Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

REVIEW ■

The Control of Voluntary Eye Movements:


New Perspectives
RICHARD J. KRAUZLIS
Systems Neurobiology Laboratory
Salk Institute for Biological Studies

Primates use two types of voluntary eye movements to track objects of interest: pursuit and saccades.
Traditionally, these two eye movements have been viewed as distinct systems that are driven automatical-
ly by low-level visual inputs. However, two sets of findings argue for a new perspective on the control of
voluntary eye movements. First, recent experiments have shown that pursuit and saccades are not con-
trolled by entirely different neural pathways but are controlled by similar networks of cortical and subcorti-
cal regions and, in some cases, by the same neurons. Second, pursuit and saccades are not automatic
responses to retinal inputs but are regulated by a process of target selection that involves a basic form of
decision making. The selection process itself is guided by a variety of complex processes, including atten-
tion, perception, memory, and expectation. Together, these findings indicate that pursuit and saccades
share a similar functional architecture. These points of similarity may hold the key for understanding how
neural circuits negotiate the links between the many higher order functions that can influence behavior and
the singular and coordinated motor actions that follow. NEUROSCIENTIST 11(2):124–137, 2005. DOI:
10.1177/1073858404271196
KEY WORDS Pursuit, Saccade, Eye movement, Attention, Perception

Primates make two kinds of voluntary eye movements to and cognitive processing capabilities of the primate cen-
place the retinal images of objects of interest onto the tral nervous system. The importance of these higher
fovea and to keep them there: saccades and pursuit. order processes, and the complexity of the underlying
Saccades are discrete ballistic movements that direct the mechanisms, pose both challenges and opportunities for
eyes quickly toward a visual target, thereby translating using voluntary eye movements as a model for under-
the image of the target from an eccentric retinal location standing the neural circuits involved in visuomotor con-
to the fovea within tens of milliseconds. Pursuit is a con- trol. This review highlights some recent findings that
tinuous movement that rotates the eyes smoothly and provide new perspectives on the functional organization
slowly to compensate for any motion of the visual target of these voluntary motor systems.
and thus minimizes the drift of the target’s image across
the retina that might otherwise blur the image and com- The Neural Pathways for
promise visual acuity. Pursuit and Saccades
Much of what we have learned about voluntary eye
movements over the past 40 years has involved treating Although the pursuit and saccadic systems have tradi-
these movements as visuomotor reflexes that act to min- tionally been viewed as anatomically distinct, more
imize visual “error” signals. Indeed, many species can recent evidence indicates that there is considerable over-
generate smooth optokinetic eye movements, which help lap in the neural pathways for pursuit and saccades. Both
stabilize the eyes during head and body movements by systems involve a similar set of areas in the cerebral cor-
minimizing the motion of the entire visual surround. tex (Fig. 1). For saccades, these cortical areas evaluate
However, when we move about in most natural environ- and update the locations of potential targets and provide
ments, it is impossible to eliminate the slip of images motor commands for saccades and include the lateral
across the retina. Instead, choices need to be made about intraparietal area (LIP), the frontal eye fields (FEFs),
which visual inputs have top priority. Primates appear to and the supplementary eye fields (SEFs). For pursuit,
be unmatched in their ability to identify individual cortical areas are involved in processing the visual
objects within a complex, dynamic visual scene and to motion and other control signals necessary for pursuit
track selected objects with their eyes. Voluntary eye and include the middle temporal (MT) and medial supe-
movements in primates are therefore not just a motor rior temporal (MST) areas and subregions of areas LIP,
phenomenon but depend on the sophisticated sensory FEF, and SEF. Thus, many of the same cortical areas are
involved in the control of both pursuit and saccades, but
each area contains separate subregions for the two types
Address correspondence to: Richard J. Krauzlis, Salk Institute for
Biological Studies, 10010 North Torrey Pines Road, La Jolla, CA of movements, and the corresponding subregions are
92037 (e-mail: rich@salk.edu). interconnected to form a closely matched pair of cortical

124 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Voluntary Eye Movements


Copyright © 2005 Sage Publications
ISSN 1073-8584
the SC. As with the direct projections to the brain stem,
the pathways through the basal ganglia are well estab-
lished for saccades but have only recently been demon-
strated for pursuit (Cui and others 2003).
In summary, although pursuit and saccades have his-
torically been viewed as anatomically distinct systems,
new data argue that they have a similar functional archi-
tecture and involve many of the same brain regions,
including the brain stem, cerebellum, superior collicu-
lus, and the cerebral cortex. In this admittedly selective
review of the recent literature, we will start at the circuits
that form and regulate the motor commands and wind
our way up through the areas that evaluate and extract
the signals needed to trigger and guide the movements.

Brain Stem
Fig. 1. Outline of the pathways for pursuit and saccadic eye
movements. Schematic diagram of the descending pathways It has been known for some decades that the motor com-
are depicted on a lateral view of the monkey brain. Shaded mands for saccades are constructed primarily by a circuit
regions indicate specific areas within the cerebral cortex, basal in the brain stem that generates the burst of neural activ-
ganglia, cerebellum, and brain stem, and arrows indicate the
anatomical connections between these areas. Regions demar- ity necessary to cause the rapid changes in muscle
cated with dashed lines indicate structures normally covered by force that propel saccades. The elements of this
the cerebral cortex. For clarity, not all relevant areas are depict- circuit are spread across several nuclei in the pons and
ed (e.g., ascending pathways are omitted), and arrows do not mesencephalon—the paramedian pontine reticular for-
always correspond to direct anatomical connections. CN = cau-
date nucleus (basal ganglia); FEF = frontal eye field; LIP = later-
mation (PPRF), the rostral interstitial nucleus of the
al intraparietal area; MT = middle temporal area; MST = medial medial longitudinal fasciculus (riMLF), and the nucleus
superior temporal area; PMN = brain stem premotor nuclei raphe interpositus (nRIP)—and contain several classes
(PPRF, riMLF, cMRF); PON = precerebellar pontine nuclei; SC = of saccade-related neurons (Luschei and Fuchs 1972;
superior colliculus (intermediate and deep layers); SEF = sup- Keller 1974; Sparks and Sides 1974; Henn and Cohen
plementary eye field; SNr = substantia nigra pars reticulate;
Verm = oculomotor vermis (cerebellum, lobules VI and VII); VN = 1976; Raybourn and Keller 1977; Van Gisbergen and
vestibular nuclei; VPF = ventral paraflocculus (cerebellum). others 1981; Henn and others 1984). Short-lead burst
neurons emit a burst of spikes whose precise timing
determines the amplitude of the saccade. Long-lead
burst neurons exhibit a prelude of activity before emit-
ting a saccade-related burst. Pause neurons in the nRIP
networks (Tian and Lynch 1996a, 1996b). Functional discharge steadily but stop firing during some or all sac-
imaging studies in humans also support the idea of par- cades (omnipause neurons [OPNs]). Several models
allel but distinct cortical pathways for pursuit and sac- (e.g., Scudder 1988) have suggested how these neurons
cades (Petit and Haxby 1999; Rosano and others 2002). might participate in saccade generation. A trigger signal,
These multiple cortical areas influence eye motor con- probably from the SC, causes OPNs to pause their firing
trol through several descending pathways. First, there are momentarily, which then disinhibits burst neurons. This
direct projections to eye-movement-related structures in disinhibition evokes a burst whose duration corresponds
the brain stem such as the superior colliculus (SC) and to the amplitude of the saccade; the burst duration is
premotor nuclei in the reticular formation (PMN). These controlled by a negative feedback circuit and is adap-
pathways, which have figured prominently in the control tively regulated in conjunction with the cerebellum.
of saccades, have been recently demonstrated to exist for New evidence indicates that parts of this brain stem
pursuit cortical areas as well (Yan and others 2001). circuit for saccades are also involved in the control of
There are also several less direct routes. One pathway pursuit. The primary brain stem nuclei for controlling
passes through the pontine nuclei to eye movement horizontal and vertical gaze (the PPRF, riMLF, and
regions of the cerebellum (oculomotor vermis, ventral cMRF) all receive direct inputs from the pursuit subre-
paraflocculus [VPF]), which access the output motor gion of the FEF as well as from the saccade-related sub-
nuclei for eye movements by projections to the vestibu- region (Yan and others 2001). Recent recording studies
lar nucleus and other brain stem motor nuclei (PMN). have shown that subsets of the neurons in these nuclei
For pursuit, this cortico-ponto-cerebellar route has been have pursuit-related as well as saccade-related activity.
traditionally considered the primary control pathway, For example, some burst neurons in the PPRF are active
whereas for saccades, it has been viewed primarily as a only during saccades, but a second category of burst
regulatory side loop. There are also descending path- neurons is active during both saccades and pursuit
ways involving nuclei of the basal ganglia, such as the (Missal and Keller 2001). Similarly, in the riMLF of the
caudate nucleus and the substantia nigra pars reticulata, cat, some burst neurons fire in relationship to eye veloc-
which exert their influence on eye movements through ity not only during saccades but also during pursuit

Volume 11, Number 2, 2005 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 125


(Missal and Keller 2001). Perhaps most surprising are
the recent findings suggesting that OPNs play a role in
pursuit. About half of the OPNs show significant
decreases in activity during the onset of pursuit as well
as pauses for saccades; they do not completely stop fir-
ing as for saccades but reduce their activity by about
one-third (Missal and Keller 2002). Microstimulation in
the region of the OPNs has long been known to halt sac-
cades, but recent experiments show that such microstim-
ulation also strongly decelerates pursuit (Fig. 2),
although it does not completely stop pursuit (Missal and
Keller 2002).
These studies indicate that the construction of the
motor commands for pursuit and saccades involves
shared circuitry in the brain stem, and Figure 3 shows
one candidate scheme for how the motor circuits for the Fig. 2. Microstimulation in the region of the omnipause neurons
two movements might be related. Analogous to the way (OPNs) decelerates pursuit eye velocity. Average eye velocity
that OPNs are believed to gate the occurrence of sac- on trials with microstimulation (thick solid line, n = 9) is com-
cades through inhibitory effects on excitatory burst neu- pared to average eye velocity on trials without microstimulation
(thin solid line, n = 8). During the period of microstimulation
rons, OPNs could regulate the gain of pursuit through (indicated by the orange bar), eye velocity is reduced com-
their inhibitory effect on pursuit neurons in the nucleus pared to the trials with no stimulation. Thinner lines and dashed
prepositus hypoglossi (NPH) and the medial vestibular lines indicate 95% confidence intervals of the mean eye veloc-
nuclei (MVN). Another novel class of pursuit-related ity. The vertical arrow indicates the onset of the 40 deg/s right-
ward target motion. Adapted from Missal and Keller (2002,
neurons, the burst neurons in the PPRF and riMLF, p 1889). Used with permission from the American Physiological
might acquire their smooth-eye-velocity modulation Society.
through excitatory inputs from the PNs. By inhibiting
the OPNs and completing a loop with the pursuit neu-
rons in the NPH/MVN, these neurons might act to latch
the pursuit system in an “on” state. deficits of large saccades, with relative sparing of both
Many important details about this putative gating pursuit and small saccades (Henn and others 1984;
mechanism remain unknown, but a circuit with these Hanson and others 1986), but larger lesions of the retic-
features could account for several properties of pursuit ular formation result in a conjugate gaze palsy that
and saccades. If the gating of pursuit and saccades affects both saccades and pursuit (Bogousslavsky and
involved shared circuitry in the brain stem, this would Meienberg 1987). Thus, depending on the size of the
provide a straightforward way to coordinate and regulate lesion, brain stem damage appears to limit the amplitude
the triggering of pursuit and saccades, consistent with range of the eye movements that can be generated, rather
behavioral evidence that there is a shared inhibitory than the type of eye movements.
mechanism for pursuit and saccades (Kornylo and others
2003). On the other hand, the difference in the level of Cerebellum
disinhibition associated with the two movements could
provide flexibility in determining what is required to The cerebellar cortex and deep cerebellar nuclei play a
trigger the two types of movements, consistent with the crucial role in supporting the accuracy and adaptation of
observations that pursuit generally has a shorter latency voluntary eye movements. Although several regions
than saccades and that pursuit and saccades usually but have been implicated in the control of eye movements,
not always agree in their choice of a target (Krauzlis and two areas are especially well understood: the VPF and
others 1999; Liston and Krauzlis 2003). The graded inhi- the midline oculomotor vermis. Output neurons in the
bition of the OPNs during pursuit would also be predict- VPF project directly to oculomotor nuclei in the brain
ed to produce a smoothly graded disinhibition of the stem, whereas in the vermis, output neurons exert their
NPH/MVN neurons, consistent with the suggestion from effect via projections to the fastigial oculomotor region
behavioral experiments that there is a variable gain con- (FOR), a deep cerebellar nucleus.
troller in the pathways for pursuit eye movements Damage to the cerebellum does not eliminate eye
(Grasse and Lisberger 1992; Krauzlis and Lisberger movements but renders them highly variable and inaccu-
1994a; Keating and Pierre 1996; Krauzlis and Miles rate. Ablation of the VPF and adjacent flocculus causes
1996c). large and lasting deficits in smooth eye movements and
These recent findings seemingly contradict clinical the ability to maintain fixation (Zee and others 1981;
observations that damage to the brain stem reticular for- Rambold and others 2002). These dramatic effects may
mation causes selective palsy for saccades (Hanson and reflect the close association of the VPF with the circuit
others 1986). However, the discrepancy is resolved when in the brain stem that integrates eye position signals
one compares lesions of different sizes. Smaller brain (Cannon and Robinson 1987). As illustrated by the
stem lesions in humans and monkeys can result in examples in Figure 4, lesions of the vermis or FOR dis-

126 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Voluntary Eye Movements


Fig. 4. Disruption of the timing, accuracy, and adaptation of
saccades after lesions of the cerebellar oculomotor vermis.
Traces show horizontal eye position as a function of time
(aligned with respect to saccade onset) during saccades before
and after lesions of the oculomotor vermis. In this experiment,
saccades were adapted by presenting a 10-degree forward
step of the target, followed by a 3-degree backward step.
Prelesion, the animal showed a decrease in saccade amplitude
late in adaptation (black arrow) as compared to early in adap-
tation. Postlesion, there was a marked increase in the variabili-
ty of saccade amplitudes (blue arrow) and an increase in laten-
cy for corrective saccades (orange arrow). These effects
persisted through the late phases of adaptation. From Takagi
and others (1998, p 1925). Used with permission from the
American Physiological Society.
Fig. 3. Possible diagram of how oculomotor nuclei in the brain
stem contribute to both pursuit and saccades. Excitatory
synapses are shown with small white circles; inhibitory synaps-
es are shown with small black circles. Note that there are two ers 1994). These bursts reflect a push-pull arrangement
distinct types of inputs to the circuit: gating signals that are
shared by pursuit and saccades and separate drive signals in which the same neurons that provide an accelerative
conveying location and motion information. The omnipause command for movements in one direction also provide a
neurons (OPNs) play a crucial role in this circuit by regulating braking signal for movements in the other direction.
when the descending drive signals are allowed to access the Similarly, neurons in the VPF exhibit overshoots in fir-
final motor pathways. EBN = excitatory burst neuron; NPH,
MVN = pursuit-related neurons in the nucleus prepositus
ing rate when pursuit eye velocity increases or decreas-
hypoglossi and the medial vestibular nuclei; OMN = ocular es. These transient overshoots in the VPF also operate in
motor neurons; trig = interneuron that inhibits OPNs, thereby a push-pull fashion and appear to reflect a calculated attempt
triggering a saccade and possibly pursuit; latch = interneuron to compensate for the sluggish mechanics of the eye mus-
that putatively keeps OPNs inhibited during the saccade and cles and orbital tissues (Krauzlis and Lisberger 1994b;
pursuit movements.
Krauzlis 2000). The timing and size of these bursts change
after adaptation of eye movements and for eye move-
ments made from different orbital positions (Kleine and
others 2003; Scudder and McGee 2003), consistent with
rupt the timing, accuracy, and dynamics of saccades and
the idea that the cerebellar output acts to maintain the
also the ability to adapt saccades (Robinson and others
accuracy of eye movements under a variety of condi-
1993; Takagi and others 1998; Barash and others 1999).
tions. Although the discharge of individual cerebellar
After damage to the vermis and FOR, saccades also
neurons is variable, the population response can provide
exhibit a dysmetria that depends on eye position, sug-
a motor command that is very precise; changes in the
gesting that the cerebellar signals normally act to coun-
contributions of individual neurons could therefore pro-
terbalance the changing mechanical forces encountered
vide a mechanism for adjusting the size and timing of
by the eye at different positions in the orbit.
eye movements (Krauzlis 2000; Thier and others 2000).
The activity of neurons in the cerebellum provides
insight into how the motor commands for eye move-
ments are shaped into their final forms. During pursuit Superior Colliculus
and saccades, neurons in the FOR emit an early burst of The SC has been traditionally described as a motor map
spikes for contraversive movements and a later burst of of saccade end points, but several lines of evidence argue
spikes for ipsiversive movements (Ohtsuka and Noda that the SC comprises a map of motor goals rather than
1991; Fuchs and others 1993, 1994; Helmchen and oth- the specific movement required to achieve that goal.

Volume 11, Number 2, 2005 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 127


First, the locus of activity in the SC does not uniquely orienting movements with a series of smaller saccades in
determine the amplitude of the eye movement that is rapid succession rather than with a single large saccade.
made. Neurons in the SC fire differently for saccades During these multistep movements, activity in the SC is
made to moving targets compared to saccades made to initially at the site corresponding to the retinal location
stationary targets, arguing that the SC neurons specify of the eccentric target and then progresses toward more
the initial retinotopic location and that additional circuits central sites in a single sweep, even though the move-
are responsible for getting the saccade to land accurate- ment itself is achieved with multiple saccades. As a
ly on the target (Keller and others 1996). SC neurons result, the locus of activity in the SC does not match the
also fire differently for saccades made to remembered amplitudes of the individual saccades used to acquire the
targets compared to saccades made directly to visual tar- target but instead indicates the remaining distance to the
gets, again indicating that activity in the SC does not target (Bergeron and others 2003). The complementary
determine the exact metrics of the saccade (Stanford and pattern holds for neurons in the rostral SC, which repre-
Sparks 1994). sent the central visual field and tend to be active during
SC activity also does not determine whether saccades fixation (Munoz and others 1991; Munoz and Wurtz
will be accomplished with the eye alone or with a com- 1993). These rostral SC neurons remain inactive during
bination of the eye and head. When the head is immobi- the multistep movement, even though the movement
lized, activity in the SC is associated with eye saccades pauses between each small saccade of the sequence,
with a specific direction and amplitude (Robinson 1972; resuming their tonic activity only as the sequence draws
Schiller and Stryker 1972). However, when the head is to a close and the target is acquired (Bergeron and
free to move, SC neurons exhibit activity that is closely Guitton 2002). Activity in the SC motor map therefore
related to the amplitude and direction of combined eye- does not appear to be exclusively involved with control-
head movements rather than to either the eye or head ling saccade end points but serves a more general func-
component alone (Freedman and Sparks 1997). In these tion associated with specifying the goal for orienting
unrestrained conditions, SC stimulation produces coor- movements.
dinated movements of both the eyes and head (Freedman One possibility that has gained support is the idea that
and others 1996). The amplitudes of these combined the SC plays a role in representing and selecting the tar-
movements are larger compared to those evoked with the gets for orienting movements. For example, decreasing
head fixed because the evoked eye movements are the the probability that a visual stimulus will be the target,
same whether or not the head is free to move. by adding a variable number of irrelevant stimuli to the
Consequently, the standard depiction of the SC motor display, decreases the visually-evoked and tonic activity
map obtained with the head restrained is distorted of many SC neurons (Basso and Wurtz 1997, 1998).
because it systematically underestimates the amplitudes These changes are correlated with the latencies of the
of encoded gaze movements. saccades that follow but are not related to the amplitude
The SC also plays a role in the control of pursuit eye or peak velocity of the saccade. Similar effects are found
movements. Activation and inactivation of the rostral with a single visual stimulus by varying the probability,
SC, which represents the central visual field, modifies between blocks of trials, that the target will appear in the
the metrics of pursuit, demonstrating a causal link neuron’s response field (Dorris and Munoz 1998). These
between SC activity and pursuit (Basso and others effects of stimulus probability are especially evident
2000). Many neurons in the rostral SC modulate their either before or soon after the visual stimuli are present-
firing rates during pursuit eye movements as well as dur- ed, indicating that prior information may be especially
ing small saccades (Krauzlis and others 1997, 2000). influential during the period of uncertainty that prevails
This activity is not simply a visual response because it before unambiguous stimulus information is available to
persists in the absence of a visual target (Krauzlis 2001). guide the eye movement choice.
This activity also does not convey motion signals for During the latent period after the candidate targets
pursuit because although SC neurons respond to motion have been presented but before the movement is initiat-
stimuli, they are not selective for the direction of motion ed, SC neurons display a preference for the stimulus that
(Krauzlis 2004). On the other hand, the complicated pat- will become the eye movement target. In a color-oddity
tern of activity exhibited by these neurons during search task using saccades, some SC neurons discrimi-
pursuit—and also fixation—can be explained by consid- nate the target from the distractor with a delay that is
ering the location of the tracked target within the neu- time locked to stimulus onset, rather than saccade laten-
ron’s retinotopically organized response field (Krauzlis cy, suggesting that they play a role in target selection in
and others 1997, 2000). The distribution of activity addition to saccade preparation (McPeek and Keller
across the SC motor map therefore appears to provide a 2002). In contrast, other neurons discriminate the target
real-time estimate of the retinal location of the eye motor with timing that is well correlated with saccade latency,
goal for pursuit and fixation, as well as for saccades. suggesting that they are more directly involved with trig-
Recent experiments in cats have underscored this idea gering saccades (McPeek and Keller 2002). In a match-
that SC activity represents the motor goal and does not to-sample task using pursuit and saccades, many SC
necessarily specify the saccade end point. These experi- neurons again exhibit selectivity for target stimuli, and
ments exploit the fact that cats tend to accomplish large this selectivity can predict the timing of pursuit as well

128 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Voluntary Eye Movements


as saccade choices (Krauzlis and Dill 2002). The signal
indicating the correct choice emerges over time, forming
a trade-off between speed and accuracy. The observed
pursuit and saccade performances fall on different parts
of the speed-accuracy curve predicted by neuronal activ-
ity, supporting the idea that pursuit and saccades are
guided by shared selection signals but involve different
trade-offs between speed and accuracy (Krauzlis and
others 1999; Liston and Krauzlis 2003).
Manipulation of the fixated visual stimulus can also
modify target-related activity in the SC. Many neurons
in the SC increase their firing rate after the fixation stim-
ulus is extinguished, even if a visual target has not yet
appeared in their response field, and these changes are
correlated with the latencies of both pursuit (Krauzlis
2003) and saccades (Dorris and others 1997; Sparks and
others 2000; Krauzlis 2003). Conversely, neurons in the
rostral SC that are typically active during fixation
decrease their firing after the offset of the fixation spot
(Dorris and Munoz 1995; Dorris and others 1997).
Fig. 5. Activation and inactivation of the superior colliculus (SC)
These changes in activity indicate a shift in the distribu- affects target selection. A, Effects of SC inactivation on sac-
tion of activity across the SC in favor of those neurons cades during a visual search task. Under normal conditions,
that are likely to represent the impending target. By monkeys were able to identify the target based on its unique
changing the baseline activity, the subsequent volley of color and make saccades directly to it (left). The pattern of sac-
activity evoked by the appearance of the target can more cades changed after a local area of the SC was inactivated by
injection of muscimol, corresponding to the portion of the visu-
readily trigger an eye movement, providing a neural cor- al field in which the target was located (blue ellipse). After local
relate for the shared effects on pursuit and saccade laten- inactivation, the monkeys made many inappropriate saccades
cies observed in this paradigm (Krauzlis and Miles to the distractor stimuli (right). Reproduced with permission
1996a, 1996b; Krauzlis 2003). From these results, it has from McPeek and Keller (2004, p 758). B, Effects of SC activa-
tion on pursuit during a discrimination task. Under normal con-
been suggested that the same signals in the rostral SC ditions, monkeys were able to correctly identify the target
that are involved in the covert preparation of saccades based on its luminance and generate a pursuit movement (hor-
might also control the gating of inputs for pursuit izontal eye speed) to follow it (right). Performance was changed
(Krauzlis 2003). This type of shared control could after a local area of the SC was activated with microstimulation,
explain the linkage that has been observed in the selec- corresponding to the portion of the visual field in which the dis-
tractor was located (orange ellipse). With local activation, the
tion of targets for pursuit and saccades (Gardner and monkeys generated many more inappropriate smooth eye
Lisberger 2001, 2002). Although the mechanism has not movements to follow the distractor (left). Note that in the case
yet been identified, one possibility is that this shared of this pursuit experiment, the affected site in the SC corre-
control is exerted by a projection from the SC to brain sponds to the location of the selected stimulus (the distractor is
on the right), even though this requires an eye movement in the
stem OPNs, the gatekeepers for saccades that have also opposite direction (the eye moves smoothly to the left). From
been recently implicated in the inhibitory control of pur- Carello and Krauzlis (2004, p 577). Copyright 2004 by Cell
suit (Missal and Keller 2002). Press.
The idea that the SC is involved in target selection has
now been directly tested in a pair of studies (Fig. 5). One
study used a visual search task in which the target was
defined as the “oddball” element in an array of visual moving toward and into the opposite hemifield
stimuli (McPeek and Keller 2004). When the region of (Rashbass 1961), making it possible to distinguish
the SC representing the target was focally inactivated, between the initial location of the target and the direction
saccades were often misdirected to distractors appearing of the eye movement, a distinction that is not possible
in unaffected areas of the visual field (Fig. 5A). with saccades. Critically, the effect of SC activation was
Importantly, the amplitude of this deficit was larger based on the target location, not the eye movement direc-
when the task of identifying the target was harder, argu- tion. For example, as illustrated in Figure 5B, when the
ing for an effect at the stage of target selection beyond stimulated region of the SC matched the distractor loca-
any effect on saccade motor execution. The other study tion (right), pursuit was more likely to follow the dis-
used a luminance discrimination task and showed that tractor, even though this required an eye movement in
weak activation of the SC (i.e., microstimulation that is the opposite direction (leftward). These results argue that
subthreshold for evoking saccades) biased the selection the SC plays a role in target choice per se, distinct from
of targets toward the stimulated location not just for sac- its traditional role in motor preparation.
cades but for pursuit as well (Carello and Krauzlis One important issue left unresolved by these studies is
2004). Using the classic “step-ramp” paradigm, the whether the SC participates in target selection by biasing
stimuli for pursuit appeared in one hemifield before the selection of the response goal or by shifting the allo-

Volume 11, Number 2, 2005 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 129


cation of visual attention. The visual responses of SC
neurons show enhancement consistent with an effect of
attention (Goldberg and Wurtz 1972; Kustov and
Robinson 1996), and many SC neurons are active during
covert shifts of attention evoked by spatially precise cues
but not by nonspatial symbolic cues (Ignashchenkova
and others 2004). These findings support the idea that
there is a common network for controlling attention and
saccades, consistent with the premotor theory of atten-
tion (Rizzolatti and others 1987; Sheliga and others
1995). Together with the activation and inactivation
results, these studies raise the intriguing possibility that
the SC not only receives the selection signal and applies
it toward implementing the motor choice but also helps
regulate the sensory-motor processing that leads to that
selection.
Another unresolved issue is how target-related activi-
ty in the SC is read out to trigger the appropriate eye
movement choice (Krauzlis and others 2004). One help-
ful approach to this problem introduces the assumption
that firing rates are proportional to the likelihood that
the target is present, and the decision is affirmed when
activity reaches a particular significance level
(Carpenter and Williams 1995; Gold and Shadlen 2001).
As shown schematically in Figure 6, the relevant deci-
sion signal for target selection might be based not only
on the firing rate associated with possible new targets
(represented by activity at caudal sites in the SC) but
also on the firing rate associated with the currently Fig. 6. Hypothetical decision mechanism explaining how activ-
ity in the superior colliculus (SC) might be read out to accom-
foveated stimulus (represented by activity at the rostral plish target selection. In the top panel, the monkey is initially
SC). If these firing rates are proportional to target likeli- fixating the central stimulus (blue square) and is considering
hood, then this comparison between caudal and rostral whether to make an eye movement to a possible new target in
sites in the SC could amount to a likelihood ratio test the periphery (orange square). The middle panel shows
(Gold and Shadlen 2001; Krauzlis and others 2004). In schematically how activity corresponding to the two stimuli is
distributed across the SC, with activity related to the fixated
general, likelihood ratio tests are useful for testing stimulus at the rostral end and activity for the new stimulus at
whether a more complex model (in this case, that the tar- a more caudal location. Available data suggest, but have not
get is at an eccentric location) provides a better descrip- yet proved, that the firing rate (FR) of SC neurons is propor-
tion of the data than the simpler model (that the target is tional to the likelihood that the target is in the response field of
the SC neuron (Krauzlis and others 2004). If so, then compari-
already foveated) because it gives values that are related son of activity across the SC amounts to a comparison of alter-
to common test statistics such as the F test and the χ2. In native hypotheses, and the difference in activity would indicate
this case, every decision by the SC to select an eccentric the relative likelihood of one hypothesis over the other. The
target would amount to a rejection of the null hypothe- decision of whether to select the new target (“GO”) or remain
sis. This type of decision framework also has the advan- fixating (“STAY”) could then be determined by comparing the
difference in firing rate between caudal and rostral neurons (the
tage of being very flexible because the source of the putative decision signal) to a threshold value.
information does not really matter; what matters is how
the information improves the estimate of target likeli-
hood. For example, target selection should be based not
just on visual evidence but also on information about
prior probability and expected rewards (Platt and
Glimcher 1999; Ikeda and Hikosaka 2003). If these dif- mild effects (Albano and others 1982). However, com-
ferent processes gave their answers in the same units bined damage to the SC and areas of the cerebral cortex
(e.g., something proportional to likelihood), it would be can eliminate voluntary saccades (Schiller and others
possible to combine and exchange these different 1980), indicating the importance of the cerebral cortex
sources of information on an equal footing and then read in providing signals that trigger and guide voluntary eye
the answer out from the SC in meaningful way. movements.

Cerebral Cortex Frontal Eye Fields


The SC plays a pivotal role in the control of voluntary The functional importance of the FEF is especially evi-
eye movements, but ablation of the SC has surprisingly dent when the outcome involves some degree of choice

130 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Voluntary Eye Movements


or self-control. For saccades, lesions of the FEF produce
only mild and temporary deficits in saccades when per-
formance is tested with solitary visual targets (Dias and
others 1995; Sommer and Tehovnik 1997; Dias and
Segraves 1999). However, the deficits after FEF lesions
are much more severe when the target stimulus is
accompanied by other irrelevant distracter stimuli
(Schiller and Chou 1998, 2000) or when the saccade is
directed to a remembered location (Dias and others
1995; Sommer and Tehovnik 1997; Dias and Segraves
1999). Similarly, disruption of FEF activity in humans
using magnetic stimulation disrupts performance in
visual search tasks (Muggleton and others 2003). For
pursuit, the effects are somewhat more dramatic (Fig. 7).
Inactivation of the smooth eye movement subfield of the
FEF (FEFsem) scales down the pursuit motor of visual Fig. 7. Deficits in pursuit eye velocity after inactivation of the
targets to about 25% of its normal value (Shi and others smooth eye movement portion of the frontal eye fields
1998), and lesions of the FEFsem eliminate the predic- (FEFsem) by injection of muscimol. Top, Single trial of step-
tive component of pursuit eye movements (Keating ramp tracking just before injection of muscimol into the right
FEFsem (dark solid line) superimposed on the first trial after
1991; MacAvoy and others 1991). injection (dashed line). At time 0 ms, the target stepped 4
Neurons in the FEF exhibit properties consistent with degrees to the left and moved at 40 deg/s to the right. In the
determining when voluntary eye movements are initiat- preinjection trial, the eye trajectory approximately matched that
ed. For pursuit, neurons in the FEFsem exhibit direc- of the target, whereas in the postinjection trial, the tracking was
tionally selective responses appropriate for guiding pur- accomplished mostly by the saccades. Bottom, Eye velocity
profiles superimposed for several trials preinjection (solid lines)
suit, and, in addition, many of them discriminate the and postinjection (dashed lines). For all six preinjection trials,
direction of motion before the onset of pursuit (Tanaka the peak velocity reached or exceeded that of the target,
and Lisberger 2002b). For saccades, the trial-to-trial whereas for all six postinjection trials, the peak velocity was far
variability in reaction times is related to the variability in below that of the target. Rapid upward and downward deflec-
tions of the velocity traces correspond to saccades. From Shi
when the firing rates of FEF neurons reach a relatively and others (1998, p 460). Used with permission from the
constant threshold value (Hanes and Schall 1996); when American Physiological Society.
an impending saccade is canceled, the firing rates drop
(Hanes and others 1998), suggesting that FEF activity
can regulate when and if a saccade will be triggered. As
in the SC (McPeek and Keller 2002), the FEF appears to
contain at least two classes of saccade-related neurons: tude of the shift depends on the strength of the visual
One type is time locked to the stimulus and therefore signal; this result argues that the perceptual evaluation of
appears to be associated with the process of target selec- the stimulus and the motor preparation of the saccade are
tion, whereas a second type is time locked to the move- not serial stages of processing but instead occur togeth-
ment onset and therefore appears to be involved with er and perhaps involve a common level of neural organ-
triggering the movement (Sato and Schall 2003). Indeed, ization. Conversely, the allocation of attention itself
some FEF neurons discriminate visual targets even in the appears to be altered by stimulation of the FEF.
absence of saccades or saccades directed elsewhere, sug- Stimulation within the FEF with currents too weak to
gesting that their activity corresponds to the allocation evoke saccades can nonetheless enhance visual respons-
of attention rather than the motor preparation of sac- es in extrastriate area V4 (Moore and Armstrong 2003)
cades (Thompson and others 1997; Murthy and others and improve performance on a visual discrimination task
2001). (Moore and Fallah 2004).
The interplay between eye motor planning and visual
functions such as selection and attention has been high- Lateral Intraparietal Area
lighted in several experiments. Stimulation of the
FEFsem evokes smooth eye movements and is the only The LIP also plays a key role in the process of visual
cortical region in which pursuit can be evoked when selection. Inactivation of the LIP does not produce
the eyes are fixating, but in addition to introducing a deficits in the latency or accuracy of saccades to single
direction-specific signal into the velocity command for targets but dramatically reduces the frequency of sac-
pursuit, stimulation also changes the gain of the pursuit cades to the affected visual field when competing stim-
response to new visual motion inputs (Tanaka and uli are present (Fig. 8) and increases the time required to
Lisberger 2001, 2002a). If saccades are evoked by FEF find the target during visual search (Wardak and others
stimulation as monkeys perform a motion discrimination 2002). The emergence of these deficits when there are
task, the movement end points are shifted toward the multiple choices indicates a competitive interaction
direction corresponding to the nascent perceptual judg- between the candidate targets and indicates how animal
ment (Gold and Shadlen 2000). Importantly, the ampli- models may be useful for addressing the visual neglect

Volume 11, Number 2, 2005 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 131


and extinction syndromes that occur in humans (Payne
and Rushmore 2003).
The activity of LIP neurons is strongly affected by
information relevant for visual selection. For example,
as in the SC and FEF, neurons in LIP respond more
strongly when the stimulus in their response field is a
target or behaviorally relevant than when it is a distrac-
tor or irrelevant (Platt and Glimcher 1997; Gottlieb and
others 1998). When monkeys are asked to discriminate
the direction of motion in a random-dot visual display
and subsequently report their answer with a saccade, LIP
activity changes during the viewing of motion in a way
that predicts the monkey’s upcoming perceptual decision
(Shadlen and Newsome 2001). Like the FEF, changes in Fig. 8. Inactivation of the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) disrupts
saccades to the affected visual field during a search task.
LIP activity related to attention and selection can be dis- Single-trial examples of visual search patterns after injection of
tinguished from motor preparation. For example, LIP muscimol into the right LIP. The small dots show eye position
activity is lower for a visual cue prompting a saccade sampled every 4 ms, large dots represent the search stimuli,
than for a visual cue indicating that a saccade should not and the open circle represents the location of the target. When
the target was in the ipsilateral visual field, unaffected by the
be made; this difference does not match the change in lesion (right), the monkey typically found the target within a
motor plans but is compatible with the idea that such small number of saccades. When the target was in the con-
changes garner increased attention (Bisley and Goldberg tralateral visual field, matching the site of the lesion (left), the
2003). number of saccades and overall search time dramatically
The activity of LIP neurons is also modulated by increased. Adapted from Wardak and others (2002, p 9882).
Copyright 2002 by the Society for Neuroscience.
reward. When the size of reward is varied across blocks
of trials, LIP neurons are more active when the expected
reward is higher (Platt and Glimcher 1999).
Interestingly, using a different experimental design, neu-
rons in the FEF did not show a reward-related modula- of an object rather than a spatial location (Olson and
tion (Leon and Shadlen 1999), raising the possibility that Gettner 1995; Tremblay and others 2002), saccades
the presence or absence of reward-related information is directed to the location opposite the visual stimulus
a point of distinction between the two cortical areas. (“antisaccades”; Schlag-Rey and others 1997), and sac-
The parietal cortex also plays some role in pursuit, but cades that occur within learned combinations or
this has been less studied. Stimulation of the LIP can sequences of saccades. During pursuit, SEF neurons
evoke smooth eye movements as well as saccades exhibit the largest changes in activity when the target
(Kurylo and Skavenski 1991), and about half of the neu- motion changes, especially when the timing of those
rons in the LIP and the ventral intraparietal area exhibit changes is predictable (Heinen and Liu 1997).
direction-specific activity during pursuit (Bremmer and Accordingly, as shown in Figure 9, stimulation of the
others 1997; Schlack and others 2003). The pursuit- SEF can facilitate smooth pursuit eye movements, and
related activity of many LIP neurons is also modulated this effect is largest if the stimulation is applied just as a
by eye position and other extraretinal signals (Bremmer period of fixation is predictably drawing to a close and
and others 1997; Schlack and others 2003), consistent the signal to initiate pursuit is about to be given (Missal
with the idea that the parietal cortex represents the goals and Heinen 2001, 2004).
for movements in coordinate frames appropriate for
effector organs such as the eyes, head, and hands MT and MST Areas
(Andersen and others 1997; Calton and others 2002).
The MT and MST areas are the major sources of visual
motion information that is critical for guiding pursuit
Supplementary Eye Field
and for adjusting the amplitudes of saccades to moving
The SEF plays a less direct role in the control of sac- targets (Newsome and others 1985; Dürsteler and Wurtz
cades and pursuit than the FEF does, but it appears to be 1988). Recent studies have clarified how visual process-
especially important for movements that are guided by ing in these areas changes over time and is related to
internal factors, rather than driven by external events. processes such as attention and perception.
During a saccade task in which monkeys are free to Most of the directional information that can be
choose either of two identical stimuli to receive their extracted from MT neurons is conveyed within the first
rewards, neurons in the SEF, FEF, and LIP exhibit activ- 100 milliseconds of the neuronal response (Osborne and
ity that anticipates the upcoming choice, but this activi- others 2004). However, the precision of the directional
ty is largest and occurs earliest in the SEF (Coe and oth- information conveyed by MT neurons is relatively poor,
ers 2002). Neurons in the SEF are also strongly modu- indicating that responses are probably pooled across the
lated during tasks in which the goal is defined by population to match the direction discrimination of pur-
abstract instructions, such as saccades directed to a part suit. One possibility is that the pursuit system relies on

132 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Voluntary Eye Movements


ing stimuli are presented that can be perceptually
grouped as a single moving object, some MT neurons
initially respond to the local motion of the stimulus com-
ponents, but over the course of a few hundred millisec-
onds, they begin to respond to the global motion of the
object as a whole. The changes in the directional tuning
of the neural activity following a time course are similar
to the changes in the direction of pursuit eye velocity
(Pack and Born 2001). In behavioral experiments, sub-
jects can readily perceive and track the veridical motion
of partially occluded objects, despite the ambiguous and
often misleading local motions of the component edges
(Stone and others 2000). The perceived and pursued
directions are initially more closely related to the aver-
age direction of the local edge motions, but they con-
verge to the veridical object motion direction after ~100
milliseconds (Masson and Stone 2002). These findings
also indicate that, over time, pursuit is guided by a sig-
nal related to the perceived motion of the object, rather
than the physical motion of the stimulus on the retina.
This idea is supported by recent studies showing that the
motion signals conveyed by some neurons in MST do
Fig. 9. Activation of the supplementary eye fields (SEFs) can
not depend on retinal inputs (Ilg and Thier 2003) and
facilitate anticipatory pursuit eye movements. On each trial, that they encode target motion in world-centered, rather
after a 500-ms fixation period (horizontal dashed lines), the tar- than retina-centered, coordinates (Ilg and others 2004).
get was extinguished for 200 ms (gap in the dashed lines).
When the target reappeared, it stepped to an eccentric position
and moved at a constant speed in the opposite direction. Top,
Conclusion and Outlook
Positions of the eye and target as a function of time from single Recent studies at a variety of levels have shown that the
trials with and without stimulation. Bottom, The effect of
microstimulation was more evident in the traces of horizontal functional organization of the pursuit and saccadic eye
eye velocity and occurred even before the target was visible movement systems are much more similar than previ-
(orange arrow). The orange bar at the bottom indicates the peri- ously recognized. Rather than composing two distinct
od of stimulation. From Missal and Heinen (2004, p 1258). Used systems that operate as visuomotor reflexes, pursuit and
with permission from the American Physiological Society.
saccades are mediated by similar and sometimes over-
lapping pathways and are guided by a variety of higher
order processes as well as by more direct sensory inputs
(Fig. 10). The picture that emerges from these studies is
quite different from that found in most textbooks, and
the center of mass of the population of MT neurons, and each point of departure from the traditional view raises
recent studies have illustrated that an estimate of target its own set of questions and challenges.
speed can be obtained by taking a weighted average of The overlap in the brain stem pathways argues that the
the responses across the population of neurons gating of pursuit and saccades involves shared circuitry
(Churchland and Lisberger 2001; Priebe and Lisberger that has been previously viewed as strictly part of the
2004). saccadic system. Working out the brain stem wiring for
When the onset of a target stimulus is accompanied by saccades alone has been difficult and is still not com-
a distractor, the initial activity of MT and MST neurons pletely resolved (Scudder and others 2002); it is unclear
exhibits very little selectivity for the target, and accord- whether extending it to pursuit will make it easier or
ingly, the initial pursuit eye velocity mainly follows the harder to understand the functional states and transitions
average of the two motion signals (Ferrera and Lisberger accomplished by this circuit.
1997; Recanzone and Wurtz 2000). The subsequent Consistent with its role in other motor systems, ocu-
activity of MT and MST neurons exhibits greater selec- lomotor regions of the cerebellum (VPF, vermis) appear
tivity, and the eye movements elicited at these longer to expertly tweak the commands for pursuit and saccades
latencies selectively follow one or the other stimulus, to compensate for mechanical constraints and to adapt
reflecting a winner-take-all mechanism (Recanzone and the movements to changing circumstances. In addition to
Wurtz 2000). However, the changes in activity are rela- understanding how the cerebellar circuits accomplish
tively small and occur in only a minority of neurons, so this function, there is also the conundrum that most of
it is not clear that these changes alone are sufficient to the descending signals that would appear relevant for the
account for the selectivity of pursuit. visual control of pursuit and saccades go to the dorsal
Solving the problem of computing motion signals for paraflocculus (Glickstein and others 1994) and not to the
tracking appears to take some time. When multiple mov- VPF and vermis.

Volume 11, Number 2, 2005 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 133


circumstances, these processes tend to give the same
answer—attention and motor preparation are typically
directed toward the most rewarding target—making it
difficult to tease them apart or to localize functions to
particular areas or classes of neurons. The broader chal-
lenge is to move beyond identifying neural correlates of
processes we expect to find and instead to begin enu-
merating the unique factors that operate in each region
and to explain how these factors interact across the net-
work of neurons and brain regions.

References
Albano JE, Mishkin M, Westbrook LE, Wurtz RH. 1982. Visuomotor
deficits following ablation of monkey superior colliculus. J
Neurophysiol 48:338–51.
Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Bradley DC, Xing J. 1997. Multimodal rep-
resentation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in
planning movements. Annu Rev Neurosci 20:303–30.
Barash S, Melikyan A, Sivakov A, Zhang M, Glickstein M, Thier P.
1999. Saccadic dysmetria and adaptation after lesions of the cere-
bellar cortex. J Neurosci 19:10931–9.
Basso MA, Krauzlis RJ, Wurtz RH. 2000. Activation and inactivation
Fig. 10. A hypothetical model of the functional organization of of rostral superior colliculus neurons during smooth-pursuit eye
voluntary eye movements. Rather than linking signals obtained movements in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 84:892–908.
from early visual processing steps directly to the motor outputs Basso M, Wurtz R. 1997. Modulation of neuronal activity by target
for pursuit and saccades (inset), the control of voluntary eye uncertainty. Nature 389:66–9.
movements involves a cascade of steps with several check- Basso MA, Wurtz RH. 1998. Modulation of neuronal activity in supe-
points that provide flexibility in how the movements are guided, rior colliculus by changes in target probability. J Neurosci
selected, and implemented. Evaluation of the sensory inputs 18:7519–34.
can be rapid but can also be influenced by higher order Bergeron A, Guitton D. 2002. In multiple-step gaze shifts: omnipause
processes such as perception, memory, and expected rewards (OPNs) and collicular fixation neurons encode gaze position error;
(sensory evaluation). The results from this evaluation proceed OPNs gate saccades. J Neurophysiol 88:1726–42.
along two tracks to influence the motor outputs. One track is Bergeron A, Matsuo S, Guitton D. 2003. Superior colliculus encodes
responsible for selecting the target and gating the motor distance to target, not saccade amplitude, in multi-step gaze shifts.
response (gating, target selection) and involves structures Nat Neurosci 6:404–13.
including the SC. The other track is responsible for providing Bisley JW, Goldberg ME. 2003. Neuronal activity in the lateral intra-
the drive signals that determine the metrics of the movements parietal area and spatial attention. Science 299:81–6.
(drive) and involves structures such as the cerebellum. In this Bogousslavsky J, Meienberg O. 1987. Eye-movement disorders in
model, the choice of whether to generate a pursuit movement brain-stem and cerebellar stroke. Arch Neurol 44:141–8.
or a saccade movement, or some combination of the two, is Bremmer F, Distler C, Hoffmann KP. 1997. Eye position effects in
not solely determined by the descending signals as in the tra- monkey cortex: II. Pursuit- and fixation- related activity in poste-
ditional view (inset) but instead depends on a comparison rior parietal areas LIP and 7A. J Neurophysiol 77:962–77.
between the descending signals and the current motor state Calton JL, Dickinson AR, Snyder LH. 2002. Non-spatial, motor-
(motor execution). specific activation in posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci
5:580–8.
Cannon SC, Robinson DA. 1987. Loss of the neural integrator of the
oculomotor system from brain stem lesions in monkey. J
Neurophysiol 57:1383–409.
In contrast to earlier descriptions of the pursuit and Carello CD, Krauzlis RJ. 2004. Manipulating intent: evidence for a
saccade systems, the SC appears to be part of a shared causal role of the superior colliculus in target selection. Neuron
mechanism for selecting targets and perhaps triggering 43:575–83.
the two types of voluntary eye movements. It remains to Carpenter RH, Williams ML. 1995. Neural computation of log likeli-
hood in control of saccadic eye movements. Nature 377:59–62.
be clarified how this target-related activity is read out to Churchland MM, Lisberger SG. 2001. Shifts in the population
trigger the appropriate motor commands, especially response in the middle temporal visual area parallel perceptual and
because the SC does not mediate visual motion signals motor illusions produced by apparent motion. J Neurosci
for pursuit (Krauzlis 2004). It is also unclear whether the 21:9387–402.
SC simply applies a selection signal that is established Coe B, Tomihara K, Matsuzawa M, Hikosaka O. 2002. Visual and
anticipatory bias in three cortical eye fields of the monkey during
elsewhere—for example, the cerebral cortex—or an adaptive decision-making task. J Neurosci 22:5081–90.
whether it also plays a crucial role in the selection Cui DM, Yan YJ, Lynch JC. 2003. Pursuit subregion of the frontal eye
process itself, perhaps by regulating shifts of visual field projects to the caudate nucleus in monkeys. J Neurophysiol
attention. 89:2678–84.
Dias EC, Kiesau M, Segraves MA. 1995. Acute activation and inacti-
Many of the signals that guide pursuit and saccades vation of macaque frontal eye field with GABA-related drugs. J
come from a network of cortical areas and involve a vari- Neurophysiol 74:2744–8.
ety of processes important for the selectivity and guid- Dias EC, Segraves MA. 1999. Muscimol-induced inactivation of mon-
ance of voluntary eye movements: motor preparation, key frontal eye field: effects on visually and memory-guided sac-
attention, perception, and expected reward. Under most cades. J Neurophysiol 81:2191–214.

134 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Voluntary Eye Movements


Dorris MC, Munoz DP. 1995. A neural correlate for the gap effect on Ilg UJ, Schumann S, Thier P. 2004. Posterior parietal cortex neurons
saccadic reaction times in monkey. J Neurophysiol 73:2558–62. encode target motion in world-centered coordinates. Neuron
Dorris MC, Munoz DP. 1998. Saccadic probability influences motor 43:145–51.
preparation signals and time to saccadic initiation. J Neurosci Ilg UJ, Thier P. 2003. Visual tracking neurons in primate area MST are
18:7015–26. activated by smooth pursuit eye movements of an “imaginary” tar-
Dorris MC, Pare M, Munoz DP. 1997. Neuronal activity in monkey get. J Neurophysiol 90:1489–502.
superior colliculus related to the initiation of saccadic eye move- Keating EG. 1991. Frontal eye field lesions impair predictive and
ments. J Neurosci 17:8566–79. visually-guided pursuit eye movements. Exp Brain Res 86:311–23.
Dürsteler MR, Wurtz RH. 1988. Pursuit and optokinetic deficits fol- Keating EG, Pierre A. 1996. Architecture of a gain controller in the
lowing chemical lesions of cortical areas MT and MST. J pursuit system. Behav Brain Res 81:173–81.
Neurophysiol 60:940–65. Keller EL. 1974. Participation of medial pontine reticular formation in
Ferrera VP, Lisberger SG. 1997. Neuronal responses in visual areas eye movement generation in monkey. J Neurophysiol 37:316–32.
MT and MST during smooth pursuit target selection. J Keller EL, Gandhi NJ, Weir PT. 1996. Discharge of superior collicular
Neurophysiol 78:1433–46. neurons during saccades made to moving targets. J Neurophysiol
Freedman EG, Sparks DL. 1997. Eye-head coordination during head- 76:3573–7.
unrestrained gaze shifts in rhesus monkeys. J Neurophysiol Kleine JF, Guan Y, Buttner U. 2003. Saccade-related neurons in the pri-
77:2328–48. mate fastigial nucleus: what do they encode? J Neurophysiol
Freedman EG, Stanford TR, Sparks DL. 1996. Combined eye-head 90:3137–54.
gaze shifts produced by electrical stimulation of the superior col- Kornylo K, Dill N, Saenz M, Krauzlis RJ. 2003. Cancelling of pursuit
liculus in Rhesus monkeys. J Neurophysiol 76:927–52. and saccadic eye movements in humans and monkeys. J
Fuchs AF, Robinson FR, Straube A. 1993. Role of the caudal fastigial Neurophysiol 89:2984–99.
nucleus in saccade generation I: neuronal discharge patterns. J Krauzlis RJ. 2000. Population coding of movement dynamics by cere-
Neurophysiol 70:1723–40. bellar Purkinje cells. Neuroreport 11:1045–50.
Fuchs AF, Robinson FR, Straube A. 1994. Participation of the caudal Krauzlis RJ. 2001. Extraretinal inputs to neurons in the rostral superi-
fastigial nucleus in smooth-pursuit eye movements: I. Neuronal or colliculus of the monkey during smooth-pursuit eye movements.
activity. J Neurophysiol 72:2714–28. J Neurophysiol 86:2629–33.
Gardner JL, Lisberger SG. 2001. Linked target selection for saccadic Krauzlis RJ. 2003. Neuronal activity in the rostral superior colliculus
and smooth pursuit eye movements. J Neurosci 21:2075–84. related to the initiation of pursuit and saccadic eye movements. J
Gardner JL, Lisberger SG. 2002. Serial linkage of target selection for Neurosci 23:4333–44.
orienting and tracking eye movements. Nat Neurosci 29:29. Krauzlis RJ. 2004. Activity of rostral superior colliculus neurons dur-
Glickstein M, Gerrits N, Kralj-Hans I, Mercier B, Stein J, Voogd J. ing passive and active viewing of motion. J Neurophysiol
1994. Visual pontocerebellar projections in the macaque. J Comp 92:949–58.
Neurol 349:51–72. Krauzlis RJ, Basso MA, Wurtz RH. 1997. Shared motor error for mul-
Gold JI, Shadlen MN. 2000. Representation of a perceptual decision in tiple eye movements. Science 276:1693–5.
developing oculomotor commands. Nature 404:390–4. Krauzlis RJ, Basso MA, Wurtz RH. 2000. Discharge properties of neu-
Gold JI, Shadlen MN. 2001. Neural computations that underlie deci- rons in the rostral superior colliculus of the monkey during
sions about sensory stimuli. Trends Cogn Sci 5:10–6. smooth-pursuit eye movements. J Neurophysiol 84:876–91.
Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH. 1972. Activity of superior colliculus in Krauzlis RJ, Dill N. 2002. Neural correlates of target choice for pursuit
behaving monkeys: II. Effect of attention on neuronal responses. J and saccades in the primate superior colliculus. Neuron 35:355–63.
Neurophysiol 35:560–74. Krauzlis RJ, Lisberger SG. 1994a. A model of visually-guided smooth
Gottlieb JP, Kusunoki M, Goldberg ME. 1998. The representation of pursuit eye movements based on behavioral observations. J Comp
visual salience in monkey parietal cortex. Nature 391:481–4. Neurosci 1:265–83.
Grasse KL, Lisberger SG. 1992. Analysis of a naturally occurring Krauzlis RJ, Lisberger SG. 1994b. Simple spike responses of gaze
asymmetry in vertical smooth pursuit eye movements in a monkey. velocity Purkinje cells in the floccular lobe of the monkey during
J Neurophysiol 67:164–79. the onset and offset of pursuit eye movements. J Neurophysiol
Hanes DP, Patterson WF II, Schall JD. 1998. Role of frontal eye fields 72:2045–50.
in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activ- Krauzlis RJ, Liston D, Carello CD. 2004. Target selection and the supe-
ity. J Neurophysiol 79:817–34. rior colliculus: goals, choices and hypotheses. Vis Res 44:1445–51.
Hanes DP, Schall JD. 1996. Neural control of voluntary movement ini- Krauzlis RJ, Miles FA. 1996a. Decreases in the latency of smooth pur-
tiation. Science 274:427–430. suit and saccadic eye movements produced by the “gap paradigm”
Hanson MR, Hamid MA, Tomsak RL, Chou SS, Leigh RJ. 1986. in the monkey. Vis Res 36:1973–85.
Selective saccadic palsy caused by pontine lesions: clinical, physi- Krauzlis RJ, Miles FA. 1996b. Release of fixation for pursuit and sac-
ological, and pathological correlations. Ann Neurol 20:209–17. cades in humans: evidence for shared inputs acting on different
Heinen SJ, Liu M. 1997. Single-neuron activity in the dorsomedial neural substrates. J Neurophysiol 76:2822–33.
frontal cortex during smooth-pursuit eye movements to predictable Krauzlis RJ, Miles FA. 1996c. Transitions between pursuit eye move-
target motion. Vis Neurosci 14:853–65. ments and fixation in the monkey: dependence on context. J
Helmchen C, Straube A, Buttner U. 1994. Saccade-related activity in Neurophysiol 76:1622–38.
the fastigial oculomotor region of the macaque monkey during Krauzlis RJ, Zivotofsky AZ, Miles FA. 1999. Target selection for pur-
spontaneous eye movements in light and darkness. Exp Brain Res suit and saccadic eye movements in humans. J Cogn Neurosci
98:474–82. 11:641–9.
Henn V, Cohen B. 1976. Coding of information about rapid eye move- Kurylo DD, Skavenski AA. 1991. Eye movements elicited by electrical
ments in the pontine reticular formation of alert monkeys. Brain stimulation of area PG in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 65:1243–53.
Res 108:307–25. Kustov AA, Robinson DL. 1996. Shared neural control of attentional
Henn V, Lang W, Hepp K, Resine H. 1984. Experimental gaze palsies shifts and eye movements. Nature 384:74–7.
in monkeys and their relation to human pathology. Brain Leon MI, Shadlen MN. 1999. Effect of expected reward magnitude on
107:619–36. the response of neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the
Ignashchenkova A, Dicke PW, Haarmeier T, Thier P. 2004. Neuron- macaque. Neuron 24:415–25.
specific contribution of the superior colliculus to overt and covert Liston D, Krauzlis RJ. 2003. Shared response preparation for pursuit
shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci 7:56–64. and saccadic eye movements. J Neurosci 23:11305–14.
Ikeda T, Hikosaka O. 2003. Reward-dependent gain and bias of visual Luschei ES, Fuchs AF. 1972. Activity of brain stem neurons during eye
responses in primate superior colliculus. Neuron 39:693–700. movements of alert monkeys. J Neurophysiol 35:445–61.

Volume 11, Number 2, 2005 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 135


MacAvoy MG, Gottlieb JP, Bruce CJ. 1991. Smooth-pursuit eye move- Rizzolatti G, Riggio L, Dascola I, Umilta C. 1987. Reorienting atten-
ment representation in the primate frontal eye field. Cereb Cortex tion across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor
1:95–102. of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 25:31–40.
Masson GS, Stone LS. 2002. From following edges to pursuing Robinson DA. 1972. Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation
objects. J Neurophysiol 88:2869–73. in the alert monkey. Vis Res 12:1795–808.
McPeek RM, Keller EL. 2002. Saccade target selection in the superior Robinson FR, Straube A, Fuchs AF. 1993. Role of the caudal fastigial
colliculus during a visual search task. J Neurophysiol 88:2019–34. nucleus in saccade generation: II. Effects of muscimol inactivation.
McPeek RM, Keller EL. 2004. Deficits in saccade target selection after J Neurophysiol 70:1741–58.
inactivation of superior colliculus. Nat Neurosci 7:757–63. Rosano C, Krisky CM, Welling JS, Eddy WF, Luna B, Thulborn KR,
Missal M, Heinen SJ. 2001. Facilitation of smooth pursuit initiation by and others. 2002. Pursuit and saccadic eye movement subregions in
electrical stimulation in the supplementary eye fields. J human frontal eye field: a high-resolution fMRI investigation.
Neurophysiol 86:2413–25. Cereb Cortex 12:107–15.
Missal M, Heinen SJ. 2004. Supplementary eye fields stimulation Sato TR, Schall JD. 2003. Effects of stimulus-response compatibility
facilitates anticipatory pursuit. J Neurophysiol 92:1257–62. on neural selection in frontal eye field. Neuron 38:637–48.
Missal M, Keller EL. 2001. Neurons active during both saccades and Schiller PH, Chou I. 2000. The effects of anterior arcuate and dorso-
smooth pursuit suggest a convergence of oculomotor systems in medial frontal cortex lesions on visually guided eye movements: 2.
the pontine reticular formation. Soc Neurosci Abstr 27:208. Paired and multiple targets. Vis Res 40:1627–38.
Missal M, Keller EL. 2002. Common inhibitory mechanism for sac- Schiller PH, Chou IH. 1998. The effects of frontal eye field and dor-
cades and smooth-pursuit eye movements. J Neurophysiol somedial frontal cortex lesions on visually guided eye movements.
88:1880–92. Nat Neurosci 1:248–53.
Moore T, Armstrong KM. 2003. Selective gating of visual signals by Schiller PH, Stryker M. 1972. Single-unit recording and stimulation in
microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature 421:370–3. superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol
Moore T, Fallah M. 2004. Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and 35:915–24.
its effects on covert spatial attention. J Neurophysiol 91:152–62. Schiller PH, True SD, Conway JL. 1980. Deficits in eye movements
Muggleton NG, Juan CH, Cowey A, Walsh V. 2003. Human frontal eye following frontal eye field and superior colliculus ablations. J
fields and visual search. J Neurophysiol 89:3340–3. Neurophysiol 44:1175–89.
Munoz DP, Pélisson D, Guitton D. 1991. Movement of neural activity Schlack A, Hoffmann KP, Bremmer F. 2003. Selectivity of macaque
on the superior colliculus motor map during gaze shifts. Science area VIP for smooth pursuit eye movements. J Physiol 551(pt
251:1358–60. 2):551–61.
Munoz DP, Wurtz RH. 1993. Fixation cells in monkey superior col- Schlag-Rey M, Amador N, Sanchez H, Schlag J. 1997. Antisaccade
liculus I. Characteristics of cell discharge. J Neurophysiol performance predicted by neuronal activity in the supplementary
70:559–75. eye field. Nature 390:398–401.
Murthy A, Thompson KG, Schall JD. 2001. Dynamic dissociation of Scudder CA. 1988. A new local feedback model of the saccadic burst
visual selection from saccade programming in frontal eye field. J generator. J Neurophysiol 59:1455–75.
Neurophysiol 86:2634–7. Scudder CA, Kaneko CS, Fuchs AF. 2002. The brainstem burst gener-
Newsome WT, Wurtz RH, Dürsteler MR, Mikami A. 1985. Deficits in ator for saccadic eye movements: a modern synthesis. Exp Brain
visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the Res 142:439–62.
middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci Scudder CA, McGee DM. 2003. Adaptive modification of saccade size
5:825–40. produces correlated changes in the discharges of fastigial nucleus
Ohtsuka K, Noda H. 1991. Saccadic burst neurons in the oculomotor neurons. J Neurophysiol 90:1011–26.
region of the fastigial nucleus of macaque monkeys. J Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. 2001. Neural basis of a perceptual deci-
Neurophysiol 65:1422–34. sion in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J
Olson CR, Gettner SN. 1995. Object-centered direction selectivity in Neurophysiol 86:1916–36.
the macaque supplementary eye field. Science 269:985–8. Sheliga BM, Riggio L, Rizzolatti G. 1995. Spatial attention and eye
Osborne LC, Bialek W, Lisberger SG. 2004. Time course of informa- movements. Exp Brain Res 105:261–75.
tion about motion direction in visual area MT of macaque mon- Shi D, Friedman HR, Bruce CJ. 1998. Deficits in smooth-pursuit eye
keys. J Neurosci 24:3210–22. movements after muscimol inactivation within the primate’s frontal
Pack CC, Born RT. 2001. Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to eye field. J Neurophysiol 80:458–64.
the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain. Nature Sommer MA, Tehovnik EJ. 1997. Reversible inactivation of macaque
409:1040–2. frontal eye field. Exp Brain Res 116:229–49.
Payne BR, Rushmore RJ. 2003. Animal models of cerebral neglect and Sparks D, Rohrer WH, Zhang Y. 2000. The role of the superior col-
its cancellation. Neuroscientist 9:446–54. liculus in saccade initiation: a study of express saccades and the
Petit L, Haxby JV. 1999. Functional anatomy of pursuit eye movements gap effect. Vis Res 40:2763–77.
in humans as revealed by fMRI. J Neurophysiol 82:463–71. Sparks DL, Sides JP. 1974. Brain stem unit activity related to horizon-
Platt ML, Glimcher PW. 1997. Responses of intraparietal neurons to tal eye movements occurring during visual tracking. Brain Res
saccadic targets and visual distractors. J Neurophysiol 78:1574–89. 77:320–5.
Platt ML, Glimcher PW. 1999. Neural correlates of decision variables Stanford TR, Sparks DL. 1994. Systematic errors for saccades to
in parietal cortex. Nature 400:233–8. remembered targets: evidence for a dissociation between saccade
Priebe NJ, Lisberger SG. 2004. Estimating target speed from the pop- metrics and activity in the superior colliculus. Vis Res 34:93–106.
ulation response in visual area MT. J Neurosci 24:1907–16. Stone LS, Beutter BR, Lorenceau J. 2000. Visual motion integration
Rambold H, Churchland A, Selig Y, Jasmin L, Lisberger SG. 2002. for perception and pursuit. Perception 29:771–87.
Partial ablations of the flocculus and ventral paraflocculus in mon- Takagi M, Zee DS, Tamargo RJ. 1998. Effects of lesions of the oculo-
keys cause linked deficits in smooth pursuit eye movements and motor vermis on eye movements in primate: saccades. J
adaptive modification of the VOR. J Neurophysiol 87:912–24. Neurophysiol 80:1911–31.
Rashbass C. 1961. The relationship between saccadic and smooth Tanaka M, Lisberger SG. 2001. Regulation of the gain of visually
tracking eye movements. J Physiol (Lond) 159:326–38. guided smooth-pursuit eye movements by frontal cortex. Nature
Raybourn MS, Keller EL. 1977. Colliculo-reticular organization in pri- 409:191–4.
mate oculomotor system. J Neurophysiol 40:861–78. Tanaka M, Lisberger SG. 2002a. Enhancement of multiple compo-
Recanzone GH, Wurtz RH. 2000. Effects of attention on MT and MST nents of pursuit eye movement by microstimulation in the arcuate
neuronal activity during pursuit initiation. J Neurophysiol frontal pursuit area in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 87:802–18.
83:777–90.

136 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Voluntary Eye Movements


Tanaka M, Lisberger SG. 2002b. Role of arcuate frontal cortex of mon- Tremblay L, Gettner SN, Olson CR. 2002. Neurons with object-
keys in smooth pursuit eye movements: I. Basic response proper- centered spatial selectivity in macaque SEF: do they represent
ties to retinal image motion and position. J Neurophysiol locations or rules? J Neurophysiol 87:333–50.
87:2684–99. Van Gisbergen JAM, Robinson DA, Gielen S. 1981. A quantitative
Thier P, Dicke PW, Haas R, Barash S. 2000. Encoding of movement analysis of generation of saccadic eye movements for burst neu-
time by populations of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nature 405:72–6. rons. J Neurophysiol 45:417–42.
Thompson KG, Bichot NP, Schall JD. 1997. Dissociation of visual dis- Wardak C, Olivier E, Duhamel JR. 2002. Saccadic target selection
crimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye deficits after lateral intraparietal area inactivation in monkeys. J
field. J Neurophysiol 77:1046–50. Neurosci 22:9877–84.
Tian JR, Lynch JC. 1996a. Corticocortical input to the smooth and sac- Yan YJ, Cui DM, Lynch JC. 2001. Overlap of saccadic and pursuit eye
cadic eye movement subregions of the frontal eye field in Cebus movement systems in the brain stem reticular formation. J
monkeys. J Neurophysiol 76:2754–71. Neurophysiol 86:3056–60.
Tian JR, Lynch JC. 1996b. Functionally defined smooth and saccadic Zee DS, Yamazaki A, Butler PH, Güçer G. 1981. Effects of ablation of
eye movement subregions in the frontal eye field of Cebus mon- flocculus and paraflocculus on eye movements in primate. J
keys. J Neurophysiol 76:2740–53. Neurophysiol 46:878–99.

Volume 11, Number 2, 2005 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 137

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen