Sie sind auf Seite 1von 4

INTELLIGENCE TESTING, HISTORY OF 1127

INTELLIGENCE QUOTIENT tient, most major intelligence tests today yield IQs but not
ratio intelligence quotients. For example, since the 1960 re-
The intelligence quotient represents a measurement concept vision of the Stanford-Binet, the ratio intelligence quotient
that was used extensively in the early days of intelligence has been replaced by the deviation IQ. Major intelligence
testing but is less commonly used today. After Alfred Binet’s tests such as the Wechsler and McCarthy scales and the
death in 1911, Stern (1914) introduced the notion of a mental Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) do not
quotient, suggesting that the index of intellectual function- yield ratio intelligence quotients. However, several other
ing derived from the Binet-Simon Scale could be expressed intelligence tests in use today retain the concept of the ratio
as the ratio of a test taker’s mental age to his or her chrono- intelligence quotient, including the Leiter International
logical age multiplied by 100 to eliminate decimals (MQ = Performance Scale, the Slosson Intelligence Test, and the
100 × MA/ CA). This MQ represented something about a Quick Test. Because of the inherent limitations of the ratio
person’s rate of mental growth up to the time of the test. If intelligence quotient, IQs yielded from these tests should
examinees earned a mental age (MA) equivalent to chrono- be interpreted cautiously.
logical age (CA), their mental quotient (MQ) would be 100.
An MQ of 100 represented average performance. REFERENCES
Working at Stanford University in California, Lewis M.
Stern, W. (1914). The psychological methods of testing intelligence.
Terman developed what was to become the most widely used
Baltimore: Warwick & York.
American version of the Binet test, the Stanford-Binet. Ter-
Terman, L. M. (1916). The measurement of intelligence. Boston:
man (1916) incorporated Stern’s notion of a mental quotient
Houghton Mifflin.
but renamed it, calling it a ratio intelligence quotient, or
Tyler, L. E., & Walsh, W. B. (1979). Tests and measurements. Engle-
IQ.
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
The concept of the ratio intelligence quotient became
increasingly popular, but it was used in a number of inap- MARK E. SWERDLIK
propriate ways. Its decline over the last quarter century Illinois State University
can be attributed to a number of inherent characteristics
that have been highly criticized by measurement specialists DEVIATION IQ
and practitioners. IQ
Because the ratio intelligence quotient has minor differ- RATIO IQ
ences in the magnitude of its standard deviation at various
ages, a constant intelligence quotient from one age to an-
other does not represent the same relative status. Similarly,
even if the test taker’s relative status remained the same
from one year to another, the intelligence quotient would
have to change. This suggests that intelligence quotients at INTELLIGENCE TESTING, HISTORY OF
different age levels are not comparable statistically (Tyler &
Walsh, 1979). For example, a very bright child could obtain a Intelligence testing, although called by many different
higher IQ at age 12 than at age 6, even if the child’s growth names and used in many different forms, has been around
rate was unchanged. This difference would simply be due for many centuries (Anastasi, 1982). The Chinese have
to the differences in the variability or standard deviations, been using mental tests for 3,000 years, and in the sev-
with the variability of the IQ distribution being greater for enth and eighth centuries the Imperial Court established
12 year olds than for 6 year olds. tests of speaking and writing and verbal and nonverbal
Critics also point to the conceptual difficulty of the ratio reasoning that are similar to tasks on today’s tests. The
intelligence quotient. For example, a 5 year old with a men- ancient Greeks, followers of Socrates, and universities in
tal age of 6 and a 10 year old with a mental age of 12 would the Middle Ages, all developed methods of assessing intel-
both have identical intelligence quotients of 120. However, lectual skills.
the 6 year old is a year advanced in mental age while the As summarized by Kaufman (1983), the 1800s saw the
10 year old is 2 years advanced. beginning of the development of ideas about mental abili-
Another criticism of the intelligence quotient relates to ties and methods of measuring intelligence; these ideas
its inability to describe adult intelligence (Tyler & Walsh, formed the foundation for contemporary assessment. Not
1979). Critics suggest that like physical growth, adult men- surprisingly, the scholars involved in the roots of intelligence
tal growth lacks the predictable regularity characteristic testing were concerned with the two extremes of ability.
of the mental development of children. Age standards lack Jean Esquirol in the early 1800s and Edouard Seguin in
meaning after the mid-teens, rendering mental age and the mid-1800s, two French physicians, studied intelligence
therefore the intelligence quotient concept meaningless. of mentally retarded individuals. Francis Galton, in the
Owing to these criticisms of the ratio intelligence quo- mid-to-late 1800s, focused on the ability of men of genius.
1128 INTELLIGENCE TESTING, HISTORY OF

In the 1890s, James McKeen Cattell brought intelligence testing were that he “discarded the specific test for the spe-
testing to the United States. cific ability and took a group of tests which seemed to cover
Esquirol’s contributions included distinguishing between in general the chief psychological characteristics that go to
those people with very low intelligence, or the mentally make up intelligence. And, further, as the norm or standard
retarded, and those people with emotional disturbances. of intelligence he took what the average child at each age
He indicated that there is a hierarchy of retardation along could do” (Pintner & Patterson, 1925, p. 7).
a continuum, and coined terms like imbecile and idiot to The Binet-Simon Scale, including the 1911 revision
describe different levels of mental deficiency. Although that extended through adulthood, was almost immediately
Esquirol studied several procedures, he concluded that a adapted and translated in the United States. The most suc-
person’s use of language is the most dependable criterion cessful revision was Terman’s Stanford-Binet Scale in 1916.
for determining intelligence, a philosophy that is apparent Terman carefully standardized his scale and introduced
on many intelligence tests today. the application of the term intelligence quotient (mental
Seguin rejected the notion that mental retardation is in- age divided by chronological age multiplied by 100). The
curable and served as a pioneer in education for the mentally Stanford-Binet was widely adopted by individual examiners
retarded. Unlike Esquirol, Seguin stressed the importance in the United States, and it is still popular today. The Binet
of sensory discrimination and motor control as aspects of was revised and restandardized in 1937, revised in 1960,
intelligence. He developed procedures that were adopted and again restandardized in 1972. The ratio IQ, although
by later developers of performance and nonverbal intel- retained in the 1937 Binet, was replaced by the deviation IQ
ligence tests. An example is the Seguin Form board, which (a standard score with a mean of 100 and standard deviation
requires rapid placement of variously shaped blocks into of 16) for the 1960 and 1972 Stanford-Binets. A thoroughly
their correct holes. new version of the Binet was released in 2004.
The English biologist Galton was primarily responsible David Wechsler, in 1939, was the first to challenge the
for developing the first comprehensive individual intelli- Stanford-Binet monopoly on individual intelligence tests by
gence test. As part of his research of men of genius and the publishing the Wechsler-Bellevue Scale. Wechsler, like Binet,
heredity of intelligence, he administered tasks of sensory included the concept of global intelligence in his scale, but
discrimination and sensory motor coordination in his An- instead of having one score, as did the Stanford-Binet, he
thropometric Laboratory. His belief that intelligence comes included three scores: a verbal IQ, a performance IQ, and a
to us through the senses led to the development of tasks full-scale IQ. Wechsler did not employ the methodology of
such as weight discrimination, reaction time, strength of the Stanford-Binet, which used a large number of brief and
squeeze, and visual discrimination. primarily verbal tasks. His scale was limited to a small num-
In the early 1890s, Cattell, an assistant in Galton’s ber of longer tasks, half of them verbal and half nonverbal.
laboratory, established similar laboratories in the United Several versions of the Wechsler scales have been published
States. During this time, Cattell used the term mental test since his Wechsler-Bellevue, and the types of tasks on later
for the first time in the psychological literature. Cattell versions are virtually identical to the first scale. The main
shared Galton’s view that intelligence is best measured sources of Wechsler’s verbal tasks were the Binet and the
through sensory tasks, but expanded his mentor’s ideas by Army Group Examination Alpha; his performance tests came
emphasizing that test administration must be standardized primarily from the Army Group Examination Beta and the
so that results are comparable from person to person and Army Individual Performance Scale Examination.
time to time. The widespread use of group intelligence tests began,
In the early 1900s, significant advances were made in like the first Binet Scale, to meet a practical need (Anas-
both individual and group intelligence testing (Sattler, 1982; tasi, 1982). The entry of the United States into World War
Vane & Motta, 1984). In France, Alfred Binet, assisted by I in 1917 required a rapid means of classifying 1.5 million
Theophile Simon and Victor Henri, rejected Galton’s no- recruits for assignment into different types of training, dis-
tions about the sensory and motor aspects of intelligence charge from service, and officer ability. A committee of the
and claimed that tests of higher mental processes more American Psychological Association, headed by Robert M.
effectively distinguish among the individual differences in Yerkes, was directed to develop two group intelligence
people’s intellectual abilities. This group developed numer- tests; these came to be known as the Army Alpha and Army
ous tests of complex intellectual functions such as memory, Beta. The former, a verbal test modeled after the Binet,
comprehension, imagination, and moral sentiments. The was designed for general use with literate recruits; the
specific appointment by the French minister of public in- latter employed nonverbal items and was designed for il-
struction to study the education of retarded children led literate recruits, or those recent immigrants who did not
to the development of the individually administered Bi- speak English well. (The Army Individual Performance
net-Simon Scale in 1905, constructed to separate mentally Scale, mentioned previously, was given to those recruits
retarded and normal children in the Paris public schools. who could not be tested validly on either the Army Alpha
Two of the key aspects of Binet’s approach to intelligence or Army Beta.)
INTELLIGENCE TESTING, HISTORY OF 1129

The tests were released for civilian use soon after the in education, business, government, and military, in cir-
war ended. Because of the belief that intelligence tests were cumstances where it is feasible to obtain valid test data
better than teacher evaluations for identifying abilities, from many individuals at once; they are useful as well with
these tests became widely accepted in education (Vane & individuals who are able to take a test by themselves with-
Motta, 1984). A short time after, revisions of the Army tests, out need of an examiner. In contrast, individual intelligence
as well as new group intelligence tests that used the Army tests are used in clinics and special education centers, where
tests as models, were being administered to thousands of an intensive study of individual clients is needed and a
preschool through graduate students all over the country, trained examiner is necessary to secure valid test results.
and to special adult groups such as employees or prisoners. The reasons for using group and individual tests are many
The group format was attractive because it allowed the and provide a basis for understanding the type of informa-
testing of many individuals simultaneously and it incorpo- tion provided by the two testing formats. A summary of these
rated simple administration procedures that required little reasons, as reported by Anastasi (1982), follows.
examiner training. The rapid growth of group intelligence Group tests can be administered to a large number of
testing resulted in the development of 37 group tests in only individuals at the same time by using booklets of printed
5 years! (Pintner, 1923). items and forms for the examinee to indicate his or her an-
A new surge in group intelligence testing occurred in swer. The training and experience required by the examiner
1958, after the launching of Sputnik. The passage of the is minimal, as most group tests only require the examiner
National Defense Education Act provided funds for states to to read simple instructions and accurately keep time. The
test the abilities of schoolchildren and identify outstanding minimal role of the examiner in group testing provides more
students. This testing was facilitated by the development uniform testing conditions than in individual testing.
of optical scanning for test scoring by Lindquist in 1955. Objective scoring is a key aspect of group tests. Test
Also in 1958, the National Merit Scholarship program was items are usually multiple choice, true-false, or some other
established to select exceptional high-school students. Ac- type that produces responses that can be scored as correct
cording to Vane and Motta (1984), another significant up- or incorrect with no deliberation. Items on group tests can
turn in testing happened in the late 1970s, and this interest usually be scored by a clerk or a computer. In addition,
continues to this day. group tests typically include answer sheets, separate from
Intelligence tests, both group and individual, are used in the test booklets that contain the items, allowing economical
many different ways today. The largest users of intelligence reuse of test booklets.
tests are schools. Group intelligence tests are used at the Because group tests can be administered to large groups
preschool and kindergarten levels to distinguish children of individuals at the same time, larger numbers of indi-
who are ready to participate in educational activities from viduals can be used in the standardization programs for
those who need remedial preparation. At the elementary, group tests than for individual tests. Group test norms
middle, and high-school levels, group tests are used to iden- are generally better established because they are based on
tify exceptional students, and to aid in forming homoge- standardization samples of 100,000 to 200,000 instead of
neous ability groups within classrooms. Group intelligence the 1,000 to 4,000 used for individual tests.
tests are commonly used as one criterion for admission into On the other hand, individual intelligence tests have
colleges and universities. Individual intelligence tests have several characteristics that make them suitable for a variety
been administered for over half a century by well-trained of clinical purposes. In individual testing, the examiner has
clinicians for psychological, psychoeducational, and neuro- the opportunity to obtain cooperation, establish rapport,
psychological diagnosis. The passage of the Education of All and enhance motivation of the examinee. The trained ex-
Handicapped Children Act of 1975 (PL 94-142) and subse- aminer in individual testing detects, reports, and uses in
quent revisions resulted in the common use of individual the interpretation of test scores the many characteristics
intelligence tests as part of larger assessment batteries for of the examinee that may affect test performance such as
the placement of children in special education programs anxiety, fatigue, and problem-solving style. In addition,
for the mentally retarded, learning disabled, emotionally some individuals such as emotionally disturbed and men-
disturbed, and so forth, and for the development of indi- tally retarded children and adults may perform better on
vidual educational programs for these children. However, individual tests than on group tests. Since most group tests
the latest revision in 2004 signaled less reliance on testing require the examinee to read instructions and test items,
and more reliance on prereferral intervention and alternate individually administered tests, which demand little or no
forms of assessment such as curriculum-based assessment. reading, are especially useful for learning-disabled and
For adults, group and individual intelligence tests are used retarded individuals, and others who may have reading
in a variety of settings, including business and industry, problems.
prisons, mental health centers, hospitals, and private clini- Individual intelligence tests, because they typically in-
cal practice. clude short questions that require oral and open-ended
Group intelligence tests find their principal application responses, allow examinees to give creative and original
1130 INTELLIGENCE TESTING

responses to items. In individual testing, examinees are Sattler, J. M. (1982). Assessment of children’s intelligence and spe-
not limited to selecting one of four multiple choice answers cial abilities (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
or indicating if an item is true or false. The contents of an Thorndike, R. L. (1985, April). An introduction to the revised
examinee’s response on an individual intelligence test can Stanford-Binet for school psychology educators. Paper presented
therefore be analyzed in order to generate hypotheses about, at the conference of the Trainers of School Psychologists, Las
for example, the examinee’s creativity, style of thinking, Vegas, NV.
cognitive development, or defense mechanisms. Vane, T. R., & Motta, R. W. (1984). Group intelligence tests. In
Another aspect of individual intelligence testing concerns G. Goldstein & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of psychological
assessment (pp. 100–116). New York: Pergamon.
the flexibility of administration. On a group test, an exam-
inee is required to respond to all items, or as many items as
ALAN S. KAUFMAN
he or she can in a certain time limit. On an individual test, Yale University Medical School
testing time is more effectively used because the examinee
is administered only those items in the range appropriate PATTI L. HARRISON
to his or her ability level. This characteristic of individual University of Alabama
tests helps avoid the boredom an examinee may have when
working on items that are too easy or the frustration of INTELLIGENCE
working on items that are too difficult. INTELLIGENCE QUOTIENT
Intelligence testing has been a controversial topic since the INTELLIGENT TESTING
1960s (Kaufman, 1979). The most pressing issues that are de- SEE SPECIFIC TESTS
bated within both professional and public forums concern test
bias, the influence of heredity versus environment on IQ, race
differences in test scores, and disproportionate placement of
minority children into special education classes such as those
for the retarded or gifted. These issues have been the subject
of research, debate, federal guidelines, laws, and lawsuits. INTELLIGENCE TESTING
Just as major law cases differ on whether intelligence tests
are unfair to minority children (Larry P. v. Riles; PASE deci- The practice of formally testing skills and abilities dates
sion in Chicago), so do professionals in the field of intelligence back nearly 3,000 years (Wang, 1993). Moreover, individual
testing continue to disagree on these issues. It is likely that differences in human performance have been discussed
the future will be filled with arguments on the appropriate among history’s greatest thinkers, including Socrates, Plato,
use of intelligence tests (Fletcher & Reschly, 2005; Kavale, and Darwin. Despite a history of interest in exploring hu-
Kaufman, Naglieri, & Hale, 2005) and claims that they should man abilities, standardized and validated intelligence tests
be banned; at the same time, it is equally certain that there first were developed in 1905 by Binet and Simon to help
will continue to be a proliferation of new and revised instru- predict how well children would achieve in school (French
ments of both the individual and group variety. & Hale, 1990). Since then, the intelligence testing move-
ment has gained considerable momentum as a result of the
REFERENCES need to differentiate among individuals when providing
educational services (French & Hale, 1990).
Anastasi, A. (1982). Psychological testing (5th ed.). New York: Mac-
millan. Intelligence testing is a means to observe the actual
performance of individuals under standardized conditions.
Fletcher, J., & Reschly, D. J. (2005). Changing procedures for iden-
tifying learning disabilities: The danger of perpetually old ideas. Its purpose is to accurately assess cognitive strengths and
The School Psychologist, 59, 1, 10–15. weaknesses. However, individual scores are samples of
Kaufman, A. S. (1979). Intelligent testing with the WISC-R. New behavior. In addition, many other nonintellective factors
York: Wiley. influence everyday functioning and behavior. As a result,
Kaufman, A. S. (1983). Intelligence: Old concepts—new perspec- intelligence testing is used to make inferences and predic-
tives. In G. W. Hynd (Ed.), The school psychologist: An introduc- tions about an individual’s ability rather than serve as a
tion (pp. 95–117). Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press. direct indicator of the traits and capacities of an individual
Kavale, K., Kaufman, A. S., Naglieri, J. A., & Hale, J. B. (2003). (Sattler, 1992).
Changing procedures for identifying learning disabilities: The Standardized measures of intelligence are used by psy-
danger of poorly supported ideas. The School Psychologist, 59, chologists on a daily basis. Data from intelligence tests
1, 16–25. often are used to predict school performance, especially
Pintner, R. (1923). Intelligence testing. New York: Holt, Rinehart, achievement. Intelligence tests do this fairly well (Neisser
& Winston. et al., 1996). Approximately 25 percent of the variance as-
Pintner, R., & Patterson, D. G. (1925). A scale of performance. New sociated with achievement can be attributed to intelligence.
York: Appleton. Although 75 percent of the variance in school performance

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen