Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Environmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jep

Green identity, green living? The role of pro-environmental self-identity in


determining consistency across diverse pro-environmental behaviours
Lorraine Whitmarsh a, b, *, Saffron O’Neill b, c
a
School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower Building, Park Place, Cardiff CF10 3AT, UK
b
Tyndall Centre for Climate Change Research, UK
c
Department of Resource Management and Geography, University of Melbourne, 221 Bouverie Street, VIC 3010, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Policy-makers are interested in cost-effective and socially acceptable ways of encouraging the public
Available online 25 January 2010 to adopt more environmentally-friendly lifestyles. One area which UK policy-makers are focussing on
is ‘catalyst behaviour’, the notion that taking-up a new behaviour (such as recycling) may cause
Keywords: people to adopt other pro-environmental behaviours. Yet, evidence for such ‘spill-over’ effects is so
Self-identity far limited, and it is unclear when and how cross-situational motivations (e.g., pro-environmental
Pro-environmental behaviour
identity) may predict behaviour and when contextual factors are more important. We report on
Spill-over effects
Theory of planned behaviour
a postal survey (N ¼ 551) of pro-environmental behaviours amongst the UK public. We assess the
Carbon offsetting influence of pro-environmental self-identify on consistency across a range of behaviours. Pro-envi-
ronmental values, perceived behavioural control, subjective norm, attitudes, and demographic factors
were also measured. Findings show self-identity to be a significant behavioural determinant over and
above theory of planned behaviour variables for carbon offsetting behaviour. However, pro-envi-
ronmental self-identity was only a significant predictor for certain other pro-environmental behav-
iours; background variables were also important predictors. Limitations of the study, and implications
for theory and policy, are discussed.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 1.1. Cross-situational environmental motivations and spill-over


effects
In recent years, the UK has positioned itself at the centre of
international efforts to address climate change, setting an ambi- There is much interest amongst UK policy-makers in finding
tious target of 80% reduction in greenhouse gas emissions by 2050 levers to produce wholesale shifts in lifestyles towards ‘greener’
(HM Government, 2008). This level of response to climate change (particularly, low-carbon) living. In general, governments are
has profound implications for individual choices and behaviour. reluctant to regulate in large part because of the fear of public
With over one-third of many nations’ carbon emissions coming backlash and loss of political support (Carter & Ockwell, 2007).
from private travel and domestic energy use (e.g., DEFRA, 2005), Consequently, across the political spectrum, there is a great interest
governments are recognising the urgent need to encourage indi- in the latest methods to ‘edit choices’ or ‘nudge’ lifestyles in
viduals to adopt low-carbon lifestyles. Policies to achieve this have a desired direction through cost-effective and socially acceptable
met with limited success: after decades of information campaigns approaches (e.g., Cialdini, 2006; Thaler & Sunstein, 2008) ‘without
and other (often economic) measures to encourage ‘green’ behav- [recourse to] huge centralised bureaucracy’ (Letwin, cited in
iours, the public is prepared to (and often does) recycle, but few Chakrabortty, 2008) or compromising consumer sovereignty
take action beyond this (e.g., DEFRA, 2002, 2007; Whitmarsh, (Hinchliffe, 1996).
2009). Travel habits remain particularly resistant to change (King One particular area in which the UK Department for Environ-
et al., 2009; Verplanken, Aarts, & van Knippenberg, 1997). ment, Food and Rural Affairs (DEFRA) has recently shown interest is
‘catalyst behaviours’, the notion that taking-up a new behaviour
(such as recycling) may lead to adoption of other, more environ-
* Corresponding author. School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower Building,
mentally-beneficial, behaviours (see DEFRA, 2008b; WWF-UK,
Park Place, Cardiff CF10 3AT, UK. Tel.: þ44 2920 876972; fax: þ44 2920 874858. 2009). Such a notion appears to hold the promise of changing
E-mail address: whitmarshle@cardiff.ac.uk (L. Whitmarsh). a suite of behaviours in a cost-effective manner with little

0272-4944/$ – see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.01.003
306 L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314

regulation or structural change. On the other hand, DEFRA also asserts that behavioural intention is determined by attitude
acknowledge that negative spill-over may exist, whereby taking up towards performing the action, subjective norm (motivations to
one behaviour (e.g., recycling) deters another (e.g., waste comply with the expectations of significant others) and perceived
prevention).1 behavioural control (the extent to which the action is considered
This view of a common motivational root underpinning pro- under one’s control) (Ajzen, 1991). While much research on pro-
environmental behaviours has intuitive appeal. It also has some environmental behaviour is focused at the broader level of ‘general
theoretical support from models of behaviour that postulate cross- conservation stance’ (e.g., Thøgersen & Ölander, 2006), the TPB
situational goals or general values (Lindenberg & Steg, 2007; (and its predecessor the Theory of Reasoned Action) emphasises
Rokeach, 1973; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1990). Furthermore, there is that specific (behaviour-oriented) attitudes are more likely than
some – albeit limited – evidence of such spill-over effects in broad orientations to predict behavioural intention (Ajzen &
relation to pro-environmental behaviour (e.g., Whitmarsh, 2009). Fishbein, 1980).
Recent studies suggest behaviour may be clustered in some way
that reflects either similar ‘types’ of behaviour, in respect of 1.2. Self-identity and pro-environmental behaviour
context or frequency or different levels of environmental
commitment (easy/difficult), or similar individual characteristics, There have been various attempts to extend the TPB to
such as values or demographics. Barr, Gilg, and Ford’s (2005) UK encompass other potentially relevant determinants of behaviour,
study identified three such clusters – which they label ‘purchase and thus improve its predictive power. A promising advance in
decisions’ (shopping, composting and reuse), ‘habits’ (domestic this respect concerns self-identity (e.g., Sparks & Shepherd, 1992).
water and energy conservation), and ‘recycling’ – and found these This is generally understood to mean the label used to describe
relate to different lifestyles (i.e., socio-demographic characteristics oneself (e.g., Cook, Kerr, & Moore, 2002), and is influenced both by
and values). This analysis did not extend to broader environ- personal motivations (for self-esteem, self-enhancement, and self-
mentally-significant action such as travel or political behaviours. understanding) as well as social interaction in the form of
Danish research on spill-over effects has found that individuals are demands and expectations of others and the various roles we
fairly consistent within similar categories of behaviour, and that perform (Ellmers, Spears, & Doosje, 2002; Stryker & Burke, 2000;
there are significant correlations across these categories – buying Tajfel & Turner, 1986). Consistent with self-perception theories,
organic food and recycling (.31, p < .05); buying organic food and individuals act in accordance with their own, and others’, expec-
using alternative transport (.16, p < .05); recycling and using tations of them (Bem, 1967). Self-identity serves both to differ-
alternative transport (.17, p < .05) – which can be accounted for by entiate oneself from others and to conform to the values, beliefs
common motivational causes (general environmental values and and behaviours of the social groups to which one belongs
concern) (Thøgersen & Ölander, 2006). Despite these promising (Christensen, Rothberger, Wood, & Matz, 2004). Assertion of
insights, it is still far from clear why or how spill-over effects occur identity may be understood as an attempt to establish consistency
and whether they are due primarily to contextual factors or in our attitudes and actions and continuity across experiences, and
individual motivations. therefore appears to be highly relevant in exploring consistency
The broader literature on pro-environmental behaviour high- (and, ultimately, spill-over effects) across pro-environmental
lights the diversity of factors which influence different environ- behaviours.
mentally-significant behaviours. Although environmental values There are various studies which highlight the identity-behav-
or concern may play a role, other motivations and structural iour link (e.g., Biddle, Bank, & Slavings, 1987; Eagly, Chaiken, &
factors often play a greater role (e.g., Bamberg & Schmidt, 2003; Jovanovich, 1993; Stets & Biga, 2003). Consumption behaviours
Jackson, 2005; Kollmuss & Agyeman, 2002; Schultz, Oskamp, & and adoption of new products, for example, are linked to identity
Mainieri, 1995; Steg, Vlek, & Slotegraaf, 2001), hampering the (Cook et al., 2002; Grewal, Mehta, & Kardes, 2000). Self-identity has
pursuit of a single model of behaviour for predicting pro-envi- been found to be a significant predictor of behaviour over and
ronmental behaviour (Darnton, 2008). Indeed, it is important to above TPB variables, including in relation to pro-environmental
consider that ‘pro-environmental behaviour’ need not be moti- action (Fekadu & Kraft, 2001; Sparks & Shepherd, 1992; Sparks,
vated by environmental concern or values at all (Stern, 2000). Shepherd, & Frewer, 1995; Terry, Hogg, & White, 1999). For
Whitmarsh (2009), for example, found that the proportion of the example, people who see themselves as typical recyclers are more
public taking action explicitly out of concern for climate change likely to recycle than those who do not perceive themselves as
was much lower than the proportion claiming to conserve energy; recyclers (Mannetti, Pierro, & Livi, 2004). Identity may even over-
further, energy conservation was more commonly motivated by ride attitude in cases where our role identity dictates we behave in
financial or health benefits than by environmental concern. There a certain way, irrespective of how we feel about that behaviour
are also various psychological, social, economic and physical (Charng, Pillavin, & Callero, 1988). Related literatures on place
barriers that mitigate against environmental concerns being identity (sense of self linked to physical and symbolic attributes of
translated into pro-environmental behaviour (Lorenzoni, particular locations; Proshansky, Fabian, & Kaminoff, 1983) also
Nicholson-Cole, & Whitmarsh, 2007; McKenzie-Mohr & Smith, show this can influence action to protect the local area/ecologies
1999). This evidence would appear to undermine any expecta- from perceived threats from development (e.g., Devine-Wright,
tion that people act consistently across diverse behavioural 2009).
domains, or that there is a common motivational basis for pro- Past behavioural frequency may moderate the relationship
environmental behaviour. between self-identity and behaviour: self-identity influences
This lack of generality across pro-environmental behaviours is intentions at low, rather than high, levels of past behaviour
consistent with the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB), which (Fekadu & Kraft, 2001; Smith et al., 2007). It may be that behaviour
informs identity construction as people seek behavioural consis-
tency (Bem, 1967), but that, as behaviour becomes routine and
1
automatic (i.e., habitual; Verplanken & Orbell, 2003), it disappears
This is consistent with the economic literature on ‘rebound effects’, where
material or energy efficiency measures free up resources that can be spent on other
from view and thus from self-identity. On the other hand, research
consuming activities thus reducing the net decrease in overall consumption (e.g., by Sparks and Shepherd (1992) found that people who identify
Herring & Sorrell, 2008). themselves as ‘green consumers’ are more likely to buy organic
L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314 307

food than those who do not, irrespective of past behaviour. Given Table 1
the possible interaction of past behaviour and identity, our Demographic characteristics of survey sample.a

research considers both of these factors in relation to pro-envi- %


ronmental behaviour. Gender
There appear to be at least two levels at which identity may Female (0) 53.4
operate in the context of pro-environmental behaviour: behav- Male (1) 44.9
Prefer not to say 1.7
iour-specific and generic. The former includes, for example,
identity as a ‘typical recycler’ (as in Mannetti et al., 2004), while Age
the latter could encompass a sub-set of environmental actions, 16–24 (1) 7.3
25–44 (2) 28.7
such as green consumption (as in Sparks & Shepherd, 1992), or 45–64 (3) 38.2
indeed all possible pro-environmental actions. A recent review of 65 and over (4) 25.5
the spill-over literature (WWF-UK, 2009) suggested that the Prefer not to say .4
former may be useful for explaining persistence in performing Household income (before tax)
a specific pro-environmental behaviour (and thus will be closely Up to £9999 per annum (0) 12.4
linked to past behaviour), while the latter may account for spill- £10,000–£19,999 per annum (1) 13.9
over between pro-environmental behaviours. To date, however, £20,000–£29,999 per annum (2) 11.8
£30,000–£39,999 per annum (3) 10.8
there have been no efforts to measure both kinds of self-identity
£40,000–£49,999 per annum (4) 7.8
together or to consider how these forms of identity may be related £50,000–£74,999 per annum (5) 11.4
or might interact. £75,000 or more per annum (6) 7.4
The aims of the present study are two-fold. First, we test an Don’t know (7) 7.0
Prefer not to say 17.5
extended model of the TPB which includes self-identity and past
behaviour. We include both specific and generic identity Qualifications
measures to consider to what extent behavioural intention to No formal qualifications (0) 19.9
GCSE/O-Level (1) 12.1
purchase carbon offsets2 is influenced by self-identity as
A-Level/Higher/BTEC (2) 10.7
a ‘carbon offsetter’ and/or broader pro-environmental self-iden- Vocational/NVQ (3) 14.2
tity. We also consider the relationship between these two types Degree or equivalent (4) 26.1
of identity. Second, we investigate the relationships between Postgraduate qualification (5) 14.6
various pro-environmental behaviours to assess the degree of Other 2.5

consistency across a range of different behaviours. We consider Qualifications in science-related subject


whether such this consistency is due to a general motivational No formal qualifications (0) 40.3
GCSE/O-Level (1) 27.2
cause (pro-environmental self-identity, pro-environmental
A-Level/Higher/BTEC (2) 12.1
values or perceptions of climate change) or to contextual or Vocational/NVQ (3) 2.7
demographic factors. Degree or equivalent (4) 12.1
Postgraduate qualification (5) 5.0
Other .6

2. Methods No. of adults (incl. you) living in your house


1 25.3
2 55.4
2.1. Participants and design
3 12.2
4 or more 7.1
Data collection was via a postal survey conducted in August–
No. of children (ie., under 16) living in your house
October 2008 in Norfolk and Hampshire, UK. Three thousand 0 77.3
questionnaires (with stamped, addressed return envelopes) were 1 9.8
distributed by hand to a random sample of residents, drawn from 2 9.1
the electoral register, within nine wards (six in Norfolk, three in 3 or more 3.9
Hampshire). The wards represented both urban and rural, and Area density
diverse socio-demographic, stratifications. City (3) 59.3
In total, we received 551 responses (representing a response Town (2) 12.0
Village or hamlet (1) 28.6
rate of 18.4%3). Participants in the postal survey were broadly
County
Norfolk (1) 63.7
2 Hampshire (2) 36.3
Carbon offsetting is defined as ‘‘the purchase of credits from greenhouse gas
emission reduction projects in one place to counter the emissions of greenhouse Political party most likely to support
gases in another place’’ (POST, 2007). Although there is debate about its efficacy Labour (1) 16.1
as a climate change mitigation strategy, it is considered one way in which corpo- Liberal Democrats (2) 13.4
rate and private consumers can help tackle climate change (Lovell et al., 2009). Conservative (3) 28.7
There has been significant growth in the offset market, which is currently worth Green (4) 11.8
over US$91 m (Hamilton, Bayon, Turner, & Higgins, 2007); approximately 15% of Other/Prefer not to say 21.3
the market are individual/private consumers (3% of UK population; DEFRA, Would not vote (0) 8.7
2007). a
3 Figures shown in brackets indicate values entered in analyses.
This response rate is relatively low since (due to budgetary constraints) we did
not use reminders or prompts to boost response rates. Although the sample is
demographically representative, we note the likelihood that respondents had
stronger opinions (and, specifically, may be more pro-environmental) than the
general public. Nevertheless, since our findings are consistent with those of a recent demographically representative of the population sampled (see
nation-wide, representative survey of English pro-environmental attitudes, behav-
iours and identity (N ¼ 3618; DEFRA, 2007, 2008a), as well as the wider literature
Table 1), although somewhat higher qualified (26% have
reviewed earlier, we do not feel our response rate undermines the validity of our a degree, slightly more than the national average of 20%
findings. according to 2001 census data). Data was analysed using SPSS.
308 L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314

2.2. Measures (alpha ¼ .7) (Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig, & Jones, 2000; Whitmarsh,
2009).4
The eight-page questionnaire included both closed and open A pro-environmental self-identity scale was developed using
questions, and addressed knowledge and attitudes in relation to measures adapted from previous research (Cook et al., 2002;
climate change, TPB and self-identity measures for carbon offsetting, Sparks & Shepherd, 1992). Four items – ‘I think of myself as an
pro-environmental values and self-identity, pro-environmental environmentally-friendly consumer’, ‘I think of myself as
behaviours, as well as background characteristics (see Table 1). someone who is very concerned with environmental issues’, ‘I
Questionnaires were piloted with 15 residents from Norfolk, would be embarrassed to be seen as having an environmentally-
following which only minor modifications to the questionnaire were friendly lifestyle’ (scoring reversed), and ‘I would not want my
required. family or friends to think of me as someone who is concerned
about environmental issues’ (scoring reversed) – were measured
2.2.1. TPB and self-identity in relation to carbon offsetting on a 5-point agreement scale and formed a reliable scale
Based on previous qualitative research (Lippincott Mercer, (alpha ¼ .7).
2006; Lovell, Bulkeley, & Liverman, 2009), 15 statements were Pro-environmental behaviour (PEB) encompasses multiple
developed to measure attitudes to offsetting on a 5-point scale from domains, frequencies and impacts of action (Stern, 2000).
‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’. Principal Components Anal- A recent UK review, led by DEFRA (2008a), has identified 12
ysis (PCA) indicated these formed two distinct components ‘headline behaviours’ which include both low and high
accounting for 58% of variance: environmental impact actions, as well as one-off and regular
decisions, relating to four behavioural domains: domestic
 Component 1 (9 items, alpha ¼ .90) comprised Positive Atti- energy/water use, waste behaviour, transport, and eco-friendly
tudes: ‘I would trust companies offering carbon offsetting to shopping.
use the money I paid in the right way’, ‘By making people more
aware of how their behaviour affects the environment, carbon 1. Domestic energy/water use includes: installing insulation
offsetting encourages more environmentally-friendly behav- products, better energy management and usage, installing
iour’, ‘People who care about the environment tend to buy domestic micro-generation through renewables, and more
carbon offsets’, ‘Carbon offsetting should be mandatory’, responsible water usage.
‘Carbon offsetting can help tackle climate change’, ‘Carbon 2. Waste behaviours include: increasing recycling and segrega-
offsetting can help people in developing countries’, ‘Carbon tion, and wasting less (food).
offsetting can help wildlife and habitats’, ‘Carbon offsetting is 3. Transport actions include: buying/using more energy-efficient
a quick and easy way of tackling climate change’, and ‘Carbon (low-carbon) vehicles, using the car less – seeking alternatives
offsetting can help reduce unavoidable emissions’. for short trips (<3 miles), and reducing non-essential flying
 Component 2 (6 items, alpha ¼ .85) comprised Negative Atti- (short haul).
tudes: ‘Carbon offsetting encourages people to carry on doing 4. Shopping choices include: buying energy-efficient products,
things that harm the environment’, ‘Carbon offsetting will eating food locally in season, and adopting a diet with lower
make no difference in the fight against climate change’, ‘Carbon environmental impacts.
offsetting is just another form of taxation’, ‘Carbon offsetting is
too much hassle’, ‘Carbon offsetting is a waste of time’, and In several cases, these headline behaviours are too broad to be
‘Carbon offsetting is a rip-off’. measured directly (e.g., ‘better energy management and use’ or
‘wasting less’), so where appropriate we have derived multiple
Perceived behaviour control for offsetting was measured with one measures to disaggregate these activities. Table 2 shows the pro-
item: ‘At the moment, how easy would you find it to purchase environmental measures used in the survey. When scaled, these 24
carbon offsets?’ on a 4-point scale from ‘very easy’ to ‘not at all items formed a reliable measure of pro-environmental behaviour
easy’. (PEB; alpha ¼ .92). The mean score on the PEB scale is 27.9
Responses to the following three items were multiplied to (SD ¼ 9.7) out of 72.
produce the subjective norm (alpha ¼ .69): ‘Do any of your friends, Finally, two measures were included on driving and flying
family or colleagues buy carbon offsets?’ (‘yes’, ‘no’); ‘How much behaviours: ‘How often do you personally use a car or van to travel,
influence do your family, friends and colleagues have on your either as a driver or as a passenger?’ (‘6–7 days a week’, ‘3–5 days
decision to purchase – or not purchase – carbon offsets?’ (4-point a week’, ‘1–2 days a week’, ‘once or twice a month’, ‘less often’,
scale from ‘large influence’ to ‘no influence’), and ‘In general, what ‘never’); ‘Did you take any flights in 2007 for leisure, holidays or
do you think your family’s, friends’ or colleagues’ views would be of visiting family or friends?’ (‘yes’, ‘no’).
you purchasing carbon offsets?’ (5-point scale from ‘very favour-
able’ to ‘very unfavourable’).
Behavioural intention was assessed on a 4-point scale (from 4
Previous research indicated that a number of people had difficulty interpreting
‘definitely will’ to ‘definitely won’t’) with the item ‘Do you think nine of the fifteen NEP items, so these items were excluded from the final question-
you will use carbon offsetting in the future?’. naire. The shortened version included the statements: ‘Humans have the right to
Two items on a 5-point agreement scale measured offsetting modify the natural environment to suit their needs’; ‘Humans are severely abusing
identity (alpha ¼ .81): ‘I am not the type of person who would buy the planet’; ‘Plants and animals have the same rights as humans to exist’; ‘Nature is
strong enough to cope with the impact of modern industrial nations’; ‘Humans
carbon offsets’ (scoring reversed) and ‘I am the type of person who
were meant to rule over the rest of nature’; ‘The balance of nature is very delicate
would buy carbon offsets’. and easily upset’. This shortened scale was used in previous research (Whitmarsh,
Past behaviour was measured with the item ‘Have you ever offset 2009), when it was also found to be reliable (Cronbach’s alpha ¼ .72). It should also
your carbon emissions?’ (‘yes’, ‘no’). be noted that the NEP technically measures environmental worldview rather than
environmental values relative to other values, in the sense of ‘Schwartz’ Value
Inventory (e.g., Schwartz & Bilsky, 1990); nevertheless, since the NEP is widely
2.2.2. Pro-environmental values and self-identity used as a measure of environmental values, and does indicate whether individuals
Pro-environmental values were measured using a reduced (6- consider nature/environment to have intrinsic or extrinsic value, we use it here as
item) version of the New Environmental Paradigm (NEP) scale a measure of environmental values.
L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314 309

Table 2
Pro-environmental behaviour measures and scores.

Mean SD Unrotated factor loadingc


a
Please indicate the last time you took this action (if at all)
Installed insulation products in your home 1.15 .96 .37
Bought or built an energy-efficient home .18 .57 .11
Installed a more efficient heating system .80 .99 .30
Installed a renewable energy system (e.g., solar panels, wind turbine) in your home .07 .39 .10
Changed to a ‘green’ energy tariff for your home .25 .73 .32
Bought a low-emission vehicle (e.g., hybrid, electric, biofuel, less than 1.4 L engine) .34 .80 .20
Bought a product to save water (e.g., water but, water ‘hippo’, low-flush toilet) 1.05 1.14 .51

Please indicate how often you take each actionb


Turn off lights you’re not using 2.56 .71 .53
Drive economically (e.g., braking or accelerating gently) 1.75 1.12 .38
Walk, cycle or take public transport for short journeys (i.e., trips of less than 3 miles) 1.86 1.00 .40
Use an alternative to travelling (e.g., shopping online) .90 .93 .36
Share a car journey with someone else 1.05 .90 .39
Cut down on the amount you fly 1.10 1.18 .45
Buy environmentally-friendly products 1.43 .79 .70
Eat food which is organic, locally-grown or in season 1.60 .85 .60
Avoid eating meat .66 .94 .44
Buy products with less packaging 1.46 .86 .66
Recycle 2.52 .81 .66
Reuse or repair items instead of throwing them away 1.88 .94 .61
Compost your kitchen waste 1.36 1.32 .48
Save water by taking shorter showers 1.59 1.13 .59
Turn off the tap while you brush your teeth 2.15 1.07 .57
Write to your MP about an environmental issue .11 .38 .30
Take part in a protest about an environmental issue .11 .38 .36
a
Response options: never (0), 5 or more years ago (1), 1–3 years ago (2), In the last year (3).
b
Response options: never (0), occasionally (1), often (2), always (3).
c
Unrotated PCA indicated one component solution, accounting for 21.7% of variance.

2.2.3. Knowledge, attitudes and perceptions in relation to climate  Self-assessed knowledge about climate change: ‘How much,
change if anything, would you say you know about climate change’
The questionnaire included a range of questions relating to (5-point scale from ‘a lot’ to ‘nothing, have never heard of it’); and
knowledge, attitudes and perceptions in relation to climate change  Belief about the causes of climate change: ‘Do you think: Climate
(these results are reported elsewhere; see Whitmarsh, O’Neill, change is caused only by natural processes, Climate change is
Seyfang, & Lorenzoni, 2009). Some of these measures are consid- caused only by human activity, Climate change is caused by both
ered relevant to the current analysis, since pro-environmental natural processes and human activity, There’s no such thing as
behaviour may be motivated by concern about climate change climate change, or I don’t know what is causing climate change’
associated with perceived risk or responsibility (O’Connor, Bord, & from which participants were asked to select one option.
Fisher, 1999; Stern, Dietz, & Kalof, 1993), and may depend on
knowledge and understanding about the causes of climate change 3. Results
(Bord, O’Connor, & Fisher, 2000; O’Connor, Bord, Yarnal, & Wiefek,
2002). In the regression analyses, we have therefore included 3.1. TPB and self-identity in predicting offsetting
measures of:
In order to test the roles of self-identity and past behaviour in
 Personal importance of climate change: ‘How important is the addition to the TPB in determining intention to purchase carbon
issue of climate change to you personally?’ (4-point scale from offsets, we included TPB variables within a regression analysis
‘very important’ to ‘not at all important’); (Model 1), then extended the TPB with offsetting identity (Model 2)
 Perceived risk from climate change: ‘Do you think climate change and past behaviour (Model 3), and finally added general pro-
is something that is affecting or is going to affect you, environmental self-identity, pro-environmental values and PEB
personally?’ (‘yes’, ‘no’, ‘don’t know’); scores (Model 4). As shown in Table 3, the extended TPB had greater

Table 3
Regression analysis for carbon offsetting intention.

Model b SE B t Sig.
1 (Constant) .17 .50 .35 .72
Positive attitude (offsetting) .04 .00 .24 5.23 .00
Negative attitude (offsetting) .02 .01 .09 2.00 .04
Perceived behaviour control (offsetting) .00 .00 .01 .29 .77
Subjective norm (offsetting) .00 .00 .02 .57 .56

2 Offsetting identity .20 .02 .32 7.15 .00

3 Past offsetting behaviour .60 .12 .16 4.87 .00

4 Pro-environmental self-identity score .04 .01 .10 2.48 .01


Pro-environmental behaviour score .00 .00 .07 1.91 .05
Pro-environmental values (NEP) score .01 .01 .04 1.30 .19
310 L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314

Table 4
Rotated PCA of pro-environmental behaviours.

Component

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Cronbach’s alpha of positive loadings .73 .73 .71 .55 .58 .66 .53 –
Installed insulation products in your home .34 .48
Bought or built an energy-efficient home .64
Installed a more efficient heating system .31 .57
Installed a renewable energy system (e.g. solar panels, wind turbine) in your home .38 .40
Changed to a ‘green’ energy tariff for your home .53
Bought a low-emission vehicle (e.g. hybrid, electric, biofuel, <1.4 L engine)
Bought a product to save water (e.g. water but, water ‘hippo’, low-flush toilet) .48 .31
Save water by taking shorter showers .45 .50
Turn off the tap while you brush your teeth .31 .61
Turn off lights you’re not using .76
Eat food which is organic, locally-grown or in season .34 .63
Avoid eating meat .66
Buy environmentally-friendly products .70
Buy products with less packaging .62
Recycle .55 .43
Reuse or repair items instead of throwing them away .55
Compost your kitchen waste .75
Write to your MP about an environmental issue .82
Take part in a protest about an environmental issue .80
Drive economically (e.g. braking or accelerating gently) .70
Walk, cycle or take public transport for short journeys (i.e. trips of <3 miles) .56 .47
Use an alternative to travelling (e.g., shopping online) .63
Share a car journey with someone else .57
Cut down on the amount you fly .39 .42
How often do you personally use a car or van to travel (as driver or passenger)? .82
Did you take any flights in 2007 for leisure, holidays or visiting family or friends? .83

predictive power (Model 2, R2 ¼ .46; Model 3, R2 ¼ .49) on intention Correlation between the two types of identity (‘offsetting’ and
to offset than the standard TPB (Model 1: R2 ¼ .39), while the ‘pro-environmental’) is significant, though relatively weak (r ¼ .19;
general pro-environmental measures improved the predictive p ¼ .001).
power somewhat further (Model 4: R2 ¼ .52). (Background vari-
ables were also included in a fifth model but only contributed 1%
3.2. Relationships between pro-environmental behaviours
additional variance; only age was a significant, weak, negative
predictor).
To investigate the relationships between behaviours and any
In the standard TPB model, attitude is the only significant
spill-over, effects, a PCA with Varimax rotation was conducted on
predictor; subjective norm and perceived behavioural control are
the 24 PEB items. This indicated 8 components with Eigenvalues
both non-significant variables. In the extended model, offsetting
over 1, explaining 54.3% of variance (Table 4). Component 1 relates
identity exerts a strong positive influence, and past behaviour is
primarily to Waste reduction (with some energy/water conservation);
also a significant positive predictor; general pro-environmental
component 2 to Eco-shopping and eating; component 3 to Regular
identity (but not PEB or environmental values) is a significant, but
water and domestic energy conservation; component 4 to One-off
fairly weak, predictor.
domestic energy conservation actions; component 5 is Eco-driving;

Table 5
Regression analysis of all pro-environmental behaviours (PEB score).

b SE B t Sig.
Pro-environmental identity score 1.09 .19 .29 5.71 .00
Pro-environmental values (NEP score) .14 .12 .05 1.12 .26

Age .11 .45 .01 .25 .79


Gender .18 .32 .02 .55 .57
Household income .24 .15 .07 1.57 .11
Qualifications (general) .12 .24 .02 .52 .60
Qualifications (scientific subject) .34 .26 .06 1.28 .20
Political preference .14 .12 .04 1.15 .25
No of adults in household .44 .40 .04 1.08 .28
No of children in household 1.34 .46 .12 2.88 .00
Area density .53 .42 .05 1.26 .20

Knowledge about climate change .64 .64 .04 1.00 .31


Belief about causes of climate change .13 .49 .01 .26 .78
Personal importance of climate change 3.02 .61 .25 4.96 .00
Perceived risk from climate change .14 .11 .05 1.25 .21

(Constant) 1.09 4.23 .26 .79


L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314 311

component 6 is Political actions; component 7 is Reducing car use

L5.27**

2.79**
7. Reducing car use

2.09*
and flights; component 8 is Flying. (Since Flying is not a pro-envi-

.07

.44

1.19
.31

1.20
.50
.43

.05
1.04

.47

1.63
.97
1.42
ronmental behaviour it is not examined in the further analysis,

t
below). This analysis suggests some spill-over effects between

and flights
(R2 ¼ .18)
similar (in respect of context or frequency) behaviours.

L.28

.13
.12
.00

.02
.01

.06
.02
.02

.00
.05

.02

.07
.05
.06
3.3. Self-identity as a predictor of pro-environmental behaviours

B
Analysis was first conducted on the full set of pro-environ-

6. Political actions

2.12*

2.42*

2.04*
1.32

.61
.08

.12
.31
1.58
.52

1.50
1.42
.71
.75

1.39

.39
mental behaviours, using score on the PEB scale as the dependent
variable in a linear regression. The results (see Table 5) show that,

t
(R2 ¼ .09)
consistent with our expectations, pro-environmental self-identity
is the strongest (positive) predictor. Other significant variables are

.11

.14
.08

.03
.00

.00
.01
.09
.02

.07
.07

.07
.04
.04

.02
personal importance of climate change and number of children in

B
the household.
Regression analyses were also conducted for each behavioural

3.24**
2.03*

2.00*
L6.62*
1.13

.63
.81

.48
.31

1.37
.61

.29

1.46
1.77

.21
.35
cluster identified in the rotated PCA. Results from these analyses

5. Eco-driving
(aggregated in Table 6) suggest that self-identity is only a signifi-

t
cant predictor for some behaviours, namely waste reduction,

(R2 ¼ .17)
regular water and domestic energy conservation, and eco-shopping

.11

.09
L.32
.06

.03
.04

.02
.01

.06
.03

.07
.10
.01

.01
.01
and eating (for which it is the strongest predictor). Further, identity

B
is a negative (though non-significant) determinant of travel-related
PEBs. Pro-environmental values, measured using the NEP scale, do

2.60**

2.70**
not positively predict any of the PEBs.

2.04*

2.40*
actions (R2 ¼ .08)

1.08

.17
.13

.36
1.72

.59

.65
.57

.73
.88
.60

.59
Different background variables are also significant predictors of

conservation

t
the PEB clusters. Waste behaviours are associated with older and
4. One-off
female respondents with larger households in rural areas. Eco- domestic
energy

.10
.13

.16
.06

.00
.00

.02
.09

.03

.03
.03

.03
.05
.03

.03
shopping and eating is predicted by a high level of education.

B
Regular water/energy conservation is predicted by a low level of
education and urban location. One-off energy conservation is asso-
ciated more with male respondents with children. Eco-driving is

L2.67**

3.03**
2.16*

2.51*
.76

.13
.77
.42
1.56

1.21
.89

.76
.18
.15

.01

1.05
more likely amongst older respondents, with children in rural
domestic energy
3. Regular water

areas; while reducing car use and flying is more prevent amongst
conservation

young, highly educated, urban respondents.


(R2 ¼ .13)

Risk perception of climate change is not a significant predictor of


.12

.12
L.16

.17
.04

.00
.03
.02
.07

.06
.04

.03
.01
.00

.00
and

behaviours; while issue importance is relevant for political actions,


B

and all conservation actions. Generally there is little influence of


knowledge (apart from for political actions) or perceived causes of
4.98**

3.17**

5.66**
climate change. .92
1.80

.09
1.29

1.02
1.83

1.40
1.69

.40
.75
1.22

1.61
.44
eating (R2 ¼ .24)
2. Eco-shopping

4. Discussion
.27

.17
.09

.04
.06

.00
.04
.08

.06
.08

.01
.04
.05

.08
.02
4.1. Self-identity and pro-environmental behaviour
and

Our findings reinforce existing evidence for the importance of


self-identity in predicting environmentally-significant behaviour.
2.60**

2.81**

L3.98**

3.84**
2.10*

2.11*
L2.11*
1. Waste reduction

.57

.79
.50

.32
1.36
1.62
1.31

1.66
1.05

We found self-identity to be a significant behavioural determinant


over and above TPB variables for carbon offsetting behaviour.
t

Behaviour-specific self-identity (as a ‘carbon offsetter’) exerted the


(R2 ¼ .14)

strongest influence on intention to offset. Generic ‘pro-environ-


.15

.14
.11

.10
L.19
L.10
.03

.04
.02

.01
.08
.09
.06

.09
.05

mental’ self-identity also influenced offsetting intention, although


B

the influence was not as strong as the more specific identity


measure. We also found that past behaviour exerted a significant
Regression analyses for PEB components.

Personal importance of climate change


Pro-environmental values (NEP score)

and independent influence on intention. This supports the growing


Belief about causes of climate change

Perceived risk from climate change

body of evidence which indicates self-identity and past behaviour


Pro-environmental identity score

Knowledge about climate change


Qualifications (scientific subject)

are important influences on behavioural intention and, when


No of children in household

combined with TPB variables, can produce a more predictive model


No of adults in household

than the TPB alone.


Qualifications (general)

*Sig. < .05 ** Sig. < .01.

In relation to the broader range of pro-environmental behav-


Political preference
Household income

iours, pro-environmental self-identity was a significant predictor


for several of these categories of behaviours, namely waste reduc-
Area density

tion, regular water and domestic energy conservation, and eco-


(Constant)
Gender

shopping and eating (for which it was the strongest predictor).


Table 6

Age

However, one-off domestic energy conservation, travel and political


behaviours were not significantly predicted by identity.
312 L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314

Background and attitudinal variables were also important The weak relationship between the two types of identity
predictors. Gender, household composition, age, urban vs. rural (behaviour-specific and generic), as well as the association of past
location, and education were salient for different behavioural behaviour and specific identity, are also intriguing findings. As
clusters. This finding reflects the important structural constraints noted earlier, the relationships between different behavioural
and drivers of environmentally-significant behaviour, identified in levels of identity have not previously been examined but the
previous research (e.g., DEFRA, 2002; Lorenzoni et al., 2007). This is recently-drawn distinction between behavioural persistence over
particularly evident for travel behaviours, which are more depen- time versus consistency across behaviours (WWF-UK, 2009) may
dent than regular domestic or consumption behaviours on be instructive here. We may understand identity as having func-
contextual factors, such as the provision of alternatives to driving tions along both temporal and contextual dimensions. In other
(which are typically more available in urban than rural areas). words, pro-environmental self-identity helps us establish consis-
Similarly, installing domestic energy conservation products or tency and continuity across experiences. Further work is needed to
systems (e.g., insulation, boilers, solar panels) depends on contex- elucidate these functions of pro-environmental self-identity and to
tual factors such as home ownership (McKenzie-Mohr & Smith, consider how the ‘contextual’ dimension might relate to place
1999). Individuals may thus be unable to translate their pro-envi- identity and social identity.
ronmental self-identity into consistent pro-environmental behav-
iours due to lack of available options. In addition, the lack of 4.2. Policy implications
influence of pro-environmental self-identity on travel behaviours
may suggest competing identities, such as strong social identities Recent policy interest in cross-situational motivations and spill-
associated with car ownership and taking foreign holidays (Barr, over effects appears at least in part to be justified. The implication of
Coles, & Shaw, 2008; Steg et al., 2001). our research is that how we think of ourselves can have an important
Since our data are correlational and not experimental or longi- influence on pro-environmental intentions. This is particularly the
tudinal, we are unable to determine whether certain behaviours case for visible consumption behaviours, namely purchasing envi-
caused the adoption of others (i.e., acted as catalyst behaviours). ronmentally-friendly products. This has implications for the design
Nevertheless, our initial investigation in this area has reinforced of marketing strategies to promote green goods which target green
findings from other studies on the role of cross-situational envi- identities. More generally, the existence of identity as a driver across
ronmental motivations. Yet, in contrast to previous studies on spill- several behaviours highlights the need to prime relevant aspects of
over effects between PEBs (e.g., Barr et al., 2005; Thøgersen & identity (e.g., via targeted information) and target particular group
Ölander, 2006), we found that pro-environmental values identities (e.g., via segmentation). In the UK, DEFRA has adopted this
(measured using the NEP) did not predict any of the PEBs. Rather, latter approach by segmenting the public according to their values,
our analysis suggests that identity is a stronger cross-situational identities and background characteristics in order to target these
motivation for behaviour than values. However, we also found that groups with appropriate messages and interventions to encourage
personal importance of climate change is a significant predictor for greener lifestyles (DEFRA, 2008a).
political actions and all energy/water conservation actions perhaps A more challenging issue concerns addressing conflicts of
implying a common value basis motivating action (both direct and identity. Although we have noted a (weak) relationship between
indirect) to tackle climate change. This conclusion is not fully behaviour-specific and generic pro-environmental self-identity,
supported by other research, though, which suggests energy there are many other examples of incompatible identities. In
conservation tends to be motivated more by financial or health particular, how can we increase the salience of ‘green identity’
considerations than out of concern for climate change (Whitmarsh, relative to other (particularly, socially-prized) identities such as
2009). being ‘well-travelled’? Even the most committed environmentalists
It is also noteworthy that there is an apparent disparity between reconcile green credentials with their decision to fly on holiday
understanding and perceptions of climate change and pro-environ- (Barr et al., 2008; Whitmarsh, 2008b); and few are willing to give
mental action. None of the PEBs (apart from political action) were up their car (King et al., 2009) which is associated with a high
influenced by knowledge, and risk perception similarly exerted no standard of living and ‘quality of life’ (Black, Collins, & Snell, 2001).
significant influence.5 This is consistent with the widely reported Targeting people’s self-identity and social identity – their need to
knowledge-action gap in relation to pro-environmental behaviour in conform and to be consistent (Christensen et al., 2004; Festinger,
general (e.g., Kollmuss & Agyeman, 2002) and climate change in 1957) – may offer opportunities for changing behaviour. There is
particular (DEFRA, 2007; Whitmarsh, 2009). The disparity between evidence that cognitive dissonance can produce more pro-envi-
awareness and concern about climate change on the one hand, and ronmental behaviour particularly amongst people who are trying to
action on the other (even amongst those likely to be worst affected, be green, but fall short (Thøgersen, 2004). Thus, people who
such as flood victims; Whitmarsh, 2008a), highlights the significant perceive an inconsistency could be encouraged to take steps to
structural, social, informational, economic and psychological barriers change their behaviour and act more sustainably. On the other
to low-carbon lifestyles (Lorenzoni et al., 2007). hand, this strategy may have the opposite effect such that if the gap
In general, our regression analyses explained low levels of between desirable behaviour and pro-environmental behaviour
variance; the highest was for shopping and eating (i.e., consump- becomes too great, individuals change their attitude towards the
tion activities). This may be due to the measure used (the self- environment, rather than their lifestyle. Innovative strategies are
identity measure included an item explicitly on consumption) but required to use people’s guilt or shame to motivate change, rather
is more likely to be because shopping for material objects (unlike than disempowering and risking denial or apathy (Cohen, 2001;
energy or water use) is conspicuous consumption and more likely to O’Neill & Nicholson-Cole, 2009). Other possible approaches may
be an expression of identity (Belk, 1988; Dittmar, 1992). hinge on the interaction of self-identity and social identities, such
that a particular social identity may be changed through the actions
of significant individuals within that group (Postmes, Haslam, &
5
Swaab, 2005).
In fact, we found flying to be positively associated with knowledge of the human
influences on climate change, and noted a significant positive correlation between
At the same time, situational factors are also clearly important
high number of flights and green self-identity (reported elsewhere; see Whitmarsh, drivers and barriers to pro-environmental behaviour as noted
2008b). repeatedly in previous research. These factors will, and do,
L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314 313

constrain opportunities for green self-identity to be translated into Bem, D. (1967). Self-perception: an alternative interpretation of cognitive disso-
nance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74(3), 183–200.
effective pro-environmental behaviour. As discussed elsewhere,
Biddle, B. J., Bank, B. J., & Slavings, R. R. L. (1987). Norms, preferences, identities and
the implications here pertain to provision of enabling and equitable retention decisions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 50(4), 322–337.
mechanisms (e.g., affordable and efficient public transport, lower Black, C., Collins, A., & Snell, M. (2001). Encouraging walking: the case of journey-
cost eco-friendly goods, personal carbon allowances, community to-school trips in compact urban areas. Urban Studies, 38(7), 1121–1141.
Bord, R. J., O’Connor, R. E., & Fisher, A. (2000). In what sense does the public need to
competitions) for reducing society’s carbon dependence and the understand global climate change? Public Understanding of Science, 9, 205–218.
environmental impacts of behaviour (Lorenzoni et al., 2007). Carter, N., & Ockwell, D. G. (2007). New labour, new environment? An analysis of the
Labour government’s policy on climate change and biodiversity loss. Report prepared
for Friends of the Earth. Centre for Ecology Law & Policy (CELP), University of York.
4.3. Limitations and further research www.york.ac.uk/res/celp/projects/foe/docs/fullreportfinal.pdf.
Chakrabortty, A. (2008). From Obama to Cameron, why do so many politicians want
a piece of Richard Thaler?. Guardian online. Available from: Guardian Website.
In this research, we found support for an extended TPB model in www.guardian.co.uk/politics/2008/jul/12/economy.conservatives.
relation to carbon offsetting intention. However, we did not test Charng, H.-W., Pillavin, J. A., & Callero, P. L. (1988). Role identity and reasoned action
this model on the full range of PEBs. This would be a useful avenue in the prediction of repeated behaviour. Social Psychology Quarterly, 51, 303–317.
Christensen, N., Rothberger, H., Wood, W., & Matz, D. (2004). Social norms and
for future research, particularly in light of the structural constraints
identity relevance: a motivational approach to normative behaviour. Personality
on pro-environmental travel behaviours and thus the possible and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(10), 1295–1309.
salience of perceived behavioural control in this context. Further Cialdini, R. B. (2006). Influence: Psychology of persuasion. Collins business essentials
edition. New York: HarperCollins.
work should also consider behavioural frequency (not just past
Cohen, S. (2001). States of denial: Knowing about atrocities and suffering. Cambridge:
behaviour as a dichotomous variable, as in this study); offsetting is Polity.
a relatively infrequent activity, but for more regular activities (e.g., Cook, A. J., Kerr, G. N., & Moore, K. (2002). Attitudes and intentions towards
domestic energy and water conservation) there is a need to explore purchasing GM food. Journal of Economic Psychology, 23(5), 557–572.
Darnton, A. (2008). Practical guide: An overview of behaviour change models and their
possible moderating effects of past behavioural frequency on the uses. Government Social Research Unit. Available from: www.gsr.gov.uk/
relationship between self-identity and behaviour. Ideally, future downloads/resources/behaviour_change_review/practical_guide.pdf.
studies should also use multi-item scales to measure all constructs, DEFRA. (2002). Survey of public attitudes to quality of life and to the environment:
2001. London: Department for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs.
since these are more reliable than single items (which we used for DEFRA. (2005). Experimental statistics on carbon dioxide emissions at local authority
PBC as well as for past behaviour). and regional level. London: Department for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs.
Further work might also develop an expanded measure of pro- DEFRA. (2007). Survey of public attitudes and behaviours toward the environment:
2007. London: Department for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs.
environmental identity which encompasses more inter-personal and DEFRA. (2008a). A framework for pro-environmental behaviours. London: Depart-
situational dimensions, giving consideration to the links between ment for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs.
self-identity, social identities, role identities and place identity. DEFRA. (2008b). Science and research projects: exploring catalyst behaviour –
EV0508. Available from: http://randd.defra.gov.uk/Default.aspx?Menu
Related to this, our observation of an association between behaviour-
¼Menu&Module¼More&Location¼None&Completed¼0&ProjectID¼16324.
specific and generic pro-environmental identity deserves further Devine-Wright, P. (2009). Rethinking NIMBYism: the role of place attachment and
investigation. More broadly, there is still much work to be done in place identity in explaining place-protective action. Journal of Community &
Applied Social Psychology, 19(6), 426–441.
investigating the functions, construction, and communication of the
Dittmar, H. (1992). The social psychology of possessions: To have is to be. New York: St
various dimensions of pro-environmental identity. Martin’s Press.
Finally, our study represents an initial empirical investigation Dunlap, R., Van Liere, K., Mertig, A. G., & Jones, R. E. (2000). Measuring endorsement
using correlational data into the role of identity in determining of the new ecological paradigm: a revised NEP scale. Journal of Social Issues,
56(3), 425–442.
consistency across diverse pro-environmental behaviours. While we Eagly, A. H., Chaiken, S., & Jovanovich. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort
have identified the importance of identity in determining several Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.
such behaviours, further (experimental/longitudinal) work is needed Ellmers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (2002). Self and social identity. Annual Review of
Psychology, 53, 161–186.
to model spill-over effects and in particular to examine whether Fekadu, Z., & Kraft, P. (2001). Self-identity in planned behavior perspective: past
identity is causally implicated in spill-over between behaviours. behavior and its moderating effects on self-identity-intention relations. Social
Behavior and Personality, 29(7), 671–686.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford
Acknowledgements University Press.
Grewal, R., Mehta, R., & Kardes, F. R. (2000). The role of the social-identity function
of attitudes in consumer innovativeness and opinion leadership. Journal of
This research was funded through the Tyndall Centre for Climate
Economic Psychology, 21, 233–252.
Change Research, which is supported by three UK research coun- Hamilton, K., Bayon, R., Turner, G., & Higgins, D. (2007). State of the voluntary carbon
cils: NERC, EPSRC and ESRC. Grateful thanks go to the survey market 2007. London: Ecosystem Marketplace and New Carbon Finance.
respondents who kindly gave their time to make this research Herring, H., & Sorrell, S. (Eds.). (2008). Energy efficiency and sustainable consumption:
Dealing with the rebound effect. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
possible and to the valuable comments of two anonymous Hinchliffe, S. (1996). Helping the earth begins at home: the social construction of
reviewers. socio-environmental responsibilities. Global Environmental Change, 6(1), 53–62.
HM Government. (2008). Climate change bill. Commons amendments at 3rd reading.
Available from: http://www.publications.parliament.uk/pa/ld200708/ldbills/
References 087/2008087.pdf London.
Jackson, T. (2005). Motivating sustainable consumption: A review of evidence on consumer
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Organizational Behavior and behaviour and behavioural change. Guildford: Sustainable Development Research
Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211. Network. Available from: http://www.sd-research.org.uk/researchreviews/
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social documents/MotivatingSCfinal_000.pdf.
behavior. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. King, S., Dyball, M., Webster, T., Sharpe, A., Worley, A., DeWitt, J., et al. (2009).
Bamberg, S., & Schmidt, P. (2003). Incentives, morality, or habit? Predicting Exploring public attitudes to climate change and travel choices: Deliberative
students’ car use for university routes with the models of Ajzen, Schwartz, and research. Final report for department for transport. Leeds: Science & Policy
Triandis. Environment and Behavior, 35(2), 264–285. Ltd & ITS. Available from: http://www.dft.gov.uk/pgr/scienceresearch/social/
Barr, S., Coles, T., & Shaw, G. Changing behaviours for a changing climate: a lifestyles climatechange/attitudestoclimatechange.pdf:People.
approach. Paper presented at the International Conference on Climate change Kollmuss, A., & Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environ-
impacts and adaptation: Dangerous rates of change. Sept 22–24, 2008, Exeter. mentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior. Environ-
Barr, S., Gilg, A. W., & Ford, N. (2005). The household energy gap: examining the mental Education Research, 8(3), 239–260.
divide between habitual- and purchase-related conservation behaviours. Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2007). Normative, gain and hedonic goal frames guiding
Energy Policy, 33, 1425–1444. environmental behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 63(1), 117–137.
Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, Lippincott Mercer. (2006). Conscious consumer: insights from qualitative research.
15, 139–168. Report commissioned by the climate group and clean air cool planet, sponsored
314 L. Whitmarsh, S. O’Neill / Journal of Environmental Psychology 30 (2010) 305–314

by BSkyB. Available from: www.cleanair-coolplanet.org/information/Glynn_ Steg, L., Vlek, C., & Slotegraaf, G. (2001). Instrumental-reasoned and symbolic-
slides.pdf. affective motives for using a motor car. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic
Lorenzoni, I., Nicholson-Cole, S., & Whitmarsh, L. (2007). Barriers perceived to Psychology and Behaviour, 4(3), 151–169.
engaging with climate change among the UK public and their policy implica- Stern, P. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior.
tions. Global Environmental Change, 17(3–4), 445–459. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 407–424.
Lovell, H., Bulkeley, H., & Liverman, D. (2009). Carbon offsetting: sustaining Stern, P., Dietz, T., & Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and environmental
consumption? Environment & Planning A, 41(10), 2357–2379. concern. Environment and Behavior, 25(3), 322–348.
Mannetti, L., Pierro, A., & Livi, S. (2004). Recycling: planned and self-expressive Stets, J. E., & Biga, C. F. (2003). Bringing identity theory into environmental soci-
behaviour. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 227–236. ology. Sociological Theory, 21(4), 398–423.
McKenzie-Mohr, D., & Smith, W. (1999). Fostering sustainable behavior: An intro- Stryker, S., & Burke, P. J. (2000). The past, present, and future of an identity theory.
duction to community-based social marketing. Gabriola Island, B.C., Canada: New Social Psychology Quarterly, 63(4), 284–297.
Society Publishers. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In
O’Connor, R. E., Bord, R. J., & Fisher, A. (1999). Risk perceptions, general environ- S. Worchel, & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24).
mental beliefs, and willingness to address climate change. Risk Analysis, 19(3), Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
461–471. Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., & White, K. M. (1999). The theory of planned behaviour: self
O’Connor, R. E., Bord, R. J., Yarnal, B., & Wiefek, N. (2002). Who wants to reduce identity, social identity and group norms. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38,
greenhouse gas emissions? Social Science Quarterly, 83(1), 1–17. 225–244.
O’Neill, S., & Nicholson-Cole, S. (2009). ‘Fear won’t do it’: promoting positive Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health,
engagement with climate change through visual and iconic representations. wealth, and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Science Communication, 30(3), 355–379. Thøgersen, J. (2004). A cognitive dissonance interpretation of consistencies and
POST. (2007). POSTnote no. 290 – Voluntary carbon offsets. London: The Parlia- inconsistencies in environmentally responsible behaviour. Journal of Environ-
mentary Office of Science and Technology (POST). mental Psychology, 24, 93–103.
Postmes, T., Haslam, S. A., & Swaab, R. I. (2005). Social influence in small groups: an Thøgersen, J., & Ölander, F. (2006). To what degree are environmentally beneficial
interactive model of social identity formation. European Review of Social choices reflective of a general conservation stance? Environment and Behavior,
Psychology, 16, 1–42. 38(4), 550–569.
Proshansky, H., Fabian, A., & Kaminoff, R. (1983). Place-identity: physical world Verplanken, B., Aarts, H., & van Knippenberg, A. (1997). Habit, information acqui-
socialisation of the self. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 3, 57–83. sition, and the process of making travel mode choices. European Journal of Social
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press. Psychology, 27, 539–560.
Schultz, P. W., Oskamp, S., & Mainieri, T. (1995). Who recycles and when: a review of Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: a self-report index
personal and situational factors. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 1313–1330.
105–121. Whitmarsh, L. (2008a). Are flood victims more concerned about climate change
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of universal content and than other people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and
structure of values: extensions and cross-cultural replications. Journal of behavioural response. Journal of Risk Research, 11(3), 351–374.
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 878–891. Whitmarsh, L. (Nov 5, 2008b). Carbon offsetting: a way of avoiding emissions reduc-
Smith, J. R., Terry, D., Manstead, A. S. R., Louis, W. R., Kotterman, D., & Wolfs, J. tions?. Environment Research Web. Available at: environmentalresearchweb.org/
(2007). Interaction effects in the theory of planned behavior: the interplay of cws/article/opinion/36551.
self-identity and past behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, Whitmarsh, L. (2009). Behavioural responses to climate change: asymmetry of
2726–2750. intentions and impacts. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29, 13–23.
Sparks, P., & Shepherd, R. (1992). Self-identity and the theory of planned behaviour: Whitmarsh, L., O’Neill, S., Seyfang, G., & Lorenzoni, I. (2009). Carbon capability: What
assessing the role of identification with green consumerism. Social Psychology does it mean, how prevalent is it, and how can we promote it? Tyndall Working
Quarterly, 55(4), 388–399. Paper, No. 132.
Sparks, P., Shepherd, R., & Frewer, L. J. (1995). Assessing and structuring attitudes WWF-UK. (2009). Simple and painless? The limitations of spillover in environmental
toward the use of gene technology in food production: the role of ethical campaigning. Godalming, UK: WWF-UK. Available from: http://assets.wwf.org.
obligation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 16(3), 267–285. uk/downloads/simple_painless_report.pdf.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen