Sie sind auf Seite 1von 15

Review of General Psychology Copyright 2004 by the Educational Publishing Foundation

2004, Vol. 8, No. 3, 179 –193 1089-2680/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.8.3.179

Multifaceted Nature of Intrinsic Motivation:


The Theory of 16 Basic Desires
Steven Reiss
The Ohio State University

R. W. White (1959) proposed that certain motives, such as curiosity, autonomy, and
play (called intrinsic motives, or IMs), have common characteristics that distinguish
them from drives. The evidence that mastery is common to IMs is anecdotal, not
scientific. The assertion that “intrinsic enjoyment” is common to IMs exaggerates the
significance of pleasure in human motivation and expresses the hedonistic fallacy of
confusing consequence for cause. Nothing has been shown scientifically to be common
to IMs that differentiates them from drives. An empirically testable theory of 16 basic
desires is put forth based on psychometric research and subsequent behavior validation.
The desires are largely unrelated to each other and may have different evolutionary
histories.

Motives are reasons people hold for initiating own sake. In contrast, means are indicated when
and performing voluntary behavior. They indi- a person performs an act for its instrumental
cate the meaning of human behavior, and they value. Examples include a professional athlete
may reveal a person’s values. Motives often who plays ball for a salary and a student who
affect a person’s perception, cognition, emo- studies to improve a grade. In each of these
tion, and behavior. A person who is highly examples, the goal (salary, grade) is desired
motivated to gain social status, for example, because it produces something else. A person
may be observant of marks of social distinction, might seek a salary, for example, as a means of
may think often about issues pertaining to enhancing social status, or high grades as a
wealth, may especially enjoy the feeling of self- means of pleasing a parent.
importance, and may behave in ways associated An analysis of a person’s behavior may iden-
with upper-class status.1 By defining motives as tify a series of instrumental acts followed by
reasons, we do not imply that motives are pri- one or more end goals that complete the “be-
marily cognitive, any more than establishing a havior chain.” For example, a person may take
motive for a crime in a court of law requires a second job for the extra salary (instrumental
conscious premeditation. A person can have a motive), desire the extra salary to purchase
reason to behave, and thus a motive, without health care (instrumental motive), and desire the
necessarily being aware of it. health care to benefit his or her family (end
Aristotle (330 BCE/1953) divided motives into goal). This example of a simple behavior chain
ends versus means on the basis of the individ- shows three behaviors, two motivated by instru-
ual’s purpose for performing the behavior. Ends
mental goals and a third motivated by an end
are indicated when a person engages in a be-
goal. Logically, only goals that are desired for
havior for no apparent reason other than that is
their own sake can serve as the “end” of a
what the person desires to do. Examples include
purposeful explanation of a series of human
a child playing ball for physical exercise and a
student reading a book out of curiosity. In each acts.
of these examples, the goal is desired for its The number of instrumental motives is, for
all practical purposes, unlimited. Only imagina-
tion limits how many different ways individuals

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-


1
dressed to Steven Reiss, Department of Psychology, Ohio This example is based partially on empirical findings of
State University, 321 McCampbell Hall, 1581 Dodd Drive, a correlation between motivation for status and motivation
Columbus, OH 43210-1296. E-mail: reiss.7@osu.edu for wealth (see Reiss & Havercamp, 1998).

179
180 REISS

can pursue the end goal of, say, power. Gener- In this article, I examine how various end
ally, there seems to be little or no point to listing goals relate to each other. I consider the mean-
instrumental motives, because no matter how ing of the concept of IM and ask, What, if
comprehensive a list happens to be, somebody anything, justifies classifying end goals into a
can probably imagine an additional instrumen- unitary, global category of intrinsic motivation?
tal motive that was overlooked and could be In discussing this issue, I do not consider the
added. In contrast, the number of ends is limited phenomenon of “undermining effects,” in
by human nature. A central issue for psychol- which extrinsic rewards decrease intrinsic mo-
ogy should be to identify and classify the end tivation. These effects have been discussed in
purposes of human behavior, because ends in- detail in numerous prior publications (Deci,
dicate the ultimate goals of much of what peo- Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Eisenberger & Cam-
ple do. eron, 1996; Reiss & Sushinsky, 1975). I pro-
Two theoretical perspectives have been ad- pose a theory of end goals, called the theory
vanced concerning end goals. Multifaceted the- of 16 basic desires, and summarize evidence
ory holds that the various end goals are largely showing the multifaceted nature of end motives.
unrelated to each other, perhaps to the point I begin the discussion by considering the behav-
where they are genetically distinct sources of iorist concept of drive, because IM was devel-
motivation with different evolutionary histories. oped to show the limitations of this concept.
Multifaceted theorists include philosophers who
have suggested lists of the most fundamental Drive Theory
motives of human nature (e.g., Spinoza, 1675/
1949), psychologists who have put forth evolu- Thorndike’s (1911) law of effect reduced hu-
tionary theories of motivation (e.g., McDougall, man motivation to categories of reward and
1926), and psychologists who have suggested punishment. This law holds that responses are
theories of human needs (e.g., Murray, 1938). strengthened when they lead to satisfaction and
In contrast, unitary or global theorists hold weakened when they lead to punishment. Psy-
that end goals can be profitably reduced to a chologists studying learning soon realized
small number of categories based on common Thorndike’s law is a tautology, or a proposition
characteristics. Unitary theorists seek the under- that is circular (true by definition). The follow-
lying psychological principles that are ex- ing statements, for example, are circular with
pressed by diverse motivational events. The an- respect to each other: “Rewards strengthen be-
cient Greek philosophers, for example, reduced havior” and “Any event that strengthens behav-
end goals into categories expressing the needs ior is a reward.”
of the body, mind, and soul (e.g., Plato, 375 The concept of drive was introduced to es-
BCE/1966). Hedonists distinguished between cape from the circularity of the law of effect
end goals associated with pleasure enhancement (Brown, 1961). Instead of identifying reward as
and those related to pain reduction (Russell, any stimulus or satisfying event that strengthens
1945). Freud (1916/1963) reduced motives to behavior, drive theorists defined it as a reduc-
sexual and aggressive instincts. tion in a state of deprivation. The statements
Today, some social psychologists classify “Drive reduction strengthens behavior” and
end goals into two global categories, called “Drive reduction occurs when a state of depri-
drives and intrinsic motives (IMs). The distinc- vation is lessened” are not circular with respect
tion has been influential—1,921 scholarly pub- to each other.
lications on intrinsic motivation (IM) appeared Hull (1943) recognized four types of drives:
during the period of January 1967 to May 2002 hunger, thirst, sex, and escape from pain. In
(source: PsycINFO). IM has been investigated many animal learning experiments, investiga-
in social psychology (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2000), tors have induced drives by depriving animals
developmental psychology (e.g., Harter, 1981), of an important need prior to the experiment.
clinical psychology (e.g., Eisenberger & Cam- The deprivation of food, for example, estab-
eron, 1996), organizational psychology (e.g., lishes food as a powerful reward, increasing the
Houkes, Janssen, de Jonge, & Nijhuis, 2001), animal’s motivation to learn responses that pro-
and educational psychology (e.g., Kohn, 1993). duce food (Skinner, 1938). Much of animal
MULTIFACETED NATURE OF INTRINSIC MOTIVATION 181

learning theory is based on the results of psy- argued that motives that are not drives have
chological studies with food-deprived or water- “very vital common properties” (p. 318). In
deprived animals.2 particular, such motives are concerned with “ef-
fective interactions with the environment, under
Unitary IM Theory the general heading of competence” (p. 317).
White’s motive for competence has been vari-
The unitary construct of IM was put forth as ously called effectance motivation, competence
an alternative to drive theory. The initial insight motivation, and mastery. Deci (1975) later em-
was that many of the motives not explained well braced White’s hypothesis that competence mo-
by drive theory—motives such as exploration tivation is a common property of nondrives,
(curiosity), autonomy, and play— have com- defining IM as behavior “which a person en-
mon properties. To a large extent, unitary IM gages in to feel competent and self-determin-
theory initially represented an attempt to show ing” (p. 61).
the essential differences between drives and White appreciated the need to validate his
what psychodynamic theorists have called ego idea of effectance motivation. He wrote, “No
motives. doubt it will at first seem arbitrary to propose a
In the past, the distinction between drives and single motivational conception in connection
IMs has been thought to have a physiological with so many diverse kinds of behavior. What
basis, at least according to some published re- do we gain by attributing motivational unity to
marks. The general idea was that drives such as such a diverse array of activities?” (White,
hunger and thirst arise from “tissue needs” in- 1959, p. 318). He cited both Piaget (1952) and
volving “peripheral” components of the nervous anecdotal observations to support his hypothe-
system, whereas IMs arise from psychological sis that effectance motivation is common to
or cognitive processes involving primarily cen- exploration, manipulation, and play.
tral neural activity. Deci (1975), for example, White speculated on the development of ef-
wrote that the primary effects of IM “are in the fectance motivation. He suggested that in both
tissues of the central nervous system rather than infants and young children,
in the non-nervous system tissues” (p. 61). This it seems sensible to conceive of effectance motivation
physiological paradigm for distinguishing as undifferentiated. Later in life it becomes profitable
drives from IMs always lacked scientific sup- to distinguish various motives such as cognizance,
port; indeed, we now know that it is physiolog- construction, mastery, and achievement. It is my view
ical nonsense. Motives such as hunger and that all such motives have a root in effectance motiva-
tion. (White, 1959, p. 323)
thirst, for example, involve significant central
nervous system or cognitive activity (Berntson Some of the appeal of White’s (1959) theory
& Cacioppo, 2000). Both the behaviorist con- is suggested by the behavior of young animals
cept of drive and the concept of IM as nondrive and children. Young lion cubs, for example,
have no precise physiological meaning and sometimes seem to be bursting with energy—
originally were put forth at a time when little they get into mischief, play with the other cubs,
was known about the physiology of motivation. explore their environment, and have a tendency
to wander away from the group. In other words,
Mastery they express self-assertive energy as play, ex-
ploration, manipulation, a desire for physical
White’s (1959) article on competence moti-
vation (mastery) was arguably the start of the 2
current era of scholarship on IM. White (1959) Inducing drive prior to experiments lessens the influ-
ence of individual differences in motivation (trait motives),
asserted that exploration, manipulation, and which I later argue is the key to understanding and predict-
play are not drives originating from states of ing much of human behavior. If John has a much stronger
deprivation partially because they are not re- appetite than Sam, for example, this may be evident under
lated to “visceral needs comparable to hunger, many circumstances but less so when both John and Sam
are significantly deprived of food and extremely hungry.
thirst, or sex” (p. 301). Exploration and play The typical behaviorist animal learning experiment was
“cannot be regarded as leading to any kind of poorly suited for observing the effects of individual differ-
consummatory response” (p. 301). He further ences in motivation (Reiss & Havercamp, 1996).
182 REISS

exercise, and a desire for autonomy. They create to become sexually effective in attracting oth-
a strong impression that these behaviors are ers. By putting forth a sufficiently general def-
linked by a common motive of self-assertion. inition of competence motivation, it seems pos-
Anecdotal observations, however, do not sible to argue that nearly any collection of di-
constitute scientific evidence. As far as I could verse motives may have common elements.
determine, no researcher has presented scien- The hypothesis that effectance motivation is
tific tests of the hypothesis that mastery is the undifferentiated at birth and later differentiates
aim of exploration, autonomy, and play. White into the various IMs has not been tested. No-
(1959) cited as evidence for his hypothesis an- body has observed the predicted process of dif-
ecdotal examples of children at play and his ferentiation of mastery motivation. Nobody has
interpretations of what those children were re- conducted measurements of effectance motiva-
ally aiming for with their behavior. Although tion in an undifferentiated state and in its pre-
his interpretations were reasonable and perhaps dicted differentiated form, showing that the
even insightful, they did not rise to the stan- former is linked to the latter.
dards of scientific evidence. Part of the diffi- In conclusion, children show a need to feel
culty with White’s hypothesis is the lack of competent and master their environments. This
quantification and measurement, which are two need, called mastery, is important in childhood
pillars of a scientific approach. As Harter (1981) development and in human behavior generally.
put it, “The global nature of this construct [ef- The relationship between mastery and other ego
fectance motivation] has made it difficult to motives, however, is not well understood. Re-
operationalize. White’s formulation does not searchers need to develop methods to explore
readily lend itself to an empirical test” (p. 301). more fully White’s hypothesis that mastery is
White (1959) did not develop a measure of the aim of important ego motives such as ex-
competence motivation. He did not assess cor- ploration, manipulation, and autonomy.
relations between competence motivation and
various IMs, showing that they are significantly Intrinsic Pleasures
higher than correlations between competence
motivation and drives. White did not propose Intrinsic pleasure is another common charac-
specific studies to test his concept. He did not teristic of IMs, according to unitary theorists.
say what might falsify his theory. He developed This viewpoint holds that people are motivated
some interesting ideas, but he did not put forth to engage in activities they expect to experience
scientific support for those ideas. as pleasurable. When the pleasures are inherent
Kagan (1972) recognized the need to develop to the behavior or activity itself—such as draw-
measures of effectance motivation. He sug- ing for its own sake—IM is imputed (Ryan &
gested that they be developed on the basis of Deci, 2000; Weiner, 1995). When the pleasures
principles similar to those used in the Thematic are external to the behavior or activity—such as
Apperception Test (TAT; Murray, 1943). No drawing for a good player award— extrinsic
such measure, however, has been validated. The motivation is imputed. As Deci and Ryan
validity of the TAT, moreover, has been called (1985) put it, “When people are intrinsically
into question (Zubin, Eron, & Schumer, 1965). motivated, they experience interest and enjoy-
Today, few researchers use the TAT or other ment, they feel competent and self-determining,
storytelling techniques to measure motivation. they perceive the locus of causality for their
White’s (1959) argument that mastery is a behavior to be internal, and in some instances,
common element of the IMs is based on a very they experience flow” (p. 34). Weiner (1995)
broad interpretation of competence motivation. defined IM as a source of motivation arising
He defined competence motivation so broadly from the enjoyment of an activity.
that it includes nearly all motives. One could Are IMs pleasurable? IM theorists may
argue, for example, that drives also express a have erred in embracing hedonism, the philos-
desire to interact effectively with the environ- ophy that pleasure motivates behavior. Over the
ment. In hunger, for example, people want to centuries, scholars have shown a number of
become effective hunters to find food. Some significant limitations of pleasure theories. Ap-
people spend a considerable amount of energy plied to the concept of intrinsic pleasures, for
MULTIFACETED NATURE OF INTRINSIC MOTIVATION 183

example, we may question whether IM theorists arouse fear (James, 1890/1950), and problem
have exaggerated the extent to which certain solving can be frustrating.
activities really are pleasurable. In conclusion, one problem with the concept
IM theorists arguably have exaggerated the of intrinsic pleasures is that it implies that in-
motivational significance of intellectual plea- trinsically motivated behavior is more enjoy-
sures. They have held that everybody is born able than it seems to be. Intrinsic pleasure may
with the potential to enjoy learning (e.g., Kohn, not be a common characteristic of IMs because
1993). When students do not enjoy learning, as sometimes IMs are not necessarily pleasurable.
in the example of high school underachievers, Does intrinsic pleasure motivate much be-
IM theorists blame ineffective teaching, boring havior? Even when the performance of an
curricula, and the widespread use of extrinsic intrinsically motivated behavior is enjoyable,
incentives such as grades (Kohn, 1993). To we cannot assume that the behavior was moti-
motivate students in school, unitary IM theorists vated by the anticipation of such pleasure. Plea-
advise teachers to find ways to make learning sure can be a consequence of behavior rather
fun (Lepper & Cordova, 1992) and to tap into than a motivating cause.
students’ natural curiosity. According to philosophical critiques of hedo-
IM theorists have presented little scientific nism (e.g., Russell, 1945), pleasure is a conse-
evidence to support the hypothesis that every- quence of gratification of a motive other than
body is born with the potential to enjoy learn- pleasure seeking. Please consider Csikszentmi-
ing. Anecdotal examples of adolescents and halyi’s (1990) hypothesis that intrinsically mo-
adults show that many people react to intellec- tivated people climb mountains in order to ex-
tual activities as if they were unpleasant. People perience a special kind of pleasure called flow.
often sustain thought on a problem for no more Reiss (2000a) suggested that flow is a conse-
than brief periods of time; the overwhelming quence of satiating the desire for physical ac-
majority of adults do not read books; documen- tivity, not an intrinsic property of climbing.
taries are among the least popular forms of film; Csikszentmihalyi (2000) replied that people
and even many academics reduce intellectual who have never climbed before may not know
activity soon after they earn a tenured teaching the joys of climbing but that after a person
position. These examples are arguably signs learns that climbing is pleasurable, that individ-
that intellectual activity is naturally unpleasant ual subsequently climbs in order to experience
under many circumstances or if engaged in for such pleasures. “If I feel happy after hiking in
more than a few minutes at a time. the mountains,” argued Csikszentmihalyi
IM theorists may have put forth a misleading, (2000), “chances are I will want to experience
almost romanticized description of the inquiry that happiness again and take another hike” (p.
process. The inquiry process is not always plea- 1163). Pleasures are both “causes and conse-
surable and often involves significant negative quences” of behavior, according to Csikszent-
emotions. Many scientists have written about mihalyi (2000).
the agony of the creative inquiry process and the Not so. If I am physically tired, for example,
emotional ups and downs of research. I will not enjoy climbing mountains. The enjoy-
Whereas IM theorists have said that the psy- ment does not arise from the act of climbing per
chological aim of inquiry is intellectual plea- se, only from the act of climbing while satiating
sure, the multifaceted theory I propose later in the motive for physical exercise. The pleasure is
this article implies that aims of inquiry are not inherent to the activity but is a nonmotiva-
learning and knowledge. Under my viewpoint, tional byproduct of satiating the desire for phys-
highly curious people desire knowledge and ical activity.
understanding so strongly they pursue the in- Suppose that Mary is a curious person who
quiry process even when they must endure anx- reads, learns, and then experiences pleasure.
ieties, severe criticism, devastating failures, and Suppose that Susan is an active person who
other frustrations. Knowledge is the end goal of climbs a mountain and then experiences plea-
curiosity, but thinking, exploration, and prob- sure. In predicting and explaining the behavior
lem solving are not necessarily pleasurable. of these two people, we need to know that one
Thinking can be frustrating, exploration can person is motivated by curiosity and the other is
184 REISS

motivated by fitness. We add nothing significant cial contact, modesty, and righteousness. Des-
by invoking unitary IM theory and arguing that cartes (1637/1958), on the other hand, listed six
it was “anticipated pleasure” or “flow” that mo- “passions of the soul.” He wrote, “There are
tivated each. only six [intrinsic motives] which are simple
and primitive, viz., wonder, love, hatred, desire,
Individual Differences joy and sadness. All others are composed of
these six” (Descartes, 1637/1958, p. 291).
Unitary theory seems inconsistent with indi- James (1890/1950) and McDougall (1926) rec-
vidual differences regarding different IMs. If ognized between 8 and 20 “instincts.”3 When
competence motivation is the developmental or- psychodynamic psychology gained influence,
igin of both autonomy and curiosity, people Murray (1938) reinterpreted McDougall’s list
with above average competence motivation also of instincts into a list of 20 basic psychological
should develop above average autonomy and needs.4 Maslow (1943) also put forth a theory
curiosity, and so trait autonomy and trait curi- of diverse human motives.
osity should be significantly correlated. Highly Some psychologists have taken strong excep-
autonomous people should be highly curious. tion to efforts to develop lists of end motives.
These implications of unitary IM theory are Critics have argued that 8 to 20 fundamental
unsupported by scientific evidence. In factor motives are too many to be studied profitably
studies of the Reiss Profile psychometric instru- (Freeman, Anderson, Azer, Girolami, & Scotti,
ment (Reiss & Havercamp, 1998), moreover, 1998). Biologists study scores of enzymes, and
the correlation between factors assessing trait chemists study 115 elements. Because we do
autonomy (independence) and trait curiosity not say that biologists study “too many” en-
was virtually nil (r ⫽ .05, N ⫽ 1,154). Because zymes or that chemists study “too many” ele-
highly autonomous people are not necessarily ments, why should we think that 8 to 20 basic
highly curious, how can autonomy and curiosity
motives are “too many” for psychologists to
be about the same thing (mastery)?
study? Scientific rules permit psychologists to
study as many fundamental motives as can be
Conclusion identified. We do not want to invent a motive
White (1959) put forth the thesis that IMs for every behavior, of course, but we also do not
such as curiosity, autonomy, and play have want to proceed according to invalid, precon-
common characteristics justifying a “single ceived notions of how many motives there are.
name” (p. 317), meaning a single category of Evolutionary theory suggests a multifaceted
motivation. No common characteristics have model of IMs. When we consider the various
been shown scientifically. Neither competence IMs, they seem relevant to different survival
motivation nor intrinsic pleasure has been needs, suggesting possibly distinct motives con-
shown to distinguish IMs from drives. White’s trolled by different genes. Efficacy, for exam-
hypothesis lacks adequate scientific support and ple, motivates building nests and other forms of
seems inconsistent with individual differences. shelter, which have the survival value of pro-
White himself admitted that his hypothesis was
speculative in that the IMs appear to be too 3
In the James–McDougall theory, a human instinct is an
diverse to have significant common automatically occurring desire or motive, not a rigid pattern
characteristics. of unlearned behavior. The idea is that people do not de-
liberately choose their psychological needs. As Aristotle
(330 BCE/1953) put it, people choose means, not ends.
Multifaceted Nature of End Goals Aristotle was making a valid point in logic (if an individual
attempted to choose an end, the option chosen would be
Throughout history many scholars have ex- instrumental to the criteria on which the choice was based).
pressed a multifaceted theory of end motivation In contrast, James and McDougall were reporting anecdotal
(“doing something for its own sake”). Aristotle, observations of human behavior.
4
Henry A. Murray (1938) acknowledged the origin of his
for example, identified 12 end motives: confi- famous list of needs when he wrote, “This classification of
dence, pleasure, saving, magnificence, honor, needs is not very different from lists constructed by Mc-
ambition, patience, sincerity, conversation, so- Dougall, Garnett, and a number of other writers” (p. 84).
MULTIFACETED NATURE OF INTRINSIC MOTIVATION 185

tecting animals from harsh environmental ele- result of this process, an initial list of nearly 500
ments. Autonomy—the desire for freedom— items was reduced to 328 items.
motivates animals to leave the nest when they Reiss and Havercamp (1998) asked research
come of age, spreading the search for food over participants to rate how important each of the
a larger geographical area. These considerations motives we identified was in determining their
suggest at least a possibility of separate evolu- behavior. Initially, we conducted four factor
tionary histories, which supports the idea that studies (three exploratory studies and one con-
IMs are multifaceted. firmatory study), each with a different sample of
participants. The combined total of 2,554 peo-
Factorial Studies of End Motivation ple included people of diverse ages (12 to 76)
and stations in life (e.g., high school students,
Since 1995, I have been developing a list of college students, military people, fast food
diverse IMs, variously called the theory of 16 workers, seminary students, human service pro-
basic desires or sensitivity theory. My graduate viders, nursing home residents). The results of
students and I have been gathering evidence an initial confirmatory factor study supported a
showing the reliability, factorial validity, and 15-factor solution. On the basis of these results,
behavioral validity of this list. By validating a we constructed a self-report instrument, called
variety of largely unrelated end motives (what the Reiss Profile of Fundamental Goals and
people seek for its own sake), my colleagues Motivational Sensitivities. The initial version of
and I have sought to show that end motivation the instrument assessed 15 fundamental
is, in fact, multifaceted. motives.
Reiss and Havercamp (1996, 1998) defined We developed a scale to assess saving, or the
basic or fundamental motives to have three fea- desire to collect, which was added as a 16th
tures: (a) end purposes (IMs), (b) universal mo- factor because we became convinced that col-
tivators, and (c) psychological importance. The lecting is an end motive we had overlooked. In
criterion of psychological importance is in- a study of 512 adults solicited from sources in
tended to focus multifaceted theory on behav- urban and rural Ohio, Havercamp (1998) con-
iors that historically have been central to psy- firmed the 16-factor solution (the original 15
chological personality theory. Eating, for exam- factors plus saving) to the revised 128-item
ple, is considered to be psychologically Reiss Profile instrument.
important because aspects of culture, many ev- In conclusion, when people were asked to
eryday activities, and some clinical disorders self-report their motives, they reported 16 fac-
are concerned with food and food preparation; tor-distinct categories of psychologically signif-
on the other hand, thirst is not considered to be icant end motives (IMs). Any effort to reduce
a fundamental motivator because it does not this list to only a few categories would result in
account for a significant amount of everyday the loss of significant information, and this
behavior of interest to psychologists. Without should produce a vastly inferior system (com-
the criterion of psychological significance, there pared with the full 16 basic desires) for analyz-
may be scores of basic motives additional to ing and predicting people’s behavior.
those considered in this article, including one Reliability of factors. Test–retest reliabili-
for each biological need. ties for the Reiss Profile have been reported
We began our effort to identify the basic (Havercamp & Reiss, 2003; Reiss & Haver-
motives of human behavior by developing a list camp, 1998), with mean correlations of .83
of every motive we could imagine. We con- (range ⫽ .80 to .96) for 2-week reliability and
sulted a variety of reference sources and asked .80 (range ⫽ .69 to .88) for 4-week reliability.
colleagues for suggestions. We obtained ideas These test–retest reliabilities exceed those re-
from Murray’s (1938) theory of needs, motiva- ported for some personality tests such as the
tional studies, psychopathology articles and Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory–2
books, and psychiatric classification manuals. (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, &
We pared down our initial list of items by Kaemmer, 1989). They support the hypothesis
eliminating redundancies and motives that have that the Reiss Profile assesses trait motives.
relatively little psychological significance. As a Reiss and Havercamp (1998) reported Cron-
186 REISS

bach’s alpha coefficients of internal reliability ism; Schwartz’s benevolence falls under Reiss’s
for each of 15 scales (all except saving). The motives of family and social contact;
median alpha value was .86 (range .70 to .92), Schwartz’s tradition falls under Reiss’s honor;
suggesting good internal reliability for the Reiss the value of conformity as defined by Schwartz
Profile scales. falls under Reiss’s motive for vengeance; and
Social desirability. Havercamp and Reiss Schwartz’s value of security falls under Reiss’s
(2003) assessed the social desirability of the motive of order. The two bodies of research,
Reiss Profile with a sample of 171 undergradu- thus, produced significantly similar results,
ate student volunteers, who completed both the showing the multifaceted nature of ultimate val-
Reiss Profile and the Marlowe–Crowne Social ues or IMs.
Desirability Scale (MCSDS; Crowne & Mar-
lowe, 1960). The correlations computed be- Theory of 16 Basic Desires
tween the MCSDS and each of the Reiss Profile
scales ranged in absolute value from .01 to .39 Reiss’s theory of 16 basic desires, which is
(Mdn ⫽ .09, M ⫽ .16). For sake of comparison, summarized in Table 1, is put forth as a multi-
Jackson (1984) reported correlations between faceted model of IM, alternative to the unitary
the Personality Research Form (PRF) and social models of IM. Hypothesis 1 is that each of
desirability scales ranging from .01 to .44 the 16 basic desires is a trait motive. With the
(Mdn ⫽ .20, M ⫽ .22). These results show that possible exceptions of the motives of idealism
the Reiss Profile self-reports are minimally af- and acceptance, the 16 basic desires motivate
fected by social desirability. animals as well as people (Hypothesis 2). The-
Universal values. Additional support for the oretically, the 16 basic desires are considered to
multifaceted model comes from research on val- be genetically distinct with different evolution-
ues. Factorial studies of values should produce ary histories (Hypothesis 3). The satiation of
results similar to factorial studies of end goals each basic desire produces an intrinsically val-
because, as a matter of logic, all end goals are ued feeling of joy, a different joy for each basic
values, and ultimate values are potentially moti- desire (Hypothesis 4). Loosely speaking, people
vating, or reasons to instigate behavior. (Because behave as if they are trying to maximize their
ends and values are logically related ideas, for experiences of the 16 intrinsic joys.
centuries motivation was studied under the topic Although everybody embraces the 16 basic
of ethics—for example, Aristotle’s lecture notes desires, individuals prioritize them differently
on motivation were published in his book Nicho- (Hypothesis 5). Generally, the most important
machean Ethics; Aristotle, 330 BCE/1953.) basic desires for explaining a person’s behavior
These considerations imply that empirically are those that are unusually strong or unusually
derived lists of basic desires and universal hu- weak compared with appropriate norms. For
man values should be similar. In a series of example, some people devote much of their
studies analyzing survey data from 97 samples time to satiating their desire for curiosity, others
from 44 countries, Schwartz (1994) identi- seek power, and still others are out for revenge.
fied 10 universal values. All 10 of Schwartz’s Those basic desires that are neither strong nor
universal values correspond to basic motives in weak compared with appropriate norms are gen-
Reiss’s system. Schwartz’s value of power, for erally less important in explaining a person’s
example, falls under Reiss’s motive called behavior. The satiation of a basic desire is al-
power. Additional “matches” are Schwartz’s ways temporary; soon after we satisfy a basic
value of achievement and Reiss’s motive of desire, the motive reasserts itself and needs to
power; Schwartz’s hedonism and Reiss’s social be satisfied again. After we socialize, for exam-
contact are similar because each includes fun- ple, the desire for social contact may reassert
loving behavior; Schwartz’s excitement is the itself within hours.
opposite of Reiss’s tranquility (the same motive Each basic desire is theoretically regarded as
but assessed from different ends of a contin- a continuum of potential motivation anchored
uum); Schwartz’s self-directing falls under by opposite values (Hypothesis 6). As shown in
Reiss’s motive of independence; Schwartz’s Figure 1, the theory of 16 basic desires holds
value universalism is Reiss’s motive of ideal- that individuals are motivated to aim for a point
MULTIFACETED NATURE OF INTRINSIC MOTIVATION 187

Table 1
Reiss’s 16 Motives
Motive name Motive Animal behavior Intrinsic feeling
Power Desire to influence (including leadership; Dominant animal eats more food Efficacy
related to mastery)
Curiosity Desire for knowledge Animal learns to find food more Wonder
efficiently and learns to avoid
prey
Independence Desire to be autonomous Motivates animal to leave nest, Freedom
searching for food over larger
area
Status Desire for social standing (including Attention in nest leads to better Self-importance
desire for attention) feedings
Social contact Desire for peer companionship (desire to Safety in numbers for animals in Fun
play) wild
Vengeance Desire to get even (including desire to Animal fights when threatened Vindication
compete, to win)
Honor Desire to obey a traditional moral code Animal runs back to herd when Loyalty
stared at by prey
Idealism Desire to improve society (including Unclear: Do animals show true Compassion
altruism, justice) altruism?
Physical Desire to exercise muscles Strong animals eat more and are Vitality
exercise less vulnerable to prey
Romance Desire for sex (including courting) Reproduction essential for Lust
species survival
Family Desire to raise own children Protection of young facilitates Love
survival
Order Desire to organize (including desire for Cleanliness rituals promote Stability
ritual) health
Eating Desire to eat Nutrition essential for survival Satiation (avoidance
of hunger)
Acceptance Desire for approval Unclear: animal self-concept? Self-confidence
Tranquility Desire to avoid anxiety, fear Animal runs away from danger Safe, relaxed
Saving Desire to collect, value of frugality Animal hoards food and other Ownership
materials

of moderation (called a set point or sensiti- the amount of social contact Henry or Jake
vity)—in other words, people generally are mo- experience is less than they desire, they are
tivated to experience what Aristotle called a motivated to socialize. When the amount of
moderate mean. Most people aim for a moder- social contact they experience is about what
ate degree of power, a moderate degree of sta- they desire, they are temporarily satiated. When
tus, a moderate degree of knowledge, and so on the amount of social contact is more than they
for each basic desire. Individual differences, desire, they are motivated to be alone to balance
however, are significant (Hypothesis 5). In Fig- out their experiences.
ure 1, for example, Henry aims for a lower Suppose that Henry and Jake attend a party
degree of social contact than does Jake. When that lasts 3 hours. Henry enjoys the party at first

Figure 1. The basic desire for social contact as a continuum of end motivation.
188 REISS

but then feels uncomfortable because he is ex- relevant to the satisfaction of our desires, and
periencing much more social activity than he we tend to ignore stimuli that do not satisfy our
desires. Jake, on the other hand, still wants more desires. A person with a strong desire for ven-
fun when the party ends. After the party, Henry geance, for example, is attentive to possible
goes home and spends the next morning alone insults or provocations, whereas a person with a
to balance his experience toward a low average weak desire for vengeance may not even notice
amount of social contact, whereas Jake goes to an insult. A person with a strong desire for order
the nearest bar to keep the party rolling. pays attention to how neat and clean a room is
The theory of 16 basic desires holds that what and notices when things are out of place. In
is motivating are discrepancies between the contrast, a person with a weak desire for order
amount of an intrinsic satisfier that is desired may not even notice when dirty dishes are in the
and the amount that was recently experienced sink or the house is a mess.
(Hypothesis 7). When a person experiences
more power than he or she desires, the individ- Validation of 16 Basic Desires
ual is motivated to be submissive for a period of
time to balance experience toward the desired In science we choose the competing theory
rate. When a person experiences less power that potentially explains the most data. Multi-
than he or she desires, the individual is moti- faceted theory seems to have significant gener-
vated to be domineering for a period of time. ality of application: Reiss (2000b) has shown
Thus, a person who generally has a powerful how it is at least theoretically possible to hold
personality may at times be motivated to be that human relationships, careers, family life,
submissive, and a person with a submissive sports, and spirituality are organized to satiate
personality may at times be motivated to be the 16 basic desires. The 16 basic desires also
dominant. have been applied to psychopathology (Reiss &
According to the theory of 16 basic desires, Havercamp, 1996) and to mental retardation
vengeful people are motivated to experience (Dykens & Rosner, 1999; Wiltz & Reiss, 2003).
high degrees of vindication, competition, con- Reiss’s model of 16 basic desires was devel-
tentiousness, conflict, and aggression, whereas oped in the tradition of comprehensive person-
peacemakers are motivated to experience low ality theories, but unlike previous global per-
degrees of these experiences. When a vengeful sonality theories, Reiss’s model is testable.
person experiences a period of time—say, a few Is self-report a valid basis to determine that
days—that is conflict free, the individual is mo- motives are multifaceted? Do people validly
tivated to get into arguments, fights, and com- self-report their motives? Are they just “talk-
petitions. The longer the period of time that is ing”? Validation research on the 16 basic de-
conflict free, the stronger becomes the motiva- sires has produced significant evidence that
tion to pick a fight with somebody. If the indi- what people say motivates them is consistent
vidual “goes too far” and provokes more of a with how they behave in the “real world.”
fight than he or she had anticipated, the person Convergent validity. The convergent valid-
becomes motivated by peacemaking behavior to ity of the Reiss Profile is the extent to which
balance experience back toward the individual’s individual factors correlate with other scales
Aristotlean mean (de Waal, 1989). On the other intended to measure the same or similar con-
hand, peacemakers are generally conflict structs. Havercamp and Reiss (2003) showed
avoidant and motivated to experience a below that the Reiss Profile power and order scales
average degree of strife. When peacemakers correlated .55 and .60, respectively, with the
experience even ordinary amounts of everyday dominance and order scales of the PRF. This is
strife and conflict, they become motivated to noteworthy because the PRF scales have been
make peace. Their tolerance for strife is much validated against the Strong Vocational Interest
below that of the average individual. Blank. For college student populations, the PRF
As was noted previously, basic desires orga- scale for dominance predicts social behavior
nize our attention, cognitions, feelings, and be- (Jaccard, 1974) and participation in student ac-
havior into a coherent action or whole (Hypoth- tivities (Pierce & Schwartz, 1971). Further, the
esis 8). We pay attention to stimuli that are Reiss Profile scale for tranquility has been cor-
MULTIFACETED NATURE OF INTRINSIC MOTIVATION 189

related with the Reiss–Epstein–Gursky Anxiety Although turning to god images for psycho-
Sensitivity Index (Peterson & Reiss, 1992) be- logical support is sometimes misunderstood as a
cause of significant overlapping item content weakness of religious people (hence, the criti-
(Havercamp & Reiss, 2003). cism that people use God as a “psychological
Validated profiles. A series of studies were crutch”), the results of testing with the Reiss
conducted to evaluate how the 16 motives are Profile showed that, at least for the sample of
related to behavior. Because motivation has di- people tested, the religious desire for psycho-
verse applications, these studies evaluated is- logical support was unrelated to any desire for
sues as varied as spirituality, sports, and choice weakness. Although religious people had a low
of club or college major. score for independence (probably reflecting the
In a study of religious motivation, Reiss desire for support from God), they had an av-
(2000c) tested 558 adults who had identified erage score for power (implying they do not
themselves as very, somewhat, or not religious. seek submission to secular leaders). Hence,
How religious a person identified himself to be many religious people find appealing the mes-
(called religiosity) was associated with above sage that opening oneself to God is a sign of
average (high) Reiss Profile motivational scores strength. The Bible, for example, teaches that
for honor and family and with below average submission to God can produce such strength
(low) scores for vengeance and independence that the faithful will be able to toss mountains
(autonomy). The study showed that religiosity into the sea.
is a valid predictor of how people self-report Since antiquity, religious leaders have taught
their 16 basic desires. that autonomy needs to be moderated; in Budd-
The finding that religiosity is associated with hism, for example, the ultimate aim is to be-
high honor suggests that people choose a reli- come One with the Nirvana. The theory of 16
gion partially out of a desire to be loyal to one’s basic desires recognizes a human need to mod-
parents and heritage. People overwhelmingly erate autonomy to an individually determined
choose the religion of their parents. The finding sensitivity level, so that too much autonomy
that low vengeance is associated with religiosity relative to the sensitivity point is experienced as
may reflect the Christian value of kindness and aversive. In unitary IM theory, however, auton-
“turning the other cheek.” Family values also omy is explicitly regarded as a joy and implic-
drive people to religion, suggesting that religion itly regarded as an infinite joy. As we have seen,
provides a psychological means of strengthen- the results of studies of spirituality support mul-
ing family life. tifaceted theory.
The finding that religiosity is associated with If the 16 basic desires are truly fundamental
a desire for low independence (low autonomy) to human behavior, however, they should be
is particularly interesting. Many religious writ- able to explain not only deeply meaningful ac-
ings encourage “opening one’s heart to God.” tivities such as religion but also recreational
Under the theory of 16 basic desires, this mes- activities such as sports. Reiss, Wiltz, and Sher-
sage expresses a desire for psychological sup- man (2001) administered the Reiss Profile to
port, which plays a central role in the universal 415 college students who had participated in
human need to moderate the psychological zero, one, or two or more varsity sports at the
sense of autonomy (existence as an independent high school or college level. How many sports
being). The results of testing with the Reiss the student participated in, called athleticism,
Profile in fact showed that the more religious a was found to be associated with motivational
person was, the greater was the individual’s traits for physical exercise, social contact, fam-
motivational score for psychological support ily, vengeance, power, and low curiosity. As
(low independence). The results implied that was expected, the single most important corre-
people embrace images of supportive and atten- late of sports participation was the intrinsic en-
tive deities not because they fear death but joyment of physical exercise. The difference in
primarily because images of these gods moder- the strength of the motive for physical exercise
ate feelings of autonomy, which many people between students who had played zero versus
experience as aversive when the feelings are too two or more varsity sports was about a full
strong. standard deviation. Although the association be-
190 REISS

tween intrinsic enjoyment of physical exercise global categories. Ancient Greek philosophers,
and athletic participation may strike some as for example, distinguished between motives as-
obvious, actually it is inconsistent with unitary sociated with the body (such as hunger and
IM theory, which has held that mastery is the thirst) and those associated with the intellect
primary intrinsic motivation shown by athletic (such as curiosity, morality, and friendship). In
participation (Deci & Ryan, 1985). the early part of the 20th century, Freud (1916/
Havercamp and Reiss (2003) showed moti- 1963) argued that all motives are ultimately
vational profiles for eight interest groups, pro- about sex. Hedonists, on the other hand, re-
viding additional evidence linking the 16 basic duced all motives to pleasure seeking versus
desires to real-world behavior. The groups were pain avoidance.
college students who joined fraternities and so- The concept of IM can be viewed as a mod-
rorities at a state university, philosophy majors, ern example of the effort in motivational reduc-
Reserve Officers’ Training Corps students, vol- tionism. IM theorists divide motives into two
unteers, culinary students, dieters, seminarians, global categories: drives (also called extrinsic
and varsity athletes. motivation) and intrinsic motivation. Drives are
P. Kavanaugh (personal communication, De- about biological survival needs, whereas IMs
cember 2002) tested on the Reiss Profile a group pertain to what some have called ego motives.
of 49 high school sophomores who were doing Hunger, thirst, and pain avoidance are paradigm
poorly in school. They scored low for curiosity, examples of drives, whereas curiosity, auton-
honor, and idealism and moderately high for omy, and play are paradigm examples of IMs.
vengeance and social contact. This pattern of White (1959) put forth the thesis of common-
results suggests that the students were easily alities among IMs; specifically, he argued that
frustrated by intellectual effort and that they IMs are about competence (mastery). In ad-
were psychologically disconnected from their
vancing his idea of competence motivation,
parents (low honor) and community (low ideal-
White explicitly acknowledged that his thesis
ism) but were connected to peers (high social
seems implausible because he was asserting
contact). How these students self-reported their
commonalities among a diverse collection of
motives was consistent with what was known
about their behavior in terms of their low motives. Although many embraced White’s
achievement and high rate of disciplinary ideas because they seemed to broaden the study
problems. of motivation to include ego motives, White
offered no scientific evidence to support his
theory of competence motivation— he devel-
Correlation Matrix oped no measure, conducted no studies to test
Evidence that the 16 basic desires are largely his idea, and did not suggest any specific studies
unrelated to each other supports multifaceted that might confirm or falsify his hypothesis. In
theory. Havercamp (1998) calculated the aver- the 50 years since White wrote his article, no
age correlations among the 16 factors of the scientific evidence has emerged directly show-
Reiss Profile at about .15. More than 81% of the ing that competence motivation is the underly-
values in the intercorrelation matrix had an ab- ing theme of diverse ego motives such as play,
solute value of less than .20, indicating that the curiosity, and autonomy. Further, no scientific
scales were largely unrelated. The most strongly evidence has been put forth directly supporting
related scales are power and status (r ⫽ .58), White’s hypothesis that IMs have a common
vengeance and status (r ⫽ .54), and honor and origin: There is no direct scientific evidence for
idealism (r ⫽ .48). These results support a mul- the hypothesis that people are born with “undif-
tifaceted approach to end motivation and argue ferentiated” competence motivation that later is
against unitary theories. manifested as the motives of mastery, auton-
omy, play, and curiosity. Further, the distinction
Conclusion between drive motivation arising from the or-
gans or periphery and intrinsic motivation aris-
Since antiquity, scholars have debated ing from the central nervous system or brain is
whether human motives can be reduced to a few physiological nonsense.
MULTIFACETED NATURE OF INTRINSIC MOTIVATION 191

The hypothesis that “intrinsic enjoyment” is motives) also have been shown to be multifac-
common to IMs also remains unproved. Argu- eted (Schwartz, 1994). Competence is only one
ably, this hypothesis implies significant similar- of the values people hold—it is not the common
ities between global IM theory and the flawed root of diverse values. People also value ends
philosophy of hedonism. It is unclear whether such as status, social contact, revenge, physical
“intrinsic enjoyment” is a unitary phenomenon: activity, and autonomy.
Ancient Greek philosophers, for example, ar- Evidence for multifaceted theory extends sig-
gued that pleasures differ in kind (Aristotle, 330 nificantly beyond the initial factorial studies
BCE/1953), implying that pleasure theory cannot suggesting 15 and 16 factor solutions for basic
support unitary or global theories of motivation. motivation. All 16 basic motives have been
Further, logicians have identified errors in plea- validated against criterion behavior outside lab-
sure theory, noting that pleasure theorists some- oratories and experiments (see Havercamp &
times confuse consequence for cause. Pleasure Reiss, 2003). Concurrent validity and social de-
is often not intrinsic to an activity; it occurs as sirability studies also support the multifaceted
a consequence of satiating motives (Russell, model.
1945). Whether pleasure is experienced de- The sensitivity model holds that 16 geneti-
pends on a person’s motivation; for example, cally distinct desires (IMs) combine to deter-
mountain climbing can be pleasant when one mine many psychologically significant motives.
desires physical exercise but unpleasant when The model supports numerous predictions about
one is tired and desires rest. Thus, it is not behavior and provides standardized measures
mountain climbing per se that is pleasurable but (such as the Reiss Profile self-report version)
the potential the activity holds for satiating mo- needed to test the validity of the predictions.
tives, such as the motive for physical activity or Future research is indicated to study this model
the motive for achievement. Logicians say that and the role of the 16 basic desires in human
pleasure is usually a nonmotivational byproduct behavior. This theory, if valid, shows the mul-
of satiating motives, not the aim of the motive tifaceted nature of human IMs. Researchers
(Russell, 1945). studying unitary IM theory need to show they
A number of empirical considerations argue can predict behavior as well as or better than
against unitary theory and in favor of multifac- Reiss’s model by reducing the 16 motives to
eted theories. Human individuality may be too one or two categories. In contrast, multifaceted
diverse to be described adequately in terms of theorists need to continue to show that behavior
global categories such as IM and extrinsic mo- is better predicted and explained with a com-
tivation. Many people who are strongly moti- prehensive, multifaceted list of IMs than is pos-
vated toward play are not necessarily strongly sible with a reductionism approach.
motivated by intellectual curiosity or autonomy.
If curiosity and autonomy both are motivational
because of a universal desire for competence,
References
why are some people motivated much more by
curiosity than autonomy or vice versa? It is Aristotle. (1953). The nicomachean ethics. New
unclear how observations of such individual York: Penguin. (Original work published 330 BCE)
differences can be made consistent with the Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2000). Psycho-
theory that play, curiosity, and autonomy are biology and social psychology: Past, present, and
really about the same motive, a desire for future. Personality and Social Psychology Re-
competence. view, 4, 3–15.
Reiss and Havercamp’s research on 16 fun- Brown, J. S. (1961). The motivation of behavior. New
York: McGraw-Hill.
damental desires provides additional evidence
Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R.,
of the multifaceted nature of end motivation. Tellegen, A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989). Minnesota
This theory provides a comprehensive analysis Multiphasic Personality Inventory–2 (MMPI-2):
of individual differences in trait motives. A Manual for administration and scoring. Minneap-
series of factorial studies have confirmed the olis: University of Minnesota Press.
16-factor model (Reiss & Havercamp, 1998); Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale
similarly, values (logically, values express end of social desirability independent of psychopathol-
192 REISS

ogy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349 – Jaccard, J. J. (1974). Predicting social behavior from
354. personality traits. Journal of Research in Person-
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology ality, 7, 358 –367.
of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row. Jackson, D. N. (1984). Personality research form
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Happiness, flow, and manual. Port Huron, MI: Research Psychologists
human economic equality. American Psycholo- Press.
gist, 55, 1163–1164. James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology (Vol.
de Waal, F. (1989). Peacemaking among primates. 2). New York: Dover. (Original work published
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 1890)
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Kagan, J. (1972). Motives and development. Journal
Plenum. of Personality and Social Psychology, 22, 51– 66.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. (1999). A Kohn, A. (1993). Punished by rewards. New York:
meta-analytic review of experiments examining Houghton Mifflin.
the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic moti- Lepper, M. R., & Cordova, D. L. (1992). A desire to
vation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627– 668. be taught: Instructional consequences of intrinsic
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motiva- motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 16, 187–207.
tion and self-determination in human behavior. Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of motivation. Psy-
New York: Plenum. chological Review, 50, 370 –396.
Descartes, R. (1958). The passions of the soul. New McDougall, W. (1926). An introduction to social
York: Modern Library. (Original work published psychology. Boston: John W. Luce.
1637) Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality: A
Dykens, E. M., & Rosner, B. A. (1999). Redefining clinical and experimental study of fifty men of
behavioral phenotypes: Personality-motivation in college age. New York: Oxford University Press.
Williams and Prader-Willi syndromes. American Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic Apperception Test.
Journal of Mental Retardation, 104(2), 158 –169. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). The detri- Peterson, R. A., & Reiss, S. (1992). Anxiety Sensi-
mental effects of reward: Myth or reality? Ameri- tivity Index revised test manual. Worthington, OH:
can Psychologist, 51, 1153–1166. IDS Publishing.
Freeman, K. A., Anderson, C. M., Azer, R. H., Gi- Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in chil-
rolami, P. A., & Scotti, J. A. (1998). Why func- dren (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: International
tional assessment is enough: A response to Reiss Press.
and Havercamp. American Journal of Mental Re- Pierce, R. A., & Schwartz, A. J. (1971). Personality
tardation, 103, 80 –91. style of student activists. Journal of Psychol-
Freud, S. (1963). Introductory lectures on psycho- ogy, 79, 221–231.
analysis. London: Hogarth Press. (Original work Plato (1966). The republic (F. M. Cornford, Trans.).
published 1916) New York: Oxford University Press. (Original
Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic work published 375 BCE)
versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Mo- Reiss, S. (2000a). Human individuality, happiness,
tivational and informational components. Develop- and flow. American Psychologist, 55, 1161–1162.
mental Psychology, 17, 100 –112. Reiss (2000b). Who am I: The 16 basic desires that
Havercamp, S. M. (1998). The Reiss Profile of moti- motivate our actions and define our personality.
vation sensitivity: Reliability, validity, and social New York: Tarcher/Putnam.
desirability. Doctoral dissertation, Department of Reiss (2000c). Why people turn to religion. Journal
Psychology, Ohio State University. for the Scientific Study of Religion, 39, 47–52.
Havercamp, S. M., & Reiss, S. (2003). A compre- Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. H. (1996). The sensitivity
hensive assessment of human striving: Test–retest theory of motivation: Implications for psychopa-
reliability and validity of the Reiss Profile. Journal thology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34,
of Personality Assessment, 81, 123–132. 621– 632.
Houkes, I., Janssen, P. M., de Jonge, J., & Nijhuis, Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. M. (1998). Toward a
F. J. N. (2001). Specific relationships between comprehensive assessment of fundamental motiva-
work characteristics and intrinsic work motivation, tion: Factor structure of the Reiss Profile. Psycho-
burnout and turnover intention: A multi-sample logical Assessment, 10, 97–106.
analysis. European Journal of Work and Organi- Reiss, S., & Sushinsky, L. W. (1975). Overjustifica-
zational Psychology, 10, 1–23. tion, competing responses, and the acquisition of
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social
York: Appleton Century. Psychology, 31, 1116 –1125.
MULTIFACETED NATURE OF INTRINSIC MOTIVATION 193

Reiss, S., Wiltz, J., & Sherman, M. (2001). Trait Weiner, B. (1995). Intrinsic motivation. In A. Man-
motivational correlates of athleticism. Personality stead, M. Hewstone, S. Fiske, M. Hoggs, H. Reis,
and Individual Differences, 30, 1139 –1145. & G. Samin (Eds.), The Blackwell encyclopedia of
Russell, B. (1945). A history of Western philosophy. social psychology (p. 341). Cambridge, UK:
New York: Simon & Schuster. Blackwell.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determina- White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The
tion theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motiva- concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66,
tion, social development, and well-being. Ameri- 297–333.
can Psychologist, 55, 68 –78. Wiltz, J., & Reiss, S. (2003). Compatibility of house-
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in mates with mental retardation. American Journal
the structure and contents of human values? Jour- of Mental Retardation, 108, 173–180.
nal of Social Issues, 50, 19 – 45. Zubin, J., Eron, L. D., & Schumer, F. (1965). An
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. experimental approach to projective techniques.
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. New York: Wiley.
Spinoza, B. D. (1949). The ethics. New York:
Hafner. (Original work published 1675) Received September 25, 2002
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence, exper- Revision received January 23, 2003
imental studies. New York: Macmillan. Accepted February 25, 2003 䡲

Wanted: Old APA Journals!


APA is continuing its efforts to digitize older journal issues for the PsycARTICLES database.
Thanks to many generous donors, we have made great strides, but we still need many issues,
particularly those published in the 1950s and earlier.

If you have a collection of older journals and are interested in making a donation, please e-mail
journals@apa.org or visit http://www.apa.org/journals/donations.html for an up-to-date list of the
issues we are seeking.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen