Sie sind auf Seite 1von 4

216 VOLUME 9, NUMBER 6, DECEMBER 2000

STP is a cornerstone of the ratio-


Nonconsequential Reasoning and Its nal theory of choice, and it also
Consequences holds in decision models that im-
pose less stringent criteria of ratio-
Anthony Bastardi and Eldar Shafir1 nality. It is an important implica-
tion of the consequentialist view of
Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey
rational choice, according to which
decisions are determined by an as-
sessment of the potential conse-
quences and their likelihood. STP
Abstract normative and prescriptive im- captures a fundamental intuition of
According to consequential- plications. what it means for a decision to be
ism, which underlies the rational determined by the anticipated con-
theory of choice, decisions Keywords sequences: If the consequences will
should be determined by an as- decision making; uncertainty; not be sufficiently altered by an un-
sessment of the potential conse- consequentialism; information certain event, then this event—
quences. People, however, do pursuit; self-perception however interesting—is irrelevant
not always consider the rel- to the decision.
evance of missing information Decisions are often made in the
in a consequentialist manner. As face of uncertainty. Although infor-
a result, they sometimes pursue mation that will resolve the uncer- NONCONSEQUENTIAL
noninstrumental information— tainty can sometimes be obtained, REASONING
information that may appear decision makers must determine
relevant but ought not alter the whether such information is worth Consequential reasoning and
decision. Having pursued such pursuing. Imagine that you are con- STP are intuitively compelling, yet
information, people miscon- sidering adding a new CD player to people’s decisions do not always
strue it as instrumental for the your stereo system and have just conform to these normative pre-
decision and proceed to make come upon an attractive player on scriptions. One study, for example,
choices they would not other- sale. However, your amplifier just presented people with hypothetical
wise have made. This pattern is broke, and you are uncertain wheth- scenarios offering an attractive va-
observed in the context of con- er the warranty will cover repairs. cation package to Hawaii following
sumer, medical, and negotiation Should you decide about purchasing an exam. Many people chose to
decisions. In one scenario, for the CD player, or should you briefly purchase the vacation both when
example, participants made a postpone the decision until you re- they thought that they had passed
hypothetical decision about solve the uncertainty regarding the the exam and when they thought
whether to purchase a CD amplifier warranty? There is no that they had failed. However, in
player. Those who chose to need to pursue information about the disjunctive case, when they did
postpone and then found out the warranty if this information is not know whether they had passed
about another component’s re- not instrumental—that is, if it will or failed, many of these same
pair cost were less likely to buy not alter your decision. For instance, people decided to postpone buying
the CD player than those who you may realize that you intend to the vacation (Tversky & Shafir,
knew about the required repair buy the CD player whether or not 1992). This is known as a disjunc-
cost from the start. Because the you must pay for the amplifier re- tion effect. When people passed the
initial pursuit and ensuing use pairs. In this case, information about exam, they presumably saw the va-
of obtained information appear the warranty is noninstrumental, cation as a time of celebration;
exceedingly reasonable, such and you should decide to buy the when they failed, the vacation be-
decision patterns may be diffi- CD player regardless of this infor- came a consolation and time of re-
cult to learn to avoid, yielding mation. More generally, according covery. When they did not know
decisions that are influenced by to what is known as the sure-thing whether they had passed or failed,
contextual nuances that ought principle (STP), if one prefers x to y however, people lacked a clear ra-
not matter. Further research given any possible state of the tionale for going and, as a result,
may explore cognitive and mo- world, then one should prefer x to y preferred to learn the outcome be-
tivational factors in nonconse- even when the exact state of the fore deciding to go.
quential reasoning and their world is not known (Savage, 1954, p. Another disjunction effect was
21). documented among participants

Published by Blackwell Publishers Inc.


Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 217

who played a version of the Pris- ranty. The data were as follows. In people to endow it with instrumen-
oner’s Dilemma game, in which the simple condition, in which par- tal value. For example, participants
they accumulated points that were ticipants knew they had to pay for who chose to wait in the uncertain
then exchanged for real monetary the repair, 91% chose to buy the condition of the CD player study
payoffs (Shafir & Tversky, 1992). CD player. (Predictably, in a sepa- were then asked to make a decision
On each encounter with a new rate control condition, even more after the uncertainty regarding the
partner, a player had to choose were disposed to buy it when they $90 repair was resolved (Bastardi &
whether to cooperate or defect. (In a did not have to pay for repairs.) Shafir, 1998). Like those in the
Prisoner’s Dilemma game, each This suggests that most people simple condition, the participants
player is better off defecting than would buy the CD player whether who chose to wait were then told
cooperating no matter what the or not they had to pay for the re- the amplifier warranty had expired
other does, yet if both defect they pairs. Nevertheless, in the uncer- and that they would have to pay
do less well than if they had both tain condition, in which the war- the $90. Recall that in the simple
cooperated.) Occasionally, the ranty status was unknown, only condition, we observed an un-
partner’s chosen strategy was di- 26% chose to buy the CD player equivocal choice to buy the CD
vulged before the participant had outright, and a full 69% of partici- player when the $90 repair was
to make his or her own decision. pants chose to wait to find out known about from the start. Nev-
Contrary to STP, many participants about the $90 repair before making ertheless, among participants who
defected when they knew their the decision (the remaining 5% re- waited and then found out about
partner’s choice—be it cooperation jected the player outright). the $90 repair, the majority chose to
or defection—but cooperated when Although information about the forgo buying the CD player. Ulti-
the partner’s choice was not $90 repair was noninstrumental for mately, only 55% chose to buy the
known. Apparently, the impulse to most participants, more than two CD player in the uncertain condi-
cooperate was greater when the thirds were compelled to pursue tion (including those who first
partner’s choice remained uncer- this information in the uncertain waited and those who did not),
tain than when it was known and condition. Perhaps they intended compared with 91% in the simple
defection was seen to yield a greater to buy the CD player if there would condition.2
number of points. The presence of be no repair costs to pay, but were The pursuit of information ap-
uncertainty can obscure one’s pref- less sure of their preference if they parently affected choice in ways
erences, which tend to crystallize would have to pay for repair. that the information would not
once the uncertainty is resolved. Rather than resolving this ques- have had it simply been known
Another way in which noncon- tion, these respondents may have from the start. The resolution of
sequential reasoning influences chosen to postpone the decision in uncertainty can have straightfor-
choice is by leading people to pur- hopes that things would become ward implications for those who
sue information that ought to clearer. People prefer to have com- await it. Finding out the repair is
prove noninstrumental. In one pelling rationales for their choices free constitutes good news and
study (Bastardi & Shafir, 1998), we (Shafir, Simonson, & Tversky, 1993), supports a decision to purchase the
asked participants to make a hypo- and the motivation for buying the CD player; in contrast, finding out
thetical choice about whether to CD player would likely feel clearer that one must pay $90 rather than
buy a new CD player on sale. In after the repair status was resolved nothing is bad news and suggests a
addition, they were to assume that than while it was unknown. reason not to buy the CD player
their amplifier just broke and Individuals and organizations (otherwise, why would one have
needed repairs. In the simple condi- spend a great deal of resources in waited?). People tend to form pref-
tion, participants were told the the pursuit of information. Such erences consistent with having cho-
warranty had expired and they pursuit can involve delays and op- sen to wait, and are thus occasion-
would have to pay $90 for the am- portunity costs. Are there any ally led to make decisions they
plifier repairs. In the uncertain con- other reasons to avoid the pursuit would not otherwise have made.
dition, participants were told they of noninstrumental information? People, we suggest, are often un-
would not know until the follow- aware of pursuing noninstrumen-
ing day whether the warranty CONSEQUENCES OF tal information. Instead, they mis-
would cover the $90 repair. These NONCONSEQUENTIAL construe such pursuit as an
participants were then given the REASONING indication that the information is
option to postpone their decision likely to prove instrumental. This
about purchasing the CD player As it turns out, the very act of conclusion is further supported by
until they found out about the war- pursuing information may lead a study in which participants con-

Copyright © 2000 American Psychological Society


Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
218 VOLUME 9, NUMBER 6, DECEMBER 2000

fronted the same decision about they would be willing to be tested tion if the group protested. When
whether to purchase a CD player in for compatibility. Note that a ma- these students learned that there
the face of uncertain repair costs, jority of those who knew they were would be no disciplinary action
but were provided with a nonin- compatible chose not to donate. (and, thus, that the cost of continu-
strumental motivation for postpon- Thus, a majority would presum- ing the struggle would be low), the
ing the decision—they would de- ably have no reason to test for com- majority voted to protest, reversing
lay because “the stores are already patibility. Nonetheless, faced with the outcome of a comparable vote
closed today.” Most participants a difficult decision (which perhaps in the simple condition.
chose to postpone but were not led they hoped to avoid if found in-
to misconstrue the uncertainty compatible), most nurses (69%)
about the repair as instrumental for chose to undergo testing. When
SELF-PERCEPTION,
the decision. Once they found out these nurses obtained their test re-
MISCONSTRUAL, AND
they would have to pay for the $90 sults (which indicated compatibil-
SELF-ERASING
repair, most of these respondents ity), they were asked whether they
VIOLATIONS
chose to buy the CD player, just as would be willing to donate, and a
they would have had they known great majority (93%) said they were
about the repair costs from the willing. In fact, overall stated will- When internal attitudes are un-
start. ingness to donate increased from clear, people construct or infer
44% when the nurses knew they their attitudes partly on the basis of
were compatible to 65% when they external cues, including their own
could elect to be tested and then behavior (for more on self-
RECENT APPLICATIONS
found out they were compatible. perception, see Bem, 1972). This of-
Similar effects using hypothetical ten leads them to construct prefer-
People typically assume that the scenarios have now been docu- ences that are consistent with
more information they have, the mented among surgeons specializ- earlier decisions. However, people
better. Psychologically, however, ing in prostate cancer, physicians can be unaware of the processes by
information sought may not be the retired from general practice, and which they reached those decisions
same as information available from college students responding as (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). As a re-
the start. The pursuit of informa- medical patients. sult, they are apt to misconstrue
tion may lead it to have greater In situations of dispute, protest- earlier decision behaviors and then
weight in ensuing decisions. Im- ing groups often have to decide form preferences consistent with
portant decisions—especially those whether to accept a compromise such misconstrual, thus making
that affect other people or for offer or to continue their struggle choices they would not otherwise
which one feels accountable—may in hopes of getting a better deal. In make. The tendency for a later de-
exacerbate both the tendency to such cases, there can be uncertainty cision to be made in a manner con-
pursue missing information and about the prospects of getting a sistent with a misconstrued earlier
the reliance on such information better deal or even about the costs behavior has been observed in
once obtained. Recent research has of prolonged dispute. In one study studies involving compliance
explored the impact of noninstru- (Bastardi & Young, 1999), students (pressure to comply with a request
mental pursuit in the contexts of responded to a hypothetical sce- leads people to alter an ensuing
medical decisions and dispute nario based on a real ongoing judgment), overjustification (a re-
resolution. dispute between the campus book- ward alters people’s perceived
In a study involving medical de- store and a student group protest- motivation for an activity), and be-
cisions (Redelmeier, Shafir, & ing unfair prices. Respondents took havioral prediction (prior mispre-
Aujla, 2000), experienced nurses the role of the latter group. In the diction shapes subsequent behav-
were surveyed regarding whether simple condition, the majority voted ior). (See Bastardi & Shafir, 1998,
they would donate a kidney to a to accept a compromise rather than for further discussion and ex-
hypothetical elderly relative with protest, though it was made clear amples.)
renal failure. Half the nurses were that the university would take no A noteworthy feature of these
to assume that they were compat- disciplinary action if the group decision patterns is that they ap-
ible donors; of these, 44% ex- protested. In the uncertain condi- pear exceedingly reasonable. Early
pressed willingness to donate and tion, however, the majority voted errors often alter ensuing behavior,
the rest were unwilling. The other to postpone the group’s decision in rendering the errors self-erasing
nurses were told that their status order to find out whether the uni- and, consequently, not easy to
was unknown and asked whether versity would take disciplinary ac- learn to avoid (Sherman, 1980).

Published by Blackwell Publishers Inc.


Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016
CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE 219

What is more sensible than to pur- seems to be the most “pure.” Occa- Notes
sue missing information and then sionally, however, the initial ab-
use it once it is available? People sence and subsequent pursuit of a
1. Address correspondence to Eldar
initially pursue noninstrumental piece of information may give it the Shafir, Department of Psychology,
information, but then proceed to attention it deserves and otherwise Green Hall, Princeton University,
make choices that endow the ob- might fail to receive. Further re- Princeton, NJ 08544; e-mail: shafir@
tained information with instru- search might explore the condi- princeton.edu.
2. Although this was a hypothetical
mental value, thereby “erasing” tions under which information is
decision, similar patterns have been
the initial mistake. At no point accorded optimal weight. observed with decisions involving ac-
need the decision maker suspect a Contrary to the classical analy- tual payoffs, including a raffle for a ste-
failure of consequential reasoning sis, people tend to approach deci- reo cassette player and a decision to
or the violation of a compelling sion problems not with clearly es- accept or reject a monetary offer in an
Ultimatum Game (see Bastardi &
principle of choice. tablished preferences, but rather
Shafir, 1998).
with the need to construct their
preference in the context of making
FUTURE DIRECTIONS a decision. In these contexts, simple
principles that would be endorsed References
AND CONCLUDING
REMARKS upon reflection can be violated at
the moment of decision. The mis- Bastardi, A., & Shafir, E. (1998). On the pursuit and
misuse of useless information. Journal of Per-
Possible motivational influences construal of such violations can sonality and Social Psychology, 75, 19–32.
contributing to the decision pat- distort people’s perception of past Bastardi, A., & Young, M. (1999). The pursuit of
missing information: Implications for dispute reso-
terns we have described await fur- preferences and alter the prefer- lution. Unpublished manuscript, Princeton
ther investigation. People may be ences they express next. Better un- University, Princeton, NJ.
Bem, D.J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L.
led to pursue noninstrumental in- derstanding of such patterns may Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
formation out of a desire to be thor- improve the decisions people psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York: Aca-
demic Press.
ough, or to appear responsible. make. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance.
Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
They may then experience cognitive Nisbett, R.E., & Wilson, T.D. (1977). Telling more
dissonance (Festinger, 1957)—that Recommended Reading than we can know: Verbal reports on mental
processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.
is, an uncomfortable tension due to Redelmeier, D., Shafir, E., & Aujla, P. (2000). On
a perceived discrepancy between Bastardi, A., & Shafir, E. (1998). See
the pursuit of unnecessary information. Unpub-
References) lished manuscript, University of Toronto, To-
their actual preference and that im- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). ronto, Ontario, Canada.
plied by the information obtained. (in press). Choices, values, and Savage, L.J. (1954). The foundations of statistics. New
York: John Wiley & Sons.
The need to reduce such dissonance frames. New York: Cambridge
Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Rea-
may alter the preferences expressed. University Press. son-based choice. Cognition, 49, 11–36.
Whenever discrepant prefer- Shafir, E., & Tversky, A. (1992). (See Shafir, E., & Tversky, A. (1992). Thinking through
References) uncertainty: Nonconsequential reasoning and
ences are observed, a question Sherman, S.J. (1980). (See Refer- choice. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 449–474.
Sherman, S.J. (1980). On the self-erasing nature of
arises as to which is the “true” ences) errors of prediction. Journal of Personality and
preference. Intuitively, the prefer- Slovic, P. (1995). The construction of Social Psychology, 2, 211–221.
ence expressed when all relevant preference. American Psychologist, Tversky, A., & Shafir, E. (1992). The disjunction
50, 364–371. effect in choice under uncertainty. Psychologi-
information is readily available cal Science, 3, 305–309.

Copyright © 2000 American Psychological Society


Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 12, 2016

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen