Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

Application of the Spacing Effect to Improve Learning

and Memory for Functional Tasks in Traumatic Brain Injury:


A Pilot Study

Yael Goverover, Juan Carlos Arango-Lasprilla, Frank G. Hillary,


Nancy Chiaravalloti, John DeLuca

KEY WORDS Research has indicated that many people with traumatic brain injury (TBI) experience learning and memory diffi-
•  activities of daily living culties because of impairments in the initial acquisition of information. We examined a strategy, the spacing effect,
known to enhance new learning in a laboratory setting in healthy control participants (HCs) and in people with TBI.
•  brain injuries
The spacing effect indicates that information is learned better when presentation trials are distributed over time
•  cognition disorders
(spaced presentation) rather than consecutively (massed presentation). In this study, we examined the application
•  learning of the spacing effect in improving functional tasks. We used a within-subject design and included 10 participants
•  memory with TBI and 15 HCs. In both the TBI and the HC groups, material learned under the spaced learning condition
was recalled better than that learned under massed learning conditions. These results provide initial evidence
supporting the use of the spacing effect to improve new learning of functional tasks for people with TBI.

Goverover, Y., Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Hillary, F. G., Chiaravalloti, N., & DeLuca, J. (2009). Application of the spacing effect
to improve learning and memory for functional tasks in traumatic brain injury: A pilot study. American Journal of
Occupational Therapy, 63, 543–548.

T
Yael Goverover, PhD, OT, is Assistant Professor, raumatic brain injury (TBI) is one of the leading causes of disability in people <35
Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Culture,
years old (Sosin, Sniezek, & Thurman, 1996). Data have indicated that cognitive
Education, and Human Development, New York University,
35 West 4th Street, 11th Floor, New York, NY 10012, and and psychosocial impairments, rather than physical impairments alone, lead to
Kessler Medical Rehabilitation Research and Education decreased adaptive and vocational functioning in people with TBI (Anderson, Bigler,
Center, West Orange, NJ; yg243@nyu.edu & Blatter, 1995; Dikmen, Machamer, Winn, & Temkin, 1995; Olver, Ponsford, &
Juan Carlos Arango-Lasprilla, PhD, is Assistant
Curran, 1996). Memory impairment is one of the most frequent cognitive symptoms
Professor, Department of Physical Medicine and reported by patients, their family members, and clinicians after TBI (Goldstein &
Rehabilitation, Virginia Commonwealth University, Levin, 2001; Rosenthal & Ricker, 2000; van Zomeren & van den Burg, 1985), and
Richmond.
it has been shown to be associated with vocational instability (Drake, Gray, Yoder,
Frank G. Hillary, PhD, is Assistant Professor, Pramuka, & Llewelly, 2000). Recent studies have demonstrated that the primary rea-
Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, son for difficulties in new learning and memory in people with TBI is difficulty in the
University Park, and Kessler Foundation Research Center, initial acquisition of information rather than the retrieval of information from long-
West Orange, NJ.
term storage (DeLuca, Schultheis, Madigan, Christodoulou, & Averill, 2000).
Nancy Chiaravalloti, PhD, is Director, Specifically, DeLuca et al. (2000) determined that when participants with TBI and
Neuropsychology and Neuroscience Lab, Kessler healthy control participants (HCs) reached a predetermined learning criterion, those
Foundation Research Center, West Orange, NJ. with TBI recalled and recognized the newly learned information at a level comparable
John DeLuca, PhD, ABPP, is Vice President for
to that of healthy adults. However, participants with TBI required significantly more
Research, Kessler Foundation Research Center, West trials to reach this learning criterion. That is, they had difficulty initially learning new
Orange, NJ, and University of Medicine and Dentistry of information but not in retrieving it from long-term storage. These findings suggest
New Jersey/New Jersey Medical School, Newark.
that the primary memory deficit after TBI involves acquiring or learning new
information.
Various learning strategies from the cognitive psychology literature have been
shown to significantly improve the learning of new information in HC samples.
The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 543
Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/17/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
However, there has been very little work in applying such had a history of neurological illness (aside from TBI), a his-
strategies to people with TBI, which could potentially result tory of major psychiatric illness, a history of alcohol or drug
in a significant improvement in cognitive and everyday func- abuse, or severe visual or motor impairment that might
tioning. One such technique is called the spacing effect. interfere with study procedures. All of the participants with
The spacing effect is the observation that information TBI were ≥1 year postinjury (mean years postinjury = 8.2,
presented using spaced repetitions is better remembered than standard deviation = 6.8). The two groups did not differ
information presented via massed repetitions (Challis, 1993). significantly with regard to age (TBI = 42.5, HC = 43.3),
A large body of literature has documented that learning trials gender (TBI, 54.5% men; HC, 45.5% men), and premorbid
spaced over time lead to better recall than massed learning intelligence (as measured by the Wide Range Achievement
(consecutive learning trials), particularly under conditions in Test 3 [WRAT–3] Reading Subtest [Wilkinson, 1993];
which the delay between study and test is long rather than mean row score = 51.6 for both groups, the scaled score of
short (Bahrick, Bahrick, Bahrick, & Bahrick, 1993; Bahrick indicates above-average reading academic skills for both
& Phelps, 1987; Cahill & Toppino, 1993; Dempster, 1987, groups). All recruitment and experimental procedures were
1988; Glenberg, 1976, 1977, 1979; Glenberg & Lehmann, approved by the institutional review board (IRB) and Health
1980; Glover & Corkill, 1987; Hintzman, 1974; Jensen & Insurance Portability and Accountability Act compliance
Freund, 1981; Melton, 1970; Rea & Modigliani, 1985; boards. Before study enrollment, all participants indicated
Shaughnessy, Zimmerman, & Underwood, 1972; Toppino, willingness to participate in the study by signing a consent
1991, 1993; Underwood, 1970; Zechmeister & Shaughnessy, form approved by the IRB.
1980). Several theoretical explanations for the success of the
spacing effect exist, but these are beyond the scope of this Procedure and Materials
article (for further review, see Dempster, 1989). We used a within-subject design in which all participants
To date, most of the research on the spacing effect has were required to complete two paragraph-learning tasks
been conducted with healthy people in laboratory settings (reading a paragraph and being asked to remember it) and
(e.g., Challis, 1993; Kahana & Howard, 2005; Janiszewski, two route-learning tasks (after being presented with a route
Noel, & Sawyer, 2003). Some researchers, however, have on a map, participants were asked to reproduce it on an
examined how the spacing effect might facilitate learning of identical map). The paragraph-learning task involved two
and memory for different tasks in clinical populations who different paragraphs, and the route-learning task included
may exhibit memory deficits, including people with amnesia two different black-and-white maps. We chose these two
(Cermak, Verfaellie, Lanzoni, Mather, & Chase, 1996), tasks for this experiment to examine verbal (i.e., paragraph)
dementia (Camp & Foss, 1997), multiple sclerosis (Goverover, and nonverbal (i.e., map) learning and memory processes on
Hillary, Chiaravalloti, Arango-Lasprilla, & DeLuca, 2008), tasks that could be encountered during daily life.
and TBI (Hillary et al., 2003). These studies indicated that the For each task, participants received a total of three learn-
spacing effect can effectively facilitate recall of newly learned ing trials to the task materials. In the massed condition, tasks
material in patients with memory impairments. However, were presented three consecutive times; in the spaced condi-
most of these studies examining the impact of the spacing effect tion, tasks were presented three times with a 5-min interval
have been conducted only with laboratory-based tasks, such as between them. The two conditions were presented in coun-
word lists. Thus, the impact of the spacing effect on everyday terbalanced order.
activities in clinical samples remains unclear. The purpose of Paragraph Learning Task. The paragraph-learning task
this study was to determine whether spaced learning enhances required participants to read aloud two short paragraphs (one
memory of functional tasks in people with TBI. in a massed condition and one in a spaced condition, as
described earlier). Both paragraphs were taken from a news-
paper and contained 5 sentences, 84 words, and 14 elements
Method of information. After the three learning trials, two recall trials
were conducted at immediate recall and then again 30 min
Participants
after initial presentation. Participants were asked to repeat
Ten participants with TBI and 15 HCs without reported the paragraph provided to them using “as close to the same
neurological disabilities participated in this study. Participants words as possible.” The dependent measures for this task
with TBI were included in the study only if positive com- were the total number of elements recalled for each of the
puted tomography or MRI neuroimaging results or a docu- immediate and delayed free-recall trials, ranging from 0 to
mented period of loss of consciousness of ≥24 hr were docu- 14. The dependent variables were the total score for the
mented. Participants were excluded from the study if they immediate and delayed free-recall trials.
544 September/October 2009, Volume 63, Number 5
Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/17/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Route-Learning Task. The route-learning task was To analyze performance on the spacing effect protocol,
designed to simulate the procedure of learning and recalling we analyzed performance on the paragraph and route tasks
a simple set of visual instructions when a new route is by means of a 2 (group: TBI vs. HC) × 2 (condition: massed
learned (e.g., when traveling to a new place). Each of the vs. spaced) × 2 (time: immediate vs. 30 min) repeated-
routes to be learned consisted of 13 different streets and 13 ­measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group as the
directional turns to be memorized. The three learning trials between-groups factor and condition and time as within-
consisted of the examiner visually presenting the route group factors.
(including the route streets and turns) using a red laser
pointer on a map. During the presentation of the route, the
examiner was positioned across a table from the participant. Results
Each presentation lasted 20 s. After three learning trials, a We analyzed cognitive functioning by using a one-way
recall trial was obtained by asking the participant to draw ANOVA to compare performance between TBI and HC
the route on another map without further presentation. An groups. These results are presented in Table 1. The TBI group
identical recall trial was performed after a 30-min delay. For took significantly longer to perform the D–KEFS Trail
every correct turn and street traced, 1 point was awarded. Making Test number–letter switching task than did the HC
The range of possible scores ranged from 0 (not remembering group (F[1,24] = 13.6, p < .01]. On the CVLT, HC partici-
anything) to 26 (accurate recollection of the route). The depen- pants recognized significantly more words than did the TBI
dent variables were the total score for immediate and delayed participants (F[1,24] = 5.5, p < .05). Additionally, participants
free-recall trials. with TBI demonstrated slower speed of processing on the
SDMT than did HC participants (F[1,24] = 4.8, p < .05).
Neuropsychological Testing
TBI can cause cognitive impairments in the areas of atten- Performance of Paragraph-Learning Task
tion (Mathias & Wheaton, 2007), information-processing Figure 1 illustrates the different scores each participant
speed (Madigan, DeLuca, Diamond, Tramontano, & group received in the different learning conditions for the
Averill, 2000), learning and memory (Deshpande, Millis, paragraph-learning task. We observed a significant interac-
Reeder, Fuerst, & Ricker, 1996), executive functions tion between time and condition (F[1, 23] = 5.9, p = .02, η2
(Macdonald, Flashman, & Saykin, 2002), and visuospatial = .20). This interaction indicates that information presented
abilities. Therefore, the battery of neuropsychological tests in a spaced manner was retained significantly better than
administered was geared toward assessing these cognitive information learned in a massed presentation after the 30-
functions. This battery consisted of the Digit Span subtest min delay and even better than recall immediately after ini-
(Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised [WAIS–R]; tial learning. This interaction was observed in both the TBI
Wechsler, 1981) to assess attention; Symbol Digit Modalities and the HC groups. It is important to note, though, that the
Test (SDMT)–Oral Version (Smith, 1982) to assess process-
ing speed; California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; Delis, Table 1. Neuropsychological Test Performance
Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987) to assess learning and mem- (Means ± Standard Deviations)
ory; and three subtests of the Delis–Kaplan Executive TBI HC
Domain Assessed and Test (n = 10) (n = 15) F(1, 24)
Function System (D–KEFS; Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer,
Speed of processing: SDMT   42.8 ± 13.9 56.3 ± 11.2 4.8*
2000)—the Trail Making Test, Verbal Fluency Test (Letter
Learning and memory
Fluency), and Color–Word Interference Test (Inhibition–
  CVLT—Sum of 5 trials   50.2 ± 13.8 54.8 ± 10.5 0.51
Switching)—to assess executive functions.   CVLT—Discriminability   89.8 ±   9.2 96.3 ±   3.7 5.5*
Working memory: Digit Span Total   18.6 ±   7.6 19.3 ±   3.4 0.10
Data Collection and Analysis
Executive functions
Participants were recruited by advertisements distributed at   D–KEFS Trail Making Test: 109.8 ± 42.6 64.5 ± 14.1 13.6**
local support groups and clinics. On initial phone contact,    Number–letter switching
potential participants were screened for participation on the   D–KEFS Verbal Fluency–   34.8 ± 19.3 43.2 ± 11.8 1.3
basis of the inclusion and exclusion criteria discussed earlier.    Letter Fluency

Participants were then scheduled for interview and testing.   D–KEFS Color–Word:   74.4± 18.5 58.4 ± 14.8 3.8
   inhibition and switching
The testing session lasted approximately 3 hr, in which the
Note. TBI = traumatic brain injury; HC = healthy control participants; SDMT =
neuropsychologic battery and the spacing effect protocol were Symbol Digit Modalities Test; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; D–KEFS
administered by Yael Goverover and Juan Carlos Arango- = Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System.
Lasprilla. All participants were paid for their participation. *p < .05. **p < .01.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 545


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/17/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
main effect of condition was not statistically significant (F[1, group and condition (spaced vs. generated) was also not sig-
23] = 3.4, p = .07, η2 = .15). Thus, tasks of paragraph learn- nificant (F[1, 23] = 0.41, p = .52, η2 = .01), suggesting that
ing (learned under the spaced condition) were recalled at a the spaced and massed conditions affected memory abilities
slightly higher rate (mean [M] = 7.85) than tasks presented to an equivalent degree in both participant groups.
in a massed condition (M = 7.1) collapsed across groups and
time. However, this difference was not significant, and the
effect size was small. Additionally, the relative difference Discussion
between the spaced versus massed conditions was equivalent The results of this pilot study are consistent with earlier lit-
across participant groups. The interaction of participant erature on the spacing effect (Hilary et al., 2003) and suggest
group (HC vs. TBI) and condition (spaced vs. massed) was that this learning strategy can play a role in rehabilitation
not significant and demonstrated a small effect size (F [1, 23] even in chronic-stage post-TBI. This study’s findings sup-
= 0.1, p = .80, η2 = .003). Thus, both HC and TBI partici- port the idea that treatment focusing on improving acquisi-
pants showed a similar pattern of learning whereby they tion may significantly increase recall of learned information
benefited from the spaced condition as compared with the and extend this support to functional tasks.
massed condition when learning the paragraph. This study demonstrated two primary findings. First,
results showed that the spacing effect can be successfully
Following a Route on a Map applied to a functional task such as paragraph and route learn-
Performance on the route-learning task is presented in Figure ing in TBI. Second, when participants acquired skills (reading
2. We observed a significant interaction between time and information from a newspaper or following a route on a
condition (F[1, 23] = 7.9, p = .01, η² = .25). This interaction map), delayed recall of this information was better when the
demonstrates that participants in both the TBI and the HC information was originally learned under spaced conditions.
groups did worse in the immediate recall when they learned the Initial performance was better, however, when the tasks were
route in a spaced manner. However, after a 30-min delay, routes learned in massed practice compared with spaced practice.
on the map that were learned in the spaced condition were These results are consistent with those of previous research.
remembered significantly better than the routes learned in the Specifically, Rohrer and Pashler (2007) concluded that the
massed condition. Thus, the pattern of results was reversed at benefits of the spacing effect generally seem to get bigger, not
the 30-min recall. There was no significant main effect of condi- smaller, when one examines longer-term retention.
tions (i.e., spaced and massed learning) for the map-learning In terms of long-term retention, the observed benefit of
task across time and groups (space, M = 16.9; mass, M = 16.9; spaced learning trials is of major importance because delayed-
condition main effect, F[1, 23] = .00, p = .98, η² = .01). recall trials are presumed to be a better reflection of recall of
The TBI and HC groups did not differ significantly in what was learned (transfer test; Hall & Magill, 1995). Spaced
the mean number of items recalled. The main effect of group practice trials clearly have a significant, long-term positive
was not significant (F[1, 23] = 0.34, p = .56, η² = .009). effect on learning and memory, and this effect is critical to
Similar to the paragraph tasks, the interaction of participant the conduct of clinical rehabilitation. These results have

Figure 1. Paragraph tasks: Recall scores for spaced vs. massed Figure 2. Map task: Recall scores for spaced vs. massed
conditions. conditions.
Note. HC = healthy control participants; TBI = traumatic brain injury. Note. HC = healthy control participants; TBI = traumatic brain injury.

546 September/October 2009, Volume 63, Number 5


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/17/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
widespread implications for instruction at many levels. Acknowledgments
Although tradition dictates that therapists present multiple,
This research was supported by National Multiple Sclerosis
different sets of instructions or exercises during each therapy
Society Grant RG3837A1/T and by the Henry H. Kessler
session, these findings show that clients might be far better
Foundation and was presented in part at the annual meet-
served if material was distributed sporadically across many
ing of the International Neuropsychological Society,
therapy sessions. The findings of the long-term benefit of
February 2005, in St. Louis, Missouri, and at the Inter­
learning through spaced learning trials are critical to the
national Neuro­psy­chological Society Congress in Dublin,
rehabilitation of people with TBI who have deficits in acquir-
Ireland, July 2005.
ing new information because it provides evidence for long-
term learning of functional information.

Clinical Implications
References
Anderson, C. V., Bigler, E. D., & Blatter, D. D. (1995). Frontal
This pilot study provides Class II evidence for the use of lobe lesions, diffuse damage, and neuropsychological func-
spaced learning trials in learning and memory interventions tioning in traumatic brain-injured patients. Journal of Clinical
(see Cicerone et al., 2000, 2005). Thus, it provides fair evi- and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 900–908.
dence to support a recommendation as to whether treatment Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, L. E., Bahrick, A. S., & Bahrick, P. E.
may be effective. Because of this, the information gained (1993). Maintenance of foreign-language vocabulary and the
spacing effect. Psychological Science, 4, 316–321.
from this pilot study provides the opportunity to proceed to Bahrick, H. P., & Phelps, E. (1987). Retention of Spanish vocab-
the next step by designing treatment plans that are based on ulary over eight years. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
individualized goals but using learning techniques that may Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 344–349.
improve long-term retention. In sum, spaced learning trials Cahill, A., & Toppino, T. C. (1993). Young children’s recogni-
can be viewed both theoretically and practically as a learning tion as a function of the spacing of repetitions and the type
strategy that can be applied as a compensatory technique of study and test stimuli. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,
31, 481–484.
with a variety of tasks with people with TBI. Camp, C. J., & Foss, J. W. (1997). Designing ecologically valid
memory interventions for persons with dementia. In D. G.
Limitations of the Study
Payne & F. D. Conrad (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and
This study’s results should be interpreted with the following applications (pp. 311–325). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
caveats. First, the small sample studied may not be fully rep- Cermak, L. S., Verfaellie, M., Lanzoni, S., Mather, M., & Chase,
resentative of the population of people with TBI. Second, K. A. (1996). Effect of spaced repetitions on amnesia patients’
recall and recognition performance. Neuropsychology, 10,
severity level was not accounted for, and it is not clear how 219–227.
injury severity may affect the usefulness of spacing effect Challis, B. H. (1993). Spacing effect on cued-memory tests depend
techniques. Third, this study has demonstrated the usefulness upon level of processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
of the encoding strategy in improving two functional activi- Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 389–396.
ties (route learning and reading a paragraph); however, it is Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Kalmar, K., Langenbahn, D. M.,
not clear how useful the techniques might be when applied Malec, J. F., Bergquist, T. F., et al. (2000). Evidence-based
cognitive rehabilitation: Recommendations for clinical prac-
to other daily tasks (e.g., vocational tasks, driving). Fourth, tice. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 81,
although the spacing effect improved recall immediately and 1596–1615.
at 30 min after initial presentation, it is not clear whether Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Malec, J. F., Langenbahn, D.
these gains are maintained over a longer follow-up period. M., Felicetti, T., Kneipp, S., et al. (2005). Evidence-based
cognitive rehabilitation: Updated review of the literature
Future Research from 1998 through 2002. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Rehabilitation, 86, 1681–1692.
Replication of these results in larger, more diverse populations Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2000). Delis–Kaplan
of people with TBI with differing levels of cognitive impair- Executive Function System. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
ment is needed to ensure adequate generalization of the find- Corporation.
ings. Further investigation of the usefulness of this technique Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (1987).
on tasks completed in the patient’s natural environment, California Verbal Learning Test: Research edition (Version 1).
instead of in a research laboratory setting, would help to deter- San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
DeLuca, J., Schultheis, M. T., Madigan, N. K., Christodoulou,
mine the spacing effect’s ecological validity. It would also be C., & Averill, A. (2000). Acquisition versus retrieval deficits
informative to study the potential benefits of the spacing effect in traumatic brain injury: Implications for memory rehabili-
on longer-term recall by including 1-week, 3-month, or 6- tation. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 81,
month follow-ups, or all of these, in the study design.  s 1327–1333.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 547


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/17/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Dempster, F. N. (1987). Effects of variable encoding and spaced Jensen, T. D., & Freund, J. S. (1981). Persistence of the spacing
presentations on vocabulary learning. Journal of Educational effect in incidental free recall: The effect of external list com-
Psychology, 79, 162–170. parisons and intertask correlations. Bulletin of the Psychonomic
Dempster, F. N. (1988). The spacing effect: A case study in fail- Society, 18, 183–186.
ure to apply the results of psychological research. American Kahana, M. J., & Howard, M. W. (2005). Spacing and lag effects
Psychologist, 43, 627–634. in free recall of pure lists. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,
Dempster, F. N. (1989). Spacing effects and their implications 12, 159–164.
for theory and practice. Educational Psychology Review, 1, Madigan, N. K., DeLuca, J., Diamond, B. J., Tramontano, G.,
309–330. & Averill, A. (2000). Speed of information processing in
Deshpande, S., Millis, S., Reeder, K., Fuerst, D., & Ricker, traumatic brain injury: Modality-specific factors. Journal of
J. (1996). Verbal learning subtypes in traumatic brain Head Trauma and Rehabilitation, 15, 943–956.
injury: A replication. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Mathias, J., & Wheaton, P. (2007). Changes in attention and
Neuropsychology, 18, 836–842. information-processing speed following severe traumatic brain
Dikmen, S. S., Machamer, J. E., Winn, H. R., & Temkin, N. injury: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 21, 212–223.
R. (1995). Neuropsychological outcome at 1-year post head McDonald, B. C., Flashman, L. A., & Saykin, A. J. (2002).
injury. Neuropsychology, 9, 80–90. Executive dysfunction following traumatic brain injury: Neural
Drake, A. I., Gray, N., Yoder, S., Pramuka, M., & Llewelly, M. substrates and treatment strategies. NeuroRehabilitation, 17,
(2000). Factors predicting return to work following mild 333–344.
traumatic brain injury: A discriminant analysis. Journal of Melton, A. W. (1970). The situation with respect to the spacing
Head Trauma and Rehabilitation, 15, 1103–1112. of repetitions and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Glenberg, A. M. (1976). Monotonic and nonmonotonic lag Verbal Behavior, 9, 596–606.
effects in paired-associate and recognition memory paradigms. Olver, J. H., Ponsford, J. L., & Curran, C. A. (1996). Outcome
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 1–16. following traumatic brain injury: A comparison between 2
Glenberg, A. M. (1977). Influences of retrieval processes on the and 5 years after injury. Brain Injury, 10, 841–848.
spacing effect in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Rea, C. P., & Modigliani, V. (1985). The effect of expanded versus
Human Learning and Memory, 3, 282–294. massed practice on the retention of multiplication facts and
Glenberg, A. M. (1979). Component-levels theory of the effects spelling lists. Human Learning, 4, 11–18.
of spacing of repetitions on recall and recognition. Memory Rohrer, D., & Pashler, H. (2007). Increasing retention with-
and Cognition, 7, 95–112. out increasing study time. Current Directions in Psychological
Glenberg, A. M., & Lehmann, T. S. (1980). Spacing repetitions Science, 16, 183–186.
over 1 week. Memory and Cognition, 8, 528–538. Rosenthal, M., & Ricker, J. H. (2000). Traumatic brain injury.
Glover, J. A., & Corkill, A. J. (1987). Influence of paraphrased In R. Frank & T. Eliott (Eds.), Handbook of rehabilita-
repetitions on the spacing effect. Journal of Educational tion psychology (pp. 49–74). Washington, DC: American
Psychology, 79, 198–199. Psychological Association.
Goldstein, F. C., & Levin, H. S. (2001). Cognitive outcome after Shaughnessy, J. J., Zimmerman, J., & Underwood, B. J. (1972).
mild and moderate traumatic brain injury in older adults. Further evidence on the MP–DP effect in free-recall learning.
Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section A: Journal Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 1–12.
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 739–753. Smith, A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test manual. Los
Goverover, Y., Hillary, F. G., Chiaravalloti, N., Arango-Lasprilla, Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
J. C., & DeLuca, J. (2008). A functional application of the Sosin, D. M., Sniezek, J. E., & Thurman, D. J. (1996). Incidence
spacing effect to improve learning and memory in persons of mild and moderate brain injury in the United States, 1991.
with multiple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Brain Injury, 10, 47–54.
Neuropsychology. Advance online publication. Retrieved Toppino, T. C. (1991). The spacing effect in young children.
February 28, 2009. doi: 10.1080/13803390802287042 Memory and Cognition, 19, 159–167.
Hall, K., & Magill, R. (1995). Variability of practice and con- Toppino, T. C. (1993). The spacing effect in preschool children.
textual interference in motor skill learning. Journal of Motor Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 27–30.
Behavior, 27, 299–309. Underwood, B. J. (1970). A breakdown of the total-time law in
Hillary, F. G., Schultheis, M. T., Challis, B. H., Millis, S. R., free-recall learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Carnevale, G. J., Galshi, T., et al. (2003). Spacing of repeti- Behavior, 9, 573–580.
tions improves learning and memory after moderate and severe van Zomeren, A. H., & van den Burg, W. (1985). Residual com-
TBI. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, plaints of patients two years after severe head injury. Journal
25, 49–58. of Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 48, 21–28.
Hintzman, D. (1974). Theoretical implications of the spacing Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised
effect. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The manual. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Loyola Symposium (pp. 77–100). New York: Erlbaum. Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). WRAT–3: Wide Range Achievement Test–3
Janiszewski, C., Noel, H., & Sawyer, A. G. (2003). A meta- administration manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range.
analysis of the spacing effect in verbal learning: Implications Zechmeister, E. B., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (1980). When you know
for research on advertising repetition and consumer memory. that you know and when you think that you know but you
Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 138–149. don’t. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 41–44.

548 September/October 2009, Volume 63, Number 5


Downloaded from http://ajot.aota.org on 02/17/2020 Terms of use: http://AOTA.org/terms

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen