Sie sind auf Seite 1von 5

Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 552–556

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Autonomy–proximity imbalance: An attachment theory perspective on


intrusiveness in romantic relationships
Shiri Lavy a,*, Mario Mikulincer b, Phillip R. Shaver c
a
Department of Behavioral Sciences and Psychology, Ariel University Center, Ariel 44837, Israel
b
The New School of Psychology, Interdisciplinary Center (IDC), P.O. Box 167, Herzliya 46150, Israel
c
Department of Psychology, University of California, One Shields Avenue, Davis, CA 95616-8686, U.S.A.

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: We examined associations between two kinds of attachment insecurity (anxiety and avoidance) and
Received 2 September 2009 intrusiveness in couple relationships. One hundred fifty-six adults completed measures of attachment
Received in revised form 29 November 2009 insecurities and variables related to intrusiveness (engaging in intrusive behavior, perceiving a partner
Accepted 2 December 2009
as intrusive, subjective experiences of being intrusive, and reacting to intrusive behavior). Attachment
Available online 6 January 2010
anxiety was associated with more intrusive behavior, more ambivalent reactions to partner intrusiveness,
and greater emotionality when being intrusive. Avoidance was associated with perceiving a partner as
Keywords:
intrusive, reacting critically and establishing distance in response to partner intrusiveness, and feeling
Attachment
Anxiety
concerned and caring when being intrusive. Results and their implications are discussed from an attach-
Avoidance ment theory perspective.
Intrusiveness Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Couple relationships
Transgressions

1. Introduction governed by other behavioral systems such as exploration. Engag-


ing in what Bowlby called exploration leads to increased cognitive,
Autonomy and relatedness, or interdependence, are important behavioral, and social skills, which contribute over time to the
issues in most close relationships (Prager & Roberts, 2004). Achiev- development of secure autonomy. When a person has regulated
ing balance between them is often challenging yet necessary for emotions effectively by relying on security-providing ‘‘attachment
partners’ mutual satisfaction (e.g., Bowlby, 1982; Mikulincer & figures” (relationship partners who provide a ‘‘safe haven” and a
Shaver, 2007; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Here, we focus on one kind of ‘‘secure base for exploration”; Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall,
imbalance between autonomy and relatedness – intrusiveness – 1978), then the person can alternate, at will, between comfortable
viewed from the perspective of attachment theory. This theory is closeness and self-confident autonomy.
a broad and extensively researched framework for understanding Problems develop when a primary caregiver is either self-preoc-
normative interpersonal processes as well as individual differences cupied and intrusive or intimacy-avoidant and unsupportive. Volu-
in couple relationships (see Cassidy & Shaver, 2008; Mikulincer & minous research, both correlational and longitudinal, indicates
Shaver, 2007, for reviews). In the study reported here we explored that a child raised under these conditions develops what Bowlby
individual differences in the behavioral and experiential processes (1982) called attachment insecurity (see Cassidy & Shaver, 2008,
involved in regulating closeness and autonomy in couple for reviews). Early in the history of attachment research, Ainsworth
relationships. et al. (1978) conceptualized the major forms of insecurity in terms
Attachment theory (Bowlby, 1982) conceptualizes human moti- of two categories, anxious and avoidant attachment, and discov-
vation in terms of several biologically evolved ‘‘behavioral sys- ered that the anxious, clingy pattern was associated with interac-
tems,” including attachment, exploration, caregiving, and sex. tions with a self-preoccupied and intrusive attachment figure,
According to the theory, the attachment system evolved to respond whereas the avoidant, compulsively self-reliant pattern was asso-
to threats and stresses by causing a person to seek protection and ciated with interactions with a caregiver who was distant and
comfort from familiar caregivers. If proximity-seeking reduces the unsupportive. A third insecure pattern, described and labeled ‘‘dis-
threat and distress, the threatened person’s mind turns to activities organized” by Main and Solomon (1990), is characterized by con-
flicting components of the other two insecure patterns.
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +972 52 8814582; fax: +972 57 7961039. Personality and social psychologists who study attachment-re-
E-mail address: shirilavy@gmail.com (S. Lavy). lated mental processes and behavior in adolescents and adults

0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.12.004
S. Lavy et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 552–556 553

(e.g., Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, With regard to the third research issue, we hypothesized that
1998) have designed self-report measures of the two major dimen- reactions to a partner’s intrusive behavior would differ among
sions identified by Ainsworth et al. (1978), attachment anxiety and individuals with different forms of attachment insecurity. For anx-
avoidance, which reflect two learned ways to regulate the attach- ious individuals, there were two possibilities: (1) They might react
ment behavioral system: hyperactivation (vigilance regarding a positively to a partner’s intrusiveness, being grateful for signs of
partner’s interest or disinterest and clingy, intrusive behavior) interest and involvement. But if the intrusive behavior was per-
and deactivation (avoidance of intimacy and strong efforts to re- ceived as critical or unpleasant, (2) they might behave as they often
main self-reliant). These patterns have been empirically associated do in response to relationship conflict, displaying and expressing
with theoretically predicted defensive strategies, relational behav- strong negative emotions. From avoidant individuals we expected
iors, and indicators of poor personal and social adjustment (Mikul- distancing responses to partner intrusiveness, responses that re-
incer & Shaver, 2007). stored personal boundaries and a sense of autonomy.
According to Pistole (1994), the anxiously attached person’s de- Regarding the fourth research issue, we hypothesized that peo-
sire for extreme closeness and the avoidant person’s extreme self- ple with different attachment orientations would have different
reliance often disrupt or damage couple relationships because they subjective experiences of being intrusive. Specifically, we expected
interfere with the negotiation of mutually satisfying closeness and more anxiously attached people to experience more negative
autonomy. Pistole suggested that avoidant individuals’ distancing thoughts and feelings when being intrusive, because their intrusive
behavior interferes with their own (assumed) needs for closeness behavior would be motivated by fear of rejection and a perceived
and with their responsiveness to their partners’ needs for close- shortage of partner affection. We were unsure what to predict
ness. In contrast, anxious individuals’ extreme need for closeness about avoidant individuals’ experiences, because intrusiveness
and fear of abandonment can lead to intrusive behavior and to mis- should be an unusual form of behavior for them.
interpretations of partners’ moves toward autonomy as signs of In this study, participants’ attachment anxiety and avoidance
rejection. Pistole’s analysis was empirically supported (Feeney, were assessed with the Experiences in Close Relationships Inven-
1999; Feeney & Noller, 1991). tory (ECR; Brennan et al., 1998). Their intrusiveness, perceptions
In the study reported here, we sought to deepen our under- of their partners’ intrusiveness, reactions to partner intrusiveness
standing of insecure people’s difficulties in regulating closeness and subjective experiences of being intrusive were assessed with
and autonomy in romantic relationships by examining the issue new questionnaires designed especially for that purpose. We ex-
of intrusiveness. Intrusiveness has been mentioned in descriptions pected attachment anxiety to be associated with more intrusive
of pathological relationships, mainly pursuer–distancer relation- behavior, more ambivalent reactions to a partner’s intrusiveness
ships (in which one person seeks more closeness and the other (a combination of negative emotions and willingness to accept
backs away) and pursuer–pursuer relationships (in which the the partner’s behavior rather than risk jeopardizing the relation-
two partners both desire closeness but seek it in ways that do ship), and more negative experiences of being intrusive. We ex-
not result in the desired outcome). These two patterns are common pected avoidant attachment to be associated with more
in cases of marital breakdown and divorce as well as cases of perceived intrusive behavior by the partner and distancing reac-
domestic violence, because one partner intensively seeks closeness tions to a partner’s intrusiveness.
and reassurance and the other responds with either intensified ef- We also considered possible gender differences, because gender
forts to maintain autonomy and independence or awkward efforts roles assign greater proximity-seeking motivation to women and
to achieve extreme closeness and dependence (e.g., Bartholomew greater autonomy-seeking motivation to men (e.g., Schmitt,
& Allison, 2006; Betchen & Ross, 2000; Fogarty, 1979). 2008; Werner et al., 2001). We hypothesized that men would per-
Intrusiveness in these kinds of relationships is thought to be ceive their female partners as more intrusive, and that men’s neg-
manifested in various ways, such as attempts to monitor and influ- ative reactions to intrusiveness would be stronger.
ence a partner’s attitudes and behaviors, invade the partner’s self-
boundaries, make unsuitable relational demands (Lavy, Mikulincer,
2. Method
Shaver, & Gillath, 2009), and be clingy, controlling, and critical (e.g.,
Savage, 2000). Intrusiveness in couple relationships is reportedly
2.1. Participants
common in couples seeking counseling (Betchen, 2005), and in ex-
treme cases it may provoke domestic violence (e.g., Bartholomew
One hundred fifty-six adult Israeli volunteers (76 men, 80 wo-
& Allison, 2006; Dutton, 2007). However, intrusiveness is also quite
men, aged 18–42, mean = 25.8, SD = 3.66) with 12–20 years of edu-
common in nonpathological relationships (Lavy et al., 2009).
cation (mean = 14.61, SD = 2.35) participated in the study. Most
Aside from preliminary, mainly clinical studies (e.g., Green &
were university students who completed the survey during or after
Werner, 1996; Werner, Green, Greenberg, Browne, & McKenna,
various classes (e.g., psychology, math). Some were acquaintances
2001), little is known about how intrusiveness operates in more
of students who voluntarily completed the survey and returned it
typical romantic relationships. In the study reported here, we
to the researchers. Participants did not receive monetary compen-
sought to examine how attachment insecurities are related to (1)
sation. Over 90% of the participants were involved in a romantic
intrusive behavior in romantic relationships, (2) perceptions of a
relationship that had lasted at least three months at the time of
partner’s intrusive behavior, (3) reactions to a partner’s intrusive
the study; the other 10% were asked to describe a previous rela-
behavior, and (4) the subjective experience of being intrusive.
tionship that had lasted at least three months.
Regarding the first and second issues, adult attachment re-
search suggests that anxiously attached individuals are especially
clingy, controlling, and preoccupied by an intense desire for close- 2.2. Materials and procedure
ness. In contrast, avoidant individuals are especially vigilant and
self-protective with regard to a partner’s attempts to increase Questionnaires were completed in a random order by individu-
closeness (see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007, for a review). Thus, we als tested either separately or in small groups. Attachment insecu-
hypothesized that anxious individuals would be more intrusive, rities were assessed with the ECR, which includes two groups of 18
and avoidant individuals would be more sensitive to threats to items, one measuring attachment anxiety (e.g., ‘‘I worry about
their autonomy and would therefore tend to view their partners being abandoned”) and the other measuring avoidance (e.g., ‘‘I pre-
as intrusive. fer not to show a partner how I feel deep down”). Each item was
554 S. Lavy et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 552–556

Table 1 the variance explained by each factor ranged from 2.9 to 23.3,
Means and SDs of attachment scores for men and women. and Cronbach alphas ranged from .71 to .87 (see Lavy, 2006 for
Men Women details).
Mean SD Mean SD A second exploratory factor analysis of experiences of being
intrusive yielded 6 factors (eigenvalues > 1) explaining 57.2% of
Anxiety 3.09 1.06 3.62 1.17
Avoidance 3.31 .91 2.80 .91
the variance. The factors were named: regret (7 items), anger (6
items), relational insecurity (7 items), care and concern for the
partner (5 items), distancing and helplessness (5 items), and feel-
ing appreciated (3 items). The percentage of the variance explained
rated on a scale ranging from 1 (agree strongly) to 7 (disagree
by each factor ranged from 3.6 to 23.1, and the Cronbach alphas
strongly). The scales were translated into Hebrew and validated
ranged from .72 to .88 (see Lavy, 2006 for details).
by Mikulincer and Florian (2000). In the present study, coefficient
In the current sample, the alphas for the seven reactions to part-
alphas were high for both anxiety (.91) and avoidance (.92). The
ner intrusiveness scales ranged from .65 to .84. The alphas for the
two scores were only modestly correlated, as intended (r
six experiences of being intrusive scales ranged from .68 to .89.
(154) = .20, p < .05), and were treated as separate independent
Scale scores were computed by averaging item ratings.
variables in regression analyses (see Table 1 for means and SDs).
Intrusive behavior was assessed with a questionnaire developed
for this study. In the first stage of scale development, participants 3. Results
from four cultures (108 Jewish Israelis, 59 Arab Israelis, 60 Ameri-
cans, and 62 Indians) described behaviors that they thought were Hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to assess un-
intrusive in romantic relationships. The answers were categorized ique and interactive effects of gender, attachment anxiety, and
according to content similarity by three independent judges (see avoidant attachment on each of the intrusiveness-related scale
Lavy, 2006; Lavy et al., 2009, for details). A 97-item questionnaire scores. In the first step of the analysis, gender (dummy coded 1
based on these categories of intrusive behavior was distributed to for men and +1 for women) and z scores for anxious and avoidant
151 new participants who were asked to rate how much each of attachment were introduced as predictors. In the second step, the
the items described intrusiveness in a romantic relationship (fol- interactions between gender and each of the two attachment inse-
lowing Fehr and Russell’s (1984) studies of category prototypes). curity variables were introduced. To avoid inflation of Type I error
The 25 descriptions that received the highest ratings were consid- due to the large number of regressions computed, we set a more
ered to be the most prototypical examples of intrusiveness in conservative significance level and here report regression coeffi-
romantic relationships (e.g., ‘‘I look through my partner’s personal cients as significant only if their significance level was less than
things without having his/her permission”). a = .01.
In the present study, participants rated on a scale ranging from
1 (never) to 7 (all the time) how often they displayed each of the 25 3.1. Self-reports of intrusive behavior
prototypical intrusive behaviors (found by Lavy, 2006) in their own
romantic relationship.1 The alpha for the 25-item questionnaire was As expected, the regression analysis for participants’ reports of
.91. Perceptions of a partner’s intrusiveness were assessed with a intrusive behaviors indicated that more anxiously attached partic-
parallel 25-item questionnaire, following appropriate reworking of ipants reported significantly higher levels of intrusiveness (see Ta-
the items (e.g., ‘‘My partner looks through my personal things with- ble 2). No other main effects or interactions were significant.
out my permission”). Alpha for this scale was also .91.
Reactions to a partner’s intrusiveness and the subjective experi- 3.2. Perceptions of partner’s intrusiveness
ences of being intrusive were assessed with two additional ques-
tionnaires developed for this study. In the first stage of scale The regression analysis for participants’ perceptions of their
development, participants from four cultures (65 Jewish Israelis, partners’ intrusiveness yielded a significant main effect of avoidant
59 Arab Israelis, 60 Americans, and 62 Indians) described their attachment (see Table 2). As expected, more avoidant participants
reactions when their partner was intrusive, and their feelings and perceived their partners to be more intrusive. Unexpectedly, how-
thoughts when they were intrusive. The answers were categorized ever, attachment anxiety was also positively associated with per-
according to content similarity by three independent judges (see ceived partner intrusiveness. Gender had no significant main or
Lavy, 2006, for details). Based on these classifications, one ques- interactive effects.
tionnaire was constructed for reactions to intrusive behavior and Another result, not shown in Table 2, was that self-reported
the other was constructed to assess the subjective experiences of intrusiveness and perceived partner intrusiveness were substan-
being intrusive, with 36 and 33 items, respectively. At the begin- tially correlated: r(154) = .63, p < .01. Further analyses revealed
ning of each questionnaire, participants were asked to describe a that the correlation was not moderated by gender or attachment
real example of their own or their partner’s intrusive behavior scores. When the analysis for perceived partner intrusiveness
and then to rate (on a 1–7 scale) how much each item reflected was recomputed, controlling statistically for the participants’
the way they felt or behaved during the remembered situation. own self-reported intrusiveness, the beta coefficient for attach-
The questionnaires were distributed to 460 Israeli participants ment anxiety was reduced from .21 (p < .01) to .03 (ns). This con-
(198 men, 262 women). An exploratory factor analysis of reactions trol did not eliminate the significant effect for avoidance,
to partner intrusiveness yielded 7 factors (with eigenvalues >1) however (the beta changed from .27 to .22, both ps < .01).
explaining 55.1% of the variance. The factors were named as fol-
lows: anger and hurt (9 items), confusion and helplessness (5
3.3. Reactions to partner intrusiveness
items), regret (6 items), negative thoughts about the partner (7
items), acceptance and giving in (4 items), verbal confrontation
Hierarchical regression analyses revealed significant associa-
(3 items), and ignoring the situation (3 items). The percentage of
tions between attachment anxiety and higher scores on anger and
hurt feelings, confusion and helplessness, negative thoughts about
1
The questionnaires can be obtained from the first author, and can also be found in the intrusive partner, and acceptance and giving in (see Table 2).
Lavy (2006). In contrast, avoidant attachment was significantly associated with
S. Lavy et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 552–556 555

Table 2
Standardized regression coefficients predicting intrusiveness-related variables from gender and attachment scores.

Gender Anxiety Avoidance Gen  Anx Gen  Avo F (7,148) R2 (%)


** **
Intrusive behavior .07 .44 .02 .05 .18 6.87 24.5
Perceived partner intrusiveness .15 .21** .27** .04 .05 4.34** 17.1
Reactions to partner’s intrusiveness
Anger and hurt .12 .35** .01 .07 .08 4.32** 16.9
Confusion and helplessness .09 .41** .04 .01 .03 5.52** 20.7
Regret .07 .14 .29** .12 .06 2.71** 11.4
Negative thoughts about the partner .03 .29** .21** .10 .18 4.60** 17.9
Accepting and giving in .16 .31** .08 .10 .05 3.36** 13.7
Verbal confrontation .03 .04 .33** .10 .09 2.84** 11.8
Ignoring .14 .09 .26** .02 .08 3.60** 14.6
Subjective experiences of being intrusive
Regret .12 .36** .01 .01 .02 4.16** 16.5
Anger .01 .31** .28** .06 .02 4.17** 16.6
Relationship insecurity .09 .47** .07 .02 .07 7.53** 26.4
Care and concern for partner .19 .16 .16 .08 .07 7.56** 25.8
Distancing and helplessness .10 .33** .13 .01 .02 4.64** 18.1
Feeling appreciated .14 .24** .05 .13 .13 2.78 7.8

Notes: Gen = Gender; Anx = Anxiety; Avo = Avoidance.


**
p < .01.

higher scores on negative thoughts about the intrusive partner and Regarding the second research question, we also hypothesized
disregarding/ignoring the intrusion, and with lower scores on that men would perceive their partners as more intrusive than wo-
regretting the incident and verbally confronting the partner. No men. However, the association between gender and perceived
main or interactive effects of gender were found. These results fully partner intrusiveness was not significant. It is possible that the fail-
supported our predictions. ure to find the expected gender differences resulted from the use of
an explicit, self-report measure for assessing perceptions of partner
3.4. The subjective experience of being intrusive responsiveness. Future studies should examine these gender differ-
ences using implicit measures that eliminate self-report biases,
Hierarchical regression analyses revealed significant main ef- such as social desirability or effects of gender-related norms and
fects of attachment anxiety on five of the six dimensions (see expectations.
Table 2). The higher a participant’s attachment anxiety, the stron- Our third research question concerned associations between
ger were his or her cognitive and emotional responses while being attachment insecurities and reactions to partner intrusiveness.
intrusive toward a partner. Avoidant attachment was significantly We hypothesized that anxious individuals would report strong po-
associated only with experiencing less anger. No main or interac- sitive and negative emotional reactions, whereas avoidant individ-
tive effects of gender were found. uals would report more distancing reactions. These hypotheses
were fully supported: As expected, attachment anxiety was associ-
4. Discussion ated with stronger anger, hurt, confusion, helplessness, and nega-
tive thoughts about one’s partner as well as greater acceptance
We explored four research questions concerning associations and willingness to give in. People who score high on attachment
between attachment insecurity dimensions and intrusiveness in anxiety are strongly affected by their desires for security and close-
romantic relationships. Our first question concerned associations ness, which cause them to focus on the potential rewards of inti-
between attachment scores and intrusive behaviors. As expected, macy (e.g., Hazan & Shaver, 1987). They are also affected,
anxiously attached participants reported being more intrusive, however, by fears of rejection and abandonment and by memories
probably because of chronic worries about their relationships of frustrating attachment relationships, which lead them to over-
and their value in their partners’ eyes. It is possible that when anx- emphasize their partners’ potentially negative traits and intentions
ious people feel threatened with rejection or abandonment, they (see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007, for a review). Both of these views
intrude into a relationship partner’s personal ‘‘space,” often making are conducive to strong relational ambivalence (Mikulincer, Sha-
matters worse rather than better. ver, Bar-On, & Ein-Dor, in press) and mental confusion about one’s
Our second question concerned associations between attach- reactions to a partner’s intrusiveness – anger and hurt feelings or
ment scores and perceptions of partner intrusiveness. Our hypoth- acceptance and giving in.
esis that avoidant participants would perceive their partners as In line with our hypothesis, we found that more avoidant indi-
more intrusive was supported by the data, suggesting that avoid- viduals reacted to intrusiveness with distancing strategies, both
ant individuals’ need for independence results in heightened sensi- psychological (having negative thoughts about their partner and
tivity to their partners’ intrusiveness. However, we also found an not feeling regret) and behavioral (ignoring and not verbally con-
unexpected association between attachment anxiety and per- fronting the partner). These responses seem to help avoidant peo-
ceived partner intrusiveness. This association became marginal ple restore their sense of autonomy by emphasizing psychological
and nonsignificant when we controlled for participants’ own self- boundaries between themselves and their partners.
reported intrusiveness. These findings may have something to do Our fourth question concerned associations between attach-
with dyadic cycles of intrusiveness in pursuer–pursuer relation- ment insecurities and subjective experiences of being intrusive.
ships (as mentioned by Bartholomew & Allison, 2006), when one We predicted that anxiously attached people would experience
anxious partner’s intrusive behavior increases the other’s intru- more negative thoughts and feelings when being intrusive, because
siveness, generating a vicious cycle. This possibility should be their intrusive behavior would be motivated by fear of rejection
examined in future studies of couples. and lack of partner affection. The findings fully supported this pre-
556 S. Lavy et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 552–556

diction; attachment anxiety was associated with all of the negative Future studies should also examine associations between the
experiential reactions we assessed: anger, regret, insecurity, and various facets of intrusiveness and relationship quality. Previous
helplessness. However, attachment anxiety was also associated studies have reported links between intrusiveness and relationship
with a sense of being appreciated by a partner, probably reflecting distress (e.g., Betchen, 2005) and between attachment insecurities
attachment-anxious people’s gratitude when intrusive responses and relationship dissatisfaction (see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007, for
result in a partner’s attention and concern. Avoidant attachment a review). Therefore, future studies should examine the possible
was negatively associated with anger, probably reflecting the sup- role of intrusiveness as a mediator of the link between attachment
pression of negative emotions that can activate attachment needs insecurities and relationship quality. With further research to
despite one’s wish to remain self-reliant (Mikulincer & Shaver, guide them, couple therapists can create effective interventions
2007). In addition, anger implies emotional involvement in a rela- to deal with aspects of intrusiveness that interfere with relation-
tionship, and such involvement may undermine an avoidant per- ship satisfaction.
son’s pursuit of autonomy and emotional distance (Mikulincer &
Shaver, 2007). Another possibility is that for avoidant individuals, References
infrequent intrusive behaviors are not caused or associated with
anger but are motivated by a genuine concern for the safety or Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
well-being of the partner in cases where there is a good external Bartholomew, K., & Allison, C. J. (2006). An attachment perspective on abusive
reason to behave intrusively. dynamics in intimate relationships. In M. Mikulincer & G. S. Goodman (Eds.),
In general, the findings indicate that both anxious and avoidant Dynamics of romantic love (pp. 102–127). New York: Guilford Press.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults:
attachment are associated with imbalances of closeness and dis- A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61,
tance. Anxious individuals tend to overemphasize the importance 226–244.
of closeness, causing them sometimes to become intrusive, Betchen, S. J. (2005). Intrusive partners – Elusive mates: The pursuer-distancer dynamic
in couples. New York: Routledge.
demanding, and coercive. When they are intrusive, they are very Betchen, S., & Ross, J. (2000). Male pursuers and female distancers in couple therapy.
emotional, and when their partner is intrusive they fail to under- Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 15, 15–31.
stand, and they experience strong negative emotions despite their Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic
Books.
own tendency to be intrusive. Avoidant individuals emphasize the
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
importance of emotional distance and self-reliance, leading them attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.),
to perceive their partners’ proximity-seeking behavior as intrusive. Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford Press.
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (2008). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
However, avoidant individuals’ tendency to ignore their partner
clinical applications (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
may cause the partner to become intrusive, which in turn may Dutton, D. G. (2007). The abusive personality (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
cause avoidant individuals to distance themselves further from Feeney, J. A. (1999). Issues of closeness and distance in dating relationships: Effects
their partners, which then may cause the partners to become even of sex and attachment style. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16,
571–590.
more intrusive. This self-perpetuating cycle may be the key to pur- Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1991). Attachment style and verbal descriptions of
suer–distancer relationships (e.g., Betchen, 2005), which can de- romantic partners. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 187–215.
stroy a couple’s relationship. Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype
perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464–486.
The present study is the first to examine the issue of intrusive- Fogarty, T. (1979). The distancer and the pursuer. The Family, 7, 11–16.
ness in light of adult attachment theory (Mikulincer & Shaver, Green, R. J., & Werner, P. D. (1996). Intrusiveness and closeness-caregiving:
2007), using newly developed measures of intrusive behavior, per- Rethinking the concept of family ‘‘enmeshment”. Family Process, 35, 115–136.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment
ceptions of partner intrusive behavior, reactions to a partner’s process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511–524.
intrusive behavior, and subjective experiences of being intrusive. Lavy, S. (2006). Expressions and consequences of intrusiveness in adult romantic
Future studies should evaluate and refine these new measures relationships: An attachment theory perspective. Doctoral dissertation. Bar Ilan
University, Israel.
and explore their connections with other theoretically relevant
Lavy, S., Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Gillath, O. (2009). Intrusiveness in romantic
constructs, such as relationship satisfaction, commitment, inti- relationships: A cross-cultural perspective on imbalances between proximity
macy, and partner responsiveness. In addition, studies should and autonomy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 26, 989–1008.
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/
examine the external validity of the new measures using data from
disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation. In M. T. Greenberg, D.
both members of a couple and from external observers, such as Cicchetti, & M. E. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory,
friends or family members. This is crucial for determining, for research, and intervention (pp. 121–160). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
example, if the association between avoidant attachment and per- Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2000). Exploring individual differences in reactions to
ceived partner intrusiveness reflects avoidant individuals’ sensitiv- mortality salience: Does attachment style regulate terror management
ity to proximity-seeking behaviors or their partners’ actual mechanisms? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 260–273.
intrusiveness. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics,
and change. New York: Guilford Press.
Couple studies will also be important for uncovering the rela- Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Bar-On, N., & Ein-Dor, T. (in press). The pushes and
tional dynamics underlying intrusiveness. Since relational dynam- pulls of close relationships: Attachment insecurities and relational
ics are influenced by interactions between both partners’ ambivalence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.
Pistole, M. (1994). Adult attachment styles: Some thoughts on closeness–distance
attachment insecurities, intrusiveness and the ways it is experi- struggles. Family Process, 33, 147–159.
enced are also probably effected by these kinds of interactions. Prager, K. J., & Roberts, L. J. (2004). Deep intimate connection: Self and intimacy in
Our findings indicate that anxious and avoidant individuals differ couple relationships. In D. J. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), Handbook of closeness and
intimacy (pp. 43–60). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
in their reactions to a partner’s intrusiveness and their subjective
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of
experience of being intrusive. Discussing these reactions and expe- intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist,
riences openly within couple therapy may be useful for treating 55, 68–78.
Savage, E. (2000). Breathing room. New Harbinger Publications.
couples with a pursuer–distancer pattern of relationship.
Schmitt, D. P. (2008). How ecological stressors influence dismissing orientations
Intrusiveness experienced in pursuer–distancer relationships is across genders and geographies. Cross-Cultural Research, 42, 220–247.
probably different from that experienced in pursuer–pursuer rela- Werner, P. D., Green, R. J., Greenberg, J., Browne, T. L., & McKenna, T. E. (2001).
tionships (usually composed of anxiously attached partners). In the Beyond enmeshment: Evidence for the independence of intrusiveness and
closeness-caregiving in married couples. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy,
latter case, intrusiveness is probably an unfortunate outcome of a 27, 459–471.
vicious cycle of disrespect for a partner’s autonomy.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen