Sie sind auf Seite 1von 3

INS IGHTS | P E R S P E C T I V E S

NEUROBIOLOGY

Cross-modal sensory transfer: Bumble bees do it


Stored sensory input permits two sensory channels to exchange and compare information

By Gerhard von der Emde1 and content. This means that both senses provide or only vision (in light, but where they could
Theresa Burt de Perera2 information about the same characteristic of not touch the objects). After training, the
an object, such as its shape or surface struc- bees discriminated between the same objects

A
nimal sensory systems acquire in- ture. In addition, the sensory inputs must using only the information from the other
formation about the physical world be encoded in a way that allows temporally sensory modality. This observation suggests
by transforming external stimuli disjointed information from two senses to that the brain encoded the sensory input in
into signals that can be read and be identified as identical, even though these a way that allowed the two sensory channels
interpreted by the nervous system. senses rely on different physical stimuli. to exchange information and to compare and
Animals possess several senses that Thus, characteristic object features are stored match object-related inputs.
provide separate streams of information in a neuronal representation that can be ac- The mechanisms by which bees accomplish
based on different physical stimuli. How- cessed by multiple senses. Up to now, spon- this task have yet to be elucidated. Perhaps
ever, objects and environments contain taneous cross-modal object recognition has the bee’s brain stores a representation of the

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on February 21, 2020


inherently multimodal information. Thus, been described only in mammalian species object in a way that allows it to be accessed
neurobiologists have sought to define the (humans, apes, monkeys, dolphins, and rats) and understood across different sensory sys-
mechanisms by which information is pro- (6–8) and in one aquatic creature, the weakly tems. The sensory information would need to
cessed when it is received by different senses electric elephantnose fish (9). be integrated to form a representation that is
(1). Cross-modal recognition—the ability to Solvi et al. trained bumble bees to discrim- independent of the sensory modality through
transfer information across senses, irre- inate two differently shaped objects (cubes which the information was introduced (that
spective of the sense that first accessed that and spheres) using only touch (in darkness) is, both senses use a matched encoding for-
information—is a highly complex mat). This would enable bees to rec-
cognitive capacity that was thought ognize objects cross-modally without
to be limited to vertebrates. Now, on General scheme of cross-modal transfer any previous experience, and cross-
page 910 of this issue, Solvi et al. (2) In both vertebrates and invertebrates, sense organs respond to physical modal object recognition would not
show that bumble bees are capable stimuli and transfer information to specific monomodal sensory nuclei require training. Alternatively, infor-
of performing the same task. in the brain (dark yellow, vision; light yellow, touch). These nuclei then mation that originates from multiple
Humans execute spontaneous communicate, in a reciprocal manner, with multimodal sensory nuclei senses might initially be unmatched
cross-modal recognition fairly eas- in higher brain centers. In principle, cross-modal sensory transfer can in format, and cross-modal object
ily (3). For example, people often occur at any stage after the first monomodal sensory nuclei. recognition could depend on sensory
rummage blindly through pockets experience and learning. In this case,
to find, by touch, a set of keys that Higher centers bees might have learned to associate
they first encountered visually. Cross- Multimodal visual and tactile inputs of basic fea-
modal recognition is also useful for tures common in other environmen-
increasing the flexibility of object- tal objects when exposed to these
recognition systems. The finding by features in the past. For example, a
Solvi et al. illustrates that tiny inver- bee might have learned to associate a
tebrates, with brain structures that visual and a tactile image of a curved
differ greatly from those of verte- edge or a corner. Subsequently, these
brates, also can experience an object Secondary
associations would generalize to new
with one sensory modality and later sensory nuclei objects and new situations.
recognize that same object with a dif- Multimodal Whatever the mechanism, cross-
ferent sensory modality. modal object recognition requires
Simple forms of cross-modal infor- storage (in the brain) of a representa-
mation transfer, which are based on tion of the object’s features that can
direct associations between two spe- be accessed by different senses. This
Primary
cific stimuli, have been observed in sensory nuclei implies that an object can be recog-
various animal groups, including in- Monomodal nized with a sense through which it
sects (4, 5). However, cross-modal ob- had never before been experienced.
ject recognition requires additional Solvi et al. show that this capability
and more complex conditions—in exists in an insect brain, which con-
GRAPHIC: V. ALTOUNIAN/SCIENCE

particular, that the two senses pro- tains a small fraction of the numbers
vide information that is matched in Sense 1 Sense 2
of neurons in vertebrate brains (10).
(e.g., vision) (e.g., touch) How the organization of sensory
1
Institute of Zoology, University of Bonn, systems achieves cross-modal object
Bonn, Germany. 2Department of Zoology, recognition remains unanswered.
University of Oxford, Oxford, UK.
Email: vonderemde@uni-bonn.de; Sense organs respond to the physi-
theresa.burt@sjc.ox.ac.uk cal stimuli of the outside world and

850 21 FEBRUARY 2020 • VOL 367 ISSUE 6480 sciencemag.org SCIENCE

Published by AAAS
transduce them into action potentials en-
coding the specific stimulus parameters.
Connections are then made with nerve cells
in dedicated sensory nuclei in the brain,
which in turn project to sensory nuclei in
higher brain centers for further processing.
These nuclei also project back to lower pro-
cessing centers, forming reciprocal circuits
and adding to the complexity (see the figure).
For multisensory integration and cross-
modal transfer to occur, information from
the different sensory modalities that encode
characteristics of the same object must come
together, eventually forming a multisensory
representation of the object. In most cases,
scientists still do not know exactly where in
the sensory pathway this occurs, particularly
in bees and fish, which do not possess spe-
cialized cortical structures. One possibility is
AGING
that higher brain structures, such as the cor-

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on February 21, 2020


tex in mammals or the mushroom bodies and
central complex in bees, form the anatomical
substrate for multisensory representations A time to grow
and a time to pause
(6, 11). Alternatively, because cross-talk be-
tween the senses also occurs at lower levels
(e.g., in the midbrain and diencephalon) (1,
12, 13) and because sensory nuclei on differ- Mechanisms of programmed arrest
ent levels are connected in a reciprocal man-
ner, a distributed representation of object protect animals from the passage of time
properties might occur in several intercon-
nected multisensory nuclei.
The fact that bees can achieve cross-modal
object recognition might have implications By Marc Van Gilst An African turquoise killifish embryo developmentally
for how we think about cognition in general. suspended in the state of diapause. This vertebrate

S
In humans, scientists assume that this ability easonal or environmental pressures model could improve the understanding of aging.
involves mental imagery (14) based on inter- are sometimes best dealt with by
nal representations in higher brain centers. putting growth and reproduction on facultative diapause, which is induced in
Thus, some researchers have argued that hold. Many animals have evolved response to environmental challenges such
this task relies on awareness. Whether that mechanisms for reversibly arresting as starvation or dehydration, or an obligate
is also the case in bumble bees is a matter development at discrete developmen- diapause, which is generally brought about
of debate, as simpler explanations are pos- tal stages, so that the arrested embryo or by seasonal pressures such as a harsh win-
sible. Whatever the underlying mechanism, larva can wait for more favorable conditions ter (3). Although diapause involves an ar-
the newly found ability of bumble bees to in which to resume development, grow, and rest of development, the arrested state is
perform cross-modal recognition shows that, reproduce. One common form of develop- not static. Indeed, diapause usually involves
like humans, they possess a sensory integra- mental arrest is diapause, which generally considerable physiological changes that
tion system that allows them to form a com- constitutes a genetically programmed ar- help to prepare the animal to survive the
plex representation of their world. j rest at a discrete point in development, forthcoming environmental challenges (4).
most often in an embryonic, larval, or pupal These physiological changes also protect
REFERENCES AND NOTES
stage (1). Diapause also involves physiologi- arrested animals, allowing them to be pre-
1. B. E. Stein et al., J. Neurosci. 40, 3 (2020).
2. C. Solvi, S. Gutierrez Al-Khudhairy, L. Chittka, Science cal changes that protect the arrested animal served for long periods of time until condi-
367, 910 (2020). from aging. On page 870 of this issue, Hu et tions are more favorable for development
3. M. O. Ernst, M. S. Banks, Nature 415, 429 (2002). al. (2) identify some of the mechanisms that and reproduction.
4. J. Guo, A. Guo, Science 309, 307 (2005).
5. L. Proops et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 947 maintain embryonic diapause in an emerg- In the simplest sense, aging is considered
(2009). ing model of vertebrate aging, the African the inevitable wear and tear brought on by
6. B. D. Winters, J. M. Reid, J. Neurosci. 30, 6253 (2010). turquoise killifish. These findings might the passage of time. The basic idea is that
7. L. M. Herman et al., J. Comp. Psychol. 112, 292 (1998).
8. A. Cowey, L. Weiskrantz, Neuropsychologia 13, 117 provide insight into the mysteries of aging the more time passes, the more an animal
(1975). and longevity. ages and the more it progresses toward its
9. S. Schumacher et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, Examples of diapause are found through- ultimate demise. This simplistic perspective
7638 (2016).
10. M. Giurfa, Trends Neurosci. 36, 285 (2013).
out the animal kingdom and have been well is somewhat fatalistic and defines time as
characterized in insects, fish, and mam- the ultimate enemy of youth. However, it has
PHOTO: HU ET AL. (2)

11. F. Peng, L. Chittka, Curr. Biol. 27, 224 (2017).


12. M. Zeymer et al., Front. Neuroanat. 12, 79 (2018). mals (1). Diapause may take the form of a been established in many animals that ag-
13. H. F. Sperdin et al., Front. Neurosci. 4, 9 (2010). ing is also heavily influenced by genetic and
14. B. Nanay, Cortex 105, 125 (2018).
Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, University physiological programs, such that aging may
10.1126/science.aba8519 of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA. Email: vangilst@uw.edu not necessarily be an inevitable consequence

SCIENCE sciencemag.org 21 FEBRUARY 2020 • VOL 367 ISSUE 6480 851


Published by AAAS
Cross-modal sensory transfer: Bumble bees do it
Gerhard von der Emde and Theresa Burt de Perera

Science 367 (6480), 850-851.


DOI: 10.1126/science.aba8519

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on February 21, 2020


ARTICLE TOOLS http://science.sciencemag.org/content/367/6480/850

RELATED http://science.sciencemag.org/content/sci/367/6480/910.full
CONTENT

REFERENCES This article cites 14 articles, 6 of which you can access for free
http://science.sciencemag.org/content/367/6480/850#BIBL

PERMISSIONS http://www.sciencemag.org/help/reprints-and-permissions

Use of this article is subject to the Terms of Service

Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published by the American Association for the Advancement of
Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. The title Science is a registered trademark of AAAS.
Copyright © 2020 The Authors, some rights reserved; exclusive licensee American Association for the Advancement of
Science. No claim to original U.S. Government Works

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen