Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

Mindfulness

DOI 10.1007/s12671-013-0213-y

ORIGINAL PAPER

Trait Mindfulness in Relation to Emotional Self-Regulation


and Executive Function
Michael Lyvers & Chloe Makin & Evan Toms &
Fred Arne Thorberg & Christina Samios

# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Abstract The present study examined relationships of trait Introduction


mindfulness to indices of mood, executive functions, and
emotion regulation in 153 university students of both genders In the context of modern Western psychology, the ancient
aged 18–30 years. Participants completed a questionnaire Buddhist notion of mindfulness has become a “hot” topic as
battery consisting of the Mindful Attention Awareness measures of mindfulness are reported to be positively asso-
Scale (MAAS), Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scales ciated with subjective well-being, self-awareness, impulse
(DASS-21), Negative Mood Regulation (NMR) scale, Frontal control, and self-regulation of emotions (e.g., Brown and
Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe), Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Ryan 2003; Chambers et al. 2009; Lee and Chao 2012;
(BIS-11), Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), and the Pakenham and Samios 2012). There are varying definitions
Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test. As predicted, of mindfulness. Nevertheless, most include reference to
MAAS mindfulness scores were significantly positively relat- focusing attention and awareness on the present. For exam-
ed to NMR expectancies and negatively related to DASS ple, the widely cited definition by Brown and Ryan (2003,
Depression, Anxiety and Stress scores, all three FrSBe indices p. 824) describes mindfulness as “awareness and attention to
of prefrontal cortex dysfunction, BIS-11 impulsivity, and present events and experiences” including emotional states.
TAS-20 alexithymia. Findings are consistent with the notion Mindfulness has been examined as a naturally occurring
that trait mindfulness reflects aspects of executive function attribute, although much of the literature has focused on
and emotion regulation, processes dependent on the functional the enhancement of mindfulness, such as through medita-
integrity of the prefrontal cortex. Such higher-order processes tion training (Brown and Ryan 2003). Meditation-oriented
may underlie the widely reported associations between mind- interventions, such as Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction
fulness and subjective well-being. (Kabat-Zinn 2003), as well as interventions that teach mind-
fulness using less meditation-oriented techniques, such as Ac-
Keywords Trait mindfulness . Subjective well-being . ceptance and Commitment Therapy (Hayes et al. 1999), are
Negative mood regulation expectancies . Frontal lobe becoming increasingly well regarded in Western psychology.
dysfunction . Impulsivity . Alexithymia Highly mindful individuals are said to be “in tune” with
their emotions and highly capable of regulating them
(Brown and Ryan 2003, p. 832). Mindfulness has consis-
M. Lyvers (*) : C. Makin : E. Toms : C. Samios
tently been found to be related to better psychological well-
Department of Psychology, Bond University, Gold Coast, being across a broad range of indices, such as lower scores
QLD 4229, Australia on depression and anxiety and higher scores on positive
e-mail: mlyvers@bond.edu.au affect and life satisfaction (see reviews by Brown and Ryan
2003; Chambers et al. 2009; Keng et al. 2011). In their
F. A. Thorberg
Centre for Addiction Issues, Department of Substance Abuse, seminal paper on trait conceptions of mindfulness and the
Innlandet Hospital Trust, Oslo, Norway development of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale
(MAAS), Brown and Ryan framed such findings in terms
F. A. Thorberg of the “impact [of mindfulness] on psychological well-
Department of Behavioural Sciences in Medicine,
Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine being” (p. 833). However, alternative interpretations are
University of Oslo, Innlandet Hospital Trust, Oslo, Norway plausible.
Mindfulness

One possibility is that mindfulness levels as indexed by with identifying and describing one’s feelings as well as an
measures such as the MAAS reflect higher-order meta- externally oriented style of thinking. The Alcohol Use Disor-
cognitive and meta-emotional processes related to executive ders Identification Test (AUDIT; Babor et al. 1992) was also
cognitive functioning and associated behavioral and emo- administered as it has previously shown consistent relation-
tional self-regulation (Chambers et al. 2009; Lee and Chao ships with the FrSBe and BIS-11 scales in young adult sam-
2012)—processes which themselves promote psychological ples (e.g., Lyvers et al. 2012a), and mindfulness has been
well-being. In other words, the correlations reported be- reported to be negatively related to risky or problematic drink-
tween indices of mindfulness and well-being may to some ing (Fernandez et al. 2010). Based on the considerations
extent reflect the successful operation of other functions that described above, in young adults MAAS scores were
underlie both. For example, a superior ability to identify and expected to be negatively related to DASS Depression,
regulate one’s emotions should mean that, all other factors Anxiety, and Stress scores as well as the FrSBe indices
being equal, negative moods are experienced less often or of frontal lobe dysfunction, BIS-11 impulsivity, risky
less intensely compared to someone who has low levels of drinking as assessed by AUDIT, and TAS-20 alexithymia,
that ability. Indeed, people with alexithymia—defined as and positively related to NMR expectancies. In other words,
difficulties identifying and describing one’s feelings, a pau- we expected that mindfulness as a trait would be linked to
city of fantasy life and an externally oriented thinking style both executive function and emotion regulation, in line with
(Nemiah et al. 1976)—frequently suffer from anxiety and the theoretical and neuroscience-based arguments of
depression (Thorberg et al. 2009), presumably due to their Chambers et al. (2009).
difficulties with emotional self-awareness and self-regulation.
Furthermore, and in line with predictions, alexithymia has
been found to be significantly negatively correlated with Method
various aspects of mindfulness (Baer et al. 2006; Stasiewicz
et al. 2012). Participants
In their review integrating notions of mindfulness and
emotion regulation, Chambers et al. (2009) noted that self- The 153 participants consisted of 87 female and 66 male Bond
awareness and self-regulation of emotions are both linked to University students aged between 18 and 30 years (M=
activity within the prefrontal cortex, a brain region essential 21.29 years, SD=3.11). The sample included 50 psychology
for normal executive cognitive functioning. Recent research undergraduates recruited via the psychology participant pool
has indicated that alexithymia is associated with elevated who participated for course credit in their undergraduate psy-
indices of frontal lobe dysfunction (Lyvers et al. 2012b), chology class, and 103 students from the general university
which in turn are negatively related to mood self-regulation population who were given the opportunity to enter a draw to
expectancies (Lyvers et al. 2010); low confidence in one’s win a gift voucher.
ability to self-regulate negative affect is consistently associ-
ated with negative moods such as depression and anxiety
(Thorberg and Lyvers 2006). In an attempt to tie these recent Materials
findings together with the concept of mindfulness, the present
study examined the MAAS index of trait mindfulness in a The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (Brown and Ryan
young adult sample in relation to self-report indices of mood, 2003) is a 15-item self-report measure of trait mindfulness.
emotion regulation, everyday frontal lobe related functioning, The MAAS captures individual differences in present-
and alexithymia, i.e., the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales moment and temporal frequencies of attention and awareness
(DASS-21; Lovibond and Lovibond 1995), which assess (Jermann et al. 2009). Items are negatively worded as Brown
levels of the corresponding negative mood states; the Negative and Ryan (2003) felt statements reflecting states of “mindless-
Mood Regulation expectancies (NMR) scale (Catanzaro and ness” are “more accessible to most individuals, given that
Mearns 1990), which measures the degree to which individ- mindless states are much more common than mindful states”
uals feel they can self-regulate their own negative mood states; (p. 826). Answers are given on a six-point Likert scale, where
the Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe; Grace and 1=almost always and 6=almost never. Questions ask about
Malloy 2001), which assesses problems with motivation, awareness and attention, and include items such as: “I find it
emotional self-control, and executive cognition in everyday difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the present,”
life; the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; Patton et al. and “I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of
1995), an index of self-reported impulsivity—which can per- what I’m doing.” Higher scores indicate greater characteristics
haps be construed as a manifestation of “mindlessness” of mindfulness. A single factor solution with loadings ranging
(Brown and Ryan 2003); and the Toronto Alexithymia Scale, .43 to .78 was yielded from a sample of 727 university
or TAS-20 (Bagby et al. 1994a), which assesses difficulties students (MacKillop and Anderson 2007), which supports
Mindfulness

Brown and Ryan’s (2003) original claim for student and The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (Patton et al. 1995) is a
general adult samples. Interestingly, MAAS scores have 30-item self-report scale designed to measure trait im-
shown a positive association with prefrontal cortex activation pulsivity. Answers are given on a four-point Likert scale
and a negative relationship with amygdala activation (1=rarely/never to 4=almost always/always). Items in-
(Creswell et al. 2007), in line with theoretical expectations. clude those reflecting cognitive instability and inattention
The Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scales-21 (Lovibond (8 items, e.g., “I don’t pay attention”), motor impulsiveness
and Lovibond 1995) is a 21-item self-report questionnaire (11 items, e.g., “I do things without thinking”), and non-
consisting of three scales (each with seven items) designed planning impulsivity (11 items, e.g., “I plan tasks careful-
to measure the emotional states of depression (e.g., “I felt ly,” reverse scored). Items are summed to produce an
downhearted and blue”), anxiety (e.g., “I felt scared without overall score, where higher scores indicate greater im-
any good reason”), and stress (e.g., “I found it hard to wind pulsivity. Total scores can range from 30 to 120. Con-
down”) upon reflection of the prior week. Items are rated on vergent validity was established with neuropsychological
a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 0=did not apply to measures (the Go/No-Go Test r=.39 and the Antisaccade Test,
me at all, to 3=applied to me very much or most of the time. r=.48) that have shown sensitivity to prefrontal cortex dys-
Items are summed within each scale, with higher scores function (Spinella 2004).
indicating higher levels of depression, anxiety, and stress. The Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (Bagby et al. 1994b) is
The Negative Mood Regulation scale (Catanzaro and a 20-item self-report questionnaire used to assess trait levels
Mearns 1990) is a 30-item questionnaire that measures the of alexithymia. Each item is rated on a five-point Likert
extent to which individuals believe they can alter their states of scale (1=strongly agree to 5=strongly disagree). Items are
negative mood through their own efforts. People are asked to summed within each subscale and then overall to create total
report what they believe they can do regarding a negative scores that range from 20 to 100, with higher scores indi-
mood. Questions are stated both negatively (e.g., “Wallowing cating a greater level of alexithymia. Cutoff scores have
in it is all I can do”) and positively (e.g., “I can do something to been empirically derived for the TAS-20, whereby ≥61=
feel better”). Answers are given on a five-point Likert scale alexithymia and ≤51=no alexithymia (Bagby et al. 1994b).
(1=strongly disagree to 5=strongly agree). Summation of The TAS-20 has a confirmed three-factor structure
item ratings yields a total score, where higher scores indicate supported in clinical (Swift et al. 2006) as well as non-
greater belief in one’s ability to alleviate negative mood clinical (O’Brien et al. 2008; Parker et al. 2003) samples:
through their own efforts. NMR scores are considered to difficulty identifying feelings (DIF; e.g., “I have feelings
reflect emotion regulation ability and have been found to be that I can’t quite identify”), difficulty describing feelings
negatively related to measures of frontal lobe dysfunction such (DDF; e.g., “People tell me to describe my feelings more”),
as the Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (Lyvers et al. 2010). and externally-oriented thinking (EOT; e.g., “I feel close to
The Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (Grace and Malloy someone, even in moments of silence,” reverse-scored).
2001) is a 46-item questionnaire designed to measure The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (Babor et
neurobehavioral traits related to dysfunction in three key al. 1992) is a widely used ten-item self-report screening tool
prefrontal systems: the anterior cingulate (Apathy subscale, for hazardous, harmful, or dependent patterns of alcohol
14 items; e.g., “Sit around doing nothing”), orbitofrontal use. Items 1–3 refer to alcohol consumption (e.g., “How
cortex (Disinhibition subscale, 15 items; e.g., “Do or say often do you have a drink containing alcohol?”), items 4–6
embarrassing things”), and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Ex- are related to alcohol dependence (e.g., “How often during
ecutive Dysfunction subscale, 17 items; e.g., “Repeat certain the last year have you found that you were not able to stop
actions or get stuck on certain ideas”). As originally intended, drinking once you had started?”), and items 7–10 inquire
for brain-injured patients, the FrSBe is used to assess changes about alcohol-related harm or consequences (e.g., “Have
in behavior by obtaining pre- and post-lesion ratings on a five- you or someone else been injured because of your drink-
point Likert scale (1=almost never, 2=seldom, 3=sometimes, ing?”). All items on the AUDIT are scored from 0 to 4 and
4=frequently, 5=almost always). The first 32 items represent can be summed to yield a total score ranging from 0 (non-
deficits and are positively phrased, whereas the last 14 items drinkers) to 40. Two demographic questions asked partici-
are negatively phrased and are reverse scored. Items are pants to indicate their age and gender.
summed with higher ratings indicating higher levels of im-
pairment for the subscales as well as the total score. In the
present study using a non-clinical sample, only self-ratings of Procedure
current behavioral functioning were obtained, as in previous
work conducted on non-brain-injured samples (e.g., Lyvers et Ethical clearance was obtained from the Bond University
al. 2012a; Spinella 2003; Stout et al. 2003; Verdejo-Garcia et Human Research Ethics Committee. Undergraduate psy-
al. 2006). chology students were recruited via a sign-up sheet that
Mindfulness

AUDIT

MAAS Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale, Dep Depression, Anx Anxiety, NMR Negative Mood Regulation Scale, Disin Disinhibition, ExeDys Executive Dysfunction, BIS-11 Barratt
described the study and stated its incentive of one credit

1
point for participation. Additionally, a wider population of
students was sampled from the university library, where
students were approached and asked if they would like to

Impulsiveness Scale, DIF Difficulty Identifying Feelings, DDF Difficulty Describing Feelings, EOT Externally Oriented Thinking, AUDIT Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test
EOT

.15
1
participate for the chance to win one of three $20 gift
vouchers. All participants first read an explanatory state-
ment that briefly outlined the purpose of the study as well

.32***
.01
as the voluntary and anonymous nature of their participa-

DIF

1
tion. Participants were instructed to not provide any identi-
fying information on the surveys in order to maintain

.58***
anonymity. They were asked to complete the demographic

.27**
DDF

.00
questions, the MAAS, DASS-21, NMR, FrSBe, BIS-11,

1
TAS-20, and AUDIT, which were presented in a uniquely
random order to each participant. Completion of these took

.33***
BIS-11

.27**

.22**
about 30 min. Upon completing these questionnaires at the

.17*
testing venue on campus, participants handed them back to

1
the researchers in a sealed envelope. Participants were then
thanked and either given a signed credit-point form as proof

.63***
.39***
.42***
.31***
ExeDys
of their participation for psychology course credit, or else

.09
1
they wrote their name and email on a list for a draw to
possibly win a $20 gift voucher. Three vouchers were
awarded following a random drawing after the study was

.67***
.58***

.28***
.24**

.21**
.21**
Disin
completed.

1
.38***
.67***

.39***
.33***
Results
Apathy

.26**

.20*
−.10
1
The final dataset of 153 cases was derived from an original
set of 160 cases that was reduced after identification and −.52***

−.42***

−.32***
−.36***
−.27**

−.21**

−.21**
removal of 5 multivariate outliers and 2 cases with missing
NMR

.06
data. Intercorrelations were calculated among all continuous
1

variables, and these results are shown in Table 1. As can be


seen in the table, trait mindfulness as indexed by MAAS
−.40***
.36***

.37***
.28***
.38***
.41***
.27**
Stress

scores was significantly positively correlated with age and


.07
−.01
1

NMR expectancies, and significantly negatively correlated


with all three FrSBe indices of frontal lobe dysfunction, all
three DASS scales of negative moods, the BIS-11 measure
.61***

.43***
.38***
−.26**
.21**

.26**
.26**
.20*

of impulsiveness, and the TAS-20 alexithymia subscales


.09
−.01
Anx

DIF and DDF. There was no relationship between trait


mindfulness and the AUDIT index of alcohol use. The only
variables that were significantly correlated with AUDIT
.53***
.69***
−.47***
.42***

.28***

.34***
.40***
.25**
Table 1 Correlations among study variables

.03
−.06
.11
Dep

were the FrSBe Disinhibition scale and the BIS-11, as con-


1

sistently found in previous work with young adult samples


(Lyvers et al. 2009, 2010, 2011, 2012a).
−.29***

−.44***

−.38***
−.36***
−.45***
−.30***
−.36***
−.46***

A sequential hierarchical regression was employed with


MAAS

−.22**

.23**

*p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001


−.14
.03

MAAS scores as the criterion. The goal was to identify


1

unique predictors of trait mindfulness among the measures


that were correlated with MAAS scores. Predictor variables
.18*

−.19*
−.00
−.03
.09
.14
−.10

−.08
−.10
−.07
−.01
−.04
−.11
Age

were entered in the order of demographic variables age and


1

gender (step 1); the three DASS negative mood scales (step
2); NMR mood regulation expectancy scores (step 3); BIS-
ExeDys

AUDIT
Apathy
MAAS

BIS-11

11 impulsivity and FrSBe frontal lobe dysfunction total


Stress
NMR

Disin

DDF

EOT
Anx
Age

Dep

DIF

scores (step 4); and TAS-20 total alexithymia scores at the


Mindfulness

last step. At step 1, the model was significant, F(2, 150)= and the R2 change for all variables at each step to dem-
4.10, p =.02. Age and gender accounted for 5 % of the onstrate the additional proportion of the variance in
variance in MAAS, R=.23, R2 =.05. At step 2, DASS signif- MAAS uniquely explained by each set of variables at their
icantly improved prediction, explaining an additional 21 % of point of entry. In the final model TAS-20 alexithymia scores,
the variance, R=.51, R2 =.26, F change (3, 147)=13.77, DASS Stress scores, FrSBe total frontal lobe dysfunction
p<.0001. At step 3, the addition of NMR scores did not explain scores, gender, and age contributed to trait mindfulness scores,
additional variance in MAAS scores, R=.51, R2 =.26, F change in descending order of importance.
(1, 146)=.11, p=.74. At step 4, BIS-11 and FrSBe scores A final analysis compared TAS-20-defined alexithymia
significantly improved prediction of MAAS scores, explaining groups on the variables of interest including the MAAS index
an additional 9.5 % of variance, R=.60, R2 =.36, F change of trait mindfulness, as a follow-up to previous work on traits
(2, 144)=10.60, p<.0001. At the last step, addition of TAS-20 associated with alexithymia (Lyvers et al. 2012b; Thorberg et
total scores further improved prediction explaining a further al. 2009). Initially, a 3 (alexithymia, borderline alexithymia,
5.5 % of variance, R=.64, R2 =.41, F change (1, 143)=13.28, no alexithymia, as defined by TAS-20 cutoff scores)×2
p<.0001. Table 2 displays the unstandardised regression co- (gender) between groups multivariate analysis of covariance
efficients (B), standardised regression coefficients (β), t scores, (MANCOVA) was conducted on MAAS scores, the three
DASS scales, NMR scores, FrSBe total scores, and BIS-11
total scores. The results showed significant differences on all
Table 2 Regression coefficients of trait and mood measures on MAAS dependent measures such that the alexithymia and borderline
mindfulness alexithymia groups both differed from the no-alexithymia
Variable B β t R2 change
group but not from each other as indicated by Tukey post
hoc comparisons at p<.05. Given the relatively small cell
Step 1 Age 0.69 .18 2.26* .05 sizes for the borderline (n=27) and alexithymia (n=19)
Gender −3.19 -.13 −1.69 groups, and the absence of significant differences between
Step 2 Age 0.89 .23 3.22** .21 them by post hoc tests, for ease of interpretation these two
Gender −2.39 -.10 −1.39
groups were combined into one alexithymia/borderline group
Depression 0.04 .03 0.28
(n=46) and compared with the no-alexithymia group (n=107)
Anxiety 0.13 .08 0.85
using MANCOVA with gender as the second independent
Stress −0.70 -.52 −4.77***
variable and age as the covariate (because it was weakly
Step 3 Age 0.87 .23 3.10** .00
Gender −2.40 -.10 −1.39
related to FrSBe Disinhibition and MAAS mindfulness as
Depression 0.06 .04 0.36
shown in Table 1). Gender was included as a factor because
a chi-square test revealed that the gender composition of the
Anxiety 0.12 .08 0.83
two alexithymia groups was significantly different, χ2 (1)=
Stress −0.69 -.52 −4.66***
7.08, p<.01, such that the alexithymia/borderline group had
NMR 0.02 .03 0.33
27 males out of 46 (or 59 %) compared to 38 males out of 107
Step 4 Age 0.67 .18 2.50* .10
Gender −3.32 −.14 −2.02 (or 36 %) in the no-alexithymia group. After square root
Depression 0.02 .01 0.14 transforms were applied to DASS Anxiety and Depression
Anxiety 0.12 .08 0.85 scores, and log 10 transforms were applied to FrSBe and BIS-
Stress −0.54 -.40 −3.74*** 11 total scores, Box’s M test was not significant (p=.19), nor
NMR −0.08 -.10 −1.18
was Levene’s test significant for any dependent variable
FrSBe −0.23 -.36 −3.77***
(all p>.15); all assumptions were thus met after transforma-
BIS-11 −0.01 -.01 −0.11
tion of skewed variables. The overall multivariate effect of
alexithymia group was significant according to Pillai’s Trace,
Step 5 Age 0.66 .17 2.54* .06
Gender −4.63 -.20 −2.87** F(7, 142)=8.84, p<.0001, partial η2 =.30, observed power=
Depression 0.02 .01 0.14 1.00. The multivariate effect of gender was not significant,
Anxiety 0.21 .13 1.51 p=.09, nor was the interaction of alexithymia group with
Stress −0.50 -.38 −3.64*** gender, p=.31. Univariate effects of alexithymia group were
NMR −0.12 -.15 −1.87 significant for all dependent measures: MAAS mindfulness,
FrSBe −0.18 -.29 −3.11** F(1, 148) = 27.05, p < .0001, partial η 2 = .16, observed
BIS-11 0.03 .02 0.25 power=1.00; DASS Stress, F(1, 148)=27.43, p<.0001, par-
TAS-20 −1.31 -.29 −3.64***
tial η2 =.16, observed power=1.00; transformed DASS Anx-
iety, F(1, 148)=23.85, p<.0001, partial η2 =.14, observed
Gender coded 1(male), 2 (female) power=1.00; transformed DASS Depression, F(1, 148)=
*
p<.05; **
p<.01; ***
p<.001 24.00, p < .0001, partial η2 = .14, observed power = 1.00;
Mindfulness

NMR, F(1, 148)=12.60, p<.001, partial η2 =.08, observed levels of frontal lobe related executive and behavioral dys-
power=.94; transformed FrSBe total scores, F(1, 148)= function, impulsivity, and negative moods compared to the
28.55, p<.0001, partial η2 =.16, observed power=1.00; and non-alexithymia group. Associations of alexithymia with neg-
transformed BIS-11 total scores, F(1, 148)=9.35, p=.003, ative moods, impulsivity, frontal lobe dysfunction and low
partial η2 =.06, observed power=.86. As shown in Table 3, NMR were entirely consistent with previous work (Lyvers et
the alexithymia/borderline group scored significantly lower al. 2012b) as well as theoretical expectations (Thorberg et al.
on MAAS mindfulness and NMR, and significantly higher on 2009). The association of alexithymia with low mindfulness is
DASS Stress, Anxiety and Depression as well as FrSBe and in line with previous research in student and alcohol-
BIS-11 total scores, compared to the no-alexithymia group dependent samples (Baer et al. 2006; Stasiewicz et al. 2012)
(only untransformed values are shown in the table for ease of and was expected given that mindfulness is said to involve
interpretation). awareness of one’s feelings in the present moment (Brown
and Ryan 2003), whereas alexithymia is defined by difficulty
in identifying and describing one’s feelings (Nemiah et al.
Discussion 1976) and thus must be the inverse of mindfulness where
emotional feelings are concerned. Taken together, the present
As predicted based on the notion that mindfulness reflects results support Chambers et al.’s conceptualization of mind-
aspects of both executive function and emotion regulation fulness as an outcome of the executive functions of self-
(Chambers et al. 2009), MAAS mindfulness scores were regulation of attention and emotion, processes linked to the
significantly positively related to the NMR scale index of functional integrity of the prefrontal cortex. In other words,
mood regulation expectancies and significantly negatively current results suggest that trait mindfulness may reflect the
related to DASS Depression, Anxiety and Stress scores, all operation of higher-order processes that themselves facilitate
three FrSBe indices of frontal lobe dysfunction, BIS-11 im- improved everyday functioning and subjective well-being—
pulsivity, and TAS-20 alexithymia (particularly the DIF and processes which appear to be deficient in alexithymia as well
DDF subscales). In the final hierarchical regression model as in highly impulsive or compulsive behaviors (Dawe and
TAS-20, DASS Stress, and FrSBe total scores were the stron- Loxton 2004).
gest predictors of MAAS scores. Further, MANCOVA com- Limitations of the current study include the non-
paring TAS-20-defined alexithymia groups as a follow-up to random sampling, which somewhat restricts the general-
previous work on the traits associated with alexithymia izability of the findings; the cross-sectional design,
showed that the alexithymia/borderline group was character- which cannot reveal the nature and causal direction of
ized by lower levels of mindfulness and NMR and higher the relationships of mindfulness with executive function,
impulsivity, mood regulation, and failure to recognize
and regulate emotion (as in alexithymia); and the
Table 3 Untransformed means for alexithymia/borderline alexithymia operationalization of psychological well-being as lower
group versus no-alexithymia group on trait and mood indices (see text scores on DASS indices of negative moods, which may
for definitions of measures) limit inferences regarding positive indicators of well-being.
Such indicators to potentially examine in future work should
Measure Group M SD
include measures of positive affect and life satisfaction, which
MAAS** Alexithymia 52.46 10.26 have been found to not only co-occur with negative indicators
Control 61.36 11.41 (Folkman and Moskowitz 2000) but have different relation-
Depression** Alexithymia 11.83 9.25 ships with mindfulness in some samples (e.g., Pakenham and
Control 5.33 6.56 Samios 2012).
Anxiety** Alexithymia 12.52 9.10 Despite the above limitations, this study provides further
Control 5.48 5.46 support for the association between trait mindfulness and
Stress** Alexithymia 16.52 9.04 psychological well-being, particularly for lower scores on
Control 9.50 7.81 stress, and suggests a potential framework for understanding
NMR** Alexithymia 102.39 11.84 mindfulness and its relationship to mood in terms of higher-
Control 111.35 15.55 order functions such as executive function and emotion
FrSBe** Alexithymia 110.48 18.84 regulation as originally proposed by Chambers et al.
Control 93.25 16.20 (2009). Longitudinal research would be required to provide
BIS-11* Alexithymia 66.43 9.79
a sound basis upon which to make practice recommenda-
Control 61.00 9.21
tions. Nevertheless, considering trait mindfulness in relation
to executive function and emotion regulation may prove
*p<.01; **p<.001 informative for practitioners who employ mindfulness-
Mindfulness

based therapies (Kabat-Zinn 2003), especially when university students. Addiction Research & Theory, 17(6), 668–
677.
supporting clients who are highly impulsive or alexithymic.
Lyvers, M., Duff, H., & Hasking, P. (2011). Risky alcohol use and age
at onset of regular alcohol consumption in relation to frontal lobe
indices, reward sensitivity and rash impulsiveness. Addiction Re-
search & Theory, 19, 251–259.
References Lyvers, M., Duff, H., Basch, V., & Edwards, M. (2012a). Influences of
rash impulsiveness and reward sensitivity on risky drinking in
university students: Evidence of mediation by frontal systems.
Babor, T. F., De la Fuente, J. R., Saunders, J., & Grant, M. (1992). Addictive Behaviors, 37, 940–946.
AUDIT: The alcohol use disorders identification test. Geneva: Lyvers, M., Onuoha, R., Thorberg, F. A., & Samios, C. (2012b).
World Health Organization. Alexithymia in relation to parental alcoholism, everyday frontal
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. lobe functioning and alcohol consumption in a non-clinical sam-
(2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of ple. Addictive Behaviors, 37, 205–210.
mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45. Lyvers, M., Thorberg, F. A., Ellul, A., Turner, J., & Bahr, M. (2010).
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (1994a). The twenty-item Negative mood regulation expectancies, frontal lobe related be-
Toronto Alexithymia Scale: Item selection and cross-validation of haviors and alcohol use. Personality and Individual Differences,
the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 23–32. 48, 332–337.
Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., & Parker, J. D. A. (1994b). The twenty- MacKillop, J., & Anderson, E. J. (2007). Further psychometric valida-
item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: II. Convergent, discriminant, tion of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). Journal
and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, of Psychopathology & Behavioural Assessment, 29, 289–293.
38(1), 33–40. Nemiah, J. C., Freyberger, H., & Sifneos, P. E. (1976). Alexithymia: A
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: view of the psychosomatic process. In O. Hill (Ed.), Modern
Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of trends in psychosomatic medicine (Vol. 3). London: Butterworths.
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848. O’Brien, C., Gaher, R. M., Pope, C., & Smiley, P. (2008). Difficulty
Catanzaro, S. J., & Mearns, J. (1990). Measuring generalized expectan- identifying feelings predicts the persistence of trauma symptoms
cies for negative mood regulation: Initial scale development and in a sample of veterans who experienced military sexual trauma.
implications. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, 546–563. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196, 252–255.
Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion Pakenham, K.I., & Samios, C. (2012). Couples coping with multiple
regulation: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, sclerosis: A dyadic perspective on the roles of mindfulness and
29, 560–572. acceptance. Journal of Behavioral Medicine (in press).
Creswell, J., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. Parker, J. D. A., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2003). The 20-Item
(2007). Neural correlates of dispositional mindfulness during Toronto Alexithymia Scale III: Reliability and factorial validity in
affect labeling. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 560–565. a community population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55,
Dawe, S., & Loxton, N. J. (2004). The role of impulsivity in the 269–275.
development of substance use and eating disorders. Neuroscience Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure
and Biobehavioural Reviews, 28, 343–351. of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psycholo-
Fernandez, A. C., Wood, M. D., Stein, L. A., & Rossi, J. S. (2010). gy, 51, 768–774.
Measuring mindfulness and examining its relationship with alcohol Spinella, M. (2003). Relationships between drug use and prefrontal-
use and negative consequences. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, associated traits. Addiction Biology, 8, 67–74.
24, 608–616. Spinella, M. (2004). Neurobehavioural correlates of impulsivity: Evidence
Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Positive affect and the other of prefrontal involvement. International Journal of Neuroscience,
side of coping. American Psychologist, 55, 647–654. 114, 95–104.
Grace, J., & Malloy, P. F. (2001). Frontal systems behavior scale. Pro- Stasiewicz, P. R., Bradizza, C. M., Gudleski, G. D., Coffey, S. F.,
fessional manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Schlauch, R. C., Bailey, S. T., et al. (2012). The relationship of
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and alexithymia to emotional dysregulation within an alcohol depen-
commitment therapy. New York: Guilford Press. dent treatment sample. Addictive Behaviors, 37(4), 469–476.
Jermann, F., Larøi, F., Bondolfi, G., Van der Linden, M., Billieux, J., Stout, J. C., Ready, R. E., Grace, J., Malloy, P. F., & Paulsen, J. S.
d’Argembeau, A., & Zermatten, A. (2009). Mindful Attention (2003). Factor analysis of the frontal systems behaviour scale
Awareness Scale (MAAS): Psychometric properties of the French (FrSBe). Assessment, 10, 79–85.
translation and exploration of its relations with emotion regulation Swift, L., Stephenson, R., & Royce, J. (2006). The 20-item Toronto
strategies. Psychological Assessment, 21, 506–514. Alexithymia Scale: Validation of factor solutions using confirma-
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, tory factor analysis on physiotherapy outpatients. Psychology and
present and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 79, 83–88.
144–156. Thorberg, F. A., & Lyvers, M. (2006). Negative mood regulation
Keng, S., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness (NMR) expectancies, mood, and affect intensity among clients
on psychological health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical in substance disorder treatment facilities. Addictive Behaviors, 31,
Psychology Review, 3, 1041–1056. 811–820.
Lee, Y. C., & Chao, H. F. (2012). The role of active inhibitory control Thorberg, F. A., Young, R., Sullivan, K. A., & Lyvers, M. (2009).
in psychological well-being and mindfulness. Personality and Alexithymia and alcohol use disorders: A critical review. Addictive
Individual Differences, 53, 618–621. Behaviors, 34, 237–245.
Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the depression Verdejo-Garcia, A., Rivas-Pereza, C., Lopez-Torrecillasa, F., & Perez-
anxiety and stress scales. Sydney: Psychological Foundation. Garcia, M. (2006). Differential impact of severity of drug use on
Lyvers, M., Czerczyk, C., Follent, A., & Lodge, P. (2009). Disinhibi- frontal behavioural symptoms. Addictive Behaviours, 31, 1373–
tion and reward sensitivity in relation to alcohol consumption by 1382.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen