infancy and low socioeconomic status: a longitudinal analysis
16. Burden M, Westerlund A, Armony-Sivan N, et al. An event related- of cognitive test scores to age 19 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc
potential study of attention and recognition memory in infants with Med. 2006;160:1108-1113.
iron deficiency anemia. Pediatrics. 2007;120:336-345. 30. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Wolf AW. Long-term developmental out-
17. Carter RC, Jacobson JL, Burden M, et al. Iron deficiency anemia come of infants with iron deficiency. N Engl J Med. 1991;325:
and cognitive function in infancy. Pediatrics. 2010;126:427-434. 687-694.
18. Kariger P, Stoltzfus R, Olney D, et al. Iron deficiency and phys- 31. Rivera F, Walter T. Efecto de la anemia ferropriva en el lactante
ical growth predict attainment of walking but not crawling in sobre el desarrollo psicológico del escolar. Rev Chil Pediatr.
poorly nourished Zanzibari infants. J Nutr. 2005;135:814-819. 1996;67:141-147.
19. Aylward GP. Infant and Early Childhood Neuropsychology. New 32. Walter T. Impact of iron deficiency on cognition in infancy and
York, NY: Plenum Press; 1997. childhood. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1993;47:307-316.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at SAGE Publications on September 7, 2011


View publication stats

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen