Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

PDFlib PLOP: PDF Linearization, Optimization, Protection

Page inserted by evaluation version


www.pdflib.com – sales@pdflib.com
Review article

The room and atmosphere as aspects of the meal: a review


John S. A. Edwards* and Inga-Britt Gustafsson†
*Foodservice and Applied Nutrition Research Group, Bournemouth University, Poole, Dorset, BH12 5BB,
UK; †Örebro University, Department of Restaurant and Culinary Art, Sörälgsvägen 2, 712 60
GRYTHYTTAN, Sweden

Abstract

Correspondence: The consumption of food takes place within what can loosely be termed ‘the
John S. A. Edwards, room’, although in reality, this could be a variety of settings, both indoors
Foodservice and Applied and outside. Aspects and features within that room contribute towards and
Nutrition Research
Group, Bournemouth
make up the atmosphere, something that is relatively easy to appreciate but
University, Poole, Dorset, rather more difficult to quantify and describe. This review considers selected
BH12 5BB, UK. Tel: +44 aspects under the headings of interior variables, background music and noise,
1202 595127; Fax: +44 and odour; layout and design variables, table layout and seating; and human
1202 515707; E-mail: variables, density and crowding, and social facilitation, which contribute
edwardsj@bournemouth.
towards the room’s atmosphere. It is important to recognise and appreciate
ac.uk
that other attributes, of similar if not of equal or more importance, also exist.
Keywords:
atmosphere, crowding,
density, music, noise,
odour, social facilitation,
table layout and seating

atmosphere, something that is relatively easy to


Background
appreciate, yet is difficult to define or quantify.
When we walk into a room and the conversation
The café is not a place where man goes for
immediately stops – there is an atmosphere. Simi-
a drink but a place he goes to in order to
larly, when we enter a restaurant, we sense that
drink in company, where he can establish
there is ‘quite a pleasant atmosphere’, but again
relationships . . .
we do not know why it should be as such.
Bourdieu P (1984). Distinction. A social critique on The purpose of this paper was to review the
the judgement of taste. Translated by Nice R, current literature on the aspects of the room,
Rouledge & Kegan Paul Ltd., p. 183. which in turn contribute towards its atmosphere,
the words room and atmosphere being used
The room, or perhaps more accurately the place almost interchangeably. This could, in view of the
where the consumption of food and beverages myriad issues involved, become an enormous,
takes place, contains a multitude of features, almost impossible undertaking, hence, this issue
ranging from the fixed, such as chairs, tables, initially provides a broad overview before consid-
lights and colours, to the moveable, such as other ering the selected aspects. It is the intention that
people and the staff. Each of these, both indepen- future issues of the journal should pick up and
dently and together, help to create the room’s develop other themes.

22 © 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al. 23

the elements of the atmosphere are experienced


Introduction
through:
Over the years, numerous articles have been Visual dimensions – colour, brightness, size and
written on atmosphere, and Kotler (1973–1974) shape
is often credited as identifying it as an important Aural dimensions – volume and pitch
area for marketers to study. Turley & Milliman Olfactory dimensions – scent and freshness
(2000), however, consider that other researchers Tactile dimensions – softness, smoothness and
used and manipulated elements of atmosphere 10 temperature
years earlier, although there are even earlier ref- Therefore, if the eating environment is to provide
erences. Campbell-Smith (1967), for example, in an ‘appropriate atmosphere’, it is the aspects of
his seminal work, identified several factors, which the senses that must be manipulated.
could be influential and are shown schematically Turley & Milliman (2000), in a review of the
in Fig. 1. experimental evidence on shopping behaviour,
Since then, various research streams have developed and added to work by Berman &
explored and evaluated the aspects of atmosphere Evans (1998), and identified a host of atmosphere
including the terminology. Kotler (1973–1974), variables, which they divided into five categories:
for example, uses terms such as ‘spatial aesthetics’ External variables, such as the entrance, signs and
before settling on ‘atmospherics’; Bitner (1992) size of the building
uses ‘the built environment’ and ‘servicescapes’; General interior variables, such as flooring,
whilst Hutton & Richardson (1995) combine music, colour, smell and temperature
atmospherics and servicescapes to form health- Layout and design variables, such as space,
scapes, all of which come under a branch of psy- waiting areas, furniture layout and traffic flows
chology known as ‘environmental psychology’ Point of purchase and decoration variables, such
(Countryman & Jang 2006). For the purpose of as signs, cards and product displays
this paper, the term ‘atmosphere’ continues to be Human variables, such as employee and customer
used and is taken to encompass the elements of characteristics, crowding, and privacy.
the surrounding environment in which the con- However, much of the research focuses on the
sumption of food and beverages takes place (the retail environment, although there are some data
room). that also consider foodservice operations. This
Kotler (1973–1974), in identifying what is paper, therefore, draws on the various research
understood by atmosphere, be it a ‘busy atmo- streams and considers selected variables com-
sphere’ or a ‘good atmosphere’, considers that its monly found in an eating-away-from-home
dimensions are detected through the senses, thus, setting, which might be experienced in the room

The Room (1) The Room (2)


Shape and Size Ambient Temperature
Type of seat booths Noise levels
Layout of seating Colour scheme
Number of views from Level of illumination
different seats Degree of comfort
Cleanliness

The Room (3)


Table Appointments
Crockery Atmosphere Nationality of Restaurant
Cutlery
Condiments

Staff
Figure 1 Elements of atmosphere. Appearance
Source: adapted from Campbell- Age
Dress
Smith (1967).

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
24 Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al.

and subsequently affect the atmosphere. These are less time shopping and queuing at the checkout
considered under the general headings of the inte- (Herrington & Capella 1996).
rior variables, background music and noise, and
odour; layout and design variables, table layout
Effects of music genres
and seating; and human variables (the meeting),
density and crowding, and social facilitation. Background music can influence the amount and
volume of sales, for example, of wine. When two
genres of music, ‘top 40’ and ‘classical’, were
Internal variables affecting the room
played in a retail outlet, there was no difference
and atmosphere
in the total number of bottles of wine sold.
However, when classical music was played, this
Background music and noise
improved the sale of more expensive wines with
consumers perhaps associating wine consumption
If music be the food of love, play on;
with prestige and sophistication (Areni & Kim
give me excess of it, that, surfeiting,
1993). Similarly, the nationality of the music can
the appetite may sicken, and so die.
influence an individual’s perception and affect
Shakespeare’s Twelfth Night (I, i, 1–3) sales. When French music is played, people, as
might be expected, are more likely to think of
Music, often referred to as ‘musicscapes’ (Oakes France and purchase French wine. Similarly, when
2000), is important in a number of eating sce- German music is played, people are more likely to
narios, as it not only helps to create and set the think of Germany and purchase German wine.
atmosphere but can also be used to disguise or What is also relevant is that customers do not
mask other background operational noises that seem to be aware of the effects music had on their
may detract from the overall experience. Given selection. (North et al. 1997, 1999; Ryan et al.
this importance, it is surprising that it is only 2000).
relatively recently that the effects music has on Music genre has been studied in a cafeteria
influencing behaviour have been investigated. (Feinstein et al. 2002) where it was shown that
Research has been undertaken in retail settings, Italian music significantly affected the choice of
and it has been argued that many eating out occa- ethnic over non-ethnic entrées. Italian music was
sions are more closely allied to retailing than ‘tra- shown to positively affect the consumption, not
ditional’ foodservice, and that results are equally only of Italian food, but also of Mexican food.
as applicable (Herrington & Capella 1996). Surprisingly, Mexican music did not significantly
In a retail setting, background music can in- affect the selection of Mexican dishes; perhaps
fluence the amount of time and money spent, because the music was of a similar tempo. The
reduce anxiety, increase positive mood ratings, authors suggest the Italian music was similar to
relieve depression, decrease frustration, increase that found in popular Italian restaurants, thereby
employee output, reduce absenteeism and reduce encouraging diners to think of and purchase
the stress associated with queuing (waiting in ethnic food. What did arise from subsequent
line) (Herrington & Capella 1996; Yalch & focus groups was that only traditional, rather
Spangenberg 2000; Sweeney & Wyber 2002). than modern, Italian music evoked images of
The right type of music can influence shoppers to Italy. Favourable effects have also been found
purchase more expensive brands; loud music can when music is played in a cafeteria where custom-
increase the rate of spend, but without a change ers showed a positive correlation between the
to the total spend (Oakes 2000). In addition, liking for the music, liking for the atmosphere and
background music can influence a shopper’s a desire to return. The more the music was liked,
evaluation of the service provided, and as a con- the more customers liked the situation, the more
sequence, the total shopping experience. Where they were attracted to the source of the music and
background music is played, shoppers get the wanted to return (North & Hargreaves 1996).
impression that shops care more about their cus- The style of the music can influence customer
tomers, and also the impression that they spend spending in restaurants (North et al. 2003;

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al. 25

Wilson 2003). When two different styles of music, amount spent on food was unaffected by the
well-known pop and ‘popular’ classical, were tempo of the music, although expenditure from
played in a restaurant serving high quality, freshly the bar was significantly more with the slow
prepared food with above average prices, the clas- music, which also provided a higher profit
sical music produced higher spending with total margin.
food and total spend being significantly different Other research (Caldwell & Hibbert 1999) has
(North et al. 2003). Other research (Sullivan similarly shown how the music tempo and pref-
2002) though has shown that only volume has a erence (Caldwell & Hibbert 2002) affect the
significant effect on meal duration and expendi- dining time, but interestingly, where slow music
ture on both food and beverages, although it was was played, customers underestimated the time
tentatively acknowledged that the presence of spent in the restaurant and overestimated the time
music of any type, compared with no music, sig- with fast music. The speed of the music appears to
nificantly affected the duration and expenditure affect the number of bites (mouthfuls) per minute.
on a meal, but further work was considered Where fast music was played, the mean number
necessary. of bites per minute was 4.40; 3.83 for slow tempo
music; and 3.23 for no music (Roballey et al.
1985). The music significantly affected the eating
Effects of music tempo
speed, although there were no significant differ-
The tempo of the music also has an effect on ences for the total mealtime.
restaurant customers (Milliman 1986). An instru-
mental music with two tempos, slow, with 72 or
Effects of music on special groups
fewer beats per minute, and fast, with 92 or more
beats per minute, was played in a restaurant that It is well established that in many organisations,
was of a ‘high’ quality, attractively decorated, such as prisons (National Audit Office 1997) and
above average price and appealing mainly to institutions where the residents are ‘mentally
middle-aged people in the upper-middle income disturbed’, mealtimes can be very stressful and
bracket. The restaurant was generally very busy, potential points of conflict. However, in the latter
but Friday and Saturday evenings were extremely case, an important part of the overall therapy is
busy and entailed a 30- to 60-min wait before for patients to be integrated into daily routines
being seated. The slow tempo music was ran- and activities including mealtimes (Goddaer &
domly played on Friday, the fast tempo on Satur- Abraham 1994). The influence that music has in
day, and alternated for the following 8 weeks, these and in similar settings is therefore impor-
with the results highlighting a number of interest- tant. A relaxing music played in a nursing home
ing observations. during the main meal of the day significantly
Although it took 2 min longer to serve custom- reduced the general level of agitation of diners
ers when the slow music was played, this differ- with severe cognitive impairment, as well as the
ence was not significant. What was significant extent to which they were physically aggressive
was that with the slow music, customers spent and verbally agitated (Goddaer & Abraham
significantly longer at the table; the tempo of the 1994).
background music appearing to affect their dining The influence that music has on food intake
speed. When the slow music was played, the and symptoms common in dementia, such as
average waiting time per group before being depressed mood, irritability and restlessness, was
seated was 47 min, compared with a waiting time assessed in a nursing home where residents were
of 34 min with the fast music. However, 10.5% of suffering from dementia. Three categories of
customers left before being seated when the slow music, soothing, nostalgic and pop music, were
music was played, and 12.0% left when the fast each played for periods of 2 weeks. Results show
music was played, although this was not signifi- a significant improvement in irritability, fear-
cant. The shorter waiting time with the faster panic and depressed mood, particularly when the
music, appeared, therefore, to have little or no soothing music was played. It was also shown
influence on the decision to wait for a table. The that patients ate significantly more, particularly

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
26 Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al.

desserts, although the increase was most marked keep away customers over 30 years of age, and
when the pop music was played. There was a that the music is turned up to drive customers
significant correlation between the amounts out (Laurance 1997). In London (New Scientist
served and the amounts consumed; the latter 2000), many eating places exceed the current
being influenced in part by the staff who served guidance and legislation (Health and Safety
larger portions of both main courses and desserts Executive 2005). In ‘modern’ restaurants where
(Ragneskog et al. 1996). there is a predominance of bare floors, walls and
ceilings, mean noise levels can be as high as 84.9
(SD ⫾ 4.5) dBA with a recorded peak level of
Effects of music volume and noise
97 dBA. In ‘traditional’ restaurants with carpets,
Music can also influence the acceptability of curtains and tablecloths, the average noise levels
single items of food. Young men, served with tend to be much less, 71.2 dBA (SD ⫾ 2.1), and in
drinks with different levels of sodium chloride pubs, 83.1 (SD ⫾ 8.29), although the loudest
(salt) and sucrose (sweet), and exposed to differ- noise peaked at over 100 dBA. Decor, therefore,
ent levels of music and noise (silence, loud music can have a noticeable effect on the level of noise,
at 90 dB; low noise at 70 dB and loud noise at hence atmosphere, in many establishments.
90 dB), were asked to rate the acceptability of
each drink. The noise caused heart rates to
Scent, smell or odour
increase according to the level of sound intensity,
but not so with the music. The ‘pleasantness’ of The use of ‘ambient’ or ‘atmospheric’ scent, smell
the sweet drinks, but not salt drinks, increased or odour, that is, an odour that does not originate
with both the music and loud noise (Ferber & from any particular source but is present in the
Cabanac 1987). atmosphere, is increasing, yet, unlike research
In a subsequent study, young men and women into product odours, it remains relatively under-
were allowed to drink non-diet cola, orange and researched (Morrin & Ratneshwar 2000). Not-
lemon, or diet cola and orange, which were freely withstanding, the same authors also report the
available and were served at room temperature. use of smells in retail settings: to ‘energise’
During the subsequent 25-min periods, each workers, to calm cancer patients during medical
group drank in silence, or with pop music playing procedures, and also to help prevent lorry drivers
at 70 and 90 dB. What is perhaps the most impor- from becoming drowsy at the wheel.
tant finding from this study is that the louder the This overall lack of research seems strange as a
music, the greater the consumption of soft drinks pleasant ambient odour has also been shown to
for both genders; the pleasantness of the drinks help potential consumers show a better recall of
also increased for females but not for males unfamiliar brands, increase attention and memory
(McCarron & Tierney 1989). (Morrin & Ratneshwar 2000), influence approach
Whilst music has been shown to have a number behaviour and mood, give the perception that
of advantages in creating the ‘right atmosphere’ consumers spend less time in a store (Spangenberg
when eating out, unwanted noise can be a nui- et al. 1996, 2006), influence the amount of money
sance, can interfere with daily activities and can gambled in slot machines (Hirsch 1995), affect the
lead to health disorders. Limited research has recall of happy memories (Ehrichman & Halpern
shown that the critical noise level is between 65 1988), encourage a helping behaviour (Baron
and 70 decibels above reference noise, adjusted 1997), and persuade individuals to linger and
(dBA). Noises above this level can contribute to a spend longer looking at items (Knasko 1995), but
number of adverse effects, and road noise can lead perhaps only under conditions of medium density
to fatigue, headache, gastroenteric disorders and (Michon et al. 2005).
a loss of appetite, depression, and irritation What is also interesting is that often subjects
(Yoshida et al. 1997). are not aware of the scent (Ward et al. 2007), that
The question therefore arises as to how loud the nature of the scent is not important, that
are the music and noise, for it has been claimed neutral scents can produce enhanced perceptions,
that some restaurant managers use loud music to and within a reasonable range, the intensity of the

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al. 27

Table 1 Effects of hedonic ratings of odour

High hedonic rating of odour Low hedonic rating of odour

Increase in hunger perception (P < 0.05) Decrease in hunger perception (NS)


Increase in consumption (P < 0.05) Decrease in consumption (NS)
Increase in acceptability (P < 0.05) Decrease in acceptability (NS)
Influence on food choice (P < 0.05)
Significant enhancement of food intake and Suppression of food intake and modification
dietary patterns (P < 0.05) of dietary patterns (P < 0.05)

Source: Blackwell (1997).


NS, not significant.

scent did not dramatically affect the results with a high hedonic aroma (grilled bacon) and
(Spangenberg et al. 1996). On the other hand, low hedonic aroma (boiled Brussels sprouts), a
other researchers have shown that the ambient number of empirical experiments were under-
odour should be congruent with the product or taken. The overall results are summarised in
class of products for where this occurs; subjects Table 1 where it can be seen that aromas which
spent additional time processing the data in a are liked have a significant influence on a number
more holistic way, often going beyond the infor- of aspects, whereas those with a low hedonic
mation given (Mitchell et al. 1995). rating, which are not liked, have an opposite
However, although ambient scent is important effect, although these are not always significant.
in creating the ‘right atmosphere’, odour is It would seem that the use of odour is as impor-
seldom experienced on its own but normally in tant as other variables in creating an atmosphere
conjunction with other stimuli. Mattila & Wirtz and attracting customers, and along with a décor,
(2001) manipulated both scent and three music theme and colour should perhaps be a distinctive
scenarios (no music, low-arousal and high- restaurant feature. However, unlike, for example,
arousal music) in a retail outlet where they found colour, an odour, so long as it is within a number
that when the arousal levels of ambient scent and of broad parameters, does not appear to be
music were matched, consumers’ evaluation of noticed and could therefore be used in a very
the experience was enhanced. In other words, subtle manner to create an atmosphere and envi-
when environmental stimuli, in this case, scent ronment, unique to a particular establishment or
and music, act together, they help to provide a chain of eating places. Termed ‘signature scent’,
coherent atmosphere. judicious use of an odour, perhaps the most
Despite the obvious need for ambient odour, evocative of the five senses, could help to ‘tangi-
very little research has been conducted in the area bilise’ elements of a room and the service compo-
of foodservice. Guéguen & Petr (2006) studied nent (Zemke & Shoemaker 2007).
the effects of diffusing lemon and lavender odours
in a restaurant, and found that only lavender had
Layout and design variables affecting
any effect, probably because of its relaxing effect
the atmosphere
where it was shown that the length of time
and the amount of money spent were positively
Table layout and seating
affected. Zemke & Shoemaker (2007) also
showed that the use of a pleasant ambient odour
All great change in America begins at the dinner
(essential oil of geranium) had a positive effect in
table.
increasing social interaction behaviours with
strangers, a useful trait in the hospitality industry. Ronald Reagan (1911–2004)
Blackwell (1997) provides a very useful insight
into the effects of odours, both in a laboratory Layout accessibility and aesthetics can have a
and restaurant. Having initially identified a food positive influence on perceived quality in a

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
28 Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al.

number of leisure settings, with seating comfort customers seated next to the window had a lower
also having an effect on perceived quality (Wake- average spend, but an average spend where tables
field & Blodgett 1996). The overall restaurant, were configured differently, diagonally vs. side by
table and seat layout can influence atmosphere, is side, was not different. The average dining dura-
often symbolic and can be used to indicate status, tion of customers sitting at anchored seats did not
can define personal space, and can regulate differ significantly to those at unanchored seats,
privacy and interaction (Robson 2002). In a large although customers seated at banquettes had a
restaurant, customers might use fixed or architec- significantly longer dining duration than those
tural features, such as columns, walls, ‘dumb seated at other types of tables. Customers in
waiters’, nooks and crannies, to help define per- larger parties stayed longer, but table size was not
sonal space. These provide a ‘buffer’ between cus- significantly related to meal duration. Table con-
tomers, and help prevent or deter others from figuration, diagonally vs. side by side, did not
encroaching on that space. In smaller restaurants, have a significant effect on dining duration,
this may not be possible, and customers might although customers seated at ‘bad’ tables stayed
resort to other tactics such as positioning their significantly shorter than those seated at ‘good’
seats in the middle or using eye contact and body tables. Tables that were more exposed and offer-
language to defend that space (Robson 2002). ing less opportunity for privacy had a lower
People tend, whenever possible, to choose seats average spend and duration than those offering
around the edges of the room, and to use items, privacy, probably because of the ‘goldfish bowl’
such as bags, shopping and other personal effects, effect (Kimes & Robson 2004).
to help define and protect their territory and These results are important for the design of a
dissuade others from sitting nearby (Robson restaurant layout and the creation of atmosphere
2002). where operators are trying to balance the amount
In fast food outlets, men tend to choose tables of space taken up by various seating configura-
where they have a view of the busiest part of the tions and the average spend per person. Booths
restaurant. Women, on the other hand, are less may require more space, but they may generate
likely to use a fast food restaurant on their own, more income; banquettes may be more space effi-
but if they do, they are more likely to use the cient and encourage people to stay longer, but
drive-through. If they do go inside, once they have may generate less income.
taken a moment or two to look around, they tend
to gravitate towards the rear of the establishment Human variables affecting the atmosphere
(Underhill 2003). (the meeting)
Cultural differences can also be seen: Swedes
tend to sit face to face when interacting, whereas Density and crowding
English prefer to sit at right angles to each other
(Robson 2002), although this may vary depending No wonder nobody comes here to eat – it’s too
on the relationships between the individuals. crowded.
North Americans prefer seats that are anchored by
New York Yankee, Yogi Berra entering a popular
either permanent or semi-permanent structures
Manhattan diner.
such as booths, columns or planters; in Chinese
restaurants, high-status seats are those in the Quoted in The Daily Telegraph, 1 Jan 2000,
middle of the restaurant that can be viewed from p. S6.
several positions in the dining room (Robson
2002). Density and crowding are two distinctly differ-
The types of tables, seating and their configu- ent phenomena. Density refers to the physical
ration have the ability to influence profit, how condition that exists in terms of the spatial
long people spend in the dining room and how parameters available, that is, the number of
much they spend (Thompson 2003). Customers consumers present. Crowding is an unpleasant
seated in booths, i.e. anchored seats, spent signifi- feeling experienced by an individual (Hui &
cantly more than those in other types of seats; Bateson 1991), although there is evidence to

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al. 29

show that perceived crowding is also a direct create the atmosphere and is an integral part of
function of density. the overall enjoyment, and on these occasions,
Density can be further subdivided into per- some degree of overcrowding might be expected,
ceived and affective density (Eroglu & Machleit tolerated or even preferred (Hui & Bateson
1990) where: 1991). Alternatively, in some crowded settings,
the occasion can be spoilt when noise levels
Perceived density is a subjective estimate of the rise, the conditions are hot and stuffy, and
number of people, the space available and its consequently, people may not be prepared to
organisation. Therefore, factors such as the wait.
number and type of people, display equipment Research into the effects of overcrowding has
and shelves, cluttered aisles and slow check- produced mixed results. University students using
outs would be important in a retail operation: a cafeteria perceived the dining room to be
insufficient room between tables, and facilities, crowded even when only half of the seats were
such as cloakrooms and toilets, which are being used (Robson 2002). People at a baseball
unable to cope with the demand, would be stadium, where overcrowding might be expected,
important in a foodservice setting. found that those who felt crowded were less
excited about the physical facilities and perceived
Affective density is an evaluation of the per- the quality of them to be lower. As a result, this
ceived density against subjective standards and had an effect on consumers’ perceptions of quality
desired levels of information and interaction. and affected their decision to return (Wakefield
Therefore, as a result of a personal appraisal of & Blodgett 1994). Other research showed that
density in a particular situation against some where customers perceived a restaurant as being
ideal standard, feelings can range from isolated very crowded, this was attributed to high food
to crowded. quality, good restaurant image and low food
prices. A quiet restaurant was attributed to low
When faced with a situation where a large food quality, high food prices and poor restaurant
number of people are present, potential consum- image (Tse et al. 2002).
ers, because of personal circumstances, may have
to remain. As a result, they have less control over Eating alone or with others (social
the situation and will probably feel crowded. If facilitation) and the effects on consumption
they have a choice, they could remain or leave, in
which case, they have more control. The degree of The best number for a dinner party is two: myself
choice will influence the control, or perceived and a damn good headwaiter.
control they have over the situation, which has a
Nubar Gulbenkian, British industrialist and
considerable impact on the service encounter.
philanthropist.
Greater choice lowers consumers’ perceived
crowding, which in turn can influence perceptions Quoted in The Observer, 19 December 1965.
of crowding, emotions and behaviour (Hui &
Bateson 1991). Eating away from home is generally regarded as a
In retail settings, a higher density results in social activity, but in many instances, people actu-
more intense feelings of being crowded. ‘Task- ally eat alone, or at least not in the company of
orientated shoppers’, that is, those who shop for others. In a hospital, they may be surrounded by
particular items, experience more crowding and other patients and staff, but eat alone in bed.
less satisfaction than non-task-orientated shop- People out shopping by themselves may stop for
pers (Eroglu & Machleit 1990). Perceived risk something to eat or drink, and although there
and time pressures associated with the purchase may be many others in the room, they will
also increase perceived crowding, but only under perhaps sit and eat alone.
conditions of high retail density. A number of studies have determined how
On many eating out occasions, for example, eating in the company of others can influence
in a ‘pub’ or bar, a crowded room can help to both the amount and type of food consumed, and

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
30 Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al.

the time spent in consuming that food; a phenom- isolated individuals simply wanted to eat and
enon generally referred to as social facilitation is finish their food so they could leave as soon as
seen as the influence of man on man, or more possible (Edelman et al. 1986). Similar results
precisely, how and why the behaviour of one indi- were found by de Castro & de Castro (1989),
vidual affects the behaviour of others (Zajonc who also showed that satiety ratios were 30%
1965). lower when eating in company.
This is not a new phenomenon, and in a Social facilitation is not confined to single,
review of early work Zajonc (1965) draws on formal meals, but can be seen across a complete
experiments conducted as early as 1897. Interest range of meals. Significant correlations have been
in this area seems to have died out in the late shown between meal size and the number of
1930s, but more recently, it has become increas- people present for all meals consumed for break-
ingly topical and pertinent, particularly in fast, lunch and dinner, when eaten in restaurants,
organisations such as hospitals, where patients at home and elsewhere, consumed with or
tend to eat alone, and the underconsumption of without alcohol, for snacks on their own, and for
food is common (Edwards & Nash 1999). Why meals on their own (de Castro et al. 1990). Where
social facilitation should be so important is far meals are consumed in the company of others and
from clear, although it has been suggested that it with alcohol, more energy is consumed than
might be either conscious or subconscious. It without alcohol, and the meals contain more of
could, for example, be that the presence of each macronutrient (de Castro et al. 1990).
others increases levels of arousal and drive, and Eating with others has also been shown to
provides cues as to appropriate or inappropriate influence the speed of food consumption. Unbe-
behaviour (Zajonc 1965); or that more food is known to the diners, one of them acted as a
provided when meals are eaten together; that ‘model’ and consumed the meal at a predeter-
individuals might be more hungry, the atmo- mined rate. Where the ‘model’ ate the meal
sphere might be more sociable, the food might quickly, the other participants also consumed
taste better or simply that the meal might last their food significantly faster than when the
longer (Feunekes et al. 1995). ‘model’ ate the meal slowly (Rosenthal &
In one early study, adult diners (294 male and McSweeney 1979). When biscuits were con-
245 female) were observed at both midday and sumed, participants eating with a fast-eating male
evening meals in a variety of dining rooms and model ate more biscuits than those eating with a
fast food operations (Klesges et al. 1984). The slow-eating male or female model. Those who
amount of energy consumed was determined by ate with a fast female model did not differ in
three main effects: gender, where males consumed the number of biscuits eaten (Rosenthal &
more than females (835 kcal vs. 716 kcal); the McSweeney 1979).
type of restaurant, where greater amounts were The social influence appears, in part at least, to
consumed in fast food restaurants than a more operate by increasing the length of the meal but
formal dining setting (842 kcal vs. 710 kcal); and not the rate of intake. It is also independent of
company, where more food was consumed when the subjective state of hunger, emotional states of
eating in groups rather than alone (828 kcal vs. elation and anxiety, and acts independently
742 kcal). of content of the stomach and pre-meal interval
Individuals, both normal and overweight, (de Castro 1990).
eating in groups of four to five at a table in a small Even when eating alone, particularly in cafes,
dining room, or alone in small booths, were the duration of the meal is often lengthened
served a standard portion (1000 g) of lasagne, but where diners read. In coffee shops, 79% of lone
could ask for more until they were satiated. individuals read compared with 15% of those in
Regardless of their weight, eating with others groups; the former remaining significantly longer
affected the total food intake with both normal than lone individuals, although it is not clear if
and overweight individuals eating more in social they actually consumed more (Sommer & Steele
conditions where they were able to talk, linger at 1997). In ‘traditional’ restaurants, the situation is
the table and nibble at any ‘leftover’ food. The similar with 57% of lone diners reading, whilst

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al. 31

none of those in groups were doing so. The setting or by participants maintaining a food
results, consistent with other research (de Castro diary for the duration of the study, for it is diffi-
& Brewer 1992), showed that group size was cult to replicate this type of study in a practical
positively associated with meal duration, with setting whilst at the same time controlling the
lone diners remaining on average of 36.5 min and other variables. In one small hospital study
groups remaining on average of 50.3 min. (Edwards & Hartwell 2004), dietary data col-
As might be expected, more food is often served lected for three consecutive 24-hour periods show
over the weekend when there is more free time a significant increase in mean daily energy intake
and groups of people can get together. All meals when seated in a group around the table rather
eaten over the weekend are significantly larger than in or by the side of the bed.
(12%) than those eaten during the week, and As can be seen, social facilitation is important
contain significantly more macronutrients and when eating out as the amount consumed,
alcohol (de Castro 1990). however classified, snacks, breakfast, midday and
Not only is the number of people present evening, eaten in the home, in a restaurant or
relevant, but their relationship to the person con- elsewhere, with or without alcohol, will increase
suming the meal is also important. Irrespective of when consumed in the presence of others. It is far
the time of the day, meals eaten with a spouse and from proven as to exactly why consumption
family are larger and eaten faster, while meals should increase when food is consumed in the
eaten with friends are larger but of a longer dura- presence of others, but recent research in restau-
tion. Male companions had a greater impact for rants suggests that it is a function of the amount
females but not for males (de Castro 1993). When of time spent eating (Bell & Pliner 2003). Meals
a group of young adults, who normally consumed consumed with others in convivial surroundings
their meals alone, were instructed to eat with last longer, and as a result, more is consumed.
others, meal size and total energy intake increased
by 212 kcal (Redd & de Castro 1992).
Summary and conclusions
The importance and robustness of the claim
that the number of people present influences meal Recent research (Reimer & Kuehn 2005) has
intake has also been demonstrated as being linear, shown a restaurant’s quality is affected by the
described as a power function clearly demonstrat- quality of the meal, but on the other hand, atmo-
ing the orderliness and lawfulness of the phenom- sphere and concomitantly the room play a greater
enon. Using data from a number of studies, the role than previously thought in influencing
strength of the relationship has been shown consumers’ evaluation of the intangible elements,
(de Castro & Brewer 1992) and summarised in which affect service quality. Similarly, Wall &
Table 2. Berry (2007) demonstrated that a restaurant,
Much of the research into social facilitation has which has excellent food and service but a poor
been undertaken, either in a controlled laboratory ambience, can be successful, although customers’
expectations were higher when the ambience and
Table 2 The relationship between the number of other design elements were positive. The restau-
people present and meal size, and the amount rant environment gives an implicit promise with
consumed customers looking for tangible clues to help
inform and influence their expectations.
Number of Increase in
people present meal size (%) The influence of atmosphere within a room or
where food is consumed is something that is easy
2 28 to appreciate but often difficult to understand or
3 41 define, and even harder to replicate. People intu-
4 53
itively know if it is a ‘good atmosphere’ or a
5 53
6 71 ‘pleasant atmosphere’, but what precisely is
7 or more 76 involved?
A number of factors contribute towards atmo-
Source: de Castro & Brewer (1992). sphere, many of which have the ability to influ-

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
32 Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al.

ence what is chosen, how much is consumed, and Caldwell C, Hibbert SA (2002). The influence of music
how well the food and the overall eating occasion tempo and musical preference on restaurant patrons’
behaviour. Psychology & Marketing 19:895–
are enjoyed. This paper has looked at the selected
917.
aspects, under the headings of interior variables, Campbell-Smith G (1967). Marketing of the Meal
background music and noise, and odour; layout Experience: a Fundamental Approach. University of
and design variables, table layout and seating; Surrey: London.
and human variables, density and crowding, Countryman GC, Jang SC (2006). The effects of atmo-
and social facilitation, contributing towards the spherics elements on customer impressions: the case
of hotel lobbies. International Journal of Contempo-
room’s atmosphere. What is important to appre-
rary Hospitality Management 18:534–45.
ciate is that there are a vast number of other de Castro JM (1990). Social facilitation of duration and
attributes of similar, if not equal, or more size but not rate of spontaneous meal intake in
importance. humans. Physiology & Behavior 47:1129–35.
A lot of the research has, to date, focused on de Castro JM (1993). The effects of spontaneous in-
gestion of particular food or beverages in the meal
the retail sector, and whilst these results may be
patterns and overall nutrient intake of humans.
valid, they are perhaps only indicative of the Physiology & Behavior 56:1133–44.
foodservice sector. Similarly, much of the research de Castro JM, Brewer EM (1992). The amount eaten in
has considered single variables, whereas an eat- meals by humans is a power function of the number
ing occasion involves a multitude of actions. or people present. Physiology & Behavior 51:
Research in the foodservice area is sparse and 121–5.
de Castro JM, Brewer EM, Elmore DK, Orozco S
patchy, hence, lines of enquiry that help us better
(1990). Social facilitation of the spontaneous meal
understand the entire eating, and in particular, the size of humans occurs regardless of time, place,
eating out process need to be developed and alcohol or snacks. Appetite 15:89–101.
explored. de Castro JM, de Castro ES (1989). Spontaneous meal
patterns of humans: influence of the presence of
other people. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition
References 50:237–47.
Edelman B, Engell D, Bronstein P, Hirsch E (1986).
Areni CS, Kim D (1993). The influence of background Environmental effects on the intake of overweight &
music on shopping behavior: classical versus top- normal weight in man. Appetite 7:71–83.
forty music in a wine store. In: Advances in Con- Edwards JSA, Hartwell HJ (2004). A comparison of
sumer Research (eds L McAlister & M Rothschild), energy intake between eating positions in a NHS
pp. 336–40. Association for Consumer Research: hospital – a pilot study. Appetite 43:323–5.
Provo, UT. Edwards JSA, Nash AHM (1999). The nutritional
Baron RA (1997). The sweet smell of . . . helping: implications of wastage in food service management.
effects of pleasant ambient fragrance in prosocial Nutrition and Food Science 2:89–98.
behavior in shopping malls. Personality and Social Ehrichman H, Halpern JN (1988). Affect and memory:
Psychology Bulletin 23:498–503. effects of pleasant and unpleasant odors on retrieval
Bell R, Pliner PL (2003). Time to eat: the relation- of happy and unhappy memories. Journal of Person-
ship between the number of people eating and meal ality and Social Psychology 55:769–79.
duration in three lunch settings. Appetite 41: Eroglu SA, Machleit KA (1990). An empirical study of
215–8. retail crowding: antecedents and consequences.
Berman B, Evans JR (1998). Retail Management. A Journal of Retailing 66:201–21.
strategic approach, 7th edn. Prentice Hall: Upper Feinstein AH, Hinkston TS, Erdem M (2002). Explor-
Saddle River, NJ. ing the effects of music atmospherics on menu item
Bitner MJ (1992). Servicescapes: the impact of physical selection. Journal of Foodservice Business Research
surroundings on customers and employees. Journal 5:3–25.
of Marketing 56:57–71. Ferber C, Cabanac M (1987). Influence of noise on
Blackwell LR (1997). The role of olfactory cues and gustatory affective ratings and preference for sweet
their effects on food choice and acceptability. PhD and salt. Appetite 8:229–35.
Thesis, Bournemouth University. Feunekes GIJ, de Graaf C, van Staveren WA (1995).
Caldwell C, Hibbert SA (1999). Play that one again: Social facilitation of food intake is mediated by
the effect of music tempo on consumer behaviour meal duration. Physiology & Behavior 58:551–58.
in a restaurant. European Advances in Consumer Goddaer J, Abraham IL (1994). Effects of relaxing
Research 4:58–62. music on agitation during meals among nursing

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al. 33

home residents with severe cognitive impair- incongruent ambient odor on consumer decision
ment. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing 8:150– making. Journal of Consumer Research 22:229–
8. 38.
Guéguen N, Petr C (2006). Odors and consumer Morrin M, Ratneshwar S (2000). The impact of
behavior in a restaurant. International Journal of ambient scent on evaluation, attention and memory
Hospitality Management 25:335–9. for familiar and unfamiliar brands. Journal of Busi-
Health and Safety Executive (2005). Noise at Work. ness Research 49:157–65.
Guidance for Employers on the Control of Noise National Audit Office (1997). HM Prison Service:
at Work Regulations 2005. Available at: http:// Prison Catering. Report by the Comptroller and
www.hse.gov.uk/pubns/indg362.pdf (accessed 10 Auditor General. The Stationery Office: London.
May 2007). New Scientist (2000). Barely Audible. Available at:
Herrington JD, Capella LM (1996). Effects of music in http://www.newscientist.com/lastword/answer/
service environments: a field study. Journal of Ser- lwa1178 mysteries.html (accessed 28 June 2000).
vices Marketing 10:26–41. North AC, Hargreaves DJ (1996). The effects of music
Hirsch AR (1995). Effects of ambient odors on slot- on responses to a dining area. Journal of Environ-
machine usage in Las Vegas casinos. Psychology & mental Psychology 16:55–64.
Marketing 12:585–94. North AC, Hargreaves DJ, McKendrick J (1997).
Hui MK, Bateson JEG (1991). Perceived control and In-store music affects product choice. Nature
the effects of crowding and consumer choice on the 390:132.
service experience. Journal of Consumer Research North AC, Hargreaves DJ, McKendrick J (1999). The
18:174–84. influence of in-store music on wine selection. Journal
Hutton JD, Richardson LD (1995). Healthscapes: the of Applied Psychology 84:217–76.
role of the facility and physical environment on con- North AC, Shilcock A, Hargreaves DJ (2003). The
sumer attitudes, satisfaction, quality assessments and effect of musical style on restaurant customers’
behaviors. Health Care Management Review 20:48– spending. Environment and Behavior 35:712–18.
61. Oakes S (2000). The influence of the musicscape within
Kimes SE, Robson SKA (2004). The impact of restau- service environments. Journal of Services Marketing
rant table characteristics on meal duration and 14:539–56.
spending. The Cornell Hotel and Restaurant Admin- Ragneskog H, Brane G, Karlsson I, Kihlgren M (1996).
istration Quarterly 45:333–46. Influence of dinner music on food intake and symp-
Klesges RC, Bartsch D, Norwood JD, Kautzman D, toms common in dementia. Scandinavian Journal of
Haugrud S (1984). The effects of selected social and Caring Sciences 10:11–7.
environmental variables in the eating behavior of Redd M, de Castro JM (1992). Social facilitation of
adults in the natural environment. International eating: effects of social instructions on food intake.
Journal of Eating Disorders 3:35–41. Physiology & Behavior 52:749–54.
Knasko SC (1995). Pleasant odors and congruency: Reimer A, Kuehn R (2005). The impact of servicescape
effect on approach behavior. Chemical Senses on quality perception. European Journal of Market-
20:479–87. ing 39:785–808.
Kotler P (1973–1974). Atmospherics as a marketing Roballey TC et al. (1985). The effect of music on eating
tool. Journal of Retailing 49:48–64. behavior. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
Laurance C (1997). Loud music keeps the tables 23:221–2.
turning. Daily Telegraph, London, 31 December Robson SKA (2002). A review of psychology and cul-
1997. tural effects on seating behaviour and application to
Mattila AS, Wirtz J (2001). Congruency of scent and foodservice settings. Journal of Foodservice Business
music as driver of in-store evaluations and behavior. Research 592:89–107.
Journal of Retailing 77:273–89. Rosenthal B, McSweeney FK (1979). Modelling influ-
McCarron A, Tierney KJ (1989). The effect of auditory ences on eating behavior. Addictive Behavior 4:205–
stimulation on the consumption of soft drinks. Appe- 14.
tite 13:155–9. Ryan CM, Northrop-Clews CA, Knox B, Thurnham DI
Michon R, Chebat J-C, Turley LW (2005). Mall atmo- (2000). The effect of in-store music on consumer
spherics: the interaction effects of the mall environ- choice of wine. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society
ment in shopping behavior. Journal of Business 57:169A.
Research 58:576–83. Sommer R, Steele J (1997). Social effects on duration in
Milliman RE (1986). The influence of background restaurants. Appetite 29:25–30.
music on the behavior of restaurant patrons. Journal Spangenberg ER, Crowley EE, Henderson PW (1996).
of Consumer Research 13:286–9. Improving the store environment: do olfactory cues
Mitchell DJ, Kahn BE, Knasko SC (1995). affect evaluations and behaviors? Journal of Market-
There’s something in the air: effects of congruent or ing 60:67–80.

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34
34 Room–atmosphere J. S. A. Edwards et al.

Spangenberg ER, Sprott DE, Grohmann B, Tracy DL Wakefield KL, Blodgett JG (1996). The effect of ser-
(2006). Gender-congruent ambient scent influences vicescapes on customers’ behavioral intentions in
on approach and avoidance behaviors in a retail leisure service settings. Journal of Services Marketing
store. Journal of Business Research 59:1281–7. 10:45–61.
Sullivan M (2002). The impact of pitch, volume and Wall EA, Berry LL (2007). The combined effects of the
tempo on the atmospheric affects of music. Interna- physical environment and employee behaviour on
tional Journal of Retail & Distribution Management customer perception of restaurant service quality.
30:323–30. Cornell Hotel and Restaurant and Administration
Sweeney JC, Wyber F (2002). The role of cognitions Quarterly 48:59–69.
and emotions in the music–approach–avoidance Ward P, Davies BJ, Kooijman D (2007). Olfaction and
behavior relationship. Journal of Services Marketing the retail environment: examining the influence of
16:51–69. ambient scent. Service Business (Published online 12
Thompson GM (2003). Optimizing restaurant-table Jan 2007) p. 22.
configurations: specifying combinable tables. Cornell Wilson S (2003). The effects of music on perceived
Hotel and Restaurant Administration Quarterly atmosphere and purchase intentions in a restaurant.
44:53–60. Psychology of Music 31:93–112.
Tse ACB, Sin L, Yim FHK (2002). How a crowded Yalch RF, Spangenberg ER (2000). The effects of music
restaurant affects consumers’ attribution behavior. in a retail setting on real and perceived shopping
International Journal of Hospitality Management times. Journal of Business Research 49:139–47.
21:449–54. Yoshida T et al. (1997). Effects of road traffic noise on
Turley LW, Milliman RE (2000). Atmospheric effects inhabitants of Tokyo. Journal of Sound and Vibra-
on shopping behavior: a review of the experimental tion 205:517–22.
evidence. Journal of Business Research 49:193–211. Zajonc RB (1965). Social facilitation. Science 149:269–
Underhill P (2003). Why We Buy. The Science of Shop- 74.
ping, p. 119. Texere: New York. Zemke DMV, Shoemaker S (2007). Scent across a
Wakefield KL, Blodgett JG (1994). The importance of crowded room: exploring the effects of ambient scent
servicescapes in leisure service settings. Journal of on social interactions. Hospitality Management
Services Marketing 8:66–76. 26:927–40.

© 2008, The Authors


Journal compilation © 2008, Blackwell Publishing Journal of Foodservice, 19, pp. 22–34

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen