Sie sind auf Seite 1von 63

Psychology, Public Policy, and Law Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association Inc

1995, Vol. 1, No. 4, 846-908 1076-8971/95/S3.00

"MEMORY WORK" AND RECOVERED MEMORIES


OF CHILDHOOD SEXUAL ABUSE:
Scientific Evidence and Public, Professional,
and Personal Issues
D. Stephen Lindsay J. Don Read
University of Victoria University of Lethbridge

The authors review and critically evaluate scientific evidence regarding recovered
memories of childhood sexual abuse and discuss the implications of this evidence for
professional psychology, public policy, and the law. The discussion focuses primarily on
abuse memories recovered through "memory work" by people who previously believed
that they were not sexually abused as children. The authors argue that memory work can
yield both veridical memories and illusory memories or false beliefs, and they discuss
factors that could be used to weigh the credibility of allegations based on recovered
memories. The article offers tentative recommendations regarding public education,
training and certification of psychotherapists, guidelines for trauma-oriented psycho-
therapy, research initiatives, legislative actions, and legal proceedings.

The controversy about psychotherapeutic and self-help techniques used to help


clients/readers remember hidden histories of childhood sexual abuse (CSA) is the
issue at the intersection of psychology, public policy, and the law in the 1990s.1
Debate centers on the questions of whether many clients are amnesic for histories of
CSA and whether "memory work" (techniques used to recover suspected hidden
histories of childhood traumas) can lead people who were not sexually abused as
children to believe that they were. This topic bristles with complexities and is charged
with political tensions and personal emotions. From any perspective, the stakes are
high. Victim advocates understandably fear that charges of iatrogenic pseudomemo-
ries of CSA threaten therapists and undermine our society's fledgling efforts to

D. Stephen Lindsay, Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia,


Canada; J. Don Read, Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada.
Work on this article was supported by grants from the Natural Science and Engineering Research
Council of Canada and the Alberta Law Foundation. We thank Brooks Brenneis, Deborah Connolly,
Vincenza Grupusso, Elizabeth F. Loftus, Seth C. Kalichman, Gary Melton, Tonia Nicholls, Jim Ogloff,
and August Piper for their helpful advice regarding earlier versions of this article, and Moira Cairns and
Elizabeth R. Wingrove for thought-provoking discussions of many of the issues addressed in this article.
We are particularly indebted to Constance J. Dalenberg for her extraordinarily detailed and incisive
criticisms of an earlier draft.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed either to D. Stephen Lindsay,
Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Post Office Box 3050, Victoria, British Columbia, V8W
3P5 Canada, or to J. Don Read, Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge,
Alberta, TIK 3M4 Canada. Electronic mail may be sent via Internet either to lindsay@uwm.uvic.ca or to
read@hg.uleth.ca.
'We use the term hidden memories rather than repressed memories because of the theoretical baggage
encumbering the concept of repression (Ofshe & Walters, 1994; Singer, 1990). By hidden memories we
mean to refer to autobiographical information in memory that is not consciously accessible but that might
be retrieved given the appropriate cues and procedures. Also, although we focus on CSA, our discussion
also applies to memories of other kinds of childhood traumata (excluding those involving brain injury).

846
RECOVERED MEMORIES 847

protect children, support survivors, and prosecute perpetrators. On the other side,
critics of memory work argue that such techniques have destroyed families and
caused clients/readers great and needless harm. Recovered memories have led to
court cases by clients against their parents, former clients against therapists, parents
against therapists and authors, and parents against their accusing adult children
(Bulkley & Horwitz, 1994; Lipton, 1994; Loftus, 1994a, 1994b; Slovenko, 1994). The
American Medical Association (1994), American Psychiatric Association (1993),
Australian Psychological Society (1994), and British Psychological Society (1995)
have released public statements on this issue, and the American Psychological
Association has released a brief pamphlet (1995) and empaneled a group to produce
a position paper. The debate has received extensive media coverage and is the
subject of many recent and pending books, chapters, articles, and special issues of
journals. This topic demands our best efforts toward understanding and communica-
tion.
This article focuses on recollections and beliefs about CSA developed through
memory work rather than on those that arise more spontaneously. This is not meant
to imply that all experiences of memory recovery occur through memory work. Briere
(in press) argued that psychotherapy that does not involve searches for hidden
memories may nonetheless sometimes support recollection of childhood traumas,
and two recent surveys of psychotherapists found that memories of CSA are often
said to be recovered outside of therapy (although these reports do not indicate
whether or how often self-help books and related material played a role in fomenting
memory recovery; Andrews et al., 1995; Feldman-Summers & Pope, 1994). Our
reading of the memory literature leads us to believe that spontaneous recovery of
previously forgotten histories of CSA is a rare phenomenon, but that same literature
also leads us to believe that if and when memory recovery is truly spontaneous there
are far fewer grounds for concern about false memories. Therefore, we focus on
memory recovery techniques and ancillary practices (specified below) used to help
clients/readers recover suspected hidden memories. For similar reasons, our
comments primarily concern cases in which adults initially believe they were not
sexually victimized as children and later come to believe that they were, rather than
cases in which people who always knew they survived such abuse as children
remember additional details or instances. It must be acknowledged, however, that it
is difficult to draw a sharp line between spontaneous and nonspontaneous memory
recovery, or between cases in which people always knew that they were abused but
only recently confronted that knowledge and cases in which people who previously
believed that they were not sexually victimized subsequently believe that they were
(see Schooler, 1994).
We have several aims in this article. First, we briefly discuss the cultural context
in which the recent popularity of memory work developed. Second, we review
scientific evidence of the validity of recovered histories of CSA. Central consider-
ations in this regard include the base rates of CSA and of forgetting CSA,
practitioners' ability to discriminate between clients with and without hidden
histories of CSA, the susceptibility of memory to suggestion, and factors that increase
the difficulty of discriminating between veridical and illusory memories. To antici-
pate (at the risk of oversimplifying), we argue that memory work can lead both to the
recovery of essentially veridical memories and to the development of essentially
illusory memories or false beliefs of CSA, and we propose a number of factors that
848 LINDSAY AND READ

may be useful when attempting to postdict the validity of alleged recovered


memories. Finally, we consider the implications of the best available evidence
regarding recovered memories for professional psychology, public policy, and the
law.
Our approach to this topic reflects the dual contributions of our training as
cognitive psychologists and our concern about the reality of CSA. We hope to reduce
use of approaches that may inadvertently lead clients to develop false beliefs and to
help fact finders discriminate between accurate and false accusations, without
undermining support for survivors of CSA and without impugning psychotherapy in
general.

Cultural Context
The debate about memory work can only be understood in its cultural context. A
major aspect of that context is the history of sexual victimization of children and
women in Western cultures2 (e.g., Brownmiller, 1975; Brundage, 1993). CSA has a
long history, and until recently the predominant cultural response has ranged from
passive acceptance to denial and minimization. Over the last century there have been
several brief periods during which the sexual victimization of children captured
public attention, but these episodes were followed by extended periods during which
the existence of CSA was largely ignored. Thanks in large measure to grassroots
efforts undertaken as part of the women's movement, in the past two or three
decades our culture has made progress toward a more sustained commitment to
protecting children, supporting survivors, and prosecuting and treating perpetrators
of CSA (Enns, McNeilly, Corkery, & Gilbert, 1995; Herman, 1992). It is now well
established that CSA is much more common than once was thought (e.g., Finkelhor,
1994) and that CSA is associated with psychological problems in adulthood (e.g.,
Beitchman, Zucker, Hood, daCosta, & Cassavia, 1992). Efforts to combat CSA
initially met with resistance from some authorities, who questioned the reliability of
child witnesses and minimized the prevalence and impact of CSA, and progress has
been hard won (e.g., Hechler, 1988). Heightened sensitivity to the reality of CSA and
its association with psychological problems in adulthood helps account for recent
interest in psychotherapeutic techniques designed to detect and support survivors
(see Conte, 1994). The history of denial and minimization of CSA helps account for
the skepticism with which some therapists have reacted to criticisms of memory work
in psychotherapy.
An anonymous reviewer of an earlier version of this article suggested that
feminism lies at the heart of the promulgation of memory work. Some critics of
memory work have made similar arguments in ways that might be characterized as
antifeminist (see Enns et al., 1995). Although there are certainly many linkages
between feminism and CSA-oriented memory work, we believe it would be incorrect
to categorize the latter as peculiarly and uniquely feminist. For one thing, there are
striking parallels between memory work and controversial therapies of earlier
decades that could hardly be described as feminist (e.g., lobotomy, insulin shock
therapy, electroconvulsive shock therapy). Indeed, it can be argued that extreme
forms of memory work are antithetical to feminism because they foster codepen-
dency between therapist and client and lead clients to define themselves as victims

2
Our brief review focuses on Europe and North America because we are not familiar with the history
of other cultures with regard to CSA.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 849

(e.g., Haaken & Schlaps, 1991). Finally, Poole, Lindsay, Memon, and Bull's (1995)
surveys of psychotherapists indicated that men and women practitioners equally
often reported use of CSA-oriented memory recovery techniques. Feminism is quite
properly involved in any debate that touches on the issue of sexual abuse, feminist
psychologists have played a major role in promoting memory work, and strong
feelings about feminism (on both sides of the issue) have contributed to the
politicization of this topic; however, in our view the controversy regarding memory
work does not reduce to a debate between feminists and nonfeminists.
It has also been suggested that popular interest in recovered memories of CSA is
a symptom of an emerging "culture of victimhood" in which people tend to attribute
their problems to the harmful actions of powerful others rather than viewing them as
their own responsibilities (e.g., Kaminer, 1992). It may or may not be true that
self-identifying as a victim is more common now than in previous decades, but many
psychologists have long claimed that childhood traumas precipitate adulthood
psychological problems and that analyzing memories of childhood trauma is
important for psychological healing. Of course, therapists differ widely in the extent
to which they cleave to these views, with many rejecting a focus on childhood trauma
in favor of more present-focused interventions (e.g., Madanes, 1990; Minuchin &
Fishman, 1981). Nonetheless, beliefs about the role of childhood trauma in causing
and treating adulthood problems have deeply permeated our culture over the course
of the last century, and many practitioners' approaches to therapy include an
emphasis on exploring childhood trauma (e.g., Andrews et al., 1995).
Because of the prevalence of beliefs about the importance of resolving childhood
trauma in treating adulthood problems, various forms of memory work have long
been used in some approaches to psychotherapy. For example, in his early article on
the "seduction" theory, Freud (1896/1962) described 18 patients whose treatment,
he argued, demonstrated that incestuous CSA is often the root cause of psychologi-
cal problems. This early work of Freud is, in two ways, a caput Nili (source of the
Nile) for recent ideas about memory recovery. First, renewed interest in hidden
memories of childhood trauma was fueled, in part, by attacks on Freud for
abandoning the seduction theory in favor of psychoanalytic theory (according to
which these clients had not really been sexually abused as children but instead had
fantasized incestuous events during childhood or therapy; e.g., Masson, 1984).
Second, many of the recent ideas about hidden memories and memory work are
similar to those Freud propounded in his early work. Although secondary sources
sometimes imply that Freud's patients disclosed instances of abuse of which they had
always been aware, Freud (1896/1962, p. 204) explicitly stated the following:
Before they come for analysis the patients know nothing about these scenes. They are
indignant as a rule if I warn them that such scenes are going to emerge. Only the
strongest compulsion of the treatment can induce them to embark on a reproduction
of them.

It appears that in this early therapy Freud used highly suggestive techniques, and
that in some or all of these cases what the clients produced in response to these
techniques were vague thoughts, associations, and images that Freud interpreted as
evidence of incestuous CSA, rather than clear recollections of specific events (Powell
& Boer, 1994; Schimek, 1987; Tabin, 1993). Freud (1896/1962) likened the therapist
to an archeologist who recruits the local people (the patient) to excavate suggestive
perturbations in the terrain (symptoms) in the hope of unearthing ancient ruins
850 LINDSAY AND READ

(memories of CSA), and he used a variety of techniques in such excavations. These


techniques, and the rationale Freud presented for using them and for viewing their
products as memories of actual CSA, are extraordinarily similar to the techniques
and rationales offered by recent advocates of memory work in psychotherapy. Similar
beliefs about the recovery of hidden memories were widely popularized in sensa-
tional books such as Sybil (Schreiber, 1973) and Michelle Remembers (M. Smith &
Pazder, 1980). The "recovery" approach to treating addiction and "codependency"
also emphasizes the importance of searching for and resolving childhood conflicts
(e.g., Bradshaw, 1988, cited in Pendergrast, 1995; cf. Kaminer, 1992; Yapko, 1994).
Together with increased awareness of the prevalence of CSA and its psychologi-
cal sequelae, these influences led to the development, promotion, and use of
CSA-focused memory-recovery techniques and ancillary practices by some counsel-
ors, social workers, psychotherapists, and psychiatrists in the 1980s. There is no
formal school or established approach to memory work in psychotherapy, but during
the past decade numerous writers have advocated various forms of CSA-focused
memory work. Such approaches have been promoted in journal articles (e.g.,
Claridge, 1992; K. R. Clark, 1993; Courtois, 1992; Ellenson, 1985; Herman &
Schatzow, 1987; Maltz, 1990; Olio, 1989; Person & Klar, 1994; Siegel & Romig,
1990), scholarly books (e.g., Courtois, 1988; Dolan, 1991; McCann & Pearlman,
1990; Meiselman, 1990), workshops for therapists (see Pendergrast, 1995), popular
self-help books (e.g., Bass & Davis, 1988; Blume, 1990; Engel, 1989; Frederickson,
1992), magazine and newspaper articles, radio and television programs, and novels
(see Loftus, 1993, and Pendergrast, 1995, for citations of popular sources). There are
important differences among proponents of memory work, and not all advocate
prolonged, multifaceted searches for suspected hidden memories. Nonetheless, it is
indisputable that CSA-oriented memory work with clients who initially report no
CSA history has been widely promulgated in recent years.
In the past few years various "special interest" groups have been formed to
represent constituencies affected by the debate about recovered memories. On one
side are advocacy groups for people who claim to have been falsely accused on the
basis of recovered memories. Of these, the False Memory Syndrome Foundation
(FMSF) has been the most active, with 17,000 reported contacts as of October 1994
(FMSF Scientific and Professional Advisory Board, 1994). On the other side, victim
advocates have responded in kind, publishing criticisms of the FMSF (e.g.,
Lawrence, 1993), forming advocacy groups (e.g., Silent No Longer) and, on occasion,
staging protests during lectures by critics of memory work (Kristiansen, 1994). Some
on each side of this debate have made inflammatory statements about the moral
character and motivations of those on the other side and tendered biased accounts of
the scientific evidence. This personal and political polarization, understandable in
view of the contrasting perspectives of advocates of the truly abused and advocates of
the falsely accused, is an important part of the context for discussions of memory
work. One of our central arguments, however, is that there is no contradiction
between being critical of suggestive memory work and being concerned about CSA.

Clarifying Criticisms of Memory Work


Criticisms of memory work have focused on approaches that combine several of
the following techniques and ancillary practices in a prolonged effort to help clients
who initially report no history of CSA recover suspected hidden memories:
RECOVERED MEMORIES 851

(a) directly or indirectly communicating to clients that their symptoms are indicative
of hidden memories of CSA, that many survivors do not remember any abuse, and
that psychological healing depends on recovering memories; (b) using techniques
such as hypnosis, age regression, guided imagery, sodium amytal, and instructions to
work at remembering CSA (e.g., giving free rein to the imagination, stream-of-
consciousness journaling, use of family photographs as retrieval cues, etc.) to help
clients remember CSA; (c) interpreting dreams and physical symptoms ("body
memories") as memories of CSA; (d) recommending that clients suspected of having
hidden memories join survivors' groups, read popular books on remembering CSA,
or both; and (e) endorsing all reports related to CSA as accurate memories and
countering clients' expressions of doubt. Again, criticism has focused on extended
(although not necessarily overtly coercive) use of a constellation of such techniques.
Critics do not claim that all memories experienced through such techniques are false
but rather that some—perhaps many—likely are false (e.g., Gardner, 1992; D. S.
Lindsay & Read, 1994; Loftus, 1993; Pendergrast, 1995; Schooler, 1994).

Prevalence of Memory Work


In response to criticisms of highly suggestive memory recovery techniques, some
have argued that few qualified practitioners actually use such approaches (e.g.,
Berliner & Williams, 1994; Brown, 1995; Enns et al., 1995; Olio, 1994; Pezdek, 1994).
Yet it is indisputable that some counselors, psychotherapists, social workers, and
psychiatrists have used suggested memory work in recent years. Individual cases are
documented in publications promoting memory work (e.g., Bass & Davis, 1988;
Blume, 1990; Claridge, 1992; K. R. Clark, 1993; Ellenson, 1985; Engel, 1989;
Frederickson, 1992; Maltz, 1990; McCann & Pearlman, 1990; Meiselman, 1990; Olio,
1989) and in therapy notes and testimony presented in legal cases (see Loftus, 1994a;
Ofshe & Walters, 1994; Pendergrast, 1995).
Recent surveys suggest that such practices are not limited to a tiny "fringe"
group of poorly trained practitioners. For example, Poole et al.'s (1995) national
surveys of certified psychotherapists in the United States (N = 145) and United
Kingdom (N = 57) found that 71% (return rate = 40%) reported using techniques
such as hypnosis, body work, and dream interpretation to help clients remember
CSA. Moreover, 25% reported a constellation of beliefs and practices that led Poole
et al. to classify them as "memory focused." Such respondents indicated that they
believe that remembering CSA is important for effective psychotherapy and that
some of their clients had hidden memories, that they are sometimes "fairly certain"
after an initial session with a client who denies a CSA history that the client has
hidden memories of CSA, and that they use two or more techniques to help clients
recover memories of CSA. These criteria are too broad to limit this subgroup only to
those who use the most suggestive approaches, but the findings are nonetheless
interesting. In Survey 1 this subgroup of respondents indicated, on average, that 60%
of women clients whom they suspected of hidden histories of abuse eventually
recovered such memories, and in Survey 2 they indicated, on average, that 34% of all
women clients who initially denied a CSA history eventually recovered such
memories. In a survey of Seattle-area practitioners, L. Berliner (personal communi-
cation, October 19, 1994) found that although most respondents did not indicate a
focus on helping clients recover memories of CSA, a minority did. Andrews et al.
(1995), in their survey of certified therapists in the United Kingdom, found that the
852 LINDSAY AND READ

10% who reported use of hypnotic regression reported more clients recovering
previously unknown histories of CSA than did other therapists. In Yapko's (1994)
and S. Smith's (1991) surveys of therapists, a substantial percentage of respondents
reported ill-founded beliefs and practices regarding memory recovery techniques
such as hypnosis.
Memory work has also been widely popularized through self-help books.
Santrock, Minnett, and Campbell's (1994) The Authoritative Guide to Self-Help
Books, provides evaluative summaries of self-help books, based in part on ratings of
350 books by 500 'experts in clinical and counseling psychology from all 50 states . ..
[who] possess the very best psychological knowledge in the United States" (p. 3; all
respondents were members of the counseling or clinical divisions of the American
Psychological Association). The first chapter of reviews is dedicated to "Abuse and
Recovery," and Bass and Davis's (1988) The Courage to Heal received a five-star
rating ("strongly recommended"):
This book gets extremely high marks. Any woman who knows she was sexually abused
as a child or has the slightest inkling she might have been abused will benefit from this
book . . . [It] can help women recognize if they were sexually abused as a child . . .
[AJfter leading women to become aware of a painful past, [it] moves on in positive
ways to help them heal and recover, (p. 12)

Consistent with this recommendation, 44% of the U.S. practitioners in Poole et al.'s
(1995) survey indicated that they had recommended The Courage to Heal to clients.
Pendergrast (1995, p. 51) reported that this particular book has sold 750,000 copies.
Although The Courage to Heal and other self-help books that include an emphasis on
recovering memories of CSA may provide much-needed support for survivors of
CSA, they are alarmingly suggestive (D. S. Lindsay & Read, 1994; Pendergrast,
1995). In such books, an authoritative source explains that many people have
histories of CSA of which they are unaware, that many common psychological
problems are evidence of hidden abuse histories, that doubting abuse is often a sign
of "denial," and that recovering memories is a painful but important step toward
healing. These books prescribe memory recovery exercises to enhance imagery and
lower memory-monitoring criteria and provide vivid personal accounts of people who
have recovered memories of previously unsuspected histories of CSA.
In summary, the majority of qualified practitioners do not focus on searching for
hidden histories of CSA in their clients, but it appears that a nontrivial minority have
taken such an approach in recent years. Given that a very large number of people
provide insight therapies of various types under various auspices in North America
(Baker, 1994), each with a number of clients, a very small percentage of therapists
translates into many thousands of clients. Moreover, it appears that a large
percentage of psychotherapists make at least some use of potentially suggestive
techniques to help some clients search for memories of CSA. Finally, suggestive
memory work has also been widely promoted in self-help books.

Scientific Evidence Regarding the Validity of Recovered Memories


Base Rate of Hidden Memories
A core premise underlying the use of CSA-oriented memory work in psycho-
therapy is the belief that a substantial percentage of clients have histories of CSA but
are unaware of those histories. The rationale for this premise is that (a) a large
RECOVERED MEMORIES 853

percentage (e.g., 33%) of women are survivors of CSA (e.g., Bass & Davis, 1988; cf.
Finkelhor, 1994); (b) a large percentage of abuse survivors (e.g., 50%) have no
conscious memories of the abuse (e.g., Blume, 1990; Maltz, 1990); and (c) because
CSA causes psychological problems, survivors with hidden memories are overrepre-
sented among psychotherapy clients. If these premises are true, then tens of millions
of women in North America suffered CSA but do not know it, and a large proportion
of people who seek therapy have hidden histories of CSA. In the following sections,
we review evidence on the base rates of CSA and amnesia for CSA histories and then
consider evidence related to practitioners' ability to discriminate between clients
with and without hidden CSA histories.
Base rate of CSA. As noted in our comments regarding the cultural context of
the debate about memory work, in the past two decades it has become clear that CSA
is much more common than once was thought. If CSA is defined sufficiently broadly
(e.g., to include inappropriate comments, looks, and gestures directed by adults at
children under 18 years old), then it is likely that most North American women are
survivors of CSA. Even if CSA is defined narrowly (e.g., restricted to sexual contact
by an adult), evidence indicates that there are millions of survivors of such abuse in
North America (e.g., Finkelhor, 1994).
It is important that our society continue to acknowledge the reality of CSA and
take steps against it. The not-so-old belief that only one in a million persons in the
United States is an incest survivor (Weinberg, 1955, cited in Russell, 1983) has been
soundly rejected. It is also important, however, that prevalence estimates based on
vague or broad definitions of CSA (e.g., those that combine offenses ranging from
sexual comments to exhibitionism to intercourse) not be misconstrued as estimates
of the prevalence of the most extreme forms of abuse. Similarly, prevalence estimates
that include survivors of an isolated instance of CSA should not be misconstrued as
estimates of the prevalence of repeated abuses. For example, Russell (1983) found
that 38% of the 930 women interviewed in her study reported at least one instance of
contact CSA by age 18, and 4.5% reported any form of "actual or attempted" contact
sexual abuse by their fathers. Burnam (1985, cited by Peters, Wyatt, & Finkelhor,
1986) found that 6% of 1,623 randomly sampled Los Angeles women reported forced
sexual contact of any kind before the age of 16 years. Wyatt (1985) found that 75% of
retrospective CSA reports in her sample of 248 Los Angeles women involved a single
instance of abuse; 1.6% of the respondents reported actual contact CSA of any kind
perpetrated by fathers. Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis, and Smith (1990), in their
national survey of 2,626 U.S. men and women, found that 34.2% of the women
reported some form of contact CSA by age 18, but "the majority of the experiences
were one-time events" (p. 21) and 2% reported any form (contact or noncontact) of
CSA perpetrated by their fathers or stepfathers. In a recent review, Finkelhor (1994)
argued that the best evidence indicates that 20% of women in the United States
experienced some form of CSA, and that 4 to 5% experienced CSA involving
penetration or oral-genital contact. Finkelhor also emphasized that retrospective
studies indicate that more than half of CSA is extrafamilial and that approximately a
third of offenders are under 18 years of age.
Our point is not to minimize the reality of incestuous sexual abuse of children,
and we do not mean to dismiss as trivial other forms of abuse. Even when abuse is
defined narrowly, the best data indicate that there are millions of survivors, and even
instances of noncontact abuse may be harmful and are to be abjured. Our point is
854 LINDSAY AND READ

that prevalence estimates that include reports of noncontact CSA, single instances of
extrafamilial CSA, and so on, should not be misconstrued as prevalence estimates for
repeated, incestuous contact CSA. We focus here on repeated incestuous contact
abuse because (a) evidence indicates that such abuse is most strongly associated with
subsequent psychological problems (e.g., Briere & Conte, 1993; Briere & Elliott,
1992; Conte & Berliner, 1988; Finkelhor & Browne, 1988) and (b) extreme forms of
incestuous abuse have been the focus of writers and practitioners who favor searches
for suspected hidden memories of abuse (e.g., Bass & Davis, 1988; Blume, 1990;
Courtois, 1992; Freud, 1896/1962; Poole et al., 1995; Wakefield & Underwager,
1992). The best available evidence suggests that a very small percentage of North
Americans have experienced repeated, incestuous contact CSA. Even if retrospec-
tive self-report studies underestimate the prevalence of such abuse, and even if the
base rate of such abuse is several times higher in the population of people seeking
psychotherapy (a point on which evidence is mixed: See Briere & Zaidi, 1989; Brown
& Anderson, 1991; Bryer, Nelson, Miller, & Kroll, 1987; Hutchings & Dutton, 1993;
Poole et al., 1995; Stinson & Hendrick, 1992), people with such histories would still
make up a small minority of clients in most general practices.
Memory for CSA. As noted earlier, a central premise for memory work is the
belief that many adult psychotherapy clients are survivors of CSA who are unaware
that they were sexually abused as children. It is claimed that children "dissociate"
during CSA in ways that impair later conscious recollection or that they later
"repress" memories of the trauma. The secret nature of CSA and the victim's
powerlessness are also said to contribute to amnesia for abuse. According to some
writers, repression, dissociation, or both, are so effective that children who are
routinely subjected to horrific abuse often show no obvious symptoms and are
oblivious to the abuse until decades later (e.g., Terr, 1994; Walker, 1994). In this
section, we review evidence for and against the idea that memories of CSA are often
hidden from awareness. Perspectives on this issue have ranged from the molecular
and cellular levels (e.g., effects of hormones and neurotransmitters on brain
functioning: Cahill, Prins, Weber, & McGaugh, 1994; LeDoux, 1992; McGaugh,
1992; Siegel, 1995; van der Kolk, 1988) to the postulation of higher level cognitive
processes (Ganaway, 1989; Freud, 1899/1962; Freyd, 1993; Morton, 1994; Terr, 1993,
1994; Yuille & Tollestrup, 1992).
It is well established that people are unable to recall many of their past
experiences, especially those of childhood, and sometimes do not recollect even
quite memorable adulthood events. For example, Loftus (1993, p. 522) cited
government research showing that adult survey respondents sometimes do not
remember relatively recent automobile accidents or hospital visits. Studies of
"directed forgetting," in which people are instructed to forget some studied material
and later receive a surprise test on that material, indicate that instructions to forget
can lower later memory test performance (e.g., Bjork, 1989; Vokey & Allen, 1995).
Rehearsing an event (either mentally or aloud) serves to improve later memory for
that event, so it is reasonable to propose that avoiding thinking or talking about an
experience can contribute to forgetting that experience (e.g., Bower, 1990). Memory
performance also sometimes shows context- and state-dependency effects (i.e.,
memory performance sometimes suffers slightly when the environmental context or
mental-emotional state of the person change from study to test; e.g., Davies &
Thomson, 1988; Eich, 1995), and although such effects are often not statistically
RECOVERED MEMORIES 855

reliable, they might contribute to poor adulthood memory for childhood traumas.
Thus, several cognitive processes may contribute to forgetting of CSA (see Freyd,
1993; Siegel, 1995). These processes are opposed, however, by other processes that
lessen the probability that a history of CSA will be forgotten: In general, the core
details of highly salient life experiences tend to be well remembered (Christiansen,
1992; Winograd & Neisser, 1992). Given these competing mechanisms, the likeli-
hood of completely forgetting a history of CSA is an important empirical question.
Below we review the seven studies published to date that addressed this question, as
well as studies of memory for other traumatic events (see other reviews by Enns et
al., 1995; Koss, Tromp, & Tharan, 1995; Pope & Hudson, 1995; Siegel, 1995).
In six studies, people were asked if they had ever experienced a period during
which they could not remember CSA that they now remember. In Briere and Conte's
(1993) questionnaire study of 450 women whose therapists were part of a sexual
abuse treatment network, 59% of the 100% who reported CSA indicated that there
had been a period during which they were unable to remember the first instance of
abuse. In Loftus, Polonsky, and Fullilove's (1994) interview study of 105 women in a
drug rehabilitation program, 19% of the 54% who reported CSA indicated a prior
period during which they forgot the abuse. In Gold, Hughes, and Hohnecker's (1994)
intake interviews of an unspecified number of women in a sex abuse survivors'
treatment program, 30% of the (presumably) 100% who reported CSA indicated
that there had been at least a 1-year period during which memories of the abuse were
"completely blocked out." In Sheiman's (1993) questionnaire study of 196 undergradu-
ates, half of the 12% who reported CSA indicated that there had been a period
during which they did not remember the abuse. In Feldman-Summers and Pope's
(1994) questionnaire study of 330 therapists, 41% of the 24% who reported CSA
indicated that there had been a period during which they could not remember some
or all of the abuse. In Williams's (1994a) prospective study of 129 women with
recorded histories of an instance of CSA, 12% of the 62% who reported the recorded
instance when interviewed 17 years later indicated that there had been a period when
they did not remember that instance of abuse.
The most global criticism of these studies concerns the ambiguity of the question
respondents were asked, such as, Was there ever a time when you could not
remember some or all of the abuse? (Loftus, Garry, & Feldman, 1994; Ofshe &
Walters, 1994). The meaning of affirmative responses to this question is not clear:
Were respondents reporting periods during which they avoided thinking of the abuse
or periods during which they would have been unable to recall it if asked? Williams
(1994a) noted that some of her respondents indicated that they were referring to
periods of time during which they avoided thinking about the abuse. The question
was phrased in different ways in different studies, but in all cases it is unclear how
well the question mapped on to complete amnesia for a CSA history. Even if it is
assumed that most respondents were referring to complete amnesia for abuse
histories rather than to avoidance, forgetting of details, or forgetting of particular
instances, questions can be raised about people's ability to assess whether they would
have been able to remember CSA during a period of time when they claim not to
have remembered it (Loftus, Garry, & Feldman, 1994; Ofshe & Walters, 1994;
Schooler, 1994).
As a more general methodological comment, an implicit assumption in these
studies has been that affirmative responses to questions about forgetting of CSA
856 LINDSAY AND READ

reveal magnitudes of memory loss peculiar to traumatic experiences; however, Read,


Seelau, and Nicholls (1995) found that 33.1% of a large group of undergraduates
affirmed that they had experienced recall of other sorts of autobiographical
experiences that had been forgotten for some period of time. In a follow-up study,
Read et al. (1995) found that 31% of 204 undergraduates reported having
experienced "recovery" of long-forgotten autobiographical memories; the vast
majority of these indicated that although they had not thought of the event for an
extended period of time, they believed they would have if they had been asked.
Finally, questions can also be raised about the representativeness of the samples in
studies of reported amnesia for CSA and about the contribution of memory work to
reports of prior amnesia: All of the people in Briere and Conte's (1993) study had
been in treatment for sexual abuse, half of Feldman-Summers and Pope's (1994)
respondents who reported a period of being unable to remember some or all of their
abuse histories said that they were in therapy when memories returned, and all of
Gold et al.'s (1994) respondents had self-selected as clients in a sex-abuse survivor's
treatment program. These points do not entirely vitiate the consistent finding that a
substantial percentage of people who report CSA histories report that there were
periods when they could not remember some or all of those histories, but they do
raise questions about what those findings tell us about the prevalence of hidden
histories of CSA in clients presenting for psychotherapy for common psychological
problems who initially report no memories of CSA.
Herman and Schatzow (1987) used the reports of 53 adult clients in a 12-week
trauma-memory-oriented group-therapy program to estimate the prevalence of
forgetting of CSA. All of the clients reported CSA histories. Sixty-four percent of the
clients were judged to have evidenced some degree of prior forgetting of CSA
because they reported new memories of CSA during the therapy program. Of the
clients who reported new memories, 74% subsequently reported corroboration of
their abuse histories, but the nature of some of the corroboration appears weak (e.g.,
clients' self-reports that family members in some way supported their beliefs), and it
is not clear whether the corroboration referred to newly remembered abuse or to
never-forgotten abuse.
Williams (1994a) interviewed 129 women who 17 years earlier, during childhood,
had been judged by hospital authorities to have experienced an instance of CSA. In
the course of a single interview that used no special memory recovery techniques,
88% of these women reported instances of CSA, and 62% were judged (by a lenient
criterion) to have described the particular recorded instance in response to questions
about their history of CSA.3 Interviewers did not ask the women about the particular
recorded instance—indeed, they were blind to it. Williams did not indicate how
many of the 12% who reported no abuse history were under 4 years of age when the
recorded instance occurred, so we cannot estimate the extent to which infantile
amnesia (i.e., the well-established, normal paucity of adulthood recollections of
events that occurred before the age of 3 or 4 years) contributed to these cases.
Williams's results suggest that a substantial minority of victims of CSA do not report
a particular instance when interviewed years later and that some survivors of a
recorded instance do not report any CSA when interviewed about their CSA
histories (probably because they do not recall the abuse). As Williams (1994b) noted,

'Several secondary sources (e.g., Walker, 1994, p. 85) have erroneously reported that 38% of
Williams's (1994a) respondents reported no history of CSA.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 857

however, the findings do not support the idea that a large percentage of people with
extensive histories of CSA are unaware that they ever experienced such abuse.
To our knowledge, the seven studies just reviewed are the only published
investigations of the base rate of forgetting of CSA (excluding a few in the literature
on multiple personality disorder [MPD], which suffer from numerous methodologi-
cal flaws; see Read & Lindsay, 1994, pp. 419-421). Taken together, these studies
show that a substantial minority of abuse survivors do not remember a recorded
instance of CSA, that a smaller minority of abuse survivors do not report any CSA,
and that a substantial percentage of people who report CSA histories indicate that
there have been periods during which they could not remember some or all of the
abuse. In our view, however, this research does not provide strong support for the
belief that a large percentage of people seeking psychotherapy have recoverable
hidden histories of CSA. First, all but the Williams (1994a) study relied on
retrospective self-reports of a period of time when respondents could not remember
CSA that they later remembered. As noted earlier, the meaning of these reports is
not clear: Were respondents indicating periods of avoidance or periods during which
they could not have recalled abuse if asked? Can people accurately answer the latter
question? Were respondents referring to periods of not remembering particular
instances or details of abuse or to periods of not remembering that any abuse had
ever occurred? Furthermore, respondents in the studies by Briere and Conte (1993),
Gold et al. (1994), and Herman and Schatzow (1987) were in therapy for sexual
abuse, which suggests that they were not amnesic for all abuse and raises questions
about the generalizability of the findings. Finally, the fact that all of Briere and
Conte's (1993) and Herman and Schatzow's (1987) respondents were in therapy, as
were half of Feldman-Summers and Pope's (1994) respondents who reported prior
periods of not remembering CSA, raises the possibility that suggestive memory work
may have played a role in some reports of previous periods of not remembering
abuse. Other research on memory for traumatic experiences, reviewed later, also
does not support the claim that a large percentage of CSA survivors are unaware that
they experienced such abuse as children.
In an unpublished dissertation study, Vardi (1994, described by Brown, 1995)
found that adults who reported histories of CSA performed significantly more poorly
than did control participants (matched on demographic variables with the exception
of amount of time in therapy) on measures of memory for names of teachers, schools
attended, and significant public events around the time of the reported abuse. This
finding is consistent with other evidence, cited by Schooler (1994), that traumata can
cause general memory disruptions, but it should not be misconstrued as evidence
that abuse survivors commonly forget that they were abused; indeed, we assume that
Vardi's abuse survivors remembered histories of abuse. It must also be emphasized
that poor memory for early childhood events is a normal phenomenon, exhibited in
most mammals (e.g., Spear, 1979; Usher & Neisser, 1993). Finally, although CSA
may impair memory for later childhood events, other factors may also have this
effect.
Cameron (1994) surveyed 72 women with reported CSA histories, most of whom
were recruited by their therapists, who were asked to recruit clients who had either
been totally amnesic for (« = 25) or had always remembered the reported abuse
(n = 21). The therapists also recruited 14 clients who reported partial amnesia, and
12 women in a prison program for sexual abuse survivors also participated. Cameron
858 LINDSAY AND READ

compared responses of these women with findings of studies of combat veterans with
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). One difference was that combat veterans
usually sought help 9 to 30 months after their return from Vietnam, whereas the
clients entered therapy 20 to 50 years after childhood. A related difference was that
the most common symptom among veterans was intrusive memories, whereas among
clients the most common symptom was the absence of trauma memories for decades
and what Cameron described as strategies for avoiding memories (e.g., alcohol or
drug use in 36%, keeping "overbusy" in 75%). Symptoms of anxiety and depression
were also reported, but it appears that these symptoms were primarily associated
with recovering memories. Cameron did not indicate what proportion of clients who
reported amnesia had recovered memories through therapy, but the article is imbued
with the beliefs that underlie memory work, and 22% of the sample reported types of
CSA that are extremely rare (torture in ritual abuse or childhood gang rapes).
Cameron's data document that some clients experience "memory recovery," but in
view of the sampling procedure and the likely role of memory work in producing
some of the reports of amnesia in her sample, the findings shed little light on the base
rate of amnesia for CSA.
Cameron (1994) was not the first to argue that amnesia for CSA is analogous to
the amnesia sometimes exhibited by combat veterans (e.g., Herman, 1992). In fact,
however, veterans with PTSD rarely if ever forget that they experienced wartime
trauma, but rather exhibit emotional numbing, avoidance of combat-related stimuli,
symptoms of depression or anxiety, generalized cognitive deficits, and perhaps the
hallmark symptom, intrusive memories of their experiences (e.g., Figley, 1986;
Sonnenberg, Blank, & Talbott, 1985; Wilson, Harel, & Kahana, 1988). Veterans with
PTSD sometimes exhibit amnesia for particular traumatic events, but Silver (1986),
in a chapter describing an inpatient program for Vietnam veterans with PTSD, stated
that "Complete amnesia may be encountered but it is rare" (p. 221). We know of no
case in which a Vietnam veteran believed for years that nothing terrible had
happened in Vietnam and then remembered combat traumas. Similarly, survivors of
the Holocaust rarely if ever forget that they experienced Nazi concentration camps
(Kraft, 1994; Wagenaar & Groeneweg, 1990). In summary, although CSA can cause
PTSD (Briere & Runtz, 1990; Rowan, Foy, Rodriguea, & Ryan, 1994), documented
cases of PTSD bear little resemblance to cases in which people believe for decades
that they were not sexually abused as children and then later come to believe that
they were.
Like combat veterans, many adult rape victims experience intrusive memories of
their assaults (e.g., Foa, Rothbaum, Riggs, & Murdock, 1991), but there are few
reports of victims who forget that they were raped. Christiansen and Nilsson (1989)
described a rape victim with amnesia for the assault, but the woman did not evidence
a smooth and seamless encapsulation of the rape with no obvious signs of
psychological impairment; rather, she exhibited a dramatic and general retrograde
amnesia (e.g., did not know who she was, did not recognize friends, and was amnesic
for her entire history). Such symptoms appear typical of psychogenic amnesia
(Schacter & Kihlstrom, 1989). In contrast, as noted earlier, some advocates of
memory work claim that sexually abused children often repress or dissociate ongoing
abuse yet show no symptoms of trauma or amnesia until years later. That claim also
conflicts with data on child victims of sexual abuse, who often exhibit psychiatric
problems (e.g., Merry & Andrews, 1994; Gates, O'Toole, Lynch, Stern, & Cooney,
J994), although some victims, while aware of the abuse, are asymptomatic (Beutler,
RECOVERED MEMORIES 859

Williams, & Zetzer, 1994; Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, 1993; Levitt &
Pinnell, 1994).
Research on children's memories for traumatic experiences also sheds light on
the plausibility of the claim that children frequently forget traumas, such that they
could be repeatedly sexually abused and not consciously remember it later. For
example, Malmquist (1986) reported a study of children who had witnessed the
murder of a parent. Far from exhibiting amnesia for the tragedy, the children
suffered intrusive memories of it. Pynoos and Eth (1984) described 40 children who
had witnessed the murder of a parent and reported that these children had
exceptionally vivid recollections. Green et al. (1994) assessed 99 survivors of a
disaster that had occurred 17 years earlier (when respondents ranged in age from 2
to 15 years) and found no reliable differences between disaster survivors and a
control group on any of the measures, including PTSD; although not specifically
mentioned, the report implies that none of the survivors forgot the disaster.
Dollinger (1985) studied 38 child survivors of a lightning strike that killed three of
their playmates; a month later one boy, described as a "side-flash victim who was
hospitalized for depression as well as medical problems" (p. 377), was said to have no
memory of the event, but there is no indication that any of the other children failed to
remember it when assessed 9 months later.4 Femina, Yeager, and Lewis (1990) found
no evidence of amnesia when physically abused children were interviewed at age 24
years. Terr (1988, 1991) studied children who had survived extremely traumatic
experiences and reported that those who had experienced a single, unique trauma
had vivid recollections of it, whereas those who had experienced repeated episodes
of trauma remembered bits and pieces of different experiences rather than each
episode as an integrated whole.
Terr (1994) claimed that her studies indicate that unique (Type I) childhood
traumas are generally well-remembered, but repeated (Type II) traumas are often
entirely forgotten. As others (Loftus & Ketcham, 1994; Ofshe & Walters, 1994) have
pointed out, Terr offered no compelling evidence in support of the latter conclusion,
which flies in the face of a host of empirical studies demonstrating that although
repeated experiences of a given kind often lead to difficulty in recalling details of
specific instances, they also lead to excellent memory for the "script" (Schank &
Abelson, 1977) of the experience. In fact, Terr's case histories do not include clear
evidence of any child being entirely amnesic for traumas that occurred beyond age 3
years. Consistent with cognitive research, evidence from a few highly publicized
molestation trials suggests that survivors usually remember the abuse and that the
likelihood of remembering increases with the number of incidents (Goleman, 1992;
Yapko, 1994). According to psychiatrist S. Grassian (personal communication,
January 6, 1995), who surveyed 43 people who alleged CSA by a priest, only 16%
indicated that they had not thought about the abuse since it had happened, and none
received memory work to remember it. Grassian found that as the reported number
of molestation experiences increased, so did the likelihood of reported later thoughts
about them; most people who said they had not thought about the molestation since
it occurred indicated one-time incidents. In summary, contrary to Terr's claims,
evidence indicates that survivors of multiple traumas are more likely to remember
they have had such experiences than are victims of an isolated trauma.

4
Closed head injuries, electroconvulsive shock, and similar brain injuries reliably cause retrograde
amnesia for events preceding the injury (e.g., Pinel, 1990, pp. 411-412).
860 LINDSAY AND READ

Howe, Courage, and Peterson (1994) interviewed children who had experienced
physical traumas (broken bones, contusions, etc.) sufficiently severe to require
hospital treatment. The children were interviewed a few days after the trauma and a
second time 6 weeks later. There was no evidence of unusual forgetting of trauma.
Instead, forgetting appeared to follow its usual course, with the children remember-
ing the central aspects of the trauma very well, but forgetting or remembering
incorrectly peripheral details over time. Furthermore, there was no relationship
between parental ratings of the child's emotional stress during the trauma or during
the hospital treatment (when some children were so hysterical that they had to be
physically restrained) and later memory. As Howe et al. pointed out, several other
studies have also found little or no evidence of a negative relationship between stress
and later memory (Goodman, Bottoms, Schwartz-Kenney, & Rudy, 1991; Goodman,
Hirschman, Hepps, & Rudy, 1991; Ornstein, Merritt, & Baker-Ward, 1995; Peter-
son, Moreno, & Harbeck-Weber, 1993). Goodman, Quas, Batterman-Faunce,
Riddlesberger, and Kuhn (1994) studied 4- to 10-year-old's memories of a medical
procedure in which the child is catheterized, his or her bladder is filled with
radioactive liquid, and the child is required to urinate on the table while being
X-rayed. Goodman et al. found that children neither entirely forgot nor perfectly
remembered the experience. Pynoos and Nader (1989) interviewed children about a
sniper attack near their school and found evidence of forgetting of details and
memory errors (including some children who did not witness the event but reported
that they had) but not for amnesia for the experience. Stuber, Nader, Yasuda,
Pynoos, and Cohen (1991) found that six children who had experienced painful bone
marrow transplant operations exhibited PTSD symptoms when interviewed up to 12
months later (e.g., intrusive thoughts, avoidance of topic), but there is no indication
in their report that any of the children had forgotten the traumatic experiences.
Peters (1991) found that children stressed by the sounding of a fire alarm
remembered less about concurrent events than did control children and were also
more suggestible, but the decrement was not in memory for the stressful event (i.e.,
the fire alarm) itself. Parker, Bahrick, Lundy, Fivush, and Levitt (1995) found that
Florida children whose homes were most dramatically damaged by Hurricane
Andrew reported less information when interviewed about the hurricane than did
children whose homes were only moderately damaged. However, the decrement was
restricted to reports of storm-related events that had occurred during the days and
weeks after the storm (e.g., home and yard repairs), and it is likely that more of the
children whose homes took the full brunt of the storm were more often moved to
nondamaged areas immediately after the storm. In any case, no child forgot the
terrifying experience of the hurricane (J. Parker, personal communication, Decem-
ber 20,1994).
Studies of corporal punishment of children provide converging evidence for the
hypothesis that forgetting of childhood trauma is the exception rather than the rule.
Severe corporal punishment at the hands of loved ones can be terrifying to children,
as well as physically painful and shaming. In some cases, such experiences overlap
with CSA in other ways as well (e.g., being spanked on bare buttocks). Physical
punishment—particularly severe punishment—is often meted out in private, away
from witnesses. During such punishments, children may "dissociate" to escape
mentally, and it seems likely that such punishments are rarely a subject of subsequent
conversation. Doubtless, children experience motivations to forget such beatings.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 861

Research suggests that most North American adults not only have such experiences
in their histories (Finkelhor & Dzubia-Leatherman, 1994; Wolfner & Gelles, 1993)
but remember them (DiTomasso & Routh, 1993). On the basis of research on
autobiographical memory, we speculate that people with multiple experiences of this
sort are likely to have the sense that memories of details of some events are blended
with others and that some specific details and individual events do not come to mind
at all; in contrast, people who only once or twice received severe physical punishment
are likely to remember the experience in some detail.
Some defenders of memory work claim that neurophysiological research
supports the belief that traumas are often entirely forgotten yet could later be
recollected under certain circumstances (e.g., Brown, 1995; Enns et al., 1995). In
fact, however, the relatively small number of studies do not offer clear and consistent
support for this idea. Trauma is associated with changes in electrochemical activity in
the brain, and some animal studies indicate that the amygdala can mediate learning
of stressful events (Kandel & Kandel, 1994; Siegel, 1995; van der Kolk, 1988), but it is
not clear to us whether or how such findings support the notion of recoverable
hidden memories. Other research with animals and humans suggests that emotional
experiences are associated with increased levels of adrenalin in the brain and that
heightened adrenalin leads to better memory (e.g., Cahill et al., 1994).
It has also been argued that task and population dissociations between direct
("explicit") and indirect ("implicit") tests of memory support the idea that people
often have recoverable hidden memories of CSA. Indirect tests of memory involve
presenting experiment participants with a task that is in some way related to
previously studied material (e.g., asking them to solve word fragments, such as
R C VE , after exposing them to a list of words that included solutions for some
of the fragments). Direct tests, in contrast, instruct participants to remember the
studied materials (e.g., recall a previously studied list or make recognition judgments
on studied and nonstudied words). People can demonstrate memory for past events
on indirect tests when they do not remember or recognize those events on a direct
test (see Kelley & Lindsay, in press, and Roediger & McDermott, 1993, for reviews).
Such findings show that people's behavior and experience can be affected by events
that they do not consciously remember, but they do not support the idea that people
could recover episodic recollections of those events. Indeed, many memory theorists
have argued that indirect tests reflect use of a memory system fundamentally
different from that which supports episodic remembering—that is, "implicit memo-
ries" cannot later become "explicit memories" (Squire, 1994).
In summary, some factors likely exacerbate forgetting of childhood traumas, but
others likely serve to preserve memories of such experiences. Research on autobio-
graphical memory suggests that people who were sexually abused in the first few
years of life are unlikely to remember the experiences as adults and that those who
experienced one or a few isolated instances of sexual abuse (especially events that
were not extremely salient at the time) may often not remember them as adults. In
our reading, however, studies of memory for traumatic events offer little support for
the idea that adults with extensive histories of extreme CSA often forget those
histories yet could recover memories under the appropriate conditions. Indeed, the
evidence reviewed in this section suggests that this is a rare phenomenon, such that
most practitioners would encounter few such cases. Of course, the validity of this
conclusion depends on (a) the aptness of criticisms of studies presented as evidence
862 LINDSAY AND READ

that complete amnesia for CSA is commonplace and (b) the generalizability of
studies of memory for childhood trauma, which indicate that people are unlikely to
forget the occurrence of highly traumatic childhood events experienced after the age
of 3 or 4 years. As Schooler (1994) eloquently argued, at present there are too few
studies, and too many differences between the studies and the question of forgetting
of CSA, to justify definitive statements. Nonetheless, existing research on children's
and adults' memory for physical traumas, murders, natural disasters, and so on,
suggests that memory for trauma follows the same principles as memories for
mundane events, and that, because of their salience, traumatic events are more,
rather than less, likely to be remembered. A recent review of research on trauma
memories by clinicians Koss et al. (1995) reached the same conclusion. The sole
published prospective study of adult survivors of CSA (Williams, 1994a) provides
converging evidence that the majority of adults who experienced contact CSA
beyond age 3 or 4 years remember that they had such experiences (although they may
not remember particular instances).
Summary. In this section, we reviewed evidence on the base rates of contact
CSA and forgetting of CSA. We argued that although CSA is much more prevalent
than once was thought, a relatively small percentage of North Americans have
experienced the most extreme forms of CSA (e.g., repeated contact CSA). We also
argued that research indicates that traumatic events experienced beyond the first few
years of life tend to be remembered relatively well. We do not claim that it is
impossible for survivors of repeated contact CSA to forget that they were abused.
Rather, our point is that evidence suggests that such forgetting is rare. Forgetting in
ways that enable later remembering is likely rarer still. Together, these findings and
arguments imply that only a very small percentage of people have hidden histories of
CSA that they could recover through memory work. Awareness of these lower base
rates might lead trauma-oriented therapists to be more cautious about attributing
clients' symptoms to hidden histories of CSA, particularly in light of evidence,
reviewed in the next section, regarding the difficulty of discriminating between
clients with and without hidden histories of CSA.

Reliability of "Indicators" of Hidden Memories of CSA


There is evidence of associations between remembered CSA and a variety of
adulthood psychological problems. Some have taken such findings (in the context of
the belief that a large percentage of clients have hidden memories of CSA) as
support for the idea that clients with certain symptoms likely have hidden histories of
CSA (e.g., Gelinas's, 1983, "disguised presentations"; cf. Freud, 1896/1962). This
view reaches its extreme form in self-help books that offer readers checklists of
numerous common symptoms said to be indicative of hidden histories of CSA
(Blume, 1990; Frederickson, 1992; Ratican, 1992). Bass and Davis (1988) presented a
similar variety of "indicators" embedded in prose rather than as a list. Similar beliefs
are reported, in less simplistic form, by some highly trained practitioners (e.g.,
Courtois, 1988, 1992; Herman, 1992; Walker, 1994). Collectively, the 212 respon-
dents in Poole et al.'s (1995) surveys of highly trained therapists listed dozens of
"indicators" of CSA, including sexual dysfunction, relationship problems, depres-
sion, anxiety, eating disorders, dissociation, substance abuse, and born-again
Christianity.
Some ideas about detecting hidden histories of CSA on the basis of symptoms
appear to reflect the common logical error of "affirming the consequent" (i.e.,
RECOVERED MEMORIES 863

assuming that if A causes B, then all cases of B must have been caused by A). The
fact that CSA is associated with particular symptoms does not mean that those
symptoms can be taken as indicators of CSA. This is because it is well established
that many other factors are also associated with such symptoms, and some of these
other factors have higher base rates than CSA. By analogy, brain tumors are
associated with headaches, but this does not mean that all or most people with
headaches have brain tumors, because many other factors—some of which have
much higher base rates than brain tumors—can also cause headaches.
Some proponents of memory work have emphasized the importance of the
simultaneous presence of multiple "indicators" for forming a clinical judgment,
hypothesis, or "index of suspicion" about hidden memories (Ellenson, 1985; Ratican,
1992). The underlying idea is that although other factors might account for individual
symptoms, clients with multiple indicators (e.g., low self-esteem and suppressed
appetite and relationship difficulties and sexual dysfunction and sleep disorders)
very likely have histories of CSA. This rationale would be reasonable if each of the
symptoms were independent of the others, and if the constellation of symptoms was
uniquely diagnostic of CSA, but in fact multiple symptoms are often highly
correlated aspects of a particular syndrome (e.g., all of the symptoms listed in the
preceding sentence are characteristic of clinical depression, which has multiple
etiologies; see Ceci & Loftus's, 1994, discussion of "incremental validity"). CSA is
associated with many adulthood psychological problems, but some people with CSA
histories do not have those symptoms, and many people who have those symptoms do
not have CSA histories. Thus, there is no well-defined post-CSA syndrome that could
be used reliably to identify people with hidden histories of CSA (Beitchman et al.,
1992).5
Research on the reliability of diagnoses raises further doubts about therapists'
abilities to discriminate between clients with and without repressed histories of CSA.
Even highly trained therapists working with well-defined diagnostic categories often
disagree about the appropriate diagnoses for particular clients (e.g., di Nardo,
Moras, Barlow, Rapee, & Brown, 1993; Herron, Schultz, & Welt, 1992). Because
there is no established post-CSA syndrome, clinical judgments regarding hidden
memories of CSA are almost certainly less reliable than formal diagnoses. In view of
the likelihood that practitioners' theoretical orientations can bias their clinical
judgments (e.g., Sarason & Sarason, 1984, p. 81), there are grounds for concern that
those who believe that CSA is a major source of adulthood psychopathology often
erroneously form the opinion that nonabused clients have hidden memories. In any
case, even if one accepted the claims of proponents of memory work regarding the
base rate of complete amnesia for CSA, and even if one assumed extraordinary
diagnostic accuracy among such therapists, the likelihood that diagnoses of re-
pressed memories would be erroneous is surprisingly high. For example, if 33% of
clients are abuse survivors, if 50% of these are amnesic for the abuse, and if clinical
judgments about hidden histories are 90% accurate, then nearly a third of all positive
judgments about hidden memories would be wrong (assuming symmetry between
positive and negative diagnoses). As any of these parameters is lowered the
likelihood of false-positive judgments increases dramatically (see D. S. Lindsay &
Read, 1994; Read & Lindsay, 1994).

5
As C. J. Dalenberg (personal communication, February 2, 1995) pointed out, checklists purporting
to evidence that respondents were not sexually abused as children are equally suspect.
864 LINDSAY AND READ

"Dissociability" appears to be emerging as the symptom most often mentioned


as an indicator of childhood trauma (e.g., Sheiman, 1993). In our view, there are
grave risks associated with using dissociability as an indicator of hidden memories of
CSA. First, Brenneis (1994) cited several studies and reviews indicating that
symptoms of dissociability may arise for numerous reasons. Second, dissociability is
closely related to suggestibility (Piper, in press). Thus, dissociability may in fact be a
measure of clients' susceptibility to suggestive memory recovery techniques. Consis-
tent with this possibility, dissociability is a hallmark of MPD (dissociative identity
disorder); many critics have argued that MPD may be an iatrogenic phenomenon
that arises from suggestive therapies used with suggestible clients (e.g., Piper, in
press; Spanos, 1994).

Memory Work and Memory Illusions


The research reviewed in the previous section suggests that adulthood recovery
of previously forgotten histories of CSA through memory work is likely a rare
phenomenon. Yet both proponents and critics of memory work have offered
evidence indicating that in recent years many thousands of North Americans have
reported memory recovery in psychotherapy or through self-help books, survivors'
groups, and so on. This section reviews evidence relevant to the possibility that some
such reports reflect iatrogenic illusory memories or false beliefs developed in
response to suggestive influences.
Memory and suggestion. Psychologists and sociologists have long been inter-
ested in memory errors and distortions. We and others have recently reviewed
relevant aspects of this literature (D. S. Lindsay & Read, 1994; Loftus, 1993; Loftus
& Ketcham, 1994; Ofshe & Walters, 1994; Read & Lindsay, 1994; Schacter, 1995), so
we confine this discussion to a brief recapitulation and mention of some new studies.
The picture that emerges from this large and varied literature is a strikingly
coherent one: Memory is largely accurate but far from perfect, people are
susceptible to suggestive influences that can lead them confidently to misremember
past events, and the likelihood that suggestive influences lead to memory errors is
jointly determined by (a) the clarity of memory for the period and events about which
suggestions are given and (b) the strength of the suggestive influences. Quite weak
suggestions often lead to memory errors when they concern trivial details in a
passively witnessed event (e.g., Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978), but stronger
suggestions are usually needed to create illusory memories of memorable, personally
experienced events (e.g., Hyman, Husband, & Billings, 1995). Factors that lead to
poor memory for the events or time period about which suggestions are given (e.g.,
delay) and those that increase the strength of suggestions increase the likelihood that
people will experience illusory memories or false beliefs in accord with suggestions
(e.g., Loftus et al., 1978). The overall strength of suggestive influences is determined
by a multitude of factors, including perceived authority and trustworthiness of the
source of suggestions, repetition of suggestions, perceived plausibility of the
suggestions, imagability of the suggestions, and memory-monitoring response criteria
(i.e., the amount and kind of cognitive evidence people require before they accept an
image or idea as a memory; see Ross, Read, & Toglia, 1994, for recent review
chapters).
RECOVERED MEMORIES 865

As explained in more detail in D. S. Lindsay and Read (1994) and Read and
Lindsay (1994), the important point is that extreme forms of memory work in
psychotherapy combine virtually all of the factors that have been shown to increase
the likelihood of illusory memories or beliefs: (a) a trusted authority communicates a
rationale for the plausibility of hidden memories of long-ago childhood trauma (that
many clients have hidden memories, that the client's psychological symptoms,
physical symptoms, and dreams evidence them, and that doubt is a sign of "denial")
and (b) a trusted authority provides motivation for attempting to recover such
memories (that healing is contingent on retrieving hidden memories); (c) the client is
repeatedly exposed to suggestive information from multiple sources (anecdotes in
popular books, other survivor's stories, comments and interpretations offered by the
therapist, etc.), providing a "script" for recovering memories as well as suggestions
about particular details; and (d) techniques such as hypnosis and guided imagery
enhance imagery and lower response criterion such that people are more willing to
interpret thoughts, feelings, and images as memories.6 In the most extreme forms of
memory work, these converging suggestive influences unfold gradually over a period
of weeks or months of therapy sessions, sometimes supplemented with homework
exercises, self-help books, and survivors' meetings. Immersion in such a program may
provide material for troubling dreams and intrusive "flashbacks." Through such
memory work, thoughts and images (which may at first be experienced with
considerable doubt) may become detailed and convincing pseudomemories.
Memory recovery techniques likely do not need to be overtly coercive to be
strongly suggestive—indeed, overt coercion may inoculate people from suggestive
influences to some extent (Bowers, 1984). Instead, therapists who use long-term,
multifaceted, suggestive forms of memory work may be viewed (and view themselves)
as sympathetic and supportive guides through unchartered territory. Rosenthal's
(1994) recent review of 30 years of research on expectancy effects is particularly
germane here: This research shows that expectations held by teachers, managers,
judges, nursing-home caregivers, and others can affect the behavior of those with
whom they interact. For example, when caregivers are led to believe that certain
patients should do better than others, those patients do in fact tend to do better.
Rosenthal argued that these expectancy effects are mediated primarily by subtle
nonverbal behaviors. The applicability of such effects to psychotherapy is supported
by the observation that therapists with particular orientations often find that their
clients fit their orientation (e.g., MPD-oriented therapists find that many of their
clients have MPD; Piper, in press).
Of course, not every client who receives suggestive memory work develops false
beliefs or illusory memories of CSA. First, these techniques might enable some
clients with hidden histories of CSA to remember actual abuse. We have argued that
research on autobiographical memory suggests that such cases are rare, not that they
6
Hypnosis, guided imagery, age regression, and other visualization and relaxation techniques appear
to have therapeutic value in other respects (e.g., Kirsch, Montgomery, & Sapirstein, 1995) but not as
memory recovery techniques. Dozens of studies have shown that although these techniques lower
response criterion and may enhance imagery (and consequently often increase both accurate and
inaccurate reports, and inflate confidence in both accurate and inaccurate reports), they do not in
themselves increase ability to remember (e.g., American Medical Association, 1994; Steblay & Bothwell,
in press). The belief that hypnosis or age regression can reliably enable people accurately to recapture
early childhood experiences has been debunked (e.g., Frankel, 1993; Nash, 1987; Spanos, Burgess, &
Burgess, 1994).
866 LINDSAY AND READ

are nonexistent. Second, there appear to be individual differences in suggestibility


(Brown, 1995; Piper, in press), which may be understood in terms of the factors listed
earlier. For example, individuals with vivid imagery and high trust in authority may
be especially susceptible to suggestion (cf. Hyman & Billings, 1995; Schooler &
Loftus, 1993).7 Thus, some clients are likely more at risk of developing illusory beliefs
through memory work than others.
It must also be emphasized that approaches to memory work vary considerably,
from those in which a single technique is used on one or two occasions to "explore
the possibility" that a selected client has hidden memories to approaches that
combine all of the suggestive elements described earlier. Research on suggestibility
indicates that few clients are likely to develop full-blown illusory memories or false
beliefs regarding CSA solely in response to a few suggestive questions; as the
frequency, number, duration, and social power of suggestive influences increases, so
too does the risk that clients who were not abused as children will come to believe
that they were.
Generalizability of research on memory illusions. Some writers have objected that
studies of memory illusions cannot be generalized to traumas remembered in
therapy (e.g., Berliner & Williams, 1994; Brown, 1995; Courtois, 1995; Enns et al.,
1995; Olio, 1994; Pezdek, 1994). One argument is that evidence from laboratory
studies of eyewitness suggestibility, in which suggestions typically refer to minor
details in a passively witnessed slide show, video, or staged event, cannot be
generalized to cases in which people report memories of traumatic childhood
experiences. This claim is challenged by several recent studies that have shown that
suggestive influences can indeed lead people to experience illusory memories of
dramatic life experiences (e.g., spending a night in the hospital for an ear infection at
age 5 years; being sexually abused as a child in a past life), provided that the
suggestive influences are sufficiently strong (Hyman, 1994; Hyman & Billings, 1995;
Hyman et al., 1995; Loftus & Pickrell, in press; Spanos et al., 1994; for similar
findings with children, see Ceci & Bruck, 1994; Poole & Lindsay, 1995). Importantly,
the same sorts of factors that laboratory studies have shown determine the strength
of suggestive influences (e.g., authority, repetition, lowered response criterion, etc.)
appear to be involved in producing illusory memories and false beliefs of autobio-
graphical experiences. For example, Loftus and Pickrell (in press) asked 24 people to
recall four childhood events, three of which had actually happened and one of which
had not; 25% indicated that they remembered the suggested nonevent. Similarly,
Hyman and his colleagues conducted a number of studies in which undergraduates
were asked to remember childhood events that they were told their parents had
reported. In fact, the to-be-remembered events included one or two events that
parents' reports indicated had never happened (e.g., knocking over a punch bowl at a
wedding reception). Few participants claimed to remember a suggested event the
first time they were interviewed, but up to a quarter of them did so in a second
interview, and a substantial minority provided detailed descriptions of a suggested
event. Hyman and Billings (1995) found that participants with high scores on the
Dissociative Experiences Scale (Bernstein & Putnam, 1986) were particularly likely

7
In view of evidence that childhood traumas are associated with adulthood problems and with
elevated scores on the Dissociative Experiences Scale, it may be that, on average, people seeking
psychotherapy are more susceptible to suggestion than the broader samples on which most of the
suggestibility research has been conducted.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 867

to "remember" the suggested event. Finally, sociologists have documented cases in


which suggestive influences led suspects to confess to crimes that they evidently did
not commit (e.g., Gudjonsson, 1992; Ofshe, 1989,1992).
Of course, there are differences between these studies and memory work. Given
that ethics bar researchers from testing the hypothesis that suggestions can give rise
to false memories of CSA, there will always be room to argue that studies
demonstrating false memories of other kinds cannot be generalized to memories of
CSA. Yet anecdotal and correlational evidence from real-world cases supports
generalization from such studies to memory work. Some "memories" recovered
through memory work are demonstrably false (Read & Lindsay, 1994, pp. 413-414),
and some clients working with fringe therapists who believe in UFO abductions, past
life regression, and intergenerational satanic cults recover memories that are difficult
to credit (e.g., Bottoms, Shaver, & Goodman, in press; Ryder, 1992; Spanos et al.,
1994). Coons (1994) reported that of 29 patients who had entered a dissociative
disorders clinic between 1984 and 1993 and eventually reported histories of satanic
ritual abuse (SRA), 17 admissions were within 2 years after a Geraldo Rivera
television show about SRA, and 10 admissions were within 1 year after a local
workshop on SRA. Hence, 92% of the SRA cases during this 10-year period occurred
shortly after one of these two programs. Coons noted that reports of SRA were
elicited through hypnosis in 14 patients, dreamwork in 10, and regressive therapies in
8; for only 2 patients was there no evidence of the use of memory recovery
techniques. Coons noted that despite attempts to corroborate the reports, none of
the memories of SRA was supported (cf. Bottoms et al., in press).
In view of the wealth of converging evidence, we believe it would be foolhardy to
contend that illusory memories or false beliefs of CSA cannot be created by
long-term, multifaceted, suggestive memory work. It should be emphasized that it is
the principles underlying suggestibility that we believe can be generalized to the
therapy situation (i.e., effects of poor memory for the time period about which
suggestions are given; perceived authority and trustworthiness of the source of
suggestions; repetition, plausibility, and imagability of suggestions; and low response
criterion), not the absolute size of the suggestibility effect. The size of the
suggestibility effect is determined by a multitude of factors that likely interact in
complex ways. Thus, we can say with confidence that approaches to memory work
that combine many or all of these factors put clients at substantial risk, but we cannot
predict exactly how many nonabused clients receiving a particular form of memory
work would come to hold false beliefs about CSA.
Another argument advanced against generalizing from the research literature on
suggestibility to memory work is that participants in research studies do not always
genuinely believe the suggestions they report under hypnosis or in misinformation
studies (e.g., Pezdek, 1994). It is true that research indicates that some people who
report witnessing suggested details or events realize (or at least are capable of
realizing when asked) that their reports are based on postevent suggestions rather
than on memories of the to-be-remembered event itself (for review, see Lindsay,
1994). It is also true that in studies in which hypnotized people give false reports,
some later indicate that the suggested event they reported under hypnosis was
imagined rather than real (Spanos, 1992; cf. Lynn, Rhue, Myers, & Weeks, 1994).
These findings should not be misconstrued, however, as evidence against generaliz-
ing from the research literature to cases of recovered memories of CSA. First, there
868 LINDSAY AND READ

are ample demonstrations that under some conditions people given misleading
suggestions confidently believe they are remembering actually experienced events
when they report things that were suggested to them (e.g., Ceci & Bruck, 1994;
Lindsay, 1994; Loftus & Ketcham, 1994; Zaragoza & Lane, 1994). Second, anecdotal
accounts and statements presented by proponents of memory work indicate that
clients too often initially worry that the thoughts and images they are experiencing
are products of imagination or suggestion rather than accurate memories of
childhood experiences (e.g., Bass & Davis, 1988; Frederickson, 1992). Third, in our
opinion no laboratory study has come remotely close to matching the overall strength
of the suggestive influences of extreme forms of memory work.
A third argument against generalization advanced by some defenders of memory
work is that trauma memories are immune to suggestion (e.g., Brown, 1995; Enns et
al., 1995). This argument misses the point, which is that people without trauma
histories are not immune to suggestive influences. If therapists were perfectly
accurate at discriminating between clients with and without hidden trauma histories
and used memory recovery techniques and ancillary practices only with the former
group, there would be much less basis for concern about such techniques (although
one might still have concerns about traumatized individuals being led to "remember"
traumas in addition to those they actually experienced). Unfortunately, as noted in a
previous section, there are good reasons to believe that practitioners are far from
perfect at discriminating between clients with nonreported CSA histories and clients
without CSA histories. Furthermore, as argued later, there are scant grounds for the
belief that memories of traumatic experiences are immutable and accurate and that
trauma survivors are immune to suggestion.
Proponents of the belief that trauma memories are inviolable and entirely
accurate cite evidence from two sources. First, there are case studies and anecdotal
accounts of people recovering detailed and vivid memories of childhood trauma,
often through therapy. People having such experiences often evidence intense
emotional anguish, sometimes behaving as if the trauma were occurring at the very
moment (see Levitt & Pinnell, 1994, and Siegel, 1995, for discussions of such
"abreactions"). We suspect that first-hand experience with such cases is a major
source of the confidence and belief evidenced by practitioners who use memory
recovery techniques in their therapies (cf. Andrews et al., 1995; Wylie, 1993). As
noted earlier, however, memories of CSA recovered in therapy are not always
accurate, and the emotional intensity of the experience of remembering cannot, in
our opinion, be taken as evidence of the accuracy of the memories. Experiencing
memorylike images of being sexually abused by a loved one, and accepting those
images as accurate memories, would be an emotionally wrenching experience
regardless of whether the images were veridical memories. Consistent with this
argument, people who experience recovery of memories of UFO abductions,
intrauterine trauma, or human sacrifices in satanic cult ceremonies also evidence
great distress in the process (e.g., Ryder, 1992; Shaffer & Cozolino, 1992; Spanos,
1994).
The second body of evidence cited in support of the belief that trauma memories
are peculiarly accurate and immune to suggestion comes from studies of combat
veterans with PTSD (e.g., van der Kolk, 1988). As argued in a previous section, such
cases provide a poor analogy to clients who initially report no knowledge of
childhood trauma and later experience memory recovery; even the subset of combat
RECOVERED MEMORIES 869

veterans with PTSD who are amnesic for particular events are aware that they
experienced combat. In any case, studies of combat veterans do not document the
claim that their memories, flashbacks, or dreams are invariably accurate (although
they are very vivid and intense). Finally, because delayed-onset PTSD is trauma-
oriented practitioners' diagnosis of choice for adult clients suspected of having
hidden histories of CSA (e.g., Claridge, 1992), one might expect frequently to see
years-delayed onset of PTSD symptoms among those known to have experienced war
trauma. However, although PTSD symptoms often emerge over a period of months
following return from combat (e.g., Sonnenberg et al., 1985), and although there are
some cases in which veterans who appeared to have adjusted to peace-time life
suddenly demonstrate dramatic PTSD symptoms years after the war, evidence
suggests that years-delayed onset of PTSD is a rare phenomenon in combat veterans
(Burstein, 1985; Helzer, Robins, & McEvoy, 1987).
Brenneis (1994) provided an informative discussion of research on combat
veterans' memories. He noted that although some have claimed that recurrent
dreams reported by veterans duplicate the actual traumatic events, the study most
often cited to support this claim (van der Kolk, Britz, Burr, Sherry, & Hartmann,
1984, cited in Brenneis, 1994) collected neither dream reports nor documentation
regarding the traumatic events; the claim that dreams mirrored past reality was
based solely on dreamers' statements to that effect. Moreover, Brenneis argued that
there is no empirical evidence for the claim that people who have no conscious
recollections of past traumas have dreams that duplicate those forgotten traumatic
experiences (although people who do remember traumas often dream about them,
and people whose waking life is dominated by searches for hidden trauma memories
are likely to have trauma dreams; cf. Nielsen & Powell, 1992).8 Brenneis also
described Boulanger's (1985) study of 275 Vietnam veterans 6 to 16 years after their
return to the United States, which found that 36% of the combat veterans met
criteria for PTSD but that 17% of noncombat and non-war-zone veterans did as well.
This finding is reminiscent of Yapko's (1993) description of a man with symptoms
characteristic of Vietnam veterans with PTSD who turned out never to have been in
Vietnam.
Flashbacks and abreactions are sometimes described as though they were vivid
relivings of past experiences, and hence necessarily veridical (e.g., K. R. Clark, 1993;
Person & Klar, 1994). Clinical evidence does not support this belief. According to
Frankel (1994), Maloney (1988) found that the spouses of some Vietnam veterans
experienced some of the same imagery that figured in their husbands' flashbacks.
Similarly, Brenneis (1994) described a study by Rynearson and McCreery (1993) of
18 family members questioned about their memories of the murder of another family
member; all but one of them reported vivid, intense, and intrusive images of the
murder, even though only one of them had witnessed it. Thus, there is little reason to
believe that trauma memories are always accurate. Moreover, people can have
"trauma memories" of events they never experienced. Finally, given that "dissociabil-
ity" is often cited as a symptom of childhood trauma, and given that people who score
high on measures of dissociability also tend to score high on measures of

8
McHugh (1994) noted that people often have troubling dreams associated with daily life stressors
(e.g., dreaming of failing an exam at school); his point was that although such dreams may be vivid and
distressing, they often do not reflect reality (e.g., one did not really fail the exam).
870 LINDSAY AND READ

suggestibility (Piper, in press), it is difficult to understand the argument that trauma


survivors are immune to suggestion. The evidence supports the opposite claim.
Differences between recovered and never-forgotten memories. Bottoms et al. (in
press) sent questionnaires to American Psychological Association clinicians who, in
response to an earlier survey, had reported one or more cases of clients reporting
satanic ritual abuse or what Bottoms et al. referred to as "religion-related abuse"
(e.g., beatings to exorcise devils). In a subanalysis, Goodman, Qin, Bottoms, and
Shaver (1993) compared practitioners' descriptions of 43 recovered memories with
447 reports of never-forgotten abuse. Abuse reported in newly recovered memories
was much more extreme (i.e., more instances, more perpetrators, more severe abuse,
and more ritual abuse, with a younger age of onset and an older age of termination)
than were never-forgotten abuses. Despite the extreme nature of the alleged events
in recovered memory cases (which often involved multiple murders, torture, etc.),
almost none of the cases was supported by any evidence. These findings led
Goodman et al. (1993) to question the validity of the recovered memory claims.
Nonetheless, most respondents indicated belief in their clients' reports, with equal
confidence in never-forgotten and newly remembered abuse. This is consistent with
research demonstrating that observers—including clinicians—often cannot discrimi-
nate between accurate and inaccurate reports that are products of suggestive
influences (e.g., Ceci & Brack, 1994).
A striking difference between recovered and never-forgotten memories of CSA
concerns the role of women as perpetrators. According to the FMSF Scientific and
Professional Advisory Board (1994), 30% of the cases in their records include
allegations that mothers sexually abused their children. Similarly, the clinicians in
Bottom et al.'s (in press) survey reported that clients who remembered ritual abuse
typically reported multiple perpetrators, with more than 40% of them women. This
contrasts with other evidence, which suggests that women rarely sexually molest
children (cf. Finkelhor, 1994).
Consistent with these views, Abrams and Abrams (1994) reported that poly-
graph examinations of 300 people accused of CSA in cases that did not involve
recovered memories indicated that 78% were deceptive in denying the allegations,
whereas polygraphs of 46 people accused of CSA on the basis of recovered memories
indicated that only 4% were deceptive in denying the abuse. Polygraphy can be
criticized on several grounds, and Abrams and Abrams's study is open to methodologi-
cal criticism (e.g., the alleged offenses were much less recent in cases that involved
claims of repressed memories), but this is nonetheless a striking finding.
Shades of gray. The foregoing discussion may appear to imply that adults can
neatly be assigned to groups of those who were and those who were not abused as
children, and that abuse survivors can be dichotomized into those who do or those
who do not remember their abuse. As Harvey and Herman (1994) and others have
pointed out, the reality is less clear-cut than this. Most people have had childhood
experiences that might be defined as "trauma," and most children and adolescents
suffer offenses that might be defined as CSA. Furthermore, remembering is not an
all-or-none phenomenon: adults recollect some childhood events vividly, others only
vaguely, and yet others not at all. Similarly, it would be simplistic to assume that
recovered memories are always 100% accurate or that they are always 100% illusory:
Essentially accurate autobiographical recollections are rarely perfect and complete
(e.g., Neisser & Winograd, 1988), and essentially inaccurate ones often blend
RECOVERED MEMORIES 871

veridical memories with products of suggestion, imagination, and interpretation


(e.g., Hyman & Loftus, in press).
As noted previously, critics of memory work have focused on cases of recovered
memories in which clients initially believe that they were not sexually abused in any
way during childhood. Of course, many cases do not fit this mold. For example, some
clients may initially report a few isolated instances of high-base-rate forms of CSA
and later experience recovery of memories of multiple instances of extreme and
low-base-rate forms of contact CSA. Such cases, in our view, warrant the same
concerns as memories recovered by people who initially remembered no CSA history
at all; if suggestive influences were involved in generating the new-found memories,
then in our opinion they should be treated with a substantial degree of skepticism. In
contrast, other clients may always have known that they experienced multiple
instances of extreme forms of contact CSA, enter therapy with fragmentary
memories of the abuse, and come to reveal or remember more details or additional
instances of such abuse. Cases of this sort raise far fewer concerns, especially if the
additional recall did not rely on suggestive influences, because this pattern is
consistent with what is known about autobiographical memory. Research on
autobiographical memory suggests that clients who were repeatedly abused are likely
to have difficulty remembering details of specific instances and that cues and
attempts to remember could help them retrieve some details and fill in others.
Although one would expect errors of omission and commission in such cases,
memory research indicates that the central events will typically be accurately
remembered. Note, however, that such cases differ importantly from those on which
criticisms of memory work have focused (i.e., clients who initially remember no
history of CSA but later believe that they were grossly sexually abused as children).
Biases and heuristics in clinical judgment. Several well-established and robust
biases of human judgment may lead some trauma-oriented therapists to have undue
confidence in the reliability of "indicators" of abuse, the accuracy of their clinical
judgments, and the efficacy of their treatments. For example, the availability
heuristic (the tendency to judge probabilities on the basis of the number of
exemplars that can be brought to mind) and confirmation bias (the tendency to look
for and focus on hypothesis-confirming information) conspire to make humans
vulnerable to "illusory correlations" (i.e., they believe that one variable predicts
another when in fact here is no relation between the two). Illusory correlations are a
robust phenomenon and have been demonstrated in situations closely analogous to
therapy (e.g., Chapman & Chapman, 1967; Dawes, 1989; Dowling & Graham, 1976;
Shweder, 1977). In a classic study by Smedslund (1963, cited in Shweder, 1977),
nurses studied 100 "case histories" that specified (a) whether each patient had a
particular symptom and (b) whether the patient had a particular disease. Statisti-
cally, there was absolutely no relationship between the symptom and the disease, yet
85% of the nurses reported that there was such a relationship and justified that
conclusion by noting that there were many cases in which the patient had both the
symptom and the disease (not noting that there were also many cases in which the
patient had the symptom but did not have the disease).
The representativeness heuristic, a tendency to judge the degree of relationship
between variables on the basis of their surface similarity, may also lead to ill-based
beliefs about the diagnosticity of particular symptoms (Shweder, 1977; Tversky &
Kahneman, 1974). As a possible example of the representativeness heuristic, the only
872 LINDSAY AND READ

"indicator" of CSA mentioned by more than 16% of the respondents in each of


Poole et al.'s (1995) three samples of highly trained psychotherapists was adulthood
sexual dysfunction. There is evidence that CSA is associated with adulthood sexual
dysfunction, but the relationship is not strikingly strong (Briere & Elliott, 1992), and
adulthood sexual dysfunction is also associated with a variety of other factors, some
of which likely have higher base rates than the kinds of CSA associated with this
symptom (e.g., Sutker & Adams, 1993). Poole et al. suggested that therapists' beliefs
about the reliability of adulthood sexual dysfunction as an indicator arose less from
reality than from their susceptibility to the representativeness heuristic.
Confirmation bias, the availability heuristic, and the representativeness heuristic
make people susceptible to illusory correlations unless they take systematic steps to
avoid them (e.g., keeping and reviewing records that allow an objective assessment of
the relationships between predictor and outcome variables). Unfortunately, this is
difficult for therapists because of the large number of variables of interest, because
outcome measures are rarely well-defined, and because disconfirming evidence is
often hard to come by (i.e., it is difficult to know when clinical intuitions are wrong;
Marsh & Hunsley, 1993). Typically, therapists do not have access to control groups,
and many judge the success or failure of their judgments and interventions on the
basis of "clinical judgment" rather than through formal pre- and posttreatment
measures. This may account for startling evidence that although confidence in
diagnostic accuracy increases as a function of years of clinical experience, accuracy of
diagnoses often does not (Dawes, 1994).
The difficulty of assessing the efficacy of therapy (Marsh & Hunsley, 1993) is
likely to be especially great in treatments, such as memory work, in which symptoms
dramatically escalate during therapy (e.g., McNulty & Wardle, 1994). Regardless of
whether all, some, or none of the CSA events a particular client remembers in
therapy actually occurred, the experience of "remembering" is likely to be a
traumatic one, leading to numerous psychological symptoms (e.g., depression,
anxiety, relationship difficulties, sleep disturbance, etc.). Such increases in symptoms
during memory recovery have been remarked on by most proponents of memory
work (e.g., Frederickson, 1992), and some critics have claimed that recovering
memories sometimes culminates in suicide (e.g., Ofshe & Walters, 1994; Pender-
grast, 1995). However, many clients gradually heal, to some extent, from the trauma
of recovering CSA memories (veridical or illusory). Whether recovery of mental
health ever attains pretherapy levels would, we posit, be difficult to ascertain without
objective pre- and posttreatment measures.
Social influence. Research demonstrates that people's beliefs and expectations
can greatly influence the behavior of other persons with whom they interact (e.g.,
Ceci, Leichtman, & White, 1995; Rosenthal, 1994). For example, Ceci et al. found
that interviewers working with child witnesses often pose suggestive and leading
questions even when they try to avoid doing so. Moreover, people often underesti-
mate their influence on others and often do not appreciate the extent to which their
own behavior is influenced by others (e.g., Bowers, 1984). Thus, a therapist's or
client's retrospective introspections about the extent to which the client's reconstruc-
tions of childhood traumas were influenced by the therapist should be viewed with
caution.
As a real-world example of the difficulty of perceiving one's influence over
others, consider recent research on "facilitated communication" (a procedure
RECOVERED MEMORIES 873

believed by some to allow severely autistic children to communicate by typing on a


keyboard with the assistance of a trained facilitator). Research reviewed by the
American Psychological Association, Division 33 (1994) confirms that severely
autistic children's typing is coherent and relevant only when the facilitator knows
what is to be typed, even though facilitators vehemently deny influencing the
children's typing. The example is particularly germane because numerous autistic
children have reported CSA through facilitated communication (see Jones, 1994, for
the introduction to a special issue of Child Abuse and Neglect on this topic).
Progress toward consensus on suggestive memory work. There is increasing
unanimity among researchers and therapists regarding the riskiness of highly
suggestive forms of memory work (i.e., those that combine multiple techniques in a
prolonged search for hidden memories). Many of those who have expressed concerns
about memory work are clinical psychologists or psychiatrists (e.g., Beutler & Hill,
1992; Brenneis, 1994; Brown, 1995; Campbell, 1992,1994; Dalenberg, 1994; Frankel,
1993; Ganaway, 1995; Gold et al., 1994; Haaken & Schlaps, 1991; McHugh, 1994;
Piper, 1993, in press; Siegel, 1995; Wakefield & Underwager, 1992; Yapko, 1993,
1994). Two recent articles by trauma-oriented practitioners published in clinical
journals (Brown, 1995; Enns et al., 1995) discussed the importance of avoiding highly
suggestive memory work (see also Siegel, 1995), and several writers whose contribu-
tions to this debate have focused on countering criticisms of memory work have
acknowledged that some approaches may lead some clients to develop illusory
memories or false beliefs (e.g., Berliner & Williams, 1994; Olio & Cornell, 1994;
Pezdek, 1994). This position is clearly articulated in statements by the American
Medical Association (1994), the American Psychiatric Association (1993), and the
Australian Psychological Society (1994), and cautions about suggestive memory work
were also offered by the British Psychological Society (1995). Bass and Davis (1994),
coauthors of an extraordinarily popular self-help book that includes an emphasis on
recovering hidden memories of CSA, have toned down their claims and recommen-
dations in the most recent (3rd) edition (although not sufficiently to quell our alarm
about its suggestiveness). Progress toward consensus on this issue is perhaps best
exemplified by guidelines proposed by trauma-oriented psychotherapist Courtois
(1994) for working with adult clients suspected of having hidden abuse memories—
guidelines that we would endorse with only minor modifications.
Unfortunately, it will likely take time for these messages to be disseminated
sufficiently broadly and clearly to overcome the effects of the widespread promulga-
tion of CSA-oriented memory work. Thus, some practitioners probably will continue
to use multiple suggestive techniques in prolonged searches for hidden memories.
Perhaps more important, evidence suggests that the much larger population of
practitioners who use selected memory recovery techniques with selected clients are
often unaware of the potential risks of those techniques. This point was dramatically
evident in many of the responses to Poole et al.'s (1995) surveys of highly trained
psychotherapists: 91% agreed that clients can develop false memories, and many
wrote comments indicating a concern about this issue, but 90% indicated that few or
none of their own clients had ever experienced illusory memories, regardless of the
memory recovery techniques used (see Andrews et al., 1995, for similar findings). A
number of Poole et al.'s respondents reported use of techniques that most
researchers view as risky, but wrote comments such as "I don't use suggestive
techniques" (see also D. S. Lindsay & Poole, in press). Similarly, Yapko (1993,1994)
874 LINDSAY AND READ

found that 19% of the practitioners in his sample of therapists attending conferences
and workshops reported cases in which they believed a client had developed illusory
memories through working with another therapist, yet these same respondents
reported truly alarming misconceptions about human memory and about the safety
and efficacy of hypnosis as a technique for remembering childhood events yet were
confident in the validity of their own clients' recovered memories.

Criteria for Weighing the Validity of Recovered Memories


In the absence of clear corroborating or disconfirming evidence, neither
practitioners nor cognitive researchers can say with certainty whether any particular
case of a recovered history of CSA is or is not veridical. On the one hand, there likely
are cases in which people with abuse histories are unaware of them for years but later
remember the abuse when given appropriate cues (see Loftus, 1993, Schooler, 1994,
and Williams, 1994a, for some compelling cases). On the other hand, evidence on the
base rate of CSA and memory for childhood trauma suggests that such cases are rare.
Recent prospective research by Henry, Moffitt, Caspi, Langly, and Silva (1994)
revealed that even without suggestive influences, memory for childhood events is
often strikingly in error, and research on suggestibility amply supports concern that
searches for hidden memories can lead some clients to experience compelling but
illusory memories and firmly held but false beliefs about CSA.
In our view, allegations of CSA based solely on recovered memories should be
treated with a considerable degree of skepticism if there is evidence of exposure to
suggestive information or memory recovery techniques. This is not to say that such
allegations should be dismissed out of hand. The likelihood of claims based on
recovered memories being valid can only be rather crudely estimated by weighing
evidence regarding (a) how memories were recovered (the more evidence of a
prolonged, multifaceted, socially influenced search for memories, the greater the
degree of skepticism); (b) the nature and clarity of the recovered memories (with
more credence given to detailed recollections than to vague feelings, beliefs, dream
images, or "body memories"); (c) the plausibility of the alleged events being
forgotten (e.g., research indicates that people are particularly unlikely to forget
repeated contact CSA extending into late childhood or teenage years); (d) the
plausibility of recovering the memories (e.g., research shows that claims of
recovering recollections of events before age 2 years should be treated with great
skepticism); and (e) the base rate of the alleged type of abuse (the lower the base
rate, the greater the degree of skepticism).
This approach to estimating the veracity of allegations based on recovered
memories will sometimes lead one to view as unlikely accounts that are in fact true
and to sometimes view as credible accounts that are not true. This is unfortunate,
and it is to be hoped that future research will establish additional bases on which to
estimate the credibility of allegations (e.g., along the lines of Briere & Elliott's, 1992,
and Briere & Runtz's, 1990, Trauma Symptom Checklist; improved techniques for
detecting deception; or relevant normative measures of individual differences in
suggestibility). At present, however, in our view the just-mentioned considerations
are the only ones in the domain of psychology on which assessments of credibility can
be based. This is intended to be a two-edged sword. Delayed accusations should not
be dismissed as implausible merely because the accused perpetrator is a pillar of the
community who seems sincere, because of a lack of corroborating evidence, or
RECOVERED MEMORIES 875

because the accuser has been in psychotherapy. Pillars of the community sometimes
sexually abuse children, perpetrators can appear wonderfully sincere, corroborating
evidence is more the exception than the rule in cases of CSA, and psychotherapy
does not necessarily involve suggestive memory work. Similarly, accusations based on
recovered memories should not be accepted as valid merely because of the presence
of certain symptoms, because of the emotional intensity and sincerity of the accuser,
or because of the confidence a therapist holds in the accuser's memories. Such
factors are not probative.

Memory Recovery, Psychotherapy, Public Policy, and the Law


The use of memory recovery techniques and ancillary practices to recover
suspected hidden memories of CSA thus raise numerous issues for public policy and
the law. In this section we discuss four large categories of issues: (a) training and
certification of psychotherapists, (b) professional guidelines for trauma-oriented
psychotherapy, (c) public policy initiatives to support research relevant to the
recovered memories debate, and (d) issues related to legal actions in cases involving
alleged recovered memories of CSA.
The goals of professional psychology, public policy, and the law with regard to
this topic can be stated quite simply: We must maximize support for survivors of CSA
while simultaneously minimizing the risk of iatrogenic illusory memories and false
beliefs. There is a tension between these two goals, but we reject the claim that
progress toward one goal must necessarily be offset by movement away from the
other (Pezdek, 1994). Psychologists in other domains have demonstrated that it is not
necessary to decrease "hits" (in this case, identification of and support for survivors
of CSA and prosecution of perpetrators) in order to reduce "false alarms" (in this
case, creation of illusory memories or beliefs and false accusations). For example, in
studies of suspect identification, R. C. L. Lindsay and Wells (1985) found that
sequential lineups, in which witnesses view mugshots one at a time, dramatically
lower false identifications of innocent people relative to simultaneous lineups,
without reducing correct identifications of perpetrators. Conversely, Fisher and
Geiselman (1988) developed a "cognitive interview" that can increase eyewitnesses'
correct recall without increasing errors. We believe that the risks of iatrogenic
illusory memories and beliefs can be dramatically reduced without lessening
sensitivity to and support for survivors of abuse; indeed, we believe that it is an
exclusive focus on one goal or the other that leads to undesirable trade-offs and that
progress can only be made by pursuing both simultaneously.
We are cognitive psychologists, with limited knowledge and expertise in clinical
psychology, public policy, and the law. Consequently, the following discussion is
limited to identification of some of the central issues in these domains, citations of
statements by experts in these fields, and tentative recommendations that we offer
for further consideration.

Training and Certification of Psychotherapists


As noted previously, in recent years some therapists have used potentially risky
memory recovery techniques and ancillary practices. Because these practices were
used without benefit of empirical evidence of their effectiveness, and because there
are grounds for concern about potential ill-effects of such approaches, we view the
widespread promulgation and use of memory work as an important failing on the
876 LINDSAY AND READ

part of psychology. Such problems are common in the history of the healing arts and
sciences (e.g., indiscriminate and excessive use of electroconvulsive shock, psychosur-
gery, radical mastectomy, multiple bypass surgery, etc.). In the case of memory work
in psychotherapy, however, the failing seems particularly egregious because existing
scientific evidence, well known within the discipline of psychology, clearly contraindi-
cated this approach to treatment. Several factors related to the training and
certification of practitioners may have contributed to this problem: (a) the preva-
lence of unqualified therapists, (b) poor public education regarding differences in
the qualifications of people offering mental health care services under various
auspices, (c) inadequacies in training provided by graduate programs in clinical
psychology and in ongoing education of practitioners, and (d) limited opportunities
for many practitioners for ongoing supervision and consultation with qualified peers.
One challenging aspect of the debate regarding memory work in psychotherapy
arises from the number and variety of North Americans providing various types of
therapy under various auspices. In addition to substantial percentages of the tens of
thousands of licensed psychiatrists, psychotherapists, and registered counselors,
insight therapies are offered by unknown percentages of the tens of thousands of
social workers (with master's or bachelor's degrees), clergy, chemical dependency
counselors, and free-lance "therapists" with little or no advanced training in clinical
psychology. Within each of these broad categories, there is wide variation in the
amount and nature of formal training in relevant skill areas (e.g., diagnosis,
treatment, ethics, scientific reasoning) and specific content areas (e.g., sequelae of
CSA, treatments for depression).
Thus the term therapist, as we and others have rather loosely used it, covers a
large and heterogenous population. This fact has several important implications.
First, as noted in a previous section, because there are many tens of thousands of
North Americans who regularly conduct psychological therapy with adults, a small
percentage of this population translates into a substantial number of practitioners,
each working with a number of clients. Second, this heterogeneity may have
hampered communication between critics of memory work and practitioners; critics
have sometimes painted psychotherapy with a broad brush and have not identified
the targets of their criticisms with sufficient clarity, which may in turn have led some
practitioners to dismiss the criticisms as unjustified because neither they nor their
professional acquaintances fit the critics' descriptions of therapists. Third, it is likely
that many people who seek psychotherapy are unfamiliar with the meaning of the
various formal degrees, titles, and certifications for providers of mental health care
and hence cannot select between qualified and unqualified caregivers.
Unqualified therapists. Because there is no protection of title for therapists in
most jurisdictions, anyone can refer to himself or herself as a therapist and advertise
as such with whatever specialization is considered desirable. Some evidence suggests
that untrained or undertrained practitioners have made a disproportionately large
contribution to the problem of risky memory work (Wakefield & Underwager, 1992;
Yapko, 1994). Our own experience is consonant with this evidence; the extreme cases
brought to our attention by lawyers or accused parents have usually involved
therapists with little formal training (although this may reflect differences in base
rates rather than differences in use of memory recovery techniques in these two
populations). Of course, certification is a general issue, and the popularization of
RECOVERED MEMORIES 877

memory work has only incrementally added to opportunities for incompetent and
unqualified individuals to influence clients in potentially harmful ways.
It is not immediately clear what the response to the prevalence of unqualified
therapists should be. At minimum, there is a need for better public understanding of
the varieties of psychological therapy and therapists, their training, practices, and
credentials. With such information, the public may make more informed choices
regarding therapists and therapeutic approaches. In view of the concerns raised in
this and other articles about the risks of memory recovery techniques, professionals
and professional groups who do not advocate such techniques may decide actively to
distance themselves from those who do, thereby enhancing the public's ability to
differentiate among therapists.
In addition to improved public education, increased financial support for people
seeking psychological treatment might lessen reliance on less expensive unqualified
therapists. Of course, the public's failure to discriminate between practitioners with
and without advanced formal training may not entirely be a matter of ignorance or
financial restriction. As Dawes (1994) has noted, studies of clinical expertise and
outcome effectiveness have not always shown a relationship between amount of
formal training and effectiveness of treatment. A recent meta-analysis by Stein and
Lambert (1995) indicated that formal training is sometimes related to therapeutic
success, but the relationship is surprisingly weak. The recommendations made below
regarding improvements in formal training, certification, and use of empirically
validated treatments would, if followed, increase the difference between licensed or
certified mental health care providers and those who offer "therapies" without
benefit of advanced formal education.
Formal training of psychotherapists. Even within the relatively narrow confines
of practitioners with advanced degrees in clinical psychology, there appears to be a
wide range in the amount, nature, and quality of training. Of course, graduate
programs in clinical psychology cannot provide advanced training in all content areas
of potential relevance to practitioners. For example, it would be unreasonable to
demand that clinical programs ensure that all graduates master research and theory
in the cognitive psychology of human memory (although such knowledge might have
protected graduates from using suggestive memory recovery techniques). Programs
quite appropriately vary, both across universities and over time, in the content areas
on which they focus. Thus, for example, it is to be hoped that in coming years clinical
training programs will reflect the increased awareness of CSA (cf. Enns et al., 1995).
It may nonetheless be reasonable to propose that all clinical graduate programs
accredited by the American Psychological Association demand a high level of
mastery of basic principles of scientific reasoning and methodology, particularly as
they apply to making diagnoses and evaluating treatment. These programs would be
in the spirit of the scientist-practitioner model (O'Sullivan & Quevillon, 1992),
which emphasizes the mutuality of research and practice (Hoshmand & Polking-
horne, 1992).
In our opinion, graduate programs in clinical psychology should also strive to
help students develop a degree of skepticism regarding the accuracy of their clinical
judgments and observations. Toward this end, clinical psychology programs might
require training regarding the cognitive biases and heuristics that lead human
reasoners to violate principles of science and logic. For example, it would be useful
for all graduates of such programs to know that a high rate of co-occurrence of two
878 LINDSAY AND READ

characteristics does not constitute evidence that those two characteristics are
correlated (see Shweder, 1977). Similarly, people with advanced degrees in clinical
psychology should understand such things as the relationship between base rates and
the likelihood of erroneous diagnoses, or the fact that causal effects cannot be
inferred from correlational evidence. No doubt many programs do successfully teach
such topics, but it appears that some do not.
We echo the recent American Psychological Association (1993) Division 12 Task
Force report in calling for an improved correspondence between which treatment
approaches are taught in clinical graduate programs and which treatment ap-
proaches have been empirically validated (cf. Campbell, 1992). The report indicated
that about 20% of the association accredited programs surveyed taught little or
nothing about 75% of the empirically validated treatments known to clinical
psychology. (It is worth noting that memory work was not identified as an empirically
validated treatment.) The task force also recommended directed supervision in
empirically validated treatment methods, noting that many programs either do not
provide supervision or do provide supervision in treatments that have not been
validated. The report argued that, at a minimum, a period of directed supervision by
a certified psychologist or psychiatrist is essential prior to undertaking the treatment
of others.
Ongoing training. A major goal of the Division 12 Task Force (American
Psychological Association, 1993) report was to increase the effectiveness of psycho-
therapy by promoting the kinds of training and supervision that will encourage
practitioners to use therapeutic techniques that have proven effective in controlled
studies. Beutler, Williams, and Wakefield (1993) reported that most respondents in
their survey of certified clinicians indicated that their therapeutic approaches drew
on empirical research, but Poole et al.'s (1995) data indicate that the majority of
psychotherapists sometimes use techniques such as hypnosis, guided imagery, and
dream interpretation as tools to help clients recover suspected hidden memories of
CSA: To the best of our knowledge, there are no published controlled studies testing
the efficacy of these approaches to treatment.
What accounts for the disparity between practitioners' self-reported reliance on
scientific research and evidence of widespread use of therapeutic interventions for
which scientific justification is absent? We suspect that one explanation is that
memory recovery techniques have been promoted in numerous books and workshops
advertised as "professional upgrading" or continuing education, offered by practitio-
ners (and endorsed by respected professionals and organizations) who are convinced
of the techniques' effectiveness (see also Beutler et al., 1993). As Dawes (1994) has
emphasized, testimonials of this type are not useful for determining the value of
therapeutic techniques.
In their survey of certified clinicians, Beutler et al. (1993) found that a
surprisingly high percentage (33%) belonged to no professional organization and, of
those affiliated with such organizations, only 30% reported attendance at discipline-
specific research presentations. When asked about sources of information for their
therapeutic knowledge and practices (beyond degree or diploma education), only
35% identified primary research journals as a source. Major sources of information
included "professional practice-oriented books" (51%), "popular lay books written
by nonacademic scholars" (33%), and "workshops and seminars led by nonacademic
professionals" (40%). Surveys by Feld (in press) indicate that MSW programs also
RECOVERED MEMORIES 879

often rely heavily on nonscientific information sources. Without entirely discounting


the potential value of these sources, we suggest that practitioners be trained and
encouraged to place greater reliance on refereed research publications and that
clinical researchers and journal editors ensure that research journals include many
articles of relevance and use to practitioners (cf. Beutler et al., 1993).
Ongoing supervision and consultation. Scientific research is an inherently public
activity, in which the procedures and results of studies are subjected to peer review
and truth value is gauged in large part by the replicability of publicly presented
findings. In contrast, most of what transpires in therapy is treated as private, and
therapeutic success is usually assessed by therapist and client, and perhaps often by
the therapist alone, without benefit of objective measures, control groups against
which to compare effects of therapy, or external review (Marsh & Hunsley, 1993).
We believe that therapy would benefit in many ways if it were a more open enterprise
(cf. Loftus & Rosenwald, 1993). For example, ongoing supervision and consultation,
conducted in ways that protect the confidentiality of the therapist-client relationship
yet provide real opportunities for therapists to receive critical feedback from
qualified others, could be very beneficial (as recommended by Courtois, 1994, Enns
et al., 1995, and Holloway & Neufeldt, 1995, among others). As evidence that
ongoing supervision is not yet normative, the American Psychological Association
(1993) Division 12 Task Force noted that of programs advertising training in new
treatment techniques or theories in the APA Monitor, only 10% required directed
supervision prior to the awarding of continuing education credit or certificates of
competence in the new technique.
Summary: Training and certification. Our recommendations regarding training
and certification of mental health care providers are similar to those made by many
others over the years. We suggest public education efforts on the part of professional
organizations to improve the public's knowledge of the differences between service
providers with varying amounts and kinds of formal training. We also suggest that
graduate programs in clinical psychology increase the extent to which their
approaches are grounded in science, and we recommend improved standards for
ongoing education. Finally, we propose that the quality and efficacy of psychotherapy
might be improved if practitioners were to have greater opportunities for ongoing
supervision and consultation with well-qualified peers.

Guidelines for Trauma-Oriented Psychotherapy


The past year or so has brought suggestions from many clinicians regarding the
need for caution and care in the treatment of suspected adult survivors of CSA and
calling for the development of guidelines for trauma-oriented psychotherapy (e.g.,
Bloom, 1994; Brown, 1995; Courtois, 1994; Enns et al., 1995; Yapko, 1994). We are
optimistic that continued efforts in this direction will raise the standard of care and
reduce the likelihood of false memories without undercutting support for survivors
of abuse. Refinement of guidelines for professional conduct in this area will rely on
future research of the sorts described in a subsequent section (Research Initiatives),
but the outlines of such guidelines can already be specified. We expect that
important components will include the following instructions: (a) Keep an open
mind about the client's history—do not assume that current symptoms can be taken
as reliable indicators of CSA, and do not assume that clients who disavow an
880 LINDSAY AND READ

abuse history are "in denial"; (b) avoid suggestive questions or statements (e.g., that
hidden memories of childhood traumas may underlie the client's problems, or that
healing may rely on recovering such memories); (c) obtain the client's informed
consent before using memory recovery techniques; (d) do not use hypnosis, guided
imagery, sodium amytal, interpretation of dreams, or "body memories" to help
clients recollect CSA; (e) be judicious in recommending self-help books, survivors'
groups, and workshops; (f) keep careful records, especially of sessions involving
memory work; and (g) do not suggest that clients who recover memories in therapy
take legal action against alleged perpetrators. The rationale for most of these
recommendations has been amply reviewed in the foregoing; in the following we
briefly discuss our recommendations regarding informed consent, record keeping,
and legal action.
Informed consent. A survey of 97 San Diego doctoral-level psychotherapists by
E. Stein and C. J. Dalenberg (personal communication, February 2, 1995) indicates
that clients frequently are not given an opportunity to give truly informed consent
prior to therapy. The American Psychological Association's (1992) Code of Ethical
Conduct (Ethical Standard 4.02) requires that clients be informed of the potential
risks of therapeutic techniques and be told whether the approach to be used has been
empirically validated or is experimental (cf. S. Simpson & Baker, 1994). In turn,
informed consent requires that practitioners be aware of the potential risks of their
interventions (Ogloff, 1995).
Record keeping. Some practitioners whose work includes a focus on CSA-
oriented memory work do not make or retain records of their interventions. For
example, an Ontario trial judge recently suspended proceedings in a case against a
teacher accused of having committed gross indecency 30 years earlier, because the
sexual assault crisis center that had provided psychological counseling for the
plaintiff destroyed the files pertaining to the case ("Rape-crisis Centre," 1994).
Reportedly, this center has a policy of shredding files whenever there is police
involvement, and the newspaper account indicated that many other Canadian
centers are following a similar policy as a self-described act of civil disobedience.
There are also cases in which individual trauma-oriented practitioners have decided
not to keep records; in some cases the therapists cited a concern that the records
would be misused in the defense of perpetrators, in others that the records put them
and clients at risk for reparation from satanic cults (e.g., Reagor, 1991; cf. Gould &
Cozolino, 1992).
Record keeping in cases in which adult clients report CSA is a difficult and
complex issue. On the one hand, it is easy to appreciate that those who work to
support CSA victims are appalled when therapy records are misused in the defense
of suspected abusers. Furthermore, detailed note-taking during a session may
detract from attention to the client. Finally, most practitioners believe that
client-therapist confidentiality is crucial for good psychotherapy, and recording
therapy sessions may be seen as threatening confidentiality. On the other hand, the
principle that people accused of crimes have a right to information that may
exonerate them is a central premise of our judicial system (although therapy records
have statutory privilege in some jurisdictions; Cram & Dobson, 1993). Therapy
records regarding reported sexual abuse can also serve the interests of survivors (e.g.,
in custody disputes or in legal defenses of survivors accused of assaulting or killing
their abusers). Walker (1994) cautioned that in the absence of therapy records,
RECOVERED MEMORIES 881

judges may bar therapists from testifying on behalf of clients. Furthermore, the
challenges of providing good psychotherapy to numerous clients over extended
periods of time are such that we suspect that competent practice requires careful
record keeping. Finally, in some cases practitioners' self-interest may contaminate
decisions not to keep notes of questionable interventions. On balance, professional
ethics demand that practitioners keep careful records of therapy sessions (American
Psychological Association, 1992, Ethical Standard 1.23; Courtois, 1994). The level of
detail and the manner of taking records is up to therapists or their employers.
Whether or not such records are released to the courts is perhaps an individual moral
question (cf. Walker, 1994), although therapists are subject to court subpoena to
testify.
Recommending legal action. Some advocates of memory work recommend that
clients who recover memories of CSA consider legal action against alleged abusers as
a way of seeking validation and healing (e.g., Bass & Davis, 1988). Recently,
however, some clinical psychologists have suggested that legal actions against alleged
perpetrators more often harm than help clients who have recovered memories
(Brown, 1995; Campbell, 1994; Mould, 1994). Courtois (1994) pointed out that
supporting or participating in legal actions compromises the therapist's role. It has
also been argued that therapists who recommend legal action are obliged to warn
clients about the risks of memory recovery techniques and seek corroborative
evidence to support the allegations (Brown, 1995; Courtois, 1994; Schneider, 1994;
Yapko, 1994). For example, clients should be informed that allegations arising from
hypnotic procedures are typically not admissible in court. It goes without saying that
these cautionary statements should be provided early in therapy.
Summary: Guidelines for trauma-oriented psychotherapy. As a way of reducing
the risk of false memories and beliefs, Ceci and Loftus (1994) suggested that
therapists doing memory work engage in a "disconfirmation" strategy in which they
test the historical accuracy of memories as they are reported, doing so in a manner
that neither questions the client's veracity nor stifles disclosure. Several practitioners
have offered similar advice: Bloom (1994), Brown (1995), Courtois (1994), Enns et
al. (1995), and Yapko (1994) all recommended that therapists adopt a scientific
attitude, continually check hypotheses, encourage clients to tolerate ambiguity, and
challenge premature closure. Guidelines are already available for forensic child-
victim assessment, and construction of guidelines for memory work should not be
difficult (see Elliott & Briere, 1994; Rogers, 1994).
Professional guidelines are intended to help both clients and therapists. It is
exciting to envision the development of psychotherapy as a profession widely known
for providing safe, reliable, and effective ways of helping people cope with or
overcome psychological problems. Pending this development, a well-established
standard of care could reduce the risks of therapy and protect therapists who follow
such standards from spurious lawsuits.

Legislative Actions to Regulate Therapists


Others have proposed that more drastic steps than those just described are
needed to protect the public from untested and potentially harmful therapies. For
example, Campbell (1994) argued that it is naive to hope that practitioner-oriented
professional organizations such as the American Psychological Association will
speak to the central issue of whether scientific evidence supports the safety and
882 LINDSAY AND READ

efficacy of searches for hidden memories of CSA. Campbell noted that the
association's Council of Representatives voted 55 to 52 against obtaining input from
its own Board of Scientific Affairs regarding who would be appointed to the task
force empaneled to produce a policy statement on memory work, and he argued that
for the association to challenge the core premises of memory work would be to
challenge the "agenda of the guild." These arguments imply that psychology, as a
profession, may be incapable of setting standards that will adequately protect the
people it attempts to serve. Sechrest (1994) noted that if the professional organiza-
tions are unable to take decisive steps to improve standards of training, efficacy, and
safety, such steps "will be imposed from the outside, perhaps in Draconian fashion"
(p. 1). For example, a group called The National Association for Consumer
Protection in Mental Health Practices is lobbying for legislation requiring that all
psychotherapy paid for with federal funds follow specified procedures for obtaining
informed consent (C. Barden, personal communication, September 6,1994).

Research Initiatives
As reviewed in previous sections, existing evidence indicates that (a) the base
rate of recoverable hidden histories of contact CSA is low, (b) people with hidden
histories of CSA cannot reliably be identified on the basis of symptoms, and (c)
memory recovery techniques pose risks for nonabused clients and their families.
Nonetheless, many questions remain concerning these and related issues, and
further research is desperately needed. Next, we identify some of the areas in which
the need for research is most keen.
Memory for trauma. As noted earlier, some psychologists argue that traumas
tend to be remembered better than mundane experiences, whereas others claim that
traumas are often repressed or otherwise forgotten. The former position is more
strongly supported by scientific research, but the two claims are not necessarily
mutually exclusive (i.e., it could be that traumas are usually well remembered but
sometimes are repressed or dissociated), and at present there are too few studies to
justify conclusive statements. Additional studies of children who have experienced
trauma, with long-term follow-up measures, are needed. Enns et al. (1995) and
Siegel (1995) have suggested a large variety of useful directions for research on
memory for trauma.
Efficacy of memory work. Clinical psychologists who believe that memory work
is an important therapeutic tool should support research designed to test the
hypothesis that clients who present with the psychological problems identified as
indicators of hidden histories of abuse (e.g., depression, sexual dysfunction, anxiety,
eating disorders), but report no abuse history in response to direct questions, have
better outcomes if they receive therapies that include a focus on recovering
memories of CSA than do matched control clients who receive other treatments.
Beutler and Hill (1992), Brown (1995), Enns et al. (1995), Haaken and Schlaps
(1991), Levitt and Pinnell (1994), and Siegel (1995) have all emphasized the
importance of studies of outcome effectiveness, but we are aware of only one modest,
unpublished assessment of memory work (Spiegel, 1994, cited in Ceci & Loftus,
1994). According to Ceci and Loftus, Spiegel found no gains of memory work over a
present-focused comparison treatment. Of course, even if differences in favor of
memory work had been obtained, further research would be needed to determine
which components of the approach were effective. A first step that would provide an
RECOVERED MEMORIES 883

essential foundation for such research is the development of treatment manuals for
memory work in psychotherapy (American Psychological Association, Division 12
Task Force, 1993; Beutler & Hill, 1992; Holloway & Neufeldt, 1995; Stein &
Lambert, 1995). The control condition against which clients receiving memory work
are compared should be whatever alternative low-risk treatment for the particular
presenting symptoms is best supported by empirical research. Ideally, such studies
would include long-term follow-up of clients.
If such studies demonstrate that memory work has differentially beneficial
effects, additional follow-up research could be designed to explore corollary issues:
Can procedures be developed to identify which clients are more or less likely to
benefit from memory work? Which memory recovery techniques are associated with
favorable outcomes and which are not? Are memory recovery techniques still
effective if steps are taken to reduce risks (e.g., informed consent, limited number or
duration of techniques)?
A related question for future research concerns therapists' ability to discrimi-
nate between clients with hidden histories of CSA and those with no history of CSA.
This is a methodologically challenging research topic, but the difficulties are not
insurmountable. For example, in Williams's (1994a) study of women with a recorded
history of an instance of CSA, 12% reported no such history when interviewed 17
years after the recorded abuse (i.e., they reported neither the recorded instance nor
any other instance of CSA). Such cases could be randomly mixed with a matched
control group of people who also report no CSA history but for whom there is no
record of such abuse, and trauma-memory-oriented therapists could attempt to
discriminate between the two groups (cf. Ceci & Bruck, 1994).
Treatment for effects of recovering memories of CSA. Many people who recover
histories of CSA report that memory recovery was a traumatic experience (e.g., Bass
& Davis, 1988; Frederickson, 1992). Some critics of memory work have argued that
symptoms characteristic of PTSD observed in clients who experience memory
recovery often stem not from CSA but from horrifying illusory memories and false
beliefs developed through suggestive memory work (Ofshe & Walters, 1994; Nelson
& Simpson, 1994). In any case, a substantial minority of clients who recover
memories of CSA in therapy subsequently confront their alleged abusers (typically
their parents), break off communication with them, or both (Poole et al., 1995). This
is, of course, painful for all parties. The literature promoting memory work addresses
the need for healing from sequelae of recovering memories, including developing
alternative forms of social support for people who recover histories of CSA and
consequently terminate contact with family members and sever friendships with
those who doubt the accusations (e.g., Bass & Davis, 1988; Frederickson, 1992).
Further development of effective supports for people who recover memories may
benefit from systematic research.
Some people who recover memories later come to doubt or even reject the belief
that they were sexually abused, and it is likely that this process also involves
considerable psychological stress. Pendergrast (1995) provided anecdotal reports of
seven individuals who retracted allegations based on recovered memories, and the
FMSF claims to have been contacted by 300 families in which the accuser has
retracted allegations (FMSF Scientific and Professional Advisory Board, 1994).
Individuals who now believe that their recovered memories were false provide
unique opportunities for research on memory (deRivera, 1994; Lief & Fetkewicz,
884 LINDSAY AND READ

1994; Nelson & Simpson, 1994). There may also be a need for research on efficacious
treatment interventions for such people.
Parents accused on the basis of recovered memories of CSA evidence psychologi-
cal suffering, largely attributable to the destruction of their families but also
sometimes to stress associated with public humiliation, legal actions, and so forth. Of
course, the suffering of accused parents may be genuine even when accusations are
partly or entirely valid. There is a need for research on effective psychotherapies for
such individuals, as well as for the development of ways of helping families cope with
disintegration and, where appropriate, reintegration.
Cognitive research on memory recovery techniques. Clinical studies could be
complemented by further cognitive research on memory recovery techniques.
Experimental research is constrained by ethical guidelines, which may raise
questions about generalizability (e.g., researchers could not test the hypothesis that
memory recovery techniques can lead people to develop illusory memories or beliefs
of CSA). However, principles of memory discovered in laboratory research have
proven to be largely consonant with evidence obtained in more naturalistic field
studies, and laboratory research has some important advantages (e.g., Banaji &
Crowder, 1989). One advantage is that experimenters can know exactly what did and
did not happen in the to-be-remembered events. Such studies could differentiate
between techniques that reliably improve recall versus those that merely lower
response criterion or facilitate imagery and hence increase false recall (cf. Fisher &
Geiselman, 1988). Research could also explore interactions between individual-
difference measures and effects of memory recovery techniques (Hyman & Billings,
1995; Schooler & Loftus, 1993). It would be important that cognitive psychologists
undertaking such research educate themselves regarding relevant aspects of the
clinical literature, so as to design studies that will be of relevance and use to clinical
psychologists and practitioners (cf. Beutler & Hill, 1992; Beutler et al., 1993; Brown,
1995).
Summary: Research initiatives. Previously, we sketched the outlines for profes-
sional guidelines for trauma-oriented psychotherapy with individuals who may have
been sexually abused as children but who report no such history in response to direct
questions. The research initiatives mentioned in this section would allow for more
detailed recommendations that could improve therapeutic gains while minimizing
the risk of illusory memories and beliefs. A more general recommendation is that
researchers and practitioners work collaboratively, so that knowledge deficiencies in
both camps can be mitigated and useful knowledge created.

Legal Issues
The vast majority of people who experience recovery of memories of CSA do not
take formal legal action against the alleged perpetrator or perpetrators. Psychothera-
pists responding to Poole et al.'s (1995) surveys indicated, on average, that only 6%
of their clients who recovered memories of CSA took any legal action against the
alleged perpetrator or perpetrators. The FMSF ("Legal actions against parents,"
1994) claimed that legal actions against alleged perpetrators were reported by only
7% of those who had contacted the organization. Presumably, only a fraction of the
cases in which legal action is taken end up in court; often the action is restricted to
filing a police report or having an attorney send a letter threatening legal action
unless certain conditions are met (e.g., a financial settlement, agreement to cease
RECOVERED MEMORIES 885

attempts at communication). Thus, formal legal action is a rare outcome of memory


work. Nonetheless, Lipton (1994) reported that the FMSF is following 800 legal
cases (most of which are civil suits) in which people who recovered memories have
taken the alleged perpetrator or perpetrators (most often their fathers) to court, and
a smaller but growing number of suits against therapists or authors by people accused
of CSA on the basis of recovered memories or by former clients who claim that
therapy led them to hold harmful false beliefs.
Criminal and civil actions concerning delayed accusations of CSA have increased
dramatically in recent years (Ewing, 1992). The increase likely results from (a)
greater acceptance of the reality of CSA and its potential long-term effects (Enns et
al., 1995); (b) increased use of CSA-focused memory work in psychotherapy and
through self-help; and (c) changes to relevant statutes of limitations that have
increased opportunities for legal actions based on delayed accusations (Bulkley &
Horwitz, 1994; Ernsdorff & Loftus, 1993; S. Simpson & Baker, 1994). Regulations in
some jurisdictions that provide for state funding of psychotherapy when adult clients
file charges against alleged abusers may also have contributed to the increase in legal
actions.
Historical context and recent changes. A thorough discussion of the history of
legislation and legal precedent related to the current controversy about recovered
memories of CSA is well beyond the scope of this article (and beyond our expertise),
but it is important to acknowledge the outlines of that history. Until the last few
decades, successful prosecution of CSA was rare. Nowadays, although approximately
50% of reports of current or recent child sexual abuse are classified as "unsubstanti-
ated," and only some substantiated cases are prosecuted, the number of reported
cases has increased dramatically, and approximately 90% of prosecutions are
successful (Finkelhor, 1994).9 There is little reason to believe that these increases in
reports and successful prosecutions are due to an increase in the base rate of CSA
(Finkelhor, 1994). Rather, in the past several decades there have been a number of
important advances in public policy and legislative initiatives that have improved
detection of CSA and facilitated prosecution of such cases (e.g., Hechler, 1988).
Although there have been some problems with implementation of these changes
(Fincham, Beach, Moore, & Diener, 1994), in our view they constitute much-needed
progress.
Legislative changes in the past several years have enhanced opportunities for
memory recovery claims (and other delayed accusations) to come forward to the
courts in many parts of the United States and Canada. For criminal charges of CSA,
there has been no need to change the statutes in some U.S. states or in Canada,
because in those jurisdictions there has been no limit on the time period in which
such charges may be brought. In many other states, statutes of limitations have been
tolled on criminal charges until alleged victims reach the age of majority (Bulkley &
Horwitz, 1994; Ernsdorff & Loftus, 1993; Hayes, 1994). Similarly, for civil actions,
most U.S. states and Canada have either extended or are considering tolling statutes
of limitations (although there is considerable variation across jurisdictions in the
number of years beyond the age of majority for which the tolled statutes runs—
Bulkley & Horwitz, 1994; Ernsdorff & Loftus, 1993; Hayes, 1994—and pretrial

'Criteria for substantiation are generally high, so unsubstantiated does not necessarily mean false (see
Finkelhor, 1994).
886 LINDSAY AND READ

hearings must be held to determine whether tolling is appropriate in each case;


Lipton, 1994, and S. Simpson & Baker, 1994).
At the heart of public and legislative willingness to alter statutes of limitation is
an acceptance of the concept of "delayed discovery." As Bulkley and Horwitz (1994)
noted, there are at least three bases for delayed discovery in cases involving CSA.
Two of these are not relevant to the concept of recovered hidden memories but
instead recognize circumstances in which complainants, now adults, have always
known that they were abused but either were prevented from disclosing the abuse by
the abuser's threats or were unaware of the harmful consequences of the abuse until
many years had passed. It is the third basis for application of the delayed discovery
rule to cases involving CSA that is most relevant here: Legislators and courts
apparently have been convinced that conscious recollections of CSA are often not
available to victims until many years after the abuse occurred (Boland & Quirk, 1994;
Lamm, 1991). Indeed, according to Lamm (1991), some courts have ruled that
statutes of limitation should be tolled only for claims that arise following a period of
total repression of CSA memories, on the grounds that existing statutes already
provide sufficient opportunity for complainants who always had knowledge of their
abuse to come forward with the allegations. Bulkley and Horwitz (1994), Ernsdorff
and Loftus (1993), and Lamm (1991) cited cases in which this "repression" argument
for delayed discovery was successfully advanced, and Spiegel and Scheflin (1994) and
S. Simpson and Baker (1994) claimed that courts have often found recovered
memory allegations more credible than delayed allegations in which the complainant
claimed always to have remembered the abuse. In one particularly intriguing
Canadian case (Regina v. Norman, 1993), the court apparently gave additional weight
to the complainant's recovered memory testimony because a friend of the victim
testified that she witnessed the alleged rape and claimed that she also repressed and
then recovered memories of it.
Science, politics, and legislative change. As previously indicated, in our view
research does not support the belief that complete amnesia for recoverable histories
of CSA is a common occurrence, nor does it support the idea that delayed
recollections are necessarily accurate. Given the speed and apparent ease with which
statutes of limitation have changed since 1988, it seems that beliefs rather different
from ours have guided the actions of legislators in the majority of states and of
justices in the Supreme Court of Canada (Bulkley & Horwitz, 1994; Lamm, 1991;
Loftus & Rosenwald, 1993; Loftus, Weingardt, & Hoffman, 1993; Schneider, 1994).
Erroneous beliefs about memory have a long history. At present, there appears to be
a widely accepted belief that the details of all life experiences are stored in memory
in such a way that they can reliably be recalled years later with high levels of accuracy
and completeness (Bower, 1990; Loftus & Loftus, 1980; Roediger, 1980). These high
expectations for human memory are woven into the fabric of our culture and are
reflected in literature and film. Such beliefs have also been played out in war-crime
trials in which Holocaust survivors have been expected to recall with accuracy and
detail persons and events they witnessed 50 years earlier (e.g., Wagenaar, 1988;
Wagenaar & Groeneweg, 1990) and in media-event court cases involving recovered
memories of CSA (e.g., Loftus & Ketcham, 1994; Terr, 1994). The publicity
surrounding these cases and their outcomes have likely served to reinforce the views
that concern us here. Survey research has revealed very optimistic views of memory
capabilities among lay people (Garry, Loftus, & Brown, 1994; Loftus & Loftus, 1980;
RECOVERED MEMORIES 887

S. M. Seelau, personal communication, January 5, 1995). Loftus and Loftus (1980),


Yapko (1994), S. Smith (1991), and Poole et al. (1995) discovered similar beliefs
among psychologists, psychotherapists, hypnotherapists, and social workers. For
example, 31% of the respondents in Yapko's survey of 860 practitioners agreed with
the statement, "When someone has a memory of a trauma while in hypnosis, it
objectively must actually have occurred," and 54% said that hypnosis enables people
to remember actual events "as far back as birth."10
It seems clear that psychological scientists have been unsuccessful in correcting
widely held but erroneous views about memory functioning. One reason for our lack
of success may be researchers' reluctance to wander into the fray of public debate;
another may be a disinclination on the part of the public and their representatives
toward critical or scientific thinking. Many commentators have referred to the "crisis
in science education" in North America, which is reflected in the documented
decline of skills in mathematics and other sciences (e.g., Sagan, 1990; Schatzman,
1993). Thus, it is not surprising that the public is not well prepared for scientific
argument. Unfortunately, most politicians are equally unprepared (Schatzman,
1993). In North America the normal educational background for career politicians is
a degree in law, not science. Thus, legislators are often ill-equipped to evaluate
scientific evidence and may fall easy prey to uninformed beliefs and vigorously
argued positions. Furthermore, even if legislators did have the appropriate back-
ground, it is not clear that they could resist the politics of the situation. As Haney
(1993) put it in a more general discussion of the pervasiveness of incorrect notions
about psychological concepts, "Even a reasonably 'informed' judiciary can be made
responsive to a largely 'uninformed' public" (p. 389). Given acceptance of the idea
that a large percentage of adults in North America have completely repressed
histories of CSA that can be accurately recovered through therapy, taking legislative
steps to allow such people to press charges against abusers would appear to be a right
and just course of action.
Future public policy regarding statutes of limitation. Liberalization of statutes of
limitation was intended to provide opportunities for legitimate delayed claims of
CSA to be brought forward. Unfortunately, such liberalization may also lead to
erroneous accusations about alleged events in the distant past (Loftus, 1994b). Such
charges are difficult to try, because they often reduce to the word of the accuser
against that of the accused. As Ernsdorff and Loftus (1993) pointed out, there may
also be grounds for concern that tellings may be extended to cases involving alleged
recovered memories of other kinds of childhood events or to events alleged to have
occurred after the age of majority. In consideration of these issues, Ernsdorff and
Loftus (1993) and Bulkley and Horwitz (1994) enumerated some potential changes
to legislation and judicial procedure in cases involving alleged recovered memories.
Potential alterations of the tolling mechanism included (a) complete exclusion of
cases based on claims of recovered memory; (b) establishment of a finite number of
years beyond the statute of limitation during which claims may be filed; (c)
requirement of corroborating evidence; (d) imposition of a higher burden of proof in
civil cases; and (e) provision of a criminal law remedy only (because of its higher
burden of proof and greater protection for accuseds). With respect to changes in
judicial procedure that could be adopted as special safeguards against wrongful
10
Issues similar to those discussed here apply to recent uses of multiple personality disorder
diagnoses in criminal defences (Piper, 1994).
888 LINDSAY AND READ

accusations, Ernsdorff and Loftus discussed (a) use of fictitious names for accusers
and accuseds; (b) a more stringent limitation on introduction of "similar act"
evidence than presently exists; (c) development of jury instructions regarding
recovered memories (akin to the Telfaire instruction regarding eyewitness testimony,
U.S. v. Telfaire, 1972); and (d) admission of expert evidence regarding memory
recovery.
Extensions of the statutes of limitations are being reconsidered in some states.
For example, Schneider (1994) reported that the Illinois Senate recently passed
legislation that essentially caps the limitation period at the age of 30 years, a
reconsideration that Schneider attributed to the Cardinal Bernardin case. The State
of New Hampshire Superior Court (State of New Hampshire v. John Morahan, 1995),
in a joint judgment in a pretrial hearing on the admissability of recovered-memory
testimony in the cases of State v. Joel Hungerford (1995) and State v. John Morahan
(1995), ruled as follows:
The phenomenon of memory repression, and the process of therapy used in these
cases to recover the memories, have not gained general scientific acceptance in the
field of psychology, and they are not scientifically reliable, (p. 1)

Mandatory reporting laws and protection of third parties. At present, many


individuals in the U.S. and Canada are legally mandated to report incidents of known
or suspected child sexual abuse to authorities (although many of those mandated do
not always report abuse when they observe or suspect it; Brooks, Perry, Starr, &
Teply, 1994). It is not clear whether mandatory reporting always, never, or
sometimes applies to cases in which adult clients report or are suspected of CSA by
an identified perpetrator who may be a continuing danger to identifiable children.11
In any case, it appears that therapists rarely report recovered memory cases to
authorities, even if they believe clients who identify alleged Satanic murderers
allegedly still active in a cult (see Bottoms et al., in press).
Mandating the reporting of recovered memory cases would likely have multiple
effects. On the one hand, mandated reporting might further increase legal actions
based on questionable recovered memories, with numerous ill-effects. Mandatory
reporting might also threaten the confidentiality of the therapist-client relationship.
On the other hand, mandatory reporting would lead to the provision of an early
record of what was remembered and (perhaps) of the steps taken to obtain such
recall (cf. Courtois, 1994; Enns et al., 1995), and it might encourage therapists to be
more cautious about forming the opinion that a client had been abused as a child
(e.g., seek collaborative and potentially disconfirming evidence, request advice from
colleagues, etc.) because it requires therapists to go on public record regarding their
beliefs about clients' memories. We raise the issue of mandatory reporting in
recovered memory cases not because we think it is a good policy but rather because
considering this issue helps drive home the gravity of the consequences of using
memory work to help clients recover suspected memories of CSA.
Jurors' beliefs about recovered memories. To what extent do jurors accept
recovered memories? Loftus et al. (1993) found that mock jurors (university
students, members of the public, and practicing lawyers) viewed accusations of CSA

"It might also be argued that a therapist who believes that a client who has recovered memories
poses a risk to an identifiable person (e.g., intends to assault the alleged perpetrator) has a duty to warn
that person (as in the Tarasoff ruling; Monahan, 1993).
RECOVERED MEMORIES 889

more skeptically when they were described as recovered memories (as opposed to
never-forgotten memories). However, moderate degrees of credibility were assigned
to allegations in both conditions. Clark and Nightingale (1994) and A. Warren
(personal communication, December 15, 1994) found that undergraduates' assess-
ments of the credibility of abuse allegations did not significantly differ for cases based
on repressed versus never-forgotten memories. The content of recovered memories
may not greatly affect credibility either, as Schutte (1994) found no difference in
mock jurors' verdicts when allegations were of recovered memories of incest versus
SRA (cf. Rind & Harrington, 1994). Other research has shown that jurors have
undue confidence in memories recovered with hypnosis (e.g., Wagstaff, Vella, &
Perfect, 1992). Further research in this area is needed, but these findings suggest that
potential jurors find recovered memories as or nearly as credible as never-forgotten
memories of CSA.
Certification for giving expert evidence. Expert psychological testimony has been
the subject of much recent debate (e.g., the 1992 special issue of Law and Human
Behavior; Melton, 1994; Moen, 1994). It has sometimes been suggested that only
clinicians have the requisite knowledge and experience to inform courts about
repressed memories (Begley & Brant, 1994; Terr, 1994). In contrast, Sales, Shuman,
and O'Connor (1994) argued that expert testimony based solely on clinical
experience and intuition raises numerous problems of evidentiary reliability. Read
and Lindsay (1994) argued that the lines do not necessarily divide as neatly along
practitioner and researcher specialties as implied by Sales et al., because there are
many individuals with both scientific research skills and clinical experience. In any
event, Sales et al. argued that the recent U.S. Supreme Court decision in Daubert v.
Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. (1993), which articulates standards for the
admissibility of expert testimony, may affect future access to federal (and some state)
courtrooms by both psychological scientists and practitioners, because expert
scientific testimony will no longer be judged by the Frye standard (United States v.
Frye, 1923). The Frye standard requires "general acceptance" of the substance of
expert evidence within the expert's professional community, whereas the more
recent Daubert decision allows consideration of a number of other bases for
accepting expert scientific evidence (e.g., whether or not the theory on which
testimony is based is falsifiable and whether or not it has been subjected to scientific
tests and is relevant to the task at hand). Questions as to whether and when clinical
judgments have a scientific basis gain new importance in light of Daubert (Melton,
1994).
As we previously argued, the current state of psychological science does not
allow psychologists to discriminate with certainty between veridical and illusory
memories in individual cases. Rather, the credibility of recovered memories claims
can only be estimated by considering various factors. Thus, in our opinion, experts on
both sides of this issue should be modest in their claims regarding specific cases.
Experts should also be careful in their use of terminology. By analogy, consider the
legal battles that arose over prosecutors' attempts to have expert witnesses assign
children the diagnosis of "child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome" (CSAAS;
Bulkley, 1992; Stewart & Young, 1992; Summit, 1983, 1992b). CSAAS is not an
established psychiatric condition and hence did not meet the Frye standard.
Nonetheless, it was apparently not unusual for psychologists admitted as expert
witnesses to provide a CSAAS "diagnosis" (Bulkley, 1992; Stewart & Young, 1992).
Summit (1992a) stated that, in retrospect, he wishes he had used the term pattern
890 LINDSAY AND READ

rather than syndrome, because he had intended only to claim that a pattern of
allegation and recantation is common in CSA, not that such a pattern verifies the
presence of CSA. The terms false memory syndrome and repressed memory syndrome
present the same dangers, as revealed in lawyers' requests to us (and, we assume,
many others) that we "diagnose" accusers as suffering from false memory syndrome.
Because courts have an interest in obtaining accurate and representative
scientific testimony, it might be appropriate to develop a registry of blue-ribbon
experts from which courts could call for expert psychological testimony (cf. Hayes,
1994). Such a group of experts could develop standards and criteria regarding the
nature and quality of the research cited in their courtroom presentations (cf.
Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists, 1991). If members of
the profession cannot agree on a representative group of experts, perhaps psycholo-
gists should not be considered qualified to give expert evidence.
Agreement on who qualifies as an expert psychologist is likely to be difficult, but
having experts agree with one another may be more difficult still. Courts are
reluctant to allow battles of experts over scientific evidence because jurors are likely
to have difficulty integrating and evaluating the information presented. As one way
to avoid the tendency toward such battles (a tendency likely to be particularly
pronounced in cases involving alleged recovered memories), a recent Royal
Commission in the United Kingdom advocated changes in procedure for criminal
trials such that both prosecution and defense provide pretrial disclosures of scientific
evidence and argument so that, as far as possible, pretrial agreement can be reached
on such evidence (Roberts, 1994).
Despite the limitations of expert psychological testimony, it seems essential that
legal professionals be educated about memory functioning, memory work in
psychotherapy, self-help, and related issues (cf. Simmons, 1994). There are a variety
of ways in which this can be done, choices that social scientists have debated
frequently (Melton, 1994; Roesch, Golding, Hans, & Reppucci, 1991). In addition to
serving as an expert witness, these include (a) presenting research findings in outlets
accessible to members of the legal profession (e.g., law reviews and informal
professional networks); (b) collaborating with other psychologists to develop
"amicus" briefs that provide summaries of social science data to the courts (e.g.,
Maryland v. Craig, 1990; New Jersey v. Margaret Kelly Michaels, 1993); and (c) working
with legal professionals to design research directly relevant to the law.
Consequences of criminal and civil proceedings for accuser and accused. In adult
recovered memory cases, it is the accuser's decision whether to pursue redress
through the courts. If the accuser lives in a jurisdiction in which both criminal and
civil options are open, consideration of the costs and benefits of each is important
(see Bulkley & Horwitz, 1994, and Enns et al., 1995, for discussions of the advantages
and disadvantages of these two types of legal action). From the accuser's perspective,
a criminal trial has the advantage that the case rests in the state's hands and thus the
state's resources may be brought to bear by the prosecution. Furthermore, the
retributive and deterrent consequences of a guilty verdict for the accused may meet
the accuser's original goals in filing a charge. On the other hand, the likelihood of a
criminal court conviction is relatively small, because the burden of proof falls on the
prosecution and that burden is high ("beyond a reasonable doubt"). Finally, because
the prosecution is managed by the state and not the accuser, accusers may more
often feel victimized than validated by the procedure (Ewing, 1992).
Accusers are more likely to succeed with civil actions than with criminal charges,
RECOVERED MEMORIES 891

because the burden of proof is lower for civil suits ("the preponderance of
evidence"). Also, a civil court, unlike a criminal court, necessarily provides an
opportunity for the accuser to confront the accused directly, which may have
therapeutic benefits for survivors of abuse (Mallia, 1993). For these reasons,
individuals seeking redress for a recovered history of CSA have been advised by the
self-help literature (e.g., Bass & Davis, 1988, p. 128) and by legal opinion (e.g., Clute,
1993) to pursue a civil rather than a criminal action. Clute, as well as Lamm (1991)
and Walker (1992), suggested that a successful litigation provides an opportunity to
gain closure and validation of one's injuries. Financial settlements are often reached
before cases are tried (Williams, 1988, cited in Bass & Davis, 1988), thus precluding
public presentation of the charges, but such settlements may still be seen as a positive
outcome for the accuser (Lamm, 1991).
There are potential risks of both types of legal action for accusers (see Enns et
al., 1995; Ewing, 1992; Penelope, 1992; Simmons, 1994; Walker, 1992, for differing
perspectives on this issue). Not surprisingly, no systematic research has been done to
test the hypothesis that suing alleged abusers has therapeutic benefits, and some
experts have cautioned against legal action. Ewing, for example, recommended
against legal action of either form when much time has elapsed since the alleged
abuse, no corroboration from other individuals exists, and the litigation is to be
funded by the accuser. In these circumstances, Ewing argued that successful
litigation will be highly unlikely, and if litigation is unsuccessful the accuser must deal
with the experience of losing the case.
From the accused's perspective, a criminal charge may often be seen as more
threatening than a civil suit. Whether the accused is convicted or not, criminal
charges can lead to widespread publicity. If convicted, the accused faces possible
incarceration and a criminal record. However, a criminal trial provides a higher level
of due process and greater protection against erroneous guilty judgments than does a
civil trial and may therefore be the preferred option for the accused in many
recovered memory cases.
Other legal actions. As noted in the introduction to this article, actions filed
against alleged perpetrators or their estates are but one of several kinds of cases
involving memory recovery now being heard in the courts (Loftus & Rosenwald,
1993; Schneider, 1994; Spiegel & Scheflin, 1994). Other permutations of clients,
alleged abusers, and therapists and authors have also been involved in legal
proceedings, such as actions by accuseds against therapists for defamation or for
neglect of duty of care, against authors of self-help books, or against accusers for
defamation of character, and actions by former clients against therapists for
negligent and injurious practices (for examples, see Loftus & Rosenwald, 1993;
Nelson & Simpson, 1994; Schneider, 1994; S. Simpson & Baker, 1994). As yet, the
U.S. Supreme Court has not dealt with issues arising from recovered memory cases,
but given the level of legal activity and political tension in this area, Supreme Court
decisions are likely and will perhaps lead to very rapid and dramatic changes in
psychotherapy (Slovenko, 1994).
Alternative forms of dispute resolution. It may be possible to develop alternative
dispute resolution (ADR) procedures that may be preferable to formal criminal
trials or civil litigations for dealing with charges based on recovered memories of
CSA. If those who allege CSA based on recovered memories were provided a forum
other than a court of law in which their complaints could be heard, perhaps a number
of the goals of legal action could be met (conflict resolution, presentation of charges
892 LINDSAY AND READ

and confrontation of the accused, and validation for the accuser) without revictimiza-
tion by an adversarial court system. ADR has often proved successful in cases of tort
liability and criminal law (see review by MacCoun, Lind, & Taylor, 1992). One option
reported to have reasonable success in recovered memory cases is mediation by a
qualified, mutually acceptable mediator (cf. Craig, 1994). Agreements reached as a
result of mediation are binding and preclude further action based on additional
delayed discoveries, and if a settlement is mediated it often includes coverage of the
accuser's medical and counseling bills as well as future therapy with a mutually
acceptable therapist. Courts are likely to be pleased with the mediation process,
because it removes cases from their cluttered dockets. Taxpayers may also support
mediation, because it is much less expensive than criminal or civil hearings. Los
Angeles County has made mediation mandatory in cases of delayed accusations of
CSA if either party requests it.
From a therapeutic point of view, mediation may offer most of the benefits and
few of the risks of civil or criminal proceedings and may help shift clients' focus from
the past to the future. From a pragmatic standpoint, ADR procedures may be
appealing because the considerable sums of money spent by both parties on a
litigation could instead be directed to more constructive ends (e.g., therapy for
accusers and accuseds).
Summary: Legal issues. Legal actions involving delayed accusations of CSA
have increased dramatically in the last decade. The increased litigation can partly be
attributed to improved support for survivors, acknowledgment of the potential
lasting ill effects of CSA, and consequent relaxation of statutes of limitations, all of
which enhanced opportunities for legitimate delayed accusations to be brought to
court. Unfortunately, it appears that many delayed accusations brought to the courts
involve alleged histories of CSA recovered through suggestive influences in therapy
or self-help, and the evidence and arguments reviewed earlier suggest that many such
accusations are ill-founded. The possible applicability of mandatory reporting laws
to adults' accounts of CSA, the possible extension of tollings of statutes of limitation
to recovered memories of childhood events other than CSA or to postchildhood
events, and publicity about successful suits against therapists may further increase
litigation in this area. Moreover, popularization of false memory syndrome is likely to
lead to widespread use of claims of false memories in legal defenses of people
accused of sex crimes (including cases in which there is no basis for such a claim).
From a legal perspective, the central issue is how triers of fact can discriminate
between veridical and false delayed accusations. In most cases there is no material
evidence, and often there is no corroborating evidence of any kind. In these
circumstances, it is critical that triers of fact be informed of evidence regarding the
likelihood of accurately recovering previously unavailable histories of CSA and the
factors that affect the likelihood of false memories or beliefs. At the same time,
expert witnesses must acknowledge the limitations of current psychological science
with regard to the veracity of recovered memories in individual cases. In view of the
drawbacks of criminal and civil actions, those seeking redress for recovered histories
of CSA may wish to consider ADR.

Summary and Conclusion


We began this article by briefly reviewing the historical and cultural context in
which the debate about recovered memories of CSA arose. The history of denial and
RECOVERED MEMORIES 893

minimization of CSA and the belief that successful psychotherapy often relies on
resolving childhood traumas illuminate the rationale for using memory recovery
techniques and help account for skepticism regarding criticisms of those techniques.
The first half of our article reviewed evidence that indicates that allegations of
CSA based solely on recovered memories should be viewed with caution. We
clarified criticisms of memory work in psychotherapy, emphasizing that concerns
have focused on long-term, multifaceted (although not necessarily coercive) searches
for hidden memories in clients who initially disavow abuse histories, and described
evidence of the prevalence of memory recovery techniques in psychotherapy. We
then critically reviewed evidence regarding the base rate of recoverable hidden
memories of CSA. In our view, this evidence indicates that only a small minority of
psychotherapy clients have recoverable hidden histories of CSA. Furthermore,
research on supposed "indicators" of CSA indicates that it is likely difficult for
practitioners to identify clients with hidden memories. We argued that, in the context
of research on the suggestibility of memory and evidence that clinical judgments are
subject to various biases and heuristics, these findings justify grave concern that
memory work may sometimes lead to iatrogenic illusory memories or false beliefs
regarding histories of CSA in clients who were not abused as children. The claim is
not that all memories recovered through suggestive memory work are false but rather
that some—perhaps many or even most—likely are. We also offered criteria for
weighing the validity of allegations of CSA based solely on recovered memories.
The second half of the article considered implications of scientific evidence
regarding recovered memories for psychology, public policy, and the law. The section
on training and certification of mental health care providers discussed issues related
to the prevalence of unqualified therapists and offered recommendations regarding
psychotherapists' training and ongoing education, supervision, and consultation.
Many of these recommendations drew on the American Psychological Association
(1993) Division 12 Task Force report, which called for improved training in scientific
thinking and methodology, greater emphasis on empirically validated treatments,
and higher standards for ongoing supervision, education, and consultation. Next, we
discussed the need for well-considered and widely disseminated professional
guidelines for practitioners whose treatments include explorations of clients'
memories of childhood traumas. Here too, our recommendations drew on those
others have offered (e.g., American Medical Association, 1994; American Psychiatric
Association, 1993; Australian Psychological Society, 1994; Brown, 1995; Courtois,
1994; Enns et al., 1995). We emphasized the importance of (a) avoiding high-risk
memory recovery techniques and ancillary practices, (b) obtaining informed consent,
(c) keeping records, and (d) being circumspect about recommending that clients take
legal action against alleged abusers.
The penultimate section of the paper called for field research on memory for
trauma, clinical research on the efficacy of memory work and on treatment for
sequelae of recovered memories, and cognitive research on memory recovery
techniques. The final section offered a discussion of legal issues related to recovered
memories. We sketched the historical backdrop for recent changes in laws regarding
civil and criminal actions against alleged perpetrators of CSA and briefly discussed
the roles of science and politics in fostering changes of the statutes of limitation for
delayed accusations of CSA. We then speculated about the applicability of
mandatory reporting laws to cases in which therapists believe an adult client was
894 LINDSAY AND READ

abused by an identified perpetrator who may be a continuing risk to children. Next


we reviewed recent studies of mock jurors' decisions in recovered memory cases and
discussed issues related to expert psychological testimony. Finally, we considered the
consequences of civil versus criminal legal actions for accusers and accuseds,
enumerated the variety of kinds of legal actions involving recovered memories that
are currently being tried, and discussed alternative forms of dispute resolution that
both accusers and accuseds may find preferable to civil or criminal actions.
The sexual abuse of children is an important problem. CSA is much more
common than once was thought, and there is ample evidence documenting an association
between CSA and adulthood psychological problems. The reality and importance of this
social problem is not diminished by the evidence and arguments presented here.
Trauma-oriented therapies vary, from approaches that most experts would agree
are dangerously suggestive to those that most would agree are not dangerously
suggestive. Likewise, people who recover memories through memory work vary, from
those who initially claimed that their childhoods involved no CSA to those who have
always remembered CSA but initially did not view the abuse as particularly
important and had forgotten some details and instances. Finally, the conditions that
lead to recovered memories also vary, from cases in which memory recovery follows a
highly suggestive search to cases in which memory recovery occurs more spontane-
ously. In our reading, scientific evidence has clear implications for cases at the
extremes of these continua: Memories recovered through suggestive memory work
by people who initially denied any such history should be viewed with skepticism, but
there are fewer grounds on which to doubt spontaneously recovered memories of
common forms of CSA or recovered memories of details of never-forgotten abuse.
Between these extremes lies a gray area within which the implications of existing
scientific evidence are less clear and experts are likely to disagree.
We noted at the outset of this article that the stakes are high for all parties in the
debate about memory work and recovered memories. It is perhaps inevitable that
there is tension between those who emphasize the importance of supporting
survivors of CSA and those who emphasize the importance of avoiding false beliefs.
As noted earlier, the history of denial and minimization of CSA understandably
predisposes victim advocates to be suspicious of claims that some recovered
memories of CSA are false. The intensity of that response is likely heightened by the
claim that illusory memories and beliefs can be caused by practitioners' interven-
tions. It is not surprising that the reaction to this charge is angry denial, sometimes
taking forms that might be described as "counterbacklash" (e.g., stating that critics
of memory work are motivated by a desire to protect perpetrators, as in Enns et al.,
1995, Herman & Harvey, 1993, or Walker, 1994).
For therapists who have used suggestive techniques to help clients remember
horrific histories of CSA perpetrated by loved ones, the cost of considering the idea
that such techniques can lead nonabused clients to develop false beliefs may be
great. In our view, however, the evidence for this possibility is too strong to ignore,
and trauma-oriented therapists will eventually realize that extensive use of memory
recovery techniques is ill-advised. Painful as such an acknowledgement is likely to be
for those who have used and prescribed such techniques, they can take comfort in the
fact that abandoning risky memory recovery techniques in no way denies the reality
and importance of CSA.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 895

References
Abrams, S., & Abrams, J. (1994). False memory syndrome versus total repression. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
American Medical Association. (1985). Scientific status of refreshing recollection by use of
hypnosis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 253, 1918-1923.
American Medical Association. (1994, June 16). Report of the Council on Scientific Affairs:
Memories of childhood abuse (CAS Report 5-A-94). Chicago, IL: Author.
American Psychiatric Association. (1993, December 22). APA issues statements on memories of
sexual abuse, gun control, television violence (News Release No. 93-58). Washington, DC:
Author.
American Psychological Association. (1992). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of
conduct. American Psychologist, 47, 1597-1611.
American Psychological Association, Task Force on Promotion and Dissemination of
Psychological Procedure. (1993, October).^ report to the Division 12 Board. Washington,
DC: Author.
American Psychological Association, Division 33. (1994, 14 August). Resolution on facilitated
communication. Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychological Association. (1995, August). Questions and answers about memories of
childhood abuse. Washington, DC: Author.
Andrews, B., Morton, J., Bekerian, D. A., Brewin, C. R., Davies, G. M., & Mollon, P. (1995,
May). The recovery of memories in clinical practice: Experiences and beliefs of British
Psychological Society practitioners. The Psychologist, 209-214.
Australian Psychological Society. (1994, October 1). Guidelines relating to the reporting of
recovered memories. Sydney, Australia: Author.
Baker, B. (1994). The changing face of social work. Common Boundary, Jan/Feb., 32-37.
Banaji, M. R., & Crowder, R. G. (1989). The bankruptcy of everyday memory. American
Psychologist, 44, 1185-1193.
Bass, E., & Davis, L. (1988). The courage to heal: A guide for women survivors of child sexual
abuse. New York: Harper & Row.
Bass, E., & Davis, L. (1994). The courage to heal: A guide for women survivors of child sexual
abuse (3rd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.
Begley, S., & Brant, M. (1994, September 26). You must remember this. Newsweek, 68-69.
Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K. J., Hood, J. E., daCosta, G. A., & Cassavia, E. (1992). A review of
the long-term effects of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 16, 101-118.
Berliner, L., & Williams, L. M. (1994). Memories of child sexual abuse: A response to Lindsay
and Read. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 379-388.
Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a
dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 727-735.
Beutler, L. E., & Hill, C. E. (1992). Process and outcome research in the treatment of adult
victims of childhood sexual abuse: Methodological issues. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 60, 204-212.
Beutler, L. E., Williams, R. E., & Wakefield, P. J. (1993). Obstacles to disseminating applied
psychological science. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 2, 53-58.
Beutler, L. E., Williams, R. E., & Zetzer, H. A. (1994). Efficacy of treatment for victims of
child sexual abuse. The Future of Children, 4, 156-175.
Bjork, R. (1989). Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In F. I. M.
Craik & H. L. Roediger, III (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness (pp. 309-330).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bloom, P. B. (1994). Clinical guidelines in using hypnosis in uncovering memories of sexual
abuse: A master class commentary. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 42, 173-178.
896 LINDSAY AND READ
Blume, E. S. (1990). Secret survivors: Uncovering incest and its aftereffects in women. New York:
Ballantine.
Boland, P. L., & Quirk, S. A. (1994). Victims may need time to recognize the offense. American
Bar Association Journal, 80, 42.
Bottoms, B. L., Shaver, P. R., & Goodman, G. S., (in press). An analysis of ritualistic and
religion-related child abuse allegations. Law and Human Behavior.
Bower, G. H. (1990). Awareness, the unconscious, and repression: An experimental
psychologist's perspective. In J. L. Singer (Ed.), Repression and dissociation: Implications
for personality theory, psychopathology, and health (pp. 209-232). Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Bowers, K. S. (1984). On being unconsciously influenced and informed. In K. S. Bowers & D.
Meichenbaum (Eds.), The unconscious reconsidered (pp. 227-273). New York: Wiley.
Brenneis, C. B. (1994). Clinical and conceptual problems in the identification and reconstruction
of trauma in the absence of autobiographical memory. Manuscript submitted for publica-
tion.
Briere, J. (in press). A self-trauma model for treating adult survivors of severe child abuse. In
J. Briere, L. Berliner, J. Bulkley, C. Jenny, & T. Reid (Eds.), The APSAC handbook on
child maltreatment.
Briere, J., & Conte, J. (1993). Self-reported amnesia for abuse in adults molested as children.
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 21-31.
Briere, J., & Elliott, D. M. (1992). Sexual abuse trauma among professional women:
Validating the Trauma Symptom Checklist—40. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 391-398.
Briere, J., & Runtz, M. (1990). Differential adult symptomology associated with three types of
child abuse histories. Child Abuse and Neglect, 14, 357-364.
Briere, J., & Zaidi, L. Y. (1989). Sexual abuse histories and sequelae in female psychiatric
emergency room patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 1602-1606.
British Psychological Society. (1995). Recovered memories: The report of the working party of the
British Psychological Society. Leicester, England: Author.
Brooks, C. M., Perry, N. W., Starr, S. D., & Teply, L. L. (1994). Child abuse and neglect
reporting laws: Understanding interests, understanding policy. Behavioral Sciences and
the Law, 12, 49-64.
Brown, D. (1995). Pseudomemories, the standard of science and the standard of care in
trauma treatment. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 37, 3-29.
Brown, G. R., & Anderson, B. (1991). Psychiatric morbidity in adult inpatients with childhood
histories of sexual and physical abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 55-61.
Brownmiller, S. (1975). Against our will: Men, women, and rape. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Brundage, J. A. (1993). Sex, law and marriage in the Middle Ages. Brookfield, VT: Voriorum.
Bryer, J. B., Nelson, B. A., Miller, J. B., & Kroll, P. A. (1987). Childhood sexual and physical
abuse as factors in adult psychiatric illness. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144,
1426-1430.
Bulkley, J. A. (1992). The prosecution's use of social science testimony in child sexual abuse
cases: National trends and recommendations. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 1, 73-93.
Bulkley, J. A., & Horwitz, M. J. (1994). Adults sexually abused as children: Legal actions and
issues. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 12, 65-87.
Burstein, A. (1985). Posttraumatic flashbacks, dream disturbances, and mental imagery.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 46, 374—378.
Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M., & McGaugh, J. L. (1994). B-adrenergic activation and
memory for emotional events. Nature, 371, 702-704.
Cameron, C. (1994). Veterans of a secret war: Survivors of childhood sexual trauma compared
to Vietnam War veterans with PTSD. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 117-132.
Campbell, T. W. (1992). Therapeutic relationships and iatrogenic outcomes: The blame-and-
change maneuver in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 29, 474-480.
Campbell, T. W. (1994, December). Good news I bad news: The burden is ours. Paper presented
RECOVERED MEMORIES 897

at the Memory and Reality: Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False Memory
Syndrome Foundation and Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1994). How reliable are children's statements? ... It depends. Family
Relations, 43, 255-257.
Ceci, S. J., Leichtman, M. D., & White, T. (1995). Interviewing pre-schoolers: Remembrance of
things planted. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Ceci, S. J., & Loftus, E. F. (1994). "Memory work": A royal road to false memories? Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 8, 351-364.
Chapman, L. M., & Chapman, J. P. (1967). Genesis of popular but erroneous psychodiagnostic
observations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 193-204.
Christiansen, S.-A. (1992). Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: A critical review.
Psychological Bulletin, 112, 284-309.
Christianson, S.-A., & Nilsson, L. G. (1989). Hysterical amnesia: A case of aversively
motivated isolation of memory. In T. Archer & L.-G. Nilsson (Eds.), Aversion, avoidance,
and anxiety (pp. 289-310). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Claridge, K. (1992). Reconstructing memories of abuse: A theory-based approach. Psycho-
therapy, 29, 243-252.
Clark, H. L., & Nightingale, N. N. (1994). When jurors consider recovered memory cases: Does
gender matter? Unpublished manuscript.
Clark, K. R. (1993). Season of light/season of darkness: The effects of burying and
remembering traumatic sexual abuse on the sense of self. Clinical Social Work Journal, 21,
25-43.
Clute, S. (1993). Adult survivor litigation as an integral part of the therapeutic process. Journal
of Child Sexual Abuse, 2, 121-127.
Committee on Ethical Guidelines for Forensic Psychologists. (1991). Law and Human
Behavior, 15, 655-665.
Conte, J. R. (1994). Child sexual abuse: Awareness and backlash. The Future of Children, 4,
224-232.
Conte, J. R., & Berliner, L. (1988). The impact of sexual abuse on children: Empirical findings.
In L. E. A. Walker (Ed.), Handbook of sexual abuse of children: Assessment and treatment
issues (pp. 72-93). New York: Springer.
Coons, P. M. (1994). Reports of satanic ritual abuse: Further implications about pseudomemo-
ries. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 1376-1378.
Courtois, C. A. (1988). Healing the incest wound: Adult survivors in therapy. New York: Norton.
Courtois, C. A. (1992). The memory retrieval process in incest survivor therapy. Journal of
Child Sexual Abuse, 1, 15-32.
Courtois, C. A. (1994, November). Some suggested guidelines of practice in therapy with adult
clients possibly abused as children (who enter therapy without memory and who do not
disclose at the outset of therapy). Paper presented at the 10th Annual Midwest Conference
on Child Sexual Abuse and Incest, Madison, WI.
Courtois, C. A. (1995). Scientist-practitioners and the delayed memory controversy: Scientific
standards and the need for collaboration. The Counseling Psychologist, 23, 294-299.
Craig, W. (1994, December). Mediation. Paper presented at the Memory and Reality:
Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False Memory Syndrome Foundation and
Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Cram, S. J., & Dobson, K. S. (1993). Confidentiality, ethical, and legal aspects for Canadian
psychologists. Canadian Psychology, 34, 347-363.
Dalenberg, C. J. (1994). Making and finding memories: A commentary on the "repressed
memory" controversy. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 3, 109-118.
Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceutical, Inc., 113 S. Ct. 2786 (1993).
Davies, G. M., & Thomson, D. M. (1988). Memory in context, context in memory. Chichester,
England: Wiley.
898 LINDSAY AND READ
Dawes, R. M. (1989). Experience and the validity of clinical judgment: The illusory
correlation. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 7, 457-467.
Dawes, R. M. (1994). The house of cards. Toronto: Maxwell Macmillan Canada.
deRivera, J. (1994, December). Survey of retractors' families. Paper presented at the Memory
and Reality: Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False Memory Syndrome
Foundation and Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
di Nardo, P. A., Moras, K., Barlow, D. H., Rapee, R. M., & Brown, T. A. (1993). Reliability of
DSM-III-R anxiety disorder categories: Using the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule—
Revised. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 251-256.
DiTomasso, M. J., & Routh, D. K. (1993). Recall of abuse in childhood and three measures of
dissociation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 477-485.
Dolan, Y. M. (1991). Resolving sexual abuse: Solution-focused therapy and Ericksonian hypnosis
for adult survivors. New York: Norton.
Dollinger, S. J. (1985). Lightning-strike disaster among children. British Journal of Medical
Psychology, 58, 375-383.
Dowling, J. F., & Graham, J. R. (1976). Illusory correlation and the MMPI. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 40, 531-538.
Eich, E. (1995). Searching for mood dependent memory. Psychological Science, 6, 67-75.
Ellenson, G. S. (1985). Detecting a history of incest: A predictive syndrome. Social Casework:
The Journal of Contemporary Social Work, 66, 525-532.
Elliott, D. M., & Briere, J. (1994). Forensic sexual abuse evaluations of older children:
Disclosures and symptomatology. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 12, 261-278.
Engel, B. (1989). The right to innocence: Healing the trauma of childhood sexual abuse. New
York: Ballantine (Ivy Books).
Enns, C. Z., McNeilly, C., Corkery, J., & Gilbert, M. (1995). The debate about delayed
memories of child sexual abuse: A feminist perspective. The Counseling Psychologist, 23,
181-279.
Ernsdorff, G. N., & Loftus, E. F. (1993). Let sleeping memories lie? Words of caution about
tolling the statute of limitations in a case of memory repression. The Journal of Criminal
Law and Criminology, 84, 129-174.
Ewing, C. P. (1992). Suing your perpetrator: Response to a survivor's story. Journal of Child
Sexual Abuse, 1, 129-132.
Feld, A. (in press). Social work education: Where's the content on memory? Journal of
Teaching in Social Work.
Feldman-Summers, S., & Pope, K. S. (1994). The experience of "forgetting" childhood abuse:
A national survey of psychologists. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62,
636-639.
Femina, D. D., Yeager, C. A., & Lewis, D. O. (1990). Child abuse: Adolescent records vs.
adult recall. Child Abuse and Neglect, 14, 227-231.
Figley, C. R. (Ed.). (1986). Trauma and its wake: Vol. 2. Traumatic stress theory, research, and
intervention. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R. H., Moore, T., & Diener, C. (1994). The professional response to
child sexual abuse: Whose interests are served? Human Relations, 43, 244-254.
Finkelhor, D. (1994). Current information on the scope and nature of child sexual abuse. The
Future of Children, 4, 31-53.
Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1988). Assessing the long-term impact of child sexual abuse: A
review and conceptualization. In E. T. Hotaling, D. Finkelhor, J. T. Kirkpatrick, & M. A.
Strauss (Eds.), Family abuse and its consequences: New directions in research (pp. 270-289).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Finkelhor, D., & Dzubia-Leatherman, J. (1994). Victimization of children. American Psycholo-
gist, 49, 173-183.
Finkelhor, D., Hotaling, G., Lewis, I. A., & Smith, C. (1990). Sexual abuse in the national
RECOVERED MEMORIES 899

survey of adult men and women: Prevalence, characteristics, and risk factors. Child Abuse
and Neglect, 14, 19-28.
Fisher, R. P., & Geiselman, R. E. (1988). Enhancing eyewitness memory with the cognitive
interview. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of
memory: Current research and issues (pp. 34-39). New York: Wiley.
FMSF Scientific and Professional Advisory Board. (1994). Frequently asked questions about the
False Memory Syndrome Foundation. Philadelphia: Author.
Foa, E. B., Rothbaum, R. O., Riggs, D. S., & Murdock, T. B. (1991). Treatment of
posttraumatic stress disorder in rape victims: A comparison between cognitive-
behavioral procedures and counseling. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59,
715-723.
Frankel, F. H. (1993). Adult reconstruction of childhood events in the multiple personality
literature. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 954-958.
Frankel, F. H. (1994). The concept of flashbacks in historical perspective. The International
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 321-336.
Frederickson, R. (1992). Repressed memories: A journey to recovery from sexual abuse. New
York: Simon & Schuster.
Freud, S. (1962). Screen memories. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard edition of the
complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 3, pp. 303-322). London: Hogarth
Press. (Original work published 1899)
Freud, S. (1962). The aetiology of hysteria. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard
edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 3, pp. 191-221).
Toronto: Clark, Irwin. (Original work published 1896)
Freyd, J. J. (1993). Theoretical and personal perspectives on the delayed memory debate. Paper
presented at the Center for Mental Health at Foote Hospital's Continuing Education
Conference: Controversies Around Recovered Memories of Incest and Ritualistic Abuse,
Ann Arbor, MI.
Ganaway, G. K. (1989). Historical versus narrative truth: Clarifying the role of exogenous
trauma in the etiology of MPD and its variants. Dissociation, 2, 201-205.
Ganaway, G. K. (1995). Hypnosis, childhood trauma, and dissociative identity disorder:
Toward an integrative theory. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
42, 127-144.
Gardner, R. A. (1992). Belated realization of child sex abuse by an adult. Issues in Child Abuse
Accusations, 4, 177-195.
Garry, M., Loftus, E. F., & Brown, S. W. (1994). Memory: A river runs through it.
Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 438-^51.
Gelinas, D. (1983). The persisting negative effects of incest. Psychiatry, 46, 313-322.
Gold, S. N., Hughes, D., & Hohnecker, L. (1994). Degrees of repression of sexual abuse
memories. American Psychologist, 49, 441-442.
Goleman, D. (1992, July 21). Childhood trauma: Memory or invention? New York Times, p. B5.
Goodman, G. S., Bottoms, B. L., Schwartz-Kenney, B. M., & Rudy, L. (1991). Children's
testimony for a stressful event: Improving children's reports. Journal of Narrative and Life
History, 1, 69-99.
Goodman, G. S., Hirschman, J. E., Hepps, D., & Rudy, L. (1991). Children's memory for
stressful events. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 109-158.
Goodman, G. S., Qin, J., Bottoms, B. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1993, December). Repressed memory
and allegations of ritual and religion-related child abuse. Paper presented at the Clark
Conference on Memories of Trauma, Worchester, MA.
Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., Batterman-Faunce, J. M., Riddlesberger, M., & Kuhn, J. (1994).
Predictors of accurate and inaccurate memories of traumatic events experienced in
childhood. Consciousness & Cognition, 3, 269-294.
Gould, C, & Cozolino, L. J. (1992). Ritual abuse, multiplicity, and mind control. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 20, 194-196.
900 LINDSAY AND READ

Green, B. L., Grace, M. C., Vary, M. G., Kramer, T. L., Cleser, G. C, & Leonard, A. C. (1994).
Children of disaster in the second decade: A 17-year follow-up of Buffalo Creek survivors.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 71-79.
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1992). The psychology of interrogations, confessions and testimony. New
York: Wiley.
Haaken, J., & Schlaps, A. (1991). Incest resolution therapy and the objectification of sexual
abuse. Psychotherapy, 28, 39-47.
Haney, C. (1993). Psychology and legal change: The impact of a decade. Law and Human
Behavior, 17, 371-398.
Harvey, M. R., & Herman, J. L. (1994). Amnesia, partial amnesia and delayed recall among adult
survivors of childhood trauma. Unpublished manuscript.
Hayes, M. L. (1994). The necessity of memory experts in prosecution for child sexual abuse
based upon repressed memories. American Clinical Law Review, 39, 69-85.
Hechler, D. (1988). The battle and the backlash: The child sexual abuse laws. New York: Heath.
Helzer, J. E., Robins, L. N., & McEvoy, L. (1987). Post-traumatic stress disorder in the general
population. The New England Journal of Medicine, 1317, 1630-1634.
Henry, B., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Langly, J., & Silva, P. (1994). On the "remembrance of
things past:" A longitudinal evaluation of the retrospective method. Psychological
Assessment, 6, 92-101.
Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York: Basic Books.
Herman, J. L., & Harvey, M. R. (1993, April). The false memory debate: Social science or
social backlash? Harvard Mental Health Letter, pp. 4-6.
Herman, J. L., & Schatzow, E. (1987). Recovery and verification of memories of childhood
sexual trauma. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4, 1-14.
Herron, W. G., Schultz, C. L., & Welt, A. G. (1992). A comparison of 16 systems to diagnose
schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 711-721.
Holloway, E. L., & Neufeldt, S. A. (1995). Supervision: Its contributions to treatment efficacy.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 207-213.
Hoshmand, L. T., & Polkinghorne, D. E. (1992). Redefining the science-practice relationship
and professional training. American Psychologist, 47, 55-66.
Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Peterson, C. (1994). How can I remember when "I" wasn't
there: Long-term retention of traumatic experiences and emergence of the cognitive self.
Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 327-355.
Hutchings, P. S., & Button, M. A. (1993). Sexual assault history in a community mental health
center clinical population. Community Mental Health Journal, 29, 59-63.
Hyman, I. E., Jr. (1994, December). False memories of childhood experiences. Paper presented
at the Memory and Reality: Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False Memory
Syndrome Foundation and Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Hyman, I. E., Jr., & Billings, F. J. (1995). Individual differences and the creation of false
childhood memories. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Hyman, I. E., Jr., Husband, T. H., & Billings, F. J. (1995). False memories of childhood
experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 181-197.
Hyman, I. E., Jr., & Loftus, E. F. (in press). Modern conceptions of the vicissitudes of early
childhood memories. In D. A. Halperin (Ed.), False memory syndrome: Therapeutic and
forensic perspectives. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Jones, D. P. H. (1994). Autism, facilitated communication, and allegations of child abuse and
neglect. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 491^93.
Kaminer, W. (1992). I'm dysfunctional, you're dysfunctional. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Kandel, M., & Kandel, E. (1994, May). Flights of memory. Discover, 32-38.
Kelley, C. M., & Lindsay, D. S. (in press). Memory dissociations. In E. L. Bjork & R. A. Bjork
(Eds.), Handbook of perception and cognition: Memory (Vol. 10). New York: Academic
Press.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 901

Kendall-Tackett, K. A., Williams, L. M, & Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact of sexual abuse on


children: A review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164-180.
Kirsch, I., Montgomery, G., & Sapirstein, G. (1995). Hypnosis as an adjunct to cognitive
behavioral psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
63, 214-220.
Koss, M. P., Tromp, S., & Tharan, M. (1995). Traumatic memories: Empirical foundations,
forensic and clinical implications. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 111-132.
Kraft, R. N. (1994, November). Down in the flood of remembrance: Holocaust memory. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis, MO.
Kristiansen, C. M. (1994). Bearing witness to the patriarchal revictimization of survivors.
SWAP Newsletter, 20, 7-16.
Lamm, J. B. (1991). Easing access to the courts for incest victims: Toward an equitable
application of the delayed discovery rule. The Yale Law Journal, 100, 2189-2208.
Lawrence, L. (1993). Backlash: A look at the abuse-related amnesia and delayed memory
controversy. Moving Forward, 2, 1,10-14.
LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Emotion as memory: Anatomical systems underlying indelible neural
traces. In S.-A. Christiansen (Ed.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and
theory (pp. 269-288). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Legal actions against parents. (1994, February). FMSF Foundation Newsletter, p. 9.
Levitt, E. E., & Pinnell, C. M. (1994). Some additional light on the childhood sexual
abuse-psychopathology axis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
43, 145-162.
Lief, H. I., & Fetkewicz, J. M. (1994, December). Demographic and description aspects of
retractors. Paper presented at the Memory and Reality: Reconciliation Conference
sponsored by the False Memory Syndrome Foundation and the Johns Hopkins Medical
Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Lindsay, D. S. (1994). Memory source monitoring and eyewitness testimony. In D. F. Ross,
J. D. Read, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.), Adult eyewitness testimony: Current trends and
developments (pp. 27-55). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lindsay, D. S., & Poole, D. A. (in press). Remembering childhood sexual abuse in therapy:
Psychotherapists' self-reported beliefs, practices, and experiences. Journal of Psychiatry
and Law.
Lindsay, D. S., & Read, J. D. (1994). Psychotherapy and memories of childhood sexual abuse:
A cognitive perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 281-338.
Lindsay, R. C. L., & Wells, G. L. (1985). Improving eyewitness identifications from lineups:
Simultaneous versus sequential lineup presentations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70,
556-564.
Lipton, A. (1994, December). Status of lawsuits. Paper presented at the Memory and Reality:
Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False Memory Syndrome Foundation and
the Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48, 518-537.
Loftus, E. F. (1994a). Therapeutic recollections of childhood abuse: When a memory may not
be a memory. The Champion, 18, 5-10.
Loftus, E. F. (1994b). Untested claims damage the constitution. American Bar Association
Journal, 80, 43.
Loftus, E. F., Garry, M., & Feldman, J. (1994). Forgetting sexual trauma: What does it mean
when 38% forget? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1177-1181.
Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory: False memories and
allegations of sexual abuse. New York: St. Martin's Press.
Loftus, E. F., & Loftus, G. R. (1980). On the permanence of stored information in the human
brain. American Psychologist, 35, 409-420.
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information
902 LINDSAY AND READ

into visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4,
19-31.
Loftus, E. F. & Pickrell, J. E. (in press). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals.
Loftus, E. F., Polonsky, S., & Fullilove, M. T. (1994). Memories of childhood sexual abuse:
Remembering and repressing. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 67-84.
Loftus, E. F., & Rosenwald, L. A. (1993). Buried memories, shattered lives. American Bar
Association Journal, 79, 70-73.
Loftus, E. F., Weingardt, K. R., & Hoffman, H. G. (1993). Sleeping memories on trial:
Reactions to memories that were previously repressed. Expert Evidence, 2, 51-59.
Lynn, S. J., Rhue, J. W., Myers, B. P., & Weeks, J. R. (1994). Pseudomemory in hypnotized
and simulating subjects. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42,
118-129.
MacCoun, R. J., Lind, E. A., & Taylor, T. R. (1992). Alternative dispute resolution in trial and
appellate courts. In D. S. Kagehiro & W. S. Laufer (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and
law (pp. 95-114). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Madanes, C. (1990). Sex, love, and violence. New York: Norton.
Mallia, M. (1993). Adult survivor litigation as an integral part of the therapeutic process: A
reply. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 2, 129-130.
Malmquist, C. P. (1986). Children who witness parental murder: Post-traumatic aspects.
Joumalofthe American Academy ofChild Psychiatry, 25, 320-325.
Maltz, W. (1990, December). Adult survivors of incest: How to help them overcome the
trauma. Medical Aspects of Human Sexuality, 42—47.
Marsh, E. J., & Hunsley, J. (1993). Assessment considerations in the identification of failing
psychotherapy: Bringing the negatives out of the darkroom. Psychological Assessment, 5,
292-301.
Maryland v. Craig, 110 S. Ct. 3157 (1990).
Masson, J. M. (1984). The assault on truth: Freud's suppression of the seduction theory. New
York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
McCann, I. L., & Pearlman, L. A. (1990). Psychological trauma and the adult survivor: Theory,
therapy, and transformation. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
McGaugh, J. L. (1992). Affect, neuromodulatory systems and memory storage. In S.-A.
Christiansen (Ed.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp.
245-268). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
McHugh, P. R. (1994, December). The do's and don'ts for the clinician managing memories of
abuse. Paper presented at the Memory and Reality: Reconciliation Conference spon-
sored by the False Memory Syndrome Foundation and Johns Hopkins Medical Institu-
tions, Baltimore, MD.
McNulty, C., & Wardle, J. (1994). Adult disclosures of sexual abuse: A primary cause of
psychological distress? Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 549-555.
Meiselman, K. C. (1990). Resolving the trauma of incest: Reintegration therapy with survivors. San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Melton, G. (1994). Expert witnesses and questions of scientific validity. In B. D. Sales & G. R.
VandenBos (Eds.), Psychology in litigation and legislation (pp. 59-99). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Merry, S. N., & Andrews, L. K. (1994). Psychiatric status of sexually abused children 12
months after disclosure of abuse. Journalof the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 33, 939-944.
Minuchin, S., & Fishman, H. C. (1981). Family therapy techniques. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Moen, S. P. (1994). The use of expert witnesses in the defence of repressed memory claims.
Shepard's Expert and Scientific Evidence Quarterly, 2, 417-425.
Monahan, J. (1993). Limiting exposure to Tarasoff liability: Guidelines for risk containment.
American Psychologist, 48, 242-250.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 903

Morton, J. (1994). Cognitive perspectives on memory recovery. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8,


389-398.
Mould, D. E. (1994, December). Memory recovery therapy: Costly care for negative gain. Paper
presented at the Memory and Reality: Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False
Memory Syndrome Foundation and Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Nader, K., Pynoos, R., Fairbanks, L., & Frederick, C. (1990). Children's PTSD reactions one
year after a sniper attack at their school. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1526-1530.
Nash, M. (1987). What, if anything, is regressed about hypnotic age regression? A review of
the empirical literature. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 42-52.
Neisser, U., & Winograd, E. (1988). Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional
approaches to the study of memory. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Nelson, E. L., & Simpson, P. (1994). First glimpse: An initial examination of subjects who have
rejected their recovered visualizations as false memories. Issues in Child Abuse Accusa-
tions, 6, 123-133.
New Jersey v. Margaret Kelly Michaels, 264 N.J. S. Ct. 579 (1993).
Nielsen, T. A., & Powell, R. A. (1992). The day-residue and dream-lag effects: A literature
review and limited replication of two temporal effects in dream formation. Dreaming
Journal of the Association for the Study of Dreams, 2, 67-77.
Dates, R. K., O'Toole, B. I., Lynch, D. L., Stern, A., & Cooney, G. (1994). Stability and change
in outcomes for sexually abused children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 945-953.
Ofshe, R. J. (1989). Coerced confessions: The logic of seemingly irrational action. Cultic
Studies Journal, 6, 1-15.
Ofshe, R. J. (1992). Inadvertent hypnosis during interrogation: False confessions due to
dissociative state: Misidentified multiple personality and the satanic cult hypothesis.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 125-156.
Ofshe, R., & Walters, E. (1994). Making monsters: False memories, psychotherapy, and sexual
hysteria. New York: Scribner's.
Ogloff, J. R. P. (1995). Navigating the quagmire: Legal and ethical guidelines for mental
health professionals. In D. Marin & A. Moore (Eds.), First steps in the art of intervention
(pp. 347-377). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Olio, K. A. (1989). Memory retrieval in the treatment of adult survivors of sexual abuse.
Transactional Analysis Journal, 19, 93-100.
Olio, K. A. (1994). Truth in memory. American Psychologist, 49, 442-443.
Olio, K. A., & Cornell, W. F. (1994). Making meaning not monsters: Reflections on the
delayed memory controversy. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 3, 77-94.
Ornstein, P. A., Merritt, K. A., & Baker-Ward, L. (1995, July). Children's recollections of
medical experiences: Exploring the linkage between stress and memory. Paper presented at
the meeting of the Society for Applied Research on Memory and Cognition, Vancouver,
British Columbia, Canada.
O'Sullivan, J. J., & Quevillon, R. P. (1992). 40 years later: Is the Boulder model still alive?
American Psychologist, 47, 67-70.
Parker, J. F., Bahrick, L., Lundy, B., Fivush, R., & Levitt, M. (1995, July). Children's memory
for a natural disaster: Effects of stress. Presented at the meeting of the Society for Applied
Research in Memory and Cognition, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
Pendergrast, M. (1995). Victims of memory: Incest accusations and shattered lives. Hinesburg,
VT: Upper Access.
Penelope. (1992). Suing my perpetrator: A survivor's story. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 1,
119-123.
Person, E. S., & Klar, H. (1994). Establishing trauma: The difficulty of distinguishing between
memories and fantasies. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 42, 1055-
1081.
Peters, D. P. (1991). The influence of stress and arousal on the child witness. In J. Doris (Ed.),
904 LINDSAY AND READ

The suggestibility of children's recollections: Implications for eyewitness testimony (pp.


60-76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Peters, S. D., Wyatt, G. E., & Finkelhor, D. (1986). Prevalence. In D. Finkelhor (Ed.), A
sourcebookon child sexual abuse (pp. 15-19). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Peterson, L., Moreno, A., & Harbeck-Weber, C. (1993). "And then it started bleeding:"
Children's and mothers' perceptions and recollections of daily injury events. Journal of
Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 345-354.
Pezdek, K. (1994). The illusion of illusory memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 339-350.
Pinel, J. P. J. (1990). Biopsychology. Toronto: Allyn & Bacon.
Piper, A., Jr. (1993). "Truth serum" and "recovered memories" of sexual abuse: A review of
the evidence. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, Winter, 447-471.
Piper, A., Jr. (1994). Multiple personality disorder and criminal responsibility: Critique of a
paper by Elyn Saks. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, Spring, 7-49.
Piper, A., Jr. (in press). A skeptical look at multiple personality disorder. In J. Bervoff, M.
Elin, & L. Cohen (Eds.), Multiple personality disorder: Critical issues and controversies.
Northvale, NJ: Aronson.
Poole, D. A., & Lindsay, D. S. (1995). Interviewing preschoolers: Effects of nonsuggestive
techniques, parental coaching, and leading questions on reports of nonexperienced
events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 129-154.
Poole, D. A., Lindsay, D. S., Memon, A., & Bull, R. (1995). Psychotherapy and the recovery of
memories of childhood sexual abuse: U.S. and British practitioners' beliefs, practices, and
experiences. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 426-437.
Pope, H. G. Jr., & Hudson, J. I. (1995). Can memories of sexual abuse be repressed?
Psychological Medicine 25, 121-126.
Powell, R. A., & Boer, D. P. (1994). Did Freud mislead patients to confabulate memories of
abuse! Psychological Reports, 74, 1283-1298.
Pynoos, R. S., & Eth, S. (1984). The child as witness to homicide. Journal of Social Issues, 2,
87-108.
Pynoos, R. S., & Nader, K. (1989). Children's memory and proximity to violence. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 236-241.
Rape-crisis centre under investigation for shredding. (1994, December 12). Globe and Mail, p.
A4.
Ratican, K. L. (1992). Sexual abuse survivors: Identifying symptoms and special treatment
considerations. Journal of Counseling and Development, 71, 33-38.
Read, J. D., & Lindsay, D. S. (1994). Moving toward a middle ground on the "false memory
debate": Reply to commentaries on Lindsay and Read. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8,
407-435.
Read, J. D., Seelau, S. M., & Nicholls, T. (1995). On the frequency of "memory gaps" in
non-clinical populations. Unpublished data.
Reagor, P. A. (1991, October). Survivors of childhood ritual abuse: Assessment and identifica-
tion. Paper presented at a conference on Working With Multiplexity and Ritual Abuse,
Justice Institute of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada.
Regina v. Norman, 87 C.C.C. (3d), 153 (1993).
Rind, B., & Harrington, E. (1994, December). Effect of type of reported abuse on beliefs in
repressed and recovered memories. Paper presented at the Memory and Reality: Reconcili-
ation Conference sponsored by the False Memory Syndrome Foundation and Johns
Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Roberts, P. (1994). Forensic science evidence after Runciman. Criminal Law Quarterly, 32,
780-792.
Roediger, H. L. (1980). Memory metaphors. Memory & Cognition, 8, 231-246.
Roediger, H. L., HI, & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human subjects.
In F. Boiler & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 8, pp. 62-131).
Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 905

Roesch, R., Golding, S. L., Hans, V. P., & Reppucci, N. D. (1991). Social science and the
courts: The role of amicus curaie briefs. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 1-9.
Rogers, M. L. (1994). Factors to consider in assessing adult litigants' complaints of childhood
sexual abuse. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 12, 279-298.
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Interpersonal expectancy effects: A 30-year perspective. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 176-179.
Ross, D. F., Read, J. D., & Toglia, M. P. (1994). Adult eyewitness testimony: Current perspectives
and developments. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Rowan, A. B., Foy, D. W., Rodriguea, N., & Ryan, S. (1994). Posttraumatic stress disorder in a
clinical sample of adults sexually abused as children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 51-61.
Russell, D. (1983). The incidence and prevalence of intrafamilial sexual abuse of female
children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 133-176.
Ryder, D. (1992). Breaking the circle ofsatanic ritual abuse: Recognizing and recovering from the
trauma. Minneapolis, MN: CompCare Publishers.
Sagan, C. (1990). Why we need to understand science. Skeptical Inquirer, 14, 263-269.
Sales, B. D., Shuman, D. W., & O'Connor, M. O. (1994). In a dim light: Admissibility of child
sexual abuse memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 399-406.
Santrock, J. W., Minnett, A. M., & Campbell, B. D. (1994). The authoritative guide to self-help
books. New York: Guilford Press.
Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1984). Abnormal psychology: The problem of maladaptive
behavior (4th ed.). Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Schacter, D. L. (1995). Memory distortion: History and current status. In D. L. Schacter, J. T.
Coyle, G. D. Fischbach, & M. M. Sullivan (Eds.), Memory distortion (pp. 1-46).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Schacter, D. L., & Kihlstrom, J. (1989). Functional amnesia. In F. Boiler & J. Grafman (Eds.),
Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 3, pp. 209-231). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Schatzman, E. (1993). A threat to science. Skeptical Inquirer, 17, 302-305.
Schimek, J. G. (1987). Fact and fantasy in the seduction theory: A historical review. Journal of
the American Psychoanalytic Association, 35, 937-965.
Schneider, J. G. (1994). Legal issues involving "repressed memory" of childhood sexual abuse.
The Psychologist's Legal Update, 5, 3-16.
Schooler, J. W. (1994). Seeking the core: The issues and evidence surrounding recovered
accounts of sexual trauma. Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 452-469.
Schooler, J. W., Gerhard, D., & Loftus, E. F. (1986). Qualities of the unreal. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 171-181.
Schooler, J. W., & Loftus, E. F. (1993). Multiple mechanisms mediate individual differences in
eyewitness accuracy and suggestibility. In J. M. Puckett & H. W. Reese (Eds.),
Mechanisms of everyday cognition (pp. 177-203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schreiber, F. R. (1973). Sybil. New York: Warner.
Schutte, J. (1994, March). Influence of ethnicity and authoritarianism on juror reactions to
repressed memory lawsuits. Paper presented at meetings of the American Psychology-Law
Society, Santa Fe, NM.
Sechrest, L. (1994, December). Educational issues in psychology. Abstract of paper presented
at the Memory and Reality: Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False Memory
Syndrome Foundation and Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Shaffer, R. E., & Cozolino, L. J. (1992). Psychotherapy in SRA/MPD patients. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 20, 188-193.
Sheiman, J. A. (1993). "I've always wondered if something happened to me:" Assessment of
child sexual abuse survivors with amnesia. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 2, 13-21.
Shweder, R. A. (1977). Likeness and likelihood in everyday thought: Magical thinking in
judgments about personality. Current Anthropology, 18, 637-658.
906 LINDSAY AND READ

Siegel, D. J. (in press). Memory, trauma and psychotherapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Practice
and Research 4, 93-122.
Siegel, D. R., & Romig, C. A. (1990). Memory retrieval in treating adult survivors of sexual
abuse. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 18, 246-256.
Silver, S. M. (1986). An in-patient program for post-traumatic-stress disorder: Context as
treatment. In C. R. Figley (Ed.), Trauma and its wake: Vol. 2. Traumatic stress theory,
research, and intervention (pp. 213-231). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Simmons, P. S. (1994). Lawyers and memory: The impact of repressed memory allegations of
abuse on the American courtroom. Issues in Child Abuse Accusations, 6, 163-175.
Simpson, S., & Baker, J. K. (1994). Causes of action against health care providers by retractors
of abuse allegations. Shepard's Expert and Scientific Evidence Quarterly, 2, 1-31.
Singer, J. L. (Ed.). (1990). Repression and dissociation: Implications for personality theory,
psychopathology, and health. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Slovenko, R. (1994, December). Duty of care to third persons. Paper presented at the Memory
and Reality: Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False Memory Syndrome
Foundation and Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
Smith, M., & Pazder, L. (1980). Michelle remembers. New York: Pocket Books.
Smith, S. (1991). Survey of 38 therapists specializing in sexual abuse recovery. Unpublished
master's thesis, Ottawa University, Phoenix, AZ.
Sonnenberg, S. M., Blank, A. S., & Talbott, J. A. (1985). The trauma of war: Stress and recovery
in Viet Nam veterans. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Spanos, N. P. (1992). Compliance and reinterpretation in hypnotic responding. Contemporary
Hypnosis, 9, 7-15.
Spanos, N. P. (1994). Multiple identity enactments and multiple personality disorder: A
sociocognitive perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 143-165.
Spanos, N. P., Burgess, C. A., & Burgess, M. F. (1994). Past-life identities, UFO abductions,
and satanic ritual abuse: The social construction of memories. The International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 433-^46.
Spear, N. E. (1979). Experimental analysis of infantile amnesia. In J. F. Kihlstrom & F. J.
Evans (Eds.), Functional disorders of memory (pp. 75-102). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Spiegel, D., & Scheflin, A. W. (1994). Dissociated or fabricated? Psychiatric aspects of
repressed memory in criminal and civil cases. The International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 411^32.
Squire, L. R. (1994, November). Memory systems of the brain. Paper presented at the Memory
and Reality: Reconciliation Conference sponsored by the False Memory Syndrome
Foundation and Johns Hopkins Medical Institutions, Baltimore, MD.
State of New Hampshire v. Joel Hungerford, No. 94-S-045 (NH Sup. Ct. filed May 23,1995).
State of New Hampshire v. John Morahan, No. 93-S-1734 (NH Sup. Ct. filed May 23,1995).
Steblay, N. M., & Bothwell, R. K. (in press). Evidence for hypnotically refreshed testimony:
The view from the laboratory. Law and Human Behavior.
Stein, D. M., & Lambert, M. J. (1995). Graduate training in psychotherapy: Are therapy
outcomes enhanced! Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 182-196.
Stewart, W. F., & Young, R. (1992). The rehabilitation of the child sexual abuse accommoda-
tion syndrome in trial courts in Kentucky. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 1, 133-141.
Stinson, M. H., & Hendrick, S. S. (1992). Reported childhood sexual abuse in university
counseling center clients. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 370-374.
Stuber, M. L., Nader, K., Yasuda, P., Pynoos, R. S., & Cohen, S. (1991). Stress responses after
pediatric bone marrow transplantation: Preliminary results of a prospective longitudinal
study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 952-957.
Summit, R. C. (1983). The child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome. Child Abuse and
Neglect, 7, 177-193.
Summit, R. C. (1992a). Abuse of the child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome. Journal of
Child Sexual Abuse, 1, 153-163.
RECOVERED MEMORIES 907

Summit, R. C. (1992b). The rehabilitation of the child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome
in trial courts in Kentucky: Commentary. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 1, 147-151.
Sutker, P. B., & Adams, H. E. (1993). Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology (2nd ed.).
New York: Plenum Press.
Tabin, J. K. (1993). Freud's shift from the seduction theory: Some overlooked reality factors.
Psychoanalytic Psychology, 10, 291-297.
Tarasoff v. University of California, 17 Cal.3d 425,551 P.2d 334,131 Cal Rptr. 14 (1976).
Terr, L. (1988). What happens to early memories of trauma? A study of 20 children under age
5 at time of documented traumatic events. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 96-104.
Terr, L. (1991). Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. American Journal of Psychiatry,
148, 10-20.
Terr, L. (1993, December). Testimony at Akiki trial. FMS Foundation Newsletter, p. 5.
Terr, L. (1994). Unchained memories: True stories of traumatic memories, lost and found. New
York: Basic Books.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgments under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.
Science, 185, 1124-1131.
United States v. Frye, 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir. 1923).
United States v. Telfaire, 469 F.2d 552 (D.C. Cir. 1972).
Usher, J. A., & Neisser, U. (1993). Childhood amnesia and the beginnings of memory for four
early life events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 155-165.
van der Kolk, B. (1988). The trauma spectrum: The interaction of biological and social events
in the genesis of the trauma response. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 1, 273-290.
Vokey, J. R., & Allen, S. W. (1995). Process dissociations of directed forgetting: Recognized and
unrecognized retrieval. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Wagenaar, W. A. (1988). Identifying Ivan. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wagenaar, W. A., & Groeneweg, J. (1990). The memory of concentration camp survivors.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4, 77-87.
Wagstaff, G. F., Vella, M., & Perfect, T. (1992). The effect of hypnotically elicited testimony
on jurors'judgments of guilt and innocence. Journal of Social Psychology, 132, 591-595.
Wakefield, H., & Underwager, R. (1992). Recovered memories of alleged sexual abuse:
Lawsuits against parents. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 10, 483-507.
Walker, L. E. (1992). When an incest survivor sues her father: A commentary. Journal of Child
Sexual Abuse, 1, 125-127.
Walker, L. E. (1994). Abused women and survivor therapy. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Williams, L. M. (1994a). Recall of childhood trauma: A prospective study of women's
memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62,
1167-1176.
Williams, L. M. (1994b). What does it mean to forget child sexual abuse? A reply to Loftus,
Garry, and Feldman. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1182-1186.
Wilson, J. P., Harel, Z., & Kahana, B. (Eds.). (1988). Human adaptation to extreme stress: From
the Holocaust to Vietnam. New York: Plenum Press.
Winograd, E., & Neisser, U. (Eds.). (1992). Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of "flashbulb"
memories (Emory Symposium on Cognition). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Wolfner, G. D., & Gelles, R. J. (1993). A profile of violence toward children: A national study.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 197-212.
Wyatt, G. E. (1985). The sexual abuse of Afro-American and White-American women in
childhood. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9, 507-519.
Wylie, M. S. (1993, September/October). The shadow of a doubt. Networker, pp. 18-29, 70, 73.
Yapko, M. (1993, September/October). The seductions of memory. Networker, pp. 31-37.
Yapko, M. (1994). Suggestions of abuse: Real and imagined memories. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
908 LINDSAY AND READ
Yuille, J. C., & Tollestrup, P. A. (1992). A model of diverse effects of emotion on eyewitness
memory. In S.-A. Christianson (Ed.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and
theory (pp. 201-216). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zaragoza, M. S., & Lane, S. (1994). Source misattributions and the suggestibility of eyewitness
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20,
934-945.

Received May 15,1995


Revision received August 23, 1995
Accepted September 15, 1995 •

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen