Sie sind auf Seite 1von 15

Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Children and Youth Services Review


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/childyouth

Preschool teachers’ transition practices and parents’ perceptions as T


predictors of involvement and children’s school readiness
Jaime Puccionia, , John Mark Froilandb, Mariola Moeyaerta

a
University at Albany, United States
b
Purdue University, United States

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Preschool teachers can help support families and children successfully transition to kindergarten by utilizing
Transition practices transition practices that support parental involvement and create smooth linkages between early care settings
Parental perceptions and formal schooling. Additionally, parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement may also play an im-
Parental involvement portant role in shaping parental involvement. Using nationally representative data from the Early Childhood
Academic achievement
Longitudinal Study – Birth cohort (N ≈ 4100) a structural equation model was estimated to examine the re-
Socio-emotional
School readiness
lationship among preschool teachers’ transition practices, parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement,
parental involvement, and measures of school readiness during the transition to kindergarten. Results indicate
that parent’s perceptions of invitations for involvement are positively associated with home-based parental in-
volvement. Additionally, home-based involvement was predictive of school readiness measures. More specifi-
cally, findings suggest that parents who have more positive perceptions about preschool teachers’ invitations for
involvement report engaging in more home-based involvement activities, which in turn, was positively asso-
ciated with children’s academic achievement and prosocial behaviors and negatively associated with conduct
problems and hyperactivity/inattention. Results from this study have important practical implications for
educators who aim to encourage parental involvement that supports children’s transition to elementary school.

1. Introduction practices to improved outcomes for children in kindergarten (Cook &


Coley, 2017; Galindo & Sheldon, 2012; Jung & Han, 2013; Schulting,
Early educators can play an important role in supporting children’s Malone, & Dodge, 2005), few studies have explored the link between
readiness for school by reaching out to parents to encourage parental preschool teachers’ transition practices and kindergarten outcomes (see
involvement that supports children’s transition to kindergarten (Pianta, Ahtola et al., 2011; LoCasale-Crouch, Mashburn, Downer, & Pianta,
Cox, Taylor, & Early, 1999). The most common types of outreach efforts 2008 for exception), and no studies to date, have examined the asso-
during the transition to kindergarten, which are commonly referred to ciations among preschool teachers’ transition practices, parental per-
as transition practices, include sending information about kindergarten ceptions about invitations for involvement, parental involvement, and
home to parents and having an orientation, while the least frequent children’s readiness for school.
activities include having preschool age children spend time in the Entering kindergarten is a critical transition in children’s lives, a
kindergarten classroom and conducting home visit (Cook & Coley, time in which early academic achievement and socio-emotional com-
2017; Little, Cohen-Vogel, & Curran, 2016). Hoover-Dempsey et al. petencies lays the foundation for later learning and development
(2005) suggest that parents’ perceptions of educators’ invitations for (Claessens & Engel, 2013; Claessens, Duncan, & Engel, 2009; Sabol &
involvement also have the potential to shape parental involvement, Pianta, 2012). Given the importance of school readiness and the tran-
which in turn influences children’s outcomes. In fact, a few studies have sition to kindergarten, it is important to understand the ways in which
demonstrated that parents of elementary school age children who have preschool teachers’ transition practices can support families and chil-
positive perceptions about teachers’ invitations for involvement report dren. This study utilizes data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal
engaging in more home and school-based involvement activities Study – Birth cohort (ECLS-B) to estimate a structural equation model
(Anderson & Minke, 2007; Walker, Ice, Hoover-Dempsey, & Sandler, that examines the associations among preschool teachers’ transition
2011). Although studies have linked kindergarten teachers’ transition practices, parental perceptions and involvement, and measures of


Corresponding author at: Department of Literacy Teaching and Learning, University at Albany, Albany, NY 12222, United States.
E-mail address: jpuccioni@albany.edu (J. Puccioni).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104742
Received 14 March 2019; Received in revised form 30 December 2019; Accepted 31 December 2019
Available online 03 January 2020
0190-7409/ © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

school readiness. Findings from this study have the potential to broaden prosocial behaviors.
our understanding of children’s early educational opportunities and Studies have also shown that parental involvement at school can
subsequent outcomes during the transition to elementary school. More have a positive impact on children’s academic achievement, however
specifically, findings have the potential to demonstrate the degree to the evidence is mixed. Several studies have demonstrated that school-
which preschool teachers’ transition practices and parents’ perceptions based involvement, in the form of attending school meetings, vo-
about educators’ invitations for involvement shape parental involve- lunteering in classrooms, and serving on committees, are positively
ment, which in turn, has the potential to positively shape children’s associated with children’s achievement (Galindo & Sheldon, 2012; Hill
academic and socio-emotional outcomes at the onset of kindergarten. & Craft, 2003; Hill, 2001). While other studies have demonstrated a
negative (Desimone, 1999; Fan, 2001), or no association (Domina,
1.1. Parental involvement and school readiness 2005; El Nokali, Bachman, & Votruba-Drzal, 2010), between school-
based involvement activities and achievement. For instance, Powell,
A wealth of scholarship shows that parental involvement during Son, File, and Froiland (2012) found that school-based involvement was
early childhood has a strong positive influence on a range of child not related to math achievement, whereas home-based involvement
outcomes during the transition to kindergarten (Booth & Crouter, 2008; predicted math achievement growth from preschool to first grade.
Huat See & Gorard, 2015; Van Voorhis, Mailer, Epstein, & Lloyd, 2013). Jeynes (2005) conducted a meta-analysis to examine the effects of
Van Voorhis et al. (2013) review of over 95 studies provide a clear link parental involvement on urban elementary school students and found
between parents’ engagement in cognitively stimulating literacy and that attending school functions did not yield statistically significant
mathematics activities and young children’s early learning and socio- effects on academic achievement. While Puccioni (2018) found that
emotional development. Research has shown that literacy-related ac- parents’ school-based involvement during preschool was negatively
tivities like reading to your child (Mol, Bus, & de Jong, 2009), singing associated with children’s reading achievement at the beginning of
songs (Dunst, Meter, & Hamby, 2011), telling stories, and making books kindergarten. Due to the inconsistent findings related to the impact of
(Boyce, Innocenti, Roggman, Norman, & Ortiz, 2010) support children’s school-based involvement on children’s achievement and the lack of
early literacy learning and development. studies examining the degree to which parents’ school-based involve-
Home-based parental involvement has also been shown to support ment during preschool shapes later achievement, additional research is
children’s mathematical learning (Froiland, Peterson, & Davison, 2013; warranted.
Kleemans, Peeters, Segers, & Verhoeven, 2012; Sonnenschein & Given the important role parents play in contributing to children’s
Galindo, 2015). More specifically, children whose parents engage their early learning and transition to school (Booth & Crouter, 2008), it is
young child in more numeracy-related activities tend to have higher important to recognize that parental involvement may vary by one’s
levels of early numeracy skills (LeFevre et al., 2009; LeFevre, Clarke, & race/ethnicity and socioeconomic status (Cheadle & Amato, 2011;
Stringer, 2002; Van Voorhis et al., 2013). Research has shown that Phillips, 2011; Puccioni, 2018). Research has shown that children of
parent-child activities such as counting or identifying shapes (Blevins- color and those from lower socioeconomic backgrounds spent less time
Knabe & Musun-Miller, 1996) and playing board games (Ramani & engaged in language, literacy, and education-related activities with
Siegler, 2008) positively influence children’s early mathematical de- their parent (Phillips, 2011) and children from lower income families
velopment. Interestingly, the home literacy environment has also been engage in fewer numeracy-related activities in their home environment
shown to predict the acquisition of early numeracy skills (Manolitsis, than their middle-income peers (Blevins-Knabe & Musun-Miller, 1996;
Georgiou, & Tziraki, 2013; Sonnenschein & Galindo, 2015) and later Vandermaas-Peeler, Nelson, Bumpass, & Sassine, 2009). Cheadle and
mathematics development (Froiland et al., 2013). Overall meta-ana- Amato (2011) developed a latent construct representing concerted
lyses have demonstrated that parental involvement and more specifi- cultivation which was comprised of school-based involvement, extra-
cally literacy-related activities have a positive impact on academic curricular activities, and learning related materials in the home and
achievement (Castro et al., 2015; Mol et al., 2009). found that parents of color and parents from lower SES backgrounds
Home-based parental involvement also predicts other important engaged in concerted cultivation less than their white counterparts and
school readiness skills, such as better social skills, reduced externalizing those from higher socioeconomic backgrounds. Similar patterns emerge
behaviors, and self-regulation of attention (Puccioni, Baker, & Froiland, with regards to school-based involvement, such that children of color
2019). Importantly, this nationally representative study entailed rig- and those living in poverty are less likely to have parents who attend
orous control variables, such as SES, gender, ethnicity, and age general meetings, school events, or volunteer their time in the school or
(Puccioni et al., 2019), whereas most related studies have included classroom (Trends, 2013). In order to isolate the relations among pre-
smaller samples and limited control variables. For instance, in a study school teachers’ transition practices, parents’ perceptions of invitations
of 144 Head Start students (96% African American), Fantuzzo, for involvement, and parental involvement it is important to include
McWayne, Perry, and Childs (2004) found that home-based parent in- child and family background characteristics as controls in the analysis.
volvement was positively associated with attention and receptive vo-
cabulary, while negatively associated with conduct problems and hy- 1.2. Early educators’ transition practices
peractivity. However, these effects were established without employing
any control variables. Importantly, school-based involvement was not Early educators can support children during the transition to kin-
significantly associated with any of these outcomes, but it is important dergarten by reaching out to parents in ways that encourage and sup-
to examine this with larger and more diverse samples. port family involvement. These transition practices, which may also be
Home-based parent involvement may provide children with practice conceptualized as invitations for involvement have the potential to
in self‐regulatory behaviors that help in school, such as sitting still and serve as a bridge for children and families during this important edu-
paying attention (Froiland & Davison, 2016; Son & Tineo, 2016). cational transition. The most common transition practices include in-
Froiland and Davison (2016) found that parent home-based involve- viting parents and children to kindergarten orientations and sending
ment in the form of parent-child shared reading frequency, number of information about kindergarten to parents, while the least common
books at home, and visits to the library put children at a lower risk for practices consist of conducting home visits prior to the start of kin-
developing inattention and hyperactivity. This nationally re- dergarten and coordinating efforts with preschool programs (Cook &
presentative study involved rigorous control variables, such as SES, Coley, 2017; Pianta et al., 1999; Schulting et al., 2005). Several studies
ethnicity, gender, diagnosis of autism or emotional disturbance, and have linked kindergarten teachers’ transition practices to children’s
family structure. However, it did not examine other key social-emo- outcomes during the transition to elementary school using nationally
tional school readiness outcomes, such as conduct problems and representative data drawn from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study

2
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

– Kindergarten cohort 1998–1999 (ECLS-K). For example, Schulting elementary age children. Murray, McFarland-Piazza, and Harrison
et al. (2005) demonstrated that more transition practices implemented (2015) found that parents’ satisfaction with information positively
in the fall of kindergarten was positively associated with children’s predicted their home-based involvement during the transition to formal
academic achievement at the end of kindergarten and this relationship schooling. The current study extends previous research by linking
was stronger for children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds. preschool teachers’ transition practices parents’ perceptions of invita-
Moreover, the authors demonstrated that school-based parental in- tions for involvement, parental involvement, and children’s readiness
volvement partially mediated the relationship between transition for school.
practices and children’s outcomes. Building upon the work by Schulting Despite the fact that many educators, researchers, and policymakers
et al. (2005), Galindo and Sheldon (2012) demonstrated that kinder- alike advocate for the implementation of outreach efforts, studies have
garten teachers’ ongoing outreach efforts during the kindergarten shown that schools and teachers serving historically disadvantaged
school year were positively associated with achievement gains in kin- student populations report using fewer transition practices (Daley,
dergarten and this relationship was also mediated by school-based Munk, & Carlson, 2011; Little et al., 2016; Pianta et al., 1999). More
parental involvement. Jung and Han (2013) found that kindergarten specifically, research has shown that elementary schools in urban lo-
teachers’ outreach efforts in the fall were positively associated with cations and those serving a larger population of students living in
sharper gains in reading achievement, and these gains were greater for poverty (Daley et al., 2011; Little et al., 2016; Pianta et al., 1999), as
children who entered kindergarten with lower reading levels and for well as those schools serving larger populations of students of color
those who read more outside of school. More recently, Cook and Coley (Little et al., 2016; Pianta et al., 1999) utilize fewer transition practices.
(2017) analyzed data from ECLS-B and found that kindergarten tea- Additionally, research suggests that kindergarten teachers with more
chers’ transition practices were positively associated with children’s years of experience report engaging in more overall transition practices
prosocial behaviors but not academic achievement. Overall, these stu- (Cook & Coley, 2017) and those with primary certification report en-
dies demonstrate a link between kindergarten teachers’ transition gaging in more individualized practices (Early, Pianta, Taylor, & Cox,
practices and child outcomes during kindergarten. Some findings in- 2001). Overall, research suggests that kindergarten students from dis-
dicate that this link is mediated by parental involvement and that advantaged backgrounds may be exposed to fewer outreach efforts than
children from disadvantaged backgrounds may benefit more from kin- children from more advantaged backgrounds (Cook & Coley, 2017;
dergarten teachers’ transition practices. Little et al., 2016; Pianta et al., 1999), yet few studies explore the types
Although most studies have focused on kindergarten teachers’ or frequency of transition practices preschool teachers utilize and no
transition practices, a few studies have examined preschool teachers’ studies to this date examine parents’ perceptions about preschool tea-
outreach efforts and children’s achievement in elementary school. For chers’ transition practices.
example, study by LoCasale-Crouch et al. (2008) examined the asso- Taken together, these studies demonstrate that educators’ outreach
ciation between pre-kindergarten teachers’ transition practices and efforts are positively associated with child outcomes during the tran-
kindergarten children’s academic and socio-emotional outcomes using a sition to formal schooling. Although some of these studies have de-
sample of approximately 700 children participating in the National monstrated that the relationship between early educators’ outreach
Center for Early Development and Learning Multi-State Pre-kinder- efforts are partially-mediated by school-based parental involvement
garten study. The authors reported that the total number of practices, and moderated by children’s socioeconomic status and children’s be-
and more specifically, practices that children experienced directly were ginning levels of achievement, no studies to date, have simultaneously
positively linked to kindergarten teachers’ perceptions of children’s examined the relationship among preschool teachers’ transition prac-
academic achievement and socio-emotional competencies. Further- tices, parental involvement, and measures of school readiness during
more, these positive associations were stronger for children who were the transition to elementary school. This study contributes to the lit-
from economically disadvantaged backgrounds. In a study of approxi- erature by using nationally representative data drawn from ECLS-B to
mately 400 children transitioning to formal schooling in Finland con- estimate a structural equation model that examines the associations
ducted by Ahtola et al. (2011), pairs of preschool and elementary school among preschool teachers’ transition practices, parents’ perceptions of
teachers provided reports of their collaborative outreach efforts in- educators’ invitations for involvement, parental involvement, and
tended to support children’s transition to formal schooling. The authors children’s academic and socio-emotional competencies during the
found that the more transition practices the pairs implemented the transition to kindergarten while accounting for potential variation in
greater children’s growth in children’s reading, writing, and mathe- preschool teachers’ transition practices as well as parental perceptions
matics skills. Even though these studies demonstrate a link between and involvement.
preschool educators’ transition practices and child outcomes during the
transition to kindergarten, they do not examine the mediating role of
parental involvement. 1.3. Control variables for measures of school readiness
Although transition practices have been positively linked to par-
ental involvement and children’s outcomes during the transition to In order to isolate the associations among preschool teachers’
kindergarten by way of parental involvement (Galindo & Sheldon, transition practices, parents’ perceptions of educators’ invitations for
2012; Schulting et al., 2005) scholars suggest that parents’ perceptions involvement, parental involvement, and children’s readiness for school
about educators’ invitations for involvement also have the potential to it is important to account for a variety of control variables that have
shape parental involvement and child outcomes. Invitations for in- been shown to be associated with measures of school readiness.
volvement may be conceptualized as outreach efforts to support and Research suggests that socioeconomic and racial/ethnic gaps in aca-
encourage parental involvement. A few studies have examined the re- demic (Coley, 2002; Rouse, Brooks-Gunn, & Mclanahan, 2005) and
lation between parents’ perceptions of teachers’ invitations for in- socio-emotional competencies (Baker, Jensen, & Tisak, 2017; Martin
volvement and parental involvement. For example, in a study with over et al., 2010; Weimer & Guajardo, 2005) exist when children start kin-
850 parents of children attending an urban elementary school, dergarten. Studies have also demonstrated that children’s gender
Anderson and Minke (2007) found that parents who held more positive (Ready, LoGerfo, Lee, & Burkam, 2005) and age (Datar, 2006; Lin,
perceptions of elementary school teachers’ invitations for involvement Freeman, & Chu, 2009) are also associated with measures of school
reported engaging in more home and school-based involvement prac- readiness. Our analysis includes a rich set of controls to help disen-
tices. While Walker et al. (2011) found that Latino parents’ who held tangle the associations among transition practices, parental perceptions
more positive perceptions of teachers’ invitations for involvement re- and involvement, and measures of school readiness.
ported engaging in more school-based involvement practices with their

3
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

1.4. Research questions and hypotheses 2 years of age), wave 3 (preschool), and waves 4 and 5 representing the
year in which the sampled child entered kindergarten. The analytical
The present study draws upon nationally representative data from sample was selected based on several steps. First, children with a valid
ECLS-B to estimate a structural equation model which examines the non-zero panel weight (WKR0), which is a specific weight designed for
relationship among preschool teachers’ transition practices, parents’ longitudinal analyses using the sample of children from wave 2
perceptions of educators’ invitations for involvement, parental in- (24 months), wave 3 (preschool), and waves 4 and 5 (kindergarten
volvement, and measures of school readiness. The following research waves) were selected. Second, because this study examines the role of
questions and hypotheses guide the present study: school-based parental involvement, children who only experienced
parental care during preschool were excluded from the study. This
1. Are preschool teachers’ transition practices and parents’ perceptions process yielded an approximate sample of 3650 children. All sample
of invitations for involvement associated with measures of school sizes reported in this analysis are rounded to the nearest 50 in ac-
readiness? Few studies have demonstrated a link between preschool cordance with IES reporting rules to account for the restricted nature of
teachers’ transition practices and children’s kindergarten achieve- ECLS-B data.
ment, and no studies to date have examined the link between par-
ents’ perceptions of preschool teachers’ invitations for involvement 2.2. Academic achievement measures
and measures of school readiness. Therefore, analyses are somewhat
exploratory in nature. The ECLS-B includes direct cognitive assessments specifically de-
2. Is home and school-based parental involvement associated with signed to measure children’s early reading and mathematical skills. This
measures of school readiness? We expect home and school-based study makes use of individually administered, oral, and untimed
involvement to be associated with measures of school readiness. reading and mathematics assessments administered in the fall of kin-
3. Are preschool teachers’ transition practices and parents’ perceptions dergarten (2006 or 2007 waves) to develop a latent construct re-
of invitations for involvement associated with parental involve- presenting a global measure of academic achievement.
ment? We expect preschool teachers’ transition practices to be as-
sociated with parental involvement. Since there are no previous
2.2.1. Early reading achievement
studies examining the relations between preschool parents’ percep-
The early reading assessment was intended to measure children’s
tions of early educators’ invitations for involvement and parental
performance in language and literacy-related domains. Therefore, the
involvement, our analyses are somewhat exploratory in nature.
assessment contained items measuring children’s performance on cer-
tain language-based items (receptive language and Peabody Picture
2. Method
Vocabulary Test items) and literacy items (e.g., conventions of print,
letter recognition, understanding of letter-sound relationships, phono-
2.1. Data SOURCE and sample
logical awareness, sight word recognition, understanding words in the
context of simple sentences). The reliability estimate for the kinder-
This study draws upon data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal
garten reading assessment was 0.84 (Bethel, Green, Kalton, & Nord,
Study – Birth cohort (ECLS-B), which is a longitudinal study that fol-
2005; Snow et al., 2009).
lows a nationally representative sample of children born in the U.S.
from birth to kindergarten (Snow et al., 2009). Developed primarily
2.2.2. Early mathematics achievement
under the sponsorship of the U.S. Department of Education, National
The mathematics assessment included items assessing number
Center for Education Statistics, the ECLS-B is a multiple-method, mul-
sense, properties, and operations; measurement; geometry and spatial
tiple-respondent study guided by a conceptual model of children’s de-
sense; data analysis, statistics, and probability; and patterns, algebra,
velopment that emphasizes the interaction between child, family,
and functions. The reliability estimate for the kindergarten mathe-
school, and community. Data for this study was collected in five waves
matics assessments was 0.89 (Bethel et al., 2005; Snow et al., 2009).
beginning when children were approximately 9 months of age. The
second wave of data collection occurred when children were approxi-
mately 24 months of age. The preschool wave (wave 3) of data col- 2.3. Social-emotional outcomes
lection was intended to represent the year before most of the children
would be expected to enter kindergarten. There are two waves of data The ECLS-B includes an indirect child assessment which is intended
representing when children began kindergarten based on age-elig- to measure social-emotional constructs. The assessment draws upon
ibility, that is, the kindergarten entry in 2006 (wave 4) or kindergarten items from the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales – Second
entry in 2007 (wave 5). Based upon recommendations from the ECLS-B Edition (PKBS-2) (Merrell, 2003) and the Social Skills Rating System
staff, outcome measures from waves 4 and 5 were pooled in order to (Gresham & Elliott, 1990). Guided by previous research by Ferretti and
measure children’s academic achievement and socio-emotional com- Bub (2017) we drew upon kindergarten teacher responses in the fall of
petencies in kindergarten. More specifically, we used the wave 4 kin- kindergarten to develop three latent constructs to measure children’s
dergarten data for children who were eligible to begin kindergarten conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and prosocial skills.
that year; and for those who were not eligible to enter in 2006, we
utilized the data from 2007. Initial kindergarten scores were used for 2.3.1. Conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention
any child who repeated kindergarten and was assessed more than once. The latent constructs representing conduct problems and hyper-
ECLS-B includes data from parents, early childcare providers, kinder- activity consists of indicator variables measuring the frequency the
garten teachers, as well as child assessment instruments in cognitive child displayed certain behaviors using a scale from 1 (never) to 5
and psychosocial domains. As such, the ECLS-B data set allows one to (often). The construct representing conduct problems consists of the
analyze the complex relationships between children’s early learning following items: child has temper tantrums; child annoys other chil-
experiences across home and school contexts to a variety of child out- dren; child has disruptive behavior; and child displays aggression. The
comes. Additional information about the sampling design, instruments, construct representing hyperactivity consisted of the following items:
and measures can be found in the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, child displays impulsive behavior; child is over active; child keeps
Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), Kindergarten 2006 and 2007 Data File User’s working until task finished (reverse coded); child pays attention well
Manual (2010–010) (Snow et al., 2009). (reverse coded); child has difficulty concentrating or staying on task;
This study makes use of data drawn from wave 2 (approximately and child is restless and fidgety.

4
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

2.3.2. Prosocial behaviors comprised of: (a) father/male guardian’s level of education; (b) mo-
The latent constructs representing prosocial behaviors consists of ther/female guardian’s level of education; (c) father/male guardian’s
indicator variables measuring the frequency the child displayed the occupation; (d) mother/female guardian’s occupation; and (e) house-
following behaviors using a scale from 1 (never) to 5 (often): child is hold income. There were categories of race/ethnicity included in the
invited by other children to play; child shows eagerness to learn new analysis: European American/non-Hispanic (reference group), African
things; child is accepted and liked by other children; child comforts American/non-Hispanic, Hispanic, Asian American, and Other. In ad-
other children who are upset; child works independently; child stands dition to socioeconomic status and race/ethnicity, children’s age at time
up for other’s rights; child tries to understand others; and child shares. of kindergarten assessment which (measured in months), and gender
which was represented by a dummy variable indicating whether the
2.4. Preschool teachers’ transition practices child was female were included as controls.

Three items were used from the preschool wave of data collection to 2.9. Preschool measures of academic achievement and socio-emotional
measure preschool teachers’ transition practices. Preschool teachers outcomes as controls for school readiness
reported whether or not the following practices (1 = yes, 0 = no) were
utilized: phoned or sent home information about the kindergarten This study also estimated a set of latent constructs to represent
program to parents; preschoolers spend some time in the kindergarten children’s preschool level of academic achievement and socio-emo-
classroom; and parents and children visit kindergarten prior to the start tional competencies as controls for subsequent measures of school
of the school year. These items were summed and scores ranged from readiness. The preschool wave of direct child assessments measuring
zero to three. children’s early reading and mathematics were used as manifest vari-
ables for the latent construct representing preschool children’s aca-
2.5. Parents’ perceptions of educators’ invitations for involvement demic achievement. The preschool wave of data collection also in-
cluded the same measures used in the kindergarten waves to assess
A latent construct representing parents’ perceptions of preschool children’s socio-emotional competencies. Drawing upon preschool tea-
teachers’ invitations for involvement was comprised of five items from cher responses about the frequency in which children displayed certain
the preschool wave of data collection. Using a scale of 1 = does it very social skills and behaviors three latent constructs were developed to
well to 3 = not at all, parents rated how well the preschool teacher measure children’s conduct problems, hyperactivity, and prosocial
provided information about their child’s progress in school; helped you skills.
gain understanding of what children are like at that age; made you
aware of chances to volunteer; provided workshops, materials, or ad- 2.10. Controls to account for potential variation in educators’ transition
vice about how to help child learn at home; and provided information practices, parents’ perceptions and involvement
on community services to help with your child or your family. Items
were reverse coded with values ranging from 0 = does not at all to We included a set of variables measuring preschool teacher char-
2 = does it very well. Cronbach’s alpha for the five manifest variables acteristics and type of school setting to account for potential variation
representing parents’ perceptions of preschool teachers’ invitations for in preschool teachers’ transition practices and parent perceptions of
involvement was 0.75 for the subsample. invitations for involvement. Preschool teachers’ reported what type of
educational degree had been obtained in addition to years of experience
2.6. Home-based parental involvement working in early care and education. Dummy codes indicating if the
preschool teacher held an Associate’s degree in child development and/
A latent construct representing home-based parental involvement or a Bachelor’s degree in early education were included as controls. A
was estimated using four items from the preschool wave parent inter- set of dummy codes indicating type of preschool setting using the fol-
views. Using a scale of 1 = not at all to 4 = every day, parents reported lowing categories: public prekindergarten (reference category), private
how often they read books, tell stories, and sing songs with their child prekindergarten, center-based prekindergarten, Head Start, and other
during the week. These items were recoded so that values ranged from type prekindergarten.
0 = not at all to 3 = every day. Parents were also asked how often they In addition to preschool teacher characteristics and type of pre-
played together with toys to build things using a scale of 1 = more than school, we also included children’s race/ethnicity and family socio-
once a day to 6 = not at all. This item was reverse coded with values economic status as a control for transition practices and parents’ per-
ranging from 0 = not at all to 5 = more than once a day, so that higher ceptions. We also included a latent variable representing parental
numbers indicate more home-based parental involvement. Cronbach’s involvement at approximately 24 months of age to account for potential
alpha for the four home-based parental involvement items was 0.60 for variation parents’ perceptions of teachers’ invitations for involvement.
the subsample. During the second wave of data collection parents reported how often
they read books, tell stories, and sing songs with their child during the
2.7. School-based parental involvement week using a scale of 1 = not at all to 4 = every day. These items were
recoded so values ranged from 0 = not at all to 3 = every day. Parents
Five items were used from the preschool wave of data collection to also reported on how often they play indoor games using a scale of
measure parental school-based involvement. Parents reported whether 1 = more than once a day to 6 = not at all. These items were reverse
or not (1 = yes, 0 = no) they attended a general meeting (e.g., open coded so that 0 = not at all to 5 = more than once a day. In order to
house, back-to-school night); a regularly scheduled parent-teacher account for variation in preschool parental involvement we included
conference; a school or class event (e.g., play); acted as a volunteer; or the child’s race/ethnicity, family socioeconomic status, gender, and
chaperoned a field trip. These items were summed to create composite parental involvement at 24 months as controls for home and school-
variable with a range of zero to five. based involvement.

2.8. Family and child-related controls for measures of school readiness 2.11. Analytical plan

This study included a set of family and child background char- In order to account for the complex survey design employed by
acteristics as controls for the analyses. Socioeconomic status was a ECLS-B all analyses in this study used the paired jackknife replication
continuous composite variable provided in the ECLS-B data set that was method (JK2) by specifying the full sample weight (WKR0) and the

5
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

corresponding replicate weights (WKR1-WKR90). This process adjusts involvement, home-based parental involvement, and measures of
for disproportionate sampling, survey nonresponse, and undercoverage school readiness which include academic achievement and socio-emo-
of the target population when analyzing complex survey data. Weighted tional outcomes. The next step involved estimating the full structural
means and proportions of model variables were estimated in Stata 13 model which included a set of family and child characteristics and
(StataCorp., 2013) using the svyset command. Use of the appropriate teacher characteristics as controls for transition practices, parents’
weight allows findings to be generalized to the national population of perceptions and involvement, and measures of school readiness (Fig. 2
children born in the United States in 2001 (Snow et al., 2009). does not display controls). Resulting fit statistics indicate good fit for
We investigated the missingness mechanism in this multivariate the data, with a RMSEA = 0.06 (90% CI [0.055–0.057], and a
setting using the R Package maniar (Tierney et al., 2019) and are able to SRMR = 0.07. Therefore, the structural model was retained and
assume that the patterns of missingess are arbitrary. Only for the kin- parameter estimates were evaluated as they pertained to the research
dergartner variables, a small proportion of missing data for a subset of questions addressed by this study.
variables across participants with different background characteristics
was identified. However, formal tests of the missing at random as-
sumption versus the missing completely at random assumption were 3.3. Associations among preschool teachers’ transition practices, parents’
conducted using the littleMCAR function within the BaylorEdPsych perceptions of invitations for involvement, parental involvement, and
package (Beaujean, 2015) and the mice package (van Buuren & measures of school readiness
Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2011). Although missing completely at random
could not be assumed for the kindergartner variables, we can conclude Table 3 presents the unstandardized coefficients of model estimates.
missing at random for these variables (which is commonly the case in For ease of interpretation Fig. 2 presents the standardized model esti-
survey data). For the other variables, missing completely at random can mates as related to the primary research questions under study. The
be assumed. As a consequence nonresponse can be ignored (Rubin, unstandardized and standardized regression coefficients can be inter-
1976; van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2011) and no missing data preted in a similar way as a conventional linear regression model.
imputations were necessary. Contrary to hypothesized associations, preschool teachers’ transition
A structural equation model was estimated to examine the re- practices and parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement were
lationship among preschool teachers’ transition practices, parents’ not significantly directly associated with measures of school readiness.
perceptions and involvement, and measures of school readiness using However, in support of hypothesized associations, the results show
raw data analyzed in Mplus version 7 with a mean and covariance significant associations of home-based involvement with academic
structure (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2012). The analytic strategy oc- achievement and socio-emotional outcomes. More specifically, home-
curred in several steps in accordance with recommendations from Kline based parental involvement was positively associated with children’s
(2011). The first step involved estimating the measurement model in academic achievement (ß = 0.06, p < .05), and negatively associated
which measured variables were estimated as indicators of the proposed with conduct problems (ß = −0.10, p < .05) and hyperactivity/in-
latent variables (Fig. 1 displays the primary latent constructs under attention (ß = −0.10, p < .01), holding all else constant. In general,
study and does not include the latent constructs used as controls). After children whose parents report engaging in more home-based involve-
goodness-of-fit for the measurement model was established, the second ment activities had children with higher average levels of academic
step involved estimating the full structural model to estimate the hy- achievement, and lower levels of conduct problems and hyperactivity/
pothesized relations among the variables of interest. Maximum like- inattention. Results also showed that school-based involvement was
lihood estimation (ML), which uses the full information maximum negatively associated with academic achievement (ß = −0.04,
likelihood (FIML) approach to handle missing data, was used as the p < .05). Comparison of standardized estimates in Fig. 2 suggests that
estimator in analyses (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2012). Model fit was home-based involvement is more strongly associated with socio-
primarily evaluated using a two-index strategy recommended by Hu emotional outcomes in comparison to academic achievement.
and Bentler (1999): root mean squared error of approximation Findings also indicate that parents’ perceptions about educators’
(RMSEA) and the standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR). invitations for involvement were positively associated with home
RMSEA less than or equal to 0.06 and a SRMR less than 0.08 indicate (ß = 0.12, p < .01) and school-based involvement (ß = 0.35,
good model fit for the data. p < .001), holding all else constant. In other words, parents who held
more positive beliefs about preschool teachers’ invitations for involve-
3. Results ment reported engaging in more parental involvement activities.
Although there was no direct association between parents’ perceptions
3.1. Descriptive Statistics and measures of school readiness, results show that parents’ perceptions
of invitations for involvement were positively associated with home-
Table 1 presents the weighted means and proportions estimated in based involvement, which in turn was positively associated with mea-
Stata 13 (StataCorp., 2011). The analytic sample consisted of approxi- sures of school readiness. As displayed in Fig. 2 the standardized cov-
mately 3650 children with a racial/ethnic composition of 57% Eur- ariance between transition practices and the latent construct re-
opean American, 15% African American, 21% Hispanic, 3% Asian, and presenting parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement is 0.13
4% Other. Bivariate correlations among primary latent variable in- (p < .001). Although not displayed in Fig. 1, the latent constructs
dicators were also examined and are displayed in Table 2. representing children’s academic achievement were negatively corre-
lated with conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention, and posi-
3.2. Model estimation tively associated with prosocial skills. While the latent construct re-
presenting prosocial behaviors was negatively correlated with the latent
Following the two-step estimation procedure recommended by constructs representing conduct problems and hyperactivity/inatten-
Kline (2011), the measurement model (Fig. 1) was assessed first. Ac- tion. This indicates a reciprocal relationship between achievement and
ceptable model fit was indicted for the overall measurement model with socio-emotional competencies. The model accounted for 67% of the
an RMSEA = 0.06 (90% CI [0.063–0.065]), and a SRMR = 0.06. Fig. 1 variation in academic achievement, 31% of the variation in conduct
includes the standardized factor loadings for the indicator variables for problems, 34% of the variation in hyperactivity/inattention, and 22%
the latent constructs representing parents’ perceptions of invitations for of the variation in prosocial behavior.

6
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

Fig. 1. Measurement model depicting primary latent constructs under study. N ≈ 3650. Model fit statistics, RMSEA = 0.06 (90% CI: [0.063–0.065]); SRMR = 0.06.
K wave represents the pooled kindergarten wave) Standardized factor loadings are all statistically significant at the p < .001 level.
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), 24 mos. (2003–2004),
preschool data collection (2005–2006), Kindergarten 2006 (2006–2007), Kindergarten 2007 (2007–2008) data collections.

3.4. Controls for measures of school readiness, preschool teachers’ Table 3 also indicates that several preschool teacher characteristics
transition practices, parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement, and included as controls in the analyses were associated with use of tran-
parental involvement sition practices. Preschool teachers who hold an Associate’s Degree in
Child Development and/or a Bachelor’s Degree in Early Childhood
Table 3 indicates child and family characteristics were associated degree reported utilizing more transition practices in comparison to
with measures of school readiness. All things being equal, socio- those preschool teachers who did not report holding a respective de-
economic status had positive and significant associations with academic gree. Additionally, preschool teachers who reported having more years
achievement and prosocial skills and a significant negative association of experience also reported utilizing more transition practices. Results
with hyperactivity/inattention. A similar pattern emerged for the re- also indicate transition practices varied in relation to the type of pre-
lations between children’s age at assessment and measures of school school program. In comparison to teachers at public prekindergarten
readiness. As expected, control measures of academic achievement and programs, preschool teachers at center-based programs reported using
socio-emotional outcomes in preschool were significantly associated fewer transition practices, while teachers at Head Start Programs re-
with respective measures in kindergarten. There was evidence that ported utilizing more transition practices. Variation in parents’ per-
children’s academic achievement and socio-emotional outcomes varied ceptions of invitations for involvement also emerged. Parents of chil-
by race/ethnicity. On average, Asian American children had higher dren who had preschool teachers with a degree in early childhood
levels of academic achievement in comparison to European American reported having more positive feelings about invitations for involve-
children, holding all else constant. Kindergarten teachers’ reported that ment. Furthermore, parents’ whose children attended Head Start held
African American children displayed more conduct problems in com- more positive perceptions about preschool teachers’ transition practices
parison to their European American counterparts, while Asian Amer- in comparison to children who did not attend Head Start.
ican displayed fewer conduct problems. Kindergarten teachers also re- There were also a set of child and family characteristics included as
ported that girls displayed fewer conduct problems and less controls for parental involvement during preschool. Other things being
hyperactive/inattentive behavior in comparison to boys. equal, parents from higher socioeconomic status backgrounds reported

7
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

Table 1
Weighted descriptive statistics.
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), 24 mos. (2003–2004),
preschool data collection (2005–2006), Kindergarten 2006 (2006–2007), Kindergarten 2007 (2007–2008) data collections.

Variable Mean or SD Min Max


Proportion

Kindergarten Achievement Indicators


Reading 45.22 14.05 12.84 82.48
Math 44.76 10.00 11.23 69.69

Kindergarten Conduct Problem Indicators


Temper tantrums 0.52 0.88 0 4
Annoys other children 0.85 0.95 0 4
Disruptive 1.22 0.98 0 4
Aggressive 0.53 0.86 0 4

Kindergarten Hyperactivity Indicators


Impulsive 1.17 1.03 0 4
Over active 1.09 1.07 0 4
Finish tasks 1.00 1.00 0 4
Pay attention 1.16 0.99 0 4
Concentrate 1.40 1.13 0 4
Fidget 1.19 1.10 0 4

Kindergarten Prosocial Indicators


Makes friends 3.22 0.83 0 4
Eagerness to learn 3.15 0.91 0 4
Accepted by other children 3.45 0.68 0 4
Communicates with others 2.44 1.04 0 4
Independent 3.07 0.89 0 4
Stands up for others 2.53 1.04 0 4
Understanding of others 2.39 1.03 0 4
Shares 3.21 0.80 0 4

Preschool Teacher Transition Practices


Info home to parents 0.85 0 1
Preschooler sent time in K class 0.50 0 1
Parent/Child visit K class 0.78 0 1
Composite Preschool transition practices 2.13 0.93 0 3

Parent Perception of Invitations for Involvement


Info child’s progress in preschool 1.79 0.46 0 2
Info about child development 1.52 0.68 0 2
Info about volunteering 1.56 0.73 0 2
Info about learning at home 1.34 0.79 0 2
Info about community services 1.08 0.85 0 2

Preschool Home-based Parental involvement


Read books to child 2.13 0.85 0 3
Tell stories 1.66 0.93 0 3
Sing songs 2.22 0.89 0 3
Play with blocks 2.84 1.23 0 5

Preschool School-based Parental Involvement


Attend general meeting 0.76
Attend scheduled conference 0.50
Attend class event 0.43
Acted as a volunteer 0.40
Chaperoned a field trip 0.26
Composite school-based parental involvement 2.35 1.52 0 5

Child and Family Characteristics


Socioeconomic status 0.05 0.82 −1.99 2.09
European American 0.57
African American 0.15
Hispanic 0.21
Asian American 0.03
Other 0.04
Female 0.48
K Age at assessment 67.81 4.21 57.30 84.50

24 months of age Home-based Parental involvement


Read books to child 2.20 0.88 0 3
Tell stories 1.69 1.02 0 3
Sing songs 2.53 0.77 0 3
Play indoor games 4.28 0.91 0 5

Preschool Achievement Indicators


Reading assessment 26.77 10.50 11.79 80.29
Mathematics assessment 30.90 9.43 9.89 65.73

Preschool Conduct Problem Indicators


(continued on next page)

8
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

Table 1 (continued)

Variable Mean or SD Min Max


Proportion

Temper tantrums 0.76 1.01 0 4


Annoys other children 0.89 0.96 0 4
Disruptive 1.18 1.02 0 4
Aggressive 0.70 0.94 0 4

Preschool Hyperactivity/Inattention Indicators


Impulsive 1.24 1.12 0 4
Over active 1.20 1.11 0 4
Finish tasks 1.11 0.98 0 4
Pay attention 1.21 0.90 0 4
Concentrate 1.34 1.07 0 4
Fidget 1.28 1.08 0 4

Preschool Prosocial Indicators


Makes friends 3.14 0.85 0 4
Eagerness to learn 3.06 0.90 0 4
Accepted by other children 3.42 0.68 0 4
Communicates with others 2.27 1.05 0 4
Independent 2.99 0.87 0 4
Stands up for others 2.37 1.09 0 4
Understanding of others 2.20 1.04 0 4
Shares 2.91 0.85 0 4

Preschool Teacher Characteristics


Early Childhood Degree 0.63
Child Development Associate’s Degree 0.34
Years of experience 13.13 8.85 0 70

Type of Prekindergarten Program


Public prekindergarten 0.19
Private prekindergarten 0.21
Center based prekindergarten 0.40
Head Start 0.17
Other Prekindergarten 0.03

Note: N ≈ 3650.

engaging in more parental involvement activities. On average, parents family, and community factors. As such, the transition process can be
of African American children reported engaging in fewer home-based considered as beginning the year prior to kindergarten entrance and
involvement activities than parents of European American children, emphasizing the interrelated connections between children, families,
holding all else constant. Results also showed that parents of girls re- early educators, and kindergarten teachers (Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta,
ported engaging in more home-based involvement in comparison to 2000). Early educators can help build strong linkages between home
parents of boys. In contrast, parents of children who are African and school, and preschool and kindergarten by utilizing transition
American, Hispanic, and Asian American reported engaging in more practices that invite parents to become involved in their child’s early
school-based involvement activities in comparison to their European learning and schooling experiences. These practices have the potential
American counterparts. In addition, the latent construct representing to support families and children during the formal transition to kin-
parental involvement when children were approximately 24 months of dergarten.
age was also positively associated with preschool reports of parental The present study utilized data from the Early Childhood
involvement. The model explained 8% of preschool teachers’ transition Longitudinal Study – Birth cohort (ECLS-B) to estimate a structural
practices, 6% of parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement, equation model that examined the associations among preschool tea-
65% of home-based parental involvement, and 17% of school-based chers’ transition practices, parents’ perceptions of invitations for in-
involvement. volvement, parental involvement, and children’s academic and socio-
emotional competencies during the transition to kindergarten. Our
findings support the Ecological and Dynamic Model of Transition, in
4. Discussion
that parental perceptions of preschool teachers’ invitations for in-
volvement predicted home-based involvement, which in turn predicted
The transition to formal schooling is an important time in the life of
higher average academic achievement, fewer conduct problems, and
children and families. During the transition to kindergarten children
less hyperactivity/inattention. Parent perceptions of preschool tea-
experience a uniquely different environment than their homes and early
chers’ invitations for involvement also predicted school-based in-
educational settings, one that can be characterized as placing an in-
volvement, but school-based involvement was not significantly related
creased demand on children’s cognitive and socio-emotional compe-
to social-emotional outcomes and it had a negative relationship with
tencies. In order to support children and families during this important
academic achievement. Overall, these findings suggests that preschool
point of transition the National Education Goals Panel (National
teachers may want to place more emphasis on creating positive home-
Education Goals Panel, 1998) recommended that “ready schools” create
school partnerships which encourage and support home-based in-
smooth linkages between home and school and to strive for continuity
volvement, as it is home-based parent involvement that better promotes
between early care and education programs and kindergarten. Rimm-
school readiness.
Kaufman and Pianta (2000) Ecological and Dynamic Model of Transi-
Contrary to hypothesized associations, preschool teachers’ transi-
tion emphasizes that successful transitions to kindergarten occur in an
tion practices were not directly associated with measures of school
environment consisting of interactions among child, school, classroom,

9
Table 2
Correlation matrix of primary variables under study.
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), 24 mos. (2003–2004), preschool data collection (2005–2006), Kindergarten
2006 (2006–2007), Kindergarten 2007 (2007–2008) data collections.
J. Puccioni, et al.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

1 –
2 0.80 –
3 −0.16 −0.16 –
4 −0.17 −0.18 0.58 –
5 −0.18 −0.20 0.51 0.74 –
6 −0.18 −0.17 0.64 0.71 0.59 –
7 −0.20 −0.22 0.49 0.61 0.72 0.52 –
8 −0.19 −0.22 0.47 0.66 0.71 0.53 0.70 –
9 −0.35 −0.40 0.35 0.45 0.50 0.36 0.50 0.51 –
10 −0.40 −0.42 0.36 0.52 0.54 0.42 0.55 0.59 0.71 –
11 −0.33 −0.38 0.40 0.55 0.61 0.42 0.60 0.68 0.71 0.74 –
12 −0.23 −0.26 0.46 0.63 0.67 0.50 0.66 0.84 0.55 0.62 0.74 –
13 0.23 0.26 −0.33 −0.36 −0.27 −0.32 −0.29 −0.24 −0.43 −0.47 −0.38 −0.29 –
14 0.40 0.42 −0.29 −0.37 −0.34 −0.29 −0.34 −0.33 −0.60 −0.69 −0.54 −0.40 0.59 –
15 0.22 0.24 −0.41 −0.50 −0.41 −0.43 −0.40 −0.36 −0.46 −0.52 −0.42 −0.38 0.73 0.53 –
16 0.20 0.21 −0.26 −0.31 −0.26 −0.31 −0.27 −0.23 −0.37 −0.42 −0.34 −0.26 0.52 0.50 0.44
17 0.36 0.41 −0.36 −0.43 −0.45 −0.35 −0.45 −0.45 −0.71 −0.66 −0.59 −0.47 0.49 0.61 0.49
18 0.25 0.25 −0.21 −0.24 −0.18 −0.23 −0.21 −0.20 −0.38 −0.40 −0.33 −0.23 0.50 0.50 0.41
19 0.23 0.24 −0.24 −0.28 −0.21 −0.27 −0.23 −0.21 −0.34 −0.39 −0.31 −0.23 0.50 0.49 0.42
20 0.22 0.24 −0.51 −0.56 −0.53 −0.51 −0.49 −0.48 −0.54 −0.55 −0.47 −0.46 0.52 0.49 0.59
21 −0.06 −0.08 0.04 0.05 0.08 0.06 0.05 0.05 0.06 0.06 0.07 0.04 −0.03 −0.03 −0.02
22 −0.01 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.02 0.02 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.02

10
23 −0.02 −0.01 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.03 0.03 −0.02 −0.02 −0.01
24 0.03 0.04 −0.03 −0.03 −0.02 −0.04 −0.02 −0.03 −0.02 −0.02 −0.01 −0.02 0.02 0.01 0.02
25 −0.05 −0.07 0.05 0.03 0.04 0.02 0.04 0.03 0.06 0.06 0.05 0.03 −0.04 −0.06 −0.04
26 −0.09 −0.10 0.04 0.04 0.05 0.03 0.03 0.04 0.05 0.06 0.06 0.04 −0.04 −0.07 −0.03
27 0.22 0.24 −0.10 −0.10 −0.09 −0.12 −0.10 −0.10 −0.12 −0.13 −0.11 0.09 0.06 0.13 0.09
28 0.11 0.11 −0.07 −0.05 −0.07 −0.07 −0.08 −0.05 −0.07 −0.08 −0.06 −0.04 0.07 0.08 0.09
29 0.07 0.02 −0.04 −0.04 −0.05 −0.05 −0.05 −0.02 −0.03 −0.06 −0.03 −0.01 0.07 0.08 0.07
30 −0.02 −0.01 −0.01 0.00 −0.01 0.01 0.00 0.02 0.00 0.01 0.02 0.04 −0.02 0.02 0.01
31 0.11 0.12 −0.03 −0.06 −0.05 −0.07 −0.04 −0.05 −0.07 −0.07 −0.06 −0.06 0.04 0.04 0.04

16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16 –
17 0.41 –
(continued on next page)
Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

progress; 23 = Info child development; 24 = Info volunteer; 25 = Info help learning; 26 = Info about community; 27 = Read books; 28 = Tell stories; 29 = Sing songs; 30 = Play building games; 31 = School-based
Note. 1 = Reading; 2 = Math; 3 = Temper tantrums; 4 = Annoys; 5 = Disruptive; 6 = Aggressive; 7 = Impulsive; 8 = Over active; 9 = Finishes task; 10 = Pays attention; 11 = Concentrate; 12 = Fidget; 13 = Invited
play; 14 = Eagerness learn; 15 = Accepted by others; 16 = Comforts others; 17 = Works independently; 18 = Stands up for others; 19 = Understands others; 20 = Shares; 21 = transition practices; 22 = Info child
readiness at the onset of kindergarten. These findings are in contrast to
previous studies which demonstrated a direct link between preschool
(Ahtola et al., 2011; LoCasale-Crouch et al., 2008) and kindergarten
31


teachers (Galindo & Sheldon, 2012; Jung & Han, 2013; Schulting et al.,
2005) use of transition practices and child outcomes during the tran-
sition to formal schooling. However, the results presented in this study

0.07
are consistent with recent findings from Cook and Coley (2017) who
30


also found no direct association between kindergarten teachers’ out-
reach efforts and children’s academic achievement at the onset of kin-
dergarten (Cook & Coley, 2017). One possible explanation for the lack

0.16
0.05
29

of direct association between preschool teachers’ transition practices



and measures of school readiness in this study could stem from the
narrow operational definition of transition practices. For example, there
were only three items available in the ECLS-B dataset that were in-
0.28
0.28
0.12
28

tended to measure preschool teachers’ transition practices. As such,


there may not have been sufficient variation to capture an association


between transition practices and measures of school readiness. In other
studies examining the relations between early educators’ outreach ef-
0.40
0.20
0.21
0.16
27

forts and child outcomes there are a greater number of items intended

to measure transition practices. Although there was no significant direct


association between parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement
and measures of school readiness, it is important to note that we found
0.07

0.09
0.18
0.01

0.02
26

a significant positive correlation between preschool teachers’ transition


practices and parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement. This


suggests that preschool teachers’ transition practices are not in vain, but
may have an impact on parental involvement and children’s readiness
0.53

0.08

0.09
0.22
0.02

0.03
25

for school by way of parental perceptions.


As predicted, the findings from this study demonstrate a positive


association between parental involvement and measures of school
readiness. More specifically, results indicate that parents who engaged
0.39
0.37
0.10
0.06
0.05
0.09
0.32
24

in more home-based parental involvement had children with higher


average levels of academic achievement, and lower levels of conduct


problems and fewer instances of hyperactivity/inattention. These
findings are consistent with a range of studies linking home-based in-
0.39
0.41
0.35
0.06
0.10

0.12
0.19
0.02
23

volvement to young children’s early academic achievement and socio-


emotional competencies (Booth & Crouter, 2008; Van Voorhis et al.,


2013). Previous studies have found that home-based parent involve-
ment in the form of home literacy predicts better attention, less hy-
0.49
0.32
0.31
0.29
0.06
0.08
0.04
0.08
0.12
22

peractivity, and less diagnosis of ADHD across the U.S. (Froiland &
Davison, 2016) and Son and Tineo (2016) also found that parents often
give students cues to pay attention during shared book reading. In
−0.06
0.04
0.04
0.09
0.09
0.10

0.04

addition, Fantuzzo et al. (2004) found that home-based involvement


0.00
0.01
0.02

parental involvement. Bold coefficient is significant at the p < .05 level or less.
21

was concurrently associated with less hyperactivity, more attention,


and less conduct problems among a small sample of African American
Head Start students. On the other hand, Fantuzzo et al. (2004) found
−0.03

−0.01

−0.03
−0.03
0.14
0.08
0.06

0.04
0.02

0.03

0.01

that school-based involvement was not related to these important


20

mental health outcomes. Our study replicated the Fantuzzo et al. (2004)
findings and extends them to a national level by finding that home-
based involvement during preschool predicted inattention/hyper-
−0.05
−0.02

−0.02

−0.03

−0.02
0.44

0.04

0.11
0.04
0.08

0.05
0.00

activity and conduct problems in kindergarten, while school-based


19

parent involvement in preschool did not predict inattention/hyper-


activity and conduct problems.
The most salient finding from this study supports the assertion
−0.03

−0.02

−0.02

−0.04
0.67
0.43

0.05
0.09
0.05
0.08
0.01

0.04

0.02

parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement (Hoover-Dempsey


18

et al., 2005) have the potential to shape parental involvement, which in


turn has a positive influence on child outcomes. Our findings indicate
that parents who have more positive perceptions about preschool tea-
−0.04
−0.04
−0.01

−0.06
0.41
0.40
0.54

0.14
0.08
0.05

0.07
0.00
0.04

0.00

chers’ invitations for involvement reported engaging children in more


17

home-based involvement, which in turn was predictive of children’s


academic achievement and socio-emotional wellbeing. These findings
are new and noteworthy because no studies to date have used nation-
−0.04
Table 2 (continued)

−0.04
−0.01
−0.01

−0.02

−0.02
0.70
0.80
0.48

0.10
0.04
0.10
0.03

0.03

ally representative data to link parents’ perceptions of preschool tea-


16

chers’ invitations for involvement to measures of school readiness by


way of home-based parental involvement. Furthermore, this is the first
study that we know of that links parent perceptions to home-based
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31

involvement, and children’s social-emotional wellbeing at the onset of

11
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

Fig. 2. N ≈ 3100. Standardized esti-


mates are presented addressing research
questions under study. Standardized
covariance estimate between transition
practices and parent perceptions reported
in figure. Figure does not depict controls.
Model fit statistics, RMSEA = 0.056
(90% CI: 0.055–0.057); SRMR = 0.07.
R2 Transition Practices = 0.08; R2 Parent
perception = 0.06; R2 Home-based
Parental Involvement = 0.65; R2 School-
based Parental Involvement = 0.17; R2
Academic Achievement = 0.67; R2
Conduct Problems = 0.31; R2
Hyperactivity/Inattention = 0.34; R2
Prosocial Behaviors = 0.22. Solid lines
indicate significance p < .05, and da-
shed line indicate no significant associa-
tion.
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education,
National Center for Education Statistics,
Early Childhood Longitudinal Study,
Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), 24 mos.
(2003–2004), preschool data collection
(2005–2006), Kindergarten 2006
(2006–2007), Kindergarten 2007
(2007–2008) data collections.

kindergarten. Given that parents’ perceptions of preschool teachers’ not attend Head Start. These are interesting and important findings
invitations for involvement in this study emphasized the sharing of considering that Head Start performance standards emphasize the im-
information about children’s progress and development in addition to portance of practices that support children’s transition to elementary
information about materials and resources intended to help families school (Rous, Hallam, McCormick, & Cox, 2010) and more importantly,
and children, these findings may be framed within a broader range of that Head Start programs make concerted efforts to build home-school
scholarship that highlights parents’ desire to know more about how to partnerships (Ansari & Gershoff, 2015; Hatcher, Nuner, & Paulsel,
help their child learn and succeed at school (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 2012). In fact, Ansari and Gershoff (2015) found that parents of chil-
2005; McIntyre, Eckert, Fiese, DiGennaro, & Wildenger, 2007). Parents dren attending Head Start who engaged in more school-based in-
of young children want information about the transition to kinder- volvement engaged their children in more cognitive stimulating activ-
garten in general and ways to help prepare children for kindergarten ities at home, which in turn was positively associated with gains in
more specifically (McIntyre et al., 2007), and early educators are un- literacy and mathematics. The authors also found that teacher and staff
iquely poised to provide parents with this type of information. There- training focused on how to involve parents was linked to more parental
fore, the evidence linking parents’ perceptions about invitations for involvement. Furthermore, research indicates that preschool teachers
involvement and parental involvement is encouraging given that (Rous et al., 2010) who received specialized training to enhance chil-
schools and policymakers alike are often interested in increasing par- dren’s transition to kindergarten reported using more types of transition
ental involvement as way to support young children’s early learning practices before and after the school year began in comparison to those
and development. teachers who did not have any special training. Additionally, preschool
Given that findings from this study provide evidence linking pre- teachers who held a degree in Early Childhood Education reported
school teachers’ transition practices, parents’ perceptions of preschool utilizing more transition practices and parents of children who were
teachers’ invitations for involvement, and parental involvement to taught by a preschool teacher who held a degree in Early Childhood
measures of school readiness it is important to consider the ways in Education held more positive beliefs about teachers’ invitations for
which transition practices, parents’ perceptions and involvement involvement. These findings point to the importance of advanced
varied. Consistent with a range of previous studies, findings presented coursework for early educators and ongoing professional development
in this study indicate there is a demographic divide in measures of focused on building productive home-school partnerships which aim to
school readiness, such that children from higher socioeconomic status shape parental involvement.
backgrounds (Reardon, 2011) and children who are older (Lin et al., There were also differences in parental involvement by socio-
2009) begin kindergarten with higher average levels of achievement demographic characteristics. Consistent with previous research parents
and demonstrate more socio-emotional wellbeing (Dee & Sievertsen, from higher socioeconomic status backgrounds reported engaging in
2015). Importantly, children from higher socioeconomic status back- more parental involvement activities (Cheadle & Amato, 2011; Phillips,
grounds are more likely to be given the gift of time and be older at the 2011). Also consistent with previous research, parents of African
beginning of kindergarten (Graue & DiPerna, 2000). Therefore, children American children reported engaging in fewer home-based involvement
from less affluent families may benefit from having a preschool teacher activities in comparison to their European American counterparts
who helps build reciprocal home-school connections that engender (Phillips, 2011; Puccioni, 2018; Rouse et al., 2005). Importantly, we
positive beliefs among families in ways that encourage and support also found that Hispanic and African American families engaged in
parental involvement. more school-based involvement, whereas European American families
Findings presented in this study suggest that parents with children engaged in more home-based involvement. This suggests that Hispanic
attending Head Start held more positive perceptions about preschool and African American families may benefit even more than European
teachers’ invitations for involvement in comparison to children who did American families from shifting preschool outreach efforts toward a

12
J. Puccioni, et al.

Table 3
Unstandardized regression coefficient for associations among transition practices, parental perceptions of invitations for involvement, parental involvement, and measures of school readiness.
SOURCE: U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), 24 mos. (2003–2004), preschool data collection (2005–2006), Kindergarten
2006 (2006–2007), Kindergarten 2007 (2007–2008) data collections.

Preschool Parents’ Perceptions of Preschool Home-based Preschool School-based Kindergarten Academic Kindergarten Conduct Kindergarten Kindergarten Prosocial
transition Invitations for Parental Involvement Parental Involvement Achievement Problems Hyperactivity/ Behaviors
Practices Involvement Inattention

Child/Family Characteristic
Controls
SES −0.05 −0.01 0.10** 0.35*** 2.37*** −0.04 −0.06* 0.11***
African American 0.09 0.03 −0.16** 0.16 −0.74 0.11* 0.05 0.02
Hispanic 0.01 0.02 −0.09 0.18 −0.92 −0.03 −0.02 0.03
Asian American 0.11 −0.06** −0.08 0.13 1.89** −0.08* −0.11 −0.07
Other 0.17 −0.01 −0.02 0.24 −0.46 0.02 −0.07 0.02
Female 0.07* −0.09 −0.52 −0.17*** −0.25*** 0.11**
Age at assessment 0.97*** −0.01 −0.02*** 0.01**
24 month parental involvement 0.04* 0.76*** 0.39***
Preschool Achievement 0.86***
Preschool Conduct Problems 0.41***
Preschool Hyperactivity 0.48***

13
Preschool Prosocial Skills 0.43***

Preschool Teacher and School


Characteristics Controls
BA degree 0.10* 0.04*
AA degree 0.12* −0.02
Years of experience 0.01*** 0.00
Private Pre-k −0.07 −0.04
Center-based Pre-k −0.26** −0.02
Head Start 0.26*** 0.10***
Other Pre-k −0.15 −0.06

Relation among primary model


variables under study
Transition Practices −0.01 0.03 0.02 0.03 −0.01
Parental Perceptions 0.27** 2.08*** 0.22 0.05 0.18 −0.09
Preschool Home-based 1.21* −0.10* −0.13** 0.08
Involvement
Preschool School-based −0.33* −0.02 −0.02 0.00
Involvement

Note. N ≈ 3100. Unstandardized estimates are presented. Model fit statistics, RMSEA = 0.06 (90% CI: 0.055–0.057); SRMR = 0.07. R2 Transition Practices = 0.08; R2 Parent perception = 0.06; R2 Home-based Parental
Involvement = 0.65; R2 School-based Parental Involvement = 0.17; R2 Academic Achievement = 0.67; R2 Conduct Problems = 0.31; R2 Hyperactivity/Inattention = 0.34; R2 Prosocial Behaviors = 0.22. Two tailed
test: *p < .05, **p < .01, *** p < .001.
Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

greater focus on home-based involvement. Importantly, we also found Declaration of Competing Interest
that parent home-based involvement when children were 24 months of
age predicted home-based involvement during preschool. This suggests The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
that some outreach to parents about home-based involvement could interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influ-
begin during late infancy, so that parents create strong home-based ence the work reported in this paper.
involvement habits well before preschool.
Appendix A. Supplementary material

5. Conclusion Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://


doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2019.104742.
This study contributes to the literature by using a nationally re-
presentative sample drawn from ECLS-B to examine the associations References
among preschool teachers’ transition practices, parental perceptions
and involvement, and measures of school readiness. The most salient Ahtola, A., Silinskas, G., Poikonen, P.-L., Kontoniemi, M., Niemi, P., & Nurmi, J.-E.
finding was that parents’ perceptions of preschool teachers’ invitations (2011). Transition to formal schooling: Do transition practices matter for academic
performance? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(3), 295–302. https://doi.org/
for involvement were positively associated with home-based involve- 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.12.002.
ment, which in turn was associated with children’s academic achieve- Anderson, K., & Minke, K. (2007). Parent involvement in education: Toward an under-
ment and socio-emotional wellbeing during the transition to kinder- standing of parents' decision making. Journal of Education Research, 100, 311–323.
Ansari, A., & Gershoff, E. (2015). Parent involvement in Head Start and children's de-
garten. Although findings presented in this study provide an important velopment: Indirect effects through parenting. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
contribution to the field, there are several limitations that should be 78(2), 562–579.
considered. Despite the benefits of using longitudinal secondary data, Baker, E. R., Jensen, C. J., & Tisak, M. S. (2017). A closer examination of aggressive
subtypes in early childhood: Contributions of executive function and single-parent
this study is correlational and direct causation cannot be assumed.
status. Early Child Development and Care. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2017.
Another issue pertains to measurement error, or the use of imprecise 1342079.
measures, which may underestimate the influence of preschool tea- Beaujean, A. (2015). R package for Baylor University educational psychology quantitative
courses. R Package. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BaylorEdPsych.
chers’ transition practices and parental involvement on measures of
Bethel, J., Green, J. L., Kalton, G., & Nord, C. (2005). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study,
school readiness. For example, this study includes few measures for BirthCohort (ECLS-B), sampling. Volume 2 of the ECLS-B methodology report for the 9-
preschool teachers’ transition practices and these items do not capture month data collection, 2001-02 (NCES 2005-147). Washington, DC: National Center
information about which families participated in these activities. In for Education Statistics.
Blevins-Knabe, B., & Musun-Miller, L. (1996). Number use at home by children and their
addition, preschool teachers only reported if they engaged in these parents its relationship to early mathematical performance. Early Development and
transition practices and do not capture the quality of the actual out- Parenting, 5, 35–45.
reach efforts. Future research should examine a wider range of pre- Booth, A., & Crouter, A. C. (Eds.). (2008). Disparities in school readiness: How families
contribute to transitions into school. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
school teachers’ transition practices and seek to measure family parti- Boyce, L. K., Innocenti, M. S., Roggman, L. A., Norman, V. K., & Ortiz, E. (2010). Telling
cipation in such efforts. Similar limitations can be found with regards to stories and making books: Evidence for an intervention to help parents in migrant
parental involvement. Although parents report on the frequency of Head Start families support their children's language and literacy. Early Education and
Development, 21(3), 343–371.
engaging in different types of parental involvement, there is no measure Castro, M., Expósito-Casas, E., López-Martín, E., Lizasoain, L., Navarro-Asencio, E., &
of the quality of those engagements. Gaviria, J. L. (2015). Parental involvement on student academic achievement: A
Despite these limitations there are several important strengths of meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 14(Supplement C), 33–46. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.edurev.2015.01.002.
this study that should be discussed. Most studies examining the role Cheadle, J. E., & Amato, P. R. (2011). A quantitative assessment of Lareau's qualitative
educators’ transition practices have on child outcomes primarily focus conlcusions about class, race, and parenting. Journal of family Issues, 32(679–706).
on academic achievement and these studies generally occur during Child Trends (2013). Parental involvement in schools. Retrieved from https://www.
childtrends.org/?indicators=parental-involvement-in-schools.
kindergarten. Fewer studies have used longitudinal nationally re-
Claessens, A., Duncan, G., & Engel, M. (2009). Kindergarten skills and fifth-grade
presentative data to examine how preschool teachers’ transition prac- achievement: Evidence form ECLS-K. Economics of Education Review, 28, 415–427.
tices have the potential to help support children’s transition to ele- Claessens, A., & Engel, M. (2013). How important is where you start? Early mathematics
mentary school by way of parental involvement. Findings from this knowledge and later school success. Teachers College Record, 115, 1–29.
Coley, R. (2002). An uneven start: Indicators of inequality in school readiness. Retrieved
study have important implications for encouraging parental involve- from Educational Testing Service: https://www.ets.org/Media/Research/pdf/
ment by demonstrating the degree to which parents’ perceptions about PICUNEVENSTART.pdf.
preschool teachers’ invitations for involvement shape parental in- Cook, K. D., & Coley, R. L. (2017). School transition practices and children’s social and
academic adjustment in kindergarten. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(2),
volvement, which in turn, has the potential to positively impact chil- 166–177.
dren’s academic outcomes during the transition to elementary school. Daley, T. C., Munk, T., & Carlson, E. (2011). A national study of kindergarten transition
Subsequent research should explore how parents role construction and practices for children with disabilities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(4),
409–419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.11.001.
invitations for involvement simultaneously contribute to parental in- Datar, A. (2006). Does delaying kindergarten entrance give children a head start?
volvement decision making processes (Hoover-Dempsey et al., 2005). Economics of Education Review, 25, 43–62.
Future studies should also explore if preschool teachers’ transition Dee, T., & Sievertsen, H. H. (2015). The gift of time? School starting age and mental
health. Health Economics, 27(5), 781–802.
practices and parents’ perceptions of invitations for involvement are Desimone, L. (1999). Linking parent involvement with student achievement: Do race and
differentially associated with parental involvement activities and income matter. Journal of Educational Research, 93, 11–30.
measures of readiness for children living in poverty and those from Domina, T. (2005). Leveling the home advantage: Assessing the effectiveness of parent
involvement in elementary school. Sociology of Education, 78, 233–249.
historically marginalized communities.
Dunst, C. J., Meter, D., & Hamby, D. W. (2011). Relationship between young children’s
nursery rhyme experiences and knowledge and phonological and printrelated abil-
ities. CELLreviews, 4(1), 1–12.
Funding Early, D., Pianta, R., Taylor, L., & Cox, M. (2001). Transition practices: Findings from a
national survey of kindergarten teachers. Early Childhood Education Journal, 28(3),
199–206. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026503520593.
This research was supported by a grant funded by the Foundation El Nokali, N. E., Bachman, H. J., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2010). Parent involvement and
for Child Development. The views presented in the article belong solely children's academic and social development in elementary school. Child Development,
81(3), 988–1005. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01447.x.
to the authors and do not reflect the view of the Foundation for Child Fan, X. (2001). Parental involvement and students’ academic achievement: A growth
Development. modeling analysis. Journal of Experimental Education, 70, 27–61.
Fantuzzo, J., McWayne, C., Perry, M., & Childs, S. (2004). Multiple dimensions of family

14
J. Puccioni, et al. Children and Youth Services Review 109 (2020) 104742

involvement and their relations to behavioral and learning competencies for urban, parent–teacher communication and parent involvement from preschool to school.
low-income children. School Psychology Review, 33, 467–480. Early Child Development and Care, 185(7), 1031–1052. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Ferretti, L. K., & Bub, K. L. (2017). Family routines and school readiness during the 03004430.2014.975223.
transition to kindergarten. Early Education and Development, 28(1), 59–77. https:// Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus user’s guide (seventh ed.). Los Angeles,
doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.1195671. CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Froiland, J. M., & Davison, M. L. (2016). Home literacy, television viewing, fidgeting and National Education Goals Panel (1998). Ready schools. Retrieved from Washington, D.C.
ADHD in young children. Educational Psychology, 36(8), 1337–1353. https://govinfo.library.unt.edu/negp/reports/readysch.pdf.
Froiland, J. M., Peterson, A., & Davison, M. L. (2013). The long-term effects of early Phillips, M. (2011). Parenting, time use, and disparities in academic outcomes. In G.
parent involvement and parent expectation in the USA. School Psychology Duncan, & R. Murnane (Eds.). Whither opportunity? (pp. 207–228). New York: Russell
International, 34(1), 33–50. Sage Foundation.
Galindo, C., & Sheldon, S. B. (2012). School and home connections and children's kin- Pianta, R. C., Cox, M. J., Taylor, L. C., & Early, D. M. (1999). Kindergarten teachers'
dergarten achievement gains: The mediating role of family involvement. Early practices related to the transition into school: Results from a national survey.
Childhood Research Quarterly, 27, 90–103. Elementary School Journal, 100(71–86).
Graue, M. E., & DiPerna, J. (2000). Redshirting and early retention: Who gets the “gift of Powell, D. R., Son, S., File, N., & Froiland, J. M. (2012). Changes in parent involvement
time” and what are its outcomes? American Educational Research Journal, 37(2), across the transition from public school prekindergarten to first grade and children's
509–534. academic outcomes. The Elementary School Journal, 113(2), 276–300. https://doi.org/
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social Skills Rating System manual. Circle Pines, 10.1086/667726.
MN: American Guidance Service. Puccioni, J. (2018). Parental beliefs about school readiness, home and school-based in-
Hatcher, B., Nuner, J., & Paulsel, J. (2012). Kindergarten Readiness and Preschools: volvement, and children’s academic achievement. Journal of Research in Childhood
Teachers' and Parents' Beliefs within and across Programs. Early Childhood Research & Education, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568543.2018.1494065.
Practice, 14(2). Puccioni, J., Baker, E. R., & Froiland, J. M. (2019). Academic socialization and the
Hill, N. E. (2001). Parenting and academic socialization as they relate to school readiness: transition to kindergarten: Parental beliefs about school readiness and involvement.
The roles of ethnicity and family income. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(4), Infant and Child Development, e2154. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2154.
686–697. Ramani, G., & Siegler, R. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-in-
Hill, N. E., & Craft, S. A. (2003). Parent-school involvement and school performance: come children's numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child
Mediated pathways among socioeconomically comparable African-American and Development, 79(2), 375–394.
Euro-American families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 74–83. Ready, D. D., LoGerfo, L. F., Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (2005). Explaining girls’ ad-
Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Walker, J. M. T., Sandler, H. M., Whetsel, D., Green, C. L., vantage in kindergarten literacy learning: Do classroom behaviors make a difference?
Wilkins, A. S., & Closson, K. E. (2005). Why do parents become involved? Research Elementary School Journal, 106(1), 21–38.
findings and implications. Elementary School Journal, 106(2), 105–130. Reardon, S. (2011). The widening academic achievement gap between the rich and the
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure poor: New evidence and possible explanations. In G. Duncan, & R. Murname (Eds.).
analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, Whither opportunity? (pp. 507–522). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
6, 1–55. Rimm-Kaufman, S., & Pianta, R. C. (2000). An ecological perspective on the transition to
Huat See, B., & Gorard, S. (2015). The role of parents in young people’s education-a kindergarten. A theoretical framework to guide empirical research. Journal of Applied
critical review of the causal evidence. Oxford Review of Education, 41(3), 346–366. Developmental Psychology, 21, 491–511. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(00)
https://doi.org/10.1080/03054985.2015.1031648. 00051-4.
Jeynes, W. H. (2005). A meta-analysis of the relation of parental involvement to urban Rous, B., Hallam, R., McCormick, K., & Cox, M. (2010). Practices that support the tran-
elementary school student academic achievement. Urban Education, 40(3), 237–269. sition to public preschool programs: Results from a National Survey. Early Childhood
Jung, E., & Han, H. S. (2013). Teacher outreach efforts and reading achievement in Research Quarterly, 25(1), 17–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.09.001.
kindergarten. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 27, 93–110. Rouse, C., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Mclanahan, S. (2005). School readiness: Closing racial and
Kleemans, T., Peeters, M., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Child and home predictors ethnic gaps. The Future of Children, 15(1), 5–14.
of early numeracy skills in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), Rubin, D. B. (1976). Inference and missing data. Biometrika, 63(3), 581–592.
471–477. Sabol, T. J., & Pianta, R. (2012). Patterns of school readiness forecast achievement and
Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (third ed.). New socioemotional development at the end of elementary school. Child Development,
York: Guilford Press. 83(1), 282–299. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01678.x.
LeFevre, J., Clarke, T., & Stringer, A. P. (2002). Influences of language and parental in- Schulting, A. B., Malone, P. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2005). The effect of school-based kin-
volvement on the development of counting skills: Comparison of French- and English- dergarten transition policies and practices on child academic outcomes.
speaking Canadian children. Early Child Development and Care, 172, 283–300. Developmental Psychology, 41(6), 860–871.
LeFevre, J., Skwarchuk, S., Smith-Chant, B., Fast, L., Kamawar, D., & Bisanz, J. (2009). Snow, K., Derecho, A., Wheeless, S., Lennon, J., Rosen, J., Rogers, J., ... Einaudi, P.
Home numeracy experiences and children's math performance in the early school (2009). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B), Kindergarten 2006
years. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science-Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du and 2007 Data File User’s Manual (2010-010). Washington, DC: National Center for
Comportement, 41, 55–66. Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
Lin, H. H., Freeman, L., & Chu, K. L. (2009). The impact of kindergarten enrollment age Son, S. C., & Tineo, M. F. (2016). Mothers’ attention-getting utterances during shared
on academic performance through kindergarten to fifth grade. European Journal of book reading: Links to low-income preschoolers’ engagement, visual attention, and
Social Science, 10, 45–54. early literacy. Infant and Child Development, 25, 259–282. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Little, M. H., Cohen-Vogel, L., & Curran, F. C. (2016). Facilitating the transition to kin- icd.1932.
dergarten: What ECLS-K data tell us about school practices then and now. Sonnenschein, S., & Galindo, C. (2015). Race/ethnicity and early mathematics skills:
2332858416655766 AERA Open, 2(3), https://doi.org/10.1177/ Relations between home, classroom, and mathematics achievement. The Journal of
2332858416655766. Educational Research, 108(4), 261–277. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2014.
LoCasale-Crouch, J., Mashburn, A. J., Downer, J. T., & Pianta, R. C. (2008). Pre-kinder- 880394.
garten teachers’ use of transition practices and children's adjustment to kindergarten. StataCorp (2013). Stata: Release 13. Statistical Software. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(1), 124–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Tierney, N., Cook, D., McBain, M., Fay, C., O'Hara-Wild, M., Hester, J., & Smith, L. (2019).
ecresq.2007.06.001. Maniar: Data Structures, Summaries, and Visualisations for Missing Data. R Package.
Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G. K., & Tziraki, N. (2013). Examining the effects of home lit- https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=naniar.
eracy and numeracy environment on early reading and math acquisition. Early van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). mice: Multivariate imputation by
Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 692–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecrq.2013.05. chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(3), 1–67. https://www.
004. jstatsoft.org/v45/i03/.
Martin, M. J., Conger, R. D., Schofield, T. J., Dogan, S. J., Widaman, K. F., Donnellan, M. Van Voorhis, F. L., Mailer, M. F., Epstein, J. L., & Lloyd, C. M. (2013). The impact of
B., & Neppl, T. K. (2010). Evaluation of the interactionist model of socioeconomic family involvementon the education of children ages 3 to 8 a focus on literacy and
status and problem behavior: A developmental cascade across generations. math achievement outcomes and social-emotional skills. Retrieved from https://
Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 695–713. https://doi.org/10.1017/ www.mdrc.org/publication/impact-family-involvement-education-children-ages-
S0954579410000374. 3-8.
McIntyre, L., Eckert, T., Fiese, B., DiGennaro, F., & Wildenger, L. (2007). Transition to Vandermaas-Peeler, M., Nelson, J., Bumpass, C., & Sassine, B. (2009). Social contexts of
kindergarten: Family experiences and involvement. Early Childhood Education development: Parent-child interactions during reading and play. Journal of Early
Journal, 35(1), 83–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-007-0175-6. Childhood Research Literacy, 9(3), 295–317.
Merrell, K. M. (2003). Preschool and kindergarten behavior scales (PKBS-2). Retrieved Walker, J. M. T., Ice, C. L., Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., & Sandler, H. M. (2011). Latino
from http://www.proedinc.com/customer/productView.aspx?ID=2285. parents' motivation for involvement in their children's schooling: An exploratory
Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., & de Jong, M. T. (2009). Interactive book reading in early edu- study. The Elementary School Journal, 111(3), 409–429.
cation: A tool to stimulate print knowledge as well as oral language. Review of Weimer, A. A., & Guajardo, N. R. (2005). False belief, emotion understanding, and social
Educational Research, 79, 979–1007. skills among Head Start and non-Head Start children. Early Education & Development,
Murray, E., McFarland-Piazza, L., & Harrison, L. J. (2015). Changing patterns of 16(3), 341–366. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1603_3.

15

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen