Sie sind auf Seite 1von 29

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 43:1 (2002), pp 103±131

Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap


and behavioural disorders
Frank M.C. Besag
Bedfordshire and Luton Community NHS Trust, UK and University of Luton, UK

Epidemiological studies indicate that there is a high rate of mental retardation and behavioural
problems in children with epilepsy. In some cases both the epilepsy and the mental retardation will have
a common cause, such as a metabolic disorder or brain trauma. However, in other children, the epilepsy
itself may cause either temporary or permanent learning problems. When permanent learning disability
can be prevented it is important to treat the epilepsy early and effectively. Children with specific learning
difficulties and memory problems can benefit greatly from appropriate management. There are many
causes of behavioural disturbance in children with epilepsy. These causes include the epilepsy itself,
treatment of the epilepsy, reactions to the epilepsy, associated brain damage/dysfunction and causes
that are equally applicable to children who do not have epilepsy. Identifying the cause or causes in each
child allows rational management to be provided. Antiepileptic treatment with medication or surgery
can either improve the situation or make matters worse. The treatment should be tailored to the needs of
the individual child. If surgery is required, there is a strong argument for performing this early in life,
both to allow the greatest opportunity for brain plasticity and also to allow the child full benefit from the
important developmental and educational years, without the problems that can be associated with the
epilepsy. Skilled management of children with epilepsy who have mental retardation and/or
behavioural problems can be very rewarding both for the family and for the professionals involved.
Keywords: Behaviour problems, cognition, epilepsy, learning difficulties, mental handicap.

There is a tendency to adopt extreme attitudes to sion on Classi®cation and Terminology of the
learning and behavioural problems in children with International League Against Epilepsy, 1981, 1989;
epilepsy: either to state that they have no problems Oxbury & Duchowny, 2000). The importance of
at all or to consider that the situation is necessarily correct diagnosis cannot be overemphasised.
disastrous. Neither extreme is appropriate. The aim
of this paper is to provide a balanced view.
Discussions of the de®nitions of mental handicap Part I Epilepsy and mental handicap
and behavioural disorders can occupy much space
Epidemiological studies on learning
without throwing much light on the fundamental
issues. Furthermore, terminology used to designate The current author (Besag, 1994a) has previously
mental handicap often has more to do with current reviewed several of the studies on epilepsy and
usage, some might say fashion, and political cor- learning. Many of these were on selected populations
rectness than underlying science or practical issues. rather than being true epidemiological studies.
The de®nition of mental handicap most widely ac- However, they have added to the overall impression
cepted is that of IQ < 70, based on the fact that this is of a relationship between learning problems and
approximately 2 standard deviations below IQ 100. epilepsy. Pond and Bidwell (1960) surveyed 14 gen-
However, wider de®nitions of impairment may be eral practices and identi®ed 39 children of school age
appropriate so as to include speci®c de®cits. It is also with epilepsy. Fourteen of these were having dif®-
very important to note any loss of skills, even if the culties in education, especially as a result of beha-
®nal IQ remains above 70. It could be argued that vioural disorders. The classical study is that of
the concept of `schooling dif®culty', although more Rutter, Graham, and Yule (1970) carried out on the
vague, has more practical importance. Isle of Wight. They found an excess of reading re-
With regard to behavioural disturbance, it is usual tardation of 2 years or more in children with epi-
to refer to standardised behavioural scales such as lepsy, even if they were of normal or above-normal
the Rutter, Connors or Achenbach scales, although intelligence. This was a true epidemiological study,
measures of more speci®c behaviours may be examining all the children around 10 years of age in
necessary. These scales may overlook more subtle a de®ned geographical area. Holdsworth and Whit-
but nevertheless important behavioural changes. more (1974a, 1974b) attempted to locate all the
Detailed de®nitions of epilepsy, seizure types and children attending ordinary schools in a particular
seizure syndromes are provided in other publica- area. However, they excluded children attending
tions (O'Donohoe, 1985; Henriksen, 1996; Commis- special schools. Eighty-®ve children with epilepsy
Ó Association for Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2002.
Published by Blackwell Publishers, 108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA
104 Frank M.C. Besag

ful®lled their criteria of attending a mainstream these had intelligence tests at 7 years of age. Ninety-
school and having had a seizure during the previous eight children with non-febrile seizures had at least
twelve months (52 children) or having been treated one sibling in the study. Using sibling controls, the
with antiepileptic drugs over that period (33 chil- authors concluded that, although the mean IQ score
dren). This yielded a low prevalence ®gure of 1.6 per of 91.5 was less than that of the general population,
thousand in primary schools and 2.4 per thousand it was not signi®cantly different from that of sibling
in secondary schools, probably because they exclu- controls. Nevertheless, their results imply a poor
ded children who were not attending mainstream intellectual performance of children with epilepsy,
schooling. They classi®ed 64 of the children into compared with those who did not have epilepsy in
three groups, based on educational performance. the general population. This paper raises the
Twenty-®ve (31%) were maintaining an average or important question of whether the poorer intellectual
superior level of performance, 35 (53%) were `holding performance of the children with epilepsy re¯ects
their own at a below-average level' and 10 (16%) were some pre-existing, genetically determined de®cit or
seriously behind. Their data did not suggest that whether it is a consequence of the epilepsy. This
poor attendance was a cause of the educational dif- issue will be discussed in later sections.
®culties. The frequency of the seizures did not seem McDermott, Mani, and Krishnaswami, (1995)
to affect educational outcome or school attendance. carried out a population-based study in the US using
Three of the 4 children with absence seizures alone data collected in the 1988 National Health Interview
were progressing well. Survey (NHIS). The NHIS staff carried out personal
Pazzaglia and Frank-Pazzaglia (1976) found 38 interviews with approximately 45,000 households
children with epilepsy in a population of 13,000 containing 150,000 individuals. Of the 121 children
school-age children 6±14 years of age in the area with parent-reported seizures, 22% had develop-
around Cesena in Italy. This yielded a prevalence of mental delays and 28% had repeated a grade in
approximately 3 per thousand, which is close to school. Of the children who had both seizures and
what other studies have found. Twenty-seven of the behavioural problems, 57% were in special educa-
38 children were of normal intelligence and 11 (29%) tion classes. The authors have pointed out a number
were `retarded', de®ned as an IQ < 80, although it of limitations. In particular, the results were based
should be noted that 10 of the 11 children had an IQ solely on parental interviews carried out by the NHIS
< 70. About half the children had a normal school staff. Furthermore, the care exercised in ensuring a
record, 17% were behind their grade in school and rigorous diagnosis of epilepsy in the UK national
36% were in special schools. child development study did not apply to this sam-
The National Child Development Study in the UK ple, in which broader inclusion criteria were used.
was a true epidemiological study of a cohort of Outstanding epidemiological work has been car-
17,733 children born in the week 3±9 March 1958 in ried out in Finland by SillanpaÈaÈ. In one of these
England, Scotland and Wales. Of the original chil- studies (SillanpaÈaÈ, 1992) he identi®ed 143 children
dren, 15,496 were traced and alive at 11 years of age, with epilepsy out of a population of 21,104 children
yielding an epilepsy prevalence of 4.1 per thousand. 4±15 years of age, a prevalence of 6.8 per thousand.
This suggests that few, if any, cases escaped identi- He found that the most frequent neurological
®cation. The authors were particularly careful about impairments were mental retardation, de®ned as an
the inclusion criteria. At 11 years of age there were IQ < 70, which was present in 31.4%, speech disor-
346 possible cases of epilepsy but the diagnosis was ders (27.5%) and speci®c learning disorders (23.1%).
®rmly established in only 64. Of these, 22 (34%) were He concluded that there was a 22-fold risk of occur-
in special education by the age of 16. Even those who rence of a handicap in children with epilepsy
remained within mainstream schooling tended to compared with controls. In his earlier reviews
have lower reading and mathematics scores than the (SillanpaÈ aÈ, 1990, 1983) he concluded that 27.5% of
age-matched general population. Most of the chil- children with epilepsy did not complete their basic
dren who were in special schools were there because education or required schooling in establishments for
of intellectual impairment rather than because of the learning disability. He subsequently (SillanpaÈ aÈ et al.,
epilepsy itself. This study led to a number of valuable 1998) followed up these children to determine the
papers (Ross, Peckham, West, & Butler, 1980; Ver- long-term prognosis of seizures with childhood onset.
ity, Ross, & Golding, 1992; Kurtz, Tookey, & Ross, Of the original 245 children identi®ed between 1961
1998). and 1964, suf®cient data were available on 220 (90%)
Ellenberg, Hirtz, and Nelson (1986), in the US, in 1992; 176 were alive and 46 had died. The re-
examined children with epilepsy as part of the maining 25 could not be traced or would not parti-
national collaborative perinatal project of the cipate. The 99 patients who had uncomplicated
National Institute of Neurological and Communica- epilepsy nevertheless had signi®cantly worse out-
tive Disorders and Stroke (NCCP). This study comes for most social and educational variables,
followed the outcome of 54,000 pregnancies in 1959. despite having similar socio-economic status. The
They identi®ed 518 children who had one or more remaining 76 patients with epilepsy and other dis-
non-febrile seizures after the newborn period; 368 of abilities, as expected, had poor outcomes on these
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 105

measures. The poorer outcome was despite the fact retardation, 6±13 years in a geographical area of
that a large proportion went into remission, with 70% Sweden. Of 378 children, 98 (26%) had `active' epi-
having been seizure free for more than 20 years. lepsy; 15% of those with mild mental retardation and
In a recent study carried out by O'Neill and co- 45% of those with severe mental retardation had
workers (Besag, O'Neill, & Ross, 1999) in the inner epilepsy.
London Borough of Lambeth, parents of children Forsgren, Edvinsson, Blomquist, Heijbel, and
with epilepsy were asked to ®ll out questionnaires. Sidenvall (1990) studied a mixed sample of children
Using multiple ascertainment methods, the preval- and adults with mental retardation in a northern
ence of epilepsy in school-age children (n ˆ 127) was Swedish county and found that 20% had `active'
found to be 4.2 per thousand, suggesting that most epilepsy; 23% of those with severe mental retarda-
were identi®ed; 60% of parents completed question- tion had epilepsy and 11% of those with mild mental
naires. Of these, 65% reported some learning dif®- retardation.
culty in their children. Although parental perception Goulden, Shinnar, Koller, Katz, and Richardson,
is not a rigorous measure of educational failure, this (1991) carried out a prospective study in a cohort of
®gure is consistent with the general conclusion that 221 children with mental retardation and reported
children with epilepsy are highly likely to have that 33 (15%) had epilepsy. An additional 16 (7%)
schooling dif®culties. had a single febrile or afebrile seizure by 22 years of
A different approach is to examine the prevalence age. The cumulative incidence of epilepsy was 9%,
of epilepsy in children with mental retardation. Air- 11%, 13% and 15% at 5, 10, 15 and 22 years
aksinen et al. (2000) have recently reported such a respectively.
study. They identi®ed 151 children with mental re- The conclusion from these studies is clear. First, a
tardation by screening four birth cohorts of nearly high proportion of children with epilepsy have global
13,000 in one Finnish province at 9±10 years of age. or speci®c learning problems. Second, in general, the
All of these children and 101 randomly selected age- greater the degree of pre-existing mental retardation,
matched controls were studied by a multidisciplinary the higher the risk of developing epilepsy.
team. They used a variety of psychometric tests and Although it should be noted that febrile convul-
de®ned mental retardation as a score of more than 2 sions are not a form of epilepsy, they may be asso-
standard deviations below the mean. By 10 years of ciated with an increased risk of epilepsy and are
age, 29 of the 151 (19%) had epilepsy. The cumulative listed in the International League Against Epilepsy
risk for epilepsy at 22 years of age was 21%, that is, classi®cation. Furthermore, febrile seizures are
32 of the 151 subjects were having recurrent, un- common, occurring in 2±4% of the general popula-
provoked epileptic seizures. The ®gure for the 77 tion (Wallace, 1988), and it would appear appropri-
subjects with severe mental retardation, de®ned as ate to discuss the cognitive effects of this childhood
IQ < 55 or 3 standard deviations below the mean, was condition in the present paper. Verity, Greenwood,
35%. The prevalence in those with mild mental re- and Golding (1998) identi®ed 398 children with
tardation was, in contrast, only 7%. The difference in febrile convulsions among 14,676 subjects enrolled
prevalence was highly statistically signi®cant (p < in the Child Health and Education study which was
.0005). Mild mental retardation was de®ned as being a national population-based study in the United
more than 2 standard deviations but less than 3 Kingdom of children born in one week in April 1970.
standard deviations below the mean for IQ. There The children were comprehensively assessed at the
have been a number of past studies on the prevalence age of 10. Sixteen children were excluded because
of epilepsy in people with mental handicap, although they had neurodevelopmental problems before the
relatively few of these have been in children. ®rst febrile convulsion and 1 child was said to be
Gustavson, Holmgren, Jansell, Son, and Blom- atypical, leaving 381 children. Of these, 287 had
quist (1977) carried out a study in children aged simple febrile convulsions and 94 complex febrile
5±16 years with mental retardation in northern convulsions, de®ned as a convulsion that lasted
Sweden. They found that 84 of 161 children (52%) longer than 15 minutes, that was focal or that
who had severe mental retardation (IQ < 50) also had occurred more than once during an episode of fever.
epilepsy. In the same area and age group, in those Only 4 of the 102 outcome measures appeared to be
with mild mental retardation (IQ ³ 50), 36 of 171 statistically signi®cantly different from the control
(21%) had epilepsy. group. The results indicated that the children with
Richardson, Koller, Katz, and McLaren (1980) ex- the febrile convulsions were more impulsive or
amined ®gures for the occurrence of one or more excitable, less skilled in gymnastics, more anxious
epileptic seizures in patients with mental retarda- and had better reading skills than the controls. The
tion, using two control groups, one with borderline last ®nding, that children with the febrile convul-
intelligence and another with normal intelligence, sions performed better than those without, was an
matched for age and sex. The ®gures were 27%, 11% unexpected result. However, it is quite possible to
and 4% respectively. have 4 apparently statistically signi®cant results out
Steffenburg, Hagberg, Viggedal, and Kyllerman, of 102 measures by chance. Of the 334 children with
(1995) examined data on all children with mental febrile convulsions, 305 had normal intellectual
106 Frank M.C. Besag

ability (91.3%) which was very close to the ®gure early damage after some prolonged febrile convul-
for the 12,017 control children, namely 92.4%. The sions. However, it would be very unwise to be com-
results for the subgroups of children who had simple placent about febrile convulsions because it would
febrile convulsions and complex febrile convulsions appear that a single prolonged convulsion can
were also similar. They concluded that the intellec- sometimes cause permanent impairment. The safest
tual and behavioural outcome for the group with approach is to view any prolonged convulsion as
complex febrile convulsions was as good as that of being a medical emergency that should be treated
the rest of the cohort and that their results should be promptly.
considered as reassuring both for parents and for The various relationships between epilepsy and
physicians. mental handicap will be discussed in the section
Chang, Guo, Huang, Wang, and Tsai (2000) fol- on causes of learning problems in children with
lowed up 103 children who had febrile convulsions epilepsy.
under 3 years of age in a population-based study of
4,340 live births in Taiwan. The children were fol- Studies on selected populations. Stores and
lowed up at least until 6 years of age. They con®rmed co-workers have published a number of papers
that febrile convulsions did not have adverse effects reporting work on various aspects of educational
on behaviour or scholastic performance. A surpri- dif®culties in children with epilepsy (Stores, 1971,
sing result was that the children with a history of a 1973; Stores & Hart, 1975; Stores, 1976, 1978;
febrile convulsion seemed to have signi®cantly better Stores, Hart, & Piran 1978; Stores & Piran, 1978;
control of distractibility and attention. Bennett-Levy & Stores, 1984). Stores and Hart
It is interesting to view these results in the light of (1975) studied the reading skills in children with
the classical paper by Annegers, Hauser, Shirts, and generalised epileptiform discharges and focal dis-
Kurland (1987) who concluded that the risk for charges. Seventeen subjects (10 boys, 7 girls) with
subsequent epilepsy after febrile seizures was as generalised discharges were matched for age and sex
follows: after focal seizures 8%, after repeated or with 17 who had persistent focal discharges on
prolonged seizures 22% and after focal and pro- either side and also with children who did not have
longed seizures 49%. This would suggest that febrile epilepsy. No difference was found between the
seizures, particularly those that are focal, repeated reading skills of children with the generalised dis-
and prolonged, might have signi®cant sequelae. With charges and matched controls who did not have
regard to the outcome of children who have pro- epilepsy. However, those with persistent focal spike
longed febrile seizures, Shinnar et al. (2001) have discharges tended to have a lower reading ability
recently published a study of 180 children aged than matched controls, especially with regard to
1 month±10 years who had febrile status epilepticus. accuracy. These effects were generally related to left
They de®ned febrile status epilepticus as being a hemisphere focal spikes. It is interesting to note that
febrile seizure lasting longer than 30 minutes. They the reading skills of boys with epilepsy, whatever the
compared this group with 244 children, including type, were worse than those of girls with epilepsy.
168 who had had simple febrile seizures and 76 This sex difference was not found in control children
with complex febrile seizures that lasted less than who did not have epilepsy. In another study, Stores
30 minutes. They concluded that the short-term et al. (1978) measured inattentiveness in 36 boys
morbidity and mortality of febrile status epilepticus and 35 girls with epilepsy. Again, the boys performed
in their series was extremely low. The follow-up as- more poorly than the girls but no sex difference was
sessment was at 1 month. They pointed out that seen in any of the measures in the control children
there were no IQ measurements in this study, nor who did not have epilepsy. A particularly interesting
any immediate neuro-imaging. They indicated that result was that the distraction applied to children
there were plans to examine neuro-imaging in the during a distractibility test lowered the scores in
entire cohort, in particular to determine factors that controls, as expected, but the performance improved
might be associated with the development of hippo- in those who had epilepsy, suggesting that these
campal sclerosis. It should be noted that prolonged children were in some way under-aroused and de-
febrile seizures have, in the past, been associated rived bene®t from the alerting effect of the noise.
with hippocampal sclerosis but there is much debate Another unexpected result was that children with 3
about whether this association represents some per second spike-wave discharges, commonly seen
preceding underlying abnormality or whether the in absence seizures, were less impaired on vigilance
hippocampal sclerosis is a true consequence of the and distractibility tests than other subgroups.
prolonged seizure (Shinnar, 1998). Aldenkamp and his co-workers have carried out a
What conclusions should be drawn from these number of studies to determine the effect of epilepsy
studies? Wallace (1988, 1996b) has provided a very on learning. For example, Aldenkamp, Overweg-
full discussion of the major issues. Taking into ac- Plandsoen, and Arends (1999) studied a selected
count both earlier and more recent work, it would sample comprising 123 children who were referred to
appear that most febrile convulsions do not result in their epilepsy centre in Holland. The inclusion cri-
serious sequelae and that there is little evidence for teria were: referral to the epilepsy programme for
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 107

neurological assessment and assessment of learning considerable variation in the type of epilepsy/seiz-
and behaviour, aged 6±12 years, in normal schooling ure type. The development of children is not neces-
(i.e., excluding those in special education), mild sarily smooth but may be `by leaps'. Perhaps the
global learning impairment, de®ned as a retardation greatest drawback of the study is that the two
of educational achievement between 6 months and groups did not have the same starting point in terms
1 year, based on school reports and a school of IQ score. The argument for epilepsy causing de-
achievement test, and a recon®rmed diagnosis of terioration would be much greater if a prospective
epilepsy. Exclusion criteria were the presence of study showed a difference after both the epilepsy
dyslexia or dyscalculia, full-scale IQ < 70, speci®c and control groups had initial psychometric as-
cognitive impairment associated with attention-de- sessment showing the same cognitive level. It should
®cit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or neurological be noted that both the studies of Aldenkamp (1999)
impairments other than epilepsy. Thirty-one pa- and Neyens et al. (1999) were open studies but it
tients ful®lled the inclusion criteria. These were split might be dif®cult to design a proper blinded study in
into two groups: group A had epilepsy and mild which both the assessors and those analysing the
global learning impairment preceded by develop- results were unaware of whether the children had
mental delay (17 patients) and group B had epilepsy epilepsy or not.
and sudden unexpected mild global learning im- Bulteau et al. (2000) carried out a retrospective
pairment (14 patients). There was a control group of study of the relationship between epilepsy syn-
13 patients who had mild global learning impair- dromes and educational factors in 251 children aged
ments without epilepsy or other neurological prob- 3±17 years referred to their specialist service in
lems. The children in group B showed more frequent Paris. Although this was a selected group of children
neuropsychological impairment (p ˆ .04). They con- and some of the classi®cations might be open to
cluded that the key ®nding of this group might be the debate, the results are of great interest. As expected,
relatively frequent demonstration of subtle seizures, the children with `idiopathic' generalised epilepsies
which could explain both the decline of school re- had a higher IQ (mean 83.7) than those with symp-
sults and the impairments found on neuropsycho- tomatic/cryptogenic epilepsies (mean 63.3). Multiple
logical testing. They added that these seizures could regression analysis showed that there was a signi®-
have been overlooked for some time. They also made cant relationship between IQ and epileptic syndrome
the important point that unexpected decline in (using four broad categories), age of onset and
school performance with lapses of concentra- number of antiepileptic drugs. Although the num-
tion might be a ®rst sign of previously undiagnosed bers in some of the groups were relatively small,
epilepsy. there were also some interesting cognitive pro®les
The same group (Neyens, Aldenkamp, & Meinardi associated with particular types of epilepsy. The
1999) carried out a prospective follow-up study of most striking de®cits were for frontal lobe epilepsy,
the intellectual development of children with recent in arithmetic and coding, and for occipital epilepsy,
onset of epilepsy. Eleven children aged 7±15 years in image completion and coding. As might have been
with a mean duration of epilepsy of 2 years and 38 expected, overall performance was poor for multifo-
control children without epilepsy were tested on the cal epilepsy. As stated elsewhere in this paper, a
Netherlands revised version of the Wechsler Intelli- syndrome-based approach to the effects of epilepsy
gence Scale for Children on three occasions. They on learning would appear to be the best way of
were also given other psychometric tests. The chil- viewing the whole subject but there remains a lack of
dren with epilepsy differed at ®rst assessment with a good epidemiological evidence to con®rm this.
full-scale IQ of 94.2 compared with 107.3 for the An issue that is central to the relationship between
controls. The children with epilepsy also showed a epilepsy and learning has been raised by Cole
lower rate of increase in the full-scale IQ score with (2000), who asked the question: `Is epilepsy a pro-
time. They were tested on three occasions with a gressive disease?' He discussed the possible neuro-
test-retest interval of approximately 6 months. The biological consequences of epilepsy. He illustrated
gain in full-scale IQ score was 12 points in the these with a number of case reports. From the case
controls and 5 points in the children with epilepsy. reports he concluded that status epilepticus may
The performance IQ also showed a trend towards have devastating consequences and recurrent sei-
difference over time with the lower gain in the epi- zures over many years may be associated with
lepsy group. The gain in the full-scale IQ score in delayed neurocognitive dysfunction. Seizures may,
children with a duration of epilepsy of less than 2 however, continue for many years without evidence
years was also signi®cantly lower than that of the of progressive neurological symptoms and even a
children who had a longer duration of epilepsy, 2±3 single seizure may be suf®cient to cause injury with
years. The authors argued that the impact of epi- prolonged consequences. He discussed a number of
lepsy might be greater during the period of devel- neurobiological consequences of seizures that might
opment below 10 years of age. However, they pointed have a role to play in causing cognitive impairments,
out a number of limitations in this study. The including ionic ¯uxes, kinase activation, immediate
experimental group was small. There was also early gene expression, late gene expression, protein
108 Frank M.C. Besag

expression, protein modi®cation, mossy ®bre lower scores for general cognitive function and visuo-
sprouting and synaptic reorganisation, cell loss, gli- spatial skills. There were selective de®cits in non-
osis, neo-neurogenesis, and increased susceptibility verbal memory and delayed recall but verbal memory
to recurrent seizures. He discussed the limitation of and language skills were relatively preserved. They
the animal models on which these mechanisms are commented that earlier age of seizure onset seemed
based. It is not clear how these might apply to people to be associated with more severe cognitive decline.
with epilepsy. Lendt, Helmstaedter, and Elger (1999) performed
Deterioration of cognitive skills in a child is a memory, attention, language and visuoconstructive
major cause for concern. Any child who deteriorates, tests on 20 children (10±16 years) before and at three
i.e., has actually lost skills, without obvious cause, and 12 month intervals after temporal resection (10
must be investigated thoroughly. However, a falling right, 10 left). The outcome was compared with 30
IQ does not necessarily imply loss of skills. IQ de- healthy age-matched control children. No preopera-
cline in epilepsy has been discussed by Besag, tive differences were found between patients and
Fowler, and Pool (1991). They showed that the ma- controls with regard to verbal and ®gural memory
jority of children in their series who had any falling performance. Memory performance did not change
IQ did not decline in absolute ability. The mental age after surgery. However, language performance and
calculated from the raw scores of the cognitive tests attention improved signi®cantly three months and
continued to improve, but at a rate that lagged be- one year after surgery respectively. Individual eval-
hind the increase in chronological age, with the re- uations of memory revealed that ®ve children im-
sult that their IQ, which is a quotient depending on proved and four deteriorated. The deterioration in
mental age and chronological age, declined. This si- memory appeared to be related to poor seizure con-
tuation is what some would call `stagnation' rather trol. They concluded that hemispheric differences in
than `deterioration'. Nevertheless, the fact that the postoperative memory were less in children than in
child is not developing at the expected rate is a cause adults, possibly because children are capable of
for concern and any intervention that might allow greater brain plasticity.
the child to develop at a more normal rate would be Dlugos, Moss, Duhaime, and Brooks-Kayal (1999)
very worthwhile. carried out comprehensive neuropsychological test-
There are still many unanswered questions with ing on eight children (11 years 10 months±16 years)
regard to memory impairment and epilepsy. This who underwent temporal lobectomy (®ve left, two
subject has recently been reviewed by Helmstaedter right) for temporal lobe epilepsy. They tested IQ,
and Kurthen (2001). Poor memory seems to be a fre- verbal learning, naming, visual memory, sight word
quent complaint among adult patients but formal recognition, reading comprehension and calculation.
testing often reveals no de®cits. This could re¯ect the All of their ®ve children who underwent left temporal
shortcomings of the tests used or it could indicate that lobectomy had signi®cant language-related cognitive
many people think that their memory is poor but in- de®cits on postoperative testing. Four of the patients
dividuals with a neurological condition such as epi- had verbal learning de®cits. The authors pointed out
lepsy have more reason to imagine that they have that IQ testing alone did not reliably identify these
particular problems in this area. Two recent studies de®cits.
(Elixhauser, Leidy, Meador, Means, & Willian, 1999; Williams, Griebel, Sharp and Boop (1998) tested
Sawrie et al., 1999) have suggested that complaints of nine children under 16 years of age who had a tem-
poor memory may be related more to depressed mood poral lobectomy (®ve left, four right). Paired t tests
than to impairment of memory itself. In contrast, did not reveal marked changes in cognitive function
Blake, Wroe, Breen, and McCarthy (2000) showed after surgery. However, there were decreases in
accelerated forgetting in patients with epilepsy who delayed verbal memory.
seemed unimpaired in usual memory tests. Szabo et al. (1998) performed neuropsychological
There is a lack of studies on memory in children tests on 14 children (7±12 years) undergoing tem-
and teenagers with epilepsy. It could be argued that poral lobe resection for refractory epilepsy. Immedi-
it is particularly important to determine such de®cits ate verbal memory decreased signi®cantly in
in an age group in which education plays such a children who performed above the median preoper-
major role, especially in view of reports of `under- atively and tended to decrease in children who had a
achievement' in young people with epilepsy and a left temporal lobe resection. The whole group had a
normal IQ. The majority of the studies have been on statistically signi®cant decrease in delayed verbal
small numbers of patients with temporal lobe epi- memory.
lepsy before and after surgery. Occasional papers There seems to be considerable evidence for the
have examined other types of epilepsy or have been suggestion that left (dominant) temporal lobectomy
based on multicentre data. is associated with verbal memory de®cits, although
Pavone et al. (2001) carried out detailed psycho- this is generally considered to be a small price to pay
logical testing of 16 right-handed children with ab- for the advantages of freedom from seizures. Right
sence epilepsy and 16 matched controls without (non-dominant) temporal lobectomy does not seem
epilepsy. The children with absence epilepsy had to be associated with the same degree of non-verbal
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 109

de®cit, and memory de®cits in children appear to be The increasing role of genetics in de®ning the causes
less than in adults (Lendt et al., 1999). The degree of of both epilepsy and learning disability has been both
memory impairment following temporal lobectomy clari®ed and clouded by the extraordinary advances
may depend on a number of factors, including the in this area over recent years. There is a steady
presence of a clear abnormality such as gross hip- stream of individual genetic disorders that are being
pocampal sclerosis in the removed temporal lobe, the identi®ed in individuals or families, resulting in the
age of seizure onset, the age at surgery and the combination of epilepsy and learning disability.
surgical technique. The role of antiepileptic drugs There is also an attempt to isolate genes responsible
and memory is much less clear. Results from larger for the combination of epilepsy and learning disabil-
numbers of children are required. This will almost ity. As more and more information is gathered it has
certainly imply cooperation between multiple centres become increasingly apparent that what seems to be
using standardised protocols in prospective studies. a single phenotype may be the result of more than one
genetic abnormality and that a single genetic abnor-
mality may result in a tremendous variation in phe-
Causes of learning problems in children
notype. An example of the latter is tuberous sclerosis,
with epilepsy
which is inherited as an autosomal dominant, al-
Relationships between epilepsy and learning. It is though many cases are spontaneous mutations. The
important to distinguish between three possible in- gene is noted for highly variable expressivity: within
terrelationships between epilepsy and learning: the same family one member may have neither epi-
1. brain damage or dysfunction causing epilepsy lepsy nor learning disability whereas another may
and also causing learning disability (Figure 1a); have severe, disabling epilepsy and profound learn-
2. epilepsy causing brain damage or dysfunction ing disability. The genetic heterogeneity is illustrated
which, in turn, results in learning disability by the fact that at least two genes have been identi®ed
(Figure 1b); as being responsible for tuberous sclerosis, on chro-
3. epilepsy directly causing learning disability mosome 16 and chromosome 9.
(Figure 1c). A particularly interesting development over recent
The ®rst situation in which brain damage or dys- years has been the identi®cation of genetic disorders
function causes epilepsy and also, independently, leading to cortical malformations, which result in
results in learning disability can result from a large both epilepsy and learning disability. For example,
number of different causes. These causes include all Allen and Walsh (1999) have identi®ed a speci®c
the usual main categories, such as genetic/chromo- gene `doublecortin' that is mutated in patients who
somal, in¯ammatory, infectious, neoplastic and have a condition known as subcortical band het-
traumatic. A full discussion of each of the causes is erotopia in which an abnormal band of neurones in
beyond the scope of this paper. For example, the the white matter underlies the relatively normal
Oxford genetics database lists several hundred con- cortex, giving an appearance of a second layer of
ditions associated with seizures or epilepsy, most of cortex deep to the usual position, generally bilater-
which are also associated with learning disability. ally. Allen and Walsh have pointed out that in the
same families double cortex may occur in females
whereas males have the much more severe condition
of lissencephaly which is characterised by an
abnormally thickened cortex with decreased or even
absent surface convolutions. There is increasing
evidence that a number of other cortical malforma-
tions may have a genetic origin. Duncan (1997) has
provided an excellent overview of cortical malforma-
tions and their role in epilepsy. Reviews of the role of
genetics in epilepsy include those by Bate and
Gardiner (1999) and Berkovic and Scheffer (1997).
Berkovic and Mulley (1996) identi®ed the ®rst gene
for an `idiopathic' epilepsy. This work implies that
not only the terminology but also the underlying
concept behind so-called idiopathic epilepsy will
need to be reconsidered. It appears that many of the
epilepsies that were previously classi®ed as `idio-
pathic' will need to be reclassi®ed as `symptomatic',
in other words they will be reclassi®ed from `cause
unknown' to `cause known', as research in the gen-
etics of the epilepsies expands (Besag, 1999b).
Figure 1 The possible relationships between epilepsy As stated in the previous section on epidemiologi-
and learning disability cal studies, in general, the greater the degree of
110 Frank M.C. Besag

mental handicap, the more likely epilepsy becomes. learning disability refers to learning disability that is
This implies that those chromosomal and genetic caused by factors currently affecting the individual
disorders that result in severe or profound mental that are potentially reversible or treatable (Besag,
handicap will be associated with a high incidence of 2001c). With regard to children who have epilepsy,
epilepsy. This trend appears to be con®rmed by data the two main factors that may cause state-depend-
(see section on epidemiological studies), although ent learning disability are antiepileptic medication
it is interesting to note that some conditions, for and the epilepsy itself. Because state-dependent
example Down syndrome, generally result in severe learning disability is potentially reversible and
learning disability but cause only a relatively modest treatable, failure to recognise it and manage the
increase in the likelihood of epilepsy (Johannsen, situation appropriately is failure to serve the patient
Christensen, Goldstein, Nielsen, & Mai, 1996). adequately. The ®rst step in managing state-
Chromosomal abnormalities and a dominantly- dependent learning disability is to think of the
inherited condition (tuberous sclerosis) have already diagnosis. Unfortunately this ®rst step is often not
been discussed brie¯y. There are several single case taken and the result is that the condition is neither
reports appearing in the literature of syndromes that recognised nor treated.
result from a genetic defect and are associated with The effects of epilepsy on learning can be placed in
both learning disability and epilepsy with no obvious the subcategories of pre-ictal, ictal and post-ictal. A
metabolic abnormality. Most of these are autosomal more complete discussion of the peri-ictal effects in
recessive conditions. In addition, metabolic condi- general appears later in the section on the beha-
tions may cause learning disability and epilepsy. The vioural effects of epilepsy. There appear to be no
classical example is phenylketonuria. This is a par- formal studies on the effects of prodrome or aura on
ticularly interesting example because dietary mani- learning, although prodrome, because it can affect
pulation, using a low phenylalanine diet, can reduce mood and sometimes lasts for days, probably also
or prevent the learning disability. Epilepsy is repor- affects performance in the classroom. Multiple auras
ted to occur in a high proportion of untreated cases, can be very anxiety provoking (see later) and this
typically 25% (Aicardi, 1992). This is only one could also affect the performance of the child at
example of many hundred rare metabolic disorders school.
that can cause epilepsy and learning disability. A dramatic example of ictal state-dependent
There are several syndromes that include learning learning disability is the situation of nonconvulsive
disability and a high prevalence of epilepsy but do status epilepticus. Some children have periods of
not have an identi®ed genetic disorder. However, the time in which the normal EEG is replaced by either
situation is changing rapidly. For example, the continuous spike-wave epileptiform discharges or
MECP2 gene for Rett syndrome has recently been such frequent discharges that the clinical effect is
identi®ed (Amir et al., 1999) and has been localised continuous. The classical form of nonconvulsive
to the distal long arm of the X chromosome. Rett status epilepticus is accompanied by generalised
syndrome is characterised by abnormal hand spike-wave discharges. However, complex partial
movements, loss of hand skills, mental retardation seizure status epilepticus may also occur. In these
and epilepsy. The classical form of Rett syndrome circumstances the EEG changes may be less obvious.
occurs only in females. In contrast, no speci®c uni- Nonconvulsive status epilepticus often responds
versal gene defect has yet been found for Sturge- promptly to treatment with an intravenous benzo-
Weber syndrome which consists of a vascular diazepine, such as diazemuls. Rarely the condition
malformation of the face (`port-wine stain') and may last for long periods. It may even continue in-
the meninges, often accompanied by epilepsy and de®nitely and be unresponsive to treatment. Some-
mental retardation. times the manifestations of nonconvulsive status
epilepticus can be remarkably subtle. The child may
Epilepsy as a cause of learning problems. The be able to interact with his or her surroundings,
obvious situation in which epilepsy may cause although less well than normally. Nonconvulsive
learning problems is that in which a child has pro- status epilepticus causes a spectrum of disability
longed, brain-damaging status epilepticus. The from the child who appears relatively alert to one who
brain damage, in turn, results in learning disability. is in a zombie-like state, hypotonic, unable to re-
The situation is represented in Figure 1b. Can epi- spond and unable to walk. The extent of the epilep-
lepsy lead directly to learning disability without tiform discharges on the EEG do not appear to
causing brain damage? In order to answer this correlate directly with the degree of impairment;
question satisfactorily it may be helpful to introduce some children with less frequent epileptiform dis-
the concept of state-dependent learning disability. It charges may be grossly impaired by them whereas
is very important to distinguish permanent learning others who have continuous discharges may never-
disability, on one hand, from state-dependent theless be able to walk and to respond in a primitive
learning disability on the other. Permanent learning way to questioning. It is clearly important to recog-
disability may arise from any of the causes discussed nise this condition and to diagnose it by carrying out
earlier in this paper. The term state-dependent an emergency EEG. If the EEG con®rms the diagno-
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 111

sis then it may be appropriate to treat immediately sample of children, the paroxysmal EEG discharges
with intravenous medication such as diazemuls. The improved in only four of the eight children. In three
longer-term antiepileptic drug treatment should also the number of discharges was similar on treatment
be reviewed if the child is subject to repeated bouts of and in the remaining case the number increased.
nonconvulsive status epilepticus. In the author's This paper illustrates the multifactorial in¯uences
experience, lamotrigine seems to be highly effective on learning and behaviour in children with epilepsy.
in preventing recurrent attacks in at least some It would be reasonable to suggest that at least some
children. children with transitory cognitive impairment might
Frequent absence seizures may affect attention improve with the right type of antiepileptic medica-
and learning. Infrequent absence seizures are usu- tion that reduces the epileptiform discharges without
ally associated with a good outcome for learning. adding signi®cant drug-related impairments.
However, as shown by the current author, absence
seizures sometimes occur thousands of times daily Frequent focal discharges. The role of frequent lo-
(Besag, 1995a). In these circumstances learning and calised epileptiform discharges, not amounting to
social interaction are grossly impaired. nonconvulsive status epilepticus but suf®cient to
cause non-transitory impairments, has been unsat-
Transitory cognitive impairment. The basic con- isfactorily described in the literature. Frequent
cept of transitory cognitive impairment is that an frontal discharges can result in highly disinhibited
epileptiform discharge that does not manifest as a behaviour. This type of behaviour is very liable to
seizure by simple observation may nevertheless im- affect both formal classroom learning and the ability
pair cognitive function. If these discharges occur to learn from social situations.
frequently then they may at least lead to frustration Left temporal discharges may affect verbal abilit-
in the classroom. Observation of videotapes of young ies, as shown by a number of studies, including
people undergoing testing during transitory cognitive those of Stores and co-workers (Stores & Hart,
impairment has demonstrated the obvious frustra- 1975). When the source of frequent left temporal
tion that they experience. Although the cognitive epileptiform discharges is removed, speech may im-
impairment itself may be transitory there may be an prove in some cases.
ongoing effect on con®dence in the classroom, which
may affect learning. Frequent hemispheric discharges. A gross example
Binnie and colleagues have carried out a number of frequent localised discharges is the situation in
of studies on transitory cognitive impairment (Aarts, which the individual has an abnormal hemisphere
Binnie, Smit, & Wilkins, 1984; Binnie & Marston, which is a continuous source of a torrent of these
1992; Marston, Besag, Binnie, & Fowler, 1993). They discharges. Many different pathological conditions
showed clearly that left-sided (dominant) discharges can result in this situation, including hemimegal-
impaired verbal tasks and right sided (non-domin- encephaly and Rasmussen's encephalitis. Congen-
ant) discharges affected a test of visuo-spatial func- ital malformations such as porencephalic cysts can
tion (Aarts et al., 1984; Binnie, 1989). This subject also cause frequent hemispheric discharges. Hemi-
has been reviewed by Binnie (1989; Binnie & Mar- spherectomy, which involves either the removal or
ston, 1992). Marston et al. (1993) found that an- disconnection of the cerebral cortex on the entire
tiepileptic drug treatment of children with transitory affected side, can abolish the discharges and in-
cognitive impairment was associated with an im- crease learning. (See later section on surgery and
provement of psychosocial function. The treatment learning.)
of obvious seizures was thought to have been optimal
before the addition of antiepileptic medication to Post-ictal state-dependent learning disability. At
treat the epileptiform discharges. However, the ®rst, the concept of post-ictal state-dependent
additional treatment resulted not only in a reduction learning disability might appear to be both obvious
of the epileptiform discharges but also in a reduction and unimportant. However, in some cases it is nei-
of the frequency of obvious seizures. The reduction of ther. If a person is having very frequent seizures,
the obvious seizures was considered to be a con- they may not have time to recover from one seizure
founding factor in this study. Ronen, Richards, before they have another. The result is that they are
Cunningham, Secord, and Rosenbloom (2000) failed in a constant post-ictal state. Treating the seizures,
to demonstrate any improvement in learning when a so that they are not so frequent, allows the person to
small group of 8 children, aged 6±12 years, who had emerge from the constant post-ictal state and to
epileptiform discharges were treated with sodium function at a much higher level. Even people who are
valproate. In contrast, treatment with valproate was experienced in managing epilepsy may incorrectly
associated with reduced performance on verbal assume that learning disability is permanent when it
memory tasks, increased delays on attentional tasks may be state-dependent, treatable and reversible.
and increased parental reports of internalising Another example of post-ictal effects on learning is
problems. It should be pointed out that, in addition the child who has frequent nocturnal seizures. In a
to the fact that this was a small and heterogeneous series of 15 patients examined by the author's team,
112 Frank M.C. Besag

a large number of nocturnal seizures were recorded seizures over the period of testing and the fourth
by video telemetry. These seizures had generally group had both epileptiform discharges and one or
been unobserved and unsuspected by awake night several seizures during the test sessions. It should
staff. In one case, 208 seizures were recorded in a be emphasised, however, that no obvious seizures
single night on the video telemetry. Frequent noc- occurred: the seizures were brief and subtle. They
turnal seizures may affect daytime performance, were only detected after extensive analysis of the
both through direct post-ictal effects and through video recordings. Patients and parents were unaware
the effect of a broken night's sleep. of these seizures. There were some possible con-
A particularly interesting example of post-ictal founding factors in this study, such as differences in
state-dependent learning disability is the situation of the types of seizures and medication between the
a child who has very frequent epileptiform dischar- groups. The results showed signi®cant differences in
ges overnight. This situation is known as continuous the performance on intelligence subtests and tests of
spike-wave during slow wave sleep (CSWS) or elec- complex information processing between the control
trical status epilepticus of slow wave sleep (ESES). group and the fourth group, i.e., the group with both
To satisfy the diagnostic criteria of ESES, at least epileptiform discharges and subtle seizures. The
85% of slow-wave sleep must be occupied by spike- authors discussed in some detail the possible rea-
wave discharges. The classical manifestation of sons for these differences and concluded that they
ESES is the Landau-Kleffner syndrome of acquired were probably the effects of both ictal and post-ictal
epileptic aphasia (Landau & Kleffner, 1957). In this changes. It would have been of great interest if these
condition the child develops an auditory agnosia and authors could have shown an improvement in this
loses the ability to understand speech. In some cases particular group of subjects at other times when they
the ability to interpret environmental sounds such were in another state with regard to the presence of
as birdsong may also be lost. Because these children subtle seizures or epileptiform discharges. Ideally
cannot understand their own speech they may be- this same group of people would have been tested in
come mute. Although the progress of this condition four states: with both discharges and subtle sei-
varies greatly from one child to another and may, in zures, with subtle seizures and no discharges, with
some cases, resolve spontaneously there is a grave discharges but no subtle seizures and with neither
concern that in some children permanent speech discharges nor subtle seizures. Although this would
de®cits occur. Treatments include antiepileptic me- have given a very informative set of test results, it is
dication such as sodium valproate and lamotrigine, highly unlikely that these different states could
steroid treatment and neurosurgery. The role of all have been achieved in the same group of real
neurosurgery in this condition is discussed in a later patients.
section. All these studies point to the fact that post-ictal
Gordon (2000) has recently reviewed the effect of effects on learning in children with epilepsy have
epilepsy on language function. He concluded that probably been underestimated and have often been
there seems to be no doubt that language develop- ignored. Successful treatment of the epilepsy, pro-
ment can be adversely affected by the presence of vided the treatment itself does not lead to cognitive
epileptic activity as demonstrated by spike-wave problems, should result in considerable bene®ts to
discharges in the EEG. He also concluded that the learning. This opportunity should not be missed. The
same activity can cause disintegration of language adverse and bene®cial effects of antiepileptic treat-
function, although he stated that further research ment are discussed in the next section.
may be needed to establish the exact correlation. It
was pointed out that other aspects of learning may
Antiepileptic treatment and learning
also be impaired selectively by electrical status epi-
lepticus of slow wave sleep. These conclusions have Antiepileptic drug treatment. Parents and physi-
considerable implications with regard to early treat- cians are naturally concerned about the possibility
ment of children who present with continuous spike- that any treatment might affect learning adversely.
wave discharges in slow wave sleep or electrical The sedative effects of the wrong type of antiepileptic
status epilepticus of slow wave sleep. (Also see sec- medication or excessive doses may be all too obvi-
tion on surgery for ESES.) ous. In children, a paradoxical effect may be seen
Aldenkamp et al. (1996) have suggested that epi- with drugs such as phenobarbital, the benzodiaze-
leptiform discharges may have a post-ictal effect on pines and vigabatrin. These drugs may cause a
cognitive function without causing transitory cogni- condition mimicking attention de®cit disorder. The
tive impairment. They compared four groups, aged correct management is not to prescribe stimulant
9±21 years: three with epilepsy and a control group. drugs such as methylphenidate but to use a different
The ®rst group were age-matched `non-neurological' antiepileptic drug that does not have this very un-
normal controls, the second group were patients desirable adverse effect. Antiepileptic drugs may af-
with established epilepsy who had no epileptiform fect schooling in more subtle ways. For example,
discharges or seizures during testing, the third excessively high blood levels of carbamazepine may
group had epileptiform discharges but no detectable cause diplopia which, in turn, may make it dif®cult
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 113

for the child to perform adequately in reading or children with epilepsy before and after drug discon-
writing. tinuation. The inclusion criteria were age between 7
There have been many studies on the effect of and 18 years, a diagnosis of epilepsy, seizure free-
antiepileptic drugs on cognition. Most of these have dom for at least 1 year and monotherapy antiepi-
been on adults but some have been on children. leptic drug treatment. The children were compared
Trimble and co-workers have made a notable con- with matched healthy controls. One hundred and
tribution to this ®eld (Trimble, 1979, 1987a; Trimble two eligible children entered the study but 19 were
& Cull, 1988). Meador (Meador, Loring, Huh, Galla- regarded as dropouts, leaving 83 children and their
gher, & King, 1990; Nichols, Meador, & Loring, 1993; controls. They concluded that there were clear dif-
Meador, 1998, 2000) and Aldenkamp (Aldenkamp ferences between the reports made by the children
et al., 1993; Tonnby et al., 1994; Aldenkamp et al., themselves and those made by the parents in that
1994; Vermeulen & Aldenkamp, 1995) have also the children did not report more complaints during
contributed signi®cantly. Vermeulen and Aldenk- treatment in comparison to the matched controls
amp (1995) have reviewed the cognitive adverse ef- and also did not report changes in cognitive com-
fects of chronic antiepileptic drug treatment but plaints after drug discontinuation, except for being
again much of this review is based on data collected less tired. The parents, however, did report signi®-
from adults. They have correctly pointed out that cant improvements after drug discontinuation, pri-
many of the studies were on a relatively small marily related to alertness. It is dif®cult to draw ®rm
number of patients, with the implication that ®nding conclusions about a study that uses subjective
`no difference' between groups may simply have been measures. On one hand, these may be more relevant
a re¯ection of a lack of power of the study. Studies on in measuring quality of life but on the other hand
children include those by Calandre, Dominguez- they appear less scienti®cally rigorous than objective
Granados, Gomez-Rubio, and Molina-Font (1990), measures. Perhaps the best solution would be to use
Forsythe, Butler, Berg, and McGuire (1991) and a combination of subjective, questionnaire-based
Aldenkamp et al. (1993). Most of these studies were measures and objective cognitive tests.
on relatively small numbers of children. Vermeulen A particularly interesting paper is that of Jamb-
and Aldenkamp concluded that, although over 90 aqueÂ, Chiron, Dumas, Mumford, and Dulac (2000)
investigations had been conducted over the pre- who examined the mental and behavioural outcome
ceding 25 years to determine what effect antiepilep- of infantile epilepsy treated with vigabatrin in chil-
tic drugs have on cognition, no satisfactory answer dren who had tuberous sclerosis. Vigabatrin is
to the question could be given, primarily because of highly effective in treating infantile spasms in chil-
poor methodology, design and analysis. They were dren who have tuberous sclerosis. They found that in
not of the opinion that they could provide a clear those children with infantile spasms whose spasms
answer to the question of whether antiepileptic drugs came under control, the mental score increased
in therapeutic doses had any cognitive effects at all, signi®cantly and the behaviour improved, although
good or bad. If the studies reviewed were limited to most of them continued to have partial seizures. In
those that were using monotherapy in patients with contrast, patients with partial epilepsy and no
epilepsy and had control group data for comparison spasms had more subtle improvements in behaviour
with appropriate repeated measures analysis and but no change in mental score. The whole group
good information, only 4 studies remained (some consisted of 13 children who underwent psycho-
reporting on multiple drugs), 2 on phenobarbitone, 3 metric and behaviour evaluation before vigabatrin
on valproate, 3 on carbamazepine and 1 on pheny- was commenced and after a mean of 3 years on
toin (Calandre et al., 1990; Ronnberg, Samuelsson, vigabatrin; 7 had infantile spasms and were all free
& Soderfeldt, 1992; Helmstaedter, Wagner, & Elger, of these seizures before 2 years of age, and 5 con-
1993; Tonnby et al., 1994). Only 2 of these studies tinued to have rare partial seizures. The authors
were on children. However, all of these studies had concluded that the cessation of spasms with vig-
low numbers of subjects in the subgroups apart from abatrin is associated with signi®cant improvement in
the Tonnby et al. (1994) study that had 56 school- cognition and behaviour in children with tuberous
age children in the carbamazepine group. Even this sclerosis. Infantile spasms are associated with a
study had only small numbers in the phenytoin gross EEG abnormality, hypsarrhythmia. It seems
group (10 children) and the valproate group (17 highly likely that such a gross disturbance of elec-
children). Vermeulen and Aldenkamp have also trical function in the brain is responsible, at least in
concluded that not even the methodology for ap- part, for the cognitive decline seen when babies de-
proaching this area has been decided. They do, velop infantile spasms. A number of workers, inclu-
however, make a number of recommendations for ding the current author, surmised that the effective
research into the cognitive effects of antiepileptic treatment of infantile spasms would lead to a pro-
drugs, the details of which remain open to debate. nounced improvement in intellectual outcome. It is
Aldenkamp et al. (1998) carried out a multicentre, pleasing to note that the study of Jambaque et al.
controlled, parallel-group, non-randomised clinical seems to con®rm this. Their results also add weight
investigation of cognitive complaints of seizure-free to the comments made earlier about state-dependent
114 Frank M.C. Besag

learning disability. It would appear that this is an concluded that their ®ndings gave some support for
example in which the epilepsy may cause learning the assumption that a reduction in the amount of
disability directly but if the abnormal discharges and inter-ictal epileptiform activity was a possible reason
infantile spasms are allowed to continue untreated for the observed improvement in behaviour and
then permanent cognitive impairment may result. It alertness. They acknowledged, however, that there
would be valuable to gain more research data to might be other reasons for increased alertness in this
con®rm this important hypothesis because it has group of patients.
major implications both for treating and preventing Another of the new antiepileptic drugs, topira-
learning disability. mate, has been used in treating a resistant form of
It is somewhat paradoxical that vigabatrin, a drug epilepsy in childhood, the so-called severe myoclonic
that is so useful for treating infantile spasms, can epilepsy in infancy or Dravet syndrome (Dravet,
probably cause learning problems in older children Bueau, Guerrini, Giraud, & Roger, 1992). This syn-
when it is used to treat partial seizures. There is drome is characterised by seizure onset in the ®rst
considerable anecdotal evidence that vigabatrin can year of life, usually with febrile convulsions or gen-
cause attention problems and overactivity in chil- eralised clonic seizures, followed by myoclonic and/
dren, especially those who already have a degree of or partial and/or typical absence seizures between 2
learning disability (Wallace, 1996a; Besag, 2001b). It and 3 years of age. Initially the EEG does not usually
would appear that the same drug can be very useful show paroxysmal abnormalities but recordings
in treating and preventing learning disability in within the second year of life show inter-ictal gen-
children who have infantile spasms whereas it may eralised spike-wave complexes and/or generalised
cause or exacerbate schooling problems in older polyspike-waves. There is delayed development from
children treated for partial seizures. In any case, the second year and unsteadiness is generally seen.
vigabatrin is currently recommended primarily for A recent publication by Nieto-Barrera, Candeau,
infantile spasms and is avoided in treating partial Nieto-Jiminez, Correa, and Ruiz del Portal (2000) on
seizures unless there is no other reasonable alter- 18 patients aged 2±22 (mean 13.3 years) demon-
native because it may cause visual ®eld constriction strated that over half (56%) had a reduction in seiz-
(Eke, Talbot, & Lawden, 1997; Luchetti, Amadi, ure frequency by more than 50%. Three became
Gobbi, & Bertani 2000; Hardus, Engelsman, Van seizure free. They found that the most dramatic re-
Veelen, & Stilma, 2000). The debate about the role sponse was with atypical absence seizures. This
for vigabatrin continues, however. Prasad, Penney, implies that such children might improve in cogni-
and Buckley (2001) treated 73 children with vig- tive performance, although they included no formal
abatrin over a 7-year period and concluded that testing to show such an effect in their report. More-
vigabatrin was effective in producing a signi®cant land, Griesemar, and Holden (1999) demonstrated a
reduction in seizure frequency. This led them to state more mixed response to this drug in 49 children with
that, despite emerging concerns regarding visual refractory seizures. They stated that more than half
®eld constriction, vigabatrin retains an important the children experienced adverse effects which could
role in the medical management of childhood epi- interfere with learning at school but 20% demon-
lepsy. This was a mixed group of patients aged 5±257 strated increased alertness or improved behaviour.
months when vigabatrin treatment was initiated. There is a strong argument for considering the way
Only 12 could undergo static threshold perimetry different childhood epilepsy syndromes affect learn-
and 2 of these had the well-described visual ®eld ing. Shields (2000) has reviewed what he terms
constriction. `catastrophic epilepsy in childhood'. In his paper he
The earlier studies by Besag (1994b, 1995a) and a discusses not only speci®c epilepsy syndromes but
more recent study by Eriksson, Knutsson, and Ner- other syndromes or clinical situations that are
gardh (2001) have con®rmed that lamotrigine is associated with epilepsy and learning disability. He
highly effective in treating epileptiform discharges. In also discusses the important question of whether
the study by Besag (1994a) 17 children and teenag- therapy can alter outcome. Some of the syndromes,
ers who had frequent spike wave discharges were such as severe myoclonic epilepsy of infancy and
monitored with a validated portable spike-wave Otahara syndrome (early infantile epileptic enceph-
monitor (Besag, Mills, Wardale, Andrew, & Craggs, alopathy with suppression-burst EEG) always
1989b). He found that 8 of the 17 patients had a appear to be associated with learning disability.
reduction in spike-wave discharges of 80% or more. However, early treatment in other forms of epilepsy,
In some cases the reduction was over 99%. Eriksson notably infantile spasms (see earlier), may result in
et al. (2001) studied 12 patients aged 4±21 years an improved outcome for learning.
using 24-hour video-EEG recording. The recordings The overall conclusions with regard to the role of
were made both during placebo and drug phases. antiepileptic drug treatment in relation to cognition
Periods of repeated epileptiform discharges longer is that some children bene®t greatly, a middle group
than 30 seconds were reduced in length and number do not appear to improve or decline and a third group
during the lamotrigine phase. It should be noted that are made worse by the medication. A good example
most of these subjects had learning disability. They would be the use of lamotrigine in the treatment of
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 115

severe myoclonic epilepsy in infancy. Guerrini et al. the duration of epilepsy was less than 10 years then
(1998) have shown that lamotrigine can aggravate the remission rate was 88% but if it was more than
seizures in severe myoclonic epilepsy infancy, almost 20 years the remission rate fell to 57%. Ten patients
certainly implying a poor cognitive outcome, but were classi®ed as learning disabled not only on the
Wallace (1998) has reported that some children with basis of low IQ but also because they had severe
this syndrome may bene®t from the drug. It could be behavioural and attention dif®culties. In this group
argued that the children who bene®ted did not have only 37% had a remission of the seizures compared
the true syndrome of severe myoclonic epilepsy of with 74% of those who were cognitively normal.
infancy but it is much more likely that the difference These results seem to emphasise the importance of
in results re¯ects individual variations between dif- early intervention. The suggestion is that if the sei-
ferent children. It is the responsibility of the clinician zures are stopped early in life then the outcome in
to assess each child with epilepsy and cognitive terms of seizure control is likely to be good. However,
problems as an individual and to monitor response there are several confounding factors. For example,
to treatment carefully. Children who have frequent early age of seizure onset may be associated with
epileptiform discharges and/or frequent seizures more severe underlying brain problems that may
should be treated effectively and as soon as possible predispose to poor outcome.
because at least some have reversible cognitive Duncan (2001) has also emphasised that if the
problems resulting from the epilepsy. rest of the brain is normal and a structural lesion is
removed then a good result is likely to be achieved,
Does epilepsy surgery improve learning ability? In even if the lesion is incompletely removed. Do these
previous sections reference has been made to the results imply that surgery should not be offered to
possibility of various types of surgery improving children with learning disability? In the past, an IQ
cognition. Examples have been given of children of less than 60 or 70 was considered to be an ex-
undergoing hemispherectomy and left temporal lob- clusion criterion by at least some clinicians. For ex-
ectomy, whose cognition has clearly improved after ample, in the second edition of his highly regarded
surgery. However, the prediction of cognitive out- textbook on childhood epilepsies, O'Donohoe (1985)
come is not straightforward. Although there are listed an IQ of less than 60 as being an exclusion
many published papers on the overall outcome of criterion for epilepsy surgery in children. Such an
epilepsy surgery, most of these concentrate on the exclusion criterion would be considered inappropri-
success in terms of reduction or elimination of sei- ate now, especially in view of the comments already
zures rather than other measures such as cognition made about the possibility of cognitive improvement
or quality of life. The question of classi®cation of following surgery. However, the analysis of Vascon-
outcome following epilepsy surgery has been dis- cellos et al. (2001) of 100 patients aged 2±20 years
cussed in a recent International League Against again con®rmed that those with lower age of onset of
Epilepsy Commission Report (Wieser et al., 2001). epilepsy (£ 24 months of age) and lower IQ had a
Again, the emphasis is on remission of seizures poorer outcome in terms of seizure frequency after
rather than other variables, although the authors surgery. They concluded that the poor outcome was
discuss the importance of quality of life outcome based on early age of seizure onset and appeared to
measurement, suggesting that this should be com- be independent of aetiology.
prehensive, re¯ecting behaviour, schooling, occupa- Bourgeois et al. (1999) analysed results from a
tion, psychosocial, self-suf®ciency, marriage and series of 200 children with epilepsy aged 1±15 years
reproduction, and mental and cognitive domains. who had surgery for a variety of brain lesions, most
Because temporal lobectomy is the most fre- commonly oligodendrogliomas, dysembryoplastic
quently performed epilepsy surgical procedure, it neuroepithelial tumours and astrocytomas. Only 9
will be discussed in some detail. (5.2%) derived no bene®t from the surgery in terms of
In a recent review (Hennessy et al., 2001), the seizure control. Improvement in cognitive function
outcome of surgical treatment of temporal lobe le- was reported in 24.6%.
sions was assessed in 80 patients, 41 of whom were It is also interesting to note that psychosis is no
20 years of age or younger at surgery. In 75 of the 80 longer considered to be an absolute contraindica-
patients the age of onset of seizures was under 20 tion to surgery. Epilepsy-associated psychosis may
years of age, with the largest group, 36 patients, occur in teenagers ± see later, and adults. The
having an age of seizure onset of 0±4 years. Good study by Reutens, Savard, Andermann, Dubeau,
outcome was particularly associated with the resec- and Olivier (1997) was in adults but the results are
tion of dysembryoplastic neuroepithelial tumours. probably applicable to teenagers. They concluded
However, the most outstanding results of this series that with appropriate psychiatric intervention, pa-
were those related to age at surgery and duration of tients with chronic psychosis and refractory epi-
epilepsy. If patients had the temporal lobe surgery lepsy can undergo the necessary investigations for
before the age of 15 years, 84% achieved a good neurosurgery and can also bene®t from the surgery.
outcome, compared with a rate of only 44% if the These authors pointed out that it is often assumed
surgery was carried out after the age of 30 years. If that disturbed behaviour will prevent adequate
116 Frank M.C. Besag

pre-operative evaluation but this is not necessarily gains of 24 points in verbal and 29 points in per-
the case. formance IQ. A girl with a full-scale IQ of 64 before a
The largest study to determine whether presurgi- right temporal lobectomy had an increase of 15
cal IQ predicts seizure outcome after temporal lob- points, with gains of 20 points in verbal and 10 in
ectomy is that by Chelune et al. in 1,034 adult performance IQ. Their overall results are very inter-
patients from 8 centres in the Bozeman consortium esting, in that they illustrate that the side of the
(Chelune et al., 1998). They found that patients who surgery does not seem to have a major effect on the
continued to have seizures after surgery had a outcome, in contrast to the conclusions of some
slightly lower mean IQ and that this difference was previous studies. They also emphasise that low IQ
statistically signi®cant. Seven of the 8 centres re- should not be considered as a contraindication
ported lower pre-operative IQ scores for patients who to temporal lobectomy, as well illustrated by the
continued to have seizures after surgery. For IQ £ 75, two children who had the greatest gains in IQ after
38% continued to have seizures. The ®gure for IQ surgery.
76±109 was 23.8% and for IQ ³ 110 it was 16.9% A major omission from these papers is any dis-
seizure free. If a structural lesion was present then cussion of the role of the pre-operative EEG. It would
those with IQ £ 75 had a 3.9 times increased risk for have been very interesting to know whether the
seizures compared to those with a higher IQ. children who improved had very abnormal EEGs
Westerveld et al. (2000) examined the cognitive with frequent epileptiform discharges before surgery
outcome of temporal lobectomy in 82 patients, aged and whether these discharges were reduced or ab-
6±17 years, from eight centres in the largest pub- sent after the surgery. The present author has sug-
lished study of this type in children; 43 had a left and gested that the strongest indicator for lack of
39 a right temporal lobectomy. With regard to verbal acquisition of skills in young people with epilepsy
cognitive skills, 67 (82%) had no signi®cant change, may be the presence of frequent epileptiform dis-
8 (10%: 5 left, 3 right) declined by more than twice charges (Besag, 1995b), but neither Westerveld et al.
the standard error (8 points) and 7 (9%: 3 left, 4 nor Miranda and Smith mention this issue. For those
right) improved after the surgery. For performance who had a poor outcome there are other unanswered
IQ, 67 (82%) had no signi®cant change, 2 (2.4%: 1 questions which might relate both to seizure and
left, 1 right) declined by more than twice the stand- cognitive outcome. Were larger resections carried out
ard error (9 points) and 13 (16%: 10 left, 3 right) in patients who had structural lesions? Are secon-
improved after the surgery. Risk factors for signi®- dary pre-operative EEG foci important? If post-
cant decline were being older at the time of the sur- operative EEG abnormalities are important, can
gery and the presence of a structural lesion other pre-operative factors predict these abnormalities?
than mesial temporal sclerosis. A slight improve- Does the poorer IQ in patients with longer follow-up
ment in global intellectual function was signi®cantly only apply to patients with ongoing epileptiform
more likely to occur than a decline. abnormalities, as has been suggested by some
Miranda and Smith (2001) have recently published authors (Besag, 1995b)?
the results of cognitive testing before and after tem- Reviews of the outcome of hemispherectomy in
poral lobectomy on 50 young people (age range 6.43± children (Goodman, 1986; Lindsay, Ounsted, &
18.25 years). They commented that only a small Richards, 1987; Knight & Oxbury, 2000; Oxbury,
subset of papers has seriously addressed cognitive Zaiwalla, Adams, Middleton, & Oxbury, 1995) have
outcome in children after temporal lobectomy. In con®rmed that this operation is effective not only in
their group, 25 patients underwent left and 25 right improving seizure control but also in improving other
temporal lobectomy. The grouped results showed a aspects of function. Oxbury et al. (1995) reported an
small increase in performance but not verbal IQ. increase in IQ on follow-up after hemispherectomy,
Examination of individual results revealed that, of with an average of 15 points gain at 5 years.
the 50 patients, 36 (72%) had no change in verbal IQ. Chugani and co-workers (Chugani, 1995; Chugani
Performance IQ results were available for 49 of the et al., 1993) have demonstrated that surgery can be
patients, of whom 33 (67)% showed no signi®cant of great bene®t in babies with infantile spasms if
change. Of the remaining 17 patients, 12 improved focal metabolic abnormalities can be demonstrated
and 4 had a lower IQ after the temporal lobectomy. by positron emission tomography (PET). Some of
Children with dual pathology and those with a longer these infants underwent cortical resection or hemi-
follow-up interval were less likely to show an im- spherectomy. He reported that cognitive outcome
proved performance IQ. In fact, none of the children was variable, with normal function in some patients.
whose performance IQ improved had dual pathology. It is interesting that a form of epilepsy that many
Of the 14 patients (28%) whose verbal IQ changed might previously have considered to be essentially
signi®cantly, half had an increase and half a de- generalised has been shown to be focal in some cases
crease. It was interesting to note marked increases in and that this can be the basis of successful surgical
IQ for individual patients who initially had a full- treatment.
scale IQ < 70. A boy with full-scale IQ of 52 before a The novel surgical procedure known as multiple
left temporal lobectomy improved 28 points, with subpial transection, pioneered by Morrell (Morrell,
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 117

Whisler, & Bleck, 1989) can be particularly effective However, multiple subpial transection offers hope for
in some children who have the Landau-Kleffner children who have de®cits arising from an epileptic
syndrome of acquired epileptic aphasia or who have focus in an `eloquent area' of the brain, i.e., an area
other de®cits arising from continuous spike-wave that cannot be resected without producing an
during slow-wave sleep (CSWS) or electrical status unacceptable de®cit.
epilepticus of slow-wave sleep (ESES) (see earlier). The Landau-Kleffner syndrome is an important
The particular advantage is that it can be used in model demonstrating that permanent de®cits may
areas of the brain which could not be resected result from state-dependent impairment if the CSWS
without causing an unacceptable functional de®cit. or ESES continues for a long period. However, CSWS
The principle of this treatment is to isolate the epi- or ESES does not necessarily cause language de®-
leptic focus by transecting around it, preventing the cits. In some cases visuo-spatial de®cits occur and
spread of the epileptic discharges. Morrell et al. are improved with surgery.
(1989) followed up 20 of the original 32 cases, aged What is the answer to the question posed at the
5±22 years, for a period of 5 years or more. Complete beginning of this section: `Does epilepsy surgery
control of seizures was achieved in 11 of the 20 cases improved learning ability?' In some cases the answer
(55%). In a subsequent study (Morrell et al., 1995), is undoubtedly yes. In this regard, the Landau-
14 children with the Landau-Kleffner syndrome of Kleffner syndrome, although rare, is an important
acquired epileptic aphasia were treated; 7 of these model because some children improve dramatically
(50%) were said to have recovered age-appropriate with multiple subpial transection. A number of
speech and were in ordinary classes in school. These children who undergo hemispherectomy also im-
results indicated a clear improvement in speech prove with regard to cognition. What the clinician
function. It should be noted that not all other work- needs to know is how to distinguish between those
ers have had such a high success rate. In one of the children who will improve and those who will not.
more recent studies, Mulligan, Spencer, and Spencer Because the data from adult series cannot neces-
(2001) reviewed 12 patients at Yale who underwent sarily be applied to children, some uncertainty re-
multiple subpial transection. This was a mixed mains with regard to selection criteria for temporal
group of children and adults (age range 8±49 years). lobectomy in children with epilepsy. Complete re-
The outcome varied greatly from one patient to an- moval of abnormal tissue containing the epileptic
other. Some of the patients had persistent de®cits or focus should be associated with a better outcome. If
even appeared to have improved in some functions the seizure onset is early then there is a good chance
while deteriorating in others. Perhaps the most ex- that brain plasticity will have ensured that most
tensive review is that of Smith (1998) who reported useful cognitive function resides in the normal tissue
on 100 patients who underwent this procedure, of left behind and it is less likely that the child will have
whom two-thirds also had resections; 69 of these a poor cognitive outcome. This is in contrast to the
patients were in Engel class 1 or 2, with regard to poorer prognosis, with regard to memory, for adults
seizure outcome, implying that they were either free who undergo removal of an apparently normal hip-
of disabling seizures or `almost seizure free'. pocampus. If the child is carefully selected after
Irwin, Edwards, and Robinson (2000) recently thorough investigation by a skilled multidisciplinary
con®rmed the bene®ts of multiple subpial transec- team, the outcome of epilepsy neurosurgery is likely
tion in ®ve children, aged 5±10 years, with the Lan- to be good.
dau-Kleffner syndrome, both in terms of speech
function and behaviour. In the same centre, Robin-
Management of learning problems
son, Baird, Robinson, and Simonoff (2001) examined
the outcome of 18 children with the Landau-Kleffner The ®rst aim should be to prevent learning disability,
syndrome and found that no child with electrical if at all possible. This issue has been discussed in
status epilepticus of slow-wave sleep (ESES) lasting earlier sections. As already stated, some types of
longer than 36 months had normal language out- learning disability associated with epilepsy are
come. They concluded that their data supported the treatable and early treatment may not only improve
recommendation that ESES should be terminated learning but may also prevent permanent learning
within 36 months, using multiple subpial transec- disability. There seems to be growing evidence that
tion if steroid treatment is ineffective or causes treating children who have frequent epileptiform
unacceptable adverse effects. discharges or chaotic EEG abnormalities may fall
The difference in outcome between the initial good into this category of leading both to an improvement
results obtained by Morrell and relatively poorer re- in cognition and to the prevention of longer-term
sults achieved by others require explanation. Cer- learning problems. This is almost certainly true for
tainly early intervention to reduce the duration of the some babies who have infantile spasms and some
ESES to no more than 36 months would seem ad- children who have the Landau-Kleffner syndrome,
visable. The surgical technique of multiple subpial for example. It now seems extraordinary that some of
transection is not straightforward and the theory of the older textbooks stated that antiepileptic treat-
the procedure may be dif®cult to put into practice. ment for the Landau-Kleffner syndrome was only of
118 Frank M.C. Besag

bene®t in treating the seizures and did not affect Effective treatment of both the seizures and the
language development. Such conclusions were epileptiform discharges by medication or surgery,
probably based on late intervention, when perma- together with a number of enabling strategies dis-
nent damage had already occurred. Early energetic cussed elsewhere in this paper, can greatly reduce
treatment is to be preferred. However, this syndrome the impact of epilepsy on learning.
is a good example of the complexity of the decision±
making process. Some children with Landau-Kleff-
ner syndrome seem to improve without therapy Part II Epilepsy and behavioural disorders
whereas others do not. It is clear that one should not
Epidemiological studies of behaviour
wait for long periods before treating but what is an
unacceptably long period in this context? Is the limit There has been a remarkable lack of systemic epi-
of 36 months suggested by Robinson et al. (2001) too demiological studies on behaviour in children with
long? There are still unanswered questions in this epilepsy since the Isle of Wight study by Rutter et al.
regard. (1970). This showed that the rate of psychiatric dis-
Treatment should be aimed at more than just re- order was approximately 7% in the general popula-
ducing seizures. There has been a recent trend to tion of 10±11-year-old children, rising to 10.3% for
assess children much more fully than simply by re- physical disorders not affecting the nervous system,
cording seizure counts. A number of quality of life 13.3% for neurological disorders at or below the
measures have become available (Sabaz et al., 2000; brain stem, 28.6% for uncomplicated epilepsy,
Cramer et al., 1999; Hoare, Mann, & Dunn, 2000). In 37.5% for lesions above the brain stem without sei-
addition, it is evident that greater emphasis should zures and 58.3% for lesions above the brain stem
be placed on serial cognitive testing in children with associated with seizures. Most of the large epidemi-
epilepsy to ensure that if there are underlying subtle ological studies carried out since then have concen-
seizures or epileptiform activity that may be affecting trated more on education than behaviour. However,
learning, the child is treated sooner rather than there are a few exceptions.
later. McDermott et al. (1995), in their population-based
In addition to treatment with medication or sur- study in the US using data collected in the 1988
gery, there are many useful strategies that can be National Health Interview Survey (NHIS), included a
employed. The strategies should be tailored to the 28-item instrument, the BPI, based on parent re-
needs of the individual child. It is beyond the scope ports of behavioural problems. As previously stated,
of this paper to describe speci®c strategies in detail the inclusion criteria for seizures did not require a
but these can be of tremendous value in managing rigorous diagnosis of epilepsy. The 121 children,
children who have a variety of de®cits, including aged 5±17 years, with seizures identi®ed from ap-
large verbal-performance differences, memory de- proximately 45,000 households were compared with
fects and autistic features. Some of the strategies are controls and children with cardiac problems. The
discussed in the section on epilepsy and behavioural adjusted odds ratio for any behavioural problem was
disorders. 4.7 for the seizure group and 3.0 for the cardiac
The child with absence seizures should be man- group. The odds ratios for speci®c behaviours were:
aged by people who realise that he or she may not be antisocial 2.3, anxiety 3.7, headstrong 4.0, hyper-
able to register and remember speech or other events active 7.4, peer con¯ict 3.8 and dependency 6.5
that occur during the brief subtle seizures. Teachers (ages 5±11 years only). Some of the limitations of this
should be very sensitive to this and be prepared to study were discussed earlier. In particular, it de-
repeat what they have just said if they suspect that pended on parent questionnaires only. Huberty,
the child has had an absence seizure. Children with Austin, Harezlak, Dunn, and Ambrosius (2000) have
very frequent absence seizures will perform better on shown that mother's evaluations of problems may be
static tasks such as jigsaw puzzles than they will in different from those of teachers and very different
dynamic tasks such as question-and-answer ses- from those of the child.
sions in class. Teachers should aim to build self- Lewis et al. (2000) identi®ed 392 young people,
con®dence by encouraging the child to carry out aged 8±22 years, with intellectual disability who
tasks that are easily achievable, despite the absence were said to be representative of the general Aus-
seizures. tralian population of young people with intellectual
Longer-term memory de®cits may be managed by disability; 115 had epilepsy. Using the Develop-
using the technique of `errorless learning' which mental Behaviour Checklist, they found no differ-
consists of telling the child the answer before asking ence in the rates of emotional and behavioural
them the question. This reinforces memory much disturbance between those with epilepsy and con-
better than `putting the child on the spot' by insisting trols. Antiepileptic medication and seizure frequency
that they guess the answer when they do not know. were not found to be associated with behavioural
There are very many other strategies that can be disturbance either. Their paper implied that the
tremendously rewarding in teaching children who degree of behavioural disturbance was related solely
have speci®c or global learning problems. to the intellectual disability. This result is surprising
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 119

and emphasises the need for whole-population unrecognised seizures before the ®rst obvious
studies of children with epilepsy to examine the seizure.
causes of behavioural disturbance in more detail. Herman has examined various psychosocial cor-
In a recent study carried out by O'Neill and co- relates of childhood epilepsy (Hermann, 1990). In
workers (Besag et al., 1999) in the inner London 101 children (44M, 57F, aged 6±11), the most fre-
Borough of Lambeth, (also see section on epidemio- quent and signi®cant predictor of behavioural prob-
logical studies on learning) 48% of the children with lems was inadequate seizure control. The second
epilepsy were found to have disturbed behaviour on most important was divorced or separated parents
standardised Rutter scales. This compares with ap- and the third was polytherapy for the epilepsy. His
proximately 10% in the general population. Even if group also examined social competence in 183 chil-
allowance is made for the type of population in dren aged 6±16, using a standard questionnaire
an inner London borough, the rate of behavioural completed by the mother (Hermann, Whitman,
disturbance is still high. Hughes, Melyn, & Dell, 1988). Social competence
It is encouraging to see an increasing number of was associated most strongly with good seizure
publications on measures of quality of life or well- control, followed by a shorter duration of epilepsy
being of children with epilepsy (Norrby, Carlsson, and higher family income. With regard to outcome
Beckung, & Nordholm, 1999; Carpay et al., 1996; following neurosurgery, he commented on the poor
Arunkumar, Wyllie, Kotagal, Ong, & Gilliam, 2000; methodology of publications available at that time,
Eiser & Morse, 2001). However, there are also dif®- and added that measures of psychosocial outcome
culties with subjective measures. were extremely sketchy (Hermann, 1990). However,
It would be helpful to have more studies that de- he concluded that there was considerable anecdotal
termined not only the rate of behavioural distur- information suggesting a favourable psychosocial
bance but also the types and causes of behavioural outcome for focal resections in children with epi-
disturbance. lepsy.
Although there is a lack of data from population-
based studies, there seems to be no doubt that a
Other studies on behavioural disturbance
high proportion of children with epilepsy attending
in children with epilepsy
specialist services have behavioural disorders in two
Hoare and co-workers have carried out a number of particular areas, namely overactivity/attention de®-
studies on epilepsy and behavioural disturbance in cit and autistic spectrum disorder. These children
children (Hoare, 1984b, 1984a; Hoare & Kerley, make very considerable demands on their families,
1991; Hoare & Mann, 1994). Hoare and Kerley education authorities and providers of medical
(1991) assessed 108 children with chronic epilepsy services.
attending a specialist clinic. The rates of psychiatric
disturbance on the Rutter Parent and Teacher scales
Causes of behavioural problems in children
were 54% and 48% respectively. They emphasised
with epilepsy
the importance of family factors in the management
of these children. It is very important, when faced with a child who has
The publications by Stores and co-workers, referred epilepsy and behavioural problems, to try to deter-
to earlier, considered the effects of epilepsy on learn- mine the cause or causes. This allows professionals
ing but also dealt with some behavioural aspects. to adopt a rational approach to management. In the
Austin and co-workers have published a number past there has been a tendency to assume that in-
of papers on epilepsy and behavioural problems in dividuals had `epileptic behaviour'. Such an ap-
children (Dunn, Austin, & Huster, 1997; Austin, proach is to be decried. Besag (Besag, Loney,
Risinger, & Beckett, 1992; Dunn & Austin, 1999; Waudby, Fowler, & Brooks, 1989a; Besag, 1995b)
Austin et al., 2001). One of their earlier papers strongly recommends that a systematic approach to
(Austin et al., 1992), based on ®ndings from 127 determining the cause or causes of behavioural dis-
children aged 8±12 years, indicated that there was a turbance in a child with epilepsy be adopted. The
signi®cant relationship between behavioural prob- scheme in Tables 1 and 2 has been suggested. This
lems and ®ve variables: female gender, family stress, scheme has proved to be very successful in the
seizure frequency and lack of both family mastery practical management of children with epilepsy be-
and extended family support. In a recent study of havioural problems. The current author surveyed a
224 children with epilepsy and 135 healthy siblings group of young people with dif®cult epilepsy at a
they found that children with previously unrecog- residential epilepsy special school, using this
nised seizures were already at risk of behavioural scheme. However, no population-based studies have
problems at the time of the ®rst recognised seizure been performed. The various causes will be dis-
(Austin et al., 2001). The interpretation of this im- cussed in more detail in the following sections.
portant ®nding is either that the behavioural dis-
turbance is due to some underlying neurological The epilepsy itself. The ®rst category includes
problem or that the children may have been having the peri-ictal disturbances of prodrome, aura,
120 Frank M.C. Besag

Table 1 Possible causes of behavioural disturbance in a child not anxiolytic medication but additional antiepileptic
with epilepsy medication, typically oral diazepam or clobazam. It
1. The epilepsy itself. should be noted, however, that in some children and
2. Treatment of the epilepsy. even in some teenagers, benzodiazepines may pre-
3. Reactions to the epilepsy. cipitate disinhibited behaviour.
4. Associated brain damage/dysfunction. Automatisms are easily misinterpreted. Although
5. Causes which are equally applicable to those without
epilepsy.
limb-¯ailing automatisms are probably an uncom-
mon manifestation of complex partial seizures, if
someone happens to be struck by the ¯ailing limb
then the child may acquire, through no fault of his or
Table 2 The epilepsy itself
her own, a reputation for being violent. If the lower
1. Peri-ictal changes limbs ¯ail and someone is kicked then the child
Prodrome may gain a reputation for being very violent. Simple
Aura
Automatism
explanation can avoid such misinterpretations
Post-ictal changes from occurring.
Confusion With regard to other ictal phenomena causing be-
Disinhibition havioural disturbance, the role of limbic seizures
Paranoid or affective states continues to be debated. There is evidence in adults
2. Inter-ictal psychoses that seizures arising in the amygdala may be asso-
Schizophreniform ciated with fear or anxiety as an ictal symptom
Affective
(Cendes et al., 1994). This, in turn, may cause
3. Focal discharges behavioural disturbance.
Temporal In the immediate post-ictal phase confusion,
Frontal
tiredness, irritability and even paranoid states may
4. Frequent generalised discharges/absence seizures occur. All of these changes can result in aggressive
behaviour. A situation that is not well recognised is
the child who has a frontal lobe complex partial
automatism and post-ictal changes. Prodrome is a seizure followed by disinhibition resulting from de-
period typically lasting from a few minutes to several layed recovery of frontal lobe function. The result can
days before a seizure or cluster of seizures during be bizarre behaviour, for example, screaming or
which mood and consequently behaviour may be acting in a very immature way. A professional wit-
disturbed. The prodrome is terminated when the nessing such behaviour may correctly state that this
seizure occurs. The parents of the child may be very is not a seizure. However, it would be very misleading
familiar with the phenomenon of prodrome but may to suggest that it was not epilepsy-related behaviour
not be aware of the fact that this situation is well because it has resulted from a disinhibited post-ictal
recognised. Their child may have been criticised by state.
teachers or others and may have been blamed for the Paranoid states may vary from brief, immediate
mood or behavioural disturbance. Explaining to post-ictal periods resulting from confusion to more
parents the phenomenon of prodrome and providing prolonged psychiatric states requiring treatment
them with the terminology can be greatly enabling. with neuroleptic medication. For example, at the
The author's current practice is to write such ter- mild end of the spectrum, a person may be confused
minology on a piece of paper while explaining it to immediately after a seizure and may mistake an offer
the parents and then to hand the parents the piece of assistance as being a threat. This may result in an
of paper to take away with them. Punishing a child aggressive reaction. Such a state would be very
for the mood disturbance of prodrome is adding short-lived. On the other hand, a full-blown post-
insult to injury. His or her activities may already ictal psychosis may occur. The phenomenon of post-
have been restricted by the epilepsy. Undeserved ictal psychosis has been reviewed by Logsdail and
punishment is likely to impair the self-esteem and Toone (1988). A post-ictal paranoid state may follow
quality of life of the child even further. a seizure or cluster of seizures after a `lucid interval'.
Aura is often described as being the warning of a This can create uncertainty about the origin of the
seizure. An aura is, strictly speaking, not a warning psychiatric disorder, causing possible diagnostic
of a seizure but is itself a simple partial seizure. confusion with a functional psychosis.
Because simple partial seizures occur in full con- Affective states may also occur in the post-ictal
sciousness, the child may ®nd the manifestation of period. Post-ictal depression is usually relatively
the seizure very distressing. The resulting anxiety brief, lasting only a few days. It would generally be
may lower the seizure threshold with the result that inappropriate to treat with antidepressant medica-
further auras may occur. A vicious circle ensues with tion because this would not have time to become
auras causing anxiety, lowering the seizure thresh- effective and because the condition is self-limiting.
old causing even more auras. This situation of re- Intensive support from carers and relatives is the
peated auras may be interrupted by giving the child appropriate management. Elevated mood states may
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 121

also occur in the post-ictal period. Again, these are Frequent absence seizures. Frequent absence sei-
usually brief and self-limiting. zures may affect behaviour in a number of different
Inter-ictal psychoses bear no speci®c time rela- ways. As stated in the section on epilepsy and
tionship to the seizures. There is a considerable learning, a child who is having very frequent absence
literature on this topic, including the classical study seizures cannot easily take part in activities requi-
by Slater and Beard (1995). Trimble (1987b) has ring rapid conversational exchange. As a result, he
reviewed this subject. He has emphasised the role of may become withdrawn and avoid social situations
alternative or reciprocal psychosis. This concept because he realises that he may not be able to cope.
implies that the psychosis is worse when the seizures Frequent absence seizures may also fragment con-
are less frequent and the psychosis is improved when centration. This can mimic attention de®cit disorder.
the seizures are more frequent. The EEG equivalent is The disjointed speech that results from frequent
`forced normalisation' implying that the EEG is relat- lapses of awareness may be mistaken for a psycho-
ively normal during the psychotic state but becomes sis. A third way in which absence seizures may affect
abnormal when the patient emerges from the psy- behaviour is when a child goes in and out of non-
chosis. Inter-ictal psychoses may be either schizo- convulsive status epilepticus. Some of these children
phreniform or affective. Schizophreniform inter-ictal appear to `make up for lost time' when they are in the
psychoses differ from functional schizophrenia in the phases of having fewer seizures. They are incapable
fact that there is preservation of warmth of affect and of being badly behaved when the absence seizures
there is no family history of psychosis. In functional are very frequent and seem to compensate during
schizophrenia there is a high risk that ®rst-degree their more able periods by being particularly badly
relatives will also have the condition. behaved.
In the author's experience the `forced normali- As stated earlier, a high proportion of children with
sation' relationship between psychosis and EEG epilepsy and behavioural problems attending spe-
abnormality does not always apply. Some patients cialist services have either overactivity/attention
may have worsening rather than improvement of the de®cit or autistic spectrum disorder. The current
EEG when the psychosis occurs. author has reviewed the multiple causes of overac-
The psychotic and affective states discussed in the tivity/attention de®cit in children with epilepsy
preceding sections are generally described in adults (unpublished). Frequent absence seizures are a
but can certainly occur in teenagers. Inter-ictal treatable cause. Management of the other causes are
psychoses may be prolonged and often require discussed in the appropriate sections of this paper.
treatment with neuroleptic medication. They may be Epilepsy is common in autism, often emerging by
remitting and relapsing. It has been suggested that the teenage years in 20±40%, but there is an in-
antipsychotic medication should be avoided in peo- creasing realisation that some younger children who
ple with epilepsy because of the danger of precipi- present with markedly autistic features have un-
tating seizures. However, if the psychosis is putting suspected epileptiform discharges either during the
the patient at risk or affecting the quality of life of the day or at night (electrical status epilepticus of slow
patient to a signi®cant degree then it would, in the wave sleep ESES or continuous spike-wave during
opinion of the author, be inappropriate to withhold slow wave sleep CSWS) (Berney, 2000; Tuchman,
treatment. In his experience it is unusual for a Rapin, & Shinnar, 1991; Taylor, Neville, & Cross,
seizure exacerbation to occur when antipsychotic 1999). Treating these children allows them to emerge
medication is prescribed. On the other hand, the from their apparently autistic state. However, follow-
patient's mental state may be dramatically improved up of teenagers who have had very frequent epilep-
by the prescription of such medication. The role of tiform discharges earlier in life has shown that they
psychotropic medication in the treatment of children may subsequently have autistic features when the
with epilepsy and behavioural problems is discussed epileptiform discharges are no longer present (Besag,
in more detail in a later section. 1999a). It would appear that these young people
have been unable to interact adequately with the
Focal discharges. There is a reported association world around them during a critical developmental
between left temporal discharges and aggressive phase because of the frequent epileptiform dischar-
behaviour (van Elst, Woermann, Lemieux, Thomp- ges at that time. If they have not had the opportunity
son, & Trimble, 2000; Devinsky et al., 1994; Amen, to develop two-way social interaction during this
Stubble®eld, Carmicheal, & Thisted, 1996; Volkow & developmental phase they may present with a very
Tancredi, 1987). Part of this may arise from the fact Asperger-like picture during teenage years. This
that left temporal discharges can be associated with suggests that early treatment of the epileptiform
language dysfunction and this, in turn, can lead to discharges, so as to allow the child to gain fully from
considerable frustration. the early developmental years, may have avoided
Frequent frontal discharges can lead to disinhib- these social impairments.
ited mood states, as already discussed in the section In all these circumstances the emphasis should be
on epilepsy and learning. This situation is not well on treating the generalised discharges/absence sei-
described in the literature. zures and then assisting the individual to gain the
122 Frank M.C. Besag

necessary skills to function competently and with patients, there have been reports of individuals who
con®dence in social situations. did not have psychosis before temporal lobectomy
but developed a psychosis after the surgery (Mace &
Treatment of the epilepsy. The behavioural effects Trimble, 1991). It is not clear whether a similar
of the older and well-established antiepileptic drugs situation might apply to some children.
are generally recognised. In particular, phenobarbi-
tone may cause gross behavioural disturbance in Reactions to the epilepsy. Limitations placed on the
younger children (de Silva et al., 1996). This situ- child or teenager because of the epilepsy may be very
ation may mimic attention de®cit hyperactivity dis- damaging to self-esteem. The general principle
order. Benzodiazepines can cause a similar clinical should be to restrict the child as little as possible but
picture. The behavioural effects of the nine new an- to ensure adequate supervision to provide a rea-
tiepileptic drugs have been reviewed recently by the sonable degree of safety. For example, swimming
current author (Besag, 2001b). The most notorious should not be discouraged but must be adequately
of the new antiepileptic drugs for causing beha- supervised. This implies supervision tailored to the
vioural disturbance is vigabatrin. This can some- individual circumstances (Besag, 2001a).
times cause a behavioural disorder similar to that Teasing may be very damaging to the child's self-
resulting from phenobarbitone or benzodiazepines. esteem, resulting in both mood and behavioural
Behavioural disturbances have also been reported disturbance. Tonic-clonic seizures may be frighten-
with gabapentin (Lee et al., 1996; Khurana et al., ing for the onlooker and may lead to teasing. Com-
1996). These authors stated that, in general, dis- plex partial seizures, although less dramatic, may be
turbed behaviour was seen in children who had pre- even more liable to cause teasing because the auto-
existing hyperactivity and/or learning disability. matisms that often occur in these seizures may be
These ®ndings were not con®rmed by the current viewed by other children as being very bizarre
author (Besag, 1996). behaviour. Ironically, the involuntary behaviour of
Fowler, Besag, Strange, and Pool (1997) reported the automatism may result in teasing which has, as
on 16 adolescents with epilepsy and learning dis- its consequence, voluntary behavioural disturbance,
ability who were assessed with Rutter Behavioural because the child is so upset by the teasing. The
Scales before and during treatment with topiramate. approach to this situation should be education both
There was a signi®cant (p < .05) increase in negative of the child who has the seizures and of the other
reported behaviour on both the parents and teachers children in the class or school. Educating teachers
scales. Topiramate has also been associated with and parents is also mandatory in such circum-
general slowing of cognition and behaviour which stances. As is the case for teasing in general, the
could be mistaken for an affective disorder. The child should be provided with strategies that help
concept of reciprocal or alternative psychosis has them to feel in control. Some children or teenagers
been discussed in previous sections. If an antiepi- develop remarkably effective strategies for them-
leptic drug results in sudden control of the seizures selves.
this may, in some circumstances, precipitate psy-
chosis. There have been a number of reports of Associated brain damage/dysfunction. As dis-
psychosis with both vigabatrin and topiramate cussed in the section on learning, brain damage or
(Trimble, Rusch, Betts, & Crawford, 2000; Besag, dysfunction may be independent of the epilepsy or
2001b). As already discussed, this situation is rel- may be caused by the epilepsy. In either case, the
atively unlikely to occur in young children but may child or teenager can be provided with speci®c en-
occur in teenagers. abling strategies that can be highly effective. Frontal
A further important concept is that of the so-called lobe dysfunction may result in grossly disinhibited
`release phenomenon'. If a person has been relatively behaviour. This usually arises because of the un-
disabled by very frequent seizures and suddenly derlying brain problem, for example injury to the
becomes more able because the seizures are better frontal lobes, rather that the epilepsy itself, although
controlled, he or she may not know how to use their frequent focal discharges can result in frontal lobe
new-found ability appropriately. The result may be dysfunction (see earlier). The disinhibition of frontal
very unacceptable behaviour. The correct way of lobe dysfunction may present with features resem-
managing the situation is not to stop the antiepi- bling attention de®cit disorder.
leptic drug but to provide the necessary professional Dominant temporal lobe damage may result in
input to teach the individual how to use the new- poor language function with accompanying frustra-
found skills to good advantage in a socially accept- tion and behavioural disturbance. Some very
able way. Such interventions can be very valuable enabling strategies can be used to help such chil-
indeed. The alternative of withdrawing the medica- dren. For example, the child with good performance
tion and allowing the young person to return to a skills but relatively poor verbal skills can be taught to
disabled state is not a good one. say: `Please bear with me; I know what I want to say
Surgery may occasionally result in worsening of but I am not very good with words' or `Please show me
behaviour rather than improvement. In some adult what you want me to do as well as telling me'.
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 123

Non-dominant hemisphere damage/dysfunction It has also been mentioned that reports of outcome
leaves the verbal abilities intact but may result in after epilepsy surgery tend to concentrate on seizure
signi®cant visuo-spatial problems. Because there is control. There have been a number of studies on
a tendency for people to judge an individual's ability temporal lobe resections, including some classical
by the way he or she speaks, such patients may be publications (Falconer & Taylor, 1968; Lindsay,
under enormous pressure to perform to a level that is Ounsted, & Richards, 1984). The reviews on hemi-
beyond their capabilities. If they fail, they are ac- spherectomy (Goodman, 1986; Lindsay et al., 1987)
cused of not trying hard enough. Careful psycho- indicate a good behavioural outcome in a large pro-
metric assessment coupled with practical advice can portion of cases. The paper by Bourgeois et al.
be very bene®cial in these circumstances. Contrary (1999), on a series of 200 children with epilepsy aged
to the traditional approach of telling the child not to 1±15 years who had surgery for a variety of brain
talk in class, these children should be encouraged to lesions, has already been mentioned with regard to
`talk themselves through' tasks, using frequent ver- improvements in learning. These authors reported
bal cues to assist themselves in organisational tasks, that behaviour improved in 31% of the children, es-
particularly those calling on their limited visuo- pecially in those with hyperkinetic disorders. A re-
spatial skills. Written, step-by-step instructions can cent paper by Lendt, Helmstaedter, Kuczaty,
also be helpful in these circumstances. Schramm, and Elger (2000) examined the early be-
Memory impairment can lead to gross frustration. havioural outcome after a mixed group of surgical
Again, many useful strategies that teachers can use procedures on 28 children (mean age 11.5 years)
with the child and that the child can be taught, can who were compared with a control group of 28 chil-
help to overcome this frustration. For example, the dren who also had epilepsy but did not undergo
teachers may use the technique of `errorless learn- surgery. Both groups were assessed with the child
ing' (see earlier) with subsequent reinforcement after behaviour checklist (CBCL). The surgery comprised
appropriate periods of time and the child may be 13 temporal and 11 extratemporal resections, two
encouraged to use diaries or other memory aids. hemispherectomies and two callosotomies. Before
the surgery 39% had signi®cant behavioural prob-
Causes that are equally applicable to children lems and a further 11% were in the borderline range.
without epilepsy. A diagnosis of epilepsy does not Three months after the surgery there were signi®cant
make the child immune to all the causes of beha- improvements in internalising, externalising and
vioural disturbance that apply equally well to chil- attention problems. The control group did not im-
dren who do not have this condition. The fact that prove. Good seizure control was the one signi®cant
the child has epilepsy does not necessarily imply predictor of behavioural improvement. The authors
that this condition is the cause of the behavioural concluded that this was because the epileptic focus
disturbance. was removed. In studies such as this, it would be of
great interest to have detailed and prolonged EEG
recordings before and after the surgery to determine
Management of behavioural problems
whether subtle seizure activity was contributing to
As each cause of behavioural disturbance has been the pre-surgery behavioural disturbance. There
discussed in the preceding sections, possible man- seems to be a consensus that if epilepsy surgery is
agement strategies have been indicated. These are needed then the earlier in life it is performed, the
not intended to be comprehensive because each better. Not only will this allow the child the possi-
child and family is unique. The strategies should be bility of going through the important developmental
tailored to the individual child and family. A multi- and educational years without all the drawbacks of
disciplinary approach is often required (Besag et al., having seizures but it will also allow the opportunity
1989a). Behavioural programmes are frequently ap- for maximum brain plasticity.
propriate, implying that the psychologist is a key
member of the team. However, other disciplines also The role of psychotropic medication. The role of
play a valuable role. For example, the speech and psychotropic medication in treating behavioural and
language therapist can greatly reduce frustrations psychiatric disorders has recently been examined by
from communication dif®culties. The occupational the Psychobiology Commission of the International
therapist can assist with social skills training. The League Against Epilepsy (Trimble & Cornaggia,
play specialist can help the child to learn that social 2001). There is an understandable reluctance to use
interaction with other people can be pleasurable. It is psychotropic medication in children with epilepsy
often important to work with the whole family so that and behavioural problems. Most of these children
a consistent approach can be maintained, and so will already be taking antiepileptic drugs and there is
that other family members can learn the necessary often a resistance to the possibility of adding further
strategies required for the individual child. medication during the developmental years. How-
There are few publications that present data on ever, there are circumstances in which psychotropic
behavioural outcome. The shortcomings of the data medication can be valuable. As indicated earlier, the
on antiepileptic drugs have already been discussed. teenager with an epilepsy-associated psychosis may
124 Frank M.C. Besag

bene®t greatly from neuroleptic medication. The atypical antipsychotic drugs in the treatment of
newer antipsychotic drugs such as risperidone and behavioural disorders in suitably selected young
olanzapine are generally highly effective. Although people with epilepsy with or without autistic spec-
standard formularies, such as the British National trum disorder. There is also a need for further
Formulary, recommend `caution in patients with information on the way that neuroleptic and
history of epilepsy', serious seizure exacerbations antiepileptic drugs interact. There is some data
are probably rare. There appears to be clear evidence indicating, as might be expected, that enzyme-
of seizure precipitation by clozapine in adults inducing drugs such as carbamazepine can reduce
(Devinsky, Honigfeld, & Patin, 1991; Devinsky & the serum concentration of neuroleptic medication.
Pacia, 1994; Pacia & Devinsky, 1994). This drug For example, carbamazepine has been shown to
should, in any case, only be used if absolutely reduce the concentration of both risperidone and the
necessary because of other adverse effects, notably active metabolite markedly (Spina et al., 2000).
agranulocytosis. However, there are reports of con- Speci®c conditions such as depression or obses-
tinued use of clozapine even when seizures have sive-compulsive disorder in teenagers may require a
been precipitated. The drug has been continued in selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor. Although
smaller doses or with antiepileptic medication. There these drugs appear to be much less likely to preci-
have been isolated case reports of seizures with pitate seizures than other antidepressant medication
olanzapine (Lee, Crismon, & Dorson, 1999), inclu- (Rosenstein, Nelson, & Jacobs, 1993), the British
ding death in status epilepticus of a female adult National Formulary states: `should be used with
patient (Wyderski, Starrett, & Abou-Saif, 1999), but caution in patients with epilepsy (avoid if poorly
the risk appears to be much less than that associ- controlled, discontinue if convulsions develop)'.
ated with clozapine (Beasley, Jr., Tollefson, & Tran, There is limited evidence from the literature that
1997; Schuld et al., 2000). Should neuroleptic medi- selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors cause seizure
cation be used when there is no clear diagnosis of exacerbations. There are some isolated reports of
psychosis? Risperidone is being used to treat beha- seizures in adults treated with ¯uvoxamine (Kim,
vioural disorders in both children and adults with Craig, & Hawley, 2000; Trabert, Hohagen, Winkel-
autism (Zuddas, Di Martino, Muglia, & Cianchetti, mann, & Berger, 1995; Deahl & Trimble, 1991) and
2000; McDougle et al., 1998). There does not seem to one case of status epilepticus (Loo, Piau, Galinowski,
be clear evidence for seizure exacerbations with ris- & Olie, 1987). There is a suggestion that ¯uoxetine
peridone, although there has been a report of a might even have antiepileptic properties (Favale,
seizure with an overdose of this drug (Acri & Hen- Rubino, Mainardi, Lunardi, & Albano, 1995),
retig, 1998). There is an argument, from the evidence although this has been challenged (Gigli et al.,
available at this time, for using risperidone for the 1994). Fluoxetine in overdose has been associated
treatment of some children with epilepsy, beha- with seizures (Neely, 1998; Braitberg & Curry, 1995;
vioural dif®culties and autistic spectrum disorder. Riddle et al., 1989). With regard to drug interactions,
However, such treatment should only be undertaken there is a report that sertraline can cause a marked
after careful consideration of the following factors. rise in lamotrigine serum levels and this can preci-
First, the clinician should decide whether some other pitate toxicity (Kaufman & Gerner, 1998). Fluoxetine
form of management would be more appropriate. For is said to decrease valproic acid levels (Droulers,
example, correct communication strategies, a Bodak, Oudjhani, Lefevre, & Bodak, 1997). Paroxe-
structured day and suitable teaching techniques tine apparently does not cause changes in plasma
may all contribute to the avoidance of behavioural concentrations of carbamazepine, valproate or
problems in autistic children. Have subtle seizures phenytoin (Andersen et al., 1991). Fluoxetine and
been excluded as a treatable cause of the autistic ¯uvoxamine do not appear to change plasma con-
features? Second, risperidone is not licensed for use centrations of carbamazepine. From the information
in children. Parents need to be made aware of this that is available at present, it would seem that se-
before it is prescribed. Third, the need for the medi- lective serotonin reuptake inhibitors provide a rea-
cation should be reviewed regularly, especially since sonable treatment option for teenagers with seizures
there are no data on possible long-term adverse who have some types of psychiatric disorders such
effects. Notwithstanding the reservations, risperi- as depression and obsessive compulsive disorder.
done, often in quite small doses, can be a useful However, particular caution should be observed
adjunct for the management of young people with when prescribing sertraline with lamotrigine and
epilepsy, autistic spectrum disorder and behavioural perhaps ¯uoxetine with valproate.
problems. A report of the use of quetiapine to treat 6 Psychotropic medication can also be of value in
male children and teenagers with autistic spectrum treating children with epilepsy and overactivity/at-
disorder concluded that it was not very effective and tention de®cit. However, as stated in the section on
was associated with a high incidence of adverse ef- frequent absence seizures, there are many possible
fects, including sedation, a possible seizure and causes for this disorder in children with epilepsy. If
weight gain (Martin, Koenig, Scahill, & Bregman, frequent epileptiform discharges are responsible for
1999). There is a need for more data on the use of the the poor attention then the treatment should be
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 125

effective antiepileptic medication. If, on the other possible. With skilled management much can be
hand, the child is being treated with an antiepileptic achieved. The outcome can be remarkably bene®cial
drug that can cause these symptoms, for example for the child and very rewarding for the professionals.
phenobarbitone or one of the benzodiazepines, then
the drug responsible should be replaced by medica-
tion that is better tolerated. If causes requiring dif- Correspondence to
ferent management are excluded then drugs such as
Frank M.C. Besag, Consultant Neuropsychiatrist and
methylphenidate or dexamphetamine can be highly
Research Director, Specialist Medical Department,
effective in some cases (Semrud-Clikeman & Wical,
Bedfordshire and Luton Community NHS Trust,
1999; Gross-Tsur, Manor, van der Meare, Joseph, &
Milton Road, Clapham, Bedfordshire, UK; Fax:
Shalev, 1997). Stimulant medication is said not to
+44 (0)1234 310584; Email: FBesag@aol.com
exacerbate well-controlled epilepsy (Gross-Tsur
et al., 1997). However, children who do not have a
diagnosis of epilepsy but have an abnormal EEG are
References
at risk of having seizures with stimulant medication
(Hemmer, Pasternak, Zecker, & Trommer, 2001). Aarts, J.H., Binnie, C.D., Smit, A.M., & Wilkins, A.J.
Hemmer et al. found that 10% of 30 children with (1984). Selective cognitive impairment during focal
epileptiform EEGs and 17% of 12 children with Ro- and generalized epileptiform EEG activity. Brain, 107,
landic spikes had a seizure with stimulant medica- 293±308.
Acri, A.A., & Henretig, F.M. (1998). Effects of risperi-
tion. The seizures were not necessarily the result of
done in overdose. [Review] [11 refs]. American Journal
prescribing the stimulant medication. Of the 175
of Emergency Medicine, 16, 498±501.
children who had a normal EEG only one (0.6%) had Aicardi, J. (1992). Diseases of the nervous system in
a seizure. There is a possibility that some children childhood (pp. 408). Mac Keith Press, London.
with epilepsy that is not controlled will have more Airaksinen, E.M., Matilainen, R., Mononen, T., Musto-
seizures when methylphenidate is prescribed (Gross- nen, K., Partanen, J., Jokela, V., & Halonen, P.
Tsur et al., 1997). There seems to be remarkably (2000). A population-based study on epilepsy in
little information on interactions between antiepi- mentally retarded children. Epilepsia, 41, 1214±
leptic drugs and stimulant medication. There is a 1220.
single paper describing rapid and severe onset of Aldenkamp, A.P., Alpherts, W.C.J., Blennow, G., Elmq-
dyskinesia and bruxism when methylphenidate was vist, D., Heijbel, J., Nilsson, H.L., Sandstedt, P.,
Tonnby, B., Wahlander, L., & Wosse, E. (1993).
added to valproic acid (Gara & Roberts, 2000).
Withdrawal of antiepileptic medication in children ±
Although psychotropic medication can be of great
Effects on cognitive function: The multicenter Holm-
value in treating some children with epilepsy and frid study. Neurology, 43, 41±50.
behavioural disorder, care should be exercised in Aldenkamp, A.P., Alpherts, W.C.J., Diepman, L., Van't
selecting cases that are likely to respond and exclu- Slot, B., Overweg, J., & Vermeulen, J. (1994). Cogni-
ding those for whom other management strategies tive side-effects of phenytoin compared with carb-
would be more appropriate. amazepine in patients with localization-related
epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 19, 37±43.
Aldenkamp, A.P., Alpherts, W.C.J., Sandstedt, P., Blen-
now, G., Elmqvist, D., Heijbel, J., Nilsson, H.L.,
Conclusions Tonnby, B., Wahlander, L., & Wosse, E. (1998).
The emphasis throughout this review has been on Antiepileptic drug-related cognitive complaints in
seizure-free children with epilepsy before and after
assessing the individual child and determining
drug discontinuation. Epilepsia, 39, 1070±1074.
management that is appropriate, according to the
Aldenkamp, A.P., Overweg-Plandsoen, W.C.G., &
outcome of that assessment. For the child who has Arends, J. (1999). An open, nonrandomized clinical
learning problems, reviewing the antiepileptic treat- comparative study evaluating the effect of epilepsy on
ment can be of great value. In some cases this implies learning. Journal of Child Neurology, 14, 795±800.
less medication because it is the medication that is Aldenkamp, A.P., Overweg, J., Gutter, T., Beun, A.M.,
causing the problem but in other cases more medi- Diepman, L., & Mulder, O.G. (1996). Effect of
cation or different medication may be required be- epilepsy, seizures and epileptiform EEG discharges
cause the seizures are impairing learning. Speci®c on cognitive function. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica,
learning disability, frontal lobe dysfunction and 93, 253±259.
memory problems can all be managed successfully Allen, K.M., & Walsh, C.A. (1999). Genes that regulate
neuronal migration in the cerebral cortex. [Review]
with appropriate interventions. The mainstay of
[75 refs]. Epilepsy Research, 36, 143±154.
management is the provision of the right environ- Amen, D.G., Stubble®eld, M., Carmicheal, B., & Thist-
ment, especially in terms of educational and beha- ed, R. (1996). Brain SPECT ®ndings and aggressive-
vioural input. A multidisciplinary approach is often ness. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 8, 129±137.
required. Psychotropic medication can be helpful in Amir, R.E., Van, d. V., I., Wan, M., Tran, C.Q., Francke,
carefully selected cases. Above all, children with U., & Zoghbi, H.Y. (1999). Rett syndrome is caused by
epilepsy should be encouraged to live as full a life as mutations in X-linked MECP2, encoding methyl-
126 Frank M.C. Besag

CpG-binding protein 2 [see comments]. Nature Besag, F.M.C. (1995b). Epilepsy, learning, and behavior
Genetics, 23, 185±188. in childhood. Epilepsia, 36, S58±S63.
Andersen, B.B., Mikkelsen, M., Vesterager, A., Dam, Besag, F.M.C. (1996). Gabapentin use with paediatric
M., Kristensen, H.B., Pedersen, B., Lund, J., & patients. Reviews in Contemporary Pharmacotherapy,
Mengel, H. (1991). No in¯uence of the antidepressant 7, 233±238.
paroxetine on carbamazepine, valproate and pheny- Besag, F.M.C. (1999b). Advances in epilepsy. Current
toin. Epilepsy Research, 10, 201±204. Opinion in Psychiatry, 12, 549±553.
Annegers, J.F., Hauser, W.A., Shirts, S.B., & Kurland, Besag, F.M.C. (2001b). Behavioural effects of the new
L.T. (1987). Factors prognostic of unprovoked sei- anticonvulsant drugs. Drug Safety, 24, 513±536.
zures after febrile convulsions. New England Journal Besag, F.M.C. (2001c). Treatment of state-dependent
of Medicine, 316, 493±498. learning disability. Epilepsia, 42, 52±54.
Arunkumar, G., Wyllie, E., Kotagal, P., Ong, H.T., & Besag, F.M.C., Fowler, M., & Pool, F. (1991) Cognitive
Gilliam, F. (2000). Parent- and patient-validated deterioration in children with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 32
content for pediatric epilepsy quality-of-life assess- [Suppl. 1], 15. Ref Type: Abstract
ment. Epilepsia, 41, 1474±1484. Besag, F.M.C., Loney, G., Waudby, E., Fowler, M., &
Austin, J.K., Harezlak, J., Dunn, D.W., Huster, G.A., Brooks, N. (1989a). A multidisciplinary approach
Rose, D.F., & Ambrosius, W.T. (2001). Behavior to epilepsy, learning dif®culties and behavioural
problems in children before ®rst recognized seizures. problems. Educational and Child Psychology, 6,
Pediatrics, 107, 115±122. 18±24.
Austin, J.K., Risinger, M.W., & Beckett, L.A. (1992). Besag, F.M.C., Mills, M., Wardale, F., Andrew, C.M., &
Correlates of behavior problems in children with Craggs, M.D. (1989b). The validation of a new ambu-
epilepsy. Epilepsia, 33, 1115±1122. latory spike and wave monitor. Electroencephalogra-
Bate, L., & Gardiner, M. (1999). Genetics of inherited phy and Clinical Neurophysiology, 73, 157±161.
epilepsies. Epileptic Disorders, 1, 7±19. Binnie, C.D. (1989). Transitory cognitive impairment
Beasley, C.M., Jr., Tollefson, G.D., & Tran, P.V. (1997). (TCI) during sub-clinical EEG discharges. Educa-
Safety of olanzapine. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, tional and Child Psychology, 6, 33±36.
58 Suppl 10, 13±17. Binnie, C.D., & Marston, D. (1992). Cognitive correlates
Bennett-Levy, J., & Stores, G. (1984). The nature of of interictal discharges. Epilepsia, 33, S11±S17.
cognitive dysfunction in school-children with epi- Blake, R.V., Wroe, S.J., Breen, E.K., & McCarthy, R.A.
lepsy. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica Supplementum, (2000). Accelerated forgetting in patients with epi-
99, 79±82. lepsy; evidence for impairment in memory consolid-
Berkovic, S.F., & Mulley, J.C. (1996). The ®rst gene for ation. Brain, 123, 472±483.
an idiopathic epilepsy: A fruitful collaboration of Bourgeois, M., Sainte-Rose, C., Lellouch-Tubiana, A.,
Australian clinical research and molecular genetics Malucci, C., Brunelle, F., Maixner, W., Cinalli, G.,
[editorial]. [Review] [13 refs]. Australian & New Zea- Pierre-Kahn, A., Renier, D., Zerah, M., Hirsch, J.F.,
land Journal of Medicine, 26, 154±156. Goutieres, F., & Aicardi, J. (1999). Surgery of epilepsy
Berkovic, S.F., & Scheffer, I.E. (1997). Genetics of associated with focal lesions in childhood. Journal of
human partial epilepsy. [Review] [39 refs]. Current Neurosurgery, 90, 833±842.
Opinion in Neurology, 10, 110±114. Braitberg, G., & Curry, S.C. (1995). Seizure after
Berney, T.P. (2000). Autism ± An evolving concept. isolated ¯uoxetine overdose. Annals of Emergency
[Review] [74 refs]. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, Medicine, 26, 234±237.
20±25. Bulteau, C., JambaqueÂ, I., Viguier, D., Kieffer, V.,
Besag, F. (1999a). Relationships between epilepsy, Dellatolas, G., & Dulac, O. (2000). Epileptic syn-
autism and learning disability. Epilepsia, 40 [Suppl. dromes, cognitive assessment and school placement:
2], 234. Ref Type: Abstract A study of 251 children. Developmental Medicine and
Besag, F., O'Neill, C., & Ross, E. (1999). A comparison Child Neurology, 42, 319±327.
between children with epilepsy in an inner-city region Calandre, E.P., Dominguez-Granados, R., Gomez-
and those within a special centre, using measures of Rubio, M., & Molina-Font, J.A. (1990). Cognitive
educational dif®culty, behavioural dif®culty and effects of long-term treatment with phenobarbital
quality of life. Epilepsia, 40, 243. and valproic acid in school children. Acta Neurologica
Besag, F.M. (1995a). The therapeutic dilemma: Treating Scandinavica, 81, 504±506.
subtle seizures or indulging in electroencephalogram Carpay, H.A., Arts, W.F., Vermeulen, J., Stroink, H.,
cosmetics? [Review] [17 refs]. Seminars in Pediatric Brouwer, O.F., Peters, A.C., Van Donselaar, C.A., &
Neurology, 2, 261±268. Aldenkamp, A.P. (1996). Parent-completed scales for
Besag, F.M. (2001a). Lesson of the week: Tonic seizures measuring seizure severity and severity of side-effects
are a particular risk factor for drowning in people with of antiepileptic drugs in childhood epilepsy: develop-
epilepsy. [see comments]. British Medical Journal, ment and psychometric analysis. Epilepsy Research,
322, 975±976. 24, 173±181.
Besag, F.M.C. (1994a). Epilepsy education and the role Cendes, F., Andermann, F., Gloor, P., Gambardella, A.,
of mental handicap. In E.M. Ross & R.C. Woody Lopes-Cendes, I., Watson, C., Evans, A., Carpenter,
(Eds.), Epilepsy (pp. 561±583). London: BaillieÁre S., & Olivier, A. (1994). Relationship between atrophy
Tindall. of the amygdala and ictal fear in temporal lobe
Besag, F.M.C. (1994b). Lamotrigine in the Management epilepsy. Brain, 117, 739±746.
of Subtle Seizures. Reviews in Contemporary Phar- Chang, Y.C., Guo, N.W., Huang, C.C., Wang, S.T., &
macotherapy, 5, 123±131. Tsai, J.J. (2000). Neurocognitive attention and be-
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 127

havior outcome of school-age children with a history Duncan, J.S. (1997). Imaging and epilepsy. Brain, 120,
of febrile convulsions: A population study. Epilepsia, 339±377.
41, 412±420. Duncan, J.S. (2001). The outcome of epilepsy surgery.
Chelune, G.J., Naugle, R.I., Hermann, B.P., Barr, W.B., Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,
Trenerry, M.R., Loring, D.W., Perrine, K., Strauss, E., 70, 432.
& Westerveld, M. (1998). Does presurgical IQ predict Dunn, D.W., & Austin, J.K. (1999). Behavioral issues in
seizure outcome after temporal lobectomy? Evidence pediatric epilepsy. [Review] [44 refs]. Neurology, 53,
from the Bozeman Epilepsy Consortium. Epilepsia, S96±S100.
39, 314±318. Dunn, D.W., Austin, J.K., & Huster, G.A. (1997).
Chugani, H.T. (1995). Infantile spasms. [Review] [54 Behaviour problems in children with new-onset epi-
refs]. Current Opinion in Neurology, 8, 139±144. lepsy. Seizure, 6, 283±287.
Chugani, H.T., Shewmon, D.A., Shields, W.D., Sankar, Eiser, C., & Morse, R. (2001). A review of measures of
R., Comair, Y., Vinters, H.V., & Peacock, W.J. (1993). quality of life for children with chronic illness.
Surgery for intractable infantile spasms: Neuroimag- Archives of Disease in Childhood, 84, 205±211.
ing perspectives. Epilepsia, 34, 764±771. Eke, T., Talbot, J.F., & Lawden, M.C. (1997). Severe
Cole, A.J. (2000). Is epilepsy a progressive disease? The persistent visual ®eld constriction associated with
neurobiological consequences of epilepsy. Epilepsia, vigabatrin [see comments]. British Medical Journal,
41, S13±S22. 314, 180±181.
Commission on Classi®cation and Terminology of the Elixhauser, A., Leidy, N.K., Meador, K., Means, E., &
International League Against Epilepsy (1981). Propo- Willian, M.K. (1999). The relationship between mem-
sal for revised clinical and electroencephalographic ory performance, perceived cognitive function, and
classi®cation of epileptic seizures. Epilepsia, 22, 489± mood in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy Research,
501. 37, 13±24.
Commission on Classi®cation and Terminology of the Ellenberg, J.H., Hirtz, D.G., & Nelson, K.B. (1986).
International League Against Epilepsy (1989). Propo- Do seizures in children cause intellectual deteri-
sal for revised classi®cation of epilepsies and epileptic oration? New England Journal of Medicine, 314,
syndromes. Epilepsia, 30, 389±399. 1085±1088.
Cramer, J.A., Westbrook, L.E., Devinsky, O., Perrine, Eriksson, A.-S., Knutsson, E., & Nergardh, A. (2001).
K., Glassman, M.B., & Cam®eld, C. (1999). Develop- The effect of lamotrigine on epileptiform discharges in
ment of the Quality of Life in Epilepsy Inventory for young patients with drug-resistant epilepsy. Epilep-
Adolescents: the QOLIE-AD-48. Epilepsia, 40, 1114± sia, 42, 230±236.
1121. Falconer, M.A., & Taylor, D.C. (1968). Surgical treat-
de Silva, M., MacArdle, B., McGowan, M., Hughes, E., ment of drug-resistant epilepsy due to mesial tem-
Stewart, J., Neville, B.G., Johnson, A.L., & Reynolds, poral sclerosis. Etiology and signi®cance. Archives of
E.H. (1996). Randomised comparative monotherapy Neurology, 19, 353±361.
trial of phenobarbitone, phenytoin, carbamazepine, Favale, E., Rubino, V., Mainardi, P., Lunardi, G., &
or sodium valproate for newly diagnosed childhood Albano, C. (1995). Anticonvulsant effect of ¯uoxetine
epilepsy. Lancet, 347, 709±713. in humans. [see comments]. Neurology, 45, 1926±
Deahl, M., & Trimble, M. (1991). Serotonin reuptake 1927.
inhibitors, epilepsy and myoclonus. British Journal of Forsgren, L., Edvinsson, S.O., Blomquist, H.K., Heijbel,
Psychiatry, 159, 433±435. J., & Sidenvall, R. (1990). Epilepsy in a population
Devinsky, O., Honigfeld, G., & Patin, J. (1991). Cloza- of mentally retarded children and adults. Epilepsy
pine-related seizures. Neurology, 41, 369±371. Research, 6, 234±248.
Devinsky, O., & Pacia, S.V. (1994). Seizures during Forsythe, I., Butler, R., Berg, I., & McGuire, R. (1991).
clozapine therapy. [Review] [26 refs]. Journal of Clin- Cognitive impairment in new cases of epilepsy ran-
ical Psychiatry, 55 Suppl B, 153±156. domly associated to carbamazepine, phenytoin and
Devinsky, O., Ronsaville, D., Cox, C., Witt, E., Fedio, P., sodium valproate. Developmental Medicine and Child
& Theodore, W.H. (1994). Interictal aggression in Neurology, 33, 524±534.
epilepsy: the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory. Epi- Fowler, M., Besag, F.M.C., Strange, M., & Pool, F.
lepsia, 35, 585±590. (1997). Effects of topiramate on behaviour in adoles-
Dlugos, D.J., Moss, E.M., Duhaime, A.C., & Brooks- cents with learning disability. Epilepsia, 38, 131.
Kayal, A.R. (1999). Language-related cognitive Gara, L., & Roberts, W. (2000). Adverse response to
declines after left temporal lobectomy in children. methylphenidate in combination with valproic acid.
Pediatric Neurology, 21, 444±449. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology,
Dravet, C., Bueau, M., Guerrini, R., Giraud, N., & 10, 39±43.
Roger, J. (1992). Severe myoclonic epilepsy in infants. Gigli, G.L., Diomedi, M., Troisi, A., Baldinetti, F.,
In J. Roger, M. Bureau, C. Dravet, F.E. Dreifuss, Marciani, M.G., Girolami, E., & Pasini, A. (1994).
A. Perret, & P. Wolf (Eds.), Epileptic syndromes in Lack of potentiation of anticonvulsant effect by ¯uo-
infancy, childhood and adolescence (pp. 75±88). xetine in drug-resistant epilepsy. Seizure, 3,
London: John Libbey & Company Ltd. 221±224.
Droulers, A., Bodak, N., Oudjhani, M., Lefevre, d. N., V., Goodman, R. (1986). Hemispherectomy and its alter-
& Bodak, A. (1997). Decrease of valproic acid con- natives in the treatment of intractable epilepsy in
centration in the blood when coprescribed with ¯uo- patients with infantile hemiplegia. [Review] [27 refs].
xetine. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17, Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 28,
139±140. 251±258.
128 Frank M.C. Besag

Gordon, N. (2000). Cognitive functions and epileptic children with diabetes. Journal of Psychosomatic
activity. Seizure, 9, 184±188. Research, 38, 859±869.
Goulden, K.J., Shinnar, S., Koller, H., Katz, M., & Hoare, P., Mann, H., & Dunn, S. (2000). Parental
Richardson, S.A. (1991). Epilepsy in children with perception of the quality of life among children with
mental retardation: A cohort study. Epilepsia, 32, epilepsy or diabetes with a new assessment question-
690±697. naire. Quality of Life Research, 9, 637±644.
Gross-Tsur, V., Manor, O., van der Meere, M.J., Joseph, Holdsworth, L., & Whitmore, K. (1974a). A study of
A., & Shalev, R.S. (1997). Epilepsy and attention children with epilepsy attending ordinary schools. I:
de®cit hyperactivity disorder: Is methylphenidate safe Their seizure patterns, progress and behaviour in
and effective? [see comments]. [republished from J. school. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
Pediatr 1997 Jan; 130: 40±4]. Journal of Pediatrics, 16, 746±758.
130, 670±674. Holdsworth, L., & Whitmore, K. (1974b). A study of
Guerrini, R., Dravet, C., Genton, P., Belmonte, A., children with epilepsy attending ordinary schools. II:
Kaminska, A., & Dulac, O. (1998). Lamotrigine and Information and attitudes held by their teachers.
seizure aggravation in severe myoclonic epilepsy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 16,
Epilepsia, 39, 508±512. 759±765.
Gustavson, K.H., Holmgren, G., Jonsell, R., Son & Huberty, T.J., Austin, J.K., Harezlak, J., Dunn, D.W., &
Blomquist, H.K. (1977). Severe mental retardation in Ambrosius, W.T. (2000). Informant agreement in
children in a northern Swedish county. Journal of behavior ratings for children with epilepsy. Epilepsy
Mental De®ciency Research, 21, 161±180. and Behavior, 1, 427±435.
Hardus, P., Engelsman, M., Van Veelen, C.W.M., & Irwin, K., Edwards, M., & Robinson, R. (2000). Psycho-
Stilma, J.S. (2000). Vigabatrin: Steering a middle genic non-epileptic seizures: Management and prog-
course between ef®cacy and (irreversible) loss of nosis. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 82, 474±478.
visual ®eld as a side effect. Nederlands Tijdschrift JambaqueÂ, I., Chiron, C., Dumas, C., Mumford, J., &
voor Geneeskunde, 144, 2066±2069. Dulac, O. (2000). Mental and behavioural outcome of
Helmstaedter, C., & Kurthen, M. (2001). Memory and infantile epilepsy treated by vigabatrin in tuberous
epilepsy: characteristics, course and in¯uence of sclerosis patients. Epilepsy Research, 38, 151±160.
drugs and surgery. Current Opinion in Neurology, Johannsen, P., Christensen, J.E., Goldstein, H., Niel-
14, 211±216. sen, V.K., & Mai, J. (1996). Epilepsy in Down
Helmstaedter, C., Wagner, G., & Elger, C.E. (1993). syndrome ± Prevalence in three age groups. Seizure,
Differential effects of ®rst antiepileptic drug applica- 5, 121±125.
tion on cognition in lesional and non-lesional patients Kaufman, K.R., & Gerner, R. (1998). Lamotrigine tox-
with epilepsy [see comments]. Seizure, 2, 125±130. icity secondary to sertraline. Seizure, 7, 163±165.
Hemmer, S.A., Pasternak, J.F., Zecker, S.G., & Trom- Khurana, D.S., Riviello, J., Helmers, S., Holmes, G.,
mer, B.L. (2001). Stimulant therapy and seizure risk in Anderson, J., & Mikati, M.A. (1996). Ef®cacy of
children with ADHD. Pediatric Neurology, 24, 99±102. gabapentin therapy in children with refractory partial
Hennessy, M.J., Elwes, R.D.C., Hovavar, M., Rabe- seizures. Journal of Pediatrics, 128, 829±833.
Hesketh, R., Binnie, C.D., & Polkey, C.E. (2001). Kim, K.Y., Craig, J.M., & Hawley, J.M. (2000). Seizure
Predictors of outcome and pathological considera- possibly associated with ¯uvoxamine. Annals of
tions in the surgical treatment of intractable epilepsy Pharmacotherapy, 34, 1276±1278.
associated with temporal lobe lesions. Journal of Knight, E.S., & Oxbury, S.M. (2000). Cognitive and
Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 70, 450± memory changes after hemispherectomy. In J.M.
458. Oxbury, C.E. Polkey, & M. Duchowny (Eds.), Intract-
Henriksen, O. (1996). Classi®cation of seizures and able focal epilepsy (pp. 819±825). Philadelphia:
epilepsies. In S. Wallace (Ed.), Epilepsy in children W B Saunders.
(pp. 113±152). London: Chapman & Hall Medical. Kurtz, Z., Tookey, P., & Ross, E. (1998). Epilepsy in
Hermann, B.P. (1990). Psychosocial outcome following young people: 23 year follow up of the British national
focal resections in childhood. Journal of Epilepsy, 3, child development study. British Medical Journal,
243±252. 316, 339±342.
Hermann, B.P., Whitman, S., Hughes, J.R., Melyn, Landau, W.M., & Kleffner, F.R. (1957). Syndrome of
M.M., & Dell, J. (1988). Multietiological determinants acquired aphasia with convulsive disorder in chil-
of psychopathology and social competence in children dren. Neurology (Minneap.), 7, 523±530.
with epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 2, 51±60. Lee, D.O., Steingard, R.J., Cesena, M., Helmers, S.L.,
Hoare, P. (1984a). Psychiatric disturbance in the fam- Riviello, J.J., & Mikati, M.A. (1996). Behavioral side
ilies of epileptic children. Developmental Medicine effects of gabapentin in children. Epilepsia, 37, 87±90.
and Child Neurology, 26, 14±19. Lee, J.W., Crismon, M.L., & Dorson, P.G. (1999).
Hoare, P. (1984b). The development of psychiatric Seizure associated with olanzapine. Annals of Phar-
disorder among schoolchildren with epilepsy. Devel- macotherapy, 33, 554±556.
opmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 26, 3±13. Lendt, M., Helmstaedter, C., & Elger, C.E. (1999). Pre-
Hoare, P., & Kerley, S. (1991). Psychosocial adjustment and postoperative neuropsychological pro®les in chil-
of children with chronic epilepsy and their families. dren and adolescents with temporal lobe epilepsy.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 33, Epilepsia, 40, 1543±1550.
201±215. Lendt, M., Helmstaedter, C., Kuczaty, S., Schramm, J.,
Hoare, P., & Mann, H. (1994). Self-esteem and beha- & Elger, C.E. (2000). Behavioural disorders in chil-
vioural adjustment in children with epilepsy and dren with epilepsy: Early improvement after surgery.
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 129

Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, surgical treatment of focal epilepsy [see comments].
69, 739±744. Journal of Neurosurgery, 70, 231±239.
Lewis, J.N., Tonge, B.J., Mowat, D.R., Einfeld, S.L., Morrell, F., Whisler, W.W., Smith, M.C., Hoeppner, T.J.,
Siddons, H.M., & Rees, V.W. (2000). Epilepsy and De, Toledo-Morrell, L., Pierre-Louis, S.J., Kanner,
associated psychopathology in young people with A.M., Buelow, J.M., Ristanovic, R., & Bergen, D.
intellectual disability. Journal of Paediatrics and (1995). Landau-Kleffner syndrome. Treatment with
Child Health, 36, 172±175. subpial intracortical transection. Brain, 118, 1529±
Lindsay, J., Ounsted, C., & Richards, P. (1984). Long- 1546.
term outcome in children with temporal lobe seizures. Mulligan, L.P., Spencer, D.D., & Spencer, S.S. (2001).
V: Indications and contra-indications for neurosur- Multiple subpial transections: The Yale experience.
gery. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Epilepsia, 42, 226±229.
26, 25±32. Neely, J.L. (1998). Tonic clonic seizures and tachycar-
Lindsay, J., Ounsted, C., & Richards, P. (1987). Hemi- dia induced by ¯uoxetine (Prozac) overdose. West
spherectomy for childhood epilepsy: A 36-year study. Virginia Medical Journal, 94, 283±285.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, Neyens, L.G.J., Aldenkamp, A.P., & Meinardi, H.M.
592±600. (1999). Prospective follow-up of intellectual develop-
Logsdail, S.J., & Toone, B.K. (1988). Post-ictal psycho- ment in children with a recent onset of epilepsy.
ses. A clinical and phenomenological description. Epilepsy Research, 34, 85±90.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 246±252. Nichols, M.E., Meador, K.J., & Loring, D.W. (1993).
Loo, H., Piau, A., Galinowski, A., & Olie, J.P. (1987). Neuropsychological effects of antiepileptic drugs: A
[Epileptogenic action of ¯uvoxamine. Apropos of a current perspective. Clinical Neuropharmacology, 16,
case]. [French]. Encephale, 13, 231±232. 471±484.
Luchetti, A., Amadi, A., Gobbi, G., & Bertani, G. (2000). Nieto-Barrera, M., Candau, R., Nieto-Jimenez, M., Cor-
Visual ®eld defects associated with vigabatrin mono- rea, A., & Ruiz del Portal, L. (2000). Topiramate in the
therapy in children. Journal of Neurology, Neurosur- treatment of severe myoclonic epilepsy in infancy.
gery and Psychiatry, 69, 566. Seizure, 9, 590±594.
Mace, C.J., & Trimble, M.R. (1991). Psychosis following Norrby, U., Carlsson, J., Beckung, E., & Nordholm, L.
temporal lobe surgery: A report of six cases. Journal of (1999). Self-assessment of well-being in a group of
Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 54, 639±644. children with epilepsy. Seizure, 8, 228±234.
Marston, D., Besag, F., Binnie, C.D., & Fowler, M. O'Donohoe, N.V. (1985). Epilepsies of Childhood,
(1993). Effects of transitory cognitive impairment on Second ed. Butterworths, London.
psychosocial functioning of children with epilepsy: A Oxbury, J.M., & Duchowny, M. (2000). Diagnosis
therapeutic trial. Developmental Medicine and Child and classi®cation. In J.M. Oxbury, C.E. Polkey &
Neurology, 35, 574±581. M. Duchowny (Eds.), Intractable focal epilepsy
Martin, A., Koenig, K., Scahill, L., & Bregman, J. (1999). (pp. 11±23). Philadelphia: W B Saunders.
Open-label quetiapine in the treatment of children Oxbury, S., Zaiwalla, Z., Adams, C., Middleton, J., &
and adolescents with autistic disorder. Journal of Oxbury, J. (1995). Hemispherectomy in childhood:
Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 9, 99± Serial neuropsychological follow-up and prolonged
107. improvement. Epilepsia, 36, S24.
McDermott, S., Mani, S., & Krishnaswami, S. (1995). A Pacia, S.V., & Devinsky, O. (1994). Clozapine-related
population-based analysis of speci®c behavior prob- seizures: Experience with 5,629 patients. Neurology,
lems associated with childhood seizures. Journal of 44, 2247±2249.
Epilepsy, 8, 110±118. Pavone, P., Bianchini, R., Tri®letti, R.R., Incorpora, G.,
McDougle, C.J., Holmes, J.P., Carlson, D.C., Pelton, Pavone, A., & Parano, E. (2001). Neuropsychological
G.H., Cohen, D.J., & Price, L.H. (1998). A double- assessment in children with absence epilepsy. Neur-
blind, placebo-controlled study of risperidone in ology, 56, 1047±1051.
adults with autistic disorder and other pervasive Pazzaglia, P., & Frank-Pazzaglia, L. (1976). Record in
developmental disorders. [see comments]. Archives grade school of pupils with epilepsy: An epidemiolog-
of General Psychiatry, 55, 633±641. ical study. Epilepsia, 17, 361±366.
Meador, K.J. (1998). Cognitive side effects of medica- Pond, D.A., & Bidwell, B.H. (1960). A survey of epilepsy
tions. Neurologic Clinics, 16, 141±155. in fourteen general practices II: Social and psycholo-
Meador, K.J. (2000). Current discoveries on the cogni- gical aspects. Epilepsia, 1, 285±299.
tive effects of antiepileptic drugs. Pharmacotherapy, Prasad, A.N., Penney, S., & Buckley, D.J. (2001). The
20, 185S±190S. role of vigabatrin in childhood seizure disorders:
Meador, K.J., Loring, D.W., Huh, K., Gallagher, B.B., & Results from a clinical audit. Epilepsia, 42, 54±61.
King, D.W. (1990). Comparative cognitive effects of Reutens, D.C., Savard, G., Andermann, F., Dubeau, F.,
anticonvulsants. Neurology, 40, 391±394. & Olivier, A. (1997). Results of surgical treatment in
Miranda, C., & Smith, M.L. (2001). Predictors of intel- temporal lobe epilepsy with chronic psychosis. Brain,
ligence after temporal lobectomy in children with 120, 1929±1936.
epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 2, 13±19. Richardson, S.A., Koller, H., Katz, M., & McLaren, J.
Moreland, E.C., Griesemer, D.A., & Holden, K.R. (1999). (1980). Seizures and epilepsy in a mentally retarded
Topiramate for intractable childhood epilepsy. Seiz- population over the ®rst 22 years of life. Applied
ure, 8, 38±40. Research in Mental Retardation, 1, 123±138.
Morrell, F., Whisler, W.W., & Bleck, T.P. (1989). Mul- Riddle, M.A., Brown, N., Dzubinski, D., Jetmalani, A.N.,
tiple subpial transection: A new approach to the Law, Y., & Woolston, J.L. (1989). Fluoxetine overdose
130 Frank M.C. Besag

in an adolescent. Journal of the American Academy of childhood [see comments]. New England Journal of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 587±588. Medicine, 338, 1715±1722.
Robinson, R.O., Baird, G., Robinson, G., & Simonoff, E. Slater, E., & Beard, A.W. (1995). The schizophrenia-like
(2001). Landau-Kleffner syndrome: Course correlates psychoses of epilepsy, V: Discussion and conclu-
with outcome. Developmental Medicine & Child Neur- sions. 1963 [classical article]. Journal of Neuropsy-
ology, 43, 243±247. chiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 7, 372±378.
Ronen, G.M., Richards, J.E., Cunningham, C., Secord, Smith, M.C. (1998). Multiple subpial transection
M., & Rosenbloom, D. (2000). Can sodium valproate in patients with extratemporal epilepsy. [Review]
improve learning in children with epileptiform bursts [33 refs]. Epilepsia, 39 Suppl 4, S81±S89.
but without clinical seizures? Developmental Medi- Spina, E., Avenoso, A., Facciola, G., Salemi, M., Scordo,
cine and Child Neurology, 42, 751±755. M.G., Giacobello, T., Madia, A.G., & Perucca, E.
Ronnberg, J., Samuelsson, S., & Soderfeldt, B. (1992). (2000). Plasma concentrations of risperidone and
Memory effects following carbamazepine monother- 9-hydroxyrisperidone: Effect of comedication with
apy in patients with complex partial epilepsy. Seizure, carbamazepine or valproate. Therapeutic Drug Monit-
1, 247±253. oring, 22, 481±485.
Rosenstein, D.L., Nelson, J.C., & Jacobs, S.C. (1993). Steffenburg, U., Hagberg, G., Viggedal, G., & Kyllerman,
Seizures associated with antidepressants: A review. M. (1995). Active epilepsy in mentally retarded chil-
[see comments]. [Review] [93 refs]. Journal of Clinical dren. I. Prevalence and additional neuro-impair-
Psychiatry, 54, 289±299. ments. Acta Paediatrica, 84, 1147±1152.
Ross, E.M., Peckham, C.S., West, P.B., & Butler, N.R. Stores, G. (1971). Cognitive function in children with
(1980). Epilepsy in childhood: Findings from the epilepsy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurol-
National Child Development Study. British Medical ogy, 13, 390±393.
Journal, 280, 207±210. Stores, G. (1973). Studies of attention and seizure
Rutter, M., Graham, P., & Yule, W. (1970). A neuropsy- disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurol-
chiatric study in childhood. London: Heinemann ogy, 15, 376±382.
Medical. Stores, G. (1976). The investigation and management of
Sabaz, M., Cairns, D.R., Lawson, J.A., Nheu, N., school children with epilepsy. Public Health, 90, 171±
Bleasel, A.F., & Bye, A.M. (2000). Validation of a 177.
new quality of life measure for children with epilepsy. Stores, G. (1978). School-children with epilepsy at risk
Epilepsia, 41, 765±774. for learning and behaviour problems. Developmental
Sawrie, S.M., Martin, R.C., Kuzniecky, R., Faught, E., Medicine and Child Neurology, 20, 502±508.
Morawetz, R., Jamil, F., Viikinsalo, M., & Gilliam, F. Stores, G., Hart, J., & Piran, N. (1978). Inattentiveness in
(1999). Subjective versus objective memory change schoolchildren with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 19, 169±175.
after temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Neurology, 53, Stores, G., & Hart, J.A. (1975). Proceedings: Reading
1511±1517. skills of children with generalized and focal epilepsy
Schuld, A., Kuhn, M., Haack, M., Kraus, T., Hinze- attending ordinary school. Electroencephalography &
Selch, D., Lechner, C., & Pollmacher, T. (2000). A Clinical Neurophysiology, 39, 429±430.
comparison of the effects of clozapine and olanzapine Stores, G., & Piran, N. (1978). Dependency of different
on the EEG in patients with schizophrenia. Pharmaco- types in schoolchildren with epilepsy. Psychological
psychiatry, 33, 109±111. Medicine, 8, 441±445.
Semrud-Clikeman, M., & Wical, B. (1999). Components Szabo, C.A., Wyllie, E., Stanford, L.D., Geckler, C.,
of attention in children with complex partial sei- Kotagal, P., Comair, Y.G., & Thornton, A.E. (1998).
zures with and without ADHD. Epilepsia, 40, 211±215. Neuropsychological effect of temporal lobe resection
Shields, W.D. (2000). Catastrophic epilepsy in child- in preadolescent children with epilepsy. Epilepsia,
hood. [Review] [30 refs]. Epilepsia, 41 Suppl 2, S2±S6. 39, 814±819.
Shinnar, S. (1998). Prolonged febrile seizures and Taylor, D.C., Neville, B.G., & Cross, J.H. (1999). Aut-
mesial temporal sclerosis [editorial; comment]. istic spectrum disorders in childhood epilepsy sur-
Annals of Neurology, 43, 411±412. gery candidates. European Child and Adolescent
Shinnar, S., Pellock, J.M., Berg, A.T., O'Dell, C., Dris- Psychiatry, 8, 189±192.
coll, S.M., Maytal, J., Moshe, S.L., & DeLorenzo, R.J. Tonnby, B., Nilsson, H.L., Aldenkamp, A.P., Alpherts,
(2001). Short-term outcomes of children with febrile W.C.J., Blennow, G., Elmqvist, D., Heijbel, J., Sand-
status epilepticus. Epilepsia, 42, 47±53. stedt, P., Wahlander, L., & Wosse, E. (1994). With-
SillanpaÈa
È , M. (1983). Social functioning and seizure drawal of antiepileptic medication in children.
status of young adults with onset of epilepsy in Correlation of cognitive function and plasma concen-
childhood. An epidemiological 20-year follow-up tration±The multicentre `Holmfrid' study. Epilepsy
study. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica Supplementum, Research, 19, 141±152.
96, 1±81. Trabert, W., Hohagen, F., Winkelmann, G., & Berger, M.
SillanpaÈa
È , M. (1990). Children with epilepsy as adults: (1995). A seizure, and electroencephalographic signs
outcome after 30 years of follow-up. [Review] [133 of a lowered seizure threshold, associated with ¯u-
refs]. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica ± Supplement, voxamine treatment of obsessive-compulsive disor-
368, 1±78. der. Pharmacopsychiatry, 28, 95±97.
SillanpaÈa
È , M. (1992). Epilepsy in children: Prevalence, Trimble, M. (1979). The effect of anti-convulsant drugs
disability, and handicap. Epilepsia, 33, 444±449. on cognitive abilities. [Review] [36 refs]. Pharmacology
SillanpaÈa
È , M., Jalava, M., Kaleva, O., & Shinnar, S. & Therapeutics ± Part B: General & Systematic Phar-
(1998). Long-term prognosis of seizures with onset in macology, 4, 677±685.
Childhood epilepsy in relation to mental handicap and behavioural disorders 131

Trimble, M., & Cornaggia, C.M.E. (2001). The use of Volkow, N.D., & Tancredi, L. (1987). Neural substrates
psychotropic drugs in epilepsy. Epilepsia, Suppl. of violent behaviour. A preliminary study with posit-
Trimble, M.R. (1987a). Anticonvulsant drugs and cog- ron emission tomography. British Journal of Psychi-
nitive function: A review of the literature. [Review] atry, 151, 668±673.
[46 refs]. Epilepsia, 28 Suppl 3, S37±S45. Wallace, S.J. (1988). The Child with Febrile Seizures,
Trimble, M.R. (1987b). Psychiatric aspects of epilepsy. pp. 81. London: Wright.
[Review] [61 refs]. Psychiatric Developments, 5, 285± Wallace, S.J. (1996a). A comparative review of the
300. adverse effects of anticonvulsants in children with
Trimble, M.R., & Cull, C. (1988). Children of school age: epilepsy. [Review] [76 refs]. Drug Safety, 15, 378±393.
the in¯uence of antiepileptic drugs on behavior and Wallace, S.J. (1996b). Febrile seizures. In S. Wallace
intellect. [Review] [27 refs]. Epilepsia, 29 Suppl 3, (Ed.), Epilepsy in children (pp. 185±198). London:
S15±S19. Chapman & Hall Medical.
Trimble, M.R., Rusch, N., Betts, T., & Crawford, P.A. Wallace, S.J. (1998). Myoclonus and epilepsy in child-
(2000). Psychiatric symptoms after therapy with new hood: A review of treatment with valproate, ethosuxi-
antiepileptic drugs: Psychopathological and seizure mide, lamotrigine and zonisamide. [Review] [46 refs].
related variables. Seizure, 9, 249±254. Epilepsy Research, 29, 147±154.
Tuchman, R.F., Rapin, I., & Shinnar, S. (1991). Autistic Westerveld, M., Sass, K.J., Chelune, G.J., Hermann,
and dysphasic children. II: Epilepsy. Pediatrics, 88, B.P., Barr, W.B., Loring, D.W., Strauss, E., Trenerry,
1219±1225. M.R., Perrine, K., & Spencer, D.D. (2000). Temporal
van Elst, L.T., Woermann, F.G., Lemieux, L., Thomp- lobectomy in children: cognitive outcome. Journal of
son, P.J., & Trimble, M.R. (2000). Affective aggres- Neurosurgery, 92, 24±30.
sion in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: A Wieser, H.G., Blume, W.T., Fish, D., Goldensohn, E.,
quantitative MRI study of the amygdala. Brain, Hufnagel, A., King, D., Sperling, M.R., & Luders, H.
123, 234±243. (2001). ILAE Commission Report. Proposal for a new
Vasconcellos, E., Wyllie, E., Sullivan, S., Stanford, L., classi®cation of outcome with respect to epileptic
Bulacio, J., Kotagal, P., & Bingaman, W. (2001). seizures following epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia, 42,
Mental retardation in pediatric candidates for epi- 282±286.
lepsy surgery: The role of early seizure onset. Epilep- Williams, J., Griebel, M.L., Sharp, G.B., & Boop, F.A.
sia, 42, 268±274. (1998). Cognition and behavior after temporal lobec-
Verity, C.M., Greenwood, R., & Golding, J. (1998). Long- tomy in pediatric patients with intractable epilepsy.
term intellectual and behavioral outcomes of children Pediatric Neurology, 19, 189±194.
with febrile convulsions [see comments]. New Eng- Wyderski, R.J., Starrett, W.G., & Abou-Saif, A. (1999).
land Journal of Medicine, 338, 1723±1728. Fatal status epilepticus associated with olanzapine
Verity, C.M., Ross, E.M., & Golding, J. (1992). Epilepsy therapy. Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 33, 787±789.
in the ®rst 10 years of life: Findings of the child health Zuddas, A., Di Martino, A., Muglia, P., & Cianchetti, C.
and education study. British Medical Journal, 305, (2000). Long-term risperidone for pervasive devel-
857±861. opmental disorder: Ef®cacy, tolerability, and dis-
Vermeulen, J., & Aldenkamp, A.P. (1995). Cognitive continuation. Journal of Child and Adolescent
side-effects of chronic antiepileptic drug treatment: A Psychopharmacology, 10, 79±90.
review of 25 years of research. Epilepsy Research, 22,
65±95. Manuscript accepted 14 August 2001

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen