Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Creativity Research Journal Copyright © 2005 by

2005, Vol. 17, No. 2 & 3, 155–166 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

The Adaptable Mind in Design: Relating Personality,


Cognitive Style, and Creative Performance
Jason Meneely
Cornell University
Margaret Portillo
University of Florida

ABSTRACT: This study examined domain-specific re- certainly we often describe their work as creative”
lationships between creative personality traits, cogni- (Lawson, 1997, p. 106). Despite these assumptions
tive styles, and creative performance in design. Design about creativity, little empirical evidence has advanced
students (n = 39) completed the Adjective Check List an explicit understanding of creative designers, their
(ACL) and the Herrmann Brain Dominance Instru- processes, and products. As Goldschmidt (1999)
ment (HBDI) to gauge personality and cognitive style, noted, “Creative design is held in great esteem, but we
respectively. The ACL was scored using Domino’s Cre- still know little about design cognition and the cogni-
ativity Scale (ACL-Cr) to identify creative personality tive abilities and strategies that contribute to creative
traits. The sample also completed a design task that design thinking” (p. 526).
was evaluated for creativity using the Consensual As- Although a need for further inquiry prevails, it is
sessment Technique (CAT). Findings indicated that commonly recognized that design problems demand
participants showing flexibility between cerebral, an integrative and holistic approach to developing so-
limbic, right, and left modes of thinking had signifi- lutions (Goldschmidt, 1999; Lawson, 1997; Rowe,
cantly higher mean scores on creative personality than 1987; Zeisel, 1981). Designers need to address dispa-
did those who exhibited a more entrenched cognitive rate artistic and technical criteria that frequently place
style. Creative personality traits (ACL-Cr) signifi- competing demands on problem solving. In recogni-
cantly predicted creative performance on the design tion of this, Mackinnon (1962) observed the following:
task. While cognitive style (HBDI) did not predict cre-
In architecture, creative products are both an expression of
ative performance, flexibility between styles was sig- the architect and thus a very personal product, and at the
nificantly correlated to the creative personality. In same time an impersonal meeting of the demands of an exter-
sum, individuals exhibiting adaptable thinking appear nal problem. … Architecture, as a field of creative endeavor,
to possess the flexibility necessary to design creatively requires that the successful practitioner be both artist and sci-
entist—artist in that his designs must fulfill the demands of
and potentially transform the domain with original and
“Delight,” and scientist in that they must meet the demands
imaginative solutions. of “Firmnesse” and “Commodity.” (pp. 485–486)

Design involves the creation of objects, spaces, places,


and other material artifacts for human use. Design is
future oriented; in fields such as architecture, interior
design, apparel design, and industrial design there is an The authors would like to acknowledge Professor Sheila Danko for
her insightful comments on the manuscript.
overriding expectation to challenge precedent by de-
Correspondence and requests for reprints should be sent to Jason
veloping original and adaptive ideas. “It is generally Meneely, Instructor, Cornell University, Design and Environmental
accepted that design is a creative occupation and that Analysis, Martha Van Rensselaer Hall, Ithaca, NY 14853–4401.
good designers are themselves creative people, and E-mail: jm349@cornell.edu

Creativity Research Journal 155


J. Meneely and M. Portillo

The polemic that design entails both artistic and scien- prise and challenge precedent. At this level, design in-
tific mindsets suggests that creativity in design may de- volves artistic criteria and becomes concerned with
pend on one’s capacity to be adaptable, exhibiting flex- aesthetics, self-expression, personal philosophy, and
ibility to negotiate complex and multi-dimensional subjective reactions to the final product. However,
design problems. what differentiates design from art is the degree to
Although a handful of studies have profiled person- which externally imposed demands factor into the cre-
ality, cognitive style, and creative self-awareness in ative process.
samples of designers (see, e.g., Clitheroe, 2001; Lam, In addition to form, designers are expected to de-
1996; Mackinnon, 1962, 1965, 1970; Portillo, 1996; velop solutions that address the functional demands of
Russ & Weber, 1995; Watson & Thompson, 2001), few their clients and end users, who have distinct needs,
studies have assessed the influence of the relationship lifestyles, goals, and objectives to consider. Conse-
among these intrinsic traits on real-world creative per- quently, if a newly designed object fails to serve its in-
formance. In contrast, the present study explored the tended user, it is rarely, if ever, deemed appropriate.
relationship between creative persons and their perfor- From this standpoint, designers must become more sci-
mance on a design task evaluated by field experts. entific and objectively focused, as they systematically
The purpose of this study was to empirically probe a evaluate functional criteria such as safety, comfort, er-
domain-specific understanding of creativity in design, gonomics, and overall product performance. Although
using multiple measures of the creative person in tan- “art is more often an introspective journey and science
dem with an authentic design task. In alignment with a more of an externally focused one” (Feist, 1999, p.
systems view of creativity, relationships between cre- 283), design criteria compel the designer to employ
ative personality traits, cognitive styles, and creative both subjective and objective approaches to prob-
performance were assessed in a sample of beginning lem-solving.
design students, who were faced with the task of devel- Design criteria guiding appropriateness not only
oping creative design solutions on work judged by channel the creative process of designing but serve as a
field-specific standards. measure of evaluation, gauging levels of suitability or
fit in ideas and solutions during the design process,
“Criteria offer both direction and adaptability in devel-
The Holistic Demands of Design oping solutions” (Portillo & Dohr, 1994, p. 407). To
address the competing demands of multiple design cri-
“Creativity is best described as the human capacity teria, designers need to employ flexible thinking as
to regularly solve problems or to fashion products in a they weigh factors that influence novelty and appropri-
domain, in a way that is initially novel but ultimately ateness. In design, flexibility appears central to cre-
acceptable in a culture” (Gardner, 1989, p. 14). Many ative adaptation, where transformation occurs in both
agree that both novelty and appropriateness define cre- the self and the domain.
ative products (Amabile, 1996; Jackson & Messick,
1973; Mackinnon, 1962; Sternberg & Lubart, 1996);
however, the relationship between these two criteria Creative Adaptation
form an interesting dialectic. Novelty implies that cre-
ative solutions depart from mainstream approaches Particularly relevant for a better understanding of
and challenge domain precedent; conversely, appropri- creativity in design is the concept of creative adapta-
ateness implies that creative solutions acknowledge or tion. According to Cohen and Ambrose (1999), cre-
fit into the established practices of a domain in some ative adaptation involves flexibility in thinking, re-
way. (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, 1996; Johnson-Laird, sponsiveness to environment (self-adaptation), and
1988; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995). transformation and evolution of the environment (do-
In design, novelty and appropriateness relate to cri- main adaptation). Situated in both internal and exter-
teria of “form” and “function”: form in the sense that nal loci, creative adaptation rests on a theoretical
designers are expected to create novel objects with foundation that is both developmental and systemic,
compositional qualities that embody delight or sur- with mature creativity marking the most profound re-

156 Creativity Research Journal


The Adaptable Mind in Design

alization of adaptation. Cohen and Ambrose defined the development of “whole-brained” theories of cre-
mature creativity in terms of this external and internal ativity that relate creative performance to an ability to
transformation: operate flexibly between opposing poles of cognitive
spectra (Herrmann, 1989; Leonard & Straus, 1997).
External transformation involves sensitivity to a context as Extending Guilford’s (1967) insights that creativity
well as awareness of the limitations of a field and the desire to incorporates divergent and convergent modes of
work hard to transform it. … Internal transformation in-
volves sensitivity to one’s self and the openness and willing-
thinking, Brophey (2001) noted higher levels of cre-
ness to modify one’s present ways of thinking in order to con- ative performance in combination thinkers when
struct a unique point of view. (p. 12) compared to those who are primarily divergent or
convergent in their thinking.
Some scholars have suggested that the paradoxes
found in the creative person appear to support adapt-
ability—in thought and behavior—as an attribute of
creative performance (Brophey, 2001; Research Questions
Csikszentmihalyi, 1996; Guastello, Shissler, Driscoll,
& Hyde, 1998; Herrmann, 1989; Mackinnon, 1962, The need for domain-specific inquiry into creativity
1970; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995, 1996). Using inter- is supported both theoretically (Csikszentmihalyi,
view data collected from over 90 luminaries across 1990, 1996) and by recently emerging studies (Baer,
varied fields, Csikszentmihalyi (1996) identified par- 1991, 1998; Han, 2003; Han & Marvin, 2002; James &
adoxical traits in his sample of mature creatives, in- Asmus, 2001) that examined creativity across multiple
cluding the capacities to be playful and disciplined, domains. Utilizing a performance-based methodology,
logical and naive, humble and proud, reality-bound Han and Marvin investigated the creativity of children
and fantastical, introverted and extroverted, and mas- on art, math, and verbal tasks. They found that individ-
culine and feminine. ual creative performance varied across tasks, providing
Paradox as an integral component of the creative support for a domain-specific rather than a global defi-
personality emerged in a classic study of personality nition of creativity.
correlates of noted male architects. Mackinnon (1962, Inquiry into a specific domain calls for a systems
1970) identified creative architects as having a rela- framework of creativity that accounts for both intrinsic
tively high level of traditionally feminine traits with an and extrinsic adaptation (Cohen & Ambrose, 1999).
increased openness to emotions, high intuition, This framework accounts for interactions between the
self-awareness, and diversity of interests atypical of individual who creates, the domain that provides the
males from a Western perspective in that time period. creative venue, and the field experts who act as gate-
Mackinnon concluded, “It would appear that the cre- keepers to the domain (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, 1996).
ative person has the capacity to tolerate the tension that A systems research approach couches individual char-
strong opposing values create in him, and in his cre- acteristics and thinking styles in the domain where the
ative striving he effects some reconciliation of them” creative activity occurs and accounts for the creative
(1962, p. 490). Mackinnon’s study also found that the performance standards of the field.
creative personality contained both introverted and in- This study examined personality and cognitive style
terpersonal characteristics. In a later study of design- profiles of beginning design students, engaged them in
ers, Watson and Thompson (2001) concluded that de- a domain-specific task, and evaluated their products
sign students exhibited dominance in more cognitive with field experts. Empirically spanning the creative
styles than nondesign majors; however, they did not re- person, process, and product, this study examined the
late this finding to creative performance. following research questions: (a) What profiles of cre-
Other studies recognized a flexible thinking style ative personality traits and cognitive styles character-
as a hallmark of the creative person. Guastello et al. ize beginning design students? (b) What relationships
(1998) found that creative performance “was highest emerge between creative personality traits and cogni-
for people who engaged in a wide repertoire of cogni- tive styles? (c) How do individual profiles relate to the
tive styles” (p. 77). Similar findings have prompted level of creativity expressed in design solutions?

Creativity Research Journal 157


J. Meneely and M. Portillo

Method ACL respondents are asked to select self-descriptive


adjectives that ultimately are compiled into a personal-
To explore a domain-specific and systemic under- ity profile. Reviews of this instrument (Buros, 1972;
standing of creativity in design, the study employed Keyser & Sweetland, 1984) reported solid
multiple measures of the creative person and product. psychometric properties. Since the ACL’s develop-
Personality trait and cognitive style profiles were col- ment in 1949, researchers have employed it to investi-
lected from a sample of beginning design students us- gate a wide range of human behaviors using several
ing two standardized self-report instruments. In addi- standardized scales (Gough & Heilbrun, 1983).
tion, performance-based measures of creativity were The ACL contains 37 standard scales, one of which
utilized to externally validate the personality and cog- is the Creative Personality Scale (ACL-Cps) that mea-
nitive style findings. Design experts were utilized to sures unorthodox and original traits. The Cps was
evaluate the creative output each participant produced normed in samples of students, professionals, and ran-
from a design-related problem solving task. This strat- domly selected adults (Gough & Heilbrun, 1983).
egy positioned the methodology beyond a Using the ACL, Domino (1970) developed a separate
unidimensional profiling of traits to explore relation- creativity scale, the ACL-Cr, during a 3-year longitudi-
ships among the individual, field, and domain. nal study of creative achievement in students. A subse-
quent cross-validation study of 800 science, art, and lit-
erature students found that the ACL-Cr significantly
Participants
differentiated creative students from the control groups
The participants were 41 beginning female design (Domino, 1970). The ACL, scored with the Domino’s
majors (M age 19.7 years; SD 1.74) enrolled in an ac- creativity scale (ACL-Cr), was endorsed as a sound
credited school of interior design at a land grant univer- measure of creativity by Davis (1999), who included
sity in the central United States. This sample reflects the 59-item scale and scoring guide in the book, Cre-
the female dominance in interior design education. The ativity is Forever (4th ed.).
Foundation of Interior Design Education and Research The ACL-Cr has been employed to examine do-
(FIDER) reported a demographic composition of 10% main-specific creativity, discriminating statistically
males and 90% females in the 142 accredited interior between student, novice, and expert photographers
design programs across North America (K. Dunn, (Domino & Giuliani, 1997); dancers (Alter, 1984);
FIDER Executive Director, personal communication, musicians (Alter, 1989); cinematographers (Domino,
June 1, 2003). 1974); and patent holders (Albaum & Baker, 1977). It
All participants volunteered and gave written con- has also gauged creativity training effects (Davis &
sent to participate in the study. The sample included Bull, 1978) and identified creative students (Sussman
two sections of a freshman interior design course. Data & Justman, 1976).
collection spanned four class sessions in November
2002. Because of absences that occurred during the Cognitive style inventory. The Herrmann Brain
second and third data-gathering sessions, the cognitive Dominance Instrument (HBDI) was used to identify
style and personality data reported in this article are each participant’s preferred thinking style (Herrmann,
based on a sample size of 39, whereas creative perfor- 1989). The HBDI is a standardized self-report instru-
mance data are based on a sample size of 37. ment comprising 120 questions. Each of the 120 ques-
tions characterizes one of four of the following cogni-
tive dimensions: cerebral (analytic thinking), limbic
Measures
(affective thinking), right (global thinking), and left
Personality inventory. The Gough Adjective (local thinking). Specific information on the
Check List (ACL), scored for Domino’s Creativity psychometric properties of the HBDI can be found in
Scale (ACL-Cr), was used to inventory personality independent reviews by Bunderson (1987) of the Edu-
traits associated with creativity. The ACL is a standard- cational Testing Service, and in the Eleventh Mental
ized self-report instrument composed of 300 alphabeti- Measurements Yearbook (Buros, 1992).
cally ordered adjectives and adjectival phrases that An internal-validation study of the HBDI using
commonly describe diverse personality attributes. 7,989 cognitive profiles from participants in profes-

158 Creativity Research Journal


The Adaptable Mind in Design

sional workshops and personal consultations found (limbic), or a flexible preference as both a thinker and a
good stability among its four discrete clusters (Ho, feeler (cerebral and limbic). The ability of the HBDI to
1988). Other studies further support the validity of the account for flexibility between styles was a deciding
instrument; Rowe and Waters (1992) found that the factor for its inclusion in this study and is consistent
HBDI was able to discriminate cognitive variability with findings and theories linking oppositional traits to
between master’s students in accountancy, business ad- creative performance (Csikszentmihalyi, 1996;
ministration, and public administration. External vali- Mackinnon, 1970, 1978).
dation studies conducted by Bunderson, Olsen, and
Herrmann (1982) correlated the HBDI to the Myers Measure of creative performance. After com-
Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). Later studies con- pleting the personality and cognitive measures, the par-
ducted by DeWald (1989), Power and Lundsten ticipants completed a timed design task. Each partici-
(1997), and Power, Kummerow, and Lundsten (1999) pant was asked to design a three-dimensional form that
replicated the correlations between the HBDI and was transformed into an original piece of furniture for
MBTI as well. Figure 1 graphically summarizes the book storage. The participants received a short list of
correlations from these studies. functional criteria to be incorporated in their final de-
As Figure 1 indicates, these studies found that op- sign solutions. These criteria included (a) storage for a
posing quadrants on the HBDI correlate to opposing specific quantity of books, (b) storage for different
components on the MBTI. For example, thinking ver- sized books, and (c) accessibility for an average height
sus feeling on the MBTI related to cerebral-left versus adult. Each participant was asked to design and con-
limbic-right quadrants on the HBDI. The decision to struct a three-dimensional model using uniform con-
use the HBDI in this study instead of the MBTI was struction materials, design instructions, and time con-
based on the fact that the MBTI scoring system does straints for task completion.
not permit a respondent to have “bipolar” responses, Following Amabile’s (1996) Consensual Assess-
whereas the HBDI does. To elaborate, MBTI scoring ment Technique (CAT), a panel of four expert judges,
places a respondent at one point on a continuum be- composed of design faculty, was asked to evaluate the
tween thinking and feeling, whereas HBDI scoring al- completed design solutions. The judges were in-
lows a respondent to have a singular preference as a structed to rate the projects relative to one another
thinker (cerebral), a singular preference as a feeler based on their own subjective definitions of creativity

Figure 1. Relationship of the HBDI to the MBTI. Based on a summation of findings from multiple studies (Bunderson, 1987; Bunderson, Olsen,
& Herrmann, 1982; DeWald, 1989; Power, Kummerow, & Lundsten, 1999; Power & Lundsten, 1997).

Creativity Research Journal 159


J. Meneely and M. Portillo

Figure 2. Two example solutions to the design task (furniture for book storage).

and to work independently. Inter-rater reliabilities of imposed time limit for completion. All participants
.72, calculated for this study, were above the accept- completed the HBDI within 45 min. During the 3rd
able level of .70 recognized by Amabile (1996) and week the design problem-solving task was assigned.
Barnard (1992). Figure 2 illustrates a high and a low After the project instructions were administered, par-
scoring creative solution to the book storage furniture ticipants were asked to work quietly at their own ta-
problem. bles and not to obtain any input from others. The par-
ticipants were allotted two consecutive days of class,
totaling 5 hr 30 min, to complete a three-dimensional
Procedures
model of their design solution.
The first author informed the participants that the The final component in the data-collection se-
study would involve completing a paper-and-pencil quence involved the evaluation of each participant’s fi-
personality inventory, a computer-administered cog- nal design solution using the CAT. All of the models
nitive style inventory, and a problem-solving task that were randomly arranged on tables and assigned a num-
involved presenting design solutions in a three-di- ber. Each judge was supplied with written instructions
mensional model. During the first data-gathering ses- and an evaluation sheet to record their responses. In ac-
sion each participant completed the ACL in an cordance with Amabile’s protocol on the consensual
untimed classroom setting. All participants finished assessment of products, each judge was then assigned a
the ACL within 20 min. The following week, a sec- different starting point within the room and was asked
ond data-gathering session occurred in a campus to evaluate each model on a 5-point scale of creativity.
computer lab where the participants individually Each judge was instructed to rate the projects relative
completed the HBDI via an Internet link with the in- to one another rather than according to some absolute
strument developer. As with the ACL there was no standard of creativity.

160 Creativity Research Journal


The Adaptable Mind in Design

Results Cognitive Flexibility


The HBDI was administered and scored to profile
Creative Personality
cognitive preferences within the sample. Aggregate
The ACL was administered and scored using Dom- HBDI scores revealed an overall group dominance in
ino’s (1970) creativity scale (ACL-Cr) to assess cre- the right-cerebral and right-limbic quadrants, indicat-
ative personality traits. Cronbach’s αs calculated from ing that the sample collectively preferred right as op-
the personality data indicate a high degree of internal posed to left modes of thinking and displayed an al-
consistency, achieving reliabilities of .97 for the total most balanced preference between cerebral and limbic
ACL instrument and .89 for the creativity scale modes. Figure 3 graphically summarizes the mean
(ACL-Cr). Out of 300 total adjectives on the ACL, the HBDI scores per quadrant, forming a composite cogni-
number of items checked per subject ranged from 40 to tive profile of the entire sample.
176 with a mean of 101.6 (SD = 35.6); which is consis- On the HBDI a respondent can exhibit a preference
tent with normative sample scores (M = 97.4, SD = in one, two, three, or all four cognitive quadrants. Ac-
34.6) found in the ACL scoring manual (Gough & cording to Herrmann’s whole-brained theory of cre-
Heilbrun, 1983). ativity, as the number of quadrant preferences on the
Creativity scale (ACL-Cr) scores were calculated HBDI increases so too does the ability to shift cogni-
from the raw ACL data using T tables to control for the tive modes flexibly during creative processes
total number of items checked. The resultant scores in- (Herrmann, 1989). Table 1 presents the distribution of
dicated that the sample ranged in creative personality cognitive flexibility (number of HBDI quadrant prefer-
from 27 to 69 with a mean of 48.5 (SD = 9.2); slightly ences) found within the sample.
higher than found in previously published research us-
ing a multidisciplinary college sample (M = 45.3, SD =
Creative Personality and Cognitive
11.5; Davis & Bull, 1978). A frequency analysis iden-
Flexibility
tified the most common ACL-Cr self-descriptors
within the sample: imaginative (92%), artistic (90%), Correlations between the ACL-Cr and HBDI were
independent (90%), curious (82%), ambitious (79%), used to examine relationships between creative person-
adventurous (77%), humorous (77%), sensitive (77%), ality and cognitive flexibility. Mean creative personal-
intelligent (74%), and energetic (72%). ity scores (ACL-Cr), grouped by the number of HBDI

Figure 3. Composite cognitive profile of the entire sample (HBDI). Means of 67 and above indicate a quadrant dominance (indicated by a
checkmark). N = 39.

Creativity Research Journal 161


J. Meneely and M. Portillo

Table 1. Distribution of Cognitive Flexibility Within the Sample (HBDI)


Sample Population Normative
Population
Cognitive Flexibility (Number of HBDI Quadrant Preferences) n % %

1 Quadrant 2 5.1 5
2 Quadrants 25 64.1 58
3 Quadrants 9 23.1 34
4 Quadrants 3 7.7 3
Total 39 100 100

Note. Normative population data supplied by Herrmann International: www.hbdi.com. HBDI = Herrmann Brain Dominance Instrument.

Table 2. Mean Creative Personality Scores (ACL-Cr) by Level of Cognitive Flexibility (HBDI)
Creative Personality (ACL-Cr)

Cognitive Flexibility (Number of HBDI Quadrant Preferences) M SD n

1 Quadrant 42.50 6.36 2


2 Quadrants 46.76 8.59 25
3 Quadrants 51.67 9.31 9
4 Quadrants 57.67 9.87 3
Total 48.51 9.16 39

Note. ACL-Cr = Adjective Check List Creativity Scale; HBDI = Herrmann Brain Dominance Instrument.

quadrant preferences per participant, are presented in tify salient items on the ACL that discriminated be-
Table 2. The reported ACL-Cr scores indicate a linear tween the two groups. Table 3 presents, in ranked order
relationship between creative personality and the ca- of significance, the discriminating adjectives for each
pacity to think flexibly. group. ACL-Cr items that were reported significantly
A linear regression analysis confirmed that cogni- more often by the high-flexibility group included the
tive flexibility (number of HBDI quadrant preferences) following: alert, logical, humorous, insightful, and as-
was a significant predictor of creative personality sertive, whereas items reported significantly more of-
scores on the ACL-Cr (r = .39, p = .014), accounting ten by the low-flexibility group included ab-
for 15% of the variance. To further test this relation- sent-minded and careless.
ship, the authors split the sample into two groups; those
preferring one or two HBDI quadrants were clustered
Creative Performance
into a low-flexibility group (n = 27), whereas those
preferring three or four HBDI quadrants were clus- Creativity evidenced by the final design solutions
tered into a high-flexibility group (n = 12). That is to was evaluated using the CAT. Ratings from each of the
say, 69% of the sample showed low flexibility, with a four judges were averaged for each participant’s solu-
preference for 1 or 2 cognitive styles, whereas 31% ex- tion, resulting in mean creativity scores ranging from a
hibited high flexibility, with a preference for 3 or all 4 low of 1.00 to a high of 4.50, with an overall sample
styles. A t test confirmed that the high-flexibility group mean of 2.34 (SD = .76). A regression analysis re-
(M = 53.17, SD = 9.39) achieved a significantly higher vealed that ACL-Cr was a significant predictor of
creative personality score on the ACL-Cr than did the judged product creativity from the CAT (r = .33, p =
low-flexibility group (M = 46.44, SD = 8.43), t (37) = .046), accounting for 11% of the variance. To further
–2.22, p = .033. test this relationship the authors performed a median
To gain further insight into how the low- and split on the ACL-Cr to produce a high-Cr group (n =
high-flexibility groups differed, the authors used 17) and a low-Cr group (n = 20). A t test confirmed that
Chi-square analyses using Fischer’s Exact Test to iden- the high-Cr group (M = 2.62, SD = .97) achieved sig-

162 Creativity Research Journal


The Adaptable Mind in Design

Table 3. Significant Adjectival Discriminators (ACL) by Level of Cognitive Flexibiltiy (HBDI)


Fischer’s
Low-Flexibility Group Preference Exact High-Flexibility Group Preference
in 1 or 2 HBDI Quadrants, n1 = 27 Test in 3 or 4 HBDI Quadrants, n2 = 12

Adjective (ACL) n % n1 p % n2 n Adjective (ACL)

6 22 .001 83 10 Alert
4 15 .002 67 8 Precise
8 30 .003 83 10 Logical
Absent-minded 10 37 .017 0 0
10 41 .018 83 11 Clear-thinking
Forgetful 16 59 .018 17 2
2 7 .020 42 5 Suspicious
5 19 .023 58 7 Thorough
0 0 .024 25 3 Cold
7 26 .031 67 8 Steady
18 67 .036 100 12 Humorous
18 67 .036 100 12 Sociable
12 44 .037 83 10 Headstrong
12 44 .037 83 10 Insightful
10 37 .041 75 9 Assertive
Careless 8 30 .042 0 0

Note. N = 39. ACL = Adjective Check List; HBDI = Herrmann Brain Dominance Instrument.

nificantly higher levels of judged creativity than did the that beginning designers with stronger creative person-
low-Cr group (M = 2.10, SD = .43), t (35) = –2.16, p = ality traits showed greater flexibility in their thinking
.038. A final regression analysis was performed to and designed more creatively than their less creative
evaluate how well the HBDI predicted creative perfor- counterparts. This finding may be explained through
mance. This analysis found no statistically significant the concept of creative adaptability. In design, flexibil-
relationship (r = .19, p = .249). ity appears central to creative adaptability, where the
In summary, the relation between the HBDI and ability to tap into oppositional traits and thinking styles
ACL-Cr indicated that participants showing flexibility facilitate holistic and dynamic problem solving.
among cerebral, limbic, right, and left modes of think- Adaptability may be particularly apropos to the design
ing had significantly higher mean scores on creative domain, enabling one to synthesize disparate artistic
personality than did those who exhibited a more en- and functional criteria into creative solutions.
trenched cognitive style (r = .39, p = .014). Creative
personality traits (ACL-Cr) significantly predicted cre-
ative performance on the design task (r = .33, p = .046). Internal Adaptability: Traits and Thinking
Although cognitive style (HBDI) did not predict cre- Styles
ative performance (r = .19, p = .249), flexibility be- The study found that the sample collectively fa-
tween styles was significantly correlated to the creative vored a right-brain thinking style, indicating a prefer-
personality. ence for big-picture issues and a broad focus to prob-
lem solving. Interestingly, those participants who
exhibited stronger creative personality traits differed
Discussion from the overall sample by further displaying a prefer-
ence for a left-brain thinking style, indicating an addi-
This study identified creative personality traits and tional propensity for details and analytic scrutiny. ACL
thinking styles in beginning design students to better items, such as alert, precise, logical, thorough, and sus-
explain domain-specific creativity. The findings from picious, were found to significantly discriminate this
self-report and external performance measures suggest high adaptability group and provided additional evi-

Creativity Research Journal 163


J. Meneely and M. Portillo

dence of their accompanying left-brain thinking style. personality and cognitive measures to an externalized
In possessing this propensity, the high adaptability measure of creative performance. It was found that be-
group appeared more open to evaluation, revision, and ginning designers with stronger creative personality
refinement of their concepts during the design process traits (ACL-Cr) produced work that was judged to be
than their predominantly right-brain peers. more creative than that of their peers who did not ex-
Supporting these findings, the linkage between hibit the same level of creative personality. This find-
flexible thinking and creativity has been well estab- ing is consistent with other research (Alter, 1984,
lished, emphasizing the integration of opposing cogni- 1989; Domino, 1970, 1974, 1994; Domino & Giuliani,
tive processes in creative problem solving (Brophey, 1997) identifying the ACL-Cr as a significant predictor
2001; Finke, Ward, & Smith, 1992; Guilford, 1967; of creative performance. Although the HBDI did not
Sternberg & Lubart, 1995). We theorize that the level directly predict creative performance, it was signifi-
of cognitive flexibility required for creativity varies be- cantly linked to the creative personality. This suggests
tween disciplines; furthermore, we maintain that the that cognitive style variables may be necessary but not
hybrid nature of design disciplines may demand a fully sufficient to account for creativity in design. For
higher degree of cognitive flexibility, to synthesize ar- example, a person could exhibit opposing cognitive
tistic and scientific problem criteria. Supporting this abilities and not have the personality or motivational
notion, Lawson (1997) stated, faculties to engage in adaptive behavior. Although this
study found evidence that beginning designers who are
Whilst we have seen that both convergent and divergent more creative in their personalities and design projects
thought are needed by both scientists and artists, it is proba- appeared to think more flexibly than their peers, fur-
bly the designer who needs the two skills in the most equal
proportions. Designers must solve externally imposed prob-
ther research is necessary to fully relate cognitive style
lems, satisfy the needs of others and create beautiful objects. to creative performance in design.
(pp. 156–157) Flexible thinking supports the concept of internal
and external adaptability advanced by Cohen and
Mounting evidence suggests that differing disciplines Ambrose (1999), who declared that experts within a
and domains attract people with relatively similar cog- field are more highly skilled at adaptability compared
nitive/personality profiles (Herrmann, 1989; Leonard to those entering a field:
& Straus, 1997; Rowe & Waters, 1992). This stratifica-
tion of cognitive styles may explain why creativity is Neophytes, who are new to a field, and novices, who have
increasingly conceptualized as domain specific. Rules some rudimentary knowledge of the field, have not devel-
and standards of a domain attract those with certain oped automaticity nor the necessary knowledge or skills.
characteristics and thinking styles and, thus, a domi- Consequently, they are not efficient adaptors within the field.
(p. 16)
nant approach to operating in a domain emerges. In
contrast, creative people may not only have a capacity
to engage the established mindset; they may also have However, our findings challenge this distinction. Al-
a capacity to defy it, thereby escaping domain en- though it appears reasonable to maintain that creative
trenchment and precedent (Sternberg & Lubart, 1995). adaptability could be more developed in field experts
The flexibility to think both within and in opposition to as opposed to beginners, that does not negate that, even
established norms characterizes creative adaptability. at beginning levels, some individuals exhibit more
adaptive thinking than others.
Although domain expertise has been identified as
External Adaptability: Creative
an important facet of creativity, entrenched assump-
Performance
tions about a domain can thwart innovation (Amabile,
Creativity does not occur in a vacuum. In design, 1983, 1996; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995, 1996). Pos-
creativity emerges in context within a person–environ- sessing some level of naiveté may facilitate the forma-
ment system that can be studied with psychological tion of novel ideas and help one move beyond estab-
and performance-based assessments. By evaluating the lished precedents. Conversely, having expertise in a
level of creativity in design solutions, this study was domain may make it easier to perceive appropriate
able to assess predictive validity by relating internal venues and opportunities for creativity to transpire. We

164 Creativity Research Journal


The Adaptable Mind in Design

support an adaptable view suggested by Barron (1963), able thinking, and adaptable processes that ultimately
who stated that the creative person “may be at once na- culminate in creative design solutions.
ive and knowledgeable, being at home equally to prim-
itive symbolism and to rigorous logic” (p. 224). This
study found that some novice designers, with distin-
References
guishable creative personality traits and projects, ap-
pear to think more adaptively than their peers. What re- Albaum, G., & Baker, K. (1977). Cross-validation of a creativity
mains to be seen is how these thinking abilities and scale for the Adjective Check List. Educational and Psycholog-
personality traits can be nurtured and developed as ex- ical Measurement, 37, 1057–1061.
pertise and experience grow. Alter, J. (1984). Creativity profile of university and conservatory
dance students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(2),
153–158.
Alter, J. (1989). Creative profile of university and conservatory mu-
sic students. Creativity Research Journal, 2(3), 184–195.
Limitations and Future Directions
Amabile, T. (1983). Social psychology of creativity. New York:
Some limitations merit discussion as we interpret Springer-Verlag.
Amabile, T. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview.
the findings. First, the sample size was not large, and
Baer, J. (1991). Generality of creativity across performance do-
the categorization of the sample by cognitive style may mains. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 23–39.
have influenced the statistical power and interpretative Baer, J. (1998). Point-counterpoint: The case for domain specificity
confidence of the study. Second, the measure of cre- of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 173–177.
ative performance was based on a single design task, Barnard, S. (1992). Interior design creativity: The development and
testing of a methodology for the consensual assessment of pro-
accounting for only one instance of work. Judging a
jects. Unpublished Dissertation, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg,
larger portfolio of work for each participant would VA.
have strengthened the external validity of the research. Barron, F. (1963). Creativity and psychological health. New York:
Ideally, researchers should examine creativity over Van Nostrand.
time in designers with differing creative profiles. An- Brophey, D. (2001). Comparing the attributes, activities, and perfor-
mance of divergent, convergent, and combination thinkers. Cre-
other limiting variable in the study is gender. The
ativity Research Journal, 13, 439–455.
all-female sample characterized the pervasiveness of Bunderson, C. (1987). The validity of the Herrmann Brain Domi-
women within interior design education. Similarly, nance Instrument. Unpublished manuscript.
all-male investigations in architecture reflected the Bunderson, C., Olsen, J., & Herrmann, W. (1982). A fourfold model
mainstream architectural profession in the 1950s of multiple brain dominance and its validation through
correlational research (Scientific and technical report #10: Pre-
(Mackinnon, 1962, 1965, 1970). Today, we can make a
pared for General Electric). Orem, UT: Wicat Incorporated
case for examining female designers to bridge the gap Learning Design Laboratories.
in knowledge and to advocate the exploration of poten- Buros, O. (1972). The seventh mental measurements yearbook. Ox-
tial differences across genders. Another aspect of the ford, England: Gryphon.
study deserves mention. Design includes a consider- Buros, O. (1992). The eleventh mental measurements yearbook. Ox-
ford, England: Gryphon.
able array of fields such as architecture, interior design,
Clitheroe, H. (2001). Mapping the creative context of architects. Un-
landscape architecture, industrial design, engineering, published doctoral dissertation, University of California,
and apparel design. Although these allied fields share Irvine, CA.
many common attributes, these findings should only be Cohen, L., & Ambrose, D. (1999). Adaptation and creativity. In M.
considered within the limits of the sampled population. A. Runco & M. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity.
New York: Academic Press.
Future extensions of this study to related fields could
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). The domains of creativity. In M. A.
reveal interesting similarities and differences. Runco & R. S. Albert (Eds.), Theories of creativity (pp.
An adaptable mind in design exhibits oppositional 190–214). London: Sage.
personality traits, thinking styles, and flexibility to en- Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of
gage the external world creatively. This ability to adapt discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins.
Davis, G. (1999). Creativity is forever (4th rev. ed.). Dubuque, IA:
one’s thinking appears synergistic with the holistic de-
Kendall Hunt.
mands of problem solving in design. What remains to Davis, G., & Bull, K. (1978). Strengthening affective components of
be fully understood are the environmental and social creativity in a college course. Journal of Educational Psychol-
conditions that nurture adaptable personalities, adapt- ogy, 70, 833–836.

Creativity Research Journal 165


J. Meneely and M. Portillo

DeWald, R. (1989). Relationships of MBTI types and HBDI prefer- Keyser, D., & Sweetland, R. (Eds.). (1984). Test Critiques. Kansas
ences in a population of student program managers. Unpub- City, MO: Test Corporation of America.
lished dissertation, Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, Lam, M. (1996). Developing architectural mindsets: Creativity and
MI. the developmental characteristics of the values, methods, and
Domino, G. (1970). Identification of potentially creative persons objectives of first, third, and sixth year architectural design stu-
from the Adjective Check List. Journal of Consulting and Clin- dents. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas A&M.
ical Psychology, 35, 48–51. Lawson, B. (1997). How designers think: The design process
Domino, G. (1974). Assessment of cinematographic creativity. Jour- demystified (3rd ed.). Oxford, England: Architectural.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 150–154. Leonard, D., & Straus, S. (1997). Putting your company’s whole
Domino, G. (1994). Assessment of creativity with the ACL: An em- brain to work. Harvard Business Review, 75(4), 110–123.
pirical comparison of four scales. Creativity Research Journal, Mackinnon, D. (1962). The nature and nurture of creative talent.
7, 21–33. American Psychologist, 17, 484–495.
Domino, G., & Giuliani, I. (1997). Creativity in three samples of Mackinnon, D. (1965). Personality and the realization of creative po-
photographers: A validation of the Adjective Check List cre- tential. American Psychologist, 20, 273–281.
ativity scale. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 193–200. Mackinnon, D. (1970). The personality correlates of creativity: A
Feist, G. (1999). Influence of personality on artistic and scientific study of American architects. In P. Vernon (Ed.), Creativity (pp.
creativity. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 289–311). Harmondsworth, England: Penguin.
273–296). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Mackinnon, D. (1978). In search of human effectiveness: Identifying
Finke, R., Ward, T., & Smith, S. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, and developing creativity. Buffalo, NY: Creative Education
research, and applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Foundation.
Gardner, H. (1989). To open minds. New York: Basic. Portillo, M. (1996). Uncovering implicit theories of creativity in be-
Goldschmidt, G. (1999). Design. In M. A. Runco & M. R. Pritzker ginning design students. Journal of Interior Design, 22(2),
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 525–535). New York: 15–24.
Academic Press. Portillo, M., & Dohr, J. (1994). Bridging process and structure
Gough, H., & Heilbrun, A. (1983). The Adjective Check List manual. through criteria. Design Studies, 15, 403–416.
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists. Power, S., Kummerow, J., & Lundsten, L. (1999). A Herrmann brain
Guastello, S., Shissler, J., Driscoll, J., & Hyde, T. (1998). Are some dominance profile analysis of sixteen MBTI types in a sample
cognitive styles more creatively productive than others? The of MBA students. Journal of Psychological Type, 49, 27–36.
Journal of Creative Behavior, 32, 77–91. Power, S., & Lundsten, L. (1997). Studies that compare type theory
Guilford, J. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: and left-brain-right-brain theory. Journal of Psychological
McGraw Hill. Type, 43, 22–28.
Han, K. (2003). Domain-specificity of creativity in young children: Rowe, F., & Waters, M. (1992). Can personality-type instruments
How quantitative and qualitative data support it. The Journal of profile majors in management programs? Journal of Education
Creative Behavior, 37, 117–142. for Business, 68, 10–15.
Han, K., & Marvin, C. (2002). Multiple creatives? Investigating do- Rowe, P. (1987). Design thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
main-specificity of creativity in young children. Gifted Child Russ, R., & Weber, M. (1995). Personality types of interior design
Quarterly, 46, 98–109. students: Implications for education. Journal of Interior De-
Herrmann, N. (1989). The creative brain (2nd ed.). Lake Lure, NC: sign, 21, 30–38.
Brain Books. Sternberg, R., & Lubart, T. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating
Ho, K. T. (1988). The dimensionality and occupational discriminat- creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press.
ing power of the Herrmann Brain Dominance Instrument. Un- Sternberg, R., & Lubart, T. (1996). Investing in creativity. American
published Dissertation, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT. Psychologist, 51, 677–688.
Jackson, P., & Messick, S. (1973). The person, the product, and the Sussman, G., & Justman, J. (1976). Characteristics of preadolescent
response: Conceptual problems in the assessment of creativity. boys judged creative by their teachers. Gifted Child Quarterly,
In M. Bloomberg (Ed.), Creativity: Theory and research. New 19(3), 210–216.
Haven, CT: College & University Press. Watson, S., & Thompson, C. (2001). Learning styles of interior de-
James, K., & Asmus, C. (2001). Personality, cognitive skills, and sign students as assessed by the Gregoric Style Delineator.
creativity in different life domains. Creativity Research Jour- Journal of Interior Design, 27(1), 12–19.
nal, 12, 149–159. Zeisel, J. (1981). Inquiry by design. Monterey, CA: Brooks & Cole.
Johnson-Laird, P. (1988). Freedom and constraint in creativity. In R.
Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity. New York: Cambridge
University Press.

166 Creativity Research Journal

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen