Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

Personality and Individual Differences 72 (2015) 208–213

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Attachment anxiety and implicit self-concept of neuroticism:


Associations in women but not men
Uta-Susan Donges a, Anne Jachmann a, Anette Kersting a, Boris Egloff b, Thomas Suslow a,c,⇑
a
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, University of Leipzig, Semmelweisstr. 10, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
b
Department of Psychology, University of Mainz, Binger Str. 14-16, 55122 Mainz, Germany
c
Department of Psychiatry, University of Münster, A.-Schweitzer-Campus 1, 48149 Münster, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Previous research has shown that adult attachment-related anxiety is associated with the explicit self-
Received 16 July 2014 concept of neuroticism. It remains to be clarified whether attachment anxiety is related to the implicit
Received in revised form 26 August 2014 self-concept of neuroticism. There is evidence that gender can moderate the strength of correlation
Accepted 28 August 2014
between implicit and explicit measures. The Experiences in Close Relationships scale and the NEO Five-
Available online 29 September 2014
Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) were administered to 106 healthy adults along with an Implicit Association Test
assessing neuroticism. Attachment anxiety correlated with NEO-FFI neuroticism, regardless of gender.
Keywords:
Attachment anxiety was correlated with neuroticism as measured by the IAT in women but not men.
Attachment style
Implicit measure of personality
Results from multiple regression analysis showed that gender was a moderator of the relationship
Neuroticism between attachment anxiety and implicit neuroticism. Our data suggest that in the associative network
Gender of women with high attachment anxiety associative representations of the self are more strongly linked
Implicit Association Test to attributes of neuroticism compared to women with low attachment anxiety.
Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction and can produce a hyper-activation of the attachment behavioral


system leading to anxious or controlling attempts to force partners
From the beginning, attachment theory was conceived as a to pay more attention and provide better care (Mikulincer &
general theory of human personality development. It was argued Shaver, 2009).
that infants form the internal working models of their selves In comparison, attachment-related avoidance relates to tenden-
mainly through non-verbal interactions with primary caregivers cies to use avoidant versus proximity-seeking strategies to regulate
(Bowlby, 1969). It was one of Bowlby’s key ideas that working attachment-related behaviors and thoughts. Avoidant people are
models of self (and others) are stored in the form of mental repre- unwilling to rely on others and withdraw from close relationships.
sentations. The internal working models were thought to be not They appear to deactivate their attachment behavioral system
necessarily accessible to consciousness or introspection, as they which is associated with a down-regulation of emotions and a
become habitual and operate in an automatic fashion (Bretherton low intensity of emotionality (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). Attach-
& Munholland, 1999). According to recent models of attachment ment security can be defined as the absence of high levels of anx-
organization anxiety and avoidance can be identified as the two iety and avoidance (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). Securely
basic dimensions underlying adult attachment (Fraley & Shaver, attached individuals have positive models of others, share feelings
2000). with others and enjoy intimate relationships (Bartholomew &
Attachment-related anxiety refers to the extent to which indi- Shaver, 1998).
viduals are vigilant to attachment-related cues. They have fears Neuroticism is a core dimension of most models of personality
of being abandoned and a desire to be close to partners (Fraley & structure (Costa & McCrae, 1995; Eysenck, 1967). It is related to
Shaver, 2000). A long series of frustrating and disappointing inter- emotional instability and is defined as the general tendency to
actions with attachment figures erodes reliance on support seeking experience negative affective states such as fear, sadness, embar-
rassment, anger, and guilt. However, despite the important role
of neuroticism in personality research, there is no consensus on
⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, University of
its definition among experts (Ormel, Riese, & Rosmalen, 2012).
Leipzig, Semmelweisstr. 10, 04103 Leipzig, Germany. Tel.: +49 341 97 18891; fax:
+49 341 97 18849. Widely used personality questionnaires such as the NEO Personal-
E-mail address: thomas.suslow@medizin.uni-leipzig.de (T. Suslow). ity Inventory – Revised and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (Costa &

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.043
0191-8869/Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
U.-S. Donges et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 72 (2015) 208–213 209

McCrae, 1992) assess anxiety, angry hostility, depression, self- the implicit self-concept of neuroticism and to study the moderat-
consciousness, vulnerability, and impulsiveness in the measure- ing role of gender in this association. Adult attachment dimensions
ment of neuroticism. According to Widiger (2009) there is most (i.e. anxiety and avoidance) were measured by the Experiences in
agreement among personality researchers that anxiety–with- Close Relationships scale (ECR; Brennan et al., 1998). An Implicit
drawal, depression–unhappiness, and vulnerability–stress reaction Association Test (IAT) was administered to assess participants’
belong to the neuroticism domain. implicit self-concept of neuroticism. The NEO-FFI (Costa &
The existing literature on the relation between attachment anx- McCrae, 1992) was applied to measure the explicit self-concept of
iety and the ‘‘Big Five’’ personality traits suggests that attachment- neuroticism. Since gender might moderate the association between
related anxiety is consistently associated with neuroticism. In explicit and implicit measures as pointed out above correlation
more than 30 studies significant correlations between attachment analyses were calculated separately for men and women. It was
anxiety and neuroticism have been found (see Mikulincer & hypothesized that women would manifest stronger associations
Shaver, 2007, for an overview). In contrast, for attachment avoid- between attachment anxiety and implicitly measured neuroticism
ance less significant correlations with neuroticism were observed. compared with men. To formally test the moderator effect of gender
Moreover, the correlations between attachment anxiety and neu- we calculated multiple regression analyses. Since depressed mood
roticism were generally higher (on average about r = .40) whereas and intelligence have been shown to be related to neuroticism we
the correlations between avoidance and neuroticism were on aver- decided to measure and control these variables in our research con-
age only around r = .20. Research on the relation between attach- text (e.g., DeYoung, 2011; Hirschfeld et al., 1983).
ment anxiety and the ‘‘Big Five’’ personality traits shows also
that attachment anxiety is most strongly associated with neuroti-
2. Method
cism compared to other personality dimensions (e.g., Picardi,
Caroppo, Toni, Bitetti, & Di Maria, 2005).
2.1. Participants
According to Back, Schmukle, and Egloff (2009) self-reported
personality traits can be conceptualized as reflecting differences
One-hundred and six healthy subjects (57 women) volunteered
in propositional or conscious representations of the self (e.g., ‘‘I
to participate (the age range was between 18 and 38 years). For all
get stressed out easily’’) that result from the typical functioning
participants, exclusion criteria were a history of neurological or
of reflective processes in situations that provoke meaningful differ-
psychiatric disease, current substance abuse or use of any psycho-
ences in how people act. In the case of neuroticism, these situa-
tropic medication. All subjects were white and attested to being
tions can be described as stressful situations in which people
native speakers of German. Participants were recruited via public
differentially behave anxiously or do not. Individual differences
notices. Notices with a brief description of the study, the exclusion
in propositional representations of the self could be termed the
criteria and a contact telephone number were posted in several
explicit self-concept of personality (Back et al., 2009).
locations on the campus of the University of Leipzig such as
In contrast, individual differences in associative representations of
libraries and canteens. The study was conducted in accordance
the self have been termed the implicit self-concept of personality
with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the local
(Asendorpf, Banse, & Mücke, 2002). Associative representations of
University Ethics Committee. More than half of our study partici-
the self (e.g., ‘‘me’’–‘‘tense’’) are a product of repeated activation of
pants (n = 60) were university students (32 women, 28 men). 2
the self, as a concept in an associative network, together with patterns
participants (2 women) were high school students. 10 participants
of impulsive behavioral activation and automatic motivational orien-
were employees without an academic degree (6 women, 4 men). 5
tations. The implicit self-concept of personality can be measured with
participants were in vocational education (3 women, 2 men). 23
indirect tests such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald,
participants were working academics or academics in further
McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). It has been shown that implicit and
training measures (13 women, 10 men). 4 participants (4 men)
explicit self-concept are only moderately associated (Back et al.,
were unemployed academics. 2 participants had a graduation but
2009) or can even be independent from each other (Steffens &
were unemployed and not university students or in vocational
Schulze-König, 2006). It appears that women manifest stronger asso-
training measures (1 woman, 1 man).
ciations between implicit and explicit measures than men (Egloff &
Schmukle, 2004; Pelham et al., 2005). Women might have greater
insight into their over-learned affect-related associations about 2.2. Psychometric measures
themselves because they are more strongly socialized to be attuned
to their feelings and intuitions than men (Pelham et al., 2005). The Experiences in Close Relationships scale (ECR; German
The IAT is a word sorting task measuring strength of associa- version: Neumann, Rohmann, & Bierhoff, 2007) was applied to
tions between concepts by comparing reaction times. IATs assess- measure attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance. The ECR
ing neuroticism were found to predict spontaneous strained or consists of 36 items that are rated on a 7-point scale ranging from
nervous behavior in healthy people above and beyond direct mea- 1 (not at all like me) to 7 (very much like me). Eighteen items assess
sures (Back et al., 2009; Egloff & Schmukle, 2002; Schnabel, Banse, attachment anxiety and 18 items measure attachment avoidance.
& Asendorpf, 2006; Steffens & Schulze-König, 2006). Cronbach’s alphas were relatively high for the anxiety and avoid-
In the past, it has been shown repeatedly that self-reported ance scales of the ECR (.85 and .88, respectively). Low scores on
attachment style is related to spontaneous cognitive or physiolog- both dimensions indicate attachment security.
ical reactions. For example, explicit measures of attachment orien- The NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; German version:
tation have been shown to be accurate indicators of physiological Borkenau & Ostendorf, 1993) was administered as an explicit mea-
arousal during situations of separation (Feeney & Kirkpatrick, sure of neuroticism. The NEO-FFI assesses five dimensions of the
1996) or heightened automatic accessibility of mental representa- normal personality, i.e. neuroticism, extraversion, openness to
tion of attachment figures (Mikulincer, Gillath, & Shaver, 2002). experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. It presents in
According to Mikulincer and Shaver (2007) self-report measures total 60 items (12 items per scale) that participants rate on a
of attachment style are useful instruments to study implicit 5-point Likert scale (from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree)).
aspects of attachment-system functioning. The internal consistency for the neuroticism scale was .88. Depres-
The aims of the present study were to investigate for the first sivity of participants was assessed with the Beck Depression Inven-
time the relation between (self-reported) attachment anxiety and tory (BDI; German version: Hautzinger, Bailer, Worall, & Keller,
210 U.-S. Donges et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 72 (2015) 208–213

1995). The internal consistency for the BDI was .76. Verbal intelli- Table 1
gence was measured by the Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT-B Demographic and psychometric characteristics of study participants as a function of
gender (means and SDs (in brackets)).
(Lehrl, Triebig, & Fischer, 1995).
The neuroticism Implicit Association Test (IAT) was administered Variable Women (n = 57) Men (n = 49) t
on a personal computer using the Inquisit program (Draine, 2004). Age (years) 24.5 (4.3) 26.5 (3.7) 2.59*
Stimuli from the categories me (me, my, own, I, self) and others (they, School education (years) 12.2 (0.7) 12.4 (0.9) 1.28
your, them, you, others) as well as items from the categories anxiety Intelligence (MWT-B, IQ score) 111.2 (11.0) 114.3 (11.4) 1.41
Depression (BDI) 7.8 (5.3) 6.9 (4.9) 0.97
(anxious, nervous, fearful, uncertain, afraid) and calmness (calm, Attachment anxiety (ECR) 65.0 (15.3) 62.7 (17.3) 0.71
relaxed, restful, at ease, balanced) were presented. The IAT comprised Attachment avoidance (ECR) 51.8 (17.1) 49.9 (14.4) 0.63
five blocks. In the first block, comprising 20 trials (each item was Neuroticism (IAT, D score) 0.37 (0.36) 0.44 (0.35) 1.02
presented twice), participants practiced discriminating items Neuroticism (NEO-FFI) 33.6 (8.6) 29.9 (7.4) 2.35*
Extraversion (NEO-FFI) 40.8 (6.0) 40.0 (6.7) 0.63
belonging to the category me from those belonging to the category
Openness (NEO-FFI) 44.7 (6.5) 46.2 (5.7) 1.23
others (target discrimination). In Block 2, the same procedure was Agreeableness (NEO-FFI) 44.3 (5.7) 41.8 (5.4) 2.31*
conducted with respect to attribute discrimination; participants Conscientiousness (NEO-FFI) 43.5 (7.1) 40.0 (6.8) 2.57*
were required to sort items into anxiety and calmness categories. *
Significant difference at p < .05 (two-tailed).
The critical Blocks 3 and 5 each consisted of 60 trials. In these trials,
participants categorized items into two combined categories, each
including the attribute and the target concept assigned to the same
the more neurotic the estimated implicit self-concept of a given
key. In Block 3 (me + anxiety block) the categories me and anxiety
participant.
were assigned to the left key, whereas the categories others and
calmness were assigned to the right key. In Block 5 (me + calmness
2.3. Procedure
block), the categories anxiety and calmness switched keys. Attribute
and target stimuli were presented alternately in a randomized order
Testing sessions were conducted in a quiet room free from audi-
for each participant. In Block 4, this switched key assignment of
tory and visual distractions. First, participants completed the ECR.
attribute discrimination was practiced (20 trials).
Then the IAT was conducted. A laptop (Dell Latitude E6510) was
Participants were instructed that they would be required to
used for stimulus presentation and response registration. Then
make a series of category judgments. In each trial, a stimulus word
subjects were given the MWT-B, the BDI, and the NEO-FFI.
was displayed in the center of a computer screen. Category labels
were shown on the left- and right-hand sides of the window. Par-
ticipants used the letter q on the left-hand side of the keyboard and 3. Results
the letter p on the right-hand side for their answers. They were fur-
ther told: ‘‘Please try to be as accurate though also as quick as pos- 3.1. Psychometric measures: comparison between women and men
sible. If your selection is incorrect, you will see a red X. To continue
to the next judgment, you must make the correct selection.’’ To According to t-tests for independent samples women were
facilitate fast responses participants had to keep their index fingers younger than men (see Table 1 for details). However, age was
on the q and p keys throughout the experiment. Intertrial intervals not significantly correlated with attachment anxiety, neuroticism
had a duration of 150 ms. Time between the start of each stimulus (NEO-FFI), or IAT neuroticism (ps > .05). Women did not differ from
presentation and correct responses were recorded. After each block men with respect to education, intelligence, attachment anxiety,
error rates and mean latencies were displayed. attachment avoidance, depression, and neuroticism as measured
IAT scores were calculated by using the D algorithm by the IAT. Women had higher scores on the neuroticism scale of
(Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003): (a) trials with reaction times the NEO-FFI than men. Moreover, women showed higher agree-
greater than 10,000 ms were eliminated (in our sample in total ableness and conscientiousness compared with men (see Table 1).
only 9 trials); (b) error trials were included in the analysis by using According to Pearson’s Chi-square (v2(7) = 6.70, p = .46) there was
the latency between stimulus presentation and correct response no relationship between gender and employment status.
(built-in error penalty); (c) mean latency for the critical trials of
the me + anxiety block was subtracted from the mean latency for 3.2. Correlations between psychometric measures in the total sample
the critical trials of the me + calmness block; (d) the IAT effect
was computed by dividing this difference by the individual respon- In Table 2 we present product-moment correlations between the
dent reaction time standard deviation. The higher the IAT D scores ECR, NEO-FFI, IAT, BDI and MWT-B in our sample. ECR attachment

Table 2
Product-moment correlations between psychometric measures in the whole sample.

ECR NEO-FFI
Avoid. IAT N E O A C BDI MWT
Attachment anxiety (ECR) .16 .14 .55** .10 .06 .14 .20° .27* .11
Attachment avoidance 1 .02 .24° .12 .04 .13 .28* .38** .14
Neuroticism (IAT) 1 .03 .11 .04 .05 .01 .04 .00
Neuroticism (NEO-FFI) 1 .44** .09 .17 .25° .54** .12
Extraversion (NEO-FFI) 1 .05 .13 .17 .31** .06
Openness (NEO-FFI) 1 .06 .17 .23° .20°
Agreeableness (NEO-FFI) 1 .23° .29* .06
Conscientiousness (NEO-FFI) 1 .36** .05
Depression (BDI) 1 .09
Intelligence (MWT-B, IQ score) 1
°
p < .05.
*
p < .01.
**
p < .001 (two-tailed).
U.-S. Donges et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 72 (2015) 208–213 211

anxiety was significantly correlated with NEO-FFI neuroticism but 3.4. Tests for interaction between explicit and implicit neuroticism and
not with IAT neuroticism. Moreover, attachment anxiety was neg- gender in multiple regression analyses
atively correlated with conscientiousness (NEO-FFI) and positively
associated with depressivity (BDI). The interaction between implicit (and explicit) neuroticism and
Attachment avoidance was significantly correlated with NEO- gender in predicting attachment anxiety was formally tested using
FFI neuroticism but not with IAT neuroticism in the whole sample. hierarchical regression analyses. In the first model, following the
In addition, attachment avoidance was also negatively correlated inclusion of explicit neuroticism and gender (Step 1) the interac-
with conscientiousness (NEO-FFI) and positively associated with tion of explicit neuroticism with gender was entered into the
depressivity (BDI). regression analysis in Step 2. Explicit neuroticism significantly
Implicit neuroticism (IAT) was not related to any of the admin- explained variations in attachment anxiety. Having higher scores
istered psychometric measures. In contrast, explicit neuroticism of explicit neuroticism predicted higher attachment anxiety. No
was negatively related to extraversion and conscientiousness significant interaction between explicit neuroticism and gender
(NEO-FFI) and positively correlated with depressivity. was found in the prediction of attachment anxiety (see Table 3).
In the second model, after the inclusion of implicit neuroticism
3.3. Relations of attachment styles with implicit and explicit and gender (Step 1) the interaction of implicit neuroticism with
neuroticism as a function of gender gender was entered (Step 2). Implicit neuroticism and gender did
not predict attachment anxiety but the interaction between impli-
In the sample of women ECR attachment anxiety correlated sig- cit neuroticism and gender was significant (see Table 4). This indi-
nificantly with the NEO-FFI neuroticism score (r = .58, p < .001) cates that the association between neuroticism as measured by the
and with the IAT neuroticism score (r = .36, p < .01) (see Fig. 1). IAT and attachment anxiety differed as a function of gender.
Importantly, the correlation between attachment anxiety and IAT
neuroticism remained significant after controlling for NEO-FFI
4. Discussion
neuroticism (r = .40, p < .01). For men, ECR attachment anxiety
correlated also significantly with the NEO-FFI neuroticism score
The objectives of our study were to examine the relation
(r = .51, p < .001) but not with IAT neuroticism (r = .09, p = .53).
between attachment anxiety and the implicit self-concept of neu-
Using the Fisher r-to-z transformation, we observed a significant dif-
roticism and to investigate the moderating role of gender in this
ference between the two correlation coefficients between ECR
context. Our correlation data suggest that self-reported attachment
attachment anxiety and IAT neuroticism for men and women
anxiety is associated with neuroticism as measured by the IAT in
(z = 2.3, p = .02 (two-tailed)). IAT neuroticism was not significantly
women but not men. As hypothesized, according to the present
related to NEO-FFI neuroticism neither for women nor for men
results attachment anxiety is more strongly associated with the
(r = .04 and .20, p > .05, respectively).
implicit self-concept of neuroticism in women compared with
In the sample of women attachment avoidance was correlated
men. Our results from multiple regression analysis indicate that
with NEO-FFI neuroticism (r = .32, p < .05) but not with IAT neurot-
gender is a significant moderator of the relationship between
icism (r = .09, p = .51). Finally, for men attachment avoidance was
attachment anxiety and implicit neuroticism. It can be concluded
neither correlated with NEO-FFI neuroticism (r = .10, p = .49) nor
that the association between attachment anxiety and neuroticism
with IAT neuroticism (r = .09, p = .55).
as measured by the IAT differed as a function of gender. For
For men, there was a significant difference between the
women, a significant positive correlation of medium size was
correlations of attachment anxiety and avoidance with NEO-FFI
observed but for men no significant correlation between attach-
neuroticism (Steigers z = 2.20, p = .03 (two-tailed)). For women, a
ment anxiety and implicit neuroticism was found.
marginally significant difference between the correlations of
attachment anxiety and avoidance with NEO-FFI neuroticism was
found (Steigers z = 1.78, p = .08 (two-tailed)). Thus, attachment Table 3
anxiety tended to be more strongly associated with explicit neu- Hierarchical multiple repression analyses with attachment anxiety as the dependent
variable and explicit neuroticism (NEO-FFI) and gender as predictors.
roticism than attachment avoidance.
Attachment anxiety DR2
b R2
Step 1
Explicit neuroticism .559* .302 .302*
Gender .056
Step 2
Explicit neuroticism  Gender .141 .303 .001
*
p < .001.

Table 4
Hierarchical multiple repression analyses with attachment anxiety as the dependent
variable and implicit neuroticism (IAT) and gender as predictors.

Attachment anxiety DR2


2
b R
Step 1
Implicit neuroticism .056 .024 .024
Gender .139
Step 2
Implicit neuroticism  Gender .391* .070 .046*
Fig. 1. Scatter plot depicting the correlation between ECR attachment anxiety and
*
IAT neuroticism (D) score (r = .36, p < .01) in the sample of women (n = 57). p < .05.
212 U.-S. Donges et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 72 (2015) 208–213

This means that women with high attachment anxiety differ icism. Another possible explanation which does not exclude the pre-
from less anxious women in the associative representations of ceding ones refers to different gender roles regarding the experience
their selves with regard to neuroticism-related characteristics. and expression of anxiety and insecurity. The expression of anxiety
According to our results, in the associative network of women with appears to be inconsistent with the traditional male gender role
high attachment anxiety associative representations of the self are (McLean & Anderson, 2009). High femininity and low masculinity
more strongly linked to attributes of neuroticism such as anxious, have been found to be related to elevated fear (Carey, Dusek, &
nervous, uncertain, afraid, or fearful compared to women with low Spector, 1988). From this perspective, it is possible that men are
attachment anxiety. more inclined to protect their self-concept from hints that they
Independently of gender, we observed relatively high positive could be anxious or insecure compared to women. Against this
correlations between attachment anxiety and the explicit background, it seems advisable to administer gender role scales
self-concept of neuroticism. That is, anxiously attached individuals and measures of private self-consciousness in future studies on
differed from less anxious persons in their conscious representa- attachment anxiety and implicit or explicit neuroticism.
tion of the self with regard to neuroticism-related characteristics The main finding of our study is the association of self-reported
such as proneness to show anxious, hostile, or depressed reactions. attachment anxiety with the implicit self-concept of neuroticism in
This finding is consistent with that of many previous studies in the women. We administered the IAT as implicit measure of self-
field (see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Moreover, we found evi- concept which is based on information that is not intentionally
dence that attachment anxiety tended to be more strongly related given to inform about the self. Our data indicate that at least in
to self-reported neuroticism than attachment avoidance which women associative representations of the self are linked to neurot-
also replicates previous findings. For example, neuroticism as mea- icism-related attributes (i.e. anxious, nervous, or uncertain).
sured by the NEO-PI showed significant correlations with attach- According to Back et al. (2009) chronic associative links between
ment anxiety as well as attachment avoidance but correlations associative network elements are the result of the typical function-
were stronger for attachment anxiety (Davis & Vernon, 2002; ing of impulsive processes (i.e. how situational cues are automati-
Noftle & Shaver, 2003). cally perceived and what behaviors are automatically performed).
It has been reported previously that women manifest stronger Associative representations of the self such as ‘‘me’’–‘‘anxious’’
associations between implicit and explicit measures than men can be in part the consequence of repeated experiences of unreli-
(e.g., Egloff & Schmukle, 2004). In this context, it was proposed able and insufficiently responsive attachment figures during child-
by Pelham et al. (2005) that women could be more strongly social- hood. Experiences with primary caregivers who are unable to allow
ized to pay attention to their feelings than men so that they might steps towards independence and risk-taking and who are exces-
develop greater insight into the affect-related associations about sively protective of their child could also represent important fac-
themselves. Gschwender, Hoffmann, and Schmitt (2006) argued tors relevant for the development of anxiety (e.g., Ainsworth,
that women’s habitual preoccupations could result in an increased Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). Anxious attachment is associated
introspective accessibility of their affective response tendencies with exaggeration of threats and life demands and an inability to
and traits. Women could ruminate more on anxiety evoking situa- cope autonomously with them (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). The
tions and anxiety reactions than men. Egloff, Weck, and Schmukle above-mentioned adverse experiences with attachment figures
(2008) showed that thinking about anxiety-arousing situations could lead to associative representations of the self linked to inse-
enhanced the correlation between implicit and explicit anxiety curity and fearfulness.
measures. Even though we observed a gender specific relation Several further limitations of our investigation have to be
between explicitly measured attachment anxiety and implicit neu- acknowledged. Sample consisted only of rather young adults with
roticism we found no correlations between explicitly and implic- rather high intelligence and education. This clearly limits the gen-
itly measured neuroticism, regardless of gender. Independence of eralizability of our findings. Against this background, the present
explicit and implicit personality measures has been reported in results are in need of confirmation by future studies performed
other studies (Steffens & Schulze-König, 2006). However, the latter on larger, more representative samples. Moreover, in our study
finding appears contradictory to the assumption that women man- we administered only a self-report or direct psychometric instru-
ifest associations between implicit and explicit personality mea- ment (i.e., the ECR) to assess attachment orientation. It seems
sures. An explanation for the observed pattern of correlation advisable to use indirect measures of attachment style in future
findings in women (i.e. correlation of implicit neuroticism with studies on implicit attachment-system functioning such as the
explicit attachment anxiety but not with explicit neuroticism) Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George, Kaplan, & Main, 1985)
could be that the IAT applied in our study assessed primarily the or the Adult Attachment Projective (AAP, George & West, 2001).
anxiety facet of neuroticism so that on a phenomenological level The AAI is a semi-structured interview for assessing adults’ strate-
similarity with attachment-related anxiety could be higher than gies for identifying, preventing, and protecting the self from dan-
with the neuroticism construct as measured by the NEO-FFI which gers tied to intimate relationships. The AAI taps into adult
encompasses more facets of neuroticism (e.g., hostility, depression, representation of attachment by assessing general and specific
and impulsivity). recollections from their childhood. The interview is coded based
Our correlational data do not allow us to make any inferences on content and quality of discourse. The AAP is a free-response
about the mechanisms underlying the differential association pat- psychological assessment of adult attachment consisting of draw-
tern between men and women concerning attachment anxiety ings of attachment relevant situations. In its administration projec-
and implicit neuroticism. One possible explanation we have pro- tive and interview methods are combined. Indirect measures such
posed is that women might have greater insight into the affect- as the AAI and the AAP appear less susceptible to social desirability
related associations about themselves compared to men. However, concerns and self-serving biases and depend less on introspective
it also possible that differences in private self-consciousness abilities. It has been repeatedly shown that there is, for example,
between male and female study participants underlie the differen- only a modest correspondence between self-reports and reports
tial correlation pattern. Private self-consciousness is a tendency to of significant others and limited insight into problematic aspects
introspect and examine one’s inner self and feelings (Nystedt & of one’s characteristic patterns of interpersonal relating (e.g.,
Ljungberg, 2002; Scheier & Carver, 1977). Individuals with higher Huprich, Bornstein, & Schmitt, 2011; Shedler, Mayman, & Manis,
private self-consciousness might develop stronger links between 1993). Thus, it appears of central importance not to rely exclusively
the associative representations of the self and attributes of neurot- on self-report but to use interview or projective measures to com-
U.-S. Donges et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 72 (2015) 208–213 213

plement self-report data in research on attachment. Research Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual
differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of
based on the AAI showed that persons employing deactivating
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.
strategies report low attachment anxiety but manifest high physi- Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the
ological arousal (i.e., implicit reactions) when recalling experiences Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of
of separation and rejection (Dozier & Kobak, 1992). Indirect assess- Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.
Gschwendner, T., Hofmann, W., & Schmitt, M. (2006). Moderatoren der Konsistenz
ment instruments could be useful in revealing relations between implizit und explizit erfasster Einstellungen und Persönlichkeitsmerkmale
attachment styles and implicit self-concepts. [Moderators of the consistency of implicitly and explicitly assessed attitudes
and personality traits]. Psychologische Rundschau, 57, 13–33.
Hautzinger, M., Bailer, M., Worall, H., & Keller, F. (1995). Beck-Depressions-Inventar
Acknowledgement (BDI). Testhandbuch. [Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). Manual]. Bern: Hans
Huber.
We thank Frank Zeitschel for his help in data acquisition. Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Clayton, P. J., Keller, M. B., McDonald-Scott, P., &
Larkin, B. H. (1983). Assessing personality: Effects of the depressive state on
trait measurement. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 695–699.
References Huprich, S. K., Bornstein, R. F., & Schmitt, T. A. (2011). Self-report methodology is
insufficient for improving the assessment and classification of axis II Personality
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 25, 557–570.
attachment: Assessed in the strange situation and at home. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. Lehrl, S., Triebig, G., & Fischer, B. (1995). Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a
Asendorpf, J. B., Banse, R., & Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence. Acta Neurologica
and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Scandinavica, 91, 335–345.
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380–393. McLean, C. P., & Anderson, E. R. (2009). Brave men and timid women? A review of
Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2009). Predicting actual behavior from the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 496–505.
explicit and implicit self-concept of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the attachment
Psychology, 97, 533–548. system in adulthood: Threat-related primes increase the accessibility of mental
Bartholomew, K., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Methods of assessing adult attachment. Do representations of attachment figures. Journal of Personality and Social
they converge? In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and Psychology, 83, 881–895.
close relationships (pp. 25–45). New York: Guilford Press. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). The attachment behavioral system in
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1993). NEO-fünf-Faktoren Inventar (NEOFFI) nach Costa adulthood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. In M. P.
und McCrae [NEO-Five-Factor Inventory (NEOFFI) according to Costa and McCrae]. Zanna (Ed.). Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 53–152).
Göttingen: Hogrefe. San Diego: Academic Press.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Attachment (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics,
Brennan, K., Clark, C., & Shaver, P. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult and change. New York: Guilford Press.
attachment. An integrative overview. In J. Simpson & W. Rholes (Eds.), Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2009). An attachment and behavioral systems
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York: Guilford Press. perspective on social support. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 261,
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal working models in attachment 7–19.
relationships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook Neumann, E., Rohmann, E., & Bierhoff, H. W. (2007). Entwicklung und Validierung
of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 89–111). New York: von Skalen zur Erfassung von Vermeidung und Angst in Partnerschaften. Der
Guilford Press. Bochumer Bindungsfragebogen [Development and validation of scales for
Carey, M. P., Dusek, J. B., & Spector, I. P. (1988). Sex roles, gender and fears: A brief measuring avoidance and anxiety in romantic relationships – The Bochum
report. Phobia Practice and Research Journal, 1, 114–120. Adult Attachment Questionnaire]. Diagnostica, 53, 33–47.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO Five Noftle, E. E., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). Attachment dimensions and the big five
Factor Inventory. Odessa: P.A. Resources. personality traits: Associations and comparative ability to predict relationship
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical personality quality. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 179–208.
assessment using the revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Nystedt, L., & Ljungberg, A. (2002). Facets of private and public self-consciousness:
Assessment, 64, 21–50. Construct and discriminant validity. European Journal of Personality, 16,
Davis, D., & Vernon, M. L. (2002). Sculpting the body beautiful: Attachment style, 143–159.
neuroticism, and use of cosmetic surgeries. Sex Roles, 47, 129–138. Ormel, J., Riese, H., & Rosmalen, J. G. M. (2012). Interpreting neuroticism scores
DeYoung, C. G. (2011). Intelligence and personality. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. across the adult life course: Immutable or experience-dependent set points of
Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 711–737). New negative affect. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 71–79.
York: Cambridge University Press. Pelham, B. W., Koole, S. L., Hardin, C. D., Hetts, J. J., Seah, E., & Dehart, T. (2005).
Dozier, M., & Kobak, R. R. (1992). Psychophysiology in attachment interviews: Gender moderates the relation between implicit and explicit self-esteem.
Converging evidence for deactivating strategies. Child Development, 63, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 84–89.
1473–1480. Picardi, A., Caroppo, E., Toni, A., Bitetti, D., & Dimaria, G. (2005). Stability of
Draine, S. C. (2004). Inquisit. Version 3.0. Seattle: Millisecond Software. attachment-related anxiety and avoidance and their relationships with the five-
Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an implicit association test factor model and the psychobiological model of personality. Psychology and
for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441–1455. Psychotherapy, 78, 327–345.
Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2004). Gender differences in implicit and explicit Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1977). Self-focused attention and the experience of
anxiety measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1807–1815. emotion: Attraction, repulsion, elation, and depression. Journal of Personality
Egloff, B., Weck, F., & Schmukle, S. C. (2008). Thinking about anxiety moderates the and Social Psychology, 35, 625–636.
relationship between implicit and explicit anxiety measures. Journal of Research Schnabel, K., Banse, R., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2006). Assessment of implicit personality
in Personality, 42, 771–778. self-concept using the implicit association test (IAT): Concurrent assessment of
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield: Ch. Thomas. anxiousness and angriness. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 373–396.
Feeney, B. C., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1996). Effects of adult attachment and presence of Shedler, J., Mayman, M., & Manis, M. (1993). The illusion of mental health. American
romantic partners on physiological responses to stress. Journal of Personality and Psychologist, 48, 1117–1131.
Social Psychology, 70, 255–270. Steffens, M. C., & Schulze-König, S. (2006). Predicting spontaneous Big Five Behavior
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical with Implicit Association Tests. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22,
developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of 13–20.
General Psychology, 4, 132–154. Widiger, T. A. (2009). Neuroticism. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Adult attachment interview protocol (2nd individual differences in social behavior (pp. 129–146). New York: The Guilford
ed.). Berkeley, CA: University of California. Press.
George, C., & West, M. (2001). The development and preliminary validation of a new
measure of adult attachment: The adult attachment projective. Attachment &
Human Development, 3, 30–61.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen